Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Hearing Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/heares

Review

Function and plasticity of the medial olivocochlear system


in musicians: A review
Xavier Perrot a, b, c, d, *, Lionel Collet a, b, c, d
a
Universit de Lyon, Lyon F-69000, France
b
INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, Universit Lyon 1, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon F-69000, France
c
Claude Bernard Lyon 1 University, Lyon F-69500, France
d
Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon Sud Teaching Hospital, Department of Audiology and Orofacial Explorations, Pierre-Bnite F-69310, France

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The outer hair cells of the organ of Corti are the target of abundant efferent projections from the olivo-
Received 23 May 2013 cochlear system. This peripheral efferent auditory subsystem is currently thought to be modulated by
Received in revised form central activity via corticofugal descending auditory system, and to modulate active cochlear micro-
11 August 2013
mechanics. Although the function of this efferent subsystem remains unclear, physiological, psycho-
Accepted 21 August 2013
Available online 30 August 2013
physical, and modeling data suggest that it may be involved in ear protection against noise damage and
auditory perception, especially in the presence of background noise. Moreover, there is mounting evidence
that its activity is modulated by auditory and visual attention. A commonly used approach to measure
olivocochlear activity noninvasively in humans relies on the suppression of otoacoustic emissions by
contralateral noise. Previous studies have found substantial interindividual variability in this effect, and
statistical differences have been observed between professional musicians and non-musicians, with
stronger bilateral suppression effects in the former. In this paper, we review these studies and discuss
various possible interpretations for these ndings, including experience-dependent neuroplasticity. We
ask whether differences in olivocochlear function between musicians and non-musicians reect differ-
ences in peripheral auditory function or in more central factors, such as topedown attentional modulation.
This article is part of a Special Issue entitled <Music: A window into the hearing brain>.

2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction concerning the inuence of musical training on peripheral auditory


function. Interestingly, experimental ndings obtained during the last
Musicians e most notably professional musicians e often show twenty years or so suggest that the activity of the medial olivocochlear
exceptional auditory abilities, especially, in perceptual tasks involving system (MOCS) e an efferent neural pathway originating in the
pitch discrimination (Kishon-Rabin et al., 2001; Micheyl et al., 2006), brainstem, which projects directly onto the cochleas e is enhanced in
auditory memory (Boh et al., 2011; Parbery-Clark et al., 2011; Strait musicians (Brashears et al., 2003; Micheyl et al., 1995a, 1997a; Perrot
et al., 2012), or auditory attention (Strait et al., 2010, 2013a). The neural et al., 1999). These ndings may have important implications for our
basis of enhanced auditory perceptual performance in musicians understanding of the neural basis of music-related changes in audi-
compared to non-musicians is not entirely clear. Several studies have tory function and processing. On the peripheral side, since the MOCS
suggested that early musical training can interact with the develop- modulates active cochlear micromechanics (ACMs), which are
ment, maturation, and plasticity of the central auditory system (Ellis involved in ne auditory sensitivity, improved frequency selectivity
et al., 2012; Herholz and Zatorre, 2012; Hyde et al., 2009; Oechslin and enhanced dynamic range, an increase in MOCS activity may
et al., 2013; Strait et al., 2013b). However, much less is known facilitate auditory perception, notably in competitive musical envi-
ronments. On the central side, since the MOCS is likely to be under a
topedown control of corticofugal descending auditory system
* Corresponding author. Centre Hospitalier Lyon-Sud, Service dAudiologie et (CDAS), a stronger MOCS may be interpreted as reecting a
Explorations Orofaciales, Pavillon Chirurgical 3A, 165, Chemin du Grand Revoyet, strengthening of corticofugal modulation, thereby open to attentional
F-69495 Pierre-Bnite Cedex, France. Tel.: 33 4 72 66 64 06;
fax: 33 4 78 86 33 41.
modulation and training-induced plasticity.
E-mail addresses: xavier.perrot@chu-lyon.fr (X. Perrot), lionel.collet@chu-lyon.fr In this article, we provide an overview of the main research
(L. Collet). ndings concerning MOCS function in musicians. We then consider

0378-5955/$ e see front matter 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.010
28 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Abbreviations MEM(R) middle-ear muscle (reex)


MOC(S) medial olivocochlear (system)
ACMs active cochlear micromechanics ms milliseconds
AN auditory nerve Mus musicians
BBN broadband noise Nmus non-musicians
BM basilar membrane OAE(s) otoacoustic emission(s)
CDAS corticofugal descending auditory system OHC(s) outer hair cell(s)
CN cochlear nucleus RE right ear
dB decibels SEA stimulus-equivalent attenuation
DPOAE(s) distortion-product otoacoustic emission(s) SFOAE(s) stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emission(s)
EOAE(s) evoked otoacoustic emission(s) SL sensation level
IDL intensity difference limen SOAE(s) spontaneous otoacoustic emission(s)
IHC(s) inner hair cell(s) SOC superior olivary complex
IN interneurons SPL sound pressure level
(k)Hz (kilo)hertz TDT tone decay test
LE left ear TEOAE(s) transiently-evoked otoacoustic emission(s)

physiological mechanisms that may be responsible for experience- bers are MOC bers (Moore, 2000). Secondly, comparative studies
dependent changes in MOCS function. In particular, we discuss with monkeys suggest that the number of crossed bers in humans
central and peripheral neuroplasticity hypotheses, as well as the is slightly higher than, or equal to, the number of uncrossed bers
functional, perceptual, and cognitive consequences of MOC activity (Guinan, 2006; Hilbig et al., 2009).
enhancement on peripheral auditory function and central auditory
processing. To put the review into context, we start with a brief 2.2. Physiology of medial olivocochlear system
description of the main anatomical and physiological characteris-
tics of the MOCS (for detailed reviews, see Guinan, 2006, 2011; 2.2.1. Neurophysiological properties of olivocochlear bers
Robles and Delano, 2008). Most electrophysiological studies have focused on MOC bers in
small mammals (Robertson and Gummer, 1985; Warren and
2. Overview of anatomical and physiological characteristics Liberman, 1989). However, the main ndings of these studies
of medial olivocochlear system have been replicated in humans (Berlin et al., 1995; Chabert et al.,
2002; Veuillet et al., 1991). One important physiological property
2.1. Organization of human olivocochlear system of olivocochlear bers is that they are responsive to many kinds of
acoustic stimulations e e.g., tones, broadband noise or amplitude-
The olivocochlear bundle, also known as the auditory efferent modulated tones e applied ipsilaterally, contralaterally or bilater-
system, was initially described in the cat by Rasmussen ally. This property is the basis of the non-invasive functional
(Rasmussen, 1946). As the name indicates, this system originates in assessment of MOC system activity through the acoustic suppres-
the superior olivary complex (SOC), which is located in the ventral sion of otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) (Collet et al., 1990; see Section
part of the pons, and it projects bilaterally onto the cochleas via the 2.4). Moreover, olivocochlear spontaneous activity coupled with
vestibular nerves. It involves two subsystems with distinct cochlear effects of transection of the olivocochlear bundle suggest
anatomical and functional features: the lateral olivocochlear sys- that the MOCS may exert a basal tonic control of the cochlea
tem, and the medial olivocochlear (MOC) system (Warr and Guinan, (Bonls et al., 1987; Zheng et al., 2000b; see also Subsection 4.1.2).
1979). Although current knowledge concerning the anatomy of
these subsystems stems primarily from experimental data in feline 2.2.2. Medial olivocochlear acoustic reex
and rodent animal models, post-mortem studies in humans and Olivocochlear bers constitute the efferent pathway of an
comparative studies in non-human primates indicate a similar acoustic reex loop (Liberman and Guinan, 1998). Two kinds of
subdivision of the human olivocochlear system in those species MOC acoustic reexes are described: ipsilateral and contralateral
(Hilbig et al., 2009; Moore, 2000). To our knowledge, the function of (see Fig. 1). The ipsilateral reex e which mobilizes both crossed
lateral olivocochlear system has never been assessed in humans; afferent and efferent bers e involves the contralateral crossed
accordingly, in this review, we focus on the MOCS. MOC bundle, whereas the contralateral reex e consisting of a
single afferent crossing e involves the ipsilateral uncrossed MOC
2.1.1. The medial olivocochlear system bundle (Guinan, 2006).
The MOCS originates in the medial part of the SOC which, in
humans, corresponds to nuclei of the periolivary region. It is 2.2.3. Olivocochlear effects on peripheral auditory system
comprised of thick myelinated nerve bers which project pre- The main neurotransmitter of MOC bers is acetylcholine. Its
dominantly onto the contralateral cochlea through the crossed synaptic release induces hyperpolarization of OHCs, resulting in
olivocochlear bundle. Other bers project onto the ipsilateral co- direct or indirect inhibition of OHC motilities1 (see Subsection 2.4.1;
chlea, through the uncrossed (or direct) olivocochlear bundle. Both for a review on cellular and molecular mechanisms of OHC efferent
types of MOC bers form synapses with outer hair cells (OHCs). modulation, see Russell and Lukashkin, 2008; Wersinger and Fuchs,
2011). The main consequences of MOCS activation on cochlear
2.1.2. Interspecies differences in mammalian olivocochlear system
Two main differences in the anatomy of the olivocochlear sys-
tem between humans and other mammals have been reported. 1
For simplicity, in this article, the expression OHC motilities is used to refer
Firstly, in humans, at least one half to two thirds of olivocochlear both to somatic electromotility and to active hair-bundle motility.
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 29

Fig. 1. Organization diagram of ipsilateral and contralateral medial olivocochlear


(MOC) reexes. Both reexes are elicited by an acoustic stimulation (shown here as a
blue note), the terms ipsilateral and contralateral referring to the side of this stim-
ulation relative to the cochlea where the MOCS acts. Afferent excitatory pathways are
shown in solid green lines, efferent inhibitory pathways in dashed red lines. The
ipsilateral reex involves the following structures: ipsilateral cochlea (in this example,
right one) e with activation of inner hair cells (IHCs) e and auditory nerve (AN) type I
bers, ipsilateral cochlear nucleus (CN) and crossing interneurons (IN), contralateral
medial superior olivary complex (SOC), crossed MOC bundle back on ipsilateral cochlea Fig. 2. Diagram of MOCS effect on auditory-nerve-ber activity according to back-
e with inhibition of outer hair cells (OHCs). The contralateral MOC reex involves the ground conditions. The four curves show different patterns of rate-versus-level func-
following structures: contralateral cochlea (in this example, right one) e with acti- tion for an auditory nerve (AN) ber in response to tone bursts. In a silent background
vation of IHCs e and AN bers, contralateral CN and crossing interneurons, ipsilateral (solid lines), MOCS acts through inhibition of AN ber responses, with a shift to higher
medial SOC, uncrossed MOC bundle onto ipsilateral cochlea (in this example, left one) intensities, resulting in a decreased sensitivity to sounds (1). In a continuous noise
e with inhibition of OHCs. background (dashed lines), the dynamic range of AN ber responses is compressed,
due to increased activity at low levels (with increased response threshold, 2a) and
adaptation at high levels (with decreases saturation threshold, 2b). In that case, MOCS
activation partially restores the effective dynamic range of AN ber, by decreasing
mechanics are a reduction of the gain of cochlear amplication and responses to background noise (3a) and increasing the level at which saturation occurs
of sound-induced basilar-membrane (BM) motion, resulting in a (3b), thereby reinstating sensitivity to transient sounds. This is called the anti-(excit-
decrease in afferent auditory-nerve-ber responses to sound2 atory) masking effect of MOCS. (Adapted from Guinan, 2006)
(Guinan, 2006; Robles and Delano, 2008). Importantly, the effect of
MOC activity on auditory-nerve-ber activity differs depending on
the presence of background noise (see Fig. 2). As a result, MOCS
medial geniculate body is mainly involved in thalamo-cortico-
activation may have opposite sensorineural e and possibly, percep-
thalamic circuits.
tual e effects on responses to transient acoustic stimuli: a reduction
It is worth noting that several studies have described descend-
in sensitivity in quiet, and an improvement of discriminability in
ing pathways from the auditory cortex that bypass the inferior
continuous background noise (Chintanpalli et al., 2012; Liberman
colliculus and reach directly the cochlear nucleus and the superior
and Guinan, 1998). This may explain discrepant ndings concern-
olivary complex (Schoeld and Coomes, 2006). Thus, the auditory
ing the functional role of MOCS in humans (see Section 5.2).
cortex is in a position to directly modulate both the cochlear
transduction of acoustic stimuli, through the MOCS, and the initial
2.3. Corticofugal modulation of medial olivocochlear system stages of neural sound processing, at the level of the cochlear
nucleus.
The CDAS can modulate the activity of subcortical and brainstem
neurons (for reviews, see Suga, 2012; Winer, 2006). Interestingly, 2.3.2. Cortico-olivocochlear modulation in humans
recent electrophysiological studies in bats have shown that this Until recently, only indirect evidence for a cerebral inuence on
topedown inuence extends all the way down to the cochlea, MOC activity and cochlear function in humans was available, based
through modulation of the MOCS and ACMs (Xiao and Suga, 2002a, mainly on assessment of contralateral suppression of OAEs (see
2002b). Subsection 2.4.2). Apart from studies on attentional modulation of
the MOCS and possible effects of auditory learning, which will be
2.3.1. Corticofugal descending auditory system discussed below (see Subsections 4.1.1 and 4.1.3), ndings of two
Broadly, the organization of the CDAS may be summarized as experimental studies in epileptic patients have provided further
follows: (i) the CDAS originates in the auditory cortex and forms evidence in favor of this attractive hypothesis.
multiple feedback loops with the ascending auditory system3 The rst study reported postsurgical changes in MOC activity
(Rouiller, 1997); (ii) the CDAS projects mainly to the ipsilateral after resection of temporal brain structures involved in auditory
side of the midbrain and the brainstem; (iii) as for ascending processing, with either a decrease in the ear contralateral to the
auditory pathways, the inferior colliculus is an almost obligatory brain surgery, or an increase in the ear ipsilateral to the surgery
synaptic relay of descending auditory pathways, whereas the (Khalfa et al., 2001a). The second study used an innovative exper-
imental procedure during presurgical functional brain mapping of
refractory epilepsy (Perrot et al., 2006). By combining evoked OAE
2
Other MOC effects e described as non-classic effects e may act independently (EOAE) recording and electrical brain stimulation, it was shown
from modulation of BM motion by the cochlear amplier (Guinan, 2011). Their that stimulation of auditory cortex induced a contralateral decrease
underlying mechanisms may involve changes in the endocochlear potential and in EOAE amplitude, whereas no change occurred under stimulation
complex micromechanical interactions between cochlear vibrational components.
3
of non-auditory areas. These ndings are of utmost importance
Since most auditory pathways are reciprocal, the descending auditory system
reproduced the organization of the ascending auditory system, with three parallel
since they provide a direct evidence of a functional cortico-
channels anatomically and functionally distinct e corresponding to the tonotopic, olivocochlear pathway in humans, originating in the auditory cor-
non-tonotopic (or diffuse) and polysensory systems (Rouiller, 1997). tex and modulating contralateral ACMs, via the MOCS.
30 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Moreover, data in normal hearing subjects have shown a func- recording conditions, OAEs presumably reect a mixture of the two
tional interaural asymmetry of MOC activity according to handedness, mechanisms.
with a stronger inhibitory effect in right ear for right-handed subjects
(Khalfa et al., 1998b). This peripheral auditory lateralization may 2.4.2. Principles of otoacoustic emission suppression
reect the well-known cerebral hemispheric asymmetry, particularly The method used for assessing MOC activity in humans is
for speech processing (Lazard et al., 2012), and its asymmetrical cor- applicable to all types of OAEs. In broad outline, the procedure
ticofugal descending inuence on MOCS (Sininger and Cone-Wesson, consists in recording OAEs in the absence and in the presence of an
2004). Besides, a reverse pattern of MOCS lateralization has been acoustic stimulation, usually a broadband noise (BBN).
observed in neuropsychiatric diseases such as autism and schizo- The most widely used technique is known as contralateral sup-
phrenia, which may be related to abnormal brain asymmetry in these pression (or attenuation) of click-EOAEs (Berlin et al., 1993; Collet
patients (Khalfa et al., 2001b; Veuillet et al., 2001). et al., 1992). It involves two types of acoustic stimulation: ipsilateral
repetitive clicks e as evoking (or elicitor) stimulation e for recording
2.4. Assessment of medial olivocochlear system through EOAEs in one ear; and contralateral continuous BBN e as suppressor
suppression of otoacoustic emissions stimulation e applied in the other ear. BBN activates the contralateral
MOC reex (see Subsection 2.2.2 and Fig. 1), which in turn inhibits
Based on cellular characteristics of OHCs and neurophysiological OHC motilities and modulates generators of OAEs, thereby leading to a
properties of MOCS, Collet and colleagues developed a non- decrease in OAE amplitude (see also Subsection 2.2.3 and above). The
invasive test for exploring MOC activity in humans, through inhibitory efferent effect is computed as the difference in EOAE
acoustic suppression of OAEs (Collet et al., 1990). amplitude with and without contralateral noise (for more detail, see
Khalfa et al., 1998a; Veuillet et al., 1991; and Table 1, note b). In this
2.4.1. What are otoacoustic emissions? context, the magnitude of the contralateral suppression effect is taken
2.4.1.1. General description. OAEs may be described as acoustic vi- as an indication of the strength of stimulus-induced MOC activity.
brations emitted by the inner ear, which go backward through the Interestingly, efferent suppression can be observed even for low
middle ear and are recordable into the outer ear canal (for a general sound level of contralateral noise (from 50 dB SPL).
review on OAEs, see Kemp, 2002). Among the four types of OAEs Other methods for assessing MOC activity include the use of
conventionally described according to the measurement method, ipsilateral or binaural BBN elicitors e for ipsilateral or binaural
the two most common are transiently-EOAEs (TEOAEs) e induced suppression of EOAEs, as well as intermittent BBN elicitors e during
by transient acoustic stimulations such as clicks or tone pips4 e and an OAE forward-masking paradigm (for more detail, see Berlin
distortion-product OAEs (DPOAEs) e which are combination tones et al., 1995 and Table 1, note k).
arising in response to continuous stimulation by two pure tones.5 It is important to note that large interindividual differences in
contralateral suppression of OAEs e i.e., in MOC activity e have
2.4.1.2. Generation mechanisms of otoacoustic emissions. OAEs are been observed. This nding, possibly originating from innate bio-
commonly regarded as by-products of vibrations induced by OHC logical factors, suggests that whatever its function, MOCES will not
motilities, which are the cellular substratum of ACMs.6 In mam- be equally effective across individuals (Backus and Guinan, 2007;
mals, two types of OHC motility have been described: (i) a somatic Collet et al., 1992; de Boer and Thornton, 2008).
electromotility, mediated by prestin e a voltage-sensitive protein
located in the lateral wall; (ii) an active hair-bundle motility, 3. Medial olivocochlear activity in musicians
dependent on myosin-based adaptation and Ca2-mechano-
transducer channels, with an important role of stereocilin e a Almost fteen years ago, it has been shown that professional
protein involved in the joint between the stereocilia (for a recent musicians had a larger bilateral olivocochlear efferent suppression
composite review on cochlear amplication mechanisms, see than non-musicians (Perrot et al., 1999). These results, indicating a
Ashmore et al., 2010).7 stronger MOC activity in musicians than in non-musicians, suggest
However, the notion that OAEs are generated through ACMs only a possible neurophysiological mechanism underlying music-
(Knight and Kemp, 2001) has been challenged. By analyzing phase related changes in the auditory system. An interesting question,
shifts of SFOAEs and DPOAEs according to the frequency of the the answer to which remains unclear, is whether this peripheral
evoking stimulus components, Shera and Guinan evidenced that effect reects training-induced changes in central auditory func-
OAEs were generated by two distinct e although not exclusive e tion, e.g., strengthening of corticofugal connections (see Subsection
mechanisms (for a recent review, see Shera, 2004). The rst one is a 2.3.2 and Section 4.1). Another interesting question, which also
passive linear coherent reection e formerly known as place-xed remains open, relates to perceptual consequences of the enhanced
emission source, which originates in pre-existing irregularities (re- MOC activity in musicians, in particular, with regards to selective-
ectors) in the cochlear partition: this mechanism is predominant listening abilities (see Subsection 2.2.3 and Sections 5.2 and 5.3).
for TEOAES and SFOAEs. The second one is an active nonlinear A caveat stems from the fact that studies of MOC activity in
distortion e formerly known as wave-xed emission source, which musicians remain scarce. On the whole, the assessment of intergroup
arises from OHC nonlinearity inherent in ACMs: this mechanism is differences addressed two main parameters: strength and interaural
predominant for DPOAEs. It is worth noting that under usual asymmetry. It is worth noting that all of the musician participants
were either high-level music students or professional classical mu-
sicians, with a formal musical training of more than 10 years and a
4
For a detailed guide on TEOAE recording, see Kemp et al., 1990. daily practice of more than 3 h. Moreover, all but one of them were
5
The two other types of OAEs are stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissions instrumentalists (for detailed comparative ndings, see Table 1).
(SFOAEs) e evoked by a single pure tone e and spontaneous otoacoustic emissions
(SOAEs) e emitted in absence of any acoustic stimulation. 3.1. Strength of efferent suppression
6
Schematically, ACMs are characterized by four properties: amplication, fre-
quency selectivity, nonlinear compression and OAEs (Manley, 2000).
7
The respective roles of these two types of cellular processes in ACMs remain a
Several studies have found that efferent suppression was stronger
matter of debate, even though a combination of the two processes in an integrated in musicians than in non-musicians, for both right (Micheyl et al.,
system is the most likely hypothesis (Peng and Ricci, 2011). 1995a, 1997a) and left (Brashears et al., 2003; Perrot et al., 1999)
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 31

Table 1
Medial olivocochlear system activity in musicians compared to non-musicians: physiological and perceptual data.

Studies Subjects Methods Main results

A/Micheyl, 11 music students from the 1. Loudness adaptation: TDT in RE 1. TDT:


Carbonnel national music conservatory - mean adaptation levels at 4 kHz smaller in Mus
and Collet, of Lyon, France (7 females, 4 males)a - test frequencies: 0.5, 1 than in Nmus (differences controlled for sex)
1995a 24 Nmus (11 females, 13 males) and 4 kHz 2. Contralateral suppression in RE:
2. MOC activity: contralateral - stronger in Mus than in Nmus (only for 62
suppression of TEOAEs in RE and 65-dB SPL ipsilateral stimulus intensities)
(Lyon procedure of stimulus- 3. Correlation analysis:
equivalent attenuationb) - no correlation between TDT and SEA
- MOC activity explains 53% of the variance of
TDT in Mus (only 6% in Nmus)

B/Micheyl, 16 music students from the MOC activity: contralateral 1. Intragroup comparisons:
Khalfa, national music conservatory of suppression of TEOAEs in RE - in both groups: signicant decrease in TEOAE
Perrot and Lyon, France (8 females, 8 males)c (Lyon procedure of SEA) amplitude under contralateral BBN
Collet, 1997a 16 Nmus (8 females, 8 males) - for Nmus: no difference in TEOAE growth-
function parametersd (y-intercept and slope
coefcient) under contralateral BBN
- for Mus: change in TEOAE growthefunction
parameters under contralateral BBN
2. Intergroup comparisons:
- no difference for TEOAE growthefunction
parameters
- greater RE SEA of TEOAEs in Mus

C/Micheyl, 20 music students from the 1. Auditory intensity discrimination: 1. IDL:


Perrot and national music conservatory of IDL in RE - no difference between IDL in quiet and IDL
Collet, Lyon, France (10 females, 10 males)e - test tone: 1-kHz tone pip with contralateral noise
1997b - 2I-2AFC procedure - average IDL decreased under bilateral
- four conditions: quiet, ipsilateral noise (IDL-shift)
noise, contralateral noise, 2. Contralateral suppression in RE:
bilateral uncorrelated noise - change in TEOAE growthefunction
2. MOC activity: contralateral parameters under contralateral BBN
suppression of TEOAEs in (with a large intersubject variability)
RE (Lyon procedure of SEA) 3. Correlation analysis:
- no correlation between normalized IDL-
shift and SEA
- positive correlation between normalized
IDL-shift and change in TEOAE growthe
function parameters

D/Perrot, 16 professional Mus or music MOC activity: contralateral suppression 1. Intragroup comparisons:
Micheyl, Khalfa students from national music of TEOAEs in RE and LE - in both groups/both ears: signicant decrease
and Collet, 1999 conservatory of Lyon, France (Lyon procedure of SEA) in TEOAE amplitude under contralateral BBN
(8 females, 8 males/13 right - in both groups/both ears: change in TEOAE
handers, 1 ambidextrous, 2 growthefunction parameters under
left handers)f contralateral BBNg
16 Nmus (8 females, 8 males/ - in both groups: RE SEA larger than LE SEA
all right handers) (stronger contralateral suppression in RE)
2. Intergroup comparisons:
- no difference in baseline TEOAE amplitude
- for both ears: change in TEOAE growthe
function parameters under contralateral BBN
larger in Musg
- for both ears: SEA was larger in Mus
- no difference in attenuation quotienth (same
interaural asymmetry of contralateral
suppression)
- for all measurements/in both ears: greater
intersubject variability in Musg
3. Correlation analysis:
- in both groups/both ears: no correlation
between baseline TEOAE amplitude and SEA
- in both groups: no correlation between
laterality quotienti and interaural asymmetry
index (attenuation quotient)g

(continued on next page)


32 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Table 1 (continued )

Studies Subjects Methods Main results

E/Brashears, 29 professional Mus from the 1. MEMR: ipsilateral and contralateral 1. Baseline intergroup comparisons:
Morlet, Louisiana philharmonic thresholds (0.5, 1, 2 and 4-kHz - no difference in baseline TEOAE amplitude
Berlin orchestra, USA (17 females, tones; BBN) - MEMR thresholds higher in Mus (esp. for
and Hood, 2003 12 males)j 2. MOC activity: binaural suppression contralateral 500 Hz tones)
28 Nmus (17 females, 11 males) of TEOAEs in RE and LE (Kresge 2. Bilateral binaural suppression:
Hearing Research Laboratory - global comparison: nearly signicant greater
procedurek) overall suppression (between 8 and 18 ms)
in Mus
- temporal comparison: greater late suppression
(between 8 and 20 ms) in Mus, for both ears
(signicant only for RE), with a nearly
signicant RE advantage (only for Mus)
- spectral comparison: greater suppression in
Mus, both for RE (between 1 and 4 kHz) and
LE (between 0.5 and 3.5 kHz)
- age effect: greater suppression in younger
than older subjects, for both ears (in Mus only)
- no signicant gender effect, in either group
and for either ear (trend to greater overall
suppression in women)
3. Correlation analysis: In both groups/both ears:
absence of correlation between binaural
suppression and:
- MEMR thresholds for BBN
- baseline TEOAE amplitude

This table summarizes the comparative data obtained in normal-hearing adult non-musicians (Nmus) and musicians (Mus), with studies presented in ascending
chronological order.
a
Brass and percussion instrumentalists were not included in the Mus group. There was no signicant age difference between groups (handedness not controlled).
b
Insofar as EOAE amplitude increases with ipsilateral click intensity, the Lyon procedure consisted in measuring amplitude of TEOAEs for 3 to 6 ipsilateral click
intensities (ranging from 56 to 80 dB SPL), without and with a contralateral continuous BBN (of 30 or 35 dB SL). The contralateral suppression was then computed
through the stimulus-equivalent attenuation (SEA) of TEOAEs, dened as the mean reduction across all tested ipsilateral click intensities which would be required to
obtain in silent background (i.e., without activation of MOCS) the same attenuation in TEOAE amplitude as that induced by contralateral BBN. The time window
analysis was [2.5e20 ms].
c
The Mus group included 12 string and 4 woodwind instrumentalists. Experimental groups were paired for age and sex (handedness not controlled).
d
The input/output linear growth function represents EOAE amplitude plotted against ipsilateral click intensity. It provides a comprehensive measure of MOCS
effect, regardless of click intensity, through change in function parameters (i.e., increase in slope coefcient and decrease in y-intercept).
e
The Mus group included 12 string and 6 woodwind instrumentalists, and 2 pianists (handedness not controlled).
f
The Mus group included 10 string instrumentalists, 1 utist, 4 pianists, and 1 singer. Experimental groups were paired for age and sex.
g
These are unpublished data.
h
The attenuation quotient was computed as the ratio of interaural algebraic difference of SEA relative to absolute value of the sum of the SEA:
(RE SEA  LE SEA)/jRE SEA LE SEAj.
i
The laterality quotient was assessed using a French version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldeld, 1971).
j
The Mus group included string, woodwind, brass and percussion instrumentalists. Experimental groups were paired for age and sex (handedness not controlled).
k
The Kresge Hearing Research Laboratory procedure is based on a forward masking paradigm: it consisted in measuring amplitude of transiently-evoked
otoacoustic emissions without and with bilateral bursts of broadband noise (at 70 dB SPL) starting 10 ms before each ipsilateral click (at 65 dB SPL). The bilateral
suppression was computed as the TEOAE root mean square amplitude difference between the sums of with-noise and without-noise conditions (two recordings for
each condition). The time and frequency window analyses were [2e20 ms] and [0.195e6.25 kHz]. Abbreviations: IDL, intensity difference limen; LE, left ear; RE,
right ear; SEA, stimulus-equivalent attenuation; TDT, tone decay test. (For other abbreviations, see Abbreviation list)

ears (Fig. 3). Interestingly, this was not only true for contralateral and amount of training, instrument played and listening bi-
suppression, which involves specically the uncrossed MOC system ography, possession of perfect pitch, musical environment at
(Perrot et al., 1999; Table 1-D), but also for binaural suppression, home (Elbert et al., 1995; Margulis et al., 2009; Schlaug et al.,
which involves the crossed MOC system as well (Brashears et al., 1995; Schneider et al., 2002, 2005; Strait et al., 2013a).8 Thus,
2003; Table 1-E). These results show that MOCS can be activated interindividual differences in these factors may partially account
not only under experimental conditions, i.e., with a monaural noise for the observed intragroup variability of MOC activity in musi-
suppressor, but also under more ecological conditions closer to cians. On the other hand, since cognitive brain functions may also
musical practice, i.e., with binaural noise suppressor (see Subsection be inuenced by musical expertise (Oechslin et al., 2013; Strait
2.4.2). et al., 2010, 2012, 2013a),9 we cannot exclude that interindi-
It must be emphasized that a greater interindividual variability vidual differences in cognitive functioning during experiment e
in OAE suppression was observed for musicians than for non- e.g., involuntary attentional orienting to experimental acoustic
musicians (Micheyl et al., 1997b; Perrot et al., 1999). This nding stimuli (Draganova et al., 2009) e may have interfered with
raises the question of whether individual factors not controlled in
these studies play a role in the development of auditory-system
specicities in musicians, in particular regarding ACMs and
8
MOCS functioning. On the one hand, several musical factors have For a recent review on brain plasticity induced by musical training, see Herholz
already been identied as being involved in more central plas- and Zatorre (2012).
9
For a recent review on the interrelationship between music and cognitive
ticity phenomena in musicians e e.g., earliness of musical practice
abilities, see Schellenberg and Weiss (2013).
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 33

only; however, the difference in suppression between the left and


right ears failed to reach the statistical signicance threshold.10

3.3. Additional ndings

Other results pertaining to OAEs may be viewed as indirect


evidence of difference in MOCS functioning between musicians and
non-musicians.
A study in normal-hearing young adults found larger DPOAEs
in professional musicians than in non-musicians paired for age
and gender, irrespective of whether the musicians had absolute
pitch or not (Morawski et al., 1999). Since this difference was only
signicant for mid-range intensities of primary tones (below
60 dB SPL), and not for high stimulation levels, which also
generate passive reection responses from the basilar membrane
(Shera, 2004), it was interpreted as an indication of the inuence
of protracted musical training on ACMs. More speculatively, this
may also suggest a particular operation mode of MOCES in mu-
sicians, e.g., through a decrease in basal inhibitory activity of the
MOC (see Section 2.2.1).
However, it is worth noting that in the works of Perrot et al.
(1999) and Brashears et al. (2003), although baseline EOAE ampli-
Fig. 3. Intragroup and intergroup comparisons of mean contralateral EOAE suppres- tudes for both ears tended to be larger (albeit not statistically
sion effects for both ears of musicians (Mus) and non-musicians (Nmus). The sup- signicantly) in professional musicians than in non-musicians, no
pression effect of MOCS activation (bar chart and left Y-axis, expressed in dB) is based correlation between efferent suppression and baseline amplitude
on the stimulus-equivalent attenuation metrics (for denition: see Note b in Table 1). of EOAEs was found (Table 1-D and 1-E). This outcome is in line
Suppression effects are signicantly larger: (i) in Mus than in Nmus, for both ears
(*p < 0.05); (ii) in right ear than in left ear, for both groups (*p < 0.05). Error bars
with earlier ndings in non-musician subjects, which showed that
represent the standard errors of the across-subject means. The attenuation quotient the amplitude of EOAEs and the magnitude of contralateral OAE
(box plots and right Y-axis) reects interaural asymmetry of contralateral efferent suppression were two independent markers of peripheral auditory
suppression (for denition: see Note h in Table 1). There is no signicant difference functioning, the former reecting ACMs whereas the latter pre-
between Mus and Nmus. Bar lines (error bars) represent 25th, 50th, and 75th (10th
sumably reects MOC activity (Hood et al., 1996; Khalfa et al.,
and 90th) percentiles; lower and upper dots represent mean of the data outside the
5th and the 95th percentiles. (Adapted from Perrot et al., 1999) 1998a).

4. Mechanisms of enhanced MOC efferent suppression in


musicians
cortico-olivocochlear modulation of peripheral auditory func-
tioning (de Boer and Thornton, 2007, 2012; Giard et al., 1994; 4.1. Central hypothesis: musical training related-plasticity of
Maison et al., 2001). It is worth noting that both hypotheses corticofugal descending auditory system
concerning the origin of the increased variability are consistent
with the existence of a cortico-olivocochlear control of MOC ac- The rst hypothesis involves a strengthening of cortico-
tivity in humans, which may be modulated by attentional pro- olivocochlear modulation of the MOCS with musical practice. For
cesses and auditory training (see Subsection 2.3.2 and for such experience-dependent plasticity in corticofugal inuences on
discussion, Section 4.1). the MOCS to occur, two conditions must be fullled: (i) a cortico-
fugal topedown control of the MOCS and (ii) a basal tonic activity
for the cortico-olivocochlear pathways, which are plastic and can
3.2. Interaural asymmetry of efferent suppression be modied by musical training.

The two studies that have assessed MOC activity in the right and 4.1.1. Corticofugal descending modulation of cochlear functioning
left ears of musicians have failed to demonstrate a clear difference In addition to studies in epileptic patients and ndings of
in interaural asymmetry of efferent suppression between musi- interaural asymmetry of the MOCS (see Subsection 2.3.2), the other
cians and non-musicians (Brashears et al., 2003; Perrot et al., 1999). main line of evidence for a functional CDAS in humans relate to
In the rst study (Perrot et al., 1999; Table 1-D), musicians attentional modulation of MOC activity and OAEs (for reviews, see
showed a bilateral enhancement of MOC activity but with the same Meric and Collet, 1994; Giard et al., 2000).
right-ear predominance in contralateral efferent suppression as
already evidenced in non-musicians (Khalfa et al., 1998b). This 4.1.1.1. Intramodal modulation: auditory selective attention.
result suggests that the effect of musical exposure and/or practice Several studies have shown an inuence of attention in auditory
on MOC activity in musicians is global, rather than side-specic. perceptual tasks, either on MOC activity-modulation of the
The result also suggests that corticofugal inuences do not elimi- contralateral suppression of OAEs (de Boer and Thornton, 2007;
nate the pattern of right-ear predominance of MOC activity, even in Maison et al., 2001) or on ACMs-change in OAEs recorded in quiet
listeners with extensive musical training; this observation may be (Giard et al., 1994). Results showed that, in a dichotic-listening
of interest in the context of studies of speech processing (Lazard condition, when attention was directed toward the evoking
et al., 2012) (see Subsection 2.3.2 and for further discussion,
Subsection 4.1.4).
In the second study (Brashears et al., 2003; Table 1-E), efferent 10
This could be related to experimental procedure used for assessing MOC ac-
suppression was found to be stronger in musicians for the right ear tivity, i.e., a binaural (rather than a contralateral) BBN.
34 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

stimuli, or toward the ear where EOAEs were being recorded, 4.1.2. Basal cortico-olivary tonic activity
efferent suppression was weaker (de Boer and Thornton, 2007) and Several experimental data support the existence of a corticofugal
EOAE amplitude increased (Giard et al., 1994), suggesting tope basal tonic activity regulating MOC activity, independently of any
down inhibition of the MOCS by attention. On the contrary, when specic brain activation. Experiments in animal models showed that
attention was directed to probe tones embedded in the noise auditory cortex deactivation, either through cortical cooling or
suppressor of the opposite ear, efferent suppression was stronger neuropharmacological agents, such as GABAergic agonist or lido-
and EOAE amplitude decreased (Maison et al., 2001), suggesting a caine, resulted in amplitude decrease in cochlear microphonics (Len
topedown enhancement of MOCS. Similar results have been evi- et al., 2012; Xiao and Suga, 2002a). These results demonstrate that
denced for active listening to speech embedded in a contralateral blocking spontaneous activity of auditory cortex leads to a temporary
BBN, with a greater suppression of click-EOAEs for both ears suspension of cortico-olivary tonic activity, thereby reducing MOCS
compared with passive listening (Garinis et al., 2011). These nd- basal inhibition. Similarly, the use of barbiturate anesthesia, known
ings suggest that topedown auditory attentional modulation can to induce cortical depression, has been shown to suspend contra-
have a differential interaural effect on cochlear functioning, lateral acoustic suppression of cochlear functioning, subsequently
increasing efferent suppression in one ear when attention is restored through electrical stimulation of the inferior colliculus
focusing on the other ear (de Boer and Thornton, 2007; de Boer (Mulders and Robertson, 2002).
et al., 2012). However, it is important to note that the effects In humans, Khalfa et al.s study in epileptic patients (2001a) has
observed in those studies were often quite small (and borderline shown that neurosurgical resections of auditory areas produced
statistically signicant), with large interindividual differences in changes in MOC activity (see Subsection 2.3.2). This is consistent
the magnitude of the effects, and that other studies have obtained with the existence of a basal cortico-olivary tonic activity and with
different results (Harkrider and Bowers, 2009) or failed to show any the possibility that this activity has a differential inuence on
signicant attentional inuence on OAEs (Michie et al., 1996).11 cochlear functioning: inhibitory in the contralateral ear, and facil-
itatory in the ipsilateral ear.
4.1.1.2. Intermodal modulation: visual attention. Regarding cross-
modal attentional modulation of cochlear functioning, the most 4.1.3. Perceptual learning and auditory training effect on MOC
commonly observed effect was a decrease in EOAE amplitudes system functioning
during visual detection task compared with no-task condition, A third line of evidence relates to learning experiments. Two
suggesting an enhancement of efferent suppression under visual studies have assessed the effects of perceptual training on MOCS
attention (Froehlich et al., 1993). However, other studies e using functioning, measured through contralateral suppression of EOAEs
binaural DPOAE recordings e showed an opposite effect, with (Veuillet et al., 2007; de Boer and Thornton, 2008).
DPOAE amplitude greater during visual tasks than during an In a study comparing dyslexic children to children with normal
auditory counting task (Srinivasan et al., 2012) whereas the de Boer reading abilities, Veuillet et al. (2007) showed that a number of
and Thorntons study (2007) e using contralateral suppression of children with reading difculties also had impaired sensitivity to
EOAEs e did not show any change during both passive and active phonemic contrast, concomitantly with abnormal interaural
visual tasks.12 asymmetry of MOC activity. In these dyslexic children, those who
improved the most their speech performance after a ve-week
4.1.1.3. Global interpretation of attentional modulation. Two main audiovisual training on voicing contrast also showed a partial
hypotheses can be suggested from these results. On the one normalization of MOCS functioning lateralization.
hand, attentional processes, subtended by cortico-olivocochlear In another study carried out in normal hearing young adult
pathways, may induce topedown modulation of MOC activity listeners, de Boer and Thornton (2008) showed that subjects with
and act as a peripheral auditory ltering mechanism (Perrot the poorer initial performance on a speech-in-noise discrimination
et al., 2006). On the other hand, the fact that attentional effects task also had the weaker initial MOC activity. After a ve-day
on cochlear functioning include both a direct modulation of OAE auditory training period on the tested task, these initial poor
amplitude in quiet and indirect modulation through noise- performers turned out to be good learners, in the sense that they
induced MOCS activation may indicate that CDAS can inuence had improved discrimination abilities, whereas the initial good
basal olivocochlear tonic activity (see Subsection 2.2.1) as well as performers but non-learners did not. Moreover, the perceptual
MOCS sensitivity in noise background (Mulders and Robertson, improvement was concomitant with a progressive increase in MOC
2002). activity over the ve days of training, eventually reaching the same
However, given the negative results, additional work is needed level as for the good performer, non-learner group.
to clarify the extent to which, and the conditions under which, Taken together, these results suggest that topedown inuences
peripheral auditory activity is modulated by attentional processes. are involved in auditory perceptual learning, presumably through
In this perspective, correlational studies based on the concurrent training-induced short term plasticity of corticofugal neural path-
assessment of MOC activity e or OAE amplitude changes e and ways, as demonstrated in ferrets and bats (Bajo et al., 2010; Suga
auditory perceptual performance e e.g., during a speech-in-noise and Ma, 2003). In this context, and although it is important to
task, while monitoring attentional load e e.g., through pupillom- note that these data were obtained in non-musician subjects, it is
etry or multimodal brain imaging, could prove instrumental in conceivable that musical training facilitates the development of the
clarifying these issues (Francis, 2012; Koelewijn et al., 2012; Lee cortico-olivocochlear pathways during childhood in order to
et al., 2014). improve peripheral sound encoding and central auditory process-
ing (Brashears et al., 2003; Hyde et al., 2009; Perrot et al., 1999;
Strait et al., 2012, 2013b).

11
This apparent discrepancy was possibly related to methodological issues, such 4.1.4. Apparent discrepancy of the similar degree of interaural
as the presentation intensity level of stimuli (for more details, see Giard et al.,
asymmetry
2000).
12
This latter interstudy variability may arise from difference in levels of difculty
The absence of increase in interaural asymmetry of efferent
and attentional load of the visual task, as already suggested by results obtained in suppression in musicians may seem at odds with the central hy-
chinchillas (Delano et al., 2007). pothesis, relative to the increased leftward brain asymmetry
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 35

commonly reported in neuroanatomical and electrophysiological multisensory neural pathways (Huffman and Henson, 1990), it may
studies on music-related neuroplasticity (Keenan et al., 2001; be argued that music-related brain plasticity has modied the
Schlaug et al., 1995; Schneider et al., 2002). central modulation of MEMR. Experimental data obtained specif-
However, it is worth noting that experimental conditions (i.e., ically in musicians in Brashears et al.s study (2003) argue against
passive listening) and acoustic stimuli used for assessing MOC ac- this hypothesis. Firstly, musicians had higher MEMR thresholds
tivity (i.e., binaural stimulation with clicks and contralateral or than non-musicians; this should result in weaker contralateral
bilateral BBN) differed from both the usual listening conditions and suppression of OAEs in musicians if the MEMR was responsible for
the auditory environment of musicians. This situation may then this effect. Secondly, the lack of correlation between bilateral sup-
have led to an unspecic bilateral activation of brain auditory areas pression of OAEs and MEMR thresholds for BBN, whatever the
(Binder et al., 1995; Zatorre et al., 1994), thereby resulting in a non- group or the tested ear, is not consistent with MEMR participation
lateralized corticofugal modulation and a normal interaural pattern (Brashears et al., 2003).
of MOC activity (Perrot et al., 1999). Alternatively, we can assume
that these non-ecological conditions have actually assessed pe- 5. Functional and perceptual consequences of enhanced MOC
ripheral change in strength of the MOC reex rather than central efferent suppression in musicians
change in corticofugal modulation of MOCS. This relates with the
peripheral hypothesis described below. What may be some of the consequences of stronger MOC in-
uences onto the cochlea? Based on currently available data
4.2. Peripheral hypothesis: sound-conditioning-related plasticity of concerning the role of the MOCS in hearing (Francis, 2012;
the MOC system Guinan, 2011; Robles and Delano, 2008), three main effects of
enhanced MOC activity may be expected in musicians: (i) pro-
The second hypothesis involves a change in the strength of the tection against acoustic trauma, (ii) improved hearing in noise or
MOC reex. In professional classical musicians, musical practice can in concurrent environment, and (iii) enhanced auditory selective
last up to 45 h per week (Pawlaczyk-quszczynska et al., 2013), with attention.
an average of 30 h per week (Emmerich et al., 2008). It is
conceivable that such protracted exposure to e often loud e sounds 5.1. Protection against acoustic trauma
engages a mechanism of sound conditioning of the peripheral
auditory system, which may involve a reinforcement of the MOC Musicians are exposed to louder sounds that non-musicians,
reex loop (Brashears et al., 2003). with longer exposure durations (Emmerich et al., 2008;
Pawlaczyk-quszczyn  ska et al., 2013). Thus, they should be at
4.2.1. Direct evidence of sound conditioning of MOC system greater risk of acoustic injury and occupational hearing loss
Several studies in rodents and cats have assessed the role of (McBride et al., 1992). Surprisingly, however, several studies have
MOCS in the protective effect of sound conditioning (see Section failed to nd any statistical difference in hearing thresholds be-
5.1). Indeed, it has been shown that repeated sound exposure in- tween musicians and non-musicians matched for age and sex
duces a functional increase in MOC ber acoustic responsiveness (Karlsson et al., 1983; Schmidt et al., 1994), or found better
(Brown et al., 1998), which could witness to a long-term neuro- thresholds than predicted from similar exposure to industrial noise
plasticity in the superior olivary complex (Illing et al., 2000). according to ISO standards (Obeling and Poulsen, 1999; Pawlaczyk-
Through this phenomenon, the MOCS may partially mediate an Luszczynska et al., 2013; Toppila et al., 2011). Taken together, these
inner-ear protection against noise-induced damage (Kujawa and ndings suggest that musicians may be less vulnerable to acoustic
Liberman, 1999; Maison et al., 2013). trauma than non-musicians. What might be the origin of this
effect?
4.2.2. Indirect evidence in musicians Electrophysiological and behavioral data in animals have shown
In musicians, two specic ndings regarding MOC activity are in that MOCS activation via electrical or binaural acoustic stimulation
favor of this peripheral hypothesis. In Micheyl et al.s study (1995a), can lead to smaller temporary threshold shifts following exposure
the fact that MOC activity e which was enhanced in musicians e to loud sounds (Rajan, 2000; Rajan and Johnstone, 1988). Similarly,
was linked with reduced loudness adaptation only in this group, smaller permanent threshold shifts were observed after sound
but not in non-musicians (Table 1-A), suggest a peripheral mech- conditioning, concomitantly with an enhancement of MOC reex
anism. In this perspective, chronic regular music exposure may strength (Kujawa and Liberman, 1999). Conversely, surgical cutting
strengthen the MOC reex in musicians. Similarly, in Brashears of the MOC system has been found to result in larger cellular and
et al.s study (2003), higher middle-ear muscle reex (MEMR) neuronal cochlear damage after noise exposure, and in increased
thresholds in musicians (Table 1-E) may be viewed as an indirect temporary and permanent threshold shifts in the de-efferented ear
sign of sound conditioning-related habituation, which may occur in (Maison et al., 2013; Zheng et al., 2000a). Moreover, the baseline
this peripheral acousticomotor reex, concomitantly with changes MOC reex strength can predict vulnerability of the ear to noise
in MOCS functioning. damage: the stronger the MOC reex, the smaller the permanent
threshold shift13 (Maison and Liberman, 2000). It is important to
4.3. Alternative hypothesis: involvement of MEM reex note that sound conditioning also protects efferent ber terminals
(Canlon et al., 1999), which may contribute to reinforce MOCS own
The MEMR shares common features with the MOC reex protective efciency over time, as a form of positive-feedback loop.
(Liberman and Guinan, 1998). Thus, the involvement of MEMR in In addition, the MOCS has been implicated in reduction of the
efferent suppression of OAEs is a long-standing hypothesis (Veuillet detrimental effects of noise on the central auditory system from the
et al., 1992). In non-musicians, several arguments have been put rst stages of development, suggesting that its protective role may
forward against this hypothesis, at least for low-level suppressors; be involved and conditioned from the early years of exposure to
these include a higher sound level needed for activation of the
MEMR and a different pattern of spectral and phase changes of
OAEs (Bki et al., 2000; Veuillet et al., 1992; Sun, 2008). In musi- 13
This phenomenon could underlie the interindividual variability of susceptibility
cians, since the MEMR may be modulated by descending to noise.
36 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

music (Lauer et al., 2011). Overall, these data support the view that et al., 2012). Interestingly, both categories of musicians have also
one function of the MOCS is to protect the inner ear, and indirectly been found to have stronger subcortical responses to speech stimuli
the central auditory system, from noise-induced damage. Together in background noise than non-musicians (Parbery-Clark et al.,
with ndings of a reduced auditory fatigability (Micheyl et al., 2009a; Strait et al., 2012).
1995a; Table 1-A) and a stronger bilateral MOC activity in musi-
cians than in non-musicians (Brashears et al., 2003; Perrot et al., 5.2.2. Links with attentional processes
1999; Table 1-D and 1-E), they suggest a possible explanation for Musical practice, especially in a musical ensemble, often re-
the lesser noise vulnerability in musicians. However, direct evi- quires an ability to attend selectively to concurrently presented
dence for a link between MOC and vulnerability to acoustic damage sounds. One way to study selective attention behaviorally involves
in musicians is lacking. the probe-signal method.14 Two studies using this method have
suggested that attentional processes were involved in MOC-related
5.2. Improved hearing in noise or in concurrent environment enhancement of signal-in-noise perception. The rst one (Scharf
et al., 1997) found that the ability to detect tones in a continuous
Musical training, especially orchestral practice, may be physio- noise background was impaired in the de-efferented ear of patients
logically and perceptually comparable with auditory perception in in whom the vestibular nerve was sectioned. This nding was
noise or in concurrent environment, so that enhanced MOC activity interpreted as evidence for a benecial involvement of the MOCS in
may be potentially useful for music perception. As previously frequency selective attention under normal conditions, i.e., in the
described, MOCS effects on peripheral auditory system (see non-operated ear. Results of a second study (Tan et al., 2008), which
Subsection 2.2.3 and Fig. 2) are likely to improve the discrimina- involved normal-hearing subjects, suggested that the detection of
bility of transient acoustic stimuli in continuous noise background diotically presented tones in binaural noise may be improved
(Liberman and Guinan, 1998; Guinan, 2006). through a frequency-specic attentional ltering mechanism.

5.2.1. Signal-in-noise perception 5.2.3. Sound localization in noise


Various lines of electrophysiological and behavioral evidence A recent study in normal-hearing listeners has evidenced a
are consistent with an anti-masking effect of the MOCS. In partic- signicant negative correlation between strength of MOC reex
ular, electrical or acoustical stimulation of MOC bers has been and noise-induced degradation of performance during a tridi-
found to result in enhanced representations of tones in noise in mensional sound-localization task (Andol et al., 2011). These re-
primary afferent ber responses (Kawase et al., 1993; Winslow and sults, which corroborate animal data (May et al., 2004), are
Sachs, 1987). Conversely, and consistent with the latter ndings, particularly interesting in the context of music processing. Indeed,
surgical cutting of MOC bers has been found to result in poorer two brain-potential studies have shown that professional music
discrimination and localization performance in noise (May and conductors had superior auditory processing in horizontal pe-
McQuone, 1994; May et al., 2004). ripheral space, compared to professional pianists and non-
In humans, the evidence for an involvement of the MOCS in musicians, and that this perceptual advantage may be related to
hearing in noise is less clear. Although some studies have found attentional processes (Nager et al., 2003; Mnte et al., 2001). Taken
statistically signicant relationships between OAE suppression and together, these ndings suggest that corticofugal strengthening of
thresholds, or performance, in tasks involving the detection or MOCS may contribute to an improvement of auditory spatial
discrimination of simple or complex signals e including speech e localization in musicians. The apparently contradictory lack of pe-
in noise (e.g., de Boer and Thorton, 2008; Garinis et al., 2011; Giraud ripheral enhancement among pianists could be related to a usual
et al., 1997; Kumar and Vanaja, 2004; Micheyl and Collet, 1996; exposure to less spatialized environments during their piano
Micheyl et al., 1997b), others have not (e.g., Stuart and Butler, practice.
2012; Wagner et al., 2008). Moreover, even in those studies in
which statistical relationships between OAE suppression and per-
5.3. Enhanced auditory selective attention
formance in noise were observed, the direction of the relationship
was not always consistent (e.g., de Boer et al., 2012; Micheyl et al.,
As previously described, various ndings indicate attentional
1995b, 1997b). The interpretation of these results is complicated by
modulation of both efferent suppression and OAEs (see Subsection
potential confounding factors, such as MEMR and attentional pro-
4.1.1), suggesting that the MOCS may be the nal link in a
cesses. For instance, correlations between MOC activity and
descending control pathway linking the auditory cortex to the co-
listening performance could be a by-product of attention (or
chlea (Giard et al., 2000; Perrot et al., 2006; Strait et al., 2010).
arousal) inuencing both measures. Arguably, the clearest
However, they raise the question of the underlying mechanisms, in
demonstration of a link between MOC activity and sound-signal
particular regarding the interindividual differences observed in
discrimination in noise so far stems from the work of Francis and
attentional effects (de Boer and Thornton, 2007, 2008; de Boer
Guinan (Francis, 2012; Francis and Guinan, 2011). In a series of
et al., 2012; Francis, 2012; Strait et al., 2010).
remarkably well designed experiments, these authors demon-
strated clear covariations between simultaneous behavioral and
5.3.1. Links with training-induced efciency of MOC system
click EOAE-amplitude measures, while subjects were engaged in a
In the de Boer and Thorton learning experiment (2008),
pure-tone intensity-discrimination task in noise. Moreover, they
although MOC activity measured initially was able to predict
found that MOC activity increased when subjects attended to the
training-induced improvement in the individual speech-in-noise
test stimuli or with task difculty, suggesting attentional processes
performance, there was no more correlation between MOC activ-
involvement in the enhancement of performance in noise back-
ity and perceptual task scores after training (see Subsection 4.1.3).
ground (see below).
In musicians, however, evidence for superior perception of
sounds in noise, compared to non-musicians, remains scarce. Two 14
In this paradigm, the listener had to detect a tone at a cued frequency, in
previous studies found that professional musicians e as well as background noise. The presentation used two kinds of stimulation: a frequent
children studying music e outperformed non-musician subjects in target one at the expected frequency, and a rare deviant one at an unexpected
various speech-in-noise tasks (Parbery-Clark et al., 2009b; Strait frequency.
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 37

These ndings suggest that subjects may or may not use a listening enhance the processing of musical sounds as well as complex
strategy involving the MOCS, depending on its individual effec- acoustic patterns e such as speech in noise e or auditory se-
tiveness in noise background. Moreover, this MOCS-mediated quences, especially in competitive environments e such as in an
strategy could be mobilized during task adaptation, through orchestral background (Krishnan et al., 2012).
training-induced corticofugal attentional processes, in order to Although the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms
facilitate speech-in-noise discrimination. Thus, all subjects do not remain unclear, a unifying e albeit speculative e conception would
spontaneously use their MOCS for auditory processing, but may involve both central and peripheral experience-dependent neuro-
learn to activate it through corticofugal recruiting, provided it re- plasticity processes: the former, related to active musical training,
sults in a perceptual benet (de Boer et al., 2012; Guinan, 2011). The should enhance CDAS topedown inuence on MOC activity and
individual prole for MOCS use, as well as interindividual differ- may be subject to an attentional control; the latter, related to
ence in MOCS basic efciency, may help to explain the experi- passive sound conditioning, should strengthen MOC reex loop and
mental discrepancies found between previous studies (e.g., de Boer may be independent of any attentional modulation.
et al., 2012; Micheyl et al., 1997b). However, one must keep in mind These ndings and hypotheses require further study, in order to
that it remains unknown whether such variability is also present in reveal all their potential for medical and scientic applications.
musicians. Among possible directions for future work, four important issues
should be explored. Firstly, since maladaptive brain plasticity has
5.3.2. Links with conditions of MOC system activation already been described in professional musicians (Elbert et al.,
An important inference may be drawn from the mixed results on 1998), one might wonder whether excessive strengthening e or
attentional modulation, in particular de Boer et al.s (2012) as well paradoxical weakening e of MOC activity could lead to auditory
as Scharf et al.s (1997) studies (see Subsection 5.2.2). It appears disorders such as tinnitus, hyperacusis or even early deafness
that the benecial effect of MOCS activation may be evidenced even (Chry-Croze et al., 1993). Secondly, since a stronger MOCS might
in monaural condition during topedown attentional processes, provide a developmental advantage and contribute to a greater
while it may require binaural condition for peripheral reexive musicianship, an alternative hypothesis to the experience-
MOC involvement e both conditions being not mutually exclusive. dependent neuroplasticity would be that this feature reects an
Recent data in normal-hearing subjects reinforce this hypothesis by innate auditory predisposition to music, thereby independent of
showing that speech processing abilities in silent background, any learning- or training-induced change (Ellis et al., 2012; Norton
namely binaural phonemic restoration in time-reversed speech, is et al., 2005). Thirdly, since in the reported studies, all the tested
correlated with a stronger MOC efferent suppression in the right musicians but one were classical instrumentalists, one may ask if
ear (Grataloup et al., 2009). In that case, the lack of noisy envi- music-related MOCS change can also be found e stronger or lesser
ronment for activating MOCS may have been made up for an in- e in musicians exposed to another musical environment e such as
crease in attentional load, in order to pass the task (Delano et al., traditional musicians e or used to process other kinds of musical
2007; Francis, 2012). stimuli e such as speech with music, for singers (Yasui et al., 2009).
Lastly, a better understanding of music-related MOCS plasticity
5.3.3. Links with cognitive abilities might help therapists better use musical training as a remediation
Several studies in musicians e both adult professionals and tool or a rehabilitation method in pathologies such as dyslexia or
child beginners e have reported that musicians have enhanced auditory processing disorders (Kraus, 2012), stroke or neurode-
basic perceptual auditory abilities, such as improved pitch- generative diseases (Altenmller et al., 2009), as well as in normal
discrimination thresholds (Kishon-Rabin et al., 2001; Micheyl or pathological aging (Sherratt et al., 2004). In this context,
et al., 2006), as well as nonmusical cognitive abilities (for a recent assessment of MOC activity as a predictive measure of training
review, see Schellenberg and Weiss, 2013). Recent ndings have benet and/or as a physiological measure of perceptual improve-
supplemented these data by showing a triple relationship between: ment could prove to be of particular interest.
(i) cognitive abilities, notably auditory attention and working
memory; (ii) basic auditory perception; and (iii) musical experience Acknowledgments
(Parbery-Clark et al., 2011; Strait et al., 2010, 2012). One interpre-
tation of these ndings is that protracted musical practice can The authors are grateful to Christophe Micheyl and two anon-
strengthen cognitive functioning, which in turn may improve ymous reviewers for constructive comments on an earlier version
auditory skills and performance, through enhanced attention and e of the manuscript.
possibly e enhanced corticofugal control (Oechslin et al., 2013;
Strait et al., 2010, 2013a). However, the latter hypothesis remains
References
untested.
Altenmller, E., Marco-Pallares, J., Mnte, T.F., Schneider, S., 2009. Neural reorga-
6. Conclusion and perspectives nization underlies improvement in stroke-induced motor dysfunction by
music-supported therapy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 395e405.
Andol, G., Guillaume, A., Micheyl, C., Savel, S., Pellieux, L., Moulin, A., 2011. Audi-
Despite the limited experimental data available for direct evi- tory efferents facilitate sound localization in noise in humans. J. Neurosci. 31,
dence, this review clearly demonstrates that musical training and/ 6759e6763.
or exposure can strengthen MOC activity in both ears of musicians, Ashmore, J., Avan, P., Brownell, W.E., Dallos, P., Dierkes, K., Fettiplace, R., Grosh, K.,
Hackney, C.M., Hudspeth, A.J., Jlicher, F., Lindner, B., Martin, P., Meaud, J.,
possibly through a reinforcement of corticofugal topedown control Petit, C., Santos-Sacchi, J., Sacchi, J.R.S., Canlon, B., 2010. The remarkable
(Perrot et al., 1999). Functional benets may include reinforcement cochlear amplier. Hear. Res. 266, 1e17.
of protection against acoustic trauma, improvement of auditory Backus, B.C., Guinan Jr., J.J., 2007. Measurement of the distribution of medial oli-
vocochlear acoustic reex strengths across normal-hearing individuals via
discrimination in noise, as well as enhancement of auditory
otoacoustic emissions. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 8, 484e496.
attention and training. More specically, this feature could give Bajo, V.M., Nodal, F.R., Moore, D.R., King, A.J., 2010. The descending cortico-
musicians an auditory-perceptual advantage enabling them to collicular pathway mediates learning-induced auditory plasticity. Nat.
improve auditory signal processing e from peripheral transduction Neurosci. 13, 253e260.
Berlin, C.I., Hood, L.J., Hurley, A.E., Wen, H., Kemp, D.T., 1995. Binaural noise sup-
to subcortical neural encoding (for reviews, see Bidelman, 2013; presses linear click-evoked otoacoustic emissions more than ipsilateral or
Kraus and Chandrasekaran, 2010). As a consequence, this may contralateral noise. Hear. Res. 87, 96e103.
38 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Berlin, C.I., Hood, L.J., Wen, H., Szabo, P., Cecola, R.P., Rigby, P., Jackson, D.F., 1993. Grataloup, C., Hoen, M., Veuillet, E., Collet, L., Pellegrino, F., Meunier, F., 2009.
Contralateral suppression of non-linear click-evoked otoacoustic emissions. Speech restoration: an interactive process. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52,
Hear. Res. 71, 1e11. 827e838.
Bidelman, G.M., 2013. The role of the auditory brainstem in processing musically Guinan Jr., J.J., 2006. Olivocochlear efferents: anatomy, physiology, function, and the
relevant pitch. Front. Psychol. 4, 264. measurement of efferent effects in humans. Ear Hear. 27, 589e607.
Binder, J.R., Rao, S.M., Hammeke, T.A., Frost, J.A., Bandettini, P.A., Jesmanowicz, A., Guinan Jr., J.J., 2011. Physiology of the medial and lateral olivocochlear systems. In:
Hyde, J.S., 1995. Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated by task Ryugo, D.K., Fay, R.R. (Eds.), Auditory and Vestibular Efferents. Springer New
subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Neurol. 52, 593e601. York, New York, NY, pp. 39e81.
Boh, B., Herholz, S.C., Lappe, C., Pantev, C., 2011. Processing of complex auditory Harkrider, A.W., Bowers, C.D., 2009. Evidence for a cortically mediated release from
patterns in musicians and nonmusicians. PLoS One 6, e21458. inhibition in the human cochlea. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 20, 208e215.
Bonls, P., Remond, M.C., Pujol, R., 1987. Variations of cochlear microphonic Herholz, S.C., Zatorre, R.J., 2012. Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity:
potential after sectioning efferent bers to the cochlea. Hear. Res. 30, behavior, function, and structure. Neuron 76, 486e502.
267e271. Hilbig, H., Beil, B., Hilbig, H., Call, J., Bidmon, H.-J., 2009. Superior olivary complex
Brashears, S.M., Morlet, T.G., Berlin, C.I., Hood, L.J., 2003. Olivocochlear efferent organization and cytoarchitecture may be correlated with function and catar-
suppression in classical musicians. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 14, 314e324. rhine primate phylogeny. Brain Struct. Funct. 213, 489e497.
Brown, M.C., Kujawa, S.G., Liberman, M.C., 1998. Single olivocochlear neurons in the Hood, L.J., Berlin, C.I., Hurley, A., Cecola, R.P., Bell, B., 1996. Contralateral suppression
guinea pig. II. Response plasticity due to noise conditioning. J. Neurophysiol. 79, of transient-evoked otoacoustic emissions in humans: intensity effects. Hear.
3088e3097. Res. 101, 113e118.
Bki, B., Wit, H.P., Avan, P., 2000. Olivocochlear efferent vs. middle-ear contributions Huffman, R.F., Henson Jr., O.W., 1990. The descending auditory pathway and
to the alteration of otoacoustic emissions by contralateral noise. Brain Res. 852, acousticomotor systems: connections with the inferior colliculus. Brain Res.
140e150. Brain Res. Rev. 15, 295e323.
Canlon, B., Fransson, A., Viberg, A., 1999. Medial olivocochlear efferent terminals are Hyde, K.L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A.C., Schlaug, G.,
protected by sound conditioning. Brain Res. 850, 253e260. 2009. Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29,
Chabert, R., Magnan, J., Lallemant, J.-G., Uziel, A., Puel, J.-L., 2002. Contralateral 3019e3025.
sound stimulation suppresses the compound action potential from the auditory Illing, R.B., Kraus, K.S., Michler, S.A., 2000. Plasticity of the superior olivary complex.
nerve in humans. Otol. Neurotol. 23, 784e788. Microsc. Res. Tech. 51, 364e381.
Chry-Croze, S., Collet, L., Morgon, A., 1993. Medial olivo-cochlear system and Karlsson, K., Lundquist, P.G., Olaussen, T., 1983. The hearing of symphony orchestra
tinnitus. Acta Otolaryngol. 113, 285e290. musicians. Scand. Audiol. 12, 257e264.
Chintanpalli, A., Jennings, S.G., Heinz, M.G., Strickland, E.A., 2012. Modeling the anti- Kawase, T., Delgutte, B., Liberman, M.C., 1993. Antimasking effects of the olivoco-
masking effects of the olivocochlear reex in auditory nerve responses to tones chlear reex. II. Enhancement of auditory-nerve response to masked tones.
in sustained noise. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 13, 219e235. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 2533e2549.
Collet, L., Kemp, D.T., Veuillet, E., Duclaux, R., Moulin, A., Morgon, A., 1990. Effect of Keenan, J.P., Thangaraj, V., Halpern, A.R., Schlaug, G., 2001. Absolute pitch and
contralateral auditory stimuli on active cochlear micro-mechanical properties planum temporale. Neuroimage 14, 1402e1408.
in human subjects. Hear. Res. 43, 251e261. Kemp, D.T., 2002. Otoacoustic emissions, their origin in cochlear function, and use.
Collet, L., Veuillet, E., Bene, J., Morgon, A., 1992. Effects of contralateral white noise Br. Med. Bull. 63, 223e241.
on click-evoked emissions in normal and sensorineural ears: towards an Kemp, D.T., Ryan, S., Bray, P., 1990. A guide to the effective use of otoacoustic
exploration of the medial olivocochlear system. Audiology 31, 1e7. emissions. Ear Hear. 11, 93e105.
de Boer, J., Thornton, A.R.D., 2007. Effect of subject task on contralateral suppression Khalfa, S., Bougeard, R., Morand, N., Veuillet, E., Isnard, J., Guenot, M., Ryvlin, P.,
of click evoked otoacoustic emissions. Hear. Res. 233, 117e123. Fischer, C., Collet, L., 2001a. Evidence of peripheral auditory activity modulation
de Boer, J., Thornton, A.R.D., 2008. Neural correlates of perceptual learning in the by the auditory cortex in humans. Neuroscience 104, 347e358.
auditory brainstem: efferent activity predicts and reects improvement at a Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Rog, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., Adrien, J.L., Barthlmy, C.,
speech-in-noise discrimination task. J. Neurosci. 28, 4929e4937. Collet, L., 2001b. Peripheral auditory asymmetry in infantile autism. Eur. J.
de Boer, J., Thornton, A.R.D., Krumbholz, K., 2012. What is the role of the medial Neurosci. 13, 628e632.
olivocochlear system in speech-in-noise processing? J. Neurophysiol. 107, Khalfa, S., Micheyl, C., Veuillet, E., Collet, L., 1998a. Peripheral auditory lateralization
1301e1312. assessment using TEOAEs. Hear. Res. 121, 29e34.
Delano, P.H., Elgueda, D., Hamame, C.M., Robles, L., 2007. Selective attention to visual Khalfa, S., Veuillet, E., Collet, L., 1998b. Inuence of handedness on peripheral
stimuli reduces cochlear sensitivity in chinchillas. J. Neurosci. 27, 4146e4153. auditory asymmetry. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 2731e2737.
Draganova, R., Wollbrink, A., Schulz, M., Okamoto, H., Pantev, C., 2009. Modulation Kishon-Rabin, L., Amir, O., Vexler, Y., Zaltz, Y., 2001. Pitch discrimination: are pro-
of auditory evoked responses to spectral and temporal changes by behavioral fessional musicians better than non-musicians? J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Pharma-
discrimination training. BMC Neurosci. 10, 143. col. 12, 125e143.
Elbert, T., Candia, V., Altenmller, E., Rau, H., Sterr, A., Rockstroh, B., Pantev, C., Knight, R.D., Kemp, D.T., 2001. Wave and place xed DPOAE maps of the human ear.
Taub, E., 1998. Alteration of digital representations in somatosensory cortex in J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 1513e1525.
focal hand dystonia. Neuroreport 9, 3571e3575. Koelewijn, T., Zekveld, A.A., Festen, J.M., Kramer, S.E., 2012. Pupil dilation uncovers
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., Taub, E., 1995. Increased cortical extra listening effort in the presence of a single-talker masker. Ear Hear. 33,
representation of the ngers of the left hand in string players. Science 270, 291e300.
305e307. Kraus, N., 2012. Biological impact of music and software-based auditory training.
Ellis, R.J., Norton, A.C., Overy, K., Winner, E., Alsop, D.C., Schlaug, G., 2012. Differ- J. Commun. Disord. 45, 403e410.
entiating maturational and training inuences on fMRI activation during music Kraus, N., Chandrasekaran, B., 2010. Music training for the development of auditory
processing. Neuroimage 60, 1902e1912. skills. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 599e605.
Emmerich, E., Rudel, L., Richter, F., 2008. Is the audiologic status of professional Krishnan, A., Gandour, J.T., Bidelman, G.M., 2012. Experience-dependent plasticity in
musicians a reection of the noise exposure in classical orchestral music? Eur. pitch encoding: from brainstem to auditory cortex. Neuroreport 23, 498e502.
Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 265, 753e758. Kujawa, S.G., Liberman, M.C., 1999. Long-term sound conditioning enhances
Francis, N.A., 2012. Auditory Task-dependent Control of Human Cochlear Responses cochlear sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 863e873.
to Sound (Ph. D. Thesis). Harvard University, Massachusetts Institute of Tech- Kumar, U.A., Vanaja, C.S., 2004. Functioning of olivocochlear bundle and speech
nology e Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Cambridge (Massachu- perception in noise. Ear Hear. 25, 142e146.
setts), p. 73. Available at http://dspace.mit.edu/handle/1721.1/70814. Accessed Lauer, A.M., May, B.J., 2011. The medial olivocochlear system attenuates the devel-
09.08.13. opmental impact of early noise exposure. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 12, 329e343.
Francis, N.A., Guinan Jr., J.J., February 19-23 2011. The dependence of medial oli- Lazard, D.S., Collette, J.-L., Perrot, X., 2012. Speech processing: from peripheral to
vocochlear feedback on signal-to-noise ratio during auditory intensity hemispheric asymmetry of the auditory system. Laryngoscope 122, 167e173.
discrimination (Abstract #180, Session D12). In: Abstracts of the 34th Annual Lee, A.K.C., Larson, E., Maddox, R.K., Shinn-Cunningham, B.G., Jul 9 2014. Using
Midwinter Research Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology. neuroimaging to understand the cortical mechanisms of auditory selective
Baltimore Maryland, ARO Abstr. 34, 2011, 61. attention. Hear. Res. 307, 111e120.
Froehlich, P., Collet, L., Morgon, A., 1993. Transiently evoked otoacoustic emission Len, A., Elgueda, D., Silva, M.A., Hamam, C.M., Delano, P.H., 2012. Auditory cortex
amplitudes change with changes of directed attention. Physiol. Behav. 53, basal activity modulates cochlear responses in chinchillas. PLoS ONE 7, e36203.
679e682. Liberman, M.C., Guinan Jr., J.J., 1998. Feedback control of the auditory periphery:
Garinis, A.C., Glattke, T., Cone, B.K., 2011. The MOC reex during active listening to anti-masking effects of middle ear muscles vs. olivocochlear efferents.
speech. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 1464e1476. J. Commun. Disord. 31, 471e482.
Giard, M.H., Collet, L., Bouchet, P., Pernier, J., 1994. Auditory selective attention in Maison, S., Micheyl, C., Collet, L., 2001. Inuence of focused auditory attention on
the human cochlea. Brain Res. 633, 353e356. cochlear activity in humans. Psychophysiology 38, 35e40.
Giard, M.H., Fort, A., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Pernier, J., 2000. Neurophysiolog- Maison, S.F., Liberman, M.C., 2000. Predicting vulnerability to acoustic injury with a
ical mechanisms of auditory selective attention in humans. Front. Biosci. 5, noninvasive assay of olivocochlear reex strength. J. Neurosci. 20, 4701e4707.
D84eD94. Maison, S.F., Usubuchi, H., Liberman, M.C., 2013. Efferent feedback minimizes
Giraud, A.L., Garnier, S., Micheyl, C., Lina, G., Chays, A., Chry-Croze, S., 1997. cochlear neuropathy from moderate noise exposure. J. Neurosci. 33, 5542e
Auditory efferents involved in speech-in-noise intelligibility. Neuroreport 8, 5552.
1779e1783.
X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40 39

Manley, G.A., 2000. Cochlear mechanisms from a phylogenetic viewpoint. Proc. Robles, L., Delano, P.H., 2008. Efferent system. In: Dallos, P., Oertel, D. (Eds.), The
Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97, 11736e11743. Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, Audition, vol. 3. Academic Press, New
Margulis, E.H., Mlsna, L.M., Uppunda, A.K., Parrish, T.B., Wong, P.C.M., 2009. Selec- York, pp. 413e445.
tive neurophysiologic responses to music in instrumentalists with different Rouiller, E.M., 1997. Functional organization of the auditory pathways. In: Ehret, G.,
listening biographies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 267e275. Romand, R. (Eds.), The Central Auditory System. Oxford University Press, New
May, B.J., Budelis, J., Niparko, J.K., 2004. Behavioral studies of the olivocochlear York, pp. 3e96.
efferent system: learning to listen in noise. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. Russell, I.J., Lukashkin, A.N., 2008. Cellular and molecular mechanisms in the
130, 660e664. efferent control of cochlear Nonlinearities. In: Manley, G.A., Fay, R.R.,
May, B.J., McQuone, S.J., 1994. Effects of bilateral olivocochlear lesions on pure tone Popper, A.N. (Eds.), Active Processes and Otoacoustic Emissions. Springer, New
intensity discrimination in cats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 2813. York, pp. 343e379.
McBride, D., Gill, F., Proops, D., Harrington, M., Gardiner, K., Attwell, C., 1992. Noise Scharf, B., Magnan, J., Chays, A., 1997. On the role of the olivocochlear bundle in
and the classical musician. Br. Med. J. 305, 1561e1563. hearing: 16 case studies. Hear. Res. 103, 101e122.
Meric, C., Collet, L., 1994. Attention and otoacoustic emissions: a review. Neurosci. Schellenberg, E.G., Weiss, M.W., 2013. Music and cognitive abilities. In: Deutsch, D.
Biobehav. Rev. 18, 215e222. (Ed.), The Psychology of Music, third ed. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego,
Micheyl, C., Carbonnel, O., Collet, L., 1995a. Medial olivocochlear system and pp. 499e550.
loudness adaptation: differences between musicians and non-musicians. Brain Schlaug, G., Jncke, L., Huang, Y., Steinmetz, H., 1995. In vivo evidence of structural
Cogn. 29, 127e136. brain asymmetry in musicians. Science 267, 699e701.
Micheyl, C., Collet, L., 1996. Involvement of the olivocochlear bundle in the detec- Schmidt, J.M., Verschuure, J., Brocaar, M.P., 1994. Hearing loss in students at a
tion of tones in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99, 1604e1610. conservatory. Audiology 33, 185e194.
Micheyl, C., Delhommeau, K., Perrot, X., Oxenham, A.J., 2006. Inuence of Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, H.G., Specht, H.J., Gutschalk, A., Rupp, A., 2002.
musical and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination. Hear. Res. Morphology of Heschls gyrus reects enhanced activation in the auditory
219, 36e47. cortex of musicians. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 688e694.
Micheyl, C., Khalfa, S., Perrot, X., Collet, L., 1997a. Difference in cochlear Schneider, P., Sluming, V., Roberts, N., Scherg, M., Goebel, R., Specht, H.J.,
efferent activity between musicians and non-musicians. Neuroreport 8, Dosch, H.G., Bleeck, S., Stippich, C., Rupp, A., 2005. Structural and functional
1047e1050. asymmetry of lateral Heschls gyrus reects pitch perception preference. Nat.
Micheyl, C., Morlet, T., Giraud, A.L., Collet, L., Morgon, A., 1995b. Contralateral Neurosci. 8, 1241e1247.
suppression of evoked otoacoustic emissions and detection of a multi-tone Schoeld, B.R., Coomes, D.L., 2006. Pathways from auditory cortex to the cochlear
complex in noise. Acta Otolaryngol. 115, 178e182. nucleus in guinea pigs. Hear. Res. 216e217, 81e89.
Micheyl, C., Perrot, X., Collet, L., 1997b. Relationship between auditory intensity Shera, C.A., 2004. Mechanisms of mammalian otoacoustic emission and their im-
discrimination in noise and olivocochlear efferent system activity in humans. plications for the clinical utility of otoacoustic emissions. Ear Hear. 25, 86e97.
Behav. Neurosci. 111, 801e807. Sherratt, K., Thornton, A., Hatton, C., 2004. Music interventions for people with
Michie, P.T., LePage, E.L., Solowij, N., Haller, M., Terry, L., 1996. Evoked otoacoustic dementia: a review of the literature. Aging Ment. Health 8, 3e12.
emissions and auditory selective attention. Hear. Res. 98, 54e67. Sininger, Y.S., Cone-Wesson, B., 2004. Asymmetric cochlear processing mimics
Moore, J.K., 2000. Organization of the human superior olivary complex. Microsc. hemispheric specialization. Science 305, 1581.
Res. Tech. 51, 403e412. Srinivasan, S., Keil, A., Stratis, K., Woodruff Carr, K.L., Smith, D.W., 2012. Effects of
Morawski, K., Sliwin  ska-Kowalska, M., Namys1owski, G., Dulikowska, H., 1999. cross-modal selective attention on the sensory periphery: cochlear sensitivity is
Otoacoustic emission nonlinear distortions in musicians with absolute and altered by selective attention. Neuroscience 223, 325e332.
relative pitch. Otolaryngol. Pol 53, 307e313. Strait, D.L., Kraus, N., Parbery-Clark, A., Ashley, R., 2010. Musical experience shapes
Mulders, W.H., Robertson, D., 2002. Inputs from the cochlea and the inferior col- top-down auditory mechanisms: evidence from masking and auditory atten-
liculus converge on olivocochlear neurones. Hear. Res. 167, 206e213. tion performance. Hear. Res. 261, 22e29.
Mnte, T.F., Kohlmetz, C., Nager, W., Altenmller, E., 2001. Neuroperception. Supe- Strait, D.L., OConnell, S., Parbery-Clark, A., Kraus, N., 2013a. Musicians enhanced
rior auditory spatial tuning in conductors. Nature 409, 580. neural differentiation of speech sounds arises early in life: developmental ev-
Nager, W., Kohlmetz, C., Altenmller, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Mnte, T.F., 2003. idence from ages 3 to 30. Cereb. Cortex. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/
The fate of sounds in conductors brains: an ERP study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain bht103, 2013 Apr 18. [Epub ahead of print].
Res. 17, 83e93. Strait, D.L., Parbery-Clark, A., Hittner, E., Kraus, N., 2012. Musical training during
Norton, A., Winner, E., Cronin, K., Overy, K., Lee, D.J., Schlaug, G., 2005. Are there early childhood enhances the neural encoding of speech in noise. Brain Lang.
pre-existing neural, cognitive, or motoric markers for musical ability? Brain 123, 191e201.
Cogn. 59, 124e134. Strait, D.L., Parbery-Clark, A., OConnell, S., Kraus, N., 2013b. Biological impact of
Obeling, L., Poulsen, T., 1999. Hearing ability in Danish symphony orchestra musi- preschool music classes on processing speech in noise. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6C,
cians. Noise Health 1, 43e49. 51e60.
Oechslin, M.S., Van De Ville, D., Lazeyras, F., Hauert, C.-A., James, C.E., 2013. Degree Stuart, A., Butler, A.K., 2012. Contralateral suppression of transient otoacoustic
of musical expertise modulates higher order brain functioning. Cereb. Cortex emissions and sentence recognition in noise in young adults. J. Am. Acad.
23, 2213e2224. Audiol. 23, 686e696.
Oldeld, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Suga, N., 2012. Tuning shifts of the auditory system by corticocortical and
inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97e113. corticofugal projections and conditioning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36,
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Kraus, N., 2009a. Musical experience limits the degra- 969e988.
dative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. Suga, N., Ma, X., 2003. Multiparametric corticofugal modulation and plasticity in the
J. Neurosci. 29, 14100e14107. auditory system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 783e794.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., Kraus, N., 2009b. Musician enhancement for Sun, X.-M., 2008. Contralateral suppression of distortion product otoacoustic
speech-in-noise. Ear Hear. 30, 653e661. emissions and the middle-ear muscle reex in human ears. Hear. Res. 237,
Parbery-Clark, A., Strait, D.L., Anderson, S., Hittner, E., Kraus, N., 2011. Musical 66e75.
experience and the aging auditory system: implications for cognitive abilities Tan, M.N., Robertson, D., Hammond, G.R., 2008. Separate contributions of
and hearing speech in noise. PLoS One 6, e18082. enhanced and suppressed sensitivity to the auditory attentional lter. Hear.
Pawlaczyk-Luszczynska, M., Dudarewicz, A., Zaborowski, K., Zamojska, M., Sli- Res. 241, 18e25.
winska-Kowalska, M., 2013. Noise induced hearing loss: research in central, Toppila, E., Koskinen, H., Pyykk, I., 2011. Hearing loss among classical-orchestra
eastern and south-eastern Europe and newly independent states. Noise Health musicians. Noise Health 13, 45e50.
15, 55e66. Veuillet, E., Collet, L., Duclaux, R., 1991. Effect of contralateral acoustic stimulation
Peng, A.W., Ricci, A.J., 2011. Somatic motility and hair bundle mechanics, are both on active cochlear micromechanical properties in human subjects: dependence
necessary for cochlear amplication? Hear. Res. 273, 109e122. on stimulus variables. J. Neurophysiol. 65, 724e735.
Perrot, X., Micheyl, C., Khalfa, S., Collet, L., 1999. Stronger bilateral efferent in- Veuillet, E., Collet, L., Morgon, A., 1992. Differential effects of ear-canal pressure and
uences on cochlear biomechanical activity in musicians than in non-musi- contralateral acoustic stimulation on evoked otoacoustic emissions in humans.
cians. Neurosci. Lett. 262, 167e170. Hear. Res. 61, 47e55.
Perrot, X., Ryvlin, P., Isnard, J., Gunot, M., Catenoix, H., Fischer, C., Mauguire, F., Veuillet, E., Georgieff, N., Philibert, B., Dalery, J., Marie-Cardine, M., Collet, L., 2001.
Collet, L., 2006. Evidence for corticofugal modulation of peripheral auditory Abnormal peripheral auditory asymmetry in schizophrenia. J. Neurol. Neuro-
activity in humans. Cereb. Cortex 16, 941e948. surg. Psychiatr. 70, 88e94.
Rajan, R., 2000. Centrifugal pathways protect hearing sensitivity at the cochlea in Veuillet, E., Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., Thai-Van, H., Collet, L., 2007. Auditory processing
noisy environments that exacerbate the damage induced by loud sound. disorder in children with reading disabilities: effect of audiovisual training.
J. Neurosci. 20, 6684e6693. Brain 130, 2915e2928.
Rajan, R., Johnstone, B.M., 1988. Binaural acoustic stimulation exercises protective Wagner, W., Frey, K., Heppelmann, G., Plontke, S.K., Zenner, H.-P., 2008. Speech-in-
effects at the cochlea that mimic the effects of electrical stimulation of an noise intelligibility does not correlate with efferent olivocochlear reex in
auditory efferent pathway. Brain Res. 459, 241e255. humans with normal hearing. Acta Otolaryngol. 128, 53e60.
Rasmussen, G.L., 1946. The olivary peduncle and other ber projections of the su- Warr, W.B., Guinan Jr., J.J., 1979. Efferent innervation of the organ of corti: two
perior olivary complex. J. Comp. Neurol. 84, 141e219. separate systems. Brain Res. 173, 152e155.
Robertson, D., Gummer, M., 1985. Physiological and morphological characterization Warren 3rd, E.H., Liberman, M.C., 1989. Effects of contralateral sound on auditory-
of efferent neurones in the guinea pig cochlea. Hear. Res. 20, 63e77. nerve responses. II. Dependence on stimulus variables. Hear. Res. 37, 105e121.
40 X. Perrot, L. Collet / Hearing Research 308 (2014) 27e40

Wersinger, E., Fuchs, P.A., 2011. Modulation of hair cell efferents. Hear. Res. 279, 1e Yasui, T., Kaga, K., Sakai, K.L., 2009. Language and music: differential hemispheric
12. dominance in detecting unexpected errors in the lyrics and melody of memo-
Winer, J.A., 2006. Decoding the auditory corticofugal systems. Hear. Res. 212, 1e8. rized songs. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 588e601.
Winslow, R.L., Sachs, M.B., 1987. Effect of electrical stimulation of the crossed olivo- Zatorre, R.J., Evans, A.C., Meyer, E., 1994. Neural mechanisms underlying melodic
cochlear bundle on auditory nerve response to tones in noise. J. Neurophysiol. 57, perception and memory for pitch. J. Neurosci. 14, 1908e1919.
1002e1021. Zheng, X.Y., McFadden, S.L., Ding, D.L., Henderson, D., 2000a. Cochlear de-
Xiao, Z., Suga, N., 2002a. Modulation of cochlear hair cells by the auditory cortex in efferentation and impulse noise-induced acoustic trauma in the chinchilla.
the mustached bat. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 57e63. Hear. Res. 144, 187e195.
Xiao, Z., Suga, N., 2002b. Reorganization of the cochleotopic map in the bats Zheng, X.Y., McFadden, S.L., Henderson, D., Ding, D.L., Burkard, R., 2000b. Cochlear
auditory system by inhibition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 99, 15743e microphonics and otoacoustic emissions in chronically de-efferented chinchilla.
15748. Hear. Res. 143, 14e22.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen