Sie sind auf Seite 1von 1154

HANDBOOK OF

CHILD PSYCHOLOGY
SIXTH EDITION

Volume Three: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development

Volume Editor

NANCY EISENBERG

Editors-in-Chief

WILLIAM DAMON and RICHARD M. LERNER

John Wiley & Sons, Inc.


HANDBOOK OF
CHILD PSYCHOLOGY
HANDBOOK OF
CHILD PSYCHOLOGY
SIXTH EDITION

Volume Three: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development

Volume Editor

NANCY EISENBERG

Editors-in-Chief

WILLIAM DAMON and RICHARD M. LERNER

John Wiley & Sons, Inc.


Copyright 2006 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved.

Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey.


Published simultaneously in Canada.

No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic,
mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States
Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the Publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy
fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, (978) 750-8400, fax (978) 646-8600, or on the web
at www.copyright.com. Requests to the Publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons,
Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, (201) 748-6011, fax (201) 748-6008.

Limit of Liability/ Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no
representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any
implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives
or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a
professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages,
including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages.

This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered. It is sold with the
understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psychological or any other
expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional person should be sought.

Designations used by companies to distinguish their products are often claimed as trademarks. In all instances where John Wiley & Sons,
Inc. is aware of a claim, the product names appear in initial capital or all capital letters. Readers, however, should contact the appropriate
companies for more complete information regarding trademarks and registration.

For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer Care Department within the United States at (800)
762-2974, outside the United States at (317) 572-3993 or fax (317) 572-4002.

Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in print may not be available in electronic
books. For more information about Wiley products, visit our web site at www.wiley.com.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data:

Handbook of child psychology / editors-in-chief, William Damon & Richard M. Lerner.


6th ed.
p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and indexes.
Contents: v. 1. Theoretical models of human development / volume editor,
Richard M. Lerner v. 2. Cognition, perception, and language / volume editors,
Deanna Kuhn, Robert Siegler v. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development /
volume editor, Nancy Eisenberg v 4. Child psychology in practice / volume editors, K.
Ann Renninger, Irving E. Sigel.
ISBN 0-471-27287-6 (set : cloth)
ISBN 0-471-27288-4 (v. 1 : cloth) ISBN 0-471-27289-2 (v. 2 : cloth)
ISBN 0-471-27290-6 (v. 3 : cloth) ISBN 0-471-27291-4 (v. 4 : cloth)
1. Child psychology. I. Damon, William, 1944 II. Lerner, Richard M.
BF721.H242 2006
155.4dc22
2005043951

Printed in the United States of America.


10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
In memory of Paul Mussen, whose generosity of spirit
touched our lives and helped build a field.
Contributors

John E. Bates John D. Coie


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
Indiana University Duke University
Bloomington, Indiana Durham, North Carolina

Sheri A. Berenbaum W. Andrew Collins


Department of Psychology Institute of Child Development
The Pennsylvania State University University of Minnesota
University Park, Pennsylvania Minneapolis, Minnesota

Daphne Blunt Bugental Pamela Davis-Kean


Department of Psychology Institute for Social Research
University of California University of Michigan
Santa Barbara, California Ann Arbor, Michigan

William M. Bukowski Kenneth A. Dodge


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
Concordia University Duke University
Montreal, QC, Canada Durham, North Carolina

Raymond Buriel Jacquelynne S. Eccles


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
Pomona College University of Michigan
Claremont, California Ann Arbor, Michigan

Joseph J. Campos Nancy Eisenberg


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Califorina Arizona State University
Berkeley, California Tempe, Arizona

Linda A. Camras Richard A. Fabes


Department of Psychology Department of Family and Human Development
DePaul University Arizona State University
Chicago, Illinois Tempe, Arizona

Avshalom Caspi Nathan A. Fox


Department of Psychology Department of Human Development
University of Wisconsin University of Maryland
Madison, Wisconsin College Park, Maryland

vii
viii Contributors

Joan E. Grusec Diane N. Ruble


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Toronto New York University
Toronto, Ontario, Canada New York, New York

Susan Harter Carolyn Saarni


Department of Psychology Department of Counseling
University of Denver Sonoma State University
Denver, Colorado Rohnert Park, California

Jerome Kagan
Ulrich Schiefele
Department of Psychology
Department of Psychology
Harvard University
Universitt Bielefeld
Cambridge, Massachusetts
Bielefeld, Germany
Donald Lynam
Department of Psychology Rebecca L. Shiner
University of Kentucky Department of Psychology
Lexington, Kentucky Colgate University
Hamilton, New York
Carol Lynn Martin
Department of Family and Human Development Tracy L. Spinrad
Arizona State University Department of Family and Human Development
Tempe, Arizona Arizona State University
Tempe, Arizona
Ross D. Parke
Department of Psychology Laurence Steinberg
University of California Department of Psychology
Riverside, California Temple University
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Jeffrey G. Parker
Department of Psychology Ross A. Thompson
Pennsylvania State University Department of Psychology
University Park, Pennsylvania University of California
Davis, California
Robert W. Roeser
The Eliot-Pearson
Elliot Turiel
Department of Child Development
Graduate School of Education
Tufts University
University of California
Medford, Massachusetts
Berkeley, California
Mary K. Rothbart
Department of Psychology Allan Wigfield
University of Oregon Department of Human Development
Eugene, Oregon University of Maryland
College Park, Maryland
Kenneth H. Rubin
Center for Children, Relationships, and Culture David Witherington
Department of Human Development Department of Psychology
University of Maryland University of New Mexico
College Park, Maryland Albuquerque, New Mexic
Reviewers

Daphne Blunt Bugental Kathryn Lemery


Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of California Arizona State University
Santa Barbara, California Tempe, Arizona

W. Andrew Collins Terrie Moffitt


Institute of Child Development Institute of Psychiatry
University of Minnesota London, United Kingdom
Minneapolis, Minnesota
Ross D. Parke
Keith Crnic Department of Psychology
Department of Psychology University of California
Arizona State University Riverside, California
Tempe, Arizona
Concetta Pastorelli
Nathan A. Fox
Department of Psychology
Department of Human Development
University of Rome
University of Maryland
Rome, Italy
College Park, Maryland
Mary Rothbart
Doran C. French
Department of Psychology
Department of Psychology
University of Oregon
Professor and Chair
Eugene, Oregon
Illinois Wesleyan University
Bloomington, Illinois Ellen Skinner
William G. Graziano Department of Psychology
Department of Psychology Portland State
Purdue University Portand, Oregon
West Lafayette, Indiana
Judi Smetana
Leon Kuczynski Department of Clinical and Social Sciences
Department of Family Relations and in Psychology
Applied Nutrition University of Rochester
University of Guelph Rochester, New York
Guelph, Ontario, Canada
Carolyn Zahn-Waxler
Brett Laursen National Institute of Mental Health and
Professor of Psychology Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry
Florida Atlantic University University of Wisconsin
Fort Lauderdale, Florida Madison, Wisconsin

ix
Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology,
Sixth Edition
WILLIAM DAMON

Scholarly handbooks play several key roles in their dis- not only has endured over time but has evolved into a
ciplines. First and foremost, they reflect recent changes thriving tradition across a number of related academic
in the field as well as classic works that have survived disciplines.
those changes. In this sense, all handbooks present their All through its history, the Handbook has drawn on,
editors and authors best judgments about what is most and played a formative role in, the worldwide study of
important to know in the field at the time of publication. human development. What does the Handbooks history
But many handbooks also influence the fields that they tell us about where we, as developmentalists, have been,
report on. Scholarsespecially younger oneslook to what we have learned, and where we are going? What
them for sources of information and inspiration to guide does it tell us about what has changed and what has re-
their own work. While taking stock of the shape of its mained the same in the questions that we ask, in the
field, a handbook also shapes the stock of ideas that will methods that we use, and in the theoretical ideas that we
define the fields future. It serves both as an indicator draw on in our quest to understand human development?
and as a generator, a pool of received knowledge and a By asking these questions, we follow the spirit of the sci-
pool for spawning new insight. ence itself, for developmental questions may be asked
about any endeavor, including the enterprise of studying
human development. To best understand what this field
THE HANDBOOKS LIVING TRADITION has to tell us about human development, we must ask how
the field itself has developed. In a field that examines
Within the field of human development, the Handbook of continuities and changes, we must ask, for the field itself,
Child Psychology has served these key roles to a degree what are the continuities and what are the changes?
that has been exceptional even among the impressive The history of the Handbook is by no means the whole
panoply of the worlds many distinguished scholarly story of why the field is where it is today, but it is a fun-
handbooks. The Handbook of Child Psychology has had a damental part of the story. It has defined the choices
widely heralded tradition as a beacon, organizer, and en- that have determined the fields direction and has influ-
cyclopedia of developmental study for almost 75 years enced the making of those choices. In this regard, the
a period that covers the vast majority of scientific work Handbooks history reveals much about the judgments
in this field. and other human factors that shape a science.
It is impossible to imagine what the field would look
like if it had not occurred to Carl Murchison in 1931 to
assemble an eclectic assortment of contributions into THE CAST OF CHARACTERS
the first Handbook of Child Psychology. Whether or not
Murchison realized this potential (an interesting specu- Carl Murchison was a scholar/impresario who edited
lation in itself, given his visionary and ambitious na- The Psychological Register; founded and edited key psy-
ture), he gave birth to a seminal publishing project that chological journals; wrote books on social psychology,

xi
xii Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition

politics, and the criminal mind; and compiled an assort- which still is fresh today, Bhler described intricate
ment of handbooks, psychology texts, autobiographies of play and communication patterns among toddlers, pat-
renowned psychologists, and even a book on psychic be- terns that developmental psychology would not redis-
liefs (Sir Arthur Conan Doyle and Harry Houdini were cover until the late 1970s. Bhler also anticipated the
among the contributors). Murchisons initial Handbook critiques of Piaget that would appear during the socio-
of Child Psychology was published by a small university linguistics heyday of the 1970s:
press (Clark University) in 1931, when the field itself
Piaget, in his studies on childrens talk and reasoning, em-
was still in its infancy. Murchison wrote:
phasizes that their talk is much more egocentric than so-
cial . . . that children from 3 to 7 years accompany all their
Experimental psychology has had a much older scientific
manipulations with talk which actually is not so much in-
and academic status [than child psychology], but at the
tercourse as monologue . . . [but] the special relationship
present time it is probable that much less money is being
of the child to each of the different members of the house-
spent for pure research in the field of experimental psy-
hold is distinctly reflected in the respective conversations.
chology than is being spent in the field of child psychol-
(Buhler, 1931, p. 138)
ogy. In spite of this obvious fact, many experimental
psychologists continue to look upon the field of child psy-
chology as a proper field of research for women and for Other Europeans included Anna Freud, who wrote on
men whose experimental masculinity is not of the maxi- The Psychoanalysis of the Child, and Kurt Lewin,
mum. This attitude of patronage is based almost entirely who wrote on Environmental Forces in Child Behavior
upon a blissful ignorance of what is going on in the and Development.
tremendously virile field of child behavior. (Murchison, The Americans whom Murchison chose were equally
1931, p. ix) notable. Arnold Gesell wrote a nativistic account of his
twin studies, an enterprise that remains familiar to us
Murchisons masculine allusion, of course, is from an- today, and Stanfords Louis Terman wrote a comprehen-
other era; it could furnish some good material for a social sive account of everything known about the gifted
history of gender stereotyping. That aside, Murchison child. Harold Jones described the developmental ef-
was prescient in the task that he undertook and the way fects of birth order, Mary Cover Jones wrote about chil-
that he went about it. At the time Murchison wrote the drens emotions, Florence Goodenough wrote about
preface to his Handbook, developmental psychology was childrens drawings, and Dorothea McCarthy wrote
known only in Europe and in a few forward-looking about language development. Vernon Joness chapter on
American labs and universities. Nevertheless, Murchison childrens morals focused on the growth of character,
predicted the fields impending ascent: The time is not a notion that was to become lost to the field during the
far distant, if it is not already here, when nearly all com- cognitive-developmental revolution, but that reemerged
petent psychologists will recognize that one-half of the in the 1990s as the primary concern in the study of
whole field of psychology is involved in the problem of moral development.
how the infant becomes an adult psychologically Murchisons vision of child psychology included an
(Murchison, 1931, p. x). examination of cultural differences as well. His Hand-
For his original 1931 Handbook, Murchison looked to book presented to the scholarly world a young anthropol-
Europe and to a handful of American centers (or field ogist named Margaret Mead, just back from her tours of
stations) for child research (Iowa, Minnesota, the Uni- Samoa and New Guinea. In this early essay, Mead wrote
versity of California at Berkeley, Columbia, Stanford, that her motivation in traveling to the South Seas was to
Yale, Clark). Murchisons Europeans included a young discredit the views that Piaget, Levy-Bruhl, and other
genetic epistemologist named Jean Piaget, who, in an nascent structuralists had put forth concerning ani-
essay on Childrens Philosophies, quoted extensively mism in young childrens thinking. (Interestingly,
from interviews with 60 Genevan children between the about a third of Piagets chapter in the same volume was
ages of 4 and 12 years. Piagets chapter would provide dedicated to showing how Genevan children took years
American readers with an introduction to his seminal to outgrow animism.) Mead reported some data that she
research program on childrens conceptions of the called amazing: In not one of the 32,000 drawings
world. Another European, Charlotte Bhler, wrote a ( by young primitive children) was there a single case
chapter on childrens social behavior. In this chapter, of personalization of animals, material phenomena, or
Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition xiii

inanimate objects (Mead, 1931, p. 400). Mead parlayed in the Handbook tradition, made an appearance as au-
these data into a tough-minded critique of Western psy- thor of a major chapter ( by far the longest in the book)
chologys ethnocentrism, making the point that animism on prenatal and perinatal growth. Three other physio-
and other beliefs are more likely to be culturally in- logically oriented chapters were added as well: one on
duced than intrinsic to early cognitive development. neonatal motor behavior, one on visual-manual func-
This is hardly an unfamiliar theme in contemporary psy- tions during the first 2 years of life, and one on physio-
chology. Mead also offered a research guide for develop- logical appetites such as hunger, rest, and sex.
mental fieldworkers in strange cultures, complete with Combined with the Goodenough and Gesell shifts in
methodological and practical advice, such as the follow- focus, these additions gave the 1933 Handbook more of a
ing: Translate questions into native linguistic categories; biological thrust, in keeping with Murchisons long-
dont do controlled experiments; dont do studies that standing desire to display the hard science backbone of
require knowing ages of subjects, which are usually un- the emerging field.
knowable; and live next door to the children whom you Leonard Carmichael was president of Tufts Univer-
are studying. sity when he organized Wileys first edition of the
Despite the imposing roster of authors that Murchison Handbook. The switch from a university press to the
assembled for the 1931 Handbook of Child Psychology, long-established commercial firm of John Wiley &
his achievement did not satisfy him for long. Barely 2 Sons was commensurate with Carmichaels well-
years later, Murchison put out a second edition, of which known ambition; indeed, Carmichaels effort was to
he wrote: Within a period of slightly more than 2 years, become influential beyond anything that Murchison
this first revision bears scarcely any resemblance to the might have anticipated. The book (one volume at that
original Handbook of Child Psychology. This is due time) was called the Manual of Child Psychology, in
chiefly to the great expansion in the field during the past keeping with Carmichaels intention of producing an
3 years and partly to the improved insight of the editor advanced scientific manual to bridge the gap between
(Murchison, 1933, p. vii). The tradition that Murchison the excellent and varied elementary textbooks in this
had brought to life was already evolving. field and the scientific periodical literature
Murchison saw fit to provide the following warning in (Carmichael, 1946, p. viii).
his second edition: There has been no attempt to sim- The publication date was 1946, and Carmichael com-
plify, condense, or to appeal to the immature mind. This plained that this book has been a difficult and expensive
volume is prepared specifically for the scholar, and its one to produce, especially under wartime conditions
form is for his maximum convenience (Murchison, (Carmichael, 1946, p. viii). Nevertheless, the project was
1933, p. vii). It is likely that sales of Murchisons first worth the effort. The Manual quickly became the bible of
volume did not approach textbook levels; perhaps he re- graduate training and scholarly work in the field, avail-
ceived negative comments regarding its accessibility. able virtually everywhere that human development was
Murchison exaggerated when he wrote that his sec- studied. Eight years later, now head of the Smithsonian
ond edition bore little resemblance to the first. Almost Institution, Carmichael wrote, in the preface to the 1954
half of the chapters were virtually the same, with minor second edition, The favorable reception that the first
additions and updating. (For the record, though, despite edition received not only in America but all over the
Murchisons continued use of masculine phraseology, world is indicative of the growing importance of the
10 of the 24 authors in the second edition were women.) study of the phenomena of the growth and development of
Some of the authors whose original chapters were the child (Carmichael, 1954, p. vii).
dropped were asked to write about new topics. So, for Carmichaels second edition had a long life: Not until
example, Goodenough wrote about mental testing rather 1970 did Wiley bring out a third edition. Carmichael was
than about childrens drawings, and Gesell wrote a gen- retired by then, but he still had a keen interest in the
eral statement of his maturational theory that went well book. At his insistence, his own name became part of the
beyond the twin studies. title of the third edition; it was called, improbably,
But Murchison also made some abrupt changes. He Carmichaels Manual of Child Psychology, even though it
dropped Anna Freud entirely, auguring the marginaliza- had a new editor and an entirely different cast of authors
tion of psychoanalysis within academic psychology. and advisors. Paul Mussen took over as the editor, and
Leonard Carmichael, who was later to play a pivotal role once again the project flourished. Now a two-volume set,
xiv Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition

the third edition swept across the social sciences, gener- plained.) In any case, Carmichael acknowledged the
ating widespread interest in developmental psychology roots of his Manuals, if not always their original editor.
and its related disciplines. Rarely had a scholarly com- His choice to start with those roots is a revealing part
pendium become both so dominant in its own field and so of the Handbooks history, and it established a strong
familiar in related disciplines. The set became an essen- intellectual legacy for our present-day descendants of
tial source for graduate students and advanced scholars the early pioneers who wrote for the Murchison and
alike. Publishers referred to Carmichaels Manual as the Carmichael editions.
standard against which other scientific handbooks were Although Leonard Carmichael took the 1946 Manual
compared. in much the same direction established by Murchison
The fourth edition, published in 1983, was now re- back in 1931 and 1933, he did bring it several steps fur-
designated by John Wiley & Sons to become once again ther in that direction, added a few twists of his own, and
the Handbook of Child Psychology. By then, Carmichael dropped a couple of Murchisons bolder selections.
had passed away. The set of books, now expanded to four Carmichael first appropriated five Murchison chapters
volumes, became widely referred to in the field as the on biological or experimental topics, such as physiologi-
Mussen handbook. cal growth, scientific methods, and mental testing. He
added three new biologically oriented chapters on ani-
mal infancy, physical growth, and motor and behavioral
WHAT CARMICHAEL CHOSE FOR THE maturation (a tour de force by Myrtal McGraw that in-
NOW EMERGENT FIELD stantly made Gesells chapter in the same volume obso-
lete). Then he commissioned Wayne Dennis to write an
Leonard Carmichael, who became Wileys editor for adolescence chapter that focused exclusively on physio-
the project in its now commercially funded and ex- logical changes associated with puberty.
panded versions (the 1946 and 1954 Manuals), made On the subject of social and cultural influences in de-
the following comments about where he looked for his velopment, Carmichael retained five of the Murchison
all-important choices of content: chapters: two chapters on environmental forces on the
child by Kurt Lewin and by Harold Jones, Dorothea Mc-
Both as editor of the Manual and as the author of a spe- Carthys chapter on childrens language, Vernon Joness
cial chapter, the writer is indebted . . . [for] extensive
chapter on childrens morality (now entitled Character
excerpts and the use of other materials previously pub-
DevelopmentAn Objective Approach), and Margaret
lished in the Handbook of Child Psychology, Revised Edi-
tion. (1946, p. viii)
Meads chapter on primitive children (now enhanced
by several spectacular photos of mothers and children
Both the Handbook of Child Psychology and the Handbook
from exotic cultures around the world). Carmichael also
of Child Psychology, Revised Edition, were edited by Dr.
stayed with three other Murchison topics (emotional de-
Carl Murchison. I wish to express here my profound appre-
ciation for the pioneer work done by Dr. Murchison in pro-
velopment, gifted children, and sex differences), but he
ducing these handbooks and other advanced books in selected new authors to cover them. But Carmichael
psychology. The Manual owes much in spirit and content dropped Piaget and Bhler.
to the foresight and editorial skill of Dr. Murchison. Carmichaels 1954 revision, his second and final edi-
(1954, p. viii) tion, was very close in structure and content to the 1946
Manual. Carmichael again retained the heart of Murchi-
The first quote comes from Carmichaels preface to sons original vision, many of Murchisons original
the 1946 edition, the second from his preface to the authors and chapter topics, and some of the same mate-
1954 edition. We shall never know why Carmichael rial that dated all the way back to the 1931 Handbook.
waited until the 1954 edition to add the personal tribute Not surprisingly, the chapters that were closest to
to Carl Murchison. Perhaps a careless typist dropped Carmichaels own interests got the most significant up-
the laudatory passage from a handwritten version of the dating. Carmichael leaned toward the biological and
1946 preface and its omission escaped Carmichaels physiological whenever possible. He clearly favored ex-
notice. Or perhaps 8 years of further adult development perimental treatments of psychological processes. Yet he
increased Carmichaels generosity of spirit. (It also still kept the social, cultural, and psychological analyses
may be possible that Murchison or his family com- by Lewin, Mead, McCarthy, Terman, Harold Jones, and
Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition xv

Vernon Jones, and he even went so far as to add one new on Carmichaels last Manual, Paul Mussen wrote, The
chapter on social development by Harold and Gladys 1954 edition of this Manual had only one theoretical
Anderson and one new chapter on emotional develop- chapter, and that was concerned with Lewinian theory
ment by Arthur Jersild. which, so far as we can see, has not had a significant
The Murchison and Carmichael volumes make for lasting impact on developmental psychology (Mussen,
fascinating reading, even today. The perennial themes of 1970, p. x). The intervening years had seen a turning
the field were there from the start: the nature-nurture away from the norm of psychological research once
debate; the generalizations of universalists opposed by fondly referred to as dust-bowl empiricism.
the particularizations of contextualists; the alternating The Mussen 1970 editionor Carmichaels Manual,
emphases on continuities and discontinuities during on- as it was still calledhad a new look and an almost
togenesis; and the standard categories of maturation, entirely new set of contents. The two-volume edition
learning, locomotor activity, perception, cognition, lan- carried only one chapter from the earlier books,
guage, emotion, conduct, morality, and cultureall Carmichaels updated version of his own long chapter
separated for the sake of analysis, yet, as authors on the Onset and Early Development of Behavior,
throughout each of the volumes acknowledged, all some- which had made its appearance under a different title in
how inextricably joined in the dynamic mix of human Murchisons 1933 edition. Otherwise, as Mussen wrote
development. in his preface, It should be clear from the outset . . .
These things have not changed. Yet, much in the early that the present volumes are not, in any sense, a revision
editions is now irrevocably dated. Long lists of chil- of the earlier editions; this is a completely new Manual
drens dietary preferences, sleeping patterns, elimina- (Mussen, 1970, p. x).
tion habits, toys, and somatic types look quaint and And it was. In comparison to Carmichaels last edi-
pointless through todays lenses. The chapters on chil- tion 16 years earlier, the scope, variety, and theoretical
drens thought and language were written prior to the depth of the Mussen volumes were astonishing. The
great contemporary breakthroughs in neurology and field had blossomed, and the new Manual showcased
brain / behavior research, and they show it. The chapters many of the new bouquets that were being produced.
on social and emotional development were ignorant of The biological perspective was still strong, grounded by
the processes of social influence and self-regulation that chapters on physical growth ( by J. M. Tanner) and phys-
soon would be revealed through attribution research and iological development ( by Dorothy Eichorn) and by
other studies in social psychology. Terms such as cogni- Carmichaels revised chapter (now made more elegant
tive neuroscience, neuronal networks, behavior genetics, by some excerpts from Greek philosophy and modern
social cognition, dynamic systems, and positive youth de- poetry). But two other cousins of biology also were rep-
velopment were of course unknown. Even Meads rendi- resented, in an ethological chapter by Eckhard Hess and
tion of the primitive child stands as a weak straw in a behavior genetics chapter by Gerald McClearn. These
comparison to the wealth of cross-cultural knowledge chapters were to define the major directions of biologi-
available in todays cultural psychology. cal research in the field for at least the next 3 decades.
Most telling, the assortments of odd facts and norma- As for theory, Mussens Handbook was thoroughly
tive trends were tied together by very little theory permeated with it. Much of the theorizing was organ-
throughout the Carmichael chapters. It was as if, in the ized around the approaches that, in 1970, were known
exhilaration of discovery at the frontiers of a new field, as the three grand systems: (1) Piagets cognitive-
all the facts looked interesting in and of themselves. developmentalism, (2) psychoanalysis, and (3) learning
That, of course, is what makes so much of the material theory. Piaget was given the most extensive treatment.
seem odd and arbitrary. It is hard to know what to make He reappeared in the Manual, this time authoring a
of the lists of facts, where to place them, which ones comprehensive (and, some say, definitive) statement of
were worth keeping track of and which ones are expend- his entire theory, which now bore little resemblance to
able. Not surprisingly, the bulk of the data presented in his 1931/1933 sortings of childrens intriguing verbal
the Carmichael manuals seems not only outdated by expressions. In addition, chapters by John Flavell, by
todays standards but, worse, irrelevant. David Berlyne, by Martin Hoffman, and by William
By 1970, the importance of theory for understanding Kessen, Marshall Haith, and Philip Salapatek all gave
human development had become apparent. Looking back major treatments to one or another aspect of Piagets
xvi Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition

body of work. Other approaches were represented as quite on center stage. Research topics now ranged from
well. Herbert and Ann Pick explicated Gibsonian the- childrens play to brain lateralization, from childrens
ory in a chapter on sensation and perception, Jonas family life to the influences of school, day care, and dis-
Langer wrote a chapter on Werners organismic theory, advantageous risk factors. There also was coverage of
David McNeill wrote a Chomskian account of language the burgeoning attempts to use developmental theory as
development, and Robert LeVine wrote an early version a basis for clinical and educational interventions. The
of what was soon to become culture theory. interventions usually were described at the end of chap-
With its increased emphasis on theory, the 1970 Man- ters that had discussed the research relevant to the
ual explored in depth a matter that had been all but ne- particular intervention efforts, rather than in whole
glected in the books previous versions: the mechanisms chapters dedicated specifically to issues of practice.
of change that could account for, to use Murchisons old This brings us to the efforts under the present edito-
phrase, the problem of how the infant becomes an adult rial team: the Handbooks fifth and sixth editions ( but
psychologically. In the process, old questions such as really the seventh and eighth editions, if the germinal
the relative importance of nature versus nurture were re- two pre-Wiley Murchison editions are counted). I must
visited, but with far more sophisticated conceptual and leave it to future commentators to provide a critical sum-
methodological tools. mation of what we have done. The volume editors have
Beyond theory building, the 1970 Manual addressed an offered introductory and/or concluding renditions of
array of new topics and featured new contributors: peer their own volumes. I will add to their efforts here only
interaction (Willard Hartup), attachment (Eleanor Mac- by stating the overall intent of our design and by com-
coby and John Masters), aggression (Seymour Feshback), menting on some directions that our field has taken in
individual differences (Jerome Kagan and Nathan Kogan), the years from 1931 to 2006.
and creativity (Michael Wallach). All of these areas of in- We approached our editions with the same purpose
terest are still very much with us in the new millennium. that Murchison, Carmichael, and Mussen before us had
If the 1970 Manual reflected a blossoming of the shared: to provide, as Mussen wrote, a comprehen-
fields plantings, the 1983 Handbook reflected a field sive and accurate picture of the current state of knowl-
whose ground cover had spread beyond any boundaries edgethe major systematic thinking and researchin
that could have been previously anticipated. New the most important research areas of the psychology of
growth had sprouted in literally dozens of separate lo- human development (Mussen, 1983, p. vii). We as-
cations. A French garden, with its overarching designs sumed that the Handbook should be aimed specifically
and tidy compartments, had turned into an English gar- for the scholar, as Murchison declared, and that it
den, a bit unruly but glorious in its profusion. Mussens should have the character of an advanced text, as
two-volume Carmichaels Manual had now become the Carmichael defined it. We expected, though, that our
four-volume Mussen Handbook, with a page-count in- audiences may be more interdisciplinary than the read-
crease that came close to tripling the 1970 edition. erships of previous editions, given the greater tendency
The grand old theories were breaking down. Piaget of todays scholars to cross back and forth among fields
was still represented by his 1970 piece, but his influence such as psychology, cognitive science, neurobiology,
was on the wane throughout the other chapters. Learning history, linguistics, sociology, anthropology, educa-
theory and psychoanalysis were scarcely mentioned. Yet tion, and psychiatry. We also believed that research-
the early theorizing had left its mark, in vestiges that oriented practitioners should be included under the
were apparent in new approaches, and in the evident con- rubric of the scholars for whom this Handbook was
ceptual sophistication with which authors treated their intended. To that end, for the first time in 1998 and
material. No return to dust bowl empiricism could be again in the present edition, we devoted an entire vol-
found anywhere in the set. Instead, a variety of classical ume to child psychology in practice.
and innovative ideas were coexisting: Ethology, neurobi- Beyond these very general intentions, we have let
ology, information processing, attribution theory, cul- chapters in the Handbooks fifth and sixth editions take
tural approaches, communications theory, behavioral their own shape. We solicited the chapters from authors
genetics, sensory-perception models, psycholinguistics, who were widely acknowledged to be among the leading
sociolinguistics, discontinuous stage theories, and con- experts in their areas of the field, although we know
tinuous memory theories all took their places, with none that, given an entirely open-ended selection process and
Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition xvii

no limits of budget, we would have invited a large num- has a way of moving in alternating cycles (or spirals, for
ber of other leading researchers whom we did not have those who wish to capture the progressive nature of sci-
the spaceand thus the privilegeto include. With entific development). In our time, developmental study
very few exceptions, every author whom we invited has cycled away from classic topics such as motivation
agreed to accept the challenge. Our only real, and great, and learningnot in the sense that they were entirely
sadness was to hear of the passing of several authors forgotten, or that good work ceased to be done in such
from the 1998 edition prior to our assembly of the pres- areas, but in the sense that they no longer were the most
ent edition. Where possible, we arranged to have their prominent subjects of theoretical reflection and debate.
collaborators revise and update their chapters. Some of the relative neglect was intentional, as scholars
Our directive to authors was simple: Convey your got caught up in controversies about whether psycholog-
area of the field as you see it. From then on, the authors ical motivation was a real phenomenon worthy of
took center stagewith, of course, much constructive study or whether learning could or should be distin-
feedback from reviewers and volume editors. No one guished from development in the first place. All this has
tried to impose a perspective, a preferred method of in- changed. As the contents of our current edition attest,
quiry, or domain boundaries on any of the chapters. The developmental science always returns, sooner or later, to
authors expressed their views on what researchers in concepts that are necessary for explaining the heart of
their areas attempt to accomplish, why they do so, how its concerns, progressive change in individuals and so-
they go about it, what intellectual sources they draw on, cial groups over time, and concepts such as learning and
what progress they have made, and what conclusions motivation are indispensable for this task. Among the
they have reached. exciting features of this Handbook edition are the ad-
The result, in my opinion, is still more glorious pro- vances it presents in theoretical and empirical work on
fusion of the English garden genre, but perhaps con- these classic concepts.
tained a bit by some broad patterns that have emerged The other concept that has met some resistance in
over the past decade. Powerful theoretical models and recent years is the notion of development itself. For
approachesnot quite unified theories, such as the some social critics, the idea of progress, implicit in the
three grand systemshave begun once again to organize notion of development, has seemed out of step with
much of the fields research and practice. There is great principles such as equality and cultural diversity. Some
variety in these models and approaches, and each is genuine benefits have accrued from that critique; for
drawing together significant clusters of work. Some example, the field has worked to better appreciate di-
have been only recently formulated, and some are com- verse developmental pathways. But, like many critique
binations or modifications of classic theories that still positions, it led to excesses. For some, it became ques-
have staying power. tionable to explore issues that lie at the heart of human
Among the formidable models and approaches that development. Growth, advancement, positive change,
the reader will find in this Handbook are the dynamic achievement, and standards for improved performance
system theories, the life span and life course ap- and conduct, all were questioned as legitimate subjects
proaches, cognitive science and neuronal models, the of investigation.
behavior genetics approach, person-context interaction Just as in the cases of learning and motivation, no
theories, action theories, cultural psychology, and a doubt it was inevitable that the fields center of gravity
wide assortment of neo-Piagetian and neo-Vygotskian sooner or later would return to broad concerns of devel-
models. Although some of these models and approaches opment. The story of growth from infancy to adulthood is
have been in the making for some time, they have now a developmental story of multifaceted learning, acquisi-
come into their own. Researchers are drawing on them tions of skills and knowledge, waxing powers of attention
directly, taking their implied assumptions and hypothe- and memory, growing neuronal and other biological ca-
ses seriously, using them with specificity and control, pacities, formations and transformations of character
and exploiting their implications for practice. and personality, increases and reorganizations in the un-
Another pattern that emerges is a rediscovery and derstanding of self and others, advances in emotional and
exploration of core processes in human development behavioral regulation, progress in communicating and
that had been underexamined by the generation of re- collaborating with others, and a host of other achieve-
searchers just prior to the present one. Scientific interest ments documented in this edition. Parents, teachers, and
xviii Preface to Handbook of Child Psychology, Sixth Edition

other adults in all parts of the world recognize and value and current set of scientific theories and findings
such developmental achievements in children, although available in the field today.
they do not always know how to understand them, let
alone how to foster them. February 2006
The sorts of scientific findings that the Handbooks Palo Alto, California
authors explicate in their chapters are needed to pro-
vide such understanding. The importance of sound sci- REFERENCES
entific understanding has become especially clear in
recent years, when news media broadcast story after Bhler, C. (1931). The social participation of infants and toddlers. In
story based on simplistic and biased popular specula- C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of child psychology. Worcester,
MA: Clark University Press.
tions about the causes of human development. The
Carmichael, L. (Ed.). (1946). Manual of child psychology. New York:
careful and responsible discourse found in these chap- Wiley.
ters contrasts sharply with the typical news story about Carmichael, L. (Ed.). (1954). Manual of child psychology (2nd ed.).
the role of parents, genes, or schools in childrens New York: Wiley.
growth and behavior. There is not much contest as to Mead, M. (1931). The primitive child. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A
handbook of child psychology. Worcester, MA: Clark University
which source the public looks to for its information and Press.
stimulation. But the good news is that scientific truth Murchison, C. (Ed.). (1931). A handbook of child psychology. Worces-
usually works its way into the public mind over the long ter, MA: Clark University Press.
run. The way this works would make a good subject for Murchison, C. (Ed.). (1933). A handbook of child psychology (2nd
ed.). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.
developmental study some day, especially if such a
Mussen, P. (Ed.). (1970). Carmichaels manual of child psychology.
study could find a way to speed up the process. In the New York: Wiley.
meantime, readers of this edition of the Handbook of Mussen, P. (Ed.). (1983). Handbook of child psychology. New York:
Child Psychology will find the most solid, insightful Wiley.
Contents

1 INTRODUCTION 1
Nancy Eisenberg

2 THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE PERSON: SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING,


RELATIONSHIPS, CONSCIENCE, SELF 24
Ross A. Thompson

3 TEMPERAMENT 99
Mary K. Rothbart and John E. Bates

4 BIOLOGY, CULTURE, AND TEMPERAMENTAL BIASES 167


Jerome Kagan and Nathan A. Fox

5 EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT: ACTION, COMMUNICATION,


AND UNDERSTANDING 226
Carolyn Saarni, Joseph J. Campos, Linda A. Camras, and David Witherington

6 PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 300


Avshalom Caspi and Rebecca L. Shiner

7 SOCIALIZATION PROCESSES 366


Daphne Blunt Bugental and Joan E. Grusec

8 SOCIALIZATION IN THE FAMILY: ETHNIC AND


ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES 429
Ross D. Parke and Raymond Buriel

9 THE SELF 505


Susan Harter

10 PEER INTERACTIONS, RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS 571


Kenneth H. Rubin, William M. Bukowski, and Jeffrey G. Parker

xxiii
xxiv Contents

11 PROSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 646


Nancy Eisenberg, Richard A. Fabes, and Tracy L. Spinrad

12 AGGRESSION AND ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR IN YOUTH 719


Kenneth A. Dodge, John D. Coie, and Donald Lynam

13 THE DEVELOPMENT OF MORALITY 789


Elliot Turiel

14 GENDER DEVELOPMENT 858


Diane N. Ruble, Carol Lynn Martin, and Sheri A. Berenbaum

15 DEVELOPMENT OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 933


Allan Wigfield, Jacquelynne S. Eccles, Ulrich Schiefele, Robert W. Roeser, and Pamela Davis-Kean

16 ADOLESCENT DEVELOPMENT IN INTERPERSONAL CONTEXT 1003


W. Andrew Collins and Laurence Steinberg

Author Index 1069

Subject Index 1113


Preface to Volume Three
Social, Emotional, and Personality Development
NANCY EISENBERG

As for the fifth edition, this volume has been a labor of and then editing them to fit within the stringent space
love. When I was a graduate student, we read many of limitations and page restrictions. The contributors to
the chapters from the 1970 edition of the Mussen Hand- this volume poured their souls into their chapters; I was
book of Child Psychology is our classes. Even then, in my fortunate to work with such a talented, cooperative, and
mind it was the "bible" of the field. Moreover, my men- personable group of people.
tor was Paul Mussen, so I frequently borrowed one of his I also thank the many other people who made this
extra copies of the Handbook, and he gave me that copy volume possible. First and foremost, I thank my hus-
as a gift when I graduated. When the 1983 edition of the band, Jerry Harris, for his continuing support through-
Handbook was being prepared, I was fortunate as a out this and my other time-consuming projects. I also
young scholar to get to review a couple of chapters; in thank Richard Lerner for his assistance in dealing with
addition, I heard the occasional grumblings of Paul administrative issues related to the volume. The senior
Mussen about the chapters that were late. At that time in editors, Richard Lerner and William Damon, allowed
my career it was my dream that I would someday get to the volume editors to shape their own volumes, which I
contribute a chapter to the Handbook. greatly appreciated. Numerous colleagues who critiqued
Thus, I was pleased and honored (as well as a little the chapters in manuscript form also provided valuable
apprehensive) when I was asked to edit a volume of this insights and suggestions that enhanced the quality of the
very important set of books for the fifth edition, and final product.
now the sixth edition. Editing the volume has been an In addition, I thank my colleagues and students who
exceptional learning experience. I have had the privi- shared their ideas with me in the course of this task. I
lege to work with some of the best people in the field also appreciate the financial support that I received over
and to read (and reread) a year or two before other the last few years from the National Institutes of Mental
people the very exciting work that is in this volume. I Health and the National Institute of Drug Abuse. Fi-
have learned much from my fellow contributors to the nally, I thank Paul Mussen, my friend and mentor, who
volume. has provided me with intellectual and emotional support
The contributors to this volume deserve much thanks. for approximately three decades. His death has left a
All of the people who were requested to contribute hole in my life and that of many others. It has been a
agreed to do so, and all of the chapters were completed. privilege and joy to carry on the tradition of the Hand-
The authors spent tremendous energy and time con- book that he edited twice in his career, and I dedicate
structing their very thoughtful and integrative chapters, this book to him.

xxi
CHAPTER 1

Introduction
NANCY EISENBERG

AN EMPHASIS ON PROCESS (MEDIATION) A FOCUS ON COGNITION 11


AND MODERATION 1 A FOCUS ON CONTEXTUAL AND ENVIRONMENTAL
A FOCUS ON EMOTION 3 INPUTS TO DEVELOPMENT 13
Temperament, Personality, and Emotion 4 Diversity 13
Emotion and Social Behavior 5 Types and Examples of Contextual Inf luence 14
Emotion, the Self, and Goals 6 A FOCUS ON BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES 15
Emotion and Morality 6 Biology, Temperament, and Personality 16
Sex and Emotion 7 Biology and Socioemotional Development 17
Emotion in Socialization and the Socialization A FOCUS ON RELATIONSHIPS 19
of Emotion 7 A FOCUS ON APPLICATION 20
A FOCUS ON REGULATION 8 SUMMARY 20
Contemporary Work on Regulation 8
REFERENCES 21
The Development of Emotion-Related Regulation 9
Modes of Regulation 10

The sixth edition of the Handbook of Child Psychology AN EMPHASIS ON PROCESS (MEDIATION)
was written approximately 8 years after the 1998 fifth AND MODERATION
edition. As was true for the last edition, the goal of the
authors in this volume of the Handbook was to present As developmental psychologists have produced and ac-
state-of-the-art reviews of conceptual and empirical cumulated more knowledge about the occurrence and
work on social, emotional, and personality develop- frequency of variables of interest (e.g., descriptive data)
ment. Each author or set of authors has provided the as well as about relations among constructs (i.e., corre-
reader with an integrative summary of the current sta- lational data), they have begun to ask more complex
tus of an important topic within the domain of social questions than in the past. This trend is very evident in
and personality development and, to some degree, with this volume of the Handbook, even more so than in the
a vision for the future. Although research on social, fifth edition (1998). For example, in addition to rou-
emotional, and personality development is a cumula- tinely questioning assumptions of directionality of
tive endeavor with few abrupt, dramatic changes in causality, there is evidence in the chapters of an in-
knowledge, the field does have a somewhat different creased concern with process, as reflected in questions
look from 8 years ago, and differs greatly from that about mediation. Mediating processes are the processes
depicted in the fourth edition of the Handbook in underlying the relation between two variables (a predic-
1983. In this chapter, I note some of the themes in this tor and a criterion). Mediators help clarify how or why a
volume, with an eye to changes in themes in the past 20 given relation occurs (Baron & Kenny, 1986; MacKin-
or so years. non, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002).

Work on this chapter was supported by a grant from the Na- for her comments on a draft of this chapter and to Joseph Cam-
tional Institute of Mental Health. Thanks to Mary Rothbart pos for his discussion with me of some of the ideas therein.

1
2 Introduction

In addition, based in part on the contemporary con- others approval of the relation between genes (as ex-
cern with context, diversity, biological substrates and pressed, for example, in temperament or attractiveness)
predispositions, and indices of individual differences and self-esteem. Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Hand-
(see later discussion), there is considerable interest in book, this volume) discuss a number of biological systems
moderating variables, that is, in variables such as sex, so- that mediate between heredity or biological structures
cioeconomic class, race/ethnicity, personality, prior so- and psychological or behavioral responses, whereas
cialization experiences, and type of situation that affect Ruble, Martin, and Berenbaum (Chapter 14, this Hand-
the direction or strength of the relation between an inde- book, this volume) note that genes and hormones are often
pendent or predictor variable and a dependent or criterion viewed as proximal mediators of the effect of evolution-
variable (Baron & Kenny, 1986). To study moderation, ary forces on gender differences. Eisenberg, Fabes, and
investigators generally examine the interaction of the in- Spinrad (Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume) present
dependent variable with the potential moderator (e.g., data indicating that sympathy at least partly mediates the
age, high or low level of regulation) when predicting an relations between moral judgment or perspective taking
outcome or criterion variable or comparing the equiva- and childrens prosocial behavior, whereas childrens reg-
lence of structural models for different groups. ulation mediates the relation between parental expression
Mediating or moderating processes are discussed di- of negative emotion in the family and childrens sympa-
rectly or indirectly by all chapter authors, but are a more thy. They also present a model in which a number of more
central focus in some chapters. For example, in their dis- proximal factors mediate the relations of more distal fac-
cussion of relations of temperament to childrens adjust- tors (e.g., biological factors, socialization, and an-
ment, Rothbart and Bates (Chapter 3, this Handbook, this tecedent sociocognitive and dispositional characteristics
volume) explicitly discuss two types of indirect relations: of the child) to prosocial behavior.
mediated linkage, as when temperament influences trans- Mediation is invoked repeatedly in discussions of par-
actions with the environment, which, in turn, shape the enting, parent-child attachment, family variables, and de-
childs developing adjustment, and moderated relations, velopmental outcomes. After a review of the literature on
as when temperament and some facet of the environment attachment, Thompson calls for additional exploration of
affect childrens adjustment. Bugental and Grusec (Chap- the mediators of the relations of attachment security to
ter 7, this Handbook, this volume) also explicitly empha- outcome variables. Rubin, Bukowski, and Parker (Chapter
size moderation in the socialization process, and Dodge, 10, this Handbook, this volume) present data indicating
Coie, and Lynam (Chapter 12, this Handbook, this vol- that lower levels of parental skill have been associated
ume) review studies of moderating effects, concluding with higher levels of antisocial behavior and lower levels
that such research is essential for delineating the combi- of academic performance, which in turn have been associ-
nations of factors that predict antisocial behavior. ated with higher levels of peer rejection. Dodge et al.
A few more specific examples of how authors discuss (Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume) discuss social-
mediation and moderation illustrate the types of issues cognitive processes as mediators of the relations between
that are the focus of contemporary work in developmental parenting (e.g., abuse) and offsprings externalizing be-
psychology and related disciplines. First, consider media- havior. They also argue that parenting is a mediator of the
tion. Collins and Steinberg (Chapter 16, this Handbook, relation between the macrolevel variable of family
this volume) conclude that the impact of pubertal matura- poverty and childrens aggression. Wigfield, Eccles,
tion on adolescent psychosocial development is more Schiefele, Roeser, and Davis-Kean (Chapter 15, this
likely to be interpersonally mediated than due to the di- Handbook, this volume) suggest that a range of parental
rect action of hormonal changes on mood or emotional beliefs and practices mediate between family demograph-
functioning. Similarly, multiple authors note that the ics and achievement-related outcomes. In their model, spe-
quality of relationships or social interactions likely medi- cific parental behaviors (e.g., time spent with the child,
ates between distal environmental factors (e.g., economic teaching strategies) at least partially mediate relations be-
resources, quality of the neighborhood), family structure, tween parental general beliefs and behaviors (e.g., locus
or social institutions and youths adjustment and other of control, gender-role stereotypes, parenting style) and
socioemotional developmental outcomes (e.g., Dodge childrens outcomes (e.g., goals, persistence, perfor-
et al., Chapter 12; Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8; Thompson, mance). Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this Handbook,
Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume). Harter (Chapter this volume) examine a range of potential mediators in re-
9, this Handbook, this volume) argues for mediation by gard to the relation of socialization to developmental out-
A Focus on Emotion 3

comes, including childrens acquired ways of cognitively than for White American children. Dodge et al. (Chapter
representing their social worlds, hormones (and neuro- 12, this Handbook, this volume) note that harsh disci-
transmitters) involved in childrens responses to social- pline ( but not abuse) is a predictor of later aggressive be-
ization experiences, and gene expression in the continuous havior for White but not African American children.
reorganization of the brain in response to experience. Fi- Further, Ruble et al., Chapter 14, and Turiel and others,
nally, Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook, this Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume, discuss ways in
volume) discuss a variety of mediating processes related which the sex of the child may moderate the effects of
to personality and note the need for further investigation social experience on various social behaviors (although
of both the proximal relationship-specific processes that authors did not always use the term moderation). Thus,
mediate personality effects on relationship outcomes and moderators that have received substantial attention in-
mediators (e.g., parental attributions) of the association clude temperament /personality, childrens sex, race,
between parental personality and parenting behavior. The culture, and parenting style or support.
list could go on and on; what is impressive is how mediat- One moderator of the effects of the socializing envi-
ing processes have become such a central focus in work on ronment that has received relatively little attention in the
socioemotional development. past is genetic differences between children. Recently,
Moderational processes are also repeatedly emphasized Caspi et al. (2002) reported such moderation for the long-
in this volume. For example, Bugental and Grusec discuss term effects of maltreatment. Those children whose
the ways that parental goals serve to moderate parental be- genotype resulted in high levels of MAOA (monoamine
havior (including their affective responses) on different oxidase A, an enzyme that metabolizes neurotransmitters
occasions, as well as the role of culture as a moderator of such as serotonin, and thus renders them inactive) were
the relation between socialization experiences and chil- more likely than children without this genetic tendency to
drens development. Collins and Steinberg (Chapter 16, exhibit antisocial problems if they were maltreated (see
this Handbook, this volume) cite evidence indicating that Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume).
the impact of puberty on psychological functioning is mod- With the new wave of research on genes, neurotransmit-
erated by the social context in which adolescents mature. ters, and other biological factors, investigators are likely
They also cite research indicating that aspects of parent- to obtain much more evidence of interactions between bi-
ingfor example, parenting style and parenting prac- ological indices and environmental factors or experiences
ticesmay interact with one another in the prediction of in the next decade (see Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Wood-
outcome variables such as youths adjustment. Numerous worth, & Stewart, 1995). Such research, as well as re-
other authors in this volume review empirical interactions search on interactions involving the kinds of variables
between childrens temperament (e.g., emotionality or reg- already discussed, is producing a more differentiated un-
ulation) or personality and their parenting experiences in derstanding of when relations and processes occuran
predicting childrens behavior or socioemotional develop- issue that is an essential complement to the more basic
ment (e.g., Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4; Rothbart & Bates, mediational question of why relations occur (Parke,
Chapter 3; Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this vol- 2004). Moreover, as dynamical systems approaches be-
ume). Components of temperament or personality may come more popular, evidence of indirect, nonlinear rela-
also interact with one another: For example, Eisenberg tions among multiple variables is likely to be more
et al. (Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume) summarize common than at the present (e.g., Lewis, 2002).
data indicating that the interaction of individual differ-
ences in emotionality and regulation predicts childrens A FOCUS ON EMOTION
prosocial behavior and sympathy better than the considera-
tion of only the main effects of these predictors. The fifth edition of the Handbook (1998) was the first to
Demographic characteristics that reflect diversity are include a chapter dedicated solely to the topic of emo-
other common moderators of predictors of developmen- tion (rather than the more general topic of socioemo-
tal outcomes. Wigfield et al. (Chapter 15, this Handbook, tional development in infancy). The neglect of emotion
this volume) suggest that competence-related beliefs and in prior editions is not surprising given the history of the
school performance may predict school performance for study of emotion in psychology in the past 50 years. Due
White but not African American children and that aca- to the influence of behaviorism and then cognitive ap-
demic self-concept of ability is less predictive of general proaches in psychology, emotion was considered a nui-
self-esteem for at least some African American children sance variable (and something of no relevance) for many
4 Introduction

years. In the past 10 to 15 years, however, emotion has ences in reactivity are fundamental constructs; thus,
become central to the study of social development, as dispositional emotional tendencies are salient topics in
well as to many other topics in psychology. the two chapters that deal with temperament (Kagan &
The current emphasis on emotion is a dramatic de- Fox, Chapter 4; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this Hand-
parture from the previous view of emotions as intrapsy- book, this volume) and the chapter on personality (i.e.,
chic events which do not play a causal role in behavior Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume).
and which are secondary by-products of more signifi- Temperament is defined by Rothbart and Bates as
cant processes (Campos, 1984, p. 148). Today emotions constitutionally based individual differences in reactiv-
are viewed as motivational forces that play a role in ity and self-regulation. Reactivity includes emotional
much of our social behavior. As noted by Parke (1994), responding, both in regard to specific emotions (e.g.,
in contemporary psychology, emotions are viewed as fear) and more general constructs of emotion (e.g., neg-
both products and processes of social interactions, re- ative emotionality or emotional intensity; see Larsen &
lationships, and contexts (p. 158). Diener, 1987; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 2, this Hand-
The central role of emotion in contemporary develop- book, this volume). Regulation pertains to the modula-
mental psychology is reflected in most of the chapters in tion of temperamental reactivity (Ahadi & Rothbart,
this volume. This focus is, of course, most evident in 1994; Eisenberg, 2002; Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981).
Saarni, Campos, Camras, and Witheringtons chapter Research on temperament /personality, and hence on
on emotional development (Chapter 5, this Handbook, emotional reactivity and self-regulation, has increased
this volume). Saarni et al. take a functionalist perspec- greatly in volume in the past 1 to 2 decades.
tive in which emotion is closely linked to the context Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume)
and what a person is trying to do. Emotion is viewed as view temperament as a biologically based bias for corre-
synonymous with the significance of a person-event lated clusters of feelings, thoughts, and actions that ap-
transaction for the individual. pear during childhood, but not always in the opening
Due to the immense body of work relevant to emo- months, and are sculpted by varied rearing environments
tion, Saarni et al. (Chapter 5, this Handbook, this vol- into a large but still limited number of traits that comprise
ume) limit their coverage, focusing primarily on the an individuals personality profile. Thus, like Rothbart
conceptualization of emotion, developmental changes and Bates (Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume), they
in emotion and emotion communication, the role of cul- include emotion in their definition of temperament. How-
ture in emotion in emotional development, and the ever, they do not emphasize regulatory components of
components of emotional competence, their develop- temperament to the same degree as Rothbart and Bates,
ment, and their relations to adjustment and social com- and they tie dispositional biases to thought patterns
petence. Saarni et al.s review of this portion of the (Rothbart & Bates view the content of thought as person-
emotion literature demonstrates that childrens under- ality rather than temperament), as well as to actions.
standing of emotion and its expression, as well as chil- Dispositional emotionality also plays an important
drens communication of, and coping with, emotion, role in concepts of personality. In fact, Caspi (1998)
change considerably with age. Moreover, emotional un- defined personality as individual differences in the
derstanding and communication seem to have a pro- tendency to behave, think, and feel in certain consis-
found influence on social interaction, although the tent ways (p. 312). Personality theorists often include
relation between social interaction and these aspects of in personality not only traits, but also personal con-
functioning is doubtlessly reciprocal. Saarni et al.s re- cerns (i.e., a wide array of motivational, developmen-
view reflects major domains of interest in recent work tal, or strategic constructs that are contextualized in
on emotion and provides a contemporary, contextually time, place, or role) and life stories (McAdams, 1995).
oriented perspective on emotional development. As noted by Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Hand-
book, this volume), personality is typically seen as
including a wider range of individual differences in
Temperament, Personality, and Emotion
feeling, thinking, and behaving than is temperament.
Emotion can be viewed in both situationally specific Similarly, Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Handbook,
and dispositional terms. In theory and research on tem- this volume) assert that, the stable variation in be-
perament and personality, enduring individual differ- haviors and emotions observed in older children, ado-
A Focus on Emotion 5

lescents and adults are personality traits, not tempera- Emotion and Social Behavior
mental biases, although the latter make a contribution
to the profile that emerges later in development. In addition to playing a role in later personality, and
Analogously, Rothbart and Bates (Chapter 3, this consistent with much of the review in the Saarni et al.s
Handbook, this volume) note that, Temperament rep- Chapter 5 (this Handbook, this volume) on emotion, in-
resents the affective, activational, and attentional core dividual differences in temperamental emotionality, in-
of personality, whereas personality includes much cluding directly experienced negative emotions such as
more than temperament, particularly the content of anger and vicariously induced emotion (e.g., sympathy
thought, skills, habits, values, defenses, morals, be- or empathy), frequently have been found to predict vari-
liefs, and social cognition. Social cognition includes ation among children in socioemotional development
the perception of the self, others, and the relation of (see Dodge et al., Chapter 12; Eisenberg et al., Chapter
self to objects, events, and others. 11; Turiel, Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume). For
Despite these definitional distinctions, as noted by example, to a much greater degree than 8 years ago,
Caspi and Shiner, there is a striking similarity between there is empirical support for relations between disposi-
the constructs of temperament and personality. Both can tional emotionalityirritability/anger, fearfulness, and
be observed in animals (at least to some degree) as well positive emotionalityand social competence or adjust-
as humans; both involve moderate genetic influence but ment, including internalizing and externalizing problem
are also affected by experience; and, importantly, behavior (see Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6; Kagan & Fox,
many traits from both domains are characterized by Chapter 4; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook,
specific habitual positive and negative emotions (Caspi this volume). Moreover, much more often than a decade
& Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). If we ago, investigators are identifying distinct relations be-
view temperament as representing the building blocks tween different types of negative emotions (e.g., anger
from which personality develops (Rothbart & Bates, versus anxiety or sadness, fear of novelty versus fear of
Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume), these similari- strangers) and the prediction of specific internalizing
ties are not surprising. and externalizing problems or related psychological
Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook, this vol- problems (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2001, 2005; Rothbart &
ume; Caspi, 1998), as well as Rothbart and Bates (Chap- Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume).
ter 3, this Handbook, this volume), speculate on how Ruble et al. (Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume)
aspects of temperament in childhood are linked to the noted an increased focus on emotion in the peer litera-
structure of adult personality (i.e., aspects of the big ture since the fifth edition of the Handbook. They list
five components of personality). For example, tempera- among topics recently introduced to the discipline the
mental negative emotionality is believed to contribute to relation of jealousy and other emotional processes to
the personality construct of neuroticism and agreeable- the maintenance and dissolution of peer relationships.
ness (inversely related) whereas temperamental positive Dispositional emotionality clearly plays a role in the
affect or surgency (and sociability) are associated with quality of social functioning in peer interactions and re-
agreeableness and extraversion in adults. In addition, as- lationships; for example, emotional reactivity has been
pects of temperament believed to be involved in the regu- linked to social withdrawal (Rubin et al., Chapter 10,
lation or control of emotionality and emotionally driven this Handbook, this volume; Spinrad et al., 2004), as
behavior have been linked to personality. For example, well as information processing in social encounters, al-
temperamental behavioral inhibition (see Kagan & Fox, though there is relatively little research on the latter
Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume) is viewed as re- issue (see Arsenio & Lemerise, 2004; Crick & Dodge,
lated to adult neuroticism and low levels of extraversion 1994). Researchers have found that cheerful children
(Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). appear to be relatively popular, whereas children prone
Moreover, temperamental attentional regulation and in- to intense negative emotions are lower in social status
hibitory control likely contribute to the adult personality (see Rubin et al., Chapter 10, this Handbook, this vol-
characteristic of constraint, and perhaps also agreeable- ume). Relations between peer interactions/relationships
ness (although the latter may also have a more proximal and emotion no doubt are reciprocal; as noted by Rubin
temperamental correlate in childhood; see Rothbart & et al. (Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume), experi-
Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume). ences with peers affect social, emotional, and cognitive
6 Introduction

functioning beyond the influences of family, school, velopmental change) in achievement-related beliefs, val-
and neighborhood. ues, and goals. Success and failure are associated with
Contextually specific emotional reactions, in addi- emotional reactions; level of anxiety can affect perfor-
tion to dispositional emotionality, are seen as playing a mance; and emotion-related self-evaluations play a role
major role in peer and other types of relationships. in achievement-related behavior and vice versa. For ex-
Rubin et al. (Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume) ample, high levels of trait-like intrinsic motivation ap-
define relationships as referring to the meanings, expec- pear to foster positive emotional experience and
tations, and emotions that derive from a succession of well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000), as well as self-esteem
interactions between two individuals known to each (Ryan, Connell, & Deci, 1985). Wigfield et al. (Chapter
other. As noted by Thompson (Chapter 2, this Hand- 15, this Handbook, this volume) note that there has been
book, this volume), Emotion is a more salient feature of increasing interest recently in the research on relations
social interaction compared to most encounters with ob- between motivation and affect, and they expect the vol-
jectsincluding the emotions that precede social inter- ume of work on this topic to increase.
action and the changes in emotions that arise from
interactive activity. Thompson also notes that differ-
Emotion and Morality
ences in attachment security are believed to affect early
emotional development and the style of young childrens The role of emotion in the study of morality has varied
emotion regulation, and that these emerging aspects of greatly as a function of the conception of morality. In
the individual influence childrens social, emotional, Kohlbergian work on moral reasoning, emotion tradi-
and personality development in subsequent years. Thus, tionally plays a minor role in comparison to cognition
social relationships are affected by dispositional differ- (see Kohlberg, 1984; Rest, 1983). In contrast, emotions
ences in emotionality as well as by emotions experi- such as empathy-related reactions or guilt have been
enced when interacting with others, and the quality of highlighted in some work on moral behavior, including
social relationships with parents, peers, and others con- theory and research on prosocial tendencies (e.g., Eisen-
tribute to emerging individual differences in situational berg & Fabes, 1990; Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11, this
and dispositional emotionality. Handbook, this volume; Hoffman, 2000), feelings of
guilt (Saarni et al., Chapter 5; Thompson, Chapter 2,
Emotion, the Self, and Goals this Handbook, this volume; Zahn-Waxler & Kochanska,
1990), and conscience (Thompson, Chapter 2, this
Emotion is also an integral aspect of conceptions of the Handbook, this volume). For example, in work on proso-
self. For example, low self-esteem seems to be highly cial behavior, both enduring tendencies toward experi-
related to feelings of depression and hopelessness (Har- encing moral emotions (i.e., dispositional sympathy)
ter, Chapter 9, this Handbook, this volume). Moreover, and situational emotional reactions (e.g., situational
emotions tied to attachment status (e.g., Kochanska, sympathy or guilt) are viewed as motivating altruistic
2001) may affect childrens self-esteem and working action (see Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11, this Handbook,
model of the self in relation to others. In addition, young this volume). And in recent work, emotional reactions
childrens understanding of emotion (e.g., identification such as guilt and empathy are one of two components of
of emotions, knowing when various emotions are likely conscience (Aksan & Kochanska, 2005).
to occur) is viewed by both Harter and Thompson Some contemporary theorists, including Wilson
(Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume) as affecting the (1993), assume that there is a biologically based emo-
childs construction of the self. tional basis to morality (see Turiel, Chapter 13, this
In the 1998 edition of this volume, Eccles, Wigfield, Handbook, this volume). According to Kagan (1984),
and Schiefele concluded that the highest priority in the moral principles are determined by the intensity of the
research on achievement was closer consideration of the communitys affective reactions to the specific content
influence of emotion on motivation. Although there is of the principle. Turiel recognizes the importance of
more work on this issue than a decade ago, there is not emotion in morality, but also emphasizes cognition more
as much as one might expect. Nonetheless, as noted than theorists such as Hoffman, Kagan, and Shweder,
by Wigfield et al. (Chapter 15, this Handbook, this concluding, as he did in 1998, As important as are emo-
volume), emotional development plays a role in some tionsespecially sympathy, empathy, and respectfor
theories regarding individual differences (and likely de- moral functioning, emotions occur in and among persons
A Focus on Emotion 7

who can think about them with regard to other people Until the 1980s, affect was discussed primarily in re-
and in relation to complicated social agendas, goals, and gard to the degree of warmth, support, and harmony ver-
arrangements. The relationships among emotions, moral sus conflict or hostility in the parent-child relationship
judgments, reflections, and deliberations require a great or expressed in parents parenting style (see Collins &
deal of attention in research and in theoretical formula- Steinberg, Chapter 16, this Handbook, this volume). In
tions. Thus, the magnitude of the role of emotion in contrast, the topic of affect /emotion permeates contem-
morality is still an issue of discussion. porary work on socialization, far beyond the emotional
tone of parents interactions with their children.
Sex and Emotion Before highlighting some of the recently emerging
topics in regard to emotion and socialization, it is note-
In the past decade, investigators have not examined sex worthy that nearly every chapter in this volume contains
differences in measures of emotional functioning as some discussion of the relation of the emotional climate
much as might be expected. As one might expect, there in the home and/or schoolthat is, between parents and
are sex differences in the emotions that boys and girls children, teachers and children, and/or between par-
tend to displayfor example, girls tend to display more entsto childrens social, emotional, or achievement-
sadness, fear, shame, and guilt (Eisenberg, Martin, & related development. Most authors of the chapters have
Fabes, 1996; Ruble et al., Chapter 14, this Handbook, noted that the emotional tone of the relationship be-
this volume)although little is known about the degree tween the socializing adult and a child is associated
to which boys and girls differ in internally experienced with the quality of childrens social behavior (e.g., so-
emotion (albeit adolescent girls report more anxiety and cial competence, prosocial and aggressive behavior, peer
depression than do boys). Nonetheless, gender differ- interactions), their conceptions of self and emotional
ences in the degree or type of expression of anger and autonomy, the quality of their interpersonal relation-
frustration may be a factor in the gender difference in ships, their academic-related outcomes, or their adjust-
childrens externalizing behavior and aggression (see ment (e.g., chapters by Bugental & Grusec; Collins &
Dodge et al., Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume). Steinberg; Dodge et al.; Eisenberg et al.; Parke &
Findings regarding gender differences in empathy or Buriel; Rubin et al.; and Thompson, this Handbook, this
sympathy are weaker, although some measures favor fe- volume). Indeed, one of the more consistent findings
males (Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11, this Handbook, this across domains of socioemotional development is the
volume). Guilt is another emotion that girls likely expe- importance of supportive, positive (versus hostile) rela-
rience more than boys; if this is true, there are implica- tionships with socializing adults for childrens healthy
tions for both the development of conscience and for development. Although this is an old topic of study, in-
childrens adjustment (see Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, vestigators are still delineating the many ways in which
1995). In addition, there is evidence that females are bet- the emotional tone of relationships with other people
ter at expressing and decoding emotions than are males may affect the course of childrens development.
(Ruble et al., Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume), According to Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this
which has implications for gender differences in social Handbook, this volume), emotion is one outcome of so-
communication and skills. Because of the centrality of cialization: it [socialization] includes their [childrens]
emotion in recent research and theory on the quality of ability and motivation to acquire individual and cultur-
childrens social functioning, it is likely that investiga- ally shared competencies at a social, emotional, and cog-
tors will attend more to gender differences in the experi- nitive level. Thus, in addition to providing an emotional
ence and expression of emotion in the near future. context for socialization, socialization-related interac-
tions are believed to affect the valence and degree of
emotionality, in part through influencing childrens felt
Emotion in Socialization and the
security and attachment, conceptions of the self, and the
Socialization of Emotion
associations, interpretations, and attributions they make
Socialization is an area of study in which emotion has regarding people, contexts, and events in their lives.
received increased attention in recent years (Parke & In addition, Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this
Buriel, Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume). As noted Handbook, this volume) suggest that emotion affects a
by Parke and Buriel (1998), affect played a relatively variety of cognitive processes fundamental to the social-
minor role in socialization theories until the recent past. ization process, including attentional focus, memory
8 Introduction

retrieval, appraisal and response selection, and the ca- A FOCUS ON REGULATION
pacity for rational or reflective processing. These
processes affect, for example, childrens responses to Because emotional experience and expression often in-
socialization attempts and parents reactions to their volve regulation (or the lack thereof ), contemporary dis-
childrens negative behaviors. Bugental and Grusecs cussion and research on emotion regulation also have
conception of the role of emotion in socialization is been revitalized. Until the early 1990s, popular ap-
more complex, multilayered, and encompassing than in proaches to the topic of regulation included emphases on
most existing theory. parental control and discipline; childrens compliance,
It is no surprise that emotion plays a central role in delay of gratification, and resistance to temptation;
both biologically oriented and culturally oriented so- childrens internalization of societal values regarding
cialization theories. In biologically-based theories, af- behaviors such as aggression and prosocial behavior; and
fect and emotion are conceptualized as basic the role of fear, anxiety, and guilt in fostering internal-
processes to be regulated, as regulators of relation- ization or at least compliance (e.g., Hoffman, 1970,
ships (e.g., attachment relationships), or as conse- 1983; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; see Dodge et al., Chap-
quences of socializing relationships. One relatively ter 12; Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11; Turiel, Chapter 13,
recent focus has been the long-term influences of so- this Handbook, this volume). Although there is still con-
cialization practices on the regulation (or dysregula- siderable interest in these topics, in recent years investi-
tion) of the childs neurohormonal responses, which gators concerned with regulatory processes also have
often co-occur with emotional experience and are part focused on mechanisms by which children regulate their
of emotional responses. Emotional processes are also emotion and emotion-driven behavior, and the relation
viewed as functional regulators of other processes cen- of individual differences in regulation to social compe-
tral to socialization (see Bugental & Grusec, Chapter tence and adjustment. Even though childrens regulation
7, this Handbook, this volume). was clearly an important emerging topic in the fifth edi-
In recent sociocultural perspectives, the expression, tion of the Handbook (1998), more authors discuss this
experience, interpretation, and naming of emotions are topic in this edition of the Handbook, and in consider-
derived, at least in part, from the culture (see Bugental & ably greater depth. Perhaps this is because many view
Grusec, Chapter 7, this Handbook, this volume; Kitayama the regulation of emotion, as much as the emotion itself,
& Markus, 1994; Saarni et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, as related to quality of social behavior and relationships
this volume). Thus, socialization by the culture influ- (Rubin et al., Chapter 10; Saarni et al., Chapter 5, this
ences emotional reactions, as well as a range of social Handbook, this volume).
behaviors. Saarni et al. illustrate this point in their dis-
cussion of how emotion communication accompanies
Contemporary Work on Regulation
and helps to inculcate cultural values, affects pre- and
perinatal emotionality, determines the types of events to Contemporary thinking on the aforementioned topics
which an infant or child is exposed, and creates the emo- has diverse origins in the discipline. The work of the
tional climate within which a person is immersed. As a Blocks (Block & Block, 1980) on ego control has had
consequence of the recent increased awareness of cultural an important impact on this topic of study. Also impor-
contributions to emotional experience and expression, a tant is work by temperament theorists on constructs
number of our current conceptions of emotional develop- such as attentional control (e.g., the ability to shift and
ment are likely to be challenged (see the discussion of focus attention), impulsivity, and effortful control (i.e.,
culture that follows). superordinate self-regulatory systems that can assert
In summary, in the past 2 decades, the topic of emo- control over the reactive and self-regulatory processes
tion has moved to center stage in the study of social and of other temperament systems; Rothbart, Ahadi, Her-
personality development. This surge of interest in emo- shey, & Fisher, 2001; Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). Simi-
tion has been accompanied by, and perhaps is related larly, mechanisms for adaptation discussed by coping
causally to, elevated interest in biological inputs to de- theorists (e.g., Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989;
velopment and temperament. In addition, contemporary Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) for decades can be viewed
concern with culture and context has had a powerful in- as modes of dealing with, or regulating, emotion and
fluence on thinking about emotional development. behavior in stressful contexts. In addition, some of the
A Focus on Regulation 9

adult personality work on constructs such as constraint achievement of goals and learning, as well as academic
or conscientiousness (Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6, this performance.
Handbook, this volume) is relevant to developmental Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this Handbook, this
scientists interested in regulatory processes. volume), Parke and Buriel (Chapter 8, this Handbook,
Self-regulation is discussed, in one form or another, this volume), Dodge and Coie (Chapter 12, this Hand-
in most of the chapters in this volume. For example, book, this volume), and Collins and Steinberg (Chapter
Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume), 16, this Handbook, this volume) focus, to varying de-
as well as Rothbart and Bates (Chapter 3, this Hand- grees, on the socialization correlates of the development
book, this volume), discuss the temperamental (includ- of childrens self-regulationa topic that has flourished
ing physiological) basis of regulation of reactivity; in in the past decade. For example, Bugental and Grusec
addition, Rothbart and Bates emphasize the attentional note the increasing interest in the long-term effects of so-
basis of some forms of self-regulation and review parts cialization practices on the regulation (or dysregulation)
of the growing literature pertaining to the relations of of the childs neurohormonal responses. They, as well
temperamental regulation to adjustment. Caspi and other authors (e.g., Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16;
Shiner (Chapter 5, Handbook, this volume) note that Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume; also
constraint is a component of all contemporary systems see A Focus on Relationships that follows), discuss the
of personality, and highlighted the role of temperamen- co-regulation of the protective care (attachment) system
tal regulation in the emergence of personality traits such by parent and offspring. This protective care relationship
as constraint (or conscientiousness) and neuroticism. (especially parental warmth), in turn, is viewed as facili-
Saarni et al. focus on social communicative mechanisms tating the acquisition of self-regulation skills. Moreover,
used by infants to regulate their behavior (e.g., social Bugental and Grusec briefly discuss socialization prac-
referencing), as well as on the relation of emotion regu- tices and behaviors most associated with childrens au-
lation to adjustment, coping, and emotional compe- tonomous regulation. In addition, Parke and Buriel
tence. They also discuss emotion and language as review literature on the potential role of emotional and
regulators of behavior. attentional regulation as mediators between parenting
Thompson (Chapter 2, Handbook, this volume) re- and child outcomes and the likely mutual causal relations
views the early development of self-regulation, between socialization experiences and individual differ-
whereas Rubin et al. (Chapter 10, this Handbook, this ences in childrens regulation.
volume) summarized literature on the role of self-
regulation in peer competence. The latter also note
The Development of Emotion-Related Regulation
that peer interactions, especially friendships, provide
opportunities to develop and use emotion regulation Based on the literature reviewed in various chapters
capacities. In addition, Dodge et al. (Chapter 12, this (e.g., Saarni et al., Chapter 5; and Thompson, Chapter
Handbook, this volume) discuss the role of childrens 2, this Handbook, this volume), several developmental
emerging regulatory abilities in age-related changes in trends in emotion-related regulation are evident (also
aggression; they also note the association of individual see Eisenberg & Morris, 2002; Thompson, 1994;
differences in childrens aggression with problems in Walden & Smith, 1997). First, with increasing age in
emotion regulation, attentional deficits, and impulsiv- early infancy and childhood, regulation of emotion
ity. Similarly, Eisenberg et al. (Chapter 11, this Hand- and behavior is shifted gradually from external
book, this volume) report findings consistent with the sources in the social world (e.g., socializers) to self-
view that regulatory processes are intimately involved initiated, internal (i.e., child-based) resources. Care-
in the vicariously induced emotions of sympathy and givers soothe young children, manage young childrens
personal distress, as well as in the performance of emotion by selecting the situations they are in, and
prosocial behavior. Further, Wigfield et al. (Chapter provide children with information (e.g., facial cues,
15, this Handbook, this volume) consider links narratives) to help the child interpret events (Thomp-
between motivation and self-regulation and how moti- son, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume). With age
vation is translated into regulated behavior. They and cognitive development, children are better able to
also discuss the importance of internally (versus manage emotion themselves. Second, mentalistic
externally) regulated motivation and behavior in the strategies for emotion regulation, such as thinking
10 Introduction

about situations in a positive light, cognitive avoid- Modes of Regulation


ance, and shifting and focusing attention, increase
with use in age. The use of such strategies is probably Conceptual issues or empirical data related to different
facilitated by the development of childrens under- types of regulation or control were not discussed much
standing of emotion, including the factors that elicit, in the 1998 Handbook (except in this introduction), and
maintain, and modulate emotion, as well as by other Rothbart and Bates note that this state of affairs has
cognitive advances and physical changes. Third, with changed. Numerous authors at least address implicitly
greater maturity, children develop greater capacity to or explicitly, several types of childrens regulatory/con-
modulate the course of their physiological and emo- trol capacities, including the regulation of attention,
tional arousal, for example, the intensity and duration physiology, or behavior, as well as the social context.
of arousal, an ability that would be expected to have For example, in the discussion of temperament and/or
dramatic effects on behavior (e.g., aggression, venting personality (e.g., Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6; Kagan &
of emotion, emotional expression). Fourth, with age, Fox, Chapter 4; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this
individuals likely become more adept at selecting, Handbook, this volume), authors review findings related
managing, and construing situations and relationships to the abilities to effortfully manage attention and to ef-
in a manner that minimizes the need to deal with neg- fortfully activate or inhibit behavior as needed when
ative emotions and stress (Carstensen, 1991; see necessary, especially when one is not inclined to do so.
Saarni et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). Developmental theorists frequently have highlighted
Fifth, the ability to match strategies with the nature of constructs such as inhibitory control, self-regulation,
stressors appears to improve with development. Thus, constraint, and ego control, which involve the ability to
children improve in the ability to select appropriate modulate the behavioral expression of impulses and
coping solutions for everyday problems. Moreover, feelings (e.g., Block & Block, 1980; Kopp, 1982;
children appear to become better at distinguishing be- Pulkkinen, 1982; Rothbart et al., 2001), and such abili-
tween stressors that can be controlled and those that ties are addressed in numerous chapters in discussion of
cannot, and at choosing the most effective strategies their relations to adjustment and social competence
for these stressors (e.g., emotion-management strate- (e.g., Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6; Dodge et al., Chapter
gies such as blunting or cognitive distraction in uncon- 12; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3; Saarni et al., Chapter
trollable contexts; see Saarni et al., Chapter 5, this 5, this Handbook, this volume).
Handbook, this volume). These developmental changes Another type of regulationmanaging or regulating
are likely to impact development in many aspects of the stressful situation that elicited the emotional
social, emotional, and academic functioning. arousalhas been discussed primarily by coping theo-
Neurological changes, especially in the prefrontal rists, who view problem-focused coping (efforts to
cortex and cingulate gyrus, likely account for some of modify the source of the problem) as an important type
the age-related changes in self-regulation and executive of coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). This type of reg-
attention (see Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3; Thompson, ulation generally includes planning and direct problem
Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume). A topic of re- solving or instrumental coping in response to the experi-
cent interest has been the continued growth and change ence of emotion. In addition, people often proactively
in multiple regions of the prefrontal cortex throughout manage situations to reduce exposure to stress and neg-
the course of adolescence, especially with respect to ative emotion in the future (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997;
processes of myelination and synaptic pruning ( both of Carstensen, 1991). An example is when socially anx-
which increase the efficiency of information process- ious individuals choose not to attend social events that
ing; see Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 14, this Hand- elicit discomfort. Unfortunately, few investigators have
book, this volume). These changes are believed to examined childrens efforts to proactively shape or se-
underlie improvements in executive functioning ( long- lect their experiences; this remains an important gap in
term planning, metacognition, self-evaluation, and the our knowledge.
coordination of affect and cognition; Keating, 2004), Appropriate regulation depends, in part, on the par-
which plays a central role in self-regulation. Research ticular context. Effective emotion-related regulation is
on the neurological bases of self-regulation is clearly an viewed as flexible and relevant to ones goals (Cole,
important emerging area of work. Michel, & Teti, 1994; Eisenberg & Morris, 2002). For
A Focus on Cognition 11

example, appropriate expression of emotion depends on well adjusted tend to be high in attentional and inhibitory
the situation, and a person skilled in regulation adjusts effortful control and moderate in impulsivity (Eisenberg
his or her behavior accordingly. Moreover, it is impor- et al., 2001, 2005).
tant to differentiate between regulation and how it is In summary, a recent theme in the developmental lit-
measured. If regulation is operationalized as control or erature has been on multidimensional, emotion-related
inhibition of behavior, particularly high levels are likely conceptions of regulation. This work is a natural accom-
to be maladaptive (Block & Block, 1980). For example, paniment to the current emphasis on emotion and tem-
some children appear to be highly inhibited tempera- perament, as well as the concern with adjustment,
mentally; these children are prone to fears, negative af- stress, and coping in the larger domain of psychology.
fect, avoidant behavior, and social withdrawal (see However, as is noted by Collins and Steinberg (Chapter
Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4; Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, 16, this Handbook, this volume), the issues of both emo-
this Handbook, this volume), and are more likely than tional development and self-regulation have attracted
other children to develop anxiety disorders in adulthood only tangential attention from adolescence researchers.
(Rosenbaum et al., 1993). We (and, to some degree, Thus, the role of various aspects of regulation and con-
Rothbart & Bates, 1998, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this trol in healthy and maladaptive adolescent development
volume) have argued that it is important to differentiate is a natural area for future investigation.
between control (i.e., inhibition) that is more effortfully
managed and that which is somewhat involuntary and,
hence, often rigid and extreme so that only the former A FOCUS ON COGNITION
should be labeled as self-regulation (e.g., Eisenberg &
Spinrad, 2004; Eisenberg et al., 2004). In reality, the de- Another trend in developmental psychology in recent
gree to which various control processes are effortful or years has been increased links between work on cogni-
not may usually vary on a continuum rather than cate- tion with theory and empirical research on emotion and
gorically. Moreover, this distinction may not be as use- social behavior (Parke, 2004). Cognition plays an obvi-
ful in regard to physiological regulation, although ous and fundamental role in most aspects of emotional
physiological processes related to emotion sometimes and social functioning. Saarni et al. (Chapter 5, this
can be modulated by effortful processes (e.g., focusing Handbook, this volume) review the early emergence of a
attention away from a distressing event, thought, or per- cognitive understanding of facial expressions and others
son) and vagal processes may be involved in effortful emotions (e.g., social referencing). They also provide
regulation (see Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this Hand- many examples of how cognitive advances in infancy and
book, this volume). early childhood are reflected in emotion-related capabil-
One important reason for differentiating between ities. For example, they note a number of competencies
more effortful and less voluntary aspects of control or the child needs to be emotionally competent, including
regulation is that they may be combined in various ways the following that involve social-cognitive skills: (a)
that seem to be associated with different types of behav- awareness of ones own emotional state; ( b) the ability
ior in children (Eisenberg & Morris, 2002). For example, to discern and understand others emotions; (c) skill in
early in elementary school, externalizing problems have using the vocabulary of emotion and expression terms
been linked to low levels of effortful attentional control commonly available in ones subculture and at more ma-
and inhibitory control, as well as with reactive undercon- ture levels skill in acquiring cultural scripts that link
trol (impulsivity). In contrast, younger children with in- emotion with social roles; (d) the capacity for empathy
ternalizing behavior problems (not comorbid with and sympathy (which involves some understanding of
externalizing) tend to be low in effortful attentional but others emotional states); (e) understanding that inner
not inhibitory control, and high on less voluntary over- emotional state need not correspond to outer expression
control (e.g., very low in impulsivity; Eisenberg et al., and that ones emotional-expressive behavior may impact
2001; also see Caspi, 2000; Caspi & Shiner, Chapter 6, on another; and (f ) awareness that the structure or nature
this Handbook, this volume). As children move through of relationships is in large part defined by how emotions
elementary school, however, internalizing problems may are communicated within the relationship. Related skills
no longer be linked to deficits in attentional control discussed by authors in this volume include the abilities
(Eisenberg et al., 2005). In contrast, children who are to comprehend and take into account unique information
12 Introduction

about others internal states (intentions, emotions, moti- The role of cognition in the motivation to succeed has
vations, cognitions), to analyze elements of a social con- been a topic of considerable discussion. Wigfield et al.
text and the consequences of various modes of action, (Chapter 15, this Handbook, this volume) organized their
and to devise appropriate cognitive strategies for sensi- review of theory and research on the motivation to suc-
tive social interaction in relationships, management of ceed around three broad questions: Can I do this task? Do
aggressive impulses, and altruistic behavior (Dodge I want to do this task and why? and What do I need to do
et al., Chapter 12; Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11; and to succeed on this task? It is obvious that cognition is cen-
Rubin et al., Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume). tral to assessing and dealing with all of these questions,
In addition, conceptions of the self are in large part although, of course, emotion also plays a critical role in
cognitive constructions, although they also are imbued achievement motivation. As an example of how cognitions
with emotion (Harter, Chapter 9, this Handbook, this affect the motivation to succeed, Wigfield et al. reviewed
volume). In fact, Harter argued that developmental literature concerning the ways in which childrens under-
achievements in understanding others behaviors and standing of competence-related constructs (e.g., ability,
cognitions (e.g., how others view the self ), as well as effort, task difficulty) affect motivation.
emotional processes, underlie age-related changes in Interest in cognitive processes as explanatory mecha-
self-conceptions. Similarly Thompson (Chapter 2, this nisms in socialization has changed markedly in recent
Handbook, this volume) argues that a host of cognitive years. In the past 2 decades, social learning theory ac-
skills underlie the early emergence of the self and the counts of socialization have become much more cogni-
understanding of self in relation to others. tive in orientation; in addition, cognitive constructs
In his chapter on moral development, Turiel (Chapter from the cognitive sciences and social psychology have
13, this Handbook, this volume) discusses a range of been assimilated into developmental conceptions of so-
ways in which cognitions are integral to moral thinking. cialization. In their chapter, Bugental and Grusec
For example, cognition obviously is critical for differen- (Chapter 7, this Handbook, this volume) argue that so-
tiating moral from nonmoral (e.g., conventional and per- cialization interactions are organized by the ways expe-
sonal) concerns, in constructing conceptions about riences are represented at a cognitive level. Cognitions
morality, in analyzing information about elements in a often mediate or moderate socialization processes, and
specific morally relevant situation, and in making cognitive processes involved in socialization may be de-
morally relevant decisions based on situational informa- liberate and reflective or relatively automatic. They also
tion and values, beliefs, and goals. As is evident from discuss the role of childrens acquired ways of cogni-
the passage from Turiels chapter quoted earlier, he ar- tively representing their social worlds (including con-
gues that cognition is central to moral development. ceptions of the self, family members, and peers) in
In his discussion of early socioemotional develop- mediating the effects of socialization on developmental
ment, Thompson (Chapter 2, this Handbook, this vol- outcomes, as well as how parental cognitionsfor ex-
ume) reviews some of the ways in which young ample, biases, attributionsaffect the quality of parent-
childrens working models of attachment figures and re- ing. In brief, socialization is achieved partly through
lationships are modified with the growth of understand- caregivers influence on the development of childrens
ing of psychological processes (e.g., work on the theory conceptions of relationships, and parents beliefs about
of mind). Individuals working models of relationships, children likely are influenced by their own working
which have a cognitive as well as an affective compo- models of relationships (also see Rubin et al., Chapter
nent, are expected to influence relationships not only in 10, this Handbook, this volume).
childhood (also see Rubin et al., Chapter 10, this Hand- Like Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this Handbook,
book, this volume), but later in life (Main, Kaplan, & this volume), Parke and Buriel (Chapter 8, this Handbook,
Cassidy, 1985; see Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16, this this volume) suggest that the role of cognition in social-
Handbook, this volume). In addition, Collins and Stein- ization is varied and multilevel: the role of cognition
berg discuss cognitive and sociocognitive changes comes in many guises, including the childs own cogni-
in adolescencefor example, in executive attention, tive capacities as a determinant of socialization strate-
decision-making processes, problem solving, abstract gies, as well as parents cognitions, beliefs, values and
reasoning, and perspective takingthat are relevant to goals concerning their parental role as constraints on
psychosocial changes during that period of life. their socialization practices. . . . Equally important is the
A Focus on Contextual and Environmental Inputs to Development 13

recognition of the importance of the ways in which par- a particular environment varies depending on their
ents perceive, organize and understand their childrens genotype or, conversely, when environmental experi-
behaviors and beliefs for appreciating how parent-child ences moderate gene expression).
relationships are regulated and change. Moreover, like Similarly, life-span psychologists also have heightened
Bugental and Grusec, Parke and Buriel view cognitions as our awareness of the interplay of historical, cultural, bio-
mediators of the relation between socialization or family logical, and psychological influences on behavior (Baltes,
experiences (parenting, marital conflict) and childrens Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 1998; Baltes, Reese, & Lip-
developmental outcomes. Parental cognitions, ideas, be- sitt, 1980). From a life-span perspective, changes in the
liefs, valuesalso are viewed as mediating the relation of individuals social context across the life span interact
family socioeconomic status to childrens development. with the individuals unique history of experiences, roles,
Finally, cognitive perspectives such as cognitive de- and biology to produce an individualized developmental
velopmental theory and schema-based models are im- pathway. Further, increased interest in individual differ-
portant in contemporary work on gender issues. Among ences in temperament, personality, and social function-
the most fundamental issues in the study of gender are ing sometimes has contributed to a focus on context as a
the role of cognition in gender-typed behavior, the de- possible explanation for these differences (e.g., Rothbart
velopment of an understanding of gender-relevant con- & Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume).
structs early in life, and the role of social factors in
childrens gender-relevant cognitions. The current focus Diversity
on cognition has contributed a conception of gender de-
velopment in which the childs conceptions play a sig- One manifestation of current interest in the context of
nificant role in his or her own development (see Ruble development is the recent emphasis in the discipline on
et al., Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume). recognizing and examining diversity (Parke, 2004). This
In brief, cognitive processes of many sorts (including trend is consistent with the life-span emphasis on indi-
those studied in neuropsychology) are being integrated vidual variation in developmental trajectories. An em-
into theory and research on diverse aspects of social and phasis on diversity can refer to a host of differences
emotional development. This trend, which has likely in- among people that are correlated with different life
creased in the past decade, has resulted in richer concep- experiences, including differences in sex, sexual orien-
tualizations of children and their social and emotional tation, and masculinity/femininity; in culture and
development, as well as of the socialization process. subcultural background/experiences; in socioeconomic
status and associated living conditions; and in the com-
position and structure of families.
A FOCUS ON CONTEXTUAL AND For years, many developmentalists have acknowl-
ENVIRONMENTAL INPUTS TO edged that research on differences among various
DEVELOPMENT groups (e.g., cultures or subcultures) is valuable in de-
lineating factors that influence diverse courses of devel-
Investigation of social and emotional development is be- opment. However, in the past decade or two, we have
coming more differentiated and sophisticated in its con- moved beyond solely identifying differences between
ception of the social context. This change in the field is groups on particular variables.
based, in part, on Bronfenbrenners (1979) early efforts Of particular importance, developmental scientists
to increase the fields awareness of the multiple levels of are acknowledging the value of studying differences in
the childs social ecology and the need to consider the processes of development in different groups. Often in
interaction between the larger social world (e.g., the the past, the implicit assumption has been that the causes
neighborhood and culture) and the family and individ- of development were similar or identical across groups
ual. The social environment provides affordances for the but that various groups differed in degree of exposure to
expression of individual characteristicsit is the niche various causal agents or in biological predispositions.
for biologically based characteristics to operate (or not). Thus, gender, ethnicity, and other group-level variables
And, as mentioned previously, the field is increasingly were considered unwanted error variance and were often
recognizing the importance of gene-environment inter- treated as control variablesnonpsychological and non-
actions (i.e., when the effect on a person of exposure to behavioral variables of little interest. Investigators are
14 Introduction

finding that contributors to development, and the config- ity groups), cultures that often conflict in particular val-
uration and operation of influential factors, sometimes ues and expectations. Similarly, the context of poverty
vary in different contexts and for different groups. a situation in which increasing numbers of families are
Examples were provided in the prior discussion of mod- finding themselvesis a topic of growing interest in the
eration effects. developmental community (see Parke, 2004; Parke &
Buriel, Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume).
Although developmental psychology has been influ-
Types and Examples of Contextual Inf luence
enced by cultural anthropology for a long time (e.g.,
The importance of the various types of contextual influ- Whiting & Whiting, 1975), interest in the role of culture
ences on social and emotional development is evident in in psychological development has increased in the past
many of the chapters in this volume. Consistent with the 15 years, particularly with regard to the study of emo-
past Handbook chapters on socialization, Parke and tion, the self, and moral development (e.g., Kitayama &
Buriel (Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume) review Markus, 1994; Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeir, 2002;
in some detail the relations of aspects of the proximal Turiel, Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume). As one
family context (e.g., parental socialization-related prac- example, Saarni et al. (Chapter 5, this Handbook, this
tices and cognitions) to social, personality, and emo- volume) proposed that culture plays a role in the con-
tional development. This ongoing interest in the role of struction of the meaning of events that can elicit emo-
the proximal family environment is also reflected in a tion (e.g., in emotion-relevant appraisals of events and
number of other chapters, such as those focused on others behaviors and reactions) and in rendering some
early social, self-related and moral development emotional responses more probable than others. Culture
(Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume), ag- also influences how members of a society regulate and
gression (Dodge et al., Chapter 12, this Handbook, this express emotion through a transactional process. Specif-
volume), prosocial development (Eisenberg et al., Chap- ically, culture determines what one notices in the feed-
ter 11, this Handbook, this volume), peer relationships back from the body; influences communication patterns
(Rubin et al., Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume), and, hence, socially induced affect; determines ones
achievement (Wigfield et al., Chapter 15, this Hand- role in society and, consequently, emotional experiences
book, this volume), and socioemotional development in that are associated with roles; and influences the selec-
adolescence (Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16, this tion and expression of emotional responses. This view of
Handbook, this volume). emotion differs somewhat from the common perspective
Parke and Buriels Chapter 8, Bugental and Grusecs that emotional expression and feeling are strongly
Chapter 7 (this Handbook, this volume) on socialization rooted primarily in biology and that many emotion-re-
processes, and, to some degree, a number of other chap- lated processes are universal.
ters include content pertaining to other aspects of con- Given the links among emotion, perceptions of the
text. These include family structure and organization self, and relationships (Harter, Chapter 9, this Hand-
(e.g., as assessed by parental employment status, marital book, this volume; Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook,
status, and number of parents in the home) and subcul- this volume), it is not surprising that contemporary theo-
tural and cultural factors. Although research on social- rists expect culture to play a role in the development of
ization in minority families and communities is still quite the self. Harter (Chapter 9, this Handbook, this volume)
limited in quantity, such work has been assigned new im- noted that the self is likely culturally saturated. Thus,
portance in the past 2 decades (see Parke & Buriel, Chap- the Western view of self may differ in important ways
ter 8, this Handbook, this volume). Developmental from that in cultures in which self-definition is deeply
scientists are increasingly acknowledging that the values, embedded in social relationships and obligations. This
socialization goals, and strategies in ethnic minority proposition is consistent with the contemporary argu-
families may differ in important ways from those in the ment that people in different cultures have different
majority culture. Moreover, there are unique issues and construals of the self due to cultural differences in con-
challenges with regard to socialization and development cepts of individuality (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In
in contexts where children must interact effectively in some cultures (e.g., many Asian cultures), the self is
two cultures (e.g., the cultures of the minority and major- viewed as interdependent and there likely is more em-
A Focus on Biological Perspectives 15

phasis on attending to others, fitting in, and harmony and status hierarchies is an important direction for re-
with others. In contrast, some have argued that in many search on moral development.
Western cultures, independence from others rather than The emphasis on different groups within a society
overt connectedness is valued. Although there may be serving as different socialization contexts is echoed in
more diversity within groups in regard to an emphasis recent work on the separate cultures of girls and boys
on individualism than sometimes is acknowledged (Maccoby, 1990). Segregation by sex in childhood
(Turiel, Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume) so seems to be a universal phenomenon, although it varies
that distinctions between individualistic and collectivis- to some degree with variables such as the availability of
tic cultures do not hold (Oyserman et al., 2002), it ap- same-sex peers and opportunities to choose ones asso-
pears that there is some variation across cultures in ciates (Ruble et al., Chapter 14, this Handbook, this vol-
normative self-conceptions. This variation probably is ume). Within sex-segregated groups, girls and boys
reflected in processes underlying the development of appear to develop different styles of interaction, goals,
self-perceptions early in life. For this reason and others and values, as well as different perceptions of the self
(e.g., the content of certain items may not be relevant or (Harter, Chapter 9; Ruble et al., Chapter 14, this Hand-
meaningful; see Harter, Chapter 9, this Handbook, this book, this volume). These subcultural differences likely
volume), measures of self-perceptions developed in the have substantial and long-term implications for social,
United States may not be appropriate for use in non- emotional, and personality development.
Western or nonindustrialized cultures. Also evident in this volume of the Handbook is the in-
Culture is an especially salient theme in Turiels creased recognition in recent decades of connections
Chapter 13 (this Handbook, this volume) on moral de- among contexts within a society, for example, among
velopment. Although coming from a predominantly family, school, and peer cultures (e.g., Collins & Stein-
cognitive perspective, Turiel notes the dynamic inter- berg, Chapter 16; Dodge et al., Chapter 13; Wigfield
play among various personal and social (including et al., Chapter 15; Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8; Rubin
cultural) goals in moral development. He also acknowl- et al., Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume). How-
edges that social reasoning is flexible and takes into ac- ever, these connections are seldom examined in empiri-
count different and varied aspects of the social world. cal study of development and in theory, or acknowledged
In discussing contrasting perspectives on cross-cultural in the real world (e.g., there often is little communica-
findings, Turiel makes the point that differences in as- tion between schools and parents). Culture doubtlessly
sumptions about reality (e.g., assumptions about prac- has important effects on the nature of the connections
tices that are harmful to the dead) and in informational across settings within a culture; for example, the links
assumptions (e.g., regarding the expected effects of between parents and schools may be stronger in majority
physical punishment on children) are important to con- culture families than in some minority groups (espe-
sider when interpreting cultural differences in moral cially in some neighborhoods) who feel little connection
and social conventional reasoning. As is evident in to the majority culture. However, research on the role of
Turiels chapter, there is disagreement in the field in re- culture in the forging (or inhibiting) of connections
gard to the interpretation of some cross-cultural differ- across settings within cultures would enrich an ecologi-
ences in reasoning about moral and social conventional cal perspective of development.
issues, with Turiel viewing moral development as being
more similar across cultures than do most cultural psy-
chologists (e.g., Shweder, Mahapatra, & Miller, 1987). A FOCUS ON BIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES
However, Turiel emphasizes another aspect of context
more than do most cultural psychologists; he argues There can be little doubt that there has been a resurgence
that a focus on contextual variations between cultures of interest in individual differences, and especially the
has led to little consideration of variations in moral rea- biological and constitutional bases of individual differ-
soning associated with contextual differences within ences. Plomin (1994) noted that 78% of the text pages in
cultures. Turiel and his colleagues work (e.g., Wainryb the 1983 Handbook of Child Psychology were devoted
& Turiel, 1995) on the diversity of perspectives within predominantly (more than half the page) to normative
cultures stemming from factors such as gender roles or group difference approaches. In contrast, individual
16 Introduction

differences were a major focus of attention in the 1998 portance of genes and environment. Increased under-
edition of the Handbook and in this edition. Indeed, in standing of these issues has led to a more balanced and
three of the chapters in Volume 3, constitutionally based complex view of the role of nature and nurture in so-
individual differences are the primary focus (i.e., Caspi cioemotional development.
& Shiner, Chapter 6; Kagan & Fox, Chaper 4; Rothbart Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook, this vol-
& Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume). ume) suggest that the use of molecular genetic tech-
The current focus on constitutionally based individ- niques is contributing to the trend to replace the
ual differences is not unprecedented. After a period of nature-nurture conjunction versus with the more ap-
heavy reliance on biological explanations of social be- propriate conjunction and. Other factors that have
havior earlier in this century, biological perspectives likely contributed to movement in this regard include so-
appeared to go out of fashion in developmental and so- phisticated discussions of interpretational and statistical
cial psychology. Behaviorism and then social learning issues (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Collins, Mac-
perspectives became more popular during the middle coby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000; Rut-
half of the century, whereas biologically based expla- ter & Silberg, 2002; Turkheimer & Gottesman, 1996) in
nations of social behavior and personality were de- premier publication outlets, as well as studies demon-
emphasized. In the past 25 years, the pendulum has strating the complexity of relations between environ-
swung back once more. mental and genetic or biological factors (see Caspi &
Noticeable differences in the field between now and Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume, and the
when the 1998 edition of the Handbook was published discussion of moderated relations later).
are in regard to the degree of acceptance of genetics and
biological factors as major contributors to socioemo-
Biology, Temperament, and Personality
tional development and, of equal importance, in the
greater understanding that genetic contributions to de- As noted previously, Rothbart and Bates (Chapter 3), as
velopment do not preclude environmental contributions. well as Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Handbook, this
The field is more cognizant that genetic factors usually volume), focus primarily on issues related to tempera-
are moderated or mediated by the environment, includ- ment. It is often assumed that behaviors with a tempera-
ing the social environment, and are not independent of mental basis are inherited, but current definitions of
environmental inputs to development. Thus, as was noted temperament are more complex. Rothbart and Bates
by Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook, this vol- (Chapter 3) define temperament as constitutionally
ume), in the past decade the pitting of nature versus nur- based individual difference in reactivity and self-
ture has increasingly come to a halt. regulation, in the domains of affect, activity, and atten-
For the most part, developmental scientists now ac- tion. . . . By the term constitutional, we refer to the
cept that both genetic and other biological factors affect biological bases of temperament, influenced over time
development, and that even behaviors with a strong by heredity, maturation, and experience. Thus, tem-
hereditary basis can be strongly affected by the environ- perament is influenced not only by heredity, but by envi-
ment in which the organism develops. Most also are ronmental factors that affect an individuals biological
aware of the relevant caveats in interpreting the results being (e.g., trauma or drugs) and by the social context.
of behavioral genetics studies, two of which are aptly Similarly, for Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this Hand-
spelled out by Dodge et al. (Chapter 12, this Handbook, book, this volume), temperament refers to a biologi-
this volume): it is understood that genetic effects may cally based bias for correlated clusters of feelings,
be mediated environmentally through gene-environment thoughts, and actions that appear during childhood, but
transactions in which genes influence surrounding envi- not always in the opening months, and are sculpted by
ronments, which, in turn, influence phenotypic expres- varied rearing environments into a large but still limited
sion. . . . In behavior genetics studies, the effects of such number of traits that comprise an individuals personal-
transactions are included in the heritability estimates ity profile. Thus, they emphasize the role of both bio-
and not counted as environmental effects. Second, all es- logical and environmental factors in childrens early
timates are context specific. That is, the influence of dispositional characteristics. Similarly, Caspi and
genes on behavior varies across social contexts, and a Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume) argue
change in the social context may change the relative im- that personality, viewed as social and cognitive elabora-
A Focus on Biological Perspectives 17

tions on temperament, is moderately influenced by both constructs that inform us about aspects of temperament
heredity and environment (especially unshared environ- that relate to them.
mental factors that are not shared by twins or siblings). After concisely summarizing the behavioral genetics
Thus, there is consensus among these authors on the im- literature, Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, this Handbook,
portance of both hereditary and the environmental in- this volume) highlight recent work on molecular genetics
fluences on temperament and personality. and personality. They note that individual genes have not
Developmental scientists increasingly are using phys- consistently mapped onto personality, but that this may
iological /neurological measures of dispositional charac- be due (among other reasons) to personality being pre-
teristics, situational reactivity, regulation, approach/ dicted by a combination of genes and gene X environ-
avoidance tendencies, and various types of cognitive, at- ment interactions. Caspi and Shiner believe that
tentional, or emotional processing related to tempera- developmental psychologists can contribute to this line
ment. For example, Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this of work by helping to refine the measurement of psycho-
Handbook, this volume) summarize research on cerebral logical phenotypes for inclusion in genetic research and
asymmetry and its association with behavioral inhibi- helping to measure developmental contexts and environ-
tion, other physiological correlates of inhibited and un- mental risks that may interact with genetic factors to
inhibited behavior, and the relation of neurochemical shape personality (or temperament) and its development.
systems in the brain to mood and action. They further
note that people with different temperaments will not
Biology and Socioemotional Development
react in the same way, behaviorally or biologically, to a
given experience. Accordingly, they suggest that we in- As is reflected in the chapters in this volume, much con-
vent constructs that capture this factthat we replace temporary work on the biological bases of socioemotional
the current constructs, which describe children and their development is based on complex frameworks that posit
environments (parents, sibling, school settings) sepa- interconnected causal roles of biological/constitutional
rately, with single synthetic constructs that represent a and environmental factors in human functioning. For ex-
particular temperamental type growing up in a particu- ample, Bugental and Grusec (Chapter 7, this Handbook,
lar set of contexts. . . . As environments shape children this volume) depict development as the result of a dynamic
of varied temperaments into different phenotypes, it co-regulation of aspects of the individual (from neural to
will be useful to invent new concepts, rather than rely behavioral) and the environment (from physical to social).
on the language of ANOVA that describes interactions The emergence of structure in both people and their envi-
between the temperamental type of child and a rearing ronments results from a process of mutual influence and
environment. regulation. They present literature consistent with the
Rothbart and Bates (Chapter 3, this Handbook, this view that children are biologically prepared for socializa-
volume) discuss associations of the major dimensions of tion, and argue that biologically based differences in chil-
temperamental reactivity (approach, fear, anger, etc.) dren (e.g., in temperament, physical attractiveness) elicit
and self-regulation (e.g., attention) with the neural sys- different socialization experiences from the environment.
tems identified as underlying these dimensions. They Similarly, biological factors that affect parenting are dis-
discuss neurological processes involved in effortful con- cussed, with a recognition that biologically influenced
trol, defined as the efficiency of executive attention, parental characteristics are played out in a social context.
including the ability to inhibit a dominant response They further argue, using an evolutionary perspective,
and/or to activate a subdominant response, to plan, and that humans may be designed for preferential receptivity
to detect errors. Rothbart and Bates also review re- to proximity-maintenance with specific others in the pres-
search on the behavioral and emotional correlates of au- ence of distress (e.g., attachments), for the use and recog-
tonomic reactivity (e.g., heart rate, vagal tone, skin nition of signals denoting power or dominance, for
conductance) and cortisol responding, as well as hemi- differentiating between in-groups and out-groups in social
spheric asymmetry, as a way of further examining the life, and for the reciprocal obligations associated with
biological bases of temperament and their expression in communal life. These biological predispositions are
behavior. These measures, which tend to be related to viewed as emerging in a social context in which cultural
some indices of temperament, are sometimes used as factors, as well as situational cognitive and emotional fac-
proxies for temperament, and sometimes as separate tors, act as mediators and moderators of their effects on
18 Introduction

the socialization process and its outcomes. Thus, Bugental also review a large body of work linking environment
and Grusec view socialization in a complex process- factors such as family and peer factors to aggression
oriented manner, influenced by the ongoing interaction of and, more importantly in regard to causal conclusions,
biological and environmental factors. research indicating that prevention /intervention pro-
As is evident in Ruble et al.s chapter (Chapter 14, grams can reduce the incidence of antisocial behavior.
this Handbook, this volume), biological approaches play Experimental interventions that involve random assign-
an increasingly visible role in the study of gender- ment are perhaps the best way to demonstrate that envi-
related development. They discuss possible evolution- ronmental factors contribute to antisocial tendencies in
ary contributions to sex differences, as well as more youth, despite the strong role of heredity. An issue that
proximal biological correlates or contributorssuch as merits attention is how partly hereditary factors such as
hormones and brain structureto gender-related devel- temperament influence the effectiveness of interven-
opment and behavior. They conclude that prenatal an- tions in deterring antisocial behavior.
drogens and hormones during early development appear Dodge et al. (Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume)
to affect gender-related self-perceptions, preferences, also discuss evidence for gene by environment interac-
or behaviors, and that the effects of sex hormones in tions. For example, they highlight research demonstrat-
adolescence are evident, but probably more modest. ing that children who are genetically predisposed to
They further note that hormones have different effects antisocial tendencies are especially likely to manifest
on different characteristics; for example, prenatal an- them if they grow up in a risky social environment, such
drogen seems to have a large effect on some aspects of as one in which they are victims of maltreatment. Dodge
functioning (activities and interests), a modest effect et al. conclude that some of the most important discover-
on others (e.g., sexual orientation), and a small effect ies in the next decade will come from studies of gene-
on others (gender identity). In addition, they conclude environment interactions, as well as from experimental
that gender socialization processes at home, at school, prevention /intervention studies.
in interaction with peers, and through the media all Eisenberg et al. (Chapter 11, this Handbook, this vol-
contribute to gender differentiation in concepts, prefer- ume) view heredity as contributing to both the develop-
ences, behaviors, and/or values, although relations ment of prosocial and empathy-related responding in the
often are found under some conditions. They conclude species and to individual differences in aspects of emo-
that biological and environmental factors interact in tionality and regulation (e.g., attentional regulation) that
complex ways: There is increasingly sophisticated un- contribute to prosocial behavior and empathy in child-
derstanding of biological effects, and recognition that hood. Similar to Dodge et al. (Chapter 12, this Hand-
they are not immutable. Genes are activated or sup- book, this volume), they review studies demonstrating
pressed by environmental factors. Hormones and brain links between environmental factors and prosocial devel-
functioning are almost certainly influenced by the dif- opment, as well as experimental prevention studies
ferent environments in which girls and boys are raised, demonstrating that childrens prosocial tendencies can
by their different toy and activity choices, and by joint be modified by environmental interventions. However, it
effects of biology and the social environment. is likely that the effects of heredity are not as strong for
In their chapter on aggression, Dodge et al. (Chapter prosocial as for antisocial behavior (e.g., Krueger, Hicks,
12, this Handbook, this volume) conclude that there is & McGue, 2001), although heredity does contribute to
indisputable evidence of the role of heredity in aggres- empathy/sympathy (see Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11, this
sion (also see Rhee & Waldman, 2002), especially for Handbook, this volume). For example, in a study of step-
those who develop aggressive tendencies early and are families, Deater-Deckard et al. (2001) found that most of
stable in their aggression into adulthood (also see Caspi the variance in adults reports of childrens prosocial be-
& Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). They havior was due to environmental rather than hereditary
further conclude that the contribution of shared or com- factors, especially aspects of the environment that were
mon environment is small whereas that of childrens not shared by the children (although there was signifi-
nonshared environment is moderate. Thus, person- cant variance for shared environmental effects). Unfor-
specific experiences of individuals in families appear tunately, there has been little research conducted as yet
to be an important environmental factor contributing to on the prediction of prosocial tendencies from the inter-
aggression and other antisocial behavior. Dodge et al. action between heredity and the environment.
A Focus on Relationships 19

A FOCUS ON RELATIONSHIPS the quality of childrens subsequent relationships be-


cause of the assumptions and expectations about rela-
As noted by Rubin et al. (Chapter 10, this Handbook, this tionships that are inherent in internal working models
volume), interest in relationships other than the (Bretherton & Waters, 1985; Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8;
parent-child relationship has grown tremendously in re- Rubin et al., Chapter 10; Thompson, Chapter 2, this
cent decades. In addition, researchers studying the family Handbook, this volume). One way early attachment rela-
increasingly have examined not just the parent-child tionships may affect other relationships is through their
dyad, but also the larger family unit, associations be- influence on the developing sense of self in the infant as
tween the quality of parent-parent and parent-child rela- lovable or unworthy of love (Bretherton, 1991; Harter,
tionships, and links between the quality of familial Chapter 9, this Handbook, this volume).
interactions and quality of sibling and peer relationships Clearly, the topic of attachment and early parent-child
(Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume). relationships is still a central issue in the study of rela-
For example, there is now evidence that marital discord is tionships in developmental psychology. Attachments are
related to problems with childrens adjustment, social hypothesized to affect the development of the self, a
withdrawal, and low social competence, all of which range of cognitions relevant to quality of relationships,
compromise the quality of peer relationships. Moreover, emotion regulation and emotions attached to various rela-
investigators have begun to study the role of social rela- tionships, sympathetic and prosocial behavior with oth-
tionships outside the family (e.g., as reflected in social ers, social competence with peers, and personality
support) for quality of interaction within the family (see development (see Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16; Eisen-
Parke & Buriel; Rubin et al., this Handbook, this volume) berg et al., Chapter 3; Harter, Chapter 9; Rubin et al.,
and for the provision of social opportunities for children Chapter 10; Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this
(e.g., adult social networks as a source of potential peer volume). However, most researchers no longer believe that
contacts for children). In addition, a relatively new em- working models consolidate in early childhood with little
phasis in the literature in recent years has been the im- or no further modification. This broader conception of
pact of settings such as schools, workplaces, volunteer working models is reflected in Thompsons discussion of
activities, leisure pursuits, and neighborhoods on devel- some of the questions that require attention in the future,
opmentally significant interpersonal experiences, espe- including the following: (a) to what extent is security of
cially in adolescence (Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16, attachment definitive of the parent-child relationship?
this Handbook, this volume). Are there important features of this relationship that are
As is discussed in some of the chapters, it has been outside the scope of attachment? (b) How is it that attach-
suggested that the oft-cited causal relation of emotion- ment security becomes increasingly an attribute of the
related capacities with quality of relationships can be person, rather than of a specific relationship with matu-
reversed (or more likely, can be bi-directional)for ex- rity? Is it possible that both relationship-specific and per-
ample, that early attachment relationships play a role in son-specific features of attachment security coexist
the development of emotion regulation and reflect within the attachment-related representational systems
strategies for regulating emotion in interpersonal con- that exist in adulthood? (c) How are multiple attachment
texts (Saarni et al., Chapter 5; Thompson, Chapter 2, relationships developmentally influential? How do the ex-
this Handbook, this volume). The securely attached in- pectations arising from multiple attachments become in-
fant whose parent is consistently and appropriately re- tegrated into coherent ways of relating to others,
sponsive to the infants distress signals is believed to representing relationships, and self-understanding? and
learn that it is acceptable to express distress and to ac- (d) why should attachment security be related to other
tively seek the assistance of others for comfort when features of psychological development? Some of these
upset. In contrast, avoidant infants, due in part to their questions are related to issues raised by Harter (1998,
parents nonresponsiveness to their distress signals, may Chapter 9, this Handbook, this volume) and merit addi-
learn to inhibit emotional expressiveness as well as tional attention in the next decade.
other-directed self-regulatory strategies (e.g., contact- Close peer relationships such as friendships have been
seeking and maintaining behaviors; Cassidy, 1994). increasingly examined by developmental scientists in the
The internal working model developed in the context past 2 decades and have been viewed as a source of sup-
of early attachment relationships is believed to affect port; a factor affecting self-perceptions; a context for
20 Introduction

learning about emotions, conflict, social negotiation, and on certain topics. Work on aggression, regulation, cop-
caring behavior; an impetus for cognitive, social, and ing, and social competence is burgeoning, for example,
emotional development; and an influence on the develop- no doubt in part because of concern in society about
ment of antisocial behavior and substance abuse (Collins childrens psychological health, violence, and related
& Steinberg, Chapter 16; Dodge et al., Chapter 12; Har- social issues (e.g., see Caspi et al., Chapter 6; Collins &
ter, Chapter 9; Rubin et al., Chapter 10, this Handbook, Steinberg, Chapter 16; Dodge et al., Chapter 12; Eisen-
this volume). However, a type of close peer relationship berg et al., Chapter 11; Rubin et al., Chapter 10; Saarni
that has received relatively little attention until the last et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). In the
decade is romantic relationships. Collins and Steinberg past decade or two, the focus on developmental psycho-
conclude that relationships with peers are a primary con- pathology, in particular, has increased in the writings of
text for the transmission and realization of expectations developmental scientists. Moreover, investigators in-
about romantic relationships. According to the research creasingly have been turning their attention to develop-
they review, the quality of romantic relationships, includ- ment in stressful contexts such as families in poverty,
ing aggression within them and feelings of rejection, is one-parent families, and families of divorce (Parke &
correlated with the quality of other relationships with Buriel, Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume). Con-
peers and family members: Youth with healthy familial cern with clinical issues and prevention is not without
and peer (e.g., friendship or peer group) relationships precedent, of course; much of the early work in child
tend to have more positive romantic relationships. They development grew out of a desire to understand the ori-
also note that the developmental outcomes of romantic re- gins of typical childhood problems.
lationships can be positive or negative, depending partly In addition, work on topics that have been a focus of
on the quality of these relationships. Consequently, the interest for a long time is increasingly being conducted
effects of early family and peer relationships on subse- outside the laboratory in real-world contexts so that
quent adjustment and well-being in adulthood (and in findings have direct applicability to prevention, clinical,
adult relationships) likely are partly mediated through and policy issues. For example, developmental scientists
experiences in adolescent romantic relationships. More- are becoming involved in the process of obtaining
over, partner relationships in late adolescence and early knowledge that can be used to design programs that
adulthood seem to play an important role in determining lessen the probability of negative effects from exposure
an individuals trajectory in antisocial behavior (i.e., its to stressors (e.g., divorce, poverty) or that promote
cessation or continuation) in early adulthood (Caspi & prosocial behavior or inhibit aggressive tendencies in
Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). Thus, the school settings (Dodge et al., Chapter 12; Eisenberg
topic of romantic relationships would seem to be an im- et al., Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume). More-
portant one for further study, especially as it relates to over, developmental scientists are deeply involved in
success in adult development. evaluating programs such as day care that have implica-
tions for both families and policy (see Volume 4). It is
likely that the increasing trend for developmental scien-
A FOCUS ON APPLICATION tists to apply their theory and methods to real-life issues
in real-world contexts will continue into the next decade
Another trend in the developmental research in the past and well into the 21st century.
2 decades has been renewed interest in application and
real-world problems. This emphasis in the discipline is
reflected in the fact that for the first time, one volume of SUMMARY
the 1998 Handbook (Volume 4) was devoted to applied
issues, and that volume is also part of this edition of the In general, the chapters in this volume highlight the
Handbook. Although much of the applied work on so- emerging themes, constructs, and methods in the field,
cioemotional development is discussed in that volume, and a recent permeability in the intellectual boundaries
the contemporary concern with application is also re- of the field. Many of the changes in the study of social
flected to some degree in this volume. and emotional development in the past 2 decades can be
This concern can be seen in both the topics of study characterized by increasing integration and differentia-
and the ways in which people are conducting research tion. In this context, I am using the term integration to
References 21

mean the assimilation (usually with some accommoda- REFERENCES


tion) into the study of socioemotional functioning of
ideas and methods from diverse approaches and topics in Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development,
developmental psychology, other subdisciplines of psy- and the big five. In C. F. Halverson Jr., G. A. Kohnstamm, &
R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and
chology, and even other disciplines such as sociology, personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 189207). Hillsdale,
genetics, and anthropology. The integration of novel NJ: Erlbaum.
methods, constructs, and theoretical perspectives has Aksan, N., & Kochanska, G. (2005). Conscience in childhood: Old
broadened not only our understanding of social and questions, new answers. Developmental Psychology, 41, 506516.
emotional development, but also the entire framework Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral de-
velopment: Integrating social information processing and moral
on which we design and interpret research findings. domain models. Child Development, 75, 9851002.
Dif ferentiation within the field of socioemotional de- Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: Self-regulation
velopment may be viewed in terms of contexts, con- and proactive coping. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417436.
structs, and causal inferences. As noted previously, the Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-
burgeoning interest in context in developmental psychol- span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon (Editor-
ogy is reflected in the study of many levels of influence, in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed), Handbook of child psychology
(5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 10291143). New York: Wiley.
including diversity in culture and subculture, race and
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life-span devel-
ethnicity, biological sex and gender, types of families opmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 65110.
and groups, and genetic and constitutional influences. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator vari-
In regard to constructs, our thinking is becoming less able distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual,
global and more conditional, multifaceted, and complex. strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.
Similarly, proposed causal influences of various social
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-
processes are becoming more multifaceted. Mediated resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),
and moderated relations, as well as those based on dy- Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 13. Development
namic systems perspectives, are more central in theory of cognition, af fect, and social relations (pp. 39101). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
and research.
Brenner, E. M., & Salovey, P. (1997). Emotion regulation during
Moreover (and related), researchers are increasingly
childhood: Developmental, interpersonal, and individual consid-
acknowledging and examining the multiplicative and co- erations. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Teaching in the heart
varying contributions of various types of environmental of the classroom: Emotional development, emotional literacy, and
and biologically based influences on socioemotional emotional intelligence (pp. 168192). New York: Basic Books.

functioning. Increasingly children are being viewed as Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bottles: The social
self as internal working model. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
producers of their environment as well as the products (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Development: Vol. 23. Self-
of socialization; parents and children are viewed as co- processes and development (pp. 141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
regulators of each others behaviors and affective states; Bretherton, I., & Waters, E. (1985). Growing points of attachment
and development is characterized as a consequence of so- theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209).
cial interactions that are shaped by contextual factors and
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
characteristics of all participants in the interaction. Al- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
though interactional and reciprocal causal models are not Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptu-
new, they are becoming a part of our everyday thinking alized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psy-
about psychological phenomena. As one might expect, im- chological Review, 101, 568586.
plementation of complex interactive models into research Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stew-
art, M. A. (1995). Genetic-environmental interaction in the gene-
designs lags behind conceptual models. However, analytic
sis of aggressivity and conduct disorders. Archives of General
methods for exploring reciprocal, additive, and interac- Psychiatry, 52, 916924.
tive causal influences, as well as analyses for examining Campos, J. (1984). A new perspective on emotions. Child Abuse and
nonlinear relations and growth curves, are becoming Neglect, 8, 147156.
more common, so developmental scientists are increas- Carstensen, L. L. (1991). Selectivity theory: Social activity in life-
ingly able to test complex conceptions of development span context. In K. W. Schaie (Ed.), Annual review of gerontology
and geriatrics (Vol. 11, pp. 195217). New York: Springer.
empirically. The next decade, like the past one, will un-
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing
doubtedly be an exciting time for the study of social, coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Per-
emotional, and personality development. sonality and Social Psychology, 56, 267283.
22 Introduction

Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related regulation:
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand- Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75, 334339.
book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland,
development (pp. 311388). New York: Wiley. A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control
Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continu- and impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child De-
ities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and So- velopment, 75, 2546.
cial Psychology, 78, 158172. Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In W.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., & Craig, I. W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
(2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
children. Science, 297, 851854. opment (5th ed., pp. 553617). New York: Wiley.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Inf luences of attachment re- Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
lationships [Special issue]. Monographs of the Society for Re- Carmichaels manual of child development (Vol. 2, pp. 261359).
search in Child Development, 59(Serial No. 240), 228249. New York: Wiley.
Cole, P. M., Michel, M. K., & Teti, L. O. (1994). The development of Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empa-
emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. thy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, behavior (pp. 281313). New York: Academic Press.
59(2/3, Serial No. 240), 73100. Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & internalization. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup
Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A sociocultural
The case nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232. perspective (pp. 236274). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of versity Press.
social information-processing mechanisms in childrens social Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implica-
adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101. tions for caring and justice. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Deater-Deckard, K., Dunn, J., OConnor, T. G., Davies, L. Golding,
J., & the ALSPAC Study Team. (2001). Using the stepfamily ge- Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.
netic design to examine gene-environmental processes in child and Keating, D. (2004). Cognitive and brain development. In R. Lerner &
family functioning. Marriage and Family Review, 33, 131156. L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed.,
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to suc- pp. 4584). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
ceed. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) and N. Eisenberg (Vol. ed.), Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (Eds.). (1994). Emotion and culture:
Handbook of child psychology (5th ed. Vol. 3, pp. 10171095). Empirical studies of mutual inf luence. Washington, DC: American
New York: Wiley. Psychological Association.
Eisenberg, N. (2002). Emotion-related regulation and its relation to Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development in children with dif-
quality of social functioning. In W. W. Hartup & R. A. Weinberg ferent attachment histories: The first three years. Child Develop-
(Eds.), Minnesota Symposia of Child Psychology: Vol. 32. Child ment, 72, 474490.
psychology in retrospect and prospect (pp. 133171). Hillsdale, Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Knaack, A., & Rhines, H. (2004). Mater-
NJ: Erlbaum. nal parenting and childrens conscience: Early security as a mod-
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, erator. Child Development, 75, 12291242.
S. A., Reiser, M., et al. (2001). The relations of regulation and Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998).
emotionality to childrens externalizing and internalizing prob- Individual differences in emotionality in infancy. Child Develop-
lem behavior. Child Development, 72, 11121134. ment, 64, 375390.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization, Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development: Vol. 2. The psy-
assessment, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motivation and chology of moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Emotion, 14, 131149. Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dis- perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199214.
positional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and anti-
quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social social behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality cor-
Psychology, 78, 136157. relates, distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 397402.
Eisenberg, N., Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (1996). Gender develop- Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1987). Affect intensity as an individual
ment and gender differences. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee difference characteristic: A review. Journal of Research in Per-
(Eds.), The handbook of educational psychology (pp. 358396). sonality, 21, 139.
New York: Macmillan. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Childrens emotion-related New York: Springer.
regulation. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and Lewis, M. D. (2002). Interacting time scales in personality (and cog-
behavior (Vol. 30, pp. 190229). Amsterdam: Academic Press. nitive) development: Intentions, emotions, and emergent forms. In
Eisenberg, N., Sadovsky, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Losoya, J. Parziale & N. Granott (Eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition
S. H., Valiente, C., et al. (2005). The relations of problem behav- processes in development and learning (pp. 183212). New York:
ior status to childrens negative emotionality, effortful control, Cambridge University Press.
and impulsivity: Concurrent relations and prediction of change. Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental
Developmental Psychology, 41, 193211. account. American Psychologist, 45, 513520.
References 23

Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual
of the family: Parent-child interaction. In P. H. Mussen (Series differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown
Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychol- (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3786).
ogy: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ed., pp. 1101). New York: Wiley.
Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. Keele (Eds.), Devel-
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and oping individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael
other intervening variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 83104. Posner (pp. 167188). Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, cal Association.
childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Rutter, M., & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in rela-
In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Monographs of the Society for tion to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of
Research in Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209), 66104. Psychology, 53, 463490.
Markus, H. Z., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). A motivational
tions for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Re- analysis of self-determination and self-regulation in education. In
view, 98, 224253. C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education
McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? (Vol. 2, pp. 1351). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Journal of Personality, 63, 365396. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-
individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assump- being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878.
tions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 372. Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and
Parke, R. D. (1994). Progress, paradigms, and unresolved problems: moral development. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence
Recent advances in our understanding of childrens emotions. of morality in young children (pp. 183). Chicago: University of
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 157169. Chicago Press.
Parke, R. D. (2004). The society for research in child development at Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Harris, E., Hanish, L., Fabes, R. A.,
70: Progress and promise. Child Development, 75, 124. Kupanoff, K., et al. (2004). The relation of childrens everyday
Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (1998). Socialization in the family: Ethnic nonsocial peer play to their emotion, regulation, and social func-
and ecological perspectives. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. tioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 6780.
Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotional regulation: A theme in search of
emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 463552). definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
New York: Wiley. opment, 59(Serial No. 240), 2552.
Plomin, R. (1994). Nature, nurture, and social development. Social Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (Edi-
Development, 3, 3753. tor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
Pulkkinen, L. (1982). Self-control and continuity from childhood to chology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
late adolescence. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life- (pp. 863932). New York: Wiley.
span development and behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 63105). New York: Turkheimer, E., & Gottesman, I. I. (1996). Simulating the dynamics
Academic Press. of genes and environment in development. Development and
Rest, J. R. (1983). Morality. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & J. H. Psychopathology, 8, 667677.
Flavell & E. M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychol- Wainryb, C., & Turiel, E. (1995). Diversity in social development:
ogy: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (4th ed., pp. 556629). New Between or within cultures. In M. Killen & D. Hart (Eds.), Moral-
York: Wiley. ity in everyday life: Developmental perspectives (pp. 283313).
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental in- Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
f luences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and Walden, T. A., & Smith, M. C. (1997). Emotion regulation. Motiva-
adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490529. tion and Emotion, 21, 725.
Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Bolduc-Murphy, E. A., Faraone, Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of six cultures:
S. V., Chaloff, J., Hirshfeld, D. R., et al. (1993). Behavioral inhi- A psychocultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
bition in childhood: A risk factor for anxiety disorders. Harvard Press.
Review of Psychiatry, 1, 216. Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: Free Press.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K., & Fisher, P. (2001). In-
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The origins of guilt. In R.
vestigations of temperament at 3 to 7 years: The childrens behav-
Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 37. So-
ior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.
cioemotional development (pp. 183258). Lincoln: University of
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Tempera- Nebraska Press.
ment and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Robinson, J. (1995). Empathy and guilt: Early
terly, 40, 2139.
origins of feelings of responsibility. In J. Tangney & K. Fischer
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions (pp. 143173). New York: Guil-
(Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child ford Press.
psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, personality development
(pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.
CHAPTER 2

The Development of the Person: Social


Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self
ROSS A. THOMPSON

SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING 26 Early Attachment and Subsequent Psychological


Early Social Discriminations and Expectations 27 Development 60
Intentions and Inferring Intentionality 30 Internal Working Models 65
Social Referencing 33 Summary 69
Understanding Social Events 35 CONSCIENCE 70
Feelings and Desires 36 Intuitive Morality of Early Childhood 70
Understanding Psychological Characteristics and Moral Emotion 71
Social Roles 39 Relational Inf luences 73
Summary 41 Summary 76
RELATIONSHIPS 42 SELF 77
Attachment and its Development 43 Developmentally Emergent Dimensions of Self 77
Differences in Attachment Security 45 Self-Regulation 80
Security of Attachment in the Strange Situation 47 Development of Autobiographical Memory 81
Other Behavioral Assessments of Attachment Security 50 Summary 84
Origins of Attachment Security 52 CONCLUSION 84
Consistency and Change in the Security of REFERENCES 85
Attachment 57

What constitutes the development of a person? In moral This chapter is concerned with early sociopersonal-
philosophy, personhood is not inherent in human exis- ity development. Because other chapters of this Hand-
tence but rather is contingent on the achievement of self- book are devoted to temperamental individuality, the
awareness, moral autonomy, and other constituents of development of emotion, peer relationships, and other
distinctly human capability. Developmental scientists processes related to personality, the goal is not to com-
offer a more nuanced answer to this question, describing prehensively describe the emergence of early personal-
how the development of personhood emerges in a contin- ity or to identify individual characteristics that
uous relational context in which infants and young chil- foreshadow adult personality traits. Instead, and consis-
dren develop their earliest understandings of who they tent with a developmental perspective, the goal is to de-
are, who others are, and how to relate to other people. scribe how central facets of social and personality

Every author of a Handbook chapter should have such an op- They have contributed to the ideas considered here, and the
portunity to write a revisionto try to portray the field more chapter is dedicated to them. My deepest appreciation also to
accurately, to correct mistakes and misinterpretations, and to Nancy Eisenberg, whose patience and good heart made it eas-
see how far the field can advance in a few years. In the previ- ier to complete this project during a period of personal chal-
ous edition, I gratefully thanked many colleagues who were lenge. Although I have sought to identify major contributors to
willing to contribute to my meandering ponderings about the each of the topics reviewed here, the length limitations prohib-
issues of this chapter. I remain grateful to them because they ited appreciative citations to all relevant and important papers.
have continued to stimulate my thinking. I am also grateful to Consequently, I offer an apology to respected colleagues
a remarkable group of student colleagues: Rebecca Goodvin, whose work is not explicitly noted as frequently as they merit,
Debbie Laible, Sara Meyer, Lenna Ontai, and Abbie Raikes. but whose thinking and research have been influential.

24
The Development of the Person 25

development emerge through the growth of social under- constructivist mind with the influence of relationships
standing, self-awareness, early conscience and coopera- in early sociopersonality development requires compre-
tion, and the relationships that infuse these early hending the nature of the shared activity of young chil-
achievements. These are some of the most important dren and those who care for them. This is an important
ways that make a 6-year-old a fundamentally different research challenge because a model of appropriated
person from a newborn and form the foundation for in- understanding through shared activity can potentially
dividuality and social relatedness in the years to come. further understanding of many features of early socio-
The development of social understanding, relationships, personality growth. In attachment theory, research on
self, and conscience constitute the most important ways the shared activities and conversations of young chil-
that developing individuality intersects with the social dren and their caregivers can help to clarify how spe-
world. These topics have also provoked the most con- cific representations of experience and self (or internal
certed research attention in the study of sociopersonal- working models) develop from relational security or in-
ity development during the past decade. security. In theory of mind, studies of shared experi-
The research literatures surveyed in this chapter ences and discourse can elucidate some of the
identify several themes about early sociopersonality de- conceptual catalysts fostering preschoolers understand-
velopment and developing persons. First, relationships ing of peoples desires, feelings, beliefs, and thoughts
are central. Indeed, this chapter is a study of relation- (Thompson, 2006a). A model of appropriated under-
ships and their developmental influence, whether con- standing from shared activity offers, more than tradi-
sidering face-to-face interaction and the growth of tional socialization or constructivist views, the
social expectations, parent-child discourse and autobio- opportunity to integrate social and cognitive aspects of
graphical self-awareness, the growth of a mutually co- early sociopersonality development.
operative orientation between parent and child, security Third, thinking and understanding in early childhood
of attachment, or childrens representations of self and is a conceptual foundation for what develops afterward.
relational processes. This chapter reflects an emerging Although this seems a truism, it was not long ago that
view that relational experience is generative of new un- characterizations of young children as egocentric, con-
derstanding, whether of emotions, self, morality, or peo- crete, preconventional, and preconceptual made this
ples beliefs, and highlights the need for a developmental developmental period seem discontinuous with the con-
relational science of the future that focuses on relational ceptual achievements of middle childhood and later. If
influences across diverse developmental domains. Such early childhood establishes the foundations for the
a developmental relational science could integrate the development of social cognition, moral judgment, and
most valuable perspectives offered by attachment the- self-understanding of the years that follow, then rela-
ory, neo-Vygotskian thinking, sociolinguistic ap- tionships and other influences experienced in the early
proaches to cognitive growth, and other perspectives years set the context for the growth of an empathic, hu-
into a thoughtful understanding of how early relational manistic orientation toward others, balanced self-
experience contributes to fundamental competencies concept, capacities for relational intimacy, social
and the emergence of individual differences in thinking, sensitivity, and other capacities conventionally viewed
sociability, and personality development. as achievements of middle childhood and adolescence.
Second, because relational experience is important, Understanding how this occurs is a current and future
early sociopersonality development is best understood research opportunity.
not as socialization or constructivism but rather as the In the contemporary climate of developmental sci-
appropriation of understanding from shared activity ence, relational influences in the family are understood
(Rogoff, 1990). The literatures reviewed in this chapter in concert with heritable influences shared by family
describe how psychological development arises from the members. Although students of early sociopersonality
powerfully inductive capacities of the young mind inter- development have been slow to enlist genetically sensi-
acting with the conceptual catalysts of social exchange, tive research designs into studies of family influences
whether in the conflict of wills between parents and a (see Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Born-
locomoting toddler, interactions about broken toys and stein, 2000), research on genetic and shared and non-
mishaps, or conversations about the days events that re- shared environmental influences on the security of
flect cultural values. Integrating understanding of the attachment and other relational variables has advanced
26 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

understanding of the interaction of heredity and environ- ture of social roles and institutions, group processes,
ment. Contemporary scholarship also benefits from a far and other social phenomenais essential to psychologi-
less polarized view of the influences of nature and nur- cal growth. At each age, social cognitive understanding
ture than what was true only a few years ago. Heritabil- contributes to social competence, interpersonal sensi-
ity estimates, while important, are now recognized as tivity, and an awareness of how the self relates to other
being both sample- and context sensitive and having lit- individuals and groups in a complex social world. Social
tle implication for the potency of environmental influ- cognition is also central to the development of emotion
ences (Committee on Integrating the Science of Early understanding, moral awareness, and self understand-
Childhood Development, 2000; Rutter, 1997). Equally ing. Early social cognitive development creates a foun-
important have been the contributions of developmental dation to these achievements as young children begin to
behavioral genetics for conceptualizing the differentiat- comprehend how human behavior is related to mental
ing experiences of siblings in the family (nonshared envi- goals, intentions, feelings, desires, thoughts, and beliefs,
ronment) and for understanding how childrens and how social interaction is affected by the juxtaposi-
characteristics are evocative of parenting practices tion of these mental states in two or more individuals.
(gene-environment correlation), both long integrated into Moreover, attachment theory and other theories of so-
developmental theory but now receiving renewed atten- cial development view early childhood as the period
tion. At the same time, an expanding body of research is when individual differences in social beliefs and dispo-
underscoring the importance of studying long-neglected sitions emerge from childrens social experiences, espe-
gene-environment interactionsby which children with cially in close relationships. Taken together, the study of
different heritable characteristics are affected differ- early social cognitive development offers the opportu-
ently by the environmentfor informing developmental nity to understand how young children derive their ini-
theory concerning family relationships (see, e.g., Ge tial insights into the psychological world of people, and
et al., 1996, and OConnor, Caspi, DeFries, & Plomin, why children begin to create markedly different expec-
2003, for illustrations). Such studies highlight that the tations for this social world. These early developmental
interaction term in the quantitative model for partition- processes color social understanding throughout life.
ing heritable and environmental influences on behavior Developmental study of social cognition has tradi-
may be the most important one. Molecular genetics re- tionally been the stepchild of research on cognitive de-
search has the power to elucidate gene-environment in- velopment, based on the assumption that the same
teractions and the probabilistic nature of genetic effects conceptual processes organize childrens thinking
(Rutter, Silberg, OConnor, & Simonoff, 1999), and about the social and nonsocial worlds. Beginning with
comparative studies highlight the influence of the envi- the Piagetian era, when the study of social-cognitive
ronment in gene expression in studies of rats and pri- development began in earnest, this meant that relatively
mates (see Gunnar & Vasquez, in press, for a review). little attention was devoted to social cognition in in-
Taken together, contemporary research is affirming the fancy and early childhood because this period was
wisdom of the lesson repeatedly learned by prior genera- theoretically characterized as one of egocentrism, con-
tions of developmental scientists: the inseparability of crete thinking, and a focus on appearances rather than
nature and nurture. What has advanced significantly is underlying, invisible realities. Students of social cogni-
the technology for elucidating their interaction. tive development also inherited from Piagetian theory
This is an exciting time for studying the development the constructivist model, with its emphasis on the au-
of the person because of a new appreciation of the gen- tonomous childs induction of understanding from indi-
erative influence of relational experience and respect vidual experience.
for what young individuals bring to these relationships. The current post-Piagetian era of cognitive develop-
mental research has offered new opportunities to explore
early social cognitive development because of a new
view of the developing mind. The assumption of early
SOCIAL UNDERSTANDING childhood egocentrism has been replaced by the realiza-
tion that understanding the mental world of other peo-
Understanding the world of peoplethe psychological ple, and the differences between peoples mental states,
processes that guide behavior and relationships, the na- is one of the early and consuming interests of infants and
Social Understanding 27

young children. In their investigations of the growth of internal world from the surround. From this perspec-
joint attention; inferences of intentionality, desires, and tive, early social cognition requires the emergence from
beliefs; theory of mind; and other conceptual processes, initial symbiosis or egocentrism. But an alternative view
researchers have highlighted how remarkably early and is offered by contemporary perceptual theory (e.g., Gib-
apparently easily young children acquire insight into the son, 1995), which argues that the integrated perceptual
psychological world and the relevance of these achieve- experiences yielded by movement and activity con-
ments to later social understanding. tribute to a fundamental distinction between internal
Contemporary study of early social cognition also experience and surrounding stimulation from shortly
contributes to a more sophisticated understanding of the after birth. According to this view, the tight synchrony
processes by which social understanding develops in of multimodal experience (e.g., integrated visual, tac-
early childhood. At a time when cognitive developmen- tile, kinesthetic, and auditory experiences) that arises
tal scholars are questioning the adequacy of explana- from self-initiated movement is perceptually different
tions of conceptual growth that focus solely on the from incoming stimulation arising from objects that
inductive, constructivist mind and are exploring the so- are acted on or that move of themselves. Gibson and
cial origins of psychological understanding (e.g., others (e.g., Neisser, 1995) have argued that, in this
Carpendale & Lewis, 2004; Hobson, 2002), research way, perception distinguishes self-initiated action from
into early face-to-face interaction, the impact of loco- surrounding activity and gradually contributes to self-
motor experience on parent-infant relations, social ref- awareness. Indeed, Gibson goes on to argue that the de-
erencing, parental socialization of social domain velopment of new behavioral capabilities coincides with
understanding, and parent-child conversation contribute the perception of new affordances of objects in the sur-
new insight into the developmental catalysts to early rounding world, such as how flat surfaces begin to be
psychological understanding. By exploring these social perceived as traversible when infants can locomote, and
catalysts, the ideas of social and cognitive developmen- how people begin to be perceived as arousing and re-
talists are usefully integrated in contemporary social sponsive when infants can interact socially. In this
cognitive research. This is especially so because inquiry sense, social cognition and self-awareness each arise
into early social cognitive development can help to clar- from the new perceptual experiences yielded by action,
ify central constructs in social developmental theories including social activity.
(such as the internal working models of attachment The social and inanimate worlds are potentially dis-
theory) while also providing insight into the conse- tinguishable early in life in several ways. People are
quences of differences in early social experiences for spontaneous agents and act in a self-initiated manner,
childrens understanding of mental states. Therefore, but this is not true of inanimate objects. People interact
contemporary research on early social cognitive devel- in a reciprocal, contingent, coordinated, and commu-
opment is not only an instantiation of the traditional nicative fashion with the infant, predictably responding
view that conceptual achievements are applied to the so- to the babys signals but responding with considerable
cial and nonsocial worlds alike but also a new opportu- variability. Emotion is a more salient feature of social
nity to explore how the scaffolding of everyday social interaction compared to most encounters with ob-
experience provides uniquely social catalysts to the de- jectsincluding the emotions that precede social inter-
velopment of psychological understanding. action and the changes in emotions that arise from
The study of early social cognition encompasses de- interactive activity. Most important, the locus of
velopments in social skills, general knowledge of the so- causality for peoples behavior is intentional goal-
cial world (including the psychological functioning of directed mentality for which no comparable sources of
people), and person-specific social expectations. In causality exist for objects.
each of these areas, infancy and early childhood is a pe- During the 1st year, infants begin to discriminate be-
riod of significant advance. tween the social and animate worlds in many of these
ways (see Raikson & Poulin-Dubois, 2001). These dis-
Early Social Discriminations and Expectations criminations are founded on early perceptual prefer-
ences that orient young infants toward social events.
In traditional developmental theory, a fundamental con- Newborns visually track facelike stimuli, reflecting the
ceptual challenge for the newborn is to distinguish the influence of dedicated subcortical neural circuits that
28 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

affect the development of, and are later supplanted by, behavioral gestures. Developmental scientists have been
cortically mediated facial preferences at 2 to 3 months interested in episodes of face-to-face play not because of
of age (Johnson & Morton, 1991; Mondloch et al., their ubiquity or universality, but rather because they
1999). Newborns exhibit a visual preference for their constitute some of the earliest experiences of focused
mothers faces based on global perceptual discrimina- social interaction that contribute to the growth of social
tions that will later become more refined when infants skills and the development of social expectations for fa-
begin scanning interior facial features at 2 to 3 months miliar caregivers.
of age (Pascalis, de Schonen, Morton, Deruelle, & Detailed microanalyses of the course of infant and
Fabre-Grenet, 1995; Walton, Bower, & Bower, 1992). By adult behavior during social interaction reveal several
3 months, when infants facial scanning has moved to characteristics of face-to-face play that underscore the
the interior of faces, infants also begin to discriminate complexity and richness of this social experience for
the pictures of familiar persons (Barrera & Maurer, young infants. First, in responding contingently to the
1981; but see Bartrip, Morton, & de Shonen, 2001, for babys socioemotional expressions, adults do not merely
evidence of earlier recognition ability). Newborns are mimic or mirror the infants actions. In addition, they
also capable of recognizing the sound of the mothers express emotion in ways that are comparable to the
voice based on prenatal auditory experience (DeCasper babys own but using different expressions, such as re-
& Fifer, 1980; DeCasper & Spence, 1986). This may be sponding with a smile and a lilting voice when the baby
related to newborns preference for the sounds of human coos. Moreover, adults also model positive expressions
speech and, in particular, for infant-directed speech and differentially reinforce the babys emotional re-
that is characterized by exaggerated prosody, repetition, sponses. Malatestas elegant microanalyses of maternal
and simple syntax (Cooper & Aslin, 1990). Infants and infant emotional expressions during face-to-face
preference for infant-directed speech endures through- play revealed that mothers maintained a generally posi-
out the early months and adult vocalizations can evoke tive demeanor and, while they matched the emotion of
emotional responses in the infant that are consistent most infant emotional expressions (including joy, inter-
with the positive or negative tone of the adult voice. In- est, surprise, and even sadness and anger), the babys
fants respond positively to vocalizations signaling affir- negative expressions (such as pain or knit brow) were
mation or warmth (with exaggerated melodic contour) likely to be ignored or, in the case of anger, evoke the
and negatively to vocalizations signaling anger or prohi- mothers surprised response (Malatesta, Culver, Tes-
bition (with sharp, staccato intonations; Fernald, 1985, man, & Shepard, 1989; Malatesta, Grigoryev, Lamb,
1996). People are, in short, uniquely compelling ele- Albin, & Culver, 1986). Mothers seemed committed to
ments of the newborns world: The constellation of maintaining the baby in a positive emotional state and,
stimulus properties they possess captivate the young in- over a period of weeks, maternal modeling and contin-
fants attention and arouse emotion, perhaps owing to gent responding to infant emotional expressions helped
the developing brains preparedness to respond to to account for increased rates of infant joy and interest
human stimulation. expressions in face-to-face play. Adult contingent re-
People are captivating to infants not only because of sponsiveness is complex and often involves responses
their stimulus properties but also because of their be- that do not match the infants own but instead are in-
havioral propensities. Young infants discover that people tended to alter or guide the babys emotional responding.
respond to their initiatives in ways that create excite- Second, although face-to-face play is commonly
ment and generate positive arousal. This becomes espe- characterized as the establishment and maintenance of
cially apparent after 2 to 3 months of age when, with the well-coordinated synchrony, with adults sensitively
behavioral state fluctuations of the neonatal period sub- scaffolding their initiatives to accord with the babys
siding and longer periods of awake alertness emerging, signals, it is mistaken to portray this social activity
infants and their caregivers begin to engage in episodes so simply. Tronick and his colleagues, based on their
of face-to-face play. These episodes are typically char- own microanalytic studies, have concluded that well-
acterized by focused social interaction without compet- coordinated interactions occur only about 30% or less
ing caregiving goals or other demands on either partner, of the time that mothers and infants engage in face-to-
with infant and adult facing each other in close proxim- face interactions, with nonsynchronous or uncoordi-
ity and interacting facially, vocally, tactilely, and with nated exchanges occurring when infants become fussy,
Social Understanding 29

mothers are distracted, or for other reasons (Gianino & gests that the expectation that people will be responsive
Tronick, 1988; Tronick, 1989). They argue that other in- is not person specific and seems to be generalized to a
teractive goalssuch as interactive reparation and range of interactive experiences in the early months.
self-regulationaccompany the goal of maintaining in- The still-face procedure was originally designed to
teractive coordination. Therefore, infants are faced with simulate the infants interactive experience when moth-
a more complex interactive activity than merely re- ers are depressed. Individual differences in maternal be-
sponding to a sensitively scaffolded social situation. In havior and affect are significant influences on how
their earliest experiences of social play, infants are also infants respond socially (Adamson & Frick, 2003). Sev-
learning that social interaction is dynamic and chang- eral studies have found that depressed mothers are less
ing, and are acquiring the social skills of managing its responsive and emotionally more negative and subdued in
dyadic course and its emotional effects. They are dis- face-to-face play with their infants, for example, and the
covering that their social and emotional responses have offspring of depressed mothers are also less responsive
effects on the adults behaviorsometimes highly pre- and emotionally less animated as early as 2 to 3 months
dictable effectsand that their emotions are central to (e.g., Cohn, Campbell, Matias, & Hopkins, 1990; Field,
the dynamics of interaction with a human partner. They Healy, Goldstein, & Guthertz, 1990; Field et al., 1988).
are also discovering that their own emotional experi- Field and her colleagues (1988) found that 3- to 6-month-
ence is affected not only by the initiatives of the care- old infants of depressed mothers remained more subdued
giver but also by the interaction that arises from mutual and less animated when subsequently interacting with a
responsiveness. nondepressed stranger. Dawson and colleagues did not
These experiences may help to explain why, by 2 to 3 replicate this finding with 13- to 15-month-old infants,
months of age, infants respond differently to people but reported that the atypical patterns of frontal brain ac-
compared to objects, directing more positive facial ex- tivity characteristic of the infants of depressed mothers
pressions and vocalizations to responsive people than during social interaction with their mothers were also ap-
toward interactive objects (e.g., puppets), and showing parent when these infants subsequently interacted with a
distress to nonresponsive people but rarely toward non- nondepressed familiar adult (Dawson et al., 1999). Dif-
interactive objects (Ellsworth, Muir, & Hains, 1993; ferences in early social experience seem to be important,
Legerstee, 1997; Legerstee, Pomerleau, Malcuit, & Fei- therefore, for how infants interact with other partners,
der, 1987). By 2 to 3 months of age, infants appear to ex- which may reflect the early emergence of generalized
pect that people will respond to them and interact with and specific social expectations. This may help to explain
them. This conclusion is supported by studies of the why early differences in infant affective and self-
still-face effect in infants in which mothers alternate regulatory behavior in the still-face paradigm predict
episodes of face-to-face interaction with an episode in later attachment security (Braungart-Rieker, Garwood,
which they look at the infant but are impassive and unre- Powers, & Wang, 2001; Cohn, Campbell, & Ross, 1992)
sponsive. Studies of infants age 2 to 6 months show that and other psychosocial sequelae. Much more research on
babies reliably respond with diminished positive affect, the origins and outcomes of individual differences in in-
withdrawal, self-directed behavior, and sometimes with fant behavior in the still-face procedure is needed to clar-
social elicitations (e.g., brief smiles, momentarily in- ify the specific social expectations it reflects. However,
creased vocalizing and reaching) and negative affect by 2 to 3 months of age, infants have begun to expect that
during the still-face episode. When mothers subse- people will respond positively to their initiatives, and
quently respond normally, infants become more sociable marked differences in adult responsiveness have signifi-
but also remain subdued (see Adamson & Frick, 2003, cant effects on the infants social and emotional reactions
for a review of this literature). The still-face effect is ro- that generalize to other partners.
bust: It has been observed in response to strangers as As these studies suggest, adult responding that is con-
well as to parents (Ellsworth et al., 1993; Kisilevsky tingent on the infants initiatives contributes to the so-
et al., 1998), in comparisons of infants from Western cially and emotionally engaging quality of early social
and non-Western cultures (Kisilevsky et al., 1998), and interaction. By 2 to 3 months of age, infants respond with
in conditions when the adults reasons for ceasing social positive emotion to contingent responding but become af-
interaction were systematically varied such as turning fectively negative if the contingency is interrupted
away to look at another person (Striano, 2004). This sug- (Lewis, Alessandri, & Sullivan, 1990; Rovee-Collier,
30 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

1989; J. Watson, 1985). Contingency in an environmental face-to-face play in which these emotional expressions
response is positively arousing perhaps because it con- are salient features of social communication would con-
tributes to the infants sense of agency. Initiating actions tribute to these forms of nascent emotion understand-
that have a predictable effect is a salient indication that ing. Second, social play also provides opportunities for
one can exert control over important outcomes, whether infants to learn about the distinctive behavioral charac-
social or nonsocial. J. S. Watson (1995) has argued that teristics of familiar partners. Fathers play differently
young infants are especially sensitive to perfectly re- than mothers in face-to-face encounters with young in-
sponse-contingent events because these indicate self- fants, for example, and infants later show differentiated
generated outcomes (e.g., movement of a mobile expectations for the social behavior of each parent (see
contingent on the babys leg kicking) and that such expe- M. Lamb, 1997, for a review). Finally, to the extent that
riences contribute to self-awareness. Later, at about 2 to in the early months, infants begin to represent others
3 months, he argues, infants become sensitized to imper- actions as like me when they can also be performed by
fect response-contingent events that are more likely to be the self (Meltzoff & Gopnik, 1993), the coordination of
social in nature (see also Gergely & Watson, 1999). In the socioemotional initiatives of the self and a sensitive
each case, contingency is salient because of the sense of partner during early episodes of social play is likely to
control and effectance it creates. Murray and Trevarthen consolidate this nascent representational capability by 3
(1985) showed that a small sample of 2- to 3-month-olds months of age.
responded animatedly when viewing live images of their Social play is not the only interactive context for the
mothers talking to them through closed-circuit television development of early social discriminations and expec-
but later, when the same images of their mothers were re- tations. In light of the salience of distress, the associa-
played (and were thus noncontingent), infants turned tion between parental soothing and subsequent relief is
away in apparent disinterest or distress (see Bigelow, likely to be meaningful and easily learned by an infant,
MacLean, & MacDonald, 1996; Hains & Muir, 1996; and contributing to expectations that an adults arrival will
Legerstee & Varghese, 2001, for partial replications and bring distress relief (M. Lamb, 1981). In this context,
extensions; but note also Rochat, Neisser, & Marian, differences in adult responsiveness are again likely to be
1998, for a failure to replicate this effect). The contin- important, at least to the extent that they affect develop-
gency of face-to-face interaction thus seems important to ing expectations for the caregivers arrival and soothing
the social and emotional potency of early social play. when infants are upset. Several research groups have
There are other advances in social cognition and so- found that by 6 months, distressed infants began quiet-
cial skills emerging from early face-to-face play and re- ing in apparent anticipation of the arrival of their moth-
lated experiences of social interaction. First, because ers when they could hear the adults approaching
social play is so richly affective, infants learn about the footsteps; infants also protested loudly if the adult ap-
emotional expressions of people. By 3.5 months, infants proached but did not pick them up (Gekoski, Rovee-
can discriminate the dynamic, multimodal expressions Collier, & Carulli-Rabinowitz, 1983; M. Lamb &
of different emotions enacted by their mothers and they Malkin, 1986). These studies suggest that during the
prefer congruence between facial and vocal expressions initial months of the 1st year, infants are learning the as-
(Kahana-Kalman & Walker-Andrews, 2001; Montegue sociation between their distress, a caregivers approach,
& Walker-Andrews, 2002; Walker-Andrews, 1997). This soothing ministrations, and subsequent comfort. Much
is not observed, however, in response to the emotional more research is needed to understand the effects of re-
expressions of unfamiliar women until 5 to 7 months of liable differences in the caregiver responsiveness (such
age. This suggests that partner familiarity may be im- as differences in the adults efficacy in soothing the in-
portant to the earliest comprehension of emotional ex- fant) on these emergent social expectations related to
pressions and their meaning, with some evidence that distress relief, especially as they are mediated by the in-
infants respond in an emotionally resonant manner to fants emotional tendencies.
the dynamic emotional expressions of their mothers by 3
months (Haviland & Lelwica, 1987). Infants as young as
Intentions and Inferring Intentionality
5 months also react in an emotionally differential man-
ner to positive and negative emotions conveyed through Interest in face-to-face play wanes after 7 months as in-
speech alone (Fernald, 1996). Regular experiences of fants become more mobile and interested in more active
Social Understanding 31

forms of interaction. The growth of self-produced loco- main sensitive to and supportive of the infants emer-
motion not only changes infant-parent interaction but gent competencies during this period contributes to
also is, according to Campos and his colleagues, a set- maintaining parent-infant harmony just as parental
ting event for a variety of socioemotional and concep- coercion and frustration are likely to undermine it.
tual advances in the child (Campos, et al., 2000; There are thus many reasons to perceive the locomo-
Campos, Kermoian, & Zumbahlen, 1992). These ad- tor transition as a catalyst for early socioemotional and
vances occur because locomotor experience dramati- conceptual development and for parent-infant relation-
cally changes the relation of the infant to the ships. But the social consequences of this transition have
environment. Rather than merely reaching toward ob- been little studied thus far. In an exploratory study,
jects or responding to events that appear before them, Campos and colleagues (1992) interviewed the mothers
infants are now capable of approaching objects and peo- of locomotor and prelocomotor 8-month-olds and found
ple of interest and initiating independent exploration. As that mothers perceptions of the child and reports of
a consequence, self-produced locomotion is associated their own activities varied significantly based on the
with a cascade of conceptual changes related to person- childs locomotor status. The parents of locomotor in-
environment relations (such as postural compensation to fants indicated that they used more verbal prohibitions,
changes in peripheral optic flow perception, advances had higher expectations for the childs compliance, and
in distance perception and increased wariness of engaged in greater disciplinary activity than did the par-
heights, and more sophisticated spatial search strate- ents of prelocomotor infants. Parents also reported that
gies), which include advances in referential communica- their offspring showed greater sensitivity to the parents
tion, means-ends understanding, and social interaction location and emotional signals, and exhibited increased
across a distance. expressions of anger and frustration, but also showed
The onset of locomotor experience is also associ- more intense affectionate behavior.
ated with socioemotional changes in the infant and Observational studies provide some support for
challenges for the family system. The infants inde- these interview results. Biringen, Emde, Campos, and
pendent locomotion means that the child is becoming Appelbaum (1995) reportedin an age-held-constant
capable of wandering away from the parent, acting in observational studythat the onset of walking was
a dangerous or disapproved manner, and pursuing in- accompanied by greater testing of wills between
dependent goals, together with the feelings of self- mothers and infants in prohibition contexts, and by di-
efficacy of doing so. Parents respond to these changes minished maternal praise of the child, although there
by more vigilantly monitoring the infants activity were no differences in infant emotionality. Zumbahlen
(and childproofing the house), using distal commu- and Crawley (1996) observed a greater number of
nicative modes (such as calling across a distance), parental prohibitions directed to crawling than nonlo-
and intervening more often with distractions, prohibi- comotor infants, and that crawling infants showed
tions, and sanctions and thus, at times, frustrating the greater anger and also more often visually checked
infants goal-directed efforts. The stage is set, there- back with the parent across a distance. In another age-
fore, for a significant conf lict of intention between held-constant observational study, Hendrix (2004) re-
the infant and parent. Indeed, at the same time that at- ported that the mothers of locomotor infants used no
tachment security is emerging, infant-parent relation- more often in a prohibitive context compared to moth-
ships are being shaped by how each partner is ers of prelocomotor infants, but there were no group
negotiating the challenges associated with the onset differences in proactive discipline (such as using dis-
of infant locomotor activity. Parental reactions to this tractors), child compliance, or infant emotional reac-
developmental transition are likely to vary signifi- tions. The parents of locomotor infants also reported
cantly. The evolution of offspring from immobile to using a greater number of discipline practices at home.
self-propelled excites most parents, but many also Some of these studies have also reported changes in
find that the monitoring, intervening, and proaction parental behavior over time regardless of the childs lo-
required to supervise a mobile child is a significant comotor status, such as greater childproofing of the
new challenge for them, along with the conf lict of home (Hendrix, 2004). This suggests that the anticipa-
wills and testing of limits that accompanies infants tion of the babys self-produced locomotion is impor-
responses to their efforts. A parents capacity to re- tant to parents, helping them to prepare for the physical
32 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

maturation and behavioral competence of their young with infants differential encoding of human and nonhu-
offspring and the new requirements of their parenting. man activity. At least by 7 months of age, for example,
That the growth of independent locomotion during infants distinguish the movement of people as being
the second half of the 1st year is associated with signif- self-initiated whereas objects move by external force
icant changes in parent-child interaction derives, in (Spelke, Phillips, & Woodward, 1995), and this may
part, from how locomotion contributes to a more agen- occur even earlier (Legerstee, 1994). Using a similar ha-
tic and goal-directed infant. The emergence of inten- bituation procedure, Woodward has also shown that by
tional, goal-oriented behavior has been a familiar 12 months, infants understand the object directedness of
characterization of the 8- to 12-month-old infant from a persons gazing (Woodward, 2003) and pointing
Piagets (1952) description of the fourth sensorimotor (Woodward & Guajardo, 2002) and have also begun to
substage. As Campos and his colleagues (2004) have comprehend the distinction between goals and the ac-
noted, locomotion spurs more sophisticated means-ends tions enacted to achieve them (Woodward & Som-
behavior because infants must maintain a specific goal merville, 2000). Baldwin, Baird, Saylor, and Clark
in mind (such as moving toward an interesting object) (2001) have likewise shown that 10- to 11-month-olds
while assembling the specific movements and second- organize their perceptions of peoples actions by the
ary strategies necessary for achieving it. Not only are completion of goal-directed activity (see Baird & Bald-
infants becoming more volitional during this transition win, 2001).
but also, as a consequence, they are being exposed to a Inferences of the intentions underlying actions like
range of social responses that underscore the discordant reaching, gazing, and pointing are easy for infants to
intentions of others, whether they consist of parental comprehend because they are the same actions that
prohibitions, verbal admonitions (conveyed in the tone often express their own intentions. By 6 months of age,
of voice), cautionary facial expressions in response to infants are familiar with the sight of their own hand
social referencing, or other forms of referential commu- reaching toward an object and may be more likely, as a
nication. Self-produced locomotion enhances the ex- consequence, to interpret other object-directed reaches
pression of infant volition, and also contributes to an they observe as similarly goal oriented. Understanding
awareness of others intentions (Campos et al., 2000). the object directedness of gazing and pointing emerges
Indeed, these may be developmentally allied achieve- later with growing comprehension of referential commu-
ments because of how parental interventions over con- nication and joint attention. Moreover, when caregivers
flicting goals enhance the salience of the volition of respond to the intentionality they infer in the behavior of
another as it contrasts with the infants own, and moti- their infant offspring, they also scaffold emergent com-
vates efforts to comprehend the difference (see also prehension of the intentional structure of behavior
Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, in press; (Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley, & Tuckey, 2001; Meins
Tomasello & Rakoczy, 2003). et al., 2003). When mothers punctuate their verbal re-
There are many indications that by 9 to 12 months, sponses to the infants goal-directed activity with affir-
infants begin to perceive peoples actions as intentional mative utterances when the goal is achieved, for
and goal directed as infants interpret actions in relation example, they help to parse the sequence of behavioral
to the objects to which they are directed. In a study by acts in terms that organize the perception of behavior in
Woodward (1998), for example, infants were habituated goal-oriented units. Caregivers who are attuned to the
to a scene of a hand reaching across a stage to grasp one intentional orientation of infant behavior (or mind-
of two toys. After habituation, the positions of the toys minded, according to Meins and her colleagues; see
were reversed and the hand either reached to grasp the Meins et al., 2001, 2003) are especially likely to interac-
original toy in its new location (requiring a different tra- tively scaffold early comprehension of the goal orienta-
jectory) or a new toy in the original location (using the tion of behavior in these ways.
same reaching motion as before). Six-month-olds and 9- The perception of people as subjective, intentional
month-olds each looked longer to the latter trials, sug- agents is a signal accomplishment for early social cogni-
gesting that infants had encoded the original action as tive development. By the first birthday or shortly there-
directed to a particular toy. In this study, infants did not after, there are further indications that infants are
respond comparably when a mechanical arm rather than responding in a more sophisticated manner to the ac-
a human hand reached to grasp the objects, consistent tions of people as subjective, intentional agents (see
Social Understanding 33

Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998, Tomasello, parent relationships. How much does the emergent test-
1995a, 1999, and Tomasello & Rakoczy, 2003 for re- ing of limits of the toddler period, for example, arise
views). Infants create joint attentional states with adults from the young childs perception of the adults inten-
by looking in the direction of the adults gaze or looking tionality when blocking, deterring, or otherwise frus-
from a toy to the adults face and back to the toy again. trating the childs goal-directed activity? How does a
They not only follow an adults gaze but also look in the 1-year-old interpret an adults emotional behavior in re-
direction of the adults pointing or gesturing. They pro- lation to objects in the framework of intentionality in-
duce protodeclarative gestures (such as pointing to or ferences? How does this infant regard the intentionality
holding up an object while alternating gaze between the of the adults emotional behavior toward herself ? Are a
object and the adults face) and protoimperative ges- caregivers nurturant actions perceived by a 1-year-old
tures (such as reaching for an object while alternating as intentional and goal directed, and how is this related
gaze between the object and the adults face), each ap- to emergent social expectations and the developing secu-
parently intended to alter the adults subjective orienta- rity of their relationship? Twelve- to fourteen-month-old
tion and elicit a desired response. They also exhibit infants enlist emotional demeanor and gaze direction in
social referencing behavior (discussed later). To be sure, their inferences of the intentions of an adult actor to-
there has been some debate over whether these behav- ward toy kittens (Phillips, Wellman, & Spelke, 2002).
iors reflect true perceptions of intentionality rather than Do toddlers derive similar intentionality judgments in
conditioned learning of social behavior (Moore & their observations of everyday social behavior?
Corkum, 1994) or affective sharing (Baldwin & Moses,
1996). However, the intercoordinated developmental
Social Referencing
emergence of these achievements suggests that a more
fundamental transition has occurred in the infants per- Limited answers to such questions can be gleaned from
son perception. By the first birthday, infants have begun the research on social referencing (Campos & Stenberg,
regarding people as intentional agents with subjective 1981; Klinnert, Campos, Sorce, Emde, & Svejda, 1983).
viewpoints that can be altered. Later in the 2nd year, Social referencing describes the use of anothers emo-
further evidence of this transition emerges with the abil- tional cues to clarify the interpretation of an ambiguous
ity of 14- to 18-month-olds to imitate adults intended or uncertain event. The enlistment of this emotional in-
rather than accidental actions (Carpenter, Akhtar, & formation may derive from active information seeking
Tomasello, 1998; Meltzoff, 1995), their enlistment of (such as when one adult looks to anothers face to clar-
inferences concerning the intentions of adult speakers in ify the meaning of an ambiguous statement) or may cap-
learning new words (Baldwin, Markman, Bill, Des- italize on the availability of anothers emotional cues in
jardins, & Irwin, 1996; Tomasello & Barton, 1994), and the course of affective sharing or seeking reassurance
their use of intention inferences in new social learning (Baldwin & Moses, 1996). In either case, the impor-
(Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello, 2002). There are many tance of social referencing is twofold. First, it indicates
further advances in intentionality understanding after that by the first birthday, infants are fairly good con-
age 2. Young children have much to learn about how in- sumers of the emotional cues of others and can enlist
tentions connect to other mental states, the influences this information in their own responses to events. Sec-
that mediate the transition from intention to action, and ond, social referencing inaugurates the processes by
the nature of nonintentional action, as well as other psy- which young children vicariously acquire an under-
chological processes. standing of events through the signals provided by others
Understanding peoples behavior as goal directed and and thus appropriate socially constructed meaning sys-
intentional takes the infant a long way toward a mental- tems. Both are lifelong features of social development.
istic comprehension of human behavior, but observing The research on social referencing indicates that it
rather prosaic behaviors like reaching and pointing does has important but modest effects on infant behavior
not provide much insight into how infants begin to under- (Feinman, Roberts, Hsieh, Sawyer, & Swanson, 1992).
stand the salient experiences of social interaction. Little The influence of social referencing is especially appar-
is known of how a dawning appreciation of other people ent when infants are uncertain how to respond, but an-
as subjective, intentional agents alters other features of others emotional cues can be influential even when
developing social cognition and the growth of infant- they are not unsure (Feinman et al., 1992; Zarbatany &
34 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Lamb, 1985). Hertenstein and Campos (2004) have ambiguous events but also in circumstances when care-
demonstrated the retention of social referencing influ- givers emotional expressions serve to alert, caution, in-
ences for up to 1 hour in 14-month-olds, but much more terest, reassure, or otherwise motivate their young
research into the longer-term effects of social referenc- offspring in relation to events of shared attention. In
ing is needed. Infant responses to uncertain situations these circumstances, the same understanding of object
can be influenced by facial expressions alone (e.g., Klin- specificity and referential intent enables infants to com-
nert, Emde, Butterfield, & Campos, 1986; Sorce, Emde, prehend that the adults sharp, imperative voice is with
Campos, & Klinnert, 1985; Zarbatany & Lamb, 1985), reference to the potted plant that the child is reaching to-
vocal cues alone (Mumme, Fernald, & Herrera, 1996), ward, or that the caregivers smiling expression provides
multimodal emotional cues (sometimes including ges- reassurance with respect to the kitten they are stroking.
tures; e.g., Hirshberg & Svejda, 1990; Hornik, Risen- In light of these early achievements in referential un-
hoover, & Gunnar, 1987; Walden & Ogan, 1988), and derstanding and emotional communication, it is some-
even televised images (Mumme & Fernald, 2003). In what surprising that there has not been exploration of
everyday situations, infants commonly have access to further development in these processes during the 2nd
the unsolicited multimodal emotional cues of their care- and 3rd years, when a young childs interpretation of the
givers as they traverse, experiment, and explore the lim- psychological meaning of an adults emotional expres-
its of their known universe. sions becomes more insightful. Studies of early language
Consistent with the research on infants understand- acquisition show that as early as 18 months, for example,
ing of the subjectivity of others actions, social refer- toddlers inferences of the intentions of adult speak-
encing studies have also shown that as young as 12 to 14 ersusually gleaned from their emotional displays
months of age, infants understand the object specificity provide a basis for initial word learning (e.g., Baldwin,
of anothers emotional message (Hornik et al., 1987; 2000; Baldwin et al., 1996), and future research might
Repacholi, 1998; Walden & Ogan, 1988), and can use be devoted to examining other conceptual achievements
that persons referential cues, such as gaze direction, to that are facilitated by early emotional communication.
guide the interpretation of the persons emotional ex- For example, what does it mean for a young childs per-
pressions (Moses, Baldwin, Rosicky, & Tidball, 2001). ception of other people, especially other family mem-
This suggests that anothers emotional message has con- bers or peers, when they are the targets of a caregivers
siderable informational value for 1-year-olds because emotionally referential focus? When do young children
they can comprehend its referential intent. But the emo- begin to comprehend that they can themselves be objects
tional cues of an adult also have emotional impact, and of an adults emotional responding, and what is the im-
studies have shown that the adults signals influence the pact of this awareness for early self-concept and the de-
infants general emotional demeanor, especially toward velopment of security in close relationships? Variations
the object of referential focus (Hirshberg & Svejda, in emotional communication appear to be relevant to the
1990; Klinnert et al., 1986; Moses et al., 2001; Mumme earliest feelings of self-confidence and pride when
et al., 1996; Sorce et al., 1985). These dual influences of the adults referential focus on the child or the childs
social referencing are not inconsistent. When reading accomplishments is accompanied by emotionally affir-
anothers emotional expressions in the presence of an mative cues (Stipek, 1995). Conversely, emotional com-
ambiguous event, infants are emotionally alerted by the munication can be enlisted by parents in conveying
adults affective demeanor at the same time that they are behavioral standards (such as looking sternly at the
interpreting the meaning of this demeanor for the event child who is initiating disapproved activity) and in in-
of shared referential focus. ducing shame or guilt when these standards are violated
Social referencing illustrates, therefore, the sensitiv- (Emde & Buchsbaum, 1990).
ity of 1-year-olds to the meaning underlying an adults The impact of social referencing experiences on the
emotional orientation. By the beginning of the 2nd year, adult also merits further research attention. Informal ob-
1-year-olds comprehend that anothers emotional ex- servations of spontaneous referencing behavior in
pressions can be evoked with reference to a specific ob- my laboratory indicate that parents are acutely aware of
ject or event, and this knowledge influences their the social referencing of their young offspring and often
interpretation of that event. This awareness is enlisted deliberately pose salient emotional expressions to reas-
not only in situations when infants are uncertain about sure, instill caution, and provide other socioemotional
Social Understanding 35

messages. If this is true, it suggests that social referenc- scribes one 2-year-old who became distressed when she
ing should be viewed as a dyadic process of referential was given her bath beforerather than afterdinner
communication through which infants and young children because she thought this meant that she would not be fed
appropriate an understanding of events of significance to that evening). These scripts increase in complexity and
them, and caregivers facilitate that understanding scope throughout the preschool years as they become in-
through deliberate emotional cuing. tegrated into broader knowledge systems.
Studies by Nelson and her colleagues indicate that the
Understanding Social Events content, organization, and structure of early event repre-
sentation is shaped not only by the childs prelinguistic
After 18 months, a transition occurs in psychological representation of experiences but also by the verbal
development when young children strive to comprehend structure applied to them in parent-child discourse. In
normative standards for the social world. Developmen- the context of shared conversations, beginning as soon
tal scientists have observed this in many behavioral do- as children can talk about events, parents help to review,
mains. With respect to early conscience, for example, reconstruct, and consolidate young childrens memory
this is the period when toddlers respond with height- of generalized routines and specific experiences
ened interest and concern to objects that are damaged or (Fivush, 1993; Hudson, 1990; Nelson, 1989, 1993a).
flawed, applying normative standards for the wholeness, Furthermore, parents often help children to anticipate
appearance, and integrity of objects (Kagan, 1981, in future events, and the verbal structure they provide may
press; S. Lamb, 1993). As discussed later, this subse- help to organize the childs representation of that expe-
quently becomes manifested in an intuitive morality that rience as it subsequently occurs (Nelson, 1989, 1993a).
causes young children to regard rules as obligatory, even There is also evidence that the style of parental dis-
though children commonly violate them (Wellman & course is important. Parents who are more elaborative in
Miller, 2003). With respect to the development of self- their conversational style provide considerable back-
awareness, the responses of 18-month-olds to the famil- ground and contextual information in their shared dis-
iar rouge task not only reveal self-recognition before a cussion of events in the childs life. Several studies have
mirror but also their evident embarrassment when de- found that the offspring of more elaborative mothers
tecting a spot of red on their noses (Lewis, 2000; Lewis have a more complete and sophisticated representation
& Brooks-Gunn, 1979). Toddlers have internalized a of their past experiences (including representations of
normative standard for their physical appearance that routine events) not only owing to the direct impact of
does not include a rouge-marked nose. One of the most parental discourse style but also because of the childs
important manifestations of the young childs search for appropriation of the adults narrative approach (Hudson,
normative standards at this age is in language develop- 1990; Nelson, 1993a; Reese & Fivush, 1993).
ment, where they strive to comprehend the appropriate Generalized event representations, or scripts, provide a
nominal reference of the words they are acquiring at foundation for young childrens understanding of social
such a rapid pace (Tomasello & Rakoczy, 2003). events. However, with researchers focus on rather prosaic
With respect to event representation, young children routines (such as restaurant visits), little is known about
reveal further their search for normative standards. By how young children represent everyday experiences that
the end of the 2nd year, they begin to create generalized involve greater emotional and relational depth such as
scripts for familiar social experiences such as bedtime separations and reunions, bedtime routines, and distress
rituals, mealtimes and other regular family routines, ar- relief. Such events are important in how young children
rivals and departures from child care, and other com- comprehend emotion and relationships, the quality of
mon events (Hudson, 1993; Nelson, 1978, 1989; Nelson parental nurturance, and the reliability of care. The
& Gruendel, 1981). These scripts provide a conceptual childs direct experience of these events, as well as the
scaffold for knowledge of general routines and for mem- verbal structure of parental discourse in subsequent con-
ory of specific experiences that incorporate routine versation, are each important to how children comprehend
events, and they constitute the young childs normative these experiences. Further study of childrens representa-
expectations for how those routines should occur in the tions of these experiences may also contribute to under-
future. Indeed, young children can be inflexible in their standing the origins of the differences in relational
fidelity to scripted expectations (Hudson, 1990, de- security and trust that underlie parent-child attachment.
36 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Feelings and Desires children regard emotions not just as behavioral events
but as subjective, psychological conditions, distinct
Social referencing research is important because it illus- from the situations and behaviors with which they are
trates the significance of emotion to the infants behav- associated. By contrast with their descriptions of pain,
ioral regulation and to social understanding. Social for example, children as young as 2 describe emotion as
referencing is important also because processes of emo- referential (e.g., sad about something) and involving vo-
tional communication are ubiquitous in child-parent in- lition, consistent with their developing understanding of
teraction, including the routine events that are the intentionality and referentiality. Moreover, even in
foundation for generalized event representations. Not these initial utterances, toddlers explicitly differentiate
surprisingly, among the most important subsequent ad- peoples feelings, often contrasting anothers emotions
vances in early social cognition is developing under- with their own in a nonegocentric manner (Bartsch &
standing of peoples desires, beliefs, and feelings Wellman, 1995; Wellman et al., 1995). By age 2.5,
(Thompson & Lagattuta, 2005). young children comprehend better the connections be-
Toddlers display a remarkable comprehension of the tween desire and emotion: People are happy when they
differences between people in what they desire, con- get or see what they want and unhappy when their de-
trary to the traditional portrayal of early egocentrism. sires are denied (Repacholi & Gopnik, 1997; Wellman
In an important study, Repacholi and Gopnik (1997) & Woolley, 1990).
presented 14- and 18-month-olds with two snacks: gold- It appears that early childhood witnesses the growth
fish crackers (which the children liked) and broccoli of young childrens intuitive theories of emotion that in-
(which the children disliked). Then the adult tasted each corporate not only the belief-desire reasoning described
snack, smiling and exhibiting pleasure (mmmm!) with by theory of mind researchers but also their dawning un-
one, and frowning and saying ewww! with the other. derstanding of the internal (including visceral) and ex-
In the match condition, the adults preferences were ternal determinants of emotion, the subjectivity and
the same as the childs; in the mismatch condition, the referentiality of emotional experience, the outcomes of
adult preferred the broccoli and disliked the crackers. emotional arousal, and emotional regulatory processes.
Then the adult extended her hand and said, I want some These intuitive theories of emotion expand markedly
more, can you give me more? The 18-month-olds ( but during the preschool years (Denham, 1998; Fabes,
not the younger toddlers) reliably gave the adult the food Eisenberg, Nyman, & Michaelieu, 1991). In their ef-
she desired in both the match and mismatch conditions. forts to comprehend the causes of emotion, preschoolers
By contrast, the 14-month-olds overwhelmingly gave begin to conceptually map the typical situations and
the adult more goldfish crackers. The sensitivity to dif- goal states that are associated with different feelings,
ferences in desire among 18-month-olds is consistent such as that blocked goals elicit anger and loss is associ-
with evidence that spontaneous verbal references to de- ated with sadness (Harris, 1989; Stein & Levine, 1989).
sire emerge by 18 months, and somewhat later children This reflects their awareness that both situational and
use contrastive statements about desire (e.g., comparing internal factors are relevant to eliciting emotion (Dunn
what one person wants with what another desires; & Hughes, 1998; Fabes et al., 1991). For example, 3-
Bartsch & Wellman, 1995). year-olds know that feelings are associated with beliefs
By age 2, toddlers can be overheard making sponta- and expectations about events such as the surprise a vis-
neous verbal references to emotions, the causes of emo- itor feels after seeing giraffes on a farm (Wellman &
tion, and even emotion regulatory efforts (e.g., I Banerjee, 1991). Young childrens understanding of the
scared of the shark. Close my eyes at 28 months; connection between emotion and thought is also re-
Bartsch & Wellman, 1995; Bretherton, Fritz, Zahn- vealed in their appreciation that feelings can be evoked
Waxler, & Ridgeway, 1986; Brown & Dunn, 1991; by mental reminders of past emotionally evocative expe-
Dunn, Bretherton, & Munn, 1987; Wellman, Harris, riences. By age 5, for example, children understand that
Banerjee, & Sinclair, 1995). The emergence of expres- someone can feel sad when seeing a cat who reminds her
sive emotion-related utterances is preceded by months of a pet who ran away (Lagattuta, Wellman, & Flavell,
of receptive comprehension of emotion-related dis- 1997; see also Lagattuta & Wellman, 2001). Young chil-
course (Ridgeway, Waters, & Kuczaj, 1985). Careful dren are thus beginning to comprehend the personal and
analysis of the content of young childrens emotion ref- idiosyncratic influences on emotional responding (Dunn
erences has shown that even in their initial utterances, & Hughes, 1998).
Social Understanding 37

As a consequence of these causal understandings, and comparison with other mental states), they are actually
perhaps also because they are more motivated to do so, quite challenging for young children to understand be-
young children better understand the causes of negative cause they are invisible, multidetermined motivators of
than positive emotions they observe in others (Dunn & behavior. Emotions have complex internal causes and
Hughes, 1998; Fabes et al., 1991). Their understanding can be manifested in diverse facial, vocal, and behav-
is limited, however. Young children have considerably ioral expressions that are not always intercoordinated,
greater difficulty understanding how emotions can be which makes understanding the associations between
based on false belief, for example, and it is not until desires, feelings, perceptions, beliefs, and behavior a
about age 6 that they appreciate that someone will feel conceptually daunting task.
delighted before opening a box of candy because she Young children are assisted in developing coherent
thinks it contains chocolates rather than the pebbles her intuitive theories of emotion, however, by their conver-
older brother has substituted (de Rosnay & Harris, sations with adults who label, describe, and explain the
2002; de Rosnay, Pons, Harris, & Morrell, 2004; Harris, causes and consequences of the emotion to them
Johnson, Hutton, Andrews, & Cooke, 1989). Moreover, (Thompson, Laible, & Ontai, 2003). The influence of
consistent with the younger childs straightforward as- these verbal references to emotion begins early: In one
sociation of emotions with specific mental states, it is study, references to feelings by mothers and older sib-
not until middle childhood that children begin to grasp lings when toddlers were 18 months were positively cor-
that multiple emotions of different valence can be expe- related with the childs emotion-related utterances at
rienced simultaneously, and that ambivalence and emo- 24 months (Dunn et al., 1987). With increasing age,
tional equivocation can occur (Harter & Buddin, 1987; emotion-related discussions are integrated into conver-
Wintre & Vallance, 1994). sations of recent events or current experiences, story
Young childrens developing understanding of how to reading, talking about upcoming events, personal story-
manage or regulate their emotions reflects these con- telling, or other conversational forums. The influence of
ceptions of the origins of emotional experience (see re- these conversations on emotion understanding derives
views by Thompson, 1990, 1994). In early childhood, from (a) the growth of language competence that pro-
preschoolers believe that emotion can be managed by vides a lexical foundation to shared understanding of
fleeing, removing, restricting perception of, or ignoring psychological experiences that are otherwise difficult to
emotionally arousing events, revealing an awareness of define, comprehend, or convey to another; and ( b) adult
the connections between emotion, perception, and mind-mindedness that causes them to induct young chil-
thought. Children also recognize that emotion can be dren into the psychological world they inhabit whenever
managed through reassuring self-talk, seeking nurtur- they talk with the child about people. Thus, whenever
ance, ceasing to think about emotionally arousing young children ask why about the feelings and behav-
events, distraction, or other strategies that change the ior they observe in others, they are tutored about the
mental states that contribute to emotional arousal (Har- mental world by adults who cannot help but do so be-
ris, Guz, Lipian, & Man-Shu, 1985; Lagattuta et al., cause psychological understandings of people have be-
1997). Consistent with their developing comprehension come intuitive to mature thinkers. Moreover, language
of the distinction between appearance and reality, older also enables thought about emotional experience outside
preschoolers also begin to understand the value of man- of its immediate context, when young children (and
aging emotional expressions to dissemble ones feelings often their parents) are more capable of thoughtful re-
or protect the feelings of others, and they begin to use flection and discussion. Indeed, language content and
display rules in everyday circumstances (Banerjee, structure has many potentially important influences on
1997; Cole, 1986). The intuitive theories of emotion the growth of psychological understanding in children
that guide young childrens understanding of the origins (see Budwig, 2002, for a review of these), but the essen-
of emotions are also enlisted in their efforts to regulate tial feature of these linguistic contributions is that they
emotional arousal. are also social.
Young children are highly motivated to understand Parent-child conversation about desires, feelings, be-
emotions because their desires and feelings are com- havior, and thought thus helps to organize psychological
pelling experiences and others emotions are salient and understanding through the lexicalization of mental and
significant influences on them. Although desires and emotional life: Words categorize psychological experi-
emotions may seem conceptually simple (especially by ence in ways that provide coherence and a basis for
38 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

shared reference and understanding. They help to make because the former are conceptually more complex and
explicit the implicit knowledge that young children have are also more troubling to the child, and thus there is a
intuited. When preschoolers discuss desires and feelings stronger inherent need to understand, regulate, and/or
with an adult, they also begin to comprehend that the prevent intense negative feelings (Lagattuta & Wellman,
same event can be experienced differently by people 2002). Parents also talk about emotions differently with
who may feel differently about it (Levine, Stein, & daughters than with sons, using more elaboration, reas-
Liwag, 1999). Conversational discourse enables young surance, and a greater relational focus in their emotion-
children to compare their own representation of an expe- related conversations with daughters (Fivush, 1998).
rience with that of the adult, and by comparing primary Research on the influence of parent-child conversa-
and secondary representations (the latter conveyed in tion on the early development of emotion understanding
shared conversation) young children are likely to derive also highlights two other conclusions. First, conversa-
new ways of understanding and thinking about personal tions with adults are not the only important conversa-
experiences. More broadly, conversations about emo- tional catalysts to emotion understanding. Young
tions provide a forum for the transmission of cultural children talk about feelings and thoughts more fre-
values, causal attributions, moral evaluations, and other quently with friends and siblings than they do with their
belief systems of the caregiver that are also part of the mothers (Brown, Donelan-McCall, & Dunn, 1996), and
adults intuitive understanding of the psychological mo- these conversations also contribute significantly to chil-
tivators of peoples behavior. As a consequence, young drens developing understanding of emotion (Hughes &
children learn about emotion in conversations that can Dunn, 1998). Sibling interactions (especially interaction
link emotion to standards of conduct and social aware- with an older sibling) offer unique contexts for the
ness. This may explain why parental conversational ref- growth of emotion understanding, such as in pretend
erences to feelings are a more significant predictor of play that permits animated role taking of feelings and
early conscience development than are parents explicit coping strategies (Dunn et al., 1991; Youngblade &
references to rules (Laible & Thompson, 2000). Dunn, 1995), and sibling conflict that involves negotiat-
Parents who discuss emotions more frequently and ing desires and needs with other family members (Dunn
with greater elaboration, therefore, have children with & Herrera, 1997; N. Howe, Patrakos, & Rinaldi, 1998).
more accurate and richer conceptualizations of emotion These contexts for emotion conversation among co-
(Brown & Dunn, 1996; Denham, Zoller, & Couchard, equals may be even more provocative of developing
1994; Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991; Dunn, Brown, emotion understanding because young children can be
Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991; Fivush, 1993; more direct in conveying their own desires and emotions
Jenkins, Turrell, Kogushi, Lollis, & Ross, 2003; Laible, and their reasons for feeling these ways.
2004a, 2004b; Ontai & Thompson, 2002). There are Second, by contrast with simple constructivist or so-
many elements of elaborated, emotion-related discourse cialization models of the development of knowledge, the
that are likely to provoke young childrens emotion un- growth of emotion understanding derives from an inter-
derstanding, including the adults descriptive state- action of a childs comprehension of psychological real-
ments, explanations of the causes of emotion or its ities with the catalysts of the adults psychological
consequences, linking emotion in another person to the references in shared conversation. Both the childs con-
childs experience, asking questions of children that fur- structivist effort and the adults provocation are impor-
ther their understanding of emotion, and coaching chil- tant, and probably necessary. Young children clearly
dren in strategies of emotion management (Ontai & have powerful inductive capacities for comprehending
Thompson, 2002). The frequency of mothers and chil- psychological states in themselves and others, but to as-
drens emotion references and, in particular, their talk sume that children build theories about mental states in-
about the causes of emotion are especially influential for dependently of the scaffolding of child-parent discourse
the development of emotion understanding (Brown & and other relational incentives may overstate either the
Dunn, 1996; Dunn & Brown, 1993; Dunn, Brown, & insightfulness of the childs inductive inferences or the
Beardsall, 1991; see also Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, clarity of the observational material on which the young
et al., 1991), although more research on this issue is nec- child relies. Considerably more research is needed, how-
essary. Parents and young children tend to discuss nega- ever, to understand how these discourse elements inter-
tive emotions more frequently than positive feelings act with the childs conceptual capabilities and other
Social Understanding 39

social influences in helping children to develop more so- emotion understanding (Cutting & Dunn, 1999; de Ros-
phisticated understanding of the psychological world. nay et al., 2004; Hughes & Dunn, 1998). Both capacities
Because of this, an essential future research task is to rely on an awareness of the subjectivity of psychological
understand the broader network of relational influences states: People can share the same experience but be psy-
that are associated with differences in parent-child con- chologically affected in different ways, thus the poten-
versational discourse about emotion. It seems likely that tial privacy of psychological experience. However, the
individual differences in the richness of adult speech association between emotion understanding and false
about psychological states would be complemented by belief awareness may also derive from their common as-
other affective dimensions of the parent-child relation- sociation with differences in language ability, family
ships. Securely attached children are more advanced in background, or childrens experiences in family rela-
emotion understanding (Laible & Thompson, 1998; Ontai tionships (Cutting & Dunn, 1999; Ruffman, Slade, Row-
& Thompson, 2002; Raikes & Thompson, 2005a), for ex- landson, Rumsey, & Garnham, 2003). As with the
ample, and the broader family emotional climate, the research on conversational discourse and emotion un-
adults emotional expressiveness, and other features of derstanding, for example, many studies have found that
early emotion socialization have important influences on childrens conversations with parents and peers about
young childrens developing emotion understanding mental and emotional themes predict later differences in
(Denham, 1998; Raikes & Thompson, 2005a). The few false belief understanding (Bartsch & Wellman, 1995;
studies that have assessed the importance of emotional Brown et al., 1996; Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991;
influences in the family in relation to conversational dis- Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, et al., 1991; Hughes &
course find that each are important to emotion under- Dunn, 1998; Ruffman, Slade, & Crowe, 2002; Ruffman
standing (e.g., Denham et al., 1994). Further study of this et al., 2003; Sabbagh & Callanan, 1998; Welch-Ross,
question is necessary, however, for understanding how 1995; see generally Astington & Baird, 2005). Sabbagh
developments in young childrens conceptual comprehen- and Callanan (1998) found that when 3- to 5-year-old
sion of emotion are facilitated by language, elements of offspring initiated conversational references to the mind
the family emotional climate, and their interaction. by implicitly contrasting different mental states or say-
ing I dont know, their parents often responded by
Understanding Psychological Characteristics highlighting the representational aspects of mental
and Social Roles states, which commonly elicited further explicit mental
state talk from their children. Other researchers have
After age 3, other significant advances occur in young also found that parents mental state causal language
childrens understanding of the psychological world. (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, et al., 1991) and explicit
Most notably, children develop a more fully representa- mental state discourse are especially important to the
tional view of the mind that incorporates an awareness development of false belief understanding in children
that beliefs can be inconsistent with reality (Wellman, (e.g., Ruffman et al., 2002). A recent training study with
2002; Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001). Young chil- 3-year-olds showed that only training conditions involv-
drens dawning understanding of false belief is signifi- ing language improved childrens subsequent perfor-
cant not only because it reflects an awareness of the mance on false belief tasks, and that language
potential independence of mental events from objective conditions involving both perspective-shifting discourse
reality but also because it is a gateway to the compre- (i.e., discussing mental deception using deceptive ob-
hension of other psychological realities such as the pri- jects, such as a pen that looks like a flower) and syntac-
vacy of personal mental experience, the induction of tic prompts (e.g., sentential complements such as Peter
mistaken beliefs in others, and the minds activity inde- knows that Mommys home) were each independently
pendent of experience (e.g., interpretations, expecta- effective (Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003).
tions). For these reasons, there has been a significant These findings are consistent with the general view
research literature exploring the origins of this develop- that adult discourse about phenomena that interest
mental achievement that is more extensively reviewed in young children is influential in conceptual growth, es-
another chapter of this Handbook. pecially when the phenomena are otherwise elusive or
Individual differences in childrens understanding of difficult for children to comprehend. Harris (in press;
false belief are strongly correlated with differences in Harris & Koenig, 2005) has argued that children accept
40 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

the testimony, or claims, of adults on a wide range of is- social competence in friendship with peers in contem-
sues of importance to them, from understanding the poraneous and longitudinal assessments (Brown et al.,
shape of the earth and other natural phenomena, to the 1996; Denham et al., 2003; Denham, Caverly, et al.,
association between mind and brain and other psycho- 2002; Dunn, 1995; Dunn, Cutting, & Demetriou, 2000;
logical phenomena, to the nature of God, the afterlife, see also Cutting & Dunn, 2002; Schultz, Izard, & Acker-
and other metaphysical phenomena. Children early de- man, 2000). Understanding the features of parent-child
velop understanding of these phenomena based on their interaction and later conversation that contribute to
acceptance of the truthfulness of what they are told but these differences in psychological understandinges-
cannot independently confirm, he argues, and this pecially in the broader context of the emotional climate
knowledge is readily integrated into knowledge systems of the familycan contribute to a better grasp of the in-
based on personal experience. Young children are not fluences that contribute to the growth of interpersonal
passive recipients of this knowledge, of course, because sensitivity in early childhood.
their inquiries about animals, peoples beliefs, or God As with the research on conversation and emotion
provoke the conversations that inform them, and as they understanding, the contexts and partners with whom
attempt to juxtapose their current conceptions with young children share their understandings of the mind
what they learn, childrens comments, queries, and ob- are also important. Children commonly discuss their
jections further guide the discussion. Research on the own feelings and mental states in conversations with
early growth of social and emotional understanding, their mothers, but when talking with peers or siblings
and of mental states, is consistent with this view. both children share their views about mutual interests or
Recent studies indicate that individual differences in concerns in positive, cooperative contexts (Brown et al.,
mental state understanding have surprisingly early ori- 1996; Dunn, 1999) or in negotiation or dispute resolu-
gins. Wellman, Phillips, Dunphy-Lelii, and LaLonde tion (Howe et al., 1998). False belief understanding was
(2004) reported that 14-month-olds who showed greater predicted, in one study, by mental state discourse be-
sensitivity to intentional human activity in a habituation tween siblings and friends involving contrastives (i.e.,
procedure (see Phillips et al., 2002, and described ear- differentiating one persons preferences from an-
lier) were more proficient on a battery of theory of mind others), activity suggestions involving mental terms
tasks at age 4. The source of the continuity over several (e.g., I think Im gonna . . .), and assertions involving
years was unexplained, but relational influences may be mental referents (Brown et al., 1996). In their encoun-
pertinent. Meins and her colleagues (2002) found that 6- ters with peers and siblings, therefore, young children
month-olds whose mothers commented on their actions are likely to encounter discrepancies between their own
in ways that reflected awareness of the babys inten- mental states and those of another, and differences be-
tions, goals, or other psychological states (i.e., mind- tween anothers descriptions of reality and the reality
mindedness) were more advanced on false belief that the child knows.
assessments at age 4. Ruffman, Perner, and Parkin Understanding false belief is complemented by other
(1999) noted that preschoolers false belief understand- advances in psychological understanding in the late pre-
ing was even predicted by mothers use of disciplinary school years. By ages 5 and 6, for example, young chil-
procedures that involved asking the child to reflect on dren begin to perceive others in terms of psychological
the victims feelings. These findings suggest that indi- motives and traits and can predict future behavior on the
vidual differences in social cognitive development basis of the traits they infer, including differences
across the early years are related to the quality of early in ability (Heyman, Gee, & Giles, 2003; Heyman &
relational experience in ways that merit further study. Gelman, 1999, 2000; Yuill & Pearson, 1998). They have
Thus, the preschoolers inductive reasoning about psy- much to learn about traits as psychological entities,
chological experiences in others has developmental an- however, and this is revealed in their optimism concern-
tecedents from early in life. ing the controllability and changeability of traits in
The developmental outcomes of these differences in others that is also reflected in self-perception, as dis-
social cognitive competence are potentially important. cussed later in this chapter (see Lockhart, Chang, &
Dunn, Denham, and their colleagues have found that in- Story, 2002). By age 3 or 4, as discussed later, young
dividual differences in false belief understanding and children distinguish behavioral violations that are moral
emotion understanding each predict young childrens from those that are social conventional, regarding moral
Social Understanding 41

violations as more serious due, in part, to their harm to feelings, punctuating their activity with nonverbal affir-
others (Smetana, 1981, 1997; Smetana & Braeges, mations of goal achievement, and talking with them
1990). In making this distinction, they are tutored about the psychological world they inhabit, they induct
by their mothers who justify moral rules because of the child into the world of the mind through their testi-
their interpersonal consequences (Smetana, Kochanska, mony. Contrary to a long tradition of social cognitive
& Chuang, 2000). Mother-child conversations also research, social cognition is not only the generalization
contribute to young childrens essentialist thinking of intellectual skills that children have independently
about gender differences (Gelman, Taylor, & Nguyen, constructed but also the unique developmental catalysts
2004). Finally, older preschoolers also begin to consider embedded into the everyday experience of social inter-
fairness issues in relation to ingroup-outgroup relations, action from early in life.
particularly associated with gender exclusion, although Second, individual differences in social experience
major advances in their comprehension of social roles are important for differences in early social understand-
and group processes awaits middle childhood (Killen, ing. Differences in infants experience in face-to-face
Pisacane, Lee-Kim, & Ardile-Rey, 2001; Theimer, interaction (such as when mothers are depressed) affect
Killen, & Stanger, 2001). how they respond to the social overtures of other part-
ners and may have more generalized influences on so-
Summary cial expectations. Differences in early parental
sensitivity and mind-mindedness during the 1st year
At least three conclusions emerge from these literatures may be important for how young children begin to com-
that point to future directions in research on early so- prehend the nature of others intentions as they are
cial cognition. gradually constructing a theory of mind. Differences in
First, social experiences are uniquely generative of the content, richness, and structure of parent-child con-
new understanding of people and the psychological versations are important for individual differences in
world. Early infant-caregiver interactions contribute the growth of emotion understanding, comprehension of
fundamentally to the development of generalized social false belief, and other elements of psychological under-
expectations and specific expectations for the behavior standing that are predictive of important dimensions of
of familiar partners. Emotional exchanges in infant- socioemotional competence in the preschool years and
parent interaction contribute to the multimodal discrim- beyond. Although the tenor of research on developing
ination of emotional signals and later, in the context of social cognition (particularly theory of mind) has little
social referencing, understanding of the referentiality of attended to individual differences in these developmen-
emotional cues. Changes in parent-child interaction as- tal processes and their implications, recent research
sociated with the growth of self-produced locomotion shows how important early social influences are for the
may help to foster developing perceptions of the inten- emergence of differences in social expectations, dispo-
tionality and subjectivity of other people. Comprehen- sitions, and beliefs in the early years. Further study
sion of everyday social events is aided by the along these lines is warranted.
organization and structure provided by adult discourse Third, integrating understanding of early social cog-
about these events. Emotion understanding is fostered nitive development from cognitivist perspectives and
not only by everyday emotional interactions between social viewpoints is important to theories of socioper-
young children and their caregivers but also by parent- sonality development, especially those that emphasize
child and peer conversations that embed insight into the the representational dimensions of early relational expe-
psychological worldpeoples desires, feelings, inten- rience. The developmental account of early psychologi-
tions, and thoughtsinto discussions of everyday expe- cal understanding is the account on which a theory
riences. The semantics and structure of such of the development of internal working models de-
conversations also usher young children into a broader rived from early attachments, for example, should be
appreciation of how mental events can be shared or di- based. More generally, such an integrative approach to
vergent, beliefs can be accurate or inaccurate, and psy- further research on early social cognition is likely to
chological experience can be hidden or disclosed. As contribute added insight to the growth of sociability and
parents naturally treat their offspring as psychological the understanding of mind by highlighting how the pow-
beings from infancy, commenting on their intentions and erful inductive capacities of the young childs thinking
42 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

juxtapose with the incentives of social experience to tivity, the emotional climate of the home, the verbal
yield conceptions of the psychological world that are richness of family interaction, incentives for exploratory
rich, informed, and individualized (Dunn, 1996; Harris, competence, expectations for mature behavior, imitative
1997, 1999). learning, conceptual catalysts in parent-child conversa-
tional discourse, parents flexibility and adaptability,
RELATIONSHIPS the use of proactive discipline, processes of negotiation
and bargaining between parent and child, family rou-
Young children develop in an environment of relation- tines and rituals, the effects of physical punishment, the
ships.1 Their experiences over time with people who childs construals of the adults behavior, and the dyads
know them well, and whose characteristics and tenden- attachment security, shared positive affect, emotional
cies children begin to comprehend, are core influences synchrony, and mutual responsiveness (Baumrind, 1973,
on early conceptual and sociopersonality development 1996; Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Grusec, Goodnow, &
(Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Child- Kuczynski, 2000; for general reviews, consult Laible &
hood Development, 2000). This theme runs across the Thompson, in press; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; see also
literatures surveyed in this chapter and in many other Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8; Saarni et al., Chapter 5, this
developmental formulations. These include the view- Handbook, this volume).
points of neo-Vygotskian theorists and other students of The outcomes of these multifaceted relational influ-
cognitive growth, discussed in the previous section, who ences on social and personality development are equally
emphasize relational influences on the construction of diverse and include the development of social skills, so-
early thinking and understanding (e.g., Nelson, 1996; cial expectations, emotion regulation, behavioral self-
Rogoff, 1990). They include research on parent-infant control, relational schemas, self-confidence, trust in
interaction, parent-child relationships, and inquiry into others, social and emotional understanding, conscience
the influence of sibling relationships, peers, and other development, and the enhancement or deterioration
social partners discussed elsewhere in this Handbook of emotional well-being and psychological competence.
(e.g., Dunn, 1993, 2004). They include the work on so- The relational influences of parents, siblings, child-care
cial networks and social support that highlights how re- providers, peers, teachers, extended kin, and others con-
lationships are developmental catalysts and avenues for tribute to these important developmental outcomes.
enhanced knowledge and information, skill acquisition, At the center of this relational network is the parent-
and emotional support through their stress buffering, child relationship, which is important because its influ-
scaffolding of new competencies, social exchange, and ences are unique, comprehensive, ubiquitous, and
other influences from an early age (e.g., Cochran, potentially enduring. Ever since Freuds (1940) famous
Larner, Riley, Gunnarsson, & Henderson, 1990; Thomp- dictum that the infant-mother relationship is unique,
son, 1995; Thompson, Flood, & Goodvin, 2005). Cur- without parallel, established unalterably for a whole
rent work in developmental psychopathology also lifetime as the first and strongest love-object and as the
emphasizes the centrality of close relationships to the prototype of all later love-relations (p. 45), develop-
constellation of risk and protective factors that predict mental theorists have in concert emphasized this rela-
the emergence of child pathology or psychological well- tionship as the foundation of personality growth. In
being (Cicchetti & Cohen, 1995; Cicchetti, Toth, & now-classic formulations, developmentalists like Baum-
Lynch, 1995). Indeed, it is time for developmental scien- rind (1978, 1989) and Hoffman (1983, 2000) integrated
tists to begin integrating these multifaceted perspec- multiple dimensions of warmth, authority, responsive-
tives into a coherent developmental relational science. ness, and demand into parenting patterns that were sig-
Relational processes have been extensively studied in nificantly predictive of the competence and adjustment
early development. In parent-child interaction, these of offspring. More recent perspectives have emphasized
processes include the caregivers warmth, sensitivity, the direct and indirect effects of family members on
and contingent responding, the scaffolding of shared ac- each other (Belsky, 1981), transactional models of fam-
ily influences extended over time (Sameroff & Chan-
1
I am indebted to my colleagues on the National Scientific dler, 1975), the embeddedness of family processes in
Council on the Developing Child for helping me to develop larger social, cultural, and economic systems (Bronfen-
this concept for the Councils working paper, which can be brenner, 1979), and the significance of childrens con-
found at www.developingchild.net. structions of experience in their interactions with
Relationships 43

family members (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Rogoff, mastery, a safe haven in stress or danger, and who con-
1990). Each of these perspectives provides significant tribute to self-regulation in difficult or anxious circum-
continuing catalysts to new thinking about early sociop- stances. Sustained separation from the attachment figure
ersonality development in family relationships. is a source of stress and disruption. Parents are typically
For more than 3 decades, developmental thinking the first and primary attachment figures for infants, but
about parent-child relationships has also been guided by other reliable, enduring caregivers can also become at-
attachment theory (Ainsworth, 1973; Bowlby, 1969/1982, tachment figures such as grandparents, stepparents, or
1973, 1980). It is not difficult to account for its influ- sometimes child-care providers. In light of typical condi-
ence. Attachment theory explores some of the most tions of infant care in the United States and elsewhere,
compelling questions about early sociopersonality devel- multiple attachment relationships are normative, al-
opment and its later consequences. How significant are though the development of such relationships is based not
early experiences (especially in intimate relationships) on the adults role or responsibilities but rather on the
for psychosocial growth? What processes guide continu- nature of the childs expectations for that persons
ity and change in personality characteristics throughout behavior from past experience. Given the functions of at-
life? How are childhood experiences of care linked to tachment figures in early childhood development, occa-
later social relatedness? In what ways do early experi- sional babysitters, older peers, and teachers are unlikely
ences in relationships contribute to psychological vulner- to be attachment figures and, at later ages, close friends
ability and strength? Such questions are central to and romantic partners may assume attachment-like
developmental theory, and the creation and validation of functions but are not attachment figures in the same
the Strange Situation and other assessment procedures sense (compare Ainsworth, 1989 with Hazen & Shaver,
has enabled developmental scientists to investigate these 1994).
questions with growing sophistication and scope. Several Attachment theory offers multilevel explanations for
decades of research on child-parent attachment have why attachments develop in infancy. On a developmental
yielded provisional answers to these central questions of level, attachment emerges from the variety of social cog-
developmental theory, and have yielded more questions nitive advances that enable infants to develop individual-
to ponder. ized expectations for the partners behavior that help to
This section is concerned with theory and research define the affective quality of their relationship. These
on early parent-child attachment, not because attach- advances, discussed in the preceding section, include:
ment theory is the only important theoretical approach
to understanding early relational influences, but because The recognition of the partners face, voice, and
the breadth of its theoretical scope and the body of em- other features
pirical literature it has produced are uniquely generative The growth of expectations for the partners behavior
of new ideas concerning the impact of early parent-child (especially related to distress relief and pleasure in
relationships on sociopersonality development. The con- sociability) that contribute to an affective preference
tinuing vitality of attachment theory will derive, how- for that person
ever, from its inclusion of other conceptual and A developing awareness of the partner as a person
empirical approaches to early relational influences. A (with subjective, mental states, and an intentional
number of recent, authoritative reviews of research in at- stance toward the infant) with whom a relationship
tachment (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999; Colin, 1996) reflect gradually develops
the breadth of developmental, personality, and clinical
research directions inspired by attachment formulations. These and other conceptual achievements contribute
The following review focuses on the early development to the consolidation of initial attachment relationships
of child-parent attachment and its enduring influence. by the first birthday.
Attachment relationships continue to develop after
the first birthday as the child becomes psychologically
Attachment and Its Development
more sophisticated and can regard the partner and the
Attachment can be defined as an enduring affectional tie relationship in more complex ways (Ainsworth, 1990;
that unites one person to another over time and across Crittenden, 2000). In early childhood, for example,
space (Ainsworth, 1973). Attachment figures are a young children increasingly rely on mental representa-
source of security that permit confident exploration and tions of the partners characteristics, especially his or
44 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

her physical and psychological accessibility when chil- including the number of siblings, environmental re-
dren are stressed. Children also acquire, as earlier sources (e.g., food, social support), the adults health
noted, enhanced capacities for understanding the mental and age, and the age, health, and other characteristics of
and emotional perspectives of the partner, comprehend- the child (Blurton-Jones, 1993; Clutton-Brock, 1991;
ing and accommodating to the partners goals and inter- Hrdy, 1999). In some circumstances, maternal with-
ests, and communicating more effectively their own drawal or psychological abandonment may be associated
needs and concerns (Harris, 1997). This development of with an inability to invest adequate nurturance in off-
attachment was described by Bowlby (1969/1982, 1973) spring. This is consistent with the report of Valenzuela
as the goal-corrected partnership. In later years, this (1990, 1997) that 93% of a sample of chronically under-
partnership becomes more complex, mutual, and dy- nourished, low-income toddlers were insecurely at-
namic as the children mature psychologically. In middle tached (compared with 50% of a comparison sample of
childhood, children understand relationships to be based adequately nourished, low-income toddlers) and that
on psychological sharing between partners, enduring de- their mothers were significantly less sensitive than the
spite conflict, and children begin to explicitly conceptu- mothers of adequately nourished toddlers during obser-
alize relational processes for the first time while vations with the child. It is also consistent, unfortu-
seeking psychological support as well as physical prox- nately, with high rates of infanticide in circumstances
imity to their attachment figures (Raikes & Thompson, and cultures characterized by resource deprivation and
2005b). They may also begin deriving security from the other obstacles to adequate early care (Hrdy, 1999). In
broader network of family relationships they share as more typical conditions, there is likely to be greater ma-
well as specific parent-child relationships (Davies & ternal investment but also significant differences be-
Cummings, 1994; Davies & Forman, 2002). In adoles- tween the mothers willingness to invest in a child
cence, attachment relationships are transformed by a (through nurturance and attention) and the childs de-
young persons efforts to clarify and differentiate self mands for further investment, such as when weaning
from others, reflect on complex abstract realities (such conflicts occur (Bateson, 1994; Trivers, 1985). In these
as the nature of human relationships), and develop ca- circumstances, the mothers insensitivity and rejection
pacities for emotional reflection and self-regulation are as biologically adaptive for her as are the childs ef-
(Allen & Land, 1999). Attachment relationships de- forts to entice greater nurturance adaptive for the child.
velop, in short, with the childs developing psychologi- Parental solicitude is, in short, a biologically contingent
cal sophistication. phenomenon, with maternal insensitivity and child-
On an ethological or evolutionary level, attachments parent conflict not only normative but also biologically
are believed to have evolved to promote infant survival adaptive at times in light of the different fitness consid-
(and inclusive fitness) by maintaining the protective erations of each partner.
proximity of adults, especially in conditions of alarm or In addition to developmental and ethological per-
danger. Seeking physical closeness to a caregiver helped spectives, Bowlbys (1969/1982, 1973, 1980) theory in-
to ensure (in the environment of evolutionary adapted- cluded two other levels of explanation for the
ness) that infants were protected and were not lost or development of attachment relationships that contribute
abandoned, and that they would also be nurtured and to the conceptual richness of attachment theory. First,
could learn from the adult behavior they observed until he borrowed concepts from cybernetic control systems
they reached maturity. theory to explain the flexible organization of specific
Complementary biologically based motivational sys- attachment behaviors into a behavioral system charac-
tems fostering nurturance in adults are also believed to terized by continuous goal-correctedness, hierarchical
have arisen from this evolutionary legacy (Thompson organization, and the functional interrelations among
et al., 2005). But the inclusive fitness considerations of specific behaviors. Thus, attachment develops as a be-
the mother are more complicated than those of the infant havioral system when the child has psychologically ma-
because maternal energy, time, and other resources tured sufficiently that the functional goals underlying
must be divided between the needs of several offspring the system (e.g., protective proximity to a caregiver) can
and the mother herself, including her survival and fu- organize specific attachment behaviors (e.g., reaching,
ture reproductive potential. From a biological perspec- locomotion to the adult, or crying). This functionalist
tive, maternal solicitude is contingent on many factors, approach to behavioral organization has been an impor-
Relationships 45

tant contribution to assessing attachment in the Strange lings similar) and nonshared environment (influences
Situation and other procedures. Second, Bowlbys de- that make siblings different) was stronger than evidence
velopmental theory was also significantly influenced by for genetic influences in explaining differences in at-
his psychoanalytic orientation. His concept of the in- tachment security.
ternal working model of self and attachment figure, Attachment theory portrays individual differences in
arising from early relational experience and coloring the security of attachment as the outcome of variations
later relationships, is similar to central features of ob- in maternal sensitivity to the infant during the 1st year.
ject relations theory. In addition, his formulations con- Moreover, differences in attachment security are be-
cerning unconscious defensive processes in children, the lieved to influence emergent features of social, emo-
influence of inconsistent mental representations arising tional, and personality development in the years that
from different experiences of care, and the importance follow. But in this general formulation there are a num-
of the therapist as an attachment figure all derive from ber of specific issues related to the interpretation of dif-
his orientation as a psychoanalytic therapist. These in- ferences in the security of attachment and their broader
fluences contribute to the depth of attachment formula- significance. The following four receive particular at-
tions although, like many concepts deriving from the tention here: (1) the concept of security as definitional
psychoanalytic tradition, their heuristic power is ac- of the child-parent relationship, (2) the developmental
companied by some conceptual ambiguity and difficulty transition from security as relationship specific to secu-
in assessment. rity as a personal attribute, (3) the integration of multi-
ple relational experiences into attachment security, and
Differences in Attachment Security (4) the association between the security of attachment
and psychological development.
Attachment theory is important as a normative theory of First, to what extent is the security of attachment de-
the development of early relationships, but the majority finitive of the parent-child relationship? Are there im-
of research attention has been devoted to individual dif- portant features of this relationship that are outside the
ferences in the security of attachment and their broader scope of attachment? Bowlby believed that even in in-
influence. The characterization of these differences in fancy attachment is only one of several dimensions of
terms of security is consistent with Bowlbys ethologi- the parent-child relationship and is supplemented by
cal view of the protective functions of attachment rela- their complementary roles in feeding, play, instruction,
tionships, and with Ainsworths observations of the and other activities that are guided by other behavioral
importance of maternal sensitivity to the infants emo- systems. The parents skill as a playmate or teacher does
tional well-being. Moreover, the concept of security is not necessarily have consequences for the attachment
also consistent with other well-known characterizations system. Moreover, there exists a rich literature describ-
of early psychosocial growth (especially the Eriksonian ing other features of parent-infant relationships that un-
concept of basic trust versus mistrust in infancy) and derscore the importance of parental teaching and
recasts the meaning of infant behaviors earlier described guidance, the intellectual richness of the home environ-
as dependent in a more positive, psychologically con- ment, and the adults sensitivity and responsiveness in
structive light. Although attachment as a species-typical fostering the childs conceptual and language develop-
phenomenon has biological origins, individual differ- ment (see Dunn, 1993, and Bornstein, 2002, for re-
ences in the security of attachment do not appear to have views). Despite this, few researchers have sought to
strong genetic foundations. Three recent studiestwo study the development of parent-child relationships
large twin studies of infants (Bokhorst et al., 2003) and more inclusively, such as by exploring how the emer-
preschool children (OConnor & Croft, 2001) and the gence of attachment security intersects with other rela-
third a study of the concordance of foster infants at- tional influences. This would be a valuable goal in light
tachment security with the foster mothers attachment of recent evidence that the security of attachment mod-
states of mind (Dozier, Stovall, Albus, & Bates, erates the influence on the child of parenting practices
2001)together suggest that nongenetic processes are such as discipline approach (Kochanska, Aksan,
predominant in the development of secure or insecure Knaack, & Rhines, 2004) and maternal conversational
attachments. Evidence for the influence of both shared discourse (Thompson et al., 2003). In addition, under-
environment (environmental influences that make sib- standing the developing dynamics of parent-infant
46 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

relationships may provide added insight into the origins multiple, perhaps somewhat inconsistent, representa-
of attachment security. As earlier described, at the same tions of self and relationships if their security varies
time that the security of attachment is emerging during with different attachment partners? Or are these repre-
the 1st year, the quality of the parent-child relationship sentations integrated or harmonized in some way? Over
is also being influenced by the growth of self-produced the years, there have been different ways of responding
locomotion and the conflict of wills that occurs as in- to this issue. Attachment relationships are believed by
fants become more agentic and goal directed. How par- some to be hierarchical in influence (with mother-child
ents manage conflict with the child may be important, attachments primary), while others believe that attach-
along with sensitive responsiveness in other contexts, to ment security affects psychosocial growth in a domain-
the development of security in offspring. specific fashion (such that maternal attachments
Second, attachment theorists agree that with growing influence different aspects of sociopersonality develop-
maturity, attachment security becomes increasingly an ment than do relationships with fathers or child-care
attribute of the person, rather than of a specific relation- providers; see, e.g., Main & Weston, 1981; Oppenheim,
ship. In infancy and early childhood, we typically think Sagi, & Lamb, 1988). At present, however, neither em-
of children as secure or insecure with respect to a spe- pirical evidence nor theory offers a clarified picture.
cific caregiver; in adolescence and adulthood, we com- Fourth, why should attachment security be related to
monly think of secure or insecure persons. But how and other features of psychological development? Thought-
why does this developmental transition occur? Investi- ful theoretical attention to this question should guide re-
gating this theoretically crucial question is impaired, to search into the sequelae of early attachment security
some extent, by assessment procedures: Measures of at- and enable researchers to interpret expected and unex-
tachment security for older persons have predominantly pected associations between attachment and later be-
incorporated the assumption that attachment styles or havior. Attachment researchers have been guided,
states of mind are characteristic of the person, and re- however, by a broad expectation that secure attachment
searchers have rarely considered whether adolescents or predicts more positive social and personality function-
adults also maintain relationship-specific forms of secu- ing. Empirically, this has resulted in a large research lit-
rity or insecurity with particular partners. Yet, when the erature in which attachment has been studied in relation
findings of studies using different procedures for assess- to a dizzying variety of later outcomes, including cogni-
ing attachment security are compared, there is evidence tive and language development; frustration tolerance;
for both security as a relationship-specific quality and self-recognition; behavior problems; relations with
security as a personal attribute in studies of children in peers, friends, and siblings; interactions with unfamiliar
middle childhood and adolescence (see Raikes & adults; exploration and play; competence in preschool
Thompson, 2005b, and contributors to Kerns & Richard- and kindergarten classrooms; curiosity; ego resiliency;
son, 2005). This suggests that security as a personal at- and math achievement (see Thompson, 1999, and fol-
tribute may develop over an extended period as lowing). As Belsky and Cassidy (1994) asked, one might
personality development becomes influenced by the rep- wonder if there is anything to which attachment security
resentational systems inspired by multiple attachment re- is not related.
lationships throughout childhood and youth. But these Why has there been a search for so many diverse se-
findings also raise another question. Is it possible that quelae of attachment security? One reason is that attach-
both relationship-specific and person-specific features ment theory provides a conceptual umbrella for broad
of attachment security coexist in the attachment-related and narrow constructions of the developmental impact
representational systems that exist in adulthood? of attachment relationships. The most narrow view, and
Third, how are multiple attachment relationships de- the one that is best supported by empirical evidence, is
velopmentally influential (Thompson, 2005)? Attach- that security of attachment should predict the childs
ment theorists agree that in infancy and at later ages, later trust and confidence in the attachment figure and
attachments commonly develop with more than one other close relational partners. Waters, Kondo-Ikemura,
caregiver, and the security of these relationships is inde- Posada, and Richters (1991) have broadened this view
pendent of the others. How do the expectations arising with their argument that because attachment security in-
from multiple attachments become integrated into co- dexes the continuing harmony of the parent-child rela-
herent ways of relating to others, representing relation- tionship, a variety of socialization outcomes should
ships, and self-understanding? Do children acquire result from attachment security related to identification,
Relationships 47

imitation, learning, cooperation and compliance, and search conceived under the umbrella of many different
prosocial motivation. A yet broader perspective is that conceptions of attachment outcomes risks both theoreti-
attachment security should foreshadow cognitive com- cal obscurantism and holding attachment theory ac-
petence, exploratory skill, and communication style countable for formulations it should not and perhaps
through its effects on the childs self-confidence, initia- cannot embrace (Sroufe, 1988).
tive, and other broader personality processes, together
with the support afforded by continuing sensitive par-
Security of Attachment in the Strange Situation
enting. Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, and Carlson (1999)
have further proposed that attachment influences later The Strange Situation has been an empirical and a con-
development as it affects (a) neurodevelopment, ( b) af- ceptual anchor for attachment research because of the
fect regulation, (c) behavioral regulation and relational careful validational work of Ainsworth and her follow-
synchrony, and (d) early representations (e.g., the inter- ers (see Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978, for
nal working models proposed by Bowlby and discussed procedural and coding details). By linking detailed lon-
later). Although they argue that attachment relation- gitudinal observations of the secure-base behavior of in-
ships should be most strongly predictive of sequelaelike fants at home with patterns of attachment in the Strange
psychological adjustment, interpersonal competence, Situation, Ainsworth demonstrated that a straightfor-
and self-understanding, it is easy to see how a much ward 20-minute laboratory procedure could capture im-
wider variety of outcomes can be encompassed in the portant and reliable dimensions of relational security in
four sources of influence they describe. Adding further infancy. As evidence accumulated for the moderate sta-
complexity is the view (now current in evolutionary biol- bility and predictive validity of attachment classifica-
ogy and behavioral ecology) that different attachment tions derived from the Strange Situation, a large body of
patterns are each evolved behavioral strategies that are research was generated to explore the origins, corre-
adapted to different conditions of environmental re- lates, and sequelae of individual differences in attach-
sources and parental solicitude (see Chisholm, 1999). ment using this procedure. To be sure, the reliance on a
Whether attachment patterns predict adaptive or mal- single attachment assessment had disadvantages: The
adaptive later behavior depends, in part, on whether the identity of the security of attachment construct with
environmental conditions characterizing early develop- Strange Situation behavior made it impossible to exam-
ment endure or change over the childs life. ine how prior experiences might affect Strange Situation
Whether conceptualized in a developmental or evolu- behavior independently of attachment security (Lamb,
tionary framework, theoretical clarity concerning the Thompson, Gardner, & Charnov, 1985). But the reliance
association between attachment security and psycholog- on a single procedure also enabled researchers to inte-
ical development is essential. When attachment re- grate a wide variety of research findings because each
searchers are unclear or disagree over the hypotheses used the same assessment. The Strange Situation has
that can be reasonably derived from the theory, it is dif- also had broader significance for attachment research.
ficult to determine whether empirical findings confirm, Attachment assessments for older children and adults
disconfirm, or do not directly address theoretical claims are often validated by showing that they yield classifica-
at all. As a consequence, both convergent and discrimi- tions that are longitudinally consistent with earlier
nate validities are obscured. Moreover, theoretical pre- Strange Situation classifications, and attachment re-
cision is necessary to guide expectations for whether the searchers still rely on adaptations of the threefold ( later
association of attachment with other psychological de- fourfold) Strange Situation classification categories
velopments will be strong or weak, direct or mediated, when they are studying attachment in older children,
moderated by other variables, or nonexistent. Once ex- adolescents, or adults.
pectations are clear, then unexpected relations between The strategy of the Strange Situation is to create
attachment security and other variables can be exam- conditions of moderately escalating stress to activate
ined more incisively (e.g., by exploring for mediating the attachment behavioral system of 1-year-olds. Based
variables). The need for theoretical clarity is perhaps on the infants behavior throughout the procedure, but
the most important challenge facing attachment theory especially during reunions with the partner after brief
and research in the next decade of its development separation episodes, an attachment classification is as-
(Thompson & Raikes, 2003). Attempting to bootstrap signed. Infants who are considered securely attached
theory development on the findings of empirical re- (Group B) organize their behavior around the caregiver
48 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

as a secure base throughout the procedure and show of infants who had been maltreated, were growing up in
fairly unequivocal pleasure at the adults return. Infants difficult family conditions, or were otherwise at risk for
who are insecure-avoidant (Group A) show relatively lit- later problems, infants in the disorganized/disoriented
tle secure-base behavior and exhibit avoidance of the group are often found in nonclinical middle-class sam-
partner during reunions either by failing to greet or de- ples, although in widely varying rates, with one meta-
laying in greeting the adult. Infants who are insecure- analytic estimate that 15% of the infants from
resistant (Group C) also show little secure-base behav- middle-class samples are classified in the D group (van
ior during preseparation episodes (during which they re- Ijzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenberg,
main preoccupied with the adult) and mingle their 1999). By contrast, roughly 25% of the infants in lower-
efforts to achieve proximity to the caregiver during income samples are in the D group, with much higher
reunions with angry resistance. Although these dual in- proportions in some clinical samples. In low-risk,
secure groups are different in their behavioral charac- middle-class samples the secondary classification for
teristics, understanding the distinctive origins and D-group infants is predominantly secure, while in
sequelae of these groups has been hindered by the long- higher-risk samples the secondary classification is more
standing tendency of researchers to combine avoidant commonly insecure (Lyons-Ruth & Jacobvitz, 1999;
and resistant classifications in their analysis, together Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, McLeod, & Silva, 1991).
with the enlistment of sample sizes that are too small to Why do infants become disorganized or disoriented
permit reliable comparisons between these groups. in the Strange Situation? In a return to Bowlbys clini-
Although these three groups constituted the extent of cal interests in the enduring effects of early trauma,
the classification options for Strange Situation research Main and her colleagues (Main & Hesse, 1990; Main &
for many years, a new insecure classification category Solomon, 1990) have argued that infant disorganization
emerged in the late 1980s as the result of difficulties in develops in response to the frightening or frightened
appropriately characterizing the attachment behavior of behavior of the caregiver, which can occur when the
certain infants, especially those in at-risk conditions. adult has an unresolved personal history of traumatic or
Main and Solomon (1986, 1990) created the classifica- frightening experiences, especially when memories of
tion of disorganized/disoriented (Group D) to describe these experiences are evoked by current circumstances
infants who, for a time, appear to lack an organized, co- (e.g., domestic violence). When caregivers act this way,
herent strategy for interacting with the caregiver in the it puts the infant in the terrible paradox of fearing the
Strange Situation. This can be manifested in many ways, person from whom they must also find comfort in
most notably in contradictory behavior (e.g., strong stress, and disorganized behavior can be the result. In
avoidance combined with strong contact-seeking), but support of this view, the incidence of infant disorgan-
also in undirected, incomplete, or interrupted move- ized attachment is much higher in samples character-
ments, inexplicable freezing or stilling, stereotyped or ized by sociodemographic risk, especially child
other anomalous postures, apparent fear of the adult, maltreatment (in which the parent necessarily acts in a
and other indications of disorganization or disorienta- frightening manner). However, parental depression or
tion. These behaviors can be fleeting and initially diffi- marital discord is not necessarily associated with in-
cult to detect, although with training and experience creased frequency of infant disorganization, suggesting
reliable assignment of the D classification can be ac- that the conditions of family risk that are most genera-
complished. In a sense, infants in the D classification tive of the D classification are those that most directly
are distinct from those in both the secure classification imperil infant-parent relationships and the childs
(group B)because infants are distinctly insecure emotional well-being (e.g., Barnett, Ganiban, & Cic-
and from the two insecure (groups A and C) classifica- chetti, 1999; Carlson, 1998; see Lyons-Ruth & Jacob-
tions, because infants are disorganized rather than vitz, 1999, and van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999 for reviews).
exhibiting an organized (albeit insecure) behavioral There is also a significant association between parental
strategy. Even so, classification as D is often accompa- classification as unresolved/disorganized in the
nied by a secondary assignment to one of the three or- Adult Attachment Interview and infant disorganized/
ganized attachment groups reflecting a best fitting disoriented attachment, which is important because of
alternative classification. Although the D classification the belief that this adult state of mind reflects contin-
originated in efforts to describe the attachment behavior ued difficulty over past experiences of trauma or loss
Relationships 49

(Hesse, 1999; van Ijzendoorn, 1995).2 The more impor- inductively from two small longitudinal follow-up stud-
tant question, however, is whether the parents stresses ies of infants earlier deemed disorganized/disoriented
and attachment state of mind are manifested in fright- (Main & Cassidy, 1988, N = 12; Wartner, Grossmann,
ening or frightened conduct in the presence of the Fremmer-Bombik, & Suess, 1994, N = 13), there is no
infant. The few studies that have directly addressed clear theoretical explanation for why children who are
this question have revealed a modest but inconsistent so distinctly disorganized in infancy should become
association between maternal frightening behavior and preschoolers who are so organized that they seek to con-
attachment disorganization in infants, often depending trol the caregivers behavior. The extent to which this
on the form of disorganization the infant exhibits reflects sequelae of disorganized attachment, changes in
(Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, & Parsons, 1999; Schuengel, parent-child interaction, psychological development in
Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van Ijzendoorn, 1999). This the child, or the influence of other variables is still
suggests that further research is necessary to fully elu- being explored. This developmental transition remains
cidate the origins of infant disorganized/disoriented at- an empirical and conceptual challenge for attachment
tachment in the infant-caregiver relationship. theory and research and, together with the need for a
The D classification in infancy is distinguished from better understanding of the origins of infant disorgani-
the other attachment classifications because it is not an zation, suggests the urgent need for further prospective
organized strategy. But surprisingly, when attachment longitudinal research on these issues.
security is assessed in separation-reunion procedures in Taken together, in typical, nonclinical middle-class
preschoolers, the manifestations of disorganization for samples, approximately 62% of infants are deemed se-
most children appear to be highly organized in the form cure, 15% avoidant, 9 to 10% resistant, and the remain-
of controlling strategies for managing and regulating ing 15% disorganized. The proportion of insecure and
mother-child interaction (Moss, Bureau, Cyr, Mongeau, disorganized groups is larger in lower-income samples,
& St.-Laurent, 2004; Teti, 1999). A variety of control- clinical groups, and families at sociodemographic risk
ling (group D) subgroups in preschoolers, including (van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999). Multiple classification
controlling-caregiving and controlling-punitive strate- subgroups associated with each category reflect consid-
gies, reflect different behavioral and affective ap- erable variation on each classification theme, but little
proaches to the caregiver. Because these categories for research has been devoted to understanding these differ-
classifying disorganization in preschoolers were derived ences. There have also been challenges to the suitability
of the Strange Situation as an attachment assessment for
2
infants with distinct experiential backgrounds, such as
Although findings such as these are commonly interpreted
those with substantial experience in child care, which
as supporting the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment
highlight the importance of understanding the back-
Interview (AAI ), they should be interpreted cautiously be-
cause the AAI was developed explicitly to predict infant at- grounds of infants in the Strange Situation when inter-
tachment classifications. The AAI was developed from a preting their responses to the separation episodes and
sample of interview responses of parents for whom the at- encounters with a stranger (Clarke-Stewart, Goossens,
tachment classifications of their infant offspring from & Alhusen, 2001).
Strange Situation assessments conducted several years earlier Attachment researchers have long recognized the
were known (Hesse, 1999). The AAI coding and classifica- analytic limitations of a categorical outcome measure
tion system were developed by searching for commonalities like attachment classification and, over the years, have
in the interview responses of parents whose infants shared proposed modifications or adaptations of the classifi-
the same attachment classification. This helps to explain why cation system to permit continuous scores (e.g., Gard-
AAI classifications so closely parallel infant Strange Situa-
ner, Lamb, Thompson, & Sagi, 1986; M. Lamb et al.,
tion classifications and why many researchers have reported
1985; Richters, Waters, & Vaughn, 1988) or dimen-
a correspondence between parental AAI groups and their in-
fants Strange Situation classifications. Predicting infant at-
sional approaches to assessing attachment security
tachment status was what the AAI was originally created to (e.g., Waters & Deane, 1985). More recently, Fraley
accomplish. But documentation of the predictive validity of and Spieker (2003) have proposed that attachments are
the AAI in this manner is not the same thing as if the adult at- fundamentally ordered along two continua (proximity-
tachment assessment had been developed completely indepen- seeking versus avoidance and high versus low anger/re-
dently of knowledge of the attachment security of offspring. sistance) and have urged attachment researchers to use
50 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

a dimensional rather than a taxonomic approach to indexed. The Cassidy-Marvin procedure focuses on
studying differences in attachment security. Although body position, affect, speech, gaze, and physical prox-
the use of multiple continua can have important advan- imity and contact, whereas Crittendens classification
tages in attachment research, the Fraley and Spieker procedure also encompasses affect regulation and open
analysis is limited because they excluded infants in the communication with the parent. The Cassidy-Marvin
D classification, rendering their conclusions of limited procedure is widely used, and individual differences in
applicability in light of growing interest in infant disor- security assessed in this procedure are modestly but
ganization. More generally, a dimensional strategy reliably associated with prior measures of maternal
would require far more than two continua to capture sensitivity and responsiveness and are also modestly
the richness of the organizational approach to attach- associated with infant Strange Situation classifica-
ment assessment and its sequelae, and it requires fur- tions3 (Barnett, Kidwell, & Leung, 1998; Moss, Bu-
ther research to determine whether a dimensional reau, Cyr, Mongeau, & St.-Laurent, 2004; Moss, Cyr,
approach can do so without undermining many of the Bureau, Tarabuley, & Dubois-Comtois, 2004; National
other advantages of this approach. Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Early Child Care Research Network, 2001; Stevenson-
Hinde & Shouldice, 1995). But by contrast with their
Other Behavioral Assessments of careful attention to the standardized use of the Strange
Attachment Security Situation with infants, attachment researchers have
tended to modify the procedure and scoring conven-
As children mature, attachment assessments must also tions of the Cassidy-Marvin procedure in different
change to accommodate the childs developing behav- studies, sometimes using the Strange Situation, some-
ioral sophistication. Two other behavioral assessments times extending the separation episodes and elim-
of attachment security have been developed for inating episodes with the stranger, and sometimes in-
preschoolers (representational assessments are dis- cluding other assessments in the midst of the procedure
cussed later in the section on internal working models). (e.g., Moss, Bureau, Cyr, Mongeau, & St.-Laurent,
Each has presented attachment researchers with the 2004; National Institute of Child Health and Human
challenge of mapping heterotypic continuity in attach- Development Early Child Care Research Network,
ment security: How can age-appropriate manifestations 2001; Stevenson-Hinde & Shouldice, 1995). This
of a secure attachment be identified that capture the
same attachment construct as is assessed in the infant 3
It is important to note that the classification of preschool at-
Strange Situation (see Solomon & George, 1999)?
tachment behavior in the Cassidy and Marvin (1992) and
One approach is the Cassidy and Marvin (1992) pro-
Main and Cassidy (1988) assessments is based on procedures
cedure for preschoolers (i.e., 3- to 5-year-olds), based created explicitly to identify early childhood correlates of
on an earlier approach by Main and Cassidy (1988) for infant attachment classifications. In a procedure resembling
6-year-olds, which focuses on reunions with the parent the development of the Adult Attachment Interview, Main
after one or more separations. Preschool attachment and Cassidy (1988) created the preschool attachment cate-
classification categories closely parallel those of gories in an iterative process involving samples of young
the Strange Situation. A similar separation-reunion children for whom their attachment classifications in infancy
procedure by Crittenden (1992, 1994; see also were known throughout measurement development. By
Crittenden, 2000) uses somewhat different classifica- searching for commonalities in the preschool separation-
tion categories for older children, including secure, reunion behavior of children who, as infants, shared the same
insecure-defended, insecure-coercive, and other inse- attachment classification, the close parallel between infant
Strange Situation and preschool attachment classifications
cure groups. Each approach borrows the strategy of the
was ensured. However, this approach makes the consistency
Strange Situation that preschoolers attachment organi-
between infant and preschool classifications less impressive
zation is activated by the stress of separations from the than if preschool classifications had been derived indepen-
caregiver, and sometimes separation episodes are dently, and because it is based on inductive rather than
lengthened to better ensure that this occurs for older deductive procedures, this approach to measurement devel-
children. Although they are similar, the two ap- opment also creates theoretical challenges (e.g., explaining
proaches differ from each other, and from the Strange why infants who are deemed disorganized become highly
Situation coding procedures, in how secure behavior is strategic, controlling preschoolers).
Relationships 51

makes it difficult to know how comparable the findings The AQS is suitable for use with children from 1 to 5
are from the different variations of the procedure and years of age.
how far the validity evidence can extend to significant A meta-analysis of research using the AQS by
alterations of the Strange Situation. van Ijzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-Kranenburg, and
A different strategy for assessing attachment security Riksen-Walraven (2004) showed that the average secu-
in preschoolers is the Attachment Q-Sort (AQS; Waters rity score for nonclinical samples was .32, with an aver-
& Deane, 1985). Based on extensive home observations, age score of .21 in clinical samples. With a theoretical
a well-trained observer or the mother sorts 90 descrip- range of security scores ( like correlation coefficients)
tive statements into nine groups based on how ac- from 1.00 to +1.00, this is consistent with Strange Sit-
curately each statement describes the child. This distri- uation evidence that most infants are secure, but that
bution is then correlated with a criterion sort to yield a there is variability in security. They also reported that
correlation coefficient that is the childs security score. AQS security scores were moderately associated with
The AQS seeks to describe secure base behavior at home security assessed in the Strange Situation (combined ef-
rather than provoking attachment behavior in the labora- fect size .23) and with measures of maternal sensitivity
tory, based on an effort to directly assess the secure (effect size .31), but were also negatively associated
base behavior that is, to some attachment theorists, the with assessments of temperamental reactivity (effect
gold standard of any attachment assessment (Waters & size .27), conclusions that are consistent with narrative
Cummings, 2000). Consequently, children are observed reviews of this literature (e.g., Thompson, 1998). These
under a variety of conditions, but inevitably less often in findings were consistent for security scores derived
circumstances that deliberately heighten the activation from observers and from maternal report, although van
of attachment behavior. The criteria for secure attach- Ijzendoorn and his colleagues (2004) concluded that re-
ment are thus broader than for Strange Situation-based search findings better support the validity of observer
procedures. In addition to secure base behavior, for sorts. Their conclusion is consistent with the greater
example, items that are high in the security criterion likelihood of report bias from mothers, but Teti and Mc-
sort include: Gourty (1996) have delineated procedures designed to
minimize this influence, and maternal sorts may be
Child follows mothers suggestions readily, even more valid with the training and supervision they sug-
when they are clearly suggestions rather than orders. gest (the meta-analysis did not distinguish maternal-
Child uses mothers facial expressions as a good report studies employing the Teti and McGourty proce-
source of information when something looks risky or dures from those that did not).
threatening. Do these behavioral assessments capture the same at-
Child recognizes when mother is upset. Becomes tachment construct that is assessed in the infant Strange
quiet or upset himself. Tries to comfort her. Situation procedure? The careful design of these mea-
Child is strongly attracted to new activities and new sures, their predicted associations with differences in
toys. maternal sensitivity, and their modest associations with
infant Strange Situation classifications each suggest that
By incorporating into the security criterion sort their shared variance indexes a consistent attachment
many of the hypothesized correlates of attachment secu- construct. However, differences in external correlates
rity (such as the childs obedience, social referencing, (such as temperament), operationalizations of security,
empathy, and exploratory interest) the AQS enlists a and measurement strategy each indicate that these as-
much broader operationalization of attachment security sessments capture significant sources of independent
that is perhaps better suited to a home observational variance as well. This is perhaps inevitable in light of
measure, in contrast with the more narrow focus on se- the challenges of mapping heterotypic continuity in be-
cure base behavior of the laboratory separation-reunion havior during a period of rapid developmental change.
procedures. The AQS is also an assessment of security But this means that attachment theorists are wise to be
alone; there are no consistent procedures for distin- cautious in generalizing findings across research studies
guishing secure from insecure attachments on the con- using different measures of the security of attachment.
tinuous security score, nor does the procedure yield As one illustration, a recent report from the National In-
differentiated forms of insecurity such as those pro- stitute of Child Health and Human Development
vided by assessments based on the Strange Situation. (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care found different
52 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

associations between mother- and caregiver-reported or the Cassidy-Marvin procedure is used to assess at-
child behavior problems at age 3 and attachment assess- tachment. Paternal sensitivity is also reliably associated
ments at 15 months (using the Strange Situation), with security of attachment, but more weakly than for
24 months (using the AQS), and 36 months (using the mothers (van Ijzendoorn & De Wolff, 1997), and sensi-
Cassidy-Marvin procedure), and there was very modest tivity is also a predictor of security with nonparental
consistency in security and disorganization scores de- caregivers (Howes, 1999). A meta-analytic review of
rived from these attachment assessments at each age the results of intervention studies designed to improve
(McCartney, Owen, Booth, Clarke-Stewart, & Vandell, maternal sensitivity concluded that carefully designed
2004). As we shall see, the interpretive cautions of gen- interventions could be effective in increasing sensitive
eralizing across attachment assessments are also re- responsiveness, especially when they were relatively
quired when generalizing to representational measures short, behaviorally focused programs. Moreover, these
of attachment in early childhood and later years. interventions also had a small but significant effect in
enhancing the security of attachment, supporting the
Origins of Attachment Security causal role of maternal sensitivity in fostering attach-
ment security (Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzen-
To attachment theorists, the caregivers sensitivity to doorn, & Juffer, 2003). Parental sensitivity is an
the infant is the adults core contribution to the develop- important and reliable but modest predictor of the secu-
ment of a secure attachment. Sensitivity is a broad con- rity of attachment.
ceptual rubric for the quality of adult caregiving that has De Wolff and van Ijzendoorn (1997) concluded that
diverse consequences for offspring, and it can have dif- other dimensions of parenting are also important in fos-
ferent meanings in different theoretical traditions. In tering security, and suggested that researchers look to
Vygotskian theory, for example, sensitivity entails the the contexts of parent-child interaction for clues about
careful scaffolding of shared activity to foster concep- these influences. Attachment researchers have re-
tual growth within the childs readiness for new chal- sponded to their suggestion. Not surprisingly, they have
lenges (Rogoff, 1990), while a learning theorist would found that the caregivers psychological attributes are
emphasize the construction of environmental contingen- predictive of attachment security. In the NICHD Study
cies that foster adaptive behavior. To attachment theo- of Early Child Care, for example, the mothers of se-
rists, sensitivity consists of a constellation of response curely attached infants were higher than mothers of inse-
attributes that includes attention to the infants signals, cure infants on a composite of measures of psychological
accurate interpretation of their meaning, and appropri- adjustment that indexed depression, neuroticism, and
ate and prompt responsiveness to promote the infants anxiety (each reverse scored), sociability, extraversion,
trust in the caregiver (Ainsworth, 1973; Ainsworth and other variables (NICHD Early Child Care Research
et al., 1978). Empirically, sensitivity tends to be opera- Network, 1997). Attachment researchers have also ex-
tionalized in ways that also include caregiver warmth, plored other psychological resources of the mother that
cooperation, interactional synchrony, and other related might foster secure attachment. Meins has reported that
processes (Belsky, 1999; De Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, maternal mind-mindedness, which describes mothers
1997; Thompson, 1998). Bowlby himself characterized tendencies to impute mental and psychological states to
sensitivity as respect for the child. their infant offspring, is associated with sensitive re-
A 1997 meta-analysis by De Wolff and van Ijzen- sponding and predicts attachment security in 1-year-olds
doorn on the association between maternal sensitivity (Meins et al., 2001, 2003). In a similar vein, Oppenheim
and infant attachment security concluded that there is a and his colleagues explored differences in maternal in-
modest but reliable association (combined effect size sightfulness into the infants internal experiences and
.22) between sensitivity and security (De Wolff & van motives and found that mothers deemed positively in-
Ijzendoorn, 1997), which is consistent with the results sightful were rated as more sensitive during mother-in-
of several other reviews of this literature (Belsky, 1999; fant play sessions, and their offspring were more likely to
Thompson, 1998) and with findings from the large, lon- be securely attached in the Strange Situation (Koren-
gitudinal NICHD Study of Early Child Care (NICHD Karie, Oppenheim, Dolev, Sher, & Etzion-Carasso,
Early Child Care Research Network, 1997, 2001). This 2002; Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, 2002). Both mind-
is true whether the infant Strange Situation, the AQS, mindedness and insightfulness assessments explained
Relationships 53

variance in infant security beyond the effects of mater- though there was some evidence that when maternal sen-
nal sensitivity. sitivity was low, greater amounts of child care and/or
Such studies are helpful in bridging the transmis- poorer quality care increased the risk of insecure attach-
sion gap highlighted by van Ijzendoorn (1995) in his ment (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,
meta-analytic review of studies associating adult attach- 1997, 2001). In other cultural settings where the quality
ment representations, parental responsiveness, and in- of child care is very poor, there is evidence that child
fant attachment security. His review focused on the care can have a directly adverse impact on infant-mother
adult representations of early childhood care, including attachment as well as interacting with maternal insensi-
recollections of feeling loved and secure and percep- tivity (Sagi, Koren-Karie, Gini, Ziv, & Joels, 2002).
tions of the feelings and motives of caregivers, which The quality of the marital relationship has also been
are characterized as attachment states of mind and found to predict attachment security in several studies,
are assessed in the Adult Attachment Interview (see with mothers who report greater marital satisfaction
Hesse, 1999, for a review of this literature). Reviewing and harmony having infants with more secure attach-
an extensive body of research, van Ijzendoorn (1995) ments, although this association is not found consis-
concluded that these adult attachment representations tently across the research literature (e.g., Belsky &
are significantly associated with independent measures Isabella, 1988; Howes & Markman, 1989; Owen & Cox,
of parental responsiveness (combined effect size .34), 1997; see Belsky, 1999). Marital conflict is likely to
with adults in the autonomous (secure) group respond- have direct and indirect implications for the security of
ing more sensitively to their offspring than adults in the attachment. Maritally conflicted couples may have
insecure, preoccupied, and dismissing groups. Further- greater difficulty maintaining sensitivity to infant sig-
more, adult attachment representations are also strongly nals and needs in the midst of their own emotional tur-
associated with the attachment classifications of infant moil. Owen and Cox (1997) also found that marital
offspring in the Strange Situation, even when adult at- conflict and sensitive responding each made indepen-
tachment was assessed prenatally (combined effect sizes dent contributions to attachment security, such that con-
.31 to .48). Autonomous adults tend to have children flict was negatively related to attachment security even
who are securely attached, and adults in the preoccu- among young children whose mothers or fathers re-
pied and dismissing groups have offspring who are more mained sensitive when interacting with them. The nega-
likely to be insecure.4 Thus, one important contribution tive emotional climate of the home may be one influence
to the security of attachment are caregivers personal that can account for the impact of marital conflict inde-
representations of the care they received as young chil- pendently of parental sensitivity: Young children may be
dren and its influence on the sensitivity of care they made anxious by parental arguing and conflict even
provide to their own offspring. As van Ijzendoorn (1995) when each parent is a sensitive caregiver.
pointed out, however, a substantial proportion of the as- This conclusion is consistent with Cummings and
sociation between adult attachment representations and Davies (1994; Davies & Cummings, 1994) portrayal of
infant attachment security is not explained by differ- how young childrens security is affected not just by
ences in parental sensitivity, and he suggested that this their relationships with each parent but also by their
transmission gap warranted further exploration by at- emotional experience in the family system as a whole.
tachment researchers. What other influences do adult at- Their emotional security hypothesis argues that mari-
tachment representations have on the development of tal conflict can threaten young childrens security in the
security in offspring that are not mediated by sensitive family and can provoke distress, motivate childrens ef-
care? Studies of maternal mind-mindedness and insight- forts to regulate conflict, and instill hostile representa-
fulness may provide one response to this question (see tions of family lifequalities that resemble insecure
Meins, 1999). attachment (see Davies & Forman, 2002; Davies, Harold,
Beyond the mother-infant dyad, the amount and qual- Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002). Security may, in
ity of child care has not been found to be a significant in- short, be a function not just of the child-parent relation-
fluence on the security of infant-mother attachment, ship but of childrens experience of the broader family
according to the NICHD Study of Early Child Care, al- emotional climate. Understanding the direct and indirect
influences of the family environment, especially as it is
4
See note 2, p. 49. affected by marital conflict, domestic violence, and
54 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

other negative family experiences, is particularly signif- further research but also compels a reconceptualization
icant in light of the relatively weak association between of the nature of parental sensitivity in a manner that is
measures of parental sensitivity and infant disorganized consistent with De Wolff and van Ijzendoorns (1997)
attachment (van Ijzendoorn et al., 1999). Family influ- call for attention to context. Attachment researchers
ences that are related to angry and frightening parental have tended to portray differences in sensitivity as char-
conduct may, independently of sensitivity, be important acterological and traitlike, deriving from the enduring
to the genesis of infant disorganization and possibly also legacy of childhood experiences captured in the Adult
other forms of attachment insecurity. Attachment Interview. But contemporary approaches to
Insecure attachment is more frequent in lower- parenting also emphasize the situationally adaptive,
income and socioeconomically stressed samples owing, flexible nature of caregiving (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994;
in part, to the greater incidence of stresses within and Grusec et al., 2000; Kuczynski, Marshall, & Schell,
around the family that can affect parental sensitivity 1997). Parents approach their children with consistent
and the security of attachment (Barnett et al., 1999; van values and goals, but their parenting is also affected by
Ijzendoorn et al., 1999; Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, the childs immediate behavior, situational and long-
Cohen, & Owens, 2001). Furthermore, De Wolff and term goals, the constraints of the circumstances, and
van Ijzendoorn (1997) noted in their meta-analysis that the behavior of other people (such as a spouse or sib-
the socioeconomic status of the family is also a signifi- ling). Their parenting is adapted to characteristics of the
cant moderator of the influence of sensitivity on attach- child but also of the family, marital relationship, and
ment. Thus, there is a weaker association between circumstances as well as the parents relational history.
maternal sensitivity and attachment security in lower- Such a view is consistent with the conclusions of a meta-
income than middle-income families. A study by Raikes analysis by Holden and Miller (1999) on the stability of
and Thompson (2004c) explored this further by show- parenting across time, children, and situations. They
ing, in a sample of low-income Early Head Start fami- found that although child-rearing practices are fairly
lies, that while the impact of economic risks (associated consistent across different children and over time, par-
with poverty) on attachment security was mediated by ents are much less consistent in their behavior across sit-
its effects on maternal responsiveness, the effects of uations, and they suggested that developmentalists must
emotional risks (such as domestic violence, alcohol or increasingly view parenting practices as both enduring
drug abuse) had direct effects on the security of attach- (rooted in adult values, goals, and beliefs concerning
ment that were unmediated by maternal behavior. These child care) and different (adapting to situational de-
risk factors, which altered the broader emotional cli- mands and childrens immediate needs). The same is
mate of the family, were associated with lower attach- likely to be true of the variations in parental sensitivity
ment security independently of variations in maternal that contribute to attachment security.
sensitivity. Emotional risk factors also moderated the Viewed in this light, variations in parental sensitivity
association between maternal behavior and child secu- may not be uniformly influential on attachment security,
rity such that material responsiveness was less strongly but rather in particular contexts and circumstances rele-
associated with attachment security in families with vant to developing security. For example, sensitivity
many emotional risks. In short, in homes with many may be an important influence when it is exhibited in
stresses and risk factors, sensitive responsiveness is less the contexts most relevant to attachmentwhen the
likely to shape the security of attachment and the diffi- child is distressed or alarmedthan during nonstressful
culties of family life are likely to have a greater direct episodes of play, teaching, or feeding (Thompson,
impact on the childs sense of security. Further research 1997). The sensitivity with which caregivers manage
of this kind, especially research that distinguishes dif- conflicts of will with their offspring may also be impor-
ferent kinds of risk, is essential to understand better the tant in light of the growth of parent-child limit testing
effects of family stresses and buffers on the security of when children become locomotor, as earlier noted. Sen-
attachment in socioeconomically stressed and middle- sitivity may be influential in relationships when the par-
income families as a way of better comprehending the ent can be a reliable, protective haven of support, in
influences on attachment beyond parental sensitivity. contrast to conditions in which marital conflict, neigh-
Taken together, the literature on the origins of at- borhood violence, or poor child care impose emotional
tachment security not only highlights new directions for threats that a sensitive parent cannot buffer. Moreover,
Relationships 55

sensitivity may be especially influential when sensitive development of attachment security and in the sequelae
care is maintained over time as a continuing source of of attachment in ways that merit further exploration.
emotional support for adaptive functioning (Belsky & Finally, an important influence on the development of
Pasco Fearon, 2002a, 2002b). In these situations, sensi- attachment security is culture. Cultural practices influ-
tivity is developmentally important because the condi- ence normative conditions of early childhood care, and
tions of care make sensitivity more salient to the infant. cultural beliefs and values shape the characteristics that
This suggests that the babys construal of the adults parents value and seek to foster in offspring. Theory and
responsiveness is also an important part of the context research on attachment has, from the beginning, grown
influencing the impact of sensitive care on developing within the conceptual tension of recognizing the impor-
attachment security. As Watson (1979) noted, the contin- tance of culture to the development of attachment while
gency perception that forms the basis for an awareness of also appreciating the evolutionarily adaptive, species-
sensitive responding is affected by the base rates of both typical process shaping attachment in humans and other
the childs behavior and the adults response: Infants animals. Understanding attachment as a universal devel-
who are temperamentally fussy may, for example, have a opmental phenomenon shaped by cultural influences
more difficult time detecting a caregivers responsive- continues to be one way that research on attachment re-
ness to their cries than infants who are temperamentally mains sensitive to context.
more pacific (Thompson, 1986). But research on the as- This conceptual tension was initially manifested in
sociation between infant temperament and attachment efforts by researchers in several Western and non-
security has yielded a fairly consistent conclusion in Western nations to use the Strange Situation to deter-
studies using the Strange Situation procedure: There is mine whether infants in their societies exhibited the
not a reliable, direct association between temperament same patterns of security and insecurity that were ini-
and attachment security (see Thompson, 1998, and tially identified in the United States. The findings of this
Vaughn & Bost, 1999 for reviews). There is also no reli- research literature, including studies in Israel, Japan,
able association between temperament and the infant dis- China, Africa, Chile, Sweden, Great Britain, and the
organized/disoriented classification (van Ijzendoorn Netherlands, yielded several conclusions (see Thompson,
et al., 1999). However, research using the Attachment Q- 1998, and van Ijzendoorn & Sagi, 1999, for reviews).
Sort has shown that infants who are temperamentally First, when the Strange Situation is used inappropriately
more negatively reactive and difficult are likely have low (e.g., allowing separation episodes to endure despite
security scores (van Ijzendoorn et al., 2004; Vaughn & heightened infant distress) or inconsistently with norma-
Bost, 1999), which probably arises from the manner in tive child-rearing practices, infant behavior in the proce-
which attachment security is operationalized in the dure does not necessarily reflect attachment security.
AQS. Taken together, the research literature does not Infants living on Israeli kibbutzim who rarely encoun-
support the view that attachment security derives from tered strangers and children in Japanese homes who had
antecedent differences in infant temperament. rarely been separated from their mothers responded with
This desirably straightforward conclusion is, in some unusual distress in the Strange Situation because the pro-
senses, unfortunate because it has caused researchers to cedure entailed experiences with strangers and separa-
fail to explore further a number of indirect associations tion that were more atypical for their background than
between temperament and attachment (see Thompson, for infants living in the United States. This is important
Connell, & Bridges, 1988). Mangelsdorf, Gunnar, because the Strange Situation was designed to be a mod-
Kestenbaum, Lang, and Andreas (1990) reported that erately stressful assessment based on the experiences of
patterns of maternal care had different consequences for typically developing children in the United States, and
the development of security when infants who were high when the procedure is highly stressful it is unlikely to
or low in temperamental proneness-to-distress were dis- yield a valid assessment of attachment security. These
tinguished. Nachmias, Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Parritz, findings underscore the significance of ensuring that as-
and Buss (1996) reported that toddlers who are behav- sessments of attachment are based on a thoughtful appre-
iorally inhibited may especially benefit from a secure ciation of the typical conditions of early childhood care
attachment relationship when coping with stressful chal- for the samples under study.
lenges. Taken together, these findings suggest that tem- Second, especially when these considerations are
perament may interact with maternal caregiving in the taken into account, studies from a wide variety of
56 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

nationalities indicate that infants develop attachments in the United States has yielded a mixed pattern of find-
to their parents and other caregivers. Moreover, with a ings as well. But there is also considerably greater diver-
few exceptions (e.g., Grossmann, Grossmann, Huber, & sity in cross-national findings concerning the origins of
Wartner, 1981), the most common attachment classifi- attachment security and its outcomes.
cation in nonstressed, nonclinical samples is secure. At- Based on these considerations related to attachment
tachment is indeed a universal phenomenon and, research in Japan, Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyake, and
although infants may manifest security through distal, Morelli (2000) have questioned the universal applicabil-
proximal, or contact-seeking behaviors, most infants ity of three core claims of attachment theory: (1) care-
appear to be securely attached. Furthermore, re- giver sensitivity leads to secure attachment, (2) secure
searchers who examined parental perceptions of desir- attachment leads to later social competence, and (3)
able child behavior found that in most countries, parents children who are securely attached use the caregiver as
endorse a profile of behavior that is consistent with that a secure base for exploration. They argue that these
of securely attached children, although parents from conclusions reflect Western beliefs about the nature of
different countries often differ in their reasons for this the child and of infant-parent relationships and thus
preference, their preferred manifestations of security, cannot properly be generalized to non-Western cultures
and their evaluations of various patterns of insecurity (see also Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, Miyake, & Weisz,
(Harwood, Miller, & Irizarry, 1995; Posada et al., 2000). The answer, according to Rothbaum and his col-
1995). It appears that secure attachment is both broadly leagues (2000), is to develop unique, indigenous theo-
desirable and normative. Third, when multiple studies ries and methods of studying parent-child attachment
were conducted within a single national group (such as relationships. Indeed, given the amount of variability in
in Japan, Israel, Germany, and the United States), they attachment observed within nationalities, their recom-
indicated that there is often considerable variability in mendation might be extended to the creation of context-
patterns of attachment within nationalities. This within- specific attachment research for different subgroups
national variability suggests that cultures are not homo- within cultural settings.
geneous in how they influence the development of By contrast, the conclusions yielded by research on
attachment security, and the values and practices attachment and culture suggest a less extreme solution.
shared within any nationality are significantly adapted Hypotheses concerning the origins and outcomes of at-
to local conditions (e.g., rural versus urban, kibbutz tachment security derived from attachment theory
versus city, or middle-income versus lower-income). should be evaluated with attention to the cultural con-
Finally, cultural research on the security of attach- texts of child care and the values guiding parent-child
ment indicates that there is somewhat less consistency interaction in specific groups. This includes constant at-
across national samples in how the quality of care con- tention to the validation of measures derived from stud-
tributes to attachment security, and in the outcomes of a ies of children in the United States for use with
secure or insecure attachment (Thompson, 1998; van non-Western groups and, when necessary, the creation
Ijzendoorn & Sagi, 1999). These are the ways in which of new assessments. To evaluate whether parental sensi-
cultural differences in child care and values concerning tivity predicts attachment security in non-Western con-
children are most likely to be influential. None of a bat- texts, for example, it is important to develop culturally
tery of measures of parental attitudes and behavior and appropriate assessments of sensitivity. Theoretical pre-
parent-child interaction obtained throughout the 1st year dictions concerning attachment outcomes must also be
succeeded, for example, in discriminating infants who evaluated with regard to the contexts and values of early
were securely attached from insecure in Sweden (M. care. Indeed, even the documentation that secure attach-
Lamb et al., 1985). Likewise, in a study of Israeli infants ment is normative in different nationalities is not neces-
raised in the kibbutz, Oppenheim et al. (1988) found sarily evidence that the Strange Situation procedure is
that attachment security to mother or father had no asso- valid until convergent evidence (such as confirming an
ciation with measures of later sociopersonality develop- association between the childs attachment behavior at
ment. To be sure, the association between parental home with secure behavior in the Strange Situation) is
sensitivity and security of attachment is only of moder- obtained. However, the existing research literature sug-
ate strength in U.S. samples, as noted earlier, and re- gests that rather than abandoning the theory and meth-
search concerning the sequelae of attachment security ods of contemporary attachment research, these tools
Relationships 57

may continue to be useful as they are adapted to work in as a result, could not identify stable, meaningful individ-
specific cultures and settings in which child-parent rela- ual differences in interactive quality (Masters & Well-
tionships develop. If such inquiry can be conducted in a man, 1974). One of the first studies of the stability of
culturally sensitive manner, it might be a preferable al- attachment classifications contributed to the validation
ternative to the generation of a collection of indigenous of the Strange Situation by showing that when evaluated
attachment methods and theories and would also inform within the organizational perspective of Ainsworths
the development of attachment theory. coding system, individual differences in infant-parent
Indeed, further studies on attachment and culture can attachment could be highly stable over a 6-month period
usefully enable research into the origins of attachment (Waters, 1978). This finding was consistent with the
security to become more context sensitive whether or theoretical tradition shaping attachment theory (derived
not it is used to evaluate the generality of attachment from psychoanalytic theory) that early parent-child rela-
theory. Sagi, van Ijzendoorn, Aviezer, Donnell, and tionships would be a consistent, formative influence on
Mayseless (1994), for example, compared the attach- sociopersonality development. Thus, the development of
ment security of Israeli infants in two kibbutz arrange- a reliable methodology and the discovery that individual
ments: (1) a familist arrangement in which infants differences in relationship quality could be stable over
returned home for the night after spending the day in time contributed to the enthusiasm initially generated
group care, and (2) a traditional arrangement entail- for attachment theory and research.
ing communal sleeping conditions involving supervision Since that time, however, there have been many stud-
by professional caretakers. From attachment theory they ies of the stability of attachment security, none of them
predicted and subsequently confirmed that infants in the confirming the initial expectation that attachments are
latter group would be more insecurely attached to their highly consistent over time. Table 2.1 summarizes stud-
mothers because of the inconsistent responsiveness of ies examining the stability of early attachment classifi-
the professional caretakers and their mothers inaccessi- cations. Studies were included in this table when the
bility to them at night. In another kibbutz study, Oppen- Strange Situation was used on each occasion because
heim and colleagues (1988) found that the security of these studies provide the most valid window into the
attachment of young children to their metaplot (commu- consistency of attachment relationships over time (stud-
nal caretakers) predicted which children were later ies using different attachment measures on each occa-
more empathic, purposive, dominant, achievement ori- sion, by contrast, confound change in attachment with
ented, and independent, even though mother-child at- measurement differences, and the Strange Situation is
tachment security did not predict these dimensions of the best-validated attachment assessment). The table
later psychosocial competence. The importance of the shows that the proportion of infants who retain the same
child-metaplot attachment relationship to these outcome attachment classification on each occasion varies
measures (which were assessed in the context of commu- widely, from under 50% to nearly 100%, over periods of
nal care) may have heightened the influence of these at- only 6 to 8 months.
tachments to context-relevant psychosocial skills. In Similar conclusions are yielded from studies using
each case, research on attachment in a different cultural other behavioral measures of attachment over longer in-
setting permitted researchers to test hypotheses that tervals. Symons, Clark, Isaksen, and Marshall (1998)
could not be readily evaluated in the United States. reported a correlation of .44 between observer-sorted
These are examples of how further studies of attachment AQS assessments of 44 children at ages 2 and 5; and
and culture can contribute to a greater understanding of Moss, Cyr, Bureau, Tarabulsy, and Dubois-Comtois
the ways that context influences the early development (2004) reported that 67% of their sample of 120 chil-
of security in close relationships. dren retained consistent classifications when assessed
in the Cassidy-Marvin procedure at age 3 to 4 and the
Consistency and Change in the Security of closely related Main and Cassidy (1988) procedure
Attachment at age 5 to 6. The first longitudinal studies comparing
infant Strange Situation classifications with mid- to
The current era of attachment research emerged out of a late-adolescent Adult Attachment Interview states
desolate period in which researchers had difficulty de- of mind have yielded mixed results: Two studies
vising reliable measures of infant-parent interaction and, (Hamilton, 2000; Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell,
58 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

TABLE 2.1 Stability of Attachment Classifications in the Strange Situation


Age at Age at
Study N Time 1 Time 2 Stability
Middle-class samples
Belsky et al. (1996)a
Pennsylvania State mothers 124 12 18 52
Pennsylvania State fathers 120 13 20 46
Pittsburgh mothers 90 12 18 46
Easterbrooks (1989) b 13 20
mothers 60 58
fathers 60 56
Frodi, Grolnick, and Bridges (1985) 38 12 20 66
Jacobsen et al. (1997) 32 12 18 50
Main and Weston (1981) 12 20
mothers 15 73
fathers 15 87
Owen et al. (1984) 12 20
mothers 59 78
fathers 53 62
Takahashi (1986, 1990) 48 12 23 60
Thompson et al. (1982) 43 12.5 19.5 53
Waters (1978) 50 12 18 96
Lower-income samples
Barnett et al. (1999) 12 18
maltreated subsample 18 66
nonmaltreated subsample 21 62
Lyons-Ruth et al. (1991) 46 12 18 30
Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptationc 12 18
Vaughn et al. (1979) 100 62
Egeland and Sroufe (1981)
maltreated subsample 25 48
excellent care subsample 32 81
Egeland and Farber (1984) 189 60
Schneider-Rosen et al. (1985) 12 18
maltreated subsample 12 42
nonmaltreated subsample 17 76
Vondra et al. (2001) 195 12 18 45
Notes: Age is in months. Overall stability of attachment classification is expressed as the proportion of the
sample maintaining the same classification at each age.
a
Pennsylvania State samples included exclusively firstborn sons. Pittsburgh sample was recruited for a
study of postpartum depression; depression was unrelated to attachment classification or its stability over
time.
b
Sample was equally divided between full-term and low-birthweight preterm infants. Term status was un-
related to attachment classification or to its stability. Strange Situation assessments with mothers and fa-
thers were separated by approximately 1 month.
c
Stability estimates from these studies are based on overlapping subsamples.

& Albersheim, 2000) found that nearly two-thirds of the necessarily be anticipated. Dozier and colleagues
sample obtained the same attachment classification in (2001) found that by only a few months after their foster
infancy and adolescence, whereas three studies (Lewis, care placements, infants attachment security had al-
Feiring, & Rosenthal, 2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Ege- ready begun to be predictable by knowledge of the foster
land, 2000; Zimmermann & Grossmann, 1997) found no mothers attachment states of mind at a level compara-
continuity. Other evidence also indicates that consis- ble to that found in biological mother-child dyads.
tency in attachment classification over time should not There is, in short, no normative stability to attachment
Relationships 59

relationships from the early years.5 Attachment relation- ened attachment insecurity in the socioeconomically
ships sometimes stay the same, but sometimes they stressed samples earlier described. In longer-term stud-
change (Thompson, 2000). ies, the frequency of negative life events between attach-
This conclusion does not threaten the validity of the ment assessments is associated with changes in the
Strange Situation because of the extensive external va- security of attachment from infancy to adulthood, espe-
lidity for the procedure. However, because it conflicts cially shifts toward insecure adult attachment represen-
with certain theoretical expectations, it requires expla- tations (Hamilton, 2000; Lewis et al., 2000; Waters,
nation. Are the changes that occur in attachment secu- Weinfield, & Hamilton, 2000; Weinfield et al., 2000; see
rity random or systematic, perhaps even lawful? Serious also Beckwith, Cohen, & Hamilton, 1999). These events
attention to this issue is important not just for theoreti- include parental divorce or serious illness, parental loss,
cal reasons. Understanding the causes of continuity and child maltreatment, and other intervening events of sig-
change in attachment security could be relevant to iden- nificance and severity, although some of them (particu-
tifying protective factors for the maintenance of secu- larly divorce) are experienced by a high proportion of
rity in the lives of some children and catalysts to children in countries like the United States.
security in the lives of others whose early experiences Stresses may not be the only influences provoking
have been relationally insecure. changes in attachment. Thompson, Lamb, and Estes
One suggestion offered by these studies is that secure (1982) found with a middle-class sample that compara-
attachments tend to be more stable than insecure ones (see tively nonstressful changes in parent-infant interaction,
Thompson, 1998). Bowlby (1969/1982) explained this in such as those resulting from the mothers return to work
terms of the self-perpetuating mutual satisfactions that and the onset of nonmaternal care, were associated with
the caregiver and infant derive from a secure relationship. changes in attachment security. These life events were
However, the handful of stability studies enlisting the D associated with changes from insecurity to security and
classification suggest that infant disorganization/disorien- the reverse. Thus, change and stress can alter familiar
tation may also be more stable than the organized, inse- patterns of parent-child interaction and, as a conse-
cure classifications, perhaps because of the extremity and quence, the security of attachment, with stress promot-
consistency of the antecedent caregiving conditions giv- ing a change toward insecurity. This may help to explain
ing rise to disorganized attachment (van Ijzendoorn et al., why the proportion of stable attachment relationships in
1979). Therefore, there can be catalysts to relational con- middle-class samples is not strikingly higher than those
sistency of both positive and negative kinds, although this for lower-income samples (Table 2.1), and why studies
clearly merits greater research attention. with samples that were specifically selected to exclude
Attachment researchers have hypothesized that at- such influences reported higher consistency in attach-
tachment relationships are more likely to change when ment relationships over time (e.g., Main & Weston,
stresses alter familiar patterns of parent-child inter- 1981). Owen, Easterbrooks, Chase-Lansdale, and Gold-
action, and there is some evidence in support of this berg (1984) did not find an association between changes
view. Vaughn, Egeland, Sroufe, and Waters (1979) found in attachment and shifts in maternal employment, how-
that the mothers of infants who shifted from securely at- ever, although the latter were associated with changes in
tached at 12 months to insecurely attached at 18 months the security of infant-father attachments.
reported significantly higher amounts of life stress com- A somewhat more refined hypothesis is that changes in
pared with the mothers of infants who maintained secure the quality or sensitivity of caregivingwhich may result
attachment at each age. These findings are consistent from changing or stressful life conditionsare associated
with the association between stressful events and height- with change in attachment security. Frodi, Grolnick, and
Bridges (1985) found no associations between the stabil-
5 ity of attachment and intervening life events or child care
Although Fraley (2002) concluded from a meta-analysis re-
view of many of these studies that there is moderate stability
patterns, but reported associations with the sensitivity of
of attachment security across the first 19 years of life, his maternal care. Infants who were either consistently se-
analysis focused exclusively on the secure-insecure distinc- cure or became secure from 12 to 20 months had mothers
tion and thus ignored important changes that occur between who were more sensitive and less controlling at 12
the insecure classifications, which tend to be more unstable months. A similar conclusion derives from the NICHD
over time than the secure group. Study of Early Child Care, which found that changes in
60 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

maternal sensitivity were significantly associated with from current experience alone (Fraley, 2002), relational
changes in attachment security from 15 months (assessed history and current experience each exert important in-
via the Strange Situation) to 36 months (assessed via the fluences on a childs psychosocial functioning.
Cassidy-Marvin procedure; NICHD Early Child Care Re- But further exploration of the determinants of stabil-
search Network, 2001). Only 46% of the sample main- ity and change in attachment security is essential for
tained the same attachment classification over time, with several reasons. First, far greater understanding of the
low or decreasing maternal sensitivity in home observa- conditions that are associated with relational changes,
tions from 24 to 36 months predicting which infants and why they are influential, is needed. The pattern of
would change from secure to insecure, and higher sensi- findings thus far suggests that different kinds of influ-
tivity over this period predicting which infants would ences are associated with changes from security to inse-
change from insecure to secure (but see Belsky, Camp- curity compared to the reverse, but these are confounded
bell, Cohn, and Moore, 1996, for a failure to find similar with the broader socioeconomic circumstances of the
differences). Thus, changes in the security of attachment family and thus require further study. Second, the asso-
may be associated with changes in the sensitivity of ciation between negative life events and change in at-
parental care, which is consistent with attachment theory. tachment is moderate but not strong, suggesting that
Changes in the sensitivity of care can derive from other influences are also relevant and perhaps preemi-
many influences. In a study using the AQS, Teti, Sakin, nent in altering prior relational patterns (Thompson,
Kucera, Corns, and Das Eiden (1996) found that the at- 2000; Waters, Weinfield, & Hamilton, 2000). These
tachment security of firstborn preschoolers decreased fol- could include the coping capacities of the parent (as
lowing the birth of a new sibling. The children whose noted earlier) or the child, as suggested by the emotional
security scores dropped most dramatically had mothers security hypothesis of Davies and Cummings (1994).
with significantly higher scores on depression, anxiety, Other potential influences include the availability of
and/or hostility compared with the mothers of children other attachment partners who provide greater relational
who maintained high security scores. In this study, fur- stability to the child, the childs personal construals of
thermore, firstborns security scores were also predicted the caregivers behavior that could moderate the impact
by measures of the mothers marital harmony and affec- of stressful events on the childs expectations for care,
tive involvement with the firstborn. Thus, the impact of and temperamental qualities that may alter a childs vul-
the secondborns birth on the security of mother-firstborn nerability or resiliency to the personal impact of nega-
attachment was moderated by the mothers capacities to tive life events. Each of these hypotheses merits further
cope successfully with the new birth, which was itself empirical exploration, and few have yet been studied.
predicted not only by her personality style but also by the Third, our understanding is especially impoverished
support she received from her partner. It seems likely that with respect to the influences that can cause formerly
similar processes of coping and adjustment would mediate insecure children to become secure, despite the rele-
the impact of family events on the sensitivity of parental vance of this to preventive and interventive efforts. Al-
care and the consistency of child-parent attachments over though parent-child therapeutic interventions informed
time. When caregivers can cope adaptively with changing by attachment theory have been shown to benefit young
life circumstances and negative events, sometimes with children growing up in at-risk circumstances (e.g., Cic-
the assistance of others, they are more likely to maintain chetti, Toth, & Rogosch, 1999), little is known about the
familiar patterns of interaction and consistent attachment ordinary conditions that can provoke transitions to se-
relationships over time. cure attachment in nontherapeutic contexts. Further
Taken together, these studies collectively portray the study of this issue can have potential importance for fos-
continuity of attachment security as a relational process tering more positive early parent-child relationships and
that is influenced both by the quality of care in infancy to the study of early childhood mental health.
and the subsequent quality of care after infancy. Such a
view is consistent with Bowlbys claim that attachment
Early Attachment and Subsequent
patterns are a product both of personal history and cur-
Psychological Development
rent circumstances. It suggests that rather than early
experience launching children on highly predictable de- If the consistency of attachment relationships is due to
velopmental pathways, or psychological growth deriving an interaction of early sensitive care and subsequent ex-
Relationships 61

perience, then this should also be true of the sequelae of were more sensitive to the effects of parenting quality
attachment security: The extent to which attachment later in life than children with secure attachment histo-
predicts later sociability, behavior problems, or other ries. Moreover, attachment security at 24 and 36 months
outcomes should depend on both early security and the (when childrens mental representations are maturing)
childs subsequent experiences, particularly of sensitive but not at 15 months was predictive of later social cogni-
care. Belsky and Pasco Fearon (2002a) confirmed this tion, and security at multiple ages was more predictive
expectation from attachment theory using data from the than a secure attachment at only one assessment. It
NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Analyzing Strange was, in short, cumulative relational experience that pre-
Situation classifications at 15 months and subsequent dicted childrens social-cognitive functioning at school
measures of maternal sensitivity at 24 months, they re- entry, which included assessments of childrens attribu-
ported that the children who obtained the highest scores tions for peer behavior, their ability to generate appropri-
on a broad range of social and cognitive measures at 36 ate responses to social problems, and self-perceived
months were those who were securely attached and who loneliness.
subsequently experienced sensitive care. Those per- These findings, taken together, indicate that early se-
forming most poorly at 36 months were insecurely at- curity interacts with the quality of subsequent experi-
tached in infancy and experienced later insensitive care. ence (particularly maternal care and broader life
Interestingly, of the two intermediate groups, children stresses) in predicting developmental outcomes. Indeed,
who were initially insecurely attached but subsequently these findings suggest that later caregiving may be at
experienced sensitive care scored higher on all outcome least as important as early security in predicting later
measures than children who were initially secure but behavior. Unfortunately, most of the research on the out-
later experienced insensitive care. Similar findings have comes of early attachment is insensitive to these devel-
been reported by other attachment researchers (e.g., opmentally interactive influences. Although virtually
Easterbrooks & Goldberg, 1990; Egeland, Kalkoske, all attachment theorists agree that the consequences of a
Gottesman, & Erickson, 1990; Erikson, Sroufe, & Ege- secure or insecure attachment arise from an interaction
land, 1985; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990). Belsky between early security and the continuing quality of
and Pasco Fearon (2002a) also found that maternal- parental care, most studies are designed in a simple pre-
report measures of life stress, depression, social sup- post manner in which security in an antecedent assess-
port, and family resources at 24 months helped to ex- ment is associated with a later behavioral outcome. This
plain why some securely attached infants subsequently makes it impossible to determine whether early security
experienced insensitive care, and why some initially in- is linked to later behavior because caregivers have re-
secure infants later experienced sensitive maternal care. mained consistently supportive (or unsupportive) over
In each case, maternal insensitivity was positively asso- time, or even whether the childs attachment has re-
ciated with the number of negative life events and lack mained consistently secure or insecure. If either is true,
of support that mothers experienced when children were then predictive relations between attachment and later
age 2. In a corollary report from the same NICHD study, behavior may be better attributed to the continuing in-
Belsky and Pasco Fearon (2002b) reported that a cumu- fluences of parental sensitivity or attachment security.
lative measure of contextual risk during the childs first Moreover, few studies are designed to enable an assess-
3 years moderated some of the associations between ment of possible moderators of the association between
early attachment and later behavior. early security and its hypothesized outcomes, which in-
In another reanalysis of the NICHD Study of Early clude not only the sensitivity of parental care and family
Child Care, Raikes and Thompson (2005d) expanded on stress but also other features of parental behavior that
these findings. They examined the association between can facilitate or impede later developmental outcomes.
multiple early assessments of attachment security (at 15, Thus, research on the predictive relations between at-
24, and 36 months) and later measures of parent-child re- tachment and later behavior is often agnostic concerning
lationship quality with childrens social-cognitive func- its causes (Thompson, 1999).
tioning at 54 months and first grade. They found that This is unfortunate because attachment theory is
both concurrent parenting quality and early attachment ready to move beyond simple pre-post research to a
security were associated with social-cognitive outcomes, more incisive exploration of the conditions underlying
and that children with insecure attachment histories continuity and change in psychological growth. It is as
62 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

important today to understand why early security is (or lier, that relationships may remain consistent or change
is not) related to later psychological functioning as it is after infancy. Consequently, although attachment secu-
to establish a predictive relation. In this regard, theory rity in infancy may inaugurate short-term consistency in
development must proceed in tandem with more sophis- the harmony of parent-child relations, the evidence con-
ticated research designs to enable the examination of cerning long-term continuity is mixed, with continuity
more complex, interactive predictive models than the likely depending on important mediating conditions in
simple expectation that early security predicts later the ecology of family life.
psychosocial functioning. What about the benefits of attachment security for
Guided by a general expectation that a secure attach- childrens experience of other close relationships? A
ment predicts better later functioning, as noted earlier, meta-analysis by Schneider, Atkinson, and Tardif (2001)
researchers have studied a wide range of hypothesized found a modest association between parent-child attach-
outcomes. It is important, however, to distinguish differ- ment and childrens peer relationships (combined effect
ent outcome domains in assessing the importance of at- size .20) and confirmed that this association is stronger
tachment security for psychological development. This for studies of childrens close friendships (effect size
is because a secure attachment might be expected to .24) than for relationships with other peers (effect size
have stronger, more enduring, and more direct associa- .14), which is consistent with other reviews of this liter-
tions with sequelae that are more specifically related to ature (Thompson, 1998, 1999). Strange Situation, AQS,
issues of relational trust and security than to outcomes and representational attachment assessments for older
that are not. children were used in the studies reviewed in this meta-
The most direct result of a secure attachment would analysis, and findings for each were consistent with
be for the parent-child relationship: An early secure these conclusions. Schneider and colleagues also con-
attachment should predict more positive subsequent cluded that this association is stronger for peer relations
parent-child interaction. This expectation is confirmed in middle childhood and adolescence than in early child-
in short-term follow-up studies during the 2nd year in hood and suggested that this derives from the consolida-
which securely attached children showed greater enthu- tion and sophistication of representational processes
siasm, compliance, and positive affect (and less frustra- related to friendship in older children. However, this
tion and aggression) during shared tasks with their conclusion integrates studies involving long-term pre-
mothers (e.g., Frankel & Bates, 1990; Matas, Arend, & diction from infant attachment with studies in which at-
Sroufe, 1978; Slade, 1987). Secure infants tend to main- tachment and peer relations were each assessed in
tain more harmonious relations with their mothers in childhood or adolescence, and thus the meaning of this
the 2nd year. However, in each of these studies, the association is not entirely clear from this meta-analysis.
mothers of securely attached infants were themselves Other studies support the conclusion that attachment
more sensitive and helpful toward offspring in follow-up security is more strongly associated with childrens
assessments, and thus supported the positive behavior of functioning in close relationships. In the Minnesota
their children. It is more appropriate, therefore, to con- Study of Risk and Adaptation (see Sroufe, Egeland,
clude that securely attached dyads tend to maintain in- Carlson, & Collins, 2005), for example, infants who
teractive harmony in the 2nd year. This continuity in were securely attached were later less dependent on
parent-child harmony provides significant benefits for their preschool teachers and functioned better in the
child socialization and personality development for se- preschool setting (Sroufe, 1983). Bost, Vaughn, Wash-
curely attached children (Waters et al., 1991). However, ington, Cielinski, and Bradbard (1998) found that secure
the beneficial effects of a secure attachment in infancy preschoolers (assessed via observer AQS scores) had
may wane over time. Researchers have not found longer- more extensive and supportive social networks and were
term associations between security in infancy and also higher on sociometric assessments of peer compe-
parent-child interaction at ages 3 (Youngblade & Belsky, tence (see Booth, Rubin, & Rose-Krasnor, 1998, and
1992) and 5 (van Ijzendoorn, van der Veer, & van Vliet- DeMulder, Denham, Schmidt, & Mitchell, 2000, for si-
Visser, 1987), even though long-term associations have miliar results). Anan and Barnett (1999) also found (in a
sometimes been demonstrated, as noted earlier, between sample of lower-income African American 6.5-year-
attachment measures at different ages. This is consistent olds) that secure attachment (assessed 2 years earlier)
with the findings of studies concerning the stability of was associated with childrens perceptions of greater
attachment classifications, which indicate, as noted ear- social support, and social support mediated the associa-
Relationships 63

tion between secure attachment and lower scores on ex- early measures were supplemented by more contempo-
ternalizing and internalizing problems. To be sure, there raneous assessments of relational functioning and/or
is evidence that securely attached infants are also more personality, especially when long-term prediction was
sociable with unfamiliar adults during the 2nd or 3rd involved (e.g., Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004).
year (e.g., Main & Weston, 1981; Thompson & Lamb, Taken together, this study yielded impressive evidence
1983), which may derive from the generalization of the of the predictable organization of personality and be-
social skills that secure infants acquire with their moth- havioral functioning from childhood to early adulthood
ers. However, mothers were present during stranger so- as a function of the interactive effects of early caregiv-
ciability assessments in these studies, and each study in ing, subsequent experiences, and relational influences.
which concurrent maternal behavior was evaluated Although some of the findings of this project have not
yielded differences indicating that the mothers of se- been replicated by others (e.g., Easterbrooks & Gold-
cure children were more supportive and child centered berg, 1990; Frankel & Bates, 1990), the study offers an
with their offspring. Thus, differences in stranger socia- important portrayal of the place of attachment security
bility may be a dyadic phenomenon. Differences in more in the multifactorial construction of personality devel-
intimate relationships appear, by contrast, to be a func- opment (Thompson, in press).
tion of the capacity of securely attached children to cre- Security of attachment as a protective or risk factor
ate more positive relationships. to the development of psychopathology has also been
Attachment researchers have also studied the associ- the focus of research inquiry. In the Minnesota study,
ations between relational security and personality devel- insecure-resistant attachment in infancy predicted anxi-
opment. The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation, a ety disorders in adolescence, but there were few other
uniquely comprehensive, prospective longitudinal study associations between the organized insecure classifica-
of children and families in poverty, has focused exten- tions and later psychopathology (Sroufe et al., 2005). In
sively on the association between attachment and per- another at-risk sample, Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, and
sonality within the organizational perspective of Cibelli (1997) reported that avoidant attachment in the
attachment theory and Sroufes portrayal of the conti- Strange Situation was associated with teacher-report in-
nuity of adaptation of age-related developmental chal- ternalizing symptomatology indexed by the Child Be-
lenges. In this study, children were recruited with their havior Checklist (CBCL) at age 7, although there was no
families in infancy and followed through age 28, with association with teacher-reported anxiety or behavior
personality characteristics assessed regularly through problems at age 5 (Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & Repacholi,
behavioral observations, interviews, observer ratings, 1993). A number of studies have failed to discern a reli-
semiprojective instruments, and self-reports. Sroufe and able association between early organized insecurity in
his colleagues found significant continuities between the Strange Situation and the development of behavioral
early attachment security (assessed in the Strange Situ- problems, especially in middle-class samples (e.g.,
ation at 12 and 18 months) and personality dimensions Bates & Bayles, 1988; Erickson et al., 1985; Fagot &
throughout childhood and adolescence, including associ- Kavanagh, 1990). However, analyses from the NICHD
ations between secure attachment and measures of emo- Study of Early Child Care found that insecurity at 24
tional health, self-esteem, agency and self-confidence, months (on the AQS) predicted maternal and caregiver
positive affect, ego resiliency, and social competence in CBCL ratings at age 3 of internalizing and externalizing
interactions with peers, teachers, camp counselors, ro- behavior problems, and insecurity at 36 months (using
mantic partners, and others (see Sroufe et al., 2005, for the Cassidy-Marvin procedure) predicted internalizing
a comprehensive report, which also includes a list of ci- problems on the same assessments (McCartney et al.,
tations to specific research reports and a comprehensive 2004). Thus, research evidence concerning the associa-
list of measures). Moreover, consistent with the empha- tion between organized insecure attachment and the de-
sis on both developmental history and current experi- velopment of behavior problems in childhood is quite
ence, Sroufe and his colleagues found that the mixed. There is some evidence that this association is
prediction of these and other personality features was stronger in lower-income families, which are subject to
enhanced when (a) early attachment measures were sup- other risk factors for child problems, than in middle-
plemented by other indicators of the quality of early class homes, but the research evidence is inconclusive.
care, ( b) there was consideration of continuity in the Further attention to the clinical implications of at-
quality of care between infancy and later ages, and (c) tachment security has accompanied the creation of the
64 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

disorganized/disoriented (group D) classification. Chil- is unsurprising that in studies that have included these
dren in this group may be at risk for the development of additional risks in predictive models, both disorganized
later clinical problems, especially when they are in attachment and other family risks combine to predict
stressed or lower-income families. In the Minnesota later child psychopathology (e.g., Carlson, 1998; Lyons-
study, infants who were classified D in the Strange Situ- Ruth et al., 1993; Shaw et al., 1997). This is one of the
ation later obtained significantly higher scores on a reasons that the association between attachment insecu-
global index of psychopathology and, in particular, of rity and later behavior problems tends to be stronger in
dissociative symptomatology in adolescence (Sroufe socioeconomically distressed families, but it also sug-
et al., 2005; see also Carlson, 1998). Lyons-Ruth and her gests that the sequelae of insecurity derive, in part, from
colleagues (1995, 1997) reported that disorganized at- continuity in the risk factors that initially contributed to
tachment in the Strange Situation was associated with attachment insecurity earlier in the childs life. Second,
teacher-reported externalizing symptoms on the CBCL it should be clear that insecure attachmenteven disor-
at age 7, and with teacher-reported hostility at age 5 ganized attachmentis not an index of psychopathology
(see also Shaw, Owens, Vondra, Keenan, & Winslow, but only a risk factor. These findings show that although
1996, for similar findings using the Strange Situation insecurity increases the chances of later behavior prob-
with a low-income sample, and Moss, Parent, et al., lems, the prediction of child psychopathology should be
1996, and Moss, Bureau, et al., 2004, for comparable viewed in the context of multifactorial models involving
findings using the Cassidy-Marvin procedure with older early caregiving influences, continuing family adversity,
children from middle-class families).6 Shaw, Keenan, ineffective parenting, and atypical child characteristics
Vondra, Delliquadri, and Giovanelli (1997) and Moss, (Greenberg, 1999).
Bureau, and colleagues (2004) also reported higher Indeed, the same conclusion is true of the other se-
scores for D children on internalizing symptomatology, quelae of the security of attachment. Because each of
and Moss, Cyr, and Dubois-Comtois (2004) have found these hypothesized outcomes is multidetermined, at-
contemporaneous associations between disorganized at- tachment security is likely to explain a significant but
tachment and behavior problems in school-age children, small proportion of variance in each, with the amount of
with the different D subgroups predicting externalizing variance declining over time as other developmental in-
and internalizing problems. However, in a somewhat dis- fluences emerge. This is one reason why future studies
cordant report, McCartney and colleagues (2004), ana- that include multiple predictors of later outcomes will be
lyzing data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care, more informative in situating the security of attachment
found no reliable associations between disorganized at- in the constellation of other influences that predict later
tachment in the Strange Situation (at 15 months) or the parent-child relationships, social competence, personal-
Cassidy-Marvin procedure (at 36 months) and mother- ity, and risk for psychopathology. Furthermore, the
or caregiver-reported behavior problems at age 3. large majority of research studies on the outcomes of at-
These mixed findings suggest that further examina- tachment security focus on child-mother attachment re-
tion of the association between insecure attachment and lationships, even though attachment theorists recognize
the development of behavior problems is warranted. In that children develop meaningful relationships with fa-
doing so, two interpretive cautions should be noted. First, thers and other attachment partners. It seems likely that
since the origins of infant attachment disorganization are studies incorporating the influence of multiple attach-
based in the same risk factors that also contribute to later ment relationships will have greater predictive power
psychopathology (such as maternal psychosocial prob- than those focusing on the child-mother relationships
lems and depression, family stress, and other factors), it alone, but few studies have considered the roles of fa-
thers and other caregivers.
6
What can we conclude, therefore, about the associa-
These findings are consistent with a meta-analytic review by
tion between early attachment and later psychological
van Ijzendoorn and De Wolff (1997) who reported that there
is a moderately strong association between disorganized/dis-
development? Early security clearly makes a difference
oriented attachment and externalizing behavior (combined ef- for the childs future in concert with other family influ-
fect size .29), but the extraordinary heterogeneity of the ences. It inaugurates a more harmonious mother-child
samples and the assessments of disorganization for the 12 relationship that provides continuing benefits for the
studies they summarized makes the meaning of this conclu- young childs receptivity to mothers socialization in-
sion uncertain. centives. It is associated with more positive personality
Relationships 65

characteristics and greater social competence, espe- Internal Working Models


cially in other close relationships with peers and adults.
Attachment security is also a protective factor in the de- One of Bowlbys most heuristically powerful formula-
velopment of psychological well-being, with insecure at- tions is the view that attachment security influences psy-
tachmentespecially disorganized insecuritya risk chological growth through childrens developing mental
factor for the development of behavioral problems. A se- representations, or internal working models (IWMs), of
cure attachment alone is not necessarily a strong predic- the social world. Internal working models are based on
tor of long-term outcomes but, in concert with young childrens expectations for the behavior of their at-
continuing supportive care, it meaningfully improves the tachment figures that develop into broader representations
odds for positive psychological growth. What is less of themselves, their attachment figures, interpretations of
clear is why these outcomes emerge. Attachment secu- their relational experiences, and decision rules about how
rity is likely to be associated with more positive social to interact with others. These working models also be-
skills, self-regulatory capacities, modes of social and come interpretive filters through which children (and
emotional understanding, motivational processes, social adults) reconstruct their understanding of new experi-
expectations, causal attributions, and self-referential ences and relationships in ways that are consistent with
beliefs that contribute to the benefits of a secure attach- past experiences and expectations, sometimes enlisting
ment and the challenges of an insecure one. Attachment unconscious defensive processes in doing so. As a conse-
security is also likely to be associated with continuing quence, children choose new partners and behave with
parental sensitivity that provides ongoing support for them in ways that are consistent with, and thus help to
healthy psychological growth. But thus far, researchers confirm, the expectations created from earlier attachment
have yet to incisively explore the psychological processes relationships. In this manner, IWMs constitute the bridge
underlying the association between early attachment se- between the infants experience of sensitive or insensitive
curity and its later psychological outcomes. This consti- care and the development of beliefs and expectations that
tutes a central agenda for future attachment research. affect subsequent experience in close relationships
A recent study by Denham, Blair, Schmidt, and (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999). Furthermore, young
DeMulder (2002) provides an example of the kind of re- children are believed to internalize conceptions of them-
search that would advance this understanding. In this selves from early relational experience that are incorpo-
study, multiple measures of attachment security (in- rated into developing IWMs and that also constitute a
cluding observer AQS) were assessed when children perceptual lens for experiences that affect self-concept
were age 3, along with multiple measures of emotional and other developing self-referential beliefs. In this man-
(in)competence, including assessments of emotion un- ner, secure or insecure attachments shape the organiza-
derstanding, regulation, and anger expression. Children tion of personality through the influence of mental
were later studied in their kindergarten classrooms to working models arising from attachment security.
assess peer competence through sociometric ratings This is a valuable way of thinking about socioemo-
and teacher-rated social competence measures. Latent tional development that is representational, integrative,
variable path-analytic procedures were used to confirm affectively oriented, and relationally based. Its breadth,
two avenues from preschool attachment security to however, poses some conceptual challenges for attachment
kindergarten social competence: (1) a direct pathway theory. Grossmann (1999) has pointed out that at least two
and (2) an indirect path through emotional competence formulations of IWMs can be found in Bowlbys theory.
(see also Denham et al., 2001). As we shall see, there is One conceptualizes IWMs in a manner resembling the dy-
considerable research elucidating how securely at- namic unconscious by which relational experience is in-
tached children develop skills of emotion understand- terpreted through the perceptual-affective schemas of
ing through the more open conversation shared with infancy; these prelinguistic models have enduring influ-
their mothers, and this research suggests that skills in ence but remain largely inaccessible to conscious reflec-
emotional competence may be one means by which the tion. The other conceptualizes IWMs as resembling other,
social skills of secure children are enhanced. To Den- conscious representational models like scripts and
ham and colleagues, the mediating influence of emo- schemas that evolve developmentally and can be con-
tional competence illustrates one of several facets of sciously accessed. These are different formulations and
the internal working models generated by the security have different implications for theory and assessment.
of attachment. Perhaps as a consequence, basic questions concerning how
66 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

IWMs develop, how their development is affected by other hope to assess but also by other influences on narrative
facets of conceptual growth, and changes in IWMs over content and quality that derive from childrens linguistic
time remain unclear in attachment theory (Thompson & skills and verbal fluency, parent-child discourse, social
Raikes, 2003). Another problem is the explanatory breadth desirability influences, culture, and ethnicity. There are
that IWMs can assume. As Hinde (1988) noted, in the also developmental considerations related to validation
very power of such a model lies a trap: it can too easily ex- because the coherence of young childrens story-comple-
plain anything (p. 378), a concern shared by other devel- tion discourse probably means something different com-
opmental scientists (Belsky & Cassidy, 1994; Rutter & pared to coherence in an adolescents or adults response
OConnor, 1999). Over the years, as attachment security to Adult Attachment Interview (AAI ) probes. Attachment
has been studied in relation to a widening array of devel- researchers have generally sought to validate representa-
opmental outcomes, the concept of internal working mod- tional measures of attachment security by establishing
els has been enlisted to account for unexpected as well as predictive or contemporaneous associations with behav-
hypothesized associations, giving credence to Belsky and ioral attachment measures, even though security assessed
Cassidys (1994) concern that IWMs would constitute a by narrative coherence and emotional openness is not the
catch-all, post hoc explanation for such research find- same thing as security assessed by secure base behavior.
ings. The inclusiveness of the IWM construct has ex- But the important task of elucidating the meaning of dif-
panded with every new empirical finding that is ferences in narrative responses to semiprojective probes
explained with reference to it. and their association with parent-child interaction at
This has also presented a considerable challenge for home remains to be accomplished (Raikes & Thompson,
efforts to assess childrens mental working models de- 2005b; Waters & Cummings, 2000).
rived from attachment relationships. Attachment re- There have been at least two recent efforts to con-
searchers have created a variety of assessments of tribute greater theoretical clarity to the IWM construct
childrens mental representations of relational experi- in ways that have implications for assessment. Brether-
ence, many of them based on semiprojective narrative ton (1990, 1991; Bretherton & Munholland, 1999) has
approaches that involve childrens responses to doll-play described mental working models in terms of the formu-
materials, story-completion probes, evocative pictures, lations of script theory and constructive memory and
and other materials (see Solomon & George, 1999, and emphasized the openness of communication between
Stevenson-Hinde & Verschueren, 2002, for reviews of parent and child as a significant developmental influ-
these methods). These procedures rely on the assump- ence on the construction of working models in early
tion that in responding to materials that are designed to childhood. More generally, she describes IWMs as a sys-
evoke attachment-related issues, children will project tem of hierarchically organized representational sys-
onto the materials their own feelings and beliefs associ- tems that involve different levels of generalizability and
ated with their attachment experiences. Surprisingly, no are relevant to various broader belief systems, suggest-
procedures have been developed to directly assess chil- ing that elements of IWMs can be studied in the context
drens expectations for the behavior of their attachment of other conceptual achievements of the childhood years.
figures in familiar situations or their scripts for social Building on this view, Thompson (1998, 2000) has
interaction with their attachment figures. proposed a developmental account that associates the
Considerable thoughtful creativity has been devoted to growth of IWMs with other developing mental processes
semiprojective measurement development, but Solomon that encode, represent, interpret, and remember social
and George (1999) have chastised the frontier mental- experiences.7 Drawing on literatures concerning the de-
ity of researchers who have produced these representa- 7
In a related view, Spangler and Delius (2003) have proposed
tional assessments with inadequate attention to their
that IWMs should be portrayed as a theory of attachment
validation, especially by comparison to the careful vali- (or, perhaps, a theory of relationships) involving coherently
dation of behavioral measures of attachment security. integrated knowledge of relational processes and causal influ-
This may be due to the challenges inherent in such valida- ences that generate specific predictions and expectations for
tional efforts. The coherence, emotional themes, and res- relational experience. Such a view, drawn from theory-theory
olution of young childrens narrative responses to of young childrens intuitive beliefs about mind, physics, and
semiprojective assessments are likely affected not only biological kinds (Wellman, 2002), also offers considerable po-
by the representations of relationships that researchers tential utility in clarifying the IWM construct.
Relationships 67

velopment of implicit memory, event representation, au- Belsky, Spritz, and Crnic (1996) hypothesized that
tobiographical memory, theory of mind, and other fea- differential processing of schema-consistent informa-
tures of social understanding, he portrays the growth of tion, owing to the influence of IWMs, would cause se-
IWMs as building on and integrating these allied con- curely attached children to remember positive events
ceptual achievements that concern, like IWMs do, more accurately than insecure children. In a study in
understanding of people and social events, self- which 3-year-olds delayed recognition memory for pos-
understanding, and interpretations of relational experi- itive and negative events during a previously viewed pup-
ence. In this developmental view, IWMs change pet show was assessed, this expectation was confirmed.
considerably with age, especially during periods of sig- A recent study using data from the NICHD Study of
nificant representational advance (such as the transition Early Child Care also showed attachment-related differ-
to symbolic representational capacities in early child- ences in attentional processes, with disorganized chil-
hood, and the emergence of abstract thought in adoles- dren showing especially poor attentional performance
cence) when earlier representational systems become (Pasco Fearon & Belsky, 2004). The conclusion that chil-
reorganized (see also Ainsworth, 1989; Crittenden, dren with different attachment histories differentially at-
2000). Thompson also argues that IWMs may have tend to and remember emotionally related events merits
greatest influence on other aspects of sociopersonality further investigation because of its relevance to under-
growth during the developmental periods when these ca- standing the influence of the mental representations as-
pabilities are maturing most significantly. The working sociated with attachment history and its broader
models associated with a secure attachment may influ- implications for understanding attachment functioning.
ence emotion understanding most strongly in early Attachment security should be associated with chil-
childhood, for example, when childrens conceptions of drens conceptions of relationships, and one study has
others feelings begin to become consolidated. Finally, confirmed this to be true of peer relationships. Cassidy,
in this view, IWMs are shaped not only by the childs di- Kirsh, Scolton, and Parke (1996) examined the associa-
rect experience of close relationships but also by the tions between attachment security and childrens sensi-
secondary representations of experience mediated by tivity to the feelings of peers and attributions concerning
language through parent-child conversation. Consistent peer motivations in response to hypothetical stories in-
with literatures reviewed elsewhere in this chapter, he volving negative actions with ambiguous intent. Although
argues, language provides young children with consider- attachment security from infant Strange Situation assess-
able insight into others feelings, thoughts, knowledge, ments did not confirm the expectation that securely at-
and motives and are likely to significantly influence de- tached 4-year-olds would be more likely to attribute
veloping IWMs as they shape childrens emergent con- benign motives to story characters, this expectation was
ceptions of emotion, intention, and mind (see Thompson confirmed when attachment security and peer measures
et al., 2003). were obtained contemporaneously in kindergarteners and
These newer portrayals of the development of mental first graders. Moreover, these representations of peer re-
working models emphasize the associations between lationships in the older children were found to mediate
IWMs and other conceptual systems and suggest that the association between attachment security and peer
rather than trying to study working models directly sociometric status. Consistent with the findings of
through semiprojective procedures and other avenues, Denham, Blair, and colleagues (2002) described earlier,
attachment researchers might equally fruitfully glean an representations of the feelings and intentions of other
understanding of their developmental influence by children helped to account for the greater social compe-
studying the representational correlates of differences tence of secure children. As noted earlier, furthermore,
in attachment security. By understanding how secure Raikes and Thompson (2005d) found that attachment his-
and insecure attachments are associated with differ- tory (especially in concert with subsequent supportive
ences in emotion understanding, self-awareness, and parenting) predicted childrens attributions for peer be-
other characteristics, it might be possible to identify the havior and social problem solving at 54 months and first
influence of working models. There is now emerging an grade, especially when children were securely attached
empirical literature documenting how the security of at- on multiple early assessments.
tachment is associated with representations of self, oth- Several studies have found that securely attached
ers, and relationships. children are more competent in emotion understanding
68 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

in contemporaneous associations using the AQS (Laible Similar influences may also account for the associa-
& Thompson, 1998; Ontai & Thompson, 2002) and in tion between attachment and conscience development. A
predictive associations from infant Strange Situation secure attachment is associated with conscience devel-
classifications (Steele, Steele, Croft, & Fonagy, 1999) opment (Laible & Thompson, 2000) but, as discussed
or early childhood AQS (Raikes & Thompson, 2005a; later, attachment security is especially influential for
see also de Rosnay & Harris, 2002). These studies also children who are temperamentally relatively fearless,
indicate that securely attached children are especially for whom the emotional incentives of the mother-child
proficient at understanding negative emotions and relationship motivate moral compliance (Kochanska,
mixed feelings, which are each conceptually complex. 1991, 1995). Attachment security is accompanied by
Why do secure children better understand emotions? mother-child discourse style in shaping early con-
There is some indication that secure children acquire science development, with mothers who more richly and
this understanding because of how they talk about emo- elaboratively discuss the feelings of other people con-
tion with their mothers. Ontai and Thompson (2002) tributing most to young childrens internalization of
found that attachment security interacted with elabora- moral values (Laible & Thompson, 2000). As noted in a
tive maternal discourse in predicting emotion under- later section, this conclusion is consistent with Hoff-
standing: More secure 5-year-olds whose mothers had mans (1983, 2000) classic formulations concerning the
earlier used a more descriptively rich, elaborative style nature of parental communication contributing to moral
of conversation about emotion with them (in storybook internalization. Further evidence of how attachment in-
reading and discussions of past events) were more ad- teracts with other parental influences derives from the
vanced in positive emotion understanding (see also findings of Kochanska and colleagues (2004) of how at-
Laible, 2004b, for similar findings). Because the moth- tachment security interacts with parental discipline
ers of securely attached children have been found to be practices on the growth of conscience. In their longitu-
generally more elaborative in their style of conversation dinal analysis, they found that for securely attached
with offspring (Farrant & Reese, 2000; Laible, 2004b; children, the parents use of responsiveness and gentle
Laible & Thompson, 2000; see Reese, 2002, for a re- discipline predicted later conscience, but for insecure
view), these findings offer support for the view from at- children there was no such association. These findings
tachment theory that secure dyads share a more open, suggest that the security of attachment moderates the in-
fluid communication style that enables discussion of fluence of other relational influences on early socializa-
feelings (particularly mixed or negative emotions) and tion. With respect to conscience, for example, the
the childs greater comprehension of them (Bretherton, adults disciplinary practices may have differential
1990). One of the benefits of more open shared commu- emotional impact depending on the broader relationship
nication between mother and childin which mothers shared by parent and child.
provide more detailed information concerning emotions Attachment theory argues that the IWMs deriving
and other psychological experiencesis that young from a secure or insecure attachment influence self-
children can share troubled or confusing feelings with concept, particularly conceptions of the self as loved
their caregivers and obtain an understanding response. and loveable. In a study of contemporaneous associations
This research thus suggests important associations be- between attachment and multiple measures of self-
tween mother-child discourse style, the development of understanding, Cassidy (1988) found that securely at-
IWMs and of emotion understanding, and attachment tached 6-year-olds described themselves in generally
security, consistent with the earlier review of research positive terms but were capable of admitting that they
concerning the growth of emotion understanding in were imperfect. Insecurely attached children either re-
preschoolers. Indeed, Raikes and Thompson (2005a) vealed a more negative self-image or resisted admitting
found that the quality of mother-child conversations flaws. Clark and Symons (2000) found stronger contem-
about emotion mediated the association between at- poraneous associations between attachment security and
tachment security and emotion understanding in 3- two assessments of self-concept at age 5 than predictive
year-olds. The influence of maternal discourse also associations with attachment at age 2, but the associa-
suggests one way that the intergenerational transmis- tions depended on the measure of self-concept. Goodvin,
sion of attachment security and related representations Meyer, Thompson, and Hayes (2005) also found that se-
might occur. curely attached preschoolers viewed themselves more
Relationships 69

positively and self-concept was more stable over time conscience development and possibly other behav-
than for insecure children, while maternal emotional dif- ioral outcomes.
ficulties (depression and parenting stress) predicted A childs relational history may also sensitize or
childrens negative self-perceptions. In problem-solving blunt attention to other features of social experience
tasks, insecurely attached preschoolers expressed in ways suggested by the findings of Belsky and his
greater frustration and inability and asked for help colleagues.
sooner and in unnecessary circumstances (Colman & For many domains of psychological development,
Thompson, 2002). These findings suggest that attach- such as theory of mind, greater reflection on the role
ment security and developing self-concept are associ- of attachment security is needed.
ated, and warrant further exploration of the mediating
processes by which this occurs. Further research on the representational correlates of
Finally, attachment theory does not make strong pre- the security of attachment may contribute to under-
dictions concerning the benefits of a secure attachment standing how multiple attachment relationships become
for understanding others thoughts and beliefs and, per- enfolded into the development of secure or insecure per-
haps as a consequence, there is mixed evidence for the sons by adolescence or early adulthood. It is noteworthy
association between attachment security and theory of that most of the research reviewed here has not sought to
mind. Meins et al. (2002) found no association between directly assess internal working models, but rather has
early attachment and childrens later performance on sought to comprehend their functioning through more
theory of mind tasks, but Symons and Clark (2000) specific analyses of the associations between attach-
found a contemporaneous association between attach- ment and conceptions of peer relationships, emotion un-
ment and theory of mind in 5-year-olds. In light of the derstanding, self-concept, and other psychological
variety of assessments of theory of mind in preschool- processes associated with the mental models derived
ers and the direct and indirect avenues by which attach- from attachment relationships. In so doing, we derive a
ment relationships might be influential in its portrayal of working models as rapidly developing
development, further reflection is warranted on processes that mediate between attachment security and
whether and why an association between these con- its behavioral outcomes, interacting with other rela-
structs might exist. tional influences between parents and offspring, and
Taken together, the research on the representational sensitizing awareness of social processes.
correlates of attachment security contributes to an ap-
preciation of why the mental models associated with se-
cure or insecure attachments are so conceptually
exciting for developmental analysis. The conclusions of
Summary
these studies suggest:
Despite its controversial status in developmental psy-
chology (Thompson, 2005), attachment theory remains
Mental representations of peer intentions, emotional uniquely generative because of how it integrates ideas
inferences, and other psychological processes medi- concerning the effects of early relational experience on
ate between attachment security and its behavioral socioemotional and personality development, construc-
outcomes, such as social competence. tivist views on the growth of social relatedness, the de-
Differences in processes of parent-child discourse velopment of representations concerning relationships
may interact with the broader security of the parent- and self, and the relevance of these for the growth of
child relationship to shape young childrens develop- psychological well-being and psychopathology. Its fu-
ing emotion understanding and, quite likely, ture potential for remaining a central view of early per-
conscience development, and possibly contribute to sonality development depends on the capacities of
the intergenerational transmission of expectations attachment theorists and researchers to update Bowlbys
and beliefs associated with the security of attachment. provocative formulations with the thinking of contem-
Attachment security and its allied representations porary developmental science and the yield of their own
may moderate the influence of other parental prac- empirical studies. The findings of the studies reviewed
tices, such as discipline approaches, on childrens in this section suggest that there remains considerable
70 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

potential for the generation of new ideas within the at- Intuitive Morality of Early Childhood
tachment framework.
In early childhood, the conceptual foundations of moral
development become established. Young children are not
egocentric but rather, as earlier noted, are intensely in-
CONSCIENCE terested in the desires, intentions, feelings, and thoughts
of other people. Their sensitivity to others reactions
Conscience development is concerned with how children contributes to their anticipation of and responsiveness to
construct and act consistently with generalizable, inter- disapproval and, later, to a dawning understanding of
nal standards of conduct. It is closely tied to moral judg- normative standards of appearance and behavior late in
ment, but the growth of conscience also encompasses the 2nd year. As noted earlier, by 18 to 19 months of
the affective, temperamental, and relational influences age, young children begin to respond with heightened in-
that, together with moral judgment, shape moral con- terest and concern to objects that are damaged or flawed
duct. The study of conscience thus provides a window (Kagan, 1981, in press; S. Lamb, 1993). Kagan (1981)
into how emotional, cognitive, and relational influences has interpreted this phenomenon as an emergent moral
intersect to guide young childrens developing views of sense, based on caregivers reactions to damaged ob-
themselves in relation to others and the broader values jects and the young childs emergent sensitivity to stan-
of the social world (Kochanska & Thompson, 1997; dards. Consistent with this view, Kochanska, Casey, and
Thompson, Meyer, & McGinley, 2006). Fukumoto (1995) found that older children (26- to 41-
Not surprisingly, contemporary research on con- month-olds) who responded with greater concern to
science has emerged in the shadow of moral develop- flawed objects also showed greater distress to rigged
ment theory. Traditional approaches to moral mishaps for which they believed they were responsible.
development, such as learning theory and the cognitive- These studies suggest that young children internalize
developmental views of Piaget and Kohlberg, have por- normative standards for appearance and integrity based,
trayed morality in early childhood as distinct from that in part, on their observations of how adults respond to
of older children and adolescents because of the younger violations of these standards in everyday experience
childs egocentric, preconventional moral orientation. (such as cleaning or discarding soiled toys). This is at
By contrast with older children who are concerned with the same time that toddlers begin to respond with em-
maintaining harmonious social relations, and adoles- barrassment and concern to a spot of rouge on their
cents who are viewed as ethical, humanistic moralists, noses, reflecting their awareness of normative standards
young children are portrayed as authoritarian, utilitar- for personal appearance (Lewis, 2000; Lewis & Brooks-
ian moralists who are guided by rewards, punishment, Gunn, 1979).
and obedience. But new research, together with new un- Young children also appropriate behavioral standards
derstandings of young childrens conceptual skills and and distinguish between different domains of behavioral
relational experiences, have contributed to a new view obligation (see Turiel, Chapter 13, this Handbook, this
of early conscience and of the importance of early child- volume). Much as adults do, 3- and 4-year-olds distin-
hood to the development of mature morality. It is now guish between moral and social-conventional standards,
becoming clear that conscience in early childhood viewing moral violations as more serious and less revo-
shares much in common with the morality of later years cable owing, in part, to their harm to others (Smetana,
because of preschoolers sensitivity to the feelings of 1981, 1997; Smetana & Braeges, 1990). In complex so-
others and the relational incentives for cooperation. cial situations, such as gender exclusion in peer play,
Parental influences encompass far more than sanctions preschoolers prioritize equal treatment over convention
and reinforcements and include maintaining a relation- in their consideration for fairness by age 5 (Killen et al.,
ship of mutual responsiveness and trust, enlisting con- 2001; Theimer et al., 2001). By age 4, furthermore, indi-
versational catalysts for moral understanding, and vidual differences in emotion understanding and knowl-
proactive efforts that foster cooperation and compliance edge of mental states (i.e., theory of mind) predict
in young children. Early childhood is increasingly differences in childrens moral judgments in friendship
viewed as providing a foundation for the morality of val- relations (Dunn, Cutting, & Demetriou, 2000; see also
ues, humanistic regard, and relationships of later years. Dunn, Brown, & Maguire, 1995). Thus, young children
Conscience 71

develop an intuitive morality that arises from the social- 1993; Smetana et al., 2000). Caregivers distinguish be-
ization efforts of caregivers in tandem with their own tween different obligatory domains in their discussion of
sensitivity to the feelings and thoughts of others and expectations with young children, justifying moral rules
their developing grasp of normative standards of appear- for their interpersonal consequences, for example, and
ance and behavior. prudential rules by safety concerns (Smetana, 1997;
Young childrens focus on normative standards is un- Smetana et al., 2000). Thus, preschoolers understand-
surprising in light of their search for other constancies ing of how things are done includes standards for how
in everyday experience. This is one reason why they ac- one should act in these and other everyday situations,
quire such an early grasp of obligation in moral, conven- and this may help to explain young childrens inflexibil-
tional, and prudential concerns. As Harris and Nunez ity with the application of behavioral expectations just
(1996) have shown, even 3-year-olds are highly skilled as they are rigid in their beliefs about how common rou-
in understanding how a prescriptive rule applies to dif- tines are conducted. Expectations for how a person acts
ferent circumstances (e.g., Mom says if Cathy rides her may become regarded as normative and obligatory just
bike she should put her helmet on), even though they as are expectations for how others will act in these pro-
are not as skilled at applying a similar descriptive, but totypical situations.
not prescriptive, maxim (e.g., When Cathy rides her Ironically, the normative absolutism of the young
bike, she always wears her helmet ). In a provocative childs thinking about obligation does not necessarily
analysis, Wellman and Miller (2003) have proposed that translate into behavioral compliance, as every parent
just as 3-year-olds have difficulty conceptualizing be- knows. This arises, in part, because many moral situa-
liefs that are discordant with reality, so also they have tions involve frustrating present desires in favor of
difficulty understanding obligation that is discordant broader (often future) goals, which is a conceptual
with behavior, so they are prone to assert that rules can- challenge for young children (Lagattuta, 2005). It is
not be broken and obligations must necessarily be ful- also challenging to comprehend the feelings and inter-
filled. Behavioral obligations describe normative reality ests of multiple participants in interpersonal conflict,
in the eyes of young children, according to Wellman and especially when self-interest is involved. As Arsenio
Miller, and violations are special sources of concern. and his colleagues have shown, for example, young chil-
This concern with what is obligatory and normative, dren perceive victimizers as feeling positively about
which is similar to the moral absolutism observed in their misconduct, partly because children focus on the
young children long ago by Piaget (1965), is consistent satisfaction of the victimizers desires rather than the
with young childrens interest in objects that are flawed victims distress (Arsenio & Lover, 1999). These stud-
and mirror appearances that are rouge marked, and sug- ies highlight that even with their sensitivity to others
gests that an important conceptual foundation for early feelings, comprehending simultaneously the emotional
conscience development is young childrens attunement perspectives of multiple people is still difficult for
to the normative standards and behavioral expectations young children. Thus, the young intuitive moralists de-
that are part of their developing representations of what ontic understanding does not readily translate into
they might typically expect in everyday experience. moral compliance. The result is everyday experience
Young childrens developing representations of nor- with the disapproval of caregivers and the feelings of
mative and behavioral standards are conceptually guilt that may result.
salient because they are likely to be embedded in
broader prototypical knowledge structures by which Moral Emotion
children represent everyday experiences (Hudson, 1993;
Nelson, 1978). Many of the moral, conventional, and Moral emotion also emerges early, and contributes to
prudential standards conveyed to young children are re- the incentives for moral compliance because of its rele-
lated to routine events and are repeatedly confirmed in vance to the self and relationships. As noted elsewhere,
these contexts, whether consisting of prohibitions about the development of self-understanding occurs in concert
making messes and breaking things, self-control with with the emergence of self-referent emotions that are
respect to waiting, sharing, aggression, and eating, with- elicited in everyday situations in which adults make
drawing from touching dangerous objects, self-care, or salient attributions of responsibility for achievement or
participation in family routines (Gralinski & Kopp, wrongdoing. With respect to guilt and shame, parental
72 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

responses to a young childs misbehavior typically make tribute best to moral internalization. By contrast, for
explicit the behavioral values the child has violated, and children who were temperamentally relatively fearless,
it is remarkable how early children begin to respond conscience was instead best predicted by the security of
with these emotions (Barrett, 1998; Lewis, 2000; attachment and maternal warm responsiveness. For
Thompson et al., 2005). Kochanska, Gross, Lin, and these children, the relational incentives of the mother-
Nichols (2002) observed childrens affective and behav- child relationship better motivated moral internaliza-
ioral responses at 22, 33, and 45 months to experimental tion and helped to consolidate a positive, mutually
situations involving rigged mishaps for which children responsive parent-child relationship (Kochanska, 1991,
believed they were responsible. Young children exhib- 1995; see Kochanska, 1997a, and Kochanska et al.,
ited concern and distress at each age, and individual dif- 2002, however, for somewhat different findings). These
ferences in these responses were stable over time and findings suggest that temperament may influence con-
were modestly predictive of a battery of conscience as- science development because it mediates childrens
sessments at 56 months. Moreover, children who dis- emotional experience of parenting practices in response
played more of these behaviors at each age were found to to wrongdoing.
be temperamentally more fearful, and their mothers Temperament may be related to conscience develop-
used less power assertion in discipline encounters. ment in other ways. Young children who are high on ef-
These developmental findings are consistent with ma- fortful (or inhibitory) control are more capable of
ternal reports concerning the development of guilt in exercising self-restraint to comply or desist, and re-
offspring, which also report significant growth in the af- search by Kochanska and her colleagues suggests that
fective and behavioral manifestations of guilt over this these children are also higher on measures of conscience
period (Kochanska, DeVet, Goldman, Murray, & Put- in both contemporaneous and longitudinal assessments
nam, 1994; Stipek, Gralinski, & Kopp, 1990; Zahn- (e.g., Kochanska, 1993; Kochanska, Murray, & Coy,
Waxler & Robinson, 1995; see Eisenberg, Fabes, & 1997). Kochanska and her colleagues (1994) also re-
Spinrad, Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume). At the ported that preschool girls who were higher on tempera-
same time that young children are becoming aware of mental reactivity obtained higher scores on a maternal
normative and behavioral expectations, they are also be- report measure of the childs guilt, consistent with the
coming prone to self-referent moral emotions that can view that reactive children would be more sensitive to
significantly motivate compliance. disapproval and criticism.
Temperamental individuality is an important media- The temperament research underscores that there are
tor of childrens experience of the affective discomfort alternative avenues to conscience development because
and anxiety associated with wrongdoing. In a theoreti- young children are not morally cooperative for the same
cal analysis, Kochanska (1993) proposed that con- reasons. For some, cooperation springs from broader ca-
science development may assume two developmental pacities for self-control; for others, maintaining good
pathways: (1) through the motivation to avoid the emo- relations with caregivers (and the threats to relational
tional discomfort associated with wrongdoing, and (2) harmony arising from misbehavior) is central; for still
through the motivation to maintain good relations with others, moral cooperation derives from efforts to avoid
caregivers by exercising behavioral self-control. She the fear and anxiety that arises from disapproval. This
proposed that a childs temperamental profile is influen- suggests that the moral socialization efforts of parents
tial in determining which developmental pathway pre- must be adapted to the childs temperamental profile
dominantly contributes to the growth of conscience. and other characteristics. Furthermore, this literature
This view was subsequently elaborated in two studies suggests that not only moral resources but also moral
showing that for temperamentally fearful young chil- vulnerabilities inhere in these temperamental profiles.
dren, conscience was predicted by maternal control Temperamentally fearful children may be vulnerable to
strategies that deemphasized power and instead enlisted becoming guilt-prone and morally inflexible as a result;
nonassertive guidance and gentle discipline. These temperamentally fearless children may misbehave when
children are naturally prone to fear and anxiety after they can escape detection or avoid worry about the care-
wrongdoing, Kochanska reasoned, and thus nonpunitive givers loss of love. Because moral emotions are such
discipline that enlists the childs preexisting worry powerful motivators of moral compliance, the influ-
without creating overwhelming distress is likely to con- ences of temperament on the emotional tendencies and
Conscience 73

self-regulatory capacities that underlie moral conduct ticipated disapproval (Campos et al., 1999; Emde &
are potentially important for healthy and unhealthy Buchsbaum, 1990). Later, as the distal warning becomes
forms of moral motivation. remembered, behavioral compliance arises from the tod-
There is yet another emotional resource for con- dler referencing the absent parent in memory (Emde,
science development that emphasizes the prosocial over Biringen, Clyman, & Oppenheim, 1991; Emde & Buchs-
the prohibitive side of morality. Empathy begins to baum, 1990). Parents intervene to remove the child from
emerge during the 2nd year and continues to unfold with disapproved conduct and sanction disobedience, but
growth in emotion understanding in early childhood they also proactively avoid discipline encounters by dis-
(Zahn-Waxler, 2000; Zahn-Waxler & Radke-Yarrow, tracting attention, providing anticipatory guidance or al-
1990; Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, 1995). To be sure, the ternative activities, or other diversionary tactics
sight and sound of another persons distress, fear, or (Holden & West, 1989). There is also considerable di-
anger is a motivationally complex and stressful event for rect instruction of young children about moral, conven-
young children. It may lead to sympathetic feelings and tional, and prudential rules of conduct by parents who
prosocial initiatives, but young children may also ignore, strive to enlist childrens cooperation (Smetana et al.,
laugh at, or aggress toward another in distress, or seek 2000). Beyond these, at least three other facets of the
comfort for themselves because of threats to their own parent-child relationship contribute significantly to con-
emotional security and limited social understanding. science development in the preschool years: (1) the over-
This is one reason why it is important to index empathy all warmth and cooperativeness of the parent-child
in young children as an affective response rather than as relationship, (2) child management strategies used by
an instrumental (i.e., prosocial) behavior. However, parents in discipline encounters, and (3) broader conver-
when adults can assist the child in understanding the sational discourse between parents and offspring that
emotions they are witnessing in another, especially by incorporates morally relevant themes.
clarifying causality and responsibility, raw empathic The warmth and cooperativeness of the parent-child
arousal can become enlisted into prosocial initiatives to- relationship is important, especially early in life, be-
ward another person, and into guilt when the child is the cause conscience development is part of a childs
perpetrator of anothers distress (Zahn-Waxler & broader induction into a relational system of reciproc-
Radke-Yarrow, 1990; Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, 1995). ity characterized by mutual obligations (Kochanska,
Moreover, as children mature throughout the preschool 1997b; Waters et al., 1991). The human consequences
years, their vicarious emotional responding becomes in- of personal conduct become experienced directly for
creasingly predictive of prosocial behavior (Eisenberg & the first time in a parent-child relationship, and thus
Fabes, 1998; see also Eisenberg & Fabes 1995; Miller, the quality of that relationship, especially its mutual
Eisenberg, Fabes, & Shell, 1996). Viewed in this light, responsiveness, helps to orient a young childs moral
empathy alone may not reliably elicit moral responding sensitivity to humanistic concerns and heighten the
in young children, but instead is a setting condition from childs receptiveness to the parents socialization ini-
which prosocial initiatives may arise. Equally impor- tiatives. In several studies in which the mutual respon-
tant, empathy is one of the emotional catalysts for young siveness of parents with young children was assessed
children developing a moral awareness in which the feel- during extended home observations, Kochanska and
ings and needs of other people are central. her colleagues found that dyadic differences in this re-
lational quality predicted measures of the childs con-
science development both contemporaneously and
Relational Inf luences
longitudinally (e.g., Kochanska, 1997b; Kochanska,
Fortunately, young children are not alone in their efforts Forman, & Coy, 1999; Kochanska & Murray, 2000; see
to comply and cooperate. Parents and other caregivers also Laible & Thompson, 2000). Related research has
contribute in many ways to the development of con- helped to explain why. Mothers in mutually responsive
science. Beginning in infancy, when animated facial and relationships use less power assertion with offspring
vocal expressions of emotion are used by parents to and they are more empathic, as are their children in
warn or deter a locomoting child from a dangerous or response to maternal simulations of distress (Kochan-
disapproved activity, social referencing is enlisted to in- ska, 1997b; Kochanska et al., 1999). In a behavior ge-
still certain behaviors with emotional meaning and an- netic study, Deater-Deckard and OConnor (2000)
74 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

concluded that dyadic mutually responsive orientation referent evaluation and affect (Bad boy! You should be
exemplifies an evocative gene-environment correlation ashamed of yourself !). In doing so, the parent not only
in the parent-child relationship, which is consistent explicitly denotes causal associations between the
with the temperament research reviewed earlier. childs behavior, consequences for another, the parents
Another index of relational quality is the security of response, and the experience of moral affect but also
attachment, and, as earlier noted, there is research evi- may provide an interpretation of the event that is differ-
dence that a secure attachment is positively associated ent from the childs own. To the extent to which this is
with cooperation and conscience development (e.g., clearly communicated and understood by young chil-
Kochanska, 1995; Laible & Thompson, 2000). Interest- dren, this experience can be conceptually provocative
ingly, neither research group has reported a significant to young children who are otherwise striving to under-
association between measures of attachment security stand others beliefs, feelings, motives, and their asso-
and mutually responsive orientation between parent and ciations with the childs own. Furthermore, parental
child, despite their apparent conceptual overlap. Taken explanations and reasoning in the discipline encounter
together, these studies underscore the importance of a also introduce young offspring to cultural and moral in-
harmonious relationship of positive mutuality between terpretations of the childs behavior. As Miller and her
parent and child as a foundation for the growth of con- colleagues have shown, for example, mothers in the
science and for cooperative conduct in young children. United States tend to attribute child misconduct to
A second feature of the parent-child relationship that spunk or mischievousness, but Chinese and Chinese-
contributes to conscience development is how the parent American mothers emphasize much more the shame in-
responds when young children misbehave. The disci- herent in misbehavior, each consistent with their
pline encounter has been the focus of extensive study cultural values (Miller, Fung, & Mintz, 1996; Miller,
for many years, and research findings with toddlers and Wiley, Fung, & Liang, 1997).
preschoolers are consistent with those of older children This straightforward and rationalist account of the
in concluding that interventions that are coercive and effects of discipline in early conscience is complicated,
power assertive elicit not only childrens situational however, in several ways (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994;
compliance but also young childrens frustration and Grusec, Goodnow, & Kuczynski, 2000). First, multiple
occasionally defiance. However, discipline that empha- parental goals are likely to compete in how parents re-
sizes reasoning and provides justification for compli- spond to any discipline encounter. In many circum-
ance is more likely to foster internalized values in stances, a priority on moral tutelage must vie with other
young children, even though children may also assert goals, including effecting immediate child compliance,
their autonomy through bargaining and negotiation enabling children to responsibly choose among behav-
(Crockenberg & Litman, 1990; Kuczynski, Kochanska, ioral alternatives, enhancing parent-child communica-
Radke-Yarrow, & Girnius-Brown, 1987; Laible & tion and understanding, allowing children to assert
Thompson, 2002). This is likely to be one reason why, themselves, and other worthwhile aims (Hastings &
over the course of the preschool years, parents increas- Grusec, 1998; Holden & Miller, 1999). The reasons for
ingly rely on verbal strategies over physical interven- the childs misbehavior, the childs characteristics (such
tions for enlisting childrens compliance (Dunn & as temperament), and situational constraints (e.g., pub-
Munn, 1987; Kuczynski et al., 1987). lic versus private setting) are among the important in-
Parental explanations, justifications, and reasoning fluences on the goals that parents choose to pursue
may be especially important for young children who, in during conflict with the child, and this helps to explain
the context of heated emotions over misbehavior, may why parents are not necessarily consistent in their par-
not immediately comprehend what is wrong or who is enting practices across different situations (Holden &
culpable. In their intervention, most parents provide a Miller, 1999). The clarity of the parents moral message
cognitive structure that explicitly links their response is thus likely to be obscured by the alternative socializa-
to the standards the parent has previously conveyed tion goals that are also being pursued, or by the parents
(You know better than to hit your sister!), invokes effort to integrate inconsistent goals (e.g., values trans-
salient attributions of responsibility (Why did you hit mission while enabling child autonomy).
her?), identifies consequences for another (Look, Second, not only what the parent says but the broader
shes crying!), and often directly induces the self- relational context influences conscience development. A
Conscience 75

clear explanation warranting compliance is likely to Fourth, although conflict is conceptually provocative
have different meaning to a child who shares a warm and and contributes to values clarification, conflict between
supportive relationship with the caregiver compared to a a young child and a parent is also threatening to young
child who has experienced considerable prior conflict or children, and the emotion generated by the discipline
distrust in that person. The view that children in warm, encounter may undermine the childs comprehension
secure relationships may be more responsive to parental and processing of the parents moral message. This is
discipline practices than children in insecure relation- consistent with Hoffmans (1983, 2000) classic formula-
ships was recently tested by Kochanska et al. (2004), tion of the discipline encounter, but it emphasizes how
who assessed attachment security at 14 months, much the difference in power between participants in
parental disciplinary practices at 14 to 45 months, and conflict can make a full and accurate comprehension of
conscience at 56 months. For securely attached the message from an authority difficult. As noted by
children, there was a significant positive longitudinal Thompson (1998), from a depth of processing memory
association between parental gentle discipline/ respon- model it is likely that a young childs coherent process-
siveness and later conscience; for insecure children, ing and understanding of the parents message will be
there was no such association. The expectation that spe- undermined by the heightened arousal created by the
cific parental practices have differential consequences discipline encounter, even when parents are careful to
based on the broader tenor of the parent-child relation- use discipline approaches that do not unduly heighten
ship is consistent with other studies of the effects of the childs discomfort. This is especially likely if the
parent-child conversational discourse on conscience de- young childs cognitive resources are also being mobi-
velopment (Thompson et al., 2003; see following), and lized for negotiation or bargaining (Crockenberg & Lit-
suggests that adult explanations may sound differently man, 1990; Kuczynski et al., 1987).
to children who share different kinds of relationships This is one reason that students of conscience devel-
with them. opment have focused on a third feature of the parent-
Third, young children are participants in the process child relationship that is associated with conscience
of values appropriation. They interpret what they are development: conversations that occur outside the disci-
told in the discipline encounter in light of their own per- pline encounter. These conversations may be planned or
ceptions of fairness, the emotional effects of the par- spontaneous and their topics may concern (a) events in
ents behavior (e.g., threats to security or a sense of the past, such as the childs prior misbehavior or ad-
autonomy), and the relevance and consistency of the mirable conduct; ( b) a shared experience in the future,
parental message with what else they know (Grusec & such as going to a public setting where good behavior is
Goodnow, 1994; Kuczynski et al., 1997). The impor- necessary; (c) immediate events, such as a siblings tem-
tance of childrens constructions of parental values is per tantrum; (d) storybook reading; (e) pretend play; or
consistent with the literature on temperament and con- (f ) other shared experiences. In these contexts, even
science discussed earlier and with the studies highlight- when parents are not explicitly intending these conver-
ing the mediating influence of the parent-child sations to be a means of transmitting moral lessons, the
relationship on discipline effects. Furthermore, parental judgments, values, inferences, assumptions, and other
attitudes and discipline practices vary in relation to the interpretations that parents naturally incorporate into
outcome expectancies of parentsparents intervene these conversations make them potentially potent fo-
based, in part, on how they anticipate the child will react rums for early moral understanding and conscience
to their intended intervention (Holden, Miller, & Harris, development. Equally important, the young childs cog-
1999; Holden, Thompson, Zambarano, & Marshall, nitive resources are more likely to be focused on under-
1997). Thus, a childs construal of the discipline en- standing and responding to the parents message with
counter is important not only for its effects on con- less competing emotional arousal than in the discipline
science development but also for how it influences the encounter.
childs behavioral response which, in turn, affects future Variations in the content and style of parental dis-
parental conduct. Studies such as these are important for course in conversation influence early conscience
reaffirming the importance of bidirectional and transac- development. Laible and Thompson (2000) recorded
tional models of early moral socialization, by contrast conversations between parents and their 4-year-
with traditional portrayals of values internalization. olds about past incidents in which the child had either
76 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

misbehaved or behaved appropriately. Mothers who ductive discipline on moral internalization occur when
more frequently referred to the feelings of other people the adult combines warmth with a rational explanation
had children who were more advanced on measures of that reduces threat to the child, similar influences occur
conscience, but maternal references to rules and the in conversations outside of the discipline encounter as
consequences of breaking them from the same conver- well. These conclusions also suggest that conscience de-
sations were unrelated to conscience. These findings velopment in young children is influenced by parent-
were replicated in a prospective longitudinal study in child discourse in a manner similar to how older
which maternal references to feelings ( but not refer- children are affected (Thompson et al., 2003).
ences to rules and moral evaluations) during conflict Finally, just as the broader affective quality of the
with the child at 30 months predicted the childs con- parent-child relationship is an important mediator of the
science development 6 months later (Laible & Thomp- effects of discipline in young children (Kochanska et al.,
son, 2002). In another study, 2- to 3-year-old children 2004), the same is true of the effects of parent-child con-
whose mothers used reasoning and humanistic con- versational discourse. Mothers references to peoples
cerns in resolving conflict with them were more ad- feelings interacts with the shared warmth of the parent-
vanced in measures of moral understanding in child relationship in its association with conscience de-
follow-up assessments in kindergarten and first grade velopment (Laible & Thompson, 2000; Thompson et al.,
(Dunn et al., 1995). These findings suggest that one of 2003). Thus, broader relational quality combines with
the most important features of parent-child conversa- specific features of parent-child discourse to shape
tions on morally relevant themes is how they sensitize young childrens conscience development.
young children to the human dimensions of misbehavior
and good conduct, helping children to comprehend the
Summary
effects of their actions on how people feel. In a sense,
these conversations put a human face on morality. The dissonance between the portrayal of conscience de-
Just as in the discipline encounter, the warmth, emo- velopment emerging from these studies and traditional
tional tone, and constructiveness of the parents de- portrayals of the self-interested, preconventional, ego-
meanor can be as important as what is said to the child. centric young child is an incentive to expanding under-
When they were in conflict with their young offspring, standing of the intuitive morality of early childhood and
mothers who took the initiative to resolve conflict, its developmental influences. These studies make it ap-
using justifications to explain and clarify their expecta- parent that young children are acquiring moral orienta-
tions, and who managed to avoid aggravating and exac- tions that are simpler, but fundamentally similar to,
erbating tension (such as through threats or teasing) had those of older children and adolescents, and therefore
young children who were more advanced on measures the experiences and influences of early childhood may
of conscience development at age 3 (Laible, 2004a; provide an essential foundation for moral development at
Laible & Thompson, 2002; see also Dunn et al., 1995 later ages. Viewed in this light, young childrens con-
described earlier). By contrast, mothers who were con- ceptual growth, developing emotional understanding,
versationally power assertive when recounting the and relational experiences may provide essential corner-
childs misbehavior in the recent pastconveying a stones for the later emergence of the internal, humanis-
critical or negative attitude, feelings of disappointment tic, self-committed morality of older children. Young
or anger, or involving reproach or punishmenthad children clearly are not premoral in any serious sense.
preschool children who obtained lower scores on mea- The study of conscience development has also of-
sures of moral cognition assessed via childrens fered developmentalists new questions and interesting
story-completion responses to moral dilemmas methodologies with which to explore them. Beyond lon-
(Kochanska, Aksan, & Nichols, 2003). As Hoffman gitudinal studies that integrate the morality of early
(1983, 2000) has long argued, power assertion in the childhood with the better-studied moral reasoning of
discipline encounter heightens childrens anxiety and middle childhood, greater exploration of how young
defensiveness and undermines retention of the parents children conceptualize moral obligations would con-
moral message, and it is likely that the same occurs in tribute to understanding how they perceive themselves
conversations about moral issues outside of discipline. as moral actors and (in Kochanskas evocative phrase)
Conversely, just as the well-documented effects of in- moral selves. The relational catalysts to conscience
Self 77

development also merit further exploration, especially in sentational self-awareness arise from the integrated per-
efforts to integrate understanding of parental practices ceptual experiences deriving from movement and activ-
in conversation, discipline, and other interactive situa- ity beginning soon after birth. The synchronous
tions that contribute to the development of moral aware- multimodal perceptual experience arising from self-
ness in young children. Given the growing evidence that produced activity fundamentally distinguishes the per-
conscience emerges not primarily from the prohibitive ceiver from objects (and people) acted on or that move
morality of parental discipline but from the incentives around the infant. Neissers portrayal of this ecological
provided by a harmonious, mutually cooperative parent- self addresses the traditional assumption that infants
child relationship, a portrayal of early moral growth that are born adualistic (i.e., incapable of differentiating the
underscores childrens appropriation of values from external from the internal world) by noting that percep-
shared activity in the family may be more appropriate tual experience itself distinguishes subjective from sur-
than the traditional internalization formulation. By un- roundin Gibsons (1995) evocative words, to
derscoring the multifaceted shared contexts in which perceive the world is to coperceive oneself (p. 6). In-
early conscience develops, such an approach highlights deed, in this Gibsonian view, subsequent developmental
how parents and children mutually create the moral envi- changes in the perception of affordances in the environ-
ronment they share as a family. ment also entail developmental changes in self-
awareness (e.g., awareness of emergent capabilities)
such as in how flat, extended surfaces afford walking to
SELF a 15-month-old toddler but not a 6-month-old. Moreover,
these integrated perceptual experiences provide avenues
The development of self-awareness provides a window for other, more complex forms of self-awareness to de-
into the psychological growth of the child. Over the velop. By 5 months, the integration of kinesthetic and
course of a few years, young children acquire capacities visual experience during movement enables a primitive
to engage with others intersubjectively, visually recog- kind of featural self-recognition: Infants can distinguish
nize their mirror images, attribute behavioral and psycho- videos of their own leg movements from those of another
logical qualities to themselves, create autobiographical infant (Bahrick & Watson, 1985; Rochat & Morgan,
accounts, and situate themselves temporally as individu- 1995). The initial organization and integration of expe-
als with continuity into the past and future. With each de- rience around an implicit frame of reference thus consti-
velopmental advance the child becomes a more complex, tutes one of the earliest forms of self-awareness.
multidimensional self while also becoming more insight- Another is the experience of agency. As earlier noted,
fully self-aware. Advances in self-awareness also trans- very young infants respond to contingency and by 2 to 3
form young childrens social interactions. They make months they respond with positive affect to contingent
children more psychologically self-conscious social ac- responding but become affectively negative if the con-
tors who also possess greater insight into others. Like tingency is interrupted (Lewis et al., 1990; Rovee-
emotional development, the growth of self in early child- Collier, 1989; Watson, 1985). Initiating actions that
hood involves the progressive elaboration of biologically have a predictable impact on objects and people, and the
basic capacities in ways that integrate the influences of positive affect that results from the awareness of con-
conceptual growth, relational processes, and the childs trol, are together likely to be highly salient experiences
own constructions of experience. These features of the contributing to self-awareness early in infancy. Early
development of self have stimulated considerable recent social interaction taps into the young infants contin-
research into this topic, although important questions re- gency awareness, which contributes to the delight of
main to be addressed. face-to-face play, and variability in caregiver respon-
siveness, such as the subdued responding of depressed
Developmentally Emergent Dimensions of Self mothers, consequently has a significant impact on infant
affect and sociability. Taken together, therefore, the
It is common to describe the 2nd year as when the self earliest forms of prerepresentational self-awareness are
emerges, but enduring aspects of self-awareness have perceptual, affective, and agentic in quality, contribut-
developmentally earlier origins. Both Gibson (1995) and ing to the emergence of initial existential self-awareness,
Neisser (1995) argue that the earliest forms of prerepre- and the foundation of Jamess (1890) I-self.
78 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

By the final months of the 1st year, infants are not nate only dynamic images), and infants of both ages
only agentic but also volitional. In their goal-directed could also discriminate the sound of their nondistressed
efforts, as Piaget noted, infants can substitute an alter- vocalizations from those of other infants. Prior experi-
native means for one that has been frustrated and act ence with vocal play and mirror images could contribute
strategically to accomplish their intentions. This inau- to these discriminations, with the strong integration of
gurates, according to Tomasello (1995b, 1999; visual-kinesthetic and auditory-kinesthetic perceptual
Tomasello & Rakoczy, 2003), a conceptual advance in experience during these activities marking them as self-
which infants begin to perceive others also as inten- initiated (see Bahrick, Moss, & Fadil, 1996, for findings
tional actors. As earlier discussed, this is manifested in with younger infants). But these results do not necessar-
a variety of behaviors that reflect the infants awareness ily imply that infants are recognizing themselves in
that other people have subjectivity that can be under- these facial and vocal displays. By 18 months, however,
stood and intentions that can be influenced, including after their noses have been surreptitiously marked with
joint attention, social referencing, imitative learning, a spot of rouge, toddlers reliably show mark-directed be-
and the emergence of intentional communicative efforts havior when placed before a mirror (Lewis & Brooks-
(Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998). Moreover, by Gunn, 1979). Featural self-recognition is based on the
the end of the 1st year, the infants experience of goal contingency between movement and motion in the mir-
directedness often conflicts with others goals and in- ror, but this behavior also reflects additional psycholog-
tentions (of which every parent of a locomotor infant is ical achievements: Young children exhibit self-referent
aware), contributing to the self-awareness that derives emotions like embarrassment at this age (Lewis, 2000),
from conflicts between his or her own goals and those of becoming aware of standards for appearance and behav-
another. As a result, social interaction involves the in- ior that also evoke special attention to soiled toys or
fants goal directedness combined with a dawning faces (Kagan, 1981; S. Lamb, 1993). Consequently, tod-
awareness that subjective, intentional states are at the dlers responses to the classic rouge task entails more
root of others behavior. According to Tomasello complex influences than mere featural self-recognition,
(1995b), it is not only the development of a new inter- and its psychological meaning incorporates greater self-
subjective capacity that inaugurates an advance in self- awareness and the application of standards for norma-
awareness but also the infants growing realization that tive appearance (i.e., my nose is not ordinarily red).
the self can be the object of anothers attention, inten- Mirror self-recognition at 18 months builds on the
tion, and emotional response. Just as infants enlist an- achievements of intersubjective self-awareness at age 1
others emotional evaluation of objects and events in to consolidate the beginning of objective self-awareness
social referencing, they also become sensitive to the in young children, or Jamess (1890) me-self.
adults emotional demeanor when attention is focused In light of the development of the me-self, it is un-
on themselves, and variability in the caregivers surprising that late in the 2nd year and early in the third,
warmth, emotional tone, and sensitivity become increas- toddlers exhibit emerging indications of other represen-
ingly important at this time. This awareness sets the tational forms of self-awareness. These include in-
stage for the growth of self-referent emotions, like em- creased verbal self-referential behavior (e.g., me big!;
barrassment, and self-referential evaluative emotions, Bates, 1990) and verbal labeling of internal experiences
like pride, guilt, and shame, later in the 2nd year. The (such as emotions; Bretherton et al., 1986), assertions of
end of the 1st year witnesses, therefore, the emergence competence and responsibility as autonomous agents
of the intersubjective self. (such as in self-monitoring, refusing assistance, and in-
When do infants begin to exhibit featural self- sisting on do it myself ; Bullock & Lutkenhaus, 1988,
recognitionthe ability to recognize their physical fea- 1990; Stipek, Gralinski, & Kopp, 1990), growing sensi-
tures? This is commonly taken as the central index of tivity to evaluative standards and the emergence of con-
self-awareness, but as noted earlier, recognition that science (Thompson, Meyer, & McGinley, 2006),
ones features and actions are familiar can mean differ- assertions of ownership (Fasig, 2000), the emergence of
ent things at different ages. Legerstee, Anderson, and self-control (Kopp & Wyer, 1994), categorizing the self
Schaffer (1998) found that 8-month-old infants could by gender and in other ways, and young childrens grow-
discriminate static and dynamic video images of their ing interest in how their behavior is regarded by others
faces from those of peers (5-month-olds could discrimi- (Emde & Buchsbaum, 1990; Stipek, Recchia, & Mc-
Self 79

Clintic, 1992). The more complex self-representations cepts, these self-descriptions are like personality traits
of early childhood are reflected also in the emergence of in that they show stability over time, are similar to how
self-referential emotions during the 2nd and 3rd years. others (such as their mothers and teachers) describe
By the end of the 2nd year and increasingly in the third, them, and show convergent validity when correlated
the simple joy of success becomes accompanied by look- with external measures of the same characteristics (Eder
ing and smiling to an adult and calling attention to the & Mangelsdorf, 1997; Goodvin et al., 2005; Marsh
feat; the simple sadness of failure becomes accompanied et al., 2002; Measelle et al., 1998). Even a preschoolers
either by avoidance of eye contact with the adult and use of a concrete feature, such as describing his- or her-
turning away or by reparative activity and confession; self as a girl or boy, is accompanied by a basic under-
and in response to conspicuous attention toddlers in- standing of the psychological attributes and stereotypes
creasingly respond with smiling, gaze aversion, and associated with being male or female (Ruble & Martin,
self-touching (Barrett, 1998; Barrett, Zahn-Waxler, & Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume). To be sure,
Cole, 1993; Kochanska et al., 2002; Lewis, 2000; young childrens personality self-descriptions show
Stipek, 1995; Stipek et al., 1992). greater stability and convergent validity with increasing
Taken together, young children are beginning to re- age, consistent with growth in childrens understanding
gard themselves in more multidimensional and evalua- of personality characteristics more generally (Marsh,
tive ways early in the 3rd year as they increasingly Craven, & Debus, 1998). Moreover, young children tend
perceive themselves as objects of the attention and to be unduly optimistic about the modifiability of indi-
thought of others. This is part of the legacy of the inter- vidual traits, particularly the stability of positive quali-
subjectivity that emerges by the first birthday, and the ties and the changeability of negative ones. Current
greater sensitivity to the evaluations of others arising research confirms, however, the emergence of the con-
from developing psychological understanding and inter- ceptual self in early childhood and of psychological self-
subjective awareness as featural self-recognition is at- descriptors in the childs self-concept.
tained. Moreover, during the 2nd year, developing Another important advance in self-awareness occurs
capacities for receptive language clarify not only the when young children can perceive themselves in a tem-
childs status as the object of others evaluations but poral context. Comprehending how past experiences can
also lexicalizes these evaluations as they are conveyed influence the present self, and the ability to anticipate
through language. Young children not only appropriate the self in future contexts, are significant advances in
others evaluations of themselves but also the evaluative self-awareness because of their relevance to strategic
standards they use as part of childrens effort to com- planning, delay of gratification, moral compliance, per-
prehend constancies and expectations for everyday ex- formance evaluation, autobiographical memory, and
perience. These processes contribute to the emergence self-understanding. These advances depend on a capac-
of the conceptual self (the cognitive self of Howe & ity to perceive an identity between the present self and
Courage, 1993, 1997) that will continue to evolve in so- the self that existed in the past and that will exist in the
phistication and scope in the years that follow. future. The realization that it is the same I in each
Somewhat later, in the 4th and 5th years, young chil- temporal context distinguishes this kind of self-
dren begin to perceive themselves in more explicitly awareness from earlier-developing capacities to evoke
characterological terms at about the same time that they expectations from past events, recall specific past expe-
begin to perceive others in terms of psychological traits riences, or anticipate future events (Moore & Lemmon,
(Marsh, Ellis, & Craven, 2002; Measelle, Ablow, 2001). Povinelli (1995, 2001) has shown that temporal
Cowan, & Cowan, 1998). To be sure, young children self-awareness begins to emerge at about 4 years of age.
often rely on concrete, observable features and action In experimental procedures that are analogous to the
tendencies in their spontaneous self-descriptions but mirror self-recognition tasks, young children were
they can also use psychological trait terms provided by videotaped playing with an experimenter who surrepti-
other people appropriately as personality self- tiously ( but on film) placed a large sticker on the childs
descriptions (e.g., I am naughty sometimes, but good head. When they later watched themselves on the video-
with adults; Eder, 1989, 1990). Although young chil- tapes, most 4-year-olds located and removed the sticker
drens use of trait terms like good and naughty lacks the that was still on their heads. By contrast, younger 3-
rich meaning inherent in how older people use these con- year-olds recognized themselves in the videos but most
80 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

did not touch the stickers on their heads, apparently un- temperamental qualities index the self-regulatory and
able to associate the event on film with their current reactive qualities of behavioral style (see Rothbart &
condition (Povinelli, Landau, & Perilloux, 1996; Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume). Differ-
Povinelli & Simon, 1998; see also Povinelli, Landry, ences in temperamental effortful control best reflect
Theall, Clark, & Castille, 1999 for similar results using this feature of individuality, which have been found to
different procedures). emerge early in childhood and to be associated with bet-
The growth of the temporal self by age 4, as indexed ter emotion regulation, conscience development, and
by the delayed self-recognition task, is believed to be as- other adaptive qualities (Kochanska, 1993; Kochanska,
sociated with at least two interrelated conceptual Murray, & Coy, 1997; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan,
achievements: (1) a dawning awareness of the represen- 2000). Although neurobiological studies of the develop-
tational nature of knowledge (also relevant to theory of ment of self-regulation are still limited with children, it
mind development) and (2) the ability to reason in a is apparent that the growth of self-control is associated
causal temporal-spatial manner (Povinelli, 2001; Welch- with maturation of multiple regions of the prefrontal
Ross, 2001). Together they contribute to the childs real- cortex that are associated with emotional, attentional,
ization that knowledge is subjective and personal, cognitive, and behavioral self-control (Johnson, 1997).
knowledge will vary even though the self remains con- Finally, an extensive body of empirical literature docu-
stant, and current experience and knowledge are af- ments the association between self-regulatory compe-
fected by past influences on the self. There has not, tence in early childhood and sensitive, supportive
however, been definitive empirical examination of these maternal care and the association between parental
ideas. Performance on the delayed self-recognition task overcontrol, punitiveness, and negative affect and chil-
is positively correlated with delay of gratification in drens behavioral dysregulation (see Eisenberg, 2002;
preschoolers (Lemmon & Moore, 2001) and with some Fox & Calkins, 2003).
aspects of autobiographical memory, although not with These advances help to account for expanding re-
performance on theory of mind tasks (Welch-Ross, search interest in the development of self-regulation, but
2001; Zelazo, Sommerville, & Nichols, 1999). Much this remains an extraordinarily difficult area of study.
more research clearly is needed. Research into emotion regulation illustrates why (see
Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004, and commentaries that
Self-Regulation follow; also Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004). Emotional reg-
ulatory processes can be automatic or effortful, but dis-
Accompanying these multifaceted changes in self- tinguishing these is important to understanding their
awareness is growth in the young childs capacities for developmental course. The processes of and influences
self-management. According to Kopps (1982; Kopp & on emotion regulation are often indistinguishable from
Wyer, 1994) well-known formulation, the preschool those affecting emotional arousal, leading to uncer-
years witness significant advances in behavioral self- tainty over whether regulatory processes can be inde-
control because of growth in childrens capacities for re- pendently identified and studied. Moreover, emotion
membering, representing, and generalizing behavioral regulation can arise from external sources (such as the
standards, conceiving the self as an autonomous and re- efforts of caregivers) as well as self-initiated efforts,
sponsible agent, altering behavior in response to remem- and, although each manages emotion, extrinsic and in-
bered standards, and (somewhat later) engaging in a trinsic regulatory efforts entail different influences and
more continuous and self-generated monitoring of com- developmental course (Thompson & Meyer, in press).
pliance with these standards. Kopp regards the 2nd and This means that a child can display moderate levels of
3rd years as central to the development of self-control. emotional arousal appropriate to the situation, but this
The more mature and autonomous skills of self- can arise because of (a) the childs temperamental ef-
regulation are an achievement of the 4th year. fortful control; ( b) the coaching, support, and incentives
This formulation has been expanded in recent years of caregivers; (c) the fact that this child was not highly
with new appreciation of the temperamental, neurobio- aroused in the circumstances (owing to temperament,
logical, and caregiving contributions to the growth of prior experience, or other factors); and/or (d) the childs
self-regulation (see Eisenberg, 2002, for a review re- enlistment of emotional self-regulatory strategies. Dis-
lated to emotion regulation). Temperament theorists, tinguishing these influences on emotionality is a con-
most notably Rothbart (1989), have long recognized that ceptual and empirical challenge. Finally, individual
Self 81

differences in emotion regulation must be studied func- liant on carefully designed nonverbal responses or the
tionally to comprehend their relevance to broader differ- interrogatory assistance of questioners, is generally co-
ences in emotional or social competence (Thompson, herent and accurate (Howe & Courage, 1997; see gener-
1994). Young children may develop strategies of emo- ally Bauer 2002a, 2002b). In their view, once toddlers
tion regulation that are adaptive in some social contexts have become capable of representing themselves physi-
but maladaptive in others, and individual differences in cally and conceptually by the end of the 2nd year, per-
attentional, behavioral, cognitive, and emotional self- sonal memories become mnemonically tagged as
regulation may have common bases but also different autobiographical.
developmental pathways. Most other developmentalists portray the emergence
Taken together, these challenges do not mitigate the of autobiographical memory at a later age, however,
value of studying the development of self-regulation and owing in part to a stronger distinction between episodic
the origins and consequences of individual differences and autobiographical memory (see Fivush, 2001). In
in self-control. But they do indicate that considerably their view, autobiographical memory is distinctive be-
greater conceptual and empirical clarification of the na- cause it incorporates an awareness of the personal, pres-
ture of self-regulationand its attentional, emotional, ent significance of the past event. To Welch-Ross (1995,
cognitive, and behavioral componentsis needed to 2001), the social metacognitive skills essential to the de-
guide future inquiry. velopment of autobiographical memory concern knowl-
edge representationunderstanding how knowledge is
Development of Autobiographical Memory connected to unique experiences in the personal past
together with a personal, subjective stance to remem-
Autobiographical memory can be defined as explicit bered events, and the ability to reason about causal
memory of past events that is organized around the sig- connections between events across time. Perner (2001;
nificance of these events for the self. The growth of au- Perner & Ruffman, 1995) likewise implicates metacog-
tobiographical memory during the preschool years nitive skills, especially the capacity to comprehend au-
reflects advances in self-awareness but also other devel- tobiographical memories as personal reexperiences of,
oping capacities, including the developing representa- and thus deriving from, past events directly experienced.
tion of events in memory, social influences on the Nelson and Fivush (2004; see also Nelson, 1993b, 1996;
reconstruction and recall of past experiences, concep- Fivush, 2001) portray autobiographical memory as a dis-
tual skills related to the representation of knowledge, tinct memory system that builds on the development of a
and the influence of language in the construction of sense of self, theory of mind, knowledge representa-
memory and its reporting (Reese, 2002). Because of its tions, and an awareness of the temporal connections be-
complexity, there has been considerable debate among tween past and present events. They also emphasize the
developmental scientists concerning the nature of auto- influence of narrative discourse between the child and a
biographical memory and its developmental influences, caregiver during reminiscence as the means by which
accompanied by significantly expanded research atten- many of these conceptual foundations of autobiographi-
tion to this phenomenon. cal memory are fostered in early childhood.
One influential view has been offered by Howe and The conclusion of these theorists that autobiographi-
Courage (1993, 1997), who have proposed that autobio- cal memory emerges at age 3.5 or 4 is easy to reconcile
graphical memory emerges late in the 2nd year after the with research findings that most adults do not remember
development of the cognitive self, a knowledge struc- personal events from earlier than about age 3.5 (the end
ture that organizes memories of personal experiences. of childhood amnesia) and to integrate with other de-
The development of the cognitive self is, according to veloping conceptual achievements of early childhood,
these theorists, revealed at 18 months by the visual self- including theory of mind, self-understanding, and com-
recognition of toddlers in the mirror-rouge task and by prehension of psychological causality. In one empirical
other indicators of self-awareness at this time. Howe and test of alternative theoretical views, Harley and Reese
Courage argue that evidence for autobiographical mem- (1999) assessed 19-month-olds self-recognition in the
ory during this period can be found in research showing mirror-rouge task along with maternal reminiscing style
that personal events can be recalled by infants and in conversation with the child. They found that each
young children several weeks or months after their oc- variable uniquely predicted childrens later memory
currence, and the recall of these children, although re- skill: children who developed featural self-recognition
82 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

earlier and the offspring of mothers with an elaborative for example, the following brief conversation between a
reminiscing style were each more proficient in recalling 21-month-old and his mother about conflict over break-
personal experiences. However, subsequent analyses re- fast cereal earlier in the morning (from Dunn & Brown,
vealed that by age 2, the effects of early self-recognition 1991, p. 97):
ability on subsequent autobiographical recall were me-
diated by maternal reminiscing style (Reese, 2002). Child: Eat my Weetabix. Eat my Weetabix. Crying.
Thus, it seems that the emergence of the cognitive self Mother: Crying, werent you? We had quite a battle.
in early childhood is an important contributor to the de- One more mouthful, Michael. And what did you
velopment of autobiographical memory, but especially do? You spat it out!
in juxtaposition with social influences on the construc- Child: (pretends to cry)
tion of memory.
Regardless of their theoretical bent concerning age of In the mothers elaborated representation of their
onset, many developmental theorists agree that the shared experience, she provides her son with a temporal
growth of autobiographical memory entails social influ- sequence of events leading to his emotional reaction
ences as well as conceptual achievements. Nelson and (which was the source of his conversational prompt),
Fivush (2004) argue, in particular, that the content and emphasizing the significance of the event for him, and at
structure of narrative discourse with an adult about the same time conveying a representation of the event
shared experiences provides essential catalysts to the that was likely to be quite different from his own. In
representation of autobiographical events and the devel- doing so, she not only enlisted his direct representation
opment of self. These conceptual catalysts through nar- into a narrative structure for verbal sharing but also se-
rative include: (a) helping young children understand quenced essential features of their shared experience in
the personal significance of remembered events, some- a manner that made the episode more memorable. She
times with reference to prior experiences; ( b) enabling also contributed to his developing self-awareness by
young children to conceptualize their experience in a clarifying that although they shared this experience,
temporal-causal framework in which past events relate their viewpoints were different and thus the understand-
to present experience; (c) contributing to childrens ing they derived from it was different (and thus that
comprehension of the distinctiveness of their subjective knowledge is subjective). The mother provided, in short,
remembrance, partly as it compares (and conflicts) with a memorable narrative structure and lessons in under-
the adults own recollection; and (d) helping to structure standing and self. Although it is uncertain how much
and reorganize the childs direct representation of the this shared retelling would, at 21months, contribute to
experience into a form that is more memorable and can the development of an autobiographical memory, the
be shared. By scaffolding a young childs memory mothers scaffolding of her sons representations of
through narrative, adults foster the temporal under- events over time would be likely to contribute to memo-
standing, sense of self, subjective orientation, and other ries that are autobiographical in nature.
metacognitive skills relevant to autobiographical mem- Consistent with this view, longitudinal research
ory (Nelson & Fivush, 2004). Similar views have been shows that mothers with a more elaborative narrative
offered by Miller (1994, Miller et al., 1997; Wiley, style (i.e., richly descriptive and evaluative, providing
Rose, Burger, & Miller, 1998), who has emphasized how background and contextual information and eliciting in-
cultural and subcultural beliefs about the self become formation from the child) have children who themselves
appropriated by young children through the content of are later found to engage in more detailed, richer remi-
narrative discourseor personal storytellingwith niscing and provide more extensive autobiographical ac-
family members. counts compared to the offspring of mothers with a less
Autobiographical memory is thus not an individual elaborative narrative style (Farrant & Reese, 2000;
recollection but rather a shared construction. This is es- Haden, Haine, & Fivush, 1997; Harley & Reese, 1999;
pecially true early in childhood when direct representa- Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1993; see Reese, 2002 for a re-
tions of experience are likely to be somewhat view). Individual differences in maternal narrative style
disorganized and incomplete, and when the adult narra- are consistent across the preschool years (Farrant &
tive can provide the structure and interpretive frame- Reese, 2000; Reese et al., 1993) and across siblings
work that establishes the significance of personal events (Haden, 1998), although child characteristics (such as
to the child and makes them more memorable. Consider, age and gender) also influence maternal elaborativeness
Self 83

(Reese, 2002). One study found that in a socioeconomi- and American mothers portrayed the childs experience
cally disadvantaged sample, mothers who were trained consistently with the broader values of their cultures.
to use an elaborative style in conversation with their Chinese and Chinese-American mothers used story-
3.5-year-olds were found to use more contextual ques- telling with their children to convey moralistic themes,
tions and open-ended prompts a year later. By age 5.5, with an emphasis on the shame inherent in misconduct,
their children themselves were more elaborative in their while American mothers used storytelling for its enter-
narrative style (Peterson, Jesso, & McCabe, 1999). tainment value, emphasizing instead the childs spunk or
Taken together, these findings suggest that the detail mischievousness over misconduct (Miller, Potts, Fung,
and richness of young childrens autobiographical mem- Hoogstra, & Mintz, 1990; Miller et al., 1996, 1997).
ories are significantly influenced by the quality of rem- Wang, Leichtman, and Davies (2000) noted that Ameri-
iniscing they share with their caregivers, and that can mothers co-constructed reminiscences with their
conceptions of self may also be conceptually elaborated preschool offspring that emphasized the childs personal
in these conversational contexts. This conclusion is con- predilections and opinions, while Chinese mothers fo-
sistent with those of earlier discussions in this chapter cused on moral rules and behavioral standards (see also
concerning the influence of conversational catalysts on Mullen & Yi, 1995). The influence of these conversa-
the development of emotion understanding, theory of tional foci is reflected in the self-descriptions and auto-
mind, and other aspects of psychological understanding. biographical accounts of Asian and American young
In the context of shared reminiscing, young children children. American children have been found to be more
are likely to appropriate not only organized personal self-focused, use more internal state language and eval-
memories and a narrative style but also much more. Re- uations, and provide more detail about specific past
consider the earlier conversation between Michael and events, by contrast with the greater emphasis on social
his mother over Weetabix and notice the other lessons roles and relationships and daily routines of Asian chil-
provided by the adult in her representation of the morn- dren (Han, Leichtman, & Wang, 1998; Wang, 2004).
ings confrontation. The mother instructed her son about These findings suggest that the shared construction of
emotion and morality: In her portrayal, crying is associ- autobiographical narrative, and the self-understanding
ated with misbehavior and defiance (not with having to that relates to it, is one way that cultural values concern-
eat horrible breakfast cereal, which may have been her ing the self, relationships, and morality are conveyed in-
sons initial representation). She provided lessons about tergenerationally (Fivush, 2004).
the self: Good boys cooperate, but Michael was uncoop- Young children do not merely appropriate the repre-
erative and that is why he cried. There were also lessons sentations of personal experience interpreted by their
about relationships, which, according to his mother, are caregivers: They are active construers of their own di-
harmonious when sons are cooperative with their moth- rect experiences. Although the language and the narra-
ers requests but are disrupted by filial defiance (rather tive structure offered by the adult are extremely
than by maternal insistence). In short, the mother inter- important influences in shaping childrens personal rep-
preted the mornings events in her framework of as- resentations through conversation (partly owing to how
sumptions, causal attributions, beliefs, and values. language articulates and clarifies internal psychological
These beliefs constitute part of the context of mother- realities), as children develop competencies in repre-
child reminiscing and, although it is unclear how many senting, interpreting, and remembering personal experi-
of these lessons are likely to be internalized at age 2, ence they are likely to object to parental constructions
they are likely to have a significant influence on young of their experience that are dissonant with their own.
childrens developing conceptions of emotion, morality, Levine, Stein, and Liwag (1999) showed that parents and
self, and relationships as such conversations become young children commonly disagree about the childs
part of the landscape of parent-child interaction during feelings and experiences during shared events, often
the preschool years (Thompson et al., 2003). when adults make assumptions about the childs goals
Adults incorporate certain values into their shared that are incorrect, and that disagreements occur most
conversations with young children because they are cul- frequently for negative emotions. Disagreements
tural members who nonconsciously embrace cultural be- between parents and offspring also commonly arise
liefs about the self, relationships, and morality. In an when adults omit from their narrative crucial aspects of
observational study, Miller and her colleagues showed, the experience that figure prominently in childrens
for example, how the personal storytelling of Chinese own recollections. Little is known, however, about how
84 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

children respond to such discrepancies in parent-child ing populations. This is certainly true of the study of
conversation, and this is a topic meriting further inquiry early sociopersonality development. The most notable
because of its relevance to childrens comprehension of reflection of this integrative potential is in the study of
the personal, subjective nature of knowledge as well as early social cognition, where the developmentally
to attachment theorists views about the origins of the downward extension of theory of mind research touches
relational origins of defensive exclusion. on the long-standing interests of social developmental-
ists in the expectations and self-awareness deriving
Summary from social interaction in infancy. Research on con-
science development likewise integrates understanding
The conclusion that the early development of self de- of emotion, temperament, cognition, and parenting in
rives from an interaction of the childs construals of new ways of conceptualizing the interaction of child and
personal experience and the adults scaffolding of per- adult in the appropriation of moral standards. Attach-
sonal understanding confers on this topic unique re- ment theory and research benefits from the integration
search opportunities and challenges. Contrary to other of research on event representation, autobiographical
forms of psychological understanding, children have memory, and parent-child narrative in theoretical con-
special expertise concerning their characteristics, expe- ceptions of the internal working models associated with
riences, and self-knowledge that will increasingly vie security. The broader science of developing relation-
with the adults interpretations as children mature and ships is also increasingly integrating biological perspec-
acquire more robust, internally consistent self-referent tives into understanding of the nature and consequences
beliefs. Yet, because of their psychological sophistica- of early family relationships.
tion, caregivers are also uniquely insightful about the In these and other domains of early sociopersonality
feelings, motivations, and individuality of the children growth, development arises from the interaction of a
they care for. Moreover, they begin to shape young chil- young child, equipped with a powerfully inductive mind,
drens self-awareness from an early age, beginning with and people with whom the child is in continuous rela-
the emotional signals they convey concerning the childs tionship. Relational partners are conceptual catalysts
activities, to their appraisals of the childs successes and because of the ubiquity of their shared experiences with
failures, to the construction of autobiographical memory the young child; their intimate knowledge of the childs
in conversations about everyday events. Yet, there are characteristics, individuality, and developmental needs;
elements of the childs experiencesand construals of and the opportunities they enlist to stimulate behavioral
that experiencethat are always private and inaccessi- and conceptual growth (often without awareness of
ble to the adult, in part because they cannot easily doing so) in interactive activities. Throughout this re-
be conveyed. Somehow, in the context of the shared ac- view of research on early social understanding, con-
tivity of a young child, who is both expert and novice in science, and development of self, the importance of
self-understanding, and an adult, who has limited but parents, peers, siblings, and other relational partners is
sophisticated insight into the childs characteristics, continuously apparent.
self-understanding is jointly constructed. Understand- For this reason, the developmental model that seems
ing this developmental process is an important and valu- most useful in comprehending early sociopersonality
able challenge for future research. development is not socialization (which emphasizes the
child as the recipient of understanding) or construc-
tivism (which emphasizes the independently inductive
mind), but rather a model of the appropriation of under-
CONCLUSION standing through shared activity (Rogoff, 1990). This
neo-Vygotskian formulation emphasizes the shared cre-
Often the most innovative thinking in developmental ation of knowledge through the interaction of the child
science is integrative. A field that commonly parcels the with a partner in the everyday activities highlighted in
developmental process into separable domains or peri- this review, such as social interaction, relief of distress,
ods benefits from efforts to integrate insight from stud- conflict of wills, and shared conversation. Such a theo-
ies of cognitive and social functioning, or across stages retical orientation enables an integration of the pro-
of growth, or between typically and atypically develop- found insights into the developing mind provided by
References 85

cognitive-developmental scholars with the exquisite Allen, J., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cas-
sidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 319335).
studies of early social interaction offered by students of
New York: Guilford Press.
sociopersonality development and has the potential of Anan, R., & Barnett, D. (1999). Perceived social support mediates
generating new insights into early development. The between prior attachment and subsequent adjustment: A study of
value of such an orientation has been highlighted urban African American children. Developmental Psychology,
throughout this review, from research describing the as- 35, 12101222.
Arsenio, W., & Lover, A. (1999). Childrens conceptions of so-
sociation of maternal mind-mindedness with the de-
ciomoral affect: Happy victimizers, mixed emotions, and other
velopment of psychological understanding, to research expectancies. In M. Killen & D. Hart (Eds.), Morality in everyday
on attachment security as a mediator of the influence of life: Developmental perspectives (pp. 87128). New York: Cam-
parental discipline practices on moral internalization, to bridge University Press.
Astington, J., & Baird, J. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for
studies of the influence of parent-child conversation on
theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
the development of autobiographical memory. In each Bahrick, L., Moss, L., & Fadil, C. (1996). Development of visual self-
case, conceptual growth arises from the generative in- recognition in infancy. Ecological Psychology, 8, 189208.
fluence of social experience on a young mind that is Bahrick, L., & Watson, J. (1985). Detection of intermodal
powerfully prepared to glean new understanding from proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-
perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963973.
that experience.
Baird, J., & Baldwin, D. (2001). Making sense of human behavior. In
Bridging the conceptual perspectives of cognitive B. Malle, L. Moses, & D. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and inten-
and sociopersonality research is a significant advance tionality (pp. 193206). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
for future research, just as are efforts to bridge biologi- Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., van Ijzendoorn, M., & Juffer, F. (2003).
cal and social perspectives to relationships, and to Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment inter-
ventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195215.
bridge understanding of typical and atypical develop-
Baldwin, D. (2000). Interpersonal understanding fuels knowledge ac-
mental processes. Each is important because in the end, quisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 4045.
the continuing vitality of research in this area derives Baldwin, D., Baird, J., Saylor, M., & Clark, M. (2001). Infants parse
from our success in reassembling the developing child: dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 708717.
one who thinks, feels, and relates, who is both biological Baldwin, D., Markman, E., Bill, B., Desjardins, R., & Irwin, J.
(1996). Infants reliance on a social criterion for establishing
and social, who maintains continuity across time while
word-object relations. Child Development, 67, 31353153.
dramatic developmental changes occur, and who is in re- Baldwin, D., & Moses, L. (1996). The ontogeny of social
lationship with multiple partners in diverse social ecolo- information-processing. Child Development, 67, 19151939.
gies. Our capacities as research scientists to see the Banerjee, M. (1997). Hidden emotions: Preschoolers knowledge of
developing child as a coherent, integrated being under- appearance-reality and emotion display rules. Social Cognition,
15, 107132.
lies our capacities to imagine the developmental process
Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, nega-
for all of its complexity, scope, and vitality. tive expressivity, and the development of type D attachments from
12 to 24 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 64(Serial No. 258) 97118.
Barnett, D., Kidwell, S., & Leung, K. (1998). Parenting and pre-
REFERENCES schooler attachment among low-income urban African American
families. Child Development, 69, 16571671.
Barrera, M., & Maurer, D. (1981). Discrimination of strangers by the
Adamson, L., & Frick, J. (2003). The still face: A history of a shared
3-month-old. Child Development, 52, 558563.
experimental paradigm. Infancy, 4, 451473.
Barrett, K. (1998). The origins of guilt in early childhood. In J.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1973). The development of infant-mother at-
Bybee (Ed.), Guilt and children (pp. 7590). San Diego, CA: Aca-
tachment. In B. Caldwell & H. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of child de-
demic Press.
velopment research (Vol. 3, pp. 194). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press. Barrett, K., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Cole, P. (1993). Avoiders versus
amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and shame
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American
during toddlerhood? Cognition and Emotion, 7, 481505.
Psychologist, 44, 709716.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1990). Some considerations regarding theory Bartrip, I., Morton, I., & de Schonen, S. (2001). Responses to
and assessment relevant to attachments beyond infancy. In M. mothers face in 3-week to 5-month-old infants. British Journal of
Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in Developmental Psychology, 19, 219232.
the preschool years (pp. 463488). Chicago: University of Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1995). Children talk about the mind.
Chicago Press. London: Oxford University Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Pat- Bates, E. (1990). Language about me and you: Pronomial reference
terns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and the emerging concept of self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly
86 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

(Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 165182). The role of upright locomotion and its timing. Child Develop-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ment, 66, 499514.
Bates, J., & Bayles, K. (1988). Attachment and the development of be- Blurton-Jones, N. (1993). The lives of hunter-gatherer children: Ef-
havior problems. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical im- fects of parental behavior and parental reproductive strategy. In
plications of attachment (pp. 253299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. M. Pereira & L. Fairbanks (Eds.), Juvenile primates
Bateson, P. (1994). The dynamics of parent-offspring relationships in (pp. 309326). New York: Oxford University Press.
mammals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 399403. Bokhorst, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., Pasco Fearon, R., van
Bauer, P. (2002a). Developments in early recall memory: Normative Ijzendoorn, M., Fonagy, P., & Schuengel, C. (2003). The impor-
trends and individual differences. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in tance of shared environment in mother-infant attachment security:
child development and behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 103152). San A behavioral genetic study. Child Development, 74, 17691782.
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Booth, C., Rubin, K., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1998). Perceptions of emo-
tional support from mother and friend in middle childhood: Links
Bauer, P. (2002b). Early memory development. In U. Goswami (Ed.),
with social-emotional adaptation and preschool attachment secu-
Handbook of cognitive development (pp. 127146). Oxford, En-
rity. Child Development, 69, 427442.
gland: Blackwell.
Bornstein, M. (2002). Parenting infants. In M. Bornstein (Ed.),
Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental competence
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed.,
through socialization. In A. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on
pp. 343). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Child Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 346). Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press. Bost, K., Vaughn, B., Washington, W., Cielinski, K., & Bradbard, M.
(1998). Social competence, social support, and attachment: De-
Baumrind, D. (1978). Parental disciplinary patterns and social com- marcation of construct domains, measurement, and paths of inf lu-
petence in children. Youth and Society, 9, 239276. ence for preschool children attending Head Start. Child
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon Development, 69, 192218.
(Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 349387). San Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. SeparationAnxiety
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. and anger. New York: Basic Books.
Baumrind, D. (1996). A blanket injunction against disciplinary use of Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. LossSadness and de-
spanking is not warranted by the data. Pediatrics, 98, 828831. pression. New York: Basic Books.
Beckwith, L., Cohen, S., & Hamilton, C. (1999). Maternal sensitiv- Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.).
ity during infancy and subsequent life events relate to attach- New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1969)
ment representation at early adulthood. Developmental Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M., Powers, B., & Wang, X. (2001).
Psychology, 35, 693700. Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: Predic-
Belsky, J. (1981). Early human experience: A family perspective. De- tors of later attachment. Child Development, 72, 252270.
velopmental Psychology, 17, 323. Bretherton, I. (1990). Open communication and internal working
Belsky, J. (1999). Interactional and contextual determinants of at- models: Their role in the development of attachment relationships.
tachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation:
attachment (pp. 249264). New York: Guilford Press. Vol. 36. Socioemotional development (pp. 57113). Lincoln: Uni-
Belsky, J., Campbell, S., Cohn, J., & Moore, G. (1996). Instability versity of Nebraska Press.
of infant-parent attachment security. Developmental Psychology, Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bottles: The social
32, 921924. self as internal working model. In M. Gunnar & L. Sroufe (Eds.),
Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment: Theory and evidence. In Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 23. Self processes
M. Rutter & D. Hay (Eds.), Development through life and development (pp. 141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 373402). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986).
Belsky, J., & Isabella, R. (1988). Maternal, infant, and social- Learning to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective.
contextual determinants of attachment security. In J. Belsky & Child Development, 57, 529548.
T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. (1999). Internal working models in
(pp. 4194). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. attachment relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P.
Belsky, J., & Pasco Fearon, R. (2002a). Early attachment security, Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 89111). New York:
subsequent maternal sensitivity, and later child development: Guilford Press.
Does continuity in development depend upon continuity of care- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
giving? Attachment and Human Development, 4, 361387. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Belsky, J., & Pasco Fearon, R. (2002b). Infant-mother attachment se- Brown, J., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about
curity, contextual risk, and early development: A moderational mental states? The significance of childrens conversations with
analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 293310. friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Development, 67, 836849.
Belsky, J., Spritz, B., & Crnic, K. (1996). Infant attachment security Brown, J., & Dunn, J. (1991). You can cry, mum: The social and de-
and affective-cognitive information processing at age 3. Psycho- velopmental implications of talk about internal states. British
logical Science, 7, 111114. Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 237256.
Bigelow, A., MacLean, B., & MacDonald, D. (1996). Infants re- Brown, J., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding
sponse to live and replay interactions with self and mother. from 3 to 6 years. Child Development, 67, 789802.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 596611. Budwig, N. (2002). A developmental-functionalist approach to mental
Biringen, Z., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Appelbaum, M. (1995). Af- state talk. In E. Amsel & J. Byrnes (Eds.), Language, literacy, and
fective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: cognitive development (pp. 5986). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 87

Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional Clark, S., & Symons, D. (2000). A longitudinal study of Q-sort at-
behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664674. tachment security and self-processes at age 5. Infant and Child
Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus, P. (1990). Who am I? Self-understanding Development, 9, 91104.
in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 217238. Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Goossens, F., & Alhusen, V. (2001). Measur-
Campos, J., Anderson, D., Barbu-Roth, M., Hubbard, E., Hertenstein, ing infant-mother attachment: Is the strange situation enough? So-
M., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. In- cial Development, 10, 143169.
fancy, 1, 149219. Clutton-Brock, T. (1991). The evolution of parental care. Princeton,
Campos, J., Kermoian, R., & Zumbahlen, M. (1992). Socioemotional NJ: Princeton University Press.
transformations in the family system following infant crawling Cochran, M., Larner, M., Riley, D., Gunnarsson, L., & Henderson, C.
onset. In N. Eisenberg & R. Fabes (Eds.), Emotion and its regula- (1990). Extending families: The social networks of parents and
tion in early development (pp. 2540). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. their children. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Campos, J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: Cohn, J., Campbell, S., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face
The onset of social referencing. In M. Lamb & L. Sherrod (Eds.), interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother-
Infant social cognition (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. infant pairs at 2 months. Developmental Psychology, 26, 1523.
Carlson, E. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of attachment dis- Cohn, J., Campbell, S., & Ross, S. (1992). Infant response in the still-
organization /disorientation. Child Development, 69, 11071128. face paradigm at 6 months predicts avoidant and secure attachment
Carlson, E., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of ex- at 12 months. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 367376.
perience: A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Cole, P. (1986). Childrens spontaneous control of facial expressions.
Child Development, 75, 6683. Child Development, 57, 13091321.
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an under- Cole, P., Martin, S., & Dennis, T. (2004). Emotion regulation as a sci-
standing of mind: The development of childrens social under- entific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for
standing within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain child development research. Child Development, 75, 317333.
Sciences, 27, 7996.
Colin, V. (1996). Human attachment. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- to 18-
Collins, W., Maccoby, E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E., & Born-
month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental
stein, M. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case
actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315330.
for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232.
Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Understanding
prior intentions enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a Colman, R. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). Attachment security and
complex task. Child Development, 73, 14311441. the problem-solving behaviors of mothers and children. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 48, 337359.
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition,
joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Develop-
months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child ment. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of
Development, 63(4, Serial No. 255). early childhood development (J. Shonkoff & D. Phillips, Eds.).
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self in 6-year-
olds. Child Development, 59, 121134. Cooper, R., & Aslin, R. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech
in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 15841595.
Cassidy, J., Kirsh, S., Scolton, K., & Parke, R. (1996). Attachment
and representations of peer relationships. Developmental Psychol- Crittenden, P. (1992). Quality of attachment in the preschool years.
ogy, 32, 892904. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 209241.
Cassidy, J., & Marvin, R. (1992). Attachment organization in 3- and Crittenden, P. (1994). Preschool assessment of attachment. Unpub-
4-year-olds: Procedures and coding manual. Unpublished manu- lished manuscript, Family Relations Institute, Miami, FL.
script, Attachment Working Group, Department of Psychology, Crittenden, P. (2000). A dynamic-maturational approach to continu-
University of Virginia, Charlottesville. ity and change in patterns of attachment. In P. Crittenden & A.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment: The- Claussen (Eds.), The organization of attachment relationships
ory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press. (pp. 343358). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Chisholm, J. (1999). Death, hope and sex: Steps to an evolutionary ecol- Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-
ogy of mind and morality. New York: Cambridge University Press. year-olds; Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and
Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. (1995). Perspectives on developmental self-assertion. Developmental Psychology, 26, 961971.
psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmen- Cummings, E., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conf lict.
tal psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 320). New New York: Guilford Press.
York: Wiley. Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understand-
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S., & Lynch, M. (1995). Bowlbys dream comes full ing, language, and family background: Individual differences and
circle: The application of attachment theory to risk and psycho- interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853865.
pathology. In T. Ollendick & R. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in clinical Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (2002). The cost of understanding other peo-
child psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 175). New York: Plenum Press. ple: Social cognition predicts young childrens sensitivity to crit-
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S., & Rogosch, F. (1999). The efficacy of icism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 849860.
toddler-parent psychotherapy to increase attachment security Davies, P., & Cummings, E. (1994). Marital conf lict and child adjust-
in offspring of depressed mothers. Attachment and Human De- ment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,
velopment, 1, 34 66. 116, 387411.
88 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Davies, P., & Forman, E. (2002). Childrens patterns of preserving Dunn, J. (1996). Childrens relationships: Bridging the divide be-
emotional security in the interparental subsystem. Child Develop- tween cognitive and social development. Journal of Child Psychol-
ment, 73, 18801903. ogy and Psychiatry, 37, 507518.
Davies, P., Harold, G., Goeke-Morey, M., & Cummings, E. (2002). Dunn, J. (1999). Mindreading, emotion, and relationships. In P. Ze-
Child emotional security and interparental conf lict. Monographs of lazo, J. Astington, & D. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of in-
the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(Serial No. 270). tention (pp. 229242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dawson, G., Frey, K., Panagiotides, H., Yamada, E., Hessl, D., & Dunn, J. (2004). Childrens friendships. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Osterling, J. (1999). Infants of depressed mothers exhibit atypi- Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about
cal frontal electrical brain activity during interactions with
feeling states between mothers and their young children. Develop-
mother and with a familiar, nondepressed adult. Child Develop-
mental Psychology, 23, 132139.
ment, 70, 10581066.
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1991). Relationships, talk about feelings, and
Deater-Decker, K., & OConnor, T. (2000). Parent-child mutuality in
the development of affect regulation in early childhood. In J. Gar-
early childhood: Two behavioral genetic studies. Developmental
ber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation
Psychology, 36, 561570.
and dysregulation (pp. 89108). Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns pre-
sity Press.
fer their mothers voice. Science, 208, 11741176.
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1993). Early conversations about causality:
DeCasper, A., & Spence, M. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech inf lu-
Content, pragmatics, and developmental change. British Journal of
ences newborns perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior
Developmental Psychology, 11, 107123.
and Development, 9, 133150.
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling
DeMulder, E., Denham, S., Schmidt, M., & Mitchell, J. (2000). Q-
states and childrens later understanding of others emotions.
sort assessment of attachment security during the preschool
years: Links from home to school. Developmental Psychology, Child Development, 27, 448455.
36, 274282. Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Maguire, M. (1995). The development of chil-
Denham, S. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New drens moral sensibility: Individual differences and emotion un-
York: Guilford Press. derstanding. Developmental Psychology, 31, 649659.
Denham, S., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L.
Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: (1991). Young childrens understanding of other peoples feelings
Pathway to social competence. Child Development, 74, 238256. and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child
Denham, S., Blair, K., Schmidt, M., & DeMulder, E. (2002). Compro- Development, 62, 13521366.
mised emotional competence: Seeds of violence sown early? Dunn, J., Cutting, A., & Demetriou, H. (2000). Moral sensibility, un-
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72, 7082. derstanding others, and childrens friendship interactions in the
Denham, S., Caverly, S., Schmidt, M., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Caal, preschool period. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
S., et al. (2002). Preschool understanding of emotions: Contribu- 18, 159177.
tions to classroom anger and aggression. Journal of Child Psychol- Dunn, J., & Herrera, C. (1997). Conf lict resolution with friends, sib-
ogy and Psychiatry, 43, 901916. lings, and mothers: A developmental perspective. Aggressive Be-
Denham, S., Mason, T., Caverly, S., Schmidt, M., Hackney, R., havior, 23, 343357.
Caswell, C., et al. (2001). Preschoolers at play: Co-socializers of Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. (1998). Young childrens understanding of
emotional and social competence. International Journal of Behav- emotions within close relationships. Cognition and Emotion,
ioral Development, 25, 290301. 12, 171190.
Denham, S., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1987). The development of justifications in
preschoolers emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, disputes. Developmental Psychology, 23, 781798.
30, 928936.
Easterbrooks, M. (1989). Quality of attachment to mother and to father:
de Rosnay, M., & Harris, P. (2002). Individual differences in chil- Effects of perinatal risk status. Child Development, 60, 825830.
drens understanding of emotion: The roles of attachment and lan-
Easterbrooks, M., & Goldberg, W. (1990). Security of toddler-parent
guage. Attachment and Human Development, 4, 3954.
attachment: Relation to childrens sociopersonality functioning
de Rosnay, M., Pons, F., Harris, P., & Morrell., J. (2004). A lag be- during kindergarten. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cum-
tween understanding false belief and emotion attribution in
mings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years (pp. 221244).
young children: Relationships with linguistic ability and moth-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
ers mental-state language. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 22, 197218. Eder, R. (1989). The emergent personologist: The structure and con-
tent of 31 2 -, 51 2 -, and 71 2 -year-olds concepts of themselves and
De Wolff, M., & van Ijzendoorn, M. (1997). Sensitivity and attach-
other persons. Child Development, 60, 12181228.
ment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attach-
ment. Child Development, 68, 571591. Eder, R. (1990). Uncovering childrens psychological selves: Individual
Dozier, M., Stovall, K., Albus, K., & Bates, B. (2001). Attachment and developmental differences. Child Development, 61, 849863.
for infants in foster care: The role of caregiver state of mind. Eder, R., & Mangelsdorf, S. (1997). The emotional basis of early per-
Child Development, 72, 14671477. sonality development: Implications for the emergent self-concept.
Dunn, J. (1993). Young childrens close relationships. Newbury Park, In R. Hogan & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychol-
CA: Sage. ogy (pp. 209240). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Dunn, J. (1995). Children as psychologists: The later correlates of in- Egeland, B., & Farber, E. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Factors
dividual differences in understanding of emotions and other related to its development and changes over time. Child Develop-
minds. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 187201. ment, 55, 753771.
References 89

Egeland, B., Kalkoske, M., Gottesman, N., & Erickson, M. (1990). pressed behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Develop-
Preschool behavior problems: Stability and factors accounting for ment, 59, 15691579.
change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 891909. Fivush, R. (1993). Emotional content of parent-child conversations
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. (1981). Attachment and early maltreat- about the past. In C. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child
ment. Child Development, 52, 4452. Psychology: Vol. 23. Memory and af fect in development
Eisenberg, N. (2002). Emotion-related regulation and its relation to (pp. 3977). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
quality of social functioning. In W. Hartup & R. Weinberg Fivush, R. (1998). Gendered narratives: Elaboration, structure, and
(Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 32. Child emotion in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years.
psychology in retrospect and prospect (pp. 133171). Mahwah, In C. Thompson & D. Herrmann (Eds.), Autobiographical mem-
NJ: Erlbaum. ory: Theoretical and applied perspectives (pp. 79103). Mahwah,
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1995). The relation of young childrens NJ: Erlbaum.
vicarious emotional responding to social competence, regulation, Fivush, R. (2001). Owning experience: Developing subjective per-
and emotionality. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 203228. spective in autobiographical narratives. In C. Moore & K. Lem-
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. mon (Eds.), The self in time (pp. 3552). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of Fivush, R. (2004). Voice and silence: A feminist model of autobio-
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel- graphical memory. In J. Lucariello, J. Hudson, R. Fivush, & P.
opment (5th ed., pp. 701778). New York: Wiley. Bauer (Eds.), The development of the mediated mind (pp. 7999).
Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75, 334339.
Fox, N., & Calkins, S. (2003). The development of self-control of
Ellsworth, C., Muir, D., & Hains, S. (1993). Social competence and emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsic inf luences. Motivation and Emo-
person-object differentiation: An analysis of the still-face effect. tion, 27, 726.
Developmental Psychology, 29, 6373.
Fraley, R. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood:
Emde, R., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R., & Oppenheim, D. (1991). The Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mecha-
moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowledge. nisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 123151.
Developmental Review, 11, 251270.
Fraley, R., & Spieker, S. (2003). Are infant attachment patterns contin-
Emde, R., & Buchsbaum, H. (1990). Didnt you hear my Mommy? uously or categorically distributed? A taxometric analysis of
Autonomy with connectedness in moral self-emergence. In D. Cic- strange situation behavior. Developmental Psychology, 39, 387404.
chetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to child-
hood (pp. 3560). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Frankel, K., & Bates, J. (1990). Mother-toddler problem solving: An-
tecedents in attachment, home behavior, and temperament. Child
Erickson, M., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (1985). The relationship be-
Development, 61, 810819.
tween quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool in
a high-risk sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psychoanalysis. New York: Norton.
Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209), 147166. Frodi, A., Grolnick, W., & Bridges, L. (1985). Maternal correlates of
Fabes, R., Eisenberg, N., Nyman, M., & Michaelieu, Q. (1991). Young stability and change in infant-mother attachment. Infant Mental
childrens appraisals of others spontaneous emotional reactions. Health Journal, 6, 6067.
Developmental Psychology, 27, 858866. Gardner, W., Lamb, M., Thompson, R., & Sagi, A. (1986). On indi-
Fagot, B., & Kavanagh, K. (1990). The prediction of antisocial behav- vidual differences in strange situation behavior: Categorical and
ior from avoidant attachment classifications. Child Development, continuous measurement systems in a cross-cultural data set. In-
61, 864873. fant Behavior and Development, 9, 355375.
Farrant, K., & Reese, E. (2000). Maternal style and childrens partic- Ge, X., Conger, R., Cadoret, R., Neiderhiser, J., Yates, W., Troughton,
ipation in reminiscing: Stepping stones in childrens autobio- E., et al. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and
graphical memory development. Journal of Cognition and nurture: A mutual inf luence model of child antisocial behavior
Development, 1, 193225. and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 574589.
Fasig, L. (2000). Toddlers understanding of ownership: Implications Gekoski, M., Rovee-Collier, C., & Carulli-Rabinowitz, V. (1983). A
for self-concept development. Social Development, 9, 370382. longutudinal analysis of inhibition of infant distress: The origins of
Feinman, S., Roberts, D., Hsieh, K.-F., Sawyer, D., & Swanson, D. social expectations? Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 339351.
(1992). A critical review of social referencing in infancy. In S. Gelman, S., Taylor, M., & Nguyen, S. (2004). Mother-child conversa-
Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of tions about gender: Understanding the acquisition of essentialist
reality in infancy (pp. 1554). New York: Plenum Press. beliefs. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to moth- ment, 69(Serial No. 275).
erese. Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 181195. Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1999). Early socio-emotional develop-
Fernald, A. (1996). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness ment: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model.
to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Devel- In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 101136). Mahwah,
opment, 64, 657674. NJ: Erlbaum.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior- Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. (1988). The mutual regulation model: The
state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of infants self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive
nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychol- capacities. In T. Field, P. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman (Eds.),
ogy, 26, 714. Stress and coping (Vol. 2, pp. 4768). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendell, D., Schan- Gibson, E. (1995). Are we automata. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in
berg, S., et al. (1988). Infants of depressed mothers show de- infancy (pp. 315). Amsterdam: North Holland-Elsevier.
90 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Goodvin, R., Meyer, S., Thompson, R. A., & Hayes, R. (2005). Self- Harris, P. L. (in press). What do children learn from testimony? In P.
understanding in early childhood: Associations with attachment Carruthers, M. Siegel, & S. Stich (Eds.), Cognitive bases of sci-
security, maternal perceptions of the child, and maternal emotional ence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
risk. Unpublished manuscript, University of Nebraska, Lincoln. Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. A. (2005). Trust in testimony: How chil-
Gralinski, J., & Kopp, C. (1993). Everyday rules for behavior: dren learn about science and religion. Unpublished manuscript,
Mothers requests to young children. Developmental Psychology, Harvard University.
29, 573584. Harter, S., & Buddin, B. (1987). Childrens understanding of the si-
Greenberg, M. (1999). Attachment and psychopathology in child- multaneity of two emotions: A five-stage developmental acquisi-
hood. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment tion sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388399.
(pp. 469496). New York: Guilford Press.
Harwood, R., Miller, J., & Irizarry, N. (1995). Culture and attach-
Grossmann, K. E. (1999). Old and new internal working models of at- ment. New York: Guilford Press.
tachment: The organization of feelings and language. Attachment
Hastings, P., & Grusec, J. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers of re-
and Human Development, 1, 253269.
sponses to parent-child disagreement. Developmental Psychology,
Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., Huber, F., & Wartner, U. (1981).
34, 465479.
German childrens behavior towards their mothers at 12 months
and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworths strange situation. Haviland, J., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 157181. week-old infants responses to three emotion expressions. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 23, 97104.
Grusec, J., & Goodnow, J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline
methods on the childs internalization of values: A reconceptual- Hazen, C., & Shaver, P. (1994). Attachment as an organizational
ization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30, framework for research on close relationships. Psychological In-
419. quiry, 5, 122.
Grusec, J., Goodnow, J., & Kuczynski, L. (2000). New directions in Hendrix, R. (2004). Emergence of parental prohibition in response to
analyses of parenting contributions to childrens internalization self-produced locomotion and its accompanying emotional changes.
of values. Child Development, 71, 205211. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nebraska, Lincoln.
Gunnar, M., & Vazquez, D. (in press). Stress neurobiology and devel- Hertenstein, M., & Campos, J. (2004). The retention effects of an
opmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), adults emotional displays on infant behavior. Child Development,
Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adap- 75, 595613.
tation (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment interview: Historical and cur-
Haden, C. (1998). Reminscing with different children: Relating ma- rent perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
ternal stylistic consistency and sibling similarity in talk about the attachment (pp. 395433). New York: Guilford Press.
past. Developmental Psychology, 34, 99114. Heyman, G., Gee, C., & Giles, J. (2003). Preschool childrens reason-
Haden, C., Haine, R., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative ing about ability. Child Development, 74, 516534.
structure in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years.
Heyman, G., & Gelman, S. (1999). The use of trait labels in making
Developmental Psychology, 33, 295307.
psychological inferences. Child Development, 70, 604619.
Hains, S., & Muir, D. (1996). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant-
Heyman, G., & Gelman, S. (2000). Preschool childrens use of trait
adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 4961.
labels to make inductive inferences. Journal of Experimental
Hamilton, C. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment
Child Psychology, 77, 119.
from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 71,
690694. Hinde, R. (1988). Continuities and discontinuities: Conceptual issues
and methodological considerations. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Studies of
Han, J., Leichtman, M., & Wang, Q. (1998). Autobiographical mem-
psychosocial risk (pp. 367383). Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
ory in Korean, Chinese, and American children. Developmental
sity Press.
Psychology, 34, 701713.
Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Hirshberg, L., & Svejda, M. (1990). When infants look to their par-
Developmental Psychology, 35, 13381348. ents: Pt. 1. Infants social referencing of mothers compared to fa-
thers. Child Development, 61, 11751186.
Harris, P. (1989). Children and emotion. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Harris, P. (1997). Between strange situations and false beliefs: Work- Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought. New York: Macmillan.
ing models and theories of mind. In W. Koops, J. Hoeksma, & Hoffman, M. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral in-
D. C. van den Boom (Eds.), Development of interaction and at- ternalization. In E. Higgins, D. Ruble, & W. Hartup (Eds.), Social
tachment (pp. 187199). Amsterdam: Elsevier. cognition and social development (pp. 236274). New York: Cam-
Harris, P. (1999). Individual differences in understanding emotion: bridge University Press.
The role of attachment status and psychological discourse. Attach- Hoffman, M. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications
ment and Human Development, 1, 307324. for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Harris, P., Guz, G., Lipian, M., & Man-Shu, Z. (1985). Insight into Holden, G., Miller, P., & Harris, S. (1999). The instrumental side of
the time course of emotion among Western and Chinese children. corporal punishment: Parents reported practices and outcome ex-
Child Development, 56, 972988. pectancies. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 908919.
Harris, P., Johnson, C., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., & Cooke, T. (1989). Holden, G., & Miller, P. C. (1999). Enduring and different: A meta-
Young childrens theory of mind and emotion. Cognition and analysis of the similarity in parents child rearing. Psychological
Emotion, 3, 379400. Bulletin, 125, 233254.
Harris, P., & Nunez, M. (1996). Understanding of permission rules by Holden, G., Thompson, E., Zambarano, R., & Marshall, L. (1997).
preschool children. Child Development, 67, 15721591. Child effects as a source of change in maternal attitudes toward
References 91

corporal punishment. Journal of Social and Personal Relation- ating group exclusion or inclusion. Developmental Psychology,
ships, 14, 481490. 37, 587596.
Holden, G., & West, M. (1989). Proximate regulation by mothers: A Kisilevsky, B., Hains, S., Lee, K., Muir, D., Xu, F., Fu, G., et al.
demonstration of how differing styles affect young childrens be- (1998). The still-face effect in Chinese and Canadian 3- to 6-
havior. Child Development, 60, 6469. month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 629639.
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of Klinnert, M., Campos, J., Sorce, J., Emde, R., & Svejda, M. (1983).
maternal positive, neutral, and negative affect communications on Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In
infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937944. R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), EmotionTheory, research,
and experience: Vol. 2. Emotions in early development
Howe, M., & Courage, M. (1993). On resolving the enigma of infan-
(pp. 5786). New York: Academic Press.
tile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305326.
Klinnert, M., Emde, R., Butterfield, P., & Campos, J. (1986). Social
Howe, M., & Courage, M. (1997). The emergence and early development
referencing: The infants use of emotional signals from a
of autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499523.
friendly adult with mother present. Developmental Psychology,
Howe, N., Petrakos, H., & Rinaldi, C. (1998). All the sheeps are 22, 427432.
deadHe murdered them: Sibling pretense, negotiation, inter-
Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the develop-
nal state language, and relationship quality. Child Development,
ment of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62, 13791392.
69, 182191.
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization
Howes, C. (1999). Attachment relationships in the context of multiple and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child
caregivers. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attach- Development, 64, 325347.
ment (pp. 671687). New York: Guilford Press.
Kochanska, G. (1995). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline,
Howes, P., & Markman, H. (1989). Marital quality and child functioning: and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging inter-
A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 60, 10441051. nalization. Child Development, 66, 597615.
Hrdy, S. (1999). Mother nature. New York: Ballantine Books. Kochanska, G. (1997a). Multiple pathways to conscience for children
Hudson, J. (1990). The emergence of autobiographical memory in with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel-
mother-child conversation. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), opmental Psychology, 33, 228240.
Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 166196). Cam- Kochanska, G. (1997b). Mutually responsive orientation between
bridge: Cambridge University Press. mothers and their young children: Implications for early social-
Hudson, J. (1993). Understanding events: The development of script ization. Child Development, 68, 94112.
knowledge. In M. Bennett (Ed.), The child as psychologist: An in- Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Knaack, A., & Rhines, H. (2004). Mater-
troduction to the development of social cognition (pp. 142167). nal parenting and childrens conscience: Early security as a mod-
New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf. erator. Child Development, 75, 12291242.
Hughes, C., & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Nichols, K. (2003). Maternal power as-
Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young sertion in discipline and moral discourse contexts: Commonali-
friends. Developmental Psychology, 34, 10261037. ties, differences, and implications for childrens moral conduct
Jacobsen, T., Huss, M., Fendrich, M., Kruesi, M., & Ziegenhain, U. and cognition. Developmental Psychology, 39, 949963.
(1997). Childrens ability to delay gratification: Longitudinal re- Kochanska, G., Casey, R., & Fukumoto, A. (1995). Toddlers sensi-
lations to mother-child attachment. Journal of Genetic Psychol- tivity to standard violations. Child Development, 66, 643656.
ogy, 158, 411426. Kochanska, G., DeVet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putnam, S.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt. (1994). Maternal reports of conscience development and tempera-
Jenkins, J., Turrell, S., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., & Ross, H. (2003). A ment in young children. Child Development, 65, 852868.
longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in Kochanska, G., Forman, D., & Coy, K. (1999). Implications of the
families. Child Development, 74, 905920. mother-child relationship in infancy for socialization in the sec-
ond year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 249265.
Johnson, M. (1997). Developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford,
England: Blackwell. Kochanska, G., Gross, J., Lin, M.-H., & Nichols, K. (2002). Guilt in
young children: Development, determinants, and relations with a
Johnson, M., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development:
broader system of standards. Child Development, 73, 461482.
The case of face recognition. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. (2000). Mother-child mutually respon-
Kagan, J. (1981). The second year: The emergence of self-awareness.
sive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
early school age. Child Development, 71, 417431.
Kagan, J. (in press). Human morality and temperament. In G. Carlo & Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. (1997). Inhibitory control as a
C. Pope-Edwards (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early
51. Moral motivation through the lifespan. Lincoln: University of school age. Child Development, 68, 263277.
Nebraska Press.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Harlan, E. (2000). Effortful control
Kahana-Kalman, R., & Walker-Andrews, A. (2001). The role of per- in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and im-
son familiarity in young infants perception of emotional expres- plications for social development. Developmental Psychology,
sions. Child Development, 72, 352369. 36, 220232.
Kerns, K., & Richardson, R. (Eds.). (2005). Attachment in middle Kochanska, G., & Thompson, R. A. (1997). The emergence and de-
childhood. New York: Guilford Press. velopment of conscience in toddlerhood and early childhood. In J.
Killen, M., Pisacane, K., Lee-Kim, J., & Ardila-Rey, A. (2001). Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens internal-
Fairness or stereotypes? Young childrens priorities when evalu- ization of values (pp. 5377). New York: Wiley.
92 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Kopp, C. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental icance of individual dif ferences in strange situation behavior.
view. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199214. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kopp, C., & Wyer, N. (1994). Self-regulation in normal and atypical Lamb, S. (1993). First moral sense: An examination of the appear-
development. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Sym- ance of morally related behaviors in the second year of life. Jour-
posium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 5. Disorders and nal of Moral Education, 22, 97109.
dysfunctions of the self (pp. 3156). Rochester, NY: University of Legerstee, M. (1994). Patterns of 4-month-old infant responses to
Rochester Press. hidden silent and sounding people and objects. Early Development
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Dolev, S., Sher, E., & Etzion- and Parenting, 3, 7180.
Carasso, A. (2002). Mothers insightfulness regarding their in- Legerstee, M. (1997). Contingency effects of people and objects on
fants internal experience: Relations with maternal sensitivity subsequent cognitive functioning in 3-month-old infants. Social
and infant attachment. Developmental Psychology, 38, 534542. Development, 6, 307321.
Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, Legerstee, M., Anderson, D., & Schaffer, A. (1998). Five- and eight-
O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young childrens month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and
noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23, 799806.
social stimuli. Child Development, 69, 3750.
Kuczynski, L., Marshall, S., & Schell, K. (1997). Value socialization
Legerstee, M., Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., & Feider, H. (1987). The
in a bidirectional context. In J. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.),
development of infants responses to people and a doll: Implica-
Parenting and childrens internalization of values (pp. 2350).
tions for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Devel-
New York: Wiley.
opment, 10, 8195.
Lagattuta, K. (2005). When you shouldnt do what you want to do:
Legerstee, M., & Varghese, J. (2001). The role of maternal affect
Young childrens understanding of desires, rules, and emotions.
mirroring on social expectancies in 3-month-old infants. Child
Child Development, 76, 713733.
Development, 72, 13011313.
Lagattuta, K., & Wellman, H. (2001). Thinking about the past: Young
Lemmon, K., & Moore, C. (2001). Binding the self in time. In C.
childrens knowledge about links between past events, thinking,
Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time (pp. 163179). Mah-
and emotion. Child Development, 72, 82102.
wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lagattuta, K., & Wellman, H. (2002). Differences in early parent-
Levine, L., Stein, N., & Liwag, M. (1999). Remembering childrens
child conversations about negative versus positive emotions: Im-
plications for the development of emotion understanding. emotions: Sources of concordant and discordant accounts between
Developmental Psychology, 38, 564580. parents and children. Developmental Psychology, 35, 790801.
Lagattuta, K., Wellman, H., & Flavell, J. (1997). Preschoolers under- Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride,
standing of the link between thinking and feeling: Cognitive shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Hand-
cuing and emotional change. Child Development, 68, 10811104. book of emotions (pp. 563573). New York: Guilford Press.
Laible, D. (2004a). Mother-child discourse surrounding a childs past Lewis, M., Alessandri, S., & Sullivan, M. (1990). Violation of ex-
behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional understanding and pectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants.
early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill-Palmer Quar- Developmental Psychology, 26, 745751.
terly, 50, 159180. Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acqui-
Laible, D. (2004b). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links sition of self. New York: Plenum Press.
with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time.
competence. Developmental Psychology, 40, 979992. Child Development, 71, 707720.
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional Lockhart, K., Chang, B., & Story, T. (2002). Young childrens beliefs
understanding in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, about the stability of traits: Protective optimism? Child Develop-
34, 10381045. ment, 73, 14081430.
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, at- Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the
tachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child
development. Child Development, 71, 14241440. Development, 74, 11301144.
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). Mother-child conf lict in the Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disorganized in-
toddler years: Lessons in emotion, morality, and relationships. fant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial prob-
Child Development, 73, 11871203. lems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (in press). Foundations of socializa- classroom. Child Development, 64, 572585.
tion. In J. Grusec & P. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons, E. (1999). Maternal
(2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. frightened, frightening, or atypical behavior and disorganized in-
Lamb, M. (1981). Developing trust and perceived effectance in in- fant patterns. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
fancy. In L. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1, velopment, 64(Serial No. 258), 6796.
pp. 101127). New York: Ablex. Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M., & Cibelli, C. (1997). Infant at-
Lamb, M. (1997). The development of father-infant relationships. In tachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive
M. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (3rd symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems
ed., pp. 104120). New York: Wiley. at age 7. Developmental Psychology, 33, 681692.
Lamb, M., & Malkin, C. (1986). The development of social expecta- Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (1999). Attachment disorganization:
tions in distress-relief sequences: A longitudinal study. Interna- Unresolved loss, relational violence, and lapses in behavioral and
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 235249. attentional strategies. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Hand-
Lamb, M., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W., & Charnov, E. (1985). book of attachment (pp. 520554). Chicago: University of
Infant-mother attachment: The origins and developmental signif- Chicago Press.
References 93

Lyons-Ruth, K., Repacholi, B., McLeod, S., & Silva, E. (1991). Dis- lives: An evaluation of the self-perception scales of the Berkeley
organized attachment behavior in infancy: Short-term stability, Puppet Interview. Child Development, 69, 15561576.
maternal and infant correlates, and risk-related subtypes. Devel- Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security and internal working models:
opment and Psychopathology, 3, 377396. Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment and Human Develop-
Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the ment, 1, 325342.
family: Parent-child interaction. In P. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Re-
Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. So- thinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers comments on infants
cialization, personality, and social development (4th ed., mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months.
pp. 1101). New York: Wiley. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 637648.
Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Categories of response to reunion Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta,
with the parent at age 6: Predictable from infant attachment clas- M., Fradley, E., et al. (2003). Pathways to understanding mind:
sifications and stable over a 1-month period. Developmental Psy- Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-
chology, 24, 415426. mindedness. Child Development, 74, 11941211.
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents unresolved traumatic experi- Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M., Fradley, E., &
ences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment
frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mech- security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child De-
anism. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), At- velopment, 73, 17151726.
tachment in the preschool years (pp. 161182). Chicago:
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-en-
University of Chicago Press.
actment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmen-
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure- tal Psychology, 31, 838850.
disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In T. Brazelton &
Meltzoff, A., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in under-
M. Yogman (Eds.), Af fective development in infancy
standing persons and developing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-
(pp. 95124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as
other minds (pp. 335366). London: Oxford.
disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation.
In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds.), Attach- Miller, P. (1994). Narrative practices: Their role in socialization
ment in the preschool years (pp. 121160). Chicago: University of and self-construction. In U. Neisser & R. Fivush (Eds.), The re-
Chicago Press. membering self (pp. 158179). Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Main, M., & Weston, D. (1981). The quality of the toddlers rela-
tionship to mother and to father: Related to conf lict behavior and Miller, P., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., & Shell, R. (1996). Relations of
the readiness to establish new relationships. Child Development, moral reasoning and vicarious emotion to young childrens proso-
52, 932940. cial behavior toward peers and adults. Developmental Psychology,
32, 210219.
Malatesta, C., Culver, C., Tesman, J., & Shepard, B. (1989). The de-
velopment of emotion expression during the first 2 years of life. Miller, P., Fung, H., & Mintz, J. (1996). Self-construction through
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, narrative practices: A Chinese and American comparison of early
54(1/2, Serial No. 219). socialization. Ethos, 24, 237280.
Malatesta, C., Grigoryev, P., Lamb, C., Albin, M., & Culver, C. Miller, P., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Nar-
(1986). Emotion socialization and expressive development in rative practices and the social construction of self in childhood.
preterm and full-term infants. Child Development, 57, 316330. American Ethologist, 17, 292311.
Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas, Miller, P., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C.-H. (1997). Personal
D. (1990). Infant proneness-to-distress temperament, maternal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and Ameri-
personality, and mother-infant attachment: Associations and can families. Child Development, 68, 557568.
goodness of fit. Child Development, 61, 820831. Mondloch, C., Lewis, T., Budreau, D., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J.,
Marsh, H., Craven, R., & Debus, R. (1998). Structure, stability, and Stephens, B., et al. (1999). Face perception during early infancy.
development of young childrens self-concepts: A multicohort- Psychological Science, 10, 419422.
multioccasion study. Child Development, 69, 10301053. Montague, D., & Walker-Andrews, A. (2002). Mothers, fathers, and
Marsh, H., Ellis, L., & Craven, R. (2002). How do preschool chil- infants: The role of person familiarity and parental involvement in
dren feel about themselves? Unraveling measurement and multi- infants perception of emotional expressions. Child Development,
dimensional self-concept structure. Developmental Psychology, 73, 13391352.
38, 376393. Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of
Masters, J., & Wellman, H. (1974). The study of human infant attach- the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349372.
ment: A procedural critique. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 218237. Moore, C., & Lemmon, K. (2001). The nature and utility of the tem-
Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. (1978). Continuity of adaptation porally extended self. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self
in the second year: The relationship between quality of attach- in time (pp. 113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547556. Moses, L., Baldwin, D., Rosicky, J., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence
McCartney, K., Owen, M., Booth, C., Clarke-Stewart, A., & Vandell, for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12 and 18
D. (2004). Testing a maternal attachment model of behavior prob- months. Child Development, 72, 718735.
lems in early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- Moss, E., Bureau, J.-F., Cyr, C., Mongeau, C., & St.-Laurent, D.
atry, 45, 765778. (2004). Correlates of attachment at age 3: Construct validity of
Measelle, J., Ablow, J., Cowan, P., & Cowan, C. (1998). Assessing the preschool attachment classification system. Developmental
young childrens views of their academic, social, and emotional Psychology, 40, 323334.
94 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Moss, E., Cyr, C., Bureau, J.-F., Tarabulsy, G., & Dubois-Comtois, K. OConnor, T., Caspi, A., DeFries, J., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genotype-
(2004). Correlates and stability of attachment between preschool environment interaction in childrens adjustment to parent separa-
and early school-age. Unpublished manuscript, University of Que- tion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 849856.
bec at Montreal. OConnor, T., & Croft, C. (2001). A twin study of attachment in pre-
Moss, E., Cyr, C., & Dubois-Comtois, K. (2004). Attachment at early school children. Child Development, 72, 15011511.
school age and developmental risk: Examining family contexts Ontai, L., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). Patterns of attachment and ma-
and behavior problems of controlling-caregiving, controlling- ternal discourse effects on childrens emotion understanding
punitive, and behaviorally disorganized children. Developmental from 3- to 5-years of age. Social Development, 11, 433450.
Psychology, 40, 519532.
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2002). Mothers insightfulness
Moss, E., Parent, S., Gosselin, C., Rousseau, D., & St.-Laurent, D. regarding their childrens internal worlds: The capacity underly-
(1996). Attachment and teacher reported behavior problems dur- ing secure child-mother relationships. Infant Mental Health Jour-
ing the preschool and early school age period. Development and nal, 23, 593605.
Psychopathology, 8, 511525.
Oppenheim, D., Sagi, A., & Lamb, M. (1988). Infant-adult attach-
Mullen, M. K., & Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the ments on the kibbutz and their relation to socioemotional devel-
past: Implications for the development of autobiographical mem- opment 4 years later. Developmental Psychology, 24, 427433.
ory. Cognitive Development, 10, 407419.
Owen, M., & Cox, M. (1997). Marital conf lict and the development
Mumme, D., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning of infant-parent attachment relationships. Journal of Family Psy-
from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child chology, 11, 152164.
Development, 74, 221237. Owen, M., Easterbrooks, M., Chase-Lansdale, L., & Goldberg, W.
Mumme, D., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants responses to (1984). The relation between maternal employment status and the
facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing para- stability of attachments to mother and to father. Child Develop-
digm. Child Development, 67, 32193237. ment, 55, 18941901.
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interac- Pascalis, O., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabre-Grenet,
tions between 2-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & M. (1995). Mothers face recognition by neonates: A replication
N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177197). Nor- and an extension. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 7985.
wood, NJ: Ablex. Pasco Fearon, R., & Belsky, J. (2004). Attachment and attention: Pro-
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R., & Buss, K. tection in relation to gender and cumulative social-contextual ad-
(1996). Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderat- versity. Child Development, 75, 16771693.
ing role of attachment security. Child Development, 67, 508522. Perner, J. (2001). Episodic memory: Essential distinctions and devel-
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early opmental implications. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self
Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child in time (pp. 181202). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory an autonoetic
Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68, 860879. consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 516548.
Child Care Research Network. (2001). Child-care and family pre- Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives
dictors of preschool attachment and stability from infancy. Devel- in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Lan-
opmental Psychology, 37, 847862. guage, 26, 4967.
Neisser, U. (1995). Criteria for an ecological self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The Phillips, A., Wellman, H., & Spelke, E. (2002). Infants ability to
self in infancy (pp. 1734). Amsterdam: North Holland-Elsevier. connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cog-
Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1978). Event knowledge: Structure and function in nition, 85, 5378.
development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York:
Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: International Universities Press.
Harvard University Press. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Har-
Nelson, K. (1993a). Events, narratives, memory: What develops. In court, Brace.
C. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. Posada, G., Gao, Y., Posada, R., Tascon, M., Schoelmerich, A., Sagi,
26. Memory and af fect in development (pp. 124). Hillsdale, A., et al. (1995). The secure-base phenomenon across cultures:
NJ: Erlbaum. Childrens behavior, mothers preferences, and experts concepts.
Nelson, K. (1993b). The psychological and social origins of autobio- Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
graphical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 714. 60(Serial No. 244), 2748.
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence Povinelli, D. (1995). The unduplicated self. In P. Rochat (Ed.),
of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge. The self in early infancy (pp. 161192). Amsterdam: North
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical Holland-Elsevier.
memory: A social-cultural developmental theory. Psychological Povinelli, D. (2001). The self: Elevated in consciousness and ex-
Review, 111, 486511. tended in time. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: (pp. 7595). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In M. Lamb & A. Povinelli, D., Landau, K., & Perilloux, H. (1996). Self-recognition in
Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a
(pp. 131158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 15401554.
References 95

Povinelli, D., Landry, A., Theall, L., Clark, B., & Castille, C. (1999). States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child
Development of young childrens understanding that the recent Development, 71, 11211142.
past is causally bound to the present. Developmental Psychology, Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G. (2000).
35, 14261439. Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan.
Povinelli, D., & Simon, B. (1998). Young childrens understanding American Psychologist, 55, 10931104.
of brief ly versus extremely delayed images of the self: Emer- Rovee-Collier, C. (1989). The joy of kicking: Memories, motives, and
gence of the autobiographical stance. Developmental Psychology, mobiles. In P. R. Solomon, G. Goethals, C. Kelley, & B. Stephens
34, 188194. (Eds.), Memory: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 151180). New
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2005a). Family emotional cli- York: Springer-Verlag.
mate, attachment security, and young childrens emotion under- Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style af-
standing in a high-risk sample. British Journal of Developmental fects false belief understanding. Social Development, 8, 395411.
Psychology, 23, 117. Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2005b). Relationships past, pres- childrens and mothers mental state language and theory-of-mind
ent, and future: Ref lections on attachment in middle childhood. understanding. Child Development, 73, 734751.
In K. Kerns & R. Richardson (Eds.), Attachment in middle child- Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C., & Garnham, A.
hood (pp. 255282). New York: Guilford Press. (2003). How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion under-
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2005c). Links between risk and standing. Cognitive Development, 18, 139158.
attachment security: Models of inf luence. Applied Developmental Rutter, M. (1997). Nature-nurture integration: The example of anti-
Psychology, 26, 440455. social behavior. American Psychologist, 52, 390398.
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2005d). Early attachment and Rutter, M., & OConnor, T. (1999). Implications of attachment theory
subsequent parenting quality as predictors of early social cogni- for child care policies. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
tive development. Unpublished manuscript, University of Califor- of attachment (pp. 823844). New York: Guilford Press.
nia, Davis. Rutter, M., Silberg, J., OConnor, T., & Simonoff, E. (1999). Genetics
Rakison, D., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the and child psychiatry: Pt. 1. Advances in quantitative and molecu-
animate-inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209228. lar genetics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 318.
Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of autobiographi- Sabbagh, M., & Callanan, M. (1998). Metarepresentation in action:
cal memory: The state of the art. Social Development, 11, 124142. 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds developing theories of mind in parent-
Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1993). Parental styles of talking about the child conversations. Developmental Psychology, 34, 491502.
past. Developmental Psychology, 29, 596606. Sagi, A., Koren-Karie, N., Gini, M., Ziv, Y., & Joels, T. (2002). Shed-
Reese, E., Haden, C., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversa- ding further light on the effects of various types and quality of early
tions about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. child care on infant-mother attachment relationship: The Haifa
Cognitive Development, 8, 403430. Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 73, 11661186.
Repacholi, B. (1998). Infants use of attentional cues to identify the Sagi, A., van Ijzendoorn, M., Aviezer, O., Donnell, F., & Mayseless,
referent of another persons emotional expression. Developmental O. (1994). Sleeping out of home in a kibbutz communal arrange-
Psychology, 34, 10171025. ment: It makes a difference for infant-mother attachment. Child
Development, 65, 9921004.
Repacholi, B., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires:
Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychol- Sameroff, A., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the con-
ogy, 33, 1221. tinuum of caretaking casualty. In F. Horowitz, E. Hetherington,
S. Scarr-Salapatek, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review of child develop-
Richters, J., Waters, E., & Vaughn, B. (1988). Empirical classifica-
ment research (Vol. 4, pp. 187244). Chicago: University of
tion of infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and
Chicago Press.
crying during reunion. Child Development, 59, 512522.
Schneider, B., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child-parent attach-
Ridgeway, D., Waters, E., & Kuczaj, S. (1985). Acquisition of
ment and childrens peer relations: A quantitative review. Devel-
emotion-descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabu-
opmental Psychology, 37, 86100.
lary norms for ages 18 months to 6 years. Developmental Psychol-
Schuengel, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., & van Ijzendoorn, M.
ogy, 21, 901908.
(1999). Frightening maternal behavior linking unresolved loss and
Rochat, P., & Morgan, R. (1995). Spatial determinants in the percep- disorganized infant attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
tion of self-produced leg movements by 3- to 5-month-old infants. cal Psychology, 67, 5463.
Developmental Psychology, 31, 626636.
Schultz, D., Izard, C., & Ackerman, B. (2000). Childrens anger attri-
Rochat, P., Neisser, U., & Marian, V. (1998). Are young infants sensi- bution bias: Relations to family environment and social adjust-
tive to interpersonal contingency? Infant Behavior and Develop- ment. Social Development, 9, 284301.
ment, 21, 355366.
Shaw, D., Keenan, K., Vondra, J., Delliquadri, E., & Giovanelli, J.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. New York: Oxford Uni- (1997). Antecedents of preschool childrens internalizing problems:
versity Press. A longitudinal study of low-income families. Journal of the Ameri-
Rothbart, M. (1989). Temperament in childhood: A framework. In G. can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 17601767.
Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in Shaw, D., Owens, E., Vondra, J., Keenan, K., & Winslow, E.
childhood (pp. 5973). New York: Wiley. (1996). Early risk factors and pathways in the development of
Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000). early disruptive behavior problems. Development and Psycho-
The development of close relationships in Japan and the United pathology, 8, 679699.
96 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Slade, A. (1987). Quality of attachment and early symbolic play. De- Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in
velopmental Psychology, 23, 7885. young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Smetana, J., & Braeges, J. (1990). The development of toddlers Development, 57(Serial No. 226).
moral and conventional judgments. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Striano, T. (2004). Direction of regard and the still-face effect in the
36, 329346. first year: Does intention matter? Child Development, 75, 468479.
Smetana, J., Kochanska, G., & Chuang, S. (2000). Mothers concep- Symons, D., & Clark, S. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother-child
tions of everyday rules for young toddlers: A longitudinal investi- relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social
gation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 391416. Development, 9, 323.
Smetana, J. G. (1981). Preschool childrens conceptions of moral and Symons, D., Clark, S., Isaksen, G., & Marshall, J. (1998). Stability of
social rules. Child Development, 52, 13331336. q-sort attachment security from age 2 to 5. Infant Behavior and
Smetana, J. G. (1997). Parenting and the development of social Development, 21, 785791.
knowledge reconceptualized: A social domain analysis. In J. Takahashi, K. (1986). Examining the strange-situation procedure
Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens internal- with Japanese mothers and 12-month-old infants. Developmental
ization of values (pp. 162192). New York: Wiley. Psychology, 22, 265270.
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment Takahashi, K. (1990). Are the key assumptions of the strange situa-
security in infancy and childhood. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver tion procedure universal? A view from Japanese research.
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 287316). New York: Guil- Human Development, 33, 2330.
ford Press. Teti, D. (1999). Conceptualizations of disorganization in the pre-
Sorce, J., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. (1985). Maternal school years. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disor-
emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1- ganization (pp. 213242). New York: Guilford Press.
year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195200.
Teti, D., & McGourty, S. (1996). Using mothers versus observers of
Spangler, G., & Delius, A. (2003, August). The inner working model childrens secure base behavior: Theoretical and methodological
as a theory of attachment. Paper presented at the 11th Euro- considerations. Child Development, 67, 597596.
pean Conference on Developmental Psychology in Milano, Italy.
Teti, D., Sakin, J., Kucera, E., Corns, K., & Das Eiden, R. (1996).
Spelke, E., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. (1995). Infants knowledge And baby makes four: Predictors of attachment security among
about object motion and human action. In D. Sperber, D. preschool-aged firstborns during the transition to siblinghood.
Premack, & A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 4478). Child Development, 67, 579596.
Oxford, England: Clarendon.
Theimer, C., Killen, M., & Stangor, C. (2001). Preschool childrens
Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of evaluations of exclusion in gender-stereotypic contexts. Develop-
adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladaptation and compe- mental Psychology, 37, 1827.
tence. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psy-
Thompson, R. A. (1986). Temperament, emotionality, and infant so-
chology: Vol. 16. Development and policy concerning children with
cial cognition. In J. Lerner & R. Lerner (Eds.), Temperament and
special needs (pp. 4183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
social interaction during infancy and childhood (pp. 3552). San
Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant-caregiver attachment in devel-
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
opment. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications
of attachment (pp. 1838). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In R. A.
Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 36. So-
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. (2005). Min-
cioemotional development (pp. 383483). Lincoln: University of
nesota Study of Risk and Adaptation from birth to maturity: The
Nebraska Press.
development of the person. New York: Guilford Press.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early
definition. In N. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the Society for Re-
experience following developmental change: Longitudinal ap-
search in Child Development, 59(Serial No. 240), 2552.
proaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Develop-
ment, 61, 13631373. Thompson, R. A. (1995). Preventing child maltreatment through so-
cial support. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Steele, H., Steele, M., Croft, C., & Fonagy, P. (1999). Infant-mother
attachment at 1 year predicts childrens understanding of mixed Thompson, R. A. (1997). Sensitivity and security: New questions to
emotions at 6 years. Social Development, 8, 161178. ponder. Child Development, 68, 595597.
Stein, N., & Levine, L. (1989). The causal organization of emo- Thompson, R. A. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In W.
tional knowledge: A developmental study. Cognition and Emo- Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
tion, 3, 343378. child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Shouldice, A. (1995). Maternal interactions opment (5th ed., pp. 25104). New York: Wiley.
and self-reports related to attachment classifications at 4.5 years. Thompson, R. A. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In
Child Development, 66, 583596. J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Verschueren, K. (2002). Attachment in child- (pp. 265286). New York: Guilford Press.
hood. In P. Smith & C. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood Thompson, R. A. (2000). The legacy of early attachments. Child De-
social development (pp. 182204). Oxford, England: Blackwell. velopment, 71(1), 145152.
Stipek, D. (1995). The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In Thompson, R. A. (2005). Multiple relationships multiply considered.
J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions Human Development, 48, 102107.
(pp. 237252). New York: Guilford Press. Thompson, R. A. (2006). Conversation and developing understand-
Stipek, D., Gralinski, J., & Kopp, C. (1990). Self-concept develop- ing: Introduction to the special issue. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
ment in the toddler years. Developmental Psychology, 26, 972977. 52, 116.
References 97

Thompson, R. A. (in press). Developing persons, relationships, and Tronick, E. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in in-
science. Human Development. fants. American Psychologist, 44, 112119.
Thompson, R. A., Braun, K., Grossmann, K. E., Gunnar, M., Hein- Valenzuela, M. (1990). Attachment in chronically underweight young
richs, M., Keller, H., et al. (2005). Early social attachment and its children. Child Development, 61, 19841996.
consequences: The dynamics of a developing relationship. In C. Valenzuela, M. (1997). Maternal sensitivity in a developing society:
Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossmann, M. Lamb, S. Porges, & N. The context of urban poverty and infant chronic under nutrition.
Sachser (Eds.), Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis (Dahlem Developmental Psychology, 33, 845855.
Workshop report 92, pp. 349383). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. van Ijzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations,
Thompson, R. A., Connell, J., & Bridges, L. (1988). Temperament, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis
emotion, and social interactive behavior in the strange situation: on the predictive validity of the adult attachment interview. Psy-
A component process analysis of attachment system functioning. chological Bulletin, 117, 387403.
Child Development, 59, 11021110. van Ijzendoorn, M., & De Wolff, M. (1997). In search of the absent
Thompson, R. A., Flood, M., & Goodvin, R. (2005). Social support father: Meta-analyses of infant-father attachment: A rejoinder to
and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen our discussants. Child Development, 68, 604609.
(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and van Ijzendoorn, M., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of at-
adaptation (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. tachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In J. Cassidy & P.
Thompson, R. A., & Lagattuta, K. (2005). Feeling and understanding: Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 713734). New York:
Early emotional development. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips Guilford Press.
(Ed.), The Blackwell handbook of early childhood development. van Ijzendoorn, M., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.
Oxford, England: Blackwell. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-
Thompson, R. A., Laible, D., & Ontai, L. (2003). Early understanding analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development
of emotion, morality, and the self: Developing a working model. and Psychopathology, 11, 225249.
In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. van Ijzendoorn, M., van der Veer, R., & van Vliet-Visser, S. (1987).
31, pp. 137171). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Attachment 3 years later: Relationships between quality of
Thompson, R. A., & Lamb, M. (1983). Security of attachment mother-infant attachment and emotional /cognitive development in
and stranger sociability in infancy. Developmental Psychology, kindergarten. In L. Tavecchio & M. van Ijzendoorn (Eds.), At-
19, 184191. tachment in social networks (pp. 185224). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Thompson, R. A., Lamb, M., & Estes, D. (1982). Stability of infant- van Ijzendoorn, M., Vereijken, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., &
mother attachment and its relationship to changing life circum- Riksen-Walraven, J. (2004). Assessing attachment security with
stances in an unselected middle-class sample. Child Development, the attachment q sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of
53, 144148. the observer AQS. Child Development, 75, 11881213.
Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (in press). The socialization of emo- Vaughn, B., & Bost, K. (1999). Attachment and temperament: Redun-
tion regulation in the family. In J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emo- dant, independent, or interacting inf luences on interpersonal
tion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. adaptation and personality development. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver
Thompson, R. A., Meyer, S., & McGinley, M. (2006). Understanding (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 198225). New York: Guil-
values in relationship: The development of conscience. In M. ford Press.
Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development Vaughn, B., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L., & Waters, E. (1979). Individual
(pp. 267297). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. differences in infant-mother attachment at 12 and 18 months:
Thompson, R. A., & Raikes, H. A. (2003). Toward the next quarter- Stability and change in families under stress. Child Development,
century: Conceptual and methodological challenges for attach- 50, 971975.
ment theory. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 691718. Vondra, J., Shaw, D., Swearingen, L., Cohen, M., & Owens, E.
Tomasello, M. (1995a). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. (2001). Attachment stability and emotional and behavioral regula-
Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention (pp. 105130). Hills- tion from infancy to preschool age. Development and Psychopath-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. ology, 13, 1333.
Tomasello, M. (1995b). Understanding the self as social agent. In P. Walden, T., & Ogan, T. (1988). The development of social referenc-
Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy (pp. 449460). Amsterdam: ing. Child Development, 59, 12301240.
North Holland-Elsevier. Walker-Andrews, A. (1997). Infants perception of expressive behav-
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cam- iors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bulletin, 121, 437456.
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in nonostensive Walton, G., Bower, N., & Bower, T. (1992). Recognition of familiar
contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639650. faces by newborns. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 265269.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (in Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobio-
press). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cul- graphical memory and self-description in European American
tural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, in press. and Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 315.
Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition Wang, Q., Leichtman, M., & Davies, K. (2000). Sharing memories
unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-
Mind and Language, 18, 121147. year-olds. Memory, 8, 159178.
Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Wartner, U., Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., & Suess, G.
Benjamin /Cummings. (1994). Attachment patterns at age 6 in South Germany:
98 The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Conscience, Self

Predictability from infancy and implications for preschool behav- Wellman, H., Phillips, A., Dunphy-Lelii, S., & LaLonde, N. (2004).
ior. Child Development, 65, 10141027. Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. Devel-
Waters, E. (1978). The reliability and stability of individual differences opmental Science, 7, 283288.
in infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 49, 483494. Wellman, H., & Woolley, J. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary
Waters, E., & Cummings, E. (2000). A secure base from which to ex- beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cogni-
plore close relationships. Child Development, 71, 164172. tion, 35, 245275.
Waters, E., & Deane, K. (1985). Defining and assessing individual Wiley, A., Rose, A., Burger, L., & Miller, P. (1998). Constructing au-
differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the tonomous selves through narrative practices: A comparative study
organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. In I. of working-class and middle-class families. Child Development,
Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Monographs of the Society for Re- 69, 833847.
search in Child Development, 50(Serial No. 209), 4165. Wintre, M., & Vallance, D. (1994). A developmental sequence in the
Waters, E., Kondo-Ikemura, K., Posada, G., & Richters, J. (1991). comprehension of emotions: Intensity, multiple emotions, and va-
Learning to love: Mechanisms and milestones. In M. Gunnar & lence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 509514.
L. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an
23. Self processes and development (pp. 217255). Hillsdale, actors reach. Cognition, 69, 134.
NJ: Erlbaum. Woodward, A. (2003). Infants developing understanding of the link
Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L. between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6, 297311.
(2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A Woodward, A., & Guajardo, J. (2002). Infants understanding of the
20-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 71, 684689. point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development,
Waters, E., Weinfield, N., & Hamilton, C. (2000). The stability of at- 17, 10611084.
tachment security from infancy to adolescence and early adult- Woodward, A., & Sommerville, J. (2000). Twelve-month-old infants
hood: General discussion. Child Development, 71, 703706. interpret action in context. Psychological Science, 11, 7377.
Watson, J. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of so- Youngblade, L., & Belsky, J. (1992). Parent-child antecedents of 5-
cial responsiveness. In E. Thoman (Ed.), Origins of the infants year-olds close friendships: A longitudinal analysis. Develop-
social responsiveness (pp. 3364). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. mental Psychology, 28, 700713.
Watson, J. (1985). Contingency perception in early social develop- Youngblade, L., & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young
ment. In T. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants childrens pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relation-
(pp. 157176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. ships and understanding of other peoples feelings and beliefs.
Watson, J. S. (1995). Mother-infant interaction: Dispositional proper- Child Development, 66, 14721492.
ties and mutual designs. In N. Thompson (Ed.), Perspectives in Yuill, N., & Pearson, A. (1998). The development of bases for trait at-
ethology (pp. 189210). New York: Plenum Press. tribution: Childrens understanding of traits as causal mecha-
Weinfield, N., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from in- nisms based on desire. Developmental Psychology, 34, 574586.
fancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, dis- Zahn-Waxler, C. (2000). The development of empathy, guilt, and in-
continuity, and their correlates. Child Development, 71, 695702. ternalization of distress: Implications for gender differences in
Weinfield, N., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (1999). The internalizing and externalizing problems. In R. J. Davidson (Ed.),
nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In Anxiety, depression, and emotion (pp. 222265). New York: Ox-
J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment ford University Press.
(pp. 6888). New York: Guilford Press. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of em-
Welch-Ross, M. (1995). An integrative model of the development of pathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107130.
autobiographical memory. Developmental Review, 15, 338365. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Robinson, J. (1995). Empathy and guilt: Early ori-
Welch-Ross, M. (2001). Personalizing the temporally extended self: gins of feelings of responsibility. In J. Tangney & K. Fischer (Eds.),
Evaluative self-awareness and the development of autobiographi- Self-conscious emotions (pp. 143173). New York: Guilford Press.
cal memory. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. (1985). Social referencing as a function of
(pp. 97120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior
Wellman, H. (2002). Understanding the psychological world: Develop- and Development, 8, 2533.
ing a theory of mind. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Handbook of childhood Zelazo, P., Sommerville, J., & Nichols, S. (1999). Age-related
cognitive development (pp. 167187). Oxford, England: Blackwell. changes in childrens use of external representations. Develop-
Wellman, H., & Banerjee, M. (1991). Mind and emotion: Childrens mental Psychology, 35, 10591071.
understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and de- Zimmermann, P., & Grossmann, K. E. (1997). Attachment and adap-
sires. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 191214. tation in adolescence. In W. Koops, J. Hoeksma, & D. van den
Wellman, H., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of Boom (Eds.), Development of interaction and attachment
theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. (pp. 281292). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Child Development, 72, 655 684.
Zumbahlen, M., & Crawley, A. (1996, April). Infants early referen-
Wellman, H., Harris, P., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A. (1995). Early tial behavior in prohibition contexts: The emergence of social ref-
understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cog- erencing. In R. A. Thompson (Chair), Taking perspective on social
nition and Emotion, 9, 117149. referencing: New viewpoints. Invited symposium conducted at the
Wellman, H., & Miller, J. (2003). Integrating deontic reasoning and biennial meeting of the International Society for Infant Studies,
theory of mind. Manuscript submitted for publication. Providence, RI.
CHAPTER 3

Temperament
MARY K. ROTHBART and JOHN E. BATES

A DEFINITION OF TEMPERAMENT 99 Behavioral Genetics 119


Temperament and Personality 100 MOLECULAR GENETICS AND TEMPERAMENT 120
HISTORY OF TEMPERAMENT: RESEARCH Approach/ Withdrawal and Hemispheric Asymmetry 122
ON CHILDHOOD 101 Autonomic Reactivity and Self-Regulation 122
Clinical Research 101 Cortisol Reactivity 124
Acceptance of Temperament Research 102 TEMPERAMENT AND DEVELOPMENT 125
HISTORY OF TEMPERAMENT IN ADULTHOOD 102 Contributions of Temperament to Development 126
Jungs Theory 102 Extraversion /Surgency versus Shyness and Behavioral
Eastern and Western Schools of Temperament and Inhibition 127
Personality 103 Attentional Orienting and Effortful Control 129
THE STRUCTURE OF TEMPERAMENT 104 TEMPERAMENT AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF
Infant Studies 104 PERSONALITY 132
Childhood Studies 105 TEMPERAMENT AND ADJUSTMENT 134
NEURAL MODELS OF TEMPERAMENT 106 Does Temperament Predict Adjustment? 135
Emotion as an Information-Processing System 107 Empirical and Theoretical Limits on Continuity 141
Attention as a Control System 107 Moderated Linkage 142
Positive Emotionality/Approach and Extraversion 107 Self-Regulation Environment 143
Fear and Behavioral Inhibition 108 Negative Emotionality Environment 146
Optimal Levels of Stimulation and Distress to Fearfulness Environment 149
Overstimulation 109 CHAPTER CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE
Anger/ Irritability 110 DIRECTIONS 152
Negative Emotionality or General Distress Proneness 110 Differentiating Temperament Dimensions 152
Affiliativeness/Agreeableness 111 Measures 152
Attentional Networks 112 Development 153
MEASUREMENT APPROACHES 113 REFERENCES 153
Meanings of Parent Reports 115
PSYCHOBIOLOGICAL RESEARCH APPROACHES 119

Recent years have witnessed major advances in our un- in temperament, differences observed by parents and
derstanding of temperament in childhood. Early views physicians long before their systematic study by stu-
on temperament as unchanging and stable have been re- dents of human development.
placed by more dynamic views of developmental change
in temperament. An early emphasis on temperament in
infancy has been extended to the study of childhood and
adolescence, and research on temperament has bur- A DEFINITION OF TEMPERAMENT
geoned (Rothbart & Derryberry, 2002). In addition,
rapid advances have been made in our understanding of Gordon Allport (1961) defined temperament as the
temperament-environment interactions. In this chapter, characteristic phenomena of an individuals emotional
we explore both historical influences and more recent nature, including his susceptibility to emotional stimu-
advances in our understanding of individual differences lation, his customary strength and speed of response,

99
100 Temperament

the quality of his prevailing mood, these phenomena cludes much more than temperament, particularly the
being regarded as dependent upon constitutional make- content of thought, skills, habits, values, defenses,
up and, therefore, largely hereditary in origin (p. 34). morals, beliefs, and social cognition. Social cognition
Allports definition focused on individual differences in includes the perception of the self, others, and the rela-
emotional reactivity. Thomas and Chess (1977), how- tion of self to objects, events, and others. Over time, so-
ever, took a broader approach to temperament, including cial cognition becomes increasingly important in
individual differences in attention and activity level. eliciting and moderating temperamental processes. This
Taking into account both of these approaches, we happens, for example, when anger comes to be elicited
have defined temperament as constitutionally based in- by judgments that others have broken the rules when we
dividual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, in have been following them. These perceptions can be in-
the domains of affect, activity, and attention (Rothbart fluenced by temperament (Derryberry & Reed, 1994),
& Bates, 1998; Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). By the but they involve separable thought processes as well.
term constitutional, we refer to the biological bases of Personality traits have been defined as patterns of
temperament, influenced over time by heredity, matura- thoughts, emotion, and behavior that show consistency
tion, and experience. Reactivity and self-regulation are across situations and stability over time, and that af-
umbrella terms that broadly organize the temperament fect the individuals getting along with other people and
domain. By reactivity, we refer to responsiveness to with himself (Hilgard, 1962, p. 447). Temperament
change in the external and internal environment. Reac- traits similarly show consistency across situations and
tivity includes a broad range of reactions (e.g., the emo- stability over time, but they are limited to basic
tions of fear, cardiac reactivity) and more general processes of reactivity and self-regulation, and do not
tendencies (e.g., negative emotionality), thus it is not include the specific content of thought or the use of con-
limited to general reactivity. Parameters of reactivity ceptually based defenses (e.g., paranoia).
are measured by the latency, duration, and intensity pa- In our view, temperament is the appropriate term for
rameters of motor, affective, and attentional reactions describing individual differences in reactivity and self-
(e.g., fear, anger, positive affect, or orienting; Rothbart regulation in nonhuman animals and young infants. Al-
& Derryberry, 1981). Emotional reactivity also includes though some researchers refer to individual differences
action tendencies. Thus, fear predisposes freezing, at- in animals as their personality (e.g., Gosling & John,
tack, and/or inhibition, and positive affectivity predis- 1999), we find it helpful to consider the aspects of indi-
poses approach. The expression or inhibition of these viduality we share with other animals separately from
behavioral tendencies can feed back to influence the on- those involving the content of thought that are more dis-
going emotional reaction. By self-regulation, we refer to tinctly human. Animal models of temperament allow in-
processes such as effortful control and orienting that vestigations of affective and cognitive neuroscience
function to modulate reactivity. We believe (as does that are not possible in humans, and aid in the study of
Kagan, 1998) that other important dimensions of tem- the neural underpinnings of temperament. Strelau
perament are likely to be identified in the future. (1983) takes a similar position to ours, arguing that
Temperament describes tendencies or dispositions temperament results from biological evolution, and is
that are not continually expressed but require appropri- peculiar to both humans and animals, which cannot be
ate eliciting conditions. Fearful children, for example, said of personality (p. 258). In addition, The individ-
are not continually distressed and inhibited, but under ual has a temperament from the moment of birth, since
conditions of novelty, sudden change in stimulation, or it is determined by inborn physiological mechanisms
signals of punishment, they may be particularly prone to which, in turn, may be modified under environmental
a fearful reaction. Easily frustrated children are not influences (p. 258).
continually irritable or angry, but when their goals are We begin this chapter with a brief history of tempera-
blocked or there is a failure of their expectations, they ment research, considering its recent history, its ancient
will be particularly prone to frustration. roots, and its study in adulthood. In the second section,
we examine the structure of temperament as it has
Temperament and Personality emerged from research on child development and from
some of the major theoretical models of the neuro-
Temperament represents the affective, activational, and sciences. We also consider results of the search for a
attentional core of personality, whereas personality in- taxonomic structure of adult personality traits, and re-
History of Temperament: Research on Childhood 101

late temperament structure to the resulting Big Three sessments by clinicians blind to their identity. These
and Big Five factors of personality. matches were more successful than would have been ex-
In the third section, we discuss methods and mea- pected by chance.
sures for the study of temperament, considering both the Gesell (1928, as cited in Kessen, 1965) identified the
benefits and liabilities of some of the major empirical critical importance of the childs temperament in what
approaches. Because the use of parent-report in tem- he called the developmental web of life. His views of
perament research has been questioned (Kagan, 1994, alternative developmental pathways, so important to re-
1998; Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4, this Handbook, this vol- cent thinking, are illustrated in the case of C. D.:
ume), we critically consider contributions of parent-
This girl exhibited a striking degree of amenability,
report. In the fourth section, we focus on temperament
sociality, and good nature as early as the age of 9
and development, considering issues of continuity and
months. . . . She is now 5 years of age, and in spite of a
change. In the fifth section, we discuss relations be-
varied experience in boarding homes and institutions she
tween temperament and behavioral adjustment. The has not lost these engaging characteristics. They are part
final section presents our conclusions and indicates fu- and parcel of her make-up quite as much as the lowered
ture directions for the study of temperament and devel- tempo and the lowered trend of her general development.
opment. Overall, we organize research findings on It can be predicted with much certainty that she will re-
temperament in a developmental framework. The litera- tain her present emotional equipment when she is an ado-
ture review is not comprehensive, but we hope it captures lescent and an adult. But more than this cannot be
some of the major issues and approaches to the study of predicted in the field of personality. For whether she be-
temperament in childhood. We now begin with a histori- comes a delinquent, and she is potentially one, will de-
cal review of temperament in childhood and adulthood. pend upon her subsequent training, conditioning, and
supervision. She is potentially also a willing, helpful,
productive worker. Environment retains a critical role
even though heredity sets metes and bounds. (Gesell,
HISTORY OF TEMPERAMENT: RESEARCH
1928, pp. 372373)
ON CHILDHOOD
Three important concepts from Gesell and Shirley
Several lines of inquiry have contributed to contempo- are further elaborated in this chapter. First, tempera-
rary temperament research on children. One is the re- ment traits are constitutionally based characteristics
search of the normative child psychologists in the 1920s that provide the core of personality and influence direc-
and 1930s, who observed large numbers of children to tions for development. Second, although some stability
establish the normal sequences of motor and mental de- of temperament is expected across age, developmental
velopment and studied small samples of children inten- outcomes will also depend on the childs experience in
sively over time. In doing so, they noted striking the social context. Finally, as in the case of C. D., a
temperamental variability among the children they ob- given set of temperamental characteristics allows for
served (Gesell, 1928, as cited in Kessen, 1965; Shirley, multiple possible outcomes. Different trajectories and
1933). Shirleys intensive study of the motor develop- outcomes may occur for children with similar tempera-
ment of a group of infants during the first 2 years led to mental traits, and children differing in temperament
her observation of the infants core of personality. may come to similar developmental outcomes via differ-
Shirley (1933) noted that developmentally, Both con- ent pathways (Kochanska, 1995).
stancy and change characterize the personality of the
baby. Traits are constant enough to make it plausible that Clinical Research
a nucleus of personality exists at birth and that this nu-
cleus persists and grows and determines to a certain de- A second major line of research on temperament in
gree the relative importance of (other) traits (p. 56). childhood came from biologically oriented clinicians.
She devoted a full volume to these traits, even though Bergman and Escalona (1949) identified children who
her original intention had been to study only motor and were particularly reactive to low intensities of stimula-
intellectual development. Fifteen years later, Neilon tion in one or more sensory modalities. In Escalonas
(1948) located 15 of Shirleys 25 babies, asking judges (1968) groundbreaking book, The Roots of Individuality,
to match Shirleys infant personality sketches to de- she proposed the concept of effective experience, the
scriptions of the children as adolescents, based on as- idea that events in childrens lives are experienced only
102 Temperament

as they are filtered through the individual childs nerv- varied from child to child (Korner, 1964), early differ-
ous system. A given event will thus differ in its effects ences could be seen as providing the raw material for
for children who differ in temperament. An adults vig- later development. Ideas originally put forward by Robert
orous play, for example, may lead to pleasure in one Sears and associates (e.g., Sears, Maccoby, & Levin,
child and distress in another. Escalona noted that objec- 1957) were also reemerging regarding bidirectionality in
tive coding of environmental events alone will not cap- the effects of socialization, from child to caregiver and
ture essential information about the individual childs caregiver to child (Bell, 1968). Finally, cognitive ap-
reaction to them. Infants observed by Escalona (1968) proaches stressed childrens influences on their own de-
were followed in developmental studies of vulnerability, velopment via their perceptual and cognitive mental
resiliency and coping by Murphy and Moriarty (1976). representations of events (Kohlberg, 1969). Research on
In other studies, Fries and Woolf (1954) identified and temperament would now introduce the idea that, in addi-
studied congenital activity type, Korner (1964) studied tion to individual differences in thought patterns, indi-
neonatal individuality and developed an extensive as- vidual differences in childrens emotional processing
sessment schedule for the newborn, and Birns (1965) could bias their affective representations of experience,
and her associates developed and implemented some of with important implications for their development.
the earliest standardized assessments of temperament.
Among clinical investigators, Thomas, Chess, Birch,
HISTORY OF TEMPERAMENT IN
Hertzig, and Korn (1963) published the first of their vol-
ADULTHOOD
umes on the extremely influential New York Longitudi-
nal Study (NYLS). Inspired by differences among their
Adult studies of temperament have a much longer history
own children, Chess and Thomas studied individual dif-
than developmental work, much of it focused on biologi-
ferences in what they called the primary reaction pat-
cal aspects of personality. Temperament ideas go back to
terns, collecting interviews from parents of infants on
Greco-Roman physicians over 2,000 years ago, and to
repeated occasions. Beginning when their initial sample
even earlier traditions in China and India (Diamond,
of 22 infants was 3 to 6 months of age, parents were ex-
1974; Needham, 1973). In this thinking, psychological
tensively interviewed about their infants behavior in
characteristics were consistently linked to the physiol-
varying contexts. Each infant reaction and its context
ogy of the individual as it was understood at the time.
was then typed on a separate sheet of paper, and Birch
Thus, ancient Greco-Roman physicians identified the
inductively sorted the descriptions into categories that
well-known fourfold typology and linked it to the bodily
came to represent the nine NYLS temperament dimen-
humors: the sanguine individual, positive and outgoing
sions (Chess & Thomas, personal communication, Octo-
(with a predominance of blood); the melancholic person,
ber, 1992; Thomas et al., 1963): (1) Activity Level, (2)
prone to fear and sadness (with a predominance of black
Approach/ Withdrawal, (3) Adaptability, (4) Mood, (5)
bile); the choleric person, irritable and prone to aggres-
Threshold, (6) Intensity, (7) Distractibility, (8) Rhyth-
sion (with a predominance of yellow bile); and the phleg-
micity, and (9) Attention Span / Persistence. Later,
matic person, slow to excitation (with a predominance of
Michael Rutter suggested the term temperament to de-
phlegm; Diamond, 1974). The fourfold typology was
scribe their area of study, and this term was adopted by
used throughout the Middle Ages and in the writings of
the NYLS group (Chess & Thomas, personal communi-
Kant. By the time of Wundt (1903), however, a shift was
cation, October, 1992).
made away from positing temperamental types to
studying dimensions of variability in temperament, a
Acceptance of Temperament Research shift that has only recently been reversed in Kagans
(1994, 1998) and others (Caspi, Sugden, et al., 2003)
Reports from the NYLS arrived at an opportune time,
use of temperament typologies.
when researchers in social development were becoming
increasingly aware of the contributions of individual
Jungs Theory
children to their own development. One influence was the
burgeoning of infancy research, with researchers study- Psychological types were put forward by Jung (1923),
ing the initial state of the individual and its subsequent but they differed in important ways from current typolo-
adaptations (Osofsky, 1979). Because the initial state gies. In Jungs view, introverted and extraverted tenden-
History of Temperament in Adulthood 103

cies were universal and reflected in thinking, feeling, labeled emotional stability-instability; Eysenck (1967)
sensation, and intuition. Jung argued that introverted would later call it Neuroticism. By 1937, Burt had also
and extraverted tendencies are present in all persons, identified the factor of Extraversion-Introversion, and
but that, for a given person, one attitude becomes more later factor-analytic research on questionnaire assessed
elaborated and conscious, while the other is less elabo- personality has repeatedly identified three factors: Ex-
rated, more primitive, and, for the most part, uncon- traversion, Neuroticism, and Conscientiousness. These
scious. Differences among children in extraversion and three factors, sometimes called the Big Three, along
introversion, he wrote, can be seen early in life: with factors of Agreeableness and Openness to experi-
ence, have been extracted from factor analyses of trait
The earliest mark of extraversion in a child is his quick
descriptive words and personality items, and constitute
adaptation to the environment, and the extraordinary at-
tention he gives to objects, especially to his effect upon
what have been called the Big Five personality factors
them. Shyness in regard to objects is very slight; the child (Costa & McCrae, 1988; Goldberg, 1990).
moves and lives among them with trust. He makes quick Soviet and Eastern European research on tempera-
perceptions, but in a haphazard way. Apparently he devel- ment began with Pavlovs (1955/1935) observations
ops more quickly than an introverted child, since he is less of individual differences among dogs in conditioning
cautious, and as a rule, has no fear. Apparently, too, he experiments, and led to an active research tradition de-
feels no barrier between himself and objects, and hence he scribed by Strelau (1983). Pavlov linked temperamen-
can play with them freely and learn though them. He tal differences among the animals, which he also
gladly pushes his undertakings to an extreme, and risks generalized to humans, to qualities of the central nerv-
himself in the attempt. Everything unknown seems allur- ous system, including strength of neural activation.
ing. (Jung, 1928, as cited in Fordham, 1953, p. 303)
Strength of activation was related to the law of
strength in classical conditioning, whereby increasing
Objects as described by psychoanalysts include both
the intensity of a conditioned stimulus led to the in-
physical and social entities, so the more introverted
creased intensity of the animals response. For some
child would be expected to dislike new social situations
animals, however, increasing stimulus intensity led to
and to approach strangers with caution and fear. Jung
failure to respond. Pavlov described these animals as
suggests that the introvert would also be inclined toward
having a weak nervous system; animals with a strong
pessimism about the future, and the extravert would
nervous system maintained the law of strength even at
show more ready approach and action on objects (impul-
high levels of stimulus intensity. Additional Pavlovian
sivity), greater sociability, and more optimism about the
temperament constructs included strength of inhibi-
future (Jung, 1923).
tion, balance between activation and inhibition, and
Eastern and Western Schools of Temperament mobilityflexibility of nervous system adjustment to
and Personality changing conditions. Soviet researchers began their
work in the laboratory, but a lack of generality of their
In addition to Jungs theoretical model of introversion- measures across stimulus and response modalities, a
extraversion, similar dimensions emerged from early fac- phenomenon they called partiality, led to a general
tor-analytic studies of temperament in adults. In Great shift in their focus from the laboratory to the use of
Britain, Webb (1915) analyzed self-report items refer- questionnaire methods (Strelau, 1983).
ring to emotionality, activity, qualities of the self, and Although British and Soviet schools took different
intelligence and thus identified two broad factors. One, historical directions in the study of temperament, with
labeled w, was defined as consistency of action re- British researchers moving from questionnaires to the
sulting from deliberate volition or will (Webb, 1915, laboratory, and Eastern European researchers from the
p. 34). This factor bears similarities to temperamental laboratory to questionnaires, both schools remain ac-
Effortful Control in childhood (Kochanska, Murray, & tively involved in the study of temperament, and both
Harlan, 2000; Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, link individual differences in temperament to hypotheti-
2001), and to the higher-order personality factors more cal nervous system function. We now review studies of
recently labeled Control, Constraint, or Conscientious- the structure of temperament as it has emerged from re-
ness (Digman & Inouye, 1986). A second factor assessed search on infancy and childhood and describe neural
distress proneness or negative emotionality, sometimes models conceptually related to this structure.
104 Temperament

THE STRUCTURE OF TEMPERAMENT average r = .64; for day-care teachers, the intercorrela-
tions ranged from .51 to .73, with the average r = .63
In this section, we consider research on temperament (Goldsmith & Rieser-Danner, 1986).
that has led to revision of the original list of nine NYLS The second shared dimension was Irritable Distress,
dimensions (Tables 3.1 and 3.2). Much of this research assessed by IBQ Distress to Limitations, RITQ Negative
employed factor analysis of large sets of items within Mood (which includes positive affect at one pole), and
the temperament domain. Factor analysis allows re- ICQ Fussy/ Difficult scales. Intercorrelations among
searchers to see simultaneously the relations and nonre- these scales for mothers ranged from .44 to .63, with an
lations among large sets of behavior descriptions. A average of .54; for day-care teachers, the correlations
major limitation of the factor analytic method is that the ranged from .66 to .74, with an average of .71. The third
dimensions yielded by the analysis depend on the de- shared temperament dimension was Activity Level, as-
scriptors included in the initial data matrix. Several sessed only on the RITQ and IBQ scales, where the cor-
broad dimensions of temperament have consistently relation for both mothers and day-care teachers was .65.
emerged from different sets of data. Gartstein and Rothbart (2003) have recently studied
the factor structure of expanded scales measuring
parent-reported infant temperament, as adapted from
Infant Studies
dimensions identified in research on temperament in
In a review of the structure of temperament as indicated childhood (Table 3.2). Factor analysis of a large data set
by infant studies (Rothbart & Bates, 1998; Rothbart & describing 3- to 12-month-old children yielded three
Mauro, 1990), six dimensions were identified that pro- broad dimensions: Surgency/Extraversion, defined pri-
vided a shorter list of temperament variables for future marily from scales of Approach, Vocal Reactivity, High
researchers (Table 3.1). Individual differences in posi- Intensity Pleasure (stimulation seeking), Smiling and
tive emotionality were differentiated from negative Laughter, Activity Level and Perceptual Sensitivity;
emotionality, and two kinds of negative emotion were Negative Af fectivity, with loadings from Sadness, Frus-
identified: fear and anger/irritable distress. tration, Fear, and loading negatively, Falling Reactiv-
Infant scales with different names often measure ity; and Orienting/Regulation, with loadings from Low
similar constructs. Goldsmith and Rieser-Danner Intensity Pleasure, Cuddliness, Duration of Orienting,
(1986) had both mothers and day-care teachers of 4- to and Soothability and a secondary loading for Smiling
8-month-old infants fill out the Revised Infant Tempera- and Laughter. As early as infancy, there is thus evi-
ment Questionnaire (RITQ; W. Carey & McDevitt, dence for three broad temperament dimensions.
1978), the Infant Characteristics Questionnaire (ICQ; A number of important conclusions have emerged
Bates, Freeland, & Lounsbury, 1979), and the Infant Be- from factor analytic studies on the structure of infant
havior Questionnaire (IBQ; Rothbart, 1981). Distress to temperament as reviewed by Rothbart and Mauro (1990)
novelty was assessed by all three of these instruments: and Rothbart and Bates (1998). First, the structure ap-
IBQ Fear, ICQ Unadaptable, and RITQ Approach-With- pears to correspond more to dimensions of reactivity in
drawal scales. Intercorrelations across these scales were the basic emotions and attention /regulation than to a
high. For mothers, they ranged from .60 to .69, with the general style. Second, bipolar constructs such as ap-

TABLE 3.1 Dimensions of Temperament in Infancy


Broad Factors Narrow Dimensions
Negative emotionality Fear Sadness
Frustration /irritability Falling reactivity
Surgency/extraversion Approach Smiling and laughter
Vocal reactivity Activity level
High intensity pleasure Perceptual sensitivity
Orienting/regulation Low intensity pleasure Cuddliness
Duration of orienting Soothability
Rhythmicity
The Structure of Temperament 105

TABLE 3.2 Dimensions of Temperament in Childhood


Broad Factors Narrow Dimensions
Negative emotionality Fear Resistance to control
Shyness Sadness
Frustration /irritability Soothability
Discomfort
Surgency/extraversion Activity level Positive anticipation
High intensity pleasure Sociability
Effortful control /self-regulation Inhibitory control Low intensity pleasure
Attentional focusing Perceptual sensitivity
Persistence
Agreeableness/adaptability Manageability Affiliation

proach versus withdrawal and good versus bad mood tional Focusing, Low Intensity Pleasure, and Perceptual
have not emerged from these analyses; instead, unipolar Sensitivity. These three factors map well on the second-
constructs of infant temperament have gained support. order factors identified by Sanson et al. (1994): Sur-
Third, these dimensions also correspond to individual gency/ Extraversion on Sociability; Negative Affectivity
differences emerging from studies of nonhuman animals on Negative Emotionality, and Effortful Control on
(Gosling & John, 1999), allowing links between tem- Self-Regulation.
perament constructs in humans and the psychobiology of The first three factors emerging from a recent factor
individual differences. analysis of NYLS-inspired Middle Childhood Tempera-
ment Questionnaire items (Hegvik, McDevitt, & Carey,
1982) for 8- to 12-year-olds (McClowry, Hegvik, & Te-
Childhood Studies
glasi, 1993) also show similarity to these factors: Ap-
Factor analyses of questionnaire items based on the proach / Withdrawal, Negative Reactivity, and Task
NYLS for older children have similarly revealed a Persistence. McClowry et al.s (1993) two smaller fac-
shorter list of broad temperament factors (Table 3.2). tors, Activity and Responsiveness, also parallel smaller
Analysis of mother reports for 3- to 8-year-olds on the factors in the ATP (Sanson et al., 1994).
Thomas and Chess (1977) Childhood Temperament Presley and Martins (1994) analysis of teacher re-
Questionnaire in the Australian Temperament Project ports of 3- to 7-year-olds on the Temperament Assess-
(ATP) yielded factors of Inf lexibility (irritability and ment Battery for Children yielded five factors
uncooperativeness), Persistence, Sociability, and Rhyth- demonstrating some overlap with those described above.
micity (Sanson, Smart, Prior, Oberklaid, & Pedlow, These include Social Inhibition, Negative Emotionality,
1994); second-order factors extracted from the ATP Agreeableness/Adaptability, Activity Level, and Task Per-
data were labeled Negative Emotionality, Self- sistence. In their review of factor analytic studies on in-
Regulation, and Sociability. fant and child temperament, Martin, Wisenbaker, and
The Childrens Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ; Roth- Huttunen (1994) note the robustness of the general tem-
bart et al., 2001) has also consistently yielded three perament factors of Negative Emotionality, Task per-
broad factors, found in U.S. replications and in research sistence, Adaptability, and Social Inhibition. They see
performed in the Peoples Republic of China and Japan Activity Level as more problematic because it is related
(Ahadi, Rothbart, & Ye, 1993; Kochanska, DeVet, Gold- to both negative and positive affect early in life.
man, Murray, & Putnam, 1993; Rothbart et al., 2001). The factors emerging from research on temperament
The first, called Surgency/Extraversion, is defined pri- in childhood show strong conceptual similarities with
marily by the scales of Approach, High Intensity Plea- the Big Three factors and three of the Big Five or FFM
sure (sensation-seeking), Activity Level, and a negative factors that have been extracted from analyses of self-
contribution from Shyness. The second, called Negative and peer descriptions of personality in adults (Gold-
Af fectivity, is defined by the scales of Discomfort, Fear, berg, 1993) and children (Ahadi & Rothbart, 1994;
Anger/ Frustration, Sadness, and loading negatively, Caspi, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume; Digman
Soothability. The third factor, labeled Ef fortful Control, & Inouye, 1986). The Negative Af fectivity factor from
is defined by the scales of Inhibitory Control, Atten- childhood measures is conceptually similar to the broad
106 Temperament

adult dimension of Neuroticism or Negative Emotionality. sistence/attention, and activity level. Shiner indicates
The Surgency and Sociability factors are similar to the the importance of placing these dimensions in a devel-
broad adult dimension of Extraversion or Positive Emo- opmental context. We can, for example, study irritabil-
tionality. The Persistence, Self-Regulation, or Ef fortful ity in infants, but aggression cannot be observed until
Control factors map upon the adult dimension of Con- later in the development. The hierarchical structure of
trol/Constraint (see Ahadi & Rothbart, 1994), and Mar- Shiners (1998) additional dimensions will also be of
tin et al.s (1994) Agreeableness/Adaptability factor onto interest. Does dominance, for example, relate more to
the adult dimension of Agreeableness. extraversion /surgency, or to agreeableness/affiliation?
In our research on adults, we have found strong one- Does anger proneness relate more to negative emotion-
to-one correlations between factor scores derived from ality or agreeableness/affiliation (Shiner, 1998)? In
factor analysis of temperament scales and Big Five Rothbart and Victors (2004) research, it loads on the
measures (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000), between Unsocialized Stimulation Seeking factor.
Negative Emotionality and Neuroticism, Positive Emo-
tionality and Extraversion, and Effortful Control and Summary
Conscientiousness. In addition, Perceptual Sensitivity As noted earlier, work to date on temperament structure
was related to Openness, and temperamental Affiliation in infancy and in childhood suggests revisions of the
to Agreeableness. Neuroticism, however, often contains original NYLS nine dimensions to include (with broad,
negative judgments about the self that may be strongly aggregated constructs in parentheses): Positive Affect
related to an individuals experiences with others; these and Activity Level (Surgency/ Extraversion), Fearful
may or may not have a strong temperamental base, and Distress, Irritable Distress (General Negative Emotion-
research on temperament and personality in childhood ality), Effortful Control / Task Persistence, and Agree-
becomes very important. ableness/Adaptability. In our next section, we make
In research with the Combined Temperament and Per- tentative links between these constructs and models de-
sonality Scales (CTPQ) describing 565 children between veloped in the neurosciences.
the ages of 3 and 12, we have extracted factors that
included Positive Emotion, Gregariousness, Warmth,
and Soothability (Sociable Extraversion); Anxiety, Self-
consciousness, Dependency, and Depression /Sadness (In- NEURAL MODELS OF TEMPERAMENT
ternalizing Negative Emotionality); Inhibitory Control,
Order, Diligence, Self-discipline, Attentional focusing, We now describe neural models developed to enhance
and low Distractibility (Conscientiousness); Aesthet- our understanding of temperament. Cloninger (1986),
ics/Creative Ideas, Intellect, and Perceptual Sensitivity Gray (1991), LeDoux (1989), Panksepp (1998), and
(Openness); and Excitement seeking, Assertiveness, Self- Zuckerman (1984) have all made contributions to the
centered, Noncompliance/Aggression, Manipulative, Ac- development of neural models for temperament (see re-
tivity, and Anger/Hostility and Impulsivity (Unsocialized view by Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). We
Stimulation Seeking; Rothbart & Victor, 2004). This re- begin by describing models for positive emotionality
search differentiates fearful and angry negative affect, and approach (Surgency/ Extraversion), and fear. Irri-
and links aspects of extraversion with both unsocialized tability/anger is also discussed, and it is seen, along
stimulation seeking and sociable extraversion. Longitudi- with fear, discomfort, and sadness, to represent part of a
nal research will be important in identifying the experi- general construct of susceptibility to negative affect or
ential and self-regulatory temperament influences on negative emotionality. The emotion-based dimensions
these two outcomes. are related to differences observed in nonhuman ani-
Shiner (1998) has also recently contributed an im- mals (Gosling & John, 1999; Panksepp, 1998), and to
portant review of the structure of personality in middle factor structures extracted from studies of personality
childhood. Her preliminary taxonomy includes these in adults and temperament in childhood. The fourth di-
dimensions: sociability, social inhibition, prosocial mension, Effortful Control, will be further discussed in
disposition, dominance, aggressiveness, negative emo- connection with neural models for individual differ-
tionality, mastery motivation, inhibitory control, per- ences in executive attention (Posner & Fan, in press).
Neural Models of Temperament 107

Emotion as an Information-Processing System the self, access to information about others is likely
to be less accessible. These are important examples of
Emotions can be seen as broadly integrative systems that
information-processing aspects of temperament that
order feeling, thought, and action (LeDoux, 1989). They
have major implications for social development, and we
also represent the output of information processing net-
discuss them again in the section on temperament and
works assessing the meaning or affective significance of
the development of personality.
events for the individual (LeDoux, 1989). Whereas ob-
ject recognition systems and spatial processing systems
address the questions, What is it? and Where is it? Positive Emotionality/Approach and
neural emotion processing networks address the ques- Extraversion
tions, Is it good for me? Is it bad for me? and What
We now briefly review neural models developed to de-
shall I do about it? When there are individual differ-
scribe a physical substrate for approach and fearful inhi-
ences in temperamental emotionality, there are thus dif-
bition. Based on animal research, Gray (1991) described
ferences in object perception as well.
the behavioral activation system (BAS), involving sensi-
In neural processing of emotion, thalamic connec-
tivity to rewards, and the behavioral inhibition system
tions route information about object qualities of a
(BIS), involving sensitivity to punishment, nonreward,
stimulus through sensory pathways (LeDoux, 1989). Si-
novelty, and innate fear stimuli (see applications to chil-
multaneously, information is routed to the limbic sys-
dren by Blair, 2003). These two systems are seen as mu-
tem and the amygdala, where memories of the affective
tually inhibitory, with their balance determining
meaning of the stimulus further influence the process.
degrees of extraversion-introversion. Gray also posited a
Later object processing can update the emotional analy-
fight-flight system that moderates unconditioned pun-
sis, but in the meantime, back projections from the
ishment (Gray, 1991). According to Grays BAS model,
amygdala can influence subsequent sensory processing.
reward-related projections from the amygdala to the nu-
Output of the amygdala to organized autonomic reac-
cleus accumbens activate a motor program that in-
tions via the hypothalamus and to motor activation via
creases proximity to the desired stimulus and facilitates
the corpus striatum reflects the motivational aspect of
goal-oriented behavior (Gray & McNaughton, 1996).
the emotions (LeDoux, 1989).
In a broader behavioral facilitation system (BFS),
Depue and Collins (1999) proposed a circuit involving
Attention as a Control System the nucleus accumbens, ventral pallidum, and dopamin-
ergic neurons that codes the intensity of the rewarding
Neuroimaging studies demonstrate connections between stimuli, with related circuits involving the medial or-
emotional processing networks and the executive atten- bital cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus integrating the
tion system that allow the selection of emotional infor- salient incentive context. Individual differences in the
mation for conscious processing so that we may or may functioning of this network are thought to arise from
not be aware of our emotional evaluations (Bush, Luu, & variation in the dopaminergic projections that encode
Posner, 2000; Posner & Rothbart, 1991). Attentional the intensity of incentive motivation. With development,
systems can select for conscious processing aspects of dopaminergic facilitation can enhance responsivity to
emotional analyses, and emotion can also influence the positive incentive stimuli (Depue & Collins, 1999) and
focusing and shifting of attention (Derryberry & Reed, provide a neural basis for a positive feedback system.
1996, 2002; Gray, 1991). An important aspect of social This system can lead initially approaching children to
adaptation involves the appropriateness of a childs so- become even more approaching, and temperamental dif-
cial interaction and the related acceptance of the child ferences in extraversion to become greater with time.
by others (Parker & Asher, 1987). Information about the Depue and Iacono (1989) used the BFS to account for
state of others will thus be an important contributor to initiation of locomotor activity, incentive-reward moti-
appropriate social action, and failure of this information vation, exploration of environmental novelty (if stronger
to access action and consciousness can be a critical ele- than opposing fear reactions), and irritable aggression.
ment in the development of disordered functioning. Panksepp (1982, 1986b, 1998) concluded that the gen-
When attention is focused on threatening stimuli or on eral function of DA [dopamine] activity in appetitive
108 Temperament

behavior is to promote the expression of motivational pus. However, the amygdala has been more often iden-
excitement and anticipatory eagernessthe heightened tified as the critical structure in the processing of con-
energization of animals searching for and expecting re- ditioned fear (LeDoux, 1989). Emotional networks
wards (1986a, p. 91). Cloninger (1986, 1987) also involving the amygdala also appear to respond more
specified a novelty-seeking dimension related to DA strongly to novel than to familiar stimuli (Nishijo, Ono,
functioning, as did Zuckerman (1984) in his dimension & Nishino, 1988). Amygdala lesions in rodents disrupt
of sensation seeking (see review by Rothbart, 1989a). autonomic and cortisol reactions, behavioral freezing,
A broad approach dimension has also been linked to and fear vocalizations; similar findings have been re-
positive affect. Tellegens (1985) research on personal- ported in primates (Lawrence & Calder, 2004). In hu-
ity yielded a broad factor of Positive Emotionality, in- mans, functional neuroimaging studies by Calder,
cluding pleasure and positive anticipation, and Watson Lawrence, and Young (2001) and others support in-
and Clark (1997) have argued that positive affect is the volvement of the amygdala in both acquiring and ex-
core of individual differences in Extraversion. The chil- pressing fear, although not in the voluntary production
drens temperament factor of Surgency/ Extraversion of facial expressions of fear (Anderson & Phelps,
(Rothbart et al., 2001) fits well with these models. In re- 2002). The amygdala also is involved in the recognition
search on infants (Rothbart, 1988; Rothbart, Derry- of fear in the human face (Calder et al., 2001), and
berry, & Hershey, 2000), expressions of smiling and there is evidence in humans that amygdala damage is
laughter were related to their rapid latency to approach related to reduced fear experience (Adolphs et al.,
objects, and predicted their anticipatory eagerness about 1999; Sprengelmeyer et al., 1999).
upcoming positive events at the age of 7 years. Projections from the amygdala implement autonomic
In recent functional Magnetic Resonance Imagery and behavioral components of fear, including startle,
(fMRI ) research with adults, Canli et al. (2001) found motor inhibition, facial expression, and cardiovascular
that persons higher in extraversion showed greater and respiratory changes (Davis, Hitchcock, & Rosen,
brain response to positive than negative stimuli in 1987). Individual variability in the structure and func-
widespread frontal, temporal, and limbic activation of tioning of any of these subsystems may be related to
both hemispheres. Those higher in neuroticism reacted variations in behavioral expressions of fear, and multi-
more to negative than to positive stimuli, showing more ple components of other affective motivational systems,
circumscribed activation (fronto-temporal on the left such as approach/positive affect and anger/irritable dis-
side) and deactivation in a right frontal area. In a fol- tress, would also be expected.
low-up study focusing on the amygdala, extraversion The amygdala also appears to affect information pro-
was correlated with left amygdala activation to happy, cessing in the cortex. For example, the basolateral nu-
but not to fearful, faces (Canli, Sivers, Whitfield, cleus of the amygdala projects to frontal and cingulate
Gotlib, & Gabrieli, 2002). Neuroticism was not corre- regions involved in the executive attention system (Pos-
lated with activation to any of the emotional faces, ner & Petersen, 1990), as well as to ventral occipital and
except for significant amygdala activation for the fear- temporal pathways involved in processing object infor-
ful expression. mation. These connections are consistent with findings
that anxious individuals show enhanced attention to
Fear and Behavioral Inhibition threatening sources of information (e.g., Derryberry &
Reed, 1994). A more detailed analysis of structures re-
Fear is an emotional response activated in the presence lated to behavioral inhibition can be found in Kagan and
of threat or signals of upcoming danger, and its func- Foxs chapter, Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume.
tion appears to be a defensive one. Fear activation is A psychobiological analysis of fear suggests there
accompanied by inhibition of ongoing motor programs may be less disagreement about temperament variables
and preparation of response systems controlling coping than had been previously thought. Fear as we have de-
options such as fleeing, fighting, or hiding (see review scribed it includes arousal, felt emotion, motor response
by Rothbart, Derryberry et al., 1994). In Grays (1991) preparation for flight and/or attack (with responses
behavioral inhibition system model (BIS), the fear- often inhibited), and attention toward the fear-inducing
related BIS is based on circuits including the orbital stimulus and/or possible escape routes (Davis et al.,
frontal cortex, medial septal area, and the hippocam- 1987). When temperament researchers study aspects of
Neural Models of Temperament 109

this construct, they sometimes stress (a) the motiva- mastery motivation (Rothbart & Hwang, 2005), and af-
tional aspects of the individuals response (e.g., Thomas fective representations of the environment (Derryberry
& Chesss, 1977, Approach/ Withdrawal; Kagans, 1994, & Reed, 1994) are all likely to be influenced by tem-
behavioral inhibition), ( b) the distress proneness aspects peramental approach and inhibition tendencies in inter-
(Buss & Plomins, 1975, Emotionality; Goldsmith & action with past experience. The coping strategies
Campos, 1982, Fear), (c) its duration and susceptibility children use will also be influenced by their tendencies
to interventions (Rothbarts, 1981, Soothability), (d) its to approach or inhibit action. Quay (1993) has em-
relation to arousal (Strelaus, 1983, reactivity), or (e) ployed Grays constructs of the BAS and BIS to analyze
multiple components of response (Rothbart & Derry- the development of undersocialized aggressive conduct
berrys, 1981, Fear). If we take the broader view of fear disorder. In the next section, we describe temperament
suggested by neuroscience work, agreement is more evi- systems related to distress, overstimulation, irritability
dent, and intercorrelations among scales measuring the and anger, and possible controls offered by affiliative
differently named constructs as discussed earlier fur- tendencies.
ther support this contention (Goldsmith, Rieser-Danner,
& Briggs, 1991). Optimal Levels of Stimulation and Distress to
We have now touched on possible neural substrates Overstimulation
for approach/positive affect systems related to reward
seeking, and for fear, linked to the inhibition of ap- We have now described individual differences in sys-
proach and of sensation seeking that might lead to pun- tems of fear and approach/extraversion. However,
ishment ( harm avoidance). An important aspect of processes related to arousability have also been pro-
these constructs for students of social development is posed to underlie approach and withdrawal. One model
that they describe individual differences in susceptibil- incorporates the idea of optimal level of stimulation.
ity to reward and punishment, suggesting that some chil- This approach derives temperament from general
dren will be more activated by reward and some children arousability or strength of nervous system, put for-
will find stopping an activity easier when there is a high ward in the Soviet and Eastern European schools
likelihood of punishment (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Hershey, Eysencks (1967) modeland ideas developed by Bell
1994). When a situation involves both potential rewards (1974). In the theory of Berlyne (1971), arousal poten-
and punishments, such as interactions with a stranger, tial (created by stimulus intensity, novelty, and sur-
the balance between approach and fear tendencies based prise) activates two motivational systems: one related
on temperament and previous experience will be critical to pleasure and approach (elicited at lower levels of
to behavioral outcomes. arousal potential), the other to distress and withdrawal
This model has direct applications to child social- (elicited at higher levels of arousal potential). The two
ization. If we consider a toddler performing an enjoy- systems oppose each other. Individual differences in
able act, such as shredding the pages of a book, the the strength of each of these two systems support a level
childs initial activities will be influenced by the ap- of optimal arousalthe point where approach and
proach or extraversion system. Now the parent gives a pleasure are at their highest, but withdrawal processes
sharp, punishing command for the child to stop. Will do not yet dominate.
the childs activity be inhibited? Patterson (1980) Schneirla (1959) put forward similar ideas, describ-
found that parents of nonproblem children were effec- ing Approach and Withdrawal systems across species
tive in stopping their childrens aversive behavior on related to the intensity of stimuli. Eysenck (1967) ar-
three out of four occasions when they punished. When gued that introverts are more arousable and sensitive to
parents of problem children used punishment, how- stimulation at low intensity levels than extraverts, link-
ever, children were likely to actually continue the pun- ing this arousal to the Ascending Reticular Activation
ished behavior (Patterson, 1980; Snyder, 1977). System. Introverts were seen to experience both pleas-
Although parenting skills are also involved, childrens ure and discomfort at lower levels of stimulus intensity;
temperament is likely to make a basic contribution to this lower optimal level of arousal would lead them to
this situation. seek lower stimulus levels. In Strelaus (1983) model,
Individual differences in risky behaviors and more strongly reactive individuals engage in self-
accident-proneness (Matheny, 1991; Schwebel, 2004), regulatory activities to maintain their optimal levels of
110 Temperament

stimulation. Soviet researchers concept of nervous sys- and Panksepp (1982) describes similar neural circuitry
tem strength of activation or endurance under high- in connection with a rage system (see review by Roth-
intensity stimulation, and Strelaus reactivity construct bart, Derryberry, et al., 1994).
are dimensions involving both sensitivity and suscepti- Important distinctions have more recently been
bility to distress that might be present in early life. More made, however, among varieties of aggression and
recently, Aron and Aron (1997) have also linked sen- anger, and their underlying neural systems. Aggression
sory sensitivity and reactivity to distress. as a self-defense reaction seems to be based on the func-
Developmental research on temperament shows tioning of the same amygdala circuits as involved in the
mixed support for a positive relation between sensitivity production of fear (Blanchard & Takahashi, 1988). Ag-
and susceptibility to distress. Miller and Bates (1986) gression linked to protection of resources, competition,
found significant positive correlations between temper- and offensive aggression, however, involves a different
ature sensitivity and susceptibility to the negative emo- system based on the monoamine dopamine (DA;
tions. In Keoghs (Keogh, Pullis, & Caldwell, 1982) Lawrence & Calder, 2004). The DA system has been
Teacher Temperament Questionnaire, the third factor, linked to both the production of offensive aggression
labeled Reactivity, included both negative affect and (Smeets & Gonzlez, 2000), and to the recognition of
sensitivity items. In research with the CBQ, positive anger in the human face (Lawrence, Calder, McGowan,
correlations between perceptual sensitivity and discom- & Grasby, 2002). In Lawrence et al.s study, DA block-
fort are regularly obtained (Goldsmith, Buss, & Lemery, ade impaired the recognition of anger, while sparing
1997; King & Wachs, 1995; Rothbart, Posner, & Her- recognition of other emotions and of facial identity.
shey, 1995), but in the CTPQ, perceptual sensitivity was We have noted earlier that Depue and Iacono (1989)
moderately related to openness and social extraversion suggest the dopaminergic system may facilitate irrita-
(including positive affect), and slightly negatively re- ble aggression aimed at removing a frustrating obstacle,
lated to the internalizing negative affects. Andersson, consistent with findings that DA agonists (e.g., amphet-
Bohlin, and Hagekull (1999) found that parent reported amine) can enhance aggressive behaviors. Their view
reactivity to sensory stimulation at 10 months was re- suggests links between approach and frustration /anger,
lated to greater stranger wariness at 10 months and so- and childrens activity level and anger have been con-
cial inhibition at 25 months. Martin et al. (1994) have sistently positively related in parent-reported tempera-
also reviewed three factor analytic studies finding sensi- ment (Rothbart & Derryberry, 2002). In addition,
tivity items to load with items assessing negative emo- infant activity level predicts not only positive emotion-
tionality. Laboratory research assessing childrens ality at age 7, but also higher anger/frustration and low
sensory thresholds along with their thresholds for pleas- soothability-falling reactivity (Rothbart, Derryberry,
ure and discomfort would be helpful in further testing & Hershey, 2000). Together with findings relating 7-
this theoretical relationship. year surgency to aggression (Rothbart et al., 2000), this
suggests that strong approach tendencies may be linked
to negative and positive emotions (Derryberry & Reed,
Anger/ Irritability 1994; Rothbart et al., 2000). Children who showed a
short latency to grasp objects at 6.5, 10, and 13.5
In Grays (1991) model, the fight-flight system is con- months showed high levels of positive anticipation and
stituted by circuits connecting the amygdala, ventrome- impulsivity at age 7, as well as high anger-frustration
dial nucleus of the hypothalamus, central gray region of and aggression, again suggesting that strong approach
the midbrain, and somatic and motor effector nuclei of tendencies can contribute to both later anger-related
the lower brain stem processing information involving negative emotions and to positive emotions.
unconditioned punishment and nonreward. When there
is detection of painful or frustrating input, the brain Negative Emotionality or General Distress
stem effectors produce aggressive or defensive behav- Proneness
ior. Individual differences in reactivity of this fight-
flight system are also thought to underlie aggressive Negative Emotionality or distress proneness is often
aspects of Eysencks general Psychoticism dimension, viewed as a general dimension subsuming emotions of
Neural Models of Temperament 111

fear, anticipatory anxiety, sadness, frustration /anger, iors may depend in part on opiate projections from
guilt, and discomfort. For example, the Five-Factor higher limbic regions (e.g., amygdala, cingulate cortex)
model of adult personality includes negative emotions to the ventromedial hypothalamus, with brain opiates
as components of the Neuroticism superfactor. Neu- promoting social comfort and bonding, and opiate with-
roticism / Negative Emotionality has been found to be drawal promoting irritability and aggressiveness. Be-
orthogonal to Extraversion / Positive Emotionality cause ventromedial hypothalamic lesions dramatically
(Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985; Tellegen, 1985; Watson & increase aggression, Panksepp (1986a) also suggests
Clark, 1992). As evident in our previous discussion, that this brain region normally inhibits aggressive be-
however, the positive relationship between anger/frus- haviors controlled by the midbrains central gray area.
tration and strong approach tendencies suggests that a Hypothalamic projections can allow for friendly, trust-
more differentiated model is needed. ing, and helpful behaviors between members of a
As noted earlier, separable neural systems have been species by suppressing aggressive tendencies. Mecha-
found to be related to different forms of negative affect. nisms underlying prosocial and aggressive behaviors
There are nevertheless several possibilities for identify- would in this way be reciprocally related, in keeping
ing higher order negative emotion reactions. One is the with the bipolar Agreeableness-Hostility dimension
link between systems supporting fear and defensive ag- found in Five Factor Models of personality. Panksepp
gression (Blanchard & Takahashi, 1988). Defensive ag- (1993) has also reviewed research suggesting links be-
gression in animal models seems to be based on the tween social bonding and the hypothalamic neuropep-
same amygdalar circuits as fear, and in humans, anger in tide oxytocin (OXY), involved in maternal behavior,
response to threat may also be linked to fear. feelings of social acceptance and social bonding, and re-
Negative affect systems are also regulated by more duction of separation distress. OXY is also released dur-
general neurochemical systems including dopaminer- ing sexual activity by both females and males.
gic and serotonergic projections arising from the mid- Agreeableness, including at the high end, the proso-
brain, and by circulating gonadal and corticosteroid cial emotions and behaviors and affiliative tendencies,
hormones (Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 1994; Zucker- and at the low end, aggression and manipulativeness,
man, 1995). Neurochemical influences may thus also has been increasingly studied in childhood (Graziano,
provide coherence of emotional states in an individual, 1994; Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997; Graziano & Tobin,
and support more general factors of temperament such 2002). Like other originally bipolar dimensions, proso-
as negative emotionality. For example, serotonergic cial and antagonistic dispositions have also been stud-
projections from the midbrain raphe nuclei appear to ied separately (Bohart & Stipek, 2001), and Graziano
moderate limbic circuits related to anxiety and aggres- and Eisenberg (1997) suggest that the two dispositions
sion (Spoont, 1992). Low serotonergic activity may may be separable, even though negatively related. On a
thus increase an individuals vulnerability to both fear related issue, Shiner and Caspi (2003) point out that
and frustration, contributing to a general factor of neg- antisocial and prosocial behavior have different etiolo-
ative affectivity, including depression. Gonadal hor- gies (Krueger, Hicks, & McGue, 2001). Any tempera-
mones are related to both positive affect and mental predisposition to prosocial behavior needs to be
aggressiveness (Zuckerman, 1991), possibly influenc- seen as an open system, interacting with social experi-
ing individual differences in positive and angry states. ence for its outcomes. In research described earlier
Neural structures can thus support variability at broad linking temperament to personality in early and middle
and specific levels, although more research in the area childhood, two forms of extraversion /surgency have
is definitely needed. been identified; one linked to prosocial behavior and
the other to antisocial behavior and aggression (Victor,
Affiliativeness/Agreeableness Rothbart, & Baker, 2006), again suggesting the impor-
tance of socialization to pro- or antisocial behavior,
We share with other animals, including mammals, birds, and reminiscent of Gesells (1928, as cited in Kessen,
and fish, systems of affiliation that support pair bonds 1965) comment earlier in our chapter that C.D. could
and the care of the young (Insel, 2003). Panksepp become either a delinquent or a willing and responsible
(1986c) indicates that affiliative and prosocial behav- worker, depending on her training.
112 Temperament

Attentional Networks the limbic system, and is thus closely tied to emotion. It
also has close connections to adjacent motor systems.
Functional neuroimaging has allowed many cognitive Activity of the anterior cingulate is modified by
tasks to be analyzed by the brain areas they activate, dopamine input from the underlying basal ganglia. The
and studies of attention have been among the most often anterior cingulate structure consists of alternating
examined (Corbetta, Kincade, & Shulman, 2002; Dri- bands of cells with close connections to the dorso-
ver, Eimer, & Macaluso, in press; Posner & Fan, in lateral frontal cortex and to the posterior parietal lobe
press). Imaging data support the presence of three net- (Goldman-Rakic, 1988), suggesting a highly integrative
works related to different aspects of attention, carrying role. The anterior cingulate thus appears to provide an
out the functions of alerting, orienting, and executive important connection between widely different aspects
attention (Posner & Fan, in press). We discuss orienting of attention (e.g., attention to semantic or emotional
and executive attention in this section, although alert- content, or visual location).
ing is also likely to prove of interest to future tempera- Persistence, a dimension of personality conceptually
ment studies. related to effortful control in temperament, has been re-
Orienting involves aligning attention to a source of lated to brain activation (Gusnard et al., 2003). The ef-
signals. It may be overt (as in eye movements) or covert fects of persistence act strongly on midline and lateral
(occurring without any movement; Posner, 1980). Ori- prefrontal areas that are quite different than those found
enting can be manipulated by presenting a cue indicat- active for positive and negative affect, suggesting regu-
ing where in space an event will occur, thereby latory aspects of persistence. An increasingly accepted
directing attention to the cued location (Posner, 1980). view (Posner & Rothbart, 2000) is that effortful control,
Orienting to visual events has been associated with pos- represented in midline frontal areas, acts to regulate
terior brain areas, including the superior parietal lobe brain areas like the amygdala that are more clearly re-
and temporal parietal junction and the frontal eye fields lated to reactive aspects of negative affect.
(Corbetta et al., 2002). Lesions of the parietal lobe and In additional research, children who showed rapid ap-
superior temporal lobe have been consistently related to proach as infants tended to be low in attentional control
difficulties in orienting (Karnath, Ferber, & Himmel- and inhibitory control at age 7, consistent with findings
bach, 2001). of a negative relation between Surgency/ Extraversion
Orienting early in life is a reactive aspect of atten- and Effortful Control (Rothbart, Derryberry, et al.,
tion, and children differ both in their latency to orient 1994), and suggesting that strong approach tendencies
and their duration of orienting (see review by Ruff & may constrain the development of voluntary self-control.
Rothbart, 1996). In the IBQ, individual differences in If approach tendencies are viewed as the accelerator
duration of orienting in infancy are positively related to toward action, and inhibitory tendencies as the
smiling and laughter and vocal activity, suggesting that brakes, stronger accelerative tendencies may weaken
orienting may be part of an early positive reactivity or the braking influence of inhibitory control (Rothbart &
interest system (Rothbart, 1988; Rothbart, Derryberry, Derryberry, 2002).
et al., 2000). Executive control of attention is often studied by
The second major control system over reactive ap- tasks that involve conflict, such as varieties of
proach and action (the first is fearful inhibition as dis- the Stroop task, where subjects are asked to respond
cussed earlier) is that of effortful control, supported by with the color of ink (e.g., red) while ignoring the color
development of the executive attention network (Posner word name (e.g., blue; Bush et al., 2000). Resolving con-
& Rothbart, 2000). Executive attention and effortful flict in the Stroop task activates midline frontal areas
control are related to volition and awareness of input (anterior cingulate) and lateral prefrontal cortex
(Posner & Rothbart, 1991). There are limits on how (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Fan,
much we can simultaneously attend to in directed Flombaum, McCandliss, Thomas, & Posner, 2003).
thought or action. Areas of the midfrontal lobe, includ- There is also evidence for activation of this network in
ing the anterior cingulate gyrus, appear to underlie a tasks involving conflict between a central target and sur-
general executive attentional network (Vogt, Finch, & rounding flankers that may be congruent or incongruent
Olson, 1992), in combination with lateral prefrontal with the target (Botvinick et al., 2001; Fan, McCandliss,
areas. The anterior cingulate represents the outflow of Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002).
Measurement Approaches 113

Regulatory Functions of Executive Attention children are unable to properly perform this task, but by
30 months, most children can perform it but are slowed
The anterior cingulate gyrus, one of the main nodes of by the conflict, as are adults (Gerardi-Caulton, 2000;
the executive attention network has been linked to a va- Rothbart, Ellis, Rueda, & Posner, 2003). Children per-
riety of specific functions (Posner & Fan, in press), in- forming more efficiently on spatial conflict were rated
cluding working memory (Duncan & Owen, 2000), by their parents as having relatively higher levels of ef-
emotion (Bush et al., 2000), pain (Rainville, Duncan, fortful control and lower levels of negative affectivity.
Price, Carrier, & Bushnell, 1997), monitoring for con-
Summary
flict (Botvinick et al., 2001), and monitoring for error
(Holroyd & Coles, 2002). In emotion studies, the cingu- Models from neuroscience have been developed related
late is often seen as part of a network involving orbital to general dimensions of Approach, Fear/ Inhibition or
frontal and limbic (amygdala) structures. The frontal Harm Avoidance, Irritability (Fight-Flight or Rage), and
areas seem to have the ability to interact with the limbic Affiliativeness or Social Reward Dependence, Orient-
system (Davidson, Putnam, & Larson, 2000), fitting ing, and executive attention as a basis of Effortful Con-
well with the idea of their supporting self-regulation. trol. Optimal level models proposing a link between
A self-regulatory role for the cingulate has been iden- sensitivity and affect have also found some support in
tified in imaging studies with adults. Cingulate activity the developmental literature. These dimensions offer a
was greater when subjects were instructed to control the beginning for future work that will more finely differen-
amount of negative affect felt in viewing a picture tiate the temperament domain, its development, and re-
(Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002). When hyp- lation to the development of personality. In our review,
notism was used to control the perception of pain due to we now move to considering measurement issues in the
heat, cingulate activity also reflected the perceived de- study of temperament in childhood, providing extensive
gree of pain rather than the physical intensity of the heat evaluation of parent reports. It will be helpful for the
stimulus (Rainville et al., 1997). Finally, large lesions of reader to consider this material in addition to that put
the anterior cingulate either in adults or children result forward by Kagan and Fox in Chapter 4, this Handbook,
in great difficulty in regulating behavior, particularly in this volume. We then discuss recent genetics research
social situations (Anderson, Damasio, Tranel, & Dama- and other psychobiological approaches to the study of
sio, 2001). Smaller lesions may produce only a tempo- temperament.
rary inability to deal with conflict in cognitive tasks
(Ochsner et al., 2002; Turken & Swick, 1999). These re- MEASUREMENT APPROACHES
sults link the anterior cingulate to regulation of neural
activity related to emotion and behavior, and provide ev- Approaches to measuring temperament in children have
idence for a role of the cingulate as a part of a network included caregiver reports, self-reports for older chil-
involved in regulation, cognition, and affect (Bush et al., dren, naturalistic observations, and structured labora-
2000; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2004). tory observations (see Table 3.3). Each of these
Development of executive attention between 2 and 7 approaches offers relative advantages for temperament
years is indexed by marked changes in the ability to deal study. Caregiver-report questionnaires, for example,
with conflict and to detect errors and slow subsequent can tap the extensive knowledge base of caregivers,
responses (Mezzacappa, 2004: Rueda, Fan, et al., 2004; who have seen the child in many different situations
see reviews by Rothbart, Posner, & Kieras, in press; over a long period of time. Questionnaires are also con-
Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). Between 30 to 36 months, venientthey are relatively inexpensive to develop, ad-
children are able to perform a variant of the Stroop task, minister, and analyze, and allow the study of multiple
the Spatial Conflict key press task, which in adults is re- variables (Bates, 1989b, 1994). Alternatively, natura-
lated to activation of the anterior cingulate. In this task, listic observations can possess high degrees of objec-
conflict occurs when a stimulus is presented on the side tivity and ecological validity, whereas laboratory
of the screen opposite its corresponding key. The domi- observations allow the researcher to precisely control
nant response is to press the key consistent with the ob- the context and specific elicitors of the childs behav-
jects spatial location; the subdominant response is to ior, as well as the time course and intensity of the
press the key that matches the stimulus. At 24 months, childs reaction.
114 Temperament

TABLE 3.3 Potential Sources of Measurement Error in Three Child Temperament Assessment Methods

A. Rater Characteristics B. Bias in Assessment C. Method Factors


Relatively independent of child As a function of child Relatively independent of
behavior behavior or rater-child both child and rater
interaction characteristics
I. Parent questionnaires 1. Comprehension of instructions, questions, 1. Observed child behavior 1. Need to inquire about
and rating scales occurring in response to rarely observed
2. Knowledge of childs behavior (and parental behavior situations
general impression rater has of the child) 2. Parents interpretations of 2. Adequacy of item
3. Inaccurate memory: recency effects, observed behavior a selection, wording, and
selective recall function of parental response options
4. State when completing rating task (e.g., characteristics
anxiety)
5. Response sets (e.g., social desirability and
acquiescence)
6. For ratings, knowledge of implicit
reference groups
7. Accuracy in detecting and coding rare but
important events
8. Kind of impression (if any) raters wants
child/self to make on researcher

II. Home observation 1. Limited capacity of coder to process all 1. Caregiver-child 1. Change in child and
measures (in vivo relevant behavior interaction moderating caregiver behavior due
coding) 2. Coding of low-intensity ambiguous behavior coded (including to presence of coder
behaviors I.8) (e.g., decreased
3. State of coder during observation 2. For ratings, halo effects conf lict)
4. Limits of precision of coding 2. Difficulties of
5. For ratings, knowledge of implicit sensitively coding the
reference groups context of behavior
3. Limitations to number
of instances of behavior
(especially rated ones)
that can be observed
4. Lack of normative data
5. Lack of stability in
observational time
windows; limited
sample of behavior

III. Laboratory measures 1. Scoring of low-intensity ambiguous 1. Effects of uncontrolled 1. Lack of adequate
(Objective measures reactions caregiver behavior or normative data
scored from videotape 2. For ratings, knowledge of implicit other experience prior to 2. Limitations of number
in episodes designed to reference groups or during testing of instances of behavior
elicit temperament- 3. Limited capacity of coder to process all 2. Selection of sample, that can be recorded
related reactions) relevant behavior including completion of 3. Carryover effects in
4. State of coder during observation testing on the basis of repeated testing
5. Limits of precision of coding child reactions (e.g., 4. Constraints on range of
6. Accuracy in detecting and coding of rare distress-prone infants not behavioral options
but important events completing procedures) 5. Novelty of laboratory
3. Subtle variations in setting
experimenter reactions to 6. Adequate identification
different children (e.g., of episodes appropriate
more soothing behavior to evoking
toward distress-prone temperamental
infants) reactions

Adapted from Bates (1989) as adapted from Rothbart & Goldsmith (1985).

In addition to their respective advantages, each tech- becomes difficult for researchers to collect an adequate
nique also is subject to error. In caregiver report mea- sample of relevant behaviors. Laboratory procedures
sures, there may be perceptual or response biases in the often limit the kinds of behavior that can be elicited,
informant. Naturalistic observations are expensive, and and the repeated testing necessary to measure a complex
often show relatively low day-to-day reliability so that it trait in the laboratory may be unfeasible or involve
Measurement Approaches 115

carryover effects. More detail on measurement issues multiple measures. We would agree with the statement
can be found in Bates (1987, 1989b, 1994), Goldsmith of Vaughn et al. (1992), discussing measures of attach-
and Rothbart (1991), Rothbart and Goldsmith (1985), ment security, that it would be most unfortunate if
and Slabach, Morrow, and Wachs (1991). In the present pretensions to methodological rigor forced investigators
chapter, we focus on the issue of the scientific accept- to ignore sources of relevant developmental informa-
ability of caregiver reports. Although the literature tion (p. 470).
often provides admonitions about parent reports, it fre-
quently does not analyze the strengths and limitations of Caregivers Vantage Point versus Bias and Inaccuracy
each approach. We have recognized limitations of care- One argument for the continued use of parent reports of
giver report (e.g., Bates, 1980), but nevertheless have temperament is that they provide a useful vantage point
found that caregiver reports have broadly established for observations. Temperament dimensions are by defi-
validity (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). nition general patterns of responses by the child, and
parents are in a good position to observe the childs be-
Meanings of Parent Reports havior on multiple occasions, including infrequently oc-
curring behavior that nevertheless may be critical to
Parent reports have been extensively used in personal- defining a particular temperament dimension. Most
ity, clinical, and developmental research, including families minimize noxious stimulation for their babies,
the study of temperament. At the same time, the valid- for example, so that it is difficult to observe such situa-
ity of parent reports about childrens temperament has tions naturalistically. Parents, in contrast, can describe
been particularly questioned, especially by Kagan an infants response to a variety of naturally occurring
(1998; Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4, this Handbook, this stimuli, like being given a shampoo, or hearing the vac-
volume). We provide an alternative view to the one he uum cleaner start up. Parent observations also meet both
has presented. concerns about ecological validity and ethical con-
straints about creating aversive situations to assess tem-
Digital versus Analog Validity
perament in the laboratory.
Determining the validity of parent reports has often Kagan (1994, 1998), on the other hand, has argued
been framed in an absolute or digital way, leading to that parent reports have problems with bias and inaccu-
a simple judgment of whether parent reports are valid. racy. Bias and inaccuracy are real concerns, but, in our
Thomas et al. (1963) asked whether a significant cor- view, they are not as great a problem as Kagan suggests.
relation existed between parent reports and indepen- Similar concerns have been extensively dealt with in
dent ratings, and finding significant correlations, personality research, and the dominant conclusion has
concluded that parent reports were valid measures of been that traits can be reliably assessed by ratings of
temperament. More typically, when statistically sig- knowledgeable informants, including the self, friends,
nificant correlations between parent ratings and inde- and parents (Kenrick & Funder, 1988; Moskowitz &
pendent ratings have been fairly small in size, the Schwarz, 1982). In addition, validity is a problem for
conclusion has been that parent reports are not valid. structured and naturalistic observational measures of
Any low correlation could be due to problems with ob- temperament as well as for parent reports, and we have
server ratings as well as parent ratings, but this issue is summarized potential sources of measurement error in
seldom addressed. three temperament assessment methods in Table 3.3.
Early in the discussion of the meaning of parent re- Behaviors reliability coded in precisely defined situ-
ports of temperament, Kagan (1982) advocated a digital ations have a high degree of objectivity. But this does
view of validity, and has continued to elaborate this not mean that the observations also have high validity.
view. In his earlier writings, Kagan (1994, 1998) argues Observational research needs to demonstrate the same
that parent reports are not worthy of use in scientific kinds of validity (content, construct, convergent, dis-
studies of temperament. Kagan and Fox (Chapter 4, this criminant) required of a parent-report measure (Bates,
Handbook, this volume), concluded that parent-report 1989b; Rothbart & Goldsmith, 1985). There are some
data should be supplemented by observation. Our own very promising laboratory assessments of temperament
position, reached more or less independently (Bates, (e.g., Garcia-Coll, Halpern, Vohr, Seifer, & Oh, 1992;
1994; Rothbart, 1995; Rothbart & Hwang, 2002), is Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1991; Kagan, Reznick, & Snid-
also that temperament research benefits from the use of man, 1988; Matheny, Wilson, & Thoben, 1987), but
116 Temperament

none has become so established that it can be seen as the Measured subjective factors thus did not overshadow
gold standard for temperament measurement. measured objective factors as explanations of differ-
Kagan (1994) has also argued that the language of an ences in parents perceptions of their children. In addi-
individual item on a temperament questionnaire is sub- tion to these components of parents perceptions, there
ject to multiple interpretations. This ambiguity, how- remained other error components.
ever, is the main reason why researchers use scales of Matheny et al. (1987) provided independent support
items rather than individual items to measure tempera- for this model, using an array of laboratory measures.
ment. Attempts are made to write the best possible Their aggregated maternal report scores correlated
items, but the researcher does not expect that by doing moderately to strongly with laboratory scores of tem-
this, all sources of error will be eliminated. Basic psy- perament: r = .52 at 12 months, .38 at 18 months, and
chometric theory holds that the reason a set of conver- .52 at 24 months. Their conclusion was that the objec-
gent, but imperfectly correlated items tends to have tive component of maternal ratings was clearly demon-
better test-retest reliability, better stability over time, strable and prominent (Matheny et al., 1987, p. 324).
and better validity is that the error components of indi- They also showed that maternal personality characteris-
vidual items tend to cancel each other out when the item tics were not only correlated with mothers perceptions
scores are added to each other, yielding a closer approx- of the child but also correlated with their childrens be-
imation to a true score. This principle is true of aggre- havior as independently observed in laboratory situa-
gation across multiple observations as well as multiple tions, a finding congruent with genetically based
items. Fortunately, one need not be limited to simply similarities between mother and child.
adding items and hoping that error is thereby reduced. A pattern of moderate to strong validity correlations
With analytical tools such as LISREL and EQS, one can for parent report can now be found in a number of places
also explicitly model linkages between items and in the literature. One important requirement for ascer-
scales error components, creating latent constructs that taining construct validity is that both measures demon-
more precisely control for measurement error. Other ap- strate adequate reliability, and often the observational
proaches to reducing concerns about validity are to use or mechanical measures, not the parent-report measures,
validity scale filters as in the Minnesota Multiphasic are deficient in this regard. To produce adequate relia-
Personality Inventory (MMPI ), and to study the ways bility, aggregation across multiple measures is often
parents construe child behavior and the items re- necessary (Rushton, Brainerd, & Pressley, 1983). Eaton
searchers present to them (Bates, 1994). (1983) recorded activity level from actometers worn by
preschoolers over repeated nursery school free play ses-
A Components-of-Variance Approach to Validity
sions. Reliability of the actometers was .13 in a single
We prefer to frame the question of validity of parent re- session, but rose to .75 when multiple sessions were ag-
ports in terms of components of variance, judging a gregated (Eaton, 1994). Aggregated scores also corre-
measures validity on a continuum rather than an ab- lated .75 with parent report temperament ratings using
solute judgment. Bates and Bayles (1984) asked how the Colorado Childhood Temperament Inventory
much variance in parent reports could be explained by (CCTI ) activity level scale and .73 with composite staff
reports of independent observers in a series of second- ratings of child activity level.
order empirical analyses on data from their longitudinal Asendorpf (1990) used multiple measures on multi-
study. They found that: (a) mother ratings of their chil- ple occasions to assess childrens behavioral inhibition
dren on an array of temperament and nontemperament to strangers (shyness) across a 4-year period beginning
traits showed appropriate convergent and discriminant at age 3. Measures included a parent-report assessment
relations on similar sets of scales from 6 months to 3 as well as observations of childrens behavior with
years of age; ( b) fathers and mothers agreed at generally strange adults and children. Of all the measures taken by
moderate levels; (c) mothers and observers (in both nat- Asendorpf, parent report consistently showed the
uralistic and structured contexts) agreed at generally strongest relations with the other measures; for example,
modest but significant levels; and (d) factors such as parent reported shyness predicted latency to talk to a
anxiety or the tendency to describe oneself in socially stranger at 3 years with r = .67; the overall average r be-
desirable ways, which could reflect subjective biases, tween parent-report and other shyness measures across
accounted for only modest portions of the variance. the 4 years ranged from .43 to .53. Bishop, Spence, and
Measurement Approaches 117

McDonald (2003) also reported convergence between ranged from .60 (for attentiveness) to .83 (for sensory
parent ratings on a new behavioral inhibition question- sensitivity). Contrary to the argument of Seifer,
naire and both teacher questionnaires and structured ob- Sameroff, Barrett, and Krafchuk (1994), this suggests
servation measures. that parents are not necessarily deficient or strongly bi-
Laboratory measures have also been found to be pos- ased in their powers of observation, especially since
itively related to the Infant Behavior Questionnaire their training for the task was minimal. Hagekull et al.
(IBQ) and Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (1984) also found that for an open time frame, general
(TBAQ) (see Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1991) and between questionnaire scales completed by the parents converged
temperament measures and model tasks designed to re- to a modest to moderately strong degree with scales
flect underlying brain function; for example, a positive based on independent direct observation, with correla-
relation was found between a laboratory spatial conflict tions ranging from .21 to .63. We attribute the apparent
task designed to assay executive attention and the CBQ improvements in observer-parent agreement coefficients
measure of inhibitory control (r = .66) for 36-month-old more to the studys careful effort to observe sufficiently
children (Gerardi-Caulton, 2000). Rothbart, Ellis, and large numbers of key events than to any conceptual or
Posner (2004) also reported relations between effortful psychometric advantages in the questionnaire they used
control scales and spatial conflict scores. These findings (the Baby Behavior Questionnaire; BBQ). Although
provide further validational support for parent reports of BBQ scales were developed through factor analysis,
temperament, but agreement is not always found (see some of the scales in this instrument have some difficul-
Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4, this Handbook, this volume). A ties in interpretation, related to apparently heteroge-
comprehensive review of validity studies in the mea- neous content.
surement of temperament is needed.
Goldsmith et al. (1991) correlated mother reports Prenatal Perception Studies
with those of day-care teachers in samples of preschool- Researchers have also studied parent expectations of
ers, toddlers, and infants. Using a variety of standard temperament before the child is born, which are often
temperament scales, they found strong convergence be- significant predictors of postnatal ratings of tempera-
tween scales from different questionnaires measuring ment (Diener, Goldstein, & Mangelsdorf, 1995; Mebert,
the same construct, and generally acceptable divergence 1991), and we discussed these in detail in our 1998
between scales expected to differ. Correlations between Handbook chapter. Diener et al. (1995), for example,
mother and day-care teacher for two older groups were looked at mother and father agreement over time. Prena-
in the typical range for correlations between parents tally, mothers and fathers temperament expectations
and other observers (.11 to .50 for preschoolers, with were only modestly to moderately correlated, and the
the highest correlation on activity level, and .00 to .35 correlation pattern was generally nondifferentiated.
for toddlers, with the highest correlation also for activ- Mothers expectations of unadaptability to novelty, for
ity), and perhaps a little above this range for infants (.21 example, were more highly related to fathers fussy/dif-
to .60, with the highest correlation on one of the mea- ficult expectations than to the fathers unadaptability
sures of approach-sociability). Day-care teachers would expectations. Postnatally, however, mother-father con-
presumably be well acquainted with the children, al- vergence was considerably stronger, and there was also a
though Goldsmith et al. did not report the degree of ac- strong pattern of discriminant validity. For example,
quaintance. However, Goldsmith et al. (1991) emphasize mothers ratings of unadaptability were correlated with
the difference in contexts between mother and teacher fathers ratings of unadaptability .67, and with fathers
observations. ratings of difficultness only .28.
As evidence for potential impact of failure to control Mebert (1991) and Diener et al. (1995) speculated
context, they cite the Hagekull, Bohlin, and Lindhagen that prenatal expectations reflected a vague internal
(1984) study. Hagekull et al. (1984) asked parents to di- working model of the infant before birth, which might
rectly record infant behavior, such as infants reactions influence temperament through expectancy confirma-
to loud sounds, over extended periods in specific situa- tion processes (Darley & Fazio, 1980). However, the
tions. Parent data converged strongly with independent fact that mother and father perceptions of the infant be-
observers data: Correlations between parents and ob- come so much closer in both a convergent and a discrim-
servers direct observation data for two, 4-hour visits inant sense from before to after their actual experience
118 Temperament

with the baby can be interpreted as evidence for an ob- Shall We Use Parent Reports?
jective component in the ratings.
As we concluded in 1998, evidence to date supports the
Recent Attempts to Make Parent and Observer careful use of parent-report measures of temperament.
Perspectives More Similar Two basic reasons to use parent-report measures are (1)
Although some writers have argued that modest par- that they provide a useful perspective on the tempera-
ent-observer agreement is simply a product of low va- ment of children because parents can see a wide range of
lidity in the parents reports, this is not a necessary child behaviors, and (2) that they have established a fair
conclusion. Modest correlations could, for example, degree of objective validity. In addition, parent-report
reflect observers simply not seeing the behavior par- measures have contributed to substantial empirical ad-
ents based their reports on. Naturalistic and struc- vances, including our understanding of the structure of
tured observation measures are often based on temperament in relation to the Big Five or Big Three
between 30 minutes and 4 hours of observation, and models, as described earlier, and their parallels in psy-
only a few are based on as much as 6 hours total. chophysiological systems (Bates, Wachs, & Emde, 1994;
There is also little evidence that these measures show Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 1994). A further reason for
high test-retest reliability. using parent reports is that the social relationship as-
Two extensive home observation studies attempted pects of temperament elicited from parents may in
to address such problems (Bornstein, Gaughran, & themselves be important to understanding development.
Segui, 1991; Seifer et al., 1994). In our 1998 handbook Although we draw the conclusion that caregiver re-
chapter, we described these studies in detail, indicating ports are useful in research, we share the concerns ex-
limitation in the designs, and concluding that the ideal pressed in the literature about their measurement issues.
large scale study for the limits of the objective compo- Caregiver reports must be carefully interpreted as re-
nent of parent reports of temperament has not yet ap- flecting a combination of subjective and objective fac-
peared (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Studies approaching tors (Seifer, 2003). Improvements in caregiver-report
the ideal would require conceptually well-developed measures should recognize possible sources of bias, such
measures with careful attention to both parent-report as parents tendency to contrast one child with another
and observer-report forms. Extra attention would be de- in rating temperament (Saudino, 2003) on some, but not
voted to validating observer- and parent-report mea- all, parent report questionnaires (Hwang & Rothbart,
sures, testing for convergence and divergence among 2003). We should also develop subscales to detect spe-
measures, as well as studying relations between the cific biases in reporters and improve the generalizibility
measures and alternate ways of observing (e.g., sum- of the observational measures we use to validate care-
mary ratings versus independently recorded molecular giver reports (Goldsmith & Hewitt, 2003).
behavior frequencies or naturalistic versus structured Observational and laboratory measures are appealing
observations). One recent example of such a study is and should also be employed in temperament studies;
that of Forman et al. (2003), who showed that aggregat- however, they should not at this time be the sole mea-
ing laboratory measures of temperament across multiple sure of temperament. The primary arguments for this
tasks enhanced convergence with mother-report ques- position are that (a) the validity of a number of such
tionnaire measures. measures of temperament is not strongly established,
The design would also pay greater attention to as- and ( b) even if the measures were well validated, they
sessing contexts of temperament-relevant behavior. Al- would often be awkward and highly expensive to use.
though the concept of temperament implies some Improvements in both parent-report and observational
degree of cross-situational consistency, there is no rea- measures are needed. Parent reports can likely be made
son to suppose that any given trait should be equally more objective, and the subjective components can be
well revealed in all contexts, given variability in insti- modeled more accurately and even controlled for (see
gation of the response. The issue of context is crucial to Bates, 1994). The construct validity of observational
all forms of temperament assessment, and will prove measures can also be improved. We now turn to a review
important as early temperament characteristics are of research on additional temperament-related mea-
linked to developing coping strategies across varying sures assessing the neural substrates of temperament-
situations. related behavior.
Psychobiological Research Approaches 119

PSYCHOBIOLOGICAL RESEARCH Tellegen et al. (1988) reported studies of adult


APPROACHES monoygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins who had
been reared either together or apart. Overall correla-
Gunnar (1990) describes five assumptions guiding psy- tions of traits for MZ twins reared apart were surpris-
chobiological research on temperament: (1) the as- ingly of a magnitude usually found for identical twins
sumption that temperament variation is regulated by raised together (average r = .49), with heritability es-
the central nervous system; (2) the assumption that timates of about .50. Correlations for MZ twins raised
measures of peripheral systems inform us about the apart were .61 for stress reaction, .48 for sense of
physiological bases of temperament because peripheral well-being, .50 for control, .49 for low risk taking, and
activity is regulated centrally, allowing the use of .46 for aggression. For the three superfactors of Posi-
nonintrusive measures such as heart rate or electroder- tive Emotionality (extraversion), Negative Emotional-
mal response; (3) the assumption that fundamental ity (neuroticism) and Constraint (effortful and fearful
temperament and emotional processes reflect a com- control), only Positive Emotionality showed evidence
mon mammalian heritage (Panksepp, 1982), allowing of higher correlations for MZ and DZ twins raised to-
research on animal models; (4) the assumption that gether in comparison with twins raised apart (MZ
the aspects of central functioning related to tempera- apart r = .34, together = .63; DZ apart r = .07, to-
ment variation are those linked to broad or general be- gether = .18).
havioral tendencies; and (5) Finally, as reflected in Goldsmith et al. (1997) have reviewed developmental
Rothbart and Derryberrys theory (1981), concepts behavioral genetics research and presented their own
such as reactivity or arousal and self-regulation or in- findings. Reviewing major twin studies, Goldsmith et al.
hibition are central to most physiological theories of (1997) reported that parent-report measures yield MZ
temperament (all quotations from p. 393). We have al- twin correlations ranging from .50 to .80, with DZ cor-
ready adopted a number of these assumptions in the relations ranging from zero to .50. For scales based on
course of this review, and they are further illustrated in Buss and Plomins (1984) Emotionality, Activity, Socia-
this section. bility (EAS) measure, DZ correlations are typically less
than half MZ correlations and often near zero, creating
problems for heritability estimates. Although this pat-
tern could also be due to the interactive effects of multi-
Behavioral Genetics
ple genes, it seems more likely to be due to perceptual
One reason for adopting a psychobiological approach to tendencies to contrast fraternal twins (Saudino, 2003).
temperament is the considerable body of research indi- Despite the tendency for contrast effects to spuriously
cating genetic contributions to the development of tem- inflate heritability estimates, there is substantial evi-
perament and personality. Results of this work are dence of heritability. For example, Silberg et al. (2004)
reported extensively by Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, showed heritability for ICQ traits in infants, even con-
this Handbook, this volume), Goldsmith (1989; Gold- trolling for contrast effects.
smith, Losoya, Bradshaw, & Campos, 1994), Plomin Evidence for larger DZ correlations that are more in
(Plomin, Chipuer, & Loehlin, 1990), Bouchard and line with expected values has been found using observa-
Loehlin (2001), and for animal studies, Wimer and tional methods (see Kagan and Foxs discussion of the
Wimer (1985). Because these reviews are available else- genetics of behavioral inhibition, Chapter 4, this Hand-
where, we briefly review findings that appear promising book, this volume), and for parent-report studies em-
for an understanding of temperament and social devel- ploying the IBQ (Goldsmith, 1993) and the TBAQ, but
opment. Heritability estimates from behavioral genetics not the CBQ (Goldsmith et al., 1997). Research employ-
studies calculate the proportion of phenotypic (observ- ing these last three measures also suggests shared fam-
able) variance in a characteristic attributable to genetic ily influence, as did the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin
variation in a population, and heritability has proven to Study (Plomin et al., 1993) for parent-reported positive
be substantial for most broad temperament and person- affect and approach (Goldsmith et al., 1997). Goldsmith
ality traits. In the area of personality, broad traits tend and Gottesman (1977) have also found evidence for ge-
to show heritability estimates in the vicinity of .50 netic and shared family influences on CBQ effortful
(Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001). control scales.
120 Temperament

In a study involving 3- to 16-month-old infants using (1995) addressed the question of What is inherited?
the IBQ and a laboratory supplement at 9 months, ge- and proposed this answer:
netic and shared environmental effects depended on the
We do not inherit personality traits or even behavior
particular dimensions of temperament studied (Gold-
mechanisms as such. What is inherited are chemical
smith, Lemery, Buss, & Campos, 1999). Additive ge- templates that produce and regulate proteins involved in
netic effects accounted fully for Distress to Limitations, building the structure of nervous systems and the neuro-
Fear, and Activity Level measures, shared family ef- transmitters, enzymes, and hormones that regulate
fects accounted for Soothability, and genetic, shared, them. . . . How do these differences in biological traits
and nonshared environmental effects accounted for shape our choices in life from the manifold possibilities
Smiling and Laughter and Duration of Orienting. The provided by environments? . . . Only cross-disciplinary,
covariation of mother and father report and lab mea- developmental, and comparative psychological research
sures of stranger distress reflected chiefly genetic influ- can provide the answers. (pp. 331332)
ences. Arseneault et al. (2003) similarly combined data
across multiple reporters and settings, finding stronger We now recognize that experiential and environmen-
genetic effects for antisocial behavior across than tal processes themselves build changes in brain struc-
within situations in 5-year-old children. Saudino and ture and functioning (Posner & Raichle, 1994), both
Cherny (2001) found that covariation between mother before and after birth (Black & Greenough, 1991). This
and father reports on the CCTI shyness scale in infants situation is a far cry from the view that genetics gives us
from 14 to 36 months was mediated by genetic factors, hardwiring that determines our future temperament and
but to a lesser extent than it was mediated by nonshared personality, and it demands developmental research. An
environment factors. exciting recent approach takes advantage of new meth-
Goldsmiths positive affect /approach findings are ods of molecular genetics, and we review briefly some of
congruent with Tellegen et al.s (1988) study of MZ and the research in this area.
DZ twins raised together and apart, in that shared family
effects are found for positive emotionality. The shared
environmental effect on effortful control reported by MOLECULAR GENETICS AND
Goldsmith et al. (1997) requires replication, but shared TEMPERAMENT
family experience may also prove to be important in the
development of attentional control. These findings may The mapping of the human genome has provided a prom-
stimulate research into conditions that promote ap- ising new direction for studying genes and environment
proach, orienting, positive affect, and self-control in the in development (G. Carey, 2003; Plomin & Caspi, 1999).
childs early social environment. Silberg et al. (2004) Genetic alleles identified in previous adult research, for
support the finding of a shared environment effect in ad- example, have been examined in children. An associa-
dition to genetic influence on an aggregated sociability tion between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D4 re-
and positive affect scale on the ICQ. They also found ev- ceptor (DRD4) and novelty seeking in adults was
idence for a shared environment effect in unadaptability reported in 1996 (Benjamin, Ebstein, & Belmaker,
(novelty distress), as well as genetic heritability. 1996; Ebstein et al., 1996), although replication of these
Although behavior genetics research indicates strong results has been inconsistent. In addition, variation in
heritability of individual differences in temperament in the 5-HTTLPR, a serotonin transporter gene, had been
the populations studied to date, these findings are based associated with Five Factor Model Neuroticism scores
on the usual environmental circumstances experienced and with measures of fear and harm avoidance (see re-
by developing children, and any heritability estimates view by Lesch, Greenberg, Higley, Bennett, & Murphy,
reflect genes and environment operating together. The 2002). In a recent imaging study, presentation of fear
results do not tell us what might be accomplished via en- stimuli also resulted in increased activation of the right
vironmental intervention. They also do not reveal the amygdala in adults with the l /s or s/s form of the 5-HT-
specific developmental processes involved in tempera- TLPR gene (Hariri et al., 2002).
ment and personality outcomes. To learn more about the Ebstein and his colleagues used a longitudinal sample
latter questions, studies furthering our understanding of to investigate these two genetic polymorphisms in rela-
temperament and development are essential. Zuckerman tion to neonatal and later infant behavior (Auerbach
Molecular Genetics and Temperament 121

et al., 1999; Ebstein et al., 1998). Ebstein and Auerbach to distress related to being reared in the nursery may be
used the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) related to their distractibility; another is that mothers
to measure temperament during the neonatal period, may buffer their infants experience so as to moderate
and the IBQ to measure temperament at 2 months the expression of the genetic characteristic. Bennett
(Auerbach et al., 1999; Ebstein et al., 1998). In the new- et al. (2002) further reported an interaction between the
born examination, the DRD4 long variant that has been short repeat form of the 5-HTTLR genotype and rearing
linked to sensation seeking in adults was associated condition in relation to CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA,
with orientation, range of state, regulation of state, and a marker for a disposition to aggressive behavior.
motor organization. In addition, an interaction was Barr et al. (in press) found that nursery-reared young
found between DRD4 and the 5-HTTLPR polymor- monkeys engaged in more social play than mother-reared
phisms. The serotonin transporter gene s/s polymor- monkeys, but if they had the short form of the distress-
phism that has been linked to fear and distress in adults related 5-HTTLR allele, nursery-reared monkeys
was related to lower orientation scores, but only for showed lower levels of social play, similar to the amount
neonates who did not have the long repeat variant of of play of mother-reared monkeys. Nursery-reared ani-
DRD4. For those who did, presence of the 5-HTTLPR mals with the l /s genotype were more aggressive than
s/s genotype had no effect. either the mother-reared or l / l nursery-reared animals,
Newborns who demonstrated high orientation and suggesting the involvement of serotonin in development
motor organization showed lower negative emotionality of aggression, but only in animals exposed to early life
at 2 months. In addition, 2-month-old infants with long stress and maternal deprivation. The authors note that
repeat DRD4 alleles had lower scores on IBQ negative human research also supports both genetic and environ-
emotionality and distress to limitations. Infants with mental contributions to the etiology of aggressive disor-
the s/s 5-HTTLPR genotype previously related to fear ders (e.g., G. Carey, 1996; Dodge & Pettit, 2003).
and distress in adults had higher scores on negative In a study of 4-year-olds, Schmidt and Fox (2002)
emotionality and distress, and infants who shared both found a relation between the long repeat form of DRD4
short repeat DRD4 alleles and short repeat 5-HTTLPR and high scores on observed disruptive behavior and
alleles showed the highest levels of negative emotional- parent-reported aggressive and delinquent behavior.
ity and distress. Thus, the balance between orientation Schmidt, Fox, Perez-Edgar, Hu, and Hamer (2001) also
and distress found at the behavioral level (Harman, found a link between the long repeat form and mothers
Rothbart, & Posner, 1997) may also be reflected at the reports of attention problems. No links were found be-
genetic level. Finally, at 1 year of age, infants with the tween serotonin transporter alleles and shyness, al-
long DRD4 allele had lower negative emotionality though they had been predicted. Children with the long
scores, and showed less fear and less social inhibition 7-repeat allele of DRD4 related to sensation seeking in
(Auerbach et al., 1999). adults show behavioral aspects of ADHD, but do not
Suomi and his colleagues have recently reported in- demonstrate deficits in conflict performance as mea-
teractions between genes and environment in rhesus sured by the color-word Stroop task (Swanson et al.,
monkey studies of the 5-HTTLR gene and development 2000). Sensation seeking might well be a prominent
(Barr et al., in press; Bennett et al., 2002). A standard- characteristic in at least some children diagnosed with
ized temperament assessment was carried out at 7, 14, ADHD. Evidence from evolutionary studies suggests
21, and 30 days of age, and monkeys with the 5-HTTLR that the 7-repeat allele is under positive selective pres-
short repeat allele showed higher levels of distress, as in sure (Ding et al., 2002), which might be related to its as-
human studies (Bennett et al., 2002). In human infants, sociation with sensation seeking, a possible advantage
the short allele was also linked to lower orientation during human evolution (Ding et al., 2002).
scores (Auerbach et al., 1999; Ebstein et al., 1998), but In research on individual differences in attention
analysis of monkey data revealed an environment by gene (Posner & Fan, in press), the anterior cingulate, associ-
interaction. The short repeat allele was related to lower ated with executive attention, is only one synapse away
orientation scores, but only for monkeys who had been from the ventral tegmental area, a major source of DA,
nursery-reared with peers, not for mother-reared mon- and the five types of DA receptors are all expressed in
keys. The authors note that a number of factors may ac- the cingulate. The Attention Network Task (ANT), which
count for the interaction. One is that a general tendency assesses efficiency of alerting, orienting, and executive
122 Temperament

attention, was used in a small-scale twin study (Fan, Wu, Whites (1994) BAS questionnaire. The BIS measure
Fossella, & Posner, 2001). In this study, the executive at- was not related to asymmetry. Buss et al. (2003) found
tention network score showed high enough heritability relations between extreme right EEG asymmetry and
(.89) to justify the search for specific genes. At least two high basal and reactive cortisol, replicating previous
candidate genes were found to be related to executive at- primate findings from Kalin and his colleagues (Kalin,
tention (Fossella, Posner, Fan, Swanson, & Pfaff, 2002): Larson, Shelton, & Davidson, 1998).
the DRD4 gene and the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) Fox, Calkins, and Bell (1994) reported that infants
gene, related to the synthesis of DA and norepinepherine. with stable right frontal EEG asymmetry between 9 and
In a neuroimaging study, these genes also were related to 24 months of age displayed more fearfulness and inhibi-
differences in brain activation in the anterior cingulate tion in the laboratory than other children. At 4 years,
gyrus (Fan, Fossella, Sommer, Wu, & Posner, 2003). The children who showed more reticence and social with-
presence of the more common 4 repeat allele, rather than drawal were also more likely to show right frontal
the long repeat allele of DRD4 that has been related to asymmetry. Calkins, Fox, and Marshall (1996) also
sensation seeking, was associated with greater difficulty found that children selected for high motor activity and
in resolving conflict (Fossella et al., 2002). negative affect to laboratory stimulation at 4 months
These findings are all recent and require further con- showed greater right frontal asymmetry at 9 months,
firmation and extension, but they indicate the possible greater mother reports of fear at 9 months, and more in-
utility of relating genetic differences to specific brain hibited behavior at 14 months. However, no concurrent
networks and temperamental characteristics. It will be relation was found between behavior and frontal asym-
particularly interesting to look at relationships at differ- metry at 9 and 14 months, and greater activation of
ent ages and in connection with different life experi- both right and left frontal areas was related to higher in-
ences. Human studies have also identified significant hibition scores at 14 months. The authors (Calkins
interactions between gene and environment in maladap- et al., 1996) suggest a need to differentiate between
tive outcomes, and we discuss these in the Temperament fearful and angry distress, as discussed earlier in the
and Adjustment section. Structure of Temperament section. They also hypothe-
size that high motor/ high negative affect and high
motor/ high positive affect may be associated with later
Approach/ Withdrawal and Hemispheric
different kinds of problems. For high motor/ high posi-
Asymmetry
tive affect, the problems would be associated with diffi-
We now consider developmental research taking a psy- culties in self-control.
chobiological perspective and employing psychophysi-
ological indicators. In the first of these, differences
Autonomic Reactivity and Self-Regulation
in cerebral hemispheric activation have been related to
temperamental tendencies toward approach versus By assuming central controls on peripheral reactivity,
inhibition-withdrawal. Evidence from electrophysio- psychobiological researchers have developed models of
logical (EEG) and lesion studies has related higher centrally regulated systems that can be studied early in
anterior left hemisphere activation in response to stim- life. In this section, we consider briefly some of the
ulation to increased positive affect and/or decreased research on electrodermal responding, heart rate, and
negative affect (see reviews by Davidson et al., 2003; vagal tone.
Davidson & Tomarken, 1989). The reverse relation-
shipshigher anterior right hemisphere activation Electrodermal Reactivity
related to higher negative affect and/or decreased posi- Several early studies reported a relationship between
tive affecthave also been reported. Resting EEG electrodermal response and introversion (see review by
asymmetries have also been related to positive and neg- Buck, 1979). Jones (1960), for example, compared the
ative emotional reactivity (e.g., Davidson & Fox, 1989; 10 highest and 10 lowest electrodermal responders age
Tomarken, Davidson, Wheeler, & Doss, 1992). Har- 11 to 18 in the Berkeley Adolescent Growth Study. High
mon-Jones and Allen (1997), for example, reported electrodermal responders were described as showing
greater left than right-frontal cortical activity in high emotional control, quiet, reserve and deliberation,
women subjects with higher scores on Carver and and as being calm and responsible. Low electrodermal
Molecular Genetics and Temperament 123

responders were rated as more impulsive, active and Cardiac vagal tone has also been related to tempera-
talkative, more attention seeking, assertive and bossy. ment (Bornstein & Suess, 2000; El-Sheikh, 2001). Res-
Adult studies have also found stable individual differ- piratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA), the fluctuation in
ence in electrodermal reactivity to be negatively related heart rate occurring at the frequency of respiration, has
to measures of extraversion (e.g., Crider & Lunn, 1971). been used to assess parasympathetic control via the
Fowles (1982) reported that electrodermal responding, vagal nerve (Porges, 1986). There is an increase in HR
but not heart rate reactivity, was related to measures of during inspiration and a decrease during expiration that
Grays Behavioral Inhibition System. Indeed, Fowles is extracted from HR variation, and Porges argued that
and Kochanska (2000) found that electrodermal reactiv- variability in RSA reflects individual differences in
ity served as a substitute for laboratory and mother tonic parasympathetic vagal tone. Higher baseline vagal
report fear, moderating relations between socialization tone is also related to greater vagal suppression to stim-
and conscience development at age 4. ulation, although some infants with regulatory disorders
Fabes et al. (1994) studied kindergarten and second- show high RSA but do not demonstrate suppression of
grade childrens facial expressions of distress and skin RSA with attention (DeGangi, DiPietro, Greenspan, &
conductance (SC) reactivity to a film about children Porges, 1991). Vagal suppression is often seen as re-
being hurt in an accident. For both ages, SC reactivity, flecting attentional strategies to cope with the environ-
used as a marker of personal distress, was positively re- ment or respond to stress (Huffman et al., 1998).
lated to facial distress and negatively related to helping. Berntson, Cacioppo, and Quigley (1993) note RSA is not
Results were seen to reflect an interference of personal a direct equivalent to tonic vagal control of the heart be-
negative affect with childrens prosocial behavior. In a cause it is determined by multiple peripheral and central
study of older children (third and sixth graders), SC was processes. They nevertheless conclude that RSA is an
positively related to facial expressions of distress to a important noninvasive measure that shows a high de-
film and negatively related to mothers report of dispo- gree of sensitivity to psychological and behavioral vari-
sitional helpfulness, but for girls only (Fabes, Eisen- ables (p. 193).
berg, & Eisenbud, 1993). Evidence has thus been found Keeping these concerns in mind, we can consider
for electrodermal response as both a sign of distress and some of the findings relating vagal tone or RSA mea-
behavioral inhibition. Lang and his associates (Lang, sures to temperament variables. Porges, Doussard-
Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1997), however, have found that Roosevelt, and Maiti (1994) reviewed studies relating
adults SC to viewing pictures increased for both aver- newborn vagal tone to irritability and found that young
sive and pleasant stimuli, so that the sympathetic infants with high baseline levels of vagal tone were also
response measured in SC may be more general than pre- highly reactive and irritable. Later in development, how-
viously thought. ever, vagal tone has been found to be related positively
to interest and positive expressiveness and negatively to
Heart Rate and Vagal Tone
internalizing distress (for a more extensive review of
A good deal of recent research has focused on heart rate these findings, see Beauchaine, 2001; Porges, 1991).
and on vagal tone as a measure of parasympathetic car- Thus, RSA after 5 to 6 months tends to be associated
diac control. In her review of heart rate (HR) research, with positive emotionality and approach as well as irri-
Von Bargen (1983) reported HR reactivity to stimula- tability. Richards and Cameron (1987) found that base-
tion to be the most stable and reliable of HR measures. line RSA was positively correlated with parent-reported
As noted by Kagan (1998), HR variability has been approach at 6- and 12-months, and Fox and Stifter
linked to low behavioral inhibition in some, but not all, (1989) reported more rapid approach to strangers in in-
studies. Fabes, Eisenberg, Karbon, and Troyer (1994) fants with higher RSA at 14 months. Stifter, Fox, and
found HR variability to be positively related to kinder- Porges (1989) found that 5-month-olds with higher RSA
garten and second-grade childrens instrumental coping looked away from the stranger more during a strangers
to a babys crying. Fabes et al. (1993) also found posi- approach and showed higher levels of interest and posi-
tive relationships between HR variability and measures tive affect, although this pattern was not found at 10
of sympathy (dispositional sympathy for girls, con- months. Evidence of stability of vagal tone is only found
cerned attention to others distress for boys) in third and after about 9 months of age (Porges & Doussard-
sixth grade children. Roosevelt, 1997).
124 Temperament

Stifter and Corey (2001) found that greater suppres- lated to attention regulation and even to the development
sion of vagal tone to cognitive challenge in 12-month- of the executive attention system across this period. Re-
olds was associated with experimenter ratings of search with adults has recently linked high heart-rate
approach, and Fox and Field (1989) reported more rapid variability to performance on tasks involving executive
adjustment to preschool in 3-year-olds with higher vagal attention (Hansen, Johnsen, & Thayer, 2003), and chil-
tone; these children also showed higher positive affect dren with higher vagal tone have been found to show
and greater adaptability. Katz and Gottman (1995) greater ability to sustain attention (Suess, Porges, &
found that children with low vagal tone at age 5 showed Plude, 1994). In sum, relations between vagal tone and
a stronger correlation between marital hostility at age 5 temperament vary depending on the age of the child and
and problem behaviors at age 8 than children with high include both reactive and self-regulative processes.
vagal tone (rs = .65 and .25), although the interaction
was not significant. Katz and Gottman saw their finding
as congruent with a buffering effect of higher vagal tone Cortisol Reactivity
that might operate through attentional self-regulation.
El-Sheikh, Harger, and Whitson (2001) also found an in- Another approach that takes into account both reactivity
teraction: 8- to 12-year-olds with higher vagal tone ap- and self-regulatory control is the work of Gunnar and
peared to be buffered against anxiety related to high others on cortisol reactivity. During stress reactions, the
verbal marital conflict. Finally, El-Sheikh (2001) found adrenal cortex secretes steroid hormones, including
that higher vagal suppression to a taped argument in 6- the glucocorticoids, cortisol and corticosterone (Carter,
to 12-year-olds was related as a protective factor to in- 1986). These hormones increase blood glucose and work
ternalizing, externalizing, and other social problems re- with catecholamines to produce glucose from free fatty
lated to parental problem drinking, whereas negative acids, also serving an anti-inflammatory function for in-
affectivity was a vulnerability factor in the effects of jury and disease. Gunnar and her associates have inves-
parental drinking. tigated cortisol reactivity in relation to individual
Beauchaines (2001) interpretation of developmental differences in temperamentally based self-regulation,
findings is that higher vagal tone is associated with and have reviewed links between stress hormone activ-
more adaptive functioning at any given age: in the ity and development (Gunnar & Cheatham, 2003). De-
neonate, it is linked to irritability; in the older infant creases in cortisol reactivity are found between 2 and 4
and child, it is linked to approach and positive affect as months, and further decreases between 6 and 18 months
well as to irritability. Later higher vagal tone is associ- (Gunnar, Brodersen, Krueger, & Rigatuso, 1996; Lewis
ated with more appropriate social behaviors and adapta- & Ramsay, 1995).
tion to stressors, along with lower depressive and Both temperament and child care are related to corti-
anxious psychopathology (see review by Beauchaine, sol levels. Dettling, Parker, Lane, Sebanc, and Gunnar
2001). However, the links between attentional regula- (2000) found that children high in temperamental nega-
tion and vagal tone or suppression suggest more specific tive emotionality and low in self-regulation showed the
interpretations of these findings. In infancy, irritability greatest increase in cortisol levels when they were in
and anger are positively related to approach, positive af- less than optimal child-care situations. Donzella, Gun-
fect, and duration of orienting (Rothbart & Derryberry, nar, Krueger, and Alwin (2000) investigated stress re-
2002), and infants high in vagal tone may be showing a sponses to competition in 3- to 5-year-olds. Children
stronger approach system linked to parasympathetic played against a familiar adult experimenter and ini-
function in these responses. Later, infants high in vagal tially won 3 games, but then lost 3. Temperamental sur-
tone and vagal suppression may be showing stronger at- gency assessed via teacher report was related to both
tentional regulation. At 9 months, Porges, Doussard- positive affect during winning and to tense and angry
Roosevelt, Portales, and Suess (1995) found vagal tone affect during losing. Although most children did not
to be positively correlated with ICQ fussy/difficultness. show increases in cortisol to competition, the 15% who
For a small longitudinal sample, however, even after did were higher in temperamental surgency and lower in
partialling out 9-month ICQ difficulty, 9-month vagal effortful control. The authors concluded that more ex-
tone predicted lower difficulty at 3 years, possibly re- traverted, surgent children are most vulnerable to stress
Temperament and Development 125

during competition. This is an important finding, be- support these outcomes. In research linking tempera-
cause it indicates links between stress hormones and ment to psychophysiology, recent investigations have re-
positive affect as well as negative affect systems. lated tendencies toward approach and withdrawal to left
Gunnar (1994) also found an initially surprising rela- and right hemisphere brain activity, respectively. In ad-
tion between cortisol levels and preschool childrens ad- dition, electrodermal responding has been linked to re-
justment to a group setting. Rather than finding higher serve and negative emotionality. HR variability and
levels of cortisol for 3- to 5-year-old inhibited (and pre- vagal tone have also been studied, with the latter taken
sumably more stress-prone) children early in the school as a measure of parasympathetic function. HR variabil-
year, Gunnar found that measures of cortisol reactivity to ity has been linked to prosocial responding and in-
the school experience were related to mother-report versely, in some studies, to behavioral inhibition. Vagal
CBQ (Rothbart et al., 2001) surgency measures of high tone has been linked to behavioral irritability, approach,
activity, stimulation seeking, and impulsivity, with a positive affect, and attention, with the direction of the
trend toward less shyness. Teachers also reported fewer linkage varying depending on the age of the child. Both
internalizing problems, greater popularity, and inde- vagal tone and cortisol research stress the importance
pendence for children with higher cortisol levels. Later of both reactive and self-regulative variables in the un-
in the school year, however, higher cortisol reactivity was derstanding of psychophysiology and development, and
associated with teacher reports of greater internalizing all of these approaches are promising for tracing links
behavior and CBQ reports of sensitivity to discomfort. between genetic inheritance, experience, and behav-
Gunnar (1994) suggests that temperamentally linked ioral outcomes.
coping activities of children may mediate their cortisol
reactions so that more shy children will be less likely to
experience stressful interactions initially because of
avoidant or inhibitory coping strategies. More outgoing TEMPERAMENT AND DEVELOPMENT
children will be more likely to seek out stress and show
its effects in early, but not later, group experience when Early theorists of temperament stressed the importance
they may have mastered the social challenge (Gunnar, of finding stability of temperament over time. Thus, for
1994). Gunnars work indicates the importance of Buss and Plomin (1975), to qualify as a temperament,
studying reactive measures in the context of regulatory a characteristic must demonstrate stability from its
coping. In more recent research, peer reactions to a early appearance to late in life. More recent approaches
childs behavior have also been found to be related to to the field, however, have noted that temperament itself
cortisol function. Gunnar, Sebanc, Tout, Donzella, and develops, and the study of this development allows us a
Van Dulmen (2003) reported that children with higher greater understanding of both normative and individual
cortisol levels in the nursery school classroom were differences (Goldsmith et al., 1997; Rothbart, 1989b;
those whose temperamentally based behavior ( high sur- Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). Temperamental mea-
gency; low effortful control) led other children to reject sures can fail to show normative stability, for example,
them. These findings reflect recent increases in com- but genetically related individuals may show strong
plexity of temperament-environment relationships that similarities in their patterns of change. These results
we discuss later in the chapter. have been found in behavioral genetics work on both ac-
tivity level (Eaton, 1994) and behavioral inhibition
Summary (Matheny, 1989).
Behavior genetics research supports the idea that the Even for dimensions showing normative stability, ex-
chemical templates we inherit are reflected in our tem- pressions of temperament are likely to change over time.
perament, social, and personality characteristics, and In measuring negative emotionality, for example, 6-
recent research suggests that gene-environment inter- year-olds spend much less time crying than do 6-month-
action may be particularly important in relation to per- olds, but worry a good deal more. To appropriately
sonality and social development. More developmental assess stability of temperamental characteristics, it is
research is needed, however, to specify how developing necessary to establish continuity in the temperament
brain mechanisms interact with environmental events to constructs studied across time. Pedlow, Sanson, Prior,
126 Temperament

and Oberklaid (1993) assessed the ATP sample at inter- Contributions of Temperament to Development
vals from infancy to 7 to 8 years of age. By using struc-
tural equation modeling, they identified factors that Temperament constructs are fundamental to thinking
applied across the entire age range (Approach/Sociabil- about trajectories of social-emotional and personality
ity, Rhythmicity), or across several of the time intervals development (Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 1994). As noted
studied (Irritability, Persistence, Cooperation-Manage- earlier, temperament is implicated in social learning,
ability, and Inflexibility). A model correcting for error with some children more responsive to reward, others to
of measurement was then used to assess individual sta- punishment. Some children will be highly responsive to
bility on these factors from year to year, and estimates both. Temperament is also closely linked to the develop-
were considerably higher than those previously re- ment of coping strategies. If one child tends to experi-
ported, mostly in the range of 0.7 to 0.8. Even with these ence high distress to strangers, for example, and another
levels of stability, however, there is considerable room child little distress, coping strategies involving avoid-
for individual change in their childrens relative position ance of strangers may be elicited and reinforced for the
on these characteristics. first child, but not for the second. If the second child
Since our last review, an important meta-analysis of also experiences delight in the interaction with a
studies on the stability of personality traits, including stranger, more rapid and confident approach to interac-
temperament, has been carried out by Roberts and tions with strangers is likely in the future. Thus, the
DelVecchio (2000). This review was organized accord- practice and reinforcement of childrens temperamen-
ing to the Five-Factor model, and began with studies of tally based responses may serve to magnify initial dif-
temperament and personality in infancy. Considerable ferences through a positive feedback process. Individual
stability was found in measures of these variables after differences in temperament also promote the childs ac-
about the age of 3 years, with estimated cross-time cor- tive seeking or avoiding of environments. Scarr and Mc-
relations for 0 to 2.9 years = .35; 3 to 5.9 years = .52; 6 Cartney (1983) describe these genotype/environment
to 11.9 years = .45; and 12 to 17.9 years = .47. The in- interactions as niche picking. The child who stays at
crease at 3 to 6 years is of interest, given evidence for the edge of a nursery school class or a party is selecting
the rapid development of executive attention and effort- a different experience than the child who goes directly
ful control during the first 3 years of life, possibly re- to the center of social excitement.
lated to early instability. As attention systems stabilize, In Grays (1991) theory, extraverts, high in positive
controls over earlier more reactive tendencies may in- affect and approach (the BAS), are seen as more suscep-
crease prospects for stability of temperament and per- tible to reward, and introverts, high in fear and shyness,
sonality. Beyond childhood, levels of stability continue to punishment (the BIS). This model suggests that care-
to increase through adolescence and young adulthood, giver treatment may have differing developmental out-
not peaking until after the age of 50 (Roberts & DelVec- comes, depending on differences among children. In
chio, 2000). other models, optimal level theories (e.g., Bell, 1974;
In the next section, we consider issues of tempera- Strelau, 1983) stress individual preferences for high or
ment stability and change in relation to social-emotional low levels of stimulation. A child easily overwhelmed by
development and the development of personality. We re- stimulation will try to keep things quiet, whereas a child
view research examining the development of tempera- who requires high levels of stimulation for pleasure will
ment in the areas of positive affect /approach and attempt to keep things exciting. Mismatches in optimal
inhibition, distress proneness, activity level, and effort- levels between a parent and child, or among siblings,
ful control. Individual differences in emotional and may require major adaptations from one or more of the
motor reactivity can be seen early in life, and they will children or parents. Situational challenges, such as an
be influenced over time by the development of more reg- intense day-care experience for an easily overstimulated
ulatory systems, one of their emotionally based (fear or infant, or demands for extended quiet time for a stimu-
behavioral inhibition), the other more directly self- lus-seeking older child, may lead to problems for both
regulative (effortful control). The first system develops child and caregiver.
earlier than the second, and both are developing during Scarr and McCartney (1983) also describe evocative
the period when Roberts and DelVecchio (2000) re- interactions where the childs temperamental character-
ported the lowest levels of normative stability. istics elicit reactions from others that may influence the
Temperament and Development 127

childs development. Thus, a positive and outgoing dis- lated behaviors (smiling and laughter; vocal and motor
position may serve as a protective factor eliciting the activity) is also found in parents reports of tempera-
support of others in a high-risk environment (Werner, ment and in home observations (Rothbart, 1986). It is
1985). Radke-Yarrow and Sherman (1990) noted that in displayed toward exciting and novel objects as well as to-
a high-risk situation, a buffering effect can occur when ward people (Bradley, 1985). However, Aksan and
the childs characteristics meet the needs of the parent Kochanska (2004a) used confirmatory factor analysis to
(these needs may be quite idiosyncratic). Acceptance by determine that observed social and non-social positive
adults can then lead children to feel there is something affect at 7 months formed two separate factors.
special or important about them personally. This notion Beyond 3 to 4 months, positive affect shows norma-
is very similar to Thomas and Chesss (1977) good- tive increases in probability and duration across the
ness-of-fit argument. first year of life, both in home observation and parent-
Because temperament itself develops (Rothbart, report data (Rothbart, 1981, 1986). Stability has also
1989b), new systems of behavioral organization (e.g., been found for individual differences on a composite
smiling and laughter, frustration, executive attention) positive emotionality measure including smiling and
will also come online over time. Any new systems that laughter, motor and vocal activity, as assessed by par-
serve to regulate action and emotion will also come to ent-report and home observation between 3 to 9 months,
modulate characteristics that were previously present, and stability of a laboratory measure of smiling and
yielding potential instability of temperament across the laughter between 3 and 13.5 months of age (Rothbart,
developmental transition. In addition to the direct ef- 1986). Smiling and laughter in infancy as observed in
fects of developing control systems of fear and effortful the laboratory also predicted both concurrent (Rothbart,
control, children who develop a given control system 1988) and 6- to 7-year-old approach tendencies (Roth-
early in life may have quite different experiences than bart et al., 2001). Pedlow et al. (1993) also found stabil-
children who develop the system later (Rothbart & Der- ity from infancy to 7 to 8 years on their dimension of
ryberry, 1981). For example, the child who develops Approach/Sociability.
fear-related inhibition late is likely to experience a Later in the first year, an important form of inhibi-
greater number of interactions with potentially threat- tion and control over approach develops: some infants
ening objects or situations than the child who develops who were highly approaching at 5 or 6 months now come
fearful inhibition early. The child who is fearful and in- to inhibit their approach responses when the stimuli are
hibited to potential dangers early in development may unfamiliar and/or intense (Rothbart, 1988; Schaffer,
spend more time watching and making sense of events in 1974). In our laboratory, we found increases in infants
the environment than the less inhibited child. We now latency to grasp novel and intense toys from 6.5 to 10
consider some of the major dimensions of temperament months of age (Rothbart, 1988). Infants approach la-
in a developmental context. tency to low-intensity stimuli showed stability from 6.5
months to later ages (10 and 13.5 months), but to high-
Extraversion /Surgency versus Shyness and intensity stimuli, it did not. This finding is congruent
Behavioral Inhibition with the idea that behavioral inhibition is developing
late in the first year, with the inhibitory reactions par-
By 2 to 3 months, infants show a pattern of smiling, vo- ticularly evident in response to high-intensity stimuli.
calization, and motor cycling of the limbs described by Once inhibition of approach is established, longitudinal
Kistiakovskaia (1965) and termed the animation com- research suggests that individual differences in approach
plex, including smiling, quick and animated generalized versus inhibition to novelty or challenge will be a rela-
movements with repeated straightening and bending of tively enduring aspect of temperament. In familiar or
hands and feet, rapid breathing, vocal reactions, eye low-intensity situations, however, chiefly positive acti-
blink, and so on (p. 39). These reactions appeared to vation will be evident. The inhibiting aspect of fear qual-
increase in duration and decrease in latency into the sec- ifies it as a control system that modulates other response
ond and third months of life (Kistiakovskaia, 1965). tendencies, and we elaborate this argument in discussing
Werner (1985) reviewed cross-cultural evidence for both Kochanskas (1993) research later in the chapter.
an increase in smiling between 2 and 4 months and in By early childhood, social inhibition with strangers
vocalization at 3 to 4 months. This cluster of intercorre- shows moderate stability (Asendorpf, 1993; Gest,
128 Temperament

1997). Honzik (1965) also noted that longitudinal Fels relatively long developmental periods, with important
subjects scores on spontaneity versus social inter- implications for social development and for the mea-
action anxiety were stable and predictive over long pe- surement of temperament (Rothbart & Sheese, in press).
riods for both males (the first 3 years to adulthood) and
females (6 to 10 years to adulthood; Kagan & Moss, Activity Level
1962). Bayley and Schafer (1963) found their most sta- Another major temperamental characteristic that can be
ble and persistent category between infancy to 18 years measured early in development is activity level. Using
to be active, extraverted versus inactive, intro- both ultrasound imaging and mothers reports, activity
verted behavior. Tuddenham (1959) reported stability level can also be measured prenatally, and evidence for
on scales indexing spontaneity versus inhibition for temperamental stability has been found over the short
subjects from 14 to 33 years in the Oakland Growth periods that have been measured (Eaton & Saudino,
Study. Finally, Honzik (1965) found that for the period 1992). In early research, Fries (Fries & Woolf, 1954)
between 21 months and 18 years of age, the two most and Escalona (1968) identified activity level as a major
stable dimensions were introversion versus extraver- dimension of individual differences among infants.
sion and excessive reserve versus spontaneity. Birns, Barten, and Bridger (1969) found no stability of
These results can be added to evidence from Kagan activity level from the newborn period to ages 3 and 4
(1998; Kagan & Fox, Chapter 4, this Handbook, this vol- months, but some stability was found from 4 weeks to
ume) on stability of behavioral inhibition, and to Caspi later assessments.
and Silvas (1995) and Pfeifer, Goldsmith, Davidson, and A possible explanation for instability of early activity
Rickmans (2002) recent work on stability of outgoing- level is the tendency for activity to be linked to both neg-
ness and inhibition. In Pfeifer et al.s (2002) research, ative and positive emotional reactivity. When high levels
children were examined at 4 and 7 years with laboratory, of activity occur in the newborn, they are often linked to
TBAQ, and CBQ assessments of behavioral inhibition the expression of negative affect (e.g., Korner, Hutchin-
and uninhibited behavior. At the younger age, children son, Koperski, Kraemer, & Schneider, 1981). Escalona
were classified as extremely inhibited, extremely unin- (1968) observed that newborns engage in their highest
hibited, or intermediate. Close to half the children re- motor activity during distress; positive states were asso-
mained in their original subgroup over the 3-year period. ciated with quiescence. Later in development, however,
More than half changed subgroup, but the change tended the infant often becomes motorically aroused while in an
to be to the intermediate group rather than to the other alert and nondistressed state, as noted by Kistiakovskaia
extreme. Caspi and Silva (1995) identified a group of (1965), and activity frequently occurs when the infant is
children high on approach or confidence at age 3 to 4, orienting toward novel objects or receiving caregiver
who were outgoing and eager to undertake tasks, and ad- stimulation (Wolff, 1965). Links between activity and
justed easily to challenging situations. At age 18, these newborn expression of negative affect may account for
children were relatively low on self-reported control (i.e., its failure to predict later activity. Indeed, when Korner
more impulsive) and high on social potency ( leadership et al. (1985) measured nondistress motor activity in the
and low shyness). Children identified in the preschool pe- newborn, vigor of activity predicted high daytime activ-
riod as inhibited (fearful, with problems in sustaining at- ity and high approach scores on the BSQ at ages 4 to 8
tention) were, at age 18, high on harm avoidance, notably years. Another finding is that activity at 4 months, cou-
low on aggression, and low on social potency. Caspi and pled with negative affect, predicted later behavioral in-
Silvas (1995) finding that inhibition or fearfulness hibition (Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt,
served as a protective factor against the later development 2001; Kagan, 1998); activity coupled with positive af-
of aggression is also congruent with the positive correla- fect predicted later uninhibited behavior (Calkins &
tions found between temperamental fearfulness and the Fox, 1994; Fox et al., 2001).
development of conscience described later. Saudino and Eaton (1995), using actometer measures
In summary, evidence for approach tendencies re- in a twin study, did not find normative stability in activ-
lated to positive affect can be seen early in development. ity level from 7 to 36 months. Nevertheless, in Saudino
Later in infancy, behavioral inhibition related to fear de- and Eatons (1995) study, MZ twins were more similar
velops. Once established, tendencies toward approach than DZ twins at both ages, and MZ twins were also
versus inhibition demonstrate significant stability over more concordant in their changes in activity from 7 to
Temperament and Development 129

36 months than were DZ twins. Lower levels of stability that determinants of fixation time change between 8
of activity level from the first year to later periods have and 27 months (p. 81). No stability was found be-
also been reported. Roberts and DelVecchio (2000) tween 4 months and later measures. These changes are
found the lowest mean levels of stability for activity in keeping with findings that signs of executive atten-
level among other temperament /personality dimensions tion begin to emerge toward the end of the first year
studied (r = .28); other dimensions ranged from .35 to (Kochanska, Murray, & Harlin, 2000), allowing in-
.47 in consistency over time. creased attentional control and planning, and presum-
At least two explanations are possible for early insta- ably changing the meaning of individual differences
bility. One, presented earlier, is that activity can be re- in looking at objects (see also discussion in Ruff &
lated to both positive and negative affect, so that the two Rothbart, 1996).
kinds of activity should be differentiated. Second, the The development of effortful controlthe efficiency of
onset of inhibition as an aspect of fearfulness late in the executive attentionincluding the ability to inhibit a dom-
first year may lead to lower activity for a number of chil- inant response and/or to activate a subdominant response,
dren under conditions of novelty or high intensity. A sec- to plan, and to detect errors, also appears to be linked to
ond form of control over impulsive activity will also be the childs developing ability to maintain a sustained focus
developing beginning late in the first year and during the of attention. Krakow, Kopp, and Vaughn (1981) studied
preschool years. This is the effortful control system, re- sustained attention to a set of toys in 12- to 30-month-old
lated to the development of executive attention, to be dis- infants. Duration increased across this period, with stabil-
cussed later in this section. Its development coincides ity of individual differences between 12 and 18 months,
with normative decreases in activity level, which in a and between 24 and 30 months. Sustained attention was
meta-analysis of activity level studies showed a peak be- also positively related to self-control measures, indepen-
tween 7 and 9 years (Eaton, McKeen, & Campbell, 2001). dent of developmental quotient, at 24 months. In a major
longitudinal study, Kochanska (Kochanska & Knaack,
Attentional Orienting and Effortful Control 2003; Kochanska, et al., 2000) used multiple methods to
assess effortful control and emotionality. Mother report of
Attention has both reactive and self-regulative aspects. effortful control was aggregated with laboratory mea-
In reactive attentionorienting to exogenous stimula- sures, which included delay, slowing motor activity, low-
tionconsistency of rates of infant looking have been ering the voice, suppressing and initiating activity to a
found across three quite different measures in 3-month- signal, and effortful attention at 22, 33 and 45 months of
olds: (1) a visual discrimination paradigm, (2) an audi- age. Focused attention at 9 months predicted childrens
tory discrimination paradigm, and (3) rate of looking later effortful control, and effortful control was related to
toward the mother in social interaction (Coldren, regulation of anger at 22 and 33 months and joy at 33
Colombo, OBrien, Martinez, & Horowitz, 1987). Byrne, months (Kochanska, et al., 2000). Measures of effortful
Clark-Touesnard, Hondas, and Smith (1985) also re- control showed increasing coherence and stability across
ported stability from 4 to 7 months in average looking 22 to 45 months, so that between 33 and 45 months, sta-
time and duration of first look in visual habituation tasks. bility was equivalent to that of IQ. Children who had
A developmental shift in visual orienting appears to shown more regulated anger and joy and more fear-related
occur late in the first year of life. Kagan, Kearsley, inhibition at 22 months demonstrated later higher levels
and Zelazo (1978) noted a U-shaped developmental of effortful control (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003). The
pattern of fixation times to clay faces with scrambled link between inhibition and later effortful control was fur-
and unscrambled features in both North American and ther supported by Aksan and Kochanska (2004b). These
Guatemalan children. From 4 to 8 months, there is a are exciting findings, worthy of future replication.
steep decline in the amount of time children spend Krakow and Johnson (1981), using measures of self-
looking at both kinds of faces. Between 13 and 36 control under verbal instructions with children age 18 to
months, however, there is an increase in looking time 30 months, found large age effects in inhibitory control.
that is stronger for scrambled than for unscrambled They also found moderate levels of stability of in-
faces. Kagan et al. (1978) argue that stability of dura- hibitory self-control across the 12-month period.
tion of orienting from 8 to 13 and 13 to 27 months, Vaughn, Kopp, and Krakow (1984) reported on two as-
without comparable 8 to 27 month continuity, suggests pects of self-control: delay and compliance. Cross-task
130 Temperament

consistency and coherence across the two broader mea- stimulus also appeared to reduce its soothing effective-
sures increased across age, and the authors concluded ness ( habituation) at 6 months, but not at 3 to 4 months.
that individual differences in self-control emerge and Regulatory behaviors were studied in a longitudinal
are consolidated during the 2nd and 3rd years of life study of 66 children seen at 3, 6, 10, and 13 months of
(p. 990). Reed, Pien, and Rothbart (1984) found strong age (Rothbart, Ziaie, & OBoyle, 1992). The infants
age effects in two measures of self-control (a pinball were presented with stimuli that varied in intensity and
game and Simon-Says game) in a cross-sectional study predictability, and children showed considerable active
of children aged 40 to 49 months. These studies together coping with their own distress and excitement. At 6
indicate increases in self-regulation across 18 to 49 months, childrens disengagement of attention could be
months of age. In our research using the Stroop-like spa- reliably coded. Overall disengagement of attention was
tial conflict tasks described earlier, children began to not stable from 6 to 10 months, but from 10 to 13 months,
demonstrate effective management of conflict at 30 children demonstrated stability in their tendency to dis-
months, and 36-month-old children who showed greater engage from distress-producing visual stimuli such as
interference in reaction time for conflicting responses masks and mechanical toys. Infant disengagement was
were reported by their mothers as exhibiting lower levels also related to lower levels of negative affect at 13
of inhibitory control (Rothbart et al., 2004). Less accu- months. Stability from 10 to 13 months was also found in
rate children were also reported as showing higher levels infants use of mouthing, hand to mouth (e.g., thumb
of anger/frustration in the IBQ (Gerardi-Caulton, 2000), sucking), approach, and withdrawing the hand, suggest-
suggesting attentional control over emotion as well as ing that some of the infants self-regulation strategies
action. Additional research with conflict tasks indicates were becoming habitual by this time.
development of conflict performance between the ages Direct links have also been found between childrens
of 2 and 7 (see review by Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). disengagement of attention and decreases in negative af-
Murphy, Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, and Guthrie fect (Stifter & Braungart, 1995). Correlations also have
(1999) studied children longitudinally at 4 to 6, 6 to 8, 8 been found between infants use of self-regulation in
to 10, and 10 to 12 years of age, using parent and teacher anger inducing situations and their early childhood abil-
report. Children were reported as increasing control over ity to delay responses (Calkins & Williford, 2003), sug-
attentional shifting and inhibitory control across age, and gesting that mechanisms used to cope with negative
girls also decreased in impulsivity. With the exception of emotion may later be transferred to control of cognition
attention shifting, the measures of self-regulation showed and behavior, as suggested by Posner and Rothbart
considerable stability across this period of development. (1998). Further support of this idea was found by Mis-
chel and his colleagues (Sethi, Mischel, Aber, Shoda, &
Attention and Distress
Rodriguez, 2000). Toddlers were briefly separated from
A consistent theme of the relation between attention and their mothers and childrens coping strategies coded.
distress is that the two mutually influence each other. In Later, at age 5, their behavior was observed in a situa-
a study of attentional orienting and soothing in 3- to 6- tion where they could delay gratification for a more val-
month-old infants (Harman et al., 1997), infants were ued reward. Children who used more distraction
first shown a sound and light display; about 50% of the strategies during the maternal separation at the younger
infants became distressed to the stimulation. They then age were later able to delay longer.
strongly oriented to interesting visual and auditory Long-term stability in the ability to delay gratifica-
soothing events when these were presented. While the tion and later attentional and emotional control has been
infants oriented, facial and vocal signs of distress disap- reported (Mischel, 1983). In Mischels work, the num-
peared. However, as soon as the orienting stopped, for ber of seconds delayed by preschool children while wait-
example, when the object was removed, the infants dis- ing for rewards that were physically present (a conflict
tress returned to almost exactly the levels shown prior to situation) significantly predicted parent-reported atten-
its presentation. Apparently, the loss of overt signs of tiveness and ability to concentrate when the children
distress is not always accompanied by a genuine loss of were adolescents (Shoda, Mischel, & Peake, 1990). Chil-
distress. Instead, some internal system, which we dren less able to delay in preschool were also reported as
termed the distress keeper, appears to hold a computation more likely to go to pieces under stress as teenagers,
of the initial level of distress. Repeating the soothing and to show lower academic competence in SAT scores,
Temperament and Development 131

even when controlling for intelligence (Shoda et al., ory of ego-control and ego-resiliency developed by Jean
1990). In follow-up studies, preschool delay predicted and Jack Block (Block & Block, 1980). The Blocks
goal setting and self-regulatory abilities when the par- posited two control systems: (1) ego-control, involving
ticipants reached their early 30s (Ayduk et al., 2000), fearful or inhibitory control over impulsive approach;
suggesting remarkable continuity in self-regulation. and (2) ego-resiliency, defined by flexible adaptation to
In Caspi and Silvas (1995) study, preschool children changing circumstances. The latter system is related to
characterized as well adjusted were described as flex- the temperamental characteristic of attentional effortful
ible in orientation, and capable of reserve and control control, and research by Eisenberg et al. (1996) supports
when it was demanded of them (p. 492). These chil- the predicted relationship between ego-resiliency and
drens flexibility of responsiveness may have been linked CBQ attentional control in kindergarten to third grade
to greater executive attention and effortful control, as children. Resiliency was also related to social status and
well as to higher ego resiliency (Block & Block, 1980) as to teacher-reported socially appropriate behavior. Ef-
described later. At age 18, children earlier identified as fects of self regulation were also significantly stronger
well adjusted by Caspi and Silva had high scores on for children who were high in negative emotionality
Social Potency, including leadership and low social shy- (Eisenberg et al., 1996).
ness. Interesting positive links have been found between Eisenberg et al. (2004) have studied parent- and
activity level as assessed through actometers and chil- teacher-reported effortful control and impulsivity in re-
drens performance on motor conflict tasks (Campbell, lation to ego resiliency in children 4.5 to 8 years, with a
Eaton, & McKeen, 2002). Active 4- to 6-year-old chil- 2-year follow-up. At both ages, effortful control and
dren, especially the younger children, showed better per- impulsivity predicted unique direct variance in re-
formance on tasks that required them to inhibit a siliency and externalizing, and they also predicted in-
habitual response to perform a nonhabitual response. ternalizing problems indirectly, through resiliency. A
Their paper highlights . . . the potentially functional, moderating effect was also found on teacher-reported
yet much neglected role that physical movement may play anger and the relation between effortful control, impul-
in young childrens development (p. 295). sivity, and externalizing. All relations held in predic-
Attention thus shows major developments over the tions from Time 1 to Time 2, except the path from
first years of life, with a more self-regulative system impulsivity to externalizing.
added to a more reactive one (Rothbart, Posner, & In the Blocks theory, resiliency or flexibility con-
Rosicky, 1994). As noted earlier, Caspi and Silvas tributes to the development of adaptation and mental
(1995) Factor 1 (Lack of Control), including a combina- health. As Block and Kremen (1996) put it:
tion of irritability and lack of self-regulation at age 3 to 4
years, was strongly related to negative emotionality at 18. Adaptability in the long-term requires more than the re-
Studies are now underway exploring contributions of both placement of unbridled impulsivity or under-control, with
temperament and parent treatment to the development of categorical, pervasive, rigid impulse control. This would
self-control, as in Silverman and Ragusas (1992) study be over-control of impulse, restriction of the spontaneity
predicting 4-year-old self-control from 24-month child that provides the basis for creativity and interpersonal con-
temperament and maternal variables. Olson, Bates, and nection. Instead and ideally, dynamic and resourceful reg-
ulation and equilibrium of impulses and inhibitions must
Bayles (1990) have also found relationships between par-
be achieved. It is this modulation of ego-control that we
ent-child interaction at 13 months and 2 years ( but not at
more formally mean by the construct of ego-resiliency. It
6 months) and childrens self-control at age 6. can be said that the human goal is to be as under-controlled
Two Control Systems as possible and as over-controlled as necessary. (p. 351)

Early individual differences in motor and emotional re- In the ego-control construct, when fear and its corre-
activity thus appear to be influenced by development of lates develop into a relatively constricted life, approach
at least two temperament-related control systems: One is tendencies are strongly opposed, and rigid functioning
part of an emotional reaction (fear and behavioral inhi- may result. Ego-resiliency, alternatively, is strengthened
bition), the other is more completely self-regulatory (at- by a set of life experiences that build on capacities for
tentional control), with the first system developing both expression and control of impulses. Effortful con-
earlier than the second. This view is related to the the- trol appears to provide an important underlying system
132 Temperament

for the development of ego resiliency, with impulsivity cognitive-affective units and in the organization
also related. The Blocks theory stresses the importance among them.
of experience in the development of adaptation, with en- Coping strategies, which may have been originally
dogenous control systems allowing cultural influence on based on temperamental predispositions, become part of
the behaviors, thoughts, and emotions that are con- these units, and may be consolidated or inhibited de-
trolled, as well as on the particular self-regulatory ca- pending in part on their consequences. Mischel and
pacities and strategies used by the child. Ayduk (2004) give the example of individual differences
in rejection sensitivity (RS):
Summary
RS is a chronic processing disposition characterized by
Because temperament systems themselves develop, in
anxious expectations of rejection and a readiness to en-
this section we have presented a brief account of the code even ambiguous events in interpersonal situations
early development of aspects of positive affect and ap- [e.g., partner momentarily seems inattentive] as indicators
proach, activity level, and distress in relation to atten- of rejection that rapidly trigger automatic hot reactions
tional control. (For further information on the [e.g., hostility-anger, withdrawal-depression, self-
development of temperamental distress, see Rothbart & silencing]. Probably rooted in prior rejection experiences,
Bates, 1998.) Some of these developmental changes lead these dynamics are readily activated when high RS people
us to expect temperamental stability in only limited encounter interpersonal situations in which rejection is a
time windows. Early reactive systems of emotionality possibility, triggering in them a sense of threat and fore-
and approach become overlain by the development of at boding. (Mischel & Ayduk, 2004, p. 118)
least two temperamentally linked control systems. The
As RS becomes habitual, the persons attention may
first, fearful inhibition, is linked to developments in
become quite narrowly focused on the likelihood of re-
fearfulness late in the 1st year of life. The second, ef-
jection, and defensive behaviors (e.g., anger or preven-
fortful attentional control, develops across the preschool
tative rejection of the other) may develop to fend off the
period and shows considerable stability. Another likely
expected rejection. Different levels of generality of
control mechanism for the support of socialization is the
such a disposition are also possible. RS, for example,
development of a social reward system, connected with
might extend to a wide range of human relationships,
childrens desires to please and to refrain from hurting
but the sensitivity may also be more specific so that
their parents and other persons, likely linked to tem-
only rejection by the childs peers, but not by adults, has
peramental affiliativeness. Any failure of these controls
been sensitized. RS may be so specific that it is limited
may be linked to the development of behavior problems.
to a single person in a single kind of situation. Mental
Because these temperamental systems are open to expe-
habits are particularly likely to develop in connection
rience, appropriate socialization will be necessary for
with intimate relationships, as in the family, but they
positive outcomes.
may be carried over to new relationships when more
positive expectations and coping methods are lacking.
Thus, the experience of early criticism and rejection,
TEMPERAMENT AND THE which may have its strongest impact on children prone
DEVELOPMENT OF PERSONALITY to distress, can have long-term consequences for prob-
lems in development.
Life experiences inf luence connections between chil- Mischel and Ayduks (2004) analysis of RS describes
drens emotional reactions, their conceptual under- an anxious or defensive set, but alternatively, childrens
standing of events, and their use of coping strategies experiences with others may be generally of accep-
to deal with these events. These mental habits are in- tance. If so, the child will be less likely to be on guard
f luenced by the childs temperament, expectancies, about rejection or to show a defensive perceptual set. In-
beliefs, values, goals, self-evaluations, appraisals, as stead, the childs attention can be directed more
well as understandings of the situation, the self, broadly, allowing greater conscious awareness of the
and/or others (Mischel & Ayduk, 2004; Teglasi & Ep- state and needs of others. More distress prone, fearful,
stein, 1998). In Mischel and Ayduks (2004) model, and irritable children may be more likely to develop
individuals differ in the ease of accessibility of such habits as RS, but after experiencing high levels of
Temperament and the Development of Personality 133

rejection, even a low distress-prone child would be In this view, the biological equipment or temperament
likely to develop RS. Surgent and approaching children is similar across cultures, but the mental habits and rep-
may also be more likely to expect acceptance, but even resentations of self, the world and other, will vary from
the more distress prone child may lack the conditions culture to culture, and, we would add, context to context.
for becoming sensitive to rejection when others are not By the time a child is a well-socialized member of the
critical and rejecting. This model stresses temperament- society, more biologically based temperament will have
environment interactions for a number of social-emo- been shaped into a set of values, goals, and representa-
tional processes that are likely to be differentiated by tions of the self and others that specify what is good and
context, and we review some of these important interac- bad for the person. Even for children who are not well-
tions in the next section. socialized, values stressed by the culture may neverthe-
When repeatedly exercised, habitual activations of less have an effect. Children in the United States, for
clusters of thoughts, emotions, and action tendencies to example, may still attempt to promote a positive self-
a particular stimulus or situation become very likely to concept, and pursue it though a delinquent peer group,
occur and difficult to change. When mental habits in- even when the goals and values followed to achieve the
volve distress, how might they be weakened or discon- positive concept may not be socially acceptable ones.
nections achieved in the habit? In Eastern traditions, Shiner and her colleagues have recently been studying
this is done partly through diminishing the role of the the continuity of personality from the period 8 to 12 to
ego so that situations can become less threatening to the 20 years (Shiner, Masten, & Tellegen, 2002) and 30 years
self. Mental discipline and meditation also allow weak- (Shiner, Masten, & Roberts, 2003). The 8 to 12 year vari-
ening of links between thoughts and emotions or ables, taken from parent and child interviews and teacher
thoughts and action tendencies. Western therapy simi- questionnaires, included measures of mastery motiva-
larly works through the clients patterns of reaction, at- tion, academic conscientiousness, surgent engagement,
tempting to reconstruct previously consolidated patterns agreeableness, and self-assurance versus anxious insecu-
and provide new frameworks for meaning. Taking a de- rity. Adaptation in childhood and adulthood was as-
velopmental view, however, one would wish to give the sessed by academic achievement, rule abiding conduct
child the kinds of experiences that will form favorable versus antisocial behavior, and social competence. Adult
and noninjurious mental habits in the first place. measures employed self- and parent-report question-
Socialization in the United States and other Western naires, including the Multidimensional Personality
cultures often strongly emphasizes habits related to the Questionnaire (MPQ; Tellegen, 1985), as well as data on
individual or ego, promoting the pursuit of individual academic achievement, rule compliance, social compe-
security, satisfaction of individual desires, and tence, job competence, and romantic competence.
achievement of a positive self-concept. In other cul- Tellegens self-report MPQ contains three broad per-
tures, the shaping of the childs mental habits can be sonality factors: (1) Positive Emotionality (PEM) in-
quite different. Mascolo, Fischer, and Li (2003) sug- cludes scales for well-being, achievement, social
gest, for example, that the biological mechanisms on potency, and social closeness; (2) Negative Emotionality
which pride and shame are based are similar across (NEM) includes scales for stress reaction, alienation,
cultures, while the responses can be shaped in quite and aggression; and (3) Constraint (CON) includes
different directions: scales for control, harm avoidance, and traditionalism.
PEM was moderately predicted at age 20 by mastery
motivation, surgent engagement, and self-assurance in
For example, in American dyads, pride experiences de-
middle childhood (Shiner et al., 2002). PEM was also re-
velop as socialization agents praise childrens accomplish-
lated to concurrent social and romantic competencies at
ments; shame experiences develop in social contexts in
20 years, but adult PEM was not linked to any of the
which children are made aware of their flawed identities.
In contrast, in China, modest self-harmonization develops childhood measures of adaptation. NEM at 20, however,
as parents efface their childrens accomplishments while was related to low adaptation in all areas in childhood,
relatives and other significant others praise them; shame and to all concurrent adaptation measures except roman-
is a normative emotion that develops as parents use ex- tic competence. Even controlling for childhood person-
plicit shaming techniques to socialize filial piety in chil- ality, lower academic achievement and greater conduct
dren. (pp. 401402) problems in childhood continued to predict adult NEM.
134 Temperament

Childhood mastery motivation and surgent engagement ance, low in social potency ( less vigorous, dynamic,
were also inversely related to NEM in adulthood. At age forceful), and low in achievement. Both previously Un-
20, CON was predicted by earlier lower self-assurance dercontrolled and Confident children were low in harm
and higher academic competence, but when childhood avoidance. Confident children were high on social po-
personality was controlled, it was not related to child- tency as adults; Inhibited children were high in Con-
hood adaptation. straint and low in Positive Emotionality.
Shiner et al. (2002) suggest that positive emotionality Caspi, Harrington, et al.s (2003) findings provide ev-
may be more closely linked to current adaptation, idence that the temperament of the child truly provides
whereas negative emotionality shows more continuity the core of aspects of the developing personality. Under-
with earlier adaptation. In our section on adjustment, controlled children, who combined extraversion /sur-
we note strong links between negative emotionality and gency, negative affect, and low attentional control at age
psychopathology, both in childhood and adulthood. Neg- 3, showed neurotic and alienated tendencies as adults.
ative emotionality is also particularly linked to behavior Confident extraverted children were confident and un-
problems when effortful control is low (Caspi, 2000; fearful as adults. More shy and fearful Inhibited and Re-
Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000). Shiner et al. served children maintained their caution and harm
(2002) note that adults high on negative emotionality avoidance into adulthood and were low in social po-
tend to be particularly upset by daily problems (Suls, tency, whereas the more extreme Inhibited children
Martin, & David, 1998). How might the development of were also high in Constraint (a mixture of fearfulness
mental habits contribute to these findings? First, habits and self-control) and low in Positive Emotionality and
related to distress may have involved attempts to de- social support. The most interesting aspect of the re-
crease distress through mental processing, which in turn sults, however, goes beyond temperament to touch on
may have included repeatedly thinking about problem- alienation, traditional values, and social support.
atic events. Positive experiences would be less of a chal- Kubzansky, Martin, and Buka (2004) related chil-
lenge and tend to be less intense, so they are likely to drens personality/temperament at age 7 as derived from
have been less rehearsed. When mental habits have been observer ratings to self ratings at age 35. Childrens be-
tied to difficult and painful situations in the past, one havioral inhibition did not predict adult functioning, but
faces not only current problems, but also representations their anger proneness (Distress) predicted adult Hostil-
in memory that bring forward the mental habits linked ity/Anger, and inappropriate interpersonal self-
to similar situations. Well-practiced associations may regulation in childhood predicted adult Interpersonal
make negative affect, cognition, and action links Sensitivity. Strong relations were found between child
stronger. Thus, early failure, for example, poor achieve- Distress Proneness and adult somatization, another very
ment in school, may create the possibility of long-term intriguing finding. Overall, we expect that these studies
negative affect or neuroticism that extends to achieve- will inspire more contributions to the longitudinal liter-
ment situations later in life. ature related to these variables in the future.
Caspi, Harrington et al. (2003) linked observations of
1000 children at age 3 to their self-reported personality TEMPERAMENT AND ADJUSTMENT
at age 26 (96% of the original sample). Undercontrolled
children (10% of the sample) had been temperamentally In the preceding section, devoted to temperament and
impulsive, restless, distractible, and negativistic at age personality, we have begun to consider temperament and
3; Confident children (28%) were friendly, eager, and some aspects of adjustment. In this section, we consider
somewhat impulsive; Inhibited children (8%) were fear- in more detail theoretical models and research findings
ful, reticent, and easily upset; Reserved children (15%) relating temperament to individual differences in adjust-
were timid but not extreme in shyness; Well-adjusted ment. By adjustment we mean not only psychopathology
children (40%) appeared to be capable of self-control, but also positive behaviors including the development of
adequately self-confident, and did not become upset conscience. We are more interested in dimensions of ad-
during testing. At age 26, previously Undercontrolled justment than in categorical diagnostic systems, and
children were higher in negative emotionality, more think of adjustment as adaptation to particular contexts.
alienated, and subject to stress reactions. They also A child may carry temperament traits from one context
tended to follow a traditional morality. Formerly Inhib- to another, but their implications for adjustment will de-
ited and Reserved children were high in harm avoid- pend on the specific context and expectations of the par-
Temperament and Adjustment 135

ent, peer, or teacher (Chess & Thomas, 1984; Lerner & perament and psychopathology even after decontami-
Lerner, 1994), in connection with experiences and adap- nation (Lemery, Essex, & Smider, 2002; Lengua,
tations to specific situations as suggested in the mental West, & Sandler, 1998; Oldehinkel, Hartman, de Win-
habits model described previously. ter, Veenstra, & Ormel, 2004). The second way of sup-
porting a distinction between temperament and
adjustment is shown by a variety of studies. One study,
Does Temperament Predict Adjustment?
for example, found that therapy led to changes in par-
Meaningful patterns of relationship exist between con- ents descriptions of their childrens psychopathology
structs of temperament and constructs of adjustment in but not their temperament (Sheeber, 1995). Other stud-
the development of children. This was clear by the late ies address the question of the developmental processes
1980s (Bates, 1989a) and has become more firmly es- through which temperament and adjustment are related,
tablished since then, with many studies showing tem- and these are discussed after considering the second
perament links with psychopathology (e.g., see reviews methodological issue.
by Eisenberg et al., 2000; Lonigan, Vasey, Phillips, & The second methodological issue is source bias. As
Hazen, 2004; Rothbart & Bates, 1998; Rothbart & Pos- we have argued, caregivers reports do show validity.
ner, in press; Sanson, Hemphill, & Smart, 2004; Wachs However, when a conceptual relation is inferred be-
& Bates, 2001). In these studies, temperament and ad- tween two constructs measured via the same source, for
justment have been measured in a variety of ways, in- example, parents, the possibility exists that relations are
cluding parent reports, teacher reports, and direct due to preconceptions in the minds of the informant
observation, with adjustment assessed at home and at rather than the behavior of the subject. Bates and Bayles
school, using both cross-sectional and longitudinal de- (1984) have nevertheless argued (on the basis of many
signs. In this review, we focus primarily on patterns of different tests of subjective and objective components in
relations that indicate a differentiated view of how tem- parents perceptions of their children) that measures of
perament might contribute to the childs adjustment. We subjective bias do not account for more of the variance
mention two methodological and conceptual issues be- than measures of objective phenomena. In addition, as is
fore describing findings. detailed later, the different measures are related to one
The issue of measurement contamination (Sanson, another within and across time in a differentiated pat-
Prior, and Kyrios, 1990) has been of continuing interest tern, for example, with early novelty distress predicting
to researchers. Sanson et al. argued that relations ob- later novelty distress or internalizing problems more
served between a temperament measure and an adjust- than externalizing problems. Recent studies have also
ment measure might be an artifact of content overlap shown credible levels of objectivity in caregivers de-
between the two supposedly distinct measures. Item con- scriptions of childrens temperament, even when subjec-
tent in a temperament scale, for example, might concern tive factors, such as depression, play some role (e.g.,
behaviors that are the same as those in the measure of Bishop et al., 2003; Forman et al., 2003). Thus, source
psychopathology or vice versa. Bates (1990) argued that biases are not as powerful as one might have feared, and
adjustment and temperament should actually have some caregivers perceive childrens behavioral traits in rela-
conceptual overlap. The childs adjustment could reflect tively differentiated rather than global or unitary ways.
a component of temperament, and psychopathology could This brings us to the central question: How does tem-
be, at least in part, an extreme point on a temperament di- perament predict adjustment?
mension. For theoretical reasons, however, we tend to re- Temperament might be involved in the development
gard temperament and adjustment as separate concepts. of behavior problems in a number of ways. Clark, Wat-
Temperament characteristics may also be contribu- son, and Mineka (1994) listed four ways in which mood
tors to adjustment rather than equivalent to adjustment. and anxiety disorders might be related to personality
This possibility can be supported, if studies show that characteristics (also see Shiner & Caspi, 2003):
temperament-adjustment links exist even after correct-
ing for content overlap, or if studies show links between 1. A vulnerability model, where there is a predisposi-
temperament and adjustment that transcend the simple tion to the development of disorders (e.g., in re-
content overlap model. Studies where expert raters and sponse to stressors)
psychometric principles are used to remove items with 2. The pathoplasty model, a variant of vulnerability in
overlapping content do demonstrate links between tem- which personality shapes the course of a disorder
136 Temperament

(e.g., by producing an environment that maintains in specific temperament-related brain circuits are
the disorder) linked to specific forms of motivation or functioning
3. The scar hypothesis, in which a disorder produces en- (Bates et al., 1994; Clark et al., 1994; Fowles, 1994;
during changes in personality (e.g., increased levels Gray, 1991; MacDonald, 1988; Rothbart, Derryberry,
of insecurity) et al., 1994; Rothbart & Posner, in press), as discussed
4. The spectrum or continuity hypothesis, where the psy- earlier. There is some, but not complete, agreement on
chopathological condition is an extreme manifesta- the specific systems and how they map onto behavioral
tion of the underlying personality trait traits. In previous sections, we discussed systems con-
trolling inhibition to novelty and conditioned signals of
punishment and nonreward, as well as unconditioned
Clark et al. (1994) point out that the four models need fear, positive affectivity and reward seeking, sensitivity
not be mutually exclusive. These models may also extend to social rewards, and attentional control. We now use
to behavioral disorders, and to positive outcomes as in: these systems as general constructs to organize the evi-
dence on temperament and adjustment. At this time,
1. A protective model, where the person is predisposed only a limited number of studies permit a differentiated
to deal adequately with challenging situations view of temperament-adjustment linkages, and none of
2. The boost from positive adaptation model, in which the studies are methodologically strong enough to stand
the experience of overcoming challenge strengthens alone in support or rejection of a psychobiological sys-
feelings of optimism and well-being tems model. However, enough convergence exists that
3. The spectrum or continuity model, in which the posi- we are confident about the broad outlines of direct link-
tive outcome is itself the manifestation of an underly- age models.
ing set of characteristics, such as a positive outlook
Theoretical Expectations
on experience
Direct linkage models will become more detailed as
These and other possible processes linking tempera- neurobehavioral systems are better understood and as
ment, risk conditions, and psychopathology are listed measures of adjustment are meaningfully differentiated.
in Table 3.4. Generally, available evidence does not For now, a researcher would expect early irritability, or
allow for a choice among the models, but, in recent general tendencies toward negative affect, to predict a
years, behavioral and molecular genetics research is of- wide variety of adaptive difficulties, including internal-
fering the promise of choices (e.g., Eaves, Silberg, & izing, or anxiety problems, and externalizing, or con-
Erkanli, 2003). duct problems, as well as deficits in positive social
competencies. As measures of irritability are more
Direct Linkage finely differentiated, however, more clearly defined
Most studies of the relations between temperament and pathways to later adjustment may be identified. For ex-
adjustment have considered direct, linear effects, where ample, sensitivity to minor aversive stimuli might pre-
a particular temperament trait contributes to the devel- dispose a child to both internalizing (e.g., whining and
opment of an adjustment pattern. Additive effects of withdrawal) and externalizing (e.g., reactive aggression)
multiple temperament traits are also possible, as when behavior problems, whereas irritability to frustration of
two or more temperament traits linearly increase the risk reward or of stimulation-seeking behavior (Rothbart,
of some disorder, such as negative affectivity and lack of Derryberry, et al., 1994) would likely pertain more to
impulse control predicting behavior problems (Eisenberg externalizing tendencies than to internalizing ones.
et al., 1996), both negative emotionality and fearfulness Temperamental tendencies toward fearfulness in
predicting levels of young boys internalizing problems novel or potentially punishing situations should predict
(Gilliom & Shaw, 2004), or both impulsivity and nega- internalizing-type adjustments most directly, although
tive emotionality associated with adolescents antisocial they may also serve to predict externalizing problems in
behavior (Stice & Gonzales, 1998). inverse or interactive ways, as discussed later. A finer
In evaluating direct linkage models, studies consider- differentiation of fearfulness will ultimately be impor-
ing multiple temperament traits in relation to multiple tant for predicting different kinds of internalizing ad-
dimensions of adjustment are critical. According to cur- justment. For example, separation distress may differ in
rent theories of psychopathology, individual differences some ways from novelty fear (see Fowless, 1994, dis-
Temperament and Adjustment 137

TABLE 3.4 Processes That May Link Temperament and Adjustment

Processes Examples
A. Direct, Linear Effects
1. Temperament extreme constitutes psychopathology or positive Extreme shyness, Attention-Deficit /Hyperactivity Disorder
adaptation. High attentional control
2. Temperament extreme predisposes to a closely related condi- Fearfulness Anxiety Disorder, Agoraphobia / Panic Disorder
tion. High attentional control good social adjustment
3. Temperament characteristics affect particular symptomatology Anxiety versus hopelessness in depression
of a disorder.
B. Indirect, Linear Effects
1. Temperament structures the immediate environment, which High stimulation seeking leaving home early, marrying poorly
then inf luences development of positive adjustment or psycho- High attentional control planning good school adjustment
pathology.
2. Temperament biases others to behave in ways that provide ex- High positive affect attention from caregivers in institutional
periences leading to risk factors, pathology, or more positive situations
outcome. Infant irritability coercive cycles in parent-child interactions
3. Temperament biases processing of information about self and Negative affectivity negatively biased social information pro-
others, predisposing to cognitively based psychopathology or cessing aggression
positive adjustment. Positive affectivity positively biased social information pro-
cessing optimism about others
C. Temperament Environment Interactions

1. Temperament buffers against risk factors or stressors. Fear protecting against aggression or criminal socialization
Positive affect protecting against peer or parent rejection
2. Temperament heightens response to event. Negative affectivity augmenting response to stress, increasing risk
of depression or likelihood of post-traumatic stress disorder
Attentional orienting augmenting response to teachers instruc-
tions.
D. Temperament Temperament Interactions
1. Self-regulation of a temperament extreme qualitatively High surgency with nonregulation Attention-Deficit /Hyperac-
changes its expression. tivity Disorder, whereas same trait with good regulation high
competence
High negative emotionality with low attentional control sensiti-
zation and increasing anxiety, whereas negative emotionality plus
high attentional control no maladjustment
2. One temperament trait protects against risk consequences of Fearfulness or higher attentional control protecting against impul-
another temperament-based trait. sivity
E. Miscellaneous
1. Different temperament characteristics may predispose to simi- Shyness, impulsivity, lack of affiliativeness, and negativity may
lar outcomes. each predispose to development of social isolation
2. Temperament or personality may be shaped by psychopatholog- Anxiety Disorder increased dependency
ical disorder.

Source: Adapted from Temperament, Attention, and Developmental Psychopathology (pp. 315340), by M. K. Rothbart, M. I. Posner, and
K. Hershey, in Manual of Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 1, D. Cicchetti and J. D. Cohen (Eds.), 1995, New York: Wiley.
Some of the wording and examples have been changed. Note that many of the examples are theoretically plausible, but not based on empirical
evidence.

cussion of theories placing separation fear in a panic or aspects of manageability, should be involved more
fight /flight brain system and novelty fear in a behavioral closely in externalizing than in internalizing problems,
inhibition system, and see our sections on Panksepps, except that depression has a strong component of low
1998, psychobiological theory). positive affectivity (Tellegen, 1985). However, a trait of
Positive affectivity or surgency, involving activity, prosocial tendency, affiliation and agreeableness, per-
stimulation seeking, assertiveness, and possibly some haps involving sensitivity to social rewards (MacDon-
138 Temperament

ald, 1992), might prove separable from the more general izing problems. Early resistance to control (perhaps akin
extraversion or surgency (positive affectivity) system, to the manageability dimension of Hagekull, 1989, and
as Rothbart and Victors (2004) findings suggest. Low perhaps at least partly related to the construct of effort-
levels of prosocial interest and concern would be ex- ful control) predicted externalizing problems more than
pected to be associated with the development of exter- internalizing problems. This was also found in predict-
nalizing and not internalizing problems, and perhaps ing externalizing problems at school in both the BLS and
with the failure to acquire positive social competencies a separate longitudinal study, the Child Development
independent of behavioral problems. Project (CDP; Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, 1998).
Finally, systems controlling attention, especially the In a structural modeling analysis of CDP data, deal-
executive attention system described earlier, would be ing with the overlap in externalizing and internalizing
expected to be related to both externalizing and inter- symptoms, Keiley, Lofthouse, Bates, Dodge, and Pettit
nalizing, but to have more to do with externalizing (2003) separated mother and teacher reports of behavior
problems than with internalizing ones. As with fear sys- problems across 5 to 14 years into pure externalizing,
tems, attentional control should also play an additive or pure internalizing, and covarying factors, and then con-
interactive role with other temperament characteristics. sidered early childhood predictors of each of these fac-
In addition, a well-functioning set of attentional con- tors. Resistant temperament (unmanageability)
trols is likely to be linked to more positive developmen- predicted the pure factors of mother- and teacher-rated
tal outcomes. externalizing problems, but not the pure internalizing
factors. Unadaptable temperament (novelty distress)
Empirical Findings of Direct Linkage
predicted positively both mother and teacher pure inter-
A number of studies provide support for the models just nalizing factors, and to a lesser degree, and negatively,
described. In general, predictive relations between tem- the pure mother and teacher externalizing factors. That
perament and adjustment are of modest to moderate is, unadaptable temperament predicted higher levels of
size. Correlations between infancy measures and adjust- internalizing problems and, less strongly, lower levels of
ment in late preschool and middle childhood tend to be externalizing problems. Although a disposition to fear-
smaller, and those between preschool or middle child- fulness would not necessarily constrain dispositions to
hood and later periods larger. Even though the correla- aggressive and uncooperative behaviors, it is intuitively
tions may be modest to moderate in size, they have been reasonable that children who are fearful and sensitive to
well replicated, and they are clearly not chance findings. potential punishment would be likely to inhibit external-
Moreover, the size of the relations is usually not less izing behavior (Bates, Pettit, & Dodge, 1995). And fi-
than and sometimes greater than predictions from other nally, difficult temperament (negative emotionality and
theoretically linked variables, such as parenting quality. demandingness) predicted, in this multivariate context,
Lytton (1995), for example, performed a meta-analysis none of the pure factors, but only the covarying exter-
of studies predicting conduct disorder (a diagnosis of ex- nalizing plus internalizing factor in mothers reports.
treme externalizing problems) and criminality, finding These predictions are all consistent with models
child temperament variables to be the single most pow- where temperament extremes either constitute pathol-
erful predictor of the outcomes, even in comparison with ogy dimensions or predispose to risk for these condi-
qualities of parenting. tions. The linkages are of modest size, but they obtain
In the Bloomington Longitudinal Study (BLS), in- from early in life, and are not eliminated by the inclu-
fancy and toddlerhood ICQ temperamental difficultness sion of family and parenting characteristics in predic-
(frequent and intense negative affect and attention de- tion, so they are not simply artifacts of family
manding) predicted later externalizing and internalizing functioning. Also supporting the general pattern,
problems as seen in the mother-child relationship, from Gilliom and Shaw (2004) found that, in a sample of pre-
the preschool to the middle-childhood periods (Bates & school-age boys from low-income families, high levels of
Bayles, 1988; Bates, Bayles, Bennett, Ridge, & Brown, negative emotionality were associated with initial levels
1991; Bates, Maslin, & Frankel, 1985; Lee & Bates, of both externalizing and internalizing problems,
1985). Early negative reactivity to novel situations (un- whereas high levels of fearfulness were associated with
adaptability) predicted less consistently, but when it did, decreases in externalizing problems over time. High ini-
it predicted internalizing problems more than external- tial levels of internalizing problems were associated
Temperament and Adjustment 139

with increases in internalizing problems over time. In lems more strongly than internalizing problems or posi-
partial contrast, Russell, Hart, Robinson, and Olsen tive competencies. The discovery of differentiated pat-
(2003) found that negative emotionality as measured by terns in studies such as the BLS, CDP, and Dunedin
parent report on the EAS did not predict preschoolers study has occurred despite the tendency for externaliz-
adjustment as rated by teachers. However, EAS shyness ing and internalizing adjustment scores to be somewhat
was related to both lower prosocial behavior and lower correlated with each other, making the pattern all the
aggressive behavior at preschool. more remarkable.
Lemery et al. (2002) also provide support for a differ- In other recent studies, Morris et al. (2002) found
ential linkage model. Composited mother CBQ ratings that irritable temperament was positively associated
of child temperament at 3 and 4 years predicted both with first and second graders externalizing and inter-
mother and father reports of behavior problem symp- nalizing problems equally, whereas effortful control
toms at age 5, in a differentiated pattern. Whether or not was negatively associated with externalizing more
the temperament scales were purified by removing strongly than internalizing problems. Patterson and
items overlapping with preschool-age behavior prob- Sanson (1999) found that 5-year-olds low persistence
lems, early anger predicted later externalizing problems (attentive and on-task) was associated with externaliz-
more strongly than it predicted later internalizing prob- ing, whereas low approach to people and novel objects
lems, early fear and sadness predicted later internaliz- was associated with internalizing problems. Lengua,
ing problems more strongly than they predicted later Wolchik, Sandler, and West (2000) found 9- to 12-year-
externalizing problems, and early inhibitory control in- old impulsivity (CBQ) to be associated with conduct
versely predicted later externalizing or Attention- problems and not with depression and positive emotion-
Deficit /Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) problems more ality Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS) to be
strongly than it predicted later internalizing problems. negatively associated with depression, an association
The Dunedin Longitudinal Study (Caspi & Silva, stronger than with conduct problems. In partial contrast
1995), mentioned previously in the context of personal- to the general pattern, however, Lengua et al. also re-
ity development, provides further support and extends ported that negative emotionality (DOTS) was associ-
measures of temperament from parent to experimenter ated with depressive symptoms more strongly than with
ratings. Ratings based on the childs behavior during conduct problem symptoms.
testing sessions, aggregated from 3 and 5 years, pre- Mun, Fitzgerald, von Eye, Puttler, and Zucker
dicted aggregated ratings of parents and teachers in late (2001), also using the DOTS, reported that withdrawal
childhood (over ages 9 and 11) and early adolescence tendencies at 3 to 5 years were more strongly predictive
(over ages 13 and 15). Early approach (outgoing re- of internalizing than externalizing problems 3 years
sponses to strangers and test materialsthe inverse of later. Their negative emotional reactivity scale also
inhibition) predicted, inversely, internalizing problems failed to confirm the general pattern of roughly equal
better than externalizing problems for boys. It did not associations with both externalizing and internalizing
predict either kind of problem for girls. Early sluggish- problems, instead predicting externalizing more
ness (a factor combining lack of positive affect, passiv- strongly than internalizing problems. Murphy, Shepard,
ity, and wariness/withdrawal from novelty) predicted Eisenberg, and Fabes (2004) found that both negative
later internalizing and externalizing problems for girls, emotionality and self-regulation (whether assessed by
but not boys, as well as the relative absence of positive teacher or parent questionnaires) in middle childhood
competencies for both girls and boys. It is not clear how predicted social competence and behavior problems in
approach and sluggishness emerged separately from a early adolescence as rated by teacher or parent reports.
factor analysis describing similar dimensions, but what- And Eisenberg et al. (2001) found that anger, impulsiv-
ever the underlying distinction between the two dimen- ity, and low self-regulation (observed and
sions, they predicted outcomes differently for the two parent /teacher-rated) were more strongly associated
genders. A third temperament dimension, combining with externalizing problems (parent /teacher-rated),
lack of control, irritability, and distractibility (corre- whereas sadness and low impulsivity were more
sponding approximately to the resistance to control or strongly related to internalizing problems.
manageability factors from parent-report question- Also notable is the study by Rothbart, Ahadi, et al.
naires), predicted, for both genders, externalizing prob- (1994). Temperamental negative affectivity (CBQ) was
140 Temperament

concurrently associated with a full range of social traits 4. In another longitudinal study with a low-income Pitts-
in 6- to 7-year-olds, including aggressiveness, guilt, help burgh sample, Keenan and Shaw (1994) found that an
seeking, and negativity (e.g., in response to suggestion ICQ composite of difficultness and resistance to control
of a new activity). However, subcomponents of the gen- predicted laboratory measures of aggression in 18-
eral negative affect factor were associated with the so- month-old boys, but not girls. Also, in a low-income
cial traits in a more differentiated way: Fear and sadness Pittsburgh sample restricted to only boys, Shaw,
were more related to traits such as empathy and anger, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, and Nagin (2003) showed that a per-
whereas discomfort was more related to aggression and sistently high trajectory of conduct problems across ages
help seeking. A small subsample in the Rothbart, Ahadi, 2 to 8 years was predicted by fearlessness in the labora-
et al. (1994) sample had been tested in the laboratory as tory at age 2.
infants. Temperament as assessed in the laboratory 5 to In the Quebec longitudinal study, Vitaro, Brendgen,
6 years earlier showed a somewhat similar pattern of and Tremblay (2002) studied the antecedents of extreme
linkage with the social behavior outcomes: Infant labo- groups of reactively aggressive, proactively aggressive,
ratory activity (again, usually regarded as part of sur- combined reactive and proactive, or nonaggressive chil-
gency or positive affectivity) predicted aggressiveness dren at age 11 and 12. Reactive aggression involves re-
and negativity, as did early smiling (another component sponse to provocations and anger, whereas proactive
of surgency); infant anger/frustration predicted both aggression involves less negative emotion and more con-
higher aggressiveness and help seeking; fear predicted cern with material and social gain. Vitaro et al. (2002)
lower levels of aggressiveness and higher levels of empa- found that temperament at age 6 (parent report on the
thy and guilt /shame. A number of other studies also de- DOTS) predicted in ways at least partly consistent with
serve mention for addressing the question of the differential linkage model. Reactively aggressive
differentiated linkages between temperament and ad- children were lower on attention span, higher on dis-
justment, including Biederman et al. (1990); Guerin, tractability, and more motorically active and intense in
Gottfried, Oliver, and Thomas (1994); Hagekull (1994); responses than both nonaggressive and proactively ag-
Hegvik et al. (1982); McClowry et al. (1993); Rende gressive children. However, both reactively and proac-
(1993); Teglasi and MacMahon (1990); and Wertlieb, tively aggressive children were more approaching and
Weigel, Springer, and Feldstein (1987). These studies adaptable to novelty than the nonaggressive group: Note
considered clinical as well as community samples of that our interpretation here, based on the DOTS scoring
children at different ages and in different countries. In instructions ( by R. Lerner, dated May 7, 1982), is oppo-
broad overview, their findings converge with the gen- site to Vitaro et al.s (2002), who assumed that the scale
eral, differential linkage pattern. signified withdrawal rather than approach. Thus, both
Several additional studies whose designs do not per- forms of aggression can be seen as reflecting consis-
mit a full test of the differential linkage model because tency with temperamental roots. Proactively aggressive
they lack a full range of temperament or adjustment children were temperamentally similar to nonaggressive
variables can be interpreted as roughly conforming to children, except for being more outgoing, whereas reac-
the pattern described here. Among a number of exam- tively aggressive children, consonant with the dysregu-
ples, Prior, Smart, Sanson, and Oberklaid (2000) found lated, emotional nature of their aggression, had been, in
that children who were stable in shyness had a higher addition to highly outgoing, also more active, intense,
likelihood of being diagnosed with an anxiety disorder and less attentive or well-regulated.
in adolescence. Eisenberg et al. (1996) found correla- The focus of this section has been primarily on
tions between teacher and parent ratings of emotional psychopathology or negative adjustment, consistent with
and behavioral low self-regulation, lower baseline heart the emphasis of the literature. However, positive adjust-
rate, and acting-out behavior problems. Keane and ment (including empathy, conscience, intelligence, self-
Calkins (2004) measured self-regulation differences in regulation, resiliency, and cooperation) is an area of at
toddlers by suppression of heart-rate variability associ- least equal importance in development. In our sections on
ated with respiration, which theoretically reflects vagal Psychobiological Approaches and Temperament and De-
regulation and indexes sustained attention and active velopment, we have presented additional evidence on
coping. Low vagal regulation in challenging tasks at age such constructs (also see the Temperament and Attach-
2 years predicted high levels of mother-rated externaliz- ment section in Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Conceptually,
ing behavior and emotional negativity and lability at age there are two ways in which positive and negative adjust-
Temperament and Adjustment 141

ment can be distinguished. The first one is by the positive conditions developmental processes that determine ad-
or negative valence of the trait or behaviors (e.g., cooper- justment. This concept fits a vulnerability model better
ative versus defiant). More interesting, the second is than a simpler continuity (or spectrum) model.
whether a given prosocial construct reflects chiefly the
inverse of a negative adjustment trait (e.g., in the relation Empirical and Theoretical Limits on Continuity
between cooperation and aggressive-disruptive behavior
problems), or whether the construct is substantially inde- As noted, predictive correlations tend to be modest to
pendent of standard negative adjustment items. This moderate in size, especially when temperament is as-
question has only occasionally been addressed (e.g., sessed in early life. This is likely due to several factors.
Bates et al., 1991). Future research may discover how Measurement error is almost always a problem, but
temperament antecedents of positive and negative adjust- when power is sufficient, it can be controlled in struc-
ment components vary, in ways paralleling the differenti- tural models. However, even with such a statistical con-
ation between internalizing and externalizing (see also trol for measurement error, there will be limited
discussion by Rothbart, 1989b, and Rothbart and Hwang, predictive power (e.g., see Keiley et al., 2003). Limited
2005, on the development of mastery motivation). Never- predictiveness can also occur because of conceptual
theless, there is evidence that measures of positive adjust- limitations in the measure of either temperament or ad-
ment are related to temperament in understandable ways. justment. For example, the sample of situations used in a
One key example is the work on moral development by set of items may not be sufficient to capture the relevant
Kochanska (1997). For example, Kochanska and Knaack construct. One particular problem is accounting for the
(2003) found that effortful control, assessed in laboratory lower levels of prediction from temperament at home to
tasks at ages 2 and 3, predicted more advanced con- adjustment at school. This is sometimes ascribed to
science development at age 4, indexed by both laboratory parental rating biases, but many differences in incentive
tasks and child self-report, and fewer mother-reported conditions are present at home and school, and even if a
externalizing behaviors at age 6. childs temperament is measured accurately, the childs
expression of that temperament could differ in the two
Summary
settings. For example, the same child could be resistant
In summary, the literature on temperament and adjust- and angry with the mother and yet inhibited and ade-
ment supports a direct linkage model. With a few excep- quately compliant at school, a pattern seen empirically
tions, specific temperament dimensions also relate in a by Dumas and LaFreniere (1993) and clinically in our
differentiated way to internalizing and externalizing ad- treatment program for young children with oppositional
justments, with early inhibition relating more to later in- behavior problems. It is not that such a child is inconsis-
ternalizing, and early unmanageability relating more to tent in temperament, but rather that a child with a dispo-
later externalizing, and with early negative affect relat- sition toward anxiety can be quite uncooperative and
ing to both outcomes. Positive adjustment dimensions disruptive in familiar situations and more reserved in
are not as clearly articulated, nor measured often enough the highly stimulating and more novel school setting.
to demonstrate differential linkages at this point. Alternatively, a child with an anxiety-prone tempera-
Evidence at this point also does not yet answer the ment could be angry as a way to reduce anxiety aroused
question of which of the direct linkage models listed in in a chaotically stressful home, by gaining a sense of
Table 3.4 applies best to the observed relations between control, and be calm in the well-ordered, supportive
temperament and adjustment. Given generally modest school environment. The habit model described earlier
predictive relations, we would favor a vulnerability or ( linking thoughts, emotions, and actions) allows for dif-
predisposition model; a spectrum /continuity model ferent experiences across situations that differ in the
might also apply. However, early individual differences constraints they offer for temperament expression and
likely become transformed, via developmental processes for different histories of experience that may be rela-
that include experience, into the more complex forms of tively idiosyncratic.
adjustment in later years, and these processes must Another factor in limiting prediction is that tempera-
shape adjustment outcomes. Many child temperament ment itself can change in the course of development, as a
researchers seem to agree with Thomas, Chess, and result of either experience or later-emerging traits such
Birth (1968) that temperament in itself does not consti- as attentional control. It remains an interesting possibil-
tute a negative versus positive adjustment, but that it ity that we may discover laws to account for changes in
142 Temperament

temperament. Asendorpf (1994) found that the adaptive The second indirect process we considered was mod-
behavior of shy children who were highly intelligent im- erated linkage in which we are primarily interested in
proved more over development than that of shy children how temperament and a feature of the environment
who were less intelligent. Fox et al. (2001) found that might interact in the development of adjustment. For ex-
negatively reactive infants, at high risk for behavioral ample, a child with high temperamental negative emo-
inhibition, were more likely to show continuous behav- tionality exposed to stress might be more likely to
ioral inhibition across age 14 months to 4 years if they develop behavior problems than a less reactive child. In
also showed right frontal EEG asymmetry (indicating in 1998, there were more relevant studies on moderated
another way a strong disposition to negative affect), and than mediated linkage, and, subsequently, there has
they were also more likely to be continuously inhibited been a striking growth in studies of moderated linkage.
if they were exclusively in the care of their parents This direction in research is especially exciting because
rather than receiving some nonparental care. developmental theory discussed in the early sections of
It is also possible that the limited size of prediction this chapter has emphasized the likelihood of tempera-
from temperament to adjustment is due to the action of ment-environment interaction. Although these connec-
other major factors in development such as parenting, tions have been posited for decades, they are just now
family stress, or school environment. In other words, beginning to take empirical shape.
temperament might be linked to adjustment through one Rothbart and Bates (1998) identified three subtopics
of the indirect processes listed in Table 3.4. These in- of moderated linkage: temperament environment inter-
clude mediator models, as when a childs negative tem- actions, temperament temperament interactions, and
perament influences negative parenting, which, in turn, temperament gender interactions. In recent years, the
plays the dominant role in producing the childs aggres- greatest empirical growth has occurred in the first of
sive behavior problems, or moderator models, as when a these, temperament environment interaction. Tem-
childs negative temperament has one implication for de- perament temperament interaction findings are more
velopment of adjustment in the context of negative par- slowly growing, and would be worth review if space
enting and another in the context of positive parenting. were available. Most notably, Eisenbergs research team
We next consider such processes, most sharply focusing and others have shown that relations between effortful
on temperament environment moderator models. control and externalizing and prosocial behavior are
stronger for children high in negative emotionality than
Moderated Linkage for children low in negative emotionality (Belsky,
Friedman, & Hsieh, 2001; Diener & Kim, 2004; Eisen-
Rothbart and Bates (1998) discussed two possible indi- berg, Fabes, et al., 2000; Eisenberg, Guthrie et al., 2000;
rect processes by which temperament and adjustment Stifter, Spinrad, & Braungart-Rieker, 1999).
could be related. The first was mediated linkage in Temperament gender interactions were somewhat
which temperament influences transactions with the en- numerous as of the time of the Rothbart and Bates
vironment, which, in turn, shape the childs developing (1998) chapter and have continued to accumulate at a
adjustment. For example, a childs negative emotional moderate pace. However, our previous impression of a
reactivity might evoke hostile responses from care- nonsystematic pattern of temperament gender findings
givers, which build habitual frustration and hostility in appears to still hold. So, to allow more space for dis-
the child. Research showing such temperament-parent- cussing the more dynamic temperament environment
ing-adjustment processes was generally lacking at the interaction literature, we leave the temperament gen-
time the 1998 chapter was prepared, especially longitu- der findings for another review. In the following, we re-
dinal research, and relatively few studies have explored view recent temperament environment interaction
this kind of process in the intervening years. There have findings extensively. In addition to Rothbart and Bates
been some careful evaluations of mediation between one (1998), consider other reviews by Bates and McFadyen-
temperament trait and another temperament trait in ac- Ketchum (2001); Gallagher (2002); Putnam, Sanson,
counting for adjustment, such as the structural modeling and Rothbart (2002); and Sanson et al. (2004). Wills,
work of Eisenberg and her colleagues (Eisenberg et al., Sandy, Yaeger, and Shinar (2001) have also reviewed a
2000; Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 2000). However, space substantial portion of the literature in connection with
does not permit review of this work. an empirical report.
Temperament and Adjustment 143

Researchers sometimes focus on how a temperament overlap with one another, but for conceptual purposes,
traits association with adjustment is moderated by an they are separated here. As developmental scientists, we
environmental characteristic, as in parental hostility are especially drawn to studies showing temperament
moderating the relation between child negative emotion- environment interaction effects in longitudinal studies
ality and adjustment. They also focus on how an environ- of development of adjustment, but some useful cross-
mental feature is moderated by the childs temperament, sectional findings have also emerged.
as in self-regulation tendencies moderating the adjust-
ment implications of family stress. Choice between per-
Self-Regulation Environment
spectives reflects the basic interests of the researchers,
but an interaction from one perspective could often have Temperamental tendencies toward dysregulation, such
also been described from the other perspective, although as impulsivity or resistance to control, may be rooted not
the results may not always be identical. only in underdeveloped effortful control systems, but
A bigger methodological challenge, however, is simply also in the behavioral approach system, or surgency, as
to find the interaction effect. Nonexperimental studies we have previously discussed. Such tendencies have
typically have to deal with correlated predictor and mod- shown direct associations with adjustment, especially
erator variables, problems in the joint distributions of the with externalizing problems, as also discussed. At least
variables, and insufficient statistical power for detecting twenty recent studies show traits in this broad domain
effects (McClelland & Judd, 1993; Stoolmiller, 2001; interacting with characteristics of the rearing environ-
Wachs & Plomin, 1991). Sometimes, interaction effects ment in the development of adjustment, and most of
may be present but not found by statistical tests or statis- these consider the effects of temperament and parenting.
tically significant effects may be sample-specific or spu- One theme emerging across studies is that dysregula-
rious. For these reasons, we focus especially on effects tion traits are more highly associated with problem be-
that have been replicated in some fashion. Because re- havior when parenting is negative or harsh rather than
search in our area so seldom exactly repeats the methods gentle. Calkins (2002) found that 18-month-old children
of even the most fundamental studies, it is too much to re- high on distress and resistance in frustrating situations
quire full replication. However, the literature is begin- were likely to be high on angry and aggressive behavior
ning to show some interesting patterns. in similar situations at 24 months when their mothers
We also describe a few failures to find interaction ef- were low in positive parenting, but not when their moth-
fects, especially when they might constitute nonreplica- ers were highly positive. Rubin, Burgess, Dwyer, and
tions of a previously found pattern. Given the assumed Hastings (2003) measured childrens self-regulation in
statistical bias against finding possible interaction ef- laboratory tasks at age 2 and mother reports on the
fects, we focus mostly on positive findings. Many, but TBAQ at age 2. They also measured intrusive and hostile
not all, consider the interaction of temperament and en- mothering in a snack situation and by mother report.
vironment in the context of the main effects, and not all Poor child self-regulation predicted mother-reported ex-
control for these effects by entering an interaction term ternalizing behavior problems at age 4 to a greater extent
in a multiple regression equation following entry of the for children who at age 2 received higher levels of intru-
main effects terms. We concentrate more on the sub- sive and hostile mothering. This pattern was found
stantive patterns of results than on methodological fea- cross-sectionally at age 2 in the same study, but only for
tures (see Bates & McFadyen-Ketchum, 2001, for more boys (Rubin, Hastings, Chen, Stewart, & McNichol,
discussion of methods). 1998).
Most of the emerging literature concerns three kinds Other cross-sectional examples of the pattern in-
of temperament trait: Those related to (a) lack of self- clude the finding that positive parenting as measured
regulation, including low effortful control, unmanage- by interview and incidental observations mattered
ability, and resistance to control, and probably related to more for temperamentally unmanageable childrens
the Big Five personality dimensions of agreeableness preschool adjustment than for less resistant children
and conscientiousness; ( b) negative emotional reactiv- (Bates, Viken, & Williams, 2003). Morris et al. (2002)
ity, sometimes called difficult temperament; and (c) found that children rated by their mothers as low in ef-
novelty distress, fear, or unadaptability. Depending on fortful control (CBQ) showed an especially strong rela-
how they are assessed, these constructs often partially tionship between mother hostility (child report) and
144 Temperament

teacher-reported externalizing behavior, whereas Pat- from elementary school to middle school) than boys who
terson and Sanson (1999) reported that mother-rated were low or medium in their unmanageability.
temperamental inflexibility (negative emotionality and Bates et al. (1998) found, in two separate studies
resistance to demands) was more strongly associated with community samples, that early childhood tem-
with mother-reported externalizing problems when the peramental resistance to control (mother report on the
mothers described themselves as relatively high in ICQ) better predicted externalizing behavior problems
harsh punishment. in middle childhood (mother and teacher reports) for
The general pattern also extends to a prosocial behav- children who received low levels of parental control
iorexpression of sympathy. Valiente et al. (2004) (observed in the home) than for children receiving high
found that parents expressivity of negative emotion levels of control. Parental control was measured as re-
(self-rated and observed) was associated with childrens actions to misbehavior, and these reactions were some-
self-reported sympathetic responses, but only when the times but not always negative, for example, scolding.
child was high in effortful control (parent and teacher The researchers almost never saw harsh discipline such
ratings on the CBQ and observation). This was true for as spanking. Although hostile parenting and lack of
self-rated general dispositions and personal distress re- warmth might well make it more likely that childrens
sponses to an empathy-inducing film, but not for sympa- dysregulated temperament traits will become acting-
thy responses to the film. In a sample of children who out behavior problems, the findings of Stice and Gon-
had experienced divorce, inconsistent maternal disci- zales (1998) and Bates et al. (1998) suggest that
pline (mother and child report) was more strongly asso- parental control might also serve to lessen the likeli-
ciated with both depression and conduct problems in hood that dysregulated temperament will lead to prob-
children who had impulsive temperaments (mother and lem behavior if unmanageability is overcome by high
child report on the CBQ) than for those who were not levels of parental management effort. However, high
impulsive (Lengua et al., 2000). Finally, elementary levels of parental control may not be ideal for all chil-
school childrens self-described externalizing behavior dren: Bates et al. (1998) also noted that high levels of
problems were more strongly associated with angry dis- maternal control with highly manageable children
cipline by both mothers and fathers, when the children sometimes resulted in higher levels of externalizing be-
described themselves as low rather than high in agree- havior than would have been predicted by temperament
ableness (Prinzie et al., 2003). This pattern was essen- alone, with the possibility that the mothers control
tially replicated by Van Leeuwen, Mervielde, Braet, and somehow prevented the development of truly internal-
Bosmans (2004) using parent ratings of child personal- ized self-control.
ity and adjustment and both parent and child descrip- A third theme concerns a somewhat different trait,
tions of parenting. not discussed in the main temperament literature, the
A second theme that has emerged in the literature is core psychopathy trait, callous-unemotional, which in-
supported by fewer studies, but it raises an important volves tendencies to be nonempathic, manipulative, and
possibilitythat disciplinary responses by parents can lacking anxiety and guilt. This pattern seems likely to
have positive rather than adverse implications for chil- be a form of temperamental dysregulation, even though
dren with temperamental or personality tendencies to- its regulatory core appears to concern low prosocial
ward dysregulation. Stice and Gonzales (1998) found orientation more than reward-sensitive impulsivity or
that adolescents ratings of parental control and support low effortful control. It may also be related to very low
were positively correlated and that, for highly impulsive levels of fear, but this does not seem likely to be the
youths, high levels of parental control and support were dominant component. Wooten, Frick, Shelton, and Sil-
more associated with low levels of adolescent antisocial verthorn (1997) found in a combined clinical and nor-
behavior than for nonimpulsive youths. Even more mal sample that when children were described by
clearly showing that control can be especially effective parents and teachers as low on the callous-unemotional
for dysregulated children, Stoolmiller (2001) found that scale, less positive parenting, as described by parent
boys who were highly unmanageable (disposed to have and child, was associated with greater conduct prob-
tantrums) in their early years showed a stronger rela- lems as measured by parent and teacher report. When
tionship between maternal unskilled discipline and in- high on the callous-unemotional scale, however, chil-
creasing externalizing problems (as rated by teachers dren were high on conduct problems whether the par-
Temperament and Adjustment 145

enting was positive or not. This pattern was replicated, ing behavior problems (teen and parent report) than did
in essence, by B. OConnor and Dvorak (2001) in a teens with nonimpulsive tendencies.
community sample, and by Oxford, Cavell, and Hughes Considering studies with nonstandard measures of
(2003) in a sample more similar to that of Wooten et al. temperament-related traits, El-Sheikh et al. (2001)
(1997). found that high vagal tone (a measure of self-regulation
The findings of regulatory temperament parent- via the parasympathetic nervous system, described ear-
ing interactions are interesting, and it is encouraging lier) reduced the risks of externalizing and internalizing
that some common patterns of findings have been behavior and health problems for children exposed to
found that can be interpreted as reflecting differential frequent marital quarrels. Although not showing an in-
effects of parenting on children with different tem- teraction as such, Donzella et al. (2000) put preschool
peraments. However, especially when interaction ef- children in a competitive game and found that those who
fects are not found, the studies cannot provide showed a cortisol increase, as opposed to those who did
sufficient evidence on developmental process. The not, were described by teachers on the CBQ as high in
parent and child are also genetically related, and inter- surgency and low in effortful control. Booth, Johnson,
action effects might be confounded or obscured by Granger, Crouter, and McHale (2003) measured testos-
gene-environment correlation. This makes it valuable terone, which could indicate temperament-like disposi-
to have relevant findings from studies considering tions toward surgency, in children and adolescents.
variables other than standard temperament /personal- Testosterone levels were not directly related to adjust-
ity, behavioral adjustment, and parenting. One exam- ment, but when quality of mother-child and father-child
ple is the Hart, Atkins, and Fegley (2003) study, which relationship (parent- and child-rated) was low, higher
shows, among other things, that Head Start experience levels of testosterone were associated with higher levels
was especially beneficial in developing academic of conduct problems and lower levels of depression.
skills for children with resilient (well-regulated) per- These relationships were lower and opposite in direction
sonalities living in highly stressful family environ- when relationship quality was high.
ments. Bates et al. (2003) found that disrupted sleep Finally, we consider a study that measured particu-
schedules (mother daily report) and lack of positive lar genes with relevance to individual differences.
parenting (mother interview) had stronger paths to The Dunedin study of a birth cohort in New Zealand
childrens maladjustment in preschool (teacher re- (Caspi et al., 2002) showed that boys (and in supple-
ports) for children who were high in resistance to con- mental analyses, girls) who had a less active allele for
trol (mother report on the ICQ). MAOA (a gene on the X chromosome coding for an
Lengua and Long (2002) reported that children in a enzyme that metabolizes neurotransmitters such as
community sample showed a stronger association be- dopamine and inf luences aggressive behavior) showed
tween family stress and internalizing behavior problems a stronger relationship between the experience of ad-
when the children were low in self-regulation (on the verse experiences in the family ( harsh and indifferent
CBQ and Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire parenting, loss of primary caregiver, and retrospec-
[EATQ]), all measures based on child and mother re- tively reported abuse) and later antisocial behavior.
ports. Fabes et al. (1999) found that preschool children The study (Caspi, Sugden, et al., 2003) also showed
whose teachers rated them high in effortful control that those with two copies of the short allele of the 5-
(CBQ) had an adaptive advantage compared to less self- HTT gene (a gene that inf luences the efficiency of
regulated children, with lower levels of negative emotion serotonin functioning in response to stress and conse-
and higher competence in peer interactions, but only in quently anxiety responses) had a stronger relationship
high intensity interactions (vigorous, highly emotional between the number of stressful life events they had
games). Effortful control did not make much difference experienced from age 21 to 26 years and their level of
when the peer interaction events were of lower intensity. depression than those with a short and a long or two
Finally, Goodnight, Bates, Newman, Dodge, and Pettit copies of the long allele of the gene. Both of these
(2004) found that teens, especially boys, with impulsive genes can be seen as affecting emotional and behav-
tendencies in a laboratory card-playing task, showed a ioral self-regulation traits of individuals, and both
stronger linkage between having antisocial friends (teen showed interactions with stressful experience in pre-
and parent report) and increases in their own externaliz- dicting adjustment outcomes.
146 Temperament

Summary of externalizing behavior at age 3 for boys who as in-


In summary, research has begun to demonstrate that fants were high rather than low in negative emotionality
child characteristics related to the temperament domain (parent IBQ ratings and lab observation of both frustra-
of dysregulation interact with a range of environmental tion and fear). The Belsky et al. (1998) study also pro-
qualities in the development of competencies and prob- vides an important illustration of the fact that although
lems. In general, negative experiences and the absence externalizing problems are often found to be substan-
of positive experiences appear to have less adverse ef- tially correlated with internalizing problems in children,
fects on the development of children with stronger self- the two kinds of problems may have different an-
regulatory tendencies, and greater effects on the tecedents (Bates, 1989a). Belsky et al. (1998) found that
development of children with weaker self-regulation. temperamentally negative infants who received more
Ten years ago, this pattern was essentially undiscovered. negative and less positive fathering were less inhibited
Now, after an inspiring flurry of scientific activity, we in the laboratory at 3 years, whereas the relation be-
can begin to envision research on the actual develop- tween fathering and inhibition was nonsignificant for in-
mental processes by which temperament and environ- fants low in negative emotionality.
ment moderate one anothers effects on child The Belsky et al. (1998) finding resembles a finding
adjustment. What are the limits of the phenomena? More of Arcus (2001) in which infants observed in the labora-
precisely, which environmental factors interact with tory to be high in negative reactivity were less likely to
which particular child characteristics? What are the de- show behavioral inhibition in the laboratory at 14
velopmental processes by which these effects are found? months if their mothers were observed to be high in
What are the psychological products of the temperament limit setting. Arcus also found that negatively reactive
and environment? In the next subsections, we review infants with boisterous and annoying siblings were less
comparable literatures on interactions involving nega- likely to show behavioral inhibition than those experi-
tive emotional reactivity and novelty distress traits, and encing less intrusive behavior from siblings, even be-
features of the environment. yond the effect of maternal limit setting. Arcus suggests
that mild frustrations and challenges, such as those from
firm mothers and intrusive siblings, may promote reac-
Negative Emotionality Environment tive infants self-regulatory abilities better than a highly
accommodating environment.
Negative emotionality traits, which tend to predict both In contrast, a study by Pauli-Pott, Mertesacker, and
internalizing and externalizing adjustment outcomes, Beckman (2004) appears to find, at least partially, the
also interact with environment characteristics in shaping opposite of the Arcus pattern: Maternal insensitivity at
the development of childrens adjustment. To an even 4 months predicted infant stranger distress at 12 months
greater extent than the preceding section, this section is more strongly for infants who had been high rather than
not conceptually pure: Temperament measures classi- low in negative emotionality in the lab at 4 months. The
fied as negative emotionality often include other tem- discrepancy could be due to many different aspects of
perament constructs that might better be treated sample and procedure, but the difference may be related
separately. For example, sometimes a negative emotion- to: (a) slight but potentially important developments
ality measure may reflect not only the general negative taking place between 12 and 14 months, ( b) differences
reactivity but also the correlated, yet conceptually sepa- in the meaning of an assessment battery confronting the
rable, discomfort in a novel situation. Or a measure child with a wide variety of novel objects versus one
might combine negative emotionality and poor self- centered on only a strange person, or less likely (c) sub-
regulation. Nevertheless, for the sake of simpler organi- tle cultural differences between the United States and
zation, studies with such measures are placed here. Germany. Paralleling the Pauli-Pott et al. (2004) find-
The majority of studies in this section concern nega- ings, although with more of an externalizing outcome,
tive emotionality in interaction with measures of parent- Feldman, Greenbaum, and Yirmiya (1999) found that
ing, consistent with the strong emphasis on parenting in mother-infant affect synchrony in play during the first
the social development literature. Belsky, Hsieh, and year, likely related to maternal sensitivity, was more
Crnic (1998) found that intrusive and negatively affec- predictive of toddlers self-control (compliance with do
tive parenting during toddlerhood was more predictive and dont commands) observed at age 2 years when the
Temperament and Adjustment 147

infants had been high rather than low on negative emo- adopted childrens biological mothers negative emo-
tionality (mother ICQ and lab observations). tionality (EAS) predicted the childrens behavior prob-
Related patterns have also been found with older chil- lems at age 12, but only if the childrens adoptive
dren. Morris et al. (2002) reported that mothers overt parents had separated. This effect was statistically sig-
hostility (child-rated) showed stronger associations with nificant for parent reports of externalizing and internal-
teacher reports of externalizing behavior problems for izing problems, in the same direction but not significant
children temperamentally high in anger to frustration for teacher reported problems, and significant for ob-
(mother CBQ) than for less irritable children. More irri- server ratings of low social responsibility. In a different
table children also showed a stronger relation between adoption study, Stams, Juffer, and van Ijzendoorn
child-rated maternal covert hostility and intrusive con- (2002) found that adopted children with the combina-
trol of the childs feelings and teacher reports of chil- tion of disorganized attachment and challenging tem-
drens internalizing. In a similar effect, high levels of perament (composite of all ICQ factors) in the early
mother-reported family conflict were associated with years showed lower levels of cognitive development and
high levels of teacher-reported externalizing and inter- less optimal ego control at age 7 than those with none or
nalizing problems, but only for children rated by teach- only one of these risk factors. Stams et al. (2002) inter-
ers on the Keogh Teacher Temperament Questionnaire preted the disorganized attachment as reflecting a less
as high on a composite of negative emotionality and dys- optimal relationship with the mother, and this is plausi-
regulation (Tschann, Kaiser, Chesney, Alkon, & Boyce, ble. However, the meaning of the disorganized attach-
1996). Also showing how difficult temperament might ment construct is not well developed (Thompson, 1998).
change the implications of stressful environments are In addition to stressful or abusive rearing conditions
findings of Kilmer, Cowan, and Wyman (2001). (Thompson, 1998), it has sometimes also been inter-
Further supporting the general trend of negativity preted as due to infants neural development having
family environment effects, Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, been compromised (Green & Goldwyn, 2002). There-
Schonberg, and Lukon (2002) reported that toddlers fore, the temperament environment interpretation in
negative emotionality (ICQ difficultness) was more pre- this instance needs to be provisional.
dictive of relatively ineffective self-regulation in a gift- Adjustment is typically measured at a point in time,
delay task 2 years later when their mothers had been but increasingly as trajectories across time. Owens and
observed to be high in negative control and low in Shaw (2003) provide a rare example of an interaction be-
warmth than when the mothers had been low in negative tween child temperament and rearing environment in
control and high in warmth. Hemphill and Sanson forecasting change in adjustment. They showed that the
(2001), in a preliminary report, described a related ef- expected decline in externalizing behavior across age 2
fect. Lengua et al. (2000) did not find an interaction be- to age 6 (e.g., Keiley, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, 2000) was
tween negative emotionality (mother and child reports slower for children who had been observed at 18 months
on the EAS) and parenting. They did, however, find that to be high on negative emotionality and who had moth-
low positive emotionality (mother and child reports on ers with high levels of depression, than for children with
the DOTS) was associated with stronger relations be- depressed mothers but lower levels of negative emotion-
tween maternal rejection (ratings by mother and child) ality. This is consistent with the general trend emerging
and child externalizing behavior and depression (mother in the literature for negative emotionality to amplify en-
and child ratings). The latter finding is described here vironmental risk factors for behavior problem develop-
because it is one of very few interactions reported be- ment. However, to enrich the picture, Owens and Shaw
tween positive emotionality and environment in relation (2003) also found that with mothers who had been low
to child adjustment. in depression across early childhood, children observed
Returning to interactions involving negative emo- to be highly negative at 18 months showed a slightly
tionality, if one assumes that a mothers negative emo- steeper decline in externalizing behavior problems than
tionality is genetically transmitted to her child, the low negative children. At age 2, the high negative chil-
findings of T. OConnor, Caspi, DeFries, and Plomin dren with nondepressed mothers had about 20% more
(2003) confirm the general finding that negative emo- symptoms than low negative emotionality children with
tionality predisposes a child to be more susceptible to nondepressed mothers, but by age 6, they were practi-
negative rearing conditions. OConnor et al. found that cally as low in symptoms as the low negative children.
148 Temperament

Wills et al. (2001) provide another example of memory task when the interviewer was supportive than
temperament-environment interaction forecasting when she was unsupportive. For low-reactive children,
growth in problem behavior. Because of the nature of however, interviewer supportiveness did not make a dif-
the temperament composites, this study could also have ference in recall, although the low-reactive children did
been reviewed in the self-regulation environment sec- offer more dont know answers to a supportive than to
tion. In a large sample of youths followed from age 11 to an unsupportive interviewer.
13, Wills et al. (2001) found a pattern in which negative
temperament (negative emotionality plus high activity Summary
level) amplified relations between parenting risk fac- In summary, although explicit replications are largely
tors, such as parent-child conflict and parent tobacco lacking and even approximate replications are some-
and alcohol use, and both intercept level and slope of times not found, there appear to be some general patterns
youths substance use. The reverse applied to positive in the findings reviewed. First, negative emotionality
temperament (positive emotionality with task orienta- traits tend to foreshadow externalizing behavior prob-
tion): High positive temperament reduced the relations lems, but to be more strongly linked to externalizing be-
between parenting risk factors and the intercept and havior problems in the presence of adverse rearing
slope of youths substance use. These findings obtained environments. Conversely, environmental adversities
for both self- and teacher-report measures of tempera- tend to predict child behavior problems more strongly in
ment. The findings are comparable to those of Mun et al. the presence of negative child temperament. Perhaps we
(2001), who found stronger links between some pre- can adapt coercion training theory (Patterson, Reid, &
school-age temperament variables (parent report on the Dishion, 1992) to envision a Velcro process by which
DOTS) and parent reports of middle childhood behavior this interaction effect operates so that negatively emo-
problems when the parents were high in alcohol and tional children tend to acquire coercive tendencies more
antisocial behavior problems than when they reported easily than less irritable children because they have the
low levels of these problems. Although activity level and relevant hooks. These coercive tendencies are espe-
distractibility showed this moderator effect, emotional cially established when there are environmental triggers
reactivity and unadaptability did not. and responses to this irritability, such as a hostile par-
Finally, we consider two studies using biological ent. And these behavioral and emotional habits become
measures and interactions between temperament-like dysfunctionally habitual and generalized.
characteristics and nonfamily environmental factors. This complements an older, fairly well-established
Dettling et al. (2000) asked how diurnal patterns of cor- pattern of findings (Bates, 1989a), showing that more
tisol in response to the stresses of day care were af- difficult infants develop less well cognitively in noisy
fected by temperament. When at home, childrens homes than do easy infants (e.g., Wachs, 1987). How-
cortisol levels peak in the early morning and decline ever, in the latter findings, strong emotional responses
over the day, but children in day care often show in- may impair the childs ability to extract meaningful in-
creases from morning to afternoon. Dettling et al. formation in a confusing environment rather than to se-
(2000) found that day-care children higher on a compos- lectively respond to and elicit negativity from the social
ite parent- and day-care teacher-report (CBQ) measure environment. Second, negative emotionality often fore-
of negative affectivity and low effortful control showed shadows internalizing problems, but environmental
a greater increase in afternoon cortisol than those lower adversities may play a different role here, with moder-
on this temperament composite, even after controlling ately challenging, directive and unsupportive behavior
for quality of day care the children received. Finally, a reducing the chances of development of anxious behav-
study by Quas, Bauer, and Boyce (2004) concerned psy- ior patterns. Arcuss (2001) suggested process makes
chophysiological reactivity measured in the laboratory sense here, with moderate challenge forcing an other-
in 4- to 6-year-olds, which we interpret as a tempera- wise withdrawing child to develop more adaptive regu-
ment index. The environmental variable was whether an latory abilities.
interviewer took a supportive or nonsupportive approach As in the self-regulation environment area, empiri-
to the child, and the adjustment variable was the cal progress in describing negative emotionality envi-
childs performance in recalling episodes that occurred ronment moderator effects has been striking. However,
in the lab. Quas et al. (2004) found that children high on much work remains to be done. More consistent and pre-
autonomic reactivity gave more correct answers in the cise operational definitions of negative emotionality will
Temperament and Adjustment 149

be important in clarifying which aspects of temperament years as the other two topics we have reviewed. How-
are interacting with the environment. As always, defini- ever, there has been progress.
tion of the crucial aspects of the environment would help Kochanska (1991) showed that highly fearful 8- to
as well (Wachs, 2000). As patterns of interaction become 10-year-old children showed more signs of conscience
more firmly established, it will also be important to when their mothers used gentle rather than harsh con-
identify the developmental processes by which an inter- trol, whereas the gentleness of maternal control did not
action effect is mediated. Finally, although not all arti- make a difference for the relatively fearless children.
cles mentioning the search for interactions found them, This finding was consistent with a theoretically based
and not all of those reported follow the same pattern, assumption that highly anxious children are susceptible
consistent-enough patterns of moderator effects have to overarousal, impeding their cognitive processing and
been reported in recent years that it seems unlikely that internalizing of rules in harsh discipline encounters,
file drawer nonreplications will swamp the published whereas fearless children are not as susceptible to over-
effects. Nevertheless, it might also be helpful if re- arousal. More recently, this finding has been supported
searchers were to reserve a portion of their work for ex- with a slightly older sample of boys, by Colder,
plicit attempts to replicate intriguing interaction effects, Lochman, and Wells (1997). Temperamentally fearful
and if journals reserved space for notes on the success or children whose parents used harsh discipline showed
failure of these attempts. It will be theoretically valuable more teacher-rated aggression than either low-fear chil-
to identify not only consistently found moderator effects, dren with harsh parents or high-fear children with gen-
but also those that are consistently not found, despite tle parents.
methodologically plausible attempts. Kochanska (1995) replicated and extended this find-
ing in a study of younger children. She reported that
gentle discipline mattered more in the self-control of
Fearfulness Environment
children above the median on novelty fear than for the
Our final section on temperament environment inter- children below the median on fear. Moreover, and very
action concerns temperamental fearfulness. Tempera- importantly, she also reported that a positive mother-
ment constructs in this domain describe distress and child relationship, indexed by the attachment Q-sort,
withdrawal or slow adaptation to novel or potentially mattered more in the self-control of relatively fearless
harmful situations. Theoretically, as discussed earlier, children than for the relatively fearful ones. The latter
they are rooted in individual variations in brain cir- finding was predicted by a model assuming that fear-
cuits, especially those comprising the fear system. Em- less children could be more easily motivated to acquire
pirically, there is evidence that highly inhibited, social rules by positive and enjoyable aspects of the
fearful, unadaptable young children are sometimes at parent-child relationship. Kochanska (1997) also ex-
greater risk of developing anxiety problems, and that tended the pattern of findings by following the toddlers
unusually fearless young children are at greater risk of in her 1995 study at two further time points. Tempera-
developing conduct problems, as discussed. In studies mental fearfulness was still measured at Time 1 (aver-
of the interaction of fearful temperament and environ- age age 33 months) by a composite of mother reports
ment, the temperament measures have been more typi- on the CBQ and observations in standard laboratory
cally pure representatives of the focal concept than in challenge situations, and maternal discipline was mea-
the other kinds of temperament-environment inter- sured by observations in toy clean-up situations at
action we have reviewed. However, even here there re- home and in the lab. At Time 2 (average age 46
main some methodological uncertainties. For example, months), Kochanska (1997) found that relatively fear-
measures of low levels of behavioral inhibition could re- ful childrens conscience (resistance to temptation to
flect not only the primary construct, lack of fear, but cheat in a game and responses to hypothetical moral
also high approach or low levels of self-regulation. dilemmas) was more advanced if their mothers had
Fearful temperament was the topic for which there was been gentle in their control rather than harsh.
the most compelling evidence for a temperament- Relatively fearless childrens conscience was not de-
parenting interaction in the development of childrens pendent on gentle versus harsh discipline, but instead
adjustment at the time of Rothbart and Bates (1998) was predicted by how securely attached they had been.
original review. In sheer numbers of relevant studies, At Time 3 (age 60 months), however, Time 1 gentle con-
this literature has not grown as fast in the intervening trol did not matter for the conscience of fearful children,
150 Temperament

and Time 1 attachment security did not matter for the Next, we ask how fearful temperament interacts with
fearless children, although Time 1 maternal responsive- rearing environment in the prediction of more standard
ness, which is conceptually and empirically related to behavior problem symptoms. First, considering possible
attachment security, did predict conscience at Time 3 moderator processes in development of internalizing be-
for fearless children. Interestingly, Fowles and Kochan- havior, Tschann et al. (1996) found that preschool chil-
ska (2000) found, in the same sample, that when fearful- dren relatively low on approach, as rated by their
ness was defined by electrodermal reactivity, the teachers on Keoghs questionnaire, were more likely to
pattern of findings was fairly similar to those based on be observed as socially withdrawn when their mothers
the behavioral definition of temperament, even though described low levels of family conflict than when their
there was little convergence between the two measures. mothers described high levels of family conflict. High-
Independent replications of the Kochanska tempera- approach children, alternatively, showed the lowest lev-
ment parenting interaction effects in predicting els of social withdrawal when their families were low in
indexes of moral development were attempted by conflict. This is conceptually similar to the effect previ-
van der Mark, Bakermans-Kranenburg, and van Ijzen- ously discussed, where negative emotionality predicted
doorn (2002) and van der Mark, van Ijzendoorn, and behavioral inhibition less when parents were more rather
Bakermans-Kranenburg (2002) in a study of girls at 16 than less directive or challenging, although the effect is
and 22 months. Fearfulness was assessed in the labora- concurrent rather than longitudinal, as in the Arcus
tory and committed compliance and empathy responses (2001) and Park, Belsky, Putnam, and Crnic (1997)
were the measures of moral development. However, van studies. Bates (2003) presented preliminary analyses
der Mark et al. did not find temperament-parenting in- from two longitudinal studies that partly converge with
teraction effects. These nonreplications may have been this effect: Mother-rated temperamental unadaptability
due to the very young age of the children in the study, (ICQ) in early childhood predicted mother-rated inter-
to the fact that the sample was restricted to girls and nalizing problems across middle childhood more
relatively high in socioeconomic status, or to a variety strongly in families where the mothers had been ob-
of other method differences. Although not explicitly at- served to be low in control than where they had been ob-
tempting to replicate the Kochanska (1997) pattern, served to be more highly directive and restrictive.
findings in a preliminary report by Hemphill and San- In partial contrast, however, a rather different kind of
son (2001) could be interpreted as showing that highly temperament parenting interaction was found by
punitive parenting appeared to amplify a small group Rubin, Burgess, and Hastings (2002). Rubin et al. found
of uninhibited childrens risk for externalizing prob- that inhibited tendencies with a peer observed at age 2
lems, in potential contrast with Kochanskas (1997) predicted social reticence with unfamiliar peers at age
finding that harsh control was not predictive of con- 4, but only for children whose mothers were observed at
science development for uninhibited children. Maternal age 2 to be high in psychological controlderisive or in-
punitiveness at age 2 was not different for the groups of trusive with affection or help on a task. Inhibition at age
children moderate or high on inhibition and with or 2 was not predictive of social inhibition at age 4 for chil-
without behavior problems. dren who received less psychological control from their
The accumulating literature provides both replica- mothers. A possible key to reconciling the earlier stud-
tions and nonreplications of the rich fear parenting ies is to note that Rubin et al. (2002) may have captured
pattern. The meaning of the nonreplications is not estab- dimensions of parenting that either underchallenged or
lished, because key methodological issues are not re- overchallenged children with fearful temperament,
solved. On balance, the fear parenting effect is well whereas the Arcus (2001) and Park et al. (1997; or Bel-
replicated for such a complex pattern, and it is poten- sky et al., 1998) and Bates (2003) studies may have mea-
tially of considerable theoretical importance. However, sured parenting dimensions more consistent with
further replications, using a variety of methods, will be Arcuss (2001) model of optimal challenge accelerating
needed for the pattern to become a solidly established self-regulation of anxiety responses. This interpretation
developmental phenomenon. And, despite the exception- is consistent with the more clinical insights of Chess and
ally well-developed theoretical background for the pat- Thomas (1984), who observed in their longitudinal
tern, studies will also be needed to identify the study that withdrawing children developed best when
processes mediating the observed interaction effect. parents provided repeated and firm, but not overwhelm-
Temperament and Adjustment 151

ing, challenges to their children to deal with novel situa- genetic risk of anxiety and exposure to stressful events
tions, with overprotectiveness not as beneficial. are more sensitive to the depressogenic life events. This
Finally, Eaves et al. (2003) offer an interesting per- study is only indirectly relevant to temperament per se,
spective on the question of how gene-environment inter- but it is exciting to consider how studies with early mea-
action and gene-environment correlation can be sures of temperament and environment in a genetically
simultaneously modeled in a twin study. This is of im- informative sample might help advance our understand-
portance to the study of temperament environment in- ing of the processes involved in the development of chil-
teractions because we assume that some portion of drens adjustment.
temperament is based on the genome. One important
window on processes where genes and temperament- Summary and Future Directions
relevant expressions of genes influence psychological To conclude this section, it is greatly encouraging to
outcomes is gene-environment interaction. These inter- have seen such a rapid accumulation of temperament-
actions have become of great interest in developmental environment interaction findings. The field has gone
psychopathology recently, as evidenced by the frequent from mostly thinking about complex processes in theo-
mention of the findings by Caspi et al. (2002; Caspi, retical writings to vigorously instantiating such effects
Sugden, et al., 2003) described previously. Children of in empirical work. As we have shown, there are several
contrasting genotype might be differently affected by a patterns with some broadly converging support. As
similar environment. usual in our complex field, these replications are far
However, equally important for understanding from exact or widespread, but some patterns are begin-
developmental process is the phenomenon of gene- ning to stand out, and they are not always patterns that
environment correlation. A child with a given genotype would have been intuitively expected. As mentioned
might be exposed to a particular environment associ- previously, methodological and definitional issues are
ated with the development of behavior problems be- important in replication studies, and more attention
cause close relatives share the same genotype, or a needs to be allocated to such studies. In addition, much
childs genotype might lead to temperament traits that work remains in detailing the mediating processes by
elicit environmental responses that, in turn, promote which temperament-environment interactions have their
the development of behavior problems. Interpreting role in development.
gene-environment interaction effects in human studies It also may be valuable to more extensively explore in-
is often difficult because it is difficult to assume that teractions between multiple temperament variables and
the environment dimension is not at the same time also environmental variables simultaneously. As of about 10
a function of the genotype. In the past, this issue was years ago, the typical limit of complexity was to consider
dealt with by only evaluating gene-environment inter- temperament and environment variables main effects as
action effects where it could be demonstrated that linear, additive contributors to a developmental outcome.
there was no correlation between environment and gene Currently, the typical limit considers main effects plus
(e.g., Caspi et al., 2002). Eaves et al. (2003), however, the interaction of one temperament variable and one en-
developed a Bayesian approach to simultaneously vironment variable as predictors of an outcome, or main
model gene main effects, gene-environment interaction effects plus the interaction of two temperament variables
effects, and gene-environment correlation effects. (e.g., negative emotionality effortful control) as pre-
Using this approach in a longitudinal study of adoles- dictors. However, for reasons suggested earlier, where
cent twin girls, Eaves et al. (2003) found that genes ex- the meaning of a given temperament variable in isolation
plained the development of depression in several paths. is not always clear, it might be helpful to consider the ef-
Some genes influenced depression specifically. Other fect of a profile of temperament variables as moderating
genes affected anxiety early on, and then depression or moderated by an environmental variable (see also
later, through three pathways: (1) a genetic main effect Rothbart & Sheese, in press).
in which girls early anxiety increased their risk of later For example, fearful temperament might have differ-
anxiety, (2) a gene-environment correlation in which ent implications depending on both its temperament
girls at high risk for anxiety were especially likely to be context, such as the tendency toward dysregulation, and
exposed to depressogenic life events, and (3) a gene- its environmental context such as family stress. Not co-
environment interaction in which girls with this higher incidentally, this example is a direction we have been
152 Temperament

exploring (Bates, Sandy, Pettit, & Dodge, 2000), and we Further evidence links anger and early surgency/extra-
hope to have useful findings to offer in the next edition version to the development of externalizing problems and
of this handbook. Similarly, profiles of environments, indicates that fear may be a protective factor against ag-
such as parental harsh discipline in the context of warm gression and other externalizing problems as well as a
involvement versus minimal involvement, might also be contributor to the early development of conscience. A
important in understanding the interactions with tem- great deal of recent research has established connections
perament. However, in a chapter on temperament, we between effortful control and the regulation of both affect
should not go further in thinking about this direction. and behavior. Future research will consider the limits of
Future progress on understanding the role of tempera- fearful and effortful control on adaptation, in connection
ment will require both relatively mundane replications with the Blocks (1980) construct of overcontrol, and
and new, more exciting, highly complex studies. allow us to study the way in which effortful control may
become part of a resilient approach to lifes challenges.
Probably the most striking new findings in this re-
CHAPTER CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE view involve temperament environment interactions.
DIRECTIONS In interaction studies, the childs effortful control, man-
ageability, and agreeableness have been found to moder-
Over the past 2 decades, there has been considerable ate the effects of adverse environments, and negative
progress in identifying the broad outlines and the more emotionality has been found to amplify the effects of
specific dimensions of temperament in childhood. The adverse experience. Unexpectedly, we have found that
general framework for temperament now constitutes a re- more fearful or inhibited children appear to benefit
vision of the NYLS dimensions, and includes broad di- from early challenge, at least in measures of the later
mensions of Positive Affect and Approach, Negative strength of this system. However, fearful or inhibited
Affectivity, including subconstructs of Irritability and preschoolers conscience appears to develop better in
Fear, Effortful Control, and possibly Affiliativeness or the context of gentle socialization methods.
Social Orientation. These broad dimensions share simi-
larities with four of the Big Five Factors of Personality Measures
(Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and
Good measures of temperament are crucial to our theo-
Agreeableness), and with all of the Big Three broad fac-
retical understanding. Further advances in defining the
tors of personality (Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Con-
structure of temperament and understanding the neural
scientiousness), but they are by no means identical.
and developmental substrates of temperament will
Research establishing linkages between measures of
continue to rely on advances in measurement. As an ad-
these dimensions of early temperament and later person-
ditional goal of research, we advocate the further devel-
ality has now begun to accumulate, and will continue to
opment of sound measures, using parent-report,
be one of the major continuing tasks for our area, as will
naturalistic observation, and structured or laboratory
further differentiating our temperament and personality
observation measures to be used in converging and com-
measures.
plementary ways. We have advocated an analog ap-
proach to questions of validation rather than a digital,
Differentiating Temperament Dimensions yes-no approach to ascertaining the value of methods
and measures. Aside from the important future work of
In our previous review, we suggested the importance of comparing results of alternative methods, another im-
differentiating between fearful and irritable distress, portant focus in research should be identification of
and in this review we note that both biological and clini- non-relationships among constructstests for discrim-
cal studies have benefited from this distinction. Find- inant as well as convergent validity. Partly on the basis
ings on the psychobiology of temperament showing of differential, discriminating patterns of correlations
overlap between networks subserving defensive fear and between parent-reports of temperament and other mea-
defensive aggression may help to account for findings of sures, we are able to argue for the validity of parent-
general neurotic tendencies, and they may also suggest reports. The use of brain marker tasks in the study of
further means of differentiating reactive and instrumen- development of executive attention and effortful control
tal aggression in relation to temperament. has made significant strides in the past 5 years. We en-
References 153

courage the continued use of marker tasks to link per- study of individual differences, cross-cultural, social
formance to the development of brain functioning. learning, and social cognition.
Developmental research in our area may also eventu-
ally answer questions like the following: To what degree is
Development temperament plastic and susceptible to change? To what
degree does experience alter only the expression of tem-
As the dimensions of temperament have been further de-
peramental characteristics? If distress and maladaptive
lineated and measures improved, real advances have oc-
social cognitions can result from a painful life history,
curred in our understanding of temperament-environment
how much of early temperament may have been overlain
interactions. Future research is needed to examine the
by these negative experiences? Could the original core of
processes supporting these effects. There may be times
temperament be uncovered by imaginative assays, inter-
when emotionality or effortful control systems are more
vention, further social experience, or even by further
sensitive to environmental conditions than others, or
changes in social or physical development? We know
times when the childs irritable and frustrative distress
someone who, through the aging process, lost many of her
might be most easily directed toward or away from coer-
memories, including information that had troubled her
cive responses and tendencies to aggressive action. These
over many years and led to major conflicts in herself and
are basic developmental questions with profound implica-
with others. What remained after her memory loss was a
tions for our understanding of the nature of temperament
positive and expressive person, loved by all who met her.
and the development of personality.
Was this the child she once was? If so, could other less se-
Establishing closer links with our understanding of
rious interventions have uncovered it? Better yet, could
the developing neurophysiological substrate of tempera-
developmental research inform both child rearing and
ment is a related task for our area. In this work, find-
childrens prospects in society so that the accumulating
ings from each domain of study will illuminate the
pain might never have occurred? We have made much
other. Thus, behavioral research on the developing
progress in our field in the past decades, but a number of
structure of temperament helps to specify the opera-
questions remain. Many of these questions are hopeful
tions necessary to link the psychology of temperament
about a future for us, our parents, and our children.
to its neurophysiology. Reviewers who relate parallel re-
search carried out in these two domains will help in this
work. The use of physiological assays, behavioral mea-
sures in research designs and the use of marker tasks REFERENCES
will lead to further advances.
Finally, we have identified possible trajectories in the Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J.,
development of social and personality traits from early Phelps, E. A., et al. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine
temperamental characteristics, most strongly in individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia,
37, 11111117.
Kochanskas (1995) work on multiple routes to con-
Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development
science. The task of identifying routes to other signifi- and the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P.
cant outcomes requires progress in all of the tasks Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and per-
described earlier, and it is of critical importance to our sonality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 189207). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
enterprise. The study of developmental trajectories re-
Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Childrens tempera-
quires establishing stronger links between our work and ment in the United States and China: Similarities and differences.
more environmentally oriented areas of our field such as European Journal of Personality, 7, 359378.
social learning and social cognition research. As we Aksan, N., & Kochanska, G. (2004a). Heterogeneity of joy in infancy.
Infancy 6(1), 7994.
have indicated, temperament constructs do not conflict
Aksan, N., & Kochanska, G. (2004b). Links between systems of inhi-
with these areas of research: The temperament dimen- bition from infancy to preschool years. Child Development 75(5),
sions we have described are open to experience, al- 14771490.
though some systems are likely more open than others. Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York:
In addition, the functioning of control systems will be Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2002). Is the human amygdala crit-
highly dependent on what the culture indicates should
ical for the subjective experience of emotion?: Evidence of intact
be controlled. Prospects for effective longitudinal re- dispositional affect in patients with amygdala lesions. Journal of
search will be much improved by an integration of the Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 709720.
154 Temperament

Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2001). Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1984). Objective and subjective compo-
Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early nents in mothers perceptions of their children from age 6 months
childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281296. to 3 years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 111130.
Anderson, K., Bohlin, G., & Hagekull, B. (1999). Early temperament Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1988). The role of attachment in the de-
and stranger wariness as predictors of social inhibition in 2-year- velopment of behavior problems. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski
olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 421434. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 253299). Hills-
Arcus, D. (2001). Inhibited and uninhibited children: Biology in the dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
social context. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Tem- Bates, J. E., Bayles, K., Bennett, D. S., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. M.
perament in context (pp. 4360). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (1991). Origins of externalizing behavior problems at eight years
Aron, E. N., & Aron A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity and its of age. In D. Pepler & K. Rubin (Eds.), Development and treatment
relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality of childhood aggression (pp. 93120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and Social Psychology, 73(2), 345368. Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Measure-
Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Rijsdijk, F. V., ment of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, 794803.
Jaffee, S. R., et al. (2003). Strong genetic effects on cross- Bates, J. E., Maslin, C. A., & Frankel, K. A. (1985). Attachment se-
situational antisocial behaviour among 5-year-old children accord- curity, mother-child interaction, and temperament as predictors
ing to mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins self- of behavior-problem ratings at age three years. Monographs of the
reports. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 832848. Society for Child Development, 50(1/2, Serial No. 209), 167193.
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). Development of inhibition during childhood: Bates, J. E., & McFadyen-Ketchum, S. (2001). Temperament and
Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor model. De- parent-child relations as interacting factors in childrens behav-
velopmental Psychology, 26, 721730. ioral adjustment. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Tem-
Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). Social inhibition: A general-developmental perament and personality across the life span (pp. 141176).
perspective. In H. C. Traue & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Emotion Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
inhibition and health (pp. 8099). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber. Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (1995). Family and child
Asendorpf, J. B. (1994). The malleability of behavioral inhibition: A factors in stability and change in childrens aggressiveness in ele-
study of individual developmental functions. Developmental Psy- mentary school. In J. McCord (Ed.), Coercion and punishment in
chology, 30, 912919. long-term perspectives (pp. 124138). New York: Cambridge Uni-
Auerbach, J., Geller, V., Letzer, S., Shinwell, E., Levine, J., Bel- versity Press.
maker, R. H., et al. (1999). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) and Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Ridge, B. (1998). The in-
serotonin transporter promoter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphisms in teraction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive
the determination of temperament in 2-month-old infants. Molec- parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Develop-
ular Psychiatry, 4, 369374. mental Psychology, 34, 982995.
Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, Bates, J. E., Sandy, J. M., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2000, Octo-
P. K., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the interpersonal self: ber). Child and adolescent adjustment as a function of child tem-
Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. perament and family stress. Paper presented at the First Expert
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 776792. Workshop on Personality Psychology, Ghent, Belgium.
Barr, C. S., Newman, T. K., Becker, M. L., Parker, C. C., Champoux, Bates, J. E., Viken, R. J., & Williams, N. (2003, April). Temperament
M., Lesch, K. P., et al. (in press). Early experience and rh5-HT- as a moderator of the linkage between sleep and preschool adjust-
TLPR genotype interact to inf luence social behavior and aggres- ment. Paper presented at meeting of Society for Research in Child
sion in nonhuman primates. Genes, brain, and behavior. Development, Tampa, FL.
Bates, J. E. (1980). The concept of temperament. Merrill-Palmer Bates, J. E., Wachs, T. D., & Emde, R. N. (1994). Toward practical
Quarterly, 26, 299319. uses for biological concepts of temperament. In J. E. Bates &
Bates, J. E. (1987). Temperament in infancy. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual dif ferences at the in-
Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 11011149). New terface of biology and behavior (pp. 275306). Washington, DC:
York: Wiley. American Psychological Association.
Bates, J. E. (1989a). Applications of temperament concepts. In G. A. Bayley, N., & Schafer, E. S. (1963). Maternal behavior, child behav-
Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament ior, and their intercorrelations from infancy through adoles-
in childhood (pp. 321355). Chichester, England: Wiley. cence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Bates, J. E. (1989b). Concepts and measure of temperament. In G. A. Development, 28, 127.
Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament Beauchaine, T. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Grays motiva-
in childhood (pp. 326). Chichester, England: Wiley. tional theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous
Bates, J. E. (1990). Conceptual and empirical linkages between tem- system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psycho-
perament and behavior problems: A commentary on the Sanson, pathology, 13, 183214.
Prior, and Kyrios study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 193199. Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in
Bates, J. E. (1994). Parents as scientific observers of their childrens studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 8195.
development. In S. L. Friedman & H. C. Haywood (Eds.), Devel- Bell, R. Q. (1974). Contributions of human infants to caregiving and
opmental follow-up: Concepts, domains, and methods social interaction. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The ef-
(pp. 197216). New York: Academic Press. fect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 119). New York: Wiley.
Bates, J. E. (2003, April). Temperamental unadaptability and later in- Belsky, J., Friedman, S., & Hsieh, K. (2001). Testing a core emotion-
ternalizing problems as moderated by mothers restrictive control. regulation prediction: Does early attentional persistence moder-
Presented at meeting of the Society for Research in Child Devel- ate the effect of infant negative emotionality on later
opment, Tampa, FL. development? Child Development, 72, 123133.
References 155

Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and mother and observer reports evaluated and compared at five
infant negativity as antecedents of boys externalizing problems months using the infant temperament measure. International Jour-
and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rear- nal of Behavioral Development, 14, 131151.
ing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301319. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen,
Benjamin, J., Ebstein, R. P., & Belmaker, R. H. (Eds.). (1996). Molec- J. D. (2001). Conf lict monitoring and cognitive control. Psycho-
ular genetics and the human personality. Washington, DC: Ameri- logical Review, 108, 624652.
can Psychiatric Publishing. Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and
Bennett, A. J., Lesch, K. P., Heils, A., Long, J. C., Lorenz, J. G., personality. Behavior Genetics, 31(3), 243273.
Shoaf, S. E., et al. (2002). Early experience and serotonin trans- Bradley, B. S. (1985). Failure to distinguish between people and
porter gene variation interact to inf luence primate CNS function. things in early infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychol-
Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 118122. ogy, 3, 281291.
Bergman, P., & Escalona, S. K. (1949). Unusual sensitivities in very
Buck, R. W. (1979). Individual differences in nonverbal sending
young children. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 3, 333352.
accuracy and electrodermal responding: The externalizing-
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: internalizing dimension. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in non-verbal
Appleton-Century-Crofts. communication: Individual dif ferences. Cambridge, MA:
Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Respira- Oelgeschlager, Gunn, & Hain.
tory sinus arrhythmia: Autonomic origins, physiological mecha- Burt, C. (1937). The analysis of temperament. British Journal of Med-
nisms, and psychophysiological implications. Psychophysiology, ical Psychology, 17, 158188.
30, 183196.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional
Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Hirshfeld, D. R., Faraone, S. V., inf luences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sci-
Bolduc, E. A., Gersten, M., et al. (1990). Psychiatric correlates of ences, 4, 215222.
behavioral inhibition in young children of parents with and without
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of personal-
psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 2126.
ity development. New York: Wiley.
Birns, B. (1965). Individual differences in human neonates re-
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing
sponses to stimulation. Child Development, 36, 249256.
personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Birns, B., Barten, S., & Bridger, W. (1969). Individual differences in
temperamental characteristics of infants. Transactions of the New Buss, K. A., Schumacher, J. R. M., Dolski, I., Kalin, N. H., Gold-
York Academy of Sciences, 31, 10711082. smith, H. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Right frontal brain activ-
ity, cortisol, and withdrawal behavior in 6-month-old infants.
Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and
Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(1), 1120.
teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibi-
tion? Child Development, 74, 18991917. Byrne, J. M., Clark-Touesnard, M. E., Hondas, B. J., & Smith, I. M.
(1985, April). Stability of individual dif ferences in infant visual
Black, J. E., & Greenough, W. T. (1991). Developmental approaches
attention. Paper presented at the meeting for the Society for Re-
to the memory processes. In J. L. Martinez Jr., & R. P. Kesner
search in Child Development, Toronto, Canada.
(Eds.), Learning and memory: A biological view (2nd ed.,
pp. 6191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., & Young, A. W. (2001). Neuropsychol-
ogy of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 352363.
Blair, C. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in
young children: Relations with self-regulation and adaptation to Calkins, S. D. (2002). Does aversive behavior during toddlerhood
preschool in children attending Head Start. Developmental Psy- matter? The effects of difficult temperament on maternal percep-
chobiology, 42, 301311. tions and behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 381402.
Blanchard, D. C., & Takahashi, S. N. (1988). No change in intermale Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (1994). Individual differences in the bi-
aggression after amygdala lesions which reduce freezing. Physiol- ological aspects of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs
ogy and Behavior, 42, 613616. (Eds.), Temperament: Individual dif ferences at the interface of bi-
Block, J., & Kremen, A. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: Their concep- ology and behavior (pp. 199217). Washington, DC: American
tual and empirical connections and separateness. Journal of Per- Psychological Association.
sonality and Social Psychology, 70, 349361. Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., & Marshall, T. R. (1996). Behavioral and
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego- psychological antecedents of inhibition in infancy. Child Develop-
resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), ment, 67, 523540.
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39101). Calkins, S. D., & Williford, A. P. (2003, April). Anger regulation in in-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. fancy: Consequences and correlates. Paper presented at the meet-
Bohart, A. C., & Stipek, D. J. (2001). What have we learned. In A. C. ing of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behav- Campbell, D. W., Eaton, W. O., & McKeen, N. A. (2002). Motor ac-
ior: Implications for family, school, and society (pp. 367397). tivity level and behavioural control in young children. Interna-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 289296.
Booth, A., Johnson, D. R., Granger, D. A., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, Canli, T., Sivers, H., Whitfield, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., & Gabrieli,
S. (2003). Testosterone and child and adolescent adjustment: The J. D. E. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of
moderating role of parent-child relationships. Developmental Psy- extraversion. Science, 296, 2191.
chology, 39, 8598. Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli,
Bornstein, M., & Suess, P. E. (2000). Child and mother cardiac vagal J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality inf luences on
tone: Continuity, stability, and concordance across the first 5 brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience,
years. Developmental Psychology, 36, 5465. 115, 3342.
Bornstein, M. H., Gaughran, J. M., & Segui, I. (1991). Multi-method Carey, G. (1996). Family and genetic epidemiology of aggressive
assessments of infant temperament: Mother questionnaire and and antisocial behavior. In D. M. Stoff & R. B. Cairns (Eds.),
156 Temperament

Aggression and violence: Genetic, neurobiological, and biosocial Darley, J., & Fazio, R. (1980). Expectancy confirmation processes
perspectives (pp. 321). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. arising in the social interaction sequence. American Psychologist,
Carey, G. (2003). Human genetics for the social sciences. London: Sage. 35, 867881.
Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the infant tem- Davidson, R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1989). Frontal brain asymmetry pre-
perament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61, 735739. dicts infants response to maternal separation. Journal of Abnor-
mal Psychology, 98, 127131.
Carter, C. S. (1986). The reproductive and adrenal systems. In
M. G. H. Coles, E. Donchin, & S. E. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiol- Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction
ogy: Systems, processes, and applications (pp. 172182). New in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: A possible prelude
York: Guilford Press. to violence. Science, 289, 591594.
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behav- Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., Goldsmith, H. H., Pizzagalli, D.,
ioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and Nitschke, J. B., Kalin, N. H., et al. (2003). Neuroscience. In R. J.
punishment: The BIS/ BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and So- Davidson, K. R. Scherer, H. H. Goldsmith, D. Pizzagalli, J. B.
cial Psychology, 67, 319333. Nitschke, N. H. Kalin, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of af fective sci-
Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continu- ences: Part I. Series in af fective science (pp. 3128). London: Ox-
ities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Personality and So- ford University Press.
cial Psychology, 78, 158172. Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1989). Laterality and emotion:
Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., & An electrophysiological approach. In F. Boller & J. Grafman
Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Childrens behavioral styles at age 3 are (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 419441). Amster-
linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal of Per- dam: Elsevier.
sonality, 71, 495513. Davis, M., Hitchcock, J. M., & Rosen, J. B. (1987). Anxiety and
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., the amygdala: Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of the
et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal- fear-potentiated startle paradigm. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psy-
treated children. Science, 297, 851854. chology of learning and motivation: Vol. 21. Advances in re-
search and theory (pp. 263305). San Diego, CA: Academic
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperament qualities at age three
Press.
predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evi-
dence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486498. DeGangi, G. A., DiPietro, J. A., Greenspan, S. I., & Porges, S. W.
(1991). Psychophysiological characteristics of the regulatory dis-
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Har-
ordered infant. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 3750.
rington, H., et al. (2003). Inf luence of life stress on depression:
Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure
386389. of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation,
and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491569.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1984). Origins and evolution of behavior
disorders. New York: Brunner/ Mazel. Depue, R. A., & Iacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of af-
fective disorders. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. Y. Porter (Eds.), An-
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, per-
nual review of psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 457492). Palo Alto, CA:
sonality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnor-
Annual Reviews.
mal Psychology, 103, 103116.
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. (1994). Temperament and attention: Ori-
Cloninger, C. R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality
enting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Jour-
and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric De-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 11281139.
velopments, 3, 167226.
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1996). Regulatory processes and the
Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description
development of cognitive representations. Development and
and classification of personality variants. Archives of General
Psychopathology, 8, 215234.
Psychiatry, 44, 573588.
Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (1997). The moderating Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional
effects of childrens fear and activity level on relations between biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Ab-
parenting practices and childhood symptomatology. Journal of normal Psychology, 111, 225236.
Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(3), 251263. Dettling, A. C., Parker, S., Lane, S. K., Sebanc, A. M., & Gunnar,
Coldren, J. T., Colombo, J., OBrien, M., Martinez, R., & Horowitz, M. R. (2000). Quality of care and temperament determine
F. D. (1987, April). The relationship of infant visual attention whether cortisol levels rise over the day for children in full-day
across social interaction and information processing tasks. Paper childcare. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 819836.
presented at the meeting for the Society for Research in Child De- Diamond, S. (1974). The roots of psychology: A sourcebook in the his-
velopment, Baltimore, MD. tory of ideas. New York: Basic Books.
Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Neural sys- Diener, M. L., Goldstein, L. H., & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1995). The
tems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial work- role of prenatal expectations in parents reports of infant tempera-
ing memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 508523. ment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 172190.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalog to classifica- Diener, M. L., & Kim, D.-Y. (2004). Maternal and child predictors of
tion: Murrays needs and the five-factor model. Journal of Person- preschool childrens social competence. Applied Developmental
ality and Social Psychology, 55, 258265. Psychology, 25, 324.
Crider, A., & Lunn, R. (1971). Electrodermal lability as a personal- Digman, J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986). Further specification of the five
ity dimension. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, robust factors of personality. Journal of Personality and Social
5, 145150. Psychology, 50, 116123.
References 157

Ding, Y. C., Chi, H. C., Grady, D. L., Morishima, A., Kidd, J. R., havioral regulation and negative emotionality. Child Development,
Kidd, K. K., et al. (2002). Evidence of positive selection acting at 71(5), 13671382.
the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proceedings of the Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland,
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 99, 309314. A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the and impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child De-
development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Devel- velopment, 75(1), 2546.
opmental Psychology, 39, 349371. El-Sheikh, M. (2001). Parental drinking problems and childrens
Donzella, B., Gunnar, M. R., Krueger, W. K., & Alwin, J. (2000). adjustment: Vagal regulation and emotional reactivity as path-
Cortisol and vagal tone response to competitive challenge in ways and moderators of risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
preschoolers: Associations with temperament. Developmental 110, 499515.
Psychobiology, 37, 209220. El-Sheikh, M., Harger, J., & Whitson, S. M. (2001). Exposure to in-
Driver, J., Eimer, M., & Macaluso, E. (in press). Neurobiology of terparental conf lict and childrens adjustment and physical
human spatial attention: Modulation, generation, and integration. health: The moderating role of vagal tone. Child Development,
In N. Kanwisher & J. Duncan (Eds.), Attention and performance 72, 16171636.
XX: Functional brain imaging of visual cognition. Escalona, S. K. (1968). The roots of individuality: Normal patterns of
Dumas, J. E., & LaFreniere, P. J. (1993). Mother-child relationships as development in infancy. Chicago: Aldine.
sources of support or stress: A comparison of competent, average,
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Spring-
aggressive, and anxious dyads. Child Development, 64, 17321754.
field, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000). Dissociative methods in the study
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual
of frontal lobe function. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control
dif ferences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum Press.
of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVII
(pp. 567576). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Eisenbud, L. (1993). Behavioral and
physiological correlates of childrens reactions to others in dis-
Eaton, W. O. (1983). Measuring activity level with actometers: Relia-
tress. Developmental Psychology, 29, 655663.
bility, validity, and arm length. Child Development, 54, 720726.
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Jones, S., Smith, M., Guthrie, I.,
Eaton, W. O. (1994). Temperament, development, and the Five-Factor
Poulin, R., et al. (1999). Regulation, emotionality, and
Model: Lessons from activity level. In C. F. Halverson Jr., G. A.
preschoolers socially competent peer interactions. Child Devel-
Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of
opment, 70, 432442.
temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood
(pp. 173187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., Bernzweig, J., Speer, A. L.,
& Carlo, G. (1994). Socialization of childrens vicarious emo-
Eaton, W. O., McKeen, N. A., & Campbell, D. W. (2001). The waxing
tional responding and prosocial behavior: Relations with mothers
and waning of movement: Implications for psychological develop-
perceptions of childrens emotional reactivity. Developmental
ment. Developmental Review, 21, 205223.
Psychology, 30, 4455.
Eaton, W. O., & Saudino, K. J. (1992). Prenatal activity level as a tem-
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., & Troyer, D. (1994). The re-
perament dimension? Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 5770.
lations of childrens emotion regulation to their vicarious emo-
Eaves, L., Silberg, J., & Erkanli, A. (2003). Resolving multiple epige-
tional responses and comforting behaviors. Child Development,
netic pathways to adolescent depression. Journal of Child Psychol-
65, 16781693.
ogy and Psychiatry, 44, 10061014.
Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., & Pos-
Ebstein, R. P., Levine, J., Geller, V., Auerbach, J., Gritsenko, I., &
ner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conf lict.
Belmaker, R. H. (1998). Dopamine D4 receptor and serotonin
NeuroImage, 18, 4257.
transporter promoter in the determination of neonatal tempera-
ment. Molecular Psychiatry, 3, 238246. Fan, J., Fossella, J. A., Sommer, T., Wu, Y., & Posner, M. I. (2003).
Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain
Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine,
activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
D., et al. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III poly-
100, 74067411.
morphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty
seeking. Nature Genetics, 12(1), 7880. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I.
(2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard,
networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340347.
S. A., Reider, M., et al. (2001). The relations of regulation and
emotionality to childrens externalizing and internalizing prob- Fan, J., Wu, Y., Fossella, J., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Assessing the
lem behavior. Child Development, 72, 11121134. heritability of attentional networks. BMC Neuroscience, 2, 14.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Poulin, R., Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-
& Shepard, S. (1996). The relations of regulation and emotional- infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-
ity to problem behavior in elementary school children. Develop- control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223231.
ment and Psychopathology, 8, 141162. Fordham, F. (1953). An introduction to Jungs psychology. New York:
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dis- Plenum Press.
positional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting Forman, D. R., OHara, M. W., Larsen, K., Coy, K. C., Gorman, L. L.,
quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social & Stuart, S. (2003). Infant emotionality: Observational methods
Psychology, 78, 136157. and the validity of maternal reports. Infancy, 4, 541565.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., Fossella, J., Posner, M. I., Fan, J., Swanson, J. M., & Pfaff, D. W.
Murphy, B. C., et al. (2000). Prediction of elementary school chil- (2002). Attentional phenotypes for the analysis of higher mental
drens externalizing problem behaviors from attentional and be- function. Scientific World Journal, 2, 217223.
158 Temperament

Fowles, D. C. (1982). Heart rate as an index of anxiety: Failure of a Goldsmith, H. H. (1989). Behavior-genetic approaches to tempera-
hypothesis. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Perspectives in ment. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart
cardiovascular psychophysiology (pp. 93126). New York: Guil- (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 111132). Chichester, En-
ford Press. gland: Wiley.
Fowles, D. C. (1994). A motivational theory of psychopathology. In Goldsmith, H. H. (1993). Temperament: Variability in developing
W. Spaulding (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 41. emotion systems. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook
Integrated views of motivation and emotion (pp. 181238). Lin- of emotion (pp. 353364). New York: Guilford Press.
coln: University of Nebraska Press. Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, K. A., & Lemery, K. S. (1997). Toddler and
Fowles, D. C., & Kochanska, G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evi-
of pathways to conscience in children: The contribution of elec- dence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Develop-
trodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 37, 788795. mental Psychology, 33, 891905.
Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (1994). Neural plasticity and Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1982). Toward a theory of infant
development in the first two years of life: Evidence from cogni- temperament. In R. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.), Attachment and
tive and socioemotional domains of research. Development and af filiative systems (pp. 161193). New York: Plenum Press.
Psychopathology, 6, 677696. Goldsmith, H. H., & Gottesman, I. I. (1977). An extension of con-
Fox, N. A., & Field, T. (1989). Individual differences in preschool struct validity for personality scales using twin-based criteria.
entry behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, Journal of Research in Personality, 11, 381397.
10, 527540. Goldsmith, H. H., & Hewitt, E. C. (2003). Validity of parental report
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, of temperament: Distinctions and needed research. Infant Behav-
L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition ior and Development, 26, 108111.
and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral inf luences Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J. (1999).
across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 121. Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Develop-
mental Psychology, 35, 972985.
Fox, N. A., & Stifter, C. A. (1989). Biological and behavioral differ-
ences in infant reactivity and regulation. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Goldsmith, H. H., Losoya, S. H., Bradshaw, D. L., & Campos, J. J.
Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (1994). Genetics of personality: A twin study of the five-factor
(pp. 169183). Chichester, England: Wiley. model and parental-offspring analyses. In C. Halverson, R. Mar-
tin, & G. Kohnstamm (Eds.), The developing structure of tempera-
Fries, M. E., & Woolf, P. (1954). Some hypotheses on the role of con-
ment and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 241265).
genital activity type in personality development. Psychoanalytic
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Study of the Child, 8, 4864.
Goldsmith, H. H., & Rieser-Danner, L. (1986). Variation among tem-
Gallagher, K. C. (2002). Does child temperament moderate the influence
perament theories and validation studies of temperament assess-
of parenting on adjustment? Developmental Review, 22, 623643.
ment. In G. A. Kohnstamm (Ed.), Temperament discussed:
Garcia Coll, C. T., Halpern, L. F., Vohr, B. R., Seifer, R., & Oh, W. Temperament and development in infancy and childhood (pp. 19).
(1992). Stability and correlates of change of early temperament in Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behavior and Development, Goldsmith, H. H., Rieser-Danner, L. A., & Briggs, S. (1991). Evalu-
15, 137153. ating convergent and discriminant validity of temperament ques-
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant tempera- tionnaires for preschoolers, toddlers, and infants. Developmental
ment via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav- Psychology, 27, 566579.
ior and Development, 26, 6486. Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Contemporary instru-
Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial conf lict and the de- ments for assessing early temperament by questionnaire and in
velopment of self-regulation in children 24 to 36 months of age. the laboratory. In A. Angleitner & J. Strelau (Eds.), Explorations
Developmental Science, 3, 397404. in temperament: International perspectives on theory and mea-
Gesell, A. (1928) Infancy and human growth. New York: MacMillan. surement (pp. 249272). New York: Plenum Press.
Gest, S. D. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability and associations Goodnight, J. A., Bates, J. E., Newman, J. P., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit,
with adaptation from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of G. S. (2004). The interactive inf luence of friend deviance, disin-
Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 467475. hibitory tendencies, and gender on the development of externaliz-
ing behavior during early and middle adolescence. Manuscript
Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and
submitted for publication.
internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and
Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in non-
Psychopathology, 16, 313333.
human animals: A cross-species review. Current Directions in
Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, Psychological Science, 8, 6975.
J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys:
Gray, J. A. (1991). The neuropsychology of temperament. In J. Stre-
Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. De-
lau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: Interna-
velopmental Psychology, 38, 222235.
tional perspectives on theory and measurement (pp. 105128).
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative description of personality: New York: Plenum Press.
The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (1996). The neuropsychology of anxi-
Psychology, 59, 12161229. ety: Reprise. In D. A. Hope (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Moti-
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality vation: Vol. 43. Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fearCurrent
traits. American Psychologist, 48, 2634. theory and research in motivation (pp. 61134). Lincoln: Univer-
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Topography of cognition: Parallel dis- sity of Nebraska Press.
tributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Review of Graziano, W. G. (1994). The development of agreeableness as a di-
Neuroscience, 11, 137156. mension of personality. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, &
References 159

R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1997). Behavioral activation
personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 339354). Hillsdale, sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: Covariation of
NJ: Erlbaum. putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders. Journal of
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Agreeableness: A dimen- Abnormal Psychology, 106, 159163.
sion of personality. In R. Hogan, J. A. Johnson, & S. Briggs Hart, D., Atkins, R., & Fegley, S. (2003). Personality and develop-
(Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795824). San ment in childhood: A person-centered approach. Monographs of
Diego, CA: Academic Press. the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 7485.
Graziano, W. G., & Tobin, R. M. (2002). Agreeableness: Dimension Hegvik, R. L., McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. (1982). The middle
of personality or social desirability artifact? Journal of Personal- childhood temperament questionnaire. Developmental and Behav-
ity, 70, 695727. ioral Pediatrics, 3, 197200.
Green, J., & Goldwyn, R. (2002). Annotation: Attachment disorgani- Hemphill, S., & Sanson, A. (2001). Matching parenting to child. Fam-
sation and psychopathologyNew findings in attachment re- ily Matters, 59, 4247.
search and their potential implications for developmental
Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Introduction to psychology: Under the general
psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and
editorship of Claude E. Buxton (3rd ed.). New York: Harcourt,
Psychiatry, 43, 835846.
Brace, & World.
Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P. H., & Thomas, C. W.
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of
(1994). Temperament and school functioning during early adoles-
human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine,
cence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 200225.
and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109,
Gunnar, M. R. (1990). The psychobiology of infant temperament. In J. 679709.
Colombo & J. Fagen (Eds.), Individual dif ferences in infancy: Reli-
Honzik, M. P. (1965). Prediction of behavior from birth to maturity
ability, stability, prediction (pp. 387409). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[Review of the book Birth to maturity]. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
Gunnar, M. R. (1994). Psychoendocrine studies of temperament and terly, 11, 7788.
stress in early childhood: Expanding current models. In J. E. Huffman, L. C., Bryan, Y. E., del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A.,
Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual dif ferences Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Porges, S. W. (1998). Infant tem-
at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 175198). Washing- perament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks
ton, DC: American Psychological Association. of age. Child Development, 69, 624635.
Gunnar, M. R., Brodersen, L., Krueger, K., & Rigatuso, J. (1996). Hwang, J., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Behavior genetics studies of in-
Dampening of adrenocortical responses during infancy: Norma- fant temperament: Findings vary across parent-report instru-
tive changes and individual differences. Child Development, ments. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 112114.
67, 877889.
Insel, T. R. (2003). Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physi-
Gunnar, M. R., & Cheatham, C. L. (2003). Brain and behavior inter- ology and Behavior, 79, 351357.
face: Stress and the developing brain. Infant Mental Health Jour-
nal, 24, 195211. Jones, H. E. (1960). The longitudinal method in the study of per-
sonality. In I. Iscoe & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.), Personality de-
Gunnar, M. R., Sebanc, A. M., Tout, K., Donzella, B., & Van Dulmen, velopment in children (pp. 327). Chicago: University of
M. M. H. (2003). Peer rejection, temperament, and cortisol activ- Chicago Press.
ity in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 43, 346358.
Jung, C. G. (1923). Psychological types or the psychology of individu-
Gusnard, D. A., Ollinger, J. M., Shulman, G. L., Cloninger, C. R., ation. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Price, J. L., Van Essen, D. C., et al. (2003). Persistence and brain
circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, Kagan, J. (1982). The construct of difficult temperament: A reply to
100, 34793484. Thomas, Chess, and Korn. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 2124.

Hagekull, B. (1989). Longitudinal stability of temperament within a Kagan, J. (1994). Galens prophecy: Temperament in human nature.
behavioral style framework. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 283297). Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In W. Damon (Editor-In-
Chichester, England: Wiley. Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:
Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed.,
Hagekull, B. (1994). Infant temperament and early childhood func-
pp. 177235). New York: Wiley.
tioning: Possible relations to the five-factor model. In J. C. J.
Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The develop- Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1978). Infancy: Its place
ing structure of temperament and personality (pp. 227240). in human development. New York: Wiley.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). Birth to maturity. New York: Wiley.
Hagekull, B., Bohlin, G., & Lindhagen, K. (1984). Validity of Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of
parental reports. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 7792. childhood shyness. Science, 240(4849), 167171.
Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (2003). Vagal inf luence Kalin, N. H., Larson, C., Shelton, S. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1998).
on working memory and attention. International Journal of Psy- Asymmetric frontal brain activity, cortisol, and behavior associ-
chophysiology, 48, 263274. ated with fearful temperament in rhesus monkeys. Behavioral
Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., Neuroscience, 112, 286292.
Goldman, D., et al. (2002). Serotonin transporter genetic variation Karnath, H.-O., Ferber, S., & Himmelbach, M. (2001). Spatial aware-
and the response of the human amygdala. Science, 297, 400403. ness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe.
Harman, C., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (1997). Distress Nature, 411, 951953.
and attention interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1995). Vagal tone protects children from
Emotion, 21, 2743. marital conf lict. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 8392.
160 Temperament

Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Predicting kindergarten peer Korner, A. F. (1964). Some hypotheses regarding the significance of
social status from toddler and preschool problem behavior. Jour- individual differences at birth for later development. Psychoana-
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(4), 409423. lytic Study of the Child, 19, 5872.
Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. S. (1994). The development of aggression in Korner, A. F., Hutchinson, C. A., Koperski, J., Kraemer, H. C., &
toddlers: A study of low-income families. Journal of Abnormal Schneider, P. A. (1981). Stability of individual differences of
Child Psychology, 22, 5377. neonatal motor and crying patterns. Child Development, 52, 8390.
Keiley, M. K., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2000). A Korner, A. F., Zeanah, C. H., Linden, J., Kraemer, H. C., Kerkowitz,
cross-domain growth analysis: Externalizing and internalizing R. I., & Agras, W. S. (1985). Relation between neonatal and later
behaviors during 8 years of childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child activity and temperament. Child Development, 56, 3842.
Psychology, 28, 161179. Krakow, J. B., & Johnson, K. L. (1981, April). The emergence and
Keiley, M. K., Lofthouse, N., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, consolidation of self-control processes from 18 to 30 months of age.
G. S. (2003). Differential risks of covarying and pure components Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
in mother and teacher reports of externalizing and internalizing Child Development, Boston, MA.
behavior across ages 5 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- Krakow, J. B., Kopp, C. B., & Vaughn, B. E. (1981, April). Sustained
ogy, 31, 267283. attention during the second year: Age trends, individual dif fer-
Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: ences, and implications for development. Paper presented at the
Lessons from the person-situation debate. American Psycholo- meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.
gist, 43, 2334. Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and anti-
Keogh, B., Pullis, M. E., & Caldwell, J. (1982). A short form of the social behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality cor-
teacher temperament questionnaire. Journal of Educational Mea- relates, distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 397402.
surement, 19, 323329. Kubzansky, L. D., Martin, L. T., & Buka, S. L. (2004). Early mani-
festations of personality and adult emotional functioning. Emo-
Kessen, W. (1965). The child. New York: Wiley.
tion, 4, 364377.
Kilmer, R. P., Cowen, E. L., & Wyman, P. A. (2001). A micro-level
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated at-
analysis of developmental, parenting, and family milieu variables
tention: Affect, activation, and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Sim-
that differentiate stress-resilient and stress-affected children.
mons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory
Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 391416.
and motivational processes (pp. 97135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
King, B. R., & Wachs, T. D. (1995, March). Multimethod measure-
Lawrence, A. D., & Calder, A. J. (2004). Homologizing human emo-
ment of stimulus sensitivity in infants, preschoolers, and parents.
tions. In D. Evans & P. Cruse (Eds.), Emotions, evolution, and ra-
Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
tionality (pp. 1547). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
Lawrence, A. D., Calder, A. J., McGowan, S. M., & Grasby, P. M.
Kistiakovskaia, M. I. (1965). Stimuli evoking positive emotions in
(2002). Selective disruption of the recognition of facial expres-
infants in the first months of life. Soviet Psychology and Psychia-
sions of anger. NeuroReport, 13, 881884.
try, 3, 3948.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain.
Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the Cognition and Emotion, 3, 267289.
development of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62,
Lee, C. L., & Bates, J. E. (1985). Mother-child interaction at two
13791392.
years and perceived difficult temperament. Child Development,
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization 56, 13141325.
and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child
Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the re-
Development, 64, 325347.
lation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by
Kochanska, G. (1995). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline, eliminating measurement confounding: Expert ratings and factor
and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging inter- analyses. Child Development, 73, 867882.
nalization. Child Development, 66, 597615.
Lengua, L. J., & Long, A. C. (2002). The role of emotionality and self-
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children regulation in the appraisal-coping process: Tests of direct and mod-
with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel- erating effects. Applied Developmental Psychology, 23, 471493.
opmental Psychology, 33, 228240.
Lengua, L. J., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Temperament as a
Kochanska, G., DeVet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K. T., & Putnam, predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing contamina-
S. P. (1993). Maternal reports of conscience development and tion of measures. Child Development, 69, 164181.
temperament in young children. Child Development, 65, 852868. Lengua, L. J., Wolchik, S. A., Sandler, I. N., & West, S. G. (2000).
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personal- The additive and interactive effects of parenting and tempera-
ity characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and ment in predicting adjustment problems of children of divorce.
consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 10871112. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 232244.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful con- Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (1994). Explorations of the goodness-
trol in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and of-fit model in early adolescence. In W. B. Carey & S. C. McDe-
implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, vitt (Eds.), Prevention and early intervention: Individual
36, 220232. dif ferences as risk factors for the mental health of childrenA
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive develop- festschrift for Stella Chess and Alexander Thomas (pp. 161169).
mental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Hand- New York: Brunner/ Mazel.
book of socialization theory and research (pp. 347480). Lesch, K. P., Greenberg, B. D., Higley, J. D., Bennett, A., & Murphy,
Chicago: Rand McNally. D. L. (2002). Serotonin transporter, personality, and behavior:
References 161

Toward a dissection of gene-gene and gene-environment inter- R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-
action. In J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, & R. H. Belmaker (Eds.), regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 99129). New
Molecular genetics and the human personality (pp. 109136). York: Guilford Press.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., &
Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. S. (1995). Stability and change in cortisol Essex, M. J. (2002). Temperamental vulnerability and negative
and behavioral response to stress during the first 18 months of parenting as interacting predictors of child adjustment. Journal of
life. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 419428. Marriage and Family, 64, 461471.
Lonigan, C. J., Vasey, M. W., Phillips, B. M., & Hazen, R. A. (2004). Moskowitz, D. S., & Schwarz, J. C. (1982). Validity comparison of
Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stim- behavior counts and ratings by knowledgeable informants. Journal
uli. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 820. of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 518528.
Lytton, H. (1995, March). Child and family factors as predictors of Mun, E. Y., Fitzgerald, H. E., von Eye, A., Puttler, L. I., & Zucker,
conduct disorder and criminality. Paper presented at the meeting of R. A. (2001). Temperamental characteristics as predictors of ex-
the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. ternalizing and internalizing child behavior problems in the con-
MacDonald, K. (1988). Social and personality development: An evolu- texts of high and low parental psychopathology. Infant Mental
tionary synthesis. New York: Plenum Press. Health Journal, 22, 393415.
MacDonald, K. (1992). Warmth as a developmental construct: An Murphy, B. C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S., & Guthrie,
evolutionary analysis. Child Development, 63, 753773. I. K. (1999). Consistency and change in childrens emotionality
Martin, R. P., Wisenbaker, J., & Huttunen, M. (1994). The factor and regulation: A longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
structure of instruments based on the Chess-Thomas model of 45, 413444.
temperament: Implications for the big five model. In C. F. Halver- Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (2004).
son, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing Concurrent and across time prediction of young adolescents so-
structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adult- cial functioning: The role of emotionality and regulation. Social
hood (pp. 339347). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Development, 13(1), 5686.
Mascolo, M. F., Fischer, K. W., & Li, J. (2003). Dynamic development Murphy, L. B., & Moriarty, A. E. (1976). Vulnerability, coping, and
of component systems of emotions: Pride, shame, and guilt in growth: From infancy to adolescence. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni-
China and the United States. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & versity Press.
H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of af fective sciences
Needham, J. (1973). Chinese science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(pp. 375410). New York: Oxford University Press.
Neilon, P. (1948). Shirleys babies after 15 years. Journal of Genetic
Matheny, A. P., Jr. (1989). Childrens behavioral inhibition over age
Psychology, 73, 175186.
and across situations: Genetic similarity for a trait during change.
Journal of Personality, 57, 215235. Nishijo, H., Ono, T., & Nishino, H. (1988). Single neuron responses in
amygdala of alert monkey during complex sensory stimulation with
Matheny, A. P., Jr. (1991). Childrens unintentional injuries and gen-
affective significance. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 35703583.
der: Differentiation by environmental and psychosocial aspects.
Childrens Environment Quarterly, 8, 5161. Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002).
Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of
Matheny, A. P., Wilson, R. S., & Thoben, A. S. (1987). Home and
emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 12151229.
mother: Relations with infant temperament. Developmental Psy-
chology, 23, 323331. OConnor, B. P., & Dvorak, T. (2001). Conditional associations be-
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of tween parental behavior and adolescent problems: A search for
detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bul- personality-environment interactions. Journal of Research in Per-
letin, 114(2), 376390. sonality, 35, 126.
McClowry, S. G., Hegvik, R., & Teglasi, H. (1993). An examination OConnor, T. G., Caspi, A., DeFries, J. C., & Plomin, R. (2003).
of the construct validity of the middle childhood temperament Genotype-environment interaction in childrens adjustment to
questionnaire. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 279293. parental separation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
and Allied Disciplines, 44(6), 849856.
Mebert, C. J. (1991). Dimensions of subjectivity in parents ratings of
infant temperament. Child Development, 62, 352361. Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., de Winter, A. F., Veenstra, R., &
Ormel, J. (2004). Temperamental profiles associated with inter-
Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention:
nalizing and externalizing problems in preadolescence. Develop-
Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an
ment and Psychopathology, 16, 421440.
epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Develop-
ment, 75, 13731386. Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1990). Early antecedents of
Miller, E. M., & Bates, J. E. (1986, April). Relationships between childhood impulsivity: The role of parent-child interaction, cogni-
mother perceptions and observed episodes of infant distress: Com- tive competence, and temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child
ponents of perceived dif ficult temperament. Paper presented at the Psychology, 18, 317334.
meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, Los Osofsky, J. D. (1979). Handbook of infant development. New York: Wiley.
Angeles, CA. Owens, E. B., & Shaw, D. S. (2003). Predicting growth curves of ex-
Mischel, W. (1983). Delay of gratification as process and as person ternalizing behavior across the preschool years. Journal of Abnor-
variable in development. In D. Magnusson & V. P. Allen (Eds.), mal Child Psychology, 31, 575590.
Human development: An interactional perspective (pp. 149165). Oxford, M., Cavell, T. A., & Hughes, J. N. (2003). Callous/unemotional
New York: Academic Press. traits moderate the relation between ineffective parenting and
Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective child externalizing problems: A partial replication and extension.
processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, 577585.
162 Temperament

Panksepp, J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of Porges, S. W. (1991). Autonomic regulation and attention. In B. A.
emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407467. Campbell, H. Hayne, & R. Richardson (Eds.), Attention and infor-
Panksepp, J. (1986a). The anatomy of emotions. In R. Plutchik & H. mation processing in infants and adults (pp. 201223). Hillsdale,
Kellerman (Eds.), EmotionTheory, research and experience: NJ: Erlbaum.
Vol. 3. Biological foundations of emotions (pp. 91124). San Porges, S. W., & Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A. (1997). The psychophysi-
Diego, CA: Academic Press. ology of temperament. In J. D. Noshpitz, S. Greenspan, S. Wieder,
Panksepp, J. (1986b). The neurochemistry of behavior. Annual Re- & J. Osofsky (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry
view of Psychology, 37, 77107. (pp. 163179). New York: Wiley.
Panksepp, J. (1986c). The psychobiology of prosocial behaviors: Sep- Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994).
aration distress, play, and altruism. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Mono-
Cummings, & R. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biolog- graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2/3),
ical and social origins (pp. 1957). Cambridge, England: Cam- 167186, 250283.
bridge University Press. Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Portales, A. L., & Suess,
Panksepp, J. (1993). Neurochemical control of moods and emotions: P. E. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: Stability and relation to diffi-
Amino acids to neuropeptides. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), cultness in infants and 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychobiology,
Handbook of emotions (pp. 87107). New York: Guilford Press. 27, 289300.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Af fective neuroscience: The foundations of Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Ex-
human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press. perimental Psychology, A, 32, 325.
Park, S. Y., Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1997). Infant emo- Posner, M. I., & Fan, J. (in press). Attention as an organ system. In J.
tionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: Exploring stability and Pomerantz (Ed.), Neurobiology of perception and communication:
lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Developmental Psychology, From synapse to society (The 4th De Lange Conference). Cam-
33, 218227. bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the
adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bul- human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.
letin, 102, 357389.
Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of the mind. New York:
Patterson, G., & Sanson, A. (1999). The association of behavioural Scientific American Library.
adjustment to temperament, parenting and family characteristics
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Attentional mechanisms and
among 5-year-old children. Social Development, 8, 293309.
conscious experience. In M. Rugg & A. D. Milner (Eds.), The
Patterson, G. R. (1980). Mothers: The unacknowledged victims. neuropsychology of consciousness (pp. 91112). London: Aca-
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, demic Press.
45(5, Serial No. 186).
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self-regulation,
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys.
and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
Eugene, OR: Castalia.
of London, B, 353, 19151927.
Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., & Beckman, D. (2004). Predicting
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of
the development of infant emotionality from maternal character-
self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.
istics. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 1942.
Presley, R., & Martin, R. P. (1994). Toward a structure of preschool
Pavlov, I. P. (1955). General types of animal and human higher nerv-
temperament: Factor structure of the temperament assessment
ous activity. In J. Gibbons (Ed.), Selected works. Moscow: Foreign
battery for children. Journal of Personality, 62, 415448.
Language Publishing House. (Original work published 1935)
Pedlow, R., Sanson, A. V., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). The sta- Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., Hellinckx, W., Grietens, H., Ghesquiere, P.,
bility of temperament from infancy to eight years. Developmental & Colpin, H. (2003). The additive and interactive effects of par-
Psychology, 29, 9981007. enting and childrens personality on externalizing behaviour. Eu-
ropean Journal of Personality, 17, 95117.
Pfeifer, M., Goldsmith, H. H., Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M.
(2002). Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited chil- Prior, M., Smart, D., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2000). Does shy-
dren. Child Development, 73, 14741485. inhibited temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in
adolescence? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality.
lescent Psychiatry, 39, 461468.
In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: The-
ory and research (2nd ed., pp. 251276). New York: Guilford Press. Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tem-
Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M., & Loehlin, J. C. (1990). Behavioral genet- perament and parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of par-
ics and personality. In A. L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality enting (2nd ed., 255278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
theory and research (pp. 225243). New York: Guilford Press. Quas, J. A., Bauer, A., & Boyce, W. T. (2004). Physiological reactiv-
Plomin, R., Emde, R. N., Braungart, J. M., Campos, J., Corley, R., ity, social support, and memory in early childhood. Child Devel-
Fulker, D. W., et al. (1993). Genetic change and continuity from opment, 75, 797814.
14 to 20 months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Quay, H. C. (1993). The psychobiology of undersocialized aggressive
Development, 64, 13541376. conduct disorder: A theoretical perspectiveToward a develop-
Porges, S. W. (1986). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Physioslogical mental perspective on conduct disorder. Development and Psycho-
basis, quantitative methods, and clinical implications. In P. pathology, 5, 165180.
Grossman, K. Janssen, & D. Vaitl (Eds.), Cardiorespiratory and Radke-Yarrow, M., & Sherman, T. (1990). Hard growing: Children
cardiosomatic psychophysiology (pp. 101115). New York: who survive. In J. Rolf, A. S. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. H.
Plenum Press. Neuchterlin, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in
References 163

the development of psychopathology (pp. 97119). New York: Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual dif ferences
Cambridge University Press. at the interface of biology and behavior (pp. 83116). Washington,
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, DC: American Psychological Association.
M. C. (1997). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament
not somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968971. and self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.),
Reed, M., Pien, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131148. (pp. 357370). New York: Guilford Press.
Rende, R. D. (1993). Longitudinal relations between temperament traits Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003).
and behavioral syndromes in middle childhood. Journal of the Amer- Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Jour-
ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 287290. nal of Personality, 71, 11131143.
Richards, J. E., & Cameron, D. (1987, April). Infant heart rate vari- Rothbart, M. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1985). Three approaches
ability and behavioral developmental status. Paper presented at to the study of infant temperament. Developmental Review, 5,
the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, 237260.
Baltimore, MD. Rothbart, M. K., & Hwang, J. (2002). Measuring infant temperament.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consis- Infant Behavior and Development, 25(1), 113116.
tency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative Rothbart, M. K., & Hwang, J. (2005). Temperament and the develop-
review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 325. ment of competence and motivation. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 167184).
Child Development, 52, 569578. New York: Guilford Press.
Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant tempera- Rothbart, M. K., & Mauro, J. A. (1990). Questionnaire approaches to
ment. Developmental Psychology, 22, 356365. the study of infant temperament. In J. W. Fagen & J. Colombo
Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Temperament and the development of inhib- (Eds.), Individual dif ferences in infancy: Reliability, stability and
ited approach. Child Development, 59, 12411250. prediction (pp. 411429). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rothbart, M. K. (1989a). Biological processes of temperament. In G. Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (in press). Temperament, attention,
Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), Hand-
childhood (pp. 77110). Chichester, England: Wiley. book of developmental psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Rothbart, M. K. (1989b). Temperament and development. In G. Kohn- Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Hershey, K. (1995). Temperament,
stamm, J. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in child- attention, and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti &
hood (pp. 187248). Chichester, England: Wiley. J. D. Cohen (Eds.), Manual of developmental psychopathology
Rothbart, M. K. (1995). Concept and method in contemporary tem- (Vol. 1, pp. 315340). New York: Wiley.
perament research [Review of the book Galens prophecy]. Psy- Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Kieras, J. (in press). Temperament,
chological Inquiry, 6, 334348. attention, and the development of self-regulation. In K. McCart-
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament ney & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early child devel-
and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and opment. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Social Psychology, 78, 122135. Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Rosicky, J. (1994). Orienting in nor-
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Tempera- mal and pathological development. Development and Psychopath-
ment and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quar- ology, 6, 635652.
terly, 40, 2139. Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The development of effort-
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K., & Fisher, P. (2001). In- ful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & S. Keele (Eds.), Developing in-
vestigations of temperament at 3 to 7 years: The childrens behav- dividuality in the human brain: A festschrift honoring Michael I.
ior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408. PosnerMay, 2003 (pp. 167188). Washington, DC: American
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon Psychological Association.
(Editor-In-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child Rothbart, M. K., & Sheese, B. (in press). Temperament and emotion
psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development regulation, In J. J. Gross (Ed.) Handbook of emotion regulation.
(5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley. New York: Guilford.
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual Rothbart, M. K., & Victor, J. (2004, October). Temperament and the
differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown development of personality. Paper presented at the Occasional
(Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 3786). Temperament Conference, Athens, GA.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & OBoyle, C. G. (1992). Self-regulation
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (2002). Temperament in children. and emotion in infancy. In N. Eisenberg & R. A. Fabes (Eds.),
In C. von Hofsten & L. Bckman (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of Emotion and its regulation in early development: Vol. 55. New di-
the millennium: Vol. 2. Social, developmental, and clinical per- rections for child developmentThe Jossey-Bass education series
spectives (pp. 1735). East Sussex, England: Psychology Press. (pp. 723). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Dwyer, K. M., & Hastings, P. D. (2003).
temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent Predicting preschoolers externalizing behaviors from toddler
report at seven years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), temperament, conf lict, and maternal negativity. Developmental
Temperament and personality development across the life span Psychology, 39, 164176.
(pp. 85119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobi- social-behavioral consequences of toddlers inhibited tempera-
ological approach to the development of temperament. In J. E. ment and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483495.
164 Temperament

Schwebel, D. C. (2004). Temperamental risk factors for childrens un-


Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P., Chen, X., Stewart, S., & McNichol, K.
intentional injury: The role of impulsivity and inhibitory control.
(1998). Intrapersonal and maternal correlates of aggression, con-
Personality and Individual Dif ferences, 37, 567578.
f lict, and externalizing problems in toddlers. Child Development,
69, 16141629. Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., Halparin, J., Gruber, D., Lercari, L. P., Mc- rearing. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
Candliss, B., et al. (2004). Development of attention during child- Seifer, R. (2003). Twin studies, biases of parents, and biases of re-
hood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10291040. searchers. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 115117.
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). Attentional Seifer, R., Sameroff, A. J., Barrett, L. C., & Krafchuk, E. (1994). In-
control and self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs fant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother
(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and appli- report. Child Development, 65, 14781490.
cations (pp. 283300). New York: Guilford Press. Sethi, A., Mischel, W., Aber, J. L., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early develop- (2000). The role of strategic attention deployment in develop-
ment: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford University Press. ment of self-regulation: Predicting preschoolers delay of gratifi-
cation from mother-toddler interactions. Developmental
Rushton, J. P., Brainerd, C. J., & Pressley, M. (1983). Behavioral de-
Psychology, 6, 767777.
velopment and construct validity: The principle of aggregation.
Psychological Bulletin, 94, 1838. Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003).
Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmen-
Russell, A., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., & Olsen, S. F. (2003). Chil-
tal Psychology, 39(2), 189200.
drens sociable and aggressive behavior with peers: A comparison
Sheeber, L. B. (1995). Empirical dissociations between temperament
of the U.S. and Australia, and contributions of temperament and
and behavior problems: A response to the Sanson, Prior, and
parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
Kyrios study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 554561.
ment, 27, 7486.
Shiner, R. L. (1998). How shall we speak of childrens personalities
Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections be-
in middle childhood? A preliminary taxonomy. Psychological Bul-
tween temperament and social development: A review. Social De-
letin, 124(3), 308332.
velopment, 13, 142170.
Shiner, R. L., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in
Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Kyrios, M. (1990). Contamination of measures
childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and
in temperament research. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 179192.
consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44,
Sanson, A. V., Smart, D. F., Prior, M., Oberklaid, F., & Pedlow, R. 232.
(1994). The structure of temperament from 3 to 7 years: Age,
Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Roberts, J. M. (2003). Childhood per-
sex, and sociodemographic inf luences. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
sonality foreshadows adult personality and life outcomes 2
40, 233252.
decades later. Journal of Personality, 71, 11451170.
Saudino, K. J. (2003). Parent ratings of infant temperament lessons Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2002). A developmental
from twin studies. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, perspective on personality in emerging adulthood: Childhood an-
100107. tecedents and concurrent adaptation. Journal of Personality and
Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2001). Sources of continuity and Social Psychology, 83, 11651177.
change in observed temperament. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt Shirley, M. M. (1933). The first two years: A study of 25 babies. Min-
(Eds.), Infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental in- neapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
f luences on developmental change (pp. 89110). New York: Ox-
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent
ford University Press.
cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay
Saudino, K. J., & Eaton, W. O. (1995). Continuity and change in objec- of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental
tively assessed temperament: A longitudinal twin study of activity Psychology, 26, 978986.
level. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 8195.
Silberg, J. L., Febo San Miguel, V., Murrelle, E. L., Prom, E., Bates,
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- J. E., Canino, G., et al. (2004). Genetic and environmental inf lu-
ronments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Devel- ences on temperament in the first year of life: The Puerto Rico In-
opment, 54, 242435. fant Twin Study. Unpublished manuscript.
Schaffer, H. R. (1974). Cognitive components of the infants re- Silverman, I. W., & Ragusa, D. M. (1992). A short-term longitudinal
sponse to strangeness. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), study of the early development of self-regulation. Journal of Ab-
The origins of fear (pp. 1124). New York: Wiley. normal Child Psychology, 20, 415435.
Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Molecular genetics of tempera- Slabach, E. H., Morrow, J., & Wachs, T. D. (1991). Questionnaire
mental differences in children. In J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, & measurement of infant and child temperament: Current status
R. H. Belmaker (Eds.), Molecular genetics and the human per- and future directions. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner ( Eds.),
sonality (pp. 245255). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on
Publishing. theory and measurement ( pp. 205234). New York: Plenum
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. H. Press.
(2001). Association of DRD4 with attention problems in normal Smeets, W. J. A. J., & Gonzlez, A. (2000). Catecholamine systems in
childhood development. Psychiatric Genetics, 11, 2529. the brain of vertebrates: New perspectives through a comparative
Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of approach. Brain Research Reviews, 33, 308379.
biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Snyder, J. A. (1977). A reinforcement analysis of interaction in prob-
Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 7, lem and nonproblem children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
pp. 297339). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. 86, 528535.
References 165

Spoont, M. R. (1992). Modulatory role of serotonin in neural infor- Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H. G., Hertzig, M. E., & Korn, S.
mation processing: Implications for human psychopathology. Psy- (1963). Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York:
chological Bulletin, 112, 330350. New York University Press.
Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Schroeder, U., Grossenbacher, Thompson, R. A. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In W.
P. G., Federlein, J., Buettner, T., et al. (1999). Knowing no fear. Damon (Editor-In-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
Sciences, 266, 24512456. opment (5th ed., pp. 25104). New York: Wiley.
Stams, G. J. M., Juffer, F., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2002). Maternal Tomarken, A. J., Davidson, R. J., Wheeler, R. E., & Doss, R. C.
sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early child- (1992). Individual differences in anterior brain asymmetry and
hood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted fundamental dimensions of emotion. Journal of Personality and
children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental Social Psychology, 62, 676687.
Psychology, 38, 806821. Tschann, J. M., Kaiser, P., Chesney, M. A., Alkon, A. & Boyce, W. T.
Stice, E., & Gonzales, N. (1998). Adolescent temperament moderates (1996). Resilience and vulnerability among preschool children:
the relation of parenting to antisocial behavior and substance use. Family functioning, temperament, and behavior problems. Journal
Journal of Adolescent Research, 13, 531. of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35,
Stifter, C. A., & Braungart, J. M. (1995). The regulation of negative 184192.
reactivity in infancy: Function and development. Developmental Tuddenham, R. D. (1959). The constancy of personality ratings over
Psychology, 31, 448455. 2 decades. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 60, 329.
Stifter, C. A., & Corey, J. M. (2001). Vagal regulation and observed Turken, A. U., & Swick, D. (1999). Response selection in the human
social behavior in infancy. Social Development, 10, 189201. anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 920924.
Stifter, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Porges, S. W. (1989). Facial expressivity
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumber-
and vagal tone in 5- and 10-month-old infants. Infant Behavior
land, A., & Losoya, S. (2004). Prediction of childrens empathy-
and Development, 12, 127137.
related responding from their effortful control and parents
Stifter, C. A., Spinrad, T. L., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (1999). To- expressivity. Developmental Psychology, 40, 911926.
ward a developmental model of child compliance: The role of
van der Mark, I. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzen-
emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 70, 2132.
doorn, M. H. (2002). The role of parenting, attachment, and
Stoolmiller, M. (2001). Synergistic interaction of child manageabil- temperamental fearfulness in the prediction of compliance in
ity problems and parent-discipline tactics in predicting future toddler girls. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20,
growth in externalizing behavior for boys. Developmental Psy- 361378.
chology, 37, 814825.
van der Mark, I. L., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-
Strelau, J. (1983). Temperament personality activity. New York: Aca-
Kranenburg, M. J. (2002). Development of empathy in girls dur-
demic Press.
ing the second year of life: Associations with parenting, attach-
Suess, P. E., Porges, S. W., & Plude, D. J. (1994). Cardiac vagal ment, and temperament. Social Development, 11, 451468.
tone and sustained attention in school-age children. Psychophysi-
Van Leeuwen, K. G., Mervielde, I., Braet, C., & Bosmans, G. (2004).
ology, 31, 1722.
Child personality and parental behavior as moderators of problem
Suls, J., Martin, R., & David, J. P. (1998). Person-environment fit and behavior: Variable and person-centered approaches. Developmen-
its limits: Agreeableness, neuroticism, and emotional reactivity tal Psychology, 40, 10281046.
to interpersonal conf lict. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
letin, 24, 8898. Vaughn, B. E., Kopp, C. B., & Krakow, J. B. (1984). The emergence
and consolidation of self-control from 18 to 30 months of age:
Swanson, J. M., Flodman, P., Kennedy, J., Spence, M. A., Moyzis, R.,
Normative trends and individual differences. Child Development,
Schuck, S., et al. (2000). Dopamine genes and ADHD. Neuro-
55, 9901004.
science and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 2125.
Vaughn, B. E., Stevenson-Hinde, J., Waters, E., Kotsaftis, A.,
Teglasi, H., & Epstein, S. (1998). Temperament and personality the-
Lefever, G. B., Shouldice, A., et al. (1992). Attachment security
ory: The perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. School
and temperament in infancy and early childhood: Some concep-
Psychology Review, 27, 534550.
tual clarifications. Developmental Psychology, 28, 463473.
Teglasi, H., & MacMahon, B. V. (1990). Temperament and common
problem behaviors of children. Journal of Applied Developmental Victor, J. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Baker, S. R. (2006). Manuscript in
Psychology, 11, 331349. preparation.
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their rel- Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and
evance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In proactively aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent char-
A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disor- acteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
ders (pp. 681706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Disciplines, 43(4), 495506.
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, Vogt, B. A., Finch, D. M., & Olson, C. R. (1992). Functional hetero-
N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared geneity in cingulate cortex: The anterior executive and posterior
apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, evaluative regions. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 435443.
54, 10311039. Von Bargen, D. M. (1983). Infant heart rate: A review of research and
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New methodology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 115149.
York: New York University Press. Wachs, T. D. (1987). Specificity of environmental action as manifest
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. G. (1968). Temperament and behav- in environmental correlates of infants mastery motivation. Devel-
ior disorders in children. New York: New York University Press. opmental Psychology, 23, 782790.
166 Temperament

Wachs, T. D. (2000). Necessary but not suf ficient: The respective roles Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., Yaeger, A., & Shinar, O. (2001). Family risk
of single and multiple inf luences on individual development. factors and adolescent substance use: Moderation effects for tem-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. perament dimensions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 283297.
Wachs, T. D., & Bates, J. E. (2001). Temperament. In G. Bremner & Wimer, R. E., & Wimer, C. C. (1985). Animal behavior genetics: A
A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development: search for the biological foundations of behavior. Annual Review
Handbooks of developmental psychology (pp. 465501). Malden, of Psychology, 36, 171218.
MA: Blackwell. Wolff, P. H. (1965). The development of attention in young infants.
Wachs, T. D., & Plomin, R. (1991). Overview of current models and Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 118, 830.
research. In T. D. Wachs & R. Plomin (Eds.), Conceptualization Wooton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997).
and measurement of organism-environment interaction (pp. 18). Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: The mod-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. erating role of callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: Gen- and Clinical Psychology, 65, 301308.
eral and specific factors of emotional experience and their rela- Wundt, W. (1903). Grundzuge der physiologischen psychologie [Lec-
tion to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441476. tures on human and animal psychology] (5th ed., Vol. 3). Leipzig,
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). The measurement and mismea- Germany: W. Engelmann.
surement of mood: Recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Per- Zuckerman, M. (1984). Sensation seeking: A comparative approach
sonality Assessment, 68, 267296. to a human trait. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 413471.
Webb, E. (1915). Character and intelligence. British Journal of Psy- Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of personality. New York:
chology, 1, 3. Cambridge University Press.
Werner, E. E. (1985). Resilient offspring of alcoholics: A longitudinal Zuckerman, M. (1995). Good and bad humors: Biochemical bases of
study from birth to age 18. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 47, 3440. personality and its disorders. Psychological Science, 6, 325332.
Wertlieb, D., Weigel, C., Springer, T., & Feldstein, M. (1987). Tem-
perament as a moderator of childrens stressful experiences.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 234245.
CHAPTER 4

Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases


JEROME KAGAN and NATHAN A. FOX

BIOLOGY AND BRAIN MATURATION 169 IRRITABILITY IN INFANCY 193


BIOLOGY AND TEMPERAMENT 170 TEMPERAMENTS IN OLDER CHILDREN 195
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND 172 Reaction to Unfamiliarity 196
Freuds Inf luence 174 INHIBITED VERSUS UNINHIBITED CHILDREN 198
RENAISSANCE OF TEMPERAMENTAL IDEAS 176 The Biological Bases for Reaction to Unfamiliarity 199
Animal Research 177 The Functions of the Amygdala 200
NEUROCHEMISTRY AND TEMPERAMENT 178 HIGH AND LOW REACTIVITY TO
THE COMPLEXITY OF BRAIN-BEHAVIOR UNFAMILIARITY 202
RELATIONS 181 Childhood Derivatives of High and Low Reactivity 202
Specificity 182 Inf luence of Experience 204
Agreements and Disagreements 183 Biological Assessments 205
SOURCES OF EVIDENCE 184 Temperaments Constrain 209
Behavioral Observations 184 TEMPERAMENT AND ATTACHMENT 210
ADULT VERBAL DESCRIPTIONS 185 PSYCHOPATHOLOGY AND TEMPERAMENT 211
SUMMARY 188 ETHNICITY AND TEMPERAMENT 214
CONTINUOUS TRAITS OR CATEGORIES 189 TEMPERAMENT AND MORAL AFFECTS 215
CURRENT VIEWS OF THE INFANT 190 CONCLUSION 216
Rothbarts Dimensions 191 REFERENCES 217

An unreserved acceptance of the idea that biological philosophers and assume that mental and bodily events
processes contribute to psychological phenomena has are joined in as seamless a unity as color, shape, and mo-
waxed and waned over time. Although attributing a feel- tion in the conscious perception of a cloud at sunset.
ing of fatigue to a bacterial infection is currently non- The basis for denying biology a significant role in
controversial, the suggestion that a dysphoric mood mood and behavior during the middle third of the twen-
could be due, in part, to an inherited physiology encoun- tieth century was the understandable desire, especially
tered more resistance during the last century. One his- among Americans, to minimize biological variation
torical source of the skepticism was the decision by among varied immigrant or ethnic groups. This ideology
Greek philosophers two millennia ago to separate soul was in the service of defending the optimistic hope that
and body rather than to follow the classic Chinese proper family experience and education could create a

Jerome Kagan is indebted to his recent colleagues, Institute, the COUQ Foundation, and past support from the
Nancy Snidman, Mark McManis, Sue Woodward, and William T. Grant and John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur
Vali Kahn, and, earlier colleagues, Doreen Arcus and Foundations. Nathan Fox thanks Peter Marshall, Louis
J. Steven Reznick, for their wisdom in the research on high Schmidt, and Heather Henderson, and acknowledges
and low reactive infants. Kagan also acknowledges current research support from NICHD in the form of Grant
research support from the Bial Foundation, Metanexus HD 17895.

167
168 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

community of citizens that possessed roughly equivalent true. When knowledge of the mediating event is incom-
ability, motivation, civility, and capacity for happiness plete, as it is for most propositions that relate brain
(May, 1959). physiology to psychology, considerable faith is required.
Second, a broad conceptual moat must be jumped The resulting feeling of disquiet mars the aesthetic feel-
when the vocabularies of biology and psychology occur ing that is a distinguishing feature of a completely satis-
in the same sentence. No one has difficulty understand- fying explanation.
ing The boy ran away because he felt afraid because That is why psychologists continue to disagree on
everyones experience validates the association between the relative contributions of genetic programs and the
a feeling of fear and the act of fleeing. But many find it epigenetic events following conception to the observed
more difficult to understand The boy ran away because phenotype, even though both influences are always for-
of a limbic discharge because they have not con- mative. A gardener who plants tomato seeds in the
sciously experienced that brain event, and, further, such spring cannot know the exact size, coloring, or taste of
a sentence occurs rarely in social discourse. Hence, the plant picked months later because of the unpre-
both the sense and referential meanings of the second dictability of temperature, rainfall, and pests. But she
sentence are less clear and a causal association between knows with certainty that the fruit picked in the fall will
flight and a limbic discharge seems less valid. Ease of be a tomato and not an apple.
assimilation is always easier when the statement con- All psychological phenotypes are the products of cas-
tains ideas to which a listener is accustomed. Fifteenth- cades of events, many unpredictable, but the genome of
century Europeans would have experienced far less the child constrains seriously the envelope of possible
difficulty than modern ones in understanding, and ac- profiles that a particular child might display. It seems
cepting as true, the declaration: The woman died be- wise, therefore, to acknowledge, as the ancients did, that
cause she was bewitched. genes and experience act coordinately and cease quar-
A third obstacle to combining biological and psycho- reling over which force is stronger.
logical terms, which is related to the issue of different The emergent nature of psychological events from bi-
vocabularies, is inherent in all emergent phenomena; the ology is analogous to the temperature and pressure of a
tides offer an example. Most people who have had the closed container of gas. Pressure and temperature de-
relevant education believe that the changing height of scribe the emergent consequences of large numbers of
the oceans during each day is due to changes in the grav- molecular collisions and are inappropriate terms for a
itational relation between the moon and the earth, even single molecule. The assumption that every psychologi-
though there is a bit of mystery surrounding the idea that cal phenomenon can be explained by or derived from the
the gravitational attraction between moon and earth af- activity of particular neuronal ensembles, as the tem-
fects the waterline at the beach. No feeling of mystery perature and pressure of a vessel of gas are explained by
or certainly much lessis engendered when the linked equations describing the collisions of large numbers of
phenomena are at the same level of description, as in atoms, is flawed because the motion of each atom is as-
The child cried after she fell, because our phenome- sumed to be independent of the motion of every other
nology supplies the mediating feeling of pain or surprise atom. This assumption does not apply to brains for each
that we know produces a cry. The contemporary public neuron is influenced by the activity of others. Anxiety
is ready to believe the recent, and surprising, declaration is a property of a person and not of the neurons that par-
that a bacterium (Heliobacter pylori) can cause ulcers ticipate in that emotional state. Thus, the scholar who
because it is easy to imagine how swarms of bacteria acknowledges that thought, feeling, and action depend
could devour the stomachs delicate mucosal lining. An on and emerge from brain events but who insists that
earlier generation was considerably more resistant to the these events must be described in a language different
psychosomatic hypothesis that conflict over dependency from the one that describes underlying brain processes
could produce ulcers because it was harder to imagine is not a metaphysical dualist. All of nature cannot be
how an unconscious psychological state could be respon- described with one vocabulary because brains have
sible for this materialistic condition. qualitatively different structures than schemata and
When the mind must leap from gravitational force to semantic networks.
tides or from psychological states to ulcers, and the in- This argument is not a rejection of attempts to under-
termediate events are not completely clear, people must stand the biological contributions to psychological
rely on faith in authority to accept both statements as processes. Even though complete translation of the lat-
Biology and Brain Maturation 169

ter events into the language of the former is probably im- sensory modalities, motor programs, and language. The
possible, research that looks for the biological correlates particular subset of features activated in a person at a
has advantages. First, the products of this work deepen particular time and place is not knowable until the incen-
our understanding of the molar events. The discovery tive is specified. Further, different incentives will acti-
that connections between temporal and frontal struc- vate different parts of the large network. The features of
tures mature during the last half of the 1st year implies the network for thunderstorms evoked in a person
that there should be major improvements in working caught outside in a summer storm are different from
memory at this time, and that inference leads to a new those activated when the same person is in an office
conceptualization of the phenomenon Piaget called ob- building or flying in an airplane as lightning is scarring
ject permanence (Diamond, 1990). the night sky. Thus, a summer storm can activate a large
A failure to find expected correspondences between number of representations and no member of this family
brain and behavior often provides fruitful seeds for new is knowable until the scientist intervenes with a probe to
ideas. The fact that lesions of the dorsolateral prefrontal measure it. No member is more essential than any other,
cortex impair working memory, but do not impair the re- and none is active when the person is sipping coffee on a
trieval of motor habits invites a distinction among dif- sunny June morning. This means that the neural pattern
ferent kinds of memories. The fact that an intact of activation in a person lying in a positron-emission to-
amygdala is necessary for a rat to display a potentiated mography (PET) or functional magnetic resonance im-
startle reflex to a loud sound presented after a condi- aging (fMRI ) scanner looking at pictures of snakes is
tioned stimulus, but is not necessary for the startle re- not to be regarded as the true or only neural configura-
flex invites a distinction between two types of startles. tion that these stimuli would provoke. The same pictures
Thus, learning more about the relation of brain activity would create a different brain state if the person were
to psychological events contributes to theory, even looking at them on a television screen in his living room.
though a complete translation of mind to brain is proba- There is no master clock for the universe and there is no
bly not possible. Gods eye view of the brains response to an incentive
One reason why the description of a profile of neu- because each person has a particular frame of mind and
ronal activation cannot be a substitute for psychological brain state in each class of situation, and that frame af-
structure is that the context and the agents past experi- fects which neuronal ensembles and representations will
ence determine which particular neural patterns, and be activated. Put differently, a number of possible brain
therefore which psychological structures will be acti- states are possible the moment a new event occurs. The
vated. No sample of adults of the same age, sex, social context, the immediately prior state, and the persons
class, and health, tested at the same time of day pro- temperament and past history combine to select one out-
duces identical profiles of brain activity to a particular come from the set of candidates: That is the phenomenon
stimulus because each person brings a different history scientists measure. Thus, probabilistic quantum princi-
to the context of evaluation. Two groups of adults sub- ples operate the moment an event occurs; classical prin-
ject to similar torture provide a more complex example ciples take over milliseconds later when one outcome
of this principle. One group comprised well-educated has been chosen.
political activists, the second consisted of less well-
educated, apolitical men. Although all prisoners experi-
enced similar acts of torture, more of the apolitical men BIOLOGY AND BRAIN MATURATION
developed anxiety, depression, or posttraumatic stress
disorder (Basoglu et al., 1997). The fact that fewer po- Biological processes affect psychological growth in two
litical activists developed these symptoms has to be due obvious but very different ways. Initially, the lawful
in part to their intellectual commitment to the causes maturation of the nervous system is accompanied by
that led to their incarceration. But the psychological universal changes in emotion, cognition, and behavior.
state we call intellectual commitment to a cause cannot For example, the 1st year consists of two important tran-
be translated into sentences whose words only describe sitions. One occurs at 2 to 3 months, and the second at 7
brain processes. to 12 months of age. The first transition is accompanied
A psychological representation of a class of events is by disappearance of newborn reflexes, endogenous smil-
a hypothetical network consisting of many interrelated ing, a decrease in crying, the appearance of circadian
features that can include representations from several rhythm, and the enhancement of recognition memory.
170 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

The disappearance of the reflexes is believed to be due BIOLOGY AND TEMPERAMENT


to cortical inhibition of brain stem neurons (Volpe,
1995). Although descending axons from the supplemen- Some of the stable psychological variation found among
tary motor area reach the brain stem and spinal cord tar- children in all cultures represents a second domain in
gets before birth, actual synaptic contacts do not appear which biology influences growth. This idea, which is the
until 2 to 3 months after birth (Kostovic, 1990). sense meaning of the concept temperament, comprises
The transition between 7 and 12 months is marked by the primary focus of this chapter. However, the biologi-
the ability to activate a representation for a past event cal processes and psychological experiences that medi-
that is no longer present, hold that representation online, ate the maturation of a particular class of behavior are
and relate it to features in the current situation in a hy- usually different from those that are responsible for the
pothetical process that is called working memory. The variation in that behavior. Fear of unfamiliar adults in
enhancement in working memory is accompanied by a infancy provides a nice example of this claim. It is be-
spurt of growth and differentiation in both pyramidal lieved that the display of distress to and avoidance of
and inhibitory interneurons in the prefrontal cortex strangers appears in most children by 7 to 9 months as a
(Kostovic, 1990). Not surprisingly, this anatomical result of maturation of circuits from limbic sites to the
growth is accompanied by increased glucose uptake in frontal lobe (Diamond, 1990; Kagan, 1994). The varia-
the lateral frontal cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal tion in the intensity and chronicity of fearful behavior to
cortex (Chugani, 1994; Huttenlocher, 1979). strangers is believed to be due to differences in the
The 2nd year is characterized by the initial compre- chemistry of the amygdala and experiences with
hension and expression of speech, the capacity to infer strangers rather than completion of the circuits that link
selected mental and feeling states in others, representa- the limbic structures with the frontal lobe (Kagan,
tions of actions that are prohibited, and the conscious 1994). Although inquiry into human temperaments is
awareness of some of the self s feelings and intentions. becoming more popular, there is no consensus on basic
One basis for these changes is the growth that occurs in terms, measurement procedures, or robust generaliza-
the neurons in layer three of the prefrontal cortex, which tions. Hence, it is not possible in one chapter to summa-
elongate and grow spines. Layer three represents the rize, in an integrative style, all that has been published.
neurons that participate in the corpus callosum, which The interested reader is referred to several books that
unites the hemispheres. When the neurons of one hemi- present the diverse views on this theme; they include
sphere make contact with those of the opposite hemi- Kohnstamm, Bates, and Rothbart (1989), Strelau and
sphere through the corpus callosum, the speed of Angleitner (1991), Bates and Wachs (1994), and Plomin
integrating information from both hemispheres is accel- and McClearn (1993). The first task in every science is
erated (Mrzljak & Uylings, 1990). to categorize the phenomena that define its domain with
Finally, after the 2nd year of life, over the next 3 or 4 concepts that capture natures plan. Four cognitive bi-
years, there is maturation of at least five cognitive abil- ases interfere with this assignment. These include (a)
ities: (1) the reliable integration of past with present, (2) the tendency to award priority to easily observed fea-
anticipation of the future, (3) appreciation of causality, tures, ( b) the desire to honor parsimony by inventing cat-
(4) enhanced reliance on semantic categories, and (5) egories with the fewest number of features, (c) a
detection of shared relations between events and cate- preference for concepts that imply temporal stability
gories. These changes are accompanied by a dramatic rather than change, and (d) the urge to believe that the
increase in the total cortical surface so that the human inferred category refers to a real entity in nature and not
brain attains 90% of its adult weight by the time the simply a clever invention.
child is 8 years old (Giedd et al., 1996). Further, the bal- The sense meaning of temperament held by many, but
ance between the number of new synapses formed and not all, scientists refers to a biologically based bias for
the number eliminated shifts after the sixth birthday to correlated clusters of feelings, thoughts, and actions
a ratio that favors the latter process. There is a parallel that appear during childhood, but not always in the
increase in myelination and an increase in the intercon- opening months, and are sculpted by varied rearing en-
nectedness that involves both hemispheres, anterior and vironments into a large but still limited number of traits
posterior cortical sites, as well as cortical and subcorti- that comprise an individuals personality profile. Hence,
cal structures. This story is being told in several chap- the stable variation in behaviors and emotions observed
ters in this Handbook. in older children, adolescents, and adults are personality
Biology and Temperament 171

traits, not temperamental biases, although the latter porally delimited pattern of brain activation, produced
make a contribution to the profile that emerges later in by an incentive, does not necessarily reflect a persons
development. In addition, students of human tempera- conscious feelings. Nor can the circuit represent the se-
ments exclude stable variation in cognitive functions quences of thoughts, preparation for action, and auto-
from this category because of the typical failure to find nomic reactions that will occur subsequently. All of
consistent correlations between a temperamental bias these events are referents for the psychological state.
and quality of perceptual, memorial, or inferential abili- The psychological meaning of fear is not a momentary
ties. There is no good evidence to suggest that irritabil- brain state, even though a brain state accompanies the
ity, activity, or reactivity in infants predicts differences psychological state and, therefore, cannot be ignored.
in IQ score or other cognitive abilities in later child- The neurophysiological phenomena should be given their
hood. The independence of these two domains is reason- own conceptual label. Three different sources of evi-
able because the presumed physiological bases for dence are often used to infer a fear state: (1) a behav-
differences in perception, memory, and reasoning are ioral profile, (2) a pattern of physiological reactions, and
different from those that mediate the temperamental bi- (3) a self-report. At the moment, the correlations among
ases. The biological biases that are the foundations of these three referents are not high enough to treat them as
temperaments can be due to heritable variation in redundant. There are two complementary, but different,
anatomy or neurochemistry or the result of prenatal frames for descriptions of human emotional states. One
events that are not strictly genetic in origin. Each of the originates in phenomenology, the other in physiology.
many temperamental profiles that has been or will be The concepts, their interrelations, and their time
discovered in the future is a concept. The more knowl- courses are sufficiently different in the two frames that
edge that accumulates around the concept, the more it is wise to distinguish clearly between the words that
complete the cognitive appreciation of the concept and are presumed to refer to the same state.
the events to which it refers. However, the biological This position is neither a defense of traditional mind-
features that enrich a temperamental concept, and are brain duality nor an attack on biological reduction. It
necessary for the emergent psychological profile, cannot merely states that all psychological phenomena, includ-
replace it. A small number of neurobiologists believe ing temperament, are emergent with respect to underly-
that one day the idea of consciousness will be reduced to ing biological events. A particular PET scan showing
a particular set of neural activities, implying that con- high metabolic activity in areas of the visual cortex does
sciousness is no more than a network of circuits. The not explain completely why a person perceives a small,
reason for rejecting that premise is the same as the ra- red sphere moving slowly to the right rather than a large,
tionale for rejecting the idea that a chemical description gray background moving slowly to the left. Each percep-
of the toxins produced by the malarial parasites is equiv- tion, behavior, emotion, and thought represents more
alent to a description of the patients malaise. A tornado than the brain circuits that are necessary for its actual-
has a shape, speed, direction of motion, and color, and ization, a position Sperry (1977) maintained during the
these features are not derivable from lengthy descrip- final years of his productive career. Thus, the descrip-
tions of groups of air molecules in the tornado. Simi- tion of a temperamental category is not equivalent to a
larly, the transparent quality of a pane of glass is not description of the biological features that comprise part
explained by a description of the chemical structure of of its foundation. A wave is more than the moving parti-
silica. Genes select or stabilize a form, but unless we cles of water that comprise it (Einstein & Infeld, 1938).
know the exact conditions under which the organism is This chapter first presents a brief historical perspec-
developing it is not possible to predict or understand the tive on the concept of temperament and the reasons for
final form (Goodwin, 1994). We inherit dispositions, its appeal, and then a description of the nodes of agree-
not destinies . . . lives are not simple consequences of ment and disagreement among investigators. The two
genetic consignments. Genetic determinism is improba- most important controversies involve the validity of
ble for simple acts of the fruit fly, implausible for com- parental reports of childrens behavior and whether tem-
plex human behavior (Rose, 1995, p. 648). peramental qualities should be conceived of as continua
The remarkable advances in neuroscience have or categories. The heart of the chapter is a summary of
tempted some to hope that many psychological concepts the most robust generalizations regarding the tempera-
will eventually be replaced with a specification of a mental characteristics of irritability in infants and so-
neural circuit. Such optimism is not warranted. A tem- ciability and shyness in older children. The final
172 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

sections consider, more briefly, the relevance of tem- drier in the fall, a melancholic mood became more
perament to attachment, psychopathology, ethnicity, and prevalent. Because humans lived in different climates
morality. The chapter focuses on infancy and early and ate different foods, they differed in these tempera-
childhood and does not consider in detail the interesting mental qualities.
research on the temperamental contributions to the be- Although the Chinese view of human nature articu-
haviors of adolescents and adults. lated two millennia earlier shared some features with
Galens ideas, it differed from it in several important
ways (Yosida, 1973). First, the critical balance was
among sources of energy rather than the bodily humors.
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND The energy of the universecalled chiis regulated by
a complementary relation between the active initiating
The Greeks and Romans believed that a balance among force of yang and the more passive, completing force of
the four humors of yellow and black bile, blood, and yin. The two forces must be in balance for optimal phys-
phlegm created an opposition in each of two comple- iological and psychological functioning. Like the
mentary universal qualities: warm versus cool and dry Greeks, the Chinese linked the emotion of sadness with
versus moist (Siegel, 1968). These qualities were related autumn, joy with early summer, and fear with winter.
to the four fundamental substances in the world: fire, But the Greeks would have been surprised that the Chi-
air, earth, and water. The Greeks assumedwithout a nese linked anger with springApril and May are the
detailed appreciation of genetics or physiologythat the months of Galens sanguine temperament. However, the
balance among these qualities created an inner state re- more important fact is that the Chinese were not inter-
sponsible for the observed variation in rationality, emo- ested in temperamental types. Because the energy of
tionality, and behavior. Children were impulsive and chi is always changing, a persons moods and behav-
irrational because they were born with an excess of the ioral style cannot be too permanent. The notion of a per-
moist quality. son inheriting a stable emotional bias was inconsistent
Galen, an extraordinarily perceptive second-century with the Chinese premise of continual transformation.
physician born in Asia Minor, elaborated these Hippo- A person might be sad temporarily, but not because he
cratic ideas by positing nine temperamental types de- or she was a melancholic type.
rived from the four humors (Roccatagliatta, 1986). The Galens inferences, which remained popular in Eu-
ideal personality was exquisitely balanced on the com- rope until the end of the nineteenth century, were not se-
plementary characteristics of warm-cool and dry-moist. riously different from contemporary speculations that
In the remaining four less ideal types one pair of quali- the brains of schizophrenics might possess an excess of
ties dominated the complementary pair; for example, dopamine while those of depressives may have insuffi-
warm and moist dominated cool and dry. These four cient norepinephrine (Healy, 1997). Kant (1785/1959)
were the temperamental categories Galen called melan- accepted Galens four types with only minor changes
cholic, sanguine, choleric, and phlegmatic. Each was the but distinguished between affect and action because he
result of an excess of one of the bodily humors that pro- recognized the imperfect relation between invisible, in-
duced, in turn, the imbalance in qualities: The melan- ternal processes and overt behavior. Kant believed that
cholic was cool and dry because of an excess of black humans possessed a will that could control the behav-
bile, the sanguine was warm and moist because of an ex- ioral consequences of strong desires.
cess of blood, the choleric was warm and dry because of This contrast was captured in the nineteenth century
an excess of yellow bile, and the phlegmatic was cool in the comparison between temperament and character.
and moist because of an excess of phlegm. The former referred to inherited emotional biases, the
Although the concentrations of the four humors and latter to the expression of these biases in actions that
the relative dominance of the derived qualities were in- were a function of both life experiences and inborn tem-
herent in each persons physiology, they were, nonethe- perament. The pragmatist, for example, was a character
less, susceptible to the influence of external events, type who could possess either a sanguine or a melan-
especially climate and diet. The body, naturally, became cholic temperament.
warmer and more moist in the spring; hence, people be- Two centuries later, Roback (1931) modernized
came more sanguine. When the body became cooler and Kants views by suggesting that individuals inherited, to
Historical Background 173

different degrees, dispositions for certain desires and The first transformation of these ideas was an expan-
emotions. But, unlike animals, humans could control be- sion of the number of revealing physical features and,
haviors that violated their ethical standards; this is more important, an appreciation that these features
Robacks version of Freuds belief that ego tames id. were only indirect signs of the real, but still unknown,
The sanguine type must inhibit, occasionally, the ten- causes. In a book that enjoyed eight editions, Joseph
dency to act impulsively because of strong feelings; the Simms (1887) awarded the face more diagnostic power
melancholic must suppress the urge to become anxious than Paul Ekman or Carroll Izard would have dared.
and withdrawn. Thus, a temperamentally sanguine per- Even American schoolteachers were indoctrinated with
son who has made too many ill-advised decisions can these ideas: Jessica Fowler (1897) wrote a manual to
become overly cautious; a melancholic who has learned help teachers diagnose their young pupils psychological
to inhibit fear may appear to others to be spontaneously qualities. A veneration for elders was predictable
sociable. The idea that the character type does not al- from excessively drooping eyes.
ways provide a reliable insight into temperament is the Cesare Lombroso (1911) and Ernst Kretschmer
essence of Jungs distinction between each persons hid- (1925), in classic treatises, suggested an association be-
den anima and public persona. tween body type, on the one hand, and crime or mental
Nineteenth-century essays on temperament focused disease, on the other. Lombroso acknowledged that
on the biology of the brain and searched for visible signs crime had social and climatic correlates, but claimed
of that biology on the surface of the body. Franz Gall that adults who fell at one of the extremes of a normal
(1835) incurred the enmity of a segment of his commu- body type were more often represented among crimi-
nity by suggesting that variation in human intentions nals, and dark-haired men were more likely to be crimi-
and emotions, derived from differences in brain tissue, nals than those who were blonde. Kretschmer invented
could actually be detected with measurements of the new namesasthenic, pyknic, and athleticfor the
skull. Galls crass materialism angered many colleagues three classical body physiques and awarded differential
who did not believe that a persons character was deter- vulnerability to major mental illness to the first two
mined by brain tissue and, therefore, was not control- body types. Schizophrenics were more often tall, thin,
lable by each agents will. A second reason for the narrow-faced asthenics; manic depressives were more
hostility toward Gall is that many nineteenth-century often chubby, broad-faced, pyknic types.
scholars did not believe that the anatomy of the brain These speculations formed the basis for Sheldons
had any implications for human behavior because psy- (1940) famous book on personality and physique. Shel-
chology was not part of natural science. don measured a large number of morphological dimen-
Spurzheim (1834) consolidated Galls ideas by sions from the photographs of 4,000 college men and
retaining the essential premise of a location for each pri- collapsed the resulting 76 categories into three basic
mary human characteristic and, reflecting nineteenth- body types, each rated on a 7-point scale and each hav-
century prejudice, assigning more space in the cranial ing a corresponding set of psychological qualities. The
cavity to emotional than to intellectual processes. Love tall, thin ectomorph was an introvert; the chubby endo-
was in the cerebellum, aggression in the temporal lobe, morph was an extrovert; and the broad, athletically built
and timidity in the upper lateral and posterior part of mesomorph was energetically assertive.
the head near the parietal area. The vigorous positivism Sheldons work began as the eugenics movement in
in Spurzheims arguments was motivated by the need America had reached a crest and was published the year
to expunge metaphysical and religious ideas from scien- that the Nazis were threatening Europe. The idea that
tific explanations of human nature; it was time to inherited physical qualities, associated with different
place human behavior in its proper place as a part of nat- ethnic groups, were associated with human behavior was
ural law. too close to Hitlers version of Aryan types, and this re-
Thus, by the end of the nineteenth century most search, as well as a growing eugenics movement,
scholars had accepted the fact that psychiatry rested on stopped suddenly. Promotion of the formerly popular
biology. Listen to Adolph Meyer in 1897: We cannot idea that the obvious physical differences among Scan-
conceive a disorder of the mind without a disorder of dinavians, Italians, Jews, and Blacks were linked to in-
function of those cell mechanisms which embody that telligence and morality had become a sign of both
part of the mind (1897/1994, p. 44). irrationality and amoral prejudice. The abrupt end to
174 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

public discussion of these hypotheses is not surprising; ory holds that depressives have low levels of central nor-
tucked away in Sheldons book is the provocative sug- epinephrine. The mechanism of one of the therapeutic
gestion that Blacks are more often aggressive meso- drugs acts to increase the concentration of norepineph-
morphs, whereas Jews are more often intellectual rine in the synaptic cleft. Moreover, infants differ in the
ectomorphs. Ernest Hootons (1939) book, which sug- vigor of motor activity and loudness of vocalizations.
gested that some bodily constitutions were naturally in- Some 4-month-olds thrash their limbs and squeal with
ferior and linked to criminal behavior, had a defensive delight; others lie passive and quiet. A high energy level
tone because he was aware of how unpopular this view leaps to mind as the best description of the former in-
had become to many Americans. Temperamental ideas, fants. In the classic monograph on hysteria, Breuer and
which had enjoyed the support of professors, presidents, Freud (1956) wrote, Differences which make up a
and corporation heads during the 1st decade of the cen- mans natural temperament are based on profound dif-
tury, were forced underground for almost 50 years. ferences in his nervous systemon the degree to which
the functionally quiescent cerebral elements liberate en-
Freuds Inf luence ergy (p. 198). The creative element in Freuds thinking
was to award the free-floating energy of libido an origin
Freud (1933/1965) was a critical figure in this story be- and a target in sexuality, while accepting the popular
cause he made important changes in the remnants of view that heredity influenced the total amount of libido
Galens views. First, he substituted one bodily sub- possessed. Although Freuds early writings awarded in-
stance, the energy of the libido, for the four humors. fluence both to temperamental differences in amount of
This idea of psychological energy, the sense meaning of libido and excitability of the nervous system, as well as
libido, was not a completely novel notion. Nineteenth- childhood experiences, the latter ascended in impor-
century physicians had elaborated the ancient belief that tance in his later writings and, accordingly, the tem-
the amount of energy was an inherited personal quality. peramental contribution faded.
The vis nervosa, an idea related to the eighteenth- The current popularity of the premise that childhood
century notion that inanimate objects varied in their experiences are part of the causal web in adult anxiety
ability to retain and give off heat, was less abundant in and depression prevents a proper appreciation of the rev-
those unfortunate persons who developed fears, depres- olutionary character of Freuds ideas. Although the an-
sion, and neurasthenia. cients were open to the suggestion that psychological
Pavlov (1928) also exploited this idea to explain why variation in the normal range could be influenced by
some dogs became conditioned easily while others, who childhood experienceeven Plato accepted that argu-
resisted the laboratory procedures, were difficult to mentthe serious mental afflictions of depression,
condition. Pavlov thought that the former group of ani- mania, and schizophrenia were regarded as solely physi-
mals had a stronger nervous system, permitting them to ological in origin. Although the ancients believed that
be more resilient to the unfamiliarity of the laboratory some environmental factors were potent, including air,
conditioning procedures. Pavlov intended that descrip- diet, exercise, rest, and excretion and retention of fluid,
tion to be flattering, because functional and adaptive none of these causes was social in nature.
evaluations, which were absent in Galen, colored tem- By softening the division between serious mental dis-
peramental concepts after Darwins seminal work. order and normal variation in worry and sadness, Freud
Galen had written as if each psychological type sought persuaded many that both a terror of leaving home and
an adaptation to fit his or her bodily humor. Pavlov in- worry about ones debts could be derivatives of the same
serted the evaluative ethic of adaptation and implied conflict. The assumption of an experiential basis for
that some temperaments functioned better than others. fears and anxieties that was appropriate for allevery-
The sanguine was the best type; the melancholic, who one felt guilt over sexual and hostile motivesimplied
had a weaker brain, was the least desirable. that every person could develop a phobia. Freud
The idea that individuals vary in psychological en- (1909/1950) let his readers believe that little Hans
ergy and, therefore, in strength of brain activity may was no different temperamentally from any other child;
seem odd to modern readers. However, norepinephrine, his extreme fear of horses was the result of very unusual
the primary neurotransmitter of the sympathetic nerv- experiences in his family. It is of interest that contempo-
ous system, maintains body temperature by producing rary reports on childrens phobias have returned to the
bodily energy (Paxinos, 1990). Current psychiatric the- notions prevalent decades before Freud. Clinical cases in
Historical Background 175

psychiatric journals are now described as if they were could produce more certain facts, and therefore, a
physiological diseases to be treated with drugs; there is deeper understanding of the mechanisms behind the
little or no discussion of conflict, trauma, or early fam- childs learning of new habits. This knowledge would
ily experience. serve the needs of an egalitarian society.
Psychoanalytic theory slowly turned minds away The European mind at the end of the nineteenth and
from a category of person who was especially vulner- the beginning of the twentieth century was more
able to acquiring a phobia to the idea of environmental friendly to temperament because it held a different
encounters that produce fear. The adjective fearful now ethic. The European city enjoyed greater loyalty than
became a continuous dimension on which any person the same size community in geographically mobile
could be placed. Because all individuals experienced America. The residents of Paris, London, and Florence
conflict, anyone could become phobic. The idea of a vul- took more pride from their cities long history than did
nerable temperamental type was replaced with the no- citizens in New York, Philadelphia, or Washington. The
tion of unusually stressful experiences. vitality and stability of the community had precedence
A metaphor that captures this contrast is a bridge that over the upwardly mobile achieving individual in Amer-
collapses under a load. The traditional assumption was ican society. That preference was correlated with a de-
that all bridges must carry loads of varying weight; sire to maintain social harmony through citizen
hence, a bridge that collapsed under a load that was in acceptance of and conformity to local mores and Euro-
the normal range must have been structurally weak. peans were receptive to the idea that variation in stabil-
This is the temperamental premise. Freud, and espe- ity could be due to biology. Americans wanted to deny
cially his followers, argued that, most of the time, the biology any force. Every person with an intact brain and
collapse was caused by an unusually heavy load. The body could actualize her goals if she worked hard and
psychological loads included childhood seduction, harsh exploited her inherent cleverness. No persons accept-
socialization of hostility and sexuality, loss of a love ob- ability to others or future success should be shackled by
ject, and fear of the anger of an authoritarian parent. their inheritance.
Even though there are many more children who are so- The appeal and acceptability of scientific ideas are
cialized harshly by autocratic parents or rejected by in- always influenced by the societal context in which they
different ones than there are hysterical patients, this appear. Darwins inference of natural selection, influ-
theoretical stance won admirers quickly because of po- enced by Malthuss suggestion that the increasing fecun-
litical factors. dity of populations would outstrip food supplies,
Many Americans were threatened when, after World overcame initial resistance quickly because many over-
War I, a number of prominent scientists joined by influ- crowded European cities contain large numbers of very
ential journalists suggested that some immigrants were poor families producing large numbers of children. A
less fit genetically than indigenous Americans (May, half century later, when many east coast American
1959). An opposing group of politically more liberal cities contained crowded ghettoes of European immi-
scientists and journalists quieted this provocative claim grants, our society had to choose between scientific dec-
by suggesting that Pavlovs discoveries of conditioning larations claiming that the foreigners were genetically
meant that all children were essentially similar at birth compromised and the arguments of egalitarian scholars
and conditioned experiences supplied the only shaping who argued that the habits and values of the immigrants
hand. McDougalls (1908) acerbic critique of this posi- were experientially based. The latter explanation be-
tion in his text Social Psychology was drowned out by the came ascendant for almost 50 years until a new wave of
rising voice of Watsonian behaviorism. America cele- immigrants with color arrived. But this time the Ameri-
brates the individual, who, through wit and persever- can economy had less need for their labor and the bio-
ance, makes or invents a reliable product that has logical sciences had made important discoveries.
pragmatic value. Thomas Edison and Henry Ford are Thus, the current receptivity to temperamental ideas
prototypic American heroes. The most celebrated Amer- cannot be understood without acknowledging history
ican scientists of this same era performed laboratory ex- and recent theory and research in neuroscience, psychol-
periments yielding hard facts presumed to have ogy, and psychiatry. The period from 1910 to 1970 was
implications for human life. Many American psycholo- characterized by the conviction that, excepting the small
gists studied the phenomenon of learning in rats because number of brain-damaged children, most were funda-
this animal permitted experimental manipulations that mentally similar, and the development of different skills
176 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

and personalities was due, in the main, to experience, tions of almost every stable behavior. Readers will re-
especially conditioned habits. This popular and domi- call, for example, that Skinner (1981) suggested that op-
nant premise was shaken by several historical events. erant conditioning principles could explain the childs
First, the conceptual gap between the principles of acquisition of speech. Contemporary neuroscientists are
classical and operant conditioning and the novel forms being equally creative when they propose neurophysio-
observed in childrens speech and behavior became dif- logical bases for many diverse and complex behaviors.
ficult to repress. The resulting dissonance led, over We do not criticize this inventive energy, but only note
time, to a broad dissatisfaction with the traditional that, as with the earlier behavioristic accounts, most of
view, but still no replacement. At the same time, Piagets these explanations will turn out to be either too simple
(1950) ideas of stages of psychological development be- or simply incorrect (Hu & Fox, 1988).
came popular. Although Piaget insisted on the impor-
tance of the childs actions in the world, his arguments
imposed some constraint on the effectiveness of experi- RENAISSANCE OF TEMPERAMENTAL IDEAS
ence. No 2-month-old could possess an object concept
no matter what his experiences. Although Piaget did not An important reason for the return of temperamental
favor the biological determinism implied by the concept ideas was the bold, influential work of Thomas and
of maturation, his writings created a renewed enthusi- Chess (1977). Although Solomon Diamond (1957) an-
asm for maturational processes. Chomskys ascerbic ticipated the current interest in temperament at about
critique of Skinners explanation of language acquisi- the same time that Alexander Thomas and Stella Chess
tion abetted the maturational argument. Thus, the com- published their first papers, historical forces awarded
munity became receptive to the influences of biology priority to the two psychiatrists because their categories
and the older ideas on temperament. were more closely related to parental experiences with
When the dissemination of PET and fMRI data prom- infants and to later childhood pathology. It is useful to
ised an eager audience objective quantification of the recall their strategy of discovery. Thomas and Chess
brains biology, the dam that was repressing an enthusi- (1977) conducted, at regular intervals, lengthy inter-
asm for biology burst. Only 2 decades ago, the probabil- views with well-educated parents of infants and inferred
ity was high that a paper on behavioral genetics nine temperamental dimensions together with three
submitted to Child Development or Developmental Psy- more abstract categories from those interviews. The
chology would be rejected. Today, similar reports are nine temperamental dimensions were: (1) activity level;
usually accepted by referees because of a change in the (2) rhythmicity or regularity of bodily functions like
communitys premises. The essential data have not hunger, sleep, and elimination; (3) initial reaction to un-
changed very much; what has changed is the credibility familiarity, especially approach or withdrawal; (4) ease
of the importance of biological processes. of adaptation to new situations; (5) responsiveness to
There is, however, a danger in an excessive enthusi- subtle stimulus events; (6) amount of energy; (7) domi-
asm for biological determinism. A nativistic view of the nant mood, primarily whether happy or irritable; (8) dis-
infant, which is gaining popularity, resembles the pre- tractibility; and (9) attention span and persistence.
formationist assumption that a tiny child was hidden in The three temperamental categories represented a
each sperm. Infants are being awarded cognitive talents profile on two or more of the nine dimensions. The most
that psychologists would have satirized 25 years ago. frequent category, about 40% of the sample, was the
The permissive attitude toward these claims could not easy child, who was regular in bodily activity and ap-
have occurred without the prior perceived failures of be- proached unfamiliar objects with a happy, engaging
haviorism and psychoanalytic theory, just as the popu- mood. The second, comprising about 15% of the sample,
larity of Picasso and other modernists required the prior was slow to warm up and, like the children Kagan,
idealism of Courbet and Monet. Picassos Nude in a Red Reznick, and Snidman (1988) called inhibited, they
Chair would not have been regarded as a great work of react to unfamiliarity with withdrawal and occasionally
art had not Western artists, during prior centuries, mild distress. The third category, comprising about 10%
painted serene, beautiful, unclothed women. of the sample, was called difficult and was character-
A quarter-century ago, psychologists loyal to stimu- ized by minimal regularity, frequent irritability, with-
lus response learning theory invented possible explana- drawal from unfamiliarity, and poor adaptation. This
Renaissance of Temperamental Ideas 177

category of child was most likely to develop psychiatric the dogs were less timid if they had been raised at home
symptomstwo-thirds had developed such symptoms rather than in the laboratory. Twenty years later, God-
by age 10. These three categories comprised about two- dard and Beilharz (1985) discovered that Labradors,
thirds of the Chess and Thomas sample; the remaining Australian kelpies, boxers, and German shepherds dif-
third were difficult to classify. fered in the avoidance of unfamiliar objects: The Ger-
Continuous evaluation of the children through the 5th man shepherds were the most timid, and the Labradors
year revealed minimal preservation of most of the di- were the least fearful.
mensions. The largest correlations, about .3, reflected Pavlov noted over 75 years ago that some dogs in his
stability across the preschool years. But there was not laboratory were unusually tame with humans while oth-
much predictability from early infancy to age 4. As a ers cowered when an adult made an unexpected move-
result, Thomas and Chess concluded that the nine tem- ment. Pavlov called the former dogs excitable and the
peramental dimensions, as phenotypes, were not very latter inhibited (Pavlov, 1928). Factor analyses of be-
stable. They revisited these subjects when they were be- havioral observations on over 15,000 dogs from 164 dif-
tween 18 and 22 years of age, using clinical interviews ferent breeds revealed a broad factor best interpreted as
and questionnaires to evaluate degree of adjustment. Al- a shy-bold continuum (Svartberg & Forkman, 2002).
though there was no relation between the possession of House cats, too, differ in timidity. The small propor-
an easy or a difficult temperament in the first 2 years tion of cats who consistently withdraw to novelty and
and later adult adjustment, the children who had been fail to attack rats have a lower threshold of excitability
classed as difficult in the 3rd and 4th years were judged in specific areas of the amygdala than the majority of
to be less able to cope with life stresses than those chil- cats who do not withdraw and generally attack rats
dren who had an easy temperament. However, Chess and (Adamec, 1991). Similar stories can be told for a great
Thomas (1990) noted, wisely, that the outcomes of a dif- many species. Mice, rats, wolves, cows, monkeys, birds,
ficult temperament depended on the goodness-of-fit and even paradise fish differ, within species or among
the matchbetween the childs temperament and the closely related strains, in the tendency to approach or to
familys ideals for the child. Both must be assessed if avoid novelty. A review of this variation by a team of
one is to predict future pathology. evolutionary biologists concluded, There can be little
doubt that the shy-bold continuum is an important
Animal Research source of behavioral variation in many species that de-
serves the attention of behavioral ecologists (Wilson,
The discovery that closely related strains of animals Clark, Coleman, & Dearstyne, 1994, p. 7).
raised under identical laboratory conditions behaved It is not surprising that fearful behavior can be bred
differently to the same intrusions provided another set in animals, but it is surprising that it requires such a
of persuasive facts. Over 35 years ago, John Paul Scott small number of generations. Some quail chicks become
and John Fuller (1965) observed over 250 puppies from chronically immobile when placed on their back in a
five different breedsbasenji, beagle, cocker spaniel, cradle and restricted by a human hand; remaining im-
Shetland sheepdog, and fox terrierat the secluded mobile is one measure of fear in birds. If chicks who dis-
Jackson laboratories in Bar Harbor, Maine. In one as- play the fearful trait are bred with other fearful animals,
sessment of an animals timidity, a handler took a puppy it takes only eight generations to produce a relatively
from its cage to a common room, placed the puppy one uniform line of birds that shows immobility for as long
or two feet away, stood still, and observed the animals as 2 minutes (Williamson et al., 2003). It is equally easy
behavior. The handler then slowly turned and walked to- to establish a pedigree of birds that shows very brief pe-
ward the puppy, squatted down, held out his hand, riods of immobility, implying minimal fear, and possible
stroked the puppy, and finally picked it up. The puppies to select quail who secrete high or low levels of corti-
that ran to the corner of the room, crouched, and issued costerone. The strain with higher levels is more fearful
a high-pitched yelp early in the sequence were classified of novelty than the strain with low levels of this steroid
as timid. The five breeds of dogs differed dramatically (Jones, Satterlee, & Ryder, 1994).
in degree of timidity, for the basenjis, terriers, and shel- The Maudsley Reactive strain of rats was bred over
ties were more timid than the beagles and cocker generations to be emotionally reactive to unfamiliarity,
spaniels. But the rearing environment was important: All where the amount of defecation in a brightly lit open
178 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

field, aversive for a rat, was the index of reactivity. A varies with the nature of the imposed stressor (Magnus-
second strain of rats was bred to be minimally reactive. son, 1988).
The differences between the reactive and nonreactive Temperamental factors are even linked to immune
strains emerge early, by 30 days, and are not due to post- function in monkeys. One group of crabeater males was
natal experiences. The reactive, compared with the non- assigned to a stable group of four or five other monkeys
reactive, animals have lower levels of catecholamines in for the 26 months of the experiment. A second, stressed
body tissue and lower levels of norepinephrine in the group of animals also lived with four or five other mon-
blood, but compensate by having an increased density of keys, but the composition of the group changed each
beta adrenergic receptors on the heart (Blizard, Liang, month. These frequent changes generated uncertainty in
& Emmel, 1980). However, later research revealed that the crabeater monkey. The scientists also observed the
the low reactive rats failed to defecate because they pos- animals twice a week for about a half hour to determine
sessed higher levels of norepinephrine in the colon due which were social and affiliativethey groomed and
to greater sympathetic activation. That condition led to stayed close to other monkeysand which were social
constriction of the smooth muscle of the colon, and isolates. After the 2 years of either stressful or mini-
therefore, less defecation (Blizard & Adams, 2002). mally stressful social experience, the integrity of each
This fact illustrates the danger of conceptual inferences animals immune system was measured by drawing
from incomplete evidence for the low reactive animals blood from each monkey for 3 weeks and evaluating the
are, in fact, high reactive sympathetically. ability of the T lymphocytes to respond appropriately to
Strain differences in the reaction to unfamiliarity an antigen. Lower levels of cell proliferation to the anti-
exist in primate groups. South American squirrel mon- gen index are assumed to reflect a compromised immune
keys of two different strains, reproductively isolated by system. Only the animals that lived under stress and, in
only a thousand miles of jungle, vary in their morphol- addition, were temperamentally prone to be social iso-
ogy, physiology, and behavior (Snowdon, Coe, & Hodun, lates showed a severely compromised immune system.
1985). About 20% of rhesus monkeys are extremely The affiliative animals that had experienced the same
timid in unfamiliar environments, have a tense muscle level of stress showed a healthier immune response. This
tone as infants, and show physiological reactivity in bod- finding illustrates the principle that a disease state re-
ily targets that are linked to fearfulness (Suomi, 1987). quires both a stress and a vulnerable organism (Cohen,
There are even sanguine, melancholic, and choleric mon- Kaplan, Cunnick, Manuck, & Rabin, 1992).
keys. When the behaviors of three closely related The fact that very small variations in the genetic
species of macaques were compared with respect to composition of closely related animals are associated
their tendency to approach or to withdraw from an unfa- with distinct profiles of behavior and physiology re-
miliar human, bonnets were most likely to approach, quires accommodation. If an animals temperament in-
whereas crabeaters, the smallest of the three species, fluences its reaction to total isolation and immune
were the most fearful. The largest animalsrhesus competence, it is likely that similar factors are opera-
were the most aggressive (Clarke, Mason, & Moberg, tive in human psychological functions. Thus, diverse, in-
1988). When these three species were observed under dependent forces combined to render temperamental
different conditions of novelty and restraint, the aggres- ideas as attractive candidates in interpretations of
sive rhesus were least disturbed and showed the smallest human behavior.
increases in heart rate. The fearful crabeaters were the
most disturbed and showed the largest increases in both NEUROCHEMISTRY AND TEMPERAMENT
heart rate and glucocorticoids. The bonnets, who are
passive and avoidant, showed modest increases in both It is likely that the biological bases for many, but cer-
heart rate and glucocorticoids. However, when tainly not all, temperamental categories are heritable
crabeater, rhesus, and pigtail monkey infants were neurochemical profiles, a hypothesis anticipated earlier
reared in isolation for 6 months, the rhesus displayed the in the twentieth century (McDougall, 1929; Rich, 1928).
most disturbed social behavior, whereas crabeater mon- Research on voles, a small rodent, is illustrative. Prairie
keys showed almost normal social behavior (Sackett, and Montane voles, two closely related strains, differ in
Ruppenthall, Farenbuch, Holm, & Greenough, 1981). a psychologically significant behavior. Males and fe-
Thus, the influence of temperament on development males from the former strain pair bond following several
Neurochemistry and Temperament 179

hours of mating, while members of the latter strain do modulator, ( b) have more receptors for a particular mol-
not. Variation in the promoter regions of the genes that ecule, (c) project to neurons that secrete a particular
influence the distribution of receptors for vasopressin in molecule, or (d) can be inhibited or disinhibited by an-
males and oxytocin in females contributes to this behav- other ensemble. Given the brains massive interconnect-
ioral difference (Insel, Wang, & Ferris, 1994). The edness, it is reasonable to assume a very large number of
DNA of the promoter region determines whether the ways in which neurochemistry can influence emotion
gene will be activated in a particular site. Both strains and behavior. Only some of these influences are inher-
secrete vasopressin and oxytocin and both strains have ited (Placidi et al., 2001).
receptors for these molecules, but the strains differ in Some molecules that appear to be significant include
the locations of the relevant receptors. Only the prairie norepinephrine, CRH, glutamate, GABA (-aminobu-
vole has receptors in limbic sites believed to mediate tyric acid), dopamine, serotonin, opioids, vasopressin,
states of pleasure. prolactin, and oxytocin. For example GABA-ergic and
There is heritable variation in the concentration of serotonergic circuits usually inhibit neuronal excite-
and density and location of receptors for more than a ment. Infants born with a compromise in either trans-
150 different molecules that affect brain function. This mitter system should be less effective in modulating
fact implies a very large number of neurochemical pro- extreme states of distress. This speculation has some
files. Even if the majority have little function or rele- support for very irritable 2-year-olds, compared with re-
vance for mood or behavior, given the extraordinarily laxed toddlers, possessing the shorter form of an allele
large number of profiles it is likely that human popula- in the promoter region for the serotonin transporter gene
tions contain many temperaments, each defined by a (Auerbach et al., 1999). It is relevant that samples of
neurochemistry that influences the usual psychological Japanese adults are more likely than samples of Euro-
reaction to classes of events. peans to possess the long version of this allele and Japa-
Two important factors determine brain neurochem- nese infants are less irritable than European-Caucasian
istry and, therefore, temperamental biases. First, some infants (Kumakiri et al., 1999). Further, adults who in-
brain molecules are excitatory and some are inhibitory herit the shorter allele show greater amygdala activity
and a balance between these processes determines the to fear-provoking stimuli, compared with adults who
brain state. For example, the balance between opioids possess the longer form (Hariri et al., 2002); although,
and corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in the locus very shy Israeli children inherit the longer form of this
ceruleus determines the organisms reaction to a stres- allele (Arbelle et al., 2003).
sor (Van Bockstaele, Bajic, Proudfit, & Valentino, The lateral nucleus of the amygdala secretes a mole-
2001). Second, the variation in the density of receptors cule called gastrin-releasing peptide that acts on the re-
for a molecule can be independent of the concentration ceptors of interneurons to release GABA, which in turn
of that molecule. Some mice strains show high levels of inhibits neural activity. Mice without the gene for this
tyrosine hydroxylase in the cortex (an enzyme involved class of receptor fail to release GABA in amygdala neu-
in the synthesis of dopamine and norepinephrine) but a rons and, as a consequence, these animals preserve
low density of receptors for norepinephrine. Other traces of the association between a conditioned stimulus
strains are high or low on both properties (Dyaglo & and electric shock for a longer period of time (Shumy-
Shishkina, 2000). If we assume that the concentration of atsky, 2002). It is possible that children who possess this
a particular neurotransmitter or modulator, and the den- allele will have compromised GABA activity in the
sity of its receptors, can be low, moderate, or high, there amygdala and a tendency to preserve a fearful posture
can be nine possible profiles for each molecule and, as- (Maren, Yep, & Goosens, 2001; Sanders, 2001).
suming 150 different molecules, at least 1400 neuro- Variation in dopamine release and in the density of
chemical profiles that could reciprocally influence each its varied receptors are related to cortical excitability,
other. This state of affairs implies a very large number intensity of sensory pleasure, and reaction to novelty.
of possible temperamental biases. There is an immediate release of dopamine in the nu-
There are at least four different ways brain chemistry cleus accumbens the moment a rat places his forepaws in
can affect the excitability of a particular neuronal en- a novel environment, which can last for as long as 8 sec-
semble with consequences for behavior. The ensemble onds (Rebec, Christianson, Guevra, & Bardo, 1997).
can (a) secrete a greater amount of neurotransmitter or Further, high dopamine levels in the cortex suppress
180 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

neuronal activity in the corpus striatum. As a result, females, are affected by the surge in testosterone se-
there are fewer volleys from striatum to cortex, and, creted by the male embryos. One consequence of this
therefore, a lower level of cortical excitability. In addi- prenatal position is increased density of mu-opioid re-
tion, higher dopamine activity in the cortex implies a ceptors in the midbrain, which, postnatally, is accompa-
smaller proportional rise in dopamine following expo- nied by a higher pain threshold (Morley-Fletcher,
sure to novelty. A person sated on chocolate experiences Palanza, Parolaro, Vigano, & Laviola, 2003).
less pleasure from a chocolate bar than one who has not CRH secreted by the hypothalamus, influences many
tasted chocolate for several days. These facts suggest systems, but especially the hypothalamic pituitary adre-
that children with higher dopaminergic activity in the nal axis (HPA). One product of activity in this axis is se-
cortex might have a lower preference for novel experi- cretion of the hormone cortisol by the adrenal cortex.
ences than those with greater dopaminergic activity. It Capuchin monkeys with high cortisol levels are more
is of interest that females have more dopamine receptors avoidant than animals with lower cortisol levels (Byrne
in the cortex than males and fewer females than males & Suomi, 2002). Further, there is evidence for a relation
seek novel experiences. between an allele at a CRH locus and behavioral inhibi-
Variation in norepinephrine and its receptors modu- tion in children, especially among those with one or
lates the preferred reaction to novelty, level of alertness, more parents with panic disorder (Smoller et al., in
sustained attention in the face of distraction, and press). Infusion of high doses of glucocorticoids, espe-
thresholds for detecting subtle changes in sensory sig- cially to the central nucleus, potentiates the release of
nals. This variation should have consequences for psy- CRH, startles (Lee, Schulkin, & Davis, 1994), and
chological qualities. Rats from the Wistar strain who freezing in rats (Takahashi & Rubin, 1994). It is also
explore unfamiliar areas have greater norepinephrine ac- relevant that monkeys who exhibited high fear to novelty
tivity in the nucleus accumbens; hence, volleys from the and had high cortisol levels showed greater relative
amygdala are enhanced when they arrive at the nucleus right, rather than left, frontal activation in the elec-
accumbens (Roozendaal & Cools, 1994). The amygdalar troencephalogram (EEG; Kalin, Larson, Shelton, &
release of norepinephrine is potentiated by epinephrine Davidson, 1998), and monkeys who showed extreme
acting on norepinephrine receptors in the basolateral nu- right frontal activation had higher levels of CRH across
cleus (McGaugh & Cahill, 2003). Hence, variation in the interval from 4 to 52 months of age (Kalin, Shelton,
the density of receptors for norepinephrine in the amyg- & Davidson, 2000).
dala and the sensitivity of the basolateral receptors to However, there is no simple relation between cortisol
epinephrine could affect behaviors that are classified as levels, on the one hand, and either the reaction to an
temperamental (Cecchi, Khoshbouei, Javors, & Mori- aversive event or self-reported mood, on the other.
lak., 2002). Adults administered either 20 or 40mg of cortisol, or a
Opioids modulate the intensity of visceral afferent placebo, were asked to rate unpleasant and neutral words
feedback from the body to the nucleus tractus solitarius and pictures and in addition to describe their mood. Al-
in the medulla. Hence, less opioid activity implies that though the subjects given cortisol showed a rise in circu-
the medullas projection to the amygdala will be more lating hormone, there was no relation between their
intense, and, as a consequence, the orbitofrontal pre- self-reported feelings, or their ratings of the words and
frontal cortex, a target of the amygdala, will be vulner- pictures, and cortisol level (Abercrombie, Kalin,
able to greater activation. One possible consequence of Thurow, Rosenkranz, & Davidson, 2003).
this cascade is a greater state of worry, tension, or dys- Gunnar (1994), who has explored the ability of sali-
phoria and/or greater difficulty extinguishing a condi- vary cortisol to detect different types of children, has
tioned fear (McNally & Westbrook, 2003). Individuals also concluded that biological variables are ambiguous
with greater opioid activity in the medulla should expe- in meaning. Salivary cortisol levels are too subject to
rience more frequent moments of serenity and imper- varied temporary states to be relied on alone as a sensi-
turbability (Miyawaki, Goodchild, & Pilowsky, 2002; tive sign of a stable temperamental type. Bold, outgoing
Wang & Wessendorf, 2002). It is important to appreciate preschool children are much more active than shy, timid
that not all variation in opioid activity is genetic in ori- ones early in the school year and have occasional days
gin for some can originate in prenatal events. For exam- with very high cortisol levels. But several months later,
ple, female mice embryos lying between two males, or when the originally less active, shy children have be-
next to a male, compared with those lying between two come acclimated to the school setting and venture forth
The Complexity of Brain-Behavior Relations 181

to socialize with others, they begin to show occasional facts like the relation between a trisomy on chromosome
days with very high salivary cortisol levels. Thus, the 21 and the mental retardation of Down syndrome. This
variation in cortisol spikes is closely related to the fact, and related discoveries, seduced many scientists
childs temporary psychological state and level of activ- into minimizing the indeterminacy, complexity, and
ity. Our laboratory found no significant relation between counterintuitive quality of the intermediate processes
early morning salivary cortisol levels in 87 infants 5 and between genes and a psychological profile (Hu & Fox,
7 months old and reactivity, smiling, or fear (Kagan, 1988). Consider the counterintuitive nature of the fol-
1994). Further, infants between 12 and 18 months of age lowing fact: The activity of the sweat glands in the skin
vary in the class of event most likely to provoke the se- of an adult is sympathetic in origin but is mediated by
cretion of cortisol, and in the time to attain a peak corti- cholinergic neurons. The puzzle is that the embryos
sol level (Goldberg et al., 2003). A particular average sweat gland is noradrenergic and it requires sympathetic
cortisol value has no univocal meaning across samples of innervation to induce a molecule that, in turn, changes
infants or children. the neurotransmitter from noradrenergic to cholinergic
Although we have emphasized the influence of neu- (Habecker & Landis, 1994). Very few biologists sitting
rochemistry, some temperamental categories could be quietly in their study 50 years ago would have imagined
derivatives of special anatomical features. Adults with a this mechanism.
larger than average volume of the right anterior cingu- Behavioral data are equally complex and it will be
late reported more frequent bouts of worry and shyness necessary to view variation in behaviors like aggression,
with strangers (Pujol et al., 2002). Also relevant, the affiliation, sociability, fear, and depression historically,
volume of the right medial ventral prefrontal cortex in for each phenotype can result from a different life his-
monkeys, an area that is active in anxious adults, is a tory. Consider two rats, one unfamiliar and one familiar
heritable feature (Lyons, Afarion, Schatzberg, Sawyer- with mice. If the septum of the former is lesioned, the
Glover, & Moseley, 2002). animal is likely to attack a mouse placed in its visual
Unfortunately, the immaturity of current knowledge field. But if the rat had been first familiarized with
relating brain neurochemistry to human psychological mice, the attack is less likely following the septal sur-
states frustrates any attempt to posit a specific relation gery. Similarly, stimulation of the periaqueductal gray
between a chemical profile and a temperament. Because of a rat that formerly would not attack mice is likely to
genetic variation accounts for less than 10% of the vari- produce an attack. But the same level of stimulation ap-
ation in most complex behaviors, it is unlikely that a sin- plied to an experienced rat that has attacked mice in the
gle allele responsible for a particular neurotransmitter past causes the animal to interrupt its attack when stim-
level or receptor distribution will determine a tempera- ulation begins (Karli, 1956, 1981, 1991).
mental type. In light of these problems, it is impossible Equivalent behavioral outcomes do not always imply
to define any temperament at the present time by a spe- similar prior conditions. The concept of equivalence in
cific neurobiological profile. Further, the number of physics holds that if the same mathematical description
possible neurochemical profiles that can affect behavior applies to an event that was produced by different condi-
is much larger than the number of behavioral profiles. tions, the phenomena are to be considered equivalent
There is a limited number of ways a child can display theoretically. A classic example is the concave surface
shyness with a group of children on a playground. The of the water in a bucket that is rotating on a table. How-
child can stand apart from the group, remain quiet, play ever, if one imagines the universe rotating and the
alone at a task, or stare vigilantly at the other children. bucket remaining still, the surface of the water will ap-
A much larger number of neurochemical patterns could pear concave. Because the mathematics that describe
accompany each of those behaviors, just as a large num- the concave surface is the same for a rotating bucket or a
ber of bodily states can create a stomach cramp. rotating universe, the two events are considered equiva-
lent. The increased reliance on complex machines that
TH E COMPLEXITY OF BRAIN- purport to measure brain states has led some scientists
BEHAVIOR RELATIONS to a tacit acceptance of a form of equivalence. They as-
sume that the PET or fMRI profile reflects a particular
Scientists appreciate that the influence of a biological psychological state, regardless of the conditions or the
profile on behavior is more complex than earlier schol- context of assessment. This assumption is probably in-
ars expected. The earlier assumptions were based on correct. The unconditioned nictitating membrane reflex
182 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

to a puff of air involves a brain state different from the the central nervous system. Each afternoon as the light
one activated by the conditioned form of the reflex, even fades, a gene is activated that initiates the sequence of
though the reflex appears identical under the two condi- protein synthesis (Takahashi & Hoffman, 1995). A
tions (the cerebellar nuclei are required for the condi- child who inherited a physiology that biased her to be
tioned reflex but not for the unconditioned one; R. fearful (or impulsive) might, through experience, gain
Thompson et al., 1987). control of that behavior. The new profile could change
One reason for the lack of a determinant relation in both the childs psychological state and the genome that
humans between an underlying biology and a psycholog- contributed to her initial behavior. An extremely inhib-
ical profile is that the persons historical and cultural ited 2-year-old boy in a longitudinal sample was not
context typically influences the profile. The journals of very fearful as an adolescent, and the reactive sympa-
the writer John Cheever (1993), who died in the second thetic nervous system he displayed as a toddler was
half of this century, and the biography of William much less apparent when he was 13 years old (Kagan,
Jamess sister Alice James (Strouse, 1980), who died 1994). Further, the neural structures necessary for the
100 years earlier, imply that both writers inherited a acquisition of a conditioned avoidance or a freezing re-
very similar, if not identical, diathesis that favored a sponse may not be necessary for long-term maintenance
chronically dysphoric, melancholic mood. But Cheever, of the same behavior. For example, the central nucleus
whose premises about human nature were formed when of the amygdala is required if an animal is to learn to
Freudian theories were ascendant, assumed that his avoid a place associated with pain. But once the associ-
angst was due to childhood experiences, and he tried to ation has been established, the avoidant behavior can
overcome the conflicts that he imagined his family had occur without involvement of the amygdala (Parent,
created with the help of drugs and psychotherapy. By West, & McGaugh, 1994). Thus, the physiology that is
contrast, Alice James believed, with a majority of her the basis for learning a habit need not be similar to the
contemporaries, that she had inherited her dour mood. physiology that maintains it.
Hence, she concluded, after trying baths and galvanic
stimulation, that because she could not change her
Specificity
heredity she wished to die. The historical era of these
creative writers exerted a profound influence on the A principle of specificity is as important as a belief in
coping strategies each selected and, by inference, on the dynamic reciprocity in probing the relation of brain to
quality of their emotional lives. behavior. The area of the cortex that is essential for the
No single peripheral physiological measure is likely retrieval of words that represent actions may be differ-
to be an especially valid index of a temperamental type ent from the area that is important in the retrieval of
because each is subject to local influences that are unre- nouns (Damasio, 1994). The areas of the brain that
lated to central brain mechanisms that are the primary cause a rat to avoid an electric probe that delivered
features of the temperament. However, an aggregate of shock are not the same as those that cause the rat to bury
different measures might do better. When the standard wood chips following the same experience of shock: The
scores for eight peripheral physiological variables that amygdala is necessary for display of the former, the sep-
are related to limbic excitability were averaged, the cor- tum for the latter (Treit, Pesold, & Rotzinger, 1993a,
relation between this aggregate index and behavioral in- 1993b). The circuit that mediates defensive aggression
hibition was much higher than the relation between any to a noxious stimulus does not require the amygdala; the
one variable and the index of inhibited behavior (Kagan freezing response to an intruder does require the amyg-
et al., 1988). dala but not the hypothalamus. However, the rise in
The traditional view that the relation between brain heart rate to an intruder is likely to involve the lateral
and behavior is unidirectional is being replaced with a hypothalamus (Fanselow, 1994).
more dynamic perspective that expects that psychologi- It is a truism that the history of every scientific disci-
cal states influence brain physiology and the activity of pline is marked by new theoretical conceptions. It is less
particular genes and their products. Glucocorticoids and well recognized that changes in the evidential bases for a
other chemicals produced by psychological states can concept, which are the product of new methods, are
turn on or off genes that control the density of receptors equally characteristic of the history of disciplines. The
on neurons and, as a consequence, alter the reactivity of microscope, for example, changed the meaning of life to
The Complexity of Brain-Behavior Relations 183

include forms that could not be seen. The radio tele- lier, to invent new concepts to name the neural circuits
scope, which permitted the measurement of microwave activated when certain psychological states occur rather
radiation, led to the idea of dark matter and a new con- than simply adopt the older, popular, psychological
ception of the mass of the cosmos. terms. For example, LeDoux, Iwata, Cicchetti, and Reis
Psychology continues to be concerned with a small (1988) have described elegantly the brain structures
number of fundamental ideas that include consciousness, necessary for a rats acquisition of a conditioned freez-
emotion, memory, thought, and pathology. Prior to the ing response or increase in blood pressure to a light that
invention of machines that could measure brain activity, had been associated with electric shock. They call these
it was understood that the definitions of each of these brain structures a fear circuit. But this meaning of fear
concepts rested either on a persons phenomenological must be distinguished from the meaning of fear to de-
statements or observations of behavior. But after scien- scribe a child who says he is afraid of failing a school
tists gained access to the EEG, and later to the PET and examination.
fMRI, the primary referents for the psychological terms
changed. It is possible that, in time, psychological infor-
Agreements and Disagreements
mation will come to be regarded as less objective, less
accurate, or both. That trend is dangerous because psy- Most investigators of temperamental biases agree on
chological data are inherent in the meanings of words several issues. One is that the major structures of the
like consciousness and emotion. limbic systemhippocampus, cingulate, septum, hypo-
Consciousness is a psychological state, not a pattern thalamus, and amygdalaand their projections to motor
of brain activity, even though the latter is the basis for and autonomic targets are important participants in the
the former. An enabling condition should never be con- variation that defines the major constructs. Second, the
fused with its emerging products. A childs perception variation in the excitability of these brain structures is
of a gull swooping down on the sea is not synonymous likely to be influenced by many genes rather than by a
with the description of the circuits that make that per- single allele. Third, the peripheral biological measure-
ception possible. No one would confuse the tides with ments often used to define the temperamental cate-
the gravitational attraction between earth and moon, a goriesfor example, cortisol, blood pressure, heart rate,
burning tree with the lightning that struck it, obesity vagal tone, EEGhave only very modest associations
with levels of lipids and carbohydrates, nor a protein with the behavioral components of the category (Bates
with the DNA and RNA that were responsible for its & Wachs, 1994; Gunnar, 1994; Kagan, 1994). Schwartz,
manufacture. Yet, some scientists are committing the Snidman, and Kagan (1999) failed to find a robust rela-
error of confusing ideas like fear, consciousness, and de- tion between any one of a large number of peripheral
pression with specific neurochemistries and brain cir- physiological variables and temperament in a group of
cuits. It is correct to state that a childs immobility to a adolescents who had been classified as inhibited or un-
spider requires a circuit that includes the thalamus, inhibited in the 2nd year of life.
amygdala, and central gray. However, that statement is This trio of agreements is set against four nodes of
not equivalent to saying that the conscious feeling of fear controversy. One source of tension is captured by the
is nothing more than, or identical with, discharge of that contrast between scientists who begin their work with a
circuit. This philosophical error is chasing psychological priori theoretical concepts and those who are comfort-
investigations of emotion and cognition to the periphery able with a host of tiny facts when an area of inquiry is
in a legitimate excitement over the powerful advances in young. The former scholars, who outnumber the latter at
our understanding of the brain conditions that form the the present time, often begin with a theoretical view of
bases for a psychological state. the human temperaments and devise measures for them.
It is unlikely that the concepts that originate in obser- The smaller group, following Francis Bacon, allows the
vations of behavior will map neatly on the concepts of data to guide the invention of temperamental concepts.
neurophysiology. The structure of psychological A second subtle issue involves the idea of essences.
processes is different from the structure of brain Some investigators conceive of a temperamental type as
processes, just as the structure of the brain is different an essence with a fixed behavioral and physiological
from the molecular structure of the genes that influ- profilethe way many diseases are classified. One
enced its formation. It will be necessary, as noted ear- strategy treats the initial temperamental profile in the
184 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

1st year as an original, enduring structure that is pre- vations. This principle is true for all of the sciences. Es-
served for life. timates of the age of an animal species, or the phyloge-
A less popular view holds that the child begins life netic relation between two species, can vary
with a particular temperamental profile that undergoes considerably if fossils rather than proteins are the basis
change as a result of experience with parents, initially, for the judgment. Hence, the validity of a theoretical po-
and later with teachers and peers. As each of these cate- sition can change when the source of information
gories of experiences is encountered and accommo- changes, even though investigators may use the same
dated, psychological changes occur. Thus, the category construct.
psychologists assign to a garrulous 10-year-old who was Statements about the origins and consequences of a
a very irritable infant should incorporate the traits of temperamental bias can be based on three very different
the former child. Because many behavioral changes sources of data. The first and most common, at present,
occur in the first dozen years, the temperamental cate- are verbal reports provided by an informant, usually a
gories assigned to infants will differ from the personal- parent, but, occasionally, by an older child, teachers, or
ity constructs used to describe adolescents. Imagine two peers. A second, less frequent source is derived from be-
containers of glycerine, but only one contains a small havioral observations in a laboratory or, less often, at
drop of black ink that, having been stirred in the con- home or in a school setting. The third, least frequent
tainer, is invisible. Despite the fact that the drop of ink source involves biological measures, like vagal tone,
cannot be seen, it has altered the composition of the heart rate, or cortisol level. Each of these sources has a
glycerine. An infant temperamental bias, like the drop unique structure and a unique set of advantages and lim-
of ink, may not be observable in adolescents, even itations. Extensive behavioral observations are not a
though it can influence the mood and reactions of youth good proxy for parental descriptions or biology. Biologi-
to particular events. This conception of temperament is cal measures are not a valid proxy for behavior or parent
similar to the biologists view of evolution. Even though perceptions, and parental reports are not always a good
the evolutionary origin of all dog breeds was the gray proxy for behavioral observations gathered across di-
wolf, the features of dogs are distinctly different from verse situations.
those of the gray wolf, and biologists differentiate the
features of beagles from those of pit bulls.
Behavioral Observations
There is no essential dog: Some are aggressive, some
are not; some are spotted, some not; some bark, some do The major advantages of gathering behavioral observa-
not. Nonetheless, all dogs share an evolutionary history tions, especially when they are recorded on film, is that
and a set of anatomical structures, physiologies, and be- the information is, presumably, closer to the referent
haviors that distinguish them from cats and cows, which that the investigators wish to knowmost theorists want
have also been domesticated. One day scientists will dis- to know how a child usually behaves in a particular class
cover the critical features that define each of the many of settings. However, there are serious limitations on
temperamental types. But because the study of tempera- most observational data. First, the usual setting is an
ments is at an early stage of inquiry, most investigators unfamiliar laboratory room where the child interacts
rely on behavioral characteristics, each mediated by dif- with an unfamiliar adult while occasionally a parent sits
ferent biologies, as the critical defining features of a nearby. Children can behave in special ways in this un-
temperament. common context. A child who is aggressive with peers
might show no aggression in this setting. Second, most
behavioral corpora are based on less than 1 hour of ob-
servation. It is unlikely that a majority of children will
SOURCES OF EVIDENCE reveal signs of their important temperamental biases in
such a short period. Finally, proper ethical restrictions
Perhaps the most important controversy refers to the limit seriously the incentives psychologists can present,
source of evidence used to define a temperamental type even though these incentives might occur in the childs
or bias (see Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, environment.
this volume). The validity of every empirically based in- In general, behavioral observations are more valid
ference is always influenced by the nature of the obser- when the constructs refer to cognitive competences,
Adult Verbal Descriptions 185

usual reactions to novel social and nonsocial events, and questionnaires would be a preferred and valuable source
degree of sociability with unfamiliar adults or children. of data. But, as with behavioral observations, this class
Behavioral observations are far less valid if the con- of evidence has its special set of limitations.
structs refer to beliefs, motives, conflicts, or usual emo- First, parents vary considerably in the accuracy of
tional reactions to frustration, danger, ethical their descriptions, where accuracy is defined by an
violations, or gaining a desired goal. objective record of the childs usual reactions. This
variability among parents is due to differential compre-
hension of the questions, an understandable desire,
often unconscious, to emphasize traits parents view as
ADULT VERBAL DESCRIPTIONS desirable and to deny features that do not fit the parents
ego ideal, and variation in the extensiveness of parental
Verbal descriptions provided by a parent or teacher have retrieval of the childs past behaviors. For example, par-
the obvious advantage of sampling behaviors across a ents of 9-month-olds did not agree in their attribution of
variety of settings over a long period. Investigators can fearfulness, smiling, or sociability to their infant be-
ask parents about their childs reaction to events that cause the fathers interpreted high activity levels in the
could not be simulated in the laboratory (e.g., reaction to young child as reflecting a positive emotional mood,
punishment, to injury, to illness, or to attack). whereas the mothers regarded the same behavior as re-
The most popular questionnaires are the ICQ (Bates, flecting anger (Goldsmith & Campos, 1990).
1989); the IBQ (Rothbart, 1981); the CBQ (Rothbart, One reason for the discrepancies between two in-
Ahadi, & Hershey, 1994); the RITQ (Carey & McDe- formants, or between a verbal description and behav-
vitt, 1978); and the EAS (Buss & Plomin, 1984). The ioral observations, is that a persons verbal products,
questionnaires designed for infants and children usually whether answers to interview questions or checkmarks
ask about characteristics that parents are interested in on questionnaires, have special features that are not
and can observe easily, especially irritability, smiling, characteristic of the phenomena the sentences are in-
activity, shyness, and fear. tended to describe. Sentences demand logical consis-
Most questionnaires do not ask about qualities that tency, possess a structure different from that of the
are subtle or of minimal interest to parents but nonethe- events they intend to describe, and pass through a psy-
less might be theoretically important; for example, how chological filter that evaluates their social desirability.
long a child takes to eat, preferred use of the right or left Over 35 years ago, Charles Osgood and his colleagues
hand, or the fullness of each smile. Obviously, investiga- (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957) demonstrated that
tors cannot ask parents about qualities that are not ob- people from many different cultures use the evaluative
servable, like asymmetry of cerebral activation or contrast good versus bad as a first dividing principle
sympathetic reactivity. Because scientists can only ask when they categorize people, objects, and events. Most
parents about psychological qualities they understand parents, too, impose a construction on their childs be-
with words that are part of a consensual vocabulary, havior that represents their conception of the ideal child.
most psychologists restrict their temperamental cate- The parent who wants an outgoing child and is threat-
gories to a small number of easily understood ideas; for ened by a quiet one may deny extreme shyness and
example, activity level, smiling, fear of strangers, crying exaggerate sociability. This evaluative frame colors in-
to limitations, soothability, and duration of attention to formants answers to all questions.
events. However, there is a small group of infants who, Individuals are sensitive to the logical consistency in
in addition to being minimally irritable, smile fre- a series of related sentences. If a mother says (or checks
quently, have a low heart rate, low muscle tension, and on a form) that her child is happy, there will be resist-
greater activation of the EEG in the left frontal area. A ance to acknowledging that her child occasionally feels
psychologist who invented a novel temperamental name sad, tense, or anxious. There is no such demand for con-
for this combination of qualities could not ask a mother sistency in a persons behavior or physiology.
to rate her child on this quality for the parent does not Each verbal description of a child competes with a
have access to the childs biology. nonverbal representation composed of prior experiences
If a majority of parents (or teachers) consistently pro- with the child. The verbal categories invite a consis-
vided accurate descriptions of childrens behaviors, tency to which the perceptual schemata are indifferent.
186 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Verbal descriptions pass through a psychological filter Variation in the parents experience with children
that removes inconsistency and exaggerates small differ- can also be important. A young mother with her first in-
ences to create a clearer, more consistent, and more de- fant has a less accurate base for judging her child than a
sirable picture of the child. An infant who both smiles parent who has had three children. The level of agree-
frequently to playful bouts but also cries to frustration ment between descriptions of infants by primiparous
presents an inconsistent profile with respect to the com- mothers and laboratory observations of the same child
plementary notions of a happy or unhappy infant. As a are poorer than the agreement between the laboratory
result, many parents exaggerate one of these profiles observations and the reports of mothers who have had
and mute the other to avoid the inconsistency. Hence, more than one child (Forman et al., 2003). Mothers with
they are likely to tell the interviewer that their baby is their first infants are prone to describe them as more ir-
usually happy or usually irritable but not both (Gold- ritable and more demanding than more experienced
smith & Campos, 1990). mothers. But if the first child of the experienced parent
Most words refer to discrete categories of events, were extremely irritable and the second only a little less
making it difficult to describe blends. There is no En- irritable, but still more irritable than most children, the
glish word that describes the feeling generated when mother is likely to rate the second child as less irritable
one hopes for good news about a hospitalized loved one than observations would reveal because the mother con-
but fears the worst, or the feeling that combines the sat- trasts the second with the first child. This phenomenon
isfaction experienced when a misfortune befalls an occurs with mothers of fraternal twins who usually rate
enemy with the guilt over the malevolent wish. Lan- the two siblings as much less similar than observers do
guages are not rich enough to describe all the important because the mother exaggerates the differences between
feelings and behaviors that are part of the human com- them (Kagan & Saudino, 2001).
petence. Hence, questionnaires must use the best words Every mother watching her child retreat from an un-
available, even though they may be inadequate. familiar adult does not conclude that her child is afraid
Further, every sentence assumes, often tacitly, a of strangers. A mother might (a) generate no categoriza-
comparison. When a parent reads the sentence Does tion, ( b) regard the child as tired, (c) categorize the
your child like to go to parties? she unconsciously com- stranger as ominous, or (d) conclude that the child is be-
pares that preference with others. If one parent com- having adaptively with a stranger. This example, and
pares going to parties with an activity the child many others like it, is stored in the parents long-term
dislikes, while a second parent compares it with one that memory until the day the psychologist asks, Is your
is also preferred, the former parent is more likely to en- child afraid of strangers? The consequences of reading
dorse the item, even though both children may like par- a question are difficult to predict because the psycholo-
ties equally well. gist does not know the categories a parent used to store
When the question asks about an emotion, like and to retrieve the relevant past observations of their
cheerfulness or fear, the opportunity to emphasize dif- child or the parents state at the moment they were being
ferent features of a concept is enhanced. If the investi- questioned. This suggestion is supported by the fact that
gator and the parent have different features of the the social class and personality of the parents influence
concept in mind, each will impose a different meaning their descriptions of their children. Mothers who never
on the question. Wittgenstein (1953) suggested in attended college describe their infants as less adaptive
Philosophical Investigations that every sentence, writ- and less sociable than college-educated parents (Spiker,
ten or spoken, assumes a comparison context. When a Klebanov, & Brooks-Gunn, 1992). Mothers experienc-
mother answers a query about her childs fear of ing stress, for whatever reason, have a lower frustration
strangers, she is unconsciously comparing her idea of tolerance and, therefore, are prone to exaggerate their
fear with related concepts that might refer, for exam- infants irritability. Depressed mothers with their first
ple, to anger, sensitivity, or developmental maturity. child described their 6-week-old infants as more irrita-
Psychologists cannot expect uniformity among parents ble than did experienced mothers or mothers free of de-
in the outcome of those comparisons and, as a result, pression (Green, 1991). The ratings made by depressed
similar parental replies can have different meanings mothers of the irritability of their 6-month-olds were
(see Forman et al., 2003). poorly correlated with laboratory observations, while
Adult Verbal Descriptions 187

the ratings made by healthy mothers were somewhat ings of avoidant behavior in the child was close to 1.0
more accurate (Forman et al., 2003). a value so high it is likely that the parents ratings were
Mothers who described their children on the Carey a serious distortion of the childrens actual behavior
Temperament Questionnaire as difficult were more anx- (Kagan & Saudino, 2001; see also, Saudino, McGuire,
ious, suspicious, and impulsive than mothers who de- Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1993; Rose, 1995).
scribed their children as easy (Vaughn, Bradley, Joffe, A well-designed study assessing the accuracy of
Seifer, & Barglow, 1987; see also Matheny, Wilson, & parental descriptions of childrens behavior with
Thoben, 1987; Mebert, 1991 for similar results). On strangers in an unfamiliar situation revealed that the
some occasions, questionnaire evidence leads to conclu- mothers descriptions were only accurate for preschool
sions that violate both biology and common sense. One children who were extremely shy or extremely sociable
team of investigators interviewed 794 pairs of adult fe- with a stranger in a laboratory setting. The parental rat-
male twins about their physical health and emotional ings were not correlated with the childs behavior for
states. The replies to these questions posed by a over 80% of the sample because many mothers who de-
stranger, revealed the surprising fact that self-esteem scribed their child as sociable had children who were
was as heritable as physical health (Kendler, Myers, & very shy with a stranger in the laboratory (Bishop,
Neale, 2000). We suspect that had the evidence con- Spence, & McDonald, 2003). Rosicky (1993) compared
sisted of a physical examination with laboratory tests the laboratory behavior of 135 1-year-olds to four events
and direct observations of behavior, the results might that often elicit a fear reactionfor example, a toy spi-
have been different. der or maskswith the mothers ratings of their chil-
An exhaustive review of the degree of agreement drens fearfulness to these same events. The mothers
among parents, teachers, and peers with respect to the were remarkably inaccurate in predicting how their
occurrence of childrens behavioral and emotional prob- child would behave.
lems, in over 269 samples, revealed poor concordance In one investigation, 50 firstborn infants were ob-
among different informants as to whether a child was served at home weekly from 4 to 6 months of age. The
fearful, aggressive, or impulsive. The average correla- parents and the observers were consistent over time in
tion between two informants was less than .3 (Achen- their independent evaluations of the babys dominant
bach, 1985; see also, Klein, 1991; Spiker, Klebanov, & mood, approach to unfamiliarity, activity, and intensity
Brooks-Gunn, 1992). of response. But the correlations between the parents
These limitations, inherent in questionnaire data, are ratings and the observers evaluations of the same quali-
the major reason why the relations between parental re- ties were low (about .2). The authors wrote, The most
ports and behavioral observations for apparently similar important implication of our findings is . . . a cautionary
traits, are usually low to modest. For example, the sta- message about the large published literature based on
bility of behavioral observations of shyness and fearful- parent report of their infants behavioral style . . . moth-
ness from 14 to 36 months in a large study of same-sex ers are a poor source of information about their infants
monozygotic and dizygotic twins was significantly behavioral style (Seifer, Sameroff, Barrett, &
smaller than the stability of parental ratings of the same Krafchuk, 1994, pp. 14881489).
qualities (.3 versus .6; Kagan & Saudino, 2001; see also In a similar study, observers visited the homes of 5-
Guerin & Gottfried, 1994; Fagot & OBrien, 1994; month-old infants on two occasions and noted the fre-
Plomin & Foch, 1980). Moreover, the heritability of the quency of smiling, vocalizing, fretting, crying, banging,
behaviorally based indexes of inhibition to unfamiliarity and kicking. The observers also asked the mothers to
decreased with age, while the heritability of the make ratings of these same behaviors in their infants.
parental descriptions of a similar quality increased with Once again, the two sources of data were in poor accord;
age (Emde & Hewitt, 2001). The heritability of behav- the correlations averaged only about .2 (Bornstein,
ioral observations of inhibition from 14 to 36 months Gaughran, & Segui, 1991). There is not even a positive
decreased from .51 at 14 months to .24 at 36 months, correlation between the degree of intentionality actually
while the heritability of parental descriptions of similar displayed by 9-month-old infants in a laboratory and the
behaviors increased from .21 at 14 months to .37 at 36 degree to which their mothers attributed intentionality
months. At some ages, the heritability of parental rat- to their infants on a questionnaire (Reznick, 1999).
188 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Thus, both theoretical analyses and empirical data used by Ekman (1992) and Izard (1991) to describe
imply that generalizations about behavior and emotion brief changes in facial muscles award meanings to words
that are based only on questionnaires (or interviews) like anger or fear that are different from those under-
have a special meaning. Norbert Schwarz (1999) notes stood by parents or observers watching one child strike
that retrospective behavioral reports are highly fallible another or flee from a large animal.
and strongly affected by the research instrument used If asking parents about their childrens behavior and
(p. 100). We view our questionnaires as measurement moods were an accurate source of information, the field
deviceswhat we overlook is that our questionnaires of personality development would be one of the most ad-
are also a source of information that respondents draw vanced domains in the social sciences. Many investiga-
on in order to determine their task and arrive at a useful tors have asked parents to describe their children and
and informative answer (p. 103). have used the data to construct theories. We interpret
The writer Julia Blackburn (2002) captures the slip- the limited progress to mean that verbal statements by
periness of words: parents, teachers, or friends, have some, but limited,
value. Over 20 years ago, Bates (1983) noted, Empiri-
I suppose I have often mistrusted the spoken word. You cal and theoretical considerations call into question the
give a quick tug on the line, and out they come from the assumption that parent reports of a difficult tempera-
dark continent of the mind. Those little raps of sound that
ment are essentially measures of characteristics residing
jostle together, shoulder to shoulder and supposed to be
within the child (p. 95). Stifter and Wiggins (2004) af-
able to give shape to what you really think or feel or know.
But words can so easily miss the point. They drift off in
firmed that conclusion 21 years later, parental reports
the wrong direction, or they insist on providing a clear of infant temperament and difficultness may be influ-
shape for something that by its very nature is lost when it enced by factors that have little association with infant
is pinned down. emotionality and behaviors (p. 88).
Thus, the review of empirical research that follows
will not, with some exceptions, cover the very large num-
SUMMARY ber of reports that have relied on parental questionnaires
as the only source of evidence. The preceding discussion
Even though parents have opportunities to observe their was detailed because it is a defense of that decision.
children in a variety of natural situations over long peri- If, as we believe, future discoveries of theoretical sig-
ods, and laboratory contexts are often artificial and the nificance are likely to come from behavioral observa-
observations of short duration, there are unique influ- tions combined with parental or teacher reports, rather
ences on parental, teacher, or peer descriptions that are than from either source of data alone, we should not treat
absent when behaviors requiring minimal inference are conclusions based on any one of these sources of infor-
recorded on film and coded by disinterested observers. mation as having equivalent meaning to the other. Georg
We are not the first to question the validity of von Bekesy, recipient of a Nobel Prize for research on
parental reports when they are intended to describe how hearing, once advised a young instructor worried about
the child behaves rather than the parents conceptions of his research career: The method is everything. The
their childrens behaviors. Over 65 years ago, a team of older scientist explained that he always measured a phe-
child psychologists noted the poor relation between what nomenon with at least five different methods on the as-
actually happened during the 1st year of an infants life sumption that the features shared across them might
and the maternal descriptions of those events less than a reveal the critical properties of the phenomenon of inter-
year later (Pyles, Stolz, & MacFarlane, 1935; see also est (Evans, 2003). Our constructive conclusion is that
Yarrow, Campbell, & Burton, 1970). each methodquestionnaire, behavioral observation, or
It is possible that the verbal categories parents use to biologyprovides different information and requires
describe their childrens behavior, compared with the distinct concepts. We do not suggest that questionnaire
categories derived from frame-by-frame analyses of evidence be ignored, only that investigators who rely
childrens behavior, are incommensurable (Goldsmith, only on this procedure recognize that the validity of
Lemery, Buss, & Campos, 1999) because the concepts their inferences is restricted to this class of information
used to describe the analyses of the films do not exist in and is not a proxy for direct observation. The same
the vocabulary of the parents. For example, the language caveat applies to those who rely only on behavioral data.
Continuous Traits or Categories 189

The authors believe, although they remain ready to be ables under study are the same at all ranges; the force
proven wrong, that future research will reveal that sub- varies only in magnitude. This persistent preference for
stantial theoretical progress will follow greater reliance continua, although tacit, has been inimical to progress in
on a combination of informant report and behavioral ob- psychology. A biologist phrased the case well for quali-
servations gathered across diverse settings. tative categories: The study of biological form begins
to take us in the direction of a science of qualities that is
not an alternative to, but complements and extends, the
CONTINUOUS TRAITS OR CATEGORIES science of quantities (Goodwin, 1994, p. 198).
Thomas and Chess (1977) regarded the three major
A final source of disagreement is whether a tempera- types of childreneasy, difficult, and slow to warm
mental category should be conceived of as a continuum upas categories, but treated the variation in each of
or as a qualitative category (Meehl, 1973, 1995). their nine dimensions as continuous. They wrote about
A central tension in empirical studies of individual the approach-withdrawal dimension as if all infants
differences is whether people differ quantitatively on could be placed on a continuum with respect to the ten-
the same set of dimensionstherefore, each individual dency to withdraw or to approach unfamiliar events. By
is described best as a set of values on factor scoresor failing to say otherwise, they seemed to reject the possi-
whether some individuals belong to qualitative groups. bility that infants who rarely approach unfamiliar peo-
A strong bias for simplicity favors continuous functions ple might be qualitatively, not just quantitatively,
over categories. The theoretical power and popular suc- different from those who occasionally avoid strangers.
cess of relativity theory in physics supports this bias. It is likely, however, that extremely shy children are
Einstein suggested that, in the frame of an observer, ob- qualitatively different from those who are moderately
jects shorten as their velocity approaches the speed of shy. One reason is that extreme shyness, which is char-
light. To universalize this law in the service of parsi- acteristic of a very small proportion of children, is
mony, he suggested that this shortening occurs even linked with other characteristics that seem unrelated to
when a person swings her tennis racket, although the ve- shy behavior, including eye color, asymmetry of EEG ac-
locity is so small the shortening of the object is not de- tivation, and sympathetic reactivity. Support for this
tectable with any instrument. However, water does not claim is presented later in this chapter.
begin to form very tiny ice crystals as it cools from 30 One reason psychologists have preferred continua
to 28 centigrade. The function relating temperature to over categories is a derivative of the contagion of ideas
the formation of ice is nonlinear. among disciplines. Before relativity theory, physicists
Nonlinear functions are common in the life sciences assumed that object and energy were qualitatively dif-
and, at transition points, novel qualities, which can be ferent things. A burning log was distinct from the heat
viewed as categories, emerge. For example, the behavior or energy the log emitted. Einstein suggested, however,
of a single ant, or a small number, appears random and that there was only the field and, therefore, only energy:
without coherence, but, When the density of a colony The difference between matter and field is a quantita-
reaches a critical value . . . chaos begins to turn into tive rather than a qualitative one (Einstein & Infeld,
order and rhythmic patterns emerge over the colony as a 1938, p. 242). Surely, if a log and the heat it can emit
whole (Goodwin, 1994, p. 189). A large colony of ants can be placed on an abstract continuum of energy, psy-
has distinct qualities that cannot be predicted from or chologists could defend the notion that no individual is
explained by an additive model that sums the behavior of qualitatively different from anyone else on any psycho-
a large number of ants considered one at a time. logical dimension.
Nonlinear functions are common in many domains of A more obvious, and perhaps less controversial, rea-
psychology; for example, the magnitude of potentiated son is the training in statistics given to young psycholo-
startle in a rat has an inverted-U function with intensity gists. By World War II, the use of inferential statistics
of shock during training (Davis, 1984). Thus, current became the mark of the sophisticated social scientist.
statistical procedures like regression can distort rela- The correlation coefficient, t-test, and analysis of vari-
tions in nature (see Hinde, Tamplin, & Barrett, 1993, ance should be computed on continuous variables. Hence,
for a similar position). These analytic procedures as- psychologists found it useful to assume that there were no
sume that the forces producing the values for the vari- qualitative types of people; all humans could be treated
190 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

as substantially similar in their sensations, perceptions, ral sciences, especially biology and chemistry, experi-
memories, and emotions. Statistical analyses were per- mental control of a phenomenon through techniques
formed on continuous scores produced by different ex- that rely on machines generates an aesthetic feeling. A
perimental conditions, not by different kinds of people. second route to beauty is through elegant formal theory
The domination of research in both personality and that presumably explains diverse phenomena. Ein-
development by analysis of variance and regression has steins theory of relativity is a classic example. Be-
frustrated a small group of investigators who have had cause no student of temperament can gain experimental
the intuition that some subjects are qualitatively different control of a childs behavior, most psychologists in this
from the majority in their sample. However, when the domain drift toward the invention of theoretical ideas
group of subjects is small in number, the usual inferential that might provide an aesthetic structure to the phe-
statistics often do not reach the popular .05 level required nomena of interest.
for referee approval. Further, there is no consensus on an It is natural to ask ontological questions when a do-
algorithm that permits an investigator to conclude that main of inquiry is young. The Greeks asked, What is
some subjects belong to a distinct group. Consider, as an matter? Twentieth-century physicists have answered
example, an investigator who did not know about Down that question with a set of mathematical functions that
syndrome studying the relation of maternal age to chil- predict and, therefore, presumably explain the events that
drens intelligence in a sample of 600 families. The corre- follow the bombardment of a hydrogen atom with high
lation between the two variables would reveal no energy. Eighteenth-century naturalists asked, What is a
statistically significant relation. However, examination of species? Biologists answered with a set of relations
a scatter plot might reveal that two children with very low among the evolutionary histories, anatomical and physio-
IQ scores had the two oldest mothers in the sample. Re- logical features, and profiles of interbreeding among dif-
flection on that fact might tempt the investigator to con- ferent animals. In most natural sciences, except
sider the possibility that these two children were psychology, a set of functions replaced abstract Platonic
qualitatively different from the other 598 and, perhaps, conceptions of an eventa set of empirical relations be-
that these two families provided a clue to a relation be- came the answer to the earlier ontological query.
tween age of mother and intelligence of the child for a Replacement of Platonic definitions with robust func-
very small proportion of the population. Hence, there is tions is moving at a slower pace in developmental psychol-
an initial enthusiasm for considering individual cases and ogy. Many journal reports still begin with an ontological
small subgroups with extreme scores. definition of a concept, like coping, secure attachment,
empathy, or reactivity rather than with a concept that was
invented a posteriori to explain observations. Thomas and
CURRENT VIEWS OF THE INFANT Chess (1977), for example, define temperament as the
style of a persons behavior; Goldsmith and Campos
Current ideas on temperament vary with the develop- (1990) regard temperaments as processes that modulate
mental stage of the subject and the sources of evidence; an emotional profile. These a priori declarations are use-
hence, this discussion accommodates to these factors. It ful early in the investigation of a domain, but they limit
is also necessary to impose a conservative attitude to- the scope of empirical work and should be abandoned
ward evidence. Infants and children differ on a large when new evidence erodes their usefulness.
number of characteristics and it is unlikely that most of All observers recognize that variation in irritability
this variation is temperamental in origin, even though an is a moderately stable characteristic in the 1st year and
investigator conducting a cross-sectional study may psychologists could declare that irritability is a tempera-
claim otherwise. Thus, we restrict this discussion to a mental trait. However, after the second birthday, many
small number of characteristics for which the evidence infants who had been highly irritable lose this quality
implies a temperamental contribution to avoid the error and become timid. Thus, either a new term is needed to
psychologists made a half-century earlier when they as- describe this class of 2-year-old or the investigator can
sumed that all variation was due to social experience. move up the ladder of abstraction and suggest that ease
Aesthetic considerations influence the selection of of arousal is the temperamental quality that explains
scientific strategies. Western standards of beauty in the relation between infant irritability and subdued be-
science celebrate two forms of discovery. In the natu- havior in a 2-year-old. One problem with this solution is
Current Views of the Infant 191

that some infants display ease of arousal by babbling, ing, and nondistressed motor activity; the referents for
rather than crying, and these children develop behav- the latter are thrashing, fretting, and crying. A low-
ioral profiles different from those who are irritable. intensity stimulus usually produces vocalization and
We do not suggest that psychologists abandon onto- smiling; a moderately intense stimulus leads to vocaliza-
logical questions, only that they remain receptive to evi- tion or fretting; an intense stimulus more often provokes
dence and move toward concepts defined by a set of cries of distress. Rothbart (1989) suggests that either a
related functions. For example, 20% of healthy 4- pleasant or unpleasant state can be expressed through so-
month-old infants become very active and fretful to au- matic, cognitive, or neuroendocrine responses and expe-
ditory, visual, and olfactory stimuli; two-thirds of these rienced as a feeling of pleasure or distress. The valence
easily aroused infants become fearful in the 2nd year. linked to the state of the reactivitywhether pleasant or
This relation begins to define a temperamental type that unpleasantwill influence the specific self-regulatory
future investigators will refine. One can emphasize the reactions displayed. Thumb sucking, clutching a part of
4-month behavior and call these infants easily aroused, the body, or moving toward or away a novel incentive are
or focus on the timid behavior at 2 years and call these three obvious self-regulatory reactions.
children fearful. This choice is less important than ap- The idea of reactivity has obvious face validity. Fe-
preciating that the primary meaning of either term is a tuses and newborns differ in their reactivity to stimula-
set of developmental functions. New temperamental tion, and the variation is related to a modest degree to
concepts will be needed once we have learned more early postnatal behavior (Madison, Madison, & Adu-
about development. We should not treat a temperamental bato, 1986; Strauss & Rourke, 1978). In addition, mater-
bias as an essence. The psychological outcomes of each nal reports of fetal movements are moderately stable
temperament will be informed by the historical era and from gestational weeks 28 to 35 (Eaton & Saudino,
culture in which children grow and the specific experi- 1992). DiPietro and colleagues (DiPietro, Hodgson,
ences they encountered. The biological conditions that Costigan, & Johnson, 1996; DiPietro et al., 2000, 2002)
are the foundation of a temperament do not produce any examined the relation between fetal activity and heart
single or fixed profile later in life; they only set a bias rate ( beginning at 20 weeks of gestation), on the one
for a particular envelope of outcomes. hand, and infant temperament at 3 and 6 months, using
the ICQ, on the other (Bates, Freeland, & Lounsbury,
1979). High levels of fetal activity predicted irritability
Rothbarts Dimensions and a less adaptive profile (a factor on the ICQ) when
the infants were 3 and 6 months old (DiPietro et al.,
Mary Rothbarts bold, synthetic ideas dominate discus-
1996). In a separate study, individual differences in
sions of infant temperament. Rothbart (1989) posits two
heart rate were preserved from 24 weeks of gestation
primary dimensions on which infants varyease of
through 1 year postnatal (DiPietro et al., 2000). How-
arousal and self-regulationand both are controlled
ever, the evidence on fetal activity from this second
continually by the social environment:
sample led to conclusions slightly different from those
implied by the earlier investigation. More active fetuses
Temperament [is defined as] constitutionally based indi-
showed less distress to limitations when they were 1
vidual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, with
constitutional referring to the persons relatively enduring
year old and were less behaviorally inhibited at 2 years
biological makeup inferred over time by heredity, matura- of age (DiPietro et al., 2002).
tion, and experience. Reactivity refers to the arousability The specific source of the arousing stimulation,
of motor activity, affect, autonomic and endocrine re- whether visual, tactile, olfactory, or auditory, cannot be
sponses. Self-regulation refers to processes that can mod- ignored when classifying an infant as high or low in re-
ulate (facilitate or inhibit) reactivity and those processes activity to stimulation. Four-month-old infants who cry
include attention, approach, withdrawal, attack, behav- to the recorded voice of a woman speaking short sen-
ioral inhibition, and self-soothing. (p. 59) tences are not psychologically similar to those who cry
to a moving visual stimulus. Goldsmith and Campos
Reactivity (1990) found no correlation between an infants ten-
Reactivity can reflect pleasant or distressed states. The dency to cry when placed on the visual cliff and the fol-
referents for the former category are vocalization, smil- lowing encounter with a stranger.
192 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

The nature of the response is also important in judg- certainty generated by the unfamiliar adult, the former
ing the infants reactivity. Infants can cry, fret, smile, infant becomes more timid and shy in the 2nd year than
move, or vocalize to auditory stimuli; and do or do not the latter. Because all infants do something when
struggle to restraint of their arms. An infants preferred aroused by events, most regulate to some degree. It is
reaction to a particular incentive is due, partly, to tem- important to attend to both the specific source of the
peramental and maturational factors that are not yet un- arousal and the specific self-regulatory behaviors that
derstood. For example, in a longitudinal sample of 23 follow, because different temperamental types may be
infants observed at 7, 10, 13, and 16 weeks, an index of hidden in these categories.
motor arousal to varied stimuli based on limb movement Although self-regulation seems an apt way to de-
increased with age, while the frequency of tongue pro- scribe some infant behaviors, it may be less appropriate
trusions, vocalization, and crying decreased with age. for older children because the incentive is not an intense
Only individual variation in tongue protrusions was sta- stimulus that produces a level of arousal, but is often an
ble from 7 to 16 weeks (Rezendes, 1993). unfamiliar event. When an adult dressed as a clown en-
Thus, a potential problem with the concept of reactiv- ters a room where a 2-year-old child has been playing
ity or ease of arousal is an indifference to the exact quietly, most children stop playing and stare at the in-
nature of the incentive. The brain circuits that mediate truder. This stereotyped reaction occurs because the
smiling are likely to be different from those that medi- clown is a discrepant event, not because it is an intense
ate babbling or thrashing (Gainotti, Caltagirone, & Zoc- stimulus, and children usually react to discrepancies
colotti, 1993). with cessation of activity. But it is not obvious that
the immobility, even if accompanied by retreat to the
Self-Regulation
mother, is self-regulating for it may not reduce the
The concept of self-regulation is a derivative of two childs arousal. Indeed, staring at an unfamiliar intruder
older ideas: The first originates in the learning theories while clutching the parent may increase the childs level
of the 1950s; the second in Freuds writings. Dollard of uncertainty and physiological arousal.
and Miller (1950) suggested that a reinforcement was Two eminent students of animal behavior have noted
any event that reduced stimulation and level of internal careful attention to specific patterns of behavior . . . is
arousal. This hypothesis is derived from Freuds sugges- prerequisite to an understanding of the relation between
tion that humans seek quiescence and a reduction in the biological and behavioral systems . . . an adequate de-
vis nervosa. This hypothesis assumes, as a deep premise, scription of behavior must include reference to the stim-
that organisms naturally seek a lowor optimallevel uli and situations that normally produce that behavior
of internal arousal. This idea remains popular despite and to its normal consequences in the environment
the fact that children prefer to run rather than to sit and (Blanchard & Blanchard, 1988, p. 63).
to explore rather than to sit quietly with a new toy. This suggestion is not intended to replace Rothbarts
Freud changed the internal state that caused symp- creative ideas. Rather, we urge psychologists to use her
toms from the popular vis nervosa to anxiety, which was fruitful hypotheses while acknowledging Whiteheads
attractive to many readers. Freud added that cognitive, (1928) admonition that we should not reason about pred-
affective, and behavioral reactions to anxiety were di- icates severed from their noun and object partners. The
rected at reducing the intensity of this unpleasant state. action verb kiss has very different meanings in the fol-
Because anxiety is an obvious enemy, the responses to it lowing three sentences: (1) The woman kissed her lover,
could reasonably be called defenses. This idea, which (2) The baby kissed his grandmother, and (3) The win-
remains popular among psychiatrists and psychologists, ning jockey kissed his horse.
renders the concept of self-regulation appealing. Biologists share a consensual meaning when they use
The idea of self-regulation, however, shares some of a theoretical predicate. Bleach describes what rods in
the same problems that burden reactivity. A 1-year-old the retina do to light. Phagocytosis refers to what natural
infant who shows a wary face to a stranger and then re- killer cells do to bacteria. Digest is what the intestinal
treats to the mother may be different temperamentally villi do to proteins, fats, and carbohydrates. Social sci-
from one who also displays a wary face but does not re- entists are looser in their use of predicates. The predi-
treat and, subsequently, vocalizes to the intruder. Even cate learn is often applied to diverse organisms, from
though both infants may appear to be regulating the un- worms to chimps, on the assumption that the process of
Irritability in Infancy 193

learning is the same in all animals. The indifference to moderately stable from 2 to 19 months (r = .5; Izard,
agents and targets is due, in part, to the fact that psy- Hembree, & Huebner, 1987). However, the frequency of
chologists are primarily interested in process rather spontaneous irritability should not be equated with ease
than in the agents in whom the processes occur. of being soothed by a pacifier. Newborns who took a
Because psychology is one of the least mature of the long time to be soothed by a pacifier were rated by their
natural sciences, its practitioners are tempted to take as mothers at 9 months as more active and less likely to
a model the highly respected discipline of physics. The avoid unfamiliar events (Riese, 1995).
Newtonian declaration that force equals mass times ac- Some, but not all, irritable infants develop a reserved,
celeration holds for all objectscars, stones, and timid, and fearful style in the preschool years. Four-
snowflakesin all earthly situations. Einstein even de- month-old infants who showed frequent irritability to-
clared that the laws of the theory of general relativity gether with vigorous limb activity to varied classes of
apply to all parts of the cosmos. If psychologists were stimuli were more fearful to unfamiliar events in the 2nd
friendlier to biology than to physics, they would realize and 3rd years than those who showed only irritability
that specificity, not generality, is the more useful rule in without motor activity, or only vigorous motor arousal
the life sciences. without irritability. Newborn twins who were unusually
irritable, hard to soothe, and minimally attentive to
stimulation were less sociable and more labile when
they were 9 to 24 months old (Matheny et al., 1987).
IRRITABILITY IN INFANCY Similarly, newborns who became extremely irritable to a
chilled metal disk placed against their thigh, compared
Studies of variation in irritability dominate all other in- with those who were far less irritable, became children
fant qualities while studies of shy-timid versus sociable- with a more serious emotional demeanor (r = .36) and
bold behavior dominate investigations of children. These less sociability (r = .38; Riese, 1987). However, varia-
facts imply that scientists who quantified other behav- tion in crying is not always predictive of later behavior
iors found less stable, or less coherent, results and did (see M. Fish, Stifter, & Belsky, 1991, for an example).
not publish their data. It is hard to believe that most de- Although the evidence implies that extreme irritabil-
velopmental psychologists have restricted their observa- ity to stimulation in the opening weeks predicts a less
tions to these few characteristics and more likely that sociable, more dour child 6 to 24 months later, the cry-
irritability in infancy and a timid or sociable posture in ing behavior in most of these studies was either sponta-
childhood are popular targets because they are obvious, neous or a reaction to varied stimuli. In a few studies,
relatively easy to code, and of concern to parents. however, the crying was produced by the frustration of
Extreme irritability in young infants is preserved to a restraint, typically holding the infants hands or arms.
modest degree through part or all of the 1st year. Crying Infants who cry to this incentive, or any other frustrat-
and fretting are stable over the first 4 months, and new- ing event, may not have the same temperament as those
born irritability predicts less frequent smiling and bab- who cry spontaneously to visual or auditory stimulation.
bling to adults at 4 months of age (Birns, Barten, & Infants who cry to restraint have high vagal tone; infants
Bridger, 1969). Newborns whose cries were of high who cry to visual or auditory stimulation have lower
pitch and of shorter durationand unpleasant to the vagal tone (Fox, 1989).
earwere rated by mothers at 3 months as more irrita- The modest stability of variation in irritability over
ble and difficult (Huffman et al., 1994). Extreme dis- the first 2 years could be due to the fact that the incen-
tress to a heel stick during the newborn period predicted tives for crying change with age. Irritability in a 2-
degree of distress to an inoculation 2 months later (cor- month-old is due, in large measure, to a low threshold of
relation of .4; Worobey & Lewis, 1989) and was related responsivity to the discomfort of cold, hunger, loud
to maternal descriptions of the 6-month-old as mini- noises, and bright lights. Irritability in a 9-month-old is
mally distressed by limitations, but not to maternal de- influenced in a more important way by threshold of re-
scriptions of soothability or frequency of smiling at 6 action to unfamiliarity (Hebb, 1946). Irritability in a 1-
months (Gunnar, Porter, Wolf, Rigatuso, & Larson, year-old is influenced by a vulnerability to separation
1995). Facial expressions, coded in the Ekman-Izard distress and prior conditioning experiences in which
scheme as either anger or sadness, to an inoculation were certain events have become acquired cues for distress.
194 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

By 3 years of age, the reasons for crying are more var- Over half of a large sample of 4-month-olds never
ied and include frustrations and prior reinforcements smiled to visual, auditory, and olfactory stimuli, while
for crying. Thus, we should not be surprised that the 10% smiled three or more times to the same incentives.
correlation between irritability at 1 month and crying at Although three smiles may seem to be a small number in
3 years is relatively low. Each of the small number of an absolute sense, it represented the 90th percentile of
responses that children can display when distressed the distribution. When these high smiling infants were
crying, withdrawal, thrashing, or freezingis preferen- matched with infants of the same sex and level of motor
tially elicited by different incentives and involves arousal and irritability who did not smile at all, the for-
different brain circuits. mer group had significantly lower sitting diastolic blood
As might be expected, extremely irritable and nonir- pressure when they were 21 months old. This result sug-
ritable infants elicit different reactions from their moth- gests that frequent smiling at 4 months is associated
ers. A group of 89 lower-class infants were observed in with low sympathetic tone in the cardiovascular system,
the first 2 weeks of life on the Brazelton Neonatal Be- an idea supported by the fact that 2-week-olds who
havioral Assessment Scale. One group of 15 infants were showed high levels of heart rate variability to stimula-
extremely irritable and another group of 15 were mini- tion were frequent smilers at 4 months (S. Fish & Fish,
mally irritable; these groups represented the top and 1995). Four-month-old infants who showed low levels of
bottom 17% of the sample. These infants and their motor activity and crying to stimulationcalled low re-
mothers were observed at home monthly over the first 6 active infantssmiled more frequently at 4, 14, and 21
months. The mothers of the highly irritable, compared months as well as at 11 years of age, compared with in-
with the minimally irritable, infants had less physical fants who showed high levels of motor activity and cry-
contact with their children but soothed them more fre- ingcalled high reactive. The low reactive-high smiling
quently during the first few months. The mothers of children had low baseline heart rates when they were 11
nonirritable infants were more constant in their soothing years old (Kagan & Snidman, 2004). Thus, frequent
overtures over time. However, the two groups of mothers smiling to nonsocial stimuli may reflect a special tem-
became increasingly similar in their responsiveness as peramental quality.
time passed. By 6 months, the two groups experienced
similar maternal behaviors, and there were few differ- Activity in Infancy
ences between the infant groups in their behavior (Van The popular temperamental trait called activity level
den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994). changes its referential meaning between infancy and 3
years of age. Hence, it is not surprising that there is not
Smiling in Infancy
much preservation of variation in activity level from in-
Crying during the opening months is more salient and fancy to the toddler years (Dunn & Kendrick, 1981;
more frequent than smiling and, in addition, has an ana- Feiring & Lewis, 1980; Matheny, 1983). One study as-
logue in the distress calls of primates. Smiling has no sessed activity in 112 healthy, middle-class newborns
obvious analogue in most animals; perhaps that is why using a pressure transducer mattress that distinguished
fewer investigators have probed individual differences activity during crying from activity during nondistress
in infant smiling, even though variation in this response periods. Fifty of the 112 infants were assessed again
appears to be heritable (Freedman & Keller, 1963; Rep- when they were between 4 and 8 years of age. Activity
pucci, 1968). Frequent smiling to adults does not appear in the older children was monitored for 24 hours using
until about 3 months of age and may be impaired by an ambulatory microcomputer along with a parental
damage to posterior sites in the right hemisphere questionnaire. There was only a modest correlation be-
(Reilly, Stiles, Larsen, & Trauner, 1995). Differences in tween the vigor of activity during the newborn period
smiling appear to be stable from 3 months to the end of and vigorous activity during the day in the older chil-
the 1st year. In one study, variation in smiles to moder- dren (r = .29). Further, there was no correlation between
ately discrepant events at 4 months predicted smiling day and night activity in older children and no relation
following success on a cognitive test at 27 months of between newborn activity and parental ratings of activ-
age, and variation in smiling is more stable over this pe- ity in older children (Korner et al., 1985). The independ-
riod than variation in attentiveness, crying, or vocaliza- ence of day and night activity implies that a concept of
tion (Kagan, 1971). general activity is not useful.
Temperaments in Older Children 195

Activity level in monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic Sroufe, 1995; Riese, 1988). A longitudinal study of
(DZ) twins, assessed at 14, 20, and 24 months, was variation in attentiveness, indexed by duration of fixa-
moderately stable and heritable (Saudino & Cherny, tion time to human faces or forms, revealed no stability
2001). The stability coefficient from 14 to 24 months from 4 months to 13 or 27 months, and only modest sta-
was only .23 and the heritability value .20 (see Saudino bility from 13 to 27 months (r = .2), and parental educa-
& Eaton, 1991, for a similar result with a smaller sample tion predicted attentiveness for girls but not boys in the
of twins, and Goldsmith & Gottesman, 1981). Matheny 2nd year (Kagan, 1971). Further, variation in infant at-
(1983) also reported very modest stability of activity in tentiveness was not related to the childs IQ or reading
a laboratory from 6 to 24 months but a more robust her- ability at 10 years of age (Kagan, Lapidus, & Moore,
itability coefficient. Apparently, a general activity con- 1978). Because the psychological bases for attentiveness
struct that does not stipulate age, context of assessment, to an event change from attention to discrepancy at 4
or time of day is probably not theoretically useful. months to the activation of cognitive structures in the
2nd year, we should not expect much preservation of
Attentional Processes
long or short periods of attentiveness to new events.
The discoveries of cognitive neuroscience have pene- In sum, irritability, smiling, activity, and attention
trated the study of temperament and led investigators to are probably influenced by temperamental biases and
examine variation in the distribution of attention. Roth- each is usually assessed in parental questionnaires for
bart, Derryberry, and Posner (1994) describe three as- temperament (see Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1991).
pects of attention that might turn out to have a
temperamental contribution. TEMPERAMENTS IN OLDER CHILDREN
Infants differ in the rapidity and consistency with
which they orient to a moving object or a sound in the The most popular temperamental qualities in children
periphery. The posterior attention network is especially older than age 2 bear some resemblance to those studied
involved in directing an infants attention to sensory in infants, even though new characteristics emerge after
stimuli. It is believed that the posterior attentional net- the first birthday. Bates (1989) summarizes some of
work involves portions of the parietal cortex, thalamus, these qualities. The concept of negative emotionality
and superior colliculus and activity in these sites is mod- refers to the display of distress, fear, and anger and is
ulated by noradrenergic axons from the locus ceruleus. similar in sense meaning to one of the three major adult
The anterior attentional network involves parts of the temperaments proposed by Buss and Plomin (1984). A
prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and the sup- second factor is dif ficultness, a derivative of the Thomas
plementary motor area. This network participates more and Chess category that refers to irritability, a vulnera-
in effortful control of behavior and the inhibition of ac- bility to stress, and a demanding posture with adults.
tivity to distracting or irrelevant stimuli, as well as in Bates (1980) acknowledges that this quality is, in part, a
the effortful search for specific targets. It is believed construction on the part of the parent. A third, adapt-
that dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental ability to novelty, describes a childs tendency to ap-
area and the basal ganglia modulate this network. proach unfamiliar events and situations. Reactivity, a
Finally, Posner and Petersen (1990) posit a vigilance fourth category, is close in meaning to Rothbarts (1989)
system that mediates maintenance of an alert state over definition, and activity is a fifth temperamental factor.
a prolonged duration. Preliminary evidence implicates Attention regulation, which refers to the tendency to
the role of the right lateral midfrontal cortex as impor- shift attention when distracted by external stimulation,
tant, which, like the posterior system, is influenced by resembles Rothbarts (1989) infant quality of soothabil-
noradrenergic axons from the locus ceruleus. ity. And Bates (1989) suggests that sociability and posi-
Observations of children at 1, 2, and 3.5 years of age tive reactivity comprise important temperaments.
revealed stability of inattentiveness from 2 to 3.5 years Finally, Eisenberg et al. (2003) argue that the variation
of age (Ruff, Lawson, Parinello, & Weissberg, 1990). among school-age children in the ability to regulate
Distractability in a laboratory playroomflitting from emotion and relevant behaviors represents important
one toy to anotherin a sample of 3.5-year-old children traits that might have a temperamental origin. This reg-
showed a modest association with teacher ratings of hy- ulation can be voluntary or involuntary, and can be in-
peractivity at 6 to 8 years of age (Carlson, Jacobvitz, & fluenced by the quality of parental emotional reactions
196 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

toward the child (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004). These in the sixth grade (Cote, Tremblay, Nagin, Zoccolillo, &
categories may turn out to be stable, but at the present Vitaro, 2002; see also Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2002). A
time the evidence is most extensive for the reaction to longitudinal study of non-European infants adopted by
unfamiliar events and situations. We now summarize Dutch parents confirms the preservation of a bias for
this evidence. shy and/or anxious behavior. Young children who had
been described by their adopted mothers as shy and
prone to a dysphoric mood in the 2nd year were most
Reaction to Unfamiliarity
likely to show internalizing traits when they were 7
Two temperamental categories studied extensively refer years old. Neither the familys social class, nor the
to a childs reaction to an unfamiliar person, object, mothers early sensitivity with the infant predicted
event, or context, whether affective restraint, caution, these qualities at age 7 (Stams, Juffer, & van Ijzen-
or avoidance, on the one hand, or a spontaneous ap- doorn, 2002).
proach, on the other. The reaction to an unfamiliar event The heritability of inhibited or uninhibited behavior
depends on whether it is perceived as a threat, the ease to unfamiliarity, based on behavioral observations of a
with which it is assimilated, and the availability of an large sample of monozygotic and dizygotic twins ob-
appropriate response. All 1-year-olds reach toward a served at 14, 20, 24, and 36 months approached .5
new toy after playing with a different one because the (Kagan & Saudino, 2001). Inhibited children remained
new object poses no threat, is assimilated at once, and a close to their mother and avoided playing with both toys
relevant action is available. However, not all 1-year-olds and peers, while uninhibited children showed the com-
reach toward a stranger who has extended a hand be- plementary behaviors. Heritability estimates were over
cause this event is not assimilated quickly, and the child .90 when the sample was restricted to children who were
is uncertain as to what response to display. Thus, chil- extremely inhibited or uninhibited in a play session con-
dren, like adults, live in a corridor bordered on one side sisting of four children (DiLalla, Kagan, & Reznick,
by the appeal of new experiences and, on the other side, 1994). However, only 10% of the children were consis-
by fear of the unfamiliar. tently shy and inhibited at all four ages from 14 to 36
There are three good reasons for an interest in the re- months (Kagan & Saudino, 2001).
action to unfamiliarity. The relevant behaviors are mod- Longitudinal data from the Berkeley Guidance Study
erately stable, easily quantified, and the intraspecific reveals that boys described by their mothers as very shy
variation in these behaviors is present in almost every in late childhood had distinct traits when they were over
species studied, including mice, rats, wolves, dogs, 40 years old. The adults who had been shy married, be-
cows, monkeys, birds, and fish. For example, the reac- came parents, and established a career later than their
tions to novel objects among infant monkeys reared less shy peers. Very shy girls, on the other hand, mar-
either with a female dog or a fur-covered plastic toy ried at normative times but, unlike their less shy peers,
were assessed regularly in the laboratory. The behav- did not develop a career, terminated a job when they
ioral variation in the reaction to novelty during the 3rd married or had a child, and conformed to the traditional
year was more clearly a function of the animals early sex-role norms for that era in American history (Caspi,
temperamental bias to remain close to or distant from its Elder, & Bem, 1988).
surrogate object (the dog or the toy) than to the different Longitudinal observations of a large group of New
conditions of rearing (Mason & Capitanio, 1988). Zealand children affirm the preservation of a shy pro-
There is preservation of a timid compared with a file. Over 1,000 3-year-olds were rated on a variety of
nontimid reaction to unfamiliarity when behavior or characteristics following a 1-hour interaction in a labo-
questionnaire data comprise the evidence (Stevenson- ratory setting. About 15% were rated as shy and sub-
Hinde & Shouldice, 1996). A group of over 1800 Cana- dued, and 30% as sociable and spontaneous. When these
dian children were rated for fearfulness, prosocial same subjects were 18 years old, they filled out a per-
behavior, and restlessness by different teachers from sonality questionnaire. The adolescents who had been
kindergarten through grade six. Most children rated as shy at 3 years of age described themselves as cautious,
very high or low in fearfulness when they were in minimally aggressive, and likely to avoid dangerous sit-
kindergarten were given a similar rating when they were uations (Caspi & Silva, 1995).
Temperaments in Older Children 197

Not all shy, timid, 2- or 3-year-olds become exces- iarity of the setting, ( b) the child feels concern over
sively shy adolescents because parents, teachers, and being evaluated by others, (c) the child prefers to play
peers have encouraged bolder behavior, and because the alone, or (d) the child has experienced traumatic, fear-
shy, inhibited children try to develop a more relaxed, so- arousing encounters with other children and has devel-
ciable profile. Those who are unsuccessful may have had oped a conditioned avoidance to peers. An investigator
a different set of experiences. Rubin and colleagues who codes only time playing apart from other children
(2002) found that 2-year-olds who were inhibited in the could have etiologically heterogeneous groups with sim-
laboratory setting were only likely to preserve that style ilar scores. On the other hand, if the investigator codes
if they had intrusive, hypercritical mothers. The shy 2- several variablestime playing alone, time staring at
year-olds were less likely to remain reticent if their peers, talking, smiling, and reaction to overtures from
mothers discouraged shyness (Rubin, Burgess, & Hast- othersit will be easier to parse the isolated children
ings, 2002). The change from shy to sociable behavior is into separate groups, only some of whom possess a tem-
of course more likely in cultures, like our own, that peramental bias to be shy. Every class of behavior is am-
favor the latter as an ideal. This is less likely in Asian biguous as to its antecedent conditions.
cultures which favor a quiet, less bold child (Kerr, Lam- The research of Rubin and his colleagues, which re-
bert, & Bem, 1996; Rickman & Davidson, 1994). lies on behavioral observations, is exemplary. Rubin
There is an intriguing relation between season of con- (1993) makes a distinction between the child who plays
ception and observer ratings of shyness in both Ameri- alone but who shows signs of anxiety (called reticent)
can and New Zealand samples. The preschool children in and the equally solitary child who is actively engaged in
a large American longitudinal cohort, the National Lon- activities but who does not show signs of uncertainty
gitudinal Sample of Youth (NLSY), were rated on shy- (called solitary-passive). Both types are stable over
ness by trained home visitors on two different occasions. time, but the former more often stares at peers and re-
The 15% of the sample who were rated as very shy on sembles the behaviorally inhibited child (Coplan, Rubin,
both occasions, separated by 2 years, were most likely Fox, Calkins, & Stewart, 1994). Social reticence is char-
to be conceived during the period from late July to late acterized by an absence of social interaction with oth-
September. Thus, the brain would be completing its ers, especially staring at peers while being unoccupied.
basic organization during the period of September to Other children who also play alone explore objects or
November when the amount of daylight is decreasing engage in constructive behaviors and these children are
most rapidly. The New Zealand children who were rated called solitary-passive (Rubin, 1982). Preschool children
as shy were most likely to be conceived in January and who are solitary-passive show neither signs of anxiety
February. Because New Zealand is in the Southern nor do they display internalizing or externalizing symp-
Hemisphere, daylight begins to decrease during these 2 toms (Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995). Further, 2-
months. Thus, both groups of fetuses who became shy year-olds who were ableor motivatedto tolerate a
children spent the first 4 months of their gestation at a two-minute delay before engaging in an interesting ac-
time of decreasing daylight (Gortmaker, Kagan, Caspi, tivity ( because the examiner made that request) and, in
& Silva, 1997). The decrease in daylight is accompanied addition, were behaviorally inhibited were most likely
by increases in level of melatonin but decreases in sero- to be reticent with peers when they were 4 years old
tonin in the pregnant mother. It is possible that these (Henderson, Marshall, Fox, & Rubin, 2004).
biochemical changes affect the brains of those embryos Asendorpf (1991) also finds shy behavior to be stable
who are genetically disposed to develop shyness and in- over time, although more intelligent children show a
creases the probability of that behavioral outcome. Na- greater decrease in shyness over time compared with
ture can act in surprising ways. their less intelligent peers (Asendorpf, 1994). Shy, reti-
We noted a modest positive relation between infant cent behavior with strangers was stable in a group of 99
irritability and a shy profile in the older child. However, German children observed in varied settings from the
as with irritability, a shy posture with children or adults preschool years through the third grade (r = .6;
can have different antecedents and, therefore, different Asendorpf, 1990). Asendorpf agrees with Rubin that a
meanings. A 4-year-old in a social setting can play alone child can be shy because of the unfamiliarity of the situ-
because (a) uncertainty is generated over the unfamil- ation, a concern over evaluation of task competence by
198 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

another, or anxiety over peer rejection (Asendorpf, were rated by observers as low in self-control showed
1989, 1991, 1993). Buss (1986) argues that some shy larger increases in cortisol secretion from morning to af-
school-age children display fearful behaviors to the un- ternoon than most other children (Dettling, Gunnar, &
familiar during the first 2 years of life, while others do Donzella, 1999; Dettling, Parker, Lane, Sebanc, & Gun-
not display this trait until they are 3 or 4 years of age. nar, 2000; Watamura, Sebanc, & Gunnar, 2002). A sim-
A similar conclusion emerged from a study of 212 ilar result was found for infant boys in surrogate care
Swedish children followed over a 6-year interval. Psy- who were rated as shy or anxious (Tout, de Haan, Camp-
chologists rated the childrens behavior in the 1st and bell, & Gunnar, 1998).
2nd years and annually until 6 years of age. (In addition,
mothers rated their infants four times during the 1st INHIBITED VERSUS UNINHIBITED
year, twice during the 2nd year, and annually until they CHILDREN
were 6 years old.) The children who were exceptionally
shy or sociable (15% at each end of the distribution) Kagan and Snidman (2004) regard shyness with
preserved their style from the second to the 6th year of strangers, whether peers or adults, as only one feature
life. The stability was greater for girls than for boys and of a broader temperamental category called inhibition to
smaller in magnitude when the whole sample was the unfamiliar (Arcus, 1991; Kagan, 1994). Inhibited
treated as if sociability were a continuum (Kerr, Lam- children react to different types of unfamiliarity with an
bert, Stattin, & Klackenberg-Larsson, 1994). initial avoidance, distress, or subdued affect when they
A second group of Swedish investigators followed reach the maturational stage when discrepancies elicit
144 firstborn children from 16 months through the 4th uncertainty, usually 7 to 9 months in humans. The com-
year of life, a 2-year interval. Some children were at- parable ages in other species are 2 to 3 months in mon-
tending day care and some were raised only at home. Shy keys, 30 to 35 days in cats, and 5 to 7 days in ducklings
behavior with an unfamiliar adult, based on observa- (Kagan, 1994). The source of the unfamiliarity can be
tions at home, was stable from 28 to 40 months (r = .4) people, animals, situations, objects, or dynamic events.
but not from 16 to 40 months, and there was no effect of An inhibited child might, with experience, learn to con-
day care attendance on shyness (Broberg, Lamb, & trol an initial avoidance of strangers and, therefore, not
Hwang, 1990). The increased contact with strange appear shy, but still retain an avoidant style to unfamil-
adults that followed from enrollment in out of home care iar challenges or places or be prone to a serious, dour
did not affect childrens inhibition at 28 and 40 months mood. An inhibited temperament assumes that a child
of age, which suggests that inhibition in the 1st year of can display an avoidant style in any one of a number of
life is best viewed as a fairly stable dimension that is not contexts. Membership in this temperamental category is
systematically affected by ordinary life changes like not defined by only one class of behavior, like shyness
those implicit in the initiation of out of home care with an unfamiliar peer. Hence, children who are not
(p. 1161). The fact that the children attending day care particularly shy might have an inhibited temperament.
were not more sociable than those at home with their The complementary category, called uninhibited to
mothers surprised the authors and will surprise some the unfamiliar, is characterized by a sociable, affec-
developmental psychologists. tively spontaneous reaction to unfamiliar people, situa-
Alternatively, placement in day care before the age of tions, and events. As with the inhibited child, the
2 years apparently influences behavioral inhibition in a category refers to an envelope of profiles whose form
middle-class sample. Infants who displayed a combina- changes with development.
tion of high motor activity and distress to unfamiliar au- Shy or sociable behavior in a specific class of situa-
ditory and visual stimuli, a profile that predicts tion can be the product of different biologies and past
behavioral inhibition at age 2, were less reticent with un- histories. The independence of an entity (in this case a
familiar peers at age 4 if they had been placed in out-of- child) from its functions ( behavior in a particular situa-
home day care than if they had remained at home with tion) was a major node of disagreement between White-
their mothers (Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & head and Russell. Russell (1940) believed that the two
Schmidt, 2001). An infants temperament interacts with ideas were independent, whereas Whitehead insisted
rearing conditions. In addition, children placed in dif- that they were a unity. In the statement Lions stalk
ferent types of surrogate care outside the home who gazelles Russell would have argued that the predicate
Inhibited versus Uninhibited Children 199

stalk was applicable to a variety of animals and could be by a circuit involving the amygdala and the ventral peri-
treated as an independent function. Whitehead (1928) aqueductal gray. However, acquiring a classically con-
would have claimed that lions stalk in a particular way ditioned rise in heart rate does not require the central
that is different from that of hyenas; therefore, no gray, but the amygdala and its projections to the hypo-
scholar should posit separate classes of agents and func- thalamus and the sympathetic chain. A startle reaction
tions that can be combined in any way. We side with to a looming object or loud sound need not involve the
Whitehead, as do all who believe that the motives, emo- amygdala. Classically conditioned avoidance of spe-
tional moods, and postures of an agent who gives an cific tastes, but not odors, can be acquired in anaes-
order to another are different if the agent is a 3-year- thetized rats, suggesting that the conditioned avoidance
old talking to a peer, a burglar with a victim, an army of tastes may be biologically different from learned
officer with a private, or a parent with an adolescent. avoidance to novel sights (Rattoni, Forthman, Sanchez,
The behavior of temperamentally inhibited children in Perez, & Garcia, 1988). Unfortunately, scientists do not
unfamiliar social situations is not exactly like the profile know the circuits that mediate the conscious reports of
of those who acquired their shy, timid demeanor through anxiety over a future threat or challenge. We do know
experience alone. The former group displays fewer that children and adults can report feeling anxious
spontaneous smiles and greater muscle tension. Thus, without any accompanying peripheral physiological
we should not treat the predicate is shy as a quality changes. In light of this evidence, it seems reasonable to
separable from the childs life history, physiology, and reject the idea of a single fear state and to assume, until
the context of observation. This suggestion applies to all data prove otherwise, that discrepancy, classical condi-
predicates referring to psychological dispositions. tioning, biologically significant events, anticipation of
Inhibited and uninhibited profiles appear to be heri- future unpleasantness, as well as separation from a tar-
table. Identical twins are more similar in the display of get of attachment create different physiological and psy-
shy, timid behavior during childhood than are fraternal chological states. It is reassuring that physiologists also
twins (Emde et al., 1992; Matheny, 1983, 1990), thus reject the idea of a unitary state of physiological stress:
matching data on adults (Davis, Luce, & Kraus, 1994). Hemorrhage, hypotension, and hypoglycemia produce
The Institute of Behavioral Genetics at the University of different profiles of secratog release (Sapolsky, 1992).
Colorado has studied a large number of same-sex twin Each brain, or peripheral, site that participates in a
pairs at 14 and 21 months. The heritability coefficients circuit that produces a behavioral or biological reaction
for inhibited and uninhibited behavior, based on direct has a different responsibility. For example, a childs
observations were between .5 and .6 (Kagan & Saudino, bodily freezing to the sudden entrance of a stranger
2001). But readers should be aware of the fact that the wearing a mask requires a sensory cortex to register the
heritability equations that produce the estimates are features of the person, a parahippocampal region to de-
vulnerable to the critique of assuming additivity of tect the strangers discrepant features, and an amygdala
genes and environment, ignoring epistasis, and failing to to provoke the neurons of the central gray to produce the
measure the environmental factors directly to compute immobility. Scientists emphasize the contribution of the
the interaction between genes and experience. amygdala because it is the most immediate origin of pro-
jections to the structures that produce the immobility.
But a child with no sensory cortex could not perceive the
The Biological Bases for Reaction to
person and therefore would not freeze. If the variable of
Unfamiliarity
interest had been the perception of the unfamiliar per-
The brain states created by unfamiliarity, classical con- son, rather than the immobility, scientists would empha-
ditioning, evolutionarily significant stimuli, and antici- size the significance of the sensory cortex.
pation of the future involve different neural circuits. Scientists are biased to attribute causal status to the
This claim rests, in part, on evidence from animal stud- structure or process that is the immediate origin of a set
ies (Treit et al., 1993a, 1993b) and the fact that the of observations. The ancients believed that the emotions
most probable profile of responses is different for these of love and anger originated in the heart because a per-
four incentives. An unfamiliar event, for example, a ception of a racing heart always preceded the emotional
person with a mask, typically produces cessation of ac- experience. Suppose that available technology permitted
tivity in a 2-year-old child. This response is mediated psychologists to quantify moment-to-moment changes in
200 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

a number of neurotransmitters and modulators during tress to separation at home, or in the laboratory, and
the presentation of visual and auditory stimuli to young cries of fear to discrepant events in 1-year-olds and why
infants and they discovered that variation in norepineph- a temperamental vulnerability to a fear reaction to dis-
rine secretion from the locus ceruleus predicted varia- crepancy and a childs attachment classification are
tion in inhibited behavior in the 2nd year of life. Under only modestly related (Kagan, 1994). One implication
these conditions, the scientists might have argued that of these facts is that the generalizations about fear or
the responsivity of the locus ceruleus was the basis for anxiety based on separation from caretakers, whether in
the temperamental biases. The dependent variable se- monkeys or children, should not be applied uncritically
lected influences, in a serious way, the brain sites or to the reactions displayed to unfamiliarity, threat, or
physiologies awarded causal status. A painful event conditioned cues for aversive events.
evokes activity in the amygdala and the HPA. Scientists The concept of a basic fear circuit is too ambitious.
who measure fMRI activity in the amygdala will empha- The meaning of fear state is different when the defin-
size the significance of this structure while those who ing referent is (a) a self-report, ( b) a change in behavior,
quantified increases in cortisol would award more im- or (c) an increased activity in a neural circuit. If this
portance to the HPA axis. No temperamental bias is lo- suggestion is valid, we will need different theoretical
cated in a particular brain structure. All a scientist can constructs for fear depending upon which referent is
say is that a particular structure is relevant. used. Even if a particular brain structure were involved
One obstacle to progress in understanding tempera- in all of the above phenomena, that fact would be insuf-
mental vulnerabilities to variation in avoidance of the ficient to assume that the states, which were identical
unfamiliar is the assumption that there is only one basic for complex events and shared a single feature, were es-
fear state with variation in intensity. An infant mon- sentially similar. Although all mammals are capable of
keys distress calls to separation from the mother, a internal fertilization, there is extraordinary diversity
quail chicks immobility to restriction of movement, an among them in other systems.
increase in heart rate or blood pressure to a conditioned Finally, there is the possibility that, as with parental
stimulus that had been paired with shock, and flight reports and behavioral observations, the description of
from a novel object are sometimes regarded as indexes of the brain events that contribute to a psychological state
the same basic fear state. These theoretical discussions of fear and the verbal descriptions of an agent are incom-
of fear ignore the variation in species, incentive event, mensurableone cannot replace one set of sentences
and specific response quantified and assume that the with the other without changing the intended meaning.
same emotional state is generated in each of the above
instances. This assumption is likely to be incorrect.
The Functions of the Amygdala
Other psychologists (Panksepp, 1990) claim that sep-
aration of a mammalian infant from its mother elicits a Observations of 4-month-old infants exposed to visual,
fear state. Separation, unlike unfamiliarity or a condi- auditory, and olfactory stimulation point to the possible
tioned stimulus that signals pain, produces distress vo- physiological bases for the inhibited and uninhibited
calizations, not freezing, defensive aggression, or flight. temperamental profiles that emerge after the first birth-
A kitten faced with an unfamiliar event often shows day. The early infant behaviors that are predictive of
arching of the back mediated by the central gray. A kit- these two categories can be understood if we assume
ten separated from its mother displays different behav- that some infants are born with a low threshold of ex-
iors. Thus, it is probably an error to use the distress that citability in the amygdala and its projections to the ven-
accompanies separation as a model for fear. This is not tral striatum, hypothalamus, cingulate, frontal cortex,
to say that the state created by separation is not interest- central gray, and medulla.
ing or of theoretical importance, but this state is proba- The amygdala consists of many neuronal collections
bly not the best probe for understanding fear to threat, a each with a distinct pattern of connectivity, neurochem-
conditioned cue, or to novelty. It is of interest that post- istry, and functions. Each collection projects to at least
traumatic stress disorder usually follows events that are 15 different sites, and receives inputs from about the
dangerous, such as earthquakes, or those that engender a same number of regions, resulting in about 600 known
combination of fear and guilt; for example, witnessing amygdalar connections (Petrovich, Canteras, & Swan-
an atrocity (Pynoos et al., 1987). That is one reason why son, 2001; Stefanacci & Amaral, 2002). Although a sim-
there is a low correlation between the occurrence of dis- plification, most anatomists conceptualize the amygdala
Inhibited versus Uninhibited Children 201

as composed of three basic areas: (1) the basolateral, (2) unfamiliar ones because not all unfamiliar events pose a
the corticomedial, and (3) the central areas. The basolat- threat and some threats are not novel. A female Diana
eral area, which transmits its information to the central monkey issues a distinct vocalization to the unexpected
nucleus, receives rich thalamic and cortical inputs from alarm call of a male leopard. However, she does not vo-
many external sensory origins, and some input from the calize to the same male leopard call if it occurs a second
viscera, and is reciprocally connected to cortex, hip- time a few minutes later, even though the leopard re-
pocampus, hypothalamus, basal ganglia, brain stem, and mains a threat because the call is no longer a discrepant
the bed nucleus. The behavioral reactions of flight or at- and therefore not a surprise. This monkey would have
tack are mediated primarily by projections from the ba- vocalized if the second sound had been an eagles shriek
solateral nucleus to the ventromedial striatum and (Seyfarth & Cheney, 2003). The monkey vocalized
ventral pallidum (Fudge, Kunishio, Walsh, Richard, & when the potentially dangerous auditory event was unex-
Haber, 2002). pected but not when the same sign of a dangerous event
The corticomedial nucleus receives primarily olfac- was anticipated. Earlier writers have suggested that
tory and taste information and projects to the hippocam- mammals show signs of fear to unfamiliar events (Hebb,
pus, thalamus, hypothalamus, and central nucleus. The 1946; Valentine, 1930), and animals with a lesioned
central nucleus, like the basolateral, receives input from amygdala fail to avoid unfamiliar animals or objects.
taste, vision, audition, and the viscera, but most impor- That is why infant monkeys with lesions of the amygdala
tant, from the basolateral and corticomedial areas. who had been permanently separated from their mother
Further, the central area is the origin of a large num- when they were 6 months of age did not preferentially
ber of projections to the bed nucleus, cortex, basal fore- choose to approach the mother rather than an unfamiliar
brain, hypothalamus, brain stem, and autonomic nervous animal (Bauman, Lavenex, Mason, Capitanio, & Ama-
system. Projections from the central nucleus are more ral, 2004). The lesioned animals showed no preference
responsible for internal, bodily changes that include se- because they did not experience the state of uncertainty
cretion of hormones, reactivity to the autonomic nerv- usually provoked by an unfamiliar adult animal.
ous system, and subtle alterations in posture and muscle Scientists began to focus on the amygdalas contribu-
tone. Some investigators have suggested that the central tion to fear rather than surprise after Brown, Kalish,
nucleus is activated primarily by acute, punctate events and Farber (1951) reported that the magnitude of an an-
to produce a transient reaction. More continuously imals body startle to a loud acoustic probe was en-
stressful conditions activate the bed nucleus of the stria hanced when a light that had been previously paired
terminalis to create a more chronic state. The level of with electric shock was presented to a rat just before the
CRH in the central nucleus is correlated with an ani- loud acoustic stimulus. These investigators assumed that
mals reactivity to a phasic event; CRH level in the bed rats should be afraid of a light that signaled electric
nucleus is more clearly correlated with reactivity to a shock, and therefore concluded that the larger startles
chronic stressor (Walker, Toufexis, & Davis, 2003). This reflected a state of fear. This assumption was followed
difference suggests a contrast between an acute state of by the elegant research of LeDoux (2000), Davis et al.
vigilance produced by seeing a snake or a spider with a (1994), and others, indicating that the thalamus and the
chronic state of worry over the future. The central nu- amygdala were necessary for the acquisition of a condi-
cleus is needed for the acute state, while the bed nucleus tioned reaction of body immobility, potentiated startle,
maintains the prolonged state of uncertainty to a longer or autonomic reactivity to a neutral stimulus that had
lasting threat. However, this rule has exceptions (Fendt, signaled an electric shock. Soon a large majority of sci-
Enders, & Apfelbach, 2003). entists had concluded that the conditioned stimulus cre-
ated a state of fear in the animal. This inference was
Fear or Surprise attractive because the concept of fear played an impor-
There is disagreement over the events most likely to ac- tant theoretical role in the middle of the twentieth cen-
tivate the amygdala and the psychological states that tury when psychoanalytic theory was popular. Fear
follow. The central issue is whether the amygdala reacts seemed closely related to anxiety, and Freud had made
primarily to imminently threatening events to produce anxiety the central culprit in neuroses. In addition, the
states of fear or to unfamiliar ones to produce states of Diagnostic Statistical Manual of the American Psychi-
surprise. Dangerous events should create distinctly dif- atric Association had made the anxiety disorders a
ferent states in brain, and, subsequently, in mind than major mental illness category. This evidence persuaded
202 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Ohman and Mineka (2001, 2003) to suggest that the ness to receive information from the parahippocampal re-
amygdala reacts primarily to signs of danger rather than gion indicating that an event that captured attention is de-
to unfamiliarity. They argued that all animals inherit a viant from the agents stored representations. Surprise is a
fear module ( located in the amygdala) that reacts, with- possible name for the psychological state created by such
out conscious awareness and free of cognitive control, to experiences (Whalen, 1998). It is less clear what name(s)
events that pose a threat to the integrity of the body. is most appropriate for the brain state. But the brain and
Snakes are presumed to be a classic example of such an emergent psychological states that define surprise, follow-
event. There are serious problems with this position. ing exposure to an unfamiliar event, are different from
First, the behavioral reactions of most monkeys, chim- those that represent a state of fear to the imminent possi-
panzees, and human infants to a snake are no different from bility of harm. It is important to differentiate between an
their reaction to discrepant events that are harmless, like a unfamiliar event and an unexpected change in the sensory
tortoise or seaweed (Marks, 1987). Monkeys born and surround (e.g., a sudden loud sound), because the latter
reared in a laboratory, and therefore protected from contact need not activate the reciprocal connections between the
with live snakes, showed a longer period of motor inhibition parahippocampal region and the amygdala.
to the presentation of a snake, whether alive or an artifact,
than to blue masking tape. However that restraint only oc-
HIGH AND LOW REACTIVITY TO
curred on the first testing session. During later sessions,
UNFAMILIARITY
the animals showed no more restraint to the snake than to
the masking tape. Moreover, a majority of animals failed to
It is assumed, but not yet proven, that 4-month-old in-
show any difference in withdrawal behavior to the snake,
fants who show high levels of vigorous motor activity, a
compared to the harmless masking tape (Nelson, Shelton,
great deal of muscle tension, and frequent irritability to
& Kalin, 2003). If snakes were a biologically potent incen-
a standard laboratory battery composed of visual, audi-
tive for a fear state, motor restraint should not have habitu-
tory, and olfactory stimulation possess low thresholds of
ated so quickly and the majority of monkeys, rather than
excitability in the basolateral and central areas of the
just 30%, should have shown a withdrawal reaction.
amygdala and their projections. These infants, who rep-
School-age children from a Dakota Indian tribe in Man-
resent about 20% of an unselected healthy Caucasian
itoba asked to recall the single most frightening event of
sample, are called high reactive. A complementary
their earlier years, most often named either a large domes-
group who showed low levels of motor arousal and mini-
tic animal, like a bull or horse, that had frightened them or
mal irritability to the same battery, about 40% of the
a ghost or witch-like figure they believed carried the chil-
sample, are assumed to have higher amygdalar thresh-
dren away if they disobeyed. Very few children named
olds to stimulation and are called low reactive. The two
snakes, even though snakes are common in this area (Wal-
patterns of reaction to unfamiliar events are less clear in
lis, 1954). A critical fact is that select neurons in the
much younger infants because the connectivity between
amygdala as well as in the bed nucleus, hippocampus, and
the amygdala and sites mediating motor activity and dis-
brain stem respond reliably to discrepant events, whether
tress are not yet mature (Weber, Watts, & Richardson,
or not they are harmful (Wilson & Rolls, 1993). Further,
2003). However, as noted earlier, the amygdala is part of
the reactivity of these neurons habituates often rapidly as
a circuit that includes the thalamus, sensory and associ-
the event loses its unfamiliarity (LaBar, Gatenby, Gore,
ation cortex, and parahippocampal region that are also
LeDoux, & Phelps, 1998). Adults in an fMRI scanner look-
activated by unfamiliar events. Although the amygdala
ing at faces with neutral expressions showed greater amyg-
is the proximal origin of the motor activity in crying, it
dalar activation to new, compared with familiar faces, even
remains possible that the variation in behavior might be
though no face had a fearful, or threatening expression
due, in part, to variation in the excitability of another
(Schwartz, Wright, Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003), as did
structure in this circuit that primes the amygdala.
adults who had shown characteristic avoidance of unfamil-
iarity during childhood (Schwartz et al., 2003).
Childhood Derivatives of High and Low Reactivity
Thus, the evidence implies that the amygdala reacts
primarily to unfamiliar events, rather than to events that Kagan and colleagues observed a large number of chil-
are threatening; unless, the animal knows the event is dan- dren at 14 and 21 months who had been high- or low-
gerous. A basic property of the amygdala is a prepared- reactive infants at 4 months. The laboratory batteries at
High and Low Reactivity to Unfamiliarity 203

14 and 21 months consisted of a variety of procedures low-reactive boys who smiled the most (more than 50
designed to elicit uncertainty, including intrusion into times) had been the most relaxed infants when they were
the childs personal space (placing electrodes on the 4 months old (Kagan, 1997).
body or a blood pressure cuff on the arm), exposure to A small proportion of high-reactives talked and
unfamiliar objects (robots, toy animals, or papier-mch smiled frequently during the examination at 4 years. Al-
puppets), and encounters with unfamiliar people who though their environmental histories probably influ-
behaved in an atypical way or wore a novel costume. A enced this profile, it is of interest that during the
child who cried to any one of these events or did not ap- original assessment at 4 months these children smiled
proach any of the unfamiliar objects when requested more often than the majority of high-reactive infants
to do so was coded as fearful for that episode. High- who were more subdued at 4.5 years. In addition, the
reactive infants were significantly more fearful at 14 spontaneous high-reactives had a lower and more vari-
and 21 months than were low-reactive infants. About able heart rate when they were 14 months old, implying
one-third of the high-reactives were highly fearful at that this small group of high-reactives may have pos-
both 14 and 21 months, and only 3% showed minimal sessed a special temperamental quality.
fear at the two ages. By contrast, one-third of the low- The fact that spontaneous conversation with a
reactives were minimally fearful at the same two ages stranger is a sensitive sign of uncertainty after 3 or 4
and only 4% showed high fear. It is of interest that the years of age is supported by a different sample of chil-
remaining children showed intermediate levels of fear- dren classified as inhibited or uninhibited at 21 or 31
fulness (Kagan, 1994). months and observed again at 5, 7, and 13 years of age.
Support for the claim that these two groups of in- Infrequent talking to an unfamiliar examiner was the
fants, defined by a combination of motor arousal and best correlate of the original classification of an inhib-
crying to stimulation, represent qualitative categories ited temperament at all three ages. It is possible that the
that should not be placed on a continuum of arousal small number of children with elective mutism represent
comes from the fact that when the duration of crying to extremely inhibited children (Black, 1992).
the 4-month battery was either zero (the infant did not Restraint on spontaneous conversation in an unfamil-
cry at all) or longer than 8 seconds, the correlation be- iar social situation seems to be analogous to freezing to
tween degree of motor activity at 4 months and fearful- a novel event in animals. Both responses are mediated by
ness at 14 months was close to zero for each of the two the fibers of the central gray that are innervated by pro-
cry groups (r = .13 for the subjects who did not cry at jections from the amygdala. But restraint on sponta-
all, and r = .10 for those who cried more than 8 sec- neous speech is not a sensitive measure of uncertainty to
onds). Conversely, when the infants were divided into the unfamiliar until after the third birthday. There was
groups with motor scores that were low (<40) or high no significant relation between spontaneous vocaliza-
(>50), the correlation between duration of crying at 4 tion in the 1st and 2nd year and spontaneous speech
months and fear at 14 months was also low (r = .25 for with an adult at 4 years of age. Although both variables
those with low motor behavior; r = .08 for those with refer to vocal sounds, the responses are different in
high motor scores). Thus, once an infant had passed the meaning. Absence of vocalization in the 1st year re-
criteria for motor arousal and crying that defined high flects low affective arousal; restraint on speech at 4
reactivity, additional motor activity or crying (in that years reflects anticipatory anxiety. One-year-old chil-
category) had minimal consequences for how fearful the dren are not old enough to be concerned with the exam-
child would become later. iners evaluation of them or to anticipate the laboratory
When the high- and low-reactive infants were evalu- procedures that might be administered.
ated at 4.5 years of age, the former group was much The high- and low-reactive 4.5-year-old children also
more subdued and talked and smiled less frequently dur- differed in their social behavior with two other unfamil-
ing a 1-hour laboratory assessment with an unfamiliar iar children of the same age and sex when trios of chil-
female examiner. By contrast, the low-reactive children dren were observed in a laboratory playroom for a half
were spontaneous; they asked questions, commented on hour. Almost 66% of the low-reactive but fewer than
the procedures, and smiled and laughed more often. The 10% of the high-reactive children were outgoing and so-
differences between the two groups in smiling were ciable with the unfamiliar children. By contrast, 40% of
more dramatic than the differences in talking. The three the high-reactives were avoidant and quiet compared
204 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

with only 10% of the low-reactives. Although there was less. Home observations on 50 high- and 50 low-reactive
significant preservation of inhibited or uninhibited tem- firstborn infants indicated that a mothers actions with
peramental styles from 4 months to 4.5 years, these the infant affected the probability that a high-reactive
data also imply that environmental factors affect each child would become inhibited. A nurturing parent who
childs phenotype. consistently protected her high-reactive infant from all
The 4-year-old children who had been high reactive minor stresses made it more, rather than less, difficult
were also more intimidated by the examiner. In one epi- for that child to control an initial urge to retreat from
sode, the female examiner asked the older child to per- strangers and unfamiliar events. Equally accepting
form some actions that would be prohibited by most mothers who set firm limits for their children, making
parents. For example, she opened a photo album contain- mundane age-appropriate demands for cleanliness or
ing pictures of herself, took out a large color photo- conformity, helped their high-reactive infants overcome
graph, and, as she handed it to the child, said, This is their fearfulness (Arcus, 1991).
my favorite picture; tear up my favorite picture. More The role of experience is illustrated by the variability
low- than high-reactives either asked her why they in each temperamental category. We examined the vari-
should perform that act or, in the case of five children, ability in behavior with the examiner at 4.5 years within
refused to do so. Moreover, their resistance was not ac- two very different groups: high-reactive girls who
companied by any obvious signs of anxiety; they simply showed high fear at 14 months (N = 16) and low-
appeared to be less afraid of disobeying the requests of reactive girls who showed low fear at 14 months (N =
an authority figure when that request required them to 28). Although the former, as expected, had significantly
violate a norm they had acquired. Almost all the high- fewer spontaneous comments and smiles compared with
reactives were reluctant to disobey and, after a 5- to 10- the latter, the variation within each group was large. For
second delay, tore a small corner from the photograph. example, although one-third of the high-reactive, fearful
Parents of high-reactive infants who became inhib- girls had fewer than 10 spontaneous comments, one-
ited preschoolers reported that their children were more third had more than 50 comments. One-third displayed
sensitive to criticism and cried, had a tantrum, or be- fewer than 5 smiles, but one-third had more than 30
came subdued when chastised. On the face of it, this re- smiles. Within the low-reactive, low-fear girls, one-
sponse does not follow from the hypothesis that third had fewer than 27 comments and fewer than 21
high-reactive-inhibited children react to unfamiliarity smiles, but one-third had more than 70 comments and
with uncertainty. Parental criticism is not an unfamiliar more than 35 smiles. It is fair to suggest that this broad
event; a person dressed in a clown costume is discrepant. range of outcomes at 4.5 years within these classes of
This relation can be understood, however, if we assume children is due, in part, to differential experience. The
that a child older than 3 or 4 years is continually gener- envelope of developmental trajectories for each tem-
ating representations of the present and immediate fu- peramental group is not fixed in a rigid way (Kagan &
ture. If an event, like a chastisement, is unexpected, it Snidman, 2004).
resembles a discrepant event. Because the amygdala and Another sample of infants, classified as high reactive
its circuits are excited by discrepancy, the child might or aroused, was observed at 9, 14, 24, and 48 months
react with crying, withdrawal, or a tantrum. It is possi- (Fox et al., 2001). About one-half of the high reactives
ble that adolescents and adults who were high-reactive- retained an inhibited persona through 2 years of age,
inhibited children are more easily threatened by and one-third preserved this profile through the fourth
encounters with beliefs, opinions, or philosophical birthday. However, the childs gender and form of rear-
premises that are not in accord with their firm beliefs. ing influenced the retention or loss of an inhibited per-
sona. High-reactive boys preserved their inhibition more
Inf luence of Experience than girls, and those who were placed in day care during
the early years were less likely to preserve behavioral
Although physiological products of genes make a modest inhibition than those raised only at home. Fox et al.
contribution to the inhibited and uninhibited behavioral (2001) speculated that placement in day care probably
profiles, they share power with experience. Over one- influences the preservation of behavioral inhibition be-
third of high-reactive infants were not exceptionally cause exposure to unfamiliar peers and settings, and
fearful or shy in the 2nd year; a small number were fear- freedom from possible overprotection at home, might
High and Low Reactivity to Unfamiliarity 205

allow these children to learn coping strategies to unfa- sociable and extraverted, although the childrens de-
miliarity. Rubin, Cheah, and Fox (2001) coded maternal scriptions of their own behavior did not correlate highly
behavior while 4-year-old inhibited children were asked with their contemporary behavior with the examiner or
to build a block tower with their mothers. Mothers who with the mothers descriptions. It is important to note
were oversolicitous and excessively responsive to their that the 4-month temperamental category was a better
childs crying were most likely to have children who predictor of behavior at age 11 than the childs fearful-
were reticent when playing with peers. ness at 14 or 21 months.
A sample of 164 of the children in Kagans sample
were evaluated when they were 7.5 years old. Data from
maternal reports, teacher descriptions and observations Biological Assessments
revealed that 26% of the total sample had anxious symp-
toms. This group of 42 anxious children was compared Four biological measures gathered on these subjects at
with 107 control children without anxious symptoms. age 11 were regarded as indirect signs of amygdalar ac-
The 7-year-olds who had been high-reactive infants tivity, even though many brain sites participate in each
were most likely to possess anxious symptoms; 45% of biological reaction.
high-reactives, but only 15% of low-reactives received
this classification. Moreover, the high-reactives with EEG Asymmetry
anxious symptoms were most likely to have screamed in One measure was an asymmetry in EEG activation that
fear during the 21-month assessment when a person favors the left or the right hemisphere, where desynchro-
dressed in a clown costume entered a room where they nization of alpha frequencies is the index of activation.
had been playing. About 20% of the sample of high- The left frontal area is usually more active than the right
reactives were consistently inhibited at four ages: 14 when individuals are relaxed and in a happy mood, but
and 21 months, as well as 4.5 and 7.5 years. Not one the right is more active than the left when the individual
high-reactive infant was consistently uninhibited across is in a state of uncertainty, fear, or anxiety (Davidson,
all four evaluations (Kagan & Snidman, 2004). 2003a, 2003b; Fox, 1991, 1994). The results from a num-
These children were evaluated again when they were ber of studies suggest that children and adults who show
between 10 and 12 years of age. About one-third of the right frontal activation in the EEG are more likely to
high- and low-reactives displayed a style of social be- react to a discrepant event, unfamiliar situation or new
havior with the examiner that was in accord with their challenge with greater dysphoria and/or an anxious
earlier infant temperament, while only 16% showed be- avoidant response. Individuals who show left frontal ac-
haviors that were inconsistent with their infant tempera- tivation show the complementary pattern of a more re-
ment. More high-reactives were quiet, spoke in a soft laxed, happy mood and an eagerness to engage new
voice, sat stiffly in the chair, and often looked away experiences or challenges (Davidson, Ekman, & Saron,
from the examiner. By contrast, more low-reactives 1994; Schmidt & Fox, 1994; Tomarken, Davidson, &
were relaxed and talked and smiled frequently with the Henriques, 1990). A similar relation was observed in in-
examiner. The number of spontaneous comments with fants. Ten-month-old infants showed left frontal activa-
the examiner at age 11 was a particularly sensitive sign tion to a smiling, but not to a crying, adult, but they were
of early temperament. High-reactives preserved a seri- more likely to show right frontal activation to the ap-
ous facial expression from 14 months to 11 years, while proach of a stranger (Fox & Davidson, 1987) and to a
the low-reactives smiled and laughed frequently at temporary separation from the mother (Davidson & Fox,
every assessment. When the children filled out Q-sorts 1989; Fox & Davidson, 1988). Further, the smiles of in-
to describe themselves, the low-reactives were more fants are usually accompanied by left frontal activation,
likely than the high-reactives to report that they were while behavioral signs of fear are accompanied by right
happy most of the time. Forty percent of the high- frontal activation (Fox & Davidson, 1987). Moreover,
reactives retained an inhibited profile from 4.5 to 11 the tendency to show right frontal activation to tempo-
years of age, and 70% of the low-reactives retained an rary separation from the mother is a stable trait in the
uninhibited profile across this same period. Further, 2nd year (Fox, Bell, & Jones, 1992). Both socially reti-
50% of low-reactives, compared with 13% of high- cent and solitary-passive childrenusing Rubins defi-
reactives, were described by their mother as extremely nitionsshow right frontal activation. But only the
206 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

reticent group shows low alpha power across the entire ing an encounter with an unfamiliar adult and a novel ob-
scalp, suggesting a higher level of cortical arousal, and ject. The children returned to the laboratory at 4 years
perhaps a hypervigilant mood. of age for a peer play session. Each child was placed in a
When confronted with a stressful or unexpected in- quartet of four unfamiliar children of the same sex. Each
centive, like temporary maternal absence, young chil- quartet consisted of one behaviorally inhibited child,
dren show greater EEG activation on the right, one uninhibited child, and two children whose inhibition
compared with the left, frontal area (Dawson, Panagi- scores were close to the mean based on their behavior at
otides, Klinger, & Hill, 1992). Thus, it is of interest that 2 years of age. The children were observed during free
inhibited, compared with uninhibited, children showed play with peers and during a set of standard tasks using
greater activation in the right frontal area under resting Rubins coding criteria (Rubin, 2000). This scale allows
conditions (Davidson, 1994a, 1994b). High-reactive in- identification of children who are socially reticent (iso-
fants showed greater activation of the right frontal area lated, staring, unoccupied, and displaying long latencies
when tested during the 1st and 2nd years; low-reactive before they speak) and socially competent children who
infants showed greater activation of the left frontal area initiate play and often smile and talk.
(Fox, Calkins, & Bell 1994). Because neural activity in Measures of EEG were recorded several weeks ear-
the amygdala is transmitted to the frontal lobe, by the lier. Children showing right frontal activation were
nucleus basalis, it is possible that greater desynchro- more likely to be reticent than those who displayed left
nization of alpha frequencies on the right frontal lobe frontal activation. Maternal ratings of the childs shy-
reflect greater activity in the right amygdala (Kapp, ness were correlated with both their play behavior and
Supple, & Whalen, 1994; Lloyd & Kling, 1991). The direction of activation. Of great interest is the fact that
distribution of receptors for CRH and/or the level of the very sociable children who showed right frontal acti-
CRH may contribute to the asymmetry of activation. vation were described by their mothers as displaying be-
Monkeys who showed a stable and extreme right frontal haviors classified as externalizing (i.e., disobedience
activation across a 4-year interval had high CRH levels and mild aggression). But the reticent 4-year-olds who
(Kalin et al., 2000). Further, rhesus monkeys who com- were also right frontal active were described as display-
bined high cortisol levels with high fear to unfamiliarity ing internalizing problems (anxious, tense, and ex-
were most likely to show right frontal activation (Kalin tremely shy). Thus, among children who showed right
et al., 1998). And 6-month-old infants with high cortisol frontal activation, typical behavior at home was a func-
levels were biased to show right frontal activation (Buss tion of their temperament. And inhibited 7-year-olds
et al., 2003). showed a more obvious increase in the level of right
The 11-year-old children who had been high-reactive frontal activation to the challenge of preparing a speech
infants had greater activation in the right than in the left than uninhibited children (Schmidt, Fox, Schulkin, &
hemisphere at parietal sites. The high-reactives who, in Gold, 1999).
addition, had been highly fearful in the 2nd year were Perhaps, the most relevant work from the Fox labora-
more likely than low-reactives to be right hemisphere tory is based on a longitudinal study of a large group of
active at frontal sites as well. Further, the low-reactive, infants who were classified at 4 months (using a battery
11-year-old boys who described themselves as happy similar to the one used by the Kagan group) into high-
most of the time had greater left frontal activation than reactive (14% of the group), low-reactive (15% of the
low-reactive boys who did not report a chronically group), and infants who displayed high motor activity,
happy mood. vocalization, and smiling, but minimal distress (9%; we
The earlier research of Fox, Rubin, and their col- might call these children aroused). The high-reactives
leagues, which was an incentive for the work by Kagans were most likely to show right frontal activation at 9
laboratory, along with recent evidence from Foxs labo- months while the aroused children were more likely to
ratory, affirms a relation between direction of asymme- show left frontal activation. In addition, the high-
try of activation and a temperamental bias for inhibited reactives had less alpha power at both left and right
or uninhibited behavior. For example, Fox et al. (1995) frontal sites, implying higher cortical arousal.
studied 48 children who were seen at 2 years of age and These children were observed again at 4 and 7 years
later at age 4. At age 2, the children were observed in of age as they played in groups of 4 unfamiliar children
settings designed to elicit behavioral inhibition, includ- of the same sex. Children who had been high-reactive as
High and Low Reactivity to Unfamiliarity 207

infants and had shown right frontal activation at 9 The P300 and N400 waveforms represent brain activ-
months were reticent with peers at 4 years of age; a sim- ity involving the relation between an incentive event and
ilar result emerged at 7 years (Polak, Fox, Henderson, & existing representations. Earlier waveforms in the ERP
Rubin, 2004). The high-reactives with right frontal acti- that occur between 100 and 200 ms reveal different rela-
vation at 9 months were not only reticent but also were tions to behavior. Fox and colleagues studied a wave-
described by their teachers as having internalizing fea- form called mismatch negativity (MMN) which occurs
tures. The aroused infants who displayed left frontal ac- to a novel tone inserted in a series of identical ones. The
tivation had a complementary set of traits. MMN, which occurs with a latency of 200 to 250 ms, is
generated by neurons in the primary auditory cortex.
Wave 5 in the Brain Stem Auditory Evoked Children in two independent samples classified as inhib-
Potential (BAEP) ited showed a smaller MMN to the infrequent auditory
The biological measure that best separated the high- stimulus than did controls (Bar-Haim et al., 2003). In
from the low-reactives at age 11 was the magnitude of addition, 9-month-olds classified as high reactive at 4
the brain stem evoked potential from the inferior col- months showed a smaller positive waveform to a dis-
liculus, the fifth structure in the auditory chain. The crepant auditory stimulus than others (Marshall,
waveform generated by the colliculus, called wave 5, oc- Hardin, & Fox, 2004). These two results appear to be in-
curs within 6 ms of the onset of sound. Amygdalar ac- consistent with the fact that high-reactives at 11 years of
tivity enhances the excitability of the inferior colliculus age showed larger negative waveforms to discrepant
through projections to the locus ceruleus and the central scenes. However, the discrepant scenes shown to the 11-
gray, which synapse on the colliculus. This anatomical year-olds were meaningful and probably activated the
fact means that children with a more excitable amygdala parahippocampal area and its reciprocal connections to
should have a larger wave 5 to a series of clicks. This ex- the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala. A large nega-
pectation was affirmed for high-reactives had larger tive waveform to an unfamiliar scene (e.g., a chair with
wave 5 values than low-reactives (Kagan & Snidman, one leg) does not have the same significance as the mis-
2004). The high-reactives who had been inhibited with match negativity waveform to a deviant tone because the
unfamiliar peers at 4.5 years of age had larger wave 5 latter need not involve the amygdala.
values than the high-reactives who had been more socia- Most school-age children show a longer response la-
ble. Further, the 15% of the sample who smiled infre- tency on trials following those on which they made an
quently at every assessment had higher wave 5 values errora psychological trait called reflectivity. It is of
than those who smiled frequently. interest, therefore, that 7-year-olds rated as high on shy-
ness and inhibitory control ( based on Rothbarts CBQ)
Event-Related Potential displayed a larger waveform, called error related nega-
A third variable that separated high- and low-reactives tivity, on trials in which they made an error, as well as a
was the event-related potential (ERP) to unfamiliar longer response time on the subsequent trial (Hender-
scenes. The amygdala sends projections to the locus son, 2002).
ceruleus, ventral tegmentum, and basal nucleus of
Meynert, which in turn project to cortical pyramidal Sympathetic Activity
neurons that mediate the magnitude of the ERP. Chil- Activity in the sympathetic nervous system also re-
dren with a more excitable amygdala, therefore, might flects amygdalar activity because the latter structure
show larger P300 or N400 to discrepant events. Again, projects to the sympathetic nervous system. A spectral
the data affirmed expectation, for high-reactives analysis of supine heart rate revealed that more high-
showed larger N400s to ecologically invalid scenes than low-reactives had more power in the low fre-
(e.g., a childs head on an animals body) than low- quency spectrum, which reflects both sympathetic and
reactives. Further, high-reactives with the largest mag- parasympathetic activity, and less power in the high
nitude waveforms to discrepancy at age 11 had more in- frequency band, which reflects vagal or parasympa-
tense symptoms of anxiety or depression 4 years later thetic activity. The combination of greater power in the
and showed shallower habituation of the N400 wave- low frequency band and a high resting heart rate was
form to a different set of discrepant scenes (Kagan & characteristic of one of every three high-reactives
Snidman, 2004). (compared with only one of five low-reactives). By
208 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

contrast, one of every two low-reactives, but only one second year and, in addition, had high values on all four
of sixteen high-reactives, displayed greater power in biological variables, but no low-reactive developed these
the higher frequency band and had a low heart rate. Not features. These results are remarkably similar to those
surprisingly, more children with high vagal tone smiled reported by Fox and his colleagues who followed three
more often during the 2nd year and described them- temperamental groups first classified at 4 months of age
selves as chronically happy at age 11. in a battery similar to the one employed in this study.
Further, more high- than low-reactive infants had However, it is important to appreciate that none of the
higher fetal heart rates (over 140 bpm) a few weeks be- biological variables has the same meaning or signifi-
fore birth, and higher 2-week postnatal sleeping heart cance across individuals with different temperaments.
rates while being held erect, but not when supine. Spec- The meaning of left frontal activation in the EEG pro-
tral analysis of the infants sleeping heart rates revealed files of the 11-year-olds in the Kagan and Snidman co-
that the high-reactive, compared with the low-reactive, hort provides an example. The low-reactives who were
infants had greater power in the low-frequency band described by their mother as extremely sociable and out-
( between .02 and .10 Hz) when held erect, suggesting going, smiled frequently with the examiner, and had low
greater sympathetic reactivity (Snidman, Kagan, Rior- levels of cortical arousal (as indexed by the beta to alpha
dan, & Shannon, 1995). A longitudinal study of 31 preg- ratio) showed extreme left frontal activation. But the
nant mothers and their fetuses revealed that fetuses with high reactives who were described as shy and timid,
high heart rates had less frequent positive affect at 6 smiled infrequently with the examiner, and had high
months of age (DiPietro, 1995). A variable heart rate in cortical arousal were equally likely to show extreme left
infancy, reflecting less sympathetic and more vagal frontal activation. This fact suggests that left frontal ac-
tone, is linked to a tendency to approach unfamiliarity tivation does not have the same meaning in youth with
(Richards & Cameron, 1989), unfamiliar people (Fox, different temperaments.
1989), and facial expressions of smiling and laughter It is likely that this result can be generalized. No
(Stifter, Fox, & Porges, 1989). Finally, 5- and 7-year-old measure, whether questionnaire reply, behavior, or biol-
inhibited, compared with uninhibited, children showed ogy, has a universal meaning across all individuals. This
greater pupillary dilation, greater cardiac acceleration, claim is supported by information on some of the 11-
and larger changes in blood pressure to cognitive stres- year-olds who were assessed at age 15 years. Six high-
sors (Kagan, 1994). reactive boys were extremely subdued and inhibited
The children Rubin classified as socially reticent dis- during a long interview with an examiner conducted in
played less vagal and higher sympathetic tone in the car- the home. The remaining 17 high-reactive boys were far
diovascular system than the solitary-passive or sociable less restrained. Right frontal activation, high sympa-
groups. However, the solitary-passive children who had thetic tone, and a high ratio of beta to alpha power, along
high baseline heart rates were more inhibited at 2 years with a large ERP to discrepant scenes at age 11 pre-
than those with low heart rates (Henderson et al., 2004). dicted the extreme inhibition in the six boys who were
Further, over the course of the laboratory observation at subdued. But low-reactive 15-year-old girls who showed
4 years of age, the children with higher heart rates began the opposite traits of garrulousness and sociability dur-
to resemble reticent children as the tasks became more ing the interview also showed high sympathetic tone and
structured. Finally, Schmidt, Fox, and Schulkin (1999) a high ratio of beta to alpha power. Once again, the the-
reported that behaviorally inhibited ( but not control) 7- oretical significance of a biological variable depended
year-olds displayed increases in heart rate and decreases on the type of individual on whom it was measured.
in heart rate variability to emotional challengessigns Despite this possibility, most studies of humans
of less vagal tone. children, college students, or aging adultstreat their
About one of every four high-reactives and one of samples as if they were relatively homogeneous; gender
every four low-reactives preserved their expected be- is the usual exception. This decision is surprising. No
havioral as well as biological profiles, while only one of behavioral biologist would gather data on a random sam-
twenty children classified as high- or low-reactive at 4 ple of dogs of different breeds or macaque monkeys
months developed a combination of behavior and biol- from different strains because they know that the strains
ogy that was characteristic of the complementary cate- would react in different ways to an incentive. Hence,
gory. Only 10% of high-reactives were fearful in the psychologists should gather some information on the bi-
High and Low Reactivity to Unfamiliarity 209

ology of their subjects. Some candidates include height, white spots on their coat that were free of melanin pig-
weight, body type, eye color, and, if possible, heart rate, mentation, floppy rather than stiff ears, an upturned
blood pressure, cortisol level, EEG asymmetry and ERP tail and a broader face. These physical features are de-
waveforms to varied incentives. rivatives of neural crest cells. In addition, the offspring
of the tame matings had lower levels of cortisol and
Facial Skeleton higher levels of serotonin metabolites and brain
High and low reactives differed in physical features, a dopamine (Trut, 1999). If a minimal fear of unfamiliar
fact supporting the belief that they belong to different adults is associated with distinct physical features in a
categories. Infants classified as high reactive at 4 fox we should not be surprised to find that high- and
months had narrower faces (the ratio of the width of the low-reactive children differ in eye color and body size.
face at the bizygomatichigh cheekboneto the length It is possible that the genes that mediate the time of mi-
of the face) when they were 14 months old compared gration of the neural crest cells, and perhaps molecular
with children classified as low reactive (Arcus & Kagan, features of these cells, are pleiotropic and contribute to
1995). The fact that facial skeleton differentiated the a cluster of physical and behavioral features. Nature
two temperamental groups implies the influence of a set works in unexpected ways.
of genes that affects features as diverse as the growth of Finally, it is worth noting that extreme values on be-
facial bone, ease of arousal in infancy, smiling, and havioral or biological variables often separated high-
avoidance of unfamiliar events. It is of interest that in- and low-reactives when mean scores did not. There were
bred mouse strains like A /JAX that are susceptible to many occasions when the correlations among variables
inhibition of palatal shelf growth following pharmaco- were low across the whole sample, but the children with
logical doses of glucocorticoids during gestation are values at either extreme were very different. For exam-
more fearful in an open field than strains like C57 BL/6 ple, 10% of high-reactives, but not one low-reactive, had
that are less susceptible to the influence of this steroid a z-score equal to or greater than .5 on the four differen-
on the growth of facial bone (W. Thompson, 1953; B. tiating biological variables; every one of these seven
Walker & Fraser, 1957). This fact implies that the genes children showed distinct behaviors at 4 months of age
that influence the growth of facial bone in response to that were indicative of high arousal. Persistent aggres-
glucocorticoids are correlated with those that monitor sion is also characteristic of a small group of children.
avoidance of novelty. A longitudinal study of boys from different laboratories
Second, 11-year-old high-reactives were a little revealed that only 4% of a very large sample showed
more likely than low-reactives to possess light blue persistent aggression across the childhood years (Brody
eyes and a small body size (24% of high-reactives, but et al., 2003). Male vervet monkeys who are at either ex-
only 7% of low-reactives had both features). The rela- treme on behavioral measures of impulsivity had a lower
tion of eye-color and body size to infant temperament social rank than a large number of animals whose scores
in Caucasian children may surprise some readers, but were in the middle of the distribution (Fairbanks,
will be less surprising to those who know about the 2001). Many psychologists place great faith in the infor-
changes in physical characteristics that accompany do- mativeness of mean values; any other parameter carries
mestication of wolves, foxes, mink, and cattle. The the stain of being less than perfect. This bias is irra-
most extensive work, conducted at a field station in tional. Because nonlinear functions are common in psy-
Siberian Russia, was initiated by D. Belyaev and car- chology, current statistical procedures that rely on the
ried on by his colleague, L. N. Trut, after Belyaevs mean often fail to reveal important relations. The reluc-
death (Trut, 1999). These investigators selectively tance to acknowledge the utility of examining extreme
bred tame male silver fox with tame females for over groups that might be qualitatively different from the
40 years. The wild form, which is not tame, has hairs rest of the sample is slowing progress in many domains
that are black at the base and silver-white at the outer of psychology.
edge, stiff, erect ears and a tail that turns down. How-
ever, the offspring of many generations of breeding
Temperaments Constrain
tame with tame animals displayed a number of physical
features that accompanied the increased tameness and Despite the predicted relation between the infant clas-
minimal fear of humans. The tame animals developed sifications of high and low reactivity and the behavior
210 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

and biological profiles at age 11, only one-third of the TEMPERAMENT AND ATTACHMENT
children in the high- and low-reactive groups actual-
ized a behavioral and biological profile that was in ac- There is a lively controversy surrounding the contribu-
cord with expectation. However, very few children tion of inhibited or uninhibited temperaments to a
showed the profile of the complementary group, a re- childs reactions in the Ainsworth Strange Situation
sult confirmed by Fox, Rubin, and their colleagues. and, therefore, to the classifications of secure or inse-
Most children displayed behavioral and biological pat- cure attachment (Connell & Thompson, 1986). The
terns characteristic of randomly selected middle-class Strange Situation is an unfamiliar setting; therefore, in-
Caucasian children. Apparently, many high-reactives hibited children should react with greater uncertainty,
had learned to cope with their earlier tendency to avoid when either left alone or with a stranger. As a result,
unfamiliar people and situations and were able to de- more of these children should be difficult to soothe
velop a persona that was not obviously shy or timid. when the mother returns and they are likely to be classi-
Thus, the prediction that a high-reactive infant would fied as type C resistant and insecurely attached.
not become a sociable, exuberant child with left frontal Infants who were classified type C at 14 months were
activation, a small wave 5, and high vagal tone can be behaviorally inhibited at 2 years, suggesting that the
made with much greater confidence than the prediction temperamental bias to be inhibited is contributing to
that this child would become a subdued, timid adoles- their behavior in the Strange Situation (Calkins & Fox,
cent with high levels of arousal in cortical, brain stem, 1992). Ten percent of Dutch infants assessed in the
and autonomic targets. Similarly, the prediction that a Strange Situation with their hired caregiver, mother, or
low-reactive will not become an extremely shy 11-year- father showed an insecure attachment to all three adults,
old with high biological arousal is more certain than implying a temperamental contribution to their behavior
the prediction that this child will be exuberant and in the laboratory (Goossens & van IJzendoorn, 1990).
show low biological arousal. Each temperament con- One group of authors, reflecting on these facts, wrote,
strains acquisition of the features of the complemen- Temperament does play a role in Strange Situation be-
tary category. Hidden beneath the common observation havior through its effects on the quality and intensity of
that the behaviors and moods of children change from the infants separation distress. Infants who are high on
infancy to adolescence is the persistence of tempera- fearfulness are likely to react more negatively to the
mental biases that prevent some from attaining a par- separation episodes (Thompson, Connell, & Bridges,
ticular psychological profile. 1988, p. 1109).
A temperamental bias eliminates many more out- A meta-analysis of data from seven different samples
comes than it determines and is like the basic form of (N = 498) revealed that the infants behavior in the labo-
the song of a particular species of bird. The animals ratory prior to the mother leaving the child predicted
genome constrains the basic architecture of the song better than chance the childs subsequent behavior and
but does not determine all of its features, for the adult the attachment classification. Infants classified as type
song depends on exposure to songs of conspecifics and C showed more crying and resistance to their mother
the opportunity to hear its own vocal sounds. The con- during episode 2, an episode that occurred prior to any
straining power of initial conditions, whether biologi- separation (Sagi, van IJzendoorn, & Koren-Karie, 1991;
cal or environmental, finds an analogy in a stone but see Fox, 1995).
rolling down a steep mountain over a 5-minute inter- Fox, Kimmerly, and Schaefer (1991) performed a
val. An observer will be able to eliminate a great many meta-analysis on studies that examined the concordance
final locations after each 10 seconds of descent, but it of attachment classifications ( based on the Strange Situ-
is not until the final second that she will be able to pre- ation) to the mother and the father in separate assess-
dict where the stone will come to rest. When the high ments. Infants classified as Type A, B, or C (avoidant,
promises of the genome project are met and parents secure, or resistant) to one parent were more likely than
can request the complete genomic analysis of their chance to be classified in the same way when they were
newborn, an expert will be better able to tell parents tested with the other parent. This concordance implies a
what the infant will not become than to inform them temperamental influence on behavior in the Strange Sit-
about the characteristics their infant will possess 2 uation, as well as the possibility that both the mother
decades later. and the father behave similarly with the infant.
Psychopathology and Temperament 211

Further support for the role of temperament is found example, only 10 of 40 school children who were kid-
in an independent study of 9- and 13-month-old infants. napped and terrorized for 2 days developed post-
A temperamental quality called proneness to distress, to- traumatic stress disorder (Terr, 1979). During the win-
gether with the mothers personality, were the best pre- ter of 1984, a sniper fired at a group of children on the
dictors of an insecure attachment in the child. playground of a Los Angeles elementary school. One
Specifically, infants who were prone to distress and, in child was killed, 13 were injured, and a siege followed.
addition, had mothers with high scores on a personality Clinicians interviewed the children 1 month later to de-
trait called constraintthese women were rigid, had termine who was experiencing extreme levels of anxiety.
traditional views, and avoided riskswere more likely Thirty-eight percent were judged anxious, but an equal
to be insecurely attached. There was no effect of the ma- proportion, 39%, seemed completely free of any unusual
ternal personality trait alone. Distressed infants reared level of anxiety. The important fact is that the children
by a mother who was low on constraint were more likely who were anxious 1 month after the trauma were those
to be securely attached. Among low-distress infants, who had shown an inhibited temperament prior to the
however, there was no relation between this maternal school violence (Pynoos et al., 1987).
personality trait and security of child attachment. Vari- A group of fourth and fifth grade children living in
ation in maternal behavior seems to be more important south Florida had been assessed for the presence of an
for infants who are vulnerable to distress than it is for anxious mood over a year before Hurricane Andrew
those who are minimally fretful and irritable (Mangels- struck the area. The 11% of the children who were still
dorf, Gunnar, Kestenbaum, Lang, & Andreas, 1990). distressed 7 months after the storm were those who had
Thus, as Van den Boom and Hoeksma (1994) argued, been anxious prior to the hurricane (La Greca, Silver-
both temperament and family experience act together to man, & Wassastein, 1998). Similarly, young British
influence a childs behavior in the Strange Situation. children less than 5 years old who became fearful after
The investigators who believe that temperament being taken from their homes during the bombing of
makes a minimal contribution to the attachment classifi- London during World War II had been extremely fearful
cations have most often relied on maternal question- before the bombing raids began (John, 1941). Thus, a
naires to measure the childs temperament. A review of temperamental bias favoring a fearful reaction to nov-
many studies suggests that, for most samples, children elty or threat renders children vulnerable to an extreme
described by their mothers as irritable at home were reaction to trauma.
more likely to be classified as insecurely attached in the The stress associated with entering school for the
laboratory. One group of authors suggested that al- first time was only associated with an increase in respi-
though temperament was not the only influence on the ratory illness in children who had shown both sympa-
attachment classification, the empirical overlap be- thetic reactivity prior to the beginning of the school year
tween these behavioral domains is greater than might (measured by an increase in heart rate and arterial blood
have been anticipated (Vaughn et al., 1992, p. 469). pressure to challenge) and, in addition, had been ex-
posed to stressful experiences at home. Children of the
same age and social class who showed low sympathetic
PSYCHOPATHOLOGY AND reactivity showed no increased rate of respiratory ill-
TEMPERAMENT ness, even though they may have lived in a highly stress-
ful home environment (Boyce & Jemerin, 1990).
It is likely that temperamental biases contribute to One team of investigators took advantage of the fact
psychopathology. Children and adults differ in their psy- that an earthquake occurred in Northern California (the
chological and physiological reactions to an unfamiliar Loma Prieta earthquake of 1989) in the middle of a
event, even when the event is unusually stressful such as study of 20 young children who were entering kinder-
an earthquake, divorce, kidnapping, or witnessing a garten. Six children showed an increase in respiratory
mass shooting (North, Smith, & Spitznagel, 1994). illness after the earthquake; five showed a decline. Vari-
The extraordinary variation in the development of ation in the change in the helper-suppressor cell ratios
symptoms of stress following a trauma is probably influ- and pokeweed mitogen response predicted the children
enced by temperament. Usually less than 40% of chil- who showed the increase in respiratory illness. The chil-
dren react to a traumatic event with anxiety or fear. For dren who showed an up-regulation of the two immune
212 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

parameters following school entry had a significant in- years of age was between 2% and 3% (Costello,
crease in respiratory illness after the earthquake. This Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003). However,
fact suggests that the children who perceived kinder- Schwartz, Snidman, and Kagan (1999) reported that
garten entrance as stressful (indexed by the change in 61% of a sample of 13-year-old adolescents who had
immune reaction) became most vulnerable to respiratory been inhibited in the 2nd year of life had developed
infections following the earthquake (Boyce et al., 1993). symptoms of social phobia. One half of a sample of 30,
Even though the differences between adults who had an 15-year-olds who had been high reactive at 4 months
easy or difficult temperament at age 3 years were small, had serious signs of anxiety over social interaction or
Chess and Thomas (1984) suggested that easy children unfamiliar situations, and three high-reactive girls, but
were better-adjusted adults; Werner (1993) reports a not one low-reactive girl, had been diagnosed with clini-
similar result. cal depression (Kagan & Snidman, 2004). However,
It is not clear whether the modulatory processes asso- most high-reactives are likely to find an adaptive adult
ciated with temperament act directly by (a) blunting the niche that will protect them from dealing frequently
limbic systems initial reaction to the stressor, ( b) short- with unfamiliar people on an unpredictable schedule.
ening the duration of the stressful reaction, or (c) acting Fortunately, many vocations and life roles permit this
indirectly through inhibitory processes to mute a con- protection while simultaneously awarding dignity, chal-
sciously experienced stress reaction that is no less in- lenge, and financial security to those with this personal-
tense physiologically than it is in the majority of ity trait. Most humans experience uncertainty over one
children. All three mechanisms are possible, and each or more of their symbolic features as a result of their life
has relevance for the development of psychopathology. histories. The usual nodes of uncertainty center on ac-
Epidemiological studies in varied Western countries ceptability to others, attractiveness, status, wealth, abil-
agree that between 1% and 5% of children have simple ity, power, and virtue. The high- and-low reactive
or social phobias. The range for the looser diagnostic temperamental biases amplify or mute the intensity of
concept of anxiety disorder is larger5% to 26%sug- the felt uncertainty. High-reactives experience this
gesting that the clinical judgment of whether the symp- emotion more acutely than most; low-reactives experi-
tom is disabling enough to be called a disorder is ence it less intensely.
unreliable (Klein & Last, 1989). No more than one-third The risk categories for low-reactives are failure to
of preschool children who are very inhibited will be di- conform to community norms because of less uncer-
agnosed with an anxiety disorder 10 years later. This tainty over criticism or the consequences of risky deci-
means that most children grow toward health. Although sions. This trait is the best predictor of adult psychiatric
this knowledge represents a major advance compared problems in contemporary North American and Euro-
with the information available 100 years ago, it is, in an pean samples because it is correlated with academic
absolute sense, only a modest beginning. failure. Several studies reveal that an extremely uninhib-
There is consensus that some children inherit a physi- ited profile in early childhood is predictive of extreme
ological vulnerability that renders them especially sus- levels of aggressive behavior during adolescence. For ex-
ceptible to developing one of the many states of anxiety ample, 6% of a large sample of lower-class boys were
or depression. About 20% to 30% of first-degree rela- persistently, and seriously, asocial from their second to
tives of children with an anxiety disorder had a similar their 8th year. The best predictor of this small group was
symptom, compared with only 10% of controls (Weiss- a lack of fear to the discrepancy of suddenly hearing the
man, 1984). sounds of a gorilla while the child was playing in the lab-
Although the absolute risk is low, children who were oratory (Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, 2003). It is
high-reactive infants are at a greater risk than most for of interest that, compared with controls, criminals show
developing social phobia in adolescence or adulthood. less, and social phobics more, amygdalar activity (as
The established lifetime prevalence of social phobia in measured by fMRI ) to neutral faces that function as
Americans and Europeans ranges between 5% and 15% conditioned stimuli for a painful unconditioned stimu-
(Merikangas, Avenevoli, Acharyva, Zhang, & Angst, lus (Veit et al., 2002). A low-reactive boy raised by a
2002). The 3-month prevalence of any anxiety disorder family that did not socialize aggressive behavior effec-
in a large sample of North Carolina youth from 9 to 16 tively who played in a neighborhood containing peer
Psychopathology and Temperament 213

temptations for crime is probably at a slightly higher The hypothesis of inherited physiological profiles
than average risk for a delinquent career (Farrington, acting as a diathesis for an anxiety disorder is an attrac-
2000). But far less risk accompanies a low-reactive boy tive explanatory candidate. These physiological condi-
in a well-integrated family without pathology that so- tions, which are the bases for temperaments, participate
cialized aggression effectivelythis boy is likely to be in three different explanatory schemes that probably in-
popular with peers. volve the orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex (OBPFC). Ac-
Although every child and adult has the capacity for tivity in the OBPFC is an important origin of an
anticipating unpleasant events, the intensity and chronic- individuals feeling tone. One part of this area receives
ity of the accompanying bodily feelings are muted in sensory information from the viscera, as well as exter-
many persons, and therefore the state does not interfere nal sources related to eating behavior. The OBPFC also
with the performance of everyday tasks. Only, a small receives input from the amygdala and sends its synthesis
proportion experience feelings, which accompany the of information to an area in the OBPFC called the ven-
unwanted anticipations, with great intensity or regular- tromedial prefrontal cortex, which is the origin of pro-
ity to a degree that they compromise their ability to deal jections to the hypothalamus and brain stem (Price,
with daily responsibilities. These are the individuals 1999). This arrangement implies that frequent or intense
who are regarded as having an anxiety disorder. Because somatic sensations could be due either to an excitable
the majority of these patients have never experienced the amygdala, an excitable OBPFC, or an active ventrome-
events they fear, or if they had, the frequency of en- dial prefrontal cortex. If the neural activity pierces con-
counter was low, theory predicts that their anxious state sciousness, the person will experience a change in
should have extinguished over time. Because it does not feeling tone and will be motivated to interpret it. The
we must ask why. There are several possible reasons. detected change in feeling tone can be interpreted as
When psychoanalytic theories were popular a half- fear, anxiety, guilt, or excitement, or as a temporary
century ago, it was assumed that some life histories compromise in bodily function. The child or adult who
could create a chronic feeling of guilt over violations of interprets the change in feeling as implying a threaten-
moral standards. One consequent of the guilt was the ex- ing event can become very anxious. However, the indi-
pectation of harm or loss as a symbolic punishment for vidual could focus on the bodily sensations and fail to
the ethical lapse. Because the guilt was chronic, the anx- impose an interpretation that implied fear or anxietya
iety persisted. Although this interpretation of chronic process more common in China than in Europe and
anxiety had intuitive appeal earlier, it is less attractive North America (Lee & Kleinman, 1997).
today because of a secular decrease in the frequency and The most parsimonious hypothesis is that there is
intensity of guilt over sexual and aggressive behavior, only one diathesisa neurochemistry that lowers the
yet no decrease in the prevalence of anxiety disorders. threshold of excitability of a brain circuit that when ac-
A second mechanism that relies on classical condi- tivated leads to somatic sensations and a change in feel-
tioning assumes that the individual had experienced in- ing tone. The individuals history determines which
tense distress during an encounter with the feared event; target will be feared. A much more reasonable hypothe-
therefore its mental representations persisted. Some sis is that several brain profiles underlie the various
anxiety disorders could have been acquired by a Pavlov- anxiety disorders. This view argues that a person with a
ian mechanism. This might be especially true for some particular physiological diathesis might develop a blood
phobias, but it cannot explain all of the anxiety disorders phobia, while another with a different physiology will
unless we make thought a conditioned stimulus. Ameri- develop social phobia.
cans who avoid public encounters with strangers are Moreover, each visceral targetheart, muscle, gut,
afraid of being evaluated in an undesirable way; they are labyrinththat sends afferent information to the brain
not afraid of strangers qua strangers. They wish to avoid is influenced by a distinct neurochemistry. It is reason-
what they imagine to be a critical evaluation by others able to assume that genetic features render a particular
because of prior experiences in which they felt an un- visceral target more or less reactive and influence the
pleasant tension when with strangers. Thus, the thought specific events that will be avoided. For example, ado-
of entering an unfamiliar social situation provoked the lescents with a phobia of blood often report feeling faint
feeling of anxiety. when they see large quantities of blood. This fear is
214 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

most likely in those who possess a brisk vasovagal reac- infants (during well-baby examinations) and less likely
tion, which is accompanied by high vagal tone. One ado- to cry to inoculation. Five-month-old European Ameri-
lescent boy in Kagans longitudinal sample who had a can infants showed distress following arm restraint more
blood phobia but no other fears had been a low-reactive quickly than did Japanese infants, implying a higher
infant, an uninhibited child, and an adolescent with ex- threshold of distress to this incentive in Asian infants
tremely high vagal tone. (Camras, Oster, Campos, Miyake, & Bradshaw, 1992).
Kagan and colleagues administered the battery of vi-
sual, auditory, and olfactory stimulations described ear-
ETHNICITY AND TEMPERAMENT lier to 4-month-old infants living in Boston, Dublin, and
Beijing. The Caucasian infants from Dublin and Boston
Differences in temperament among varied ethnic groups were more easily aroused and distressed than the Chi-
remain a delicate issue because of the racial and ethnic nese infants from Beijing (Kagan et al., 1994).
strife around the world. Many psychologists, under- These differences in ease of arousal to unfamiliarity
standably, shy away from studies that might reveal ge- during the 1st year have some parallels in older children.
netically based differences in mood or behavior among Mothers of 6- to 7-year-old children living in Shanghai
populations that have been reproductively isolated for a described them as less active, less impulsive, more con-
long time. A team of scientists compared the frequencies trolled, and more shy than did mothers of children living
of over 100 different alleles for physiological markers in in the Pacific Northwest (Ahadi, Rothbart, & Ye, 1993).
the worlds geographically separate human populations Further, the parents of school-age Thai children, com-
by averaging the difference in frequencies to create an pared with those of European American children, were
index of genetic distance between any pair of popula- more concerned over low energy, low motivation, so-
tions (Cavalli-Sforza, Menozzi, & Piazza, 1994). As ex- matic problems, and forgetfulness, whereas the parents
pected, the index of genetic distance was largest when of European American children reported more concern
Asians, Africans, and European Caucasians were com- with disobedience, aggression, and hyperactivity (Weisz
pared with each other. But even in the Caucasoid Euro- et al., 1987, 1988).
peans, people from Scandinavia, England, and Northern It is relevant that Asian American adult psychiatric
Europe were genetically different from populations liv- patients require a lower dose of psychotropic drugs than
ing in Spain, Italy, and the Balkans. In general, the European American patients (Lin, Poland, & Lesser,
greater the geographical and linguistic distance between 1986), implying that Asian populations may be at a
any two populations, and, therefore, the greater the re- lower level of limbic arousal. There is, in addition,
productive isolation, the greater the genetic distance. It greater genetic diversity in many loci determining
is not unreasonable to assume that some of the alleles blood groups and proteins among Caucasians than
have implications for emotions and behavior. among Asians. Europeans and Asians have been repro-
The most consistent evidence relating temperament ductively isolated for over 30,000 yearsover 1000
to geographically separated populations compares generations. It requires only 15 to 20 generations of se-
Asian with Caucasian infants. Over 30 years ago, Freed- lective breeding to produce obviously different behav-
man and Freedman (1969) reported that newborn Asian ioral profiles in many animal species (Mills & Faure,
American infants, compared with European Americans, 1991). Perhaps, scientists should consider the ethnic
were calmer, less labile, less likely to remove a cloth composition of their samples when the psychological
placed on their face, and more easily consoled when dis- variables they quantify bear some relation to reactivity
tressed. Nine years later, Kagan, Kearsley, and Zelazo and ease of arousal.
(1978) found that Chinese American infants living in The influence of culture has been lost in the excite-
Boston were less active, less vocal, less likely to smile to ment over the many significant discoveries in biology. A
stimulation, and more inhibited during the 1st year com- comparison of European and Asian cultures in the sev-
pared with European American infants from Boston. enteenth and eighteenth centuries, before the Wests in-
Caudill and Weinstein (1969) observed Japanese in- fluence on the latter, illustrates the power of culture.
fants to be less easily aroused than European American The primary entity in European society is the individ-
infants, and Lewis, Ramsay, and Kawakami (1993) ual; each person must attain salvation, wealth, status, or
found Japanese infants to be less reactive than American happiness on their own. By contrast, the imperative for
Temperament and Moral Af fects 215

Asian youth is to seek harmony with, and become part ing of anxiety over risking money on an investment or
of, a groupfirst family and later peers and commu- changing jobs. This patient, who had been an intelligent
nity. Although children and adolescents can develop and successful man prior to the surgery, began to make
either an individualistic or a communal attitude, high- impulsive decisions after the surgery despite no change
and low-reactives may find the two ethics differentially in his measured intelligence.
friendly. High-reactive-inhibited children feel more se- Consider a hypothetical but common situation. A 5-
cure in a social network that sets strict rules for behav- year-old wants a toy that another is enjoying and thinks
ior, does not regularly pose demands for excessive risk, about seizing it. One of the factors that will influence
and rewards loyalty to the community standards with the probability of a seizure by the envious child is a feel-
praise. This type of child is vulnerable to uncertainty in ing of anxiety over the possible consequences of the ag-
an individualistic society where accomplishment re- gressive act. Although socialization in the home will
quires entrepreneurial risk, competitive posture, dealing influence the intensity of that feeling, it is reasonable to
with strangers, and confronting unpredictability. suggest that children with equivalent socialization expe-
The low-reactive-uninhibited child is less threatened riences will differ in the intensity of the anxiety state
by such an imperative, enjoys the excitement of risk and because of temperamental factors. This variation is re-
meeting strangers, and is more likely to bridle when de- lated to the activation of the sympathetic nervous sys-
viance is punished, whether in the form of extreme tal- tem and the receipt of information from limbic targets
ent, lack of civility, or domination of others. Both by the frontal cortex. For example, low-reactive 1- and
socialization and temperament contribute to the devel- 2-year-old infants show less fear to an examiners criti-
opment of a personality type that conforms to the de- cism than high-reactives. Kochanska (1991, 1993) has
mands of the local culture. shown that shy, timid children raised by mothers who
used reasoning in their socialization had a very strict
conscience (using a projective measure of conscience).
TEMPERAMENT AND MORAL AFFECTS Neither the form of maternal socialization nor the
childs shyness, considered alone, predicted variation in
Variation in the intensity of moral emotions might be in- the conscience measure.
fluenced by temperament. The experience of anxiety, It is important to emphasize that most children are
shame, or guilt as an accompaniment to the contempla- capable of the moral emotions of anxiety, shame, and
tion or commission of an act that violates personal or guilt. Further, although some children inherit a tempera-
community standards is an important source of restraint ment that favors an exaggerated guilt reaction, such chil-
on those actions. It is likely (as Kant believed) that indi- dren need not show any pathology later in life. Nor is it
viduals vary in the intensity of their shame and guilt, al- likely that most children with a temperament favoring a
though the biology that accounts for this variation may less intense affective response will become juvenile
be different from the physiology that represents the delinquents. Most parents of these latter children will
foundation for the appearance of the moral emotions in impose heavier socialization demands on them.
most children. Even though parental practices and atti- Nonetheless, if the environment is permissive of aggres-
tudes are most influential, temperamental factors might sion, stealing, and lying, the child with a temperamental
play a small role. The intensity of the experienced moral bias for a sluggish anxiety/fear reaction is probably at
affects are due to efferents that originate in limbic sites greater risk for aggressive or delinquent behaviors than
and excite peripheral organs, as well as the quality of af- other children growing up in the same social context
ferent activity in the periphery to the medulla, amyg- (Kochanska, 1995).
dala, and, eventually, frontal cortex. The children who had been high-reactive infants
Damasio (1994) described the case of an adult male should be more vulnerable than others to bouts of guilt
who lost, through surgery, the ventromedial surface of because of greater sympathetic activity, and therefore,
his prefrontal cortex. This neural tissue receives affer- greater visceral feedback to the amygdala and the
ent information from the amygdala, which, in turn, re- OBPFC. However, a verbal report of guilt can occur with
ceives it from the heart, lung, gut, and muscles via a or without an appropriate change in physiology at the time
nucleus in the medulla. Without this neuronal surface of the ethical violation. Some children might say they feel
the individual cannot have the subtle anticipatory feel- guilty, but this confession might not be correlated with a
216 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

physiological reaction. Eleven-year-old children were Venables, & Williams, 1990; see also, Katz & Gottman,
asked in the home setting to rank 20 Q-sort items de- 1994).
scriptive of their personality from most to least charac- It is possible that the small group of criminals who
teristic. One item was, I feel bad if one of my parents commit violent crimes (probably fewer than 5% of all
says that I did something wrong. There was no differ- delinquents and criminals) possess a special tempera-
ence between high- and low-reactives in the mean rank ment. In a longitudinal study of a large New Zealand co-
assigned this item (the mean rank was 10), however, high- hort, the young adults who were violent had been rated
reactives who ranked this item as more characteristic of at 3 and 5 years of age as low on control of behavior
self (a rank less than 10) showed a larger number of bio- (Henry, Caspi, & Silva, personal communication). A mi-
logical signs of amygdalar reactivity than the low-reac- nority of impulsive, minimally fearful 5-year-old boys
tives who rated this item as equally characteristic of self became adolescent delinquents; only 28% of high-
or the high-reactives who did not admit to feeling guilty. delinquent boys had been rated by their teachers 8 years
The high-reactives who confessed to feeling guilty had a earlier as highly asocial (Tremblay, Pihl, Vitaro, &
mean standard score greater than .00 across the biologi- Dubkin, 1994). Thus, only a small proportion of asocial
cal variables that reflected cortical and autonomic adolescents were born with a temperament that placed
arousal. The low-reactives who admitted to equally fre- them at risk for this profile.
quent feelings of guilt had a mean standard score less
than .00 (Kagan & Snidman, 2004). The fact that only
high-reactives who admitted feeling bad following
parental criticism showed signs of cortical and autonomic CONCLUSION
reactivity suggests that they may be especially vulnerable
to bouts of guilt. Most children can be socialized to feel The inclusion of biological evidence in studies of tem-
shame or guilt following violation of a standard, but perament and personality is a welcome development.
a small proportion are especially vulnerable to these The history of science is rich with examples of the ac-
emotions because of their temperament (Kagan & Snid- celerated progress that occurs when two or more previ-
man, 2004). ously isolated domains probe common problems with
The mothers of these children ranked 28 statements different vocabularies and methods. The fields of bio-
describing their child from most to least characteristic physics, molecular biology, and radio astronomy are ob-
of their son or daughter. One item referred to the childs vious examples. The union of such domains, by
behavior when chastised: is sensitive to punishment. providing new information, refines existing terms and
The high- and low-reactive girls differed in the rank the eliminates concepts that have outlived their usefulness.
mothers awarded this item, for many more high- than This first phase of a collaboration between biology and
low-reactive girls were described this way. And the psychology has led to some new ideas, for example, the
high-reactive girls described by their mothers as sensi- realization that the concept of one fear state is not theo-
tive to punishment showed greater right parietal activa- retically useful.
tion in the EEG than the high-reactive girls who were As Galen anticipated, children, like animals, inherit
less sensitive to punishment. different biologies that, in turn, affect the manner in
Low-reactive boys who had high vagal tone repre- which environmental events influence their psychologi-
sented a special temperamental category. These boys cal growth. Developmental scientists should assume
are likely to become group leaders if they grow up in from the beginning that different temperamental types
typical American middle-class homes with loving par- will not react in the same way, behaviorally or biologi-
ents who socialize school achievement and the control cally, to a given experience and invent constructs that
of aggression. The same boys raised by indifferent par- capture that fact.
ents in large cities may become delinquents. Antisocial Psychologists may eventually replace the current
adolescents who showed minimal autonomic reactivity constructs, which describe children and their environ-
to simple stimulationa lower heart rate and less fre- ments (parents, sibling, and school settings) separately,
quent skin conductance responseswere more likely to with single synthetic constructs that represent a partic-
continue a criminal career than equally antisocial ado- ular temperamental type growing up in a particular set
lescents who did not become adult criminals (Raine, of contexts. To illustrate, instead of writing about high-
References 217

reactive infants, on the one hand, and protective, per- evolution required mutation, recombination, geograph-
missive families, on the other, psychologists might in- ical isolation, and a natural selection of those features
vent a new construct that describes the envelope of that were adaptive in particular settings.
possible profiles for the category of child developing in
this environment. As environments shape children of
varied temperaments into different phenotypes, it will REFERENCES
be useful to invent new concepts rather than rely on the
language of analysis of variance that describes interac- Abercrombie, H. C., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E., Rosenkranz, M. A.,
tions between the temperamental type of child and a & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Cortisol variation in humans affects
memory for emotionally laden and neutral information. Behav-
rearing environment. ioral Neuroscience, 117, 505516.
This suggestion is a special instance of the more gen- Achenbach, T. M. (1985). Assessment and taxonomy of child and ado-
eral rule that the construct chosen depends on the investi- lescent psychopathology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
gators purpose. Light can be described as a wave, a Adamec, R. E. (1991). Anxious personality in the cat. In B. J. Carroll
& J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Psychopathology and the brain
particle, or as a source of heat or illumination. It can be
(pp. 153168). New York: Raven Press.
useful on some occasions to describe a psychological pro- Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Childrens tempera-
file with a construct that defines its current features, bio- ment in the United States and China. European Journal of Psychi-
logical origins, and experiential history, as we do with atry, 7, 359377.
the concept of bipolar illness and mathematical genius. Arbelle, S., Benjamin, J., Galin, M., Kremer, P., Belmaker, R. H., &
Ebstein, R. P. (2003). Relation of shyness in grade school children
Temperament and history act together to create a psy- to the genotype for the long form of the serotonin transporter pro-
chological barrier between a potentially emotionally moter region polymorphism. American Journal of Psychiatry,
charged external event and reaction of the limbic system 160, 671676.
that renders the event less potent. Fifty years ago, these Arcus, D. M. (1991). Experiential modification of temperamental bias
in inhibited and uninhibited children. Unpublished doctoral dis-
phenomena would have been called a defense. Although sertation, Harvard University, Boston, MA.
all individuals can learn defenses to protect them from Arcus, D. M., & Kagan, J. (1995). Temperament and craniofacial
distress to threat, a small proportion of children may in- skeleton in children. Child Development, 66, 15291540.
herit a temperament that makes it easier for them to es- Asendorpf, J. B. (1989). Shyness as a final common pathway for two
tablish these defenses. A childs experiential history different kinds of inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 481492.
determines the meaning imposed on an event; a combi-
Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). Development of inhibition during childhood.
nation of temperament and history determines both the Developmental Psychology, 26, 721730.
ease with which the interpretation of an event gains ac- Asendorpf, J. B. (1991). Development of inhibited childrens coping
cess to varied brain circuits as well as the excitability of with unfamiliarity. Child Development, 62, 14601474.
those structures. Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). Abnormal shyness in children. Journal of
Child Psychiatry, 34, 10691081.
The return of temperamental constructs will, in-
Asendorpf, J. B. (1994). The malleability of behavioral inhibition.
evitably, increase scientific interest in affective phe- Developmental Psychology, 30, 912919.
nomena and, in so doing, alert psychologists to those Auerbach, J., Geller, V., Lezer, S., Shinwell, E., Belmaker, R. H.,
events that have their primary effect on feeling tone Levin, J., et al. (1999). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4VR) and sera-
and emotions rather than on behaviors, especially the tonin transporter promoter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphisms in the
determination of temperament in two-month-old infants. Molecu-
emotion families called guilt, shame, fear, anger, pride, lar Psychiatry, 4, 369373.
anxiety, sadness, excitement, surprise, and joy. Indi- Bar-Haim, Y., Marshall, P. J., Fox, N. A., Schorr, E. A., & Gordon-
vidual variation in the frequency and intensity of those Salant, S. (2003). Mismatch negativity in socially withdrawn chil-
affect states is influenced by temperament and the con- dren. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1724.
Basoglu, M., Mineka, S., Parker, N., Aker, T., Livanov, M., & Gok, S.
structions that children create from their encounters. It
(1997). Psychological preparedness for trauma as a protective fac-
is reasonable to be optimistic about the future of devel- tor in survivors of torture. Psychological Medicine, 27, 14211437.
opmental psychology if scientists search for the coher- Bates, J. E. (1980). The concept of difficult temperament. Merrill-
ent profiles that emerge from biological predispositions Palmer Quarterly, 26, 299319.
and life histories and do not insist on reducing each Bates, J. E. (1983). Issues in the assessment of difficult temperament.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 8997.
profile either to the actions of genes or the conse-
Bates, J. E. (1989). Concepts and measures of temperament. In G. A.
quences of experiences. We will then attain a synthesis Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament
as fruitful as the one that followed the recognition that in childhood (pp. 326). New York: Wiley.
218 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A., & Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Measure- Byrne, J., & Suomi, S. J. (2002). Cortisol reactivity and its relation to
ment of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, 950959. home cage behavior and personality ratings in tufted capuchin
Bates, J. E., & Wachs, T. D. (1994). Temperament. Washington, DC: (Cebux apella) juveniles from birth to 6 years of age. Psychoneu-
American Psychological Association. roendocrinology, 27, 139154.
Bauman, M. D., Lavenex, D., Mason, W. A., Capitanio, J. P., & Ama- Calkins, S., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations among infant tem-
ral, D. G. (2004). The development of mother-infant interactions perament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at 24
after neonatal amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys. Journal of months. Child Development, 63, 14561472.
Neuroscience, 24, 711721. Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D.
Birns, B., Barten, S., & Bridger, W. (1969). Individual differences in (1992). Japanese and American infants response to arm restraint.
temperamental characteristics of infants. Transactions of the New Developmental Psychology, 28, 578583.
York Academy of Sciences, 31, 10711082. Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the infant tem-
perament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61, 735739.
Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and
teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibi- Carlson, E. A., Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). A developmen-
tion? Child Development, 74, 18991917. tal investigation of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. Child De-
velopment, 66, 3754.
Black, B. (1992). Elective mutism as a variant of social phobia. Jour-
nal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1988). Moving away from the
31, 10901094. world. Developmental Psychology, 24, 824831.
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age 3
Blackburn, J. (2002). Old man Goya. New York: Random House.
predict personality traits in young adulthood. Child Development,
Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (1988). Ethoexperimental ap- 66, 486498.
proaches to the biology of emotion. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W.
Caudill, W., & Weinstein, H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behav-
Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 4368).
ior in Japan and America. Psychiatry, 32, 1243.
Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P., & Piazza, A. (1994). The history
Blizard, D. A., & Adams, N. (2002). The Maudsley reactive and non-
and geography of human genes. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer-
reactive strains. Behavior Genetics, 32, 277299.
sity Press.
Blizard, D. A., Liang, B., & Emmel, D. K. (1980). Blood pressure, Cecchi, M., Khoshbouei, H., Javors, M., & Morilak, D. A. (2002).
heart rate, and plasma catecholamines under resting conditions in Modulatory effects of norepinephrine in the lateral bed nucleus of
rat strains selectively bred for differences in response to stress. the stria terminalis on behavioral and neuroendocrine responses
Behavioral and Neural Biology, 29, 487492. to acute stress. Neuroscience, 112, 1321.
Bornstein, M. H., Gaughran, J. M., & Segui, D. (1991). Multivariate Cheever, J. (1993). The journals of John Cheever. New York: Ballan-
assessment of infant temperament. International Journal of Behav- tine Books.
ioral Development, 14, 131151.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1984). Genesis and evolution of behavior
Boyce, W. T., Chesterman, E. A., Martin, N., Folkman, S., Cohen, F., & disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 19.
Wara, D. (1993). Immunologic changes occurring at kindergarten Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1990). New York Longitudinal Study: The
entry predict respiratory illnesses after the Loma Prieta earth- young adult periods. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 557561.
quake. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 296303.
Chugani, H. T. (1994). Development of regional brain glucose metab-
Boyce, W. T., & Jemerin, J. M. (1990). Psychobiological differences olism. In G. Dawson & K. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the
in childhood stress response. Journal of Developmental and Be- developing brain (pp. 153175). New York: Guilford Press.
havioral Pediatrics, 11, 8694.
Clarke, A. S., Mason, W. A., & Moberg, G. P. (1988). Differential be-
Breuer, J., & Freud, S. (1956). Studies in hysteria [Standard edition]. havioral and adrenocortical responses to stress among three
London: Hogarth. (Original work published 18931898) macaques species. American Journal of Primatology, 14, 3752.
Broberg, A., Lamb, M. E., & Hwang, P. (1990). Inhibition: Its stabil- Cohen, S., Kaplan, J. R., Cunnick, J. E., Manuck, S. B., & Rabin, B. S.
ity and correlates in 16- to 40-month-old children. Child Develop- (1992). Chronic social stress, affiliation, and cellular immune re-
ment, 61, 11531163. sponse in nonhuman primates. Psychological Science, 3, 301304.
Brody, L. M., Tremblay, R. E., Brane, B., Fergusson, D., Horwood, Connell, J. T., & Thompson, R. (1986). Emotion and social inter-
J. L., Laird, et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood action in the strange situation. Child Development, 57, 733745.
disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency. Developmental Coplan, R. J., Rubin, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Stewart,
Psychology, 39, 222245. S. L. (1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone. Child
Brown, J. S., Kalish, H. I., & Farber, I. E. (1951). Conditioned fear as Development, 65, 129137.
revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003).
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 317328. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood
Buss, A. H. (1986). A theory of shyness. In W. H. Jones, J. M. Cheek, and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837844.
& S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treat- Cote, S., Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D. S., Zoccolillo, M., & Vitaro, F.
ment (pp. 3946). New York: Plenum Press. (2002). The development of impulsivity, fearfulness, and helpful-
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing ness during childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. try, 43, 609618.
Buss, K. A., Schumacher, J. R. M., Dolski, I., Kalin, N. H., Gold- Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes error. New York: Putnam Press.
smith, H. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Right frontal brain activ- Davidson, R. J. (1994a). Asymmetric brain function, affective
ity, cortisol, and withdrawal behavior in 6-month-old infants. style, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1120. 6, 741758.
References 219

Davidson, R. J. (1994b). Temperament, affective style, and frontal ior and novel environments. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiol-
lobe asymmetry. In G. P. Dawson & K. P. Fischer (Eds.), Human ogy, 30, 327330.
behavior in the developing brain (pp. 518536). New York: Guil- Eaton, W. O., & Saudino, K. J. (1992). Prenatal activity level at
ford Press. the temperamental dimension. Infant Behavior and Develop-
Davidson, R. J. (2003a). Affective neuroscience and psychophysiol- ment, 15, 5770.
ogy. Psychophysiology, 40, 655665. Einstein, E., & Infeld, L. (1938). The evolution of physics. New York:
Davidson, R. J. (2003b). Right frontal brain activity, cortisol, and Simon & Schuster.
withdrawal behavior in 6-month-old infants. Behavioral Neuro- Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland,
science, 117, 1120. A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Facial expressions of emotion. Psy-
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., & Saron, C. D. (1990). Approach withdrawal chological Science, 3, 3438.
and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiol- Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion related regulation.
ogy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330341. Child Development, 75, 334339.
Davidson, R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1989). Frontal brain asymmetry pre- Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Sci-
dicts infants responses to maternal separation. Journal of Abnor- ence, 3, 3448.
mal Psychology, 98, 127131.
Emde, R. N., & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.). (2001). Infancy to early child-
Davis, M. (1984). The mammalian startle response. In R. C. Eaton hood. New York: Oxford University Press.
(Ed.), The neural mechanisms of startle behavior (pp. 287351).
Emde, R. N., Plomin, R., Robinson, J., Corley, R., DeFries, J., Fulker,
New York: Plenum Press.
D. W., et al. (1992). Temperament, emotion, and cognition at 14
Davis, M. H., Luce, C., & Kraus, S. J. (1994). The heritability of months: MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child Development,
characteristics associated with dispositional empathy. Journal of 63, 14371455.
Personality, 62, 369391.
Evans, R. B. (2003). Georg von Bekesy. American Psychologist,
Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Klinger, L. G., & Hill, D. (1992). The
58, 742746.
role of frontal lobe functioning in the development of infant self-
regulatory behavior. Brain and Cognition, 20, 152175. Fagot, B. I., & OBrien, M. (1994). Activity level in young children.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 378390.
Dettling, A. C., Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (1999). Cortisol levels
of young children in full-day childcare centers: Relations with age Fairbanks, L. A. (2001). Individual differences in response to a
and temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 519536. stranger. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 2228.
Dettling, A. C., Parker, S. W., Lane, S., Sebanc, A., & Gunnar, M. R. Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defensive behav-
(2000). Quality of care and temperament determine changes in ior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
cortisol concentrations over the day for young children in child- 1, 429438.
care. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 819836. Farrington, D. P. (2000). Psychosocial predictors of adult anti-social
Diamond, A. (1990). Developmental time course in human infants personality and adult convictions. Behavioral Science and Law,
and infant monkeys and the neural bases of inhibitory control and 18(5), 605622.
reaching. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1980). Sex differences and stability in
of higher cognitive functions (pp. 637676). New York: New York vigor, activity, and persistence in the first 3 years of life. Journal
Academy of Sciences. of Genetic Psychology, 136, 6575.
Diamond, S. (1957). Personality and temperament. New York: Harper. Fendt, M., Enders, T., & Apfelbach, R. (2003). Temporary inactiva-
DiLalla, L. F., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1994). Genetic etiology of tion of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis but not of the amyg-
behavioral inhibition among 2-year-old children. Infant Behavior dala blocks freezing induced by trimethylthiazoline, a component
and Development, 17, 401408. of fox feces. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 2328.
DiPietro, J. (1995, March). Fetal origins of neurobehavioral function Fish, M., Stifter, C. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Conditions of continuity
and individual dif ferences. Presented at the meeting of the Soci- and discontinuity in infant negative emotionality. Child Develop-
ety for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. ment, 62, 15251537.
DiPietro, J. A., Bornstein, M. H., Costigan, K. A., Pressman, E. K., Fish, S. E., & Fish, M. (1995, April). Variability in neonatal heart
Hahn, C., Painter, K., et al. (2002). What does fetal movement rate during orientation tasks and its relation to later social and
predict about behavior during the first two years of life? Develop- coping behavior. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for
mental Psychology, 40, 358371. Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
DiPietro, J. A., Costingan, K. A., Pressman, E. K., & Doussard- Forman, D. R., OHara, M. W., Larsen, K., Coy, K. C., Gorman, L. L.,
Roosevelt, J. A. (2000). Antenatal origins of individual differ- & Stuart, S. (2003). Infant emotionality. Infancy, 4, 541565.
ences in heart rate. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 221228. Fowler, J. A. (1897). A manual of mental science for teachers and stu-
DiPietro, J. A., Hodgson, D. M., Costigan, K. A., & Johnson, T. R. B. dents. New York: Fowler & Walls.
(1996). Fetal antecedents of infant temperament. Child Develop- Fox, N. A. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of emotional reac-
ment, 67, 25682583. tivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology,
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy. 25, 364372.
New York: McGraw-Hill. Fox, N. A. (1991). If its not left, its right: Electroencephalogram
Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1981). Studying temperament and parent- asymmetry and the development of emotion. American Psycholo-
child interaction. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child gist, 46, 863872.
Development, 415430. Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion
Dyaglo, N. N., & Shishkina, G. Q. (2000). Genetic differences in the regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
synthesis and reception of adrenaline in the mouse brain in behav- opment, 59(2/3, Serial No. 240), 52166.
220 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Fox, N. A. (1995). Of the way we were: Adult memories about attach- Goldsmith, H. H., & Gottesman, I. I. (1981). Origins of variations in
ment experiences and their role in determining infant-parent rela- behavioral style. Child Development, 52, 91103.
tionshipsA commentary on van IJzendoorn. Psychological Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J. (1999).
Bulletin, 117, 404410. Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Develop-
Fox, N. A., Bell, M. A., & Jones, N. A. (1992). Individual differences mental Psychology, 35, 972985.
in response to stress and cerebral asymmetry. Developmental Neu- Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Contemporary instru-
ropsychology, 8(2/3), 161184. ments for assessing early temperament by questionnaire and in
Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (1994). Neural plasticity and the laboratory. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations
development in the first 2 years of life. Developmental Psycho- in temperament (pp. 249272). New York: Plenum Press.
pathology, 6, 677696. Goodwin, B. (1994). How the leopard changed its spots. New
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1987). EEG asymmetry in 10-month- York: Scribners.
old infants in response to approach of a stranger and maternal Goossens, F. A., & van I J z e n d o o r n , M. H. (1990). Quality of in-
separation. Developmental Psychology, 23, 233240. fants attachment to professional caregivers. Child Development,
Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Pattern of brain electrical ac- 61, 832837.
tivity during the expression of discrete emotions in 10-month-old Gortmaker, S. L., Kagan, J., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1997). Day-
infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230236. light during pregnancy and shyness in children. Developmental
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Psychobiology, 31, 107114.
Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral Green, J. M. (1991). Mothers perception of their 6-week-old babies.
inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral Irish Journal of Psychology, 12, 133144.
inf luences across the first 4 years of life. Child Development, Guerin, D. W., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Developmental stability
72(1), 121. and change in parent reports of temperament. Merrill-Palmer
Fox, N. A., Kimmerly, N. L., & Schaefer, W. D. (1991). Attachment to Quarterly, 40, 334350.
mother/attachment to father: A meta-analysis. Child Development, Gunnar, M. R. (1994). Psychoendocrine studies of temperament and
62, 210225. stress in early childhood. In J. Bates & T. Wachs (Eds.), Tempera-
Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R., Coplan, R. J., ment: Individual dif ferences at the interface of biology and behav-
Porges, S. W., et al. (1995). Frontal activation asymmetry and social ior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
competence at 4 years of age. Child Development, 66, 17701784. Gunnar, M. R., Porter, F. L., Wolf, C. M., Rigatuso, J., & Larson, M. C.
Freedman, D. G., & Freedman, N. (1969). Behavioral differences (1995). Neonatal stress reactivity. Child Development, 66, 113.
between Chinese-American and American newborns. Nature, Habecker, B. A., & Landis, S. C. (1994). Noradrenergic regulation of
224, 1227. cholinergic differentiation. Science, 264, 16021604.
Freedman, D. G., & Keller, B. (1963). Inheritance of behavior in in- Hariri, A. R., Mattoy, V. S., Tessitore, A., Fera, F., Smith, W. G.,
fants. Science, 140, 196. & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). Dextroamphetamine modulates the
Freud, S. (1950). Analysis of a phobia in a 5-year-old boy. In A. Stra- response of the human amygdala. Neurosystems Pharmacology,
chey & J. Strachey (Trans.), Collected papers of Sigmund Freud 27, 10361040.
(Vol. 3, pp. 149295). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work Healy, D. (1997). The antidepressant era. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
published 1909) University Press.
Freud, S. (1965). New introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. New Hebb, D. O. (1946). On the nature of fear. Psychological Review,
York: Norton. (Original work published 1933) 53, 259276.
Fudge, J. L., Kunishio, K., Walsh, P., Richard, C., & Haber, S. N. Henderson, H. A. (2002). Tempermental contributions to problem
(2002). Amygdaloid projections to ventromedial striatral subter- solving: Cognitive and af fective processes [Dissertation Abstract].
ritories in the primate. Neuroscience, 110, 257275. Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Fox, N. A., & Rubin, K. H. (2004).
Gainotti, G., Caltagirone, L., & Zoccolotti, P. (1993). Left-right and Psychophysiological and behavioral evidence for varying forms
cortical /subcortical dichotomies in neuropsychological study of and functions of nonsocial behavior in preschoolers. Child Devel-
human emotions. In F. N. Watts (Ed.), Neuropsychological per- opment, 75, 251263.
spectives on emotion (pp. 7193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hinde, R. A., Tamplin, A., & Barrett, J. (1993). Social isolation
Gall, F. J. (1835). On the organ of the moral qualities and intellectual in 4-year-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
faculties and the plurality of the cerebral organs (W. Lewis, 11, 211236.
Trans.). Boston: Marsh, Copen, & Lyon. Hooton, E. A. (1939). Crime and the man. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., University Press.
Kuzuch, T. L., et al. (1996). Quantitative magnetic resonance im- Hu, M. W., & Fox, S. W. (1988). Evolutionary processes and
aging of human brain development. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 551560. metaphors. New York: Wiley.
Goddard, M. E., & Beilharz, R. G. (1985). A multivariate analysis of Huffman, L. C., Bryan, Y. E., Pedersen, F. A., Lester, B. M., New-
the genetics of fearlessness in potential guide dogs. Behavioral man, J. D., & del Carmen, R. (1994). Infant cry acoustics and
Genetics, 15, 6989. maternal ratings of temperament. Infant Behavior and Develop-
Goldberg, S., Levitan, R., Levog, E., Masellis, M., Basile, V. S., Ne- ment, 17, 4553.
meroff, C. B., et al. (2003). Cortisol concentrations in 12- to 18- Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex.
month-old infants. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 719726. Brain Research, 163, 195205.
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The structure of temperamen- Insel, T. R., Wang, Z., & Ferris, C. (1994). Patterns of vasopressin re-
tal fear and pleasure in infants. Child Development, 61, 19441964. ceptor distribution associated with social organization in monog-
References 221

amous and non-monogamous microtine rodents. Journal of Neuro- Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., & Bem, D. J. (1996). Life course sequel-
science, 14, 53815392. lae of childhood shyness in Sweden. Developmental Psychology,
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: 32, 11001105.
Plenum Press. Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., Stattin, H., & Klackenberg-Larsson, I.
Izard, C. E., Hembree, E. A., & Huebner, R. R. (1987). Infants (1994). Stability of inhibition in the Swedish longitudinal sample.
emotion expressions to acute pain. Developmental Psychology, Child Development, 65, 138146.
23, 105113. Klein, R. G. (1991). Parent-child agreement in clinical assessment of
John, E. M. (1941). A study of the effects of evacuation and air raids anxiety and other psychopathology. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
on children of pre-school age. British Journal of Educational Psy- 5, 182198.
chology, 11, 173182. Klein, R. G., & Last, C. G. (1989). Anxiety disorders in children.
Jones, R. B., Satterlee, D. G., & Ryder, F. H. (1994). Fear of humans Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
in Japanese quail selected for low or high adrenocortical re- Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the develop-
sponse. Physiology and Behavior, 56, 379383. ment of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62, 13791392.
Kagan, J. (1971). Change and continuity in infancy. New York: Wiley. Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization
Kagan, J. (1994). Galens prophecy. New York: Basic Books. and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child
Development, 64, 325347.
Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity.
Child Development, 68, 139143. Kochanska, G. (1995). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline,
and security of attachment. Child Development, 66, 597615.
Kagan, J., Arcus, D., Snidman, N., Yufeng, W., Hendler, J., & Greene,
Kohnstamm, G. A., Bates, J. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Tempera-
S. (1994). Reactivity in infants: A cross-national comparison. De-
ment in childhood. New York: Wiley.
velopmental Psychology, 30, 342345.
Korner, A. F., Zeanah, C. H., Linden, J., Berkowitz, R. I., Kraemer,
Kagan, J., Kearsley, R., & Zelazo, P. (1978). Infancy. Cambridge,
H. C., & Agras, W. S. (1985). The relation between neonatal and
MA: Harvard University Press.
later activity and temperament. Child Development, 56, 3842.
Kagan, J., Lapidus, D. R., & Moore, M. (1978). Infant antecedents of
Kostovic, I. (1990). Structural and histochemical reorganization of
cognitive functioning. Child Development, 49, 10051023.
the human prefrontal cortex during prenatal and postnatal life.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of Progress in Brain Research, 85, 223240.
childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167171.
Kretschmer, E. (1925). Physique and character (W. J. H. Sprott,
Kagan, J., & Saudino, K. J. (2001). Behavioral inhibition and related Trans., 2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace.
temperaments. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to
Kumakiri, C., Kodama, K., Shimizu, E., Yamanouchi, N., Okada, S.,
early childhood (pp. 111122). New York: Oxford University Press.
Noda, S., et al. (1999). Study of the association between the sero-
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. tonin transporter gene regulating polymorphism and personality
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. traits in a Japanese population. Neuroscience Letters, 263, 205207.
Kalin, N. H., Larson, C., Shelton, S. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1998). La Bar, K. S., Gatenby, C., Gore, J. C., Le Doux, J. E., & Phelps,
Asymmetric frontal brain activity, cortisol, and behavior associ- E. A. (1998). Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear
ated with fearful temperament in Rhesus monkeys. Behavioral acquisition and extinction. Neuron, 29, 937945.
Neuroscience, 112, 286292.
La Greca, A. M., Silverman, W. K., & Wassastein, S. B. (1998). Chil-
Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Cerebrospinal drens predisaster functioning as a predictor of post-traumatic
f luid corticotropin-releasing hormone levels are elevated in mon- stress following Hurricane Andrew. Journal of Consulting and
keys with patterns of brain activity associated with fearful tem- Clinical Psychology, 66, 883892.
perament. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 579585. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of
Kant, I. (1959). Foundations of the metaphysics of morals (L. Beck, Neuroscience, 23, 155184.
Trans.). Indianapolois, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. (Original work pub- LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, D., & Reis, E. J. (1988). Different
lished 1785) projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic
Kapp, B. S., Supple, W. F., & Whalen, R. (1994). Effects of electrical and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. Journal of Neuro-
stimulation of the amygdaloid central nucleus on neocortical science, 8, 25172529.
arousal in the rabbit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 8193. Lee, S., & Kleinman, A. (1997). Mental illness and social change in
Karli, P. (1956). The Norway rats killing response to the white China. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 5, 4346.
mouse. Behavior, 10, 81103. Lee, Y., Schulkin, J., & Davis, M. (1994). Effect of corticosterone on
Karli, P. (1981). Conceptual and methodological problems associated enhancement of the acoustic startle ref lex by corticotropin releas-
with the study of brain mechanisms underlying aggressive behav- ing factor. Brain Research, 666, 9398.
ior. In P. F. Brain & D. Benton (Eds.), Biology of aggression Lewis, M., Ramsay, D. S., & Kawakami, K. (1993). Differences be-
(pp. 323361). Alphenaandenrign, Holland: Sijt-hoff Noordhoff. tween Japanese infants and Caucasian-American infants in be-
Karli, P. (1991). Animal and human aggression. Oxford, England: Ox- havioral and cortisol response to inoculation. Child Development,
ford University Press. 64, 17221731.
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1994). Vagal tone buffers children Lin, K. M., Poland, R. E., & Lesser, I. N. (1986). Ethnicity and psy-
from the effects of marital hostility. Psychophysiology, 31, S80. chopharmacology. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 10, 151165.
Kendler, K. S., Myers, J. M., & Neale, M. C. (2000). A multidimen- Lloyd, R. L., & Kling, A. S. (1991). Delta activity from amygdala in
sional twin study of mental health in women. American Journal of squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus): Inf luence of social and envi-
Psychiatry, 157, 506517. ronmental contexts. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 223229.
222 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Lombroso, C. (1911). Crime and its causes. Boston: Little, Brown. Meyer, A. (1994). A short sketch of the problems of psychiatry.
Lyons, P. M., Afarion, H., Schatzberg, A. F., Sawyer-Glover, A., & American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 4347. (Original work pub-
Moseley, M. E. (2002). Experience dependant asymmetric varia- lished 1897)
tion in monkeys (Saimiri sciurencis). Behavioral Brain Research, Mills, A. D., & Faure, J. M. (1991). Diversion selection for duration
136, 5159. of chronic immobility and social reinstatement behavior in Japa-
Madison, L. S., Madison, J. K., & Adubato, S. A. (1986). Infant be- nese quail (Coturnix Japonica chicks). Journal of Comparative
havior and development in relation to fetal movement and habitu- Psychology, 105, 2538.
ation. Child Development, 57, 14751482. Miyawaki, T., Goodchild, A. K., & Pilowsky, P. M. (2002). Activa-
Magnusson, D. (1988). Individual development from an interactional tion of mu-opioid receptors in rat ventrolateral medulla selec-
perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. tively blocks baroreceptor ref lexes while activation of delta
opioid receptors blocks somato-sympathetic ref lexes. Neuro-
Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas,
science, 109, 133144.
D. (1990). Infant proneness to distress temperament, maternal
personality, and mother-infant attachment. Child Development, Morley-Fletcher, S., Palanza, P., Parolaro, D., Vigano, D., & Laviola,
61, 820831. G. (2003). Intra-uterine position has long term inf luences on
brain u-opiod receptor densities and behavior in mice. Psychoneu-
Maren, S., Yap, S. A., & Goosens, K. A. (2001). The amgydala is es-
roendocrinology, 28, 386400.
sential for the development of neuronal plasticity in the medial
geniculate nucleus during auditory fear conditioning in rats. Jour- Mrzljak, L. H. B., & Uylings, B. H. (1990). Neural development in
nal of Neuroscience, 21, RC135. human prefrontal cortex in prenatal and postnatal stages. Progress
in Brain Research, 85, 185222.
Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias, and rituals. New York: Oxford
Nelson, E. E., Shelton, S. E., & Kalin, N. H. (2003). Individual dif-
University Press.
ferences in the responses of naive rhesus monkeys to snakes.
Marshall, P., Hardin, M., & Fox, N. A. (2004). Temperament and elec- Emotion, 3, 311.
trophysiological responses to novelty in infancy. Manuscript in
North, C. S., Smith, E. M., & Spitznagel, E. L. (1994). Posttraumatic
preparation.
stress disorder and survivors of a mass shooting. American Jour-
Mason, W. A., & Capitanio, J. P. (1988). Formation and expression of nal of Psychiatry, 151, 8288.
filial attachment in rhesus monkeys raised with living and inanimate
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness.
mother substitutes. Developmental Psychobiology, 21, 401430.
Psychological Review, 108, 483522.
Matheny, A. (1983). A longitudinal twin study of stability of compo-
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent. Current Di-
nents from Bayleys infant behavior record. Child Development,
rections in Psychological Science, 12, 59.
54, 356360.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The mea-
Matheny, A. (1990). Developmental behavior genetics. In M. E.
surement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Hahn, J. K. Hewitt, N. D. Henderson, & R. H. Benno (Eds.), De-
Panksepp, J. (1990). The psychoneurology of fear. In G. D. Burrows,
velopmental behavior genetics: Neural, biometrical, and evolution-
M. Roth, & R. Noyes (Eds.), The neurobiology of anxiety: Vol. 3.
ary approaches (pp. 2538). New York: Oxford University Press.
Handbook of anxiety (pp. 358). New York: Elsevier.
Matheny, A., Wilson, R. S., & Thoben, A. S. (1987). Home and
Parent, M. B., West, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). Memory of rats
mother: Relations with infant temperament. Developmental Psy-
with amygdala regions induced 30 days after footshock-motivated
chology, 23, 323331.
escape training ref lects degree of original training. Behavioral
May, H. (1959). The end of American innocence. New York: Knopf. Neuroscience, 108, 10801087.
McDougall, W. (1908). Introduction to social psychology. Lon- Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned ref lexes (W. H. Gantt,
don: Methuen. Trans., Vol. 1). New York: International Publishers.
McDougall, W. (1929). The chemical theory of temperament applied Paxinos, G. (1990). The human nervous system. New York: Aca-
to introversion and extraversion. Journal of Abnormal and Social demic Press.
Psychology, 24, 293309.
Petrovich, G. D., Canteras, N. S., & Swanson, L. W. (2001). Combi-
McGaugh, J. L., & Cahill, L. (2003). Emotion and memory. In R. J. natorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal and hypothalamic be-
Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of havioral systems. Brain Research Reviews, 38, 247289.
af fective science (pp. 93116). New York: Oxford University Press. Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge
McNally, G. P., & Westbrook, R. F. (2003). Opioid receptors regulate & Kegan Paul.
the extinction of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuro- Placidi, G. P., Oquendo, N. A., Malone, K. M., Huang, Y. Y., Ellis,
science, 117, 12921301. S. P., & Mann, J. J. (2001). Aggresivity, suicide attempts, and de-
Mebert, C. J. (1991). Dimensions of subjectivity in parents ratings of pression. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 783791.
infant temperament. Child Development, 62, 352361. Plomin, R., & Foch, T. T. (1980). A twin study of objectively as-
Meehl, P. E. (1973). Maxcov-Hitmax: A taxonomy search for loose sessed personality in childhood. Journal of Personality and Social
genetic syndromes. In P. E. Meehl (Ed.), Psychodiagnosis: Se- Psychology, 39, 680688.
lected papers (pp. 200224). Minneapolis: University of Min- Plomin, R., & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.). (1993). Nature, nurture, and
nesota Press. psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Meehl, P. E. (1995). Bootstrap taxometrics. American Psychologist, Polak, C., Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., & Rubin, K. H. (2004). Be-
50, 266275. havioral and physiological correlates of socially wary behavior in
Merikangas, K. R., Avenevoli, S., Acharyya, S., Zhang, H., & Angst, middle childhood: Does social wary behavior mediate the relation
J. (2002). The spectrum of social phobia in the Zurich cohort between infant temperament and later child maladjustment. Manu-
study of young adults. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 5591. script in preparation.
References 223

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the Roccatagliatta, J. (1986). A history of ancient psychiatry. New York:
human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542. Greenwood Press.
Price, J. L. (1999). Prefrontal cortical networks related to visceral Roozendaal, B., & Cools, A. R. (1994). Inf luence of the noradrener-
function and mood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, gic state of the nucleus accumbens in basolateral amygdala me-
877, 383396. diated changes in neophobia of rats. Behavioral Neuroscience,
Pujol, J., Lopez, A., Deus, J., Cardoner, N., Vallejo, J., Capdevila, A., 108, 11071118.
et al. (2002). Anatomical variability of the anterior cingulate Rose, R. J. (1995). Genes and human behavior. In J. T. Spence, J. M.
gyrus and basic dimensions of human personality. Neuroimage, Darley, & D. P. Foss (Eds.), Annual review of psychology
15, 847855. (pp. 625654). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Pyles, M. K., Stolz, H. R., & MacFarlane, J. W. (1935). The accuracy Rosicky, J. (1993, March). The assessment of temperamental fearful-
of mothers reports on birth and developmental data. Child Devel- ness in infancy. Poster presented at the Society for Research in
opment, 6, 165176. Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Pynoos, R. S., Frederick, C., Nader, K., Arroyo, W., Steinberg, A., Eth, Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy.
S., et al. (1987). Life threat and post traumatic stress disorder in Child Development, 52, 569578.
school-age children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 10571063. Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Temperament and the development of inhib-
Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Williams, M. (1990). Autonomic orient- ited approach. Child Development, 59, 12411250.
ing responses in 15-year-old male subjects and criminal behavior Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament in childhood. In G. A. Kohn-
at age 24. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 933937. stamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in
Rattoni, F. B., Forthman, D. L., Sanchez, M. A., Perez, J. L., & Gar- childhood (pp. 5973). New York: Wiley.
cia, J. (1988). Odor and taste aversions conditioned in anes- Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament
thetized rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 726732. and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40,
Reilly, J. S., Stiles, J., Larsen, J., & Trauner, D. (1995). Affective fa- 2139.
cial expression in infants with focal brain damage. Neuropsy- Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobi-
chologia, 33, 8399. ological approach to the development of temperament. In J. E.
Rebec, G. V., Christianson, J. R., Guevra, C., & Bardo, M. T. (1997). Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament (pp. 83116). Washing-
Regional and temporal differences in real time dopamine eff lux ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
in the nucleus accumbens during food choice novelty. Brain Re- Rubin, K. H. (1982). Non-social play in preschoolers: Necessarily
search, 776, 6167. evil? Child Development, 53, 651657.
Reppucci, C. (1968). Hereditary inf luences upon distribution of atten- Rubin, K. H. (1993). The Waterloo longitudinal project. In K. H.
tion in infancy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Uni- Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition,
versity, Boston, MA. and shyness in childhood (pp. 291314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rezendes, M. O. (1993). Behavioral and autonomic transitions in Rubin, K. H. (2000). The play observation scale (Rev. ed.). University
early infancy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Univer- of Maryland.
sity, Cambridge, MA. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, D. D. (2002). Stability and
Reznick, J. S. (1999). Inf luences on maternal attribution of infant in- social behavioral consequences of toddlers inhibited tempera-
tentionality. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson ment and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483495.
(Eds.), Developing theories of intention (pp. 243267). Mahwah, Rubin, K. H., Cheah, C., & Fox, N. A. (2001). Emotion regulation,
NJ: Erlbaum. parenting, and display of social reticence in preschoolers. Early
Rich, G. J. (1928). A biochemical approach to the study of personal- Education and Development, 12, 97115.
ity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 23, 158175. Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1995). Emo-
Richards, J. E., & Cameron, D. (1989). Infant heart rate variability tionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers social adaptation.
and behavioral developmental status. Infant Behavior and Devel- Development and Psychopathology, 7, 4962.
opment, 12, 4558. Ruff, H. A., Lawson, K. R., Parinello, B., & Weissberg, R. (1990).
Rickman, M. D., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Personality and behavior Long-term stability of individual differences in sustained atten-
in parents of temperamentally inhibited and uninhibited children. tion in the early years. Child Development, 61, 6075.
Developmental Psychology, 30, 346354. Russell, B. (1940). An inquiry into meaning and truth. London: Allen
Riese, M. L. (1987). Temperament stability between the neonatal pe- & Unwin.
riod and 24 months. Developmental Psychology, 23, 216222. Sackett, G. P., Ruppenthall, G. C., Fahrenbuch, C. H., Holm, R. A., &
Riese, M. L. (1988). Temperament in full-term and pre-term infants. Greenough, W. T. (1981). Social isolation rearing effects in mon-
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, 611. keys vary with genotype. Developmental Psychology, 17, 313318.
Riese, M. L. (1995). Mothers ratings of infant temperament. Journal Sagi, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Koren-Karie, N. (1991). Primary
of Genetic Psychology, 156, 2332. appraisal of the strange situation. Developmental Psychology, 27,
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Early, D. M., Cox, M. J., Saluja, G., Pianta, 587596.
R. C., Bradley, R. H., et al. (2002). Early behavior attributes Sanders, S. K. (2001). Cardiovascular and behavioral effects of
and teachers sensitivity as predictors of competent behavior in GABA manipulation in the region of the anterior basolateral
the kindergarten classroom. Applied Developmental Psychology, amygdala of rats. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section, B,
23, 451470. Sciences and Engineering, 62, 1060.
Roback, A. (1931). Psychology of character (2nd ed.). New York: Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and the mechanisms
Harcourt Brace. of neuron death (p. 181). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
224 Biology, Culture, and Temperamental Biases

Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2001). Sources of continuity and the corticotropin releasing hormone locus with behavioral inhibi-
change in observed temperament. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt tion. Biological Psychiatry.
(Eds.), Infancy to early childhood (pp. 89100). New York: Ox- Snidman, N., Kagan, J., Riordan, L., & Shannon, D. (1995). Cardiac
ford. function and behavioral reactivity in infancy. Psychophysiology,
Saudino, K. J., & Eaton, W. O. (1991). Infant temperament and genet- 32, 199207.
ics. Child Development, 62, 11671174. Snowdon, C. T., Coe, C. L., & Hodun, A. (1985). Population recogni-
Saudino, K. J., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & tion of infant isolation peeps in the squirrel monkey. Animal Be-
Plomin, R. (1993). Clarifying the confusion. Paper presented at havior, 33, 11451156.
the Center for Developmental and Health Genetics, Pennsylvania Sperry, R. W. (1977). Bridging science and values. American Psy-
State University, Philadelphia, PA. chologist, 32, 237245.
Schmidt, L., & Fox, N. A. (1994). Patterns of cortical electrophysiol- Spiker, D., Klebanov, P. K., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1992, May). Environ-
ogy and autonomic activity in adults shyness and sociability. Bi- mental and biological correlates of infant temperament. Poster
ological Psychology, 38, 183198. presented at the meeting of the International Society for Infant
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., & Schulkin, J. (1999). Behavioral and Studies, Miami, FL.
pscyhophysiological correlates of self-presentation in tempera- Spiker, D., Kraemer, H. C., Constantine, N. A., & Bryant, D. (1992).
mentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, Reliability and validity of behavior problem checklist as measures
119135. of stable traits in low birth weight premature preschoolers. Child
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). Behav- Development, 63, 14811496.
ioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation in Spurzheim, J. G. (1834). Phrenology. Boston: Marsh, Copen, & Lyon.
temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35,
Stams, G. J. M., Juffer, F., & van IJzendoorn, A. H. (2002). Maternal
119135.
sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early child-
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social hood to predict adjustment in middle childhood. Developmental
anxiety and outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Jour- Psychology, 38, 806821.
nal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Stefanacci, L., & Amaral, D. G. (2002). Some observations on corti-
38, 10081015.
cal inputs to the macaque amygdala. Journal of Comparative Neu-
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. E., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., & Rauch, S. L. rology, 451, 301323.
(2003). Inhibited and uninhibited children Grown up: Adult
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Shouldice, A. (1996). Fearfulness: Develop-
amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 19521953.
mental consistency. In A. Sameroff & M. Haith (Eds.), The five to
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. E., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P. J., seven year shift: Age of reason and responsibility (pp. 237252).
McMullin, K. G., et al. (2003). Differential amygdalar response Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces. Biological Psychia-
Stifter, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Porges, S. W. (1989). Facial expressivity
try, 53, 854862.
and vagal tone in 5- and 10-month-old infants. Infant Behavior
Schwarz, N. (1999). Self-reports. American Psychologist, 54, 93105. and Development, 12, 127137.
Scott, J. P., & Fuller, S. (1965). Genetics and the social behavior of Stifter, C. A., & Wiggins, C. N. (2004). Assessment of disturbances
the dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. in emotion regulation and temperament. In R. Del Carmen-
Seifer, R., Sameroff, A. J., Barrett, L. C., & Krafchuk, E. (1994). In- Wiggins & A. Carter (Eds.), Handbook of infant, toddler, and pre-
fant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother school mental health assessment (pp. 79103). New York: Oxford
report. Child Development, 65, 14781490. University Press.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2003). Signalers and receivers in Strauss, M. E., & Rourke, D. C. (1978). A multivariate analysis of the
animal communication. In S. T. Fiske, D. L. Schacter, & C. Zahn- neonatal behavioral assessment scale in several samples. Mono-
Waxler (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 54, graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43(5/6),
pp. 145173). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews. 8191.
Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. (2003). Tra- Strelau, J., & Angleitner, A. (1991). Explorations in temperament.
jectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental New York: Plenum Press.
Psychology, 39, 189200. Strouse, J. (1980). Alice James. Boston: Houghton Miff lin.
Sheldon, W. H. (1940). The varieties of human physique. New York: Suomi, S. J. (1987). Genetic and maternal contributions to individual
Harper. differences in rhesus monkey biobehavioral development. In N. A.
Shumyatsky, G. P., Tsvetkov, E., Malleret, G., Vronskaya, S., Hatton, Krasnegor, E. M. Blass, M. A. Hofer, & W. P. Smotherman (Eds.),
M., Hampton, L., et al. (2002). Identification of a signaling net- Perinatal development (pp. 397420). New York: Academic Press.
work in lateral nucleus of amygdala important for inhibiting mem- Svartberg, K., & Forkman, B. (2002). Personality traits in the domes-
ory specifically related to learned fear. Cell, 11, 905918. tic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behavior Science, 79,
Siegel, R. E. (1968). Galens system of physiology and medicine. 133156.
Basel, Switzerland: Karger. Takahashi, J. S., & Hoffman, M. (1995). Molecular biological clocks.
Simms, J. (1887). Physiognomy illustrated (8th ed.). New York: Mur- American Scientist, 83, 158165.
ray Hill. Takahashi, L. K., & Rubin, W. W. (1994). Corticosteriod induction of
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213, treat-induced behavioral inhibition in preweanling rats. Behav-
501504. ioral Neuroscience, 107, 860868.
Smollar, J. W., Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Kennedy, J., Dai, D., Terr, L. C. (1979). Children of Chowchilla. Psychoanalytic Study of
Racitte, S., et al. (in press). Association of the genetic marker at the Child, 34, 547623.
References 225

Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New Walker, D. L., Toufexis, D. J., & Davis, M. (2003). Role of the bed nu-
York: Brunner/ Mazel. cleus of the stria terminalis versus the amygdala in fear, stress,
Thompson, R. A., Connell, J. P., & Bridges, L. J. (1988). Tempera- and anxiety. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463, 199216.
ment, emotion, and social interactive behavior in the strange situ- Wallis, R. S. (1954). The overt fears of Dakota Indian children. Child
ation. Child Development, 59, 11021110. Development, 25, 185192.
Thompson, R. F., Donegan, N. H., Clark, G. A., Levond, D. G., Lin- Wang, H., & Wessendorf, M. W. (2002). Mu- and delta-opioid recep-
coln, J. S., Madden, J., et al. (1987). Neural substrates of discrete tor mRNAs are expressed in periaqueductal gray neurons project-
defensive conditioned ref lexes, conditioned fear states, and their ing to the rostral ventromedial medulla. Neuroscience, 109,
interaction in the rabbit. In I. Gormezano, W. F. Prokasy, & R. F. 619634.
Thompson (Eds.), Classical conditioning (3rd ed., pp. 371399).
Watamura, S. E., Sebanc, A. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2002). Rising cor-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tisol at childcare: Relations with nap, rest, and temperament. De-
Thompson, W. R. (1953). The inheritance of behavior. Canadian velopmental Psychobiology, 40, 3342.
Journal of Psychology, 7, 145155.
Weber, M., Watts, N., & Richardson, R. (2003). High illumination
Tomarken, A. J., Davidson, R. J., & Henriques, J. B. (1990). Resting levels potentiate the acoustic startle response in prewenaling rats.
frontal brain asymmetry predicts affective responses to films. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 14581462.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 791801.
Weissman, M. M. (1984). Depression and anxiety disorder in parents
Tout, K., de Haan, M., Kipp-Campbell, E., & Gunnar, M. R. (1998).
and children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 847849.
Social behavior correlates of adrenocortical activity in daycare:
Gender differences and time of day effects. Child Development, Weisz, J. R., Suwanlert, S., Chaiyasit, W., Weiss, B., Achenbach, T.,
69, 12471262. & Walter, B. R. (1987). Epidemiology of behavioral and emo-
tional problems among Thai and American children. Journal of
Treit, D., Pesold, C., & Rotzinger, S. (1993a). Dissociating the anti-
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26,
fear effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions using two pharmaco-
890898.
logically validated models of rat anxiety. Behavioral
Neuroscience, 107, 770785. Weisz, J. R., Suwanlert, S., Chaiyasit, W., Weiss, B., Walter, B. R., &
Treit, D., Pesold, C., & Rotzinger, S. (1993b). Noninteractive effects Anderson, W. W. (1988). Thai and American perspectives on over
of diazepam and amygdaloid lesions in two animal models of anx- and under control of child behavior problems. Journal of Consult-
iety. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 10991105. ing and Clinical Psychology, 56, 601609.
Tremblay, R. E., Pihl, R. O., Vitaro, F., & Dubkin, P. L. (1994). Pre- Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk resilience and recovery. Development and
dicting early onset of male antisocial behavior from preschool be- Psychopathology, 5, 503515.
havior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 732735. Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity. Current Direc-
Trut, L. N. (1999). Early canid domestication. American Scientist, 87, tions in Psychological Science, 7, 177187.
160169. Whitehead, A. N. (1928). Science and the modern world. New York:
Valentine, C. W. (1930). The innate basis of fear. Journal of Genetic Macmillan.
Psychology, 37, 394420. Williamson, D. E., Coleman, K., Bacanu, S., Devlin, B. J., Rogers, J.,
van Bockstaele, E. J., Bajic, D., Proudfit, H., & Valentino, R. J. Ryan, N. D., et al. (2003). Heritability of fearful-anxious en-
(2001). Topographic architecture of stress-related pathways tar- dophenotypes in infant rhesus macaques. Biological Psychiatry,
getting the noradrenergic locus ceruleus. Physiology and Behav- 53, 284291.
ior, 73, 273283. Wilson, D. S., Clark, A. B., Coleman, K., & Dearstyne, T. (1994).
Van den Boom, D. C., & Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The effect of infant Shyness and boldness in human and other animals. Trends in Ecol-
irritability on mother-infant interaction. Developmental Psychol- ogy and Evolution, 9, 442446.
ogy, 30, 581590.
Wilson, F. A., & Rolls, E. T. (1993). The effect of stimulus novelty
Vaughn, B. E., Bradley, C. F., Joffe, L. S., Seifer, R., & Barglow, R. and familiarity on neuronal activity in the amygdala of monkeys
(1987). Maternal characteristics measured prenatally are predic- performing recognition memory tasks. Experimental Brain Re-
tive of ratings of temperamental difficulty on the Carey infant search, 93, 367382.
temperament questionnaire. Developmental Psychology, 23,
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York:
152161.
Macmillin.
Vaughn, B. E., Stevenson-Hinde, J., Waters, E., Kotsaftis, A.,
Lefaver, G. B., Shouldice, A., et al. (1992). Attachment security Worobey, J., & Lewis, M. (1989). Individual differences in the reac-
and temperament in infancy in early childhood. Developmental tivity of young infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 663667.
Psychology, 28, 463473. Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1970). Recollections
Veit, R., Flor, H., Erb, M., Hermann, C., Lotze, M., Grudd, W., et al. of childhood. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child De-
(2002). Brain circuits involved in emotional learning in antisocial velopment, 35(5).
behavior and social phobia in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328, Yosida, M. (1973). The Chinese concept of nature. In S. Nakayama &
231233. N. Sivin (Eds.), Chinese science (pp. 7190). Cambridge, MA:
Volpe, J. (1995). Neurology of the newborn. Philadelphia: Saunders. MIT Press.
Walker, B. E., & Fraser, F. C. (1957). The embryology of cortisone
induced cleft palate. Journal of Embryology and Experimental
Morphology, 5, 201209.
CHAPTER 5

Emotional Development: Action,


Communication, and Understanding
CAROLYN SAARNI, JOSEPH J. CAMPOS, LINDA A. CAMRAS, and DAVID WITHERINGTON

A CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK FOR FEELING Social Effectiveness and Skill 2: Ability to Discern and
AND EMOTION 227 Understand Others Emotions 254
A Working Definition of Emotion 227 Social Effectiveness and Skill 3: Use of a Vocabulary of
Culture, Emotion, and Emotional Development 235 Emotion and Expression 258
THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONAL Social Effectiveness and Skill 4: The Capacity for
COMMUNICATION IN EARLY LIFE 239 Empathic and Sympathetic Involvement 262
Phase 1 (Prenatal to 6 Weeks): Initial Reactions to Social Effectiveness and Skill 5: Skill in Differentiating
Emotion Signals 240 Internal Emotional Experience from External
Phase 2 (6 Weeks to 9 Months): Pre-Referential Emotional Expression 266
Communication 241 Social Effectiveness and Skill 6: Skill in Adaptive
Phase 3 (9 Months to 18 Months): Behavioral Regulation Coping with Aversive Emotions and Distressing
and Referential Communication 243 Circumstances 271
Phase 4 (18 Months/2 Years and Beyond): The Rise of Social Effectiveness and Skill 7: Awareness of Emotion
Other-Conscious Emotions 246 Communication in Relationships 279
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT IN CHILDHOOD AND Social Effectiveness and Skill 8: Capacity for Emotional
ADOLESCENCE: SOCIAL EFFECTIVENESS AND Self-Efficacy 282
POSITIVE ADAPTATION 248 CONCLUSION 284
Social Adjustment and Emotion Regulation 248 REFERENCES 288
Emotional Competence 249
Social Effectiveness and Skill 1: Awareness of Our
Emotions 250

Extraordinary changes have been taking place in the measured with specificity. The 1983 chapter described
study of emotion in the past 30 years. When the chapter the emergence of a functionalist approach to emotions,
dealing with socioemotional development was published and showed how, contrary to prior thought, emotions
in the fourth edition of this Handbook (Campos, Barrett, profoundly affected cognitive, perceptual, social, and
Lamb, Goldsmith, & Stenberg, 1983), the study of emo- self-regulatory processes. It also described the close
tion and emotional development was just emerging from link between emotion and temperamental dispositions,
decades of neglect. As was noted in that chapter, there attachment, and parent-child interactions. It also docu-
were two principal reasons for the neglect. The first was mented major advances in the measurement of emotion
the widespread conviction that emotions were epiphe- in face, voice, and action. The chapter in the fifth edi-
nomenal, and the second was that emotions could not be tion of this Handbook detailed the elaborations in theory

Preparation of this chapter was conducted with the partial We gratefully acknowledge the assistance in the preparation
support of grant number HD-25066 from the National Insti- of this chapter of Carl Frankel and Rosemary Campos. The
tutes of Health, grant number MH-47543 from the National chapter was written with the support of grants HD-399-25
Institute of Mental Health, and a research grant from the and the National Science Foundation (Grant #BCS-
John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation. 0002001).

226
A Conceptual Framework for Feeling and Emotion 227

and research in emotion and emotional development person-environment transactions. This framework in-
since 1983, and extended the treatment of emotion to in- cludes communication as a central aspect of action.
clude new concerns about emotion competence.
The remarkable surge of investigation on emotion has
A Working Definition of Emotion
continued unabated in the past 10 years, and the results
of such investigations are dramatically changing our Emotion is thus the persons attempt or readiness to es-
conceptualization of both the nature of emotions and tablish, maintain, or change the relation between the per-
their function in development. Three major themes in re- son and her or his changing circumstances, on matters of
cent research stand out: (1) the close link between emo- significance to that person (Campos, Frankel, & Cam-
tion and action, (2) the social functions of emotion, and ras, 2004).
(3) the closing of the gap in knowledge about develop- The definition may initially appear to be odd because
ment between infancy and adolescence. of the absence of any reference to the traditional ele-
The present chapter reflects these three emphases. ments found in the most prevalent definitions of emo-
First, we describe a recently revived way of conceptual- tion. There is no allusion to feeling, vegetative states,
izing emotionone that traces its roots to the long-ig- facial indices of internal states, or other intrapersonal
nored work of John Dewey (1894, 1895). Second, we criteria. Instead, emotion is determined by the signifi-
stress childrens understanding of emotion, and how cance of a person-event transaction. Because the defini-
children cope with their emotions and the environmental tion emphasizes what the person is trying to accomplish,
transactions that evoke them. Third, we are concerned and because it comes from a conception of emotion that
with emotional development in preschool and middle stresses the consequences of emotional states, this
childhood, when the significance of emotion is espe- working definition of emotion is often called a function-
cially broad in scope. The chapter also contains a num- alist one (Barrett & Campos, 1987; Campos, Mumme,
ber of subordinate themes. For instance, we review some Kermoian, & Campos, 1994; Frijda, 1986, 1987;
of the intriguing research that has been done with infants Lazarus, 1991).
and toddlers on how they develop systems of emotional There are at least four ways by which events become
communication. In the process, we show how the emo- significant. The first is a particularly powerful and per-
tional expressions of others regulate the behavior of in- vasive one: goal relevance and its corollaries. Lazarus
fants and children and result in empathic behavior, (1991) specifically links the first step in the generation
emotion regulation, and coping. Furthermore, we dis- of emotion to this factor; however, goal relevance en-
cuss why action has become so important in contempo- sures the generation only of some kind of affect. To ac-
rary approaches to emotion, and how cultural approaches count for whether the affect has a positive or negative
to emotion are beginning to draw our attention to the im- hedonic tone and a behavioral valence of approach or
portance of emotion communication in development. withdrawal, Lazarus posits the congruence or incongru-
ence of an event to personal goals: Goal congruent
transactions produce positive hedonic tone, and goal in-
congruent transactions bring about negative tone. To ex-
A CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK FOR plain how a specific emotion such as fear, anger, or
FEELING AND EMOTION shame comes about, he proposes the factor of ego in-
volvement (this determines the specific nature of the
Emotions seem to be most closely linked to what a person emotion elicited). So, regardless of the specific goal one
is trying to do. Ones perception and interpretation of is working toward, a person who overcomes obstacles to
events is never independent of the action that one can per- goal attainment is likely to experience happiness or re-
form on them (Adolph, Eppler, & Gibson, 1993; Dewey, lief. A person who relinquishes a goal experiences sad-
1894, 1895). Indeed, an event can be defined as an oppor- ness, regardless of whether that goal involves physical,
tunity for action. However, not all events generate emo- social, or psychological loss. A person who encounters
tiononly those in which one has a stake in the outcome. obstacles to goal attainment will show frustration or
Hence, we propose a working definition of emotion that anger. The specific nature of the goal can also affect the
emphasizes action, the preparation for action, and ap- experience of a given emotion. Thus, avoidance of
praisal of the significance or relevance to concerns of threat is linked to fear, wanting to atone is related to
228 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

guilt, and the wish to escape the scrutiny of others fol- The fourth way that events become significant comes
lowing a transgression is linked to shame. Table 5.1 lists from memory of transactions from the past. Although
the factors that Barrett and Campos (1987) proposed for all emotion theories stress the role of memory in gener-
the generation and manifestation of a variety of emo- ating affect, we would like to emphasize the importance
tional states. Some of these emotions are called pri- of past experience for the selection of strategies for re-
mordial emotions, to denote their likely presence in the sponding emotionally. Such a link is best represented in
neonate and their rudimentary appraisal demands; oth- the research on working models in attachment (Brether-
ers are called concurrent state emotions, to specify ton, 1985). For example, as Cassidy (1994) has said,
their close link to flexible goals and strivings; still oth- avoidantly attached infants typically have a history of
ers are called social emotions, to indicate their origin interactions in which their attachment figure has ig-
in social rules backed by emotion communication from nored the infants social signals such as bids for comfort.
significant others. For all of these emotions, goal rele- When these bids are consistently rejected by the care-
vance is typically the most fundamental principle of giver, the child is predisposed toward muted affect dur-
emotion generation ing reunions with the caregiver. The past history of
Not all emotions are generated by the relation of ignoring social bids makes the risk of present rejection
events to goals. A second way in which emotion can be too great. By contrast, infants who are classified as am-
generated is through the social signals of others, which bivalently attached have a history of interaction with a
have powerful capacities to render a person-environment figure who has responded inconsistently to their social
transaction significant (Klinnert, Campos, Sorce, signals. When such children are reunited with the at-
Emde, & Svejda, 1983; McIntosh, Druckman, & Zajonc, tachment figure following separation, they show exag-
1994). They do so because social signals can generate a gerated, rather than muted, emotional reactions. Such
contagious emotional response and tendency for action exaggeration serves the function, in part, of ensuring the
in the perceiver (Hatfield, Caccioppo, & Rapson, 1994). parents responsiveness and avoiding the parents insen-
Social signals can also give meaning to a transaction as- sitivity. Thus, past experiences determine not only the
sociated with the signal (such as when an infant catches precise nature of the emotion a child undergoes (as in
the mothers fear of dogs and begins to avoid them; e.g., the case of desire and aversion discussed earlier) but
Bowlby, 1972). Finally, social signals play a central, also the manner in which the child responds to, or copes
though under-investigated, role in generating emotions with, contemporary interactions with significant others.
such as pride, shame, and guilt through the enduring ef-
fects that they can have as accompaniments to the ap- Feeling and Emotion
proval and disapproval of others. What is the role in the emotion process of what we call
A third source of significance comes about through feelingthe irreducible quality of consciousness that
hedonic processesspecifically, when hedonic stimula- accompanies evaluations? The laypersons conception
tion is experienced and becomes the object of ones places feeling at the core of emotion. That conception
strivings (Frijda, 1986). Hedonic stimulation refers to goes as follows: Events elicit feeling, feeling organizes
the sights, sounds, tastes, smells, and tactile stimula- expressions (or outward signs) of feeling and autonomic
tions that intrinsically produce irreducible sensations of and instrumental behavioral reactions designed both to
pleasure or pain. With pleasurable hedonic experience, manifest outwardly and, in addition, to deal with the
we are more likely to want to repeat such experience and feeling. In development, many theorists (e.g., Lewis &
thus we approach objects and people. It is the opposite Michalson, 1983; Sroufe, 1979) have proposed that feel-
with painful experience. Pleasure and pain are affecto- ing is absent in the young infant and comes about only
genic in the following way, taken from Frijda (1986): If, after the infant has acquired the capacity to distinguish
after one experiences pleasant stimulation and one self from otheran accomplishment that begins to be
wants to repeat the experience, the emotion of desire is shown by 9 months of age.
generated; similarly, if one experiences pain and wants The functionalist approach to emotion gives feeling a
not to repeat the experience, the emotion of aversion is major role as a facet of emotion, but not as its core. Feel-
created. Desire and aversion, with further development, ings are not prior in time to other processes in emotion
can become the core of much more complex emotional generation, as orthodox conceptualizations require. We
transactions, including envy, jealousy, and rage. propose that the origins of feeling come from four
TABLE 5.1 Characteristics of Some Emotion Families

Emotion Appreciation Appreciation Physiological


Family Goal re Self re Other Action Tendency Adaptive Functions Facial Expression b Reaction Vocalic Pattern f
a
Disgust Avoiding contamina- This stimulus may Active rejection Avoid contamination Brows lowered, nose Low heart rate and Nasal, slightly tense,
tion or illness contaminate me, or and illness; learn about wrinkled, with widened skin temperature; very narrow, but
cause illness substances/events/ nasal root; raised increased skin fairly full and
attributes to avoid; cheeks and upper lip resistance c powerful voice
alert others re
contamination
a
Fear Maintaining integrity This stimulus threatens Flight; active Avoid danger (physical Brows raised and often High, stable heart Narrow, extremely
of the self (physical my integrity withdrawal and psychological); learn pulled slightly together; rate; low skin tense, very weak, thin,
or psychological about events/attributes eyes very wide and temperature; high voice
integrity) that are dangerous; alert tense, rigidly fixated gasping
others re danger on stimulus respiration c
a
Anger Any end state that There is an obstacle to Active forward Attain difficult goals; Brows lowered and High heart rate and Narrow, medium to
the organism my obtaining my goal movement, especially to learn to overcome pulled together; mouth skin temperature; very tense, medium to
currently is invested eliminate obstacles obstacles and achieve open and square or lips facial f lushing c extremely full voice
in achieving goals; communicate pressed tightly together
power/dominance
a
Sadness Any end state that My goal is unattainable Disengagement; passive Conserve energy; learn Inner corners of brows Low heart rate; d Narrow, thin, lax,
the organism cur- withdrawal which goals are moved upward; corners low skin tempera- slow, or halting voice
rently is invested realizable; encourage of mouth pulled ture and skin
in achieving nurturance by others downward, often with resistance
middle of chin pulled

229
upward
Shame Maintaining others I am bad (self-esteem Someone/everyone Active or passive Behave appropriately; Low heart rate; Narrow, moderately
respect and is perceived to be notices how bad withdrawal; avoiding learn /maintain social blushing e lax, thin voice
affection; preserving impaired) I am others; hiding of self standards; communicate
self-esteem submission to others and
to others standards
Guilt Meeting ones own I have done something Someone has been Outward movement; Behave prosocially; High heart rate Narrow, tense,
internalized contrary to my injured by my act inclination to make learn /maintain moral and skin moderately full voice
standards standards reparation, to inform and prosocial behavior; conductance;
others, and to punish communicate contrition / irregular respiration e
oneself good intentions
Pride Maintaining the I am good (I have Someone/everyone Outward/upward Behave appropriately; High heart rate e Wide, medium
respect of oneself respect for myself ) thinks (or will movement; inclination learn /maintain tense, full voice
and others think) I am good to show/inform others social standards;
about ones communicate ability to
accomplishments meet standards
a
No appreciation re other is central to primordial or concurrent-goal emotions; however, particular family members might involve such an appreciation.
b
These facial movements are adapted from Izard (1979).
c
These are adapted from Ekman, Levenson, and Friesen (1983).
d
Ekman et al. (1983) found increased heart rate with sadness; however, decreased heart rate is consistent with our theoretical position on sadness. We think it possible that most subjects in Ekman et al.s study
experienced an agitated grief state rather than a sad giving-up state.
e
These are hypothesized physiological reactions.
f
These vocalic patterns are adapted from Scherer (1986).
230 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

sources, some of which are different from the sources circumstances in which expressions by another most di-
that generate emotion and were delineated earlier (Cam- rectly generate a similar feeling in the perceiver, those
pos et al., 1994). One of these is the conscious accompa- which generate an emotional state that is similar in va-
niment to the process of appraisalthe determination lence but different in quality, those in which an oppo-
of how an event impinges on ones goals (Lazarus, site emotional state is elicited, and those in which no
1991). The appreciation of the meaning of an event feelings are generated at all. Research on the ontogeny
shows that it matters to the individual, and feeling is the and consequences of affect contagion and socially in-
registration of this significance. In short, feeling ac- duced affect is sorely lacking, despite exciting work
companiesnot precedesthe registration of events. with socially deprived infant monkeys suggesting that
The second source of feeling is the consciousness of no social experience is necessary for social signals to
the activation of goal-oriented central motor commands affect behavior in affectively appropriate ways (Ken-
(efference). Efference, unlike return sensory flow to the ney, Mason, & Hill, 1979; Sackett, 1966; see also the
brain (afference), has rarely been linked to conscious- subsequent section in this chapter on the related pro-
ness in psychological theories, yet efference plays a role cess of empathy). Socially induced affect renders feel-
in the perception of self-motion as well as in the sense ing simultaneous with the detection of social signals,
of volitionof willing a body movement to take place not prior to them. Such considerations are what have led
(Teuber, 1960). This notion of the importance of effer- us to propose that feeling is a facet of the emotion pro-
ence in generating feeling is consistent with the reason- cess, but not its core. In addition, because infants can
ing of Ekman, Levenson, and Friesen (1983), who show facial and instrumental behavior patterns of spe-
discussed the importance of motoric commands to cre- cific emotions very early in life (e.g., Gaensbauer,
ate facial patterns and for bringing about both emotion 1982; Stenberg & Campos, 1990), appraisal, efference,
and feeling. It is also consistent with recent theorizing and afference are available to the young infant, and so
by Damasio (1994) who stated, the brain learns to con- may affect emotion contagion (Haviland & Lelwica,
nect the fainter image of an emotional body state, 1987). We thus find no reason to deny infants younger
without having to reenact it in the body proper (p. 155). than 9 months of age the experience of feeling.
The link between efference and emotion again renders
Facial Expressions and Emotion
feeling contemporary with emotion generation, not as an
antecedent. Facial expressions have been hypothesized to play a
The third way feeling can be generated is through the particularly important role in the emotion process. One
perception of sensations coming from both smooth and historically influential emotion theorist (Tomkins,
striated muscles and from the effects of hormones. Our 1962) virtually equated emotion with facial respond-
language is full of references to these internal states ing. For preverbal infants, facial expressions have been
such as when we talk of feeling a cold fear, being proposed to have additional importance because they
flushed in anger, having butterflies in the stomach, are presumed by some to be the sole means by which
and so forth. In addition, many cultures literally em- emotions can be communicated before the advent of
body emotion by referring to somatic states that occur language. Thus, some scholars have proposed a virtu-
when one is in distress or euphoria (Shweder, 1993). The ally one-to-one correspondence between emotion and
role of feedback seems undeniable in creating aspects of facial expression for postneonatal infants (Izard, 2004;
feeling (Laird, 1984), but feeling is again not primary. It Izard, Ackerman, Schoff, & Fine, 2000) and developed
follows response generation. coding systems for infant emotions that rest on the
The fourth way that feeling can be generated is identification of prespecified facial configurations.
through the direct perception of emotional expressions Such an approach to emotion measurement would have
in the face, voice, and gesture of another (Hatfield considerable appeal because it provides an easy solu-
et al., 1994). This is the phenomenon referred to as so- tion to the problem of identifying emotion in infants.
cially induced af fect (McIntosh et al., 1994), which is However, we believe it is fundamentally flawed on both
defined as the generation of a like or complementary an empirical and conceptual level. Recent studies have
feeling state in the other as a result of the perception of documented numerous examples of nonconcordance be-
social displays in another. This phenomenon is quite tween emotion and these prespecified facial expres-
context specific. At this time, we do not understand the sions. For example, infants on the visual cliff display
A Conceptual Framework for Feeling and Emotion 231

clear indications of fear (e.g., refusal to crawl) but do action of doing nothing can be in the service of sadness
not show prototypic fear expressions. Indeed, they (as in depressive withdrawal), fear (as in keeping still to
often smile! Conversely, infants typically produce the avoid detection), or anger (as in passive aggressiveness).
prototypic facial configuration of surprise (involving Emotions are best considered as syndromesalterna-
raised brow and open mouth) as they introduce an ob- tive patterns of behavior, any of which can under the
ject into their mouth for oral exploration (Camras, right circumstances specify the emotion (Lazarus &
Lambrecht, & Michel, 1996). Such mismatches do not Averill, 1972). It is not possible to identify a priori an
imply that facial expressions are misleading or irrele- operational definition of a given emotion that can be ap-
vant to infant emotion. As we further argue, we believe plied in all circumstances, such that knowing the re-
that facial expressions serve as critically important sponse or response pattern by itself one can predict the
components in a larger pattern of information that ob- emotional state of a person. A discrete emotion thus
servers perceive and integrate in making an emotion lacks a gold standardan ostensive definition. Neither
judgment (see Oster, in press, for a somewhat different the face, voice, gesture, specific instrumental behavior
but related view of infant emotional facial expressions). nor autonomic signatures are likely to have more than a
probabilistic relation to an emotional state; even then,
Action Tendencies and the Flexible
context must be taken into account to interpret the mean-
Manifestation of Emotion
ing of a response (see Camras et al., 2002).
In the course of studying blind infants, Fraiberg (1971) At present, the concept of af fect families (Barrett &
discovered that many parents of such children showed Campos, 1987; Dewey, 1934; Kagan, 1994) is used to
profound disappointment when they encountered low convey the notion that each experience of a given emo-
levels of facial responsiveness and eye contact in their tion such as anger or fear is likely to differ in important
children. The parents seemed to withdraw from their ways from other emotional experiences of the same
children and to lack the incentive to provide them with class. Each instance of an emotion differs from another
physical and social stimulation after noting their chil- in social signaling, type of behavior shown in context,
drens apparent unresponsiveness. Fraiberg (1971) dis- and pattern of appraisal, yet, so long as the adaptational
covered that although blind infants were indeed intent is the same for two different experiences, it can be
relatively unresponsive facially during social encoun- said that the different instances bear a family resem-
ters, they seemed extraordinarily articulate in express- blance to each other. Through such adaptational in-
ing their emotions and social responses through the tents, emotions can be classified and their differential
actions of their fingers. When this responsiveness was consequences understood (Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, &
pointed out to the parents, they dramatically increased OConnor, 1987), not by similarity in morphology.
their levels of interaction with the infants; the infants, in
turn, were able to maintain their digitally mediated level Measuring Emotion via Action Tendencies
of social responsiveness. The absence of an ostensive criterion for a given emo-
Fraibergs observations document an important prin- tional state creates serious problems of inference. One
ciple about emotions: Many different responses can be attempt to resolve this dilemma has been proposed by
in the service of any given emotionemotional re- Frijda (1986) in his concept of action tendencies. Avoid-
sponses exhibit the property of equipotentiality. To ex- ance of threat, for instance, is the action tendency for
pect, as some theories do (e.g., Ekman et al., 1983; fear; avoidance of social contact of the scrutinizing
Izard, 1977, 1991; Tomkins, 1962, 1963), a close corre- other is that for shame; devotion of effort to remove an
spondence between a given response or response pattern obstacle is the action tendency for anger, and so on.
(e.g., a facial expression) and a given emotional state is Table 5.2 lists Frijdas proposed action tendencies and
likely to lead to errors of inference. The opposite is also the specific emotions that they denotea list that he
true: The same response can be recruited to express considers incomplete but representative. Note that Fri-
many different emotions. Some years ago, Kagan (1971) jdas list of action tendencies yields a much larger num-
put it well. He said that the smile serves many masters. ber of emotions than does reliance on universality of
Consider that the action of smiling can be in the service recognition of facial expressions, which usually are lim-
of joy, scorn, nurturance, embarrassment, and other ited to fear, sadness, joy, surprise, sadness, disgust, and
emotions, or stereotyped social greeting. Similarly, the possibly contempt.
232 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

TABLE 5.2 Relational Action Tendencies, Activation Modes, and Inhibitionsa


Action Tendency End State Function Emotion
Approach Access Producing situation permitting consummatory activity Desire
Avoidance Own inaccessibility Protection Fear
Being with Contact, interaction Permitting consummatory activity Enjoyment, confidence

Attending (opening) Identification Orientation Interest


Rejecting (closing) Removal of object Protection Disgust
Nonattending No information or contact Selection Indifference

Agonistic Removal of obstruction Regaining control Anger

Interrupting Reorientation Reorientation Shock

Dominating Retained control Generalized control Arrogance


Submitting Def lected pressure Secondary control Humility
Deactivation (Recuperation?) Sorrow
Bound activation Action tendencys end state Aim achievement Effort

Excitement Readiness Excitement


Free activation Generalized readiness Joy
Inactivity Recuperation Contentment
Inhibition Absence of response Caution Anxiety
Surrender Activation decrease? Activation decrease or social cohesion? Laughter, weeping
a
Adapted from Frijda (1986).

For Frijda, the concept of action tendency in no way stored to its place in scientific study a few years ago,
refers to a response that can be measured by electromyo- there is a major precedent for measuring the organization
graphy or by operational definition of a given response. of behaviora precedent that is both intellectually per-
Rather, action tendency refers to any of a number of suasive and highly influential (Sroufe & Waters, 1977).
flexibly organized phenomena that serve the function of, In attachment theory, Bowlby (1969) posited that attach-
for example, avoiding threat or overcoming an obstacle. ment could be measured by proximity seeking in times of
In this sense, action tendency is similar to the etholo- fear or distress. Although proximity seeking can be oper-
gists conception of a behavioral systema conception ationalized by measuring the physical distance of the
that replaced notions of fixed action patterns with ap- child from the attachment figure (Coates, Anderson, &
preciation of the multiplicity of ways by which an ani- Hartup, 1972; L. Cohen & Campos, 1974), such an ap-
mal can attain an end (Bischof, 1975). The behavioral proach reveals little in the way of stability of individual
system for the ethologist, like the concept of a specific differences in attachment, nor is it an index that retains
emotion, is defined by the function those behaviors its manifestation as the child grows older and shows at-
serve. How is function measured? The functionalists tachment patterns in a variety of different ways. Attach-
answer is: (a) by inference from the organization of be- ment theorists (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall,
havior, ( b) by suppositions about what the person is try- 1978) have solved this problem of measuring proximity
ing to accomplish, and (c) by noting whether progress seeking by noting first of all whether different behav-
toward the inferred goal is proceeding smoothly or with iors shown by the child in the context of reunion with the
difficulty. The identification of the operation of a dis- caregiver are in the service of proximity-seeking, even
crete emotion is intimately tied in to the context in though there may be no approach toward the caregiver.
which the person is found and the types of behavior pat- There are many alternative ways in which attachment se-
tern the person shows in that setting. curity can be manifested: smiling at the caregiver, mak-
Although the task of measuring emotion is much more ing pickup bids, sharing the joy of playing with a toy, and
difficult than initially thought when emotion was re- so on. These alternative behavioral strategies are taken as
A Conceptual Framework for Feeling and Emotion 233

partial evidence for what Sroufe (1979; Sroufe & Waters, the components. For example, an individual may experi-
1977) calls the organization of behavior. The organiza- ence road rage only when he is in a hurry (i.e., the im-
tion stems from the similar ends that morphologically pediment has high significance), when he can find no
quite different behaviors serve. The crucial factors of way around the impeding driver, when he appraises the
avoidance and ambivalence in attachment are similarly driver as deliberately obstructive, and when the driver
inferred by judging the many alternative ways that a child resembles his estranged father. As emphasized by the
can give the parent the cold shoulder treatment speci- dynamical systems approach, emotion cannot be re-
fying avoidance, or the angry yet relieved expression of duced to any one of these components; specific compo-
ambivalence. In short, we think investigators of emotion nents themselves do not engender emotion, only the
can learn useful lessons from the literature on attach- relations among components. Thus, fully capturing the
ment, especially to the extent that both emotion and at- complexity of emotion and its generation requires a
tachment exhibit the property of equipotentiality of view of emotion as relational, deriving from the interac-
responses. tions of many components (Fogel & Thelen, 1987; M. D.
This approach to measurement of the action tenden- Lewis, 2000; M. D. Lewis & Granic, 1999; Mascolo &
cies related to attachment needs to be generalized to the Harkins, 1998).
study of other emotional states. It should not be thought Such a view has immediate implications for our con-
that such flexibility of behavior organized around an ceptualization of emotional development. Understand-
emotion is limited to the older school-age child and the ing the difference in emotion between a 12-month-old
adult. Fraibergs (1971) observations of blind infants and an 18-month-old extends beyond a simple acknowl-
social responsiveness described earlier demonstrate this, edgment of differences in how the two appraise events,
and so do 8- to 9-month-old infants tested on the visual and must include consideration of those developmental
cliff, a highly reliable fear elicitor (Scarr & Salapatek, changes that engender such alterations in appraisal:
1970). At that age, infants can manifest fear by literal changes in the infants goals and concerns as well as the
avoidance of descending onto the glass-covered deep side means available to the infant for acting on the world and
of the cliff, or they can approach the mother, but in a thus achieving his goals. The source of developmental
manner indicative of fear. The infants do this by detour- transformation in emotion thus resides in the relations
ing around the deep side, hitching along the sidewalls of among components that comprise the system (Fogel
the cliff table until they reach the mother (Campos, et al., 1992; Griffin & Mascolo, 1998; Witherington,
Hiatt, Ramsay, Henderson, & Svejda, 1978). Behavioral Campos, & Hertenstein, 2001). As envisioned by dy-
flexibility is the rule, not the exception, in the manifesta- namical systems approaches, these components influ-
tion of emotion. Restriction of such flexibility in the in- ence one another so that the system self-organizes into
terests of measuring one or more responses chosen a relatively stable patterns that differ across age. By way
priori puts at risk the internal validity of a given study, of contrast, traditional explanations have attributed
as well as its external or ecological validity. emotional development to factors that they consider to
be external to the emotion system. A classic example of
Component Systems Approaches to Emotion and such an approach is evident in Emde, Gaensbauer, and
its Development: Dynamical Systems and Harmons (1976) biobehavioral shift model of emo-
Functionalist Perspectives tional development. By their account, the emergence of
In the previous section on action tendencies, we empha- social smiling around 2 months and the emergence of
sized how emotions lack a gold standard because vari- fear in multiple contexts around 8 months ref lect overar-
ous aspects of emotional actionfacial, vocal, and ching neuromaturational change. To quote Emde et al.:
gestural signals, or specific instrumental behaviorare The emergent affect behavior is not the organizer, it
flexibly organized depending on context. Just as any merely indicates it (p. 8). From the standpoint of
emotion action cannot be fully understood independent biobehavioral shifts, emotional development is deter-
of context, so any component of emotion, whether it be mined by outside factors (Emde, Kligman, Reich, &
action or action tendency, goals and concerns, physio- Wade, 1978). Most cognitive theories of emotional de-
logical patterning, appraisals, experiential feeling velopment follow a similar line of approach by arguing
states, or social and physical contexts, cannot explain that emotional development is attendant on the develop-
emotion itself independent of the other components that ment of specific cognitive prerequisites that are consid-
comprise the system and the relations that exist among ered external to emotion, such as new representational
234 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

and memory abilities, the emergence of objective self- transactions, the functionalist appeals to those themes
awareness, or the emergence of an ability to evaluate be- involving basic matches/mismatches between an indi-
havior against a standard (e.g., Kagan, 1984; M. Lewis, viduals concerns and events in the world (Withering-
1998). From a dynamical systems perspective, these ton, 2003). For example, many different concrete
factors are intrinsic components of the emotion system events can call forth the emotion of anger: the stubbing
in that they become organized in specific configurations of a toe, a traffic jam, or misplacing ones keys. For the
during emotion episodes. functionalist, what unites all of these events is their
When emotion is viewed as self-organization rather serving as obstacles to goal attainment. Similarly,
than organization from without, it also becomes clear many different actions can be in the service of the
that development may involve components unique to the emotion of anger: striking out at others, stonewalling,
emotion system such as appraisal. Appraisal, unlike ob- or finding an alternate route to work. What unites all of
jective self-awareness or recall memory, is an emotion- these actions is the function they can servethe re-
specific form of cognition, an evaluation of events by an moval of an obstacle.
individuals goals and concerns. As such, appraisal is a The functionalist thus adopts an abstract level of ex-
cognitive-motivational process rather than a purely cog- planation for emotion and its development that is
nitive one (Barrett & Campos, 1987; Mascolo & Fis- grounded in the particularities of a persons real-time
cher, 1995). The concept of appraisal again highlights encounters with the environment but that extracts from
the importance of viewing the components of emotion in these particulars a set of general functional relations. In
relation to one another. Causality in the emotion system this way, the functionalist approach provides an invalu-
is multiply determined by the relation of many compo- able interpretive framework for understanding the emo-
nents. Thus, environmental events do not cause the de- tion process in all of its complexity (Witherington,
velopment of emotion independent of an individuals 2003). However, the question of how specific emotion
concerns and goals and vice versa. Similarly, evalua- actions emerge during specific emotion episodes re-
tions of events do not cause the development of emotion mains largely unaddressed by the functionalist frame-
independent of the individuals action repertoire, their work. Dynamic systems approaches to the study of
goals and concerns, and so on. All of the components emotional development complement the functionalist
that comprise the emotion system thus assume formative approach by attempting to address this question (e.g.,
significance in understanding stability and transition in Camras, 1992; Fogel et al., 1992; Lewis & Granic,
emotional development (Fogel et al., 1992). 2000). As earlier described, both the functionalist ap-
As an organizational framework, the functionalist proach and the dynamic systems approach view emotion
approach also provides a useful heuristic for integrat- as a self-organizing, multicomponential system, the pat-
ing our understanding of the relations that exist among terning of which is multiply determined such that no one
the components of the emotion system. The functional- component is any more primary in the emergence of pat-
ist approach adopts as its central level of analysis the tern than any other. But whereas the functionalist ap-
goal-mediated relation between person and environ- proach sets its sights on providing a meaningful
ment and identifies certain commonalities or themes in organizational framework for interpreting person-envi-
significant person-environment transactions (Barrett & ronment transactions, the dynamic systems approach
Campos, 1987; Campos et al., 1994). What any given takes as its charge the explanation of the emotion pro-
person is doing in a particular context can be organized cess at the more specific level of real-time action in con-
by a set of abstracted functional relationswhat text. For example, with respect to smiling, Dickson,
Lazarus (1991) has termed core relational themes Walker, and Fogel (1997) have shown how the fine-
that hold for all potential person-environment transac- grained morphology of infant smiling patterns varies in
tions, such as trying to overcome an obstacle to obtain accordance with the particular form of play in which in-
a goal or trying to avoid a threat to ones well-being fants and parents are engaged. When play involved book
and safety. We have already discussed these general reading, 12-month-olds predominantly displayed smiles
themes in the context of emotion families and rela- consisting only of lip corner raises, but when parent-in-
tional action tendencies. To organize the enormous be- fant interactions turned to physical play, infant smiling
havioral and contextual variability that characterize most characteristically involved opened mouth smiling,
the emotion process in real-time person-environment and when parents playfully vocalized with their infants,
A Conceptual Framework for Feeling and Emotion 235

infant smiling typically included both lip corner raises thal & Campos, 1990; Saarni, Mumme, & Campos,
and contraction of the orbicularis oculi muscles sur- 1998). For example, experience with self-produced loco-
rounding the eyes (termed the Duchenne smile after the motion, either through crawling or through the use of a
nineteenth-century French neurologist). Furthermore, walker, gives rise to the phenomenon of wariness of
Duchenne and non-Duchenne smilessmiles not involv- heights (Campos, Bertenthal, & Kermoian, 1992; Cam-
ing orbicularis oculi contractionmay grade into one pos et al., 1978). Parents also report major increases in
another, reflecting a quantitative dimension of positive their infants displays of anger and temper tantrums fol-
engagement. In a longitudinal sample of 1- to 6-month- lowing crawling onset (Campos, Kermoian, & Zum-
olds observed during play with their mothers, bahlen, 1992). Furthermore, the emergence of crawling
Messinger, Fogel, and Dickson (1999) found that non- affects the whole socioemotional climate in which the in-
Duchene smiles typically preceded in close temporal fant resides. Parents of crawling infants embark on new
proximity Duchenne smiles and that this relational cor- forms of emotional communication with their infants
respondence between the two forms did not vary across (Campos et al., 1992; Zumbahlen & Crawley, 1996).
the age range studied. These findings reveal how inti- Once infants begin to crawl, parents direct much more
mately involved contextual factors are in the formation positive affect toward their infants in the context of ex-
of emotion-related actions during real-time parent-in- ploration and the discovery of new events and situations.
fant interactions. Current applications of dynamic sys- At the same time, parents begin to regard their infants as
tems principles to emotional development, however, are more sophisticated and intentional, assigning them more
not specific to emotion; rather, the same principles responsibility for their actions. This change, coupled with
apply to any action-in-context, emotional or otherwise. the increased chance for a mobile infant to encounter dan-
In the absence of an organizational framework like that gerous situations, produces a substantial increase in
provided by the functionalist approach, dynamic sys- parental targeting of fear and anger to their infants once
tems approaches have yet to offer specific insight into crawling begins.
emotion itself as a content domain. Thus, the contribu- Like crawling, infant walking has long been regarded
tion of dynamic systems approaches to the study of emo- as functionally related to infant emotional development
tional development as emotional development remains (Mahler, Pine, & Bergman, 1975; Spitz, 1965). Work by
largely unexplored. Biringen, Emde, Campos, and Appelbaum (1995) pro-
vides preliminary support for such a relation. Infants
Change in Person-Environment Relations and and their mothers in the study showed increased ten-
Emotional Development dencies to test each others wills and engage in open
Given the importance of person-environment relations confrontations once the infants began to walk indepen-
in the generation of emotion, it should come as no sur- dently. Infants acted more willful and defiant, and
prise that factors that alter the relation of person to en- mothers viewed their infants in much more emotionally
vironment have important consequences for emotional negative terms following walking onset. Evidence,
development (Witherington et al., 2001). In the period therefore, suggests that motor milestones such as walk-
of infancy, motor achievements such as visually guided ing and crawling fundamentally alter the infant, her so-
reaching, crawling, and walking fundamentally alter the cial world, and her relation to that world and in the
way infants psychologically engage the world and are process reorganize her emotional life and the emotional
widely regarded as important points of transition in cog- climate in which she lives.
nitive and perceptual development. Treating emotion as
a multicomponent system suggests that our investiga- Culture, Emotion, and Emotional Development
tions of emotional development should target such points
of motoric transition as well as pervasive ecological Our attempts to understand how culture affects emotion
changes, such as entering preschool. Developments in a and emotional development have changed considerably
variety of domains result in changing the relation be- in the past 30 years. In the 1970s and 1980s, researchers
tween the person and the world and thus lead to impor- were mostly concerned with universals in emotion ex-
tant changes in emotion. pression. The search for universals generated impressive
Considerable evidence supports a link between experi- evidence on the similarity of recognition of facial ex-
ence with crawling and emotional development (Berten- pressions by preliterate tribes (Ekman, 1973; Ekman,
236 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969), and judges in both Western profound direct and indirect effects on the preverbal
and non-Western countries (Izard, 1972). In turn, this child. The direct effects result from diet, housing, and
evidence led to the widespread use of facial expressions the material and physical implements of the culture that
as the preferred indices of emotional states, and moti- are used in child rearing. The indirect effects are largely
vated the emotion revolution of the 1970s and 1980s. mediated by two factors: (1) the physical /social context
The apparent universality of recognition of facial ex- in which the infant is raised, and (2) the exposure of the
pression also led to studies on the elicitation of facial ex- child to the characteristic behavior patterns and nonver-
pression patterns of anger and fear in infants of different bal communication strategies of members of that culture
cultures (Camras, Oster, Campos, & Bakeman, 2003; (Gordon, 1989). So, subtle yet powerful are these direct
Camras, Oster, Campos, Miyake, & Bradshaw, 1992; and indirect effects that the infant can be said to be ac-
Camras et al. 1998) and the development of methods of culturated beginning at birth and maybe even before
facial expression measurement based on anatomical cri- (Tronick & Morelli, 1991).
teria and judgments of emotion by coders (Izard &
Dougherty, 1982; Oster, 1995). Although many criti- Parental Practices. Although the demand for pro-
cisms have been leveled at research on universality of vision and protection of infants and meeting their needs
recognition (Fridlund, 1994; Russell, 1994, 1995), they must be universal, the way in which those needs are de-
have on the whole not proven entirely convincing fined and met varies enormously. One way that culture
(Ekman, 1995; Izard, 1995). As a result, the search for influences the infant is through the mothers selection
universals continues in cross-cultural studies of patterns of interventions for regulating social signaling, includ-
of appraisal (Mesquita & Frijda, 1992), speculations ing the babys crying and struggling. For this reason,
about child-rearing functions (Trevarthen, 1988), and swaddling methods have received a great deal of atten-
attributional biases (Morris & Peng, 1994). tion from anthropologists. They have discussed how in
Recently, the study of culture and emotions has Middle Eastern societies swaddling facilitates sleep and
broadened considerably beyond the issue of universality transport (Whiting, 1981), soothes the child, and per-
to the role of culture in the generation, manifestation, mits the mother to work nearby in kibbutzim (Bloch,
and regulation of emotion (DAndrade, 1984; Kitayama 1966), maximizes proximity between mother and child,
& Markus, 1994; Lazarus, 1991). Because a complete facilitates responsiveness to the childs social signals,
review of culture and emotion is beyond the scope of such as in the Navaho nation (Chisholm, 1983, 1989),
this chapter, we will limit ourselves to an illustration of and brings about desirable habituation and autonomic
how emotion communication accompanies and helps to regulation in response to stimuli in noisy environments
inculcate cultural values, affects pre- and perinatal (Landers, 1989). In the United States, by contrast,
emotionality, determines the types of events to which an swaddling has been unpopular largely because it re-
infant or child is exposed, and creates the emotional cli- stricts freedom of movement (Lipton, Steinschneider, &
mate in which a person is immersed. Richmond, 1965) and possibly produces undesired yet
distinctive effects on the formation of characteristic
What Is Culture and Does Culture Influence Infants? emotional dispositions (Mead, 1954), some of which,
The concept of culture is rarely defined. For our pur- such as passivity, are not valued in the United States
poses, culture refers to a set of traditional, explicit and (Chisholm, 1989).
implicit beliefs, values, actions, and material environ- Another cultural variation in parenting practice evi-
ments that are transmitted by language, symbol, and be- dent even in the neonatal period is that of co-sleeping.
havior in an enduring and interacting group of people. Co-sleeping has been proposed as a socialization mech-
Because of the centrality of symbols, language, and val- anism that fosters attachment throughout life by creat-
ues for culture, most studies of culture and emotion deal ing a powerful motivation to remain close to the parent
with adults, and especially the language of adults (Abbott, 1992). Although sleeping in separate beds and
(Wierzbicka, 1992). Infants and children with minimal separate rooms is the norm in the United States, data
language skills are generally assumed to be beyond the collected in eastern Kentucky exemplifies the wide-
pale of symbolic influence (Winn, Tronick, & Morelli, spread regional variation that can occur in co-sleeping
1989). However, symbols, language, and values can have (Abbott, 1992). Co-sleeping occurred across all social
A Conceptual Framework for Feeling and Emotion 237

classes in eastern Kentucky, but was less common the attachment literature, there is evidence that infants
among the college educated. Interview data suggested from northern Germany show a preponderance of appar-
that co-sleeping did facilitate greater interdependence ent avoidant patterns of attachment (Grossmann, Gross-
in the family and fostered close emotional ties early in mann, Huber, & Wartner, 1981). By contrast, in Japan,
life. Findings such as these contradict widespread be- there is a preponderance of apparently ambivalent and
liefs that the effects of co-sleeping are uniformly nega- hard-to-soothe infants (Miyake, Chen, & Campos,
tive (see discussion in Morelli, Rogoff, Oppenheim, & 1985). In the kibbutzim in Israel, still another pattern of
Goldsmith, 1992). behavior is shown: Infants are extremely upset by the
Physical activity and infant positioning are other ex- entry of strangers in the attachment testing situation.
amples of parenting practices related to emotional devel- What accounts for such different patterns of behavior
opment and showing considerable variation across (Sagi, Lamb, Lewkowicz, Shoham, Dvir, & Estes, 1985)?
cultures. Compared to Americans, Gusii infants are ex- Why do children in three different areas of the world
posed to more light tossing and vigorous handling. Provi- react to the same events in such dramatically different
sion of such vigorous stimulation has been proposed to ways? One interpretation is that the value system of dif-
explain how Gusii infants overcome fear by 3 to 4 months ferent cultures affects what events infants are exposed to
of age (Keefer, Dixon, Tronick, & Brazelton, 1991). and thus to what events they become emotionally respon-
A traumatic influence with potentially long-lasting sive. In northern Germany, for example, infants are fre-
consequences for the newborn is circumcision. Circum- quently left alone outside of stores or supermarkets or in
cision is a painful procedure often conducted without the home while the mother steps out briefly. The pattern
anesthesia or analgesia. It may well form the nucleus of of exposure (the adaptation level) to being alone renders
disturbing and enduring memories for pain. Moreover, maternal separations in attachment testing not a very
cultural variations exist in the circumcision procedure: great departure from that to which the infant is accus-
What was at one time a religious ceremony conducted in tomed. As a result, infants with such a background may
an intimate family gathering has become routine med- show little or no upset on a brief maternal separation and
ical practice involving medical personnel with the fam- have little reason to give a strong response to the mother
ily excluded. The emotional climate provided to the on reunion. Not surprisingly, 49% of infants tested in the
infant during the circumcision ritual in the home (the Ainsworth Strange Situation in Germany show the A
bris) is vastly different from that provided on a plastic pattern of not directing much attention to the reentry of
restraint board in a hospital nursery and may also influ- the mother.
ence the infants memories for pain. In addition, in reli- In Japan, there is a very different value systemone
gious ceremonies, the circumcised newborn is often in which the mother desires very close proximity to her
given small amounts of sweetened water to drink. Such child. In Japan, babysitting is rare, and when it occurs, it
oral stimulation may help soothe the infants pain reac- is usually done by the grandparents. Accordingly, Japa-
tions, in the same manner that Blass and Ciaramitaro nese infants have very few experiences with separation
(1994) have reported that sucrose does for other painful from the mother. As a result, when the mother leaves the
procedures. infant alone or with a stranger in the attachment test, the
separation is extremely discrepant from the infants past
The Significance of Exposure to Events. Culture experience. As a result, the infant shows considerable
determines the types of events to which the child is ex- upset, and it is thus no surprise that the infant is hard to
posed. Emotional reactions are determined not only by console after experiencing intense distress on separa-
transactions taking place in the present but also by the tion. The difficulty in consoling the child results in clas-
history of prior encounters with similar events in the sifying the child as a C infant.
past. It is as if an adaptation level of experience is built In the kibbutzim in Israel, security measures and the
up, and depending on the discrepancy of an event from history of unexpected terrorist attacks make for a strong
that adaptation level, the child will show intense, moder- form of xenophobia. Strangers are looked on askance,
ate, or weak emotional reactions. and they are typically not allowed to approach infants.
This principle of adaptation level is well exemplified Because infants are very sensitive to the emotional com-
in the literature on culture and attachment patterns. In munication of significant others by 12 months of age,
238 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

they have become sensitized to be wary of strangers mates. In some cultures, loudness and extremes of
themselves. As a result, when a stranger enters the room gesticulation are encouraged or tolerated; in others,
and initiates contact or approach to the child, the infant quiet and peaceful expression is the expectation. Such
is set to become intensely fearful. Interestingly, in urban emotional climate may inf luence the emotional reac-
Israel, where the xenophobia is usually much less evi- tion of infants, children, and adults quite profoundly
dent, infants do not show such intense negative reactions (Briggs, 1970).
to strangers. The adaptation level of the kind of reaction Consider that vocal expression of affect is very per-
that significant others typically give to the infants deter- vasive and quite closely linked to the communication
mines the intensity level of their negative responses. of discrete emotion (Scherer, 1986). It is now well
Much xenophobia in caregivers results in high levels of known that the fetus can hear sounds in the womb
stranger distress; less xenophobia results in considerably from the seventh gestational month onward. As a re-
lower levels. In sum, the value system of each culture sult of the transmission of sounds through the amniotic
(expectations of independence in northern Germany, de- fluid (DeCasper & Fifer, 1980; DeCasper, Lecanuet,
sire for extreme proximity in Japan, and the need to pro- Busnel, Granier-Deferre, & Maugeais, 1994; Fifer &
tect the community in Israeli kibbutzim) leads to Moon, 1995), the unborn infant can acquire consider-
different levels of experience against which new experi- able experience about patterns and intensities of vo-
ences are compared. The culture thus determines both calic emotional communication. Just as the newborn
exposure to events and the context for differential emo- can identify his or her mothers speech within 3 days
tional reactions. of birth, it is possible that the newborn can come into
Other examples of how exposure and values interact the world with built-in expectations of what the typi-
to influence emotional-expressive behavior can be found cal emotional climate is in the society in which he or
in patterns of eye contact: It is usually encouraged in she is born.
Western culture, where looking into a babys eyes forms In Japan, the emotional climate is one of soft vocal-
a window into the soul. However, in certain African izations, few verbalizations, and much gentle stroking
societies, eye contact is generally discouraged. Certain of the infant (Miyake, Campos, Kagan, & Bradshaw,
tribes believe that eye contact allows another person to 1986). This pattern of softness and low frequency and
cast an evil eye on the infant. As a result, infants are volume of speech has been attributed to the rice paper
often kept in dark corners of the living hut, and held in a walls of the typical Japanese household, together with
manner that minimizes the chance of eye contact. The the Japanese value for harmony and tranquility in the
end result for the infant is a cool, subdued demeanor home. To attain these cultural goals of harmony, moth-
(LeVine et al., 1994). ers are charged with the responsibility of keeping vol-
Still another instance of culture determining what an ume of communication low and to keep the infants
infant is exposed to stems from the work with the Efe crying to a minimum. Thus, Japanese mothers communi-
in central Africa on multiple mothering (e.g., Tronick, cate with their infants much more by touch and less by
Morelli, & Ivey, 1992). Because maternal mortality is vocalization than do American mothers.
extremely high among the Efe, the culture attempts to Emotional climate is thus a crucial means by which
compensate for the negative consequences of maternal culture affects emotion. It is known that in cultures dif-
loss by fostering multiple caregiving. Infants are thus fering emotional climates have important effects on
often passed from one person to another, and the infant children. Thus, parental quarreling and fighting can re-
becomes accustomed to being handled by more than sult in infants and children of that family becoming mal-
one caregiver. As a result, the loss of the mother be- adjusted and insecure (Davies & Cummings, 1994).
comes much less traumatic for Efe infants than for in- In concluding this discussion of culture and emotion,
fants elsewhere. the emphasis in this chapter should be clear: Emotions are
relational and functional (in that they serve a purpose),
Culture and Emotional Climate. Emotional they are embedded in social communicative relations,
climate refers to the characteristic patterning and in- they are flexibly responsive to context, and they link our
tensity of verbal and nonverbal emotional communi- actions with our goals. Consistent with the preceding ma-
cation that is within earshot and eyeshot of an terial on culture and emotional development and with a
audience. Cultures often differ in such emotional cli- functionalist approach, in the remaining sections of the
The Development of Emotional Communication in Early Life 239

chapter we take a systems approach to emotional commu- his studies of avian communication. He distinguished
nication as multichannel (or multibehavioral), which in- between the message and the meaning of a communica-
cludes facial expression, vocal quality, gesture, touch, eye tive signal. According to Smith, message refers to the
contact, interpersonal distance, and so forth (Scheflen, invariant information encoded in the environmental dis-
1974). With increased exposure and experience, young play that comprises a signal, while the meaning of the
childrens emotional-expressive behavior begins to resem- message refers to the signals predictive value about the
ble the normative emotional communicative patterns, as behavior of the communicator within a particular situa-
prescribed by the culture in which they live. Social refer- tional context.
encing, which is reviewed in the next section, is a key Put another way, the message is that which can be
interactive process for facilitating this learning of emo- physically described in the action of a communicator
tional meaningfulness. what can be put on a photograph or an audiotape. The
In addition, a systems approach to communication is meaning is what the message forecasts about the future
very useful for understanding the kinds of emotional- behavior of the communicatorwhose smile can predict
social phenomena that develop in the preschool and ele- approach, scornful rejection, manifestations of pity, or a
mentary school years, which are discussed in later simple farewell. The point in Smiths distinction that is
sections. These phenomena include self presentation so crucial for the study of communication between par-
strategies, empathy-mediated prosocial behavior, emo- ent and infant is that one cannot predict meaning from a
tion management, and coping strategies, among others. message with very much accuracy.
Systems communication theorists (e.g., Watzlawick, Smith emphasized that messages are interpreted in
Beavin, & Jackson, 1967) emphasize further that what their context for many reasons. These contextual factors
may be most important about communication is its in- can change the meaning of a message, further weakening
volvement in the regulation of relationships, and their the apparent close relation between message and mean-
notion of metacommunication describes this regulatory ing we so often take for granted. Sometimes, features of
function: A message, conveyed by nonverbal behavior, the context enter into the perception of the message, and
communicates how the content of what is said should be cause the impact of messages on recipients to be very
understood. In short, communication about communi- different than what one would expect. Take the mother
cation is intended to influence us, and such communi- smiling at her infant as an example. That smile (a mes-
cations are typically emotion laden. We turn next to a sage), has a different meaning and will have a differen-
discussion of the early development of emotional com- tial impact on the infant, depending on whether the
munication, emphasizing social referencing, not only mother smiles when the infant is distressed (which can
because it is a particularly well-investigated emotional imply misattunement by the mother with the infants
communication process, but also because it illustrates state) or if the mother smiles while the infant is playing
this metacommunicative function of relationship with the mother, which would be an expectable part of a
regulation. social transaction. Because our research methods in-
volve manipulating the message, they are often not
suited to identifying what the meaning is for the child to
the extent that there is a cleavage between message and
THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONAL meaning. To maximize the chances that we are studying
COMMUNICATION IN EARLY LIFE meaning in emotional communication, we therefore con-
sider emotion communication to have occurred only
In previous sections, we have discussed the evocation of when a manipulation is shown to have an emotional im-
emotions in response to alterations in some significant pact. Studies that only address discrimination of the dis-
aspect of the infants relations in the environment. We play by infants (i.e., habituation studies) will not be
now discuss how emotions provide signals indicating emphasized here.
such a relational change and how such signals thereby In brief, we consider emotional communication to
can produce an effect on the infant. occur whenever one person exhibits emotional behavior
It proves surprisingly difficult to study emotional and another person witnesses and is affected by that
communication in early life. The difficulty stems from a emotional behavior. We look for three components in a
point made over 30 years ago by John Smith (1977) in thorough study of emotional communication: (1) on the
240 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

input side, registration of the emotion signal by the per- Phase 1 (Prenatal to 6 Weeks): Initial
ceiver, and (2) on the output side, a valenced response to Reactions to Emotion Signals
that signal manifested in the recipients own expressive
or instrumental activity, and (3) in between, some degree In the neonatal period that extends from birth to 4 to 6
of appraisal of the input such that the appraisal may weeks, rudimentary valenced responses to emotional
change both the significance of the input and the nature messages clearly exist as evidenced by two findings.
of the emotional response. Registration refers to the per- First, as a consequence of prenatal exposure, newborns
ception of an expression of emotion (or some component respond to the valence of speech prosody produced in
of that expression) and includes the ability to distinguish their mothers native language but not in nonmaternal
among expressions of different emotions. (Habituation languages (Mastropieri & Turkewitz, 1999). Second,
and preferential looking studies focus on the registration newborns appear capable of responding with cries and
of emotion signals.) A valenced response is one that can negative facial expressions to the cries of another
be reasonably interpreted as reflecting the content of the neonate (Dondi, Simion, & Caltran, 1999).
internal appraisal and motivation processes on the part
of the infant such as approach or avoidance, smiling or Neonates Responsiveness to Emotion Prosody
frowning. Valenced responses, whether expressive or in- Because the newborns behavioral repertoire is ex-
strumental, can be diffuse (oriented at no particular tremely limited, garnering evidence for the infants re-
event in the world) or targeted at some specific object in sponsiveness to emotional expression presents a major
the world or at ones own actions. We also emphasize challenge to researchers. In an ingenious study, Mas-
that we use the term appraisal broadly without implying tropieri and Turkewitz (1999) examined neonates eye
the necessary involvement of higher-order cognitive widening in reaction to speech produced in either the
processes. As we review empirical findings, one concern mothers native language or in a novel language using
that we raise is about the rush that sometimes exists to neutral, happy, sad, or angry prosody. Increased eye
attribute to the infant understandings of meanings when widening was found in response to happy prosody, but
these three criteria have not been met. only for speech produced in the maternal language.
We delineate four phases of increasingly complex This pattern of results suggest that early discrimination
emotional communication between an adult and the in- of vocalic emotion is not an innate capacity, but instead
fant. Phase 1 (prenatal to 6 weeks) describes the infants appears to be based on prenatal experience with a spe-
initial valenced reactions to emotion signals. Phase 2 (6 cific language. Furthermore, there is as yet no evidence
weeks to 7 to 9 months) covers the developmental period for discrimination beyond the distinction between posi-
preceding the advent of referentiality (i.e., understand- tive and neutral or negative vocalic emotion.
ing that a communicative signal may refer to some exter-
nal aspect of the environment). Phase 3 (9 months to 18 Neonatal Crying Contagion. Although to date no
months/2 years) focuses on the development of referen- one has demonstrated discrimination among vocalic
tial emotion communication, behavioral regulation (i.e., expressions of different discrete negative emotions
where the expressive and instrumental behaviors of the (e.g., anger, sadness, fear), there is strong evidence for
child are affected by the others emotional expressions), a valenced response to crying. The contagious crying
and retention of the emotion signals impact over pro- phenomenon has been documented repeatedly, and con-
gressively longer durations. Phase 4 (18 months/2 years stitutes a remarkable demonstration of the presence of
and beyond) is marked by the development of what the emotional communication in the neonatal period (e.g.,
literature calls self-conscious emotions, but which, fol- Martin & Clark, 1982; Sagi & Hoffman, 1976; Simner,
lowing Watson (personal communication, April 1999), 1971). Newborns tend to cry in response to the cries of
we call other-conscious emotions because they de- another newborn, though not those of an older infant, a
pend on the childs detecting the expressive and instru- chimpanzee, or white noise. In a methodologically rig-
mental reactions of others to his or her own behaviors. orous replication of earlier studies, Dondi et al. (1999)
We also hypothesize that during this period, marked im- found that neonates decreased their rate of sucking and
provements take place in the childs comprehension of also showed increased facial distress to cries of unfa-
the different meanings carried out by different nega- miliar infants. Because their expressions of distress
tive emotional messages such as fear versus anger. were demonstrated in a different modality than the dis-
The Development of Emotional Communication in Early Life 241

tress stimulus itself (viz., the cry of the other neonate), proach. She reported that 5-month-olds smiled more in
newborns apparently may not have been merely mim- response to infant-directed messages specifying ap-
icking the vocal stimulus, but rather truly reacting to proval than to messages specifying prohibition irre-
the meaning of the cry of the other child. Interestingly, spective of whether the message was produced in
Martin and Clark (1982) reported two fascinating phe- English or in an unfamiliar language (German or Ital-
nomena: (1) that recordings of the babies own cries re- ian). Negative affect was more likely to occur in re-
sulted in some reduction of their own cries, and (2) that sponse to prohibitions. Fernalds investigation offers a
the neonatal crying contagion dropped out by 5 months strong demonstration that by 5 months of age, infants
of age. Both phenomena pose challenges for future re- are able to discriminate the emotional valence of
search because they require replication and, if repli- acoustic messages and to transform differential mes-
cated, careful study of the processes involved in each. sage content into congruent behavioral reactions that
are either appropriately positive or negative. The issue
Phase 2 (6 Weeks to 9 Months): Pre-Referential of whether infants can respond at this age to more spe-
Communication cific emotional meanings (e.g., fear versus anger) at
this age has not been addressed.
During this period, the infant can engage in synchro- A number of other studies investigate infants differ-
nous dyadic interaction with the caregiver. This phe- entially valenced responses in dyadic setting. Numer-
nomenon indicates some limited ability to apprehend ous investigations (e.g., Cohn & Tronick, 1988;
the caregivers emotional valence, understand when Feldman, Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999; Field, Healy,
the caregiver is targeting her or his emotion toward the Goldstein, & Guthertz, 1990; Jaffe, Beebe, Feldstein,
infant, and then align her or his own emotional valence Crown, & Jasnow, 2001; Moore & Calkins, 2004) have
and behavior to be congruent with that exhibited by the demonstrated what many call interactional syn-
caregiver. Two other phenomena appear to be well es- chrony, but which we conservatively call expressive
tablished about emotional communication in this time coincidencecontingencies in the timing of positive
period. First, infants respond differentially to the va- or negative expressions during face-to-face interactions
lence of mothers vocal contours (Fernald, 1993). Sec- between mothers and infants even younger than 3
ond, infants discriminate facial expressions as stimulus months of age. More specific matching (what we pre-
patterns but show no convincing evidence of compre- fer to call co-occurrence) in the level of positive or
hending their specific emotion meanings. Some investi- negative affect has also been shown (Feldman et al.,
gators have obtained data that they interpret as 1999; Tronick & Cohn, 1989; Weinberg, Tronick, Cohn,
demonstrating more substantial communication com- & Olson, 1999).
petenciesa relatively full comprehension of the rich Many of these studies claim to have demonstrated a
emotional messages generated by the caregiver. While process of direct and unmediated emotion contagion
these interpretations are plausible, the research para- (Haviland & Lelwica, 1987; Stern, 1985; Trevarthen
digms employed are not suitable to yield the unambigu- & Hubley, 1978). However, in our own view, what ap-
ous inferences that have been drawn from them. pears to be direct mutual mirroring between mother
and child may instead result from the operation of two
Evidence for Rudimentary Emotional Exchange other powerful and rudimentary determinants of emo-
and Its Constituents tioncontingency and agency (or lack thereof ). Lewis
As in the previous period, the strongest evidence of in- and his colleagues (e.g., Lewis, Hitchcock, & Sullivan,
fants responsiveness to emotional signals comes from 2004), following the pioneering work of J. S. Watson
studies using acoustic stimuli. The responsiveness to (1972), have unequivocally shown that infants as
vocalic communication markedly exceeds that evi- young as 2 months of age smile when their actions pro-
denced in the neonatal period, exhibiting differentially duce a contingent effect on the world and evidence
valenced responses to the valence of adults emotional distress when a previously operative contingency fails.
signals. For example, in a study noteworthy for its Studies of affective synchrony may sometimes con-
methodological rigor and conceptual richness, Fernald found contingency and its failure with direct affective
(1993) presented 5-month-old infants with acoustic matching. Thus when contingent reciprocal smiling
stimuli specifying prohibition or encouragement of ap- takes place, it might produce similar emotional
242 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

responses in mother and baby. However, such shared neutral or sad expression. This finding suggests that in-
emotion need not depend on either direct contagion or fants affinity/preference for happy expressions can
(alternatively) on the infants in-depth comprehension overcome the negative emotional impact of contingency
of the emotional meaning of the smile. A simpler ex- disruption that normally occurs in the still-face proce-
planation is that the infant smiles contingently at the dure, although the effect of the smile is not strong. Al-
mother not because the baby sees a smile, but because ternatively, infants may find the happy still-face to be
the baby notices a contingent reaction that happens to less discrepant from their normal experience because
be a smile. they are exposed to smiles more often than neutral or
In some of the broadest claims in this literature, re- sad expressions in the course of their day-to-day social
searchers propose that processes of synchrony and interactions. Interestingly, Rochat et al. (2002) found
matching may contribute to the development of emotion that 4- and 6-month-old infants responded equivalently
regulation (Feldman et al., 1999; Moore, Cohn, & to still-face disruptions involving happy, neutral, or sad
Campbell, 2001) and attachment (Jaffe et al., 2001). For expressions. However, in a study that manipulated moth-
example, mothers positive emotion signals may induce ers sequencing of happy or sad expressive behavior dur-
a more positive response in their infants and may also ing the course of nondisrupted social interaction,
contribute to the development of a secure attachment re- DEntremont and Muir (1999) found that 5-month-old
lationship. This conclusion has obvious intuitive and infants smiled more to happy than to sad facial expres-
emotional appeal, in that it reflects the notion that emo- sions (irrespective of the presence or absence of the
tional bonds are built on shared evaluations and experi- voice). Taken together, these findings suggest that older
ences. However, we think it critically important to also babies perceive differences among emotional expres-
emphasize that not all maternal smiles directed at the in- sions but their common response to happy and sad still-
fant are positive in their impact: Maternal responses face poses may have been determined by the disruption
must be context sensitive rather than always synchro- of contingency that was equivalent across the expression
nous. For example, Stern (1974) reports that if the infant conditions. This again illustrates the important point
is attempting to terminate an interaction and the mother that emotion communication, including the impact of the
continues to force her smiling presence into the infants signal on the recipient, will depend in part on contextual
visual field (effectively making a demand that the infant featuresin this case, the maintenance and disruption
suppress her or his negative affect and return the smile), of previously learned contingencies. The flow of contin-
then such maternal insensitivity may result in interac- gencies may be as or more important in determining the
tional failure. The same may be said if the mothers first infants emotional responses than the flow of emotional
response to the infants crying is to smile, rather than to contents between persons.
present the infant first with sympathy, which the mother
then transforms into a smile, paced with the infant Weaknesses in the Evidence for Discrimination of
(Holodinski & Friedlmeier, in press). If such failures are Discrete Emotional Signals
chronic, they may contribute to the etiology of psycho- The evidence reviewed earlier considered infants dis-
pathology (Gergely & Watson, 1996, 1999; Holodinski crimination of and responsiveness to the valence of
& Freidlmeier, in press). emotional signals. Another substantial body of research
The still-face paradigm provides another interesting has focused on characterizing infants abilities in this
context in which to appreciate the ambiguities of study- age range to discriminate among discrete emotional sig-
ing emotion communication in young infants. In the nals. Much of this research has employed experimental
still-face procedure, normal face-to-face infant-adult in- procedures (i.e., habituation and preferential looking
teraction is disrupted by an episode in which the adult procedures), originally designed to investigate the dis-
ceases all movement and thus fails to respond to the in- crimination of one display from a second, when those
fants signals. The still-face effect rests primarily on two displays differ in only one dimension or feature, for
the cessation of facial rather than vocal signals (Striano example, in pitch, hue, size, numerosity, and so forth. In
& Bertin, 2004). Furthermore, differential effects of our view, the application of these techniques to the
positive versus negative or neutral expressions also have problem of differential perception of emotional signals
been reported. Rochat, Striano, and Blatt (2002) showed is problematic. The expressive stimuli presented in
that 2-month-old infants maintained their smiling and these studies involve a complex set of features and these
gazing if the experimenter posed a smile rather than a studies fail to isolate those aspects of the stimuli to
The Development of Emotional Communication in Early Life 243

which infants are attending and responding. For exam- Studies purporting to demonstrate in postneonatal in-
ple, using a habituation procedure that employed sev- fants contagion not of crying but of quite specific emo-
eral models showing variations of each emotional tions also are subject to overinterpretation. For example,
expression, Serrano, Iglesias, and Loeches (1992, 1995) in a study of 10-week-old infants, Haviland and Lelwica
claim infants in this age period distinguish between and (1987) reported that babies mirrored happy and angry
respond differentially to static facial expressions of facial expressions in response to their mothers facial
happiness, sadness, anger, and fear on the basis of their and vocal displays, and exhibited nonspecific mouthing
emotion-defining features. However, the Serrano et al. movements in response to sad displays. While all these
studies, like many others, make these claims without findings are intriguing, we think it an overinterpretation
systematically isolating which stimulus features infants to claim (as do Haviland and Lelwica) that emotion con-
attend to and influence their behavior. tagion occurred in this study. In our view, to distinguish
Similar problems exist with studies claiming to emotion contagion from the superficial imitation of fa-
demonstrate that infants in this age range distinguish cial actions, infants must be observed to display clear in-
among multimodal expressions of emotion. For exam- dicators of the emotion in a form that differs from the
ple, Montague and Walker-Andrews (2002) employed a observed stimulus. The strong evidence needed to sup-
familiar infant game (peek-a-boo), reporting that 4- port such broad attributions has not been presented in
month-old infants distinguish between facial and vocal any study to date.
displays of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear. Yet, it
is hard to identify in this study the specific determi-
Phase 3 (9 Months to 18 Months): Behavioral
nants of infants differential responses, whether the
Regulation and Referential Communication
stimulus or the violation of some contingent relation
that the infant has come to expect, is the basis for their The infant undergoes a major set of cognitive, social,
findings. emotional, agentic, and perceptual changes in the age pe-
The problem of attributing to the infant a rich partic- riod that we are about to review. These changes have
ipation in emotional exchange is not restricted to dis- marked impact on the emotional communication of the
crimination studies using static displays of emotions but infant. The most significant change for our purposes is
extends as well to studies of dynamic emotion stimuli. the emergence of the infants ability to engage in refer-
Investigations utilizing the intermodal preference ential gestural communication (i.e., what it is that the
method (involving preferential looking toward a facial mother is emoting about). Prior to 8 to 10 months, in-
expression that corresponds to an emotion vocalization) fants typically do not show any reliable tendency to fol-
have reported that 14-week-olds can match facial and low the gaze or pointing gesture of the parent. By 9
vocal expressions for happiness, sadness, and anger months, infants begin to show such referential under-
when they are displayed by their mothers (although not standing, which becomes progressively more specific
when they are displayed by unfamiliar persons; Kahana- with the childs advancing age, and culminates in the
Kalman & Walker-Andrews, 2001; Montague & Walker- baby being able to identify the approximate coordinates
Andrews, 2002). Walker-Andrews (1997) has argued of where the experimenter or mother is looking or point-
that intermodal matching constitutes evidence for some ing (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984; Campos et al., 2000;
understanding of affective meaning and has labeled this Mumme, Bushnell, DiCorcia, & Lariviere, in press). The
phenomenon emotion recognition. However, because in- implications of this new ability is that the infant becomes
termodal matching may involve simple associative capable of engaging in what has been called a two-per-
mechanisms, we prefer to interpret these results as evi- son communication about a third event, becomes able to
dence for discrimination of one multimodal affective link quite precisely the target of the mothers or experi-
signal pattern from another, but we are skeptical that menters pointing and gaze, and increasingly becomes
these studies get at the issue of the infants recognition able to retain the emotional impact of prior emotional
of emotion meaning. As with studies involving only fa- messages (i.e., shows affective memory).
cial or only vocal emotion signals, these studies do not During this period, the infant also becomes able to
tell us what the physical basis for the infants discrimi- draw another persons attention to events of significance
native responding in multimodal displays, much less the to herself (i.e., the infant shows affective sharing). In
emotional meaning of the displays (in Smiths terms, as addition, the infant becomes increasingly able to retain
discussed previously). the affective impact of prior emotional signals (i.e.,
244 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

shows affective memory). The changes shown by the in- 12-month-old infants referenced their mother (i.e.,
fant during this age period enable her to imbue environ- looked toward her) when they reached a mid-level drop-
mental events with affective meaning as imposed by off on the visual cliff and most proceeded to cross if she
parents or others emotional messages. In some cases, displayed a facial expression of happiness or interest but
these emotion messages may even alter the valence of not of sadness, anger, or fear. The mid-level drop-off in-
infants reactions to events. However, during this age pe- duced a state of uncertainty in the infants and they
riod, the infant may still show major deficiencies in pro- therefore sought information from their mother to help
cessing certain aspects of the emotion message (e.g., the them determine whether to proceed across the cliff.
specific negative emotion indicated by a negatively va- Through their actions, infants demonstrated that they
lenced signal). As such deficiencies are overcome, the did more than merely register their mothers emotion
infant is prepared for a fourth stage in development of signals; they displayed a valenced response reflecting
emotional communication. an attempt to maintain or change their relation to the
environment.
Onset of Emotional Communication Involving
Environmental Objects Primacy of Facial versus Vocal Signals
Two studies clearly document how the infant by 8.5 A current controversy among investigators of behavioral
months of age becomes capable of reacting to the social regulation centers on the relative effectiveness of emo-
signals of the mother directed at a third event. In one tion signals produced in different modalities (e.g., facial
study (Boccia & Campos, 1989), the infants reaction to versus vocal). Because it is virtually impossible to en-
strangers was markedly affected by whether the mother sure that signal intensity is equated across modalities, at-
posed a stern or cheery greeting and facial expression tempts to make direct comparisons between facial and
when a stranger walked into the room. In the second vocal expressions may be misleading. Furthermore, the
study (Svedja, 1981), the mothers vocalization of the fact that vocal signals can be inescapably imposed on the
babys name followed by a nonsense phrase ( tat fob- infant more readily than facial signals may create a false
ble) resulted in cessation of the infants approach to a impression that vocal expressions are more effective be-
toy that was significantly longer when the vocalization havioral regulators. In reality, this differential effective-
was uttered in a fearful or angry manner than when it ness may sometimes be attributable to the differential
was uttered joyfully. These studies demonstrate that ref- registration of the signal by the infant. Empirical studies
erential communication occurs in infants as young as 8.5 of facial versus vocal signaling have produced mixed re-
months of age for facial and vocal expressions combined sults. Mumme, Fernald, and Herrera (1996) found that
and for vocal expressions produced alone. Regarding re- facial but not vocal expressions influenced infants be-
sponses to facial expressions produced alone, while sev- havioral reaction toward a toy, and Vaish and Striano
eral studies have demonstrated regulatory effects in (2004) found that infants responded more readily on the
12-month-olds (e.g., Camras & Sachs, 1991; Gunnar & visual cliff to maternal positive vocal signals than to
Stone, 1984; Klinnert, 1984; Zarbatany & Lamb, 1985), smiling. However, as cited earlier, other investigations
none has investigated infants at younger ages. In addi- show that infants do indeed alter their behavior in re-
tion, no studies to date have examined behavioral regula- sponse to facial expressions produced without accompa-
tion in response to emotion signals produced in either nying vocalizations. Some inconsistencies across studies
modality by babies younger than 8.5 months of age. may be due to differences in the emotions that are exam-
Thus, the precise age of onset for behavioral regulation ined and in the nature of the regulatory response ob-
with respect to environmental objects or events is not served (e.g., behavior toward the mother versus toward
currently known. the target object, response to the first delivery of the sig-
nal versus later trials). Thus, further research is neces-
Effects of Emotional Communication in Older Infants sary to identify both the signal and the situational
In contrast to younger infants, there has been consider- parameters that determine the specific effects of both
able research on older (10- to 14-month-old) infants be- facial and vocal signals of emotion on infants behavior.
havioral regulation in response to emotion signals. In an In addition, modalities other than facial expression and
early and powerful demonstration of this phenomenon, vocalization should be examined. For example, in a re-
Sorce, Emde, Campos, and Klinnert (1985) showed that cent study, Hertenstein and Campos (2001) showed that
The Development of Emotional Communication in Early Life 245

infants respond to maternal tactile behaviors that might dence leaves little doubt that infants respond to the
be interpreted as indicating a negative emotional re- meaning of these signals at the level of their positive or
sponse (i.e., gently squeezing the abdomen as the infant negative emotional valence. However, in contrast to ref-
reached toward a toy). erential specificity, the issue of affect specificity has
received relatively little attention.
Referential Specificity
Regarding distinctions among the negative emotions,
Referential specificity constitutes the notion that in- Sorce et al. (1985) have attempted to systematically
fants understand that an emotional expression is compare fear, anger, and sadness expressions in the
uniquely directed toward the object of the expressers same experimental procedure. In their study, almost no
attention rather than to other objects in the environment infants crossed the modified visual cliff when mothers
or to no particular object at all. Without referential displayed an expression of fear or anger, while approxi-
specificity, infants may misinterpret emotion signals, mately a third of the babies did proceed to cross in re-
for example, linking mothers disgust display directed, sponse to the sad expression. Although these findings
say, at an insect, to a nontargeted object, or even to the suggest that 12-month-old infants distinguish among
infant herself. Thus, referential specificity requires the different negative emotions, one possible interpretation
infant to identify more precisely the object of an emo- is that they distinguish on the basis of emotional inten-
tion signal and respond in an appropriately selective sity (i.e., degree of negativity) but do not understand the
manner. Several recent studies (e.g., Hertenstein & qualitatively different relational meanings and func-
Campos, 2004; Moses, Baldwin, Rosinsky, & Tidball, tional implications of fear versus anger versus sadness.
2001; Mumme & Fernald, 2003; Phillips, Wellman, & Another indication that infants in this phase of devel-
Spelke, 2002; Rapacholi, 1998) have investigated refer- opment may not be differentially responsive to discrete
ential specificity by comparing infants responses to negative emotional signals comes from a study by Bing-
targeted versus nontargeted objects. Such studies have ham, Campos, and Emde (1987). In this study, 13- to
found evidence for referential specificity in infants as 15-month-old infants encountered a doll whose arm ap-
young as 12 months of age. Referentially specific re- peared to break and fall off when the infant touched it.
sponding implies that the infants behavior reflects more At the same time, the experimenter uttered the nonsense
than merely the general induction of emotion that might phrase Tat fobble using a facial expression and vocal
affect behavior with respect to all objects and events en- tone appropriate for one of six basic emotions: fear, sad-
countered in the environment. Nonetheless, a general- ness, surprise, joy, disgust, or anger. Videotapes of the
ized emotional reaction and more specific referential procedure allowed for manipulation checks of the exper-
responding are not mutually exclusive processes. In- imenters emotion poses and examination of the infants
deed, a number of studies have analyzed infants own own expressive and instrumental responses. This study
emotional expressions in response to the adults emotion yielded highly significant differentiation of behavior
signals and produced evidence for an alteration of the along a hedonic tone dimension, such that negative emo-
infants own expressions of emotion as well as the tions elicited withdrawal behaviors and positive emo-
childs instrumental, voluntary behaviors (e.g., Boccia tions elicited greater duration of play with the doll than
& Campos, 1989; Mumme & Fernald, 2003). However, did the negative signals. However, infants did not re-
these effects are inconsistently found (e.g., Hertenstein spond differently in either expression or instrumental
& Campos, 2004) and the factors that determine behavior to the different negative emotions. This lack of
whether or not infants own emotional expressions are response differentiation took place despite the paradigm
altered have yet to be determined. affording the opportunity for the infant to respond dif-
ferently (e.g., showing nurturance to the doll or the ex-
Affect Specificity
perimenter in response to experimenter sadness versus
Affect specificity refers to the infants ability to make showing avoidance of either the doll or the experimenter
qualitative distinctions among emotions of the same va- in response to anger). The authors concluded that per-
lence (e.g., distinguish anger from fear). Most studies of haps infants at this age were not yet able to react differ-
behavioral regulation have compared reactions to posi- entially to emotional signals more specific than positive
tive versus negative emotional expressions (typically versus negative. They also cautioned for the need of fur-
happiness versus fear or disgust). The accumulated evi- ther research to validate their tentative conclusion about
246 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

the lack of differential behavioral regulation by discrete bals while the mother was located some distance away.
emotion signals. She reported that 11.5-month-old infants ( but not 9-
month-old infants) directed emotional expression to-
Retention of Valenced Effects of Emotional ward their mothers more often when their mothers were
Communication facing toward the infant rather than facing away. Like-
Another important issue in the study of behavioral regu- wise, Jones, Collins, and Hong (1991) found that 10-
lation is the duration of a signals effect on the behavior month-old infants produced anticipatory smiles (i.e.,
of the infant. If emotional expressions are to play a sig- smiles that were followed by looks toward their mother)
nificant role in social and emotional development, their more often when mothers were attending to their play
impact must extend beyond the immediate context of rather than reading a magazine. Similar results also were
occurrence. obtained by Striano and Rochat (2000) and Venezia,
Relatively few studies have examined the enduring Messinger, Thorpe, and Mundy (2004) for 10-month-old
effects of emotion communication. However, not sur- infants using a different emotion induction procedure.
prisingly, these have found the period of effectiveness Results of these studies converge to suggest that inten-
to increase with infant age. For example, Svejdas tional affect sharing by infants emerges at around the
study (1981; summarized in Campos, Thein, & Owen, same time as their understanding of referential speci-
2003) of vocal communication provided evidence of ficity in the emotion communications of others. Beyond
carryover effects across trials separated by 1-minute this, infants appear to be attempting to influence the re-
delays for 8.5-month-old infants. Examining older in- cipient by bringing affective events to her attention.
fants, Hertenstein and Campos (2004) found that 11-
month-olds showed differential behavioral regulation
(i.e., referential specificity) when the delay between Phase 4 (18 Months/2 Years and Beyond):
exposure to the signal and the infants opportunity to The Rise of Other-Conscious Emotions
respond was 3 minutes but not when it was extended to
60 minutes. In contrast, the effectiveness of the emo- In the next phase of the development of emotional com-
tion signal was retained across a 1-hour delay in 14- munication, we believe that two significant changes take
month-old babies. One study of behavioral regulation place in the infants reactions to emotional signals from
has also provided suggestive data indicating that there others. One is the establishment of differential expres-
may be systematic individual differences in infants sive and instrumental behavioral responses to different
retention of emotion signals. In an investigation of 10- emotion signals of the same valence (e.g., anger versus
and 15-month-olds, Bradshaw (1986; Campos et al., fear). The second is a major change in the infants con-
2003) found behavioral effects across a 25-minute strual of the two-person communication about a third
delay period for only a subset of participants. event leading to what are commonly called the self-con-
scious emotions (e.g., shame, guilt, pride), but which,
The Development of Affective Sharing by the Infant the reader will recall, we prefer to call other-conscious
While studies of behavioral regulation have largely fo- emotions to note the importance of the emotional reac-
cused on infants responses to adults affective expres- tions of others in their generation. In contrast to the pre-
sions, emotion communication is a bidirectional process vious age periods, we acknowledge that our description
in which both parties in a dyadic interaction may gener- of this phase includes considerablealbeit grounded
ate as well as receive emotion signals. Evidence for in- speculation. Therefore, several of our proposals regard-
fants deliberate targeting of their own emotional ing Phase 4 remain to be confirmed empirically.
expressions can be found for babies as young as 7
months of age. In a study of infants responses to arm Affect Specificity Revisited
restraint, Stenberg and Campos (1990) found that in- To date, strong evidence demonstrating infants abilities
fants at this age directed their negative expressive re- to make qualitative distinctions among emotions of the
sponses toward their mothers rather than toward the site same valence is lacking. Nonetheless, in an exploratory
of the frustration (i.e., their restrained hands). In a fur- study involving a small number of participants, E. An-
ther investigation of the phenomenon, Conrad (1994; derson (1994) observed a tendency for infants in this
Campos et al., 2003) studied the consequences of in- age range to respond differentially when an experi-
fants encountering a display of a monkey clashing cym- menter gazed at an unfamiliar food item while verbaliz-
The Development of Emotional Communication in Early Life 247

ing Look at that using sad, angry, disgusted, fearful, these emotions, on the other hand. As indicated earlier,
or happy vocalizations and facial expressions. For exam- we prefer to designate them as other-conscious emotions
ple, 18-month-old female infants tended to give the item because they rest on the childs detecting other persons
to the experimenter in response to a sad emotion mes- reactions to the child or her behavior.
sage but tended to pick it up and eat it in response to a We believe that emotional communication plays a nec-
happy message. essary role in the development of these emotions. Indeed,
Beyond such preliminary data, a survey of the infant as noted earlier, the emotions of embarrassment, guilt,
literature suggests that by 18 months of age infants may shame, and pride may come about as a result of the
indeed understand the specific meanings and implica- emerging appreciation by the child of the meaning of the
tions of some negative emotions. Infants affect vocabu- communication by others of anger, sadness, fear, con-
lary develops rapidly around this age (Dunn, Bretherton, tempt, and other emotions. In other words, the child must
& Munn, 1987) and includes words for several negative first perceive these emotions in others, differentiate
emotions (e.g., scary, yucky, mad). Furthermore, Zahn- them one from the other, know to whom these emotions
Waxler and Radke-Yarrow (1990) found that by 2 years are being targeted, and have a sense of responsibility by
of age many infants show appropriate empathic/sympa- the child in the elicitation of these emotions in others. We
thetic responses to other persons expressions of also believe that some of these emotions, particularly
distress. In a study involving experimenter-produced ex- anger and contempt, are likely to be part of the elicitation
pressions of disgust, Rapacholi (1998) showed that 18- of some complex emotions (especially shame), but not of
month-old infants produced an emotionally appropriate others (particularly guilt).
response (i.e., avoiding the disgust-targeted food item More specifically, we propose that embarrassment
when choosing a food item for the experimenter). How- and shame come about when the infant conducts an ac-
ever, data produced in all these studies are subject to tion toward a third event in the world (including toward
possible interpretations that do not involve the necessary himself, considered as an object) and that action elicits
imputation of affect specificity in the infants under- certain negative emotional signals such as scorn /dis-
standing of the emotion message. Therefore, this issue gust, anger, sadness, and general disapproval. By way of
remains an important challenge in the area of emotion contrast, we propose that guilt occurs when the infant
communication. becomes aware that his or her actions produce emotions
in another person that take the form of sadness,
The Development of Other-Conscious Emotions pain /suffering, disappointment, fear, and other variants
The fourth phase in the development of emotional com- of these emotion families. Thus, the set of emotion sig-
munication also involves the generation of complex nals that produce shame and guilt are overlapping but
emotions that may require, for their generation, the inte- not identical. For example, if another person shows fear
gration of a number of higher-order cognitive, percep- in response to the infants action, guilt is more likely to
tual, and retentive capacities. These emotions include be produced than shame because the emotions that pro-
embarrassment, shame, guilt, and pride. According to duce shame are emotions that typically lower the power
Michael Lewis (e.g., Lewis, 1993) these emotions begin and status of the child in the eyes of the other. Fear is not
to develop between 15 and 18 months, and are in part usually relevant to such status reduction. In sum, dis-
the consequences of the development of self-recognition crete emotions may be necessary, and the infant may
as indexed by the rouge task. In this well-known para- need clearly to distinguish their different meanings, in
digm, an infant in this developmental phase will detect order for the more complex emotions of shame, guilt,
and respond appropriately to the sight of a dot of rouge and pride to form.
surreptitiously placed on her nose and viewed only in a If this reasoning is supported by empirical data (and
mirror reflection. Successful performance on the we know of none relevant to the point at this writing),
rouge/mirror self-recognition task is taken to indicate the child must begin to differentiate negative emotions
the origins of a reflective self that in turn permits the from one another as a necessary prerequisite for gener-
emergence of self-conscious emotions (see Lewis & ating two related but quite different emotions of shame
Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Lewis & Ramsay, 2004). Lewis has and guilt. Indeed, the general action tendencies linked
called these emotions self-conscious because of the to shame (attempts to discontinue social intercourse)
link between mirror self-recognition and other indices and guilt (attempts to engage in reparation of damage
of self-development, on the one hand, and the onset of to another, which damage has been created by ones
248 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

actions) require that the emotional signaling to which emphasis on the functional nature of emotional experi-
the shamed or guilty person is subjected be different. ence is especially relevant to how social goals are the
Another persons fear has no bearing on discontinu- fulcrum around which a great deal of emotion is
ance of ones interaction with that person; another per- elicited, experienced, and expressed.
sons scorn or anger does. By contrast, when a person Social psychologists have long examined the question
reacts with scorn or anger directed at a child who has of what constitutes well-being or positive adaptation,
done something damaging to another, the offending and a review of that literature is not appropriate here (for
party is not likely to experience or to express guilt. a brief review, see Diener & Lucas, 2000); however, what
Guilt is a response to the suffering of another presum- that research does consider is the extent to which social
ably caused by ones self; scorn and anger are not indi- effectiveness and well-being are personality traits or de-
cators of suffering (Campos, Thein, & Owen, 2003). pendent on the situation a person finds her- or himself
Furthermore, we believe that the same emotion in, especially if there has been a sudden change. The re-
message (e.g., sadness) may generate different self- search on childrens emotion regulation suggests that to
conscious emotions depending on the context in which it some extent a proneness to negative emotion, which may
occurs. For instance, sadness may elicit guilt when it is function much like a temperamental disposition, may be
expressed after a child engages in a disapproved action related to social adjustment (for reviews, see Eisenberg
but may elicit shame when it is directed to the child her- & Morris, 2002; Swanson, Hemphill, & Smart, 2004). As
self. Anger may not elicit guilt when it is expressed after an illustration of such research that attempts to tease
a child engages in a hurtful action. apart the influences on childrens resilience and adjust-
In conclusion, our focus has been on early development ment, we consider a recent longitudinal study by Eisen-
in illustrating the theoretical perspective taken in this berg and colleagues (Eisenberg et al., 2004).
essay. In the next section, we turn to an extended discussion
of recent empirical research undertaken with preschoolers, Social Adjustment and Emotion Regulation
school-age children, and adolescents. Much of the recent re-
search with these older children and youth embeds emo- Eisenberg and her colleagues (2004) differentiate be-
tional experience in social interaction, whether the focus is tween ef fortful controlthe ability to voluntarily in-
on socialization of emotional expression norms or on emo- hibit or activate behaviorand reactive controlthe
tion knowledge as applied to social effectiveness. There is relatively inflexible tendency to be either overly inhib-
also a greater emphasis on the development of the self as re- ited or impulsive. Although both types of control have
lated to emotional development, and we address this link in their roots in childrens temperament, the former con-
our discussion of self-conscious emotions as well as in our struct is considered by Eisenberg and her colleagues as
discussion of adolescent true self development. Finally, pivotal to their definition of ef fortful emotion regulation
we once again examine emotion regulation research in this (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004), whereas the latter con-
older age group relative to temperament influence and the struct, reactive control, is less accessible to voluntary
development of coping strategies. Throughout the next sec- control and is linked to temperamental reactivity. Reac-
tion, we also suggest topics for further research, which are tive control is also more often linked to problems of ad-
also highlighted in our conclusion. justment when this characteristic is particularly
pronounced in the individual child, in large part due to
its involuntary nature. Eisenberg and colleagues cite
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT IN Block and Kremens theorizing (Block & Kremen,
CHILDHOOD AND ADOLESCENCE: 1996), which stipulates that most of us would like to op-
SOCIAL EFFECTIVENESS AND erate with as little control as possible and with only as
POSITIVE ADAPTATION much control as necessary. When these are adaptively
functioning and in balance, then the individual is operat-
Noteworthy in recent research has been the greater em- ing in an ego-resilient fashion. However, the individual
phasis given to how childrens and youths emotional de- can be prone to excessive control and thus be maladap-
velopment is manifest in their social competence. tively inhibited or prone to insufficient control and sim-
Although appraisal processes and the regulation of emo- ilarly maladaptively impulsive. If these less adaptive
tion continue to garner much scientific attention, our tendencies are elicited when faced with taxing stressors,
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 249

then Block and Kremen suggest that the individual is not predicted by the young adolescents emotion regulation
responding with resilience. and mood-related disposition toward academic activities:
Eisenberg et al. (2004) examined how effortful control Those adolescents who reported more negative affect re-
and reactive undercontrol were related to childrens inter- garding ordinary academic routines obtained lower grade
nalizing and externalizing problems over a 2-year period. point averages, even when cognitive ability was con-
Both parents and teachers ratings of the childrens be- trolled for. Lawson and Ruff do caution that both atten-
havior were obtained, and the children themselves were tion and emotionality consist of a variety of components:
observed doing a puzzle that required sustained effort to Attention includes persistence, intensity, and flexibility;
solve. The sample ranged from 4.5 to 8 years old, and they negative emotionality can refer to fear, anger (irritabil-
were reassessed 2 years later. The results were very com- ity), or sadness (p. 164). They note that further research
plex, indicating that there is no simple path between emo- is needed to tease apart these respective components for
tion-laden qualities such as effortful control and reactive us to have a complex understanding of how negative emo-
control to subsequent social adjustment. The clearest out- tionality and attention processes work together in chil-
come was that impulsivity and insufficient effortful con- drens development.
trol were directly predictive of externalizing problems, Other studies undertaken by Eisenberg and her col-
and this relationship was even stronger if the children leagues with preschoolers (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1997;
were rated by their teachers as high in dispositional anger. Fabes, Hanish, Martin, & Eisenberg, 2002) suggest a
Proneness to sadness did not moderate this relationship, continuity that may start relatively early in life (e.g., the
but as the authors point out, such externalizing children Lawson and Ruff research), continue through the pre-
often do experience sadness due to peer rejection. The lat- school years, and as the most recent Eisenberg study de-
ter is understood to be a situational response of sadness as scribed earlier and the Gumora and Arsenio study
opposed to a dispositional proneness to sadness. suggest, may continue through the elementary school
A recent study undertaken with toddlers (Lawson & years and extend into middle school as well. Indeed, in a
Ruff, 2004) used constructs similar to the Eisenberg longitudinal study Valiente et al. (2003) found that when
et al. research. The results also indicated that negative attention shifting and focusing and childrens persist-
emotionality and ability to sustain attention predicted ence at a puzzle task were aggregated, they predicted a
later behavioral outcomes. More specifically, maternal reduced level of externalizing behavior over time: This
ratings of emotional lability and proneness to irritability effect was most pronounced for children prone to nega-
at age 2, defined by the authors as their index of nega- tive emotionality. These authors suggested that negative
tive emotionality, and trained observers ratings of at- emotionality may be a moderator of the linkages be-
tentiveness during frustrating play episodes with the tween attentional processes (which they subsumed under
mother combined to predict cognitive function (IQ) and the construct of effortful control) and externalizing be-
problem behavior ratings (maternal ratings) at age 3.5 havior. Such research, albeit complex, does indicate that
years. Their results indicated that when young children developmental psychologists have provided the prelimi-
have both risk factors, low attentiveness and proneness to nary data needed to inform public policy about early in-
negative emotionality, at a young age, they are likely to tervention, which may include parent guidance as well
obtain both lower IQ scores and be rated as significantly as appropriately structured preschool education for ad-
demonstrating more problem behavior. The authors refer dressing the needs of children who are faced with the
to this as the double hazard of combined risk factors double hazard of low attentiveness and proneness to
for both concurrent and predicted outcomes. Those chil- negative emotionality at a young age.
dren who showed both high levels of negative emotional-
ity and low attentiveness showed a decline in IQ from age Emotional Competence
2 to age 3.5, possibly due to the cumulative effects of this
double hazard. Children who were prone to negative emo- Another way to conceptualize childrens emotional de-
tionality but had high attentiveness appeared to be pro- velopment relative to their overall psychological ad-
tected against this deleterious outcome (especially for justment or well-being is to examine their level of
behavior problems). Interestingly, Gumora and Arsenio functioning according to the degree to which they ac-
(2002) found with a considerably older group of children cess the various skills characteristic of emotional
(sixth to eighth graders) that grade point average could be competence. Similar to such constructs as well-being,
250 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

TABLE 5.3 Skills of Emotional Competence


1. Awareness of ones emotional state, including the possibility that one is experiencing multiple emotions, and at even more mature levels,
awareness that one might also not be consciously aware of ones emotions due to unconscious dynamics or selective inattention.
2. Skill in discerning others emotions, based on situational and expressive cues that have some degree of cultural consensus as to their
emotional meaning.
3. Skill in using the vocabulary of emotion and expression terms commonly available in ones subculture and at more mature levels skill in
acquiring cultural scripts that link emotion with social roles.
4. Capacity for empathic and sympathetic involvement in others emotional experiences.
5. Skill in understanding that inner emotional state need not correspond to outer expression, both in oneself and in others, and at more
mature levels understanding that ones emotional-expressive behavior may impact on another and to take this into account in ones self-
presentation strategies.
6. Skill in adaptive coping with aversive emotions and distressing circumstances by using self-regulatory strategies that ameliorate the
intensity or temporal duration of such emotional states (e.g., stress hardiness) and by employing effective problem-solving strategies for
dealing with problematic situations.
7. Awareness that the structure or nature of relationships is largely defined by how emotions are communicated in the relationship such as
by the degree of emotional immediacy or genuineness of expressive display and by the degree of emotional reciprocity or symmetry in the
relationship (e.g., mature intimacy is in part defined by mutual or reciprocal sharing of genuine emotions, but a parent-child relationship
may have asymmetric sharing of genuine emotions).
8. Capacity for emotional self-efficacy: The individual views her- or himself as feeling, overall, the way he or she wants to feel. Emotional
self-efficacy means that one accepts ones emotional experience, whether unique and eccentric or culturally conventional, and this
acceptance is in alignment with the individuals beliefs about what constitutes desirable emotional balance. In essence, one is living in
accord with ones personal theory of emotion and moral sense, when one demonstrates emotional self-efficacy.

social adjustment, and ego-resilience, the construct ing to generalize these skills to non-Western societies
emotional competence is a superordinate term that sub- (e.g., Mesquita, 2001).
sumes a number of emotion-related skills. The defini- Later, we review a variety of studies that illustrate
tion of emotional competence is straightforward: It is how each of the skills of emotional competence develops
the demonstration of self-efficacy in emotion-elicit- or is manifest, and we emphasize research that focuses
ing social transactions. Elsewhere one of us (Saarni, on how the emotional competence skill facilitates an in-
1999) has extensively reviewed the developmental con- dividuals effectiveness in relationships. In many cases,
tributors to emotional competence; briefly, they in- as children mature, their enhanced developmental func-
clude the self or ego identity, a moral sense or tioning reveals itself in more complex manifestations of
character, and a persons developmental history. The a given skill with concomitant advances in how inter-
components of emotional competence are those skills personal exchanges are negotiated.
necessary for self-efficacy in emotion-eliciting social
transactions. Table 5.3 summarizes the skills of emo-
Social Effectiveness and Skill 1: Awareness of
tional competence.
Our Emotions
The derivation of these eight skills was largely based
on a survey of empirical investigations in the field of On some very basic level, knowing what we feel clarifies
emotional development, although there is relatively less what we want. Theorists who emphasize close links be-
research that directly addresses the last two skills. How- tween emotion and motivation readily acknowledge that
ever, these last two skills reflect implicit assumptions in the intended target or goal is critical for how we under-
many studies on emotional development in Western so- stand our subjective experience of emotion (e.g.,
cieties: We live in social-emotional systems (reflected Lazarus, 1991). Likewise, our emphasis on the func-
in Skill 7 and see especially the work by Gottman and tional nature of emotion is consistent with this perspec-
his colleagues on meta-emotion in family functioning, tive. By the time children are 2 to 3 years of age,
e.g., Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997) and emotional awareness of emotional state is usually empirically ex-
competence should ultimately address personal in- amined from the standpoint of how children use emotion
tegritywe can discern what works best for us, relative labels or descriptive phrases to refer to their subjective
to our values (Skill 8). These skills of emotional compe- feelings. A number of studies have shown that young
tence also reflect a Western cultural bias, which is of children spontaneously talk about their own affective
concern, and thus caution should be exercised when try- states as well as about others emotions (e.g., Bloom,
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 251

1998; Harris, 1997). The conversations between these Children who know what they feel are more able to
young children and their family members also imply that negotiate with others when there is a conflict or a need
they have expectancies for how they will feel as well as to assert themselves. However, as Gottman, Katz, and
memories for how they did feel. Young childrens con- Hooven (1997) argue, children are more likely to be ef-
ceptualization about their own subjective emotional ex- fective negotiators when they can down-regulate or
perience encompasses the past, present, and future, and de-escalate their internal arousal sufficiently that they
it is most reliably elicited in familiar interpersonal con- can attend to the social exchange and respond with use-
texts. These everyday sorts of emotion-related commu- ful social compromises to ease the impasse or conflict.
nicative exchanges imply to young children that their Arsenio and Lemerise (2001) take this idea one step fur-
emotions are part of a whole scenario of events, behav- ther in their model of social information processing,
iors, and other people (see also Thompson, Chapter 2, which is integrated with emotion processes such as en-
this Handbook, this volume). In short, emotional experi- coding of affective cues from peers, the affective nature
ence is contextualized. of the relationship a child has with the peer (e.g., hostile
Furthermore, research with young children has versus friendly), and empathic responsiveness. With
shown that they construe their emotions as directed at this model, they suggest that a researcher can examine
something or someone as opposed to what elicited or more effectively how and why children respond to some
caused the emotional response (Harris, 1995). For ex- peer interactions with aggression and to others with so-
ample, consistent with an earlier section, when children cial competence.
verbally describe their emotional reactions, they include By early adolescence, well-functioning youth have
the target of their feeling in their statements (e.g., mad the confidence to disclose their emotions and opinions
at you, scared of snakes, happy about the party). They to others, thereby revealing a true self to others in so
are less likely to talk about the causes of their emotional far as they choose to express what their genuine emo-
reactions (e.g., a conflict over a toy that led to one child tions are, despite negative interpersonal consequences.
feeling treated unfairly and thus becoming angry). How- Relevant research has been undertaken by Harter and
ever, when young children talk about experiencing pain, her colleagues on adolescents perceptions of their true
they do include the cause of pain, saying that they would self and under what conditions they present a dissem-
feel pain on being pricked by a pin, but they would not bled self to others (Harter, 1999; Harter & Lee, 1989;
say that they feel pain about pins. Harter, Waters, Whitesell, & Dastelic, 1998). In addi-
Levine (1995) examined more thoroughly how tion, an early study (Saarni, 1988) found that some
younger children construed beliefs about the causes of preadolescents recognized that although negative social
anger and sadness in hypothetical vignettes. Her sam- consequences could occur if they revealed their genuine
ple consisted of 5- to 6-year-olds, and she found that for emotions, nonetheless, they contended that they would
anger they focused both on the aversiveness of the out- indeed express their genuine emotions because the emo-
come and on the need to have the desired goal rein- tions themselves were deemed important. With this kind
stated. For sadness, the children more often mentioned of awareness of ones emotions, we can begin to under-
the loss felt by the protagonist and the impossibility or stand how emotions themselves begin to constitute a
futility of having the goal reinstated. Interestingly, part of a developing individuals definition of self (see
Levine used the same event for both anger and sadness Harter, 1999). This capacity to be aware of ones self
stories, but she varied the attributions surrounding goal and ones emotional responses is also relevant to the de-
outcome. Her vignettes featured a child who could not velopment of self-conscious emotionspride, shame,
go out to play because of an injury. In one version, the guilt, hubris, envy, and embarrassmentand we turn
child had to stay inside but did not want to, and in the next to a discussion of these emotions and their function
other version, the child wanted to play outside but help- in childrens experience.
lessly could not. The former elicited attributions of
anger and the latter sadness. Her results indicated that Self-Conscious Emotions
children appear to learn (in our culture at least) that Lewis work (1993, 1995) on the development of pride,
their emotions can be explained by the status of their hubris, shame, embarrassment, and guilt constitutes a
goalswhether the goals are met, violated, endan- cognitive appraisal view of how such emotions come
gered, or lost. about (see also Mascolo & Fischer, 1995). According to
252 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Lewis, these self-conscious emotions require that an then I have to be a good girl for her, she may then ego-
objective self has developed: Children can refer to centrically conclude she must have been a bad girl,
themselves and have conscious awareness of themselves when her mother simply expresses negative feelings that
as distinct from others. The cognitive appraisals in- are unrelated to her childs behavior. Research by Zahn-
volved include (a) recognition that there are standards Waxler and her associates (Zahn-Waxler, Cole, & Bar-
to be met, ( b) evaluation of the self s performance rel- rett, 1991; Zahn-Waxler, Kochanska, Krupnick, &
ative to these standards, and (c) attribution of responsi- McKnew, 1990; Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, 1995) sug-
bility to the self on success or failure in meeting the gests that young children (especially girls) growing up
standard. At around the time children acquire objective with depressed mothers may be particularly at risk for
self-awareness (15 to 24 months, as measured by self- developing excessive accountability for their mothers
referential behavior; e.g., Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979), feelings and mood state. Such children were very care-
they also become aware of parental standards for behav- ful in their interaction with others, as though others
ior, rules that they are expected to follow, and desirable were quite fragile, and their behavior included higher
goals for comportment. Children learn about these stan- levels of appeasement, apologizing, and suppression of
dards through their familys disciplinary practices, and negative emotion than comparable children of nonde-
over the next few years their increasing cognitive so- pressed mothers. Thus, these 2- to 3-year-old children
phistication also allows them to gauge the degree to appeared to believe unrealistically that they had control
which they have met the standards. The sorts of stan- over events and over their mothers emotional responses
dards young children learn about are rather simple and and/or were responsible for them.
concrete, for example, you should say please if you The last cognitive appraisal that has to develop before
want something, or how terrible of you to bite your self-conscious emotions are experienced is a focus on
little sister! As children become older, their beliefs be- ones self from an evaluative standpoint such that either
come more differentiated relative to the standards and the whole self or a particular aspect of the self is consid-
rules that they believe they should follow and the goals ered the focus of the success or failure at living up to the
that they think are worthy. Meeting these standards pre- standard, rule, or reaching a goal. Lewis (2000) con-
sumably yields positive emotional experience, both tends that the more that the whole self is globally as-
from the experience of mastery and in receiving social sumed to be responsible for the success or failure, the
approval (e.g., Stipek, 1995). more that either hubris (arrogance) or shame will be
Children also develop an appraisal of self-agency or felt, respectively. When specific aspects of the self are
responsibility: Have they failed or succeeded at reach- seen as leading to the success or failure, then pride or
ing the goal or at living up to the standard, or are they guilt will be felt, respectively. The emotional responses
performing according to the rule? Although Lewis does of pride and guilt are specific self-attributions as in my
not directly address issues of controllability, Weiners effort paid off (pride) or it was my mistake and Ill
work certainly informs us of how perception of control- deal with this fiasco (guilt). The prideful feelings of
lability is directly implied in whether people feel re- accomplishment and pleasure allow the individual to un-
sponsible for events (e.g., Weiner & Handel, 1985). dertake still further challenges; the guilt felt on ones
Dweck and Leggett (1988) have also contended that if failure at a particular event or in a particular situation
individuals view themselves or their world as modifiable allows for interpersonal repair and future improvement
and thus controllable, they will have a different ap- (see also Barrett, Zahn-Waxler, & Cole, 1993, for a dis-
praisal of such a context (and themselves in it), leading cussion of avoiders versus amenders in toddlerhood,
to different emotional sequelae, than those who view with the amenders apparently experiencing guilt
themselves and their environments as fixed and static. rather than shame or embarrassment).
Young children may believe they do cause things to There is also a critical interpersonal context that
happenthat they are in control of events, when they needs to be taken into account in distinguishing shame
are not, due to their cognitive egocentrism that blurs de- and guiltwhether we are observed or alone. We do not
sire and reality (e.g., Harris, 1989). On a simple level, a need social exposure to feel guilt (although it might
2-year-old may believe and act as though if I want a help), but it is a significant feature in our feeling
cookie, then I should have a cookie. But if that same ashamed and wanting to hide from others view (e.g.,
young child believes that if I want Mommy to love me, Barrett et al., 1993). Exposure contributes to another
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 253

self-conscious emotion, embarrassment, which is not not care about the emotion-laden responses of their pri-
necessarily the same as shame. As an illustration of such mary caregivers to their behavior. Thus, for self-
research, Lewis and Ramsay (2002) proposed that there conscious emotions to develop, young children would
are two types of embarrassment, one is simply due to also need to be able to recognize and understand their
being the object of others attention (exposure embar- parents emotion-laden communicative behavior as di-
rassment) and the other is a more self-evaluative embar- rected toward them, and that such instances of parental
rassment that may be linked to shame. They studied emotion communication have meaning for the desirabil-
4-year-old childrens reactions to a performance task ity or undesirability of their behavior. This may account
(i.e., yielding success or failure) and a situation de- for why young children do not demonstrate clear in-
signed to emphasize focus on the self (e.g., receiving stances of shame, pride, or guilt until the 2nd year of life.
lavish compliments) and examined the childrens corti- Research on childrens self-conscious emotions has
sol responses after both conditions to see if they dif- received considerable attention in the previous decade:
fered. The preschoolers who expressed behaviors Tangney and Fischers (1995) volume on self-conscious
indicative of shame or of evaluative embarrassment dur- emotions contains a number of chapters that address de-
ing failure at the task responded with higher cortisol lev- velopmental issues. Reimer (1996) has sought to inte-
els, whereas the exposure to attention situation did not grate research with young childrens self-conscious
result in elevated cortisol responses. They concluded emotions with clinical work on adolescents and adults to
that shame and evaluative embarrassment are more examine the development of shame in later childhood and
stressfulas evidenced by the higher cortisol secre- adolescence. The development and functioning of self-
tionthan feeling oneself to be the object of others fo- conscious emotions clearly needs more attention at all
cused (and positive) attention. age levels. It would appear that the development of self-
Differences between shame and guilt have also been conscious emotions are especially relevant to clinical
extensively studied by Tangney and her colleagues, practice, whether it be the treatment of depression that
using self-reported experiences rated according to di- occurs with a greater frequency among female adoles-
mensions devised by the authors (reviewed in Tangney cents or the development of effective interventions to fa-
and Fischer, 1995). She found with young adults that cilitate a childs coping with the emotional aftermath of
personal shame experiences were more often associated sexual abuse (see Tangney, Burggraf, & Wagner, 1995,
with a sense of powerlessness and having less control for a review of shame, guilt, and psychopathology).
over the situation as well as feeling exposed to others
judgments. Furthermore, shame-prone individuals were Awareness of Multiple Emotions
more likely to externalize blame onto some other person Also relevant to childrens and adolescents social effec-
or event. Shame experiences were also reliably associ- tiveness and adaptation is their ability to be aware of ex-
ated with withdrawal or avoidance of others, whereas periencing multiple emotions or conflicting emotions
guilt was associated with reparation and nonavoidance (as in ambivalence). This development may appear as
of others. Fergusson and Stegge (1995) reviewed the at- early as 5 to 6 years of age (Stein, Trabasso, & Liwag,
tribution literature on the development of guilt and 2000) or not until late childhood (Harter & Whitesell,
shame, and they argue that not until middle childhood do 1989), depending on the criteria and methods for elicit-
children more reliably use a causal analysis of events in ing such understanding from children. Stein and Tra-
reporting their emotions. basso examined 5- to 6-year-olds and determined that at
A somewhat different view of how shame, guilt, and this age children could readily describe people who
pride develop has been proposed by Barrett (1997). Her make them feel good and bad or whom they liked and
emphasis is on emotion communication as a necessary did not like. However, Stein and Trabasso cautioned that
component for the development of self-conscious emo- this did not mean that children simultaneously felt con-
tions. She attributes the primary significance for the flicting emotions: Rather they first focused on one situ-
emergence of the self-conscious emotions to the signifi- ation to which they attached values and attributions,
cant relationships in childrens lives, most notably, their responded emotionally to its impact on them (e.g., I
parents. Her argument is that the standards by which dont like her because she took my Halloween candy),
children view their behavior as shameful, guilt-inducing, and then focused on another situation with its accompa-
or prideful would never be internalized if children did nying values and attributions and respond emotionally
254 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

to its impact (e.g., But I like her when she plays with in the long term. In terms of social effectiveness, know-
me). Thus, ambivalence for Stein and Trabasso was ing how ones self tends to react, whether it is with
viewed as a sequential process with different appraisals shame or with a conflicted set of emotions leading to
attached to the different or polarized emotional re- ambivalence, is still a source of important information
sponses, and they suggested that this process was the for the developing child or teen to integrate into his or
same for adults, just much more rapid. her self-definition, especially knowing that some emo-
Studies conducted by Harter and Whitesell (1989) tional experiences render ones self acutely vulnerable
and by Donaldson and Westerman (1986) were similarly in interpersonal situations.
concerned with childrens cognitive construction of
their own emotional experience, particularly when mul-
Social Effectiveness and Skill 2: Ability to
tiple emotions are involved. Harter and Whitesell fo-
Discern and Understand Others Emotions
cused on the cognitive developmental prerequisites for
understanding the simultaneity of multiple emotions To understand others emotions and motives, children
embedded in a situation or relationship. Not until chil- need (a) to make sense of others expressive behavior
dren had the ability to coordinate multiple attributes of and action tendencies, ( b) to understand common situa-
a situation with the dimension of emotional valence (at tional elicitors of emotions, and (c) to comprehend that
about 10 years of age) could they make sense of opposite others have minds, intentions, beliefs, and inner states.
valence emotions ( happy and sad) about different tar- There is a fairly substantial research literature on these
gets that co-occur in a situation (e.g., Im glad I get to topics and the reader is referred to reviews in Denham
live with my dad, but Im sad about not being able to live (1999); Dunn (2000); Halberstadt, Denham, and Duns-
with my mom too). Young adolescents developed fur- more (2001); Harris (2000); Underwood (1999); von
ther and could integrate simultaneously opposite va- Salisch (2001); and Thompson, Easterbrooks, and
lence emotions about the same target (e.g., I love my Padilla-Walker (2003). A variety of studies indicate that
dad, even though Im mad at him right now). Harter children who are more accurate in understanding oth-
and Whitesell acknowledged that what may occur as we ers emotional experience also tend to be more socially
cognitively integrate contrasting emotions about the competent (see reviews in Halberstadt et al., 2001).
same target is a rapid oscillation between the multiple These studies typically establish a given childs social
emotion-eliciting aspects of a relationship or situation. competence either by teachers ratings or by peers so-
Harter and Buddin (1987) also pointed out that it is ciometric choices. Then the childrens understanding of
not known whether children might experience simulta- emotion terms, of facial expressions, of elicitors of emo-
neously two (or even more) emotions but can only cogni- tion, and so forth are assessed and correlated with the
tively construct an explanation about the experience that childrens social competence ratings (e.g., Hubbard &
focuses on one emotion. They also noted that children Coie, 1994). Other investigators have examined chil-
may in some situations experience only one overwhelm- drens maladaptive social interaction to see whether
ing emotion, for example, fear, but as they seek to cope deficits in their understanding of others emotions con-
with the scary situation or have to communicate about it tribute to their ineffectual social behavior. Specific
to some one else, they begin to cognitively construct a clinical populations have also been studied for how their
more complex system of appraisals about the emotion- understanding of others emotional experience may dif-
eliciting situation or relationship. fer from nonclinic samples, for example, autistic chil-
dren, children who have been abused, and children who
Summary
have witnessed family violence (for brief reviews, see
This first skill of emotional competencethe ability to Saarni, 1999, and Southam-Gerow & Kendall, 2002).
be aware of an emotional experiencefacilitates chil- Two studies have considerable potential for broaden-
drens problem solving, for knowing how to respond ing research on this topic. In the first study, Barth and
emotionally to a particular eliciting encounter is crucial Bastiani (1997) investigated childrens biases in label-
to deciding on a course of action, especially if a first im- ing the expressions of their classmates facial expression
pulse to action is potentially going to incur some unde- photos. Accuracy scores were based on congruence of
sirable consequences, and thus be less self-efficacious the judged expression and which expression the class-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 255

mate was intending to produce expressively for the consistent with that found by Underwood and Hurley
photo. Bias scores were calculated as the proportionate (1999). We would like to see research that combines ele-
number of times a child used a particular expression ments of the Barth and Bastiani approach with Hub-
label (e.g., sad, happy, mad, surprised, or afraid) rela- bards approach: For example, would rejected children
tive to the total number of classmate photos that were demonstrate a bias in their perception of emotion in oth-
judged. The children made these ratings at the beginning ers, or are they simply oblivious and insensitive to emo-
of the school year and again 5 months later. The chil- tional cues? Why did this game context not elicit anger
drens peer acceptance, based on sociometric ratings, in children rated by their peers for being likely to start
and their general social adjustment, based on teacher fights? Is this a context in which a potential bias for per-
ratings, were also assessed. ceiving anger in others is not elicited?
Noteworthy among their results was that those chil-
Understanding Others Feelings and Cognitive
dren who had a bias for seeing angry facial expres-
Development
sions (contrary to what the familiar classmate was
trying to produce expressively) were also the ones who The ability to understand what others are experiencing
had less satisfactory peer relations, and their teachers emotionally does not develop in isolation from other as-
rated their adjustment more often as hostile dependent. pects of emotional development and cognitive develop-
This outcome is consistent with research with older ment. Emerging insight into others emotions develops
children who used a hostile attribution bias in their in interaction with increasing awareness of an individ-
peer relationships (e.g., Crick & Dodge, 1994). These uals own emotional experience, with the ability to em-
results may also be linked to Isley and colleagues re- pathize, and with the ability to conceptualize causes of
search on parent-child expressed affect and the chil- emotions and their behavioral consequences. The more
drens subsequent social competence with peers (Isley, children learn about how and why people act as they do,
ONeil, Clatfelter, & Parke, 1999). In that study, nega- the more they can infer their emotional state, even if it is
tive expressed affect by parents toward their children not especially obvious or may even be counterintuitive.
was associated with their childrens impaired social The studies reviewed show that the growth in emotional
functioning with peers. Similarly, Schultz, Izard, and development that children show over time is deeply tied
Ackerman (2000) found that parental depression and to their cognitive development as well. (For a recent re-
family instability predicted preschool childrens anger view of childrens cognitive understanding of emotions,
attribution bias, which in turn was associated with peer see Harris, 2000.)
rejection. These children were also rated as aggressive
by their teachers. Facial Expressions and Emotion-Eliciting Situa-
The second study we describe in some detail was on tions. We concur with the view espoused by Lewis
how rejected children behave when mildly interperson- and Michalson (1985) that facial expressions can have a
ally stressed. Hubbard (2001) had 7- and 8-year-olds dual function; they can be signs, in which case they
rate their peers for how liked or not-liked they were as bear a one-to-one correspondence to internal emotional
well as who starts the most fights. The former sociomet- state, or they can function as symbols, in which case
ric rating yielded a rejection index and the latter an ag- they refer to something else. When facial expressions
gression index. She then observed the children in a are symbolic, they are referring to metacommunicative
rigged game with a confederate whom they did not know processes (e.g., Wagner & Lee, 1999; Weber & Laux,
and who was 1 year older. The children were under pres- 1993), for example, placating someone, deterring some-
sure to win the game to obtain a prize. Compared to ac- one, or presenting oneself in a more favorable light. The
cepted children, the rejected children were more likely dissociation of facial expression from internal emo-
to express both facial and verbal anger during the game. tional experience is taken up in detail in the discussion
They also expressed more happiness, but only when of Skill 5.
some game maneuver was to their advantage. Interest- By mid to late childhood, most children recognize
ingly, aggressive children were not necessarily more and can verbalize that a persons expression may
likely to express anger (or happiness or sadness) than be both a social and an emotional response (e.g., Un-
nonaggressive children. This particular finding is also derwood & Hurley, 1999). Gross and Ballif (1991)
256 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

reviewed the early research on childrens understand- kindergarten more often made use of others personal-
ing of emotions in others based on facial expression ity traits in their construal of what gave rise to the
cues and situational elicitors of emotion. They con- emotional response. This use of personalized informa-
cluded that as children matured, they became more ac- tion in understanding others emotional experience is
curate in their inferences about what others were further examined in the next section.
feeling. The easiest emotions to figure out were posi-
tive ones: Smiling faces and situations depicting Taking into Account Unique Information about
pleasure and getting what one wants were readily com- the Other. The most relevant studies for more fully
prehended as associated with happiness. Negative fa- describing this feature of understanding others emo-
cial expressions depicting sadness, fear, or anger were tional experience were conducted by Gnepp and Gould
more difficult for children to decode. However, if (1985) and Gnepp and Chilamkurti (1988) and theo-
paired with a detailed emotion-eliciting situational retically elaborated by Gnepp (1989). Gnepp and
context, children were much more likely to infer the Gould examined whether children (ages 5 to 10) could
negative emotion in question. use information about a story characters past experi-
As children grow older, they combine both facial ence (e.g., being rejected by your best friend) to pre-
and situational cues as they attempt to discern and dict how the character would feel in some new
understand the emotional experience of others. Wig- situation (e.g., subsequently meeting the best friend
gers and van Lieshout (1985) suggested that when on the playground). Not unexpectedly, the youngest
there was a contradiction between a facial expression children were more likely to use the current situa-
and the emotion-eliciting situation, school-age chil- tional information to infer what the character was
dren were more likely to opt for whichever cue was feeling (e.g., she would be happy at seeing her best
more clearly presented. An example used by Wiggers friend) and older children were more likely to infer the
and van Lieshout is a scenario depicting a boy with a characters emotional state by taking into account the
weak smile about to get a fearsome injection. The sit- prior experience (e.g., she would feel sad on seeing her
uation, in this case, is more definitively portrayed best friend). An interaction also occurred between the
than the boys facial expression, and as a result, chil- hedonic tone (positive/negative) of the emotion and
dren conclude that the boy is anxious or afraid, de- the use of personal information: If the story character
spite the attempt at a smile. Children also recognize experienced a negative emotion at Time 1 but encoun-
that others might feel a mixture of feelings about a tered a commonly assumed positive situation at Time
situation. 2, children were more likely to use prior personal his-
The preceding conclusions were largely based on tory information when inferring how the character
research that was done by having children appraise hy- would feel at Time 2. Gnepp (1989) suggests that chil-
pothetical vignettes, photos, and the like. Fabes, dren must first recognize what a persons perspective
Eisenberg, Nyman, and Michealieu (1991) investi- was at Time 1 and then must apply that inferred per-
gated 3- to 6-year-old childrens understanding of oth- spective from Time 1 to Time 2 to come up with the
ers emotions in naturalistic settings. They found that atypical emotional response.
happy reactions were more often correctly identified In an analogous investigation, Gnepp and Chil-
(according to adult standards) than negative reactions, amkurti (1988) presented stories to elementary school
thus replicating the general outcome of interview- children and adults in which characters personality
based research. Fabes et al. also examined childrens traits were systematically described as either desirable
understanding of the causes of emotions and found or undesirable traits. The story characters then had
that children could more readily identify causes for some experience befall them, and the children were to
negative emotions. They interpreted this result as infer the emotional reaction of the character to this
based on the greater intensity of negative emotional new experience. Older children and adults were more
states and concluded that children more readily evalu- likely to take into account the prior trait information in
ate the causes for goal failure (i.e., any undesired out- inferring the emotional response of the character in the
come). A developmental difference found by Fabes new situation. The younger children (6-year-olds) were
et al. was that their youngest children tended to attrib- less consistent in doing so, but a number were able to
ute causes to wants and needs, but the older children in take personality trait information into account when
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 257

inferring how someone might emotionally respond to and expression linkages? Custrini and Feldman (1989)
an emotion-eliciting event, even when the emotional reported that among girls, but not among boys, degree
reaction might be atypical for the eliciting event. La- of social competence greatly influenced their overall
gattuta and her colleagues (Lagattuta, Wellman, & accuracy score in encoding and decoding others emo-
Flavell, 1997), determined that even preschoolers could tions. Girls who were below average in social compe-
figure out that the same situation could evoke different tence scored well below boys, regardless of their social
emotional responses in story characters if the cues competence level, and the highest scoring children
were presented very explicitly (e.g., pictorially show- were girls who were above average in social compe-
ing the story protagonists experiencing atypical emo- tence. Other research undertaken by Walden and
tional reactions). Knieps (1996) suggested that preschoolers who ob-
These investigations show us that by school entry, tained high sociometric peer preferences as play part-
children are well on their way to superimposing multiple ners were also those who tended to be better at
frames of reference onto one another across time inter- discriminating among emotional facial displays and
vals to predict or infer other peoples emotional re- who tended to demonstrate high spontaneous expres-
sponses. In Gnepps research, a distinction was not sivity ( but they did not excel in posed expressions). An-
made between emotional state and emotional-expressive other study undertaken by Edwards, Manstead, and
behavior: The assumption was that children would infer McDonald (1984) with somewhat older children
emotional state. Whether children could also infer what demonstrated a similar relation: Childrens sociomet-
sort of expressive behavior would be displayed, and ric rating was positively related to their ability to rec-
whether it would be congruent with an atypical internal ognize facial expressions of emotion.
emotional state or with the consensually defined typi- Further support for links between social effectiveness
cal emotion response to the situation, was not part of and emotion knowledge can be found in research under-
the focus of these studies. Little research has since been taken by Denham, McKinley, Couchoud, and Holt (1990)
undertaken on this topic, an omission that we would like on preschoolers peer interaction and in a study on
to see remedied. family-peer connections by Cassidy, Parke, Butkovsky,
and Braungart (1992). The Denham et al. results showed
Social Competence and Discerning Others Emo- that children who demonstrated greater knowledge of
tional States. A number of studies indicate that chil- emotion in the puppet task (especially in understanding
dren with emotional problems or who have been abused anger and fear) were perceived by their peers as more
show deficits in their understanding of links between fa- likeable. In the Cassidy et al. (1992) study of kinder-
cial expression and emotion, in producing facial expres- garten children, emotion understanding was measured by
sions, and in discriminating emotion expressions (e.g., interviewing the children about identification of emo-
Camras, Grow, & Ribordy, 1983; R. J. Casey, 1996; Pol- tional facial expressions, how particular emotions were
lack, Cicchetti, Hornung, & Reed, 2000; Shipman, situationally elicited, and what were the social conse-
Zeman, Penza, & Champion, 2000). The research by quences to such emotions. They found that children who
Pollack et al. (2000) warrants further description: These demonstrated more complex emotion understanding
investigators found that neglected children had difficul- were more accepted by their peers.
ties in discriminating emotions, whereas physically More recent research undertaken by Garner and
abused children appeared to have a bias to perceive Estep (2001) examined the relations among preschool-
angry facial expressions. Thus, the sorts of early emo- ers emotion knowledge and their social skills. Their
tion communication experiences that children receive results indicated that childrens understanding of situa-
influence how they construct their understanding of oth- tional clues to infer what emotions would likely be felt
ers emotional expressions and emotional states, and as was a negative predictor of childrens use of noncon-
we elaborate under our discussion of Skill 6, this early structive anger reactions with their peers. Their assess-
emotion communication exposure is also linked to their ment of childrens ability to provide explanations for
ability to regulate their own emotional arousal with the causes and consequences of others emotions was a
peers and in school (e.g., Gottman et al., 1997). significant predictor of the childrens initiating social
Do children who are exceptionally socially compe- interaction as well as being chosen as recipients of
tent show an enhancement of understanding emotion social bids.
258 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Problematic Outcomes and Understanding Oth- ers, and one hypothesis has been the stress and affilia-
ers Emotional States. Discerning and understand- tion effect (reviewed in Luminet, Bouts, Delie,
ing others emotional experience does not necessarily Manstead, & Rim, 2000): When we feel badly, we want
always contribute to ones skill at emotional communi- the company of others, and, more specifically, we want
cation. Paradoxically, in certain circumstances, some to tell others how we are feeling (with the interesting ex-
children would be better off tuning out others emo- ception of when we feel shame). This implies that com-
tional behavior. Children who are exposed to and in- municating our feelings to another initiates change. Such
volved in their parents depressed feelings represent one change may be found in how we experience our subjec-
group who are at risk for an aversive emotion socializa- tive feeling state (e.g., internal emotion regulation is af-
tion experience, and another group consists of those fected), or with the support of others, we may devise
children exposed to marital conflict accompanied by different ways to cope with a problematic situation.
overt anger. It is unrealistic to assume that children can From a developmental perspective, the stress and af-
tune out their parents negative emotions, but outcomes filiation pattern is the hallmark of many attachment
for children if such experiences are frequent, intense, studies with young children. We also know that young
and started early in life do not bode well. Investigations children and their mothers are more likely to use emo-
of how children of chronically depressed parents (for tion-laden language when there is a dispute or some neg-
the most part, mothers) develop emotionally has been ative event occurs (Denham, Mitchell-Copeland,
reviewed by Downey and Coyne (1990) and by Zahn- Strandberg, Auerbach, & Blair, 1997; Dunn & Brown,
Waxler and colleagues (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1990). Re- 1991; Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991). Indeed, as
search on children who witness interadult anger and Chambers review indicates (1999), family discourse is
domestic violence has been reviewed by Osofsky critical to young childrens learning about emotions,
(1999). how to speak about feelings, and how to cope with emo-
tion-laden situations. Awaiting further study is how the
Summary
social psychological research on emotion disclosure un-
Childrens social effectiveness is closely linked to their dertaken with adults might manifest itself in childrens
accurate appraisal of emotional states in others. Even if emotion disclosures to peers. On one hand, Gottman
the expressive cues are ambiguous, they learn to infer et al. (1997) would contend that school-age children
what emotions others might be experiencing, based on should not disclose their feelings to peers if they are to
their expanding knowledge of common elicitors of emo- be accepted. On the other hand, with the advent of close
tion relevant to their subculture. Preschoolers can antic- friendships in childhood and early adolescence, disclo-
ipate that atypical emotional responses may be sure of vulnerability-inducing feelings (e.g., anxiety,
experienced, if they are provided with fairly explicit sadness, or hurt) is more likely to occur (e.g., Asher &
cues. By the early school years, children take into ac- Rose, 1997; Saarni, 1988). However, let us return to the
count what they know about anothers personality or beginning: how young children acquire a lexicon of emo-
unique circumstances to infer the targets emotional re- tion that permits them to represent their own and others
sponse. Research on individual differences indicates emotional experience.
that maltreatment of children compromises or distorts
the development of these social-cognitive skills. Ability to Use Concepts, Lexicon, and Scripts
Relevant to Emotion and Expression

Social Effectiveness and Skill 3: Use of a The ability to represent emotional experience through
Vocabulary of Emotion and Expression words, imagery, and symbolism of varied sorts allows
children to communicate to others what they want and
With language and symbols, we can traverse time and what problems they are encountering as well as to de-
space to communicate with others about our own and scribe their delight and pleasure. With words, the child
their emotions. Modern technology seems especially de- can further elaborate these representations of emotional
signed to promote such communication with instant experience, integrate them across contexts, and compare
messaging, e-mail, and cell phones everywhere. Social them with others representations about emotional expe-
psychologists have investigated why it is that we are so riences. Some of the developments in awareness of ones
compelled to share our emotional experience with oth- own multiple emotional responses or in understanding
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 259

others atypical emotions, described earlier, could not Children can also apply emotion terms to pretend
be undertaken if children did not have access to a lan- play by age 2 to 3, and, indeed, listening to children talk
guage or representational system (e.g., sign language) as they enact fantasies with their figurative toys (e.g.,
for symbolically encoding and communicating their dolls, action figures, stuffed animals) is an excellent
emotional experiences. way to observe a young childs competence with emo-
Assuming an intact nervous system and an environ- tion language, for they construct both the causes of the
ment that is not overwhelmingly trauma filled, children figures emotional response and the consequences of the
do show some commonalities in our culture in learning emotion, including how the figure copes. Denham and
how to represent emotion. However, individual differ- Auerbach (1995) analyzed the emotional content of
ences and cultural influence are again strong forces in mothers and preschoolers dialogues while looking at
the development of language-based emotion concepts, picture books together (whose contents were emotion
which is not surprising, since one of the critical func- laden). They found that such an interaction was rich
tions of an emotion lexicon is to be able to communi- with adult-child exchanges that included affect labeling
cate with others, which obviously entails their and causes and consequences of emotional experience.
reciprocal communication about emotion (e.g., Dunn & In addition, both mothers and children used their emo-
Brown, 1991). tion-descriptive language in ways that suggested social
influence of the other; for example, mothers who lim-
Development of Emotion Lexicon. Bretherton, ited themselves to simple comments about the emotion-
Fritz, Zahn-Waxler, and Ridgeway (1986) reviewed the laden material in the picture books had children who
relevant literature on childrens acquisition of emotion asked more questions to engage their mothers more.
words, and they noted that many toddlers could use emo- However, those mothers who made use of verbal expla-
tion words toward the end of the 2nd year. However, they nation to a very great degree appeared to stimulate their
cautioned that what was included as an emotion word, children further to use more complex and elaborated
for example, crying, may simply be a behavioral action emotion-descriptive language. These children tended to
noted by the young child and conceivably was not used respond with more guiding and socializing language
by the child to indicate an internal emotional state. By 3 about the characters emotional experience.
years of age, children could much more readily label the
emotions of others in addition to their own feelings. In- Conversations about Emotional Responses. The
creasingly, they could also verbally address the conse- classic research on this topic was undertaken by Dunn
quences of emotional states as well as the situational et al. (1987) who investigated naturally occurring con-
causes of emotions; for example, Grandma mad. I versations in the home between young children and their
wrote on wall. mothers and siblings. They were particularly interested
Researchers have also studied parents reports of in determining what sorts of functions conversations
their childrens understanding and use of emotion-de- about emotional reactions had in the social exchange in
scriptive words. Using parents ratings on checklists of the home and how children communicated causes of
words indicating emotion states (e.g., happy), emo- emotional reactions in their exchanges with others. They
tion traits (e.g., good), and physical states (e.g., followed the young children from age 18 months to 24
sleepy, clean), Ridgeway, Waters, and Kuczaj months and found that conversations about causes of
(1985) tabulated the percentages of children at 6-month emotional experience increased significantly in the 6
intervals (starting at 18 months and extending to 6 months they tracked the children. They also found that
years of age) comprehending the emotion word and also the vast majority of emotion-laden conversations were
using it. The most frequently understood words at 18 with the mother as opposed to the older sibling, although
months were sleepy, hungry, good, happy, clean, tired, such conversations involving all three occurred more
and sad (50% to 83% comprehension). By age 6, chil- often than between just the two children. This finding
dren comprehended such words as nervous (83%), suggests that access to an adult who is interested in ones
embarrassed (77%), jealous (60%), and miser- emotional reactions may be pivotal to children having
able (53%). Their corresponding production of these opportunities both to talk about emotions and have their
words was one-half to two-thirds of the percentages for understanding of emotions elaborated. Mothers tended
comprehension. to use conversations about emotional responses as a
260 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

functional way to guide or explain something to their emotion-descriptive language is anchored in relation-
children, whereas the children were more likely to use ship contexts: If everyone is angry or distressed a lot
emotion-descriptive words simply to comment on their of the time, an episode of distress on the part of a
own reaction or observation of another. Thus, they were child may be viewed as trivial. What may be metacom-
learning to communicate their own self-awareness of municated in such families is that a childs emotional
emotion states to their mothers, who in turn were likely reaction is not very important. We can surmise how
to communicate meaningfulness to their children by differences in the families emotional milieus provide
using guiding, persuading, clarifying, or otherwise inter- varying preparatory stages for their childrens later
pretive emotion-related language. emotional experiences in the world beyond the home
Dunn et al. (1991) continued their research on natu- (e.g., Du Rocher Schudlich, Shamir, & Cummings,
rally occurring conversations about emotions in the 2004).
home, but this time extended their longitudinal study A recent study by Laible (2004) on mother-child
of children from when they were 3 years old to age 6. discourse sheds further light on the role that the par-
They again focused on childrens and mothers ex- ent-child relationship plays in childrens acquisition
changes around causes and consequences of emotions of emotion understanding and internalization of
and additionally tracked how disputes and conflicts in behavioral expectations. Attachment classification
the home provided occasions for emotional growth by and proneness to negative emotional reactivity were
how children were exposed to and had to use emotion- evaluated in her preschool sample, and then conversa-
descriptive language to negotiate the conflict. They tions between the mother and her child were examined
found a tremendous range in variability among the for elaborative style and discussion of negative emo-
children in frequency of emotion talk from 0 to 27 tions. Two discourse situations were sampled where
occasions per hour of observation. The mothers also (1) the mother and child reminisced, and (2) they read
showed a similar variability, ranging from 0 to 22 oc- a story together. It was primarily in the first discourse
casions of emotion talk per hour of observation. Un- context in which the mother and her child discussed
fortunately, Dunn et al. did not provide information as the childs past experience that Laible found that
to whether the mother-child dyads were matched in attachment security was related to maternal elabora-
their rates of emotion talk. Given the findings of tion and the dyads discussion of negative emotions.
their earlier study where mothers were pivotal in pro- In turn, such maternal elaboration was associated
viding opportunities for children to verbally communi- with children demonstrating higher levels of behav-
cate about their emotional reactions, we can only ioral internalization and higher levels of emotion
speculate that if a mother did not talk much about understanding. Interestingly, if mothers perceived
emotions and related inner states, very likely her child their children as prone to negative emotional reactiv-
did not either. ity, they were more likely to elaborate in the reminis-
Another study by Dunn and Brown (1994) sheds cence task and discuss negative emotions more
further light on the effects of family emotional ex- frequently (probably because their children experi-
pression on childrens acquisition of emotion-descrip- enced them more often due to their proneness to nega-
tive language. Again, they found that occasions of tive reactivity).
negative emotional reactions on the part of the child
were when most emotion-related discourse occurred Structural Analysis of Emotion-Descriptive Lan-
between mother and child. This study also docu- guage. Russell and Ridgeway (1983) examined emo-
mented that families characterized as high in fre- tion-descriptive adjectives used by children in
quency of anger and distress expression had children elementary school. They found that these words could be
who were less likely to be engaged in discourse about statistically analyzed with principal-components analy-
emotional experience. However, if the families were ses and multidimensional scaling. The result was that
low in frequency of negative emotional expression, two bipolar dimensions were found that provided an or-
when a negative emotional event did occur for the ganizational structure for the many emotion-related
child, there was a greater likelihood of an emotion-re- terms used by school children. These two dimensions
lated conversation to ensue between child and parent. were: (1) degree of pleasure or hedonic tone and (2) de-
This research suggests that childrens acquisition of gree of arousal.
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 261

This dimensional analysis of emotion concepts is of additional emphases or omissions if a persons


interest for it suggests something about the way emotion machismo or femininity is implicated in the anger epi-
is categorized and perhaps even organized as subjective sode. What is important to consider is that scripts for
experience. A number of cross-cultural comparisons different emotions may merge (or one emotion may
have been made on emotion concepts used by adults in cycle into another) under certain circumstances, par-
other languages (reviewed by Russell, 1991), and the ticularly if individuals appraise a situation as salient
pleasure/displeasure dimension has been reliably found for certain beliefs they hold about themselves (see also
in all the cultures studied. Russell suggests that hedonic Lewis, 1992, for a discussion of links between gender
tone is relevant in all cultures in how feeling states are and shame scripts).
differentiated, but the English word emotion is not Emotion scripts may merge with gender role social-
necessarily present in all languages, although an equiva- ization as suggested by some of the sex differences
lent term is more the rule than the exception (exceptions found for how anger and sadness are talked about in
appear to be the Tahitians, the Bimin-Kuskusmin of families. For example, gender differences in learning to
Papua New Guinea, the Gidjingali aborigines of Aus- talk about emotional experience were found by Dunn
tralia, the Ifalukians of Micronesia, the Chewong of et al. (1987) in their study of young British children and
Malaysia, and the Samoans; cited in Russell, 1991). In their mothers. Little girls received more comments and
these few cultures where no term similar to emotion ex- inquiries about emotions from their mothers and from
ists, internal affective states may be referred to as aris- their older siblings than did little boys; however, the
ing in certain body parts or organs; for example, the boys and girls themselves were similar in their initia-
Chewong view the liver as the source of what for them tion of conversations about emotions. In a similar vein,
might be called thoughts and affective responses. The Fivush (1991) undertook an exploratory study with
significance of the virtually universal presence of the mothers of 3-year-old boys and girls and found that
pleasure/aversion dimension in emotion-descriptive con- mothers tended to talk in a more elaborated fashion
cepts may indicate that what experiencing pleasure/aver- about sadness with their daughters and more about
sion does for us is a basic function: It is embedded in our anger with their sons. She found that mothers tended to
approach-avoidance actions in relation to the contexts we embed their discussions of emotions in social frame-
live in and thus supports the functionalist theoretical works more with their daughters than with their sons.
perspective espoused in this chapter. Relative to script notions, she also found that when
anger was involved, mothers emphasized relationship
Emotion Script Learning. Acquisition of emo- repair with their daughters and were more accepting of
tion-descriptive concepts continues throughout child- retaliation by their angry sons.
hood and into adolescence, but little research has More recently, Widen and Russell (2002) examined
examined these older age groups. Further development young childrens attributions of emotion to story char-
of emotion language in the school-age child and adoles- acters who varied in gender but who were engaged in
cent may be found in their greater ability to add vari- identical emotion-eliciting situations. Through com-
ety, subtlety, nuance, and complexity to their use of puter image manipulation, the faces of two young ado-
emotion-descriptive words with others. What may also lescents, a boy and a girl, were merged into a single
develop is that childrens scripts for understanding androgynous image. The images were then further ma-
emotional experience are reciprocally influenced by nipulated by having either a masculine or a feminine
their growing access to increasing complexity of emo- stereotyped hairstyle. Boys more often inferred the
tion concepts. Russell (1991) defines emotion scripts male protagonist to be feeling disgusted and girls more
as a knowledge structure for a type of event whereby often inferred the female protagonist to be feeling fear-
the event is thought of as a sequence of subevents ful. Prior research had indicated that preschoolers
(p. 442). Russell notes that even in the same culture often attributed anger to boys and sadness to girls
scripts for the same emotions may differ from person when the emotion being felt was ambiguously pre-
to person because emotion scripts are linked to other sented (e.g., Karbon, Fabes, Carlo, & Martin, 1992).
belief networks. This is a significant point because, for But what the authors suggestthat is even more in-
example, if a script for anger is linked to a network of triguing from a developmental perspectiveis that dif-
concepts about sex role, the anger script may well have ferent emotions may be conceptualized by young
262 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

children by relying on different cues. Their results in- Social Effectiveness and Skill 4: The Capacity
dicated that clear facial expressions of happiness, sad- for Empathic and Sympathetic Involvement
ness, and anger were not swayed by gender cues,
Empathy and sympathy are emotional responses that
whereas the emotional displays of disgust and fear ap-
connect us with others. Beginning with early infancy, it
pear to be influenced more by gender stereotypes and
is clear that very young babies respond to the crying
the verbal label provided.
states of other neonates (Martin & Clark, 1982). This
Last, cultural inf luence and the acquisition of an
early attentiveness to emotional-expressive cues may
emotional lexicon are inseparable, for societies use
pave the way for later vicariously induced emotion, for
language to regulate emotion in social interaction.
they cannot be induced to experience anothers emo-
Many societies emphasize some emotional responses
tional state unless they notice it and consider it salient.
over others by attaching special importance to certain
Sympathy differs from empathy in that it can also be ex-
emotion-descriptive words. In American culture, the
perienced when responding to purely symbolic infor-
word love is such an emotionally loaded word.
mation, such as reading about someones distress or by
Much anthropological research has been done that ex-
hearing about someones unfortunate circumstances.
amines emotion-descriptive language. The reader is
Empathy tends to be defined as a more immediate emo-
referred to an edited volume on this topic (Russell,
tional response that is experienced by the observer on
Fernndez-Dols, Manstead, & Wellenkamp, 1996).
witnessing someones emotional state. Empathy may in-
Another useful perspective is that of ethnotheories
clude emotion contagion, but with older children and
(or folk theories) of emotion lexicons (e.g., Lutz,
adults we more often assume that there is some ability
1988). Illustrative of this perspective are Ochs
to take the perspective of the distressed person, and,
(1986) review of Samoan childrens acquisition of
consequently, we experience vicariously what we be-
emotion-descriptive language and A. Eisenbergs
lieve the target person to be experiencing. Sympathy is
(1986) ethnographic study of the emotional and social
an affective response that contains elements of sorrow
functions of verbal banter (teasing) in recently immi-
or concern for the distressed person. When feeling sym-
grated Mexican children.
pathetic, we do not necessarily vicariously experience
the same or similar negative affect of the target (Eisen-
Summary
berg, 2003).
Young children show a rapid increase in acquiring an It is hard to imagine that a person could be socially
emotion lexicon. Having words for their emotional ex- effective and not be empathically involved in their inter-
perience allows for seeking support in distressing cir- action with others. Yet, paradoxically, sometimes too
cumstances, for reciprocal sharing with others about much empathy with anothers negative emotional re-
emotional experience, and for being able to conceptu- sponse can disrupt socially effective engagement with
alize lexically their emotions and how they came about the distressed person, resulting in personal distress
as well as what the consequences for the self and oth- rather than being able to focus on the other person and
ers might be. Without an emotion lexicon, how could respond appropriately. In a number of well-conducted
children and youth reflect on themselves as emotion- studies, Eisenberg and her colleagues found that chil-
experiencing individuals? Indeed, we see in cases of dren also need to establish psychological boundaries so
severely abused and traumatized children and youth a that they can respond sympathetically and not become
deficit in the ability to conceptualize what they experi- overwhelmed by anothers distress (e.g., Eisenberg
ence emotionally (e.g., alexithymia; see also Camras, et al., 1996). Such personal distress leads to a preoccu-
Sachs-Alter, & Ribordy, 1996). This skill of emotional pation with their own negative affective response,
competence functions somewhat like a pivot for the which then short-circuits their prosocial, sympathetic
other skills, for it is with access to the language of behavior. Zahn-Waxler and Robinson (1995) have ar-
emotion concepts that children learn to predict how gued that personal distress reactions are most likely to
they themselves are likely to emotionally react, to un- occur when the others emotional-expressive display is
derstand others emotional responses, and to respond particularly vivid and intense, and if there appears to be
empathically and sympathetically to others, as we dis- little that an individual can do to ameliorate the others
cuss next. distressing situation. An individual might also experi-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 263

ence personal distress when his or her sympathy is un- slavsky, & Baumeister, 2001). If a person could tolerate
desired by the target of their concern, as might occur uncertainty about a course of action, such as to how to
when a distressed individual is further distressed by intercede or not, and thereby gather more information,
being made to feel self-conscious, helpless, or somehow they might ultimately be more capable of responding not
inadequate through anothers sympathetic overtures. We only prosocially but also more effectively in ameliorat-
are not aware of any empirical developmental research ing the others distress. This question has not been in-
on this latter topic. vestigated to our knowledge, or at least not with children
There has been considerable debate among social and and youth; it would appear that tolerance for ambiguity
developmental psychologists as to whether empathy is a would also be moderated by self-regulatory abilities and
mediator of altruistic and prosocial behavior. Readers cognitive perspective-taking skill. Indeed, Eisenberg
are referred to Eisenberg (2003), Eisenberg, Fabes, and and colleagues (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1996) provide data
Spinrad (Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume; Eisen- that show, for children to experience sympathy rather
berg & Fabes, 1998) for a review of the issues. Prosocial than personal distress in an emotion-evocative situation,
behavior can occur simply from wanting to be sociable they need to be capable of neuro-physiological regula-
with others, not necessarily from being empathic to tion, use attentional control processes, accurately ap-
their feelings (Dunn, 1988). However, Roberts and praise emotion-eliciting events, infer others internal
Strayer (1996) present fairly persuasive evidence that emotional states, and cope with situational demands.
empathy, in conjunction with being able to take the per- We turn next to a more in-depth discussion of individual
spective of others, does predict prosocial behavior to differences in empathy and sympathy, and their relation
some degree. to socially adaptive behavior in children and youth.
What seems critical to address in empathy research
and its influence on childrens and youths relationships Disposition for Over-Arousal. A significant con-
is how it combines with a sense of values to predict so- tributor to experiencing personal distress rather than
cially responsible behavior that is accompanied by a the more functional sympathetic response is the dispo-
sense of compassion. Children who feel a sense of re- sition to experience emotional over-arousal and more
sponsibility to help others are among those most likely intense levels of vicariously induced negative emotion
to behave prosocially (Chapman, Zahn-Waxler, Cooper- (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1988). These researchers found
man, & Iannotti, 1987). There is also research that that heart rate acceleration was associated with a per-
demonstrates that failures in empathy are implicated in sonal distress response, but sympathy co-occurred with
adolescents with conduct disorders (D. Cohen & heart rate deceleration. In the latter study, childrens
Strayer, 1996). Arsenio and Lemerise (2001) are partic- heart rate and facial expressions were better predictors
ularly vocal in their call for the need to address moral of their subsequent helpful overtures than their
values in research on aggression, and one of the pivotal self-report.
mediators between values and prosocial behavior may
well prove to be empathy. Early Parental Attunement. While there may
We need to know more about the sources of individ- also be genetic and biological contributions to individ-
ual differences in the capacity for empathic engagement ual differences in empathic responsiveness and sympa-
without becoming overwhelmed by ones own personal thy (reviewed in Eisenberg, 2003), we concur with the
distress. One possibility is how children develop a toler- position taken by Zahn-Waxler (1991), who argued that
ance for ambiguity. Would those children and youth who the origins of prosocial and altruistic behavior are to be
can tolerate ambiguity to greater extent also be those found in the dynamic emotional exchanges of the attach-
children who turn out to have a higher threshold for tol- ment relationship between parent and infant. She notes
erating anothers intense distress without being over- that the processes of joint attention, social exchange,
whelmed by it and becoming personally distressed and cooperative turn taking between caregiver and in-
themselves? Social psychological research indicates that fant create a world of shared meaning, empathic under-
when people are distressed, they are more likely to im- standing and appropriate linking of ones own emotions
pulsively indulge themselves in something pleasurable to with those of others that then generalize beyond the par-
make themselves feel better, but what they indulge ent-child dyad (p. 156). In sum, the emotional attune-
themselves in is not likely to be prosocial (Tice, Brat- ment between parent and baby is essentially the crucible
264 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

in which empathy and concern for others well-being parental behaviors (e.g., displays of anger, contempt,
are forged. criticism, domineering, threats, stonewalling) shown to-
Related to this early crucible as the foundation for ward the child. When the parents were unable to provide
empathy is that for the most part it is mothers whose at- the child with sensitive and constructive ways to handle
tunement with their infant is evaluated. Indeed, women anger, their children tended to carry grudges and de-
undertake the majority of nurturing and caring for oth- velop covert antisocial behavior (e.g., retaliation). The
ers in close relationships. Zahn-Waxler and Robinson authors also found that the children displayed relatively
(1995) noted that girls are socialized to be attuned more less fear and sadness, and they suggested that a deficit in
to their relations with others and to feel responsible for those more vulnerable feelings might also be associated
others well-being. When feeling responsible for others with a deficit in empathy in these angry, belittled chil-
is conjoined with discomfort, guilt and empathy may dren. Their use of hazard analyses (essentially analyz-
merge together, particularly if family socialization pat- ing the risk of recurrence rates of childrens anger
terns have led children to acquire an overgeneralized displays during parent-child interaction as a function of
sense of responsibility for others well-being. They ar- parents negative emotion) to infer the likelihood of sub-
gued that young girls are more likely to develop this pat- sequent emotional responses also constituted a method-
tern and consequently may become more vulnerable to ological innovation in how to examine the mutually
establishing beliefs about their over-responsibility, un- influencing effects of parent and child when in an imme-
worthiness, and blameworthiness for the problems of diate interaction.
others (Zahn-Waxler & Robinson, 1995, p. 165). Such Strayer and Roberts (2004b) also investigated more
self-attributions are evident in depression, and, by early intensively how childrens anger and aggression might
adolescence, girls exceed boys in incidence of depres- be related to their empathy. They observed the play be-
sion. It appears that a healthy dose of self-interest may havior of 5-year-olds and found that whereas empathy
inoculate young girls against such feelings, and a mascu- accounted for 18% of the variance in observed anger
line sex-role orientation (regardless of ones gender) ap- and aggression, it only accounted for 8% of the variance
pears to protect against depression as it is associated in prosocial behavior. Thus, empathic children are less
with assertive self-interest (see also Ruble & Martin, angry, less verbally and physically aggressive, and were
1998; Zahn-Waxler, 2000). involved in fewer object struggles with their peers. In
terms of social effectiveness and the ability to be em-
Inf luence of Socialization. A recent multi-method, pathically engaged, this study suggests that more har-
multi-source study on how parents might influence their monious relationships with others are clearly associated
childrens empathy was carried out by Strayer and with higher levels of empathy.
Roberts (2004a) who found paths between parents empa- In terms of sympathy, modeling by the parents ap-
thy and their childrens empathy, but the relationship was pears to be an important factor in children showing more
mediated by childrens anger. More specifically, em- sympathy (see review by Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, &
pathic parents had children who were less angry and who Karbon., 1992). Adult women who report empathy after
also demonstrated more empathy. Parents who were low watching distressing films also describe their families
in empathy were also more controlling, and they subse- of origin in ways that indicate that positive emotions and
quently had children who were more angry and less sympathetic and vulnerable feelings were freely ex-
empathic. It was the childs anger and the parents con- pressed. Along similar lines, in the investigation under-
trolling disciplinary style and associated parenting prac- taken by Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Carlo, and Miller
tices that appeared to mediate the relationship between (1991), parents attitudes were assessed using the
parent and child empathy. Parental Attitude toward Childrens Expressiveness
This relationship between parental insensitivity and Scale (PACES; Saarni, 1990), which was modified for
negative reactions toward their child and subsequent in- use with preschoolers. Parents of children who reported
effective social behavior in the child was also substanti- restrictive attitudes toward their childrens emotional
ated by Snyder, Stoolmiller, Wilson, and Yamamoto displays had children who seemed more inclined to ex-
(2003). In their observational study, they found that perience personal distress rather than sympathetic con-
childrens anger was associated with frequency of cern when describing their reaction to anothers
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 265

distress. This effect was more noticeable when the par- berg and McNalley (1993) examined relations between
ents espoused controlling beliefs about their childrens mothers child-rearing practices over an 8-year period
emotional displays, even when the emotional displays and their adolescents (age 15 to 16) perspective taking
simply expressed the childs own vulnerable feelings and vicariously induced emotion. They found that
(e.g., sadness, anxiety) as opposed to showing ones gen- mothers who expressed positive emotions and minimal
uine feelings without regard for whether they could also negative affect with their children were more likely to
hurt someone elses feelings (e.g., showing annoyance have sympathetic daughters and sons who scored lower
toward a well-intentioned gift giver). Parents who re- in personal distress. Warm maternal communication
stricted their childrens emotional displays in circum- was also associated with increased perspective taking
stances where others feelings might be hurtbut not in youth of both genders. However, mothers own sym-
when their children expressed vulnerable feelingsap- pathy was not significantly related to their adolescents
peared to have more sympathy-oriented children. The sympathy.
most impacted children appeared to be boys of mothers The last study was undertaken by Koestner, Franz,
who endorsed controlling attitudes about their sons dis- and Weinberger (1990) with adults who had been partic-
play of emotions in situations where only self-related ipants in a longitudinal study since their preschool
vulnerable feelings were involved; these boys were the years. Empathic concern at age 31 was most strongly re-
most likely to show personal distress reactions. lated to the following variables assessed when the par-
Recently, Valiente et al. (2004) examined the rela- ticipants were 5 years old: Fathers involvement in child
tions between parental expressive style, childrens abil- care, mothers tolerance of their childrens dependency,
ity to access effortful control, and the childrens inhibition of childrens aggression, and satisfaction with
likelihood of responding with empathy. They found that the role of mother (fathers had not been asked about
children prone to personal distress were more likely to their satisfaction with their parental role, but their high
have low effortful control; in contrast, children who involvement with their children would seem to show that
could access greater effortful control were more likely they were). The authors conclusion is worth quoting:
to respond with sympathy rather than with personal dis- children are most likely to grow up to be empathically
tress. Several complex relationships were found with concerned adults when both of their parents enjoyed
parental expressive style, and the authors concluded that being involved with them and when their affiliative and
a high level of parental negative expressive style was as- aggressive needs were differentially responded to, with
sociated with their childrens likelihood of experiencing the former being permitted and encouraged and the lat-
personal distress, irrespective of their childrens at- ter inhibited (p. 714).
tempts at effortful control. We infer from this research
that parental derogation (as indexed here by a negative Summary
expressive style) undermines childrens abilities to regu- Empathy and its derivative, sympathy, are critical to
late their empathic response such that they experience emotional communication; indeed, responsiveness to
personal distress rather than being able to invoke effort- others emotions is critical to human evolution (e.g.,
ful control and transform their empathy into sympa- Cosmides & Tooby, 2000). The development of empathy
thetic behavior toward a distressed victim. Research by such that it becomes linked with altruistic, prosocial be-
Gottman et al. (1997) yielded similar results: Parental havior obviously promotes the well-being of those who
derogation of the child ill prepares the child for harmo- need support or help, but it also facilitates the well-
nious peer relationships or, for that matter, academic being of individuals who respond sympathetically. The
achievement. Indeed, Raver (2002) in a Social Policy preceding research suggests that such individuals enjoy
Report for the Society for Research on Child Develop- more favorable relations with their peers, may them-
ment also called for intervention at the levels of family, selves be more effective parents, and are able to regulate
community, and child-care setting to facilitate young their emotional arousal such that they can effectively in-
childrens emotional development so that their school tervene to assist another. The reviewed research also in-
readiness would be enhanced. dicates that complex, often indirect, relationships exist
A pair of longitudinal studies that examined empa- between parenting behaviors and childrens empathic,
thy and family influence are worth mentioning. Eisen- sympathetic, and prosocial behavior toward others.
266 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Social Effectiveness and Skill 5: Skill in leagues (1997) demonstrated in their longitudinal re-
Differentiating Internal Emotional Experience search that children who were most effective with their
from External Emotional Expression peers knew how to regulate their external expressive be-
havior, but we contend that children believe that it is
Whether a person is trying to protect his or her vulnera- important to find an adaptive balance between self-
bility, enhance some advantage to her- or himself, or presentation, which may require dissemblance as well as
promote the well-being of another about whom they genuine display of emotion. An early interview study in-
care, being able to monitor individual emotional-expres- dicated that school-age children believed that the display
sive behavior and action tendencies strategically is of genuine emotion was as regulated as the display of
adaptive, and children learn to do so with increasing fi- dissembled emotion (Saarni, 1989b). In older age
nesse as they mature (Saarni, 1989, 1999). In the fol- groups, children nominated more reasons or occasions
lowing discussion, this discrepancy between internal for when it would be appropriate to express ones gen-
emotional state and external emotional expression is re- uine emotional response. In descending order of fre-
ferred to as emotional dissemblance. The term emotion quency, the categories for when it would be appropriate
management will also be used to refer to childrens reg- to express ones genuine emotional response were: (a) if
ulating their experience of emotion by monitoring their the emotion was very intense; ( b) if one was sick, in-
expressive behavior. This last topic is also a significant jured, or bleeding; (c) if one was with certain people,
link to the discussion of Skill 6 involving childrens cop- such as parents or friends (note the close relationship di-
ing with aversive emotions in social contexts. mension here); (d) if special or unusual misfortunes oc-
By the preschool years, if not earlier, young children curred such as being in a fire; (e) if one was in a special
learn how to introduce disparities between their internal setting that allowed for the display of genuine emotion,
emotional state and their external expressive behavior. such as in an amusement park or while attending a hor-
Such discrepancies indicate that young children have ror movie; (f ) if one was a young child; (g) if one was
begun to differentiate their inner emotional experience being scolded or had just been caught doing something
from what they express in their behaviorespecially to wrong; and finally ( h) if one had been unjustly accused,
others. Perhaps the earliest form of this differentiation one should show how one felt about it.
between internal state and external expression is the ex- The pattern of responses indicated that with increas-
aggeration of emotional-expressive behavior to gain ing age children demonstrated increased flexibility in
someones attention (a trivial injury becomes the occa- the deployment of emotional-expressive behavior,
sion to howl loudly and solicit comfort and attention). whether it was dissembled or genuine in display. Their
An early observational study by Blurton-Jones (1967) expectations also suggest the implicit use of social
reported that children, ages 3 to 4, in a free-play situa- scripts that take into account age status and relation-
tion were more likely to cry after injuring themselves if ships (items f and c, respectively), setting (item e), and
they noticed a caregiver looking at them; they were less implied lack of controllability (items a, b, and d). The
likely to cry if they thought they were unattended. Mini- last two categories (items g and h) suggest interesting
mization may be the next to appear; it consists of damp- relationship contexts that appear to be associated with
ening the intensity of emotional-expressive behavior, childrens recognition that their expressive behavior can
despite feeling otherwise. Neutralization describes the influence the affective and cognitive states of those who
adoption of a poker face, but it is probably relatively are either scolding them or unjustly accusing them. That
difficult to carry off, and, indeed, in early research by topic has little empirical research associated with it
Ekman and Friesen (1975) it was found that substitution among older childrenwith several important excep-
of another expression that differs from what one gen- tions (Fuchs & Thelen, 1988; Saarni, 1992; Shipman,
uinely feels is probably a more successful strategy (e.g., Zeman, Nesin, & Fitzgerald, 2003). Research under-
smiling despite feeling anxious). taken by Shipman and Zeman (2001) indicated that chil-
Children learn to manage their expressive behavior by dren do indeed have expectations that their
taking into account relationship dimensions such as emotional-expressive displays will be responded to sup-
closeness of relationship, power or status similarity/dif- portively by others, especially parents or adult care-
ference, and the degree to which they are exposed (e.g., givers. Shipman et al. (2003) also found in their
public versus private situations). Gottman and his col- interview study that younger elementary school children
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 267

(7 years old) were more likely than older children (10 the salience of authority and possible risk if anger were
years old) to believe their expressive behaviors would be to be directly expressed. Underwood (1996) also found
acceptable and would elicit support. Interestingly, in that age differences in reporting dissemblance of emo-
this research, which used vignettes as the vehicle for tion varied according to the emotion felt, with older
eliciting childrens expectations, there was little differ- children reporting greater likelihood of masking disap-
ence whether the expressive behavior would be directed pointment and blunting of very positive affect and
at mother, father, or best friend (although for the latter, younger children (8 years) more likely to mask anger.
children thought there might be more tolerance of ag- Across emotion, girls expected more negative reactions
gressive displays). from peers to mismanaged emotional-expressive be-
havior than boys did, and children generally expected
Components of Emotional Dissemblance
less positive peer response to extremely honest emo-
As summarized some time ago by Shennum and Bugen- tional displays (see also Saarni, 1988).
tal (1982), children gradually acquire knowledge about Parker and her colleagues (2001) examined childrens
when, where, with whom, and how to express behav- knowledge of dissemblance strategies for anger in a hy-
iorally their emotional reactions. They also need to have pothetical vignette about unfair treatment of one child
the ability to control the skeletal muscles involved in by another and then compared their conceptual knowl-
emotional-expressive behavior. They need to have the edge with their actual behavior in playing a competitive
motivation to manage their emotional-expressive behav- game with an unfamiliar peer confederate in which they
ior in the appropriate situations. They also need to have were unfairly made to lose and the confederate overtly
reached a certain complexity of cognitive representation. cheated. Relative to the vignette, childrens strategies
We address each of these components in turn as they are for dissembling angry expressive behavior were coded as
reflected in recent research. behavioral (e.g., leave the situation, cover their face) or
as cognitive (e.g., focus their mind on a happier event).
Knowledge. As noted earlier, children can readily The investigators also monitored whether children who
nominate reasons for showing their genuine emotions to had been nominated by their peers as aggression prone
others, and indeed, across all ages, the most common would behave any differently during the unfair game.
reason cited for when genuine emotions would be ex- Their results were complex, but essentially what they
pressed was if they were experienced as very intense found was that children reported they would feel an-
(i.e., and thus less controllable; Saarni, 1979). School- grier, be more likely to express their anger, and be less
age children can also nominate reasons for dissembling likely to dissemble their anger in the hypothetical story
their expressive behavior, and that early study found that in contrast to what they reported and expressed after
the majority of their reasons referred to wanting to avoid having lost to the cheating confederate in the unfair
embarrassment or derision from others for revealing game (similar pattern of results obtained by R. Casey,
vulnerable emotions such as hurt or fear. Getting atten- 1993). The children rated as aggression prone did report
tion, making someone feel sorry for you, and getting feeling angrier and their displays were more intense and
help were also among the reasons mentioned for dissem- lasted longer during the unfair game, but otherwise
bling emotional expressive behavior (cf. Shipman et al., there was little to distinguish them from the nonaggres-
2003). The older children were more likely to make ref- sive children (i.e., both groups of children had similar
erence to the degree of affiliation with an interactant, understanding of dissemblance strategies).
status differences, and controllability of both emotion Another interesting study examined childrens ability
and circumstances as contextual qualities that affected to conceptualize the difference between internal emo-
the genuine or dissembled display of emotion. tion state and facial expression and how this conceptual-
Other research has elaborated on these contextual in- ization might be related to their miniaturization of
fluences in childrens understanding of emotional-ex- emotional expression when alone versus when engaged
pressive behavioral management. For example, interpersonally (Holodynski, 2004). Children between
Underwood, Coie, and Herbsman (1992) found that ele- the ages of 6 and 8 were exposed to a slot machine that
mentary school children reported that they would be dispensed a package of candy or an empty package.
more likely to mask angry expressive behavior with They coped with the capricious slot machine in two
their teachers than with their peers, thus recognizing conditions: (1) alone or (2) accompanied by an adult
268 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

research assistant. They were also extensively inter- sample produced scary faces instead (as though getting
viewed about their understanding of discrimination be- ready for Halloween).
tween expression and feeling, with a summary score A more recent study examined childrens ability to
denoting the degree of conceptual complexity in making control their facial muscles when asked to suppress ex-
this discrimination. Holodynski argued that the minia- pressions of pleasure, which was elicited by a funny rou-
turization of expressive behavior when alone emerges at tine acted out by a clown (Ceschi & Scherer, 2003).
around 6 years of age as children learn to self-regulate, Seven- and 10-year-olds were interviewed about their
similar to the earlier transition from private speech to knowledge of emotional expression control strategies
internal speech serving to facilitate self-regulation and then divided into two groups: one group saw the
(Berk, 1992). Indeed, his results revealed that his oldest clown routines without any instruction to suppress their
age group (8 years old) reliably reduced the intensity of expression and the second group were asked to try to
their expressions when alone as opposed to being with conceal their amusement during the clown routine. Both
the research assistant; the 6-year-olds revealed similarly groups apparently found the routine fairly intensely
intense expressions across both conditions. Likewise, he amusing, and thus the second groups ability to suppress
found a significant and positive correlation between their mirth was limited. They were able to reduce the
complexity of conceptualization about emotion state duration of their positive expressive behaviors, but not
and expression and subsequent miniaturization of ex- the frequency. They did try to use control behaviors such
pressive behavior when alone and coping with the capri- as pressing their lips together and showed more false
cious slot machine. This research is very thought smiles. Noteworthy is that the children were not alone:
provoking, because Holodynski may have provided the both the clown and the experimenter were present (cf.
explanation for some of the discrepancies found in re- Holodynskis research, 2004). There was no noteworthy
search on childrens expression of emotion when faced age difference in expressive behavior in the two groups
with different emotion-eliciting circumstances. As or in the knowledge of emotion control strategies. The
noted earlier, the degree of exposure, alone or engaged knowledge of emotion control strategies also did not pre-
with others, affects emotional-expressive behavior in dict either frequency or duration of expressive control
ways that can evoke expression amplification when with strategies in the suppression group. However, Ceschi
supportive others (Holodynskis study) or expressive and Scherer did confirm that those control strategies
dissemblance when faced with disagreeable confeder- that were used (false smiling, lip press, lip suck /pucker)
ates (the Parker et al. study, 2001; see also Underwood, were all in the lower part of the face, and muscle con-
Hurley, Johanson, & Mosley, 1999). tractions in the upper part of the face and around the
eyes were not influenced by the suppression condition.
Ability to Implement Emotional Dissemblance. They also suggested that evaluating childrens knowl-
Control of skeletal muscles, especially in the face, is edge of emotion control strategies might be better done
critical to being able to modify ones emotional-expres- by having them engage in a recognition task rather than
sive behavior and thus dissemble the outward expression having to freely produce or nominate their control
of ones emotional response. Children become capable strategies.
of this modification voluntarily at a young age (2 to 3
years), and it is readily apparent in their pretend play; Motivation. One of us has investigated childrens
for example, they mimic postures, expressions, vocal knowledge of how to manage emotional-expressive be-
qualities, and the like of assorted fantasy characters. havior and their expectations about what motivated
However, when it comes to deliberately adopting emo- story characters to undertake such management strate-
tional expressions, posing of facial expressions proves to gies (Saarni, 1979). When the children were asked to ex-
be difficult, especially negatively toned expressions plain why the story characters emotional reaction had
(e.g., M. Lewis, Sullivan, & Vasen, 1987). The diffi- not been genuinely expressed, four broad categories of
culty in posing fear, disgust, sadness, and the like may motivation were apparent in their responses. These four
be due to the fairly consistent socialization pressure in motivation categories are elaborated as follows:
our culture to inhibit negative displays of emotion. As
Lewis et al. point out, when asked to produce a scared 1. Avoidance of negative situational outcomes or en-
face (i.e., looking afraid), the young children in their hancement of positive situational outcomes: This com-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 269

mon motive is well illustrated in a study by Davis provided a review of deception and its links to self-
(1995). She had children play a game in which a desir- presentation in relationships; although their review fo-
able prize and an undesirable one were placed in two cuses on adults, it is suggestive for how research could be
boxes, visible only to the child. The children were told to extended to children and youth and framed in a develop-
deceive the experimenter by pretending to like both mental inquiry.
prizes, and if they succeeded in tricking the experi- 3. Maintenance or enhancement of relationships and
menter to believe they really liked both, they would be concern for others well-being: As an illustration of this
able to keep both prizes. If they did not succeed, then the last motive for emotional dissemblance, von Salisch
experimenter took both prizes. Thus, for the children to (1991, 1996) probed how children actually regulated a
get the attractive prize, they had to persuasively manage relationship by monitoring what they expressed. She de-
their expressive behavior so as to look positive for both veloped a computer game that was rigged: The computer
attractive and unattractive prizes. The results showed was cast as the opponent and two children were to
that the girls were more successful at suppressing nega- play as a team. If the airplane crashed on the screen, it
tive expressive behaviors toward the unattractive prize meant the children had lost; however, its demise was
than the boys. The girls also revealed a greater number random but appeared to the children to have been caused
of social monitoring behaviors (e.g., rapid glancing at by one of them. The participating children were 11-
the experimenter) as well as tension behaviors (e.g., years old, and she was able to have the pairs consist of
touching ones face), and they appeared to monitor the either best friends or of casual acquaintances. In her
social exchange more closely than the boys, which may analyses of the actual conflict episodes, the most fre-
have facilitated their expression management. Davis quent expressive behavior was smiling, followed by signs
concludes that girls do have more ability in managing of tension, then contempt, and last by anger (only 3% of
the expression of their negative emotions, and she sug- the expressions). In many cases, the children also ver-
gests that individual differences (e.g., temperament) balized reproaches about the crash, but then accompa-
may interact with sex-role socialization to yield the gen- nied the reproach by smiling. With close friends, the
der pattern she observed. incidence of smiling was even greater than with ac-
2. Protection of ones self-esteem: Meerum Terwogt quaintances, and among girls in close friend pairs, gen-
and Olthof (1989) found that boys were reluctant to ex- uine smiles were especially notable in their reciprocity,
press fear because they worried they would be viewed as even through these girls more frequently verbalized
cowards by their peers. Fuchs and Thelen (1988) also re- their negative feelings about their friends game-playing
ported that boys were loathe to reveal their sadness to skill (or ostensible lack thereof ). The boys in close
their fathers but might consider doing so with their friendship pairs tended to verbalize less, but they
mothers. Maintenance of self-image appeared to be the showed more signs of tension than any other group. In
chief motive for these boys and emotion management essence, these preadolescent boys and girls used their
was sought by adoption of a stoic emotional front. smiles to reassure their friend that the relationship was
DePaulo (1991) reviewed the earlier literature on what still on firm ground, despite their reproaching their
is known about the development of self-presentation, em- friend for their incompetence in making them lose the
phasizing nonverbal behavior. One point she made was game against the computer. Expressive behavior has
that as children grow older, more of their peers and adult among its functions more than simply the display of
networks hold them accountable for being able to regulate emotion; it is also a social message. What von Salischs
and manage their emotional-expressive behavior. Thus, research shows us is that children are adept at using this
there is a continual reinforcement of motivation to man- social function of emotional-expressive behavior to
age how one presents oneself to others. There is a great manage their relationships, and they do so in a discrimi-
need for further research on how childrens needs to self- nating fashion.
protect or to enhance their self-image in the eyes of oth- 4. Observance of norms and conventions: These are
ers are linked to emotion dissemblance strategies and the cultural display rules that provide us with consensu-
self-presentation, but this topic, relative to children, has ally agreed on scripts for how to manage our emotions.
been underresearched in recent years. More common is A couple of 9- to 10-year-old childrens responses illus-
research on lying and deception (e.g., Talwar & Lee, trate their notions of what are norms for emotional dis-
2002). Anderson, Ansfield, and DePaulo (1999) have semblance: You should smile when you get a gift, even
270 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

if you dont like it much, You shouldnt yell at a Harris and Gross (1988) examined young childrens ra-
grown-up, and you should apologize, even though you tionales for why story characters would conceal their
dont feel like it. It is probably noteworthy that cultural emotions by adopting misleading facial expressions. A
display rules often have shoulds associated with them significant number of the 6-year-olds interviewed gave
(e.g., Gnepp & Hess, 1986). At least a couple of factors very complex justifications that included describing the
might account for why children do not consistently per- intent to conceal their feelings and to mislead another to
form cultural display rule scripts, despite knowing believe something other than what was really being emo-
them: First, the social stakes may not be sufficiently tionally experienced (e.g., she didnt want her sister to
high for them to feel motivated to do so; second, their know that she was sad about not going to the party).
distressed, hurt, or angry emotional responses may be Children younger than age 6 can readily adopt pretend
experienced as too intense to allow for emotional dis- facial expressions, but they are not likely to be able to
semblance. As mentioned earlier (Saarni, 1989b), inten- articulate the embedded relationships involved in delib-
sity of emotional response was cited by school-age erate emotional dissemblance. Developmental research
children as the chief reason for when emotions would be that illustrates childrens understanding of false beliefs
genuinely expressed. Research by Garner and Power and social perspective taking and the relation between
(1996) also indicated that emotional intensity as a tem- these two cognitive markers and coordinated communi-
perament factor may influence the likelihood of adopt- cation with a playmate was undertaken by Slomkowki
ing emotional dissemblance in certain situations. and Dunn (1996). They suggested that these cognitive
aptitudes are indicative of childrens abilities to read
These four categories for why we may be motivated to each other as they regulate and coordinate their con-
dissemble the expression of our emotional responses are versations and behavior with their peers. Harris (1998)
not necessarily exhaustive nor are they mutually exclu- also reviewed the relevant literature on emotional ap-
sive, but they all have one significant feature in common: praisal as it relates to young childrens pretend play, and
They are concerned with interpersonal consequences, one of the features that he emphasized was young chil-
and it is the varying nature of these social consequences drens attributions of agency to make-believe others and
that yields the differences among motives. Even the self- their appraisal of fictional material in ways that induces
esteem motive for dissemblance does not occur in a so- emotional experience. In his words, the creation and
cial vacuum, for the self is embedded in a history of consumption of fictional worlds is a pervasive human
social relationships. enterprise (Harris, p. 353) and is sustained by our en-
gagement with emotional experiences derived by our ap-
praisal of imaginary events. By the 2nd and 3rd year of
Cognitive Representation. As suggested by life, normal children show this capacity for imagination
Josephss (1994) research, a pragmatic or implicit and fantasy play, thus indicating that critical cognitive
knowledge of emotional dissemblance is likely to pre- skills are developing that also become part of their cog-
cede an articulated and verbalized understanding of ex- nitive repertoire for representing what is real and what
pressive dissimulation. In the theory of mind may be dissembled and being able to attribute agency
literature, a large body of research has emerged con- and emotionally significant meaning to both the genuine
cerned with childrens understanding of real versus ap- emotion and the dissembled expression.
parent phenomena, and this distinction has been applied
to inner emotional state as real and external expres-
Summary
sive behavior as apparent. By school entry, children
generally understand that how one looks on ones face is A persons ability to maneuver emotional-expressive
not necessarily how one feels on the inside (e.g., Harris, behavior according to interpersonal contexts and emo-
1989; Harris & Gross, 1988). Thus, relatively young tional responses gives them a rich repertoire of commu-
children understand that the appearance of ones facial nicative behavior. The intermingling of emotional
expression can be misleading about the actual emotional experience and social interaction is also evident in chil-
state experienced. By age 6, many children can provide drens acquisition of emotional dissemblance and emo-
justifications for how appearances can conceal reality, tion management strategies. There are highly adaptive
in this case, the genuine emotion felt by an individual. and functional reasons for humans to be able to dissoci-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 271

ate their emotional-expressive behavior from their in- ticular, the distinction should be made between emotion
ternally felt, subjective emotional experience: One is regulating something else (e.g., anothers social re-
being able to have reasonably satisfactory relationships sponse to the self ) and emotion itself being regulated
with others; another is to be able to get others to provide (e.g., self-soothing). As noted earlier, emotion regula-
support and validation for oneself; still another is to tion is dynamically integral to emotion generation itself,
exert ones influence on othersas in impression man- and the complexity of feedback in emotion processes
agement, persuasive communication, and the like. A will occupy researchers for some time to come. However,
reason that children are particularly likely to endorse is for the purposes of linking emotion regulation with so-
that it helps one to avoid getting into trouble, and last, cial effectiveness, we emphasize that being able to mod-
the omnipresent self-appraisal system has its antennae ulate ones degree of emotional arousal facilitates ones
out to try to create experiences that strengthen or pro- coping with an environmental stressor or conflict. This
tect the self rather than undermine it. Coping effec- does not mean simple inhibition; amplification of emo-
tively with interpersonal conflict and other situational tional experience and expressive display may also be
stressors has much to do with how we regulate both our strategically effective in a particular situation.
subjective experience of emotion as well as with what Research on childrens emotion regulation may take
we communicate expressively to others. into account:

Temperamental reactivity (see Rothbart & Bates, 1998)


Social Effectiveness and Skill 6: Skill in
Adaptive Coping with Aversive Emotions and Processes that involve deployment of attention (in-
Distressing Circumstances cluding effortful control as operationally defined by
Eisenberg and colleagues; e.g., Valiente et al., 2003)
Accumulated research indicates that adaptive coping re- Components of emotion (physiological, expressive,
quires at least three conditions to be met: (1) regulation and subjective experience)
of ones emotional arousal, (2) adequate appraisal of the
Approach/avoidance tendencies, whereby the latter is
problematic situation and what is realistically under
understood to include individual differences in inhibi-
ones control, and (3) resolution that yields a sense of
tion and niche picking (i.e., seeking out situations
mastery and/or resilience (e.g., Aldwin, 1994; Compas,
in which desired emotional experiences are likely and
Connor, Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, 1999;
avoiding those situations in which aversive emotions
Wolchik & Sandler, 1997). We address each of these
are likely to be evoked; see Campos et al., 2004)
three conditions relative to how they contribute to social
effectiveness. In this section, we also briefly comment
on research that raises interesting questions about so- Brenner and Saloveys (1997) definition of emotion reg-
cialization influence and context effects on the develop- ulation combines these elements: It is the relative capac-
ment of adaptive coping. ity to manage ones emotional reactivity (including
intensity and duration of arousal) such that alterations
in ones physiological-biochemical system, behavioral-
Emotion Regulation
expressive system, and experiential-cognitive system
The issues surrounding emotion regulation recur in this are affected. We also add the emphasis of the relational
chapter for good reason: The topic is theoretically rich and functionalist perspective used in this chapter such
with possibilities for understanding emotion processes that emotional regulatory processes should be under-
and for clinical and educational application. We ad- stood as occurring in contexts construed as personally
dressed previously some of the contemporary research meaningful to the individual. Finally, optimal emotion
on emotion regulation and implications for social com- regulation over time should also contribute to a sense of
petence, and we elaborate further some of the current well-being or emotional equilibrium, a sense of self-
research in this area. The reader is referred to the spe- efficacy, and a sense of connectedness to others to the
cial issue on emotion regulation that appeared in Child extent that effective emotion regulation facilitates con-
Development (vol. 75, 2004) for thoughtful discussions structive problem-solving strategies and appropriate ap-
on how to conceptualize emotion regulation, and, in par- praisal of social contexts.
272 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Development of Emotion Regulation and Coping emotional-expressive signals has a long history of re-
search, for the most part with infants and their care-
Thompson et al. (2003) reviewed the maturing emotion givers (e.g., Gianino & Tronick, 1988; Trevarthen,
regulatory capacities of the infants nervous system and 1993). Less research has been undertaken with school-
concluded that during the 1st year, excitatory and in- age children, but a number of studies suggest that chil-
hibitory processes are stabilized so that infants gradually dren are well aware that their expressive displays
develop a greater ability to inhibit or minimize the inten- (self-presentation) influence their peers subsequent
sity and duration of emotional reactions, and at the same responses to them (e.g., Carlson Jones, Abbey, & Cum-
time they also acquire a greater diversity of emotional re- berland, 1998; Gottman, Guralnick, Wilson, Swanson,
sponses. Examples of early regulation of emotional & Murray, 1997; Halberstadt et al., 2001; Hubbard,
arousal are young infants soothing themselves through 2001; Parker et al., 2001; Saarni & Weber, 1999; von
sucking or withdrawal from excessive stimulation, but Salisch, 1996; Zeman & Shipman, 1997).
equally critical is that caregivers assist infants in learning
how to regulate their arousal through attending to their
The Influence of Temperament
infants distress and providing comfort. Caregivers also
regulate the situations that infants are exposed to so that The notion of temperament is a multifaceted one and
infants emotional experience is moderated. Thompson fraught with many definitional and measurement prob-
has also argued that parents emotion regulatory inter- lems (e.g., Rothbart & Bates, 1998, and Chapter 3, this
ventions may, over time, contribute significantly to their Handbook, this volume), but it is a useful construct for
childrens style of emotion regulation. His illustration is thinking about what are some of the influences on how
that of parents who wait until their childs upset has esca- children develop different styles of emotion regulation
lated to high levels before they intervene. The effect is and coping. Temperament may be viewed as a collection
that they reinforce their childs rapid rise time of distress of dispositions that characterize the individuals style in
and high intensity of responding, which in turn makes it responding to environmental change (or the lack thereof ).
harder for the parents to soothe their infant due to his or These dispositions include reactivity, arousability, and
her high level of emotional arousal. temporal dimensions such as latency of response. Some
By toddlerhood, we can more readily see how emo- theorists also include as temperamental traits sociability,
tion regulation plays a mediating role between evoca- approach/avoidance tendencies, and degree of attentional
tive stressors and how young children cope with a control. Most theorists working with temperament regard
particular taxing situation: By regulating their emo- these dispositions as applicable to both emotional and
tional arousal, they can delay their reaction such that nonemotional behavior, and many theorists contend that
they may be able to adopt a different sort of coping be- temperamental dispositions have a biophysiological con-
havior than simply fleeing or lashing out at the stressor tribution that is influenced by ones genetic makeup.
(e.g., by age 2 children can approach a peer or small an- When we look at temperament as applied to how a per-
imal). However, emotion regulation, viewed from the son responds emotionally to evocative stimulation, we
standpoint of management of emotional-expressive be- can examine the intensity of emotional response ( both
havior, might in other contexts play a moderating role negative and positive valence), the threshold of arousal of
between the interpersonal circumstances one faces and emotional response, the duration (and other temporal as-
ones motives for a social outcome. For example, sus- pects) of the emotional response, and even the proclivity
taining the duration of the expressive display of happi- for what sort of hedonic tone of emotional response is
ness (a genuine smile) or amplifying ones smile generated (i.e., negative versus positive reactions to
influences the likelihood that ones interactant will re- change). The construct emotionality has typically been
spond positively in kind. Managing ones emotional- used to refer to temperaments influence on emotional
expressive behavior can be used to increase or decrease experience; high emotionality is often assumed to refer
the sorts of social interaction one desires with another to high intensity of emotional reaction, frequently com-
(e.g., see the discussion of emotion communication in bined with a negative hedonic tone ( but see Strelau,
interpersonal negotiation in Saarni & Buckley, 2002; 1987, for a review of six different definitions of emotion-
for preschool examples, see Barrett, 1997). Mutually ality, some of which do not associate a negative hedonic
regulating behaviors that involve the exchange of tone with high levels of emotional intensity of response).
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 273

Although temperament is a general proclivity in an thetical situations. Among their very complex results
individual to approach or withdraw from a novel situa- were that greater social competence of boys ( but not
tion and to react strongly or not to a novel situation, tem- girls) could be predicted by their displaying construc-
perament is not the same as the emotional response tive coping strategies (e.g., problem solving) and not
elicited in the individual that requires regulation, rather displaying excessive negative emotion. For girls, social
temperament can be thought of as the background on competence could be predicted from their use of
which the emotion plays out. Emotions are also highly avoidant coping strategies rather than by their engaging
contextualized and dependent on the individuals goals in acting out or conflict-escalating behaviors. For both
and motives of the moment (Campos et al., 1994; M. boys and girls, high emotional intensity was associated
Lewis, 1997); their nuances and variety are captured by with lower levels of constructive coping and with lower
the huge number of emotion-related words in the En- levels of attentional control (shifting and distractable
glish language (White, 2000). attention versus focused attention, as assessed by
Metaphorically, temperament is rather like a season teachers). In short, those 4- to 6-year-olds who fre-
of the year, but emotions are the mercurial weather con- quently showed high intensity negative emotions were
ditions that shift from day to day, demanding adjustment more likely to be distractable and to demonstrate less
and accommodation on a frequent basis. In this constructive coping. They were also regarded by their
metaphor, the season provides constraints on the daily teachers as less socially mature and by their peers as
weather, just as temperament may provide some degree less attractive as playmates.
of limitation on the experience of emotional response. In a second study with the same 4- to 6-year-olds,
Temperament influences emotion regulation as in figure- Eisenberg, Fabes, Nyman, Bernzweig, and Pinuelas
ground relationships; it is the ground, but emotions and (1994) investigated relations among temperament (more
their regulation are the figure. Thus, the child who is rel- specifically, emotionality, operationally defined as in-
atively inhibited in temperament (prone to withdrawal tensity of reaction and negative tone of emotion, and
from novel situations) and who tends to react strongly to their ability to control their attention), coping skills, and
novel situations ( less likely to modulate intensity) may their management of anger with their peers. The pattern
be the child who will more often experience emotions of of their findings was complex with some results occur-
anxiety, fear, and shame. But this same child may also ring only for teacher-rated behaviors but not for mother-
respond to familiar situations with pleasure, sympathy, rated behaviors. Gender of the child was again a variable
and caring. Indeed, we find that the more inhibited child that affected some of the patterning of results. Overall,
is also the one who is more compliant with parents and children whose temperament was characterized by low
internalizes a moral conscience more readily than those levels of emotionality, displayed reactions that were so-
young children characterized as more bold and active cially desirablethe children used nonhostile verbal
(reviewed in Kochanska & Thompson, 1997). strategies to try to deal with the anger provocation. This
Using temperament in this fairly global fashion as pattern was stronger for boys than girls, and boys who
having to do with how we dispositionally tend to modu- used socially desirable and constructive responses to
late our emotional reactions, we can examine how indi- anger were also rated as higher in attentional control.
vidual differences in temperament may influence Girls who tended to escape the situation when angered
coping efficacy. This approach was taken by Eisenberg were viewed by teachers as socially skilledthe girls
and her colleagues in several different research proj- avoidance of anger was apparently seen as not contribut-
ects on preschoolers coping efficacy relative to their ing to an escalation of conflict, a desirable outcome from
social functioning. In one investigation, Eisenberg et teachers standpoint. Although the authors were not
al. (1993) looked at 4- to 6-year-old children whose studying sex-role socialization, it is noteworthy that
temperament-influenced emotional intensity level was teachers approval of sex-typed behaviors (e.g., the girls
rated by both their mothers and their teachers and then avoidance of conflict), even at this relatively young age,
examined by the childrens social competence (teacher may be influencing childrens subsequent style of coping
ratings) and sociometric ratings (peer popularity). with such gender role-laden emotions as anger.
They also evaluated the childrens coping strategies by Although the construct of temperament allows us to
having the teachers and mothers rate the childrens consider what children might inherently bring with them
likelihood of using assorted coping strategies in hypo- as they seek to cope with stressful circumstances, it is
274 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

unlikely that temperament solely affects how construc- may indirectly affect childrens relations with peers,
tive ones coping style is. The social environment has but they do so by influencing childrens emotion regu-
also been modulating and giving meaning to the young lation ability.
childs emotional behavior all along, which includes Another study also examined maternal self-reported
such temperamental dimensions as intensity, hedonic acceptance or control of their childrens emotional ex-
tone, temporal factors, and the like. Cultures that value pressiveness as it related to their childrens attachment
expressive restraint might ascribe rather different mean- status and their emotion regulation in a frustrating
ings to, for example, intensity of emotional response, game (Beat the Bell). Berlin and Cassidy (2003) modi-
than cultures that do not have such an orientation. In the fied the Parent Attitude toward Child Expressiveness
end, adequacy of coping will be best determined by Scale (Saarni, 1985, 1990) for use with mothers of
whether individuals experience themselves as effica- preschoolers, who were participants in a longitudinal
cious in the sociocultural context in which they find study. As infants, the children had been classified in the
themselves. Strange Situation as securely attached, insecure-
avoidant, or insecure-anxious. Their results indicated
Effective and Ineffective Coping and Emotion that children with insecure-avoidant attachment classi-
Regulation with Peers and Family fications were more likely to suppress their anger and
We find that children who are good copers can mod- not share their sadness in the frustrating game with
ulate their emotions such that they can continue to at- their mothers, and their mothers reported that they
tend to what is going on socially (e.g., Gottman et al., were more controlling of their childrens emotional ex-
1997) and that they can avoid negative emotional esca- pressivity. Children with an insecure-anxious attach-
lation by circumventing problematic situations (Eisen- ment had mothers who reported significantly less
berg et al., 1993, 2004). Research has also shown that control of their childrens emotional expressivity.
children who enjoy substantial family support and mod- Mothers of securely attached children were moderate in
erate structure have a larger repertoire of coping strate- both their acceptance and control of their childrens
gies (Hardy, Power, & Jaedicke, 1993), and it makes emotional expressivity. Interestingly, no significant re-
sense that a broader repertoire of coping strategies per- lations were found between attachment classification
mits adaptation to a wider variety of emotion-provok- and childrens emotion regulation during the frustrat-
ing circumstances. ing Beat the Bell game. The authors speculated that this
One provocative study by Ramsden and Hubbard particular game may not have tapped into the emotion
(2002) described how parental coaching might affect regulation abilities of children that are associated with
childrens emotion regulation (as measured by the earlier infant-mother attachment.
Emotion Regulation Checklist; Shields & Cicchetti, Peer interaction has also been studied relative to
1997), which in turn was hypothesized to predict emotion regulation and coping. As an illustration,
childrens aggression at school. Their coding of Wilton, Craig, and Peplers (2000) research on victims
parental coaching of emotion included three dimen- of bullies determined that there appeared to be two pro-
sions: (1) acceptance of childs emotion, (2) awareness totypical coping styles when accosted by a bully in the
of childs emotion, and (3) instruction about emotion classroom. The first coping style consisted of a cluster
to the child. Their results indicated that the most ro- of behaviors that escalated the bullying episode: aggres-
bust link to aggression was the childs own emotion sive reactivity, expressive displays of anger, contempt,
regulation rating, but parental acceptance of child interest, and joy, and an emotion regulation disposition
emotion was positively related to higher emotion regu- to high intensity arousal of negative affect. This cluster
lation, and the overall degree of negative expressivity was found in 43% of the bullying episodes.
in the family (as measured by the Family Expressive- The second coping style was more oriented toward
ness Questionnaire; Halberstadt, 1986) influenced the problem solving and tended to de-escalate the bullying
childs emotion regulation rating in a negative way. episode. Fifty-two percent of the bullying episodes fell
Thus, family emotion processes, such as acceptance of in this second group. This cluster was subdivided into
childrens emotion, and the emotional climate of the two further patterns, which the authors referred to as
family, in this case, negative emotional expressivity, the passive problem-solving approach (84%) and the ac-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 275

tive problem-solving approach (only 16%). Both pat- ing that both are implicated when children use available
terns yielded either resolution or de-escalation of the strategies to manage stressful encounters. Coping re-
bullying, but with rather different outcomes: For the search has typically focused on strategies used to man-
passive problem solvers, there was essentially capitula- age stress-provoking experience or aversive emotions
tion to the bully or avoidance. The authors inferred that that are evoked by challenging circumstances. From this
such victims were primarily reacting with anxiety and perspective, coping follows emotion regulation in a tem-
sadness. The authors surmised that this submissive poral sense: first, a person modulates his or her emo-
stance would invite further bullying because the bully tional arousal and then seeks to resolve the stressful
gets his or her way. As for the active problem solvers, encounter to his or her benefit. However, given the dy-
they responded assertively and the bullying episodes namics of transactional and reciprocal relations in chil-
were resolved. What is unknown is whether these chil- drens and youths social worlds, it is unlikely that
dren effectively reduced their being threatened by the emotion regulation and coping are so simplistically dis-
classroom bullies over time. Last, the authors examined tinguished according to a linear temporal path (e.g.,
same gender and cross-gender bully victim interactions Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004; Lazarus, 1999).
and found no differences in the prevalence of these two Coping researchers often develop new categories to
general patterns in victim response. Given the recent at- which they assign various coping strategies: (a) Lazarus
tention to girls social and relational aggression (e.g., and Folkman (1984; Lazarus, 1999) proposed a basic di-
Underwood, 2003), we would be very interested in see- chotomy between emotion-focused coping and problem-
ing observational research that looks at coping styles of focused coping, ( b) others simply use approach (active)
the victims who are targeted by girls (and boys) for rela- versus avoidance (passive) to characterize coping ef-
tional aggression scapegoating. forts, or (c) primary versus secondary control coping
Research that links coping and emotion regulation strategies (Marriage & Cummins, 2004), or (d) engage-
with relationships among children and youth needs to ment versus disengagement coping (Ebata & Moos,
address the influence of context, for example, which 1991). Ayers and Sandler (1996) used factor analyses to
emotion is elicited under what sort of circumstances and distill a four-factor model of dispositional coping strate-
in what sort of relationship. Much of the existing re- gies that were also invariant across gender in a preado-
search is also based on hypothetical vignettes or on lescent sample; the factors were active, distraction,
adult ratings of childrens coping. Interestingly, in a re- avoidant, and support-seeking. Sandstrom (2004) com-
cent study that did compare observations of childrens bined various approaches to the assessment of childrens
anger management strategies with their verbal hypothet- coping strategies into a new instrument for evaluating
ical reports, children showed less anger in the live situa- how children cope specifically with peer rejection. Her
tion compared to what they reported they would express four coping categories for describing how children cope
in the hypothetical situation (Parker et al., 2001). Alter- with this socially aversive context included:
natively, Dearing, Hubbard, and Ramsden (2002) found
that children nominated by their peers as aggressive 1. Active coping (e.g., problem solving, assertiveness,
were more likely to handle roughly the materials of a getting help/support, constructive distraction, humor)
rigged game, yet their awareness or knowledge of anger 2. Aggressive coping (e.g., retaliation, teasing, arguing,
regulation strategies was not directly related to aggres- getting angry, getting others to turn against them)
sive status. 3. Denial coping (e.g., tell oneself it does not matter, do
not care, forget about it, ignore)
Coping Strategies
4. Ruminative coping (e.g., worry about it, withdraw,
It is not always clear whether coping is different from wish it were not happening)
emotion regulation. Some researchers refer to coping as
an aspect of self-regulation because effortful or pur- One of us (Saarni, 1997) examined childrens beliefs
poseful responses may be involved when one copes with about best and worst coping strategies. Five
a challenging situation (e.g., Compas, 1987). Other in- categories of coping strategies were compiled from
vestigators use the terms coping and emotion regulation the research literature, including problem solving,
interchangeably (e.g., Brenner & Salovey, 1997), argu- support- seeking (subdivided into solace-seeking and
276 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

help-seeking), distancing/avoiding, internalizing, and presentation strategies that garner social approval. They
externalizing. The children (ages 6 to 12) read five dif- expand their capacity to tolerate aversive emotion to the
ferent vignettes accompanied by schematic drawings degree that appraisal processes can be redirected and
and each vignette featured a negative emotion (e.g., thus reduce distress (e.g., Band & Weisz, 1988). If ap-
fear, hurt, anger, shame, or sadness). All vignettes praisal indicates that control over the situational stres-
were ascertained to have moderate controllability of sor or conflict is minimal or extremely risky, effective
outcome, featured the protagonist as experiencing emotional regulation may also involve distraction, cog-
moderately intense emotions, concerned peer inter- nitively reframing the meaning of the difficult situation,
action, and were gender neutral. The results indicated and use of cognitive blunting or sensitizing (Miller &
that the younger childrens justifications were less Green, 1985). Denial and dissociation appear to be less
complex for which coping strategy was nominated as adaptive coping strategies in that emotions are split off
best or worst, but there were no age differences in the from their eliciting context for short-term gain but at
choice of what was thought to be the best or worst long-term expense (e.g., Fischer & Ayoub, 1993).
strategy. Overall, children preferred beneficial coping Last, perceived control over the stressful situation is
strategies such as problem solving and support- relevant to how coping efforts are undertaken. As chil-
seeking, except for the vignette featuring hurt feel- dren mature, they become better able to distinguish un-
ings for which children picked the distancing strategy controllable stressors from controllable ones (Aldwin,
as the best. Aggressive externalizing coping re- 1994). For the uncontrollable situations, older children
sponses were most often selected as the worst option are more likely to nominate secondary control coping
across all emotion-category vignettes. For the most strategies, which include reframing, distraction, and
part, childrens justifications for their best coping avoidance through anticipatory planfulness (e.g., Mar-
choices emphasized the social situational gains, and riage & Cummins, 2004). Younger childrens avoidance
parallel losses were cited as justifications for the is more often of the escape sort such as hiding under the
worst coping choice (e.g., externalizing responses). bed to avoid an unpleasant event (Aldwin, 1994). A large
More research is needed to determine if children literature has developed examining the different coping
know what generally adaptive coping strategies are, and strategies mentioned earlier, and for further elaboration,
what gets in the way when they do not employ them. One the reader is referred to Aldwins (1994) volume on
possibility is self-appraisal, attribution of responsibility stress, coping, and development, and reviews by Compas,
for the outcome, and controllability (of ones own emo- Connor-Smith, Saltzman, and Thomsen (2001); Skinner
tion and the situational aspects) all interact to influence and Wellborn (1994); Wolchik and Sandler (1997); and
how children and youth cope in taxing circumstances. Wyman, Sandler, Wolchik, and Nelson (2000).
To add to this complexity is the role that temperament, Brenner and Salovey (1997) proposed a framework
especially proneness to negative emotionality and atten- for the analysis of coping that includes several impor-
tion deficits, plays in childrens ability to recruit adap- tant dimensions that are relevant to developmental
tive and socially effective coping strategies. change in coping strategy use: (a) the controllability of
the stressor, ( b) the degree to which the individual in-
Developmental Change in Coping Strategies vokes solitary strategies as opposed to socially interac-
In examining what changes about coping strategies as tive ones, and (c) the use of internal /intrapsychic
children mature, we find that although use of situation- strategies as opposed to situational-focused strategies.
oriented problem solving is accessible throughout child- We elaborate on the dimensions of solitary-social
hood, it becomes more targeted to the specific problem strategies and intrapsychic-situational strategies below
at hand, and childrens repertoire of problem-solving as they manifest themselves in children at different de-
strategies broadens with age (e.g., Altshuler & Ruble, velopmental stages.
1989). With age, childrens ability to consider a stressor The solitary-social dimension in regulatory strate-
from a number of different angles increases, thus older gies is readily illustrated by our use of physical exercise
children can more readily consider different problem so- to dissipate tension, frustration, and other dysphoric
lutions relative to these different perspectives (Aldwin, states. Children soothe themselves with physical activi-
1994). They learn to recruit social support more effec- ties such as thumb sucking; they also make good use of
tively and subtly, for example, through effective self- solitary fantasy play as a regulatory strategy (Slade,
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 277

1994). Alternatively, throughout our lives we seek so- chic strategies, although they cannot verbalize for us
cial support (e.g., help, comfort) as a way to cope with that they are doing so. For example, dissociation is an
stressors and regulate our emotional experience. How- intrapsychic coping strategy, and it is used by young
ever, the younger the child, the more significant is the children as a way of distracting themselves from an in-
need for social support (e.g., Thompson et al., 2003). tensely stressful situation. It is also useful when chil-
Young children rely on adults to provide safe environ- dren feel powerless and overwhelmed.
ments such that their ability to cope is not over-
whelmed, and caregivers provide direct teaching of Family Influences on Childrens Coping
coping strategies and modeling on how to cope (e.g.,
Miller, Klieweer, Hepworth, & Sandler, 1994; Valiente, Attachment. A potentially significant influence
Fabes, Eisenberg, & Spinrad, 2004). Rossman (1992) on childrens coping strategies is their early attachment
found that young elementary school-age children (6 to 7 experience with significant caregivers. Cassidy (1994)
years) were more likely to cite parents as sources of has theorized how attachment history and emotion reg-
support, whereas older children were more likely to ulation may be linked; the reader is referred to her work
turn to their peers. for further detail and to the Handbook of Attachment
The internal-external dimension becomes more (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999). We provide only a brief sum-
salient in older childrens coping and emotion regula- mary of her thinking. She argues that negative emo-
tion due to their ability to introspect and use metacog- tions, such as anger and fear, for the securely attached
nition in their understanding of themselves. Meerum infant come to be associated with maternal sympathetic
Terwogt and Olthof (1989) reviewed a number of stud- assistance and that these negative feelings are neither
ies that suggested that the cognitive developmental associated with any sort of invalidation of the young
gains of middle childhood facilitated self-reflection, child nor with denial of the negative feelings. For emo-
thus permitting children to use more cognitive strate- tional regulation, this means that the young child comes
gies to render emotional experience less aversive ( by to be able to tolerate aversive emotion temporarily such
using distraction, thinking optimistically, and being that it can begin to make sense of the frustrating or con-
able to shift perspectives to allow for more positive ap- flictual situation that faces it and figure out an adaptive
praisals). However, Harris and Lipian (1989) found coping response. The anxious-avoidant attached infant,
what they called cognitive slippage in school-age however, has often experienced its caregivers rejection
children when dealing with immediate and acute stress when it sought comfort for its distress. Such an infant
( hospitalization). Compared with unstressed children, learns that some emotions are not acceptable and maybe
the hospitalized children seemed to regress to less ma- not even safe. The infant develops a wariness and avoid-
ture ways of thinking and understanding cognitive and ance of his or her caregiver and begins to regulate his or
emotional processes. Similarly, in a recent review of de- her distress by minimizing their emotional expression
pression, temperament, and coping, Compas and his when in the presence of the caregiver. The infants
colleagues (Compas, Connor-Smith, & Jaser, 2004) sug- strategy appears to be that when experiencing emo-
gested that children and youth who had more control tional distress, it suppresses any negative emotional dis-
over attentional processes were more likely to make use play so as to maintain caregiver involvementthe
of more complex cognitive coping strategies, including infants emotional regulation strategy seems to be,
shifting their attention away from pain (e.g., blunting), Mom will stay with me if I dont raise any fuss. The
reinterpreting the situation in more positive ways, and cost, however, to the infant is constant emotional vigi-
being able to selectively attend to positive thoughts and lance and suppression of normal distress. In short,
circumstances, thereby ameliorating the likelihood of development of adaptive problem-solving and support-
depression. seeking coping strategies may be short-circuited for in-
A methodological concern is that virtually all of secure infants.
these studies used childrens and youths verbal self- Related research on infants attachment classifica-
report, which may confound the increased ability to use tion and their propensity to experience different kinds
internally focused strategies with the greater verbal of emotion was examined by Kochanska (2001). She
skills that also accompany maturation. It is probable that found that over a 26-month period that (a) infants clas-
young children (preschoolers) can also access intrapsy- sified as avoidantly attached become progressively
278 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

more fearful, ( b) resistantly attached children appeared Children growing up with depressed or psychiatri-
to have difficulty responding with joy or pleasure, (c) cally disturbed parents have also been studied for how
young children with disorganized attachment classifi- such a family environment influences childrens emo-
cations became more angry, and (d) the securely at- tional and social functioning. Obviously, parental dys-
tached children showed less fear and anger than function co-occurs with higher frequency with other
children with the other attachment classifications, in stressful events for children such as divorce, chronic
spite of being placed in situations designed to elicit unemployment, and spousal conflict. Goodman, Bro-
those emotions. One study worth mentioning examined gan, Lynch, and Fielding (1993) investigated the so-
adolescents attachment classification relative to how cioemotional functioning of children (ages 5 to 10) who
they coped with distress. Howard and Medway (2004) had a depressed mother; the children were subdivided
included measures of attachment, coping style, life further into three groups: (1) some also had a disturbed
stress, and a questionnaire about whom the respondent father in the home, (2) some were in mother-custody
would turn to in times of stress. Their results indicated homes, and (3) some had a well father in the home.
that adolescents with secure attachment status were They also had a comparison sample of children whose
more likely to use family communication as a coping mothers and fathers were neither depressed nor psychi-
resource and less likely to turn to substance use as a atrically disturbed.
way to avoid coping with distress. Insecure attachment, Their results indicated that it was the combination of
alternatively, was related to negative avoidant coping a depressed mother and a disturbed father that was as-
strategies. Less report of stress was also associated sociated with the greatest number of problems among
with secure attachment and more stress was reported older children as opposed to younger children (cf.,
by those with insecure attachment scores. Adolescents Zahn-Waxler, 2000). Apparently, as the children ma-
also tended to prefer their friends over their parents tured, living in an emotionally strained household with
when they did feel distress. two psychiatrically ill parents began to take its toll.
Younger children did not yet demonstrate such negative
Family Conf lict and Dysfunction. Given the rela- effects. Their study also reconfirmed the problematic
tively few studies that have tracked quality of attach- effect that divorce has on children when living with a
ment to childrens subsequent coping competence, the depressed parent, particularly on self-regulation vari-
ways that families contribute to individual childrens ables. Such children tended to be rated as undercon-
coping competence are far from being well understood. trolled, for example, more often aggressive and
There is a larger body of research that has examined the impulsive. Children who had a well father and a de-
effects of marital conflict and anger on childrens func- pressed mother and who were still married and living
tioningthe latter having some links with how well together did not differ from the children of well parents
children cope with the aversive feelings that they them- except for being rated by their teachers as somewhat
selves experience. Cummings and his colleagues (e.g., less popular among their peers.
Cummings, Goeke-Morey, Papp, & Dukewich, 2002)
have conducted a number of investigations on this topic, Parenting Style. Valiente et al. (2004) looked at
and, not surprisingly, the general conclusion that they mothers and fathers expressive style (self-reported)
reach is that many children do not fare well when faced and their supportiveness toward their children as the
with frequent and intense marital conflict. If verbal and latter coped with ordinary, daily stressors. Their results
physical aggression is common between spouses, the indicated that mothers who more often used negative-
boys in particular appear to develop aggressive, exter- dominant expressive style, which included hostile and
nalizing behavior problems. Daughters also demonstrate derogating expressive behavior, had children whose
behavior problems, but more of the girls also show acute coping was less constructive, and who, perhaps as a
distress. Angry exchanges between parents are felt by consequence, reported experiencing more stress in their
children as very stressful, even when they play no role in daily diaries, maintained over a 2-week period. Fathers
the dispute, and the immediate coping strategies that expressive style did not correlate with their childrens
children bring to bear on such a family crisis probably coping ability. Mothers who reported using more sup-
pivot on the childrens perception of controllability of portive strategies had children who, in turn, were more
the dispute. able to access and use constructive coping strategies.
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 279

Hardy, Power, and Jaedicke (1993) examined several Block, 1980) for degree of ego resiliency, which is often
parenting variables (supportiveness, structure, and con- used as a personality prototype. The results showed that
trol) and childrens coping with daily hassles. Given the securely attached teens obtained a robustly positive
the homogeneous middle-class sample, they found that correlation between adaptive emotion regulation and
only maternal supportiveness and structure were related ego resiliency (r = .57) whereas the correlation coeffi-
to childrens coping. Specifically, very supportive cient for dismissing (insecure-avoidant) teens was .32
mothers in moderately low-structured homes had chil- and for preoccupied (insecure-preoccupied) it was .41
dren who generated more coping strategies across situa- ( both significant). This study suggests that ego re-
tions; mothers who provided more structure had siliency is likely influenced by style of emotion regula-
children who used fewer aggressive coping strategies. tion, which in turn may be influenced by attachment
Supportive mothers also had children who reported history. The small sample size prohibited this kind
more avoidant coping strategies when the children per- of analysis, but Zimmermanns work is very useful for
ceived the stressor as uncontrollable. Overall, parental suggesting how personality organization may change as
supportiveness was found to be significantly related to a function of emotion regulation style and attachment
the breadth of repertoire of coping strategies. We infer status.
that parental supportiveness is likely to be associated
with an ongoing secure attachment between child and Summary
parent; thus, supportiveness may be a proxy for how As children mature, their growing cognitive sophistica-
attachment may mediate childrens development of cop- tion, exposure to varied social models, and breadth of
ing strategies. emotional-social experience contribute to their being
Eisenberg and her colleagues (Eisenberg, Gershoff, able to generate more coping solutions to problematic
et al., 2001) found that nonsupportive mothers, in par- situations. The older they are when faced with serious
ticular mothers who expressed hostile negative emotion, distress, the more able they are to see the situation from
appeared to influence their childrens social compe- various perspectives (including those held by other peo-
tence and externalizing behaviors through the childrens ple who may be part of the problematic situation) and
ability to regulate their emotional arousal. They exam- figure out a way to resolve it. With maturity, they be-
ined a couple of other alternative models, but found that come more accurate in their appraisals of how much
the earlier pattern best fit their data. Interestingly, they control they really have over the situation and what risks
found less relationship between maternal negative ex- might accompany taking control of a very difficult situ-
pressivity and childrens manifesting internalizing ation (e.g., intervening in a fight). Effective coping in
problems. Western cultures involves acknowledgment of ones
Turning now to an older group of youth, Zimmermann emotional responses, awareness of ones self as having
(1999) assessed adolescents attachment classification, some degree of agency, and a functional appraisal of the
using the Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, problematic situation and ones role in it. By late child-
& Main, 1985). He also had the adolescents respond to hood or early adolescence, Western children who have
several questions about five hypothetical social rejec- enjoyed secure attachment in their supportive families
tion vignettes, about which they were to imagine that the and escaped severe trauma should generally be capable
various incidents of social rejection happened to them. of emotionally competent coping with concomitant so-
He evaluated their open-ended responses by how flexi- cial effectiveness.
ble their appraisal of the distressing social rejection
was, how flexible and variable their behavioral strate-
gies were for coping with the social rejection, and how
Social Effectiveness and Skill 7: Awareness of
clearly they could articulate how they would feel as well
Emotion Communication in Relationships
as what sort of rationale they provided for why they
would feel that way. The resulting scores were trans- This skill requires that the individual minimally recog-
formed and aggregated to provide a score indicative of nize that emotions are communicated differently de-
adaptive emotion regulation. The adolescents were pending on a persons relationship with an interactant,
also rated by their best friend, their parents, and by two but this particular skill goes beyond that of impression
psychologists with the California Adult Q-sort (Block & management or self-presentation strategies as defined in
280 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Skill 5. With this skill, we also want to include the youngest children (5 to 6 years) were more likely to tell
awareness and use of emotional experience to differenti- mother more secrets than the older children, whereas
ate the organization of a persons relations with others. the older children were more likely to share secrets with
Implied then are the following skills: their friendsas an expression of, maybe even testi-
mony to, their friendshipthan with their mother. The
Recognition of the interpersonal consequences of exception was sharing of embarrassing secrets (i.e., wet-
ones emotion communication, not only of how the ting ones pants) by the older boys: Very few would con-
other is impacted, but also of how the relationship it- fess this to their friend, whereas the older girls were
self is affected. more willing to share bodily loss of control with their
The ability to distinguish among different sorts of re- same-sex friends. Humiliation and fear of loss of reputa-
lationships and thus tailor ones emotion communica- tion were among the reasons cited by the older boys for
tion accordingly. not telling a friend, but they might tell their mother, be-
An understanding of how emotion communication is cause she could be counted on not to embarrass them. As
a vehicle for power or control and thus has the po- for guilty or dangerous secrets, older children felt a
tential to shape the relational space between the clear sense of tension about whether to maintain their
interactants. friends secret as an act of trustworthiness, even as they
also worried that perhaps an adult should know about
The use of the term emotion communication entails ver- the guilt-laden activity (i.e., theft) or dangerous event
bal statements and all channels of nonverbal expression (i.e., lighting a fire in an empty garage). As one girl put
that can convey affective information to an onlooker or it, Mother is like half a friendso sometimes you can
interactant. share some secrets with her (p. 448449, Watson &
By middle to late childhood, children recognize and Valtin, 1997).
can articulate to some degree that emotion communica- From the standpoint of understanding how relation-
tion varies as a function of the nature of the relationship ship structure affects our emotional communication, the
that they have with someone (e.g., Saarni, 1988). Re- older children were more consistently concerned with
search also confirms that children distinguish emotion how trust defined a relationship, and by implication,
communication between close friends versus ordinary with trust comes a degree of reciprocity and mutuality.
peers (e.g., Asher, Parker, & Walker, 1996). Likewise, Good friends should be able to trust one another. Emo-
children differentiate how they communicate with tional communication is profoundly affected by the de-
mothers versus fathers (Fuchs & Thelen, 1988). Not sur- gree of trust one feels toward another with the result
prisingly, disclosure of emotionally vulnerable informa- that the greater the trust, the more likely one will dis-
tion is more often made to close friends and mothers. close information about ones experience that is emo-
Observational research also indicates that children use tionally vulnerable. Emotionally vulnerable information
expressive strategies to maintain relationship equilib- about oneself or about another, such as in secrets, is in-
rium (such as smiling more often) even as they simulta- variably anxiety provoking, and it takes a mutually re-
neously express negative feelings (such as contempt or spectful relationship for anxiety-laden exchanges to be
irritation) toward their friends (von Salisch, 1991). In reassuringly heard.
addition, girls (in Western societies) may be more likely
to smile than boys as a way of influencing the relational Interpersonal Closeness
dynamics (e.g., Saarni, 1992). Strayer and Roberts (1997) undertook an ingenious
study to examine how emotion processes such as empa-
Secret Keeping and Secret Sharing thy might mediate or moderate childrens felt close-
Watson and Valtin (1997) investigated childrens under- ness toward video-taped characters. They used a felt
standing of interpersonal relations relative to keeping or wall hanging that had a mirror mounted in the center,
sharing secrets, and secrets by their very nature are in- and the children were asked to place Velcro-attached
tensely emotion laden. Their results are important here photos of video characters relatively close or far from
for how they revealed childrens use of relationship the reflected image of themselves, depending on how
knowledge in choosing whether to share a secret. The comfortable they felt with the video-taped character
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 281

(e.g., a physically punitive parent versus a girl who is children are socialized into expressive styles that are
excited about an elephant ride at the circus). The results gender-coded (e.g., the conciliatory style of little girls
indicated a nearly linear relationship between the de- when faced with a conflict compared with the more co-
gree of empathy (as measured by the Empathy Contin- ercive style of little boys). She also noted the dearth of
uum, which evaluates the degree of affective sharing, studies on the developmental processes that link emo-
perspective taking, and interpersonal understanding) tional development with gender identity development.
the children felt for the character and how close they Harter and her colleagues (1998) looked at level of
placed the characters photo near their own mirror voice in adolescents relative to the degree to which
image. This relationship increased with age and was they adhered to sex-role stereotypes. Level of voice was
somewhat stronger in girls. Familiarity, status, author- assessed by a questionnaire developed by the authors to
ity, and intensity of affect might all play a role in how ascertain whether the adolescent felt confident about
close we allow another into our personal space, yet this being able to disclose his or her feelings and opinions to
study suggested that if empathy were present, then the others (i.e., to reveal a true self ). Their interesting re-
emotion communication that transpired defined the rela- sults showed that stereotypically feminine adolescents
tionship as different: We can step into the shoes of the had a lower level of voice in public exchange compared
other, even if their personal attributes are quite differ- to their more androgynous female peers but no differ-
ent from our own and they are unfamiliar persons. We ence was obtained in private exchanges. For male ado-
would like to encourage the use of this particular lescents, stereotypically masculine male adolescents
methodological technique in other studies of emotion had a more assertive level of voice in public exchange
communication with children and youth, especially but were not as confident or open in true self level of
those that are also concerned with self-representations, voice in private exchanges (e.g., between close friends)
boundaries, and relations with others. as were androgynous male adolescents. Thus, we see in
this research a convergence on emotion communication,
Social Roles and Emotion Communication sex-role beliefs, and the context of relationship (public
Relatively uninvestigated is how emotion is differen- versus private and close).
tially communicated and conceptualized relative to dif-
Summary
ferent social roles. Social roles refer to age roles, sex
roles, occupational roles, authority and/or leadership Although we have research that substantiates that so-
roles, and so forth. Some research exists on how gender cialization is mediated by parental and peer communi-
(and by implication, sex role) influences both the pattern cated emotion (e.g., derogation is an emotion-laden
and frequency of emotion discourse (e.g., Ruble & Mar- communication that directly informs the target that the
tin, 1998; Shields, 1995). But we do not know how chil- interactant disapproves of both the target and the behav-
dren integrate their knowledge about social roles with ior of the target), we have relatively little research that
beliefs about emotion communication. Research on chil- examines how children and youth recognize and use
drens acquisition of social scripts as they are related to emotional communication to differentiate the organiza-
different venues of emotion communication is minimal, tion of their relations with others. Related issues
although childrens use of scripts may be implicit in include how children and youth construe the interper-
their expectations about emotion expression (e.g., Un- sonal consequences of their emotional communication
derwood et al., 1992) and about best versus worst ways with the relationship for themselves and for their inter-
to cope with aversive feelings (e.g., Saarni, 1997). actants (the focus being on the relational space between
Relative to sex role, young children endorse emotion- the interactants); how they maintain the relationship
specific gender stereotypes that are common in Western quality (e.g., equilibrium) or alter it (e.g., by deepening
societies, such as males are expected to experience more or attenuating it); and how they apply power and control
anger and females more sadness (Karbon et al., 1992). in the relationship. Some research on the meaningful-
Shields (1995) provided a review of how various aspects ness of friendship is relevant (e.g., Asher & Rose,
of emotional experience are gender-coded (e.g., the ex- 1997), as is Gottman et al.s (1997) work on parents
pressive/emotional female versus the instrumental /ra- coaching their children in ways that facilitate their chil-
tional male), and she was particularly interested in how drens understanding of emotional experience. Infant
282 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

intersubjectivity and attunement are also examples of emotional intelligence (e.g., Davies, Stankov, &
emotion communication, but what is missing in our re- Roberts, 1998), by appraisal style (e.g., a positive ap-
search base is how children and youth acquire an aware- praisal style promotes a general sense of subjective
ness that emotion communication varies as a function well-being; Lazarus, 1991), which in turn is associated
of their affectional ties to an interactant, their status with mood states, and by temperament (e.g., Eisenberg
(or dominance) relative to the interactant, and the & McNally, 1993; Eisenberg et al., 2004). As diverse as
unique emotion-evoking circumstances in which the in- these research inquires are, they may help to guide us in
teraction is embedded (e.g., public or private). figuring out how to conceptualize the relations among
personality traits, mood states, and emotion processing
Social Effectiveness and Skill 8: Capacity for relative to the benefits for emotional self-efficacy for
Emotional Self-Efficacy the developing adolescent. We turn next to that debate.

This final skill of emotional competence entails an indi- Personality, Mood, and Emotion Processes
viduals acceptance of his or her emotional experience, How does an individual maintain some degree of emo-
whether eccentric or conventional, negative or positive. tional balance or even a sense of well-being in the face
With this skill, individuals can tolerate and not feel of adverse events and painful circumstances? Granted,
overwhelmed by intense negative emotion (e.g., despair, there are buffering or protective factors that facilitate
melancholy, outrage, or anguish) because they do not resilience (e.g., secure attachment, supportive relation-
view their emotional responses as unjustified. They feel ships), but what does the individual bring to that cru-
relatively in control of their emotional experience from cible of adversity that allows him or her to experience
the standpoint of mastery and positive self-regard. In- appropriate negative affect but then be able to resume
deed, a sense of global self-worth may lie at the heart of her or his life course with equanimity and perhaps even
emotional self-efficacy (Harter, 1999). In our opinion, greater insight into her- or himself ? Clinicians may
this sense of emotional self-efficacy is probably not have intuitive answers to this question (e.g., Janoff-Bul-
achieved until adolescence, for it is undoubtedly de- man & Frantz, 1997), and perhaps the Blocks research
pendent on cognitive development, including the ability on ego resilience is relevant (e.g., Block & Block, 1980).
to consider the realm of possibility and of reality. We What we would like to see investigated from a develop-
also hypothesize that the emotionally self-efficacious mental psychology perspective is how emotion-directed
individual has acquired the preceding seven skills of information processes such as perception, attention,
emotional competence to a relatively mature degree. judgment, and memory recognition and recall (including
As for relationships, emotionally self-efficacious recall of ones own personal life-history events) are in-
adolescents learn that they cannot have it all: Not every- fluenced by personality and mood states ( both enduring
one will adore them or want to spend time with them, and transient moods). It seems to us that the mainte-
and they may feel lonely at times. But rejection does not nance of emotional self-efficacy would require the abil-
leave them in a puddle of devastation and self-pity; in- ity to access at least two sorts of emotion-directed
stead, emotionally self-efficacious individuals seek to information processes: First, the emotion-directed in-
resolve the situation relative to their values. They treas- formation processes need to provide feedback about our
ure their friendships (Asher & Rose, 1997) and choose a own emotional experience such that we can evaluate it;
course that is also guided by a moral compass (e.g., second, these processes need to provide feedback about
Walker & Hennig, 1997). Research on relationship qual- the emotion-evoking circumstances such that we can ac-
ity in adolescence also suggests that adherence to a true cept our emotional response to the circumstances and
self is more often associated with perception of self- then turn our focus to resolving or accepting the adverse
worth and with youth engaged in relationships that were circumstances. As an example, an adolescent might ex-
supportive as opposed to contingent on the youths perience the divorce of his or her parents as very aver-
pleasing either parents or friends (reviewed in Harter, sive, but if some degree of emotional self-efficacy has
1999; see also Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Morris, 2002). been developed, he or she can accept the feelings of sad-
Individual differences in emotional self-efficacy ness, anger, and anxiety as appropriate to the painful sit-
may be influenced by personality; for example, agree- uation, but then redirect his or her focus toward his or
ableness appears implicated as a significant aspect of her own positive goals and values (e.g., friendships, aca-
Emotional Development in Childhood and Adolescence: Social Ef fectiveness and Positive Adaptation 283

demic achievement, or community service) rather than mation processing. This body of research often involves
ruminating on his or her parents conflicts and breakup. inducing either a positive or negative mood in the re-
Interestingly, a study by Lyubomirsky and Nolen- search participants and then giving them various cogni-
Hoeksema (1995) provides empirical support for the tive tasks that involve perception, attention, judgment,
value of a person redirecting their focus, which they and recall. An early study by Carlson and Masters
refer to as distraction. They found that rumination by (1985) did just that: They induced positive emotional
dysphoric participants led to generating less productive states (self-focused or other-focused) in one group of 5-
solutions to hypothetical interpersonal problems and to 6-year-old children and a control group had a neutral
having a more pessimistic outlook about their future. induction. The first group displayed happier facial ex-
However, a second group of dysphoric participants were pressions and did not demonstrate the usual reduced
told to distract themselves (i.e., redirect their focus), generosity after inequality of rewards as long as they
and this group did not differ from the nondysphoric par- had received the self-focused happy mood induction.
ticipants in optimism and effectiveness in problem solv- The authors interpreted their results as supportive of the
ing. In sum, a person can feel miserable (dysphoric), position that temporary positive mood states facilitate
and their misery might be very well justified by the cir- tolerance of aversive experiences.
cumstances, but being able to redirect their focus rather What Rusting proposedand which is relevant to
than to ruminate on the adversity may well facilitate ef- emotional self-efficacyis that both temporary mood
fective problem solving and influence their expecta- states and more stable personality styles or traits will in-
tions for an improved future. This outcome is similar to fluence how one processes emotion-laden cues, for these
that found by Weyer and Sandler (1998) in a short-term cues evoke memory associations, which in turn are in-
longitudinal study of children whose parents had re- fluenced by both personality disposition and temporary
cently divorced: Efficacy of coping was negatively re- mood. Reframing this so that it helps to elucidate emo-
lated to propensity to ruminate. Likewise, OBrien, tional self-efficacy is suggested by the following case:
Margolin, and John (1995) found that children who
could distance themselves from their parents divorce Lilys younger sister, Sharon, age 10, died of cystic fibro-
appeared to fare better. sis, and the family was in profound grief, for it had seemed
Rusting (1998) has provided us with a valuable inte- for awhile that Sharon might survive for a number of years
gration of the research on mood states and personality more and then she unexpectedly worsened and died. Lily
traits as they affect emotion-congruent information (age 15) had been neither a difficult nor an easy baby:
She had had her share of difficult-to-soothe crying spells
processing. She noted that it is the trait theorists, who
in infancy and seemed to have a rather low threshold for
have posited that an individuals enduring personality
pain (e.g., reacting quite intensely to vaccinations and in-
traits, for example, proneness to positive or negative af- jections). Upon Sharons death, she felt very bereft and
fectivity, most strongly influence their mood-congruent experienced her grief somatically as well (abdominal
information processing. More specifically, if one is pre- pain). She stayed home from school for a week, and subse-
disposed to positive affectivity (trait), one is more quently, with her parents help, she created a photo mon-
likely to report experiencing more positive mood states, tage to take to Sharons elementary school classroom. She
and this results in ones retrieval of more positive mem- also collected Sharons drawings that she had made over
ory associations, which in turn probably facilitates the the past few years while undergoing therapy in conjunc-
continuance of the desirable mood (and the converse tion with her frequent medical interventions and assem-
being true if one is predisposed to negative affectivity). bled them into a book. She had the book copied in color
This proneness to positive versus negative affectivity and gave the copies to various relatives as gifts to remem-
ber Sharons creativity. Lily focused on Sharons vibrancy
sounds similar to the earlier discussion in this chapter
rather than her final months of ravaging coughing and
on temperamental disposition to negative emotionality
breathlessness, and Lilys turning outward to the former
and reflects some personality development theoretical school mates and to the extended family drew her into sup-
positions that advocate that personality traits are sig- portive and rewarding social contact. At the time of the
nificantly influenced by temperamental dispositions first anniversary after Sharons death, Lily was able to
(e.g., Caspi & Silva, 1995). weep easily over the loss of her sister, but she was also
However, other theorists have emphasized the influ- able to feel good about herself, appreciated her parents
ence that temporary mood states have on emotion infor- more from an empathic perspective of what they had gone
284 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

through, was perceived by her peers as a mature and trust- portive family history and a personality style that is not
worthy person, enjoyed a moderate social life, and had burdened by the double hazard of proneness to negative
easily maintained her standing on the honor roll. Lily was affect and deficits in attention, then a persons memory
optimistic about her future plans, and she was looking for- associations should be benign or resilient and their cur-
ward to a community service volunteer position for the rent emotional state should be moderately regulated.
summer in the chronically ill childrens ward at the local
The challenging social encounter should then unfold
hospital. (author developed, C. Saarni)
with minimal defensiveness, appropriate assertiveness,
Noteworthy in this case is that Lily is not a rumina- and demonstration of personal integrity (i.e., a persons
tor, yet she does not minimize or avoid her negative af- moral sense).
fect. Her personality style appears to be one of In conclusion, further theoretical development of the
moderation; her coping style is a socially engaged one construct of emotional competence is needed; for exam-
rather than one of withdrawal or avoidance of others, in ple, how does it differ from emotional intelligence
spite of some initial somatogenic reactions to Sharons (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2000; Saarni, in press) and
death. Importantly, her parents appear to be able to how might emotional competence skills be structured
provide supportive scaffolding, even as they too hierarchically (as has been done with the construct so-
grieved for Sharon. Lily is future oriented and planful cial competence; Rose-Krasnor, 1997). The skills of
with the expectancy that what lies ahead will be posi- emotional competence are dynamic and transactional,
tive and satisfying. She is expressive but neither over- for these skills are part of an interpersonal exchange
nor undercontrolled. The fact that her peers view her as that unfolds in a unique context. Indeed, one could de-
trustworthy and mature suggests that her emotional sign interesting studies simply by pairing together chil-
communication with them is respectful, genuine, and dren or teens who differ in the degree to which they can
insightful. She is probably a good listener, and her employ the skills of emotional competence and then ob-
peers confide in her. Lily demonstrates emotional self- serve how their interpersonal negotiations unfold.
efficacy: She accepts her grief, she accepts the loss of
her sister, yet she finds balance and meaningfulness in
her life. In spite of deeply felt adversity, she demon-
strates well-being and resilience over the long term CONCLUSION
(see also Wyman et al., 2000).
In science, progress is measured not so much by how
Summary many questions have been answered, but by how many
Emotional self-efficacy is obviously a superordinate new ones have been raised. Such is the case in the study
construct, much the way that emotional competence is. of emotion, where there has been a plethora of signifi-
It overlaps with resilience in the face of adversity and cant contributions to knowledge in recent years. In con-
entails well-developed skills of emotional competence clusion, we review a few of the unresolved issues that we
as described in the preceding sections. One of us believe represent the frontier of research into emotional
(Saarni, 1999) has also argued that emotional self- development. Given the generally functionalist perspec-
efficacy must entail a moral sense and a willingness to tive taken in this chapter, our proposed research ques-
make choices that support ones beliefs that one is doing tions emphasize goals, the relational rather than the
the right thing even if it is uncomfortable or unpopu- intrapsychic properties of emotions, the flexibility
lar (Colby & Damon, 1992; Walker & Hennig, 1997; rather than the reflexive nature of emotional behavior,
Wilson, 1993). Such moral choices require thoughtful- and the embeddedness of emotion in interpersonal
ness and self-reflection and very likely a sense of moral transactions.
justice tempered with sympathy and compassion. Social The functionalist approach liberalizes the study of
effectiveness is also part of emotional self-efficacy: A emotion, but has not yet profoundly influenced how
persons emotion-directed information processing while emotions are studied. Consider the following: If one
engaged in a social encounter, especially a challenging were to do a census of studies on emotions, one would
or emotionally evocative one, will be influenced by his find, even today, an extraordinary overrepresentation of
or her memory associations of similar past encounters research in which emotions have been treated primarily
and by their current emotional state. Assuming a sup- as responsesas outward signs of internal states. Stud-
Conclusion 285

ies of emotions as antecedents and organizers of per- another way, rules and standards, and the construct of
sonal and social behaviors have been much less preva- the self, may not be sufficient to generate later-appear-
lent. When emotions are considered purely as responses, ing emotions like pride, shame, and guilt; these emo-
the tendency is to stop there, and not consider how those tions may also require the affective sting of the
responses can be in the service of changing or maintain- emotional communications of others. If so, we need to
ing person-environment relations. This imbalance be- study the value added by emotional signals to the impo-
tween emotions considered as responses and emotions sition of rules and standards.
conceptualized as organizers generates a number of Another topic at the interface of developmental and
major new areas of research. social psychology is that of attention in dyadic and
One of these areas concerns new aspects of emotional group settings. Basic issues such as the targeting of joint
communication. For instance, there is a discrepancy be- attention between two individuals and the quality of the
tween the wealth of information we have about individual emotional messages exchanged between them may de-
differences in emotional responding (i.e., temperament), termine the specific emotion generated in such inter-
and individual differences in emotional perception and changes. The meaning of a joyful reaction by a
subsequent behavior regulation (a phenomenon for which significant other in the presence of a child has different
we do not even have a noun, and about which there are functional consequences depending on (a) whether the
consequently few studies). One of the first research ques- joy is targeted toward an action of the child ( laying the
tions we raised in several sections about emotional devel- basis for the child to experience pride), ( b) whether the
opment concerned the little we know about how emotion joy is merely witnessed by the child ( leading to an em-
perception originates, how such perceptions lead to func- pathic affect sharing but not pride), or (c) whether the
tional consequences for the child, and how infants and joy is oriented toward the child himself or herself (re-
children come to react, and subsequently to become dis- sulting in the child developing affiliative or attachment
missive, hyper-vigilant, or appropriately attuned, toward bonds). Similarly, the quality of the emotion message
different cues of emotion expressed by others. We also manifested by the other in such social interactional ex-
need to investigate which contributing contextual fea- changes (i.e., its fearful nature or its scornful nature)
tures promote sensitivity to such cues, how such sensitiv- may have dramatically different results on the child.
ities eventually become biases to respond, and what are The first permits the child to catch the fear of an ob-
the personal characteristics of infants and children that ject expressed by another; the second potentially enters
lead them to be disposed to react appropriately or to into the generation of shame (as noted before). It seems
make biased attributions of emotion. Volumes await the an inescapable conclusion that emotions are relational
description and explanation of the answers to these processes. If so, the field must develop paradigms that
questions. do justice to that relational nature, and permit func-
If emotions are primarily social and relational, the tional consequences of those emotions to be observed.
boundary of social psychological research and develop- Few paradigms exist that permit the richness of emotion
mental study becomes very permeable. For instance, the to be discerned and quantified. Where the paradigms do
self, it has been said, develops under the watchful eye of exist, as in studies involving naturalistic or field obser-
the other. This statement captures our point that it is not vations, the coding schemes often do not do justice to
just cognitive developments linked to the self that lead the many different facets and manifestations of emo-
to so-called self-conscious emotions. The child must no- tional behavior.
tice the presence of such watchful eyes, realize that the Emotions, though, do not always seem to be relational.
eyes reside in significant others, identify what emotion Indeed, they seem to be intrapsychic and private events.
the eyes are communicating, and subsequently behave to For instance, we weep for the loss of a loved one in the
have an appropriate effect on those eyessuch as by privacy of our own room, with no one present, and no ap-
hiding from them in shame, or showing off to them in parent social target for the tears. Is it the case that emo-
pride. The social context and the social signals (expres- tions are primarily private and nonrelational? If they are
sions) provided by significant others are thus constitu- relational, as we propose they are, how do they become
tive of new emotions. This phenomenon has not been private, and do such private events themselves have func-
adequately studied, in comparison to studies of the rela- tional sequelae? We cannot answer these questions at this
tion between cognitive and emotional development. Put time. The process by which emotions considered as
286 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

external and relational become internal and private has same emotion can result in quite different, indeed
not been the focus of any systematic research to date. We equipotential, transactions with the world. The equipo-
know that in language speech shifts at 5 to 6 years of age tentiality of emotional behaviors renders them flexible
from being primarily or exclusively external to poten- rather than reflexive. However, most research on emo-
tially internal and private. Such interiorization has great tion to date has not done justice to the flexibility and
relevance for the growth of self-regulation. Is there a equipotentiality characteristics of emotion. Our meth-
similar process of interiorization that occurs for emo- ods often constrain the possibility of observing flexibly
tion? Does the interiorization of emotion take place at manifested emotional behaviors, or of noting how the
the same age as the interiorization of speech? And does same event can be construed differently by different
such possible interiorization have implications for emo- children. Moreover, the research objectives in most
tion regulation, by analogy to the consequences of the in- studies typically center on the search for the coherence
teriorization of speech for self-regulation? The recent of emotional behaviorsthe more highly intercorrelated
work of Holodinski and Friedlmeier (2006) draws our at- we find behaviors to be, the better we think our findings
tention to such interiorization processes. We believe that are. However, emotional behaviors are rarely highly in-
these authors have identified what may be a major yet tercorrelated. Such low-to-modest correlations are pre-
unsuspected developmental transition in emotional cisely what one would expect if emotional behaviors are
lifeone that takes place in the middle of the 1st decade equipotential. Low correlations due to equipotentiality
of life. Their theorizing and research proposals can help of response would then reflect, not the presence of error
reconcile two drastically differing views of emotion: (1) variance, but a true state of affairs. How does one sta-
emotion as intrapersonal (a relatively late stage in the tistically and conceptually tease apart situations in
ontogeny of emotion) and (2) emotion as interpersonal / which low correlations among behaviors are expectable
relational (a process present throughout the life span but from those in which there is error variance? This is a
not necessarily evident in all manifestations of emotion vexing problem for researchers to address, and our usual
in older children and adults). statistical models do not help disentangle the two possi-
There is another challenge to the view that emotions bilities. Only research designed to predict what behav-
are relational. Some emotional phenomena such as es- iors are shown in what contexts can do so. Attachment
thetic emotions do not appear to be in the service of any researchers address this problem by the use of interpre-
apparent goal. Music not only soothes the savage beast tational methods and judgmental, though reliable, cod-
but also the very young infant. Is the infants reaction to ing systems such as those that quantify maternal
music an exception to the view that emotions serve a sensitivity or classify the meaning of the infants behav-
persons strivings? Or is music, as Lazarus argues, a iors into categories such as avoidant, ambivalent, disor-
process that draws on reintegration of cues from mem- ganized, or secure. Perhaps we need to quantify
ory in which some aspects of a musical composition emotions using similar approaches but on different emo-
serve as prods bringing to mind previously experienced tional phenomena than attachment. To reiterate, we be-
emotions (in the manner that the sound of drumbeats in lieve that we cannot continue to ignore the problems
music can remind us of our heartbeat in a state of fear)? created (a) by the same event being interpreted differ-
This type of question is amenable to test, but to date no ently by different individuals, and ( b) by different be-
thorough study has been conducted on music and emo- haviors being recruited in the service of the same
tion in early life. Therefore, we cannot provide evidence emotion. We stress again: Our paradigms must allow for
relevant to Lazaruss functionalist argument or its na- the manifestation of the flexibility of emotion.
tivisit counterpart. Systematic study of esthetic emo- Another important issue in the functionalist approach
tions thus seems very relevant in evaluating the limits of is the critical role of context. One instantiation of the im-
applicability of a functionalist approach to emotion. portance of context is again at the interface of social and
The approach to emotion we have taken is not only re- developmental psychology. It relates to when and how
lational but also nonmechanistic. It considers emotions childrens disclosure of differently valenced emotions to
not as reflexive but as flexible, even in the very young different categories of people takes place (e.g., peers ver-
infant. The functionalist approach stresses that the same sus adults, close relationships versus distant ones, and so
event can produce quite different emotions, and the forth). Relevant individual differences to consider in
Conclusion 287

such an investigation include personality disposition ness as they begin to learn how to be warmly and gen-
(e.g., degree of inhibition) and childrens cognitive per- uinely intimate with a partner? How do children and
spective-taking skills. Childrens social cognitive ex- adolescents figure out how emotional communication is
pectancies about the reactions of others to emotion-laden different in intimate relationships as compared to their
disclosures are also relatively underinvestigated. earlier same-sex peer friendships? Daily diary studies
Also related to context is the process of how individ- may well be a methodology that would prove useful, as
ual differences in the capacity for empathic engagement would be studies that recruit dyads of adolescents who
by the child without the child becoming overwhelmed by self-nominate themselves as partnered.
his or her own personal distress. We raised the possibil- Another frontier of research deals with factors that
ity of tolerance for ambiguity as related to having a influence emotion-directed information processes of
higher threshold for tolerating anothers distress as the perception, attention, judgment, and memory. How do
witnessing person considered avenues of appropriate ac- individual differences in a childs personality and his or
tion for how to help or intervene. Important contextual her development affect these psychological processes?
features of the relationship that exist between target and How do enduring and transient mood states affect them?
sufferer are also under investigated; such features can We raised the question of how emotional self-efficacy
differentially contribute to personal distress reactions. in adolescence may require the ability to access these
For example, if the target is more dominant or powerful emotion-directed information processes in ways that
than the actor and now something affects the target to allow for feedback to their evaluation of their own emo-
render her or him distressed, at what age will children tional experience so that they can accept their emotional
intervene to assist sympatheticallyand in a genuine response (as opposed to denying their emotional re-
manner, rather than instrumentally or strategicallyas sponse and having to defend themselves against it).
opposed to feeling personally distressed? With such emotional self-efficacy, they can then turn
A central aspect of context in emotion is that of the their focus to resolving the adverse circumstances.
role a person is expected to assume. One cannot under- A final set of questions brings us full circle to the ob-
stand many emotions without understanding the role a jective of this chapter: Emotional development is the de-
person is playing. However, we do not know very much velopment of what? We need to do research on how some
about how children and youth integrate their knowledge emotions are present in some rudimentary form at birth
about social roles (e.g., age roles, occupational roles, au- or shortly thereafter. These nonemergent emotions de-
thority/ leadership roles), how roles set the stage for dif- velop in the sense that new events elicit them, or new
ferent emotional reactions and different beliefs about motives are served by them, but the relation between the
emotional communication. Some research on gender event and the motive stays invariant. However, what is
roles exists, but it appears to be more related to social- the process by which different events in relation to dif-
ization patterns (e.g., girls conciliatory versus boys ferent goals yield the same emotion? We do not know the
coercive styles when faced with a conflict) or with attri- answer to the question of how such totally different
butions of propensity about which gender is more likely transactions yield the same or similar emotions. Other
to experience what sort of emotion. We thus propose emotions are not present from birth but become organ-
more of a focus on how children and youth organize ized due to the intercoordination of processes such as
their relationships with regard to social expectations, cognition, exposure to events in the world, the social re-
how those expectations affect the quality, frequency, actions and attitudes of others, the biological constitu-
and intensity of emotions, and how roles have an impact tion of the child, and the differentiation of the physical
on various dimensions of emotional communication. For and social self. Do these emotions emerge? Or are they
example, more smiling appears to occur among friends, evident in some rudimentary, not-readily measurable
even as they may also reproach or express negative affect form early in life? Some recent research suggests that
more frequently with their friends (von Salisch, 1996). complex emotions such as jealousy, shame, and pride
Presumably, there is also more emotion-laden disclosure may be evident even in the 1st year of life, contrary to
to friends. How do youth differentiate their emotion widespread belief (Draghi-Lorenz, Reddy, & Costall,
communication strategies when they first embark on an 2001). In cognitive development, we have learned that
intimate relationship? What is it that creates awkward- phenomena once deemed to emerge in late infancy can
288 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

be shown in some meaningful form in the newborn or (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 555578).
young baby. Are similar rudimentary precocities also New York: Wiley.
Barrett, K. C. (1997). Emotion communication and the development
identifiable in the development of nonbasic emotions?
of the social emotions. New Directions for Child Development, 77,
The possibility that emotions may be evident much ear- 6988.
lier than the usual indices of emotion leads us to infer Barrett, K. C., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Cole, P. M. (1993). Avoiders ver-
that this is an important question. We hope that future sus amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and
shame during toddlerhood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 481505.
editions of this Handbook may report answers to the un-
Barth, J., & Bastiani, A. (1997). A longitudinal study of emotion reg-
resolved issues we pose. ulation and preschool childrens social behavior. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 43, 107128.
Berk, L. E. (1992). Childrens private speech: An overview of theory
REFERENCES and the status of research. In R. M. Diaz & L. E. Berk (Eds.), Pri-
vate speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 1753).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Abbot, S. (1992). Holding on and pushing away: Comparative per- Berlin, L. J., & Cassidy, J. (2003). Mothers self-reported control
spectives on an eastern Kentucky child-rearing practice. Ethos, their preschool childrens emotional expressiveness: A longitudi-
20, 3365. nal study of associations with infant-mother attachment and chil-
Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M. A., & Gibson, E. J. (1993). Development of drens emotion regulation. Social Development, 12, 477495.
perception of affordances. In C. K. Rovee-Collier & L. P. Lipsitt Bertenthal, B., & Campos, J. (1990). A systems approach to the orga-
(Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 8, pp. 5097). Nor- nizing effects of self-produced locomotion during infancy. In
wood, NJ: Ablex. C. L. Rovee-Collier (Ed.), Advances in infancy research
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of (pp. 260). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bingham, R., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. N. (1987, April). Negative
Aldwin, C. (1994). Stress, coping, and development. New York: Guil- emotions in a social relationship context. Paper presented at the
ford Press. Society for Research on Child Development, Baltimore, MD.
Altshuler, J., & Ruble, D. (1989). Developmental changes in chil- Biringen, Z., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Appelbaum, M. (1995). Affec-
drens awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable tive reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad. Child
stress. Child development, 60, 13371349. Development, 66, 499514.
Anderson, D. E., Ansfield, M., & DePaulo, B. (1999). Loves best Bischof, N. (1975). A systems approach toward the functional connec-
habit: Deception in the context of relationships. In P. Philippot, tions of attachment and fear. Child Development, 46, 801817.
R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coates (Eds.), The social context of non- Blass, E., & Ciaramitaro, V. (1994). A new look at some old mecha-
verbal behavior (pp. 372409). Cambridge, England: Cambridge nisms in human newborns. Monographs of the Society for Research
University Press. in Child Development, 59(Serial No. 239).
Anderson, E. (1994). Young childrens understanding of facial and Bloch, A. (1966). The Kurdistani cradle story. Clinical Pediatrics, 5,
vocal expressions of emotion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, 641645.
DePaul University, Chicago, IL. Block, J., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-re-
Arsenio, W., & Lemerise, E. (2001). Varieties of childhood bullying: siliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),
Values, emotion processes, and social competence. Social Devel- Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 13. Development of
opment, 10, 5973. cognition, af fect, and social relations (pp. 39101). Hillsdale, NJ:
Asher, S., Parker, J., & Walker, D. (1996). Distinguishing friendship Erlbaum.
from acceptance: Implications for intervention and assessment. In Block, J., & Kremen, A. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual
W. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. Hartup (Eds.), The company and empirical connections and separateness. Journal of Personal-
they keep: Friendship during childhood and adolescence ity and Social Psychology, 70, 349360.
(pp. 366405). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context.
Asher, S., & Rose, A. (1997). Promoting childrens social-emotional In w& D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psy-
adjustment with peers. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emo- chology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed.,
tional development and emotional intelligence (pp. 196224). New pp. 309370). New York: Wiley.
York: Basic Books. Blurton-Jones, N. (1967). An ethological study of some aspects of so-
Ayers, T. S., & Sandler, I. (1996). A dispositional and situational as- cial behaviour of children in nursery school. In D. Morris (Ed.),
sessment of childrens coping: Testing alternative models of cop- Primate ethology (pp. 347368). London: Weidenfeld and Nicol-
ing. Journal of Personality, 64, 923958. son.
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to peo- Boccia, M., & Campos, J. J. (1989). Maternal emotional signals, so-
ple and objects in mother-infants and peer-infant interaction. cial referencing, and infants reactions to strangers. In N. Eisen-
Child Development, 55(4), 12781298. berg (Ed.), New directions of child development (Vol. 44,
Band, E., & Weisz, J. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: pp. 2549). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Childrens perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Develop- Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New
mental Psychology, 24, 247253. York: Basic Books.
Barrett, K., & Campos, J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional develop- Bowlby, J. (1972). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York:
ment: Vol. 2. A functionalist approach to emotions. In J. Osofsky Basic Books.
References 289

Bradshaw, D. L. (1986). Immediate and prolonged ef fectiveness of 1000. Emotions Inside Out. New York: New York Academy of Sci-
negative emotion expressions in inhibiting infants actions. Un- ences.
published doctoral dissertation, University of Denver, CO.
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J., Campos, R., Ujiie, T., Miyake,
Brenner, E., & Salovey, P. (1997). Emotion regulation during child- K., et al. (1998). Production of emotional facial expressions in
hood: Developmental, interpersonal, and individual considera- American, Japanese and Chinese infants. Developmental Psychol-
tions. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional literacy and ogy, 34, 616628.
emotional development (pp. 168192). New York: Basic Books.
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. (1992). Japanese and American infants responses to arm re-
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, straint. Developmental Psychology, 28, 578583.
50(1/2, Serial No. 209), 335.
Camras, L. A., & Sachs, V. B. (1991). Social referencing and care-
Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). taker expressive behavior in a day care setting. Infant Behavior
Learning to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective. and Development, 14, 2736.
Child Development, 57, 529548.
Camras, L., Sachs-Alter, E., & Ribordy, S. (1996). Emotion under-
Briggs, J. (1970). Never in anger. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
standing in maltreated children: Recognition of facial expressions
sity Press.
and integration with other emotion cues. In M. Lewis & M. W.
Campos, J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Sullivan (Eds.), Emotional development in atypical children
Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens (pp. 203225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149219.
Carlson, C. R., & Masters, J. C. (1986). Inoculation by emotion: Ef-
Campos, J., Bertenthal, B., & Kermoian, R. (1992). Early experience fects of positive emotional states on childrens reaction to social
and emotional development: The emergence of wariness of comparison. Developmental Psychology, 22, 760765.
heights. Psychological Science, 3, 6164.
Carlson Jones, D., Abbey, B. B., & Cumberland, A. (1998). The de-
Campos, J., Frankel, C., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emo- velopment of display rule knowledge: Linkages with family ex-
tion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377394. pressiveness and social competence. Child Development, 69,
Campos, J., Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svejda, M. 12091222.
(1978). The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In M. Lewis & Casey, R. (1993). Childrens emotional experience: Relations among
L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of af fect (pp. 149182). expression, self-report, and understanding. Developmental Psy-
New York: Plenum. chology, 29, 119129.
Campos, J., Kermoian, R., & Zumbahlen, M. (1992). Socioemotional
Casey, R. J. (1996). Emotional competence in children with external-
transformations in the family system following infant crawling
izing and internalizing disorders. In M. Lewis & M. W. Sullivan
onset. In N. F. Eisenberg & R. A. Fabes (Eds.), New directions for
(Eds.), Emotional development in atypical children (pp. 161183).
child development: Vol. 55. Emotion and its regulation in early de-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
velopment (pp. 2540). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age
Campos, J., Thein, S., & Owen, D. (2003). A Darwinian legacy to un-
three predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudi-
derstanding human infancy: Emotional expressions as behavior
nal evidence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66,
regulators. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000,
486498.
110134.
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Inf luences of attachment re-
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sten-
lationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
berg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In M. Haith & J.
velopment, 59(2/3, Serial No. 240), 228249.
Campos (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy
and developmental psychobiology (4th ed., pp. 783915). New Cassidy, J., Parke, R., Butkovsky, L., & Braungart, J. (1992). Family-
York: Wiley. peer connections: The roles of emotional expressiveness within
the family and childrens understanding of emotions. Child Devel-
Campos, J. J., Mumme, D., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. G. (1994). A
opment, 63, 603618.
functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. In N. Fox
(Ed.), The development of emotion regulation (pp. 284303). Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. New York: Guilford Press.
Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development and basic emotions. Ceschi, G., & Scherer, K. (2003). Childrens ability to control the fa-
Cognition and Emotion, 6, 267283. cial expression of laughter and smiling: Knowledge and behav-
Camras, L. A., Grow, G., & Ribordy, S. C. (1983). Recognition of iour. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 385411.
emotional expressions by abused children. Journal of Clinical and Chambers, S. M. (1999). The effect of family talk on young chil-
Child Psychology, 12, 325328. drens development and coping. In E. Frydenberg (Ed.), Learning
Camras, L. A., Lambrecht, L., & Michel, G. (1996). Infant surprise to cope: Developing as a person in complex societies
expressions as coordinative motor structures. Journal of Nonver- (pp. 130149). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
bal Behavior, 20, 183195. Chapman, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cooperman, G., & Iannotti, R.
Camras, L. A., Meng, Z., Ujiie, T., Dharamsi, S., Miyake, K., Oster, (1987). Empathy and responsibility in the motivation of chil-
H., et al. (2002). Observing emotion in infants: Facial expression, drens helping. Developmental Psychology, 23, 140145.
body behavior, and rater judgments of responses to an ex- Chisholm, J. S. (1983). Navajo infancy. New York: Aldine.
pectancy-violating event. Emotion, 2(2), 179193. Chisholm, J. S. (1989). Biology, culture and the development of tem-
Camras, L. A., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., & Bakeman, R. (2003). Emo- perament: A Navajo example. In J. K. Nugent, B. Lester, & T. B.
tional facial expressions in European-American, Japanese, and Brazelton (Eds.), The cultural context of infancy: Vol. 1. Biology,
Chinese Infants. In P. Ekman & J. Campos, R. Davidson, & F. de culture, and infant development (pp. 341364). Norwood,
Waal (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. NJ: Ablex.
290 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Coates, B., Anderson, E., & Hartup, W. (1972). Interrelations in the Dearing, K., Hubbard, J., & Ramsden, S. (2002). Childrens self-
attachment behavior of human infants. Developmental Psychology, reports about anger regulation: Direct and indirect links to social
6, 218237. preference and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48,
Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct disordered and 308336.
comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32, 988998. DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J. P., Busnel, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C.,
Cohen, L., & Campos, J. (1974). Father, mother and stranger as elici- & Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal
tors of attachment behaviors in infancy. Developmental Psychol- speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 159164.
ogy, 10, 146154. DeCasper, W., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns pre-
Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face inter- fer their mothers voices. Science, 208, 11741176.
action: Inf luence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles Denham, S. (1999). Emotional development in young children. New
in either partners behavior. Developmental Psychology, 24(3), York: Guilford Press.
386392.
Denham, S., & Auerbach, S. (1995). Mother-child dialogue about
Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of emotions. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs,
moral commitment. New York: Free Press. 121, 301319.
Compas, B. E. (1987). Coping with stress during childhood and ado- Denham, S., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E., & Holt, R. (1990). Emo-
lescence. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 393403. tional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child
Compas, B. E., Connor, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H., & development, 61, 11451152.
Wadsworth, M. (1999). Getting specific about coping: Effortful Denham, S., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., &
and involuntary responses to stress in development. In M. Lewis & Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers emotion
D. Ramsey (Eds.), Soothing and stress (pp. 229256). Mahwah, competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion,
NJ: Erlbaum. 21, 6586.
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., & Jaser, S. (2004). Temperament, DEntremont, B., & Muir, D. (1999). Infant responses to adult
stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for depression in child- happy and sad vocal and facial expressions during face-to-
hood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent face interactions. Infant Behavior and Development., 22(4),
Psychology, 33, 2131. 527539.
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., Saltzman, H., & Thomsen, A. H.
DePaulo, B. (1991). Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation: A de-
(2001). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence:
velopmental perspective. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rim (Eds.),
Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psycho-
Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 351397). New York:
logical Bulletin, 127, 87127.
Cambridge University Press.
Conrad, R. (1994). Infant af fect sharing and its relation to maternal
Dewey, J. (1894). The theory of emotion: Pt. 1. Emotional attitudes.
availability. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cal-
Psychological Review, 1, 553569.
ifornia, Berkeley.
Dewey, J. (1895). The theory of emotion: Pt. 2. The significance of
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the
emotions. Psychological Review, 2, 1332.
emotions. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
emotions (2nd ed., pp. 91115). New York: Guilford Press. Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Minton, Balch.
Crick, N., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review of social-information pro- Dickson, K. L., Walker, H., & Fogel, A. (1997). The relationship be-
cessing mechanisms in childrens social adjustment. Psychologi- tween smile type and play type during parent-infant play. Devel-
cal Bulletin, 115, 74101. opmental Psychology, 33, 925933.
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conf lict. Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2000). Subjective emotional well-being.
New York: Guilford. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
(2nd ed., pp. 325337). New York: Guilford Press.
Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., Papp, L. M., & Dukewich,
T. L. (2002). Childrens responses to mothers and fathers emo- Donaldson, S. K., & Westerman, M. A. (1986). Development of chil-
tionality and tactics in marital conf lict in the home. Journal of drens understanding of ambivalence and casual theories of emo-
Family Psychology, 16, 478492. tion. Developmental Psychology, 22, 655662.
Custrini, R., & Feldman, R. S. (1989). Childrens social competence Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discrimi-
and nonverbal encoding and decoding of emotion. Journal of Child nate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn in-
Clinical Psychology, 18, 336342. fant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418426.
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes error: Emotion, reason, and the Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parents:
human brain. New York: Grosset & Dunlap. An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 5076.
DAndrade, R. (1984). Cultural meaning systems. In R. Shweder & R. Draghi-Lorenz, R., Reddy, V., & Costall, A. (2001). Rethinking the
LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion development of nonbasic emotions: A critical review of existing
(pp. 88119). New York: Cambridge University Press. theories. Developmental Review, 21, 263304.
Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (1998). Emotional intelli- Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge,
gence: In search of an elusive construct. Journal of Personality MA: Harvard University Press.
and Social Psychology, 75, 9891015. Dunn, J. (2000). Mind-reading, emotion understanding, and relation-
Davies, P., & Cummings, M. (1994). Marital conf lict and child ad- ships. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24,
justment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bul- 142144.
letin, 116, 387411. Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about
Davis, T. (1995). Gender differences in masking negative emotions: feeling states between mothers and their young children. Develop-
Ability or motivation? Developmental Psychology, 31, 660667. mental Psychology, 23, 132139.
References 291

Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1991). Relationships, talk about feelings, and Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Murphy, B., Guthrie, I., Jones,
the development of affect regulation in early childhood. In J. Gar- S., et al. (1997). Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of
ber & K. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation childrens social functioning from regulation and emotionality.
and dysregulation (pp. 89108). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Child Development, 68, 642664.
University Press. Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Cumberland, A.,
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1994). Affect expression in the family, chil- Losoya, S., et al. (2001). Mothers emotional expressivity and
drens understanding of emotions, and their interactions with oth- childrens behavior problems and social competence: Mediation
ers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 120137. through childrens regulation. Developmental Psychology, 37,
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling 475490.
states and childrens later understanding of others emotions. De- Eisenberg, N., & McNally, S. (1993). Socialization and mothers and
velopmental Psychology, 27, 448455. adolescents empathy-related characteristics. Psychological Bul-
Du Rocher Schudlich, T., Shamir, H., & Cummings, E. M. (2004). letin, 101, 91119.
Marital conf lict, childrens representations of family relation- Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Childrens emotion-related
ships, and childrens dispositions towards peer conf lict strategies. regulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30,
Social Development, 13, 171192. 190229.
Dweck, C., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to mo- Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. (2004). Emotion-related regulation:
tivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256273. Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75(2), 334339.
Ebata, A., & Moos, R. (1991). Coping and adjustment in distressed Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T., Fabes, R., Reiser, M., Cumberland, A.,
and healthy adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy- Shepard, S., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control and
chology, 12, 3354. impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child Devel-
Edwards, R., Manstead, A., & MacDonald, C. J. (1984). The relation- opment, 75, 2546.
ship between childrens sociometric status and ability to recog- Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Regulation, re-
nize facial expressions of emotion. European Journal of Social siliency, and quality of social functioning. Self and Identity, 1,
Psychology, 14, 235238. 121128.
Eisenberg, A. R. (1986). Teasing: Verbal play in two Mexicano Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression. New York: Aca-
homes. In B. Schieff lin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization demic Press.
across cultures (pp. 182198). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Ekman, P. (1995). Strong evidence for universals in facial expres-
University Press. sions: A reply to Russells mistaken critique. Psychological Bul-
Eisenberg, N. (2003). Prosocial behavior, empathy, and sympathy. letin, 115, 268287.
In M. Bornstein & L. Davidson (Eds.), Well-being: Positive de- Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood
velopment across the life course (pp. 253265). Mahwah, NJ: Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Erlbaum. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In N. nervous activity distinguishes between emotions. Science (221),
Eisenberg (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development 12081210.
(5th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 701778). New York: Wiley. Ekman, P., Sorensen, E., & Friesen, W. (1969). Pan-cultural elements
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Bernzweig, J., Karbon, M., Poulin, R., & in the facial expression of emotions. Science (164), 8688.
Hanish, L. (1993). The relations of emotionality and regulation to Emde, R., Gaensbauer, T., & Harmon, R. (1976). Emotional expres-
preschoolers social skills and sociometric status. Child Develop- sion in infancy: A biobehavioral studyPsychological issues (Vol.
ment, 64, 14181438. 10, No. 37). New York: International Universities Press.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R., Miller, P. A., & Emde, R. N., Kligman, D. H., Reich, J. H., & Wade, T. D. (1978).
Lindholm, E. (1988). Differentiation of vicariously induced emo- Emotional expression in infancy: Vol. 1. Initial studies of social
tional reactions in children. Developmental Psychology, 24, signaling and an emergent model. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum
237246. (Eds.), The development of af fect (pp. 125148). New York:
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Carlo, G., & Karbon, M. (1992). Emotional Plenum Press.
responsivity to others: Behavioral correlates and socialization an- Fabes, R., Eisenberg, N., Nyman, M., & Michaelieu, Q. (1991). Young
tecedents. In N. Eisenberg & R. Fabes (Eds.), New directions in childrens appraisals of others spontaneous emotional reactions.
child development: Vol. 5. Emotion and its regulation in early de- Developmental Psychology, 27, 858866.
velopment (pp. 5773). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Fabes, R., Hanish, L., Martin, C. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2002). Young
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M., & childrens negative emotionality and social isolation: A latent
Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of childrens dispositional empa- growth curve analysis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 284307.
thy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and so- Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-in-
cial functioning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 195209. fant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Nyman, M., Bernzweig, J., & Pinuelas, A. control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223231.
(1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to young Fergusson, T., & Stegge, H. (1995). Emotional states and traits in
childrens anger-related reactions. Child Development, 65, children. In K. Fischer & J. Tangney (Eds.), Self-conscious emo-
109128. tions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Schaller, M., Carlo, G., & Miller, P. A. (pp. 174197). New York: Guilford Press.
(1991). The relations of parental characteristics and practices to Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness
childrens vicarious emotional responding. Child Development, to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Devel-
62, 13931408. opment, 64, 657674.
292 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Field, T., Healy, B. T., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behav- capacities. In T. Field, P. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman (Eds.),
ior-state matching and synchrony in mother infant interactions of Stress and coping across the development (pp. 4768). Hillsdale,
nondepressed versus depressed dyads. Developmental Psychology, NJ: Erlbaum.
26, 714. Gnepp, J. (1989). Childrens use of personal information to under-
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Dis- stand other peoples feelings. In C. Saarni & P. Harris (Eds.),
crimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Sci- Childrens understanding of emotion (pp. 151180). New York:
ence, 218, 179181. Cambridge University Press.
Fifer, W. P., & Moon, C. M. (1995). The effects of fetal experience Gnepp, J., & Chilamkurti, C. (1988). Childrens use of personality at-
with sound. In J.-P. Lecanuet, W. P. Fifer, N. A. Krasnegor, & tributions to predict other peoples emotional and behavioral re-
W. P. Smotherman (Eds.), Fetal development: A psychobiological actions. Child Development, 59, 743754.
perspective (pp. 351366). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gnepp, J., & Gould, M. E. (1985). The development of personalized
Fischer, K., & Ayoub, C. (1993). Affective splitting and dissociation inferences: Understanding other peoples emotional reactions in
in normal and maltreated children: Developmental pathways for light of their prior experiences. Child Development, 56,
self in relationships. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester 14551464.
Symposium on Development and Psychopathology: Vol. 5. Disor- Gnepp, J., & Hess, D. L. (1986). Childrens understanding of verbal
ders and dysfunctions of the self (pp. 149222). Rochester, NY: and facial display rules. Developmental Psychology, 22, 103108.
University of Rochester Press. Goodman, S., Brogan, D., Lynch, M., & Fielding, B. (1993). Social
Fivush, R. (1991). The social construction of personal narratives. and emotional competence in children of depressed mothers.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 5982. Child Development, 64, 516531.
Fogel, A., Nwokah, E., Dedo, J., Messinger, D., Dickson, L., Matusov, Gordon, S. (1989). The socialization of childrens emotions: Emo-
E., et al. (1992). Social process theory of emotion: A dynamic tional culture, competence, and exposure. In C. Saarni & P. Har-
systems approach. Social Development, 1, 122142. ris (Eds.), Childrens understanding of emotions (pp. 319349).
Fogel, A., & Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and New York: Cambridge University Press.
communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dy- Gottman, J. M., Guralnick, M., Wilson, B., Swanson, C., & Murray,
namic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, J. (1997). What should be the focus of emotion regulation in chil-
747761. dren? A nonlinear dynamic mathematical model of childrens
Fraiberg, S. (1971). Insights from the blind. New York: Basic Books. peer interaction in groups. Development and Psychopathology, 9,
Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expressions: An evolutionary view. 421452.
New York: Academic Press. Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion.
Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
University Press. Griffin, S., & Mascolo, M. F. (1998). On the nature, development,
Frijda, N. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency. and functions of emotions. In M. F. Mascolo & S. Griffin (Eds.),
Cognition and Emotion, 1, 115143. What develops in emotional development? (pp. 327). New York:
Plenum Press.
Fuchs, D., & Thelen, M. (1988). Childrens expected interpersonal
consequences of communicating their affective states and re- Gross, A. L., & Ballif, B. (1991). Childrens understanding of emo-
ported likelihood of expression. Child development, 59, tion from facial expressions and situations: A review. Develop-
13141322. mental Review, 11, 368398.
Gaensbauer, T. (1982). The differentiation of discrete affects: A case Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., Huber, F., & Wartner, U. (1981).
report. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 37, 2966. German childrens behavior toward their mothers at 12 months
and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworths Strange Situation.
Garner, P. W., & Estep, K. (2001). Emotional competence, emotion
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 157181.
socialization, and young childrens peer-related social compe-
tence. Early Education and Development, 12, 2948. Gumora, G., & Arsenio, W. (2002). Emotionality, emotion regula-
tion, and school performance in middle school children. Journal
Garner, P. W., & Power, T. G. (1996). Preschoolers emotional con-
of School Psychology, 40, 395413.
trol in the disappointment paradigm and its relation to tempera-
ment, emotional knowledge, and family expressiveness. Child Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1984). The effects of positive maternal
Development, 67, 14061419. affect on infant responses to pleasant, ambiguous, and fear-pro-
voking toys. Child Development, 55, 12311236.
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). The attachment interview
for adults. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Halberstadt, A. (1986). Family socialization of emotional expression
Berkeley. and nonverbal communication styles and skills. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 51, 827836.
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of
parental affect-mirroring. International Journal of Psychoanaly- Halberstadt, A., Denham, S., & Dunsmore, J. (2001). Affective social
sis, 77, 11811212. competence. Social Development, 10, 79119.
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1999). Early socio-emotional devel- Hardy, D., Power, T., & Jaedicke, S. (1993). Examining the relation
opment: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback of parenting to childrens coping with everyday stress. Child De-
model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understand- velopment, 64, 18291841.
ing others in the first months of life (pp. 101136). Mahwah, NJ: Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion. Oxford, England: Black-
Erlbaum. well.
Gianino, A., & Tronick, E. (1988). The mutual regulation model: The Harris, P. L. (1995). Childrens awareness and lack of awareness of
infants self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive mind and emotion. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester
References 293

Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 6. Emotion, Hubbard, J. (2001). Emotion expression processes in childrens peer
cognition and representation (pp. 3537). Rochester, NY: Univer- interaction: The role of peer rejection, aggression, and gender.
sity of Rochester Press. Child Development, 72, 14261438.
Harris, P. L. (1997). Between strange situations and false beliefs: Hubbard, J., & Coie, J. (1994). Emotional correlates of social compe-
Working models and theories of mind. In W. Koops, J. Hoeksma, tence in childrens peer relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
& D. van den Boom (Eds.), Early mother-child interaction and at- 40, 120.
tachment: Old and new approaches (pp. 187199). Amsterdam: Isley, S. L., ONeil, R., Clatfelter, D., & Parke, R. D. (1999). Parent
Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. and child expressed affect and childrens social competence:
Harris, P. L. (1998). Fictional absorption: Emotional responses to Modeling direct and indirect pathways. Developmental Psychol-
make-believe. In S. Brten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication ogy, 35, 547560.
and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 336353). Paris: Cambridge Izard, C. E. (1972). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Cen-
University Press. tury-Crofts.
Harris, P. L. (2000). Understanding emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Havi- Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.
land (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (2nd ed., pp. 281292). New
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum
York: Guilford Press.
Press.
Harris, P. L., & Gross, D. (1988). Childrens understanding of real
Izard, C. E. (1995). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence
and apparent emotion. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R.
from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological
Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 295314). Cam-
Bulletin, 115, 288299.
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Harris, P., & Lipian, M. S. (1989). Understanding emotion and expe- Izard, C. E. (2004). The generality-specificity issue in infants emo-
riencing emotion. In C. Saarni & P. Harris (Eds.), Childrens un- tion responses: A comment on Bennett, Bendersky, and Lewis,
derstanding of emotion (pp. 241258). Cambridge: Cambridge 2002. Infancy, 6, 417423.
University Press. Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B., Schoff, K., & Fine, S. (2000). Self-
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self. New York: Guilford organization of discrete emotions, emotion patterns and emotion-
Press. cognition relations. In M. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion,
development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches
Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Childrens understanding of the
to emotional development (pp. 1536). Cambridge: Cambridge
simultaneity of two emotions: A five-stage developmental acqui-
University Press.
sition sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388399.
Izard, C. E., & Dougherty, L. (1982). Two complementary systems
Harter, S., & Lee, L. (1989, April). Manifestations of true and not
for measuring facial expressions in infants and children. In C. E.
true selves in adolescents. Paper presented at the biennial meeting
Izard (Ed.), Measuring emotions in infants and children
of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City,
(pp. 97126). New York: Cambridge University Press.
MO.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D.
Harter, S., Waters, P. L., Whitesell, N., & Dastelic, D. (1998). Level
(2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in
of voice among female and male high school students: Relational
development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
context, support, and gender orientation. Child Development, 34,
velopment, 66(Serial No. 265), vi131.
892901.
Harter, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (1989). Developmental changes in chil- Janoff-Bulman, R., & Frantz, C. (1997). The impact of trauma on
drens understanding of single, multiple, and blended emotion meaning: From meaningless world to meaningful life. In M. Power
concepts. In C. Saarni & P. Harris (Eds.), Childrens understand- & C. Brewer (Eds.), The transformation of meaning in psychologi-
ing of emotion (pp. 81116). Cambridge: Cambridge University cal therapies (pp. 91106). London: Wiley.
Press. Jones, S. S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. (1991). An audience effect on
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional con- smile production in 10-month-old infants. Psychological Science,
tagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2(1), 4549.
Haviland, J., & Lelwica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: Josephs, I. E. (1994). Display rule behavior and understanding in pre-
Ten-week-old infants responses to three emotional expressions. school children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 301326.
Developmental Psychology, 23, 97104. Kagan, J. (1971). Change and continuity in infancy. New York:
Hertenstein, M., & Campos, J. (2001). Emotion regulation via mater- Wiley.
nal touch. Infancy, 2, 549566. Kagan, J. (1984). The idea of emotion in human development. In C. E.
Hertenstein, M. J., & Campos, J. J. (2004). The retention effects of an Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and
adults emotional displays on infant behavior. Child Development, behavior (pp. 3872). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
75(2), 595613. sity Press.
Holodynski, M. (2004). The miniaturization of expression in the de- Kagan, J. (1994). On the nature of emotion. Monographs of the Soci-
velopment of emotional self-regulation. Developmental Psychol- ety for Research in Child Development, 59(2/3), 724, 250283.
ogy, 40, 1628. Kahana-Kalman, R., & Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). The role of
Holodynski, M., & Freidlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions person familiarity in young infants perception of emotional ex-
and their regulation: A socioculturally based internalization pressions. Child Development, 72(2), 352369.
model. Boston: Kluwer Academic. Kaitz, M., Meschulach-Sarfaty, O., Auerback, J., & Eidelman, A.
Howard, M. S., & Medway, F. (2004). Adolescents attachment and (1988). A reexamination of newborns ability to imitate facial ex-
coping with stress. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 391402. pressions. Developmental Psychology, 24, 37.
294 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Levine, L. (1995). Young childrens understanding of the causes of


Karbon, M., Fabes, R., Carlo, G., & Martin, C. L. (1992). Preschool-
anger and stress. Child Development, 66, 697709.
ers beliefs about sex and age differences in emotionality. Sex
Roles, 27, 377390. LeVine, R., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H.,
Keefer, C. H., et al. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from
Keefer, C., Dixon, S., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. (1991). Cultural
Africa. New York: Cambridge University Press.
mediation between newborn behavior and later development: Im-
Lewis, M. (1992). Shame: The exposed self. New York: Free Press.
plications for methodology in cross-cultural research. In J. Nu-
gent, B. Lester, & T. Brazelton (Eds.), The cultural context of Lewis, M. (1993). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride,
infancy: Vol. 2. Multicultural and interdisciplinary approaches to shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), The handbook
parent-infant relations (pp. 3961). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. of emotions (pp. 563573). New York: Guilford Press.
Kenney, M., Mason, W., & Hill, S. (1979). Effects of age, objects and Lewis, M. (1995). Embarrassment: The emotion of self-exposure and
visual experience on affective responses of rhesus monkeys to evaluation. In J. Tangney & K. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emo-
strangers. Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 176184. tions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride
(pp. 198218). New York: Guilford Press.
Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. (1994). Introduction to cultural psychol-
Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate: Why the past does not predict the fu-
ogy and emotion research. In S. Kitayama & H. Markus (Eds.),
ture. New York: Guilford Press.
Emotion and culture (pp. 119). Washington, DC: American Psy-
chological Association. Lewis, M. (1998). The development and structure of emotions. In M.
Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal Mascolo & S. Griffin (Eds.), What develops in emotional develop-
facial expression. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 447465. ment? (pp. 2950). New York: Plenum Press.
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. Lewis, M. (2000). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride,
(1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in in- shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of
fancy. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, re- Emotions (2nd ed., pp. 623636). New York: Guilford Press.
search and experience (pp. 5786). New York: Academic Press. Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acqui-
Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development in children with dif- sition of self. New York: Plenum Press.
ferent attachment histories: The first three years. Child Develop- Lewis, M., Hitchcock, D. F., & Sullivan, M. W. (2004). Physiological
ment, 72, 474490. and emotional reactivity to learning and frustration. Infancy, 6,
Kochanska, G., & Thompson, R. (1997). The emergence and develop- 121143.
ment of conscience in toddlerhood and early childhood. In J. Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Childrens emotions and moods:
Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens internal- Developmental theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press.
ization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 5377). Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1985). Faces as signs and symbols. In G.
New York: Wiley. Zivin (Ed.), The development of expressive behavior
Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins (pp. 153178). New York: Academic Press.
of empathic concern: A 26-year longitudinal study. Journal of Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2002). Cortisol response to embarrassment
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709717. and shame. Child Development, 73, 10341045.
Lagattuta, K., Wellman, H., & Flavell, J. (1997). Preschoolers un- Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition,
derstanding of the link between thinking and feeling: Cogni- personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the second year.
tive cuing and emotional change. Child Development, 68, Child Development, 75, 18211831.
10811104. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., & Vasen, A. (1987). Making faces: Age and
Laible, D. (2004). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with emotion differences in the posing of emotional expressions. De-
child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional com- velopmental Psychology, 23, 690697.
petence. Developmental Psychology, 40, 979992. Lewis, M. D. (2000). Emotional self-organization at three time
Laird, J. (1984). The real role of facial response in the experience of scales. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development,
emotions: A reply to Tourangeau and Ellsworth and others. Jour- and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 909917. development (pp. 3769). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
Landers, C. (1989). A psychobiological study of infant development versity Press.
in South India. In J. K. Nugent & B. M. Lester (Eds.), The cultural Lewis, M. D., & Granic, I. (1999). Self-organization of cognition-
context of infancy: Vol. 1. Biology, culture and infant development emotion interactions. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Hand-
(pp. 169207). Westport, CT: Ablex. book of cognition and emotion (pp. 683701). New York: Wiley.
Lawson, K. R., & Ruff, H. A. (2004). Early attention and negative Lewis, M. D., & Granic, I. (Eds.). (2000). Emotion, development, and
emotionality predict later cognitive and behavioural function. In- self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional de-
ternational Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(2), 157165. velopment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford Lipton, E. L., Steinschneider, A., & Richmond, J. B. (1965). Swad-
University Press. dling, a child care practice: Historical, cultural and experimental
Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. New observations. Pediatrics, 34, 521567.
York: Springer. Luminet, O., Bouts, P., Delie, F., Manstead, A., & Rim, B. (2000).
Lazarus, R., & Averill, J. (1972). Emotion and cognition with special Social sharing of emotion following exposure to a negatively va-
reference to anxiety. In C. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current di- lenced situation. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 661688.
rections in theory and research (pp. 242283). New York: Aca- Lutz, C. (1988). Ethnographic perspectives on the emotion lexicon.
demic Press. In V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, & N. Frijda (Eds.), Cognitive per-
Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New spectives on emotion and motivation (pp. 399419). Norwell, MA:
York: Springer Verlag. Kluwer Academic.
References 295

Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self- graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1/2,
focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal prob- Serial No. 209) 276297.
lem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
Montague, D., & Walker-Andrews, A. (2002). Mothers, fathers, and
176190.
infants: The role of person familiarity and parental involvement in
Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth infants perceptions of emotion expressions. Child Development,
of the human infant. New York: Basic Books. 73, 13391352.
Marriage, K., & Cummins, R. (2004). Subjective quality of life and Moore, G. A., & Calkins, S. (2004). Infants vagal regulation in the
self-esteem in children: The role of primary and secondary con- still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother-in-
trol in coping with everyday stress. Social Indicators Research, 66, fant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40, 10681080.
107122.
Moore, G. A., Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (2001). Infant affective
Martin, G. B., & Clark, R. D. (1982). Distress crying in neonates: responses to mothers still face at 6 months differentially predict
Species and peer specificity. Developmental Psychology, 18, externalizing and internalizing behaviors at 18 months. Develop-
39. mental Psychology, 37, 706714.
Mascolo, M., & Fischer, K. (1995). Developmental transformations in Morelli, G., Rogoff, B., Oppenheim, D., & Goldsmith, D. (1992). Cul-
appraisals for pride, shame, and guilt. In J. Tangney & K. Fischer tural variation in infants sleeping arrangements: Questions of in-
(Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, dependence. Developmental Psychology, 28, 604613.
embarrassment and pride (pp. 64113). New York: Guilford
Morris, M., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and
Press.
Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Per-
Mascolo, M. F., & Harkins, D. (1998). Toward a component systems sonality and Social Psychology, 67, 949971.
approach to emotional development. In M. Mascolo & S. Griffin
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Ev-
(Eds.), What develops in emotional development? (pp. 189217).
idence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12
New York: Plenum Press.
and 18 months. Child Development, 72, 718735.
Mastropieri, D., & Turkewitz, G. (1999). Prenatal experience and
Mumme, D., Bushnell, E., DiCorcia, J., & Lariviere, L. (in press). In-
neonatal responsiveness to vocal expressions of emotion. Develop-
fants use of gaze cues to interpret others actions and emotional
mental Psychobiology, 35(3), 204214.
reactions. In R. Flom, K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.), The ontogeny of
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2000). Emotional intelligence gaze following. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
as zeitgeist, as personality, and as a mental ability. In R. Bar-On
Mumme, D., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning
& J. D. Parker (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence
from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child
(pp. 92117). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Development, 74, 221237.
McIntosh, D. N., Druckman, D., & Zajonc, R. B. (1994). Socially in-
duced affect. In D. Druckman & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Learning, re- Mumme, D., Fernald, A., & Herrera, C. (1996). Infants responses to
membering, believing: Enhancing human performance facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing para-
(pp. 251276, 364371). Washington, DC: National Academy digm. Child Development, 67, 32193237.
Press. OBrien, M., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1995). Relation among
Mead, M. (1954). The swaddling hypothesis: Its reception. American marital conf lict, child coping, and child adjustment. Journal of
Anthropologist, 56, 395409. Clinical Child Psychology, 24, 346361.
Meerum Terwogt, M., & Olthof, T. (1989). Awareness and self- Ochs, E. (1986). From feelings to grammar: A Samoan case study. In
regulation of emotion in young children. In C. Saarni & P. Harris B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across
(Eds.), Childrens understanding of emotion (pp. 209237). Cam- cultures (pp. 251272). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
bridge: Cambridge University Press. sity Press.
Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist con- Osofsky, J. (1999). The impact of violence on children. Future of
texts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 6874. Children, 9(3), 3349.
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. (1992). Cultural variations in emotion: A Oster, H. (1995). Baby FACS: Analyzing facial movement in infants.
review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 179204. Unpublished manuscript.
Messinger, D. S., Fogel, A., & Dickson, K. L. (1999). Whats in a Oster, H. (in press). The repertoire of infant facial expressions: An
smile? Developmental Psychology, 35, 701708. ontogenetic perspective. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), The future
Miller, P., Kliewer, W., Hepworth, J., & Sandler, I. (1994). Maternal of emotions. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
socialization of childrens postdivorce coping: Development of a Parker, E. H., Hubbard, J., Ramsden, S., Relyea, N., Dearing, K.,
measurement model. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol- Smithmyer, C., et al. (2001). Childrens use and knowledge of
ogy, 15, 457487. display rules for anger following hypothetical vignettes versus
Miller, S. M., & Green, M. L. (1985). Coping with stress and frustra- following live peer interaction. Social Development, 10,
tion: Origins, nature, and development. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni 528557.
(Eds.), The socialization of emotions (pp. 263314). New York: Phillips, A., Wellman, H., & Spelke, E. (2002). Infants ability to con-
Plenum Press. nect gaze and emotion expressions to intentional actions. Cogni-
Miyake, K., Campos, J., Kagan, J., & Bradshaw, D. (1986). Issues in tion, 85, 5378.
socioemotional development in Japan. In H. Azuma, K. Hakuta, & Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recog-
H. Stevenson (Eds.), Kodomo: Child development and education in nizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and
Japan (pp. 238261). San Francisco: Freeman. neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36, 679688.
Miyake, K., Chen, S., & Campos, J. (1985). Infant temperament, Ramsden, S., & Hubbard, J. (2002). Family expressiveness and
mothers mode of interaction, and attachment in Japan. Mono- parental emotion coaching: Their role in childrens emotion
296 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

regulation and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Saarni, C. (1989a). Childrens beliefs about emotion. In M. Luszez &
30, 657667. T. Nettelbeck (Eds.), Psychological development: Perspectives
Rapacholi, B. (1998). Infants use of attentional cue to identify the across the life-span (pp. 6978). North-Holland: Elsevier Science
referent of another persons emotional expression. Developmental Publishers.
Psychology, 34, 10171025. Saarni, C. (1989b). Childrens understanding of strategic control of
Raver, C. C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for the role of emotional expression in social transactions. In C. Saarni & P.
young childrens emotional development for early school readi- Harris (Eds.), Childrens understanding of emotion (pp. 181208).
ness. Social Policy Report, 16(3), 318. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Reimer, M. (1996). Sinking into the ground: The development and Saarni, C. (1990). Psychometric properties of the Parental Attitude
consequences of shame in adolescence. Developmental Review, 16, toward Childrens Expressiveness Scale (PACES). Unpublished
321363. manuscript. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED317-301)
Ridgeway, D., Waters, E., & Kuczaj, S. A. (1985). Acquisition of
emotion-descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabu- Saarni, C. (1992). Childrens emotional-expressive behaviors as regu-
lary norms for ages 18 month to 6 years. Developmental Psychol- lators of others happy and sad states. New Directions for Child
ogy, 21, 901908. Development, 55, 91106.
Saarni, C. (1997a). Coping with aversive feelings. Motivation and
Roberts, W. L., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy, emotional expressive-
Emotion, 21, 4563.
ness, and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67, 449470.
Saarni, C. (1997b). Emotional competence and self-regulation in
Rochat, P., Striano, T., & Blatt, L. (2002). Four 6-month-olds respond
childhood. In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional develop-
to still face irrespective of expression ( happy, sad, neutral) while
ment and emotional intelligence (pp. 3566). New York: Basic
2-month-olds disrupted less by happy expression more evidence
Books.
for early contagion? Infant and Child Development, 11,
289303. Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New
York: Guilford Press.
Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoret-
ical review. Social Development, 6, 111135. Saarni, C. (in press). The development of emotional competence:
Rossman, B. R. (1992). School-age childrens perceptions of coping Pathways for helping children become emotionally intelligent. In
with distress: Strategies for emotion regulation and the modera- R. Bar-On, J. Maree, & M. J. Elias (Eds.), Educating children and
tion of adjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, adults to be emotionally intelligent: Guidelines for improving per-
33, 13731397. formance. Rondebosch, South Africa: Heinemann Educational
Publishers.
Rothbart, M., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg
(Ed.), Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., Vol. Saarni, C., & Buckley, M. (2002). Childrens understanding of emo-
3, pp. 105176). New York: Wiley. tion communication in families. In R. A. Fabes (Ed.), Emotions
and the family: Vol. 34. Marriage and Family Review (Special
Ruble, D., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In N. Eisen-
issue, pp. 213242). New York: Guilford Press.
berg (Ed.), Social, emotional and personality development (Vol. 3,
pp. 9331016). New York: Wiley. Saarni, C., Mumme, D. L., & Campos, J. J. (1998). Emotional devel-
Russell, J., & Ridgeway, D. (1983). Dimensions underlying childrens opment: Action, communication, and understanding. In W.
emotion concepts. Developmental Psychology, 19, 795804. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotion. Psy-
opment (5th ed., pp. 237309). New York: Wiley.
chological Bulletin, 110, 426450.
Saarni, C., & Weber, H. (1999). Emotional displays and dissemblance
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from
in childhood: Implications for self-presentation. In P. Philippot,
facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psycho-
R. S. Feldman, & E. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal
logical Bulletin, 115, 102141.
behavior (pp. 71105). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
Russell, J. A. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies be- sity Press.
yond minimal universality? Psychological Bulletin, 118(3),
379391. Sackett, G. (1966). Monkeys reared in isolation with pictures as vi-
sual input. Science, 154, 175176.
Russell, J. A., Fernandez-Dols, J. M., Manstead, A., & Wellenkamp,
J. (Eds.). (1996). Everyday conceptions of emotion: An introduc- Sagi, A., Lamb, M., Lewkowicz, K., Shoham, R., Dvir, R., & Estes, D.
tion to the psychology, anthropology and linguistics of emotion. (1985). Security of infant-monther, -father, and -metapelet at-
Hingham, MA: Kluwer Press. tachments among kibbutz reared Israeli children. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development. 50(1-2, Serial No.
Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of
209) 257275.
emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 124, 165196. Sagi, A. H., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the new-
born. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175176.
Saarni, C. (1979). Childrens understanding of display rules for ex-
pressive behavior. Developmental Psychology, 15, 424429. Sandstrom, M. J. (2004). Pitfalls of the peer world: How children
Saarni, C. (1985). Indirect processes in affect socialization. In M. cope with common rejection experiences. Journal of Abnormal
Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), The socialization of emotions Child Psychology, 32, 6781.
(pp. 187209). New York: Plenum Press. Scarr, S., & Salapatek, P. (1970). Patterns of fear development during
Saarni, C. (1988). Childrens understanding of the interpersonal con- infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 16, 5390.
sequences of dissemblance of nonverbal emotional-expressive be- Schef len, A. (1974). How behavior means. Garden City, NY: Anchor
havior. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 275294. Books.
References 297

Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review and model plays, and early child antisocial behavior. Social Development, 12,
for further research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 143165. 335360.
Schultz, D., Izard, C., & Ackerman, B. (2000). Childrens anger attri- Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Ma-
bution bias: Relations to family environment and social adjust- ternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior
ment. Social Development, 9, 284301. of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195200.
Serrano, J. M., Iglesias, J., & Loeches, A. (1992). Visual discrimina- Southam-Gerow, M., & Kendall, P. (2002). Emotion regulation and
tion and recognition of facial expressions of anger, fear, and sur- understanding: Implications for child psychopathology and ther-
prise in 4- to 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, apy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 189222.
25, 411425.
Spitz, R. (1965). The first year of life. New York: International Uni-
Serrano, J. M., Iglesias, J., & Loeches, A. (1995). Infants responses versities Press.
to adult static facial expressions. Infant Behavior and Develop- Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. Osofsky
ment, 18, 477482. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 462516). New York:
Sethi, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1997). Gender differences in inter- Wiley.
nal and external focusing among adolescents. Sex Roles, 37, Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational
687700. construct. Child Development, 48, 11841199.
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & OConnor, C. (1987). Emotion Stein, N., Trabasso, T., & Liwag, M. (2000). A goal appraisal theory
knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of emotional understanding: Implications for development and
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 10611086. learning. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
Shennum, W. A., & Bugental, D. B. (1982). The development of con- (2nd ed., pp. 436457). New York: Guilford Press.
trol over affective expression in nonverbal behavior. In R. S. Feld- Stenberg, C., & Campos, J. (1990). The development of anger expres-
man (Ed.), Development of nonverbal behavior in children sions in infancy. In N. L. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso
(pp. 101118). New York: Springer Verlag. (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among (pp. 247282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
school-age children: The development and validation of a new cri- Stern, D. (1974). Mothers and infants at play: The dyadic interaction
terion q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906916. involving facial, vocal, and gaze behavior. In M. Lewis & L.
Shields, S. (1995). The role of emotion beliefs and values in gender Rosenblum (Eds.), The ef fect of the infant upon the caregiver
development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Review of personality and (pp. 187214). New York: Wiley.
social psychology (Vol. 15, pp. 212232). Thousand Oaks, Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York:
CA: Sage. Basic Books.
Shipman, K., & Zeman, J. (2001). Socialization of childrens emotion Stipek, D. (1995). The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In
regulation in mother-child dyads: A development psychopathology J. Tangney & K. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psy-
perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 317336. chology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 237252).
Shipman, K., Zeman, J., Nesin, A., & Fitzgerald, M. (2003). Chil- New York: Guilford Press.
drens strategies for displaying anger and sadness: What works Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1997). Childrens personal distance and
with whom? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 100122. their empathy: Indices of interpersonal closeness. International
Shipman, K., Zeman, J., Penza, S., & Champion, K. (2000). Emotion Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 385403.
management skills in sexually maltreated and nonmaltreated Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. L. (2004a). Childrens anger, emotional ex-
girls: A developmental psychopathology perspective. Develop- pressiveness, and empathy: Relations with parents empathy,
ment and Psychopathology, 12, 4762. emotional expressiveness, and parenting practices. Social Devel-
Shweder, R. A. (1993). The cultural psychology of the emotions. In opment, 13, 229254.
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. L. (2004b). Empathy and observed anger
(pp. 417434). New York: Guilford Press. and aggression in five-year-olds. Social Development, 13, 113.
Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborns response to the cry of another in- Strelau, J. (1987). Emotion as a key concept in temperament re-
fant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136150. search. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 510528.
Skinner, E. A., & Wellborn, J. G. (1994). Coping during childhood Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2004). Contribution of facial and vocal cues
and adolescence: A motivational perspective. In R. Lerner (Ed.), in the still-face response of 4-month-old infants. Infant Behavior
Life-span development and behavior (pp. 91133). Hillsdale, NJ: and Development, 27, 499508.
Erlbaum. Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social refer-
Slade, A. (1994). Making meaning and making believe. In A. Slade & encing in infancy. Infancy, 1, 253264.
D. Wolf (Eds.), Children at play: Clinical and developmental ap- Svejda, M. J. (1981). The development of infant sensitivity to af fective
proaches to meaning representation (pp. 81107). New York: Ox- messages in the mothers voice. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
ford University Press. tion, University of Denver, CO.
Slomkowski, C., & Dunn, J. (1996). Young childrens understanding Swanson, A., Hemphill, S., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between
of other peoples beliefs and feelings and their connected commu- temperament and social development: A review. Social Develop-
nication with friends. Developmental Psychology, 32, 442447. ment, 13, 142170.
Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating. Harvard Univer- Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2002). Development of lying to conceal a
sity Press. transgression: Childrens control of expressive behaviour during
Snyder, J., Stoolmiller, M., Wilson, M., & Yamamoto, M. (2003). verbal deception. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
Child anger regulation, parental responses to childrens anger dis- ment, 26, 436444.
298 Emotional Development: Action, Communication, and Understanding

Tangney, J. P., Burggraf, S., & Wagner, P. (1995). Shame-proneness, Underwood, M., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (1996). Childhood
guilt-proneness, and psychological symptoms. In J. P. Tangney & peer sociometric status and aggression as predictors of adoles-
K. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of cent childbearing. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6(2),
shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 343367). New York: 201223.
Guilford Press. Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Con-
Tangney, J. P., & Fischer, K. (Eds.). (1995). Self-conscious emotions: tributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds social ref-
The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. New erencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7(3), 261269.
York: Guilford Press. Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R., Shepard, S., Cumberland, A.,
Teuber, H. L. (1960). Perception. In J. Field, H. Magain, & V. Hall & Losoya, S. (2004). Prediction of childrens empathy-related re-
(Eds.), Handbook of physiology: Vol. 3. Neurophysiology sponding from their effortful control and parents expressivity.
(pp. 15951688). Washington, DC: American Physiological Developmental Psychology, 40, 911926.
Society. Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Fabes, R.,
Thompson, R., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Padilla-Walker, L. (2003). So- Losoya, S., et al. (2003). The relations of effortful control and re-
cial and emotional development in infancy. In R. Lerner, M. A. active control to childrens externalizing problems: A longitudi-
Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. nal assessment. Journal of Personality, 71, 11711196.
Developmental psychology (pp. 91112). New York: Wiley. Valiente, C., Fabes, R., Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. (2004). The rela-
Tice, D., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. (2001). Emotional dis- tions of parental expressivity and support to childrens coping
tress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel with daily stress. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 97106.
bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, Venezia, M., Messinger, D. S., Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (2004). The
5367. development of anticipatory smiling. Infancy, 6(3), 397406.
Tomkins, S. (1962). Af fect, imagery, and consciousness: Vol. 1. The von Salisch, M. (1991). Kinderfreundschaften. Gottingen, Germany:
positive af fects. New York: Springer-Verlag. Hogrefe.
Tomkins, S. (1963). Af fect, imagery, and consciousness: Vol. 2. The von Salisch, M. (1996). Relationships between children: Symmetry
negative af fects. New York: Springer-Verlag. and asymmetry among peers, friends, and siblings. In A. E. Auha-
Trevarthen, C. (1988). Universal cooperative motives: How infants gen & M. von Salisch (Eds.), The diversity of human relationships
begin to know the language and culture of their parents. In G. Jo- (pp. 5977). New York: Cambridge University Press.
hoda & I. M. Lewis (Eds.), Acquiring culture: Cross-cultural stud- von Salisch, M. (2001). Childrens emotional development: Chal-
ies in child development (pp. 3790). London: Croom Helm. lenges in their relationships to parents, peers, and friends. Inter-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 310319.
Trevarthen, C. (1993). The function of emotions in early infant com-
munication and development. In J. Nadel & L. Cumaioni (Eds.), Wagner, H., & Lee, V. (1999). Facial behavior alone and in the pres-
New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 4881). ence of others. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & E. J. Coats
London: Routledge. (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 262286).
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity:
Confidence, confiders, and acts of meaning in the first year of Walden, T., & Knieps, L. (1996). Reading and responding to social
life. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence signals. In M. Lewis & M. W. Sullivan (Eds.), Emotional develop-
of language (pp. 183229). New York: Academic Press. ment in atypical children (pp. 2942). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tronick, E., & Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face inter- Walker, L. J., & Hennig, K. (1997). Moral development in the broader
action: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occur- context of personality. In S. Hala (Ed.), The development of social
rence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60, 8592. cognition (pp. 297327). East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Tronick, E., & Morellli, G. (1991). Foreword. In B. L. J. Nugent & T. Walker-Andrews, A. (1997). Infants perception of expressive behav-
Brazelton (Eds.), The cultural context of infancy: Vol. 2. Multicul- iors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological
tural and interdisciplinary approaches to parent-infant relations Bulletin, 121, 437456.
(pp. ixxiii). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Watson, A. J., & Valtin, R. (1997). Secrecy in middle childhood. In-
Tronick, E., Morellli, G., & Ivey, P. (1992). The Efe forager infant ternational Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 431452.
and toddlers pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simul- Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing and The Game. Merrill-
taneous. Developmental Psychology, 28, 568577. Palmer Quarterly, 18, 341347.
Underwood, M. (2003). Social Aggression among Girls.New York: Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. (1967). Pragmatics of
Guilford Press. human communication: A study of interactional patterns, patholo-
Underwood, M., Coie, J., & Herbsman, C. (1992). Display rules for gies, and paradoxes. New York: Norton.
anger and aggression in school-age children. Child Development, Weber, H., & Laux, L. (1993). Presentation of emotion. In G. V.
63, 366380. Heck, P. Bonaiuto, I. Deary, & W. Nowack (Eds.), Personality
Underwood, M., & Hurley, J. (1999). Emotion regulation and peer re- psychology in Europe (Vol. 4). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg
lationships during the middle childhood years. In C. Tamis- University Press.
LeMonda & L. Balter (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of Weinberg, K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., & Olson, K. L. (1999). Gen-
contemporary issues (pp. 237258). Philadelphia: Psychology der differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation dur-
Press/ Taylor & Francis. ing early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 175188.
Underwood, M., Hurley, J., Johanson, C., & Mosley, J. (1999). An ex- Weiner, B., & Handel, S. J. (1985). A cognition-emotion-action se-
perimental, observational investigation of childrens responses to quence: Anticipated emotional consequences of causal attribu-
peer provocation: Developmental and gender differences in mid- tions and reported communication strategy. Developmental
dle childhood. Child Development, 70, 14281446. Psychology, 21, 102107.
References 299

Weyer, M., & Sandler, I. (1998). Stress and coping as predictors of tion: Theory and intervention. In D. Cicchetti, J. Rappaport, I.
childrens divorce-related ruminations. Journal of Clinical Child Sandler, & R. Weissberg (Eds.), The promotion of wellness in chil-
Psychology, 27, 7886. dren and adolescents (pp. 133184). Washington, DC: Child Wel-
White, G. M. (2000). Representing emotional meaning: Category, fare League of America Press.
metaphor, schema, discourse. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones Zahn-Waxler, C. (1991). The case for empathy: A developmental re-
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 3044). New York: view. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 155158.
Guilford Press. Zahn-Waxler, C. (2000). The development of empathy, guilt, and in-
Whiting, J. (1981). Environmental constraint on infant care practices. ternalization of distress: Implications for gender differences in
In R. H. Munroe, R. L. Munroe, & B. Whiting (Eds.), Handbook of internalizing and externalizing problems. In R. J. Davidson (Ed.),
cross-cultural human development. New York: Garland STPM Anxiety, depression, and emotion (pp. 222265). New York: Ox-
Press. ford University Press.
Widen, S., & Russell, J. A. (2002). Gender and preschoolers percep- Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P. M., & Barrett, K. C. (1991). Guilt and em-
tion of emotion. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 248262. pathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of
Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Talking about emotions: Semantics, culture, depression. In J. Garber & K. Dodge (Eds.), The development of
and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 285319. emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 243272). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Wiggers, M., & van Lieshout, C. (1985). Development of recognition
of emotions: Childrens reliance on situational and facial expres- Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G., Krupnick, J., & McKnew, D.
sive cues. Developmental Psychology, 21, 338349. (1990). Patterns of guilt in children of depressed and well moth-
ers. Developmental Psychology, 26, 5159.
Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: Free Press.
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of em-
Wilton, M., Craig, W., & Pepler, D. (2000). Emotional regulation and
pathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14(2), 107130.
display in classroom victims of bullying: Characteristic expres-
sions of affect, coping styles and relevant contextual factors. So- Zahn-Waxler, C., & Robinson, J. (1995). Empathy and guilt: Early
cial Development, 9, 226245. origins of feelings of responsibility. In J. Tangney & K. Fischer
(Eds.), Self-conscious emotions (pp. 143173). New York: Guil-
Winn, S., Tronick, E. Z., & Morelli, G. A. (1989). The infant and the
ford Press.
group: A look at Efe caretaking practices in Zaire. In J. K. Nu-
gent, B. M. Lester, & T. B. Braelton (Eds.), The cultural context Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. E. (1985). Social referencing as a func-
of infancy: Vol. 1. Biology, culture, and infant development tion of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Be-
(pp. 87109). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. havior and Development, 8, 2533.
Witherington, D. C. (2003). Frameworks for understanding emotions Zeman, J., & Shipman, K. (1997). Social-contextual inf luences on ex-
and their development: Functionalist and dynamic systems ap- pectancies for managing anger and sadness: The transition from
proaches. Unpublished manuscript. middle childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 33,
917924.
Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2001). Prin-
ciples of emotion and its development in infancy. In G. Bremner & Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working
A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development models of attachment and their role for emotion regulation. At-
(pp. 427464). Oxford, England: Blackwell. tachment and Human Development, 1, 291306.
Wolchik, S. A., & Sandler, I. N. (Eds.). (1997). Handbook of chil- Zumbahlen, M., & Crawley, A. (1996, April). Infants early referen-
drens coping: Linking theory and intervention. New York: Plenum tial behavior in prohibition contexts: The emergence of social ref-
Press. erencing? Paper presented at the meetings of the International
Society for Infant Studies, Providence, RI.
Wyman, P. A., Sandler, I., Wolchik, S., & Nelson, K. (2000). Re-
silience as cumulative competence promotion and stress protec-
CHAPTER 6

Personality Development
AVSHALOM CASPI and REBECCA L. SHINER

THE DEVELOPING STRUCTURE PERSONALITY CONTINUITY AND CHANGE 335


OF PERSONALITY 302 Types of Continuity Observed in Longitudinal
Temperament and Personality: How Are They Similar? Research 335
How Are They Distinct? 302 Personality Coherence 338
Personality Structure across the Life Span 304 PERSONALITY AND THE LIFE COURSE: HOW
Personality Types 308 EARLY-EMERGING PERSONALITY
TEMPERAMENT AND PERSONALITY TRAITS: A DIFFERENCES SHAPE DEVELOPMENTAL
PROCESS-FOCUSED, DEVELOPMENTAL PATHWAYS 339
TAXONOMY FROM CHILDHOOD TO Cultivating Relationships: How Personality Shapes
ADULTHOOD 310 Friendships, Intimate Relationships, and Parenting 339
Extraversion 311 Striving and Achieving: How Personality Shapes
Neuroticism 313 Performance in School and Work Settings 341
Conscientiousness 316 Health Promotion and Maintenance: How Personality
Agreeableness 320 Shapes Health Trajectories 342
Openness to Experience/ Intellect 323 Predicting All of Behavior All of the Time? 344
Directions for Future Developmental Research on PERSONALITY AND THE EMERGENCE OF
Personality Structure 325 PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 344
THE DEVELOPMENTAL ELABORATION OF New Directions in Research on Personality
PERSONALITY TRAITS 326 and Psychopathology 344
Learning Processes 326 Empirical Evidence for Specific Associations between
Environmental Elicitation 327 Personality Traits and Psychological Disorders 346
Environmental Construal 328 Processes Linking Personality Differences
Social and Temporal Comparisons 329 and Psychopathology 346
Environmental Selection 329 CONCLUSION 349
Environmental Manipulation 330 REFERENCES 350
THE ORIGINS OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
IN PERSONALITY 330
Genetic and Environmental Inf luences on Personality
Development across the Life Span 330
Molecular Genetic Analysis of Personality 333

This chapter focuses on individual differences in per- salient personality differences between people? What
sonality, because differences among individuals are the gives rise to these differences? Do personality differ-
most remarkable feature of human nature. After all, in ences shape important life outcomes? How might per-
both genetic and cultural evolution, selection pressures sonality influence the emergence of psychopathology?
operate on differences among people. Not surprisingly, Answers to these questions are crucial for those who
individual differences pervade all aspects of life, and wish to describe, explain, and predict the nature of indi-
they demand scientific inquiry: What are the most vidual lives across time.

Work on this chapter was supported by grants from the National thank Terrie E. Moffitt, Brent W. Roberts, William G. Graziano,
Institute of Mental Health (MH-45070 and MH-49414), the Med- and Nancy Eisenberg for their helpful comments and ideas. This
ical Research Council, and the William T. Grant Foundation. We chapter reviews material available to us through July, 2004.

300
Personality Development 301

The fifth edition of the Handbook of Child Psychology behavioral attributes as samples of response classes, a
was the first edition to include a chapter devoted to per- realist conception treats them as indicators or signs of
sonality differences (Caspi, 1998; in editions prior to internal [psychological] structures (Wiggins, 1973,
that, coverage of personality development had been scat- pp. 368370). Whereas the dispositional, if-then con-
tered about in specialized chapters dealing, for example, ception of traits is agnostic with regard to explanation, a
with aggression or motivation). We were pleased to be realist conception attempts to postulate underlying
invited to contribute a new chapter, but we had to ask processes that lead traits to cause certain intentional
ourselves: Was a new chapter needed? Given the slow states (Tellegen, 1991):
progress of soft psychology (Meehl, 1978), it is hearten-
Personality is and does something. . . . It is what lies be-
ing to recognize that there have been significant ad-
hind specific acts and within the individual. The systems
vances in research on personality development over the that constitute personality are in every sense determining
past decade. In this chapter, we have tried to reflect these tendencies, and when aroused by suitable stimuli provoke
advances and to anticipate future directions as well. those adjustive and expressive acts by which personality
In reflecting on the past decade of research, we are comes to be known. (Allport, 1937, pp. 4849)
struck that debates pitting the person versus the situa-
tion, nature versus nurture, and continuity versus According to the realist, neo-Allportian conception
change are increasingly being brought to a halt. These (Funder, 1991), traits are not observable entities but hy-
three tired debates have given way to a more nuanced pothetical constructs and, like all such constructs, their
understanding of personality development. usefulness needs to be demonstrated and refuted
At the heart of the person-situation debate was the through the procedures of construct validation. The pro-
ontological status of personality traits: Are traits real? cess of construct validation is nothing more or less
It is possible to distinguish among descriptive, disposi- than hypothesis testing in which a construct becomes
tional, and explanatory conceptions of traits (Zuroff, known by virtue of the interlocking system of laws in
1986). According to the first, descriptive-summary con- which it occurs (Hogan & Nicholson, 1988, p. 622), and
ception, traits are summary variables that describe ob- the task of empirical research is to keep tightening the
servable consistencies in a persons past behavior. As nomological net (Meehl, 1986). In this sense, re-
demonstrated by criterion-oriented studies, a descrip- searchers need to embed traits in process theories that
tive-summary conception serves useful predictive pur- lead to new and testable hypotheses about social, psy-
poses. However, because this conception bypasses the chological, and biological phenomena throughout the life
explanatory work of psychology, it is unlikely, by itself, course. Trait explanations are not an end; rather, they
to yield theoretical insights about personality develop- are placeholders in an evolving search for fuller explana-
ment: It contributes no more to the science of psychol- tions of action and motivated behavior (Fletcher, 1993;
ogy than rules for boiling an egg contribute to the Wakefield, 1989).
science of chemistry (Loevinger, 1957, p. 641). Whereas the 1998 version of this chapter had to jus-
According to a second dispositional conception, traits tify its neo-Allportian focus on personality traits as
represent a tendency to behave in certain kinds of ways real, this is no longer necessary because of advances in
if in certain kinds of situations. Personality differences understanding the structure of personality. The first two
are here treated as if-then conditional propositions sections of this chapter review this new evidence in de-
(Mischel, 1990). Dominant individuals dominate when tail. We delineate a taxonomy of measurable individual
there are subjects for domination but not when they are differences in temperament and personality in child-
alone. Likewise, intelligent persons solve problems hood and introduce a process-focused analysis of per-
given the presence of problems: A person does not con- sonality traits that details what is known about their
stantly act smart. Dispositions differ from descriptive developmental antecedents and their psychological and
summaries in that they indicate nothing about the occur- biological underpinnings. A personality taxonomy
rence of behavior in the absence of eliciting stimuli serves at least three research purposes. First, it im-
(Wakefield, 1989; Zuroff, 1986). proves research communication; connecting multiple
According to a third realist conception, traits are ex- and different measures of personality to an established
planatory concepts. Whereas the descriptive and dispo- and validated personality structure helps to organize
sitional conceptions of traits outlined earlier regard and integrate diffuse research findings. Second, it helps
302 Personality Development

researchers to develop new measures of personality; lo- tions review new and accumulating evidence about con-
cating new measures in relation to what is already tinuities in personality development from childhood to
known eliminates redundancy and elucidates psycholog- adulthood and how personality differences influence
ical constructs. Third, it enables researchers to connect various life outcomes. Whereas the 1998 version of
personality measures to more elaborate nomological net- this chapter did not include much information about
works and thereby to interpret research and generate psychopathology, in this edition we devote the seventh
new hypotheses about individual differences in person- section to review how personality differences influ-
ality. As becomes evident, the personality taxonomy dis- ence the development of psychiatric disorders. We are
cussed in the second section organizes the remaining able to do this because the unproductive bifurcation of
sections of our review. personality and clinical psychology is drawing to an
A second, related debate has pitted nature versus end, and new, developmental research is highlighting
nurture. This is the longest-lived controversy in psychol- etiological and practical considerations linking normal
ogy, and there are signs that it too is dissipating. We personality variants and clinical syndromes.
begin the fourth section with an overview of research
showing a genetic contribution to personality. But
whereas the 1998 version of this chapter devoted a good THE DEVELOPING STRUCTURE
deal of space to elaborating the logic of behavioral ge- OF PERSONALITY
netic designs, in this edition we proceed directly to sum-
marizing and updating the empirical evidence and move In this first section, we address three issues that are
on to discuss the contributions of molecular genetics to central to the study of temperament and personality
psychological research. The use of molecular genetic across the life course: (1) How should we conceptualize
techniques is helping to replace the nature-nurture con- temperament and personality, and what is similar and
junction versus with the more appropriate conjunction distinctive about these two types of individual differ-
and. Psychological research is being revolutionized by ences? (2) How are temperament and personality differ-
direct measures of specific genotypes for individuals, ences structured from infancy through adulthood? and
and this will increasingly allow researchers to investi- (3) What do we know about personality types, and do
gate how nature and nurture work together to shape be- these types add something important to our understand-
havioral phenotypes. ing of personality development? It is important that we
A third debate has focused on continuity versus address these foundational issues from the outset before
change, which also subsumes the question of whether turning to questions of how individual differences de-
personality traits matter. Trait models are often cari- velop and affect the life course.
catured as static, nondevelopmental conceptions of
personality. This misapprehension arises because per-
sonality traits are thought to represent stable and en-
Temperament and Personality: How Are They
during psychological differences between persons;
Similar? How Are They Distinct?
therefore, they are static. Few personality researchers
subscribe to this conclusion. Rather, contemporary Humans display a wide range of individual differences
personality research has sought to formulate the ways during the life spanfrom birth to old age. Both child
in which personality differences, in transaction with psychologists and adult personality researchers study
environmental circumstances, organize behavior in dy- these individual differences, but historically the two
namic ways over time. Personality traits are thus orga- groups have done so in different research traditions:
nizational constructs; they influence how individuals Child psychologists have typically studied temperament
organize their behavior to meet environmental demands traits, whereas adult personality researchers have typi-
and new developmental challenges. As Allport (1937) cally studied personality traits.
noted, personality traits are modi vivendi, ultimately The contemporary empirical study of early tempera-
deriving their significance from the role they play in ment was spurred largely by Thomas and Chess, who ini-
advancing adaptation within, and mastery of, the per- tiated the New York Longitudinal Study to examine the
sonal environment (p. 342). The fifth and sixth sec- significance of biologically based temperament traits in
The Developing Structure of Personality 303

infancy and childhood (Thomas, Chess, Birch, Hertzig, infancy and early childhood is only partially heritable
& Korn, 1963). Thomas and Chess challenged the way and is influenced by environmental events (Emde & He-
that social development was studied at the time because witt, 2001), including both pre- and postnatal experi-
they emphasized that children are not merely the prod- ences. Occasionally, researchers claim that individual
ucts of their rearing environments; rather, infants come differences measured later in childhood are not tem-
into the world with biologically based behavioral tenden- perament because such traits have already been affected
cies. Like Thomas and Chess, most temperament re- by environmental experiences, implying that only indi-
searchers continue to focus on individual differences vidual differences at birth represent genetically influ-
that emerge early in life, include differences in emo- enced temperament. The behavioral genetic findings
tional processes, and have a presumed biological basis reviewed later in this chapter reveal the fallacy of such a
(Goldsmith et al., 1987). However, most contemporary claim: From infancy through adulthood, to varying de-
researchers also recognize that temperament is shaped grees and at varying times, genetic and environmental
by both hereditary and environmental influences and influences are at work shaping both temperament and
that temperament includes components of self-regulation personality traits. A fourth key similarity between tem-
and emotion (Rothbart & Derryberry, 2002). perament and personality traits is that many traits from
Personality is typically seen as including a wider both domains are characterized by specific habitual
range of individual differences in feeling, thinking, and positive and negative emotions (Rothbart, Ahadi, &
behaving than is temperament. Personality differences Evans, 2000; Watson, 2000). Although this point is
include personality traits such as Extraversion and Neu- widely accepted for the so-called temperament traits of
roticism, but they also encompass goals, coping styles, Extraversion and Neuroticism, there is evidence that
defensive styles, motives, attachment styles, life stories, other major traits involve the experience or expression
identities, and various other processes (McAdams, of emotions as well (Tobin, Graziano, Vanman, & Tassi-
1995). Although much of the research on childrens in- nary, 2000; Trierweiler, Eid, & Lischetzke, 2002; Wat-
dividual differences has focused on traits labeled tem- son & Clark, 1992). A recent study demonstrated that
perament, a great deal of productive research has infants positive and negative emotional expressions pre-
already been done on childhood traits that could rightly dicted their standing on all of the Big Five personality
be considered personality. Developmentalists have in- traits at age 3 (Abe & Izard, 1999). Thus, emotional ex-
vestigated a vast array of childrens traitsaggression, perience and expression are associated with a wide vari-
delay of gratification, dominance, achievement striv- ety of traits across the life span.
ings, empathy, anxiousness, and the list goes onbut Many of the distinctions between temperament and
sometimes have not explicitly labeled these traits as- personality traits seem to be breaking down. There may
pects of childrens emerging personalities per se be some advantages to keeping a temperament perspec-
(Shiner, 1998). tive in view. Temperament traits tend to be defined as
Recent empirical work has demonstrated a number of more narrow, lower-level traits (Strelau, 2001), a useful
striking similarities between temperament traits and complement to the emphasis on higher-level traits in
personality traits. First, although temperament differ- adult personality research. The concept of temperament
ences between nonhuman animals have been recognized also reminds researchers to investigate the early expres-
at least as long as humans have bred animals, important sions, biological underpinnings, and basic processes un-
aspects of personality traits can be observed in nonhu- derlying traits. But temperament and personality
man animals as well (Gosling, 2001; Gosling & John, increasingly should be studied side by side. A useful ap-
1999). Observers can rate traits as accurately in nonhu- proach may be to consider the broadest possible range of
man animals as they can in humans (Gosling, Kwan, & individual differences at each point in development
John, 2003). Second, like temperament traits, nearly all rather than to exclude some individual differences from
self-reported and observed personality traits show mod- study on the presupposition that some traits are not as-
erate genetic influence (Bouchard, 2004; Bouchard & pects of temperament. Because temperament and per-
Loehlin, 2001). Third, like personality traits, tempera- sonality traits share so much in common, we discuss
ment traits are affected by experience: Behavioral ge- temperament and personality systems together through-
netic studies have established that temperament in out this review.
304 Personality Development

Personality Structure across the Life Span Krueger, & Watson, 2005). We now turn to a discussion
of the traits that can be observed (a) during infancy and
One of the most striking points of convergence between early childhood and ( b) during the preschool through
temperament and personality is their similar structure adult years.
across most periods of the life span; by structure, we
mean the reliable patterns of covariation of traits across Structure of Individual Differences in Infancy
individuals. We later describe research findings on the and Early Childhood
structure of individual differences in infants and tod-
dlers and the structure observed in young children, ado- Lower-Order Traits. During infancy and early
lescents, and adults. childhood, children display a limited range of traits.
The establishment of a personality structure for de- Much of the early research on temperament in these de-
scribing adult personality has been a complicated, con- velopmental periods was derived from Thomas and
tentious enterprise; work on the structure of young Chesss nine-trait model (Thomas et al., 1963). Thomas
childrens individual differences is inherently even and Chess identified a number of traits that have proven
more complex. As childrens motor, cognitive, emo- to have great clinical significance, particularly chil-
tional, and language abilities develop, the range of traits drens tendencies toward the intense expression of anger
they can express similarly expands. For example, al- and frustration and toward fearful withdrawal from new
though infants may differ in temperament traits that are situations (Maziade et al., 1990). More recent research
likely to be related to later aggression, infants cannot ex- has uncovered some limitations of the original Thomas
hibit differences in aggression until they develop the and Chess model, however (for a summary, see Rothbart
motor and language skills necessary to direct aggressive & Derryberry, 2002, and Shiner & Caspi, 2003), and
actions toward others. Similar rapid growth occurs in other models are increasingly used in research instead.
childrens emotional development. Children develop Current models of temperament in infancy and early
rapidly from manifesting only a small number of emo- childhood derive in part from research on caregiver-re-
tions during early infancyinterest, contentment, and port questionnaires. A number of different caregiver
distressto manifesting an expanded set of emotions temperament questionnaires for young children have
including joy, sadness, anger, fear, empathy, pride, yielded very similar sets of lower-order traits. Some of
shame, and guiltby age 3 (Eisenberg, 2000; Lewis, the relevant caregiver questionnaires include: the Infant
2000). Thus, the structure of individual differences is Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart, 1981); the Infant
likely to change over the course of childhood because of Characteristics Questionnaire (Bates, Freeland, &
childrens increasing capacities. Like many other as- Lounsbury, 1979); the Colorado Childhood Tempera-
pects of development, childrens individual differences ment Inventory (Rowe & Plomin, 1977); and the Toddler
are likely to become increasingly differentiated and Behavior Assessment Questionnaire (Goldsmith, 1996).
complex over development. Despite the challenges in Additional evidence for early childhood traits derives
mapping the structure of individual differences in in- from item-level factor analyses of the questionnaires de-
fancy and childhood, substantial progress has been made signed to measure Thomas and Chesss original nine-
in this area. trait model: Although these factor analyses generate
As we illustrate in our description of temperament fewer than nine factors, they provide consistent support
and personality structure, individual differences are or- for a smaller set of traits measured across several ques-
ganized hierarchically across the life span. Covariation tionnaires (Martin, Wisenbaker, & Huttunen, 1994).
among specific behavioral descriptors (e.g., talkative or Structured laboratory tasks and home observational sys-
friendly) is explained by lower-order traits, and the co- tems also provide evidence that a number of traits can be
variation among these more narrow, lower-order traits observed and measured reliably in infants and toddlers.
(e.g., sociability or social potency) is explained by For example, the Laboratory Temperament Assessment
broad, higher-order traits (e.g., Extraversion). Individ- Battery assesses five traits in young children: (1) Plea-
ual differences exhibit such a hierarchical structure in sure/Joy, (2) Fearfulness, (3) Anger Proneness, (4) In-
infancy and early childhood (Putnam, Ellis, & Rothbart, terest / Persistence, and (5) Activity Level (Goldsmith &
2001), middle childhood and adolescence (Shiner & Rothbart, 1991). A home-observation coding system de-
Caspi, 2003), and adulthood (Digman, 1990; Markon, veloped by Bornstein and colleagues (Bornstein, Gaugh-
The Developing Structure of Personality 305

ran, & Homel, 1986) permits assessment of Positive Af- naires (Martin et al., 1994); this trait taps childrens
fect, Negative Affect, Persistence, Motor Responsivity, sensitivity to various sensory experiences.
and Soothability. Attention: Between the 4 and 8 month period, infants
Taken together, the caregiver-questionnaire studies, vary in their attentiveness to environmental stimuli
laboratory-based tasks, and observational models pro- (Rothbart, Chew, et al., 2001). Questionnaire mea-
vide the strongest support for the following lower-order sures of this trait tap infants duration of attention to
temperament traits in the infant and toddler years stimuli and their ability to notice environmental vari-
(Kochanska, Coy, Tjebkes, & Husarek, 1998; Lemery, ation. In toddlers, this trait also includes the ability
Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek, 1999; Martin et al., to sustain attention over time and persist at a task
1994; Rothbart & Bates, 1998; Rothbart & Mauro, 1990): (Martin et al., 1994).
Activity level: Activity level is an important compo-
Positive emotions/pleasure: This trait measures the nent of most temperament models; however, the mean-
childs propensity toward the expression of positive ing of this trait is likely to change with development.
emotions, including smiling and laughter as well as Motor movement in infancy is associated with both
pleasure and excitement in social interaction. Obser- anger and positive emotions, whereas motor move-
vational and questionnaire measures for infants ment in the toddler years is linked in complex ways
demonstrate that the disposition toward positive with early markers of high Extraversion and low self-
emotions is distinct from the disposition toward neg- control (Eaton, 1994). When activity level is defined
ative emotions (Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, 1996; as positive activity, it is already highly correlated
Kochanska et al., 1998). with markers of Extraversion by the toddler years
Fear/inhibition: This trait addresses the childs ten- (Lamb, Chuang, Wessels, Broberg, & Hwang, 2002).
dency to withdraw and express fear in the face of Soothability/adaptability: The evidence for this final
stressful or novel situations ( both social and nonso- trait is not as consistent as that for the preceding six
cial). This trait expresses itself in fearful, with- traits. Soothability, as measured by the Infant Behav-
drawn, and avoidant behavior in situations with ior Questionnaire (Rothbart, 1981), assesses chil-
strangers and unfamiliar, unpredictable objects drens capacity to be soothed when comforted by
(Kagan, 1998; Kochanska et al., 1998). caregivers. A similar trait indexing early regulation
Irritability/anger/frustration: In early childhood, this of emotion emerges in factor analyses of question-
trait includes fussing, anger, and poor toleration of naires stemming from Thomas and Chesss model.
frustration and limitations. This trait is an important This trait, labeled Adaptability, taps childrens ten-
component of Thomas and Chesss difficult child dencies to exhibit mild emotional responses and to
type. Infants expressions of distress during the first adjust quickly and quietly to various potentially
3 months of life often do not predict later expressions stressful environmental events (Martin et al., 1994).
of distress (Barr & Gunnar, 2000). However, by Adaptability is moderately negatively related to mea-
around 4 months, infants tendencies toward fearful- sures of Irritability, and further work is needed to
ness and anger can be distinguished and show some determine if these two traits are truly distinct from
predictive validity for later temperament (Rothbart, one another.
Chew, & Gartstein, 2001). Further, fear and irritabil-
ity each appear to be influenced by unique genetic Higher-Order Traits. Most research on early tem-
and environmental sources, further demonstrating perament has focused on narrowly defined, lower-order
the distinct nature of these two traits (Goldsmith, traits. Rothbart and colleagues have more recently ex-
Lemery, Buss, & Campos, 1999). plored the structure of higher-order temperament traits
Discomfort: Infants and toddlers differ in the extent in infancy and the toddler years by examining the factor
of their negative emotional reactions to irritating or structure of two newly expanded caregiver-report tem-
painful sensory stimulation (e.g., loud noises, cold perament questionnaires (Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003;
touches, or sour tastes; Kochanska et al., 1998). A Putnam et al., 2001; Rothbart & Derryberry, 2002). In
similar trait termed Threshold is obtained in factor samples of American infants and toddlers, three factors
analyses of New York Longitudinal Study question- emerge. In infancy and the toddler years, a Surgency
306 Personality Development

factor taps childrens tendencies toward an eager, posi- (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988; McCrae, Costa, & Busch,
tive approach to potentially pleasurable activities; vocal 1986); in other words, the Big Five traits emerge in mea-
reactivity (in infants) and sociability (in toddlers); ex- sures designed to assess other sets of traits. Second, re-
pression of positive emotions; enjoyment of high-inten- search stemming from the lexical tradition provides
sity activities; and high activity level. In infancy and the some support for the five-factor model. According to the
toddler years, a Negative Affectivity factor taps both lexical hypothesis, the most socially relevant and salient
childrens tendencies toward sadness, irritability and personality characteristics have become encoded in
frustration, and fear as well as their abilities to quiet everyday language. Accordingly, the personality terms
themselves after high arousal (reversed). The third fac- contained in the natural language may provide an exten-
tor differs in the two periods. In infancy the third factor sive, yet finite, set of attributes that people who share
measures soothability, cuddliness, ability to sustain that language have found important and useful in their
attention, and pleasure in low-intensity situations, interactions with each other. In most lexical studies, a
whereas in the toddler years this factor ( labeled Effort- set of adjectives is drawn from the dictionary to provide
ful Control) includes these traits and more sophisticated a representative sample of personality traits that are im-
self-regulatory abilities. The third factor appears to tap portant in the natural language. Factor analyses of ad-
young childrens emerging behavioral constraint and jectives drawn from dictionaries in countries, such as
regulation. As described in the next section, these three the United States, Germany, Poland, and Holland, have
higher-order traits are highly similar to three higher- resulted in five factors that are at least somewhat con-
order temperament and personality traits observed gruent across samples (Saucier, Hampson, & Goldberg,
among older children and adults. 2000); the strongest support has been obtained from lex-
ical analyses of languages deriving from northern Euro-
Structure of Individual Differences from pean origins.
Childhood through Adulthood Several three-factor models of adult personality have
We begin by describing studies of personality structure also received some support: Eysencks (1991) three-fac-
among adults, and then turn to the study of personality tor system, Tellegens (1985) model of personality
structure in younger age groups. structure, and Cloningers model of temperament
(Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993). For example,
Personality Structure in Adulthood. One of the biologically oriented theorists have often pointed to-
great achievements in the study of adult personality over ward three higher-order domains that correspond with
the past 2 decades is greater clarity about the higher- postulated neural structures underlying personality: (1)
order structure of personality. Prior to this emerging An approach domain manifested in positive emotions,
consensus, debate raged about which traits are most (2) an avoidance domain manifested in negative emo-
valid and important. Researchers were prone to the jin- tions, and (3) a constraint domain manifested as tenden-
gle-jangle fallacy of studying the same trait under dif- cies to inhibit or express emotion and impulse.
ferent names ( jingle) or using the same name to describe Although there are important differences among
different traits ( jangle) (Block, 1996). Research on these three-factor models and the five-factor model,
adult personality has been energized by emerging con- they overlap to a considerable degree. Extraversion or
sensus about personality structure because researchers Positive Emotionality is common to all systems; it de-
can now focus their attention on a common set of traits. scribes the extent to which the person actively engages
The most widespread support has been obtained for a the world or avoids intense social experiences. Neuroti-
five-trait structure, dubbed the Big Five or the five-fac- cism or Negative Emotionality is also common to all sys-
tor model (John & Srivastava, 1999; McCrae & Costa, tems; it describes the extent to which the person
1999); these traits include Extraversion, Neuroticism, experiences the world as distressing or threatening.
Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Openness to Ex- Conscientiousness or Constraint describes the extent
perience/ Intellect. Support for this model derives from and strength of impulse control in task-focused do-
two main sources of evidence. First, a number of factor mains; whether the person is able to delay gratification
analyses of questionnaires designed to measure a broad in the service of more distant goals or is unable to modu-
range of individual differences yield the Big Five traits late impulsive expression. To these three dimensions, the
The Developing Structure of Personality 307

five-factor model adds two more: (1) Agreeableness and dren and adolescents. As noted previously, Rothbart and
(2) Openness to Experience/ Intellect. Agreeableness de- colleagues have identified three higher-order tempera-
scribes a persons interpersonal nature on a continuum ment traits in infants and toddlersSurgency, Negative
from warmth and compassion to antagonism. Agreeable Affectivity, and Effortful Control. Rothbart and col-
persons are empathic, altruistic, helpful, and trusting, leagues have obtained evidence for the same three
whereas antagonistic persons are abrasive, ruthless, ma- higher-order traits in children ages 3 to 7 (Ahadi, Roth-
nipulative, and cynical. Openness to Experience (also bart, & Ye, 1993; Putnam et al., 2001; Rothbart, Ahadi,
called Intellect) describes the complexity, depth, and Hershey, & Fisher, 2001) and in young adolescents ages
quality of a persons mental and experiential life. 10 to 15 (Capaldi & Rothbart, 1992; Putnam et al.,
2001). An additional fourth factor is obtained in early
Personality Structure in Childhood and Adoles- adolescence; this factor is labeled Af filiativeness and in-
cence. Consensus about the structure of adult person- cludes some components similar to the positive end of
ality traits has important implications for developmental the Agreeableness trait. Possibly, a similar Affiliative-
research: We now have greater clarity about the adult ness trait could be identified in younger children as well,
personality traits that childhood studies should be try- if such items were included in the relevant question-
ing to predict over time. Developmental researchers have naires. This temperament model yields traits highly
explored the possibility that childhood personality similar in content to several of the Big Five traits: Sur-
structure might map onto the structure observed in gency (Extraversion), Negative Affectivity (Neuroti-
adults, and there is now evidence (from the preschool cism), Effortful Control (Conscientiousness), and
years through adolescence) from a variety of sources Affiliativeness (Agreeableness).
that such is the case. First, factor analyses of question- Third, a variety of behavioral-task and observational
naires, adjective lists, and the California Child Q-Set measures provide support for traits similar to the Big
have often produced factors similar to the Big Five traits Five (Shiner & Caspi, 2003). We describe specific be-
in studies of children from approximately age 3 through havioral-task and observational measures of traits in our
late adolescence. A five-factor structure has been ob- detailed discussion of the content of the Big Five traits.
tained in parent reports (Barbaranelli, Caprara, Thus, data from personality and temperament ques-
Rabasca, & Pastorelli, 2003; Halverson et al., 2003; tionnaires, behavioral tasks, and observational measures
John, Caspi, Robins, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, converge on a Big Five trait structure in children and
1994; Lamb et al., 2002; Mervielde & De Fruyt, 1999, adolescents. To illustrate the meaning of the factors,
2002; van Lieshout & Haselager, 1993, 1994) and in Table 6.1 lists items defining the Big Five traits in three
teacher reports (Barbaranelli et al., 2003; Digman, types of child measures: (1) teacher reports using a list
1994; Digman & Inouye, 1986; Digman & Shmelyov, of trait descriptors (Digman & Shmelyov, 1996), (2)
1996; Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Goldberg, parent reports using the California Child Q-Sort (John
2001; Graziano & Ward, 1992; Mervielde, Buyst, & De et al., 1994; van Lieshout & Haselager, 1993, 1994), and
Fruyt, 1995; Resing, Bleichrodt, & Dekker, 1999; van (3) childrens self reports using a puppet interview
Lieshout & Haselager, 1993, 1994; Victor, 1994). Factor (Measelle et al., 2005).
analyses of self-report questionnaires have found evi- Some caveats must be noted regarding the appropri-
dence for five-factor structures in one study of children ateness of the five-factor model for describing the struc-
ages 9 and 10 (Barbaranelli et al., 2003) and in studies ture of childrens and adolescents individual
of adolescents (De Fruyt, Mervielde, Hoekstra, & Rol- differences. First, the traits are sometimes measured in
land, 2000; McCrae & Costa, 2004). There is also some a less internally consistent fashion in younger children,
evidence that many preadolescents, even as young as 5 especially preschool-age children (e.g., Lamb et al.,
years of age, can provide coherent, reliable self-reports 2002). Second, studies using the California Child Q-Set
of the Big Five traits (Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & have provided evidence for additional traits such as irri-
Finch, 1997; Markey, Markey, Tinseley, & Ericksen, tability and dependency that are related to but distinct
2002; Measelle, John, Ablow, Cowan, & Cowan, 2005). from the Big Five traits (e.g., John et al., 1994); the pos-
Second, further support for several of the Big Five sibility of factors beyond the Big Five in children war-
traits derives from temperament research in older chil- rants further study. Third, the Big Five traits are
308 Personality Development

TABLE 6.1 Examples of Trait Descriptors, California Child Q-Sort Items, and Self-Report Puppet Interview Items for Five
Higher-Order Personality Traits in Children

Sample Items

Higher-Order
Personality Trait Trait Descriptors a Child Q-Sort Items b Puppet Interview Items c

Extraversion Gregarious Emotionally expressive Im not shy when I meet new people
Cheerful A talkative child Its easy for me to make new friends
Energetic Fast-paced; moves and reacts to things quickly If kids are playing, I ask if I can play too
Withdrawn (rev.) Inhibited or constricted (rev.)
Neuroticism Afraid Fearful and anxious Im sad a lot
Touchy Tends to go to pieces under stress; becomes I get nervous when my teacher calls on me
Tearful rattled and disorganized I dont like myself
Steady (rev.) Appears to feel unworthy
Self-reliant, confident (rev.)
Conscientiousness Diligent Attentive and able to concentrate I think its important to do well in school
Planful Planful; thinks ahead I try my best in school
Careful Persistent in activities; does not give up easily When I cant figure something out, I dont
Focused Reflective; thinks and deliberates before give up
speaking or acting
Agreeableness Considerate Warm and kind toward others I dont get mad at kids at school
Trusting Helpful and cooperative If someone is mean to me, I dont hit them
Spiteful (rev.) Tends to give, lend, and share I dont pick on other kids
Rude (rev.) Teases and picks on others (rev.)
Openness to experience Original Curious and exploring I learn things well
Perceptive Appears to have high intellectual capacity I have good ideas
Knowledgeable (whether expressed in achievement or not) Im a smart kid
Curious Creative in perception, thought, work, or play
Has an active fantasy life

Note: Rev. = Item is scored in the reversed direction.


a
Items defining the factor in a study of 480 Russian children aged 8 to 10 whose teachers rated them. Source: From The Structure of Tem-
perament and Personality in Russian Children, by J. M. Digman and A. G. Shmelyov, 1996, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
pp. 341351.
b
Abbreviated California Child Q-sort items defining the factor in two independent studies: (1) a study of 720 Dutch boys and girls who were Q-
sorted by parents and teachers and (2) a study of 350 African American and Caucasian boys aged 12 to 13 enrolled in the Pittsburgh Youth Study
who were Q-sorted by their mothers. Source: From Personality Development across the Life Course (pp. 311388), by A. Caspi, in Handbook
of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Emotional and Personality Development, fifth edition, N. Eisenberg (Ed.), 1998, New York: Wiley.
c
Berkley Puppet Interview items defining the factor in a study of 95 children aged 5 to 7. Source: From Can children provide coherent, sta-
ble, and valid self-reports on the Big Five dimensions? by J. Measelle, O. P. John, J. Ablow, P. A. Cowan, and C. P. Cowan, 2005, Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 90106.

sometimes defined by somewhat different clusters of school years because children undergo such rapid devel-
items in children than in adults (van Lieshout & Hase- opmental changes during these periods.
lager, 1993, 1994). For example, in one study Extraver-
sion was defined by a much broader range of traits than
Personality Types
the typical adult trait and included aspects of Agree-
ableness and Openness to Experience (Halverson et al., Thus far we have used the term personality structure to
2003). All of these findings highlight the need for con- refer to the pattern of covariation of traits across indi-
tinued study of potential developmental differences in viduals. Personality structure can also refer to the orga-
the nature of the Big Five traits. More work is particu- nization of traits in the individual. Most research on
larly needed in the preschool and early elementary personality development is variable-centered; it focuses
The Developing Structure of Personality 309

on the relative standing of persons on dimensional vari- concentrate on tasks. Overcontrollers are without many
ables. However, the more appropriate unit of analysis interpersonal skills, shy, and inward looking. Undercon-
may be the person, not the variable. Person-centered re- trollers are impulsive, willful, and disagreeable and they
search focuses on the configuration of multiple person- show little concern for others. This convergence of three
ality variables in the person, on how different personality types across multiple studies is noteworthy
dimensional variables are organized in the person and as the studies differ in numerous ways, including age,
how this organization defines different types of persons. gender, ethnicity, geographic location in which the
Such a model of the person as a system of interacting study participants grew up, the source of personality in-
components is absent from most studies of personality, formation used to derive the types, and even the statisti-
although investigators have called for approaches in cal methods by which the types were derived.
which the person, not the variable, is the focus of analy- However, the convergence across studies is not per-
sis (e.g., Bergman & Cairns, 2000). fect, especially in adult samples (Asendorpf, Caspi, &
Just as the study of personality traits has been ham- Hofstee, 2002). More typological research needs to be
pered by the absence of a structural model, the study of done before anything close to a comprehensive, general-
personality types has been held back by the absence of izable personality typology can be said to exist. In this
empirically derived personality typologies. Typological regard, five issues should be kept in mind. First, atten-
models of personality need to be held to the same em- tion should continue to be given to the replicability of
pirical standards as dimensional models of personality: the types across different instruments of trait assess-
replicability, generalizability, and construct validity. ment, the judge or rater providing the personality data,
The history of empirical research on these problems is and the method of deriving the types. Second, more in-
relatively new, dating to Blocks (1971) Lives Through formation is needed about the generalizability of the
Time. Block had clinically trained judges complete in- types across sex, both in the structure of the types and
dependent Q-sorts of the study participants. In the Q- their developmental correlates. Third, attention must be
sort technique, a sorter describes an individuals given to possible age differences in the personality
personality by sorting a set of cards containing person- types that are identified. Fourth, the search for replica-
ality attributes into piles ranging from attributes that ble subtypes must continue. Although the three types
are least characteristic to those that are most character- identified thus far are good candidates to become an in-
istic of the individual. This produces a person-centered tegral part of any generalizable person-centered typol-
description because the sorter explicitly compares each ogy, this does not mean that there are only three
attribute with other attributes in the same individual. personality types, just as the five factor model does not
The resemblance between two individuals is indexed by imply that there are only five personality dimensions. It
the correlation between their respective Q-sorts, which simply means that, at the broadest level of generaliza-
reflects the degree to which the attributes specified by tion, psychological theories must account for the devel-
the Q-sort are ordered the same way in the two individ- opment of these types. Fifth, attention needs to be given
uals. The method of inverse factor analysis can then be to the utility of types and their relative benefits, if any,
used to identify clusters of individuals with similar Q- over dimensional, trait models. Some researchers have
sort profiles. suggested, on empirical grounds, that knowledge of a
Over the past decade, several research teams have persons type membership is useful only because it is a
built on Blocks approach and uncovered evidence point- convenient summary of his or her trait standing (Costa,
ing to the existence of three personality types in child- Herbst, McCrae, Samuels, & Ozer, 2002); the evidence
hood and adolescence (Asendorpf, Borkenau, for the incremental validity of type membership (over
Ostendorf, & van Aken, 2001; Asendorpf & van Aken, dimensions) is sparse. Other researchers have suggested
1999; Dubas, Gerris, Janssens, & Vermulst, 2002; Hart, that fair tests of the incremental validity of types have
Atkins, & Fegley, 2003; Hart & Hare, 1997; Robins, not yet been carried out. Although types may not win, or
John, Caspi, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1996; Weir fare better, in a head-to-head comparison of the predic-
& Gjerde, 2002). The largest type of persons, labeled tive validity of personality types versus dimensions,
Resilients, is characterized by being adaptable to change, Robins and Tracy (2003) suggest that for developmental
self-confident, independent, verbally fluent, and able to researchers, adopting a type approach is particularly
310 Personality Development

important because it is unlikely that environmental velopmental research. Further, adult personality re-
events and contexts ever influence a single trait in isola- search is increasingly international in scope (Church,
tion. Parents, teachers, and other socializing agents in- 2001), particularly the research deriving from the lexi-
teract with the whole child, not with one trait at a time cal tradition. This international work has the potential
(p. 114). Still others have noted that, even if types do not to create a personality taxonomy that is more generaliz-
offer incremental validity, they offer intuitive appeal able and replicable (Saucier & Simonds, in press).
and clarity. The practical implications of person-cen- It is important to include lower-order traits in our pro-
tered research are easier than trait-based research to posed taxonomy. The Big Five are too broad to capture
communicate to policymakers and research consumers all the interesting variations in human personality, and
(Asendorpf, 2003; Hart et al., 2003). distinctions at the level of more specific traits are neces-
sary. The advantage of broad categories, such as those
TEMPERAMENT AND PERSONALITY described by the five-factor model, is their substantial
TRAITS: A PROCESS-FOCUSED, bandwidth; the disadvantage of broad categories is their
DEVELOPMENTAL TAXONOMY FROM low fidelity (John & Robins, 1993). Lower-order traits
CHILDHOOD TO ADULTHOOD may provide better prediction of behavioral outcomes
than the higher-order traits (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001).
In this section, we elaborate a taxonomy of personality Further, behavioral genetic research suggests that per-
traits in children and adolescents. We draw on three sonality is inherited as a large number of genetic di-
sources of data: (1) recent research on the structure of mensions that have relatively specific effects on
personality in children and adolescents, (2) developmen- personality phenotypes and a smaller number of genetic
tal research on single traits, and (3) international stud- dimensions that have broader effects (Livesley, Jang, &
ies of adult personality structure. Each of these sources Vernon, 2003, p. 78). Thus, the lower-order traits are
of information is important for different reasons. First, shaped in part by genetic influences that have effects on
the factor-analytic questionnaire studies in youths gen- all of the components of the higher-order traits, but each
erate especially useful information about the structure lower-order trait is also influenced by unique genetic in-
of the higher-order traits. Second, developmental re- fluences. It will not be possible to understand the genetic
search provides a strong source of information about the (and possibly environmental) origins of personality fully
nature of the lower-order traits because these traits have without considering the lower-order traits.
been studied using a variety of methods, including natu- In this taxonomy, we integrate what is known about
ralistic observation and lab-based studies. Third, the re- diverse aspects of each trait. First, we present a descrip-
cent work on personality structure in adults is also tion of the Big Five traits and the lower-order traits
critically important to the study of individual differ- likely to be subsumed by them (see Table 6.2). There is
ences in children. The adult personality research helps no a priori reason to assume that each higher-order trait
to link higher-order traits with their lower-order compo- will include an equal number of lower-order traits; thus,
nents and highlights potentially valid and important the number of lower-order components varies for each of
lower-order traits that may have been overlooked in de- the higher-order traits. We also note some of the lower-

TABLE 6.2 A Proposed Taxonomy of Higher-Order and Lower-Order Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence

Openness to
Higher-Order Traits Extraversion (E) Neuroticism (N) Conscientiousness (C) Agreeableness (A) Experience (O)

Lower-order traits Sociability Fear Attention Prosocial tendencies Intellect


Energy/activity level Anxiety Self-control Antagonism Creativity
Sadness Achievement motivation Willfulness Curiosity
Orderliness
Low E + N N + low A C+A
Social inhibition Anger/irritability Responsibility
Alienation /mistrust
Note: The lower-order traits shown at the bottom of the table typically load on both of the higher-order traits shown.
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 311

order traits that have been identified in adults that have traitsocial inhibitionis related to both Extraversion
not been studied in children but that may well emerge in and Neuroticism. Sociability (or gregariousness) is the
childhood or adolescence. Some of the lower-order traits most prototypical lower-order component of Extraver-
appear to load onto more than one higher-order trait in sion. It includes the preference for being with others
factor-analytic studies; we note instances where this oc- rather than alone (A. Buss & Plomin, 1984; D. Buss &
curs. We have presented earlier versions of this taxon- Plomin, 1975) and a variety of behaviors that suggest
omy elsewhere (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Shiner, vigorous, active ways of making connections with oth-
1998; Shiner & Caspi, 2003) but have made revisions to ers: talkativeness, friendliness, vivaciousness, and ex-
it based on more recent research. Second, we review pressiveness (Peabody & De Raad, 2002).
what is known about the early temperamental an- Sociability can be distinguished conceptually and
tecedents of each trait; although there is much more to empirically from social inhibition, feelings of discom-
study, a great deal has already been learned about the fort and reluctance to act in novel situations. As noted
early childhood precursors of the personality traits seen previously, fear/inhibition is a trait readily identified in
in adults and children. Third, we survey theories and ev- infants and toddlers. Shyness appears to be one aspect of
idence about the processes underlying each trait. One of a broader inhibition trait in older children. Inhibition
the great benefits of a consensually agreed-upon taxon- consists of a number of related but distinct behaviors:
omy of traits is that it allows researchers to train their hesitance with new peers and adults, wariness in physi-
lenses on how personality traits express themselves in cally challenging and unfamiliar situations, difficulty
everyday life and on the fundamental processes underly- with separation from parents, and acute discomfort in
ing variations in these traits. We thus review some of the performance situations (Bishop, Spence, & McDonald,
most interesting current work on the psychological and 2003). This trait has been measured through a variety of
biological underpinnings of each Big Five trait. observed behaviors in toddlers, preschoolers, and older
children, such as a fearful response to novel situations
Extraversion (e.g., a toy robot, a gorilla head mask, an adult dressed
as a clown) and reticent, withdrawn behavior with unfa-
Children vary in their tendencies to be vigorously, ac- miliar adults or children (Kagan, Snidman, & Arcus,
tively, and surgently engaged with the world around 1998; Pfeifer, Goldsmith, Davidson, & Rickman, 2002;
them. Extraverted children and adolescents are de- Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002). Sociability and so-
scribed in Big Five studies as sociable, expressive, high- cial inhibition represent distinct traits: Sociability is a
spirited, lively, socially potent, physically active, and pure marker of Extraversion, whereas social inhibition
energetic. In contrast, introverted youths are quiet, in- appears to be a more complex blend of low Extraversion
hibited, and lethargic. Observations of preschoolers re- and high fear or anxiety in the presence of novel situa-
veal a similar, coherent set of behaviors: high positive tions (Asendorpf & van Aken, 2003b; Eisenberg, Fabes,
affect, energy and zestful engagement, and eager antici- & Murphy, 1995; Markon et al., 2005; Nigg, 2000).
pation of enjoyable events (Buckley, Klein, Durbin, Energy and activity level are aspects of Extraversion
Hayden, & Moerk, 2002). The Revised Class Play, a that are easily observed among children. Energetic en-
peer nomination measure for elementary school chil- gagement with pleasurable tasks is a component of Ex-
dren, also yields a factor resembling Extraversion traversion by around age 2 or 3 (Halverson et al., 2003;
(Morison & Masten, 1991); children high on this factor Lamb et al., 2002). Because people become less motori-
are described by peers as outgoing, sociable leaders who cally active with age, activity level may no longer be a
wield considerable social influence. Based on observa- separate lower-order component of Extraversion by
tional measures, extraverted children indeed are more adulthood and instead may be manifest in greater talka-
talkative, more dominant, and more involved and en- tiveness, enthusiasm, and energy (Eaton, 1994). As
gaged in interaction than their introverted peers noted previously, activity level can be observed as a re-
(Markey, Markey, & Tinsley, 2004). liable individual difference in infants and is sometimes
associated with negative emotions. Similarly, older chil-
Extraversion: Lower-Order Traits
dren are also likely to exhibit individual differences in
Extraversion encompasses the lower-order traits of so- poorly regulated motor output (Goldberg, 2001), as evi-
ciability and energy/activity level. Another lower-order denced, for example, in the fidgeting and impulsive
312 Personality Development

motor movements associated with Attention- (Arcus, 2001; Park, Belsky, Putnam, & Crnic, 1997;
Deficit /Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD; American Rubin et al., 2002). Finally, a 23-year longitudinal study
Psychiatric Association, 1994). Such poorly controlled, found that highly confident, friendly, and zealous 3-
impulsive activity is not typically focused on the pur- year-olds exhibited high Extraversion as adults, whereas
suit of productive ends as in high Extraversion and is socially reticent, fearful 3-year-olds exhibited low
more likely to be associated with low Conscientiousness scores on this trait in adulthood (Caspi, Harrington,
and low Agreeableness in children (Goldberg, 2001). et al., 2003).
The two types of motor activity need to be distin-
guished in childhood. Extraversion: Underlying Processes
Another possible component of Extraversion is social Three main models have been advanced to explain the
dominance, the tendency to be assertive, to exert control basis of the Extraversion trait. Although each model em-
over others, and to capture and enjoy others attention. phasizes different aspects of the trait, the models are
Such tendencies are related to Extraversion in adults, clearly related and can be integrated. Moreover, all
but also may be associated with low Agreeableness three models of Extraversion help to make sense of the
(Markon et al., 2005). Because these traits are likely to findings regarding the associations between the emer-
be an important contributor to childrens emerging ca- gence of Extraversion and positive emotions, high en-
pacities for leadership, positive expressions of social ergy and activity, and active social behavior in the early
dominance are worthy of research attention in children. years of development.
First, Extraversion is often conceptualized as the
Extraversion: Early Childhood Antecedents
predisposition to experience positive emotions (Telle-
A number of early individual differences predict aspects gen, 1985; Watson & Clark, 1997). As noted, the expres-
of Extraversion later in childhood. First, questionnaire sion of positive emotions in infancy is predictive of later
and observational measures of positive emotions, such markers of Extraversion. These links between Extraver-
as smiling and laughter, predict childhood Extraversion sion and the experience of positive emotions are robust
(Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001; in adulthood as well; a meta-analysis obtained an aver-
Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, 2000). In one study, age correlation of .37 between Extraversion and the con-
full-face positive emotions in the Strange Situation at current experience of positive affect (Lucas & Fujita,
18 months (a presumed marker of high-intensity positive 2000). Individuals vary in the extent to which they act
emotions) predicted Extraversion at 3.5 years (Abe & extraverted throughout the course of a day; at those
Izard, 1999). Other aspects of childhood Extraversion times when they act more extraverted, individuals expe-
are also predicted by infant measures of sociability rience greater positive emotions (Fleeson, Malanos, &
(Hagekull & Bohlin, 2003), positive activity level Achille, 2002). Extraverts also appear to be more moti-
(Hagekull & Bohlin, 2003; Korner et al., 1985), shorter vated and skilled at prolonging the experience of posi-
observed latency to grasp small objects (Rothbart, Der- tive emotions than introverts (Hemenover, 2003).
ryberry, et al., 2000), and lower observed fear (Roth- Why are Extraversion and positive affect linked so
bart, Derryberry, et al., 2000). Measures of higher consistently? One possibility is that more extraverted
infant frustration also predict later aspects of childhood individuals engage in activities that promote positive af-
Extraversion (Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 2000); we fect, such as spending time with friends. Extraverted
return shortly to this interesting link between adults do engage in more social activity, which results in
anger/frustration and Extraversion. positive affect, but social activity alone does not ac-
Longitudinal research on social inhibition suggests count for the Extraversion-positive affect link (Watson,
that infant negative emotional reactivity to overstimula- Clark, McIntyre, & Hamaker, 1992). An additional ex-
tion predicts later inhibition and that inhibition is some- planation is a temperamental view of Extraversionthat
what stable for a subset of children, particularly those there are endogenous links between Extraversion and
who are extreme on these traits (Kagan et al., 1998; positive affect. There is good evidence for this con-
Pfeifer et al., 2002). However, maternal behavior ap- tention: Extraversion is robustly associated with both
pears to moderate these relations, such that inhibited pleasant (e.g., happy, good) and activated (e.g., alert, ex-
children receiving intrusive, derisive, or overprotective cited) positive emotions in emotionally neutral condi-
parenting remain more consistently inhibited across tions (Lucas & Baird, 2004). Extraverts experience
time than inhibited children receiving other parenting more positive emotions than introverts even when alone.
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 313

Extraversion is also sometimes associated with in- sociated with Extraversion are simply correlates of the
creased activated ( but not pleasant) positive emotions in pursuit of social attention.
pleasant conditions (Lucas & Baird, 2004); thus, there is
some more limited evidence that extraverts show greater
Neuroticism
positive emotional reactivity to positive events. Behav-
ior genetic research provides support for both a geneti- Just as children vary in their predisposition toward pos-
cally and an environmentally mediated link between itive emotions, they vary in their susceptibility to nega-
Extraversion and positive emotions: Sociability and the tive emotions and general distress. In the Big Five
positive affects covary because of shared genetic and studies, children and adolescents who are high on Neu-
nonshared environmental influences (Eid, Riemann, An- roticism are described as anxious, vulnerable, tense,
gleitner, & Borkenau, 2003). easily frightened, falling apart under stress, guilt-
Second and relatedly, Extraversion has been concep- prone, moody, low in frustration tolerance, and insecure
tualized as a biologically based behavioral activation, in relationships with others. Fewer descriptors define
approach, or appetitive system. The most influential the lower end of this dimension; these include traits
framework for understanding this approach system has such as stability, being laid back, adaptability in novel
been Grays (1987, 1990) model of the Behavioral Acti- situations, and the ability to bounce back after a bad
vation System (BAS). According to Gray, the BAS is a experience. As these descriptions of childhood Neuroti-
neurobiological system that responds to incentives for cism illustrate, the trait appears to include both the
appetitive behavior, including signals of reward, nonpun- childs experience of negative emotions and the childs
ishment, and escape from punishment. Individuals with effectiveness at self-regulating such negative emotions.
a stronger BAS should be highly attentive to such incen- Neurotic individuals tend to be self-critical, insecure,
tives: When this system is activated, individuals begin and sensitive to criticism and teasing. Neuroticism may
to approach or pursue goals. Measures of BAS function- actually be part of an underlying personality dimension
ing in children have recently been developed (Blair, that includes self-esteem, locus of control, and general-
2003; Colder & OConnor, 2004). Biological evidence ized self-efficacy (Judge, Erez, Bono, & Thoresen,
for an approach system derives from Davidson and col- 2002). Neuroticism thus may be one key aspect of a
leagues work demonstrating that specialized neural more general tendency to view oneself and the world
substrates for behavioral approach exist in the left ante- through a negative lens (Erez & Judge, 2001). Neurotic
rior cerebral cortex (Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & adults tend to be dissatisfied with major aspects of their
Kalin, 2003). Adults with greater BAS sensitivity do ap- lives, including their relationships, work, and health
pear to seek out experiences that produce higher levels (Heller, Watson, & Ilies, 2004). Behavioral observations
of positive affect (Gable, Reis, & Elliot, 2000) and de- confirm the questionnaire descriptions of children high
rive their sense of well-being from such positive emo- on this trait; childhood Neuroticism is associated with
tional experiences (Updegraff, Gable, & Taylor, 2004). behaviors such as making self-critical statements, ex-
This approach model of Extraversion helps to make pressing a sense of self-pity and guilt, acting irritated,
sense of the fact that Extraversion can sometimes be as- and showing signs of physical tension (Markey et al.,
sociated with experiences of anger and frustration in 2004). Higher Neuroticism may also be linked with a va-
both adults and children, in addition to its more typical riety of aversive interpersonal behaviors in childhood.
associations with positive affect (Carver, 2004; In an observational study of parent-child interaction,
Donzella, Gunnar, Krueger, & Alwin, 2000); anger may higher Neuroticism was correlated with keeping parents
be experienced to a greater degree among extraverted at a distance, seeming detached, speaking sarcastically,
individuals when they fear they may not or actually do and exhibiting low levels of upbeat, enthusiastic behav-
not obtain the rewards they pursue with such vigor. ior (Markey et al., 2004).
A third, potentially fruitful model of Extraversion is
that it represents the tendency to attract, maintain, and Neuroticism: Lower-Order Traits
enjoy social attention (Ashton, Lee, & Paunonen, 2002). Neuroticism is likely to include a number of lower-order
According to this model, Extraversion serves an adap- traits, including fear, anxiety, and sadness. Two other
tive, evolutionary function by holding others attention lower-order traits appear to be related to both high Neu-
in ways that provide rewards; thus, this model posits that roticism and low Agreeableness: (1) anger/irritability
the reward-seeking tendencies and positive emotions as- and (2) alienation /mistrust.
314 Personality Development

Based on the extensive literature on negative emo- standing of how early Neuroticism develops could yield
tions deriving from research on psychopathology, it may crucial information for prevention programs. Such re-
be possible to break down the negative emotions in- search is important in light of evidence that childrens
cluded in Neuroticism into at least three distinct but re- average level of anxiety appears to have increased from
lated lower-order traits: (1) fear, (2) anxiety, and (3) the 1950s to the 1990s (Twenge, 2000).
sadness (Barlow, 2000; Chorpita, Albano, & Barlow, As we noted previously, individual differences in
1998; Muris, Schmidt, Merckelbach, & Schouten, anger and frustration relate to both the higher-order
2001). Fear represents negative affect and bodily symp- Neuroticism and Agreeableness traits. Anger/irritability
toms arising from exposure to an actual or an imagined taps outer-directed, hostile emotions such as anger, jeal-
object or situation. The definition of fearfulness in the ousy, frustration, and irritation (Halverson et al., 2003);
psychopathology literature is highly similar to the di- in children, such hostility is often evoked by limits set
mensions of fear/social inhibition described previously, by adults. In samples of American children and adults,
in that these individual differences all involve negative an anger/irritability trait is moderately correlated with
emotions arising from actual exposure to a feared situa- both high Neuroticism and low Agreeableness (Halver-
tion (e.g., a novel stimulus or exposure to unfamiliar son et al., 2003; Kochanska, Friesenborg, Lange, &
peers); future research will need to determine whether Martel, 2004; Markon et al., 2005). In a number of in-
fear and social inhibition should be seen as distinct ternational lexical studies, anger/irritability is more
traits. Anxiety taps tendencies toward nervous apprehen- clearly associated with low Agreeableness than with
sion, general distress, worry, and physical tension when high Neuroticism (Ashton et al., 2004).
there is no imminent threat. The symptom-based mea- In some studies with both adults and children,
sures of anxiety are highly comparable to the general anger/irritability has been viewed as part of an overar-
measures of Neuroticism used with children and adults. ching Negative Emotionality trait that includes both
Sadness includes behaviors associated with depression, Neuroticism and anger/irritability (Buckley et al., 2002;
including lowered mood, hopelessness, and dejection Lengua, 2002; Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 2001; Tellegen,
arising from experiences of disappointment and loss. 1985). Questionnaire studies, lab tasks, and naturalistic
Sadness is included as a lower-order trait in some tem- observations have all demonstrated that children and
perament and personality models (e.g., Costa & Mc- adults who are prone to experiencing one type of nega-
Crae, 1992; Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 2001) but it is tive emotion are prone to experiencing other types of
usually only a minor component of most Neuroticism negative emotions as well. It is important to separate
measures, which tend to emphasize anxiety. The dis- Neuroticism from anger/irritability for several reasons.
tinctive aspects of sadness thus may be obscured when As noted previously, it is possible to distinguish fear/in-
only the general Neuroticism trait is assessed (Moon, hibition from anger/irritability beginning in infancy.
Hollenbeck, Humphrey, & Maue, 2003). For example, These two types of negative emotions appear to require
relative to other aspects of Neuroticism, adult sadness is different regulatory strategies; for example, distraction
a more robust predictor of life satisfaction (Schimmack, works to reduce anger but not fear in infants (K. Buss &
Oishi, Furr, & Funder, 2004) and global self-esteem Goldsmith, 1998). Fear and anger have different and
(Watson, Suls, & Haig, 2002). sometimes opposite effects on cognitive processing
In general, much more work is needed to understand (e.g., fear promotes risk aversion, whereas anger pro-
the development of individual differences in fear, anxi- motes risk-seeking; Lerner & Keltner, 2000). The adap-
ety, and sadness, especially given their links with a tive profiles associated with each trait differ as well.
broad spectrum of psychiatric disorders (Watson, 2001). For example, whereas fearfulness appears to protect
A great deal of productive work has focused on fear/in- against childhood aggression (Raine, Reynolds, Ven-
hibition as a risk factor for the development of anxiety ables, Mednick, & Farrington, 1998), anger and irri-
disorders (Kagan & Snidman, 1999). The emergence of tability put children at risk for later aggression (Loeber
individual differences in anxiety and sadness will be & Hay, 1997).
equally important to study, especially given evidence A final lower-order trait, alienation/mistrust has
that most adolescents with anxiety disorders do not been identified in adults and, like anger/irritability, is
show inhibited temperament as younger children (Prior, related to high Neuroticism and low Agreeableness
Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000). A better under- (Kochanska et al., 2004; Markon et al., 2005; Martin,
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 315

Watson, & Wan, 2000). This trait taps an individuals emerges as these children encounter the ill effects of
tendency to mistrust others and to feel mistreated (Tel- their behavior.
legen & Waller, 1992). Individual differences in inter-
personal alienation and mistrust have been identified in Neuroticism: Underlying Processes
research on social information processing in youths Research with adults has helped to characterize the cog-
(Crick & Dodge, 1994) and in the attachment literature nitive style, daily experiences, and interpersonal func-
(Sroufe, Carlson, Levy, & Egeland, 1999). In adults, this tioning of individuals high on Neuroticism.
trait is highly linked with poor life adaptation and may Developmental researchers have also studied the links
emerge in part from repeated experiences of failure between attention and executive control and childrens
across development (Shiner, Masten, & Tellegen, 2002). experiences of negative emotions. In this section, we
Childhood Neuroticism may include other aspects that focus on the processes underlying the anxiety, fear, and
are worthy of consideration, including childrens tenden- sadness aspects of Neuroticism and discuss anger/irri-
cies toward dependence, low self-confidence, vulnerabil- tability in our discussion of Agreeableness.
ity in the face of stress and emotional instability. In cognition, adults high on trait anxiety show atten-
tional biases toward information relevant to their per-
Neuroticism: Early Childhood Antecedents
sonal fears; such biases are consistent with a model of
A number of early childhood traits have been identified anxiety as helping to prepare individuals for potentially
as predictors of Neuroticism and its components. Emo- dangerous situations by rapidly focusing attention on
tional expression in the Strange Situation procedure at threatening material (Mineka, Rafaeli, & Yovel, 2003).
18 months predicts Neuroticism at 3.5 years (Abe & In contrast, tendencies toward depression are associated
Izard, 1999); specifically, high-intensity full-face nega- with biases toward remembering and ruminating over
tive emotions (sadness and anger) predict Neuroticism past negative experiences (Mineka et al., 2003). Adults
positively, and milder, more regulated positive emotions high on trait anxiety and adults high on depression are
(interest and joy) predict Neuroticism negatively (Abe biased toward assuming they will encounter unduly neg-
& Izard, 1999). Consistent with this finding, childhood ative experiences in the future. More generally, adult
fearfulness and sadness are predicted by infant mea- Neuroticism is associated with an emotionally negative
sures of high fear and low positive emotions (Rothbart, tone in individuals narratives about key experiences in
Derryberry, et al., 2000). Childhood sadness is addition- their lives (McAdams et al., 2004) and with greater
ally predicted by low infant frustration (Rothbart, Der- cognitive processing of unpleasant, negative informa-
ryberry, et al., 2000). In contrast, childhood tion (Gomez, Gomez, & Cooper, 2002). Thus, there is
anger/frustration is not predicted by infant fear but, in- good evidence that Neuroticism and its lower-order
stead, is predicted by infant high frustration, high activ- components are associated, at least in adulthood, with
ity level, and short latency to grasp small objects biases toward processing negative information, though
(Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 2000). Childhood the biases may vary somewhat for different lower-order
anger/frustration is predicted by early markers of Extra- components.
version, consistent with the previously discussed claim Developmental research provides an interesting per-
that high Extraversion may generate high frustration spective on why Neuroticism may be linked with vari-
when goals are blocked. Thus, childhood fear and anger ous cognitive biases. Beginning in infancy and
appear to have relatively distinct and separate an- continuing throughout childhood, greater attentional
tecedents, whereas childhood sadness shares some com- control is associated with more effective regulation of
mon infant antecedents with both. negative emotionality (Eisenberg, Smith, Sadovsky, &
Although fear and anger appear to be separate in Spinrad, 2004; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2004). Ini-
childhood, over time greater anger may come to predict tially, parents are an important source of assistance
higher anxiety and distress. For example, in one longitu- with such emotional regulation, but over time children
dinal study, preschool-age children who were irritable, develop more of their own capacities for self-
distractible, labile, and uncontrolled grew up to be more regulation. Neurotic individuals bias toward attending
Neurotic as adults (Caspi, Harrington, et al., 2003); it to negative information may arise in part from difficul-
may be the case that the greater adult anxiety experi- ties in executive functioning, particularly with atten-
enced by previously angry, undercontrolled children tion. The converse could also be true: Neuroticism may
316 Personality Development

bias individuals toward focusing on negative cues, children have recently been developed (Blair, 2003;
which could interfere with the allocation of attention Colder & OConnor, 2004). As with Extraversion,
(MacCoon, Wallace, & Newman, 2004). Davidson and colleagues have shown that there are spe-
Just as Neuroticism is associated with negative cog- cialized neural substrates for behavioral withdrawal, in
nitive biases, it is also linked with more negative daily this case in the right anterior cerebral cortex (Davidson
experiences in adults. In lab-based studies, neurotic et al., 2003). In situations that present both incentives
adults report more negative emotional responses to a va- and threats, individuals may experience an approach-
riety of negative stimuli than less neurotic adults avoidance conflict. In such situations, the goal of avoid-
(Gross, Sutton, & Ketelaar, 1998). Experience-sampling ance is likely to win out over the goal of approach (Gray
studies have demonstrated that this negative emotional & McNaughton, 1996), perhaps because negative emo-
reactivity occurs in daily life as well: Neurotic adults tions have more widespread and lasting effects than pos-
have stronger negative emotional reactions to everyday itive emotions (Larsen & Prizmic, 2004). Although
problems, including both interpersonal conflicts and more Neurotic individuals may be motivated to avoid
stress at work and at home (Bolger & Schilling, 1991; and minimize aversive experiences, they do not appear
Gable et al., 2000; Suls, Martin, & David, 1998). Neu- to be successful at doing so. Rather, they tend to experi-
rotic individuals may find daily problems to be more ence more negative life events than less Neurotic indi-
stressful, in part, because they tend to use ineffective viduals (Magnus, Diener, Fujita, & Pavot, 1993) and
coping responses, such as escape and avoidance, and experience higher levels of negative emotions in re-
high levels of interpersonal confrontation (OBrien & sponse to such experiences (Gable et al., 2000). The
DeLongis, 1996). Finally, more neurotic individuals findings on the biological and psychological processes
tend to show some distinguishing patterns of interper- associated with Neuroticism should provide impetus to
sonal behavior, including more disagreeable and submis- study similar processes associated with the development
sive behavior and less agreeable and dominant behavior of Neuroticism in children.
(Cote & Moskowitz, 1998). Neurotic adults also show
more lability across situations in their interpersonal be- Conscientiousness
haviors (Moskowitz & Zuroff, 2004), which may lead
others to form the impression that they are unpre- An overarching Conscientiousness trait taps childrens
dictable. The interpersonal behaviors associated with individual differences in self-control, in large part as
Neuroticism in adults are consistent with the previously control is used in service of completing tasks and striv-
described aversive interaction style observed in more ing to meet standards. In Big Five studies, highly Con-
neurotic children. In summary, the process-oriented scientious children and adolescents are described as
studies with adults have demonstrated that Neuroticism responsible, attentive, persistent, orderly and neat,
is associated with a variety of difficulties in emotional planful, possessing high standards, and thinking before
and behavioral regulation. acting. Children low on this trait are depicted as irre-
All of these findings regarding Neuroticism are con- sponsible, unreliable, careless, distractible, and quitting
sistent with the claim that individual differences in Neu- easily. The higher-order Conscientiousness trait is de-
roticism are associated with variation in a biologically fined by a remarkably similar set of descriptors in lexi-
based withdrawal, inhibition, or avoidance system. As cal studies with adults across a wide variety of
with Extraversion, one of the most important frame- languages and countries, thereby providing strong inter-
works for understanding this system has been a model national evidence for the nature of this trait in adult-
developed by Gray (1987, 1990). According to Gray, in- hood (Peabody & De Raad, 2002). Based on parental
dividuals differ in the sensitivity of a neurobiological descriptions of children from a number of countries,
Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), which serves to in- parents rarely describe their children by traits linked
hibit behavior in the face of potential punishment, non- with Conscientiousness at age 3 years but do use such
reward, and novelty. Thus, individuals with a strong BIS descriptors more often by age 6 years (Slotboom, Hav-
should be sensitive to signals of threats and should be ill, Pavlopoulos, & De Fruyt, 1998); parents may see
quick to withdraw or inhibit their behavior when they these descriptors of Conscientiousness as inappropriate
perceive such signals. Measures of BIS functioning in until their children are closer to school age. There is ev-
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 317

idence from at least one study that some of these more will be needed to clarify the similarities and differences
complex manifestations of self-control can be measured between the personality and temperament conceptions
with moderate reliability in children as young as ages 3 of this higher-order trait. In particular, as we elaborate
and 4 years (Halverson et al., 2003). The numerous Big in our discussion of Agreeableness, temperamental Ef-
Five questionnaire studies demonstrate that these more fortful Control may represent differences in control that
complex traits can certainly be measured in children by can be applied to tasks and achievement (as in the case
middle childhood. of Conscientiousness) and to social relationships (as in
Rothbart, Ahadi, and colleagues (2001) have identi- the case of Agreeableness). There is some evidence that
fied in children a similar temperament trait labeled Ef- childhood Effortful Control is linked with Agreeable-
fortful Control, which includes childrens capacities to ness (Cumberland-Li, Eisenberg, & Reiser, 2004).
plan behavior, inhibit inappropriate responses, focus and Questionnaire studies of the Big Five in adults indicate
shift attention, take pleasure in low intensity situations, that Conscientiousness and Agreeableness tend to co-
and perceive subtle external stimuli. In a series of stud- vary and coalesce to form a superordinate trait (Markon
ies, Kochanska and colleagues have developed a battery et al., 2005); it is certainly possible that a basic tendency
of tasks to measure childrens emerging Effortful Con- toward behavioral constraint versus disinhibition under-
trol (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Kochanska, Murray, lies both of these traits.
& Coy, 1997; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000;
Conscientiousness: Lower-Order Traits
Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Keonig, & Vandegeest,
1996): All of the tasks require a child to exert self- Conscientiousness in children includes a number of
control by suppressing a dominant response in favor of lower-order components: attention, self-control, achieve-
carrying out a subdominant response. The tasks include ment motivation, orderliness, and responsibility. Atten-
delay of a pleasant behavior (e.g., waiting to unwrap a tion versus distractibility taps childrens capacity to
toy), slowing down fine or gross motor movements, sup- focus attention, regulate attention by shifting mental
pressing a response to one signal and producing a re- sets, and persist at tasks in the face of distractions. As
sponse to another (e.g., producing different responses to noted previously, individual differences in attention
red and green signs), whispering, and Stroop-like atten- emerge in infancy. Attention versus distractibility is a
tion tasks requiring the child to ignore prominent fea- lower-order component of Conscientiousness and Effort-
tures of a stimulus and to attend to other less salient ful Control in questionnaire measures for older children
features. Although childrens performance on the tasks (Halverson et al., 2003; Putnam et al., 2001). Although
is less internally consistent at 22 months, the tasks re- attention is an important trait in most childhood tem-
veal a more coherent set of behaviors by 33 months and perament models (Shiner, 1998), it is not prominent in
measure a highly coherent, stable trait by 45 months. adult personality models. Descriptors related to atten-
Thus, questionnaire and observational studies confirm tion may have been left out of adult personality question-
that children differ reliably in their manifest levels of naire studies because such terms are often seen as more
self-control. relevant to the domains of intellect and cognition than to
Although temperament and personality models both the domain of personality. Further, by adulthood individ-
include dimensions related to self-control, the content of ual differences in attention and executive control may
these traits differs somewhat. Temperament models tend underlie most of the components of Conscientiousness;
to emphasize attention and impulse control, which are we return to this point in our discussion of the processes
individual differences that can be identified in a rudi- underlying Conscientiousness.
mentary form in very young children. In contrast, per- Four other lower-order components of Conscientious-
sonality models include not only impulse control but ness have been identified in factor-analytic question-
also traits that children do not exhibit until they are naire studies with children and adults. Self-control taps
older, such as orderliness, dependability, and motivation tendencies to be planful, cautious, deliberate, and be-
to meet goals and complete work. There is some prelim- haviorally controlled (Peabody & De Raad, 2002;
inary evidence from one questionnaire study that Big Roberts, Bogg, Walton, Chernyshenko, & Stark, 2004;
Five Conscientiousness is highly related to Effortful Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 2001). Achievement motivation
Control (Halverson et al., 2003) but much more work (also called work or industriousness) taps the tendency
318 Personality Development

to strive for high standards, to work hard and be produc- highly stable trait from 33 to 45 months (Kochanska &
tive, and to pursue goals over time in a determined, per- Knaack, 2003). Early IQ has also been found to predict
sistent manner (Halverson et al., 2003; Peabody & De persistence at tasks (Guerin et al., 2003) and Effortful
Raad, 2002; Roberts et al., 2004). Orderliness (or orga- Control (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003) later in childhood.
nization) reflects a propensity to be neat, clean, and or- Early differences in emotional reactivity also predict
ganized rather than sloppy, disorganized, and disorderly later self-control in several studies. Milder, more regu-
(Halverson et al., 2003; Mervielde & De Fruyt, 2002; lated positive emotions in the Strange Situation proce-
Roberts et al., 2004). These are all behaviors that in- dure at 18 months have been found to predict
volve the active structuring of a persons tasks and envi- Conscientiousness at 3.5 years (Abe & Izard, 1999). In
ronment. Responsibility ranges from the tendency to be contrast, several early individual differences predict
reliable and dependable to the tendency to be irresponsi- lower levels of self-control in childhood: Earlier anger
ble and unreliable (Goldberg, 2001; Peabody & De and intense joy negatively predict preschool Effortful
Raad, 2002; Roberts et al., 2004); this subcomponent Control (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003), and shorter la-
appears to measure Conscientiousness manifested in re- tency to approach and grasp small objects in infancy
lation to other people and may be a blend of Conscien- negatively predicts attention and self-control in child-
tiousness and Agreeableness (Goldberg, 2001; Roberts hood (Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 2000). These find-
et al., 2004). Achievement motivation, orderliness, and ings regarding the links between early emotional
responsibility are traits typically left out of tempera- reactivity and later self-control are provocative but dif-
ment models; these three lower-order traits seem likely ficult to interpret. It seems likely that infants and tod-
to be important for childrens development and warrant dlers with stronger approach tendencies (those with
more thorough study. intense positive emotions and a quick approach) and
Observations of delay of gratification have been used stronger anger may have more difficulty developing self-
in a number of studies to assess childrens self-control; control because they have stronger emotions to regulate;
the most well-known work on observed delay of gratifi- they must work against their eager or angry tendencies
cation is Mischels longitudinal research (Mischel & to exhibit self-control. However, it is also possible that
Ayduk, 2004). Delay of gratification is typically as- childrens early expressions of anger and high-intensity
sessed in these studies by placing children in a situation positive emotions may partly tap early difficulties with
in which they have to choose between an immediate but self-control, which could account for why these early
smaller prize and a delayed but larger prize. Although emotions predict later self-control. More work will be
childrens ability to delay in this paradigm is related to needed to clarify the transactions between the emotion-
their attentional capacities, this measure is probably not based traits and self-control across childhood.
a pure measure of self-control because childrens ap-
proach tendencies are also likely to influence their abil- Conscientiousness: Underlying Processes
ities to delay (Eisenberg, Smith, et al., 2004). Conscientiousness indexes a childs or adults active en-
gagement with various tasks; an individual high on this
Conscientiousness: Early Childhood Antecedents
trait invests greater energy in completing work, uphold-
Childhood markers of self-control are predicted in con- ing commitments, and maintaining order (Ashton & Lee,
ceptually coherent ways by several early individual dif- 2001). Conscientiousness thus should tap a persons ca-
ferences, and childhood self-control itself is remarkably pacity to exercise self-control in the service of effective
stable by the preschool years. Not surprisingly, the abil- task completion. The adaptive profile associated with
ity to focus attention in infancy predicts Effortful Con- Conscientiousness is consistent with such a view of the
trol later in childhood (Kochanska et al., 2000). trait. As we review later in this chapter, childhood Con-
Individual differences in persistence at tasks (similar to scientiousness predicts better academic achievement
the lower-order attention trait described previously) and improvement in academic achievement over time,
have been found to be highly stable from the toddler to and adult Conscientiousness is the best personality trait
preschool years and from middle childhood to adoles- predictor of work success (Judge, Higgins, Thoreson, &
cence (Guerin, Gottfried, Oliver, & Thomas, 2003). As Barrick, 1999). One reason that more conscientious
noted previously, observed Effortful Control itself is a adults may excel in school and work is that they tend to
moderately stable trait from 22 to 33 months and is a use planful problem solving as a way of handling stres-
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 319

sors in these domains of their life rather than trying to (Kochanska, Clark, & Goldman, 1997; Kochanska,
escape or avoid such problems (OBrien & DeLongis, Coy, & Murray, 2001). Conscientiousness thus may re-
1996). Conscientious adults also tend to set higher goals flect childrens and adults adoption of societys norms
for themselves, are more committed to meeting those for regulated behavior.
goals, and have greater confidence that they can meet Having considered the nature of Conscientiousness
those goals (Barrick, Mount, & Strauss, 1993; Judge & from a theoretical point of view, it is important to exam-
Ilies, 2002). The data on Conscientiousness make clear ine the more basic biological and psychological
that this trait is highly relevant to effectiveness in the processes that are likely to underlie individual differ-
areas of striving and achieving; however, it is important ences in this trait. Conscientiousness is not the same as
to recognize that Conscientiousness may be detrimental self-regulation because self-regulation is relevant to
to performance in certain contexts (see Tett & Burnett, other individual differences as well; however, research
2003, and Yeo & Neal, 2004, for reviews). An important on self-regulation may shed some light on Conscien-
area of future research will be examining in closer detail tiousness. Researchers studying the biological basis of
the ways that conscientious children and adults approach self-regulation have pointed to the importance of the
and accomplish their daily tasks and goals. prefrontal cortex for a variety of self-regulatory skills,
The importance of Conscientiousness is not restricted including working memory, emotional processing, plan-
to task-focused endeavors; rather, Conscientiousness is ning, novelty detection, resolving conflicting informa-
often associated with effective social functioning as tion, initiating action, and inhibiting inappropriate
well. For example, a study of observed interactions be- responses (Banfield, Wyland, Macrae, Munte, &
tween school-age children and their parents showed that Heatherton, 2004; Nigg, 2000). Posner, Rothbart, and
child Conscientiousness was (unsurprisingly) associated colleagues (Posner & Rothbart, 2000; Rueda et al.,
with greater exhibited intelligence and ambition and 2004) consider many of these capacities to reflect indi-
(unexpectedly) with better social skills, warmth, and co- vidual differences in an overarching executive attention
operativeness (Markey et al., 2004). Childhood Consci- capacity, which they likewise link with the development
entiousness also often predicts concurrent and later peer of the frontal cortex, particularly the anterior cingulate
social competence and rule-abiding behavior (Lamb cortex. These researchers have examined the develop-
et al., 2002; Shiner, 2000; Shiner, Masten, & Roberts, ment of executive attention across childhood. Infants
2003) and better conflict resolution with peers (Jensen- show differences in alerting and orienting, which are
Campbell & Graziano, 2001). One straightforward rea- both manifestations of a more reactive attentional sys-
son that Conscientiousness may be linked with effective tem. However, by 9 and 18 months of age, infants also
social functioning is that self-regulation is clearly im- begin to show evidence of more voluntary control of at-
portant for maintaining social relationships in both tention. Children show marked growth in executive at-
childhood and adulthood (Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & tention during the 3rd year of life, and this growth
Reiser, 2000; Vohs & Ciarocco, 2004). continues throughout childhood. Based on this model,
A second, deeper reason for these links between childrens manifest differences in Effortful Control are
Conscientiousness and social functioning may involve driven in large part by differences in executive atten-
the underlying nature of the Conscientiousness trait. tion. Some empirical evidence is beginning to substanti-
Based on socioanalytic theory, Hogan and Ones (1997) ate the link between executive attention and Effortful
have argued that individual differences in Conscien- Control (Rothbart, Ellis, Rueda, & Posner, 2003).
tiousness reflect variations in the adoption of and com- The trait of Conscientiousness is typically seen as in-
pliance with the rules and expectations of the group, as volving more voluntary control of behavior, as implied
conveyed by various authority figures (e.g., parents and by the labels Ef fortful Control (Rothbart, Ahadi, et al.,
teachers in childhood; work supervisors in adulthood). 2001) and Will (Digman & Inouye, 1986). This type of
Indeed, in adulthood some aspects of Conscientious- executive control is separate from other types of rela-
ness are linked with greater valuing of conformity tively more involuntary tendencies toward inhibited be-
(Roccas, Sagiv, Schwartz, & Knafo, 2002). Consistent havior (Eisenberg, Smith, et al., 2004; Nigg, 2000). As
with this model, Effortful Control in childhood is asso- we noted in our discussion of Extraversion and Neuroti-
ciated with toddlers and preschoolers committed cism, childrens behavior may be inhibited because of
compliance or internalization of parental rules low approach tendencies ( low Extraversion) or because
320 Personality Development

of high fear or anxiety ( high Neuroticism); these types dren interacting with their parents are consistent with
of more emotion-based inhibition can be distinguished questionnaire descriptions of such children (Markey
from the executive control associated with Conscien- et al., 2004): Childrens high Agreeableness is posi-
tiousness (Kindlon, Mezzacappa, & Earls, 1995; Nigg, tively associated with expressing agreement and
2000). Childrens emerging capacities for executive warmth, seeking agreement from parents, and seeming
control may serve, in part, to regulate the approach and to like parents and is negatively associated with compet-
avoidance systems in the service of adaptive behavior itiveness, condescending behavior, and criticalness. In
(Rothbart, Ellis, & Posner, 2004). Active, effortful con- short, Agreeableness is linked with a variety of behav-
trol in early childhood predicts better self-regulation of iors that are likely to foster congenial relationships with
anger and joy later in childhood (Kochanska et al., both peers and adults. Given the adaptive significance
2000), and, as noted previously, better attentional con- of this trait, it is not surprising that parents from many
trol is associated throughout childhood with better regu- countries spontaneously offer a large number of traits
lation of negative emotions. from this domain when they are asked to describe their
However, much of self-regulation is likely to occur at children (Havill, Besevegis, & Mouroussaki, 1998).
an automatic, nonvoluntary level. Research with adults
suggests that self-regulation of cognition (even working Agreeableness: Lower-Order Traits
memory) is often automatic, and the same is proving to In our discussion of Neuroticism, we already noted two
be true for regulation of emotion and behavior (Fitzsi- lower-order traits that are linked with both Neuroticism
mons & Bargh, 2004). Nonvoluntary self-regulation is and Agreeableness: (1) anger/irritability and (2) alien-
important, in part, because the ability to exercise volun- ation /mistrust. A number of other lower-order traits ap-
tary control of cognition, emotion, and behavior appears pear to be aspects of Agreeableness in childhood:
to be limited. In a number of studies with adult samples, prosocial tendencies, antagonism, and willfulness.
voluntary self-control appears to operate as a strength Prosocial tendencies (also called helpfulness or nurtur-
that can be depleted temporarily with use rather than as ance) encompasses childrens individual differences in
an unlimited resource (Schmeichel & Baumeister, traits that demonstrate concern for other people rather
2004). In short, automatic nonvoluntary regulation may than interest only in themselves. Children differ in their
be necessary as a complement to the more effortful tendencies to be empathic, kind, and nurturant (Eisen-
forms of self-regulation, given that effortful regulation berg, Fabes, & Spinrad, Chapter 11, this Handbook, this
has limits. It will be important for developmental re- volume). Individual differences in prosocial behavior
search to explore the development of both automatic and are moderately stable during the preschool- and school-
more voluntary forms of self-regulation in childhood. age years (Eisenberg et al., 1987; Graziano & Eisen-
berg, 1997); there is some preliminary evidence that
Agreeableness prosocial behavior may be stable from childhood to
early adulthood (Eisenberg et al., 1999) This aspect of
Agreeableness includes a variety of traits seen as very Agreeableness may possibly comprise two sets of traits,
important by developmental psychologists; yet, histori- one tapping warmth and affection and the other tapping
cally, these traits have been left out of temperament altruism and generosity (Saucier & Ostendorf, 1999).
models. The high end of Agreeableness includes de- Antagonism ranges from the tendency to be peaceful
scriptors such as warm, considerate, empathic, gener- and gentle to the tendency to be aggressive, spiteful,
ous, gentle, protective of others, and kind. The low end quarrelsome, and rude (Halverson et al., 2003): Children
of Agreeableness includes tendencies toward being ag- who are high on this trait express hostility openly to-
gressive, rude, spiteful, stubborn, bossy, cynical, and ward others. The lower-order trait antagonism includes
manipulative. In studies with both children and adults, both physical aggression and relational aggression (e.g.,
Agreeableness also includes being willing to accommo- gossiping and social exclusion). There is some evidence
date others wishes rather than forcing ones own de- from older children, adolescents, and adults that physi-
sires and intentions on others; for children this aspect of cal aggression and relational aggression tend to covary
the trait also involves how manageable the child is for in individuals (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004; Rose, Swen-
parents and teachers. Observations of Agreeable chil- son, & Waller, 2004; Tellegen & Waller, 1992), although
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 321

it is also important to separate the two types of aggres- cial concern and respect for others, whereas the other
sion for more fine-grained developmental analyses. trait involves irritable, aggressive, and hostile disregard
Willfulness refers to the extent to which an individual for others. Although prosocial and aggressive tendencies
attempts to assert his or her will over others through tend to covary negatively later in childhood, they also
domineering behavior (Halverson et al., 2003). Children co-occur in some youths (Haselager, Cillessen, van
and adults who are high on this trait are described as Lieshout, Riksen-Walraven, & Hartup, 2002) and, when
bossy, manipulative, overbearing, and defiant rather combined, may confer social benefits in some Machi-
than accommodating and flexible (Halverson et al., avellian children (Hawley, 2003). The two aspects of
2003; Peabody & De Raad, 2002). This trait captures Agreeableness are likely to have some overlapping ori-
some of the most central aspects of Thomas and Chesss gins, but also some distinct origins (Krueger, Hicks, &
difficult child construct (Bates, 1989) and is also simi- McGue, 2001).
lar to a childhood trait labeled by Bates as resistance to
Agreeableness: Early Childhood Antecedents
control (Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, 1998). Chil-
dren high on this trait are likely to pose significant man- Although Agreeableness has emerged robustly and con-
agement problems for parents and teachers. Willfulness sistently in questionnaire studies that tap a wide range
involves childrens tendencies to assert their wills over of childrens behaviors, the traits encompassed by this
others; however, it is important to recognize that actual superfactor are not included in most temperament ques-
dominance over others is determined by multiple traits, tionnaires. Perhaps these traits have been seen as less
including aspects of extraversion, as noted previously. basic than other temperament traits: Temperament re-
Specifically, observed dominance is linked with Extra- searchers may have considered prosocial and hostile
version, low Neuroticism, and greater physical aggres- tendencies as the developmental products of more basic,
sion in both humans and animals (Anderson, John, early-emerging temperaments. There is some support
Keltner, & Kring, 2001; Gosling & John, 1999; Hawley, for the idea that Agreeableness arises, in part, from
2003; Pellegrini & Bartini, 2001). early differences in positive and negative emotions and
Other potential aspects of Agreeableness may exist in from early self-regulation, which we discuss later. How-
children. Modesty versus conceitedness involves the ex- ever, it is also likely that there are unique genetic and
tent to which an individual is humble rather than arro- environmental contributors to Agreeableness as well.
gant, self-important, or boastful (Goldberg, 2001; Aspects of Agreeableness emerge fairly early them-
Peabody & De Raad, 2002; Saucier & Ostendorf, 1999). selves (e.g., aggression; Tremblay, 2002) and seem to
Integrity refers to the tendency to be honest, principled, develop alongside other temperament traits. Further,
sincere, and loyal versus deceptive and disloyal many nonhuman animals display Agreeableness-like
(Peabody & De Raad, 2002). Integrity appears to be traits, even though most of these same species do not
close in content to internalized conscience and the display behaviors indicating Conscientiousness (Gosling
moral self as these constructs have been studied by & John, 1999). Thus, it is likely that Agreeableness is
Kochanska and colleagues (Kochanska, Gross, Lin, & not merely a product of other temperament traits. It
Nichols, 2002). would be useful for more longitudinal research to mea-
Although Agreeableness is a one-dimensional trait sure early individual differences that may be more
spanning prosocial traits at the high end and antisocial uniquely associated with later Agreeableness, such as
traits at the low end, it may actually turn out to be better early behaviors indicating affection, closeness to others,
thought of as at least two separate dimensions and soothability.
(Graziano, 1994; Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997). Agree- Graziano and Eisenberg (1997) have argued that
ableness forms a single trait in some lexical studies of Agreeableness is likely to have its origins in the self-
adults (e.g., English and German), but it splits into two regulation of negative emotions. Recent studies have pro-
separate factors in a number of languages (Peabody & vided good evidence for this claim. Agreeableness itself
De Raad, 2002; Saucier, 2003). One of these factors is and several of its components are predicted negatively by
typically defined by prosocial tendencies, modesty, and early differences in high-intensity irritability and frus-
integrity, and the other factor is typically defined by an- tration and positively by early attention and self-control
tagonism and willfulness. Thus, one trait involves proso- (Abe & Izard, 1999; Eisenberg et al., 2000; Kochanska,
322 Personality Development

Murray, & Coy, 1997; Kochanska et al., 2000; Laursen, together, the data suggest that well-regulated early Ex-
Pulkkinen, & Adams, 2002; Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., traversion is likely to predict prosocial tendencies,
2000; Rubin, Burgess, Dwyer, & Hastings, 2003). Good whereas unregulated early Extraversion is likely to pre-
attentional control may be particularly important for dict antisocial tendencies.
helping children to shift their focus from negative emo-
tions to positive emotions when they are angry, frus- Agreeableness: Underlying Processes
trated, or aroused (Wilson, 2003). In contrast to the There are large psychological literatures associated
negative link between early irritability and later Agree- with the processes underlying the various components
ableness, early fearfulness may actually promote higher of Agreeableness (for reviews, see the chapters by
Agreeableness because fearfulness presages greater Dodge, Coie, & Lynam on aggression, Chapter 12;
compliance and a stronger moral self (Kochanska et al., Eisenberg et al., Chapter 11, this Handbook, this vol-
2002), higher empathy (Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., ume, on prosocial behavior): This developmental re-
2000), and lower aggression (Raine et al., 1998). How- search is highly relevant for understanding the nature of
ever, fear and anxiety may be negatively associated with Agreeableness in childhood. Rather than attempt to de-
prosocial behavior toward strangers (reviewed in Eisen- scribe these literatures, we focus here on research that
berg & Fabes, 1998). The picture that is emerging is that specifically examines the Big Five trait of Agreeable-
Disagreeableness develops most strongly among children ness in children and adults and describe potential under-
whose high irritability is not constrained by either good lying biological systems.
self-regulation or by the inhibiting power of fearfulness A number of researchers have argued that Agreeable-
(see Caspi, Harrington, et al., 2003, for converging longi- ness reflects individual differences in the motivation to
tudinal evidence). maintain harmonious relationships with others; from
Positive emotions and Extraversion appear to predict this point of view, Agreeableness taps differences in the
childhood Agreeableness in a complex pattern. Mild, willingness to forgo individual interests out of concern
regulated positive emotions presage later Agreeableness for others (Digman & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Graziano
(Abe & Izard, 1999). Similarly, positive emotionality & Eisenberg, 1997; Graziano, Hair, & Finch, 1997; Mac-
and sociability are concurrently associated with proso- Donald, 1995). A number of recent studies have provided
cial behavior and with empathy in children (reviewed in evidence that Agreeableness does reflect individual dif-
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998, and Graziano & Eisenberg, ferences in the motivation to maintain harmonious rela-
1997) and with prosocial tendencies in adults (Krueger tionships. One rather straightforward piece of evidence
et al., 2001). Agreeableness itself is linked with higher is that, among college students, Agreeableness is moder-
jovial mood in adults (e.g., happy, cheerful, enthusiastic; ately correlated with prizing benevolence as a value,
Watson & Clark, 1992). Conversely, inhibited tempera- meaning concern for people one knows (Roccas et al.,
ment is linked with less expressed empathy (Young, Fox, 2002). Based on data from experience-sampling studies,
& Zahn-Waxler, 1999). Thus, regulated positive emo- high-Agreeable adults react differently to interpersonal
tions and sociability are likely precursors of later proso- situations than do low-Agreeable adults. Specifically,
cial tendencies, though more evidence is needed. In high-Agreeable adults are more distressed than low-
contrast, Extraversion positively predicts later aggres- Agreeable adults when they face interpersonal conflicts
sion and externalizing behavior problems (X. Chen (Suls et al., 1998) and report more negative affect when
et al., 2002; Shiner, 2000). High-intensity positive emo- they themselves behave in a quarrelsome manner (Cote
tions and shorter observed latency to grasp small ob- & Moskowitz, 1998). They also report more positive af-
jects in infancy likewise predict childhood aggression fect when they engage in warm, agreeable behavior than
(Rothbart, Derryberry, et al., 2000). Higher approach do low-Agreeable adults (Cote & Moskowitz, 1998).
tendencies may particularly lead to greater externaliz- When more Agreeable college students anticipate partic-
ing, antisocial behavior when self-regulation is poor ipating in a competitive situation, they expect the situa-
(Eisenberg, Spinrad, et al., 2004). As we described pre- tion to be less rewarding and more challenging than do
viously, childrens Extraversion may result in high levels less Agreeable students (Graziano et al., 1997). The data
of frustration and anger when goal seeking is thwarted; from these diverse studies support the idea that high-
in turn, this anger and frustration may lead children Agreeable individuals are concerned with maintaining
with poor self-regulation to behave aggressively. Taken harmonious relationships and are distressed by potential
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 323

and real interpersonal conflicts. This fundamental moti- warmth, and nurturance may arise from a biological
vation for peaceable, close relationships is reflected in system designed to promote parental investment in off-
the life stories told by Agreeable individuals; in life nar- spring and close family bonds. Some researchers (e.g.,
ratives provided by college students and adults, higher MacDonald, 1992, 1995) have argued that evolution
Agreeableness is associated with themes of love/friend- yielded a human biological system that typically en-
ship, caring for others, and unity (McAdams et al., sures that an intimate relationship and the care of close
2004). others is inherently rewarding and pleasurable and that
Does the motivation to maintain positive relation- the loss of such relationships is painful and distressing.
ships translate into distinct approaches to handling con- Such a system would confer adaptive benefits because it
flict among high-Agreeable children and adults? A would promote successful care of offspring through the
variety of studies yield an affirmative answer to this establishment of strong attachment relationships be-
question. First, low-Agreeable children, adolescents, tween infants and their caregivers. Some evolutionary
and adults are more likely than high-Agreeable individ- theorists have argued that the primary purpose of this
uals to endorse destructive tactics for handling conflict, evolved system was to promote parent-child attachment
such as manipulation, coercion, and power assertion, al- and that the system only secondarily began to serve the
though even low-Agreeable individuals acknowledge role of facilitating pair-bonding between reproductive
that better tactics could be used (D. Buss, 1992; partners (Diamond, 2004). In their tend-and-befriend
Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & Hair, 1996; Jensen- model, Taylor et al. (2000) have argued that a biological
Campbell, Gleason, Adams, & Malcolm, 2003; Jensen- affiliation system is likely to be especially important
Campbell, Graziano, & Hair, 1996). Agreeableness is for understanding the ways that females respond to
also positively associated in children and adolescents stress and threat. According to this model, females may
with stronger endorsement of constructive conflict tac- be evolutionarily primed to respond to stress by tend-
tics, such as negotiation and compromise (Jensen-Camp- ing (caring for others, particularly offspring) and be-
bell & Graziano, 2001; Jensen-Campbell et al., 2003). friending (seeking and offering support). Research
Second, Agreeableness predicts more effective handling with animals has pointed to several potential biological
of actual conflicts. Teachers and parents describe high- substrates of this affectional system, including endoge-
Agreeable children as negotiating conflict better nous opioids and the neuropeptide oxytocin (Carter,
(Jensen-Campbell et al., 2003), whereas peers describe 1998; Taylor et al., 2000). Further, there is some prelim-
low-Agreeable adolescents as more aggressive (Gleason, inary evidence in humans that affectional bonds may
Jensen-Campbell, & Richardson, 2004). When children activate brain areas that support positive emotions and
are observed in conflict situations in the lab, low Agree- deactivate brain areas that are linked with aggression,
ableness is associated with higher levels of conflict and fear, and sadness (Diamond, 2004); this finding is con-
tension, as well as more destructive conflict tactics such sistent with the emotional profile associated with
as stand-offs, name-calling, and withdrawals (Jensen- Agreeableness.
Campbell et al., 2003). In similar lab-based situations
with college students, low Agreeableness is likewise
Openness to Experience/ Intellect
predictive of greater conflict (Graziano et al., 1996). In-
terestingly, greater Agreeableness does not predict more Openness to Experience/ Intellect is perhaps the most
observed submissive behavior in children and adults debated and least understood of the Big Five traits, yet it
(Cote & Moskowitz, 1998; Jensen-Campbell et al., includes a number of potentially important characteris-
2003); apparently, Agreeable people do not simply solve tics. This trait does not appear in temperament models,
their interpersonal problems by giving in to other peo- despite parents from a number of countries sponta-
ple. In short, more Agreeable youths and adults appear neously using words from this domain of individual dif-
to generate fewer conflicts for themselves and have a ferences to describe their children (Mervielde, De
greater capacity for handling the interpersonal conflicts Fruyt, & Jarmuz, 1998). In the Big Five studies, chil-
that do arise. dren who are high on this trait are described as eager
There is increasing research interest in understand- and quick to learn, clever, knowledgeable, perceptive,
ing the biological systems underlying individual differ- imaginative, curious, and original. In previous reviews
ences in Agreeableness. Differences in empathy, (Shiner, 1998; Shiner & Caspi, 2003), we argued that the
324 Personality Development

evidence for the existence of this trait in children was positive emotions and active exploration are likely pre-
equivocal. More recently, the accumulating data provide cursors of both Openness and Extraversion. Openness
convincing evidence that this trait can be measured reli- and Extraversion tend to covary across the life span
ably by at least age 6 or 7 years. The childhood trait (Digman, 1997; Markon et al., 2005); the two traits may
does not appear to be as broad as some adult conceptual- stem from some of the same underlying early processes.
izations of the trait, however. For example, in Costa and Third, orienting sensitivity, which includes the ten-
McCraes (1992) influential version of Openness to Ex- dency to be sensitive to internal and external sensory
perience, the trait includes openness to ideas, fantasy, stimulation, is concurrently related to Openness in
aesthetics, actions, feelings, and values. In most child- adulthood (Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 2000). Thus, the ex-
hood Big Five studies, the Openness trait focuses more tant data on the antecedents of Openness are limited but
narrowly on openness to ideas and actions and tends to suggest several potentially interesting early manifesta-
emphasize intellectual capacities and creativity. In tions of the trait.
some studies with children, when the trait is measured
with a broader range of items, it does not form an inter- Openness to Experience: Underlying Processes
nally coherent trait (Lamb et al., 2002; Markey et al., McCrae and Costa (1997) suggested that Openness in-
2002). By high school, adolescents can reliably describe cludes two particularly important processes: Openness
themselves on most aspects of the broader Openness as a psychic structure and Openness as a motivation to
trait (McCrae & Costa, 2004; McCrae et al., 2002). pursue new, complex experiences. First, open individu-
Thus, it is safe to say that some form of Openness als have access to more thoughts, feelings, and impulses
emerges as an individual difference by middle child- in awareness, and can maintain many of these simulta-
hood, but the trait is likely to undergo significant devel- neously (McCrae and Costa, 1997, p. 838). Openness
opmental elaboration across the years from early is associated with numerous indicators of greater access
childhood through adolescence. to varied inner experiences in adults: more differenti-
The lower-order components of Openness in child- ated self-reports of emotions (Terracciano, McCrae,
hood are not yet clear, but intellect (Halverson et al., Hagemann, & Costa, 2003), reduced tendencies to
2003; Mervielde & De Fruyt, 1999, 2002) and curiosity screen out previously irrelevant stimuli (Peterson,
and creativity (Goldberg, 2001; Mervielde & DeFruyt, Smith, & Carson, 2002), greater dissociation and per-
1999, 2002) have received some support. In interna- ceptual aberration (McCrae & Costa, 1997), and
tional lexical studies with adults, Openness also appears heightened experiences of inspiration (Thrash & Elliot,
to include components of unconventionality (Ashton 2003). This greater access to inner experience may be a
et al., 2004). mixed blessing; for example, in one study of women un-
dergoing a major move, greater Openness predicted both
Openness to Experience: Early Childhood
heightened self-esteem and increased depression
Antecedents
(Kling, Ryff, Love, & Essex, 2003). More highly open
The developmental precursors of Openness are un- adults are more creative, at least in supportive circum-
known, but there is suggestive evidence from three lines stances (George & Zhou, 2001); it seems likely that a
of research. First, in one study, full-face positive emo- very important source of this creativity is access to a
tions in the Strange Situation at 18 months (a presumed complex world of inner ideas and emotions. Second,
marker of high-intensity positive emotions) predicted open individuals are motivated to seek out interesting
Openness at 3.5 years (Abe & Izard, 1999). Second, new experiences. This view of Openness is consistent
early signs of Openness may include curiosity and ex- with some of the markers of Openness in children, in-
ploration of new situations; these behaviors are markers cluding eagerness to learn new things ( both academic
of an Openness-like trait in a number of animal species and nonacademic).
(Gosling & John, 1999). Indirect evidence for this possi- Among adults, Openness expresses itself in a wide
bility comes from longitudinal studies showing that the variety of observable behaviors and attitudes. More
tendency to seek stimulation and to explore new envi- open adults tend to be more politically liberal, less au-
ronments actively in early childhood predicts later aca- thoritarian in their attitudes, and less traditional in their
demic achievement and IQ (Guerin et al., 2003; Raine, beliefs (Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003;
Reynolds, Venables, & Mednick, 2002). High-intensity McCrae, 1996). They produce more structurally com-
Temperament and Personality Traits: A Process-Focused, Developmental Taxonomy from Childhood to Adulthood 325

plex narratives about their lives (McAdams et al., 2004), sonality differences across the life course. First, much
have more distinctive offices and bedrooms (Gosling, more work is needed to specify lower-order traits. We
Ko, Mannarelli, & Morris, 2002), and possess more var- have highlighted some potential lower-order traits that
ied collections of books (Gosling et al., 2002). In con- warrant consideration in children and adults; other
trast, we know strikingly little about the behaviors lower-order traits undoubtedly exist as well.
associated with Openness in children. It will be impor- Second, the fields of child development and personal-
tant to look in the right contexts to find the behavioral ity psychology will continue to benefit from creative
signatures of Openness in children. Adult Openness is measurement of individual differences, beyond the sole
accurately observed in some contexts and not others; for use of questionnaires. A strength of the temperament
example, it is more accurately inferred from adults pan- field has been its use of multiple measures of tempera-
tomimes and conversations on hobbies than from their ment traits, including behavior observations and struc-
role playing of various social scenarios (Borkenau, tured laboratory tasks. Additional types of measures can
Mauer, Riemann, Spinath, & Angleitner, 2004). be explored. For example, implicit measures have been
used to assess anxiety and shyness in adults (Asendorpf,
Directions for Future Developmental Research Banse, & Mucke, 2002; Egloff & Schmukle, 2002);
on Personality Structure rather than directly inquiring about a persons self-view
of personality, these instruments measure indirectly an
In this section, we have reviewed a proposed taxonomy individuals automatic associations between trait de-
of individual differences in personality. A personality scriptors and the self. Physiological measures can be
taxonomy is an evolving classification system whose used to parse groups of individuals into more homoge-
purpose is to integrate and guide research. This also neous subtypes (Kagan, Snidman, McManis, Wood-
means that any such system must be open to empirical ward, & Hardway, 2002). Puppet interviews have been
refutation and requisite modification. There are histori- used to assess self-views of traits in children as young as
cal parallels between the use of structural models in age 5 years and could be used to measure a wide range of
personality psychology and the use of a standardized traits early in childhood (Arseneault, Kim-Cohen, Tay-
model for describing and diagnosing mental illness in lor, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2005; Measelle, Ablow, Cowan, &
psychiatry. Prior to the advent of the American Psychi- Cowen, 1998; Measelle et al., 2005).
atric Associations Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Third, more research is needed about the cross-cul-
Mental Disorders III (DSM-III), clinicians and re- tural generalizability of the taxonomic system reviewed
searchers did not have available explicit criteria to de- here for children and adolescents. Parents from the
fine the boundaries of diagnostic categories. Clinical United States, China, and several European countries
diagnoses were difficult to compare and cross-sample consider the Big Five traits to be important in describing
replications were hard to conduct. The development of their children (Kohnstamm, Halverson, Mervielde, &
DSM-III was a big improvement because it provided a Havill, 1998). Cross-cultural studies of adult personal-
common language with which clinicians and researchers ity structure have been pursued vigorously over the past
could communicate about the disorders they were treat- decade (Church, 2001). There is some research on child-
ing or investigating. DSM-III had its share of problems, hood temperament and personality structure in coun-
and subsequent modifications (DSM-III-R, DSM-IV) tries outside the United States and Europe, such as
testify to the need for a flexible and evolving system that China, Japan, and Russia (Digman & Shmelyov, 1996;
can accommodate new empirical information, as do rec- Halverson et al., 2003; Rothbart, Ahadi, et al., 2001),
ommended modifications in anticipation of DSM-V but it will be important to explore the structure of child-
(Kupfer, First, & Regier, 2002). We can similarly hope hood personality in other countries. Another crucial
that the use of a generally accepted trait taxonomy will task will be to determine when in the life course mean-
help to impose structure on unintegrated research find- level cross-cultural differences in personality emerge.
ings, reduce the likelihood that old traits will be rein- Fourth, it will be important to chart the development
vented under new labels, and advance the study of of sex differences in mean levels of personality traits.
personality development across the life course. Although there do not appear to be any sex differences
Toward these goals, we suggest four ways to build on in the structure of personality, there are some differ-
the current success of elucidating the structure of per- ences in the mean levels of personality traits (Feingold,
326 Personality Development

1994). A deeper understanding of the causes of such sex pear in nearly all sections of this chapter. The ubiqui-
differences will be important for explaining both per- tous presence of process-focused personality analysis in
sonality development and the development of psycho- this chapter is not an accident. As we noted in our chap-
pathology (Rutter, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003). ter introduction, personality research is increasingly
based on the recognition that traits are not merely se-
mantic labels but rather reflect organizing and motivat-
THE DEVELOPMENTAL ELABORATION OF ing biological and psychological processes (Derryberry,
PERSONALITY TRAITS Reed, & Pilkenton-Taylor, 2003; Tellegen, 1991). The
purpose of this section is to provide an organizing
The process of developmental elaboration refers to the framework for thinking about and studying the
mechanisms by which those temperament attributes processes by which personality traits develop and in-
that are part of each individuals genetic heritage accu- creasingly shape behavior.
mulate response strength through their repeated rein-
forcement and become elaborated into cognitive and
affective representations that are quickly and fre- Learning Processes
quently activatedinto personality traits. This elabo-
ration may involve at least six processes (Table 6.3), Temperament differences may influence several learn-
which we now describe in the order of their hypothe- ing mechanisms that are involved in the elaboration pro-
sized emergence. For example, learning processes and cess, including positive and negative reinforcement,
environmental elicitation are hypothesized to influence punishment, discrimination learning, and extinction. In
the course of personality development in the first few the second section, we described current models posit-
months of life; environmental construal and social com- ing that Extraversion and Neuroticism reflect individual
parison processes can influence personality develop- differences in a BAS and BIS, respectively (Gray, 1987,
ment only following the emergence of necessary 1990). In essence, proponents of these models argue that
cognitive functions in early and middle childhood; and Extraversion and Neuroticism reflect differences in var-
environmental selection and manipulation generally re- ious learning mechanisms (i.e., Extraversion indexes
quire the emergence of self-regulatory functions in sensitivity to potential rewards and Neuroticism indexes
childhood and are likely to become particularly impor- sensitivity to potential threats). If these formulations of
tant as youths move into adolescence. Extraversion and Neuroticism are correct, the two traits
Before describing these six processes, we hasten to should correlate with different patterns of learning.
remind the reader that examples of these processes ap- There is some evidence for this in adults (Avila, 2001).

TABLE 6.3 Processes through Which Early Temperament / Personality Shapes the Development of Later Personality, Adaptation,
and Psychopathology

Process Definition Example


Learning processes Temperament shapes the childs experience of Children high on Openness may find complex
classical and operant conditioning. and novel stimuli to be reinforcing.
Environmental elicitation Temperament shapes the response of adults and Children high on Extraversion may attract peers
peers to the child. to play with them.
Environmental construal Temperament shapes the ways that children Children low on Agreeableness may interpret
interpret the environment and their experiences. requests from adults as hostile impositions on
their freedom.
Social and temporal comparisons Temperament shapes the ways children evaluate Children high on Neuroticism may wrongly view
themselves relative to others and to themselves themselves as inadequate relative to their peers.
across time.
Environmental selection Temperament shapes childrens choices about Children high on Conscientiousness may pursue
their everyday environments. challenging activities.
Environmental manipulation Temperament shapes the ways that children alter, Children high on Extraversion may actively
modify, and manipulate their environments. persuade other children to choose them as
leaders of school groups.
The Developmental Elaboration of Personality Traits 327

Differences in sensitivity to rewards and threats should life (Bell & Chapman, 1986). Research on evocative ef-
also predict biases in perception, memory, or attention fects of childrens temperament on parents is especially
(Canli, 2004). We previously reviewed some evidence well developed in relation to infants and young children
that this is indeed the case (e.g., anxiety predicts selec- with difficult temperaments (i.e., children who are ir-
tive attention to threats). Other traits in addition to Ex- ritable, hostile, prone to cry, and hard to soothe). Many
traversion and Neuroticism should affect learning studies have documented that mothers of difficult in-
processes as well. For example, as we have reviewed, fants experience lower confidence, greater depression,
children differ strikingly in their persistence and atten- and lower self-efficacy than do mothers of more tem-
tion, two temperament traits that are likely to influence peramentally easy infants (Crockenberg & Leerkes,
learning. Agreeableness may be related to sensitivities 2003). Childrens intense irritability has rippling nega-
to anger- or frustration-inducing stimuli, whereas Open- tive effects on fathers and the family system more
ness may be associated with attraction to complex or broadly (Crockenberg & Leerkes, 2003), including
novel stimuli. All of these differences in learning detrimental immediate and short-term effects on moth-
processes should be amenable to investigation through ers work outcomes (Hyde, Else-Quest, Goldsmith, &
behavioral and neuroscience methods. Biesanz, 2004). Childrens negative emotions also pre-
More generally, different parental socialization dict differential negative treatment by parents; more
processes are likely to interact with childhood tempera- emotionally negative children evoke more negative
ment in the development of personality (for recent re- parental responses than less emotionally negative chil-
views, see Bates & McFadyen-Ketchum, 2000; dren in the same family (Jenkins, Rasbash, & OCon-
Gallagher, 2002; Putnam, Sanson, & Rothbart, 2002). nor, 2003).
One example may serve to illustrate how childrens tem- The evidence for child effects on parents is the most
perament differences in learning interact with parental robust in relation to individual differences in negative
socialization. Kochanska (1997) demonstrated that emotions, but other temperament traits appear to predict
fearful children (i.e., those with greater sensitivity to parental responses as well (Crouter & Booth, 2003;
threats) show more positive conscience development Parke, 2004; Putnam et al., 2002). In addition, it is im-
when mothers use subtle, gentle, psychological disci- portant to recognize that the effects of childrens tem-
pline than when mothers use strongly power-assertive peraments extend beyond the family environment to
discipline. For such fearful children, their own internal other caregivers, teachers, and peers. For example, chil-
experiences of distress may facilitate the feeling of guilt dren who express more positive affect are liked by peers
when they do something wrong, which appears to pro- and are seen by teachers as friendly and cooperative,
mote greater compliance (Kochanska et al., 2002). In whereas children who express high levels of anger are
contrast, gentle maternal discipline does not predict disliked by peers and are viewed as unfriendly, aggres-
conscience development among fearless children, most sive, and uncooperative by teachers (reviewed in Denham
likely because gentle discipline does not create enough et al., 2001). Thus, childrens temperaments play an im-
discomfort; fearless children instead tend to develop portant role in shaping the interpersonal experiences
stronger internalization when they are securely attached they encounter in multiple settings. In turn, the responses
to their mothers and when their mothers are more re- that children evoke from others are likely to be internal-
sponsive to them. Identification of other family modera- ized as part of childrens emerging self-concepts.
tors of temperament outcomes should be a high priority Research has begun to uncover some of the micro-
for research on childrens individual differences. A bet- processes through which childrens temperament elicits
ter understanding of how temperament influences basic responses from others. Individual differences in tem-
learning processes should help researchers identify perament and personality traits are reliably expressed in
which moderators to examine. unique verbal and nonverbal behaviors, and other per-
sons in the immediate environment react to these behav-
Environmental Elicitation iors and use this information to make inferences and
attributions (Borkenau & Liebler, 1995; Gifford, 1994).
Temperament differences also elicit different reactions Perhaps most striking is evidence linking individual dif-
from the environment and influence how other people ferences in adolescent personality and psychopathology
react to children, beginning in the first few months of to facial expressions of discrete emotions. For example,
328 Personality Development

Extraversion predicts facial expressions of social ap- ronment (Hartup & van Lieshout, 1995). Research about
proach, Agreeableness is negatively correlated with fa- the construal process stems from the cognitive tradition
cial expressions of anger, and Conscientiousness is in personality psychology, which emphasizes each per-
associated with reduced facial expressions of negative sons subjective experience and unique perception of
emotion and with embarrassment (Eisenberg et al., the world. This research focuses on what people do
1989; Keltner, 1998; Keltner, Moffitt, & Stouthamer- mentally (Cervone & Mischel, 2002), demonstrating
Loeber, 1995). Such facial expressions of emotions help that social information processingincluding attention,
to coordinate social interactions: These expressions encoding, retrieval, and interpretationis a selective
convey information about the motivations and personal- process shaped by individual differences in tempera-
ity of the sender, evoke complementary or similar emo- ment and personality (Derryberry & Reed, 2003;
tions from others, and provide incentives for particular Matthews, Derryberry, & Siegle, 2000).
responses (e.g., positive emotions reinforce desired so- The role of cognitive factors in personality and
cial behaviors; Keltner, Ekman, Gonzaga, & Beer, psychopathology has been detailed by Crick and Dodge
2003). In addition to the facial expression of emotion, (1994), whose social information-processing model of
childrens other individuating characteristics (e.g., childrens social adjustment includes six steps: (1) to en-
vocal properties; Lin, Bugental, Turek, Martorell, & Ol- code information about the event, (2) to interpret the
ster, 2002) are likely to provoke particular interpersonal cues and arrive at some decision about their meaning
responses as well. and significance, (3) to clarify goals, (4) to search for
Temperament characteristics elicit not only behaviors possible responses to the situation, (5) to consider the
on the part of others but also expectations. Adults have consequences of each potential response and to select a
implicit theories about developmental trajectories that response from the generated alternatives, and (6) to
they associate with particular temperament attributes. carry out the selected response. Temperament and per-
As such, childrens temperament-based behaviors may sonality have the potential to shape social information
elicit expectancy-based reactions from adult caregivers processing at each of these steps.
(Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & Sullivan-Logan, 1998). One of the most important reasons that temperament
Finally, it is important to recognize that childrens ef- is likely to influence cognitive processing is that tem-
fects on others are likely to be moderated by the charac- perament involves emotional processes that are known to
teristics of the interaction partner. Even among parents shape cognition (Derryberry & Reed, 2003). Lemerise
of emotionally negative children, there are some parents and Arsenio (2000) presented a reformulation of Crick
in some contexts who respond with heightened attention and Dodges social information-processing model that
and sensitivity (Crockenberg & Leerkes, 2003), which amplifies the crucial role of emotional processes. These
suggests the presence of important moderators of authors argue that childrens individual differences in
parental responses. Parents beliefs and attitudes mod- emotionality and emotion regulation affect processes at
erate how parents respond to children with particular each of the six stages of social information processing
temperaments (Bugental & Johnston, 2000). Parents (see also Arsenio & Lemerise, 2004). For example, chil-
own personalities and moods are likely to moderate drens differences in positive and negative emotions will
child effects as well, given the evidence discussed in the affect the cues they notice in the environment, the goals
sixth section that parental personality predicts parent- that are salient to them, and the types of potential re-
ing styles. Future research on moderators of child ef- sponses they generate. Childrens differences in self-
fects will help to spell out more clearly how regulation will help to determine the responses they
transactions between childrens temperaments and their select and their ability to enact those responses. Al-
contexts shape their developing personalities. though personality influences cognitive processes, cog-
nitive processes likewise influence ongoing emotional
Environmental Construal experiences (Matthews et al., 2000); thus; there are bidi-
rectional effects between cognitive/perceptual and emo-
With the emergence of belief systems and expectations, tional processing (Derryberry & Reed, 2003).
temperament differences may also begin to influence Throughout our personality taxonomy section, we
how environmental experiences are construed, thus presented examples of how personality shapes social-
shaping each persons effective experience of the envi- cognitive processing. Two other examples illustrate how
The Developmental Elaboration of Personality Traits 329

such processes occur. As noted, Neuroticism in adults is paring and contrasting themselves to others (social com-
associated with a variety of cognitive biases. Recent re- parisons) as well as to themselves over time (temporal
search has demonstrated that higher Neuroticism is as- comparisons). The salience and relative importance of
sociated with specific detrimental ways of appraising social and temporal comparisons may change across the
and coping with difficult situations. Children who are life course (Suls & Mullen, 1982). Age-related changes
higher on negative emotionality (including both neuroti- in social cognition and social roles make it likely that
cism and irritability) are more likely to appraise nega- social comparisons may be especially influential from
tive life events as threatening and to use avoidant coping childhood to adolescence and into adulthood and that
as a way of dealing with these life events, which leads to temporal comparisons may become increasingly impor-
poorer adjustment (Lengua & Long, 2002). Similarly, tant during the adult years.
Neuroticism in adults predicts heightened distress and The microprocesses through which personality may
worry in response to experimentally induced stress in shape social and temporal comparisons also deserve at-
the lab; this link between Neuroticism and distress is, in tention. Temperament and personality may shape a range
part, mediated by threat appraisals and by emotion-fo- of relevant processes (Cassidy, Ziv, Mehta, & Feeney,
cused coping (Matthews et al., 2000). These studies 2003; Derryberry & Reed, 2003), including (a) the kinds
demonstrate that one of the reasons that Neuroticism is of feedback that people deliberately seek out about
so robustly associated with poor adjustment is because it themselves, ( b) attentional biases to comparison infor-
is linked with specific social-cognitive biases. mation, (c) standards used for comparison, and (d) emo-
Social-cognitive processes are also implicated in ag- tional responsivity to comparison information. Extant
gression. Children who are high in reactive aggression research offers some hints about the ways in which per-
(aggression aimed at retaliation against someone) are sonality differences may shape comparison processes.
those who show both high frustration /anger and a ten- For example, greater anxiety and sadness in children pre-
dency to assume hostile intentions on the part of others dict negative self-views, which predict poorer estimates
(Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates, & Pettit, 1997). An of competence relative to actual competence over time
interesting, similar pattern predicts aggression and hos- (Pomerantz & Rudolph, 2003). Other research suggests
tility in adults. In one study (Meier & Robinson, 2004), that subsets of aggressive children have extremely high
adult participants were asked to categorize words as or extremely low views of their competence relative to
blameworthy (e.g., murder or adultery) or not (e.g., others (Brendgen, Vitaro, Turgeon, Poulin, & Wanner,
baldness or hurricane). Among participants low on 2004). More systematic research is needed regarding
Agreeableness, greater speed at categorizing words as these processes.
blameworthy predicted greater hostility and arguments
in daily life. Thus, for both children and adults, a Environmental Selection
heightened bias toward assuming and assigning blame
predicts greater hostility and aggression among some As self-regulatory competencies increase with age, indi-
individuals. viduals begin to make choices and display preferences
As these examples illustrate, although social-cogni- that may reinforce and sustain their characteristics.
tive and trait approaches to personality are often por- Childrens emerging personalities shape the environ-
trayed as antagonistic, they are perfectly complementary ments they select, whether consciously or unconsciously.
and mutually informative. Future work can aim to ex- Processes of environmental selection are likely to be-
plore further the ways that childrens early tempera- come increasingly important across the years from child-
ments influence the ways that children construe and hood to adulthood (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Even
make sense of their worlds. among very young children, temperament is likely to
shape the spheres children occupy in the environments
Social and Temporal Comparisons chosen for them by adults (e.g., inhibited toddlers may
avoid interactions with other children in child care, or
With increased cognitive sophistication (e.g., role-tak- children high on intellect may choose more stimulating
ing skills), two social-psychological processes are hy- activities at home). As children move into middle child-
pothesized to influence self-evaluations and identity hood, they are given greater freedom to choose the envi-
development: Children learn about themselves by com- ronments in which they spend their time (Cole & Cole,
330 Personality Development

1996). During childhood and adolescence, youths per- Environmental Manipulation


sonalities may help determine the activities in which
Once the self-concept is firmly established, and with
they participate and the ways in which they choose to
the development of more sophisticated self-regulatory
spend their free time (McHale, Crouter, & Tucker, 2001;
capacities, individuals also begin to alter, modify, and
Shanahan & Flaherty, 2001). Personality effects on chil-
manipulate the environments in which they find them-
drens peer relationships may be particularly important;
selves (D. Buss, 1987). These processes may become
particular traits appear to predict the peer groups chil-
particularly important as children become more skilled
dren join (Denham et al., 2001), childrens experiences
in regulating their own behavior and more insightful into
of peer rejection and acceptance (Hay, Payne, & Chad-
the causes of others behaviors. Like adults, children
wick, 2004), and the quality of childrens friendships
vary in the goals that they pursue in various circum-
(Pike & Atzaba-Poria, 2003). Childrens individual dif-
stances (Rose & Asher, 1999), and these goals are likely
ferences also predict the life events they experience; for
to influence the ways that children attempt to modify
example, children with externalizing behavior problems
their environments. The ways that individuals select and
experience a greater number of controllable negative life
shape their environments may be especially relevant for
events than children without these problems (Masten,
self-regulation. Individuals regulate their behavior in
Neeman, & Andenas, 1994).
the midst of an ongoing emotional experience. But indi-
In adulthood, individuals make personality-based
viduals also regulate their behavior and others behavior
choices regarding education, occupation, and intimate
proactively by anticipating potential situations and se-
relationships (reviewed in a later section); all of these
lecting how to handle those situations according to their
choices shape individuals everyday environments. In-
goals (Eisenberg, 2001; Gross, 1999).
deed, by adulthood the most striking personality differ-
We have described six processes through which an
ences between individuals are to be found not by
initial disposition is elaborated so that it increasingly
studying their responses to the same situation but by
organizes emotion, thought, and action. Research is now
studying how they choose and construct new situations
needed about each of these processes in relation to dif-
(Wachtel, 1973). A persons selection and creation of
ferent temperament and personality traits.
environments is thus one of the most individualizing and
pervasive expressions of his or her personality.
The process of environmental selection may also ac-
count for the empirical observation that measures typi- THE ORIGINS OF INDIVIDUAL
cally used to study the environment are subject to DIFFERENCES IN PERSONALITY
substantial genetic influence (Plomin & Bergeman,
1991). A key issue for personality researchers is the ex- In this section, we review what is known about genetic
tent to which personality contributes to genetic influ- influences on personality variation between people, and
ences on measures of the environment. So far, genetic discuss how measured genes can be incorporated into re-
effects on the Big Five personality traits have been re- search on personality development.
ported to explain genetic influences on some life events
(Saudino, Pedersen, Lichtenstein, McClearn, & Plomin,
Genetic and Environmental Inf luences on
1997), and personality traits account for about 30% of
Personality Development across the Life Span
the genetic influence on divorce risk (Jockin, McGue, &
Lykken, 1996). The main implication from these results, To estimate the relative roles that genes and environ-
as discussed later, is that associations between environ- ments play in personality development, behavioral ge-
mental measures and personality cannot be assumed to neticists employ two basic research designs: (1) twin
be caused environmentally, and, in some instances, the studies and (2) adoption studies. The logic behind using
likely direction of effects is the other way around: Indi- the twin method to estimate heritable influences is
viduals differentially select and are differentially ex- straightforward, and it has three parts. First, a genetic
posed to environments (e.g., divorce or stressful life contribution to personality is indicated when the simi-
events) as a result of their genetically influenced per- larity of monozygotic (MZ) twins personalities is
sonality traits. greater than the similarity of dizygotic (DZ) twins per-
The Origins of Individual Dif ferences in Personality 331

sonalities. This inference is based on the fact that MZ significant variance component called child-specific,
twins share all their genes, but DZ twins, like all sib- nonshared, or unique environmental variance, often la-
lings, share on average only half of their polymorphic beled E. It indexes environmental effects on personal-
genes. Quantitative model fitting usually labels this ity that can be detected because they have created
A, for additive genetic effects. To use this logic, re- differences between family members in the study. Phe-
searchers must test the critical assumption that all of notype measurement errors can produce such effects,
the greater similarity between MZ and DZ twins can too, because errors in measurement produce scores that
safely be ascribed to MZ twins greater genetic similar- look different for twins in a pair.
ity. This is called the equal environments assumption. The fundamental logic behind using the adoption
In other words, researchers must show that MZ twins method to estimate heritable influences is also straight-
have not been treated more alike than DZ twins in ways forward. The correlation between adoptee and biologi-
that are related to their personality outcomes. Research cal parent personality represents genetic transmission,
into this question suggests that MZ and DZ twins are not whereas the correlation between adoptee and adoptive
perfectly equal on some environmental experiences. parent personality represents social (i.e., environmen-
However, some part of the greater MZ than DZ twin tal) transmission. To use this logic researchers must test
similarity in treatment arises because MZ twins geneti- the critical assumptions that adoptees share no more
cally influenced similar behavior evokes similar treat- than random genes with their adoptive parents (i.e.,
ment. Evoked similar treatment does not violate the adoption was extrafamilial and the adoption agency did
assumption unless it further exacerbates MZ twin simi- not try to match the adoptive and birth familys charac-
larity. Moreover, despite the fact that such inequality teristics), and adoptees share not more than random en-
may have inflated some heritability estimates by a small vironments with their biological parents (e.g., the
amount, it has not done so enough to invalidate the infer- quality of prenatal and orphanage care were uncorre-
ence that genes influence personality differences. lated with adoptees biological backgrounds). Like twin
Second, twin studies can show whether environmen- data, adoption data can be modeled to ascertain A, C,
tal experiences influence twin similarity over and above and E components of variance.
genetic influences. MZ twins genetic similarity is With data from large studies throughout the world
twice that of DZ twins, and therefore, if nothing more (Boomsma, Busjahn, & Peltonen, 2002), research has
than genes were influencing their personalities, MZ uncovered increasingly reliable and robust evidence that
twins personalities should be at least twice as similar as personality traits are substantially influenced by ge-
DZ twins. If not, this indicates that something more netic factors. Bouchard and Loehlin (2001; Bouchard,
than genes has made the twins similar (i.e., environ- 2004) provide a comprehensive review of this research,
ments that the siblings share in common must have en- pointing to heritability estimates across the Big Five
hanced their similarity). In model fitting, this yields a factors in the range of .50 .10. There are some fluctua-
significant variance component called family wide, tions from study to study, but in general (a) all five su-
shared, or common environmental variance, often la- perfactors appear to be influenced by genetic factors to
beled C. It indexes environmental effects on personal- the same extent and ( b) genetic and environmental fac-
ity that can be detected because they have increased the tors also affect individual differences in mens and
personality similarity between family members in the womens personalities to the same extent.
study and because the family members shared the expe- Three clarifications and qualifications deserve spe-
rience for reasons completely apart from their genetic cial notice. First, twin studies using peer ratings of
similarity. personality, rather than self-report personality ques-
Third, twin studies also address the perennial ques- tionnaires, show genetic influences similar to those
tion of why family members differ from each other found in self-report studies (Reimann, Angleiter, &
(Plomin & Daniels, 1987), by using the following logic. Strelau, 1997). Moreover, multivariate genetic analy-
If MZ twins, despite sharing all their genes, are not per- ses indicate that the same genetic factors are largely in-
fectly identical in their personality, this indicates that volved in self-reports and peer ratings of personality,
nonshared experience unique to each family member has which provides strong evidence for the genetic validity
reduced their similarity. In model fitting, this yields a of self-report ratings. It does not appear to be the case
332 Personality Development

that heritability estimates derived from twin studies effects. Although adoption studies are far fewer and
are simply an artifact of self-report methodologies in much smaller than twin studies of personality, it is note-
which MZ and DZ twins are asked to rate themselves. worthy that they suggest less genetic influence than twin
Some studies using observational measures of behavior studies, and the lack of correspondence, at least in rela-
(e.g., empathy; Emde & Hewitt, 2001) yield lower her- tion to personality research, between the results of twin
itability estimates than have been found using ques- and adoption /family studies merits further scrutiny.
tionnaire measures. However, there are still too few Four novel findings in behavioral genetics research
such observational studies to know if this is a robust on personality merit mention. First, measures of person-
methodological difference. ality that incorporate multiple viewpoints or perspec-
A second, related methodological challenge has been tives (e.g., by consolidating information from multiple
levied at temperament and personality research with reporters or across multiple situations) yield larger esti-
children. Twin studies of younger age groups have relied mates of genetic influences (as well as smaller, but more
primarily on ratings by parents, which have yielded an reliable, estimates of nonshared environmental influ-
odd result: Correlations for identical (MZ) twins are ences) than measures based on a single viewpoint (e.g.,
high, and correlations for fraternal (DZ) twins are very Arseneault et al., 2003; Philips & Matheny, 1997;
low, sometimes even negative (Saudino & Cherny, Scourfield, van den Bree, Martin, & McGuffin, 2004).
2001). The suggestion is that parents may provide biased This finding has been uncovered in studies of young
ratings of their twins and that behavioral genetic studies children, adolescents, and adults. It has been suggested
that rely on parents for data may not yield valid esti- that consensus trait measures, which eliminate speci-
mates of genetic and environmental influences on per- ficities or idiosyncracies in different viewpoints about a
sonality functioning. However, the situation is rather person, could be used to better identify both specific
less alarming than often claimed. Parents sometimes genes and specific experiences that are correlated with
contrast their twins in ways that generate greater than personality (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001).
expected differences between MZ versus DZ twins, but Second, as noted earlier, behavioral genetic studies
this problem may be restricted to some traits (e.g., have been used to examine whether there is etiological
symptoms of hyperactivity, temperament ratings of ac- differentiation in the lower-order traits (or narrow
tivity level; Simonoff et al., 1998) and may be attenu- facets) that make up the broader personality superfac-
ated by different rating measures (Goldsmith, Buss, & tors such as the Big Five. The question is whether
Lemery, 1997). higher-order dimensions, or superfactors, represent the
Third, family and adoption studies of personality best level of analysis for research in genetics. Analyses
yield lower estimates of genetic influences than twin at the lower-order levels of the personality hierarchy
studies (Martin et al., 2000; Plomin, Corley, Caspi, offer additional, useful information about the origins of
Fulker, & DeFries, 1998). Specifically, parent-child and individual differences in personality. For example,
biological sibling correlations for personality traits aver- analyses at the lower-order trait level suggest that sib-
age about .1 to .2, with corresponding heritabilities of .3 lings resemble each other in their altruism and prosocial
that are considerably lower than the heritabilities of .5 behavior (facets of Agreeableness), in part, because of
obtained in twin studies. One possibility is that the dis- the rearing environments they share (Jang, McCrae, An-
crepant findings result from the fact that parent-off- gleitner, Riemann, & Livesley, 1998, Sample 1; Krueger
spring and sibling-sibling correlations are derived from et al., 2001), but analyses of the Agreeableness super-
different-age pairs and thus (dis)similarity is con- factor (which includes many other facets) may conceal
founded by age and cohort differences. Another possibil- this shared environmental influence. Earlier we noted
ity is that nonadditive genetic effects play a larger role in that, for behavioral prediction, it is often a short-sighted
personality than suggested by MZ and DZ correlations. strategy to rely exclusively on measures of broad super-
Nonadditive genetic effects refer to effects of genes that factors. Likewise, such exclusive reliance may limit re-
interact to influence a trait, in contrast to additive ge- search into the etiology of personality differences.
netic effects in which genes add up. Nonadditive ef- Third, although it is often said disparagingly that be-
fects only contribute slightly to the resemblance of DZ havioral genetics is adevelopmental and static, recent
twins and other first-degree relatives, whereas MZ twins findings in three different areas of research demonstrate
are identical for all (additive and nonadditive) genetic that behavioral genetic methods are ideal for application
The Origins of Individual Dif ferences in Personality 333

to questions about age-related changes in etiology, in re- may produce short-term changes in personality pheno-
lation to both normal and abnormal development. First, types but that genetic factors contribute to individual
research on cognitive development shows that the heri- set-points to which individuals will regress (Lykken &
tability of IQ increases from early childhood through Tellegen, 1996). Longitudinal data with multiple, re-
late adolescence (McGue, Bacon, & Lykken, 1993; peated measurementsover both short and long inter-
Plomin, Fulker, Corley, & DeFries, 1997). In this in- valsare needed to fully test the predictions derived
stance, one can think of a heritability estimate as an from the model.
outcome variable. When it changes with age, this sug-
gests that the balance of genetic versus environmental
Molecular Genetic Analysis of Personality
causal processes differs at successive developmental
stages. In the case of IQ, the findings suggest that the ef- One of the most exciting directions for genetic research
fect on IQ of environmental factors shared by siblings on personality involves the use of molecular genetic
dissipates with age, as each child increasingly seeks out techniques to identify some of the specific genes re-
environments that are correlated with his or her genetic sponsible for genetic influences on personality. Quanti-
endowments. Second, research on the development of tative genetic studies are widely seen as a necessary
drug dependence suggests that the causes of initiation preliminary to identifying heritable phenotypes that can
and of dependence are not identical so that the factors be usefully examined at the molecular genetic level
that lead adolescents to sample drugs are not necessarily (Martin, Boomsma, & Machin, 1997), and finding
the same factors that lead to drug dependence (Kendler genes for personality will revolutionize psychological
et al., 1999). Third, research in developmental psycho- research by providing direct measures of specific geno-
pathology suggests that the pattern of antisocial behav- types for individuals.
ior that begins early in life, is pervasive across settings, The search for genes for personality is difficult be-
and persists into adulthood is more likely to be influ- cause, unlike classical single-gene disorders in which a
enced by genetic factors than is the pattern of late- single gene is necessary and sufficient to produce the
onset, situational, transient delinquency (Taylor et al., disorder, there is no evidence for such major effects of
2000). In combination, these three sets of findings illus- genes for personality. For quantitative traits like per-
trate how quantitative genetic studies can play an impor- sonality, genetic influence is much more likely to in-
tant role in illuminating developmental processes. volve multiple genes of varying but small effect size,
Fourth, behavioral genetic methods are also being which greatly increases the difficulty of detecting such
used to address the processes of continuity and change genes. Genes for complex traits influenced by multiple
in personality development. The quantitative methods genes and multiple environmental factors are known as
that behavioral geneticists use to estimate genetic and quantitative trait loci (QTLs; Plomin & Crabbe, 2000).
environmental components of phenotypic variance at a The goal is not to find the gene for a personality trait but
given point in time can be extended to estimate genetic rather some of the many genes that make contributions
contributions to continuity and change across time by of varying effect sizes to the variance of the trait.
analyzing cross-twin correlations across different times. Researchers attempting to find QTLs for personality
Longitudinal personality data from twins show that have investigated allelic association using DNA markers
such MZ cross-twin correlations are consistently and that are in or near genes thought to be relevant to the
significantly larger than DZ cross-twin correlations, trait. Allelic association refers to a correlation between
providing evidence that a major source of personality alleles of a DNA marker and trait scores across unre-
continuity in individual differences is attributable to ge- lated individuals: Allelic association occurs when indi-
netic factors (McGue et al., 1993; Pedersen & Reynolds, viduals with a particular allele for the marker have
1998; Viken, Rose, Kaprio, & Koskenvuo, 1994). This higher scores on the trait. Genes thought to be relevant
does not mean that genes fix personality but rather to a particular trait are often called candidate genes,
that genetic factors contribute to the preservation of in- which is something of a misnomer. For example, for car-
dividual differences over long stretches of the life span, diovascular disease, several cholesterol-related genes
at least from adolescence onwards. A possible interpre- were good candidate genes because it was known that
tation of these findings is the set-point model (Carey, cholesterol is involved in the process leading to heart
2003), which argues that environmental fluctuations disease. However, for personality, much less is known
334 Personality Development

about relevant physiological mechanisms, which means heritability coefficient indexes not only direct effects
that few specific genes can be suggested as candidates. of genes but also effects of interactions between genes
The phrase candidate gene has thus been corrupted to and environments (Rutter & Silberg, 2002). Gene-envi-
include any genes that might conceivably be related to ronment interactions (GxE) occur when the effect on a
personality. The problem with this loose use of this person of exposure to a particular environment is condi-
phrase is that any gene expressed in the brain could be tional upon their genotype (or conversely, when envi-
considered as a candidate gene, so the phrase loses its ronmental experiences moderate gene expression).
meaning (Plomin & Caspi, 1999). Because interactions are independent of main effects, it
A meta-analysis of studies reporting data on associa- is possible that even genome-wide scans of very large
tions between candidate genes and personality traits numbers of people whose personalities are carefully
concluded there were few replicable associations (Mu- measured will fail to detect genes whose effects are
nafo et al., 2003). Much of the initial excitement about conditional on environmental risk.
research on molecular genetics and personality has In various branches of medicine that deal with com-
given way to a more sober appreciation of the pitfalls in- plex multifactorial outcomes (e.g., cardiovascular dis-
volved in this kind of research (Benjamin, Ebstein, & ease), GxE are being discovered and replicated. In the
Belmaker, 2002). The situation is not much better in behavioral sciences, several studies also suggest the pos-
psychiatric genetics (Plomin & McGuffin, 2003). Sev- sibility that some complex traits, instead of resulting
eral explanations have been invoked to explain some of from many genes of small effect, result from relatively
the failures to find psychiatric genes that withstand fewer genes whose effects are conditional on exposure
replication over time, including but not limited to publi- to environmental risk. One study showed that a func-
cation bias, phenotypic heterogeneity, allelic hetero- tional polymorphism in the promoter region of the gene
geneity, weak prior probabilities of association, multiple encoding the neurotransmitter-metabolizing enzyme
testing, population stratification, and inadequate sam- monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) moderated the effect of
ple size (Cardon & Palmer, 2003; Colhoun, McKeigue, child maltreatment in the cycle of violence (Caspi et al.,
& Smith, 2003; Lohmueller, Pearce, Pike, Lander, & 2002). Specifically, maltreated children whose geno-
Hirschhorn, 2003; Merikangas & Risch, 2003; Sullivan, type conferred low levels of MAOA expression more
Eaves, Kendler, & Neale, 2001; van den Oord & Sulli- often developed conduct disorder than children whose
van, 2003). These same explanations may account for genotype conferred high levels of MAOA. A second
the slow pace of discovering, and replicating, genes for study showed that a functional polymorphism in the pro-
personality. It may be useful to step back and reflect on moter region of the serotonin transporter gene moder-
the surge of interest in the search for personality genes, ated the influence of stressful life events on depression
which was stimulated by psychiatrists for two reasons. (Caspi, Sugden, et al., 2003). Specifically, individuals
First, it was hoped that the use of quantitative personal- with one or two copies of the short allele exhibited more
ity traits (rather than categorical psychiatric diagnoses) depression following stressful life events than individu-
would offer greater statistical power and thus facilitate als homozygous for the long allele. Both studies re-
the identification of genetic effects of very small effect ceived initial, independent replication (Foley et al.,
size. So far, more power has not proven to be a panacea. 2004; Kaufman et al., 2004; Kendler, Kuhn, Vittum,
Second, personality traits were thought to represent en- Prescott, & Riley, 2005; but this is a fast-moving field,
dophenotypes for psychiatric disordersvariables that and readers are encouraged to monitor developments
are intermediate on a causal chain from genes to disor- that will have no doubt occurred since this writing).
der. There was hope that gene associations would be It is possible that bringing together measured geno-
found more successfully with endophenotypes than has types and measured environments to study personality
been the case with diagnosed disorders because the for- traits and psychiatric disorders more accurately reflects
mer are thought to have simpler genetic underpinnings the processes by which personality phenotypes develop
than disorders themselves. Whether personality traits than does the study of genetic main effects. However,
fulfill the criteria for endophenotypes is debatable such GxE research requires careful, deliberate, theory-
(Gottesman & Gould, 2003). guided hypothesis testing. Moffitt, Caspi, and Rutter
It is also likely that genes do not directly encode for (2005) have outlined several strategic steps for GxE
personality traits. In fact, it is now recognized that the tests in developmental psychopathology, which may be
Personality Continuity and Change 335

extended to study personality phenotypes. Up to now, which the relative ordering of individuals on a given
developmental psychologists have played a limited role trait is maintained over time. Two contradictory predic-
in molecular genetics research. We suggest that they may tions have been proposed about the rank-order stability
contribute in two fundamental ways. First, they can help of personality traits. The classical trait perspective ar-
refine the measurement of psychological phenotypes for gues that personality traits in adulthood are biologically
inclusion in genetic research. Second, they can help to based temperaments that are not susceptible to the influ-
measure developmental contexts and correlated environ- ence of the environment and thus do not change over
mental risks that may interact with genetic factors to time (McCrae et al., 2000). From this essentialist per-
shape personality development. It may be that ignoring spective, we would expect the test-retest correlations to
nurture has handicapped the ability of the psychological be high, even early in life. In contrast, the radical con-
sciences to better understand nature, and developmental textual perspective emphasizes the importance of life
psychologists may be able to rectify the situation. changes and role transitions in personality development
and suggests that personality should be fluid and prone
to change and should yield low test-retest correlation co-
PERSONALITY CONTINUITY AND CHANGE efficients, especially during developmental periods
characterized by rapid physical, cognitive, and social
The assertion that an individuals personality has changes (Lewis, 2001).
changed or remained the same over time is ambiguous. Existing longitudinal studies do not support either of
The boy who has daily temper tantrums when he is age 2 these positions. A meta-analysis of the rank-order sta-
but weekly tantrums when he is age 9 has increased his bility of personality (organized according to the five-
level of emotional control; he has changed in absolute factor model) revealed six major conclusions (Roberts &
terms. But if he ranks first in temper tantrums among DelVecchio, 2000):
his peers at both ages, he has not changed in relative
terms. Further ambiguity arises if the form of the be-
1. Test-retest correlations over time are moderate in
havior changes. If this boy emerges into adulthood as a
magnitude, even from childhood to early adulthood.
man who is irritable and moody, we may grant that the
2. Rank-order stability increases with age. Test-retest
phenotype has changed but claim that the underlying
correlations (unadjusted for measurement error) in-
personality has not. A third ambiguity arises when a
creased from .41 in childhood to .55 at age 30, and then
claim of continuity rests on observations not of an indi-
reached a plateau around .70 between ages 50 and 70.
vidual but of a sample of individuals. The continuity of
an attribute at the group level may be masking large but 3. Rank-order stability decreases as the time interval
mutually canceling changes at the individual level. between observations increases.
There are several meanings denoted by the term continu- 4. Rank-order stability does not vary markedly across
ity. The purpose of this section is to disentangle those the Big Five traits.
meanings. First, we review evidence about three types of 5. Rank-order stability does not vary markedly accord-
continuity and change observed in longitudinal re- ing to assessment method (i.e., self-reports, observer
search: differential, mean-level, and ipsative. Second, ratings, and projective tests).
we review the conceptual challenge of testing and docu- 6. Rank-order stability does not vary markedly by gender.
menting coherence in personality functioning across
time and in diverse circumstances. Several implications can be drawn from this meta-
analysis. First, the level of continuity in childhood and
Types of Continuity Observed in adolescence is much higher than originally expected
Longitudinal Research (Lewis, 2001), especially after age 3. Even more impres-
sive is the fact that the level of stability increases in a
Differential Continuity and Change
relatively linear fashion through adolescence and young
Continuity and change are most often indexed by corre- adulthood. Young adulthood has been described as de-
lations between personality scores across two points in mographically dense, in that it involves more life-chang-
time (i.e., test-retest correlations). These differential, or ing roles and identity decisions than any other period in
rank-order stability correlations, reflect the degree to the life course (Arnett, 2000). Despite these dramatic
336 Personality Development

contextual changes, personality differences remain re- strate mean-level changes after adulthood is reached
markably consistent during this time period. Second, (Costa & McCrae, 1997). In contrast, proponents of a
personality continuity in adulthood peaks later than ex- life-span developmental perspective emphasize the im-
pected. According to one prominent perspective, per- portance of life changes and role transitions in personal-
sonality traits are essentially fixed and unchanging after ity development and suggest that mean-level changes do
age 30 (McCrae & Costa, 1994). However, the meta-an- occur and often at ages much later than young adulthood
alytic findings show that rank-order stability peaks (Helson, Kwan, John, & Jones, 2002).
some time after age 50, but at a level well below unity. A meta-analysis synthesized and organized (accord-
Thus, individual differences in personality traits con- ing to the five-factor model) data from 87 longitudinal
tinue to change throughout adulthood, but only modestly studies spanning the period from age 10 to 101 years
after age 50. Third, the magnitude of differential stabil- (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, in press). The pattern
ity of personality traits, although not as high as essen- of change in the first domain of the Big Five, Extraver-
tialists would claim, is still remarkably high. In this sion, was complex until this superfactor was divided into
regard, it is interesting to compare personality traits to constituent elements of social dominance (assertiveness,
two overlapping trait domains, which show even higher dominance) and social vitality (talkativeness, sociabil-
differential stability: (1) cognitive abilities and (2) in- ity). Traits associated with social dominance increased
terests. Measures of cognitive ability exhibit more lon- from adolescence through early middle age, whereas
gitudinal consistency than measures of personality traits associated with social vitality increased in adoles-
traits. It is unlikely that this difference is simply a func- cence and then showed decreases in young adulthood
tion of the differential reliability of the two kinds of and old age. Consistent with evidence from cross-sec-
measures because this difference holds up even when tional comparisons of different age groups (McCrae
the measures are corrected for unreliability. However, a et al., 2000), traits belonging to the domains of Agree-
related methodological consideration is that ability mea- ableness and Conscientiousness increased in young
sures show greater continuity because ability tests de- adulthood and middle age. Traits belonging to the do-
mand maximal performance, whereas personality main of Neuroticism decreased mostly in young adult-
questionnaires assess representative, typical perfor- hood. Finally, traits from the Openness to Experience
mance; the former test format may yield evidence of domain showed increases in adolescence and young
greater continuity (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). adulthood and a tendency to decrease in old age.
Measures of interests also exhibit more longitudinal In general, the longitudinal evidence documents that,
consistency than measures of personality traits, espe- at least from adolescence through adult life, most people
cially during the 20-year period from early adolescence become more psychologically mature. Two distinct defi-
through early adulthood (Low, Yoon, Roberts, & nitions of maturity prevail in developmental theories
Rounds, 2005). Interests may stabilize at an earlier age (Hogan & Roberts, 2004). The first, humanistic defini-
because, relative to personality traits, they are more tion equates maturity with self-actualization and per-
likely to involve motivational processes that lead indi- sonal growth and with the process of becoming less
viduals to select congruent and reinforcing experiences. defensive and rigid and more creative and open to feel-
ings. According to the longitudinal evidence (Roberts
Mean-Level Continuity and Change
et al., in press), the data do not support this developmen-
Mean-level change refers to changes in the average trait tal progression; after young adulthood, people do not
level of a population. This type of change is thought to grow increasingly open to experience and toward old age
result from maturational processes shared by a popula- they actually exhibit declines on traits related to Open-
tion and is typically assessed by mean-level differences ness to Experience. The second, functional definition
in specific traits over time, which indicate whether the equates maturity with the capacity to become a produc-
sample as a whole is increasing or decreasing on a trait. tive and involved contributor to society, with the process
Contradictory perspectivessimilar to those guiding of becoming more planful, deliberate, and decisive, but
predictions about differential stabilityhave also also more considerate and charitable (traits encom-
guided expectations about mean-level changes in per- passed by higher levels of Emotional Stability, Consci-
sonality traits. Proponents of the five-factor model of entiousness, and Agreeableness). According to the
personality argue that personality traits do not demon- longitudinal data, most people do appear to become
Personality Continuity and Change 337

more functionally mature with age, and those who de- itly refers to continuity at the individual level. Ipsative
velop the cardinal traits of psychological maturity earli- continuity denotes continuity in the configuration of
est are more effective in their love, work, and health (as variables in an individual across time. Ipsative continu-
reviewed later). ity could also be called person-centered continuity. The
Three additional aspects of these longitudinal find- latter term derives from Blocks (1971) distinction be-
ings deserve note. First, there are no discernible sex dif- tween a variable-centered approach to personality,
ferences in patterns of mean-level continuity and change which is concerned with the relative standing of persons
across the Big Five. Apparently, men and women change across variables, and a person-centered approach, which
in the same ways over the life course, although mean- is concerned with the salience and configuration of
level differences between the sexes are maintained over variables in the person (see the first section). An ip-
time. This suggests that the causes of personality conti- sative approach to the study of development seeks to dis-
nuity and change across the life course are likely to be cover continuities in personality functioning across
the same for the sexes. Second, the majority of mean- development by identifying each persons salient attri-
level personality change occurs in young adulthood not butes and their intraindividual organization.
in adolescence, as we might suspect given traditional Relatively little longitudinal research has been con-
theories of psychological development. This pattern of ducted from an ipsative point of view. An exception is
change is not simply a recent historical phenomenon be- Blocks (1971) Lives Through Time in which he em-
cause it was observed in different cohorts across the ployed the Q-sort technique of personality description
twentieth century. This finding suggests that the causes to analyze continuity and change. Continuity and
of normative personality change are likely to be identi- change were indexed by computing correlations across
fied by narrowing research attention to the study of the set of attributesQ-correlationsbetween an indi-
young adulthood. Third, for select trait categories, viduals Q-sort profiles from different measurement oc-
change occurs well past young adulthood, demonstrat- casions; the higher the correlation, the more the
ing the continued plasticity of personality well beyond configuration of attributes in the individual remained
typical age markers of maturity. stable across time. Blocks analysis showed that aggre-
The evidence base about continuity and change still gate indices of continuity masked large individual dif-
has several important gaps. First, the best data about ferences in personality continuity. For example, the
personality continuity and changeand hence the most average Q-correlations between early and late adoles-
reliable conclusionscontinue to be restricted to adult cence exceeded .70 and those between late adolescence
samples. Relatively few studies have used a comprehen- and adulthood exceeded .50, but the intraindividual Q-
sive set of personality variables to characterize young correlations ranged from moderately negative to the
children and to track continuities and changes in their maximum imposed by measurement error. Other studies
personalities over time. Second, most longitudinal stud- of personality continuity and change between childhood
ies continue to estimate continuity and change over only and adolescence report average Q-correlations ranging
two waves of assessment, despite the advent of new from .43 to .71, with considerable variability in the dis-
methodological approaches that are appropriate for an- tribution of these scores; intraindividual Q-correlations
swering more nuanced questions about both short- and ranged from .44 to .92, indicating that from childhood
long-term temporal dynamics (Biesanz, West, & Kwok, to adolescence people vary widely in how much continu-
2003). Finally, and most important, the next generation ity or change they exhibited (Asendorpf & van Aken,
of studies should move beyond description and attempt 1991; Ozer & Gjerde, 1989).
to explain patterns of continuity and change (Mroczek & An interesting discovery is that there are meaningful
Spiro, 2003). individual differences in intraindividual continuity. In a
longitudinal American study, Block (1971) originally
Ipsative Continuity and Change
reported that those persons whose personalities re-
Differential and mean-level continuities are indexed by mained stable from adolescence to adulthood (non-
statistics that characterize a sample of individuals. changers) were more intellectually, emotionally, and
However, continuity at the group level may not mirror socially successful as adolescents than the changers, and
continuity at the individual level. For this reason, some a measure of adjustment also showed them to be better
researchers examine ipsative continuity, which explic- adjusted. In European samples, Asendorpf and van
338 Personality Development

Aken (1991) also found that the most resilient children its psychological meaning as revealed in relationships to
showed the most continuity of personality patterns a variety of other measures (Ozer, 1986, p. 52) across
across time. In a New Zealand sample, Roberts, Caspi, time and in different contexts. Accordingly, the investi-
and Moffitt (2001) reported that traits associated with gator who claims to have discovered coherence must
the domains of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and have a theoryno matter how rudimentary or implicit
Emotional Stability were positively correlated with in- that specifies the basis on which the diverse behaviors
creased personality consistency: People who were inter- and attributes can be said to belong to the same equiva-
personally effective, planful, decisive, and considerate lence class. The theories behind claims of coherence
were less likely to change over time. Although there is often amount to appeals to the readers intuition. In the
no obvious explanation for this replicated finding, one personality taxonomy that we presented earlier in this
possibility is that Agreeable, Conscientious, and Emo- chapter, we described current theories about the
tionally Stable people are better equipped to deal with processes underlying the Big Five traits. As researchers
social-developmental challenges across the life course. develop richer and more comprehensive theories about
They have more personal capital in the form of in- the nature of particular individual differences, the task
creased resilience, which allows them to master more of making theory-based predictions about coherence
efficiently the life challenges that they face and to recu- should become easier. We now review three conceptual
perate more quickly from aversive and disappointing life approaches to the problem of studying personality co-
events that they encounter. In contrast, their more brittle herence across the life course. Each of these social-de-
counterparts may be more susceptible to the influence velopmental approaches provides a framework for
of their environment. The robust finding that some peo- understanding coherence by focusing on the distinctive
ple are more prone to change than others calls for re- ways in which individuals organize their behavior to
search that systematically tests for an explanation. meet new environmental demands and developmental
challenges.

An Organizational-Adaptational Perspective
Personality Coherence
An organizational-adaptational perspective focuses on
The kinds of continuity discussed so far refer to homo- tasks and milestones that are encountered during the
typic continuitycontinuity of similar behaviors or course of development and on how these are met by dif-
phenotypic attributes over time. The concept of coher- ferent personalities (Masten & Coatsworth, 1995; Mas-
ence enlarges the definition of continuity to include het- ten et al., 1999; van Lieshout, 2000). According to this
erotypic continuitycontinuity of an inferred attribute perspective, personality traits influence problem-solv-
presumed to underlie diverse phenotypic behaviors. Spe- ing modalities that individuals use when meeting new
cific behaviors in childhood may not predict phenotypi- developmental challenges at different points in the life
cally similar behavior later in adulthood but may still be course (e.g., developing competent peer relationships in
associated with behaviors that are conceptually consis- childhood, establishing appropriate cross-sex relation-
tent with the earlier behavior (Livson & Peskin, 1980). ships in adolescence, learning to parent in early adult-
Kagan (1969) noted that heterotypic continuities are hood, or providing for dependent parents in middle age).
most likely to be found from the earlier years of life, Some of these developmental tasks are universal,
when children go through numerous rapid changes. In whereas others are specific to a socicocultural context
contrast, homotypic continuities are more likely to be and historical period. A useful example of the organiza-
found after puberty, when psychological organization tional-adaptational perspective comes from Sroufe and
nears completion. colleagues work on the links between childrens early
With the coming of age of various longitudinal sam- attachment to mothers and the childrens adaptational
ples, examples of heterotypic continuities now abound in profiles during later developmental phases. This general
the psychological literature, as we see in the following approach enabled Sroufe and his colleagues to confer
section. But it is important to emphasize that coherence conceptual coherency on their findings that individuals
and heterotypic continuity refer to conceptual rather who were securely attached as infants later explored
than a literal continuity among behaviors: The notion their environments as toddlers (Matas, Arend, & Sroufe,
of coherence refers to a pattern of findings where a con- 1978), were less dependent on their teachers in the pre-
struct, measured by several different methods, retains school years (Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, 1983), attained
Personality and the Life Course: How Early-Emerging Personality Dif ferences Shape Developmental Pathways 339

higher sociometric status and displayed greater compe- course are expressed not only through the constancy of
tence in peer relations in late childhood (Urban, Carl- behavior across time and in diverse circumstances but
son, Egeland, & Sroufe, 1991), and appeared to also through the consistency over time in the ways that
establish appropriate cross-sex relationships in adoles- persons characteristically modify their changing con-
cence (Sroufe, Carlson, & Shulman, 1993). texts as a function of their behavior. We now turn to re-
view evidence of such continuities across the life course.
A Sociological Perspective
Beyond childhood the search for coherence becomes PERSONALITY AND TH E LIFE COURSE:
more complicated, and it may be that a purely psycho- HOW EARLY-EMERGING PERSONALITY
logical approach is insufficient for the analysis of per- DIFFERENCES SHAPE
sonality continuity and change as the individual DEVELOPMENTAL PATHWAYS
increasingly negotiates social roles defined by the cul-
ture (Settersten, 2003). Some researchers have found it Two events have served to make research on personality
useful to adopt a sociocultural perspective and to con- trait development more vibrant over the past 10 years.
ceive of the life course as a sequence of culturally de- First, developmental psychologists have begun to mea-
fined, age-graded roles (e.g., marriage, work, and sure personality traits rather than ignore them. Second,
parenting) that the individual enacts over time (Caspi, personality psychologists have become increasingly in-
1987; Helson, Mitchell, & Moane, 1984). In this fashion, terested in relating measures of personality traits to
the life course can be charted as a sequence of age- something besides other personality measures. The re-
linked social roles, and personality coherence can be ex- sult is robust evidence that early-emerging individual
plored by investigating consistencies in the ways differences in personality shape how individuals experi-
different persons select and perform different sociocul- ence, interpret, and respond to the developmental tasks
tural roles; for example, in whether they opt for conven- they face across the life course. In this section, we re-
tional or unconventional career paths or in whether they view longitudinal evidence about how personality traits
are off-time in relation to normative, age-graded tasks shape (a) the cultivation of social relationships, ( b) the
such as getting married. mastery of educational and work tasks, and (c) the pro-
An Evolutionary Psychology Perspective motion and maintenance of physical health. For each de-
velopmental task, we identify the most relevant
Bouchard (1995) correctly argued that a purely socio-
personality variables and outline the mechanisms by
cultural perspective on the life course ignores the fact
which these personality traits are hypothesized to exert
that life-histories themselves are complex evolved adap-
their influence.
tations (p. 91). An evolutionary perspective on the life
course complements the sociocultural perspective by
Cultivating Relationships: How Personality
exploring how personality variation is related to those
Shapes Friendships, Intimate Relationships,
adaptively important problems with which human be-
and Parenting
ings have had to repeatedly contend: It focuses research
on the genetically influenced strategies and tactics that One of the most important tasks faced by children and
individuals use for survival and reproduction. Evolu- adolescents is the establishment of friendships and ac-
tionary psychology thus focuses attention on the coher- ceptance among peers (Hartup & Stevens, 1999; Masten
ence of behavioral strategies that people use in, for & Coatsworth, 1998). Among children, all of the higher-
example, mate selection, mate retention, reproduction, order Big Five traits except Openness are important pre-
parental care, kin investment, status attainment, and dictors of social competence. Perhaps so many aspects
coalition building (D. Buss, 1999). Although these ideas of personality predict social competence because social
have not yet been tested in the context of long-term lon- functioning requires a wide array of skills, including
gitudinal studies, they show the promise of evolutionary emotional expression, emotional understanding, and
psychology for organizing longitudinal-developmental emotional and behavioral regulation (Rubin, Bukowski,
data on personality coherence. & Parker, Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume).
These three approaches, or road maps for studying Agreeable and Extraverted children show better social
personality across the life course, share an important competence concurrently and across time and experi-
assumption: Continuities of personality across the life ence growth in perceived social support from early to
340 Personality Development

late adolescence (Asendorpf & van Aken, 2003a; plications for the course of personality development be-
Branje, van Lieshout, & van Aken, 2004; Shiner, 2000; cause similarities between couple members create inter-
Shiner et al., 2003). Children high on Negative Emotion- personal experiences that reinforce initial tendencies
ality or low on aspects of Conscientiousness (e.g., atten- (Alwin, Cohen, & Newcomb, 1991; Caspi & Herbener,
tion and self-control) have a variety of social 1990). Second, personality differences influence peo-
difficulties concurrently and across time (Eisenberg ples exposure to relationship events. For example, peo-
et al., 2000); the interaction of high Negative Emotion- ple high in Neuroticism are more likely to be exposed to
ality and low self-regulation may be especially problem- daily conflicts in their relationships (Bolger & Zucker-
atic for social functioning (Eisenberg et al., 2000). man, 1995). Third, personality differences shape peo-
Personality continues to be an important predictor of ples reactions to the behavior of their partners. For
relationships in adulthood. Extraversion predicts posi- example, high-Disagreeable individuals may escalate
tive relationships (Shiner et al., 2002), whereas Neu- negative affect during conflict (e.g., Gottman, Coan,
roticism and Disagreeableness are the strongest and Carrere, & Swanson, 1998). Similarly, high-Agreeable
most consistent personality predictors of negative rela- people are better able to regulate emotions during inter-
tionship outcomesincluding relationship dissatisfac- personal conflicts (Jensen-Campbell & Graziano,
tion, conflict, abuse, and, ultimately dissolution 2001). Cognitive processes also come online in creating
(Karney & Bradbury, 1995; Shiner et al., 2002). These trait-correlated experiences (Snyder & Stukas, 1999).
effects of Neuroticism and low Agreeableness have For example, highly neurotic individuals may overreact
been uncovered in long-term studies following samples to minor criticism from their partner, believe they are no
of children into adulthood and in shorter-term longitu- longer loved when their partner does not call, or assume
dinal studies of adults. The potential contribution of infidelity on the basis of mere flirtation. Fourth, person-
personality differences to shaping abusive relationships ality differences evoke behaviors from partners that
has been further underscored by longitudinal studies contribute to relationship quality. For example, people
that find associations between early developing aggres- high in Neuroticism and low in Agreeableness may be
sive traits in childhood and subsequent abusive behavior more likely to express four behaviors identified as detri-
in adult romantic relationships (Andrews, Foster, Ca- mental to relationships: criticism, contempt, defensive-
paldi, & Hops, 2000; Ehrensaft, Moffitt, & Caspi, ness, and stonewalling (Gottman, 1994).
2004). One study that followed a large sample of adoles- Whereas a great deal of research has investigated the
cents across their multiple relationships in early adult- influence of personality on friendships and intimate re-
hood discovered that the influence of Neuroticism and lationships, fewer studies have considered the possibil-
low Agreeableness on relationship quality showed ity that parents personalities shape their parenting
cross-relationship generalization: It predicted the same styles and relationships with their children (Belsky &
abusive relationship experiences across relationships Barends, 2002). This is a curious omission. Although re-
with different partners (Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, searchers interested in psychiatric disorders have docu-
2002). Increasingly, sophisticated studies that include mented that maternal psychopathology can compromise
dyads (not just individuals) and multiple methods (not effective parenting (Goodman & Gotlib, 2002), devel-
just self reports) demonstrate that the link between per- opmental researchers have been slower to recognize that
sonality traits and relationship processes is more than parental personality forms a critical part of childrens
simply an artifact of shared method variance in the as- developmental context (Goldsmith, Losaya, Bradshaw,
sessment of these two domains (Donnellan, Conger, & & Campos, 1994). Moreover, behavioral genetic studies
Bryant, 2004; Watson, Hubbard, & Wiese, 2000). suggest that some parenting behaviors may be heritable
An important research goal is to uncover the proximal (Spinath & OConnor, 2003). This does not mean that
relationship-specific processes that mediate these per- there is a gene for parenting styles. Rather, it suggests
sonality effects on relationship outcomes (Reiss, Capo- that individual differences in parenting behaviors may
bianco, & Tsai, 2002). Personality traits affect be related to personality characteristics that are influ-
relationships by influencing and altering microinterac- enced by genetic factors.
tional processes. First, individuals select their interac- The handful of studies that have examined personal-
tional contexts by choosing partners who resemble them. ity parenting associationsusing self-reports as well
The tendency to form unions with similar others has im- as observations of parentingsuggest that Extraversion
Personality and the Life Course: How Early-Emerging Personality Dif ferences Shape Developmental Pathways 341

and Agreeableness are related to sensitive and respon- traits from the domain of Conscientiousness are the
sive parenting, whereas aspects of Neuroticism, such as most important noncognitive predictors of educational
anxiety and irritability, are related to less competent achievement and occupational attainment (Judge et al.,
parenting (e.g., Belsky, Crnic, & Woodsworth, 1995; 1999; Shiner, in press). In fact, childhood Conscien-
Clark, Kochanska, & Ready, 2000; Kochanska, Clark, tiousness predicts improvements in academic achieve-
et al., 1997; Kochanska et al., 2004; Losoya, Callor, ment across time into adulthood (Shiner, 2000; Shiner
Rowe, & Goldsmith, 1997; Metsapelto & Pulkkinen, et al., 2003). Similarly, adult Conscientiousness predicts
2003; Prinzie et al., 2004). Much more work needs to be job performance across a wide variety of measures and
done: First, most of the research to date has focused on across nearly all types of jobs (Barrick, Mount, &
parents of very young children to the virtual exclusion of Judge, 2001). Conscientiousness encompasses many
adolescents. Second, most of the research has not tested traits that are necessary for completing work effec-
mediators (e.g., parental attributions) of personality tively: the capacities to sustain attention, to strive to-
parenting associations. Third, most of the research has ward high standards, and to inhibit impulsive behavior.
focused on the main effects of personality and has not In contrast, childhood Neuroticism predicts lower adult
addressed the conditions under which particular person- occupational attainment (Judge et al., 1999). Adult Neu-
ality attributes are more or less important in explaining roticism appears to have small negative effects on job
parenting behavior (e.g., Are personality main effects performance (Barrick et al., 2001; Hurtz & Donovan,
moderated by qualities of the martial relationship or by 2000) and is associated with lower academic attainment
the childs temperament?). For example, there is some (Shiner et al., 2002).
evidence that difficult children are particularly likely to Links between the other Big Five traits and academic
be rejected by highly Conscientious mothers during and work achievement are less consistent and robust but
problem-solving tasks (Neitzel & Stright, 2004). Fourth, are still found. Openness to Experience/ Intellect pre-
to our knowledge, no study has examined personality ef- dicts academic achievement in samples of school-age
fects on parenting behavior in relation to multiple chil- children, adolescents, and college students (Farsides &
dren in the same family, and this is a design that has the Woodfield, 2003; Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & Finch
power to test the cross-situational generalizability of 1997; John et al., 1994), and child and adult Agreeable-
personality effects (across offspring) and to estimate ness sometimes do as well (Shiner, 2000; Shiner et al.,
the influence of parental personality on family life inde- 2002). Meta-analyses reveal that Extraversion, Agree-
pendently of other family-wide environmental effects. ableness, and Openness predict some more limited as-
The study of personality effects on social relation- pects of work performance in a subset of occupations
ships is exciting territory where hypotheses about per- (Barrick et al., 2001). Research with children, adoles-
sonality dynamics can be tested using multiple and cents, and adults demonstrates that many of the links
creative methodologies. These approaches need not be between personality traits (especially Conscientious-
confined to close relationships. Bugental (2000) pro- ness) and various indices of achievement remain signifi-
posed a taxonomy of social relationships that offers the cant after controlling for individual differences in
promise of helping to coordinate personality research by ability (Judge et al., 1999; Shiner, 2000; Shiner et al.,
focusing attention on how personality variables shape 2003), but sometimes the links disappear (Schmidt &
behaviors in five domains of social life: (1) attachment Hunter, 2004). The predictive associations between tem-
relations, (2) mating relations, (3) hierarchical power re- perament and personality traits and achievement are ap-
lations between persons of unequal status, (4) reciprocal parent early in life, at the time that children first enroll
relations among persons of equal status, and (5) coali- in school (Miech, Essex, & Goldsmith, 2001). The find-
tional-group relations. ing that personality effects on achievement emerge early
in life is important because school adjustment and aca-
Striving and Achieving: How Personality Shapes demic performance have cumulative effects over time
Performance in School and Work Settings (Entwisle & Alexander, 1993).
The personality processes involved may vary across
During the life course, individuals assume multiple per- different stages of development, and at least four candi-
formance tasks (e.g., pursuing an education, assuming a date processes deserve research scrutiny (see Schneider,
job, or managing and allocating resources). Personality Smith, Taylor, & Fleenor, 1998). First, the personality
342 Personality Development

achievement associations may reflect attraction ef- dence that this occurs in part via the processing of infor-
fects, or active niche-picking, whereby people ac- mation. For example, higher positive emotions facilitate
tively choose educational and work experiences whose the efficient processing of complex information and are
qualities are concordant with their own personalities. associated with creative problem solving (Ashby, Isen,
For example, people who are more conscientious prefer & Turken, 1999; Fredrickson, 2003). In addition to
conventional jobs such as accounting and farming (Got- these effects on task effectiveness, personality may di-
tfredson, Jones, & Holland, 1993). People who are more rectly affect other aspects of work performance such as
extraverted prefer jobs that are described as social or en- interpersonal interactions (Hurtz & Donovan, 2000).
terprising such as teaching or business management Personality traits may also directly influence perfor-
(Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). Moreover, extraverted mance motivation (e.g., Conscientiousness consistently
individuals are more likely to assume leadership roles in predicts stronger goal setting and self-efficacy, whereas
multiple settings (Anderson et al., 2001). All of the Big Neuroticism predicts these positive motivations nega-
Five have substantial relations with better performance tively; Erez & Judge, 2001; Judge & Ilies, 2002).
when the personality predictor is appropriately aligned Personality traits not only affect performance in
with work criteria (Hogan & Holland, 2003). This indi- school and at work; they are also influenced by these ex-
cates that if people find jobs that fit with their disposi- periences. The corresponsive principle summarizes
tions they will experience greater levels of job the empirical observation that the most likely effect of
performance, which should lead to greater success, achievement-setting experiences on personality develop-
tenure, and satisfaction across the life course (Judge ment is to deepen the characteristics that lead people to
et al., 1999). those experiences in the first place (Roberts, Caspi, &
Second, personality achievement associations re- Moffitt, 2003). For example, if people assume leadership
flect recruitment effects, whereby people are se- positions because they are socially dominant, they will
lected into achievement situations and are given become more socially dominant through their experi-
preferential treatment on the basis of their personality ence as leaders. Similarly, some individuals may per-
characteristics. These recruitment effects begin to ap- form less well in educational settings as a function of
pear early in development. For example, childrens their high irritability and aggressiveness. In turn, they
personalities influence their emerging relationships are at a heightened risk of becoming increasingly antag-
with teachers at a young age (Birch & Ladd, 1998). In onistic and alienated over time through their failure ex-
adulthood, job applicants who are more extraverted, periences (Shiner et al., 2002). The corresponsive
conscientious, and less neurotic are liked better by in- principle thus links two mutually supportive life-course
terviewers and are more often recommended for the dynamics: social-selection, wherein people select en-
job (Cook, Vance, & Spector, 2000). vironments that are correlated with their personality
Third, some personality achievement associations traits, and social-influence, wherein environmental
emerge as consequences of attrition or deselection experiences shape personality functioning. According to
pressures, whereby people leave achievement settings longitudinal data, the traits that select people into
(e.g., schools or jobs) that do not fit with their personal- specific experiences are the traits that are most influ-
ity or are released from these settings because of their enced in response to those experiences. Life experi-
trait-correlated behaviors (Cairns & Cairns, 1994). For ences do not impinge themselves on people in a random
example, longitudinal evidence from different countries fashion causing widespread personality transformations;
shows that children who exhibit a combination of high rather, the traits that people already possess are changed
irritability/antagonism and poor self-control are at (i.e., deepened and elaborated) by trait-correlated expe-
heightened risk of unemployment (Caspi, Wright, Mof- riences that they create in achievement settings.
fitt, & Silva, 1998; Kokko, Bergman, & Pulkkinen,
2003; Kokko & Pulkkinen, 2000). Health Promotion and Maintenance: How
Fourth, personality achievement associations Personality Shapes Health Trajectories
emerge as a result of direct, proximal effects of person-
ality on performance. Personality traits may promote The lifelong interplay between psyche and soma is
certain kinds of task effectiveness; there is some evi- nowhere more apparent than in research documenting
Personality and the Life Course: How Early-Emerging Personality Dif ferences Shape Developmental Pathways 343

that personality traits contribute to the maintenance of tion, which are positively associated with health out-
physical integrity and health. Especially impressive are comes (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000). In
life-span studies documenting associations between per- contrast, individuals low in Conscientiousness engage in
sonality traits related to Conscientiousness with a variety of health-risk behaviors such as smoking, un-
longevity (Friedman et al., 1995). Individuals high in healthy eating habits, lack of exercise, unprotected sex-
traits related to Disagreeableness (e.g., anger and hostil- ual intercourse, and dangerous driving habits (Bogg &
ity) appear to be at greatest risk of disease (e.g., cardio- Roberts, 2004). The association between Conscientious-
vascular illness; Miller, Smith, Turner, Guijarro, & ness-related traits and health-risk behaviors is espe-
Hallet, 1996). The evidence for the involvement of Neu- cially robust and appears to be stronger among
roticism in ill health is more mixed, with some research adolescents than adults, suggesting that this risky per-
pointing to links with increased risk of actual disease sonality trait merits greater research and public-health
and other studies documenting links with illness behav- attention. Future personality research could be usefully
ior only (Smith & Spiro, 2002). integrated with developmental research from a decision-
The study of health also serves to illustrate the utility theory perspective to better understand the decision-
of hierarchical structural models of personality in inte- making processes that may mediate the links between
grating and interpreting research findings. For example, traits from the Conscientiousness domain and health-
some of the inconsistency that has been observed in risk behaviors (Hampson, Andrews, Barckley, Lichten-
studies of hostility and cardiovascular disease may be stein, & Lee, 2000). Such research has the potential to
due to the fact that hostility is a facet or component of contribute to a psychology of public health.
both Neuroticism and Agreeableness (versus Antago- Third, personality differences may be related to re-
nism; Smith & Williams, 1992). Measures of hostility actions to illness. This includes a wide class of behav-
that reflect overt interpersonal expressions of anger are iors, including the possibility that personality
facets of Agreeableness that may be the lethal personal- differences affect the selection and execution of coping
ity risk factor for coronary heart disease, whereas mea- behaviors (e.g., Scheier & Carver, 1993), modulate dis-
sures of hostility that tap irritation and self-focused tress reduction, and shape treatment adherence (Ken-
negativity are facets of Neuroticism and may be better ford et al., 2002).
predictors of health complaints rather than actual health The previous processes linking personality traits to
outcomes. A taxonomic model of personality can help physical health are not mutually exclusive. Moreover,
researchers to make conceptual and measurement re- different personality traits may affect physical health
finements in testing psychosomatic hypotheses. via different processes. For example, facets of Disagree-
Personality health associations may reflect at least ableness may be most directly linked to disease
three distinct processes (Contrada, Cather, & OLeary, processes, facets of low Conscientiousness may be more
1999; Rozanski, Blumenthal, & Kaplan, 1999). First, per- clearly implicated in health-damaging behaviors, and
sonality differences may be related to pathogenesis facets of Neuroticism may contribute to ill-health by
mechanisms that promote disease. This has been shaping reactions to illness.
evaluated most directly in studies relating various facets The study of personality and health has historically
of Disagreeableness/ hostility to greater reactivity in re- been confined to adults. However, this may well change
sponse to stressful experiences (Smith & Gallo, 2001). as health psychologists turn their attention to earlier
However, part of the complexity of testing hypotheses periods in development to understand public-health
about the role of personality in the physiological puzzles. Consider research on social inequalities in
processes of a disease involves the need for greater clar- health, which has tended to focus on low socioeco-
ity about the disease processes involved and during which nomic status in adulthood as the main causal variable
disease phases personality effects may be implicated. and on adults stress experiences as the main mediating
Second, personality differences may be related to mechanism. However, mounting evidence from life-
physical-health outcomes because they are associated course research points to the contribution of early life
with health-promoting or health-damaging behaviors. experiences and to the cumulative impact of sustained
For example, individuals high in Extraversion may foster social disadvantages on adult health, compelling health
social relationships, social support, and social integra- psychologists to turn their attention to examine the role
344 Personality Development

that personality factors may play in mediating the asso- ences accumulate over a lifetime, a focus on a single
ciations between early social experiences and poor outcome variable measured at a single point in time may
health across the life course (Chen & Mathews, 2002; underestimate the contribution of personality to the
Gallo & Mathews, 2003). course of developmental trajectories. Abelson (1985)
makes this point in noting that differences between
Predicting All of Behavior All of the Time? baseball players are trivial if considered on the basis of a
single at-bat but become meaningful over the course of a
Although personality traits have been shown to shape game, a season, and a career. These observations are not
developmental outcomes in multiple domains and in dif- intended to breed smug self-satisfaction. Rather, they
ferent age groups, a common refrain is that these predic- are meant to foster reasonable expectations and aspira-
tive associations only account for a fraction of the tions for research on personality development.
variance in outcomes of interest. This observation must
be balanced by four considerations. First, it seems nec-
PERSONALITY AND THE EMERGENCE
essary to periodically reissue the reminder that even
OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
small effect sizes are of theoretical and practical signif-
icance (McCartney & Rosenthal, 2000). By way of com-
Just as individual differences in personality shape
parison, epidemiological and clinical studies repeatedly
adaptation over time, childhood personality plays an
uncover associations whose effect sizes range between
important role in the development of psychopathology.
.1 and .3 (e.g., the association between decreased bone
One of the primary questions that has fueled scholarly
mineral density and risk of hip fracture or between the
and popular interest in temperament research is
nicotine patch and smoking abstinence), leading a recent
whether children with particular temperaments are at
expert panel to recommend rethinking conventional in-
greater risk of developing psychiatric problems. Much
terpretations of psychological research. Given adequate
of the early interest in temperament traits was gener-
attention to sampling considerations, researchers should
ated by Thomas and Chesss suggestion that childrens
be pleased with associations around .2 to .3 (Meyer
early individual differences could help set off a chain
et al., 2001). Second, debates about the size of personal-
of transactions between the child and the environment
ity effects are based on the implicit assumption that
that could lead eventually to the development of clini-
every behavior is the product of a single trait. This is im-
cal disorders (Thomas, Chess, & Birch, 1968). In this
plausible, because each individual is characterized by a
section, we address links between temperament /per-
personal pattern of multiple traits working additively
sonality and psychopathology. First, we survey several
and interactively to influence behavior. This multiple-
new directions in research about the association be-
trait perspective has important implications for effect-
tween personality and psychopathology. Second, we
size estimates: Simulation studies demonstrate that it is
summarize what is known about the predictive correla-
unreasonable and statistically inconceivable in multiply
tions between childhood temperament /personality dif-
determined systems for any single trait to explain much
ferences and later psychopathology. Third, we present a
more than 10% of the variance (Ahadi & Diener, 1989).
conceptual model of possible associations between
Further research on the interactions of personality traits
temperament /personality and psychopathology and
may enhance our ability to predict particular outcomes
provide empirical examples.
(see, e.g., Witt, Burke, Barrick, & Mount, 2002, on the
prediction of work performance from the interaction of New Directions in Research on Personality
Agreeableness and Conscientiousness). Third, social be- and Psychopathology
havior is a product of multiple personalities acting in
concert and influencing one another (Asendorpf, 2002). The links between personality and psychopathology are
Consider the case of relationship outcomes. If personal- turning out to be complex, and recent research recog-
ity effects are additive across partners, the true impact nizes this complexity. Next we describe three new direc-
of a personality trait on a relationship should be re- tions and advances taken in this area of research.
garded as the summed effect of two personalities not a A first advance springs from evidence that clinical
single individuals trait (Moffitt, Robins, & Caspi, disorders often include subgroups of individuals char-
2001). Fourth, because the effects of personality differ- acterized by different temperament or personality
Personality and the Emergence of Psychopathology 345

profiles. These subgroups may differ from each other they spring in part from similar causes, including shared
in at least four ways: They may (1) have developed the temperament /personality risk factors, whereas each dis-
disorder through different pathways, (2) have differ- order may also be related to other personality traits that
ent profiles of comorbidity and life adaptation, (3) are specific to that disorder (Lilienfeld, 2003; Weiss,
have different prognoses, and (4) respond differently Susser, & Catron, 1998). Specific models have been pro-
to different treatments. The presence of personality- posed to account for the comorbidity of internalizing and
based subgroups in some clinical disorders is consis- externalizing disorders. Watson and Clark (Clark & Wat-
tent with the principle of equifinality (i.e., children son, 1991; Watson et al., 1995) developed a tripartite
who are following widely different life paths may model to account for co-occurring internalizing disorders
eventually develop the same set of pathological symp- in adults. According to this model, depression and anxi-
toms). For example, the diagnosis of Anorexia Ner- ety tend to co-occur because both share high levels of
vosa appears to contain a subgroup defined by high Negative Affect (or Neuroticism); in addition, depression
levels of perfectionism, obsessive-compulsive fea- is specifically characterized by low Positive Affect (or
tures, and rigidity as well as excessive inhibition and Extraversion) and anxiety is specifically characterized
harm avoidance (Keel et al., 2004; see Westen & by physiological arousal. There is general support for this
Harnden-Fischer, 2001, for a similar subgroup): Rela- model in both children and adults (reviewed in Laurent &
tive to other individuals with Anorexia, this subgroup Ettelson, 2001; also Lonigan, Phillips, & Hooe, 2003),
appears to show poorer adaptive functioning and is but more recent research has added that social phobia is
likely to have a worse outcome. also specifically characterized by low Positive Affect
A second example comes from work on callous-un- (Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow, 1998). As for the external-
emotional traits in children with conduct disorder; cal- izing disorders, there is evidence that personality traits
lous-unemotional traits include lack of empathy, associated with low Conscientiousness (often called dis-
self-serving use of manipulation, restricted emotional inhibition) may underlie the co-occurrence of substance
expression, and impoverished conscience. Conduct-dis- dependence and antisocial behavior in adults; further,
ordered children with this constellation of traits show Conscientiousness appears to be linked with these
higher proactive (unprovoked) aggression, greater delin- disorders by virtue of shared genetic origins (Krueger
quency, and greater variety of conduct problems over et al., 2002).
time than conduct-disordered children without these A third prominent research question involves the ex-
traits (Frick, Cornell, Barry, Bodin, & Dane, 2003). tent to which normal-range personality traits can pro-
This subgroup is also characterized by different pat- vide adequate coverage of individual differences
terns of social-information processing; for example, relevant to psychopathology. In other words, can promi-
greater focus on positive aspects of aggression and nent trait models like the Big Five capture the relevant
lesser hostile attributional biases (Frick, Cornell, et al., variation in abnormal functioning? This question has
2003; Pardini, Lochman, & Frick, 2003). As these two been particularly important in relation to personality
examples demonstrate, personality research is helping disorders, where some research demonstrates that com-
to parse heterogeneous diagnostic groups into more ho- binations of the Big Five traits can capture the symp-
mogeneous subgroups, which may eventually help clini- toms of Borderline Personality Disorder (Trull,
cians in their treatment planning (Harkness & Widiger, Lynam, & Costa, 2003) and psychopathy
Lilienfeld, 1997). (Miller, Lynam, Widiger, & Leukefled, 2001), particu-
A second direction followed in this area of research is larly when combinations of lower-lower traits are used
the recognition that personality differences may help to (Reynolds & Clark, 2001). Further, a meta-analysis
explain patterns of comorbidity among disorders. Two of demonstrated that abnormal personality traits appear to
the most important patterns of comorbidity in both chil- share a hierarchical structure with normal-range per-
dren and adults involve the co-occurrence of depression sonality traits (Markon et al., 2005). However, normal-
and anxietythe internalizing disordersand the co-oc- range personality measures may need to be refined
currence of conduct problems and hyperactivitythe ex- somewhat to cover personality disorder variation more
ternalizing disorders. Several researchers have suggested completely. Some normal-range personality measures
that personality differences may account for patterns of may do better than others in tapping pathological per-
comorbidity: Certain disorders may co-occur because sonality functioning, and scales measuring some Big
346 Personality Development

Five traits (e.g., Agreeableness and Conscientiousness) against the development of externalizing symptoms.
include few negative descriptors at their extremes. Fu- Neuroticism also appears to be a risk factor for sub-
ture work will need to investigate whether current per- stance use problems. As we noted earlier, early
sonality measures could be strengthened by including anger and irritability predict externalizing problems
more extreme depictions of certain traits (Haigler & such as aggression and conduct problems; later in
Widiger, 2001). life, these traits may also lead to greater internaliz-
The personality traits relevant to some disorders are, ing symptoms.
quite possibly, not part of typical personality models. 3. Childrens self-control and attention promote the de-
Recent work on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) velopment of rule-abiding behavior versus externaliz-
provides a good example. Among preschool-age chil- ing, antisocial behavior; in other words, children who
dren, obsessive-compulsive behaviors are highly herita- are low on Conscientiousness are at risk of develop-
ble, but their genetic origins are largely separate from ing externalizing behaviors. Low Conscientiousness
the genetic origins of other anxiety-related behaviors also predicts problems with later drug use.
(Eley et al., 2003); OCD may be less related to general 4. Low Agreeableness (as well as low prosocial tenden-
distress or Neuroticism than are many of the other anxi- cies and high antagonism) is associated with later
ety disorders. Instead, OCD may have more specific as- antisocial, externalizing behaviors, and boys who are
sociations with a particular psychological feature: a chronically physically aggressive are at a heightened
disturbance in the emotion-based system that typically risk of serious delinquency and violence. Children
enables individuals to feel that closure has been reached with difficult temperaments (which is most akin to
and safety has been secured (Szechtman & Woody, low Agreeableness) likewise develop greater exter-
2004). Although this deficit may be related to Big Five nalizing problems than children with less difficult
traits in some way, it also appears likely to capture an temperaments.
aspect of psychological functioning that is largely sepa-
5. There is no evidence that childhood Openness is pre-
rate from the Big Five. The search for other individual
dictive of psychopathology, although it should be
differences outside the Big Five is likely to be important
noted that Openness has been studied less often than
in future research linking personality and psychopathol-
the other childhood traits.
ogy, because such traits may interact with Big Five
Research on personality and psychopathology is in
traits to give rise to specific disorders.
need of significant improvement, as both design and
measurement limitations continue to compromise the
Empirical Evidence for Specific external and internal validity of many studies: Most
Associations between Personality Traits samples are small or unrepresentative, reports of per-
and Psychological Disorders sonality and psychopathology are often obtained from
the same source, and measures of temperament and
Numerous studies have examined associations between
symptoms often overlap in content. Future research
temperament and personality differences and psycho-
about personality and psychopathology can adopt the
pathology, both concurrently and longitudinally. Two
more general recommendations outlined by Kraemer and
reviews (Shiner, in press; Tackett & Krueger, 2005; see
colleagues about how to define and measure risk factors
also Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this
for psychopathology (Jacobi, Hayward, de Zwaan, Krae-
volume) surveyed the research on the predictive rela-
mer, & Agras, 2004; Kraemer et al., 1997).
tions between childhood personality and disorders; the
conclusions were as follows:
Processes Linking Personality Differences
and Psychopathology
1. Extraversion appears to put children at risk of exter-
nalizing behaviors and aggression and protects Much of the current research on personality and psycho-
against internalizing symptoms. In contrast, behav- pathology simply documents correlations between
ioral inhibition predicts heightened risk for anxiety, temperament or personality traits and aspects of
depression, and general internalizing symptoms. psychopathology without articulating how the two do-
2. Neuroticism predicts later internalizing difficulties, mains may be connected. We present a conceptual model
whereas early fearfulness appears to protect children of possible associations between temperament /personal-
Personality and the Emergence of Psychopathology 347

ity and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. these children may have temperament traits that lead to
In laying out this conceptual model, we draw on models difficulty learning to inhibit behavior when faced with
elaborated by others (Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994; potential rewards. Second, observational studies show
Rothbart & Bates, 1998; Widiger, Verheul, & van den that adopted children who are at genetic risk for anti-
Brink, 1999). As much as possible, we borrow their ter- social behavior receive more negative control and coer-
minology to describe the possible associations, to avoid cive parenting from their adoptive parents than do
unnecessarily introducing new labels. First, personality adopted children not at genetic risk (OConnor, Deater-
may set in motion processes that cause the development Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & Plomin, 1998). These find-
of psychopathology (vulnerability association). Second, ings point to environmental elicitation in which
psychopathology may represent the extreme end of a childrens genetically influenced temperaments evoke
continuously distributed personality trait or cluster of coercive parenting behaviors.
traits (spectrum association). Third, personality may Environmental construal may be seen in the way that
protect against the development of psychopathology in aggressive children misinterpret the intentions of oth-
the face of stress and adversity (resilience association). ers. For example, such children seek less information
Fourth, personality may influence the course and prog- about social situations and are more likely to assume
nosis of a disorder, even if the personality trait is not a hostile intent on the part of other persons (Dodge et al.,
cause of the disorder (maintenance association). Fifth, Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume). Related to the
psychopathology may influence the course of personal- process of environmental construal are social compari-
ity development itself (scarring association). We now son processes in which temperament shapes the way
elaborate on these five types of association from a de- that individuals evaluate themselves in relation to oth-
velopmental perspective. ers. In some studies, externalizing children overesti-
mate their social competence relative to others
Vulnerability Association: Personality May Put
(Hughes, Cavell, & Grossman, 1997; Patterson, Kuper-
Children at Risk for the Development of
smidt, & Griesler, 1990).
Psychopathology
Environmental selection may be seen in the ways by
According to this model, which has garnered the most which some children select situations that can then re-
interest among researchers, personality traits set in mo- inforce particular behaviors. For example, children with
tion the processes that lead to psychopathology. How- poor self-control are more likely to form ties to delin-
ever, whereas there are numerous published reports of quent peers who, in turn, promote their antisocial be-
predictive associations between personality traits and havior (Wright, Caspi, Moffitt, & Silva, 2001). Through
psychiatric disorders, less empirical attention has been environmental manipulation, an individuals personality
given to causal processes. Previously, we outlined six alters and shapes the environment. For example, dis-
processes through which early temperament differences agreeable youth not only perceive more interpersonal
become elaborated into more broad personality disposi- conflict in their environment but also attempt to resolve
tions (see Table 6.3). These same six processes are conflict with destructive tactics (Jensen-Campbell &
likely to be the ones through which temperament differ- Graziano, 2001).
ences put children at risk for psychopathology as well.
Research on the development of conduct disorder Spectrum Association: Psychopathology May Be
serves to demonstrate how these six processes may op- an Extreme Manifestation of Personality
erate. Longitudinal research shows that conduct disorder Although psychiatric disorders are typically measured
and severe antisocial behavior are predicted by an early categorically, it is possible that some disorders are not
history of high negative emotionality, poor self-control, discrete conditions but represent extreme ends of con-
and high unmanageability (Sanson & Prior, 1999). How tinuously distributed personality dimensions or combi-
might these temperament differences contribute to the nations of dimensions (Sonuga-Barke, 1998; Widiger &
development of severe antisocial behavior over time? Clark, 2000). For example, some researchers have ar-
First, children with conduct disorder are especially gued that ADHD may be an extreme variant of an un-
sensitive to signals of reward (OBrien & Frick, 1996) derlying temperament or personality trait rather than a
but are relatively insensitive to punishing stimuli (Lyt- discrete condition that is clearly separable from normal
ton, 1990). Learning processes may be at work here; functioning (Jensen et al., 1997). Children with ADHD
348 Personality Development

combined type and hyperactive-impulsive type may ders; Beauchaine, 2003) and addressing sources of co-
have a basic deficit in inhibiting a prepotent response morbidity (Ruscio & Ruscio, 2004). Such methods also
when faced with potential reinforcement for that re- are highly relevant to developmental research because
sponse (i.e., ADHD may represent the extreme low ends they can be used to identify children who are at risk for
of the traits of attention and inhibitory control; Barkley, disorders and can help pinpoint sensitive periods when
1997). ADHD thus appears to be particularly associ- discrete disorders may emerge (Beauchaine, 2003).
ated with markers of low Conscientiousness (Nigg Second, psychopharmacological studies can also be
et al., 2002). used to generate evidence about whether personality
An understudied area where spectrum relationships traits and psychiatric disorders exist on a continuum
are likely to be observed is childhood and adolescent (e.g., Ekselius & von Knorring, 1999). For example, the
personality disorders. Extensive research has linked di- antidepressant paroxetine has been shown to reduce neg-
mensional personality traits with categorical personal- ative affect levels in persons without a history of mental
ity disorders in adults: Some researchers have argued disorder (Knutson et al., 1998): Some treatments thought
that adult personality disorders should be conceptual- to be targeted at specific syndrome disorders may exert
ized as complex combinations of adaptive and maladap- their influence via broader personality variables.
tive personality traits rather than as categorically Third, methods in behavioral genetics research can be
distinct conditions (Costa & Widiger, 2002; Widiger used to address the question of whether the heritability
et al., 1999; Widiger & Simonsen, 2005). Little is of a disorder (e.g., ADHD) is the same or different from
known, however, about how child and adolescent person- that of individual differences in a trait (e.g., continu-
ality is related both to concurrent personality disorders ously distributed symptoms of hyperactivity and inat-
and to later-appearing personality disorders in adult- tention). For example, with regard to ADHD, genetic
hood, although some recent theoretical work has elabo- analyses suggest that DSM-III-R ADHD may be best
rated on potential links (Cohen & Crawford, in press; viewed as an extreme end of a dimension that varies ge-
Geiger & Crick, 2001; Kernberg, Weiner, & Barden- netically in the population (Levy, Hay, McStephen,
stein, 2000). Childrens early personalities are likely to Wood, & Waldman, 1997).
be important predictors of the processes through which
personality functioning goes awry and becomes set into Resilience Association: Personality May Avert
maladaptive, rigid patterns (Mervielde, De Clerq, De the Development of Psychopathology in the
Fruyt, & van Leeuwen, 2005; Shiner, 2005). Face of Stress
Although we have presented vulnerability and spec- Although some personality traits may put children at
trum associations as if they are distinct, the line be- risk of psychopathology in adverse environments, other
tween the two is blurry. Most of the associations that traits may promote resilience in the face of adversity:
have been found between personality and psychopathol- Some personality traits may be protective factors under
ogy could be explained equally well by either the vulner- conditions that, on average, put children at risk for psy-
ability or the spectrum model. Some disorders may be chiatric disorders (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000;
better described as discrete entities, whereas others may Masten, 2001). For example, Werner and Smith (1992)
be better described as dimensional conditions. Further, studied a group of high-risk children who were exposed
some aspects of the same disorder may be discrete, to perinatal stress, poverty, and multiple family prob-
whereas other aspects may be dimensional (Pickles & lems. Children who showed positive, resilient adult out-
Angold, 2003). comes were described in infancy as very active: Males
Several research strategies can be harnessed to study were also described as easygoing, and females were also
the distinction between discrete disorders and spectrum described as affectionate.
associations. First, taxometric methods can be used to It seems obvious that attention must be given to the
distinguish whether differences between groups of indi- possibility that some personality factors protect against
viduals (e.g., depressed versus not depressed persons) psychopathology, whereas others predispose to it.
represent quantitative differences of degree or qualita- However, to the extent that protective and risk factors
tive differences in kind (Cole, 2004). Taxometric meth- operate dimensionallyat opposite ends of a contin-
ods are useful in addressing the issues that we raised in uumthere is little to be gained, either in theory or in
an earlier section (i.e., identifying subtypes in disor- practice, from focusing on the beneficial effects of, for
Conclusion 349

example, low Neuroticism more than on the harmful ef- sonality traits may increase the risk of recurrence of
fects of high Neuroticism. But there is a great deal of psychiatric conditions (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). Al-
value in testing two hypotheses: (1) that some person- though these types of associations have been studied in
ality factors provide protection in the presence of risk, adults (Widiger et al., 1999), they have received very
even though they have no effect in the absence of such little attention in research with children, despite their
risk and (2) that higher levels of a trait are necessary potential developmental significance.
for protection under adverse conditions than are neces-
sary for competent functioning in low-risk conditions. Scarring Association: Psychopathology May Alter
Theoretically, the processes through which individual Personality Functioning
differences in childrens temperament and personality The experience of significant psychopathology has the
traits promote resilience should be the same as those potential to alter childrens personalities in lasting
six processes described in reference to personality as a ways. Such a relationship is often referred to as a scar-
vulnerability factor. ring effect of psychopathology on personality (e.g.,
Much remains to be learned about the potential pro- Rohde, Lewinsohn, & Seeley, 1990). Personality
tective role of individual differences in childrens per- changes that are secondary to physical disorders are well
sonality traits. Researchers have called for increasing documented, but, at least in adulthood, there is little
focus on the processes underlying resilience (Luthar solid evidence of lasting changes to personality second-
et al., 2000; Masten, 1999), and personality research ary to psychopathology. For example, the possibility of
should be an important part of future work in this area. scar effects has been explored in several studies of adult
Behavior genetic research should also be used to address depression; the evidence thus far indicates that, al-
genetic and environmental sources of resilience. A re- though depression may have negative long-term effects
cent study demonstrated that, for example, childrens on other aspects of functioning, it does not appear to re-
outgoing temperament promoted resilience in the face of sult in personality change (Ormel, Oldehinkel, & Volle-
socioeconomic adversity (Kim-Cohen, Moffitt, Caspi, bergh, 2004; Zuroff, Mongrain, & Santor, 2004). The
& Taylor, 2004): This link between resilience and socia- situation may be very different earlier in life. Because
ble temperament was largely accounted for by genetic identity and a sense of self are under construction
factors but included environmental processes as well. throughout childhood and adolescence, children may be
particularly vulnerable to the negative effects of psycho-
Maintenance Association: Personality May
pathology that emerges early in the life course. This hy-
Influence the Course and Prognosis of a Disorder
pothesis has received practically no research attention
Most risk-factor research on personality has focused on and warrants further exploration.
the role of individual differences in causing (or averting)
the onset of disorder. In addition, individual differences
in personality may influence the manifestation, course, CONCLUSION
and prognosis of a disorder once it has started. In some
cases, such traits may have played a role in the onset of Throughout this chapter, we have summarized definitive
the disorder; in other cases, these traits may not be etio- findings, identified promising research leads and hy-
logically related to the disorder. Studies on depression potheses, and underscored existing methodological limi-
have examined the potential role of personality in main- tations. Our concluding comments are thus devoted to
taining a depressive episode. For example, in one study sketching the requirements for improved research.
of depressed adults, those with lower levels of the BAS Longitudinal research is the lifeblood of developmen-
after the onset of the depressive episode had worse out- tal psychology, but simply tracking people over time is
comes than those with more initially positive BAS func- not good enough. There is room for improvement on three
tioning (Kasch, Rottenberg, Arnow, & Gotlib, 2002). fronts. First, longitudinal research on personality can
Some personality traits appear to predict response to be improved through better trait measurement. The avail-
treatment. For example, research on tobacco dependence ability of a taxonomy of measurable individual differ-
shows that persons with high Neuroticism are more ences in temperament and personality is an indispensable
likely to relapse following participation in smoking ces- aid to developmental research. However, few off-the-shelf
sation treatments (Kenford et al., 2002). Still other per- measures assess the full range of higher- and lower-order
350 Personality Development

traits described in this chapter: The development of reli- pants are successively studied alongside their signifi-
able, valid, and comprehensive measures of child and cant others at different points in the life course. These
adolescent personality remains an important task. Until types of studies will include both global ratings of indi-
these become available, researchers need to consider sev- vidual differences and minute-to-minute assessments
eral issues when selecting methods and instruments for of social interactions to document how behavior
measuring temperament and personality: (a) Ideally, patterns are evoked and sustained. Second, just as re-
more than one method should be used to provide a more search in social cognition inspired deeper understand-
valid assessment of a particular trait; ( b) more than one ing of personality dynamics in the latter part of the
trait should be measured, even in studies focused on sin- twentieth century, the fusion of differential psychology
gle traits in isolation, because this provides critical infor- and neuroscience will lead the way to a fuller under-
mation about etiological specificity and discriminant standing of how personality traits are linked to pro-
validity; (c) researchers need to consider carefully cessing emotional stimuli.
whether a measure truly taps the trait of interest because Finally, research into personality development will
labels for measures (i.e., scale names on questionnaires) need to embrace genetics. Questions about the extent to
are often misleading; and (d) researchers should consider which genetic factors influence individual differences
not just what is included in a particular instrument, but in personality are increasingly less interesting, if only
also what is left out, because many measures do not tap because it is by now so well established that genetic fac-
the full range of individual differences observed in chil- tors do have a large influence. But this does not mean
dren. Second, longitudinal research can be improved by that behavioral genetics research has served its purpose
relying on theoretically informed data-collection sched- and worn out its welcome. To the contrary, discoveries
ules. Rather than dictated by convenience, longitudinal about the human genome open up new research possibil-
studies should make an effort to organize data collection ities in which measured genotypes will be used to study
around well-defined developmental tasks, whether these the origins of personality differences and the links be-
tasks are defined by evolutionary imperatives, sociologi- tween personality and psychopathology. To ignore ge-
cal realities, or maturational changes. The organization netics is not only irresponsible but also a missed
of data collection across periods of environmental- opportunity.
maturational changes offers an opportunity to test hy- These concluding observations are intended to stimu-
potheses about how individuals select and shape their en- late new research into personality development and also
vironments (processes of social selection) and how to promote discussion about the kind of multidiscipli-
environments influence individuals (processes of social nary (re)training that is increasingly required of new
causation). Third, longitudinal research can be improved students (and seasoned researchers), spanning psycho-
by integrating epidemiological concepts and methods into metric theory, epidemiology, neuroscience, and genet-
studies of personality development to test hypotheses ics. It is a daunting and exciting task.
about risk factors and causal processes.
Longitudinal research is not always developmental REFERENCES
research and, conversely, many developmental ques-
tions require different types of research designs. In Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). A longitudinal study of emotion ex-
particular, little is known about how early-emerging in- pression and personality relations in early development. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 566577.
dividual differences become elaborated into the consis-
Abelson, R. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is
tent ways of behaving, thinking, and feeling that we a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 129133.
call personality. Throughout this chapter, we listed Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personal-
some ideas and working hypotheses about these ity, and interests. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219245.
processes. These will need to be examined using tradi- Ahadi, S., & Diener, E. (1989). Multiple determinants and effect
sizes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 398406.
tional observational methods and, increasingly, the Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. M. (1993). Childrens tem-
tools of neuroscience as well. First, to the extent that perament in the U.S. and China: Similarities and differences. Eu-
the most important sources of influence on the ropean Journal of Personality, 7, 359377.
processes of developmental elaboration are to be found Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation.
New York: Holt.
in interpersonal settings, the ideal study of individual
Alwin, D. F., Cohen, R. L., & Newcomb, T. M. (1991). Political atti-
development ought to be conceived of as a study of so- tudes over the life-span: The Bennington women after 50 years.
cial relationships, one in which longitudinal partici- Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
References 351

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the major di-
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. mensions of personality. European Journal of Personality, 15, 353.
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & Paunonen, S. V. (2002). What is the central
attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attrac- feature of extraversion? Social attention versus reward sensitiv-
tiveness in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 245252.
chology, 81, 116132. Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di
Andrews, J. A., Foster, S. L., Capaldi, D. M., & Hops, H. (2000). Adoles- Blas, L., et al. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-de-
cent and family predictors of physical aggression, communication, scriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in
and satisfaction in young adult couples: A prospective analysis. seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 195208. 86, 356366.
Arcus, D. (2001). Inhibited and uninhibited children: Biology in the Avila, C. (2001). Distinguishing BIS-mediated and BAS-mediated
social context. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Tem- disinhibition mechanisms: A comparison of disinhibition models
perament in context (pp. 4360). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of Gray (1981, 1987), and of Patterson and Newman (1993). Jour-
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 311324.
from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, Banfield, J. F., Wyland, C. L., Macrae, C. N., Munte, T. F., &
55, 469480. Heatherton, T. F. (2004). The cognitive resolution of self-
Arseneault, L., Kim-Cohen, J., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of
(2005). Psychometric evaluation of 5- and 7-year-old childrens self regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 6283).
self-reports of conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- New York: Guilford Press.
chology, 33, 537550. Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., Rabasca, A., & Pastorelli, C. (2003).
Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Rijsdijk, F., Jaf- A questionnaire for measuring the big five in late childhood. Per-
fee, S., et al. (2003). Strong genetic effects on cross-situational sonality and Individual Dif ferences, 34, 645664.
antisocial behavior among 5-year-old children according to moth- Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New
ers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins self-reports. Jour- York: Guilford Press.
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 832848. Barlow, D. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disor-
Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral de- ders from the perspective of emotion theory. American Psycholo-
velopment: Integrating social information processing and moral gist, 55, 12471263.
domain models. Child Development, 75, 9871002. Barr, R. G., & Gunnar, M. (2000). Colic: The transient responsiv-
Asendorpf, J. B. (2002). Personality effects on personal relationships ity hypothesis. In B. Hopkins & J. A. Green (Eds.), Crying as a
over the life span. In A. L. Vangelisti, H. T. Reis, & M. A. Fitz- sign, a symptom, and a signal: Clinical, emotional and develop-
patrick (Eds.), Stability and change in relationships (pp. 3656). mental aspects of infant and toddler crying (pp. 4166). New
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. York: Cambridge University Press.
Asendorpf, J. B. (2003). Head-to-head comparison of the predictive Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and
validity of personality types and dimensions. European Journal of performance at the beginning of the new millennium: What do we
Personality, 17, 327346. know and where do we go next? International Journal of Selection
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mucke, D. (2002). Double dissocia- and Assessment, 9, 930.
tion between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Conscientious-
case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ness and performance of sales representatives: Tests of mediating
ogy, 83, 380393. effects of goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 715722.
Asendorpf, J. B., Borkenau, P., Ostendorf, F., & van Aken, M. A. G. Bates, J. E. (1989). Concepts and measures of temperament. In G. A.
(2001). Carving personality description at its joints: Confirma- Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament
tion of three replicable personality prototypes for both children in childhood (pp. 326). New York: Wiley.
and adults. European Journal of Personality, 15, 169198. Bates, J. E., Freeland, C. A. B., & Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Measure-
Asendorpf, J. B., Caspi, A., & Hofstee, W. K. B. (2002). The puzzle ment of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, 715722.
of personality types. European Journal of Personality, 16, S1S5. Bates, J. E., & McFadyen-Ketchum, S. (2000). Temperament and par-
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1991). Correlates of the ent-child relations as interacting factors in childrens behavioral
temporal consistency of personality patterns in childhood. Jour- adjustment. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament
nal of Personality, 59, 689703. and personality development across the life span (pp. 141176).
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1999). Resilient, overcon- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
trolled, and undercontrolled personality prototypes in childhood: Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Ridge, B. (1998). Inter-
Replicability, predictive power, and the trait-type issue. Journal action of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive par-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 815832. enting in the development of externalizing behavior.
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003a). Personality-rela- Developmental Psychology, 34, 982995.
tionship transaction in adolescence: Core versus surface person- Beauchaine, T. P. (2003). Taxometrics and developmental psycho-
ality characteristics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629666. pathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 501527.
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003b). Validity of big five Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in studies using ex-
personality judgments in childhood: A 9-year longitudinal study. perimental or brief longitudinal approaches to socialization. De-
European Journal of Personality, 17, 117. velopmental Psychology, 22, 595603.
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, U. (1999). A neuropsychological Belsky, J., & Barends, N. (2002). Personality and parenting. In M.
theory on positive affect and its inf luence on cognition. Psycho- Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (pp. 415438). Mahwah,
logical Review, 106, 529550. NJ: Erlbaum.
352 Personality Development

Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Woodsworth, S. (1995). Personality and par- Bouchard, T. J., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and per-
enting: Exploring the mediating role of transient mood and daily sonality. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243274.
hassles. Journal of Personality, 63, 905929. Branje, S. J. T., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2004).
Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1996). Infant positive and negative Relations between big five personality characteristics and per-
emotionality: One dimension or two? Developmental Psychology, ceived support in adolescent families. Journal of Personality and
32, 289298. Social Psychology, 86, 615628.
Benjamin, J., Ebstein, R. P., & Belmaker, H. (Eds.). (2002). Molecu- Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Turgeon, L., Poulin, F., & Wanner, B.
lar genetics and the human personality. Washington, DC: Ameri- (2004). Is there a dark side of positive illusions? Overestimation
can Psychiatric Association. of social competence and subsequent adjustment in aggressive and
Bergman, L. R., & Cairns, R. B. (Eds.). (2000). Developmental sci- nonaggressive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
ence and the holistic approach. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 32, 305320.
Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Structural rela-
From social integration to health. Social Science and Medicine, tions among dimensions of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disor-
51, 843857. ders and dimensions of negative affect, positive affect, and
autonomic arousal. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 179192.
Biesanz, J. C., West, S. G., & Kwok, O.-M. (2003). Personality
over time: Methodological approaches to the study of short-term Buckley, M. E., Klein, D. N., Durbin, E., Hayden, E. P., & Moerk,
and long-term development and change. Journal of Personality, K. C. (2002). Development and validation of a q-sort procedure to
71, 905941. assess temperament and behavior in preschool-age children. Jour-
nal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 525539.
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Childrens interpersonal behav-
iors and the teacher-child relationship. Developmental Psychol- Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of algorithms of social life: A do-
ogy, 34, 934946. main-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 187219.
Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents and Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cogni-
teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibi- tions in the context of the family. Annual Review of Psychology,
tion? Child Development, 74, 18991917. 51, 315344.
Blair, C. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and behavioral activation in Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory on personal-
young children: Relations with self-regulation and adaptation to ity development. New York: Wiley.
preschool children attending Head Start. Developmental Psychobi- Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing
ology, 42, 301311. personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft. Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation, and manipulation. Journal
Block, J. (1996). Some jangly remarks on Baumeister and Heather- of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 12141221.
ton. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 2832. Buss, D. M. (1992). Manipulation in close relationships. Journal of
Personality, 60, 477499.
Bogg, T., & Roberts, B. W. (2004). Conscientiousness and health be-
haviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
mortality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 887919. Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in
Bolger, N., & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expres-
everyday life: The role of Neuroticism in exposure and reactivity sion. Child Development, 69, 359374.
to daily stressors. Journal of Personality, 59, 355386. Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of
Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying per- youth in our time. New York: Cambridge University Press.
sonality in the stress process. Journal of Personality and Social Canli, T. (2004). Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neu-
Psychology, 69, 890902. roticism: Learning from individual processes in emotion process-
Boomsma, D., Busjahn, A., & Peltonen, L. (2002). Classical twin ing. Journal of Personality, 72, 11051132.
studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics, 3, 872882. Capaldi, D. M., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Development and valida-
Borkenau, P., & Liebler, A. (1995). Observable attributes as manifes- tion of an early adolescent temperament measure. Journal of Early
tations and cues of personality and intelligence. Journal of Per- Adolescence, 12, 154173.
sonality, 63, 125. Cardon, L. R., & Palmer, L. J. (2003). Population stratification and
Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner, A. spurious allelic association. Lancet, 361, 598604.
(2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and intelli- Carey, G. (2003). Human genetics for the social sciences. Thousand
gence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 599614. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bornstein, M. H., Gaughran, J. M., & Homel, P. (1986). Infant tem- Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendoicrine perspectives on social attach-
perament: Theory, tradition, critique, and new assessments. In ments and love. Psychoneuroendicrinology, 23, 779818.
C. E. Izard & P. B. Read (Eds.), Measuring emotions in infants and Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral
children (pp. 172199). New York: Cambridge University Press. approach system. Emotion, 4, 322.
Bouchard, T. J. (1995). Longitudinal studies of personality and intel- Caspi, A. (1987). Personality in the life course. Journal of Personal-
ligence: A behavior genetic and evolutionary psychology perspec- ity and Social Psychology, 53, 12031213.
tive. In D. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In
handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 81106). New York: W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
Plenum Press. book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality
Bouchard, T. J. (2004). Genetic inf luence on human psychological development (5th ed., pp. 311388). New York: Wiley.
traits: A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., &
13, 148151. Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Childrens behavioral styles at age 3 are
References 353

linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal of Per- tionnaire assessment of the BAS and BIS. Journal of Abnormal
sonality, 71, 495513. Child Psychology, 32, 435451.
Caspi, A., & Herbener, E. S. (1990). Continuity and change: As- Cole, D. A. (2004). Taxometrics in psychpathology research: An in-
sortive marriage and the consistency of personality in adulthood. troduction to some of the procedures and related methodological
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 250258. issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 39.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Cole, M., & Cole, S. R. (1996). The development of children (3rd ed.).
et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal- New York: W. H. Freeman.
treated children. Science, 297, 851854. Colhoun, H. M., McKeigue, P. M., & Smith, G. D. (2003). Problems
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. (2005). Personality development: of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes. Lancet,
Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453484. 361, 865872.
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Har- Contrada, R. J., Cather, C., & OLeary, A. (1999). Personality and
rington, H., et al. (2003). Inf luence of life stress on depression: health: Dispositions and processes in disease susceptibility and
Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, adaptation to illness. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook
386389. of personality (2nd ed., pp. 576604). New York: Guilford Press.
Caspi, A., Wright, B. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Early Cook, K. W., Vance, C. A., & Spector, P. E. (2000). The relation of
failure in the job market: Childhood and adolescent predictors of candidate personality with selection-interview outcomes. Journal
unemployment in the transition to adulthood. American Sociologi- of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 867885.
cal Review, 63, 424451. Costa, P. T., Herbst, J. M., McCrae, R. R., Samuels, J., & Ozer, D. J.
Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Mehta, T. G., & Feeney, B. C. (2004). Feedback (2002). The replicability and utility of three personality types.
seeking in children and adolescents: Associations with self- European Journal of Personality, 16, S73S87.
perceptions, attachment representations, and depression. Child Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A 6-
Development, 74, 61216628. year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the
Cervone, D., & Mischel, W. (2002). Personality science. In D. Cer- NEO personality inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
vone & W. Mischel (Eds.), Advances in personality science chology, 54, 853863.
(pp. 126). New York: Guilford Press. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality In-
Chen, E., & Mathews, K. (2002). Socioeconomic differences in chil- ventory (NEO-PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-
drens health: How and why do these relationships change with FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
age? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 295329. Assessment Resources.
Chen, X., Liu, M., Rubin, K. H., Chen, G., Gao, X., & Li, D. (2002). Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1997). Longitudinal study of adult
Sociability and prosocial orientation as predictors of youth ad- personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Hand-
justment: A 7-year longitudinal study in a Chinese sample. Inter- book of personality psychology (pp. 269292). San Diego, CA:
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 128136. Academic Press.
Chorpita, B. F., Albano, A. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The struc- Costa, P. T., & Widiger, T. A. (2002). Personality disorders and the
ture of negative emotions in a clinical sample of children and ado- five factor model of personality (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Amer-
lescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 7485. ican Psychological Association.
Church, T. A. (Ed.). (2001). Culture and personality. Journal of Per- Cote, S., & Moskowitz, D. S. (1998). On the dynamic covariation be-
sonality, 69, 6. tween interpersonal behavior and affect: Prediction from neuroti-
Cillesson, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforce- cism, extraversion, and agreeableness. Journal of Personality and
ment: Developmental changes in the association between aggres- Social Psychology, 75, 10321046.
sion and social status. Child Development, 75, 157163. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of
Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers personal- social information processing mechanisms in childrens social ad-
ity and its interaction with child temperament as predictors of justment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101.
parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Crockenberg, S., & Leerkes, E. (2003). Infant negative emotionality,
79, 274285. caregiving, and family. In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Chil-
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartate model of anxiety and drens inf luence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family
depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. relationships (pp. 5778). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316336. Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (2003). Childrens inf luence on family
Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, per- dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah,
sonality and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 103, 103116. Cumberland-Li, A., Eisenberg, N., & Reiser, M. (2004). Relations of
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psy- young childrens agreeableness and resiliency to effortful control
chobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of and impulsivity. Social Development, 13, 193212.
General Psychiatry, 39, 12421247. Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, E., Nitschke, J. B., & Kalin, N. H. (2003).
Cohen, P., & Crawford, T. (in press). Development issues: Personal- Parsing the subcomponents of emotion and disorders of emotion:
ity disorder in children and adolescents. In J. M. Oldham, A. E. Perspectives from affective neuroscience. In R. J. Davidson, K. R.
Skodol, & D. Bender (Eds.), American Psychiatric Publishing Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of af fective sciences
Textbook of Personality Disorders. Washington, DC: American (pp. 824). New York: Oxford University Press.
Psychiatric Publishing. De Fruyt, F., Mervielde, I., Hoekstra, H. A., & Rolland, J. (2000).
Colder, C. R., & OConnor, R. M. (2004). Grays reinforcement sen- Assessing adolescents personality with the NEO PI-R. Assess-
sitivity model and child psychopathology: Laboratory and ques- ment, 7, 329345.
354 Personality Development

Denham, S., Mason, T., Caverly, S., Hackney, R., Caswell, C., Di- Eid, M., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Borkenau, P. (2003). Socia-
ener, E., et al. (2001). Preschoolers at play: Co-socializers of bility and positive emotionality: Genetic and environmental con-
emotional and social competence. International Journal of Behav- tributions to the covariation between different facets of
ioral Development, 25, 290301. extraversion. Journal of Personality, 71, 319346.
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2003). Information processing ap- Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development.
proaches to individual differences in emotional reactivity. In Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665697.
R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Hand- Eisenberg, N. (2001). The core and correlates of affective social com-
book of af fective sciences (pp. 681697). New York: Oxford Uni- petence. Social Development, 10, 120124.
versity Press.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W.
Derryberry, D., Reed, M. A., & Pilkenton-Taylor, C. (2003). Tem- Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
perament and coping: Advantages of an individual differences child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality devel-
perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 10491066. opment (5th ed., pp. 701778). New York: Wiley.
Diamond, L. M. (2004). Emerging perspectives on distinctions be- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dis-
tween romantic love and sexual desire. Current Directions in Psy- positional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting
chological Science, 13, 116119. quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five- Psychology, 78, 136157.
factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417440. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy,
Digman, J. M. (1994). Child personality and temperament: Does the R. M., et al. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to
five-factor model embrace both domains. In C. F. Halverson, prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality
G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing struc- and Social Psychology, 57, 5566.
ture of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1995). Relations of
(pp. 323338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
shyness and low sociability to regulation and emotionality. Jour-
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the big five. Journal of nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 505517.
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1256.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumber-
Digman, J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986). Further specification of the five land, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of
robust factors of personality. Journal of Personality and Social prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development,
Psychology, 50, 116123. 70, 13601372.
Digman, J. M., & Shmelyov, A. G. (1996). The structure of tempera- Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., Pasternack, J., Lennon, R., Beller, R., & Mathy,
ment and personality in Russian children. Journal of Personality R. M. (1987). Prosocial development during middle childhood: A
and Social Psychology, 71, 341351. longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 23, 712718.
Digman, J. M., & Takemoto-Chock, N. K. (1981). Factors in the natu-
Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004).
ral language of personality: Re-analysis, comparison, and inter-
Effortable control: Relations with emotional regulation, adjust-
pretation of six major studies. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
ment and socialization in childhood. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D.
16, 49170.
Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and
Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, applications (pp. 259282). New York: Guilford Press.
G. S. (1997). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland,
and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal
A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control
of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 3751.
and impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child De-
Donnellan, M. B., Conger, R. D., & Bryant, C. M. (2004). The big velopment, 75, 2546.
five and enduring marriages. Journal of Research in Personality,
Ekselius, L., & von Knorring, L. (1999). Changes in personality
38, 481504.
traits during treatment with sertaline or citalopram. British Jour-
Donzella, B., Gunnar, M. R., Krueger, W. K., & Alwin, J. (2000). nal of Psychiatry, 174, 444448.
Cortisol and vagal tone responses to competitive challenge in
Eley, T. C., Bolton, D., OConnor, T. G., Perrin, S., Smith, P., &
preschoolers: Associations with temperament. Developmental
Plomin, R. (2003). A twin study of anxiety-related behaviors in
Psychobiology, 37, 209220.
preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Dubas, J. S., Gerris, J. R. M., Janssens, J. M., & Vermulst, A. (2002).
44, 945960.
Personality types in adolescents: Concurrent correlates, an-
tecedents, and type X parenting interactions. Journal of Adoles- Emde, R. N., & Hewitt, J. (Eds.). (2001). Infancy to early childhood:
cence, 25, 7292. Genetic and environmental inf luences on developmental change.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Eaton, W. O. (1994). Temperament, development, and the five-factor
model: Lessons from activity level. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Entwistle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1993). Entry into school. An-
Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of nual Review of Sociology, 19, 401423.
temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood Erez, A., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluation
(pp. 173187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. to goal setting, motivation, and performance. Journal of Applied
Egloff, B., & Schmuckle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Im- Psychology, 86, 12701279.
plicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personal- Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5 or 3? Criteria
ity and Social Psychology, 83, 14411455. for a taxonomic paradigm. Personality and Individual Dif ferences,
Ehrensaft, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2004). Clinically abusive 12, 773790.
relationships in an unselected birth cohort: Mens and womens Farsides, T., & Woodfield, R. (2003). Individual differences and un-
participation and developmental antecedents. Journal of Abnor- dergraduate success: The roles of personality, intelligence, and ap-
mal Psychology, 113, 258270. plication. Personality and Individual Dif ferences, 34, 12251243.
References 355

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta- Gleason, K. A., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Richardson, D. S. (2004).
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429456. Agreeableness as a predictor of aggression in adolescence. Ag-
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Automatic self-regulation. gressive Behavior, 30, 4361.
In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self- Goldberg, L. R. (2001). Analyses of Digmans child-personality data:
regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 151170). New Derivation of big five factor scores from each of six samples.
York: Guilford Press. Journal of Personality, 69, 709743.
Fleeson, W., Malanos, A. B., & Achille, N. M. (2002). An intrain- Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of
dividual process approach to the relationship between extraver- the toddler temperament behavior assessment questionnaire.
sion and positive affect: Is acting extraverted as good as Child Development, 67, 218235.
being extraverted? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas,
83, 14091422. A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament?
Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). The scientific credibility of common-sense Child Development, 67, 218235.
psychology. In K. H. Craik, R. Hogan, & R. N. Wolfe (Eds.), Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, K. A., & Lemery, K. S. (1997). Toddler and
Fifty years of personality psychology (pp. 251268). New York: childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evi-
Plenum Press. dence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Develop-
Foley, D. L., Eaves, L. J., Wormley, B., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H., mental Psychology, 33, 891905.
Kuhn, J., et al. (2004). Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, A., & Campos, J. J. (1999).
A genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Archives of General Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Develop-
Psychiatry, 61, 738744. mental Psychology, 35, 972985.
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, Goldsmith, H. H., Losoya, S. H., Bradshaw, D. L., & Campos, J. J.
L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibi- (1994). Genetics of personality: A twin study of the five-factor
tion and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral inf lu- model and parent-offspring analyses. In C. F. Halverson, G. A.
ences across the first 4 years of life. Child Development, 72, 121. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of
Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions. American temperament and personality from infancy to childhood
Scientist, 91, 330335. (pp. 241266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., & Dane, H. E. Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Contemporary instru-
(2003). Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the ments for assessing early temperament by questionnaire and in
prediction of conduct problem severity. Journal of Abnormal the laboratory. In J. Strelau & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Explorations
Child Psychology, 31, 457470. in temperament: International perspectives on theory and mea-
Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & surement (pp. 249272). New York: Plenum Press.
Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmen- Gomez, R., Gomez, A., & Cooper, A. (2002). Neuroticism and extra-
tal problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity. Devel- version as predictors of negative and positive information pro-
opmental Psychology, 39, 246260. cessing: Comparing Eysencks, Grays, and Newmans theories.
Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Schwartz, J. E., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., European Journal of Personality, 16, 333350.
Martin, L. R., Wingard, D. L., et al. (1995). Psychosocial and be- Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (Eds.). (2002). Children of depressed
havioral predictors of longevity. American Psychologist, 50, 6978. parents. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Funder, D. C. (1991). Global straits: A neo-Allportian approach. Psy- Gosling, S. D. (2001). From mice to men: What can we learn about
chological Science, 2, 3139. personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin, 127,
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation 4586.
and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in non-
Psychology, 78, 11351149. human animals: A cross-species review. Current Directions in
Gallagher, K. C. (2002). Does child temperament moderate the influence Psychological Science, 8, 6975.
of parenting on adjustment? Developmental Review, 22, 623643. Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A
Gallo, L. C., & Mathews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the associa- room with a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bed-
tion between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do rooms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379398.
negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129, Gosling, S. D., Kwan, V. S. Y., & John, O. P. (2003). A dogs got per-
1051. sonality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality
Garstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant tempera- judgment in dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social
ment via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav- Psychology, 85, 11611169.
ior and Development, 26, 6486. Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept
Geiger, T. C., & Crick, N. R. (2001). A developmental psychopathol- in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Jour-
ogy perspective on vulnerability to personality disorders. In R. E. nal of Psychiatry, 160, 636645.
Ingram & J. M. Price (Eds.), Vulnerability to psychpathology Gottfredson, G. D., Jones, E. M., & Holland, J. L. (1993). Personality
(pp. 57102). New York: Guilford Press. and vocational interests: The relation of Hollands six interest di-
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and mensions to five robust dimensions of personality. Journal of
conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interac- Counseling Psychology, 40, 518524.
tional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 513524. Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between
Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework for understanding marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in non- Gottman, J. M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predict-
verbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ing marital happiness and stability from newlywed interactions.
66, 398412. Journal of Marriage and Family, 60, 522.
356 Personality Development

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). New Hart, D., Atkins, R., & Fegley, S. (2003). Personality and develop-
York: McGraw-Hill. ment in childhood: A person-centered approach. Monographs of
Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cog- the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 1109.
nition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269288. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1997). Psychopathy: Assessment and as-
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (1996). The neuropsychology of anxi- sociation with criminal conduct. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, &
ety: Reprise. In D. A. Hope (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Moti- J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 2235).
vation: Vol. 43. Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fear New York: Wiley.
(pp. 61136). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1999). Friendships and adaptation
Graziano, W. G. (1994). The development of agreeableness as a di- across the life span. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
mension of personality. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & 8, 7679.
R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and Hartup, W. W., & van Lieshout, C. F. M. (1995). Personality develop-
personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 339354). Hillsdale, ment in social context. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 655687.
NJ: Erlbaum. Haselager, G. J. T., Cillesson, A. H. N., van Lieshout, C. F. M., Rik-
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Agreeableness: A dimen- sen-Walraven, J. M. A., & Hartup, W. W. (2002). Heterogeneity
sion of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), among peer-rejected boys across middle childhood: Developmen-
Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795824). San Diego, tal pathways of social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38,
CA: Academic Press. 446456.
Graziano, W. G., Hair, E. C., & Finch, J. F. (1997). Competitiveness Havill, V. L., Besevegis, E., & Mouroussaski, S. (1998). Agreeable-
mediates the link between personality and group performance. ness as a diachronic human trait. In C. S. Kohnstamm, C. F.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 13941408. Halverson, I. Mervielde, & V. L. Havill (Eds.), Parental descrip-
Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Hair, E. C. (1996). tions of child personality: Developmental antecendents of the big
Perceiving interpersonal conf lict and reacting to it: The case five (pp. 4964). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hawley, P. H. (2003). Prosocial and coercive configurations of re-
70, 820835. source control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapted
Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Finch, J. F. (1997). The Machiavellian. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 279309.
self as mediator between personality and adjustment. Journal of Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in child-
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 392404. hood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 84108.
Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Sullivan-Logan, G. Heller, D., Watson, D., & Ilies, R. (2004). The role of person versus
(1998). Temperament, activity, and expectations for later person- situation in life satisfaction: A critical examination. Psychologi-
ality development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, cal Bulletin, 130, 574600.
74, 12661277. Helson, R., Kwan, V. S. Y., John, O. P., & Jones, C. (2002). The grow-
Graziano, W. G., & Ward, D. (1992). Probing the big five in adoles- ing evidence of personality change in adulthood: Findings from
cence: Personality and adjustment during a developmental transi- research with personality inventories. Journal of Research in Per-
tion. Journal of Personality, 60, 425439. sonality, 36, 287306.
Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion and emotion regulation. In L. A. Pervin Helson, R., Mitchell, V., & Moane, G. (1984). Personality and pat-
& O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and re- terns of adherence and nonadherence to the social clock. Journal
search (2nd ed., pp. 525552). New York: Guilford Press. of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 10791096.
Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K., & Ketelaar, T. (1998). Relations between af- Hemenover, S. H. (2003). Individual differences in rate of affect
fect and personality: Support for the affect-level and affect-reac- change: Studies in affective chronometry. Journal of Personality
tivity views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, and Social Psychology, 85, 121131.
279288. Hogan, J., & Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality
Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P. H., & Thomas, C. W. and job-performance relations: A socioanalytic perspective.
(2003). Temperament: Infancy through adolescence. New York: Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 100112.
Kluwer Press. Hogan, J., & Ones, D. S. (1997). Conscientiousness and integrity at
Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (2003). Early temperament and attach- work. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of per-
ment as predictors of the five factor model of personality. Attach- sonality psychology (pp. 849870). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
ment and Human Development, 5, 218. Hogan, J., & Roberts, B. W. (2004). A socioanalytic model of matu-
Haigler, E. D., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Experimental manipulation of rity. Journal of Career Assessment, 12, 207217.
NEO-PI-R items. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 339358. Hogan, R., & Nicholson, R. A. (1988). The meaning of personality
Halverson, C. F., Havill, V. L., Deal, J., Baker, S. R., Victor, J. B., test scores. American Psychologist, 43, 621626.
Pavlopoulos, V., et al. (2003). Personality structure as derived from Hughes, J., Cavell, T., & Grossman, P. (1997). A positive view of
parental ratings of free descriptions of children: The inventory of self: Risk or protection for aggressive children? Development and
child individual differences. Journal of Personality, 71, 9951026. Psychopathology, 9, 7594.
Hampson, S. E., Andrews, J. A., Barckley, M., Lichtenstein, E., & Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job perfor-
Lee, M. E. (2000). Conscientiousness, perceived risk, and risk- mance: The big five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology,
reduction behaviors: A preliminary study. Health Psychology, 85, 869879.
19, 247252. Hyde, J. S., Else-Quest, N. M., Goldsmith, H. H., & Biesanz, J. C.
Harkness, A. R., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). Individual differences (2004). Childrens temperament and behavior problems predict
science for treatment planning: Personality traits. Psychological their employed mothers work functioning. Child Development,
Assessment, 9, 349360. 75, 580594.
References 357

Jacobi, C., Hayward, C., de Zwaan, M., Kraemer, H. C., & Agras, Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Arcus, D. (1998). Childhood deriva-
W. S. (2004). Coming to terms with risk factors for eating disor- tives of high and low reactivity in infancy. Child Development,
ders: Application of risk terminology and suggestions for a gen- 69, 14831493.
eral taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 1965. Kagan, J., Snidman, N., McManis, M., Woodward, S., & Hardway, C.
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, (2002). One measure, one meaning: Multiple measures, clearer
W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural meaning. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 463475.
twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal course of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 15561565. marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method and re-
Jenkins, J. M., Rasbash, J., & OConnor, T. G. (2003). The role of the search. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 334.
shared family context in differential parenting. Developmental Kasch, K. L., Rottenburg, J., Arnow, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2002).
Psychology, 39, 99113. Behavior activation and inhibition systems and the severity of de-
Jensen, P. S., Mrazek, D., Knapp, P. K., Steinberg, L., Pfeffer, C., pression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 589597.
Schowalter, J., et al. (1997). Evolution and revolution in child psy- Kaufman, J., Yang, B.-Z., Douglas-Palumberi, H., Houshyar, S., Lip-
chiatry: ADHD as a disorder of adaptation. Journal of the Ameri- schitz, D., Krystal, J. H., et al. (2004). Social supports and sero-
can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 16721679. tonin transporter gene moderate depression in maltreated
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Gleason, K. A., Adams, R., & Malcolm, children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
K. T. (2003). Interpersonal conf lict, Agreeableness, and person- 101, 1731617321.
ality development. Journal of Personality, 71, 10591085.
Keel, P. K., Fichter, M., Quadf lieg, N., Bulik, C. M., Baxter, M. G.,
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Graziano, W. G. (2001). Agreeableness Thornton, L., et al. (2004). Application of a latent class analysis
as a moderator of interpersonal conf lict. Journal of Personality, to empirically define eating disorder phenotypes. Archives of Gen-
69, 323362. eral Psychiatry, 61, 192200.
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Graziano, W. G., & Hair, E. C. (1996). Per- Keltner, D. (1998). Facial expressions of emotion and personality. In
sonality and relationships as moderators of interpersonal conf lict C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult
in adolescence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 148164. development, and ageing (pp. 385401). San Diego, CA: Aca-
Jockin, V., McGue, M., & Lykken, D. T. (1996). Personality and di- demic Press.
vorce: A genetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Keltner, D., Ekman, P., Gonzaga, G. C., & Beer, J. (2003). Facial ex-
chology, 71, 288299.
pression of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H.
John, O. P., Caspi, A., Robins, R. W., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer- Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of af fective sciences (pp. 415432).
Loeber, M. (1994). The little five: Exploring the five-factor model New York: Oxford University Press.
of personality in adolescent boys. Child Development, 65, 160178.
Keltner, D., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1995). Facial
John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Gordon Allport: Father and
expressions of emotion and psychopathology in adolescent males.
critic of the five-factor model. In K. H. Craik, R. T. Hogan, &
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 644652.
R. N. Wolfe (Eds.), Fifty years of personality research
(pp. 215236). New York: Plenum Press. Kendler, K. S., Kuhn, J. W., Vittum, J., Prescott, C. A., & Riley, B.
(2005). The interaction of stressful life events and a serotonin
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: His-
transporter polymorphism in the prediction of episodes of major
tory, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin
depression: A replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
& O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and re-
529535.
search (2nd ed., pp. 102138). New York: Guilford Press.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Po- Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Sullivan, P., Corey, L. A., Gardner,
litical conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). A population-based twin study in
Bulletin, 129, 339375. women of smoking initiation and dependence. Psychological Med-
icine, 29, 299308.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are mea-
sures of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and general- Kenford, S. L., Smith, S. S., Wetter, D. W., Jorenby, D. E., Fiore,
ized self-efficacy indicators of a common core construct? Journal M. C., & Baker, T. B. (2002). Predicting relapse back to smoking:
of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 693710. Contrasting affective and physical models of dependence. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 216227.
Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999).
The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career Kernberg, P. F., Weiner, A. S., & Bardenstein, K. K. (2000). Personal-
success across the life-span. Personnel Psychology, 52, 621652. ity disorders in children and adolescents. New York: Basic Books.
Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to perfor- Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2004). Ge-
mance motivation: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied netic and environmental processes in young childrens resilience
Psychology, 87, 797807. and vulnerability to socioeconomic deprivation. Child Develop-
Kagan, J. (1969). The three faces of continuity in human develop- ment, 75, 651668.
ment. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and Kindlon, D., Mezzacappa, E., & Earls, F. (1995). Psychometric
research (pp. 9831002). Chicago: Rand McNally. properties of impulsivity measures: Temporal stability, validity
Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In W. Damon (Editor-in- and factor structure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: 36, 645661.
Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., Kling, K. C., Ryff, C. D., Love, G., & Essex, M. (2003). Exploring the
pp. 177235). New York: Wiley. inf luence of personality on depressive symptoms and self-esteem
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1999). Early childhood predictors of adult across a significant life transition. Journal of Personality and So-
anxiety disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 15361541. cial Psychology, 85, 922932.
358 Personality Development

Knutson, B., Wolkowitz, O. M., Cole, S. W., Chan, T., Morre, E. A., Kupfer, D. J., First, M. B., & Regier, D. A. (2002). A research agenda
Johnson, R. C., et al. (1998). Selective alteration of personality for DSM-V. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
and social behavior by serotonergic intervention. American Jour- Lamb, M. E., Chuang, S. S., Wessels, H., Broberg, A. G., & Hwang,
nal of Psychiatry, 155, 373379. C. P. (2002). Emergence and construct validation of the big five
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children factors in early childhood: A longitudinal analysis of their on-
with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel- togeny in Sweden. Child Development, 73, 15171524.
opmental Psychology, 33, 228240. Larsen, R. J., & Prizmic, Z. (2004). Affect regulation. In R. F.
Kochanska, G., Clark, L., & Goldman, M. (1997). Implications of Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation:
mothers personality for parenting and their young childrens de- Research, theory, and applications (pp. 4061). New York: Guil-
velopmental outcomes. Journal of Personality, 65, 389420. ford Press.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development Laurent, J., & Ettelson, R. (2001). An examination of the tripartate
of self-regulation in the first 4 years of life. Child Development, model of anxiety and depression and its application to youth.
72, 10911111. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 4, 209230.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998). Laursen, B., Pulkkinen, L., & Adams, R. (2002). The antecedents and
Individual differences in emotionality in infancy. Child Develop- correlates of agreeableness in adulthood. Developmental Psychol-
ment, 64, 375390. ogy, 38, 591603.
Kochanska, G., Friesenborg, A. E., Lange, L. A., & Martel, M. M. Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of
(2004). Parents personality and infants temperament as contrib- emotion processes and cognition in social information processing.
utors to their emerging relationship. Journal of Personality and Child Development, 71, 107118.
Social Psychology, 86, 744759. Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A.
Kochanska, G., Gross, J. N., Lin, M.-H., & Nichols, K. E. (2002). Guilt (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood tempera-
in young children: Development, determinants, and relations with ment. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189204.
a broader system of standards. Child Development, 73, 461482. Lengua, L. J. (2002). The contribution of emotionality and self-
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personal- regulation to the understanding of childrens responses to multi-
ity characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and ple risk. Child Development, 73, 144161.
consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 10871112. Lengua, L. J., & Long, A. C. (2002). The role of emotionality and
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory con- self-regulation in the appraisal-coping process: Tests of direct
trol as a contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to and moderating effects. Journal of Applied Developmental Psy-
early school age. Child Development, 68, 263277. chology, 23, 471493.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful con- Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model
trol in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and of emotion-specific inf luences on judgment and choice. Cognition
implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, and Emotion, 14, 473193.
36, 220232. Levy, F., Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or
Vandegeest, K. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and continuum? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, lescent Psychiatry, 36, 737744.
490507. Lewis, M. (2000). The emergence of human emotions. In M. Lewis &
Kohnstamm, G. A., Halverson, C. F., Mervielde, I., & Havill, V. L. J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed.,
(1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental pp. 265280). New York: Guilford Press.
antecedents of the big five? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lewis, M. (2001). Issues in the study of personality development.
Kokko, K., Bergman, L. R., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Child personal- Psychological Inquiry, 12, 6783.
ity characteristics and selection into long-term unemployment in Lilienfeld, S. O. (2003). Comorbidity between and within childhood
Finnish and Swedish longitudinal studies. International Journal of externalizing and internalizing disorders: Ref lections and direc-
Behavioral Development, 27, 134144. tions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 285291.
Kokko, K., & Pulkkinen, L. (2000). Aggression in childhood and long- Lin, E. K., Bugental, D. B., Turek, V., Martorell, G. A., & Olster,
term unemployment in adulthood: A cycle of maladaptation and D. H. (2002). Childrens vocal properties as mobilizers of stress-
some protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 36, 463472. related physiological responses in adults. Personality and Social
Korner, A. F., Hutchinson, C. A., Koperski, J., Kraemer, H. C., Psychology Bulletin, 28, 346357.
Berkowitz, R. L., & Agras, W. S. (1985). Relation between neonatal Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., & Vernon, P. A. (2003). Genetic basis of
and later activity and temperament. Child Development, 52, 8390. personality structure. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Hand-
Kraemer, H. C., Kazdin, A. E., Offord, D. R., Kessler, R. C., Jensen, book of psychology: Personality and social psychology
P. S., & Kupfer, D. J. (1997). Coming to terms with the terms of (pp. 5983). New York: Wiley.
risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 337343. Livson, N., & Peskin, H. (1980). Perspectives on adolescence from
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and anti- longitudinal research. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent
social behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality cor- psychology (pp. 4798). New York: Wiley.
relates, distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 397402. Loeber, R., & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of ag-
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Ianoco, W. G., gression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual
& McGue, M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance de- Review of Psychology, 48, 371410.
pendence, antisocial behavior and personality: Modeling the exter- Loevinger, J. (1957). Objective tests as instruments of psychological
nal spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 411424. theory. Psychological Reports, 3, 635694.
References 359

Lohmueller, K. E., Pearce, C. L., Pike, M., Lander, E. S., & veloping structure of temperament and personality from infancy to
Hirschhorn, J. N. (2003). Meta-analysis of genetic association adulthood (pp. 157172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
studies supports a contribution of common variants to suscepti- Masten, A. S. (1999). Resilience comes of age: Ref lections on the
bility to common disease. Nature Genetics, 33, 177182. past and outlook for the next generation of research. In M. D.
Lonigan, C. J., Phillips, B. M., & Hooe, E. S. (2003). Relations of Glantz & J. L. Johnson (Eds.), Resilience and development: Posi-
positive and negative affectivity to anxiety and depression in chil- tive life outcomes (pp. 281296). New York: Kluwer Press.
dren: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study. Journal Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in devel-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 464481. opment. American Psychologist, 56, 227238.
Losoya, S. H., Callor, S., Rowe, D. C., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1977). Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1995). Competence, resilience,
Origins of familial similarity in parenting: A study of twins and and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Develop-
adoptive siblings. Developmental Psychology, 33, 10121023. mental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation
Low, K. S. D., Yoon, M., Roberts, B. W., & Rounds, J. (2005). The (pp. 715752). New York: Wiley.
stability on interests from early adolescence to middle adulthood: Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of
A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bul- competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons
letin, 131, 713737. from research on successful children. American Psychologist,
Lucas, R. E., & Baird, B. M. (2004). Extraversion and emotional reac- 53, 205220.
tivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 473485. Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy, N.,
Lucas, R. E., & Fujita, F. (2000). Factors inf luencing the relation be- & Ramirez, M. L. (1999). Competence in the context of adversity:
tween extraversion and pleasant affect. Journal of Personality and Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late
Social Psychology, 79, 10391056. adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 143169.
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of re- Masten, A. S., Neeman, J., & Andenas, S. (1994). Life events and ad-
silience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. justment in adolescents: The significance of event independence,
Child Development, 71, 562. desirability, and chronicity. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
4, 7197.
Lykken, D. T., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phe-
nomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186189. Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adapta-
tion in the second year: The relationship between quality of at-
Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys conduct disorder:
tachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547556.
A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, 683697.
Matthews, G., Derryberry, D., & Siegle, G. J. (2000). Personality and
MacCoon, D. G., Wallace, J. F., & Newman, J. P. (2004). Self-
emotion: Cognitive science perspectives. In S. E. Hampson (Ed.),
regulation: Context-appropriate balanced attention. In R. F.
Advances in personality psychology (pp. 199237). Philadelphia:
Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Re-
Psychology Press.
search, theory, and applications (pp. 422444). New York: Guil-
Maziade, M., Caron, C., Cote, R., Merette, C., Bernier, H., Laplante,
ford Press.
B., et al. (1990). Psychiatric status of adolescents who had ex-
MacDonald, K. (1992). Warmth as a developmental construct: An treme temperaments at age 7. American Journal of Psychiatry,
evolutionary analysis. Child Development, 63, 753773. 147, 15311536.
MacDonald, K. (1995). Evolution, the five-factor model, and levels of McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person?
personality. Journal of Personality, 63, 525567. Journal of Personality, 63, 365376.
Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion McAdams, D. P., Anyidoho, N. A., Brown, C., Huang, Y. T., Kaplan,
and neuroticism as predictors of objective life events: A longi- B., & Machado, M. A. (2004). Traits and stories: Links between
tudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, dispositional and narrative features of personality. Journal of Per-
65, 525567. sonality, 72, 761784.
Markey, P. M., Markey, C. N., & Tinsley, B. J. (2004). Childrens be- McCartney, K., & Rosenthal, R. (2000). Effect size, practical im-
havioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality. portance, and social policy for children. Child Development,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 423432. 71, 173180.
Markey, P. M., Markey, C. N., Tinsley, B. J., & Ericksen, A. J. (2002). McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness.
A preliminary validation of preadolescents self-reports using the Psychological Bulletin, 120, 761784.
five-factor model of personality. Journal of Research in Personal- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. J. (1994). The stability of personality:
ity, 36, 173181. Observation and evaluations. Current Directions in Psychological
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the Science, 3, 173175.
structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hi- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Conceptions and correlates of
erarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, openness to experience. In J. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs
88, 139157. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research
Martin, M., Boomsma, D., & Machin, G. (1997). A twin-pronged at- (pp. 825847). San Diego, CA: Academic.
tack on complex traits. Nature Genetics, 17, 387392. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A five-factor theory of person-
Martin, R., Watson, D., & Wan, C. K. (2000). A three-factor model of ality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personal-
trait anger: Dimensions of affect, behavior, and cognition. Jour- ity: Theory and research (pp. 139153). New York: Guilford Press.
nal of Personality, 68, 869897. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the
Martin, R. P., Wisenbaker, J., & Huttunen, M. (1994). Review of fac- NEO five-factor inventory. Personality and Individual Dif fer-
tor analytic studies of temperament measures based on the ences, 36, 587596.
Thomas-Chess structural model: Implications for the big five. In McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., & Busch, C. M. (1986). Evaluating com-
C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The de- prehensiveness in personality systems: The California Q-set and
360 Personality Development

the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Metsapelto, R. L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Personality traits and
ogy, 54, 430446. parenting: Neuroticism, extraversion, and openness to experi-
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hrebick- ence as discriminative factors. European Journal of Personality,
ova, H., Avia, M. D., et al. (2000). Nature over nurture: Tempera- 17, 5978.
ment, personality, and life span development. Journal of Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L.,
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 173186. Kubiszyn, T. W., et al. (2001). Psychological testing and psycho-
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., Parker, W. D., Mills, logical assessment: A review of evidence and issues. American
C. J., De Fruyt, F., et al. (2002). Personality trait development Psychologist, 56, 128165.
from age 12 to age 18: Longitudinal, cross-sectional and cross- Miech, R., Essex, M. J., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2001). Self-regulation
cultural analyses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, as a mediator of the status-attainment process: Evidence from
83, 14561468. early childhood. Sociology of Education, 74, 102120.
McGue, M., Bacon, S., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Personality stability Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001).
and change in early childhood: A behavioral genetic analysis. De- Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality
velopmental Psychology, 32, 604613. dimensions: Can the five-factor model adequately represent psy-
McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Tucker, C. J. (2001). Free-time ac- chopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253276.
tivities in middle childhood: Links with adjustment in early ado- Miller, T. Q., Smith, T. W., Turner, C. W., Guijarro, M. L., & Hallet,
lescence. Child Development, 72, 17641778. A. J. (1996). A meta-analytic review of research on hostility and
Measelle, J., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowen, C. P. (1998). physical health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 322348.
Assessing young childrens views of their academic, social, and Mineka, S., Rafaeli, E., & Yovel, I. (2003). Cognitive biases in emo-
emotional lives: An evaluation of the self-perception scales tional disorders: Information processing and social-cognitive per-
of the Berkeley puppet interview. Child Development, 69, spectives. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith
15561576. (Eds.), Handbook of af fective sciences (pp. 9761009). New York:
Measelle, J., John, O. P., Ablow, J., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. Oxford University Press.
(2005). Can children provide coherent, stable, and valid self- Mischel, W. (1990). Personality dispositions revisited and revised. In
reports on the big five dimensions? Journal of Personality and So- L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality (pp. 111134). New
cial Psychology, 89, 90106. York: Guilford Press.
Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-effective
Sir Roland, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 806834. R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research,
Meehl, P. E. (1986). What social scientists dont understand. In D. W. theory, and applications (pp. 99129). New York: Guilford Press.
Fiske & R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Metatheory in social science Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2005). Strategy for investi-
(pp. 315338). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. gating interactions between measured genes and measured envi-
Meier, G. H., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Does quick to blame mean ronments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 473481.
quick to anger? The role of agreeableness in dissociating blame and Moffitt, T. E., Robins, R. W., & Caspi, A. (2001). A couples analysis
anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 856867. of partner abuse with implications for abuse prevention. Crimi-
Merikangas, K., & Risch, N. (2003). Will the genomics revolution rev- nology and Public Policy, 1, 401432.
olutionize psychiatry? American Journal of Psychiatry, 160,
Moon, H., Hollenbeck, J. R., Humphrey, S. E., & Maue, B. (2003).
625635.
The tripartite model of neuroticism and the suppression of de-
Mervielde, I., Buyst, V., & De Fruyt, F. (1995). The validity of the pression and anxiety within an escalation of commitment
big five as a model for teachers ratings of individual differences dilemma. Journal of Personality, 71, 345368.
among children aged 4 to 12 years. Personality and Individual
Morison, P., & Masten, A. S. (1991). Peer reputation in middle child-
Dif ferences, 18, 525534.
hood as a predictor of adaptation in adolescence: A 7-year follow-
Mervielde, I., De Clerq, B., De Fruyt, F., & van Leeuwen, K. (2005). up. Child Development, 62, 9911007.
Temperament, personality and developmental psychopathology as
Moskowitz, D. S., & Zuroff, D. C. (2004). Flux, pulse and spin: Dy-
childhood antecedents of personality disorders. Journal of Person-
namic additions to the personality lexicon. Journal of Personality
ality Disorders, 19, 171201.
and Social Psychology, 86, 880893.
Mervielde, I., & De Fruyt, F. (1999). Construction of the Hierarchi-
cal Personality Inventory for Children (HiPIC). In I. Mervielde, Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2003). Modeling intraindividual change
I. L. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psy- in personality traits: Findings from the Normative Aging Study.
chology in Europe: Proceedings of the eighth European conference Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 58B, 153165.
on personality psychology (pp. 107127). Tilburg, The Nether- Munafo, M. R., Clark, T. G., Moore, L. R., Payne, E., Walton, R., &
lands: Tilburg University Press. Flint, J. (2003). Genetic polymorphisms and personality in
Mervielde, I., & De Fruyt, F. (2002). Assessing childrens traits with healthy adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Molecular
the hierarchical personality inventory for children. In B. De Raad Psychiatry, 8, 471484.
& M. Perugini (Eds.), Big five assessment (pp. 129142). Ash- Muris, P., Schmidt, H., Merckelbach, H., & Schouten, E. (2001). The
land, OH: Hogrefe & Huber. structure of negative emotions in adolescents. Journal of Abnor-
Mervielde, I., De Fruyt, F., & Jarmuz, S. (1998). Linking openness mal Child Psychology, 29, 331337.
and intellect in childhood and adulthood. In C. S. Kohnstamm, Neitzel, C., & Stright, A. D. (2004). Parenting behaviors during child
C. F. Halverson, I. Mervielde, & V. L. Havill (Eds.), Parental de- problem solving: The roles of child temperament, mother educa-
scriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the tion and personality, and the problem-solving context. Interna-
big five? (pp. 105142). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 166179.
References 361

Nigg, J. T. (2000). Inhibition /disinhibition in developmental behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychol- 129138.
ogy and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, Pickles, A., & Angold, A. (2003). Natural categories or fundamental di-
126, 220246. mensions: On carving nature at the joints and the rearticulation of
Nigg, J. T., John, O. P., Blaskey, L. G., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Willcutt, psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 529551.
E. G., Hinshaw, S. P., et al. (2002). Big five dimensions and ADHD Pike, A., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2003). Do sibling and friend relation-
symptoms: Links between personality traits and clinical symp- ships share the same temperamental origins? Journal of Child
toms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 451469. Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 598611.
OBrien, B. S., & Frick, P. J. (1996). Reward dominance: Associa- Plomin, R., & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic
tions with anxiety, conduct problems, and psychopathy in chil- inf luences on environmental measures. Behavioral and Brain
dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 223240. Sciences, 14, 373386.
OBrien, T. B., & DeLongis, A. (1996). The interactional context of Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality.
problem-, emotion-, and relationship-focused coping: The role of In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality the-
the big five personality factors. Journal of Personality, 64, 775813. ory and research (pp. 251276). New York: Guilford Press.
OConnor, T. G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D. W., Rutter, M., & Plomin, R., Corley, R., Caspi, A., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. C.
Plomin, R. (1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late (1998). Adoption results for self-reported personality: Evidence
childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavioral problems of non-additive genetic effects? Journal of Personality and Social
in coercive parenting. Developmental Psychology, 34, 970981. Psychology, 75, 211218.
Ormel, J., Oldehinkel, A. J., & Vollebergh, W. (2004). Vulnerability Plomin, R., & Crabbe, J. (2000). DNA. Psychological Bulletin,
before, during, and after a major depressive episode: A three- 126, 806828.
wave population-based study. Archives of General Psychiatry, Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same
61, 990996. family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sci-
Ozer, D. J. (1986). Consistency in personality: A methodological ences, 10, 116.
framework. New York: Springer. Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Corley, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1997). Na-
Ozer, D. J., & Gjerde, P. F. (1989). Patterns of personality consis- ture, nurture, and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: A
tency and change from childhood through adolescence. Journal of parent-offspring study. Psychological Science, 8, 442447.
Personality, 57, 483507. Plomin, R., & McGuffin, P. (2003). Psychopathology in the postge-
Pardini, D. A., Lochman, J. E., & Frick, P. J. (2003). Callous/unemo- nomic era. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 205228.
tional traits and social-cognitive processes in adjucated youths. Pomerantz, E. M., & Rudolph, K. D. (2003). What ensues from emo-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- tional distress? Implications for competence estimation. Child
atry, 42, 364371. Development, 74, 329345.
Park, S. Y., Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1997). Infant emo- Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of
tionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: Exploring stability and self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.
lawful discontinuity in a male sample. Developmental Psychology,
Prinzie, P., Onghena, P., Hellinckx, W., Grietens, H., Ghesquiere, P.,
33, 218227.
& Colpin, H. (2004). Parent and child personality characteristics
Parke, R. D. (2004). Development in the family. Annual Review of as predictors of negative discipline and externalizing behavioral
Psychology, 55, 365399. problems in children. European Journal of Personality, 18, 73102.
Patterson, C., Kupersmidt, J., & Griesler, P. (1990). Childrens per- Prior, M., Smart, D., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2000). Does shy-
ceptions of self and of relationships with others as a function of inhibited temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in
sociometric status. Child Development, 61, 13351349. adolescence? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big five factors and facets lescent Psychiatry, 39, 461468.
and the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The structure
Psychology, 81, 524539. of temperament from infancy through adolescence. In A. Eliasz &
Peabody, D., & De Raad, B. (2002). The substantive nature of A. Angleneiter (Eds.), Advances in research on temperament
psycholexical personality factors: A comparison across languages. (pp. 165182). Miami, FL: Pabst Science.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 983997. Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tem-
Pederson, N. L., & Reynolds, C. A. (1998). Stability and change in perament and parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of par-
adult personality: Genetic and environmental components. Euro- enting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255277).
pean Journal of Personality, 12, 365386. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2001). Dominance in early adoles- Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (2002).
cent boys: Affiliative and aggressive dimensions and possible Stimulation seeking and intelligence: A prospective longitudinal
functions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 142163. study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 674.
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. (2002). Openness and Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., Mednick, S. A., & Farring-
extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Repli- ton, D. P. (1998). Fearlessness, stimulation-seeking, and large
cation and commentary. Personality and Individual Dif ferences, body size at age 3 years as early predispositions to childhood ag-
33, 11371147. gression at age 11 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55,
Pfeifer, M., Goldsmith, H. H., Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M. 745751.
(2002). Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited chil- Reimann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and envi-
dren. Child Development, 73, 14741485. ronmental inf luences on personality: A study of twins reared to-
Phillips, K., & Matheny, A. P. J. (1997). Evidence for genetic inf lu- gether using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of
ence on both cross-situation and situation-specific components of Personality, 65, 449476.
362 Personality Development

Reiss, H. T., Capobianco, A., & Tsai, F.-T. (2002). Finding the person psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
in personal relationships. Journal of Personality, 70, 813850. (5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.
Resing, W. C. M., Bleichrodt, N., & Dekker, P. H. (1999). Measuring Rothbart, M. K., Chew, K. H., & Garstein, M. A. (2001). Assessment
personality traits in the classroom. European Journal of Personal- of temperament in early development. In L. T. Singer & P. S. Ze-
ity, 13, 493509. skind (Eds.), Biobehavioral assessment of the infant
Reynolds, S. K., & Clark, L. A. (2001). Predicting dimensions of per- (pp. 190208). New York: Guilford Press.
sonality disorder from domains and facets of the five-factor Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (2002). Temperament in children.
model. Journal of Personality, 69, 199222. In C. von Hofsten & L. Backman (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of
Roberts, B. W., Bogg, T., Walton, K. E., Chernyshenko, O. S., & the millennium: Vol. 2. Social, developmental, and clinical per-
Stark, S. E. (2004). A lexical investigation of the lower-order spectives (pp. 1735). New York: Taylor & Francis.
structure of conscientiousness. Journal of Research in Personality, Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. L. (2000). Stability
38, 164178. of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to
Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2001). The kids are al- parent report at 7 years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.),
right: Growth and stability in personality development from ado- Temperament and personality across the life span (pp. 85119).
lescence to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 81, 582583. Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament
Roberts, B. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Work experiences and self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.),
and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Per- Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
sonality and Social Psychology, 84, 582593. (pp. 357370). New York: Guilford Press.
Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consis- Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L. K., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, M. I. (2003).
tency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Jour-
review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 2530. nal of Personality, 71, 11131143.
Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., & Viechtbauer, W. (in press). Patterns in Rothbart, M. K., & Mauro, J. A. (1990). Questionnaire approaches to
mean-level change in personality traits across the life-course: A the study of infant temperament. In J. W. Fagen & J. Colombo
meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. (Eds.), Individual dif ferences in infancy: Reliability, stability, and
Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2002). Its not just who prediction (pp. 411429). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
youre with, its who you are: Personality and relationship ex-
Rowe, D. E., & Plomin, R. (1977). Temperament in early childhood.
periences across multiple relationships. Journal of Personality,
Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 150156.
70, 925964.
Rozanski, A., Blumenthal, J. A., & Kaplan, J. (1999). Impact of psy-
Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-
chological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease
Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient, overcontrolled, and undercontrolled
and implications for therapy. Circulation, 99, 2217.
boys: Three replicable personality types. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 70, 157171. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Dwyer, K. M., & Hastings, P. D. (2003).
Predicting preschoolers externalizing behaviors from toddler
Robins, R. W., & Tracy, J. L. (2003). Setting an agenda for a person-
temperament, conf lict, and maternal negativity. Developmental
centered approach to personality development. Monographs of
Psychology, 39, 164176.
the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(Serial No.
272), 110122. Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and
Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., Schwartz, S. H., & Knafo, A. (2002). The big social-behavioral consequences of toddlers inhibited tempera-
five personality factors and personal values. Personality and So- ment and parenting behaviors. Child Development, 73, 483495.
cial Psychology Bulletin, 28, 789801. Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). Attentional
Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1990). Are people control and self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs
changed by the experience of having an episode of depression? A (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and appli-
further test of the scar hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- cations (pp. 283300). New York: Guilford Press.
ogy, 99, 264271. Rusico, J., & Rusico, A. M. (2004). Clarifying boundary issues in
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (1999). Childrens goals and strategies in psychopathology: The role of taxometrics in a comprehensive
response to conf licts within a friendship. Developmental Psychol- program of structural research. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy, 35, 6979. ogy, 22, 2438.
Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and rela- Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Using sex differences
tional aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental dif- in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: Unifying issues
ferences in concurrent and prospective relations. Developmental and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
Psychology, 40, 378387. try, 44, 10921115.
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Rutter, M., & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in rela-
Child Development, 52, 569578. tion to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament Psychology, 53, 463490.
and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Sanson, A., & Prior, M. (1999). Temperament and behavioral precur-
Social Psychology, 78, 122135. sors to oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. In
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). In- H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive
vestigation of temperament at 3 to 7 years: The childrens behav- behavior disorders (pp. 397417). New York: Kluwer Aca-
ior questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408. demic/ Plenum Press.
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon Saucier, G. (2003). An alternative multi-language structure for per-
(Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child sonality attributes. European Journal of Personality, 17, 179205.
References 363

Saucier, G., Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2000). Cross-lan- Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Roberts, J. M. (2003). Childhood per-
guage studies of lexical personality factors. In S. E. Hampson sonality foreshadows adult personality and life outcomes 2
(Ed.), Advances in personality psychology (pp. 136). Philadel- decades later. Journal of Personality, 71, 11451170.
phia: Taylor & Francis. Shiner, R. L., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2002). A developmental
Saucier, G., & Ostendorf, F. (1999). Hierarchical components of the perspective on personality in emerging adulthood: Childhood an-
big five personality factors: A cross-language replication. Journal tecedents and concurrent adaptation. Journal of Personality and
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 613627. Social Psychology, 83, 11651177.
Saucier, G., & Simonds, J. (in press). The structure of personality Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Hervas, A., Silberg, J. L., Rutter, M., &
and temperament. In D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Hand- Eaves, L. (1998). Genetic inf luences on childhood hyperactivity:
book of personality development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Contrast effects imply parental rating bias, not sibling inter-
Saudino, K. J., & Cherney, S. C. (2001). Parental ratings of tempera- action. Psychological Medicine, 4, 825834.
ment in twins. In R. N. Emde & J. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early Slotboom, A.-M., Havill, V. L., Pavlopoulos, V., & De Fruyt, F.
childhood: Genetic and environmental inf luences on developmental (1998). Developmental changes in personality descriptions of
change (pp. 7388). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. children: A cross-national comparison of parental descriptions of
Saudino, K. J., Pederson, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., McClearn, G. E., children. In G. A. Kohnstamm, C. F. Halverson, I. Mervielde, &
& Plomin, R. (1997). Can personality explain genetic inf lu- V. L. Havill (Eds.), Parental descriptions of child personality: De-
ences on life events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- velopmental antecedents of the big five? (pp. 127153). Mahwah,
ogy, 72, 196206. NJ: Erlbaum.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- Smith, T. W., & Gallo, L. (2001). Personality traits as risk factors for
ronments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Devel- physical illness. In A. Baum, T. Revenson, & J. Singer (Eds.),
opment, 54, 424435. Handbook of health psychology (pp. 139174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power of positive baum.
thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 2630. Smith, T. W., & Spiro, A. (2002). Personality, health, and aging: Pro-
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (2004). Per- legomenon for the next generation. Journal of Research in Person-
sonality and life satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Personality ality, 36, 363394.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 10621075. Smith, T. W., & Williams, P. G. (1992). Personality and health: Ad-
Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Self-regulatory vantages and limitations of the five-factor model. Journal of Per-
strength. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of sonality, 60, 395423.
self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 8498). Snyder, M., & Stukas, A. (1999). Interpersonal processes: The inter-
New York: Guilford Press. play of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activities in social
Schmidt, L. A., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 273303.
world of work: Occupational attainment and job performance. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (1998). Categorical models of childhood disor-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 162173. der: A conceptual and empirical analysis. Journal of Child Psy-
Schneider, B., Smith, D. B., Taylor, S., & Fleenor, J. (1998). Person- chology and Psychiatry, 39, 115133.
ality and organizations: A test of the homogeneity of personality
Spinath, F. M., & OConnor, T. G. (2003). A behavioral genetic study
hypothesis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 4.
of the overlap between personality and parenting. Journal of Per-
Scourfield, J., van den Bree, M., Martin, N., & McGuffin, P. (2004). sonality, 71, 785808.
Conduct problems in children and adolescents. Archives of Gen-
Sroufe, L. A., Bennet, C., Englund, M., Urban, S., & Shulman, S.
eral Psychiatry, 61, 489496.
(1993). The significance of gender boundaries in preadolescence:
Settersten, R. A. (2003). Age structuring and the rhythm of the life
Contemporary correlates and antecedents of boundary violation
course. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of
and maintenance. Child Development, 64, 455466.
the life course (pp. 81102). New York: Kluwer Academic.
Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., Levy, A. K., & Egeland, B. (1999). Im-
Shanahan, M. J., & Flaherty, B. P. (2001). Dynamic patterns of time
plications of attachment theory for developmental psychopathol-
use in adolescence. Child Development, 72, 385401.
ogy. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 113.
Shiner, R. L. (1998). How shall we speak of childrens personalities
in middle childhood? A preliminary taxonomy. Psychological Bul- Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., & Shulman, S. (1993). Individuals in
letin, 124, 308332. relationships: Development from infancy through adolescence. In
D. C. Funder, R. D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman
Shiner, R. L. (2000). Linking childhood personality with adapta-
(Eds.), Studying lives through time (pp. 315342). Washington,
tion: Evidence for continuity and change across time into late
DC: American Psychological Association.
adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
310325. Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N., & Pancake, V. (1983). Attachment and dependency
in developmental perspective. Child Development, 54, 16151627.
Shiner, R. L. (2005). A developmental perspective on personality dis-
orders: Lessons from research on normal personality development Strelau, J. (2001). The concept and status of trait in research on tem-
in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Personality Disorders, perament. European Journal of Personality, 15, 311325.
19, 202210. Sullivan, P. F., Eaves, L. J., Kendler, K. S., & Neale, M. C. (2001).
Shiner, R. L. (in press). Temperament and personality and childhood. Genetic case-control association studies in neuropsychiatry.
In D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of personality Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 10151024.
development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Suls, J., Martin, R., & David, D. P. (1998). Person-environment fit
Shiner, R. L., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in child- and its limits: Agreeableness, Neuroticism, and emotional reac-
hood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and conse- tivity to interpersonal conf lict. Personality and Social Psychology
quences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 232. Bulletin, 24, 8898.
364 Personality Development

Suls, J., & Mullen, B. (1982). From the cradle to the grave: Compar- Updegraff, J. A., Gable, S. L., & Taylor, S. E. (2004). What makes ex-
ison and self-evaluation across the life span. In J. Suls (Ed.), periences satisfying? The interaction of approach-avoidance mo-
Psychological perspectives on the self (pp. 97125). Hillsdale, tivations and emotions in well-being. Journal of Personality and
NJ: Erlbaum. Social Psychology, 86, 496504.
Szechtman, H., & Woody, E. (2004). Obsessive-compulsive disorder Urban, J., Carlson, E., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1991). Patterns
as a disturbance of security motivation. Psychological Bulletin, of individual adaptation across childhood. Development and
111, 111127. Psychopathology, 3, 445460.
Tackett, J. L., & Krueger, R. F. (2005). Interpreting personality as a van den Oord, E. J. C. G., & Sullivan, P. F. (2003). False discoveries
vulnerability for psychopathology: A developmental approach to and models for gene discovery. Trends in Genetics, 19, 537542.
the personality-psychopathology relationship. In B. L. Hankin & van Lieshout, C. F. M. (2000). Lifespan personality development:
J. R. Z. Abela (Eds.), Development of psychopathology: A vulnera- Self-organizing goal-oriented agents and developmental outcome.
bility-stress perspective (pp. 199214). Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 276288.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lews, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Haselager, G. J. T. (1993). The big five per-
R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to sonality factors in the Nijmegen California Child Q-Set (NCCQ).
stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-f light. Psycho- Nijmegen, The Netherlands: University of Nijmegen.
logical Review, 107, 411429.
van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Haselager, G. J. T. (1994). The big five per-
Teasdale, J. D., & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Af fect, cognition, and change: sonality factors in Q-sort descriptions of children and adoles-
Re-modeling depressive thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. cents. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structure of mood and personality and their rel- (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality
evance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In from infancy to adulthood (pp. 293318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disor- Victor, J. B. (1994). The five-factor model applied to individual dif-
ders (pp. 681706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ferences in school behavior. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm,
Tellegen, A. (1991). Personality traits: Issues of definition, evidence, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament
and assessment. In W. M. Grove & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Thinking and personality from infancy to childhood (pp. 355370). Hills-
clearly about psychology: Vol. 2. Personality and psychopathology dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 1035). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota. Viken, R. J., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (1994). A de-
Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1992). Exploring personality through velopmental genetic analysis of adult personality: Extraversion
test construction: Development of the Multi-Dimensional Person- and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age. Journal of Personality
ality Questionnaire (MPQ). Unpublished manuscript. and Social Psychology, 66, 730.
Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Vohs, K. D., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2004). Interpersonal functioning re-
Individual difference variables, affective differentiation, and the quires self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.),
structures of affect. Journal of Personality, 71, 669703. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based inter- (pp. 392407). New York: Guilford Press.
actionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied Psychol- Wachtel, P. L. (1973). Psychodynamics, behavior therapy, and the im-
ogy, 88, 500517. placable experimenter: An inquiry into the consistency of person-
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1968). Temperament and behav- ality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 324334.
ior disorders in children. New York: New York University Press. Wakefield, J. C. (1989). Levels of explanation in personality theory.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H., Hertzig, M., & Korn, S. (1963). Be- In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality: Recent trends and
havioral individuality in early childhood. New York: New York emerging directions (pp. 333346). New York: Springer.
University Press. Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.
Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Inspiration as a psychological con- Watson, D. (2001). Neuroticism. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes
struct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 871889. (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sci-
Tobin, R. M., Graziano, W. G., Vanman, E. J., & Tassinary, L. G. (2000). ences (pp. 1060910612). Oxford, England: Elsevier.
Personality, emotional experience, and efforts to control emotions. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: Gen-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 656669. eral and specific factors of emotional experience and their rela-
Tremblay, R. E. (2002). The origins of youth violence. In C. von Hof- tion to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441476.
sten & L. Backman (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millen- Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Extraversion and its positive emo-
nium: Vol. 2. Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives tional core. In J. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook
(pp. 7788). New York: Taylor & Francis. of personality psychology (pp. 767793). San Diego, CA: Aca-
Trierweiler, L. I., Eid, M., & Lischetzke, T. (2002). The structure of demic Press.
emotional expressivity: Each emotion counts. Journal of Personal- Watson, D., Clark, L., McIntyre, C. W., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Af-
ity and Social Psychology, 82, 10231040. fect, personality, and social activity. Journal of Personality and
Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., Lynam, D. R., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Border- Social Psychology, 63, 10111025.
line personality disorder from the perspective of general personal- Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss,
ity functioning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 193202. M. E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995). Testing a tripartate model:
Twenge, J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? Birth cohort change in anx- Pt. 2. Exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression
iety and neuroticism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- in student, adult, and patient samples. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
ogy, 79, 10071021. chology, 104, 1525.
References 365

Watson, D., Hubbard, B., & Wiese, D. (2000). General traits of per- book of personality: Theory and research (pp. 347366). New
sonality and affectivity as predictors of satisfaction in intimate York: Guilford Press.
relationships. Journal of Personality, 68, 413449. Wiggins, J. S. (1973). Personality and prediction: Principles of per-
Watson, D., Suls, J., & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation sonality assessment. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
to structural models of personality and affectivity. Journal of Per- Wilson, B. J. (2003). The role of attentional processes in childrens
sonality and Social Psychology, 83, 185197. prosocial behavior with peers: Attention shifting and emotion.
Weir, R. C., & Gjerde, P. F. (2002). Preschool personality prototypes: Development and Psychopathology, 15, 313329.
Internal coherence, cross-study replicability, and developmental Witt, L. A., Burke, L. A., Barrick, M. A., & Mount, M. K. (2002).
outcomes in adolescence. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- The interactive effects of conscientiousness and agreeableness on
letin, 28, 12291241. job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 164169.
Weiss, B., Susser, K., & Catron, T. (1998). Common and specific fea- Wright, B. R. E., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (2001). The
tures of childhood psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psy- effects of social ties on crime vary by criminal propensity: A life-
chology, 107, 118127. course model of interdependence. Criminology, 39, 321351.
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High Yeo, G. B., & Neal, A. (2004). A multilevel analysis of effort, prac-
risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Uni- tice, and performance: Effects of ability, conscientiousness, and
versity Press. goal orientation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 231247.
Westen, D., & Harnden-Fischer, J. (2001). Personality profiles in eat- Young, S. K., Fox, N. A., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1999). The relations be-
ing disorders: Rethinking the distinction between Axis I and Axis tween temperament and empathy in 2-year-olds. Developmental
II. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 547562. Psychology, 35, 11891197.
Widiger, T. A., & Clark, L. A. (2000). Toward DSM-V and the classifi- Zuroff, D. C. (1986). Was Gordon Allport a trait theorist? Journal of
cation of psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 946963. Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 9931000.
Widiger, T. A., & Simonsen, E. (2005). Alternative dimensional Zuroff, D. C., Mongrain, M., & Santor, D. A. (2004). Conceptualizing
models of personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, and measuring personality vulnerability to depression: Comment
19, 110130. on Coyne and Whiffen, 1995. Psychological Bulletin, 130,
Widiger, T. A., Verheul, R., & van den Brink, W. (1999). Personality 489511.
and psychopathology. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Hand-
CHAPTER 7

Socialization Processes
DAPHNE BLUNT BUGENTAL and JOAN E. GRUSEC

THEORETICAL APPROACHES AND BIOLOGICAL PROCESSES THAT CREATE THE


CHANGING EMPHASES 368 PLATFORM FOR SOCIALIZATION 378
Psychoanalytic Theory 368 Processes in Different Domains 379
Attachment Theory 368 Biological Preparation for Different Environments 391
Social Learning Theories 369 Integration and Segue 391
Parenting Attitudes and Styles 369 SOCIOCULTURAL APPROACHES 392
Cognitive Approaches to Socialization 370 Dimensions of Socialization 393
CURRENT DEVELOPMENTS: BIOLOGICAL AND Socialization Practices 398
CULTURAL PERSPECTIVES 371 Socialization and the Cultural Context 403
Biological Perspectives 371 OUTCOMES OF THE YOUNG AS A RESULT OF
The Cultural Perspective 372 THEIR LIFE HISTORY 409
CROSS-CUTTING THEMES: DIRECTION OF Child Characteristics as Sources of Inf luence on and
EFFECT IN SOCIALIZATION RELATIONSHIPS Reactivity to the Environment 410
AND SITUATIONAL SPECIFICITY OF What Is the Route by Which Childrens Experiences
SOCIALIZATION 373 Lead to Different Outcomes? 412
Direction of Effect in Socialization Relationships 373 INTEGRATION 414
Situation-Specific Socialization 376 REFERENCES 416

Throughout history, parents and communities have Socialization research began with the assumption that
been concerned with ways to influence the young. the process was based more or less entirely on the tuition
Children are born into many different types of worlds, of the young by those who were invested in the childs
and caregivers hope to prepare them to cope well and/or their own welfare, and the childs successful ac-
in their own particular world. Socialization repre- quisition of relevant knowledge and skills. More recently,
sents the preparation of the young to manage the developmentalists have come to two new realizations.
tasks of social life and involves the continuous inter- First, the brain contains the blueprints that determine the
play between: routes by which and the ease with which socialization
occurs (and thus may be thought of as experience-
Biological mechanisms that facilitate receptivity and expectant ). Socializing experiences act to modify not
motivation to acquire competency in the experienced only the cognitive, socioemotional, and behavioral com-
environment petencies of the developing individual but also the cor-
Social-cultural mechanisms by which the environment rected design of the brain (which thus may be thought of
serves to shape and strengthen those competencies as experience-dependent ). Second, social context has a
Variations in the childs biological, social, and cogni- significant impact on the effect of socializing experi-
tive outcomes that occur in response to the experi- ences. What socialization is and how it works depends on
enced environment both the immediate social context and the long-term cul-
tural context. Although there are many continuities
In this system, biological and social-cultural factors across time and cultures with respect to socialization,
build on each other in a recursive fashion. there are also many variations, reflecting the fact that

366
Socialization Processes 367

socialization is an umbrella term that refers to a suite of ment relationship between child and primary caregiver
processes that serve many different purposes. A related could not be ignored. With the emergence of neuro-
realization is that the domain in which socialization science, theorists and researchers began to explore addi-
takes place (e.g., protection, tuition in the rules of group tional pieces of the socialization puzzle, suggesting ways
life, collaborative work to accomplish a shared task, or in which socialization processes are routed through and
the exertion of control over a child whose actions may be influenced by the central nervous system and associated
dangerous to others) also determines the impact of par- neurohormonal processes. In addition, cultural psychol-
ticular socialization experiences. ogists alerted them to the fact that differing societal be-
Socialization can produce either benefits or costs for liefs and goals affect the practices in which agents of
both the young and others in their environment. On the socialization engage in different cultural contexts and
one hand, it can function to foster childrens individual the meaning assigned to these practices.
success in managing their lives and their shared man- At the same time, researchers began to understand
agement of life challenges with others in their world. that a complete understanding of the socialization pro-
Thus, it includes their ability and motivation to acquire cess was not possible without a consideration of the
individual and culturally shared competencies at a so- complexity of those who were doing the socializing.
cial, emotional, and cognitive level. On the other hand, Parents, for example, have their own set of expectations
socialization may create threats to these ends. Parents, and predilectionsthe ways in which they think about
or other agents of socialization, may lack the knowl- the parenting relationshipthat affect the way they ap-
edge, investment, or competency to assist the young in proach the tasks of socialization. Initially these expec-
ways that add to the individual and common good. Fi- tations and predilections were seen to be deliberate and
nally, there are necessary conflicts between the motives reflective but increasingly it became clear that at least
of the young and their elders. Such conflicts may occur some of them occurred automatically and with little
at points of transition in their individual and shared awareness. Parenting approaches affect the responses of
lives, for example, when the young seek increasing au- the child, which feed back to affect parental expecta-
tonomy or when parents have priorities that are dis- tions and predilections by confirming and expanding
crepant from those of their children. Celebrities them or, at times, contradicting them. Thus, any discus-
receiving awards credit their parents with their suc- sion of socialization is now seen to require attention not
cesses. Prison inmates curse their parents as causal in only to the processes involved in childrens socialization
their downfall. To some extent, these variations reflect but also to biological and cultural processes involved in
post hoc biases in causal reasoning. At the same time, the production of caregiver socialization practices and
the reality is such that parentsin collaboration with to the ways in which children and caregivers have a mu-
othersmay indeed expand or limit the childs positive tual and reciprocal impact on each other.
possibilities. We begin this chapter with a consideration of the
Socialization research has been guided by many dif- changing nature of the content, theoretical perspectives,
ferent theoretical perspectives. It began with psychoana- and models in the field of socialization. We then move
lytic notions about the conflict between the wishes of on to give attention to (a) the biological platform on
the individual and the demands of society and moved to which socialization is built, ( b) the sociocultural
a variety of learning theory conceptualizations reflect- processes that organize the socialization experiences of
ing approaches dominant across the field of psychology the young, and (c) the ways in which biological and soci-
at the time. Increasing attention to cognitive processes ocultural history combine to influence the childs life
(including the developing abilities of individuals to cog- outcomes. Finally, we integrate our view of the current
nitively represent themselves and their social world) and status of socialization theory and consider what we be-
to the role of emotion expression and emotion regulation lieve to be its future directions.
resulted in work on the part of social and developmental The approach that we follow is not without limita-
psychologists to determine the way in which social cog- tions. The research cited is necessarily representative
nitions and emotions were linked. Although attachment rather than exhaustive. In addition, attention is focused
theory remained formally apart from traditional theo- on socialization processes that involve children and ado-
ries of socialization, it became increasingly evident that lescents. Socialization continues during the lifetime, but
the foundation for socialization provided by the attach- the present discussion is limited to the age grouping of
368 Socialization Processes

interest in this Handbook. The early years are those in or loss of love. To maintain the repression, as well as
which socialization produces its greatest effects. Fi- elicit parental approval, they identified with or internal-
nally, central attention is given to socializing relation- ized the values and rules of their parents. Here was the
ships between parents and children. Socialization basis for self-punishment and guilt as well as an answer
involves a wide spectrum of influences. Beyond parents, for the puzzling question of how individuals behave in
for example, there are the powerful influences of peers socially acceptable ways without constant surveillance.
who come to expand the lens through which children un- The concept of internalization also laid the groundwork
derstand and manage their world. But parents, we be- for a position that values are transmitted in their totality
lieve, are primary for a number of reasons, including the rather than modified or constructed by their recipients.
fact that they and their children are unique in being part
of a biologically intertwined system that strongly sup-
ports the socialization process. Attachment Theory
In this first section, we review the theoretical ap-
Psychoanalytic theory was adapted and modified in sub-
proaches that have been taken to socialization and then
sequent approaches to understanding socialization. In at-
highlight changing emphases in the field. The latter in-
tachment theory, for example, there was a sharp break in
clude shifting models in the exploration of causal
the notion of a conflictual relationship between society
processes in socialization and the emerging view of so-
and child, which was replaced by a more positive view
cialization as variable across domain and context.
of the adaptive quality of parent-child relationships.
Bowlby drew from ethology and evolutionary biology to
argue that, over evolutionary time, relationships between
THEORETICAL APPROACHES AND
caregivers and their offspring had taken shapes that fos-
CHANGING EMPHASES
tered both the survival and the adaptive skills of the
young. The first steps toward that goal took the form of
There have been several classical approaches to social-
actions, on both sides, which ensured proximity and pro-
ization and caregiving relationships. These include psy-
tection of the young, as well as facilitating compliance
choanalytic theory, attachment theory, and social
with the directives of caregivers (Stayton, Hogan, &
learning theory.
Ainsworth, 1971). Retained from the psychoanalytic
perspective was a strong concern with the impact of
early experiences on later development. That concern is
Psychoanalytic Theory
at the core of proposals that the attachment patterns de-
The first formal approach to understanding socializa- veloped in early life are carried forward in the form of
tion emerged from psychoanalytic theory, which viewed working models (mental representations of the caregiv-
the child as hedonistic, with expression of aggressive ing relationship that include both cognitive and emo-
and sexual impulses needing parental and societal inter- tional components). These working models subsequently
vention to create a civilized human being (Freud, 1965). influence individuals close relationships, including the
The importance of early experience was underlined in relationship with their own offspring (e.g., Bowlby,
the premise that patterns of responding learned when in- 1980; Bretherton, 1980). With attachment theory came,
dividuals solved conflicts between gratification of bod- as well, a change in content studied. Of primary interest
ily desires and the demands of society formed was the quality of the relationship formed between care-
prototypes for later personality functioning (Erickson, giver and child, a relationship based on the biological
1959), including characteristic ways of dealing with de- need for the provision of protection and comfort as
pendency, aggression, gender roles, and conscience. opposed to the linkage between the satisfaction by a
Freuds concept of incorporation or internalization was caregiver of an oral or hunger drive as posited by psycho-
particularly influential in guiding subsequent thinking analytic theory ( hence the distinction between attach-
about socialization. Thus, children were assumed to re- ment and dependency). Ultimately, attention turned to
sent the imposition of societal values that threatened the role of attachment in the way in which children
their autonomy but to repress their hostile feelings be- learned to regulate their negative emotions (e.g., Cas-
cause of fear of punishment in the form of abandonment sidy, 1994), which is a further extension of content.
Theoretical Approaches and Changing Emphases 369

Social Learning Theories childrens efforts rather than being a function of events
over which they have no control.
Psychoanalytic theory contributed to the thinking of so-
Emerging separately from the social learning ap-
cial learning theorists who combined the rich clinical
proaches of Sears and Bandura was Pattersons social
material of psychoanalysis with the basic principles of
interactional perspective (e.g., Patterson, 1980, 1982,
learning theory to produce a dynamic but empirically
1997). Patterson maintains that the reinforcement con-
testable view of socialization (e.g., Sears, Maccoby, &
tingencies embedded in social interactions are the im-
Levin, 1957; Sears, Rau, & Alpert, 1964). The approach
portant determinants of childrens behavior and has
involved a set of processes that relied heavily on primary
shown in a series of finely detailed studies how some
and secondary or learned drives that had their origin in
mothers have difficulty obtaining compliance from their
maternal reduction of primary drives, such as hunger and
children because of their negative reinforcement of
thirst, similar to the psychoanalytic focus on hunger but
those childrens coercive behavior. Thus, Patterson
different from the attachment theory focus on protec-
(1997) suggests that problem families function as sim-
tion. These learned drives included dependencythe
ple short-term maximizing systems that inadvertently
need to be near the mother and identificationthe need
contribute to their own long-term misery (p. 209).
to be like the mother (including the reproduction of her
Moreover, when positive reinforcement by peers is
values and beliefs). The theory accounted in learning
added to this pattern of parenting, the outcome is high
terms for the transmission and internalization of values
levels of aggressive behavior (Snyder & Patterson,
and promoted research on topics relevant to socialization
1995). Missing in Pattersons approach is the concern
such as techniques of discipline. Studies of parenting
with internalization that plays such a prominent role in
suggested that, for example, reasoning and withdrawal of
other approaches to socialization, with Patterson (1997)
love worked well, particularly if they occurred in a con-
arguing that reinforcement contingencies that control
text of warmth (Sears et al., 1957). The explanation was
coercive behaviors are embedded in social exchanges
that they, unlike punishment or assertion of power, took
and that these events are highly unlikely to be actively
advantage of the reinforcement that came from reproduc-
processed. Thus, behaviors during conflict are over-
ing the (withdrawn) behavior of a warm and loving agent
learned and shaped without awareness. In a similar vein,
of socialization.
Patterson and Fisher (2002) question what is to be
In a further refinement of social learning theory,
gained by developmental models that hypothesize events
Bandura and Walters (1963) presented a sociobehavior-
such as internalized values as mediators between parent-
istic approach that eschewed psychoanalytic notions as
ing practices and child outcomes.
well as reliance on the concept of acquired drives. They
argued that the social nature of human functioning
needed greater emphasis, focusing on the acquisition of
Parenting Attitudes and Styles
novel responses through observational learning, and ar-
guing that this, as opposed to learning through rein- Early theories of socialization emphasized parent actions
forcement, was the central and most important form of as predictors of child outcomes. Some researchers, how-
learning. Internalization of societal standards comes ever, came to believe that parenting attitudes might prove
about through self-regulation, with the suggestion that to be better predictors of child socialization outcomes.
people maintain their beliefs and values despite chang- Again, ideas emerged from psychoanalytic approaches.
ing external circumstances because they judge their own Horney (1933) and Levy (1943), for example, explored so-
actionsjudgments learned through observation and di- cialization through the study of unconscious emotional re-
rect learning (Bandura, 1977). Bandura (1977) also actions, such as overprotection or excessive parental
moved away from the notion of wholesale incorporation control, which determined parenting behavior and there-
of parental values when he argued that children select fore childrens social, emotional, and cognitive outcomes.
from the conflicting information they receive to estab- Over time, parent attitudes came to be considered as
lish their own standards of behavior, with selection de- straightforward events that were accessible to conscious
pending on a number of variables including differences awareness rather than as reflections of unconscious
in perceived competence between the model and the self processes. Their content was also expanded to include con-
and the degree to which behavior is seen to arise from cepts such as warmth versus coldness, acceptance versus
370 Socialization Processes

rejection, and autonomy versus control (e.g., A. Baldwin, tion, researchers often gave consideration to the role of
1955; Schaefer, 1965). Parental attitudes were seen to pro- the childs cognitive development in their responses to
vide the emotional climate in which parental values and socialization experiences. In doing so, they were con-
beliefs are transmitted and so they provided a context in cerned with the changing abilities of the child as a
which that transmission occurred and presumably affected processor of information. In addition, the child, at all
its success. In addition to dimensions of parenting style, ages, came to be understood as acquiring social knowl-
some socialization researchers focused on categories of edge structures. Bowlby, for example, borrowed from the
parenting style, providing a qualitative assessment that developing interests in cognitive processes to talk about
captured the complexity and subtlety of combinations of the ways in which the young carry forward their early
parenting dimensions. The most enduring and influential history with parents as internal working models. Be-
of these categorical systems is Baumrinds (e.g., 1967), in ginning in the 1980s, Dodge, Coie, and their colleagues
her division of parents into three groupings: (1) authoritar- (see Coie & Dodge, 1998, for an overview) applied a
ian, characterized by rigid psychological control; (2) au- cognitive approach to childrens antisocial responses,
thoritative, marked by firm control, warmth and with a particular focus on attributional biases that led to
responsiveness to the childs needs; and (3) permissive. the interpretation of the ambiguous actions of others as
reflecting hostile intentions. From a social information-
processing framework, children were seen as approach-
ing social situations with a history of social experiences
Cognitive Approaches to Socialization
represented in memory. Their consequent behavior was
In the 1960s, there was an emergent interest in experi- determined by the way they encoded, represented, and
mental psychology in the role of cognitions, an interest processed events as a function of interactions between
that redirected the field of psychology away from a pre- biology, memorial events, and cues in the immediate so-
dominant focus on behavior. The new approach focused cial situation. In a reconceptualization of parental disci-
on the ways in which information is processed in re- pline practices and childrens internalization of values,
sponse to relevant stimuli, along with the ways in which Grusec and Goodnow (1994) emphasized two features of
such processing serves to guide subsequent responses. childrens cognition that were important in this process.
However, the guise that cognitive formulations took var- The first was their accurate perception of the parental
ied across fields and theorists. Social psychology had a message and the second was their perception of such fea-
long history of concern with such constructs as social tures of discipline as its fairness and its noncoerciveness
perception, causal reasoning (attribution theory), moti- that made acceptance of the message more likely.
vation for cognitive consistency, and so forth. Bandura Systematic consideration of parental cognitions
(1986) formally adopted the title of social cognitive emerged in the early 1980s (although it was predated by
theory for a model that had employed cognitive con- earlier work on parenting attitudes). Parke (1978)
structs such as attention and imaginal encoding from the pointed out that, up to that point, most analyses of
very beginning. M. Hoffman (1970) elaborated on the mother-infant interaction had credited the mother with
social learning theorists analysis of discipline tech- the same degree of cognitive complexity as the infant.
niques in a cognitive developmental approach. The ap- Realization that the understanding of socialization
proach emphasized the use of other-oriented processes was seriously limited by failure to consider
discipline (providing a rationale that focused on the im- the important role of parental cognitions led to a re-
pact of the childs antisocial actions on others) as par- framing of this area of research. The 1980s saw an up-
ticularly effective for moral development. This pattern surge of research activity designed to clarify linkages
of discipline was contrasted to power assertion (use of between childrens actions, parental thinking, parent
the parents superior power to exert control), which pre- actions, and child outcomes (e.g., Bugental & Johnston,
dicted low levels of moral maturity, and love-with- 2000; Goodnow & Collins, 1990; S. Miller, 1995; Sigel,
drawal (withholding approval or affection when the McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & Goodnow, 1992). Parental
child misbehaves), which was not consistently related to cognitions also appeared in the work of attachment the-
moral maturity. orists who focused on the way in which caregivers
The inclusion of cognitive constructs in the field of think about their ability to provide protection for their
socialization took many different forms. As one varia- children (e.g., George & Solomon, 1996).
Current Developments: Biological and Cultural Perspectives 371

The approach taken to parental cognitions has fol- diating processes in parent-offspring interactions
lowed two complementary pathways. The first focuses on (Carter, 1998; Fleming, 1990; Fleming, Corter,
parental cognitions as stable knowledge structures or Stallings, & Steiner, 2002; Liu et al., 1997; Meaney,
schemas (as described by Bugental, 1992, and Grusec, Aitken, Bodnoff, Iny, & Tatarewicz, 1985). In addition,
Hastings, & Mammone, 1994); as such, they represent there has been increasing interest in the long-term in-
implicit cognitions that operate automatically, effort- fluences of socialization practices on the regulation (or
lessly, and outside of awareness (Bugental, Lyon, Cortez, dysregulation) of the childs neurohormonal responses
& Krantz, 1997; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Parental (e.g., Bugental, Martorell, & Barraza, 2003; Gunnar,
cognitions as stable knowledge structures are learned as 2000). Behavioral neuroscientists working predomi-
summary representations of parents own past history as nantly with nonhuman models have shown an increased
a child (e.g., George & Solomon, 1996; Zeanah, Benoit, interest in applying their findings to human processes,
Hirschberg, Barton, & Regan, 1994) or they may reflect and social scientists have increasingly discovered that
a shared cultural history (e.g., Bornstein et al., 1998). these findings help to inform their understanding of
The second approach to parental cognitions focuses on human relationships (e.g., Collins, Maccoby, Stein-
parental cognitions as event-dependent and data driven; berg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000).
as such, they involve reflective appraisal of ongoing As a second shift, a bridge has emerged between the
events in the caregiving relationship (see Bugental, John- traditional interests of socialization or developmental
ston, New, & Silvester, 1998, for a review of these two researchers and those of evolutionary psychologists
approaches to parental cognitions). who focus on parental investment theory (Trivers,
1974). These fields have moved together in a shared
concern with the experiences of children who are
CURRENT DEVELOPMENTS: BIOLOGICAL thought of as costly from an evolutionary standpoint
AND CULTURAL PERSPECTIVES and at risk in developmental psychology (e.g., Bugen-
tal & Beaulieu, 2004; Geary, 2000; Geary & Bjorklund,
Two new areas of focus have emerged in recent years, as 2000; Hertwig, Davis, & Sulloway, 2002). These theo-
socialization researchers have responded to increasing retical approaches converge to provide predictions with
interest in biology and culture. No real understanding of regard to both the negative and positive outcomes for
any psychological phenomenon is possible without such children as a function of the costs or benefits they
knowledge of how these two interact repeatedly to ar- provide to parents in the latters reproductive success.
rive at the outcome of interest. More generally, increasing reference is being made to
Biological Perspectives an emerging field of evolutionary developmental psy-
chology (e.g., Bjorklund & Pellegrini, 2000, 2002;
In the past decade, the integration of biological and social Geary & Bjorklund, 2000). Evolutionary psychology
processes has emerged across a variety of areas, where it has focused attention on the domain-specific nature of
has been recognized that these processes act in a comple- adaptations. Consistent with this perspective, social-
mentary fashion. It has been proposed that when these ization may be conceptualized as involving distinctive
processes are considered together, new insights may be af- algorithms in accomplishing different goals or tasks
forded regarding the mechanisms involved in complex in- (Bugental, 2000).
terpersonal interactions (Cacioppo, Berntson, Sheridan, Also seen are continuing shifts in the contribution of
& McClintock, 2000). In addition, there has been in- developmental behavior genetics to the socialization pro-
creased communication and cross-referencing between in- cess. Traditional approaches have focused on the main
vestigators concerned with parenting relationships among effects of genes on childrens social, emotional, and cog-
humans and those concerned with nonhuman models. The nitive responses. More recently, interest has centered on
combined perspectives have provided new insights into the interplay between genetic and environmental influ-
many aspects of socialization. These perspectives, dis- ences (e.g., Collins et al., 2000). Evidence is mounting in
cussed in turn, include developmental neuroscience, evo- support of evocative gene-environment correlations
lutionary psychology, and behavior genetics. the ways in which genetically based characteristics of
Developmental neuroscience has moved into a posi- the child come to influence the socializing environment,
tion of central importance as researchers consider me- which feeds back to influence the childs behavior. As a
372 Socialization Processes

case in point, OConnor, Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rut- In summary, developmental neuroscience, evolution-
ter, and Plomin (1998) found that children who were at ary psychology, and behavior genetics all converge in
genetic risk for antisocial behavior (an assumption based their focus on the brain. Evolutionary psychology, com-
on the prebirth behavior of their biological mothers) bined with developmental neuroscience, tells much
were more likely to elicit coercive parenting from their about the experience-expectant brain. Concerns with
adoptive parentsa pattern that fostered increases in gene expression tell about the experience-dependent
antisocial behavior of children. Evocative gene-environ- brain. Developmental neuroscience offers information
ment correlations have also been found for other social- regarding the couriers and routes through which social-
izing influences; for example, the mutual responsiveness izing influences occur. In doing so, these disciplines si-
shown between parents and very young children multaneously inform understanding of problematic and
(Deater-Deckard & OConnor, 2000), or the negative adaptive socialization of the young.
conflict shown between parents and adolescents (Nei-
derhiser, Reiss, Hetherington, & Plomin, 1999).
The Cultural Perspective
As another causal route, genetic differences between
children may serve to moderate the effects of the socializ- Major changes in the orientation of socialization re-
ing environment; for example, Caspi et al. (2002) found searchers are also evident in a dramatically increasing
that the long-term effects of maltreatment are moderated interest in the impact of context and culture on social-
by genetic patterns. Those children whose genotype con- ization outcomes. Earlier approaches to the study of cul-
ferred high levels of monoamine oxidase A (MAOA; an tures impact focused on cross-cultural comparisons as
enzyme that metabolizes neurotransmitters, such as sero- a way of validating the universality of psychological the-
tonin, and thus renders them inactive) were more likely ories or of obtaining a wider variation in variables of in-
than children without this genetic pattern to respond to terest than would be otherwise possible (e.g., Barry,
maltreatment with antisocial problems. The evidence that Child, & Bacon, 1959; Whiting, 1976). More recently,
genotypes can moderate the childs sensitivity to environ- the focus has been on how psychological events can be
mental insults is significant; more generally, Belsky (e.g., understood as a reflection of shared cultural meanings
Belsky in press; Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, 1997) and col- and practices, with attempts to find new ways of think-
leagues have proposed that there are genetic variations in ing about psychological functioning that cannot be ac-
children that lead to differential susceptibility to their counted for in existing theories (J. Miller, 2002).
parenting experience. This line of research has led to im- Culture has also been expanded to include not only so-
portant insights regarding the types of parenting that are cialization in different self-contained groups but also
more or less effective with children with different tem- socialization in the context of ethnic and minority
perament patterns (e.g., Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, groups existing in a larger and different cultural context
1998; Kochanska, 1997). In addition, increased consider- (primarily in North America) and interactions between
ation is being given to the ways in which environments in- socioeconomic status and cultural context.
fluence gene expression (e.g., Brown, 1999; Bruer & Harkness and Super (2002) note that studies of culture
Greenough, 2001). For example, a very stressful early en- and socialization, regardless of disciplinary paradigm,
vironment may lead to the expression of genes that control have in common four assumptions. First, settings, such as
the presence of cortisol receptors in the brain; the result- types of dwellings and expectable activities for members
ant changes in childrens response reactivity may act back of the group, are important because they determine the
to influence the nature of their social environment. Early boundaries of childrens experience as well embody cul-
experience in the home environment not only may foster tural meanings. Second, the activities and routines that
problematic gene expression but also hold the possibility occur in different settings instantiate themes that matter
for fostering adaptive gene expression in response to envi- to agents of socialization and thereby communicate cul-
ronmental challenges. Newer concern with the two-way ef- tural messages. Third, there are themes in a culture such
fects of genetic and experiential influences counter the that the same ideas or images occur in the meanings that
earlier concerns expressed by many socialization theo- are inherent in settings and activities; as well, the way
rists (e.g., Baumrind, 1993; L. Hoffman, 1985) concern- settings and activities are organized for younger mem-
ing earlier genetic approaches that focused on just one bers of the group reflects an integrated system on the
side of the picture. part of socialization agents. And, finally, what agents of
Cross-Cutting Themes: Direction of Ef fect in Socialization Relationships and Situational Specificity of Socialization 373

socialization do to children is affected not only by the issues that must be addressed in any conceptualization
cultural experiences of the agents themselves but also by of the socialization process. They both reflect changes
characteristics of children with whom they interact, with in the way researchers have thought about socialization
these characteristics including temperament differences, as a process whereby the agent of socialization trans-
skill potentials, and species-specific potentials for mits standards, rules, and procedures to the child but the
growth, transformation, and the organization of experi- nature of the standard, rule, procedure, and so on is ir-
ence into meaning (Super & Harkness, 1997). One way in relevant in the transmission process.
which these ideas have been formalized is in ecocul-
tural (Weisner & Garnier, 1992) or developmental
niches. Super and Harkness (1997), for example, suggest Direction of Effect in Socialization
that children are socialized in a developmental niche that Relationships
includes the physical and social settings of daily life, cul-
A continuing concern in the field of socialization has
turally regulated customs of child rearing, and cultural
been with the direction of effects in relationships. De-
belief systems or ethnotheories of caregivers. These
velopmentalists have moved away from very simple
features are coordinated, they are influenced by outside
models that fail to capture the complex nature of causal
events, and they are affected by the childs individual
processes toward the increasing use of models that are
characteristics to yield particular socialization outcomes.
concerned with the interplay of causal forces of many
Cultural psychologists argue that cultures can be un-
types. Models have moved from the depiction of unidi-
derstood as created, sustained, and communicated in
rectional causality (socializing agent to child, or child
everyday practices and behavioral routines (Goodnow,
to socializing agent) to bidirectional causality (interde-
Miller, & Kessel, 1995) and that development occurs
pendence in the effects of causal influences in socializ-
through participation in activities rather than through
ing relationships). As a new direction, contingent
the acquisition of knowledge and skills (Rogoff, Pardies,
causality is being explored in socialization processes; in
Arauz, Corres-Chavez, & Angelillo, 2003). Fiske, Ki-
such models, causal influences are seen as contingent
tayama, Markus, and Nisbett (1998) write that social-
on context. Despite the increasing sophistication of so-
ization involves the incorporation of cultural models,
cialization models, empirical research has not kept pace
meanings, and practices into basic psychological
with the emerging models. The preponderance of so-
processes, which maintain or transform the cultural sys-
cialization research (in both the questions asked and
tem. Moreover, behavioral responses cannot be under-
the methods of analysis) continues to employ unidirec-
stood without knowledge of the culturally based
tional models (as noted by Collins et al., 2000, and
meaning they have and the cultural practices to which
Kuczynski, 2003).
they are linked (J. Miller, 2002).
The earliest way of thinking about socialization
These various proposals have affected and will con-
processes by philosophers, educators, and social scientists
tinue to affect the way researchers construe the develop-
was unidirectionalinfluence flows from parents and so-
mental process. The work of cultural psychologists has
ciety to children. As described by Hirschfeld (2001):
underlined the importance of context and meaning for
socialization theorists and led to the realization that the
Socialization has been seen as a causal process in which
central focus on a few specific socialization practices
knowledge is acquired from communal living in a particu-
(e.g., reinforcement, discipline, modeling), characteriz- lar social environment. In this view, structure and organi-
ing much of the history of research in the area, has lim- zation in cultural learning flow from society to child, and
ited understanding and needs considerable expansion. consequently from adult to child. . . . Children are treated
largely as if they were passive receptacles into whom cul-
CROSS-CUTTING THEMES: DIRECTION ture is poured or on whom it is impressed. (p. 109)
OF EFFECT IN SOCIALIZATION
RELATIONSHIPS AND SITUATIONAL The pervasiveness of this view has been documented
SPECIFICITY OF SOCIALIZATION in comprehensive reviews (e.g., Corsaro, 1997). This
view was virtually unchallenged until Bells highly in-
Before moving to a detailed discussion of biology and fluential critique (1968). In reviewing what was known
culture in the remainder of the chapter, we discuss two about the influence process that occurred between
374 Socialization Processes

parents and children, Bell made a compelling case that often contribute to understanding such transactional
children have just as much (if not more) influence on par- systems in that experimental variations can be produced
ents than parents do on children. Children strongly influ- at various points in the relationships making causal in-
ence their own environments, either by serving to ference possible. Transactional models are also consis-
trigger parental responses or by selecting some environ- tent with emerging work in the field of developmental
ments more than others (e.g., Scarr & McCartney, 1983). behavior genetics, with demonstrations of the two-way
Ultimately, it came to be accepted that the influence influence processes found between genes and environ-
process between socializing agents and children was ment, for example.
bidirectional in nature. As well described by Kuczynski
(2003), bidirectional models come in many different Circular Models
forms. As a variant of Kuczynskis categorization, we Circular models represent a variant of transactional
consider ways of conceptualizing socialization that fol- models (Kuczynski, 2003). In such models, the parent
low from (a) transactional models, ( b) circular models, and child relationship involves a recursive interactional
(c) ecological models, (d) systems models, and (e) con- loop that has no beginning or end. As is true for other
tingent causality models. transactional models, such processes are not easily mea-
sured: It is only possible to enter the loop at defined
Transactional Models points, and the direction of effects is not easily ascer-
The earliest break from unilateral models came with the tained. The notion of circularity does, however, have
introduction of the notion of socialization transactions good explanatory value in accounting for observations
by Sameroff (1975). From this perspective, there are of socializing interactions. This has been particularly
continuous reciprocal interchanges between parents and notable in explaining the vicious circles described by
children. Thus, the parent responds to the childs behav- Patterson and his colleagues (e.g., Patterson, Reid, &
ior at one point in time, and the parents response to the Dishion, 1998). These investigators have found repeated
childs behavior comes to influence the childs response evidence for reciprocal influence processes between
in the future. Consequently, socializing relationships un- mothers and aggressive children. In these families, the
dergo constant transformation. However, continuous mother often begins an interchange with an aversive de-
transactional processes are not easily measured. As a re- mand, the child counterattacks, the mother backs off,
sult, research following from this approach typically ap- and the child terminates the counterattack. As the
proximates the tenets of the model by measuring a series mothers influence attempts fail, her confidence de-
of unidirectional influences; for example, the influence clines, and thus her ability to elicit compliance is fur-
of the child on the parent at one point in time, followed ther eroded. In similar fashion, longitudinal research
by the influence of the parent on the child at a later point conducted by Eisenberg and her colleagues (Eisenberg
in time. Causal inference in such models is possible when et al., 1999) found bidirectional effects between chil-
interventions lead to differences in parental behavior (as drens negative emotionality and parents use of puni-
a result of an experimental manipulation), which influ- tive tactics.
ence child outcomes. LaFreniere and Capuanos (1997)
Ecological Models
intervention with the mothers of anxious-withdrawn pre-
school children provides an illustration: This interven- In ecological approaches to socialization, consideration
tion led to reductions in maternal intrusiveness, which has been given to bidirectionality, not only in the fam-
was associated with increases in childrens social com- ily, but also between the family and the larger social
petence. networks in which the family is rooted. The earliest
In biological approaches that focus on the interrela- ecological model in the field of socialization emerged
tionship of physiological processes and social interac- with Bronfenbrenners conceptualizations (1979). The
tions, transactional models fit well with the evidence. term ecology was used to describe the various levels of
For example, the regulatory systems of mothers and in- socializing influencefrom the forces operating in the
fants are linked, with the internal states of each open to nuclear family to extended family or friends, neighbor-
regulation by the presence or responses of the other hoods, and larger community structures. The descrip-
(e.g., Hofer, 1987). As is noted later, animal models tive adjective geographical was applied to this
Cross-Cutting Themes: Direction of Ef fect in Socialization Relationships and Situational Specificity of Socialization 375

approach (Goodnow et al., 1995) as a way of highlight- poses a challenge to existing statistical methodsa
ing the metaphor contained in an ecological model. In challenge receiving increased attention (Kenny, Korch-
models of this kind, the socializing role of parents may maros, & Bolger, 2003).
follow from their role as guides or managers (e.g., Parke
& Bhavnagri, 1989): Rather than serving as direct Contingent Causality Models
sources of influence, parents may act to manage or cre- Conditional causality models focus on the if-then na-
ate environments, which, in themselves, serve as social- ture of socialization processes: Socializing forces,
izing influences. rather than being understood primarily as additive or re-
ciprocal effects, are understood to involve contextual
Family Systems Models contingencies. This type of model has served to influ-
A systems model of family processes make uses of prin- ence thought emerging from different theoretical per-
ciples drawn from general systems theory (as reviewed spectives. Concerns with the fit between socializing
by Cox & Paley, 1997). In this approach, the focus is on style and child temperament represent the earliest use of
the mutual influences that occur between family mem- a conditional causality model (Thomas & Chess, 1977).
bers and between subsystems in the family. For example, From this framework, it was proposed that effective so-
the parent-child relationship is understood in the context cialization was contingent on the match between
of the relationship between parents. Thus, the term co- parental practices and the characteristics of a particular
parenting is used to describe the activity of joint care- child. More recently, a number of investigators (e.g.,
givers, with effectively functioning co-parenting units Kim, Conger, Lorenz, & Elder, 2001; Kochanska, 1995;
in which the caregivers together convey a context of sol- Patterson & Sanson, 1999; Stoolmiller, 2001) have ex-
idarity and support between them, a consistent set of panded this approach, showing how various features of
rules, and a secure home base (McHale et al., 2002). childrens temperament, such as fearfulness or irritabil-
Family processes are conceptualized as organized sys- ity, interact with the socialization practices to which
tems whose properties are not derivable from knowledge they are exposed.
about the individuals or family subsystems. For exam- In another line of work, research concerned with the
ple, it has been suggested that childrens emotional se- organizing effects of parenting contexts and parental
curity cannot be understood outside the context of the goals has stressed the appropriate variations in parental
family; in particular, it has been suggested that the practices across settings (e.g., Dix, 1992; Grusec &
childs sense of security will be damaged by destructive Goodnow, 1994; Grusec, Goodnow, & Kuczynski, 2000;
conflict between parents (e.g., Cummings & Wilson, Hastings & Grusec, 1998). From this point of view, par-
1999). This approach has stressed consideration of the ents understanding of a particular childs characteris-
role of fathers (as well as mothers) if a full understand- tics and perspective and the characteristics of the
ing is to be obtained of childrens development (Cox, immediate situation, rather than the use of specific
Paley, & Harter, 2001). strategies or styles, determines their effectiveness
Finally, the approach has stressed the extent to which (Grusec et al., 2000). Thus, maternal perspective-taking
families can reorganize adaptively in response to chal- ability has been linked to mother-adolescent conflict in-
lenges and transitions external to the family (e.g., the tensity (Lundell, Grusec, McShane, & Davidov, 2004;
childs entrance to school). All components in the so- Smetana, 1996) and mothers knowledge of their chil-
cializing system (the individuals in it, the social net- drens reactions to different discipline interventions has
works in which it is embedded, along with other aspects been linked to their childrens compliance (Davidov &
of the larger environment) are understood to be in con- Grusec, 2005). Similarly, parents knowledge of their
stant change, in particular, at points of transition (e.g., adolescents thoughts and feelings during conflicts pre-
Cowan & Cowan, 2000; Fogel & Branco, 1997; Valsiner dicts positive outcomes (Hastings & Grusec, 1997). In
& Cairns, 1992). Effective parenting is understood to addition, socializing practices appropriately vary with
involve collaboration in response to these changes, con- shifts in parental goals or aims (e.g., child safety or
flicts, and contradictions (e.g., Holden & Ritchie, 1988). compliance; Grusec et al., 2000).
As a constraint posed by this approach, the measurement From a very different perspective, parental investment
of reciprocal processes between family subsystems theory proposes that the level of parental care provided to
376 Socialization Processes

the young is contingent on the resources available to the intrinsically appealing in that it offered a guidebook
parents (Mann, 1992; Trivers, 1974), combined with the for parents (or those concerned with facilitating or un-
costs imposed by provision of care to a particular child derstanding optimal parenting) regarding the social-
(e.g., the costs of providing care to an at-risk child versus ization processes that worked. The durability of
other children). Thus, a high-risk child might be ne- Baumrinds (e.g., 1967, 1971) approach to socializa-
glected by parents who lack resources (i.e., economic, tion is a case in point.
cognitive, or social) but receive exceptionally high levels Challenges to this view have emerged from different
of care by parents who have adequate access to resources theoretical perspectives. Increased attention has been
(Bugental & Beaulieu, 2004). directed to the possibility that socialization involves
Finally, biological approaches include many instances processes that are qualitatively distinct across context
of conditional causality in social relationships. For exam- or domain. Researchers in some areas have suggested
ple, reference is often made to switching mechanisms that the processes of interest to them are controlled by
(Zupanc & Lamprecht, 2000) in which a pattern of social mechanisms that are specific to a particular response
response (e.g., manifestation of parental behavior) is system or a particular type of social relationship. Lan-
turned on in response to hormonal change. As another guage acquisition (following the work of Chomsky,
example, the hormonal changes that occur during preg- 1988) is one such area, with the thesis that children are
nancy are associated with increased interest in care of the innately wired to develop language. Attachment
young (among both mothers and their partners). processes (following the work of Bowlby, 1973) is an-
In summary, models of causality in socializing rela- other area with its focus on maternal protection as a
tionships have moved away from simple concern with foundation of personality. Research concerned with
unidirectional effects (parent to child, child to parent) empathy and sympathetic distress (following the lead
to models that consider the role of reciprocal influences, of M. Hoffman, 1981) is a third area in its emphasis on
along with the role of context. Children are increasingly the importance of other-oriented inductionreasoning
understood as engaged in continuous transactions in that addresses the impact of the childs negative ac-
their socializing environment, an environment that in- tions on others.
cludes not only their families but also more extended so- In this section, we review the various ways in which
cial networks, the larger community, and the resources single process theories have come to be questioned. We
available to the family in that community. In addition, begin with some of the earliest challenges and end
interest has emerged in models suggesting a contingent with a relatively recent and systematically organized
relationship between socialization and context: The out- challenge that has been spearheaded by evolutionary
comes of the same socializing practices are expected to psychology.
differ (qualitatively as well as quantitatively) as a func-
Privileged Learning
tion of contextual variables.
The earliest challenges to general learning theory came
from within the field itself. Researchers from this theo-
Situation-Specific Socialization
retical perspective were faced with limits on the extent
In the history of socialization theory, different per- to which the postulated learning mechanisms were true
spectives have been taken on the cross-contextual con- for all situations. For example, evidence emerged that
tinuity of socialization. An earlier view focused on some types of learning appeared to be privileged: Learn-
general processes that were believed to operate across ing occurred with exceptional ease and was highly re-
ages, settings, and cultures. This view was centered in sistant to attempts to override it. Garcias classic
a learning theory approach that focused on domain- research showing that food aversion can be learned by
general processes. From this perspective, the child is nonhumans in a single episode provides a case in point
socialized by a set of processes that are equally appli- (as summarized in Garcia & Koelling, 1996). Seligman
cable across context. Although variations might be ex- (1970) described these predispositions as prelearning.
pected in quantitative processes (e.g., variations in the He proposed that organisms are differentially prepared
intensity of rewards and punishments across ages), to associate different kinds of events, with food aversion
variations were not predicted for the qualitative nature as an example of those very easily acquired and the es-
of socialization across contexts. The perspective was tablishment and maintenance of responses incompatible
Cross-Cutting Themes: Direction of Ef fect in Socialization Relationships and Situational Specificity of Socialization 377

with species-specific responses extremely difficult to fers from that between parents and children. In accord
learn (Breland & Breland, 1966). with Piagetian views, Youniss, McLellan, and Strouse
(1994) suggested that [p]eer relationship[s] are marked
Challenges from Attachment Theory
by use of symmetrical reciprocity and guided by the
Early challenges to the continuity of socializing overarching principle of cooperation by equals
processes also arose in the study of attachment relation- (p. 102), an organizational principle that differs from
ships where the processes suggested by learning theo- the unilateral authority or power asymmetry that is more
rists as organizers of socialization did not appear to characteristic of adult-child relationships.
operate in any simple fashion. Thus, contrary to what
would be predicted by learning theory, babies whose Domains of Social Knowledge and Judgment
mothers responded most quickly and frequently to their Concern with variations in socialization processes
crying in the first 3 months of life cried less at the end also emerged with respect to the demonstration of dis-
of their 1st year of life (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & tinctions children make in their judgments about so-
Wall, 1978). Although the provision of food is a benefit cial concepts. Specifically, Turiel and his colleagues
of the attachment relationship, it does not serve as a (see Turiel, 1998) proposed that there are distinctions
basis for selective attraction to attachment figures or for shown between morality and conventionality across
strengthening of the attachment relationship (Harlow, cultures and that these emerge very early in the course
1973; Harlow & Harlow, 1965). Instead, such attraction of development. Morality involves universal concerns
is organized around the species-specific stimulus fea- with justice, welfare, and rights whereas convention-
tures that have been associated with mothers across the ality involves consensually agreed-on behavioral rou-
relevant evolutionary history. In addition, infants show tines that are important for the organized functioning
privileged learning in identifying the characteristics of of group life. Although both domains are understood
their own mother. In similar fashion, shared play activ- as legitimately subject to regulation by those in au-
ity serves as a benefit that is often associated with early thority (e.g., parents in the home environment and
parent-child relationships but is not associated with at- teachers in the school environment), the moral domain
tachment: The young seek contact with attachment fig- is seen to be obligatory and unalterable, whereas the
ures when they are in a state of distress but may seek out social conventional domain can be altered by agree-
a variety of others for social play (e.g., Bretherton, ment or consensus.
1985; Higley et al., 1992). Building on these concepts, Smetana and her col-
Some investigators have focused specifically on the leagues (e.g., Nucci & Smetana, 1996; Smetana, 1997;
distinctiveness of the function of attachment. For exam- Smetana & Asquith, 1994) have been concerned with the
ple, MacDonald (1992), arguing from an evolutionary changing or nonchanging ways that parents and children
perspective, suggested that attachment relationships are understand and negotiate moral, conventional, prudential
organized by fear responses (fear of separation), (e.g., safety maintenance and property protection), and
whereas affectional relationship are organized on the personal (e.g., choice of friends) domains across the
basis of emotional warmth. Goldberg, Grusec and Jenk- course of development. Thus, parental regulation and en-
ins (1999) pointed out that the key function of attach- forcement are always considered legitimate with respect
ment is the safety of the young and that the fundamental to the moral domain but grow somewhat less so with re-
element in the production of secure attachment revolves spect to the social conventional domain. The big differ-
around provision by a caregiver of protection and com- ence comes in what is deemed by adolescents to be
fort in response to infant distress as opposed to sensitive increasingly under their personal jurisdiction and there-
responding to other forms of affect. fore not subject to parental control. Even sharper dis-
agreement occurs over issues that combine conventional
Peers versus Parents as Agents of Socialization and personal considerations. Keeping ones room clean
Another challenge to the notion of single-process expla- and neat may be seen as involving the conventional do-
nations of socialization came from developmental psy- main by parents, with the room seen as part of the entire
chologists who were concerned with peer relationships. house. The same activities may be seen as involving the
From these earliest concerns (Piaget, 1948), it has been personal domain by adolescents, with the room seen as
suggested that the influence process between peers dif- their territory (Smetana & Daddis, 2002).
378 Socialization Processes

Parenting Goals fluenced by evolutionary psychology, propose that dif-


ferent social domains are organized by distinctive algo-
A number of socialization theorists have been con-
rithms, and that they operate in the accomplishment of
cerned with the role of parental goals as sources of vari-
social tasks that have had adaptive significance across
ability in their socialization practices across contexts.
cultures and species. The greatest utility of this ap-
Parental goals serve to moderate the ways in which par-
proach for socialization theory follows from the im-
ents behave (including their affective responses) on dif-
plicit need for different types of parental practices in
ferent occasions. From this perspective, parents will
different social domains.
employ different tactics in different settings depending
Although different theorists have developed different
on what they hope to achieve. Attention has been di-
taxonomies, five domains have regularly been repre-
rected to the specific nature of parental goals (e.g., Dix,
sented (four are directly relevant to the discussion of so-
1992; Hastings & Grusec, 1998). So, for example, Dix
cialization processes; Bugental, 2000):
(1992) suggested that parenting practices vary on the
basis of the activation of (a) parents personal goals
(goals that are relevant to efforts to obtain child obedi- 1. Protective care: Interaction in this domain is organized
ence), ( b) parents empathic goals (goals focused on by mechanisms (e.g., proximity-maintenance) that pro-
satisfying childrens emotional needs), and (c) parents vide for the safety and feeding of dependent offspring.
socialization goals (goals based on the motivation for 2. Coalitional groups: Interaction in this domain is or-
children to learn culturally defined values). Hastings ganized to facilitate the establishment and mainte-
and Grusec (1998) focused on shifting parental prac- nance of shared benefits from an in-group and shared
tices as a function of a similar clustering of goals: (a) defense against threat from outsiders.
parent-centered goals (fostering power assertion), ( b) 3. Hierarchical power: Interaction in this domain in-
relationship-centered goals (fostering warmth, negotia- volves the management of control between individu-
tion, and cooperation), and (c) child-centered goals (fos- als who differ in social dominance and resource
tering reasoning). holding potential.
4. Reciprocity/mutuality: Interaction in this domain in-
Features of the Child as Determinants of volves the regulation of matched benefits between
Socialization Effectiveness functional equals.
In addition to observing that agents of socialization may 5. Mating: Interaction in this domain serves to facilitate
have different outcomes they wish to achieve, socializa- the selection and protection of access to high-value
tion theorists have also had to incorporate the differen- sexual partners.
tial susceptibility of different kinds of children to
socialization experiences in different domains. Thus,
the impact of particular socialization practices depends BIOLOGICAL PROCESSES THAT CREATE
on a number of variables including the social behavior in THE PLATFORM FOR SOCIALIZATION
question (e.g., violations of moral or social conven-
tions), the age and sex of the child, the sex of the parent, In this section, we review the biological mechanisms
and, as noted earlier, the childs temperament and mood that can be thought of as creating the platform for the
(Grusec & Goodnow, 1994). processes that are involved in socialization. At one level,
there are shared genetic influences that reflect the
Domains of Social Life
brains best guess as to the kinds of social experi-
A number of theoretical approaches have emerged that ences the young will have. In addition, there are genetic
are specifically concerned with the nature and func- variations in children in the extent to which they are re-
tioning of the different domains of social life. Domains ceptive to socialization (considered later in our integra-
may be conceptualized as representing bodies of knowl- tion of biological and sociocultural theories). We
edge that act as guides to partitioning the world and organize our understanding of the biological platform
that facilitate the solving of recurring problems faced for socialization in four social domains that appear to
by organisms in that world (Hirschfeld & Gelman, have distinctive features (introduced in Bugental &
1994). These approaches, which have typically been in- Goodnow, 1998).
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 379

Processes in Different Domains sion across their offspring (and thus optimizing their
own reproductive success). Their interests may be
For each domain, we describe (a) the basic tasks of the thought of as competing. Mothers may have many chil-
domain, ( b) the ways in which parents and the young are dren, and the level of care provided for one child must be
biologically prepared for the domain, and (c) the ways in balanced against the level of care provided for other chil-
which interaction in a domain serves to accomplish dren. Children, however, cannot choose alternative care-
proximal tasks (e.g., time-limited tasks characteristic of givers; as a result, they are prepared to initially maintain
a particular stage of development) as well as providing contact with the individual (or individuals) who are
experiences beneficial for tasks that emerge at later ages. available to them in a caregiving role.

Protective Care Domain


What Are the Tasks of the Protective Care Do-
The protective care domain is the domain that most main? The proximal tasks of the protective care do-
uniquely involves parents and the young in a relation- main are (a) maintenance of safety and nourishment
ship. Although there are variations in the extent to (provisioning) of the young before they are able to do so
which such a relationship may be established with other for themselves and ( b) comforting of the young follow-
caregivers, mothers are the most extensively prepared ing stress. The long-term tasks of this domain are (a) fa-
for this relationship as a result of biological processes. cilitation of the stress regulation ability of the young
Fathers (among those species that involve cooperative and ( b) facilitation of the childs understanding of and
care) are also prepared for this relationship, as a result competence in the caregiving relationship.
of biological processes, but somewhat differently and
more variably than are mothers. How Are the Young Prepared for the Protective
The provision of care to the young must be thought of Care Domain? The protective care system (regulated
as conditional in nature: Parents invest more in those by attachment processes) represents a co-evolved sys-
offspring who are more likely to serve their reproductive tem that involves the distress system of the young and
interests; that is, they are more likely to invest in those the protective system of the caregiver (Bowlby, 1969;
children who are likely to grow up to have healthy chil- Panksepp, 1996). Under naturally occurring circum-
dren of their own (Bugental & Beaulieu, 2004; Wilson & stances, the reunion of mother and infant not only al-
Daly, 1994). In addition, parents (without conscious lows safety but also, simultaneously, nursing access.
awareness) weigh their investment in one child against However, attachment processes are linked to stimulus
their investment in other offspring (or potential off- features of the mother rather than to provision of nour-
spring in the future). Parents also invest more in the ishment (e.g., Harlow, 1973).
young when they have the resources to do so (e.g., access
to food and economic resources or access to supportive Experience-Expectant Responses of the Young. Because
others who will facilitate the process of parental care). the protective care domain needs to be operational at
To some extent, the processes involved in protective birth, most of the timing mechanisms that regulate it are
care serve the shared interests of parents and their off- online immediately (or in some cases, even prior to
spring. For the infant, the relationship is a safety-mainte- birth). Very early in life ( beginning on the 1st day), in-
nance system that is essential for survival and normal fants show unique sensitivities to the kinds of auditory
development. For the parents, the relationship, in promot- and visual stimuli that are species-typical (and thus are
ing the survival and healthy development of the young, likely to be characteristic of parents). The ability to
enhances the replication of their own genes. At the same quickly recognize their parents is important for survival
time, the protective care system also involves tasks that in that it insures that infants will attempt to maintain
are not shared between parent and child (Trivers, 1974; contact and proximity with those individuals who are
Wilson & Daly, 1994). Human offspring have a very long most likely to provide for their care.
period of dependency and therefore there is a need for an As an example, infants are highly receptive to facial
extended period of parental care: The infants best inter- cues, and may recognize their mothers face, as early as
ests are served by obtaining the largest share possible of 2 days of age (Field, Cohen, Garcia, & Greenberg,
the parents provision of care; in contrast, the parents 1984). Although it was once believed that this process
best interests are best served by dividing their care provi- suggested an innate face recognition mechanism, it is
380 Socialization Processes

now thought of as a privileged learning process in which number of children, their attraction to this domain is
there is high visual engagement with faces early in life broadly based, including attraction to the young (in par-
(Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991). This pro- ticular, infants) as a whole. It is important for parents to
cess fosters the development of relevant neural struc- be attracted to the stimulus features that characterize
tures. In the same way, infants appear to show a the young if they are to become invested in their care.
privileged learning process in identifying their mothers One of the most general stimulus cues to immaturity
voice. Indeed, the human fetus shows an ability to rec- is appearance. Humans have been found to show a very
ognize the mothers voice. Kisilosevksy et al. (2003) ex- general attraction to the facial configuration that char-
posed human fetuses to a tape recording of the voice of acterizes immaturity, as in the very general attraction
their own mother or the voice of a female stranger read- to baby-faces (Zebrowitz, 1997). In addition, the
ing the same passage. The fetuses heart rate was found human cry has the capability of eliciting very general
to increase in response to the voice of their own mother attention from others (Ostwald, 1963). Some sex differ-
but decrease in response to the voice of a female ences are present, however, in the ways in which men
stranger (differential responses that are consistent with versus women respond to the distress calls of the young.
reactions to familiar versus unfamiliar stimuli). At Nonparental males typically respond with heart rate de-
birth, this early preparation is demonstrated by the fact celeration in response to such signals, whereas non-
that neonates show a preference for their own mothers parental females are more likely to respond with heart
voice (DeCasper & Fifer, 1987; Ward & Cooper, 1999). rate acceleration (Lin, 1999; Power, Hildebrandt, &
Finally, infants quickly come to recognize their Fitzgerald, 1982). These differences may be adaptive in
mothers smellan odor that is associated with breast that heart rate increases are more consistent with
milk (Bartoshuk & Beauchamp, 1994; Schaal, 1988). preparation for action (e.g., provision of care), whereas
This sensitivity allows infants to easily locate the heart rate decreases are more consistent with vigilance
mothers breast. (e.g., monitoring the environment for the source of the
infants distress).
Activation of Distress Signals. Unlike precocial ani- Adults share the tendency to interact with the young
mals, the human infant is unable to move away from the using prosodic features that are distinctive to this do-
mother in the first few months of life. It is not until the main. The properties of this style of speech has been re-
infant becomes mobile (during the third trimester of the ferred to as infant-directed speech (Fernald et al.,
1st year of life) that distress calls are shown in response 1989), which is a speech style observed very generally
to separation or the presence of a stranger. Even though across language groups (Kitamura, Thanavisuth, Burn-
the care of the young shows many different patterns ham, & Luksaneeyanawin, 2002). Infant-directed speech
across cultures, the age at which distress calls emerge is (IDS) has been found to be a uniquely effective means of
quite regular (Konner, 1972). In the same way, the dis- providing comfort to the distressed infant (Papousek &
tress call system is also deactivated at a later age. Papousek, 1995). IDS may serve the goals of socializa-
Among nonhumans, such calls are deactivated in re- tion at younger ages and language acquisition at later
sponse to increasing levels of testosterone in pubescence ages (Kitamura et al., 2002).
(Herman & Panksepp, 1981). Changing levels of testos-
terone among human adolescents may possibly con- Prenatal and Perinatal Preparation for Parental Care.
tribute to declines in their dependency on parents at this Preparation for parental care needs to be considered
time (Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986). separately for females and males. Biological prepara-
tion of females for the mothering experience begins pre-
How Are Parents Prepared for the Protective natally (among both human and nonhuman mammals).
Care Domain? Preparation for the protective care do- During (and immediately following) pregnancy, there
main occurs at different levels. This includes both gen- are hormonal changes that prepare the mother for partu-
eral preparation and specific preparation at particular rition and lactation among both humans and other
time periods. mammals (e.g., oxytocin, prolactin). In addition, there
are hormonal changes that lead to increases in the
Generalized Preparation for Protective Care. Because prospective mothers interest in the young and the pro-
parents are prepared for caregiving relationships with a vision of care. In this chapter, we are primarily con-
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 381

cerned with hormonal influences on the mothers social Recognition of the Infant. After birth, it is important
responses to the young. that mothers quickly come to recognize their offspring. In
Among human mothers, there are increases in the doing so, they show privileged learning. For example,
levels of cortisol, estradiol, and prolactin across the human mothers very quickly learn to identify the sound of
course of pregnancy (Bridges, 1996; Coe, 1990; Storey, their own infants cry (Green & Gustafson, 1983; Wiesen-
Walsh, Quinton, & Wynne-Edwards, 2000). Of these feld, Malatesta, & De Loach, 1981). Fathers also show
hormones, circulating levels of cortisol during preg- some level of privileged learning in the recognition of
nancy appear to be most clearly related to subsequent their infant cries; however, their accuracy is considerably
maternal behavior (Storey et al., 2000). Elevated cortisol less than that of mothers. In addition, parents autonomic
levels predict higher levels of approach behaviors in re- responses are different in response to the cries of their
sponse to infants (e.g., patting, cuddling, or kissing the own versus unrelated infants (Wiesenfeld et al., 1981).
baby; talking, singing, or cooing to the baby; Fleming, Oxytocin may also play a role in the easy recognition
1990). In addition, those women who show higher levels of ones own offspring (among both humans and nonhu-
of cortisol are more likely to recognize and be attracted mans). Oxytocin has been found to facilitate social
to the odor of their own newborn infant (Fleming, recognition processes in general; for example, elevations
Steiner, & Corter, 1997). Among human mothers, other in oxytocin increase the ability of rodents to recognize a
hormonal changes appear to be more associated with the conspecific only seen in a brief encounter (Popik & van
physiological processes of pregnancy rather than with Ree, 1991), with influences appearing to be at the level
maternal responses (Storey et al., 2000). of the hippocampus and the amygdala (Ferguson, Aldag,
Storey, Walsh, Quinton, and Wynne-Edwards (2000) & Insel, 2001; van Wimersma, Greidanus, & Maigret,
found that men and women showed equivalent levels of 1996). Findings are consistent with the general observa-
stage-specific changes in hormonal levels; for example, tion that the brain processes social stimuli differently
both show higher levels of prolactin and cortisol late in than nonsocial stimuli (Young, 2002).
the pregnancy. Men who showed higher levels of reac-
tivity to infant stimuli were found to show the highest Coregulation of the Protective Care System.
levels of prolactin increase. In addition, new fathers Regulation of the protective care domain involves key
with higher prolactin levels were more alert and more time-limited tasks to be accomplished, as well as the so-
positive in response to infant cries than were those with cialization of the young for later life experiences. In-
lower prolactin levels (Fleming et al., 2002). Similar deed, attachment relationships, which develop as a
benefits (in parental care) were found in both of these result of the infants need for care and protection, are
studies as a result of declines in males testosterone often not included in discussions of the topic of social-
(and associated aggressive tendencies) across the preg- ization. Such relationships are included here on the
nancy of their partner. These findings are interpreted as basis that the ways in which the tasks of this domain are
showing that prospective fathers hormonal changes be- carried out will differentially affect the ability of the
fore and at the time of their childs birth facilitate their young to manage stress, as well as their ability to pro-
demonstration of parental care. At a more general level, vide parental care themselves, at later times. Therefore,
married men with children have lower testosterone lev- in discussing coregulation processes, we are simultane-
els during the day than do unmarried men or married ously concerned with proximal and distal tasks.
men without children (Gray, Kahlenberg, Barrett, Lip-
son, & Ellison, 2002). Safety Maintenance. Early protective care involves a
Shared parental care of the young is typically greater dyadic system maintained by the parent (typically the
in species that are also monogamous (Yogman, 1990), mother) and the child in this relationship. Caregiving rela-
and shared parental care is maintained by continued tionships, if they are to be adaptive, require the coordi-
contact with mates. Suggesting the effects of this expe- nated activity of the neurohormonal systems as well as the
rience on gene expression, males experience in provid- behavioral responses of both individuals. As a caveat, it
ing care for the young may also lead to changes at the should be noted that the activation of hormones in re-
level of the brain: The medial preoptic area (an area that sponse to stressful experiences need not act as a continu-
is important for maternal behavior) becomes similar to ing regulator of protective care systems. Ultimately such
that of females (Gubernick, Sengelaug, & Kurz, 1993). response systems come to operate efficiently with
382 Socialization Processes

minimal cues. This short cut occurs as a function of the fants show direct benefits as a result of direct touching
involvement of the amygdala. The initial activation of contact with their mothers. For example, preterm infants
stress hormones in response to separation has an effect on in neonatal intensive care units have been found to
the amygdala, which plays an important role in memory demonstrate significant increases in the presence of cir-
consolidation, as described by McGaugh (2002): The ba- culating beta-endorphins in response to skin-to-skin con-
solateral region of the amygdala plays a crucial role in tact with their mothers (Mooncey, Giannakoulopoulos,
making significant experiences memorable (p. 456). Glover, Acolet, & Modi, 1997). In addition, preterm in-
The operation of protective care is best observed in re- fants have been found to show a variety of benefits (e.g.,
sponse to separation or presence of threat and reunion. decreases in cortisol levels or weight gain) in response to
When separated from their mothers, nonhuman infants massage (Field, 1998; Kuhn & Schanberg, 1998).
(typically based on observations of rats and nonhuman pri- The protective care relationship also serves to facili-
mates) show hormonal changes consistent with physiologi- tate the acquisition of self-regulation skills. Evidence
cal stress responses (e.g., Hofer, 1996; Keverne, Nevison, & with respect to the benefits of protective care comes
Martel, 1997; Laudenslager, Boccia, Berger, & Gennaro- from both human and nonhuman research. The research
Ruggles, 1995). Their responses include increases in the program of Meaney and his colleagues (e.g., Francis,
levels of corticosterones that are produced, combined with Caldji, Champagne, Plotsky, & Meaney, 1999; Meaney,
declines in their production of beta-endorphins. In response Aitken, Bodnoff, Iny, & Tatarewicz, 1985) has demon-
to these changes, their level of distress calls increases. Such strated the long-term regulatory advantages of high lev-
calls are well-known triggers to maternal recovery and els of early maternal care: Pups who received extra
caregiving responses, as mediated by hormonal responses licking and grooming in response to distress demon-
(e.g., increases in production of oxytocin). strated an enhanced ability to cope with later stress.
Although such research informs us about processes in Specifically, they more quickly habituated to novelty. In
nonhuman species, questions may be raised about equiv- addition, the experience of high maternal care predicted
alent processes among humans. It is useful then to con- the willingness of offspring to move out to explore the
sider the supportive evidence (albeit nonexperimental) environment without fear. As is true with humans, early
that may be observed in relationships between mothers provision of reliable protection acts to foster later capa-
and infants. Infants have been found to show cortisol in- bilities for autonomy. In the same way, human infants are
creases in response to 30 minutes of separation from unable to self-regulate either their behavior or emotional
their mother (Gunnar, Larson, Hertsgaard, Harris, & states in the first few months of life and use the mother
Brodersen, 1992). Spangler and Grossman (1993), as- as an as external organizer of their biobehavioral regula-
sessing infant response to the Strange Situation (with tion (e.g., Spangler, Schieche, Ilg, & Maier, 1994).
more limited separation times), did not find a general
pattern of cortisol increase; instead such increases were Preparation of the Young to Become Parents. Finally, the
limited to infants who were insecurely attached. They protective care relationship provides a learning experi-
did, however, find increases (across attachment groups) ence that produces effects when the young grow up to be-
in heart rateanother indicator of distress. come parents themselves. Again, we turn to nonhuman
Among both humans and nonhumans, reunion is asso- research to identify some of the mediating mechanisms
ciated with increased affiliative activity. Among hu- that may operate here. Francis, Diorio, Liu, and Meaney
mans, this includes the infants positive greeting and (1999) conducted a program of research exploring the
clinging. However, cortisol responses to reunion depend cross-generational effects of early maternal care. A com-
on attachment style. If the attachment relationship is in- parison was made between a strain of rats in which moth-
secure, cortisol levels continue to be high following re- ers engaged in high levels of maternal care (e.g., licking
union (Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson, & Nachmias, and grooming) versus one that engaged in low levels of
1995; Spangler & Grossman, 1993). care. To separate out the effects of genetic influences
from experiential ones, the rat pups were cross-fostered
Regulatory Systems. The protective care relationship by a different strain of rat (i.e., rats from a high
serves both directly and indirectly to foster stress regu- lick/groom strain were reared by mothers from a low
lation among the young: It serves to both buffer against lick/groom strain, and vice versa). Rat pups were then
stress and ultimately facilitate self-regulatory skills. In- tracked to observe the parental care they provided to their
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 383

own young. The parental care provided by the second gen- group derogation /aggression (Brewer, 1999b). Therefore,
eration reflected the care they had received from their it is useful to consider the biological preparation for these
foster mother (the experience route) rather than the care processes separately. Indeed, the developmental course
that was provided by their biological mother to other pups of relevant processes appears to be different for the two
(the genetic route). This suggests the importance of early aspects of this domain.
care as a precursor to effective parenting at a later age. For the protective care domain, we focused exclu-
Among humans, the attachment literature has provided sively on the relationship between parents and the young.
abundant evidence for the cross-generational transmission For the socialization of the coalitional group domain, we
of attachment styles. A high degree of convergence has focus on the socialization processes both by parents and
been found between the attachment styles of mothers (as by peers. Although some of the same processes may fol-
reflected by responses to the Adult Attachment Inter- low in both cases, there may also be differences.
view) and the attachment styles of their own infants
(Benoit & Parker, 1994; Ward & Carlson, 1995). How Are the Young Prepared for the Coalitional
Group Domain? There is an emerging body of work
Summary. More than is true for any other social that suggests the time course for (a) recognition of and
domain, parents and the young are strongly prepared at a investment in maintaining in-group similarities and ad-
biological level for the tasks of protective care. The tight herence to group rules and ( b) for the identification of
programming of this domain can be understood by the the features of out-groups and the generation of negative
centrality of its role in the early survival of the young. responses to those groups.
Among humans, mothers and fathers are both prepared
prior to the birth of offspring for parental care. At a gen- Preparation for the In-Group. Imitation or mimicry of
eral level, they show interest in infant stimuli. More the actions of others appears to occur automatically in
specifically, hormonal changes during pregnancy in- both humans (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999) and group-living
crease the probability that they will provide appropriate nonhumans (Macdonald, 1983). One of the central func-
care of the young. The young, in turn, are designed to tions of social groups is mutual defense through the abil-
easily come to recognize their parents and to signal them ity of groups to intimidate potential predators (through
in response to distress (with separation being a proto- their appearance as a large cohesive entity as opposed to
typical elicitor of distress). The hormonal responses and an aggregation of individuals who can be more safely at-
signaling systems of mothers and infants are closely co- tacked). Among humans, mindless mimicry occurs soon
ordinated to optimize both retrieval and relief from dis- after birth. At the most basic level, a neural basis has re-
tress. In addition, protective care in infancy also acts to cently been found for the capacity of primates to share
socialize the child for later experiences: It serves to in- their experiences (Gallese, Ferrari, Kohler, & Forgassi,
fluence the ability of the young to regulate their own re- 2002). Mirror neurons have been found in which equiv-
sponses to stress and to prepare them for the provision of alent neural firings occur when an individual carries out
protective care when they themselves become parents. an action and when he or she observes that same action
being executed by another individual. These neural
Coalitional Group Domain processes may underlie the operation of imitationa pro-
The young are also prepared for adaptive functioning cess that is manifested on the 1st day of life (Meltzoff &
within coalitional groups. This process involves biological Moore, 1999). Late in infancy, categorization processes
design as well as selective inputs from parents and peers. lead to a perceptual use of group entities: Children begin
to show both that they respond to groups of objects and
What Are the Tasks of the Coalitional Group Do- that they expect groups to move together, which is a core
main? The central tasks of the coalitional group do- feature of social coalitions (Sugarman, 1983).
main are (a) the mutual acquisition and sharing of By 2 years of age, children became receptive to the
resources within in-groups and ( b) mutual defense rules of group lifethe proper ways of executing
against external sources of threat (including out-groups). eating, dressing, cleanliness, politeness, and other con-
Although this sounds like a coordinated system, there is ventional routines (e.g., Dunn & Munn, 1985; Emde,
good evidence in support of the independence of in-group Biringen, Clyman, & Oppenheim, 1991; Smetana,
favoritism, provision of group-based benefits, and out- Kochanska, & Chuang, 2000). This is also the point that
384 Socialization Processes

children show self-aware emotions (e.g., shame, guilt). ated with more complex use of social categories, along
The possibility now exists for self-regulation to occur in with increases in social comparison processes (Rholes,
response to violation of group norms. Newman, & Ruble, 1990). Children now respond differ-
There are indications that childrens increasing ently to the implications of different groups. For exam-
awareness of and concern with family conventions is ac- ple, thoughts of death lead to increasing in-group bias
companied by a positive valuation of broader in-groups, (Florian & Mikulincer, 1998), which is a response con-
for example, their own racial group (other ways of defin- sistent with the protection offered by their own group.
ing us). During these early years, children show a
strong in-group attachment /favoritism (Aboud, 2003; How Are Humans Generally Prepared for the
Cameron, Alvarez, Ruble, & Fuligni, 2001) but do not Coalitional Group Domain? A central prerequisite
show a negative out-group bias. As noted earlier for the functioning of the coalitional domain is the recog-
(Brewer, 1999b), in-group favoritism appears to repre- nition of social similarities and differences. However,
sent a different process than out-group hostility/deroga- there does not appear to be any biological design for the
tion. Aboud determined that in-group attachment is specific kinds of the classical coalitional group catego-
particularly apparent during early childhood. Indeed, it rizations (e.g., racial or ethnic groupings). Instead, these
has been argued that there is a stronger evolutionary are groupings that are arbitrary and are not acquired
preparation for in-group cohesiveness than for out-group until there is a readiness to do so (at the point that the
hostility (Hirschfeld, 2001). This may follow from the child becomes involved in group life beyond the family
fact that in-group coalitions were highly adaptive at all and needs to be sensitive to groupings that have signifi-
ages in our distant evolutionary past, whereas out-group cance for that particular group at that particular time).
hostility only became adaptive when children moved Although infants show a very early ability to catego-
further away from the family and needed to be aware of rize others based on sex (or age), as measured by the ha-
potential dangers from neighboring groups. bituation paradigm, they show no equivalent ability to
categorize others by such visually distinctive but arbi-
Preparation for the Out-Group. A full understanding trary features as color (Fagan & Singer, 1979; Leinbach
of out-groups does not appear until middle childhood. It & Fagot, 1993). Kurzban, Cosmides, and Tooby (2003)
is not until children are at least 5 years of age that they found through the use of a memory confusion paradigm
first begin to show an association between in-group fa- (confusing two exemplars from the same social category
voritism and negative bias to out-groups (Aboud, 2003). in memory) that humans are quite rigid in their catego-
As observed by Hirschfeld (1996), younger children rization of others by sex but are much more flexible in
may know a group label (e.g., a racial label) but do not their categorization of others by race. It has been persua-
easily categorize others or respond to others perceptu- sively argued that humans are designed by their evolu-
ally on the basis of that label. By middle childhood, they tionary history to make categorizations by sex (and age),
demonstrate out-group hostility and active segregation which has shared significance across time and setting;
processes based on the arbitrary ways in which children however, their categorization of others by other group-
are grouped. As noted by Hirschfeld (1996): Children ings is arbitrary. The arbitrary nature of group catego-
do not find races because they are there to be found. rization is reasonable in light of the ecology that
They find races because they are following an impulse characterized human associations in the distant evolu-
to categorize the sorts of things there are in the social tionary past. Because of mobility constraints, humans
world (p. 345). Supporting this notion, children at this typically came in contact with other groups who could
age show group biases (favoring their own group over an not easily be distinguished or categorized on the basis of
out-group) when they are arbitrarily distinguished (e.g., appearance. Instead, they had to make use of and become
by color of shirts worn; Bigler, Spears Brown, & sensitive to subtle markers that distinguished a group
Markell, 2001). Children from minority groups have that might pose an immediate threat to their own groups
been identified as at particularly high risk for bullying resources. Thus, it is likely that (as is still true univer-
(Prothrow-Stith & Quaday, 1996). sally) the coalitional group made use of deliberately cre-
By middle childhood, there is increasing maturation ated markers (variations in appearance) or variations in
of the frontal cortex (Stauder, Molenaar, & Van der ways of communicating. As the simplest contemporary
Molen, 1999), which facilitates the childs ability to in- example, think of the colors worn and hand signals used
tegrate information. This change is temporally associ- by gang members to promote recognition.
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 385

Humans are also hormonally sensitive to the relative older ages, these self-aware emotions have been found to
resource advantage (win-loss) of their own group versus have autonomic consequences, which may influence the
a competing group. Among males in particular, the ex- nature of the childs response to rule violations. Shame
perience of winning (or watching their team win) is associated with immediate increases in activation of
leads to elevation of testosterone, whereas the experi- the sympathetic nervous system, consistent with energy-
ence of losing (or watching their own team lose) leads to mobilization and perceived threat (e.g., increases in
declines in testosterone (Dabbs, 2000). heart rate and blood pressure, Harrald & Tomaka,
2002), followed by a rapid transition to parasympathetic
Coregulation of the Coalitional Group Domain. activity, consistent with energy conservation and with-
At the point the young demonstrate an awareness of drawal (Schore, 1998). Gilbert and McGuire (1998) have
their own first group (the family), they are receptive suggested that the parasympathetic-demobilization re-
to socialization processes regarding the defining charac- sponse (associated with shame in humans) is part of an
teristics and routines of that group. Dunn and her col- ancient, basic defense response that signals the individ-
leagues (e.g., Dunn & Brown, 1991; Dunn, Brown, ual to stop.
Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991; Dunn & Munn, The strong motivation to comply with group rules
1985) studied mothers as those most likely to be in- may have its origins in the fear of exclusion, which is
volved in socialization of the rules of social life in the one of the most intense fears present at later ages
United Kingdom and the United States. In other cul- (Baumeister & Tice, 1990; Caporael & Brewer, 1991).
tures, it is often the case that peers serve as the primary The group provides an extremely important source of
socializers of group rules (Harris, 1995). safety and loss of group support provides a threat to
Although the content of the rules is taught by others, safety. Supporting this notion, depletions in the opioid
the acquisition of those rules does not appear to occur as system (which leads to declines in feelings of safety)
a result of reinforcement; instead, the rules are easily in- motivate approach to the group (Panksepp, Siviy, &
ternalized without the influence of external conse- Normansell, 1985). This feature of groups may provide
quences. Between the ages of 2 and 4 years, children the primary basis for in-group preference and cohesion.
have been observed to display ritualistic and repetitive It might be speculated that as the childs dependence on
behavior (Evans et al., 1997). Their focus on the proper the mother for safety declines, the dependence on the
display of conventional routines has an almost obsessive coalitional group for safety emerges.
quality to it (Emde et al., 1991). Children of younger or By middle childhood, when children are able to cate-
older ages do not display the same ritualistic style in the gorize others into arbitrary groups (as defined by others
enactment of everyday activities such as eating, going to at that time), they are then subject to the possibility of
bed, or getting dressed. Thus, there appears to be a win- responding negatively ( jointly with members of their
dow of opportunity in which mothers (or others who act own group) to those out-groups. At this age, there is a
to socialize the child with respect to group norms) are high level of stereotyping and prejudice against
able to easily and effortlessly exert behavior control racial /ethnic out-groups (Bigler, 1999). As noted by
over the young. Bigler, the shift from more virulent forms of racial bias
During this time period, children come to believe that to more subtle forms of bias in society as a whole has not
there is a certain way that we (the family) do things been reflected in a comparable softening of responses
and there is resistance to efforts to change established shown by children. In addition, no significant relation-
routines. This may be thought of as the first indication ship has been found between the biases of parents and
of sensitivity to the in-group and the ways in which that those of their children; for example, parents who show
group is defined by how members act and what they say little bias often have children who hold strong racial
and what their routines are. What is seen is the combina- stereotypes (Aboud & Doyle, 1996). Without encour-
tion of a positive valuation of the familys way of doing agement from adults, children show aggression to arbi-
things and a concern with violation of those rules trarily created out-groups when those groups offer
(which, at a broader level, implies the possibility of competition for scarce resources (Sherif, Harvey,
group disapproval or exclusion). White, Hood, & Sherif, 1954).
Although mothers rarely punish children for their
failures in carrying out accepted routines, children ap- Summary. Biological preparation for coalitional
pear ashamed or guilty when they dont get it right. At group processes (including both the preparation for the
386 Socialization Processes

socialization of the conventional rules that act to main- relationships in childhood help to prepare the young for
tain similarity with ones own group and a devaluing of the successful negotiation of power-based relationships
and hostility toward those in other groups) occurs in a at later ages. The accomplishment of this task requires
set of steps. Sensitivity to the conventional rules of preparation for (a) the appropriate use of dominant re-
group life, along with in-group preference, emerges at sponses (affording the opportunity for acquiring/and
an age when children become subject to self-aware emo- maintaining resources) and ( b) the upper-limit control
tions, such as shame, an emotion that may be triggered over dominant responses (i.e., a shut-off mechanism that
by violation of group rules or standards and that has prevents lethal harm to kin). As suggested by Kelley and
powerful physiological effects (parasympathetic demo- Thibaut (1978), interdependent relationships, even when
bilizing responses). Although parents (as well as peers) they involve unequal resources or power, typically in-
are models and advocates of conventional rules, such volve benefit to both parties.
rules are likely to be acquired through spontaneous in- Parents, siblings, and unrelated adults or children
ternalization. Intergroup hostility, on the other hand, may all act to socialize hierarchical power relationships.
appears to be under the control of peer, rather than The negotiation of power relationships among the young
parental influence. themselves represents an omnipresent activity across
species, in particular among young males.
The Hierarchical Power Domain
The hierarchical power domain is centrally concerned How Are the Young Prepared for the Hierarchical
with the establishment and management of interpersonal Power Domain? The preparation of the young for
relationships that are unequal in the resource holding power-based relationships is shared for some features
potential (RHP) of interactants. The basic notion of but sexually dimorphic for others. All children need to
RHP includes not only the relative dominance or formi- be prepared to respond to the warning signals of adults to
dability of two individuals but also their relative access avoid harm. Even infants show an early preparation for
to desired objects or events (Parker & Rubenstein, power messages contained in the speakers vocal proper-
1981). Unlike protective care or coalitional relation- ties. Infants are sensitive (and reactive) to the vocal pro-
ships, processes in this domain are unstable and may in- hibitions of adults. Fernald (1993) has demonstrated the
volve frequent renegotiation of relationships (whereas universality with which young infants respond with be-
protective care or coalitional relationships tend to be havioral inhibition to vocal prohibition signals (vocal-
relatively stable once a bond is established to an individ- izations that involve short, loud, and staccato prosody).
ual or group). This infant response pattern is shown whether adult
Humans (as is true for other species) are motivated to messages are voiced by their mothers or voiced by unre-
acquire or maintain their RHP. Some level of dominance lated women speaking a different language.
and access to resources is necessary for survival. The Across many species, there is also a biologically pre-
motivation for dominance is more salient for males than pared sensitivity to cues to physical power or formida-
females ( because the relative power of males has greater bility. Infants, across species, show greater fear in
implications for successful mating). However, females response to unfamiliar adult males than to unfamiliar
are also motivated to maintain a favorable RHP in that adult females (Cheney & Seyfarth, 1985; Skarin, 1977).
higher access to resources provides benefits not only for As pointed out by Cheney and Seyfarth, sensitivity to
themselves but also for their young. unfamiliar males (among nonhumans) precedes sensi-
tivity to predators, suggesting the high importance of
What Are the Tasks of the Hierarchical Power Do- this response. In addition, there is a high level of conti-
main? As is true for other domains, there are both nuity in response to power signals across ages (Keating
proximal and distal tasks to be accomplished in the hier- & Bai, 1986).
archical power domain. At a proximal level, the success- As well as the general preparation for power-based
ful regulation of this domain acts to keep the child safe relationships, there is also sexually dimorphic prepara-
from harm (and thus simultaneously serves the repro- tion of the young for this domain. First of all, there ap-
ductive interests of parents). In accomplishing this task, pears to be some level of prenatal preparation for the
the child is prepared for wariness and submission to power domain among males as a function of the in-
power cues from others. At a distal level, power-based trauterine hormonal environment. The presence of ele-
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 387

vated levels of androgens (male sex hormones) late dur- human primates have both informational and motiva-
ing gestation or shortly after birth acts to predict later tional properties (Marler, Evans, & Hauser, 1992). As
sex differences that are relevant for this domain noted earlier, Fernald (1993) has demonstrated that
(Breedlove, 1992; Mazur & Booth, 1996). Although mothers produce a distinctive prohibition vocal signal to
questions have been raised about this process among hu- preverbal infants as a means of stopping some ongoing
mans (see Ruble & Martin, 1998), there are indications action. Such signals are often used by mothers to pro-
that, across species, these early hormonal differences hibit touching an object that may pose a hazard to the
organize the architecture of the brain in ways that are child, and they have the advantage in that they stop the
manifested in behavioral patterns at later ages. When fe- infants behavior before the child is old enough to under-
males are exposed prenatally to androgens (either stand speech. This signaling system carries the advan-
through the introduction of exogenous androgens or as a tages of effective inhibition of dangerous actions and
result of naturally occurring hormonal anomalies), they the preparation of the child for associated prohibitional
show subsequent social responses that are closer to speech (No!). In short, it allows human parents to
those usually shown by males, including rough and tum- bootstrap off of a biologically prepared sensitivity to fa-
ble play. Among nonhuman primates, there are also dif- cilitate their tuitional role. Thus, the accomplishment of
ferences in the nature of their social signals in response a proximal task may also extend to the parents later use
to threat. Nonhuman primate females are more likely to of communications that serve in a socializing role.
produce vocalizations that act to recruit attention (and
help) from kin. Males ( beginning as juveniles), although Regulation of Rough-and-Tumble Play. A distal task of
they are at much greater risk, are less likely to engage in the hierarchical power domain involves the preparation of
vocal signaling behavior (Bernstein & Ehardt, 1985). the young for power engagements at older ages. Juvenile
Across the course of postnatal development, sex differ- play (in particular, rough-and-tumble play) serves to
ences also emerge in the presence of testosterone, a differ- safely prepare the young for later more serious types of
ence that becomes most striking at puberty. At this age, hierarchical power encounters. This play style, much more
the increasing levels of testosterone among males allows common among boys than girls, begins in early childhood.
for more serious forms of power assertion, including ag- Although social play does not constitute aggression, it
gression. There are also some indications that the chang- does prepare the young for later relationships that carry
ing hormonal environment of the young at pubescence may the potential for aggression. The nature of rough-and-
lead to an altered relationship with adults in a position of tumble play is typically managed (by children themselves)
power. A number of investigators (e.g., Inoff-Germain to be 50/50 in wins and losses (Pellis, 2002). In peer rela-
et al., 1988; Moffitt, 1993; Susman et al., 1987; Udry & tionships, this balance is fairly easily attained.
Talbert, 1988) have found that that antisocial behavior in Adaptive timing features are shown in the deactiva-
the young (e.g., resistance to authority) rises with in- tion of the playful power struggles of the young. The
creases in testosterone, even when the visible signs to the clock for manifestations of rough-and-tumble play is
individuals stage of puberty are statistically controlled. set in such a way as to maintain the utility of this power-
relevant social response pattern: Rough-and-tumble play
Coregulation of the Hierarchical Power Domain. follows a well-defined course during its development
The coregulation of the hierarchical power domain across species. The activation pattern represents an in-
serves multiple purposes. These include the maintenance verted U function in which this play style increases and
of child safety and regulation of rough and tumble play. then declines late in middle childhood (Panksepp,
1993). These declines are associated with increases in
Use of the Power Domain in Maintaining Child Safety. the presence of testosterone. At this point, young males
The regulation of the hierarchical power domain in- would be more likely to inflict serious physical harm in
volves the accomplishment of tasks that serve proximal the course of such play bouts (thus decreasing their util-
goals as well as the accomplishment of tasks that serve ity). It appears that increases in testosterone at the onset
to prepare the child for later power engagements. As one of pubescence mediate the declines in playful types of
of the proximal tasks, the safety needs of the young are power struggles. Before this age, rough-and-tumble play
met by parental regulation at a distance when infants be- involves a dopamine-based reward system in which par-
come mobile. The vocal signals of both human and non- ticipants (in particular, males) experience this activity
388 Socialization Processes

as positive (Panksepp, 1993). In contrast, serious (rather young to optimize their power position is critical. For
than playful) aggression between the young may be reg- example, males experiencing social defeat (as opposed
ulated by testosterone (Sanchez-Martin et al., 2000). to nonsocial stress) show hypercortisolismstrong in-
Among humans, fathers (as well as juveniles) show creases in the activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-
an involvement in social play with the young. At one adrenal (HPA) axis (Abbott et al., 2003). As noted
level, social play provides preparation for novel events, earlier, social defeat or subordination also leads to in-
an important aspect of socialization (Bekoff & Allen, creases in noradrenaline, a change viewed as a de-esca-
2002). More relevant to the current discussion, fathers lating response pattern. In contrast, those individuals
are differentially involved in play activity (rough-and- who achieve higher levels of power show higher levels of
tumble play) that may serve to prepare the young for re- serotonin relative to others (i.e., a hormonal advantage).
lationships that involve power asymmetry (Jacklin, Although most of the evidence for this hormonal corre-
DiPietro, & Maccoby, 1984). In doing so, they provide late of power has come from nonhuman primate research
experience with different distributions of power. For ex- (e.g., Raleigh, McGuire, Bramner, Olkkacjm, &
ample, at one time, the father may win disproportion- Yuviller, 1991), similar patterns have also been found
ately; at other times, he can shift the balance of power in with humans (e.g., Madsen, 1986).
the play relationship in such a way that the young win
disproportionately (Bekoff & Allen, 2002). These expe- Summary. The experiences of the young in the hi-
riences train the child for ways of negotiating power- erarchical power domain serve the childs immediate
based relationships at later ages: They provide needs (e.g., responsiveness to parents in safety regula-
experience with winning and losing, including the sub- tion at a distance) and set the stage for later relation-
missive or dominance behavior associated with both ships. Their experiences are orchestrated by an
roles. They also provide experience in escalating the integrated system of social signals and hormonal pat-
challenge posed to a more dominant individual as a terns. Preparation for the power domain reveals both uni-
means of testing to see whether they can increase their formities and variations between males and females.
hierarchical position. Fathers provide realistic ( but Beginning in infancy, both males and females are re-
safe) consequences when a juveniles play responses be- sponsive to the power signals of adults. However, the pre-
come too intense. In this way, the young also learn natal hormonal environment of males selectively
upper-limit control over fighting with those of their own influences their preparation for physically competitive
coalitional group. For example, nonhuman primate fa- encounters (e.g., rough-and-tumble play). Rough-and-
thers make use of play bites that are limited in intensity tumble play at younger ages is hormonally regulated in
(Bekoff & Allen, 2002), which is an upper-limit control ways that are consistent with positive play and provides
that is learned by the young. Power contests are used to safe experience in power negotiation. In contrast, serious
establish hierarchical position (rather than destruction involvements in power contests (more common by pu-
of another person), and therefore modulation of aggres- berty) are regulated by testosterone. The engagement of
sion is adaptive. fathers in social play with the young also provides an
As an indication of the significance of fathers for early training role for the effective management of fu-
their childrens socialization in this domain, Flinn, ture relationships involving power asymmetry.
Quinlan, Decker, Turner, and England (1996) found hor-
The Mutuality/Reciprocity Domain
monal effects that followed from father absence. In par-
ticular, the endocrine responses of sons (more than As the last social domain to be considered, humans
daughters) were sensitive to the presence or absence of (along with other primates) are prepared by biological
the father. Sons whose fathers were absent, although processes for interactions based on mutuality or reci-
showing low cortisol levels during infancy, demon- procity. Such relationships are based on the mutual pro-
strated a combination of high cortisol and low testos- vision of benefits between functional equals. Kin
terone levels in adolescence. This combination is selection theory (Hamilton, 1964) offers an easy ac-
consistent with a subordinate role in power-based rela- count of the reasons why an individual benefits by the
tionships with peers. provision of aid to kin: The provision of benefits to kin
The long-term occupation of a subordinate role car- simultaneously serves to benefit a persons own repro-
ries high costs; thus, the presence of preparation for the ductive success. Less easily explained is the provision of
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 389

benefits to nonkin. However, when the relationship be- Adamson, 1984). This process involves the infants ac-
tween individuals is based on the possibility of recipro- tive coordination of his or her interest in some object or
cal benefits, such relationships can be seen as adaptive event with the ongoing attentional engagement and in-
(Cosmides & Tooby, 1992). Reciprocity in social rela- tentions of another person. This new capability involves
tionships has been described as the hallmark of primate what may be thought of as coordination or matching of
behavior, although some forms of reciprocity have occa- motives. At this point, the young can coordinate their
sionally been observed in lower species (Wilkinson, activities with others with a new level of planning and
1988). Among primates, reciprocity extends past simple purpose. This capability can be understood as important
tit-for-tat short-term exchanges, and is reflected in an in the negotiation of reciprocal or shared activities.
elaborate cost-benefit accounting that transcends spe-
cific behavioral currencies or contexts (Silk, 1992). Theory of Mind. By age 4, children have moved one
Friendships (an important example of mutual/recipro- step further in the MR domain by showing that they now
cal relationships) are distinct from other kinds of peer in- understand that others act on the basis of their beliefs,
teractions (Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). Most notably, even when those beliefs are false, that is,they have ac-
friendships are characterized by high levels of reciprocity quired a theory of mind. This capacity allows the child
and intimacy (although intimacy is more commonly linked to understand the thoughts and desires of others in es-
with female than male friendships). In addition, the bene- tablishing mutually beneficial interactions. Secondary
fits that follow from peer associations are greater for representational capacity (e.g., pretense, means-end rea-
friends than nonfriends (Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). soning), thought to reflect an early stage in the develop-
ment of theory of mind, has been observed among the
What Are the Tasks of the Mutuality/ Reciprocity great apes, suggesting some limited level of continuity
Domain? The proximal tasks of the mutuality/reci- across species (Suddendorf & Whiten, 2001).
procity (MR) domain include (a) the provision of mutual
benefits with unrelated others by sharing and recipro- What is the General Preparation for the Mutual-
cating tangible and/or social-emotional benefits and ( b) ity/ Reciprocity Domain? The regulation of the MR
the collaboration of two individuals to accomplish a goal domain appears to involve basic computational and neu-
that neither one can reach alone (e.g., coordinated activ- ral mechanisms for which there is an evolutionary de-
ity in reaching a goal or defending against an opponent). sign. Trevarthen, Kokkinski, and Flamenghi (1999) have
The long-term tasks of the MR domain include prepara- suggested that humans are born ready to reciprocate in-
tion of the young by parents for mutual /reciprocal rela- teractions with others. Indeed, Forman and Kochanska
tionships with their peers. (2001) have observed a correlation between young chil-
drens willingness to imitate their parent in a teaching
How Are the Young Prepared for the Mutuality/ context and to comply in a control context, and they sug-
Reciprocity Domain? In early infancy, infants show gest that both reflect a responsiveness or receptive
pleasure in response to contingency; for example, they stance toward parental socialization. Unlike automatic
smile when an object moves in response to their actions mimicry or imitation in the coalitional domain, imita-
(e.g., Watson & Ramey, 1987). Later during the 1st year, tion in the MR domain appears to involve intentional
they react to contingently responding objects as though response matching or turn-taking and may be better
they are human. For example, Johnson and her colleagues thought of as emulation (a distinction in concepts that
(Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991) found that, has been proposed by Want and Harris, 2002).
by 1 year of age, infants react to an object (even when it is At a higher level, humans (and other primates) appear
a nondescript blob) as having intentions after it has been to have a cheater detection mechanism that provides a
observed responding contingently to another person (e.g., sensitivity to violation of implicit social contracts (e.g.,
making contingent sounds or flashing a light in response providing benefits to another but not receiving benefits
to the speech of another person). in return; Cosmides & Tooby, 1992). When other indi-
viduals fail to reciprocate benefits, they are subject to
Joint Attention as the Basis for Coordination. By 1 penalties in the withholding of future benefits by the in-
year of age, children show the ability to share attention dividual who has been cheated. Over a program of re-
and to solicit the joint attention of others (Bakeman & search, Cosmides and Tooby have demonstrated the
390 Socialization Processes

greatly enhanced ability of individuals to solve complex premature infants in establishing such dialogues (e.g.,
logical problems (the Wason task) when they are framed Lester, Hoffman, & Brazelton, 1985).
as social contracts. The later initiation by mothers of ritualized MR
games (e.g., peekaboo) also serves to further prepare the
Coregulation of the Mutuality/ Reciprocity Do- infant for reciprocal, coordinated activity (Parrott &
main. The ability of young children to engage in suc- Gleitman, 1989). Thus, children who were observed to
cessful MR relationships is facilitated by their early engage in high levels of mutuality in play (play initia-
interactions with parents (in particular, mothers). tions and compliance to initiations) were found to be
more socially competent with peers, which was a rela-
Early Parental Socialization in MR Relationships. Be- tionship found to be mediated by increases in childrens
fore infants engage in peer interactions, they may be emotional knowledge (Lindsey, 1998; Lindsey, Mize, &
thought of as having a practice period in which their Pettit, 1997). In short, mutuality in interactions with
skills in joint regulation processes develop and are parents provides an opportunity for guided practice in
honed. Although children may be thought of as experi- mutual /reciprocal interactions with peers.
ence-expectant for MR interactions, a guided appren-
ticeship with parents, involving jointly coordinated Coregulation with Friends. The basic processes that
interactions, may be needed before this domain is fully operate to regulate the MR domain are in place when
operational. However, this finding may be limited to so- children first form social ties with other children
cieties in which mothers (rather than peers) play a pre- (processes that are shared with nonhuman primates).
dominant role in the early play activities of the young. Some of the mechanisms are fully operational early on,
Thus, there are variations across cultures in the exten- whereas others come online with childrens increased
siveness of the early role of parents as opposed to peers cognitive ability. At the most basic level, the interactive
in the socialization process. system and physiological regulatory systems of friends
Suggestive support for the experience-expectant na- ( beginning when children are only toddlers) appear to
ture of MR interactions comes from research conducted be coordinated. Among toddlers, this concordance ap-
in the still face paradigm. The exchange of eye contact pears in their baseline heart rate and cortisol levels as
and smiles is an important part of the early relationship well as their play behaviors (Goldstein, Field, & Healy,
between mothers and infants, and is associated with pos- 1989). This suggests that young children become syn-
itive affect. For example, a high level of contingent re- chronized with each other not only in their attention and
sponsiveness has been shown by mothers ( by their facial actions but also at a physiological level.
expressions) to the facial expression of their infants dur- As one outcome of successful MR relationships with
ing nursing (Leveille, Cossette, Blanchette, & Gaudreau, peers, the child is buffered against stressful experiences,
2001). If the mother violates the infants expectations by both at a direct and an indirect level. At the most direct
displaying an immobile face, children show heart rate in- level, friends serve to buffer the child against bullying by
creases, a decrease in vagal tone, and negative affect other children (Schwartz, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2000;
(Weinberg & Tronick, 1996). The level of distress shown Smith, Shu, & Madsen, 2001). By the alliance with a
by infants in this setting indicates the significance of friend, children are able to accomplish something that
mutual interactional engagement with the mother. they could not do on their own. More indirectly, the pres-
Another early manifestation of parental guidance in ence of a friend serves to buffer against stress: Children
MR processes is seen in the vocal communication be- show reduced levels of stress (as reflected in cortisol lev-
tween mothers and infants. At around 3 or 4 months of els) when a preferred friend is present (Field et al.,
age, infants are ready for coordinated vocal dialogues 1992). In addition, childrens level of norepinephrine in
with parents that for the first time involve turn-taking response to painful medical experiences has been found
(e.g., Papousek & Papousek, 1995), through a dialogue to be buffered by their level of perceived support from
in which parents take the lead. Consistent with the af- friends (Hockenberry-Eaton, Kemp, & DiTorio, 1994).
fective consequences of mutuality/reciprocity in rela- A long-term benefit of early involvement in MR ties
tionships, vocal dialogues between infants and mothers with peers is the increased ability to engage in affilia-
are regularly associated with positive affect. The impor- tive activity of many kinds (e.g., Lindsey, 2002; Vaughn,
tance of the infants stage of cortical development for Colvin, Azria, Caya, & Krzysik, 2001). Early ties may
such exchanges is suggested by the deficits shown by provide an internship in which there is an opportunity to
Biological Processes That Create the Platform for Socialization 391

more fully develop both the regulation of mutual bene- tached children (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
fits and the more complete understanding of the minds Wall 1978). At the same time, the response strategies
and feelings of others. For example, young children who characterized by insecurely attached children may be
have a stable and mutual friendship were shown to out- optimal for maintaining contact with a particular type
perform, on a theory of mind task, children who lacked of mother (Cassidy, 1994; Simpson, 1999). At a more
such a relationship (Peterson & Siegal, 2002). Finally, general level, such children may develop an opportunis-
MR relationships also serve to facilitate positive emo- tic rather than a stable pattern of close relationships. In
tional states, as mediated by both the opioid system and mating relationships, this plays out in the form of short-
the dopamine system, which are involved in social re- term mating strategies. For example, avoidantly at-
ward (Depue, Luciana, Arbisi, Collins, & Leon, 1994; tached adults tend to become involved in unstable
McClelland, Patel, Stier, & Brown, 1987; Panksepp, short-term sexual relationships that include low parental
1993; Vanderschuren, Nissick, & Van Ree, 1997). investment (Brennan & Shaver, 1995; Simpson, 1999).
Mating occurs early and in a way that maximizes the
Summary. The MR domain serves to accomplish number of progeny.
mutually beneficial relationships between unrelated in- In addition, there are other more direct cues early in
dividuals who have a peer relationship. Such relation- life that are diagnostic with respect to future life experi-
ships facilitate the shared provision of social-emotional ences. For example, a high level of early stress (e.g.,
benefits (e.g., generation of positive affect or reduction parental divorce or marital conflict) or lack of resources
in stress), joint collaboration in shared defense (e.g., may signal a harsh future (Belsky et al., 1991). Such ex-
against bullying), or task accomplishment. In addition, periences have been found to be associated with faster
humans (and other primates) have demonstrated the ca- rates of sexual maturationa pattern that might influ-
pacity to keep track of the provision of reciprocal bene- ence mating and parental investment strategies in ways
fits with specific others, thus facilitating the likelihood that maximize the number of progeny early in life.
that an equitable, mutually beneficial relationship will These formulations are not without their critics (Mac-
be maintained. The capacity for higher-level MR func- coby, 1991). In addition, there are indications that pu-
tions (complex coordination or mutual understanding) bertal timing as a result of early experience follows
follows as children develop cognitively. Early in in- more from the benefits associated with positive-harmo-
fancy, children may be thought of as expecting the expe- nious family relationships rather than the costs associ-
rience of mutuality/and reciprocity. Mothers respond by ated with negative-coercive family relationships (Ellis,
facilitating the infants affectively positive engagement McFayden-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1999):
with reciprocal and mutual interaction (e.g., coordi- Girls who experienced a more positive early history
nated communication activities or games). (e.g., greater paternal investment or more mother-
daughter affection) showed later pubertal onset.
Biological Preparation for
Different Environments Integration and Segue

Some researchers have been concerned with the extent In this section, we have considered the ways in which bi-
to which children come to demonstrate an adaptive fit ological processes prepare both the child and the parent
with the experienced environment. From a biological for the domains of social life. We have observed that the
standpoint, this is thought of as facultative polymor- brain is experience-expectant. As such, there is a pre-
phism: The young are designed to respond in adaptive pared sensitivity to social experiences as well as privi-
ways to variations in the world to which they are born. leged learning in acquiring the social knowledge that is
For example, Belsky, Steinberg, and Draper (1991) pro- essential to accomplishing the basic tasks of social life.
posed that early social experiences provide the young In reviewing these processes, we have suggested that
with information that is diagnostic with respect to the there are meaningful distinctions in both the tasks and
kinds of environments they will face later in their lives. the mechanisms that regulate interactions in different
In a facultative fashion, the young may then select re- social domains. However, the domains cannot be seen as
sponse strategies best suited to those probable environ- entirely distinctive in that some tasks are accomplished
ments. Thus, mothers who are low in sensitivity and by one domain at one stage of development (e.g., safety
high in rejection are more likely to have insecurely at- provided by parents in infancy) and by another domain
392 Socialization Processes

at later stage (e.g., safety provided by coalitional groups cial context and of social experiences on development. In
at later ages). However, there are meaningful distinc- so doing, we keep in mind the biological foundation de-
tions in the time course, the mechanisms, and the hor- scribed in the preceding pages of this chapter. The bulk
monal and emotional processes that serve to regulate the of empirical knowledge about socialization processes and
different domains. mechanisms, however, comes from decades of research
We have given considerable attention to the role of hor- conducted in a different framework. This perspective ac-
mones in the preparation of the young to function adap- knowledged the existence of biological pressures and pre-
tively in different domains. Such processes may be dispositions but focused on the actions and attitudes of
thought of as key mediators in the preparation of the young agents of socialization and the impact of these actions
and their caregivers for the socialization experience. and attitudes on childrens behavior, cognitions, and
We have also suggested that evolutionary history (the emotions. We provide an overview of the research using
mechanisms that have evolved to solve recurrent prob- that framework. What is added in the present discussion,
lems in human history) provides some of the basic de- however, is an attempt to see how the findings from a di-
sign features of socialization. We have also pointed out rection that has historically largely characterized the
that evolutionary history prepares individuals for the re- study of socialization can be considered in the domain
current variations (as well as the regularities) in their framework proposed in the previous section. Such a dis-
experiences. For example, there have been variations in cussion will reveal large gaps in knowledge that need to
the resources available to parents and in the characteris- be filled. At the same time, it may help to organize and to
tics of the young. As a result, there are variable strate- make sense of the existing data.
gies available for optimizing the reproductive success of In this section we consider how agents of socializa-
parents and the young in different ecologies. tion operate to produce individuals who fit into their
Socialization is accomplished most effectively when cultural milieu. The focus is on human research. In
parents are able to read the changing states of their chil- these points of focus, different kinds of linkages can be
dren, as well as the changing motives and capabilities of drawn with the biological platform discussed in the last
children across the course of development. In the frame- section. In cases where there is a high degree of simi-
work offered here, this process may also be thought of as larity in processes across sociocultural contexts, the
involving the parents ability to shift domains in appro- linkages are clearer; for example, the central features
priate ways. So, for example, if an infant is distressed, ef- of the parental care domain show a high level of conti-
fective parenting involves parental responsiveness to the nuity across cultures, species, and presumably across
childs state and the ability to activate responses consis- human history. In other cases, the linkages are less ob-
tent with the protective care domain. In contrast, if a vious. As humans have accommodated to circum-
parent responds to a distressed child with efforts to en- stances that would have been less central in their
gage him or her in a reciprocal game (a domain mis- evolutionary past, socialization processes have arisen
match), parenting is more likely to be unsuccessfulthe culturally to manage contemporary needs. For exam-
child will not be calmed and the parent will not succeed ple, processes relevant to the socialization of inde-
in efforts to engage the child. pendence would have been less relevant in highly inter-
In short, biology creates a platform on which socio- dependent hunter-gatherer societies. Nonetheless, biol-
cultural influences are built. Acceptable variations in ogy places some level of constraints on socialization:
socialization processes are afforded by virtue of the fact Socialization practices that focused too much or too
that the evolutionary history of humans has provided al- early on autonomy, without sufficient concern for in-
ternative designs for the different kinds of environments terdependence, would be likely to meet with resistance.
they have faced. Similarly, those who ignored the biologically based
need for some form of separation from the group and
attempted to inhibit autonomous action would also be
SOCIOCULTURAL APPROACHES likely to meet with resistance.
Much of the focus in this section, as in the previous
We move now from a discussion of the biological mecha- section, will be on parents as the primary agents of so-
nisms that operate during socialization to a different cialization who prepare children for their social roles.
level of analysisa consideration of the impact of the so- The focus on parents is because, as discussed earlier,
Sociocultural Approaches 393

children and parents comprise a biosocial system that is life. Finally, we discuss the role of cultural context as an
set up to favor parents as having a heavy investment in important qualifier of the effects of different socializa-
child rearing and children as being equipped very early tion practices.
on to respond to parental cues, including such events as
recognition of the face, voice, and smell of their primary
Dimensions of Socialization
caregiver. A fundamental need of children is for secu-
rity and protection, and parents are in a unique position Contemporary models of personality and social devel-
to satisfy this need. Indeed, the need for protection opment frequently converge on two critical aspects of
forms a basis for the development of a strong relation- human functioning: (1) the ability to develop and main-
ship between parent and child and, as will be shown, the tain close interpersonal relationships and (2) the growth
quality of relationship between child and agent of so- of self-definition or autonomy (e.g., Blatt, 1995; Deci &
cialization is of major significance in ensuring the suc- Ryan, 1991; Grotevant & Cooper, 1986). This echoes
cess of socialization practices. Moreover, the parent- the continuing theme of shared regulatory processes and
child relationship is fixed and immutable, unable to be self-regulatory processes in the last section. Thus, in the
altered or terminated except under the most unusual cir- context of relatedness to others, infants, children, and
cumstance. In addition to being constrained by legal and adolescents acquire the ability to function as separate
kinship definitions, this relationship, because of the and independent individuals who make their own deci-
long period of dependency of child on parent, also de- sions and guide their own behavior. These two processes
mands long histories of interaction and facilitates the of interpersonal relatedness and self-definition develop
development of routinized patterns of interaction that synergistically so that, as one unfolds, changes in the
foster accommodation to family values and expectations other are enhanced. In attachment, for example, as in-
(Collins, Gleason, & Sesma, 1997). As well, parents are fants develop a feeling of security with their caregivers
usually the local representatives of the social mores and they become more ready to explore their environment
therefore society formally assigns them the task of so- and their own mental states. This exploration promotes a
cialization. In this way, society and biology work to- more detailed sense of self, which promotes the ability
gether to designate the family setting as the primary to relate to others (Blatt, 1995). Those individuals who
context in which children are prepared to function in the are optimally developed maintain relationships without
larger social milieu. From a practical perspective, the losing their autonomous view of selfstriving to
prolonged period of dependency and close contact be- achieve individuality without sacrificing links to others.
tween the parent-child dyad also motivates parents to in- Those who emphasize interdependence with others and
still appropriate and desirable behavior in children with sacrifice autonomy are alleged to develop a dependent
whom they have to share a comfortable daily existence. personality style, whereas those who emphasize self at
And, finally, close and frequent contact between parent the expense of relationships are alleged to develop a
and child affords opportunities for parents to monitor self-critical personality style (Blatt, 1998). In addition,
their children and to come to understand and anticipate the two processes assume differential importance in dif-
their beliefs and actions (conditions that are essential ferent contexts (Brewer, 1999a).
for successful socialization). The concepts of interdependence or interrelatedness
We begin this section with a discussion of the basic and autonomy appear consistently in treatments of so-
dimensions of interrelatedness and autonomy in the so- cialization practices as researchers have tried to under-
cialization processtraditional concerns of socializa- stand how children learn to fit in with the social group
tion theory. These map well on to the socialization but become in some sense independent of or differently
domains already discussed because they involve both dependent on that group. Thus, there is a continuing in-
different forms of interaction with others and separa- terest in maintenance of the protective care, coalitional
tion from others. It is the latter that is necessitated by group, hierarchical power, and MR domainsbut in
the inequality that is inherent in the hierarchical power changing ways across the course of development and
domainan inequality that does not exist in the other shifting settings.
domains. We then move on to a discussion of different Socialization involves continued close ties to others,
socialization practices and strategies that are employed but decreasing power imbalances. The caveat to be sig-
in the implementation of the different domains of social naled here is that the notion of independence from the
394 Socialization Processes

group is a culturally bound one and that much thinking Protection and warmth expose children to socializa-
and research about interrelatedness and autonomy has tion attempts by keeping them near socialization agents
been conducted in a middle-class and Western European and attentive to their actions. But protection may be of
cultural context. The extent to which existing notions particular significance in the socialization of emotion
about interpersonal relatedness and self-definition and regulation because of the focus on negative affect and
their interactions are universal is an open question that distress. Warmth may play a more prominent role in
we address later in this section. To anticipate, however, shared actions and shared identity that is part of the
current conceptions seem to be that these are universal coalitional domain. Thus, being a valued and accepted
aspects of human development and functioning, albeit member of the group (as conveyed through social inclu-
with different expressions as a function of the ecological sion processes) may motivate conformity to the stan-
niche in which individuals currently find themselves. dards of that group both to confirm a feeling of
belongingness and to avoid alienation and separation as
Interdependence/Interrelatedness
well as anxiety about threat from outside.
Already discussed is the importance of the caregiver-
child dyadic relationship in the parental care domain: Autonomy and Control
Attachment is a major foundation of social development, The basis of successful socialization is the creation of a
promoting safety, comfort in response to distress, emo- positive relationship with parents or other agents of so-
tional self-regulation, and trust in others. Attachment is cialization that fosters a willingness or desire to be re-
not the only form of relationship that children develop ceptive to their directives. Central here is the notion of
with socializing agents (Belsky, 1999; Goldberg, willingness, with its implication that behavior is self-
Grusec, & Jenkins, 1999; Thompson, 1999). For exam- directed or autonomous. Children are responsive be-
ple, in addition to providing protection to their children cause of positive aspects of the relationship they have
in the face of threat and stress, parents can demonstrate with socialization agents. A considerable part of social-
warmth and verbal and physical affection in their inter- ization, however, involves control and the imposition of
actions with them. They can spontaneously hug and standards of conduct, and it is here that the hierarchical
praise their offspring, frequently without any obvious power domain comes into play. Indeed, control has his-
eliciting action on the part of the offspring. Warmth has torically been the major focus of attention for socializa-
played an important role in early conceptions of social- tion researchers. Often, but not always, the desires of the
ization and identification (e.g., Sears, Maccoby, & child and the parent are somewhat or distinctly at odds,
Levin, 1957) and operates in a different way from pro- and one goal of socialization for parents is to make the
tection in facilitating the socialization process; for ex- childs desires more concordant with their own. This has
ample, making it pleasurable for children to reproduce to be done, however, in a way that encourages the childs
the actions of nurturant and rewarding agents of social- feelings of autonomously directed action. Parents have
ization. Warmth, however, is governed by a different the advantage in this particular situation because they
system from that which operates in the attachment do- control more resources (although children do have many
main (MacDonald, 1992; Goldberg et al., 1999) and pro- resources in the relationship involving their ability to
vides a different mechanism for assumption of social provide gratification to the parent; see Kuczynski, 2003;
roles. For example, the provision of protection and re- Rheingold, 1969). The challenge is to achieve concor-
sponsiveness to the childs distress makes a unique con- dance between adult and child desires, or at least some
tribution to the prediction of childrens negative affect degree of concordance, in a way that minimizes antago-
and empathy and prosocial behavior when the effects of nism and maximizes willing compliance and a feeling
warmth are controlled for, whereas warmth does not on the part of the child that behavior is self-directed or
when the effects of responsiveness to distress are con- autonomously chosen. Indeed, the existence of conflict
trolled for. Moreover, warmth makes a unique contribu- and its resolution is frequently considered to be an es-
tion to the prediction of childrens regulation of positive sential ingredient in the development of autonomy and
affect when the effects of responsiveness to distress are self-regulation (Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Yau &
controlled for, whereas responsiveness to distress does Smetana, 1996) given that it sharpens the distinction
not when the effects of warmth are controlled for (Davi- between self and other and helps in the achievement of
dov & Grusec, in press). self-direction.
Sociocultural Approaches 395

Events in the hierarchical power domain are compli- clarification and consistency of limits in child rearing
cated by the fact that, as described earlier, relationships (Baumrind, 1971; Baumrind & Thompson, 2002). Those
change in a far more complex way than in the other do- who see it as detrimental to the childs development re-
mains. Although parental care, coalitional, and MR do- gard it as coercive and synonymous with the use of force
main relationships all change with childrens expanding to gain compliance, an approach that undermines willing
competencies and interests, relationships in the hierar- and autonomous action on the part of the recipient (e.g.,
chical power domain are particularly likely to change as Deci & Ryan, 1991). The meaning of the term needs fur-
children become less willing to accept control in areas ther clarification. The most recent way in which this has
that they now come to see as personal and where they occurred is through a distinction between psychological
consider intervention by authority figures to be less ac- and behavioral control (Barber, 1996, 2002; Steinberg,
ceptable (Smetana, 1988, 1997). In the hierarchical 1990; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992).
power domain, there is constant negotiation and compro-
mise as socializers and objects of their socialization try Psychological versus Behavioral Control. A historical
to find a middle ground between what the two are will- description of approaches to control is helpful in under-
ing to tolerate (although the extent to which this occurs standing the confusion surrounding its meaning. In early
may be culturally determined; e.g., Trommsdorff & Ko- analyses of socialization, control carried a substantially
rnadt, 2003). Driving this particular aspect of develop- negative connotation. Levy (1943), for example, argued
ment is the latters changing notion of autonomy and the that parents whose own emotional needs had been unmet
formers adjustment as to which actions can be tolerated in childhood developed attitudes toward their own chil-
or negotiated (Goodnow, 1994). Always the focus is on dren that reflected these unmet needs. These attitudes
autonomy and self-regulation and the childs acceptance manifested themselves either in overprotection and in-
of standards of action and conduct as self-regulated and trusiveness in their child-rearing practices or in the be-
self-imposed. Without such acceptance, socialization is havioral opposite of rejection. Schaefer (1965), in his
deemed to be unsuccessful, arousing anger, reactance, treatment of control or, more specifically, the dimension
and hostility (M. Hoffman, 1970). Indeed, support for of psychological control versus psychological autonomy,
the emphasis placed by socialization theorists on auton- treated it as involving intrusiveness, parental direction,
omy and self-regulation emerges from findings by bio- and control through guilt. For Baumrind (1971), control
logical researchers who, as noted earlier, have found was an important feature of both authoritarian and au-
that the long-term occupation of a subordinate role has thoritative parenting, but the nature of the control was
high costs with its accompanying detrimental levels of not distinguished. Disaggregation of authoritarian and
cardiovascular strain and activation of the HPA system authoritative parenting into three partsacceptance,
(Abbott et al., 2003). Thus, there is a strong biological behavioral control, and psychological controlhas
basis for the traditional position that successful social- helped to make sense of when control is positive and
ization requires an approach that minimizes feelings of when it is negative in its consequences (e.g., Steinberg,
subordination and force. And obviously, in this unstable Elmen, & Mounts, 1989).
system, there are developmental differences in what is Behavioral control, the positive aspect of control,
perceived to be subordination. Indeed, there may be sex refers to the rules, regulations, and restrictions that par-
differences as well, with boys more likely to resist au- ents impose on their children as well as their knowledge
thority as their testosterone levels increase and they be- or awareness of their childrens activities that is ob-
come more aggressive (Panksepp, 1993). tained in an active way through inquiry and observation.
It focuses on control over daily actions, and includes
Varieties of Control. Control is a complex vari- parental monitoring of the whereabouts of their children
able, which can, for example, be used in either an auton- and of the people with whom they associate. Psycholog-
omy-supportive or an autonomy-destructive way. This ical control, in contrast, refers to control that under-
feature of its use has led to considerable confusion in mines the childs autonomous development. It includes
analyses of socialization, as researchers and theoreti- parental intrusiveness, guilt induction, and love with-
cians have focused on either the harmful or beneficial drawal. Psychologically controlling parents are not re-
consequences of a clearly multifaceted construct. Those sponsive to the psychological needs and emotions of
who view control as positive, for example, emphasize their children; their actions imply derogation of the
396 Socialization Processes

child and they interfere with the childs establishment of the production of guilt, or in an overprotectiveness of
a sense of identity (Barber, 1996, 2002). Psychological the child. Each of these features of psychological con-
control seems more relevant to the emotional climate in trol may have different outcomes that remain to be elu-
which parenting is conducted. cidated (Morris et al., 2002). Consider, for example,
High levels of psychological control predict both ex- suppression of independence of thinking, or what
ternalizing problems and internalizing problems (e.g., has been labeled by some as intrusive control (e.g.,
anxiety, depression, and loneliness) and difficulties Grolnick, 2003; Pomerantz & Eaton, 2001). Adults
with academic achievement, low self-esteem, low self- who are intrusively controlling are overbearing and in-
reliance, and self-derogation. In contrast, low levels of hibiting in their interventions with children, allowing
behavioral control have been linked with externalizing them little choice in their actions. They do not, how-
problems, including drug use, truancy, and antisocial ever, engage in emotional manipulation. Intrusively
behavior (Barber & Harmon, 2002). Moreover, the evi- controlling agents of socialization value compliance,
dence seems to indicate that, in accord with a bidirec- pressure children to engage in specified outcomes, and
tional analysis, problematic children and adolescents, to do not engage in discussion and verbal give-and-take.
some extent, elicit these parenting behaviors but that In spite of the fact that they inhibit a childs autonomy
parental behaviors also promote problem behaviors in and feelings of choice, their intrusiveness can be an in-
children (e.g., Dodge & Pettit, 2003; Laird, Pettit, dication of caring. Moreover, under certain conditions,
Bates, & Dodge, 2003). Although the consequences of intrusive control can be predictive of positive child out-
psychological and behavioral control are opposite in comes (Pomerantz & Eaton, 2001).
their valence, it appears that the two forms of control
are not simply opposite ends of the same continuum: Autonomy Support versus Intrusive Control. Intrusive
Behavioral control culminates in increased social re- control appears to overlap with the autonomy-threatening
sponsibility and impulse control, whereas psychological style of intervention described by Deci, Ryan, and their
control has its outcome in the development of negative colleagues (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1991; Grolnick, Deci, &
self-processes. The motivation behind parental use of Ryan, 1997) in their discussions of self-determination
psychological control is not the enhancement of social theory. According to self-determination theory, children
responsibility in children but the domination and ma- require structure or the clear setting out of rules and ex-
nipulation of emotional and psychological boundaries pectations. Structure can be imposed, however, in either
between parent and child in a way that works to satisfy an autonomy-supportive or controlling way. Even in high-
the needs of the parent. In this sense, it has more to do risk, dangerous environments where parents impose more
with the nature of the parent-child relationship than it rules and restrictions (a practice that does not work so
does with the imposition of demands for socially ac- well in low-risk environments at least with respect to the
ceptable behavior. Finally, low psychological control development of childrens social competence), those who
and psychological autonomy are not synonymous: Al- do it in a democratic way and provide explanations have
though a parent may not be psychologically controlling, children who are more competent (A. Baldwin, Baldwin,
a parent may not engage in the kind of supportive be- & Cole, 1990). Nor does autonomy support suggest lack
havior that facilitates autonomy either (Barber, Bean, & of parental involvement. High involvement and high
Erickson, 2002). autonomy support together foster the most positive out-
comes academically and socioemotionally. High involve-
Psychological Control Elaborated. Although the dis- ment with low autonomy support, however, stifles
tinction between behavioral and psychological control development, whereas low involvement deprives children
marks a significant step forward in understanding of of resources and assistance (Grolnick, 2003). Further re-
socialization processes, the story does not end here, finement in an understanding of intrusive control also
leaving room for still further distinctions. Note, for ex- requires attention to the specific areas in which control is
ample, psychological control includes a variety of so- exerted. For example, adolescents who rate parents as re-
cialization practices that may have somewhat different strictive in areas deemed by the adolescents to be per-
impacts on childrens social, emotional, and cognitive sonal issues, as opposed to moral or social conventional,
development. Specifically, psychological control can rate their parents as higher in psychological control than
manifest itself in several different waysthrough the do those who see those same issues as legitimately sub-
suppression of independence of thinking, through ject to parental authority (Smetana & Daddis, 2002).
Sociocultural Approaches 397

A significant feature of autonomy support is the provi- tivities with children (Waizenhoffer, Buchanen, &
sion of choice (Grolnick, 2003). Some caution is in order, Jackson-Newsom, 2004), and conversations with teachers,
however, with the observation that too much choice can peers, and other parents (Crouter, Helms-Erikson, Upde-
be overly challenging. Here, the evidence comes from graff, & McHale, 1999). For young children monitoring
studies that show adults have difficulty managing large involves direct supervision by parents themselves or by
amounts of choice (Dhar, 1997; Shafir, Simonson, & others, whereas for older children and adolescents more
Tversky, 1993; Shafir & Tversky, 1992). When faced distal forms of monitoring, such as parent-initiated con-
with a large number of alternative possibilities, they find versation and imposition of rules, are the norm. Many in-
the experience enjoyable but feel frustrated and dissatis- vestigators have found significant relations between
fied with the alternatives they have chosen (Iyengar & parental monitoring and positive child outcomes (see
Lepper, 2000). It does not stretch the imagination to sug- Crouter & Head, 2002, for a review), presumably in part at
gest that similar outcomes may exist for children. There least because monitoring or knowledge of their childrens
comes a point when making choices is so demanding that activities enables parents to ensure that their children are
individuals may defer to the more expert opinion of oth- not exposed to influences that would have a detrimental
ers, including agents of socialization. effect on their socialization. (Again, the linkage is bidi-
rectional, with high levels of delinquent behavior predict-
Guilt. Possibly more harmful than intrusive control is ing decreases in parental knowledge, in addition to low
parental production of guilt. Some time ago, M. Hoffman levels of parental knowledge predicting increases in delin-
(1970) suggested that parental withdrawal of love was a quent behavior; e.g., Laird et al., 2003). An extension of
practice that promoted neurotic guilt. Children who the concept of monitoring comes with the notion of col-
were recipients of love withdrawal felt guilt from aware- lective socialization (Brody et al., 2001) that alludes to
ness of unacceptable impulses rather than from harm neighborhood monitoring and refers to feelings of trust
done to others. In a similar vein, Grolnick et al. (1997) and cohesiveness in neighbors that promote agreement
allude to introjected regulation, a form of internaliza- about what is acceptable behavior. Brody et al. found that
tion of social standards that have been taken in by the collective socialization, or the willingness of adults to
child but are maintained in their original form. Thus, the monitor and supervise the behavior of both their own chil-
regulation that results from this form of internalization dren and that of other members of the community, com-
is internally, rather than externally, imposed and is not bined with positive parenting, was associated with a
integrated with the self and thereby becomes a source of reduction in childrens associations with deviant peers,
tension and inner conflict. As an example, introjected particularly as neighborhoods became more disadvan-
regulation is apparent in individuals who pressure them- taged. They hypothesize that collective socialization re-
selves to do well on a test rather than to perform well be- duces the negative impact of deviant peers by decreasing
cause it is important to be academically proficient or the amount of antisocial behavior in the community, by de-
because achievement is an integrated or cohesive part of termining appropriate standards of conduct, and by pro-
their self-concept. Withdrawal of love seems to be par- viding more opportunities for supervision of children.
ticularly implicated in the production of neurotic guilt Although monitoring, as noted earlier, is seen as at-
and may also reflect emotional manipulation that threat- tention to and tracking of a childs activities and the set-
ens feelings of being respected and included as an im- ting of limits, Stattin and Kerr (2000) point out that it
portant member of the social group (Eccles, 2002): In has been frequently operationalized simply as knowl-
this way, it moves into or takes advantage of events in the edge of the childs activities and whereabouts. They
coalitional group domain, involving the desire to be an suggest that knowledge can come from one of three
integral and valued member of the group. sources: (1) parents solicitation of information, (2) par-
ents control over their childrens activities, and (3) dis-
Monitoring and Knowledge about Children and Their closure by children. Thus, they argue that it is premature
Activities. Monitoring is a feature of behavioral control to suggest that tracking and limit-setting account for
that has received considerable attention from socializa- positive links between monitoring and prosocial out-
tion theorists as an approach to parenting. It is conceptu- comes when monitoring is measured as knowledge. In-
alized as close surveillance in the form of parents deed, in a study of Swedish adolescents and their
requests for information about their childrens activities families, Stattin and Kerr (2000) found that disclosure
(Laird, Pettit, Mize, Brown, & Lindsey, 1994), shared ac- was the most important source of parents knowledge
398 Socialization Processes

about their childrens activities and friends, with the for it to be used effectively it must not threaten chil-
clear implication that children who engage in antisocial drens feelings of autonomy. Researchers have distin-
behavior probably hide their activities from their parents guished among different forms of control, with some
and that this, at least in part, accounts for the correlation more effective than others at achieving positive social-
between knowledge and outcomes. Kerr and Stattin ization outcomes.
(2000) report that low levels of child disclosure were
highly predictive of adolescent maladjustment, whereas
Socialization Practices
solicitation of information and control were not.
Kerr and Stattin (2000) and Stattin and Kerr (2000) To this point, we have discussed the manner in which par-
operationalized parental solicitation and control as par- ents influence behavior through their relationship with
ents asking adolescents about their activities and friends the child and through the particular way in which they ex-
and setting limits on their behavior. When monitoring ercise control. Relationships and the manner in which
was defined as parents participating in activities with control is exercised have to do with the context in which
adolescents and questioning knowledgeable adults about caregivers socialize children. Ultimately, they convey the
their children, Waizenhoffer, Buchanan, and Jackson- generalized motives of socializing agents rather than
Newsom (2004) found that parental monitoring was, their motives with respect to a childs specific actions.
contrary to the Swedish results, predictive of their knowl- But socialization also involves specific content as well as
edge. Waizenhoffer et al.s report of mixed results with goals or outcomes that parents hope to achieve (Darling
respect to links between sources of information and ado- & Steinberg, 1993). Included in the content are not only
lescent maladjustment underlines the complexity of the discipline practices, such as punishment or various types
issue and suggests that, among other things, monitoring of reasoning, but also a whole range of other practices.
that is too controlling may indeed have a negative impact. Some of these practices are deliberate and intentional in-
In accord with this interpretation, Kerr and Stattin terventions on the part of socializers, designed to achieve
(2000) found that adolescents who reported feeling desired goals, and others are less intentional and deliber-
overly controlled by their parents also reported higher ate. Thus, children are deliberately exposed to models
levels of both external (e.g., delinquency or school prob- deemed worthy of emulation, but at other times they are
lems) and internal (e.g., depression or low self-esteem) exposed to the unintended influences of others. Such un-
maladjustment. One conclusion is that surveillance and intended influences may either be consistent with or
questioningdone in an autonomy-supportive manner counter to the intended goals of socialization. For exam-
helps parents head off problems and respond to warning ple, parents (or agents of socialization) may model proso-
signs. But an additional contributor to the knowledge re- cial actions in their daily lives. Conversely, children may
quired for such responding involves the fostering of an at- also be inadvertently exposed to less worthy actions mod-
mosphere of warmth and trusta relationship that makes eled by agents of socialization themselves, deviant mem-
children and adolescents more likely to disclose informa- bers of society, or the media. The use of strongly power
tion about their activities. Not to be overlooked, children assertive discipline techniques is another example of a
who have few school problems and are low in delinquency practice that can be deliberate or unintentional as when it
are no doubt more willing to disclose. sends the message that force and coercion are good ways
to achieve wanted outcomes.
Summary
The domain approach that forms the core of the pres-
Children need to feel interrelated with, as well as sepa- ent analysis of socialization processes suggests that dif-
rate and autonomous from, others. Interrelatedness and ferent types of socialization practices will be appropriate
autonomy take place in the context of the domains of so- in different social domains, so we organize a discussion
cialization we describe in this chapter, with autonomy as of these practices by their relevance to the four domains
a feature of human behavior that has implications for of protective care, coalitional group, hierarchical power,
successful handling of events in the hierarchical power and mutuality/reciprocity. As noted earlier, such organi-
domain. Control is the operative concern in the power zation may help to make sense of disparate bodies of re-
domain, as the nature of relative power changes over the search as well as clarifying where there are missing gaps
course of development. Positive relationships foster so- in information and underemphases in the focus of empir-
cialization. But control is also a part of socialization and ical investigations of socialization practices. We also
Sociocultural Approaches 399

remind the reader that, as noted earlier, these strategies tural practices and routines, or everyday ways of acting
are differentially effective as a function of a whole series that simply happen, such as assignment of roles and activ-
of variables including child characteristics (e.g., age, sex, ities to boys and girls, sleeping arrangements, routine
or temperament). These are interactions that are revisited work around the house, how people dress, divisions of
in the concluding section of the chapter. parenting between mothers and fathers, and distinctions
between what is allowed to occur in public and what must
Socialization Practices in the Protective Care be done only in private (Goodnow, 1997). No rationale or
Domain: Sensitive and Responsive Caregiving explanation is provided for these actions, and they de-
In the protective care domain, parental sensitivity is the velop a momentum of their own that mitigates against re-
major parenting practice that promotes positive out- flection or questioning. They are to be distinguished
comes for children, with these outcomes including mas- from special moments that have been the principal focus
tery, emotion regulation, and interpersonal closeness of socialization theorists for understanding the way in
(Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby, which rules and standards of behavior are most likely to
1969; Sroufe, 1988). Caregivers are positioned along a be made explicit (Bugental & Goodnow, 1998). Rituals
series of continua involving their sensitivity to the needs (e.g., holiday celebrations) also belong in the category of
of their children, their acceptance or rejection of those activities that need no explanation but nevertheless im-
needs, the extent to which they cooperate with or are in- part considerable information about what is proper and
trusive in their interactions with their offspring, and the expected. They are different from routines by virtue of
degree to which they are accessible (Ainsworth et al., their symbolism, endurance, associated affect, and mean-
1978). Those who are sensitively responsive, accepting, ing that extend across generations (Fiese et al., 2002).
and accessible best prepare their offspring to effectively Routines and rituals play their part in socialization
self-regulate negative affect associated with distress. by supporting feelings of group belongingness. Families,
They enable the young to trust their caregivers (and oth- for example, develop a group identity through their rou-
ers with whom they form strong attachment relation- tines and rituals and these events foster a sense of par-
ships) to fulfill their needs for physical and emotional ticipation in and feeling of group membership (Moore &
protection and make reasonable demands for compliance Myerhoff, 1977). In this way, they foster the transmis-
to social standards of behavior (Bretherton, Golby, & sion of values from one generation to another and
Cho, 1997). Thus, the human species has evolved to strengthen intergenerational relationships that are im-
be willingly compliant, given that such compliance portant for successful transmission. We use the word
markedly increases the chances of individual survival transmission deliberately in this context, struck by the
(Stayton, Hogan, & Ainsworth, 1971). Securely attached fact that childrens input into routine and ritual may be
individuals are also able to explore their physical and less than it is in other socialization contexts. Routine
psychological worlds because they have a protective and ritual denote an unchanging set of conditions where
base to which they can return. In this way, their develop- discussions about usefulness or appropriateness are not
ing sense of competence is fostered by the belief that even contemplated. Indeed, once such discussion en-
they are supported, and by adjustments they learn to sues, acceptance may well break down.
cope with the stress of impending threats of danger. Re- Other socialization practices can be assigned to the
jecting and interfering, or rejecting and neglecting, coalitional group domain. One of these is joint play (to
caregivers promote feelings of insecurity, lack of trust, be distinguished from rough-and-tumble play associated
anger, and feelings of helplessness. As a result, they pro- with the hierarchical power domain). Parents who
mote lack of willing compliance (Stayton et al., 1971). engage in enjoyable activities with their children, in ad-
dition to providing them with the experience of harmo-
Socialization Practices in the Coalitional Group nious and rewarding relationships, are also structuring
Domain: Cultural Practices, Routines, Rituals, their childrens time. Time spent in joint play at age 3
Joint Play, and Observational Learning years predicts improvement in conduct problems at age
The coalitional domain is marked by childrens interest 4, independent of the initial level of those problems
in abiding by rules and conventions, doing things in a rit- (Gardner, Ward, Burton, & Wilson, 2003). In adoles-
ualized way, and conforming to avoid implicit exclusion cence, joint play transforms itself into time spent
from the group. This moves us into a discussion of cul- together in mutual activities and continues to be an
400 Socialization Processes

important feature of parent-child functioning that is setting where instruction is intended as opposed to oth-
linked with reduced conflict with fathers although not ers where it is not. Rogoff, Pardies, Arauz, Correa-
with mothers for whom time in joint activity and con- Chavez, and Angelillo (2003) talk about third-party
flict are not correlated. For fathers and sons, there is observation that involves keen attention and listening in
some suggestion that it is the time spent together that in or eavesdropping in anticipation of engaging in a similar
part accounts for reduction in conflict, in addition to the activity at a future time. Although less studied than
obvious observation that high conflict makes fathers and other forms of teaching, intent participation is a par-
adolescents less likely to share activities (Dubas & Ger- ticularly powerful form of learning, especially in cul-
ris, 2002). Although joint play might seem to have some tural communities where children have easy access to
of the features of mutual reciprocity, the evidence sug- everyday activities of adults and other children and
gests that reciprocity is not a major component of joint where they can engage in collaborative actions with
play in well-functioning families and that parents of other members of the group. H. L. Rheingolds (1982)
children who do not have conduct problems tend to initi- observations about the early appearance of helping pro-
ate and organize play in contrast to parents of children vide a good example of intent participation in American
with conduct problems who are more likely to sit back families. She noted that children between 18 and 30
and let their children take the lead (Gardner, 1994). months of age eagerly helped adults engaged in house-
Another way in which parents socialize their children hold work, appearing to enjoy themselves and to under-
that is of relevance to the coalitional group domain is stand that they were contributing to the management of
through management of their experiences of and expo- the household as opposed to playing. Intent participation
sure to desirable influences. Parents select childrens stands in sharp contrast to the form of tuition in which
schools and their after-school activities, and they deter- teachers deliver knowledge (either in the classroom or in
mine the neighborhoods in which their children live. the home) and provide incentives for the successful ac-
They monitor their activities and arrange formal and in- quisition of that knowledge. In keeping with the hori-
formal play contacts. Through their own social net- zontal nature of the process, intent participation fosters
works, they provide the opportunity for social contacts complementarity of roles and cooperation. Moreover,
for their children (Parke et al., 2002). They also cocoon the intrinsic merit of the activity is self-evident so that,
their children or protect them from undesirable events once again, the need for internalization of values and
and people (e.g., restricting exposure to certain forms of guidelines for behavior is minimized.
media or to particular peers) as well as prearm them by
warning them of temptation or providing them with Socialization Practices in the Hierarchical Power
ways of avoiding temptation (e.g., P. Miller & Sperry, Domain: Reward and Punishment
1988; Thornton, Chatters, Taylor, & Allen, 1990; Wat- Discipline and reward are the parenting practices that
son-Gegeo, 1992). have been the primary focus of socialization researchers
Finally, observational learning shares with cultural over many years. Historically, they have been addressed
practices, routines, rituals, joint play, and management a by their ability to facilitate internalizationthe taking
lack of linkage to special moments, a lack of reaction to over by children of societal attitudes and values as their
a childs specific behavior, and, often, a lack of specifi- own (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994). Both have a strong ele-
cally intended transmission of information or intimation ment of control, although it may be easier for children to
about the desirability of particular actions. It is not im- attribute prosocial behavior to their autonomously self-
mediately obvious to observers why they imitate partic- chosen actions after reasoning than after punishment or
ular actions, and there may be less occasion or need for power assertion (Lepper, 1983).
discussions about the usefulness or appropriateness of In addition to disciplining undesirable actions, par-
modeled actions. Indeed, it is striking how receptive ents reward desirable ones. Although rewards detract
young children become to proper ways of acting or from an inference of freely chosen action and, there-
conventional routines, many of which are acquired fore, internalization, socialization agents still use them
through observation (e.g., Dunn & Munn, 1985; Emde (e.g., Warton & Goodnow, 1995) and they play a major
et al., 1991). part in some socialization theories. Already discussed
Observational learning occurs in different circum- in the introductory section of this chapter is the impor-
stances that may well affect its potency in the socializa- tant work of Patterson and his colleagues (Dishion, An-
tion process. In some cases, for example, it happens in a drews, & Crosby, 1995; Patterson, 1980), who have
Sociocultural Approaches 401

demonstrated how coercive cycles of interaction de- against children younger than two or against teenagers
velop in problem families through inadvertent use of and should never involve use of an object, such as a belt
negative reinforcement and how this sets the stage for or ruler, and never include a slap or blow to the head.
the exposure of children to positive reinforcement for Opponents of corporal punishment see it as a predic-
antisocial action from deviant peer groups. Socializa- tor of later antisocial behavior (e.g., Strassburg, Dodge,
tion researchers have distinguished between reinforce- Pettit, & Bates, 1994) and as a potential way station
ment of actions and attributions of good behavior to to physical abuse (e.g., Straus, 1994). Supporters (e.g.,
childrens prosocial dispositions, with the latter being a Baumrind, Larzelere, & Cowan, 2002; Baumrind &
more effective form of reinforcement (Grusec & Redler, Thompson, 2002) regard occasional mild corporal pun-
1980; Kuczynski, 1984). ishment (spanking of a childs extremities with an open
hand) that is used only between toddlerhood and pu-
The Case of Corporal Punishment. Power as- berty and occurs in an authoritative context as a helpful
sertive strategies of discipline include verbal criticism, tool in parental efforts to socialize their children and
social isolation, and corporal punishment. Given space one that young children are willing to accept as a rea-
limitations we briefly discuss corporal punishment only, sonable and fair practice on the part of their parents
in part because of the considerable debate that exists (Siegal & Barclay, 1985). Certainly, at very young ages,
with respect to its potentially harmful consequences to children are unable to understand the rationale or justi-
children. In spite of the controversy, it remains an ac- fication for corporal punishment and, in such instances,
ceptable intervention for many parents, at least in the it may serve only as a source of fear or stress. Support-
United States where most parents report that they have ing this notion, Bugental, Martorell and Barraza (2003)
spanked their young children (Straus & Gelles, 1988). found that children who were the recipients of corporal
This level of usage is in sharp contrast to many countries punishment below the age of 1 year were more likely
(specifically, Austria, Croatia, Cyprus, Denmark, Fin- than other children to show high reactivity (production
land, Germany, Israel, Italy, Latvia, Norway, and Swe- of cortisol) to stress-inducing events (separation from
den) in which corporal punishment has been outlawed. It the mother) at older ages. Similarly, high levels of harsh
is also in contrast to Canada, a country where only 48% physical punishment are predictive of childrens exter-
of mothers of children aged 0 to 17 years reported that nalizing problems even when previous levels of problem
they had used it in mild form (smacking, slapping, behavior are held constant (e.g., Nix, Pinderhughes,
pinching) in the past year as opposed to 97% of parents Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1999).
responding to the same question in the United States Evidence for the argument that corporal punishment
(Oldershaw, 2002; Straus & Gelles, 1988). Although 14 can easily shade into physical abuse is seen to come from
years separates these two reportsthe first Canadian the observation that the two are correlated (Gershoff,
and the second Americanthe relative figures seem un- 2002). Moreover, statistics with respect to rates of child
likely to have changed substantially and point to consid- homicide support a hypothesis that there are links be-
erable philosophical differences between two countries tween favorable attitudes toward corporal punishment
in close geographical proximity and seemingly with and extreme physical abuse: In Sweden, in 1996, three
many shared values. Holden (2002) notes that in the children between the ages of 0 and 4 years were the vic-
United States legislation banning corporal punishment is tims of homicide. In Canada, in 1997, 24 children of
highly unlikely to succeed given the importance placed that same age were killed. In the United States, in 1998,
in American history on corporal punishment as an im- the comparable homicide figure was 723 (WHO, 2002).
portant part of child rearing and beliefs about the pri- Even controlling for differences in population the per
vacy of the family and personal freedoms that mitigate capita rate is markedly discrepant among the three
against societal and governmental intervention at this countries. These sorts of correlations do not imply
level. In Canada, where history and attitudes about per- causality, and it is plausible that positive attitudes to and
sonal freedom are somewhat different, the Supreme the use of corporal punishment and child abuse have dif-
Court, in January, 2004, upheld a provision of the Crim- ferent antecedents (Baumrind et al., 2002). Indeed, Jaf-
inal Code that allowed teachers, parents, and guardians fee et al. (2004) have found that shared genetic
to use reasonable force to correct children in their influences account for a very large portion of the corre-
charge. It did, however, set guidelines for reasonable lation between childrens antisocial behavior and corpo-
force, quoting experts as saying it should not be used ral punishment, whereas child maltreatment does not
402 Socialization Processes

appear to be genetically mediated. Thus, they argue that drens willing compliance with caregiver requests, as
corporal punishment and child maltreatment have dis- opposed to situational or forced compliance based on
tinct origins, with the latter lying in the family environ- externally imposed threat of punishment or hope of re-
ment and characteristics of the abuser and the former in ward. Moreover, it is seen as a precursor to the develop-
characteristics of the child. This does not mean of ment of conscience or early internalization (Kochanska
course that corporal punishment does not contribute to & Aksan, 1995). Indeed, Kochanska and Murray (2000)
increases in childrens antisocial behavior, as well as report that parent-child relationships characterized by
being elicited by their antisocial behavior. mutual compliance, harmony, and positivity during the
Holden (2002) suggests that appropriate analyses of the toddler and preschool years predict childrens con-
impact of corporal punishment need to consider childrens science at early school age (guilt after deviation, resist-
physiological and affective reactions to the intervention ance to temptation in the absence of surveillance,
and their cognitive appraisals of its meaning. Thus, pun- reluctance to violate rules, and maternal reports of
ishment that arouses fear, anger, or pain in the child is moral and prosocial behavior), even after controlling for
likely to reduce the childs willing compliance and to in- the developmental continuity of conscience.
crease the childs avoidance of the parent, thereby reduc- Unlike rewards and punishment that function in the hi-
ing opportunities for socialization or the building of a erarchical domain, with parents imposing demands on
positive relationship. Alternatively, if punishment is seen their children, parent responsiveness moves the socializa-
as well-intentioned, or as an accepted and expected part tion process into the MR domain. As well as setting the
of the cultural context in which it occurs, then it may be stage for future willing compliance, it is also one aspect of
viewed in a less negative way by the child. Ultimately, the the guided apprenticeship involving jointly coordinated
issue may be reduced at least in part to one of impulsivity interactions that is necessary for the MR domain to be
versus instrumentality in parental use of corporal punish- fully operative with others. Although parents (as kin with
ment as well as the meaning its use has for the child. shared interests with the child) will be willing to produce
There is still much to learn about the long-term ef- benefits for their related children, other unrelated individ-
fects of corporal punishment and the factors (means of uals will be much less likely to do so. When parents create
implementation, responses of children) that serve to experiences that foster reciprocal relationships (in which
moderate its effects. It is typically associated with nega- there are two-way benefits), the child can be expected to
tive outcomes for the child, but the associated pathway learn the pragmatic rules of exchange that govern much of
and the qualifying variables for this relationship are un- social life. Such experiences also create a means of nego-
clear. Corporal punishment in some contexts may be less tiating the relationship between parents and children
detrimental in its impact than other forms of punishment when the young decline in dependence and increase in
such as derogation and humiliation. Socialization re- poweras the relationship becomes more symmetrical.
searchers have argued for some time that power assertive As suggested, there is empirical evidence to support the
discipline techniques, including physical punishment, notion that mutual reciprocity continues to play a signifi-
will be effective only if they are moderate and mild in cant role in socialization through middle childhood. La-
usage, combined with reasoning, and used in the context beled as synchrony and referring to interactions that are
of a warm and loving relationship (e.g., Hoffman, 1983). reciprocal, responsive, interconnected, and engaged, mu-
Under all circumstances, corporal punishment, even tual reciprocity is predictive both of low levels of harsh
when mild, does convey the message that the use of force parenting and low levels of antisocial behavior (Criss,
is a justifiable way in which to solve conflicts, and this is Shaw, & Ingoldsby, 2003). In synchronous relationships
a criticism that does not seem to have been adequately children engage in mutual exchange with their parents.
answered by its proponents and a message that most More than this, they also impart greater amounts of infor-
adults probably do not wish to send. mation to their parents (an important feature of effective
socialization as noted earlier) and have the opportunity to
Socialization in the Mutuality/Reciprocity Domain practice positive social skills (Criss et al., 2003).
Caregivers who are responsive to the reasonable de-
Summary
mands of their children have children who, in turn, are
responsive to their demands (Parpal & Maccoby, 1985). Different social domains require different socialization
This mutually responsive orientation facilitates chil- strategies to achieve the tasks associated with that do-
Sociocultural Approaches 403

main. According to this analysis, parents who use the ization dimensions of interrelatedness and autonomy.
wrong strategy for a particular domain will not be Some (generally North American and Western Euro-
successful in their socialization goals. Children cannot pean) focus on separation and autonomy of the individ-
learn the pragmatic rules of exchange that govern much ual, with the self seen as a distinct entity having unique
of social life by being punished, nor can they learn to ac- internal attributes. These individualist cultures empha-
cept rules of behavior through sensitive and accepting size competition, self-actualization, dominance, and
parenting. What works when a child is ill is not the same open expression of emotion. In contrast, collectivist
as what works when a child is well, given that different cultures, more characteristic of most of the rest of the
social domains are activated. There are clear interac- world, highlight interrelatedness and connectedness
tions between social domain and socialization strategy. with the group, social harmony, and the organization of
behavior around relationships with others. Cooperation,
empathy, accommodation with the needs of others, sub-
Socialization and the Cultural Context tle expressions of emotion, and, sometimes, deference
to the authority of others are central in descriptions of
One of the most powerful moderators of the impact of ex- collectivist approaches to psychological functioning.
perience on childrens development is the cultural context Individualistic and collectivistic orientations arose as a
in which the experience occurs. We turn now to a discus- result of adaptation to different environmental conditions
sion of its role in socialization. Context gives meaning to a and restraints, with specific economic and environmental
socialization activity; thus, the same action may have a conditions favoring different developmental pathways
different outcome in a different cultural context because it (e.g., Berry, 1976). Thus, the interdependent route is
has a different meaning in each of those contexts. Investi- linked to small communities and a subsistence economy,
gation of culture also increases understanding of mecha- where cooperation and interpersonal harmony are neces-
nisms of socialization and features of human behavior by sary features for survival. In contrast, the independent
demonstrating the role of goals and practices that are in- pathway is an adaptive response to large, anonymous,
fluenced by the cultural context, as well as identifying urban centers and a commercial economy, or modes of
those goals and practices that cut across cultures. Context production that do not require group support. In the small
helps address questions, such as why it is that authoritar- and interdependent community, ideas tend to be transmit-
ian parenting leads to negative outcomes in one culture ted from one generation to the next in a vertical fashion,
and not in another, or whether autonomy and separation and so they maximize continuity and facilitate respect for
are universal features of psychological functioning inde- authority. In the more independent context, ideas are ne-
pendent of the context in which they are exhibited. gotiated horizontally in generations and subject to influ-
A frequently employed distinction in cultural re- ence from outside sources. As a result, there may be less
search is that between individualism and collectivism. continuity and respect for the traditional ways of doing
The distinction has both its advocates and its critics as things (Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003).
we note in the following discussion. We use it, however, Pure examples of individualism and collectivism are
because it parallels so closely the dimensions of related- not easy to find in a world that is changing speedily and
ness and autonomy that have been a prominent part of where technological developments break down cultural
the discussion of socialization presented in this chapter. distinctions. The dichotomy has been the object of con-
siderable criticism, much of it a result of the tendency of
Individualism versus Collectivism as
researchers to treat individualism-collectivism as an in-
Organizers of Culture
dividual difference variable rather than as a feature of
Differences between national cultures have been high- differences between nations as the original proponents
lighted through the use of dichotomies, such as of the distinction (e.g., Hofstede, 1983) intended. Indi-
individualist-collectivist (Hofstede, 1983), agentic- vidualism and collectivism are orientations that are
communal (Kashima et al., 1995), and independent- found in all societies and individuals (Brewer, 1999a;
interdependent (Markus & Kitiyama, 1991), all of Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Killen & Wainryb, 2000; J.
which emphasize the distinct way that each values the Miller, 2002; Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002).
self versus others. Thus, cultures are broadly character- Turiel (2002), who maintains that a desire for autonomy
ized as focusing on different aspects of the two social- from the group is universal rather than just a feature of
404 Socialization Processes

individualistic cultures, argues that individuals can re- Weisz (2000) in their analysis of close relationships in
spond to ideological belief systems in ways that show ac- Japan and the United States, with the analysis shifting
tive acceptance as well as tacit disapproval or rejection. from a focus on the importance and strength of relation-
As an example, the Druze in Israel are a hierarchically ships to one that concentrates on the meaning and dy-
organized society in which there are differences be- namics of relationships. Their argument is that
tween men and women in their rights and individual au- individuation, which characterizes American thinking,
tonomy. Druze women frequently support the rights of and accommodation, which characterizes Japanese
men to exercise their power, but they see it as unjust and thinking, influences the nature of relationships in those
they see their own endorsement of male rights simply as cultures rather than individuation diluting relationships.
a way to avoid severe social sanctions (Wainryb & Specifically, the need for affiliation with others in-
Turiel, 1994). In a similar vein, Helwig, Arnold, Tan, volves either a pull toward adaptation of the self to fit
and Boyd (2003) report that Chinese children express the needs of othersthe path of symbiotic harmony, or
concepts of rights, individual autonomy, and democratic a tug between the desire for proximity and closeness and
norms in their social reasoning as well as use them to that for separation and explorationthe path of genera-
critically evaluate existing social practices. Further tive tension. Examples are provided from a variety of
criticisms emerge in observations that the concepts ap- domains. In the protective care domain, Japanese infants
pear to change as they move across major cultural areas, derive security from the mothers indulgence of their
with collectivism in Asian societies different from that needs and American infants from the use of the mother
in African or Latin American societies, and individual- as a secure base from which to explore the world, a dis-
ism in the United States different from that in Europe tinction reflected in greater body contact by Japanese
(Harkness, Super, & van Tijen, 2000). mothers and greater eye contact by American mothers
In spite of these concerns, Oyserman et al. concluded (e.g., Barratt, Negayama, & Minami, 1993). In the hier-
in a meta-analysis that there is good evidence to support archical domain, American parents show respect for
the speculation that so-called individualist- and collec- noncompliance but exercise more direct control. Japa-
tivist-focused societies differ in organization, with the nese parents avoid confrontations and often back down
latter promoting in-group harmony and group obligation when children resist their requests. As well, they are
and the former making personal uniqueness salient and more likely to use guilt and anxiety induction, shaming,
requiring separation from others. Oyserman et al. has- and modeling (Lebra, 2000). American parents threaten
ten to add that this does not mean that societies uni- to ground their disobedient children inside the home,
formly enforce these mandates on all members. But they whereas Japanese parents threaten to banish them out-
suggest the dichotomy does signal that some features of side the home (Johnson, 1993). In the MR domain,
human functioning believed to be universal may not be American adolescents increasingly seek intimacy with
so. For example, not all individuals make sense of the peers in sharp contrast to Japanese adolescents who
self through high self-esteem or positive self-views to view the parent-child relationship as enduring and mu-
the extent that European Americans do, nor do all indi- tually supportive across the life span (Lebra, 2000). In-
viduals have attributional and cognitive styles that lead deed, in Japan there are, in contrast to North America,
them to ignore contextual influences on human behavior no laws concerning legal and economic independence
as much as do European Americans. People, as a func- from parents (Hsu, 1983). In essence, relationships are
tion of the cultural context in which they find them- central in both cultures, but they take a different form as
selves, are likely to be different in what they consider to a function of differential emphasis on individuation and
be rewarding outcomes and may treat in-group as op- accommodation. The Japanese model is only one con-
posed to out-group members differently. Ultimately, trasting possibility, but it offers a means of expanding
well-being is linked to a considerable degree to the at- conceptualizations of how the nature of relationships is
tainment of culturally valued outcomes. affected by cultural goals.
Some have suggested that, given that elements of au- The usefulness of the collectivist-individualist com-
tonomy and interrelatedness occur in all cultures, a bet- parison is reflected in the frequency with which it is em-
ter way of conceptualizing the impact of different goals ployed in many studies (a number of which are discussed
in so-called collectivist and individualist societies may later) as a source of hypotheses for cultural differences
be to focus on the relative balance of each. This is a in the socialization of social, emotional, and cognitive
point made by Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, Miyake, and development. Moreover, as noted earlier, it maps well
Sociocultural Approaches 405

onto the basic socialization mechanisms that appear in comes for the child. Familial obligation and the duty of
the social domain analysis presented in this paper. Given children in the maintenance of the household, for exam-
these observations, we treat different attitudes toward ple, marks a prime difference between collectivist and
interdependence and independence as useful organizing individualist orientations. Although family obligations
and explanatory devices in the following discussion. Ul- are valued less by adolescents than by their parents re-
timately, we conclude that interrelatedness and auton- gardless of culture (Phinney, Ong, & Madden, 2000)
omy/control are important in all cultures, but the way in there is greater endorsement of these obligations by col-
which they manifest themselves differs as a function of lectivist youth than by individualist youth. Thus, Asian
the values and goals that exist in a particular cultural American and Mexican American adolescents are more
setting (e.g., Greenfield et al., 2003; Rothbaum, Pott, likely than their European American peers to believe
et al., 2000; Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyake, & Morelli, they should assist parents and siblings throughout the
2000). Thus, one challenge for investigators is to see life span and be willing to make sacrifices for the family
how interrelatedness and autonomy are differentially ex- (Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999). In a parallel way, proso-
pressed as a function of goals and belief systems that cial behavior or a concern with the needs of others is
characterize a particular cultural group. more duty-based and seen as an important achievement
In the following discussion, we first describe how of justice among Hindus, whereas concern for the needs
parental goals are affected by different cultural contexts and welfare of others involves an element of choice and
and how those goals are manifested in different social- personal decision in Anglo-Western contexts (J. Miller
ization practices. We ask what context reveals about & Bersoff, 1992). Greenfield et al. (2003) contrast con-
mechanisms of socialization in different social domains cepts of intelligence as a function of cultural orienta-
and discuss developmental pathways and the expansion tion: Individualists value scientific intelligence because
of meaning of interrelatedness and autonomy that cul- it emphasizes the person in relation to the world of ob-
tural comparisons provide. jects. The collectivist contrast is valuation of social in-
telligence and the development of a child who can
What Context Reveals about the Mechanisms
contribute to the well-being and cohesiveness of the
of Socialization
group rather than one who stands out as different and
In socialization situations, values are transmitted not better. Attention is on shared perspectives rather than
only in the form of statements about valued behavior different perspectives, one possible reason for why col-
(Family is important, Learn to think for yourself ) lectivists do less well on theory of mind tasks (Vinden,
but also in modes of intervention. Power assertion, for 1999). Finally, with respect to autonomy, American par-
example, sends the message that obedience is a desirable ents value independence, assertiveness, and self-
and important outcome, whereas reasoning indicates expression, whereas Chinese parents place the emphasis
that willing acceptance of a particular position is an im- on obedience, reliability, proper behavior, social obliga-
portant social goal. Negotiation and compromise indi- tion, and group achievement (Chao, 1995).
cate that maintenance of positive relationships should be
an aim in social interactions and that what constitutes The Impact of Goals on Socialization Practices.
acceptable behavior can be stretched as a function of The attainment of different goals is achieved by the use
discussion and conciliation. The way in which socializa- of different practices. Japanese American families dis-
tion is carried out, as well as the specific content that is cuss group activities and shared experiences at the din-
conveyed, reflects general values inherent in the culture. ner table and Caucasian American families focus on
We begin with a general discussion of the impact of childrens activities and individual experiences during
culture on goals and socialization practices and then con- dining encounters (Martini, 1996). Similarly, American
sider, in greater detail, socialization practices in two do- and Israeli comparisons reveal a concern with the indi-
mains that have received the most attention from cultural viduals daily experience among the former and the
researchersprotective care and hierarchical power. telling of stories that include multiple members of the
group among the latter (Blum-Kulka, 1997). American
The Impact of Culture on Goals. The collectivist children are encouraged to discuss their own feelings
focus on interdependence and group harmony is re- and those of others as a way of increasing their under-
flected in different emphases from the individualist standing of emotion and ability to regulate it, whereas,
focus on autonomy on what are important goals or out- in Chinese families, attunement to the feelings of others
406 Socialization Processes

but restraint in the expression of ones own feelings is ence and how it reveals itself in the way mothers re-
encouraged as a key to group harmony (Chao, 1995). spond to distress and bids for attention in their infants.
Chinese children are expected to read or infer the They suggest that, for example, sensitivity is expressed
thoughts and feelings of others without being told to American infants in distal forms of contact and en-
(Wang & Leichtman, 2000). Chinese parents are also couragement of expression of negative emotion as op-
more likely to remind children of their past transgres- posed to its expression to Japanese infants through
sions, using story-telling, for example, as a way to teach prolonged skin-to-skin contact and discouraging of emo-
social norms and behavioral standards and to engender a tion expression. Sensitivity occurs in response to chil-
sense of shame over bad behavior: In contrast, American drens signals in the American context and in
parents avoid stories of transgression so as not to dam- anticipation of childrens signals or needs in the Japa-
age their childrens self-esteem (P. Miller, Fung, & nese context; sensitivity fosters exploration and auton-
Mintz, 1996; P. Miller, Wiley, Fung, & Liang, 1997). omy in the former and emotional closeness in the latter.
European American parents are more likely to engage in Secure attachment promotes social competence, but
authoritative parenting, emphasizing the growth of competence differs in the two culturesexploration,
separation and autonomy in a supportive and responsive autonomy, willingness to express emotion, and a positive
relationship; Asian American, Latino, and African self-concept as opposed to dependence, emotional re-
Americans are more likely to engage in authoritarian straint, self-effacement, and indirect expression of feel-
parenting with its greater emphasis on obedience and ings. In this cultural approach the universals of
conformity (Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn, & Dorn- attachmentpursuit of proximity and protection and
busch, 1991). Learning in cultures that foster interde- distress at separationremain unchanged but some of
pendence involves keen observation, attentiveness, and the basic tenets, including the meaning of sensitivity,
focused listening (Greenfield et al., 2003; Rogoff et al., competence, and the function of the caregiver as a se-
2003) as opposed to direct tuition and formal schooling. cure base for exploration, assume more culture-specific
Even in school classrooms, however, children in interde- forms. With this sort of cultural analysis, the under-
pendent cultures are more likely than teachers to take standing of what it means to be securely attached is per-
responsibility for classroom management and to solve ceived in more diverse and context-relevant ways.
problems together than those in independent cultures Different goals in different cultures channel caregiving
(Rogoff et al., 2003). practices, so that close body contact, immediate or an-
Maternal behavior in the protective care domain of- ticipatory reactions to infant distress signals, and disap-
fers a detailed example of how different cultural values proval of mother-child separation feature in a variety of
are mirrored in the ways that parents handle the tasks of collectivistic cultural contexts as a way of fostering
socialization. For example, Keller et al. (2004) have early close relationships, in contrast to practices more
identified two styles of parenting of very young infants: familiar to researchers in individualist cultures (Green-
(1) distal with an emphasis on object stimulation and field et al., 2003).
face-to-face exchange (mutual eye contact and frequent Turning to practices in the hierarchical power do-
use of language) and (2) proximal with an emphasis on main, we note that many studies have found Asians to be
body contact and body stimulation. The former, found in more restrictive in their parenting (Chao, 2002) and
individualist settings (German and Greek), links infants numbers of others have failed to find the usual relations
to the nonpersonal world of objects and informs them of between authoritative and authoritarian parenting and
their ability to influence the world. The latter, charac- childrens cognitive and social outcomes in different
teristic of collectivist settings (Cameroon Nso, Costa cultural groups. For example, the association between
Rican, and, partially, Indian Guujarati Rajput), is asso- authoritative parenting and academic achievement is
ciated with social cohesion, feelings of belongingness, stronger for European American than for Asian and
and the experience of the body as an agent situated in African American adolescents (Darling & Steinberg,
the environment. In an analysis of how sensitivity in 1993). Authoritarianism is positively related to exter-
mother-infant interactions manifests itself in different nalizing problems for European Americans but unre-
cultural settings, Rothbaum, Weiss, et al. (2000) fo- lated for Mexican Americans (Lindahl & Malik, 1999),
cused on the contrasting emphasis in Japanese and and authoritarian parenting has positive effects on ado-
American cultures on independence and interdepend- lescents school performance among the Chinese in
Sociocultural Approaches 407

Hong Kong (Leung, Lau, & Lam, 1998). Supervision tility and derogation (resulting from negative attribu-
and consistency of discipline are negatively related to tions), and lack of ability and/or willingness to take the
delinquency among European Americans but unrelated perspective of the child. Accordingly, levels of emo-
among Mexican Americans (C. Smith & Krohn, 1995), tional expression but not parent control and directive-
and corporal punishment is associated with externaliz- ness are related to childrens social acceptance (Isley,
ing problems in European American but not African ONeil, & Parke, 1996), and warmth and involvement
American children (Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & are better predictors of academic achievement and so-
Pettit, 1996). cioemotional outcomes among Korean American, Mexi-
A number of factors can plausibly account for the dif- can American, and Anglo European adolescents than is
ferential impact of authoritarian parenting. First, so- control (Kim & Rohner, 2002; Knight, Virdin, & Roosa,
cialization styles convey values, and values differ in 1994). As well, when coercive discipline is operational-
different social contexts. In collectivist contexts, defer- ized as observed rejection, criticism, and failure to say
ence to authority would be supported by authoritarian good-bye or to greet children in a day care setting it is a
approaches to child socialization to the extent that such predictor of conduct problems even among African
deference fosters family coherence and harmony. Sec- American children (Kilgore, Snyder, & Lentz, 2000).
ond, authoritarian parenting appears to have different Similarly, when parental control is defined for Asians
meaning in different contexts. Chao (1994, 2001, 2002) not as guan, but as domineering or overprotective behav-
notes that authoritarian parenting in China can involve ior, it has negative effects (Herz & Gullone, 1999; Stew-
the concept of guan or training that emphasizes the im- art et al., 1998) just as it does in a Western European
portance of hard work, self-discipline, and obedience. context. Barber and Harmon (2002) also suggest that
When it has this meaning it may well have a more posi- psychological control has negative correlates in a variety
tive outcome, particularly in the area of academic of cultural contexts. These findings all point to the fact
achievement. that relationships and parenting style are central in so-
The adverse impact of strict and controlling parent- cialization and that they moderate the meaning of con-
ing in Western European cultures may stem to a consid- trol for the developing child.
erable extent from its association with a variety of
negative cognitions and emotions experienced by au- Linkages to Developmental Pathways
thoritarian parents in interaction with their children. The last question to be addressed in this section has to do
These include rejection and lack of warmth (Baumrind, with differences among cultures in developmental path-
1967), negative attributions for childrens actions (Dix, ways. If the ideal developmental outcome involves a bal-
Ruble, & Zambarano, 1989), low feelings of control in ance between interrelatedness and autonomy (Blatt,
interactions with difficult children (Bugental, Brown, & 1998), what might one expect with respect to greater em-
Reiss, 1996), and inability to take the perspective of the phasis on the one rather than the other in different cul-
child (Dekovic, Gerris, & Janssens, 1991). In contrast, tural contexts? Does the emphasis on autonomy and
control has been found to be unrelated to these negative separation in the West serve the needs of the culture but
emotions and cognitions in collectivist cultures (Rudy impede optimal development? The general question has
& Grusec, 2001, in press) and even to have positive been most frequently addressed with respect to the col-
associations with warmth and acceptance (Rohner & lectivist model with its weight on interdependence and
Pettengill, 1985; Stewart et al., 1998; Trommsdorff & group harmony. Rothbaum, Pott, et al. (2000) suggest
Iwawaki, 1989). Authoritarian parenting in Western Eu- that adaptation to group needs over self-assertion may
ropean contexts frequently may be pursued to establish lead to internalizing problems, although Lewis (2000)
authority over children who are felt and thought about in notes that the suicide rate is lower in Japan than in the
relatively negative terms. In other cultural groups, how- United States and that Japanese high school students re-
ever, authoritarianism may often be pursued in chil- port fewer feelings of stress, anxiety, and depression.
drens interests. Indeed, it may be the negative Lebra (2000) describes the difficulty Japanese children
cognitions and emotions that ultimately produce the have in breaking away from their mothers and the fact
kinds of outcomes that have been attributed to authori- that marital relationships are sometimes strained be-
tarian parenting across all cultural contexts. In this cause exclusive intimacy is not legitimized and, as a re-
sense, control is not the issue so much as rejection, hos- sult, husbands and wives become distanced. The reliance
408 Socialization Processes

on shame that characterizes some collectivist cultures oriented issues were more difficult for people to inter-
may be problematic given that it gives rise to more in- nalize, possibly because vertical issues frequently re-
tense levels of physiological arousal than does guilt quire individuals to give up their own freedom of choice
(Scherer & Wallbott, 1994) and that it is less likely to as well as restricting the set of people with whom inti-
lead to reparative behaviors than is guilt (Tangney, macy and connectedness can be established.
Wagner, Hill-Barlow, Marschall, & Gramzow, 1997). The focus on autonomy as willing enactment and
Alternatively, the psychological well-being of Chinese endorsement of a position has been labeled reflective
American adolescents in the United States does not ap- autonomy (Koestner & Losier, 1996; Koestner et al.,
pear to be harmed by the fact that they have more family 1999) and distinguished from reactive autonomy, with
obligations than their European American counterparts individuals who are high in reflective autonomy more
(Fuligni, Yip, & Tseng, 2002). willing to follow expert advice and those high in reac-
tive autonomy more likely to reject the influence of oth-
Autonomy. Considerable attention has been paid ers even when such action is counterproductive. We note
to the issue of autonomy and whether or how interde- the parallel distinction between willing and situational
pendence and group harmony undermine it. An exten- compliance (Maccoby & Martin, 1983) and the fact that
sion of this question has to do with whether autonomy is willing or receptive compliance promotes a positive and
indeed the universal human need suggested by self- cooperative stance in relation to agents of socialization
determination (Deci & Ryan, 1991) and other theorists that may fit better with collectivist than individualist
(e.g., Killen & Wainryb, 2000). One stance is that the goals. Indeed, one is reminded of the fact that Asian
values of autonomy are opposed to those of group cohe- parents in general will often give in to their childrens
sion and that autonomy is not important across all cul- demands rather than encourage conflict and confronta-
tures. Another suggestion is that yielding to controlling tion (Chao, 2002), either as a way of avoiding con-
pressure is more satisfying in some cultures than is frontation or because they see young children as not
making decisions in the absence of any external influ- responsible for their actions. This is a practice that may
ences (e.g., Iyengar & Lepper, 2000; J. Miller, 1997). In not work well when the goal is separation and individua-
accord with this viewpoint, the adoption of choices tion, but may work well with a goal that actions be seen
made by trusted others has been found to enhance in- as freely chosen.
trinsic motivation for Asian Americans but not for Euro-
pean Americans (Iyengar & Lepper, 2000). Some (e.g., Conf lict. Conflict is a significant contributor to
Chirkov & Ryan, 2003; Ryan & Deci, 2000), however, the development of individuation and separation. An im-
argue that decisions made in conformity with the guid- portant question has to do with the role and extent
ance of others is not an accurate rendering of the con- of conflict in collectivist contexts. Although parent-
cept of autonomy and that autonomy is experienced adolescent conflict may be more discouraged in collec-
when individuals perceive their actions to be willingly tivist settings it does exist and is widespread (Schlegel
enacted and when they themselves fully endorse these & Barry, 1991). Thus, even under conditions in which
actions. The autonomous individual is not defined as in- interdependence and family harmony are stressed, there
dividualistic or independent ( both of which are indica- is expressed disagreement between the generations, an
tors of low relatedness) nor does the autonomous indication of growing autonomy. In a study of Chinese
individual act in independent, detached, or selfish ways. (Hong Kong) adolescents Yau and Smetana (1996), for
The opposite of autonomy is not dependence or lack of example, report the existence of conflictual interac-
physical separation (Kagitcibasi, 1996) but, rather, het- tions, albeit fewer than in American families. Despite
eronomy in which choices are seen as controlled by the particularly great emphasis on respect for parental
those who do not share ones own values or interests. In authority in Chinese families, Yau and Smetana report
accord with this distinction, Chirkov and Ryan (2003) that Chinese adolescents reasoned about conflicts as is-
found that autonomy and well-being were correlated in sues of exercising or maintaining personal jurisdiction,
both individualist and collectivist countries (South similar to the way in which American adolescents ex-
Korea, Turkey, Russia, and the United States). They press their growing need for individuation and auton-
also found that values involving vertical or status- omy. These appeals to personal jurisdiction may reflect
Outcomes of the Young as a Result of Their Life History 409

the fact that parental respect and filial piety seem to veals how cultures affect goals and, thereby, socializa-
have more of the characteristics of family harmony and tion practices. For example, research suggests that
love and affection than they do of strict obedience when authoritarian parenting harms children it is be-
(Sung, 1995). Smetana and Yau did find some differ- cause of its association with lack of warmth and respect
ences in the content of conflicts, with more disagree- for the child. Thus, authoritarian parenting, guided by
ment over academic matters and the behavior of parents goals of the culture, has a different meaning in those
themselves (e.g., their smoking or their relationship cultures. Cultures have been distinguished by a differ-
with other family members), with each of these reflect- ential balance of the two basic dimensions of socializa-
ing the greater Chinese emphasis on academic achieve- tion: interrelatedness and autonomy. Nevertheless,
ment and family functioning. Yau and Smetana conclude interdependence or interconnectedness appears to be
that similarities between adolescents in Hong Kong and important in all cultures, as does autonomya willing
American adolescents reflect the universal developmen- enactment of a position.
tal task of separation or de-individuation from parents,
with conflict one way of achieving this de-individuation.
However, the way in which conflict is expressed, and the
extent to which it occurs, is obviously directed by the OUTCOMES OF THE YOUNG AS A RESULT
cultural context in which it occurs. OF THEIR LIFE HISTORY
What do we conclude about the links between relat-
edness, autonomy, and developmental pathways? An im- To this point, we have considered the sociocultural
portant observation is that interrelatedness and processes that operate in socialization as built on a bio-
autonomy appear to be important features of all cul- logical platform that is common to all members of the
tures. Analyses of their manifestations in a variety of species. We now move to consider individual variation
contexts suggest differences in emphasis and differ- in biological characteristics and how they combine with
ences in form but an underlying and common concern sociocultural experiences in determining the outcomes
with feeling related to others and feeling some degree of of the young. Researchers have proposed different mod-
control over ones own actions. There may be tradeoffs els of contingent causality in conceptualizing the ways
in the ways in which these motives are expressed. Close in which child characteristics and life history combine
family ties provide the benefit of a high sense of safety to influence the childs ultimate outcomes. As a begin-
but at the cost of reductions in the individuals ability to ning to our discussion, we briefly review some of these
achieve personal goals. Rothbaum and Trommsdorff (in models. The models differ in their starting points. Some
press) talk about tradeoffs between forms of related- investigators begin with a consideration of childrens re-
ness, contrasting trust and assurance. They argue that activity to novelty and change in their environment as a
trust, or faith in the intentions of others that allows peo- function of genetic variables (e.g., sex or temperament
ple to seek spontaneous relationships with new partners, differences). Others begin by considering the impact of
is associated with autonomy, self-esteem, and self- a variety of child characteristics (e.g., learning disor-
actualization. However, assurance, or commitment to ders or negative emotionality) on their receptivity to so-
and guarantee of loyalties and reciprocity from mem- cialization per se. Still others begin with a focus on the
bers of a tightly knit group, promotes group cohesion shared (and unshared) characteristics of children that
and family security. Thus, culture may be characterized lead them to be able to adapt to changing ecologies. De-
as preference for different social domains or different spite variations in their starting points, there is a high
points of balance but with no one centrality more adap- level of overlap in these models.
tive than another. We then move on to consider the route through which
childrens long-term outcomes are determined as a func-
Summary
tion of these interactions. Specifically, we are con-
Culture provides an example of an interactional model cerned with the life outcomes of the young as a function
of socialization, pointing to the importance of meaning of the mediating role of (a) childrens acquired ways of
in understanding mechanisms of socialization. The cognitively representing their social worlds, ( b) hor-
study of socialization in different cultural contexts re- mones in the responses shown to their experiences, and
410 Socialization Processes

(c) gene expression in the continuous reorganization of science than are those who are low in fearfulness or
the brain in response to experience. than do those who are high in fearfulness and have gen-
tle parents (Kochanska, 1997). In addition, fearful boys
Child Characteristics as Sources of Inf luence exposed either to harsh or overinvolved parenting show
on and Reactivity to the Environment elevated levels of depression (Colder, Lochman, &
Wells, 1997). At the same time, young children who are
Childrens early environment necessarily includes both fearful or display discomfort in strange situations show
change and stability. The child is increasingly con- a greater level of conscience development to the extent
fronted with novel or surprising events that potentially their mothers use a gentle disciplinary style. In addi-
serve as sources of stress. At the same time, children tion, fearful children may manifest unique regulatory
differ in the extent to which and the ways in which they benefits ( habituation to repeated stress) when they ex-
respond to such normative experiences. perience more responsive parenting (Schwartz & Bugen-
tal, 2004).
Genetic Variations in Childrens Reactivity to
Supporting the notion that temperament and experi-
Stress-Inducing Events
ence interact in a way that allows clear causal inference,
Variations in childrens reactivity to the environment Suomi (1997) has demonstrated that rhesus macaques
have often been found to be due to genetic differences in that show a highly fearful temperament, when cross-fos-
children. Researchers initially focused on the extent to tered by average mothers, showed deficits in their ex-
which children are selectively vulnerable to negative ex- ploratory behavior and in their responses to stressful
periences; for example, boys have generally been found events. In contrast, temperamentally fearful animals
to be more reactive to environmental stress than are that were cross-fostered by very nurturant mothers
girls. They show faster rise times and higher levels of showed high levels of exploratory behavior and ability to
physiological arousal to commonly occurring sources of cope with stress.
stress (Maccoby, 1988). On a more ongoing basis, boys In the same way, those children who demonstrate ir-
are more negatively affected than are girls by single par- ritability (or negative emotionality) early in life, show
enthood (Hetherington, 1993), poverty (Elder & Rock- different outcomes based on the responses shown by
well, 1979), and divorce (Needle, Su, Doherty, 1990). their parents. For example, Crockenberg (1987) ob-
There has also been a continuing line of research fo- served that infant irritability, when combined with
cused on childrens temperament as an influence on angry and punitive mothering, predicted child anger and
their reactivity to potential stress (e.g., Gunnar, Porter, noncompliance at 2 years of age; no such relationship
Wolfe, Rigatuso, & Larson, 1995; Kochanska, 1993; was found for nonirritable infants. At the same time,
Ramsay & Lewis, 2003; Schmidt, Fox, Rubin, & Stein- children who show early negative emotionality are more
berg, 1997). Among the ways in which children differ likely to show benefits when they experience positive
on the basis of temperament, child fearfulness or inhibi- parenting. For example, Blair (2002) found that infants
tion has been most strongly associated with vulnerabil- who showed high negative emotionality demonstrated
ity to potentially stress-inducing events (Goldsmith & greater cognitive and social benefits (fewer externaliz-
Lemery, 2000; Kagan, 2001). An initial focus on tem- ing problems at later ages) than did other children when
perament differences that led to differential vulnerabil- their parents participated in an early intervention pro-
ity to stress was extended to include temperament gram that afforded enriched rearing experiences.
differences (e.g., positive emotionality) that led to dif-
ferential resilience in the face of stress or adversity Childrens Responsiveness to Socialization or Tuition
(Wills, Sandy, Yaeger, & Shinar, 2001). Consistent differences have been found between chil-
In considering these genetically influenced differ- dren in their receptivity or responsiveness to socializa-
ences in children, researchers have focused more re- tion (Belsky, in press). Both at home and school,
cently on the interaction between genes and the childs children differ in the extent to which they are positively
socializing environment in determining their long-term responsive or unresponsive to social influence and/or tu-
outcomes (Collins et al., 2000). For example, tempera- itional efforts. In some cases, child unresponsiveness is
mentally fearful children whose mothers use harsh due to learning disorders such as attention deficit disor-
discipline are more likely to exhibit lower levels of con- der (ADD) or attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
Outcomes of the Young as a Result of Their Life History 411

(ADHD). In other cases, it may involve child character- in an adaptive way: Children are seen as designed by
istics that have sometimes been summarized as involving their evolutionary history to show a facultative respon-
a difficult temperament (Bates, 1980). Finally, some siveness to the environment in which they are born. If the
researchers have been concerned with childrens physio- environment is harsh, such context sensitivity may lead
logical response patterns as predictive of their receptiv- to increased vigilance for potential threat. If, however,
ity to socialization. the environment offers positive support, such context
Ordinarily, childrens reduced responsiveness to so- sensitivity may lead to increased receptivity to positive
cialization is maladaptive, and thus might be thought of aspects of the social environment. In either case, chil-
as having been weeded out by natural selection. In con- drens context-sensitivity can be expected to increase
temporary, stable environments, a childs unresponsive- their reproductive success (in the particular ecology that
ness to socialization typically poses a potential problem confronts them). High levels of context sensitivity may
to parents (or teachers). However, Belsky and his col- set the child in quite different directions on a phenotypic
leagues (Belsky, in press; Belsky, Hsieh, & Crnic, 1998) basis. This sensitivity allows children to be able to adapt
suggest that there are advantages for such unresponsive- to the environment they come to experience.
ness under some circumstances. In unpredictable envi- In addition, early experience with stress (or adver-
ronments, such children may be less constrained by a sity) may also heighten the context-sensitivity of the
socialization history that is not well-suited to the chang- child. Children who experience early medical problems
ing demands. For example, it has been suggested (Jensen (and thus experience the stress associated with the
et al., 1997) that children with ADHD might have fared neonatal intensive care unit or stress-inducing medical
well in harsh, unstable environments more characteris- treatments) represent one such example. Research fo-
tic of our evolutionary past. These authors suggested cused on response to early stress or adversity has docu-
that such children show an exceptional readiness to ex- mented the very different life outcomes such children
plore their environment ( high levels of motor activity) may experience based on their parenting history. Al-
and rapidly changing responses (impulsivity) in reac- though early stress may yield negative life outcomes
tion to it. During harsh times, they may have had an when parents fail to buffer children against the effects
edge in avoiding danger and taking advantage of un- of such experiences (the risk pathway), early stress
stable benefits. may also yield exceptionally positive life outcomes
Attention has also been given to variations in chil- when parents provide a high level of support (an out-
drens emotional and physiological reactions as indica- come that may be described as thriving, e.g., Bugen-
tors of their receptivity to socialization. For example, tal, 2003).
Kochanska (1993) has suggested that children are more Infant prematurity represents a case in point, in that it
receptive to socialization to the extent that they are is accompanied by the greater likelihood of early stress,
emotionally reactive to punishment and that they engage combined with limited self-regulation abilities. Although
in effective self-regulation. In infancy, physiological re- children who were born prematurely are hyper-reactive
activity (e.g., heart rate or cortisol elevations) of chil- to environmental stimuli, they often down-regulate their
dren in response to stress acts as a predictor of later responses in ways that lead to hypo-reactivity (suggested
responsiveness to socialization (Gunnar et al., 1995; to serve as protective responses against overstimulation;
Kagan & Snidman, 1991). Children who show this type Tronick, Scanlon, & Scanlon, 1990). Mothers often re-
of reaction to aversive stimuli may be thought of as spond to these reactions with excess stimulation efforts,
showing adaptive neurobehavioral organization (Gunnar which foster further problems (Brachfield, Goldberg, &
et al., 1995). Sloman, 1980); in contrast, when mothers show a re-
sponse style that is better synchronized to infant needs,
Context Sensitivity of Children such children have more positive self-regulatory and in-
It has also been proposed that children are quite gener- teractional outcomes (Feldman, Weller, Sirota, & Eidel-
ally sensitive to context (Boyce & Ellis, in press), with man, 2002).
genotypic variations in this sensitivity. Boyce and Ellis The parenting history of context-sensitive children
conceptualized context-dependent effects as conditional may ultimately influence their health outcomes. For ex-
adaptations that have evolved as a basis for calibrating ample, Bugental and Beaulieu (2004) observed that chil-
the childs response patterns to match their environment dren who were born at medical risk (as assessed by their
412 Socialization Processes

premature status or elevated Apgar scores) demon- predictive of the propensity to become abusive in their
strated either exceptionally positive or negative health relationships with their own children (Bugental & Hap-
outcomes as toddlers as a function of their parenting paney, 2004). In the same way, socialization history
history. Those born at medical risk (in comparison with leads some children to acquire a victim schema, which
low-risk children) demonstrated exceptionally positive is predictive of their being more likely to be victimized
health outcomes if their mothers participated in a cogni- by other children (Perry, Hodges, & Egan, 2001). Even
tively based home visitation intervention (which, in broader links are found between childrens representa-
turn, fostered reductions in maternal depression). Med- tions of family, peer, and self; negative representations
ically at-risk children whose mothers were in control are predictive of dysfunctional social behavior and low
conditions were more likely to show negative health out- peer status (Rudolph, Hammen, & Burge, 1995).
comes. In support of the observation that early stress, Relationships with significant others early in life
under appropriate conditions, can have a beneficial ef- serve as the basis for cognitive representations of rela-
fect on health, young adults whose history included tional information. M. Baldwin (1992) proposed that in-
early medical complications have been found to show dividuals, as a result of their relational history, form
exceptionally high levels of health (as well as habitua- cognitive structures that include a script for the inter-
tion to repeated stress) when they had been the recipi- action pattern, a schema for the role of the other person
ents of supportive parenting (Bugental, Beaulieu, in the relationship, and a schema for the role of self in
Cayan, et al., 2004). In contrast, young adults with a his- the relationship. Such relational schemas come to influ-
tory of early medical complications showed exception- ence the individuals expectations and experiences in
ally low levels of health (and poor ability to habituate to subsequent relationships.
repeated stress) when they had been the recipients of
harsh parenting. No equivalent effects were found for The Mediating Role of Hormones
young adults who had not experienced medical risk Developmental neuroscience has provided insights into
early in life. Such findings are consistent with Boyce another route through which socialization may influ-
and Elliss (2000) proposal that parental support and ence childrens outcomes. In doing so, the field has fo-
protection serve as key moderators of the extent to cused on regulation of stress in early relationships,
which context-sensitive children experience negative or including parent-child relationships. In the socialization
positive outcomes. literature, increasing attention has been given to a key
stress regulation systemthe HPA axis. Across ages
(and species), the HPA axis is activated in response to
What Is the Route by Which Childrens events that might pose a threat; for an infant, this could
Experiences Lead to Different Outcomes? include as little as an unfamiliar toy or as much as a
frightening parent.
Childrens outcomes as a result of early experiences can Although an activated HPA axis facilitates the effec-
be mediated by different means. Consideration is given tive management of short-term stress, such is not the
here to both cognitive and biological routes. case if the HPA axis is repeatedly activated without re-
covery. In this case, there is destructive wear and tear
The Mediating Role of the Childs Cognitive
on the system, a process that McEwen (2000) refers to
Representations of the Social World
as allostatic load. As an end product of this process,
One route through which children may come to have dif- the individual became decreasingly able to cope with
ferent life outcomes involves their acquisition of differ- future stress. Over long time periods, the childs dysreg-
ent ways of representing the social world as a function of ulated HPA axis (as indicated either by cortisol hyper-
their early socialization experiences. In family relation- reactivity, hypo-reactivity, or deviant basal levels),
ships, childrens experiences with their own parents following from unrelieved stress early in life, may pro-
form a central influence on their cognitive representa- duce long-term changes in the capacity of the system to
tions of the caregiving relationship (Grusec, Hastings, & respond to stress, and in the effective functioning of
Mammone, 1994). For example, girls who have experi- other parts of the central nervous system that are af-
enced maltreatment early in life come to have a low per- fected by the HPA axis.
ception of their social power in relationship with others When the young child is exposed to very high levels
(Bugental & Shennum, 2002). Such representations are of unbuffered stress, the normatively well-functioning
Outcomes of the Young as a Result of Their Life History 413

HPA axis is overactivated. As a result, this regulatory ment, along with their ability to cope with future
system becomes more limited in its ability to manage sources of stress. As described earlier, these benefits
new sources of stress, and other linked regulatory sys- were mediated by the response of rat mothers to their
tems (e.g., the growth system, the immune system) do distressed pups (Francis et al., 1999). When the pups
not function optimally. Ultimately, the continuous acti- were returned to the litter, their mothers engaged in vig-
vation of the HPA axis (and associated changes in the orous licking and grooming, a response that served to
functioning of other aspects of the central nervous sys- calm the pups and led to changes in the development of
tem) may lead to deficits not only in brain development the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF). This develop-
but also in the social-emotional and cognitive function- mental change led to changes in the expression of behav-
ing of the child and the health and growth of the child ioral, endocrine, and autonomic responses to stress
(e.g., Bremner & Narayan, 1998). (through the activation of forebrain noradrenergic sys-
The immediate effects of an unsupportive environ- tems), which led to an increased ability to manage
ment on childrens stress regulation abilities have been stress, and subsequent increases in their health as adults
demonstrated in a number of different ways. For exam- (as a result of their reduced vulnerability to the effects
ple, maternal depression has often found to be associated of stress-induced illness). This research provides a con-
with childrens stress regulation problems. Depressed crete example of more general processes that are operat-
mothers, as well as mothers subject to panic disorder, are ing in the area of stress and parental support. Thus,
less responsive to infant distress than are nondepressed Francis, Champagne, Liu, and Meaney (1999) propose:
mothers and thus are less likely to buffer the young
against stressful experiences (e.g., Donovan, Leavitt, & the relationship between early life events and health in
Walsh, 1998; Warren et al., 2003). Not surprisingly, chil- adulthood is mediated by parental influences on the devel-
dren of such mothers are dysregulated in their produc- opment of neural systems that underlie the expression of
tion of cortisol (e.g., Bugental et al., 2003; Dawson & behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. (p. 64)
Ashman, 2000; Essex, Klein, Cho, & Kalin, 2002; Field,
1994; Newport, Stowe, & Nemeroff, 2002). In the same Gunnar and her colleagues (Gunnar et al., 2003), in a
way, stress-inducing experiences later on during the life review of the literature, concluded that mother-infant in-
of the child produce problems if those children fail to re- teractions have effects on the neuroendocrine stress ac-
ceive supportive parenting. For example, the loss of a tivity of humans as well as nonhumans. For example,
parent, when combined with a hostile or rejecting family maltreatment or unresponsiveness early in life influ-
environment, predicts long-term dysregulation of the ences stress hormone activity. In addition, children who
HPA axis (e.g., Luecken, 1998). manifest reactive temperament patterns show increased
McEwen (2001) has suggested that stress (in particu- stress responses as the quality of parental care declines.
lar, early stress) may have positive as well as negative
The Mediating Role of Gene Expression
long-term outcomes. There may be circumstances under
which high levels of stress lead to enhanced functioning Molecular biology provides insights into the activation
of the systemenhanced allostatic efficiency may mechanisms by which the effects of the caregiving en-
occur. If stress occurs repeatedly but is regularly fol- vironment are accomplished. Thus, there has been an
lowed with recovery, the HPA axis may become more ef- emerging focus on gene expressionthe ways in which
ficient in the management of future stress. In short, we the environment influences the activation of genes
see the adaptive possibilities for the socializing system (Brown, 1999). This line of inquiry has led to the gen-
to foster resilience (and even exceptionally positive out- eral study of differential gene expression in response
comes) in response to early stress. Such accommodations to environmental variables. Specific to our topic, re-
may operate when the parent serves in a buffering role. searchers have become increasingly concerned with the
What is the route by which parental support serves to ways in which experiences (including socialization ex-
foster the positive outcomes of infants who experience periences) influence gene expression and resultant vari-
early stress? Some of the best evidence comes from ex- ations in brain development (e.g., Bruer & Greenough,
perimental work with nonhuman animals. Meaney and 2001). For example, stress may lead to changes in the
his colleagues (e.g., Meaney et al., 1985) found that rat production of hormones, which serve as messengers to
pups that were exposed to repeated stress ( handling by relevant brain cells and the subsequent expression (acti-
humans) showed later benefits in their brain develop- vation) of genes. Although genes are typically located at
414 Socialization Processes

inactive sites in brain cells, those genes that have most In child development, gene expression responds to love;
recently been activated are repositioned to locations in security; effective role models; stimulating language and
the nucleus in which they are easily accessed in re- cognitive environments; a positive family environment in-
sponse to recurring environmental triggers (Lamond & cluding support, discipline, values and positive directions;
Earnshaw, 1998). and appropriate management of stress and anxiety. (p. 40)
This altered process of gene expression lies at the
heart of the benefits of maternal buffering of pup dis-
tress observed by Meaney and his colleagues (2000). It INTEGRATION
has been suggested that there are two distinct CRF sys-
temsone of which is adaptive and one of which is mal- In reviewing the current status of the field of socializa-
adaptive (Schulkin, Gold, & McEwen, 1998). The first tion, we have presented an integrated account that spans
system leads to restraint of CRF receptor gene expres- many aspects of developmental psychology as well as in-
sion in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus, corporating insights from other disciplines. Countering
which then serves to turn off the HPA axis when the increasing specialization in the field of psychology,
source of threat has passed. The second system fosters there has been a recent movement toward integration in
CRF gene expression in the central nucleus of the amyg- and across fields. The life outcomes of the developing
dala, and the resultant perpetuation of chronic eleva- child have come to be viewed as situated in both the bio-
tions in fear and anxiety. Maternal licking and logical and social-cultural networks of the family. As
grooming of rat pups leads to restraint of CRF receptor one example, Dodge and Pettit (2003) provided a synthe-
gene expression in the hypothalamus, with the net effect sis of studies looking at the impact of biological predis-
that the HPA axis is more easily turned off. In contrast, positions, sociocultural context, parenting, peers, and
the young who are exposed to unbuffered stress are mental processes on chronic conduct problems in adoles-
more likely to experience the continuing costs associ- cence. Similarly, Repetti, Taylor, and Seeman (2002) of-
ated with the involvement of the amygdala. fered a model that links risky parenting with mental and
The extent to which processes that follow early stress physical health disorders, showing linkages between
are reversible is unclear. In addition, it is unclear harsh family experiences, deficits in emotional control,
whether efforts to counteract the effects of early stress emotional expression and social competence, distur-
produce effects at the level of structural changes or bances in physiologic and endocrine system regulation,
functional changes in the brain. Francis, Diorio, Plot- and health-threatening behavior. Relevant processes
sky, and Meaney (2002) concluded that an enriched en- begin even before birth and cross-influence each other
vironment served to reverse the hormonal and fear both in adaptive and maladaptive ways to influence the
responses that resulted from earlier stress but did not mental health, social competencies, physical health, and
alter gluco-corticoid receptor gene expression. Others productivity of the young.
have argued that altered life experiences may indeed act In describing the bridges across coordinated biologi-
back to influence gene expression in ways that reverse cal systems, evolutionary psychology has suggested
the effects of earlier stress (e.g., McEwen, 2001). These basic design features; developmental neuroscience has
questions will undoubtedly stimulate considerable re- suggested potential mediators operating at the level of
search in the coming years. the central nervous system; and behavior genetics has
In summary, behavioral neuroscientists have increas- suggested the ways in which the brain is receptive to
ingly asked about the role of molecular biology in the de- changes in structure and function as a result of the expe-
veloping child. From this perspective, the human riences of the child. Thus, biological systems represent
genome does not automatically create a working brain; recursive processes that involve continuous corrections
instead, hormonal processes are influenced by the envi- of the childs capacities in light of changing environ-
ronment and subsequently act to foster brain develop- ments. Although there are biological constraints on such
ment (via the activation of gene expression), which flexibility, there is also considerable openness to modi-
ultimately promotes the childs social development (e.g., fication. Within this flexibility arises the possibility for
Brown, 1999; Schulkin, Gold, & McEwen, 1998). The the benefits (or costs) that may be experienced by chil-
promise of this field for human development (and social- dren as result of their socialization history. This coun-
ization) is suggested by Brown (1999): ters an early (and limited) view of biological forces as
Integration 415

dictating fixed outcomes for the young as a function of labs concerned with socialization are exploring changes
individual or shared genetic endowment. As develop- that occur in neurotransmitters and hormones and the
mentalists have come to realize the fallacy of the notion functioning of the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary sys-
of nature and nurture as competing forces, they have si- tem. Concern even extends to the level of the brain and
multaneously come to appreciate the mutual facilitation its development. New technology (e.g., functional mag-
that occurs between environment and the brain. netic resonance imaging) and new findings in other
Just as developmentalists have come to understand fields (e.g., research in molecular biology that has impli-
the biosocial processes as recursive and integrated, they cations for gene expression) can be expected to expand
have changed in their conceptualization of the social- horizons still further.
cultural factors that influence the outcomes of the In another direction, socialization researchers now
young. In particular, there has been movement away incorporate knowledge drawn from anthropology and
from the view of effective socialization as involving a cultural psychology to consider the differing processes
fixed design that has general applicability. One of the and outcomes of socialization in different settings. Such
earliest questionings of this approach followed from explorations increase understanding not only of the di-
consideration of the variations between children in tem- versity of experiences that foster healthy outcomes for
peramentand thus the socialization methods likely to the young but also the ways in which there is continuity
be effective. Subsequently, there was a questioning of across cultures in the socialization of the young. At the
the effectiveness of different patterns of socialization same time, consideration is given to the possibility that
across cultures. It became apparent that there were many there are unhealthy societies (e.g., societies that foster
variations possible in socialization practices and styles extreme discrepancies in the opportunities for the young
that lead children to live healthy, happy, productive lives or that foster continuing stress without recovery) that
in their cultural framework. Finally, increasing consid- limit the life experiences of the young.
eration has been given to the role of social context (or Finally, the ways in which investigators in the field of
domain) in determining the differing processes that op- socialization operate is changing. With the increasing
erate in socialization. In different contexts and settings complexity of knowledge and methodology comes the
(acting in the service of different goals or tasks), social- need for greater collaborative efforts across labs and
ization appears to operate according to different algo- across disciplines. Behavioral neuroscientists now work
rithms. Such algorithms have biological, motivational, either to incorporate the concepts drawn from socializa-
behavioral, and cognitive components that work together tion theory or to collaborate with socialization re-
to facilitate the accomplishment of the relevant social- searchers to bridge understanding of processes that
ization goals or tasks. occur across species or that differ in important ways
The models used to represent socialization are shift- across species. In those projects concerned with human
ing in ways that reflect the changing processes thought socialization, researchers have capitalized on the oppor-
to operate. Unidirectional models (and exclusive re- tunities for determining the outcomes of natural exper-
liance on self-report) can be expected to be replaced iments. Researcher teams (as notable illustrations,
with bidirectional models that make use of an expanded Rutter and his colleagues, e.g., Rutter & OConnor,
range of dependent (and mediating) variables. In addi- 2004; and Gunnar and her colleagues, e.g., Gunnar,
tion, the increased focus on context can be expected to Morison, Chisholm, & Schuder, 2001) have tracked (and
lead researchers to make greater use of moderator mod- continue to track) the changes in life outcomes that fol-
els in which socialization outcomes are measured differ- low from the socialization history introduced into the
entially as a function of contextual variables. In doing lives of adopted children. In conceptualizing the out-
so, conditional models can be used to represent the cir- comes of children who experience adversity early in
cumstances under which different types of socialization life, there are also changing expectations. The circum-
processes may operate or under which different biologi- stances that foster later risk have expanded to consider
cal switching mechanisms may occur. not only the circumstances that allow resilience but also
Along with changes in the ways of conceptualizing the circumstances that foster advantage or thriving.
socialization, there have been corresponding changes in In these changes are hopeful outlooks for social pol-
the variables of interest. In one direction, there has been icy. As developmentalists come to understand the ways
an increasing interest in physiological processes. Many the life course of the young changes as a result of their
416 Socialization Processes

socialization history, doors open for ways to foster the dren and adolescents (pp. 263289). Washington, DC: American
opportunities that are available for them. National fund- Psychological Association.
Barber, B. K., & Harmon, E. L. (2002). Violating the self: Parental
ing agencies concerned with facilitating physical and
psychological control of children and adolescents. In B. K. Barber
mental health have created opportunities for researchers (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control af fects chil-
across many disciplines to come together to consider dren and adolescents (pp. 1552). Washington, DC: American
how shared knowledge can encourage new collabora- Psychological Association.
Barratt, M., Negayama, K., & Mirami, T. (1993). The social environ-
tions, allow new hypotheses, and resolve puzzles that ments of early infancy in Japan and the United States. Early De-
plague researchers who may acquire new insights re- velopment and Parenting, 2, 5164.
garding resolutions available from other fields. Finally, Barry, H. I., Child, I. L., & Bacon, M. K. (1959). Relations of child train-
it becomes the obligation of such collaborative teams to ing to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 5163.
Bartoshuk, L. M., & Beauchamp, O. K. (1994). Chemical senses. An-
inform policymakers of the possibility and feasibility of
nual Review of Psychology, 45, 419449.
sustainable socializing environments that can be ex- Bates, J. E. (1980). The concept of difficult temperament. Merrill-
pected to optimize the life outcomes of the young. Palmer Quarterly, 26, 299319.
Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Ridge, B. (1998). Inter-
action of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive par-
enting in the development of externalizing behavior.
REFERENCES Developmental Psychology, 34, 982995.
Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990). Anxiety and social exclu-
Abbott, D. H., Keverne, E. B., Bercovitch, F. B., Sjively, C. A., Men- sion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 165195.
doza, S. P., Saltzman, W., et al. (2003). Are subordinates always Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns
stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 73, 4388.
levels among primates. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 6782. Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Develop-
Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of in-group favoritism and out- mental Psychology, 4, 1103.
group prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable environment is not good
Developmental Psychology, 39, 4860. enough: A response to Scarr. Child Development, 64, 12991317.
Aboud, F. E., & Doyle, A. B. (1996). Parental and peer inf luences on Baumrind, D., Larzelere, R. E., & Cowan, P. A. (2002). Ordinary
childrens racial attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural physical punishment: Is it harmful? Comment on Gershoff, 2002.
Relations, 20, 371383. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 580589.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Baumrind, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2002). The ethics of parenting. In
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situa- M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 5, pp. 334).
tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to Bekoff, M., & Allen, C. (2002). The evolution of social play: Interdis-
people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. ciplinary analysis of cognitive processes. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen,
Child Development, 55, 12781289. & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal (pp. 429435).
Baldwin, A. (1955). Behavior and development in childhood. New London: MIT Press.
York: Dryden Press. Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in
Baldwin, A. L., Baldwin, C., & Cole, R. E. (1990). Stress-resistant studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 8195.
families and stress-resistant children. In J. E. Rolf, A. S. Masten, Belsky, J. (1999). Interactional and contextual determinants of at-
D. Ciccchetti, K. H. Nuechterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk tachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
and protective factors in the development of psychopathology of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications
(pp. 257280). New York: Cambridge University Press. (pp. 249264). New York: Guilford Press.
Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of so- Belsky, J. (in press). Differential susceptibility in rearing inf luence:
cial information. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461484. An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In B. Ellis & D.
Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psy-
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
chology and child development. New York: Guilford Press.
Prentice-Hall.
Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A so-
infant negativity as antecedents of boys externalizing problems
cial cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
and inhibition at age 3: Differential susceptibility to rearing in-
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning theory and per- f luence? Development and Psychopathology, 10, 301319.
sonality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience,
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a ne- interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolu-
glected construct. Child Development, 67, 32963319. tionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647670.
Barber, B. K. (Ed.). (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychological Benoit , D., & Parker, K. C. H. (1994). Stability and transmission
control af fects children and adolescents. Washington, DC: Ameri- of attachment across three generations. Child Development,
can Psychological Association. 65, 1444 1456.
Barber, B. K., Bean, R. L., & Erickson, L. D. (2002). Expanding the Bernstein, I. S., & Ehardt, C. L. (1985). Age-sex differences in the
study and understanding of psychological control. In B. K. Barber expression of agonistic behavior in rhesus monkey (Macca mu-
(Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control af fects chil- latta) groups. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 115132.
References 417

Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: Comparative Bretherton, I., Golby, B., & Cho, E. (1997). Attachment and the
studies in cultural and psychological adaptation. New York: Sage. transmission of values. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.),
Bigler, R. S. (1999). The use of multicultural curricula and materials Parenting and childrens internalization of values: A handbook of
to counter racism in children. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 687705. contemporary theory (pp. 103134). New York: Wiley.
Bigler, R. S., Spears Brown, C., & Markell, M. (2001). When Brewer, M. B. (1999a). Multiple identities and identity transition:
groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the for- Implications for Hong Kong. International Journal of Intercultural
mation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, Relations, 23, 167197.
72, 11511162. Brewer, M. B. (1999b). The psychology of prejudice: In-group love or
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2000). Child development and out-group hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429444.
evolutionary psychology. Child Development, 71, 16871708. Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this we? Levels of
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of human na- collective identity and self-representation. Journal of Personality
ture: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington, DC: and Social Psychology, 71, 8393.
American Psychological Association. Bridges, R. S. (1996). Biochemical basis of parental behavior in the
Blair, C. (2002). Early intervention for low birth weight preterm in- rat. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 25, 215242.
fants: The role of negative emotionality in the specification of ef-
Brody, G., Ge, X., Conger, R., Gibbons, F. X., McBride, M. V., Ger-
fects. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 311332.
rard, M., et al. (2001). The inf luence of neighborhood disadvan-
Blatt, S. J. (1995). Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition: Two tage, collective socialization, and parenting on African
personality configurations and their implications for psychopath- American childrens affiliation with deviant peers. Child Devel-
ology and psychotherapy. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dis- opment, 72, 12311246.
sociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
and health (pp. 299335). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Blatt, S. J. (1998). Contributions of psychoanalysis to the under-
Brown, B. (1999). Optimizing expression of the common human
standing and treatment of depression. Journal of the American
genome for child development. Current Directions in Psychologi-
Psychoanalytic Association, 46, 723752.
cal Science, 8, 3741.
Blum-Kulka, S. (1997). Dinner talk: Cultural patterns of sociability
and socialization in family discourse. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Bruer, J. T., & Greenough, W. T. (2001). The subtle science of how
experience affects the brain. In D. B. Bailey, J. T. Bruer, F. J.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Azuma, H., Galerin, C., Maital, S.,
Symons, & J. W. Lichtman (Eds.), Critical thinking about critical
Ogino, M., et al. (1998). A cross-sectional study of self-
periods (pp. 209232). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
evaluations and attributions in parenting: Argentina, Belgium,
France, Israel, Italy, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Bugental, D. B. (1992). Affective and cognitive processes within
Psychology, 34, 662676. threat-oriented family systems. In I. E. Sigel, A. McGillicuddy-
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New DeLisi, & J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems: The psy-
York: Basic Books. chological consequences for children (pp. 219248). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York:
Basic Books. Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A
domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 26, 187209.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York:
Basic Books. Bugental, D. B. (2003). Thriving in the face of childhood adversity.
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (in press). Biological sensitivity to context: New York: Psychology Press.
Pt. 1. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and Bugental, D. B., & Beaulieu, D. A. (2004). Maltreatment risk among
functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology. disabled children: A bio-social-cognitive approach. In R. Kail
Brachfield, S., Goldberg, S., & Sloman, J. (1980). Parent-infant inter- (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31,
action in free play at 8 and 12 months: Effects of prematurity and pp. 129164). New York: Academic Press.
immaturity. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 289305. Bugental, D. B., Beaulieu, D., Cayan, L., Fowler, E., OBrien, E., &
Breedlove, S. (1992). Sexual differentiation of the brain and behav- Kokotay, S. (2004). Stress immunization in young adults: The
ior. In J. Becker, S. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral en- role of early medical adversity and supportive parenting. Unpub-
docrinology (pp. 3968). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. lished paper.
Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1966). Animal behavior. New York: Macmillan. Bugental, D. B., Brown, M., & Reiss, C. (1996). Cognitive represen-
Bremner, J. D., & Narayan, M. (1998). The effects of stress on mem- tations of power in caregiving relationships: Biasing effects on in-
ory and the hippocampus throughout the life cycle: Implications terpersonal interaction and information processing. Journal of
for childhood development and aging. Development and Psycho- Family Psychology, 10, 397407.
pathology, 10, 855871. Bugental, D. B., & Goodnow, J. G. (1998). Socialization processes. In
Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attach- W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
ment, affect regulation, and romantic functioning. Personality book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 267283. development (5th ed., pp. 389462). New York: Wiley.
Bretherton, I. (1980). Young children in stressful situations: The sup- Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K. (2004). Predicting infant maltreat-
porting role of attachment figures and unfamiliar caregivers. In ment in low-income families: The interactive effects of maternal
G. V. Coelho & P. I. Ahmed (Eds.), Uprooting and development attributions and child status at birth. Developmental Psychology,
(pp. 179210). New York: Plenum Press. 40, 234243.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Pt. 1. Retrospect and Bugental, D. B., & Johnston, C. (2000). Parental and child cogni-
prospect. Monograph of the Society for Research on Child Develop- tions in the context of the family. Annual Review of Psychology,
ment (1/2, Serial No. 209), 335. 51, 315344.
418 Socialization Processes

Bugental, D. B., Johnston, C., New, M., & Silvester, J. (1998). Mea- Colder, C. R., Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (1997). The moderating
suring parental attributions: Conceptual and methodological is- effects of childrens fear and activity level on relations between
sues. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 459480. parenting practices and childhood symptomatology. Journal of
Bugental, D. B., Lyon, J. E., Cortez, V., & Krantz, J. (1997). Whos Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 251263.
the boss? Accessibility of dominance ideation among individuals Collins, W. A., Gleason, T., & Sesma, A. (1997). Internalization, au-
with low perceptions of interpersonal power. Journal of Personal- tonomy, and relationships: Development during adolescence. In
ity and Social Psychology, 72, 12971309. J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens in-
Bugental, D. B., Martorell, G. A., & Barraza, V. (2003). The hor- ternalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory
monal costs of subtle forms of infant maltreatment. Hormones and (pp. 7899). New York: Wiley.
Behavior, 43, 237244. Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., &
Bugental, D. B., & Shennum, W. A. (2002). Gender, power, and vio- Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The
lence in the family [Special issue]. Child Maltreatment, 7, 5664. case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232.
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Sheridan, J. F., & McClintock, Corsaro, W. A. (1997). The sociology of childhood. Thousand Oaks,
M. K. (2000). Multilevel integrative analyses of human behav- CA: Pine Forge Press.
ior: Social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social
and biological approaches [Special issue]. Psychological Bulletin, exchange. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The
12, 829843. adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of cul-
Cameron, J. A., Alvarez, J. M., Ruble, D. N., & Fuligni, A. J. (2001). ture (pp. 163228). London: Oxford University Press.
Childrens lay theories about ingroups and outgroups. Personality Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (2000). When partners become parents:
and Social Psychology Review, 5, 118128. The big life change for couples. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Caporael, L. R., & Brewer, M. B. (1991). Reviving evolutionary psychol-
Cox, M. J., & Paley, B. (1997). Families as systems. Annual Review of
ogy: Biology meets society. Journal of Social Issues, 47, 187195.
Psychology, 48, 243267.
Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment
Cox, M. J., Paley, B., & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conf lict and
and love [Special issue]. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779818.
parent-child relationships. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.),
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et Interparental conf lict and child development (pp. 249272). Cam-
al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated bridge: Cambridge University Press.
children. Science, 297, 851854.
Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., & Ingoldsby, E. M. (2003). Mother-son
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Inf luences of attachment re-
positive synchrony in middle childhood: Relation to antisocial be-
lationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De-
havior. Social Development, 12, 379400.
velopment, 59, 228283.
Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian par-
and punitive control of toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child De-
enting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cul-
velopment, 58, 964975.
tural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 11111119.
Chao, R. K. (1995). Chinese and European American cultural mod- Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitoring and
els of the self ref lected in mothers childrearing beliefs. Ethos, knowledge of children. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
23, 328354. parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 461483). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chao, R. K. (2001). Extending the research on the consequences of Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erikson, H., Updegraff, K., & McHale, S. M.
parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. (1999). Conditions underlying parents knowledge about chil-
Child Development, 72, 18321843. drens daily lives in middle childhood: Between- and within-fam-
ily comparisons. Child Development, 70, 246259.
Chao, R. K. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied par- Cummings, E. M., & Wilson, A. (1999). Contexts of marital conf lict
enting (2nd ed., pp. 5993). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. and childrens emotional security: Exploring the distinction be-
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The tween constructive and destructive conf lict from the childrens
perception-behavior link and social interactions. Journal of Per- perspective. In M. Cox & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Conf lict and
sonality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910. closeness in families: Causes and consequences (pp. 105129).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Social and nonsocial knowl-
edge in vervet monkeys. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Dabbs, J. M. (2000). Heroes, rogues, and lovers. New York: McGraw-
Society of London, 308, 187201. Hill.
Chirkov, V., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). Parent and teacher autonomy-sup- Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1984). A sociobiological analysis of human
port in Russian & U.S. adolescents: Common effects on well- infanticide. In G. Hausfater & S. B. Hrdy (Eds.), Infanticide:
being and academic motivation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Comparative and evolutionary perspectives (pp. 487502). New
Psychology, 32, 618635. York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge. Cam- Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An in-
bridge, MA: MIT Press. tegrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487496.
Coe, C. L. (1990). Psychobiology of maternal behavior in nonhuman Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2005). Mothers knowledge of chil-
primates. In N. A. Krasnegor & R. S. Bridges (Eds.), Mammalian drens reactions to discipline and its role in compliance and inter-
parenting (pp. 157183). Oxford: Oxford University Press. nalized conduct. Unpublished paper.
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behav- Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (in press). Untangling the links of
ior. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psy- parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes.
chology (Vol. 3, pp. 779862). New York: Wiley. Child Development.
References 419

Dawson, G., & Ashman, S. B. (2000). On the origins of a vulnera- and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child
bility to depression: The inf luence of the early social environ- Development, 62, 13521366.
ment on the development of psychobiological systems related to Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1985). Becoming a family member: Family
risk for affective disorder. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota conf lict and the development of understanding. Child Develop-
Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 31. The ef fects of early ad- ment, 56, 480492.
versity on neurobehavioral development (pp. 245280). Mah-
Eccles, J. (2002). Introduction. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive par-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
enting: How psychological control af fects children and adoles-
Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. cents (pp. 12). Washington, DC: American Psychological
(1996). Physical discipline among African American and Euro- Association.
pean American mothers: Links to childrens externalizing behav-
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy,
iors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 10561072.
B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to childrens nega-
Deater-Deckard, K., & OConnor, T. G. (2000). Parent-child mutual- tive emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of childrens so-
ity in early childhood: Two behavioral genetic studies. Develop- cial functioning. Child Development, 70, 513534.
mental Psychology, 16, 15611570.
Elder, G. H., & Rockwell, C. (1979). The life-course and human de-
DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1987). Of human bonding: New- velopment: An ecological perspective. International Journal of
borns prefer their mothers voices. In J. Oates & S. Sheldon Behavioral Development, 21, 121.
(Eds.), Cognitive development in infancy (pp. 111118). Hills-
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., &
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and indi-
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: vidual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: A
Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Sym- longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Journal of Personality
posium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation and Social Psychology, 77, 387401.
(pp. 237288). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Emde, R. N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R. B., & Oppenheim, D. (1991).
Dekovic, M., Gerris, J. R., & Janssens, J. M. (1991). Parental cogni- The moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowl-
tions, parental behavior, and the childs understanding of the par- edge. Developmental Review, 11, 251270.
ent-child relationship. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 523541.
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Interna-
De Pue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). tional Universities Press.
Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-
Essex, M. J., Klein, M. H., Cho, E., & Kalin, N. H. (2002). Maternal
induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of
stress beginning in infancy may sensitize children to later stress
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 485498.
exposure: Effects on cortisol and behavior. Biological Psychiatry,
Dhar, R. (1997). Consumer preference for a no-choice option. Journal 52, 776784.
of Consumer Research, 24, 215231.
Evans, D. W., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, C.,
Dishion, T. J., Andrews, D. W., & Crosby, L. (1995). Antisocial King, R. A., et al. (1997). Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: The
boys and their friends in early adolescence: Relationship charac- prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in nor-
teristics, quality, and interactional process. Child Development, mal young children. Child Development, 68, 5868.
66, 139151.
Fagan, J. F., & Singer, L. T. (1979). The role of simple feature differ-
Dix, T. (1992). Parenting on behalf of the child: Empathic goals in
ences in infants recognition of faces. Infant Behavior and Devel-
the regulation of responsive parenting. In I. E. Sigel, A. V.
opment, 2, 3945.
McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief sys-
tems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd ed., Feldman, R., Weller, A., Sirota, L., & Eidelman, A. I. (2002). Skin-
pp. 319346). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) promotes self-regulation in pre-
mature infants: Sleep-wake cyclicity, arousal modulation, and
Dix, T., Ruble, D. N., & Zambarano, R. (1989). Mothers implicit the-
sustained exploration. Developmental Psychology, 38, 194207.
ories of discipline: Child effects, parent effects, and the attribu-
tion process. Child Development, 60, 13731391. Ferguson, J. N., Aldag, J. M., & Insel, T. R. (2001). Oxytocin in the
medial amygdala is essential for social recognition in the mouse.
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the
Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 82788285.
development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 39, 349371. Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness
Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Walsh, R. O. (1998). Conf lict and to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Devel-
depression predict maternal sensitivity to infant cries. Infant Be- opment, 64, 657674.
havior and Development, 21, 505517. Fernald, A., Taescdhner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., DeBoyson-
Dubas, J. S., & Gerris, J. R. M. (2002). Longitudinal changes in the Bardies, B., & Fukui, J. (1989). A cross-language study of
time parents spend in activities with their adolescent children as a prosodic modifications of mothers and fathers speech to prever-
function of child age, pubertal status and gender. Journal of Fam- bal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477501.
ily Psychology, 16, 415462. Field, T. M. (1994). The effects of mothers physical and emotional
Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1991). Becoming American or English? Talk- unavailability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society
ing about the social world in England and the United States. In for Research in Child Development, 59, 208227, 250283.
M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting: Cross- Field, T. M. (1998). Touch therapy effects on development. Interna-
currents in contemporary psychology (pp. 155172). Hillsdale, tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 22, 779797.
NJ: Erlbaum. Field, T. M., Cohen, D., Garcia, R., & Greenberg, R. (1984). Mother-
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Infant Behavior and
(1991). Young childrens understanding of other peoples feelings Development, 7, 1925.
420 Socialization Processes

Field, T., Greenwald, P., Morrow, C. J., Healy, B. T., Foster, T., Fuligni, A. J., Yip, T., & Tseng, V. (2002). The impact of family obli-
Guthertz, M., et al. (1992). Behavior state matching during inter- gation on the daily activities and psychological well-being of Chi-
actions of preadolescent friends versus acquaintances. Develop- nese American adolescents. Child Development, 73, 302314.
mental Psychology, 28, 242250. Gallese, V., Ferraari, P., Kohler, E., & Fogassi, L. (2002). The eyes,
Fiese, B. H., Tomcho, T. J., Douglas, M., Josephs, K., Poltrock, S., & the hand, and the mind: Behavioral and neurophysiological as-
Baker, T. (2002). A review of 50 years of research on naturally pects of social cognition. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M.
occurring family routines and rituals: Cause for celebration? Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal (pp. 451461). London:
Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 381390. MIT Press.
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1996). Relation of cue to consequence
The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. in avoidance learning. In L. D. Houck & L. C. Drickamer (Eds.),
Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th Foundations of animal behavior: Classic papers with commen-
ed., Vol. 2, pp. 915981). Boston: McGraw-Hill. taries (pp. 374375). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Fleming, A. S. (1990). Hormonal and experiential correlates of ma- Gardner, F. (1994). The quality of joint activity between mothers and
ternal responsiveness in human mothers. In N. A. Krasnegor & their children with behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychol-
R. S. Bridges (Eds.), Mammalian parenting (pp. 184208). Ox- ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 935948.
ford, England: Oxford University Press. Gardner, F., Ward, S., Burton, J., & Wilson, C. (2003). The role of
Fleming, A. S., Corter, C., Stallings, J., & Steiner, M. (2002). mother-child joint play in the early development of childrens con-
Testosterone and prolactin are associated with emotional re- duct problems: A longitudinal observational study. Social Devel-
sponses to infant cries in new fathers. Hormones and Behavior, opment, 12, 361378.
42, 399413.
Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human
Fleming, A. S., Steiner, M., & Corter, C. (1997). Cortisol, hedonics, paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 26, 5577.
and maternal responsiveness in human mothers. Hormones and
Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental
Behavior, 32, 8598.
psychology. Child Development, 71, 5765.
Flinn, M. V., Quinlan, R. J., Decker, S. A., Turner, M. T., & England,
B. G. (1996). Male-female differences in effects of parental ab- George, C., & Solomon, J. (1996). Representational models of rela-
sence on glucorticoid stress response. Human Nature, 7, 125162. tionships: Links between caregiving and attachment. Infant Men-
tal Health Journal, 17, 198216.
Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Terror management in child-
hood: Does death conceptualization moderate the effects of mor- Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associ-
tality salience on acceptance of different others? Personality and ated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theo-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 11041112. retical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539579.
Fogel, A., & Branco, A. U. (1997). Meta-communication as a source Gilbert, P., & McGuire, M. T. (1998). Shame, status, and social roles:
of indetermination in relationship development. In A. Fogel, M. C. Psychobiology and evolution. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.),
Lyra, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in devel- Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture
opmental and social processes (pp. 6592). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (pp. 99125). New York: Oxford University Press.
Forman, D. R., & Kochanska, G. (2001). Viewing imitation as child Goldberg, S., Grusec, J. E., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). Confidence in
responsiveness: A link between teaching and discipline domains protection: Arguments for a narrow definition of attachment.
of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 37, 198206. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 475483.
Francis, D. D., Caldji, C., Champagne, F., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, Goldsmith, H. H., & Lemery, K. S. (2000). Linking temperamental
M. J. (1999). The role of corticotropin-releasing factor-norepi- fearfulness and anxiety symptoms: A behavior-genetic perspec-
nephrine systems in mediating the effects of early experience on tive. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 11991209.
the development of behavioral and endocrine responses to stress. Goldstein, S., Field, T., & Healy, B. T. (1989). Concordance of play
Biological Psychiatry, 46, 11531166. behavior and physiology in preschool friends. Journal of Applied
Francis, D. D., Champagne, F. A., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Developmental Psychology, 10, 337351.
Maternal care, gene expression, and the development of individual
Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Acceptable disagreement across generations.
differences in stress reactivity. In N. E. Adler & M. Marmot
In J. Smetana (Ed.), Beliefs about parenting: Origins and develop-
(Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 896. So-
mental implications (pp. 5164). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
cioeconomic status and health in industrial nationsSocial, psy-
chological, and biological pathways (pp. 6684). New York: New Goodnow, J. J. (1997). Parenting and the transmission and internal-
York Academy of Sciences. ization of values: From social-cultural perspectives to within-
Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nonge- family analyses. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Handbook
nomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and of parenting and the transmission of values (pp. 333361). New
stress responses in rats. Science, 286, 11551158. York: Wiley.
Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (2002). En- Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, A. W. (1990). Development according to
vironmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separa- parents: The nature, sources, and consequences of parents ideas.
tion on stress reactivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 78407843. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Freud, S. (1965). Normality and pathology in childhood. New York: Goodnow, J. J., Miller, P. J., & Kessel, F. (Eds.). (1995). Cultural
International Universities Press. practices as contexts for development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Fuligni, A. J., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes toward family Gray, P. B., Kahlenberg, S. M., Barrett, E. S., Lipson, S. F., & Elli-
obligations among American adolescents with Asian, Latin Amer- son, P. T. (2002). Marriage and fatherhood are associated with
ican, and European backgrounds. Child Development, 70, lower testosterone in males. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23,
1103011044. 193201.
References 421

Green, J. A., & Gustafson, G. E. (1983). Individual recognition of ethnotheories of the child. In S. Harkness, C. Raeff, & C. M.
human infants on the basis of cries alone. Developmental Psy- Super (Eds.), Variability in the social construction of the child:
chobiology, 16, 485493. Vol. 87. New directions for child and adolescent development
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). (pp. 2339). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review Harlow, H., & Harlow, M. (1965). The affectional system. In A.
of Psychology, 54, 461490. Schrier, H. F. Harlow, & F. Stolnitz (Eds.), Behavior of nonhuman
Greenwald, A. B., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cogni- primates (Vol. 2, pp. 287334). New York: Academic Press.
tion: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Re- Harlow, H. F. (1973). Learning to love. Oxford, England: Ballan-
view, 102, 427. tine Books.
Grolnick W. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How well- Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the childs environment? A group social-
meant parenting backfires. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458489.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization Hastings, P. D., & Grusec, J. E. (1997). Conf lict outcomes as a func-
within the family: The self-determination theory perspective. In tion of parental accuracy in perceiving child cognitions and af-
J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and childrens in- fect. Social Development, 6, 7690.
ternalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory Hastings, P. D., & Grusec, J. E. (1998). Parenting goals as organizers
(pp. 135161). New York: Wiley. of responses to parent-child disagreement. Developmental Psy-
Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. R. (1986). Individuation in family re- chology, 34, 465479.
lationships: A perspective on individual differences in the devel- Helwig, C. C., Arnold, M. L., Tan, D., & Boyd, D. (2003). Chinese
opment of identity and role-taking skill in adolescence. Human adolescents reasoning about democratic and authority-based de-
Development, 29, 82100. cision making in peer, family, and school contexts. Child Develop-
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). The impact of parental disci- ment, 74, 783800.
pline methods on the childs internalization of values: A recon- Herman, B. H., & Panksepp, J. (1981). Ascending endorphin inhibi-
ceptualization of current points of view. Developmental tion with distress vocalization. Science, 211, 10601062.
Psychology, 30, 419. Hertsgaard, L., Gunnar, M., Erickson, M. F., & Nachmias, M. (1995).
Grusec, J. E., Goodnow, J. J., & Kuczynski, L. (2000). New directions Adrenocortical responses to the strange situation in infants with
in analyses of parenting contributions to childrens acquisition of disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships. Child Develop-
values. Child Development, 71, 205211. ment, 66, 11001106.
Grusec, J. E., Hastings, P., & Mammone, N. (1994). Parenting cogni- Hertwig, R., Davis, J. N., & Sulloway, F. J. (2002). Parental invest-
tions and relationship schemas. In J. Smetana (Ed.), Beliefs about ment: How an equity motive can produce inequality. Psychological
parenting: Origins and developmental implications (pp. 520). Bulletin, 128, 728745.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Herz, L., & Gullone, E. (1999). The relationship between self-esteem
Grusec, J. E., & Redler, E. (1980). Attribution, reinforcement, and and parenting style: A cross-cultural comparison of Australian
altruism: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, and Vietnamese Australian adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural
16, 525534. Psychology, 30, 742761.
Gubernick, D. J., Sengelaug, D. R., & Kurz, E. M. (1993). A neu- Hetherington, E. M. (1993). An overview of the Virginia Longitudi-
roanatomical correlate of paternal and maternal behavior in the nal Study of Divorce and Remarriage with a focus on early ado-
biparental California mouse (Peromyscus californicus). Behav- lescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 3956.
ioral Neuroscience, 107, 194201. Higley, J. D., Hopkins, W. D., Thompson, W. W., Byrne, E. A.,
Gunnar, M. R. (2000). Early adversity and the development of stress re- Hirsch, R. M., & Suomi, S. J. (1992). Peers as primary attachment
activity and regulation. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia sources in yearling rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Develop-
on Child Psychology: Vol. 31. The ef fects of early adversity on neu- mental Psychology, 28, 11631171.
robehavioral development (pp. 163200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and
Gunnar, M. R., Larson, M. C., Hertsgaard, L., Harris, M. L., & childs construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Brodersen, L. (1992). The stressfulness of separation among 9- Hirschfeld, L. A. (2001). On a folk theory of society: Children, evo-
month-old infants: Effects of social context variables and infant lution, and mental representations of social groups. Personality
temperament. Child Development, 63, 290303. and Social Psychology Review, 5, 107117.
Gunnar, M. R., Morison, S. J., Chisholm, K., & Schuder, M. Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Toward a topography of
(2001). Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Ro- mind: An introduction to domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld
manian orphanages [Special issue]. Development and Psycho- & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind (pp. 336). Cambridge,
pathology, 13, 611628. England: Cambridge University Press.
Gunnar, M. R., Porter, F. L., Wolfe, C. M., Rigatuso, J., & Larson, Hockenberry-Eaton, M., Kemp, V., & DiLorio, C. (1994). Cancer
M. C. (1995). Nenonatal stress reactivity: Predictions to later stressors and protective factors: Predictors of stress experienced
emotional temperament. Child Development, 66, 113. during treatment for childhood cancer. Research in Nursing and
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). Genetical evolution of social behavior. Jour- Health, 17, 351361.
nal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152. Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early social relationships: A psychobiologists
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2002). Biology and ecology of parent- view. Child Development, 58, 633647.
ing. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (2nd ed., Hofer, M. A. (1996). Multiple regulation of ultrasonic vocalization.
Vol. 2, pp. 253280). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21, 203217.
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., & van Tijen, N. (2000). Individualism and Hoffman, L. W. (1985). The changing genetics/socialization balance.
the Western mind reconsidered: American and Dutch parents Journal of Social Issues, 41, 127148.
422 Socialization Processes

Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S., Gelfand, M. J., &
Carmichaels manual of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 261360). Yuki, S. (1995). Culture, gender, and self: A perspective from in-
New York: Wiley. dividualism-collectivism research. Journal of Personality and So-
Hoffman, M. L. (1981). Is altruism part of human nature? Journal of cial Psychology, 69, 925937.
Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 121137. Keating, C. F., & Bai, D. L. (1986). Childrens attributions of social
Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral in- dominance from facial cues. Child Development, 57, 12691276.
ternalization. In E. T. Higgins, D. Ruble, & W. Hartup (Eds.), So- Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Kuensemueller, P., Abels, M., Yovsi, R.,
cial cognition and social development: A sociocultural perspective Voelker, S., et al. (2004). The bio-culture of parenting: Evidence
(pp. 236274). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. from five cultural communities. Parenting, 4, 2550.
Hofstede, G. (1983). National cultures revisited. Behavior Science Re- Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A the-
search, 18, 285305. ory of interdependence. New York: Wiley.
Holden G. (2002). Perspectives on the effects of corporal punishment: Kenny, D. A., Korchmaros, J. D., & Bolger, N. (2003). Lower level me-
Comment on Gershoff, 2002. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 590595. diation in multilevel models. Psychological Methods, 8, 115128.
Holden, G. W., & Ritchie, K. L. (1988). Child rearing and the dialec- Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it,
tics of parental intelligence. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Child develop- and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a
ment within culturally structured environments: Parental cognition reinterpretation of monitoring. Child Development, 36, 366380.
and adult-child interaction (pp. 3059). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Keverne, E. B., Nevison, C. M., & Martel, F. L. (1997). Early learn-
Horney, K. (1933). Maternal conf licts. American Journal of Or- ing and the social bond. In C. S. Carter & I. Lederhendler (Eds.),
thopsychiatry, 3, 445463. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 807. The inte-
Hsu, F. L. K. (1983). Rugged individualism reconsidered: Essays in psy- grative neurobiology of af filiations (pp. 329339). New York: New
chological anthropology. Knoxville: University of Tennessee Press. York Academy of Sciences.
Inoff-Germain, F., Arnold, G. S., Nottelmann, E. D., Susman, E. J., Kilgore, K., Snyder, J., & Lentz, C. (2000). The contribution of
Cutler, G. B., & Chrousos, G. P. (1988). Relations between hor- parental discipline, parental monitoring, and school risk to early-
mone levels and observational measures of aggressive behavior of onset conduct problems in African American boys and girls. De-
young adolescents in family interactions. Developmental Psychol- velopmental Psychology, 36, 835845.
ogy, 24, 129139. Killen, J., & Wainryb, C. (2000). Independence and interdependence
Isley, S., ONeil, R., & Parke, R. D. (1996). The relation of parental in diverse cultural contexts. In S. Harkness, C. Raeff, & C. M.
affect and control behaviors to childrens classroom acceptance: Super (Eds.), New directions for child and adolescent develop-
A concurrent and predictive analysis. Early Education and Devel- ment: Vol. 87. Variability in the social construction of the child
opment, 7, 723. (pp. 521). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Kim, K., & Rohner, R. P. (2002). Parental warmth, control, and in-
Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality volvement in schooling: Predicting academic achievement among
and Social Psychology, 79, 9951006. Korean American adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-
Jacklin, C. N., DiPietro, J. A., & Maccoby, E. E. (1984). Sex-typing ogy, 33, 127140.
behavior and sex-typing presence in child/parent interaction [Spe- Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O., & Elder, G. (2001). Parent-
cial issue]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 13, 413425. adolescent reciprocity in negative affect and its relation to early
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Polo-Tomas, M., Price, T. S., & adult social development. Developmental Psychology, 37, 775790.
Taylor, A. (2004). The limits of child effects: Evidence for genet- Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H.,
ically-mediated child effects on corporal punishment, but not on et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition.
physical maltreatment. Developmental Psychology, 40, 10471058. Psychological Science, 14, 220224.
Jensen, P. S., Mrazek, D., Knapp, P. K., Steinberg, L., Pfeffer, C., Kitamura, C., Thanavisuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin,
Schowalter, J., et al. (1997). Evolution and revolution in child psy- S. (2002). Universality and specificity in infant-directed
chiatry: ADHD as a disorder of adaptation. Journal of the Ameri- speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex
can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 16721681. in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Develop-
Johnson, F. A. (1993). Dependency and Japanese socialization: Psy- ment, 24, 372392.
choanalytic and anthropological investigation into amae. New Knight, G. P., Virdin, L. M., & Roosa, M. (1994). Socialization and
York: New York University Press. family correlates of mental health outcomes among Hispanic and
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., & Morton, S. (1991). Anglo American children: Consideration of cross-ethnic scalar
Newborns preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its sub- equivalence. Child Development, 65, 212224.
sequent decline. Cognition, 40, 121. Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization
Kagan, J. (2001). Temperamental contributions to affective and be- and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child
havioral profiles in childhood. In S. G. Hofman & P. M. DiBartolo Development, 64, 325347.
(Eds.), From social anxiety to social phobia: Multiple perspectives Kochanska, G. (1995). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline,
(pp. 216234). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. and the security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging in-
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Temperamental factors in human ternalization. Child Development, 66, 597615.
development. American Psychologist, 46, 856862. Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children
Kagitcibasi, C. (1996). The autonomous-relational self: A new syn- with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel-
thesis. European Psychologist, 1, 180186. opmental Psychology, 33, 228240.
References 423

Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (1995). Mother-child mutually positive Lester, B. M., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The rhythmic
affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibi- structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm in-
tions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. fants. Child Development, 56, 1527.
Child Development, 66, 236254. Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W. L. (1998). Parenting styles and
Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother-child mutually re- achievement: A cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
sponsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to 44, 157172.
early school age. Child Development, 71, 417431. Leveille, E., Cossette, L., Blanchette, I., & Gaudreau, M. (2001). The
Koestner, R., Gingras, I., Abutaa, R., Losier, G. F., DiDio, L. G., & socialization of emotions during nursing: The role of contingent
Gagne, M. (1999). To follow expert advice when making a deci- maternal facial expressions. International Journal of Psychology,
sion: An examination of reactive versus ref lective autonomy. 36, 260273.
Journal of Personality, 67, 851872. Levy, D. (1943). Maternal overprotection. New York: Columbia Uni-
Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (1996). Distinguishing reactive versus versity Press.
ref lective autonomy. Journal of Personality, 64, 465494. Lewis, C. S. (2000). Human development in the United States and
Konner, M. J. (1972). Aspects of the developmental ethology of a Japan: New ways to think about continuity across the lifespan.
foraging people. In N. B. Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child Child Development, 71, 11521154.
behaviour (pp. 285304). Oxford, England: Cambridge Univer- Lin, E. K. (1999). The signal value of childrens voices and its ef-
sity Press. fect on adults physiological and emotional responses. Disserta-
Kuczynski, L. (1984). Socialization goals and mother-child inter- tion Abstracts International: Section B: Sciences & Engineering,
action: Strategies for long-term and short-term compliance. De- 60, 855.
velopmental Psychology, 20, 10611073. Lindahl, K. M., & Malik, N. M. (1999). Marital conf lict, family
Kuczynski, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral conceptual processes, and boys externalizing behavior in Hispanic American
frameworks for understanding dynamics in parent-child relations. and European American families. Journal of Clinical and Child
In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-child rela- Psychology, 28, 1224.
tionships (pp. 324). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lindsey, E. W. (1998). Parents as play partners: Mechanisms
Kuhn, C. M., & Schanberg, S. M. (1998). Responses to maternal sep- linking parent-child play to childrens social competence. Dis-
aration: Mechanisms and mediators [Special issue]. International sertation Abstracts International: Section B: Sciences and En-
Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 261270. gineering, 58, 5700.
Kurzban, R., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2003). Can race be erased? Lindsey, E. W. (2002). Preschool childrens friendships and peer
Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of acceptance: Links to social competence. Child Study Journal,
the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 1538715392. 32, 145156.
LaFreniere, P. J., & Capuano, F. (1997). Preventive intervention as Lindsey, E. W., Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mutuality in parent-
means of clarifying directions of effects in socialization: Anx- child play: Consequences for childrens peer competence. Journal
ious-withdrawn preschoolers case. Development and Psychopath- of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 523538.
ology, 9, 551564. Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, C., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freed-
Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2003). Par- man, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid
ents monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents delinquent receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to
behavior: Evidence of correlated developmental changes and re- stress. Science, 277, 16591662.
ciprocal inf luences. Child Development, 74, 752768. Luecken, L. J. (1998). Childhood attachment and loss experiences af-
Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Mize, J., Brown, E. G., & Lindsey, E. fect adult cardiovascular and cortisol function. Psychosomatic
(1994). Mother child conversations about peers. Family Relations: Medicine, 60, 765772.
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 43, 425432. Lundell, L., Grusec, J. E., McShane, K., & Davidov, M. (2004).
Lamond, A. I., & Earnshaw, W. C. (1998). Structure and function in Mother-adolescent conf lict: Adolescent goals and maternal man-
the nucleus. Science, 280, 547553. agement of those goals. Unpublished manuscript.
Laudenslager, M. L., Boccia, M. L., Berger, C. L., & Gennaro-Rug- Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental
gles, M. M. (1995). Total cortisol, free cortisol, and growth hor- Psychology, 24, 755765.
mones associated with brief social separation experiences in Maccoby, E. E. (1991). Different reproductive strategies in males and
young macaques. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 129211. females. Child Development, 62, 676681.
Lebra, T. S. (2000). New insight and old dilemma: A cross-cultural Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context
comparison of Japan and the United States. Child Development, of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington
71, 11471149. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., Vol. 4, pp. 1102).
New York: Wiley.
Leinbach, M. D., & Fagot, B. I. (1993). Categorical habituation to
male and female faces: Gender schematic processing in infancy. Macdonald, D. W. (1983). The ecology of carnivore social behavior.
Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 317332. Nature, 301, 379384.
Lepper, M. (1983). Social control processes, attributions of motiva- MacDonald, K. (1992). Warmth as a developmental construct: An
tion, and the internalization of social values. In E. T. Higgins, evolutionary analysis. Child Development, 63, 753773.
D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social Madsen, D. (1986 ). Power seekers are different: Further bio-
development: A sociocultural perspective (pp. 294330). New chemical evidence. American Political Science Review, 80,
York: Cambridge University Press. 261269.
424 Socialization Processes

Mann, J. (1992). Nurturance or negligence: Maternal psychology and Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. L. (1988). Early talk about the past: The
behavioral preference among preterm twins. In J. H. Barkow, L. origins of conversational stories of personal experience. Journal
Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 367390). of Child Language, 15, 293315.
New York: Oxford University Press. Miller, P. J., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., & Liang, C. H. (1997). Personal
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and Ameri-
tions for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Re- can families. Child Development, 68, 557568.
view, 98, 224253. Miller, S. A. (1995). Parents attributions for their childrens behav-
Marler, P., Evans, C. S., & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals: ior. Child Development, 66, 15571584.
Motivational, referential, or both. In H. Papousek, J. Jurgens, & Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Compara- antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
tive and developmental approaches (pp. 6686). Cambridge, En- Review, 100, 674701.
gland: Cambridge University Press. Mooncey, S., Giannakoulopoulos, X., Glover, V., Acolet, D., & Modi,
Martini, M. (1996). Whats new? at the dinner table: Family dy- N. (1997). The effect of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact on
namics during mealtimes in two cultural groups in Hawaii. Early plasma cortisol and beta-encorphin concentrations in preterm
Development and Parenting, 5, 2334. newborns. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 553557.
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1996). Testosterone and dominance in men. Moore, S. F., & Myerhoff, B. G. (1977). Secular ritual. Amsterdam:
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353391. Van Gorcum.
McClelland, D. C., Patel, V., Stier, D., & Brown, D. (1987). The rela- Morris, A. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., Silk, J. S., &
tionship of affiliative arousal to dopamine release. Motivation Essex, M. J. (2002). Measuring childrens perceptions of psycho-
and Emotion, 11, 5166. logical control: Developmental and conceptual considerations. In
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implica- B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control
tions for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, af fects children and adolescents (pp. 125159). Washington, DC:
22, 108124. American Psychological Association.
McEwen, B. S. (2001). The end of stress as we know it. Washington, Needle, R. H., Su, S. S., & Doherty, W. J. (1990). Divorce, remar-
DC: Joseph Henry Press. riage, and adolescent substance use: A prospective longitudinal
McGaugh, J. L. (2002). Memory consolidation and the amygdala: A study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 157169.
systems perspective. Trends in Neurosciences, 25, 456461. Neiderhiser, J. M., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R.
McHale, J., Khazan, I., Erera, P., Rotman, T., DeCourcey, W., & Mc- (1999). Relationships between parenting and adolescent adjust-
Connell, M. (2002). Coparenting in diverse family systems. In ment over time: Genetic and environmental contributions. Devel-
M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 3, opmental Psychology, 35, 680692.
pp. 75107). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Childrens friendship rela-
Meaney, M. J., Aitken, D. H., Bodnoff, S. R., Iny, L. J., & Tatarewicz, tions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 306347.
J. E. (1985). Early postnatal handling alters glucocorticoid recep- Newport, D. J., Stowe, Z. N., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2002). Parental de-
tor concentrations in selected brain regions. Behavioral Neuro- pression: Animal models of adverse life events. American Journal
science, 99, 765770. of Psychiatry, 159, 12651283.
Meaney, M. J., Diorio, J., Francis, D., Weaver, S., Yau, J., Chapman, Nix, R. L., Pinderhughes, E. E., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit,
K., et al. (2000). Postnatal handling increases the expression of G. S. (1999). Do parents hostile attribution tendencies function
cAMP-inducible transcription factors in the rat hippocampus: as self-fulfilling prophecies? An empirical examination of ag-
The effects of thyroid hormones and serotonin. Journal of Neuro- gressive transactions. Child Development, 70, 896909.
science, 20, 39263935.
Nucci, L., & Smetana, J. G. (1996). Mothers concept of young childrens
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, K. (1999). Persons and representation: areas of personal freedom. Child Development, 67, 18701886.
Why infant imitation is important for theories of human develop-
OConnor, T. G., Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D., Rutter, M., &
ment. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy:
Plomin, R. (1998). Genotype-environment correlations in late
Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 935).
childhood and early adolescence: Antisocial behavior problems
New York: Cambridge University Press.
and coercive parenting. Developmental Psychology, 34, 970981.
Miller, J. G. (1997). Cultural conceptions of duty: Implications for
Oldershaw, L. (2002). A national survey of parents of young children.
motivation and morality. In D. Munro, J. E. Schumaker, & S. C.
Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Invest in Kids.
Carr (Eds.), Motivation and culture (pp. 178192). New York:
Routledge. Ostwald, P. F. (1963). Soundmaking: The acoustic communication of
Miller, J. G. (2002). Bringing culture to basic psychological theory: emotion. Oxford, England: Charles C. Thomas.
Beyond individualism and collectivismComment on Oyserman Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmaier, M. (2002). Rethinking
et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 97109. individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assump-
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judg- tions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 372.
ment: How are conf licts between justice and interpersonal re- Panksepp, J. (1993). Rough and tumble play: A fundamental brain
sponsibilities resolved. Journal of Personality and Social process. In K. McDonald (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions
Psychology, 62, 541554. and implications (pp. 147184). New York: State University of
Miller, P. J., Fung, H., & Mintz, J. (1996). Self-construction through New York Press.
narrative practices: A Chinese and American comparison of early Panksepp, J. (1996). Affective neuroscience: A paradigm to study the
socialization. Ethos, 24, 237280. animate circuits for human emotions. In R. D. Kavanaugh & B.
References 425

Zimmerberg (Eds.), Emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives Phinney, J. S., Ong, A., & Madden, T. (2000). Cultural values and in-
(pp. 2960). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tergenerational value discrepancies in immigrant and non-immi-
Panksepp, J., Siviy, S. M., & Normansell, L. A. (1985). Brain opioids grant families. Child Development, 71, 528539.
and social emotions. In M. Reite & T. Field (Eds.), The psychobi- Piaget, J. (1948). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL:
ology of attachment and separation (pp. 349). Orlando, FL: Aca- Free Press.
demic Press. Pomerantz, E. M., & Eaton, M. M. (2001). Maternal intrusive support
Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1995). Intuitive parenting. In M. H. in the academic context: Transactional socialization processes.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecol- Developmental Psychology, 37, 174186.
ogy of parenting (pp. 117136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Popik, P., & van Ree, J. M. (1991). Oxytocin, but not vasopression, fa-
Parke, R. D. (1978). Parent-infant interaction: Progress, paradigms, cilitates social recognition following injection into the medial pre-
and problems. In G. P. Sackett (Ed.), Observing behavior (Vol. 1, optic of the rat brain. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1,
pp. 6994). Baltimore: University Park Press. 555560.
Parke, R. D., & Bhavnagri, N. P. (1989). Parents as managers of chil- Power, T. G., Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1982). Adults
drens peer relationships. In D. Belle (Ed.), Childrens social net- responses to infants varying in facial expressions and perceived
works and social supports (pp. 241259). New York: Wiley. attractiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 5, 3344.
Parke, R. D., Simpkins, S. D., McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., Killian, C., Prothrow-Stith, D., & Quaday, S. (1996). Communities, schools, and
Dennis, J., et al. (2002). Relative contributions of families and violence. In A. M. Hoffman (Ed.), Schools, violence, and society
peers to childrens social development. In P. K. Smith & C. H. (pp. 153161). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Craig (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development Raleigh, M. J., McGuire, M. T., Bramner, G. L., Olkkacjm, D. B., &
(pp. 156177). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Yuwiler, A. (1991). Serotonergic mechanisms promote domi-
Parker, G. A., & Rubenstein, D. I. (1981). Role assessment, reserve nance acquisition in adult male vervet monkeys. Brain Research,
strategy, and acquisition of information in asymmetric animal 559, 181190.
conf licts. Animal Behaviour, 29, 221240. Ramsay, D., & Lewis, M. (2003). Reactivity and regulation in cortisol
Parpal, M., & Maccoby, E. E. (1985). Maternal responsiveness and and behavioral responses to stress. Child Development, 74, 456464.
subsequent child compliance. Child Development, 56, 13261334. Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families:
Family social environments and the mental and physical health of
Parrott, W. G., & Gleitman, H. (1989). Infants expectations in
offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330336.
play: The joy of peek-a-boo [Special issue]. Cognition and Emo-
tion, 3, 291311. Rheingold, H. (1969). The social and socializing infant. In D. A.
Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research
Patterson, G. R. (1980). Mothers: The unacknowledged victims.
(pp. 779790). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 45.
Rheingold, H. L. (1982). Little childrens participation in the
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR:
work of adults, a nascent prosocial behavior. Child Develop-
Castalia Press.
ment, 53, 114125.
Patterson, G. R. (1997). Performance models for parenting: A social Rholes, W. S., Newman, L. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1990). Understanding
interactional perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), self and other: Developments and motivational aspects of per-
Parenting and childrens internalization of values: A handbook of ceiving persons in terms of invariant dispositions. In E. T. Hig-
contemporary theory (pp. 193226). New York: Wiley. gins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and
Patterson, G. R., & Fisher, P. A. (2002). Recent developments in our cognition: Vol. 2. Foundations of social behavior (pp. 369407).
understanding of parenting, bidirectional effects, causal models, New York: Guilford Press.
and the search for parsimony. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook Rogoff, B., Pardies, R., Arauz, R. M., Correa-Chavez, M., & An-
of parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 5988). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. gelillo, C. (2003). Firsthand learning through intent participation.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1998). Antisocial boys. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 175203.
In J. M. Jenkins & K. Oatley (Eds.), Human emotions: A reader Rohner, R. P., & Pettengill, S. M. (1985). Perceived parental accep-
(pp. 330336). Malden, MA: Blackwell. tance-rejection and parental control among Korean adolescents.
Patterson, G. R., & Sanson, A. (1999). The association of behavioural Child Development, 56, 524528.
adjustment to temperament, parenting and family characteristics Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000).
among 5-year-old children. Social Development, 8, 293309. The development of close relationships in Japan and the United
Pellis, S. M. (2002). Keep in touch: Play fighting and social knowl- States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child
edge. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cog- Development, 71, 11211142.
nitive animal (pp. 421427). London: MIT Press. Rothbaum, F., & Trommsdorff, G. (in press). Do roots and wings com-
Perry, D. B., Hodges, E. V. E., & Egan, S. K. (2001). Determinants of plement or oppose one another: The socialization of relatedness
chronic victimization by peers: A review and new model of family and autonomy in cultural context. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings
inf luence. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment in (Eds.), Handbook of socialization. New York: Guilford Press.
school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 73104). Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G.
New York: Guilford Press. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2002). Mindreading and moral aware- Japan. American Psychologist, 55, 10931104.
ness in popular and rejected preschoolers. British Journal of De- Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W.
velopmental Psychology, 20, 205224. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
426 Socialization Processes

child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel- Seligman, M. E. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning.
opment (5th ed., pp. 9331016). New York: Wiley. Psychological Review, 77, 406418.
Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., & Burge, D. (1995). Cognitive repre- Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice.
sentations of self, family, and peers in school-age children: Links Cognition, 49, 1136.
with social competence and sociometric status. Child Develop- Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. (1992). Thinking through uncertainty:
ment, 66, 13851402. Nonconsequential reasoning and choice. Cognitive Psychology,
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (2001). Correlates of authoritarian parent- 24, 449474.
ing in individualist and collectivist cultures and implications for Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C.
understanding the transmission of values. Journal of Cross-Cul- (1954). Experimental study of positive and negative intergroup at-
tural Psychology, 32, 202212. titudes between experimentally prejudiced groups: Robbers Cave
Rudy, D., & Grusec, J. E. (in press). The correlates and outcomes of experiment. Norman: University of Oklahoma.
authoritarian parenting in individualist and collectivist groups. Siegal, M., & Barclay, M. S. (1985). Childrens evaluations of fa-
Journal of Family Psychology. thers socialization behavior. Developmental Psychology, 21,
Rutter, M., & OConnor, T. G. (2004). Are there biological program- 10901096.
ming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., & Goodnow, J. J. (Eds.).
of Romanian adoptees. Developmental Psychology, 40, 8194. (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well- Silk, J. B. (1992). The patterning of intervention among male bonnet
being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878. macaques: Reciprocity, revenge, and loyalty. Current Anthropol-
Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models of early social relations. ogy, 33, 318324.
Human Development, 18, 6579. Simpson, J. A. (1999). Attachment theory in modern evolutionary
Sanchez-Martin, J. R., Fano, E., Ahedo, L., Cardas, J., Brain, P. F., & perspective. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
Azpiroz, A. (2000). Relating testosterone levels and free play so- attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications
cial behavior in male and female preschool children. Psychoneu- (pp. 117141). New York: Guilford Press.
roendocrinology, 25, 773783. Skarin, K. S. (1977). Cognitive and contextual determinants of
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- stranger fear in 6- and 11-month-old infants. Child Development,
ronment: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Devel- 48, 537544.
opment, 54, 424435. Smetana, J. (1988). Adolescents and parents conceptions of
Schaal, B. (1988). Olfaction in infants and children: Developmental parental authority. Child Development, 59, 321335.
and functional perspectives. Chemical Senses, 13, 145190. Smetana, J. (1996). Adolescent-parent conf lict: Implications for
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Childrens reports of parental behavior: An adaptive and maladaptive development. In D. Cicchetti & S. L.
inventory. Child Development, 36, 413424. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopath-
Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality ology: Vol. 7. AdolescenceOpportunities and challenges
and cultural variation of differential emotion response pattern- (pp. 146). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310328. Smetana, J. (1997). Parenting and the development of social knowl-
Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1991). Adolescence: An anthropological in- edge reconceptualized: A social domain analysis. In J .E. Grusec
quiry. New York: Free Press. & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and the internalization of val-
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., & Steinberg, E. M. (1997). ues: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 162192). New
Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Devel- York: Wiley.
opmental Psychology, 30, 127140. Smetana, J. G., & Asquith, P. (1994). Adolescents and parents con-
Schore, A. N. (1998). Early shame experiences and infant brain devel- ceptions of parental authority and personal autonomy. Child De-
opment. In P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal velopment, 65, 11471162.
behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 5777). New York: Smetana, J. G., & Daddis, C. (2002). Domain-specific antecedents of
Oxford University Press. parental psychological control and monitoring: The role of parent-
Schulkin, J., Gold, P. W., & McEwen, B. S. (1998). Induction of cor- ing beliefs and practices. Child Development, 73, 563580.
ticotropin-releasing hormone gene expression by glucocorticoids: Smetana, J. G., Kochanska, G., & Chuang, S. (2000). Mothers con-
Implications for understanding the states of fear and anxiety and ceptions of everyday rules for young toddlers: A longitudinal in-
allostatic load. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 219243. vestigation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 391416.
Schwartz, A., & Bugental, D. B. (2004). Infant habituation to re- Smith, C., & Krohn, M. D. (1995). Delinquency and family life
peated stress as an interactive function of child temperament and among male adolescents: The role of ethnicity. Journal of Youth
maternal depression. Unpublished paper. and Adolescence, 24, 6993.
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2000). Smith, P. K., Shu, S., & Madsen, K. (2001). Characteristics of vic-
Friendship as a moderating factor in the pathway between early tims of school bullying: Developmental changes in coping strate-
harsh home environment and later victimization in the peer group. gies and skills. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer
Developmental Psychology, 36, 646662. harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child (pp. 332351). New York: Guilford Press.
rearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson. Snyder, J. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). Individual differences in so-
Sears, R. R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1964). Identification and child cial aggression: A test of a reinforcement model of socialization
rearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. in the natural environment. Behavior Therapy, 26, 371391.
References 427

Spangler, B., & Grossman, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization Suomi, S. J. (1997). Long-term effects of different early rearing
in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, experiences on social, emotional and physiological develop-
64, 14391450. ment in nonhuman primates. In M. S. Keshewen & R. M. Murra
Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Ilg, U., & Maier, U. (1994). Maternal (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental models of adult psychopathology
sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in (pp. 104116). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
infancy. Development and Psychobiology, 27, 425437. Press.
Sroufe, L. A. (1988). The role of infant-caregiver attachment in de- Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1997). Basic processes and human de-
velopment. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implica- velopment. In J. W. Berry, P. R. Dassen, & T. S. Saraswathi
tions of attachment: Child psychology (pp. 1838). Hillsdale, (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2,
NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 139). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpreta- Susman, E. J., Inoff-Germain, G., Nottelman, E. D., Loriaux, D. L.,
tion. Child Development, 71, 10721085. Cutler, G. B., & Chrousos, G. P. (1987). Hormones, emotional
Stauder, J. E. A., Molenaar, P. C. M., & Van der Molen, M. W. (1999). dispositions, and aggressive attributes in young adolescents. Child
Brain activity and cognitive transition during childhood: A longi- Development, 58, 11141134.
tudinal event-related brain potential study. Child Neuropsychol- Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., Hill-Barlow, D., Marschall, D. E., &
ogy, 5, 4159. Gramzow, R. (1997). Relation of shame and guilt to constructive
Stayton, D. J., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. (1971). The origins of versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespan. Journal
socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42, 10571069. of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 797809.
Steinberg, L. (1990). Autonomy, conf lict, and harmony in the family Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Ox-
relationship. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the ford, England: Brunner/ Mazel.
threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 255276). Cambridge, Thompson, R. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In J.
MA: Harvard University Press. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S. M., & Brown, B. B. (1992). Ethnic dif- research, and clinical applications (pp. 265286). New York:
ferences in adolescent achievement: An ecological perspective. Guilford Press.
American Psychologist, 47, 723729. Thornton, M. C., Chatters, L. M., Taylor, R. J., & Allen, W. R.
Steinberg, L., Elmen, J., & Mounts, N. (1989). Authoritative parent- (1990). Sociodemographic and environmental correlates of racial
ing, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adoles- socialization by Black parents. Child Development, 61, 401409.
cents. Child Development, 60, 14241436. Trevarthen, C., Kokkinski, T., & Flamenghi, G. A., Jr. (1999). In J.
Steinberg, L., Mounts, N. S., Lamborn, S. D., & Dornbusch, S. M. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy: Cambridge
(1991). Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 127185). New
across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research in Adoles- York: Cambridge University Press.
cence, 1, 936. Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-offspring conf lict. American Zoologist,
Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. B. (1986). The vicissitudes of auton- 14, 249264.
omy in early adolescence. Child Development, 57, 841851. Trommsdorff, G., & Iwawaki, S. (1989). Students perceptions of so-
Stewart, S. M., Rao, N., Bond, M. H., McBride-Chang, C., Fielding, cialization and gender role in Japan and Germany. International
R., & Kennard, B. D. (1998). Chinese dimensions of parenting: Journal of Behavioural Development, 12, 485493.
Broadening Western predictors and outcomes. International Jour- Trommsdorff, G., & Kornadt, H.-J. (2003). Parent-child relations in
nal of Psychology, 33, 345358. cross-cultural perspective. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of
Stoolmiller, M. (2001). Synergistic interaction of child manageability dynamics in parent-child relations (pp. 271306). Thousand
problems and parent-discipline tactics in predicting future external- Oaks, CA: Sage.
izing behavior for boys. Developmental Psychology, 37, 814825. Tronick, E. Z., Scanlon, R. N., & Scanlon, J. W. (1990). Protective
Storey, A. E., Walsh, C. J., Quinton, R. L., & Wynne-Edwards, K. E. apathy, a hypothesis about behavioral organization and its rela-
(2000). Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new tion to clinical and physiological status of the preterm infant dur-
and expectant fathers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 7995. ing the newborn period. Clinics in Perinatology, 17, 125154.
Strassburg, L., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Spank- Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. (1998). In N. Eisen-
ing in the home and childrens subsequent aggression toward kinder- berg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 863932).
garten peers. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 445461. New York: Wiley.
Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them: Corporal punish- Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, con-
ment and American families. New York: Lexington Book. text, and conf lict. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1988). How violent are American fami- Udry, J. R., & Talbert, L. M. (1988). Sex hormone effects on per-
lies?: Estimates from the National Family Violence Resurvey and sonality at puberty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
other studies. In G. T. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, J. T. Kirkpatrick, & 54, 291295.
M. A. Straus (Eds.). Family abuse and its consequences: New direc- Valsiner, J., & Cairns, R. (1992). Theoretical perspectives on conf lict
tion in research (pp. 1436). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. and development. In C. U. Shantz & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Con-
Sugarman, D. B. (1983). The development of childrens physical f lict in child and adolescent development (pp. 1535). New York:
and social causal explanations. Dissertation Abstracts Interna- Cambridge University Press.
tional, 44, 363. Vanderschuren, L. J., Niesink, R. J., & Van Ree, J. M. (1997). The
Sung, K. (1995). Measures and dimensions of filial piety in Korea. neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neuroscience and
Gerontologist, 35, 240247. Biobehavioral Reviews, 21, 309326.
428 Socialization Processes

van Wimersma, Greidanus, T. B., & Maigret, C. (1996). The role of The Jossey-Bass education series (pp. 5166). San Francisco:
limbic vasopressin and oxytocin in social recognition. Brain Re- Jossey-Bass.
search, 713, 153159. Weinberg, K. M., & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions
Vaughn, B. E., Colvin, T. N., Azria, M. R., Caya, L., & Krzysik, L. to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face.
(2001). Dyadic analyses of friendship in a sample of preschool- Child Development, 67, 905914.
age children attending Head Start: Correspondence between mea- Weisner, T. S., & Garnier, H. (1992). Nonconventional family life-
sures and implications for social competence. Child Development, styles and school achievement: A 12-year longitudinal study.
72, 862878. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 605632.
Vinden, P. (1999). Childrens understanding of mind and emotion: A Whiting, J. W. M. (1976). The cross-cultural method. In E. H. Chasdi
multi-culture study. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 1948. (Ed.), Culture and human development: The selected papers of
Wainryb, C., & Turiel, E. (1994). Dominance, subordination, and John Whiting (pp. 7688). New York: Cambridge University Press.
concepts of personal entitlements in cultural contexts. Child De- Wiesenfeld, A. R., Malatesta, C. Z., & DeLoach, L. L. (1981). Dif-
velopment, 65, 17011722. ferential parental response to familiar and unfamiliar infant dis-
Waizenhoffer, R. N., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-Newson, J. (2004). tress signals. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 281295.
Mothers and fathers knowledge of adolescents daily activities: Wilkinson, G. (1988). Reciprocal altruism in bats and other mam-
Its sources and its links with adolescent adjustment. Journal of mals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 85100.
Family Psychology, 18, 348360. Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., Yaeger, A., & Shinar, O. (2001). Family risk
Wang, Q., & Leichtman, M. D. (2000). Same beginnings, different factors and adolescent substance use: Moderation effects for tem-
stories: A comparison of American and Chinese childrens narra- perament dimensions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 283297.
tives. Child Development, 71, 13291346. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1994). The psychology of parenting in evolu-
Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying con- tionary perspective and the case of human filicide. In S. Parmi-
cepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental giani & F. S. von Saal (Eds.), Infanticide and parental care
study of imitation in children. Developmental Science, 5, 113. (pp. 73134). London: Academic Press.
Ward, C. D., & Cooper, R. P. (1999). A lack of evidence in 4-month- World Health Organization. (2002). World Report on Violence and
old human infants for paternal voice preference. Developmental Health. Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Psychobiology, 35, 4059. Yau, J., & Smetana, J. G. (1996). Adolescent-parent conf lict among Chi-
Ward, M. J., & Carlson, E. A. (1995). Associations among adult at- nese adolescents in Hong Kong. Child Development, 67, 12621275.
tachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant-mother Yogman, M. W. (1990). Male parental behavior in humans and nonhu-
attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Child Develop- man primates. In N. A. Krasnegor & R. S. Bridges (Eds.), Mam-
ment, 66, 6979. malian parenting (pp. 461491). London: Oxford University Press.
Warren, S. L., Gunnar, M. R., Kagan, J., Anders, T. F., Simmens, Young, L. J. (2002). The neurobiology of social recognition: Ap-
S. J., Rones, M., et al. (2003). Maternal panic disorder: Infant proach, and avoidance. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 1826.
temperament, neurophysiology, and parenting behavior. Journal Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., & Strouse, D. (1994). Were popular,
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, but were not snobs: Adolescents describe their crowds. In R.
42, 814825. Montemayor & G. R. Adams (Eds.), Personal relationships during
Warton, J. J., & Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Money and childrens house- adolescence: Vol. 6. Advances in adolescent developmentAn an-
hold jobs: Parents views of their interconnections. International nual book series (pp. 101122). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 235350. Zeanah, C. H., Benoit, D., Hirschberg, L., Barton, M. L., & Regan, C.
Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. T. (1987). Reactions to response-contin- (1994). Mothers representations of their infants are concordant
gent stimulation in early infancy. In J. Oates & S. Sheldon with infant attachment classification. Developmental Issues in
(Eds.), Cognitive development in infancy (pp. 7785). Hillsdale, Psychiatry and Psychology, 1, 114.
NJ: Erlbaum. Zebrowitz, L. (1997). Reading faces. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Watson-Gegeo, K. A. (1992). Thick explanation in the ethnographic Zupanc, G. K. H., & Lamprecht, J. (2000). Towards a cellular under-
study of child socialization: A longitudinal study of the problem standing of motivation: Structural reorganization and biochemi-
of schooling for Kwaraae (Solomon Islands) children. In W. A. cal switching as key mechanisms of behavioral plasticity.
Corsaro & P. J. Miller (Eds.), New directions for child develop- Ethology, 106, 467477.
ment: Vol. 58. Interpretive approaches to childrens socialization
CHAPTER 8

Socialization in the Family:


Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives
ROSS D. PARKE and RAYMOND BURIEL

THEORETICAL APPROACHES TO SOCIALIZATION THE IMPACT OF SOCIAL CHANGE ON FAMILY


IN THE FAMILY: HISTORICAL AND SOCIALIZATION 461
CONTEMPORARY PERSPECTIVES 430 Womens and Mens Employment Patterns and Family
Historical Perspectives on Family Socialization Socialization 461
Theory 430 Job Loss and Unemployment 464
Contemporary Perspectives on Family Socialization Single versus Multiple Transitions 464
Theory 431 Children and Families of Color in the United States:
FAMILY SYSTEMS APPROACH TO Issues of Race, Ethnicity, and Culture 465
SOCIALIZATION 434 Socialization of Children of Color 469
The Parent-Child Subsystem: A Tripartite Approach 434 REMAINING ISSUES AND FUTURE TRENDS 487
The Sibling System as a Contributor to Childrens Unit of Analysis 487
Socialization 452 Parenting and Family Variation 487
The Family Unit as a Contributor to Childrens Types of Developmental Change 488
Socialization 455 Monitoring Secular Trends 488
Putting the Pieces Together: Toward a Multiple Sources Methodological and Design Issues 488
Model of Socialization in the Family 456
Contextual Issues 490
DETERMINANTS OF FAMILY SOCIALIZATION Locating Families in a Network of Socialization
STRATEGIES 457 Inf luences 490
Child Characteristics 458 A Final Word 491
Personal Resources 458
REFERENCES 491
Socioeconomic Status as a Determinant of Family
Socialization Strategies 460

Socialization is a process in which an individuals stan- dependent on families for nurturance and support from
dards, skills, motives, attitudes, and behaviors change to an early age, which accounts, in part, for their promi-
conform to those regarded as desirable and appropriate nence as a socialization agent.
for his or her present and future role in any particular In this chapter, we have several goals. Our primary
society. Many agents and agencies play a role in the so- goal is to expand our framework for conceptualizing the
cialization process, including family, peers, schools, and familys role in socialization. This takes several forms,
the media. Moreover, it is recognized that these various including treating the family as a social system in which
agents function together rather than independently. parent-child, marital, and sibling subsystems, among
Families have been recognized as an early pervasive and others, are recognized. The diversity of family forms
highly influential context for socialization. Children are has increased in the past several decades and a second

Preparation of this chapter was facilitated by National Sci- NICHD grant HD 32391 to Parke. Finally, thanks to Faye
ence Foundation grants BNS 8919391 & SBR 9308941 and Harmer for her preparation of the manuscript.

429
430 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

goal is to explore the implications of various family con- of later social and personality was a major force in so-
figurations for the socialization process. Third, cultural cialization research for nearly half a century. Although
and ethnic variations in family traditions, beliefs, and it was difficult to test as originally formulated, the the-
practices are increasingly being recognized, and a fur- ory provided the outlines for the major socialization
ther aim of this chapter is to explore how ethnic diver- products, such as aggression, dependency, moral devel-
sity informs our understanding of family socialization. opment, and sex typing, as well as the major sets of
Fourth, our goal is to locate family socialization in an formative experiences in the family, especially the
ecological context to appreciate how family environ- mother-child relationship.
ments shape and constrain their socialization practices. In the 1930s, the fusion of Hullian learning theory
We demonstrate the value of a life-course perspective with psychoanalysis provided the opportunity to empiri-
on socialization that recognizes the importance of both cally evaluate the propositions of psychoanalytic theory
developmental changes in adult lives and the historical by translating them into drive-reduction language. This
circumstances under which socialization unfolds. Fi- led to several renowned studies by Whiting and Child
nally, we recognize that families are increasingly di- (1953) and Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957). As Mac-
verse in their organization, form, and lifestyle. Some coby (1992) noted, These large scale efforts to merge
issues are beyond the scope of the chapter including the psychoanalytic and behavior theory and then to predict
recent work on gay and lesbian families and research on childrens personality attributes from parental social-
adopted children (see Brodzinsky & Pinderhughes, ization methods, were largely unsuccessful (p. 1009).
2002; C. Patterson, 2002, for reviews). The 1960s and 1970s marked the advent of further
developments in socialization theory. First, social learn-
ing theory (Bandura, 1977; Bandura & Walters, 1963)
THEORETICAL APPROACHES TO emerged as an alternative to the Hullian-Freudian
SOCIALIZATION IN THE FAMILY: legacy (see Grusec, 1992). The distinguishing feature
HISTORICAL AND CONTEMPORARY was the emphasis on imitation or observational learn-
PERSPECTIVES ing, which emphasized the central role of cognition in
social learning and reduced dependence on external
In this section, we examine historical and contemporary reinforcement for the acquisition of new behaviors. In
theoretical perspectives on the role of socialization in terms of method, social learning theory relied on the ex-
the family. perimental analog approach in contrast to the retrospec-
tive interview of Sears and colleagues. Moreover, in a
break with the Freudian tradition, social learning theo-
Historical Perspectives on Family
rists placed greater emphasis on the plasticity and
Socialization Theory
modifiability of the organism at different points in de-
To appreciate these goals, an overview of historical velopment and downplayed the psychoanalytically based
changes in socialization theory is necessary. The history concept of early experience as a constraining condition
of this field over the past century can be traced to two on later development. At the same time, the role of so-
theoretical perspectives: behaviorism and psychoanaly- cial contingencies in shaping parent-child interaction
sis (Maccoby, 1992). In the 1920s, Watson offered a was receiving increased attention (Gewirtz, 1969).
learning theory approach to socialization, which was Studies of parent-infant interaction (e.g., Stern, 1977)
based on conditioning as an explanatory mechanism. and interactions of parents and children, especially
This legacy continued under Skinner and followers in aggressive and/or deviant children, were flourishing
the behavior modification movement who applied these (G. Patterson, 1981).
principles to childrens behavior (Bijou & Baer, 1961). As approaches to socialization, these theories were
The other legacy was Freudian psychoanalysis. Per- limited in several ways. First, they were largely nonde-
haps no other view has had so wide an influenceeven velopmental, and it was generally assumed that the prin-
if often unrecognized and unacknowledgedas ciples applied equally to children at all ages. This is
Freudian theory. Freuds stage theory with its emphasis surprising in view of the increased interest in the role of
on the importance of early experience as a determinant cognition in social learning theory (Bandura, 1977).
Theoretical Approaches to Socialization in the Family: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives 431

Second, the role of affect was given a comparatively mother, and fatherremains a useful level of analysis,
minor role. Third, genetic factors and biological con- recognition of relationships among family members as
straints were given relatively short shrift. Fourth, the units of analysis is necessary. The marital relationship,
agents of socialization were still narrowly defined as the mother-child relationship, and the father-child rela-
primarily mothers. In the late 1960s, John Bowlbys fu- tionship require separate analysis (Parke et al., 2001).
sion of psychoanalysis and ethology into his theory of at- Finally, the family as a unit that is independent of the in-
tachment and loss foreshadowed changes that dividual or dyads in the family requires recognition
materialized in the 1970s and beyond. Concepts of bio- (Cook, 2001; Sigel & Parke, 1987).
logical preparedness for social interaction combined A fourth shift is from unidirectional to transactional
with emphasis on the importance of early close relation- models of relationships among family members. There
ships served to anticipate modern concepts of biological have been various phases in the conceptual thinking in
constraints (Schaffer, 1971) and revive early notions of this domain. First, scholars traditionally were guided by
the importance of early experience and critical periods. unilateral models of parent-child relations (Kuczynski,
Finally, Bowlbys focus on the dyad as the unit of analy- 2003) in which the direction of causality was unidirec-
sis led to the recognition of the importance of relation- tional, from parent to child. The childs role was rela-
ships for development. tively passive, the focus was on individuals rather than
In the late 1970s to the present, a variety of changes relationships, and power relations were relatively static.
have taken place in our theoretical approaches to social- In addition, the mother rather than either the father or
ization that have corrected some of the shortcomings of other family members was the major focus of both theo-
these earlier analysis and extended our frameworks into retical and empirical work.
new domains as well. In the 1960s with Bells (1968) classic reformulation,
the field began to recognize the bidirectional nature of
parent-child relationships. This shift occurred in a cli-
Contemporary Perspectives on Family
mate of reevaluation of infant competence; instead of
Socialization Theory
the passive creatures of earlier times, infants and chil-
Several themes are evident in current theoretical ap- dren were viewed as more competent and active in their
proaches to socialization. First, systems theory own development. A bilateral model has emerged as the
(Sameroff, 1994; Thelen & Smith, 1994) has transformed dominant paradigm in the parent-child relationship do-
the study of socialization from a parent-child focus to an main (Kuczynski, 2003) in which the direction of
emphasis on the family as a social system (Parke, 2004a). causality is bidirectional, equal agency on the part of
To understand fully the nature of family relationships, it parent and child is recognized, and power relations are
is necessary to recognize the interdependence among the characterized as interdependent asymmetry.
roles and functions of all family members. For example, At the same time, views of the pathways through
as mens roles in families shift, changes in womens roles which parents can influence their children expanded.
in families must also be monitored. Historically, socialization models directed attention to-
Second, family membersmothers, fathers, and ward the nature of the parent-child relationship and the
childreninfluence each other both directly and indi- types of child-rearing practices that parents employ. Re-
rectly (Minuchin, 2002; Parke, Power, & Gottman, search on infant-parent attachment and on parenting
1979). Examples of fathers indirect impact include var- styles exemplifies this tradition. More recently, views of
ious ways in which fathers modify and mediate mother- parenting have expanded to include parents as active
child relationships. In turn, women affect their children managers of the childs social environment outside the
indirectly through their husbands by modifying both the family. In this role, a parent actively regulates the
quantity and the quality of father-child interaction. childs access to physical and social resources outside
Children may indirectly influence the husband-wife re- the family (Parke, Killian, et al., 2003) and serves as
lationship by altering the behavior of either parent that regulator of opportunities for social contact with ex-
changes the interaction between spouses. trafamilial social partners. Although peer influence in-
Third, different units of analysis are necessary to un- creases as children develop, parents continue to play an
derstand families. Although the individualchild, important regulatory role as gatekeepers and monitors
432 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

of childrens social contacts, even in adolescence contexts (Rogoff, 2003). Second, social class differ-
(Mounts, 2000). However, recent thinking has recog- ences in socialization challenged the generality of
nized that in the managerial domain, as in other parts of findings even in one cultural or national context (Gau-
the parent-child relationship, both children and parents vain, 2001; Hoff, Laursen, & Tardif, 2002). Cur-
play active roles in decisions about childrens social op- rently, there is an increased awareness of the
portunities (Kerr & Stattin, 2000). importance of both recognizing and studying varia-
Fifth, under the influence of Bronfenbrenners eco- tions in families and family socialization strategies in
logical theory (1989), recognition is being given to the both other cultures (Rogoff, 2003) and across ethnic
embeddedness of families in other social systems as well groups in our own culture (Parke, 2004b). It is impor-
as the cultures in which they exist (Parke & Kellam, tant not only to examine the diversity of familial orga-
1994). These include a range of extrafamilial influences, nization, goals, and strategies across ethnic groups
such as extended families, and informal community ties but it is equally critical to explore variations within
such as friends and neighbors, work sites, and social, ed- different ethnic groups (Garcia Coll & Magnuson,
ucational, and medical institutions (Repetti, 1994). 1999; Parke, 2004b). Although there are many simi-
Sixth, the importance of considering family relation- larities across and within groups, appreciation of the
ships from a developmental perspective is now recog- variations is of central concern.
nized. Although developmental changes in infant and Another assumption that guides current theorizing
child capacities continue to represent the most com- involves the recognition of the impact of secular shifts
monly investigated aspect of development, other as- on families. In recent years, there have been a variety of
pects of development are viewed as important too. social changes in American society that have had a pro-
Under the influence of life-course and life-span per- found impact on families including the decline in fertil-
spectives (Elder, 1998; Parke, 1988), examination of ity and family size, changes in the timing of the onset of
developmental changes in adults is gaining recognition parenthood, increased participation of women in the
because parents continue to change and develop during workforce, rise in the rates of divorce, and the subse-
adult years. For example, age at the onset of parenthood quent increase in the number of single-parent families as
has implications for how females and males manage well as remarried step families (Elder, 1998; Hethering-
their parental roles. This involves an exploration of the ton & Kelly, 2001). The ways in which these societal
tasks faced by adults such as self-identity, education, changes impact relationships between parents and chil-
and career, and an examination of the relation between dren merit examination.
these tasks and parenting. A related theme involves the recognition of the im-
Developmental analysis need not be restricted to the portance of the historical time period in which the
individual leveleither child or parent. Relationships family interaction is embedded. Historical time peri-
(e.g., the marital, the mother-child, or the father-child ods provide the social conditions for individual and
relationship) may follow separate and partially indepen- family transitions: Examples include the 1960s (the
dent developmental courses over childhood (Parke, Vietnam War era), the 1930s (the Great Depression),
1988). In turn, the mutual impact of different sets of re- or the 1980s (Farm Belt Depression; Conger & Elder,
lationships on each other will vary as a function of the 1994; Elder & Conger, 2000). Across these periods,
nature of the developmental trajectory. Families change family interaction may be quite different due to the
their structure (e.g., through the addition of a new child unique conditions of the era. The distinctions among
or the loss of a member through death or divorce), different developmental trajectories, as well as social
norms, rules, and strategies over time. Tracking the fam- change and historical period effects, are important be-
ily unit itself over development is an important and ne- cause these different forms of change do not always
glected task (Cook, 2001). harmonize (Modell & Elder, 2002). For example, a
A major shift over the past 2 decades is the chal- family event such as the birth of a child may have pro-
lenge to the universality of our socialization theories. found effects on a man who has just begun a career in
This challenge takes several forms. First, as cross- contrast to the effects on one who has advanced to a
cultural work in development accumulated, it became stable occupational position. Moreover, individual and
evident that generalizations from a single culture family developmental trajectories are embedded in
(e.g., American) may not be valid in other cultural both the social conditions and the values of the histor-
Theoretical Approaches to Socialization in the Family: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives 433

ical time in which they exist. The role of parents, as is of cognition comes in many guises, including the
the case with any social role, is responsive to such childs own cognitive capacities as a determinant of
fluctuations. socialization strategies and parents cognitions, be-
To understand the nature of parent-child relation- liefs, values, and goals, concerning their parental role
ships in families, a multilevel and dynamic approach as constraints on their socialization practices (Dix &
is required. Multiple levels of analysis are necessary Branca, 2003). Equally important is the recognition
to capture the individual, dyadic, and family unit of the importance of the ways in which parents per-
aspects of operation in the family itself, and to ref lect ceive, organize, and understand their childrens be-
the embeddedness of families in a variety of extrafa- haviors and beliefs for appreciating how parent-child
milial social systems. The dynamic quality ref lects relationships are regulated and change (Goodnow,
the multiple developmental trajectories that warrant 2002). Underlying much of current research is the
consideration in understanding the nature of families. recognition that these processes are interdependent,
The central processes that are involved in account- mutually inf luencing each other. Cognition and affect
ing for both the choice of and regulation of socializa- for example, generally operate together in determin-
tion strategies and the effects of socialization on the ing parenting practices (e.g., Dix, 1991; Dix &
developing child have undergone a major revision (see Branca, 2003).
Bugental & Grusec, Chapter 7, this Handbook, this vol- Just as processes are viewed as interdependent, there
ume). The renewed interest in the biological bases of is an increasing appreciation of the need for perspectives
behavior has clearly altered our views of socialization. from a variety of disciplines to understand the family so-
This interest takes several forms including the role of cialization process. No longer restricted to developmen-
genetics across development, which has produced not tal psychology, the field of family socialization is
only a more sophisticated understanding of the poten- increasingly multidisciplinary. History, anthropology,
tial role genetics can play in the onset of certain behav- sociology, demography, pediatrics, psychiatry, and eco-
iors but also in the unfolding of behavior across nomics are all fields that are playing a role in the study of
development. Moreover, the reformulation of genetic socialization (Parke, 2004b).
questions has led to studies of the effects of nonshared Finally, the methodological rigor of the field has in-
family environment on childrens development (OCon- creased in recent years. Instead of sole reliance on cross-
nor, 2003). Finally, this work has suggested that indi- sectional and/or correlational studies, greater weight is
vidual differences between childrensome of which being given to carefully designed longitudinal studies
are genetically basedplay a central role in eliciting (Gottfried, Gottfried, & Bathurst, 2002) and experimen-
and shaping parents socialization strategies (recogni- tal studies (Cowan & Cowan, 2002) because these ap-
tion that the child is an active contributor to his or her proaches allow more confidence in interpreting direction
own socialization). A second focus is found in studies of effects. Second, more recent studies typically avoid
of hormones and behavior, especially during infancy the problems of shared method or reporter variance by
and adolescence (Corter & Fleming, 2002). Third, the reliance on either multiple reporters and/or multiple
increased use of psychophysiological assessments with methods. Third, in view of the challenges from other
families represents a further instance of how biological fields, such as behavior genetics (Harris, 1998; Plomin,
processes are changing socialization studies (Eisen- 1994), there is increasing recognition of rival explana-
berg, 2000). Fourth, the resurgence of interest in evolu- tions of apparent socialization effects. Both shared ge-
tionary approaches to socialization is producing new netic effects and contextual influences are commonly
and provocative hypotheses and research directions viewed as alternative perspectives that merit considera-
(Geary & Bjorklund, 2000). tion in explaining socialization outcomes. Finally, there
Affect is increasingly viewed as a central social- is a clear trend to move beyond description by the emer-
ization process. The study of affect has assumed a va- gence of theories that specify the mediating variables
riety of forms, including the development of emotion that can account for the relation between parenting and
regulation (Denham, 1998), emotional production, child outcomes (Parke et al., 2004). Closely related is the
and understanding of the role of emotion in the enact- recognition of moderating influences, such as social con-
ment of the parenting role (Dix, 1991). Cognition is text, ethnicity, or family structure on the operation of
also viewed as central to socialization. Again the role socialization processes (Mounts, 2002). Throughout our
434 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

review, we focus on work that meets these new standards itly set out to educate their children concerning appro-
of scientific rigor whenever possible. priate norms, rules, and mores of the culture. This sec-
ond socialization pathway may take a variety of forms.
FAMILY SYSTEMS APPROACH Parents may serve as coaches, teachers, and supervisors
TO SOCIALIZATION as they provide advice, support, and directions about
strategies for managing new social situations or negoti-
Consistent with a family systems viewpoint, recent re- ating social challenges. In a third role, parents function
search has focused on a variety of subsystems, includ- as managers of their childrens social lives and serve as
ing parent-child, marital, and sibling-sibling systems. In regulators of opportunities for social contacts and cog-
the next several sections, we focus on each of these sub- nitive experiences. Researchers have begun to recognize
systems as contexts for socialization. Finally, we exam- the managerial function of parents and to appreciate the
ine recent attempts to conceptualize the family as a unit impact of variations in how this managerial function in-
of analysis. fluences child development (Parke, Killian, et al., 2003).
By managerial, we refer to the ways in which parents or-
The Parent-Child Subsystem: A ganize and arrange the childs home environment and
Tripartite Approach the opportunities for social contact with playmates and
socializing agents outside the family. Although the
In this section, we consider the parent-child subsystem model has been largely applied to the issue of family
and the relation between this parent-child subsystem peer relationships, it is useful for explaining a wide
and childrens social adaptation. Although it has been range of socialization outcomes such as gender roles and
common in traditional paradigms to focus on the impact aggression (see S. McHale, Crouter, & Whiteman, 2003,
of the parent-child relationship or parental child-rearing for an application to gender roles).
styles, according to the Parke, Burks, Carson, Neville,
and Boyum tripartite model (1994), this represents only Parent-Child Relationships: Interaction and
one pathway (see Figure 8.1). Child-Rearing Styles
In this case, the goal of parent-child interaction is not In this section, we consider descriptive studies of differ-
explicitly to modify or enhance childrens relationships ences in both the quantity of mother versus father in-
with extrafamilial social partners. In addition, this volvement with their children and qualitative differences
scheme posits that parents may influence their children in styles of interaction. Then, we explore the implica-
through a second pathway namely as direct instructor, tions of parent-child interactive style and level of in-
educator, or consultant. In this role, parents may explic- volvement for childrens socialization outcomes.
Not all forms of parental involvement are conceptu-
ally equivalent. Lamb, Pleck, and Levine (1985) have
distinguished various types of parental involvement: in-
teraction, availability, and responsibility (see Lamb,
Parent as Parent as 2004). Each is further defined as follows:
Parent-Child
Direct Provider of
Interaction
Instructor Opportunities
Interaction refers to the parents direct contact with his
child through caregiving and shared activities. Availabil-
ity is a related concept concerning the childs potential
availability for interaction, by virtue of being present or
Peer
accessible to the child whether or not direct interaction is
Competence
occurring. Responsibility refers to the role the parent
takes in ascertaining that the child is taken care of and ar-
Figure 8.1 A tripartite model of family-peer relationships. ranging for resources to be available for the child. (Lamb
(Adapted from Family-Peer Relationships: A Tripartite et al., 1985, p. 125)
Model (pp. 115145), in Exploring Family Relationships
with Other Social Contexts: Family Research Consortium
Advances in Family Research, R. D. Parke, V. M. Burks, J. V. Several further distinctions have been offered. Specifi-
Carson, B. Neville, and L. A. Boyum, 1994, In R. D. Parke & cally, it is important to distinguish involvement in child-
S. Kellam (Eds.), (pp. 115145). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. care activities and involvement in play, leisure, or
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 435

affiliative activities with the child because there are 2003). In middle childhood (6- to 7-year-olds), Russell
different determinants of these two types of parental in- and Russell (1987) found that Australian mothers were
volvement. Absolute and relative involvement need to be available to children 54.7 hours/week compared to 34.6
distinguished because prior work suggests that these in- hours/week for fathers. Mothers also spent more time
dices are independent and may affect both childrens alone with children (22.6 hours/week) than did fathers
and adults views of role distributions in different ways (2.4 hours/week). However, when both parents and child
(Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004). were together, mothers and fathers initiated interactions
with children with equal frequency and childrens initi-
Quantitative Assessments of Mother and Father ations toward each parent were similar. Adolescents
Involvement in Intact Families spend less time with their parents than younger children
Despite current shifts in cultural attitudes concerning and less time alone with their fathers than their mothers
the appropriateness and desirability of shared roles and (Larson & Richards, 1994). From infancy through ado-
equal levels of participation in routine caregiving and lescence, mothers and fathers clearly differ in their de-
interaction for mothers and fathers, the shifts toward gree of involvement with their children.
parity are small but nonetheless real in the majority of
intact families. Fathers spend less time with their in- Qualitative Effects: Stylistic Differences in
fants and children than mothers (Pleck & Masciadrelli, Mother and Father Interaction. Fathers participate
2004) not only in the United States but also in other less than mothers in caregiving but spend a greater per-
countries such as Great Britain, Australia, France, Bel- centage of the time available for interaction in play ac-
gium, and Japan (Zuzanek, 2000). Mothers and fathers tivities than mothers do. In North American families,
differ in the amount of time that they spend in actual fathers regardless of ethnicity (European American,
interaction with their children. Pleck and Masciadrelli African American, or Hispanic American) spent a
(2004) document that fathers involvement has in- greater percentage of their time with their infants in
creased, even if slowly. Compared to the 1970s, propor- play than mothers, although in absolute terms mothers
tional engagement (relative to mothers) was about 33%, spent more time than fathers in play with their children
whereas accessibility was approximately 50%. In con- (Yeung et al., 2001). The quality of play across mothers
trast, recent estimates for the 1990s suggest that pro- and fathers differs, too. For young infants, older infants,
portional engagement increased to approximately 70%, and toddlers, fathers play more physically arousing
whereas accessibility was over 70%. games than mothers. In contrast, mothers played more
Studies of African American and Hispanic-American conventional motor games or toy-mediated activities and
families confirm the pattern found for European Ameri- were more verbal and didactic (Parke, 1996, 2002).
cans. Middle-class and lower-class African American Nor are these effects evident only in infancy. Mac-
and Latino fathers were less involved in caregiving with Donald and Parke (1984) found that fathers engaged in
their infants than mothers (Roopnarine, 2004; Roopnar- more physical play with their 3- and 4-year-old children
ine, Fouts, Lamb, & Lewis-Elligan, 2005). Comparisons than mothers, whereas mothers engaged in more object-
across ethnic groups (African, Hispanic, and European mediated play than fathers. According to a survey (Mac-
American) revealed few differences in the level of father Donald & Parke, 1986), fathers distinctive role as a
involvement (Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, & Hofferth, physical play partner changes with age. Physical play
2001). These findings are important given past negative was highest between fathers and 2-year-olds, and be-
characterizations of low-income African American and tween 2 and 10 years of age, there is a decrease in fa-
Hispanic American fathers as uninvolved. The stereo- ther-child physical play.
type surrounding fathers of different ethnic backgrounds Despite the decline in physical play across age, fa-
needs to be discarded as inaccurate and outdated (Roop- thers remain more physical in their play than mothers.
narine, 2004). Much of the earlier work was based on In an Australian study of parents and their 6- to 7-year-
single-parent families and failed to recognize differ- old children (Russell & Russell, 1987), fathers were
ences within cultural groups. more involved in physical /outdoor play interactions and
The pattern of contact time between mothers and fa- fixing things around the house and garden than mothers.
thers with their children in infancy continues into mid- In contrast, mothers were more actively involved in
dle childhood and adolescence (Collins & Madsen, caregiving and household tasks, school work, reading,
436 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

playing with toys, and helping with arts and crafts. In crease their salience despite more limited time. Biological
adolescence, the quality of maternal and paternal in- factors cannot be ignored given that male monkeys show
volvement continues to differ. Just as in earlier develop- the same rough-and-tumble physical style of play as Amer-
mental periods, mothers and fathers may complement ican human fathers and tend to respond more positively to
each other and provide models that reflect the tasks of bids for rough-and-tumble play than females (Parke &
adolescenceconnectedness and separateness. Evi- Suomi, 1981). Perhaps [both monkey and human] males
dence suggests that fathers may help adolescents de- may be more susceptible to being aroused into states of
velop their own sense of identity and autonomy by being positive excitement and unpredictability than females
more peer-like and more playful (e.g., joking and teas- (Maccoby, 1988, p. 761)speculation that is consistent
ing), which is likely to promote more equal and egalitar- with gender differences in risk taking and sensation seek-
ian exchanges. Fathers, more than mothers conveyed ing. In addition, human males, whether boys or men, tend
the feeling that they can rely on their adolescents, thus to behave more boisterously and show more positive emo-
fathers might serve as a facilitating environment for tional expression and reactions than females (Maccoby,
adolescent attainment of differentiation from the family 1998). Together these threads of the puzzle suggest that
and consolidation of independence (Shulman & Klein, predisposing biological differences between males and fe-
1993, p. 53). Although the style of fathers involvement males may play a role in the play patterns of mothers and
as a play or recreational partner appears to have reason- fathers. Yet, the cross-cultural data underscore the ways
able continuity from infancy through adolescence, the in which cultural and environmental contexts shape play
meaning and function of this interaction style shifts patterns of parents and remind us of the high degree of
across development. The positive affect associated with plasticity of human social behaviors.
fathers play in infancy is not as evident in adolescence,
although other goals of this age period may be facili- Parent-Child Interaction and Childrens Adapta-
tated by this more playful egalitarian style. tion. Two approaches to this issue of the impact of
A word of caution is in order because fathers in sev- parent-child interaction on childrens socialization out-
eral other cultures do not show this physical play style. comes have been utilized in recent research. Some have
In some cultures that are similar to U.S. culture, such as adopted a typological approach and examined styles or
England and Australia, there are similar parental sex types of child-rearing practices (Baumrind, 1973). Oth-
differences in play style. In contrast, findings from sev- ers have adopted a social interaction approach by focus-
eral other cultures do not find that physical play is a cen- ing on the nature of the interchanges between parent and
tral part of the father-infant relationship (Roopnarine, child (G. Patterson, 2002).
2004). Neither Swedish nor Israeli kibbutz fathers were
more likely to play with their children or to engage in The Typological Approach. Perhaps the most influen-
different types of play (Hwang, 1987). Similarly, Chi- tial typology has been offered by Baumrind (1973) who
nese Malaysian, Indian, and Aka pygmy (Central Africa) distinguished between three types of parental child-
mothers and fathers rarely engaged in physical play with rearing typologies. She found that authoritative but not
their children (Hewlett, 2004; Roopnarine, 2004). In- authoritarian or overly permissive behavior by parents
stead, both display affection and engage in plenty of led to positive emotional, social, and cognitive develop-
close physical contact. In other cultures, such as Italy, ment in children. Baumrind has followed her authoritar-
neither mothers nor fathers but, instead, other women in ian, authoritative, and permissive parents and their
the extended family or in the community were likely to children from the preschool period through adolescence
play physically with infants (New & Benigni, 1987). in a longitudinal study (Baumrind, 1991). She found
Why do mothers and fathers play differently? Both bi- that authoritative parenting continued to be associated
ological and environment factors probably play a role. Ex- with positive outcomes for adolescents as with younger
perience with infants, the amount of time spent with children and that responsive, firm parent-child relation-
infants, the usual kinds of responsibilities that a parent as- ships were especially important in the development of
sumesall of these factors influence the parents style of competence in sons. Moreover, authoritarian child rear-
play. The fact that fathers spend less time with infants and ing had more negative long-term outcomes for boys than
children than mothers may contribute as well. Fathers for girls. Sons of authoritarian parents were low in both
may use their distinctive arousing style as a way to in- cognitive and social competence. Their academic and
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 437

intellectual performance was poor. In addition, they drens levels of compliance to parental control, which is
were unfriendly and lacking in initiative, leadership, a useful bridging of the two levels of analyses.
and self-confidence in their relations with their peers. In an attempt to resolve this issue, Darling and Stein-
Maccoby and Martin (1983) extended the Baumrind ty- berg (1993) have argued that parental style and parental
pology based on combinations of the warm/responsive, practices need to be distinguished. Parenting style is a
unresponsive/rejecting dimension and the restrictive/de- constellation of attitudes toward the child that are com-
manding, permissive/undemanding dimension and in- municated to the child and create an emotional climate
cluded a fourth type of parenting style, which is in which parents behaviors are expressed (p. 493). In
characterized by neglect and lack of involvement. These contrast to style, parenting practices are behaviors de-
are disengaged parents who are motivated to do whatever fined by specific content and socialization goals
is necessary to minimize the costs in time and effort of in- (p. 492). These authors cite attending school functions
teraction with child (Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Such and spanking as examples of parenting practices. Style is
parents are motivated to keep the child at a distance and assumed to be independent of both the content of parent-
focus on their own needs rather than the needs of the child. ing behavior and the specific socialization content. Crit-
They are parent centered rather than child centered. With ical to their model is the assumption that parenting style
older children, this is associated with the parents failure has its impact on child outcomes indirectly. First, style
to monitor the childs activity or to know where the child transforms the nature of parent-child interaction and
is, what the child is doing, and who the childs companions thereby moderates the impact of specific practices. Sec-
are. In infants, such a lack of parental involvement is asso- ond, they posit that style modifies the childs openness
ciated with disruptions in attachment; in older children, it to parental influence, which, in turn, moderates the asso-
is associated with impulsivity, aggression, noncompliance, ciation between parenting practices and child outcomes.
moodiness, and low self-esteem (Baumrind, 1991). Older A second concern focuses on the issue of direction of
children also show disruptions in peer relations and in effects. It is unclear whether the styles described by
cognitive development, achievement, and school perfor- Baumrind are, in part, in response to the childs behav-
mance (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992). It is the com- ior. Placing the typology work in a transactional frame-
bined impact of not having the skills to be able to gain work (Sameroff, 1994) would argue that children with
gratification in either social or academic pursuits that fre- certain temperaments and/or behavioral characteristics
quently leads to delinquency in children with neglecting would determine the nature of the parental style.
parents (Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002). Parental in- A third concern is the universality of the typological
volvement plays an important role in the development of scheme. Recent studies have raised serious questions
both social and cognitive competence in children. about the generalizability of these styles across either
socioeconomic status (SES) or ethnic/cultural groups.
The Status of the Typological Approach. A major con- Two issues are involved here. First, does the rate of uti-
cern about the focus on parental style is the limited at- lization of different styles vary across groups? Second,
tention to the delineation of the processes that account are the advantages of positive social outcomes associ-
for the effects of different styles on childrens develop- ated with a particular style (e.g., authoritative) similar
ment. Throughout the history of socialization research, across groups? The answer to both questions seems to
there has been a tension between molar and molecular be negative. In lower-SES families, parents are more
levels of analysis. Over the past 3 decades, the pendulum likely to use an authoritarian as opposed to an authori-
has swung back and forth between these levels of analy- tative style, but this style is often an adaptation to the
sis. Currently, these two strands of research coexist and ecological conditions, such as increased danger and
are seldom united in a single study. On the molecular threat, which may characterize the lives of poor fami-
side, the work of G. Patterson (2002), and Gottman lies (Furstenberg, Cook, Eccles, Elder, & Sameroff,
(1994) can be cited. On the molar side, the search for ty- 1999). Moreover, studies find that the use of authori-
pological answers to parenting style continues (Baum- tarian strategies under these circumstances may be
rind, 1991; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992). linked with more positive outcomes for children (Bald-
Some exceptions can be noted. For example, Hethering- win, Baldwin, & Cole, 1990). A second challenge to
ton and Clingempeel (1992) have used both parenting the presumed universal advantage of authoritative
style in combination with sequential analyses of chil- child-rearing styles comes from cross-ethnic studies.
438 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Accumulating evidence underscores the nonuniversal- offsprings development. Quality rather than quantity of
ity of these stylistic distinctions and suggests the im- parent-child interaction is the important predictor of
portance of developing concepts that are based on an cognitive and social development.
indigenous appreciation of the culture in question Differences in interactive style associated with chil-
(R. K. Chao, 1994). In summary, it is evident that con- drens social competence and the emotional displays
textual and cultural considerations need more attention during parent-child interaction are important. The affec-
in typological approaches to child rearing. tive quality of the interactions of popular children and
their parents differs from the interactions of rejected
The Parent-Child Interactional Approach. Re- children and their parents (Parke, Cassidy, Burks, Car-
search in this tradition is based on the assumption that son, & Boyum, 1992). Consistently higher levels of posi-
face-to-face interaction with parents may provide the op- tive affect have been found in both parents and children
portunity to learn, rehearse, and refine social skills that in popular dyads than in the rejected dyads. Negative
are common to successful social interaction with other parental affect is associated with lower levels of peer ac-
social partners. This work has yielded several conclu- ceptance (Isley et al., 1996, 1999): Carson and Parke
sions. First, the style of the interaction between parent (1996) found that children of fathers who are likely to
and child is linked to a variety of social outcomes includ- respond to their childrens negative affect displays with
ing aggression, achievement, and moral development. To negative affect of their own are less socially skilled
illustrate this approach, studies of childrens social com- (e.g., less altruistic, more avoidant, and more aggressive)
petence are considered. Recent studies have found that than their preschool classmates. The results for the reci-
parents who are responsive, warm, and engaging are more procity of negative affect were evident only for fathers,
likely to have children who are more socially competent which suggests that men may play a particularly salient
(Grimes, Klein, & Putallaz, 2004). Moreover, high levels role in childrens learning how to manage negative emo-
of positive synchrony and low levels of nonsynchrony in tions in the contexts of social interactions. Boyum and
patterns of mother-child interaction are related to school Parke (1995) confirmed the importance of parental neg-
adjustment rated by teachers, peers, and observers (Har- ative affect for childrens social development but
rist, Pettit, Dodge, & Bates, 1994). In contrast, parents demonstrated that father anger is a particularly salient
who are hostile and controlling have children who experi- predictor of childrens social acceptance by peers. Less
ence more difficulty with age-mates in the preschool pe- accepted children were likely to receive angry affect
riod (Harrist et al., 1994) and middle childhood. from their fathers during observations of family dinner.
Evidence suggests that family interaction patterns This finding underscores the importance of distinguish-
not only relate to concurrent peer relationships but also ing among specific affective displays rather than re-
predict peer relationships across time. In their study of liance on categories of negative or positive emotions.
third grade children, Henggeler, Edwards, Cohen, and Together these findings lead to a revision in tradi-
Summerville (1992) found that children of fathers who tional thinking about the ways that mothers and fathers
were responsive to their childrens requests became influence their childrens development. This work sug-
more popular over the school year than children of less gests that fathers may play a larger role in socialization
responsive fathers. Similarly, J. Barth and Parke (1993) of childrens emotion than earlier theories suggested.
found that parents who were better able to sustain their And it is through the management of their own emotions
children in play predicted better subsequent adaptation and their reactions to their childrens emotions that fa-
to kindergarten. thers may have their greatest impact on their childrens
Although there is an overlap between mothers and fa- social relationships with peers and friends. In summary,
thers, evidence is emerging that fathers make a unique both the nature of parent-child interaction and affective
and independent contribution to their childrens social quality of the relationship are important correlates of
development. Studies (Hart et al., 1998; Isley, ONeil, & childrens social development.
Parke, 1996) have shown that fathers continue to con-
tribute to childrens social behavior with peersafter Beyond Description: Processes Mediating the Re-
accounting for the mothers contribution. Although fa- lations between Parent-Child Interaction and Chil-
ther involvement is quantitatively less than mother in- drens Social Competence. A variety of processes
volvement, fathers have an important impact on their have been hypothesized as mediators between parent-
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 439

child interaction and peer outcomes. These include One set of skills that is relevant to successful peer in-
emotion-encoding and emotion-decoding skills, emo- teraction and may, in part, be acquired in the context of
tional regulatory skills, cognitive representations, attri- parent-child play, especially arousing physical play, is
butions and beliefs, and problem-solving skills and the ability to clearly encode emotional signals and to
attention-deployment abilities (Eisenberg, 2000; Ladd, decode others emotional states. Through physically
1992; Parke, McDowell, Kim, & Leidy, 2006; Parke & playful interaction with their parents, especially fa-
ONeil, 1999). These abilities or beliefs are acquired in thers, children may be learning how to use emotional
parent-child interchanges during development and, in signals to regulate the social behavior of others. In ad-
turn, guide the nature of childrens behavior with their dition, they may learn to accurately decode the social
peers. We focus on three sets of processes that seem and emotional signals of other social partners. Several
particularly promising candidates for mediator status: studies have found positive relations between childrens
(1) affect-management skills, (2) cognitive representa- ability to encode emotional expressions and childrens
tional processes, and (3) attention regulatory processes social competence with peers (Halberstadt, Denham, &
(see Figure 8.2). Dunsmore, 2001). Successful peer interaction requires
not only the ability to recognize and produce emotions
Affect-Management Skills as a Mediating Mech- but also a social understanding of emotion-related ex-
anism. Children learn more than specific affective periences, of the meaning of emotions, of the cause of
expressions, such as anger or sadness or joy, in the fam- emotions, and of the responses appropriate to others
ily. They learn a cluster of processes associated with the emotions. Cassidy, Parke, Butkovsky, and Braungart
understanding and regulation of affective displays, (1992), in a study of 5- and 6-year-old children, found
which we term af fect-management skills (Parke, Cas- that a higher level of peer acceptance was associated
sidy, Burks, Carson, & Boyum, 1992). These skills are with greater (a) ability to identify emotions, ( b) ac-
acquired during the course of parent-child interaction knowledgment of experiencing emotion, (c) ability to
and are available to the child for use in other relation- describe appropriate causes of emotions, and (d) expec-
ships. Moreover, it is assumed that these skills play a tations that they and their parents would respond appro-
mediating role between family and childrens social priately to the display of emotions. Family emotional
competence. expressivenessan index of the extent to which family

Parent-Child Interaction

Parent Emotional Expressiveness Parent Cognitive Representations

Childrens Emotional Mediators:


Childrens Cognitive Attentional Processes:
1. Decoding skills
Representations: 1. Selective attention
2. Encoding skills
1. Goals
3. Understanding of emotional 2. Sustained attention
2. Strategies
causes and consequences 3. Ability to shift focus
3. Anticipated consequences
4. Display rules 4. Joint attention abilities
4. Efficacy beliefs
5. Emotional regulation

Peer Interaction

Peer Sociometric Status

Figure 8.2 Emotional, cognitive, and attentional mediating links between family and peer systems.
440 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

members express emotion in the course of everyday in- Gottman, Katz, and Hooven (1997) on parents meta-
teractionhas emerged as a further link between fam- emotion. By meta-emotion these researchers refer to
ily and peer systems, providing guidelines for the use of parents emotions about their own and their childrens
emotion in ongoing social interchanges. The expressive- emotions, and meta-cognitive structure refers to an or-
ness concept extends emotional learning beyond the ac- ganized set of thoughts, a philosophy, and an approach
quisition of specific skills, such as encoding or to ones own emotions and to ones childrens emotions.
decoding, to the utilization of rules about emotion in Gottman et al. (1997), in a longitudinal analysis, found
multiple contexts (Halberstadt et al., 2001). Several that fathers acceptance and assistance with childrens
studies have found cross-generational similarities be- sadness and anger at 5 years of age was related to their
tween mothers (Denham, 1998), fathers (Boyum & childrens social competence with peers at 8 years of
Parke, 1995), and childrens levels of expressiveness. age. Moreover, fathers assistance with anger predicted
Consistent with the assumption that expressiveness is academic achievement. The gender of child influenced
learned in the family and that children transfer their ex- these relationships: When fathers help daughters with
pressive style to their interactions with others outside sadness, the daughters are rated as more competent by
the family, several studies (Boyum & Parke, 1995; Cas- their teachers. When fathers help their daughters regu-
sidy et al., 1992) found links between family expres- late anger, girls are rated as more socially competent by
siveness and peer competence. These studies suggest their teachers, show higher academic achievement, and
that family emotional expressiveness may be one path- their dyadic interaction with a best friend is less nega-
way by which children learn to understand and express tive. Fathers who are more accepting of their sons
their emotions in a socially appropriate manner. anger and assist them in regulating anger have sons who
are less aggressive.
Emotional Regulation. Research suggests that parental Together, these studies suggest that various aspects of
support and acceptance of childrens emotions is related emotional developmentencoding, decoding, cognitive
to childrens ability to manage emotions in a construc- understanding, and emotional regulationplay an impor-
tive fashion. Several investigators (Eisenberg, 2000; tant role in accounting for variations in peer competence.
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992; Parke et al., 2006) have found Our argument is that these aspects of emotion may be
links between the ability to regulate emotional arousal learned in the context of family interaction and serve as
and social competence. Similarly, children who either mediators between the parents and peers. At the same
have limited knowledge of emotional display rules time, the contribution of genetics to individual differences
(Saarni, 1999) or are poor at utilizing display rules are in emotionality and emotional regulation probably plays a
less well accepted by their peers (McDowell, ONeil, & role in the emergence of these emotional processes
Parke, 2000; McDowell & Parke, 2000). Parental com- (Eisenberg, 2000; Kochanska, 1993). Finally, the direc-
forting of children when they experience negative emo- tion of effects remains unclear in these relations; probably
tion has been linked with constructive anger reactions both parent and child mutually influence each other in the
(Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994). Moreover, childrens emo- development of affect-management skills.
tional regulation and their knowledge and affective dis-
play rule utilization are linked with high positive Cognitive Representational Models: A Second Me-
parental affect and low levels of parental control (Mc- diator between Parents and Peers. One of the prob-
Dowell & Parke, 2000, 2005; McDowell et al., 2000). In lems facing the area of family peer relationships is how
addition, parental willingness to discuss emotions with children transfer the strategies they acquire in the family
their children is related to childrens awareness and un- to their peer relationships. Several theories assume that
derstanding of others emotions (Denham, Cook, & individuals possess internal mental representations that
Zoller, 1992; Dunn & Brown, 1994). Eisenberg, Fabes, guide their social behavior. Attachment theorists offer
Schaller, and Miller (1991) found that parental emphasis working models (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999),
on direct problem solving was associated with sons whereas social and cognitive psychologists have sug-
sympathy, whereas restrictiveness in regard to express- gested scripts or cognitive maps that could serve as a
ing ones own negative emotions was associated with guide for social action (Grusec & Ungerer, 2003).
sons physiological and facial indicators of personal dis- Attachment researchers have found support for
tress. These findings are consistent with work by Bowlbys argument that representations vary as a func-
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 441

tion of child-parent attachment history (see Bretherton other children, had children who were more assertive,
& Munholland, 1999). For example, children who had prosocial, and competent social problem solvers.
been securely attached infants were more likely to rep- McDowell, Parke, and Spitzer (2002) explored the
resent their family in their drawings in a coherent man- links between parent and child cognitive representations
ner, with a balance between individuality and of social relationships. Parents and their children re-
connection, than were children who had been insecurely sponded to vignettes reflecting interpersonal dilemmas
attached (Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004). by indicating how they may react in each situation.
Research in a social interactional tradition reveals Open-ended responses were coded for goals, causes,
links between parent and child cognitive representations strategies, and advice. The cognitive representations of
of social relationships. Burks and Parke (1996) found social behavior of both fathers and mothers were related
some evidence for similarities between children and to their childrens representations. Moreover, fathers
mothers in their goals, attributions, and anticipated con- but not mothers cognitive models of relationships were
sequences when they responded to a series of hypotheti- linked to childrens social competence. Fathers strate-
cal social dilemmas. This study suggests that children gies that were related high on confrontation and instru-
may learn cognitive representational schemes through mental qualities were associated with low teacher
their family relationships, although the precise mecha- ratings of childrens social competence. Fathers with re-
nism through which these schemas are acquired is not lational-prosocial goals have children who are rated as
yet specified. more competent by both teachers and peers. Perhaps fa-
Next, we turn to an examination of the hypothesis thers are more influential in conflict-laden domains,
that parents of children of different sociometric status whereas mothers are more influential in social domains
differ in their cognitive models of social relationships. involving personal and relationship issues. These data
Several aspects of cognitive models including attribu- suggest that fathers cognitive representations of social
tions, perceptions, values, goals, and strategies have relationships are important correlates of childrens so-
been explored. Pettit, Dodge, and Brown (1988) found cial competence.
that mothers attributional biases concerning their chil- Together, these studies suggest that cognitive models
drens behavior (e.g., the extent to which they view an of relationships may be transmitted across generations
ambiguous provocation as hostile or benign) and the en- and these models, in turn, may serve as mediators be-
dorsement of aggression as a solution to interpersonal tween family contexts and childrens relationships with
problems were related to childrens interpersonal prob- others outside of the family. Finally, this work implies
lem-solving skill that was related to their social compe- that both children and parents actively construct their
tence. Other evidence suggests that parents hold own dyadic relationships and other social relationships.
different patterns of beliefs about problematic social be- Moreover, both are influenced in their behavior with
haviors such as aggression and withdrawal and that each other by these cognitive constructions. One issue
these patterns are associated with their childrens mem- that needs more attention is how child and adult con-
bership in various sociometric status groups (Rubin & structions change across development and how the pat-
Mills, 1990). This work suggests that parents do have a tern of mutual influence between parent and child
set of beliefs concerning childrens social behavior that changes as the child develops (Kuczynski, 2003; Mac-
may, in part, govern their behavior (Goodnow, 2002; coby, 1992). Coordination and coregulation rather than
Parke, 1978). simply a bidirectional pattern of influence probably in-
MacKinnon-Lewis and her colleagues (1994) found creasingly characterizes the parent-child relationship in
that mothers and sons hostile attributions were signif- middle childhood and adolescence.
icantly related to the coerciveness of their interactions.
Moreover, mothers attributions were related to reports Attention Regulation: A Third Mediating Mecha-
of their childrens aggression in their classrooms. Simi- nism. In concert with emotional processes, atten-
larly, Rubin, Mills, and Rose-Krasnor (1989) found a tional regulatory processes have come to be viewed as
link between mothers beliefs and their preschoolers another mechanism through which familial socialization
social problem-solving behavior in the classroom. Moth- experiences may influence childrens social compe-
ers who placed higher values on skills, such as making tence. These processes include the ability to attend to
friends, sharing with others, and leading or influencing relevant cues, to sustain attention, to refocus attention
442 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

through such processes as cognitive distraction and cog- mechanism for managing negative affect. Additionally,
nitive restructuring, and other efforts to purposefully fathers who reported expressing more negative dominant
reduce the level of emotional arousal in a situation that emotions, such as anger and criticism, in everyday inter-
is appraised as stressful. Attentional processes are calations had children who reported greater difficulty
thought to organize experience and to play a central role controlling negative emotions. Avoidant coping and neg-
in cognitive and social development, beginning early in ative emotionality, in turn, were related to higher levels
infancy. Thus, B. Wilson and Gottman (1994) aptly con- of parent-reported problem behaviors.
sidered attention regulatory processes as a shuttle Recent findings from the National Institute of Child
linking emotional regulation (ER) and sociocognitive Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child
processes because attentional processes organize both Care Research Network study of child care and youth
cognitions and emotional responses and thus influence development are relevant (NICHD Child Care Research
the socialization of relationship competence. Although Network, 2003b). The role of attention in a laboratory
studies are only beginning to emerge, evidence suggests task as a mediator between parenting and peer outcomes
that attentional regulation may have direct effects on was examined. Parenting was measured by the HOME
childrens social functioning (Eisenberg, 2000; B. Wil- Observation for Measurement of the Environment
son & Gottman, 1994) and, in some circumstances, at- (HOME) scale, by maternal sensitivity, and by cognitive
tentional control may function in interaction with stimulation. Attention was indexed by sustained atten-
dimensions of emotionality and social information pro- tion and impulsivity. Children viewed a matrix of famil-
cessing. Other work (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992) suggests iar objects and were required to note when an object
that attentional control and emotional negativity may in- appeared and to refrain when a nontarget stimulus was
teract when predicting social competence. Attention presented. Errors of omission occurred when children
regulatory skills appear to be more critical among chil- responded to a nontarget stimulus. Children who had
dren who experience higher levels of emotional negativ- fewer errors of omission had greater ability to sustain
ity. Eisenberg, Gutherie, Fabes, Shepard, Losoya, et al. attention; children with errors of commission were more
(2000). argued that when children are not prone to expe- impulsive. Social competence and externalizing behav-
rience intense negative emotions, attention regulatory iors were rated by child-care caregivers at 54 months.
processes may be less essential to positive social func- There were links between higher-quality family envi-
tioning. In contrast, the social functioning of children ronments and better social competence and lower exter-
who experience anger and other negative emotions may nalizing. Second, sustained attention and less
only be undermined when these children do not have the impulsivity were associated with higher social compe-
ability to use attention regulatory processes, such as tence scores. Third, impulsivity served as a mediator be-
cognitive restructuring, and other forms of emotion-fo- tween family- and social-outcome measures.
cused coping (see also, Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spin- In recent follow-up (NICHD Child Care Network,
rad, 1998). 2006), attention regulation mediated between mother
Attentional processes may work in tandem with emo- and father parental sensitivity measures and teacher rat-
tional regulatory abilities to enhance social functioning ings of childrens peer competence and self-reports of
(ONeil & Parke, 2000). Parenting style may be an im- loneliness in first grade. In this study, attention was
portant antecedent of childrens abilities to refocus at- measured by both the Continuous Performance Task
tention away from emotionally distressing events. Data (CPT) and by maternal reports of attentional regulatory
from fifth graders indicated that when mothers adopted abilities. Using third grade outcomes of aggression, ex-
a negative, controlling parenting style in a problem-solv- clusion by peers, and friendship, and earlier measures
ing discussion, children were less likely to use cognitive (first grade) of mother and father sensitivity predicted
decision making as a coping strategy. Additionally, chil- these outcomes. Moreover, attentional regulation com-
dren were more likely to report greater difficulty in con- petence served as a mediator between parenting and
trolling negative affect when distressed. Lower levels of later peer outcomes. Together these studies provide evi-
cognitive decision making and higher levels of negative dence for the role of attention as a mediator of the links
affect were associated with more problem behaviors and between family and peer systems.
higher levels of negative interactions with classmates.
Similarly, when fathers adopted a negative, controlling Parental Instruction, Advice Giving, Consulta-
style, children were more likely to use avoidance as a tion, and Rule Provision. Learning about relation-
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 443

ships through interaction with parents can be viewed as was positively related to adolescents academic compe-
an indirect pathway because the goal is often not explic- tence and psychological adjustment (e.g., low levels of
itly to influence childrens social relationships with ex- depression; Furstenberg et al., 1999) and negatively re-
trafamilial partners such as peers. In contrast, parents lated to childrens antisocial behavior and association
may influence childrens relationships directly in their with delinquent peers in late elementary and middle
role as a direct instructor, educator, or advisor. In this school (Reid et al., 2002) even though parents are not di-
role, parents may explicitly set out to educate their chil- rectly involved (e.g., giving instructions) in childrens
dren concerning appropriate ways of initiating and interactions with peers. Perhaps with older children, an
maintaining social relationships and learning social and adults presence keeps the childrens behavior in line
moral rules. with parental expectations.
Several studies have examined these issues. In a Past research on advice giving often has focused on
study of parental supervision, Bhavnagri and Parke either the content of parents advice or the manner in
(1991) found that children exhibited more cooperation, which it is given. There is a shift in recent research to-
turn taking, and had longer play bouts when assisted by ward an integrative approach to this topic. As Grusec
an adult than when playing without assistance. Adult as- and Goodnow (1994) suggested, both style and content
sistance enhanced the quality of play for younger (2 to need to be considered together in determining the im-
3.5 years of age) children more than older (3.5 to 6 years pact of parental advice giving on childrens peer out-
of age) children. Although both fathers and mothers comes. The combined impact of these two aspects of
were effective facilitators of their childrens play with parental advice giving is just beginning to achieve
peers, under natural conditions, mothers are more likely recognition. In their study of children aged 3 to 5 years,
to play this supervisory role than fathers (Bhavnagri & Mize and Pettit (1997) found that maternal information
Parke, 1991; Ladd & Pettit, 2002). giving and guidance (content) predicted, over and above
The quality of advice that mothers provided their mothers warmth and responsiveness (style), childrens
children prior to entry into an ongoing play dyad varied peer acceptance (as rated by teachers) during a play in-
as a function of childrens sociometric status (Russell & teraction context. These studies did not assess the ways
Finnie, 1990). Mothers of well-accepted children were in which children may be contributing to the advice-giv-
more specific and helpful in the quality of advice that ing task. McDowell, Parke, and Wang (2003) found sim-
they provided. In contrast, mothers of poorly accepted ilar results showing that the style and content of peer
children provided relatively ineffective kinds of verbal relationships made independent contributions, after
guidance such as have fun and stay out of trouble. controlling for childrens behavior during the task.
The advice was too general to be of value to the children Most of the research examining parental advice giv-
in their subsequent instructions. ing assumed that parents advise children; thus, parents
As children grow, caregiver forms of management hold the key to socialization when giving advice. Many
shift from direct involvement or supervision of the ongo- studies neglect the fact that children often raise the sub-
ing activities of children and their peers to a less public jects that are discussed in the parent-child interaction.
form of management, involving advice or consultation In effect, children are actively contributing to their own
concerning appropriate ways of handling peer problems. socialization by selecting issues on which to receive ad-
This form of direct parental management has been vice. Moreover, whatever the relations are between
termed consultation (Ladd, & Pettit, 2002). Parents re- parental advice giving and childrens social compe-
port using verbal guidance (e.g., discussion about future tence, the child necessarily holds the power to put into
consequences, talk of values, and offering their advice) action any advice that is offered. In this sense, children
more often than direct interventions (e.g., limiting the and parents may find themselves as equal partners in fa-
adolescents activities with peers or inviting friends cilitating peer relationships. Again, children are active
over to the house to shape peer influence; Mounts, solicitors of parental responses. Either behavioral char-
2000). Parental limiting of activities with peers and acteristics during a parent-child interaction or beliefs
inviting childrens friends over, however, were reported about the childs behavior in general may affect the rel-
more frequently when parents attempt to influence ative quality of the parent-child interaction and thus the
friend selection (Mounts, 2000). These indirect forms of effectiveness of advice giving. It is also important to
supervision that emerge as the child reaches adolescence note that children may be more or less receptive to
are linked with positive outcomes. Parental supervision parental advice and will shape their own socialization
444 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

experiences by selectively attending to or ignoring tionships is the work of Furstenberg et al. (1999), which
parental offers of advice. explores the relations between parents restrictions on
Both parents and children, however, may regard ex- the activities of their adolescents social behavior and
plicit parental advice as less necessary and/or less appro- adolescents social adjustment. The construct of re-
priate as children develop adequate social skills. Instead, strictions included, among other aspects, monitoring,
by middle childhood, provision of advice would be ex- rules, teaching good judgment, and restriction of activi-
pected to be used by parents for remediational goals in ties. A higher number of restrictions was linked to fewer
which the advice giving would be highest for children problem behaviors (e.g., school truancy or substance
who functioned poorly socially. McDowell et al. (2003) abuse) and higher social involvement with peers. The op-
found that when parents offered more advice and more erationalization of the restrictions construct combined
specific advice about peer dilemmas, children were rated rules with several parenting skills so that the role of
as less positive and more negative by teachers and peers. rules alone in the regulation of childrens behavior with
However, children may act as agents of their own devel- peers is unclear. Simpkins and Parke (2002) explored
opment by eliciting more specific advice to compensate the relations between parental play rules and sixth-grade
for poor social functioning away from the parent. Other childrens loneliness, depression, and friendship quality.
evidence suggests that the type of advice changes further Boys whose parents had fewer play rules reported lower
as children enter adolescence and develop a future orien- levels of depression and more conflict in their best
tation. In adolescence, parents are likely to try to keep friendship. As in other aspects of parental management,
their children from being influenced by peers by talking the process by which rules are jointly negotiated by chil-
to them about future consequences of their behavior. dren and their parents is poorly understood. Issues of
Mounts (2000) found that 37% of mothers of adolescents mutual trust, parental perception of the childs level of
used this direct strategy with their ninth-grade children. responsibility, and self-regulatory ability, and, for the
This parental guidance approach (e.g., My parents child, the perceived fairness of the rules probably all
tell me who I have for friends will affect my future) was play a role in both the negotiation process and the extent
associated with selection of friends with low levels of to which the rules are likely to be followed.
antisocial behavior and high levels of academic achieve-
ment. Advice giving is a bilateral process in which both Parents as Managers of Childrens Opportunities
parents and children are active participants at all stages Parents influence their childrens social relationships not
of the advice process, from selection of topics to accep- only through their direct interactions with their children
tance or rejection of advice. but also as managers of their childrens social lives
These studies suggest that direct parental influence (Furstenberg et al., 1999; Parke, Killian, et al., 2003). This
in the form of supervision and advice giving can in- parental role is of theoretical importance given the recent
crease the competence of young children and illus- claims that parents impact on childrens development is
trates the utility of examining direct parental limited and peer group-level processes account for major
strategies as a way of teaching children about social socialization outcomes (Harris, 1998). In contrast, we con-
relationships. In these studies, the direction of effects ceptualize the parental management of access to peers as a
is difficult to determine and parents may be respond- further pathway through which parents influence their
ing to their childrens level of social skill. Experimen- childrens development (Parke, Killian, et al., 2003). Moth-
tal and longitudinal studies would help place these ers and fathers differ in their degree of responsibility for
studies on a clearer interpretative footing. management of family tasks; mothers are more likely to as-
Another avenue through which parents can regulate sume the managerial role than fathers (Parke, 2002).
their childrens peer relationships is the provision of Parents make choices about neighborhoods and schools
rules or guidelines. Rules concerning childrens peer re- as well as the formal and informal activities in which their
lationships include the guidelines regarding what activi- children can participate. In these ways, parents act as de-
ties the children can engage in and when, with whom, signers when they seek to control or influence the settings
and where they can play. To date, little attention has in which children are likely to meet and interact with
been given to the role of rules in the development and peers (Ladd & Pettit, 2002, p. 286). These design deci-
maintenance of childrens peer relationships. Particu- sions can influence childrens social and academic out-
larly relevant to the links between rules and peer rela- comes. Instead of viewing parents as acting alone in their
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 445

designer roles, we prefer to view parents and children as functioning. Children who could easily access ( by walk-
co-designers in recognition of childrens roles as shapers ing or bike) community resources, such as structured and
and negotiators across development. Many decisions unstructured activities at formally sponsored organiza-
even in the designer domainare influenced by childrens tions, were higher both in their acceptance of individual
and parents needs, wishes, and decisions. In this section, differences and perspective taking. Similarly, using U.S.
we consider each of these aspects of the designer role. Census tract data, Coulton and Pandey (1992) found that
youngsters in areas with high levels of poverty differed
Neighborhoods as Determinants of Peer Contact. from those in low-poverty areas on several outcomes, in-
Although it is assumed that parents choose their neigh- cluding reading scores, birth weight, infant death, and ju-
borhoods, many constraints limit the range of locations venile delinquency. A related Australian study (Homel &
from which to choose, especially economic (i.e., cost) Burns, 1989) found that children in the most disadvan-
and geographic (i.e., distance from work or transporta- taged neighborhoods, reported higher loneliness, feelings
tion). Choice of neighborhood is not equally available to of rejection by peers, worry, and lower life satisfaction
parents; lower-SES and minority group parents have a compared to children in less disadvantaged neighbor-
more restricted set of options than higher-SES and non- hoods. Although recent studies find evidence of neigh-
minority parents. However, there is considerable variabil- borhood effects across a range of developmental
ity in neighborhood effects on children because of the outcomes in both adolescents and young children, these
ways in which parents manage their childrens access to effects appear to be modest after taking into account fam-
aspects of their neighborhood setting. Neighborhood ily effects (Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000). The ef-
choice, therefore, is simply one phase of a multiphase fects of neighborhoods on childrens outcomes are often
process in which choices made by different parents in mediated by parenting practices such as supervision and
similar neighborhoods, as well as the initial choice of monitoring. ONeil, Parke, and McDowell (2001) found
neighborhood, are important (Furstenberg et al., 1999). that when mothers and fathers perceived their neighbor-
A second conceptual assumption about neighborhoods hoods as dangerous and low in social control, they placed
concerns childrens role in neighborhood selection. Al- more restrictions on their fourth grade childrens activi-
though childrenespecially young childrenare not ties. Parental perceptions were more consistently related
usually direct participants in the choice of neighbor- to parenting practices than objective ratings of neighbor-
hoods, their needs, their safety, and their access to other hood quality. Moreover, parental regulatory strategies
children and play space usually are considered by parents serve as mediators of the relation between parental per-
in their deliberations about choice of neighborhood. Ado- ceptions of neighborhood quality and social competence.
lescents may be more active participants by articulating Mothers and fathers who perceived problems in their
their concerns about moving to a new neighborhood that, neighborhood had children who were more prosocial and
for example, involves loss of community-based friend- less aggressive, but this was mediated by shifts in
ships and shifts in school district. Testimony to childrens parental management strategy. Similarly, N. Hill and
power is evidenced by increases in residential mobility of Herman-Stahl (2002) found that interviewers ratings of
families after children complete high school. neighborhood safety were associated with mothers use
Neighborhoods vary in their opportunities for peer- of hostile socialization strategies. Both mothers and in-
peer contact. Especially for young children who have terviewers reports of safety were linked with maternal
limited mobility, neighborhoods form significant por- depression. In turn, mothers depression mediated the
tions of their social world. The most systematic evi- links between neighborhood safety and inconsistent dis-
dence concerning the impact of variations in the quality ciplinary practices. Neighborhoods are an important fac-
of neighborhood environments comes from Medrich tor in accounting for childrens developmental outcomes
(1982), who isolated a number of factorssafety, ter- but much remains to be understood about the mechanisms
rain, distance from commercial areas, and child popula- that account for neighborhood effects and how these
tion densitythat affect the amount and type of peer mechanisms shift across development.
social experience.
What is the impact of neighborhood variations on peer Parents and Children as Partners in Schooling.
competence? Bryant (1985) found that accessibility to Parents choose not only neighborhoods but also, espe-
neighborhood resources is a correlate of socioemotional cially for middle-class families, the type and quality of
446 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

day care and elementary schools that their children will parent involvement at school but in different ways than
attend. These choices make a difference to childrens in earlier school grades. These developmental changes
later development. As studies of child care have shown, can be interpreted as evidence of the childs role in
the quality of and, to some extent, the amount of time in shaping the form that the parent-school partnership
care are linked to childrens cognitive and social devel- will assume at different points in the childs educa-
opment (Clarke-Stewart & Allhusen, 2005). Higher tional career.
quality child care is associated often with higher cogni-
tive functioning. Social behavior, despite the opportu- Parents and Children as Active Agents in Involve-
nity to have increased peer contact, is less consistently ment in Religious Organizations. Parental facilita-
linked with day-care quality: Some evidence suggests tion of childrens involvement in religious institutions is
that children who are in day care for more than 40 hours another potentially important way in which parents man-
per week may show some increases in aggression age their childrens lives. It is important to distinguish
(NICHD Early Childcare Research Network, 2003a). As between the issue of involvement in religious institutions
children develop, parents select neighborhoods as a and religious beliefs because these two aspects of reli-
function of quality of the schools that are available gion may have partially independent effects on family
(Furstenberg et al., 1999). However, these choices are functioning and child outcomes (see Mahoney, Parga-
constrained by existing economic and social opportuni- ment, Tarakeshwar, & Swank, 2001, for a review of reli-
ties (p. 226) and are mainly available to middle-class gious beliefs and parenting practices). Elder and Conger
families. Moreover, the ability to choose is not inconse- (2000) found that church involvement is a family affair.
quential because exercising the ability to choose a school When both parents attended church on a regular basis,
has been linked to adolescent academic outcome children were more likely to be involved in religious or-
(Furstenberg et al., 1999). As a reminder that children ganizations. Similarly, actively involved grandparents
can play a role in this process of school choice, there is tend to have actively involved grandchildren. Church at-
some evidence that childrens behavior in schooltheir tendance involved more than contact with a broadened
successes and failures in both social and academic do- network of adults who share similar family and religious
mainsinfluences the nature of the parent-child rela- values; it also involved exposure to a network of age-
tionship. Repetti (1996) found that childrens positive mates with common beliefs and values. Involvement in
and negative experiences at school during the day altered church activities was associated with higher endorse-
the nature of parent-child interaction in the home after ment of not only church but also school, good grades,
school. Although the study did not address the issue of andespecially for boyscommunity activities. For
the impact on subsequent decisions to change schools those who were less involved in religious activities, ath-
due to this school-based child effect on family dynamics letics and school were given high priority. Religiously
at home, consistent negative school experiences may lead involved youth perceived their friends to be less likely to
parents to consider shifting schools as an option to re- encourage deviant activities, viewed their friends and
duce negative effects on the child and the family. themselves as less involved in deviance, and were less
School choice is not the only way in which families likely to see friends disapproved of by their parents.
and schools are linked. The extent to which parents are Religious involvement in the 8th grade was predictive
involved in school-related activities (e.g., parent- of competence by the 12th grade in grades and peer suc-
teacher associations or school conferences) is posi- cess. Moreover, adolescents who become more reli-
tively related to childrens academic outcomes giously involved by the end of high school tend to rank
(Epstein & Sanders, 2002). Practices of partnerships higher on a variety of competence dimensionsfrom
between parents and schools decline across child devel- academic and peer success to self confidence and rela-
opment. Parents of children in elementary school are tions with parents. A reciprocal influence model best
more likely to volunteer, attend parent-teacher confer- accounted for those findings. Although the primary
ences, and supervise childrens homework. In recogni- flow of influence moved from religious activity and so-
tion of adolescents need for autonomy and cialization to individual competence in achievement,
independence, parental involvement decreases in high some adolescents who were successful academically and
school, but young adolescents still want their families socially and became more involved in religious activi-
to support their learning and activities at home (Ep- ties, further enhanced achievement. Similarly, it is
stein & Sanders, 2002). Even older adolescents endorse likely that both parents and children are active players
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 447

in the process of involvement in religious activities. Al- view consistent with the one guiding our chapter. They
though parentsthrough their own involvement and suggested that monitoring may be a function of the ex-
through their introduction of the child to religious be- tent to which children share information about their ac-
liefs and functionsplay an important initial role, chil- tivities and companion choices with their parents. Given
dren, and especially adolescents, themselves are central this reconceptualization, prior research could be reinter-
agents in choosing to continue their regular participation preted to suggest that children with poorer social adjust-
in religious institutions. These findings are most easily ment discussed their activities with parents less than did
understood through the lens of the bilateral model that well-adjusted children. Paternal attempts to learn more
guides our chapter. Finally, Brody, Stoneman, and Flor about their childrens activities must be met with the
(1996) found that parental religiousness (frequency of childs own willingness to discuss such information
church attendance and importance of religion) was asso- (Mounts, 2000). Schell (1996), in a qualitative study of
ciated with better child adjustment as well. Specifically high-risk adolescents, found that youth often actively
higher maternal and paternal religiousness was associ- thwart their parents monitoring attempts. For example,
ated with less externalizing problems among 9- to they may mislead the parent concerning their destina-
12-year-olds. The effects were mediated by family cohe- tion or location, which makes it difficult for the parents
siveness and lower marital conflict. However, the rela- to accurately track the childs activities. As Kuczynski,
tive importance of beliefs or involvement in organized Marshall, and Schell (1997) noted: the proactive and
religious activities in accounting for these effects re- management techniques of parents occur in tandem with
mains unclear. proactive management techniques of their children
(p. 43). This reconceptualization is consistent with re-
Parental Monitoring. Another way in which par- cent findings with adolescents (Mounts, 2000). Parental
ents can affect their childrens social relationships is guidance (e.g., discussion of future consequences of
through monitoring of their childrens social activities. childrens relationships or giving advice) rather than
This form of management is particularly evident as chil- monitoring was consistently correlated with less delin-
dren move into adolescence and is associated with the quency, less drug use, and children having more friends
relative shift in importance of family and peers as with high grade point averages and positive attitudes to-
sources of social influence. Moreover, direct monitoring ward school. The role of parents in the regulation of ado-
is more common among younger children, whereas dis- lescents peer relationships in adolescence is not to
tal monitoring is more evident among adolescents. Mon- monitor or to keep track of their childrens activities;
itoring refers to a range of activities, including the parents role is to listen to children concerning their
supervision of childrens choice of social settings, activ- relationships, give advice on current dilemmas, and
ities, and friends. Parents of delinquent and antisocial discuss future consequences their children might not
children engage in less monitoring and supervision of think about otherwise. Such willingness may be related
their childrens activities, especially concerning chil- to temperamental characteristics of the adolescent.
drens use of evening time, than parents of nondelin- Parental knowledge of child behavior in middle child-
quent children (G. Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, hood has been linked to characteristics such as expres-
1984). Poorly monitored children have lower academic siveness and sociability (Crouter, Helms-Erikson,
skills and lower peer acceptance (Sandstrom & Coie, Updegraff, & McHale, 1999). In sum, the relations be-
1999), and they participate in more delinquent and ex- tween parental monitoring and adolescent peer activity
ternalizing behavior (Xiaoming, Stanton, & Feigelman, are more complicated than previously thought. This
2000). Nor are the effects of monitoring limited to a re- reconceptualization of monitoring as a shared process
duction in the negative aspects of peer relations. As between a parent and child is consistent with the theme
Krappmann (1986) found, preadolescents of parents of both parent and child as active agents in the manage-
who were well informed about their childrens peer rela- ment of childrens social lives.
tionships and activities had closer, more stable, and less
problem-ridden peer relationships. Recognition of the Interdependence among Com-
Although monitoring has been viewed as a parent to ponents of the Tripartite Socialization Model. Al-
child effect, Kerr and Stattin (2000) have reconceptual- though we have treated parental style and/or
ized this issue and argued that monitoring is a process parent-child interaction, advice giving, and parental
that is jointly co-constructed by the parent and childa management as separate influences, these components
448 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

often operate together to achieve their effects on chil- ferent parenting styles: with authoritative parents, high
drens socialization outcomes. A similar argument has levels of monitoring were associated with lower levels
been offered by Grusec and Goodnow (1994) who sug- of friends and low levels of drug use. For other parent-
gested that parental strategies vary in their effective- ing styles, there were no links between monitoring and
ness as a function of the quality of the parent-child friends or drug use. Perhaps adolescents view monitor-
relationship. As argued elsewhere, these three compo- ing differently (i.e., less intrusive) in the context of an
nents can be usefully viewed as a cafeteria model (Parke authoritative parenting stylea view consistent with
et al., 1994). monitoring as a dyadic construct. The link between pro-
Two issues need to be addressed. First, are there nat- hibiting and drug use varied as a function of parental
ural occurring combinations of these components? Sec- style as well; prohibiting was negatively related to drug
ond, do the different components moderate the relative use for the authoritarian and authoritative style groups
effectiveness of each component depending on the level but positively linked for the uninvolved parental style
of the other components? To illustrate, Mounts (2002) group. Because these effects were evident across 1 year,
examined the co-occurrence of different types of it suggests that a low level of parental involvement may
parenting management practicesprohibiting, guiding, have created a climate in which parents efforts to pro-
monitoring, supportingwith various parenting styles hibit drove adolescents toward higher levels of drug use
(authoritarian, authoritative, permissive, and unin- over time. This underscores the bidirectionality of in-
volved). She found that all parents, regardless of their fluence between parents and children in the socializa-
parenting style, use prohibiting and guiding as manage- tion process as well. In sum, these studies illustrate the
ment strategies. In contrast, monitoring and supporting interdependence among various components of our tri-
are more common in authoritative style homes relative partite model and suggest that a full understanding of
to the other parenting style environments. Several other socialization processes requires attention to the moder-
studies illustrate this interdependence across socializa- ating effects among the components.
tion components by demonstration of the joint contribu-
Co-Parenting as a Socialization Strategy
tions of parenting style and practices to child outcomes.
Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch, and Darling (1992) A recent trend is the focus on co-parenting in recogni-
found that when parents were authoritative, it reduced tion that mothers and fathers operate as a parenting
the effects of parental school involvement in determin- team and individual parents (J. McHale & Rasmussen,
ing school performance, whereas parental involvement 1998). This work has identified a variety of forms that
in school had a stronger effect in the presence of au- co-parenting alliances can assume, including a pattern
thoritative parenting style. Similarly, Mize and Pettit signifying antagonistic and adult centered or hostile
(1997) found that when mothers were low in parental competitive, co-parenting dynamics, a pattern marked
responsiveness (parental style) higher levels of con- by significant imbalance or parenting discrepancy in
structive coaching (parental practices) aimed at im- levels of parental engagement with the child and a pat-
proving peer relationships were linked to lower levels of tern reflecting cooperation, warmth, cohesion, and child
aggression than when mothers had low levels of respon- centeredness or high family harmony (J. McHale, Lau-
siveness and low levels of constructive coaching. In retti, Talbot, & Pouquette, 2002, p. 142). These patterns
contrast, when mothers had moderate or high levels of have been observed across studies with infants,
responsiveness, their level of coaching was unrelated to preschoolers, and school-age children, and in both Euro-
the level of childrens aggression. In this case, coaching pean and African American families (Brody, Flor, &
compensated for a less adequate parenting style. Fi- Neubaum, 1998; Fivaz-Depeursinge & Corboz-Warn-
nally, Mounts (2001) demonstrated a moderating role of ery, 1999). Recent work has moved beyond description
parental style on the effectiveness of parental manage- and revealed links between early co-parenting dynamics
ment of adolescent peer relationships. Parental style and later indices of social adaptation. J. McHale and
was indexed by authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, Rasmussen (1998) found that hostile-competitive co-
and uninvolved approaches, whereas parental manage- parenting during infancy was related to aggression,
ment was represented by guiding, prohibiting, monitor- whereas large parenting discrepancies were related to
ing, and supporting their adolescents friendships. parent-rated anxiety. Others have found links between
Monitoring operated differently in the context of dif- problematic family alliances in the 1st year and insecure
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 449

mother-child attachments and clinical symptomatology marital conflict (see Grych & Fincham, 2001, for a re-
in the preschool years (Fivaz-Depeursinge & Corboz- cent review). Although less empirical work has been
Warnery, 1999; J. McHale et al., 2002). directed specifically toward examination of the carry-
Co-parenting accounts for unique variance in child over of exposure to marital conflict to the quality of
measures and clearly needs to be distinguished from tra- childrens relationships with significant others (e.g.,
ditional parent-child and marital level processes (J. peers and siblings), exposure to marital discord is asso-
McHale et al., 2002). Less is known about the processes ciated with poor social competence and adjustment
that control these various patterns of co-parenting, but problems (Cummings & Davies, 1994; Davies, Harold,
recent work on gatekeeping (Allen & Hawkins, 1999; Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002).
Beitel & Parke, 1998) that focuses on ways in which
couples facilitate or hinder the involvement of their part- Mechanisms Linking Marital Discord to
ners interactions with their children is promising. The Childrens Adjustment
similarities and differences of the co-parenting relation- Three alternative, but not mutually exclusive, models
ship for intact and nonintact (divorced or single parent) have been proposed to account for the impact of marital
families are poorly understood (Emery, Kitzmann, & relations on childrens developmental outcomes. Until
Waldron, 1999). Extensions of theory and empirical recently, theoretical frameworks typically conceptual-
work to other family forms (foster parents and birth par- ized marital discord as an indirect influence on chil-
ents; Erera, 1997), parents and grandparents (Smith & drens adjustment that operated through its effect on
Drew, 2002), and lesbian-parenting partners or these family functioning and the quality of parenting (Fauber
partners and a donor father as co-parent (C. J. Patterson, & Long, 1991). Factors such as affective changes in the
2002) would help define the uniqueness of co-parenting quality of the parent-child relationship, lack of emo-
forms and process in various family types. tional availability, and adoption of less optimal parent-
ing styles have been implicated as potential mechanisms
Beyond the Parent-Child Dyad: The Marital
through which marital discord disrupts parenting
Subsystem as a Contributor to Childrens
processes. A second model (Cummings & Davies, 1994;
Socialization
Grych & Fincham, 2001) focuses on the direct ef fects of
In the preceding section, parents were conceptualized witnessed marital conflict on childrens outcomes
as active influences, both directly and indirectly, on the rather than on the indirect or mediated effects. Re-
development of childrens social competence and under- cently, Cummings, Goeke-Morey, and Raymond (2004)
standing of relationships. However, childrens experi- have proposed a third model: the interrelations among
ences in families extend beyond their interactions with marital quality, parents psychological functioning, and
parents. Childrens understanding of relationships is childrens outcomes. Labeled the parental mental
shaped also through their active participation in other health hypothesis, this alternative focuses on the role
family subsystems (e.g., child-sibling) and through ex- of parental psychological functioning in accounting for
posure to the interactions of other dyadic subsystems the effects of marital conflict either as a risk or protec-
(e.g., parent-parent) or participation in triadic relation- tive factor.
ships (e.g., child-sibling-parent, child-parent-parent).
Indirect Effects Model. A sizable body of litera-
Influence of Marital Satisfaction and Discord on
ture supports the view that these two family subsystems
Child Outcomes
are related. Erel and Burman (1995) completed a meta-
Considerable evidence indicates that marital function- analytic review of 68 studies that met a variety of crite-
ing is related to childrens short-term coping and long- ria, including independent assessment of marital and
term adjustment. Although the size of the associations parent-child relationships. Their review provided sup-
are not always large, a range of studies link marital dis- port for a positive relation between the quality of the
cord and conflict to outcomes in children that are likely marital relationship and the quality of the parent-child
to impair the quality of interpersonal relationships, in- relationship. As Erel and Burman (1995) concluded:
cluding: antisocial behavior; internalizing and external-
izing behavior problems; and changes in cognitions, The composite mean weighted effect size representing the
emotions, and physiology in response to exposure to association between marital and parent-child quality was
450 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

0.46 or approximately one standard deviation in the direc- conflict and parenting supported the fathering vulnera-
tion of more positive parent-child relationships in families bility hypothesis including relations with a variety of
with more positive relations and more negative parent- aspects of parenting, including control, acceptance,
child relationships in families with more negative marital harsh discipline, and overall quality of parenting. At the
relations. (p. 126) same time, many studies show parenting vulnerability
(Cummings et al., 2004) in which marital discord af-
Even when their meta-analysis was restricted to studies fects both mothering and fathering. However, when gen-
of high-quality research or to studies using independent der differences do occur, they favor the fathering
raters and a between-subjects design, the effect sizes vulnerability view, with less support for the mothering
were reduced but remained significant. Their review vulnerability hypothesis (Cummings et al., 2004).
leaves little doubt about the relation between marriage A number of factors may aid in explaining the greater
and parent-child relationships. vulnerability of fathers. First, fathers level of partici-
Theoretically, several models have been offered to pation is, in part, determined by the extent to which the
account for these effectsthe spillover hypothesis and mother permits participation (Beitel & Parke, 1998).
the compensatory hypothesis. According to the spillover Second, because the paternal role is less well articulated
perspective, mood or behavior in one subsystem trans- and defined than the maternal role, spousal support may
fers to another subsystem (e.g., from marital subsystem serve to help crystallize the boundaries of appropriate
to parent-child subsystem). In contrast, the compensa- role behavior (Parke, 2002). Third, men have fewer op-
tory hypothesis suggests that positive parent-child rela- portunities to acquire and practice skills that are central
tionships can be maintained even in the face of martial to caregiving activities during socialization and there-
conflict and can serve as a buffer on children (Erel & fore may benefit more than mothers from informational
Burman, 1995). The meta-analysis clearly supports the (i.e., cognitive) support (Parke & Brott, 1999).
spillover hypothesis and offers no support for the com- Even when research indicates that both mother-child
pensatory concept. Their analysis underscores the diffi- and father-child relationships are both associated with
culty of buffering children from marital conflict and marital relations, mothers and fathers may influence
discord. Parents may try to buffer their children by lim- their childrens outcomes in different ways. Gottman
iting their opportunities to witness marital conflicts and et al. (1997) found that when parents used a mutually
disputes; however, as Erel and Burman (1995) suggest, hostile pattern of conflict resolution, fathers were more
they cannot shield them from the negative impact that likely to be intrusive and children were more likely to
marital discord has on the parent-child relationships express anger during a parent-child interaction task. In
(p. 128). Unfortunately, Erel and Burmans conclusions addition, fathers intrusiveness predicted more negative
were largely restricted to Caucasian and intact families. peer play and more aggressive play with a best friend.
Several factors have been proposed as moderators of Interestingly, an individual parents style of handling
the relation between these two subsystems, including conflict may be related to the quality of his or her part-
gender of parent, gender of child, age of child, and birth ners relationships with children in the family. When
order (Cummings et al., 2004). The quality of the father- fathers were angry and withdrawn in a conflict resolu-
child relationship is more consistently associated with tion task, mothers were more critical and intrusive dur-
the quality of the marital relationship and/or with the ing interactions with their child. Maternal criticism and
amount and quality of marital support than is the intrusiveness, in turn, were associated with unrespon-
mother-child relationship (Parke, 2002). Cummings siveness or tuning out by the child during mother-
et al. (2004) labeled this view as the fathering vulnera- child interactions and higher levels of teacher-rated
bility hypothesis. This literature suggests that spousal internalizing symptoms (see also Cowan, Cowan,
support is more critical for adequate parenting on the Schulz, & Heming, 1994).
part of fathers than mothers. As marriages deteriorated, Family systems theory suggests that marital discord
men became more negative and intrusive fathers, not only interferes with the mother-child or father-child
whereas mothers were less affected by shifts in marital relationship but also impairs qualities of the mother-fa-
quality (Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling, 1991). ther-child triadic relationship by interfering with the ef-
Moreover, recent meta-analysis (Krishnakumar & fectiveness of how the mother and father work together
Buehler, 2000) of the relation between interparental with the child (Cox, Paley, & Harter, 2001). Westerman
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 451

and Schonholtz (1993) found that fathers, but not sponsibility, self blame, and fear of being drawn into the
mothers, reports of marital disharmony and disaffec- conflict (Grych, & Cardoza-Fernandez, 2001).
tion were significantly related to the effectiveness of Resolution of conflict, even when it was not viewed
joint parental support toward their childs problem-solv- by the child, reduces childrens negative reactions to ex-
ing efforts. Joint parental support was, in turn, related posure to interadult anger and conflict. Exposure to un-
to fathers and teachers reports of childrens behavior resolved conflict has been found to be associated with
problems. As Gottman (1994) has shown, women tend negative affect and poor coping responses in children
to engage and confront, whereas men tend to withdraw (Kerig, 1996). In addition, the manner in which conflict
in the face of marital disharmony. Mens lack of in- is resolved may also influence childrens adjustment.
volvement in the triadic family process may account for Katz and Gottman (1993) found that couples who exhib-
these findings. ited a hostile style of resolving conflict had children
Although much of the prior work has focused on the who tended to be described by teachers as exhibiting
transfer of negativity between marital and parent-child antisocial characteristics. When husbands were angry
subsystems, some evidence suggests that marital satis- and emotionally distant while resolving marital con-
faction is a predictor of positive parenting (Russell, flict, children were described by teachers as anxious
1997). For mothers, but not fathers, greater marital satis- and socially withdrawn.
faction was linked to higher levels of warmth, affection, Conflict is inevitable in most parental relationships
positive involvement, and overall positive parenting. and is not detrimental to family relationships and chil-
drens functioning under all circumstances. However,
Direct Effects of Marital Relationships on Chil- disagreements that are extremely intense and involve
drens Outcomes. Despite progress in elucidating threat to the child are likely to be more disturbing to the
specific parenting processes that are impaired by inter- child. In contrast, when conflict is expressed construc-
parental conflict, parental conflict is also associated tively, is moderate in degree, is expressed in the context
with behavior problems independent of its influence on of a warm and supportive family environment, and
the parent-child relationship. Accordingly, attention has shows evidence of resolution, children may learn valu-
turned to elucidating specific processes by which the able lessons regarding how to negotiate conflict and re-
marital relationship itself directly influences childrens solve disagreements (Cummings & Davies, 1994).
immediate functioning and long-term adjustment. A
parallel research trajectory has been a movement away Parental Mental Health and Marital Quality.
from a focus on global measures of marital satisfaction Support is beginning to emerge for the view that parental
to a focus on specific aspects of marital interaction that psychological functioning may help account for the ef-
are most likely to influence childrens immediate cogni- fects of marital conflict on childrens functions. Consis-
tive, emotional, and physiological functioning. These tent with a view of parental psychopathology as a risk
immediate responses, or microprocesses, have been factor for children, family discord mediated the effects
hypothesized to be critical links to childrens long-term of maternal depressive symptoms on adolescent girls so-
social adjustment when interparental conflict exists cial and emotional adjustment (Davies & Windle, 1997).
(Grych & Fincham, 2001). Recent lab analog studies In a later study, marital distress mediated the effects of
show that the form of expression of marital conflict maternal depression on male and female adolescent ex-
plays a critical role in how children react. More frequent ternalizing, whereas maternal depression mediated the
interparental conflict and more intense or violent forms impact of marital distress on adolescent depressive
of conflict have been found to be particularly disturbing symptoms (Davies, Dumenci, & Windle, 1999). Other
to children and likely to be associated with externaliz- work provides support for the links among poor parental
ing and internalizing difficulties (Cummings et al., functioning (parental dysphoria), marital conflict, and
2004). McDonald, Jouriles, Norwood, Ware, and Ezell childrens adjustment (DuRocher Schudlich & Cum-
(2000) found that fathers interparental aggression was mings, 2003). In this case, depressive conflict style me-
related to childrens internalizing problems. Conflict diated the relations between parental dysphoria and
that was child related in content was more likely than internalizing problems of 8- to 16-year-old children.
conflict involving other content to be associated with be- Consistent with a protective perspective when fathers
havior problems in children, such as greater shame, re- are supportive of their partners during marital conflict,
452 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

the impact on the children is reduced (Huffman & Cum- berg & Langrock, 2001) that children would display
mings, 2002). Although this mental health perspective is anger in response to conflict and that they would imitate
gaining some support, a wider range of mental health same-sexed parents was not supported. However, the
problems needs to be examined to determine their links value of distinguishing among specific types of emo-
with both marital conflict and childrens adjustment. tions, such as fear versus anger, rather than on undiffer-
Moreover, the relative importance of this perspective for entiated negative affect was clearly supporteda
the direct versus indirect models of influence is not yet position stressed by Crockenbergs specific emotions
established. perspective on this issue (Crockenberg & Langrock,
2001). In a follow-up study of this sample, Davies et al.
Recent Progress in Methodology and Theory. A found further support for the emotional security posi-
noteworthy methodological advance is the evaluation of tion. Child emotional security (assessed by emotional,
the impact of marital conflict not just through the use reactivity, behavioral regulation, and internal represen-
of lab analog or questionnaire approaches but the use of tations) mediated the links between interparental con-
parental diary reports of naturally occurring incidents flict and internalizing and externalizing symptoms
of marital conflict in the home. In support of earlier lab across a 2-year period. Although the models provided
analog studies (Cummings & Davies, 1994), these stud- stronger support for the emotional security predictions
ies suggested that everyday marital conflict (Goeke- than cognitive appraisal processes, both sets of
Morey, Cummings, Harold, & Shelton, 2003) and marital processes received some support. In sum, more work is
physical aggression (OHearn, Margolin, & John, 1997) needed before definitive conclusions concerning the
were associated with negative emotional and behavioral theoretical power of these competing, even if overlap-
reactions. In contrast, parental support and affection ping, theoretical perspectives can be drawn. Moreover,
were linked with childrens positive reactions (Goeke- as others (Crockenberg & Langrock, 2001) have argued
Morey et al., 2003). These studies increase the ecologi- it is critical to examine links among marital conflict,
cal validity of the prior findings and the generalizability child adjustment, and specific goals (versus a general
of the prior work to naturalistic family contexts. emotional security goal) that are both directly relevant
Several competing theoretical frameworks have to emotional security (e.g., worries about family disso-
emerged that focus on cognitive-processing or lution or fears about being involved in the conflict) and
emotional-regulatory mechanisms. Using a cognitive nonsecurity-related concerns (e.g., being a partner in
contextual model, Grych and Fincham (Grych & family decision making). It is critical to integrate the re-
Cardoza-Fernandes, 2001; Grych & Fincham, 1990) cent work on emotional regulation and coping (Eisen-
have focused on the cognitive and affective meaning berg, 2000; Kerig, 2001) more centrally into these
that exposure to conflict has for the child. Davies and theoretical debates (e.g., Crockenberg & Langrock,
Cummings (1994) have offered an emotional security 2001). Finally, prior work has been largely adevelop-
hypothesis that suggests that marital conflict nega- mental and less is known about the impact of exposure to
tively affects childrens emotional regulatory abili- marital conflict on adolescents, especially the effects
ties, influences childrens motivation to regulate their on their own emerging friendships and close same-sex
parents behaviors, and alters their cognitive represen- and opposite-sex relationships.
tations of family relationships. A third position
(Crockenberg & Langrock, 2001) focuses on the social
The Sibling System as a Contributor to
learning role of modeling and the role of specific emo-
Childrens Socialization
tions, such as fear and anger, in accounting for the ef-
fects of marital conflict on childrens adjustment. Descriptions of the normative patterns that characterize
In a recent test of competing theoretical positions sibling relationships over the course of development sug-
(i.e., social learning, cognitive appraisal, and emotional gest that, in addition to parents, siblings play a critical
security), Davies et al. (2002) found that children re- role in the socialization of children. Most children are
sponded to interparental conflict with fear and regula- likely to spend more time in direct interaction with sib-
tion efforts (avoidance or intervention)reactions that lings than parents and significant others (Dunn, 1993;
are consistent with emotional security theory. In con- Larson & Richards, 1994) and that interactions with
trast, the social learning theory prediction (Crocken- siblings provide a context for the expression of a range of
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 453

positive social behaviors as well as numerous conflic- skills may generalize to relationships with other chil-
tual encounters and experiences with conflict resolution dren. According to Stocker and Dunn (1990), interac-
(Dunn, 1993). Further, this array of interactions be- tions with siblings provide a setting in which children
tween siblings has been found to be typified by greater develop social understanding skills which may enable
emotional intensity than the behavioral exchanges that them to form particularly close relationships with a
characterize other relationships. Developmental shifts child of their choice, a close friend.
in sibling relationships suggest that perceptions of Studies show only modest evidence of straightfor-
warmth and conflict between siblings declined over ward carryover of interaction styles between chil-
time, between late middle childhood and early adoles- drens relationships, and when associations emerge they
cence (Slomkowski & Manke, 2004). A decline in inti- may be complicated by birth order effects and other
macy in sibling relationships is evident from preschool processes (Dunn, 1993, 2004; Stocker & Dunn, 1990).
to early adolescence (Dunn, Slomkowski, Beardsall, & Adding another complexity to the picture of how sibling
Rende, 1994). However, gender influences these devel- and peer relationships are linked are findings from stud-
opmental patterns: sisters reported more warmth and ies, which suggest that sibling relationships may play a
self-disclosure over this period (Dunn et al., 1994; role in compensating for other problematic relationships
Slomkowski & Manke, 2004). Despite these shifts, wide by providing an alternative context for experiencing sat-
individual differences in the quality of sibling relation- isfying social relationships and protecting children from
ships and recent evidence suggests change in quality of adjustment difficulties. East and Rook (1992), for exam-
sibling relationships continues during adolescence ple, found that children who were socially isolated in
(Conger, Bryant, & Brennom, 2004). their peer relationships were buffered from adjustment
Sibling relationships have been hypothesized to con- problems when they reported positive relationships with
tribute to childrens socialization in a number of signif- a favorite sibling. Similarly, Stocker (1994) reported
icant ways. A social-learning framework analogous to support for the compensatory role of at least one posi-
the one posited to explain parental contributions to the tive relationship (sibling, friend, or mother) as protec-
development of childrens social competence (Parke & tion from the development of behavioral conduct
ONeil, 1999) predicts that through their interactions difficulties.
with siblings children develop specific interaction pat- The prosocial or deviant interest and activities of
terns and social-understanding skills that generalize to siblings are important determinants of the positive or
relationships with other children. Relationships with negative influence of siblings on one another. Adoles-
siblings also may provide a context in which children can cents who have both close and satisfying relationships
practice the skills and interaction styles that have been and share common deviant interests with their older
learned from parents or others. Older siblings function brothers may be at increased risk for antisocial delin-
as tutors, managers, or supervisors of their younger quent behaviors, higher rates of problem behaviors, and
brothers or sisters behavior during social interactions problematic interaction patterns with romantic partners
(Edwards & Whiting, 1993) and may function as gate- (Slomkowski, Rende, Conger, Simmons, & Conger,
keepers who extend or limit opportunities to interact 2001). Just as for peer-peer relationships, both the type
with other children outside of the family (Zukow- of behavior that they engage in and the quality of the
Goldring, 2002). Also paralleling the indirect influence ties between siblings need to be understood before the
that the observation of parent-parent interaction has on positive or negative impact of sibling relationships can
children, a second avenue of influence on childrens de- be predicted.
velopment is their observation of parents interacting In view of our focus on bidirectionality of influence,
with siblings. These interactions have been hypothe- it is important to consider the impact of friendships on
sized to serve as an important context in which children sibling relationships. Kramer and Gottman (1992) exam-
deal with issues of differential treatment and learn about ined the role that positive relationships with peers play
complex social emotions such as rivalry and jealousy. in childrens adaptation to the birth of a new sibling.
Children who displayed a more positive interaction style
Influence of Siblings on Child Outcomes with a best friend and who were better able to manage
Childrens experiences with siblings provide a context in conflict and negative affect, behaved more positively to-
which interaction patterns and social understanding ward their new sibling at both 6 months and 14 months.
454 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

They suggest that management of conflict, a valuable lationships. Further, there appear to be different role ex-
skill when interacting with siblings, may be more likely pectations for sibling versus friend relationships that
to be learned in interactions with peers than in direct in- may differentially influence interaction styles. There is
teractions with parents. Recently, Kramer (2004) has a need to systematically examine the moderating and
developed a social skill training program aimed at im- mediating influences of these factors to uncover norma-
proving childrens relationships with their siblings. In tive patterns of associations between sibling and peer
comparison to a control group of 4- to 6-year-olds, chil- relationships.
dren with a younger sibling who received social skills
training showed more positive and less negative sibling Siblings as Managers of Childrens Social Lives
relationships and more perspective taking. Although the Just as parents function as managers of childrens so-
processes that underlie the success of this program are cial lives, siblings in many cultures perform similar
not yet specified, early evidence suggests that emotional management functions in relation to their younger sib-
regulatory and attentional factors that are important lings. Cross-cultural work indicates that in African,
correlates of peer competence (Eisenberg, 2000; Parke Polynesian, and Mexican cultures children, especially
et al., 2006) may play a role in harmonious sibling rela- girls, become involved in sibling caretaking and teach-
tionships as well. Brody, Stoneman, Smith, and Gibson ing activities at a relatively early age (Weisner, 1993).
(1999) found that 9- to 12-year-old African American Maynard (2002) found that by 4 years of age children
children who were higher in self-regulation (e.g., ability took responsibility for initiating teaching situations
to set and attain goals, plan, and persist) experienced (e.g., weaving) with their toddler siblings and that by 8
more harmonious and less conflictual sibling relation- years of age children become highly skilled teachers of
ships. Similarly, Volling, McElwain, and Miller (2002) culturally relevant skills. Relatively little is known,
found that preschool-age older siblings who were higher however, about the caregiving role of siblings in con-
in emotional understanding were less likely to show be- temporary European American families. Patterns of
havioral dysregulation and negative emotions in a social sibling interaction in New England families suggest
triangle paradigm in which the mother directed her at- that formal caregiving responsibilities may not be as
tention to a younger sibling. Moreover, higher behavioral common in American culture as in other cultures (Ed-
regulation was associated with more positive sibling- wards & Whiting, 1993). However, Bryant (1989) sug-
sibling relationships. gests that although parents may not formally assign
The challenge is to discover the contexts under which caretaking duties to children, children frequently vol-
strong, weak, or compensatory connections may be ex- untarily assume the roles of caretaker, tutor, and
pected between relationship systems and the processes teacher of younger siblings and make unique contribu-
through which childrens experiences with siblings are tions to the socialization of young children. Most work
translated into relationship skills that are used in other examining these roles has focused on the influence that
relationships. For example, greater generalization of instruction from older siblings may have on childrens
hostile, aggressive interaction styles in both sibling and cognitive development (Rogoff, 1990, 2003). Rela-
peer systems may emerge when children lack adequate tively little is known about the role that siblings play as
relationship skills or when children are experiencing supervisors, managers, or advisors of childrens social
stressful, negative family relationships (Dunn, 1993, lives. Given the amount of time that most children
2004). In contrast, under other circumstances, the asso- spend in the company of siblings, this is an area that is
ciation between sibling relationships and relationships ripe for future investigation.
outside the family may be moderated by a number of
Future Directions for Sibling Research
features that uniquely characterize each relationship.
As Dunn (1993) has argued, friendship involves a mu- More fruitful investigation of the links between rela-
tual and reciprocated relationship with another individ- tionships may come with movement from a socializa-
ual, whereas siblings do not necessarily feel this way tion framework to a relationships framework. Dunn
about one another. In contrast to sibling-sibling relation- (1993) notes that one disadvantage of a socialization
ships, friend and peer relationships represent a more approach is that it does not adequately take into account
unique combination of backgrounds, experiences, and that even when a child acquires social competencies
temperaments that may generate interaction styles that through interactions in one relationship, he or she may
are the result of two unique individuals approach to re- not be motivated to apply these skills in another rela-
Family Systems Approach to Socialization 455

tionship. In contrast, a relationships perspective takes 1994). Wamboldt and Reiss (1989) argue that family
into account that each relationship reflects a unique set myths influence mate selection and marital satisfaction.
of demands and rewards as well as different challenges Individuals can set aside destructive family myths by
to a childs sociocognitive abilities. This may lead to marrying a person with a different and perhaps healthier
the generation of questions concerning the unique as- history of family myths. To date, there is little direct
pects of child (e.g., temperament, attachment security, evidence of the impact of family myths on childrens
or self confidence), the relationship partner, the dy- development.
namic of the relationship itself, and the broader social
ecology (e.g., family stress or life transitions), which Family stories: Family stories have received more atten-
may contribute to a child being motivated or disinclined tion as vehicles for socialization of young children
to behave in a socially competent manner. As Dunn (Pratt & Fiese, 2004). Stories are vehicles for the trans-
(1993) points out, the goal is to specify for which chil- mission of family values and for teaching family roles.
dren, at which stages of development, which dimensions The study of stories as socialization vehicles has taken
of particular relationships are likely to show associa- a variety of formsin part, depending on the dis-
tions with other relationships (p. 125). A final value of ciplinary perspective of the investigator. Culturally-
the renewed focus on siblings is the contribution that oriented investigators (e.g., Miller & Sperry, 1987)
this work is making to our understanding of the relative have established that stories occur in naturalistic con-
roles of genetics and environment in studies of social- texts in exchange between parents and children or while
ization and development (Dunn, 1993; Plomin, 1994). children are present. In home observations of African
American toddlers in south Baltimore, they found that
mothers told informal narratives in the presence of
The Family Unit as a Contributor their children about events in which someone became
to Childrens Socialization angry or responded with verbal or nonverbal aggres-
sion. Through these stories, children learn to distin-
Parent-child, marital, and sibling influences are clearly guish between justified and unjustified anger.
the most well-researched aspects of socialization. How- Family of origin experiences may be transmitted
ever, consideration of these units of analysis alone is in- across generations through stories and shared memories
sufficient because they fail to recognize the family unit and shape contemporary interaction between family
itself as a separate and identifiable level of analysis members. Fiese et al. (1999) provided a useful frame-
(Minuchin, 2002; Parke, 1988). Consistent with a sys- work for studying family stories by focusing on three
tems theory perspective (Sameroff, 1994), the proper- narrative dimensions: (1) narrative coherence, (2) narra-
ties, functions, and effects of the family unit cannot tive styles, and (3) relationship beliefs that characterize
necessarily be inferred from these smaller units of the form that the content of family stories assumes. This
analysis. Families as units change across development in report provides evidence of the value of this approach
response to changes in the individual members, life cir- for understanding premarital couples (Wamboldt, 1999),
cumstances, and scheduled and unscheduled transitions. family dinner interactions (Fiese & Marjinsky, 1999),
Families develop distinct climates (Moos & Moos, couples with an adopted child (Grotevant, Fravel,
1981), styles of responding to events (D. Reiss, 1989) Gorall, & Piper, 1999), and depressed couples (Dick-
and distinct boundaries (Boss, 1999), which provide stein, St. Andre, Sameroff, Seifer, & Schiller, 1999).
differing socialization contexts for the developing child. This approach yielded important insights into child
Several investigators (Fiese et al., 2002; D. Reiss, 1989) functioning attitudes toward open versus closed adop-
have argued that the family regulates the childs devel- tion, marital satisfaction, and diagnosis of depression
opment through a range of processes, including myths, (Fiese et al., 1999). Stories are related to family inter-
stories, and rituals. Recent evidence suggests the poten- action patterns and are linked to childrens social com-
tial importance of these family level processes for un- petence as well. Putallaz, Costanzo, and Smith (1991)
derstanding socialization in the family. found that mothers with predominantly anxious/ lonely
recollections of their own childhood experiences with
Family myths: Myths refer to beliefs that influence fam- peers took an active role in their childrens social devel-
ily process, provide continuity across generations, and opment and had the most socially competent children.
are generally not open to discussion or debate (Sameroff, However, these mothers may be compensating for their
456 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

own difficult childhoods. Although mothers with posi- Wolin, Bennett, and Jacobs (1988) have identified three
tive memories also had socially competent children, types of family rituals: (1) family celebrations (e.g., hol-
mothers with memories of peer rejection had the least idays like Christmas), (2) rites of passage (e.g., wed-
socially competent children. Later work (Putallaz, dings), (3) family traditions (e.g., birthday customs
Klein, Costanzo, & Hedges, 1994) involving parent nar- or family vacations). Failure to attend an important fam-
rations of videotaped interaction of their child with a ily event, such as a wedding, often indicates a shift in
peer confirmed that mothers and fathers interpretative family alliances and definitions of who is in or out of the
narrations were shaped, in part, by their recollections of family (Fiese, 2006a, 2006b). Rituals serve a protective
their own childhood peer experiences. Similarly, Prin- function as well (Fiese et al., 2002), and Wolin, Bennett,
stein and La Greca (1999) found that mothers of well- and Jacobs (1988) found that children who came from
liked kindergarteners had positive childhood peer families that were able to preserve family rituals, such as
memories, whereas mothers with anxious peer-related holiday routines, were less likely to become alcoholic as
memories had less well-liked children. More work is adults. Other studies (Fiese et al., 2002) report that fam-
needed to specify when childhood social adversity leads ilies who attach more meaning to their rituals have ado-
to compensatory reactions and when it leads to intergen- lescents who are higher in self-esteem. In sum, rituals
erational continuity of social difficulties (see Grimes, and routines are powerful indices of family functioning
Klein, & Putallaz, 2004, for a recent review). and may serve as protective factors for the child.
Questions remain about the uniqueness of rituals rel-
Family rituals and routines: Rituals have been recog- ative to other forms of family patterns or child-rearing
nized for decades as an important aspect of family life practices. Are rituals independent vehicles of socializa-
(Bossard & Boll, 1950), but only in the past decade has tion or merely a reflection of more central causal influ-
the socialization function of rituals and routines become ences such as the quality of the parent-child relationship
apparent (Fiese, 2006a, 2006b; Fiese et al., 2002). Fiese (Fiese et al., 2002)? For example, rituals may be less
et al. (2002) argue that routines and rituals can be con- likely in families of alcoholics, which suggest that the
trasted along the dimensions of communication, com- degree of alcoholism, either alone or in combination
mitment, and continuity: with the lack of rituals, may contribute to future drink-
ing problems. Evidence is needed concerning the contri-
Routines typically involve instrumental communication in bution of family level variables independently of
conveying information that is what needs to be done.
individual or dyadic levels of analysis. Similarly, the di-
The language of routines is direct, implies action, and
rection of causality in these studies remains unclear. Do
often includes designation of roles. Routines involve a mo-
mentary time commitment and once the act is completed
harmonious families participate more in family rituals,
there is little afterthought. Routines are repeated over or does active participation contribute to increased fam-
time, with little alteration, and can be directly observable ily well-being? Although the answers to these questions
by outsiders. Rituals, on the other hand, involve symbolic are unclear, it is evident that we need to expand our
communication and covey this is who we are as a group. repertoire of avenues through which socialization is en-
There is an affective commitment that leaves the individ- acted in families. Finally, the origin of family level dif-
ual feeling that the activity has a rightness and provides a ferences is an issue that has received little attention.
sense of belonging. When rituals are disrupted there is a Given demonstrations (e.g., Plomin, 1994) that genetics
threat to group cohesion. (p. 382) may play a role in variations in measures of family home
environments, answers are most likely to derive from
Family routines are associated with better child designs that recognize the contributions of both genetic
health and better behavioral regulation in intact families and environment factors in the emergence of family
(Fiese et al., 2002). For example, families who observe level differences.
medication routines are more likely to report higher ad-
herence to medical regimens concerning the management Putting the Pieces Together: Toward a Multiple
of asthmatic children and, in turn, the children have less Sources Model of Socialization in the Family
asthma-related illness (Fiese, 2006a). Similarly, routines
serve a protective function and are linked to better ad- Our family systems viewpoint argues for the construc-
justment for both parents and children in single parent, tion of a comprehensive model in which the contribution
divorced, and remarried households (Fiese et al., 2002). of parent-child, parent-parent, and sibling relationships
Determinants of Family Socialization Strategies 457

are all recognized. Figure 8.3 outlines a comprehensive tal dyads (Cook, 2001). As a next step, can we character-
model of family socialization that includes the influence ize families usefully by the relative importance of vari-
of all family members. To date, few studies have simul- ous subsystems? Some families may invest heavily in
taneously addressed how these subsystems combine to directly parenting their children but tend to protect their
produce their impact on childrens relationship learning. children from their marital problems. Earlier evidence
Little is known about the relative weighting of parent- suggests that exposure to marital conflict is higher for
child relationships versus other family relationships boys than girls (Hetherington & Kelly, 2001). Similarly,
(Parke & ONeil, 1999). Nor do we understand how the some families may encourage close sibling-sibling rela-
impact of these different relationships changes as the tionships, whereas others tend to encourage sibs to form
child develops. The most crucial issue remains the spec- separate social spheres. This kind of social arrangement
ification of the pathways through which these different will result in different types of socialization outcomes.
relationships exert their influence. In our model, multi- Do all combinations produce equally socially compe-
ple pathways are possible and there is support for both tent children, or are some ingredients in this mix more im-
direct and mediated effects. As noted earlier, marital portant than others? Do different combinations produce
relationships exert both direct (e.g., witnessed effects) different, but equally well-adjusted, children in their so-
and indirect effects (e.g., marital relationships influ- cial relationships? Can children in a family with a poor
ence parent-child patterns). Similarly, parent-child rela- marriage compensate by investing relationship energy
tionships could influence marital relationships. For into another subsystem such as the sibling-sibling or par-
example, a disciplinary encounter with a difficult-to- ent-child system? Studies of divorce (Hetherington &
control child could begin a marital conflict due to dis- Kelly, 2001) suggest that a close sibling-sibling relation-
agreement about the childs misbehavior or management ship can help buffer children during a stressful divorce.
of the child, the carryover of negative mood, or the
alignment of parent and child against a third party. Less
is known about the impact of parent-child relationships DETERMINANTS OF FAMILY
on marital interactions than the reverse. SOCIALIZATION STRATEGIES
Moreover, recent research has begun to identify indi-
vidual differences across families or family typologies One of the major advances in the field has been recogni-
as well as at the level of family subsystems such as mari- tion of the importance of understanding the determinants

ParentChild Martial/
Subsystem Co-Parent
Subsystem ParentParent
Interaction Subsystem
Direct Instruction
Provision of Opportunities

Childrens Socialization
Outcomes

SiblingChild
and Family Unit
SiblingParent System
Subsystems

Figure 8.3 Model indicating the hypothesized relations among family subsystems and childrens socialization outcomes.
458 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

of parenting behavior (Belsky, 1984). For a long time, de- et al., 2002), temperament plays an important role as a
velopmentalists were concerned about the impact of par- determinant of parental socialization tactics. Infants
ticular styles or practices on children. There was less with difficult temperaments elicit more arousal and dis-
concern with the conditions that alter parenting behav- tress from caregivers than less difficult infants (Putnam
iors themselves. Several trends have converged to stimu- et al., 2002). Children who are more difficult may elicit
late this interest: First, the recognition of child effects increasingly coercive strategies from parents (Reid
(Bell, 1968) led to a reevaluation of the role of the child et al., 2002). Alternatively, fearful children may respond
in eliciting and shaping parental behaviors. Second, a re- optimally to subtle parental socialization strategies
newed interest in contextual-ecological issues (Bronfen- (Kochanska, 1997). Other characteristics, in addition to
brenner, 1989) played a role. Third, closely linked is the temperament, have been examined, including activity
increased focus on how cultural, racial, and ethnic back- level, social responsiveness, and compliance level. In
ground modify parenting behavior (Garcia Coll & Mag- general, the more active, less responsive, and more non-
nuson, 1999). Fourth, interest in the life-course compliant children elicit more negative parenting and
perspective (Elder, 1998) has led to a focus on how tim- more negative parental arousal and affect (Crouter &
ing of entry into parenting roles alters the enactment of Booth, 2003). The impact of these individual differences
parental behaviors. Fifth, recognition of the role of on parental socialization behavior is not independent of
parental cognitions (e.g., goals, values, and attributions) environmental conditions. Crockenberg and Leerkes
has fueled interest in how cognition shapes parental be- (2003) showed that the impact of a difficult infant tem-
havior (Dix & Branca, 2003). perament on the parent-infant attachment relationship
In this section, a variety of factors are considered. varied as a function of the degree of social support avail-
Belsky (1984) proposed a three-domain model of the de- able to the mother, which underscores the potential mod-
terminants of parenting, which included the personal re- ifiability of temperament-based influences.
sources of the parents, the characteristics of the child,
and the contextual sources of stress and support. Some
Personal Resources
of the work relevant to this model is reviewed and recent
work on ethnic variations in parenting is reviewed to ex- Several studies support the prediction that personal re-
pand on this earlier theoretical scheme. sourcesconceptualized as knowledge, ability, and mo-
tivation to be a responsible caregiveralter parenting
Child Characteristics behaviors (Belsky, 1984). Particularly striking are re-
cent studies of how parental psychopathology, such as
Child characteristics take two forms: (1) universal pre- depression, will alter parenting behavior (Goodman &
dispositions that are shared by all children and (2) indi- Gotlib, 2002). From early infancy onward, the patterns
vidual differences in particular characteristics. An of interaction between depressed and nondepressed par-
impressive amount of evidence has documented that in- ents (usually mothers) and their offspring are less posi-
fants are biologically prepared for social, cognitive, and tive, less stimulating, and less contingent. In turn, their
perceptual challenges and these prepared responses play infants showed less attentiveness, fewer contented ex-
a significant role in facilitating childrens adaptation to pressions, more fussiness, and lower activity levels
their environment. This evolutionary approach has con- (Field, 1992). Differences are particularly evident when
tinued to receive support (Geary & Bjorkland, 2000). depression is protracted and not merely transient
Under the influence of recent advances in behavior ge- (Campbell, Cohn, & Meyers, 1995).
netics (e.g., Plomin, 1994), there is increasing recogni- These differences in interaction may place the in-
tion of the role of individual differences in a wide fant at risk for later developmental problems. Infants of
variety of behavioral characteristics in shaping parental depressed mothers are more likely to develop insecure
socialization strategies. Perhaps the most well- attachments (Goodman & Gotlib, 2002). Recent inves-
researched determinant of parenting behavior is child tigations have found links between severe and chronic
temperament (Putnam, Sanson, & Rothbart, 2002). Al- depression and disorganized attachment behavior
though debates about the relative contributions of ge- (Lyons-Ruth, Lyubchik, Wolfe, & Bronfman, 2002).
netic and experimental factors to the emergence of This attachment category refers to infants who lack a
individual differences in temperament continue (Reid coherent strategy for accessing their attachment fig-
Determinants of Family Socialization Strategies 459

ures and who show confused, conflictful, and fearful Parents own social networks of other adults, as well as
behavior in the Strange Situation (see Thompson, Chap- the child members of parental social networks, provide a
ter 2, this Handbook, this volume). Nor are the effects on source of possible play partners for children. Cochran
child-parent attachment restricted to infancy. Teti, and Niego (1995) suggested several ways in which these
Gelfand, Messinger, and Isabella (1995) found a similar two sets of relationships may be related. First, the child
pattern of insecure attachment among preschool-age is exposed to a wider or narrower band of possible social
children of depressed mothers. Although no follow-up interaction partners by exposure to the members of a
studies have revealed the long-term outcomes for these parents social network. Second, the extent to which the
children, other studies of children with poor attachment child has access to the social interactions of his or her
histories reveal that these children are at risk for later parents and members of their social network may deter-
relationship difficulties in adolescence (Carlson et al., mine how well the child acquires a particular style of so-
2004). Others personal problems (e.g., antisocial per- cial interaction. Third, in view of the social support
sonality disorder or schizophrenia; limited education function of social networks, parents in supportive social
and poverty) contribute to poorer parenting (Cummings networks may be more likely to have positive relation-
et al., 2004). At the same time, positive personal charac- ships with their children, which may positively affect
teristics (e.g., high intelligence and self-regulation) and the childs social adjustment both within and outside the
a transpersonal orientation (i.e., a focus on family, family. Cochran and Niego (1995) reported that there is
work, and child rearing) are linked with better quality overlap between parent and child social networks; thirty
parenting (Pulkkinen, Nurmi, & Kokko, 2002). Just as percent to 40% of 6-year-olds social networks were also
with individual differences in infants and children, re- included in the mothers networks. Children often listed
cent theorists have argued that some of these individual other children as play partners who were children of
differences across parents, such as depression and their mothers friends. Finally, the overlap was higher
proneness to abuse or coerciveness, may, in part, be ge- for relatives than nonrelatives, but both kin and nonkin
netically based (Caspi et al., 2002). Studies addressing adult networks provided sources of peer play partners
the interplay among genetically based individual differ- for young children.
ences among infants and parents and environmental fac- Community networking has implications for youth
tors that enhance or suppress the influence of these development. Adolescent boys were found to have better
characteristics would be valuable. school performance and attendance and more positive
social behavior when their social networks included
Parents, Children, and Social Capital large numbers of nonrelated adults (Cochran & Bo,
The concept of social capital considers the relations 1989). In a study by Fletcher, Darling, Steinberg, and
among people, institutions, and organizations of the Dornbusch (1995), when nonrelated adults such as ado-
community outside the immediate family structure. As lescents friends parents were perceived as authorita-
described by Coleman (1988), social capital is both the tive in their parenting style, adolescents were lower in
flow of information and the sharing of norms and values delinquency and substance abuse, especially when they
that serve to facilitate or constrain the actions of people percieve their own parents to be authoritative. Another
who interact in the communitys social structures (e.g., way these two networks may be linked was proposed by
schools, places of worship, or business enterprises). Coleman (1988), who argued that when both parents
Children benefit from the presence of norm and value and their children are acquainted with other parents and
consensus among members of their family and the wider their children, they form network closure. When net-
community (Coleman, 1988). Monitoring of children is work closure exists, more shared values and more so-
facilitated, as is their socialization, through multiple ef- cial control over their offspring are likely, which would
forts of network members who hold shared family com- be related to better social outcomes. Darling, Steinberg,
munity norms and values (Elder & Conger, 2000). Gringlas, and Dornbusch (1995) found that social inte-
Moreover, if a childs own family is negligent in fulfill- gration (as indexed by network closure) and value con-
ing the socialization role, other adults are available to sensus were related to adolescent social and academic
assume the responsibility. outcomes. Adolescents who reported high degrees of
One important aspect of social capital is the network contact among their parents, their own friends, and
of social relationships in which families are embedded. their friends parents were less deviant and higher in
460 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

academic achievement than their peers who were less tive players in the distribution of social capital. More at-
socially integrated. tention needs to be given to childrens role as active
The quality of adult social networks is related to chil- agents in this process.
drens social behavior. In an Australian study, Homel,
Burns, and Goodnow (1987) found positive relations be- Socioeconomic Status as a Determinant of
tween the number of dependable friends that parents Family Socialization Strategies
report and 11-year-old childrens self-rated happiness,
the number of regular playmates and maternal ratings of There is a long history of research concerning the links
childrens social skills. Recently, Simpkins, ONeil, between socioeconomic status (SES) and/or social class
Lee, and Parke (2005) found that the more parents en- and parenting beliefs and practices. Although the debate
joyed their own friends, the less the child was disliked concerning the best strategy for measuring SES contin-
and perceived as aggressive by peers. The more contact ues (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003; Entwisle & Astone,
the parents had with relatives, the less disliked children 1994), most scholars agree that SES is multiply deter-
were by their peers. mined, and therefore the links with parenting are likely
Moreover, the quality of the relationship that adults to be multiple as well. Second, in contrast to traditional
develop with friends in their social network is an impor- assumptions that SES is a static state, most (e.g., Feath-
tant correlate of their childrens friendship quality. erman, Spenner, & Tsunematsu, 1988; Hoff et al., 2002)
Doyle and Markiewicz (1996) found that mothers who argue that SES is a dynamic concept. Over the course of
perceived their own best friends as providing more stim- childhood and adolescence, families change social class
ulating ideas and activities but also felt less secure in and change is greatest in the youngest ages. Over 50% of
their friendships had children who experienced more American children change social class prior to entering
closeness with their best friend. If mothers felt less se- school (Featherman et al., 1988).
cure about their best friendship, their own children were Despite the controversies surrounding the interpreta-
more likely to have a best friend. The findings concern- tion of this variable, there are SES differences in
ing the links between lack of mothers security about parental socialization practice and beliefs (Hoff et al.,
their friendships is consistent with work on maternal 2002). First, SES differences in parenting style have
recollections of their childhood peer experiences (Putal- been found. Lower-SES parents are more authoritarian
laz et al., 1991), which supports a compensatory model and more punitive than higher-SES families (Kelley,
of parenting. More recently, Simpkins and Parke (2001) Sanchez-Hucies, & Walker, 1993; Straus & Stewart,
found that the quality of both maternal and paternal 1999). Second, interaction styles differ across SES lev-
concurrent friendships was related to childrens friend- els. Lower-SES mothers are more controlling, restric-
ship quality. As these studies illustrate, the quality and tive, and disapproving than higher-SES mothers (Hart &
scope of adult friendship and social networks are impor- Risley, 1995). Additionally, there are more SES differ-
tant correlates not only of childrens peer competence ences on language measures than on nonverbal measure
but also of their friendship qualities. with higher-SES mothers being more verbal than lower-
In sum, the social capital in a community can aid par- SES mothers (Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff et al., 2002).
ents socialization of their children through several Higher SES mothers not only talk more, but provide
pathways. First, when parents and children have commu- object labels, sustain conversational topics longer, re-
nity ties, more social support is available. Second, spond more contingently to their childrens speech, and
parental awareness of community services and their par- elicit more talk from their children than lower SES
ticipation in shaping the institutions of the community mothers (Hoff-Ginsberg & Tardif, 1995, p. 177). Some
promote the maintenance of values and norms that influ- SES differences are independent of race and poverty. In
ence their children. Third, parental participation with China, where there are relatively small differences in
their children enables closer supervision of children and income across groups who vary in terms of education,
reduces the time children spend with their own peers. Tardif (1993) found that less educated parents used
The concept of social capital embodies the notion not more imperatives with their toddlers than better edu-
only that parenting is a community enterprise (Elder & cated mothers. Similarly, Hess and Shipman (1965) in
Conger, 2000) but also that children and adults are ac- their early classic studies of cognitive socialization
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 461

found clear SES differences in African American lower- are explored to illustrate the impact of social change on
class and middle-class families. family relationships (for reviews of other issues such as
Parental cognitionsideas, beliefs, valuesclearly timing of parenthood, see Moore & Brooks-Gunn, 2002;
play a major mediating role in accounting for SES differ- for divorce, see Clarke-Stewart & Brentano, 2006; Het-
ences (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003). Similarly, self- herington & Kelly, 2001). Some of these changes are
efficacy mediates between SES and parenting goals and scheduled or planned such as reentry into the workforce
practices (Brody, Stoneman, Smith, & Gibson, 1999). or delaying the onset of parenthood; other changes, such
Second, ecological factors, such as neighborhood condi- as job loss or divorce, are unscheduled or nonnormative
tions, play a role. One of the challenges is to determine transitions. According to a life-course view both sched-
the roles of parental ideas and beliefs and the ecological uled and unscheduled transitions need to be examined
conditions under which families are operating in deter- (Elder, 1998) to fully appreciate how these different
mining parental socialization strategies. A more de- types of change alter family socialization strategies.
tailed exploration of socioeconomic circumstances is These family transitions are adult-focused in contrast to
clearly a first step. Perhaps extreme circumstances, such child-focused transitions (e.g., entry to day care or jun-
as unsafe and dangerous living conditions, will override ior high school) and underscore our assumption that
parental beliefs and play a more determining role, adult developmental issues need to be directly addressed
whereas beliefs may play a role under less extreme con- to understand how these transitions alter parental so-
ditions. Specification of the types of parenting behavior, cialization beliefs and behaviors. At the same time,
which are altered by different factors, is also of interest. child developmental status will play a major role in de-
Perhaps, differences in verbal stimulation will not vary termining how adults respond to these transitions. We
across contexts in class, but control strategies may be argued earlier it is insufficient to focus on individual
more responsive to environmental circumstances. levels of analysiseither adult or child. Instead, indi-
vidual, dyadic, triadic, and family units each follow
their own developmental trajectory and the interplay
among these separate developmental trajectories can
THE IMPACT OF SOCIAL CHANGE ON produce a diverse set of effects on the functioning of the
FAMILY SOCIALIZATION units themselves. In addition, the role that these units
(i.e., individual, dyad, or family) play in modifying the
Families are not static but dynamic and are continuously impact of family transitions will vary as a result of these
confronted by challenges, changes, and opportunities. A interlocking developmental curves. Both the timing and
number of society-wide changes have produced a variety nature of family transitions and reactions to these alter-
of shifts in the nature of family relationships. Fertility ations will be determined by the points at which partic-
rates and family size have decreased, the percentage of ular individuals, dyads, triads, or families fall along
women in the workforce has increased, the timing of their respective developmental life-course trajectories.
onset of parenthood has shifted, divorce rates have Moreover, individual families can vary widely in the
risen, and the number of single-parent families have in- particular configuration of life-course trajectories. The
creased (Teachman, Tedrow, & Crowder, 2000). These central premise is that the particular configuration of
social trends provide an opportunity to explore how fam- these multiple sets of developmental trajectories needs
ilies adapt and change in response to these shifting cir- to be considered to understand the impact of societal
cumstances and represent natural experiments in family change on families.
adaptation. Moreover, they challenge our traditional as-
sumptions that families can be studied at a single point Womens and Mens Employment Patterns and
in historical time because the historical contexts are Family Socialization
constantly shifting. Our task is to establish how social-
ization processes operate similarly or differently under The relations between employment patterns of both
varying historical circumstances. In this section, one women and men and their family roles are increasingly
issue from this myriad of changes, the effects of recent being recognized (Deutsch, 1999; Hoffman, 2000).
shifts in family employment and unemployment patterns In this section, a variety of issues concerning the links
462 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

between the worlds of work and family are considered to 1999). Sons of working mothers, in contrast to sons of
illustrate the impact of recent shifts in work patterns on unemployed mothers, not only perceive females as
both mens and womens family roles. We examine the more competent, but also view men as warmer and
impact of changes in maternal employment on both more expressive. Moreover, duration of employment
quantitative and qualitative aspects of mother and fa- among African American mothers is associated with
ther participation and the influence of variations in longer school attendance in their daughters; no link was
family work schedules. found for European American mothers (Wolfer &
Since the 1960s, there has been a dramatic shift in Moen, 1996).
the participation rate of women in the labor force. The One of the limitations of earlier studies was the lack
rise has been particularly dramatic for married women of long-term follow-up to assess delayed effects of ear-
with children. In the United States in 1998, over 75% lier maternal employment on childrens development. In
of married women with school-age children and over a longitudinal study, Gottfried et al. (2002) found that
63% of mothers with children under age 6 were in the maternal employment was not related to childrens de-
paid workforce. In contrast, in 1960 fewer than 19% of velopment across age (infancy to age 17), developmental
mothers with children were employed (Statistical Ab- domain, and gender. Moreover, prospective analyses in-
stracts, 1999). How have these shifts affected the dicated that there were no sleeper effects associated
quantity and quality of the mothers and fathers con- with maternal employment. The children of employed
tribution to family tasks such as housework and child and nonemployed mothers were similar in cognitive, so-
care, and what are the implications for childrens de- cioemotional, academic, motivational, and behavioral
velopment? domains from infancy through adolescence. This con-
clusion is not surprising given that the home environ-
Maternal Employment and
ment and parenting of employed and nonemployed
Childrens Development
mothers were very similar in stimulation, nurturing,
How does maternal employment alter mother-child in- parent-child interactions, and family climate. The pat-
volvement? There is little difference in the amount of tern of findings suggests that variations in employed and
time that mothers spend with their children or in the nonemployed mothers are more significant for child out-
types of activities engaged in dual or father-only em- comes than differences between groups. In support of
ployed families (Gottfried et al., 2002). According to this shift away from a social address model to a pro-
Bianchi (2000), between 1981 and 1997, there was lit- cess-oriented approach, Gottfried et al. (2002) found
tle change in mothers time with children even though that processes such as parental involvement and the
there were dramatic increases in maternal employ- quality of the home environment were clearly linked to
ment. Similar findings have been reported for the childrens developmentregardless of maternal em-
United States (Galinsky & Swanberg, 2000) and Ger- ployment status.
many (Ahnert, Rickert, & Lamb, 2000). Moreover, Cohort effects need to be considered in interpreting
there are few negative outcomes of maternal employ- these findings. As maternal employment becomes more
ment on children, in part, because there has been re- common, the differential effects on select areas of de-
allocation of mothers time and priorities, delegation velopment (e.g., sex roles and independence) may de-
of family work to others, increased preschool enroll- crease. In part, this may be due to the fact that the shift
ment of children of employed and nonemployed moth- in maternal employment is part of a changing set of cul-
ers and redefinition of parenting roles (Gottfried tural attitudes concerning male and female roles that all
et al., 2002, p. 214). children, regardless of their family employment arrange-
Several domains of childrens development have ments, are exposed to.
been examined including gender roles, achievement, There is some evidence that the child-rearing prac-
and behavior problems. Maternal employment is asso- tices of working mothers may differ from those of
ciated with more egalitarian views of sex roles by their nonemployed mothers, particularly in the area of inde-
children, particularly by their daughters (Hoffman & pendence training. Except in cases where mothers feel
Youngblade, 1999). In middle-class families, maternal guilty about leaving their children to work, employed
employment is related to higher educational and occu- mothers encourage their children to become self-
pational goals in children (Hoffman & Youngblade, sufficient and independent and to assume responsibil-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 463

ity for household tasks at an earlier age (Hoffman & in marital interactions after high-stress shifts and
Youngblade, 1999). This early independence training tended to be behaviorally and emotionally withdrawn
may be beneficial in leading to high achievement moti- during interactions with their children as well. Distress-
vation, achievement behavior, and competence ing social experiences at work were associated with
(Hoffman & Youngblade, 1999). Finally, mothers sat- higher expressions of anger and greater use of discipline
isfaction with their employment is related to child out- during interaction with the child later in the day. Repetti
comes as well in Korean immigrants (K. Kim & Honig, and Wood (1997) found similar effects for mothers who
1998) and European Americans (Gottfried et al., withdrew from their preschoolers on days when the
2002). mothers experienced greater workloads or interpersonal
In summary, the results of studies of maternal em- stress on the job. Similarly, Crouter, Bumpus, Maguire,
ployment suggest it does not usually have detrimental ef- and McHale (1999) found that parents who reported
fects on children; positive consequences have usually high work pressure and role overload had more conflicts
been obtained, especially for girls. However, the effects with their adolescents.
of maternal employment can be evaluated only in rela- Other research suggests that positive work experi-
tion to other factors, such as the reason why the mother ences can enhance the quality of fathering. Grossman,
is working, the mothers satisfaction with her role, the Pollock, and Golding (1988) found that high job satis-
demands placed on other family members, the attitudes faction was associated with higher levels of support
of the other family members toward the mothers em- for their 5-year-olds autonomy and affiliation, de-
ployment, and the quality of substitute care and supervi- spite the fact that positive feelings about work were
sion provided for the children. negatively related to the quantity of time spent inter-
acting with their child. This finding underscores the
Quality of Mother and Father Work importance of distinguishing quantity and quality of
and Family Socialization involvement.
Instead of examining whether one or both parents are In contrast to the Repetti studies, the Grossman et al.
employed, researchers have begun to address the impact study focused on general job satisfaction and demand-
of the quality and nature of work on both mother and fa- ingness rather than daily fluctuations in the level of pos-
ther parenting behavior. This shift in focus is due to the itivity or negativity experienced in the work setting.
fact that many workers experienced an increase in work Future studies need to assess these two aspects of job-
hours, a decrease in job stability, a rise in temporary related affect and involvement separately.
jobs, and, especially among low-wage workers, a de- Research in the second tradition of family work link-
crease in income (Mishel, Bernstein, & Schmitt, 1999). age (the effects of the nature of mens occupational
As Crouter (1994) noted, there are two types of linkage roles on their fathering behavior) dates back to the clas-
between family and work. One type of research focuses sic work of Kohn (1995). Several researchers extended
on work as an emotional climate, which may have car- this work by focusing on the outcomes of job character-
ryover effects to the enactment of roles in home settings. istics for childrens development. Cooksey, Menaghan,
The focus is generally on short-term effects. A second and Jekielek (1997) found that children had fewer be-
type of linkage focuses on the type of skills, attitudes, havior problems when their mothers work involved
and perspectives that adults acquire in their work-based more autonomy working with people and more problem-
socialization as adults and how these variations in job solving opportunities. Similarly, fathers with greater
experience alter their behavior in family contexts. In job complexity and autonomy were less authoritarian
contrast to the short-term perspective of the spillover of (Grimm-Thomas & Perry-Jenkins, 1994) and responded
emotional climate research, this type of endeavor in- with greater warmth to their children and with more
volves more enduring and long-lasting effects of work verbal explanations (Greenberger, ONeil, & Nagel,
on family life. 1994). However, the process probably operates in both
Work in the first tradition has been conducted by directions: The home experience of parents affects
Repetti (1994) who studied the impact of working in a their job performance as well. Arguments at home with
high-stress job (air-traffic controller) on subsequent a wife or with a child were negatively related to work
family interaction patterns. She found that the male air performance (Frone, Yardley, & Markel, 1997). These
traffic controllers were more withdrawn and less angry studies underscore the importance of moving beyond
464 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

employment status per se to a detailed exploration of Girls respond to stress with internalizing problems,
the nature of work in studies of family work linkages. whereas boys tend to show externalizing behaviors
(Conger & Elder, 1994). Recent longitudinal studies
Job Loss and Unemployment confirm earlier cross-sectional findings. Conger, Ge,
Elder, Lorenz, and Simons (1994) interviewed parents
Another unscheduled transition that has received atten- and their adolescents over a 3-year period (seventh to
tion is the impact of job loss on families (Conger & ninth grades). Economic pressure at Wave 1 directly im-
Elder, 1994; McLoyd, 1998). Several aspects of the par- pacted parent-child financial conflict at Wave 2, which
ent-child relationship are altered by economic stress in- was related to adolescent internalizing and externalizing
cluding parenting style, parent discipline, parental behavior at Wave 3. Moreover, these investigators found
problem solving, and levels of parent-child conflict and support for an indirect path between parental depressed
monitoring. Parenting behavior is adversely affected as mood and marital conflict, and, in turn, altered parental
indexed by increased parental hostility and less consis- hostility was linked to adolescent internalizing and ex-
tent, less effective, harsher discipline (Conger & Elder, ternalizing. Even though there were predictable gender
1994; Elder, 1974; McLoyd, 1998). Recent extensions of differences in levels of symptomatology for boys and
this basic finding to African Americans (Conger et al., girls (i.e., greater externalizing for boys and greater in-
2002) and Mexican Americans (Parke et al., 2004) have ternalizing for girls) the paths were similar for both
been reported. Moreover, under conditions of stress, sexes. Similar support for an indirect pathway model
parental monitoring will be adversely affected with less comes from McLoyds (1998) examination of the impact
vigilance on the part of the parent (Crouter, MacDer- of unemployment among African American single moth-
mid, McHale, & Perry-Jenkins, 1990). Although condi- ers on parenting and adolescent socioemotional func-
tions of unemployment or underemployment increase tioning, as well as the Conger et al. (2002) report of the
fathers availability and involvement in child care, un- impact of economic pressure on 10- and 11-year-old
employed fathers report fewer nurturing behaviors than African American children. Finally, Parke et al. (2004)
employed fathers (Harold-Goldsmith, Radin, & Eccles, have found that the economic stress model explains the
1988). Recent research has focused on modifying and impact of economic pressure on family processes and
mediating variables. Several studies indicate that social child outcomes in Mexican American families as well.
support has a positive impact on parent-child relation- In summary, studies of the impact of unemployment
ships under conditions of stress (Conger & Elder, 1994; have clearly been fruitful avenues for exploring how
McLoyd, 1998). Child temperament and physical attrac- families adapt and cope in response to stressful change
tiveness also modify parenting practices. Temperamen- and underscore the value of a family systems approach
tally difficult children are treated more harshly by to the study of socialization.
unemployed fathers than temperamentally easy off-
spring (Elder, Nguyen, & Caspi, 1985). Although physi-
cally unattractive daughters are treated more harshly Single versus Multiple Transitions
than attractive girls, in some cases they received more
support and less harshness (Elder et al., 1985). The To date, societal changes, such as shifts in the timing of
quality of the prior father-child relationship is another parenting, work participation, or divorce, have been
determinant of how stress impacts changes in the father- treated relatively independently, but these events co-
child relationship. A positive relationship prior to job occur rather than operate in any singular fashion. As
loss served as a protective factor in buffering the child earlier work (Simmons & Blyth, 1987) on the impact of
from deterioration of the father-child relationship multiple transitions, such as the onset of puberty and
(Elder et al., 1985). Similarly, a positive (warm and af- entry into junior high school, on childrens adjustment
fectionate) mother-child relationship reduced harsh has found, co-occurrence of several changes can have a
treatment of the child by the father (Elder et al., 1985). cumulative impact on the adolescents adaptation. Simi-
A variety of adverse effects on children accompany larly, as the number of environmental risk variables in-
unemployment and economic stress, including increased crease, the level of family functioning and child
depression and loneliness and lowered self-esteem outcomes decrease (Sameroff, 1994). One would expect
(McLoyd, 1998). Gender differences are evident as well. that the co-occurrence of the arrival of a new infant ac-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 465

companied by job loss would have different effects than Nationally, people of color today comprise approxi-
either of these events occurring singly. Moreover, the mately 35% of the U.S. population, and it is estimated
impact of any historical change may be different as a re- that by the year 2020, 40% of all children will be African
sult of its occurrence in the same period as another American or Latino. Child development research, to be
change or changes. For example, womens increased relevant in this country, must be more inclusive of chil-
presence in the workplace and delay in the onset of par- dren of color and their unique developmental challenges.
enthood vary, and probably each event has different Parke (2004b) noted the responsibility that the field of
meaning without the other change. This implies the re- child development has to the growing number of children
search need for multivariate designs to capture the si- of color:
multaneous impact of multiple events on family
socialization strategies. In view of these demographic shifts there is both an op-
portunity to evaluate the generalizability of our assump-
tions about developmental processes and a moral
obligation to understand better a larger segment of our
Children and Families of Color in the United
population. (p. 10)
States: Issues of Race, Ethnicity, and Culture

Every 10 years, the Census Bureau reaffirms the con- Ethnicity refers to an individuals membership in a
cept of race, which has a long legacy in this country. group sharing a common ancestral heritage based on na-
Although race has no scientific basis, it is nevertheless tionality, language, and culture (Betancourt & Lopez,
a potent social construction that impacts the lives of all 1993). Psychological attachment to the group is also a
people in the United States, especially those consid- dimension of ethnicity, referred to as ethnic identity
ered to be non-White. In the recent past, non-White (Phinney, 2003). Sometimes ethnicity includes a biolog-
groups were generally referred to as ethnic minorities. ical (F. Barth, 1969), or racialized component, that is
However, because Whites, or European Americans, can evident in the phenotype of the group members. Culture
also claim ethnic group membership (Waters, 1990), is a multidimensional construct referring to the shared
the term ethnic minority does not accurately capture values, behaviors, and beliefs of a people that are trans-
the most salient aspects in the lives of non-Whites, mitted from one generation to the next. Unlike ethnicity
which distinguish them from the White population and race, which are usually self- and other-ascribed at-
skin color and physical appearance. For this reason, the tributes, respectfully, culture is learned behavior and
term people of color has gained greater acceptance as can thus vary both across and within ethnic and racial
the preferred designation for groups typically consid- groups. For this reason, it is invalid to equate ethnicity
ered ethnic minoritiesAmerican Indians, African and race with culture. Although in some populations
Americans, Latinos, and Asian Americans. The histo- there is an overlap in race, ethnicity, and culture, this
ries of these groups are different but share common ex- convergence should neither be taken for granted nor any
periences of exploitation and subordination by the of these three attributes considered as a proxy for the
White majority: (a) the forceful removal of American other. Consequently, researchers should describe in de-
Indians from their ancestral homelands and relocation tail the self-ascribed ethnicity of respondents, their
on reservations, ( b) the enslavement of African Ameri- commonly ascribed racial classification, and salient cul-
cans and segregation after their emancipation, (c) Lati- tural characteristics as determined by appropriate mea-
nos incorporation through military conquest of the sures of acculturation.
Southwest and ambivalent immigration policies, and The terminology regarding race, ethnicity, and cul-
(d) racist immigration policies toward Asians and their ture is changing as a result of demographic shifts and
internment during World War II. In addition, mixed more informed awareness of how these factors con-
race children and families, who now make up 2.4% of tribute to development. Nevertheless, minority group
the U.S. population (U.S. Bureau of the Census, status in the form of powerlessness and discrimination
2001a), should be included as people of color. Often, (Greenfield & Cocking, 1994) is still prevalent among
these children and families of color experience preju- non-White groups in society. Therefore, to maintain con-
dice and discrimination based on anti-miscegenation sistency between past and present group designations in
attitudes (Root, 1992). the literature, the terms minority group, ethnic minority,
466 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

and people of color will be used interchangeably, when Parke, 2004b). Such studies tell us how within-group
appropriate, in the remaining section of this chapter. cultural variations may account for differences in out-
comes among children of the same ethnic/racial group.
Conceptual and Methodological Issues for One of the problems in cross-cultural or intracultural
Studying Children and Families of Color research about different ethnic groups is the issue of the
Recent cross-cultural and intracultural theories have equivalence of measures across groups. Because most
emphasized the importance of socialization goals, val- standard measures of family functioning are developed
ues, and beliefs as organizing principles for understand- and standardized in White middle-class populations, ef-
ing cultural variations (Harkness & Super, 1995). In forts have been made to develop culturally and linguisti-
contrast to the older cultural deficit models of social- cally equivalent measures. One innovation is the use of
ization, the recent models emphasize how ecological de- focus groups consisting of members of the ethnic/racial
mands shape socialization goals, values, and practices, group of interest to generate items and issues that are
and are viewed as adaptive strategies to meet the de- culturally relevant (De Ment, Buriel, & Villanueva,
mands of the ecological settings. Ecological (Bronfen- 2005; Gomel, Tinsley, Clark, & Parke, 1998; Vazquez-
brenner, 1989) and family systems perspectives Garcia, Garcia Coll, Erkut, Alarcon, & Tropp, 1995).
(Minuchin, 2002) have been useful in explaining how Focus groups are also being used as an integral part of
socialization goals for children derive from their par- the scale-construction process; they make recommenda-
ents experiences with adaptive strategies that have tions for wording changes and identify culturally inap-
helped them meet the challenges faced as people of propriate items. Another innovation is the use of
color (Harrison, Wilson, Pine, Chan, & Buriel, 1990). translation and back translation to ensure that the mean-
Furthermore, Rogoff (2003) notes that to achieve a ing is retained in the translation process. In addition, a
valid understanding of development in its cultural con- dual-focus approach (Vazquez-Garcia et al., 1995) is
text, it is helpful to separate value judgments from ob- being used in which new concepts and items that arise in
servation of events: the course of the translation process are generated si-
multaneously in both languages. Work by Knight and
colleagues (Knight, Tein, Prost, & Gonzales, 2002;
Interpreting the activity of people without regard for their
meaning system and goals renders observations meaning-
Knight, Virdin, & Roosa, 1994) have provided models
less. We need to understand the coherence of what people for establishing scalar equivalence of commonly used
from different communities do, rather than simply deter- questionnaires for assessment of family functioning. Re-
mining that some other group of people do not do what cent advances in scaling have been applied to this issue
we do, or do not do it as well or in the way that we do it, as well. Specifically, Reise and his colleagues (Flan-
or jumping to conclusions that their practices are bar- nery, Reise, & Widaman, 1995; S. Reise, Widaman, &
baric. (p. 17) Pugh, 1993) have utilized item response theory (IRT)
techniques to address the equivalence of scales across
Earlier cultural deficit perspectives were reinforced groups. The utility of this approach for establishing gen-
by the popularity of two-group studies that compared der equivalence (Flannery et al., 1995) and cross-cul-
samples of European Americans with ethnic/racial mi- tural equivalence (e.g., China versus the United States;
norities and assumed that differences between the Reise et al., 1993) suggests that this strategy can be used
groups were cultural in nature. In effect, ethnicity and to establish scalar equivalence across different ethnic
race were equated with culture as if all members of an groups in our own culture.
ethnic/racial group were equally involved with the cul- The methodological issues involved in doing research
ture of their group. An assumption of these studies was with families of color are multifaceted (Bernal, Trim-
that people of color needed to assimilate or become like ble, Burlew, & Leong, 2003; Betancourt & Lopez, 1993;
European Americans to correct deficiencies in their de- McLoyd & Steinberg, 1998). In addition to language and
velopment (Ramirez & Castaneda, 1974; Rogoff, 2003). scalar equivalence issues, other methodological issues
More recently, the focus on families of color has shifted need to be considered. First, families of color are com-
away from majority-minority comparisons toward prised of diverse populations whose presence in the
within-group studies (Garcia Coll & Magnuson, 1999; United States has come about through conquest (Ameri-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 467

can Indians), involuntary immigration through slavery Many children of immigrant families learn English
(African Americans), and voluntary immigration (Asian as their second language. Although many speak English
Americans and Latino Americans). The nature of these at school, they may not be proficient in literacy skills
groups incorporation into the United States has neces- such as reading and writing (Gandara, 1997; Hakuta,
sitated diverse adaptation strategies, making it useless Butler, & Witt, 2000). It is imperative to obtain infor-
to combine members of these groups into a single re- mation about childrens English language proficiency if
search sample. Even among contemporary immigrant they come from homes where parents are immigrants.
groups, such as Mexican Americans and Chinese Amer- One in five children in this country comes from homes
icans, there are communities whose members have lived where at least one parent is an immigrant (U.S. Census
in this country for several generations. Consequently, Bureau, 2002).
researchers need to identify the generation of their sam- Sample specification should include information on
ples and their length of U.S. residence (for immigrants) social class, group density, and region of the country, as
and to avoid overgeneralizations to all members of the these variables can impact on within-group differences
group. Researchers must also not overgeneralize cul- in outcomes. Social class, based on education and in-
tural characteristics from the country of origin to mem- come, can differentiate members of a group sharing a
bers of the group who have lived in the United States for common ethnic/racial identity. Social class can impact
generations and who have culturally adapted to new eco- cultural orientations in counterintuitive directions, such
logical challenges. as higher social class Mexican Americans engaging
Among the dominant ethnic/racial groups in the in more familism (Keefe & Padilla, 1987) and less uti-
United States, there is considerable within-group diver- lization of professional child-care centers (Buriel &
sity related to national origin and tribal affiliation that Hurtado-Ortiz, 2000). Due to immigration and mobility,
could help researchers understand within-group differ- regions of the country are experiencing population
ences in outcomes. For example, combining Korean booms involving new groups of people of color, such as
Americans and Vietnamese Americans together as the influx of Latinos into the South and the Northeast.
Asian Americans, or Mexican Americans and Cuban Because these new groups are acculturating in unfamil-
Americans as Latinos, can mask historical, language, iar environments, lacking high densities of same-group
and social class variables that may contribute to differ- members and familiar social supports, their accultura-
ences in outcomes among members of these groups. Re- tion pathways and outcomes may be different from those
searchers need to identify the national origins of of group members in other parts of the country. Geo-
participants in their ethnic/racial sample, and recognize graphic location is also important to consider in research
any cultural and sociodemographic variables that may with mixed-race populations. As Root (1999) notes, for
contribute to within-group differences. example, being Asian American and growing up in Hon-
The complexity of diversity means that some children olulu is a different experience than growing up in Los
of color either belong to two or more ethnic/racial Angeles or Minneapolis. Finally, researchers should take
groups or claim an identity that is not consistent with advantage of the natural experiments arising from immi-
our ethnic/racial categorization system based primarily gration that produce emic- or group-specific behaviors
on color. For example, many biracial children, despite a that have not been considered in traditional developmen-
phenotype of color, claim the ethnic/ racial identity (and tal theories and models (Buriel, 2003; Parke, 2004b).
cultures) of both their colored and white parents. In Examples of research in this vein include children who
other cases, children of color may claim an ethnic iden- interpret for their immigrant parents (Buriel, Perez,
tity not typically associated with their phenotype. Thus, DeMent, Chavez, & Moran, 1998), Chinese-American
some Afro-Latinos from Puerto Rico or the Dominican reconstruals of the meaning of authoritative parenting as
Republic may self-identify as Latinos, whereas they are a form of training (Chao, 1994), and a heightened sense
identified as African American on the basis their skin of family obligation among children of immigrants
color. Researchers need to allow children and families (Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999).
of color to self-identify rather than to assume member- These diverse conceptual and methodological issues
ship in a racial /ethnic group on the basis of phenotype represent distal and proximal ecological factors impact-
or surname. ing on the lives of children and families of color.
468 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

2 6
Racism Child Characteristics
Prejudice Age
Discriminatioin Temperament
Oppression Health Status
Biological Factors
Physical Characteristics

1 4 5 8
Social Position Promoting/Inhibiting Adaptive Culture Development
Variables Environments Traditions and Cultural Legacies Competencies
Race Schools Economic and Political Histories Cognitive
Social Class Neighborhoods Migration and Acculturation Social
Ethnicity Health Care Current Contextual Demands Emotional
Gender Linguistic
Biculturalism
Coping with Racism

3 7
Segregation Family
Residential Structure and Roles
Economic Family Values, Beliefs, and Goals
Social and Racial Socialization
Psychological Socioeconomic Status

Figure 8.4 Integrative model for the study of developmental competence in minority children. Source: An Integrative Model
for the Study of Developmental Competencies in Minority Children, by C. Garcia-Coll et al., 1996, Child Development, 67,
pp. 18911914.

Garcia Coll et al. (1996) have provided a conceptual assimilation was completed by the second generation.
model for studying developmental competencies in chil- Assimilation was motivated both by the desire to be-
dren of color that includes many of the ecological factors come part of the mainstream and to eliminate societal
presented here (see Figure 8.4). This model outlines rel- discrimination against ethnic immigrants (A. Portes &
evant sociocultural and demographic variables and their Rumbaut, 1990). Assimilation was aided by their Eu-
contribution to child outcomes. ropean phenotype, which they shared with members of
the larger society, who were themselves descendants
Acculturation and Assimilation in U.S. Society of earlier English and German immigrants. Although
European immigrants to the United States at the turn ethnicity persists in varying degrees among assimi-
of the past century underwent acculturation and as- lated European Americans, it is largely a symbolic
similation in their transformation into European ethnicity or an optional vestigial attachment to a few
Americans. Acculturation is a process of learning the ethnic symbols that impose little cost on everyday life
language, values, and social competencies of the larger (Waters, 1990).
society. Assimilation is a possible outcome of accul- Racial, ethnic, and cultural variation from the Euro-
turation that involves the replacement of the ancestral pean American mainstream remain enduring issues for
culture with the culture of the host society (Ramirez, peoples of non-European descent, including American
1983). As European immigrants and their descendants Indians, Latinos, African Americans, and Asian Ameri-
acculturated and replaced their immigrant culture cans. Although these groups have been in contact with
with the mainstream culture, and were accepted as European Americans for centuries, and have accultur-
Americans by the larger society, they achieved assimi- ated to varying degrees, they have not assimilated. The
lation. For most European immigrants, the process of non-European phenotype of these groups (and other
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 469

group markers related to physiognomy, religion, and so- 1985). Harrison et al. (1990) have adopted an ecological
cial class) heightens awareness of their racial and ethnic orientation that views the socialization of ethnic/racial
otherness, that is associated with beliefs about their minority children by the interconnectedness between the
behaviors. Often, these beliefs are stereotypes that dis- status of ethnic/racial minority families, adaptive
parage the lives and cultures of ethnic/racial minorities strategies, socialization goals, and child outcomes. Fam-
and limit access to European American society (Padilla ily status involves socioeconomic resources available to
& Perez, 2003). According to some theorists, these group members such as housing, employment, health
forms of racism elicit reactionary behaviors that eventu- care, and education. Despite considerable within-group
ally become incorporated as part of the minority groups diversity in SES, a growing number of ethnic minority
culture. Ogbu (1991) describes the tendency of some children live in poverty (National Center for Children in
caste-like minorities to develop secondary cultural Poverty, 2000). Adaptive strategies are the cultural pat-
characteristics that include an oppositional identity terns that promote the survival and well-being of group
against the achievement values of European Americans. members. Some of these cultural patterns are adapta-
Jones (2003) theorizes that Culture can be defined as tions of the original ethnic/racial culture to life circum-
both an antecedent and consequent of behavior stances in the United States. Other cultural patterns
(p. 282), and as a result, Reactions to racial biases and may arise as a result of coping with the conflicting be-
stereotypes produce the very racial differences that the havioral demands of being an ethnic/racial minority in a
stereotypes presuppose (p. 282). As individual mem- predominately European American society. Thus, bicul-
bers of ethnic minority groups internalize European turalism, which is the simultaneous adoption of two cul-
Americans stereotypes (Buriel & Vasquez, 1982), they tural orientations, arose originally in response to
may lead to self-fulfilling prophecies. Steeles (1997) conflicting cultural demands but is now part of what
research on stereotype threat describes this situation constitutes the ethnic minority/racial culture. Bicultur-
with African Americans in test-taking situations. Racial alism, for example, characterizes the lives of many Mex-
stereotyping can also negatively affect young African ican American and other ethnic/racial minorities
American childrens occupational aspirations (Bigler, (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). In addition
Averhart, & Liben, 2003). to family extendedness and biculturalism, other adap-
tive strategies include role flexibilities and ancestral
Socialization of Children of Color worldviews. Emerging out of the adaptive strategies of
adults are the socialization goals that they endeavor to
As with most children, the socialization of children of inculcate in children to help them meet the ecological
color usually takes place in a family setting that includes challenges they will face as ethnic/racial minorities.
adult caregivers. These adult caregivers are usually bio- Ethnic/racial pride and interdependence are two social-
logical parents but may include grandparents, relatives, ization goals that enable ethnic/racial minority children
godparents, and other adults who are not biologically re- to function competently as members of both their minor-
lated to the children. In some Mexican immigrant fami- ity and majority cultures (Harrison et al., 1990). Eth-
lies, older children have responsibility for the care and nic/racial pride imparts a sense of personal self-worth
conduct of younger siblings (Valdes, 1996). In addition in the face of societal prejudice and discrimination
to ensuring childrens physical health and survival, par- (Walker, Taylor, McElroy, Phillip, & Wilson, 1995). In-
ents attempt to inculcate in children values and behav- terdependence sustains effective intergroup relations
iors that help them adapt to their environment as it is that strengthen ethnic/racial group solidarity (Staples &
perceived by the parents. The parents history of inter- Johnson, 1993).
action with the larger sociocultural context, including
Families of Color
their awareness of their ethnic/racial groups history in
the larger society, affects the manner in which they so- Between 1990 and 2000, all groups of people of color in-
cialize their children. An important dimension of social- creased in size, whereas the number of Whites de-
ization in ethnic minority families is teaching children creased from 80% to 75% of the U.S. population (U.S.
how to interact effectively in dual cultural contexts; the Bureau of the Census, 2000). The percentage of African
context of their ethnic/racial group and the context of Americans and American Indians rose slightly from
the larger European American society (Boykin & Toms, 12.1% to 12.3% and from 0.8% to 0.9%, respectively.
470 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Asian Americans and Latinos had the greatest popula- veloped by immigrants in this country. As the children
tion increases in the past decade, growing from 2.8% to of immigrants start their own families, their children,
3.6% and 9.0% to 12.5%, respectively. Latinos now are the third generation, are socialized in family ecologies
the largest minority group in the nation and account for that are socioculturally distinct from the ecologies of
more than half of all new births in California (Richard- their parents. The generational status of parents and
son & Fields, 2003). Whites have a higher median age children contributes to variations in the ecologies of
and a smaller percentage of children under the age of 18 families that have implications for child rearing (Buriel,
(37.7 years and 23.5%, respectively) than all groups of 1993b). The importance of generational status to diver-
people of color: African Americans (30.2 years and sity in family ecologies is illustrated with an example
31.4%, respectively); American Indians (28 years and using Mexican Americans.
33.9%, respectively); Asian Americans (32.7 years and
24%, respectively); and Latinos (25.8 years and 35%, Generational Differences in Family Ecologies
respectively; U.S. Census Bureau, 2002). This demo- The first generation includes those persons born in Mex-
graphic shift is visible in public schools where nation- ico who later immigrated to the United States. Some im-
ally approximately 40% of students in kindergarten migrate as single young adults or as married couples,
through 12th grade are children of color (Young, 2002). whereas others are brought to this country as young chil-
The growth of the Asian American and Latino popu- dren by their parents. Some parents immigrate with only
lations was due in large measure to increases in immi- some of their children, leaving the other children in
gration. Overall, foreign-born persons constitute 56 Mexico under the care of relatives. As the parents eco-
million people with more than half coming from Latin nomic condition improves, children are brought to the
America and a quarter from Asia. In 1997, the majority United States. These children often experience multiple
(61%) of Asian Americans were born in foreign coun- socializing influences in both Mexico and the United
tries (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1999; 2001b). As im- States. This transnational socialization experience may
migrant groups, Latinos and Asian Americans share also give rise to a dual frame of reference for these par-
common characteristics, including diverse subpopula- ents and children (Perez, 2004; Valenzuela, 1999) that
tions with distinct histories, non-English native lan- shapes how they perceive opportunities in the United
guages, and relatively young age. Both groups include States. At the time of immigration, adults have usually
economic immigrants who seek a better quality of life completed the extent of their formal education in Mex-
and political immigrants who seek refuge from persecu- ico, 7 years on average (Bean, Chapa, Berg, & Sowards,
tion in their countries of origin. Owing to their different 1994), which is 1 year over compulsory education in
motivations for immigration, economic and political im- Mexico. School-age immigrant children have usually
migrants may have different adaptation strategies that begun their schooling in Mexico and then continue it in
influence their socialization goals. For example, com- this country. Children with prior schooling in Mexico
pared to economic immigrants, adult political immi- often achieve higher academic levels in U.S. schools rel-
grants expecting to return to their countries of origin ative to native-born Mexican Americans (Padilla &
may be more discouraging of their childrens accultura- Gonzalez, 2001; Valenzuela, 1999). Preschool immi-
tion in areas such as individualism and autonomy (Rum- grant children begin and complete their schooling in the
baut, 1995). United States. Because these childrens formative years
The influx of Latino and Asian immigrants into this are spent in the United States, they are often referred to
country means that these two groups will be constantly as the one and a half generation. Family income is
characterized by within-group differences in genera- typically low in the first generation due to parents
tional status and degree of acculturation. First-genera- lower education and limited knowledge of English. In
tion immigrant parents generally acculturate more the first 6 to 8 years after immigration, it is not uncom-
slowly than their children (Szapocznik & Kurtines, mon for immigrants to live with families of relatives or
1980), particularly after the onset of childrens school- friends (Blank, 1993; Chavez, 1990) who assist parents
ing. Yet, despite the more rapid acculturation of chil- with child care. After 8 years, the rate of immigrant
dren in immigrant families, the socialization of these families living in single family households is about 75%,
children is heavily influenced by the socialization goals which is the same as for U.S. Mexican Americans
of their parents culture and the adaptive strategies de- (Blank, 1993). First-generation children are socialized
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 471

in home environments influenced by immigrant Mexican thority during the more rapid acculturation of children.
culture, which includes elements of Mexican culture, as Socialization of first- and second-generation children is
well as the adaptive strategies of parents associated similar, particularly in areas such as respect for adults,
with the immigrant experience that parents convert into personalismo (Valdes, 1996), and family obligation
socialization goals (Buriel, 1993b; Delgado-Gaitan, (Fuligni et al., 1999). The academic achievement of the
1994; Valdes, 1996). Elements of Mexican cultural so- second generation is often higher than the third genera-
cialization include familism, respect for adults, and in- tion (Rodriguez, 2002). The second generations life-
terdependence among family and ethnic group members long exposure to European American culture impacts on
(Delgado-Gaitan, 1994; Rueschenberg & Buriel, 1989). their child-rearing practices as adults. For example, the
Socialization goals related to the immigrant experience longer families live in the United States, the more so-
are self-reliance, productive use of time (Buriel, 1993b), cialization practices and child behavior shift in an indi-
and biculturalism (Buriel, 1993a). Immigrant parents vidualistic direction, particularly in the area of critical
and their children both prefer a Mexican ethnic identity thinking (Delgado-Gaitan, 1994). In the area of ethnic
(Buriel & Cardoza, 1993), and use Spanish as their pri- identity, foreign-born parents prefer a Mexican identity,
mary home language. The parents and other family whereas their second-generation children prefer either a
members exposure to English often comes through chil- Mexican American or Chicano identity (Buriel & Car-
drens participation in the U.S. schooling system. As a doza, 1993). Individuals calling themselves Chicano are
result, many immigrant children serve as interpreters or more likely to be aware of prejudice against Mexican
cultural brokers for their parents, which means they Americans, and to work through political avenues to im-
are often given adultlike responsibility when acting as prove the status of their group.
the familys representative to the outside English-speak- The third generation refers collectively to all persons
ing world. Child cultural brokers play an important role of Mexican descent whose parents were born in the
in helping immigrant families adapt and survive in a new United States. This includes persons in the fourth and
environment. subsequent generations whose grandparents and great
The second generation represents the U.S. born chil- grandparents were born in this country. Due to immigra-
dren of immigrant parents. The family environments of tion and birth rate differences between generations,
these children are in many ways similar to those of their third-generation children are in the minority in the Mex-
first-generation peers owing to the foreign-born status ican American population (Edmonston & Passel, 1994).
of their parents. There are some important differences The third generation is distinguished from previous gen-
between the two generations that are reflected in the so- erations by the absence of any direct parental links to
ciocultural characteristics of the family. In some cases, Mexico involving immigration. Consequently, socializa-
the immigrant parents of second-generation children tion goals derived from immigrant adaptation strategies
came to the United States as single young adults who are not a direct part of the socialization experiences of
later became partners in generationally endogamous these children (Buriel, 1993b). Nevertheless, because
marriages (Murguia, 1982). As a result, they have lived many members of this generation live in ethnic neigh-
longer in the United States. In those cases where these borhoods ( barrios) populated by immigrants, socializa-
parents came from Mexico as young children, they may tion practices retain some immigrant influences. For
have attended U.S. schools for some or all of their edu- example, familism, or the expectation of support from
cation. Cultural synergisms are most apparent in the family members, continues as a socialization goal into
families of second-generation children. Thus, although later generations (Keefe & Padilla, 1987) even after
Spanish is usually the native language of second-genera- controlling for SES (Sabogal et al., 1987). Persons in
tion children, English becomes their dominant language this generation are also socialized in homes where all
after the onset of schooling. However, Spanish continues family members are U.S. citizens, where English is the
as the primary language of parents, which creates a primary language (Lopez, 1982), where parental school-
strong motivation for the development of bilingualism. ing has taken place exclusively in the United States, and
Parents strongly encourage the learning of English (Es- where children and parents express a Mexican American
posito, 2004) but also stress the retention of Spanish as ethnic identity (Buriel & Cardoza, 1993). Laosa (1982)
it is the language used to demonstrate respect to adults. theorizes that U.S. schooling alters the child-rearing
Retention of Spanish may help to preserve parental au- practices of Mexican American parents. Mothers with
472 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

less education, who are likely to be immigrants, use included in the category of immigrants. Landrine and
more modeling to instruct children, whereas mothers Klonoff (1996) suggested that acculturation is useful for
with more schooling, who are likely to be native born, deconstructing race and reviving culture among African
use more inquiry and praise to instruct children. This Americans, and have developed an African American
shift in teaching style occurs because more highly acculturation scale. In addition, African American fam-
schooled mothers adopt the teaching style of school, ilies moving from the ghetto into a predominately Euro-
which emphasizes inquiry and praise. Buriel (1993b) pean American neighborhood may describe their
also found that among parents of third-generation chil- experiences as having to adapt or acculturate to an all-
dren, parental schooling was associated with a child- White environment. The same set of experiences may
rearing style involving more support, control, and also hold true for American Indian families moving off
equality. Divorce is more common among parents of the reservation to live in a mostly non-Indian environ-
third-generation children, which has implications for ment or for American Indian children attending govern-
child socialization (Buriel, 1993b; Oropesa & Landale, ment boarding schools off their reservation (Garcia
1995). Teacher ratings of Mexican American children Coll, Meyer, & Brillon, 1995).
indicate more school maladjustment in boys from single- Acculturation across generations is not a uniform
parent (mothers only) homes than in boys from two-par- process. In each generation, there is considerable diver-
ent homes or in girls of either family type (LeCorgne & sity in individuals involvement with both native and Eu-
Laosa, 1976). Although family incomes are higher in the ropean American culture. In addition, acculturation is
third generation, schooling outcomes are often lower not a unidirectional process such that movement toward
than in the previous generation. Second-generation chil- European American culture is necessarily associated
dren complete more years of schooling and have higher with a corresponding loss of the native culture. Ecologi-
educational aspirations than their third-generation peers cal variables such as degree of societal discrimination,
(Buriel, 1987, 1994; Valenzuela, 1999). educational and employment opportunities, and oppor-
tunities to participate in the native culture can all con-
Acculturation. Researchers have focused on the tribute to variations in both the rate and direction of
construct of acculturation in an effort to unpackage the acculturation across generations.
cultural components of generational status that account
for within-group diversity. Acculturation is the process Bicultural Adaptation
of learning a new culture and is typically measured by For Mexican Americans, the proximity of Mexico and
increasing English proficiency, English media prefer- the fact that the southwestern United States was once a
ences, and European American friendships (Cuellar, part of Mexico create many opportunities for members
Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995). Recently, the measure- of this group to participate in their native culture. For
ment of acculturation has included culturally related Latinos and Asian Americans, a high rate of immigra-
values, attitudes, and identity in acknowledgment of the tion resulting in densely populated ethnic communities
multidimensional nature of this construct (Felix-Ortiz also creates powerful environmental influences for re-
de la Garza, Newcomb, & Meyers, 1995). Multidimen- tention of many aspects of the native cultures. Finally,
sional measures can provide information about the cul- for all ethnic/racial minorities, a non-European pheno-
tural processes associated with behavioral changes in type triggers many societal stereotypes and prejudices
immigrant groups, both within and across generations. that limit access to the larger society (Buriel, 1994).
The relative predictive power of generation and accul- Padilla and Perez (2003) argue that a darker phenotype
turation varies with the constructs under investigation. stigmatizes individuals, making it harder and even less
For example, acculturation may be a better predictor of desirable for them to acculturate. The combination of
ethnic identity than generation (Cuellar, Nyberg, Mal- these environmental and ethnic/racial group influences
donado, & Roberts, 1997), whereas generation is a bet- differentially operate within and between groups, giv-
ter predictor of students academic achievement ing rise to adaptation strategies that do not conform to
(Fuligni, 1997; Padilla & Gonzalez, 2001). the assimilationist orientation of European immigrants.
The developmental and socialization outcomes asso- Instead, many ethnic/racial minority group members
ciated with generation and acculturation also have rele- strive for a bicultural orientation that allows for selec-
vance for other U.S. ethnic/racial groups not typically tive acculturation to European American culture while
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 473

simultaneously retaining aspects of the native culture. ferent rates. Cultural involvement is represented on a
This bidirectional adaptation strategy permits individu- scale of 1 to 5. Thus, persons expressing a bicultural ori-
als to meet the dual cultural expectations that character- entation are those above 3 in both Mexican and Euro-
ize the lives of ethnic/racial minorities as they move in pean American culture. The Mexican orientation is
and out of minority and majority cultural environments. characterized by those individuals who are primarily in-
The bicultural person learns to function optimally in volved in Mexican culture. This category usually in-
more than one cultural context and to switch repertoires cludes many adult recent immigrants, as well as a few
of behavior appropriately and adaptively as called for by later-generation persons living in rural areas.
the situation (Harrison et al., 1990). Although all eth- Also included in this category are the elderly parents
nic/racial minority groups have expressed biculturalism of immigrants who are brought to this country to live with
in some form as an adaptive strategy (Harrison et al., the families of their adult children after the children be-
1990), most research has focused on immigrant groups, come financially stable. For the elderly, a Mexican orien-
especially Mexican Americans (Chun & Akutsu, 2003; tation seems well suited to their life experiences, which
Chun, Balls Organista, & Marin, 2003). at their stage in life, revolves around family and commu-
Using Mexican Americans as an example, Figure 8.5 nity rather than schooling and the workplace. The mar-
illustrates a bidirectional model of cultural adaptation. ginal orientation includes a minority of individuals who
This bidirectional model posits four acculturation adap- have become deculturated (Berry, 1980) from their an-
tation styles for Mexican immigrants and their descen- cestral culture and simultaneously alienated from Euro-
dants, depending on their involvement with both pean American society. Deculturation arises from
Mexican immigrant culture and European American societys denigration of the ethnic/racial group and the
culture. The four acculturation styles are: (1) the bicul- internalization of societys stereotypes of the group
tural orientation, (2) the Mexican orientation, (3) the (Buriel, 1984). Ogbus (1987) description of Mexican
marginal orientation, and (4) the European American Americans (as well as American Indians and African
orientation. Ramirez (1983) has defined biculturalism Americans) as caste-like minorities is probably accurate
as the simultaneous adoption of the language, values, only for the minority of individuals who adopt a marginal
and social competencies of two cultures. Because cul- orientation. For example, Vigil (1988) has described
ture is multidimensional in nature, involvement in either hard-core Mexican American gang members in terms
culture can vary along different dimensions and at dif- characteristic of the marginal orientation. Non-gang
members are described as expressing either a traditional
or bicultural orientation. Individuals in the European
(High) American quadrant of the bidirectional model are those
5 who are primarily involved and identified with European
American culture; their preference for friends, language,
Cultural Identification

and social activities are those characteristic of European


Euro- Bicultural American culture.
American Orientation
To date, most empirical research on acculturation
and biculturalism has been done with adult and adoles-
cent samples. More research is needed with young chil-
Mexican American Cultural Identification
(Low) 1 3 5 (High) dren to better understand the developmental pathways to
bicultural competency (Buriel, 1993a). Some research
indicates that ethnic/racial minorities who develop bi-
Euro-American

Marginal Mexican cultural competencies have better physical and psycho-


Orientation Orientation logical health than those who do not (Buriel & Saenz,
1980; Chun et al., 2003; LaFromboise, Coleman, & Ger-
ton, 1993). Gutierrez and Sameroff (1990) found that
mothers biculturalism is positively associated with the
1 complexity of their concepts about childrens develop-
(Low)
ment and that more acculturated and more bicultural
Figure 8.5 Bicultural model of acculturation. mothers scored higher in their concepts of development.
474 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Mothers who were the most bicultural had the highest Valdes, 2003). With responsibility as interpreters of
concept of development scores when compared to the the new culture and language, immigrant children are
other Mexican American mothers and a sample of highly often in a position with no one to translate or interpret
acculturated European American mothers. They con- for them. Traditional intergenerational authority rela-
cluded that bicultural mothers have insights into two tionships change and the child also becomes very in-
cultures and realize that child development beliefs asso- volved in the worries and concerns of the family, such as
ciated with each culture are appropriate in different hassles with landlords, arranging for medical care, and
contexts in the same society. LaFromboise et al. (1993) dealing with the legal system (Olsen & Chen, 1988,
connect biculturalism to self-efficacy theory (Bandura, p. 31). Children who serve as interpreters for their non-
1977) by using the term bicultural ef ficacy to describe English speaking parents are referred to as language
the belief, or confidence, that one can live effectively, brokers. Because these children of immigrants repre-
and in a satisfying manner, in two cultural groups with- sent the link between their parents culture and Euro-
out compromising ones sense of identity. pean American society, they can also be considered
cultural brokers.
Emic Developmental Research Issues Child cultural brokers are unique because in addition
Immigrants and their children face many sociocultural to the stress related to their own acculturation, they ex-
adaptation challenges that have implications for parent- perience additional stressors arising from their role as
ing and child development. Psychologys interest in im- mediators between their parents and U.S. society. In
migrant groups has generally been threefold. First, to public, child cultural brokers act with adult authority on
document between-group differences in constructs and behalf of their parents, but at home they are expected to
measures developed on European American populations behave as children and show deference and respect to
(Ramirez, 1983). These studies often erroneously parents. These conflicting expectations and responsibil-
equate race/ethnicity with culture, and conclude that ities represent a form of role strain that may raise chil-
differences are due to culture. A second research line drens anxiety to debilitating levels and lower their
attempts to extend theories, also developed on European general well-being. The stress connected to language
Americans, to immigrant groups and their descendents brokering may be particularly pronounced among young
(Parke et al., 2004). The goal is to understand how cul- children because their cognitive and social capacities
ture affects the hypothesized pattern of relationships are still in the early stages of development (Weisskirch
between variables for the different groups and to adjust & Alva-Alatorre, 2002). However, among adolescents,
the theories accordingly. A third area involves identifi- there is little evidence that language brokering is associ-
cation of emic or unique aspects of a groups behavior ated with psychological distress (Buriel, Love, & De-
arising from the immigrant experience itself. Three ex- Ment, in press), particularly among girls. In some
periences common to immigrant families and children groups, such as Mexican Americans, language brokering
include language and cultural brokering, children as seems to be associated more with the gender role re-
family workers, and dual frames of reference. sponsibilities of girls (Buriel et al., in press). In addi-
tion, a strong affective parent-child bond buffers
Language and Cultural Brokers. It is estimated adolescents against stress connected to language broker-
that approximately one in every five children in the ing (Buriel et al., in press).
United States comes from a home where at least one par- Language brokers act as adults in interactions with
ent is of foreign birth (Federal Interagency Forum on their parents, which often gives rise to role reversals in-
Child and Family Statistics, 2002). Most of these chil- volving the transmission of information. Language bro-
dren are the first members of their families to learn En- kers must sometimes teach parents things about the new
glish and attend U.S. schools. As a result, these children culture while still demonstrating deference and respect
are often delegated adultlike responsibilities by their consistent with their status as children. Child language
parents, such as interpreting and making decisions with brokers must assume a higher-status teaching role with-
English-speaking agents that affect the whole family out causing parents to lose face in public or in the fam-
(Chao, 2001; DeMent, Buriel, & Villanueva, 2005; ily. The instructional demands inherent in brokering are
Orellana, Dorner, & Pulido, 2003; Tse, 1995, 1996; thus likely to promote instructional strategies by chil-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 475

dren that achieve the transmission of information to In addition to household chores, children in immi-
adults without causing embarrassment to parents. Based grant families often assume adultlike responsibilities as
on her in-depth study, Valdes (2003) concluded that par- workers whose labor is beneficial, and sometimes essen-
ent and child act as a team in a language brokering situ- tial, to the financial well-being of the family (Orellana,
ation to present the impression of the parent that will 2001). Many immigrants work in manual and service
be most effective in a given context and that will evoke labor occupations where it is not unusual to bring chil-
positive responses from majority individuals (p. 96). dren along to help with the work and make extra
Drawing on Gardners theory of multiple intelligences, money. In the past, when Mexican immigrants were in-
Valdes (2003) argues that competent language brokers volved mostly in agricultural labor, children often
exhibit multiple types of cognitive, social, and interper- worked in the fields with their parents. This situation
sonal intelligences and should therefore be considered still exists today, especially during summers, but at a
gifted. Buriel et al. (1998) found that among Latino lesser scale due to child labor laws. More typical today,
adolescents, more language brokering was associated however, is the situation of children working with par-
with greater biculturalism and more social self- ents in service and manual labor sectors in jobs such as
efficacy. Children who broker in diverse settings such masonry, gardening and landscaping, painting, con-
as stores, banks, hospitals, and schools have more oppor- struction, cleaning, restaurants, street vending, auto
tunities to develop accelerated linguistic, cognitive, and shops, and cottage industries pertaining to garment
interpersonal skills and have higher school grades. work and food preparation.
Tse (1995) notes that unlike formal translators, lan- The constructs of parent involvement, family cohe-
guage brokers sometime influence the content and na- sion, and parent-child bonding are typically examined in
ture of the messages they convey between adult parties. the context of routine domestic activities and recreation
They sometimes favorably paraphrase messages be- experiences. However, for many immigrant families,
tween teachers and their parents. In addition, given economic survival creates roles for children that may
their greater knowledge of U.S. culture, language bro- promote parental involvement and contribute to family
kers are often assigned responsibility for making deci- cohesion and parent-child bonding in work-related set-
sions with English-speaking agents that affect the entire tings. From a social learning perspective, children in
family (DeMent et al., 2005; Olsen & Chen, 1988). family worker roles may have more opportunities to de-
Thus, as a result of the language-brokering role, there is velop personal responsibility, autonomy, and self-
the potential for the modification of traditional inter- efficacy by observing and modeling their parents in
generational authority relationships in immigrant fami- work-related activities. The topic of children as family
lies. However, the strong sense of family obligation in workers is an area arising from the immigrant experi-
immigrant families (Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999) may ence that deserves further attention.
mitigate the threat to authority relationships. Rather
than threatening parental authority, greater language Dual Frames of Reference. The immigrant adap-
brokering is associated with a stronger parent-child tation experience may give rise to a dual frame of ref-
bond (Buriel et al., in press). erence that allows immigrant children to compare their
socioeconomic and cultural status in the United States
Children as Family Workers. Family obligation to their past situation in their country of origin. A dual
and duty to family are strong values among immigrant frame of reference has been discussed in various con-
children from collectivist cultures (Fuligni et al., 1999). texts as an enabling quality that gives foreign-born
The operationalization of these values often takes the children higher expectations and feelings of positive
form of young children devoting time assisting parents self-worth relative to their native-born counterparts
in their occupations. This assistance, however, is viewed (Ogbu, 1991; Suarez-Orozco & Suarez-Orozco, 1995).
not so much as helping parents as much as contributing Although economically poor by U.S. standards, the
to the welfare of the entire family. These work-related families of immigrant children experience an immedi-
situations involve shared parent-child activities that can ate increase in their SES on arriving in this country,
influence childrens perceptions and values about work, leading to a relative interpretation of their deprivation.
family relations, and gender roles. This can bolster the familys sense of optimism and
476 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

expectations for the future. For example, many immi- ment and life in America were perhaps facilitated by
grant children are frequently exhorted by their parents the West African heritage of African Americans. From
to take advantage of the opportunities in this country, her studies of West African culture, she contends that
including education (Perez, 2004; Valenzuela, 1999), African extended-family traditions may have proved
and constantly reminded of the hard economic condi- useful in preserving family ties and for the socializa-
tions they left behind in their native country. A dual tion of children in the face of the disruptive aspects of
frame of reference may buffer children against the psy- slavery and its aftermath. Characteristics of African
chologically damaging effects of societal prejudice and American extended-kin systems include: (a) a high de-
discrimination. By having been raised in a culturally gree of geographical propinquity; ( b) a strong sense of
supportive environment in their native country, immi- family and familial obligations; (c) fluidity of house-
grant children have a frame of reference to counter the hold boundaries, with great willingness to absorb rela-
often negative stereotypes ascribed to many immigrant tives, both real and fictive; (d) frequent interaction
groups in this country (Buriel & Vasquez, 1982; Perez, with relatives; (e) frequent extended-family get-togeth-
2004). A dual frame of reference represents a useful ers for special occasions and holidays; and (f ) a system
psychological mechanism for understanding genera- of mutual aid (Harrison et al., 1990; Hatchett & Jack-
tional differences in school achievement, motivation, son, 1993). Some may surmise that extended-kin be-
and feelings of self-worth. havior among African Americans is a response to
poverty rather than an authentic cultural characteristic
Socialization Concerns in Ethnic Minority Families
of the group. However, higher-SES African Americans
African American Families. Today, approximately have greater activity within-kin networks than their
96% of all African Americans are descendants of en- lower-SES counterparts (H. McAdoo, 1978). This sug-
slaved people (Reed, 1982) who exhibited extraordinary gests that higher-SES African Americans continue to
resiliency to survive in this country without benefit of derive physical and psychological benefits from these
any human rights. The socially disruptive effects of slav- behaviors. Boykin (1983; Boykin & Toms, 1985) has
ery were felt in all spheres of life, including family for- noted similarities between the West African traditions
mation and functioning. According to M. Wilson (1992), of spirituality, harmony, affect, and communalism and
the focus on African American family research has African American culture.
shifted from a pathological /disorganizational model to a The influence of the extended family among
strength/resilient model. This shift is characterized by African Americans is important because of the large
(a) an examination of African Americans in an African number of female-headed households that require
American sociocultural context, ( b) a consideration of child-rearing assistance and economic support (M.
the role of grandmothers and other extended family Wilson, 1992). The proportion of African American
members in child-rearing activities, and (c) an analysis households with elderly heads that have young family
of the presence of fathers rather than their absence in the members is also high, numbering about one in three
family. To this list can be added the role of grandfathers families (Pearson, Hunter, Ensminger, & Kellam,
in the transmission of family values and beliefs 1990). When coupled with the fact that many African
(McWright, 2002). American grandparents live in close proximity to their
The African American family is a term used to married children and families, African American
characterize a group of people who are biologically grandparents have many opportunities to influence the
and spiritually bonded or connected and whose mem- development of their grandchildren. Pearson et al.
bers relations to each other and the outside world are (1990) found that in multigenerational households,
governed by a particular set of cultural beliefs, histori- mothers were the primary caregivers, followed by
cal experiences and behavioral practices (Nobles, grandmothers and then fathers. Grandmothers also
Goddard, Cavil, & George, 1987, p. 22). Sudarkasa showed more supportive behaviors in mother-grand-
(1993) notes that to understand African American fam- mother families than in mother-father-grandmother
ilies, households, and children, it is important to under- families. In mother-absent families, grandmothers
stand that these groupings evolved from an African were more involved in control and punishment of chil-
family structure where coresidential families were the dren. Tolson and Wilson (1990) found that the pres-
norm. The type and quality of adaptations to enslave- ence of grandmothers in African American families
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 477

increases the moral-religious emphasis in the house- American children live in two-parent families
hold. Nobles et al. (1987) note that such religious em- (U.S. Census Bureau, 1998). According to R. Hill
phasis helps to sustain the African American family (1988), this decline occurred as the result of economic
and reinforce the sense of family and family solidar- recessions in the 1970s, which were experienced as
ity. Although some research suggests that children are depressions in the African American community. De-
better adjusted in grandmother households (Staples & spite the statistical norm of two-parent families until
Johnson, 1993), other research suggests that intergen- recently, most research on African American fathers
erational conflict may offset the positive effects of focused either on fathers absence or their maladap-
grandmother presence. Using systematic observation tive responses to familial roles (Bowman, 1993). More
measures, Wakschlag, Chase-Lansdale, and Brooks- research is beginning to focus on African American
Gunn (1996), found that mothers whose interactions husband/fathers who remain with their families and
with their own mothers were characterized by an document their high level of involvement in child rear-
open, flexible, and autonomous style, provided their ing, family decision making, as well as the economic
children with more competent parenting. In addition, provider. J. McAdoo (1993) notes that African Ameri-
grandmother directness (e.g., nonconfrontational or can spouses share equally in the major decisions in the
assertive maturity demands) was positively related to family. From an exchange theory perspective, cooper-
problem parenting, whereas grandmother emotional ation in decision making has been essential because
closeness was negatively associated with problematic in most families both spouses have had to work to
parenting. These results were strongest when the overcome the lower wages earned by the husband
mother and grandmother were not living together, sug- (J. McAdoo, 1993.).
gesting that intergenerational modeling effects of par- Previous research suggests that persistent economic
enting may occur more readily from a distance. In a marginality among African American fathers may
compensatory situation where mothers must live with lessen their perceptions of the quality of family life and
grandmothers, shared child rearing may contribute to contribute to their separation from their families (Farley
tensions between the two adults, which may have neg- & Allen, 1987; W. Wilson, 1987). Bowman (1993) has
ative implications for children. In situations where fa- adopted a role strain model to investigate how African
thers live with mothers and grandmothers, paternal American males perceive economic marginality and
involvement with caregiving activities is associated how cultural resources facilitate adaptive coping. Sub-
with positive relationships with mothers and grand- jective cultural strengths, which are transmitted across
mothers (Krishnakumar & Black, 2003). When grand- generations, appear to reduce harmful effects of
mothers reported a positive relationship with both provider role barriers among husband-fathers and to fa-
mother and father, the mother reported a positive rela- cilitate coping. African American husband/fathers were
tionship with the father, leading the authors to suggest much more likely to have jobs (75%) than unmarried fa-
that grandmothers may play a gatekeeping role in thers (58%); joblessness is a major factor distinguishing
these intergenerational families (Krishnakumar & unmarried fathers from traditional husband/fathers. Ed-
Black, 2003). ucation and employment opportunities are increasing the
The role of grandfathers has also begun to receive at- population of middle-income African American fami-
tention. Given that two-parent households were the plu- lies. However, research with such families is lagging.
rality in the African American community before 1980, Due to discrimination, middle-income African Ameri-
many grandfathers are currently involved in the social- can parents face difficulties not encountered by affluent
ization of grandchildren. In a study of the transmission parents of some other racial groups. According to Strom
of family values through the use of proverbs, McWright and colleagues (2000), affluent fathers who grew up in
(2002) found that grandfathers influence was greatest low-income areas teach children to distinguish between
in the area of family connectedness. right and wrong and to expect punishment for breaking
Male-present households were the norm in poor rules. As a result, adolescents saw their fathers as overly
African American communities in the period between strict and seldom or never patient. Interestingly, fathers
1880 and 1925 (Staples & Johnson, 1993). Until the agreed with their childrens assessment. Smetana and
1980s, most African American families included two Chuang (2001) also found that middle-class parents
parents; today, approximately 35% of all African rated limiting adolescents behavior as more important
478 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

than encouraging independence, suggesting that parents tile social environment they will encounter as African
view firm limits necessary to maintain social order Americans, it is necessary to teach them to be comfort-
given their particular social ecology. able with their Blackness (Harrison, 1985; Peters,
Working-class African American fathers use more 1985). Bowman and Howards (1985) study of a national
physical than verbal discipline and deliver it in accor- sample of African American three-generational families
dance to the transgressions consequences rather than indicated that the manner in which parents oriented
the childs intent (Staples & Johnson, 1993). Although their children toward racial barriers was a significant
these parents use physical discipline, they rarely couple element in childrens motivation, achievement, and
this with withdrawal of love from children, which may prospects for upward mobility. Parents of successful
eliminate some of the anxiety and resentment associated children emphasized ethnic pride, self-development,
with this method. Because African American socializa- awareness of racial barriers, and egalitarianism in their
tion stresses obedience to adults and physical discipline, socialization practices. Using a national sample, Thorn-
parents have often been described as harsh, rigid, and ton, Chatters, Taylor, and Allen (1990) also report that
strict (P. Portes, Dunham, & Williams, 1986). The dis- two out of three African American parents indicate that
ciplinary style of African American parents is some- they either spoke or acted in a manner intended to
times referred to as being parent centered rather than racially socialize children. African American parents
child centered because it does not focus on the desires of envisioned racial socialization as involving messages re-
the child (Kelley, Power, & Wimbush, 1992). These de- garding their experiences as minority group members,
scriptions fail to take into account the settings in which themes emphasizing individual character and goals, and
parents raise children and the adaptive value of this ap- information related to African American cultural her-
proach to child rearing. Growing up in dangerous neigh- itage. In addition, Thornton et al. (1990), found that (a)
borhoods brings with it greater risks for involvement in older parents were more likely than younger parents to
antisocial behavior. Under these circumstances, strict view racial socialization information as a necessary ele-
obedience to parental authority is an adaptive strategy ment of socialization; ( b) that mothers were more likely
that parents may endeavor to maintain through physical than fathers to educate children about race; (c) that
discipline (Dodge, McLoyd, & Lansford, 2005; Kelley never-married parents both women and men, were less
et al., 1992). This disciplinary method may also serve to likely than their married counterparts to racially social-
impress on children the importance of following rules in ize their children; (d) that fathers in the Northeast were
society and the consequences incurred from breaking more likely than those in the South to racially socialize
those rules when one is a member of an ethnic/racial children; and (e) that mothers living in racially inte-
group that is unfairly stereotyped as violent (Willis, grated neighborhoods were more likely to socialize their
1992). There is considerable diversity in the disciplinary children to racial matters than were mothers living in
methods used by African American parents. Younger all-African American communities. Murray and Man-
mothers raising their children alone use more physical dara (2002) found that African American youth exposed
discipline. Mothers with less education use more re- to race empowerment strategies were higher in racial
strictive disciplinary practices that include insensitiv- identity and self-concept than those exposed to a race-
ity, inflexibility, and inconsistent parental behavior. defensiveness strategy, which taught dislike of other
Mothers who are more involved in organized religion groups and the usefulness of acting White. They con-
also express more child-oriented disciplinary attitudes clude that, a proactive racial socialization agenda
(Kelley et al., 1992). According to Whaley (2000), buffers and prepares African American children to face
African American parents use of spanking is more a the challenges of racial discrimination (p. 89).
consequence rather than a cause of problem behaviors,
unlike European American families where the positive American Indian Families. In the recent past,
association between spanking and child behavior prob- American Indians were known as the Vanishing Amer-
lems is bidirectional. icans. Since 1940, however, the American Indian popu-
An important socialization goal of many ethnic mi- lation has increased at every census count. The Indian
nority parents is fostering a sense of ethnic/racial pride population was 345,000 in 1940, but in 2000 numbered
in children (Harrison et al., 1990). Some parents believe over 2,400,000 American Indians and Alaskan Natives
that for their children to successfully confront the hos- (U.S. Census Bureau, 2002). American Indians are a so-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 479

cioculturally diverse group consisting of over 450 dis- social problems faced by these families (e.g., poverty,
tinct tribal units who speak over 100 different languages alcoholism, accidents, and adolescent suicide) the ma-
(Trimble & Medicine, 1993). Typically, American Indi- jority are two-parent families. In 1990, nearly 7 out of
ans prefer their tribal designation over American Indian 10 American Indian families included married couples
(Burgess, 1980). The Navajo of New Mexico and Ari- living together (U.S. Census Bureau, 1995).
zona are the largest tribe, with more than 170,000 mem- Although there are variations among tribes in their
bers. Other large tribes include the Cherokee, the Sioux, value orientations, some common themes can be charac-
the Chippewa, the Aleuts, and the Eskimos. terized as traditional American Indian values. These in-
Today, approximately 70% of American Indians live clude (a) present-time orientationa primary concern
off reservations (Banks, 1991), mostly in urban areas, for the present and acceptance of time as fluid and not
although most research on American Indians focuses on segmented; ( b) respect for elderswith age comes ex-
those living on reservations. Due to cultural differences perience that is transmitted as knowledge that is essen-
and discrimination, many American Indians have a dif- tial for group survival and harmony in life; (c) identity
ficult time adjusting to life in urban areas. For this rea- with groupself-awareness has meaning only in the
son, many reservation American Indians who migrate to context of the family and tribe so that the interests of the
urban areas tend to settle in cities and towns near reser- family and tribe are the same as ones own self-interest;
vations and to maintain contact with their family on the (d) cooperation and partnershipamong the Pueblo In-
reservation (Banks, 1991). Such living arrangements dians a common saying is Help each other so the bur-
close to the reservation are more conducive to the de- den wont be so heavy (Suina & Smolkin, 1994,
velopment of biculturalism than when Indians live in p. 121); (e) the concept of partnership is stressed as the
large urban areas removed from reservations. Contact desirable way of conducting most activities; and (f ) liv-
with European Americans and other cultural groups, ing in harmony with naturenature, like time, is indi-
such as Latinos, has introduced changes in traditional visible, and the person is an integral part of the flow of
Indian values and behaviors. Consequently, it is neces- nature and time.
sary to consider level of acculturation in research in- Traditional American Indian values constitute a
volving American Indians even though most studies worldview that is fundamentally different from Western
with American Indians describe the tribe (e.g., Navajo assumptions about the proper relationships among
or Sioux) without reference to the study participants people, the environment, and time. Trimble and Medicine
level of acculturation. (1993) argue that in its present form, psychology cannot
American Indian families may be characterized as a properly describe and explain traditional-oriented Amer-
collective cooperative social network that extends from ican Indian affective and behavioral patterns. A tradi-
the mother and father union to the extended family and, tional-oriented American Indian worldview represents an
ultimately, the community and tribe (Burgess, 1980). indigenous psychology (U. Kim & Berry, 1993;
American Indian tribes are resilient in that they have Ramirez, 1983) that can only be understood when exam-
withstood attempts at extermination, removal from their ined from ecological and sociohistorical perspectives
traditional lands, extreme poverty, removal of their chil- that do not assume the superiority of a Western world-
dren to boarding schools, loss of self-governance, and view. For example, in the Iroquoian language, uncles and
assimilationist policies aimed at destroying Indian lan- aunts are called fathers and mothers, respectively. This
guages, traditions, dress, religions, and occupations naming practice implies that parenting is spread across
(Harjo, 1993). A strong extended-family system and several extended family members. Thus, the parents pri-
tribal identity characterizes many urban and rural mary role in tribal culture is to give children affection
American Indian families (Harrison et al.1990). Ameri- and support, whereas supervision and discipline are usu-
can Indian patterns of extended family include several ally provided by aunts and uncles (Machamer & Gruber,
households representing significant relatives that give 1998). This may lead to the misperception that parenting
rise to village-like characteristics even in urban areas. is permissive when viewed from a nuclear family per-
In such families, grandparents retain an official sym- spective. When viewed from an extended family perspec-
bolic leadership role. Children seek daily contact with tive, however, parenting involves both affection and the
grandparents, who monitor childrens behavior and have setting of limits. According to Machamer and Gruber
a voice in child rearing (Lum, 1986). Despite the many (1998), the loss of extended family by American Indian
480 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

adolescents as a result of out-migration (from reserva- ond area of knowledge involves learning an occupation or
tions) signifies the loss of a principal mechanism through the means to making a living. This learning often re-
which values are transmitted and accountability is quires an apprenticeship and involves a narrower range of
learned and enforced (p. 367). Differences in world- teaching experts such as herders, weavers, and hunters.
views represent important theoretical, conceptual, and Learning is through listening, watching, and doing, with
methodological challenges facing researchers studying a strong emphasis placed on modeling and private prac-
American Indian populations (see Table 8.1). Method- ticing of the emerging skill (Suina & Smolkin, 1994). At
ological and ethical considerations are beginning to this stage, children learn the appropriate context for the
receive attention in the literature (Hudson & Taylor- use of their knowledge. The person learns how knowl-
Henley, 2001; Stubben, 2001). edge is enmeshed with the history, culture, and survival
In traditional-oriented Indian culture, the uses of goals of the tribe. The third category of knowledge is the
knowledge and learning are prescribed to help individu- most restrictive because it is reserved for those inter-
als live fulfilling lives as fully integrated members of the ested in becoming healers and religious leaders (Joe,
family and tribe. For example, among the Navajo, knowl- 1994). These are lifetime commitments involving spe-
edge is organized around three life goals. First, there is cialized instruction that is usually in addition to learning
knowledge that lasts throughout ones lifetime and con- other means of livelihood.
cerns language, kinship, religion, customs, values, be- Differences in the uses and value of knowledge and
liefs, and the purpose of life (Joe, 1994). This kind of education have implications for the relative effective-
knowledge is usually taught informally and using a vari- ness of tribal schools versus public schools. Because
ety of sources. Among the Pueblo Indians, teaching and tribal schools are embedded in a supportive cultural con-
learning at this stage is thought to be the responsibility of text, they may ameliorate the cultural conflicts in class-
all Pueblo members (Suina & Smolkin, 1994). The sec- rooms that can arise when children attend public schools

TABLE 8.1 American Indian Culture versus Dominant Culture Values


American Indian Culture Dominant Culture
Support of large extended family Support of, and from, immediate
family
Slower, softer speech Louder, faster speech
Avoids speaker and listener with little Addressed listener directly, often
or no eye contact by name
Interjects less Interrupts frequently
Deep sense of humor Light humor
Allows time for thought Gives instant answers to questions
Nonverbal communication prized Verbal skills highly prized
Cooperation Competition
Group needs are most important Personal goals are most important
Harmony with nature Power and control of nature
Control of self not others Control of self and others
Sharing, keep only what one needs Material things are important
Time is here, be patient Time is very important, get things
done
Noninterference Need to control all
Patient, allows others to go first Aggressive and competitive
Talk about good things before criticizing Criticism is immediate, blunt, to
the point
Group-centered society; group emphasis Individual-centered society, self-
emphasis

Source: From Working with and Conducting Research among American Indian Families, by
J. D. Stubben, 2001, American Behavioral Scientist, 44, pp. 14661481. Reprinted with permission.
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 481

where they are exposed to societal values and prejudices. Although infant death at birth is among the lowest of any
Wall and Madak (1991) found that students attending racial /ethnic group, the rate of sudden infant death syn-
tribal schools felt that their parents and favorite teacher drome (SIDS) is three times the national average. In ad-
held higher educational aspirations for them than their dition, due to high rates of alcoholism, American Indian
peers in public schools. The greater family connected- children have a 500% greater chance of being born with
ness found on reservations is also positively associated Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (Fuller, 2004). Indian Health
with more favorable attitudes toward school and less Services and greater tribal self-determination in the
risk-taking behavior among adolescents (Machamer & areas of education (the Indian Self-Determination and
Gruber, 1998). Public schools may subscribe to aca- Education Assistance Act of 1975), family life (The In-
demic expectations and practices that are at variance dian Child Welfare Act of 1978), and culture (The
with American Indian learning styles, which can con- American Indian Religious Freedom Act of 1978) have
tribute to poorer achievement and lower motivation. Del- made it possible for some tribes to sustain healthy fami-
pit (1995) notes that the American Indian prohibition lies and to recover traditional child-rearing practices.
against speaking for others makes tasks such as writing The role of fathers in the socialization of children, espe-
summaries or book reports difficult for some American cially boys, has special implications for the survival of
Indian children. When given such assignments, Ameri- tribal cultural traditions. An exploratory study with
can Indian children often prefer to write about their own Ojibwa families found a positive association between
experiences (Delpit, 1995) and their families experi- the amount of time fathers spent as primary caregivers
ences, especially those of their grandparents. and their sons academic performance and social devel-
The establishment of boarding schools for American opment (Williams, Radin, & Coggins, 1996). In addi-
Indian children, far removed from the reservation, was tion, fathers with more paternal involvement also had
an attempt to destroy traditional child-rearing practices fathers who participated more in their own upbringing.
(Harjo, 1993). Between 1890 and 1920, children were The significance of father involvement for cultural sur-
forcibly removed from their families for up to 12 years, vival may arise from the expectation that as boys grow
and parents and relatives were not allowed to visit chil- into adulthood they will assume gender-prescribed lead-
dren during the school year (Harjo, 1993). From 1920 to ership roles and therefore need appropriate role models.
the 1970s, boarding schools were still a usual part of the Leadership expectations for their sons may promote
childhood and adolescent experience of most American greater father participation in child rearing because fa-
Indian children. Among the many deleterious effects of thers are more involved in community leadership activi-
boarding schools was the deprivation of children from ties (Williams et al., 1996).
adult parenting models and an undermining of parental Zimmerman, Ramirez, Washienko, Walter, and Dyer
authority. Several boarding schools currently exist, al- (1998) have proposed an enculturation hypothesis to
though attendance is voluntary. Today, many American explain how involvement with American Indian culture
Indian grandparents and parents are products of govern- buffers children from the negative effects of accultura-
ment boarding schools, which impacts on the quality of tion, which is associated with alcohol and substance
their parenting. Having been deprived of a parent-child abuse. They developed an empirically derived measure
relationship during their childhood, many American In- of enculturation that included, (a) cultural affinity, ( b)
dian parents now experience problems in relations with family activities, and (c) Native American identity. In
their own children. Child abuse, particularly in the form their research with Odawa and Ojibwa tribal members,
of neglect, is cited as a major reason for the removal of they found that cultural affinity positively predicted
Indian children from their families and tribes. Ameri- youths self-esteem. Youth with the highest levels of
can Indian children are placed out of homes at a rate self-esteem and cultural identity had the lowest levels of
five times higher than other children (Harjo, 1993; alcohol and substance abuse, which was consistent with
Spicer, 1998). the enculturation hypothesis. Zimmerman et al. (1998)
Two major concerns of some tribes are infant health concluded that enculturation is an important protective
and the child-rearing abilities of adolescents, which are factor for American Indian youth. The findings also
critical for the tribes survival (Berlin, 1987). American have implications for preventing depression and reduc-
Indian teens are nearly two and a half times more likely ing suicide, which is three times the national average
to become pregnant before reaching their 18th birthday. among American Indian youth (Fuller, 2004).
482 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Asian American Families. The Asian American structure is also applied to the family, and each mem-
population includes people from 28 Asian countries or bers role is dictated by age and gender. Typically, Asian
ethnic groups. It is a very diverse group in terms of lan- American families are seen as patriarchal, with the fa-
guages, cultures, number of generations in the United ther maintaining authority and emotional distance from
States, and reasons for immigrating to the United States. other members (Ho, 1986; Wong, 1988, 1995). Tradi-
The first Asian immigrants came from China in the tionally, the family exerts control over family members,
1840s. However, due to discriminatory immigration who are taught to place family needs before individual
policies against Asians, the number of Asian Americans needs. Children show obedience and loyalty to their par-
remained low until recently. The growth of the Asian ents and, especially for male children, are expected to
American population in recent years has been accompa- take care of elderly parents (filial piety). In many Asian
nied by shifts in ethnicity and national origin. Before countries, subjugation of personal will to elders is an in-
1970, Japanese Americans were the largest Asian ethnic dicator of maturity and persists in intergenerational re-
group (41%), followed by Chinese (30%), and Filipinos lationships among Asian American adolescents (Ying,
(24%). Today, the largest groups, in millions, are Chi- Coombs, & Lee, 1999). Confucian influences on family
nese Americans (2.7), Filipino Americans (2.4), Asian life are stronger in some Asian American populations
Indians (1.9), Vietnamese Americans (1.2), Korean (e.g., Chinese and Vietnamese) than others (e.g., Japa-
Americans (1.2), and Japanese Americans (1.1; U.S. nese) due to differences in immigration patterns and de-
Census Bureau, 2002). gree of Westernization of the country of origin. Length
Historically, Japanese, Chinese, and Filipino immi- of U.S. residence and acculturation also contribute to
grants came to the United States primarily to improve extensive within-group differences in family structure
their economic status. However, Indo-Chinese have ar- and roles. Kibria (1993) found that large Vietnamese
rived primarily as political immigrants or refugees. At families varying in age and gender fared better econom-
the end of the Vietnam War in 1975, 130,000 refugees ically than smaller nuclear families. The larger extended
found asylum in the United States. Beginning in 1978, family enabled households to connect to a variety of so-
a massive flow of Indo-Chinese refugees ( boat people cial and economic resources. In Vietnamese families,
or second wave) occurred, abruptly ending in 1992 the kin group is seen as more important than the indi-
(Rumbaut, 1995). Refugees may suffer more psycho- vidual. This perspective has its source in Confucian
logical problems and have a more difficult time adjust- principles, especially ancestor worship (Kibria, 1993).
ing to life in the United States than economic Ancestor worship for Vietnamese Americans consists of
immigrants. They tend to experience more undesirable devotion in caring for an altar containing pictures of de-
change in the process of acculturation, a greater threat ceased family members and praying at ritually pre-
of danger, and a decreasing sense of control over their scribed times (Chao, 1992). This act affirms the
lives (Rumbaut, 1991). sacredness, unity, and timelessness of the kin group.
Little research exists on the structure and process of Aspects of traditional Asian child-rearing practices
Asian American families. Most studies have sampled appear to be continued by Asian American families
from Chinese and Japanese American populations. (Uba, 1994). Studies tend to be focused primarily on
Often, examination of Asian American families social- characteristics of parental control. Chiu (1987) com-
ization processes is for the purpose of identifying the pared the child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Chinese
family characteristics that contribute to childrens aca- American, and European American mothers. Chinese
demic performance (Huntsinger & Jose, 1995; mothers endorsed restrictive and controlling behavior
Huntsinger, Jose, & Larson, 1998). Of late, research on more than Chinese American and European American
the adaptive strategies and socialization goals of Asian mothers, and Chinese American mothers were more re-
American parents that bears on the socioemotional de- strictive and controlling in their child-rearing attitudes
velopment of children has received more attention (see than European American mothers. The intermediate po-
Okagaki & Bojczyk, 2002, for a review). Discussions of sition of Chinese American mothers suggests that their
Asian American families usually invoke Confucian child-rearing attitudes are shifting toward European
principles to explain family structure and roles. Confu- American norms due to acculturation.
cius developed a hierarchy defining a persons roles, du- Chao (1994) has argued that the traditional view of
ties, and moral obligations in the state. This hierarchical Chinese parents as authoritarian, restrictive, and con-
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 483

trolling is misleading because these parenting behaviors child-rearing values and styles that are distinct from the
do not have cross-cultural equivalence for European conventional U.S. child-rearing schemes. The same can
Americans and Chinese: these child-rearing concepts be said about the construct of parent-child closeness.
are rooted in European American culture and are not Immigrant Chinese American parents express closeness
relevant for describing the socialization styles and goals through behaviors emphasizing family harmony, whereas
of Chinese parents. The authoritarian concept has European American families express closeness through
evolved from American culture and psychology that is romantic themes that include more parent-child physical
rooted in both evangelical and Puritan religious influ- contact (Rothbaum, Morelli, Pott, & Liu-Constant, 2000).
ences (Chao, 1994, p. 1116). Instead, Chinese parent- Future research with Asian American families should
ing should be viewed from the concepts of chiao shun take into account within-group difference in child-rear-
and guan. Chiao shun means training or teaching in ing practices due to generation and acculturation. Stud-
appropriate behaviors. Parents and teachers are respon- ies have found that larger acculturation gaps between
sible for training children by exposing them to examples Asian immigrant parents and their children are associ-
of proper behavior and limiting their view of undesirable ated with more parental difficulties and communication
behaviors. However, training in the European American problems and lower parenting satisfaction (Buki, Ma,
sense is conceptualized as strict discipline. This is not Strom, & Strom, 2003; Tseng & Fuligni, 2000).
the case for Chinese, for whom training is accomplished
in the context of a supportive and concerned parent or Latino Families. The term Latino is used here to
teacher. The word guan means to govern, to care for describe those persons often referred to as Hispanics.
or to love, and parental care and involvement is seen as Hispanic is a word coined by the Department of Com-
an aspect of guan. Thus, control and governance have merce to enumerate persons in the United States whose
positive connotations for the Chinese. Chao (1994) com- ancestry derives from the Spanish-speaking countries
pared European American and immigrant Chinese and peoples of the Americas. Many people in this group
American mothers on standard measures of control and prefer Latino over Hispanic because Latino is the Span-
authoritarian parenting, as well as measures of chiao ish word for describing this group, whereas Hispanic is
shun and guan. Chinese American mothers scored an English word imposed on the group. The Latino pop-
higher on standard measures of parental control and au- ulation consists primarily of Mestizo peoples born of
thoritarian parenting. However, they also scored higher the Spanish conquest of the Americas. They are descen-
on measure reflecting the concepts of chiao shun and dents of Spanish fathers and American Indian women.
guan. Thus, although Chinese American mothers scored Although the language of Mestizos is Spanish, much of
high on the European American concepts of parental their culture is a hybrid of Spanish and American Indian
control and authoritarian parenting, their parenting influences. Child rearing, in particular, is heavily influ-
style could not be described using European American enced by American Indian cultures because it was the
concepts. Instead, the style of parenting used by the American Indian women who bore the children of the
Chinese American mothers is conceptualized as a type Spanish conquistadores and raised them in their ex-
of training performed by parents who are deeply con- tended families. As these children grew, they formed
cerned and involved in the lives of their children. unions with other Mestizos and American Indians and
The value of this approach is that is helps resolve para- extended the predominately American Indian child-rear-
doxes in the literature. In studies on ethnicity achieve- ing practices across generations through their children.
ment, Steinberg, Dornbusch, and Brown (1992) found that Latinos are now the largest minority group in the
Asian American students rated their parents higher on au- nation. Mexican Americans make up the vast majority of
thoritarian parenting than European American and His- Latinos (67%), followed by Central and South Ameri-
panic groups. Although their parents were lower on the cans (14%), Puerto Ricans (9%), and Cuban Americans
optimal parental style of authoritativeness, Asian students (4%; U.S. Census Bureau, 2002). It is estimated that if
had the highest achievement scores (Steinberg et al., current immigration trends continue, more than half the
1992). Chaos (1994) study suggests that confusion be- Latino population for the next 50 years will be made up
tween authoritarian and training child-rearing con- of immigrants and their children (Edmonston & Passel,
cepts among Chinese respondents may account for the 1994). The Latino population will be characterized by
paradox. Chinese parents may have a different set of sociocultural diversity and change as new immigrants
484 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

and their children adapt to life in the United States. The peoplehood shared by persons of the Americas who are
direction and nature of this change will be influenced by of Mestizo ancestry. The concept of La Raza suggests
the sociocultural ecology of the United States, which it- that many Latinos have a more flexible definition of
self is being gradually transformed by the growing num- race than the rigid categories typically used in the
bers of Latinos and Asians of immigrant families. For United States and by the Census Bureau. The Census
many Latinos, especially Mexican Americans, retention Bureau and demographers consider Latinos an ethnic
of their ethnic and cultural identity is fostered by several group belonging to the White race. However, when re-
factors that include: (a) proximity to the United States- sponding to the question on race in the 2000 census,
Mexican border, ( b) the former historical status of the 48% of Latinos reported only White, whereas approxi-
United States Southwest as a part of Mexico, (c) the con- mately 42% reported only Some Other Race (U.S.
tinuing growth of the Latino population, (d) mass media Census Bureau, 2001a). In contrast, only 0.2% of the
and communication between the United States and Latin total non-Latino population reported Some Other
America, and (e) a non-European phenotype that marks Race. These data point toward the need for a critical re-
Latinos in this country as others (Buriel, 1994). examination of how ethnic and racial identity are con-
Ramirez and Castaneda (1974) have described the cul- ceptualized and measured among Latinos.
tural values of Latinos by four conceptual categories: (1)
identification with family, community, and ethnic group; Personalization of Interpersonal Relationships. Latino
(2) personalization of interpersonal relationships; (3) culture places a heavy emphasis on sensitivity to the so-
status and roles based on age and gender; and (4) Latino cial domain of the human experience. Individuals are
Catholic ideology. The following discussion of these val- socialized to be sensitive to the feelings and needs of
ues recognizes that there are important subgroup varia- others and to personalize interpersonal relationships
tions (e.g., Cuban American, Mexican American, Puerto (personalismo). This socialization goal encourages the
Rican, and Central and South American) and variations development of cooperative social motives while dis-
due to acculturation, generation, and social class. couraging individual competitive behaviors that set
apart the individual from the group (Kagan, 1984).
Identification with Family, Community, and Ethnic The importance of the social domain for Latinos is
Group. Latino child-rearing practices encourage the reflected in the term bien educado, which means well
development of a self-identity embedded firmly in the educated. In Latino culture, however, the term is used
context of the familia (family). Ones individual identity to refer not only to someone with a good formal educa-
is therefore part of a larger identity with the familia. For tion but also to a person who can function successfully
many Latinos, the word family refers to a combination in any interpersonal situation without being disrespect-
of nuclear and extended family members, including fic- ful. Okagaki and Sternberg (1993) found that Mexican
tive kin such as godparents. immigrant parents emphasized social skills as equal to
The desire to be close to the familia often results in or as more important than cognitive skills in defining an
many members of the same familia living in the same intelligent child. Children in particular are expected
community. The familia network extends further into to be bien educados in their relations with adults
the community through kinships formed by intermar- (Valdes, 1996). Addressing adults in Spanish with the
riage among familias and el compadrazco, which is the formal you (usted) rather than the informal you (tu)
cultural practice of having special friends become god- is an example of being bien educado. Thus, if children
parents of children in baptisms. Adults united through el lose Spanish and cannot communicate with Spanish
compadrazco, called compadres and comadres, have mu- monolingual adults, they may be unable to achieve the
tual obligations to each other similar to those of brothers status of being bien educado in their community.
and sisters. Vidal (1988) found that Puerto Rican god-
parents served as role models and social supports for Status and Roles Based on Age and Gender. Latino cul-
their godchildren and regarded themselves as potential ture has clearly defined norms of behavior governing an
surrogate parents in the event of the parents death. Ex- individuals actions in the familia and the community.
tended familia ties in the community give rise to a sense Age and gender are important determinants of status
of identity with ones community. and respect. Children are expected to be obedient and
The worldview of many Latinos includes a sense of respectful toward their parents, even after they are
identity with La Raza (the Race), which is a sense of grown and have children of their own. An authoritarian
The Impact of Social Change on Family Socialization 485

parenting style has been reported among Latinos (Dorn- have their first child after marriage. Female-headed
busch, Ritter, Leiderman, Roberts, & Fraleigh, 1987; households are twice as common among Puerto Ricans
Schumm et al., 1988). Yet, as Chao (1994) has shown, in relative to Mexican Americans and Cuban Americans
the context of non-Western cultures, this parenting style (see Cauce & Domenech-Rodriguez, 2002, for a
may be experienced as parental support and concern. summary of family characteristics between Latino
Grandparents, and older persons in general, receive re- subgroups).
spect and have considerable status owing to their knowl- The longer immigrants live in the United States, the
edge of life. Consequently, children are taught to model more their family networks expand. Family networks
themselves after adults; as a result, modeling becomes a grow through marriage and birth and from continued
preferred teaching style (Laosa, 1980). immigration of family members. Thus, even as individ-
Gender also influences a persons role in the familia ual family members become acculturated, their local ex-
and community. Males are expected to have more knowl- tended family becomes larger (Keefe & Padilla, 1987).
edge about politics and business, whereas females are Second- and third-generation Mexican Americans have
expected to know more about child rearing, health care, larger and more integrated extended families than immi-
and education. Because politics and business expose grants (Keefe & Padilla, 1987). Griffith and Villavicen-
males more to the outside world, they are often per- cio (1985) report that for Mexican Americans education
ceived as the dominant figures in the familia. However, and income are the best predictors of increased contact
decision-making studies in the United States reveal that and support from family members.
Latino husbands and wives most often share responsibil- Buriel (1993b) found that early assumption of respon-
ity for major family decisions (Cooney, Rogler, Hurrell, sibility was a dominant socialization goal of Mexican
& Ortiz, 1982; Zavella, 1987). immigrant parents that persists into adolescence. He also
found greater similarity in socialization styles among
Latino Catholic Ideology. Religion strongly influences immigrant mothers and fathers than among native-born
the lives of Latinos because Latino Catholicism rein- mothers and fathers. Consensus in socialization styles
forces and supports cultural values. Latino Catholicism may reflect an area of domestic interdependence condi-
is a synthesis of Spanish European Catholicism and in- tioned by the immigrant experience. Because immigrants
digenous religious beliefs and practices. Identity with lack extended kinship networks, parents may depend
family and community is facilitated through religious more on each other for socialization of children, which
practices, such as weddings and el compadrazco, which encourages agreement in parents socialization styles.
help extend family networks. Identity with the ethnic Family research has often focused on how immigra-
group is reinforced by the common Catholic religion tion and acculturation affect the adaptation of individ-
shared by more than 80% of La Raza. Religious symbols ual family members. However, the family can be viewed
are often used as markers of ethnic identity. For exam- as an adapting entity with its own developmental
ple, the image of the Virgin of Guadalupe, the Mestizo processes. The family unit undergoes its own develop-
equivalent of the Virgin Mary, is both a religious symbol ment, which transcends the development of its individ-
and a symbol for La Raza. The cultural emphasis on re- ual members. From a systems perspective (Minuchin,
spect, group harmony, and cooperation in interpersonal 2002), the Mexican American family can be thought of
relations is in line with the religious themes of peace, as an open system with both internal and external as-
community, and self-denial. Many cultural celebrations pects of functioning. Internal aspects include the fam-
and developmental milestones are celebrated in religious ilys patterns of relationships and interactions and also
contexts, such as a quinceaera,a coming-of-age cele- the structure of the family. External aspects include the
bration for a young girl on her fifteenth birthday that familys interactions with outside social systems includ-
marks the beginning of adulthood. ing social institutions and the larger context of U.S. so-
ciety. Rueschenberg and Buriel (1989) have shown that
The Role of Family in Latino Adaptation the Mexican American family is capable of adapting to
Latino families have 3.71 members on average com- U.S. social systems while retaining many of its internal
pared to 2.97 for European American and 3.31 for characteristics that are cultural in nature.
African American families. Puerto Rican women tend Research is beginning to examine how traditional the-
to have their first child before marriage, whereas Mex- ories of parenting and socialization fit with family
ican American and Cuban American mothers usually relations and child outcomes in Latino families. N. Hill,
486 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Bush, and Roosa (2003) studied Mexican American and ily and their competence in family matters were positively
European American mothers and their children to map related to their involvement with their preschool-age chil-
the relationship between harsh parenting and negative dren (Roopnarine & Ahmeduzzaman, 1993).
child outcomes. For both groups, families with low lev- In addition to the role of fathers, research on grand-
els of conflict and hostile control had children with parent involvement, particularly grandmothers, has
fewer conduct problems and depressive symptoms. In begun. Owing to the values of familism and status and
addition, in the Mexican American sample, lower accul- respect based on age and gender, grandmothers are often
turation, as measured by Spanish/ English usage, was as- the symbolic heads of extended families and are sought
sociated with a stronger negative relationship between after for advise and support in child rearing (Ramos-
maternal acceptance and child conduct problems. More- McKay, Comas-Diaz, & Rivera, 1988). The cultural di-
over, among Spanish-speaking parents, hostile control versity in the Latino population suggests that the impact
co-occurred with acceptance, which is generally incon- of grandmother support may vary as a function of accul-
sistent with the traditional European American model of turation. Using a Puerto Rican sample, Contreras,
parenting. The combination of hostile control and ac- Lopez, Rivera-Mosqueda, Raymond-Smith, and Roth-
ceptance may represent an adaptive parenting strategy stein (1999), examined how acculturation affected rela-
for families living in culturally unfamiliar environments tions between grandmother involvement and adolescent
involving high levels of acculturative stress. mothers adjustment. Greater grandmother support was
Parke et al. (2004), in a longitudinal study examining related to less symptomatology and parenting stress
a family stress model involving economic hardship, fam- among less acculturated mothers. When mothers were
ily relations, and child outcomes, found that for both more unidirectionally acculturated, greater grandmother
Mexican American and European American families, support was associated with more symptomatology and
feelings of economic hardship were positively related to parenting distress. This research, like the work with fa-
depression for both parents. In addition, paternal hostile thers, underscores the importance of the moderating role
parenting was related to higher levels of childrens inter- of acculturation in family relationships with Latinos.
nalizing and externalizing behaviors in both European
Perspectives on Ethnic Influences on
American and Mexican American families. However,
Family Socialization
among Mexican American families, maternal accultura-
tion was associated with higher levels of marital prob- Research on ethnic minority families in the United
lems and lower levels of both maternal and paternal States has not kept pace with their rapid rate of growth.
hostile parenting. Although higher maternal accultura- Until recently, ethnic/racial minority families were
tion may disrupt traditional male-centered authority lumped together under the category of minority,
patterns in the family, it may also serve as the catalyst which overlooked important differences among eth-
for altering parenting styles in a less hostile direction. nic/racial groups and significant diversity in individual
A widely misunderstood and understudied area in groups. Most often, research with ethnic/racial fami-
Latino family research involves the role of fathers in par- lies has compared them against European American
enting and socialization. The stereotype of machismo has families to identify group differences. An implicit as-
led to the belief that Latino fathers are neither caring nor sumption in two-group studies is that ethnic/racial mi-
involved with their spouses and children. However, less ac- nority families are not yet like European American
culturated Mexican American men supervise and engage families and that this developmental lag is responsible
their children in conventionally feminine activities more for the problems besetting ethnic/racial minority fami-
than their more acculturated counterparts (Coltrane, lies. Recently, some researchers have begun to eschew
Parke, & Adams, 2004). Paternal participation in family two-group studies in favor of single-group studies that
rituals, which are often cultural in nature, is positively as- examine variations in ethnic/racial minority families
sociated with monitoring and interacting with children in and their relation to child outcomes. This research is
these families (Coltrane et al., 2004). Father involvement guided by either an ecological or systems perspective
may represent an important dimension of familism, which that seeks to understand how parents adapt to the chal-
is the most important value in Latino culture. This is sup- lenges they face as ethnic/racial minorities in U.S.
ported by a study with Puerto Rican fathers, which found society and how these experiences contribute to social-
that fathers assessments of their commitment to the fam- ization goals they hold for their children.
Remaining Issues and Future Trends 487

The value of the concepts of collectivism and individ- ify the determinants and processes, and outline the con-
ualism that have been used to distinguish European sequences of family-child relationships. These include
American families from ethnic minority families the choice of the unit of analysis, the effects of family
(Greenfield & Cocking, 1994; Okagaki & Divecha, variation, types of developmental change, the role of his-
1993) is being questioned as well (Scott-Jones, Lewis, & torical change, methodological, and contextual issues.
Shirley, 1996). As our review has indicated, both indi-
vidualism and collectivism are important elements in Unit of Analysis
the socialization of both European American and ethnic
minority families and to continue to draw sharp con- Current work is recognizing the importance of consider-
trasts using these terms serves only to promote stereo- ing parents from a family systems perspective. However,
types that are no longer valid. Scott-Jones et al. (1996) our conceptual analysis of dyadic and triadic units of
have suggested the adoption of Sampsons (1988) notion analysis is still limited (Dickstein, Seifer, et al., 1998;
of ensembled individualism, which reflects the impor- Parke, 1988). Considerable progress has been made in
tance of both individual development and commitment describing the behavior of individual interactants (e.g.,
to family as complementary, intertwined elements in the mother, father, or child) in dyadic and to a lesser extent
socialization goals of ethnic families (Scott-Jones triadic settings, but less progress has been achieved in
et al., 1996, p. 8). Although the precise terminology that developing a language for describing interaction in
is best suited to capture the complexity of ethnic family dyadic and triadic terms. Even if such terms as recipro-
socialization is still evolving, it is clear that both indi- cal or synchronous hold promise, there remains little
vidual and collectivist features will continue to be rec- real advance in this regard. In addition, greater attention
ognized as important in describing ethnic families. needs to be paid to the family as a unit of analysis. A
At present, and for the foreseeable future, the growth number of researchers have offered differing tax-
of minority families will be due primarily to immigration onomies of family types or typologies that move us to
from Latin America and Asia. Research with families of this level of analysis (Cook, 2001), but, to date, little ef-
these groups needs to take into account the acculturation fort has been made to apply these notions systematically
level and generational status of parents and children and to family relationships in childhood.
the effects these factors have on family processes and
child outcomes. Together with acculturation, recognition
Parenting and Family Variation
of biculturalism as both an adaptation strategy and social-
ization goal should guide future research. The effects of One of the clear advances of the past decade is recognition
prejudice and discrimination on ethnic minorities, in such of the importance of individual differences in children;
areas as social and emotional development, ethnic/racial one of the next advances will be the recognition of indi-
identity, and achievement motivation, deserve more atten- vidual differences among families. Recognition of in-
tion. Language development research should also give dividual variability across families implies the necessity
greater attention to second-language acquisition (usually of expanding sampling procedures. Despite demands for a
English) and bilingualism and their relation to cognitive greater awareness of family diversity, the range of family
development and school achievement. More attention must types that are studied is still relatively narrow. Although
be given to the role of fathers, grandparents, and extended progress has been made in describing interaction patterns
family members in the socialization of children. Finally, of parents and children in different cultures (Rogoff,
observational studies with ethnic/racial minority families 2003) and in different ethnic groups in the United States
have the potential for yielding a contextual richness about (Contreras, Kerns, & Neal-Barnett, 2002; McLoyd, Hill,
the family environments of ethnic/racial minorities that & Dodge, 2005; McLoyd & Steinberg, 1998), this work
can lead to more culturally relevant insights and theories represents only a beginning. Particularly critical is an ex-
about their socialization experiences. tension of earlier work on parent-child relationships in
other ethnic groups to other subsystems such as the mari-
REMAINING ISSUES AND FUTURE TRENDS tal, sibling, and family systems. Evaluation of family
systems notions in families with different ethnic back-
A number of issues remain to be examined in future re- grounds, organizations, and structure will provide an op-
search if we are to describe fully the complexities, spec- portunity to test the generality of this theoretical
488 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

perspective. Finally, recent work on gay and lesbian fami- and which processes are less amenable to change (Parke,
lies (Patterson, 2002) and the effects of growing up in 2004a). For example, the structural dynamics of early
these alternative family arrangements on children is rais- interaction (Stern, 1977) and some qualitative aspects
ing important questions about the necessity of male- of early parent-infant interactive style may be insulated
female family arrangements for the rearing of children from the influence of secular shifts. Are fathers biologi-
(Parke, 2002). cally prepared to interact in a more physical way, and
mothers in a more verbal mode? If this assumption
Types of Developmental Change about differences in parental play style is true, rates of
interactions would be more likely to change than style
Developmental issues need to be addressed more fully to when employment opportunities for men and women be-
include children at a wider range of ages. Despite Mac- come more equal. Alternatively, the restraints may be
cobys (1984) plea, we still are only beginning to map more solely environmental, and as opportunities for
developmental changes in parental socialization strate- adult male and female participation in child care and
gies. Moreover, we need to move beyond childhood and child rearing equalize, some maternal-paternal stylistic
examine more closely parental relationships with their differences may diminish.
adult childrenif we are to achieve a life-span view of
family socialization. Although development tradition-
Methodological and Design Issues
ally has marked change in the individual child, it is evi-
dent from this review that this perspective is too limited It is likely that not a single methodological strategy will
and parents, as well as children, continue to develop suffice to understand the development of family social-
across time (Elder, 1998; Parke, 1988). Parents man- ization. Instead, a wide range of designs and data collec-
agement of a variety of life-course tasks, such as mar- tion and data analysis strategies is necessary. To date,
riage, work, and personal identity, will clearly there is still a paucity of information concerning inter-
determine how they will execute parental tasks; in turn, relations across molar and molecular levels of analysis.
these differences may find expression in measures of However, it is becoming increasingly clear that a micro-
parent-child interaction. Because developmental shifts analytic strategy is not always more profitable in de-
in childrens perceptual, cognitive, and social develop- scribing relationships among interactive partners; in
ment may alter parental attitudes and behaviors and/or some cases, ratings may be a more useful approach. A
the nature of the adults own developmentally relevant set of guidelines concerning the appropriate level of
choices, such as work or career commitment, this clearly analysis for different questions would be helpful.
argues for the recognition of two developmental trajecto- Parents own reports are increasingly being recog-
riesa child developmental course and an adult devel- nized as important sources that can aid in interpretation
opmental sequence. The description of the interplay of observed patterns (Goodnow, 2002; Parke, 1978).
between these two types of developmental curves is nec- Types of self-reports other than structured interviews
essary to capture adequately the nature of developmental and questionnaires need to be more frequently em-
changes in a families role in the socialization process. ployed. Focus groups are commonly used in other disci-
plines but are used less often by psychologists. A focus
Monitoring Secular Trends group is a type of group interview, which relies on an
emergent process of interaction between group mem-
There is a continuing need to monitor secular trends and bers to produce data and insights that would not be
to describe their impact on family relationships (Modell found without such interaction. Focus groups are excel-
& Elder, 2002). Secular change is complex and clearly lent forums in which to explore microlevel experiences
does not affect all individuals equally or all behavior of families and examine similarities and differences
patterns to the same extent. It is a serious oversimplifi- across different genders, socioeconomic classes, and
cation to assume that general societal trends can isomor- ethnic groups. These provide a unique opportunity for
phically be applied across all individual children, parents and children to articulate their concerns, values,
parents, and families. Moreover, better guidelines are and goals in a context that is less constrained than the
necessary to illuminate which particular processes in usual interview format. This technique is of particular
families are most likely to be altered by historical events value in the early stages of research with understudied
Remaining Issues and Future Trends 489

populations (see Gomel et al., 1998, and Parke et al., ing the range of family members or subsystems that are
2003, for illustrations of the use of focus groups with measured, to include nontargeted individuals and sub-
African American and Hispanic American groups). As systems, the impact of modifying the behavior of one
Rutter (2002) has argued, qualitative and quantitative family member on other parts of the family system
approaches are not incompatible. Quantitative analytic could be evaluated. These interventions can serve as a
strategies have recently become available for use with vehicle for evaluation of alternative theoretical views of
the type of qualitative verbal reports generated by focus parenthood and socialization. In addition to interven-
groups (Richards & Richards, 1991). Recent computer tion designs, natural experiments continue to be a useful
programs (e.g., Nudist, Nonnumerical Unstructured tool for aiding us in sorting out causal issues (Rutter,
Data Indexing, or Searching and Theory Building) allow 2002). For example, recent work on adopted Romanian
the researcher to explore patterns in the data that aid in children has shown that the length of institutionaliza-
grounded theory construction, while simultaneously al- tion is a major predictor of later functioning (Rutter,
lowing for the application of a coding scheme to the Pickles, Murray, & Eaves, 2001).
transcript text and then converting the coded text into More recognition of the utility of combined sampling
quantitative information. Recently, Gomel et al. (1998) strategies is needed. Recently, researchers have begun to
successfully utilized this program in a focus group use multistage sampling strategies in which survey level
study of the impact of economic downturn on ethnic mi- information on a national representative sample is se-
nority families. Ethnographic methodologies can play an cured in combination with the selection of a subsample
important role in family research as well, particularly to for purposes of more intensive analyses such as in-depth
gain a better understanding of contextual factors that interviews or observations (Reiss, Neiderhiser, Hether-
affect parental functioning (see Burton, & Price- ington, & Plomin, 2000). Together, this strategy ensures
Spratlen, 1999). greater confidence in the generalizability of the find-
Reliance on nonexperimental strategies may be insuf- ings and permits access to process-level variables.
ficient to address the important issue of direction of ef- For research designs, under the influence of the be-
fects in work on the impact of parents on children and havior geneticists (Plomin, 1994), there has been an in-
families. Experimental strategies have been underuti- creased focus on the value of nonshared environmental
lized in studies of families. By experimentally modify- designs, which allow measurement of the differential
ing either the type of parental behavior or level of impact of families on different children in the same
involvement, firmer conclusions concerning the direct family. Although some have argued for the decreased
causative role that parents play in modifying their chil- use of traditional between-family designs that still form
drens and their spouses development will be possible. the foundation of most of our knowledge of family ef-
As Cowan and Cowan (2002) recently argued, interven- fects (Plomin, 1994), others (e.g., Hoffman, 1991) have
tion studies provide the gold standard for testing argued for the continued utility of both types of designs
causal hypotheses. Intervention studies (e.g., Fagan & with the goal of discovering what environmental condi-
Hawkins, 2001; Parke, Hymel, Power, & Tinsley, 1980) tions might lead to sibling similarity and dissimilarity
aimed at modifying fathering behavior provide models (Hoffman, 1991, p. 199). More conceptual work is
for this type of work and, by extending these studies to needed to provide guidelines concerning the value of
include measures of child, mother, and father develop- within- and between-family designs for different vari-
ment, they could provide evidence of the impact of ables and issues.
changes in parenting behavior on developmental out- The field has progressed beyond the simple environ-
comes. Moreover, these experimentally based interven- ment-gene partitioning argument (Harris, 1998) toward
tions have clear policy implications by exploring the a more complex conceptual framework that reframes the
degree of plasticity of parenting behavior. Similarly, debate as gene x environment interactions. According to
studies in which child behavior is either experimentally this view (Reiss, 2003), family processes mediate ge-
modified (Bell & Chapman, 1986) or children with netic influence in childrens outcomes, and the future
known characteristics (e.g., hyperactivity, conduct dis- challenge is to determine how this gene x environment
order) are paired with parental surrogates (Anderson, family model plays out across development. Several
Lytton, & Romney, 1986) are needed to capture the designs, including cross-fostering studies with nonhu-
bidirectionality of family influence effects. By expand- man primates (Suomi, 2000), modified sibling designs
490 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

(Reiss et al., 2000), and prospective adoption designs on childrens outcomes than previously thought. As we
(Reiss, 2003) are promising new approaches for address- have argued, families serve not just as direct influences
ing this issue. on children but as indirect influences as well in their
roles as managers, modifiers, and negotiators on behalf
of children in relation to these social institutions. How-
Contextual Issues
ever, our understanding of the ways in which families in-
Greater attention needs to be paid to the role of context in fluence their childrens socialization through their links
determining family relationships. How do parent-child with other institutions is still poorly understood. More-
interaction patterns shift between home and lab settings over, these agents and institutions (e.g., schools, or
and across different types of interaction contexts such as peers) play a direct as well as indirect role through the
play, teaching, and caregiving? Moreover, it is important family in the socialization process. Several issues need
to consider the social a well as the physical context. to be addressed. What are the unique roles that families
Recognition of the embeddedness of parents in family play in socialization? Are some kinds of outcomes
contexts is critical, and conceptualizing families as em- specifically in the family purview such as the develop-
bedded in a variety of extrafamilial social settings is ment of early social attachments? Are other outcomes
important for understanding variation in family function- influenced largely by other groups (e.g., tastes in music
ing. In this regard, it is necessary to recognize that varia- and fashion)? How does the relative role of family and
tions in family structure and in ethnicity and social class other agents shift across development? Perhaps the most
will modify significantly the ways in which social net- interesting question concerns the ways in which families
works are organized and utilized. For example, the role of coordinate their socialization roles with other agents
the extended family is much more prominent in some and institutions. Successful socialization requires a
groups, such as African American, than in other groups gradual sharing of responsibility for socialization with
(Gadsen, 1999; McLoyd, Hill, & Dodge, 2005). Simi- other groups, but we know relatively little about this
larly, single-parent families may be more directly embed- process of coordination and mutual sharing across so-
ded in a community-based social network than two-parent cialization agents. It is unhelpful to continue to posit lin-
families. Descriptions of these variations are necessary ear models of decreasing family influence across
for an adequate understanding of the role of extrafamilial development. Instead, we need models that help us un-
networks on parent and family functioning. derstand the changing nature of family influence rela-
tive to other groups and the mechanisms that maintain
Locating Families in a Network of family values and orientation after direct influence has
Socialization Inf luences subsided. Recent work on the intergenerational trans-
mission of working models of relationships (Bretherton
One of the major challenges is to determine the unique & Munholland, 1999) testifies to the prolonged influ-
contribution of families to socialization outcomes and ence of childhood socialization on later adult parenting
the limits of family effects (Harris, 1998). As Maccoby roles. This work anticipates one of the major themes of
and Martin (1983) argued in their review of the relations future research: the impact of childhood socialization
between parental functioning and characteristics of patterns on later adult developmentnot just in parent-
children, in most cases the relationships that have ap- ing but in other types of adult relationships, including
peared are not large, if one thinks in terms of the amount marital, friendship, and work relationships. Closely re-
of variance accounted for (p. 82). Their conclusion is lated is the issue of how families and other institutions
still valid, if we assume a narrow view of family influ- are linked. Earlier work has focused on family peer
ence as the direct impact of parents on their children. linkages (Parke et al., 2003), family-work ties (Perry-
However, our increased recognition of the family as a Jenkins, Repetti, & Crouter, 2000), and family-school
partner with other institutions, such as peers, schools, relationships (Epstein & Sanders, 2002). More recently,
media, religious institutions, and government policy- there is emerging evidence that the prior neglect of fam-
makers that together influence childrens development, ily links with other institutions such as the legal system
has significantly expanded our view of the familys role (Parke & Clarke-Stewart, 2003), religious institutions
in the socialization process and suggests that the fam- (Mahoney et al., 2001), social service systems (Olds
ilydirectly and indirectlymay have a larger impact et al., 1999), and health delivery organizations (Tinsley,
References 491

2003) is being corrected. More work on these cross-con- Baumrind, D. (1991). Parenting styles and adolescent development.
text relationships is needed. Nor are the processes that In R. Lerner, A. C. Peterson, & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), The ency-
clopedia of adolescence (pp. 746758). New York: Garland.
promote or constrain family involvement with other in-
Bean, F. D., Chapa, J., Berg, R. R., & Sowards, K. (1994). Educational
stitutions well understood. Finally, the bidirectional na- and sociodemographic incorporation among Hispanic immigrants
ture of the linkage needs more attention so that we to the United States. In B. Edmonston & J. S. Passel (Eds.), Immi-
understand the dynamic and mutual influence of fami- gration and ethnicity: The integration of Americas newest ar-
rivals (pp. 73100). Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
lies on other institutions and vice versa.
Beitel, A., & Parke, R. D. (1998). Paternal involvement in infancy:
The role of maternal and paternal attitudes. Journal of Family
Psychology, 12, 268288.
A Final Word
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in
studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 8195.
Families continue to play a central role in the socializa-
Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in studies using ex-
tion process but their role has undergone dramatic perimental or brief longitudinal approaches to socialization. De-
change during the past several decades. To maintain our velopmental Psychology, 22, 595603.
understanding, it is critical to monitor how changing Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model.
Child Development, 55, 8396.
ecologies of families of different ethnic backgrounds
Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., Rovine, M., & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns
are modifying the socialization of families. Only by a of marital change and parent-child interaction. Journal of Mar-
better understanding of these changes will we be able to riage and the Family, 53, 487498.
offer meaningful assistance and support for families. Berlin, I. N. (1987). Effects of changing Native American cultures on
And only by achieving these goals will we be able to ful- child development. Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 299306.
Bernal, G., Trimble, J. E., Burlew, A. K., & Leong, F. T. L. (2003).
fill our goal of providing optimal conditions for promot-
Handbook of racial and ethnic minority psychology. Thousand
ing childrens development. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Berry, J. W. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In
A. M. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, models, and some new
findings (pp. 925). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
REFERENCES
Betancourt, H., & Lopez, S. R. (1993). The study of culture, ethnic-
ity, and race in American psychology. American Psychologist,
Ahnert, L., Rickert, H., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Shared caregiving: 48, 629637.
Comparisons between home and child care settings. Developmen- Bhavnagri, N. P., & Parke, R. D. (1991). Parents as direct facilitators
tal Psychology, 36, 339351. of childrens peer relationships: Effects of age of child and sex of
Allen, S. M., & Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Moth- parent. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 423440.
ers beliefs and behavior that inhibit greater father involvement in Bianchi, S. M. (2000). Maternal employment and time with chil-
family work. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 199212. dren: Dramatic change or surprising continuity? Demography,
Anderson, K. E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1986). Mothers in- 37, 401414.
teractions with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects Bigler, R. S., Averhart, C. J., & Liben, L. S. (2003). Race and the
whom? Developmental Psychology, 22, 604609. workforce: Occupational status, aspirations, and stereotyping
Baldwin, A. L., Baldwin, C., & Cole, R. E. (1990). Stress resistant among African American children. Developmental Psychology,
families and stress-resistant children. In J. E. Rolf, A. S. Masten, 39, 572580.
K. Neuchterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective fac- Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1961). Child development: Vol. 1. A systematic
tors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 257280). New and empirical theory. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
York: Cambridge University Press. Blank, S. (1993, April). Household formation and Mexican immi-
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: grants: An alternative strategy for meeting the goals of recent mi-
Prentice-Hall. gration. Paper presented at the 20th Annual Center for Studies of
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality the Family Conference, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT.
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Bornstein, M. H., & Bradley, R. H. (Eds.). (2003). Socioeconomic sta-
Banks, J. A. (1991). Teaching strategies for ethnic studies (5th ed.). tus, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Boss, P. (1999). Ambiguous loss: Learning to live with unresolved
Barth, F. (1969). Ethnic groups and boundaries: The social organiza- grief. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
tion of culture dif ference. Boston: Little, Brown. Bossard, J. H. S., & Boll, E. (1950). Ritual in family living. Philadel-
Barth, J. M., & Parke, R. D. (1993). Parent-child relationship inf lu- phia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
ences on childrens transition to school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Bowman, P. J. (1993). The impact of economic marginality among
39, 173195. African American husbands and fathers. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.),
Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental competence Family ethnicity: Strength in diversity (pp. 120137). Thousand
through socialization. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Oaks, CA: Sage.
Child Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 346). Minneapolis, MN: University Bowman, P. J., & Howard, C. (1985). Race-related socialization, mo-
of Minnesota Press. tivation and academic achievement: A study of Black youths in
492 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

three-generation families. Journal of the American Academy of Buriel, R. (1993a). Acculturation, respect for cultural differences
Child Psychiatry, 24, 134141. and biculturalism among three generations of Mexican American
Boykin, A. W. (1983). The academic performance of Afro-American and Euro-American school children. Journal of Genetic Psychol-
children. In J. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement mo- ogy, 154, 531543.
tives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 321371). Buriel, R. (1993b). Childrearing orientations in Mexican American
San Francisco: Freeman. families: The inf luence of generation and sociocultural factors.
Boykin, A. W., & Toms, F. D. (1985). Black child socialization: A Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55, 9871000.
conceptual framework. In H. P. McAdoo & J. L. McAdoo (Eds.), Buriel, R. (1994). Immigration and education of Mexican Americans.
Black children: Social, educational, and parental environments In A. Hurtado & E. E. Garcia (Eds.), The educational achieve-
(pp. 3351). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. ment of Latinos: Barriers and successes (pp. 197226). Santa
Boyum, L. A., & Parke, R. D. (1995). The role of family emotional Cruz: Regents of the University of California.
expressiveness in the development of childrens social compe- Buriel, R. (2003, April). Methodological and conceptual issues for re-
tence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 593608. searching Mexican Americans. Paper presented at the biennial meet-
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models ing of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
in attachment relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & Buriel, R., & Cardoza, D. (1993). Mexican American ethnic labeling:
P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, An intrafamilial and intergenerational analysis. In M. E. Bernal &
and clinical applications (pp. 89111). New York: Guilford Press. G. P. Knight (Eds.), Ethnic identity: Formation and transmission
Brody, G. H., Flor, D. L., & Gibson, N. M. (1999). Linking maternal among Hispanics and other minorities (pp. 197210). Albany:
efficacy beliefs, developmental goals, parenting practices and State University of New York Press.
child competence in rural single-parent African American fami- Buriel, R., & Hurtado-Ortiz, M. T. (2000). Child care practices and
lies. Child Development, 70, 11971208. preferences among native- and foreign-born Latina mothers and
Brody, G. H., Flor, D. L., & Neubaum, E. (1998). Coparenting Euro American mothers. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences,
processes and child competence among rural African-American 22, 314331.
families. In M. Lewis & C. Feiring (Eds.), Families, risk and com- Buriel, R., Love, J. A., & DeMent, T. L. (in press). The relationship
petence (pp. 227243). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of language brokering to depression and parent-child bonding
Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & Flor, D. (1996). Parental religiosity, among Latino adolescents. In M. H. Bornstein & L. R. Cote
family processes and youth competence in rural two-parent African (Eds.), Acculturation and parent-child relationships. Mahwah, NJ:
American families. Developmental Psychology, 32, 696706. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., Smith, T., & Gibson, N. M. (1999, Buriel, R., Perez, W., DeMent, T. L., Chavez, D. V., & Moran, V. R.
April). Parent psychological functioning, family processes and sib- (1998). The relationship of language brokering to academic per-
ling relationship quality in rural two parent African American formance, biculturations, and self-efficacy among Latino adoles-
families. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society cents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 20, 283297.
for Research and Child Development, Albuquerque, NM. Buriel, R., & Saenz, E. (1980). Psychocultural characteristics of col-
Brodzinsky, D. M., & Pinderhughes, E. (2002). Parenting and child lege-bound and noncollege-bound Chicanas. Journal of Social Psy-
development in adoptive families. In M. H. Bornstien (Ed.), chology, 110, 245251.
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., Buriel, R., & Vasquez, R. (1982). Stereotypes of Mexican descent
pp. 279311). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. persons: Attitudes of three generations of Mexican Americans
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta and Anglo American adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy-
(Ed.), Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and chology, 13, 5970.
current issues (pp. 187249). Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley. Burks, V. S., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Parent and child representations
Bryant, B. (1985). The neighborhood walk: Sources of support in of social relationships: Linkages between families and peers.
middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 358378.
Development, 50(3, Serial No. 210). Burton, L. M., & Price-Spratlen, T. (1999). Through the eyes of chil-
Bryant, B. K. (1989). The childs perspective of sibling caretaking dren: An ethnographic perspective on neighborhoods and child
and its relevance to understanding social-emotional functioning development. In A. S. Masten (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child
and development. In P. G. Zukow (Ed.), Sibling interaction across Psychology: Vol. 29. Cultural processes in child development
cultures: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 143164). (pp. 7796). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
New York: Springer-Verlag. Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., & Meyers, T. (1995). Depression in
Buki, L. P., Ma, T.-C., Strom, R. D., & Strom, S. K. (2003). Chinese first-time mothers: Mother-infant interaction and depression
immigrant mothers of adolescents: Self-perceptions of accultura- chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349357.
tion effects on parenting. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction
Psychology, 9, 127140. of experience: A longitudinal study of representation and behav-
ior. Child Development, 75, 6683.
Burgess, B. J. (1980). Parenting in the Native American community. In
M. D. Fantini & R. Cardenas (Eds.), Parenting in a multicultural Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in
society: Practice and policy (pp. 6373). New York: Longman. parent-child interactions and childrens peer competency. Child
Development, 67, 22172226.
Buriel, R. (1987). Academic performance of foreign- and native-born
Mexican Americans: A comparison of first-, second-, and third- Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W.,
generation students and parents (New Directions for Latino Pub- et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal-
lic Policy Research, Working Paper No. 14). A project for the treated children. Science, 297, 851854.
Inter-University Program for Latino Research and the Social Sci- Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., & Braungart, J. M. (1992).
ence Research Council, New York. Family-peer connections: The role of emotional expressiveness
References 493

within the family and childrens understanding of emotions. Child Conger, R. D., Ge, X., Elder, G. H., Lorenz, F. O., & Simons, R. L.
Development, 63, 603618. (1994). Economic stress, coercive family process and develop-
Cauce, A. M., & Domenech-Rodriguez, M. (2002). Latino families: mental problems of adolescents. Child Development, 65, 541561.
Myths and realities. In J. M. Contreras, K. A. Kerns, & A. M. Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C.,
Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families in the United & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in African American
States: Current research and future directions (pp. 325). West- families: A replication and extension of the family stress model.
port, CT: Praeger. Developmental Psychology, 38, 179191.
Chao, C. M. (1992). The inner heart: Therapy with Southeast Asian Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K. A., & Neal-Barnett, A. M. (Eds.).
families. In L. A. Vargas & J. D. Koss-Chioino (Eds.), Working with (2002). Latino children and families in the United States. West-
culture: Psychotherapeutic intervention with ethnic minority chil- port, CT: Praeger.
dren and adolescents (pp. 157181). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Contreras, J. M., Lopez, I. R., Rivera-Mosquera, E. T., Raymond-
Chao, R. (2001, April). The role of childrens linguistic bordering Smith, L., & Rothstein, K. (1999). Social support and adjustment
among immigrant Chinese and Mexican families. Paper presented among Puerto Rican adolescent mothers: The moderating effects
at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child De- of acculturation. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 228243.
velopment, Minneapolis, MN. Cook, W. L. (2001). Interpersonal inf luence in family systems: A so-
Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian par- cial relations model analysis. Child Development, 72, 11791197.
enting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cul- Cooksey, E. C., Menaghan, E. G., & Jekielek, S. M. (1997). Life
tural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 11111119. course effects of work and family circumstances on children. So-
Chavez, L. (1990). Co-residence and resistance: Strategies for sur- cial Forces, 76, 637667.
vival among undocumented Mexicans and Central Americans in Cooney, R. S., Rogler, L. H., Hurrell, R. M., & Ortiz, V. (1982). De-
the United States. Urban Anthropology, 19, 3161. cision making in intergenerational Puerto Rican families. Journal
Chiu, L.-H. (1987). Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Chinese- of Marriage and the Family, 44, 621631.
American, and Anglo-American mothers. International Journal of Corter, C. M., & Fleming, A. S. (2002). Psychobiology of maternal
Psychology, 22, 409419. behavior in human beings. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
Chun, K. M., & Akutsu, P. D. (2003). Acculturation among ethnic parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd ed.,
minority families. In K. M. Chun, P. Balls Organista, & G. Marin pp. 14182). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and ap- Coulton, C. V., & Pandey, S. (1992). Geographic concentration of
plied research (pp. 95119). Washington, DC: American Psycho- poverty and risk to children in urban neighborhoods. American
logical Association. Behavioral Scientist, 35, 238257.
Chun, K. M., Balls Organista, P., & Marin, G. (Eds.). (2003). Accul- Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Interventions as tests of family
turation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research. systems theories: Marital family relationships in childrens devel-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. opment and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology,
14, 731759.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Allhusen, V. (2005). What we know about
childcare. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., Schulz, M. S., & Heming, G. (1994). Pre-
birth to preschool family factors in childrens adaptation to
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., & Brentano, C. (2006). Divorce: Causes &
kindergarten. In R. D. Parke & S. G. Kellam (Eds.), Exploring
consequences. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
family relationships with other social contexts (pp. 75114). Hills-
Cochran, M., & Bo, I. (1989). The social networks, family involve- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ment and pro and anti-social behavior of adolescent males in Nor-
Cox, M., Paley, B., & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conf lict and
way. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 18, 377398.
parent-child relationships. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.),
Cochran, M., & Niego, S. (1995). Parenting and social networks. In Interparental conf lict and child development: Theory, research,
M. H. Bornstien (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and so- and applications (pp. 249272). New York: Cambridge Univer-
cial conditions of parenting (pp. 393418). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sity Press.
Coleman, J. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. Crockenberg, S., & Langrock, A. (2001). The role of emotion and
American Journal of Sociology, 94(Suppl.), S95S120. emotional regulation in childrens responses to interparental con-
Collins, W. A., & Madsen, S. D. (2003). Developmental changes in f lict. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conf lict
parenting interactions. In L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Handbook of dy- and child development: Theory, research, and applications
namics in parent-child relations (pp. 4966). Thousand Oaks, (pp. 126156). New York: Cambridge University Press.
CA: Sage. Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2003). Parental acceptance,
Coltrane, S., Parke, R. D., & Adams, M. (2004). Complexity of fa- postpartum depression, and maternal sensitivity: Mediating and
ther involvement in low-income Mexican American families. moderating processes. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 8093.
Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Crouter, A. C. (1994). Processes linking families and work: Implica-
Studies, 53, 179189. tions for behavior and development in both settings. In R. D.
Conger, K. J., Bryant, C. M., & Brennom, J. M. (2004). The changing Parke & S. G. Kellam (Eds.), Exploring family relationships with
nature of adolescent sibling relationships: A theoretical frame- other social contexts (pp. 928). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
work for evaluating the role of relationship quality. In R. D. Con- Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (Eds.). (2003). Childrens inf luence on
ger, F. O. Lorenz, & K. A. S. Wickrama (Eds.), Continuity and family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mah-
change in family relations: Theory, methods and empirical find- wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ings (pp. 319344). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Crouter, A. C., Bumpus, M. F., Maguire, M. C., & McHale, S. M.
Conger, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (Eds.). (1994). Families in troubled (1999). Linking parents work pressure and adolescents well
times: Adapting to change in rural America. Hawthorn, NY: Al- being: Insights into dynamics in dual-earner families. Develop-
dine de Gruyter. mental Psychology, 35, 14531461.
494 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erikson, H., Updegraff, K., & McHale, S. M. pact of maternal psychopathology on family functioning. Journal
(1999). Conditions underlying parents knowledge about chil- of Family Psychology, 12, 2340.
drens daily lives in middle childhood: Between- and within-fam- Dickstein, S., St. Andre, M., Sameroff, A., Seifer, R., & Schiller, M.
ily comparisons. Child Development, 70, 246259. (1999). Maternal depression, family functioning and child out-
Crouter, A. C., MacDermid, S. M., McHale, S. M., & Perry-Jenkins, comes: A narrative assessment. Monographs of the Society of Re-
M. (1990). Parental monitoring and perceptions of childrens search in Child Development, 64(2, Serial No. 257), 84103.
school performance and conduct in dual- and single-earner fami- Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and
lies. Developmental Psychology, 26, 649657. maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 325.
Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Dix, T., & Branca, S. H. (2003). Parenting as a goal-regulation pro-
Scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA cess. In L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-
Scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275304. child relations (pp. 167187). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cuellar, I., Nyberg, B., Maldonado, R. E., & Roberts, R. E. (1997). Dodge, K. A., McLoyd, V. C., & Lansford, J. E. (2005). The cultural
Ethnic identity and acculturation in a young adult Mexican-origin context of physically disciplining children. In V. C. McLoyd, N. E.
population. Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 535549. Hill, & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), African American family life
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conf lict: (pp. 245263). New York: Guilford Press.
The impact of family dispute and resolution. New York: Guilford Dornbusch, S. M., Ritter, P. L., Leiderman, P. H., Roberts, D. E., &
Press. Fraleigh, M. J. (1987). The relation of parenting style to adoles-
Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Raymond, J. A. (2004). cent school performance. Child Development, 58, 12441257.
Fathers in family context: Effects of marital quality and marital Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (1996). Parents interpersonal rela-
conf lict. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child de- tionships and childrens friendships. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F.
velopment (4th ed., pp. 196221). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep:
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An in- Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 115136). New
tegrative model. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487496. York: Cambridge University Press.
Darling, N., Steinberg, L., Gringlas, B., & Dornbusch, S. (1995). Dunn, J. (1993). Young childrens close relationships: Beyond attach-
Community inf luences on adolescent achievement and deviance: A ment. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
test of the functional community hypothesis. Unpublished manu- Dunn, J. (2004). Childrens friendships: The beginnings of intimacy.
script, Temple University. London: Blackwell.
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conf lict and child Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1994). Affect expression in the family, chil-
adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bul- drens understanding of emotions and their interactions with oth-
letin, 116, 387411. ers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 120137.
Davies, P. T., Dumenci, L., & Windle, M. (1999). The interplay be- Dunn, J., Slomkowski, C., Beardsall, L., & Rende, R. (1994). Adjust-
tween maternal depressive symptoms and martial distress in the ment in middle childhood and early adolescence: Links with ear-
prediction of adolescent adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the lier and contemporary sibling relationships. Journal of Child
Family, 61, 238254. Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 491504.
Davies, P. T., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, DuRocher Schudlich, T. D., & Cummings, E. M. (2003). Parental dys-
E. M. (2002). Child emotional security and interparental conf lict. phoria and childrens internalizing symptoms: Martial conf lict
Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 67 styles as mediators of risk. Child Development, 74, 16631681.
(3, Serial No. 270). East, P. L., & Rook, K. S. (1992). Compensatory patterns of sup-
Davies, P. T., & Windle, M. (1997). Gender-specific pathways be- port among childrens peer relationships: A test using school
tween maternal depressive symptoms, family discord, and adoles- friends, nonschool friends, and siblings. Developmental Psychol-
cent adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 657668. ogy, 28, 163172.
Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1994). Socializing young children in Mexican- Edmonston, B., & Passel, J. S. (1994). The future immigrant popula-
American families: An intergenerational perspective. In P. M. tion of the United States. In B. Edmonston & J. S. Passel (Eds.),
Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minor- Immigration and ethnicity: The integration of Americas newest
ity child development (pp. 5586). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. arrivals (pp. 317353). Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
Delpit, L. (1995). Other peoples children: Cultural conf lict in the Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (1993). Mother, older sibling and
classroom. New York: New Press. me: The overlapping roles of caregivers and companions in the
DeMent, T., Buriel, R., & Villanueva, C. (2005). Children as lan- social world of 2- to 3-year-olds in Ngeca, Kenya. In K. MacDon-
guage brokers: Recollections of college students. In S. Farideh ald (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications
(Eds.), Language in multicultural education (pp. 255272). (pp. 305329). Albany: State University of New York Press.
Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development.
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665697.
York: Guilford Press. Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. (1998). Parental social-
Denham, S. A., Cook, M., & Zoller, D. (1992). Baby looks very ization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241273.
sad: Implications of conversations about feelings between Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion, regulation and the de-
mother and preschooler. British Journal of Developmental Psy- velopment of social competence. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion
chology, 10, 301315. and social behavior: Vol. 14. Review of personality and social psy-
Deutsch, F. M. (1999). Halving it all: How equally shared parenting chology (pp. 119150). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
works. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1994). Mothers reactions to chil-
Dickstein, S., Seifer, R., Hayden, L. C., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A. J., drens negative emotions: Relations to childrens temperament
Keitner, G., et al. (1998). Levels of family assessment: Pt. 2. Im- and anger behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 138156.
References 495

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., & Miller, P. (1991). Person- Fiese, B. H. (2006a). Family routines and rituals: Family transactions
ality and socialization correlates of vicarious emotional respond- and individual health. In D. Snyder, J. Simpson, & I. N. Hughes
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 459470. (Eds.), Emotional regulation in families. Washington, DC: Ameri-
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., can Psychological Association.
Muprhy, B. C., et al. (2000). Prediction of elementary school chil- Fiese, B. H. (2006b). Family routines and rituals: Promising prospects
drens externalizing problem behavior from attentional and be- for the 21st century. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
havioral regulation and negative emotionality. Child Development, Fiese, B. H., & Marjinsky, K. A. T. (1999). Dinnertime stories: Con-
71, 13671382. necting family practices with relationship beliefs and child ad-
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the great depression: Social justment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
change in life experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Development, 64(2, Serial No. 257), 5268.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). The life course as developmental theory. Fiese, B. H., Sameroff, A. J., Grotevant, H. D., Wamboldt, F. S.,
Child Development, 69, 112. Dickstein, S., & Fravel, D. L. (1999). The stories that families
Elder, G. H., Jr., & Conger, R. D. (2000). Children of the land: Ad- tell: Narrative coherence, narrative interaction, and relationship
versity and success in rural America. Chicago: University of beliefs. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Develop-
Chicago Press. ment, 64(2, Serial No. 257).
Elder, G. H., Jr., Nguyen, T. V., & Caspi, A. (1985). Linking family Fiese, B. H., Tomcho, T., Douglas, M., Josephs, K., Poltrock, S., &
hardship to childrens lives. Child Development, 56, 361375. Baker, T. (2002). A review of 50 years of research on naturally
occurring family routines and rituals: Cause for celebration?
Emery, R. E., Kitzmann, K. M., & Waldron, M. (1999). Psychological
Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 381390.
interventions for separated and divorced families. In E. M. Het-
herington (Ed.), Coping with divorce, single parenting, and remar- Fivaz-Depeursinge, E., & Corboz-Warnery, A. (1999). The primary
riage: A risk and resiliency perspective (pp. 323344). Mahwah, triangle: A developmental systems view of mothers, fathers, and
NJ: Erlbaum. infants. New York: Basic Books.
Flannery, W. P., Reise, S. P., & Widaman, K. E. (1995). An item re-
Entwisle, D. R., & Astone, N. M. (1994). Some practical guidelines
sponse theory analysis of the general and academic scales of the
for measuring youths race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status.
self-description questionnaire II. Journal of Research in Personal-
Child Development, 65, 15211540.
ity, 29, 168188.
Epstein, J. L., & Sanders, M. G. (2002). Family, school, and commu-
Fletcher, A. C., Darling, N. E., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S. (1995).
nity partnerships. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parent-
The company they keep: Relation of adolescents adjustment be-
ing: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (pp. 407438). Mahwah,
havior to their friends perceptions of authoritative parenting in
NJ: Erlbaum.
the social network. Developmental Psychology, 31, 300310.
Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations
Frone, M. R., Yardley, J. K., & Markel, K. S. (1997). Developing and
and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
testing an intergrative model of the work-family interface. Jour-
Bulletin, 118, 108132.
nal of Vocational Behavior, 50, 145167.
Erera, P. I. (1997). Step- and foster families: A comparison. Mar-
Fuligni, A. J. (1997). The academic achievement of adolescents from
riage and Family Review, 26, 301315.
immigrant families: The role of family background, attitudes, and
Esposito, S. (2004). Immigrant dreams: A study of Mexican mothers behavior. Child Development, 68, 351363.
and language choice. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Clare-
Fuligni, A. J., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes toward family ob-
mont Graduate University, CA.
ligations among American adolescents with Asian, Latin American,
Fagan, J., & Hawkins, A. J. (Eds.). (2001). Clinical and educational and European backgrounds. Child Development, 70, 10301044.
interventions with fathers. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Clinical Fuller, G. (2004, October 10). A snapshot report on American Indian
Practice Press. youth and families. Retrieved from http://www.ocbtracker.com
Farley, R., & Allen, W. R. (1987). The color line and the course qual- /007/snapshot.html.
ity of life in America. New York: Russell-Sage Foundation. Furstenburg, F. F., Jr., Cook, T. D., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H., Jr., &
Fauber, R. L., & Long, N. (1991). Children in context: The role of the Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make it: Urban families and
family in child psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical adolescent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Psychology, 59, 813820. Gadsen, V. (1999). Black parenting in intergenerational and cultural
Featherman, D. L., Spenner, K. I., & Tsunematsu, N. (1988). Class perspective. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), Parenting and child development
and the socialization of children: Constancy, change, or irrele- in nontraditional families (pp. 221246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
vance. In E. M. Hetherington, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter Galinsky, E., & Swanberg, J. E. (2000). Employed mothers and fa-
(Eds.), Child development in life-span perspective (pp. 6790). thers in the United States: Understanding how work and family
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. fit together. In L. L. Haas, P. Hwang, & G. Russell (Eds.), Orga-
Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. (2002). nizational change and gender equity: International perspectives on
Americas children: Key national indicators of well-being. Wash- fathers and mothers at the workplace (pp. 1528). Thousand
ington, DC: Author. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Felix-Ortiz de la Garza, M., Newcomb, M. D., & Meyers, H. F. Gandara, P. (1997). Review of research on the instruction of limited
(1995). A multidimensional measure of cultural identity for English proficient students. Davis: University of California Lin-
Latino and Latina adolescents. In A. M. Padilla (Ed.), Hispanic guistic Minority Research Institute, Education Policy Center.
psychology: Critical issues in theory and research (pp. 2642). Garcia Coll, C., Crnic, K., Lamberty, G., Wasik, B. H., Jenkins, R., &
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Garcia, H. V. (1996). An integrative model for the study of devel-
Field, T. (1992). Infants of depressed mothers. Development and opmental competencies in minority children. Child Development,
Psychopathology, 4, 4966. 67, 18911914.
496 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Garcia Coll, C. T., & Magnuson, K. (1999). Cultural inf luences on child Grotevant, H. D., Fravel, D. L., Gorall, D., & Piper, J. (1999). Narra-
development: Are we ready for a paradigm shift? In A. S. Masten tives of adoptive parents: Perspectives from individual and couple
(Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 29. Cultural interviews. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
processes in child development (pp. 124). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. opment, 64(2, Serial No. 257), 6993.
Garcia Coll, C. T., Meyer, E. C., & Brillon, L. (1995). Ethnic and mi- Grusec, J. E. (1992). Social learning theory and developmental psy-
nority parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parent- chology: The legacies of Robert Sears and Albert Bandura. Devel-
ing: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 189209). opmental Psychology, 28, 776786.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental disci-
Gauvain, M. (2001). The social context of cognitive development. New pline methods on the childs internalization of values: A recon-
York: Guilford Press. ceptualization of current points of view. Developmental
Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental Psychology, 30, 419.
psychology. Child Development, 71, 5765. Grusec, J. E., & Ungerer, J. (2003). Effective socialization as prob-
Gewirtz, J. (1969). Mechanisms of social learning: Some roles of lem solving and the role of parenting cognitions. In L. Kuczynski
stimulation and behavior in early human development. In D. (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations
Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 211228). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
(pp. 57212). Chicago: Rand-McNally. Grych, J. H., & Cardoza-Fernandes, S. (2001). Understanding the im-
Goeke-Morey, M. C., Cummings, E. M., Harold, G. T., & Shelton, pacts of interparental conf lict on children: The role of social cog-
K. H. (2003). Categories and continua of destructive and con- nitive processes. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.),
structive marital conf lict tactics from the perspective of U.S. and Interparental conf lict and child development: Theory, research,
Welsh children. Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 327338. and applications (pp. 157187). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Gomel, J., Tinsley, B. J., Clark, K., & Parke, R. D. (1998). The effects sity Press.
of economic hardship on family relationships among African Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conf lict and childrens
Americans, Latino, and Euro-American families. Journal of Fam- adjustment: A cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological
ily Issues, 19, 436467. Bulletin, 108, 267290.
Goodman, S. M., & Gotlib, I. (Eds.). (2002). Children of depressed Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (Eds.). (2001). Child development and
parents: Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment. Wash- interparental conf lict: Theory, research, and applications. New
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. York: Cambridge University Press.
Goodnow, J. J. (2002). Parents knowledge and expectations: Using Gutierrez, J., & Sameroff, A. (1990). Determinants of complexity in
what we know. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Mexican-American and Anglo-American mothers conceptions of
Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed., pp. 439460). Mah- child development. Child Development, 61, 384394.
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hakuta, K., Butler, Y. G., & Witt, D. (2000). How long does it take En-
Gottfried, A. E., Gottfried, A. W., & Bathurst, K. (2002). Maternal glish learners to gain proficiency? (Policy Rep. 2000-1). Stanford
and dual-earner employment status and parenting. In M. H. Born- University: University of California Language Minority Institute.
stein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affec-
parenting (2nd ed., pp. 207230). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tive social competence. Social Development, 10, 79119.
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between Harjo, S. S. (1993). The American Indian experience. In H. P.
marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McAdoo (Ed.), Family ethnicity: Strength in diversity
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: (pp. 199207). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
How families communicate emotionally. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1995). Culture and parenting. In M. H.
Greenberger, E., ONeil, R., & Nagel, S. K. (1994). Linking work- Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecol-
place and homeplace: Relations between the nature of adults ogy of parenting (pp. 211234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
work and their parenting behaviors. Developmental Psychology, Harold-Goldsmith, R., Radin, N., & Eccles, J. S. (1988). Objective
30, 9901002. and subjective reality: The effects of job loss and financial stress
Greenfield, P. M., & Cocking, R. R. (1994). Cross-cultural roots of on fathering behaviors. Family Perspective, 22, 309325.
minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out
Griffith, J., & Villavicencio, S. (1985). Relationships among accul- the way they do. New York: Free Press.
turation, sociodemographic characteristics, and social supports in Harrison, A. O. (1985). The Black familys socializing environment:
Mexican American adults. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sci- Self-esteem and ethnic attitude among Black children. In H. P.
ences, 7, 7592. McAdoo & J. L. McAdoo (Eds.), Black children: Social, educa-
Grimes, C. L., Klein, T. R., & Putallaz, M. (2004). Parents relation- tional, and parental environments (pp. 174193). Thousand
ships with their parents and peers: Inf luences on childrens social Oaks, CA: Sage.
development. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. Dodge (Eds.), Childrens Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, S. Q., & Buriel, R.
peer relations: From development to intervention (pp. 141158). (1990). Family ecologies of ethnic minority children. Child Devel-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. opment, 61, 347362.
Grimm-Thomas, K., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1994). All in a days work: Harrist, A. W., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (1994).
Job experiences, self-esteem and fathering in working-class fami- Dyadic synchrony in mother-child interaction: Relation with chil-
lies. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family drens subsequent kindergarten adjustment. Family Relations: In-
Studies, 43, 174181. terdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 43, 417424.
Grossman, F. K., Pollock, W. S., & Golding, E. (1988). Fathers and Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful dif ferences in the
children: Predicting the quality and quantity of fathering. Devel- everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul
opmental Psychology, 24, 8291. H. Brookes.
References 497

Hart, C. H., Yang, C., Nelson, D. A., Jin, S., Bazarskava, N., Nelson, Homel, R., Burns, A., & Goodnow, J. (1987). Parental social networks
L. J., et al. (1998). Peer contact patterns, parenting practices, and and child development. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
preschoolers social competence in China, Russia, and the ships, 4, 159177.
United States. In P. Slee & K. Rigby (Eds.), Peer relation Hudson, P., & Taylor-Henley, S. (2001). Beyond the rhetoric: Imple-
amongst children: Current issues and future directions menting a culturally appropriate research project in first nation
(pp. 330). London: Routledge. communities. American Indian Culture and Research Journal,
Hatchett, S. J., & Jackson, J. S. (1993). African American extended 25, 93105.
kin systems: An assessment. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Family ethnic- Huffman, D. G., & Cummings, E. M. (2002). Childrens reactions to
ity: Strength in diversity (pp. 90108). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. marital conf lict simulations featuring mutual hostility and parental
Henggeler, S. W., Edwards, J. J., Cohen, R., & Summerville, M. B. depression. Unpublished paper, University of Notre Dame.
(1992). Predicting changes in childrens popularity: The role of Huntsinger, C. S., & Jose, P. E. (1995). Chinese American and Cau-
family relations. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, casian American family interaction patterns in spatial rotation
12, 205218. puzzle solutions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 471496.
Hess, R. D., & Shipman, V. C. (1965). Early experience and the so- Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., & Larson, S. L. (1998). Do parent prac-
cialization of cognitive modes in children. Child Development, tices to encourage academic competence inf luence the social ad-
36, 869886. justment of young European American and Chinese American
Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (Eds.). (1992). Coping children? Developmental Psychology, 34, 747756.
with marital transitions: A family systems perspective. Mono-
Hwang, P. (1987). The changing role of Swedish fathers. In M. E.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57(2/3,
Lamb (Ed.), The fathers role: Cross-cultural perspectives
Serial No. 227), 1242.
(pp. 115138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. (1985). Long-term effects
Isley, S. L., ONeil, R., Clatfelter, D., & Parke, R. D. (1999). Parent
of divorce and remarriage on the adjustment of children. Journal
and child expressed affect and childrens social competence:
of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 518530.
Modeling direct and indirect pathways. Developmental Psychol-
Hetherington, E. M., & Kelly, J. (2001). For better or for worse: Di- ogy, 35, 547560.
vorce reconsidered. New York: Norton.
Isley, S., ONeil, R., & Parke, R. D. (1996). The relation of parental
Hewlett, B. (2004). Fathers in nonindustrial cultures. In M. E. Lamb
affect and control behaviors to childrens classroom acceptance:
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (4th ed.,
A concurrent and predictive analysis. Early Education and Devel-
pp. 182195). New York: Wiley.
opment, 7, 723.
Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family
Joe, J. R. (1994). Revaluing Native-American concepts of develop-
socialization strategies and childrens mental health: Low in-
ment and education. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.),
come, Mexican-American and Euro-American mothers and chil-
Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 107113).
dren. Child Development, 74, 189204.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hill, N. E., & Herman-Stahl, M. A. (2002). Neighborhood safety and
Jones, J. M. (2003). Constructing race and deconstructing racism: A
social involvement: Associations with parenting behaviors and de-
cultural psychology approach. In G. Bernal, J. E. Trimble, A. K.
pressive symptoms among African-American and Euro-American
Burlew, & F. T. L. Leong (Eds.), Handbook of racial and ethnic
mothers. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 209219.
minority psychology (pp. 276290). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hill, R. B. (1988). Cash and noncash benefits among poor Black fam-
Kagan, S. (1984). Interpreting Chicano cooperativeness: Methodolog-
ilies. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Black families (2nd ed.,
ical and theoretical considerations. In J. L. Martinez Jr. & R. H.
pp. 306318). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mendoza (Eds.), Chicano psychology (2nd ed., pp. 289333). Or-
Ho, D. Y. F. (1986). Chinese patterns of socialization: A critical re-
lando, FL: Academic Press.
view. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people
(pp. 137). London: Oxford University Press. Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conf lict pre-
dict childrens internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Devel-
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and
opmental Psychology, 29, 940950.
parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol.
2. Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 231252). Mah- Keefe, S. E., & Padilla, A. M. (1987). Chicano ethnicity. Albu-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. querque: University of New Mexico Press.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E., & Tardif, T. (1995). Socioeconomic status and Kelley, M. L., Power, T. G., & Wimbush, D. D. (1992). Determinants
parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. of disciplinary practices in low-income Black mothers. Child De-
2. Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 161188). Hillsdale, velopment, 63, 573582.
NJ: Erlbaum. Kelley, M. L., Sanchez-Hucies, J., & Walker, R. (1993). Correlates of
Hoffman, L. W. (1991). The inf luence of the family environment on disciplinary practices in working- to middle-class African-Amer-
personality: Accounting for sibling differences. Psychological ican mothers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 252264.
Bulletin, 110, 187203. Kerig, P. K. (1996). Assessing the links between interparental con-
Hoffman, L. W. (2000). Maternal employment: Effects of social con- f lict and child adjustment: The conf lict and problem-solving
text. In R. D. Taylor & M. C. Wang (Eds.), Resilience across con- scales. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 454473.
texts: Family, work, culture, and community (pp. 147176). Kerig, P. K. (2001). Childrens coping with interparental conf lict. In
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conf lict and
Hoffman, L. W., & Youngblade, L. M. (1999). Mothers at work: Ef fects child development (pp. 213248). New York: Cambridge Univer-
on childrens well-being. New York: Cambridge University Press. sity Press.
Homel, R., & Burns, A. (1989). Environmental quality and the well- Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know
being of children. Social Indicators Research, 21, 133158. it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support
498 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology, parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed.,
36, 366380. pp. 269309). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kibria, N. (1993). Family tightrope: The changing lives of Vietnamese LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H. L., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psychological
Americans. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bul-
Kim, K., & Honig, A. S. (1998). Relationship of maternal employ- letin, 114, 395412.
ment status support for resilience with child resilience among Ko- Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (2004). The role of the father in child development
rean immigrant families in the United States. Early Child (4th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Development and Care, 141, 4160. Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., & Levine, J. A. (1985). The role of the fa-
Kim, U., & Berry, J. W. (1993). Indigenous psychologies: Research ther in child development: The effects of increased paternal in-
and experience in cultural context. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. volvement. In B. Lahey & E. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical
Knight, G. P., Tein, J.-Y., Prost, J. H., & Gonzales, N. A. (2002). Mea- child psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 229266). New York: Plenum Press.
surement equivalence and research on Latino families: The impor- Landrine, J., & Klonoff, E. A. (1996). African American acculturation:
tance of culturally informed theory. In J. M. Contreras, K. A. Deconstructing race and reviving culture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kerns, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Latino children and families Laosa, L. M. (1980). Maternal teaching strategies in Chicano and
in the United States (pp. 181201). Westport, CT: Praeger. Anglo-American families: The inf luence of culture and education
Knight, G. P., Virdin, L. M., & Roosa, M. (1994). Socialization and on maternal behavior. Child Development, 51, 759765.
family correlates of mental health outcomes among Hispanic and Laosa, L. M. (1982). School, occupation, culture, and family: The
Anglo American children: Consideration of cross-ethnic scalar impact of parental schooling on the parent-child relationship.
equivalence. Child Development, 65, 212224. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 791827.
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization
Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1994). Divergent realities: The emo-
and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child
tional lives of mothers, fathers, and adolescents. New York:
Development, 64, 325347.
Basic Books.
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children
LeCorgne, L. L., & Laosa, L. M. (1976). Father absence in low-in-
with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel-
come Mexican-American families: Childrens social adjustment
opmental Psychology, 33, 228240.
and conceptual differentiation of sex-role attributes. Developmen-
Kohn, M. V. (1995). Social structure and personality through time tal Psychology, 12, 470471.
and space. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder Jr., & K. Luscher (Eds.), Ex-
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they
amining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human de-
live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and ado-
velopment (pp. 141168). Washington, DC: American
lescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309337.
Psychological Association.
Lopez, D. E. (1982). Language maintenance and shift in the United
Kramer, L. (2004). Experimental interventions in sibling relation-
States todayThe basic patterns and their social implications:
ships. In R. D. Conger, F. O. Lorenz, & K. A. S. Wickrama (Eds.),
Vol. 1. Overview and summary. Los Alamitos, CA: National Cen-
Continuity and change in family relations: Theory, methods and
ter for Bilingual Research.
empirical findings (pp. 345380). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lum, D. (1986). Social work practice and people of color: A process-
Kramer, L., & Gottman, J. M. (1992). Becoming a sibling: With a lit-
stage approach. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
tle help from my friends. Developmental Psychology, 28, 685699.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Lyubchik, A., Wolfe, R., & Bronfman, E. (2002).
Krappmann, L. (1986, December). Family relationships and peer rela-
Parental depression and child attachment: Hostile and helpless pro-
tionships in middle childhood: An exploratory study of the associ-
files of parent and child behavior among families at risk. In S. H.
ations between childrens integration into the social network of
Goodman & I. H. Gotlib (Eds.), Children of depressed parents:
peers and family development. Paper presented at the Family Sys-
Mechanisms of risk and implications for treatment (pp. 89120).
tems and Life-Span Development Conference at the Max Planck
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Institute, Berlin, Germany.
Krishnakumar, A., & Black, M. M. (2003). Family processes Maccoby, E. E. (1984). Socialization and developmental change.
within three-generation households and adolescent mothers sat- Child Development, 55, 317328.
isfaction with father involvement. Journal of Family Psychology, Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental
17, 488498. Psychology, 24, 755765.
Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conf lict and Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of
parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Family Relations: children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology,
Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 49, 2544. 28, 10061017.
Kuczynski, L. (Ed.). (2003). Handbook of dynamics in parent-child Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming to-
relations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. gether. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.
Kuczynski L., Marshall, S., & Schell, K. (1997). Value socialization Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of
in a bidirectional context. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.),
Parenting and the childrens internalization of values: A handbook Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality,
of contemporary theory (pp. 2350). New York: Wiley. and social development (4th ed., pp. 1101). New York: Wiley.
Ladd, G. W. (1992). Themes and theories: Perspective on processes MacDonald, K., & Parke, R. D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent-
in family-peer relationships. In R. D. Parke & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), child play interaction and peer interactive competence. Child De-
Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 334). Hillsdale, velopment, 55, 12651277.
NJ: Erlbaum. MacDonald, K., & Parke, R. D. (1986). Parent-child physical play:
Ladd, G., & Pettit, G. (2002). Parenting and the development of chil- The effects of sex and age of children and parents. Sex Roles,
drens peer relationships. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of 15(7/8), 367378.
References 499

Machamer, A. M., & Gruber, E. (1998). Secondary school, family, Medrich, E. (1982). The serious business of growing up: A study of chil-
and educational risk: Comparing American Indian adolescents drens lives outside school. Berkeley: University of California Press.
and their peers. Journal of Educational Research, 91, 357369. Miller, P., & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and ag-
MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Volling, B. L., Lamb, M. E., Dechman, K., Ra- gression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 131.
biner, D., & Curtner, M. E. (1994). A cross-contextual analysis of Minuchin, P. (2002). Looking toward the horizon: Present and future
boys social competence: From family to school. Developmental in the study of family systems. In J. P. McHale & W. S. Grolnick.
Psychology, 30, 325333. Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of families
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Tarakeshwar, N., & Swank, A. B. (pp. 259278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2001). Religion in the home in the 1980s and 1990s: A meta-ana- Mishel, L., Bernstein, J., & Schmitt, J. (1999). The state of working
lytic review and conceptual analysis of links between religion, mar- America. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
riage, and parenting. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 559596.
Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers social coaching, mother-
Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of
child relationship style, and childrens peer competence: Is the
teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development,
medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312332.
73, 969982.
Modell, J., & Elder, G. H. (2002). Children develop in history: So
McAdoo, H. P. (1978). Factors related to stability in upwardly mobile
whats new. In W. W. Hartup & R. A. Weinberg (Eds.), Min-
Black families. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 40, 761776.
nesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 32. Child psychology
McAdoo, J. L. (1993). Decision-making and marital satisfaction in in retrospect and prospect (In celebration of the 75th anniver-
African-American families. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Family ethnic- sary of the Institute of Child Development) (pp. 173206). Mah-
ity: Strength in diversity (pp. 109119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McDonald, R., Jouriles, E. N., Norwood, W., Ware, H. S., & Ezell, E.
Moore, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Adolescent parenthood. In
(2000). Husbands marital violence and the adjustment problems
M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and
of clinic-referred children. Behavior Therapy, 31, 649665.
becoming a parent (2nd ed., pp. 173214). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McDowell, D. J., ONeil, R., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Display rule ap-
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). Family environment scales man-
plication in a disappointing situation and childrens emotional re-
ual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
activity: Relations with social competence. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 46, 306324. Mounts, N. S. (2000). Parental management of adolescent peer rela-
tionships: What are its effect on friend selection. In K. A. Kerns,
McDowell, D. J., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Differential knowledge of
J. M. Contreras, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Family and peers:
display rules for positive and negative emotions: Inf luences from
Linking two social worlds (pp. 169194). Westport, CT: Praeger.
parents, inf luences on peers. Social Development, 9, 415432.
McDowell, D. J., & Parke, R. D. (2005). Parental control and affect as Mounts, N. S. (2001). Young adolescents perceptions of parental
predictors of childrens display rule use and social competence management of peer relationships. Journal of Early Adolescence,
with peers. Social Development, 14, 440457. 21, 92122.
McDowell, D. J., Parke, R. D., & Spitzer, S. (2002). Parent and child Mounts, N. S. (2002). Parental management of adolescent peer rela-
cognitive representations of social situations and childrens social tionships in context: The role of parenting style. Journal of Family
competence. Social Development, 4, 486496. Psychology, 16, 5869.
McDowell, D. J., Parke, R. D., & Wang, S. J. (2003). Differences be- Murguia, E. (1982). Chicano intermarriage: A theoretical and empir-
tween mothers and fathers advice-giving style and content: Re- ical study. San Antonio, TX: Trinity University Press.
lations with social competence and psychological functioning in Murray, C. B., & Mandara, J. (2002). Racial identity development in
middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 5576. African American children: Cognitive and experiential an-
McHale, J. P., Lauretti, A., Talbot, J., & Pouquette, C. (2002). Retro- tecedents. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Black children: Social, educa-
spect and prospect in the psychological study of coparenting and tional, and parental environments (2nd ed., pp. 7396). Thousand
family group process. In J. P. McHale & W. S. Grolnick (Eds.), Oaks, CA: Sage.
Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of families National Center for Children in Poverty. (2000, Fall). News and Is-
(pp. 127165). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sues, 10 (NDRI Monograph No. 3). New York: Mailman School of
McHale, J. P., & Rasmussen, J. L. (1998). Coparental and family Pubic Health, Columbia University.
group-level dynamics during infancy: Early family precursors of National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
child and family functioning during preschool. Development and Child Care Research Network. (2003a). Does amount of time
Psychopathology, 10, 3958. spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the
McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Whiteman, S. D. (2003). The family transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 9761005.
contexts of gender development in childhood and adolescence. So- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
cial Development, 12, 125148. Child Care Research Network. (2003b). Do childrens attention
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child devel- processes mediate the link between family predictors and school
opment. American Psychologist, 53, 185204. readiness? Developmental Psychology, 39, 581593.
McLoyd, V. C., Hill, N. E., & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.). (2005). African National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
American family life. New York: Guilford Press. Child Care Research Network. (2005). Family-peer linkages: At-
McLoyd, V. C., & Steinberg, L. (Eds.). (1998). Studying minority tributional and attentional processes as mediators. Unpublished
adolescents: Conceptual, methodological, and theoretical issues. manuscript, National Institute of Child Health and Human Devel-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. opment, Bethesda, MD.
McWright, L. (2002). African American grandmothers and grandfa- New, R., & Benigni, L. (1987). Italian fathers and infants: Cultural
thers inf luence in the value socialization of grandchildren. In H. P. constraints on paternal behavior. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The fa-
McAdoo (Ed.), Black children: Social, educational, and parental thers role: Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 139167). Hillsdale,
environments (2nd ed., pp. 2744). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. NJ: Erlbaum.
500 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Nobles, W. W., Goddard, L. L., Cavil, W. E., & George, P. Y. (1987). Theory and applications in mental retardation (pp. 6995). Balti-
African-American families: Issues, insight, and directions. Oak- more: University Park Press.
land, CA: Black Family Institute. Parke, R. D. (1988). Families in life-span perspective: A multi-level
OConnor, T. G. (2003). Behavioral genetic contributions to under- developmental approach. In E. M. Hetherington, R. M. Lerner, &
standing dynamic processes in parent-child relationships. In L. M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Child development in life-span perspective
Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations (pp. 159190). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 145163). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Parke, R. D. (1996). Fatherhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
Ogbu, J. U. (1987). Variability in minority responses to schooling: sity Press.
Nonimmigrants versus immigrants. In G. Spindler & L. Spindler
Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
(Eds.), Interpretive ethnography of education: At home and
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd
abroad (pp. 255280). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ed., pp. 2773). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ogbu, J. U. (1991). Immigrant and involuntary minorities in compar-
Parke, R. D. (2004a). Development in the family. Annual Review of
ative perspective. In M. A. Gibson & J. U. Ogbu (Eds.), Minority
Psychology, 55, 365399.
status and schooling: A comparative study of immigrant and invol-
untary minorities (pp. 336). New York: Garland. Parke, R. D. (2004b). The Society for Research in Child Development
at 70: Progress and promise. Child Development, 75, 124.
OHearn, H. G., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1997). Mothers and fa-
thers reports of childrens reactions to naturalistic martial con- Parke, R. D., & Brott, A. A. (1999). Throwaway dads: The myths and
f lict. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent barriers that keep men from being the fathers they want to be.
Psychiatry, 36, 13661373. Boston: Houghton-Miff lin.
Okagaki, L., & Bojczyk, K. E. (2002). Perspectives on Asian American Parke, R. D., Burks, V. M., Carson, J. V., Neville, B., & Boyum, L. A.
development. In G. C. Nagayama Hall & S. Okazaki (Eds.), Asian (1994). Family-peer relationships: A tripartite model. In R. D.
American psychology: The science of lives in context (pp. 67104). Parke & S. Kellam (Eds.), Exploring family relationships with
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. other social contexts: Family research consortiumAdvances in
Okagaki, L., & Divecha, D. J. (1993). Development of parental be- family research (pp. 115145). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
liefs. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological Parke, R. D., Cassidy, J., Burks, V. M., Carson, J. L., & Boyum, L.
perspective (pp. 3567). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1992). Family contributions to peer relationships among young
Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental beliefs and chil- children: The role of affective and interactive processes. In R. D.
drens school performance. Child Development, 64, 3656. Parke & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family-peer relationships: Modes of
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Kitzman, H. J., Eckenrode, J. J., Cole, linkage (pp. 107134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
R. E., & Tatelbaum, R. C. (1999). Parental and infancy home vis- Parke, R. D., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2003). Effects of parental in-
itation by nurses: Recent findings. Future of Children, 9, 4465. carceration on children: Perspectives, promises and policies. In J.
Olsen, L., & Chen, M. T. (1988). Crossing the schoolhouse border: Travis & M. Waul (Eds.), Prisoners once removed: The impact of
Immigrant students and the California public schools. San Fran- incarceration and reentry on children, families, and communities
cisco: California Tomorrow Policy Research Report. (pp. 189232). Washington, DC: Urban Institute Press.
ONeil, R., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Family-peer relationships: The Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., Bothwick-Duffy, S., Powers, J., Adams,
role of emotion regulation, cognitive understanding and atten- M., Fabricius, W., et al. (2003). Measurement of father involve-
tional processes as mediating processes. In K. A. Kerns, J. M. ment in Mexican-American families. In R. Day & M. E. Lamb
Contreras, & A. M. Neal-Barnett (Eds.), Family and peers: Link- (Eds.), Reconceptualizing and measuring father involvement
ing two social worlds (pp. 195226). Westport, CT: Praeger. (pp. 1738). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ONeil, R., Parke, R. D., & McDowell, D. J. (2001). Objective and Parke, R. D., Coltrane, S., Duffy, S., Buriel, R., Dennis, J., Powers, J.,
subjective features of childrens neighborhoods: Relations to et al. (2004). Economic stress, parenting and child adjustment in
parental regulatory strategies and childrens social competence. Mexican American and European American families. Child De-
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 135155. velopment, 75, 16321656.
Orellana, M. F. (2001). The work kids do: Mexican and Central Parke, R. D., Hymel, S., Power, T. G., & Tinsley, B. R. (1980). Fa-
American childrens contributions to households and schools in thers and risk: A hospital based model intervention. In D. B.
California. Harvard Educational Review, 71, 366389. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, & J. H. Penticuff (Eds.),
Orellana, M. F., Dorner, L., & Pulido, L. (2003). Accessing assets: Emotional infant: Psychosocial risks in infant-environment trans-
Immigrant youths work as family translators or para-phrasers. actions (pp. 174189). New York: Brunner/ Mazel.
Social Problems, 50, 505524. Parke, R. D., & Kellam, S. (Eds.). (1994). Advances in family re-
Oropesa, R. S., & Landale, N. S. (1995). Immigrant legacies: The so- search: Vol. 4. Family relationships with other social systems.
cioeconomic circumstances of children by ethnicity and generation Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
in the United States (Working Paper 95-01R). Population Research Parke, R. D., Killian, C. M., Dennis, J., Flyr, M. V., McDowell, D. J.,
Institute, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA. Simpkins, S., et al. (2003). Managing the external environment:
Padilla, A. M., & Gonzalez, R. (2001). Academic performance of im- The parent and child as active agents in the system. In L. Kuczyn-
migrant and U.S. born Mexican heritage students: Effects of ski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parent-child relations
schooling in Mexico and bilingual / English language instruction. (pp. 247270). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
American Educational Research Journal, 38, 727742. Parke, R. D., Kim, M., Flyr, M., McDowell, D. J., Simpkins, S., Kil-
Padilla, A. M., & Perez, W. (2003). Acculturation, social identity, lian, C. M., et al. (2001). Managing marital conf lict: Links with
and social cognition: A new perspective. Hispanic Journal of Be- childrens peer relationships. In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham
havioral Sciences, 25, 3555. (Eds.), Interparental conf lict and child development: Theory, re-
Parke, R. D. (1978). Parent-infant interaction: Progress, paradigms, search, and applications (pp. 291314). New York: Cambridge
and problems. In G. P. Sackett (Ed.), Observing behavior: Vol. 1. University Press.
References 501

Parke, R. D., McDowell, D. J., Kim, M., & Leidy, M. S. (2006). Fam- social class, early age intervention, and scholastic achievement.
ily-peer relationships: The role of emotional regulatory processes. Adolescence, 21, 723735.
In D. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, & J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotional Pratt, M. W., & Fiese, B. H. (Eds.). (2004). Family stories and the life
regulation in families: Pathways to dysfunction and health. Wash- course: Across time and generations. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (1999). Links between mothers
Parke, R. D., & ONeil, R. (1999). Social relationships across con- and childrens social competence and associations with maternal
texts: Family-peer linkages. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen (Eds.), adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 197210.
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 30. Relationships
Pulkkinen, L., Nurmi, J.-E., & Kokko, K. (2002). Individual differ-
as developmental contexts (pp. 211239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ences in personal goals in mid-thirties. In L. Pulkkinen & A.
Parke, R. D., Power, T. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1979). Conceptualiza- Caspi (Ed.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the
tion and quantifying inf luence patterns in the family triad. In life course (pp. 331352). New York: Cambridge University Press.
M. E. Lamb, S. J. Suomi, & G. R. Stephenson (Eds.), Social inter-
Putallaz, M., Costanzo, P. R., & Smith, R. B. (1991). Maternal recol-
action analysis: Methodological issues (pp. 231253). Madison:
lections of childhood peer relationships: Implications for their
University of Wisconsin Press.
childrens social competence. Journal of Social and Personal Re-
Parke, R. D., & Suomi, S. (1981). Adult male-infant relationships: lationships, 8, 403422.
Human and nonhuman primate evidence. In K. Immelman, G. W.
Putallaz, M., Klein, T. P., Constanzo, P., & Hedges, L. A. (1994). Re-
Barlow, L. Petrinovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral develop-
lating mothers social framing to their childrens entry compe-
ment: The Bielefeld interdisciplinary project (pp. 700725). New
tence with peers. Social Development, 3, 222237.
York: Cambridge University Press.
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tem-
Patterson, C. J. (2002). Lesbian and gay parenthood. In M. H. Born-
perament and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of
stein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a
parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (pp. 255278). Mahwah,
parent (2nd ed., pp. 317338). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
NJ: Erlbaum.
Patterson, G. R. (1981). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
Ramirez, M., III. (1983). Psychology of the Americas: Mextizo perspec-
Patterson, G. R. (2002). The early development of coercive family
tives on personality and mental health. New York: Pergamon Press.
process. In J. B. Reid, G. R. Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.), Anti-
Ramirez, M., III, & Castaneda, A. (1974). Cultural democracy, bicog-
social behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental
nitive development and education. New York: Academic Press.
analysis and model for intervention (pp. 2544). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association. Ramos-McKay, J. M., Comas-Diaz, L., & Rivera, L. A. (1988).
Patterson, G. R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1984). The correlation of Puerto Ricans. In L. Comas-Diaz & E. E. H. Griffith (Eds.), Clin-
family management practices and delinquency. Child Develop- ical guidelines in cross-cultural mental health (pp. 204232). New
ment, 55, 12991307. York: Wiley.
Pearson, J. L., Hunter, A. G., Ensminger, M. E., & Kellam, S. G. Reed, J. (1982). Black Americans in the 1980s. Population Bul-
(1990). Black grandmothers in multigenerational households: Di- letin, 37, 137.
versity in family structure and parenting involvement in the Reid, J. B., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (Eds.). (2002). Antisocial
Woodlawn community. Child Development, 61, 434442. behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis
Perez, W. (2004). Is there a dual frame of reference in Mexican immi- and model for intervention. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
grant adolescents? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford logical Association.
University, CA. Reise, S. P., Widaman, K. F., & Pugh, R. H. (1993). Confirmatory fac-
Perry-Jenkins, M., Repetti, R. L., & Crouter, A. C. (2000). Work tor analysis and item response theory: Two approaches for explor-
and family in the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and the Family, ing measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 552566.
62, 981998. Reiss, D. (1989). The represented and practicing family: Contrasting
Peters, M. F. (1985). Racial socialization of young Black children. In visions of family continuity. In A. J. Sameroff & R. N. Emde
H. P. McAdoo & J. L. McAdoo (Eds.), Black children: Social, ed- (Eds.), Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A develop-
ucational, and parental environments (pp. 159173). Thousand mental approach (pp. 191220). New York: Basic Books.
Oaks, CA: Sage. Reiss, D. (2003). Child effects on family systems: Behavioral genetic
Petitt, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Brown, M. M. (1988). Early family ex- strategies. In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Childrens inf lu-
perience, social problem solving patterns and childrens social ence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relation-
competence. Child Development, 59, 107120. ships (pp. 325). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Phinney, J. S. (2003). Ethnic identity and acculturation. In K. M. Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R.
Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Marin (Eds.), Acculturation: Ad- (2000). The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social in-
vances in theory, measurement and applied research (pp. 6382). f luences on adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. University Press.
Pleck, J. H., & Masciadrelli, B. P. (2004). Paternal involvement: Lev- Repetti, R. V. (1994). Short-term and long-term processes linking job
els, sources and consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the stressors to father-child interactions. Social Development, 3, 115.
father in child development (pp. 222271). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Repetti, R. V. (1996). The effects of perceived daily social and aca-
Plomin, R. (1994). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1993: Ge- demic failure experiences on school-age childrens subsequent in-
netic research and identification of environmental inf luences. teractions with parents. Child Development, 67, 14671482.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 817834. Repetti, R. V., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily stress at work on
Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. G. (1990). Immigrant America: A portrait. mothers interactions with preschoolers. Journal of Family Psy-
Berkeley: University of California Press. chology, 11, 90108.
Portes, P. R., Dunham, R. M., & Williams, S. (1986). Assessing Richards, L., & Richards, T. (1991). The transformation of qualita-
child-rearing style in ecological settings: Its relation to culture, tive method: Computational paradigm and research processes. In
502 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

N. Fielding & R. M. Lee (Eds.), Using computers in qualitative re- Rutter, M. (2002). Family inf luences on behavior and development:
search (pp. 3853). London: Sage. Challenges for the future. In J. P. McHale & W. S. Grolnick
Richardson, L., & Fields, R. (2003, Feb. 6). Latino majority arrives: (Eds.), Retrospect and prospect in the psychological study of fami-
Among states babies. Los Angeles Times, pp. A2, A25. lies (pp. 321351). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rodriguez, J. L. (2002). Family environment and achievement among Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., & Eaves, L. (2001). Testing hy-
three generations of Mexican American high school students. Ap- potheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior.
plied Developmental Science, 6, 8894. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 291324.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New
in social context. London: Oxford University Press. York: Guilford Press.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Lon- Sameroff, A. (1994). Developmental systems and family functioning.
don: Oxford University Press. In R. D. Parke & S. G. Kellam (Eds.), Exploring family relation-
Roopnarine, J. (2004). African-American and African Caribbean fa- ships with other social contexts: Family research consortiumAd-
thers: Level, quality, and meaning involvement. In M. E. Lamb vances in family research (pp. 199214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(Ed.), The role of the father in child development (4th ed., Sampson, E. E. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indigenous psy-
pp. 5897). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. chologies of the individual and their role in personal and societal
Roopnarine, J. L., & Ahmeduzzaman, M. (1993). Puerto Rican fa- functioning. American Psychologist, 43, 1522.
thers involvement with their preschool-age children. Hispanic Sandstrom, M. J., & Coie, J. D. (1999). A developmental perspective
Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 15, 96107. on peer ejection: Mechanisms of stability and change. Child De-
Roopnarine, J. L., Fouts, H. N., Lamb, M. E., & Lewisi-Elligan, T. Y. velopment, 70, 955966.
(2005). Mothers and fathers behaviors toward their 3- to 4-month Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The growth of sociability. Harmondsworth,
old infants in lower, middle and upper socioeconomic African England: Penguin.
American families. Developmental Psychology, 41, 723732. Schell Witt, K. (1996). Family-peer linkages: Adolescents and high
Root, M. P. P. (1992). Racially mixed people in America. Thousand risk behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Oaks, CA: Sage. Guelph, Ontario, Canada.
Root, M. P. P. (1999). The biracial baby boom: Understanding ecolog- Schumm, W. R., McCollum, E. E., Bugaighis, M. A., Jurich, A. P.,
ical constructions of racial identity in the 21st century. In R. Her- Bollman, S. R., & Reitz, J. (1988). Differences between Anglo
nandez-Sheets & E. R. Hollins (Eds.), Racial and ethnic identity and Mexican American family members on satisfaction with fam-
in school practices: Aspects of human development (pp. 6789). ily life. Hispanic Journal of Behavior Sciences, 10, 3953.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Scott-Jones, D., Lewis, L. T., & Shirley, K. (1996, March). Successful
Rothbaum, F., Morelli, G., Pott, M., & Liu-Constant, Y. (2000). Im- pathways through middle childhood: The role of ethnic families of
migrant-Chinese and Euro-American parents physical closeness color. Paper presented at the meeting of the MacArthur Founda-
with young children: Themes of family relatedness. Journal of tion Joint Task Force on Family Processes, Philadelphia, PA.
Family Psychology, 14, 334348. Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child-
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. (1990). Maternal beliefs about adaptive rearing. Oxford, England: Row, Peterson.
and maladaptive social behaviors in normal, aggressive and Shulman, S., & Klein, M. M. (1993). Distinctive role of the father in
withdrawn preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, adolescent separation-individuation. In S. Shulman & A. W.
18, 419435. Collins (Eds.), Father-adolescent relationships: New directions for
Rubin, K. H., Mills, R. S. L., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1989). Maternal child development (pp. 4157). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
beliefs and childrens competence. In B. H. Schneider, G. Attili, Sigel, I. E., & Parke, R. D. (1987). Structural analysis of parent-
J. Nadel, & R. Weissberg (Eds.), Social competence in develop- child research models. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol-
mental perspective (pp. 313331). New York: Klewer ogy, 8, 123137.
Academic/ Plenum Press. Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence: The
Rueschenberg, E. J., & Buriel, R. (1989). Mexican American family impact of pubertal change and school context. Hawthorne, NY: Al-
functioning and acculturation: A family systems perspective. dine de Gruyter.
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 11, 232244. Simpkins, S. D., ONeil, R., Lee, J., & Parke, R. D. (2005). The rela-
Rumbaut, R. G. (1991). The agony of exile: A study of the migration tion between parent and childrens social networks and childrens
and adaptation of Indochinese refugee adults and children. In peer acceptance. Unpublished manuscript, University of Califor-
F. L. Ahearn Jr. & J. L. Athey (Eds.), Refugee children: Theory, nia, Riverside.
research and services (pp. 5391). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Uni- Simpkins, S. D., & Parke, R. D. (2001). The relations between
versity Press. parental friendships and childrens friendships: Self report and
Rumbaut, R. G. (1995). Vietnamese, Laotian, and Cambodian Amer- observational analyses. Child Development, 72, 569582.
icans. In P. G. Min (Ed.), Asian Americans: Contemporary trends Simpkins, S. D., & Parke, R. D. (2002). Maternal monitoring and
and issues (pp. 232270). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. rules as correlates of childrens social adjustment. Merrill-Palmer
Russell, A. (1997). Individual and family factors contributing to Quarterly, 48, 360377.
mothers and fathers positive parenting. International Journal of Slomkowski, C., & Manke, B. (2004). Sibling relationships during
Behavioral Development, 21, 111132. childhood: Multiple perceptions from multiple perspectives. In
Russell, A., & Finnie, V. (1990). Preschool childrens social status R. D. Conger, F. O. Lorenz, & K. A. S. Wickrama (Eds.), Conti-
and maternal instructions to assist group entry. Developmental nuity and change in family relations: Theory, methods and empiri-
Psychology, 26, 603611. cal findings (pp. 293318). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Russell, G., & Russell, A. (1987). Mother-child and father-child rela- Slomkowski, C., Rende, R., Conger, K. J., Simons, R. L., & Conger,
tionships in middle childhood. Child Development, 58, 15731585. R. D. (2001). Sisters, brothers, and delinquency: Evaluating so-
References 503

cial inf luence during early and middle adolescence. Child Devel- Acculturation: Theory, models, and some new findings
opment, 72, 271283. (pp. 139161). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Smetana, J., & Chuang, S. (2001). Middle-class African American Tardif, T. (1993). Adult-to-child speech and language acquisition in
parents conceptions of parenting in early adolescence. Journal of Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale Uni-
Research on Adolescence, 11, 177198. versity, New Haven, CT.
Smith, P. K., & Drew, L. M. (2002). Grandparenthood. In M. H. Teachman, J. D., Tedrow, L. M., & Crowder, K. D. (2000). The chang-
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becom- ing demography of Americas families. Journal of Marriage and
ing a parent (2nd ed., pp. 141172). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. the Family, 62, 12341246.
Spicer, P. (1998). Drinking, foster care, and the intergenerational Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., Messinger, D. S., & Isabella, R. (1995).
continuity of parenting in an urban Indian community. American Maternal depression and the quality of early attachment: An ex-
Indian Culture and Research Journal, 22, 335360. amination of infants, preschoolers, and their mothers. Develop-
mental Psychology, 31, 364376.
Staples, R., & Johnson, L. B. (1993). Black families at the crossroads:
Challenges and prospects. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Thelen E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the
development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape in-
tellectual test performance of African Americans. American Psy- Thornton, M. C., Chatters, L. M., Taylor, R. J., & Allen, W. R.
chologist, 52, 613629. (1990). Sociodemographic and environmental correlates of racial
socialization by Black parents. Child Development, 61, 401409.
Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S. M., & Brown, B. B. (1992). Ethnic dif-
ferences in adolescent achievement: An ecological perspective. Tinsley, B. J. (2003). How children learn to be healthy. New York:
American Psychologist, 47, 723729. Cambridge University Press.
Tolson, T. F., & Wilson, M. N. (1990). The impact of two- and three-
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., & Darling, N.
generational Black family structure on perceived family climate.
(1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement:
Child Development, 61, 416428.
Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement
to succeed. Child Development, 63, 12661281. Trimble, J. E., & Medicine, B. (1993). Diversification of American
Indians: Forming an indigenous perspective. In U. Kim & J. W.
Stern, D. N. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cam-
Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Research and experi-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ence in cultural context (pp. 133151). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Stocker, C. M. (1994). Childrens perceptions of relationships with Sage.
siblings, friends, and mothers: Compensatory processes and
Tse, L. (1995). Language brokering among Latino adolescents: Preva-
links with adjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
lence, attitudes, and school performance. Hispanic Journal of Be-
try, 35, 14471459.
havioral Sciences, 17, 180193.
Stocker, C., & Dunn, J. (1990). Sibling relationships in childhood: Tse, L. (1996). Who decides? The effects of language brokering on
Links with friendships and peer relationships. British Journal of home-school communication. Journal of Educational Issues of
Developmental Psychology, 8, 227244. Language Minority Students, 16, 225234.
Straus, M. A., & Stewart, J. H. (1999). Corporal punishment by Tseng, V., & Fuligni, A. J. (2000). Parent-adolescent language use
American parents: National data on prevalence, chronicity, sever- and relationships among immigrant families with east Asian, Fil-
ity, and duration, in relation to child and family characteristics. ipino and Latin American backgrounds. Journal of Marriage and
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 5570. the Family, 62, 465477.
Strom, R. D., Amukamara, H., Strom, S. K., Beckert, T. E., Moore, Uba, L. (1994). Asian Americans: Personality patterns, identity, and
E. G., Strom, P. S., et al. (2000). African-American fathers: Per- mental health. New York: Guilford Press.
ceptions of two generations. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 513516. U.S. Census Bureau. (1995). General family and household characteris-
Stubben, J. D. (2001). Working with and conducting research tics of American Indian tribes: 1990. Retrieved January 19, 2004,
among American Indian families. American Behavioral Scientist, from http//www.census.gov/prod/1/pop/profile/95/23_ps.pdf.
44, 14661481. U.S. Census Bureau. (1998, March). Households and family charac-
Suarez-Orozco, C., & Suarez-Orozco, M. (1995). Transformations: teristics (Current population reports, P2-215). Washington, DC:
Migration, family life, and achievement motivation among Latino U.S. Department of Commerce.
adolescents. Stanford: Stanford University Press. U.S. Census Bureau. (1999). Profile of the foreign-born population in
Sudarkasa, N. (1993). Female-headed African American households: the United States: 1997 (Current Population Reports, Series
Some neglected dimensions. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Family ethnic- pp. 23195). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
ity: Strength in diversity (pp. 8189). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. U.S. Census Bureau. (2000). USA statistics in brief: Population and
Suina, J. H., & Smolkin, L. B. (1994). From natal culture to school vital statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce.
culture to dominant society culture: Supporting transitions for Retrieved January 15, 2004, from http://www.census.gov./statab
Pueblo Indian students. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking /www/poppart.htm.
(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development U.S. Census Bureau. (2001a). Overview of race and Hispanic origin
(pp. 115130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Census 2000 Brief ). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Print-
Suomi, S. J. (2000). A biobehavioral perspective on developmental ing Office.
psychopathology: Excessive aggression and serotonergic dysfunc- U.S. Census Bureau. (2001b). Profile of the foreign-born population
tion in monkeys. In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, & S. M. Miller in the United States (Current Population Reports Series,
(Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 23206). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
pp. 237256). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press. U.S. Census Bureau. (2002, March). Census 2000, Summary File
Szapocznik, J., & Kurtines, W. (1980). Acculturation, biculturalism, 1. Retrieved January 10, 2004, from http://www.census.gov
and adjustment among Cuban Americans. In A. M. Padilla (Ed.), /population /cen2000/ Phc-TQ8.
504 Socialization in the Family: Ethnic and Ecological Perspectives

Valdes, G. (1996). Con respeto: Bridging the distances between cul- Willis, W. (1992). Families with African American roots. In E. W.
turally diverse families and schoolsAn ethnographic portrait. Lynch & M. J. Hanson (Eds.), Developing cross-cultural compe-
New York: Teachers College Press. tence: A guide for working with young children and their families
Valdes, G. (2003). Expanding definitions of giftedness: The case of young (pp. 121150). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
interpreters from immigrant communities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Wilson, B. J., & Gottman, J. M. (1994). Attention: The shuttle be-
Valenzuela, A. (1999). Subtractive schooling: U.S.-Mexican youth and tween emotion and cognitionRisk, resiliency and physiological
the politics of caring. Albany: State University of New York Press. bases. In E. M. Hetherington & E. A. Blechman (Eds.), Stress,
Vazquez-Garcia, H. A., Garcia Coll, C., Erkut, S., Alarcon, O., & coping, and resiliency in children and families: Family research
Tropp, L. (1995, April). An instrument for the assessment of fam- consortiumAdvances in family research (pp. 189228). Hills-
ily values and functioning of Latin parents. Poster presented at the dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- Wilson, M. N. (1992). Perceived parental activity of mothers, fa-
ment, Indianapolis, IN. thers, and grandmothers in three-generational Black families. In
Vidal, C. (1988). Godparenting among Hispanic Americans. Child A. K. H. Burlew, W. C. Banks, H. P. McAdoo, & D. A. Azibo
Welfare, 67, 453459. (Eds.), African American psychology: Theory, research, and prac-
Vigil, J. D. (1988). Barrio gangs: Street life and identity in Southern tice (pp. 87104). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
California. Austin: University of Texas Press. Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner city, the un-
Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. J., & Miller, A. L. (2002). Emotional derclass, and public policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
regulation in context: The jealousy complex between young sib- Wolfer, L. T., & Moen, P. (1996). Staying in school: Maternal em-
lings and its relations with child and family characteristics. Child ployment and the timing of Black and White daughters school
Development, 73, 581600. exit. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 540560.
Wakschlag, L. S., Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Wolin, S. L., Bennett, L. A., & Jacobs, J. S. (1988). Assessing family
Not just Ghosts in the nursery: Contemporaneous intergenera- rituals. In E. Imber-Black, J. Roberts, & R. Whiting (Eds.), Rituals
tional relationships and parenting in young African-American in families and family therapy (pp. 230256). New York: Norton.
families. Child Development, 67, 21312147.
Wong, M. G. (1988). The Chinese American family. In C. H. Mindel,
Walker, K., Taylor, E., McElroy, A., Phillip, D., & Wilson, M. N. R. W. Habenstein, & R. Wright Jr. (Eds.), Ethnic families in
(1995). Familial and ecological correlates of self-esteem in America: Patterns and variations (3rd ed., pp. 230257). New
African American children. In M. N. Wilson (Ed.), New Direc- York: Elsevier.
tions for Child Development: Vol. 68. African American family
Wong, M. G. (1995). Chinese Americans. In P. G. Min (Ed.), Asian
lifeIts structure and ecological aspects (pp. 2334). San Fran-
Americans: Contemporary trends and issues (pp. 5894). Thou-
cisco: Jossey-Bass.
sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Wall, C. S., & Madak, P. R. (1991). Indian students academic self
concept and their perceptions of teacher and parent aspirations Xiaoming, L., Stanton, B., & Feigelman, S. (2000). Impact of per-
for them in a band-controlled school and a provincial school. ceived parental monitoring on adolescent risk behavior over 4
Canadian Journal of Native Education, 18, 4351. years. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 4956.
Wamboldt, F. (1999). Co-constructing a marriage: Analyses of young Yeung, W. J., Sandberg, J. F., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Hofferth, S. L.
couples relationship narratives. Monographs of the Society for (2001). Childrens time with fathers in intact families. Journal of
Research in Child Development, 64(2, Serial No. 257), 3751. Marriage and the Family, 63, 136154.
Wamboldt, F., & Reiss, D. (1989). Defining a family heritage and a Ying, Y.-W., Coombs, M., & Lee, P. A. (1999). Family intergenera-
new relationship identity: Two central tasks in the making of a tional relationship of Asian American adolescents. Cultural diver-
marriage. Family Process, 28, 317335. sity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 5, 350363.
Waters, M. C. (1990). Ethnic options: Choosing identities in America. Young, B. A. (2002, April). Public school students, staf f, and gradu-
Berkeley: University of California Press. ate counts by state: School year 20002001. Washington, DC: Na-
Weisner, T. S. (1993). Overview: Sibling similarity and difference in tional Center for Education Statistics.
different cultures. In C. W. Nuckolls (Ed.), Siblings in south Asia: Zavella, P. (1987). Womens work and Chicano families: Cannery
Brothers and sisters in cultural context (pp. 117). New York: workers of the Santa Clara Valley. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univer-
Guilford Press. sity Press.
Weisskirch, R. S., & Alva-Alatorre, S. (2002). Language brokering Zimmerman, M. A., Ramirez, J., Washienko, K. M., Walter, B., &
and acculturation of Latino children. Hispanic Journal of Behav- Dyer, S. (1998). Enculturation hypothesis: Exploring direct and
ioral Sciences, 24, 369378. protective effects among Native American youth. In H. I. McCub-
Westerman, M. A., & Schonholtz, J. (1993). Marital adjustment, joint bins, E. A. Thompson, A. I. Thompson, & J. E. Fromer (Eds.), Re-
parental support in a triadic problem-soling task, and child behav- silency in family series: Vol. 2. Resiliency in Native American and
ior problems. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 97106. immigrant families (pp. 199220). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Whaley, A. L. (2000). Sociocultural differences in the developmental Zukow-Goldring, P. (2002). Sibling caregiving. In M. H. Bornstein
consequences of the use of physical discipline during childhood (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent
for African Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority (pp. 253286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 6, 512. Zuzanek, J. (2000). The ef fects of time use and time pressure on child
Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personal- parent relationships. Waterloo, Ontario, Canada: Otium.
ity: A cross-cultural study. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Williams, E., Radin, N., & Coggins, K. (1996). Parental involvement
in childrearing and the school performance of Ojibwa children:
An exploratory study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 578595.
CHAPTER 9

The Self
SUSAN HARTER

ANTECEDENTS OF THE SELF AS A COGNITIVE Early Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative Self-
AND SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION 506 Representations and Self-Evaluations 531
The I-Self and the Me-Self 508 Middle Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative Self-
Global versus Domain-Specific Evaluations 509 Representations and Self-Evaluations 540
Historical Perspectives 509 Late Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative Self-
POSSIBLE GENETIC INFLUENCES ON SELF- Representations and Self-Evaluations 545
ESTEEM 512 STABILITY VERSUS CHANGE IN SELF-
DEVELOPMENTAL DIFFERENCES IN SELF- REPRESENTATIONS 551
REPRESENTATIONS DURING CHILDHOOD 513 Normative-Developmental Change 551
Toddlerhood to Early Childhood: Verbal Cameo Individual Differences 553
of Normative Self-Representations and GENDER DIFFERENCES IN GLOBAL AND DOMAIN-
Self-Evaluations 513 SPECIFIC SELF-EVALUATIONS 554
Early to Middle Childhood: Verbal Cameo of Normative CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISONS 558
Self-Representations and Self-Evaluations 521 ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN OUR OWN CULTURE 559
Middle to Late Childhood: Verbal Cameo of Normative CONCLUSIONS 561
Self-Representations and Self-Evaluations 526
REFERENCES 561
Developmental Differences in Self-Representations
during Adolescence 530

Interest in self-processes has recently burgeoned in our understanding of how early socialization experi-
many branches of psychology. Cognitive-developmental- ences come to shape the structure and content of self-
ists of a neo-Piagetian persuasion have addressed nor- evaluations and contribute to psychopathology (Blatt,
mative changes in the emergence of a sense of self (e.g., 1995; Bleiberg, 1984; Kernberg, 1975; Kohut, 1977;
Case, 1992; Fischer, 1980; Harter, 1997; Higgins, Winnicott, 1965). Moreover, social and personality the-
1991). Developmentalists interested in memory orists have devoted considerable attention to those
processes have described how the self is crafted through processes that produce individual differences in percep-
the construction of narratives that provide the basis for tions of self, particularly among adults (see Baumeister,
autobiographical memory (see Fivush, 1987; Nelson, 1987, 1993; Epstein, 1991; Kihlstrom, 1993; Markus &
1993; Snow, 1990). Contemporary attachment theorists, Wurf, 1987; Steele, 1988).
building on the earlier efforts of Ainsworth (1979) and Although there is a new look to many of these con-
Bowlby (1980), have provided new insights into how in- temporary formulations, the field has also witnessed a
teractions with caregivers shape the representations of return to many of the classic issues that captured the at-
self and others that young children come to construct tention of historical scholars of the self. For example,
(see Bretherton, 1991, 1993; Cassidy, 1990; Cicchetti, new life has been breathed into Jamess (1890, 1892)
1993; Cicchetti & Beeghly, 1990; Cicchetti & Cohen, distinction between the I-self as subject, agent, knower
1995; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996; Pipp, 1990; Rutter & and the Me-self as object, as known. In addition,
Sroufe, 2000; Sameroff, 2000; Sroufe, 1990). Clinicians Jamess analysis of the causes of self-esteem is alive and
in the psychodynamic tradition have also contributed to well. There has also been a resurgence of interest in

505
506 The Self

symbolic interactionists, such as Baldwin (1897), Coo- Because the self is not only a cognitive construction but
ley (1902), and Mead (1934), who placed heavy empha- also a social construction (Harter, 1999) crafted in the
sis on how interactive processes with caregivers shape crucible of interactions with significant others, norma-
the developing self. tive-developmental manifestations of the self will nec-
In this chapter, the emphasis is on self-representations, essarily be affected by socialization at the hands of
beginning with toddlers and very early childhood, through parents and peers, two key influences.
late adolescence, where most of recent work on self- Third, I indicate how at each developmental period
development has been conducted. The critical period of there are individual dif ferences, against a normative
the development of the self in infancy was covered in de- backdrop, the causes of which can lead to healthy forms
tail in the previous handbook volume (see Harter, 1998; of self-development. Alternatively, self-development
also Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume) can be seriously derailed due to socialization influences
where there continues to be agreement on the basic that can result in maladaptive outcomes. These are to be
processes that govern the period of infancy. More recent distinguished from normative-developmental liabilities
treatments (see Fonagy, 2002; Mascolo, Fischer, & in their severity and the extent to which they compro-
Neimeyer, 1999; Rochat & Striano, 2002) echo and refine mise the functioning of the child or adolescent.
the role of interactional experiences embedded in the cru- Before describing each of these three issues at six
cible of the primary parent-infant relationship, where sen- stages of development during childhood and adoles-
sitive caregiving is critical to healthy self-development. cence, several general themes need to be presented as
These recent treatments also focus on the importance of background: (a) the antecedents of the self as a cogni-
the differentiation of the self from caregivers as a founda- tive and social construction, ( b) distinctions between
tion for later self-concept development and on intentional- the I-self and the Me-self, (c) recent perspectives on the
ity in early social relationships. These acquisitions help differentiation of the self and the creation of multiple
motivate the infant to realize that he or she constitutes a selves during adolescence, (d) historical formulations
self, both independent of, but dependent on, caregivers and contemporary perspectives, and (e) recent genetic
that supports self-definition. positions on the heritability of self-esteem.
The first part of this chapter deals with six stages of After reviewing the normative-developmental changes
self-development. Specifically, the focus is on changes in the self, the normative liabilities, and the roots of in-
in self-understanding and self-evaluation during three dividual differences leading to healthy versus more
periods of childhood, very early, middle, and late child- maladaptive self-development at each of the six stages,
hood, and three periods of adolescence, early, middle, the following issues are explored, drawing on consider-
and late adolescence. In each of these six periods, three able recent research. These themes include (a) the sta-
issues are covered. First, the normative-developmental bility of self-esteem, ( b) gender differences and the
features of self-description and self-evaluation are pre- special impact of perceived appearance on self-esteem,
sented. Features include the salient content of the self, (c) cross-cultural differences, and (d) ethnic differ-
the structure or organization of self-constructs, the va- ences in self-perceptions. For those interested in impli-
lence and accuracy of self-representations, the nature of cations for intervention, the reader is referred to Harter
social comparisons in forming self-judgments, and sen- (1999).
sitivity to others as sources of information that may be
relevant to self-representations.
Second, the normative-developmental liabilities that
mark the emergence to each period or stage are de- ANTECEDENTS OF THE SELF
scribed. The very fabric of development involves ad- AS A COGNITIVE AND
vances to new stages that may bring with them SOCIAL CONSTRUCTION
normative liabilities that should not be interpreted as
pathological, liabilities that will dissipate as more ad- In examining self-development, this chapter focuses on
vanced developments and skills are acquired. Movement the antecedents of self-representations as well as on
to a new stage of cognitive development inevitably leads their consequences. With regard to antecedents, the self
to liabilities given that the individual lacks cognitive is a cognitive and a social constructiontwo major
control (see Fischer, 1980) over emerging new skills. themes around which the material to be presented is or-
Antecedents of the Self as a Cognitive and Social Construction 507

ganized. From a cognitive-developmental perspective, across various domains of experience. Moreover, they
changes in self-representations are inevitable. As neo- permit the older child to distinguish between real and
Piagetians (e.g., Case, 1992; Fischer, 1980) and self the- ideal self-concepts that can then be compared to one an-
orists (e.g., Epstein, 1981, 1991; Greenwald, 1980; other, creating potential discrepancies that have conse-
Kelly, 1955; Markus, 1980; Sarbin, 1962) have force- quences for the self. During adolescence, newfound
fully argued, our species has been designed to actively cognitive capabilities support the creation of multiple
create theories about our world, to make meaning of our selves in different relational contexts.
experiences, including the construction of a theory of With regard to integration, cognitive abilities that
self. Thus, the self is, first and foremost, a cognitive emerge across development allow the individual to con-
construction. struct higher-order generalizations about the self in the
As a result, the self will develop over time as cogni- form of trait labels (e.g., demonstrated skills in math,
tive processes undergo normative-developmental change. science, and language arts are subsumed under the self-
Thus, because the self is a cognitive construction, the concept of smart ). Abilities that emerge in middle
particular cognitive abilities and limitations of each de- childhood also permit the individual to construct a con-
velopmental period will dictate the features of the self- cept of his or her worth as a person, an evaluation of his
system or how self-representations are conceptually or her global self-esteem. Further cognitive advances in
organized. As such, emphasis is given to the processes adolescence allow one to successfully intercoordinate
responsible for those normative-developmental changes seemingly contradictory self-attributes (e.g., How can
that result in similarities in self-representations at a I be both cheerful and depressed?) into meaningful ab-
given developmental level. stractions about the self (e.g., Im a moody person).
Previous ontogenetic accounts highlighted major Each of these themes is addressed.
qualitative differences in the nature of self-descriptions In addition to an exploration of the cognitive-devel-
associated with broad stages of development. Observers opmental antecedents of the self, emphasis is placed on
were initially struck by the dramatic differences that the self as a social construction. Thus, attention is de-
defined the stage models of the day (e.g., Piaget, 1960). voted to how socialization experiences in childrens in-
More recent treatments of self-development fill in the teractions with caregivers, peers, teachers, and in the
gaps by providing a more detailed account of the pro- wider sociocultural context will influence the particular
gression of the substages of self-understanding. As a re- content and valence of their self-representations. Those
sult, we have necessarily had to alter our views about building on the symbolic interactionist perspective
whether self-development is best viewed as a discontinu- (Baldwin, 1897; Cooley, 1902; Mead, 1934), as well as
ous or continuous process. Employing frameworks of the those of an attachment theory persuasion (Bretherton &
past, self-development was viewed as largely discontinu- Munholland, 1999), have focused on how socialization
ous, with an emphasis on the conceptual leaps from one experiences with caregivers produce individual differ-
broadly defined stage to another. Thus, theorists high- ences in the content of self-representations, including
lighted the dramatic differences between the self- whether self-evaluations are favorable or unfavorable.
descriptions and evaluations of young children, older The reactions of significant others determine whether
children, and adolescents. However, there has been a the child comes to view the self as competent versus in-
shift in emphasis. The development of self- capable, as lovable versus undeserving of others affec-
representations is now viewed as more continuous, in tion and esteem. Although cognitive-developmentalists
that investigators specify more ministeps or substages emphasize the fact that children are active agents in their
that occur, including how such levels build on, and own development, including the construction of self, the
transform, one another. symbolic interactionist and attachment perspectives
In focusing on normative-developmental changes, we alert us to the fact that children are also at the mercy of
see how cognitive development impacts two general the particular caregiving hand they have been dealt.
characteristics of the self-structure, both the dif ferenti- To summarize, with regard to antecedents, the self is
ation and the integration that an individual can bring to both a cognitive and a social construction. In examining
bear on the postulates in his or her self-theory. With re- the self as a cognitive construction, attention focuses
gard to differentiation, emerging cognitive abilities on those cognitive-developmental processes that result
allow the individual to create self-evaluations that differ in changes in the structure of the self-system, namely,
508 The Self

how self-representations are organized. This approach developing I-self, the self as subject, is to develop the
provides an account of normative, developmental realization that it is existential in that it exists as sep-
change, and emphasizes the similarities among individ- arate from others. The Me-self, the self as object, is re-
uals at a given stage of development. In treating the self ferred to as categorical in that the developing child
as a social construction, attention turns to those social- must construct categories by which to define the self
ization processes that reflect how children are treated (e.g., age and gender labels). Wylie (1979, 1989) has
by caregivers, interactions that primarily impact the summarized the essence of the distinctions that have
evaluative content of self-representations. Child-rearing been drawn by numerous theorists. The I-self is the ac-
practices do impact normative-developmental changes; tive observer, whereas the Me-self is the observed, the
however, they are also the major causes of individual product of the observing process when attention is fo-
differences in whether judgments about the self are fa- cused on the self.
vorable or unfavorable. More recently, Lewis (1994) has adopted new termi-
nology. He now refers to the I-self as the machinery of
The I-Self and the Me-Self the self, the basic biological, perceptual, and cognitive
processes that allow for the construction of the Me-self
In addressing these themes, we can draw on a distinction as the idea of me. Such cognitive representations of
in the literature between the I-self and the Me-self. The the self begin to emerge in rudimentary form in the sec-
majority of scholars who have devoted thoughtful atten- ond half of the 2nd year. Both the machinery of the
tion to the self have come to a similar conclusion: Two self as well as the idea of me undergo considerable
distinct but intimately intertwined aspects of self can be change during the course of development.
meaningfully identified: (1) self as subject (the I-self ) Historically, major attention has been devoted to the
and (2) self as object (the Me-self ). William James Me-self (to the study of the self as an object of ones
(1890) introduced this distinction defining the I-self as knowledge and evaluation) as evidenced by the myriad
the actor or knower, whereas the Me-self was the object number of studies on self-concept and self-esteem (see
of ones knowledge, an empirical aggregate of things Harter, 1983; Wylie, 1979, 1989). More recently, the I-
objectively known (p. 197). James also identified par- self, which James himself regarded as an elusive if not
ticular features or components of both the I-self and the incorrigible construct, has become more prominent in
Me-self. Components of the I-self included (a) self- accounts of self-development. As we come to appreciate,
awareness, an appreciation for ones internal states, both the structure and content of the Me-self at any
needs, thoughts, and emotions; ( b) self-agency, the given developmental level depend on the particular I-
sense of the authorship over ones thoughts, and actions; self capabilities (those cognitive processes that define
(c) self-continuity, the sense that one remains the same the knower). Thus, cognitive-developmental changes in
person over time; and (d) self-coherence, a stable sense I-self processes will directly influence the nature of the
of the self as a single, coherent, bounded entity. Compo- self-theory that the child is constructing.
nents of the Me-self included the material me, the Most scholars conceptualize the self as a theory that
social me, and the spiritual me. In contemporary must be cognitively constructed. Those theorists in the
models, this translates into new domains of the self- tradition of adult personality and social psychology have
concept and supports the current multidimensional ap- suggested that the self-theory should possess the char-
proaches to the self (see Harter, 1999). acteristics of any formal theory, defined as a hypo-
The distinction between the I-self and the Me-self thetico-deductive system. Such a personal epistemology
has proved amazingly viable and is a recurrent theme in should, therefore, meet those criteria by which any good
many theoretical treatments of the self. While more re- theory is evaluated: that it is parsimonious, empirically
cent scholars have employed somewhat different termi- valid, internally consistent, coherently organized,
nology, the essence of the distinction has been retained. testable, and useful. From a developmental perspective,
Dickstein (1977), for example, contrasted the dy- however, the self-theories created by children cannot
namic self that possesses a sense of personal agency to meet these criteria, given numerous cognitive limita-
the self as the object of ones knowledge. Lewis and tions that have been identified in Piagetian (1960) and
Brooks-Gunn (1979) initially defined this duality as the neo-Piagetian formulations (e.g., Case, 1992; Fischer,
existential self and the categorical self. The task of the 1980). The I-self in its role as constructor of the Me-self
Antecedents of the Self as a Cognitive and Social Construction 509

does not, in childhood, possess the capacities to create a In this chapter, the term self-concept is primarily re-
hierarchically organized system of postulates that are served for evaluative judgments of attributes in discrete
internally consistent, coherently organized, testable, or domains, such as cognitive competence, social accep-
empirically valid. It is not until late adolescence or early tance, physical appearance, and so forth, or domain-
adulthood that the cognitive abilities to construct a self- specific self-evaluations. Such a focus allows the
portrait meeting the criteria of a good formal theory po- investigator to construct a profile of self-evaluations
tentially emerge. Therefore, it is essential to examine across domains for individuals or for particular sub-
how the changing characteristics of the I-self processes groups of interest. Moreover, the separation of global
that define each developmental stage directly impact the self-esteem or self-worth from domain-specific evalua-
Me-self (the self-theory that is being constructed). tions allows us to address the issue of whether evalua-
tions in some domains are more predictive of global
Global versus Domain-Specific Evaluations self-esteem than are others.
Developmental advances allowing older children and
The increasing ability with development to differentiate adolescent to differentiate self-domains also extend to
self-domains, as well to integrate self-perceptions into a the development of multiple selves that become highly
larger global concept of self, has led contemporary salient in early adolescence (Harter, 1999). Thus, an indi-
scholars to separate domain-specific perceptions from a vidual comes to develop a self with each parent, a best
global concept of a persons worth or self-esteem. Thus, friend, a romantic other, and classmates of each gender
it has become increasingly important to distinguish be- selves that often are defined by very different self-
tween self-evaluations that represent global characteris- descriptors. Although cognitive-developmental processes
tics of the individual (e.g., I am a worthwhile person) contribute to the ability to construct multiple selves,
from those that reflect the individuals sense of ade- social forces, including the pressure to be a particular
quacy across particular domains such as their cognitive self in each relational context, also contribute to this
competence (e.g., I am smart ), social competence differentiation.
(e.g., I am well liked by peers), athletic competence
(e.g., I am good at sports), and so forth (see Bracken, Historical Perspectives
1996; Epstein, 1991; Harter, 1986b, 1998; Marsh, 1986,
1987; Rosenberg, 1979). Conceptualizations and instru- These contemporary themes find their roots in the writ-
ments that aggregate domain-specific self-evaluations ings of historical scholars of the self. William James
into a single score (e.g., Coopersmith, 1967) have been clearly contributed with regard to his formulations re-
found wanting in that they mask meaningful distinctions garding the origins of our self-esteem, to the differenti-
between an individuals sense of adequacy across do- ation of domains of the self, and to what he labeled the
mains. Moreover, the separation of the evaluation of an conflict of the different Mes across differing rela-
individuals global worth as a person from more do- tional constructs. The symbolic interactionists, such as
main-specific attributes has allowed investigators to Cooley (1902), Baldwin (1895), and Mead (1934),
construct hierarchical models of the relationship among clearly articulated the role of social processes in con-
these self-constructs. structing the self.
With regard to terminology, global self-evaluations The field has witnessed a return to classic issues that
have typically been referred to as self-esteem (Rosen- captured the attention of historical scholars of the self.
berg, 1979), self-worth (Harter, 1983, 1999) or general Thus, it behooves us to briefly review the scripts of the
self-concept (Marsh, 1986, 1987). In each case, the major actors in this drama as a conceptual backdrop
focus is on the overall evaluation of ones worth or value against which more contemporary issues can be exam-
as a person. In this chapter, the terms self-esteem and ined. The history of interest in the self can be traced
self-worth are employed interchangeably. It is important back to ancient Greek philosophy, as revealed in the in-
to appreciate that this general evaluation is tapped by a junction to know thyself. However, contemporary
separate set of items that explicitly tap ones perceived scholars of the self-concept typically pay major intellec-
worth as a person (e.g., I feel that I am a worthwhile tual homage to James (1890, 1892) and to the previously
person). It is not a summary statement of self- listed symbolic interactionists such as Cooley, Mead,
evaluations across specific domains. and Baldwin. The reader interested in the history of the
510 The Self

self prior to the turn of the twentieth century is referred the list carefully, and pick out the one on which to stake
to excellent treatments by Baumeister (1987), Broughton his salvation (1890, p. 14).
(1987), and Logan (1987). The repudiation of particular attributes or roles was
not, for James (1890), necessarily damaging to the indi-
The Legacy of William James
viduals overall sense of worth, the average tone of
The contributions of James (1890, 1892) were legion. Of self-feeling which each one of us carries about
paramount importance was his distinction between two (p. 171). Thus, his own deficiency at Greek led to no
fundamental aspects of the self, the I-self as subject or sense of humiliation because he made no pretensions to
knower and the Me-self as object or known. It is the Me- be proficient at Greek. The role of pretensions became
self that took center stage as the major focus of empiri- paramount in Jamess formulation of the causes of self-
cal attention. Those interested in individual differences esteem. Self-esteem could not simply be reduced to the
in the self-concept focused primarily on the correlates aggregate of perceived successes but rather represented
of favorable versus unfavorable self-evaluations (see a ratio of successes to ones pretensions. If perceived
Wylie, 1979). In developmental psychology, earlier successes were equal to or greater than ones preten-
attempts to identify age-related changes in self- sions or aspirations for success, high self-esteem would
representations concentrated exclusively on the Me-self result. Conversely, if pretensions exceeded successes (if
(see Harter, 1983, 1999). Thus, the data consisted of the an individual were unsuccessful in domains deemed im-
differing self-descriptions produced by children at dif- portant), he or she would experience low self-esteem.
ferent age levels, with little analysis of what accounted Critical to this formulation is the assumption that lack
for such shifts (see Montemayor & Eisen, 1977). How- of success in an area in which one does not have preten-
ever, contemporary developmentalists have afforded the sions (e.g., Greek for James) will not erode self-esteem
I-self a far greater role as the architect of the Me-self. It because it can be discounted. Thus, both the presence
has become apparent that an appreciation for develop- and absence of pretensions figured heavily in Jamess
mental changes in I-self processes is critical to under- theorizing. He argued that abandoning certain preten-
stand how and why the structure and content of the sions can be as much a relief as striving to meet goals:
Me-self changes with age (see Damon & Hart, 1988; How pleasant is the day when we give up striving to be
Harter, 1983, 1999). young (p. 201).
In differentiating various aspects of the self, includ- For James, therefore, we find many themes that antici-
ing the multiplicity of social selves, James (1890) noted pate contemporary issues about the self. First and fore-
that these multiple selves may not all speak with the most is the distinction between I and Me selves.
same voice. For example, James observed that many a Jamess multidimensional view of the Me-self has been
youth who is demure enough before his parents and modernized in recent treatments of the self-structure,
teachers, swears and swaggers like a pirate among his where investigators have sought to examine the particular
tough young friends (p. 169). James further noted that relationships among global and domain-specific self-
this multiplicity can be harmonious; for example, when evaluations. Moreover, the potential conflict between dif-
an individual is tender to his children but stern to the ferent Me-selves that James observed has served as a
soldiers under his command. Alternatively, there may be springboard to contemporary interest in the construction
a discordant splitting if ones different selves are ex- of multiple selves. As we come to see, differing attributes
perienced as contradictory. across role-related selves that appear contradictory (e.g.,
The conflict of the different Mes could also be ob- depressed with parents but cheerful with friends) usher
served in the incompatibility of potential adult roles. in the potential for conflict. Finally, Jamess formulation
James, himself, fantasized about his own desires to be concerning the causes of self-esteem has been revived,
handsome, athletic, rich, and witty, a bon vivant, lady- leading to empirical investigations of its viability.
killer, philosopher, philanthropist, statesman, warrior,
The Contribution of the Symbolic Interactionists
African explorer, as well as a tone-poet and a saint. He
knowingly concluded that because all of these roles In contrast to James, the symbolic interactionists placed
could not possibly coexist, it was necessary to selec- primary emphasis on how social interactions with oth-
tively choose, suppressing the alternatives. Thus, the ers profoundly shaped the self. For Cooley (1902), Mead
seeker of his truest, strongest, deepest self must review (1934), and Baldwin (1897), the self is viewed as a so-
Antecedents of the Self as a Cognitive and Social Construction 511

cial construction, crafted through linguistic exchanges tentions regarding the determinants of global self-esteem.
(symbolic interactions) with others. Several themes James focused largely on those cognitive processes in
have found their way into contemporary theorizing. For which an individual actively compares particular aspira-
example, beginning in childhood, the child (a) engages tions to perceived successes in corresponding domains.
in the imitation of significant others behaviors, atti- For Cooley, the antecedents were far more social in na-
tudes, and values or standards; ( b) adjusts his or her ture, and less consciously driven, in that children in-
behavior to garner the approval of salient socializing evitably internalized the opinions that they believed
agents; (c) comes to adopt the opinions that significant significant others held toward the self. Cooley also spoke
others are perceived to hold toward them (these re- more directly to developmental changes, including the
flected appraisals come to define ones self as a person). consequences of the internalization process for adults. He
The fact that these processes occur in multiple social contended that the more mature sense of self is not buf-
contexts adds to the complexity of the construction of a feted about by potentially transient or disparate views of
self that can be experienced as coherent, as integrated, significant others. As Cooley (1902) observed, the person
and as authentic. with balanced self-respect has stable ways of thinking
Charles Horton Cooley was perhaps the most influen- about the image of self that cannot be upset by passing
tial. His formulation was the most metaphorical, given phases of praise or blame (p. 201). His thesis anticipates
his postulation of the looking glass self. In his now- contemporary interest in whether self-concepts are mal-
famous couplet, he observed that: leable versus resistant to change.
In George Herbert Mead (1925), we find an elabora-
Each to each a looking glass tion of the themes identified by Cooley, with an even
Reflects the other that doth pass greater insistence on the role of social interaction. For
Mead, We appear as selves in our conduct insofar as we
For Cooley, significant others constituted a social mir- ourselves take the attitude that others take toward us.
ror into which the individual gazes to detect their opin- We take the role of what may be called the generalized
ions toward the self. These opinions, in turn, are other (p. 270). Mead spoke to the origins of these atti-
incorporated into ones sense of self. Cooley contended tudes in childhood. He postulated a two-stage develop-
that what becomes the self is what we imagine that oth- mental process through which the child adopted the
ers think of us, including our appearance, motives, attitudes of others toward the self, labeling these stages
deeds, character, and so on: We come to own these re- as the play and the game. The play involved the imitation
flected appraisals. Such a self-idea was comprised of of adult roles, which Mead documented in his descrip-
three components: (1) the imagination of our appear- tion of the young child continually acting as a parent, a
ance to the other person; (2) the imagination of that per- teacher, a preacher, a grocery man, a policeman, a pi-
sons judgment of that appearance; and (3) some sort of rate, or an Indian (p. 270). In the subsequent stage of
self-feelingan affective reaction to these reflected games, there are proscribed procedures and rules:
appraisals. These components gradually become psycho-
logical removed from their initial social sources through The child must not only take the role of the other, as he
does in the play, but he must assume the various roles of
an implied internalization process. Cooley writes that
all the participants in the game and govern his actions ac-
the adult is not immediately dependent on what others
cordingly. If he plays first base, it is as the one to whom
think; he has worked over his reflected self in his mind the ball will be thrown from the field or from the catcher.
until it is a steadfast portion of his thought, an idea and Their organized reaction becomes what I have called the
conviction apart, in some measure, from its external ori- generalized other that accompanies and controls his
gin. Hence this sentiment requires time for its develop- conduct. And it is this generalized other in his experience
ment and flourishes in mature age rather than in the which provides him with a self. (p. 271)
open and growing period of youth (1902, p. 199).
Cooleys views on the internalization of others opin- For Mead, the individual adopts the generalized per-
ions about the self paved the way for a more developmen- spective of a group of significant others that shares a
tal perspective on how the opinions of others are particular perspective on the self. In predicting global
incorporated into the self. Moreover, his looking glass judgments of self, Meads formulation implies a process
self-perspective provides an alternative to Jamess con- through which the evaluations of significant others are
512 The Self

somehow psychologically weighted to produce an over- clearly specified. The very cornerstone of behavioral
all sense of self-worth as a person. However, Mead was approaches rested on a functional analysis of behavior.
not explicit on precisely how others judgments were In contrast, approaches to the self did little more than
combined. Contemporary researchers have begun to ad- implicate self-representations as correlates of behavior,
dress these processes more directly. affording them little explanatory power as causes or me-
For Cooley and Mead, several themes have found diators of behavior.
their way into contemporary treatments of the self. Several shifts in emphasis, later in the twentieth cen-
Paramount is the role of the opinions of others in shap- tury, allowed self-constructs to become more palatable.
ing the self-concept through social interaction. Cooley Hypothetical constructs, in general, gained favor as par-
hinted at a developmental internalization process that simonious predictors of behavior, often far more eco-
has implications for the stability of the self-concept in nomical in theoretical models than a multitude of
which the reflected appraisals of specific others become discrete observables. Moreover, we witnessed a cogni-
incorporated into relatively enduing attitudes about the tive revolution in both child and adult psychology
self. For Mead, a more generalized sense of self was in- (Bruner, 1990). For developmentalists, Piagetian and
ternalized, although just how the opinions of various neo-Piagetian models came to the forefront. For experi-
others are psychologically homogenized into a collective mental and social psychologists, numerous cognitive
sense of self remains elusive. Finally, Cooleys observa- models found favor. In this revolution, self theorists
tion that self-judgments are accompanied by self- jumped on the bandwagon, resurrecting the self as a
feelings highlighted the role of affective processes in cognitive construction, as a mental representation that
self-concept development, particularly self-conscious constitute a theory of self (e.g., Brim, 1976; Case, 1985;
emotions such as pride and shame (see Harter, 1999). Epstein, 1973, 1981; Fischer, 1980; Greenwald, 1980;
Kelly, 1955; Markus, 1980; Sarbin, 1962). Finally, self
Self-Psychology in the Second Half of the
representations gained increased legitimacy as behav-
Twentieth Century
iorally oriented therapists were forced to acknowledge
During the eras of James, Cooley, and later Mead, in- that the spontaneous self-evaluative statements of their
quiry into topics concerning the self and psyche flour- clients seemed powerfully implicated in their pathology.
ished. However, with the emergence of radical
behaviorism, such constructs were excised from the sci-
entific vocabularies of many theorists and the writings POSSIBLE GENETIC INFLUENCES ON
of James and the symbolic interactionists gathered dust SELF-ESTEEM
on the shelf. It is of interest to ask why the self became
an unwelcome guest at the behaviorists table. Why did The discussion thus far has focused on psychological
constructs such as self, including self-esteem, ego mechanisms that account for a childs level of self-
strength, sense of omnipotence, narcissistic injury, and esteem, describing the contribution of cognitive-devel-
so on, do little to whet the behaviorists appetite? Sev- opmental and social determinants. For many years,
eral reasons appear responsible. these have been the prevailing approaches. Recently,
The very origins of the behaviorist movement rested neurological and genetic models have come to the fore-
on the identification of observables. Thus, hypothetical front, the 1990s were declared the decade of the brain,
constructs were both conceptually and methodologically and it became obvious that our splintered subfields
unpalatable. Cognitions, in general, and self- needed to be integrated if we are truly to understand de-
representations, in particular, could not be operational- velopment and human behavior. Thus, how might such a
ized as observable behaviors. Moreover, self-report genetic perspective be applied to differences in levels of
measures designed to tap self-constructs were not in- self-esteem in children? Several investigators have re-
cluded on the methodological menu, because people cently presented statistical findings (from twin studies)
were assumed to be very inaccurate judges of their own that suggest the heritability of self-esteem. The empiri-
behavior. Those more accepting of introspective cal findings cannot be disputed (see McGuire et al.,
methodologies found the existing measures of self- 1999; Neiss, Sedikides, & Stevenson, 2002).
concept ungratifying because their content was overly However, does this mean that there is a self-esteem
vague. Finally, self-constructs were not satisfying to the gene? I think not. Might there be heritability, yes. What
behaviorists palate because their functions were not might be a thoughtful explanation? We know a great deal
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 513

about heritability from many compelling studies of in- self-descriptive cameo that reflects the cardinal fea-
telligence, temperament, athleticism, and creativity to tures of the content and structure of the self at that de-
name but a few characteristics. There are also consider- velopmental level. Discussion will focus on (a) the
able data demonstrating the heritability of conditions normative-developmental changes that are critical as a
such as various learning disabilities, autism, and atten- backdrop and against which we can judge whether a
tion-deficit / hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; see Pen- childs self-representations are age-appropriate; ( b)
nington, 2002). These studies can be linked to the issue the normative-developmental liabilities for the self at
of the heritability of self-esteem. If the genetic throw of this period; and (c) what cognitive and social factors
the dice causes a child to be intellectually competent, at each period lead to individual differences in self-
athletically competent, or attractive by current societal development, producing positive adjustment outcomes
standards, and if these domains are deemed important, for some versus deviations that can be considered
then according to James (and our own findings), this more maladaptive for others.
child is on the path to high self-esteem. Conversely, ge-
netically driven negative attributes, where success is Toddlerhood to Early Childhood: Verbal
viewed as important, will lead to low self-esteem. Cameo of Normative Self-Representations
A behavioral-genetic twin study by Hur, McGue, and and Self-Evaluations
Iacono (1998) comes to a similar conclusion. Findings
revealed that 30% of individual differences in the self- Im almost 3 years old and I live in a big house with my
concept subscales on the Piers-Harris instrument (Piers, mother and father and my brother, Jason, and my sister,
Lisa. I have blue eyes and a kitty that is orange and a tele-
1976) were associated with genetic factors. The authors
vision in my own room. I know all of my ABCs, listen: A,
suggest two possible mechanisms by which genetic fac-
B, C, D, E, F, G, H, J, L, K, O, P, Q, R, X, Y, Z. I can run
tors could affect self-concept. They first note that the real fast. I like pizza and I have a nice teacher at pre-
genetic influence could reflect the well-documented ge- school. I can count up to 100, want to hear me? I love my
netic influence on actual abilities, personality traits, dog Skipper. I can climb to the top of the jungle gym, Im
and physical make-up. Alternatively, they note that ge- not scared! Im never scared! Im always happy. I have
netic factors might affect various dimensions of the brown hair and I go to preschool. Im really strong. I can
self-concept by predisposing identical twins to psycho- lift this chair, watch me! (adapted from Harter, 1999)
logically evaluate themselves similarly, if they form a
close emotional bond. They indicate that sorting out Such descriptions will typically be observed in 3- to
these interpretations will require further research. 4-year-olds. Noteworthy is the nature of the attributes
Moreover, if children are genetically blessed with a selected to portray the self. Theory and evidence (see
sociable temperament and are rewarded for their socia- Fischer, 1980; Fischer & Canfield, 1986; S. Griffin,
bility by parents, peers, teachers, extended family, and 1992; Harter, 1998, 1999; Higgins, 1991; Watson, 1990)
others, from a looking glass self-perspective, this posi- indicate that the young child can only construct concrete
tive feedback will enhance their self-esteem. Thus, it cognitive representations of observable features of the
would seem that the constructs identified by James self (e.g., I know my ABCs, I can count, I live in
(1892) and Cooley (1902), and documented in our own a big house). Damon and Hart (1988) label these as cat-
research (Harter, 1999), are likely to be the mediators of egorical identifications; the young child understands the
genetic influences, rather than that there exists a direct self only as separate, taxonomic attributes that are phys-
connection between genes and self-esteem. ical (e.g., I have blue eyes), active (e.g., I can run real
fast, climb to the top), social (e.g., I have a brother,
Jason, and a sister, Lisa), or psychological (e.g., I am
DEVELOPMENTAL DIFFERENCES IN SELF- happy). Particular skills are touted (running, climbing)
REPRESENTATIONS DURING CHILDHOOD rather than generalizations about being athletic or good
at sports. Moreover, often these behavioral descriptions
In the sections to follow, we examine the nature of will spill over into actual demonstrations of ones
self-representations and self-evaluations at three peri- abilities (Im really strong. I can lift this chair,
ods of childhood: (1) toddlerhood to very early child- watch me!), suggesting that these emerging self-
hood, (2) early to middle childhood, and (3) middle to representations are still directly tied to behavior. From
late childhood. For each period, there is a prototypical a cognitive-developmental perspective, they do not
514 The Self

represent higher-order conceptual categories through concept. Rather, their descriptions represent a litany of
which the self is defined. In addition to concrete de- talents that may transcend reality (Harter & Pike,
scriptions of behaviors, the young child defines the self 1984). For contemporary cognitive-developmentalists,
by preferences (e.g., I like pizza; I love my dog Skip- such overstated virtuosity stems from another cognitive
per ) and possessions (I have an orange kitty and a tel- limitation of this period: The inability of young children
evision in my own room). With regard to possessions, to bring social comparison information to bear mean-
Fasig (2000) has specifically studied toddlers basic un- ingfully on their perceived competencies (Frey &
derstanding of ownership as a facet of the extended self. Ruble, 1990; Ruble & Dweck, 1995). The ability to use
Those with rudimentary ownership understanding pro- social comparison toward the goals of self-evaluation
vided richer self-representations than those who did not requires that the child be able to relate one concept (e.g.,
possess such knowledge. On balance, as Rosenberg his or her own performance) to another (e.g., someone
(1979) cogently observes, the young child acts as a de- elses performance), a skill that is not sufficiently de-
mographer or radical behaviorist in that his or her self- veloped in the young child. Thus, self-descriptions typi-
descriptions are limited to characteristics that are cally represent an overestimation of personal abilities.
potentially observable by others. It is important to appreciate, however, that these appar-
From the standpoint of organization, the self- ent distortions are normative in that they reflect cogni-
representations of this period are highly differentiated tive limitations rather than conscious efforts to deceive
or isolated from one another. The young child is inca- the listener.
pable of integrating these compartmentalized represen- Another manifestation of the self-structure of very
tations of self, and thus self-descriptive accounts appear young children is their inability to acknowledge that
quite disjointed. This lack of coherence is a general cog- they can possess attributes of opposing valence, for ex-
nitive characteristic that pervades the young childs ample, good and bad or nice and mean (Fischer, Hand,
thinking across a variety of domains (Fischer, 1980; Watson, Van Parys, & Tucker, 1984). This all-or-none
Harter, 1999). As Piaget (1960) observed, young chil- thinking can be observed in the cameo, in that all of the
drens thinking is transductive in that they reason from attributes appear to be positive. Self-representations
particular to particular in no logical order. may also include emotion descriptors (e.g., Im always
Neo-Piagetians have elaborated on these processes. happy). However, children at this age do not acknowl-
Case (1992) refers to this level as Interrelational, in edge that they can experience both positive and negative
that young children can forge rudimentary links in the emotions, particularly at the same time. The majority
form of discrete structures that are defined by physical will deny that they have negative emotions (e.g., Im
dimensions, behavioral events, or habitual activities. never scared!) as salient features of their descriptive
However, they cannot coordinate two such structures self-portrait. Other procedures reveal that they do have
(see also S. Griffin, 1992), in part, because of working rudimentary concepts of such single negative emotions
memory constraints that prevent young children from as mad, sad, and scared (see Bretherton & Beeghly,
holding several features in mind simultaneously. Fis- 1982; Dunn, 1988; Harter & Whitesell, 1989). Evidence
chers (1980) formulation is very similar. He labels now indicates that young children report that they cannot
these initial structures Single Representations. Such experience seemingly opposing emotional reactions si-
structures are highly differentiated from one another multaneously (Carroll & Steward, 1984; Donaldson &
because the cognitive limitations at this stage render the Westerman, 1986; Gnepp, McKee, & Domanic, 1987; P.
child incapable of integrating single representations into Harris, 1983; Harter & Buddin, 1987; Reissland, 1985;
a coherent self-portrait. Selman, 1980). For Fischer and colleagues (e.g., Fischer
Moreover, self-evaluations during this period are & Ayoub, 1994), this dichotomous thinking represents
typically unrealistically positive (e.g., I know all of my the natural fractionation of the mind. Such affecting
ABCswhich he or she doesnt) because young chil- splitting constitutes a normative form of dissociation
dren have difficulty distinguishing between their de- that is the hallmark of very young childrens thinking
sired and their actual competence, which is a confusion about both self and other.
initially observed by both S. Freud (1952) and Piaget Cognitive limitations of this period extend to the in-
(1932). Thus, young children cannot yet formulate an ability of young children to create a concept of their
ideal self-concept that is differentiated from a real self- overall worth as a person: a representation of their
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 515

global self-esteem (Harter, 1990b). This self- 1. Failure to display confidence, curiosity, initiative,
representation requires a higher-order integration of do- and independence that included: doesnt trust his or
main-specific attributes that have first been her own ideas, lacks confidence to initiate, lacks con-
differentiated. Young children do describe themselves in fidence to approach challenge, is not curious, does not
terms of concrete cognitive or physical abilities, how explore, hangs back, watches only, withdraws and sits
they behave, how they look, and the friendships they apart, describes self in negative terms, and does not
have formed (Harter, 1990b, 1999). However, these do- show pride in his or her work.
mains are not clearly differentiated from one another, as 2. Difficulty in reacting to change or stress that in-
revealed through factor-analytic procedures (Harter, cluded: gives up easily when frustrated, reacts to
1998; Harter & Pike, 1984). stress with immature behavior, and reacts inappropri-
ately to accidents.
Behaviorally-Presented Self-Esteem
in Young Children
Of particular interest are the categories of behaviors
The fact that young children cannot cognitively or ver- that do not seem to discriminate, according to teachers,
bally formulate a general concept of their worth as a between high and low self-esteem children. Most note-
person does not dictate that they lack the experience of worthy was the fact that competence was not viewed by
self-esteem. Rather, our findings (see Harter, 1990a, teachers as indicative of self-esteem in young children.
1999) indicate that young children manifest self-esteem It would appear that confidence, as a behavioral style, is
in their behavior. In examining the construct of behav- not synonymous with competence, at least at this age
iorally presented self-esteem, we first invoked the aid level. This is illuminating because it suggests that the
of experienced nursery school and kindergarten teach- origin of a sense of confidence during early childhood
ers. We found that for early childhood educators, self- does not necessarily reside in the display of skills, more
esteem is a very meaningful concept that distinguishes objectively defined. During later childhood, the link be-
children from one another. Teachers first provided pro- tween confidence in the self and level of competence ap-
totypic descriptors of the high and low self-esteem parently becomes stronger. As becomes apparent in
child, as well as attributes that did not discriminate the describing the antecedents of self-esteem that emerge in
two groups. A second group of teachers performed a Q- middle childhood, competence becomes a much more
sort procedure on 84 behavioral descriptors, indicating critical factor. We would argue that, in bridging these
which described the high self-esteem child, the low self- two developmental periods, socialization practices that
esteem child, and those that were not relevant to self- reward displays of confidence will lead the child to en-
esteem. gage in behaviors that would allow him or her to develop
There were two classes of behaviors that were viewed skills and competencies that will subsequently become a
as characteristic of the high self-esteem child: defining predictor of self-esteem.

1. Active displays of confidence, curiosity, initiative, Additional Functions of the


and independence that included: trusts his or her own Socializing Environment
ideas, approaches challenge, initiates activities confi- Higgins (1991), building on the efforts of Case (1985),
dently, takes initiative, sets goals independently, is Fischer (1980), and Selman (1980), focuses on how self-
curious, explores and questions, is eager to try doing development during this period involves the interaction
new things, describes self in positive terms, and between the young childs cognitive capacities and the
shows pride in his or her work. role of socializing agents. He provides evidence for the
2. Adaptive reaction to change or stress that included: contention that at Cases stage of interrelational devel-
able to adjust to changes, comfortable with transi- opment and Fischers stage of single representations,
tions, tolerates frustration and perseveres, and able to the very young child can place him- or herself in the
handle criticism and teasing. same category as the parent who shares his or her gen-
der, forming an initial basis for identification with that
Similar categories describing the low self-esteem parent. Thus, the young boy can evaluate his overt be-
child representing the converse of these two sets of havior with regard to the question: Am I doing what
items emerged: Daddy is doing? Similarly, the young girl evaluates her
516 The Self

behavior, asking Am I doing what Mommy is doing? However, for the young child, such narratives are still
Attempts to match that behavior impact which attributes highly scaffolded by the parents, who reinforce aspects
become incorporated into the young childs self- of experience that they feel are important to codify and
definition. Thus, these processes represent one way in remember (Fivush, Gray, & Fromhoff, 1987; Fivush &
which socializing agents impact the self. Hudson, 1990; Nelson, 1989). Through these interac-
Higgins (1991) observes that at the interrelational tions, an autobiographic account of the self is created.
stage, young children can also form structures allowing Of further interest are findings demonstrating individ-
them to detect the fact that their behavior evokes a reac- ual differences in maternal styles of narrative construc-
tion in others, notably parents, which causes psychologi- tion (see Bretherton, 1993; Nelson, 1990, 1993). For
cal reactions in the self (see Thompson, Chapter 2, this example, Tessler (1991) has distinguished between an
Handbook, this volume). These experiences shape the elaborative style (where mothers present an embellished
self to the extent that the young child chooses to engage narrative) and a pragmatic style (focusing more on use-
in behaviors designed to please the parents. Stipek, Rec- ful information). Elaborative mothers were more effec-
chia, and McClintic (1992) have provided empirical evi- tive in establishing and eliciting memories with their
dence for this observation, demonstrating that slightly young children.
before the age of 2, children begin to anticipate adult re- More recently, Nelson (2003) has introduced the con-
actions, seeking positive responses to their successes cept of a social-cultural-linguistic self that arises from
and attempting to avoid negative responses to failure. At autobiographical memory and a narrative self, between
this age, they also find that young children show a rudi- the ages of 2 and 5. The transition to a cultural self is
mentary appreciation for adult standards, for example, dependent on social-linguistic exchanges and exposure
by turning away from adults in seeming distress and to cultural messages that are meaningful to the self. It
hunching their shoulder in the face of failures (see also requires the childs social and cognitive awareness and
Kagan, 1984). For Mascolo and Fischer (1995), such re- the capacity for new levels of mental representations
actions constitute rudimentary forms of shame. Shame that develop during this period. For Nelson, this is a
at this period, like self-esteem, can only be behaviorally gradual process eventuating in a culturally saturated
manifest. Children do not understand the concept at a concept of self, an autobiographical self with both a spe-
verbal level (see Harter, 1999). Moreover, although cific self-history, and an imagined future self that re-
young children are beginning to recognize that their be- flects the values and expectations of the culture in
havior has an impact on others, their perspective-taking which one is embedded. Bems (1985) gender schema
skills are extremely limited (see Harter, 1999; Selman, theory is quite consistent with this conceptualization in
1980). Thus, they are unable to incorporate others opin- that she describes how the initial labeling of ones gen-
ions of the self into a realistic self-evaluation that can be der leads the young child to look to the culture where
verbalized. they learn that gender distinctions are very important,
causing them to attend to the content of gender roles for
The Role of Narrative in the Co-Construction
males and females. Learning that boys are expected to
of the Self
be strong, brave, and assertive but girls are expected to
Another arena in which parental figures, in particular, be good, nice, and quiet, boys and girls acquire gender
impact childrens self-development involves the role of schemas, adopting the characteristics that the culture
narratives in promoting the young childs autobio- deems appropriate for their gender.
graphic memory: a rudimentary story of the self. The in- For most developmental memory researchers, lan-
fantile amnesia that one observes before the age of guage is the critical acquisition allowing one to establish
approximately 2 can only be overcome by learning from a personal narrative and to overcome infantile amnesia
adults how to formulate their own memories as narra- (Budwig, 2000; Fivush & Hammond, 1990; Hudson,
tives. Initially, parents recount to the child stories about 1990a; Nelson, 1990, 2003; Pillemer & White, 1989).
his or her past and present experiences. With increasing The mastery of language, in general, and of personal
language facility, children come to take on a more active pronouns, in particular, enables young children to think
role in that parent and child co-construct the memory of and talk about the I-self and to expand their categorical
a shared experience (Eisenberg, 1985; Hudson, 1990a, knowledge of the Me-self (Bates, 1990; P. Miller, Potts,
1990b; Nelson, 1990, 1993; Rogoff, 1990; Snow, 1990). Fung, Hoogstra, & Mintz, 1990). Moreover, representa-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 517

tions of the self in language are further facilitated by ac- meaning that memories are not as bound by specific con-
quisition of the past tense, which occurs toward the lat- textual or proximal cues, whereas changes in such cues
ter half of the 3rd year. can disrupt or preclude memory retrieval in younger in-
Howe (2003) and Howe and Courage (1993) argue, fants. Older infants have been found to gradually utilize
however, that the emergence of language is not sufficient more or different retrieval cues allowing them to access
to explain the demise of infantile amnesia and the emer- memories in a wider range of situations. These newer ex-
gence of an ability to create autobiographical memories. planations are not incompatible with earlier theories of
Self-knowledge is also required, in that an appreciation the function of language and self-development but rather
for the self as an independent entity with actions, attri- provide additional explanations for the phenomenon of
butes, affects, and thoughts that are distinct from those infantile amnesia.
of others is necessary for the development of autobio-
graphical memory. Without the recognition of an inde- Normative Liabilities for Self-Development
pendent I-self and Me-self, there can be no referent during Very Early Childhood
around which personally experienced events can be or- Infantile amnesia precludes a conscious sense of self for
ganized. Thus, for Howe and Courage, the emergence of the toddler. Even once very young children are able to
the toddlers sense of self is the cornerstone in the de- verbally describe the self, their self-representations are
velopment of autobiographical memory that further still limited in that they reflect concrete descriptions of
shapes and solidifies ones self definition. behaviors, abilities, emotions, possessions, and prefer-
Linguistic interactions with parents also impact the ences that are potentially observable by others. These
developing childs representation of self in semantic attributes are also highly differentiated or isolated from
memory (Bowlby, 1969, 1973; Nelson, 1993; Snow, one another, leading to rather disjointed accounts, be-
1990). As Bowlby first noted, early semantic memory is cause at this age, young children lack the ability to inte-
conferred by caregivers. Parents convey considerable grate such characteristics. For some, this lack of a
descriptive and evaluative information about the child, logical self-theory may be cause for concern if not con-
including labels to distinguish one from others (e.g., sternation. However, these features are normative in
Youre a big boy), evaluative descriptors of the self that the I-self processes (i.e., the cognitive structures
(e.g., You are so smart ; Youre a good girl), as well available at this developmental period) preclude a more
as rules and standards and the extent to which the child logical rendering of the Me-self.
has met parental expectations (Big boys dont cry). Self-representations are also likely to be unrealisti-
Consistent with Cooleys (1902) model of the looking cally positive for several reasons (see Harter, 1999).
glass self, children incorporate these labels and evalua- First, they lack the cognitive ability to engage in social
tions into their self-definition in the form of general comparison, for the purpose of self-evaluation. From a
trait knowledge (represented in semantic memory). cognitive-developmental perspective, this skill, like
Thus, the linguistic construction of the self is a highly many of the abilities that are unavailable to the preoper-
interpersonal process, with caregivers making a major ational child as Piaget (1960) revealed, requires that one
contribution to its representation in both autobiographi- be able to simultaneously hold two dimensions in mind
cal and semantic memory. to compare them (cf. conservation tasks). We apply this
More recently, experts on infant memory develop- analysis to the inability to hold in mind an evaluation of
ment have suggested additional processes that may ac- ones own attributes while simultaneously thinking
count for childhood amnesia (see Hayne, 2004). about anothers attributes and comparing them.
Reviewing numerous studies of infant memory, Hayne Second, and for similar reasons, the very young child
concludes that there are three developmental processes is unable to distinguish between their actual self-
that may add to our understanding of the infants failure attributes and their ideal self-attributes. This requires
to retain autobiographical content. The large corpus of making a discrimination between the two, holding each
research first reveals that the speed with which infants in mind simultaneously, and comparing the two judg-
encode information increases as a function of age. Sec- ments, a cognitive ability that the very young child
ondly, the retention interval also dramatically increases lacks. As a result, self-evaluations are unrealistically
as a function of age during infancy. Third, the f lexibility positive because the fusion of the two favors the ideal or
of memory retrieval also improves during infancy, desirable self-concept. When we are dealing with older
518 The Self

children, we might interpret such a tendency to reflect very positive perceptions of self, even if potentially un-
socially desirable responding (i.e., the conscious distor- realistic. Positive self-views may serve as motivating
tion of ones self-evaluation) to be favorable. Cognitive- factors and emotional buffers, contributing to the young
developmental interpretations lead to a different childs development. They may propel the child toward
conclusion: that the very young childs positive evalua- growth-building mastery attempts, they may instill a
tions reflect cognitive limitations rather than a con- sense of confidence, and they may lead the child to re-
scious attempt to deceive. buff perceptions of inadequacy, all of which may foster
Third, young children lack the perspective-taking positive future development. From an evolutionary per-
ability to understand and therefore incorporate the per- spective, such liabilities may represent critical
ceived opinions of significant others toward the self strengths, at this developmental level. This issue is re-
(Harter, 1999; Selman, 1980). As becomes evident in the visited as we move up the ontogenetic ladder of repre-
discussion of middle childhood, the ability to appreciate sentations and evaluations of the self.
others evaluations of the self becomes a powerful deter-
Individual Differences: Adaptive and
minant of a childs sense of worth as he or she emerges
Maladaptive Outcomes during Toddlerhood and
in middle childhood.
Early Childhood
Cognitive limitations also lead to young childrens
inability to acknowledge that they can possess both pos- In the previous two sections on the period of toddler-
itive and negative self-attributes. The all-or-none, black- hood to early childhood, the focus was on normative self
and-white thinking that is characteristic of the development, including normative liabilities. It is also
preoperational child extends to his or her conceptualiza- critical to identify the causes of individual differences
tions of self: One must be one or the other. To the extent in self-development at each period. Individual differ-
that the majority of socializing agents are relatively ences will reflect a combination of the competencies, at-
benevolent and supportive, the psychological scale will tributes, and temperament that the child brings into the
tip toward the imbalance of positive self-attributes. world. They will also be heavily dependent on the role of
Thus, the young child will bask in the glow of overall socializing agents in the childs life. Thus, we explore
virtuosity (even if it is unrealistic). what facilitates positive self-evaluations and what com-
The inability to possess a verbalizable concept of promises the self in terms of negative evaluations. An
self-esteem can also be explained by the cognitive limi- important goal of this chapter is to distinguish between
tations of this period. As is documented, the subsequent normative liabilities in the formation of the self and
ability to compare ones actual self-attributes with more maladaptive or pathological processes and out-
ones ideal self-attributes will become an important de- comes at each developmental level.
terminant of ones level of self-esteem. Perspective-tak- Findings from the socialization literature in general
ing abilities will also become critical given that the (see Bugenthal & Goodnow, 1998; Thompson, 1998, and
internalization of the opinions of significant others be- Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume), and attachment
comes a powerful predictor of a childs overall sense of theory, in particular (see Bretherton & Munholland,
personal worth. It was noted that behavioral manifesta- 1999) highlight the critical role of care, sensitivity, and
tions of self-esteem do emerge during early childhood, feedback from socializing agents. Theory, going back to
as has been documented. However, it is an interesting Winnicotts (1965) concept of good enough mother-
empirical question as to whether level of self-esteem as ing, suggests that sensitive responding to the infants
so displayed parallels or predicts the concept of a childs and toddlers needs sets the stage for positive self-
self-esteem that will emerge in middle childhood. development (see also Stern, 1985). Soothing, positive
The description of the normative liabilities that im- affect, interest in the infants activities, support for
pact conceptions and manifestations of the self during mastery attempts, praise, and (nonintrusive) encourage-
early childhood follow from normative cognitive limita- ment all lay the groundwork for a healthy sense of self
tions. One may question, however, the extent to which during toddlerhood and early childhood.
these reflect psychological liabilities. Many of the cog- Attachment theory (see Bretherton and Munholland,
nitive limitations of this period may serve as protective 1999) focuses on the development of working models of
factors, to the extent that the very young child maintains self and other, constructed in the crucible of early care-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 519

giving experiences. For securely attached infants, a to 36 months) report less internal-state language, partic-
working model of self as valued, loved, and competent ularly negative internal feeling and physiological reac-
will emerge in the context of a working model of parents tions than do their nonmaltreated, securely attached
as emotionally available, loving, sensitive, and support- counterparts (see also Beeghly & Cicchetti, 1994). Sim-
ive of mastery attempts. Conversely, a working model of ilar findings by Beeghly, Carlson, and Cicchetti (1986);
the self as devalued and incompetent is the counterpart Coster, Gersten, Beeghly, and Cicchetti (1989) have
of a working model of parents as rejecting or ignoring of also reported that maltreated toddlers use less descrip-
attachment bids and interfering with exploration and tive speech, particularly about their own feelings and
mastery attempts. actions. Gralinski, Feshbach, Powell, and Derrington
Moreover, during toddlerhood to very early child- (1993) have also observed that older, maltreated chil-
hood, other factors could serve to seriously derail nor- dren report fewer descriptions of inner states and feel-
mative self-development, leading to outcomes that ings than children with no known history of abuse. Thus,
would seriously compromise a very young childs psy- there is a growing body of evidence that the defensive
chological development. Typically, the causes of severe processes that are mobilized by maltreated children in-
maladjustment involve an interaction between the terfere with one of the primary tasks of the I-self
childs level of cognitive development and chronic, neg- namely awareness of inner thoughts and feelings. Lack
ative treatment at the hands of caregivers. of self-awareness should also interfere with the ability
to develop autobiographical memory.
The Effects of Abuse. It is not uncommon for chil- Attachment theorists have also contributed to our un-
dren who experience severe and chronic sexual abuse to derstanding of how maltreatment in early childhood can
have also been subjected to other types of maltreatment, adversely influence self-development. There is consid-
including verbal, physical, and emotional abuse (see Ci- erable consensus that the vast majority of maltreated
cchetti, 2004; Harter, 1999; Rossman & Rosenberg, children form insecure attachments with their primary
1998). The normative penchant for very young children caregivers (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001; Crittenden &
to engage in all-or-none thinking (e.g., all good versus Ainsworth, 1989; Erickson, Egeland, & Pianta, 1989;
all bad) will lead abused children to view the Me-self as Schneider-Rosen, Braunwald, Carlson, & Cicchetti,
all bad. As noted earlier, the more typical pattern for 1985; Westen, 1993). Findings have documented that
children who are socialized by benevolent, supportive maltreated infants are more likely to develop disorgan-
parents is to view the self as all good. Abuse, as well as ized-disoriented type D attachment relationships (Bar-
severe neglect (Bowlby, 1980), can lead to early forms nett, Ganiban, & Cicchetti, 1999; Carlson, Cicchetti, &
of depression in which the very young child eventually Barnett, 1989). Thus, the effects of early sexual and/or
becomes listless, unconnected to caregivers, and, even- physical abuse, coupled with other forms of parental in-
tually numb, emotionally. sensitivity, disrupt the attachment bond, which inter-
Abuse or maltreatment can also affect I-self func- feres with the development of positive working models
tions, for example, self-awarenessone of the basic of self and others. The foundation of attachment theory
functions of the I-self as originally described by James rests on the premise that if the caregiver has fairly con-
(1892). Briere (1992) points to a feature of abusive rela- sistently responded to the infants needs and signals,
tionships that interfere with the victims lack of aware- and has respected the infants need for independent ex-
ness of self. The fact that the child must direct sustained ploration of the environment, the child will develop an
attention to external threats draws energy and focus internal working model of self as valued, competent,
away from the developmental task of self-awareness. and self-reliant. Conversely, if the parent is insensitive
Thus, the hyper-vigilance to others reactions, what to the infants needs and signals, inconsistent, and re-
Briere (1989) terms other directedness, interferes with jecting of the infants bid for comfort and exploration,
the ability to attend to the childs own needs, thoughts, the child will develop an internal working model of
and desires. the self as unworthy, ineffective, and incompetent
Research findings with children support these con- (Ainsworth, 1979; Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1991,
tentions. Cicchetti (1989) and colleagues (Cicchetti & 1993; Crittenden & Ainsworth, 1989; Sroufe & Fleeson,
Rogosch, 2001) found that maltreated children (ages 30 1986). The parental practices that have been associated
520 The Self

with child abuse represent precisely the kind of treat- certain episodes should not be retold or are best forgot-
ment that would lead children to develop insecure at- ten (Dunn, Brown, & Beardsall, 1991). Bretherton de-
tachments and a concept of self as unlovable and lacking scribes another manifestation (defensive exclusion) in
in competence. which negative information about the self or the other is
As noted earlier, one critical function of parenting is not incorporated because it is too psychologically threat-
to assist the child in creating a narrative of the self, an ening (see also Cassidy & Kobak, 1988). Wolf (1990)
autobiographical account that includes the perceptions further describes several mechanisms, such as deceit
of self and other (see Hudson, 1990a, 1990b; Nelson, and fantasy, whereby the young child, as author of the
1986, 2003; Snow, 1990). Initially, these narratives are self, can select, edit, or change the facts in the service of
highly scaffolded by parents, who reinforce aspects of personal goals, hopes, or wishes (see also Dunn, 1988).
experience that they, the parents, feel are important to Such distortions may well contribute to the forma-
codify and to remember or to forget (Fivush & Hudson, tion of a self that is perceived as unauthentic if a
1990; Hudson, 1990a; Nelson, 1986, 1990, 1993; Ro- person accepts the falsified version of experience.
goff, 1990; Snow, 1990). Findings have indicated that Winnicotts (1958) observations alert us to the fact that
the narratives of maltreated children contain more nega- intrusive or overinvolved mothers, in their desire to
tive self-representations and more negative maternal comply with maternal demands and expectations, lead
representations compared to nonmaltreated children infants to present a false outer self that does not repre-
(Toth, Cicchetti, Macfie, Maughan, & Vanmeenen, sent their own inner experiences. Moreover, such par-
2000). Moreover, such narratives show less coherence; ents may reject the infants felt self, approving only
that is, the self that is represented is less coherent (Crit- of the falsely presented self (Crittenden, 1994). As
tenden, 1994; Macfie, Cicchetti, & Toth, 2001). These Stern (1985) notes, the display of false-self, incurs the
findings document greater signs of dissociative symp- risk of alienating a person from those inner experi-
toms that reflect disruptions in the integration of memo- ences that represent their true self (see also Main &
ries and perceptions about the self. Thus, maltreatment Solomon, 1990). Thus, linguistic abilities not only
severely disrupts normative self-development, which allow a person to share his or her experiences with oth-
leads to associated pathological symptoms, where it has ers but also to withhold them as well.
been found that conflictual themes in young childrens
narratives predict externalizing problems. Moreover, se- The Impoverished Self. As noted in the discussion
vere, chronic abuse has been associated with disorders, of normative-development during early childhood, an im-
such as borderline personality, where symptoms emerge portant function of parenting is to scaffold the childs
later in development during adulthood (Putnam, 1993; construction of autobiographical memory in the form of a
Westen, 1993). narrative of ones nascent life story. However, clinicians
observe that maltreatment and neglect sow the seeds for
Language and False-Self Behavior. Language children, beginning in early to middle childhood, to de-
clearly promotes heightened levels of relatedness and al- velop what we (Marold & Harter in Harter, 1999) have
lows for the creation of a personal narrative. Stern come to call an impoverished self, which has its roots
(1985) also alerts us to the liabilities of language. He ar- in the early socialization practices of caregivers who fail
gues that language can drive a wedge between two si- to assist the child in the co-construction of a positive,
multaneous forms of interpersonal experience: as it is rich, and coherent self narrative. Research (Tessler,
lived and as it is verbally represented. The very capacity 1991) has revealed individual differences among mothers
for objectifying the self through verbal representations in that some help to construct an embellished narrative,
allows us to transcend, and potentially distort, our im- whereas others focus on a more restricted narrative that
mediate experience and to create a fantasized construc- focuses on useful information leading to fewer autobio-
tion of the self. As noted earlier, there is the potential graphical memories. Clinical observations reveal that
for incorporating the biases of caregivers perspectives there is another group of parents who, because of their
on the self, because initially, adults dictate the content own dysfunction (e.g., depression) and parental inadequa-
of narratives incorporated in autobiographical memory cies, do little to nothing in the way of co-constructing
(Bowlby, 1980; Bretherton, 1987; Crittenden, 1994; with the child a self-narrative. The seeds of an impover-
Pipp, 1990). Children may receive subtle signals that ished self, therefore, begin in early childhood and con-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 521

tinue into middle childhood and beyond if such children might be available (the scant photograph, perhaps from
do not receive therapeutic intervention. the school picture; a childs drawing; anything that may
When these children come to the attention of family make a memory more salient) are collected and talked
therapists, they lack a vocabulary to define the self, in about. Where such materials are not available, Marold
that there is little in the way of autobiographical mem- suggests cutting pictures out of magazines that represent
ory and descriptive or evaluative concepts about the the childs favorite possessions, activities, preferences,
self. An impoverished self represents a liability in that the very features that define the young childs sense of
the individual has few personal referents or self- self. If there have been no routines that help to solidify
concepts around which to organize present experiences. the childs sense of self, Marold recommends that par-
As a result, the behavior of such children will often ap- ents be counseled to establish routines, for example, es-
pear to be disorganized. Moreover, to the extent that a tablishing some family rituals (e.g., Friday night pizza)
richly defined self promotes motivational functions in around a childs favorite food. Obviously, these tech-
terms of guides to regulate behavior and to set future niques require collaboration with the parents and de-
goals, such children may appear aimless, with no clear pend on their ability to recreate their childs past
pursuits. A clinical colleague of mine, Donna Marold, experiences, something that not all parents may be
has astutely observed that these children do not have equipped to do. Here, the therapist can serve as an im-
dreams for the future, whereas most children do have portant role model. From the standpoint of our develop-
future aspirations (Marold, personal communication, mental analysis, the prevention of an impoverished self
August 1998). For example, the prototypical child in requires this type of support in early childhood. In the
early to middle childhood indicates that he or she is absence of such support, such impoverishment will con-
going to be on a team someday. Marold notes that the tinue into later childhood.
families of such children typically do not create or con-
struct the type of narratives that provide a basis for au- Early to Middle Childhood: Verbal Cameo of
tobiographical memory and a sense of self. Nor do such Normative Self-Representations and Self-
parents provide the type of personal labels or feedback Evaluations
that would lead to the development of semantic memory
for self-attributes. Often, these are parents who do not I have a lot of friends, in my neighborhood, at school, and
take photographs of the children or the family, nor do at my church. Im good at schoolwork, I know my words,
they engage in such activities as posting the childs art- and letters, and my numbers. I can run fast, and I can
climb high, a lot higher than I could when I was little and
work or school papers on the refrigerator door. Marold
I can run faster, too. I can also throw a ball real far, Im
has also observed that such parents do not have special
going to be on some kind of team when I am older. I can do
rituals such as cooking the childs favorite food or read- lots of stuff real good. Lots! If you are good at things you
ing (and rereading) cherished bedtime stories. cant be bad at things, at least not at the same time. I know
What type of therapeutic interventions might be ap- some other kids who are bad at things but not me! My par-
plicable, and how can they be guided by developmental ents are real proud of me when I do good at things. It
theory and research? Therapists (myself included) have makes me really happy and excited when they watch me!
learned through trial and error that one cannot, with (adapted from Harter, 1999)
older children, simply try to instill, teach, or scaffold
the self-structures appropriate for their age level; Such self-descriptions are typical of children ages 5
namely, trait labels that represent generalizations that to 7. Some of the features of the previous stage persist in
integrate behavioral or taxonomic self-attributes. With that self-representations are still typically very positive,
such children, there are few attributes to build on. Thus, and the child continues to overestimate his or her virtu-
we must begin at the beginning, utilizing techniques that osity. References to various competencies, for example,
help the child create the missing narratives, the autobio- social skills, cognitive abilities, and athletic talents are
graphical memory, and the self-labels. Marold has suc- common self-descriptors. With regard to the advances
cessfully employed a number of very basic techniques to of this age period, children begin to display a rudimen-
achieve this goal, techniques that necessarily enlist the tary ability to intercoordinate concepts that were previ-
aid of parents. She suggests that the parent and child ously compartmentalized (Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980;
create a scrapbook in which whatever materials that Harter, 1999). For example, they can form a category or
522 The Self

representational set that combines a number of their incompatible. The inability to acknowledge that a per-
competencies (e.g., good at running, jumping, school- son can possess both favorable and unfavorable attri-
work, having friends in the neighborhood, at school, and butes, or experience both positive and negative
at church). However, all-or-none thinking persists. In emotions, represents a cognitive liability that is a hall-
Cases (1985) model and its application to the self (S. mark of this period of development. Unlike the previous
Griffin, 1992), this stage is labeled unidimentional period, the child is now, due to greater cognitive and lin-
thinking. Such black-and-white thinking is supported by guistic abilities, able to verbally express his or her
another new cognitive process that emerges at this stage. staunch conviction that a person cannot possess both
The novel acquisition is the childs ability to link or re- positive and negative characteristics at the same time.
late representational sets to one another, to map repre- As one 5-year-old vehemently put it, Nope, there is no
sentations onto one another, to use Fischers (1980) way you could be smart and dumb at the same time. You
terminology. Of particular interest to self-development only have one mind!
is one type of representational mapping that is extremely
common in the thinking of young childrena link in the The Role of the Socializing Environment
form of opposites. For example, in the domain of physi- Socializing agents also have an impact on self-
cal concepts, young children can oppose up versus development, in interaction with cognitive acquisitions.
down, tall versus short, and thin versus wide or fat. Thus, children acquire an increasing cognitive apprecia-
Opposites can also be observed in the realm of the tion for the perspective of others that influences self-
descriptions of self and others, where the childs ability development. The child at this level comes to realize that
to oppose good to bad is especially relevant. As ob- socializing agents have a particular viewpoint (not
served earlier, the child develops a rudimentary concept merely a reaction) toward them and their behavior (Hig-
of the self as good at a number of skills. Given that good gins, 1991). As Selman (1980) also observes, the
is defined as the opposite of bad, this cognitive con- improved perspective-taking skills at this age permit
struction typically precludes the young child from being children to realize that others are actively evaluating the
bad, at least at the same time. Thus, the oppositional self (although children have not yet internalized these
mapping takes the necessary form of Im good and evaluations sufficiently to make independent judgments
therefore I cant be bad. However, other people may be about their attributes.) Higgins observes that the view-
perceived as bad at these skills, as the cameo descrip- points of others begin to function as self-guides as the
tion reveals (I know other kids who are bad at things child comes to further identify with what he or she per-
but not me!). Therefore, the structure of such mappings ceives socializing agents expect of the self. These self-
typically leads the child to overdifferentiate favorable guides also function to aid the child in the regulation of
and unfavorable attributes, as demonstrated by findings his or her behavior. However, at this age level, cognitive-
revealing young childrens inability to integrate attri- developmental limitations preclude the internalization
butes such as nice and mean (Fisher et al., 1984) or of others standards and opinions about the self, which
smart and dumb (Harter, 1986a). Moreover, the map- will, with later advances, allow the child to personally
ping structure leads to the persistence of self- come to own such standards and opinions.
descriptions laden with virtuosity. As Higgins (1991) and Selman (1980) have pointed
These principles also apply to childrens understand- out, although children at this age do become aware that
ing of their emotions, in that they cannot integrate emo- others are critically evaluating their attributes, they
tions of opposing valance such as happy and sad (Harter lack the type of self-awareness that would allow them to
& Buddin, 1987). There is an advance over the previous be critical of their own behavior. In I-self/ Me-self ter-
period in that children come to appreciate the fact that minology, the childs I-self is aware that significant oth-
they can have two emotions of the same valence (e.g., ers are making judgments about the Me-self, yet the
Im happy and excited when my parents watch me). I-self cannot directly turn the evaluative beacon on the
However, the representational set for positive emotions Me-self. These processes will only emerge when the
is cognitive separate from negative emotions (e.g., sad, child becomes capable of internalizing the evaluative
mad, or scared). Thus, children at this stage cannot yet judgments of others for the purpose of self-evaluation.
integrate the sets of positive and negative emotions sets As a result, children at this age period will show little
that are viewed as conceptually opposites and therefore interest in scrutinizing the self. As Anna Freud (1965)
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 523

has cogently observed, young children do not naturally combines a number of their competencies (e.g., good at
take themselves as the object of their own observation. running, jumping, and schoolwork). However, all-or-
They are much more likely to direct their inquisitiveness none thinking persists due to a new cognitive acquisition
toward the outside world of events rather than the inner in which different valence attributes are verbally con-
world of intrapsychic experiences. ceptualized as opposites (e.g., good versus bad or nice
There are additional forms of interaction between versus mean). Typically this all-or-none structure leads
cognitive-developmental level and the socializing envi- to self-attributes that are all positive, these beliefs are
ronment that affect the self, including certain advances even more intractable than in the previous period given
in the ability to utilize social comparison information. cognitive and linguistic advances that bring such beliefs
Frey and Ruble (1985, 1990) as well as Suls and into consciousness to the extent that the socializing envi-
Sanders (1982) provide evidence that at this stage chil- ronment supports such positivity.
dren first focus on temporal comparisons ( how I am Rudimentary processes allow the child to appreciate
performing now, compared to when I was younger) and the fact that others are evaluating the self, although cog-
age norms rather than individual difference compar- nitive-developmental limitations preclude the child from
isons with age-mates. As our prototypical subject tells internalizing these evaluations. Advances include the
us, I can climb a lot higher than when I was little and I ability to make temporal comparisons between ones
can run faster, too. Suls and Sanders observe that such past performance. Given the rapid skill development
temporal comparisons are particularly gratifying to during these years, such comparisons contribute to the
young children given the rapid skill development at this highly positive self-evaluations that typically persist at
age level. As a result, such comparisons contribute to this age level. The failure to use social comparison in-
the highly positive self-evaluations that typically per- formation for the purpose of self-evaluation, however,
sist at this age level. contributes to the persistence of unrealistically favor-
Evidence (reviewed in Ruble & Frey, 1991) now re- able self-attributes. As noted in describing the previous
veals that younger children do engage in certain rudi- period, children at this stage are not consciously distort-
mentary forms of social comparison; however, it is ing their self-perceptions. Rather, they have not yet ac-
directed toward different goals than for older children. quired the cognitive skills to develop more realistic
For example, young children use such information to de- self-perceptions.
termine if they have received their fair share of rewards,
rather than for purposes of self-evaluation. Findings also Individual Differences: Adaptive and Maladaptive
indicate that young children show an interest in others Outcomes during Early to Middle Childhood
performance to obtain information about the task de- A major source of individual differences in self-
mands that can facilitate their understanding of mastery representations and self-evaluations continues to derive
goals and improve their learning (Frey & Ruble, 1985; from the caregiving of significant others. During this
Ruble & Dweck, 1995). However, they cannot yet utilize particular developmental period, rudimentary begin-
such information for the purposes of self-evaluation, in nings of looking glass self-processes emerge (Cooley,
large part due to the cognitive limitations of this period; 1902), namely, some appreciation for the opinions of
thus, their evaluations continue to be unrealistic. significant others that will come to shape opinions of
oneself. However, the capacities to engage in looking
Normative Liabilities for Self-Development
glass self-processes, as well as to construct working
between Early to Middle Childhood
models of self, do not emerge at one particular point in
Many of the features of the previous stage persist, in development but evolve gradually over the course of
that self-representations are typically very positive, and childhood. Children begin, toward middle childhood, to
the child continues to overestimate his or her abilities. introject parental values and to realize (through rudi-
Moreover, the child at this period still lacks the ability mentary perspective-taking skills) that not only do par-
to develop an overall concept of his or her worth as a ents have standards that they expect will be met, but
person. With regard to advances, children do begin to also that parents form an evaluative opinion about the
display a rudimentary ability to intercoordinate self- child (Higgins, 1991; Leahy & Shirk, 1985; Ooster-
concepts that were previously compartmentalized; for wegel & Oppenheimer, 1993; Selman, 1980). In I-
example, they can construct a representational set that self/ Me-self terms, the I-self of the child can realize
524 The Self

that others are forming evaluative opinions about ones Similarly, from an attachment theory perspective, a
Me-self. The I-self has not yet internalized or come to working model of self emerges in the crucible of the
own the parental evaluations and therefore the I-self caregiver-infant relationship, a process that continues
cannot yet directly evaluate the Me-selfa process that through childhood: Internal working models are be-
requires such internalizations. Further advances that lieved to reflect experienced interaction patterns be-
allow children to fully engage in Cooleys looking glass tween the child and his or her attachment figures, and
self-processes will emerge in the subsequent period of therefore the developmental working models of self and
middle to late childhood. of the attachment figures are necessarily complemen-
The ability to engage in even the rudimentary tary. Thus, as Bowlby (1969) initially contended, the
processes that emerge during early to middle childhood children who experiences parents as emotionally avail-
point to the importance of early socialization in the able, sensitive to his or her needs, loving, and supportive
family as a source of individual differences in childrens of his or her mastery attempts will construct a working
self-representations. There is considerable evidence model of the self as lovable and competentthe pattern
from different theoretical perspectives that the quality for securely attached children (see also Bretherton,
of caregiving has a tremendous impact on the nature of 1993). Conversely, a working model of self as devalued
the childs self-representations; for example, how favor- and incompetent is associated with a working model of
ably one evaluates the self (the content of ones self parents as rejecting or ignoring of attachment behavior,
image) as well as how features of the self are organized. including interference with exploration.
Thus, in general, parents who are nurturant, sensitive, Although these themes were introduced in describing
responsive, and approving in the context of demanding the earlier period of development, they continue to be
realistically high standards will produce children with relevant during early to middle childhood, from an at-
positive self-evaluations. tachment perspective. Classic models of attachment
Traditional psychodynamic theorists such as Sullivan (e.g., Bowlby, 1969) implied more of a one-time inocu-
(1953) and Winnicott (1958) placed heavy emphasis on lation process in which early experiences of maternal
how the quality of mother-infant interactions impacted sensitivity, emotional availability, love, and support for
self-development, a theme amplified in more contempo- mastery and exploration would lead, during the critical
rary treats of the self (see Stern, 1985). For example, period of infancy and toddlerhood, to positive self-
Winnicott described a pattern of good-enough moth- representations that would carry a person through child-
ering that would promote healthy self-development. The hood, adolescence, and beyond. The precursors of a
good-enough mother responds promptly and appropri- secure attachment relationship leading to positive self-
ately to the infant and toddlers demands, thereby ini- development are necessarily revisited at subsequent
tially promoting feelings of omnipotence or power, stages, given revisions of this earlier model (see Cassidy
which certain theorists consider to be a critical precur- & Shaver, 1999). These neo-attachment theorists would
sor of positive feelings about the self (see also Erikson, argue that a persons attachment style and resulting
1959; Kohut, 1977). The good-enough mother also re- working models or representations of self and others
sponds positively to mastery attempts. During periods will only remain consistent to the extent that the same
when the young childs basic needs are met, such a caregiving pattern continues in subsequent developmen-
mother retreats, supporting the capacity for her child to tal periods. Thus, a more appropriate framework in-
play alone, which Winnicott considered essential to the volves a booster shot model in which consistency will
development of a stable and positive sense of self to only be observed if the same pattern of parenting contin-
emerge in early to middle childhood. Small failures in ues, be it contributing to secure or insecure attachment
parental responsiveness at this period will lead to some styles and their associated working models (see Thomp-
disappointment for the child and less exaggerated feel- son, 1998).
ings of omnipotence. However, according to both Winni-
cott and Kohut, these experiences play a vital role in Patterns That Are More Maladaptive. In the at-
self-other differentiation, allowing the children to both tachment literature (see Bretherton, 1991; Bretherton
separate from the parent and to become more reality- & Munholland, 1999; Cassidy & Shaver, 1999; Critten-
oriented (see also Mahler, 1967). den, 1990; Main, 1995), there have been further dis-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 525

tinctions between less than optimal parenting styles Much of what has been addressed earlier documents
that are associated with three patterns of insecurely at- negativity in the content of self-evaluations. However,
tached children, which have implications for the self- of further interest are findings revealing that interac-
development of children described as (1) having an tions with caregivers also impact the structure or the
avoidance attachment style, (2) being ambivalently at- organization of working models (Bretherton, 1991;
tached, and (3) being disorganized (a more recent style Bretherton and Munholland, 1999). Insensitive care-
identified by Main). givers who ignore the childs signals will produce inse-
The (anxious) avoidant style leads the young child to curely attached children whose working models are less
perceive that the mother is unavailable, nonnurturing, coherently organized from the outset and are less likely
and not sharing positive affect. She is viewed as non- to become well integrated. Parental underattunement
soothing in times of need, as turning away when the leads to impoverished working models because it under-
child is distressed, and sometimes angry. Not feeling mines the infants ability to attend to and subsequently
loved, the child cuts the self off from emotionally label his or her affective states and thereby incorporate
threatening situations. Given this working model of the them into a self-portrait (Crittenden, 1994). The child
mother, the working model of the self follows directly. may defensively exclude painful experiments at the
Thus, the child does not feel lovable, nor does he or she hands of insensitive caregivers. At the other extreme,
feel capable of getting people to meet his or her needs. Stern (1985) observes that parental overattunement (or
Sensitivity to being rebuffed leads to occasional periods intrusiveness) represents a form of emotional theft in
of anger and hostility. Moreover, the precursors of this which the parent accentuates how the infant should feel
style lead the child to eventually feel ineffective in the rather than how the child actually does feel. Thus, ac-
social domain with peers. tual feeling states and related perceptions are not
The (anxious) ambivalent child, also labeled as re- shared but become isolated, contributing to fragmenta-
sistant by some, perceives the mother to be inconsis- tion or a lack of self-coherence.
tently available, sometimes there, sometimes not, Crittenden (1994) further distinguishes between se-
leading to the inability to predict and therefore to trust curely attached and two types of insecurely attached in-
whether she will meet basic and psychological needs. dividuals. Her findings reveal that those with a history
Therefore, distress is expressed in the absence of assur- of secure attachment can access and integrate the vari-
ance, leading to a sense that one is not loved and that the ous memory systems, can view themselves from several
mother is not there to support the mastery of new skills. perspectives, and can accept both their desirable and
Sometimes, when the mother is present, the child feels undesirable features. As a result, they evaluate the self
good. When she is not available, fussiness and resistance more realistically, including the narrative that they con-
are expressed. struct of the self. For example, Cassidy (1988) found
The most recent category describes disorganized-dis- that securely attached 6-year-olds described themselves
oriented infants (Main, 1995). This type of child has in generally positive terms, but they were also able to
even more severe doubts about his or her caregivers point out negative attributes, revealing a self-portrait in
ability to adequately provide comfort or reassurance which they envisage themselves as imperfect.
when needed. The caregiver is experienced as highly in- Those with an avoidant attachment history, whom
consistent, disorganized, and perhaps neglectful or abu- Crittenden labels as defended and Bartholomew and
sive. Cicchettis findings (see Cicchetti, Beegley, Horowitz (1991) identify as dismissing, have less ac-
Carlson, & Toth, 1990) reveal that maltreated, abused cess to their various memory systems given that some
infants are more likely to exhibit this D style. Some (see features of the true self have been held out of awareness,
Crittenden, 1990) have suggested that such children whereas others have been defensively corrected.
manifest a maladaptive combination of child rearing Those with an ambivalent attachment history, which she
that leads to both avoidant and ambivalent tendencies, (1994) labels as coercive, also have more fragmented
including contradictory patterns of behaviors, signs of and distorted working models. Their tendency to blame
fear and confusion, crying, depression, freezing, and others for their misbehavior robs them of the opportu-
numbing that reflect more severe disturbances, negativ- nity to integrate certain behavioral aspects of the self
ity, and inconsistencies in self-development. into their working model. Moreover, the inconsistent
526 The Self

parenting that they have experienced prevents them motives of the perpetrator). As Herman points out, the
from developing an organized or coherent set of internal child must construct some version of reality that justi-
representations. fies continued abuse and therefore inevitably concludes
The potentials for maladaptive self-development that that his or her innate badness is the cause.
were identified for very early childhood exist for this Finally, the preceding section on very early childhood
subsequent period of development, therefore particu- described the rudimentary antecedents of the impover-
larly if the caregiving of socializing agents remains con- ished self that reside in the fact that caregivers do not
sistently negative. However, the effects may become adequately support the childs construction of an autobio-
more evident because the child is more able, given lin- graphical narrative of the childs sense of self. The effects
guistic and cognitive advances, to better verbalize nega- of such lack of scaffolding should become more evident as
tive self-evaluations. children move into middle childhood and should be able to
Abuse at the hands of socializing agents can also verbally express his or her autobiographical sense of
continue to derail the self-system. In chronic and se- selfa narrative of ones past life story, with implica-
vere abuse, the major coping strategy is dissociation tions for the future. However, the failure to express
in which the individual attempts to cognitively split off dreams for the future, to positively describe ones capa-
the traumatic event from consciousnessto detach the bilities, to express pride in ones accomplishments all
self from the traumatic event (Herman, 1992; Putnam, reflect maladaptive if not pathological distortions of self-
1993; Terr, 1991). When such abuse occurs at this pe- development. These symptoms should represent serious
riod of childhood, it conspires with the natural or nor- red flags that require clinical intervention.
mative penchant for cognitive dissociation, splitting, or
fragmentation (Fischer & Ayoub, 1994). Moreover, the
very construction of cognitive structures that con- Middle to Late Childhood: Verbal Cameo of
sciously lead the child of this age to think in opposites, Normative Self-Representations and Self-
one must be all good or all bad, lead to a painful aware- Evaluations
ness that one must be all bad or that the self is totally Im in fourth grade this year, and Im pretty popular, at
flawed. This can lead to compromising symptoms of least with my girl friends. Thats because Im nice to peo-
depression. ple and helpful and can keep secrets. Mostly I am nice to
Briere (1992), based on clinical cases, provides a my friends, although if I get in a bad mood I sometimes
complementary analysis of the sequential logic that say something that can be a little mean. I try to control my
governs the abused childs attempt to make meaning of temper, but when I dont, Im ashamed of myself. Im usu-
his or her experiences. Given maltreatment at the hands ally happy when Im with my friends, but I get sad if there
of a parent or family member, the child first surmises is no one to do things with. At school, Im feeling pretty
that either I am bad or my parents are bad. However, smart in certain subjects like language arts and social
studies. I got As in these subjects on my last report card
the assumption of young children that parents or adult
and was really proud of myself. But Im feeling pretty
authority figures are always right leads to the conclusion
dumb in math and science, especially when I see how well
that parental maltreatment must be due to the fact that a lot of the other kids are doing. Even though Im not
they, as children, are bad (that the acts were their fault), doing well in those subjects, I still like myself as a person,
and that therefore they deserve to be punished. When because math and science just arent that important to me.
children are repeatedly assaulted, they come to conclude How I look and how popular I am are more important. I
that they must be very bad contributing to the sense also like myself because I know my parents like me and so
of fundamental badness at their core. do other kids. That helps you like yourself. (adapted from
From a cognitive-developmental perspective, the Hartner, 1999)
young child who is abused will readily blame the self
(Herman, 1992; Piaget, 1932; Watson & Fischer, 1993; Such self-descriptions are typically observed in chil-
Westen, 1993): Given young childrens natural egocen- dren ages 8 to 11. In contrast to the more concrete self-
trism, they will take responsibility for events they did representations of younger children, older children are
not cause and cannot control. Moreover, as Piaget much more likely to describe the self in terms such as
demonstrated, young children focus on the deed (e.g., popular, nice, helpful, mean, smart, and dumb. Children
the abusive act) rather than on the intention (e.g., the moving into late childhood continue to describe them-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 527

selves in terms of their competencies (e.g., smart, on one target given the emergence of representational
dumb). However, self-attributes become increasingly systems that better allow the child to integrate emo-
interpersonal as relations with others, particularly tion concepts that were previously differentiated.
peers, become an increasingly salient dimension of the Sample responses from our empirical documentation
self (see also Damon & Hart, 1988; Rosenberg, 1979). of this progression (Harter & Buddin, 1987) were as
From the standpoint of emerging cognitive-develop- follows: I was happy that I got a present but mad that
mental (I-self ) processes, these attributes represent it wasnt what I wanted; If a stranger offered you some
traits in the form of higher-order generalizations, inte- candy, you would be eager for the candy but worried
grating more specific behavioral features of the self about whether it was okay. Elsewhere, we have ex-
(see Fischer, 1980; Siegler, 1991). Thus, in the cameo, tended the topic of emotional representations to devel-
the higher-order generalization that she is smart is opmental changes in the understanding of self-affects
based on the integration of scholastic success in both such as pride and shame. The reader is referred to
language arts and social studies. That she also feels Harter (1999).
dumb represents a higher-order construction based
Social Processes
on her math and science performance. Popular also
combines several behaviors: being nice, helpful, and A more balanced view of self in which positive as well
keeping secrets. as negative attributes of the self are acknowledged is
This developmental analysis has focused primarily on also fostered by new social comparison skills. As our
advances in the ability to conceptualize self-attributes. prototypical subject reports, Im feeling pretty dumb
However, these processes can also be applied to emotion in math and science, especially when I see how well the
concepts. Thus, the child develops a representational sys- other kids are doing. A number of studies conducted in
tem in which positive emotions (e.g., Im usually happy the 1970s and early 1980s presented evidence revealing
with my friends) are integrated with negative emotional that it is not until middle childhood that the child can
representations (e.g., I get sad if there is no one to do apply comparative assessments with peers in the service
things with), as a growing number of empirical studies of self-evaluation. From a cognitive-developmental per-
reveal (Carroll & Steward, 1984; Donaldson & Wester- spective, the ability to use social comparison informa-
man, 1986; Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, 1990; Gnepp tion toward the goal of self-evaluation requires that the
et al., 1987; P. Harris, 1983; P. Harris, Olthof, & child have the ability, which is not sufficiently devel-
Meerum-Terwogt, 1981; Harter, 1986b; Harter & Bud- oped at younger ages, to relate one concept to another si-
din, 1987; Reissland, 1985; Selman, 1980). multaneously. In addition to the contribution of
This represents a major conceptual advance over advances in cognitive development (see also Moretti &
the previous two age periods during which young chil- Higgins, 1990), age stratification in school stimulates
dren deny that they can have emotions of opposing va- greater attention to individual differences between age-
lences. Our own developmental findings (see Harter & mates (Higgins & Bargh, 1987; Mack, 1983). More re-
Buddin, 1987) reveal that at this age, the simultaneous cent findings reveal that the primary motive for children
experience of positive and negative emotions can ini- in this age period to utilize social comparison is for per-
tially only be brought to bear on different targets. As sonal competence assessment.
one child subject observed, I was sitting in school The ability to utilize social comparison information
feeling worried about all of the responsibilities of a for the purpose of self-evaluation is founded on cogni-
new pet, but I was happy that I got straight As on my tive-developmental advances or the ability to simultane-
report card. In Fischerian (1980) terms, the child at ously compare representations of self and others.
this level demonstrates a shift of focus, directing the However, it is also supported by the socializing environ-
positive feeling to a positive target or event and then ment. For example, evidence reveals that as children
shifting to the experience of a negative feeling that is move up the academic ladder, teachers make increasing
attached to a negative event. In middle childhood, the use of social comparison information (Eccles & Midg-
concept that the very same target can simultaneously ley, 1989; Eccles, Midgley, & Adler, 1984) and that stu-
provoke both a positive and a negative emotion is not dents are well aware of these educational practices
yet cognitively accessible. However, by late childhood, (Harter, 1996). Moreover, parents may contribute to the
positive and negative emotions can be brought to bear increasing salience of social comparison, to the extent
528 The Self

that they make comparative assessments of how their Moreover, findings (see Glick & Zigler, 1985; Leahy &
child is performing relative to siblings, friends, or Shirk, 1985; Oosterwegel & Oppenheimer, 1993) reveal
classmates. that the real-ideal discrepancy tends to increase with de-
velopment. Two causes of such an increase can be identi-
Normative Liabilities for Self-Development during
fied. First, as noted earlier, social comparison processes
Middle to Late Childhood
lead older children to lower the valence of their self-
A cardinal thesis of this chapter is that cognitive ad- perceptions, viewing themselves less positively. Second,
vances bring about, paradoxically, normative liabilities given increasing perspective-taking skills, children are
for the self-system. The ability to be able to construct a becoming increasingly cognizant of the standards and
global perception of ones worth as a person represents ideals that socializing agents hold for their behavior.
a major developmental acquisitiona milestone, as it Moreover, parents, teachers, and peers may normatively
werein terms of a shift from mere domain-specific raise the bar in terms of their expectations, leading to
self-perceptions to an integrated sense of ones overall higher self-ideals.
self-esteem. However, other cognitive-developmental Increased perspective-taking skills can also directly
acquisitions can serve to lower the valence of this global impact self-perceptions, leading them to be more realis-
perception of self, leading to lowered self-esteem. Find- tic. Protected by limitations in the ability to divine what
ings clearly reveal (see Harter, 1999) that beginning in others truly think of the self, younger children can
middle childhood self-perceptions become more nega- maintain very positive self-perceptions. The developing
tive, normatively, compared to the very positive self- ability to more accurately assess the opinions that oth-
perceptions of the majority of young children. The ers hold about ones characteristics, coupled with in-
emergence of three cognitive skills is noteworthy in this creasing concern about the importance of the views of
regard: (1) the ability to use social comparison for the others toward the self, normatively leads many older
purpose of self-evaluation, (2) the ability to differenti- children to realistically lower their self-evaluations.
ate real from ideal self-perceptions, and (3) increases in We can ask whether the processes that lead to more
social perspective-taking skills. realistic self-evaluations represent liabilities. Many
The ability to employ social comparison for the pur- have argued (see review in Harter, 1999) that realistic
pose of self-evaluation (see Maccoby, 1980; Moretti & self-evaluations are more adaptive beginning in middle
Higgins, 1990; Ruble & Frey, 1991) leads many, with to late childhood, unlike in early childhood where an
the exception of the most competent or adequate in any overestimation on ones capacities may have a positive
given domain, to fall short in their self-evaluations. If a motivational function. Yet, in middle to late childhood,
child therefore judges him- or herself deficient, com- despite some potential blows to the ego, the child must
pared to others, in domains that are deemed important seek to realistically readjust his or her self-perceptions
to the self and others, global self-esteem will be and pursue more adaptive paths of development that are
eroded. Thus, the very ability and penchant, supported consistent with his or her actual attributes.
by the culture (e.g., family, peers, schools, and the
media) to compare oneself with others makes one vul- Individual Differences: Adaptive and Maladaptive
nerable in valued domains (e.g., appearance, popularity, Outcomes in Middle to Late Childhood
scholastic competence, athletic performance, and be- Several formulations, supported by empirical evidence,
havioral conduct). speak to the emergence of individual differences in self-
A second newfound cognitive ability to emerge in representations and associated self-evaluations. From a
middle to late childhood involves the capacity to make Jamesian perspective, those who are genetically blessed
the distinction between ones real and ones ideal self. with talents and/or who are praised for competence in
From a Jamesian perspective, this skill involves the abil- domains deemed important to success will fare the best
ity to distinguish between ones actual competencies or in terms of positive self-evaluations.
adequacies and those to which they aspire and deem im- Moreover, child-rearing practices continue to be crit-
portant. The cognitive realization that one is not meet- ical during middle to late childhood. Parental or care-
ing ones expectations (an ability that young children do giver approval is particularly critical in the childs
not possess) will necessarily lower ones overall level of domain-specific sense of competence and adequacy as
self-esteem, as Jamess formulation accurately predicts. well as global self-worth. Coopersmith (1967), in his
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 529

seminal efforts to unravel the causes of high and low the question Mirror, mirror on the wall, whose opinion
self-esteem in children, described how the socialization is the most critical of all? At this particular develop-
practices of parents impact childrens self-esteem. Par- mental level, we have documented in numerous studies
ents of children with high self-esteem were more likely that of four sources of support: (1) parent, (2) teacher,
to (a) be accepting, affectionate, and involved in their (3) classmate, and (4) close friend. Parental and class-
childs activities; ( b) enforce rules consistently and en- mate support correlate most highly with global self-
courage children to uphold high standards of behavior; esteem. Why is close friend support not more predictive?
(c) prefer noncoercive disciplinary practices, discussing We have argued (Harter, 1999) that, by definition, close
the reasons why the childs behavior was inappropriate; friend support must be high. Furthermore, when one ex-
and (d) be democratic in considering the childs opinion amines the various functions of support from different
around certain family decisions. significant others, support from close friends typically
More recent evidence also reveals that parental sup- manifests itself in the form of empathy, caring, and sen-
port, particularly in the form of approval and accep- sitivity to emotions and solutions to personal problems.
tance, is associated with high self-esteem and the sense Classmate support, in contrast, is more consistent with
that one is lovable (see review by Feiring and Taska, Meads (1934) model of the generalized other, more
1996). Other studies have built on Baumrinds (1989) seemingly objective feedback about ones competencies,
typology of parenting styles, linking them to child and adequacy, and worth as a person.
adolescent self-evaluations. For example, Lamborn, As we see in the next sections on adolescence, recent
Mounts, Steinberg, and Dornbusch (1991) reported that work has begun to address the complexity of the balance
those with more authoritative or democratic parents re- of support from different significant others. Claims of
ported significantly higher self-evaluations in the do- the past (e.g., Rosenberg, 1979) and the present (J. Har-
mains of social and academic competence than did those ris, 1998) suggest that the impact of parent support de-
with authoritarian or neglectful parenting. clines as a child moves from late childhood to early
These findings are consistent with the theorizing of adolescence when the peer influence becomes para-
Cooley (1902) and attachment theorists (Bretherton, mount. However, current research, including our own
1991; Bretherton & Munholland, 1999; Sroufe, 1990; (Harter, 1999), demonstrates that nothing is further
Thompson, Chapter 2, this Handbook, this volume). from the truth.
Benevolent socializing agents who readily provide nur- To return to the theme of the importance of parental
turance, approval sensitivity, emotional availability, and child rearing, and more pathological implications in the
support for mastery attempts will produce children who extreme, children subjected to severe and chronic abuse
mirror and eventually internalize this support in the continue to create images of the self that are despicable,
form of positive self-evaluations. However, in their given the difficulty overcoming post-traumatic stress
search for their image in the social mirror, other chil- disorder (PTSD), including the psychological pain and
dren may well gaze through a glass darkly. Caregivers symptoms that endure in the form of flashbacks and dis-
lacking in responsiveness, nurturance, encourage, and sociative symptoms (Briere, 1992; Fischer & Ayoub,
approval, as well as socializing agents who are rejecting, 1994; Herman, 1992; McCann & Pearlman, 1992; Terr,
punitive, or neglectful, will both cause their children to 1990; van der Kolk, 1987; Westen, 1993; Wolfe, 1989).
develop tarnished images of self, feeling unlovable, in- More than constructing negative self-perceptions, they
competent, and generally unworthy. view the self as fundamentally flawed. Often exces-
Thus, there is considerable evidence that support sively high and unrealistic parental standards that are
from parents as significant others in the childs life will unattainable contribute to these negative views of the
have a powerful influence on self-evaluations ( be they self. Thus, the Me-self, both at the level of domain-spe-
domain-specific or global in nature) or overall self- cific self-perceptions and ones sense of global self-
esteem (see Harter, 1999). Our own research documents esteem, may be irrevocably damaged.
the fact that parental or caregiver support is a major pre- Finally, to return to the interaction of socializing
dictor of global self-worth throughout the childhood practices and movement to new stages of cognitive de-
years. However, as the child moves up the developmental velopment, such movement can be fostered by socializ-
ladder, other sources of support emerge, where peer sup- ing agents, or alternatively, can be delayed if such
port becomes increasingly important. Thus, one can ask environmental support is not forthcoming. One can
530 The Self

imagine scenarios in which there would be little envi- support for a model in which endorsement of the societal
ronmental support for the integration of positive and standards of appearance leads to low self-esteem that
negative attributes or positive and negative emotions. predicts both depression and eating-disordered behav-
For example, in child-rearing situations where children ior. Finally, genetic factors leading to physical charac-
are chronically and severely abused, family members teristics that do not meet cultural standards of
typically reinforce negative evaluations of the child that attractiveness can also contribute to this pattern that
are then incorporated into the self-portrait (Briere, may be particularly resistant to change.
1992; Fischer & Ayoub, 1994; Harter, 1999; Herman,
1992; Terr, 1990; Westen, 1993). As a result, there may Developmental Differences in Self-
be little scaffolding for the kind of self-structure that Representations during Adolescence
would allow the child to develop and integrate both pos-
itive and negative self-evaluations. Moreover, negative The period of adolescence represents a dramatic devel-
self-evaluations that become automatized (Siegler, opmental transition, given pubertal and related physical
1991) will be even more resistant to change. changes, cognitive-developmental advances, and chang-
Thus, to the extent that there is little or no support for ing social expectations. With regard to cognitive-devel-
the normative integration of positive and negative attri- opmental acquisitions, adolescents develop the ability to
butes, children will not advance cognitively. If the ma- think abstractly (Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980; Flavell,
jority of feedback from socializing agents is negative, 1985; Harter, 1999; Higgins, 1991). From a Piagetian
children in this age range (8 to 12) may remain at the pre- (1960) perspective, the capacity to form abstractions
vious level of all-or-none thinking, viewing their behav- emerges with the stage of formal operations in early
ior as overwhelmingly negative. In addition, at a more adolescence. These newfound acquisitions, according to
affective level, there is a considerable body of research Piaget, should equip the adolescent with the hypo-
(see review in Harter, 1999) reveals that there is a very thetico-deductive skills to create a formal theory. This
robust relationship between negative self-perceptions, observation is critical to the topic of self-development,
including low self-esteem, and depression. Depressive given the claims of many (e.g., Epstein, 1973, 1981,
symptoms include lack of energy, profound sadness in 1991; Greenwald, 1980; Kelly, 1955; Markus, 1980;
the form of depressed affect, and hopelessness. Depres- Sarbin, 1962) that the self is a personal epistemology, a
sion, in turn, is highly predictive of suicidal ideation and cognitive construction, or a theory that should possess
suicidal behavior. Thus, caregiving practices resulting in the characteristics of any formal theory. Therefore, a
very negative perceptions of the self put children at risk self-theory should meet those criteria by which any
for serious forms of depressive pathology. good theory is evaluated. Such criteria include the de-
In addition to the incorporation of the opinions of gree to which it is parsimonious, empirically valid, in-
significant others, children come to internalize the stan- ternally consistent, coherently organized, testable, and
dards and values of the larger society. Perceptions of useful. From a Piagetian perspective, entry into the pe-
ones physical attractiveness, in relation to the impor- riod of formal operations should make the construction
tance that is attached to meeting cultural standards of of such a theory possiblebe it a theory about elements
appearance, contribute heavily to ones overall sense of in the world or a theory about the self.
worth as a person (see Harter, 1993, 1999). Those few However, as becomes apparent, the self-
who feel they have attained the requisite physical attri- representations during early and middle adolescence
butes will experience relatively high levels of self- fall far short of these criteria. The self-structure of
esteem. Conversely, those who feel that they fall short these periods is not coherently organized, nor are the
of the punishing standards of appearance that represent postulates of the self-portrait internally consistent.
the cultural ideal will suffer from low self-esteem and Moreover, many self-attributes fail to be subjected to
depression. Moreover, a related liability can be observed tests of empirical validity; as a result, they can be ex-
in the eating-disordered behavior of females, in particu- tremely unrealistic. Nor are self-representations partic-
lar, many of whom display symptoms (e.g., associated ularly parsimonious. Thus, the Piagetian framework
with anorexia) that are life threatening (Harter, 1999). fails to provide an adequate explanation for the dramatic
Our own recent findings (Kiang & Harter, 2004) provide developmental changes in the self-structure that can be
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 531

observed across the substages of adolescence. Rather, as cial appeal are typically quite salient, as findings by
in our analysis of how self-representations change dur- Damon and Hart (1988) indicate. Thus, our prototypical
ing childhood, a neo-Piagetian approach is needed to un- young adolescent admits to being talkative, rowdy,
derstand how changes in cognitive-developmental I-self funny, good-looking, and downright awesome, charac-
processes result in very different Me-self organizational teristics that may enhance acceptance by peers. In addi-
and content at each three age levels: early adolescence, tion to social attributes, self-representations also focus
middle adolescence, and late adolescence. As in our ex- on competencies such as ones scholastic abilities (e.g.,
amination of self-development during childhood, for Im intelligent ) and affects (e.g., Im cheerful and
each age level we first provide a cameo self-description. Im depressed).
What follows is (a) an analysis of the normative-devel- From a developmental perspective, there is consider-
opmental changes in self-representations and self- able evidence that the self becomes increasingly differ-
evaluations, ( b) the exploration of the normative entiated (see Harter, 1998, 1999). During adolescence,
liabilities of each age period, and (c) the discussion of there is a proliferation of selves that vary as a function of
the implications for adaptive and maladaptive self- social context. These include self with father, mother,
development at each period of adolescence. close friends, romantic partners, peers, as well as the
self in the role of student, on the job, and as an athlete
(Gecas, 1972; N. Griffin, Chassin, & Young, 1981; Hart,
Early Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative
1988; Harter, Bresnick, Bouchey, & Whitesell, 1997;
Self-Representations and Self-Evaluations
Harter & Monsour, 1992; Smollar & Youniss, 1985). For
Im an extrovert with my friends: Im talkative, pretty example, as the cameo reveals, the adolescent may be
rowdy, and funny. Im fairly good-looking if I do say so. All cheerful and rowdy with friends, depressed and sarcastic
in all, around people I know pretty well Im awesome, at with parents, intelligent, curious, and creative as a stu-
least I think my friends think I am. Im usually cheerful dent, and shy and uncomfortable around people whom he
when Im with my friends, happy and excited to be doing or she does not know. A critical developmental task,
things with them. I like myself a lot when Im around my therefore, is the construction of multiple selves that will
friends. With my parents, Im more likely to be depressed. I
undoubtedly vary across different roles and relation-
feel sad as well as mad and also hopeless about ever pleasing
ships, as James (1892) observed over 100 years ago.
them. They think I spend too much time at the mall with my
friends, and that I dont do enough to help out at home. They
In keeping with a major theme of this chapter, both
tell me Im lazy and not very responsible, and its hard not cognitive and social processes contribute to this prolif-
to believe them. I get real sarcastic when they get on my eration of selves. Cognitive-developmental advances,
case. The fact of the matter is that what they think about is described earlier, promote greater differentiation (see
still really important. So when they are on my case, it makes Fischer, 1980; Fischer & Canfield, 1986; Harter, 1990b;
me dislike myself as a person. At school, Im pretty intelli- Harter & Monsour, 1992; Keating, 1990). Moreover,
gent. I know that because Im smart when it comes to how I these advances conspire with socialization pressures to
do in classes, Im curious about learning new things, and develop different selves in different relational contexts
Im also creative when it comes to solving problems. My (see Erikson, 1968; Grotevant & Cooper, 1986; Hill &
teacher says so. I get better grades than most, but I dont Holmbeck, 1986; Rosenberg, 1986). For example, bids
brag about it because thats not cool. I can be a real introvert
for autonomy from parents make it important to define
around people I dont know well. Im shy, uncomfortable,
oneself differently with peers in contrast to parents (see
and nervous. Sometimes Im simply stupid, I mean I act re-
ally dumb and say things that are just plain stupid. I worry a
also Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986; White, Speisman, &
lot about what others my age who are not my closest friends Costos, 1983). Rosenberg points to another component
must think of me, probably that Im a total dork. I just hate of the differentiation process in observing that as one
myself when that happens, because what they think is really moves through adolescence, one is more likely to be
important. (adapted from Harter, 1999) treated differently by those in different relational con-
texts. In studies from our own laboratory (see Harter &
With regard to the content of the self-portraits of Monsour, 1992; Harter et al., 1997), we have found that
young adolescents, interpersonal attributes, and social the percentage of overlap in self-attributes generated for
skills that influence interactions with others or ones so- different social contexts ranges from 25% to 30%
532 The Self

among seventh and eighth graders and decreases during self-representations during early adolescence is more
adolescence, to a low of approximately 10% among the rule than the exception (Fischer & Ayoub, 1994;
older teenagers. Harter & Monsour, 1992).
Many (although not all) of the self-descriptions to Another manifestation of the compartmentalization
emerge in early adolescence represent abstractions of these abstract attributes can be observed in the ten-
about the self, based on the newfound cognitive ability dency for the young adolescent to be unconcerned about
to integrate trait labels into higher-order self-concepts the fact that across different roles, certain postulates ap-
(see Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980; Flavell, 1985; Harter, pear inconsistent, as the prototypical self-description
1983; Higgins, 1991). For example, as the prototypical implies (in contrast, at middle adolescence, there is con-
cameo reveals, one can construct an abstraction of the siderable concern). However, during early adolescence,
self as intelligent by combining such traits as smart, the inability to integrate seemingly contradictory char-
curious, and creative. Alternatively, one may create an acteristics of the self (e.g., intelligent versus airhead, ex-
abstraction that the self is an airhead given situations trovert versus introvert, or depressed versus cheerful)
where one feels dumb and just plain stupid. Similarly, has the psychological advantage of sparing the adoles-
an adolescent could construct abstractions that he or she cent conflict over opposing attributes in his or her self-
is an extrovert (integrating the traits of rowdy, talka- theory (Harter & Monsour, 1992). Moreover, as Higgins
tive, and funny) and that he or she is also an introvert observes, the increased differentiation functions as a
in certain situations (when one is shy, uncomfortable, cognitive buffer, reducing the possibility that negative
and nervous). With regard to emotion concepts, one can attributes in one sphere may spread or generalize to
be depressed in some contexts (combining sad, mad, and other spheres (see also Linville, 1987; Simmons & Blyth,
hopeless) as well as cheerful in others (combining happy 1987). Thus, although the construction of multiple
and excited). Thus, abstractions represent more cogni- selves sets the stage for attributes to be contradictory,
tively complex concepts about the self in which various most young adolescents do not identify potential contra-
trait labels can now be appropriately integrated into even dictions or experience conflict, given the compartmen-
higher-order generalizations. talized structure of their abstract self-representations.
Although the ability to construct such abstractions Evidence comes from our own research (see Harter
reflects a cognitive advance, these representations are et al., 1997; Harter & Monsour, 1992), in which we
highly compartmentalized; that is, they are quite dis- asked adolescents at three developmental levels, early
tinct from one another (Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980; Hig- adolescence (seventh grade), middle adolescence (ninth
gins, 1991). For Fischer, these single abstractions are grade), and late adolescence (11th grade) to generate
overdifferentiated, and therefore the young adolescent self-attributes across several roles and then indicate
can only think about each of them as isolated self- whether any of these attributes represented opposites
attributes. According to Fischer, structures that were (e.g., cheerful versus depressed, rowdy versus calm, stu-
observed in childhood reappear at the abstract level. dious versus lazy, at ease versus self-conscious). After
Thus, just as single representations were compartmen- identifying any such opposites, they were asked whether
talized during early childhood, Fischer argues that when any such pairs caused them conflictwere they per-
the adolescent first moves to the level of abstract ceived as clashing in their personality? Across studies,
thought, he or she lacks the ability to integrate the many the specific roles have varied. They have included self
single abstractions that are constructed to define the with a group of friends, with a close friend, with parents
self in different relational contexts. As a result, adoles- (mother versus father), in romantic relationships, in the
cents will engage in all-or-none thinking at an abstract classroom, and on the job. Across a number of converg-
level. For Fischer, movement to a qualitatively new level ing indices (e.g., number of opposites, number of con-
of thought brings with it lack of cognitive control, flicts, or percentage of opposites in conflict) the
and, as a result, adolescents at the level of single ab- findings revealed that attributes identified as contradic-
stractions can only think about isolated self-attributes. tory and experienced as conflicting were infrequent
Thus, contrary to earlier models of mind (Piaget, 1960), among young adolescents.
in which formal operations usher in newfound cogni- An examination of the protocols of young adolescents
tive-developmental abilities that should allow one to reveals that there are potential opposites that go unde-
create an integrated theory of self, fragmentation of tected. Examples not identified as opposites by young
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 533

adolescents ( but that appeared contradictory to our re- ity (the remaining three-fourths) report that their self-
search team) included being talkative as well as shy in esteem did vary significantly as a function of the rela-
romantic relationships, being uptight with family but tional context. In the extreme, one female participant
carefree with friends, being caring and insensitive with reported the lowest possible self-esteem score with par-
friends, being a good student and a troublemaker in ents and the highest possible self-esteem score with fe-
school, being self-conscious in romantic relationships male classmates.
but easygoing with friends, as well as being lazy as a In addition to documenting such variability, our goal
student but hardworking on the job. These observations has been to identify potential causes of these individual
bolster the interpretation (from Fischers theory) that differences. In addressing one determinant, we adopted
young adolescents do not yet have the cognitive ability Cooleys (1902) looking glass self-perspective, in which
to simultaneously compare these attributes to one an- the opinions of significant others are incorporated into
other, and therefore they tend not to detect, or be con- ones sense of personal worth. Building on our previous
cerned about, self-representations that are potential empirical efforts (see Harter, 1990b), we hypothesized
opposites. As one young adolescent put it, when con- that context-specific support, in the form of validation
fronted with the fact that he had indicated that he was for whom one is as a person, should be highly related to
both caring and rude, Well, you are caring with your self-esteem in the corresponding context. The findings
friends and rude to people who dont treat you nicely. corroborated the more specific prediction that support
Theres no problem. I guess I just think about one thing in a given relationship was more highly associated with
about myself at a time and dont think about the other relational self-esteem in that relationship, compared to
until the next day. When another young adolescent was self-esteem in the other three contexts. Thus, the pattern
asked why opposite attributes did not bother her, she of results suggests a refinement of the looking glass self-
succinctly exclaimed, Thats a stupid question. I dont formulation, in that validation from particular signifi-
fight with myself ! As becomes apparent, this pattern cant others will have its strongest impact on how one
changes dramatically during middle adolescence. evaluates ones sense of worth in the context of those
The differentiation of role-related selves, beginning particular others.
in early adolescence, can also be observed in the ten- These findings highlight the fact that with the prolif-
dency to report differing levels of self-esteem across re- eration of multiple selves across roles, adolescents be-
lational contexts. In the prototypical description, the come very sensitive to the potentially different opinions
young adolescent reports that with friends, I like my- and standards of the significant others in each context.
self a lot ; however, with parents, I dislike myself as a As the cameo description reveals, the adolescent reports
person. Around people I dont know well, I just hate high self-esteem around friends who think he or she is
myself. Although the concept of self-esteem has, until awesome, lower self-esteem around parents who think
now, been reserved for perceptions of global self- he or she is lazy and irresponsible, and the lowest
esteem, we have introduced the construct of relational level of self-esteem around strangers who probably
self-esteem (Harter, Waters, & Whitesell, 1998). think Im a total dork. As Rosenberg (1986) observes,
Beginning in the middle school years, adolescents adolescents demonstrate a heightened concern with the
discriminate their level of perceived self-esteem (i.e., reflected appraisals of others. He notes that other peo-
how much they like themselves as a person, across rela- ples differing views of the self (e.g., the respect of the
tional contexts). We have examined these perceptions peer group in contrast to the critical stance of parents)
across a number of such contexts including self-worth will inevitably lead to variability in the self-concept
with parents, with teachers, with male classmates, and across contexts.
with female classmates. Factor-analyses reveal clear In addition to their sensitivity to feedback from oth-
factor patterns with high loadings on the designated fac- ers, young adolescents continue to make use of social
tors (i.e., each relational context) with negligible cross- comparison information. However, with increasing age,
loadings. We have also examined the discrepancy children shift from more conspicuous to more subtle
between individuals highest and lowest relational self- forms of social comparison as they become more aware
esteem scores. Although a minority of adolescents (ap- of the negative social consequences of overt compar-
proximately one-fourth) were found to report little isons; for example, they may be accused of boasting
variation in self-esteem across contexts, the vast major- about their superior performance (Pomerantz, Ruble,
534 The Self

Frey, & Greulich, 1995). As the prototypical young ado- self, depending on the situation or moment in time.
lescent describes in the cameo, I get better grades than Third, adolescents concern with what others think of
most, but I dont brag about it because thats not cool. them leads to efforts at impression management, pro-
voking variations in the self across relational contexts.
Normative Liabilities for Self-Development during Finally, at times, adolescents are treated as more adult-
Early Adolescence like (e.g., on a job) whereas at other times, they are
As with the entry into any new developmental level, treated as more childlike (e.g., with parents at home).
there are liabilities associated with these emerging self- Thus, the self fluctuates in tandem.
processes. For example, although abstractions are devel- Our own findings on the emergence of how self-
opmentally advanced cognitive structures, they are esteem varies as a function of ones relationships (what
removed from concrete, observable behaviors and there- we have termed relational self-esteem) is consistent with
fore more susceptible to distortion. The adolescents Rosenbergs analysis (Harter, 1999). The young adoles-
self-concept, therefore, becomes more difficult to ver- cent is not yet troubled by what could be viewed as in-
ify and is often less realistic. As Rosenberg (1986) ob- consistent self-representations because he or she cannot
serves, when the self comes to be viewed as a collection simultaneously evaluate them as contradictory. How-
of abstractions, uncertainties are introduced because ever, there are liabilities associated with this inability.
there are few objective and unambiguous facts about The compartmentalization of abstractions about the self
ones sensitivity, creativity, morality, dependability, precludes the construction of an integrated portrait of
and so on (p. 129). Moreover, the necessary skills to self. The fact that different significant others may hold
apply hypothetico-deductive thinking to the postulates differing opinions about the self makes it difficult to de-
of ones self-system are not yet in place. Although the velop the sense that the self is coherent. With movement
young adolescent may have multiple hypotheses about into middle adolescence, abstract self-descriptors be-
the self, he or she does not yet possess the ability to cor- come far less isolated or compartmentalized. However,
rectly deduce which are true, leading to distortions in the emerging structures that follow bring with them new
self-perceptions. liabilities.
The all-or-none thinking of this period, in the form of Finally, there are domain-specific normative liabili-
overgeneralizations that the young adolescent cannot ties that are associated with educational transitions.
cognitively control (Fischer, 1980), also contributes to Young adolescents all shift from an elementary school to
unrealistic self-representations, in that at one point in either a middle school or junior high school that typi-
time one may feel totally intelligent or awesome, whereas cally draws on several elementary feeder schools. Thus,
at another point in time one may feel like a dork. Thus, they must now move into a group of peers, many of
the adolescent sense of self will vacillate, given the in- whom they have previously not known (typically two-
ability to cognitively control ones self-representations. thirds to three-fourths of the peer group will be new).
In describing this barometric self during adoles- Given the young adolescents heightened concern with
cence, Rosenberg (1986) points to a different set of how others view the self, an important source of global
more social causes. He cites considerable literature re- self-esteem, there may be understandable shifts in
vealing that adolescents experience an increased con- global self-esteem, if individuals perceive that their so-
cern with what their peers think of them, findings that cial acceptance is higher or lower than when they were
are relevant to Cooleys looking glass self model. This in elementary school.
heavy dependence on the perceptions of others opin- Eccles and Midgley (1989) have also pointed to dif-
ions tends to set the stage for volatility in ones assess- ferent emphases in the educational system during the
ment of the self. However, there is inevitable ambiguity transition to middle school or elementary school that
about others attitudes toward the self because one can have implications for perceptions of a childs scholastic
never have direct access to the mind of another. Thus, at- competence. They note that there is considerably more
tributions about others thought processes may change emphasis on social comparison (e.g., public posting of
from one time period to another. The second reason for grades, ability grouping, or teachers, in their feedback
fluctuating self-evaluations inheres in the fact that dif- to classes, verbally acknowledging the personal results
ferent significant others have different opinions of the of competitive activities). These educational practices
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 535

represent a mismatch given the adolescents needs. At a We do not question these statistical findings; they are
time when young adolescents are painfully self- consistent with our own. We do question the conclusions
conscious, the school system heightens the salience of and suggest that the comparison of these correlations is
social comparison in conjunction with publicizing each not the appropriate test (see Harter, 1999).
students performance. In addition to the greater em- Why might merely correlating domain-specific com-
phasis on social comparison, the standards for perfor- petence or adequacy scores with global self-esteem or
mance shift from ef fort to ability, according to Eccles self-worth result in values comparable to those based on
and colleagues. They note that in elementary school, correlating these domain-specific values with global
there is more emphasis on effort: Try harder and you self-worth for just those individuals rating given do-
can do better. In middle and junior high schools, how- mains as important? The answer lies in the fact that the
ever, poorer performance is attributed to lack of vast majority of older children and adolescents (approx-
scholastic ability, leading the young adolescent to feel imately 80%, on average) rate these domains as impor-
that he or she does not have the aptitude to succeed or tant, and thus the two correlations will be based on
that he or she lacks intelligence. For those not perform- virtually the same set of participants. This is not sur-
ing well, these practices can lead to declines in self- prising because as Marsh, himself, acknowledges, those
perceptions of academic ability, shifts that will be of us that have developed domain-specific measures of
exacerbated in contexts of high public feedback and self-concept have purposely included those domains im-
greater social comparison. portant to individuals at various developmental levels.
It is our contention that the more statistically parsi-
Individual Differences: Adaptive and Maladaptive monious empirical models espoused by Marsh and his
Outcomes during Early Adolescence colleagues in which importance ratings are ignored may
The frameworks of James (1892) and Cooley (1902), in obscure the actual psychological processes through
conjunction with attachment theory, provide perspec- which individuals formulate an overall sense of their
tives on the tremendous individual differences that one worth as a person. We would claim, beginning in adoles-
can observe in self-evaluations beginning in adoles- cence, that processes including the ability to think
cence. From a Jamesian perspective, the congruence or about, to reflect on ones self and the causes of ones
discrepancy between ones perceptions of competence overall feelings of self-worth, are not necessarily parsi-
in age-appropriate domains and the importance of suc- monious. Considerable literature (see reviews in Harter,
cess attached to each domain have been demonstrated to 1990a, 1999) reveals the heightened introspectiveness
be a major determinant of ones global self-esteem or and self-consciousness that emerges in adolescence.
self-worth (1990). Thus, those who are able to positively Thus, if we truly want to understand the processes un-
evaluate their successes in domains deemed important derlying adolescents construction of evaluative judg-
to the self will report high self-esteem. A parallel pro- ments about the self (rather than merely predict a value
cess is the ability to tout the importance of those do- statistically) our own findings indicate that one must
mains in which one is succeeding. Conversely, those take into account the importance attached to those self-
reporting failures in domains of importance will report concept domains that may be relevant to the formulation
low self-esteem. Such individuals appear unable to dis- of an overall view of self.
count the importance of domains in which they are not Elsewhere (Harter, 1999) we have reported on four
successful. different empirical approaches that demonstrate the im-
Not all researchers in the field endorse the notion portance of ratings. Additional recent work by MacDon-
that the importance attached to success adds to the pre- ald, Saltzman, and Leary (2003) has demonstrated, with
diction of global self-esteem, notably Marsh and his col- college students, that perceived importance, particu-
leagues (Hattie & Marsh, 1996; Marsh, 1993). They larly as it might affect others judgments of self, di-
base their claims on findings indicating that merely cor- rectly impacted global self-esteem. In addition, for
relating perceived competence or adequacy scores with those interested in clinical or educational interventions,
global self-esteem (ignoring importance) yields values we have also suggested how interventions will be quite
that are comparable to those based on procedures in different if one takes an approach that only focuses on
which perceived importance is also taken into account. perceptions of domain-specific competence or adequacy
536 The Self

versus an approach in which one also takes into account peer approval report more positive self-evaluations
perceptions of the importance of success in correspon- (Dubois, Reach, Tevendale, & Valentine, 2001; Roberts,
ding domains (Harter, 1999). Seidman, & Pederson, 2000).
Cooleys (1902) looking glass formulation and at- Therefore, beginning in early adolescence, there is a
tachment theorists explorations into working models of heightened concern with how others view the self, a nor-
the self (see Bretherton & Munholland, 1999), bolster mative process that has implications for the salience of
the social framework for viewing individual differences those determinants of self-esteem that have been articu-
in self-worth, particularly as young adolescents are be- lated in Cooleys (1902) looking glass self-formulation.
coming more cognizant of their own thinking about If significant others provide support for whom the young
themselves. However, more cognizant, as our earlier adolescent is as a person, for those attributes that the
developmental analysis reveals, does not necessarily young adolescent feels truly define the self, he or she
translate into more realistic. The more abstract self- will experience the self as authentic. However, the con-
evaluations are further removed from behavioral reality struction of a self that is too highly dependent on the in-
(see Harter, 1999). In early to middle adolescence, ternalization of the opinions of others can, under some
teenagers do not have the ability to engage in hypo- circumstances, lead to the creation of a false self that
thetico-deductive thinking to arrive at realistic conclu- does not mirror his or her authentic experience. In our
sions about the self. It is for this reason that recent own research (Harter, 1999), we have found that it is not
findings (reviewed in Harter, 1999) and more classic re- until early adolescence that the concept of acting as a
views (see Shrauger & Schoeneman, 1979) have con- false self becomes very salient in the consciousness of
cluded that self-perceptions of approval from significant young teenagers. The detection of hypocrisy, not only in
others will be a better predictor of constructs such as others but also in the self, emerges as a critical filter in
self-esteem than actual measures of support from signif- evaluating others as well as the self.
icant others. Our own findings (Harter, Marold, Whitesell, &
In the previous section on late childhood, the issue Cobbs, 1996) reveal that unhealthy levels of false-self
was raised as to whose opinionparents or peersis behavior are particularly likely to emerge if caregivers
most critical to the continuing development of a childs make their approval contingent on the young adoles-
overall sense of self-worth or self-esteem. For many cent living up to unrealistic standards of behavior,
years, textbook renditions of the impact of parents and based on unattainable standards dictated by parents.
peers have made the assumption that the influence of We have labeled this phenomenon conditional sup-
parents wanes as a child enters adolescence (see Berndt port although from interviews we have learned that
& Burgy, 1996; Harter, 1999). A resurgence of a focus this is a misnomer in that adolescents do not perceive
on the impact of peers can also be seen in the work of J. parental responses, in the face of such demands, as
Harris (1998) who makes strong arguments for why par- supportive. Rather, conditionality reflects the psy-
ents, with the exception of their genetic contribution, chological hoops through which young adolescents
matter little in the psychological development of their must jump to please the parents, given the parental
children. In contrast, attachment theorists (see Brether- agenda. Those adolescents who experience such a con-
ton & Munholland, 1999) continue to assert, as did ditional atmosphere are forced to adopt a socially im-
Bowlby, that the initial attachment with the mother, in planted self: They must learn to suppress what they
particular, is critical to developing a positive working feel are true self-attributes, in an attempt to garner the
model of self and other that will impact future relation- needed approval from parental caretakers. Here, the
ships with peers. terminology purposely switches from caregiver to
An increasingly sophisticated body of research ad- caretaker in a metaphorical effort to convey the fact
dresses the more interesting and complex question of that such socialization practices take away from the
the balance between parental and peer support because care of whom one is as a person, ones true self. Our
it impacts self-evaluations. Using more advanced statis- findings indicate that those experiencing high levels of
tical techniques, including cluster-analyses and longitu- conditionality from parents will express hopelessness
dinal predictive designs, recent studies have about their ability to please the parents that then trans-
demonstrated that young adolescents who are able to lates in high levels of false-self behavior in an attempt
sustain positive parental support and garner positive to garner some level of needed parental support. Of
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 537

particular relevance is that high levels of false-self be- Peer Rejection, Humiliation, and Implications for
havior are directly related to low levels of self-esteem. the High Profile School Shootings. More recently,
As our model has revealed (Harter, 1999), low levels we have become focused on the role of peer rejection,
of self-esteem are highly correlated with self-reported not merely the lack of peer approval. Our initial interest
depressive symptomatology that can, for some adoles- in this construct came from an analysis of the emerging
cents, lead to suicidal thoughts and actions. profiles of the, now, eleven high-profile cases in which
Chronic and severe abuse continues to put an adoles- White, middle-class older children and adolescents,
cent at even more extreme risk for suppressing his or her from small cities or suburbs, have gone on shooting
true self and displaying various forms of inauthentic or sprees killing peers, and in a few cases, school officials
false-self behavior. Such a process has its origins in who were random targets rather than specifically identi-
childhood, given the very forms of parenting that consti- fied individuals. What became evident, in the analysis of
tute psychological abuse. As described earlier, parenting media reports, is that all of these male youth killers had
practices that represent lack of attunement to the childs a history of peer rejection and humiliation. As a psychol-
needs, empathic failure, lack of validation, threats of ogist who for many years has contributed to (and kept
harm, coercion, and enforced compliance all cause the up with) the literature on emotional development in
true self to go underground (Bleiberg, 1984; Stern, children and adolescents, it was astounding to learn that
1985; Winnicott, 1958, 1965) and lead to what Sullivan we have no literature on humiliation. There is ample lit-
(1953) labeled as not me experiences. erature on shame, guilt, embarrassment, but virtually
Our model of the determinants, correlates, and con- nothing about humiliation. Yet, we can all appreciate the
sequences of self-esteem (see Harter, 1999) becomes in- fact ( be it from our own experience or the experience of
creasingly relevant at early adolescence and beyond our children) that humiliation is a daily event in schools
where there is strong empirical support across numerous for many children. For the school shooters, extreme feel-
studies (see review in Harter, 1999). Findings reveal that ings of chronic humiliation by peers, due to excessive
lack of both parental and peer support can lead to patho- teasing, taunting, and physical insults, eventually led
logical levels of low self-worth, depressed affect, and them to psychologically snap, leading to random
hopelessness, which may provoke suicidal ideation if not deaths and in the case of the Columbine teens to suicide.
suicidal behaviors. An examination of the media accounts of the school
Our findings (see Harter, 1999) document that while shooters made it obvious that many of the determinants
peer support increases in its predictability of global in our model could be found in the lives of these adoles-
self-esteem between late childhood and early adoles- cents (see Harter, Low, & Whitesell, 2003). As a result,
cence, the impact of parental support does not decline. we examined a revised model in which we added angry
Previous textbook portrayals of adolescence imply that aggression as well as violent ideation. We examined this
parental influences decline as a child moves into adoles- model in a normative sample of middle school students.
cence. However, nothing is further from the truth when Through path-analytic techniques, we demonstrated
we examine the impact of parental support, including that the data fit the model exceedingly well: The an-
conditionality, on self-processes including false-self be- tecedents in the model, domain-specific perceived inad-
havior, global self-esteem, and the related correlates of equacies predicted lack of approval from peers and
depressed affect, hopelessness, and suicidal ideation. parents alike. These determinants, in turn, predicted
However, the peer culture does come to loom large in low self-esteem, depressed affect, angry affect, and
adolescence. Peer support and approval, or its absence, hopelessness, all of which predicted both suicidal
is a powerful predictor of what we have labeled the de- ideation and violent ideation. Consistent with the clini-
pression /adjustment composite that includes self- cal literature on the comorbidity of internalizing and
esteem or self-worth, affect /mood (along a continuum externalizing symptoms, we found a correlation of r =
of depressed to cheerful), and hope ( hopeless to hope- .55 between suicidal and violent ideation toward others.
ful) that has empirically been demonstrated to predict Thus, the determinants in our model, if negative, put
suicidal thinking. Lack of peer approval appears to be adolescents at pathological risk for endangering their
more directly linked to perceived inadequacies in the own and others lives.
domains of physical appearance, likability by peers, and We have also pursued the emotion of humiliation and
athletic competence. its role in contributing to violent ideation. In this Harter,
538 The Self

Low, & Whitesell study (2003), we wrote vignettes that troversy over whether they may also be a dark side to
simulated some of the types of humiliating events that high self-esteem.
were experienced by the school shooters. We then asked So what is the controversy? One vocal group of theo-
middle school students what other emotions they might rists (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996) has argued
experience (e.g., anger or depression) and what behav- that there is a subset of individuals with high but fragile
iors they might exhibit, along a continuum from doing self-esteem who are often aggressive in respond to per-
nothing to acting violently toward the perpetrators or to- ceived ego threats. Baumeister and colleagues contend
ward anyone (given the randomness of the actual school that individuals who report high self-esteem in combi-
shooting events). While the majority of students re- nation with high narcissism, low empathy, and sensitiv-
ported that they would be humiliated (given that the vi- ity to rejection will, in the face of threats to the ego,
gnettes were designed to be humiliating) we identified a exhibit violent tendencies.
group of violent ideators (in the minority) and a group We (Harter & McCarley, 2004) believe that Baumeis-
who did not report that they would think about violent ters composite of predictors has some merit, with the
revenge. We then sought to determine what distin- exception of the role of self-esteem. Specifically, we
guished the two groups, finding that those entertaining predicted that high narcissism, low empathy, sensitivity
violent thoughts expressed higher levels of anger and de- to rejection coupled with low self-esteem (not high self-
pression. In addition, the violent ideators reported esteem) would lead young adolescents, in the face of
higher levels of negative determinants in the model, such threats to the ego, to violent ideation. To assess violent
as more peer rejection, less parental support, lower self- ideation, participants read humiliating scenarios that
concept scores (e.g., appearance or peer likability), represented threats to the ego and were then asked to
lower self-worth, and greater hopelessness. Thus, cer- rate how they would typically response in such a situa-
tain factors in histories of violent ideators propel them tion, ranging from doing nothing to seriously harming
to thoughts of seriously harming others and themselves, the perpetrator. Self-esteem was measured by our
which are pathological outcomes that may require clini- Global Self-Worth Scale (Harter, 1985). Narcissism,
cal interventions given that they may be putting them- empathy, and sensitivity to rejection were assessed by
selves and others at serious risk. adaptations of previously developed measures, all of
In a subsequent study, we sought to more specifically which were found to be reliable (alphas ranging from .76
investigate what were some of the factors that lead hu- to .87). Regression analyses demonstrated that high nar-
miliation to result in violent ideation as well as suicidal cissism and low empathy significantly predicted violent
ideation, given the paucity of work on the emotion of ideation. However, in contrast to Baumeisters claims,
humiliation. Our findings have documented that teasing higher self-esteem was associated with lower levels of
and taunting and bullying, particularly in the presence violent ideation. Further, when self-reported conduct
of an audience who mocks the victim, lead to humilia- (positive to negative) and the frequency of experiencing
tion. Humiliation, in turn, serves to provoke prototypi- humiliating events were included in the model, they both
cal reactions, including revenge, wanting to hide, or explained a substantial proportion of the variance in vi-
attempts to minimize the humiliation (Harter, Kiang, olent ideation.
Whitesell, & Anderson, 2003). We are pursuing this In addition to regression analyses, identification of
prototypical approach to humiliation currently. two extreme groups, those high and low on violent
ideation differed significantly with violent ideators re-
Is There a Dark Side to High Self-Esteem? Our porting higher narcissism, lower empathy, and greater
own modeling efforts demonstrate that it is low self- sensitivity to rejection, as Baumeister would predict,
esteem that is consistently related to suicidal and vio- but lower self-esteem that is not consistent with his for-
lent thinking. These findings are consistent with the mulation. In a subsequent study, we added fluctuating
broader literature (see Harter, 1999) revealing that high self-esteem, conduct, and the frequency of humiliating,
self-esteem is a psychological commodity associated rejection events (Harter & McCarley, 2004). These in-
with positive adjustment and mental-health outcomes. creased the predictability of violence ideation, both in
Low self-esteem, in contrast, has been viewed as a lia- regression analyses and in comparisons of violent and
bility and has been associated with poor adjustment in nonviolent ideators. Violent ideators reported fluctua-
the literature (e.g., depression, anxiety, conduct prob- tion self-esteem, greater conduct problems and a greater
lems, or teen pregnancy). However, there is current con- frequency of having established humiliating events that
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 539

were ego threatening. Thus, our understanding of but are nevertheless at pathological risk. Many of these
thoughts qualifies and broadens the precursors. Not only processes begin in early adolescence (and even late
is self-esteem not predictive of violence but self-esteem childhood) but continue into middle and later adoles-
and narcissism are uncorrelated (r = .01), indicating cence, as the range of ages of the actual school shooters
that these are different constructs both conceptually, as reveals, continuing the presence of risk factors.
scales define the two constructs, and empirically. Nar-
cissism entails a sense of entitlement, of superiority, of Pathological Eating-Disordered Behavior. Our
exhibitionism, whereas high self-esteem is defined as model identifies one self-concept domain that robustly
liking and respecting who one is as a person. There is a affects global self-esteem across ages and cultures,
need to distinguish between these two concepts, concep- namely, perceived physical appearance or attractive-
tually, methodologically, and empirically. ness. In reviewing the inextricable link between per-
Finally, in this study, we uncovered an interesting ceived appearance and self-esteem, between the outer
finding that is cause for concern. In the high profile self and the inner self (see Harter, 1999), it became very
cases of the school shooters, the vast majority had not apparent that this link is profoundly impacted by cul-
been in any major trouble with the law and had not come tural standards of appearance for each gender. That cul-
to the attention of teachers or school personnel as poten- tures tout physical attractiveness as the measure of ones
tial troublemakers. Teachers, school officials, and stu- worth as a person has been amply demonstrated in con-
dents were astounded that these boys committed such temporary society, as well as historically (Hatfield &
violent acts. Debriefing efforts of the surviving students Sprecher, 1986). The empirical findings (reviewed in
and families at Columbine, conducted by University of Harter, 1999) indicate that Pearson correlations range
Denver clinicians and some in private practice, revealed from the .40s to the .80s. Moreover, investigators have
that students and their families were realistically fearful revealed that these relationships are not merely statisti-
that they would not be able to detect who, in the future, cal but are very much embedded in the consciousness of
might commit such acts because there were few warning individuals who are aware of this link. In our own work
signs that they were capable of such behavior. These (Kiang & Harter, 2004), we have found strong support
concerns are real, given findings from Harter and Mc- for a model in which awareness of current cultural val-
Carley (2004). In this study, we asked teachers to rate ues (e.g., being attractive will lead to higher self-
student conduct (e.g., getting in trouble or potential for esteem, meeting standards of appearance will make
violent thinking) and among the subgroup of violent people more popular, and people who are overweight are
ideators who had rated their own conduct as quite nega- discriminated against) are highly endorsed. However,
tive, teachers incorrectly rated one-third of the group, there is enough variability in these scores to relate such
giving them very positive scores on conduct. This is awareness to perceptions of ones own appearance,
concerning the difficulty in assessing certain students at which, in turn, predict level of self-esteem and eating-
risk. Although such violent ideators may not be at risk disordered perceptions and behaviors. Specifically,
for violent action (given the low percentage of such acts those endorsing these cultural values or links reported
in most schools), violent ideation represents a different more negative views of their appearance, lower self-
kind of pathological risk factor to the extent that it inter- esteem, more psychological correlates of eating disor-
feres with attention and concentration of scholastic en- ders and more eating-disordered behaviors.
deavors, with socially appropriate behaviors that would This particular study was conducted with college stu-
promote positive peer interactions, and so on. The very dents. However, the seeds of such a model are sown in
presence of this subgroup of violent ideators suggests early adolescence (if not earlier) as teenagers of both
that they represent a type of student with different in- genders are well aware of the prevailing norms for desir-
trapsychic dynamics compared to those students with able appearance. For adult females in the 2000s, one
histories of conduct problems, delinquency, and acting must be tall, very thin, weigh very little (around 110 to
out patterns, all of whom readily come to the attention 115), have ample breasts, and a pretty face and hair, all
of teachers, school officials, and peers. Those not show- of which is an unattainable combination for more than
ing these patterns are of concern in terms of their iden- 90% of the female population. Recent statistics (Kil-
tification. One of our goals is to devise a short-form of bourne, 1994) indicated that the average American
our instruments that will allow schools to identify those woman is 54 and weights 140. Standards have been ex-
who may have escaped the attention of school personnel ceedingly punishing for females for decades. What is
540 The Self

new in the past 2 decades is the fact that the bar has been back and forth, which means I dont do all that well in
raised for males in our society. No longer is sex appeal terms of my grades. But that causes problems at home,
to be judged by status, wealth, position, and power but where Im pretty anxious when Im around my parents.
by physical standards of attractiveness as well. Muscular They expect me to get all As, and get pretty annoyed with
build, abs, biceps, physique, hair (on head as well as me when report cards come out. I care what they think
about me, and so then I get down on myself, but its not
face) have all come to define the new ideals for men (see
fair! I mean I worry about how I probably should get better
Harter, 1999).
grades, but Id be mortified in the eyes of my friends if I
These standards are not lost on our young adoles- did too well. So, Im usually pretty stressed-out at home,
cents. Children succumb to the same discouragement and can even get very sarcastic, especially when my par-
about not being able to emulate the models, singers, and ents get on my case. But I really dont understand how I
movie stars in the limelight. The importance of meeting can switch so fast from being cheerful with my friends,
these standards becomes particularly salient during then coming home and feeling anxious, and then getting
early adolescence as teenagers face inevitable pubertal frustrated and sarcastic with my parents. Which one is the
changes that signal their impending adulthood. Thus, real me? I have the same question when Im around boys.
they look to the adult standards as the physical markers Sometimes, I feel phony. Say I think some guy might be in-
for what defines attractiveness, appeal, social accept- terested in asking me out. I try to act different, like Brit-
ability, all of which determine ones self-esteem. ney Spears, Ill be a real extrovert, fun-loving and even
flirtatious, and think I am really good-looking. Its impor-
The genetic throw of the dice lead some young ado-
tant to be good-looking like the models and movie stars.
lescent males and females to fare better than others in
Thats what makes you popular. I know in my heart of
the appearance wars. For example, early maturing girls hearts that I can never look like her, so why do I even try.
are at a distinct disadvantage given the current emphasis Its makes me hate myself and feel depressed. Plus, when I
on thinness and height because on average they are try to look and act like her, then everybody, I mean every-
heavier and shorter, compared to later maturing girls body else is looking at me like they think I am totally
who tend to be thinner and taller. The pattern is just the weird! They dont act like they think Im attractive so I
opposite for adolescent males in that earlier maturing end up thinking I look terrible. I just hate myself when
teens tend to be taller and more muscular, which gives that happens! Because it gets worse! Then I get self-
them a physical edge. Thus, beginning in early adoles- conscious and embarrassed and become radically intro-
cent, evaluations of ones appearance take on critical verted, and I dont know who I really am! Am I just acting
implications for ones global self-esteem. Those not like an extrovert, am I just trying to impress them, when
really Im an introvert! But I dont really care what they
meeting the gold standards are at serious risk for patho-
think, anyway. I mean I dont want to care, that is. I just
logical forms of depression and possibly suicide, as well
want to know what my close friends think. I can be my
as eating disorders that can be life threatening. Al- true self with my close friends. I cant be my real self with
though this preoccupation initially becomes salient in my parents. They dont understand me. What do they
early adolescent, it continues throughout the life span. know about what its like to be a teenager? They treat me
like Im still a kid. At least at school, people treat you
more like youre an adult. That gets confusing, though. I
Middle Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative mean, which am I? When youre 15, are you still a kid or
Self-Representations and Self-Evaluations an adult? I have a part-time job and the people there treat
me like an adult. I want them to approve of me, so Im very
What am I like as a person? Youre probably not going to responsible at work, which makes me feel good about my-
understand. Im complicated! With my really close self there. But then I go out with my friends and I get
friends, I am very tolerant. I mean, Im understanding and pretty crazy and irresponsible. So, which am I, responsible
caring. With a group of friends, Im rowdier. Im also usu- or irresponsible? How can the same person be both? If my
ally friendly and cheerful but I can get pretty obnoxious parents knew how immature I act sometimes, they would
and intolerant if I dont like how theyre acting. Id like to ground me forever, particularly my father. Im real distant
be friendly and tolerant all of the time, thats the kind of with him. Im pretty close to my mother though. But its
person I want to be, and Im disappointed in myself when hard being distant with one parent and close to the other,
Im not. At school, Im serious, even studious every now especially if we are all together, like talking at dinner.
and then, but on the other hand, Im a goof-off too, be- Even though I am close to my mother, Im still pretty se-
cause if youre too studious, you wont be popular. So I go cretive about some things, particularly the things about
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 541

myself that confuse me. So I think a lot about who is the self-representations in a manner that would resolve ap-
real me, and sometimes I try to figure it out when I write parent contradictions. Therefore, at the level of abstract
in my diary, but I cant resolve it. There are days when I mappings, the awareness of these opposites causes con-
wish I could just become immune to myself ! (adapted siderable intrapsychic conflict, confusion, and distress
from Harter, 1999) (Fischer et al., 1984; Harter & Monsour, 1992; Higgins,
Self-descriptions are likely to increase in length during 1991), given the inability to coordinate these seemingly
this period, as adolescents become increasingly intro- contradictory self-attributes. For example, our prototyp-
spective and morbidly preoccupied with what others ical adolescent agonizes over whether she is an extrovert
think of them (Broughton, 1978; Elkind, 1967; Erikson, or an introvert (Am I just acting like an extrovert, am I
1959, 1968; Harter, 1990a; Lapsley & Rice, 1988; just trying to impress them, when really Im an intro-
Rosenberg, 1979). The unreflective self-acceptance of vert? So which am I, responsible or irresponsible?
earlier periods of development vanishes, and, as Rosen- How can the same person be both?). Such cognitive-de-
berg observes, what were formerly unquestioned self- velopmental limitations contribute to the emergence of
truths now become problematic self-hypotheses. The what James (1892) identified as the conflict of the dif-
tortuous search for the self involves a concern with what ferent Mes.
or who am I (Broughton, 1978), a task made more diffi- In addition to such confusion, these seeming contra-
cult given the multiple Mes that crowd the self- dictions lead to very unstable self-representations that
landscape. There is typically a further proliferation of are also cause for concern (e.g., I dont really under-
selves as adolescents come to make finer differentia- stand how I can switch so fast from being cheerful with
tions; in the cameo, the adolescent describes a self with my friends, then coming home and feeling anxious, and
really close friends (e.g., tolerant) versus with a group then getting frustrated and sarcastic with my parents.
of friends (e.g., intolerant) and a self with mother (e.g., Which one is the real me?). The creation of multiple
close) versus father (e.g., distant). The acquisition of selves, coupled with the emerging ability to detect po-
new roles, for example, self at a job, may also require tential contradictions between self-attributes displayed
the construction of new context-specific attributes (e.g., in different roles, naturally ushers in concern over
responsible). which attributes define the true self. However, from a
Moreover, additional cognitive I-self processes emerge normative perspective, the adolescent at this level is not
that give the self-portrait a very new look (Case, 1985; equipped with the cognitive skills to fully solve the
Fischer, 1980). Whereas, in the previous stage, single ab- dilemma (e.g., So I think a lot about who is the real me,
stractions were isolated from one another, during middle and sometimes try to figure it out when I write in my
adolescence one acquires the ability to make comparisons diary, but I cant resolve it ).
between single abstractions, namely, between attributes in As introduced in the previous section on early adoles-
the same role-related self or across role-related selves. cence, our own research has been directed toward an ex-
Fischer labels these new structures abstract mappings, amination of the extent to which adolescents at three
in that the adolescent can now map constructs about the developmental levels identify opposing self-attributes
self onto one another or directly compare them. There- and report that they are experienced as conflictual (Har-
fore, mappings force the individual to compare and con- ter & Monsour, 1992; Harter et al., 1997). We have de-
trast different attributes. It should be noted that abstract termined, across several studies, that young adolescents
mappings have features in common with the representa- infrequently detect opposites in their self-portrait. How-
tional mappings of childhood, in that the cognitive links ever, it was predicted, according to the analysis pre-
that are initially forged often take the form of opposites. sented earlier, that there would be a dramatic rise in the
During adolescence, these opposites can take the form of detection of opposing self-attributes and an acknowl-
seemingly contradictory abstractions about the self (e.g., edgment that such apparent contradictions lead to con-
tolerant versus intolerant, extrovert versus introvert, re- flict in the self-system at mid-adolescence. Our most
sponsible versus irresponsible, and good-looking versus recent procedure for examining these issues is described
unattractive as in the cameo). in Harter (1999), as are the findings.
However, the abstract mapping structure has limita- Across three different studies (see Harter et al.,
tions as a means of relating two concepts to one another 1997), we have found that the number of opposing self-
in that the individual cannot yet truly integrate such attribute pairs, as well as the number of opposites in
542 The Self

conflict, increases between early and middle adoles- In extrapolating from these observations, we have
cence. This pattern of findings supports the hypothesis suggested that in an effort to maintain the multiple rela-
that the abstract mapping structures that emerge in mid- tionships that girls are developing during adolescence,
dle adolescence allow one to detect, but not to meaning- and to create harmony among these necessarily differ-
fully integrate, these apparent contradictions. Thus, entiated roles, opposing attributes in the self become
they lead to the phenomenological experience of in- particularly salient as well as problematic. Boys, in con-
trapsychic conflict. We have asked teenagers to verbally trast, can move more facilely among their different roles
elaborate on the opposites and conflicts that they re- and multiple selves to the extent that such roles are logi-
ported on our task. As one 14-year-old put it, I really cally viewed as more independent of one another. How-
think I am a happy person and I want to be that way with ever, these general observations require further
everyone, not just my friends; but I get depressed with refinement, including an empirical examination of pre-
my family, and it really bugs me because thats not what cisely which facets of the relational worlds of adoles-
I want to be like. Another 15-year-old, in describing a cent females and males are specifically relevant to
conflict between self-attributes in the realm of romantic gender differences in opposing attributes displayed
relationships, exclaimed, I hate the fact that I get so across different contexts.
nervous! I wish I wasnt so inhibited. The real me is Closer examination of the gender effects reveals that
talkative. I just want to be natural, but I cant. Another it is a subset of female adolescents who report more op-
15-year-old girl explained, I really think of myself as posites and greater conflict compared to males. We
friendly and open to people, but the way the other girls have determined that adolescent females who endorse a
act, they force me to become an introvert, even though I feminine gender orientation (eschewing masculine
know Im not. In exasperation, one ninth-grader ob- traits) may be particularly vulnerable to the experience
served of the self-portrait she had constructed, Its not of opposing attributes and associated conflict. Feminine
right, it should all fit together into one piece! These adolescent females, compared to females who endorse
comments suggest that at this age level, there is a need an androgynous orientation, report more conflict, par-
for coherence; there is a desire to bring self-attributes ticularly in roles that involve teachers, classmates, and
into harmony with one another, yet in mid-adolescence, male friends (in contrast to roles involving parents and
the cognitive abilities to create such a self-portrait are female friends). Several hypotheses would be worth
not yet in place. pursuing in this regard. Is it that feminine girls report
For across-role opposites, at every age level, females more contradictions in those public contexts where they
detect more contradictory attributes than do males. feel they may be acting inappropriately by violating
These findings replicate two other studies in which sim- feminine stereotypes of behavior? Given that feminin-
ilar gender differences were obtained (see Harter et al., ity as assessed by sex-role inventories is largely defined
1997). Moreover, in one study in which we asked sub- by caring, sensitivity, and attentiveness to the needs
jects to indicate how upset they were over conflicting and feelings of others, might female adolescents who
attributes, the pattern revealed that females become adopt this orientation be more preoccupied with rela-
more upset over conflicting attributes across early, mid- tionships, making opposing attributes and accompany-
dle, and late adolescence, whereas males become less ing conflict more salient? Moreover, might it be more
upset. Elsewhere, we have offered a general interpreta- important for feminine girls to be consistent across re-
tion of this pattern, drawing on those frameworks that lationships, a stance that may be difficult to sustain, to
emphasize the greater importance of relationships for the extent that significant others in different roles are
females than males (Chodorow, 1989; Eichenbaum & encouraging or reinforcing different characteristics?
Orbach, 1983; Gilligan, 1982; Jordan, 1991; J. Miller, These are all new directions in which attention to gen-
1986; Rubin, 1985). These theorists posit that the so- der issues should proceed.
cialization of girls involves far more embeddedness in The challenges posed by the need to create different
the family and more concern with connectedness to oth- selves are also exacerbated for ethnic minority youth in
ers. Boys, in contrast, forge a path of independence and this country who must bridge multiple worlds, as
autonomy in which the logic of moral and social deci- Cooper and her colleagues point out (Cooper, Jackson,
sions takes precedence over affective responses to sig- Azmitia, Lopez, & Dunbar, 1995). Minority youth must
nificant others. move between multiple contexts, some of which may be
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 543

with members of their own ethnic group, including fam- stage make one aware that no human being can have di-
ily and friends, and some of which may be populated by rect access to anothers mind, leading to inevitable am-
the majority culture, including teachers, classmates, biguity about others attitudes toward the self,
and other peers who may not share the values of their producing yet another source of doubt and confusion.
family of origin. Rather than assume that all ethnic mi- The potential for displaying differing levels of self-
nority youth will react similarly to the need to cope esteem across relational contexts is also exacerbated
with such multiple worlds, these investigators high- during this period, to the extent that the significant oth-
lighted several different patterns of adjustment. Some ers are providing different levels of validation for whom
youth are able to move facilely across the borders of one is as a person (see also Rosenberg, 1986). For exam-
their multiple worlds, in large part, because the values ple, the cameo self-description reveals that the adoles-
of the family, teachers, and peers are relatively similar. cent gets down on herself for not getting the grades her
Others, for whom there is less congruence in values parents expect. She hates herself when she feels peers
across contexts, adopt a bicultural stance, adapting to think she is weird, but she feels good about herself on
the world of family and to that of the larger community. the job, where supervisors give her more positive feed-
Others find the transition across these psychological back. Our own evidence has revealed that not only does
borders more difficult, and some find it totally unman- self-esteem become differentiated by context beginning
ageable. Particularly interesting is the role that certain in early adolescence but also further differentiated in
parents play in helping adolescents navigate these tran- middle to late adolescence. For example, individuals
sitions, leading to more successful adaptations for some come to develop different levels of self-esteem with
than others. their mothers and their fathers (Harter, 1999); levels
As observed earlier, adolescents during this period that are directly related to their perceptions of approval
become extremely preoccupied with the opinions and from each parent.
expectations of significant others in different roles. As
our prototypical respondent indicates, I care what my Normative Liabilities during Middle Adolescence
parents think about me; I want to know what my close Mid-adolescence brings a preoccupation with what sig-
friends think ; I dont care what (everybody else) nificant others think of the self, a task that is made more
thinks. I mean I dont want to care, that is; I want challenging given the proliferation of roles that demand
them (adults at work) to approve of me. Adolescents the creation of multiple selves. The addition of new role-
gaze intently into the social mirror for information related selves can be observed in the fact that adoles-
about what standards and attributes to internalize. How- cents make finer discriminations (e.g., self with a close
ever, as the number of roles proliferates, leading to mes- friend versus self with a group of friends, and self with
sages from different significant others that are mother versus self with father). Moreover, there is rela-
potentially contradictory, adolescents may become con- tively little overlap in the personal attributes that define
fused or distressed about just which characteristics to the self in each role. The proliferation of multiple selves
adopt. We see this in the cameo self-description with re- ushers in the potential for such attributes to be viewed
gard to scholastic performance, in that the adolescent as contradictory. Moreover, the emergence of new cogni-
feels she should get better grades to please her parents tive processes, such as abstract mappings, forces the
but confesses that Id be mortified in the eyes of my adolescent to compare and contrast different attributes,
friends if I did too well. As Higgins (1991) observes, in exacerbating the likelihood that contradictions will be
their attempt to incorporate the standards and opinions detected. Mappings, in the form of the identification of
of others, adolescents at this level develop conflicting - opposites, are problematic in that the individual cannot
self-guides across different relational contexts as they yet truly integrate such self-representations in a manner
attempt to meet the incompatible expectations of par- that would resolve the contradictions. Thus, the adoles-
ents and peers. He reports evidence indicating that such cent is likely to experience conflict, confusion, and dis-
discrepancies have been found to produce confusion, un- tress. Opposites and associated conflict are particularly
certainty, and indecision with regard to self-evaluation likely to occur for attributes in different roles rather
and self-regulation, which is consistent with our own than in the same role. Females are particularly likely to
findings. Moreover, as Rosenberg (1986) notes, the seri- display these negative outcomes. Opposing self-
ous efforts at perspective taking that emerge at this attributes also lead to unstable self-representations, in
544 The Self

addition to concern over which characteristics represent As our prototypical respondent exclaims, Everybody, I
ones true self. mean everybody else is looking at me like they think I
With regard to the impact of the socializing environ- am totally weird! With regard to lack of cognitive con-
ment, adolescents gaze intently into the social mirror for trol, this phenomenon represents overgeneralization (or
information about what standards and attributes to in- failure to differentiate) in that adolescents project their
ternalize. However, contradictory messages from differ- own concerns onto others.
ent significant others can lead to confusion about just Interestingly, the inability to control and to effec-
what characteristics to adopt. Differential support, in tively apply new cognitive structures can result not only
the form of approval or validation, will also lead to dif- in a lack of differentiation between self and other, as in
fering levels of self-worth across relational contexts. the imaginary audience phenomenon, but also in exces-
The contradictory feedback that adolescents may re- sive or unrealistic differentiation. The latter penchant
ceive from different sources will, therefore, lead to can be observed in another form of egocentrism that
volatility in self-esteem across interpersonal contexts. Elkind (1967) identified as the personal fable. In cre-
Contradictory standards and feedback can also con- ating narratives that come to define the self, the adoles-
tribute to a lowering of global self-esteem between early cent asserts that his or her thoughts and feelings are
and middle adolescence (see findings reviewed by Har- uniquely experienced. No one else can possibly under-
ter, 1990a, 1990b), to the extent that one cannot meet the stand or experience the ecstasy of his or her rapture or
expectations of everyone in each relational context. To the intensity of his or her despair. Adults, particularly
the extent that the adolescent does not meet the stan- parents, are likely to be singled out in this regard. As the
dards of others, he or she is likely to experience less ap- prototypical adolescent exclaims, My parents dont un-
proval, which will lead to lower global self-esteem. derstand me. What do they know about what its like to
Moreover, the abstract mapping structure, coupled with be a teenager? Her initial comment to the interviewer
the penchant for introspection, may also contribute to when asked to describe what she was like (Youre prob-
lowered self-esteem in that it facilitates the comparison ably not going to understand) also reflects this type of
of ones ideal and real self-concepts. Such a focus can overdifferentiation between self and other.
lead to a heightened awareness of the discrepancy be- The liabilities of this period, therefore, are legion
tween how one perceives the self to be in reality (e.g., I with regard to potential conflict and confusion over con-
can get pretty obnoxious and intolerant ) and how one tradictory attributes and messages, concern over which
would ideally like to be (e.g., Id like to be friendly and characteristics define the true self, distortions in the
tolerant all of the time. Thats the kind of person I want perception of self versus others, and a preoccupation
to be, and Im disappointed in myself when Im not ). with discrepancies between the real and ideal self-
The realm of physical appearance is particularly criti- concepts, all of which can lead to lowered self-worth.
cal. Thus, this adolescent wants to look like Britney Some of these processes would appear to be problematic
Spears, knows she doesnt, and this sets up another for particular subgroups of adolescents, for example, fe-
painful discrepancy between how she would ideally like males who adopt a feminine gender orientation or ethnic
to look and how she does. In reality, she does not value minority youth who are challenged by the need to create
her appearance, falling far short of the cultural stan- selves that bridge multiple worlds, with ones family,
dards for beauty. ethnic peers and in the mainstream majority culture.
Cognitive-developmental advances during mid-ado- An appreciation for the ramifications of these norma-
lescence also represent limitations that can lead to dis- tive processes is critical in interpreting the unpre-
tortions in the interpretation of the opinions of dictable behaviors, shifting self-evaluations, and mood
significant others. As observed earlier, with the advent swings that are observed in many adolescents during
of any new cognitive capacities comes difficulty in con- this age period. Such displays are less likely to be
trolling and applying them effectively. For example, viewed as intentional or pathological, and more likely to
teenagers have difficulty differentiating their own men- meet with empathy and understanding to the extent that
tal preoccupations from what others are thinking, lead- normative cognitive-development changes can be in-
ing to a form of adolescent egocentrism that Elkind voked as in part responsible. For many parents, as well
(1967) has labeled the imaginary audience. Adoles- as other adults working closely with teenagers, these
cents falsely assume that others are as preoccupied with seemingly inexplicable reactions often lead to perplex-
their behavior and appearance as they themselves are. ity, exasperation, and anger, provoking power struggles
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 545

and altercations that strain the adolescent-adult rela- this period may act more impulsively on his thoughts.
tionship. The realization that this is a normative part of Recent work on the adolescent brain supports the view
development that should not persist forever may provide that the frontal cortex is not yet completely developed,
temporary comfort to adults who feel beleaguered and leading to gaps in executive functions that could serve to
ineffectual in dealing with adolescents of this age. In- curb such impulsive, violent intentions and behaviors.
deed, it gives a more charitable rendering to this period While the fragmented self is a normative liability of
of development. this period of middle adolescence, interactions with a
history of severe and chronic physical and sexual abuse
Individual Differences: Adaptive and Maladaptive may lead to pathological outcomes that can continue as
Outcomes in Middle Adolescence PTSD symptoms even though the abuse occurred in
With regard to the focus on meeting cultural standards early childhood. The effects of abuse on the self-system
appearance, females are much more likely to suffer from are legion (see review in Harter, 1999). From a develop-
processes that move into the realm of pathology, includ- mental perspective, a history of abuse can lead to disso-
ing depression and eventual eating disorders. From the ciative symptoms that serve to further fragment the
perspective of our own model of the causes and corre- fragile multiple selves in the process of psychological
lates of self esteem, an intense preoccupation with at- construction (see also Putnam, 1993; Westen, 1993) at a
tempts to meeting the impossible standards of beauty, time when adolescents have normative challenges to in-
coupled with very negative perceptions of ones body tegrating their various selves. As a result, there is no
image, can lead to extremely low self-esteem, depres- core self at the helm, there is little communication be-
sion, and in the extreme, eating-disordered behaviors. tween multiple selves that become alters, comprising
We have documented the links between the high impor- the ability to develop an integrated self. As a result,
tance attached to physical appearance and negative per- there is the risk for dissociative identity disorders that
ceptions of ones body image, leading to extremely represent severe pathological conditions that may re-
negative reports of self-esteem and depression among quire years of treatment.
those in mid-adolescence. In the subsequent section on
later adolescence and emerging adulthood, we provide Late Adolescence: Verbal Cameo of Normative
further documentation about how these processes can Self-Representations and Self-Evaluations
lead to pathological eating-disordered behaviors.
However, numerous findings (reviewed Harter, 1999; Im a pretty conscientious person, particularly when it
see also Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994) reveal that comes to things like doing my homework. Its important
to me because I plan to go to college next year. Eventually
dramatic gender differences in depression emerge in
I want to go to law school, so developing good study
middle adolescence. The discrepancy between impossi-
habits and getting top grades are both essential. (My par-
ble ideals for appearance and ones perception of ones ents dont want me to become a lawyer; theyd rather I go
own body image contributes to very low self-esteem for into teaching, but law is what I want to pursue.) Every
some, particularly those who are overweight, which now and then I get a little lackadaisical and dont com-
leads to profound depression that can require clinical plete an assignment as thoroughly or thoughtfully as I
intervention. could, particularly if our high school has a big football or
While the potential for such internalizing symptoms basketball game that I want to go to with my friends. But
looms large for girls during middle adolescence, the po- thats normal, I mean, you cant just be a total grind.
tential for the escalation of violence and males, as in the Youd be pretty boring if you were. You have to be flexi-
case of the high profile cases of school shootings by ble. Ive also become more religious as I have gotten
White, middle-class adolescents is apparent. Intense re- older, not that I am a saint or anything. Religion gives me
a sense of purpose, in the larger scheme of things, and it
jection by peers, at a time when self-consciousness and
provides me with personal guidelines for the kind of adult
the need for approval are so salient, sets the stage for vi-
Id like to be. For example, Id like to be an ethical per-
olent ideation that can turn to action. The fragile and son who treats other people fairly. Thats the kind of
vacillating self-structures of this particular period can, lawyer Id like to be, too. I dont always live up to that
in the face of humiliation, lead to lack of control, both standard; that is, sometimes I do something that doesnt
over cognitions about the self (Harter, 1999) and behav- feel that ethical. When that happens, I get a little de-
iors that these cognitions may drive. Given the lack of pressed because I dont like myself as a person. But I tell
cognitive control (Fischer, 1980), the adolescent during myself that its natural to make mistakes, so I dont really
546 The Self

question the fact that deep down inside, the real me is a the adolescent expresses the personal desire to go to col-
moral person. Basically, I like who I am, so I dont stay lege, which requires good grades and discipline in the
depressed for long. Usually, I am pretty upbeat and opti- form of study habits. Although classmates tout athletics
mistic. I guess you could say that Im a moody person. as the route to popularity, there is less concern at this age
Im not as popular as a lot of other kids. You have to look with what others think (I used to care but now what I
a certain way, have the right body image, wear the right
think is important ). In addition, there is a more realistic
clothes, to be accepted. At our school, its the jocks who
focus on ones future selves (e.g., not only becoming a
are looked up to. Ive never been very athletic, but you
cant be good at everything, lets face it. Being athletic
lawyer, but also an ethical lawyer, as a personal goal).
isnt that high on my own list of what is important, even Noteworthy in this narrative is the absence of an explicit
though it is for a lot of kids in our school. I try to think reference to the potential origins of these goals; for ex-
that, anyway. But I dont really care what they think any- ample, parental encouragement or expectations that one
more, at least I try to convince myself that I dont. I try to pursue such a career. Moreover, this adolescents career
believe that what I think is what counts. After all, I have choice does not conform to the parents occupational
to live with myself as a person and to respect that person, goals for their child.
which I do now, more than a few years ago. Im pretty The failure to acknowledge the socialization influ-
much being the kind of person I want to be. Im doing ences that might have led to these choices does not nec-
well at things that are important to me like getting good essarily indicate that significant others, such as peers
grades. Thats what is probably most important to me
and parents, had no impact. Findings (see Steinberg,
right now. Having a lot of friends isnt that important to
1990) reveal that the attitudes of adolescents and their
me. I wouldnt say I was unpopular, though. While I am
basically an introvert, especially on a date when I get
parents are quite congruent when it comes to occupa-
pretty self-conscious, in the right social situation, like tional, political, and religious decisions or convictions.
watching a ball game with my friends, I can be pretty ex- That the impact of significant others is not acknowl-
troverted. You have to be adaptive around other people. It edged suggests that older adolescents and young adults
would be weird to be the same kind of person on a date have come to own various values as personal choices,
and with my friends at a football game! For example, rather than attribute them to the sources from which
when our team has a winning season and goes to the play- they may have been derived (Damon & Hart, 1988). In
offs, everyone in the whole school is proud; what the team Higgins (1991) terminology, older adolescents have
does reflects on all of us. On a date, the feelings are much gone through a process in which they have actively se-
more intimate, just between you and the other person. As lected among alternative self-guides and are no longer
much as I enjoy my high school friends and activities, Im
merely buffeted about by the expectations of significant
looking forward to leaving home and going to college,
others; that is, self-guides become increasingly internal-
where I can be more independent, although Im a little
ambivalent. I love my parents, and really want to stay
ized and less tied to their social origins. Moreover, there
connected to them, plus, what they think about me is still is a greater sense of direction as the older adolescent
important to how I feel about myself as a person. So leav- comes to envisage future or possible selves (Markus
ing home will be bittersweet. But sometimes its hard to & Nurius, 1986) that function as ideals toward which
be mature around them, particularly around my mom. I one aspires.
feel a lot more grown-up around my dad; he treats me Another feature of the self-portrait of the older ado-
more like an adult. I like that part of me because it feels lescent can be contrasted with the period before, in that
more like my true self. My mom wants me to grow up, but many potentially contradictory attributes are no longer
another part of her wants me to remain her little baby. described as characteristics in opposition to one an-
Ill probably always be somewhat dependent on my par- other. Thus, being conscientious as a student does not
ents. How can you escape it? But Im also looking for-
appear to conflict with ones lackadaisical attitude to-
ward to being on my own. (adapted from Harter, 1999)
ward schoolwork: Thats normal, I mean, you cant just
With regard to the content of the self-representations be a total grind. Youd be pretty boring if you were.
that emerge in late adolescence and early adulthood, typ- You have to be flexible. Similarly, ones perception of
ically, many of the attributes reflect personal beliefs, val- the self as ethical does not conflict with the acknowl-
ues, and moral standards that have become internalized, edgment that one also has engaged in some unethical be-
or alternatively, constructed from their own experiences haviors (Its natural to make mistakes). Nor does
(see findings by Damon & Hart, 1988). These character- introversion conflict with extroverted behaviors. You
istics are exemplified in the prototypical cameo, in that have to be adaptive around other people. It would be
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 547

weird to be the same kind of person on a date and with dividuals may not attain a given level at the designated
my friends at a football game! age period. Development may be delayed or even ar-
There are cognitive acquisitions that allow the older rested if there is not sufficient support for the transition
adolescent to overcome some of the liabilities of the pre- to a new level of conceptualization, particularly for the
vious period, where potentially opposing attributes were higher stages.
viewed as contradictory and as a cause of internal con-
flict. The general cognitive advances during this period The Role of the Socializing Environment
involve the construction of higher-order abstractions More recent evidence (see Harter et al., 1997) indicates
that involve the meaningful intercoordination of single that the ability to resolve potentially contradictory at-
abstractions (see Case, 1985; Fischer, 1980; Fischer & tributes may be more difficult for some role-pair com-
Canfield, 1986). For example, the fact that one is both binations than for others, particularly for females. For
introverted and extroverted can be integrated through example, when all role pairs are combined, there is no
the construction of a higher-order abstraction that de- decline in the number of opposing attributes identified
fines the self as adaptive. The observation that one is across roles. For older adolescent females, there is ac-
both depressed and cheerful or optimistic can be inte- tually a further increase. The fact that six roles were
grated under the personal rubric of moody. Similarly, included in the study generating the new data (com-
flexible can allow one to coordinate conscientious- pared to only four in the original Harter and Monsour
ness with the tendency to be lackadaisical. The higher- study) may have been partly responsible, because the
order concept of ambivalence integrates the desire to inclusion of additional roles increased the probability
be independent yet still remain connected to parents. that opposing attributes might be detected; there were
Moreover, bittersweet reflects a higher-order abstrac- 15 possible role pairs that might contain contradictions
tion combining excitement over going to college with compared to only six role pairs in the original study. In
sadness over leaving ones parents. Such higher-order increasing the number of roles, we also separated the
abstractions provide self-labels that bring meaning and reports of self-attributes with mother and with father
therefore legitimacy to what formerly appeared to be (whereas in the earlier study, we merely inquired about
troublesome contradictions in the self. self with parents).
Neo-Piagetians, such as Case (1985), Fischer (1980), The separation of self-attributes with each parent po-
and colleagues, observe that developmental acquisitions tentially enhances the likelihood that characteristics
at these higher levels typically require greater scaffold- with each may contradict attributes in roles with peers.
ing by the social environment in the form of support, ex- Examples generated by adolescent respondents in mid-
periences, instruction, and so on for individuals to dle to late adolescence included being short-tempered
function at their optimal level. If these new skills are with mother versus a good listener in romantic relation-
fostered, they will help the adolescent to integrate op- ships; respectful with father versus assertive with
posing attributes in a manner that does not produce con- friends; distant from father but attentive with a romantic
flict or distress. Thus, efforts to assist the adolescent in interest. Adolescent bids for autonomy from parents
realizing that it is normal to display seemingly contra- (Cooper, Grotevant, & Condon, 1983; Hill & Holm-
dictory traits, and perhaps quite appropriate, may alle- beck, 1986; Steinberg, 1990), coupled with the increas-
viate perceptions of conflict. Moreover, helping ing importance of the peer group (Brown, 1990;
teenagers to provide higher-order labels that integrate Savin-Williams & Berndt, 1990), would lead to the ex-
opposing attributes (e.g., flexible, adaptive, moody, and pectation that attributes expressed with mother and fa-
inconsistent) may avert some of the distress that was ther might well differ from those displayed with peers
salient during middle adolescence. These suggestions (i.e., friends and romantic partners), leading to a greater
derive from the observations of Fischer, Case, and oth- potential for contradictions.
ers to the effect that these cognitive solutions will not However, the separation of self with mother and self
necessarily emerge automatically with development. with father also creates the potential for attributes with
Nor will the potential benefits derived from movement mother to be in opposition to attributes with father. The
to late adolescence and early adulthood necessarily ac- potential for attributes with each parent to appear con-
crue; that is, the levels described in this chapter repre- tradictory can be observed in the cameo, where the pro-
sent a normative sequence of development. However, totypical older adolescent feels much more mature with
the age levels are somewhat arbitrary in that certain in- father than with mother. Moreover, such conflicts begin
548 The Self

to be observed in mid-adolescence, where the prototypi- are not as likely to be called on simultaneously. These
cal teenager indicated that she was close with her particular conditions may exacerbate the contradictions
mother but distant with her father, a difference that and conflicts that adolescents experience in their re-
became problematic if they were all together such as spective roles with mother versus father. These family
talking at dinner. dynamics appear to be relevant to the increase in across-
When the findings are broken down by relationship role opposing attributes for female adolescents in partic-
pairs, opposing attributes and associated conflict were ular, who may be more likely to be sensitive to the fact
most frequent for the combination of self with mother that they are behaving differently with mother versus fa-
versus self with father, beginning in mid-adolescence, ther. As observed in the previous section on the period
and increasing in late adolescence, a pattern that we of mid-adolescence, females display more concern over
have since replicated in a subsequent study. Examples relationship issues, which may make opposing attributes
have included being close with mother versus distant more salient. Adolescent females may also feel that, to
with father; stubborn with mother versus respectful remain connected to both mother and father, it is impor-
with father; open with mother but not with father; at tant to be consistent across these relationshipsa task
ease with mother but defensive with father; and hostile that can be problematic for the reasons cited earlier.
with mother but cheerful with father. Moreover, such Although the gender literature suggests that connect-
opposites between self with mother versus self with fa- edness is more critical to females than to males
ther, as well as associated conflict, increased dramati- (Chodorow, 1989; Eichenbaum & Orbach, 1983; Gilli-
cally for the older girls in particular. gan, 1982; Gilligan, Lyons, & Hanmer, 1989; Jordan,
The fact that older female adolescents reported in- 1991; J. Miller, 1986; Rubin, 1985), the adolescent liter-
creasing contradiction, whereas male adolescents did ature reveals that it is important for teenagers of both
not, suggests that cognitive-developmental explanations genders to remain connected to parents in the process of
are incomplete. The separation of attributes with individuation and the establishment of autonomy
mother and with father, in particular, would appear to (Cooper et al., 1983; Hill & Holmbeck, 1986; Stein-
make it more difficult for certain adolescents to cogni- berg, 1990). As our prototypical subject reveals, while it
tively resolve or normalize the contradictions (produced is important to go to college where he or she can be
by the opposing attributes with mother versus father) more independent, it is also important to stay connected
that are provoked by these roles, as well as those attri- to parents.
butes with each parent that contradict the characteris- Contextual factors, such as the family, therefore will
tics that one displays with peers. Contradictions conspire with cognitive development to impact the ex-
between self with parents and self with peers are more tent to which opposites and conflicts are experienced. In
understandable, given developmental bids for autonomy. another example, the role of context can be observed in
However, why should adolescents (particularly females) cross-cultural research by Kennedy (1994). Kennedy
report increasingly different characteristics with mother has adapted our procedure in comparing the self-
and father? understanding of American and Korean youth. He finds
Here, we can only speculate. Family therapists ob- that there are different age-related peaks in conflict
serve that children and adolescents typically develop among adolescents in the two cultures. Korean youth re-
different relationships with each parent, which may port increased conflict between opposing attributes in
cause the salient attributes in each relationship to vary 10th and 12th grades, findings that he interprets as the
considerably. Contributing to these dynamics is the fact demands of the school context at those particular grade
that each parent may have a different set of expectations levels. In Korea, 10th grade is the 1st year of high
about those characteristics that he or she values and school, and the new students are required to be deferen-
therefore attempts to foster. First, the adolescent may tial to the juniors and seniors, a relationship that many
become caught in a struggle between two parents who 10th graders find oppressive. Kennedy argues that these
are encouraging and reinforcing different facets of his demands exert a strain on the self-system and destabi-
or her personality, provoking opposing attributes and re- lize students sense of self as they struggle to find a
sulting conflict. Second, both of these roles, self with niche in the peer hierarchy of high school. During 12th
mother and self with father, occur in the same general grade, there are different demands, for example, intense
context (i.e., the family), whereas other multiple roles preparation for the college entrance exams. This pres-
Developmental Dif ferences in Self-Representations during Childhood 549

sure leads to challenges in balancing the demands of ac- These others include parents. Although it has been
ademics, peer relationships, and family commitments, common in treatments of adolescent development to sug-
resulting in greater conflict. gest that the influence of peers increases, whereas the
Future research should attend to such contextual fac- impact of parental opinion declines, findings do not sup-
tors and attempt to assess the underlying processes more port the latter contention. As our cameo subject indi-
directly. To return to our own findings in this regard, it cates, What my parents think about me is still
would be of interest to determine whether the conflict important to how I feel about myself as a person. Our
between self-attributes with mother versus father is own findings reveal that the correlation between class-
more intense if the adolescent is living in a two-parent mate approval and global self-esteem does increase dur-
family where both mother and father are in the same ing childhood and adolescence; however, the correlation
household, or if the parents are divorced and living between parental approval and global self-esteem,
apart. One hypothesis is that living under the same roof which is high in childhood, does not decline during ado-
with both parents makes it difficult to avoid conflict if lescence (Harter, 1990b). The latter correlation does de-
different attributes in each relationship are demanded cline, however, during the college years among students
simultaneously. Alternatively, conflict may be exacer- who are away from home.
bated in the situation of divorce to the extent that in an More specific evaluations of self-esteem continue to
acrimonious separation, each parent intensifies his or vary by relationship context (Harter, Waters, & White-
her differential expectations for the attributes they want sell, 1998), throughout the high school years, as adoles-
the adolescent to display as part of a power struggle in cents make finer distinctions (e.g., between their
which the adolescent becomes a pawn. Such processes self-esteem with mother and with father). However, we
would be intriguing to investigate. did not anticipate the fact that for the vast majority of
Finally, with regard to developmental changes in the individuals, self-esteem in one particular relational do-
self, evidence from longitudinal studies documents that main is much more highly related to global self-esteem
self-esteem or global self-worth improves in later ado- than is relational self-esteem in all other contexts. The
lescence (see Engel, 1959; OMalley & Bachman, 1983; specific domain occupying this position varies from
Rosenberg, 1986; Simmons, Rosenberg, & Rosenberg, adolescent to adolescent. For example, with our proto-
1973). Several interpretations of these gains have been typical adolescent, self as student in the academic do-
suggested (see Harter, 1990b; McCarthy & Hoge, 1982). main is most important (Getting good grades is what is
Reductions in the discrepancy between ones ideal self most important to me now) and his or her self-esteem
and ones real self, between ones aspirations and ones in that particular context is higher than in other do-
successes, according to Jamess (1892) formulation, mains. Thus, focusing on that particular context would
may be in part responsible. As the prototypical adoles- appear to be very adaptive in that it should promote
cent indicates, he or she has more self-respect now, com- more positive feelings of global self-esteem.
pared to a few years ago and observes that Im pretty
much being the kind of person I want to be. Im doing Normative Liabilities during Late Adolescence
well at things that are important to me like getting good Many of the limitations of the preceding period of mid-
grades and being ethical. Gains in personal autonomy adolescence would appear to be overcome as a result of
and freedom of choice may also play a role, in that the changes during late adolescence. Attributes reflecting
older adolescent may have more opportunity to select personal beliefs, values, and standards become more in-
performance domains in which he or she is successful. ternalized, and the older adolescent would appear to
Such freedom may also provide one with more opportu- have more opportunity to meet these standards, thereby
nity to select those support groups that will provide the leading to enhanced self-esteem. The focus on future
positive regard necessary to promote or enhance self- selves also gives the older adolescent a sense of direc-
esteem, consistent with the looking glass self- tion. A critical cognitive advance can be observed in the
formulation. Increased role-taking ability may also lead ability to construct higher-order abstractions that in-
older teenagers to behave in more socially acceptable volve the meaningful integration of single abstractions
ways that enhance the evaluation of the self by others, that represent potential contradictions in the self-
such that the favorable attitudes of others toward the self portrait (e.g., depressed and cheerful do not conflict be-
are internalized as positive self-worth. cause they are both part of being moody). The older
550 The Self

adolescent can also resolve potentially contradictory at- For example, in one of our studies conducted by
tributes by asserting that he or she is flexible or adap- Danis (see Harter, 1999), two eating-disordered groups
tive, thereby subsuming apparent inconsistencies under among women college students were identified, those
more generalized abstractions about the self. Moreover, with symptoms of anorexia and those with symptoms of
older adolescents are more likely to normalize potential bulimia. These two groups were compared to a control
contradictions, asserting that it is desirable to be differ- sample, college women who did not display such symp-
ent across relational contexts and that it would be weird toms. Both the anorexic and bulimic group reported sig-
or strange to be the same with different people. nificantly higher scores on the importance of
Nevertheless, conflict between role-related attributes appearance, toward the very top of the four-point scale,
does not totally abate in later adolescence. Conflict will compared to the normative sample. They each reported
be more likely to occur if the new skills that allow for an extremely low scores with regard to their evaluation of
integration of seeming contradictions are not fostered by their physical appearance, creating a large discrepancy:
the socializing environment. Furthermore, opposing at- importance scores vastly higher than perceived appear-
tributes across particular role combinations, notably self ance scores. This discrepancy, clearly predicted very
with mother versus self with father, continue to be prob- low self-esteem scores for the two eating-disordered
lematic in late adolescence, especially for girls. To the groups compared to the normal sample. These low self-
extent that ones mother and father elicit or reinforce esteem scores, in turn, were highly predictive of ex-
opposing attributes, cognitive solutions for integrating tremely high levels of self-reported depression. Of
seeming contradictions would appear to be more diffi- particular interest were the findings that although both
cult to invoke. eating-disordered groups reported this negative constel-
Last, although the internalization of standards and lation of symptoms, those in the bulimic group were
opinions that the adolescent comes to own as personal most at risk given the lowest ratings of perceived physi-
choices and attitudes toward the self represents a devel- cal appearance, self-esteem, and depression. Danis
opmental advance, there are liabilities as well associ- interpreted this difference between the two eating-dis-
ated with this process. As Rosenberg (1986) observes, ordered groups in terms of perceptions of control. She
the shift in the locus of self-knowledge from an exter- argued that those with symptoms of anorexia were ob-
nal to internal source can introduce uncertainty. As jectively thinner, leading them to possibly feel more
long as major truths about the self derive from omnis- successful in terms of their weight control. The bulim-
cient and omnipotent adults, then there is little doubt ics, who by definition binge and purge, were interpreted
about their veracity. However, when the locus of self- to be less in control; moreover, on average, this group
knowledge shifts inward and adolescents must rely on was heavier. That said, those with anorexia are more at
their own autonomous judgment and insight to reach risk for malnourishment that can affect bone develop-
conclusions about the self, the sense of certainty can be ment, brain development, body development in general,
compromised. and, in the extreme, they are at risk for death. Those
with bulimic symptoms are also at risk for a variety of
Individual Differences: Adaptive and Maladaptive
physical symptoms, including damage to the mouth and
Self-Processes and Outcomes in Late
esophagus as well as other compromising physical
Adolescence/Early Adulthood
growth symptoms. There are also numerous psychologi-
Many of the pathological processes that have been de- cal symptoms including disruptions in the family, com-
scribed in the earlier periods of adolescence can be ob- promised academic achievement, and impaired social
served, even if in a somewhat different form, due to functioning.
developmental advances. Preoccupation with impossible Male adolescents are at continued risk for violence,
cultural standards of attractiveness looms even larger as particularly the type of violence that emanates from
the older adolescent anticipates emerging adulthood, peer rejection and humiliation. Chronic rejection and
making it even more critical to attain these standards to humiliation are likely culprits for violent ideation
be socially acceptable and successful in the new adult (Harter, 2004) and for violent action, as in the case of
world order (Harter, in press). For females, failure to the school shooters. Unlike the impulsive acts of the
meet these standards can lead to more pathological school shooters in middle adolescents, the acts of those
processes that may include eating disorders. (e.g., Eric Harris and Dylan Kleibold from Columbine)
Stability versus Change in Self-Representations 551

who were older teens were far more planful. For over a defined, normatively, as beneffectance by Greenwald
year, they had developed their strategies, some of (1980); namely, seeing ones positive attributes as cen-
which were revealed in Harriss written manifesto. tral to the self and ones negative attributes as more
While this is speculative, at this point, in examining the peripheral.
media accounts of the 11 high-profile school shooting Our colleagues Fischer and Ayoub (1994) employed
cases, it would appear that the dynamics may be differ- our multiple selves procedure with an inpatient sample
ent from what we normally consider to be delinquent, of seriously abused older adolescent girls, finding just
conduct-disordered behavior that had come to the atten- the opposite pattern. Compared to a normative sample,
tion of teachers, school officials, school psychologists, the abused patients identified negative attributes as their
peers, and parents. In most of these cases, there had core self, relegating what few positive characteristics
been few warning signs with regard to the male shoot- they could identify as peripheral. Herein, we can detect
ers having been in trouble with the law, having been another deleterious effect of abuse on self-processes
identified as troublemakers in the school, having clini- leading to potential pathological outcomes that require
cal diagnoses, or being placed in special classes for stu- clinical intervention that can hopeful restore a more
dent with a penchant for acting out. As noted earlier positive balance of self-perceptions.
(Harter & McCarley, 2004), we found that 33% of
those in a normative sample reporting to us that they
STABILITY VERSUS CHANGE IN
had serious thoughts of harming others who humiliated
SELF-REPRESENTATIONS
them went undetected by their classroom teachers who
were given parallel rating forms. Thus, there is a need
Initially, it is important to address the question of whether
to discriminate the form of violence that has recently
concepts of self, either at a domain-specific or more
emerged from previous acts that have been committed
global level, are immutable or subject to change. If self-
by known delinquents and conduct-disordered youth
representations are relatively stable, then practitioners
who have come to the attention of school and mental
should be less sanguine about the possibility of promoting
health professional, and who commit different types of
positive self-evaluations in individuals with negative self-
crimes; for example, drive-by shootings to target one
images. Alternatively, if self-representations are poten-
individual versus the random shooting of as many
tially malleable, practitioners can be more optimistic,
classmates as possible. The dynamics may be different
particularly if there is a cogent analysis of the particular
at different developmental stages (e.g., more planful
causes of a given individuals negative self-evaluations.
among older adolescents).
The initial focus in this section is on literature rele-
The construction of multiple selves, while a norma-
vant to the actual stability and/or change in the valence
tive process, can also have pathological implications. It
of self-representationshow favorably the self is evalu-
was pointed out in the section on middle adolescence
ated. A central theme in the literature has been do self-
that the effects of abuse can lead to dissociative symp-
evaluations, notably self-esteem and domain specific
toms that prevent ones multiple selves from being inte-
self-concepts, change normatively with development?
grated. In the severest cases, this can lead to dissociative
Equally of interest is whether there are individual dif-
identity disorder (what used to be termed multiple per-
ferences in the extent to which self-evaluations change.
sonality disorder). Abuse has also been found to impact
Herein is the big debate. Is self-esteem more traitlike or
the valence (positive or negative) of those attributes
statelike? We return to this controversy after reviewing
judged to be ones core self (versus more peripheral at-
the normative patterns of self-esteem development (a
tributes). Normatively, we have found that when asked
description of a third issue, do self-evaluations change
to rate the attributes across multiple relational context
as function of the situation or short-term intervals, can
with regard to whether they are central core characteris-
be found in Harter, 1999).
tics or more peripheral, less important attributes that
define the self, normative samples of older attributes Normative-Developmental Change
will define their most important attributes as positive
and assign their more negative characteristics ( less im- With regard to normative-developmental change, the ev-
portant attributes) to the periphery of the self (Harter & idence reveals that self-evaluative judgments become
Monsour, 1992). This self-protective strategy has been less positive as children move into middle childhood
552 The Self

(Frey & Ruble, 1985, 1990; Harter, 1982; Harter & They are the most dissatisfied with their bodies, in part,
Pike, 1984; Stipek, 1981). Investigators attribute such a because they tend to be somewhat heavier and do not fit
decline to the greater reliance on social comparison in- the cultural stereotype of female attractiveness empha-
formation and external feedback, leading to more realis- sizing thinness, as is discussed further in the section on
tic judgments about ones capabilities (see also Crain, the link between self-esteem and perceived appearance.
1996; Marsh, 1989). A growing number of studies sug- This, in turn, has a negative effect on their self-worth.
gest that there is another decline at early adolescence Furthermore, according to the developmental readiness
(ages 11 to 13), after which global evaluations of worth hypothesis (Simmons & Blyth, 1987), early maturing
and domain-specific self-evaluations gradually become girls are not yet emotionally prepared to deal with the
more positive over the course of adolescence (Dusek social expectations that surround dating or with the
& Flaherty, 1981; Marsh, Parker, & Barnes, 1985; greater independence that early maturity often demands
Marsh, Smith, Marsh, & Owens, 1988; OMalley & (see Lipka, Hurford, & Litten, 1992, for a general dis-
Bachman, 1983; Piers & Harris, 1964; Rosenberg, 1986; cussion of the effects of being off-time in ones level
Savin-Williams & Demo, 1993; Simmons, Rosenberg, & of maturational development).
Rosenberg, 1973). Several interpretations have been offered for the
Many of the changes reported coincide with the edu- gradual gains in self-esteem that follow from eighth
cational transition to junior high school. Eccles and col- grade through high school (McCarthy & Hoge, 1982).
leagues (Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Eccles et al., 1984; Gains in personal autonomy may provide more opportu-
Wigfield, Eccles, Mac Iver, Reuman, & Midgley, 1991), nity to select performance domains in which one is
and Simmons and colleagues (Blyth, Simmons, & competent, is consistent with a Jamesian analysis. In-
Carlton-Ford, 1983; Simmons & Blyth, 1987; Simmons, creasing freedom may allow more opportunities to se-
Blyth, Van Cleave, & Bush, 1979; Simmons & Rosen- lect support groups that will provide esteem-enhancing
berg, 1975) have postulated that differences in the approval, is consistent with the looking glass self-
school environments of elementary and junior high formulation. Increased role-taking ability may also lead
schools are in part responsible. Junior high school brings teenagers to behave in more socially acceptable ways
more emphasis on social comparison and competition, that garner the acceptance of others. A study by Hart,
stricter grading standards, more teacher control, less Fegley, and Brengelman (1993) provides some confirm-
personal attention from teachers, and disruptions in so- ing evidence. In describing their past and present selves,
cial networks, all of which lead to a mismatch between adolescents asserted that with time, they have become
the structure of the school environment and the needs of more capable, mature, personable, and attractive, de-
young adolescents. The numerous physical, cognitive, scribing how they shed undesirable cognitive, emo-
social, and emotional changes further jeopardize the tional, and personality characteristics.
adolescents sense of continuity, which may, in turn, An analysis of changes in mean level of self-worth,
threaten self-esteem (Leahy & Shirk, 1985). A number however, may mask individual differences in response to
of these studies (e.g., Blyth et al., 1983; Nottelmann, educational transitions (see also Block & Robins, 1993).
1987; Simmons & Blyth, 1987; Simmons et al., 1979; Findings from our own laboratory (see Harter, 1999) on
Wigfield et al., 1991) also report lower self-esteem for both the transition to junior high school and to college
girls than for boys (see also Block & Robins, 1993, who have identified three groupsthose whose self-worth
find that the gender gap widens from ages 14 to 23). increases, decreases, or remains the same. In our own
The magnitude of the decline in perceptions of over- framework, we contend that self-worth leading to an ex-
all worth is also related to the timing of school shifts amination of instability or stability as a function of
and to pubertal change (Brooks-Gunn, 1988; Brooks- those determinants identified by James (competence in
Gunn & Peterson, 1983; Simmons & Blyth, 1987). domains of importance) and Cooley (approval from sig-
Those making the shift from sixth to seventh grade show nificant others). Results indicate that those whose self-
greater losses of self-esteem than those who make the worth increased across educational transitions displayed
school transition a year later, from seventh to eighth greater competence in domains of importance and re-
grade. Moreover, students making the earlier change, ported more social approval in the new school environ-
particularly girls, do not recover these losses during the ment. Students whose self-worth decreased reported
high school years. Early-maturing girls fare the worst. both a decline in competence for valued domains and re-
Stability versus Change in Self-Representations 553

ported less social support after the transition. Students atively consistent over time; however, there are also sit-
showing no changes in self-worth reported negligible uational variations around this core self-portrait. Others
changes in both competence and social support. Demo & have come to a similar conclusion, postulating that indi-
Savin-Williams (1992) have also adopted a more idio- viduals display both trait and state self-esteem
graphic approach, demonstrating that while nearly half (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991; Leary & Downs, 1995).
of their sample demonstrated stability in their percep- According to some, an individuals baseline sense of self
tions of overall worth, the remaining subjects mani- is difficult to alter. Theorists have argued, in the context
fested varying degrees of instability. of hierarchical models of the self, that higher-order
In this literature, there remains controversy, how- schemas, such as global self-worth or esteem, are far
ever, among those who claim that self-representations more resistant to modification than lower-order, situa-
are relatively enduring and those who contend that tion-specific constructs (Epstein, 1991; Hattie, 1992).
self-representations are more malleable. One camp of Epstein notes that such higher-order schemas have typi-
investigators reports evidence that the self-concepts cally been acquired early in development and are often
of adults are relatively stable. For example, Swann derived from emotionally significant experiences to
(1996) provides evidence demonstrating individ- which the individual may have little conscious access,
uals elaborate and ingenious strategies for self- making the beliefs difficult to alter.
verification; people go to great lengths to seek infor- With regard to the barometric self, adolescence is a
mation that confirms their self-concept and are highly time when fluctuations appear to be the more flagrant
resistant to information that threatens their view of (Blos, 1962; Demo & Savin-Williams, 1992; Harter,
self (see also Baumeister, 1993; Epstein, 1991; Green- 1990a; Leahy & Shirk, 1985; Rosenberg, 1986). Those of
wald, 1980; Rosenberg, 1979). According to Swann, a cognitive-developmental persuasion (e.g., Fischer,
people do not want feedback that may contradict their 1980; Harter, 1990a; Harter & Monsour, 1992; Higgins,
existing identities. Because such identities provide a 1991) have attributed these fluctuations to limitation in
psychological blueprint for action, they are the guide- the ability cognitively to control seemingly contradictory
posts for how we are to behave. Epstein similarly self-attributes (shy versus outgoing), particularly during
observes that people have a vested interest in main- middle adolescence. Psychoanalytic thinkers (e.g., Blos,
taining the stability of their personal theories of real- 1962; Kohut, 1977) attribute fluctuations to the intense
ity, for they are the only systems they have for making heightened narcissism and self-preoccupation of adoles-
sense of their world and guiding their behavior cents whose self-esteem swings from grandiosity to bat-
(p. 97). In Swanns (1996) most recent treatment of tered self-devaluation. Rosenberg (1986) focuses more
this topic, he observes that those with negative self- on how socialization factors influence the volatility of
evaluations are actually ambivalent, in that praise puts the self during adolescence. Thus, he observes that the
them in conflict. Although favorable evaluations adolescent is preoccupied with what others think of the
would be welcome, they also require unfavorable eval- self but has difficulty divining others impressions, lead-
uative feedback, to the extent that such individuals de- ing to ambiguity about the self. Moreover, different sig-
sire verification. He notes that such people are caught nificant others may have different impressions of the
in a crossfire in which the warmth produced by favor- self, creating contradictory feedback. We have recently
able evaluations is chilled by incredulity (p. 14). taken a different perspective, demonstrating that for
Precisely how this relates to children and adolescents some adolescents self-esteem remains quite constant
has yet to be determined, given our developmental across time and context, whereas for others there is
analysis of normative fluctuations in self-esteem. Just considerable variation. We have attempted to investigate
how the adult literature relates to phenomena among the causes, as is discussed in the next section (see also,
younger individuals remains an intriguing issue, all the Harter, 2004).
more so as the field integrates genetic and brain devel-
opmental factors. There is a growing consensus that, as
Individual Differences
James originally suggested, individuals possess both a
baseline self-concept and a barometric self-concept (see Considerable attention has been given to the issue of
reviews by Demo & Savin-Williams, 1992; Rosenberg, whether self-esteem is best viewed as a state or trait
1986). Thus, people have a core sense of self that is rel- (see Trzesniewski, Donnellan, & Robins, 2003, for a
554 The Self

recent review of this issue). A focus on individual dif- In a second study (Harter, Stocker, & Robinson,
ferences in the extent to which self-evaluations are sta- 1996) we found that certain adolescents reported virtu-
ble or malleable is the primary context in which issues ally no short-term fluctuations in their self esteem over
of stability and change in self-evaluations have been a period of months, whereas others reported great insta-
examined. Studying adult participants, Kernis and col- bility, patterns that could be accounted for by their ori-
leagues have examined this issue in its complexity entation to the approval of others. In a third study
(Greenier, Kernis, & Waschull, 1995; Kernis, 1993; (Harter, Waters, & Whitesell, 1998), we found that self-
Kernis, Cornell, Sun, Berry, & Harlow, 1993). Accord- worth varied across relational context (a concept we
ing to these investigators, there are those whose self- have labeled relational self-worth), with the majority
evaluations are more stable and there are those prone to of adolescents reporting different levels of self-worth or
short-term fluctuations in self-esteem. The latter esteem depending on whom they were withfather,
group demonstrate enhanced sensitivity to evaluative mother, female peers, or male peers. To summarize, we
events, ego involvement (versus task involvement), pre- found that over a relatively long period of time (in the
occupation with self-evaluation, and over reliance on first study) and over short periods of time (in the second
social sources of self-esteem (see also Deci & Ryan, study) and across relationship contexts some individuals
1987; Rosenberg, 1986). Our own findings indicating behaved in a traitlike fashion, whereas others behaved in
greater fluctuations in self-worth for those who con- a statelike fashion. Thus, trait and state attributions lie
sciously endorse a looking glass self-orientation (ap- in individuals, not in the constructs themselves.
proval determines self-worth) are consistent with this Does the conceptualization of self-esteem as neither
individual-difference approach (Harter, Stocker, & a trait nor a state cast doubt on the field of personality,
Robinson, 1996). Moreover, we speculated that the de- which depends on the identification of relatively stable
velopmental precursors may have involved parenting traits? Not necessarily. Many thoughtful investigators
characterized by inconsistent and/or conditional ap- are asking just such questions. In a recent study by
proval. Greenier et al. (1995) also conjecture that in- Hair and Graziano (2003), these investigators examined
consistent as well as controlling feedback will the stability of self-esteem and the Big Five Personal-
undermine the development of a stable sense of worth ity attributes in a longitudinal study of those moving
(see also Deci & Ryan, 1987, 1995). from middle school to high school. Findings revealed
Our own position (see Harter, 2004), more consis- much greater stability over the transition for the per-
tent with Kerniss, asks whether self-esteem is stable sonality attributes (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Con-
over time for individuals (is it a trait?) or is it subject to scientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness to
fluctuations (more statelike) and therefore the ques- Experience) than for self-esteem, suggesting that the
tion is false and misguided. We have taken the stance Big Five, as conceptualized, are much more traitlike in
( based on several strands of research) that the con- nature than is self-esteem, which is amenable to change
struct of self-esteem (or self-worth), in and of itself, is if its documented causes change, or stability if the
neither a trait nor a state per se (see also DuBois et al., causes do not change.
2002). Rather for some individuals self-esteem is
stable, whereas for others self-esteem is subject to
change. Among adolescents, we have found evidence GENDER DIFFERENCES IN GLOBAL AND
for this position with regard to self-esteem during the DOMAIN-SPECIFIC SELF-EVALUATIONS
transition to junior high school. As mentioned earlier,
some students enhance their self-esteem, others de- There is now an emerging body of literature that has ex-
cline in self-esteem, and for a third group self-esteem amined gender differences in subscale scores among
remains stable. We linked changes versus stability to older children, adolescents, and college students. For the
change or stability in the competence/importance rela- most part, the findings are quite consistent with regard
tionship (from James) and to stability or change in so- to a number of gender differences as well as similarities.
cial support (from Cooley). Others (see Fenzel, 2002) A major and consistent finding is that females, at every
have related change versus stability in self-worth age beginning in middle to late childhood report lower
across transitions to the ability to negotiate the chal- global self-worth than do males. We find this across the
lenges of middle school. life span; however, differences are greatest in middle to
Gender Dif ferences in Global and Domain-Specific Self-Evaluations 555

late adolescence. An impressive meta-analysis on gen- comes with members of their own disadvantaged
der differences in self-esteem by Kling, Hyde, Showers, group rather than the advantaged group, and (c) de-
and Buswell (1999) confirms this finding in that the valuing those domains in which their group does not
largest mean effect size favoring boys in the 15- to 18- do well, and, in contrast, valuing those domains in
year-old group. In their meta-analysis these differences which their group does do well.
are significant, but small and robust across many sam- Kling et al. (1999) put forth a powerful explanation
ples. Interestingly, these differences have not changed in for gender differences in self-esteem, consistent with
the period between the 1970s and the 1990s despite our own interpretation on the inextricable link between
seeming societal gains for females. perceived physical appearance and global self-worth,
Kling et al. (1999) speculate on several reasons for where correlations range from .66 to .82 across numer-
these gender differences. One potential cause involves ous studies. We have argued elsewhere that (Harter,
the gender role stereotypes that are reinforced in the 1999), consistent with Klings observations, combina-
school setting. Boys are socialized to use dominance, tion of the importance of appearance for females com-
whereas girls are oriented toward shared social activi- bined with the punishing standards of appearance for
ties (see Maccoby, 1990). Girls influence attempts are females profoundly contributes to their devaluation of
more likely to take the form of a polite suggestion, their looks. Movies, magazines, and TV all tout the im-
whereas boys are more likely to try to influence others portance of good looks that are impossible to achieve, in
by direct demands (Leaper, 1991). These differing part, because many of these looks are due to air-brush-
strategies designed to the other present imbalances in ing, computer simulation, and the combining of differ-
mixed-gender interactions in which boys come to ignore ent (the best) body parts from models or movie starts to
the girls attempt to interact with or influence boys achieve the look. Very few ads tout the importance of
(Maccoby, 1990). Kling et al. conclude that girls gen- a physically fit female as desirable but rather showcase
eral ability to influence and to gain valuable resources, thinness combined with height and large breasts as the
particularly in unsupervised mixed-gender groups, may glamour, the ideal of the decade.
make them feel less important and less powerful than Elsewhere, we have depicted gender data across 13 of
boys, which could adversely impact their self-esteem. our own samples from the domains of older children and
Maccoby reports that these differences can be observed adolescence where differences are quite marked with
as early as the preschool years. girls scoring only at the midpoint of the scale and with
Kling et al. (1999) also suggest that different oppor- boys reporting much higher on both appearance and ath-
tunities for athletic participation could also contribute letics (Harter, 1999).
to gender differences in self-esteem. Although Title IX Moreover, this pattern has been found to be highly ro-
certainly opened more doors for female children and bust across different countries where gender differences
adolescents to participate in sports, more emphasis and for physical appearance and athletic competence are
status has been given to male athletes and male sports similar in magnitude and highly significant. Thus, in ad-
programs. Furthermore, despite greater opportunities dition to other findings in the United States (see also
for girls, many do not take advantage of the options, Hagborg, 1994), the more favorable perceptions of phys-
fearing it will undermine their femininity. However, ical appearance and athletic competence by male chil-
studies do show that among males and females who do dren and adolescents has been found in other
participate in sports, self-esteem is higher than for English-speaking countries such as England (Fox, Page,
those who do not engage in athletics. Our own research Armstrong, & Kirby, 1994), Australia (Trent, Russell,
still reveals a significant mean difference in perceived & Cooney, 1994), and Ireland (Grandlese & Joseph,
athletic ability, favoring males. 1993). The very same pattern has been documented
J. Crocker and Majors (1989) theoretical model re- across a range of non-English-speaking countries, in-
garding social stigma and self-esteem provides an- cluding the French-speaking areas of Switzerland
other explanation for the modest gender differences in (Bolognini, Plancheral, Bettschart, & Halfon, 1996;
self-esteem. Stigmatized grounds, in this case fe- Pierrehumbert, Plancheral, & Jankech-Caretta, 1987),
males, protect their self-concept by (a) attributing Italy (Pedrabissi, Santinello, & Scarpazza, 1988), Hol-
negative feedback they receive to prejudice against land (van Dongen-Melman, Koot, & Verhulst, 1993),
members of their group, ( b) comparing their own out- China (Meredith, Abbott, & Zheng, 1991; Stigler,
556 The Self

Smith, & Mao, 1985), Japan (Maeda, 1997), and Korea cally prepared for the sexual advances or taunts that
(Rhee, 1993). they may received. As Williams and Currie (2000) doc-
The very same gender pattern favoring males on both ument, two predictors of self-esteem combine during
perceived athletic ability and attractiveness persists at this period to predict self-esteem: (1) body size (Im
the college level, and the two domains would appear to too fat, whereas many late maturers feel they are too
interact. Our own data reveal that female students feel thin) and (2) the perception of how good-looking they
significantly worse about their appearance (M 2.57) are overall.
and their athletic competence (M = 2.67) than do males Moreover, when both genders are considered, evi-
(Ms of 2.88 and 3.00, respectively). These same gender dence indicates that girls and boys experience pubertal
differences have been reported in other college samples changes differently (Graber, Peterson, & Brooks-Gunn,
as well (P. Crocker & Ellsworth, 1990; Klein, OBryant, 1996). Boys express greater satisfaction with the
& Hopkins, 1996; McGregor, Mayleben, Buzzanga, changes (e.g., becoming taller, more muscular, and
Davis, & Becker, 1992). lower voice) changes that signal masculinity (Nolen-
Interpretations of the gender differences in perceived Hoeksema & Girus, 1994). In contrast, girls lose their
athletic competence have focused on the fact that, his- prepubertal body (an image currently valued in our soci-
torically, sports have been largely a male domain, with ety with regard to thinness) and can be distressed by
far more opportunities for athletic competition that their new sexual status (Usmiani & Daniluk, 1997; see
would allow boys to develop their physical skills. More- also Furnham, Badmin, & Sneade, 2002, for links be-
over, male sports figures represent powerful role models tween body dissatisfaction, low self-esteem, and eating-
that male children and adolescents are eager to emulate. disordered behavior).
Despite the gains that some females have achieved in en- Body dissatisfaction becomes critical to the extent
tering the world of sports, women athletes have not, for that it leads to other mental health concerns such as eat-
the most part, been viewed as role models for those girls ing-disordered behavior. There is overwhelming evi-
and female adolescents in the mainstream culture. dence that it is also associated with depression (see
In the United States, the current female role models reviews by Allgood-Merton, Lewinsohn, & Hops, 1990;
are glamorous women who are extremely thin, an image Harter, 1999; Marcotte, Fortin, Potvin, & Papillon,
that is not consistent with the muscular, mesomorphic 2002; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994; Rosenberg,
body types of most female athletes. Moreover, images of Schoenback, Schooler, & Rosenberg, 1995; Tobin-
female attractiveness are very punishing in that they are Richard, Boxer, Kavrell, & Peterson, 1984). Pollack also
unattainable by the vast majority of girls and women in reports a strong relationship between body dissatisfac-
the culture. As a result, most females fall far short of tion and depression such that those who perceived them-
these ideals, resulting in the pattern of findings obtained selves to be of normal weight were less depressed than
for perceived physical appearancethat females feel those who thought where were overweight or under-
particularly inadequate. weight. In a related study, Furnham and Calnan (1998)
The role of adherence to cultural standards of appear- presented findings revealing that over two-thirds of ado-
ance, in consort with pubertal development for adoles- lescent females were dissatisfied with their weight. All
cent girls extends previous findings (see reviews by females wanted to weigh less. However, males were di-
Graber, Peterson, & Brooks-Gunn, 1996; Simmons & vided between those (38%) who wanted to gain weight
Blyth, 1987). Early maturing girls, in particular, report and those (31%) wanted to lose weight. The wish to gain
lower self-esteem than do those whose pubertal timing weight was associated with the desire to become more
is more normative (see Lamb, Jackson, Cassidy, Priest, muscular and achieve the masculine ideal of the V-
1993; William and Currie, 2000). Given the current shaped figure.
ideal of thinness for females, early maturing girls must The playing field has shifted for men in recent years.
contend with the fact that they are heavier, with larger In former years, males could be judged attractive not
breasts and wider hips. Because they do not fit the phys- only on the basis of their physical features, where they
ical mold that the culture dictates, their self-esteem is was much more latitude than for females, but by virtue
compromised and they thus acknowledge that they are of the fact that they have money, status, or power. (A
simply too fat. Their early maturation status also intro- magazine poll of women just after the Gulf War ended
duces social problems in that they are not psychologi- revealed that General Norman Schwartzkopf was judged
Gender Dif ferences in Global and Domain-Specific Self-Evaluations 557

to be the sexiest man in America.) This observation was To the extent that they view athletics as a male domain,
made 15 years ago. It is my conjecture, as I look around they are likely to avoid sports activities that would allow
my world and steep myself in gender literature on ap- them to develop physical skills. However, their primarily
pearance and contemporary magazine articles and ad- feminine orientation would appear to lead them to em-
vertisements that trends are changing rapidly and the phasize the importance of physical attractiveness. At-
bar has been raised for men. Standards of appearance tentiveness to this domain may well serve to highlight
for men have become more important, more salient in the difficulty of attaining the impossible standards of
our culture as well as more difficult to obtain. Muscles, beauty that are touted by the culture. As a result, they
abs, calves, the V-body shape, and hair ( both facial hair judge their appearance quite unfavorably relative to the
and head hair or its absence) all must conform to new judgments of androgynous females and to both androgy-
and punishing expectations for males, beginning in nous and masculine males.
childhood. Workout centers and plastic surgeons are re- Femininity, therefore, will represent a liability in
peating big benefits but is our culture, when the focus each of these physical domains to the extent that it is
has become so much more on the outer, physical self not combined with masculine attributes. It should be
than on the inner, psychological self ? noted that the feminine girls also reported significantly
Returning to the overall pattern of gender differences more negative perceptions of their scholastic compe-
obtained for both athletic competence and physical ap- tence (M = 2.80) compared to their androgynous fe-
pearance, the pattern favoring males is qualified by an male peers (M = 3.14). Eschewing such masculine
examination of particular subgroups of females. For ex- attributes as assertiveness and competitiveness, while
ample, in the study of college students by P. Crocker and identifying primarily with such interpersonal attri-
Ellsworth (1990), the investigators separately examined butes as connectedness and concern for others, may di-
a subgroup of physical education majors. They found vert their attention from academic pursuits. However,
that the females in this group reported significantly such a feminine orientation leads to perceptions of so-
higher perceptions of their athletic ability than did the cial acceptance and behavioral conduct that are compa-
normative sample as a whole. Moreover, among physical rable to those of androgynous females, as might be
education majors, the female students did not differ expected given its interpersonal focus.
from the male students. The advantage conferred by the With regard to gender differences in other domains,
physical education program did not, however, transfer to at the college level, significant gender differences favor-
the domain of physical appearance, where females con- ing males have also been found for perceived creativity
tinued to feel significantly worse about their looks than (Klein et al., 1996; McGregor et al., 1992). In our own
did males. Marsh and Jackson (1986) report a similar data, the gender difference in creativity approaches sig-
pattern in that female athletes, beginning at the high nificance. Thus, while females do not differ from males
school level, reported higher physical ability than did in their perceptions of either general intellectual ability
nonathletes, although the groups did not differ signifi- or scholastic performance, they do judge their creativity
cantly in their perceptions of their physical appearance. to be inferior relative to the ratings of male college stu-
We have also found across several adolescent samples dents. The gender socialization literature (see Basow,
that gender orientation impacts the perceptions of fe- 1992; Beale, 1994; Eagly, Beall, & Sternberg, 2004;
males in particular. Those females endorsing a feminine Eisenberg, Martin, & Fabes, 1996; Ruble & Martin,
orientation (where they identify with feminine sex-role 1998) emphasizes that boys receive more encouragement
stereotypes but reject masculine attributes) report more and opportunities for exploration and inventiveness that
negative perceptions of their athletic ability (M = 2.41) may, in turn, lead to males enhanced perceptions of
and their physical appearance (M = 2.43) than do an- creativity at the college level. (Because the domain of
drogynous females who endorse both feminine and mas- creativity has not been included on the instruments for
culine items (Ms = 2.92 and 2.87, respectively). The older children and adolescents, researchers have not yet
scores of these androgynous females do not differ signif- determined whether there are gender differences at
icantly from those of either masculine or androgynous younger ages.)
males, for whom scores range from 2.92 to 3.18 across Across some samples in this country and abroad,
the two subscales. Thus, feminine girls are at particular gender differences in perceived behavioral conduct
risk for unfavorable evaluations of their physical selves. favor girls. Two studies at the college level report that
558 The Self

females score higher on the morality subscale. How- Santinello, & Scarpazza, 1988), Greece (Makris-Bot-
ever, other studies find no gender differences in con- saris & Robinson, 1991), Japan (Maeda, 1997; Sakurai,
duct or morality. Moreover, when differences are 1983), Korea (Rhee, 1993), and China (Stigler et al.,
obtained, as in our own samples, they are much smaller 1985). For the most part, the reliabilities have been
in magnitude than the highly consistent gender differ- good to adequate. In certain countries, particular items
ences found for athletic competence and physical have attenuated the reliability for a given subscale and
appearance, favoring males. With regard to global self- have not loaded on their designated factor (although the
worth, studies in this country as well as abroad reveal overall subscale structure has been demonstrated).
either no gender differences, or a small but nonsignifi- Some of these item difficulties may reflect translation
cant difference, favoring males. Thus, males and fe- issues. However, the existence of such items should
males evaluate themselves similarly with regard to their serve as a red flag that a given instrument may require
perceptions of overall worth as a person, where scores revisions at the item level to be culturally sensitive to
typically remain around 3.0. In summary, the pattern potential differences in how the domains are best de-
reveals markedly more favorable self-evaluations for fined in a given country.
males with regard to perceptions of both athletic There are further cautions about the use of our in-
prowess and physical attractiveness. At the college struments in countries such as China and Japan. For
level, males also report greater creativity than do fe- example, Meredith et al. (1991) have reported that
males. These differences have been documented across only 20 of the 36 items on the Self-Perception Profile
numerous samples and are exacerbated when compar- for Children factored appropriately. Moreover, in both
isons are made between feminine girls and males of this Chinese sample as well as the sample studied by
either androgynous or masculine orientations. Stigler et al. (1985), reliabilities were far from accept-
able. The Global Self-Worth subscale was particularly
problematic in both studies (alphas of .57 and .54).
CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISONS Lee (C. Lee, personal communication, April 5, 1987)
found similar problems with a Chinese American sam-
It has become increasingly common for investigators in ple and thoughtfully concluded that the concept of
other countries to administer self-concept scales, such global self-worth as defined in the American main-
as our own, to children and adolescents in their own cul- stream culture may not be an appropriate construct to
ture. As noted earlier, the finding that males feel better include on an instrument examining meaningful self-
about their athletic competence and physical appearance perceptions among the Chinese. The Meredith et al.
than do females has been exceedingly consistent across (1991) study reported relatively low reliabilities
countries. However, there are potential pitfalls in admin- across all subscales (ranging from .44 to .61) suggest-
istering measures developed for a given culture to those ing that there exist items that are inappropriate for
from other countries. At a minimum, any meaningful in- each of the domains.
terpretation requires that these instruments show com- Meredith, Wang, and Zheng (1993) have also argued
parable psychometric properties. However, attention that there are additional domains of relevance to Chi-
must also be directed to culturally relevant content, be- nese children that are not included on our American in-
cause domains and/or items in a given subscale may struments. As a first step, they added several other
need to be tailored to each culture. possible dimensions and asked Chinese children to rate
Across the studies in non-American countries, the their importance. Among these additions were items
factor pattern itself has been shown to be quite robust. It tapping group orientation (e.g., willingness to help oth-
has been replicated in other English speaking countries, ers), social conduct (e.g., respect for parental and
such as Canada (P. Crocker & Ellsworth, 1990) and Aus- teacher authority), and social acceptance (e.g., engaging
tralia (Trent, Russell, & Cooney, 1994). It has also been in behaviors such as getting good grades that would meet
replicated in non-English-speaking samples from Que- with peer and adult approval). Such an approach is com-
bec (Boivin, Vitaro, & Gagnon, 1992; Gavin & Herry, mendable in that it addresses culturally sensitive issues
1996), Switzerland (Pierrehumbert et al., 1987), Ger- involving the inclusion of domains that are most relevant
many (Asendorpf & van Aken, 1993), Italy (Pedrabissi, for a given culture.
Ethnic Dif ferences in Our Own Culture 559

Of further concern is that in Chinese (Stigler et al., Cultural Revolution extended to psychology where, for 2
1985), Japanese (Maeda, 1997; Sakurai, 1983), and Ko- to 3 decades, progress and productivity was effectively
rean (Rhee, 1993) samples, the means are considerably halted. This vacuum has exacerbated the current search
lower than are scores in U.S., Canadian, Australian, and for methods, measures, and paradigms from Western
European samples. (The domain of social acceptance is countries that may be applicable. As I observed at a re-
perhaps the only exception.) Stigler et al. (1985) offer cent conference in Beijing, many Western psychologists
two possible interpretations for the low scores of his are eager to share their theoretical and methodological
Chinese sample. The first is that the Chinese appear to wares with their Chinese colleagues. However, in our
display a self-effacing style that leads them to be more zeal to be benevolent (or in our less than benevolent
modest in their report of personal qualities. The second quest for fame), we need to guard against imposing
is that our structured alternative format, in which we frameworks and related instruments that may be inap-
contrast statements about Some kids versus Other propriate for a given culture either because they do not
kids, implicitly demands a form of social comparison adequately tap the construct in question or because the
with others. Stigler and colleagues observe that such so- construct may not be that critical to the functioning of
cial comparison is frowned on in China, where individ- individuals in that culture, or both.
ual differences in competence are downplayed. Thus,
Chinese childrens unwillingness to report that they may
be superior to others leads to a pattern of low scores that
may not truly reveal their private perceptions of per- ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN
sonal adequacy. These same interpretations may well OUR OWN CULTURE
apply to other Asian countries such as Japan and Korea.
In summary, the use of our instruments would appear Most of the work on ethnic differences has been com-
to be particularly problematic in Asian cultures where parisons between the self-esteem of African Americans
(a) the content of certain items may not be relevant or and Europeans in this country. For many years is was
meaningful, ( b) other culturally sensitive content is merely assumed that Blacks, as they were called at the
needed, and (c) response tendencies (e.g., a self-effacing time, would have lower self-esteem due to their initial
style coupled with an avoidance of social comparison) status as slaves, their treatment by White society, their
may require different item content, a different response status as second-class citizens, and therefore their cul-
format, and an instructional set to maximize the report tural marginalization. However, with the advent of at-
of a true evaluation of ones perceived competencies. tention to the Black community by psychologists, using
It should be noted that we have never recommended appropriate methodologies, these myths and assump-
the use of our instruments in other countries, particu- tions were challenged. Two recent, excellent meta-analy-
larly in cultures in which the self may be construed dif- ses (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000; Twenge & Crocker,
ferently, or in which perceptions of self may not be that 2000) have clearly documented that African Americans
central to individuals functioning. Rather, we urge that in our culture have higher self-esteem than European-
investigators adopt a more culture-specific perspective, Americans, and these and other investigators have devel-
focusing on the very meaning of self-constructs and oped trenchant analyses of why this might be the case.
their potential correlates for a given culture. An empha- The majority of the studies they have reviewed focus
sis on correlates and consequences of self-perceptions is on subject populations ranging from late childhood to
particularly essential because it is important to address early adulthood. These analyses indicate few gender dif-
the issue of whether self-judgments do have any pre- ferences. However, the studies do suggest an increase in
dictable impact on other systems (e.g., behavioral or self-esteem as African Americans go through the stages
emotional) of interest: Investigators in any country, our of childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. The
own included, need to be clear about the purpose of ex- question becomes what are the reasons for what has
amining self-perceptions and should attend to their seemed to be, for some, counterintuitive findings?
functional role. Gray-Little and Hafdahl (2000) form two related
The need for such an approach in China is particu- questions that capture the readers attention. First, why
larly pressing. The intellectual vacuum created by the do Blacks not have lower self-esteem? Second, why do
560 The Self

they have higher self-esteem compared to Whites? From individual incorporates the attitudes of significant oth-
a Cooley, symbolic-interactionist perspective, which the ers toward the self, the context for self-esteem develop-
authors embrace as a social framework on reflected ment in African Americans involves the African
self-appraisals, one needs to ask to whom do Blacks turn American family, peers, and community. Thus, African
as the significant others for feedback about the self ? American children and adolescents internalize the opin-
They suggest that Blacks do not turn to the larger White ions of parents and siblings, as well as African Ameri-
society as their reference groups but rather turn to the can friends, teachers, and coaches, who serve as their
Black community as their source of support and accep- primary social reference groups. Interestingly, the rela-
tance; these are the people whose opinions are most im- tionship between the attitudes of significant others to-
portant to them. Adhering to these values, accepting ward the self and self-esteem has been found to be
them, makes them less vulnerable to their marginaliza- somewhat stronger among African Americans than
tion by the White culture, and allows them to develop a among European-Americans (Rosenberg & Simmons,
sense of meeting the expectations of their ethnic in- 1972). It has been suggested that the African American
group, thereby experiencing high self-esteem (as the community is a source of positive self-concept in
Jamesian hypothesis would predict). African American children and that, under certain con-
It is not that the findings indicate that Black self- ditions, the African American family can filter out de-
esteem is equal to that of Whites. Rather, meta-analyses structive racist messages from the White community,
confirm that it is actually higher. Several processes supplanting such messages with more positive feedback
emerge as explanations. For example, social comparison that will enhance self-esteem (Barnes, 1980).
looms large as a factor in impacting an individuals self- In keeping with one theme of this chapter, the predic-
esteem. To the extent that Blacks are comparing them- tiveness of Jamess formulation, more recent work
selves to other Blacks, rather than White norms for (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000) applies such an analysis
success, this potentially enhances their self-esteem. to the level of self-esteem in African American youth
A related mechanism suggested by Gray-Little and (see also Harter, 1990a). To the extent that African
Hafdahl (2000) is that Blacks engage in a quest for a pos- American values differ from those of Whites, different
itive social identity that emphasizes their desirable dis- domains will be judged important. For example, there
tinctiveness. To do this means to adopt a Black racial is a stronger correlation between school grades and
identity, to view your racial heritage as positive, articu- self-esteem among European Americans than among
lated, and meaningful, and that identity is therefore in- African Americans, suggesting that the two racial
corporated into your sense of worth as a person. groups may well base their self-esteem on different at-
According to Twenge and Crocker (2002), stigmati- tributes (Epps, 1975). If we assume that people value
zation, previously thought to be a potential source of those things at which they do well, and try to do well in
negative self-esteem, has been cast in a new light in those domains that they value, we see that African
these reviews. Developing a positive racial identify may American adolescents may come to value those nonacad-
enable racial minorities to attribute negative outcomes emic arenas in which they feel they excel and over
to prejudice, to make in-group comparisons, or to use which they have some control and devalue their negative
other self-protective mechanisms that membership in a academic experiences (see Hare, 1985). It has also been
stigmatized group affords. Thus, one devalues the stig- suggested (Hunt & Hunt, 1977) that African American
matizing of the majority culture. These authors also male youth, in particular, substitute compensatory val-
point to interesting cohort effects in that the civil rights ues in areas where they can perform more successfully.
movement has had an effect. They are quick to point out For example, athletic prowess, musical talent, acting
that there is a time lag in the effects of such movements, ability, sexuality, and certain antisocial behaviors may
approximately 20 years, and they liken this to gains for become more highly valued than academic performance.
women given the womens movement. Thus, self-esteem While these theories can be applied to ethnic groups
effects in favor of Blacks have increased more recently. as a whole, it is critical to appreciate the fact that while
It should be noted that the basic processes underlying average levels of self-esteem are somewhat higher
self-esteem formation among African Americans ap- among African Americans compared to European
pears to be similar to that of White adolescents (see re- Americans there is still tremendous overlap in the dis-
view in Harter, 1990b). Given the notion that an tributions, and considerably variability in each group
References 561

that can also be explained by the theories advanced. We ical perspectives have concentrated more heavily on the
now need to focus on understanding how these processes social construction of the self. More recent treatments
affect the lives of individual youth, with an eye toward have respected this historical perspective, expanding it
prevention and intervention. Twenge and Crocker (2002) into how social influences vary depending on the devel-
also suggest that we now need to move away from ques- opmental level of the child and the individual differ-
tions about which broad racial groups have higher or ences in the role of caregivers.
lower self-esteem on average and that we need to focus Cognitive-developmental differences, at the impetus
more on questions about individual differences in each of neo-Piagetians, have heightened our appreciation for
culture, including their causes. how more subtle and discrete changes in cognitive ad-
More recently, toward this goal, a recent study vances and limitations influence self-development. The
(Bean, Bush, McKenny, & Wilson, 2003) examined the I-self has been transmitted into those changing cogni-
support offered to African American high school stu- tive processes that determine how the Me-self (ones
dents for academic achievement and global self- verbalizable sense of self ) will necessarily change with
esteem. Findings indicated that maternal support and age. The field has far more appreciation for how broad
acceptance was significantly related to academic stages, previously conceived as childhood and adoles-
achievement and to adolescent self-esteem (whereas fa- cence, must be broken down into the mini-substages in
ther support was unrelated). They observed that moth- each broad categories of development. With regard to
ers occupy a central role in many African American changes in self-development, we have identified three
families including more responsibility for child rearing substages in childhood and three substages in adoles-
(see also Collins, 1993). They point out that the cence, where self-development makes major leaps in
mothers role can be seen as especially pivotal because content and organization. Normative cognitive advances
they often take on the instrumental responsibilities and and limitations clearly define the self. The contribu-
expressive and emotional functions in the family due to tions of child-rearing practices also make major contri-
the difficulties that African American males have his- butions in terms of the positivity or the negativity of
torically encountered in carrying out provider roles self-evaluations. Genetic advances have also led to spec-
(Fine & Schwebel, 1988). ulations about the self.
Rowley, Sellers, Chavous, and Smith (1998) provide Two themes have been emphasized: (1) the role of
perhaps a summary statement that can account for many cognitive-development and (2) the role of socializing in-
of the more recent findings. They focus on the relation- fluences on the self. These clearly lead to normative-de-
ship between private racial regard and personal self- velopmental trends in self-development. Equally
esteem. Attitudes regarding African Americans were important is how such influences lead to individual dif-
significantly related to the self-esteem of those individ- ferences, particularly in self-evaluations. Thus, major
uals for whom race plays an important role in defining attention has been devoted to theory and empirical find-
themselves. Such attitudes were unrelated to self- ings on such individual differences.
esteem for those whose race is less central to their defi- Our explorations need to extend beyond individual
nition of self. Thus, African Americans attitudes about differences, to include gender and ethnic differences in
their race are only important to their self-esteem if race our own cultural and cross-cultural considerations.
is a personally affirmed identity. Sensitivity to gender and cultural differences are criti-
cal in understanding how the self is constructed. These
CONCLUSIONS are the future directions that the study of self-
development must take.
The study of self-development continues to thrive as
new theoretical, methodological, and empirical perspec-
tives emerge. There continues to be an interesting mar- REFERENCES
riage between historical formulations about the self,
stemming from James (1890), Cooley (1902), Baldwin Ainsworth, M. (1979). Infant-mother attachment. American Psychol-
ogist, 34, 932937.
(1897), and Mead (1934), and more recently Bowlby
Allgood-Merten, B., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, R. (1990). Sex dif-
(1979) and Ainsworth (1979) and many contemporary ferences and adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
perspectives that are represented in this chapter. Histor- ogy, 99, 5563.
562 The Self

Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1993). Deutsche versionen Bleiberg, E. (1984). Narcissistic disorders in children. Bulletin of the
der Selbstkonzeptskalen von Harter [German versions of Harters Menninger Clinic, 48, 501517.
Self-Concept Scales]. Zeittscrift fur Entwicklungspsychologie und Block, J. H., & Robins, R. W. (1993). A longitudinal study of consis-
Padagogische Psycholigie, 25, 6486. tency and change in self-esteem from early adolescence to early
Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Social and ethical interpretations in mental de- adulthood. Child Development, 64, 909923.
velopment: A study in social psychology. New York: Macmillan. Blos, P. (1962). On adolescence. New York: Free Press.
Barnes, E. J. (1980). The Black community as a source of positive self- Blyth, D. A., Simmons, R. G., & Carlton-Ford, S. (1983). The adjust-
concept for Black children: A theoretical perspective. In R. Jones ment of early adolescents to school transitions. Journal of Early
(Ed.), Black psychology (pp. 231250). New York: Harper & Row. Adolescence, 3, 105120.
Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, nega- Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Gagnon, C. (1992). A reassessment of the
tive expressivity, and the development of type D attachments from self-perception profile for children: Factor structure, reliability,
12 to 24 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in and convergent validity of a French version among second
Child Development, 64(3, Serial No. 258), 97118. through sixth grade children. International Journal of Behavioral
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles Development, 15, 275290.
among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Bolognini, M., Plancheral, B., Bettschart, W., & Halfon, O. (1996).
Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226244. Self-esteem and mental health in early adolescence: Developmen-
tal and gender differences. Journal of Adolescence, 233245.
Basow, S. A. (1992). Gender stereotypes and roles (3rd ed.). Pacific
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New
York: Basic Books.
Bates, E. (1990). Language about me and you: Pronominal reference
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York:
and the emerging concept of self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly
Basic Books.
(Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 115).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and de-
pression. New York: Basic Books.
Baumeister, R. F. (1987). How the self became a problem: A psycho-
logical review of historical research. Journal of Personality and Bracken, B. (1996). Clinical applications of a context-dependent
Social Psychology, 52, 90113. multi-dimensional model of self-concept. In B. Bracken (Ed.),
Handbook of self-concept (pp. 463505). New York: Wiley.
Baumeister, R. F. (1993). Understanding the inner nature of low self-
Bretherton, I. (1987). New perspectives on attachment relations: Se-
esteem: Uncertain, fragile, protective, and conf licted. In R. F.
curity, communication, and internal working models. In J. D. Os-
Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem, the puzzle of low self-regard
ofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed.,
(pp. 201218). New York: Plenum Press.
pp. 10611101). New York: Wiley.
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of
Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bottles: The social
threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of
self as internal working model. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe
self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 533.
(Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Development: Vol. 23. Self
Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon processes and development (pp. 141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 349378). San Bretherton, I. (1993). From dialogue to internal working models: The
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. co-construction of self in relationships. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.),
Beale, C. R. (1994). Boys and girls: The development of gender roles. Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 26. Memory and
New York: McGraw-Hill. af fect (pp. 237363). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bean, R. A., Bush, K. R., McKenny, P. C., & Wilson, S. M. (2003). Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states:
The impact of parental support, behavioral control, and psycho- The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psy-
logical control on the academic achievement and self-esteem of chology, 18, 906921.
African American and European American Adolescents. Journal Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models
of Adolescent Research, 18, 523541. in attachment relationships. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.),
Beeghly, M., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D. (1986, April). Child mal- Handbook of attachment (pp. 89111). New York: Guilford Press.
treatment and the self: The emergence of internal state language in Briere, J. (1989). Therapy for adults molested as children. New
low SES 30-month-olds. Paper presented at the International Con- York: Springer.
ference on Infant Studies, Beverly Hills, CA. Briere, J. (1992). Child abuse trauma: Theory and treatment of the
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, lasting ef fects. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
and the self-system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon at Brim, O. B. (1976). Life span development of the theory of oneself:
high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 530. Implications for child development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Ad-
Bem, S. (1985). Androgyny and gender schema theory. In T. B. Son- vances in child development and behavior (Vol. 11, pp. 82103).
deregger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol.32, New York: Academic Press.
pp. 180226). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Brooks-Gunn, J. (1988). Antecedents and consequences of variations
Berndt, T., & Burgy, L. (1996). The social self-concept. In B. A. in girls maturational timing. Journal of Adolescent Health Care,
Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept (pp. 171209). New 9, 365373.
York: Wiley. Brooks-Gunn, J., & Peterson, A. (1983). Girls at puberty: Biological
Blatt, S. J. (1995). Representational structures in psychopathology. In and psychological perspectives. New York: Plenum Press.
D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Develop- Broughton, J. (1978). The development of the concepts of self, mind,
mental Psychology: Vol. 6. Emotion, cognition, and representation reality, and knowledge. In W. Damon (Ed.), Social cognition
(pp. 134). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. (pp. 75100). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
References 563

Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S. S. Feldman Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001). Diverse patterns of neuren-
& G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent docrine activity in maltreated children. Development and Psycho-
(pp. 171196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. pathology, 13, 677694.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- Collins, M. (1993). School social context, self-esteem, and locus-of-
sity Press. control among White, Black, and Hispanic youth (Doctoral dis-
Budwig, N. (2000). Language and the construction of self. In N. Bud- sertation, University of Maryland, College Park). Dissertation
wig & I. Uzgiris (Eds.), Advances in applied developmental psy- Abstracts International, 54, 692693.
chology (pp. 83112). Westport, CT: Ablex. Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York:
Bugental, D. B., & Goodnow, J. J. (1998). Socialization Processes. In Charles Scribner & Sons.
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand- Cooper, C. R., Grotevant, H. D., & Condon, S. M. (1983). Individual-
book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality ity and connectedness both foster adolescent identity formation
development (5th ed., pp. 389462). New York: Wiley. and role taking skills. In H. D. Grotevant & C. R. Cooper (Eds.),
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1989). Disorganized/disori- Adolescent development in the family: New directions for child de-
ented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmen- velopment (pp. 4359). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
tal Psychology, 25, 525531. Cooper, C. R., Jackson, J. F., Azmitia, M., Lopez, E., & Dunbar, N.
Carroll, J. J., & Steward, M. S. (1984). The role of cognitive develop- (1995). Bridging students multiple worlds: African American
ment in childrens understandings of their own feelings. Child De- and Latino youth in academic outreach programs. In R. F. Marcias
velopment, 55, 14861492. & R. G. Garcia-Ramos (Eds.), Changing schools changing stu-
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New dents: An anthology of research on language minorities
York: Academic Press. (pp. 211234). Santa Barbara: University of California Linguistic
Minority Research Institute.
Case, R. (1992). The minds staircase. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coopersmith, S. A. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San Fran-
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self in 6-year-
cisco: Freeman.
olds. Child Development, 59, 121134.
Coster, W. J., Gersten, M. S., Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1989).
Cassidy, J. (1990). Theoretical and methodological considerations in
Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmen-
the study of attachment and the self in young children. In M. T.
tal Psychology, 25, 10201029.
Greeberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in
the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention Crain, R. M. (1996). The inf luences of age, race, and gender on child
(pp. 87120). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. and adolescent multi-dimensional self-concept. In B. A. Bracken
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relationship (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept (pp. 395420). New York: Wiley.
to other defensive processes. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski Crittenden, P. M. (1988). Relationships at risk. In J. Belsky & T. Nez-
(Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300326). Hills- worski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 136174).
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment. Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Internal representational models of attach-
New York: Guilford Press. ment relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 259277.
Chodorow, N. (1989). Feminism and psychoanalytic theory. New Crittenden, P. M. (1994). Peering into the black box: An ex-
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ploratory treatise on the development of self in young children.
Cicchetti, D. (1989). How research on child maltreatment has in- In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on
formed the study of child development: Perspectives from devel- Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 5. Disorders and dysfunc-
opmental psychology. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child tions of the self (pp. 79148). Rochester, NY: University of
maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and conse- Rochester Press.
quences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 309350). New York: Crittenden, P. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Child maltreatment
Cambridge University Press. and attachment theory. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), The-
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, re- ory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and
f lects, and projections. Developmental Review, 13, 471502. neglect (pp. 432463). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cicchetti, D. (2004). An odyssey of discovery: Lessons learned Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem:
through 3 decades of research on child maltreatment. American The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review,
Psychologist, 59, 728741. 96, 608630.
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1990). The self in transition: Infancy to Crocker, P. R. E., & Ellsworth, J. P. (1990). Perceptions of compe-
childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. tence in physical education students. Canadian Journal of Sports
Science, 15, 262266.
Cicchetti, D., Beeghly, M., Carlson, V., & Toth, S. (1990). The emer-
gence of the self in atypical populations. In D. Cicchetti & M. Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and
Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 309344). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the
Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (1995). Perspectives in developmental control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Develop- 53, 10241037.
mental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 322). Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis for
New York: Wiley. true-self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Ef ficacy, agency, and self-
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality esteem (pp. 3146). New York: Plenum Press.
in developmental psychopathology. Developmental and Psycho- Demo, D. H., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (1992). Self-concept stability
pathology, 8, 597600. and change during adolescence. In R. P. Lipka & T. M. Brinthaupt
564 The Self

(Eds.), Self-perspectives across the life span (pp. 116150). Al- the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect
bany: State University of New York Press. (pp. 647684). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dickstein, E. (1977). Self and self-esteem: Theoretical foundations and Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Is-
their implications for research. Human Development, 20, 129140. sues, 1, 18164.
Donaldson, S. K., & Westerman, M. A. (1986). Development of chil- Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York:
drens understandings of ambivalence and causal theories of emo- Norton.
tion. Developmental Psychology, 22, 655662. Fasig, L. G. (2000). Toddlers understanding of ownership: Implications
DuBois, D. L., Reach, K., Tevendale, H., & Valentine, J. (2001, for self-concept development. Social Development, 9, 370382.
April). Self-esteem in early adolescents: Trait, state, or both? Feiring, C., & Taska, L. S. (1996). Family self-concept: Ideas on its
Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Re- meaning. In B. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept
search in Child Development, New Orleans, LA. (pp. 317373). New York: Wiley.
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, Fenzel, L. M. (2002). Prospective study of changes in global self-
MA: Harvard University Press. worth and strain during the transition to middle school. Journal of
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling Early Adsolescence, 20, 93116.
states and childrens later understanding of others emotions. De- Fine, M. A., & Schwebel, A. (1988). An emergent explanation of
velopmental Psychology, 27, 445448. differing racial reactions to single parenthood. Journal of Di-
Dusek, J. B., & Flaherty, J. (1981). The development of the self dur- vorce, 12, 175.
ing the adolescent years. Monograph of the Society for Research in Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The con-
Child Development, 46(Whole No. 191), 161. trol and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Re-
Eagley, A. H., Beall, A. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (2004). The psychology view, 87, 477531.
of gender (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Fischer, K. W., & Ayoub, C. (1994). Affective splitting and dissocia-
Eccles, J. P., & Midgley, C. (1989). State/environment fit: Develop- tion in normal and maltreated children: Developmental pathways
mentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. In R. for self in relationships. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.),
Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 5.
(Vol. 3, pp. 139181). New York: Academic Press. Disorders and dysfunctions of the self (pp. 149222). Rochester,
NY: University of Rochester Press.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. (1984). Grade-related changes
in the school environment: Effects on achievement motivation. In Fischer, K. W., & Canfield, R. (1986). The ambiguity of stage and
J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation structure in behavior: Person and environment in the develop-
(pp. 282331). Greenwich, CT: JSI Press. ment of psychological structure. In I. Levin (Ed.), Stage and
structure: Reopening the debate (pp. 246267). New York:
Eichenbaum, L., & Orbach, S. (1993). Understanding women: A femi-
Plenum Press.
nist psychoanalytic approach. New York: Basic Books.
Fischer, K. W., Hand, H. H., Watson, M. W., Van Parys, M., &
Eisenberg, A. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in con-
Tucker, J. (1984). Putting the child into socialization: The devel-
versations. Discourse Processes, 8, 177204.
opment of social categories in preschool children. In L. Katz
Eisenberg, N., Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (1996). Gender develop- (Ed.), Current topics in early childhood education (Vol. 5,
ment and gender effects. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), pp. 2772). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 358396). New York:
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P., & Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions de-
Simon & Schuster/ Macmillan.
velop and how they organize development. Cognition and Emo-
Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, tion, 4, 81127.
38, 10251034. Fivush, R. (1987). Scripts and categories: Inter-relationships in de-
Engel, M. (1959). The stability of the self-concept in adolescence. velopment. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual develop-
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 211217. ment: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization
Epps, E. G. (1975). Impact of school desegregation on aspirations, (pp. 223248). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
self-concepts, and other aspects of personality. Law and Contem- Fivush, R., Gray, J. T., & Fromhoff, F. A. (1987). Two-year-olds talk
porary Problems, 39, 300313. about the past. Cognitive Development, 2, 393409.
Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited or a theory of a theory. Fivush, R., & Hamond, N. R. (1990). Autobiographical memory
American Psychologist, 28, 405416. across the preschool years: Toward reconceptualizing childhood
Epstein, S. (1981). The unity principle versus the reality and pleasure amnesia. In R. Fivush & A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remem-
principles, or the tale of the scorpion and the frog. In M. D. Lynch, bering in young children (pp. 223248). New York: Cambridge
A. A. Norem-Hebeisen, & K. Gergen (Eds.), Self-concept: Advances University Press.
in theory and research (pp. 82110). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Fivush, R., & Hudson, J. A. (Eds.). (1990). Knowing and remember-
Epstein, S. (1991). Cognitive-experiential self-theory: Implications ing in young children. New York: Cambridge University Press.
for developmental psychology. In M. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development (2nd ed.). Englewood
(Eds.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Development: Vol. 23. Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Self-processes and development (pp. 111137). Hillsdale, NJ: Fonagy, P. (2002). Understanding of mental states, mother-infant in-
Erlbaum. teraction, and the development of the self. In J. Martin Maldon-
Erickson, M. F., Egeland, B., & Pianta, R. (1989). The effects of mal- ado-Duran (Ed.), Infant and toddler mental health: Models of
treatment on the development of young children. In D. Cicchetti & clinical intervention with infants and their families (pp. 5872).
V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
References 565

Fox, K., Page, A., Armstrong, N., & Kirby, B. (1994). Dietary re- Griffin, N., Chassin, L., & Young, R. D. (1981). Measurement of
straint and self-perceptions in early adolescence. Personality and global self-concept versus multiple role specific self-concepts in
Individual Dif ferences, 17, 8796. adolescents. Adolescence, 16, 4956.
Freud, A. (1965). Normality and pathology in childhood. New York: Griffin, S. (1992). Structural analysis of the development of their
International Universities Press. inner world: A neo-structured analysis of the development of in-
Freud, S. (1952). A general introduction to psychoanalysis. New York: trapersonal intelligence. In R. Case (Ed.), The minds staircase
Washington Square Press. (pp. 189206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1985). What children say when the Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, C. R. (1986). Individuation in family re-
teacher is not around: Conf licting goals in social comparison and lationships. Human Development, 29, 83100.
performance assessment in the classroom. Journal of Personality Hagborn, W. J. (1994). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale and Har-
and Social Psychology, 48, 550562. ters self-perception profile for adolescents: A concurrent valid-
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1990). Strategies for comparative evalua- ity study. Psychology in the Schools, 30, 132136.
tion: Maintaining a sense of competence across the life span. In Hair, E. C., & Graziano, W. G. (2003). Self-esteem and personality in
R. J. Sternberg & J. Kolligian Jr. (Eds.), Competence considered high school students: A prospective longitudinal study in Texas.
(pp. 167189). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Journal of Personality, 71, 971994.
Furnham, A., Badmin, N., & Sneade, I. (2002). Body image dissatis- Hare, B. R. (1985). Stability and change in self-perception and
faction: Gender differences in eating attitudes, self-esteem, and achievement among Black adolescents: A longitudinal study.
reasons for exercise. Journal of Psychology, 136, 581596. Journal of Black Psychology, 11, 2942.
Furnham, A., & Calnan, A. (1998). Eating disturbance, self-esteem, Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out
reasons for exercising, and body weight dissatisfaction in adoles- the way they doParents matter less than you think. New York:
cent males. European Eating Disorders Review, 6, 5872. Free Press.
Gavin, D. A., & Herry, Y. (1996). The French self-perception profile Harris, P. L. (1983). What children know about the situations that
for children: Score validity and reliability. Educational and Psy- provoke emotion. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), The socializa-
chological Measurement, 56, 678700. tion of af fect (pp. 162185). New York: Plenum Press.
Gecas, V. (1972). Parental behavior and contextual variations in ado-
Harris, P. L., Olthof, T., & Meerum-Terwogt, M. (1981). Childrens
lescent self-esteem. Sociometry, 36, 332345.
knowledge of emotion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Gergen, K. J. (1968). Personal consistency and the presentation of 36, 490509.
self. In C. Gordon & J. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social inter-
Hart, D. (1988). The adolescent self-concept in social context. In
action (pp. 299308). New York: Wiley.
D. K. Lapsley & F. C. Power (Eds.), Self, ego, and identity
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a dif ferent voice: Psychological theory and (pp. 7190). New York: Springer-Verlag.
womens development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hart, D., Fegley, S., & Brengelman, D. (1993). Perceptions of past,
Gilligan, C., Lyons, N., & Hanmer, T. J. (1989). Making connections.
present and future selves among children and adolescence. British
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 265282.
Glick, M., & Zigler, E. (1985). Self-image: A cognitive-developmen-
Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children.
tal approach. In R. Leahy (Ed.), The development of the self
Child Development, 53, 8797.
(pp. 154). New York: Academy Press.
Harter, S. (1988). Developmental and dynamic changes in the nature
Gnepp, J., McKee, E., & Domanic, J. A. (1987). Childrens use of sit-
of the self-concept: Implications for child psychotherapy. In S.
uational information to infer emotion: Understanding emotionally
Shirk (Ed.), Cognitive development and child psychotherapy (pp.
equivocal situations. Developmental Psychology, 23, 114123.
119160). New York: Plenum.
Graber, J. A., Peterson, A. C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Pubertal
processes: Methods, measures, and models. In J. A. Graber (Ed.), Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self-system. In
Transitions through adolescence: Interpersonal domains and con- P. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psy-
text (pp. 2353). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. chology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development
(4th ed., pp. 275385). New York: Wiley.
Gralinski, J., Feshbach, N. D., Powell, C., & Derrington, T. (1993,
April). Self-understanding: Meaning and measurement of mal- Harter, S. (1985). The self-perception profile for children. Unpub-
treated childrens sense of self. Paper presented at the meeting of lished manual, University of Denver, CO.
the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA. Harter, S. (1986a). Cognitive-developmental processes in the inte-
Grandlese, J., & Joseph, S. (1993). Factor analysis of the self- gration of concepts about emotions and the self. Social Cognition,
perception profile for children. Personality and Individual Dif fer- 4, 119151.
ences, 15, 343345. Harter, S. (1986b). Processes underlying the construction, mainte-
Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. R. (2000). Factors inf luencing racial nance, and enhancement of the self-concept in children. In J. Suls
comparisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review. Psychological & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self
Bulletin, 126, 2654. (Vol. 3, pp. 137181). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Greenier, K. D., Kernis, M. H., & Waschull, S. B. (1995). Not all high Harter, S. (1990a). Adolescent self and identity development. In
or low self-esteem people are the same: Theory and research on S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The devel-
stability of self-esteem. In M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Ef ficacy, agency, oping adolescent (pp. 352387). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
and self-esteem (pp. 5168). New York: Plenum Press. versity Press.
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revi- Harter, S. (1990b). Causes, correlates and the functional role of
sion of personal history. American Psychologist, 7, 603618. global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In R. Sternberg & J.
566 The Self

Kolligian Jr. (Eds.), Competence considered (pp. 6798). New concepts. In C. Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Childrens under-
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. standing of emotion (pp. 81116). Cambridge, England: Cam-
Harter, S. (1996). Teacher and classmate inf luences on scholastic bridge University Press.
motivation, self-esteem, and choice. In K. Wentzel & J. Juvonen Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Mirror, mirror. . . . The impor-
(Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding childrens school ad- tance of appearance in everyday life. New York: State University
justment (pp. 1142). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer- of New York Press.
sity Press. Hattie, J. (1992). Self-concept. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Harter, S. (1997). The personal self in social context: Barriers to au- Hattie, J., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on the
thenticity. In R. D. Ashmore & L. Jussim (Eds.), Self and iden- structure of self-concept. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of
tity: Fundamental issues (pp. 81105). New York: Oxford self-concept (pp. 3890). New York: Wiley.
University Press.
Hayne, H. (2004). Infant memory development: Implications for
Harter, S. (1998a). The development of self-representations. In W. childhood amnesia. Developmental Review, 24, 3373.
Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel- Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of
opment (5th ed., pp. 553617). New York: Wiley. a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 60, 895910.
Harter, S. (1998b). The effects of child abuse on the self-system. In
B. B. Rossman & M. S. Rosenberg (Eds.), Multiple victimization of Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
children: Conceptual, developmental, research, and treatment is- Higgins, E. T. (1991). Development of self-regulatory and self-
sues (pp. 147170). New York: Haworth Press. evaluative processes: Costs, benefits, and tradeoffs. In M. R.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self. New York: Guilford Press. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child De-
velopment: Vol. 23. Self processes and development (pp. 125166).
Harter, S. (2004). The developmental emergence of self-esteem: Indi-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
vidual differences in change and stability. In D. Mroczek & T.
Little (Eds.), The handbook of personality (pp. 4459). New York: Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social
Erlbaum. perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369425.
Harter, S., Bresnick, S., Bouchey, H. A., & Whitesell, N. R. (1997). Hill, J. P., & Holmbeck, G. N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy dur-
The development of multiple role-related selves during adoles- ing adolescence. In G. J. Whitehurst (Ed.), Annals of child devel-
cence. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 835854. opment (Vol. 3, pp. 145189). Greenwhich, CT: JAI Press.
Harter, S., & Buddin, B. J. (1987). Childrens understanding of the Howe, M. L. (2003). Memories from the cradle. Current Directions in
simultaneity of two emotions: A five-stage developmental acqui- Psychological Science, 12, 6265.
sition sequence. Developmental Psychology, 23, 388399. Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of in-
Harter, S., Kiang, L., Whitesell, N. R., & Anderson, A. V. (2003, fantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305326.
April). A prototype approach to the emotion of humiliation in col- Hudson, J. A. (1990a). Constructive processes in childrens
lege students. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Soci- autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 26,
ety for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL. 180187.
Harter, S., Low, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (2003). What have we Hudson, J. A. (1990b). The emergence of autobiographical memory in
learned from Columbine: The impact of the self-system on sui- mother-child conversation. In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.),
cidal and violent ideation among adolescents. Journal of Youth Knowing and remembering in young children (pp. 166196). New
Violence, 2, 326. York: Cambridge University Press.
Harter, S., Marold, D. B., Whitesell, N. R., & Cobbs, G. (1996). A Hunt, J. G., & Hunt, L. L. (1977). Racial inequality and self-image:
model of the effects of parent and peer support on adolescent Identify maintenance as identity diffusion. Sociology and Social
false self behavior. Child Development, 55, 19691982. Research, 61, 539559.
Harter, S., & McCarley, K. (2004, April). Is there a dark side to high Hur, Y., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (1998). The structure of self-
self-esteem leading to adolescent violence? Poster presented at the concept in female preadolescent twins: A behavior genetic
American Psychological Association Convention, Honolulu, HI. approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Harter, S., & Monsour, A. (1992). Developmental analysis of conf lict 10691077.
caused by opposing attributes in the adolescent self-portrait. De- James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. Chicago: Encyclopedia
velopmental Psychology, 28, 251260. Britannica.
Harter, S., & Pike, R. (1984). The pictorial scale of perceived compe- James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. New York:
tence and social acceptance for young children. Child Develop- Henry Holt.
ment, 55, 19691982. Jordan, J. V. (1991). The relational self: A new perspective for under-
Harter, S., Stocker, C., & Robinson, N. S. (1996). The perceived di- standing womens development. In J. Strauss & G. Goethals
rectionality of the link between approval and self-worth: The lia- (Eds.), The self: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 136149). New
bilities of a looking glass self orientation among young York: Springer-Verlag.
adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6, 285308. Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.
Harter, S., Waters, P., & Whitesell, N. R. (1998). Relational self- Keating, D. P. (1990). Adolescent thinking. In S. S. Feldman & G. El-
worth: Differences in perceived worth a person across interper- liot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent
sonal contexts. Child Development, 69, 756766. (pp. 5490). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Harter, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (1989). Developmental changes in chil- Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New
drens understanding of single, multiple and blended emotion York: Norton.
References 567

Kennedy, B. (1994). The development of self-understanding in adoles- Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against
cence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and
Cambridge, MA. Social Psychology, 52, 663676.
Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological nar- Lipka, R. P., Hurford, D. P., & Litten, M. J. (1992). Self-in school:
cissism. New York: Aronson. Age and school experience effects. In R. P. Lipka & T. M.
Kernis, M. H. (1993). The roles of stability and level of self-esteem Brinthaupt (Eds.), Self-perspectives across the life span (Vol.3,
in psychological functioning. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self- pp. 93115). Albany: State University of New York Press.
esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 167180). New York: Logan, R. D. (1987). Historical change in prevailing sense of self. In
Plenum Press. K. Yardley & T. Honess (Eds.), Self and identity: Psychological
Kernis, M. H., Cornell, D. P., Sun, C., Berry, A., & Harlow, T. perspectives (pp. 1326). Chichester, England: Wiley.
(1993). Theres more to self-esteem than whether it is high or Maccoby, E. (1980). Social development. New York: Wiley.
low: The importance of stability of self-esteem. Journal of Per- Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationship: A developmental ac-
sonality and Social Psychology, 65, 11901204. count. American Psychologist, 45, 513520.
Kiang, L., & Harter, S. (2004). Socialcultural values or appearance MacDonald, G., Saltzman, J. L., & Leary, M. (2003). Social approval
and attachment processes: An integrated model of eating disorder and trait self-esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 2340.
symptomatology. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (2001). The development of dis-
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1993). What does the self look like. In T. K. Srull & sociation in maltreated preschool-aged children. Development and
R. S. Wyer Jr. (Eds.), The mental representation of trait and auto- Psychopathology, 13, 233254.
biographical knowledge about the self: Vol. 5. Advances in social
Mack, J. E. (1983). Self-esteem and its development: An overview.
cognition (pp. 7990). New York: Academic Press.
In J. E. Mack & S. L. Ablong (Eds.), The development and sus-
Kilbourne, J. (1994). Still killing us softly: Advertising and the ob- taining of self-esteem (pp. 144). New York: International Uni-
session with thinness. In P. Fallon, M. Katzman, & S. Wooley versities Press.
(Eds.), Feminist perspectives on eating disorders (pp. 395418).
Maeda, K. (1997). [The self-perception profile for children adminis-
New York: Guilford Press.
tered to a Japanese sample]. Unpublished raw data, Ibaraki Pre-
Klein, H. A., OBryant, K., & Hopkins, H. R. (1996). Recalled fectural University of Health Sciences, Ibaraki, Japan.
parental authority style and self-perception in college men and
Mahler, M. S. (1967). On human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of in-
women. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 157, 517.
dividuation. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association,
Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). 15, 740763.
Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychologi-
Main, M. (1995). Recent studies in attachment: Overview with se-
cal Bulletin, 125, 470500.
lected implications for clinical work. In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, &
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York: International J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clin-
Universities Press. ical implications (pp. 407474). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Lamb, C. S., Jackson, L., Cassidy, P., & Priest, D. (1993). Body fig- Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as
ure preferences of men and women: A comparison of two genera- disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation.
tions. Gender Roles, 28, 345358. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & M. Cummings (Eds.), Attach-
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbush, S. M. ment during the preschool years: Theory, research, and interven-
(1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment among adoles- tion (pp. 121160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
cents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent and neglectful Makris-Botsaris, E., & Robinson, W. P. (1991). Harters self-
families. Child Development, 62, 10491065. perception profile for children: A cross-cultural validation in
Lapsley, D. K., & Rice, K. (1988). The new look at the imaginary Greece. Education and Research in Education, 5, 135143.
audience and personal fable: Toward a general model of adoles- Marcotte, D., Fortin, L., Potvin, P., & Papillon, M. (2002). Gender
cent ego development. In D. K. Lapsley & F. Power (Eds.), Self, differences in depressive symptoms during adolescence: Role of
ego, and identity: Integrative approaches (pp. 109129). New gender-typed characteristics, self-esteem, body image, stressful
York: Springer. life events, and pubertal status. Journal of Emotional and Behav-
Leahy, R. L., & Shirk, S. R. (1985). Social cognition and the develop- ioral Disorders, 10, 10634266.
ment of the self. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.), The development of the self Markus, H. (1980). The self in thought and memory. In D. M. Wegner
(pp. 123150). New York: Academic Press. & R. R. Vallacher (Eds.), The self in social psychology
Leaper, C. (1991). Inf luence and involvement in childrens discourse: (pp. 4269). New York: Oxford University Press.
Age, gender, and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797811. Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psycholo-
Leary, M. R., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Interpersonal functions of the gist, 41, 954969.
self-esteem motive: The self-esteem system as a sociometer. In Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social
M. H. Kernis (Ed.), Ef ficacy, agency, and self-esteem psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38,
(pp. 123140). New York: Plenum Press. 299337.
Lewis, M. (1994). Myself and me. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of
M L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: De- self-concept: Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of Edu-
velopmental perspectives (pp. 2034). New York: Cambridge Uni- cational Psychology, 81, 417430.
versity Press. Marsh, H. W. (1993). Academic self-concept: Theory, measurement,
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acqui- and research. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the
sition of self. New York: Plenum Press. self (Vol. 4, pp. 5998). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
568 The Self

Marsh, H. W., & Jackson, S. A. (1986). Multidimensional self- Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in devel-
concepts, masculinity, and femininity as a function of womens opment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
involvement in athletics. Sex Roles, 15, 391415. Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA:
Marsh, H. W., Parker, J., & Barnes, J. (1985). Multidimensional ado- Harvard University Press.
lescent self-concepts: Their relationship to age, sex, and aca- Nelson, K. (1990). Remembering, forgetting, and childhood amnesia.
demic measures. American Educational Research Journal, 22, In R. Fivush & J. A. Hudson (Eds.), Knowing and remembering
422444. in young children (pp. 301316). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Marsh, H. W., Smith, I. D., Marsh, M. R., & Owens, L. (1988). The sity Press.
transition from single-sex to co-educational high schools: Ef- Nelson, K. (1993). Events, narratives, memory: What develops. In
fects on multiple dimensions of self-concept and on academic C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol.
achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 25, 26. Memory and af fect (pp. 124). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
237269.
Nelson, K. (2003). Narrative and self, myth, and memory: Emergence
Mascolo, M., Fischer, K., & Neimeyer, R. (1999). The dynamic co- of a cultural self. In R. Fivush & C. A. Haden (Eds.), Autobio-
development of intentionality, self, and social relations. In graphical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Devel-
R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Action and self-development: Theory and opmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 7290). Mahwah, NJ:
research through the life span (pp. 7298). Thousand Oaks, CA: Erlbaum.
Sage.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gen-
Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transforma- der differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological
tions in appraisals for pride, shame, and guilt. In J. P. Tangney & Bulletin, 115, 424443.
K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of
Nottelmann, E. D. (1987). Competence and self-esteem during the
shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 64113). New York:
transition from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychol-
Guilford Press.
ogy, 23, 441450.
McCann, I. L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1992). Psychological trauma and
OMalley, P., & Bachman, J. (1983). Self-esteem: Change and stabil-
the adult survivor. New York: Brunner/ Mazel.
ity between 13 and 23. Developmental Psychology, 19, 257268.
McCarthy, J., & Hoge, D. (1982). Analysis of age effects in longitudi-
Oosterwegel, A., & Oppenheimer, L. (1993). The self-system: Devel-
nal studies of adolescent self-esteem. Developmental Psychology,
opmental changes between and within self-concepts. Hillsdale, NJ:
18, 372379.
Erlbaum.
McGregor, K. N., Mayleben, M. A., Buzzanga, V. L., Davis, S. F., &
Pedrabissi, L., & Santinello, M. (1992). Il Self-Perception profile for
Becker, A. H. (1992). Selected personality characteristics of
children di Susan Harter. Psicologia e Scuola, 61, 314.
first-generation college students. College Student Journal, 18,
231234. Pedrabissi, L., Santinello, M., & Scarpazza, V. (1988). Contributo al-
ladattamento italiano del self-perception profile for children di
McGuire, S., Manke, B., Saudino, K. J., Reiss, D., Hetherington,
Susan Harter. Bollettino di Psicologia Applicata, 185, 1926.
E. M., & Plomin, R. (1999). Perceived competence and self-worth
during adolescence: A longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Pennington, B. F. (2002). The development of psychopathology. New
Child Development, 70, 12831296. York: Guilford Press.
Mead, G. H. (1925). The genesis of the self and social control. Inter- Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Har-
national Journal of Ethics, 35, 251273. court, Brace & World.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a Piaget, J. (1960). The psychology of intelligence. Patterson, NJ: Lit-
social behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. tlefield-Adams.
Meredith, W. H., Abbott, D. A., & Zheng, F. M. (1991). Self- Pierrehumbert, B., Plancheral, B., & Jankech-Caretta, C. (1987).
concept and sociometric outcomes: A comparison of only chil- Image de soi et perception des compentences propres chez len-
dren and sibling children from urban and rural areas in the fant. Revue de Psycholgie Appliquee, 37, 359377.
Peoples Republic of China. Journal of Psychology, 126, Piers, E. V. (1976). The Piers-Harris Childrens Self-Concept Scale
411412. (Research Monograph No. 1). Nashville, TN: Counselor Record-
Miller, J. B. (1986). Toward a new psychology of women (2nd ed.). ings and Tests.
Boston: Beacon Press. Piers, E. V., & Harris, D. B. (1964). Age and other correlates of self-
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). concept in children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55,
Narrative practices and the social construction of self in child- 9195.
hood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292311. Pillemer, D. B., & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by
Montemayor, R., & Eisen, M. (1977). The development of self- children and adults. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child devel-
conceptions from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psy- opment and behavior (Vol. 21, pp. 297340). San Diego, CA: Aca-
chology, 23, 314319. demic Press.
Moretti, M. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1990). The development of self- Pipp, S. (1990). Sensorimotor and representational internal working
esteem vulnerabilities: Social and cognitive factors in develop- models of self, other, and relationship: Mechanisms of connection
mental psychopathology. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Kolligian Jr. and separation. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in
(Eds.), Competence considered (pp. 286314). New Haven, CT: transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 243264). Chicago: Univer-
Yale University Press. sity of Chicago Press.
Neiss, M. B., Sedikides, C., & Stevenson, J. (2002). Self-esteem: A Pomerantz, E. V., Ruble, D. N., Frey, K. S., & Greulich, F. (1995).
behavioral genetic perspective. European Journal of Personality, Meeting goals and confronting conf lict: Childrens changing per-
16, 351367. ceptions of social comparison. Child Development, 66, 723738.
References 569

Putnam, F. W. (1993). Dissociation and disturbances of the self. In Sarbin, T. R. (1962). A preface to a psychological analysis of the self.
D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Develop- Psychological Review, 59, 1122.
mental Psychopathology: Vol. 5. Disorders and dysfunctions of Savin-Williams, R. C., & Berndt, T. J. (1990). Friend and peer rela-
the self (pp. 251266). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester tions. In S. S. Feldman & G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The
Press. developing adolescent (pp. 277307). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Reissland, N. (1985). The development of concepts of simultaneity in University Press.
childrens understanding of emotions. Journal of Child Psychol- Savin-Williams, R. C., & Demo, P. (1993). Situational and transi-
ogy and Psychiatry, 26, 811824. tional determinants of adolescent self-feelings. Journal of Person-
Rhee, U. (1993). Self-perceptions of competence and social support ality and Social Psychology, 44, 820833.
in Korean children. Early Child Development and Care, 85, Schneider-Rosen, K., Braunwald, K. G., Carlson, V., & Cicchetti, D.
5766. (1985). Current perspectives in attachment theory: Illustration
Roberts, A., Seidman, E., & Pedersen, S. (2000). Perceived family from the study of maltreated infants. Monographs of the Society
and peer transactions and self-esteem among urban early adoles- for Research in Child Development, 50, 104210.
cents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 6892. Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding.
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Whos in the mirror? SelfOther New York: Academic Press.
discrimination in specular images by 4- and 9-month-old infants.
Shrauger, J. S., & Schoeneman, T. J. (1979). Symbolic interactionist
Child Development, 73, 3546.
view of self-concept: Through the looking glass darkly. Psycho-
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. New York: Oxford Uni- logical Bulletin, 86, 549573.
versity Press.
Siegler, R. S. (1991). Childrens thinking (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs,
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Self-concept from middle childhood through Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence: The
adolescence. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological impact of pubertal change and school context. New York: Aldine
perspective on the self (Vol. 3, pp. 107135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- de Gruyter.
baum.
Simmons, R. G., Blyth, D. A., Van Cleave, E. F., & Bush, D. (1979).
Rosenberg, M., Schoenback, C., Schooler, C., & Rosenberg, F. (1995). Entry into early adolescence: The impact of school structure, pu-
Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, berty, and early dating on self-esteem. American Sociological Re-
different outcomes. American Sociological Review, 60, 141156. view, 44, 553568.
Rosenberg, M., & Simmons, R. G. (1972). Black and White self- Simmons, R. G., & Rosenberg, F. (1975). Sex, sex roles, and self-
esteem: The urban school child. Washington, DC: American Psy- image. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 4, 229258.
chological Association.
Simmons, R. G., Rosenberg, F., & Rosenberg, M. (1973). Distur-
Rossman, B. B., & Rosenberg, M. S. (Eds.). (1998). Multiple victim- bances in the self-images at adolescence. American Sociological
ization of children. New York: Haworth Press. Review, 38, 553568.
Rowley, S. J., Sellers, R. M., Chavous, T. M., & Smith, M. A. (1998). Smollar, J., & Youniss, J. (1985). Adolescent self-concept develop-
The relationship between racial identity and self-esteem in ment. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.), The development of self (pp. 247266).
African college and high school students. Journal of Personality New York: Academic Press.
and Social Psychology, 74, 715724.
Snow, K. (1990). Building memories: The ontogeny of autobiography.
Rubin, L. (1985). Just friends: The role of friendship in our lives. New In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: In-
York: Harper. fancy to childhood (pp. 213242). Chicago: University of Chicago
Ruble, D. N., & Dweck, C. (1995). Self-conceptions, person concep- Press.
tion, and development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Review of personal- Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. In D.
ity and social psychology: Vol. 15. The interface (pp. 109139). Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. childhood (pp. 281308). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Ruble, D. N., & Frey, K. S. (1991). Changing patterns of comparative Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the construction
behavior as skills are acquired: A functional model of self- of relationships. In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships
evaluation. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Con- and development (pp. 5171). New York: Cambridge University
temporary theory and research (pp. 70112). Hillsdale, NJ: Press.
Erlbaum.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of affirmation: Sustaining the
Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of mental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261302). San Diego, CA:
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel- Academic Press.
opment (5th ed., pp. 9331016). New York: Wiley. Steinberg, L. (1990). Interdependency in the family: Autonomy, con-
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathol- f lict, autonomy in the parent-adolescent relationship. In S. Feld-
ogy: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathol- man & G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing
ogy, 12, 265296. adolescent (pp. 255276). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Sakurai, S. (1983). Development of the Japanese version of Harters Press.
Perceived Competence Scale for Children. Japanese Journal of Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. B. (1986). The vicissitudes of auton-
Educational Psychology, 31, 245249. omy in adolescence. Child Development, 57, 841851.
Sameroff, A. (2000). Development systems and psychopathology. De- Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York:
velopment and Psychopathology, 12, 297312. Basic Books.
570 The Self

Stigler, J. W., Smith, S., & Mao, L. (1985). The self-perception of Twenge, J. M., & Crocker, J. (2002). Race and self-esteem: Meta-
competence by Chinese children. Child Development, 56, analyses comparing Whites, Blacks, Hispanics, Asians, and
12591270. American Indians and comment on Gray-Little and Hafdahl
Stipek, D. (1981). Childrens perceptions of their own and their (2000). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 371408.
classmates ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, Usmiani, S., & Daniluk, J. (1997). Mothers and their adolescent
404410. daughters: Relationship between self-esteem, gender role iden-
Stipek, D. (1995). The development of pride and shame in toddlers. In tity, and body image. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26(1),
J. P. Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: 4562.
The psychology of shame, embarrassment, and pride van der Kolk, B. A. (1987). Psychological trauma. Washington, DC:
(pp. 237252). New York: Guilford Press. American Psychiatric Press.
Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in van Dongen-Melman, J. E., Koot, H. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (1993).
young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Cross-cultural validation of Harters self-perception profile in a
Development, 57, 184. Dutch sample. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53,
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New 739753.
York: Norton. Watson, M. (1990). Aspects of self development as ref lected in chil-
Suls, J., & Sanders, G. (1982). Self-evaluation via social comparison: drens role playing. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self
A developmental analysis. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personal- in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 281307). Chicago: Uni-
ity and social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 6789). Beverly Hills, CA: versity of Chicago Press.
Sage. Watson, M. W., & Fischer, K. (1993). Structural change in childrens
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1996). Self-traps. New York: Freeman. understanding of family roles and divorce. In R. R. Cocking &
Terr, L. (1990). Too scared to cry. New York: Basic Books. K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of psycho-
logical distance (pp. 123144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Terr, L. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. Ameri-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1020. Westen, D. (1993). The impact of sexual abuse on self structure. In
D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Develop-
Tessler, M. (1991). Making memories together: The inf luence of
mental Psychopathology: Vol. 5. Disorders and dysfunctions of the
mother-child joint encoding on the development of autobiographi-
self (pp. 223250). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
cal memory style. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City Univer-
sity of New York Graduate Center, New York. White, K., Speisman, J., & Costos, D. (1983). Young adults and their
parents: Individuation to mutuality. In H. D. Grotevant & C. R.
Thompson, R. (1998). Early sociopersonal development. In W.
Cooper (Eds.), New directions for child development: Adolescent
Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
development in the family (pp. 6176). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
opment (5th ed., pp. 25104). New York: Wiley. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Mac Iver, D., Reuman, D. A., & Midgley,
C. (1991). Transitions during early adolescence: Changes in chil-
Tobin-Richard, M. H., Boxer, A. M., Kavrell, S. A., & Peterson, A. C.
drens domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem
(1984). Puberty and its psychological and social significance. In
across the transition to junior high school. Developmental Psy-
R. M. Lerner & N. L. Galambos (Eds.), Experiencing adolescents:
chology, 27, 552565.
A sourcebook for parents, teachers, and teens (pp. 1750). New
York: Garland. Williams, J., M., & Currie, C. (2000). Self-esteem and physical de-
velopment in early adolescence: Pubertal timing and body image.
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., & Emde, R. N. (1997). Repre-
Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 129149.
sentations of self and others in the narratives of neglected, physi-
cally abused, and sexually abused preschoolers. Development and Winnicott, D. W. (1958). From paediatrics to psychoanalysis. Lon-
Psychopathology, 9, 781796. don: Hogarth Press.
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., Maughan, & Vanmeenen, K. Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitat-
(2000). Narrative representations of caregivers and self in male ing environment. New York: International Universities Press.
pre-schoolers. Attachment and Human Development, 2, 271305. Wolf, D. P. (1990). Being of several minds: Voices and version of the
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., Rogosch, F. A., & Maughan, A. self in early childhood. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The
(2000). Narrative representations of moral-affiliative and con- self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 183212). Chicago:
f lictual themes and behavioral problems in maltreated preschool- University of Chicago Press.
ers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 307318. Wolfe, D. (1989). Child abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Trent, L. M., Russell, G., & Cooney, G. (1994). Assessment of self- Wylie, R. C. (1979). The self concept: Vol. 2. Theory and research on
concept in early adolescence. Australian Journal of Psychology, 46, selected topics. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
2128. Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of self-concept. Lincoln: University of
Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M., & Robins, R. W. (2003). Stabil- Nebraska Press.
ity of self-esteem across the life span. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 205220.
CHAPTER 10

Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups


KENNETH H. RUBIN, WILLIAM M. BUKOWSKI, and JEFFREY G. PARKER

PEER INTERACTIONS AND RELATIONSHIPSAN Childrens Behaviors and Interactions with Peers 603
HISTORICAL OVERVIEW 573 Childrens Relationships with Friends 605
Early North American Research 573 Childrens Peer Acceptance 607
Post-World War II 574 Assessments of the Peer Group 607
INTERACTIONS, RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS: THE PROXIMAL CORRELATES OF CHILDRENS
ORDERS OF COMPLEXITY IN CHILDRENS PEER RELATIONSHIPS 608
PEER EXPERIENCES 575 Proximal CorrelatesPeer Acceptance 609
Interactions 576 Variations in the Behavioral Correlates of Popularity:
Relationships 577 Sex, Group, and Cultural Differences 611
Groups 578 Social Cognitive Correlates of Peer Acceptance
Culture 579 and Rejection 612
Summary 580 Self-System Correlates of Peer Acceptance
THEORIES RELEVANT TO THE STUDY OF and Rejection 615
CHILDRENS PEER INTERACTIONS, Childrens Friendships: Correlates and
RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS 581 Individual Differences 615
Personality Theorists 581 DISTAL PREDICTORS OF CHILDRENS SOCIAL
Cognitive Developmental Perspectives 582 SKILLS AND PEER RELATIONSHIPS 617
Learning and Social Learning Theories, Peer Temperament, Social Behaviors, and
Interaction, and Peer Relationships 584 Peer Relationships 617
Human Ethology 584 Parent-Child Attachment Relationships, Social
Group Socialization Theory 585 Behaviors, and Peer Relationships 619
PEER INTERACTIONS, RELATIONSHIPS, AND Parenting and Childrens Social Behaviors and
GROUPS: A DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE 586 Peer Relationships 621
Infancy and the Toddler Years 586 CHILDHOOD PEER EXPERIENCES AND
Early Childhood 589 LATER ADJUSTMENT 625
Middle Childhood and Early Adolescence 592 Academic Adjustment 625
Adolescence 599 Psychological Adjustment 627
Conclusion 602 CONCLUSIONS 629
SOCIAL BEHAVIORS, INTERACTIONS, Three Critical Challenges 630
RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS: Three Questions in Search of Answers 631
ASSESSMENT ISSUES 602 REFERENCES 631

Experiences with peers constitute an important devel- of family, school, and neighborhood. In this chapter, we
opmental context for children. In these contexts, chil- present the current research related to these claims. We
dren acquire a wide range of behaviors, skills, attitudes, begin by commenting briefly on developments in the
and experiences that influence their adaptation during study of childrens peers since the publication of the
the life span. Experiences with peers affect social, emo- last Handbook of Child Psychology in 1998. Our previ-
tional, and cognitive functioning beyond the influences ous chapter distinguished between processes and effects

The writing of this manuscript was supported, in part, by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada
a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health and the Fonds Qubcois de la recherche sur la socit et la
(# MH58116) to Kenneth H. Rubin and by grants from the culture to William M. Bukowski.

571
572 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

at the levels of the interactions, relationships, and research and public attention, but the study of the peer
groups. Our goal is to provide an updated examination system has also become increasingly diverse, more ar-
of current theory and research on peer relationships and ticulated, and more naturalistic. Its diversity is seen
development. not only in the wide range of topics that are studied but
The task of reviewing the literature on peer interac- also in the participation of children from cultures other
tions, relationships, and groups becomes more challeng- than those typically found in Western research. Topics
ing as the literature becomes more extensive and recently introduced to the discipline include the signif-
diverse. The number of relevant papers published in the icance of peers and friendships as children mark tran-
past 8 years is substantially larger than the number pub- sitions from one school setting to another; cultural and
lished in any previous 8-year period. Since the publica- cross-cultural meanings of acceptable and unaccept-
tion of our 1998 chapter, several major books have able social behaviors and relationships; perceived pop-
appeared, including: ularity; jealousy and other emotional processes related
to the maintenance and dissolution of peer relation-
Childrens Peer Relations (Slee & Rigby, 1998) ships; the statistical (and conceptual) modeling of
Sociometry Then and Now: Building on 6 Decades of growth and change in peer interactions and relation-
Measuring Experiences with the Peer Group ships; peer victimization; mutual antipathies; early ro-
(Bukowski & Cillessen, 1996) mantic relationships; and the relative contribution of
Family and Peers: Linking Two Social Worlds (Kerns, peers and friendships to well- and ill-being.
Contreras, & Neal-Barnett, 2000) Prior emphases on rejection as necessarily bad and
The Role of Friendship in Psychological Adjustment friendship as necessarily good have been replaced by
(Nangle & Erdley, 2001) models that emphasize how sets of variables function to-
Peer Harassment in School: The Plight of the Vulner- gether via mediation and moderation to affect outcome.
able and Victimized (Juvonen & Graham, 2001) The past decade has seen several changes in peer re-
search, including an increase of interest in victimiza-
How Children and Adolescents Evaluate Gender and
tion, a decrease in interest in sociometric rejection,
Racial Exclusion (Killen, Lee-Kim, McGlothlin, &
increases in the emphasis on biology and emotion, a con-
Stangor, 2002)
cern with the peer group per se, and an interest in devel-
Peer Rejection: Developmental Processes and Inter- oping process-oriented explanatory models to account
vention Strategies (Bierman, 2003) for the factors underlying risk.
Enemies and the Darker Side of Peer Relations In parallel to these changes, research on peer interac-
(Hodges & Card, 2003) Childrens Friendships: The tions, relationships, and groups has focused on an in-
Beginnings of Intimacy (Dunn, 2004) creasingly articulated set of measures. Although the use
Childrens Peer Relations: From Development to In- of omnibus measures of aggression, withdrawal, socia-
tervention (Kupersmidt & Dodge, 2004) bility, sociometric status, and victimization continues,
the use of more specific measures, drawn from more
Perhaps even more important, a number of trade books refined thinking about individual characteristics and so-
on peer relationships are now available for parents and cial interactions, has increased. Now, for example, ag-
teachers, including: gression is measured according to whether it is direct,
indirect, relational, physical, reactive, or proactive;
Queen Bees and Wannabees: Helping Your Daughter withdrawal is measured to the extent that it reflects so-
Survive Cliques, Gossip, Boyfriends, and Other Reali- cial reticence, social immaturity, preference for objects
ties of Adolescence (Wiseman, 2002) rather than people, or social exclusion. Research designs
The Unwritten Rules of Friendship: Simple Strategies have changed also. The once prevalent preference for
to Help Your Child Make Friends (Elman & Kennedy- one-time-only studies has been coupled with the more
Moore, 2003) frequent use of longitudinal designs that allow an exam-
The Friendship Factor (Rubin, 2003) ination of prediction as well as intra-individual change.
Thus, the literature on peer interactions, relationships,
Not only have the topics of childrens peer interac- and groups continues to evolve toward higher levels of
tions, relationships, and groups experienced increased complexity and specificity.
Peer Interactions and RelationshipsAn Historical Overview 573

In the first section of this chapter, we pay homage to were published in German. Among these early German
those researchers who established areas of investigation studies were developmental examinations of social inter-
that are still active today. Next, we suggest that the peer action in infants and toddlers; studies of antisocial ten-
system consists of multiple levels of analysis, namely in- dencies in children and adolescents; investigations of
dividual characteristics, social interactions, dyadic rela- the evolution of different leadership roles played by chil-
tionships, and group membership and composition. Our dren in their peer groups, and observational studies of
thesis is that interactions, relationships, and groups re- the development of friendship networks. Thus, prior to
flect social participation at different interwoven orders World War II, German laboratories were producing re-
of complexity. Our goal, in introducing these levels of search on topics not unknown to contemporaneous peer
analysis, is to establish a framework for further discus- relationships researchers. Often, the correlates or con-
sion of the development and significance of childrens comitants of these relationship variables were examined,
peer experiences. Moreover, discussion of the inter- such as family constellation, institutionalization, and
action, relationships, and group levels of social com- poverty. We continue to grapple with these topics today.
plexity allows subsequent commentary on conceptual
and assessment issues that pertain to individual differ-
Early North American Research
ences in childrens behavioral tendencies and peer rela-
tionships. These different levels of analysis receive North American research concerning childrens peer in-
different amounts of treatment in the theoretical ac- teractions and relationships began to blossom in the
counts of the significance of peer experiences for nor- 1920s when the first Child Welfare Research Stations
mal development. These issues are discussed in the next came into existence. These interdisciplinary research
section where we present theories relevant to the under- centers produced new observational and statistical pro-
standing of the peer system. cedures to examine developmental and individual differ-
Next, we describe normative patterns of development ences in childrens social behaviors, interactions, and
from infancy through late childhood and early adoles- peer relationships. Research reports from these centers
cence. Researchers who study childrens peer experi- emphasized the development of social participation
ences have long maintained a healthy interest in (Parten, 1932); assertiveness (Dawe, 1934); sympa-
measurement and measurement issues. In the fifth sec- thetic and altruistic behaviors (L. Murphy, 1937);
tion, we distinguish between individuals, interactions, conflict and aggression (Maudry & Nekula, 1939); lead-
relationships, and groups in a discussion of measure- ership, dominance, and ascendant behavior (Hanfmann,
ment issues. In the final sections, we update the volumi- 1935); friendship (Challman, 1932); group dynamics
nous literature that has emerged concerning the origins (K. Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939); and peer group
and consequences of individual differences in chil- structure and composition (Moreno, 1934).
drens experiences with peers. We pay particular atten- By the beginning of World War II, the study of chil-
tion to the proximal and distal correlates of variables drens social behaviors, interactions, and peer rela-
associated with individual differences in popularity tionships began to wane as many persons who did develop-
and friendship. We consider also the developmental mental research had joined the war effort. Nevertheless,
prognosis for children whose peer interaction patterns during this period, increased attention was directed to
and relationships are deviant from the norm. The chap- topics relevant to group processes and democratic values.
ter concludes with a discussion of some of the direc- Interest in these topics had been heightened by the so-
tions that future research might take. ciopolitical events associated with the war and led to re-
search on the interface between individual characteristics
PEER INTERACTIONS AND (e.g., leadership), interactions between group members,
RELATIONSHIPSAN HISTORICAL and group dynamics. For example, one line of research
OVERVIEW was concerned with factors that might evoke and maintain
intra- and intergroup harmony and conflict. It is not a co-
The study of childrens peer interactions and relation- incidence that one of the conditions of the K. Lewin
ships has had a long and rich history. Charlotte Buhler et al.s (1939) classic study of the effect of leadership on
(1931), in the first Handbook chapter on peer interac- peer group processes was labeled democratic, whereas
tions and relationships, cited 253 papers, 156 of which another was labeled authoritarian.
574 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

A second area of research that flourished during this early education programs for which the primary foci
time of concern with group composition and processes were cognitive and language development and the devel-
was sociometry. Following the work of Moreno (1934), opment of an achievement orientation in young children.
the war period is noted for the further development of Accordingly, nursery schools moved away from empha-
sociometric techniques that provided researchers with a sizing the development of relationships and social skills
means of studying acceptance and rejection (see Bron- and instead aimed to prepare at-risk children for ele-
fenbrenner, 1944). These techniques were immediately mentary school. Additionally, the achievement-oriented
used to study a variety of questions concerning the cor- middle classes of the 1960s and 1970s increasingly fa-
relates of childrens experiences with peers. Publica- vored cognitively oriented preschool programs.
tions by Northway (1944) and Bonney (1944) serve as Despite the emphasis on early cognitive and language
historical exemplars for current researchers interested in development, the preschool and day-care movements of
the factors related to childrens experiences in groups. the 1960s and 1970s may have been partly responsible
for the reemergence of peer relationships research. In
PostWorld War II particular, the growth of early education and care cen-
ters in North America was dictated, not only by the need
The arrival of the Cold War fostered limited research to prevent educational failure among the socioeconomi-
concern about children and their extrafamilial social re- cally impoverished, but also by the need for out-of-home
lationships. Instead, attention was directed to childrens care for dual income middle-class households. Given
academic and intellectual prowess. With the launching that North American children were entering organized
of the Sputnik satellite by the USSR in 1957, the pres- peer group settings at earlier ages than ever before and
sures to train children to become academically oriented given that children were remaining with peers in age-
and skilled at earlier ages and at faster rates than ever segregated schools for more years than their cohorts of
before moved developmental researchers away from the previous generations, it would have been shortsighted
earlier focus on childrens social worlds. and irresponsible for developmental researchers to ig-
In the 1960s, the rediscovery of the Piagets develop- nore the importance of childrens peer relationships and
mental theory provided an impetus for a structurally ori- social skills.
ented research climate that captured the interest of The current theories about the significance of peer
psychologists throughout the Western world. A brief interactions and relationships for normal development
glance at archival child psychology and development are certainly not new. Piaget (1932) himself implicated
journals during the 1960s and 1970s will reveal the dom- peer interaction, discourse, and negotiation as crucial
ination of the Piagetian Zeitgeist, in conjunction with, or elements likely to provoke higher levels of operational
in opposition to, the behaviorist Zeitgeist. This focus on thinking. Mead (1934) and Sullivan (1953) also wrote
cognition, coupled with continued interest in achieve- persuasively about the importance of friendship and
ment motivation and behavior, created an environment peer relationships for adaptive development. Thus, by
that was not particularly attuned to the significance of the end of the 1960s, the time appeared ripe for child de-
peer interaction and relationships. Nevertheless, re- velopmentalists to be reminded of their early roots. This
searchers in the 1960s and early 1970s appeared to ac- reminder was issued by Hartup in his 1970 Manual of
cept the premise that young children were egocentric and Child Psychology chapter. This chapter, and Hartups
were neither willing, nor able, to understand the (1983) revision, proved provocative. In 1998, we pro-
thoughts, feelings, and spatial perspectives of their vided the Handbook with an updated look at the litera-
peers. Egocentrism also stood in the way of making ma- ture on peer interactions, relationships, and groups.
ture moral judgments and decisions. Given these assump- The large amount of research attention directed to the
tions, the mind-set seemed to be that studying childrens study of childrens peer interactions, relationships, and
peer relationships would not be productive, at least until groups in the 1980s occurred at the same time that a new
the mid-elementary school ages when concrete opera- approach to understanding the development of psycho-
tions emerged and when egocentric thought vanished. pathology was proposed. A basic tenet of the approach
The coupling of this research and educational climate was that the study of normative development and indi-
with the social policy mandate of the mid-1960s regard- vidual differences and the study of psychopathology
ing the eradication of poverty, led to the development of were mutually enriching activities (Sroufe & Rutter,
Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Orders of Complexity in Childrens Peer Experiences 575

1984). The study of peer interactions and relationships aroused physiologically to social stimuli, and a reper-
was ideally suited to the field of developmental psycho- toire of social skills for social perception, cognition,
pathology. Researchers recognized that theories, con- and social problem solving. Over the short term, their in-
structs, variables and measures of peer interactions, and teractions with other children vary in form and function
relationships were valuable and useful for the study of in response to fluctuations in the parameters of the so-
normal development and for the study of maladjustment. cial situation, such as the partners characteristics, over-
The result of this confluence has been that the study of tures, and responses. Further, most interactions are
peer interactions, relationships, and groups and the embedded in longer-term relationships and thus are in-
study of the development of psychopathology have be- fluenced by past and anticipated future interactions. Re-
come highly complementary activities (e.g., Deater- lationships may take many forms and have properties
Deckard, 2001). On the one hand, childrens problems that are not relevant to interactions. At the same time,
with peers, regardless of their source, may contribute to the nature of a relationship is defined partly by the char-
the genesis of behavioral or emotional disorder; on the acteristics of its members, its constituent interactions,
other hand, children with behavioral and emotional dif- and, over the long term, the kinds of relationships indi-
ficulties may be rejected and/or victimized by their viduals form depend on their history of interactions in
peers from the earliest years of life (Hay, Payne, & earlier relationships. Finally, individual relationships
Chadwick, 2004). A prominent example of the liaison are embedded in groups or networks of relationships
between the study of peers and the study of maladjust- with more or less clearly defined boundaries (e.g.,
ment can be seen in current research on bullying and cliques, teams, or school classes). As the highest level of
victimization. Following extreme incidents in schools social complexity, groups are defined by their con-
and among youth in groups, investigators became in- stituent relationships and, in this sense, by the types and
creasingly interested in identifying the complex interac- diversity of interactions that are characteristic of the
tions between individual and group factors that account participants in those relationships. But groups are more
for the harm that peers can inflict on each other. than mere aggregates of relationships; through emergent
In summary, the study of peer interactions, relation- properties, such as norms or shared cultural conven-
ships, and groups has a long and rich history. The topics tions, groups help define the type and range of relation-
that have attracted the attention of peer researchers have ships and interactions that are likely or permissible.
varied in response to intellectual Zeitgeists, advances in Further, groups have properties and processes, such as
theory and research in other domains of developmental hierarchical organization and cohesiveness, which are
psychology, and to social and political events. Currently, not relevant to description of childrens experiences at
peer research balances concerns with the study of indi- lower levels of social complexity.
vidual differences with the study of basic processes. To further complicate matters, at any level of social
The features of this balance are evident in the sections organization the understanding of participants will nec-
that follow. essarily differ from that of outsiders. Humorous anec-
dotes shared between friends, for example, can strike
outsiders as unnecessarily cruel (e.g., gossip). Children
with many friends can still feel lonely; and seemingly
INTERACTIONS, RELATIONSHIPS, AND innocuous acts can have great significance to members
GROUPS: ORDERS OF COMPLEXITY IN of a friendship, who understand them differently than
CHILDRENS PEER EXPERIENCES do outsiders. Given that neither insiders nor outsiders
can claim any specific hegemony on the truth, re-
Childrens experiences with peers can be best under- searchers must be prepared to cross and re-cross per-
stood by referring to several levels of social complex- spectives as the problem dictates.
ityin individuals, in interactions, in relationships, and The complexity of the multiple, interrelated levels of
in groups (Hinde, 1987). Moreover, events and social organization that underpin peer experiences can
processes at each level are constrained and influenced make the prospect of understanding these experiences
by events and processes at other levels. Individuals bring and their influence on children seem truly dim. Histori-
to social exchanges more or less stable social orienta- cally, distinctions between the various levels and per-
tions, temperaments that dispose them to be more or less spectives of childrens peer experiences often have been
576 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

blurred. For example, investigators have confused phe- ordinarily not be considered true interaction unless it
nomena from different levels (e.g., failing to distinguish was amply clear that they were jointly undertaken. In-
between group acceptance and friendship) or perspec- stead, the term interaction is reserved for dyadic behav-
tives (e.g., accepting one childs declaration as evidence ior in which the participants actions are interdependent
of friendship without verifying the reciprocity of this such that each actors behavior is both a response to, and
sentiment), and have also sometimes been too facile in stimulus for, the other participants behavior. At its core,
making inferences about experiences at one level from an interaction comprises such incidents as Individual A
measurements at another (e.g., assuming that children shows behavior X to Individual B, or A shows X to B and
who are aggressive in interaction cannot be well-liked or B responds with Y (Hinde, 1979, p. 15). Conversational
those who are socially removed and withdrawn from in- turn-taking is a quintessential illustration: Thus, Child A
teraction cannot have friends). Nevertheless, over the requests information from Child B (Whats your
past 25 years, recognition and articulation of the multi- name?), Child B responds (My name is Lara. Whats
ple levels of analysis and perspectives that comprise the yours?), Child A replies (Camilla.), and so on.
peer system have greatly increased. Especially signifi- Such a simple exchange as that of Camilla and Lara
cant in this regard has been the contribution of Robert belies the richness and complexity of the ways that chil-
Hinde (e.g., 1987, 1995) who has articulated the fea- dren of most ages communicate with and influence one
tures and dialectical relations between successive levels another. Thus, besides introducing themselves, children
of social complexity. in conversation may argue, gossip, comfort, and support
Borrowing heavily from Hinde, in this section we dis- one another, self-disclose, and joke, among other things.
cuss the nature of three successive levels of complexity And, during interaction, children cooperate, compete,
in childrens experiences with peersinteractions, rela- fight, withdraw, respond to provocation, and engage in a
tionships, and groups. Our goal is to set the framework host of other behaviors that includes everything from
for subsequent discussion of the development and signif- ritualized sexual contact to rough-and-tumble (R&T)
icance of childrens peer experiences. The interaction, play to highly structured sociodramatic fantasy. Typi-
relationship, and group levels of social complexity are cally, researchers have been less interested in catalogu-
also important to the conceptualization and assessment ing the myriad of interactional experiences than in
of individual differences in childrens behavioral tenden- understanding the origins and consequences of three
cies because individuals can be compared with respect to broad childhood behavioral tendencies: (1) moving to-
their functioning at these levels; therefore, the present ward others, (2) moving against others, and (3) moving
section serves as an orienting framework for our later away from others. As a consequence, our understanding
discussion of measurement issues. As we indicated, a hi- of childrens experiences at the interactional level is dis-
erarchy of social complexity should include processes at proportionately organized around the constructs of so-
work at the individual (versus interactional, relationship, ciability and helpfulness, aggression, and withdrawal.
or group) level of description. These processes would in- As much of this literature is oriented toward individual
clude childrens socioemotional /temperamental disposi- differences among children along these dimensions of
tions, and social knowledge and skills repertoires. In the interaction, we review this research in later sections.
literature on childrens peer experiences, the individual Developmental trends in these behaviors are described
level has been the focus of much interest. However, in the subsequent section.
rather than introduce this well-developed literature here, Although many social exchanges have their own in-
we embed its discussion into sections on childrens inter- herent logic (as in the question-answer sequence of
actions and relationships. Camilla and Lara), it is also the case that the forms and
trajectories of episodes of interaction are shaped by the
Interactions relationships in which they are embedded. For example,
friends are more committed to resolving conflict with
The simplest order of complexity of peer experience in- each other than nonfriends, are more likely than non-
volves interactions. Interaction refers to the social ex- friends to reach equitable resolutions, and continue to
change of some duration between two individuals. interact following a disagreement (Laursen, 1993;
Behaviors that simply (and only) complement one an- Laursen, Finkelstein, & Betts, 2001; Laursen, Hartup,
other ( like riding on either end of a teeter-totter) would & Koplas, 1996; Laursen & Koplas, 1995; Newcomb &
Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Orders of Complexity in Childrens Peer Experiences 577

Bagwell, 1995). Beyond this, children engaged in inter- family, a class, or a team. In a particularly insightful
action vary their behavior as a function of their short- analysis, McCall (1988) noted that dyads, like larger or-
term and long-term personal goals, their understanding ganizational structures, undergo role differentiation,
of their partners thoughts and feelings in the situation, specialization, and division of labor: Members lines
the depth of their repertoire of alternative responses, of action differ one from the other yet remain interde-
and various ecological features of the context of the pendent in certain ways (p. 473). Moreover, partici-
interactions (such as the presence of bystanders), the pants in a relationship are aware that their relationship,
physical setting, their own and their partners relative though it may be very much their own local creation, is
standing in the group, and the operative local customs supported by an objectified, institutionalized social
or scripts for responding. It is precisely the demon- form: When persons say they are friends, usually they
stration of such range and flexibility in responding to can point to cultural images, rules of conduct, and cus-
the challenges of interpersonal interaction, when consid- tomary modes of behavior to confirm their claims (Sut-
ered at the individual level of analysis that many writers tles, 1970, p. 98). In addition, parties to a relationship
think of as social competence (e.g., Bukowski, Rubin, & have a sense of shared membership and belonging: A
Parker, 2001; Rose-Krasnor, 1997). sense of shared fate tends to arise as members discover
that the surrounding world treats them not so much as
Relationships separate individuals but rather as a couple, or unit
(McCall, 1988, p. 471). Finally, the creation of a shared
Relationships introduce a second and higher-order level culture is a vital part of dyadic relationships. This
of complexity to childrens experiences with peers. Re- shared culture includes normative expectations regard-
lationships refer to the meanings, expectations, and ing appropriate activities, patterns of communication
emotions that derive from a succession of interactions and revelation, relations to external persons and organi-
between two individuals known to each other. Because zations, and so on. It also includes private terms, or ne-
the individuals are known to each other, the nature and ologisms, for shared concerns or common activities, and
course of each interaction is influenced by the history of rituals, or dyadic traditions, arising from the rou-
past interactions between the individuals as well as by tinization of recurrent dyadic activities (such as meet-
their expectations for interactions in the future. It has ing at the same place after school, flipping a special coin
been suggested that the degree of closeness of a relation- to resolve a dispute, or engaging in an exclusive buddy
ship is determined by such qualities as the frequency shake to mark a joint promise or planned behavior).
and strength of influence, the diversity of influence These are all features that relationships have in com-
across different behaviors, and the length of time the re- mon with other, larger social organizations. However,
lationship has endured. In a close relationship, influence McCall indicates that there are certain attitudinal fea-
is frequent, diverse, strong, and enduring. Alternatively, tures of the participants in a dyadic relationship that are
relationships can be defined with reference to the pre- distinct to this level of social organization and vital to
dominant emotions that participants typically experi- understanding its functioning and impact on interac-
ence in them (e.g., affection, love, attachment, or tions and individuals. For example, unlike most social
enmity). Hinde (1979) further suggests that an essential organizations, dyadic relationships do not vary in mem-
element of a relationship is commitment or the extent to bership size. Having only two members, the dyad is
which the partners accept their relationship as continu- peculiarly vulnerable, for the loss of a single member
ing indefinitely or direct their behaviors toward ensur- terminates the dyads existence. Because members
ing its continuance or toward optimizing its properties appreciate this vulnerability, issues of commitment, at-
(p. 29). Finally, it is important to note that, although as tachment, and investment loom larger in dyadic relation-
social scientists we may speak of abstract categories of ships than in other forms of social organization. Indeed,
relationships (e.g., sibling, best friend, or enemy), chil- an understanding of the surface behavior of members of
dren view each instance of these relationships in a par- relationships can be elusive unless note is taken of the
ticularized way; to children, relationships of even the deeper meaning of behavior in relation to the relation-
same general category are not interchangeable. ships mortality. This same sense of mortality is likely
As a form of social organization, dyadic relationships to contribute to a special sense of uniqueness ( there
share features with larger social organizations, such as a has never been a friendship quite like ours) and to what
578 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

McCall calls a sense of consecration, or a feeling that Until recently, the study of childrens dyadic rela-
each member must take responsibility for what happens tionships with peers was focused almost exclusively on
in the relationship. the study of friendship. Researchers are now turning to
the study of mutual antipathies and enmities (e.g.,
Friendship
Abecassis, Hartup, Haselager, Scholte, & van Lieshout,
In the literature on childrens peer experiences, one 2002; Hodges & Card, 2003). Whereas the topic of dis-
form of dyadic relationship has received attention above liking is certainly not new (e.g., Hayes, Gershman, &
all othersfriendship. The issue of what constitutes Bolin, 1980), the emphasis of recent research has been
friendship is a venerable philosophical debate beyond on the frequency of mutual antipathies, their correlates,
the scope of this chapter. However, some points from and their developmental significance.
this debate warrant noting here because of their opera- A final point is that relationships must be understood
tional significance. according to their place in the network of other relation-
First, there is widespread agreement that friendship ships. For example, childrens friendships are influ-
is a reciprocal relationship that must be affirmed or rec- enced by the relationships they have at home with
ognized by both parties. Reciprocity is the factor that parents and siblings. Childrens conceptualizations and
distinguishes friendship from the nonreciprocal attrac- feelings about their primary relationships are internal-
tion of only one partner to another. From an assessment ized and lead to (a) expectations about what relation-
perspective, methods that do not verify that the percep- ships outside of the family might and should be like, and
tion of friendship is shared between partners prove dif- ( b) particular interpersonal behaviors and interactions
ficult because children are sometimes motivated by with peers that reflect their internalized models of rela-
self-presentational goals to designate as friends other tionships (Belsky & Cassidy, 1995). Whereas parent-
children who do not view them as friends in return. child relationships may influence the early development
Thus, in the absence of assessing reciprocity, methods and maintenance of childrens peer relationships, it
of identifying friends may confuse desired relationships would make sense to expect that the relations between
with actual ones. relationship systems become increasingly reciprocal and
A second point of consensus is that reciprocity of af- mutual with increasing child age: The quality of the
fection represents an essential, though not necessarily childs peer relationships is likely to influence the qual-
exclusive, tie that binds friends together (Hays, 1988). ity of the parent-child relationship and perhaps even the
The interdependence of the two partners derives primar- relationship between the childs parents.
ily from socioemotional rather than instrumental mo-
tives. It is customary for children to seek one another
Groups
out for instrumental reasons. Similarity of talents or in-
terests may bring together children who might not other- A group is a collection of interacting individuals who
wise interact. For example, work and sports teams, have some degree of reciprocal influence over one an-
musical groups, and even delinquent gangs include mem- other. Groups can be formed spontaneously, out of com-
bers who are not necessarily friends. Similarities or mon interests or circumstances, or due to formal external
complementarities of talents and interests may lead to structures (e.g., groups of students organized into classes
friendship and can help sustain them; however, they do in school). Hinde (1979) suggests that a group is the
not constitute the basis of the friendship itself. The basis structure that emerges from the features and patterning
is reciprocal affection. of the relationships and interactions present in a popula-
Third, friendships are voluntary, not obligatory or tion of children. Accordingly, groups possess properties
prescribed. In some cultures and in some circumstances, that arise from the manner in which the relationships are
children may be assigned their friends, sometimes patterned but are not present in the individual relation-
even at birth (Krappmann, 1996). Although these rela- ships themselves. Examples of such properties include
tionships may take on some of the features and serve cohesiveness, or the degree of unity and inclusiveness ex-
some of the same interpersonal ends as voluntary rela- hibited by the children or manifest by the density of the
tionships, most scholars would agree that their in- interpersonal relationships; hierarchy, or the extent of in-
voluntary nature argues against confusing them with transitivity in the ordering of the individual relationships
friendship. along interesting dimensions (e.g., If Fred dominates
Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Orders of Complexity in Childrens Peer Experiences 579

Brian and Brian dominates Peter, does Fred dominate structure and organization. However, recently a number
Peter?); and homogeneity or homophily, or consistency of complex statistical procedures have allowed the study
across members in the ascribed or achieved personal of peer groups and peer group effects on children.
characteristics (e.g., sex, race, age, intelligence, or atti- Finally, it is worth noting that the construct that has
tudes toward school). Finally, every group has norms, or dominated the peer literature during the past 25 years,
distinctive patterns of behaviors and attitudes that char- namely that of popularity, is both an individual- and a
acterize group members and differentiate them from group-oriented phenomenon. Measures of popularity
members of other groups. refer to the groups view of an individual in relation to
Many of our most important means for describing the dimensions of liking and disliking (Bukowski &
groups speak to these core characteristics or processes. Hoza, 1989; Bukowski, Sippola, Hoza, & Newcomb,
Thus, researchers may address the degree to which the 2000; Parker, Saxon, Asher, & Kovacs, 1999). In this re-
relationships and interactions in a group are segregated gard, popularity is a group construct and the processes
along sex or racial lines (e.g., Killen, Crystal, & Watan- of rejection and acceptance are group processes. Yet,
abe, 2002; Killen, Lee-Kim, McGlothlin, & Strangor, despite this reality, most peer researchers treat popular-
2002); they may compare the rates of social isolation ity as characteristic of the individual (Asher, Parker, &
among groups that differ in composition; or they may Walker, 1996; Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993).
investigate the extent to which a groups hierarchies of This confusion exemplifies the significance of recogniz-
affiliation, dominance, and influence are linear and in- ing the inextricable links between different levels of
terrelated. In addition, group norms can be used as a analysis. As Bronfenbrenner (1944) wrote over 50 years
basis for distinguishing separate crowds in the net- ago, the study of the peer system requires the envisage-
works of relationships among children in high school ment of the individual and the group as developing or-
(e.g., Brown, 1989). The emergent properties of groups ganic units (p. 75).
also shape the experiences of individuals in the groups
(e.g., Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003). Thus, crowd la-
Culture
bels constrain, in important ways, adolescents freedom
to explore new identities; status hierarchies influence It is important to recognize that each of the social levels
the formation of new friendships; segregation influ- described earlier falls under the all-reaching umbrella of
ences the diversity of childrens experiences with oth- the cultural macrosystem (e.g., Bronfenbrenner &
ers; and cohesiveness influences childrens sense of Crouter, 1983). By culture is meant the set of attitudes,
belonging. As such, the group can influence the individ- values, beliefs, and behaviors shared by a group of peo-
ual. Indeed, many of the classic developmental studies ple, communicated from one generation to the next
concerned the peer group per se, including that of K. (Matsumoto, 1997, p. 5). Cultural beliefs and norms
Lewin et al. (1939) concerning group climate, and help interpret the acceptability of individual character-
Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif s (1961) ex- istics and the types and ranges of interactions and rela-
amination of intragroup loyalty and intergroup conflict. tionships that are likely or permissible.
In addition, theorists stressing the importance of chil- As it happens, the cultural and cross-cultural study of
drens peer experiences (e.g., Cairns, Xie, & Leung, childrens peer interactions, relationships, and groups
1996; Xie, Cairns, & Cairns, 1999, 2001) have gener- has a brief history. A central question asked in this body
ally conceptualized the group as an important develop- of work is rather intriguing: Do the meanings and sig-
mental context that shapes and supports the behaviors nificance of given social behaviors or social relation-
of its constituent members. ships differ from culture to culture, or are there cultural
In spite of the importance of the group, there has universals in interpreting given social behaviors and re-
been, until recently, little attention paid to the assess- lationships? For example, is social competence defined
ment of group phenomena (see Bukowski & Sippola, in a similar fashion across cultures? And what about ag-
2001). This is surprising because researchers often cite gression or socially wariness? Are these behaviors simi-
experiences with peers with reference to the peer larly defined and interpreted from culture to culture?
group. Cairns et al. (1996) argued that this neglect Are childrens friendships conceptualized in similar
could be attributed to the complex conceptual and ways across culture? Are such relationships viewed as
methodological issues related to the study of group similarly significant from culture to culture? These are
580 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

but a few questions that are only now being examined by searchers have not always demonstrated a clear under-
researchers the world over. standing of the important ways in which processes at
Given that the majority of the worlds inhabitants do one level are influenced by those at the others. They
not reside in culturally Westernized countries, the cross- have sometimes overlooked ways in which conclusions
cultural work on peer interactions, relationships, and drawn at single levels of analysis can be limited. For ex-
groups requires careful note: Child development is influ- ample, the observation of two children at play can reveal
enced by many factors. In any culture, children are the rates at which they display different behaviors and
shaped by the physical and social settings in which they the patterning of these behaviors with respect to one an-
live as well as culturally regulated customs, childrearing other. It can be misleading, however, to attribute these
practices, and culturally based belief systems (Harkness characteristics of interaction solely to individual differ-
& Super, 2002). The bottom line is that the psychologi- ences in social competence or temperament; one must
cal meaning attributed to any given social behavior is, also consider relational interdependenciesunique ad-
in large part, a function of the ecological niche in which justments made by Person A and Person B to one an-
it is produced. If a given behavior is viewed as accept- other that define their particular relationship. And
able, then parents (and significant others) will attempt events transpiring in a given relationship also reflect re-
to encourage its development; if the behavior is per- alities outside the relationship; for example, tensions
ceived as maladaptive or abnormal, then parents (and produced by individuals loyalties to other friends in the
significant others) will attempt to discourage its growth peer group may affect the quality of social interaction
and development. And the very means by which people between two specific children.
go about encouraging or discouraging the given behavior Until recently, studying individual, dyadic, and group
may be culturally determined and defined. Thus, in measures was challenging, both conceptually and statis-
some cultures, the response to an aggressive act may be tically. Advances in multilevel modeling techniques and
to explain to the child why the behavior is unacceptable; in the availability of more-or-less user-friendly software
in others, physical discipline may be the accepted norm; have given researchers the tools to examine the effects
in yet others, aggression may be ignored or perhaps even of group, dyadic, and individual variables simultane-
reinforced (for discussions, see Bornstein & Cheah, in ously. These procedures can be used to assess how the
press; Harkness & Super, 2002). Another issue is the de- effects of variables describing individual tendencies
gree to which cultures allow or encourage peer interac- (e.g., aggressiveness, sociability, or inhibition) on an
tions. For example, in kin-based societies, such as outcome (e.g., ones subsequent aggressiveness, socia-
Kenya, peer interactions are discouraged because par- bility, or reticence) will vary as a function of dyadic-re-
ents fear the potential for competition and conflict (Ed- lationship characteristics (e.g., quality of friendship;
wards, 1992). It would appear most sensible for the quality of the mother-child relationship). In turn, a re-
international community of child development re- searcher can assess variations in dyadic effects due to
searchers not to generalize to other cultures their own the characteristics of the groups in which they are em-
culture-specific theories of normal and abnormal devel- bedded. The use of these techniques is nearly perfectly
opment. In this regard, we describe relevant extant re- suited to some forms of peer relationships research.
search pertaining to cross-cultural similarities and They have been used with success already (e.g.,
differences in childrens peer interactions and relation- Kochenderfer-Ladd & Wardrop, 2001).
ships throughout this chapter. Yet, despite the remarkable methodological advan-
tages of procedures, such as multilevel modeling, they
alone cannot deal with the conceptual ambiguity of
Summary
many measures currently used in peer research. Specifi-
To understand childrens experiences with peers, re- cally, a measure that putatively assesses one level of so-
searchers have focused on childrens interactions with cial analysis may, to some extent, reflect phenomena at
other children and on their involvements in peer rela- another level. For example, having dyadic friendships
tionships and groups. Analyses in each levelinterac- with aggressive peers, or belonging to an aggressive
tions, relationships, groupsare scientifically peer group may reflect individual tendencies such as so-
legitimate and raise interesting questions. However, re- ciability, risk-taking, and tolerance of aggressiveness
Theories Relevant to the Study of Childrens Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups 581

and those who are aggressive. At the same time, friend- lationship. Perhaps the only psychoanalytically oriented
ships with aggressive others also carry meaning at the theorist to ascribe developmental significance to chil-
relationship (dyadic friendship) or group levels. Thus, drens peer relationships is Peter Blos. For Blos (1967),
when researchers are examining the effects of group the major event of adolescence is the process of individ-
membership, they must also distinguish between the ef- uation by which adolescents restructure their childhood
fects of the group per se and the effects of having dyadic relationships with their parents and strive to achieve
relationships in that group. This problem is especially qualitatively different relationships with peers. Individ-
important when one wishes to distinguish between uation involves renegotiating dependency relationships
friendship effects and group effects. To the extent that a with parents; such renegotiation is precipitated, in part,
childs friendships are likely to be embedded in the by adolescent sexual drives. It also involves the introduc-
childs group, researchers need to carefully account for tion of new themes into relationships with peers. Re-
all of these effects and to distinguish between them. At- sponding to erotic drives, the adolescent turns toward
tempts to distinguish between the effects of friendship the peer group as a means of finding sexual outlets and
and the effects of belonging to a peer group are inade- venues of emotional closeness; previously, such close-
quate, or at least limited, when the effects of only one ness was available only from parents.
friend are accounted for. In such an instance, some As a function of restructuring their relationships
group effects may actually be friendship effects or with parents, adolescents come to experience turmoil
the other way around. and anxiety accompanied by feelings of despair, worth-
Finally, our emphasis on multiple levels of analysis lessness, discouragement, and vulnerability. According
provides us with a basic conceptual model of social com- to Blos, adolescents capacities to cope with these feel-
petence. Researchers have often treated measures of ings and experiences rest with their ability to establish
peer experiences (e.g., sociometric status) as indices of qualitatively distinct forms of supportive relationships
social competence. Our view is that social competence with peers. In the process of separating from parents
in the peer system refers to a childs capacity to engage and prior to achieving a state of personal autonomy,
effectively and successfully at each level of analysis and adolescents turn to peers for stimulation, belonging-
in his or her relevant culture. A competent child will be ness, loyalty, devotion, empathy, and resonance (Blos,
able to (a) become engaged in a peer group structure and 1967, p. 177).
participate in group-oriented activities, ( b) become in- One potential pitfall of the individuation process for
volved in satisfying relationships constructed on bal- adolescents is that some teenagers become overly de-
anced and reciprocal interactions, and (c) satisfy pendent on peers, conforming to the norms and stan-
individual goals and needs and develop accurate and dards of the group too readily as part of their search for
productive means of understanding experiences with security outside the family. Blos (1967) argued that, in
peers on both the group and dyadic levels. such cases, dependence on peers is problematic because
it precludes the promotion of independence and auton-
omy. But more generally, it is argued that the peer
group is a major determinant of an adolescents ability
THEORIES RELEVANT TO THE STUDY OF to achieve a sense of autonomy and independence from
CHILDRENS PEER INTERACTIONS, the family.
RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS The effects of the psychoanalytic tradition on peer
relationships research can be seen most strongly in two
Personality Theorists areas of research. The first one takes its inspiration
from the argument embedded in attachment theory
Psychoanalytic Perspectives (Bowlby, 1969/1982) that peer relationships are moti-
Psychoanalytic or neo-psychoanalytic theorists have vated by a human need for relatedness. According to this
rarely ascribed developmental significance to childrens view, being associated with others increases security
peer interactions or relationships. Instead, they regard because it reduces anxiety and promotes the internaliza-
much of the childs development as resulting from tion of positive relational schemas of others. As children
parental behavior and the quality of the parent-child re- develop mechanisms to distinguish between friends and
582 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

enemies, they are increasingly able to manage their emo- the exceedingly unpleasant and driving experience
tions and behavior. A second idea taken from psychoan- connected with the inadequate discharge of the need for
alytic theory has appeared more recently. Sandstrom human intimacy (Sullivan, 1953, p. 290).
and Cramer (2003) applied the concept of defense mech-
Symbolic Interactionism
anisms to the understanding of girls responses to rejec-
tion. Their findings indicate that the use of denial and Following the lead of William James (1890), who
projection following rejection vary as a function of so- posited that humans have an innate propensity to get
ciometric status with their use highest among rejected ourselves noticed, and noticed favorably, by our kind
and neglected girls. They point to the potential adaptive (p. 293), Mead (1934) argued that people defined them-
benefits of this use. selves according to how they believed they were per-
ceived by others. To Mead, for example, the ability to
Sullivans Theory of Personality Development self-reflect, to consider the self in relation to others,
In his developmental model of interpersonal relation- and to understand the perspectives of others was largely
ships, Sullivan characterized childrens peer relation- a function of participation in organized, rule-governed
ships during the early childhood and the early activities with peers. He suggested that exchanges
school-age years as organized largely around play and among peers, whether experienced in the arenas of co-
common activities. During the juvenile period (from ap- operation or competition, conflict or friendly discus-
proximately age 7 to 9 years), children become increas- sion, allowed the child to gain an understanding of the
ingly concerned about their place in the peer group as a self as both a subject and an object. Understanding that
whole and a sense of belonging to the group becomes in- the self could be an object of others perspectives grad-
creasingly important. ually evolved into the conceptualization of a general-
As children entered early adolescence, Sullivan pro- ized other or an organized and coordinated perspective
posed that they begin to develop chumships or close, of the social group. In turn, recognition of the gener-
intimate mutual relationships with same-sex peers. As a alized other led to the emergence of an organized sense
relationship between co-equals, chumships were dis- of self. Thus, according to symbolic interactionist the-
tinct from the hierarchical relationships that children ory, exchanges between the individual and the peer
experienced with their parents. Accordingly, Sullivan group are essential to the formation of a self concept
argued that this close relationship was a childs first and a concept of the other, two constructs thought to
true interpersonal experience based on reciprocity and be mutually interdependent.
exchange between equals and that the function of peer
relationships was to promote a sense of well-being. He Cognitive Developmental Perspectives
proposed that it was in chumships that children had their
first opportunities to experience a sense of self- The Piagetian Perspective
validation. This validation would emanate, in large part, Piaget (1932) suggested that childrens relationships
from their recognition of the positive regard and care with peers could be distinguished, in both form and
that their chums held for them. Sullivan went so far as to function, from their relationships with adults. The latter
argue that the positive experiences of having a chum relationships were construed as being complementary,
in adolescence would be so powerful as to enable adoles- asymmetrical, and falling along a vertical plane of dom-
cents to overcome trauma that may have resulted from inance and power assertion. As such, childrens interac-
prior family experiences. Conversely, Sullivan believed tions with adults about cognitions, ideas, and beliefs
that the experience of being isolated from the group, were thought to be marked by more emotional wariness
during the juvenile period, would lead a child to have and less openness and spontaneity than their interac-
concerns about his or her own competencies and his or tions with age-mates. By contrast, peer exchanges al-
her acceptability as a desirable peer. Consequently, Sul- lowed children to actively explore their ideas rather than
livan suggested that children who are unable to establish to risk their devaluation and criticism by adult authority
a position in the peer group would develop feelings of figures. It was also proposed that children come to ac-
inferiority that could contribute to a sense of psycholog- cept adults notions, thoughts, beliefs, and rules, not
ical ill-being. One posited outcome of the lack of sup- necessarily because they understand them, but rather
portive chumships was the development of loneliness, or because obedience is viewed as required. Along the
Theories Relevant to the Study of Childrens Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups 583

same lines, adults were less likely to follow the dictates more sensitive to each others needs, and more support-
of children. Peer relationships, alternatively, were por- ive of each others thoughts and well-being than non-
trayed as being balanced, egalitarian, and as falling friends, it may be that children are more likely to talk
along a more-or-less horizontal plane of dominance and openly and challenge each others thoughts and deeds in
power assertion. Thus, it was in the peer context that the company of friends than nonfriends. If this were the
children could experience opportunities to examine con- case, one would expect exchanges between friends to be
flicting ideas and explanations, to negotiate and discuss more promoting of cognitive and social-cognitive
multiple perspectives, to decide to compromise with, or growth than nonfriend peer exchanges. Data supportive
to reject, the notions held by peers. These peer interac- of this view are reviewed in later relevant sections.
tive experiences were believed to result in positive and
adaptive developmental outcomes for children, such as Vygotskys Perspective
the ability to understand others thoughts, emotions, According to Vygotsky (1978), cognitive growth and
and intentions. development are a function, in large part, of interper-
Empirical support for these contentions is drawn sonal exchange. Vygotsky invoked the principle of the
from neo-Piagetian research demonstrating that when zone of proximal development (ZPD) to explain the
children work together to solve given problems, they are significance of social interaction. The ZPD represented
more likely to advance their knowledge base through the distance between what the child could do indepen-
discussion than if they work independently and alone. dently and what he or she could do with the collabora-
Developmental change occurs because differences of tion or assistance of others. Vygotsky indicated that
opinion provoke cognitive disequilibria that are suffi- typically assistance was provided by the childs parents.
ciently discomforting so as to elicit attempts at resolu- Researchers such as Tudge (1992; Hogan & Tudge,
tion. Each interactor must construct, or reconstruct, a 1999) and Rogoff (1997) have argued that the childs
coordinated perspective of the original set of ideas to re- peers can play the role of co-constructivist. Thus, pair-
instate a sense of cognitive equilibrium. ing with a more competent, expert peer may assist
From this perspective, it is intrapersonal cognitive the childs movement through the ZPD (e.g., Duran &
conflict that evokes a search for homeostasis and result- Gauvain, 1993).
ant developmental change. This intrapersonal conflict One difference between the Piagetian and Vygot-
may be instigated by disagreements about ideas, skian perspectives of the links between peer interaction,
thoughts, beliefs; however, it is unlikely that mean-spir- peer relationships, and growth and development lies in
ited interpersonal conflict and hostility brings with it Piagets belief that it was peer conf lict that evoked
cognitive advancement. Recent views on the role of con- change, whereas Vygotsky contended that it was cooper-
flict center on the notion that disagreements between ation and the pooling of ideas that promoted change.
peers about things personal, interpersonal, and imper- Contemporary accounts suggest that conflicting ideas
sonal are best resolved through the cooperative exchange and differences in opinion actually elicit cooperation
of explanations, questions, and reasoned dialogue (e.g., between partners. If partners are positively disposed to
Laursen et al., 2001; Shulman & Laursen, 2002). If the one another, it behooves them to discuss their differ-
exchange of conflicting ideas is marked by hostility, ences, to negotiate, to compromisein short, to cooper-
dysregulated or disabling emotions are not likely to pro- ate and to move forward, not only cognitively, but also
mote cognitive growth and development. emotionally in their relationship. Thus, studies of the
Contemporary perspectives on the role of peer ex- role that conf lict plays in cognitive and social-cognitive
change for developmental growth can be seen in the growth include, in the phenomenons definition, compo-
work of co-constructivist thinkers such as Azmitia nents of disagreement as well as explanation, questions,
(Azmitia, Lippman, & Ittel, 1999; Azmitia & Mont- agreements, and compromise. A rapprochement between
gomery, 1993) and Rogoff (1997). These writers intro- the Piagetian and Vygotskian positions would suggest
duce the notion that the quality of the relationship that intrapersonal cognitive conflict triggers the childs
between the peers who are interacting with each other attempts to regain some semblance of cognitive home-
may contribute to cognitive and social-cognitive growth ostasis. If such intrapersonal cognitive conflict is asso-
and development. For example, friends can challenge ciated with conflictual, negative-spirited interpersonal
each other with relative impunity. Given that friends are exchange, cognitive growth is less likely to result than
584 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

anger, fear, or some other disabling emotion. Alter- information about the consequences of specific social
nately, if cognitive conflict is associated with reasoned behaviors to guide their own exhibition or inhibition of
dialogue (Damon & Killen, 1982), cooperative co-con- these behaviors. This modeling perspective provides a
struction may occur resulting in a new, more cognitively powerful argument for how the social behaviors of chil-
mature perspective. dren are quickly and effectively organized, reorganized,
In summary, research based on the constructivist the- and redirected. Observational learning promotes adap-
ories of Piaget and Vygotsky reveals that: tation to new circumstances and new relationships
(Cairns, 1979). As Cairns noted, however, once learned,
Children can, and do, make cognitive advances when social behaviors are subject to maintenance and change;
they cooperatively exchange and discuss conflicting thus, it is argued that the demonstration of socially
perspectives on various issues (MacDonald, Miell, & learned behaviors will be maintained or inhibited by its
Morgan, 2000). actual or expected consequences. Further, the social
Children working together can solve problems that contexts in which reinforcement and punishment occur
neither partner is capable of solving alone (Gol- (or are expected to occur) matter. The source of the re-
beck, 1998). inforcement or punishment, how, when, and where the
Discussing problems with a peer who has superior consequences are administered, and whether the child
knowledge is more likely to evoke intrapersonal con- believes that he or she can actually produce the desired
flict and cognitive advancement than discussions behavior all affect the production, reproduction, or inhi-
with a less competent peer (Duran & Gauvain, 1993; bition of the given behavior. For example, Bandura
Garton, 2001; Tudge, 1992). (1989) speculated that children set standards of achieve-
Transactive exchanges during which children openly ment for themselves and that they are likely to self-
criticize each others ideas and clarify and elaborate administer reinforcement when the standards are met
their own ideas are more often observed in the com- and punishment when they are not. Self-reinforcement is
pany of friends than of nonfriends (e.g., Azmitia & applied when children see themselves as having ex-
Montgomery, 1993). ceeded the norms for their relevant comparison group of
peers; self-punishment is consequent to having failed to
meet perceived group norms.
Learning and Social Learning Theories, Peer
Also, childrens beliefs, cognitions, and ideas about
Interaction, and Peer Relationships
the administrators of rewards/ punishment can influence
Although its influences are less explicit than implied, the strength of the given behaviors. Is the administrator
social learning theory has had a powerful effect on the a competent or incompetent peer, an aggressive or
study of peer interactions, perhaps more so than any nonaggressive age-mate, or a younger or older child?
other perspective. The traditional learning theory per- Moreover, the age of the child who is processing this so-
spective has been that children are behavior control and cial information must assuredly be of some signifi-
behavior change agents for each other. Peers punish or cance. To the extent that researchers have generally
ignore nonnormative social behavior and reward or rein- ignored these issues, social learning theory still has
force positively those behaviors considered culturally some way to go in advancing an understanding of the es-
appropriate and competent. Thus, to the extent that chil- tablishment, maintenance, and dissolution /inhibition of
dren behave in a socially appropriate manner, they de- childrens peer-directed behaviors.
velop positive relationships with their peers; to the
extent that children behave in a socially incompetent or Human Ethology
nonnormative manner, peer rejection may result.
Perhaps the most relevant and influential social Ethology is the subdiscipline of biology concerned
learning was that formulated originally by Bandura and with the biological bases of behavior, including its evo-
Walters (1963). In their monograph, Social Learning and lution, causation, function, and development (Cairns,
Personality Development, Bandura and Walters noted 1979, p. 358). Although there is no particular ethologi-
that children can learn novel social behaviors by observ- cal theory pertaining specifically to the evolutionary
ing others. Moreover, children could use observational significance of peer interaction or peer relationships,
Theories Relevant to the Study of Childrens Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups 585

the methods and constructs used by animal behaviorists of examining the gestures and facial expressions of the
have often been adopted by those who study childrens interacting individuals, as well as the ecological (ven-
social behaviors, peer relationships, and the structural ues) and interpersonal (quality of relationships) contexts
dynamics of the peer group (e.g., Hawley, 2003). To the in which social interactions occur.
extent that Bowlbys (1973) ethologically oriented the- Ethological theory and the questions derived from it
ory of parent-infant attachment relationships has come evoke analyses of the psychological meanings of differ-
to influence the study of peer relationships, some con- ent forms of the same behavior (Hawley, 2003). For ex-
sideration of human ethological theory is warranted. ample, do instrumental and hostile aggressions have
The questions asked by ethologists were outlined by different developmental origins and different proximal
Tinbergen (1951). He suggested that when an organism and distal causes (e.g., Prinstein & Cillessen, 2003)?
produces a given behavior, the scientist must ask: (a) Similarly, does the frequent expression of behavioral
Why did the individual demonstrate the particular be- solitude when engaging in constructive activity have dif-
havior at the specific time she or he did? ( b) How did the ferent developmental origins and different proximal and
individual come to produce such a behavior at such distal causes than the frequent expression of behavioral
times? and (c) What is the functional significance or sur- solitude when observing others from afar (Coplan,
vival value of the produced behavior? These questions Rubin, Fox, Calkins, & Stewart, 1994; Henderson, Mar-
focus concern on features of motivation, learning and shall, Fox, & Rubin, 2004)? Likewise, does a given be-
development, and evolutionary adaptation, respectively. havior have the same psychological meaning when
A central tenet of ethological theory is that social be- produced by a 2-, 4-, and 10-year-old? And finally, does
havior, relationships, and organizational structures are a given behavior have the same psychological meaning
limited by biological constraints related to their adap- when produced by children of the same age, but in dif-
tive, evolutionary function (Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, ferent cultures? These are questions pertinent to the
1976). Thus, aggression, for example, is viewed as a study of peer interaction. And, given the normalcy/ab-
means by which members of the species survive; protect normalcy of social behaviors in different contexts and at
themselves, their significant others, and their progeny; different ages (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004), it is also
and ensure reproductive success (Lorenz, 1966). Altru- clear how questions derived from ethological theory are
ism is also seen as a basic facet of human nature, ensur- relevant to the study of childrens peer relationships.
ing survival of the species. Likewise, the attachment
relationship formed during infancy between parent and Group Socialization Theory
child not only guarantees the protection of the young
from discomfort and threatening predators but also pro- If there was a publication that brought the study of the
vides the child with an internalized working model peer group to the attention of the general reader during
(Bowlby, 1973) of what human relationships could, the past 10 years it was Judith Rich Harriss essay on
should, or might be like. In this latter case, the quality of group socialization theory (1995) and the book, The
the primary relationship engenders a set of internalized Nurture Assumption, based on it (1998). Issued just
relationships expectations that affect the initiation and after the writing of our earlier Handbook chapter, Har-
maintenance of extrafamilial (e.g., peer) relationships. riss essay and book claimed: (a) The effects of parent-
Given the assumption that behavior is best under- ing on development were, at best, small; ( b) the effects
stood when observed in natural settings, ethological the- of genes on development were strong; and (c) the effect
ory has influenced contemporary methodologies. Thus, of peer relationships, and especially the peer group,
investigators have devoted considerable effort to distin- were strong also. At the risk of oversimplification, Har-
guish observationally between different forms and func- riss ideas can be summarized as follows: First, she crit-
tions of what, on the surface, appear to be the same basic icized research on parenting as being methodologically
behavioral phenomena. For example, one can distinguish and substantively flawed. She objected to the claim that
between physical, verbal, and relational aggression (the parents influence their children because most studies of
forms) and between hostile and instrumental aggression parenting failed to produce strong effect sizes and were
(the putative functions; see Little, Jones, Henrich, & methodologically flawed due to their use of correla-
Hawley, 2003). Such distinctions are drawn on the basis tional designs rather than of explicitly experimental
586 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

methods. Second, in support of her arguments regarding socialization (Collins et al., 2000). Perhaps at the writ-
the influence of genes, she appropriately called on find- ing of the next version of this Handbook chapter, reli-
ings that point to genetic effects on various aspects of able evidence pertaining to the power of peer group
development. Third, Harriss claims about the effects influence will be presented as supporting the provoca-
of the peer system were, in part, predicated on the view tive thesis proposed by Harris. In the meantime, con-
that young people are driven by an atavistic desire to temporary research (described later) on the ways in
be part of a group. According to Harris, an impor- which the composition of peer networks change as a
tant repercussion of these tribal motivations is that function of childrens individual interests and behav-
young people, in an effort to be part of a group, will ioral characteristics may be taken as some evidence of
change their behavior in response to group norms and the transactional push-and-pull between individual in-
expectations. clinations and peer group norms.
Thus, it was proposed that once children find them-
selves outside the home, they take on the norms preva-
lent in the groups in which they spend their PEER INTERACTIONS, RELATIONSHIPS,
time . . . and, for the most part, those groups comprise AND GROUPS: A DEVELOPMENTAL
other children. Drawing from social psychological per- PERSPECTIVE
spectives on the significance of group norms (a motiva-
tion to fit in), in-group biases and out-group Childrens peer experiences become increasingly di-
hostilities, and social cognitive views of group verse, complex, and integrated with development. In
processes, it was argued that childrens identities de- some cases, the impetus for these developments rests in
velop primarily from their experiences in the peer children (i.e., changes in interpersonal understanding or
group. Although Harriss (1998) views that parent-child interpersonal concerns), while others derive from situa-
and other dyadic relationships (including friendships) tional or contextual phenomena (Bierman, 2003). In the
are relatively unimportant for individual development following sections, we review many developmental mile-
has drawn many criticisms (e.g., Collins, Maccoby, posts in the interactional (changes in the frequency or
Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000), the ele- forms of specific behaviors), relational (changes in
ments of her thesis that stressed the significance of peer qualities of friendships or patterns of involvement in
interactions, relationships, and groups for normal and friendships), and group (changes in configurations of
abnormal development provided some vindication to and involvement in cliques and crowds) levels of chil-
peer researchers. For decades, these researchers have drens involvement with other children.
been challenged by theorists, researchers, and policy-
makers who have cited the primacy of parenting and the
Infancy and the Toddler Years
parent-child relationship. With Harriss counterchal-
lenge, a gauntlet was dropped; researchers were called Research on the normative development peer interac-
on to address some central questions about the causal tions and relationships during infancy and toddlerhood
roles that genes, biology, family, and peers play in child has waned during the past decade. Instead, the focus has
and adolescent adjustment and maladjustment. appeared to shift from normative development to indi-
The claims of this book were presented and debated vidual differences to the extent that toddlers initiate so-
in the review sections of newspapers and magazines cial interaction and are capable of regulating social and
and on many prime time television programs. The emotional behavior.
essay managed to win some public praise, typically
from people who do not study peer relationships. For Interaction
good reason, persons who have been advocates of be- Early researchers of childrens peer experiences were
havioral genetic explanations of a wide variety of so- impressed by what they regarded as the significant so-
cial behaviors and personality characteristics (e.g., cial shortcomings in infants. Buhler (1935), for exam-
Pinker, 2002; Rowe, 1995) were supportive of Harriss ple, reported that prior to the first 6 months of life,
(1995, 1998) claims of genetic influence. Scholars who babies were oblivious to each others presence. And it
study parenting wrote reasoned critiques of Harriss was argued that throughout much of the 1st year in-
thesis regarding the relative unimportance of parental fants were interested in each other as objects but not as
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 587

social partners with whom the development of a rela- These developments promote more effective social
tionship was possible (e.g., Maudry & Nekula, 1939). commerce between toddlers and contribute a generally
Such a view appears less often in contemporary read- positive affective quality to their interaction (Hay, Cas-
ings, but it has not disappeared completely; for exam- tle, Davies, Demetriou, & Stimson, 1999). However,
ple, it has been noted that the peer interactions of toddler social interaction is also marked by conflict
infants are diffuse and fragmented. These interactions (e.g., Hay, Castle, & Davies, 2000; Hay & Ross, 1982;
are seen as illustrating the inability of babies to com- Rubin, Hastings, Chen, Stewart, & McNichol, 1998).
prehend the social and cognitive needs, capacities, or Rubin et al. (1998) found that over 70% of 25-month-old
zones of proximal development of their age-mates children participated in a conflict situation at least once
(Hay, 1985). in a 50-minute laboratory setting. In a comparable set-
Infants do have obvious social limitations. Yet, care- ting, Hay and Ross (1982) observed 87% of 21-month-
ful observation of infants reveals remarkable strides old toddlers engaged in at least one conflict. As such, it
taken during the 1st year of life. These include (a) the appears that conflict is neither infrequent nor limited to
seemingly intentional direction of smiles, frowns, and a small percentage of toddlers.
gestures to their play partners (Hay, Nash, & Pederson, Indeed, it appears as if many of those toddlers who
1983); ( b) the careful observation of peers representing frequently instigate conflicts with peers are the most
a clear sign of social interest (Eckerman, 1979); and (c) socially outgoing and initiating (National Institute of
the response, often in kind, to their play partners be- Child Health and Human Development Early Child
haviors (Mueller & Brenner, 1977). During the 2nd year Care Research Network, 2001; Rubin et al., 1998). It is
of life, toddlers demonstrate monumental gains in their also the case that (a) toddlers who lose conflicts are
social repertoires. With the emergence of locomotion more likely than the initial victor to initiate the imme-
and the ability to use words to communicate, interactive diately subsequent conflict (Hay & Ross, 1982); and
bouts become lengthier (Eckerman & Stein, 1990), and ( b) toddlers are highly attentive to, and are more
toddler play becomes organized around particular likely to imitate and initiate interactions with,
themes or games. According to Ross (1982), the typical highly sociable age-mates (Howes, 1983, 1988). Taken
toddler game involves extended and patterned inter- together, these data suggest that during the 2nd year
changes characterized by the mutual exchange of gaze, of life, toddlers do display social skills of modest
the direction of social actions to one another, the pro- complexity.
duction of appropriate responses to these social actions,
Relationships
and the demonstration of turn-taking behaviors. Often,
these toddler games are marked by reciprocal imitative It has been demonstrated that toddlers are more likely to
acts. Reciprocity of imitation suggests not only that a initiate play, direct positive affect to, and engage in
given child is socially interested in the playmate to the complex interactions with familiar than unfamiliar play-
point at which she or he is willing to copy that play- mates (Howes, 1988, 1996). But can familiarity be
mates behavior but also that she or he is also aware of equated with the existence of a relationship? According
the partners interest in him or her (i.e., an awareness of to Ross and Lollis (1989), toddlers do develop positive
being imitated). Mutual imitation, which increases rap- relationships as they become increasingly familiar with
idly during the 2nd year, appears to lay the basis for one another. Indeed, these toddler relationships allow
later emerging cooperative interchanges involving pre- the observer to predict the sorts of interchanges that
tense (Howes, 1992). will transpire between dyadic partners (Ross, Conant,
In summary, social skills in toddlerhood comprise (a) Cheyne, & Alevisos, 1992). It is the predictability of the
the ability to coordinate behavior with that of the play quality of interchange that marks a dyad as constituting
partner; ( b) imitation of the peers activity and an a friendship.
awareness of being imitated; (c) turn-taking that Ross and colleagues have carried out an elegant series
involves observe peerrespond to peerobserve and of studies to demonstrate that toddlers can and do
waitrespond to peer interchange sequences; (d) the develop relationships and that their relationships can
demonstration of helping and sharing behaviors; and be characterized in several different ways. Ross et al.
(e) the ability to respond appropriately to the peer part- (1992) begin by noting that a relationship may be
ners characteristics. inferred when:
588 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Neither the characteristic behavior of Child One, nor the more central to their functioning than others, perhaps il-
behavior that others typically direct to Child Two, nor the lustrating the earliest examples of individual differences
independent, additive influences of both factors taken to- in popularity and influence.
gether are sufficient to predict the behavior of Child One
to Child Two. In that sense, relationships cannot be de- New Directions
rived from the individual characteristics of the partici-
Major advances in the study of infants and toddlers
pants; the relationship itself influences the interaction
peer interactions and relationships have derived primar-
between them. (p. 1)
ily from examinations of individual dif ferences in fac-
To this end, Ross and colleagues have demonstrated tors such as sociability, behavioral inhibition, conflict,
that toddlers develop reciprocal relationships, not only and the regulation of emotional and behavioral tenden-
by the mutual exchange of positive overtures, but also by cies. Much of this research meshes with the current
agonistic interactions. Positive interactions are directed Zeitgeist in which the study of developmental psycho-
specifically to those who have directed positive initia- pathology dominates in many quarters. Thus, re-
tions to the child beforehand; conflict is initiated specif- searchers have discovered that those toddlers who
ically with those who have initiated conflictual frequently initiate conflict with age-mates, especially
interactions with the child beforehand. those who are unable to regulate their emotions and be-
To the extent that reciprocal interchanges of positive haviors, evidence difficulties of an externalizing nature
overtures may characterize particular dyads, it may be in subsequent years (Hay et al., 2000; Rubin, Burgess,
said that toddlers do have friendships. Although the Dwyer, & Hastings, 2003). And those toddlers who evi-
terms of reference vary from those of Ross and col- dence fearfulness and wariness when faced with unfa-
leagues, other researchers have proposed that toddlers miliarity in social settings evidence difficulties
have friends. For example, Howes (1988) defined tod- associated with social reticence, shyness, and anxiety as
dler friendship as encompassing the response to a peers preschoolers (e.g., Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002)
overture at least once, the production of at least one and elementary school children (Reznick et al., 1986).
complementary or reciprocal dyadic exchange, and the Because the focus appears to be turning primarily in the
demonstration of positive affect during at least one such direction of the development of psychopathology, it is
exchange. Vandell and Mueller (1980) identified toddler important to note that researchers are beginning to find
friends as those who initiated positive social interaction that early individual differences in cooperative, sharing
more often with each other than with other potential and helping behaviors presage consequent positive as-
partners. During the toddler period, friendships, as de- pects of peer interaction. Thus, for example, Howes and
fined earlier, do exist; however, it is doubtful that they Phillipsen (1998) have demonstrated that toddlers com-
carry the same strength of psychological meaning as the petent play with peers predicts socially competent ac-
friendships of older children. Nevertheless, these early tivity at 4 years and less maladaptive interactive
relationships may lay the groundwork for the establish- activity at 9 years. Whether individual differences in
ment and maintenance of friendships throughout the the peer interactional tendencies of infants and toddlers
childhood years. predict subsequent social relationship and/or group phe-
nomena is, as yet, unknown.
Groups Another new direction derives from the cross-cul-
Even young toddlers spend much of their time in small tural observation of toddlers. Researchers have recently
groups such as with day-care mates. But there is not found that Asian toddlers are more compliant than their
much empirical evidence that this level of social organi- North American counterparts (Chen, Rubin, et al.,
zation is salient to, or influential on, these young chil- 2003); and Chinese and Korean toddlers appear to be
dren. Nevertheless, some authors (e.g., Legault & more socially inhibited than Italian and Australian tod-
Strayer, 1991) have observed dominance hierarchies dlers (Chen et al., 1998; Rubin et al., in press). Why
even in small groups of young toddlers, as well as in sub- these early differences in social behavior exist is only
sets of children who invest greater attention and inter- now being explored. Researchers interested in chil-
action to one another than to outside nonmembers. drens peer relationships would do well to examine
Interestingly, some members of these groups appear whether such early cultural differences predict vari-
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 589

ability in the peer acceptance of children who best masks the functional significance of the behavior.
match their respective cultural norms for compliant Watching and playing near, but not with, others are not
and socially outgoing behaviors. necessarily immature. Rather, these behaviors may be
sequenced in a competent manner to gain entry into an
ongoing play activity.
Early Childhood Further attesting to the limits of Partens original so-
cial participation categories is the fact that the cate-
Interaction gories of solitary, parallel, and group behavior comprise
From 24 months to 5 years, the frequency of peer inter- a variety of play forms that differ in cognitive complex-
action increases and becomes more complex. Parten ity (see Rubin, Fein, & Vandenberg, 1983, for a review).
(1932) described six social participation categories that Thus, whether alone, near, or with others, children may
purportedly unfolded as stages as children matured. In produce simple sensorimotor behaviors (functional play,
order of presumed maturity, these categories included: e.g., aimlessly bouncing a ball), construct structures
unoccupied behavior, solitary play, onlooker behavior from blocks or draw with crayons (constructive play), or
(the child observes others but does not participate in the engage in some form of pretense (dramatic play). These
activity), parallel play (the child plays beside but not cognitive forms of play, when examined in their social
with other children), associative play (the child plays context, reveal interesting developmental trends. For ex-
and shares with others), and cooperative play (the child ample, solitary-sensorimotor behaviors become increas-
engages others in interaction that is well coordinated ingly rare over the preschool years, while the relative
and defined by a division of labor). From her data, frequency of solitary-construction or exploration re-
Parten concluded that between the ages of 2 and 5 years, mains the same (Rubin et al., 1978). Furthermore, the
children engage in increasing frequencies of associative only types of social interactive activity to increase over
and cooperative play and in decreasing frequencies of the preschool years are sociodramatic play and games-
idle, solitary, and onlooker behavior. with-rules (see Goncu, Patt, & Kouba, 2002, for a recent
A more critical reading of Partens study and subse- review): Age differences are apparent only for particu-
quent attempts at replication, however, suggests a more lar forms of social participation. Thus, in contrast to
complex set of conclusions (e.g., Rubin, Watson, & Jam- Partens characterization, it does not appear to be a sim-
bor, 1978). To begin with, children at all ages engage in ple matter of solitary play disappearing over time and
unoccupied, onlooking, solitary, parallel, and group ac- being replaced by social interactive activity. Impor-
tivities. Even at 5 years, children spend less of their free tantly, it is the form that solitary or parallel or social ac-
play time in classroom settings interacting with others tivity takes that is of developmental significance.
than being alone or near others (Rubin et al., 1978). In- Perhaps the most complex form of group interactive
deed, the frequency of parallel play appears to remain activity during the preschool years is sociodramatic play
constant from 3-to-5 years (Rubin et al., 1978). Yet, de- (Goncu et al., 2002). The ability to engage easily in this
spite its modest placement in Partens hierarchy of so- form of social activity represents mastery of one of the
cial participation, parallel play appears to serve as an essential tasks of early childhoodthe will and skill to
important bridge to more truly interactive exchanges. share and coordinate decontextualized and substitutive
More precise, sequential observations of preschool in- activities. Researchers have reported that by the 3rd
teraction reveal that parallel play often serves as an en- year of life, children are able to share symbolic mean-
tre into more complex, cooperative activity (Robinson, ings through social pretense (e.g., Howes, 1988). This is
Anderson, Porter, Hart, & Wouden-Miller, 2003). Put a remarkable accomplishment, as it involves the capacity
another way, competent entry into ongoing peer activity to take on complementary roles, none of which matches
appears to involve the ability to observe what the play real-world situations, and to agree on the adoption of
participants are doing (onlooking activity), to approach these imaginary roles in a rule-governed context.
and play beside potential play partners (parallel play), The ability to share meaning during pretense has
and, finally, to engage the players in conversation about been referred to as intersubjectivity (Goncu, 1993).
the ongoing activity. A simple consideration of the fre- Goncu (1993) has reported that quantitative differences
quency of particular forms of social participation are present in the extent to which the social interchanges
590 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

of 3- versus 4.5-year-olds comprise indices of shared And the disposition to behave in a caring, sharing, and
meaning or intersubjectivity. For example, the social in- helpful manner in early childhood appears rather stable
teractions of older preschoolers involve longer se- (Eisenberg et al., 1999). Dodge, Coie, & Lynam (Chap-
quences or turns. With increasing age, play partners ter 12, this Handbook, this volume) note that aggression
become better able to agree with each other about the increases until age 3 and then declines.
roles, rules, and themes of their pretense. They are also Importantly, the nature of conflict changes from the
better able to maintain their play interactions by adding toddler to the preschool period. During toddlerhood,
new dimensions to their expressed ideas. These develop- most conflict appears to center on toys and resources;
ments reflect the preschoolers capacity to take the per- during the preschool years, conflict becomes increas-
spective of the play partner and, even more important, ingly centered on differences of opinion (Chen, Fein, &
reflect the increasing sophistication of preschoolers Tam, 2001; Laursen & Hartup, 1989)a reflection of
naive theory of mind (Watson, Nixon, Wilson, & Ca- the childs growing ability to focus on others ideas, at-
page, 1999). titudes, and opinions.
The demonstration of elaborate forms of social pre- Finally, preschoolers spend a great deal of time sim-
tense during the preschool years is impressive. But is ply conversing with their playmates. And their conversa-
the experience of sociodramatic play developmentally tions reflect numerous interpersonal goals (e.g.,
significant? According to Howes (1992), sociodramatic negotiating roles and rules in play; arguing and agreeing;
play serves three essential developmental functions. Hay et al., 2004). Older preschool-age children direct
First, it creates a context for mastering the communica- more speech to their peers than do their younger coun-
tion of meaning. Second, it provides opportunities for terparts (Levin & Rubin, 1983). And they are more
children to learn to control and compromise; these op- likely to try to make explicit communicative connec-
portunities arise during discussions and negotiations tions with their play partners ideas (Goncu, 1993;
concerning pretend roles and scripts and the rules guid- Sawyer, 1997). However, the successful outcome of ver-
ing the pretend episodes (Sawyer, 1997). Third, social bally directed communication is predicted by its techni-
pretense allows for a safe context in which children cal quality. Preschoolers whose language is
can explore and discuss issues of intimacy and trust. Re- comprehensible, who assure that they have obtained lis-
searchers have demonstrated that engaging in sociodra- tener attention, and who are within arms length of their
matic play is associated with social perspective-taking social targets are more likely to meet their social goals
skills and the display of skilled interpersonal behavior. than those who verbal directives are less skillfully
In summary, as pretend play becomes more interac- evinced (Mueller, 1972). Relatedly, throughout the pre-
tive, it serves increasingly sophisticated psychological school years, children demonstrate age-related increases
functions. At first, social pretense provides opportuni- in social-communicative competence. For example, they
ties for developing communication skills (Sawyer, begin to alter their speech to suit the needs of their lis-
1997). Subsequently, it allows children opportunities teners (Shatz & Gelman, 1973). Similar adjustments to
to negotiate over roles, rules, and play themes and to the characteristics of their social targets have been re-
practice a variety of roles in particular play scripts ported in studies of interpersonal problem solving over-
(Goncu, 1993). Thus, the addition of understanding tures (Krasnor & Rubin, 1983). These data reflect
pretense and sharing this understanding with others developmental growth in metacommunicative awareness
represents a significant milestone in the social lives of and mind-reading (Dunn, 1999).
young children. Taken together, the data reviewed earlier raise ques-
Beyond the developmental differences in how much tions concerning Piagets assumption that the speech of
children interact with one another or engage in coopera- preschoolers is characterized primarily by its socially
tive endeavors requiring shared meanings, several other egocentric quality. Indeed, approximately 60% of
significant advances are made during the preschool pe- preschoolers utterances are socially directed, compre-
riod. For one, prosocial caring, sharing, and helping be- hensible, and result in appropriate responses (Levin &
haviors become more commonplace with increasing age. Rubin, 1983; Mueller, 1972). Furthermore, it has been
Researchers have demonstrated that 4-year-olds direct shown that young children recognize when their verbal
prosocial behavior to their peers more often than 3-year- repertoires are limited and, in such circumstances, will
olds (e.g., Benenson, Markovits, Roy, & Denko, 2003). resort to the use of gestures to communicate meaning
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 591

(e.g., Sawyer, 1997). Studies of gestural communica- one that relies on friendship nominations from both
tion actually shed light on Piagets original ideas con- dyadic partners; a unilateral, nonreciprocated friend-
cerning egocentric thought and speech. Piaget ship has often been taken to mean wishful thinking
recognized the significance of gestural communication on the part of the single nominator. In keeping with this
and wrote that in the explanations of young children, perspective, Vaughn (2001) recently reported that (a)
gestures play as important a part as words. (Piaget, older preschoolers are more likely to participate in re-
1959, p. 77). It may well be that Piagets take on ciprocated friendships than are younger preschoolers;
communicative competence has been poorly under- ( b) preschoolers who nominate each other as friends
stood, or at best, misjudged. In Piagets own research, interact more frequently with each other than those
he indicated that only 35% to 40% of young childrens dyads in which only a unilateral nomination of friend-
utterances were egocentric. This leads to the conclu- ship is evinced.
sion that in almost 60% of the cases, young children Importantly, not all young children have a best friend.
demonstrated communicative competence. If one were Approximately 75% of preschoolers have reciprocally
to add to verbal expression the comprehensible use of nominated best friendships (Dunn, 1993). Friendless
gestures, preschoolers would clearly be regarded as preschoolers are less likely than befriended children to
communicatively skilled. initiate and maintain play with peers (e.g., Howes,
Matheson, & Wu, 1992). And during this period of early
Relationships childhood, the ability to make friends, friendship qual-
During early childhood, children express preferences for ity, and stability of young childrens friendships are as-
some peers over others as playmates. It appears that one sociated with, and predicted by, social-cognitive and
important influence on this process is that preschoolers emotional maturity. For example, the abilities to under-
are attracted to peers who are similar to them in some stand emotional displays and social intent and to per-
noticeable regard. For example, similarities in age and spective-take are associated with friendship formation,
sex draw young children together. Furthermore, maintenance, and friendship quality (Dunn & Cutting,
preschoolers appear to be attracted to, and become 1999; Dunn et al., 2002; Ladd & Kochenderfer, 1996).
friends with peers whose behavioral tendencies are sim- Furthermore, the young childs ability to regulate emo-
ilar to their own, a phenomenon known as behavioral ho- tions is associated with and predictive of both the num-
mophily (e.g., Kandel, 1978; Ryan, 2001). ber of mutual friends and friendship quality (Walden,
Once preschoolers form friendships, their behavior Lemerise, & Smith, 1999).
with these individuals is distinctive from their behavior It is not only the positive aspects of behavior that dif-
with other children who are familiar but not friends. ferentiate preschool friendships from nonfriendships
Among the features that mark the friendships of pre- compared to nonfriends, preschool friends also
school-age children are supportiveness and exclusivity demonstrate more quarreling and more active (assaults
(Sebanc, 2003). Children as young as 3.5 years direct and threats) and reactive hostility (refusals and resist-
more social overtures, engage in more social interac- ance; Dunn & Cutting, 1999; Laursen & Hartup, 1989).
tions, and play in more complex ways with friends than Moreover, Hartup and his colleagues (Hartup, Laursen,
nonfriends (e.g., Dunn & Cutting, 1999; Dunn, Cutting, Stewart, & Eastenson, 1988) demonstrated that pre-
& Fisher, 2002). As well, preschool-aged friends tend to school children engage in more conflicts with their
cooperate and exhibit more positive social behaviors friends than with neutral associates. These differences
with each other than with nonfriends (e.g., Dunn et al., are best understood by recognizing that friends spend
2002). Ladd, Kochenderfer, and Coleman (1996) have much more time actually interacting with each other
shown that not all friendships in early childhood are than do nonfriends. Hartup and his colleagues also re-
equally stable. Those friendships that involve higher lev- ported qualitative differences in how preschool friends
els of positive friendship qualities (e.g., validation) and and nonfriends resolve conflicts and in what the out-
lower levels of negative friendship qualities (e.g., low comes of these conflicts are likely to be. Friends, as
conflict) are most likely to be stable. compared with nonfriends, make more use of negotiation
Typically, researchers who study friendship rely on and disengagement, relative to standing firm, in their
children as informants about with whom they are resolution of conflicts. In conflict outcomes, friends are
friends. It has been argued that a true friendship is more likely to have equal resolutions, relative to win or
592 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

lose occurrences. Also, following conflict resolution, strated that, as early as the preschool period, childrens
friends are more likely than neutral associates to stay in groups comprise individuals who are behaviorally simi-
physical proximity and continue to engage in interaction. lar (van den Oord, Rispens, Goudena, & Vermande,
In summary, preschoolers behave differently with 2000; Vermande, van den Oord, Rispens, & Goudena,
friends than nonfriends. Preschoolers engage in more 2000). Aggression is the most important determinant of
prosocial behaviors as well as more conflicts when in- social clustering in the preschool classroom: A re-
teracting with friends than with nonfriends. These con- searcher can best predict the peer group composition for
flicts are most likely to be resolved through negotiation, aggressive childrena finding that repeats itself in
and the outcomes are usually equitable. These differ- older groups of children (see following).
ences suggest that preschoolers view friendship as a
unique context, separate and qualitatively different
Middle Childhood and Early Adolescence
from their experiences with nonfriends.
The school-age years represent a dramatic shift in social
Groups
context for most children in Western cultures. During
Many researchers have found that the social dominance this time, the proportion of social interaction that in-
hierarchy is an important organizational feature of volves peers increases. Whereas approximately 10% of
the preschool peer group (e.g., Hawley, 2002; Vaughn, the social interaction for 2-year-olds involves peers, the
1999; Vaughn, Vollenweider, Bost, Azria-Evans, & comparable figure for children in middle childhood is
Snider, 2003). And, in keeping with a central tenet of more than 30%. Other changes include: the size of the
the ethological perspective, researchers have argued peer group (which becomes considerably larger) and
that dominance hierarchies develop naturally in groups how peer interaction is supervised (it become less
to serve adaptive functions. In the case of preschool- closely supervised by adults). Thus, in the years leading
aged children, dominance hierarchies appear to reduce up to adolescence, children are brought into contact with
overt aggression among members of the group. Obser- a more diverse set of peers, although generally with
vations of exchanges between children in which physi- those who are similar to them in age.
cal attacks, threats, and object conflicts occur reveal a The settings of peer interaction also change. Pre-
consistent pattern of winners and losers. And children school childrens peer contacts are centered in the home
who are losers in object struggles rarely initiate con- and in day-care centers, whereas school-age children
flict with those who have proven victorious over oth- come into contact with peers in a wide range of settings.
ers or who have been victorious over them (Strayer & Although the settings for peer interaction in middle
Strayer, 1976). childhood have not been well described, there are some
key studies. Zarbatany, Hartmann, and Rankin (1990)
Summary
reported that the most frequent contexts for peer inter-
Even in early childhood, one can identify children who action, among middle class young adolescents include,
are more or less skilled in manipulating their peers or in in order of their frequency, conversing, hanging out,
meeting their interpersonal goals. Dominance hierar- being together at school, talking on the telephone, trav-
chies reflect primarily differences in childrens success eling to and from school, listening to TV and records,
in struggles over objects. However, achieving the acqui- and noncontact sports. Boys and girls differed on only
sition of desired objects is only one of many interper- one of these activitiesmore peer interaction took
sonal goals that preschool children may have. place during phone conversations for girls than for boys.
Consequently, it remains unknown whether preschool In terms of their perceived importance, this sample of
children who have risen to the top of the preschool dom- early adolescents viewed noncontact sports, watching
inance hierarchy are those who develop and maintain TV or listening to records, conversing, talking on the
positive relationships with their peers, not only in pre- telephone, physical games, parties, and hanging out as
school, but thereafter as well. the most important contexts for peer interaction. An im-
As noted earlier, new statistical techniques are now portant aspect of this research is that these contexts
allowing researchers to examine the quantity, composi- were associated with different types of peer interaction.
tion, and stability of networks in the peer group. In a Noncompetitive activities facilitated socializing and the
series of studies, van den Oord and colleagues demon- development of relationships, whereas competitive ac-
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 593

tivities provided opportunities for identifying unique as- ate, are more skeptical of gossip than of first-hand infor-
pects of the self. According to Zarbatany et al. (1990), mation, and are likely to assume that gossipers spread
the full range of activities is necessary for early adoles- false information out of jealousy.
cents to derive broad benefits from peer experiences. Two additional forms of interaction have received
specific attention in the recent literature. Dishion, Mc-
Interaction
Cord, and Poulin (1999) coined the term deviancy train-
During middle childhood, verbal and relational aggres- ing to refer the processes of praise, encouragement,
sion (insults, derogation, threats, gossip) gradually re- imitation, and expectancy by which children increase
place direct physical aggression. Further, relative to the level of aggression or antisocial behavior in their
preschoolers, the aggressive behavior of 6- to 12-year- peers. Essentially, deviancy training occurs when chil-
olds is less frequently directed toward possessing ob- dren model and reward aggressive behaviors in each
jects or occupying specific territory and more other; the process by which these exchanges take place
specifically hostile toward others (Dodge, Coie, & is thought to increase individual tendencies in aggres-
Lynam, Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume). With siveness and to strengthen ties to aggressive and sub-
regard to positive social behavior, Eisenberg, Fabes and stance-abusing friends and delinquent peer groups. In
Spinrad (Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume) report this regard, deviancy training hits at all levels of the
the levels of generosity, helpfulness, or cooperation that social enterprise.
children direct to their peers increases somewhat during A form of interaction that affects internalizing prob-
the primary and middle school years. lems has been identified also. Rose (2002) has shown
The frequency of pretend or nonliteral aggres- that in the interactions of close friends, especially in
sion, or R&T play fits a U-shaped developmental func- the friendships of early adolescent girls, there can be a
tion (Pellegrini, 2002). Rough-and-tumble play pattern of interaction described as co-rumination in
comprises approximately 5% of preschoolers social ac- which negative thoughts and feelings are shared and
tivities. In early elementary school, the frequency of discussed. This joint focus on worries and negative ex-
R&T ranges from 10% to 17%, thereafter declining in periences appears more often in the friendships of
middle childhood and early adolescence to 5% young adolescent girls than boys and is associated with
(Humphreys & Smith, 1987). Interestingly, it has been internalizing problems for 12- to 14-year-olds, but not
proposed that the primary function of R&T, especially for 8- to 10-year-olds. Ruminative thoughts in individ-
among early adolescent boys, is to establish dominance uals and shared rumination among peers may play a
status and thereby delimit aggression among peers (Pel- role in sustaining or exacerbating problems of an inter-
legrini, 2002). Finally, by middle childhood, increases nalizing nature, thus this topic seems ripe for addi-
are found in the frequencies of games with or without tional study.
formal rules. In these latter activities, childrens inter- Yet another form of interaction emerging fully blown
actions with peers are highly coordinated, involving during middle childhood and early adolescence is bully-
both positive (cooperative, prosocial) and negative ing and victimization (Espelage, Bosworth, & Simon,
(competitive, agonistic) forms of behavior. 2000). Bullying refers to acts of verbal and physical ag-
Childrens concerns about acceptance in the peer gression on the part of an individual that are chronic and
group rise sharply during middle childhood, and these directed toward particular peers (victims). Bullying ac-
concerns appear related to an increase in the salience counts for a substantial portion of the aggression that
and frequency of gossip (Kuttler, Parker, & La Greca, occurs in the peer group (Olweus, 1978, 1993). The di-
2002). Gossip, at this age, reaffirms childrens member- mension that distinguishes bullying from other forms of
ship in important same-sex social groups and reveals, to aggressive behavior is its specificitybullies direct
its constituent members, the core attitudes, beliefs, and their behavior toward only certain peers, comprising ap-
behaviors comprising the basis for inclusion in or exclu- proximately 10% of the school population (National In-
sion from these groups. Thus, gossip may play a role in stitute of Child Health and Human Development Early
fostering friendship closeness and in promulgating chil- Child Care Research Network, 2001; Olweus, 1984).
drens social reputations. Kuttler et al. (2002) recently Research on bullying suggests that bullies are character-
reported that preadolescents label most talk about a non- ized by strong tendencies toward aggressive behavior,
present other as gossip and consider it to be inappropri- relatively weak control over their aggressive impulses,
594 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

and a tolerance for aggressive behavior (1978, 1993). promote these positive aspects of group functioning and
Further, Perry, Perry, and Kennedy (1992) noted that as a result they are victimized.
bullies are most likely to use force unemotionally and
Relationships
outside of an ongoing flow of conflict or interaction.
Also, bullies generally do not experience much resist- The period of middle childhood and early adolescence
ance to their aggressive acts. brings marked changes in childrens understanding of
Children who are greatest risk for victimization are friendship. To chart these changes, researchers have
those who have elevated scores on measures of aggres- asked children questions such as What is a best
sion or social withdrawal. Nearly every study that has friend? or What do you expect from a best friend?
assessed the association between aggressiveness and (Bigelow, 1977). Although children of all ages indicate
victimization has revealed a positive correlation (e.g., that a reciprocal giving-and-taking is necessary for
Camodeca, Goossens, Terwogt, & Schuengel, 2002; friendship (Hartup & Stevens, 1997), researchers have
Hanish & Guerra, 2004; Hodges, Malone, & Perry, shown that young childrens conceptions of a friend are
1997; Snyder et al., 2003). These findings appear to be anchored in the here and now, and not easily separated
culturally universal; thus victimization and aggression from social activity itself. Early school-age children
have been found to be positively associated in North have friendship concepts that transcend any specific ac-
American, Southern Asian (Khatri & Kupermidt, 2003) tivity, and imply the continuity of relationships over
and East Asian (Schwartz, Farver, Chang, & Lee-Shin, time. Nevertheless, during the early school years, chil-
2002) samples. When bullies direct their aggression to dren can still be instrumental and concrete in what they
other aggressive children, it facilitates a transactional view as a friendship or appropriate friendship behavior.
relationship that appears to facilitate the stability of ag- For example, Bigelows (1977) findings show that chil-
gression in the bully victim partners (Camodeca et al., drens friendship conceptions at the start of middle
2002; Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1997; Kochenderfer-Ladd childhood (7 to 8 years) involve rewards and costs
2003). Finally, there is evidence that anxious and so- friends are individuals who are rewarding to be with,
cially reticent children are victims of bullying behavior whereas nonfriends are individuals who are difficult or
(Hanish & Guerra, 2004; Kochenderfer-Ladd 2003; Ol- uninteresting to be with. For children of this age, a
weus, 1993). friend is someone who is convenient (i.e., who lives
As implied by Graham and Juvonen (2001) and nearby), has interesting toys or possessions, and shares
Schafer, Werner, and Crick (2002), victimization may the childs expectations about play activities. This con-
occur at multiple levels of social complexity, such as the ception evolves during middle childhood and early ado-
dyad (Crick & Nelson, 2002) or the group (Bukowski & lescence. By about 10 to 11 years, children recognize
Sippola, 2001). There are at least two explanations for the value of shared values and shared social understand-
the observation that aggression and social withdrawal ing. Friends at this age are expected to stick up for and
are associated with victimization. One explanation be loyal to one another. Later, at 11 to 13 years, children
notes that a withdrawn child is likely to be victimized acquire the view that friends share similar interests, are
because she or he is an easy and nonthreatening prey required to make active attempts to understand each
who is unlikely to retaliate when provoked (e.g., the other, and are willing to engage in self-disclosure.
construct of whipping boy; Olweus, 1978, 1993); al- According to Berndt (1996), children do not abandon
ternatively, an aggressive child is victimized because initial notions about play and mutual association when
his or her behavior is irritating and likely to provoke vic- they eventually recognize the importance of intimacy
timization from others ( the provocative victim; and loyalty. In support of this view, school-age chil-
Hodges et al., 1997; Olweus, 1993). According to this drens drawings of their friends show clearly that
view different mechanisms underlie victimization for friends are perceived as being similar to each other in
different types of children. Another view uses a single many observable ways while at the same time, they show
model to explain victimization. It claims that children their loyalty and closeness to each other (Pinto, Bombi,
victimize peers who do not promote the basic group & Cordoli, 1997). Moreover, even school-age children
goals of coherence, harmony, and evolution. According appear to recognize that while friends may share objec-
to this view, aggressive and withdrawn children do not tive experiences in their friendships they may be differ-
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 595

ent from each other in their subjective experiences (Lit- is also true in early childhood. For example, Ladd,
tle, Brendgen, Wanner, & Krappman, 1999). Kochenderfer, and Coleman (1996) found higher levels
Children draw sharper distinctions between the sup- of positive friendship qualities (e.g., validation) and
portiveness of friends and nonfriends with increasing lower levels of negative friendship qualities (e.g., low
age (Berndt & Perry, 1986). Moreover, childrens de- conflict) in stable friendships of kindergarteners, rela-
scriptions of their friendships indicate that loyalty, self- tive to unstable friendships. Furthermore, stable friend-
disclosure, and trust increase with age (Berndt, 2002), ships in middle childhood and early adolescence are
although these trends are more likely to be observed in more likely to comprise dyads in which the partners are
girls than in boys (Berndt & Perry, 1986; Strough, sociable and altruistic; friendships that dissolve during
Swenson, & Cheng, 2001). Older children of both sexes the course of a school year are more likely to comprise
also possess more intimate knowledge of their friends, partners who are aggressive and victimized by peers
describe their friends in a more differentiated and inte- (Hektner, August, & Realmuto, 2000; Wojslawowicz,
grated manner, and see their friendships as more exclu- Rubin, Burgess, Booth-LaForce, & Rose-Krasnor, in
sive and individualized (Berndt, 2002; Smollar & press). In addition, childrens liking for, and friendship
Youniss, 1982). involvement with, opposite-sex peers drops off precipi-
Significantly, there is little cross-cultural research on tously after 7 years of age (Leaper, 1994).
childrens understanding of friendship. Keller (2004a) Friendship dissolution may have a serious impact on
has recently questioned whether the notion of emotional childrens adjustment. For example, disruptions of close
intimacy that so characterizes friendship in Western peer relationships have been associated with depression,
cultures during the later years of childhood and beyond loneliness, physiological dysregulation, guilt, and anger
is also typical in non-Western societies. She notes that in (e.g., Laursen et al., 1996; Parker & Seal, 1996). In addi-
some cultures, especially in those that have subsistence tion, friendship loss in preadolescence (typically defined
economies, a primary function of friendship is instru- as the late years of middle or junior high school) may be
mental aid and not emotional support (see also Beer, particularly painful, due to the special role of friends
2001). Moreover, in comparing such Western countries loyalty during this developmental period (Buhrmester &
as Germany and the United States with non-Western Furman, 1987). Recently, for example, Wojslawowicz
China, Keller found that children in that latter culture et al. (in press) reported that 10- and 11-year-old chil-
emphasized moral issues and the importance of altruism dren who had a best friend at the beginning of the school
in their understanding of close friendships. Children in year but who lost that friendship and failed to replace it
the Western countries were more likely to emphasize re- by the end of the school year were at increased risk for
lationship intimacy (Keller, 2004b). Given these signifi- victimization by peers. Thus, it may be that if a dis-
cant differences in conceptions of friendship, it solved best friendship is not replaced, the advantages
behooves researchers to examine cultures beyond those of once having a best friend may quickly vanish.
investigated by Keller and colleagues. Indeed, it would Significantly, researchers have found that the lack of
seem important to study within-cultural /ethnic differ- a best friendship, whether at a given point in time or
ences as well (e.g., Way & Chen, 2000). chronically, can be accompanied by numerous risks.
Changes in the understanding of friendship are ac- Friendless children are more likely to be lonely and vic-
companied by changes in the patterns and nature of in- timized by peers (Boulton, Trueman, Chau, Whitehand,
volvement in friendships across middle childhood. & Amatya, 1999; Brendgen, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 2000;
Childrens friendship choices are more stable and more Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1997). Chronic friendlessness
likely to be reciprocated in middle childhood than at during childhood has been associated contemporane-
earlier ages, although it is not clear that either the reci- ously with social timidity, sensitivity, and the lack of
procity or stability of friendship increases during mid- social skills (Parker & Seal, 1996; Wojslawowicz et al.,
dle childhood (Berndt & Hoyle, 1985). Stability of in press), and predictively with subsequent internaliz-
friendships is thought to derive from the positive quali- ing problems (Ladd & Troop-Gordon, 2003). Relatedly,
ties of, and the positive interactions between, children. investigators have demonstrated that friendship can be
Friendships that are high in relationship quality are an important buffer for children; for example, Hodges
more likely to persist over time (Berndt, 2004), and this and colleagues (Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowksi,
596 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

1999) found that peer victimization predicted increases friends is that of conf lict. Research has shown repeat-
in internalizing and externalizing problems during the edly that after early childhood, pairs of friends engage
school year only for those children who lacked a mutual in about the same amount of conflict as pairs of non-
best friendship. friends (Laursen et al., 1996). There is, however, a
The protective function ascribed to friendship is con- major difference in the conflict resolution strategies
sistent with the view that close relationships function as that friends and nonfriends adopt. Friends are more
security systems (e.g., Sullivan, 1953). Hodges, Malone, concerned about achieving an equitable resolution to
and Perry (1997), for example, showed that children conflicts. More specifically, researchers report that
who are at risk for victimization because of their own friends are more likely than nonfriends to resolve con-
personal characteristics (i.e., being aggressive and/or flicts in a way that will preserve or promote the conti-
withdrawn) are less likely to experience victimization nuity of their relationship (see Laursen et al., 2001, for
in the peer group if they are also befriended rather than a recent review). Consistent with these findings,
friendless. In such studies, it is argued that individual friendship motives related to conflict resolution have
differences in victimization are associated with per- been observed to be associated with lower levels of
sonal characteristics but that these associations are anger and more constructive forms of behavior (B.
heightened by the lack of a friendship. Murphy & Eisenberg, 2002). However, the beneficial
With respect to the features of childrens friendships effects of friendship are qualified by the characteris-
in middle childhood and early adolescence, Newcomb tics of the best friend: Young adolescents with aggres-
and Bagwell (1995) reported that children are more sive friends, compared with those who have
likely to behave in positive ways with friends than non- nonaggressive friends, adopt increasingly aggressive
friends or to ascribe positive characteristics to their in- solutions to social conflicts; young adolescents who are
teractions with friends. Although the effect size of this nonaggressive and who have nonaggressive friends use
difference may, in some cases be small (Simpkins & more prosocial solutions to conflicts (Brendgen,
Parke, 2002), this pattern of findings is observed Bowen, Rondeau, & Vitaro, 1999). In this respect, ex-
across a broad range of studies using a variety of meth- perience in a best friendship is linked to the develop-
ods, including direct observations (e.g., Simpkins & ment of social competence; in the best friendship,
Parke, 2002), interviews (Berndt, Hawkins, & Hoyle, children and adolescents show a concern for a balance
1986), and hypothetical dilemmas (Rotenberg & Slitz, between individual and communal goals.
1988). More important, Newcomb and Bagwells There appear to be consistent qualitative differences
(1995) meta-analysis showed that the expression of af- in boys and girls best friendships in the middle child-
fect varied considerably for pairs of friends and non- hood and early adolescent years. For example, the
friends during middle childhood and early friendships of girls are marked by greater intimacy,
adolescence. In their interactions with friends, relative self-disclosure, and validation and caring than those of
to interaction with nonfriends, children show more af- boys (Leaper, 1994; Rubin, Dwyer, et al., 2004;
fective reciprocity and emotional intensity, and en- Zarbatany, McDougall, & Hymel, 2000). Ironically, it is
hanced levels of emotional understanding. Moreover, because of the intimacy of girls best friendships that
young adolescent friends use distraction to keep their they appear to be less stable and more fragile than those
friends from potentially harmful rumination about so- of boys (Benenson & Christakos, 2003; Hardy,
cial attributions that may induce guilt or shame (Den- Bukowski, & Sippola, 2002). Males best friendships are
ton & Zarbatany, 1996). In this regard, friendship is a characterized by physical activities that do not require
socially and positive relational context, and it provides the exchange of personal information. According to Be-
opportunities for the expression and regulation of af- nenson and Christakos, intimate disclosure between fe-
fect (Salisch, 2000). Consistent with the aforemen- male friends may become hazardous when best friends
tioned views of Sullivan (1953), it has been found that have a conflict. In such cases, the conflicting friends
these friend-nonfriend differences are stronger during can divulge personal information to outsiders (Crick &
early adolescence than during either middle childhood Grotpeter, 1995). Moreover, girls close friendships are
or during the preschool years. more likely to occur in isolation, whereas boys friend-
One of the few dimensions of interaction in which ships are more likely to occur in a larger social network
there are no differences between friends and non- (Baumeister & Sommer, 1997). Conflict resolution may
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 597

be aided by third party mediators and allies in the larger & Boivin, 2000). As Hartup and Abecassis (2002) put
group context. it: No evidence exists to suggest that opposites at-
As noted earlier, girls also report more co-rumination tract (p. 291).
(e.g., negative dwelling on emotionally charged and inti- Finally, as noted earlier, researchers have begun to
mate everyday occurrences and feelings), in their study enmity and mutual antipathies. Abecassis et al.
friendships than do boys (Rose, 2002). Significantly, (2002) have shown that rates vary across classrooms,
when childrens peer activities are marked by communal with the frequencies of dyadic enmity being as high as
rather than competitive/agentic activities, friendship in- 58% in some classrooms. Although mutual antipathies
timacy is higher. And when boys best friendships are are experienced by all children, they are most com-
with girls rather than boys, intimacy is higher, thus sug- mon among rejected children and they are more com-
gesting that there may be two different worlds of rela- mon among boys than girls, especially during middle
tionships defined by context and activity (Zarbatany childhood compared with adolescence (Rodkin &
et al., 2000). Hodges, 2003). But it is important to note that enmity
Thus far we have examined how children think about is not simply due to elevated levels of rejection. The
friendship and how they interact when with their best specific characteristics of particular pairs of ene-
friends. We have also described the stability of best mies appear to be connected to attachment-related
friendships during the middle childhood/early adoles- experiences (Hodges & Card, 2003). Children whose
cent period. But who is it that children are attracted to? attachment-related coping styles are incompatible
And with whom do they form best friendships? Just as (e.g., one has an avoidant style and the other is preoc-
is the case with young children, older children and cupied) are more likely to become enemies than are
young adolescents are drawn to others who are like other children.
them. Throughout this age period, children are attracted The developmental significance of mutual an-
to and become best friends with those who resemble tipathies is unclear. Children in such relationships tend
them in age, sex, ethnicity, and behavioral status to be more depressed than are other children, and the
(Hartup & Abecassis, 2002). For example, it has been presence of a mutual antipathy appears to exacerbate the
reported that children and young adolescents are at- effect of other negative experiences. Nevertheless, par-
tracted to peers whose behavioral tendencies are similar ticipating in the process of mutual disliking may be one
to their own (Rubin, Lynch, Coplan, Rose-Krasnor, & means by which young people develop a clearer sense of
Booth, 1994). Hamm (2000), for example, showed that self as they identify the characteristics that they like
similarity on a particular dimension varied across chil- and dislike in others.
dren largely due to the importance the child ascribed it. Many issues related to the study of mutual an-
Similarity to their friend on academic performance was tipathies require further exploration. Perhaps the most
highest among children who saw academic performance important concerns the issue of how we define and mea-
as important. sure the concept of enemy. Just as mutual liking is sim-
Researchers in both Western and Eastern cultures ply the minimum criterion for friendship, mutual
have reported that greater behavioral similarities exist disliking must be considered the minimum criterion for
between friends than nonfriends, and children share the presence of enmity. To paraphrase the important dis-
friendships with other children who resemble them- cussions provided by Hartup and Abecassis (2002),
selves in terms of prosocial and antisocial behaviors having an enemy implies warfare. Consequently, re-
(e.g., Haselager, Hartup, van Lieshout, & Riksen-Wal- searchers would do well to examine whether children
raven, 1998; Liu & Chen, 2003; Poulin & Boivin, who nominate each other as Someone I do not like, ac-
2000), shyness and internalized distress (e.g., Hogue & tually interact. It may be that mutual antipathies merely
Steinberg, 1995; Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Burgess, capture an affective dimension, not an interactional one.
Booth-LaForce, & Rose-Krasnor, in press), sociability, True enemies may be proactive about their relation-
peer popularity, and academic achievement and moti- ship. They may spread gossip about one another and en-
vation (Altermatt & Pomerantz, 2003; Liu & Chen, gage in relational or other forms of aggression. They
2003). Children also dislike those who are different may be members of different identifiable groups, each
from themselves and terminate relationships with of which exclude the other (research on peer exclusion
those who are behaviorally unlike themselves (Poulin may be particularly relevant, e.g., Horn, 2003; Killen &
598 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Stangor, 2001; Killen, Stangor, Horn, & Sechrist, 2004; Thus, in recent studies of preadolescents conducted in
Killen et al., 2002). At present, there are virtually no both Western (e.g., Canada, Finland, United States) and
data indicating how and whether those who mutually Eastern (e.g., China) cultures, group membership has
nominate each other as Someone I do not like actually been found to comprise children similar with regard to
have a clearly defined relationship. As a result, re- the following characteristics: aggression (Espelage
searchers must be careful about how they define and et al., 2003; Gest et al., 2003; Kiesner et al., 2003; Xie
measure the presence of enmity. et al., 1999), bullying (e.g., Salmivalli, Huttunen, &
Lagerspetz, 1997), attitudes about bullying (Espelage
Groups et al., 2003; Salmivalli & Voeten, 2004), and school mo-
During the upper elementary school and middle school tivation and performance (e.g., Chen et al., 2003; Kin-
years, the structure of the peer group changes from a dermann, 1993; Liu & Chen, 2003; Ryan, 2001; Sage &
relatively unified whole to a more differentiated struc- Kindermann, 1999).
ture. In this new structure, children organize themselves Apart from cliques, the other primary organizational
into social groups, clusters, networks, or cliques (Bag- feature of childrens groups in middle childhood and
well, Coie, Terry, & Lochman, 2000; Degirmencioglu, early adolescence is the popularity hierarchy. There
Urberg, Tolson, & Richard, 1998). Peer networks and have been recent attempts to distinguish between socio-
cliques are voluntary, friendship-based groups, and metric popularity and perceived popularity. In the case
stand in contrast to the activity or work groups to which of sociometric popularity or peer acceptance, the ques-
children can be assigned by circumstance or by adults. tions asked of children are Who do you most like? and
Cliques generally include three to nine same-sex chil- Who do you most dislike? (see following for details
dren of the same race (Chen, Chang, & He, 2003; Kin- about assessment). In the case of perceived popularity,
dermann, McCollom, & Gibson, 1995). By 11 years of the child is asked who he or she believes is the most pop-
age, children report that most of their peer interaction ular in the classroom, grade, or school (Parkhurst &
takes place in the context of the clique, and nearly all Hopmeyer, 1998; LaFontana & Cillessen, 1998, 2002).
children report being a member of one. Unwittingly, these efforts follow Northways (1946) as-
With respect to group size, boys, compared with sertion that being accepted and being popular are differ-
girls, show a preference for larger groups (Benenson, ent phenomena that have different antecedents and
Apostoleris, & Parnass, 1997). It may be that this sex different consequences. Whereas being liked or ac-
difference has functional significance. Specifically, in- cepted occurs at the dyadic level (i.e., one person has af-
teraction in smaller groups is less likely than experience fection for someone else), the perception of someone as
in large groups to promote competitiveness and self- being popular in a classroom or school reflects a group
criticism (Benenson, Nicholson, Waite, Roy, & Simp- level of analysis (i.e., the person is perceived according
son, 2001). Because girls are more likely than boys to to her/ his position in the group). Thus, in the study of
have experience in small groups, they may have fewer peer group relationships, the word (and traditional mea-
experiences with competition than boys have. This dif- surement of ) acceptance is most properly taken as a di-
ference could explain why girls tend to be less competi- rect assessment of the extent to which a child is liked by
tive than boys (Roy & Benenson, 2002) and why they her/ his peers, whereas the word popularity refers to a
feel less comfortable with competition (Benenson et al., childs perceived standing or status in the group.
2002). This avoidance of competition in favor of more Recently, researchers have focused on the study of
egalitarian strategies may explain why girls experience the peer relationship correlates of such negative charac-
greater fragility in their same-sex friendships (Benen- teristics such as aggression, bossiness, and untrustwor-
son & Christakos, 2003). thiness to clarify the distinction between the meanings
Peer networks, whether identified observationally and measurement of peer acceptance and perceived pop-
(e.g., Gest, Farmer, Cairns, & Xie, 2003) or via peer re- ularity. Thus, for example, in contradiction to the gen-
ports (e.g., Bagwell et al., 2000), or whether identified eral finding that aggression impedes a childs
in or out of school (Kiesner, Poulin, & Nicotra, 2003), acceptance among peers, aggression appears to promote
are typically organized to maximize within-group ho- a childs perceived popularity (Buskirk, Rubin, Burgess,
mogeneity (Rodkin, Farmer, Pearl, & Van Acker, 2000). Booth-LaForce, & Rose-Krasnor, 2004; Hawley, Little,
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 599

& Pasupathi, 2002; Lease, Kennedy, & Axelrod, 2002). Three final points must be made. First, consistent
Research regarding the association between aggression terminology is a prerequisite for learned discussion. As
and popularity is approached according to basic aspects the distinction between how much a child is liked and
of group process such as dominance, resource control, how popular a child is becomes more frequent, the clar-
and regulation of retaliatory gestures between group ity of the terms used to refer to these constructs be-
members (Hawley, 2003). Findings show that children comes increasingly important. The word acceptance
whose level of aggression is moderately above the mean should be used to refer to a direct assessment of the ex-
and who use aggression for instrumental reasons are tent to which a child is liked by her/ his peers, whereas
perceived as more popular in their groups than are chil- the word popularity should be used to refer to a persons
dren who are low in aggression or whose aggression is place in the peer group. The meaning of acceptance cap-
high and undifferentiated (Hawley, 2003; Little, Jones, tures the essence of the construct it refers to (i.e., the
Henrich, & Hawley, 2003; Prinstein & Cillessen, 2003; extent to which a person is received with favor or ap-
Vaughn at al., 2003). proval by others). The word popularity is also ideally
Although the association between aggression and suited to the construct it is used to represent. Popularity,
popularity may be seen even during the preschool period by definition, refers to someones position or status
(Vaughn et al., 2003), this association appears to be among the people. In this respect it is essentially a
stronger during early adolescence (Cillessen & Mayeux, group-oriented construct.
2004; Prinstein & Cillessen, 2003). Whereas aggression Second, the traditional measures of popularity
is positively associated with measures of popularity dur- have been sociometric measures. Sociometry refers to
ing early adolescence (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004; La- the attractions and repulsions between individuals. To
Fontana & Cillessen, 1998, 2002; Parkhurst & the extent that a measure of perceived popularity is nei-
Hopmeyer, 1998), it is not related to acceptance ther a measure of attraction nor repulsion, it is not a so-
(Buskirk et al., 2004). Moderately aggressive children ciometric measure. Third, most of the research on
may be given status and power in the peer group; how- acceptance and popularity has been empirically driven.
ever, this does not mean they are adjusted or that they Because researchers have been largely interested in
will receive or benefit from the affection or kindness identifying the different correlates of these constructs,
from their peers. little direct attention has been devoted to understanding
These findings are consistent with ideas about how the conceptual differences between these constructs by
groups function and how groups reward persons who their psychological or functional significance. Lease and
promote the groups functioning (see Bukowski & Sip- her colleagues have gone furthest in discussing the link
pola, 2001). Whereas the main reward that one can pro- between popularity and power in the peer group (Lease,
vide at the level of the dyad is affection, the main Kennedy, et al., 2002; Lease, Musgrove, & Axelrod,
rewards that can be provided at the level of the group 2002). Their discussions of how power and status are
are power, attention, and status. And whereas group fused in the construct of popularity provide a strong base
members victimize peers who impede the groups evo- for further exploration of how peer groups function and
lution and coherence, groups give power, attention, and of how their dynamics are controlled by particular peers.
status to group members who promote the groups well-
being. Given that group leaders may, at times, have to
be forceful, strong, assertive, indeed Machiavellian, Adolescence
their behavior may include a larger coercive or aggres-
Interaction
sive component than is seen among other children. This
tendency to ascribe power and status to moderately ag- The trend of spending increasingly substantial amounts
gressive individuals may be more pronounced in adoles- of time with peers that begins in middle childhood con-
cence when aggression is seen as a more normative tinues in adolescence (Larson, Brown, & Mortimer,
entity than among younger children (Moffitt, 1993). As 2002). For example, during a typical week, even dis-
a result, status, leadership, and aggression may often go counting time spent in classroom instruction, high
together especially for young adolescents (Prinstein & school students spend almost one-third (29%) of their
Cillessen, 2003). waking hours with peers, an amount more than double
600 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

that spent with parents and other adults (13%; Csik- interaction. Unlike at earlier ages, self-disclosure dur-
szentmihalyi & Larson, 1984). Moreover, adolescent ing adolescence prompts lengthy and sometimes emo-
peer interaction takes place with less adult guidance and tional discussions about the nature of the problem and
control than peer interaction in middle childhood, and is possible avenues to its resolution.
more likely to involve individuals of the opposite-sex During adolescence, boys and girls have clear con-
(Brown & Klute, 2003). These phenomena are largely ceptions of the properties that distinguish romantic rela-
consistent across cultural groups. tionships from friendships (Collins, 2003; Connolly,
Craig, Goldberg, & Pepler, 2004). Whereas romantic re-
Relationships lationships are conceived in terms of passion and com-
As they enter adolescence, both boys and girls already mitment, other-sex friendships are largely characterized
understand a great deal about the reciprocal operations by affiliation. Although even the youngest of adoles-
and obligations of friendship, about the continuity of cents distinguish between romantic relationships and
friendships, and about the psychological grounds that other-sex friendships, distinctions between these rela-
evoke behavior. During early adolescence, friendship tionships increase with age and with experience in ro-
can be seen in overly exclusive terms in the sense that mantic relationships. The study of adolescent romantic
relationships with third parties are inimical to the basic relationships by developmental psychologists is a sur-
nature of friendship commitment. During adolescence, prisingly new enterprise with nearly all research on this
however, youngsters begin to accept the others need to topic stemming from the past 10 years. Relevant re-
establish relationships with others and to grow through search is organized around three questions: (1) When do
such experiences. In particular, perhaps in parallel to these relationships first emerge and for whom do they
their struggles for independence from their parents, occur? (2) What are the characteristics of these rela-
adolescents recognize an obligation to grant friends a tionships and what accounts for individual differences
certain degree of autonomy and independence. Thus, in their quality? (3) How do romantic relationships af-
their discussions of friendship and friendship issues fect development?
show fewer elements of possessiveness and jealousy, and With regard to developmental timing, romantic re-
more concern with how the relationship helps the part- lationships are first seen during early adolescence
ners enhance their respective self-identities (Berndt & with approximately 25% of 12-year-olds claiming they
Hoyle, 1985). have had a romantic relationship during the past 18
During adolescence, friendships are best maintained months (Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003). This fre-
when the partners have similar attitudes, aspirations, quency increases in a largely linear fashion during
and intellect (e.g., Smollar & Youniss, 1982). Based on adolescence with roughly 70% of boys and 75% of
this perspective, it appears that children who are dif- girls making this claim at age 18 (Carver et al., 2003;
ferent from the other boys and girls in the group are Seiffge-Krenke, 2003). The average duration of a ro-
those who are less likely to have a friend. Nonetheless, mantic relationship has been observed to be 3.9
same-sex friends account for an increasingly larger months at age 13, and 11.8 months at age 17 months
proportion of adolescents perceived primary social (Seiffge-Krenke, 2003).
network, and friends equal or surpass parents as Dating and romantic relationships appear to follow a
sources of support and advice to adolescents in many developmental sequence. Connolly et al. (2004) showed
significant domains (e.g., Buhrmester, 1998; Furman that affiliation in mixed-sex groups and dating were
& Buhrmester, 1992). Moreover, the friendships of qualitatively different phenomena that were sequen-
adolescents are relatively stable (Berndt, Hawkins, tially organized. This sequential order followed a path
et al., 1986). that started with same-sex friendships and moved
One hallmark of friendship in adolescence is its em- through an affiliative period of mixed-sex group activ-
phasis on intimacy and self-disclosure. Studies consis- ities and mixed-sex festive occasions (e.g., parties),
tently indicate that adolescents report greater levels of followed by dating and being involved in a roman-
intimacy in their friendships than do younger children tic dyad.
(Buhrmester & Furman, 1986). Furthermore, observa- There are large differences between those adoles-
tions of adolescent friends indicate that intimate self- cents who do and do not participate in romantic rela-
disclosure is a highly salient feature of friendship tionships. These differences vary during the adolescent
Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: A Developmental Perspective 601

period and they are often characterized by complex pat- lationships. Perceived negative interactions in the three
terns (Collins, 2003). Early involvement in romantic re- types of relationships were also significantly associated
lationships has been linked to problem behaviors and with each other. This pattern of results indicates greater
emotional difficulties during adolescence (e.g., Davila, generalizability of negative than positive features across
Steinberg, Kachadourian, Cobb, & Fincham, 2004), al- relationship types.
though this difference appears to be strongest among Romantic relationships also appear to have both pos-
boys and girls who are unpopular among their same-sex itive and negative effects on development although the
peers (Brendgen, Vitaro, Doyle, Markiewicz, & literature on these matters is not yet clear. Whereas
Bukowski, 2002). It has been reported also that early there is evidence that participation in romantic relation-
daters show lower levels of scholastic achievement ships can be associated with elevated levels of de-
(Seiffge-Krenke, 2003), especially among girls (Brend- pressed mood, higher levels of conflict, and emotional
gen et al., 2002). Among older adolescents, however, lability (Joyner & Udry, 2000), these findings appear to
participation in romantic relationships is associated be the result of breakdowns of romance rather than of
with positive experiences among same-sex peers and romance per se. On the positive side, being involved in a
emotional and behavioral well-being (Seiffge-Krenke, romantic relationship indirectly affects the adolescents
2003). Connolly, Furman, and Konarski (2000) re- sense of well-being via its direct effects on his or her
ported that being part of a small group of close same- sense of romantic competence (Kuttler, La Greca, &
sex friends predicted being involved in other-sex peer Prinstein, 1999).
networks, which, in turn, predicted the emergence of
future romantic relationships. The observation that in- Groups
volvement in a romantic relationship is linked to accep- As in middle childhood, cliques are readily observed in
tance with same-sex peers resembles prior findings adolescence, and membership in cliques is related to
regarding same-sex acceptance and other-sex friend- adolescents psychological well-being and ability to
ship. These findings, however, have shown that partici- cope with stress (Hansell, 1981). Also, as in middle
pation in friendship with other-sex peers is linked to childhood and early adolescence, group membership
same-sex acceptance in a linear and a curvilinear man- comprises individuals who are similar with regard to
ner in which children who are most liked by same-sex school achievement (Kindermann, 1995), substance use
peers, and those who are least liked, have other-sex (cigarettes and alcohol; Urberg, Degirmencioglu, & Pil-
friends (Bukowski, Sippola, & Hoza, 1999; Kovacs, grim, 1997), and delinquency (Kiesner et al., 2003).
Parker, & Hoffman, 1996). There is evidence also that Nevertheless, Shrum and Cheek (1987) reported a
the quality of a childs same-sex friendships predicts sharp decline from 11 to 18 years of age in the propor-
the quality of their concurrent and subsequent romantic tion of students who were clique members and a corre-
relationships (Connolly et al., 2000). Future research sponding increase with age in the proportion of children
needs to clarify whether this pattern of findings regard- who had ties to many cliques or children whose primary
ing romantic relationships is equally valid for hetero- ties were to other children who existed at the margins of
and homosexual youth. one or more cliques. These authors concluded that there
Although there appears to be some inter-relatedness is a general loosening of clique ties across adolescence,
between romantic relationships and other relationship a process they label degrouping. This interpretation
experiences, this association is often complex. Using an meshes well with data suggesting that both the impor-
attachment framework, Furman, Simon, Shaffer, and tance of belonging to a group and the extent of inter-
Bouchey (2002) studied adolescents internal working group antagonism decline steadily during high school
models for their relationships with parents, friends, and years (Gavin & Furman, 1989). It is consistent also
romantic partners. Adolescents perceived support in re- with the observations of ethnographers, who report a
lationships with their parents tended to be related to dissipation of clique boundaries and a sense of cohe-
their perceived support in romantic relationships and siveness among senior high school class members
friendships; support in friend and romantic relation- (Larkin, 1979).
ships, however, were not related to each other. Neverthe- Whereas cliques represent small groups of individu-
less, self and other controlling behaviors in friendships als linked by friendship selections, the concept of peer
were related to corresponding behaviors in romantic re- subcultures, or crowds (Brown & Klute, 2003), is a
602 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

more encompassing organizational framework for seg- The percentage of students who are able to correctly
menting adolescent peer social life. A crowd is a reputa- identify their peer-rated crowd membership increases
tion-based collective of similarly stereotyped with age (Brown, Clasen, & Neiss, 1987). An abbreviated
individuals who may or may not spend much time to- list of crowds (populars, jocks, brains, burnouts, noncon-
gether. Crowds are defined by the primary attitudes or formists, and none) used by Prinstein and La Greca
activities their members share. Thus, crowd affiliation (2002) revealed that self-nominations to groups over-
is assigned through the consensus of the peer group and lapped strongly with findings from peer assignments.
is not selected by the adolescents themselves. Brown Despite the differences that exist in the structures of
(1989) listed the following as common crowd labels peer groups, all of them inevitably disintegrate in the
among American high school students: jocks, brains, late adolescent years. This is largely due to the integra-
eggheads, loners, burnouts, druggies, populars, nerds, tion of the sexes that accompanies this period. To begin
and greasers. Crowds place important restrictions on with, mixed-sex cliques emerge. Eventually, the larger
childrens social contacts and relationships with peers groups divide into couples, and by late adolescence, girls
(Brown, 1989); for example, cliques are generally and boys feel comfortable enough to approach one an-
formed within (versus across) crowds. Crowd labels may other directly without the support of the clique. Another
also constrain adolescents abilities to change their contributing factor to the decline in importance of
lifestyles or explore new identities by channeling crowds results from adolescents creating their own per-
them into relationships and dating patterns with those sonal values and morals. In this regard, they no longer
sharing the same crowd nomenclature or classification see it as necessary to broadcast their membership in a
(Eckert, 1989). particular social group and are therefore content to be
Crowd membership is an especially salient feature of separate and apart from particular crowds.
adolescent social life and childrens perceptions of
crowds change in important ways with age. For example, Conclusion
between the ages of 13 and 16 years, adolescents alter
In this section, we have outlined developmental differ-
the ways that they identify and describe the crowds in
ences that mark the changing nature of social interac-
their school (OBrien & Bierman, 1987). Whereas
tions and peer relationships from infancy to
young adolescents focus on the specific behavioral pro-
adolescence. Hopefully, this review will prove sufficient
clivities of group members, older adolescents center on
to provide a normative basis for the discussion that fol-
members dispositional characteristics and values. This
lows concerning the development of individual differ-
observation reflects broader changes that characterize
ences in childrens social behaviors and peer
developmental shifts in person perception between the
relationships.
childhood and adolescent years.
The nature of childrens peer experiences changes
The stigma that is placed on members of a particular
with age because of a complex mix of developments with
crowd channels adolescents into relationships and dating
regard to intrapersonal (i.e., changes in interpersonal
patterns with those sharing a similar crowd label. This
understanding and concerns), interpersonal (changes in
may prevent adolescents from the exploration of new
the frequency or forms of specific behaviors), dyadic
identities and discourages a shift to other crowd mem-
(changes in qualities of friendships or patterns of in-
berships. There is recent evidence that the stigma asso-
volvement in friendships), and group (changes in config-
ciated with some large peer groups or crowds influences
urations of and involvement in cliques and crowds)
the judgments that adolescents form about their peers
factors. Furthermore, these different factors are not or-
(Horn, 2003). Consistent with findings from research
thogonal; rather, they interlock in complex ways.
focused on childrens aggressive reputations and social
cognitions (e.g., Dodge, 1986), Horn (2003) found that
adolescents are biased in their use of reputational or SOCIAL BEHAVIORS, INTERACTIONS,
stereotypical information about particular groups, par- RELATIONSHIPS, AND GROUPS:
ticularly when presented with ambiguous situations. It ASSESSMENT ISSUES
is likely that these crowd-specific evaluations help to
perpetuate group stereotypes and the structure of peer The perspective we have adopted for this chapter as-
groups in a school. sumes that childrens experiences with peers occur at
Social Behaviors, Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Assessment Issues 603

several orders of social complexityfrom interactions Conner, 2003). In addition, overt and relational aggres-
to relationships to groups. Implied in such a formulation sion, R&T play, unoccupied and onlooker behaviors, and
is that these levels of analyses provide separate windows conversations with peers are recorded. Observational
on the adjustment of individual children with peers: To procedures such as the POS are useful in targeting chil-
the extent that individual differences exist in childrens dren whose behaviors (e.g., different forms of aggres-
adaptation or success with peers, such differences will sion and social withdrawal) deviate from age-group
be reflected in their (a) social interactions, ( b) abilities norms. Such procedures can be used to validate peer and
to develop and sustain friendships, and (c) acceptance in teacher assessments of childrens social behavior.
peer groups. We examine procedures by which re- Additional observational protocols assess appropriate
searchers assess peer interactions, relationships, and and inappropriate behavior (e.g., M. L. Lewin, Davis, &
groups in this section. Hops, 1999), social competence (e.g., Vaughn et al.,
2003), peer group entry (e.g., Putallaz & Gottman,
Childrens Behaviors and Interactions with Peers 1981), multiple forms of aggression (e.g., Bierman,
Smoot, & Aumiller, 1993), how existing peer dyads re-
Although parents, clinicians, and archival data have all spond to newcomers (Zarbatany, Van Brunschot, Mead-
served as sources of information about the valence and ows, & Pepper, 1996), adolescent conversation and
nature of childrens peer interactions, the most common discussion (e.g., Hops, Albert, & Davis, 1997), and
sources are the reports of other children or teachers or group planning (Englund, Levy, Hyson, & Sroufe, 2000).
structured observations. Although observational methods offer many advan-
tages over the assessments discussed next, they also
Observations of Behavior
have specific limitations. First, observations are time-,
There has been a long tradition of observing children in energy-, and money-consuming. Whereas peer and
either naturalistic or laboratory-based play groups and teacher assessments can be conducted in minutes or
then coding their behavior to reflect particular con- hours, observations can require weeks or months of data
structs. For example, observational procedures have been collection. Second, as children get older, it becomes in-
used to index the frequency with which individuals en- creasingly difficult to observe them during free play
gage in particular behavioral styles (e.g., aggression, so- (although recent advances in remote audio-visual
ciodramatic play, reticence/social wariness, or sharing), recording allow observations of childrens conversations
adopt particular roles in relation to their partners (e.g., and interactions from afar; Atlas & Pepler, 1998).
dominant versus submissive roles), or demonstrate so- Third, situational demands strongly influence the types
cial competence (e.g., are successful at entering play- of behaviors displayed and their frequency. Unless re-
groups). Several well-known coding systems have been searchers carefully consider or control how subjects be-
developed for these purposes, and discussions of these havior is being influenced by setting demands,
techniques can be found elsewhere (e.g., Bierman, observation methods can lead to false conclusions of the
2003). These coding schemes have been used profitably willingness of certain individuals to engage in behaviors
to reliably distinguish between children along a variety of interest. Fourth, observations may be reactive; for ex-
of behavioral dimensions. For example, Rubin (2001) de- ample, children who are aware that they are being ob-
veloped the Play Observation Scale (POS), a norm- served may behave in atypical manners, perhaps
based time-sampling procedure to assess free play suppressing negative behaviors or increasing the produc-
behaviors in early and middle childhood. During free tion of prosocial behaviors. Finally, it should be men-
play or unrestricted activity time (in a classroom, on a tioned that observational strategies have been used
playground, or in a laboratory playroom), behaviors with rarely to study peer interactions and relationships from
and without peers are coded on a checklist that includes a cultural and cross-cultural perspective.
the cognitive play categories of functional-sensorimotor,
exploratory, constructive, dramatic, and games-with- Peer Assessments of Social Behavior
rules behaviors nested in the aforementioned social par- In lieu of direct observations, researchers have often re-
ticipation categories of solitary, parallel, and group lied on children for information about who it is in the
activities (e.g., Coplan, Gavinski-Molina, Lagace- peer group that behaves competently or incompetently,
Seguin, & Wichmann, 2001; Guralnick, Hammond, & or has qualitatively good or poor relationships. Hymel
604 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

and Rubin (1985) noted the following advantages of (e.g., Bowker, Bukowski, Zargarpour, & Hoza, 1998;
peer informants. First, as insiders, peers can identify Rubin & Mills, 1988) and between different forms of
characteristics of children and relationships that are aggression (Crick & Bigbee, 1998; Poulin & Boivin,
considered relevant from the perspectives of those who 1999). In the case of aggression, peer assessment proce-
ultimately determine a childs social status and integra- dures to distinguish between physical aggression (fight-
tion in the peer group. Second, the judgments of peers ing, kicking, hitting), verbal aggression (threats,
are based on many extended and varied experiences teasing), and relational aggression (spreading rumors,
with those being evaluated. For example, peers may be excluding from play) have been developed. Recently,
able to consider low frequency but psychologically sig- Burgess and colleagues (Burgess, Wojslawowicz, Rubin,
nificant events (e.g., a punch in the nose or taking some- Rose-Krasnor, & Booth, 2003) developed a reliable and
ones valued possession) that lead to the establishment valid extension of the Revised Class Play to distinguish
and maintenance of particular social reputations. These between forms of social withdrawal, as well as to mea-
latter events may be unknown to nonpeer outsiders. sure sociability, prosocial /altruistic behavior, and vic-
Third, peer assessments of childrens behaviors and re- timization. Embedded items assess peer acceptance,
lationships represent the perspectives of many ob- perceived popularity, and rejection.
servers with whom the target child has had a variety of Like the Revised Class Play, the PEI (Pekarik et al.,
personal relationships. The chance that error will be in- 1976) has undergone revision. Pope, Bierman, and
troduced by some idiosyncratic aspect of any single Mumma (1991) condensed the original scales and
reporters experience with the child is therefore corre- added items describing inattentive/immature and dis-
spondingly reduced. ruptive/ hyperactive behaviors. By so doing, Pope et al.
In most peer assessment techniques, children are provided an instrument that could distinguish aggres-
given a set of target behaviors or personality descrip- sive children who are rejected from those who are ac-
tions and asked to nominate peers on the basis of a vari- cepted by peers.
ety of behavioral roles or character descriptions (e.g., Peer behavioral assessment assumes that childrens
is a good leader, gets into fights, or likes to play impressions of one another are established over time. In-
alone). Nominations received from peers are summed deed, it has long been assumed that a major advantage of
in various ways to provide indices of a childs typical so- this technique is that it permits researchers to identify
cial behavior or reputation in the peer group, whether children who engage in behaviors that are salient to
that group comprises a classroom or school grade. other children but too infrequent or too subtle for re-
Two commonly used peer assessment techniques are searchers to observe with any reliability. But a disad-
the Revised Class Play (Masten, Morrison, & Pellegrini, vantage of peer assessments is that once behavioral
1985) and the Pupil Evaluation Inventory (PEI; Pekarik, reputations consolidate they can be resistant to change
Prinz, Liebert, Weintraub, & Neale, 1976). Factor (Hymel, 1986). Thus, even though a childs behavior
analysis of childrens nominations using these two mea- may have changed, their reputation for this behavior per-
sures has yielded three similar behavioral factors. For sists with peers. As such, the data reaching the re-
the PEI, the factors obtained were Likeability, Aggres- searcher may not fully reflect the current state of
sion, and Withdrawal. The factors obtained for the Re- reality. In addition, reputations are probably unduly
vised Class Play are labeled Sociability-Leadership, influenced by infrequent but salient events (e.g., embar-
Aggressive-Disruptive, and Sensitive-Isolated. rassing social gaffs or poignant aggressive outbursts).
Recent advances in the use of peer assessments have Although characteristic of the child, the childs reputa-
provided a more refined articulation of the dimensions tion for this behavior may overstate the frequency with
underlying childrens social behavior. Thus, Zeller, Van- which it appears in his or her social interchanges. Relat-
natta, Schafer, and Noll (2003) computed a confirma- edly, there is evidence that childrens recall of their
tory factor analysis for the Revised Class Play and peers abilities and behavior is affected by their own be-
discovered that the model that best fit the data needed to havioral reputation, level of peer status, age, and liking
be substantially more differentiated than a simple three for the target; situational factors; and the targets gen-
factor model would imply. In a sense, this result is not der, age, and sociometric status (e.g., Hymel, 1986). Fi-
surprising given previous analyses that have distin- nally, a main challenge to the study of peer assessments
guished between different forms of social withdrawal is the potential variance across cultural contexts in the
Social Behaviors, Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Assessment Issues 605

organization of social constructs (see Bukowski & Sip- conditions (e.g., pairs of teachers; pairs of parents); .28
pola, 2001). Because social demands and practices may between different types of informants seeing the child
vary across cultures, childrens representations of social under different conditions (e.g., parents versus teach-
constructs may vary also. The cross-cultural research of ers); and .22 between childrens self-reports and reports
Chen and colleagues is particularly relevant in this re- by others, including parents, teachers and mental health
gard and is discussed later (e.g., Chen, Cen, Li, & He, workers. Age, sex, and the specific topography of the be-
2005; Chen, Rubin, Li, & Li, 1999; Chen et al., 2004). havior under consideration have all been shown to be im-
portant factors influencing agreement. For example,
Teacher Assessments
agreement between teachers and peers concerning social
Teachers can provide useful data concerning low fre- withdrawal appears to increase with age from early to
quency social exchanges that may contribute toward the late childhood (Hymel, Rubin, Rowden, & LeMare,
quality of a childs peer relationships. One advantage of 1990), primarily because social withdrawal takes on in-
teacher assessments over peer assessments is that the creased salience to peers ( but not teachers) with increas-
collection of data is more efficient and less time con- ing age. Thus, it would appear as if no single source can
suming. A second advantage is that, because they them- substitute for all the others. The goal is not to determine
selves are not members of the peer group, teachers may which assessment procedure yields the singular truth
be more objective in their assessments of social behav- about the child but to use what each one reveals about the
ior. However, teachers may bring with them an adulto- childs functioning in particular areas or contexts.
morphic perspective that carries with it value
judgments about social behaviors that might differ from
Childrens Relationships with Friends
those of children. Furthermore, teachers may carry with
them biases that influence the ways in which they react Friendship is a subjective relationship and an inherently
to their pupils; such teacher reactions may strongly in- dyadic construct. Children perceive their friendship
fluence childrens peer preferences and judgments partners in particularized rather than role-related ways.
(White & Kistner, 1992). They stress the uniqueness of the relationship and reject
Teacher referrals are one source of data on children efforts to treat particular friendship partners as inter-
with behavioral difficulties. Many objections might be changeable with others. Researchers and other ob-
leveled against this approach, however. In the first place, servers may note commonalities in personalities or
teachers refer children for academic behaviors (e.g., behavioral tendencies across the friendships of a focal
learning disabilities or motivational problems) that may child, but the focal child him- or herself is likely to be
have only minor implications for social difficulties with impressed by the distinctions and diversity among his or
peers. Second, even when problematic behavior toward her individual partners and relationships.
peers is the basis for referral, it is not clear that such re- These subjective and reciprocal properties are chal-
ferrals will take place when the behavior is not also dis- lenges to understanding and require special caution in
ruptive of classroom routines and academic progress. assessment. In early childhood, it is common to ask par-
Many standardized measures presently exist and an ents or teachers to identify whether a child is a friend of
excellent review of teacher ratings of child behavior may another child (Howes, 1988). Typically, researchers do
be found in Bierman (2003). Generally, these measures not give these informants specific criteria by which the
can be broken down into several socioemotional clusters presence of a friendship should be determined. Instead,
or factors that fall along dimensions of sociability/ like- it is often simply assumed that these informants share
ability/ leadership, aggression / hostility/conduct disor- the researchers definition of friendship, which may or
der, hyperactivity/impulsivity, and anxiety/fearfulness/ may not always be the case. In the assessment of the
withdrawal. friendships of older elementary school-age children, the
focal childs perceptions of his or her circle of friends
Agreement among Sources
must be sought and aligned with independent evidence
Achenbach, McConaughy, and Howell (1987) reported of reciprocity of affection obtained directly or indi-
that the correlations between reports of childrens be- rectly from each of these implicated individuals (Asher
havioral problems average about .60 between similar in- et al., 1996; Bukowski & Hoza, 1989). Typically,
formants seeing children under generally similar preschoolers and elementary school-age children are
606 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

presented with a roster or a set of pictures of their same- not spend their school time in a single class comprising
sex classmates (or some other functionally similar the same group of peers. In North American middle and
group) and asked to circle or otherwise indicate which high schools, for example, it is often the case that stu-
members are their best or close friends. Researchers dents take different courses with different classmates.
who study middle-schoolers may use classroom lists or In this regard, the use of classroom nominations makes
they may simply ask children to write down the names of little sense. One would fare better by asking all children
their best friends. The pattern of choices is then exam- in a given grade to list their best friends in that particu-
ined to identify children who nominate one another. Less lar grade. With increasing age, however, it may also
often, investigators have used reciprocated high ratings make sense to ask children to nominate their best
as an index of friendship, either alone or in conjunction friends in the given school.
with friendship nominations (Bukowski, Hoza, & New- To solve the problem of identifying friendships in a
comb, 1994). Both procedures are consistent with the given classroom or school, some researchers have begun
definition of friendship that we presented earlier to use diary data: They ask children in late middle
friendship requires reciprocity, refers to a free choice on school ( junior high school) or high school to keep a log
the part of the two children involved, and is predicated of who it is they spend time with when they are not at
on affectional concerns rather than instrumental issues. school (e.g., Laursen, Wilder, Noack, & Williams, 2000;
Evidence of reciprocity of affection alone may be in- Laursen & Williams, 1997). These logs or diaries allow
sufficient to presume or substantiate claims of friend- children to indicate the length of time and quality of
ship. Children may enjoy each others company in their interactions with friends.
school but never spend time together outside of school or A second problem occurs when children are permit-
in other ways have experiences together that lead them ted only a limited number of friendship nominations
to think of each other as friends. Indeed, sometimes (e.g., three choices). This practice may arbitrarily re-
children have only limited direct contact with other strict the number friendships a child may have. Further-
children they report liking. For example, children can more, when the number of choices is specified, children
admire another child from a distance, can be grateful to who have one or two classroom friends may feel com-
someone who is only an acquaintance, or have affection pelled to add to their list the names of children who are
for someone whose leadership facilitates groups func- not actually their best friends. This creates the possibil-
tioning (Parker, Saxon, et al., 1999). Yet, friendship ity for overestimating the actual number of friendships
generally implies that the individuals involved in the re- these children have.
lationship not only like or admire one another but also A central benefit of friendship may also be one of its
label their relationship a friendship, have some shared challenges. Whereas friendship may help protect chil-
history together, are committed to one another, and are dren from inadequacies in their families, the interface
comfortable being perceived as a pair of friends by oth- between friendship and family may present difficult de-
ers. Normally, friendship cannot be presumed unless mands on children. As French (2004) has shown, in cul-
children have been expressly asked whether the relation- tures that ascribe considerable power or authority to the
ship in question is a friendship. family system, the significance and meaning of friend-
One problem that can limit the validity of friendship ship may differ substantially from the meaning of
measures is whether one has adequately assessed the en- friendship in Western cultures. It is conceivable that
tire domain of a childs peer relationships. Although the friendship may even be seen as a threat to the expected
peer group at school is typically a childs most salient structure and influence of the family. Accordingly, peer
peer group, it is almost always the case that children research needs to be increasingly sensitive to the cul-
have friends outside of schoolin their neighborhood or tural variations in the way that friendship is constructed
in connection with sports or recreational activities. In and in the role that friendship is given in childrens lives.
this regard, the sole use of school-based data underesti-
mates the extent of childrens friendship relations. This Friendship Quality
problem is further exacerbated if assessments allow only In addition to determining whether a child has a close
for the nomination of classmatesfriendships with chil- dyadic friendship, investigators have shown an increas-
dren in other classrooms at school are overlooked. This ing concern with the characteristics or qualities of chil-
is a particular problem in schools in which children do drens relationships with their best friends. Given that
Social Behaviors, Interactions, Relationships, and Groups: Assessment Issues 607

childrens understanding of friendship changes with age, Dodge, and Coppotelli (1982), gave researchers a means
it is not surprising that there are age differences in the by which to represent the extent to which a child is liked
properties of childrens friendships. And considering the and disliked by peers. Much of the activity regarding so-
wide variations in individual characteristics that chil- ciometry is aimed at the challenge of developing valid
dren bring with them to their friendships, it is reasonable and efficient measures of the two fundamental sociomet-
to expect that not all friendships will be alike. The most ric forces, specifically acceptance and rejection, and the
common approach involves assessing the features of chil- measures that derive from them. Acceptance refers to
drens friendships through childrens own reports (e.g., how much a child is liked by peers; rejection refers to
Berndt & Perry, 1986; Furman & Buhrmester, 1992; how much a child is disliked. The challenge of creating
Parker & Asher, 1993). Furman (1996) has noted that categorical measures results from the lack of independ-
assessments of this type are usually conducted with ence between acceptance and rejection. These measures
questionnaires or interview procedures and are predi- are neither the opposite of each other nor are they unre-
cated on the belief that a childs impression of a relation- lated. Accordingly, a child high in acceptance is not nec-
ship is the best index of this relationship for the child. essarily low in rejection and a child high in rejection is
Drawing from theoretical accounts of friendship (e.g., not always low in acceptance. Some children could be
Sullivan, 1953), the dimensions typically assessed relate high on both dimensions or low on both dimensions.
to (a) the functions of friendship (e.g., provision of com- To account for these different patterns of association,
panionship, level of intimate disclosure, degree of help- derivative scores can be computed to index a childs
ful and advice), ( b) conf lict and disagreements, and (c) general likeableness (i.e., sociometric preference) and
the af fective properties of the friendship (e.g., the affec- the childs visibility in the peer group (i.e., sociomet-
tive bonds between friends). ric impact). These scores have been used in various
Observational techniques have also sometimes been ways, most notably to make categorical assignments to
used to study friends behavior with one another (e.g., the following sociometric groups: (a) popularchildren
Dunn, Cutting, & Fisher, 2002; Lansford & Parker, who are high in acceptance and low in rejection (i.e.,
1999), although far less frequently than self-reports. high impact, high preference); ( b) rejectedchildren
Part of the reluctance of researchers to use observational who are low in acceptance and high in rejection (i.e.,
approaches may stem from the formidable task of isolat- high impact, low preference); (c) neglectedchildren
ing the contributions of individual members to the ob- who are low in both acceptance and rejection (i.e., low
served patterns of dyadic interaction (Hinde & impact, mid-range in preference); (d) averagechildren
Stevenson-Hinde, 1987). This is a real concern, but some who are average in acceptance and rejection (i.e., mid-
promising observational methods for describing inter- range on both variables), and (e) controversialchildren
dyad variation are available (e.g., Howes, 1988; Simp- who are high in acceptance and rejection (i.e., high im-
kins & Parke, 2002; Youngblade, Park, & Belsky, 1993). pact, mid-range on preference). Note well that in the
Presumably, any interpersonal behavior between friends case of sociometric classifications, the term popular is
may be amenable to observational assessments. Re- used as a synonym for accepted rather than as an index
searchers have generally been interested in dimensions of social prestige or status. Discussions of the stability
of behavior that relate to the putative functions of friend- of sociometric scores and classifications can be found in
ship (e.g., provision of companionship, level of intimate Cillessen, Bukowski, and Haselager (2000), and in our
disclosure, degree of helpful and advice) or address the previous Handbook chapter on childrens peer interac-
affective properties of the relationship (e.g., the affec- tions, relationships, and groups (Rubin, Bukowski, &
tive bonds between friends). Childrens conflict and dis- Parker, 1998). Discussion of the conceptual, method-
agreement with friends have also been of interest. ological, and potential ethical problems with sociomet-
ric techniques classification can be found in Rubin et al.
Childrens Peer Acceptance (1998) and Bukowski and Cillessen (1996).

Much of the dramatic increase in interest in childrens


Assessments of the Peer Group
peer relationships during the past 25 years can be traced
to advances in sociometry. Techniques for measuring Typically, groups have been studied for three reasons.
popularity, especially a procedure developed by Coie, First, investigators have sought to determine whether
608 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

and how a child is embedded into a naturally and spon- community of peers. And group structure refers to how
taneously formed group structure. Two techniques, So- many links there are between group members. In a dense
cial Network Analysis (SNA; Richards, 1995) and the group, most members would be linked to others; in a
Social Cognitive Map (SCM; Cairns, Gariepy, & Kin- loosely organized group, some members would have no
dermann, 1989) are often used to identify peer net- links to others at all.
works. Social Network Analysis is based on friendship Finally, a third goal is to assess the psychological char-
nominations. Children are asked to list the friends with acteristics of childrens groups. Examples of this ap-
whom they hang out most often in the school. Group proach can be seen in the work of Chen and colleagues
members, liaisons, dyads, and isolates based on pat- (Chen, Chang, & He, 2003), Gest et al. (2003), and Kin-
terns of friendship links and the strengths of the links dermann et al. (1995). In their research, group profiles
are identified (Richards, 1995). Group members are are schematized, representing the interest and character-
those individuals who belong to a rather exclusive social istics that its members share. Kindermann and col-
group that comprises at least three individuals who are leagues, for example, have shown that groups vary
linked with other members in the same group and who considerably in their emphasis on academic performance.
are connected by paths entirely in the group. Liaisons Whereas group clustering techniques have been used
are individuals who have friendships with group mem- to account for differences between groups that comprise
bers, but are not group members themselves. Dyads the larger peer system, they may be an excellent way to
comprise individuals who have one reciprocated friend- capture differences between the structures of the peer
ship. They do not belong to a group per se, but have mu- group in different cultures. One would expect that, for
tual friendships. Finally, Isolates are children who have example, in collectivist cultures, peer groups might be
no reciprocated friendships. Given that SNA is based on larger in that there might be more links in and between
friendship (either reciprocal or nonreciprocal) nomina- groups than one would see in individualistic cultures.
tions, groups identified through the program represent This is an untested empirical question.
friendship networks.
Compared with SNA, the SCM technique, developed
by Cairns et al. (1989), assesses peer groups more di- THE PROXIMAL CORRELATES OF
rectly. Children are asked, Are there people in school CHILDRENS PEER RELATIONSHIPS
who hang around together a lot? Who are they? To en-
sure that the respondents include themselves, a follow up The understanding of the origins and correlates of indi-
question is asked What about you? Do you hang around vidual differences in childrens experiences with peers
together a lot with a group? Who are these people you comprises the largest corpus of peer relationships re-
hang around with? Children are expected to report on search in the past 25 years. Much of this research has fo-
groups about which they are most knowledgeable. Based cused on the processes and variables that either provide
on the reports of all participants, a matrix is constructed the basis for, or are correlated with childrens accep-
from the number of occasions that any two persons co- tance or rejection by the peer group; a much smaller
occurred in the same group. Specifically, each partici- proportion of the research extant is focused on the cor-
pants group-membership profile is first generated relates and antecedents of individual differences at the
based on the frequencies of nominations of group-mem- level of the dyad (e.g., friendship).
bership with every other child in the class. Then a pro- The literature on individual differences in popularity
file similarity index is derived by correlating pairs of and friendship can be divided into two domains. First,
individual group-membership profiles. the largest concentration of investigations center on the
Second, following the determination that a child is a individual characteristics associated with (a) acceptance
member of a group, a researcher can assess the groups or rejection in the peer group at large, ( b) the ability to
structural properties. These properties typically consist make and keep friends, and (c) the quality of friendship.
of (a) group size, ( b) the position of the group in the Most of this work focuses on either the display of partic-
broader community of peer groups, and (c) the patterns ular forms of social behavior or the ways that children
of association in the group. Size refers simply to the think about their social environments and relationships.
number of children in the group. The position of the A second body of research is concerned with the as-
group in the broader peer group refers to how many sociations between peer acceptance and rejection and
links the group has to other collectives in the general friendship and both the childs family relationship expe-
The Proximal Correlates of Childrens Peer Relationships 609

riences and the social environments in which the child ing playgroup and to establish themselves as sharing in
functions. This literature deals with the distal correlates this frame of reference (Putallaz, 1983). Popular chil-
of peer acceptance and friendship. Although re- dren are also less likely to draw unwarranted attention
searchers appear to have their own preferences with re- to themselves when entering ongoing playgroups: They
gard to whether they examine proximal or distal do not talk exclusively or overbearingly about them-
correlates, these factors are truly interdependent; in- selves and their own social goals or desires, and they are
deed, the study of the links between proximal and distal not disruptive of the groups activity (Dodge, Mc-
factors has become the central theme of much contem- Claskey, & Feldman, 1985). In addition, when entering
porary research. the ongoing play of both familiar and unfamiliar chil-
dren, popular children speak clearly, respond contin-
Proximal CorrelatesPeer Acceptance gently to their prospective playmates, and otherwise
demonstrate communicative competence that allows the
Over 20 years ago, researchers set out to develop a be- maintenance of connected, coherent interaction (Black
havioral explanation of peer acceptance and rejection. & Hazan, 1990). They are seen as cooperative, friendly,
Studies were conducted using several approaches and sociable, and sensitive by peers, teachers, and observers
designs most notably involving play groups (e.g., Coie & (e.g., Coie et al., 1982; Newcomb & Bukowski, 1983,
Kupersmidt, 1983) and peer-assessment techniques 1984; Parkhurst & Hopmeyer, 1998). Specifically, pop-
(Newcomb & Bukowski, 1983). In these investigations, ular children are more likely to be helpful, to interact
researchers typically examined differences between actively with other children, to show leadership skills,
children who had been classified as sociometrically pop- and to engage in constructive play (e.g., Pakaslahti, Kar-
ular, rejected, neglected, controversial and average. Lit- jalainen, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, 2002). When involved
erally, hundreds of studies were conducted, making in conflict, sociometrically popular children believe that
sociometric studies the bread and butter of peer re- negotiation and compromise will help them get what
search throughout much of the 1980s. they want while simultaneously maintaining positive re-
A thorough review of the literature on the concomi- lationships with peers (e.g., Hart, DeWolf, Wozniak, &
tants of popularity was presented in the previous version Burts, 1992).
of this chapter (Rubin et al., 1998). Whereas some re- Sociometrically popular children do not differ from
views of research serve as renaissances that renew the average children on all aspects of aggression. In a meta-
study of a topic, the reviews of the sociometric classifi- analysis of research on popularity, Newcomb et al.
cation studies served as a requiem. Although many of (1993) distinguished between assertive/agonistic behav-
the basic questions of sociometric classification remain iors and behaviors that reflected disruptiveness. Popu-
unanswered, research on the differences between chil- lar children did not differ from others on the former
dren in the five sociometric groups has waned. Here we category of behavior whereas they did on the latter.
provide a cursory discussion of what this literature has Popular children, it appears, can engage in some forms
informed us. of assertive behavior, but they rarely engage in behav-
iors that are likely to interfere with the actions and
Popular Children goals of others.
Popular children are high in acceptance and low in re-
jection. Keep in mind that whereas the term popular has Rejected Children
been traditionally used to refer to these children, this The most commonly cited behavioral correlate of peer
usage varies with the more recent trend to use the word rejection is aggression, regardless of whether aggres-
popular to refer to children who are high in status and sion is indexed by peer evaluations, teacher ratings, or
prestige in the group. The children traditionally known observations (e.g., Crick, Casas, & Mosher, 1997;
as sociometrically popular have been shown to have Haselager, Cillessen, van Lieshout, Riksen-Walraven,
the following characteristics. Relative to other children, & Hartup, 2002; McNeilly-Choque, Hart, Robinson,
those of popular status are skilled at initiating and Nelson, & Olsen, 1996). The association between
maintaining qualitatively positive relationships. When rejection and aggression appears to be rather broad;
entering new peer situations, popular children are more Newcomb et al. (1993) revealed that rejected children,
likely than members of other sociometric status groups relative to average popular and neglected children,
to consider the frame of reference common to the ongo- showed elevated levels on three forms of aggression
610 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

specifically, disruptiveness, physical aggression, and tions between neglected status and specific behaviors
negative behavior (e.g., verbal threats). A small num- is unsurprising.
ber of studies provide evidence of a causal link between
Controversial Children
aggression and rejection. Two of these are the ground-
breaking playgroup studies of Dodge (1983) and Coie Sociometrically, this group is unique in that controver-
and Kupersmidt (1983). In these cleverly designed in- sial children are high on both acceptance and rejection.
vestigations, the interactions between unfamiliar peers Accordingly, controversial children appear to have many
in small groups were observed in a laboratory context of the characteristics of both popular and rejected chil-
over several days. Each childs behavior was observa- dren. Coie and Dodge (1988), for example, reported that
tionally coded; in addition, each child was assessed in a controversial boys, like rejected boys, were aggressive
sociometric interview at the end of each play session. and disruptive, socially withdrawn, prone to anger and
Gradually, some of the children became popular and rule violations, and highly active. Alternatively, they re-
others became rejected. The behavior that most clearly ported that controversial boys were like popular boys in
predicted peer rejection was aggression. that they showed high levels of helpfulness, cooperation,
However, aggression is not the only factor linked to leadership, and, in some instances, social sensitivity.
rejection. Detailed analyses indicate that aggressive
Summary
children comprise only between 40% to 50% of the re-
jected group (Bierman, Smoot, & Aumiller, 1993; Cil- General conclusions can be drawn as to the features that
lessen, van IJzendoorn, van Lieshout, & Hartup, 1992). distinguish sociometrically popular, rejected, neglected,
Indeed, with increasing age, it appears as if aggression controversial, and average children from one other.
becomes decreasingly associated with rejection, espe- These differences generally fall along a positive/nega-
cially among boys (e.g., Sandstrom & Coie, 1999). Also, tive continuum. Sociometrically rejected children show
the data extant indicate that aggression may not lead to high levels of negative behaviors and low levels of posi-
rejection if it is balanced by a set of positive qualities tive behaviors, whereas the opposite pattern is charac-
(e.g., social skill) that facilitate links with other chil- teristic of popular children. Average children show
dren (Farmer, Estell, Bishop, ONeal, & Cairns, 2003). moderate amounts of positive and negative behaviors,
Researchers have found that there is a high level of neglected children demonstrate low levels of each form
heterogeneity among the behavioral tendencies of re- of social behavior, and controversial children show high
jected children. For example, children who are highly levels of both positive and negative behaviors. It should
withdrawn, timid, and wary comprise between 10% to be remembered that the conclusions regarding differ-
20% of the rejected group (e.g., Cillessen et al., 1992; ences between sociometric groups are based on consis-
Parkhurst & Asher, 1992). Another perspective on this tencies across studies. Nevertheless, these general
latter statistic is that when extremely withdrawn children conclusions do not always represent powerful effects.
are identified, approximately 25% of them fall into Accordingly one cannot conclude that all aggressive chil-
the sociometrically rejected group (e.g., Rubin, Chen, & dren will be sociometrically rejected and one should not
Hymel, 1993). be surprised to discover that some aggressive children
Finally, victimization has been observed to be associ- are actually liked by their peers. The next wave of re-
ated with peer rejection, either as a correlate (Kochen- search on acceptance, rejection, and childrens individ-
derfer-Ladd, 2003; Schwartz, 2000), as a mediator that ual characteristics needs to sort out why some features
explains the association between withdrawal and victim- lead to rejection in one case and acceptance in another.
ization, or as a moderator that increases the stability of Recent research has provided some guidance as to
victimization (e.g., Hanish & Guerra, 2004). what the next set of studies should be like. One direction
would be to give further attention to the interactions be-
Neglected Children tween variables. Hawley (2003), for example, has shown
Few, if any, discrete behaviors have been found to be that aggression is linked with competence when it co-
distinctive of sociometrically neglected children (see occurs with prosocial tendencies. Alternatively, Prin-
Newcomb et al., 1993). Sociometric neglected status is stein and Cillessen (2003) have pointed to the
relatively unstable, even over short periods, and in importance of studying nonlinear effects such as exam-
that light, the fact that there are few strong associa- ining whether the association between aggression and
The Proximal Correlates of Childrens Peer Relationships 611

competence with peers is best represented as curvilin- opposite-gender preferred play scores and popularity
ear. As we noted earlier, several researchers have made were nonsignificant for females in either age group. For
the distinction between traditional sociometric dimen- males, however, the frequent demonstration of female
sions of acceptance and rejection and the conceptualiza- preferred play was significantly, and negatively, associ-
tion of perceived popularity as an index of a childs ated with acceptance, not only by boys, but also by girls.
status in the group (e.g., Parkhurst & Hopmeyer, 1998). And this relation held only for the 10-year-old males
who frequently produced female sex-stereotyped play.
These latter data are in keeping with a study by Berndt
Variations in the Behavioral Correlates and Heller (1986). Using scenarios in which they de-
of Popularity: Sex, Group, and scribed a child who had chosen activities either consistent
Cultural Differences or inconsistent with gender stereotypes, the participants
were asked to make judgments of the actors popularity
Groups have norms, or standards, regarding the good- among peers. The authors found that 9- and 12-year-old
ness of particular acts. The acceptability of a behavior, children demonstrated a greater negative reaction to gen-
and of the child who displays that behavior, is determined der inconsistent behavior than did 5-year-olds. Further-
by whether the behavior conforms to the groups norms. If more, they found this intolerance was greater for boys
a behavior is universally valued, it should correlate with than for girls in that it was more appropriate for girls to
peer acceptance; if the normalcy of a behavior varies behave in a gender inconsistent manner. Taken together,
across groups, the extent to which the behavior is linked to the research on the correlates of popularity for boys ver-
popularity should vary across these groups also. It is this sus girls reveals one consistent finding. Males who display
logic that has provided the basis for much of the research female-stereotyped behavior are disliked by both same-
on group variations in the correlates of popularity. and opposite-sex peers; females who display male-stereo-
typed behavior are generally accepted by both same- and
Sex Differences
opposite-sex peers. These relations appear to gain
Given the widespread concern with sex differences in the strength with increasing age in childhood. Nevertheless,
literature on child development, it seems surprising to the relevant data base for examining sex differences in the
discover how little work exists on the topic of sociomet- correlates of peer acceptance and rejection is sparse. This
ric peer acceptance. Typically, researchers have failed to gap in the literature is striking and it severely compro-
examine whether general findings are equally valid for mises our current understanding of the peer system (see
boys and girls. For that matter, much of the early work Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, Chapter 14, this Handbook,
focused solely on boys (e.g., Coie & Kupersmidt, 1983; this volume, for further discussion).
Dodge, 1983) despite published calls for the examination
Variations across Groups
of sex differences in the causes, proximal and distal cor-
relates, and prospective outcomes of peer acceptance and The argument that a childs popularity will be associ-
rejection (e.g., Rubin, 1983). Further, sex differences ated with particular peer group norms has been the cen-
have been neglected despite (a) the long-standing view tral focus of a number of investigations. Wright,
that the relationships formed and maintained by females Giammarino, and Parad (1986) examined the differ-
are qualitatively distinct from those of males (Leaper, ences in the correlates of popularity in groups at a sum-
1994) and ( b) the evidence that some aspects of social mer camp for boys with behavioral and emotional
behavior may be differentially normative for boys and problems. For highly aggressive groups of children, the
girls (e.g., Humphreys & Smith, 1987). correlation between peer preference and aggression was
Sex differences do exist when behavior that is typical very low. In nonaggressive groups, this association be-
for a gender is considered. For example, in an observa- tween preference and aggression was of moderate
tional study of 8- and 10-year-olds, Moller, Hymel, and strength. The opposite pattern was seen in these same
Rubin (1992) distinguished styles of play that were en- groups when social withdrawal was considered. With-
gaged in more frequently by females from styles of play drawal was strongly and negatively correlated to prefer-
that were engaged in more frequently by males. The chil- ence in the high aggression groups and uncorrelated to
dren were also administered a sociometric rating. The preference in the low aggression groups. Boivin, Dodge,
authors found that the relations between the same- or and Coie (1995) reported that reactive aggression,
612 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

proactive aggression, and solitary play were more nega- have found that among young Chinese children, sensi-
tively linked to a measure of social preference when high tive, cautious, and inhibited behavior are positively as-
levels of these specific behaviors were nonnormative and sociated with competent and positive social behavior
unrelated to preference when high levels on these behav- and with peer acceptance (Chen et al., 1999; Chen,
iors were normative. Stormshak et al. (1999) also found Rubin, & Sun, 1992). More recently, however, Hart and
support for the person-group similarity model. These re- colleagues (2000) found that social reticence, defined as
searchers reported that for boys, social withdrawal was unoccupied and onlooking behavior (Coplan et al.,
associated with peer acceptance in those classrooms in 1994), was associated with a lack of peer acceptance,
which withdrawal was normative; for boys, aggression not only in young American children, but also among
was linked to peer preference in those classrooms in Russian and Chinese youngsters. Furthermore, Chang
which aggression was more normative. Findings for girls and colleagues (2005) have recently found that social
were, complex and in some cases not supportive of the withdrawal among young Hong Kong Chinese adoles-
person-group similarity model. For example, in class- cents predicted the lack of peer acceptance 1.5 years
rooms marked by high aggression, aggressive girls were hence. Relatedly, Chen et al. (2005) have reported that
not better liked than nonaggressive girls. over the years, since the early 1990s, shy, reserved be-
These studies show clearly that the association be- havior among Chinese elementary school children has
tween a particular form of behavior and popularity de- increasingly become associated with negative peer repu-
pends on whether the behavior is normative for a group. tations. Chen and colleagues have argued that the chang-
Considering the importance of group norms as modera- ing economic and political climate in China is being
tors of the associations between behaviors and popular- accompanied by preferences for more assertive, yet
ity, researchers should be cautious about drawing broad competent, social behavior. In short, researchers would
conclusions about the correlates of popularity. Indeed, do well not to generalize findings drawn from children
researchers would do well to assess the person/group in- of one cultural group to children from another context.
teraction and similarity as a major determinant of peer Moreover, changing socioeconomic climates may prove
acceptance and rejection. to have significant influences on that which is deemed
Lastly, the recent study of deviancy training is ger- acceptable behavior by significant peers and adults in
mane. In this work, researchers find that children who the childs environment (Silbereisen, 2000).
deviate from the norm (typically insofar as their aggres-
sive behavior is concerned) find social support in peer Summary
networks of like-behaved counterparts (Bagwell & Coie, We have highlighted the notion that acceptance by the
2004; Dishion, Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996). peer group (typically defined by classroom composi-
It is in such groups that popularity may be determined tion) is driven by conformity to or deviation from behav-
by behavior that is dysfunctional. ioral norms. Such a view is admittedly simple
Variations across Culture conceptually and does not take into account the possibil-
ity that correlates of popularity may vary according to
Cross-cultural research on the correlates of peer accep- whether a child is acquiring or maintaining their status
tance and rejection has been aimed at asking whether in a group. This latter issue seems ripe for study in the
given behaviors known to be associated with acceptance next generation of studies on patterns of liking and dis-
or rejection in North American samples demonstrate liking among children.
similar relations in other cultures. One shortcoming in
this work may be that investigators have taken measures
originally developed for use in a Western cultural con-
Social Cognitive Correlates of Peer Acceptance
text, and have employed them in other cultural milieus.
and Rejection
The general conclusion from this research has been that
aggression and helpfulness are associated with rejection Those who study social cognition believe that the
and popularity respectively in a wide range of cultures childs thoughts about the social universe, especially
(e.g., Casiglia, Lo Coco & Zappulla, 1998; Chang et al., about specific children, can be evocative or inhibitory
2005; Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1995; Cillessen et al., 1992; because behavioral expression is concerned. Thus, if a
Tomada & Schneider, 1997). Alternatively, researchers child has difficulty understanding the sorts of behaviors
The Proximal Correlates of Childrens Peer Relationships 613

required to make proper entry into a group or to obtain consists of six stages: (1) the encoding of social cues, (2)
desirable objects or to avoid harassment by peers, it may the interpretation of encoded cues, (3) the clarification
be reflected in their behavior in the peer group. In short, of goals, (4) the accessing and generation of potential re-
the connection between social cognition and peer accep- sponses, (5) the evaluation and selection of responses,
tance and rejection is best understood by suggesting that and (6) the enactment of the chosen response. Recently,
thoughts (and emotions for that matter) about things so- Lemerise and Arsenio (2000) integrated emotional expe-
cial and relational can evoke particular forms of behav- riences into the Crick and Dodges social information-
ior. In turn, these behaviors lead to acceptance or processing model. For example, aggressive childrens
rejection by peers. emotional reactions to problematic social situations
With this in mind, researchers have studied a wide might include frustration or anger; anxious/withdrawn
range of social cognitive variables that appear to have children may react with fear. These emotions, in turn,
some bearing on the childs acceptance by peers. In gen- may influence the information that is attended to and the
eral, the more social-cognitively astute the child is, the information that is recalled. This mood-congruent infor-
more popular she or he is found to be (e.g., Slaughter, mation processing might reinforce aggressive childrens
Dennis, & Pritchard, 2002). In this section, we review social schemas or working models that the social
research in which social cognition has been associated world is a hostile one or withdrawn childrens notions
with sociometric status. that the social world is fear inducing. These emotional
responses may explain, in part, why aggressive and with-
Social Information Processing drawn children respond in predictable ways to negative
We begin with a brief description of social information- events befalling them.
processing models that are relevant to the study of chil- Much research on social cognition and peer relation-
drens skilled and unskilled social behaviors. In one ships has focused on rejected childrens deficits or qual-
model, Rubin and Rose-Krasnor (1992) speculated that, itative differences in performance at various stages of
when children face an interpersonal dilemma (e.g., mak- these social information-processing models. First, re-
ing new friends, acquiring a desired object from some- jected children are distinguished from their nonrejected
one else, or stopping others from acting against them), counterparts on the basis of their spontaneous motives
their thinking follows a particular sequence. First, chil- for social engagement. Popular children, for example,
dren select a social goal or a representation of the de- are more inclined to indicate the reason for interacting
sired end state of the problem-solving process. Second, with others is to establish new, or enhance ongoing, rela-
they examine the task environment; this involves reading tionships. Rejected children, however, are more likely to
and interpreting relevant social cues. For example, so- be motivated by goals that would reasonably be ex-
cial status, familiarity, and age of the participants in the pected to undermine their social relationships, such as
task environment are likely to influence the childs goal getting even with or defeating their peers (e.g., Ra-
and strategy selection (Krasnor & Rubin, 1983). Third, biner & Gordon, 1992).
they access and select strategies; this process involves Second, when considering the motives or intentions
generating possible plans of action for achieving the per- of others, rejected-aggressive children are more dis-
ceived social goal, and choosing the most appropriate posed than their popular counterparts to assume that
one for the specific situation. Fourth, they implement negative events are the product of malicious, malevolent
the chosen strategy. Finally, it is proposed that children intent on the part of others (e.g., Dodge et al., 2003).
evaluate the outcome of the strategy; this involves as- This bias is evident when children are asked to make at-
sessing the situation to determine the relative success of tributions for others behaviors in situations where
the chosen course of action in achieving the social goal. something negative has happened but the motives of the
If the initial strategy is unsuccessful, the child may re- instigator are unclear. In these ambiguous situations, re-
peat it or she/ he may select and enact a new strategy or jected-aggressive children appear unwilling to give a
abandon the situation entirely. provocateur the benefit of the doubtfor example, by
Crick and Dodge (1994) proposed a similar social- assuming that the behavior occurred by accident. This
cognitive model designed specifically to account for ag- intention cue bias is often suggested as an explanation
gression in children (see Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, for why it is that aggressive and oppositional-defiant
Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume). This model children choose to solve their interpersonal problems in
614 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

hostile and agonistic ways (e.g., see Orobio de Castro, interpersonal rejection by peers, withdrawn children
Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & Monshouwer, 2002, for a re- may begin to attribute their social failures to internal
cent review). causes; they may come to believe that there is something
But why would aggressive children think that when wrong with themselves rather than attributing their so-
negative, but ambiguously caused events befall them, cial failures to other people or situations. Supporting
the protagonist means them harm? In keeping with these notions, Rubin and Krasnor (1986) found that ex-
Lemerise and Arsenio (2000), a transactional perspec- tremely withdrawn children tended to blame social fail-
tive would suggest that aggressive children, many of ure on personal, dispositional characteristics rather
whom are already rejected (and victimized) by their than on external events or circumstances. These results
peers, believe that certain others do not like them, those are in keeping with recent findings by Wichmann,
others have a history of rejecting of them or acting mean Coplan, and Daniels (2004) who reported that when 9-
toward them, and thus the negative act must be inten- to 13-year-old withdrawn children were presented with
tionally caused. This conclusion of intentional malevo- hypothetical social situations in which ambiguously
lence is posited to elicit anger and a rapid fire response caused negative events happened to them, they attrib-
of reactive aggression. Many researchers have found that uted the events to internal and stable self-defeating
when asked how they would react to an ambiguously causes. Moreover, withdrawn children suggested that
caused negative event, aggressive children respond with when faced with such negative situations, they were
a choice of agonistic strategies (Orobio de Castro et al., more familiar with failure experiences and that a pre-
2002). And aggressive children also regard aggression to ferred strategy would be to withdraw and escape.
be an effective and appropriate means to meet their in- Some have suggested that there is a particular group
teractive goals (Vernberg, Jacobs, & Hershberger, of victimized children who are characterized by a so-
1999). The processes leading to the enactment of aggres- cially withdrawn demeanor. For example, Olweus (1978,
sion and the behavioral display itself no doubt reinforces 1993) has referred to whipping boysa group of vic-
an already negative peer profile. timized children perceived as easy marks by peers.
By the elementary and middle school years, many so- Hodges and colleagues have referred to some victimized
cially withdrawn children are also rejected by their children as physically weak and withdrawn
peers. Thus, one may ask whether these children view (Hodges, Boivin, et al., 1999; Hodges, Malone, & Perry,
their social worlds in ways that vary from those of non- 1997; Rodkin & Hodges, 2003). Not surprisingly, re-
withdrawn and/or nonrejected children. To begin with, searchers have found that children victimized by peers
when socially withdrawn 4- and 5-year-olds are asked are also rejected by them (Kochenderfer-Ladd, 2003;
how they would go about obtaining an attractive object Schwartz, 2000). Given the conceptual associations be-
from another child, they produce fewer alternative solu- tween social withdrawal, victimization, and peer rejec-
tions, display more rigidity in generating alternative re- tion, the earlier noted findings by Wichmann et al.
sponses, and are more likely to suggest adult (2004) are reminiscent of work by Graham and Juvonen
intervention to aid in the solution of hypothetical social (1998, 2001). These latter researchers reported that
problems when compared to their more sociable age- youngsters who identified themselves as victimized by
mates (Rubin, Daniels-Beirness, & Bream, 1984). peers tended to blame themselves for their peer relation-
Observational research has demonstrated that so- ship problems. And Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, and
cially reticent and withdrawn children produce fewer Seligman (1992) have argued that self-blame can lead to
overtures to their peers than nonwithdrawn children a variety of negative outcomes of an internalizing na-
(Nelson, Rubin, & Fox, 2005; Stewart & Rubin, 1995). ture, such as depression, low self-esteem, and with-
Yet, the overtures produced are typically unassertive. drawal, thereby suggesting a self-reinforcing cycle of
Despite this production of unassertive strategies, with- negative socioemotional functioning (see also Dill,
drawn children are more often rebuffed by their peers Vernberg, & Fonagy, 2004).
than are nonwithdrawn children (Nelson et al., 2005; Taken together, the findings reported earlier suggest
Stewart & Rubin, 1995). This connection between peer that if children interpret social experiences negatively,
rebuff and social withdrawal or reticence may be taken inappropriately, and inaccurately, they may prove to be
as an in vivo assessment of peer rejection. their own worst enemies. Such negative biases are likely
Rubin and colleagues (e.g., Rubin, Burgess, Kennedy, to contribute to their already problematic social rela-
& Stewart, 2003) have argued that as a result of frequent tionships. In the case of rejected-aggressive children,
The Proximal Correlates of Childrens Peer Relationships 615

demonstrated deficits in social-cognitive processing tencies or their relationships with peers (Zakriski &
suggest that these children may have difficulty under- Coie, 1996). These findings are in keeping with the re-
standing the consequences of their behaviors for others sults of studies concerning withdrawn and aggressive
and that their social failures can be attributed to inter- children conducted in Western cultures; it is only the
nal, stable causes. In short, they may not claim responsi- former group that reports having difficulty with their
bility for their production of agonistic social behaviors social skills and peer relationships (Rubin, Chen, &
( They made me do it!) or for their negative social rep- Hymel, 1993).
utations. Indeed, given their social-cognitive inadequa- Given rejected-withdrawn childrens negative per-
cies, rejected-aggressive children may not realize that ceptions of their social competencies and relationships,
their interactive styles are perceived negatively by and given their negative experiences in the peer group, it
peers. After all, as noted earlier, they do regard aggres- is not surprising that these children report more loneli-
sion as an effective and appropriate means to meet their ness and social detachment than popular children or
interactive goals. children who are rejected but aggressive (e.g., Gazelle
Alternatively, the rejected socially withdrawn child & Ladd, 2003). These relations have been reported
may be able to think through interpersonal dilemmas in throughout childhood and early adolescence (e.g., Crick
an adequate, competent manner. Nevertheless, when & Ladd, 1993; Parkhurst & Asher, 1992).
confronted by the real-life social world, withdrawn A further distinction between rejected children is the
children may be less able to meet their social goals than chronicity of their peer problems. Whereas rejection is
are their nonwithdrawn peers. The experience of peer temporary for some children, it is an enduring experi-
noncompliance noted earlier is likely to have an unfortu- ence for others. Ladd and Troop-Gordon (2003) showed
nate outcome for the sensitive, wary, withdrawn child. It that chronic rejection was related to subsequent views of
is this type of sensitive rejected child who would attrib- the self and that these negative self-perceptions par-
ute social failures to internal, stable characteristics, and tially mediated the relation between peer difficulties
who would respond to peer rebuff by expressing (a) and internalizing problems and loneliness.
loneliness, ( b) self-blame, (c) dissatisfaction with his or In summary, although rejected children tend to report
her social relationships, and (d) negative self-appraisals that they are less competent, less efficacious, and less
of social skills. satisfied with reference to their social skills and peer
relationships, this conclusion appears true only for re-
Self-System Correlates of Peer Acceptance jected children who are withdrawn, timid, or submis-
and Rejection sive. The study of the association between the self and
peer relations is part of a large and enduring research
An important repercussion that has been ascribed to the tradition, which treats that self as the result of peer
experiences with peers is their effect on the self- processes. More recent research suggests that children
concept. In the foundational ideas of Sullivan and of the who are high and low in self-esteem manage and use
symbolic interactionists, peer relationships were de- their peer relations for different purposes. Research on
scribed as a critical source of the self. Specifically, boys the association between peer relationships and the self
and girls were said to use their peer relationships as im- may benefit from the adoption of new perspectives such
portant sources of information about themselves. Most as the ideas from dynamic systems models of the self.
positive experiences were believed to provide a strong
sense of validation that reinforced the perception that
Childrens Friendships: Correlates and
one is well-functioning and grounded. Accordingly, re-
Individual Differences
search on peer relationships has often addressed theo-
retically derived hypotheses about the effects of peer In an earlier section, we described developmental issues
experiences on aspects of the self. pertaining to friendship, such as its understanding by
Researchers have consistently reported that it is children, its prevalence, features, and functions. In this
mainly rejected-withdrawn children (also variously de- section, we examine the correlates of friendship and in-
scribed as submissive, sensitive, wary) who believe they dividual differences in those aspects of friendship de-
have poor social skills and relationships (Hymel, scribed earlier.
Bowker, & Woody, 1993). Rejected-aggressive children Children who lack friends may miss out on the ad-
do not report thinking poorly about their social compe- vantages thought to be garnered by such relationships.
616 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Furthermore, not only is the presence of friendship increases in internalizing and externalizing difficulties
viewed as important, but the quality of the relationship during the school year for those children who lacked a
is also considered significant. Qualitative dimensions of mutual best friendship. The relation between peer vic-
friendship include intimacy, companionship, and emo- timization, internalizing, and externalizing problems
tional and social support. Notably, friendship quality was nonsignificant for children who possessed a mutual
has been positively associated with indices of psychoso- best friendship, thereby suggesting that friendship may
cial adjustment and functioning, such as self-esteem function protectively for children who are victimized by
(Berndt, 1996). their peers.
In an attempt to illustrate the distinction between We now compare the friendships of those children
peer acceptance and friendship, several researchers who appear at greatest risk for peer rejection (i.e., those
have examined the relation between sociometric status, who have been identified as aggressive or socially with-
friendship prevalence, and relationship quality. For ex- drawn) with their age-mates who have do not evidence
ample, Parker and Asher (1993) showed that while not such behavioral or psychological difficulties.
all highly accepted children had best friends and not all
children low in peer acceptance were without best Friendship Prevalence and Quality
friendships, highly accepted and average-accepted chil- Investigators have shown that the majority of aggressive
dren were twice as likely as low-accepted children to children have a mutual best friendship and are as likely
have a mutual best friend. Additionally, low-accepted as well-adjusted children to have mutual friends (e.g.,
children reported qualitatively poorer friendships than Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2000). Aggression, how-
the other two groups. From a dyadic perspective, Brend- ever, does seem to be negatively related to friendship
gen, Little, and Krappmann (2000) found that the degree stability (e.g., Hektner, August, & Realmuto, 2000), a
of parallelism in friendship quality, or perceptual con- finding that is not too surprising considering the adverse
cordance, varied as a function of sociometric status. nature of aggression. Moreover, aggressive children
Whereas the perceptions of friendship quality of aver- have friends who are more aggressive and their relation-
age-accepted and highly accepted children and their re- ships are more confrontational and antisocial in quality
spective best friends were highly correlated, there was (e.g., Dishion, Eddy, Haas, Li, & Spracklen, 1997). High
little relation between rejected childrens own percep- levels of relational aggression (e.g., threatening friend-
tions and their best friends perceptions of the relation- ship withdrawal) within the friendship, and high levels
ship quality, particularly concerning the extent to which of exclusivity/jealously, and intimacy characterize the
they viewed the relationship as close and being fun. friendships of relationally aggressive children. In con-
Although an examination of the relation between so- trast, overtly aggressive children direct their overt ag-
ciometric status (group level) and friendship (dyadic gression outside their friendship dyads, and report low
relationship level) is important, there is also a need to levels of intimacy (Grotpeter & Crick, 1996).
describe how childrens individual characteristics are The prevalence of best friendships among young so-
related to the prevalence of friendship and the quality of cially withdrawn children is not significantly different
their dyadic relationships with peers. Given that many from that among nonwithdrawn children (Ladd &
rejected children appear to be aggressive and/or with- Burgess, 1999), and approximately 60% of withdrawn 8-,
drawn, it is surprising to note that few investigators have 9-, and 10-year-olds have reciprocated friendships
examined the friendships of these children. Not all ag- (Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Burgess, Booth-LaForce, & Rose-
gressive and withdrawn children and certainly not all re- Krasnor, in press; Schneider, 1999). These data suggest
jected children experience later adjustment difficulties. that social withdrawal and shyness are individual charac-
Thus, the best friendships of these children may func- teristics that do not influence the formation, prevalence,
tion protectively and buffer them from later problems. and maintenance of friendship in childhood.
Alternately, some best friendships may actually serve to Relationship qualities have been studied in relation
exacerbate existing problems. An example of the protec- to different subtypes of aggression, such as relational
tive role that friendship may play for children who have and overt aggression (Grotpeter & Crick, 1996), and
difficulties in the peer group may be drawn from re- proactive and reactive aggression (Poulin & Boivin,
search by Hodges, Boivin, et al. (1999). These re- 1999). Results from the aforementioned studies suggest
searchers found that peer victimization predicted that aggressive behaviors may negatively affect the qual-
Distal Predictors of Childrens Social Skills and Peer Relationships 617

ity of friendships. Recently, it has been shown that the child. Drawing from Hinde (1987), for example, it
friendships of withdrawn children are viewed as rela- seems reasonable to suggest that such individual charac-
tively lacking in fun, intimacy, helpfulness and guid- teristics as biological or dispositional factors (e.g., tem-
ance, and validation and caring (Rubin, Wojslawowicz, perament; self-regulatory mechanisms) may influence
et al., in press). These findings suggest a misery loves childrens peer interactions and relationships. It is
company scenario for withdrawn children and their equally plausible that the interactions and relationships
best friends. One may conjure up images of victimized children experience with their parents are important.
friends coping poorly in the world of peers, images re- In the following section, we present a brief review of
flected in recent newspaper and television accounts of some of the distal factors that may influence childrens
peer victimization and its untimely consequences. social interactions and peer relationships. We begin
There is some evidence to suggest that socially with- with a short discussion of the role of individual or dispo-
drawn children are more likely than their age-mates to sitional temperament and biological factors. Following
be chronically friendless. In a summer camp study con- this, we examine the association between the parent-
ducted by Parker and Seal (1996), chronically friend- child and child-peer relationship systems. We focus pri-
less children were rated by their peers to be more shy marily on research conducted in the framework of
and timid, to spend more time playing alone, and to be attachment theory. Following our discussion of attach-
more sensitive than children who possessed a mutual ment theory, we examine the relevant literature on par-
best friendship during the summer camp program. Addi- enting beliefs and behaviors.
tionally, counselors rated these friendless children as
less mature, less socially skilled, and as displaying more
Temperament, Social Behaviors,
withdrawn and anxious behaviors than children with
and Peer Relationships
friends. The aforementioned study is the only investiga-
tion to date of chronically friendless children, and the Recently, temperament has been construed as constitu-
summer camp setting may have influenced the results in tionally based individual differences in emotional, mo-
a significant fashion. If some socially withdrawn chil- toric, and attentional reactivity and the regulation
dren are shy (e.g., Rubin, Burgess, Kennedy, & Stewart, thereof (Rothbart, Ellis, & Posner, 2004). Researchers
2003), then establishing friendships amongst unfamiliar who study temperament report that individuals differ
others may prove somewhat overwhelming for many of not only in the ease with which positive and negative
these children. However, in a familiar setting, such as emotions may be aroused (emotionality) but also in the
school, withdrawn children may have less difficulty ease with which emotions, once aroused, can be regu-
forming and keeping friendships over the course of the lated (Rothbart et al., 2004). In some respects, a better
school year. Conversely, as noted earlier, social with- term for emotionality is reactivity in that most research
drawal increases in salience with age to peers. Thus, it on the phenomenon is focused on the extent to which
is possible that the negative reputation accorded so- children react to situations or events with anger, irri-
cially withdrawn children may hinder friendship forma- tability, or fear. And again, most contemporary re-
tion and maintenance processes. In any event, an searchers have been interested in the ways in which
investigation of the consistent absence of friendship reactive responses can be self-regulated. Thus, re-
among withdrawn and aggressive children relative to searchers have centered on the ef fortful self-control of
their nonwithdrawn, nonaggressive age-mates may prove emotional, behavioral, and attentional processes (San-
illuminating. son, Hemphill, & Smart, 2004).
The constructs of difficult temperament, activity
level, inhibition, and sociability merit special attention
in the study of peer interactions and relationships. Dif fi-
DISTAL PREDICTORS OF cult temperament refers to the frequent and intense ex-
CHILDRENS SOCIAL SKILLS pression of negative affect (Thomas & Chess, 1977).
AND PEER RELATIONSHIPS Fussiness and irritability would be characteristic of a
difficult infant or toddler. In reactivity/regulation ter-
The quality of childrens extrafamilial social lives is minology, the dif ficult child is one whose negative emo-
likely a product of factors internal and external to the tions are easily aroused and difficult to soothe or
618 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

regulate. The highly active baby/toddler is one who is reticent children display less socially competent and
easily excited and motorically facile. Again, these chil- prosocial behaviors, employ fewer positive coping
dren are easily arousedthat is, highly reactive. Inhib- strategies, and are more likely to develop anxiety prob-
ited infants/toddlers are timid, vigilant, and fearful lems than their nonreticent age-mates (e.g., Coplan
when faced with novel social stimuli; like the other et al., 1994; Eisenberg, Shepard, Fabes, Murphy, &
groups of children, their emotions are easily aroused Guthrie, 1998). Moreover, reticence and social with-
and difficult to regulate. Finally, children who are out- drawal has been found to predict peer rejection and vic-
going and open in response to social novelty are de- timization from as early as the preschool years (e.g.,
scribed as sociable (Kagan, 1999). Gazelle & Ladd, 2003; Hart et al., 2000).
Each of these temperamental characteristics is rela- It has been suggested that dispositional characteristics
tively stable (e.g., Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, related to emotion regulation may lay the basis for the
2000), and each is related to particular constellations of emergence of childrens social behaviors and relation-
social behaviors that we described earlier as character- ships. For example, Rubin, Coplan, Fox, and Calkins
istic of either popular or rejected children. The concep- (1995) have argued that the social consequences of emo-
tual model that drives much of the longitudinal tion dysregulation vary in accord with the childs behav-
research connecting temperament to peer interactions ioral tendency to approach and interact with peers during
and relationships is rather straightforward. Tempera- free play. They found that sociable children whose ap-
ment processes, such as emotional reactivity or effortful proach behaviors lacked regulatory control were disrup-
control, are posited to underpin the presentation of tive and aggressive; those who were sociable but able to
given social behaviors; these behaviors, in turn, are regulate their emotions were socially competent. Unso-
thought to predict childrens relationships with their ciable children who were good emotion regulators ap-
peers (e.g., Eisenberg, 2002). peared to suffer no ill effects from their lack of social
In keeping with this perspective, infants and toddlers behavior; when playing alone, they were productive en-
who have been identified as having difficult and/or ac- gagers in constructive and exploratory activity. They
tive temperament, or as emotionally reactive are more were neither anxious amongst peers nor rated by parents
likely to behave in aggressive, impulsive ways in early as having socioemotional difficulties. Unsociable chil-
childhood (e.g., Hay, Castle, & Davies, 2000; Rubin, dren who were poor emotion regulators were more behav-
Burgess, Dwyer, & Hastings, 2003). Contemporaneous iorally anxious and wary, more reticent than constructive
and predictive connections between negative emotional- when playing alone, and were viewed by parents as having
ity and/or difficult temperament and childrens aggres- more internalizing problems than their age-mates. Thus,
sive and oppositional behavior have been discovered by emotionally dysregulated preschoolers may behave in
researchers the world over (e.g., Keenan, Shaw, & Delli- ways that will elicit peer rejection and inhibit the devel-
quadri, 1998; Russell, Hart, Robinson, & Olson, 2003; opment of qualitatively adaptive friendships. Further,
Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2002). And, as we noted this is the case for emotionally dysregulated sociable as
earlier, undercontrolled, impulsive, and aggressive be- well as unsociable children.
havior is associated contemporaneously and predictively The results of Rubin et al.s (1995) study are clearly
with peer relationships characterized by rejection. In- in keeping with findings from Eisenberg and colleagues
deed, negative emotionality itself has been associated extensive research program on young childrens emo-
with peer rejection (e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & tional arousal and regulation. Eisenberg and colleagues
Reiser, 2000). have consistently found that emotion dysregulation is a
Similarly, behavioral inhibition, an individual trait concomitant and predictor of behavioral solitude (e.g.,
identified in infancy and toddlerhood predicts the dis- Fabes, Hanish, Martin, & Eisenberg, 2002; Spinrad
play of shyness and socially withdrawn behavior in early et al., 2004) and externalizing forms of behavior (e.g.,
and middle childhood (Kagan, 1999; Rubin, Burgess, & Eisenberg, Cumberland, et al., 2001) in the peer group.
Hastings, 2002). Contemporaneous connections be- Relatedly, researchers have found that the abilities to
tween behavioral inhibition and childrens shy or so- regulate negative emotions and to inhibit the expression
cially reticent behavior have been found during early of undesirable affect and behavior (regulatory control)
and middle childhood and adolescence (e.g., Pfeifer, are associated with, and predictive of, social compe-
Goldsmith, Davidson, & Rickman, 2002). Shy, socially tence and peer acceptance (e.g., Eisenberg, Pidada, &
Distal Predictors of Childrens Social Skills and Peer Relationships 619

Liew, 2001; Eisenberg, Spinrad, Fabes, Reiser, et al., features of the social universe, thereby enhancing the
2004; Gunnar, Sebanc, Tout, Donzella, & Van Dulmen, development of social skills. Third, it is in the parent-
2003; Kochanska, Murray, & Coy, 1997), while an in- child relationship that the child begins to develop expec-
ability to regulate affect is associated with socially in- tations and assumptions about interactions and
competent behavior (e.g., Calkins & Dedmon, 2000; relationships with other people.
Calkins, Gill, Johnson, & Smith, 1999). Importantly,
these findings appear to be consistent across cultures The Parent-Child Attachment Relationship
(e.g., Eisenberg, Pidada, & Liew, 2001; Zhou, Eisen- A basic premise of attachment theory is that the early
berg, Wang, & Reiser, 2004). mother-infant relationship lays the groundwork for chil-
drens understanding of, and participation in, subse-
Temperament and Friendship quent extrafamilial relationships. And, since the quality
Most research associating temperament-related con- of attachment relationships with the mother may vary,
structs and peer relationships have focused on peer pop- subsequent social success and relationships with peers
ularity or rejection. There has been little work in which is expected to vary as well.
temperament has been associated with aspects of The putative, proximal causes of the development of a
friendship. Stocker and Dunn (1990) found that sociable secure attachment relationship are the expressions of
children were rated as having more positive relation- parental responsivity, warmth, and sensitivity (e.g., Bel-
ships with friends; highly emotional children had less sky & Cassidy, 1995). The sensitive and responsive par-
successful relationships with friends. Dunn and Cutting ent recognizes the infants or toddlers emotional
(1999), in a study of young children, found that negative signals, considers the childs perspective, and responds
emotionality was associated with the observed fre- promptly and appropriately to the childs needs. In turn,
quency of failed social bids and with less amity directed it is posited that the child develops a belief system that
to the best friend; as a counterpoint, children showed incorporates the parent as someone who can be relied on
less amity to friends who were inhibited or shy. More re- for protection, nurturance, comfort, and security; a
cently, in a study of young adolescents, Pike and Atz- sense of trust in relationships results from the secure in-
aba-Poria (2003) reported that sociability was related to fant /toddler-parent bond. Furthermore, the child forms
positive aspects of perceived friendship quality, a belief that the self is competent and worthy of positive
whereas negative emotionality was associated with response from others. The process by which a secure at-
friendship conflict. tachment relationship is thought to result in the develop-
ment of social competence and positive relationships
Summary with peers may be described briefly as follows. The in-
ternal working model of the securely attached young
In summary, researchers suggest that individual, dispo-
child allows him or her to feel secure, confident, and
sitionally based characteristics may set the stage for the
self-assured when introduced to novel settings; this
development of particular types of parent-child relation-
sense of felt security fosters the childs active explo-
ships and for the development of social behavioral pro-
ration of the social environment (Sroufe, 1983). In turn,
files that ultimately predict the quality of childrens
exploration of the social milieu leads to peer interaction
peer relationships.
and play. And as we noted earlier, peer interaction and
play allow children to experience the interpersonal ex-
Parent-Child Attachment Relationships, Social change of ideas, perspectives, roles, and actions. From
Behaviors, and Peer Relationships such social interchanges, children develop skills that
lead to the development of positive peer relationships.
According to Hartup (1985), parents serve at least three Alternatively, the development of an insecure attach-
functions in the childs development of social compe- ment relationship is posited to result in the childs devel-
tence and qualitatively positive peer relationships. oping an internal working model that interpersonal
First, parent-child interaction is a context in which many relationships are rejecting or neglectful (Bowlby, 1973).
competencies necessary for social interaction develop. Attachment theorists have suggested also that the expec-
Second, the parent-child relationship constitutes a tations and assumptions that infants hold about others,
safety net permitting the child the freedom to examine and the means by which they cope with these cognitions,
620 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

are internalized and carried forward into subsequent re- tent than their insecure counterparts, whilst insecure
lationships. Thus, it has been proposed that, in their sub- children exhibit more aggression and withdrawal (Allen,
sequent peer relationships, insecure avoidant infants Moore, Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998; Rose-Krasnor, Rubin,
are guided by previously reinforced expectations of Booth, & Coplan, 1996; Schmidt, DeMulder, & Den-
parental rejection; hence, they are believed to perceive ham, 2002; Simons, Paternite, & Shore, 2001; Steven-
peers as potentially hostile and tend to strike out proac- son-Hinde & Marshall, 1999). Extensive reviews of
tively and aggressively (Troy & Sroufe, 1987). Insecure related literature may be found in Thompson (Chapter 2,
ambivalent infants, alternatively, are thought to be this Handbook, this volume).
guided by a fear of rejection; consequently, in their ex-
The Parent-Child Attachment Relationship and
trafamilial peer relationships they are posited to attempt
Childrens Peer Relationships: Empirical Support
to avoid rejection through passive, adult-dependent be-
havior and withdrawal from the prospects of peer inter- If the quality of the attachment relationship is associ-
action (Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & ated with, and predictive of, patterns of social inter-
Sroufe, 1989). action, it seems logical to propose a relation between
attachment status and the childs standing in the peer
The Parent-Child Attachment Relationship and
group. In a recent meta-analysis of the extant literature
Childrens Social Behaviors: Empirical Support
on links between attachment and peer acceptance,
Securely attached infants are more likely than their in- Schneider, Atkinson, and Tardiff (2001) found a small-
secure counterparts to demonstrate socially competent to-moderate effect size between these domains.
behaviors amongst peers during the toddler (e.g., Pastor,
Attachment and Friendship
1981), preschool (e.g., Booth, Rose-Krasnor, & Rubin,
1991), and elementary school periods (e.g., Elicker, En- According to Booth, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, and Burgess
glund, & Sroufe, 1992). Insecure babies, especially (2004), although associations between attachment secu-
those classified as avoidant, later exhibit more hostility, rity and social competence and peer acceptance are the-
anger, and aggressive behavior in preschool settings than oretically meaningful, there is an even more compelling
their secure counterparts (e.g., Burgess, Marshall, rationale for the link between attachment security and
Rubin, & Fox, 2003; Shaw, Owens, Vondra, Keenan, & friendship. From attachment theory, one would expect
Winslow, 1996). Insecure-ambivalent infants are more that the trust and intimacy characterizing secure child-
easily frustrated, and socially inhibited at 2 years than parent relationships should produce an internalized
their secure age-mates (e.g., Fox & Calkins, 1993). At 4 model of relationship expectations that affects the qual-
years of age, children classified at 1 year as ambivalent ity of relationships with friends. In support of this theo-
have been described as fearful and lacking in assertive- retically driven expectation, Schneider et al. (2001), in a
ness (Kochanska, 1998). Spangler and Schieche (1998) meta-analysis, found a larger effect size linking attach-
have reported that of the 16 C babies they identified ment security with friendship than with peer relation-
in their research, 15 were rated by their mothers as be- ships more generally.
haviorally inhibited. As noted earlier, it has been sug- For example, Youngblade and Belsky (1992) reported
gested that inhibition in infancy and toddlerhood is a that securely attached infants were less likely than in-
precursor of social withdrawal in early and middle secure infants to have negative and asynchronous
childhood. Finally, evidence that disorganized/disori- friendships at 5 years of age. Freitag, Belsky, Gross-
ented attachment status in infancy predicts the subse- mann, Grossmann, and Scheurer-Englisch (1996) found
quent display of aggression amongst preschool and that children who had positive early relationships with
elementary school peers derives from several sources their parents were more likely to have a close friend at
(e.g., Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, & Cibelli, 1997). age 10. Also, secure parent-child attachment in late
It is also the case that secure and insecure attach- childhood and early adolescence is associated posi-
ments, as assessed in early and middle childhood, as tively (and contemporaneously) with positive qualities
well as in early adolescence are associated contempora- of childrens close peer relationships (Lieberman,
neously with and predictive of adaptive and maladaptive Doyle, & Markiewicz, 1999; Rubin, Dwyer, et al.,
social behaviors respectively. For example, children who 2004). And Clark and Ladd (2000) have reported that
experience a secure relationship with their mothers (and parent-child connectedness, an essential element of the
fathers) have been found to be more sociable and compe- attachment bond, is associated with higher levels of
Distal Predictors of Childrens Social Skills and Peer Relationships 621

harmony and lower levels of conflict in the friendships about how children become socially competent, how
of young children. family contexts should be structured to shape childrens
Whether specific attachment classifications predict behaviors, and how and when children should be taught
types of friendships characteristics is, as yet, unknown. to initiate and maintain relationships with others (Bu-
Recently, Hodges, Finnegan, and Perry (1999) have sug- gental & Happaney, 2002; Rubin & Burgess, 2002).
gested that an important feature in close relationships is These ideas about child rearing and about what is ac-
the balance between autonomy and connectedness. They ceptable and unacceptable child behavior in the social
suggested that this relationship orientation or relation- world are culturally determined. Extended discussions
ship stance may be conceptualized and coded as of such cultural determination may be found in Rubin
avoidant or preoccupied: An avoidant child may be char- and Chung (in press).
acterized by showing very little emotion on reunion
Parents Beliefs about Adaptive Child Behaviors
with or on separation from the relationship partner, may
and Relationships
avoid the partner when in a state of distress, and re-
nounce the importance of the relationship. A preoccu- Parents of socially competent children believe that, in
pied child may show extreme distress when the early childhood, they should play an active role in the
relationship partner is needed but absent, and may be socialization of social skills via teaching and providing
acutely sensitive to the possibility of rejection by, and peer interaction opportunities (Rubin, Mills, & Rose-
separation from, the partner. Hodges et al. (1999) found Krasnor, 1989). They believe also that when their chil-
that relationship orientation with a best friend could be dren display maladaptive behaviors, it is due to
moderately predicted from the childs relationship ori- transitory and situationally caused circumstances. Par-
entation with his or her parents, a finding that is ex- ents whose preschoolers display socially incompetent
plained by attachment theory and Bowlbys (1969/1982) behaviors, alternatively, are less likely to endorse strong
notion of generalized internal working models. Hodges beliefs in the development of social skills (Rubin et al.,
and colleagues (1999) do not consider their measure of 1989). Furthermore, they are more likely to attribute
relationships stance to be equivalent to an attachment the development of social competence to internal factors
classification. However, their research represents an im- (Children are born that way), to believe that incompe-
portant step in the direction of matching characteristics tent behavior is difficult to alter, and to believe that in-
of parent-child and friend relationships. terpersonal skills are best taught through direct
instructional means (Rubin et al., 1989).
Summary
One conclusion that may derive from these findings
There is growing evidence that the quality of parent- is that parental involvement in the promotion of social
child attachment relationships is associated with and competence is mediated by strong beliefs in the impor-
predictive of qualitatively good friendships. This being tance of social skills. When a socially competent child
the case, we might expect future research to focus on re- demonstrates poor social performance, parents who
lations between relationships systems and examine place a relatively high value on social competence are
whether (and when) childrens friendships can augment likely to become the most involved and responsive. Over
(or exacerbate) the relations between parent-child at- time, such involvement may be positively reinforced by
tachment and adaptation or maladaptation. the childs acquisition of social skills. At the same time,
parents are likely to value the social skills displayed by
Parenting and Childrens Social Behaviors and their children, and these children will be perceived as
Peer Relationships interpersonally competent and capable of autonomous
learning. Hence, parental beliefs and child characteris-
Parental Beliefs
tics will influence each other in a reciprocal manner
Parents ideas, beliefs, and perceptions about the devel- (Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, Bigras, Mills, & Booth, 1996).
opment and maintenance of childrens social behaviors
The Child as Parental Belief Evocateur
and relationships predict, and presumably partially ex-
plain the development of socially adaptive and maladap- In keeping with the perspective that the parent-child re-
tive interactive behaviors and peer relationships in lationship reflects the contributions of both partners, it
childhood. This is true because parents child-rearing is important to understand that parental beliefs may be
practices represent a behavioral expression of their ideas evoked by child characteristics and behavior (Bornstein,
622 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

2002). For example, the problematic child who when children produce a high frequency of socially
demonstrates maladaptive social behaviors and who wary, withdrawn behaviors their parents (a) recognize
does not get along with her or his peers is likely to evoke this as a problem; ( b) express feelings of concern, sym-
different parental emotions and cognitions than the pathy, guilt, embarrassment, and, with increasing child
normal child (Bugental, 1992). When this latter group age, a growing sense of frustration; and (c) are more in-
of children behaves in maladaptive or socially inappro- clined than parents of nonwary children to attribute
priate manners, they may activate parental feelings of their childrens social reticence to dispositional traits
concern, puzzlement, and, in the case of aggression, (Hastings & Rubin, 1999). Perhaps in an attempt to reg-
anger. These parental emotions are regulated by the par- ulate their own expressed guilt and embarrassment ema-
ents attempts to understand, rationalize, or justify the nating from their childrens ineffectual behaviors,
childs behavior and by the parents knowledge of the mothers of socially withdrawn preschoolers indicate
childs social skills history and the known quality of that they would react to their childrens displays of so-
the childs social relationships at home, at school, and in cial withdrawal by providing them with protection and
the neighborhood. Thus, in the case of nonproblematic direct instruction (Mills & Rubin, 1998). To release the
children, the evocative stimulus produces adaptive, solu- child from social discomfort, the parents of socially
tion-focused parental ideation that results in the par- wary children have indicated that they would solve the
ents choice of a reasoned, sensitive, and responsive childs social dilemmas by asking other children for in-
approach to dealing with the problem behavior (Bugen- formation desired by the child, obtaining objects desired
tal, 1992). In turn, the child views the parent as support- by the child, or requesting that peers allow the child to
ive and learns to better understand how to behave and join them in play (Rubin & Burgess, 2002).
feel in similar situations as they occur in the future. As In summary, it is suggested that parental beliefs in-
such, a reciprocal connection is developed between the fluence parental behavior; in turn, parental behavior in-
ways and means of adult and child social information fluences the development, maintenance, and inhibition
processing. of childrens social behaviors, which, as we noted ear-
But how does the socially incompetent childs pre- lier, influence the quality of their peer relationships.
sentation of socially maladaptive behavior affect the Consistent with this view, parents of aggressive and
parent? In the case of aggressive children, any hostile withdrawn children have been found to differ from those
behavior, whether directed at peers, siblings, or parents of typical children in the ways in which they think about
may evoke (a) strong parental feelings of anger and frus- socializing social skills and in the ways that they report
tration (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Eisenberg, reacting to their childrens maladaptive behaviors.
Gershoff, et al., 2001) and ( b) biased attributions that
blame the childs noxious behavior on traits, inten- Parenting Behaviors, Childrens Social Skills,
tions, and motives internal to the child (e.g., Strassberg, and Peer Relationships
1995). These parental cognitions and emotions, predict Parents may influence the development of social behav-
the use of power assertive and restrictive disciplinary iors, interaction patterns, and ultimately, the quality of
techniques (Colwell, Mize, & Pettit, 2002; Coplan, their childrens peer relationships by (a) providing op-
Hastings, Lagace-Seguin, & Moulton, 2002). This type portunities for their children to have contact with peers;
of low warmth-high control parental response, mediated ( b) monitoring of their childrens peer encounters (when
by affect and beliefs/cognitions about the intentionality necessary); (c) coaching their children to deal compe-
of the child behavior, the historical precedence of child tently with interpersonal peer-related tasks; and (d) dis-
aggression, and the best means to control child aggres- ciplining unacceptable, maladaptive peer directed
sion, is likely to evoke negative affect and cognitions in behaviors (e.g., Parke & ONeill, 1999).
the child. The result of this interplay between parent and
child beliefs, affects, and behavior may be the reinforce- Parental Coaching and Managing
ment and extension of family cycles of hostility (Carson Research suggests that parents vary widely in the extent
& Parke, 1996; Dishion, Duncan, Eddy, Fagot, & of their efforts to provide opportunities for peer inter-
Fetrow, 1994; Granic & Lamey, 2002). action for their children and to coach their children in
Parental reactions to social wariness and fearfulness specific social skills. Moreover, the available evidence
are less well understood. Researchers have found that suggests that parents efforts in these areas have implica-
Distal Predictors of Childrens Social Skills and Peer Relationships 623

tions for their young childrens success with peers (see and incompetent children are a consequence, rather than
Kerns, Cole, & Andrews, 1998; Mize & Pettit, 1997; the cause, of their childrens success with peers. Thus, it
Pettit, Brown, Mize, & Lindsey, 1998). Ladd and Golter would be timely to examine whether very young children
(1988), for example, found that parents who actively identified as being relatively unpopular with peers could
arranged peer contacts and who indirectly supervised shake their early reputations if their parents (mothers
these contacts had preschoolers who were better liked by and fathers) were trained in parental monitoring and
their peers. In addition, children whose parents relied on coaching skills.
indirect rather than direct monitoring of their childrens
peer contacts were less hostile toward peers. These find- Parenting Behaviors
ings have been supported in subsequent studies (e.g., Parents (usually mothers) of unpopular and/or peer re-
Pettit et al., 1998). In a follow-up, short-term longitudi- jected children have been reported to use inept, intru-
nal study, Ladd and Hart (1992) found that mothers sive, harsh, and authoritarian disciplinary and
over- and underinvolvement in arranging and monitoring socialization practices more frequently than those of
peer contacts could be detrimental to childrens social their more popular counterparts (e.g., Carson & Parke,
success, at least among boys. Boys whose mothers were 1996; McDowell & Parke, 2000; Pettit, Clawson,
moderately involved in initiating their childs peer con- Dodge, & Bates, 1996). These data seem to hold true for
tacts displayed significant gains in peer status over time parents of preschoolers through elementary school chil-
compared to boys with over- and underinvolved mothers. dren. Alternately, parents of popular children use more
Girls made significant gains in peer status only when feelings-oriented reasoning and induction, responsivity,
their own efforts to initiate contact with other children warm control (authoritative), and positivity during com-
were large in comparison to those of their mothers (i.e., munication than their unpopular counterparts (e.g.,
when their mothers were underinvolved). Mize & Pettit, 1997).
Finnie and Russell (1988) found that during play with In regard to the actual process that links parenting to
an unfamiliar age-mate, mothers of unpopular children the childs peer relationships, it is possible to consider
were more likely to avoid supervising their children and that parenting styles may promote particular child be-
to supervise their children less skillfully than mothers haviors that mark a child for acceptance or rejection. To
of more popular children. Mothers of more popular chil- this end, researchers have demonstrated that mothers of
dren were more active and effective in supervising their socially competent children are more child-centered,
childrens peer related behaviors than mothers of less more feelings-oriented, warmer, and more likely to use
well-accepted children. In a follow-up study, Russell positive verbalizations, reasoning, and explanations
and Finnie (1990) examined mothers instructions to than mothers of less competent children (e.g., Mize &
their child immediately prior to the childs opportunity Pettit, 1997; Rose-Krasnor et al., 1996).
to play with an unfamiliar child. Mothers of popular With regard to socially incompetent behaviors, re-
children were more likely than mothers of low-status searchers have shown consistently that aggressive chil-
(rejected and neglected) children to make group-ori- dren have parents who model and inadvertently
ented statements during both the anticipatory instruc- reinforce aggressive and impulsive behavior, and who
tion period as well as during the play session itself. are cold and rejecting, physically punitive, and inconsis-
Mothers of low-status children were more disruptive of tent in their disciplinary behaviors. In addition to
their childrens play. parental rejection and the use of high power-assertive
In summary, research indicates that when mothers and inconsistent disciplinary strategies, parental per-
are involved in effective ways in coaching their children missiveness, indulgence, and lack of supervision have
through difficulties with peers, facilitating their chil- often been found to correlate with childrens aggressive
drens play with peers, and providing their children with behavior (see Rubin & Burgess, 2002, and Dodge et al.,
opportunities to play with peers, their children are more Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume, for recent re-
popular among their age-mates. However, all of the re- views). It may not be difficult to understand these asso-
search in this area is correlational and virtually none of ciations given that parental tolerance and neglect of the
the extant research is focused on fathers. It is entirely childs aggressive behavior may actually have the impli-
possible that the observed differences between the cation of legitimization and encouragement of aggres-
mothers of socially popular and unpopular or competent sion. Importantly, these findings appear to have
624 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

cross-cultural universality (e.g., Cheah & Rubin, 2004; mance; antisocial behavior and poor academic perfor-
Chen & Rubin, 1994; Schneider, Attili, Vermigli, & mance, in turn, were associated with higher levels of
Younger, 1997). peer rejection.
Relative to the literature on the parenting behaviors These findings have been replicated and extended in
associated with undercontrolled, aggressive children lit- a similar study conducted in the Peoples Republic of
tle is known about social wariness and withdrawal. Re- China (Chen & Rubin, 1994). The pathway from
search concerning the parenting behaviors and styles parental authoritarian, punitive disciplinary practices to
associated with social withdrawal focuses clearly on child aggression to peer rejection was replicated, but the
two potential socialization contributorsovercontrol authors also found that parental warmth and authorita-
and overprotection. Parents who use high power-as- tive control predicted social competence, which pre-
sertive strategies and who place many constraints on dicted peer acceptance. These latter results suggest that
their children tend to rear shy, reserved, and dependent the pathways to peer acceptance and rejection may be
children. Thus, the issuance of parental commands com- generalized across cultures.
bined with constraints on exploration and independence There is also the possibility that the link between
may hinder the development of competence in the social parenting and child outcomes of an adaptive or maladap-
milieu. Restrictive control may also deprive the child of tive nature can be attenuated by the quality of the
opportunities to interact with peers. It should not be sur- childs status in the peer group or the quality of his or
prising that children who are socially withdrawn are on her friendships. For example, the longitudinal relation
the receiving end of parental overcontrol and overpro- between harsh parenting and negative outcomes of an
tection (e.g., Rubin, Burgess, & Hastings, 2002; Rubin, externalizing nature is augmented when children have
Cheah, & Fox, 2001). These findings concerning poor peer relationships (e.g., Criss, Pettit, Bates,
parental overcontrol and restriction stem from very few Dodge, & Lapp, 2002; Lansford, Criss, Pettit, Dodge, &
studies, most of which center on children of preschool Bates, 2003). And Schwartz, Dodge, Pettit, and Bates
age. Furthermore, the contexts in which parents of so- (2000) found that children who experienced harsh home
cially withdrawn children display overcontrol and over- environments in the preschool years were more likely to
protection have not been well specified. Thus, unlike the be victimized by peers in the third and fourth grades;
literature on the parents of aggressive children, the so- however, this correlation was stronger for those who had
cialization correlates and causes of social withdrawal a lower number of friendships.
are not well-known. This dearth of data represents an Researchers have shown that the relation between in-
open research agenda for future investigation. security of attachment and negative outcome can be
moderated by friendship quality. Thus an insecure at-
Parenting Behaviors and Childrens Social tachment relationship may predict difficulties of an ex-
Competence: A Model ternalizing or internalizing nature, but only for those
In summary, there is some support for the contention children or young adolescents who lack friendship or
that parental behavior is associated, not only with the qualitatively rich friendship (e.g., Rubin, Dwyer, et al.,
development of childrens social competence, but also 2004). Thus, in recent models pertaining to the links
with their peer relationships (see Ladd & Pettit, 2002, between parenting and adaptive or maladaptive out-
for a review). The assumption has been that parenting come, it appears as if, by middle to late childhood, chil-
leads to social competence or incompetence, which drens friendships may buffer or exacerbate the
leads to peer acceptance or rejection. This causal model statistical associations.
has been tested in a number of studies.
Dishion (1990) examined the relations among grade- Summary
school boys sociometric status, academic skills, anti- The existing research supports the general conclusion
social behavior, and several elements of parental that socially successful children have mothers (and,
discipline practices and family circumstances. Causal where examined, fathers) who are more feelings-ori-
modeling suggested that the relation between inept par- ented, more positive, more skillful, more likely to use
enting and peer rejection was mediated by boys anti- inductive reasoning, and less negative and coercive in
social behavior and academic difficulties: Lower levels their interaction with their children than their socially
of parental skill were associated with higher levels of unsuccessful counterparts. The limits that the correla-
antisocial behavior and lower levels of academic perfor- tional nature of this workplace on our interpretations
Childhood Peer Experiences and Later Adjustment 625

should be recognized, however. Although it is likely that School is where many children meet peers, form friend-
parents behaviors have an influence on their childrens ships, and take part in groups. For friendless, rejected,
behavior and success with peers, it must be acknowl- or victimized children, the school must be a less-than-
edged that parental behavior may be elicited by their desirable context and certainly a place that is unlikely to
childrens characteristics (Belsky, 1997; Putnam, San- promote learning or well-being. This is likely to be the
son, & Rothbart, 2002). Relatedly, it should be noted case, not only for the child who was doing poorly in
that, with few exceptions, research in this area focuses school to begin with, but also for the intellectually com-
on the concurrent relations between parental practices petent child who has trouble becoming part of the peer
and childrens social adjustment with peers and not system. For these individuals, withdrawing via truancy
these relations over time. Thus, although we take this or by dropping out may serve as the escape route to avoid
work as generally supportive of a link between early constant rejection or victimization by peers. Alterna-
parental behaviors contributing to childrens later social tively, having a friend with whom one can share the
success, this link has not been thoroughly demonstrated. struggles associated with acquiring new forms of aca-
demic competence may prove entirely helpful. For these
reasons, peer relationships have been studied as a form
CHILDHOOD PEER EXPERIENCES AND of social engagement and social motivation that has
LATER ADJUSTMENT wide ranging positive and negative effects on academic
performance and a childs sense of belonging and ad-
Our goal, in this section, is to provide a summary of re- justment (Juvonen & Wentzel, 1996).
search in which the primary focus has been to identify It has been shown that adjustment to school derives
aspects of childhood peer relationship experiences that from several aspects of childrens relationships with
predict subsequent adaptation and maladaptation. The peers. Wentzel and Asher (1995) found that popular
predictors we examine fall at the levels of dyadic children were viewed as helpful, good students. Re-
(friendship) and group (peer acceptance) relationships. jected/aggressive students, relative to average and re-
Although we fully recognize that social behaviors (e.g., jected/submissive children, showed little interest in
aggression), dispositions (e.g., temperament), and inter- school, were perceived by teachers as dependent, and
actions (e.g., interactive conflict evoked by differences were seen by peers and teachers as inconsiderate, non-
of opinion) may predict adaptive and malevolent out- compliant, and prone to causing trouble in school. Many
comes, relevant discussions are presented elsewhere in of the problems that lead to rejection, such as the display
this volume. of disruptive and aggressive behavior, make it difficult
Significantly, the associations between the quality of for a child to adjust to the climate of most classrooms.
peer relationships in childhood and subsequent difficul- These findings were consistent with longitudinal
ties have generally been examined in one of two ways. findings reported by Ollendick, Weist, Borden, and
First, using case-control or follow-back designs, re- Greene (1992) who showed that children who were ac-
searchers have asked whether maladjusted and adjusted tively disliked by their peers were anywhere from two to
adolescents or adults differed as children in their ad- seven times more likely to fail a subsequent grade than
justment with peers. Second, with the cohort, prospec- better accepted children. Similarly, Coie, Lochman,
tive, or follow-up design, researchers have asked Terry, and Hyman (1992), in a 3-year longitudinal
whether popular and unpopular children differ in their study, found that higher levels of social rejection pre-
incidence of later psychological and educational adapta- dicted later grade retention and poorer adjustment to the
tion. We provided a lengthy overview of retrospective transition to middle school. Likewise, based on a 4-year
studies in the previous iteration of this chapter (Rubin, longitudinal study, DeRosier, Kupersmidt, and Patter-
Bukowski, & Parker, 1998). Here, we focus only on son (1994) reported that the experience of peer rejection
studies in which prospective, follow-forward designs in any 1 of the first 3 years of their study placed chil-
have been employed. dren at significantly greater risk for absenteeism in the
4th year, even after statistically controlling for initial
Academic Adjustment levels of absenteeism.
Given these longitudinal connections between peer
For many children and adolescents, the primary venue rejection and later poor school performance and tru-
for their experiences with peers is the school context. ancy, it is not surprising to learn that children who have
626 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

troubled relationships with their peers are more likely to transition to kindergarten. Although childrens personal
drop out of school than are other children. For example, attributes (mental age and prior school experience) pre-
Ollendick and colleagues (1992) found (in a 5-year lon- dicted early school performance, measures of social ad-
gitudinal study) that 17.5% of rejected children had justment with peers were much better predictors by
dropped out of school before the end of ninth grade com- comparison. Children with many friends at the time of
pared to 5.4% of popular or average children. school entry developed more favorable attitudes toward
Factors other than peer rejection appear to be impor- school in the early months than children with fewer
tant also. Most notably friendships appear to influence friends. Those who maintained their friendships also
school adjustment in many ways. In a longitudinal study liked school better as the year went by. Making new
with a representative sample of 475 12-year-olds, Cairns friends in the classroom also predicted gains in school
et al. (1989) found little reason to conclude that peer re- performance. By comparison, measures of school per-
jection by itself carries risk of later dropping out. In- formance at the start of the transition to kindergarten
stead, the most powerful precursors of later dropping did not generally forecast gains in social adjustment. In
out were aggression and academic difficulties, espe- addition, children who were rejected by peers were less
cially when the latter were simultaneously present. They likely than other children to have positive attitudes to-
showed that many school dropouts appeared to have sat- ward school and they were less likely to show a positive
isfactory social lives and, as a result, gravitate to peers school performance. These findings show clearly that
who shared their negative dispositions toward school. even during the early childhood years, friendships with
These conditions, in turn, lead to lower academic per- and acceptance by peers are strongly linked to chil-
formance and, in some cases, school drop out. drens academic success. Because Ladd used a longitu-
These latter findings are important because they dinal design in which initial assessments of academic
show that peer group norms may influence academic orientation and peer relationships were accounted for,
performance. For example, Kindermann (1993) identi- his findings suggest a causal link between friendship
fied the subgroups that constituted the larger peer and academic outcome.
groups in the childrens elementary school classrooms. In a subsequent study, Ladd, Kochenderfer, and Cole-
Each group was assessed according to its overall level of man (1997) examined the association between chil-
the academic motivational orientation. He found that drens perceptions of best friendship quality in
children typically associated with peers who had a mo- kindergarten and indices of scholastic adjustment
tivational orientation similar to their own. Moreover, (school-related affect, perceptions, involvement, and
using a longitudinal design, he found that childrens mo- performance) in grade school (transition from kinder-
tivational orientations toward school were in accord garten to grade school). Their main finding replicated,
with the initial orientation of the peer group in which at the dyadic level, one of the findings observed by Kin-
they were constituents. Recently, Hymel, Comfort, dermann (1993) at the group level. Specifically, Ladd
Schonert-Reichl, and McDougall (2002) noted that ado- et al. (1997) reported that children who viewed their
lescents who drop out of school are more likely than friendships as a source of validation or aid, tend to (a)
other students to have associated with peers who do not feel happier at school, ( b) see their classmates as sup-
regard school as useful and important. These authors ar- portive, and (c) develop positive attitudes toward school.
gued that the two variables from the peer system that In two studies, the effect of early adolescent friend-
appear to be associated with school drop out are (1) peer ship was demonstrated clearly and in richer ways than
rejection and (2) close association with peers who place seen previously. Berndt, Hawkins, and Jiao (1999)
little emphasis on academic achievement and active showed that adjustment to junior high school was facili-
school participation. tated by engagement in friendships that were stable and
Similar factors seem to be important with younger of high quality (e.g., rated as high in closeness and sup-
children also. In a series of studies, Ladd and colleagues port). Wentzel, McNamara-Barry, and Caldwell (2004)
(Ladd, 1990, 1991; Ladd, Kochenderfer, & Coleman, also examined friendship and the adjustment to a junior
1996, 1997) demonstrated the potential influence of high school. They showed that friendless children were
close dyadic relationships on academic performance. lower in prosocial behavior and higher in affective dis-
Ladd (1990) obtained repeated measures of friendship, tress both concurrently and 2 years later. They noted
sociometric status, and school adjustment during the that friends characteristics can act as a form of social
Childhood Peer Experiences and Later Adjustment 627

motivation that can either increase or decrease an early as having more conduct problems, aggression, motor ex-
adolescents adjustment to school. cess, and attention problems than their popular and av-
In summary, it appears reasonable to conclude that erage counterparts. Moreover, rejected children
childrens peer relationships play a central role in pro- reported higher levels of conduct disturbance and sub-
moting or maintaining academic adaptation. This role stance abuse and committed more delinquent offenses
occurs at several levels of peer group analysis: Peer re- than the popular and average children. Controversial
jection may serve the purpose of making school an un- children were similar to rejected children on most mea-
welcome venue for children and adolescents, and the sures. For example, children in the two groups commit-
lack of friends may fail to provide the necessary support ted similar numbers of delinquent offenses.
for children and adolescents to fare well in school. Al- Similar findings concerning the predictive outcomes
ternatively, a childs peer group may actually serve to of rejected status have been reported by Bierman and
develop and reinforce poor school-related goals and be- Wargo (1995) and Coie, Terry, Lenox, Lochman, and
haviors. Thus, the role of the peer culture appears too Hyman (1995). In both of these longitudinal studies, peer
significant to be dismissed in practical efforts designed rejection in combination with the early display of aggres-
to encourage promising school aspirations and perfor- sive behavior, predicted externalizing problems. More re-
mance; indeed, this is an area that requires further em- cent research has shown that early peer rejection provides
pirical and practical substantiation in the future. a unique increment in the prediction later antisocial out-
comes, even when controlling for previous levels of ag-
gression and externalizing problems (Ladd & Burgess,
Psychological Adjustment
2001; Miller-Johnson, Coie, Maumary-Gremaud, Bier-
Ample evidence exists that difficulties with peers place man, & Conduct Problems Prevention Research, 2002;
a child at risk for developing subsequent problems of a Miller-Johnson, Coie, Maumary-Gremaud, Lochman, &
psychological nature. Consistent with the general trends Terry, 1999; Wentzel, 2003). For example, Laird, Jordan,
of the peer literature, research on the long-term conse- Dodge, Pettit, and Bates (2001) followed 400 children
quences of peer experiences has focused largely on re- from early childhood through to adolescence. They re-
jection and friendship. ported that sociometric rejection at ages 6 to 9 years pre-
dicted externalizing problems in adolescence, even when
Externalizing Problems controlling for the stability of externalizing problems
Results of longitudinal studies have indicated that peer over this age period.
rejection in childhood predicts a wide range of exter- Given the less than perfect stability of rejected sta-
nalizing problems in adolescence, including delin- tus, it would seem reasonable to ask whether psycholog-
quency, conduct disorder, attention difficulties, and ical risk status is equivalent for children with chronic
substance abuse. These findings are not particularly versus episodic and transient rejection by peers. To ad-
surprising given the well-established link between ag- dress this question, DeRosier, Kupersmidt, and Patter-
gression and peer rejection, and especially given that son (1994) followed 640 7- to 9-year-old children for 4
aggressive-rejected children are more likely to remain years. They found that children who were more chroni-
rejected over time. cally rejected over the first 3 years of the study were at
Kupersmidt and Coie (1990) reported the findings of greatest risk for behavior problems in the 4th year, even
a longitudinal study in which they followed-forward a after controlling for initial level of adjustment. More re-
group of fifth grade children for 7 years. Children iden- cently, Miller-Johnson et al. (2002) showed that peer re-
tified as sociometrically rejected were twice as likely to jection in first grade added incrementally to the
be delinquent (35%) in adolescence than was the case prediction of early starting conduct problems in third
for the sample base rate (17%). In a second study, Ol- and fourth grades, over and above the effects of aggres-
lendick et al. (1992) followed sociometrically rejected, sion. Similarly, Dodge and colleagues (2003) reported
neglected, popular, controversial, and average status 9- that peer rejection predicted longitudinal growth in
year-old children for 5 years; at the follow-up, rejected aggression over time (controlling for original levels of
children were perceived by their peers as less likable aggression) from early to middle childhood, and from
and more aggressive than popular and average children. middle childhood to adolescence. These researchers
Rejected children were also perceived by their teachers also found a developmental pathway in which peer
628 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

rejection led to more negative information processing distress across adolescences whereas Brains showed
patterns (i.e., hostile cue interpretation), which led to increases in internalizing distress between childhood
increased aggression. Certainly part of the association and adolescence.
between rejection and externalizing involves the net- Researchers have also recently begun to explore the
work of peer involvement experiences by rejected chil- unique role of peer rejection in the prediction of inter-
dren. Brendgen, Vitaro, and Bukowski (1998) showed nalizing problems. For example, in a longitudinal study
that rejected children were more likely than other boys following 405 children from kindergarten to grade 7,
and girls to associate with delinquent peers and that Kraatz-Keily, Bates, Dodge, and Pettit (2000) reported
these associations accounted for their subsequent delin- that peer rejection predicted increases in both internal-
quency. Consistent with expectations related to the pro- izing and externalizing problems over time. Moreover,
cess of deviancy training (Dishion et al., 1996), at-risk Burks, Dodge, and Price (1995) found that chronic re-
children, especially boys, who have aggressive friends jection in middle childhood predicted the subsequent
appear to influence each other with reinforcements and development of internalizing difficulties (depression,
enticements (Bagwell & Coie, 2004) so as to increase loneliness) 6 years hence. Their results held only for
each others aggression. These processes likely explain boys who had been rejected for 2 consecutive years;
why gang membership is a good predictor of develop- chronicity of rejection did not predict internalizing
mental trajectories of aggression (Lacourse, Nagin, problems for girls. The authors speculated that girls re-
Tremblay, Vitaro, & Claes, 2003). These mechanisms jection by the larger peer group is less severe than the
appear to account for the development of substance lack of close, intimate relationships with a friend.
abuse problems also (Dishion, Capaldi, & Yoerger, Relatedly, Gazelle and Ladd (2003) found that shy-
1999; Dishion & Owen, 2002). anxious kindergarteners who were also excluded by
peers displayed a greater stability in anxious solitude
Internalizing Problems
through the fourth grade and had elevated levels of de-
Results from a growing number of studies have indi- pressive symptoms as compared to shy-anxious peers
cated that anxious-withdrawal is contemporaneously who did not experience peer exclusion. Indeed, Gazelle
and predictively associated with internalizing problems and Rudolph (2004) recently found that over the course
during the life span, including low self-esteem, anxiety of fifth and sixth grade, high exclusion by peers led anx-
problems, loneliness, and depressive symptoms (e.g., ious solitary youth to maintenance or exacerbate the ex-
Coplan, Prakash, ONeil, & Armer, 2004; Gest, 1997). tent of social avoidance and depression; increased social
Rubin and colleagues followed a group of children from approach and less depression resulted from the experi-
kindergarten (age 5 years) to the ninth grade (age 14 ence of low exclusion.
years). They reported that withdrawal in kindergarten In understanding the link between peer rejection and
and second grade predicted the following outcomes in psychosocial adjustment, it may also be important to
fifth grade: peer rejection, self-reported feelings of de- consider the role of childrens perceptions of their own
pression, loneliness, and negative self-worth and teacher peer rejection. Childrens perceived rejection has been
ratings of anxiety (Hymel et al., 1990; Rubin & Mills, associated with increases in depression over time (e.g.,
1988). In turn, social withdrawal in the fifth grade pre- Kistner, Balthazor, Risi, & Burton, 1999). Moreover,
dicted self-reports of loneliness, depression, negative Sandstrom, Cillessen, and Eisenhower (2003) demon-
self-evaluations of social competence, feelings of not strated that childrens self-appraisal of peer rejection
belonging to a peer group that could be counted on for was associated with increased internalizing and exter-
social support, and parental assessments of internalizing nalizing problems even after controlling for actual peer
problems in the ninth grade (Rubin, Chen, McDougall, rejection. Thus, childrens beliefs that they are rejected
Bowker, & McKinnon, 1995). Using a follow-back de- may play an influential role in the development of psy-
sign with a group of adolescents who had been classified chosocial maladjustment.
according to clique membership, Prinstein and La Greca The majority of the research regarding friendship and
(2002) found adolescents self-reports of peer crowd af- subsequent internalizing problems has considered the
filiation to be concurrently associated with self-concept effects of friendship as either a moderator or as a medi-
and levels of internalizing distress. Their analyses of in- ator. Hodges, Boivin, et al. (1999) examined whether
ternalizing trajectories revealed that Populars/Jocks friendship would moderate the associations between
had experienced significant declines in internalizing victimization and depressed affect. Using a longitudinal
Conclusions 629

design they showed that young adolescents with friends studies precludes conclusions about causality. An inter-
and who were victimized subsequently showed lower lev- pretation of causality is warranted only when other po-
els of depressed affect than did young adolescents who tential pathways between the initial peer measures and
were friendless and victimized. Specifically, for young the subsequent adjustment variables have been ac-
adolescents without a friend, being victimized at the be- counted for (Kupersmidt, Coie, & Dodge, 1990). For ex-
ginning of the school year predicted increases in inter- ample, the initial level of adjustment must be controlled
nalizing and externalizing behaviors from the beginning for if an unequivocal conclusion about causal relations is
to the end of the school year, while there was no link be- to be reached. Second, the possibility of multicollinear-
tween being victimized and adjustment for those with ity must be considered. We have noted that there is nei-
friends. Relatedly, Rubin et al. (2004) found that when ther conceptual nor empirical independence between
fifth graders (10- to 11-year-olds) reported difficulties measures of peer experiences taken from different levels
in their relationships with their mothers and fathers, of social complexity. For example, measures of aggres-
having a strong supportive best friendship buffered sion and group acceptance are intercorrelated. One
them from negative self-perceptions and internalizing repercussion of such associations is that if researchers
problems. want to conclude that a given measure from the peer do-
The notion that friendship may buffer rejected chil- main predicts some outcome, it is necessary to control
dren from negative outcomes has been examined in a for the other measures with which the predictor may be
number of recent studies. However, the findings in these confounded. Third, although the growth of the literature
studies have been somewhat counterintuitive. For exam- on peer relationships was inspired by studies that fol-
ple, Hoza, Molina, Bukowski, and Sippola (1995) and lowed individuals over long periods, most current stud-
Kupersmidt, Burchinal, and Patterson (1995) reported ies are of a short duration.
that having a best friend actually augmented negative Considering how a set of measures will function to-
outcomes for children who were earlier identified as re- gether to affect outcome will also satisfy substantive ob-
jected and aggressive. One explanation for these find- jectives as well as methodological concerns. Inherent in
ings emanates from findings noted earlier that the theoretical positions regarding the peer system is the no-
friendship networks of aggressive-rejected children tion that experience in one domain of the peer system
comprise other aggressive children; the existence of a may compensate for, or enhance, experience in another
friendship network supportive of maladjusted behavior domain. For example, if it is true that friends influence
may actually exacerbate the prospects of a negative de- one another, the experience of having a friend will vary
velopmental outcome for rejected children (Cairns et al., according to what the friend is like. Or, as we have
1989; Tremblay, Msse, Vitaro, & Dobkin, 1995). Fi- shown, the experience of being rejected by peers ap-
nally, Nangle, Erdley, Newman, Mason, and Carpenter pears to be different for children who are aggressive and
(2003) examined whether the association between being those who are nonaggressive. The implication of these
well-liked by peers (i.e., being accepted) and feelings of concerns regarding the associations among measures
loneliness would be explained by the mediating effects from different domains of the peer system is that using a
of friendship. In the model supported by their data, ac- single factor model to understand the link between peer
ceptance was an antecedent to friendship, which, in experiences and outcome is likely to result in both an
turn, negatively predicted loneliness and depression. empirical and conceptual dead end.

Summary
Studies of the predictive relations between childrens CONCLUSIONS
peer relationships and their subsequent academic and
psychological adjustment generally support the notion In this chapter, we have reviewed literature concerning
that experiences with peers represent a risk factor for (a) developmental norms in childrens peer interactions,
maladjustment. The extant data reveal that the types of relationships, and groups; ( b) the developmental signifi-
friends a child may have, or the groups in which she or cance of peer interaction, discussion, and shared differ-
he participates, may influence individual adaptation. ences of opinion; (c) the importance of friendship; (d)
Despite these conclusions, however, it is important to the significance of social skills and social competence;
note that most of the longitudinal studies are typically (e) the assessment of childrens peer experiences; (f ) the
limited in a few critical ways. First, the design of most proximal and distal predictors of peer acceptance; and
630 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

(g) the outcomes of qualitative differences in peer rela- they interface with other systems. Opportunities for
tionships histories. The study of peer relationships has peer interaction and relationships vary from one culture
never been as active or as diverse as it is now. As we to another and different cultures ascribe different de-
have tried to show, remarkable progress has been made grees of significance to them. The content of peer in-
in describing and explaining the features, processes, and teractions and relationships is likely to vary, for
effects of childrens experiences with their age-mates. example, as a function of how much power is ascribed to
A consequence of this progress is that peer research kinship structures and by who makes primary decisions
must now answer new questions and deal with new chal- about allowable extrafamilial relationships. Because the
lenges. An additional repercussion of our progress is defining features or characteristics of what it means to
that the gaps in our understanding of the peer system be- be adapted to ones social context will differ across con-
come clear. We address these concerns in this conclud- texts, the impact on adaptation of particular characteris-
ing section. Specifically, we identify three current tics of peer relationships is likely to vary also. Finally,
challenges and opportunities for peer research, and we in a culture, the effect of the peer system is likely to
identify three topics that deserve more attention than vary according to differences between children in provi-
they have received in the past. sions they obtain in their families. Indeed, a central
tenet of the seminal views of Sullivan (1953) was that
Three Critical Challenges the developmental significance of friendship will be
higher for children whose relationships with parents was
First, we propose that the efforts to study peer relation- less than optimal than for other children.
ships as a system need to be continued and intensified. A third challenge concerns the development of inter-
The study of peer relationships has been frequently ventions to help children who have troubled experiences
predicated on the concept that peer relationships, how- with peers and to more generally promote and facilitate
ever construed, must be viewed as either an antecedent more positive peer experiences among children. There
or consequence. Consistent with the view that develop- now exists an extensive literature addressing how to im-
ment is a dynamic, multidirectional process (Sameroff prove childrens social skills (Bierman, 2003). Neverthe-
& MacKenzie, 2003), the study of peer relationships less, further development of techniques is needed to help
needs to be understood as a complex system. Children children develop healthy friendships, to decrease the fre-
bring various behaviors, needs, and cognitions into their quency and the effects of victimization, to regulate emo-
peer experiences at the dyadic and group level. In turn, tions and inhibit maladaptive behavior, and to enhance
these individual characteristics affect the features of the power of the peer system as a positive factor in devel-
these experiences and the provisions that children derive opment. It is important to remember that intervention re-
from these experiences leading to changes, for better or search provides an important assessment of the causal
worse, in the childs subsequent short-term and long- pathways implicated in the link between poor peer rela-
term functioning. Although this approach has already tionships and later adjustment. Specifically, through in-
been widely used, the adoption of a fully integrated tervention, researchers can learn whether improvements
model such as the one we have proposed has been rare. in adjustment with peers also reduce childrens relative
The study of transactional models of development has risk for subsequent adjustment disturbances.
been aided by the evolution of statistical procedures An important feature of the literature on intervention
(e.g., structural equation modeling, growth curve analy- is developmental sensitivity. The role that childrens
ses, hierarchical linear modeling, and survival analyses) peer relationships play in development appears to vary
that allow examination of bidirectional and reciprocal with age. For example, we have noted that childrens
influences in multivariate longitudinal data sets. Al- ideas about friendship become increasingly abstract with
though researchers of peer relationships have used these age. Furthermore, childrens friendships are posited to
analytic procedures for at least 20 years, the number of play an increasingly important role with age. Yet, little is
investigations incorporating these techniques remains known about the potential adaptive effects of friendship
lower than one might expect. or about when it is in childhood that friendship can serve
Second, the features and effects of experiences with as an accelerator, promoter, or inhibitor of adaptation or
peers need to be understood according to the larger sys- as a buffer against the ill-effects of parental or peer neg-
tems in which they are embedded and according to how lect or rejection. This issue of the functional signifi-
References 631

cance of friendship may prove helpful in the planning of Third, what are the provisions of peer relationships?
intervention programs for children who have poorly de- Friendship, acceptance, and popularity have been studied
veloped social skills and peer relationships. extensively. We know how to measure these constructs,
and we know a good deal about their antecedents and
their consequences. Yet, we know little about what it is
Three Questions in Search of Answers
that children and adolescents get from these relation-
In spite of its diversity and breadth, at least three funda- ships. To be sure there have been theoretical propositions
mental aspects of peer interactions, relationships, and about why friendship is important and how acceptance
groups are nearly absent from our review. First, what and rejection can influence child and adolescent develop-
accounts for interpersonal attraction? The question of ment. But there have been few studies of the specific op-
attraction may be implied in many of the topics we have portunities and experiences that are afforded by
discussed; nevertheless, its explicit presence as a topic friendship, acceptance, and popularity. And there have
of study in the contemporary literature is, at best, weak. been fewer studies of the significance of friendship
This gap is surprising, and regrettable, given the poten- and/or peer acceptance and rejection for children who
tial significance of interpersonal attraction as a phe- vary with regard to sex, ethnicity, and behavioral charac-
nomenon at the front end of the relationship process. If teristics. Certainly, the role of culture remains to be fully
we are going to claim that who one befriends or is at- explored. This question is not simply one of description.
tracted to makes a difference, knowing something about Research on friendship, for example, is based on claims
the factors underlying attraction is necessary. Thus far, about the putative provisions of this relationship. Similar
two sets of ideas have been proposed. Whereas one comments can be offered about acceptance and, to a
model has emphasized general patterns of attraction lesser extent, popularity. Further inquiry into what these
(i.e., children are generally drawn to helpful peers), an- experiences provide for children would help us better un-
other has emphasized the importance of similarity on a derstand the value of the theories we have relied on.
dyadic level. Similarity as an explanation of attraction Our review is now complete. We have examined that
has elicited some empirical scrutiny (e.g., Hamm, 2000; which we know and we have attempted to raise ques-
Rubin et al., 1994), but the model has not been pushed tions about that which we must come to know in the fu-
hard or analyzed carefully. This inattention is surprising ture. There is no doubt that many interesting and
given the ease with which similarity between peers can important questions remain unanswered. This should not
be studied, especially by exploiting the advantages of be surprising given that the modern history of peer re-
such statistical techniques such as multilevel modeling. search began only 35 years ago with Willard Hartups
Second, what aspects of peer interactions, relation- 1970 chapter in this Handbook. But growth begets
ships, and groups af fect boys and girls dif ferently? The growth, and it is encouraging to realize that there is no
study of sex differences is covered sporadically shortage of topics for us to study.
throughout this chapter and is seen also in Ruble, Mar-
tin, and Berenbaum (Chapter 14, this Handbook, this
volume). There are many exemplary studies of how peer REFERENCES
interactions and relationships differ for boys and for
girls. A central gap in the literature is the understanding Abecassis, M., Hartup, W. W., Haselager, G. J. T., Scholte, R. H. J., &
van Lieshout, C. F. M. (2002). Mutual antipathies and their sig-
of whether some aspects of peer interactions and rela- nificance in middle childhood and adolescence. Child Develop-
tionships affect boys and girls differently. This question ment, 73, 15431556.
is not about whether there are differences between the Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987).
features of peer interactions and relationships of boys Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implica-
tions of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity.
and girls. Instead, it is concerned with potential differ- Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213232.
ences in the functions and the developmental signifi- Allen, J. D., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G., & Bell, K. (1998). Attach-
cance of peer experiences for boys and girls. Knowing if ment and adolescent psychosocial functioning. Child Develop-
and how the peer system works differently for boys and ment, 69, 14061419.
girls would certainly add to our understanding of peer Altermatt, E. R., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2003). The development of
competence-related and motivational beliefs: An investigation of
relationships; it would augment our understanding of similarity and inf luence among friends. Journal of Educational
sexual differentiation as well. Psychology, 95, 111123.
632 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Asher, S. R., Parker, J. G., & Walker, D. L. (1996). Distinguishing Berndt, T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social development. Cur-
friendship from acceptance: Implications for intervention and rent Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 710.
assessment. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Berndt, T. J. (2004). Childrens friendships: Shifts over a half-cen-
Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship during child- tury in perspectives on their development and their effects. Mer-
hood and adolescence (pp. 366405). New York: Cambridge Uni- rill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 206223.
versity Press. Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Hoyle, S. G. (1986). Changes in
Atlas, R., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the friendship during a school year: Effects on childrens and adoles-
classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 92(2), 8699. cents impressions of friendship and sharing with friends. Child
Azmitia, M., Lippman, D. N., & Ittel, A. (1999). On the relation of Development, 57, 12841297.
personal experience to early adolescents reasoning about best Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Jiao, Z. (1999). Inf luences of friends
friendship deterioration. Social Development, 8, 275291. and friendships on adjustment to junior high school. Merrill-
Azmitia, M., & Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dia- Palmer Quarterly, 45, 1341.
logues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Devel- Berndt, T. J., & Heller, K. A. (1986). Gender stereotypes and social
opment, 2, 202221. inferences: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and So-
Bagwell, C. L., & Coie, J. D. (2004). The best friendships of aggres- cial Psychology, 50, 889898.
sive boys: Relationship quality, conf lict management, and rule- Berndt, T. J., & Hoyle, S. G. (1985). Stability and change in child-
breaking behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, hood and adolescent friendships. Developmental Psychology,
88(1), 524. 21, 10071015.
Bagwell, C. L., Coie, J. D., Terry, R. A., & Lochman, J. E. (2000). Berndt, T. J., & Perry, T. B. (1986). Childrens perceptions of
Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence. friendships as supportive relationships. Developmental Psychol-
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280305. ogy, 22, 640648.
Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Bierman, K. L. (2003). Peer rejection: Developmental processes and
Annals of child development: Vol. 6. Six theories of child intervention. New York: Guilford Publications.
development-Revised formulations and current issues (pp. Bierman, K. L., Smoot, D. L., & Aumiller, K. (1993). Characteristics
1 60). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. of aggressive-rejected, aggressive (nonrejected), and rejected
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality (nonaggressive) boys. Child Development, 64, 139151.
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Bierman, K. L., & Wargo, J. B. (1995). Predicting the longitudinal
Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? Gen- course associated with aggressive-rejected, aggressive (nonre-
der differences and two spheres of belongingnessComment on jected), and rejected (nonaggressive) status. Development and
Cross and Madson, 1997. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 3844. Psychopathology, 7, 669682.
Bigelow, B. J. (1977). Childrens friendship expectations: A cogni-
Beer, B. (2001). Anthropology of friendship. In N. J. Smelser & P. B.
tive developmental study. Child Development, 48, 246253.
Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behav-
ioral sciences (pp. 58055808). Kidlington, England: Elsevier. Black, B., & Hazan, N. (1990). Social status and patterns of commu-
nication in acquainted and unacquainted preschool children. De-
Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental inf lu-
velopmental-Psychology, 26, 379387.
ence: An evolutionary argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182186.
Blos, P. (1967). The second individuation process of adolescence: Vol.
Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1995). Attachment: Theory and evidence. In
22. Psychoanalytic study of the child. New York: International
M. Rutter, D. Hay, & S. Baron-Cohen (Eds.), Developmental prin-
Universities Press.
ciples and clinical issues in psychology and psychiatry. Oxford,
England: Blackwell. Boivin, M., Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1995). Individual-group be-
havioral similarity and peer status in experimental play groups of
Benenson, J. F., Apostoleris, N. H., & Parnass, J. (1997). Age and sex
boys: The social misfit revisited. Journal of Personality and Social
differences in dyadic and group interaction. Developmental Psy-
Psychology, 69, 269279.
chology, 33, 538543.
Boivin, M., Hymel, S., & Bukowski, W. M. (1995). The roles of social
Benenson, J. F., & Christakos, A. (2003). The greater fragility of fe- withdrawal, peer rejection, and victimization by peers in predict-
males versus males closest same-sex friendships. Child Develop- ing loneliness and depressed mood in childhood. Development and
ment, 74, 11231129. Psychopathology, 7, 765785.
Benenson, J. F., Markovits, H., Roy, R., & Denko, P. (2003). Behav- Bonney, M. E. (1944). Relationship between social success, family
ioural rules underlying learning to share: Effects of development size, socioeconomic background, and intelligence among school
and context. International Journal of Behavioural Development, children grades three to five. Sociometry, 7, 2639.
27, 116121. Booth, C., Rubin, K. H., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Burgess, K. (2004). At-
Benenson, J. F., Nicholson, C., Waite, A., Roy, R., & Simpson, A. tachment and friendship predictors of psychosocial functioning in
(2001). The inf luence of group size on childrens competitive be- middle childhood and the mediating roles of social support and
havior. Child Development, 72, 921928. self-worth. In K. Kerns & R. A. Richardson (Eds.), Attachment in
Benenson, J. F., Roy, R., Waite, A., Goldbaum, S., Linders, L., & Simp- middle childhood (pp. 161188). New York: Guilford Press.
son, A. (2002). Greater discomfort as a proximate cause of sex dif- Booth, C. L., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Rubin, K. H. (1991). Relating
ferences in competition. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 225247. preschoolers social competence and their mothers parenting be-
Berndt, T. J. (1996). Friendship quality affects adolescents self- haviors to early attachment security and high-risk status. Journal
esteem and social behavior. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 363382.
& W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship dur- Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Parenting infants. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
ing childhood and adolescence (pp. 346365). New York: Cam- Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed.,
bridge University Press. pp. 343). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 633

Bornstein, M. H., & Cheah, C. S. L. (in press). The place of culture Bugental, D. B. (1992). Affective and cognitive processes
and parenting in the ecological contextual perspective on devel- within threat-oriented family systems. In I. E. Sigel, A. V.
opmental science. In K. H. Rubin & O. Boon Chung (Eds.), McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief
Parental beliefs, parenting, and child development in cross-cul- systems: The psychological consequences for children
tural perspective. London: Psychology Press. (pp. 219248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Boulton, M. J. (1996). A comparison of 8- and 11-year-old girls and Buhler, C. (1931). The social behavior of the child. In C. Murchison
boys participation in specific types of rough-and-tumble play and (Ed.), A handbook of child psychology. New York: Russell & Russell.
aggressive fighting: Implications for functional hypotheses. Ag- Buhler, C. (1935). From birth to maturity: An outline of the psycho-
gressive Behavior, 22, 271287. logical development of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Boulton, M. J., Trueman, M., Chau, C., Whitehand, C., & Amatya, K. Burhmester, D. (1998). Need fulfillment, interpersonal competence,
(1999). Concurrent and longitudinal links between friendships and the developmental contexts of early adolescent friendship. In
and peer victimization: Implications for befriending interven- W. M. Bukowski, A. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The com-
tions. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 461466. pany they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence
Bowker, A., Bukowski, W., Zargarpour, S., & Hoza, B. (1998). A (pp. 158185). New York: Cambridge University Press.
structural and functional analysis of a two-dimensional model of Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of compan-
social isolation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 447463. ionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58, 11011103.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation, anxiety Bukowski, W. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (Eds.). (1996). Sociometry
and anger. New York: Basic Books. then and now: Recent developments in the study of childrens peer
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New groups. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1969) Bukowski, W. M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularity and friendship:
Brendgen, M., Bowen, F., Rondeau, N., & Vitaro, F. (1999). Effects Issues in theory, measurement, and outcome. In T. J. Berndt &
of friends characteristics on childrens social cognitions. Social G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relations in child development
Development, 8, 4151. (pp. 1545). New York: Wiley.
Brendgen, M., Little, T. D., & Krappmann, L. (2000). Rejected chil- Bukowski, W. M., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (1994). Using rating
dren and their friends: A shared evaluation of friendship quality? scale and nomination techniques to measure friendships and popu-
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46(1), 4570. larity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 485488.
Bukowski, W. M., Rubin, K. H., & Parker, J. (2001). Social compe-
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1998). Affiliation
tence. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.) & N. Eisenberg (Sec.
with delinquent friends: Contributions of parents, self-esteem,
Ed.), International encyclopedia of social and behavioral sciences
delinquent behavior, and rejection by peers. Journal of Early Ado-
(pp. 1425814264). Oxford, England: Elsevier Science.
lescence, 18(3), 244265.
Bukowski, W. M., & Sippola, L. K. (2001). Groups, individuals, and
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (2000). Deviant friends
victimization: A view of the peer system. In J. Juvonen & S. Gra-
and early adolescents emotional and behavioral adjustment. Jour-
ham (Eds.), Peer harassment in school: The plight of the vulner-
nal of Research on Adolescence, 10(2), 173189.
able and victimized (pp. 355377). New York: Guilford Press.
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Doyle, A. B., Markiewicz, D., & Bukowski,
Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L. K., & Hoza, B. (1999). Same and other:
W. M. (2002). Same-sex peer relations and romantic relation-
Interdependency between participation in same- and other-sex
ships during early adolescence: Interactive links to emotional,
friendships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 439459.
behavioral, and academic adjustment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
48, 77103. Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (2000).
Pages from a sociometric notebook: An analysis of nomination
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1944). A constant frame of reference for so-
and rating scale measures of acceptance, rejection, and social
ciometric research: Pt. 11. Experiment and inference. Sociome-
preference. In A. Cillessen & W. Bukowski (Eds.), Recent ad-
try, 7, 4075.
vances in the measurement of acceptance and rejection in the peer
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The evolution of envi- system (pp. 1126). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
ronmental models in developmental research. In P. H. Mussen (Se- Burgess, K. B., Marshall, P., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2003). In-
ries Ed.) & W. Kessen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: fant attachment and temperament as predictors of subsequent be-
Vol. 1. History, theory, and methods (4th ed., pp. 357414). New havior problems and psychophysiological functioning. Journal of
York: Wiley. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 113.
Brown, B. B. (1989). The role of peer groups in adolescents adjust- Burgess, K. B., Wojslawowicz, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Rose-Krasnor, L.,
ment to secondary school. In T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), & Booth, C. L. (2003, April). The extended class play: A longi-
Peer relationships in child development (pp. 188216). New tudinal study of its factor structure, reliability, and validity. Poster
York: Wiley. resented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in
Brown, B. B., Clasen, D. R., & Neiss, J. D. (1987, April). Smoke Child Development, Tampa, FL.
through the looking glass: Adolescents perceptions of peer group Burks, V. S., Dodge, K. A., & Price, J. M. (1995). Models of internal-
status. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for izing outcomes of early rejection. Development and Psychopathol-
Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD. ogy, 7, 683696.
Brown, B. B., & Klute, C. (2003). Friends, cliques, and crowds. In Buskirk, A. A., Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K., Booth-LaForce, C. L., &
G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of Rose-Krasnor, L. (2004, March). Loved, hated . . . but never
adolescence (pp. 330348). Malden, MA: Blackwell. ignored: Evidence for two types of popularity. Paper presented
Bugental, D., & Happaney, K. (2002). Parental attributions. In M. as part of Symposium on the Many Faces of Popularity at the
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becom- biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Adolescence,
ing a parent (2nd ed., pp. 509535). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Baltimore, MD.
634 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Cairns, R. B. (1979). Social development: The origins and plasticity Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, D., & Li, Z. (1999). Adolescent out-
of interchanges. San Francisco: Freeman. comes of social functioning in Chinese children. International
Cairns, R. B., Gariepy, J. L., & Kindermann, T. (1989). Identifying Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 199223.
social clusters in natural settings. Unpublished manuscript, Uni- Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., Liu, M., Chen, H., Wang, L., & Li, D. (2003).
versity of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Social Development Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers. International
Laboratory. Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 428436.
Cairns, R. B., Xie, H., & Leung, M. C. (1996). The popularity of Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Sun, Y. (1992). Social reputation and peer
friendship and the neglect of social networks: Toward a new bal- relationships in Chinese and Canadian children: A cross-cultural
ance. In W. M. Bukowski & A. H. N. Cillessen (Eds.), Sociometry study. Child Development, 63, 13361343.
then and now: Recent developments in the study of childrens peer Chen, X., Zappulla, C., Lo Coco, A., Schneider, B., Kaspar, V., de
groups (pp. 2553). San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Oliveira, A. M., et al. (2004). Self-perceptions of competence in
Calkins, S. D., & Dedmon, S. A. (2000). Physiological and behav- Brazilian, Canadian, Chinese, and Italian children: Relations with
ioral regulation in 2-year-old children with aggressive/destruc- social and school adjustment. International Journal of Behavioural
tive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Development, 28, 129138.
28, 103118. Cillessen, A., & Bukowski, W. (1998). Sociometry then and now:
Calkins, S. D., Gill, K. L., Johnson, M. C., & Smith, C. L. (1999). Building on 6 decades of measuring childrens experiences with the
Emotional reactivity and emotional regulation strategies as pre- peer group. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
dictors of social behavior with peers during toddlerhood. Social Cillessen, A. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Haselager, G. T. (2000). Stabil-
Development, 8(3), 310334. ity of sociometric categories. In A. Cillessen & W. Bukowski
Camodeca, M., Goossens, F. A., Terwogt, M. M., & Schuengel, C. (Eds.), Recent advances in the measurement of acceptance and re-
(2002). Bullying and victimization among school-age children: jection in the peer system (pp. 7593). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Stability and links to proactive and reactive aggression. Social
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforce-
Development, 11, 332345.
ment: Developmental changes in the association between aggres-
Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in sion and social status. Child Development, 75, 147163.
parent-child interactions and childrens peer competency. Child
Cillessen, A. H., van IJzendoorn, H. W., van Lieshout, C. F., &
Development, 67, 22172226.
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Heterogeneity among peer-rejected boys:
Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2003). National estimates of Subtypes and stabilities. Child Development, 63, 893905.
adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), Adoles-
Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy
cent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research,
support in parent-child relationships: Links to childrens socioe-
and practical implications (pp. 2356). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
motional orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psy-
Casiglia, A. C., Lo Coco, A., & Zappulla, C. (1998). Aspects of so- chology, 36, 485498.
cial reputation and peer relationships in Italian children: A cross-
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1988). Multiple sources of data on social
cultural perspective. Developmental Psychology, 34, 723730.
behavior and social status. Child Development, 59, 815829.
Challman, R. C. (1932). Factors inf luencing friendships among pre-
school children. Child Development, 3, 146158. Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and
types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
Chang, L., Lei, L., Li, K., Liu, H., Guo, B., Wang, Y., et al. (2005).
Psychology, 18, 557570.
Peer acceptance and self perceptions of verbal and behavioral ag-
gression and social withdrawal. International Journal of Behav- Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerging
ioral Development, 29, 4857. social status in boys groups. Child Development, 54, 14001416.
Cheah, C. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (2004). A cross-cultural examination Coie, J. D., Lochman, J. E., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. (1992). Predict-
of maternal beliefs regarding maladaptive behaviors in preschool- ing early adolescent disorder from childhood aggression and
ers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 8394. peer rejection. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
60, 783792.
Chen, D. W., Fein, G., & Tam, H. P. (2001). Peer conflicts of pre-
school children: Issues, resolution, incidence, and age-related pat- Coie, J. D., Terry, R., Lenox, K., Lochman, J., & Hyman, C. (1995).
terns. Early Education and Development, 12, 523544. Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable
Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and ad- patterns of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathol-
justment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. ogy, 7, 697714.
Child Development, 76, 182195. Collins, W. A. (2003). More than a myth: The developmental signifi-
Chen, X., Chang, L., & He, Y. (2003). The peer group as a context: cance of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of
Mediating and moderating effects on the relations between aca- Research on Adolescence, 13, 124.
demic achievement and social functioning in Chinese children. Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., &
Child Development, 74, 710727. Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218232.
S. L. (1998). Child-rearing practices and behavioral inhibition in Colwell, M. J., Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Contextual determi-
Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Develop- nants of mothers interventions in young childrens peer interac-
mental Psychology, 34, 677686. tions. Developmental Psychology, 38, 492502.
Chen, X., & Rubin, K. H. (1994). Family conditions, parental accep- Connolly, J., Craig, W., Goldberg, A., & Pepler, D. (2004). Mixed-
tance, and social competence and aggression in Chinese children. gender groups, dating, and romantic relationships in early adoles-
Social Development, 3, 269290. cence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14, 185207.
Chen, X., Rubin, K. H., & Li, B. (1995). Social and school adjustment Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of peers in
of shy and aggressive children in China. Development and Psycho- the emergence of heterosexual romantic relationships in adoles-
pathology, 7, 337349. cence. Child Development, 71(5), 13951408.
References 635

Coplan, R. J., Gavinski-Molina, M. H., Lagace-Seguin, D. G., & DeRosier, M., Kupersmidt, J., & Patterson, C. (1994). Childrens aca-
Wichmann, C. (2001). When girls versus boys play alone: Non-so- demic and behavioral adjustment as a function of the chronicity and
cial play and adjustment in kindergarten. Developmental Psychol- proximity of peer rejection. Child Development, 65, 17991813.
ogy, 37, 464474. Dill, E. J., Vernberg, E. M., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Negative affect in
Coplan, R. J., Hastings, P. D., Lagace-Seguin, D. G., & Moulton, C. E. victimized children: The roles of social withdrawal, peer rejec-
(2002). Authoritative and authoritarian mothers parenting goals, tion, and attitudes toward bullying. Journal of Abnormal Child
attributions, and emotions across different childrearing contexts. Psychology, 32, 159173.
Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 126. Dishion, T. J. (1990). The family ecology of boys peer relations in
Coplan, R. J., Prakash, K., ONeil, K., & Armer, M. (2004). Do you middle childhood. Child Development, 61, 874892.
want to play? Distinguishing between conf licted shyness and Dishion, T. J., Capaldi, D. M., & Yoerger, K. (1999). Middle child-
social disinterest in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, hood antecedents to progressions in male adolescent substance
40(2), 244258. use: An ecological analysis of risk and protection. Journal of Ado-
Coplan, R. J., Rubin, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calkins, S., & Stewart, S. L. lescent Research, 14(2), 175205.
(1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: Distinguish- Dishion, T. J., Duncan, T. E., Eddy, J. M., Fagot, B. I., & Fetrow, R.
ing between reticence and passive- and active-solitude in young (1994). The world of parents and peers: Coercive exchanges and
children. Child Development, 65, 129138. childrens social adaptation. Social Development, 3, 255268.
Crick, N. R., & Bigbee, M. A. (1998). Relational and overt forms of Dishion, T. J., Eddy, M., Haas, E., Li, F., & Spracklen, K. (1997).
peer victimization: A multi-informant approach. Journal of Con- Friendships and violent behavior during adolescence. Social De-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(2), 337347. velopment, 6(2), 207223.
Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt ag- Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions
gression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 579588. harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist,
Crick, N., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of so- 54(9), 755764.
cial information processing in childrens social adjustment. Psy- Dishion, T. J., & Owen, L. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of friend-
chological Bulletin, 115, 74101. ships and substance use: Bidirectional inf luence from adoles-
cence to adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 480491.
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gen-
der, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D., & Patterson, G.
66, 710722. (1996). Deviancy training in male adolescents friendships. Behav-
ior Therapy, 27(3), 373390.
Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Childrens perceptions of their
peer experiences: Attributions, loneliness, social anxiety, and so- Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status.
cial avoidance. Development Psychology, 29, 244254. Child Development, 54, 13861399.
Crick, N. R., & Nelson, D. A. (2002). Relational and physical victim- Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of so-
ization within friendship: Nobody told me thered be friends like cial competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota
these. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 599607. Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77125). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Criss, M. M., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Lapp, A. L.
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S.,
(2002). Family adversity, positive peer relationships, and chil-
Fontaine, R., et al. (2003). Peer rejection and social information-
drens externalizing behavior: A longitudinal perspective on risk
processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior
and resilience. Child Development, 73, 12201237.
problems in children. Child Development, 74(2), 374393.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent. New
Dodge, K. A., McClaskey, C. L., & Feldman, E. (1985). A situational
York: Basic Books.
approach to the assessment of social competence in children.
Damon, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interaction and the process of Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 344353.
change in childrens moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
Dunn, J. (1993). Young childrens close relationships: Beyond attach-
28, 347378.
ment. London: Sage.
Davila, J., Steinberg, S. J., Kachadourian, L., Cobb, R., & Fincham, Dunn, J. (1999). Making sense of the social world: Mindreading,
F. (2004). Romantic involvement and depressive symptoms in emotion, and relationships. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, &
early and late adolescence: The role of a preoccupied relational D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social under-
style. Personal Relationships, 11, 161178. standing and self-control (pp. 229242). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dawe, H. C. (1934). Analysis of two hundred quarrels of preschool Dunn, J. (2004). Childrens friendships: The beginnings of intimacy.
children. Child Development, 5, 139157. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Deater-Deckard, K. (2001). Annotation: Recent research examining Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. (1999). Understanding others and individual
the role of peer relations in the development of psychopathology. differences in friendship interactions in young children. Social
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 565579. Development, 8, 201219.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior Dunn, J., Cutting, A., & Fisher, N. (2002). Old friends, new friends:
problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation Predictors of childrens perspective on their friends at school.
by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161175. Child Development, 73, 621635.
Degirmencioglu, S. M., Urberg, K. A., Tolson, J. M., & Richard, P. Duran, R. T., & Gauvain, M. (1993). The role of age versus expertise
(1998). Adolescent friendship networks: Continuity and change in peer collaboration during joint planning. Journal of Experimen-
over the school year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 313337. tal Child Psychology, 55, 227242.
Denton, K., & Zarbatany, L. (1996). Age differences in support Eckerman, C. O. (1979). The human infant in social interaction. In R.
processes in conversations between friends. Child Development, Cairns (Ed.), The analysis of social interactions: Methods, issues,
67, 13601373. and illustrations (pp. 163178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
636 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Eckerman, C. O., & Stein, M. R. (1990). How imitation begets imita- Farmer, T., Estell, D., Bishop, J., ONeal, K., & Cairns, B. (2003).
tion and toddlers generation of games. Developmental Psychol- Rejected bullies or popular leaders? The social relations of ag-
ogy, 26, 370378. gressive subtypes of rural African American early adolescents.
Eckert, P. (1989). Jocks and burnouts: Social categories and identity Developmental Psychology, 39(6), 9921004.
in the high school. New York: Teachers College Press. Finnie, V., & Russell, A. (1988). Preschool childrens social status
Edwards, C. P. (1992). Cross-cultural perspective on family-peer re- and their mothers behavior and knowledge in the supervisory
lations. In R. D. Parke & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family-peer relation- role. Developmental Psychology, 24, 789801.
ships: Modes of linkage (pp. 285316). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression and so-
Eisenberg, N. (2002). Emotion-related regulation and its relation to cial withdrawal: Interactions among temperament, attachment,
quality of social functioning. In W. W. Hartup & R. A. Weinberg and regulation. In K. H. Rubin & J. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social with-
(Eds.), Child psychology in retrospect and prospect: In celebration drawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 81100). Hills-
of the 75th anniversary of the Institute of Child Development dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 133171). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Freitag, M. K., Belsky, J., Grossmann, K., Grossmann, J. E., &
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Shepard, S. A., Scheurer-Englisch, H. (1996). Continuity in child-parent relation-
Reiser, M., Murphy, B. C., et al. (2001). The relations of regula- ships from infancy to middle childhood and relations with friend-
tion and emotionality to childrens externalizing and internaliz- ship competence. Child Development, 67, 14371454.
ing problem behavior. Child Development, 72, 11121134. French, D. C. (2004). The cultural context of friendship. ISSBD
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dis- Newsletter, 28, 1920.
positional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting Furman, W. (1996). The measurement of friendship perceptions:
quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Conceptual and methodological issues. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F.
Psychology, 78, 136157. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep:
Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E. T., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumber- Friendships in childhood and adolescence (pp. 4165). Cam-
land, A. J., Losoya, S. H., et al. (2001). Mothers emotional ex- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
pressivity and childrens behavior problems and social Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in
competence: Mediation through childrens regulation. Develop- perceptions of networks and personal relationships. Child Devel-
mental Psychology, 37, 475490. opment, 63, 103115.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumber- Furman, W., Simon, V. A., Shaffer, L., & Bouchey, H. A. (2002). Ado-
land, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and the development of lescents working models and styles for relationships with parents,
prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, friends, and romantic partners. Child Development, 73, 241255.
70, 13601372.
Garton, A. F. (2001). Peer assistance in childrens problem solving.
Eisenberg, N., Pidada, S., & Liew, J. (2001). The relations of regula- British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 307319.
tion and negative emotionality to Indonesian childrens social
functioning. Child Development, 72, 17471763. Gavin, L. A., & Furman, W. (1989). Age differences in adoles-
cents perceptions of their peer groups. Developmental Psychol-
Eisenberg, N., Shephard, S. A., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B. C., &
ogy, 25, 827834.
Guthrie, I. K. (1998). Shyness and childrens emotionality, regu-
lation, and coping: Contemporaneous, longitudinal, and across- Gazelle, H., & Ladd, G. W. (2003). Anxious solitude and peer exclu-
context relations. Child Development, 69, 767790. sion: A diathesis-stress model of internalizing trajectories in
childhood. Child Development, 74, 257278.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland,
A., Shephard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful con- Gazelle, H., & Rudolph, K. D. (2004). Moving toward and moving
trol and impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child away from the world: Social approach and avoidance trajectories
Development, 75, 2546. in anxious youth. Child Development, 75(3), 829849.
Elicker, J., Englund, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Predicting peer com- Gest, S. D. (1997). Behavioral inhibition: Stability and association
petence and peer relationships in childhood from early parent-child with adaptation from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of
relationships. In R. Parke & G. Ladd (Eds.), Family-peer relation- Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 467475.
ships: Modes of linkage (pp. 77106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gest, S. D., Farmer, T., Carins, B., & Xie, H. (2003). Identifying chil-
Elman, N. M., & Kennedy-Moore, E. (2003). The unwritten rules of drens peer social networks in school classrooms: Links between
friendship: Simple strategies to help your child make friends. New peer reports and observed interactions. Social Development,
York: Little, Brown. 12(4), 513529.
Englund, M. M., Levy, A. K., Hyson, D. M., & Sroufe, A. (2000). Golbeck, S. L. (1998). Peer collaboration and childrens representa-
Adolescent social competence: Effectiveness in a group setting. tion of the horizontal surface of liquid. Journal of Applied Devel-
Child Development, 71(4), 10491060. opmental Psychology, 19, 542572.
Espelage, D. L., Bosworth, K., & Simon, T. R. (2000). Examining the Goncu, A. (1993). Development of intersubjectivity in the dyadic play
social context of bullying behaviors in early adolescence. Journal of preschoolers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 99116.
of Counseling and Development, 78, 326333. Goncu, A., Patt, M. B., & Kouba, E. (2002). Understanding young
Espelage, D., Holt, M., & Henkel, R. (2003). Examination of peer- childrens pretend play in context. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart
group contextual effects on aggression during early adolescence. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood social development
Child Development, 74, 205220. (pp. 418437). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Martin, C. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2002). Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (1998). Self-blame and peer victimization
Young childrens negative emotionality and social isolation: A la- in middle school: An attributional analysis. Developmental Psy-
tent growth curve analysis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 284307. chology, 34, 587599.
References 637

Graham, S., & Juvonen, J. (2001). An attributional approach to peer Hartup, W. W., Laursen, B., Stewart, M. A., & Eastenson, A. (1988).
victimization. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer harassment Conf licts and the friendship relations of young children. Child
in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 4972). Development, 59, 15901600.
New York: Guilford Press. Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendships and adaptation in
Granic, I., & Lamey, A. V. (2002). Combining dynamic systems and the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355370.
multivariate analyses to compare the mother-child interactions of Haselager, G. J. T., Cillissen, H. N., van Lieshout, C. F. M., Riksen-
externalizing subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Walraven, J. M. A., & Hartup, W. W. (2002). Heterogeneity
30, 265283. among peer-rejected boys across middle childhood: Developmen-
Grotpeter, J. K., & Crick, N. R. (1996). Relational aggression, overt tal pathways of social behavior. Child Development, 73, 446456.
aggression, and friendship. Child Development, 67, 23282338. Haselager, G. J. T., Hartup, W. W., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Riksen-
Gunnar, M. R., Sebanc, A. M., Tout, K., Donzella, B., & Van Dulmen, Walraven, J. M. A. (1998). Similarities between friends and non-
M. M. H. (2003). Peer rejection, temperament, and cortisol activ- friends in middle childhood. Child Development, 69, 11981208.
ity in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 43, 346358. Hastings, P., & Rubin, K. H. (1999). Predicting mothers beliefs
Guralnick, M. J., Hammond, M. A., & Connor, R. T. (2003). Subtypes about preschool-aged childrens social behavior: Evidence for ma-
of nonsocial play: Comparisons between young children with and ternal attitudes moderating child effects. Child Development,
without developmental delays. American Journal of Mental Retar- 70(3), 722741.
dation, 108, 347362. Hawley, P. H. (2002). Social dominance and prosocial and coercive
Hamm, J. V. (2000). Do birds of a feather f lock together? The vari- strategies of resource control in preschoolers. International Jour-
able bases for African American, Asian American, and European nal of Behavioral Development, 26, 167176.
American adolescents selection of similar friends. Developmen- Hawley, P. H. (2003). Strategies of control, aggression and morality
tal Psychology, 36(2), 209219. in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Experi-
Hanfmann, E. P. (1935). Social structure of a group of kindergarten mental Child Psychology, 85(3), 213235.
children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 5, 407410. Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., & Pasupathi, M. (2002). Winning
Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2004). Aggressive victims, passive friends and inf luencing peers: Strategies of peer inf luence in
victims, and bullies: Developmental continuity or developmental late childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
change. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 1738. 26, 466474.
Hansell, S. (1981). Ego development and peer friendship networks. Hay, D. F. (1985). Learning to form relationships in infancy: Paral-
Sociology of Education, 54, 5163. lel attainments with parents and peers. Developmental Review,
Hardy, C. L., Bukowski, W. M., & Sippola, L. K. (2002). Stability and 5, 122161.
change in peer relationships during the transition to middle-level Hay, D. F., Castle, J., & Davies, L. (2000). Toddlers use of force
school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 22, 117142. against familiar peers: A precursor of serious aggression? Child
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2002). Culture and parenting. In M. H. Development, 71, 457467.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecol- Hay, D. F., Castle, J., Davies, L., Demetriou, H., & Stimson, C.
ogy of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 253280). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (1999). Prosocial action in very early childhood. Journal of Child
Harris, J. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 905916.
way they do. New York: Touchstone. Hay, D. F., Nash, A., & Pedersen, J. (1983). Interaction between 6-
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the childs environment? A group social- month-old peers. Child Development, 54, 557562.
ization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458489. Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in child-
Hart, C. H., DeWolf, D., Wozniak, P., & Burts, D. C. (1992). Mater- hood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 84108.
nal and paternal disciplinary styles: Relations with preschoolers Hay, D. F., & Ross, H. (1982). The social nature of early conf lict.
playground behavioral orientations and peer status. Child Devel- Child Development, 53, 105113.
opment, 63, 879892. Hayes, D. S., Gershman, E., & Bolin, L. J. (1980). Friends and ene-
Hart, C. H., Yang, C., Nelson, L. J., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, J. A., mies: Cognitive bases for preschool childrens unilateral and re-
Nelson, D., et al. (2000). Peer acceptance in early childhood and ciprocal friendships. Child Development, 51, 12761279.
subtypes of socially withdrawn behavior in China, Russia, and Hays, R. B. (1988). Friendship. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of
the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Develop- personal relationships: Theory, research, and interventions
ment, 24, 7381. (pp. 391408). London: Wiley.
Hartup, W. W. (1970). Peer interaction and social organization. In Hektner, J. M., August, G. J., & Realmuto, G. M. (2000). Patterns and
P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichaels manual of child psychology temporal changes in peeraffiliation among aggressive and nonag-
(Vol. 2, pp. 361456). New York: Wiley. gressive children participating in a summer school program. Jour-
Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Hand- nal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(4), 603614.
book of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and so- Henderson, H., Marshall, P., Fox, N. A., & Rubin, K. H. (2004). Psy-
cial development (4th ed., pp. 103196). New York: Wiley. chophysiological and behavioral evidence for varying forms of
Hartup, W. W. (1985). Relationships and their significance in cogni- nonsocial behavior in preschoolers. Child Development, 75, 251263.
tive development. In R. A. Hinde, A. Perret-Clermont, & Hinde, R. A. (1979). Towards understanding relationships. London:
J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Social relationships and cognitive de- Academic Press.
velopment (pp. 6682). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships and culture. Cam-
Hartup, W. W., & Abecassis, M. (2002). Friends and enemies. In bridge: Cambridge University Press.
P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood Hinde, R. A. (1995). A suggested structure for a science of relation-
social development (pp. 286306). Malden, MA: Blackwell. ships. Personal Relationships, 2, 115.
638 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Hinde, R. R., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1976). Toward understanding Hymel, S., & Rubin, K. H. (1985). Children with peer relationship
relationships: Dynamic stability. In P. Bateson & R. Hinde (Eds.), and social skills problems: Conceptual, methodological, and de-
Growing points in ethology (pp. 451479). Cambridge: Cam- velopmental issues. In G. J. Whitehurst (Ed.), Annals of Child De-
bridge University Press. velopment (Vol. 2, pp. 251297). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Hinde, R. R., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1987). Interpersonal relation- Hymel, S., Rubin, K. H., Rowden, L., & LeMare, L. (1990). Chil-
ships and child development. Developmental Review, 7, 121. drens peer relationships: Longitudinal predictions of internaliz-
Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999). ing and externalizing problems from middle to late childhood.
The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of Child Development, 61, 20042021.
peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94101. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
Hodges, E. V. E., & Card, N. A. (Eds.). (2003). Enemies and the Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2000). You dont bring me anything but
darker side of peer relations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. down: Adolescent romance and depression. Journal of Health and
Hodges, E. V. E., Finnegan, R. A., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Skewed au- Social Behavior, 41, 369391.
tonomy-relatedness in preadolescents conceptions of their rela- Juvonen, J., & Graham, S. (Eds.). (2001). Peer harassment in
tionships with mother, father, and best friend. Developmental school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized. New York:
Psychology, 35(3), 737748. Guilford Press.
Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). Individual Juvonen, J., & Wentzel, K. (1996). Social motivation: Understand-
risk and social risk as interacting determinants of victimization ing childrens school adjustment. New York: Cambridge Univer-
in the peer group. Developmental Psychology, 33, 10321039. sity Press.
Hogan, D., & Tudge, J. (1999). Implications of Vygotskys theory for Kagan, J. (1999). The concept of behavioral inhibition. In L. A.
peer learning. In A. M. ODonnell & A. King (Eds.), Cognitive Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.), Extreme fear, shyness, and social
perspectives on peer learning (pp. 3965). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. phobia: Origins, biological mechanisms, and clinical outcomes
Hogue, A., & Steinberg, L. (1995). Homophily of internalized distress (pp. 313). London: Oxford University Press.
in adolescent peer groups. Developmental Psychology, 31, 897906.
Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in ado-
Hops, H., Albert, A., & Davis, B. (1997). The development of same- lescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427436.
and opposite-sex social relations among adolescents: An analogue
Keenan, K., Shaw, D., & Delliquadri, E. (1998). Evidence for the con-
study. Social Development, 6(2), 165183.
tinuity of early problem behaviors: Application of a developmen-
Horn, S. (2003). Adolescents reasoning about exclusion from social tal model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 441452.
groups. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1184.
Keller, M. (2004a). A cross cultural perspective on friendship re-
Howes, C. (1983). Patterns of friendship. Child Development, search. Newsletter of the International Society for the Study of Be-
54, 10411053. havioral Development, 28, 1014.
Howes, C. (1988). Peer interaction of young children. Monographs of
Keller, M. (2004b). Self in relationship. In D. K. Lapsley & D. Nar-
the Society for Research in Child Development, 53(217).
vaez (Eds.), Morality, self, and identity: Essays in honor of Au-
Howes, C. (1992). The collaborative construction of pretend. Albany: gusto Blasi (pp. 269300). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
State University of New York Press.
Kerns, K. A., Cole, A. K., & Andrews, P. B. (1998). Attachment secu-
Howes, C. (1996). The earliest friendships. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. rity, parent peer management practices, and peer relationships in
Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: preschoolers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 504522.
Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 6686). Cambridge:
Kerns, K. A., Contreras, J. M., & Neal-Barnett, A. M. (Eds.). (2000).
Cambridge University Press.
Family and peers: Linking two social worlds. London: Praeger.
Howes, C., Matheson, C. C., & Wu, F. (1992). Friendships and social
pretend play. In C. Howes, O. Unger, & C. C. Matheson (Eds.), Khatri, P., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (2003). Aggression, peer victimiza-
The collaborative construction of pretend. Albany: State Univer- tion, and social relationships among Indian youth. International
sity of New York Press. Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 8795.
Howes, C., & Phillipsen, L. (1998). Continuity in childrens relation- Kiesner, J., Poulin, F., & Nicotra, E. (2003). Peer relations across
ships with peers. Social Development, 7, 340349. contexts: Individual network homophily and network inclusion in
and after school. Child Development, 74, 13281343.
Hoza, B., Molina, B., Bukowski, W. M., & Sippola, L. K. (1995). Ag-
gression, withdrawal and measures of popularity and friendship as Killen, M., Crystal, D. S., & Watanabe, H. (2002). Japanese and
predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems during American childrens evaluations of peer exclusion, tolerance of
early adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 787802. differences, and prescriptions for conformity. Child Development,
73, 17881802.
Humphreys, A. P., & Smith, P. K. (1987). Rough-and-tumble, friend-
ship, and dominance in school children: Evidence for continuity Killen, M., Lee-Kim, J., McGlothlin, H., & Stangor, C. (2002). How
and change with age. Child Development, 58, 201212. children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion.
Hymel, S. (1986). Interpretations of peer behavior: Affective bias in Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(4).
childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 57, 431445. Killen, M., & Stangor, C. (2001). Childrens reasoning about social
Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive versus with- inclusion and exclusion in peer group contexts. Child Develop-
drawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-perceptions ment, 72, 174186.
in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 879896. Killen, M., Stangor, C., Horn, S., & Sechrist, G. B. (2004). Social
Hymel, S., Comfort, C., Schonert-Reichl, K., & McDougall, P. reasoning about racial exclusion in intimate and nonintimate rela-
(2002). Academic failure and school dropout: The inf luence of tionships. Youth and Society, 35, 293322.
peers. In K. Wentzel & J. Juvonen (Eds.), Social motivation: Un- Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for indi-
derstanding childrens school adjustment (pp. 313335). Cam- vidual development: The case of childrens motivation in school.
bridge: Cambridge University Press. Developmental Psychology, 29, 970977.
References 639

Kindermann, T. A. (1995). Distinguishing buddies from by- Kuttler, A. F., Parker, J. G., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Developmen-
standers: The study of childrens development within natural peer tal and gender differences in preadolescents judgments of the ve-
contexts. In T. A. Kindermann & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Development of racity of gossip. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 105132.
person-context relations (pp. 205226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lacourse, E., Nagin, D., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., & Claes, M.
Kindermann, T. A., McCollam, T. L., & Gibson, E., Jr. (1995). Peer (2003). Developmental trajectories of boys delinquent group
networks and students classroom engagement during childhood membership and facilitation of violent behaviors during adoles-
and adolescence. In K. Wentzel & J. Juvonen (Eds.), Social moti- cence. Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), 183197.
vation: Understanding childrens school adjustment Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, making friends,
(pp. 279312). New York: Cambridge University Press. and being liked by peers in the classroom: Predictors of childrens
Kistner, J., Balthazor, M., Risi, S., & Burton, C. (1999). Predicting early school adjustment? Child Development, 61, 312331.
dysphoria from actual and perceived acceptance in childhood. Ladd, G. W. (1991). Family-peer relations during childhood: Path-
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 94104. ways to competence and pathology? Journal of Social and Per-
Kochanska, G. (1998). Mother-child relationship, child fearfulness, sonal Relationships, 8, 307314.
and emerging attachment: A short-term longitudinal study. Devel- Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (1999). Charting the relationship trajec-
opmental Psychology, 34, 480490. tories of aggressive, withdrawn, and aggressive/withdrawn chil-
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., & Coy, K. (1997). Inhibitory control as a dren during early grade school. Child Development, 70, 910929.
contributor to conscience in childhood: From toddler to early Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (2001). Do relational risks and protec-
school age. Child Development, 68(2), 263277. tive factors moderate the linkages between childhood aggression
Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). Victimized childrens re- and early psychological and school adjustment? Child Develop-
sponses to peers aggression: Behaviors associated with reduced ment, 72, 15791601.
versus continued victimization. Development and Psychopathol- Ladd, G. W., & Golter, B. S. (1988). Parents initiation and monitor-
ogy, 9, 5973. ing of childrens peer contacts: Predictive of childrens peer rela-
Kochenderfer-Ladd, B. (2003). Identification of aggressive and aso- tions in nonschool and school settings? Developmental
cial victims and the stability of their peer victimization. Merrill- Psychology, 24, 109117.
Palmer Quarterly, 49(4), 401425. Ladd, G. W., & Hart, C. H. (1992). Creating informal play opportu-
Kochenderfer-Ladd B., & Wardrop, J. L. (2001). Chronicity and in- nities: Are parents and preschoolers initiations related to chil-
stability of childrens peer victimization experiences as predic- drens competence with peers? Developmental Psychology,
tors of loneliness and social satisfaction trajectories. Child 28(6), 11791187.
Development, 72, 134151. Ladd, G. W., & Kochenderfer, B. (1996). Linkages between friend-
Kovacs, D. M., Parker, J. G., & Hoffman, L. W. (1996). Behavioral, ship and adjustment during early school transitions. In W. M.
affective, and social correlates of involvement in cross-sex friend- Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company
ship in elementary school. Child Development, 67, 22692286. they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence
Kraatz-Keily, M., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2000). (pp. 322345). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
A cross-domain analysis: Externalizing and internalizing behav- Ladd, G. W., & Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. (1996).
iors during 8 years of childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Friendship quality as a predictor of young childrens early school
chology, 28, 161179. adjustment. Child Development, 67, 11031118.
Krappmann, L. (1996). Amicitia, drujba, shin-yu, philia, freund- Ladd, G. W., & Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. C. (1997). Class-
schaft, friendship: On the cultural diversity of a human relation- room peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: Distinct re-
ship. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), lational systems that contribute uniquely to childrens school
The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence adjustment? Child Development, 68, 11811197.
(pp. 1940). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ladd, G. W., & Pettit, G. S. (2002). Parenting and the development of
Krasnor, L., & Rubin, K. H. (1983). Preschool social problem solving: childrens peer relationships. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook
Attempts and outcomes in naturalistic interaction. Child Develop- of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed.,
ment, 54, 15451558. pp. 269309). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kupersmidt, J. B., Burchinal, M., & Patterson, C. J. (1995). Develop- Ladd, G. W., & Troop-Gordon, W. (2003). The role of chronic peer
mental patterns of childhood peer relations as predictors of exter- difficulties in the development of childrens psychological adjust-
nalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, ment problems. Child Development, 74(5), 13441367.
7, 649668. LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (1998). The nature of chil-
Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Preadolescent peer status, drens stereotypes of popularity. Social Development, 7, 301320.
aggression, and school adjustment as predictors of externalizing LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2002). Childrens percep-
problems in adolescence. Child Development, 61, 13501362. tions of popular and unpopular peers: A multimethod assessment.
Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1990). The role of Developmental Psychology, 38, 635647.
poor peer relationships in the development of disorder. In S. R. Laird, R. D., Jordan, K. Y., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates,
Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood J. E. (2001). Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with anti-
(pp. 274305). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. social peers in early adolescence, and the development of exter-
Kupersmidt, J. B., & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.). (2004). Childrens peer re- nalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology,
lations: From development to intervention. Washington, DC: 13, 337354.
American Psychological Association. Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J.
Kuttler, A. F., La Greca, A. M., & Prinstein, M. J. (1999). Friendship (2003). Friendship quality, peer group affiliation, and peer anti-
qualities and social-emotional functioning of adolescents with social behavior as moderators of the link between negative parent-
close, cross-sex friendships. Journal of Research on Adolescence, ing and adolescent externalizing behavior. Journal of Research on
9, 339366. Adolescence, 13, 161184.
640 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Lansford, J. E., & Parker, J. G. (1999). Childrens interactions in triads: childhood and early adolescence: Associations with peer rela-
Behavioral profiles and effects of gender and patterns of friend- tions. Child Development, 70, 202213.
ships among members. Developmental Psychology, 35, 8093. Little, T. D., Brendgen, M., Wanner, B., & Krappmann, L. (1999).
Larkin, R. W. (1979). Suburban youth in cultural crisis. New York: Childrens reciprocal perceptions of friendship quality in the so-
Oxford University Press. ciocultural contexts of East and West Berlin. International Jour-
Larson, R. W., Brown, B. B., & Mortimer, J. T. (Eds.). (2002). Ado- nal of Behavioral Development, 23, 6389.
lescents preparation for the future: Perils and promiseA report Little, T. D., Jones, S. M., Henrich, C. C., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Dis-
of the study group on adolescence in the twenty-first century. Lon- entangling the whys from the whats of aggressive behavior.
don: Blackwell. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 122133.
Laursen, B. (1993). Conf lict management among close peers. In B. Liu, M., & Chen, X. (2003). Friendship networks and social, school,
Laursen (Ed.), Close friendships in adolescence (pp. 3954). San and psychological adjustment in Chinese junior high school stu-
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. dents. Psychology in the Schools, 40, 517.
Laursen, B., Finkelstein, B. D., & Betts, N. T. (2001). A developmen- Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. London: Methuen.
tal meta-analysis of peer conf lict resolution. Developmental Re- Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Cibelli, C. D. (1997). Infant
view, 21, 423449. attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive
Laursen, B., & Hartup, W. W. (1989). The dynamics of preschool symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems
childrens conflicts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 281297. at age seven. Developmental Psychology, 33, 681692.
Laursen, B., Hartup, W. W., & Koplas, A. L. (1996). Towards under- MacDonald, R. A. R., Miell, D., & Morgan, L. (2000). Social
standing peer conf lict. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 76102. processes and creative collaboration in children. European Jour-
Laursen, B., & Koplas, A. L. (1995). Whats important about impor- nal of the Psychology of Education, 15, 405416.
tant conf licts? Adolescents perceptions of daily disagreements. Masten, A. S., Morrison, P., & Pellegrini, D. S. (1985). A revised
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 536553. class play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology,
Laursen, B., Wilder, D., Noack, P., & Williams, V. (2000). Adoles- 3, 523533.
cent perceptions of reciprocity, authority, and closeness in rela- Matsumoto, D. (1997). Culture and modern life. Pacific Grove, CA:
tionships with mothers, fathers, and friends. International Journal Brooks/Cole.
of Behavioral Development, 24, 464471. Maudry, M., & Nekula, M. (1939). Social relations between children
Laursen, B., & Williams, V. A. (1997). Perceptions of interdepend- of the same age during the first 2 years of life. Journal of Genetic
ence and closeness in family and peer relationships among adoles- Psychology, 54, 193215.
cents with and without romantic partners. In S. Shulman & W. A. McCall, G. J. (1988). The organizational life cycle of relationships.
Collins (Eds.), New Directions for Child Development: Vol. 78. Ro- In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships
mantic relationships in adolescenceDevelopmental perspectives (pp. 467484). New York: Wiley.
(pp. 320). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. McDowell, D. J., & Parke, R. D. (2000). Differential knowledge of
Leaper, C. (1994). Exploring the consequences of gender segregation display rules for positive and negative emotions: Inf luences from
on social relationships. In C. Leaper (Ed.), Childhood gender seg- parents, inf luences on peers. Social Development, 9, 415432.
regation: Causes and consequences (pp. 6786). San Francisco: McNeilly-Choque, M. K., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., Nelson, L. J.,
Jossey-Bass. & Olsen, S. F. (1996). Overt and relational aggression on the
Lease, A. M., Kennedy, C. A., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002). Childrens so- playground: Correspondence among different informants. Journal
cial constructions of popularity. Social Development, 11, 87109. of Research in Childhood Education, 11, 4767.
Lease, A. M., Musgrove, K. T., & Axelrod, J. L. (2002). Dimensions Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of
of social status in preadolescent peer groups: Likability, per- Chicago Press.
ceived popularity, and social dominance. Social Development, Miller-Johnson, S., Coie, J. D., Maumary-Gremaud, A., Bierman, K.,
11, 508533. & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002). Peer re-
Legault, F., & Strayer, F. F. (1991). The emergence of sex-segrega- jection and aggression and early starter models of conduct disor-
tion in preschool peer groups. In F. F. Strayer (Ed.), Behaviour, der. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 217230.
119, 285301. Miller-Johnson, S., Coie, J., Maumary-Gremaud, A., Lochman, J., &
Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of Terry, R. (1999). Relationship between childhood peer rejection
emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. and aggression and adolescent delinquency severity and type
Child Development, 71, 107118. among African-American youth. Journal of Emotional and Behav-
Levin, E., & Rubin, K. H. (1983). Getting others to do what you ioral Disorders, 7, 137146.
wanted them to do: The development of childrens requestive Mills, R. S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1998). Are behavioral control and
strategies. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Child language (Vol. 4, psychological control both differentially associated with child-
pp. 157186). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. hood aggression and social withdrawal? Canadian Journal of Be-
Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive havioral Sciences, 30, 132136.
behavior in experimentally created social climates. Journal of Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mothers social coaching, mother-
Social Psychology, 10, 271299. child relationship style and childrens peer competence: Is the
Lewin, M. L., Davis, B., & Hops, H. (1999). Childhood social predic- medium the message? Child Development, 68, 312332.
tors of adolescent antisocial behavior: Gender differences in pre- Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
dictive accuracy and efficacy. Journal of Abnormal Child antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
Psychology, 27(4), 277292. Review, 100, 674701.
Lieberman, M., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (1999). Developmen- Moller, L., Hymel, S., & Rubin, K. H. (1992). Sex typing in play and
tal patterns in security of attachment to mother and father in late popularity in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 26, 331353.
References 641

Moreno, J. L. (1934). Who shall survive? A new approach to the prob- vaco (Eds.), Aggression in children and youth (pp. 104136). Den
lem of human interrelations. Washington, DC: Nervous and Men- Haag, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
tal Disease. Olweus, D. (1993). Victimization by peers: Antecedents and long-
Mueller, E. (1972). The maintenance of verbal exchanges between term outcomes. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social
young children. Child Development, 43, 930938. withdrawal, inhibition and shyness in childhood (pp. 315341).
Mueller, E., & Brenner, J. (1977). The origins of social skills and inter- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
action among playgroup toddlers. Child Development, 48, 854861. Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Mon-
Murphy, B. C., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). An integrative examination shouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive
of peer conf lict: Childrens reported goals, emotions, and behav- behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916934.
iors. Social Development, 11, 534557. Pakaslahti, L., Karjalainen, A., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2002).
Murphy, L. B. (1937). Social behavior and child psychology: An ex- Relationships between adolescent prosocial problem-solving
ploratory study of some roots of sympathy. New York: Columbia strategies, prosocial behaviour, and social acceptance. Interna-
University Press. tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(2), 137144.
Nangle, D. W., & Erdley, C. A. (Eds.). (2001). The role of friendship Parke, R. D., & ONeil, R. (1999). Social relationships across con-
in psychological adjustment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. texts: Family-peer linkages. In W. A. Collins & B. Laursen
Nangle, D. W., Erdley, C. A., Newman, J. E., Mason, C. A., & Car- (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 30,
penter, E. (2003). Popularity, friendship quantity, and friendship pp. 211239). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
quality: Interactive inf luences on childrens loneliness and de- Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship qual-
pression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, ity in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and
32(4), 546555. feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Psychology, 29, 611621.
Child Care Research Network. (2001). Child care and childrens Parker, J. G., Saxon, J., Asher, S. R., & Kovacs, D. (1999). Dimen-
peer interaction at 24 and 36 months: The National Institute of sions of childrens friendship adjustment: Implications for
Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. studying loneliness. In K. J. Rotenberg & S. Hymel (Eds.), Lone-
Child Development, 72, 14781500. liness in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge Uni-
Nelson, L. J., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2005). Social and nonsocial versity Press.
behaviors and peer acceptance: A longitudinal model of the de- Parker, J. G., & Seal, J. (1996). Forming, losing, renewing, and re-
velopment of self-perceptions in children ages 4 to 7 years. Early placing friendships: Applying temporal parameters to the assess-
Education and Development, 20, 185200. ment of childrens friendship experiences. Child Development,
Newcomb, A., & Bagwell, C. (1995). Childrens friendship relations: 67, 22482268.
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 306347. Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle
Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1983). Social impact and social school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and inter-
preference as determinants of childrens peer group status. Devel- personal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231241.
opmental Psychology, 19, 856867. Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and
Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1984). A longitudinal study of peer-perceived popularity: Two distinct dimensions of peer sta-
the utility of social preference and social impact sociometric tus. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125144.
classification schemes. Child Development, 55, 14341447. Parten, M. B. (1932). Social participation among preschool children.
Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Childrens Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 27, 243269.
peer relations: A meta-analyic review of popular, rejected, ne- Pastor, D. L. (1981). The quality of mother-infant attachment and its
glected, controversial, and average sociometric status. Psycholog- relationship to toddlers initial sociability with peers. Develop-
ical Bulletin, 113, 99128. mental Psychology, 17, 326335.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. (1992). Pre- Pekarik, E. G., Prinz, R. J., Liebert, D. E., Weintraub, S., & Neale,
dictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A J. M. (1976). The pupil evaluation inventory: A sociometric tech-
5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, nique for assessing childrens social behavior. Journal of Abnor-
101(3), 405422. mal Child Psychology, 4, 8397.
Northway, M. L. (1944). Outsiders: A study of the personality pat- Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). Rough-and-tumble play from childhood
terns of children least acceptable to their age mates. Sociome- through adolescence: Development and possible functions. In
try, 7, 1025. P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood
Northway, M. L. (1946). Sociometry and some challenging problems social development (pp. 438453). London: Blackwell.
of social relations. Sociometry, 9, 187198. Perry, D. G., Perry, L., & Kennedy, E. (1992). Conf lict and the devel-
OBrien, S. F., & Bierman, K. L. (1987). Conceptions and perceived opment of antisocial behavior. In C. Shantz & W. W. Hartup
inf luence of peer groups: Interviews with preadolescents and ado- (Eds.), Conf lict in child and adolescent development
lescents. Child Development, 59, 13601365. (pp. 301329). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ollendick, T. H., Weist, M. D., Borden, M. G., & Greene, R. W. Pettit, G. S., Brown, E. G., Mize, J., & Lindsey, E. W. (1998).
(1992). Sociometric status and academic, behavioral, and psycho- Mothers and fathers socializing behaviors in three contexts:
logical adjustment: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Con- Links with childrens peer competence. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 8087. terly, 44, 173193.
Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools: Bullies and whipping Pettit, G. S., Clawson, M. A., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. (1996). Sta-
boys. Oxford, England: Hemisphere. bility and change in peer-rejected status: The role of child be-
Olweus, D. (1984). Stability in aggressive and withdrawn, inhibited havior, parenting, and family ecology. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
behavior patterns. In R. M. Kaplan, V. J. Konecni, & R. W. No- 42, 267294.
642 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Pfeifer, M., Goldsmith, H. H., Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M. Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. (2000). Het-
(2002). Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited chil- erogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial configura-
dren. Child Development, 73, 14741485. tions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 1424.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Rodkin, P. C., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2003). Bullies and victims in the
Free Press. peer ecology: Four questions for psychological and school profes-
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. New York: sionals. School Psychology Review, 32, 384401.
Harcourt, Brace, and World. Rogoff, B. (1997). Evaluating development in the process of partici-
Pike, A., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2003). Do sibling and friend relation- pation: Theory, methods and practice building on each other. In
ships share the same temperamental origins? A twin study. Jour- E. Amsel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Change and development: Is-
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, sues in change, method, and application (pp. 265285). Mahwah,
44, 598611. NJ: Erlbaum.
Rose, A. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys.
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human na-
Child Development, 73, 18301843.
ture. New York: Penguin Putnam.
Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoret-
Pinto, G., Bombi, A. S., & Cordioli, A. (1997). Similarity of friends
ical review. Social Development, 6, 111135.
in three countries: A study of childrens drawings. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 453469. Rose-Krasnor, L., Rubin, K. H., Booth, C. L., & Coplan, R. J. (1996).
Maternal directiveness and child attachment security as predic-
Pope, A. W., Bierman, K., & Mumma, G. H. (1991). Aggression, hy-
tors of social competence in preschoolers. International Journal of
peractivity, and inattention-immaturity: Behavior dimensions as-
Behavioral Development, 19, 309325.
sociated with peer rejection in elementary school boys.
Developmental Psychology, 27(4), 663671. Ross, H. S. (1982). The establishment of social games amongst tod-
dlers. Developmental Psychology, 18, 509518.
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (1999). Proactive and reactive aggression
Ross, H. S., Conant, C., Cheyne, J. A., & Alevizos, E. (1992). Rela-
and boys friendship quality in mainstream classrooms. Journal of
tionships and alliances in the social interactions of kibbutz tod-
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 7(3), 168177.
dlers. Social Development, 1, 117.
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). The role of proactive and reactive ag-
Ross, H. S., & Lollis, S. P. (1989). A social relations analysis of tod-
gression in the formation and development of boys friendships.
dler-peer relationships. Child Development, 60, 10821091.
Developmental Psychology, 36, 233240.
Rotenberg, K., & Slitz, D. (1988). Childrens restrictive disclosure to
Prinstein, M., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer crowd affiliation
friends. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 203215.
and internalizing distress in childhood and adolescence: A lon-
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000). Stability of
gitudinal follow-back study. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent
12(3), 325351.
report at 7 years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Tem-
Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. (2003). Forms and functions of ado- perament and personality development across the life span
lescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. (pp. 85119). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49(3), 310342.
Rothbart, M. K., Ellis, L., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Temperament and
Putallaz, M. (1983). Predicting childrens sociometric status from self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Hand-
their behavior. Child Development, 54, 14171426. book of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications
Putallaz, M., & Gottman, J. (1981). An interactional model of chil- (pp. 357370). New York: Guilford Press.
drens entry into peer groups. Child Development, 52(3), 986994. Rowe, D. C. (1995). The limits of family inf luence: Genes, experience,
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tem- and behavior. New York: Guilford Press.
perament and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Roy, R., & Benenson, J. F. (2002). Sex and contextual effects on chil-
parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 163179). drens use of interference competition. Developmental Psychol-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ogy, 38, 306312.
Rabiner, D., & Gordon, L. (1992). The coordination of conf licting so- Rubin, K. H. (1983). Recent perspectives on sociometric status
cial goals: Differences between rejected and nonrejected boys. in childhood: Some introductory remarks. Child Development,
Child Development, 63, 13441350. 54, 13831385.
Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., & Sroufe, Rubin, K. H. (2001). The Play Observation Scale (POS). University of
L. A., (1989). Early childhood antecedents of aggression and pas- Maryland: Author.
sive-withdrawal in early elementary school. Journal of Personal- Rubin, K. H. (2003). The friendship factor: Helping our children nav-
ity, 57, 257281. igate their social worldAnd why it matters for their success and
Reznick, J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & happiness. New York: Viking Penguin.
Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibited and uninhibited children: A fol- Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interactions,
low-up study. Child Development, 57, 660680. relationships, and groups. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N.
Richards, W. D. (1995). NEGOPY 4.30: Manual and users guide. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada: School of Communication, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 619700).
Simon Fraser University. New York: Wiley.
Robinson, C. C., Anderson, G. T., Porter, C. L., Hart, C. H., & Rubin, K. H., & Burgess, K. (2002). Parents of aggressive and with-
Wouden-Miller, M. (2003). Sequential transition patterns of drawn children. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting
preschoolers social interactions during child-initiated play: Is (2nd ed., Vol. 1, 383418). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
parallel-aware play a bidirectional bridge to other play states? Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Dwyer, K. D., & Hastings, P. (2003).
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, 321. Predicting preschoolers externalizing behaviors from toddler
References 643

temperament, conf lict, and maternal negativity. Developmental opmental perspective (pp. 313331). Dordrecht, The Netherlands:
Psychology, 39, 164176. Kluwer Press.
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., & Hastings, P. D. (2002). Stability and Rubin, K. H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1992). Interpersonal problem solv-
social-behavioral consequences of toddlers inhibited tempera- ing. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of social
ment and parenting. Child Development, 73, 483495. development (pp. 283323). New York: Plenum Press.
Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Kennedy, A. E., & Stewart, S. (2003). Rubin, K. H., Rose-Krasnor, L., Bigras, M., Mills, R. S. L., & Booth,
Social withdrawal in childhood. In E. Mash & R. Barkley (Eds.), C. (1996). La prediction du comportement parental: Les inf lu-
Child psychopathology (2nd ed., pp. 372406). New York: Guil- ences du contexte, des facteurs psychosociaux et des croyances de
ford Press. parents [The prediction of parental behavior: Contextual, psy-
Rubin, K. H., Cheah, C. S. L., & Fox, N. A. (2001). Emotion regula- chosocial and cognitive inf luences]. In G. M. Tarabulsy & R.
tion, parenting, and the display of social reticence in preschool- Tessier (Eds.), La Dveloppement Emotionnel et Social De lEn-
ers. Early Education and Development, 12, 97115. fant (pp. 1132). Sainte Foy, Qubec, Canada: Presses de lUni-
Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., & Hymel, S. (1993). Socioemotional charac- versite du Qubec.
teristics of withdrawn and aggressive children. Merrill-Palmer Rubin, K. H., Watson, K., & Jambor, T. (1978). Free play behav-
Quarterly, 39, 518534. iors in pre-school and kindergarten children. Child Develop-
Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., McDougall, P., Bowker, A., & McKinnon, J. ment, 49, 534536.
(1995). The Waterloo longitudinal project: Predicting internaliz- Rubin, K. H., Wojslawowicz, J. C., Burgess, K. B., Booth-LaForce, C.,
ing and externalizing problems in adolescence. Development and & Rose-Krasnor, L. (in press). The best friendships of shy/with-
Psychopathology, 7, 751764. drawn children: Prevalence, stability, and relationship quality.
Rubin, K. H., & Chung, O. B. (Eds.). (in press). Parental beliefs, par- Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
enting, and child development in cross-cultural perspective. Lon- Russell, A., & Finnie, V. (1990). Preschool social status and mater-
don: Psychology Press. nal instructions to assist group entry. Developmental Psychology,
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. (1995). Emo- 26, 603611.
tionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers social adaptation. Russell, A., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., & Olsen, S. F. (2003). Chil-
Development and Psychopathology, 7, 4962. drens sociable and aggressive behavior with peers: A comparison
Rubin, K. H., Daniels-Beirness, T., & Bream, L. (1984). Social isola- of the U.S. and Australia, and contributions of temperament and
tion and social problem solving: A longitudinal study. Journal of parenting styles. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1725. ment, 27, 7486.
Rubin, K. H., Dwyer, K. M., Booth, C. L., Kim, A. H., Burgess, K. B., Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the development
& Rose-Krasnor, L. (2004). Attachment, friendship, and psy- of young adolescent motivation and achievement. Child Develop-
chosocial functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Early Ado- ment, 72, 11351150.
lescence, 24, 326356. Sage, N. A., & Kindermann, T. A. (1999). Peer networks, behavior
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G., & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In P. H. contingencies, and childrens engagement in the classroom. Mer-
Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Vol. Ed.), Handbook rill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 143171.
of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality and social Salisch, M. von (2000). The emotional side of sharing, emotional sup-
development (4th ed., pp. 693774). New York: Wiley. port, and conf lict negotiation between siblings and between
Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P., Chen, X., Stewart, S., & McNichol, K. friends. In R. Mills & S. Duck (Eds.), Developmental psychology of
(1998). Intrapersonal and maternal correlates of aggression, con- personal relationships (pp. 49-70). Chichester, England: Wiley.
f lict, and externalizing problems in toddlers. Child Development, Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. (1997). Peer net-
69, 16141629. works and bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychol-
Rubin, K. H., Hemphill, S., Chen, X., Hastings, P., Sanson, A., Lo ogy, 38, 305312.
Coco, A., et al. (in press). Parenting beliefs and behaviors: Initial Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections between attitudes,
findings from the International Consortium for the Study of So- group norms, and behaviour in bullying situations. International
cial and Emotional Development (ICSSED). In K. H. Rubin & Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 246258.
O. B. Chung (Eds.), Parental beliefs, parenting, and child develop-
Sameroff, A. J., & MacKenzie, M. J. (2003). Research strategies for
ment in cross-cultural perspective. London: Psychology Press.
capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the
Rubin, K. H., & Krasnor, L. R. (1986). Social-cognitive and social
possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 613640.
behavioral perspectives on problem solving. In M. Perlmutter
(Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 18. Cogni- Sandstrom, M. J., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Eisenhower, A. (2003). Chil-
tive perspectives on childrens social and behavioral development drens appraisal of peer rejection experiences: Impact on social
(pp. 168). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and emotional adjustment. Social Development, 12, 530550.
Rubin, K. H., Lynch, D., Coplan, R., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Booth, C. L. Sandstrom, M. J., & Coie, J. D. (1999). A developmental perspective
(1994). Birds of a feather . . .: Behavioral concordances and on peer rejection: Mechanisms of stability and change. Child De-
preferential personal attraction in children. Child Development, velopment, 70, 955966.
65, 17781785. Sandstrom, M., & Cramer, P. (2003). Girls use of defense mechanisms
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1988). The many faces of social iso- following peer rejection. Journal of Personality, 71, 605627.
lation in childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections be-
ogy, 6, 916924. tween temperament and social development: A review. Social De-
Rubin, K. H., Mills, R. S. L., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1989). Maternal velopment, 13, 142170.
beliefs and childrens social competence. In B. Schneider, G. At- Sawyer, R. K. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation: Conversation in
tili, J. Nadel, & R. Weissberg (Eds.), Social competence in devel- the preschool classroom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
644 Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups

Schafer, M., Werner, N. E., & Crick, N. R. (2002). A comparison of two Simpkins, S. D., & Parke, R. D. (2002). Do friends and nonfriends be-
approaches to the study of negative peer treatment: General victim- have differently? A social relations analysis of childrens behav-
ization and bully/victim problems among German schoolchildren. ior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 263283.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 281306. Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind
Schmidt, M. E., Demulder, E. K., & Denham, S. (2002). Kinder- and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of De-
garten social-emotional competence: Developmental predictors velopmental Psychology, 20(4), 545564.
and psychosocial implications. Early Child Development and Slee, P. T., & Rigby, K. (Eds.). (1998). Childrens peer relations. New
Care, 172, 451462. York: Routledge.
Schneider, B. H. (1999). A multimethod exploration of the friend- Smollar, J., & Youniss, J. (1982). Social development through friend-
ships of children considered socially withdrawn by their school ship. In K. H. Rubin & H. S. Ross (Eds.), Peer relationships and so-
peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 115123. cial skills in childhood (pp. 277298). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child-parent at- Snyder, J., Brooker, M., Patrick, M. R., Snyder, A., Schrepferman, L.,
tachment and childrens peer relations: A quantitative review. De- & Stoolmiller, M. (2003). Observed peer victimization during
velopmental Psychology, 37(1), 86100. early elementary school: Continuity, growth, and relation to risk
Schneider, B. H., Attili, G., Vermigli, P., & Younger, A. (1997). A for child antisocial and depressive behavior. Child Development,
comparison of middle class English-Canadian and Italian moth- 74, 18811898.
ers beliefs about childrens peer-directed aggression and social Spangler, G., & Schieche, M. (1998). Emotional and adrenocortical
withdrawal. International Journal of Behavioral Development, responses of infants to the strange situation: The differential
21, 133154. function of emotional expression. International Journal of Behav-
Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in chil- ioral Development, 22, 681706.
drens peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Harris, E., Fabes, R. A., Kupanoff, K.,
28(2), 181192. Ringwald, S., et al. (2004). The relation of childrens everyday
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2000). nonsocial peer play behavior to their emotionality, regulation, and
Friendship as a moderating factor in the pathway between early social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 6780.
harsh home environment and later victimization in the peer group.
Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of
Developmental Psychology, 36(5), 646662.
adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladaptation. In M. Perl-
Schwartz, D., Farver, J. M., Chang, L., & Lee-Shin, Y. (2002). Vic- mutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 16,
timization in South Korean childrens peer groups. Journal of Ab- pp. 4183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
normal Child Psychology, 30, 113125.
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental
Sebanc, A. M. (2003). The friendship features of preschool children: psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1729.
Links with prosocial behavior and aggression. Social Develop-
Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Marshall, P. J. (1999). Behavioral inhibition,
ment, 12, 249268.
heart period, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia: An attachment
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2003). Testing theories of romantic development perspective. Child Development, 70, 805816.
from adolescence to young adulthood: Evidence of a developmen-
Stewart, S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1995). The social problem solving
tal sequence. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
skills of anxious-withdrawn children. Development and Psycho-
27, 519531.
pathology, 7, 323336.
Shatz, M., & Gelman, R. (1973). The development of communication
Stocker, C., & Dunn, J. (1990). Sibling relationships in childhood:
skills: Modifications in the speech of young children as a function
Links with friendships and peer relationships. British Journal of
of the listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Developmental Psychology, 8, 227244.
Development, 38(38).
Shaw, D., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Keenan, K., & Winslow, E. B. Stormshak, E. A., Bierman, K. L., Bruschi, C., Dodge, K. A., Coie,
(1996). Early risk factors and pathways in the development of J. D., & the Conduct Preventions Research Group. (1999, Janu-
early disruptive behavior problems. Development and Psychopath- ary/ February). The relation between behavior problems and peer
ology, 8, 679699. preference in different classroom contexts. Child Development,
70(1), 169182.
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C.
(1961). Inter-group conf lict and cooperation: The robbers cave ex- Strassberg, Z. (1995). Social information processing in compliance
periment. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press. situations by mothers of behavior-problem boys. Child Develop-
ment, 66(2), 376389.
Shrum, W., & Cheek, N. H. (1987). Social structure during the school
years: Onset of the degrouping process. American Sociological Re- Strayer, F. F., & Strayer, J. (1976). An ethological analysis of social
view, 52, 218223. agonism and dominance relations among preschool children.
Child Development, 47, 980989.
Shulman, S., & Laursen, B. (2002). Adolescent perceptions of con-
f lict in interdependent and disengaged friendships. Journal of Re- Strough, J., Swenson, L. M., & Cheng, S. (2001). Friendship, gender
search on Adolescence, 12, 353372. and preadolescents representation of peer collaboration. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 47, 475499.
Silbereisen, R. K. (2000). German unification and adolescents devel-
opmental timetables: Continuities and discontinuities. In L. A. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New
Crockett & R. K. Silbereisen (Eds.), Negotiating adolescence in York: Norton.
times of social change (pp. 104122). Cambridge, MA: Cam- Suttles, G. D. (1970). Friendship as a social institution. In G. Mc-
bridge University Press. Call, M. McCall, N. Denzin, G. Suttles, & S. Kurth (Eds.), Social
Simons, K. J., Paternite, C. E., & Shore, C. (2001). Patterns of par- relationships (pp. 95135). Chicago: Aldine.
ent /adolescent attachment and aggression in young adolescents. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New
Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 182203. York: Brunner/ Mazel.
References 645

Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. London: Oxford Univer- Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). The characteristics, quality, and correlates
sity Press. of friendship among African American, Latino, and Asian Ameri-
Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (1997). Relational aggression, gen- can adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 274301.
der, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over Wentzel, K. R. (2003). Sociometric status and adjustment in middle
time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psy- school: A longitudinal study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 528.
chology, 33, 601609. Wentzel, K. R., & Asher, S. R. (1995). The academic lives of ne-
Tremblay, R. E., Msse, L. C., Vitaro, F., & Dobkin, P. L. (1995). The glected, rejected, popular, and controversial children. Child De-
impact of friends deviant behavior on early onset of delinquency: velopment, 66, 754763.
Longitudinal data from 6 to 13 years of age. Development and Wentzel, K. R., McNamara-Barry, C., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004).
Psychopathology, 7, 649668. Friendships in middle school: Inf luences on motivation and school
Troy, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1987). Victimization among preschoolers: adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(2), 195203.
Role of attachment relationship history. Journal of the American White, K. J., & Kistner, J. (1992). The inf luence of teacher feedback
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 166172. on young childrens peer preferences and perceptions. Develop-
Tudge, J. (1992). Processes and consequences of peer collaborations: mental Psychology, 28, 933940.
A Vygotskian analysis. Child Development, 63, 13641379. Wichmann, C., Coplan, R. J., & Daniels, T. (2004). The social cognitions
Urberg, K. A., Degirmencioglu, S. M., & Pilgrim, C. (1997). Close of socially withdrawn children. Social Development, 13, 377392.
friend and group inf luence on adolescent cigarette smoking and Wiseman, R. (2002). Queen bees and wannabes: Helping your daugh-
alcohol use. Developmental Psychology, 33, 834844. ter survive cliques, gossip, boyfriends, and other realities of ado-
lescence. New York: Crown.
Vandell, D. L., & Mueller, E. (1980). Peer play and friendships dur-
ing the first 2 years. In H. Foot, A. Chapman, & J. Smith (Eds.), Wojslawowicz, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B., Booth-LaForce, C.,
Friendship and social relations in children (pp. 181208). New & Rose-Krasnor, L. R. (in press). Behavioral characteristics asso-
York: Wiley. ciated with stable and f luid best friendship patterns in middle
childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly.
van den Oord, E. J. C., Rispens, J., Goudena, P. P., & Vermande, M.
Wright, J. C., Giammarino, M., & Parad, H. (1986). Social status in
(2000). Some developmental implications of structural aspects of
small groups: Individual group similarity and the social misfit.
preschoolers relations with classmates. Journal of Applied Devel-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 523536.
opmental Psychology, 21, 619639.
Xie, H., Cairns, B. D., & Cairns, R. B. (2001). Predicting teen moth-
Vaughn, B. E. (1999). Power is knowledge (and vice versa): A com-
erhood and teen fatherhood: Individual characteristics and peer
mentary on On winning some and losing some: A social relations
affiliations. Social Development, 10, 488511.
approach to social dominance in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
terly, 45, 215225. Xie, H., Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1999). Social networks and
configurations in inner-city schools: Aggression, popularity, and
Vaughn, B. E. (2001). Dyadic analyses of friendship in a sample of
implications for students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Be-
preschool children attending Head Start: Correspondence be-
havioral Disorders, 7, 147155.
tween measures. Child Development, 72, 862878.
Youngblade, L., & Belsky, J. (1992). Parent-child antecedents of 5-
Vaughn, B. E., Vollenweider, M., Bost, K. K., Azria-Evans, M. R., & year-olds close friendships: A longitudinal analysis. Develop-
Snider, J. B. (2003). Negative interactions and social competence mental Psychology, 28, 107121.
for preschool children in two samples: Reconsidering the interpre-
Youngblade, L., Park, K., & Belsky, J. (1993). Measurement of young
tation of aggressive behavior for young children. Merrill-Palmer
childrens close friendship: A comparison of two independent as-
Quarterly, 49, 245278.
sessment systems and their associations with attachment security.
Vermande, M., van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Rispens, J., & Goudena, International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 563587.
P. P. (2000). Structural characteristics of aggressor-victim rela-
Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D., & Rankin, D. (1990). The psychologi-
tionships in school classes of 4- to 5-year-olds. Aggressive Behav-
cal functions of preadolescent peer activities. Child Development,
ior, 26, 1131.
61, 10671080.
Vernberg, E. M., Jacobs, A. K., & Hershberger, S. L. (1999). Peer vic- Zarbatany, L., McDougall, P., & Hymel, S. (2000). Gender-differenti-
timization and attitudes about violence during early adolescence. ated experience in the peer culture: Links to intimacy in preado-
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28(3), 386395. lescence. Social Development, 9, 6279.
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2000). Inf luence of de- Zarbatany, L., Van Brunschot, M., Meadows, K., & Pepper, S. (1996).
viant friends on delinquency: Searching for moderator variables. Effects of friendship and gender on peer group entry. Child De-
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(4), 313325. velopment, 67, 22872300.
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and Zakriski, A., & Coie, J. (1996). A comparison of aggressive-re-
proactively aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent char- jected and nonaggressive rejected childrens interpretations of
acteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied self-directed and other-directed rejection. Child Development,
Disciplines, 43, 495506. 67, 10481070.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psy- Zeller, M., Vannatta, K., Schafer, J., & Noll, R. (2003). Behavioral
chological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. reputation: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology,
Walden, T., Lemerise, E., & Smith, M. C. (1999). Friendship and 39, 129139.
popularity in preschool classrooms. Early Education and Develop- Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., & Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese chil-
ment, 10, 351371. drens effortful control and dispositional anger/frustration: Rela-
Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A., & Capage, L. (1999). Social tions to parenting styles and childrens social functioning.
interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. Develop- Developmental Psychology, 40, 352366.
mental Psychology, 35, 386391.
CHAPTER 11

Prosocial Development
NANCY EISENBERG, RICHARD A. FABES, and TRACY L. SPINRAD

PHILOSOPHICAL ROOTS OF Parental Socialization Style and Practices 666


PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 647 Other Familial and Extrafamilial Inf luences 678
PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES 648 COGNITIVE AND SOCIOCOGNITIVE CORRELATES
Psychoanalytic Theory 648 OF PROSOCIAL DEVELOPMENT 683
Behaviorism and Social Learning Theory 649 Intelligence, Cognitive Capacities, and
Cognitive Developmental Theory 649 Academic Achievement 683
Current Conceptual Emphases: Positive Psychology and Perspective Taking and Understanding of Emotion 683
Positive Youth Development 650 Person Attributions and Expressed Motives 685
BIOLOGICAL DETERMINANTS OF Moral Reasoning 685
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 650 EMPATHY-RELATED EMOTIONAL RESPONDING 686
Evolutionary Explanations 651 DISPOSITIONAL AND PERSONALITY
Heritability of Prosocial Tendencies 652 CORRELATES OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 688
Neurophysiological Underpinnings of Prosocial Consistency of Prosocial Behavior 688
Responding 653 Sociability and Shyness 689
DEVELOPMENTAL TRENDS IN THE EMERGENCE Social Competence and Socially Appropriate Behavior 689
OF PROSOCIAL TENDENCIES 654 Aggression and Externalizing Problems 690
Theory 654 Assertiveness and Dominance 691
Empirical Studies of the Development of Self-Esteem and Related Constructs 691
Prosocial Behavior 655 Values and Goals 692
Moderators of Age Trends across Childhood Religiosity 692
and Adolescence 658 Regulation 693
Processes Potentially Related to Changes with Age in Emotionality 694
Prosocial Responding 659
THE ROLE OF RELATIONSHIP HISTORY IN
CULTURAL DETERMINANTS OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 695
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 662
SEX DIFFERENCES IN CHILDRENS
Laboratory or Adult- and Self-Report Studies 662
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 696
Naturalistic Observational Research 663
AN INTEGRATIVE MODEL OF
Moral Reasoning, Values, and Beliefs about
PROSOCIAL ACTION 698
Social Responsibility 664
CHALLENGES AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS 700
SOCIALIZATION WITHIN AND OUTSIDE
THE FAMILY 665 Methodological Issues 700
Demographic Features of Families and Conceptual and Content-Related Directions 701
Family Members 665 REFERENCES 702

Prosocial behaviorvoluntary behavior intended to ben- the consequences of aggression, criminality, and im-
efit anotheris of obvious importance to the quality of morality had greater salience for society.
interactions between individuals and among groups. Prosocial behaviors may be performed for a host of
However, scientists did not devote much attention to reasons including egoistic, other-oriented, or practical
prosocial development prior to 1970, perhaps because concerns (Boxer, Tisak, & Goldstein, 2004; Eisenberg,
1986). Of particular importance is the subgroup of
Writing of this chapter was supported by a grant from the Na-
tional Institute of Mental Health to Nancy Eisenberg and Development to Richard A. Fabes. Appreciation also is ex-
Tracy Spinrad and by grants from the National Science Foun- pressed to Carolyn Zahn-Waxler for comments on earlier
dation and the National Institute of Child Health and Human drafts of this manuscript.

646
Philosophical Roots of Prosocial Behavior 647

prosocial behaviors labeled altruism. A common defini- sections of the chapter, a model for integrating the
tion of altruism is intrinsically motivated voluntary be- factors believed to relate to prosocial responding is
havior intended to benefit another acts motivated by presented brief ly, and gaps in the field and future di-
concern for others or by internalized values, goals, and rections are discussed.
self-rewards rather than by the expectation of concrete In this chapter, we review many of the major topics
or social rewards or the avoidance of punishment (Eisen- in the literature on prosocial development. Due to
berg & Mussen, 1989). However, because it usually is space constraints, we have sometimes built on previ-
impossible to differentiate between altruistically moti- ously published reviews. We generally have empha-
vated actions and actions motivated by less noble con- sized topics of central importance to prosocial
cerns, it is necessary to focus on the broader domain of development and issues that have emerged in the past
prosocial behaviors. decade or two. Further, we have confined our coverage
Emotion plays a particularly important role in the de- to a somewhat narrow definition of prosocial respond-
velopment of prosocial values, motives, and behaviors. ing. For example, the literature on cooperation, the
Especially relevant are empathy-related emotions. Defi- personality trait of agreeableness, or the allocation of
nitions of empathy vary; we define it as an affective rewards generally is not emphasized, although some in-
response that stems from the apprehension or compre- vestigators of prosocial behavior included cooperation
hension of anothers emotional state or condition, and as well as other types of prosocial behavior in their
which is identical or very similar to what the other per- index of prosociality (in these cases, we sometimes
son is feeling or would be expected to feel. have included the study with other citations, but often
It is necessary to differentiate empathy from re- refer to it in listings under also see . . .). Again due to
lated vicarious emotional responses, particularly space limitations, we often cite the more recent studies
sympathy and personal distress. Sympathy is an affec- when there are numerous reports pertaining to a given
tive response that frequently stems from empathy, but issue. Interested readers can refer to the earlier version
can derive directly from perspective taking or other of this chapter in the fifth edition of this Handbook
cognitive processing, including retrieval of informa- (1998) to obtain additional citations, especially refer-
tion from memory. It consists of feeling sorrow or ences prior to 1990.
concern for the distressed or needy other (rather than
feeling the same emotion as the other person is expe- PHILOSOPHICAL ROOTS OF
riencing or is expected to experience). Personal dis- PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
tress also frequently stems from exposure to anothers
state or condition; however, it is a self-focused, aver- Philosophical concepts of prosocial behavior and sym-
sive emotional reaction to the vicarious experiencing pathy often have their roots in religious doctrine. The
of anothers emotion (e.g., discomfort, anxiety; see commandment Thou shalt love thy neighbor as thy-
Batson, 1991; Eisenberg, Shea, Carlo, & Knight, self is a basic tenet in Judaism and Christianity. Simi-
1991). As discussed later, empathy and sympathy larly, the parable of the Good Samaritan, who pitied
have been strongly implicated in prosocial develop- and helped an injured man (Luke 10:2937), often is
ment and action. Thus, these vicarious emotional cited as an example for Christians. In Buddhism, the via
reactions are discussed to some degree throughout positiva outlines the virtues necessary to reach Nirvana
the chapter. (ultimate happiness), including dana (giving), metta
In the initial sections of this chapter, we brief ly ( kindness), mudita (sympathetic joy), and karuna
discuss philosophical perspectives on prosocial devel- (compassion).
opment, as well as several grand psychological theo- Given the influence of religion in philosophy, it is not
ries that have inf luenced the field. Then the empirical surprising that philosophers have discussed the origins
literature related to prosocial responding in children of prosocial and moral behaviors for centuries. Of par-
is reviewed. Because there have been few recent stud- ticular relevance, philosophers have debated whether
ies on the role of situational factors such as cost and any human action is truly unselfish and, relatedly, the
benefits, situational skills, or mood inductions on doctrine of ethical egoism (i.e., whether it is unreason-
prosocial behavior, these topics are not reviewed (see able to behave in a manner contrary to ones own self-
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998, for a review). In the final interest). According to Thomas Hobbes (1651/1962), a
648 Prosocial Development

vocal advocate of egoism and self-love, selfishness of others, they sometimes produce rationality and may,
might produce helping, but the motivation for such in addition, induce more and higher quality prosocial
prosocial action would primarily be to relieve the behavior than does rationality. Similarly, Slote (2001,
helpers own distress. He also believed that the only mo- 2004) argued that caring is a true virtue that is involved
tivation for cooperative action lay in the fear of some in moral judgment and that empathy is essential to the
outside agent. development of morally based caring about others. Re-
Later philosophers began to refute the doctrine of latedly, current writings on altruistic (or compassion-
ethical egoism. Rousseau (1773/1962) believed that ate) love (which correlates with sympathy; L.G.
human nature was basically good and that humans have Underwood, 2001) and agape (altruistic love universal-
an innate sensitivity toward others. In his view, if indi- ized to all humanity; Post, 2001) in theology and philos-
viduals were able to develop this natural state of nobility ophy are relevant to the notions of selfless love and to
and sensitivity, a strong sense of moral obligation to extending caring to people outside ones ingroup.
others and concern for the common good would develop. In summary, philosophers have viewed people as pri-
He believed society corrupts this innate moral nature. marily egoistic, primarily noble and generous, or some-
Kant (1785/1956) also refuted the doctrine of ethical where in between. Philosophical debate about the nature
egoism and argued that if an action is ones duty, that is and existence of altruism is alive and well in contempo-
reason enough to do it, independent of ones own inter- rary psychology, particularly in social (e.g., Batson &
ests. According to Kant, prosocial and moral behavior Powell, 2003) and evolutionary (Konner, 2002) psychol-
and values involve ones will and self-control, and stem ogy. However, it is often difficult to discriminate peo-
from universal, impartial principles that are totally de- ples motives and conceptions of their prosocial
tached from emotion. behavior. Thus, philosophical concerns are not highly
Nagel (1970) differentiated between pure rational al- salient in developmental work and are reflected primar-
truism and behavior motivated by sympathy, love, or ily in work on moral judgment influenced by cognitive
other emotions. In his view, the involvement of affect in developmental theory.
the helping process tainted its purity. In contrast, David
Hume (1748/1975) argued that moral emotions such as
sympathy, benevolence, and concern for humanity are PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES
fundamental incentives of human action and that proso-
cial behaviors often are based on these incentives. Sus- As might be expected, the grand theories that have had
ceptibility to sympathy and empathy was viewed as an considerable influence on developmental psychology
innate human propensity. Similarly, sympathy and re- have affected thinking about prosocial development,
lated affective responses were core elements of A. particularly in the past. Thus, pertinent ideas in psycho-
Smiths (1759/1982) moral and social system. Smith be- analytic theory, behaviorism and social learning theory,
lieved that sympathy was an innate endowment, insti- and cognitive developmental theory are discussed
gated by the perception of others conditions and the briefly. In addition, recent work on prosocial behavior
desire to see them happy for purely altruistic reasons. has been influenced by minitheories such as Hoffmans
For Smith, sympathy was not solely a primitive aware- theoretical contributions to understanding empathy
ness of others suffering; it was a complex capacity in- (1982, 2000) and socialization (1970, 1983) and
fluenced by awareness of aspects of the situation or the Grusecs (e.g., Grusec & Goodnow, 1994) and Staubs
person involved. (1979, 1992, 2003) thinking about socialization. Some
Lawrence Blum (1980) has been particularly vocal in of these conceptual frameworks are referred to briefly
refuting some of Kants ideas about the role of emotion later in this chapter.
in morality. He pointed out that rational processes do
not always produce moral action and that the sense of
duty (viewed by Kant as rational) is no more immune to Psychoanalytic Theory
the distorting and weakening effects of personal feel-
ings than is sympathy for another. Blum further sug- In Freuds psychoanalytic theory, children are born with
gested that because emotions such as sympathy and innate, irrational sexual and aggressive impulses di-
empathy promote perspective taking and understanding rected toward self-gratification (the id). They develop a
Psychological Theories 649

conscience (superego) at about age 4 to 6 years as a learn the likely consequences of a behavior through ob-
means of resolving the conflict between their own hos- servation and verbal behavior. Imitation is viewed as a
tile and sexual impulses and their fears of parental hos- critical process in the socialization of moral behavior
tility or the loss of parental love. The superego is the and standards (Bandura, 1986).
outcome of the process of identification, by which chil- In current cognitive social learning theory, the inter-
dren internalize their same-sex parents values and in- play of cognition and environmental influences in moral
troject these values. Once children develop a superego, development is complex. According to Bandura (1986;
they may behave prosocially to avoid the guilt inflicted also see Hoffman, 2000), moral rules or standards of be-
by the conscience for not doing so or based on the inter- havior are fashioned from information from a variety of
nalization of values consistent with prosocial behavior sources such as intuition, others evaluative social reac-
(e.g., Freud, 1933/1968). In many versions of psychoan- tions, and models. Based on experience, people learn
alytic theory, guilt, self-destructive tendencies, and sex- what factors are morally relevant and how much value to
ual strivings underlie altruism (Fenichel, 1945; Glover, attach to each one. Socializers provide information
1968). Prosocial actions often are defense mechanisms about behavioral alternatives, expectations, and possible
used by the ego (the rational part of personality) to deal contingencies for different courses of action; model
with the irrational demands of the superego. moral behaviors; reinforce and punish children for vari-
However, Freud and other psychoanalysts sometimes ous actions; and influence the development of self-
have acknowledged more positive roots of altruism. evaluative reactions (e.g., guilt). Moreover, thought,
Freud (1930) asserted, Individual development seems behavior, and environmental events all interact and in-
to us a product of the interplay of two trends, the striv- fluence one another, and the individuals attentional and
ing for happiness, generally called egoistic, and the regulatory processes play a role in the learning of moral
impulse toward merging with others in the community, behavior. Moral and prosocial functioning are thought to
which we call altruistic (1930, p. 134). Other theo- be governed by self-reactive responses (e.g., self
rists such as Ekstein (1978) have built on Freuds em- processes such as self-sanctions, personal agency) and
phasis on the importance of the early mother-child other self-regulatory processes rather than by dispas-
relationship for the development of empathy, identifica- sionate abstract reasoning (Bandura, 2002). Addition-
tion, and internalization. ally, the regulation of affect has an important influence
Perhaps the greatest contribution of psychoanalytic on prosocial behavior. Support for this argument has
work to theory on prosocial responding is the construct been found: Perceived self-efficacy in the regulation of
of identification. Social learning theorists in the 1960s positive affect was related to perceptions of empathic
and 1970s adapted this construct to refer to childrens efficacy, which in turn were related to prosocial behav-
internalization of parents norms, values, and standards ior (Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Gerbino, & Pas-
as a consequence of a positive parent-child relationship torelli, 2003). Thus, perceived self-efficacy to manage
(e.g., Hoffman, 1970). This theoretical perspective had basic affective states plays a pivotal role in the causal
a significant impact on the early work on the socializa- processes determining the likelihood of empathic re-
tion of altruism. sponding and prosocial behavior.

Behaviorism and Social Learning Theory Cognitive Developmental Theory

Early behaviorists posited that children learn primarily The cognitive developmental perspective on morality, as
through mechanisms such as conditioning. This per- represented by the work of Piaget (e.g., 1932/1965) and
spective is reflected in some of the relatively early work Kohlberg (e.g., 1969, 1984), concerns primarily the de-
on the role of reinforcement and punishment in promot- velopment of moral reasoning and other social cognitive
ing prosocial behavior (e.g., Hartmann et al., 1976) and processes rather than moral behavior. Kohlberg de-
in work concerning the development of empathy through scribed moral development as an invariant, universal,
conditioning (Aronfreed, 1970). and hierarchical sequence of stages progressing as a
Social learning theorists allowed internal cognitive function of sociocognitive development (e.g., perspec-
processes to play a greater role. For example, contingen- tive taking). Kohlberg emphasized the contributions of
cies need not actually occur; people can vicariously cognition, particularly perspective taking, to morality
650 Prosocial Development

and minimized ( but did not fully neglect) the contribu- tial for systematic change in behavior . . . as a conse-
tions of emotion and socialization (Kohlberg, 1969). quence of mutually influential relationships between the
Moreover, because of Piagets and Kohlbergs assump- developing person and his or her biology, psychological
tion that young children have limited perspective-taking characteristics, family, community, culture, physicial
abilities, investigators influenced by cognitive develop- and designed ecology, and historical niche (Lerner
mental work assumed for years that other-oriented et al., 2005, p. 13; also see Lerner, Dowling, & Ander-
prosocial behavior was not likely to emerge until the son, 2003).
early school years. Although prosocial behavior has not been a primary
The cognitive developmental perspective is discussed topic of interest for those researchers most associated
by Turiel (Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume). Its with the positive psychology movement, some psycholo-
primary relevance for this chapter is that Kohlbergs gists (Aspinwall & Staudinger, 2003a; Eisenberg & Ota
theory influenced Eisenbergs (e.g., Eisenberg, 1986) Wang, 2003) have argued that interpersonal and rela-
work on prosocial moral reasoning. However, although tional strengths such as sympathy, compassion, coopera-
Eisenberg views sociocognitive development as playing tion, tolerance, and altruism are important topics of
an important role in the development of prosocial moral investigation for those investigators concerned with pos-
reasoning, she does not view all stages of prosocial rea- itive psychological development. In fact, prosocial and
soning (especially the higher ones) as universal or as in- empathic development are discussed in some books on
volving the hierarchical integration of lower stages. positive psychology (e.g., Aspinwall & Staudinger,
Rather, environmental and emotional factors are be- 2003b; Lopez & Snyder, 2003), and the positive psy-
lieved to play a considerable role in the development and chology movement has stimulated renewed interest in
use of prosocial moral reasoning. Thus, Eisenbergs con- prosocial behavior and sympathy by including the topic
ception of moral reasoning differs considerably from the in various books and conferences. Similarly, caring is
traditional cognitive developmental perspective. viewed as one of five components of positive youth de-
velopment (along with competence, confidence, connec-
tion, and character); thus, some relevant research
Current Conceptual Emphases: Positive
contains measures of sympathy or related constructs
Psychology and Positive Youth Development
(e.g., Lerner et al., 2005).
Positive psychology and positive youth psychology are Now that the conceptual roots of work on prosocial
not fully developed theories, but perspectives that re- responding have been reviewed briefly, we turn to the
cently have influenced the study of prosocial behavior. review of the empirical literature. We first examine
Although prosocial behavior was a popular topic of study theory and empirical work on developmental trends in
in the 1970s and early 1980s, interest declined in the late prosocial responding, followed by discussion of the
1980s and the 1990s. Since the late 1990s, there has been potential origins of prosocial behavior ( biological,
a resurgence of interest in the positive aspects of human cultural, and socialization). Next we consider the
development, spurred by the positive psychology move- sociocognitive, empathy-related, dispositional, and sit-
ment. This movement is an effort to counteract the focus uational correlates of prosocial behavior. In the final
on negative aspects of psychological functioning (e.g., sections, age and sex differences in prosocial behavior
problems with psychological adjustment) and highlight are considered.
human strengths. As summarized by Seligman and Csik-
szentmihalyi (2000), the field of positive psychology BIOLOGICAL DETERMINANTS OF
concerns subjective experiences (e.g., well-being, opti- PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
mism), positive personal traits (e.g., the capacity for
love, interpersonal skills, forgiveness, wisdom), and In examining the major theoretical and empirical ap-
civic virtues and the institutions that move individuals proaches to understanding the determinants of prosocial
toward better citizenship: responsibility, nurturance, al- behaviors, most efforts have been directed at identifying
truism, civility, moderation, tolerance, and work ethnic the situational, social, and individual factors that affect
(p. 1). Similarly, the positive youth development per- the degree to which prosocial behavior is learned and
spective is a strength-based conception of adolescence enhanced (see M.S. Clark, 1991; Eisenberg, 1986, for
that highlights plasticity in development and the poten- reviews and examples). Relatively little of the empirical
Biological Determinants of Prosocial Behavior 651

work on prosocial behavior has focused on the genetic played among species membersmore altruism would
and neurohormonal substrates of such behavior. The be expected to be directed toward more closely related
lack of empirical work is somewhat surprising given the kin than toward distant kin or those who are unrelated
attention that genetic, evolutionary, and neurohormonal (Hastings, Zahn-Waxler, & McShane, 2005). Thus, for
factors have received in the literature on antisocial, ag- kin selection to be effective (in an evolutionary sense),
gressive, and criminal behavior (see Ellis & Hoffman, altruists must be able to distinguish between individuals
1990). As noted by Eisenberg, Fabes, and Miller (1990), who are their kin and those who are not. Rushton and as-
some of the biological factors that affect antisocial be- sociates (Rushton, Russell, & Well, 1984) proposed that
havior are also likely to account for variations in proso- that there is an innate ability to recognize someone who
cial behavior and therefore warrant consideration in any is genetically similar. Evidence from the study of a wide
major review of prosocial behavior and development. variety of species supports the conclusion that certain
animals may be genetically programmed to identify
Evolutionary Explanations their own kin (Alberts, 1976; Leon, 1983). Evidence for
a similar genetic predisposition in humans is much less
Prosocial actions such as helping and sharing have fre- clear-cut (Fabes & Filsinger, 1988). There is, however,
quently been observed among nonhuman animals (e.g., evidence that humans are more willing to assist others
E. O. Wilson, 1975, 1978). Various social insects (such who are genetically related to themselves (Bar-Tal, Bar-
as certain honeybees, ants, and wasps) frequently sacri- Zohar, Greenberg, & Hermon, 1977) and that the degree
fice their own lives while defending their hives or nests of biological relatedness is positively associated with
from intruders. Similarly, some birds give off a warning willingness to help (Cunningham, 1985/1986). In addi-
call that informs other birds of a predators presence. tion, the more valuable the helpful act is, the more likely
The call, however, occasionally helps predators locate it is to come from kin (Borgida, Conner, & Manteufel,
the call giver, thereby resulting in its capture and death. 1992; Essock-Vitale & McGuire, 1985). People also are
Sharing and cooperation also have been observed likely to seek out and assist others who are similar to
among nonhuman animals (Trivers, 1971; Wilson, themselves (Eisenberg, 1983; Rushton et al., 1984). Be-
1975), as have consoling behaviors and empathy among cause individuals who share proximity and who are
chimpanzees (Preston & de Waal, 2002). Van Lawick- physically similar are likely to share more genes than
Goodall (1968) reported that chimpanzees often hand dissimilar others, the predisposition to help others who
over portions of their catch to other chimpanzees who are similar may enhance the survival of persons likely to
beg for food. Similarly, certain wild African dogs that share genes with the altruist.
live in packs share the prey they catch with members of In many species (including humans), prosocial be-
the pack who stay behind to guard the pups. Common to havior also is extended toward nonrelatives. Hall and
all these examples is that in some way one animal has im- DeVore (1965) described the tendency for baboons to
proved the chances of one or more animals reaching some form alliances and fight as a unit in aggressive encoun-
sort of goal (protection, feeding, care of young, etc.). ters. Female bluebirds occasionally provide foster par-
Explanations have been proposed for the prosocial enting to young birds deserted by their mothers (Hayes,
actions of animals. Wilson (1975, 1978) and others Felton, & Cohen, 1985).
(e.g., Barash, 1977) have advanced the notion of kin se- Trivers (1971, 1983) uses the term reciprocal altru-
lection, which is a broadened view of natural selection. ism to explain instances of prosocial behavior that are
They argue that through self-sacrificing or cooperative directed to recipients so distantly related to the organ-
actions, the prosocial animal increases the probability ism performing the altruistic act that kin selection can
that its relatives, who share its genes, will survive and be ruled out. Trivers argues that under certain condi-
reproduce. Thus, even if the prosocial animal dies, its tions natural selection favors these altruistic behaviors
genes will be passed on to the next generation by its sur- because in the long run they benefit the organism per-
viving relatives. The genes selected for by evolution forming them. Cleaning symbiosis is a case in point.
contribute to their own perpetuation, regardless of the Both host and cleaner benefit from the relationship
individual carrying the animals genes. (e.g., the host is cleaned of parasites and the cleaner is
The percentage of shared or common genes is hypoth- fed and sometimes protected). There also apparently
esized to be an important determinant of altruism dis- has been selection for the host to avoid eating ones
652 Prosocial Development

cleaner (Trivers, 1971). These behaviors cannot be ex- variance in twins altruism, empathy, and nurturance
plained by kin selection because they are performed by (Hastings et al., in press). Most of the remaining vari-
members of one species for the benefit of members of ance was accounted for by idiosyncratic differences in
another species. the environments of the twins rather than by their shared
Another evolutionary explanation of prosocial behav- environment (Rushton, Fulker, Neale, Nias, & Eysenck,
ior is that of group selection (Wynne-Edwards, 1962). 1986; also see Davis, Luce, & Kraus, 1994), although in
According to this view, altruism among group members one study of adults, the variance in prosocial behavior
may benefit the survival of the group. Thus, groups with was linked primarily to shared and nonshared environ-
altruistic members are less likely to become extinct than ment (Krueger, Hicks, & McGue, 2001). It is likely that
groups comprised of nonaltruistic members. This per- the common shared variance decreases with age (Scarr
spective, however, has not received strong support & McCartney, 1983; Scourfield, John, Martin, &
(Boorman & Leavitt, 1980). Group selection works McGuffin, 2004).
very slowly and it would take an exceedingly long time W. Johnson and Krueger (2004) examined the heri-
for an entire group to become extinct. In the short run, tability of middle-aged adults personality traits that
selfish members would have a competitive edge over al- likely relate to prosocial qualities. Using twin data,
truistic members (Krebs & Miller, 1985). Altruistic they found that about 50% of the variance in extraver-
members would die out long before the group does. sion and neuroticism was explained by genetic influ-
Therefore, the forces underlying group selection do not ences; however, this was not the case for agreeableness,
appear compatible with the evolution of a group with openness, and conscientiousness. Agreeableness is be-
altruistic members. lieved to contribute to, or overlap with, prosocial ten-
In summary, evolutionary perspectives on prosocial dencies (Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997). Thus, although
behavior suggest that these behaviors result from evolu- genetics appear to contribute to childrens prosocial
tionary forces (Sober & Wilson, 1998). Prosocial behav- tendencies, genetically informed studies also provide
iors may have been selected because they (a) increase evidence for the role of the environment in the origins of
individuals survival to reproductive age, ( b) increase prosocial behavior.
the reproductive capacity of the individual, and (c) in- Relatively few twin studies involve children, and the
crease either or both of these tendencies in other mem- strength of the heredity estimates has varied somewhat
bers of the species that likely carry the same genes. across studies. In one study of 5- to 16-year-olds, the es-
Inherent in this argument is that evolutionary forces fa- timate for the genetic contribution was about 52% for
voring altruistic behaviors often come into conflict with parental reports, but considerably higher for teachers
those forces that favor behaviors maximizing the sur- reports of prosocial behavior (Scourfield et al., 2004). In
vival of the individual. Out of this complex interplay of another study, Zahn-Waxler and colleagues (Plomin
competing forces comes the potential to act prosocially et al., 1993; Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, & Emde, 1992;
and to account for individual differences in prosocial re- Zahn-Waxler, Schiro, Robinson, Emde, & Schmitz,
sponding (Hofer, 1981). 2001) examined twins behavioral reactions to simula-
tions of distress in others. Estimates of heritability indi-
Heritability of Prosocial Tendencies cated a significant genetic component for empathic
concern, prosocial acts, and maternal reports of proso-
Twin studies have been used to examine the genetic con- cial acts at 14 months of age, albeit the variance ac-
tribution to individual differences in prosocial respond- counted for was much less than 50% for all but maternal
ing. In these studies, if the correlation between scores reports (indicating that environmental factors also con-
on prosocial responding is higher for identical twins tributed to prosocial development). At 20 months, em-
than for fraternal twins, the difference is attributed to pathic concern (sympathy) and prosocial acts continued
genetic effects to the degree that common environmen- to evidence significant genetic contributions. Active in-
tal sources are assumed to be roughly equal for the two difference also showed significant genetic influence at
types of twins. 14 months; however, there was no evidence of heritabil-
In twin studies involving adults self-reports of ity for self-distress at either 14 or 20 months (Zahn-
prosocial tendencies, researchers have found that ge- Waxler et al., 1992). Plomin et al. (1993) found no
netic factors accounted for between 40% and 70% of the evidence of genetic influence on change in a composite
Biological Determinants of Prosocial Behavior 653

index of childrens empathy from 14 to 20 months of sympathetic, and prosocial interpersonal behavior
age, although genetic factors partially accounted for sta- (Mervis & Klein-Tasman, 2000), perhaps even more so
bility over time in empathy. than for normal children or those with some other disor-
In follow-ups in which additional twins were added to ders such as Prader-Willi or fragile X syndrome (Jones
the sample, empathic concern continued to show evi- et al., 2000; see Semel & Rosner, 2003). Thus, the highly
dence of genetic influence at 24 and 36 months, whereas specific and sensitive social profile of individuals with
prosocial acts and indifference did so only at 36 months. Williams syndrome suggests that hemizygous deletion
Mothers reports of childrens prosocial behavior of one or more genes is involved in biasing ( but not de-
showed a genetic influence only at 14 months; it was termining) development toward these components of
predicted by shared environmental variance at older prosocial behavior.
ages (Zahn-Waxler et al., 2001). Moreover, there is evi-
dence that heritable differences may account for tod-
Neurophysiological Underpinnings of
dlers empathy-related responding toward an unfamiliar
Prosocial Responding
adult, whereas shared environmental influences account
for concern toward the mother (Robinson, Zahn-Waxler, Behavioral genetics research provides information re-
& Emde, 2001). The differences in the findings reported garding the presence and size of genetic contributions to
at earlier and older ages may have been due to the prosocial behavior, but does not identify the conditions
smaller sample in the assessments conducted in the 2nd or processes of organism-environment interaction
year of life. Regardless, the magnitude of any genetic in- through which genotypes are transformed into pheno-
fluences on these observed measures of concern gener- types. Research and theory on the neurological
ally were modest. Moreover, Robinson and colleagues processes may provide a mechanism for mediation be-
found there was no significant genetic variance in chil- tween genetics and overt behavior (see Hastings et al.,
drens positive reactions to others distress at 14 2005, for a recent review). Panksepp (1986) suggested
months; it was moderately strong at 20 months and dis- that brain opioids influence the degree to which social
appeared again at 24 months (Robinson, Emde, & contact is reinforcing and that fluctuations in brain opi-
Corley, 2001). Thus, there appears to be considerable oids and the underlying emotive systems affect altruis-
variability in heritability estimates across age and mea- tic behavior. Panksepp also hypothesized that during
sures of prosocial responding. social interactions (which are affected by brain opi-
The role of genetic and environmental influences in oids), animals may become better attuned to the emo-
childrens prosocial tendencies also has been tested in tions of their conspecifics and thereby become better
other types of genetically informed studies. In a study able to alleviate their distress when it occurs.
of stepfamilies, Deater-Deckard, Dunn, et al. (2001) Panksepp asserted that all mammalian helping behav-
found that most of the variance in adults reports of chil- ior arises from the nurturant dictates of brain systems
drens (mostly preschool and school age) prosocial be- that mediate social bonding and maternal care (1986,
havior was due to environmental (not genetic) factors, p. 44). This view is consistent with that of MacLean
especially aspects of the environment that were not (1985), who argued that the basis for altruism lies in
shared by the children, although there was significant maternal behavior, affiliation, and play, which are medi-
variance for shared environmental effects. Moreover, in ated in part by the limbic system of the brain. MacLean
a study involving only identical pre-school-age twins, further suggested that the prefrontal neocortex, which
Deater-Deckard, Pike, et al. (2001) obtained additional developed relatively recently in evolution and is most
evidence of the role of nonshared environment (e.g., ma- distinctive in humans, provides the basis for concern for
ternal supportive and punitive behaviors) in predicting others and a sense of responsibility and conscience.
childrens prosocial behavior. There have been direct attempts to identify the neu-
Other evidence relevant for examining the role of ge- ral roots of prosocial behavior and emotions. It has been
netics in prosocial behavior is found in studies of chil- argued that the perceptual bases of empathy may be
dren with certain genetic abnormalities. Williams mirror neuronsneurons that fire not only when a
syndrome, caused by a microdeletion of part of the long monkey executes an action but also when it observes an-
arm of chromosome 7, is associated with a specific per- other monkey or human performing the same action
sonality profile that includes highly sociable, empathic, (Gallese, 2001). In addition, Decety and Chaminade
654 Prosocial Development

(2003) used positron emission tomography neuroimag- fluence of biology being probabilistic rather than deter-
ing (i.e., PET scanning) to demonstrate that the neural ministic; Wachs, 1994).
structures known to be involved in emotional responding
(e.g., amygdala and the adjacent orbitofrontal cortex
and the insula) were activated when people listened to DEVELOPMENTAL TRENDS IN THE
sad stories designed to elicit sympathy; listening to neu- EMERGENCE OF PROSOCIAL TENDENCIES
tral stories did not cause similar neural activation. In-
creased neural activity was also found in the cortical According to both theory and empirical findings, proso-
regions involved in shared motor representations (e.g., cial behavior and empathy emerge early in life. In this
dorsal premotor cortex, right inferior parietal lobule) section, we first briefly review Hoffmans theory of the
areas of the brain thought to be important when taking development of prosocial behavior, and we then examine
the perspective of others (Ruby & Decety, 2001). age changes in prosocial behavior and empathy-related
Other researchers have highlighted the importance responding.
of frontal cortical activity in sympathetic responses.
Harmon-Jones, Vaughn-Scott, Mohr, Sigelman, and
Theory
Harmon-Jones (2004) found that anger provocation in-
creased left frontal cortical activity and decreased right Hoffman (1982, 2000) proposed a four-level theoretical
activity; however, high levels of sympathetic responses model that delineates the role of infants and childrens
were found to eliminate these effects. Moreover, Es- affect and cognitive sense of self-awareness and self-
linger and colleagues (1998; Eslinger, Eastin, Grattan, other differentiation in the emergence of prosocial be-
& Van Hoesen, 1996) studied individuals with front le- havior. Specifically, he outlined the developmental shift
sions and found that when the lesion was in the dorsolat- over time from self-concern in response to others dis-
eral front system, deficits in cognitive aspects of tress to empathic concern (i.e., sympathy) for others
empathy resulted. In contrast, when lesions were in the that results in other-oriented prosocial behavior.
orbitofrontal system, deficits in the more emotional as- In Hoffmans first stage, newborns and infants dis-
pects of empathy resulted. Such findings suggest that play rudimentary empathic responses that are mani-
complex neural responses likely are involved in proso- fested as global empathy. Hoffman argues that the
cial actions and reactions, a conclusion that is consistent young infant has not acquired a sense of self-other dif-
with Panksepps (1986) assertion that it may be unreal- ferentiation (at least in regard to emotional states) and
istic to assume that functional unitary brain circuits will experiences empathic distress through one or more of
be discovered for global constructs such as altruism, the simpler modes of empathy (e.g., based on reactive
sympathy, and prosocial behavior. crying, conditioning, mimicry). Because young infants
In summary, it is likely that biological factors play cannot differentiate their own distress from that of an-
some role in individual differences in empathy and other, they often experience self-distress in response to
prosocial behavior. However, much of the relevant re- anothers distress, as evidenced in their reactive crying
search on biological mechanisms comes from work with in response to the sound of anothers cry (viewed as a
nonhumans, and existing behavioral genetics work is simple form, or precursor, of global empathy). Begin-
limited in quantity and scope. Moreover, it is unclear ning around the end of the 1st year of life, infants expe-
whether some of the aforementioned biological corre- rience egocentric empathic distress and are thought to
lates of empathy or prosocial behavior play a causal role seek comfort for themselves when exposed to others
in individual differences among people in prosocial ten- distress. At this level, infants have begun developing a
dencies (e.g., they may simply be correlates or conse- sense of self as separate from others; however, this
quences of empathy). Pertinent theory is speculative and sense is quite immature (i.e., they cannot fully differen-
underdeveloped. Finally, there is evidence that the envi- tiate between their own distress and that of another).
ronment plays a critical role in prosocial development, Thus, the infant is likely to respond to empathic and ac-
even in the behavioral genetics research. The key to un- tual distress situations in the same way.
derstanding human prosocial behavior lies in determin- Early in the 2nd year of life, toddlers begin to make
ing how biological factors, prior environmental helpful advances toward a victim of distress (i.e., pat-
influences on the child, and the current context jointly ting, touching). Around the same age, they may inter-
affect prosocial behavior and development (with the in- vene by hugging, giving physical assistance, or getting
Developmental Trends in the Emergence of Prosocial Tendencies 655

someone else to help (Zahn-Waxler & Radke-Yarrow, or class of people, such as the impoverished or the polit-
1982). Hoffman labels this level quasi-egocentric em- ically oppressed. Thus, Hoffman (1982) proposed that
pathic distress. According to Hoffman (2000), toddlers with increasing cognitive maturation, children are bet-
in this developmental period can differentiate between ter able to respond with concern to others distress.
self and other, although they still do not distinguish well
between their own and anothers internal states.
Empirical Studies of the Development of
Nonetheless, toddlers can experience empathic concern
Prosocial Behavior
for another, rather than solely seek comfort for them-
selves. They also can and sometimes do try to comfort In this section, we review empirical studies that provide
another person, but such prosocial behavior is likely to insight into the development of prosocial tendencies. To
involve giving the other person what the toddlers them- organize these, we review them according to the ages of
selves find comforting. Empathy at this level differs the participants in the study.
from the previous stage because toddlers are not purely
egocentric and are more likely to respond with appropri- Infancy and Childhood
ate empathic affect. Compared with research in older children, adolescents,
Stern (1985) has argued that young children develop and adults, research examining prosocial behavior in
a subjective self capable of recognizing the subjectivity young children is relatively limited. Nonetheless, there
of the other earlier than stated by Hoffman (2000). Al- is some empirical support for Hoffmans theory. There
though this issue has not been resolved, the affect at- is evidence that newborn infants exhibit some form of
tunement (a recasting or restatement of a subjective global empathy as displayed by their reactive crying in
state, p. 161) or emotional resonance between parent response to the cries of another infant (Martin & Clark,
and child discussed by Sternalbeit believed to be 1982; Sagi & Hoffman, 1976). Of particular interest, in-
largely out of the childs conscious awarenessmay fos- fants exhibit more distress in response to another in-
ter the early development of affective empathy, espe- fants crying than to their own (Dondi, Simion, &
cially if parents are empathic in their interactions. Caltran, 1999), suggesting that they are biologically pre-
Sometime during the 2nd year of life, children enter disposed to experience a rudimentary form of empathy.
the period of veridical empathic distress. According to However, some researchers have questioned the inter-
Hoffman (1982, 2000), this stage marks the period in pretation of these findings (Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983);
which children are increasingly aware of other peoples for example, infants may simply find a novel cry to be
feelings and are capable of understanding that other more aversive than their own cry.
peoples perspectives and feelings may differ from their Around 6 months of age, infants will sometimes re-
own. Thus, prosocial actions reflect an awareness of the spond to the cry of another infant by crying, but they fre-
other persons needs (versus the egocentric empathy of quently ignore it or merely orient toward the peer (Hay,
the previous stage), and children can be more accurate Nash, & Pederson, 1981). By 38 to 61 weeks of age, in-
in their empathic responses and help others in less ego- fants sometimes react to others distress by orienting
centric ways. Moreover, with the development of lan- and distress cries, but they occasionally display positive
guage, children are able to empathize and sympathize affect, such as smiling or laughing (Zahn-Waxler &
with a wider range of emotions than they previously Radke-Yarrow, 1982).
could. However, according to Hoffman, childrens em- Thus, it appears that infants are responsive to others
pathic responses are restricted to anothers immediate, emotional signals. In a study in which mothers ex-
or situation-specific, distress. pressed sadness or joy in view of their 9-month-old in-
As children develop more sophisticated perspective- fants, the infants displayed more negative emotional
taking skills and the ability to think abstractly, the abil- expressions and tended to avert their gaze away from
ity to experience empathic responses even when the their mothers in the sadness condition and expressed
other person is not physically present (e.g., if they hear more joy when they viewed their mothers expressions
or read about someone in distress) emerges (Hoffman, of joy (Termine & Izard, 1988). Moreover, studies of so-
1982). Moreover, by mid to late childhood, children can cial referencing show that infants not only are respon-
empathize with another persons general condition or sive to others emotional signals, but also make use of
plight. Further, the adolescent is capable of compre- them to guide their own behavior in an ambiguous situa-
hending and responding to the plight of an entire group tion (see Saarni, Mumme, & Campos, 1998; Saarni
656 Prosocial Development

et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). During attempts to label or understand why the other is dis-
the 2nd year of life, toddlers display the ability to dis- tressed] or social referencing) in the 2nd year of life and
cuss their own and others emotions and show signifi- at age 4 to 5 years has been positively related to their
cant improvements in this skill between 18 and 36 prosocial behaviors (Kiang, Moreno, & Robinson, 2004;
months of age (Bretherton, Fritz, Zahn-Waxler, & Zahn-Waxler, Cole, Welsh, & Fox, 1995; Zahn-Waxler,
Ridgeway, 1986). These findings demonstrate that very Robinson, et al., 1992). Similarly, preschool childrens
young children are affected by the emotions they ob- emotion knowledge has been positively related to proso-
serve in others. cial behavior toward adults who express negative emo-
Around 12 to 18 months of age, infants clearly react tion (Denham & Couchoud, 1991) and toward younger
to others negative emotions (often with orienting and siblings (Garner, Jones, & Palmer, 1994).
distress reactions) and sometimes react to others dis- Other types of prosocial behavior besides sympa-
tress with concerned attention and prosocial behavior, thetic or comforting responses to others distress have
including positive contact and verbal reassurance been examined in young children. The tendency to give
(Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, et al., 1992). These patterns objects to other people is common in early childhood,
have been found in interactions with mothers (Zahn- and young children have been observed sharing objects
Waxler, Robinson, et al., 1992), siblings (Dunn, 1988), with parents, other adults, siblings, and peers (Hay,
peers (Denham, 1986; Howes & Farver, 1987), and 1994). Object sharing seems to emerge around 8
strangers (Johnson, 1982). months of age and is increasingly evident during the
In one of the earliest studies of childrens sympathy next year (Hay & Rheingold, 1983). In general, proso-
and prosocial behavior, Lois Murphy (1937) found that cial behavior has been found to increase in the early
preschool children reacted to anothers distress in a va- years of life (Zahn-Waxler & Radke-Yarrow, 1982;
riety of ways. Childrens responses ranged from sympa- Zahn-Waxler, Schiro, et al., 1992, 2001). For exam-
thy and prosocial initiations to egocentric and ple, Zahn-Waxler, Schiro, et al. (1992, 2001) and
unsympathetic reactions, such as laughing, aggression, Robinson et al. (2001) studied toddlers empathy-re-
or ignoring. These findings have been replicated in other lated responding to an experimenter and the mother
samples with young children (Radke-Yarrow & Zahn- feigning injuries at 14, 20, 24, and 36 months of age.
Waxler, 1984; Zahn-Waxler & Radke-Yarrow, 1982; They found an increase with age in empathic concern,
Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, & Chapman, hypotheses testing, and prosocial behavior. Van der
1992). Moreover, researchers have found that respon- Mark, van IJzendoorn, and Bakermans-Kranenburg
siveness to peers distress in naturalistic settings is rela- (2002) also found an increase in empathy/prosocial re-
tively infrequent among toddlers and preschoolers sponding (combined) from 16 to 22 months when tod-
(Caplan & Hay, 1989; Howes & Farver, 1987; Phinney, dlers mothers were distressed. Further, Lamb and
Feshbach, & Farver, 1986). In a naturalistic study ex- Zakhireh (1997) found that age was positively related
amining toddlers responses to peers distress in day to toddlers prosocial behavior toward peers.
care, Lamb and Zakhireh (1997) found that toddlers re- Moreover, nonempathic responses (e.g., self-oriented
sponded to a peers distress with prosocial behavior in distress reactions) seem to decrease in the second and
only 11 out of 345 incidents. Factors that appear to re- 3rd year of life (Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, et al.,
late to childrens prosocial responding include whether a 1992; Zahn-Waxler et al., 2001). Toddlers indifference
peers distress persists for a long period or if the partic- toward anothers distress has been found to decline from
ular peer is one who infrequently becomes distressed 14 to 20 months of age and then increase between 24 and
(Caplan & Hay, 1989). 36 months (Zahn-Waxler et al., 2001). Nonetheless, with
As proposed by Hoffman (1982, 2000), prosocial be- increasing age, preschoolers are more likely to respond
haviors have been associated with indices of cognitive to others distress with empathy and prosocial behaviors
development. Toddlers who display evidence of self- (Hastings, Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, Usher, & Bridges,
recognition (indicating a self-other distinction) tend to 2000; Lennon & Eisenberg, 1987; Phinney et al., 1986).
be relatively empathic and are likely to display prosocial Although many empirical studies have demonstrated
behaviors (Bischof-Koehler, 1991; Johnson, 1982; Zahn- the hypothesized increase in prosocial behavior over
Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, et al., 1992; Zahn-Waxler, time, Hay (1994; Hay, Caplan, Castle, & Stimson, 1991)
Schiro, et al., 2001). Further, childrens perspective proposed a developmental model which predicted that
taking (as indexed by their hypothesis testing [e.g., prosocial action would emerge in the 2nd year of life and
Developmental Trends in the Emergence of Prosocial Tendencies 657

decline after that. She argued that after the age of 2, taro (2002) examined the continuity of trajectories for
prosocial action becomes more regulated such that it is helpfulness across early elementary school (measured
shown to some but not to all potential recipients (e.g., annually from age 6 to age 12 years). Generally, children
prosocial actions become increasingly differentiated who entered kindergarten with specific levels of help-
based on gender and personality). In one study with fulness finished primary school at similar levels. The
girls and boys in three age cohorts (18 to 24 months, 24 observed degree of stability in these trajectories was
to 30 months, and 30 to 36 months), Hay found the hy- impressive considering that ratings of helpfulness were
pothesized decline in sharing with peers between 18 and provided by independent raters (i.e., different teachers
24 months of age; however, the trend was not reliable at different years).
thereafter. In addition, the tendency to share was more
stable with older toddlers (24- to 36-month-olds) than Adolescence
with younger toddlers (18- to 24-month-olds; Hay, Cas- Age Trends. Prosocial tendencies appear to in-
tle, Davies, Demetriou, & Stimson, 1999). The fact that crease from childhood into adolescence. According to
Hay and colleagues studied sharing with only familiar Eisenberg and Fabess (1998) meta-analysis, adolescents
peersand in fact best friendsmay have con- tend to be higher in prosocial behavior than children
tributed to the pattern observed; most studies have not aged 7 to 12 years, albeit on sharing/donating, but not
involved this type of sharing context. Additionally, the instrumental helping or comforting. Both young adoles-
meaning of prosocial behavior may differ across child- cents (13 to 15 years) and older adolescents (16 to 18
hood. At young ages, children may exchange toys as part years) were higher than elementary school students in
of simple play or to communicate with their friend about their prosocial tendencies (Fabes, Carlo, Kupanoff, &
the objects they are using (e.g., to show the peer some- Laible, 1999). Thus, adolescents exhibit more prosocial
thing about a toy or to interest the peer in it). behavior than do younger children; however, this pattern
To bring coherence to the many studies of age-related was noted only for particular types of studies. Although
change in prosocial behavior, Eisenberg and Fabes there was not an overall increase in prosocial responding
(1998) conducted a meta-analysis of relevant studies. across adolescence (from age 12 to 17 or 18), prosocial
Overall, there were significant increases in prosocial be- behavior increased in adolescence for the few studies of
havior within both the infant ( less than 3 years of age) sharing/donating ( but not helping), and in experimen-
and the preschool (3 to 6 years) age groups (effect sizes tal /structured studies ( but not naturalistic/correlational
= .24 and .33). In addition, there were increases in studies; see Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; also see Jacobs,
prosocial behavior when comparing the preschool group Vernon, & Eccles, 2004). Moreover, helping of victims
with either the childhood or adolescent age groups. of aggression may actually decline across adoles-
However, there was no difference between the infancy cence (Lindeman, Harakka, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen,
and preschool periods, perhaps due to the relatively 1997; also see Pakaslahti, Karjalainen, & Keltikangas-
small number of studies that compared these age groups Jarvinen, 2002).
(n = 11). In addition, school-age children were higher in In the meta-analysis, prosocial behavior directed
prosocial behavior than preschoolers (effect size = .30). toward adults did not change with age in adolescence.
In the meta-analysis, prosocial behavior generally This finding may primarily reflect findings in the
increased across the preschool and school years (also family setting. Investigators have found nonlinear age-
see Benenson, Markovits, Roy, & Denko, 2003). How- related changes or no consistent change in adolescents
ever, some of the findings were based on relatively and parents reports of adolescents parent-directed
small samples, particularly for comparisons of the prosocial behaviors (e.g., Eberly & Montemayor, 1998,
youngest children in these samples. We also recognize 1999; also see Keith, Nelson, Schlabach, & Thompson,
that the findings of our meta-analysis were based 1990), as well as a decline in helpfulness toward
largely on cross-sectional data and on aggregations of parents between fifth and ninth grades (Eberly, Mon-
data from studies that varied greatly in their quality temayor, & Flannery, 1993).
and methodologies. Based on Hoffmans theory, one would expect an
Despite possible age-related changes in childrens age-related increase in empathy-related responding dur-
prosocial behavior, there appears to be considerable in- ing adolescence, especially in situations in which empa-
terindividual stability in childrens levels of prosocial thy or sympathy is directed toward abstract groups (e.g.,
responding. Ct, Tremblay, Nagin, Zoccolillo, and Vi- deprived groups). In studies conducted before about
658 Prosocial Development

1986, findings regarding age trends in empathy-related Allen, 1996; Yates & Youniss, 1996a). In general, re-
responding in adolescence were inconsistent, although searchers have found volunteering is associated with in-
there was some evidence of an increase from childhood creases in adolescents self-esteem and self-acceptance,
into adolescence (e.g., Saklofske & Eysenck, 1983; see moral development, and belief in ones personal respon-
Lennon & Eisenberg, 1987). Since 1987, there has been sibility to help (Conrad & Hedin, 1982; see Switzer,
additional longitudinal evidence of an increase in empa- Simmons, Dew, Regalski, & Wang, 1995, for similar re-
thy-related responding from 9th to 10th grade, espe- sults for a required helping program), as well as concern
cially for sympathetic concern, and of a decline in for social issues and future intended service (Metz,
personal distress (Davis & Franzoi, 1991). In a cross- McLellan, & Youniss, 2003).
sectional study, Strayer and Roberts (1997b) also found In a panel design of youth volunteers and nonvolun-
that both reported empathic sadness and facial con- teers in which the initial levels of variables correlated
cerned reactions to evocative videotapes (perhaps in- with volunteering were controlled, volunteering was re-
dicative of sympathy) increased with age from lated to gains in subsequent intrinsic work values and
childhood into adolescence (although there was no age the anticipated importance of community involvement
difference in affective matching of the emotion in the (Johnson, Beebe, Mortimer, & Snyder, 1998). There is
film). However, some investigators who have conducted also evidence that service participation (voluntary or
cross-sectional studies have obtained mixed evidence of not) is related to decreases in course failure, truancy,
sympathy increasing between 6th and 12th grade (Ol- suspension from school, school dropout, disciplinary
weus & Endresen, 1998) or have found little change in problems, and pregnancies, as well as with improved
sympathy or personal distress from 8th to 11th grades reading skills (see Allen, Kuperminc, Philliber, &
(Karniol, Gabay, Ochion, & Harari, 1998). Finally, a Herre, 1994; Allen et al., 1997; Calabrese & Schumer,
longitudinal study (Eisenberg, Cumberland, Guthrie, 1986; Moore & Allen, 1996; Switzer et al., 1995; also
Murphy, & Shepard, 2005) did not find change in sym- see Eccles & Barber, 1999). Finally, in a prospective
pathy from age 15 to 16 into the 20s, although personal longitudinal study, volunteer work negatively predicted
distress declined with age. Thus, there may be a modest subsequent arrests, even when controlling for the effects
increase in sympathy with age, especially in early to of antisocial propensities, prosocial attitudes and behav-
mid-adolescence, although it is not clear that sympathy ior, and commitment to conventional lines of action
increases in mid- and late-adolescence. (Uggen & Janikula, 1999). Quality of the program (e.g.,
allowing adolescents autonomy and choice, being chal-
The Potential Effects of Adolescents Participa- lenging and enjoyable), length of the program (programs
tion in Prosocial Service. A type of prosocial behav- 12 weeks or more tend to be more successful than
ior that appears to be much more common in shorter programs), and age of adolescents (in some pro-
adolescence than at younger ages is volunteering. Ap- grams, older youth benefited more) all appear to affect
proximately half of all adolescents engage in some type potential benefits of volunteering (Moore & Allen,
of community service or volunteer activity (National 1996). Thus, participation in service activitiesa com-
Center for Education Statistics, 1997). Volunteerism is mon adolescent activityis related to both prosocial
an interesting type of prosocial behavior because it gen- and other developmental outcomes.
erally is sustained over some period of time (rather than
performed only once) and is expected to have some en-
Moderators of Age Trends across Childhood
during effect on youths prosocial, civic, and personal
and Adolescence
development.
Although motives for volunteering vary and are Viewed more generally, the extant literature appears to
sometimes self-related rather than altruistic (Clary & support the conclusion that as children get older, they
Snyder, 1999), investigators have found that high school exhibit more sympathy and prosocial behavior. This
students who volunteer appear to benefit from the expe- trend does not hold, however, for children of all ages or
rience. Of these studies, few have used random assign- for all measures of prosocial behavior (see Radke-
ment (for an exception, see Allen, Philliber, Herrling, & Yarrow, Zahn-Waxler, & Chapman, 1983; Zarbatany,
Kuperminc, 1997), although most have included a com- Hartmann, & Gelfand, 1985). In fact, in the previously
parison control group or a pre/post design (see Moore & mentioned Eisenberg and Fabes (1998) meta-analysis,
Developmental Trends in the Emergence of Prosocial Tendencies 659

age differences in prosocial behavior sometimes varied years of age. Moreover, naturalistic/correlational de-
as a function of study characteristics. These differences signs were relatively likely to be used with very young
did not vary as a function of type of prosocial behavior children, whereas experimental /structured designs were
for studies conducted with young children; moreover, more often used with older children. Additionally,
across the remaining age group comparisons (involving adults were likely to be used as targets of childrens
older children), the magnitude of age differences was prosocial behavior in studies with the youngest and old-
relatively constant in size when the type of prosocial be- est age groups, whereas children were likely to be the
havior was sharing, comforting, or an aggregated index. potential recipients of prosocial behavior for children
In contrast, the magnitude of the age-related effect size not at the age extremes. Thus, age-related differences in
for instrumental helping varied more across the older prosocial behavior may have varied as a function of dif-
age group comparisons. The magnitude of this effect ferences in study characteristics that differed across
size was relatively high when the type of prosocial be- age groups.
havior was instrumental help for the childhood versus To explore this possibility, we examined age differ-
preschool and within-childhood comparisons and rela- ences in prosocial behavior while controlling for study
tively low for the adolescent versus childhood and the characteristics (through hierarchical regression analy-
within-adolescence comparisons. ses). Age differences in prosocial behavior were found
The magnitude of the effect sizes differed signifi- to be smaller as the mean age of the sample increased, as
cantly by the method of data collection (e.g., obser- the sample size increased, and in studies published more
vation, self-report, other-report) only for childhood/ recently. Moreover, although type of prosocial behavior
preschool, childhood/childhood, and adolescent /child- was related to effect sizes for age prior to controlling for
hood comparison groups. For both the childhood/ study characteristics, effect sizes were not affected by
preschool and childhood/childhood age comparison type of prosocial measure (instrumental help, sharing/
groups, effect sizes for age differences were signifi- donating, aggregated, comforting) after partialling out
cantly higher when prosocial tendencies were mea- other study characteristics. However, after controlling
sured with observations or self-reports than when for study characteristics (and not before), a larger in-
assessed with reports obtained from other people. For crease in prosocial behavior with age was found when
the adolescent versus childhood comparisons, effect prosocial behavior was measured with self- or other-re-
sizes were significantly higher when measured with ports rather than with observations.
observational or other report methods rather than with In brief, the findings of our meta-analysis suggested
self-report methods. that age differences in prosocial behavior differed in
For all age-comparison groups, effect sizes were magnitude as a function of the specific age comparison,
greater in experimental /structured designs than in natu- the measure of prosocial behavior, and the type of analy-
ralistic/correlational designs (although the difference sis. However, combining across all studies and study
was not significant for infant /infant and preschool / characteristics, we still found a significant, positive ef-
preschool comparisons). Finally, the magnitude of the fect size for age differences in prosocial behavior. Thus,
effect size differed significantly by the target of the our data support the conclusion that as children get
prosocial behavior, but this was true only for child- older, prosocial behaviors generally are more likely to
hood/preschool, childhood/childhood, and adolescent / occur, although there may be variation within age
adolescent comparison groups. In the first two age com- groups and for various measures and methods.
parison groups, effect sizes were larger when the target
was an adult and lowest when the target was unknown /
Processes Potentially Related to Changes with
unspecified (with child targets in between). In contrast,
Age in Prosocial Responding
for the adolescent /adolescent comparison, the effect
size was greater when the target was a child compared For some theorists, the primary source of the increase in
with an adult. prosocial and altruistic behavior across age is sociocog-
There also were differences in the procedures used nitive development, including understanding and decod-
to measure prosocial behavior in different age groups. ing others emotions, evaluative processes (evaluating
Instrumental help was relatively unlikely to be used as a behaviors and situations in terms of moral standards),
measure of prosocial behavior with children under 7 and planning processes (Krebs & Van Hesteren, 1994).
660 Prosocial Development

Aspects of socioemotional responding (e.g., moral emo- leagues sought to delineate a developmental sequence in
tions, regulatory capacities) also partially account for prosocial motivation, the data concerning this issue are
age-related changes in prosocial behavior (Hart, Burock, inconclusive (i.e., it is unclear whether all their pro-
London, & Atkins, 2003). posed stages actually emerge in the specified order; see
Eisenberg, 1986). Nonetheless, childrens reported mo-
Sociocognitive Processes tives for their prosocial behavior change in ways that
As noted by Krebs and Van Hesteren (1994) and Hoff- generally are consistent with Bar-Tals stages. Although
man (1982), attention to the needs of others transforms even preschoolers sometimes give simple other-oriented
egoistic affect to other-oriented affect, rendering it in- and pragmatic reasons for their peer-directed prosocial
creasingly altruistic. Throughout infancy and childhood, actions (Eisenberg, Lundy, Shell, & Roth, 1985; Eisen-
children develop an increasingly refined understanding berg, Pasternack, Cameron, & Tryon, 1984), re-
of others emotional states and cognitive processes, and searchers generally have found a decrease with age in
are better able to decode other peoples emotional cues self-oriented, hedonistic reasons for helping and an in-
(see Eisenberg, Murphy, & Shepard, 1997, for a review). crease in other-oriented, internalized, and altruistic
As is discussed later, such perspective taking and re- motives and reasons for prosocial behavior (e.g., Bar-
lated sociocognitive skills are associated with prosocial Tal, Raviv, et al., 1980; see Bar-Tal, 1982; Eisenberg,
responding. Moreover, with age, children are more 1986; cf. Hertz-Lazarowitz, 1983).1 Thus, in general,
likely to have the social experience necessary to per- the evidence of developmental change in childrens mo-
ceive anothers need in social contexts in which overt tives for assisting others is relatively compelling (see
cues of distress are ambiguous or subtle (see Pearl, Eisenberg, 1986).
1985), and to distinguish real versus apparent emotional Like Bar-Tal (1982), Krebs and Van Hesteren (1994)
states (Gosselin, Warren, & Diotte, 2002). In addition, proposed age-related forms of altruism, ranging from
younger children appear to weigh costs to the self more egocentric and exchange stages (e.g., egocentric accom-
than do older children when deciding whether to assist modation and instrumental cooperation, Stages 1 and 2,
others (see Eisenberg, 1986) and are less attuned to the respectively), to concern with others evaluation and be-
benefits of prosocial behavior (Lourenco, 1993; Perry, having in a socially acceptable manner (Stage 3), to al-
Perry, & Weiss, 1986). These age-related differences in truism motivated by the desire to fulfill an internalized
the analysis of costs and benefits likely contribute to sense of social responsibility (e.g., conscientious altru-
age-related differences in prosocial behavior. ism, Stage 4). The higher level adult stages are moti-
Moreover, numerous researchers have suggested that vated by the desire to uphold self-chosen, internalized
the quality of childrens motivation for assisting others utilitarian values (e.g., maximizing benefits to all; au-
changes with age (e.g., Eisenberg, 1986; Erdley & Asher, tonomous altruism, Stage 5), the goal of fostering maxi-
1999; Krebs & Van Hesteren, 1994). Bar-Tal, Raviv, and mally balanced and integrated social relationships (e.g.,
Leiser (1980) proposed that childrens helping behavior upholding the rights of all people, including the self; in-
develops through six stages that differ in quality of moti- tegrated altruism, Stage 6), and the goal of universal
vation. The first three stages involve helping behaviors love stemming from a cosmic feeling of oneness with the
that are compliant and in which the child anticipates the universe and a selfless ethic of responsible love, service,
gain of material rewards (or the avoidance of punish- and sacrifice that is extended to others without regard
ment). The next two stages involve compliance with so- for merit (universal self-sacrificial love, Stage 7). Of
cial demands and concern with social approval and course, children or adolescents would not be expected to
generalized reciprocity. The final stage represents true obtain the higher level stages. Although Krebs and his
altruism in which helping is an end in itself. colleagues have not explicitly tested the validity of their
Bar-Tal and colleagues found some support for their stages, their position is supported in the data collected
hypothesized developmental changes in childrens mo-
tives for helping. For example, older children tend to as- 1
Here and throughout the chapter, the abbreviation cf.,
sist more often than do younger children in contexts in meaning compare with, signifies contrast with. It indi-
which the effects of compliance and rewards or costs cates that contrary findings were obtained in a study. Also
are minimized (Bar-Tal, Raviv, et al., 1980; see Bar- see generally indicates that the results in the listed studies
Tal, 1982; Eisenberg, 1986). Although Bar-Tal and col- are also relevant to discussion of the issue at hand.
Developmental Trends in the Emergence of Prosocial Tendencies 661

by other investigators concerned with the development nitive skills required for high-level moral reasoning (see
of moral reasoning, prosocial behavior, and empathy. Eisenberg, 1986).
Age-related changes in childrens evaluative Sociocognitive processes may underlie the develop-
processes and prosocial-relevant goals are reflected in ment of childrens prosocial behaviors, but engaging in
childrens prosocial moral reasoning (i.e., reasoning these processes does not ensure the enacting of proso-
about moral dilemmas in which one persons needs or cial actions. Eisenberg and Fabes (1992) suggested that
wants conflict with those of others in a context where individuals who are well regulated are relatively likely
authorities, laws, rules, punishment, and formal obliga- to engage in costly, other-oriented prosocial behavior.
tions play a minimal role). In research on prosocial Because regulatory capacities likely increase with age
moral reasoning, individuals typically are presented (Eisenberg, Smith, Sadovsky, & Spinrad, 2004), we
with hypothetical moral conflicts (e.g., about helping an would expect older children, relative to younger ones, to
injured child rather than going to a social event), and be more likely to respond sympathetically and with
their reasoning about the conflicts is elicited. prosocial behavior in emotionally evocative situations.
Based on both cross-sectional and longitudinal re- Support for the hypothesized relations between chil-
search, Eisenberg and her colleagues have identified an drens prosocial tendencies and their behavioral and
age-related sequence of childrens prosocial reasoning. emotional regulation is discussed later.
Preschool and early elementary school students tend to
use primarily hedonistic reasoning or needs-oriented Age Changes in Empathy-Related Responding
(primitive empathic) prosocial reasoning. Hedonistic Developmental change in both childrens emotion regu-
reasoning decreases sharply in elementary school and lation and in their sociocognitive skills (e.g., Hoffman,
increases slightly in adolescence. Needs-oriented rea- 1982, 2000) would be expected to contribute to age-re-
soning increases until mid-childhood and then levels off lated changes in prosocial behavior, in part by influenc-
in use. In elementary school, childrens reasoning begins ing childrens tendencies to respond empathically or
to reflect concern with others approval and enhancing sympathetically. Lennon and Eisenberg (1987), in a re-
interpersonal relationships, as well as the desire to be- view of the literature, found that age differences in em-
have in stereotypically good ways. However, such rea- pathy varied with the specific index of empathy used. In
soning (particularly approval-oriented reasoning) general, self-report of empathy/sympathy was positively
appears to decline somewhat in high school. associated with age in preschool and elementary school
Beginning in late elementary school or thereafter, years. Facial /gestural indices appeared to be either in-
children begin to express reasoning reflecting abstract versely related or unrelated to age in the early school
principles, internalized affective reactions (e.g., guilt or years, perhaps due to increases with age in childrens
positive affect about the consequences of ones behavior ability to mask their emotions. As discussed, more re-
for others or living up to internalized principles and val- cent studies show some evidence (albeit mixed) for in-
ues), and self-reflective sympathy and perspective tak- creased empathy-related responding in adolescence.
ing. Thus, although children and adolescents sometimes Eisenberg and Fabes (1998) conducted a separate
verbalize immature modes of reasoning, childrens meta-analysis of age differences in empathy (rather than
moral reasoning becomes more abstract, somewhat less prosocial behavior) in studies published since 1983 and
self-oriented, and increasingly based on values, moral found an overall unweighted effect size of .24 (favoring
principles, and moral emotions with age (Carlo, Eisen- older children). Moreover, they found that effect sizes in
berg, & Knight, 1992; Carlo, Koller, Eisenberg, De- empathy varied significantly by method; they were sig-
Silva, & Frohlich, 1996; Eisenberg, Carlo, Murphy, & nificant and larger for observational and self-report in-
Van Court, 1995; Eisenberg-Berg, 1979; also see Hart dices than for nonverbal (facial /physiological) or
et al., 2003; Helwig & Turiel, 2003). As discussed later, other-report measures (for which the effect sizes were
these age-related changes are linked to prosocial behav- not significant).
ior; thus, the processes reflected in childrens moral Vitaglione and Barnett (2003) found evidence that
reasoning likely play some role in the age-related in- empathic anger on behalf of a victimized person moti-
crease in quantity and quality of prosocial behavior. vates desires to help. As children develop the ability to
However, these processes may include age-related empathize with others, empathic anger may increasingly
changes in goals and values, as well as in the sociocog- motivate prosocial behavior.
662 Prosocial Development

Changes in Experience-Based Competence der, 1996; Williams, 1991), and the Papago tribe in Ari-
Developmental changes in childrens experience-based zona (Rohner, 1975; see Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989). In
competencies also affect their ability to engage in proso- contrast, other social and behavioral scientists have de-
cial behavior. Peterson (1983) found that when children scribed cultures in which prosocial behaviors were rare
were specially trained on relevant tasks, age-related in- and cruelty or hostility was the norm, such as the Ik of
creases in helping evaporated. The data in our meta- Uganda (Turnbull, 1972) or the Alorese (on an island
analysis (see Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998) also suggested east of Java; Rohner, 1975). Moreover, societal experi-
that experience-based developmental competencies may ments such as the communally oriented kibbutzim in Is-
contribute to age-related differences in prosocial behav- rael (see Nadler, Romek, & Shapira-Friedman, 1979)
ior. For example, age differences in prosocial behavior support the view that subcultural variations can have a
were relatively pronounced when the index of prosocial substantial impact on prosocial values and behavior.
behavior was instrumental helping. Older children may The perceived practical value of prosocial behavior
provide more direct, instrumental assistance because varies across cultures; such differences may affect even
they possess greater physical and social competence early socialization. It has been reported that in some
than do younger children. cultures such as in West Africa, prosocial behavior is
encouraged as early as infancy (e.g., infants are offered
Summary objects and then encouraged to return the gifts) to foster
sharing and exchange norms believed to bind the social
Developmental changes in prosocial behavior are complex group together (Nsamenang, 1992).
and are influenced by methodological factors. Moreover, In many cases, reports of cultural differences in
the precise developmental mechanisms involved in pro- prosocial responding are based on single-culture studies
ducing these changes are not yet fully explicated and and qualitative data (or mere observation /inference).
likely involve cognitive, social, motivational /emotional, Empirical studies of prosocial behaviors and values
and physical processes and capabilities. The next wave of sometimes include only one culture, sometimes more.
research should include studies devoted to identifying Although the results of the empirical research generally
when and how age-related changes in the sociocognitive, are consistent with qualitative cultural studies in high-
emotional, and regulatory capabilities jointly affect pro- lighting the importance of culture in prosocial develop-
social responding. ment, little is known about cross-cultural differences in
actual (rather than reported) prosocial actions directed
toward those who are not part of the childs family or
CULTURAL DETERMINANTS OF community. Nor is it clear what factors mediate or mod-
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR erate the cultural factors that have been found.

Research on the cultural bases of prosocial responding


Laboratory or Adult- and Self-Report Studies
provides insights into the role of the social environ-
mentin contrast to strictly biological factorsin Much of the work on cross-cultural and subcultural
prosocial development. People in different cultures may variation in prosocial behavior is embedded in the re-
differ somewhat genetically from one another, but these search on cooperation, competition, and reward-alloca-
differences are unlikely to fully account for any large tion behavior. In many studies, the measure of
cultural differences found in human social behavior. cooperation involved overt self-gain; this work is not re-
Research in non-Western cultures suggests that soci- viewed. However, researchers consistently have found
eties vary greatly in the degree to which prosocial and that children from traditional rural and semi-agricul-
cooperative behavior are normative, and such differ- tural communities and from relatively traditional sub-
ences appear to affect prosocial development. In field cultures (e.g., Mexican American children) are more
studies of individual cultures, some writers have de- cooperative than children from urban or Westernized
scribed societies in which prosocial and communal val- cultures (see Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989).
ues and behaviors are (or were in the past) highly valued In other studies, children were asked to make a series
and common, such as the Aitutaki (a Polynesian island of choices concerning the distribution of objects (i.e.,
people; Graves & Graves, 1983), the Javanese (e.g., Mul- chips) to the self and a peer when giving the peer more
Cultural Determinants of Prosocial Behavior 663

chips did not change the childs own yield. Brazilian U.S. first graders shared candy more than did Colom-
children (Carlo, Roesch, Knight, & Koller, 2001) and bian children of the same age, although some ( but not
Mexican American children generally give more to the all) of this sharing was passive (i.e., they allowed a peer
peer than do Euro-American children (Kagan & Knight, to take the candies; Pilgrim & Rueda-Riedle, 2002).
1981; Knight, Nelson, Kagan, & Gumbiner, 1982), and More consistent cross-group differences might be
the difference for Mexican Americans increases in mag- found when comparing Eastern and Western cultures.
nitude from age 5 to 6 years to age 8 to 9 years (Knight Although Trommsdorff (1995) did not find a difference
& Kagan, 1977b). Sometimes, however, there have been in German and Japanese 5-year-olds prosocial behavior
no significant differences between Mexican or Mexican with a distressed peer, Stewart and McBride-Chang
American children and Euro-American children in the (2000) found that Asian second graders (from a range of
selection of options in which the peer could receive ethnic groups) were more likely than Western Caucasian
more chips than the child (e.g., Kagan & Knight, 1981; children in Hong Kong to donate gifts for participating
Knight, Nelson, Kagan, & Gumbiner, 1982). The ten- in the study to other children in the classroom who could
dency to choose more for the peer than for the self is not participate. Similarly, Rao and Stewart (1999) found
stronger in second- than in third-generation Mexican that Asian (Chinese Hong Kong and Indian) kindergart-
American children (Knight & Kagan, 1977a), suggest- ners shared more food with a peer than had been found
ing that acculturation is associated with a decline in in a sample in the United States, and Asian children
prosocial tendencies. Consistent with the latter finding, were more likely to do so spontaneously and to allow the
de Guzman and Carlo (2004) found that acculturation peer to take some food. Thus, in initial small studies, it
was negatively related to Hispanic adolescents self- appears that Asian children are more likely to engage in
reported prosocial behavior. prosocial behavior than are Western Caucasian children.
In another variation on allocation tasks, some of the This finding may be due to the greater focus on main-
choices allow children to give more to the peer at a cost taining good relationships with group members (and
to the self. Mexican American or Mexican children still on the interrelatedness of self and other) in at least
tend to give more prize chips overall to a peer than do some Asian cultures, compared with Western cultures
Euro-American children (e.g., Knight, Kagan, & Buriel, (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
1981). Mexican American children with a stronger eth-
nic identity have been found to display more concern
Naturalistic Observational Research
with others outcomes on this type of task (Knight,
Cota, & Bernal, 1993). On a similar task, Cook Island Systematic observation of prosocial behavior in differ-
Polynesian children were more generous than were New ent cultures is rare. In the classic study by Whiting and
Zealand city and rural children of European origin Whiting (1975), prosocial behavior was operationalized
(Graves & Graves, 1983). as a composite index of offering helping (including
In other studies, cross-national or cross-cultural dif- food, toys, and helpful information), offering support,
ferences in sharing or helping have been examined. Few and making helpful suggestions. Cultures in which
consistent differences have been found among Western, children scored relatively high on prosocial behavior
industrialized countries such as Germany, Russia, Aus- (Kenya, Mexico, Philippines) tended to differ from the
tralia, and the United States (e.g., Kienbaum & Tromms- other three cultures (Okinawa, India, and the United
dorff, 1999; Russell, Hart, Robinson, & Olsen, 2003), States) on several dimensions. In prosocial cultures,
although young Italian adolescents report more prosocial people tended to live together in extended families, the
behavior than Hungarian youth, who report more than female role was important (with women making major
Czech youth (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, Cermak, contributions to the economic status of the family),
& Rosza, 2001). In studies within North and South work was less specialized, and the government was less
America, Mexican rural children and Euro-American centralized. Further, childrens prosocial behavior was
city children were equally likely to help a peer in a non- associated with early assignment of chores and taking
competitive context (Kagan & Madsen, 1972) and Mexi- on responsibility for welfare of family members and the
can American and Euro-American children did not familys economic well-being (also see Whiting & Ed-
differ in anonymous sharing of candy with an unspeci- wards, 1988). Similar to Whitings data on chores and
fied classmate (Hansen & Bryant, 1980). In contrast, family structure, Graves and Graves (1983) found that
664 Prosocial Development

Aituaki (Polynesian) children, particularly girls, from moral responsibility to help that person (Miller &
urban settings performed fewer chores and were less Bersoff, 1998). Both groups, however, reported feeling
prosocial than were children raised in traditional ex- less obligation to help people on the other side of the
tended families. world than those in their own town. Miller and Bersoff
Consistent with some of the aforementioned labora- (1992; Baron & Miller, 2000) argued that a personal
tory research on Asian and Western childrens prosocial morality of interpersonal responsiveness and caring
behavior, Stevenson (1991) found that the observed inci- (such as that in the United States) is linked to a strong
dence of sharing, comforting, and helping in Taiwanese, cultural emphasis on individual rights and autonomy.
Japanese, and U.S. kindergarten classes was lowest in The research on prosocial and caring-related moral
the United States (albeit relatively high in all groups). reasoning is a body of work relevant to an understanding
Stevenson and others have argued that Chinese and of cross-cultural variation in cognitions about prosocial
Japanese societies generally put great emphasis on so- behavior. Among industrial Western cultures, relatively
cializing children to be responsible and prosocial toward few cross-cultural differences in prosocial or caring-
others in their group (e.g., the family, the classroom, related reasoning have been noted, although minor dif-
and the society; also see Hieshima & Schneider, 1994). ferences have been found (see Eisenberg, Boehnke,
Privileges and social acknowledgment in the classroom Schuhler, & Silbereisen, 1985; Eisenberg, Hertz-
are dependent on group rather than on individual ac- Lazarowitz, & Fuchs, 1990; Skoe et al., 1999). More-
complishments. Researchers have also suggested that over, the reasons that German, Polish, Italian, and
Japanese mothers traditionally use empathic sensitivity American adolescents attribute to themselves for help-
in their parenting to promote their childrens empathy ing or not helping were somewhat similar, although some
with them and with others needs (Lebra, 1994; differences have been found (Boehnke, Silbereisen,
Trommsdorff & Kornadt, 2003). However, parental Eisenberg, Reykowski, & Palmonari, 1989). In general,
valuing of prosocial behavior appears to have declined however, the similarities in the care- or prosocial-
from the 1950s and 1960s to the 1980s in the Peoples related moral reasoning or prosocial self-attributions of
Republic of China (Lee & Zhan, 1991), so it is unclear individuals from Western cultures are much greater than
whether the findings would be replicated today in Asian the differences.
countries that are undergoing rapid cultural transitions. The prosocial-related moral reasoning of children
and adults from non-Western or less industrial cultures
Moral Reasoning, Values, and Beliefs about may differ considerably from that of people from West-
Social Responsibility ern cultures, especially with age; however, the pattern is
not very consistent. Carlo et al. (1996) found that
Cultural norms regarding the importance of harmony Brazilian urban adolescents used less internalized (i.e.,
among people and social responsibility differ across higher level) prosocial moral reasoning than did adoles-
cultures and subcultures. Miller and her colleagues cents from the United States, although their reasoning
found that Hindu Indians held a broader and more strin- was similar otherwise. Kumru, Carlo, Mestre, and Sam-
gent duty-based view of social responsibility than did per (2003) found that Turkish adolescents scored higher
people in the United States. Hindu Indians, school-age than Spanish adolescents on mid-level modes of proso-
and adult, tended to focus more than North Americans cial moral reasoning (i.e., needs-oriented and stereo-
on responsiveness to others needs when discussing typic), whereas Spanish adolescents scored higher on
moral conflicts and viewed interpersonal responsibili- both lower ( hedonistic and approval oriented) and
ties as at least as important as justice-related obligations higher (internalized) types of moral reasoning. When
(Miller & Bersoff, 1992). In contrast, people in the justifying hypothetical moral decisions involving oth-
United States tended to view interpersonal responsive- ers needs, Ma (1989) found that English adolescents
ness and caring as less obligatory and more of a personal were more oriented to their own survival and less to be-
choice, particularly if the others need was moderate or longingness and to affective and altruistic motives than
minor, or if friends or strangers (rather than parents and were Chinese adolescents from Hong Kong and main-
children) were potential recipients (Miller, Bersoff, & land China. However, Stewart and McBride-Chang
Harwood, 1990). Adults in the United States, for exam- (2000) found no differences in Western Caucasian and
ple, were more likely than Indian adults to report that Asian (mostly Chinese) second graders moral reason-
their liking of a needy sibling or colleague affected their ing; and Japanese childrens prosocial moral reasoning
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 665

resembled that of children from urbanized Western cul- tional, and transactional in influence (Bugental &
tures (although there are some differences; Munekata & Grusec, Chapter 7, this Handbook, this volume), and this
Ninomiya, 1985). In the one study of a nonindustrial, relation is embedded in the macro environment (e.g.,
traditional sample, Tietjen (1986) found that although family, neighborhood, culture). However, this complex-
younger Maisen children from Papua New Guinea dif- ity generally is not reflected in the existing empirical re-
fered little in their prosocial moral reasoning from chil- search on the socialization of prosocial behavior.
dren in Western cultures, Maisen adults moral
reasoning was less sophisticated than that of Western
Demographic Features of Families and
adults. Maisen adults reasoning, however, was probably
Family Members
appropriate for a small traditional society in which oth-
ers physical and psychological needs, costs for proso- Intuitively, one might expect childrens prosocial behav-
cial behavior, and pragmatic concerns are paramount to ior to be related to the socioeconomic status (SES) of
everyday life. their families. Poorer children might be expected to
Making cross-cultural comparisons can be difficult horde scarce resources or, due to increased demand for
because cultures differ considerably in their valuing of participation in caregiving chores, to be relatively help-
different types of prosocial action. Hindu Indians ful and likely to comfort others in distress (see Whiting
viewed prosocial behavior performed because of reci- & Whiting, 1975).
procity considerations as more moral than did American Findings are inconsistent about the relation of indices
adults (Miller & Bersoff, 1994). Further, Middle East- of socioeconomic status such as family income or
ern third graders in Israel seemed to value requested parental education to most types of prosocial behavior
acts of consideration more, and spontaneous acts less, (Laible, Carlo, & Raffaelli, 2000; see Eisenberg &
than did Israeli Jewish children of Western heritage (Ja- Fabes, 1998). However, many of the relevant studies in-
cobsen, 1983). Thus, Westerners may value prosocial clude relatively few study participants. In a large study
acts that appear to be based on endogenous motivation in England, factors such as social support for parents,
more than do people from traditional cultures whereas favorable housing, and fewer transitions in maternal
people from traditional cultures value prosocial actions partner relationships, in addition to higher maternal ed-
that reflect responsiveness to others stated needs and ucation, higher family income, and lower levels of finan-
reciprocal obligations. cial problems, were associated with higher levels of
mother-reported prosocial behavior for school-age chil-
dren ( but less so for 4-year-old younger siblings; Dunn
et al., 1998). Furthermore, findings are consistent for
SOCIALIZATION WITHIN AND OUTSIDE adolescents volunteering behavior. In a large study of
THE FAMILY volunteerism among at-risk adolescents, family poverty
was negatively associated with males involvement in
Family structure, socialization within the family, and volunteering and community activity (Lichter, Shana-
socialization by peers and in the schools may augment or han, & Gardner, 2002); a similar relation was obtained
counteract cultural influences. However, the existing re- for both sexes in another large study involving a more
search has limitations, including an overreliance on par- representative sample (Hart, Atkins, & Ford, 1998) and
ents reports of the childs prosocial proclivities and of in other studies on volunteering in the United States
their own socialization practices or style, the use of (Huebner & Mancini, 2003; Lichter et al., 2002; Na-
very brief observations to measure behavior (which may tional Center for Education Statistics, 1997; Uggen &
not be generalizable), and a dearth of data from fathers Janikula, 1999; Youniss, McLellan, Su, & Yates, 1999)
and from minority and non-Western populations. It is and Hong Kong (Chou, 1998). Nonetheless, most of
likely that the relations of aspects of parental control these relations are modest in magnitude.
and punitiveness to developmental outcomes (including Findings on the relation of family structure and fam-
prosocial and moral development) vary somewhat across ily size to prosocial behavior are mixed. Rehberg and
cultures (Trommsdorff & Kornadt, 2003). Further, most Richman (1989) found that preschool boys from father-
of the work is correlational; thus, causal relations cannot absent homes comforted ( but did not help) a peer more
be ascertained. The prevailing view of socialization is than did girls and boys from two-parent homes. Other
that the parent-child relationship is complex, bidirec- researchers have not found effects of father absence on
666 Prosocial Development

measures of prosocial responding (Call, Mortimer, & tices to childrens prosocial behavior and empathy/
Shanahan, 1995; Dunn et al., 1998), and some re- sympathy.
searchers have found that adolescents in two-parent
Parental Warmth and Quality of the
families volunteer more than those in one-parent homes
Parent-Child Relationship
(Huebner & Mancini, 2003; Keith et al., 1990; Lichter
et al., 2002; Youniss et al., 1999). Investigators have Intuitively, it would seem that warm, supportive socializ-
found that family size and prosocial behavior or sympa- ers would rear prosocial children. However, support for
thy are unrelated (e.g., Chou, 1998; Gelfand, Hartmann, this assumption is mixed. In some studies, a positive rela-
Cromer, Smith, & Page, 1975); that children in a large tion between an index of maternal warmth/support or sen-
family volunteer more (Zaff, Moore, Papillo, & sitivity (often versus negativity) and childrens and
Williams, 2003); and that children with siblings are less adolescents prosocial or empathic/sympathetic respond-
likely to help in an emergency situation (Staub, 1971b) ing has been obtained, at least for some measures (Asbury,
or to comfort a peer (Rehberg & Richman, 1989). Staub Dunn, Pike, & Plomin, 2003; Bryant & Crockenberg,
speculated that children from small families are more 1980; Deater-Deckard, Dunn, et al., 2001; Dunn, Cutting,
self-assured and, consequently, are more likely to take & Fisher, 2002; Eberly et al., 1993; Eberly & Mon-
initiative and intervene spontaneously to help someone temayor, 1998; Janssens & Dekovic, 1997; Janssens &
else. In contrast, children in larger families, perhaps due Gerris, 1992; Kiang et al., 2004; Krevans & Gibbs, 1996;
to the need to engage in chores, are particularly likely to Laible & Carlo, 2004; Lerner et al., 2005; Robinson,
learn everyday helping and sharing behaviors. Consis- Zahn-Waxler, & Emde, 1994; Strayer & Roberts, 2004b;
tent with this reasoning, Weissbrod (1976) found that Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & King, 1979; also see Shek
large family size was related to slower helping in an & Ma, 2001). In contrast, other investigators have failed
emergency but higher levels of generosity. to obtain evidence of a relation between parental warmth
Findings concerning ordinal position are few and (or rejection) and childrens prosocial behavior or empa-
limited in scope. Firstborn children, particularly girls, thy/sympathy (Eberly & Montemayor, 1999; Iannotti
have been found to be more willing than their peers to et al., 1992; Kienbaum, Volland, & Ulich, 2001; Koestner,
give commodities to peers (Sharma, 1988) and to inter- Franz, & Weinberger, 1990; Stewart & McBride-Chang,
vene in an emergency (Staub, 1971b). Moreover, older 2000; Turner & Harris, 1984) or have found very different
siblings, compared with younger siblings, more often be- relations of parental support with childrens prosocial be-
have prosocially in sibling interactions (Bryant & havior and sympathy (Carlo, Roesch, & Melby, 1998).
Crockenberg, 1980; Dunn & Munn, 1986; Furman & Sometimes the relation of parental warmth to childrens
Buhrmester, 1985; Stoneman, Brody, & MacKinnon, prosocial responding has been weak and only significant
1986; Whiting & Whiting, 1975), perhaps due in part to through mediation; for example, Zhou et al. (2002) found
their older age (rather than ordinal position per se) and that the relation of parental warmth to elementary school
their greater engagement in chores and caregiving that students facial and self-reported empathy was indirect
provide opportunities for prosocial behavior (de Guz- through its positive relation with parental expressions of
man, Edwards, & Carlo, 2005). Other investigators have positive emotion in contexts involving others emotions
found no relation between birth order and measures of (especially others positive emotions).
prosocial responding (e.g., Gelfand et al., 1975; Rhein- Support for the role of parental nurturance or warmth
gold, Hay, & West, 1976) or sympathy (Wise & Cramer, can be gleaned from several other bodies of data. Parents
1988), or have obtained mixed findings (Eisenberg, report of childrens helpfulness is higher for adolescents
Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, Carlo, et al., 1996). In general, who share more time and activities with their parents
older children seem to be somewhat more prosocial, es- (Eberly & Montemayor, 1998) and when fathers in
pecially in their actual (rather than reported) prosocial two-parent families are more involved in child care
behavior and in interactions with younger children. (Bernadett-Shapiro, Ehrensaft, & Shapiro, 1996). A
study in which parenting was assessed with observations
(Kochanska, Forman, & Coy, 1999) found that maternal
Parental Socialization Style and Practices
responsivity (contingent, appropriate responding) to their
Many investigators have examined the relations of par- infants at 9 ( but not 14) months predicted higher levels of
enting style and a range of specific socialization prac- toddlers empathy/prosocial responsiveness at 22 months
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 667

(cf. van der Mark et al., 2002). Moreover, Spinrad (1999) significant psychological problems, the link between at-
found that observed maternal sensitivity to their infants tachment and prosocial behavior or empathy/sympathy
at 10 months was positively related to toddlers con- may vary in a complex manner (e.g., Radke-Yarrow,
cerned attention at 18 months of age to adults feigned Zahn-Waxler, Richardson, Susman, & Martinez, 1994).
distress. Further, Clark and Ladd (2000) found that It is likely that the degree of association between
parental connectedness (including mutual parent-child childrens prosocial responding and parental warmth is
positive engagement, warmth, intimacy, and happy emo- moderated by other socialization practices. Dekovic and
tional tone, as well as reciprocity) was positively related Janssens (1992) found that democratic parenting, in-
to kindergartners teacher-reported prosocial tendencies. volving parental warmth and support, combined with
There also is limited evidence that children with se- inductions, demandingness, and the provision of sugges-
cure attachments to their mothers at a young age are tions, information, and positive comments, was associ-
more sympathetic at 3.5 years of age (Waters, Hay, & ated with Dutch childrens prosocial behavior as
Richters, 1986) and display more prosocial behavior and reported by teachers and peers (also see Janssens &
concern for others at approximately age 5 years (Ian- Dekovic, 1997). Similarly, Robinson et al. (1994) found
notti et al., 1992; Kestenbaum, Farber, & Sroufe, 1989). that mothers who were relatively negative and control-
In a study with 22-month-old children, the relation be- ling had children who tended to decrease rather than in-
tween attachment and empathy/sympathy was positive crease in empathy from 14 to 20 months of age (for
but weak and somewhat inconsistent (Van der Mark those moderate or high in empathy at 14 months). More-
et al., 2002). Moreover, adolescents reports of attach- over, as discussed in the section on modeling, socializ-
ment to their parents have been associated with Turkish ers who are nurturant and model prosocial behavior
early adolescents empathy/sympathy/perspective tak- seem to promote costly prosocial behavior in children
ing (Kumru & Edwards, 2003), middle or late adoles- (e.g., Yarrow & Scott, 1972; Yarrow, Scott, & Waxler,
cents sympathy/perspective taking and prosocial 1973). Nurturance may serve as a background or contex-
behavior (Laible, Carlo, & Roesch, 2004; Markiewicz, tual variable that enhances the childs receptivity to
Doyle, & Brendgen, 2001), and parents reports of ado- parental influence, including parental inductions,
lescents helpfulness (Eberly & Montemayor, 1998), al- preachings, and moral standards (Hoffman, 1970).
beit not in all studies (de Guzman & Carlo, 2004; Eberly
& Montemayor, 1999), and not across 2 years time Inductions
(Laible, Carlo, & Raffaelli, 2000). Because securely at- A disciplinary practice of particular importance in the
tached offspring tend to have sensitive and warm par- study of prosocial behavior is parental induction (i.e.,
ents, the finding of a relation between the security of verbal discipline in which the socializer gives explana-
childrens attachments and their prosocial tendencies is tions or reasons for requiring the child to change his or
indirect support for an association between parental her behavior; Hoffman, 1970). Hoffman (2000) argued
warmth and childrens prosocial development. that inductions are likely to promote moral development
Why might children with warm parents and secure because they induce an optimal level of arousal for
attachments be more prosocial? Waters et al. (1986) learning (i.e., elicit the childs attention, but are un-
suggested that children with secure attachments differ- likely to disrupt learning). Further, inductions are not
entially attend to their parent, are positively oriented to likely to be viewed as arbitrary by the child and thereby
the parent, are familiar with and reproduce parents ac- induce resistance; rather, they focus childrens attention
tions, and are responsive to parental control and wish to on the consequences of their behavior for others, thereby
avoid parental censure. These tendencies would be ex- capitalizing on childrens capacity to empathize and ex-
pected to enhance the effectiveness of parents attempts perience guilt. Hoffman further suggested that over
to encourage prosocial behavior. Staub (1992) also ar- time, inductive messages are experienced as internal-
gued that the quality of early attachments is important ized because the child plays an active role in processing
to the development of a sense of connection to others the information (which is encoded and integrated with
and positive valuing of other peopletwo characteris- information contained in other inductions) and the focus
tics with conceptual links to intrinsically based caring is on the childs action and its consequences rather than
for other people (also see Oliner & Oliner, 1988). on the parent as the disciplinary agent. Thus, over time,
Nonetheless, in families in which the child or parent has children are likely to remember the causal link between
668 Prosocial Development

their actions and consequences for others rather than the haps because the toddlers were unlikely to attend or to
external pressure or the specific disciplinary context. think that their mother was serious. Similarly, Miller,
Investigators usually have tried to assess the degree Eisenberg, Fabes, Shell, and Gular (1989) found that in-
to which parents use inductions as a general mode of ductions regarding peers were positively related to chil-
discipline, not simply to promote prosocial behavior (as drens sad reactions to viewing others in distress and,
for experimental studies on preaching). Inductions vary when delivered by mothers with affective intensity, to
in their content: They can appeal to justice, including low levels of facial distress (an index of personal dis-
fairness of the consequences of the childs behavior for tress rather than sympathy). However, parental induc-
another; appeal to legitimate authorities; or provide tions delivered in situations involving relatively high
matter-of-fact, nonmoralistic information. In addition, degrees of anger, particularly inductions that are guilt-
inductions may be focused on the consequences of the inducing, seem to be associated with low levels of
childs behavior for either the parent or for the other preschoolers parent-directed prosocial behavior (Den-
person involved in the situation (often called peer- ham, Renwick-DeBardi, & Hewes, 1994).
oriented inductions). Hoffman (1970) argued that The configuration of parenting practices appears to
peer-oriented inductions are likely to be most effective influence the effectiveness of inductions. They are
because they are most apt to induce sympathy. likely to be more effective at promoting prosocial be-
There is support for an association between parental havior or empathy when verbalized by parents who typ-
use of inductions and childrens prosocial tendencies, ically do not use power-assertive (punitive) techniques
although significant findings often have been obtained (Hoffman, 1963; also see Dlugokinski & Firestone,
for one sex, age, or socioeconomic status group, or for 1974) or are part of a pattern of democratic or authori-
one measure of prosocial behavior (or empathy/sympa- tative parenting (Dekovic & Janssens, 1992; Janssens &
thy), and not another. Nonetheless, positive associations Gerris, 1992).
have been found in studies in which the type of reason- Some of the inconsistency in the findings on induc-
ing was not specified (Bar-Tal, Nadler, & Blechman, tions may stem from a failure by researchers to assess
1980; Dlugokinski & Firestone, 1974; Feshbach, 1978; critical dimensions of parental messages. Grusec and
Janssens & Gerris, 1992; Oliner & Oliner, 1988; cf. Goodnow (1994) argued that internalization of parental
Trommsdorff, 1991), as well as in those in which messages likely depends on childrens accurate percep-
parental inductions focused on peers or others feelings tion of the message (including its content, the rules im-
or states (Hoffman, 1975; Karylowski, 1982; Krevans & plied in the message, and the parents intentions and
Gibbs, 1996; Stanhope, Bell, & Parker-Cohen, 1987). investment in the message) and childrens acceptance of
Victim-oriented discipline seems to enhance the level of it. They suggested that the clarity, redundancy, and con-
childrens interpersonal understanding (e.g., perspec- sistency of the message, as well as its fit to the childs
tive taking), which is associated with higher guilt, in- developmental level, influence childrens accurate per-
cluding concern about harm to another (de Veer & ception of the message. Children are more likely to ac-
Janssens, 1994). Further, inductions that emphasize how cept the message if they perceive it as appropriate, find
others (including the parent) react to childrens behavior it motivating (e.g., if it arouses empathy or insecurity),
have been found to predict higher levels of prosocial be- and believe that the value inherent in the message is self-
havior (Krevans & Gibbs, 1996). Stewart and McBride- generated. Grusec and Goodnow also hypothesized that
Chang (2000) found that parental emphasis on the parental responsivity or past willingness to comply with
effects of the childs misbehavior in the family and what the childs wishes promotes the childs willingness to
others think of the child was positively related to the comply with the parents wishes. Thus, it may be pro-
anonymous donations of Asian children in Hong Kong. ductive to examine the clarity of parents messages and
The tone in which inductions are delivered often may variables related to childrens acceptance of the mes-
contribute to their effectiveness, particularly with sage as moderators of the relation between parental in-
young children. Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, and King ductions and childrens prosocial behavior.
(1979) noted that maternal use of affectively charged
explanations, particularly those that included moraliz- Power-Assertive, Punitive Techniques of Discipline
ing, was positively associated with toddlers prosocial Researchers generally have found that socializers use
behavior in the second and 3rd years of life. Explana- of power-assertive techniques of discipline such as phys-
tions delivered without affect were not effective, per- ical punishment or deprivation of privileges is either un-
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 669

related (e.g., Janssens & Gerris, 1992; Kochanska et al., attributing their own donating to internal motives
1999; Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, & King, 1979) or (Smith et al., 1979). Thus, it is possible that social dis-
negatively related to childrens prosocial behavior (As- approval (verbal punishment) can be used to enhance in-
bury, Dunn, Pike, & Plomin, 2003; Bar-Tal, Nadler, ternally motivated prosocial behavior; indeed, maternal
et al., 1980; Deater-Deckard, Dunn, et al., 2001; Dlu- expressions of disappointment have been linked to
gokinski & Firestone, 1974; Krevans & Gibbs, 1996), greater prosocial behavior (Stewart & McBride-Chang,
empathy (Janssens & Gerris, 1992; Krevans & Gibbs, 2000). Although most middle-class mothers in Western
1996), or sympathy (Spinrad et al., 1999). Likewise, a cultures such as the United States rarely use punishment
punitive, authoritarian parenting style has been unre- (especially physical punishment) to induce helping or in
lated (Iannotti, Cummings, Pierrehumbert, Milano, & response to childrens failure to help (Grusec, 1991;
Zahn-Waxler, 1992; Russell et al., 2003, for mothers; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1979), this may be less true in Asian
also see Diener & Kim, 2004) or negatively related societies (see Stewart & McBride-Chang, 2000).
(Dekovic & Janssens, 1992; Hastings et al., 2000; Rus-
sell et al., 2003, for fathers) to childrens prosocial be- Appropriate versus Inappropriate Parental Control
havior and sympathy, and its negative relation with Perhaps the critical issue when thinking about parental
sympathy may increase with age (Hastings et al., 2000). punishment and control is whether the degree of power
Moreover, physical abuse of children has been linked to asserted by the parent is perceived as excessive and ar-
low levels of childrens empathy and prosocial behavior bitrary versus reasonable in the given context or culture.
(Howes & Eldredge, 1985; Main & George, 1985; Miller Parental demands and expectations for socially respon-
& Eisenberg, 1988; see Koenig, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, sible and moral behavior (often expressed in an authori-
2004, for mixed findings). tative parenting style) have been associated with
Nonetheless, there is a difference between the occa- socially responsible and prosocial behavior (e.g.,
sional, measured use of power-assertive techniques in Dekovic & Janssens, 1992; Janssens & Dekovic, 1997;
the context of a positive parent-child relationship and Janssens & Gerris, 1992; Lidner-Gunnoe, Hetherington,
the use of punishment as the preferred, predominant & Reiss; 1999), adolescents endorsement of caring val-
mode of discipline. Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe re- ues (Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, & Alisat, 2003), and
ported that the punishment they had received from their caring moral reasoning (Pratt, Skoe, & Arnold, 2004).
parents was not a routine response and was linked to In contrast, strict, rejecting control has been linked to
specific behaviors rather than used gratuitously (Oliner low levels of sympathy (Laible & Carlo, 2004). Some-
& Oliner, 1988). Further, Miller et al. (1989) found that what related, in Western cultures, parental emphasis on
maternal report of using physical techniques (including adolescents autonomy also has been linked with proso-
physical punishment) was positively associated with cial development (Bar-Tal, Nadler, et al., 1980; Pratt
preschoolers empathic sadness when viewing others in et al., 2004); this relation may hold less in early child-
distress, but only for children whose mothers also used hood (Clark & Ladd, 2000). In Asian cultures that em-
relatively high levels of inductive discipline (cf. Hoff- phasize parental training and filial piety (Stewart et al.,
man, 1963). 1998), training of this sort was associated with anony-
Punishment can induce immediate compliance with mous prosocial behavior, whereas restrictive control was
socializers expectations for prosocial behavior if the marginally, negatively related (Stewart & McBride-
socializer monitors the childs behavior (Morris, Mar- Chang, 2000). Other researchers have found a positive
shall, & Miller, 1973), particularly if the contingency association between appropriate parental control (rather
between lack of prosocial behavior and punishment is than leniency) and childrens empathy (Bryant, 1987) or
specified (Hartmann et al., 1976). However, these ef- girls ( but not boys) sympathy years later in adulthood
fects often extinguish when punishment is removed (Koestner, Franz, & Weinberger, 1990). Analogously,
(Hartmann et al., 1976), and children tend to attribute parental monitoring of adolescents activities was posi-
prosocial behavior induced by power-assertive tech- tively related to adolescents volunteerism in large sur-
niques to external motives such as fear of detection or vey research (Huebner & Mancini, 2003; Zaff et al.,
punishment (Dix & Grusec, 1983; Smith, Gelfand, Hart- 2003). For middle-class families, parental demands for
mann, & Partlow, 1979). Nonetheless, social disap- prosocial behavior appear to be part of a child-rearing
proval, unlike material punishment (e.g., fines for not pattern in which mature behavior is expected (Green-
helping), has been positively associated with childrens berger & Goldberg, 1989). In contrast, parental valuing
670 Prosocial Development

of mere compliance, which often may lead to arbitrary prosocial behavior. Thus, the generalizability of much of
overcontrol, has been linked to low levels of childrens the laboratory research to real-life settings involving fa-
prosocial behavior with mothers and peers (Eisenberg, miliar models and to other types of prosocial actions can
Wolchik, Goldberg, & Engel, 1992). be questioned. The experimental laboratory literature is
supplemented by a smaller body of work, often correla-
Parental Emphasis on Prosocial Values tional in design, in which real-life situations and famil-
Because parents who hold prosocial values would be ex- iar models have been used; and similar results have been
pected to teach and model prosocial behavior, it is rea- obtained in these studies.
sonable to expect a relation between parental prosocial In the prototypic laboratory study of modeling proso-
values and childrens prosocial behavior. Parents reports cial behavior, children earn prizes, tokens, or money by
of holding prosocial values have been associated with winning a game, view or do not view a model, and then
peer nominations of fifth graders prosocial behavior (in- are provided an opportunity to donate to needy children
cluding prosocial behavior, guilt, and rule-following; or to children who did not get to play the game. Because
Hoffman, 1975) and older adolescents caring moral rea- this topic was reviewed in considerable detail in Eisen-
soning (Pratt et al., 2004; also see Eisenberg, Wolchik, berg and Fabes (1998) and there have been few new
et al., 1992). Although some investigators have found no studies since 1998, this work is briefly summarized
evidence of a relation between parental emphasis on here. In general, children who view a generous model or
prosocial responding (reported or observed) and chil- helpful model are more generous or helpful than those
drens prosocial behavior or empathy (Turner & Harris, exposed to a control condition (often a model who had
1984), others have obtained mixed (Bryant & Crocken- no opportunity to donate; e.g., Rice & Grusec, 1975;
berg, 1980) or positive relations (Trommsdorff, 1991) Rushton & Littlefield, 1979; Rushton & Teachman,
(also see section on modeling and preachings). 1978) or a selfish model (e.g., Bryan & Walbek, 1970;
Perhaps the most compelling evidence for the impor- Rushton, 1975). Further, multiple models may be more
tance of parental prosocial values comes from studies of effective than inconsistent models for inducing precise
adults who have displayed unusual acts of altruism. Res- imitation of donating (Wilson, Piazza, & Nagle, 1990).
cuers in Nazi Europe often recalled learning values of In most laboratory studies of modeling, prosocial be-
caring from their parents or the other most influential havior is modeled only once; thus, it is impressive that
person in their lives (Oliner & Oliner, 1988; also see some investigators have obtained evidence of general-
Hart & Fegley, 1995; London, 1970). Rescuers reported ization to new behaviors or settings (Midlarsky &
that their parents felt that ethical values were to be ex- Bryan, 1967; Rushton, 1975), although others have not
tended to all human beings. Interestingly, rescuers did (Rushton & Littlefield, 1979; Rushton & Teachman,
not differ from nonrescuers in reported exposure to non- 1978). Further, investigators have found effects of mod-
prosocial values such as honesty or equity. However, eling days to months later (Israel & Raskin, 1979; Rice
real-life moral exemplars often solidify their values or & Grusec, 1975; Rushton, 1975; Rushton & Littlefield,
even develop new moral values in adulthood when inter- 1979; Wilson et al., 1990).
acting with other adults who discuss value-related issues Adults who control valued resources (Grusec, 1971)
and jointly engage in moral activities with the individual appear to be relatively powerful models, as are models
(Colby & Damon, 1992). Thus, it is likely that the so- perceived as competent (Eisenberg-Berg & Geisheker,
cialization of other-oriented values, even if it begins in 1979). Moreover, nurturant prosocial models whom
ones family of origin, is a continuing dynamic process. children have just met seem to promote prosocial behav-
ior when the prosocial behavior is not costly and is
Modeling
something they probably want to do (e.g., help when they
Because of the importance of modeling in social learn- hear someone in distress; Weissbrod, 1976; also see
ing theory (e.g., Bandura, 1986), numerous researchers Staub, 1971a). In contrast, when prosocial behavior in-
have examined whether childrens prosocial behavior volves self-denial (e.g., donations), short-term exposure
varies as a function of exposure to prosocial versus self- to a warm model seems to have little effect or may even
ish models. Much of the relevant research has been con- reduce donating behavior (Grusec, 1971; Midlarsky &
ducted in laboratory studies using strangers or brief Bryan, 1967; Weissbrod, 1976). Thus, short-term non-
acquaintances as models and donating as the index of contingent warmth seems to disinhibit children to do as
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 671

they please, including assisting distressed others as well In regard to high-cost real-life helping behavior,
as keeping valued commodities for themselves. How- Rosenhan (1970) found that Caucasian civil rights ac-
ever, in the classroom context in which warmth probably tivists in the late 1950s and 1960s who were highly in-
is not entirely noncontingent, preschool children model volved and committed to the cause despite considerable
the prosocial behaviors and nurturance of adults with danger and cost reported that their parents were both
whom they have had a relatively extended nurturant re- nurturant and actively involved in working for altruistic
lationship (Yarrow & Scott, 1972; Yarrow et al., 1973). and humanitarian causes. In contrast, individuals who
In addition to the laboratory studies, investigators were less involved and committed reported that their
have examined whether children appear to model real- parents preached prosocial values but often did not prac-
life socializers such as parents. In the first 2 years of tice altruism. Further, rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe
life, children do not seem to consistently model mater- described their parents as having acted in accordance
nal sharing or helping of a distressed person (Hay & with strong moral convictions (London, 1970; Oliner &
Murray, 1982; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1979). However, Oliner, 1988).
mothers modeling of helping behaviors (such as partic- The data from studies of adult altruists are not only
ipation in household chores) seems to enhance the likeli- correlational in design but involve retrospective data.
hood of 1- and 2-year-olds helping with similar tasks Even if peoples recall of parental practices were unbi-
(Rheingold, 1982). Moreover, the data on real-life altru- ased and accurate, it is possible that their altruism
ists suggest an effect of parental modeling. Youth volun- stemmed from family factors other than modeling, such
teerism has been found to be related to the degree to as optimal discipline or exposure to prosocial cultural or
which their parents volunteer; moreover, the types of community values. Nonetheless, research findings on
voluntary activities chosen by youths tend to be similar parents of prosocial offspring converge with the experi-
to those of their parents (e.g., in providing a social ser- mental laboratory findings that implicate modeling in
vice or working for a cause; Keith et al., 1990; McLellan the development of prosocial tendencies.
& Youniss, 2003; National Center for Education Statis-
tics, 1997; also see Hart & Fegley, 1995; Janoski & Preachings
Wilson, 1995; Stukas, Switzer, Dew, Goycoolea, & The verbalizations of adults relevant to prosocial behav-
Simmons, 1999). ior have been examined in nondisciplinary contexts ( lab-
Consistent with the notion that parental modeling oratory situations in which the adult is not responding to
fosters childrens prosocial tendencies, sympathetic the childs misbehavior), as well as in disciplinary situa-
parents, who likely model sympathy, tend to have same- tions (e.g., inductions). In studies of the effects of
sex elementary school children who are helpful (Fabes, preachings or exhortations, the preacher states what
Eisenberg, & Miller, 1990) or prone to sympathy rather should be done (sometimes in regard to his or her own
than to egoistic personal distress (Eisenberg, Fabes, earnings that can be donated), but does not directly and
Carlo, Troyer, et al., 1992; Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, explicitly direct the child to assist. Often the preacher
Carlo, & Miller, 1991; Eisenberg & McNally, 1993; also gives reasons that one should or should not assist.
Fabes et al., 1990). In contrast, links between parental Preachers may verbalize to themselves, as if thinking
empathy (rather than sympathy) and childrens empathy through the issue (Eisenberg-Berg & Geisheker, 1979),
have been mixed, with some researchers obtaining posi- or direct their preaching to the child (e.g., Bryan & Wal-
tive relations (Barnett, Howard, King, & Dino, 1980; bek, 1970; Rushton, 1975). Preachings often are norma-
Strayer & Roberts, 2004b; Trommsdorff, 1991) and oth- tive in content, with the preacher stating what should be
ers obtaining no relations or inconsistent correlations done and stating either prosocial or selfish norms (e.g.,
(e.g., Bernadett-Shapiro, Ehrensaft, & Shapiro, 1996; Its a nice thing [not such a nice thing] to give to the
Strayer & Roberts, 1989). Some parents prone to empa- crippled children; Bryan & Walbek, 1970). In a neutral
thy may become overly aroused and personally dis- control group, the preacher typically would make nor-
tressed, which would be expected to lead to lower levels matively neutral statements such as This game is fun.
of helping in many contexts. Multiple mechanisms, in- Most researchers have found no effects, or inconsis-
cluding the heritability of emotionality related to sym- tent effects, of normative preachings by nonparental
pathy or other characteristics, could explain the adults on childrens donating behavior (e.g., Bryan &
significant findings that have been obtained. Walbek, 1970; cf. Zarbatany, Hartmann, & Gelfand,
672 Prosocial Development

1985). However, normative preachings seem to foster and, consequently, may not enact prosocial behavior in
generosity if the preacher promoting donating is an adult an unsupervised setting (see McGrath & Power, 1990).
who is likely to have direct power over the children
Reinforcement for Prosocial Behavior
(Eisenberg-Berg & Geisheker, 1979). In addition, empa-
thy-inducing preachings that emphasize the emotional Consistent with learning theory, concrete (Fischer,
consequences of assisting for the recipients of aid have 1963) and social (Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, et al., 1993;
been found to elicit more donating in private than do Gelfand et al., 1975; Grusec & Redler, 1980; Rushton &
neutral control preachings (Dlugokinski & Firestone, Teachman, 1978; cf. Mills & Grusec, 1989) reinforce-
1974; Eisenberg-Berg & Geisheker, 1979; Perry, ments have been found to increase childrens prosocial
Bussey, & Freiberg, 1981; Smith, 1983) or punitive, behavior, at least in the immediate context. Further,
threatening preachings (Perry et al., 1981). Empathy-in- parental reports of reinforcement for childrens sympa-
ducing preachings also have been found to enhance the thetic and prosocial behavior have been associated with
effort and success of children in elementary school girls ( but not boys) concerned or sad reactions to oth-
when helping a peer (Ladd, Lange, & Stremmel, 1983) ers in distress (Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, et al., 1992).
and have been related to prosocial behavior in another Although concrete rewards may induce prosocial be-
setting or at a later date (Grusec, Saas-Kortsaak, & havior in the given context, the long-term effect of con-
Simutis, 1978; Smith, 1983). crete rewards may be negative. Consistent with Leppers
Not all researchers have found effects of empathy-in- (1983) notion that the provision of concrete rewards un-
ducing preachings. The wording in some studies may dermines intrinsic motivation (and also may induce chil-
have led the children to believe that the adult or the ben- dren to attribute their prosocial actions to external
eficiary would be angry at them for not helping, which motivation), Szynal-Brown and Morgan (1983) found
might evoke reactance rather than empathy (McGrath & that third-grade children who were promised tangible
Power, 1990), or compliance rather than internalization. rewards if the younger children they tutored did well
Preachings seem to work best if children feel that they were less likely to engage in teaching activities during a
have a choice of whether to assist and if the preachings subsequent free-choice period than were tutors who
highlight the positive outcomes of helping for another were not promised rewards for teaching. Those children
(Grusec, Saas-Kortsaak, & Simutis, 1978; McGrath, promised rewards that were not contingent on the pupils
Wilson, & Frassetto; 1995). Further, the results of one learning were between the aforementioned two groups in
study suggest that empathic preachings are effective pri- regard to teaching, but did not differ significantly from
marily for children who have been exposed to inductive either. Further, Fabes, Fultz, Eisenberg, Plumlee, and
discipline at home (rather than a relatively high degree Christopher (1989) found that the use of material re-
of power assertion; Dlugokinski & Firestone, 1974). wards for school childrens helping behavior under-
mined their subsequent, anonymous prosocial behavior
Prompts and Directives during a free-choice situation, particularly for children
Children who are instructed or prompted to help or whose mothers valued the use of rewards. Moreover,
share tend to do so (Gelfand et al., 1975; Hay & Murray, mothers who felt relatively positive about using rewards
1982; Israel & Raskin, 1979), and the effects of direc- reported that their children were less prosocial than did
tive instructions have been found to persist over 11 days mothers who were less enthusiastic about the use of re-
(Israel & Brown, 1979) or 4 weeks (Israel & Raskin, wards. Rewards may be salient for these children and,
1979). Direct requests for prosocial behavior may be consequently, they may be particularly likely to attrib-
particularly important for younger children because of ute their initial prosocial behavior to the external reward
their limited abilities to understand others emotions (rather than to an internal motive).
and situational cues (Denham, Mason, & Couchoud, The effects of social reinforcement may vary as a
1995). However, there is evidence that constraining di- function of type of praise and the age of the child. For
rectives are less effective with older children than with young children, reinforcement for prosocial behavior
younger ones (White & Burnam, 1975), particularly does not seem to increase prosocial tendencies in an-
over time (Israel & Raskin, 1979; cf. Israel & Brown, other setting or over time and may even undermine it
1979). Highly constraining instructions may induce re- (Eisenberg, Wolchik, et al., 1992; Grusec, 1991). More-
actance; moreover, after the early years, children are over, praise that attributes the childrens positive behav-
unlikely to attribute forced behavior to internal reasons ior to their dispositional kindness or internal motives
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 673

(e.g., because they enjoy helping others) appears to be school who were induced to engage in prosocial behavior
more effective than praise that simply labels the act as and provided with internal attributions were more help-
positive (Grusec & Redler, 1980; Mills & Grusec, 1989; ful if they demonstrated the ability to label traits accu-
dispositional attribution is a special type of praise and rately. Thus, it is possible that an understanding of traits
is discussed in the following subsection). Grusec and is essential if internal attributions are to foster chil-
Redler (1980) found that social reinforcement for proso- drens prosocial behavior.
cial actions (without an internal attribution) increased
Learning by Doing (and the Foot-in-the-Door Effect)
elementary school childrens prosocial behavior in the
immediate context; however, it was associated with the Childrens participation in prosocial activities seems to
generalization of prosocial behavior to a different, foster prosocial behavior at a later time, although boys
anonymous situation only for 10-year-old children (not sometimes may exhibit some reactance in the short-term
for 5- or 8-year-olds). Grusec and Redler (1980) hypoth- (Staub, 1992). This pattern of findings has been ob-
esized that older children may interpret reinforcement tained using both experimental procedures (Staub, 1979;
for a specific action as having implications for a variety although effects may be stronger for older children;
of situations, whereas younger children do not view Eisenberg, Cialdini, et al., 1987) and in research linking
praise for a given act as having broader relevance. prosocial proclivities to participation in household
chores (perhaps particularly those that benefit others;
Provision of Attributions or Dispositional Praise Graves & Graves, 1983; Rehberg & Richman, 1989;
Elementary school children are likely to behave in a Whiting & Whiting, 1975; cf. Gelfand et al., 1975). In
prosocial manner on a subsequent occasion if they ini- some cultures, guided participation (Rogoff, 2003) may
tially are induced to behave prosocially and are provided be a major way in which children are socialized into a
with internal attributions (i.e., dispositional praise) for variety of activities, including prosocial ones (Whiting
their actions (e.g., I guess youre the kind of person & Whiting, 1975).
who likes to help others whenever you can. Yes, you are In a study of 9- and 14-year-old children, Grusec,
a very nice and helpful person; Grusec & Redler, Goodnow, and Cohen (1996) found that routine ( but not
1980). Children provided with such praise are more requested) participation in household chores was related
helpful or generous even weeks later than are children to youths prosocial behavior in the family, but primar-
who are provided with no attribution (Grusec, Kuczyn- ily for older youth and girls. Routine participation in
ski, Rushton, & Simutis, 1978; Grusec & Redler, 1980; chores was not related to helping strangers. Thus, if
Holte, Jamruszka, Gustafson, Beaman, & Camp, 1984; chores benefit a delimited group of individuals, any
cf. Eisenberg, Cialdini, McCreath, & Shell, 1987) or prosocial tendencies fostered may not extend to those
with one attributing prosocial behavior to the fact that beyond that group.
the adult experimenter expected such behavior (Grusec, Participation in organized youth activities and non-
Kuczynski, et al., 1978). voluntary service required by school programs also has
The provision of internal attributions is believed to been linked to prosocial behavior, especially subsequent
foster a prosocial self-image that then results in en- volunteerism or intentions to volunteer (Metz &
hanced prosocial behavior (Grusec & Redler, 1980). Youniss, 2003; Stukas, Switzer, et al., 1999; Youniss &
However, support for this supposition is mixed (e.g., Metz, 2004). In addition, adolescents and young adults
Holte et al., 1984; Mills & Grusec, 1989). If changes in participation in voluntary community service some-
childrens self-concepts mediate the effects of disposi- times has been linked to greater feelings of commitment
tional attributions, the provision of internal attributions to helping others (Yates & Youniss, 1996b; see discus-
would not be expected to be effective until children have sion of these programs in the section on adolescence).
some understanding of personality traits and their sta- Of particular interest, Youniss and Metz (2004) found
bility. Consistent with this logic, Grusec and Redler that required school-based service was related to in-
(1980) found that the provision of internal attributions creased volunteerism and intentions to volunteer for stu-
was effective in enhancing prosocial behavior both im- dents who were less inclined to participate; it had little
mediately and long term (e.g., a week or more later) for effect for those students who quickly completed their
middle and later elementary school children, but not for requirement and went on to participate in voluntary ac-
kindergartners. Further, Eisenberg, Cialdini, McCreath, tivities. In contrast, Stukas, Snyder, and Clary (1999)
and Shell (1989) found that children in elementary found that mandatory volunteerism undermined college
674 Prosocial Development

students future intentions to volunteer only for individ- perience a self-focused, aversive response (i.e., per-
uals who otherwise would not have been volunteering sonal distress) when confronted with anothers distress,
(i.e., they felt that their service was solely due to exter- whereas children who can regulate their emotions tend
nal force) or for those who had the preexisting belief to experience sympathy (Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy,
that they would not freely choose to engage in any volun- et al., 1994, 1996).
teer activities. For most students who are not generally For example, Buck (1984) hypothesized that punitive
opposed to volunteer service activities and do not focus reactions by parents when children exhibit negative
on external pressures to engage in such activities, emotion result in childrens increased arousal when they
mandatory service participation seems likely to in- experience negative emotion, as well as in attempts to
crease prosocial responding. hide such feelings. Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Carlo,
The findings on the effects of practice and compul- and Miller (1991) found that mothers who emphasized
sory service activities are similar to those obtained by to their sons the need to control their own negative emo-
social psychologists studying compliance (i.e., the tions (e.g., sadness and anxiety) had sons who exhibited
foot-in-the-door effect) in adulthood. Although the facial and physiological (skin conductance and heart
processes underlying the findings for adults are not en- rate) markers of distress when they viewed a sympathy-
tirely clear (Burger, 1999; Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004), inducing film, but reported low distress in reaction to
a common explanation is that engaging in the initial the film. Thus, these boys seemed prone to experience
prosocial behavior changes the actors self-perceptions distress when confronted with others distress, but ap-
about his or her own prosocial disposition or the actors peared not to want others to know what they were feel-
attitude about helpfulness. A self-concept explanation is ing. In contrast, same-sex parents restrictiveness in
consistent with Eisenberg, Cialdini, et al.s (1989) find- regard to emotional displays that could be hurtful to oth-
ing that the effects of an initial helping experience were ers (e.g., gasping at a disfigured person) has been posi-
primarily for children with a rudimentary understanding tively related to elementary school childrens reports of
of trait labels ( because an understanding of traits is nec- dispositional and situational sympathy (Eisenberg,
essary for a stable self-concept) and with Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Carlo, & Miller, 1991). Parents who
Cialdini, et al.s (1987) finding that practice had an ef- discourage their children from expressing emotions
fect only for children old enough to understand consis- hurtful to others may educate their children about the ef-
tency in personality. However, there is little direct fects of emotional displays on others. However, maternal
evidence that a more sophisticated understanding of the restrictiveness in regard to the display of hurtful emo-
stability of personality is necessary for the foot-in-the- tions was associated with distress in kindergarten girls,
door effect to be effective. perhaps because mothers who were restrictive in this re-
It also is possible that engaging in prosocial activities gard with kindergarten girls were less supportive in gen-
enhances subsequent prosocial behavior because the ex- eral. Thus, for younger children, such maternal
perience provides empathic rewards, helping skills, and restrictiveness may reflect age-inappropriate restric-
social approval. Further, investigators have argued that tiveness or low levels of support (Eisenberg, Fabes,
service activities can promote identity formation, a et al., 1992).
sense of personal competence and civic responsibility, Parents can also demonstrate methods of coping with
and the adoption of prosocial norms, as well as opportu- emotions or encourage the use of certain means of cop-
nities to learn about systems of meaning (e.g., about so- ing. Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Carlo, and Miller (1991)
ciety, social injustice; McLellan & Youniss, 2003; Yates found that boys whose parents encouraged them to deal
& Youniss, 1996a, 1996b, 1998). instrumentally with situations causing their own sadness
or anxiety were relatively likely to experience sympathy
Emotion Socialization
rather than personal distress in empathy-inducing con-
Parental practices that help children to cope with their texts. Further, parents encouragement of direct problem
negative emotion in a constructive fashion tend to be solving as a way to cope with emotion has been associ-
associated with childrens sympathy (rather than per- ated with the amount that girls ( but not boys) comfort a
sonal distress) and prosocial behavior. This may be crying infant (Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, et al., 1993).
partly because children who cannot adequately cope Mothers discussions of their own and their chil-
with their emotions tend to become overaroused and ex- drens emotions also seem to relate to childrens vicari-
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 675

ous emotional responding. When mothers verbally related and prosocial responding. Fabes, Eisenberg,
linked the events in an empathy-inducing film with chil- Karbon, Bernzweig, et al. (1994) found that mothers
drens own experiences, children exhibited heightened displays of positive rather than negative emotion while
vicarious emotional responding of various sorts (sad- telling their kindergarten-age children empathy-induc-
ness, distress, and sympathy). Further, mothers refer- ing stories were associated with childrens sympathy,
ences to their own sympathy and sadness and their low personal distress, and relatively high helpfulness
statements about perspective taking or the film protago- on a behavioral task. Mothers displayed more of this
nists feelings or situation were associated with boys positive expressiveness with kindergartners if they
reports of sympathy and sadness (Eisenberg, Fabes, viewed their child as reactive to others distresses.
Carlo, et al., 1992). In addition, mothers reports of try- Thus, it appeared as if mothers were reacting to charac-
ing to find out why their child is feeling badly, helping teristics of their children (i.e., age and emotional vul-
their children talk about negative emotions, and listen- nerability) and were attempting to buffer younger and
ing to their children when they are anxious or upset have vulnerable children from emotional overarousal (also
been associated with girls comforting of an infant see Zhou et al., 2002). In contrast, for second-grade
(Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, et al., 1993). Similarly, children, helpfulness, as well as sympathy and low per-
Belden, Kuebli, Pauley, and Kindleberger (2003) found sonal distress (assessed with physiological and facial
that mothers questions about their childrens emotional measures), were positively associated with a maternal
reactions, states of mind, or interpretations about the style that combined warmth with directing the childs
motivation for a good deed performed by their child in attention to the stories. For older children, buffering of
the past were positively correlated with childrens self- negative emotion may not be necessary, whereas it may
reported empathy. Moreover, Denham and Grout (1992) be important to direct the childs attention to others in
found that preschoolers prosocial behavior at school was a way that does not induce reactance.
positively related to mothers tendencies to explain their In brief, findings are consistent with the view that
own sadness, and Kojima (2000) found that young chil- parental practices that help children regulate their nega-
drens prosocial behaviors with their siblings were posi- tive emotion to avoid becoming overaroused may foster
tively related to the degree to which their mothers made sympathy and prosocial behavior rather than personal
reference to the siblings actions and emotional states. distress. However, there may be a fine line between the
The positive association between parental discussion parental practices that help children regulate and under-
of emotion and prosocial tendencies has not been found stand their own emotion and the practices that overly
in all studies (Eisenberg, Losoya, et al., 2001; Garner, focus childrens attention on negative emotion. More-
Jones, Gaddy, & Rennie, 1997; Eisenberg, Fabes, over, the effects of parental emotion-related practices
Schaller, Carlo, & Miller, 1991). Trommsdorff (1995) likely are moderated by individual differences in chil-
found that German and Japanese mothers who focused drens emotional reactivity, regulation, and other as-
on their childs emotions in stressful situations by ver- pects of temperament and personality.
balizing or matching their emotions had 5-year-old
daughters who were prone to experience distress rather Expression of Emotion and Conflict in the Home
than sympathy when exposed to anothers sadness. Frequency and valence of emotion expressed in the
Trommsdorff suggested that girls who experience too home appear to be linked to childrens prosocial behav-
strong a degree of empathy from their caretaker may ex- ior, albeit in a complex manner. Parental expression of
perience more distress in empathy-inducing contexts be- positive emotion in the family tends to be positively cor-
cause of less developed self-other differentiation. related with childrens prosocial tendencies (Denham &
Another possibility is that some mothers may over- Grout, 1992; Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, Miller, et al.,
arouse their children by focusing too much on distress, 1991; Garner, Jones, & Miner, 1994), a finding that is
with the consequence that the children do not learn to consistent with the modest associations between proso-
regulate their distress. cial behavior and parental support, warmth, and sympa-
It is likely that the manner in which mothers talk thy. However, researchers sometimes have found no
about emotional events partially accounts for the de- relations between familial or maternal positive emotion
gree and valence of the relation between maternal emo- and childrens sympathy (Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo,
tion-related verbalizations and childrens empathy- Troyer, et al., 1992) or prosocial behavior (Denham &
676 Prosocial Development

Grout, 1993). These weak relations may be due to the tional expressions of anger (Denham & Grout, 1992).
relation between parental positive expressivity and Similarly, high levels of familial or maternal dominant
prosocial behavior or sympathy being quadratic or mod- negative emotion (e.g., anger) have been linked to low
erated by childrens dispositional regulation. Valiente levels of sympathetic concern and high levels of per-
et al. (2004) found that moderate (compared with low or sonal distress, both in the United States (Crockenberg,
high) levels of parental positive expressivity were most 1985; Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, et al., 1992) and in In-
highly, positively related to childrens sympathy. donesia (Eisenberg, Liew, & Pidada, 2001).
Culture also may moderate the relation between To summarize, Cummings and his colleagues found
parental expression of positive emotion and childrens that exposure to conflict involving one or both parents,
sympathy. Unlike in the United States, Eisenberg, including ongoing conflict in the home, was related to
Liew, and Pidada (2001) did not find a relation be- increased prosocial reactions toward childrens mothers
tween these two constructs in Indonesia. This finding and siblings ( but not peers; Cummings & Smith, 1993);
may not be surprising given that anthropological and whereas in other studies, reports and displays of mater-
sociological reports indicate that the expression of high nal anger and externalizing emotion tend to be associ-
levels of emotionpositive or negativeis discouraged ated with low levels of peer-directed prosocial behavior
in that culture. and sympathy, as well as high levels of personal distress.
At first glance, findings about negative emotion in Perhaps exposure to adult conflict undermines chil-
the home appear inconsistent and puzzling. Conflict in drens emotional security and induces distress, resulting
the family has been positively associated with prosocial in children coping in ways that are likely to minimize
behavior toward family members. Even very young chil- the stress in their social environment (see Davies &
dren exposed to parental conflict sometimes try to com- Cummings, 1994). Because children frequently cannot
fort or help their parents, and this tendency increases readily escape from conflict in the home, they may at-
with age in the early years (Cummings, Zahn-Waxler, & tempt to alleviate their distress by intervening and com-
Radke-Yarrow, 1984). Further, siblings ( but not peers) forting family members. However, children exposed to
exposed to conflict between their mother and another high intensity or ongoing parental anger may become
adult seem to try to buffer the stress for one another overaroused by others negative emotions and experi-
(Cummings & Smith, 1993). Young children are more ence self-focused personal distress in reaction to oth-
likely to respond with prosocial behavior toward a par- ers negative emotion (see Eisenberg et al., 1994). If this
ent, as well as with anger, distress, and support-seeking, were true, they would be expected to try to escape from
if familial conflict is frequent (Cummings, Zahn- dealing with others distress if possible. Exposure to
Waxler, & Radke-Yarrow, 1981) or is physical in nature high levels of anger and conflict may induce attempts by
(Cummings, Pellegrini, & Notarius, 1989). children to minimize self-related negative emotional
Other investigators have examined the relation of (and physical) consequences of conflict but likely does
prosocial tendencies to reported prevalence of hostile, not foster the capacity for sympathy or other-oriented
negative emotion in the home environment or maternal (rather than self-oriented) prosocial behavior.
simulations of anger situations. Some investigators have Another reason for the inconsistency in the general
not found significant relations between mothers reports pattern of findings for parental expression of dominant
of dominant negative affect or their own anger directed (assertive) negative emotion may be that the relation be-
toward the child and childrens observed prosocial be- tween parental expression of dominant negative emotion
haviors (Garner & Estep, 2001; Garner, Jones, & Miner, in the family and childrens sympathy appears to be
1994; also see Hastings et al., 2000). In contrast, Den- quadratic, with moderate levels of expressivity being
ham and her colleagues found that preschoolers real-life most highly associated with childrens sympathy (Va-
prosocial reactions to their peers emotional displays liente et al., 2004). Valiente and colleagues also found a
were negatively related to mothers reports of the fre- quadratic relation such that childrens personal distress
quency of their own anger at home (Denham & Grout, was higher for mean and high levels of parental negative
1992) and intense maternal simulations of anger (when expressivity than for low parental negative expressivity.
enacting events in a photograph; Denham et al., 1994), In addition, the relation of parental negative expressivity
and were positively related to mothers reports of the ra- to childrens sympathy appears to be moderated by chil-
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 677

drens regulation. Valiente and colleagues found a signif- home were positively related to childrens peer-oriented
icant negative relation between situational sympathy and prosocial behavior if mothers expressed their tension in
parents negative expressivity, but only for children high a positive manner or explained their sadness.
in regulation. Furthermore, for children who were mod-
Summary of Research on Adults Socialization-
erate or low in regulation, dispositional personal distress
Relevant Practices, Beliefs, and Styles
was relatively high regardless of the level of parental ex-
pression of negative emotion, whereas for well-regulated A constellation of parental practices, beliefs, and char-
children, personal distress was low when parents ex- acteristics, as well as the emotional atmosphere of the
pressed little negative emotion but increased with the home, seems to be related to childrens prosocial devel-
level of parental expression of negative emotion. opment. The findings generally are consistent with
Negative emotions need not always be harsh and dom- Staubs (1992, 2003) assertion that the development of
inant; often emotions such as sadness, fear, and loss are prosocial behavior is enhanced by a sense of connec-
expressed in the home. The findings about the relation tion to others (e.g., through attachment and a benign
between the childrens exposure to parents softer nega- social environment), exposure to parental warmth
tive emotions and their prosocial tendencies are incon- (which fosters a positive identity and sense of self as
sistent. In studies of children from typical families, well as attachment), adult guidance, and participation
maternal report of such submissive negative emotion has in prosocial activities. Moreover, parents coaching and
been negatively related to childrens caregiving toward a other behaviors that teach children to understand and
younger sibling (Garner, Jones, & Miner, 1994), posi- regulate their emotions also are likely related to sym-
tively related to girls ( but not boys) sympathy in the pathetic capacities.
United States (Eisenberg, Fabes, Carlo, Troyer, et al., Although it is likely that the social environment of
1992), and negatively related to Indonesian childrens children, especially their parents, has a causal effect on
sympathy (Eisenberg, Liew, & Pidada, 2001). Further, prosocial behavior and empathy-related responding,
preschoolers prosocial reactions to peers emotions heredity may partially account for such relations, espe-
have been related to mothers low rather than high inten- cially when predicting aspects of prosociality based on
sity enacted sadness (Denham et al., 1994). In contrast, the experience of empathic emotion (see Caspi &
childrens peer-oriented prosocial actions have not been Shiner, Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). It is pos-
significantly related to frequency of mothers reported sible that prosocial, sympathetic parents have prosocial
expressions of sadness or tension at home in front of children because of shared genetic predispositions to-
their child (Denham & Grout, 1992) or mothers reports ward regulation and emotionality. Moreover, biologi-
of experiencing internalizing negative emotions (Den- cally based dispositions (e.g., as partly reflected in
ham & Grout, 1993). temperament) undoubtedly play a major role in em-
Findings about maternal depression are also mixed. pathic and prosocial functioning. However, Plomin et al.
Maternal depression has been linked to lower levels of (1993) found that nonshared (unique) environmental ex-
childrens prosocial behavior in general (Dunn et al., perience accounted for some consistency and for the
1998), to lower mother- and, to a lesser degree, teacher- substantial degree of change in twins empathy over the
reported prosocial behavior but higher child-reported early years of life. Similarly, as discussed, there is evi-
prosocial behavior (Hay & Pawlby, 2003), and to higher dence of shared and especially unshared environmental
empathy or prosocial behavior for some children in variance in the prediction of empathy-related respond-
some circumstances (Radke-Yarrow et al., 1994; Zahn- ing and prosocial behavior (e.g., Deater-Deckard, Dunn,
Waxler, Cummings, McKnew, & Radke-Yarrow, 1984). et al., 2001; Zahn-Waxler et al., 2001). For example, dif-
Perhaps what is important is whether such emotion is ferences in parenting (i.e., warmth versus harsh parent-
dealt with constructively in the home and if children ing) partly explain differences in the prosocial behavior
learn ways to manage emotions such as sadness so that of monozygotic twins, especially for parents who treat
they are likely to experience sympathy rather than per- their twins quite differently (Asbury et al., 2003; also
sonal distress when exposed to others negative emo- see Deater-Deckard, Pike, et al., 2001). Further, genetic
tion. Denham and Grout (1992) found that mothers explanations cannot account for findings in experimental
reported expressions of tension or fear and sadness at studies in which parents were not involved (e.g., many of
678 Prosocial Development

the studies on modeling, preaching, attributions for siveness to unfamiliar younger children (Berman &
helping, directives, and learning by doing). In brief, al- Goodman, 1984).
though biological factors, including genetics, play a Because older siblings often act as caregivers to
major role in prosocial development, environmental fac- younger siblings, the sibling relationship provides chil-
tors also play an important role and undoubtedly interact dren with opportunities to learn about others needs and
with biological factors. caring effectively for others. In addition, children with
Most researchers who have studied socialization cor- supportive sibling relationships may be less preoccupied
relates of prosocial responding have taken into account with their own feelings of distress, so that they are bet-
only the effects of parental behaviors and characteristics ter able to attend to and understand the feelings and
on children; the role of the childrens behavior and char- need states of otherspromoting prosocial behavior and
acteristics in the socialization process has been virtually action (Sawyer et al., 2002). The link between the pres-
ignored. Yet, as was demonstrated by Valiente et al. ence of siblings and prosocial behavior is not always
(2004), it is highly likely that childrens personality and consistent, and it has been argued that the quality of the
temperament interact with parental characteristics and sibling relationship may be more predictive of childrens
beliefs in determining the quality of the parent-child re- positive behavior than the mere presence of siblings in
lationship and parental socialization efforts. Consistent the home (Cutting & Dunn, 1999).
with the possibility of child effects, adults use more rea- As suggested, the childs ordinal position in the sib-
soning about the consequences of actions and less bar- ling dyad probably affects opportunities and expecta-
gaining with material rewards to induce prosocial tions for prosocial behavior. Older children are more
behavior for children who are responsive and attentive likely to enact prosocial behaviors directed toward
than for children who are not (Keller & Bell, 1979). The younger siblings and younger siblings accept reciprocal
role of the child and dyadic processes (e.g., mutual par- roles by displaying high rates of compliance and model-
ent-child responsivity) in the socialization of prosocial ing (Dunn & Munn, 1986; Stoneman et al., 1986). More-
behaviors is a key topic for further attention. over, there is evidence that older sisters are particularly
likely to engage in prosocial interactions with their
siblings (Sawyer et al., 2002; Stoneman et al., 1986;
Other Familial and Extrafamilial Inf luences
Whiting & Whiting, 1975; cf. Brody, Stoneman, &
People and institutions other than parents in childrens MacKinnon, 1986). Due to gender roles, older girls may
environments are potential socializers of childrens be expected to help, comfort, and teach younger siblings.
prosocial actions. Research on the role of nonparental Tucker and colleagues (Tucker, Updegraff, McHale, &
influences is still in the rudimentary stages, and re- Crouter, 1999) found that older siblings personal quali-
searchers studying environmental influences seldom ties and sibling relationship experiences were related to
have simultaneously examined multiple familial models the empathy of younger sisters, but not younger brothers.
(including multiple family members) or multiple types By early adulthood, people are less defensive about ac-
of potential socializers (e.g., peers and the school con- cepting aid from a sister, particularly from an older sis-
text). (For a discussion of the effects of television, see ter, than from a brother (especially a younger brother;
Huston & Wright, 1998). Searcy & Eisenberg, 1992).
Siblings prosocial behavior may be related in degree,
Siblings although the data are sparse and inconsistent. In a study
Because siblings are familiar and relatively uninhibited of Japanese children, siblings prosocial behaviors to-
with one another, they would be expected to play a con- ward one another were positively related (Kojima,
siderable role in the development of childrens social un- 2000). In contrast, Dunn and Munn (1986) found little
derstanding and interpersonal skills, including prosocial correlation between older and younger siblings proso-
behavior (Dunn & Munn, 1986). Even 1- to 2-year-old cial behavior (also see Bryant & Crockenberg, 1980), al-
children exhibit prosocial behavior toward their siblings though younger siblings cooperation and prosocial
(Dunn & Kendrick, 1982). Preschool-age children enact behavior were positively related to older siblings giving
relatively high rates of comforting behavior to dis- and cooperation 6 months later. Furthermore, in that
tressed younger siblings (Howe & Ross, 1990; Stewart & study, siblings who expressed negative affect in a high
Marvin, 1984), but show relatively low rates of respon- percentage of their interactions were relatively unlikely
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 679

to behave prosocially with one another (cf. Stillwell & lation (although perspective taking was related to
Dunn, 1985, using a small sample). friendly behavior between siblings). In addition, Garner,
Characteristics of siblings may affect the degree of Jones, and Palmer (1994) found that emotional role-tak-
prosocial behavior between them. For example, sibling ing skills, but not cognitive perspective taking, pre-
relationships in families of children with autism were dicted sibling caregiving behavior. Perspective taking
characterized by less intimacy, prosocial behavior, and about emotions may be a more relevant skill for sibling
nurturance than those that occurred between typically caregiving than is cognitive perspective taking, al-
developing siblings or a typical child and a sibling with though the latter has been emphasized in most studies of
Down syndrome (Kaminsky & Dewey, 2001). Children perspective taking and sibling interactions. A relation
with autism rarely seek out others for comfort, affec- between perspective taking and siblings prosocial be-
tion, or help, decreasing the likelihood that siblings re- havior may be partly because high perspective-taking
spond in a helpful and affectionate way (Knott, Lewis, siblings are especially likely to be asked by parents to
& Williams, 1995). Thus, when one sibling has diffi- take care of younger siblings (Stewart & Marvin, 1984).
culty initiating, maintaining, or promoting positive in- In summary, sibling interactions may be an important
teractions, prosocial and nurturing sibling interactions context for learning caregiving behaviors (particularly
are likely to be negatively affected. for older siblings) and the development of perspective
Because sibling relationships are embedded in the taking. However, little is known about the ways in which
family, it is not surprising that mothers behaviors are the larger familial context moderates the development of
linked to prosocial behavior between siblings. When prosocial responding in the sibling relationship.
mothers discussed their newborns feelings and needs
with an older sibling, the older child was more nurturant Peer Influences on Prosocial Development
toward the infant. Further, friendly interest in the infant Developmental theorists frequently have tied the acqui-
persisted and predicted prosocial behavior toward the sition of morality to processes inherent in social interac-
younger sibling 3 years later (Dunn & Kendrick, 1982). tions with peers (Piaget, 1932/1965). These theorists
Kojima (2000) found that Japanese mothers references have argued that because peer interactions involve the
to the actions or emotional states of a sibling were posi- association with equals and, frequently, cooperation,
tively correlated with the other childs prosocial inter- reciprocity, and mutuality, peer interaction may provide
actions with that sibling. In another study, nurturant an optimal atmosphere for the acquisition of concepts
maternal responsiveness to young daughters needs was and behaviors reflecting justice, kindness, and concern
positively related to younger siblings comforting and for anothers welfare (Youniss, 1980). Consistent with
sharing with their older sibling. In contrast, mothers this view, Tesson, Lewko, and Bigelow (1987) found that
unavailability was associated with older daughters prosocial themes pertaining to issues such as reciproc-
prosocial behavior toward their younger sibling (Bryant ity, sincerity and trust, helping and solving problems,
& Crockenberg, 1980). The latter finding is similar to and sensitivity to others feelings were prominent in 6-
Brody et al.s (1986) finding that maternal valuing of a to 13-year-old childrens reports of the social rules they
separate life from children was associated with older used in peer relationships. Additionally, having at least
siblings helping and managing their younger sibling. one reciprocated friendship has been related to higher
Perhaps older siblings, especially daughters, are ex- levels of prosocial behavior (Wentzel, Barry, & Cald-
pected to take a nurturant helping role when the mother well, 2004).
is unavailable relatively often. Researchers also have found that the quality of chil-
Because sibling caregiving provides children with op- drens prosocial behavior directed toward peers and
portunities to learn about others perspectives and emo- adults differs somewhat, particularly at younger ages.
tions, children with sibling caregiving experience may When asked to give examples of kindness directed to-
develop relatively mature perspective-taking skills and ward peers, 6- to 14-year-olds tended to cite giving and
therefore respond relatively appropriately and effec- sharing, playing, physical assistance, understanding, and
tively in caregiving situations (see section on perspec- teaching. In contrast, they cited primarily being good or
tive taking). Stewart and Marvin (1984) found a positive polite, doing chores, and obeying in regard to kindness
relation between perspective taking and sibling caregiv- toward adults (Youniss, 1980). Further, preschoolers
ing; however, Howe and Ross (1990) did not find this re- provide more authority- and punishment-related reasons
680 Prosocial Development

for complying with adults than peers requests, and ceived frequent rewards than the behavior of a nonre-
more other-oriented or relational (friendship, liking) warding peer. In contrast, children with a history of in-
motives for complying with peers requests (Eisenberg, frequent peer reinforcement imitated the prosocial
Lundy, et al., 1985). With age, children appear to be behavior of a nonrewarding rather than a rewarding peer
slightly more likely to define kindness toward adults in a (Hartup & Coates, 1967). Thus, characteristics of the
manner similar to peer-directed kindness; that is, as in- child and the peer model influence whether children im-
volving acts demonstrating concern rather than compli- itate peers prosocial actions.
ance (Youniss, 1980). Thus, peer interactions may Peers sometimes respond in a reinforcing manner to
provide a context that is conducive to the development of peers prosocial actions (Eisenberg, Cameron, Tryon, &
prosocial behavior motivated by other-oriented concerns Dodez, 1981), and such reinforcement may affect chil-
rather than compliance, particularly for prosocial ac- drens prosocial behavior. Eisenberg et al. (1981) found
tions directed toward individuals outside the family. that preschool girls ( but not boys) who engaged in rela-
Other research also is consistent with the notion that tively high levels of spontaneous prosocial behavior
peer interactions are important for the development of were those who received marginally more positive rein-
empathy, sympathy, and an other-orientation. According forcement for their prosocial actions from peers. How-
to maternal reports, infants and toddlers cry more in re- ever, preschoolers (especially boys) who were high in
sponse to cries of peers than of adults (Zahn-Waxler, compliant (requested) prosocial actions received low
Iannotti, & Chapman, 1982). Children observed adults levels of positive reinforcement for their compliant
cry relatively infrequently, and when they did, they gen- prosocial actions. Sociable children were relatively
erally did not cry. When children cried in response to likely to receive positive peer reactions when they en-
adults distress, it usually was in reaction to angry inter- acted compliant prosocial actions, and children who re-
actions such as fights between parents. Moreover, proso- sponded positively to other childrens spontaneous
cial behavior (when it occurred) was enacted more often prosocial behaviors were likely to receive positive peer
in response to a childs than to an adults distress. reactions for their own spontaneous and compliant
Peers also may affect prosocial development because prosocial behavior. Thus, children who were more socia-
of their roles as models. Adolescents who volunteer are ble and positive may have elicited the most peer rein-
relatively likely to have friends who feel it is important forcement when they engaged in prosocial behavior. A
to engage in activities such as sports, clubs, or school cyclical process may occur in which socially competent
events (Huebner & Mancini, 2003), to do well in school, children elicit more positive peer reactions for prosocial
and to be involved in community and volunteer work behavior, which in turn increases their prosocial behav-
(Zaff et al., 2003). In contrast, adolescents are relatively ior (with the reverse process occurring for children low
unlikely to report the intention to volunteer if they be- in social skills).
long to a crowd that places a high value on having fun Related to this cyclical process, Fabes, Martin, and
(Youniss, Mclellan, & Mazer, 2001; also see Pugh & Hanish (2002) analyzed the degree to which low- and
Hart, 1999). Although such data are only correlational high-prosocial children (i.e., those at least 1 standard
and do not demonstrate causality, prosocial peer models deviation below or above the mean in teacher-reported
sometimes have been found to be effective in eliciting prosociality) interacted with each other. Rarely were
prosocial behavior in the laboratory (e.g., Owens & As- low and high prosocial children observed interacting
cione, 1991). Familiarity and liking of peer models may with each other (about 5% of the time). Fabes et al. re-
be important factors in influencing childrens prosocial ferred to this as a type of prosocial segregation. Of
behavior: Children may have greater identification with importance, the more exposure that preschool children
fellow peers and may experience more freedom to try had to prosocial peers at the beginning of the school
out new behaviors with peers than they do with adults. year, the greater the degree of positive peer interactions
However, findings in this regard are sparse and are not later in the school year. In a longitudinal extension of
readily interpretable (see Owens & Ascione, 1991). In these analyses, Fabes, Moss, Reesing, Martin, and Han-
one study, children with a history of receiving social re- ish (2005) found that exposure to prosocial peers was re-
inforcement from peers were more likely to model the lated to heightened prosocial behavior 1 year later. In
donating behavior of a peer from whom they had re- addition, Wentzel et al. (2004) found that students with
Socialization Within and Outside the Family 681

initially low levels of prosocial behavior relative to those Mivtzari, and Shavit (1978) found that Israeli kinder-
of their friends improved when exposed to their more gartners enrolled in classes that contained younger peers
prosocial peers, and students with initially higher levels were more altruistic than those enrolled in classes that
of prosocial behavior decreased their levels of prosocial were homogeneous in age. Further, elementary school
behavior when exposed to their less prosocial peers. Israeli students in active classrooms in which coopera-
Such findings demonstrate the potential potency of tion and individualized learning were emphasized
peers as influences on the subsequent likelihood of helped peers more than students in traditional class-
prosocial and positive development. rooms (Hertz-Lazarowitz et al., 1989).
Peer interactions seem to provide unique opportuni- Some investigators have tried to assess the effects of
ties for prosocial behavior, and peer responses in such preschool and day care on childrens prosocial develop-
contexts may influence the type and degree of potential ment by comparing children who attend preschool with
prosocial responses. The role of peer interaction in older those who do not (e.g., are reared at home). Clarke-
childrens and adolescents prosocial behavior has sel- Stewart (1981) suggested that attendance at group day
dom been examined and little is known about the degree care has a temporarily accelerating effect on social de-
to which the effects of peers are moderated by other velopment and found that prosocial behavior was higher
variables (e.g., the nature of interactions with other so- for children with nonparental care. However, evidence in
cializers and characteristics of the child, the peer group, support of this contention is equivocal. Schenk and
or the context) or the exact processes that underlie such Grusec (1987) found that home-care children were more
influences. likely than day-care children to behave prosocially in
situations involving an adult stranger, whereas the two
School Programs groups were similar on helping unknown children. Other
Children likely receive considerable moral education and researchers have produced results indicating that out-of-
training in school, but little is known about the effects of home care per se does not have any reliable or consistent
school experiences on childrens prosocial behavior. effects on childrens emerging prosocial development
One avenue for examining the potential impact of the (Austin et al., 1991).
school context on childrens prosocial behavior is to as- Although differences between home versus group
sess the natural occurrence of prosocial behavior in the care children may be limited, quality of the caregiving
classroom. Hertz-Lazarowitz (1983; Hertz-Lazarowitz, situation likely moderates the degree and type of influ-
Fuchs, Sharabany, & Eisenberg, 1989) found that natu- ence preschools have on childrens prosocial behavior
rally occurring prosocial behaviors in school classrooms and attitudes (Love et al., 2003). Quality of the day care
(Grades 1 to 12) were relatively rare (only 1.5% to 6.5% or preschool environment has been associated with chil-
of total behaviors). Similarly, researchers usually have drens self-regulation (Howes & Olenick, 1986), empa-
noted low frequencies of prosocial behavior in preschool thy and social competence (Vandell, Henderson, &
classes, although estimates vary considerably with the Wilson, 1988), considerateness (Phillips, McCartney, &
operationalization of prosocial behavior (e.g., Caplan & Scarr, 1987), and positive peer-related behaviors (in-
Hay, 1989; Denham & Burger, 1991; Eisenberg et al., cluding prosocial behaviors; Broberg, Hwang, Lamb, &
1981; Fabes et al., 2002; Strayer, Wareing, & Rushton, Ketterlinus, 1989). Moreover, warm, supportive interac-
1979). Further, in studies of preschoolers, teachers tions with teachers have been associated with preschool
rarely reinforced (Eisenberg et al., 1981) or encouraged childrens modeling of teachers prosocial actions
(Caplan & Hay, 1989) childrens prosocial behavior. (Yarrow et al., 1973), sympathetic-prosocial reactions to
Findings such as these suggest that the typical class- distress (Kienbaum, Volland, & Ulich, 2001), and posi-
room environment may not be conducive to eliciting fre- tive interactions among students in the elementary
quent prosocial interactions among children. Salient and school classroom (Serow & Solomon, 1979). In addition,
unambiguous expectations regarding prosocial behavior Howes, Matheson, and Hamilton (1994) found that chil-
may be necessary to elicit more spontaneous prosocial dren classified as securely attached to their current and
actions in the classroom. Moreover, structuring classes first preschool teachers were rated as more considerate
to provide children with opportunities to help others and empathic with unfamiliar peers than were children
may promote prosocial behavior. Bizman, Yinon, classified as having an insecure relationship (especially
682 Prosocial Development

ambivalent) with their teachers. Contemporaneous teachers to develop their techniques and fully integrate
teacher-child relationships better differentiated peer-re- the program into the ongoing routine of the classroom.
lated outcomes for children than did contemporaneous In another longitudinal test of the effects of the CDP,
maternal attachment relations or day-care history. Thus, the program was used with a cohort of students who
degree and type of influence exerted by school experi- began in kindergarten and continued through eighth
ences, as well as durability of effects on prosocial re- grade (Solomon, Battistich, & Watson, 1993). Of partic-
sponding, probably varies as a function of quality of care ular interest, measures of prosocial reasoning and con-
received and the childs relationship with the teacher (as flict resolution were obtained each year. Comparison
well as quality of care received from parents at home). students reasoned higher than CDP children at kinder-
Based on the previously described literature concern- garten, but CDP students reasoned at higher levels from
ing the socialization of prosocial attitudes and behavior, first grade on, although the within-year difference was
some investigators have attempted to design school- significant only in second grade. In general, CDP stu-
based programs aimed at fostering prosocial respond- dents also evidenced higher conflict resolution scores
ing. Solomon and colleagues (Solomon, Battistich, than comparison students (indicating consideration of
Watson, Schaps, & Lewis, 2000; Solomon, Watson, others needs and a reliance on compromise and shar-
Delucchi, Schaps, & Battistich, 1988) developed a pro- ing). Program effects appeared to be greater when com-
gram (The Child Development Project, henceforth re- bined across years (effects were not consistently
ferred to as the CDP) in which teachers were trained to significant within years). However, the CDP initially
maintain positive personal relationships with their stu- was implemented in schools with mostly advantaged
dents by using a child-centered approach to classroom Caucasian children. More recently, the CDP was imple-
management that emphasized inductive discipline and mented in six school districts over a 3-year period, with
student participation in rule-setting. Other aspects of two additional schools in each district serving as com-
the program were designed to promote social under- parison groups (Battistich, Schaps, Watson, Solomon, &
standing, highlight prosocial values, and provide helping Lewis, 2000; Solomon et al., 2000). For those schools
activities; however, these program components were that made significant progress in implementing the pro-
viewed as playing a more limited, supportive role in the gram, students showed positive gains in personal, so-
program (Battistich, Watson, Solomon, Schaps, & cial, and ethical values, attitudes, and motives, and a
Solomon, 1991). reduction of substance abuse and other problem behav-
Across 5 consecutive years of implementation iors (also see Battistich, Solomon, Kim, Watson, &
( kindergarten through fourth grade), students in the Schaps, 1995).
program classrooms, compared with control classes, Other school-based programs have been designed to
generally scored higher on ratings of prosocial behavior. promote empathy. Although some seem to have been
These patterns held when both teachers general compe- minimally effective (e.g., Kalliopuska & Tiitinen,
tence and students participation in cooperative activi- 1991), Feshbach and Feshbach (1982) found that empa-
ties were controlled, suggesting that program effects on thy training significantly increased incidents of proso-
childrens prosocial behavior were not due simply to dif- cial behavior in schoolchildren. Moreover, the use of
ferences in teacher-initiated cooperative interactions or cooperative educational techniques in classroom activi-
to more efficiently organized and managed classrooms ties has been found to promote acceptance of others
(Solomon et al., 1988). (Johnson & Johnson, 1975), as well as cooperation and
Children enrolled in the program ( but not children in prosocial behavior (Hertz-Lazarowitz & Sharan, 1984;
the control group) evidenced the highest ratings for Hertz-Lazarowitz, Sharan, & Steinberg, 1980).
prosocial behavior and harmony in kindergarten. Thus, Some researchers have developed school-based pro-
it appears that the impact of this program was greatest grams that include a formal curriculum component. As-
when first introduced. The degree to which program ef- cione (1992) studied the effects of a humane education
fects generalized beyond the immediate classroom envi- program when used with first, second, fourth, and fifth
ronment was unclear (Battistich et al., 1991). However, graders for nearly 40 hours over the school year. There
the teachers in the program had only 1 year of experi- was relatively little evidence of an immediate effect for
ence in implementing the program and the effects may younger children, although there was an effect on hu-
have been more sustained given additional time for mane attitudes a year later (Ascione & Weber, 1993).
Cognitive and Sociocognitive Correlates of Prosocial Development 683

Humane attitudes were enhanced for the fourth graders gence (e.g., IQ, vocabulary or reading skills, language
in the immediate posttest and for fourth and fifth development, developmental level) and self-reported
graders a year later. Human-directed empathy increased (Carlo, Hausmann, Christiansen, & Randall, 2003; Cas-
for fourth and fifth graders on both the initial and 1- sidy, Werner, Rourke, Lubernis, & Balaraman, 2003;
year posttests. Hart et al., 1998; Ma & Leung, 1991; also see Goodman,
In summary, although prosocial behavior often may 1994) or other measures of prosocial behavior (Krebs &
not be directly promoted in the classroom, quality early Sturrup, 1982; Slaughter, Dennis, & Pritchard, 2002;
schooling and supportive relationships between chil- van der Mark et al., 2002; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1982; also
dren and their teachers have been associated with the see Lourenco, 1993; Zaff et al., 2003). Grade point aver-
development of prosocial tendencies. Moreover, school- age also has been linked to prosocial goals and behavior
based programs designed to enhance prosocial values, (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli, Bandura, & Zim-
behaviors, and attitudes in children can be effective in bardo, 2000; Huebner & Mancini, 2003; Johnson et al.,
fostering childrens prosocial attitudes and behaviors. 1998; Uggen & Janikula, 1999; Wentzel, 2003; Zeldin &
However, most programs have involved relatively weak Topitzes, 2002; also see Lichter et al., 2002), as have
and short interventions that may not be adequate for teachers ratings of school performance combined with
some groups of children. Variation in instruction grades (e.g., Welsh, Parke, Widaman, & ONeil, 2001).
among teachers within a treatment group often is prob- In addition, there is some support for a positive relation
lematic, as is the application of these programs to large between scores on achievement tests and childrens em-
and diverse samples. These issues are critical if one pathy (Feshbach, 1978) or sympathy (Wise & Cramer,
hopes to argue that such programs are cost-effective 1988), and between academic self-efficacy and proso-
and impactful, especially in contexts where resources cial behavior (Bandura et al., 2001, 2003). Not surpris-
and time are limited. ingly, given the array of measures used, some
researchers have found no significant relations between
tests of intelligence (or scholastic ability) and childrens
COGNITIVE AND SOCIOCOGNITIVE prosocial behavior (e.g., Jennings, Fitch, & Suwalsky,
CORRELATES OF PROSOCIAL 1987; Turner & Harris, 1984) or have obtained mixed or
DEVELOPMENT inconsistent relations with prosocial behavior (e.g.,
Strayer & Roberts, 1989) or sympathy (Wise & Cramer,
Numerous theorists have hypothesized that cognitive 1988). Intelligence and academic skills likely are asso-
and sociocognitive skills, particularly perspective tak- ciated with certain types of prosocial responding or
ing and moral reasoning, foster prosocial responding prosocial behavior in some contexts.
(Batson, 1991; Eisenberg, 1986; Hoffman, 1982). More-
over, although not discussed, it is likely that certain
Perspective Taking and Understanding of Emotion
types of prosocial experiences provide experiences that
enhance childrens sociocognitive skills (see Eisenberg, As noted, it is commonly assumed that perspective-tak-
1986, for a review of childrens understanding of, and ing skills increase the likelihood of individuals identify-
attributions about, their own and others kindness). ing, understanding, and sympathizing with others
distress or need (e.g., Batson et al., 2003; Eisenberg,
Intelligence, Cognitive Capacities, and Shea, et al., 1991; Feshbach, 1978; Hoffman, 1982).
Academic Achievement Hoffman (1982) proposed that improvement in young
childrens perspective taking is critical to childrens
Because cognitive abilities may underlie the ability to abilities to differentiate between their own and others
discern others needs or distress, as well as the capacity distress and to accurately understand others emotional
to devise ways to respond to others needs, it would be reactions. These skills are believed to foster empathy
logical to expect a modest relation between measures of and sympathy and, consequently, more and higher qual-
intelligence and prosocial responding, particularly ity prosocial behavior.
prosocial behavior involving sophisticated cognitive Information about others internal states can be ob-
skills. Some investigators have obtained modest to mod- tained by imagining oneself in anothers position or
erate positive correlations between measures of intelli- through processes such as accessing stored knowledge,
684 Prosocial Development

mental associations, and social scripts or deduction tained when age was controlled (e.g., Garner, Jones, &
(Karniol, 1995). Children also may have theories Palmer, 1994; see Underwood & Moore, 1982). As
about others internal states that they use to infer how might be expected, the relation seems to be stronger
others feel (see Eisenberg, Murphy, & Shepard, 1997). when there is a match between the type of perspective-
For convenience, and because it generally is difficult to taking skills assessed and the type or level of under-
identify the processes underlying performance on per- standing likely to promote prosocial behavior in the
spective-taking tasks, the term perspective taking is given context (Carlo, Knight, Eisenberg, & Rotenberg,
used to refer to the ability to engage in any of these 1991). In some circumstances, perspective-taking skills
processes when they result in knowledge about others may be unimportant because prosocial actions are en-
internal states. acted in a relatively automatic fashion due to either their
Researchers have found an association between per- low cost or the compelling, crisis-like nature of the situ-
spective taking ( broadly defined, and including an un- ation. In other contexts, prosocial behavior likely is mo-
derstanding of theory of mind) and prosocial behavior tivated by any number of factors other than knowledge
(including comforting skills) or empathy/sympathy, al- of anothers internal states.
though findings sometimes have been obtained for only Some people may take others perspectives but lack
some of the examined associations (e.g., Bengtsson, the motivation, skills, or social assertiveness required to
2003; Bengtsson & Johnson, 1992; Bosacki, 2003; Carlo take action. Thus, the relations of measures of perspec-
et al., 2003; Cassidy et al., 2003; Charbonneau & Nicol, tive taking or emotion understanding with prosocial re-
2002; Denham, Blair, et al., 2003; Denham & Cou- sponding are likely moderated by other variables.
choud, 1991; Denham et al., 1994; Dekovic & Gerris, Perspective taking has been linked to prosocial behavior
1994; Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; Eisenberg, Zhou, & for children who are socially assertive (Barrett &
Koller, 2001; Estrada, 1995; Garner & Estep, 2001; Yarrow, 1977; Denham & Couchoud, 1991), but not for
Garner, Jones, & Miner, 1994; Ginsburg et al., 2003; children who are less assertive. Similarly, the relation of
Kumru & Edwards, 2003; Litvack-Miller, McDougall, perspective taking to prosocial behavior sometimes has
& Romney, 1997; Roberts & Strayer, 1996; Slaugher been mediated or moderated by childrens empathic/
et al., 2002; Strayer & Roberts, 2004b; also see Eisen- sympathetic responding (Barnett & Thompson, 1985;
berg & Fabes, 1998; Matsuba & Walker, 2005). Al- Roberts & Strayer, 1996). In one study, children who
though no such association has been found in a minority donated money to help a child who had been burned
of studies (e.g., Astington & Jenkins, 1995; Hughes, were those who not only evidenced relatively sophisti-
White, Sharpen, & Dunn, 2000; Lalonde & Chandler, cated perspective-taking skills, but also were sympa-
1995; Peterson, 1983; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1982), to our thetic and understood units and value of money (Knight,
knowledge, perspective taking seldom has been signifi- Johnson, Carlo, & Eisenberg, 1994). In another study,
cantly negatively related to childrens prosocial behav- perspective taking was not directly related to reported
ior (e.g., Barrett & Yarrow, 1977; LeMare & Krebs, prosocial behavior; it was indirectly related through its
1983, for low assertive boys only). Moreover, the match prediction of both sympathy and moral reasoning
between childrens facial reactions and reported reac- (Eisenberg, Zhou, & Koller, 2001).
tions to empathy-inducing stimuli ( believed to reflect In summary, children with higher perspective-taking
emotional insight) has been positively related to their skills generally are somewhat more prosocial, particu-
empathy (Roberts & Strayer, 1996), whereas young ado- larly if their perspective-taking abilities are relevant to
lescents self-understanding has been associated with the prosocial task and if they have the social skills (e.g.,
high levels of prosocial behavior (Bosacki, 2003). assertiveness) and emotional motivation (e.g., sympa-
Positive findings were obtained in many studies al- thy) to act on the knowledge obtained by perspective
though most researchers used single measures of per- taking. Perspective-taking skills may be involved in dis-
spective-taking abilities or prosocial behavior rather cerning others needs, providing sensitive help, and
than more reliable indexes created by aggregation across evoking the affective motivation for prosocial action
measures. The association does not seem to be due (i.e., sympathy, empathy, or guilt). Moreover, it is likely
merely to increases in both perspective taking and that children with well-developed perspective-taking
prosocial behavior with age; often the age range of the abilities have more opportunities to be prosocial; for ex-
study participants was narrow or findings were main- ample, older siblings with better perspective-taking
Cognitive and Sociocognitive Correlates of Prosocial Development 685

skills are more frequently asked by their mothers to which one might expect associations between general
provide caregiving to younger siblings (Stewart & Mar- level of moral reasoning and observed prosocial actions.
vin, 1984). However, prosocial behavior motivated by a particular
type of factor (e.g., sympathy) is likely to be correlated
Person Attributions and Expressed Motives with the types or levels of reasoning reflecting that fac-
tor, although not necessarily with an individuals overall
Although children sometimes may report socially desir- level of reasoning.
able motives or may have little access to their motives In published studies involving child participants,
(see Eisenberg, 1986, for a discussion of these issues), prosocial behavior has been inconsistently related to as-
there appears to be some relation between childrens ex- pects of Piagets scheme of moral judgment (e.g., inten-
pressed motives and the quantity (e.g., Bar-Tal, Raviv, tionality, distributive justice), but generally (albeit not
et al., 1980) or quality (i.e., maturity; see Bar-Tal, consistently) positively related to Kohlbergian prohibi-
1982) of their prosocial behavior (see Eisenberg, 1986, tion- and justice-oriented moral reasoning (or modified
for a review). As discussed by Eisenberg (1986), it is un- versions thereof; see Eisenberg, 1986; Eisenberg &
clear whether childrens motives influence their proso- Fabes, 1998; Underwood & Moore, 1982). However,
cial responding or if children formulate motives post there appears to be a stronger correspondence between
hoc to the execution of behavior based on self- moral reasoning and prosocial behavior if the moral rea-
observation. In support of the former explanation, Smith soning dilemma concerns reasoning about prosocial be-
et al. (1979) found that individual differences in ex- havior rather than another type of behavior. Levin and
pressed internality of motives were associated with do- Bekerman-Greenberg (1980) found that the strength of
nating, whereas environmental contingencies (e.g., the positive relation between reasoning about sharing
rewards and punishments) that might influence post hoc and actual prosocial behavior was somewhat greater if
evaluations were not. In any case, it is likely that people the dilemma and sharing task were similar in content.
have greater access to their cognitive processes (includ- Moreover, when researchers have assessed childrens
ing motives) when a task is not so overlearned that it can moral reasoning about dilemmas involving helping or
be performed in a mindless manner. Therefore, it is sharing behavior, generally moral reasoning has been as-
probable that expressed motives are more accurate for sociated in the predicted manner with at least some mea-
prosocial acts that are not performed automatically; that sures of prosocial behavior (e.g., Carlo & Randall, 2002;
is, when the potential benefactor must consider whether Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; Eisenberg, Miller, et al.,
to assist. At this time, data to test this idea are not avail- 1991; Eisenberg, Zhou, & Koller, 2001; Janssens &
able (see, however, Eisenberg & Shell, 1986). Dekovic, 1997; Kumru et al., 2003; Larrieu & Mussen,
1986; Stewart & McBride-Chang, 2000; also see Eisen-
Moral Reasoning berg, 1986, and Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998, for reviews).
In addition, children who reason at developmentally ma-
In general, investigators have hypothesized that there ture levels are less likely than children who reason at
should be some link between childrens moral reasoning lower levels to say they would discriminate between
and their behavior. Krebs and Van Hesteren (1994) as- people close to them and others when deciding whether
serted: [A]dvanced stages give rise to higher quantities to help (Eisenberg, 1983; also see Ma, 1992).
of altruism than less advanced stages because they give Types of reasoning that reflect an other- versus self-
rise to greater social sensitivity, stronger feelings of re- orientation or are developmentally mature for the age
sponsibility, and so on. . . . We propose that advanced group are most likely to predict prosocial responding.
stage-structures give rise for forms of altruism that are Hedonistic reasoning and needs-oriented reasoning (i.e.,
(1) purer (i.e., more exclusively devoted to enhancing the rudimentary other-oriented reasoning) tend to be nega-
welfare of others, as opposed to the self ) and (2) deeper tively and positively related, respectively, to prosocial
(i.e., that benefit others in less superficial and less tran- behavior (e.g., Carlo et al., 1996; Carlo et al., 2003;
sient ways) than less advanced structures (p. 136). Eisenberg, Boehnke, et al., 1985; Eisenberg, Carlo,
Prosocial actions can be motivated by a range of con- et al., 1995; Eisenberg, Miller, et al., 1991; Eisenberg &
siderations, including altruistic, pragmatic, and even Shell, 1986). In addition, sometimes a mode of reason-
self-oriented concerns; this attenuates the degree to ing that is relatively sophisticated for the age group
686 Prosocial Development

(Carlo & Randall, 2002; Miller, Eisenberg, Fabes, & for hospitalized children and who were relatively high
Shell, 1996; Schenk & Grusec, 1987) has been signifi- in use of needs-oriented reasoning were especially
cantly associated with prosocial behavior. It is possible likely to help hospitalized children at a cost to them-
that the relation of moral reasoning and prosocial behav- selves. Affective motivation such as sympathy (and per-
ior increases with age across adolescence (Pratt et al., haps guilt) often may be necessary to spur the
2004) because moral reasoning becomes more mature individual to action. Thus, it is important to identify
and internalized with age (Eisenberg, 1986). moderators and mediators of the relation between moral
The nature of the enacted prosocial behavior also reasoning and prosocial responding.
seems to be a critical variable. Higher level self-reported
internalized prosocial moral reasoning tends to be posi- EMPATHY-RELATED EMOTIONAL
tively correlated with adolescents reports of altruistic RESPONDING
prosocial actions and helping in emotional and anony-
mous situations, whereas lower level reasoning (i.e., As noted, psychologists (e.g., Eisenberg, 1986; Fesh-
approval-oriented or hedonistic) tends to be related posi- bach, 1978; Hoffman, 1982; Staub, 1979) and philoso-
tively to reported public helping and negatively to altru- phers (Blum, 1980; Hume, 1748/1975; Slote, 2004) have
ism or helping in emotional or dire circumstances (Carlo, proposed that prosocial behavior, particularly altruism,
Hausmann, et al., 2003). In observational studies, proso- often is motivated by empathy or sympathy. Links be-
cial moral reasoning most often has been significantly tween empathy or sympathy and prosocial behavior have
positively related to preschoolers spontaneous sharing been presumed to exist both within specific contexts
behaviors rather than helping behaviors (which, in these (e.g., Batson, 1991; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990) and at the
studies, generally entailed little cost) or prosocial behav- dispositional level (i.e., people with a dispositional ten-
iors performed in compliance with a peers request dency toward empathy/sympathy are expected to be al-
(Eisenberg et al., 1984; Eisenberg-Berg & Hand, 1979). truistic in general; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987).
Preschoolers spontaneous prosocial behaviors predict Although many psychologists have assumed that em-
a prosocial, sympathetic orientation across childhood pathy plays a role in prosocial behavior, in a meta-ana-
and into early adulthood (Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., lytic review, Underwood and Moore (1982) found that
1999, 2002). empathy was not significantly related to prosocial be-
In laboratory studies involving elementary or high havior. Many of the studies they reviewed were con-
school students, prosocial moral reasoning more fre- ducted with children, and most involved a particular
quently has been associated with prosocial actions that type of measurethe picture/story measure of empathy.
incur a cost (e.g., donating or volunteering time after With this type of measure, children are presented with a
school) than with those low in cost (e.g., helping pick up series of short vignettes, usually illustrated (rather than
dropped paper clips; Eisenberg, Boehnke, et al., 1985; videotaped), about children in emotionally evocative
Eisenberg & Shell, 1986; Eisenberg, Shell, et al., 1987; contexts (e.g., when a child loses his or her dog). After
also see Miller et al., 1996). Eisenberg and Shell (1986) each vignette, the child is asked, How do you feel? or
hypothesized that low-cost behaviors are performed a similar question. If children say they felt an emotion
rather automatically, without much cognitive reflection, similar to that which the story protagonist would be ex-
moral or otherwise. In contrast, moral reasoning is pected to feel, they typically are viewed as empathizing.
likely to be associated with childrens prosocial behav- The validity of this sort of measure has been ques-
ior in situations involving a cost because consideration tioned, in part because these measures were not very
of the cost may evoke cognitive conflict and morally rel- evocative (see Eisenberg & Lennon, 1983; Lennon,
evant decision making. Eisenberg, & Carroll, 1983). In fact, the degree of asso-
It also is likely that other variables moderate the re- ciation between measures of empathy-related respond-
lation between moral judgment and prosocial behavior, ing and prosocial behavior appears to vary as a function
particularly for lower level modes of reasoning (at of the measure of empathy. In a meta-analytic review of
higher levels, moral principles may be sufficient moti- the literature, Eisenberg and Miller (1987) found no sig-
vation to help). Sympathetic responding is a probable nificant relation between prosocial behavior and pic-
moderator. Consistent with this view, Miller et al. ture/story measures (or childrens self-reported
(1996) found that preschoolers who reported sympathy reactions to enactments or videotapes of others in dis-
Empathy-Related Emotional Responding 687

tress or need). In contrast, there were significant posi- markers of sympathy tended to be relatively prosocial
tive associations with prosocial behavior for some non- when given an opportunity to assist someone in the
self-report measures of empathy-related responding and film or people similar to those in the film (e.g., hospi-
self-report measures for older adolescents and adults. At talized children). In contrast, children who exhibited
the time of the Eisenberg and Miller review, there were evidence of personal distress ( higher heart rate or skin
few published studies including facial or physiological conductance) tended to be less prosocial (Eisenberg,
measures or the use of questionnaires with children in Fabes, et al., 1993; Eisenberg, Fabes, et al., 1990;
preschool or early elementary school. Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, Carlo, et al., 1996;
In recent years, it has become clear that it is essential Eisenberg, Fabes, Miller, et al., 1989; Fabes, Eisen-
to differentiate among empathy-related emotional reac- berg, Karbon, Bernzweig, et al., 1994; Fabes, Eisen-
tions. Batson (1991) hypothesized that sympathy (as de- berg, Karbon, Troyer, & Switzer, 1994; Miller et al.,
fined at the beginning of this chapter, although labeled 1996). Self-report measures in these studies tended to
empathy by Batson) is intimately linked with other- be less consistently related to childrens prosocial be-
oriented motivation and, consequently, with other-ori- haviors (see Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990). Fabes, Eisen-
ented, altruistic helping behavior. In contrast, personal berg, and Eisenbud (1993) also found that skin
distress is viewed as involving the egoistic motivation of conductance (a marker of personal distress) predicted
alleviating ones own distress; therefore, it is expected girls ( but not boys) low dispositional (rather than sit-
to motivate prosocial behavior only when the easiest uational) helpfulness (i.e., parental ratings of helpful-
way to reduce ones own distress is to reduce the others ness rather than prosocial behavior in the same
distress (e.g., when one cannot easily escape contact context). Moreover, facial reactions of sympathy have
with the empathy-inducing person). been linked to prosocial behavior in another context
Consistent with his theorizing, Batson and his col- (Eisenberg, Fabes, et al., 1990; Eisenberg, McCreath,
leagues, in laboratory studies with adults, have found & Ahn, 1988).
that sympathy is more likely to be positively associated As one would expect, not all markers of sympathy or
with helping than is personal distress when it is easy personal distress in Eisenberg, Fabes, and their col-
for people to escape contact with the person needing leagues research predicted prosocial behavior (or some-
assistance (see Batson, 1991). In a series of studies, times for both sexes; e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon,
Eisenberg, Fabes, and their colleagues obtained similar Murphy, Carlo, et al., 1996; Miller, et al., 1996). In addi-
findings with children. In their studies, childrens tion, heart rate markers of reactions to empathy-inducing
prosocial behavior was as anonymous as possible and films predicted prosocial behavior within, but not across,
children did not have to interact in any way with the contexts (e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, et al., 1990). Nonethe-
needy other(s) if they did not want to do so. Eisenberg less, the overall pattern of findings is consistent. Fur-
et al. (1994) argued that people tend to experience per- ther, other investigators have obtained similar findings.
sonal distress when they are physiologically over- Zahn-Waxler and her colleagues found that sympathetic
aroused, whereas they experience sympathy when they concern and prosocial actions seemed to co-occur in the
experience moderate vicarious arousal. Thus, the re- behavior of children aged 14 and 26 months (Zahn-
searchers hypothesized that high levels of autonomic Waxler, Robinson, & Emde, 1992; Zahn-Waxler et al.,
arousal would be associated with personal distress, 2001) and 4 to 5 years (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1995), al-
whereas the reverse would be true for sympathy (except though self-distress in reaction to anothers emotion
when low arousal is likely an index of no empathy-re- (Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, & Emde, 1992) and arousal
lated responding, especially to a mild stimulus). In ad- (Zahn-Waxler et al., 1995) were unrelated to prosocial
dition, heart rate deceleration tends to occur when behavior in toddlers (also see Trommsdorff, 1995).
individuals are oriented to information in the environ- Zahn-Waxler et al. (1995) also found (a) childrens heart
ment outside the self; this is another reason one might rate deceleration during exposure to sadness (at the peak
expect an association between experiencing sympathy interval) was associated with 3 of 4 measures of proso-
and heart rate deceleration. Across studies in which cial responding, and ( b) behavioral / facial measures of
children were shown empathy-inducing videotapes, concerned attention were positively related to prosocial
children who exhibited facial or physiological (i.e., behavior directed toward the target of concern. Simi-
heart rate deceleration or lower skin conductance) larly, Volling (2001) found that preschoolers who turned
688 Prosocial Development

their backs or moved away from a distressed younger sib- self-reported empathy are not highly consistent (e.g.,
ling were also more likely to display personal distress re- Larrieu & Mussen, 1986; Strayer & Roberts, 1989; also
actions. Trommsdorff and Friedlmeier (1999) reported see Roberts & Strayer, 1996). However, empathy ques-
that German childrens facial sympathy was positively tionnaires often contain items that may reflect personal
correlated with intensity of observed helping, unless distress or sympathy in addition to empathy. Childrens
they were distracted by another task. self-reported personal distress on questionnaires tends
Preschoolers personal distress reactions also have not to be related to childrens prosocial behavior (e.g.,
been positively related to the childrens tendency to en- Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; Eisenberg, Miller, et al.,
gage in compliant, requested prosocial behaviors in 1991; Litvack-Miller et al., 1997), although a weak neg-
other contexts (Eisenberg, Fabes, et al., 1990; Eisenberg ative relation was obtained with adolescents (Estrada,
et al., 1988). Compliant prosocial behavior, in contrast 1995). It may be that questionnaire measures of personal
to spontaneously emitted prosocial behavior, has been distress, which have been adapted from work with
correlated with low assertiveness, low levels of positive adults, are not optimal for children.
peer reinforcement, low levels of positive response to In brief, recent research findings are consistent with
peers prosocial actions, and low levels of social inter- the conclusion that sympathy and sometimes empathy
action. Children high in compliant prosocial responding, (depending on its operationalization) are positively re-
especially boys, seem to be nonassertive and perhaps lated to prosocial behavior, whereas personal distress,
are viewed as easy targets by their peers (Eisenberg particularly as assessed with nonverbal measures, is
et al., 1981; Eisenberg et al., 1988; Larrieu, 1984). Un- negatively related (or unrelated for self-reports) to
like frequency of spontaneous sharing, young childrens prosocial behavior. As might be expected, there is more
compliant prosocial behaviors generally do not predict evidence of associations within contexts than across
their sympathy at older ages, although there are a few contexts, although children with a sympathetic disposi-
correlations of compliant sharing with self-reported tion appear to be somewhat more prosocial in general
measures evident in adolescence and early adulthood than are other children. In addition, there is evidence
(Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 1999, 2002). It is possible that the relation of sympathy to prosocial behavior is
that young children who exhibit high levels of compliant moderated by dispositional perspective taking (Knight
behavior with peers are relatively low in social compe- et al., 1994) and moral reasoning (Miller et al., 1996).
tence and emotion regulation, and engage in requested Thus, it is important to identify dispositional and situa-
prosocial behaviors as a means of curtailing unpleasant tional factors that influence when and whether empa-
social interactions. thy-related situational reactions and dispositional
Studies since Eisenberg and Millers (1987) review characteristics are related to prosocial behavior.
support the view that questionnaire measures tapping
empathy (Albiero & Lo Coco, 2001; Eisenberg, Miller,
DISPOSITIONAL AND PERSONALITY
et al., 1991; Eisenberg, Shell, et al., 1987; Hoffner &
CORRELATES OF PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Haefner, 1997; cf. Stewart & McBride-Chang, 2000),
sympathy (Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; Eisenberg,
Some, but not all, aspects of personality likely have a
Miller, et al., 1991; Estrada, 1995; Knight et al., 1994;
substantial genetic basis. Thus, some of the research on
Litvack-Miller et al., 1997), sympathy and empathy
personality correlates (particularly those viewed as part
combined (e.g., Krevans & Gibbs, 1996), or empathic
of temperament, such as negative emotionality) is rele-
self-efficacy (i.e., perceived ability to experience empa-
vant to an understanding of the constitutional bases of
thy/sympathy; Bandura et al., 2003) are positively re-
prosocial behavior and empathy. Moreover, information
lated to some measures of childrens prosocial behavior
on the personality correlates of prosocial behavior could
in Asian (e.g., in Japan; Asakawa, Iwawaki, Mondori, &
provide clues to the environmental origins of prosocial
Minami, 1987), mid-Eastern (Kumru & Edwards,
behavior when there is evidence of a link between a
2003), or European samples (Bandura et al., 2003), as
given aspect of personality and socialization.
well as in North American samples. Relations between
dispositional empathy or sympathy and prosocial behav- Consistency of Prosocial Behavior
ior seem to be most consistent for self-reported or rela-
tively costly prosocial behavior (Eisenberg, Miller, The assertion that there are personality correlates of
et al., 1991; Eisenberg, Shell, et al., 1987). Findings for prosocial behavior implies a more basic assumption: that
Dispositional and Personality Correlates of Prosocial Behavior 689

there is some consistency in childrens prosocial re- Karbon, Murphy, Carlo, et al., 1996; Hart et al., 2003;
sponding. Consistency of the existence of an altruistic Howes & Farver, 1987; Ingls, Hidalgo, Mendz, & In-
(or moral) personality has been an issue of debate for derbitzen, 2003; Russell et al., 2003; Silva, 1992; cf.
many years and continues to be discussed in the social Farver & Branstetter, 1994). Moreover, behavioral inhi-
psychological literature (see Batson, 1991; Eisenberg, bition at age 2 years has been associated with lower em-
Guthrie, et al., 2002). The empirical findings are re- pathy and prosocial behavior, especially with strangers
viewed in some detail in other sources (Eisenberg & (Young, Fox, & Zahn-Waxler, 1999). In one study, how-
Fabes, 1998; Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997) and, conse- ever (Volling et al., 2004), preschoolers who were high
quently, are merely summarized briefly here. in social fear were relatively likely to provide caregiving
Although findings differ considerably across mea- to a young sibling during a separation from mother, per-
sures of prosocial responding and age, there is evidence haps because they were especially likely to experience
of modest consistency across situations and time. Evi- their siblings distress and were not inhibited in interac-
dence of consistency is weakest in studies of infants and tions with the sibling.
preschoolers (e.g., Dunn & Munn, 1986; Eisenberg There is some reason to believe that early adolescents
et al., 1984; Strayer & Roberts, 1989), but sometimes high in evaluative concerns are more prosocial and less
modest or even compelling evidence of consistency has aggressive toward others (if one controls for depression;
been obtained (e.g., Denham et al., 1994; Gill & Rudolph & Conley, 2005). Perhaps children and youth
Calkins, 2003; Kienbaum et al., 2001; Robinson et al., prone to social anxiety are particularly likely to engage
2001; van der Mark et al., 2002). Although nonsignifi- in prosocial behavior with those they know; they also
cant correlations have been obtained in some studies may be more easily socialized to comply with adults
(e.g., Koenig et al., 2004), positive relations among mea- expectations for prosocial behavior. In one of the few
sures of prosocial or empathy-related responding, across other studies of adolescents, social anxiety was posi-
situations, raters, or time, often have been obtained in tively correlated with dispositional personal distress but
studies of elementary school children (e.g., Dekovic & not sympathy (Davis & Franzoi, 1991).
Janssens, 1992; Hastings et al., 2000; Rushton & Teach- Sociability is particularly likely to be associated
man, 1978; Strayer & Roberts, 1997b; Tremblay, Vitaro, with the performance of prosocial behaviors that are
Gagnon, Piche, & Royer, 1992, Vitaro, Gagnon, & Trem- spontaneously emitted (rather than in response to a re-
blay, 1990, 1991; Warden, Cheyne, Christie, Fitz- quest for assistance; Eisenberg et al., 1981; Eisenberg
patrick, & Reid, 2003; Welsh et al., 2001) and et al., 1984; Eisenberg-Berg & Hand, 1979) or directed
particularly adolescents (Davis & Franzoi, 1991; Eberly toward an unfamiliar person in an unfamiliar setting
& Montemayor, 1999; Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; (rather than a familiar person at home; Stanhope et al.,
Goodman, 2001; Savin-Williams, Small, & Zeldin, 1987; Young et al., 1999). Further, extroversion (which
1981; Wentzel, 2003). Given the diversity of motives includes an element of sociability) was related to ele-
likely to be associated with prosocial- and empathy-re- mentary school childrens helping in an emergency when
lated responses, it is impressive that investigators fre- another peer was present ( but not when the child was
quently have found significant relations across alone) and to helping that involved approaching the other
situations or time, even if many are modest in size. person; introverts tended to help in ways that did not in-
volve approaching the injured individual (Suda & Fouts,
Sociability and Shyness 1980). Thus, sociable children seem to be more proso-
cial than their less social peers when assisting another
Sociability, which likely has a temperamental basis (see involves social initiation or results in social interaction.
Kagan & Fox or Rothbart & Bates, Chapter 3, this
Handbook, this volume), appears to influence if and
Social Competence and Socially
when children assist others. In preschool and beyond,
Appropriate Behavior
children who are prone to participate in activities at
school (Jennings et al., 1987), who tend to approach Because prosocial behavior is socially appropriate in
novel people and things (Stanhope et al., 1987), and who many contexts, it is not surprising that childrens proso-
are sociable and low in shyness, social anxiety, or social cial behavior often is correlated with indexes of socially
withdrawal are somewhat more likely to help than are appropriate behavior. Although not all researchers have
other children (Diener & Kim, 2004; Eisenberg, Fabes, obtained significant results (e.g., Sawyer et al., 2002),
690 Prosocial Development

prosocial children tend to be viewed by adults as so- linked to having few and lower quality friends (Rose &
cially skilled and constructive copers (Cassidy et al., Asher, 2004). Clark and Ladd (2000) obtained concur-
2003; Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, Wosinski, rent relations consistent with the hypothesis that chil-
et al., 1996; Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, et al., 1996; drens prosocial tendencies mediate the relation
Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 1997; Ingls et al., 2003; Pe- between a positive, warm parent-child relationship and
terson, Ridley-Johnson, & Carter, 1984) and are high in childrens peer acceptance and number of mutual
social problem-solving skills (Marsh, Serafica, & friends. In addition, mature prosocial moral reasoning
Barenboim, 1981; also see Warden & Mackinnon, has been positively correlated with sociometric status,
2003), positive social interaction with peers (Farver & as well as with teachers reports of social competence
Branstetter, 1994; Howes & Farver, 1987; also see War- and low levels of acting-out behavior (Bear & Rys,
den & Mackinnon, 2003), developmentally advanced 1994). Thus, children who are prosocial tend to have
play (Howes & Matheson, 1992), and cooperation (e.g., positive relationships and interactions with peers.
Dunn & Munn, 1986; Jennings et al., 1987). In addition, Degree of social competence or popularity also may
sympathy and empathy have been correlated (sometimes affect the types of prosocial behavior children prefer to
over years) with enacted or adult-reported socially com- perform. Hampson (1984) found that popular prosocial
petent behavior (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1995; Eisenberg, adolescents tended to engage in peer-related prosocial
Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, et al., 1996; Murphy, Shepard, behavior, whereas less popular helpers preferred non-
Eisenberg, Fabes, & Guthrie, 1999; see Eisenberg & peer-related tasks. Peer acceptance may affect chil-
Miller, 1987) or with self-reports of number of friends drens comfort level when helping peers; alternatively,
(Coleman & Byrd, 2003). people who prefer to help in ways that do not involve so-
Consistent with the link between socially appropriate cial contact with peers may be less popular due to their
behavior and prosocial behavior, preschoolers prosocial avoidant behavior.
and sympathetic responding have been linked to having
a close friend or more friends (Clark & Ladd, 2000;
Aggression and Externalizing Problems
Coleman & Byrd, 2003; Farver & Branstetter, 1994;
McGuire & Weisz, 1982; Sebanc, 2003; cf. Huebner & Prosocial children are relatively likely to evaluate ag-
Mancini, 2003), supportive peer relationships (de Guz- gression negatively (Nelson & Crick, 1999) and are low
man & Carlos, 2004; Laible et al., 2000; Lerner et al., in aggression and externalizing problems (e.g., Caprara,
2005; Sebanc, 2003), the receipt of prosocial actions Barbaranelli, & Pastorelli, 2001; Caprara et al., 2000;
from peers (Persson, 2005), less conflict with friends Crick, Casas, & Mosher, 1997; Denham, Blair, et al.,
(Dunn, Cutting, & Fisher, 2002), low levels of peer vic- 2003; Diener & Kim, 2004; Goodman, 1994; Hughes
timization (Johnson et al., 2002; cf. Coleman & Byrd, et al., 2000; Ingls et al., 2003; Keane & Calkins, 2004;
2003), and being popular (rather than rejected) with Ma & Leung, 1991; Muris, Meesters, & van den Berg,
peers (Caprara et al., 2000; Clark & Ladd, 2000; Cole- 2003; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001; Uggen & Janikula, 1999;
man & Byrd, 2003; Dekovic & Gerris, 1994; Dekovic & Warden et al., 2003; Welsh et al., 2001; Wilson, 2003;
Janssens, 1992; Denham, Blair, et al., 2003; Eisenberg, also see Haselager et al., 2002; Silva, 1992; Slaughter
Fabes, Murphy, et al., 1996; Hampson, 1984; Keane & et al., 2002; Youniss, McLellan, Su, & Yates, 1999). Re-
Calkins, 2004; Pakaslahti & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, lations are found across time: Hay and Pawlby (2003)
2001; Ramsey, 1988; Slaugher et al., 2002; Tremblay found that externalizing problems at age 4 predicted low
et al., 1992; Warden et al., 2003; Welsh et al., 2001; levels of prosocial behavior at age 11. Furthermore, sym-
Wentzel, 2003; Wilson, 2003; also see Haselager, Cil- pathy (Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, et al., 1996; Laible,
lessen, Van Lieshout, Riksen-Walraven, & Hartup, Carlos, & Raffaelli, 2000; Murphy et al., 1999; Zahn-
2002; LaFontana & Cillessen, 2002; Pakaslahti et al., Waxler et al., 1995) and empathy (Albiero & Lo Coco,
2002; cf. McGuire & Weisz, 1982). Stability of rejection 2001; Braaten & Rosen, 2000; Cohen & Strayer, 1996;
by peers in early elementary school is predicted by low Endresen & Olweus, 2001; Strayer & Roberts, 2004a;
levels of childrens prosocial behavior (Vitaro et al., Warden & Mackinnon, 2003; see Miller & Eisenberg,
1990); childrens skill at comforting predicts whether 1988, for a review) have been linked to low levels of ex-
children are rejected, neglected, or accepted by peers ternalizing problem behaviors (including aggression or
(Burleson et al., 1986); and nonsupportive goals or ADHD). Childrens and adolescents self-reported delin-
strategies in hypothetical help-giving situations are quency and externalizing problem behaviors also have
Dispositional and Personality Correlates of Prosocial Behavior 691

been negatively related to their self-reported empathic pathy versus personal distress reactions (Eisenberg,
efficacy (Bandura et al., 2001, 2003). Fabes, et al., 1990) and prosocial behavior (Barrett &
The relation of prosocial responding to aggression Yarrow, 1977; Denham & Couchoud, 1991; Ingls et al.,
likely varies depending on the actors motive for engag- 2003; Larrieu & Mussen, 1986), particularly sponta-
ing in prosocial behavior. Although prosocial actions neously emitted (unrequested) instances of helping and
that involve a positive affective response to an individ- sharing (Eisenberg et al., 1984; cf. Eisenberg et al.,
ual and those not motivated by personal gain tend to be 1981). A certain level of assertiveness may be necessary
negatively related to adolescents reports of aggression for many children to spontaneously approach others
(and their belief that aggression is acceptable), reports needing assistance. In contrast, nonassertive, nondomi-
of prosocial actions performed for personal gain have nant children tend to be prosocial in response to a re-
been positively related to reported aggressive actions quest (Eisenberg et al., 1981; Eisenberg et al., 1984;
and the acceptance of aggression (Boxer et al., 2004). Larrieu, 1984), apparently because they frequently are
The relation between aggressiveness and prosocial asked for help or sharing (probably due to their compli-
behavior may be more complex in the early years than at ance; Eisenberg et al., 1981; Eisenberg, McCreath, &
older ages. Gil and Calkins (2003) found that aggressive Ahn, 1988). Children who are not simply assertive but
toddlers displayed more evidence of empathy or concern seek to dominate others may be low in prosocial behav-
than less aggressive toddlers. Moreover, Yarrow et al. ior (Krebs & Sturrup, 1984).
(1976) found a positive correlation between prosocial
and aggressive behavior for preschool boys ( but not
Self-Esteem and Related Constructs
girls) below the mean in exhibited aggression, whereas
there was a negative relation between prosocial behavior It appears that there is a positive relation between chil-
and aggression for boys above the mean in aggression. drens self-esteem and their prosocial tendencies, but
For those young children who are relatively nonaggres- more so for older than for younger children. In studies of
sive overall, aggression often may be indicative of as- preschoolers and elementary school children, investiga-
sertiveness rather than hostility or the intent to harm tors typically have found no evidence of a relation be-
another (Eisenberg & Mussen, 1989). Moreover, the tween self-reports of self-esteem or self-concept and
lack of regulation reflected in aggression may allow measures of prosocial behavior (Cauley & Tyler, 1989;
young children to approach and exhibit concern toward Rehberg & Richman, 1989). In studies of children in
an unfamiliar adult (the measure of concern used by Gil fourth grade to high school, investigators generally have
and Calkins, 2003). found that prosocial children have a positive self-
Thus, a negative relation between aggression and concept (Laible & Carlo, 2004; Larrieu & Mussen,
prosocial tendencies may develop with age. Although 1986; Rigby & Slee, 1993; also see Jacobs et al., 2004;
Hastings and colleagues (2000) did not find a relation cf. Huebner & Mancini, 2003; Karafantis & Levy,
between concern for others and the behavior problems of 2004), are high in self-efficacy (Bandura et al., 2001,
4- to 5-year-olds, children with clinical behavior prob- 2003; Lichter et al., 2002; Sugiyama, Matsui, Satoh,
lems decreased in their concern and were reported by Yoshimi, & Takeuchi, 1992), and tend to have prosocial
both mothers and themselves to be relatively low in con- self-schemas (that affect donating when children are
cern by age 6 to 7 years. Moreover, greater concern at 4 self-aware; Froming, Nasby, & McManus, 1998). John-
to 5 years predicted a decline in the severity of external- son et al. (1998) found that girls, but not boys, with
izing problems over the 2 years. Thus, the inverse rela- higher academic and positive self-esteem in ninth grade
tion between sympathy and externalizing problems were more likely to volunteer in grades 10 to 12. Perhaps
seems to begin consolidation during the preschool to young childrens self-reports do not adequately tap rele-
early school years. vant dimensions of their self-concepts. However, it is
also possible that young childrens self-concept often is
Assertiveness and Dominance not based on enduring characteristics that are relevant to
prosocial responding (see Harter, Chapter 9, this Hand-
Assertiveness and dominance also have been associated book, this volume).
with frequency and type of childrens prosocial behav- It also is probable that the relation between self-
iors. Assertive children (e.g., those who issue commands concept or self-esteem and prosocial behavior varies as a
or defend their possessions) are relatively high in sym- function of the psychological significance or quality of
692 Prosocial Development

the prosocial act. Children who are anxious or emotion- parents about the importance of moral values for the self
ally unstable may enact prosocial behaviors to ingratiate, 2 years later than did their less involved peers.
avoid disapproval, or prevent overreactivity to social dis- More generally, there is evidence that prosocial be-
tress. In fact, there is some evidence that boys who are havior is positively associated with measures of moral
particularly high in prosocial behavior performed or functioning, including other-oriented values and beliefs
promised in a public context are anxious, inhibited, and (Dlugokinski & Firestone, 1974; Janoski, Musick, &
emotionally unstable (Bond & Phillips, 1971; OConnor, Wison, 1998; Larrieu & Mussen, 1986); social responsi-
Dollinger, Kennedy, & Pelletier-Smetko, 1979). Simi- bility, responsibility goals, or low levels of irresponsi-
larly, Jacobs et al. (2004) found that although socially bility (Savin-Williams et al., 1981; Wentzel, 2003);
confident adolescents were relatively high in self- integrative goals (i.e., concern with the maintenance
reported prosocial activities, so were anxious adoles- and promotion of other individuals or social groups;
cents. Youth who had a low social self-concept but were Estrada, 1995); guilt or need for reparation (Caprara
not worried about their standing with peers were lower in et al., 2001; Chapman, Zahn-Waxler, Cooperman, &
prosocial activities than the socially confident or anx- Iannotti, 1987); and low levels of moral disengagement
ious adolescents. (Bandura et al., 2001). Further, adolescents sometimes
The association between older childrens self- cite moral values and responsibility for others as rea-
conceptions and prosocial behavior probably is bidirec- sons for enacting prosocial behaviors (e.g., Carlo, Eisen-
tional in causality. Children who feel good about berg, & Knight, 1992; Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995).
themselves may be able to focus on others needs be- Thus, it appears that older children and adolescents who
cause their own needs are being met; further, they may have internalized moral (including altruistic) values and
feel that they have the competencies needed to assist oth- who view morality as central to their self-concept are
ers. In addition, it has been argued that involvement in particularly likely to be altruistic. In addition, prosocial
activities that help others may foster the development of tendencies appear to be linked to relational rather than
self-efficacy (Yates & Youniss, 1996b). It is reasonable instrumental goals (Nelson & Crick, 1999) and to col-
to assume that the performance of socially competent laborative goals in the school environment (Cheung, Ma,
behavior, including prosocial behavior, and childrens & Shek, 1998).
self-concept are complexly related during development. In addition, empathic or sympathetic youth not only
exhibit values and a social conscience (Lerner et al.,
Values and Goals 2005), but also may be more likely than less responsive
youth to extend their prosocial behaviors to members
An important component of the self is ones values. outside their own group. Empathic youth are more likely
Colby and Damon (1992) noted two morally relevant than their less empathic peers to say that they are com-
characteristics that were dramatically evident in adult fortable being near children who are different from
moral exemplars: (1) exemplars certainty or excep- them and who might be viewed negatively (e.g., a child
tional clarity about what they believed was right and who is depressed, immature, aggressive, overweight, or
about their own personal responsibility to act in ways doing poorly academically; Bryant, 1982; cf. Strayer &
consistent with those beliefs; and (2) the unity of self Roberts, 1997a). Similarly, sympathetic youth value di-
and moral goals, that is, the central role of exemplars versity (Lerner et al., 2005), and school children feel
moral goals in their conceptions of their own identity less interpersonal distance from those with whom they
and the integration of moral and personal goals. empathize/sympathize (Strayer & Roberts, 1997a; see,
Consistent with Colby and Damons findings, Hart however, Batson, Chang, Orr, & Rowland, 2002). Inclu-
and Fegley (1995) found that adolescents who demon- sive reactions such as these would be expected to en-
strated exceptional commitments to care for others were hance prosocial behavior directed toward out-group
particularly likely to describe themselves in terms of members (Oliner & Oliner, 1988).
moral personality traits and goals and to articulate the-
ories of self in which personal beliefs and philosophies
Religiosity
were important. Moreover, Pratt et al. (2003) found that
adolescents who were more actively involved in commu- Religiosity (as measured by attending religious services)
nity helping activities reported closer agreement with has been positively related to participation in volunteer
Dispositional and Personality Correlates of Prosocial Behavior 693

activities during adolescence (Huebner & Mancini, personal distress sometimes has been associated with
2003; Lichter et al., 2002) and predicts subsequent vol- low regulation (Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, Karbon,
unteering behavior in early adulthood (Zaff et al., 2003). et al., 1994, Ungerer et al., 1990; Valiente et al., 2004;
Similarly, going to a Catholic or church-based school cf. Eisenberg & Fabes, 1995). The few findings for em-
( but not being Catholic; Youniss, McLellan, Su, & Yates, pathy are mixed, some positive (Sneed, 2002), some not
1999) predicted adolescents community service (Saklofske & Eysenck, 1983). In addition, resilient
(Youniss, McLellan, & Yates, 1999). Because involve- children, who may be viewed as optimally regulated,
ment in church and other community-based youth tend to be prosocial and empathic (Atkins, Hart, &
groups is related to doing volunteer service (McLellan Donnelly, 2005; Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 1997;
& Youniss, 2003), it is likely that religious institutions Strayer & Roberts, 1989; also see Hart et al., 1998). In
provide opportunities for organized prosocial activities. contrast, boys with ADHD were found to be lower on
In addition, Youniss, McLellan, and Yates (1999) argued empathy than boys without a diagnosis of ADHD. Be-
that involvement in church-sponsored services makes it cause children diagnosed with ADHD have low atten-
more likely that youth will internalize or adopt the reli- tional control, these children may be at a disadvantage
gious rationales provided for engaging in service. More for the development of empathy and prosocial behavior
generally, a religious identity, if it involves moral over- (Braaten & Rosen, 2000).
tones, has been linked with a prosocial personality (Fur- It appears that well-regulated children can modulate
row, King, & White, 2004). At this time, it is unclear their vicarious arousal and, consequently, focus their at-
whether prosocial behavior is differentially linked to tention on others emotions and needs rather than on
identification with, or acceptance of, various religions. their own aversive vicarious emotion (Trommsdorff &
Friedlmeier, 1999). Consistent with this idea, Bengtsson
(2003) found that Swedish elementary school students
Regulation
who were high in self-reported empathy and teacher-re-
In studies involving adult-reported or behavioral mea- ported prosocial behavior tended to experience moder-
sures of self-regulation (generally defined in terms of ate (rather than high) levels of threat and to modulate
processes involved in modulating emotional states and the emotional significance of empathy-eliciting stimuli
behaviors; Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Sadovsky, 2005), through cognitive restructuring (which can be viewed as
prosocial children tend to be relatively well regulated, a mode of emotion regulation). Moreover, well-regu-
as well as low in impulsivity (e.g., Eisenberg, Fabes, lated children would be expected to be relatively likely
Karbon, Murphy, Wosinski, et al., 1996; Eisenberg, to sustain their attentional focus on others and to sup-
Fabes, Karlo, Murphy, Wosinski, et al., 1996; Eisen- press any tendencies to try to avoid contact with dis-
berg, Guthrie, et al., 1997; Moore, Barresi, & Thomp- tressed or needy individuals.
son, 1998; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Hershey, 1994; Silva, Findings for measures of physiological emotional regu-
1992; Thompson, Barresi, Moore, 1997; Wilson, 2003; lation are somewhat inconsistent and may vary as a
also see Deater-Deckard, Dunn, et al., 2001). The asso- function of age of the child or evocativeness of the
ciation between regulation and prosociality is not sur- empathy-inducing situation. In the relevant studies, physi-
prising because engaging in prosocial actions often ological emotion-related regulation often is assessed with
requires regulated behavior and emotion (e.g., control- higher heart rate variance, high vagal tone, or vagal sup-
ling ones own negative emotion) or involves actions that pression. These intercorrelated measures, especially the
help regulate others emotions (Bergin, Talley, & latter two, are viewed as reflecting emotion-related physi-
Hamer, 2003). In fact, degree of regulation is a stronger ological regulation based on the control of parasympa-
positive predictor of prosocial behavior for children thetic functioning by the vagal nerve (Porges,
prone to negative emotions such as anger (Diener & Doussard-Roosevelt, & Maiti, 1994; see Rothbart &
Kim, 2004; also see Eisenberg, Guthrie, et al., 1997). Bates, Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume). Such mea-
Similarly, sympathy has been associated with high sures have been positively related with elementary school
levels of childrens regulation (Eisenberg & Fabes, students observed comforting (Eisenberg, Fabes, Kar-
1995; Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, et al., 1994; bon, Murphy, Carlo, & Wosinski, 1996) and dispositional
Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, et al., 1996; Eisenberg, sympathy (Fabes et al., 1993), although findings for girls
Liew, & Pidada, 2001; Murphy et al., 1999), whereas have been positive for maternal report of girls sympathy
694 Prosocial Development

(Fabes et al., 1993), but negative for girls self-reported Branstetter, 1994; Hart et al., 2003), albeit not for some
sympathy (and positive for boys sympathy; Eisenberg, measures of depression or internalizing problems (Ban-
Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, Smith, et al., 1996). dura et al., 2003; Goodman, 1994; Hay & Pawlby, 2003;
Moreover, contrary to expectations, toddlers vagal Muris et al., 2003). In addition, intensity of emotional
suppression in response to a crying infant was negatively responding in general may be negatively related to
related to observed concern in response to an adult prosocial tendencies (Garner & Estep, 2001). However,
feigning distress to an injury (Gill & Calkins, 2003). relations of negative emotionality (intensity and/or fre-
Similarly, Zahn-Waxler et al. (1995) found that pre- quency) to empathy/sympathy have been negative
school childrens concerned reactions during the same (Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, et al., 1996; Eisen-
type of feigned injury task were negatively related to berg, Fabes, Shepard, et al., 1998; Murphy et al., 1999;
their vagal tone. The same childrens vagal tone was Roberts & Strayer, 1996, for anger; Strayer & Roberts,
weakly negatively related to teacher- ( but not parent- or 2004a; van der Mark et al., 2002), nonsignificant
child-reported) prosocial behavior 2 years later (Hast- (Braaten & Rosen, 2000; Denham, Blair, et al., 2003),
ings et al., 2000). Gill and Calkins suggested that a pos- and positive (Saklofske & Eysenck, 1983), although
itive relation between concern and physiological positive findings have been obtained primarily when
regulation might develop with age. Alternatively, it may negative emotionality was measured during the early
be difficult to differentiate between personal distress years and related to empathy (or mixed empathy and
and sympathy with some of the measures (e.g., reactions sympathy) rather than sympathy (Howes & Farver,
to feigned distress) typically used with younger chil- 1987; Robinson et al., 1994; Rothbart et al., 1994).
dren. It is not clear whether the complex pattern of find- There also is some evidence that children who are ex-
ings is due to age-related factors, to differences in the tremely worried about the well-being of family members
measures of prosocial behavior used with younger and are relatively prosocial (Hay & Pawlby, 2003).
older children, or to other moderating factors. Thus, in general, prosocial behavior and sympathy or
empathy have been linked to dispositional positive
emotionality. Further, low negative emotionality has
Emotionality
been consistently associated with childrens prosocial
Children who are emotionally positivea characteristic behavior, but not young childrens empathy/sympathy.
that may be viewed as partly an outcome of emotional The inconsistencies in findings may be partly due to
regulationalso tend to be prosocial (Denham, 1986; both type and intensity of the negative emotion experi-
Denham & Burger, 1991; Eisenberg et al., 1981; Garner enced and type of measure. Relations between negative
& Estep, 2001; also see Bandura et al., 2003; cf. Braaten emotionality and empathy/sympathy or prosocial be-
& Rosen, 2000; Denham, Blair, et al., 2003; Farver & havior seem to be negative especially for externalizing
Branstetter, 1994) and empathic/sympathetic (Eisen- types of emotions (e.g., anger) rather than depression,
berg, Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, et al., 1996; Robinson anxiety, or dysphoric emotions (e.g., Laible et al., 2000;
et al., 1994; also see Eisenberg et al., 1994; cf. Volling, Strayer & Roberts, 2004a). Childrens anger and frus-
Herrera, & Poris, 2004). In contrast, the data pertaining tration seem to be salient to adults and, like aggression,
to the relation between negative emotionality and proso- covary inversely with prosocial behaviors and empathy-
cial responding are more complex. Prosocial behavior related emotions.
generally (albeit sometimes for one sex or the other) has In addition, intensity of negative emotion may be re-
been negatively related to negative emotionality, includ- lated to whether people experience sympathy or per-
ing anger, fear, anxiety, or sadness (Bandura et al., sonal distress, which, in turn, predicts prosocial
2001; Denham, 1986; Denham & Burger, 1991; Diener behavior. Eisenberg et al. (1994) proposed that situa-
& Kim, 2004; Eisenberg, Fabes, Karbon, Murphy, tional emotional overarousal due to empathy is associ-
Wosinski, et al., 1996; Hoffner & Haefner, 1997; Ma & ated with personal distress, whereas moderate empathic
Leung, 1991, Tremblay et al., 1992; Volling et al., 2004; responding is associated with sympathy (also see Hoff-
Wentzel & McNamara, 1999; also see Caprara, Bar- man, 1982). If people can maintain their vicarious emo-
baranelli, Pastorelli, et al., 2001; Strayer & Roberts, tional reactions at a tolerable range, they are likely to
2004a, 2004b; cf. Denham & Burger, 1991; Farver & vicariously experience the emotion of needy or dis-
The Role of Relationship History in Prosocial Behavior 695

tressed others, but are relatively unlikely to become tensity (Eisenberg, Fabes, Murphy, Karbon, et al., 1996;
overwhelmed by the emotion and, consequently, self- also see Eisenberg et al., 1998).
focused. In contrast, people who are overaroused by vi- Thus, there is initial support for the notion that emo-
carious negative emotion are expected to experience tional intensity (including intensity of both positive and
that emotion as aversive and as a distressed, self- negative emotions) interacts with regulation in predict-
focused reaction (personal distress). Consistent with ing childrens sympathy, although the pattern of rela-
this view, general negative emotional arousal has been tions is complex and depends on the type of regulation.
found to result in a self-focus (Wood, Saltzberg, & For children in mid-elementary school, behavioral regu-
Goldsamt, 1990), and empathically induced distress re- lation was positively related to dispositional sympathy
actions are associated with higher skin conductance re- for boys who were average or high, but not low, in the
activity than is sympathy (Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, tendency to experience emotions intensely. In contrast,
Carlo, & Miller, 1991; Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, attentional regulation predicted high dispositional sym-
Miller, et al., 1991). pathy (for both sexes) only for children low in general
Based on this line of reasoning, Eisenberg and col- emotional intensity. For children low in emotional inten-
leagues argued that individual differences in the dispo- sity, attentional control may be important in helping
sitional tendency to experience sympathy versus children focus on and process others emotions and
personal distress vary as a function of dispositional dif- needs (Eisenberg et al., 1998).
ferences in both typical level of emotional intensity and Positive relations between some measures of negative
individuals abilities to regulate their emotional reac- emotionality and empathy/sympathy in the literature
tions. People high in effortful regulation (e.g., who have also may be due to empathic or sympathetic people
control over their ability to focus and shift attention) are being relatively likely to express or report their emo-
hypothesized to be relatively high in sympathy regard- tions (see Roberts & Strayer, 1996), in empathy-induc-
less of their emotional intensity. Well-regulated people ing contexts (Roberts & Strayer, 1996; also see
would be expected to modulate their negative vicarious Eisenberg, Losoya, et al., 2001). In future work on em-
emotion and to maintain an optimal level of emotional pathy-related reactions, it will be useful to differentiate
arousal that has emotional force and enhances attention, among types of negative emotion (e.g., externalizing
but is not so aversive and physiologically arousing that it and internalizing emotions), between expressed (i.e.,
engenders a self-focus. In contrast, people low in the observable) and experienced emotion, and between indi-
ability to regulate their emotion, especially if they are viduals general emotional intensity and the intensity of
emotionally intense, are hypothesized to be low in dis- solely negative emotions.
positional sympathy. Further, measures of tendencies to
display anger and frustration probably partly reflect low
regulation and high emotional reactivity and, conse-
quently, would be expected to relate to personal distress THE ROLE OF RELATIONSHIP HISTORY
and low prosocial behavior. IN PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Modest support has been obtained for these ideas. As
noted, regulation has been linked to high sympathy and The degree to which children are prosocial frequently
low personal distress. Further, low and moderate levels depends on the identity and characteristics of the poten-
of negative emotional intensity, but not high levels, have tial recipient. Children prefer to help people who are
been associated with situational concern (Eisenberg & relatively important in their lives, such as family mem-
Fabes, 1995) and children who experience more nega- bers (e.g., Killen & Turiel, 1998; Rheingold et al., 1976;
tive emotion than that of the stimulus person eliciting van der Mark et al., 2002; Young et al., 1999). In adoles-
empathy (i.e., become overaroused) are relatively low in cence, help is as likely or more likely to be directed to-
empathy/sympathy (Strayer, 1993). In addition, there is ward known peers as toward known, nonfamilial adults
limited evidence that unregulated children are low in (e.g., Zeldin, Savin-Williams, & Small, 1984). More-
sympathy regardless of their level of emotional intensity over, children often share or help friends or liked peers
whereas, for moderately and highly regulated children, more than less liked peers (Buhrmester, Goldfarb, &
level of sympathy increases with level of emotional in- Cantrell, 1992) or acquaintances (Buhrmester et al.,
696 Prosocial Development

1992; Farver & Branstetter, 1994; Pilgram & Rueda- chivalrous assistance. Sex differences in helping were
Riedle, 2002; Rao & Stewart, 1999). In fact, children as inconsistent across studies and were successfully pre-
young as age 4 or 5 years or in elementary school report dicted by various attributes of the studies. Carlo et al.
more sympathy toward the plight of a friend or liked (2003) also found sex differences varied with type of re-
peer than toward an acquaintance (Costin & Jones, ported prosocial behavior: Adolescent girls were more
1992). Prosocial behavior among friends appears to be likely to report altruistic and emotional prosocial behav-
motivated by not only liking and concern (Costin & iors than were boys; boys were more likely to report
Jones, 1992), but also loyalty, consideration of reciproc- prosocial tendencies in public situations; and no sex dif-
ity obligations, and the fact that friends more often ask ferences were found in situations involving anonymous
for sharing or help (Birch & Billman, 1986). or compliant prosocial behavior or helping in dire cir-
Sometimes children are equally prosocial to friends cumstances. Becker and Eagly (2004) examined extreme
and other peers or even help or share less with friends forms of prosocial behaviorheroismand found that
(Berndt, Hawkins, & Hoyle, 1986). In studies in which men were overrepresented in some forms of heroism
children have had to choose between friends and (e.g., Carnegie Hero Fund medalists who engaged in
strangers, children apparently sometimes assisted peo- life-risking rescue actions), but in other heroic actions
ple they did not know well to eliminate inequities be- (organ donors, peace corps volunteers, holocaust res-
tween a stranger and a friend because they believed that cuers), the percentage of women was at least equal to
their friend would understand, they wanted to gain the and, in several cases, higher than that found for men.
unknown persons approval or friendship, or they were Such findings suggest that the qualities associated with
competing with the friend (Berndt, 1982; Staub & No- different types of prosocial behavior (e.g., the role of
erenberg, 1981). risk taking in extremely dangerous heroic acts) more
likely explain differences in males and females ten-
dencies to engage in prosocial actions than a general sex
SEX DIFFERENCES IN CHILDRENS difference model of prosociality per se.
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR Eisenberg and Fabes (1998) reported a meta-analysis
of sex differences in childrens prosocial behavior in-
Based on stereotypic gender roles, females generally are volving 259 studies yielding a total of 450 effect sizes
expected and believed to be more responsive, empathic, (M age = 7.93 years). Only one effect size was used per
and prosocial than males, whereas males are expected to sample (i.e., when different variables were used for a
be relatively independent and achievement oriented single sample, one was selected randomly). For both the
(e.g., Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974). Further, full and partial sample of effect sizes, the mean un-
cross-cultural work has verified that gender differences weighted effect size was modest (.18) and favored girls.
in prosocial responding are not limited to only a few cul- Although effect sizes were significant for all types of
tures and may develop with age. Whiting and Edwards prosocial behavior and for various design, method, or re-
(1973) found that helpfulness and support giving gener- cipient characteristics, they varied in strength by the
ally were greater for girls than boys across six different type of prosocial behavior studied. Sex differences were
cultures, although these differences were significant for significantly greater when prosocial responding was
older but not younger children. More recent work con- measured with self-reports or reports from others than
firms the cross-cultural tendency of girls to be more with observational methods. The effect size also was
prosocial than boys (e.g., Carlo, Reoesch, Knight, & significantly greater for aggregated indices or indices
Koller, 2001; Russell et al., 2003). reflecting kindness/consideration than for indices re-
Despite the prevailing view that females are more flecting instrumental help, comforting, or sharing, and
prosocial than males, findings vary depending on the in correlational /naturalistic studies than in structured/
age of the actor and the type of prosocial behavior. Eagly experimental studies. However, the latter two differ-
and Crowley (1986) conducted a meta-analysis of sex ences disappeared when study characteristics were
differences in older adolescents and adults helping be- controlled in regression analyses, probably because self-
havior and found that men helped more than women, report measures have been used disproportionately in
particularly in situations involving instrumental and assessment of kindness/consideration and aggregated in-
Sex Dif ferences in Childrens Prosocial Behavior 697

dices, and in observational /correlational studies. In ad- 1971; Shigetomi, Hartmann, & Gelfand, 1981). Further-
dition, sex differences in prosocial behavior were signif- more, parents have been found to attribute girls actions
icantly greater when the target was an adult or was to inborn factors significantly more often than boys ac-
unspecified than when the target was another child. tions, whereas boys prosocial actions are more likely to
When controlling for other study or participant vari- be viewed as due to environmental factors (Gretarsson &
ables, the sex difference in prosocial behavior was Gelfand, 1988). These findings are consistent with the
greater for larger samples and when the age span of view that girls reputations for prosocial behavior are
study participants was relatively small. Instrumental greater than the actual sex difference. In addition, chil-
help also was significantly less predictive of sex differ- dren may self-socialize their prosocial tendencies by
ences in prosocial behavior than were other types of means of having their thoughts, emotions, and behav-
prosocial indices. ioral scripts conform to parents, teachers, and peers
These findings support Eagly and Crowleys (1986) expectations (Maccoby, 1998). Nonetheless, there is a
conclusion that sex differences in adults prosocial be- small sex difference favoring girls even in observational
havior vary as a function of the qualities of the studies. studies (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998), so there likely is
In contrast to Eagly and Crowleys findings for adults some truth to the stereotype.
and older adolescents (combined), Eisenberg and Fabes Sex differences in the literature may also be due, in
(1998) found that girls tended to be more prosocial than part, to biases in measures of prosocial behavior.
boys. The finding that the sex difference was weakest Zarbatany, Hartmann, Gelfand, and Vinciguerra (1985)
for instrumental helping is particularly interesting be- argued that measures used to evaluate childrens proso-
cause many of the studies in the adult literature in which cial tendencies include a disproportionate number of
men helped more were assessments of instrumental sex-biased items favoring girls (items pertaining to
helping (Eagly & Crowley, 1986). feminine activities). They found that masculine items
With increasing age, sex differences in prosocial be- (e.g., helping get a cat out of a tree) elicited endorse-
havior tended to get larger (see Eisenberg & Fabes, ments for boys, and feminine-related and neutral items
1998; Fabes, Carlo, Kupanoff, & Laible, 1999). How- elicited endorsements for girls. Masculine items likely
ever, the effect for age in the meta-analysis was elimi- included acts of instrumental helping, the category for
nated once other study qualities were controlled, which there was the smallest sex difference favoring
probably because type of study was associated with age, girls (when study characteristics were controlled) in the
with older children involved in more naturalistic/corre- meta-analysis.
lational studies. Findings about sex differences in empathy and sym-
Since the Eisenberg and Fabes meta-analysis, inves- pathy, like those for prosocial behavior, vary with the
tigators have continued to find sex differences in reports method used to assess empathy-related responding. As
of childrens prosocial behaviors (e.g., Bosacki, 2003; mentioned, Eisenberg and Lennon (1983; also see
Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Pastorelli, 2001). Peers, espe- Lennon & Eisenberg, 1987), in a meta-analytic review,
cially girls, are more likely to nominate girls as being found large differences favoring girls for self-report
prosocial and to nominate boys as being bullies (Warden measures of empathy/sympathy, especially question-
et al., 2003; Warden & Mackinnon, 2003). Fewer differ- naire indices. No gender differences were found when
ences have been found in some observational studies the measure of empathy was either physiological or un-
(Fabes, Martin, & Hanish, 2002; contrast with Zahn- obtrusive observations of nonverbal behavior. In work in
Waxler et al., 2001). To some degree, sex differences in which sympathy and personal distress have been differ-
self- and other-reported prosocial behavior may reflect entiated, investigators have obtained similar findings,
peoples conceptions of what boys and girls are sup- although they occasionally have found weak ( but signif-
posed to be like rather than how they actually behave. icant) sex differences in facial reactions (generally fa-
Parents emphasize prosocial behaviors and politeness voring females; see Eisenberg, Fabes, Schaller, &
more with their daughters than with their sons (Power & Miller, 1989) and in observational assessments of young
Parke, 1986). Moreover, peers, parents, and teachers children using developmentally appropriate stimuli such
tend to perceive girls as more prosocial than either be- as puppets to elicit distress (Kienbaum et al., 2001) or
havioral or self-reported data indicate (Bond & Phillips, feigned distress (Zahn-Waxler et al., 2001). Eisenberg
698 Prosocial Development

and Lennon (1983) suggested that the general pattern of lated to internalized prosocial moral reasoning ( but also
results was due to differences among measures in the related to hedonistic reasoning for males; Carlo et al.,
degree to which both the intent of the measure was obvi- 1996). It is unclear the degree to which these sex differ-
ous and people could control their responses. Sex differ- ences, which generally are relatively weak, are due to
ences were greatest when demand characteristics were real differences in moral reasoning or to differences in
high (it was clear what was being assessed) and individ- the ways that adolescent males and females view them-
uals had conscious control over their responses (i.e., selves and desire to be viewed by others.
self-report indices were used); gender differences were In summary, although girls appear to be more proso-
virtually nonexistent when demand characteristics were cial than boys, the issue of sex differences in prosocial
subtle and study participants were unlikely to exercise responding and their origins is far from resolved. It is
much conscious control over their responding (i.e., phys- difficult to determine the degree to which the sex differ-
iological indices). Thus, when gender-related stereo- ence reflects a difference in moral or other-orientation
types are activated and people can easily control their versus other factors (e.g., self-presentation). It also is
responses, they may try to project a socially desirable unclear whether the sex difference changes with age.
image to others or to themselves. Although age was related to the prosocial effect size in
Eisenberg and Fabes (1998; Fabes & Eisenberg, the univariate analysis in our meta-analysis, there was
1996) also conducted a follow-up meta-analysis of em- no effect of age when study characteristics were con-
pathy/sympathy data published since Eisenberg and trolled. There is a need to better assess the developmen-
Lennons (1983) first review and found an overall un- tal trajectory of the sex differences and to investigate
weighted effect size (favoring girls) of .34. Relatively the origins of sex differences in prosocial behavior.
large effect sizes were found in self-report studies (sig-
nificantly larger than in the studies involving other
methods) and in studies in which the targets of the em- AN INTEGRATIVE MODEL OF
pathic response were unspecified or unknown individu- PROSOCIAL ACTION
als. Moreover, sex differences were larger for older
children. When sex differences were examined by Based on the available evidence, prosocial action ap-
method, significant sex differences favoring girls were pears to be the outcome of multiple individual (includ-
obtained for self-report indices (weighted effect size of ing biological) and situational factors. A simplified
.60) and observational measures (in which a combina- model of the major variables believed to contribute to
tion of behavioral and facial reactions usually were the performance of prosocial behavior (and steps in the
used, .29). The gender difference in observed reactions, process itself ) is depicted in Figure 11.1 (see Eisenberg,
especially for young children, suggests that there is a 1986, for extended discussion of this model). This
real, albeit modest, difference in childrens empathy. No heuristic model can be used to integrate many of the
sex differences were obtained for nonverbal facial and topics discussed in this chapter.
physiological measures. Further, the sex difference in In our model, biological factors are viewed as having
self-reported empathy/sympathy increased with mean an effect on both the childs individual characteristics
age of the sample ( beta = .24). Sex differences in re- (e.g., sociocognitive development, empathy, sociability)
ported empathy may increase as children become more and parental interactions with the child (i.e., socializa-
aware of, and perhaps are more likely to internalize, tion experiences). The childs individual characteristics
sex-role stereotypes and expectations into their self- and socialization experiences affect one another and, to-
image (Karniol et al., 1998). gether with objective characteristics of the situation (see
Although there are no sex differences in prosocial Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998, for a review of situational
moral reasoning in young children, in later elementary influences), influence how the child interprets events in-
school and beyond, girls use more of some relatively so- volving anothers need or distress in a specific context.
phisticated types of prosocial moral reasoning, whereas For example, individual differences in perspective tak-
boys sometimes verbalize more of less mature types of ing and in decoding skills, which likely are influenced by
reasoning (Eisenberg, Carlo, et al., 1995; Eisenberg, socialization experiences as well as heritability (e.g., ge-
Miller, et al., 1991; also see Jaffee & Hyde, 2000). netic effects on intelligence), may affect whether a child
Moreover, in adolescence, femininity is positively re- notices anothers distress, as might the clarity of the dis-
An Integrative Model of Prosocial Action 699

Antecedent Antecedent Motivationally Relevant


Sociocognitive Temporary Relevant Situational Personal
Development, Affective States Evaluations and Characteristics
Dispositional Emotional Reactions
Characteristics,
and Person
Variables

Biology Interpretation of, Identification of Hiearchy of Intention Prosocial Consequences


and Attention to, Helping Actions Personal Goals to Assist Action (Including
the Situation and Recognition in the Specific Self-Evaluation)
(Including of Ability to Engage Situation
Needy Others) in These Actions

Socialization Characteristics Changes in


of the Situation Situation or
Person over Time

Figure 11.1 Heuristic model of prosocial behavior. Adapted from Altruistic Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior, by N. Eisen-
berg, 1986, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

tressed others nonverbal and verbal cues of emotion (a people interpret others verbal statements and facial ex-
situational factor). Moreover, socialization influences pressions (Clark, Milberg, & Erber, 1983).
and person variables likely interact; as discussed, Va- In addition, a child who feels capable of assisting
liente et al. (2004) found that parental expressivity re- must then decide whether he or she intends to assist. The
lated differently to childrens sympathy and personal childs emotional reactions (e.g., sympathy or personal
distress depending on the childrens regulation. It also is distress), relationship with the other person (which af-
likely that antecedent characteristics interact when pre- fects the childs emotional reactions and perceived costs
dicting childrens prosocial tendencies; examples were and benefits of assisting), and attributions about the
discussed in our review (e.g., between regulation and cause of the others need or distress (e.g., whether the
emotional intensity, or between perspective taking and needy person is responsible for his or her situation) are
sympathy; Diener & Kim, 2004; Eisenberg, Fabes, Kar- examples of motivationally relevant situational evalua-
bon, Murphy, Wosinski, et al., 1996; Knight et al., 1994). tions and emotional reactions that can play a role in this
How the child interprets the situation logically leads decision. The decision of whether to engage in prosocial
to and affects the childs identification of prosocial ac- action also is affected by antecedent person variables
tions and the childs recognition of his or her ability to such as individual differences in concern about social
engage in these actions. A temporary state such as the approval, values, personal goals, and self-identity in re-
childs mood may determine his or her attention to, or gard to the trait of altruism (see Figure 11.1).
interpretation of, a situation (see Eisenberg & Fabes, In the given context, the various relevant moral and
1998). Level of arousal seems to alter the ways in which nonmoral factorsbe they perceived costs and benefits,
700 Prosocial Development

values, sympathetic emotion, or other factorsinflu- ing another also may affect socializers efforts to pro-
ence the individuals relative hierarchy of goals in the mote the childs prosocial behavior and the degree to
particular situation. Often goals, needs, or values con- which an individual develops a prosocial self-concept
flict in a situation and must be prioritized. This order- (e.g., Eisenberg, Cialdini, et al., 1987). These conse-
ing of personal goals undoubtedly varies across quences are reflected in the future in terms of the childs
individuals and across situations for a given person (see ongoing dispositional or person variables (see Figure
Figure 11.1). In a situation in which important people 11.1), as well as in the range of the childs prosocial-rel-
are present, social approval needs may be salient (par- evant personal competencies. Thus, there is a cycle by
ticularly for people who value such approval). In an- which childrens prosocial behavior (or the lack thereof )
other situation in which there are material costs for has consequences for future prosocial responding.
assisting, valuing of the object to be shared or donated
will be particularly relevant for some people ( but not
others who do not value the commodity). Moreover, if CHALLENGES AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
the situation evokes an emotional reaction such as sym-
pathy or personal distress, then other- or self-related As is evident in this review, there is considerable re-
goals linked to those emotional reactions will be salient search on antecedents and correlates of childrens
and perhaps activated. prosocial responding. This work has provided a rudi-
The values, goals, and needs that underlie personal mentary understanding of the factors that may foster
goals and their relative importance (in general and in prosocial action, although in many cases, it is premature
specific contexts) change with age (e.g., Bar-Tal, Raviv, to confidently assume causation. Many of the deficien-
et al., 1980). An individuals values, goals, and needs, cies in the research on prosocial development noted in
as expressed in his or her prosocial moral reasoning, 1998 still exist. Although there is more research on
provide some insight into the childs typical hierarchy of some topics (e.g., volunteering, personality/person cor-
goals, needs, and values (i.e., ones general hierarchy relates), the field would benefit from new emphases in
rather than ones hierarchy in a specific context), al- methods, conceptual frameworks, and empirical foci.
though, as noted, different factors will be particularly
salient in different situations (see Eisenberg, 1986). Be-
cause other-oriented values based in part on perspective Methodological Issues
taking, sympathetic reactions, or the capacity for ab-
stract principles increase with age (Eisenberg, 1986), In this chapter, we have discussed a few of the mediators
one would expect prosocial moral goals to rank higher and moderators of the bivariate relations associated
in the hierarchies of older children than in those of with the development of prosocial behavior. There is ini-
young children. tial evidence that regulation mediates the relation be-
Thus, the hierarchy of an individuals goals or priori- tween parental expression of negative emotion and
ties in the particular situation is viewed as determining childrens sympathy (Eisenberg, Liew, & Pidada, 2001)
whether the child wants to assist, as well as the intention and that sympathy and prosocial moral reasoning medi-
to assist. However, even if the child intends to perform a ate the relation of perspective taking to prosocial behav-
prosocial behavior, he or she may not be able to do so ior (Eisenberg, Zhou, & Koller, 2001). A greater focus
due to the lack of relevant personal competencies (phys- on mediation would enhance our understanding of the
ical, psychological, or material) needed to intervene or processes related to prosocial development and behavior.
provide appropriate helping skills. In addition, the situa- Little is known about factors that mediate the relations
tion may change, as might the potential benefactors sit- of parental inductions or assignment of responsibilities
uation, prior to the actual helping opportunity. For to children and their prosocial behavior or empathy-re-
example, the potential benefactor may receive help from lated responding. Consistent with Hoffmans (2000)
someone else before the child can assist. thinking, inductions may affect perspective taking and
Finally, there are consequences of engaging in proso- empathy, which then foster prosocial action. Addition-
cial behavior or choosing not to do so. Children who help ally, certain types of interactions with peers or teachers
may develop new helping competencies or sociocogni- may promote childrens understanding of others emo-
tive skills that can be applied in future situations. Assist- tions and mental states in a manner that in turn fosters
Challenges and Future Directions 701

sympathy. Sympathy may act as a mediator of the rela- laboratory situations, researchers have tended to shy
tion of many environmental influences or genetic predis- away from experiments in the past decade or two. Yet
positions (e.g., regulation) to childrens prosocial experiments, especially those performed in more natu-
behavior. Examination of such mediational processes re- ral settings (e.g., at school), can be valuable in testing
quires that investigators refine their conceptual explana- ideas about causality. Interventions and prevention pro-
tions and go beyond looking at global associations to grams provide a rigorous test of causal relations.
focus on process-oriented explanations. A multimethod approach in the design of studies also
In contrast, a focus on moderation forces investiga- is necessary because different methods address some-
tors to think about the ways in which predictors of what different questions, including questions about
prosocial responding interact in their potential influ- causality. Moreover, all methods of measurement have
ence. The strength of many predictors of prosocial re- limitations, but these differ for different measures.
sponding (e.g., perspective taking or parental use of Thus, the convergence of findings across methods in-
inductions) likely varies based on factors such as sex, creases ones confidence in the veracity of the findings.
age, general parenting style, cultural experiences, per- In addition, as illustrated by the results of the Eisenberg
sonality predispositions, or childrens susceptibility to and Fabes (1998) meta-analyses, certain types of meth-
experience empathy or sympathy. We live in a multi- ods tend to be used with certain ages of children, and
variate world, and prosocial behavior in specific such confounds may undermine our ability to under-
situations, is determined by numerous additive and in- stand the development of prosocial behavior.
teracting factors. For example, as mentioned, Knight
et al. (1994) found that children who donated to needy
Conceptual and Content-Related Directions
children were not only high in perspective taking, but
also high in sympathy and understood the commodity The study of prosocial behavior would benefit from
to be donated (money; also see Eisenberg, Zhou, & greater integration with conceptual work on related is-
Koller, 2001, who found that sympathy and perspective sues. Prosocial behavior can be considered in a manner
taking jointly predicted prosocial moral reasoning). As similar to most interpersonal behaviorsin terms of
noted, parental expression of emotion and childrens its social appropriateness and social and personal out-
regulation also interact when predicting childrens em- comes both in specific situations and in the long term.
pathy-related responding (Valiente et al., 2004). Based In many, but not all, settings, prosocial behavior is a
on the work of Kochanska (1995), who found that chil- socially appropriate behavior; indeed, prosocial behav-
drens temperament moderated the relations between iors frequently are used in measures of social compe-
maternal gentle discipline and measures of conscience, tence. Thus, conceptual work on social competence
it is likely that temperament affects the relations of and the development of interpersonal competence in at-
parental practices to childrens prosocial tendencies. In tachment and peer relationships is relevant to the un-
addition, it is important to go beyond moderational derstanding of prosocial development. Moreover,
models to examine the ways in which configurations of research on moral emotions such as guilt, moral cogni-
numerous variables (e.g., child-rearing practices) pre- tions, and the development of an egoistic or antisocial
dict prosocial outcomes. orientation could be used to a greater degree than
Most of the research on prosocial development con- in the past to inform our understanding of prosocial
tinues to be correlational. To better examine issues of behavior, particularly altruism.
causality, longitudinal designs and structural equation As an example, we have seen that individual differ-
modeling can be used to test causal hypotheses (al- ences in childrens emotionality and their ability to reg-
though structural modeling can only assess if a causal ulate emotional arousal appear to be related to whether
sequence is consistent with the data and does not prove children experience sympathy or egoistic, personal dis-
causality). Longitudinal data are especially important tress in helping contexts. Moreover, enactment of pro-
for testing mediated relations; concurrent data provide a social behaviors often involves not only emotional
weak test of causal, mediated relations. Further, experi- regulation, but also behavioral regulation, particularly if
mental research designs could be used more frequently prosocial action requires self-denial. Thus, developmen-
to test causal assumptions. Although experimental de- tal change and individual differences in childrens abili-
signs usually ( but need not) require relatively artificial ties to inhibit their behavior, delay gratification, and
702 Prosocial Development

activate behavior when desirable are of considerable im- ences would benefit if the boundaries among content
portance to understanding prosocial development. Envi- areas, as well as across disciplines, were more permeable.
ronmental factors associated with optimal regulation
and moderate levels of emotional reactivity likely foster
REFERENCES
prosocial responding, including sympathy. Thus, the
growing bodies of literature on the socialization of emo-
Alberts, J. R. (1976). Olfactory contributions to behavioral develop-
tion and coping, as well as cultural influences on emo- ment in rodents. In R. L. Doty (Ed.), Mammalian olfaction, re-
tion and its regulation, are highly relevant to a productive processes, and behavior (pp. 6795). New York:
comprehensive perspective on prosocial development. A Academic Press.
Albiereo, P., & Lo Coco, A. (2001). Designing a method to assess
better understanding of these issues may be especially empathy in Italian children. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.),
useful in delineating the emergence of sympathy and Constructive and destructive behavior: Implications for family,
prosocial tendencies in the first years of life when chil- school, and society (pp. 205223). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
drens regulatory skills are changing rapidly.
Allen, J. P., Kuperminc, G., Philliber, S., & Herre, K. (1994). Program-
Work on prosocial behavior too often has been iso- matic prevention of adolescent problem behaviors: The role of auton-
lated from work on related topics, and greater integra- omy, relatedness, and volunteer service in the teen outreach
tion across content domains would have broad benefits. program. American Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 617638.
This situation has improved somewhat in the past Allen, J. P., Philliber, S., Herrling, S., & Kuperminc, G. P. (1997).
Preventing teen pregnancy and academic failure: Experimental
decade, especially in the literature on peer relationships evaluation of a developmentally based approach. Child Develop-
and social competence. We have tried to make further ment, 64, 729742.
inroads in that direction in this chapter, although our at- Aronfreed, J. (1970). The socialization of altruistic and sympathetic
tempts were limited by the need to cover much material behavior: Some theoretical and experimental analyses. In J.
Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior
in a restricted space. Moreover, prosocial behaviors can (pp. 103126). New York: Academic Press.
be characterized as attractors (absorbing states that Asakawa, K., Iwawaki, S., Mondori, Y., & Minami, H. (1987, July).
pull the behavior of the system from other potential Altruism in school and empathy: A developmental study of Japa-
states) that affect the organization of individual and nese pupils. Paper presented at the International Society for the
Study of Behavioral Development, Tokyo, Japan.
group behaviors (Martin, Fabes, Hanish, & Hollensetin, Asbury, K., Dunn, J. F., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2003). Nonshared en-
in press). As such, integration of concepts from dynamic vironmental inf luences on individual differences in early behav-
systems may lead to new insights in research and theo- ioral development: A monozygotic twin differences study. Child
Development, 74, 933943.
rizing about prosocial behavior and development.
Ascione, F. R. (1992). Enhancing childrens attitudes about the hu-
Advances in some fields of the behavioral sciences mane treatment of animals: Generalization to human-directed
are just beginning to provide methods and data that can empathy. Antrozoos, 5, 176191.
inform our understanding of prosocial development. De- Ascione, F. R., & Weber, C. V. (1993, March). Childrens attitudes
velopments in brain-scanning procedures are providing about the humane treatment of animals and empathy: One-year
follow up of a school-based intervention. Paper presented at the bi-
new venues for studying emotion, attention, and deci- ennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,
sion making and, hence, processes related to sympathy New Orleans, LA.
and prosocial behavior. This technology may provide Aspinwall, L. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2003a). Introduction. In
new insights on the role of emotion and attentional L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of
human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for
processes in prosocial decision making, although it is a positive psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological
unlikely to provide in-depth information relevant to the Association.
role of antecedent biological and environmental influ- Aspinwall, L. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2003b). A psychology of
ences on prosocial development. human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for
a positive psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Finally, not only has the field of prosocial behavior Association.
been relatively intellectually isolated from relevant liter- Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1995). Theory of mind development
ature on other topics, but investigators studying other is- and social understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 151165.
sues (e.g., psychopathology, information processing, Atkins, R., Hart, D., & Donnelly, T. M. (2005). The association of
childhood personality type with volunteering during adolescence.
peer relationships, academic success) also have not at- Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51, 145162.
tended sufficiently to findings in the domain of prosocial Austin, A. M. B., Braeger, T., Schvaneveldt, J. D., Lindauer, S. L. K.,
development. The broader field of developmental sci- Summers, M., Robinson, C., et al. (1991). A comparison of help-
References 703

ing, sharing, comforting, honesty, and civic awareness for chil- Battistich, V., Watson, M., Solomon, D., Schaps, E., & Solomon, J.
dren in home care, day care, and preschool. Child and Youth Care (1991). The child development project: A comprehensive program
Forum, 20, 183194. for the development of prosocial character. In W. M. Kurtines &
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A so- J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and develop-
cial cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. ment: Vol. 3. Application (pp. 134). New York: Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of Bear, G. G., & Rys, G. S. (1994). Moral reasoning, classroom behav-
moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101119. ior, and sociometric status among elementary school children. De-
velopmental Psychology, 30, 633638.
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino, M., & Pas-
torelli, C. (2003). Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in Becker, S. W., & Eagly, A. H. (2004). The heroism of women and
diverse sphere of psychosocial functioning. Child Development, men. American Psychologist, 59, 163178.
74, 769782. Belden, A., Kuebli, J., Pauley, D., & Kindleberger, L. (2003, April).
Predicting childrens empathy from family talk about past good
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Re-
deeds. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for
galia, C. (2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms gov-
Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
erning transgressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 125135. Benenson, J. F., Markovits, H., Roy, R., & Denko, P. (2003). Behav-
ioural rules underlying learning to share: Effects of development
Barash, D. P. (1977). Sociobiology and behavior. New York:
and context. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
Elsevier/ North-Holland.
27, 116121.
Barnett, M. A., & Thompson, S. (1985). The role of perspective tak- Bengtsson, H. (2003). Childrens cognitive appraisal of others dis-
ing and empathy in childrens Machiavellianism, prosocial be- tressful and positive experiences. International Journal of Behav-
havior and motive for helping. Journal of Genetic Psychology, ioral Development, 27, 457466.
146, 295305.
Bengtsson, H., & Johnson, L. (1992). Perspective taking, empathy, and
Baron, J., & Miller, J. G. (2000). Limiting the scope of moral obliga- prosocial behavior in late childhood. Child Study Journal, 22, 1122.
tions to help: A cross-cultural investigation. Journal of Cross-Cul-
Bergin, C., Talley, S., & Hamer, L. (2003). Prosocial behaviors of young
tural Psychology, 31, 703725.
adolescents: A focus group study. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 1332.
Barrett, D. E., & Yarrow, M. R. (1977). Prosocial behavior, social in- Berman, P. W., & Goodman, V. (1984). Age and sex differences in
ferential ability, and assertiveness in young children. Child Devel- childrens responses to babies: Effects of adults caretaking re-
opment, 48, 475481. quests and instructions. Child Development, 55, 10711077.
Bar-Tal, D. (1982). Sequential development of helping behavior: A Bernadett-Shapiro, S., Ehrensaft, D., & Shapiro, J. L. (1996). Father
cognitive-learning approach. Developmental Review, 2, 101124. participation in childcare and the development of empathy in
Bar-Tal, D., Bar-Zohar, Y., Greenberg, M. S., & Hermon, M. (1977). sons: An empirical study. Family Therapy, 23, 7793.
Reciprocity behavior in the relationship between donor and recip- Berndt, T. J. (1982). The features and effects of friendship in early
ient and between harmdoer and victim. Sociometry, 40, 293298. adolescence. Child Development, 53, 14471460.
Bar-Tal, D., Nadler, A., & Blechman, N. (1980). The relationship be- Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Hoyle, S. G. (1986). Changes in
tween Israeli childrens helping behavior and their perception on friendship during a school year: Effects on childrens and adoles-
parents socialization practices. Journal of Social Psychology, cents impressions of friendship and sharing with friends. Child
111, 159167. Development, 57, 12841297.
Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Leiser, T. (1980). The development of altru- Birch, L. L., & Billman, J. (1986). Preschool childrens food sharing
istic behavior: Empirical evidence. Developmental Psychology, with friends and acquaintances. Child Development, 57, 387395.
16, 516524. Bischof-Kohler, D. (1991). The development of empathy in infants.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psycho- In M. E. Lamb & H. Keller (Eds.), Infant development: Perspec-
logical answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. tives from German-speaking countries (pp. 245273). Hillsdale,
Batson, C. D., Chang, J., Orr, R., & Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, at- NJ: Erlbaum.
titudes and action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized Bizman, A., Yinon, Y., Mivtzari, E., & Shavit, R. (1978). Effects of
group motivate one to help the group. Personality and Social Psy- the age structure of the kindergarten on altruistic behavior. Jour-
chology Bulletin, 28, 16561666. nal of School Psychology, 16, 154160.
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Carpenter, A., Dulin, L., Harjusola- Blum, L. A. (1980). Friendship, altruism and morality. London: Rout-
Webb, S., Stocks, E. L., et al. (2003). . . . As you would have ledge & Kegan Paul.
them do unto you: Does imagining yourself in the others place Boehnke, K., Silbereisen, R. K., Eisenberg, N., Reykowski, J., &
simulate moral action? Personality and Social Psychology Bul- Palmonari, A. (1989). The development of prosocial motivation:
letin, 29, 11901201. A cross-national study. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology,
Batson, C. D., & Powell, A. A. (2003). Altruism and prosocial behav- 20, 219243.
ior. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Bond, N. D., & Phillips, B. N. (1971). Personality traits associated
Vol. 5. Personality and social psychology (pp. 463484). New with altruistic behavior of children. Journal of School Psychol-
York: Wiley. ogy, 9, 2434.
Battistich, V., Schaps, E., Watson, M., Solomon, D., & Lewis, C. Boorman, S. A., & Leavitt, P. R. (1980). The genetics of altruism.
(2000). Effects of the child development project on students New York: Academic Press.
drug use and other problem behaviors. Journal of Primary Preven- Borgida, E., Conner, C., & Manteufel, L. (1992). Understanding kid-
tion, 21, 7599. ney donation: A behavioral decision-making perspective. In S.
704 Prosocial Development

Spacapan & S. Oskamp (Eds.), Helping and being helped Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Bandura, A., & Zim-
(pp. 183212). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. bardo, P. G. (2000). Prosocial foundations of childrens academic
Bosacki, S. L. (2003). Psychological pragmatics in preadolescents: achievement. Psychological Science, 11, 302306.
Sociomoral understanding, self-worth, and school behavior. Jour- Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Cermak, I., & Rosza,
nal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 141155. S. (2001). Facing guilt: Role of negative affectivity, need for repa-
Boxer, P., Tisak, M. S., & Goldstein, S. E. (2004). Is it bad to be good? ration, and fear of punishment in leading to prosocial behaviour
An exploration of aggressive and prosocial behavior subtypes in and aggression. European Journal of Personality, 15, 219237.
adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 91100. Carlo, G., Eisenberg, N., & Knight, G. P. (1992). An objective mea-
Braaten, E. B., & Rosen, L. A. (2000). Self-regulation of affect in At- sure of adolescents prosocial moral reasoning. Journal of Re-
tention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and non-ADHD search on Adolescence, 2, 331349.
boys: Differences in empathic responding. Journal of Consulting Carlo, G., Hausmann, A., Christiansen, S., & Randall, B. A.
and Clinical Psychology, 68(2), 313321. (2003). Sociocognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of
Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). prosocial tendencies for adolescents. Journal of Early Adoles-
Learning to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective. cence, 23, 107134.
Child Development, 57, 529548. Carlo, G., Knight, G. P., Eisenberg, N., & Rotenberg, K. (1991). Cog-
Broberg, A., Hwang, C., Lamb, M., & Ketterlinus, R. D. (1989). nitive processes and prosocial behavior among children: The role
Child care effects on socioemotional and intellectual competence of affective attributions and reconciliations. Developmental Psy-
in Swedish preschoolers. In J. S. Lande, S. Scarr, & N. Gunzen- chology, 27, 456461.
hauser (Eds.), Caring for children: Challenge to America Carlo, G., Koller, S. H., Eisenberg, N., DaSilva, M. S., & Frohlich,
(pp. 4976). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. C. B. (1996). A cross-national study on the relations among
Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & MacKinnon, C. E. (1986). Contribu- prosocial moral reasoning, gender role orientations, and prosocial
tions of maternal child-rearing practices and play contexts to sib- behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 231240.
ling interactions. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, Carlo, G., & Randall, B. A. (2002). The development of a measure of
7, 225236. prosocial behaviors for late adolescents. Journal of Youth and Ado-
Bryan, J. H., & Walbek, N. H. (1970). The impact of words and lescence, 31, 3144.
deeds concerning altruism upon children. Child Development,
Carlo, G., Roesch, S. C., Knight, G. P., & Koller, S. H. (2001). Be-
41, 747757.
tween- or within-culture variation? Culture groups as a moderator
Bryant, B. K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adoles- of the relations between individual differences and resource alloca-
cents. Child Development, 53, 413425. tion preferences. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 559579.
Bryant, B. K. (1987). Mental health, temperament, family, and Carlo, G., Roesch, S. C., & Melby, J. (1998). The multiplicative rela-
friends: Perspectives on childrens empathy and social perspec- tions of parenting and temperament to prosocial and antisocial be-
tive taking. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its haviors in adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 266290.
development (pp. 245270). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L. S., & Balarman,
University Press.
G. (2003). The relationship between psychological understanding
Bryant, B. K., & Crockenberg, S. B. (1980). Correlates and dimen- and positive social behaviors. Social Development, 12, 198221.
sions of prosocial behavior: A study of female siblings with their
mothers. Child Development, 51, 529544. Cauley, K., & Tyler, B. (1989). The relationship of self-concept to
prosocial behavior in children. Early Childhood Research Quar-
Buhrmester, D., Goldfarb, J., & Cantrell, D. (1992). Self-presentation
terly, 4, 5160.
when sharing with friends and nonfriends. Journal of Early Ado-
lescence, 12, 6179. Chapman, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cooperman, G., & Iannotti, R.
(1987). Empathy and responsibility in the motivation of chil-
Burger, J. M. (1999). The foot-in-the-door compliance procedure: A
drens helping. Developmental Psychology, 23, 140145.
multiple-process analysis and review. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Review, 3, 303325. Charbonneau, D., & Nicol, A. A. M. (2002). Emotional intelligence
and prosocial behaviors in adolescents. Psychological Reports,
Burleson, B., Applegate, J. L., Burke, J. A., Clark, R. A., Delia, J. G.,
90, 361370.
& Kline, S. L. (1986). Communicative correlates of peer accep-
tance in childhood. Communication Education, 35, 349361. Cheung, P. C., Ma, H. K., & Shek, D. T. L. (1998). Conceptions of
Calabrese, R. L., & Schumer, H. (1986). The effects of service activ- success: Their correlates with prosocial orientation and behaviour
ities on adolescent alienation. Adolescence, 21, 675687. in Chinese adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 21, 3142.
Call, K. T., Mortimer, J. T., & Shanahan, M. J. (1995). Helpfulness Chou, K. L. (1998). Effects of age, gender, and participation in vol-
and the development of competence in adolescence. Child Devel- unteer activities on the altruistic behavior of Chinese adolescents.
opment, 66, 129138. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159, 195201.
Caplan, M. Z., & Hay, D. F. (1989). Preschoolers responses to peers Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. (2004). Social inf luence: Compliance
distress and beliefs about bystander intervention. Journal of Child and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591621.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 231242. Clark, K. E., & Ladd, G. W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Pastorelli, C. (2001). Prosocial be- support in parent-child relationships: Links to childrens socioe-
havior and aggression in childhood and pre-adolescence. In A. C. motional orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psy-
Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behav- chology, 36, 485498.
ior: Implications for family, school, and society (pp. 187203). Clark, M. S. (1991). Review of personality and social psychology:
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Prosocial behavior (Vol. 12). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
References 705

Clark, M. S., Milberg, S., & Erber, R. (1984). Effects of arousal on Davis, M. H., Luce, C., & Kraus, S. J. (1994). The heritability of
judgments of others emotions. Journal of Personality and Social characteristics associated with dispositional empathy. Journal of
Psychology, 46, 551560. Personality, 62, 369391.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1981). Observation and experiment: Comple- Deater-Deckard, K., Dunn, J., OConnor, T. G., Davies, L., Golding,
mentary strategies for studying day care and social development. J., & the ALSPAC Study Team. (2001). Using the stepfamily ge-
In S. Kilmer (Ed.), Advances in Early Education and Day Care netic design to examine gene-environmental processes in child and
(Vol. 2, pp. 227250). Greenwich, CN: JAI Press. family functioning. Marriage and Family Review, 33, 131156.
Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1999). The motivations to volunteer: Deater-Deckard, K., Pike, A., Petrill, S. A., Cutting, A. L., Hughes,
Theoretical and practical considerations. Current Directions in C., & OConnor, T. G. (2001). Nonshared environmental
Psychological Science, 8, 156159. processes in social-emotional development: An observational
Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered and study of identical twin differences in the preschool period. Devel-
comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32, 988998. opmental Science, 4, F1F6.
Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of Decety, J., & Chaminade, T. (2003). Neural correlates of feeling sym-
moral commitment. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Free Press. pathy. Neuropsychologia, 41, 127138.
de Guzman, M. R. T., & Carlo, G. (2004). Family, peer, and accultur-
Coleman, P. K., & Byrd, C. P. (2003). Interpersonal correlates of peer
ative correlates of prosocial development among Latino youth in
victimization among young adolescents. Journal of Youth and
Nebraska. Great Plains Research, 14, 185202.
Adolescence, 32, 301314.
de Guzman, M. R. T., Edwards, C. P., & Carlo, G. (2005). Prosocial
Conrad, D., & Hedin, D. (1982). The impact of experiential education
behaviors in context: A study of the Gukuyu children of Ngecha,
on adolescent development. In D. Conrad & D. Hedin (Eds.),
Kenya. Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 542558.
Youth participation and experiential education. New York: Ha-
worth Press. Dekovic, M., & Gerris, J. R. M. (1994). Developmental analysis of
social cognitive and behavioral differences between popular and
Costin, S. E., & Jones, D. C. (1992). Friendship as a facilitator of
rejected children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
emotional responsiveness and prosocial interventions among
15, 367386.
young children. Developmental Psychology, 28, 941947.
Dekovic, M., & Janssens, J. M. A. M. (1992). Parents child-rearing
Ct, S., Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D., Zoccolillo, M., & Vitaro, F.
style and childrens sociometric status. Developmental Psychol-
(2002). The development of impulsivity, fearfulness, and helpful-
ogy, 28, 925932.
ness during childhood: Patterns of consistency and change in the
trajectories of boys and girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emo-
Psychiatry, 43, 609618. tion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development,
57, 194201.
Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational an overt
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K.,
aggression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33, 579588.
Auerbach-Major, S., et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence:
Crockenberg, S. (1985). Toddlers reactions to maternal anger. Mer- Pathway to social competence. Child Development, 74, 238256.
rill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 361373.
Denham, S. A., & Burger, C. (1991). Observational validation of rat-
Cummings, J. S., Pellegrini, D. S., & Notarius, C. I. (1989). Chil- ings of preschoolers social competence and behavior problems.
drens responses to angry adult behavior as a function of marital Child Study Journal, 21, 185291.
distress and history of interparent hostility. Child Development,
Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E. A. (1991). Social-emotional predic-
60, 10351043.
tors of preschoolers responses to adult negative emotion. Journal
Cummings, E. M., & Smith, D. (1993). The impact of anger between of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 595608.
adults on siblings emotions and social behavior. Journal of Child
Denham, S. A., & Grout, L. (1992). Mothers emotional expressive-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 14251433.
ness and coping: Relations with preschoolers social-emotional
Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). competence. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Mono-
Young childrens responses to expressions of anger and affection graphs, 118, 75101.
by others in the family. Child Development, 52, 12741282.
Denham, S. A., & Grout, L. (1993). Socialization of emotion: Path-
Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1984). De- way to preschoolers emotional and social competence. Journal of
velopmental changes in childrens reactions to anger in the home. Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 205227.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 6374. Denham, S. A., Mason, T., & Couchoud, E. A. (1995). Scaffolding
Cunningham, M. R. (1985/1986). Levites and brothers keepers: A young childrens prosocial responsiveness: Preschoolers re-
sociobiological perspective on prosocial behavior. Humboldt Jour- sponses to adult sadness, anger, and pain. International Journal of
nal of Social Relations, 13, 3567. Behavioral Development, 18, 489504.
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion under- Denham, S. A., Renwick-DeBardi, S., & Hewes, S. (1994). Emotional
standing, language, and family background: Individual differ- communication between mothers and preschoolers: Relations with
ences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853865. emotional competence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 488508.
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conf lict and child de Veer, A. J. E., & Janssens, J. M. A. M. (1994). Victim-orientated
adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bul- discipline, interpersonal understanding, and guilt. Journal of
letin, 116, 387411. Moral Education, 23, 165182.
Davis, M. H., & Franzoi, S. (1991). Stability and change in adoles- Diener, M. L., & Kim, D. Y. (2004). Maternal and child predictors of
cent self-consciousness and empathy. Journal of Research in Per- preschool childrens social competence. Applied Developmental
sonality, 25, 7087. Psychology, 25, 324.
706 Prosocial Development

Dix, T., & Grusec, J. E. (1983). Parental inf luence techniques: An at- moral reasoning in adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of
tributional analysis. Child Development, 54, 645652. Research in Adolescence, 15, 235260.
Dlugokinski, E. L., & Firestone, I. J. (1974). Other centeredness and Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization,
susceptibility to charitable appeals: Effects of perceived disci- assessment, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motivation and
pline. Developmental Psychology, 10, 2128. Emotion, 14, 131149.
Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discrimi- Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion, regulation, and the
nate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn in- development of social competence. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review
fant? Developmental Psychology, 35, 418426. of personality and social psychology: Vol. 14. Emotion and social
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, behavior (pp. 119150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
MA: Harvard University Press. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1995). The relation of young chil-
Dunn, J., Cutting, A., & Fisher, N. (2002). Old friends, new friends: drens vicarious emotional responding to social competence, reg-
Predictors of childrens perspective on their friends at school. ulation, and emotionality. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 203228.
Child Development, 73, 621635. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In W.
Dunn, J., Deater-Deckard, K., Pickering, K., OConnor, T. G., Gold- Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
ing, J., & the ALSPAC Study Team. (1998). Childrens adjustment child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality devel-
and prosocial behaviour in step-, single-parent, and non-stepfam- opment (5th ed., pp. 701778). New York: Wiley.
ily settings: Findings from a community study. Journal of Child Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Speer, A. L., Switzer, G., Kar-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 10831095. bon, M., et al. (1993). The relations of empathy-related emotions
Dunn, J., & Kendrick, D. (1982). Siblings: Love, envy, and under- and maternal practices to childrens comforting behavior. Journal
standing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 131150.
Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1986). Siblings and the development of proso- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Troyer, D., Speer, A. L., Kar-
cial behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, bon, M., et al. (1992). The relations of maternal practices and
9, 265284. characteristics to childrens vicarious emotional responsiveness.
Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A Child Development, 63, 583602.
meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psy- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Karbon, M., Murphy, B. C., Carlo, G., &
chological Bulletin, 100, 283308. Wosinski, M. (1996). Relations of school childrens comforting
Eberly, M. B., & Montemayor, R. (1998). Doing good deeds: An exami- behavior to empathy-related reactions and shyness. Social Devel-
nation of adolescent prosocial behavior in the context of parent-ado- opment, 5, 330351.
lescent relationships. Journal of Adolescent Research, 13, 403432. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Karbon, M., Murphy, B. C., Wosinski,
Eberly, M. B., & Montemayor, R. (1999). Adolescent affection and M., Polazzi, L., et al. (1996). The relations of childrens disposi-
helpfulness toward parents: A 2-year follow-up. Journal of Early tional prosocial behavior to emotionality, regulation, and social
Adolescence, 19, 226248. functioning. Child Development, 67, 974992.
Eberly, M. B., Montemayor, R., & Flannery, D. J. (1993). Variation in Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Miller, P. A. (1990). The evolutionary
adolescent helpfulness toward parents in a family context. Journal and neurological roots of prosocial behavior. In L. Ellis & H.
of Early Adolescence, 13, 228244. Hoffman (Eds.), Crime in biological, social, and moral contexts
Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, volunteering, (pp. 247260). New York: Praeger.
basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular in- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Mathy, R. M.,
volvement matters? Journal of Adolescent Research, 14, 1043. Shell, R., et al. (1989). The relations of sympathy and personal
Eisenberg, N. (1983). Childrens differentiations among potential re- distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of
cipients of aid. Child Development, 54, 594602. Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 5566.
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Shell, C., Shea, R., & May-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Plumlee, T. (1990). Preschoolers vicarious emotional responding
Eisenberg, N., Boehnke, K., Schuhler, P., & Silbereisen, R. K. (1985). and their situational and dispositional prosocial behavior. Merrill-
The development of prosocial behavior and cognitions in German Palmer Quarterly, 36, 507529.
children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16, 6982. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P.,
Eisenberg, N., Cameron, E., Tryon, K., & Dodez, R. (1981). Social- Smith, M., et al. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regula-
ization of prosocial behavior in the preschool classroom. Develop- tion to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding.
mental Psychology, 17, 773782. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 776797.
Eisenberg, N., Carlo, G., Murphy, B., & Van Court, P. (1995). Proso- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B. C., Karbon, M., Smith, M., &
cial development in late adolescence: A longitudinal study. Child Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of childrens dispositional empa-
Development, 66, 11791197. thy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and so-
Eisenberg, N., Cialdini, R., McCreath, H., & Shell, R. (1987). Consis- cial functioning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 195209.
tency-based compliance: When and why do children become vulner- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Carlo, G., & Miller, P. A.
able? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 11741181. (1991). The relations of parental characteristics and practices to
Eisenberg, N., Cialdini, R. B., McCreath, H., & Shell, R. (1989). childrens vicarious emotional responding. Child Development,
Consistency-based compliance in children: When and why do 62, 13931408.
consistency procedures have immediate effects? Journal of Behav- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., & Miller, P. A. (1989). In-
ioral Development, 12, 351367. tercorrelations and developmental changes in indices of empathy.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), New directions in child development (Vol.
Shepard, S. A. (2005). Age changes in prosocial responding and 44, pp. 86126). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
References 707

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Schaller, M., Miller, P. A., Carlo, G., Eisenberg, N., Shea, C. L., Carlo, G., & Knight, G. (1991). Empathy-
Poulin, R., et al. (1991). Personality and socialization correlates related responding and cognition: A chicken and the egg
of vicarious emotional responding. Journal of Personality and So- dilemma. In W. Kurtines & J. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral
cial Psychology, 61, 459470. behavior and development: Vol. 2. Research (pp. 6388). Hillsdale,
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphy, B. C., Jones, J., NJ: Erlbaum.
& Guthrie, I. K. (1998). Contemporaneous and longitudinal pre- Eisenberg, N., & Shell, R. (1986). The relation of prosocial moral
diction of childrens sympathy from dispositional regulation and judgment and behavior in children: The mediating role of cost.
emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 34, 910924. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 426433.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., Pasternack, J., Lennon, R., Beller, R., &
S. A., Zhou, Q., et al. (2002). Prosocial development in early Mathy, R. M. (1987). Prosocial development in middle child-
adulthood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social hood: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 24,
Psychology, 82, 9931006. 712718.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Murphy, B. C., Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004).
Holgren, R., et al. (1997). The relations of regulation and emo- Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment,
tionality to resiliency and competent social functioning in ele- and socialization in childhood. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs
mentary school children. Child Development, 68, 295311. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and appli-
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumber- cations (pp. 259282). New York: Guilford Press.
land, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Sadovsky, A. (2005). Empathy-
prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana
70, 13601372. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517549). Hillsdale,
Eisenberg, N., Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., & Fuchs, I. (1990). Prosocial NJ: Erlbaum.
moral judgment in Israeli kibbutz and city children: A longitudi-
Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S., Goldberg, L., & Engel, I. (1992). Parental
nal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 273285.
values, reinforcement, and young childrens prosocial behavior: A
Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Gender differences in empathy longitudinal study. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 1936.
and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100131.
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., & Koller, S. (2001). Brazilian adolescents
Eisenberg, N., Liew, J., & Pidada, S. (2001). The relations of parental
prosocial moral judgment and behavior: Relations to sympathy,
emotional expressivity with the quality of Indonesian childrens
perspective taking, gender-role orientation, and demographic
social functioning. Emotion, 1, 107115.
characteristics. Child Development, 72, 518534.
Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., Reiser, M.,
Eisenberg-Berg, N. (1979). The development of childrens prosocial
Murphy, B. C., et al. (2001). Parental socialization of childrens
moral judgment. Developmental Psychology, 15, 128137.
dysregulated expression of emotion and externalizing problems.
Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 183205. Eisenberg-Berg, N., & Geisheker, E. (1979). Content of preachings
Eisenberg, N., Lundy, N., Shell, R., & Roth, K. (1985). Childrens jus- and power of the model /preacher: The effect on childrens gen-
tifications for their adult and peer-direct compliant (prosocial and erosity. Developmental Psychology, 15, 168175.
nonprosocial) behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 21, 325331. Eisenberg-Berg, N., & Hand, M. (1979). The relationship of
Eisenberg, N., McCreath, H., & Ahn, R. (1988). Vicarious emo- preschoolers reasoning about prosocial moral conf licts to proso-
tional responsiveness and prosocial behavior: Their interrela- cial behavior. Child Development, 50, 356363.
tions in young children. Personality and Social Psychology Ekstein, R. (1978). Psychoanalysis, sympathy, and altruism. In
Bulletin, 14, 298311. L. G. Wispe (Ed.), Altruism, sympathy, and helping: Psychologi-
Eisenberg, N., & McNally, S. (1993). Socialization and mothers and cal and sociological principles (pp. 165176). New York: Aca-
adolescents empathy-related characteristics. Journal of Research demic Press.
on Adolescence, 3, 171191. Ellis, L., & Hoffman, H. (1990). Views of contemporary criminolo-
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1987). The relation of empathy to proso- gists on causes and theories of crime. In L. Ellis & H. Hoffman
cial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91119. (Eds.), Crime in biological, social, and moral contexts
Eisenberg, N., Miller, P. A., Shell, R., McNalley, S., & Shea, C. (pp. 5058). New York: Praeger.
(1991). Prosocial development in adolescence: A longitudinal Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2001). Self-reported empathy in Nor-
study. Developmental Psychology, 27, 849857. wegian adolescents: Sex differences, age trends, and relationship
Eisenberg, N., Murphy, B., & Shepard, S. (1997). The development of to bullying. In A. C. Bohart & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive
empathic accuracy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy and destructive behavior: Implications for family, school, and so-
(pp. 73116). New York: Guilford Press. ciety (pp. 147165). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Eisenberg, N., & Mussen, P. (1989). The roots of prosocial behavior in Association.
children. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Erdley, C. A., & Asher, S. R. (1999). A social goals perspective on
Eisenberg, N., & Ota Wang, V. (2003). Toward a positive psychology: childrens social competence. Journal of Emotional and Behav-
Social developmental and cultural contributions. In L. G. Aspin- ioral Disorders, 7, 156167.
wall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths: Eslinger, P. J. (1998). Neurological and neuropsychological bases of
Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive psy- empathy. European Neurology, 39, 193199.
chology (pp. 117129). Washington, DC: APA Books. Eslinger, P. J., Easton, A., Grattan, L. M., & Van Hoesen, G. W.
Eisenberg, N., Pasternack, J. F., Cameron, E., & Tryon, K. (1984). (1996). Distinctive forms of partial retrograde amnesia after
The relation of quality and mode of prosocial behavior to moral asymmetric temporal lobe lesions: Possible role of the occipi-
cognitions and social style. Child Development, 155, 14791485. totemporal gyri in memory. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 530539.
708 Prosocial Development

Essock-Vitale, S., & McGuire, M. (1985). Womens lives viewed Freud, S. (1930). Civilization and its discontents (J. Riviere, Trans.).
from an evolutionary perspective: Pt. 2. Patterns of helping. London: Hogarth Press.
Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 155173. Freud, S. (1968). New introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. Lon-
Estrada, P. (1995). Adolescents self-reports of prosocial responses don: Hogarth. (Original work published 1933)
to friends and acquaintances: The role of sympathy-related cogni- Froming, W. J., Nasby, W., & McManus, J. (1998). Prosocial self-
tive, affective, and motivational processes. Journal of Research on schemas, self-awareness, and childrens prosocial behavior. Jour-
Adolescence, 5, 173200. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 766777.
Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Kupanoff, K., & Laible, D. (1999). Early ado- Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Childrens perceptions of the
lescence and prosocial /moral behavior: Pt. 1. The role of individ- personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental
ual processes. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 516. Psychology, 21, 10161024.
Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1996). An examination of age and sex Gallese, V. (2001). The Shared Manifold Hypothesis: From mirror
dif ferences in prosocial behavior and empathy. Unpublished manu- neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 3350.
script, Arizona State University at Tempe. Garner, P. W., & Estep, K. M. (2001). Emotional competence, emo-
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Eisenbud, L. (1993). Behavioral and tion socialization, and young childrens peer-related social com-
physiological correlates of childrens reactions to others distress. petence. Early Education and Development, 12, 2948.
Developmental Psychology, 29, 655663. Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., Gaddy, G., & Rennie, K. M. (1997). Low-
income mothers conversations about emotions and their chil-
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., Bernzweig, J., Speer, A. L.,
drens emotional competence. Social Development, 6, 3752.
& Carlo, G. (1994). Socialization of childrens vicarious emo-
tional responding and prosocial behavior: Relations with mothers Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., & Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence
perceptions of childrens emotional reactivity. Developmental among low-income preschoolers: Emotion socialization practices
Psychology, 30, 4455. and social cognitive correlates. Child Development, 65, 622637.
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Karbon, M., Troyer, D., & Switzer, G. Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., & Palmer, D. J. (1994). Social cognitive
(1994). The relations of childrens emotion regulation to their vi- correlates of preschool childrens sibling caregiving behavior. De-
carious emotional responses and comforting behavior. Child De- velopmental Psychology, 30, 905911.
velopment, 65, 16781693. Gelfand, D. M., Hartmann, D. P., Cromer, C. C., Smith, C. L., & Page,
B. C. (1975). The effects of instructional prompts and praise on
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1990). Maternal correlates
childrens donation rates. Child Development, 46, 980983.
of childrens vicarious emotional responsiveness. Developmental
Psychology, 26, 639648. Gill, K. L., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). Do aggressive/destructive tod-
dlers lack concern for others? Behavioral and physiological indi-
Fabes, R. A., & Filsinger, E. E. (1988). Odor communication and par-
cators of empathic responding in 2-year-old children.
ent-child interaction. In E. Filsinger (Ed.), Biosocial perspectives
Development and Psychopathology, 15, 5571.
on the family (pp. 93118). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Ginsburg, H. J., Ogletree, S. M., Silakowski, T. D., Bartels, R. D.,
Fabes, R. A., Fultz, J., Eisenberg, N., May-Plumlee, T., & Christo- Burk, S. L., & Turner, G. M. (2003). Young childrens theories of
pher, F. S. (1989). The effects of reward on childrens prosocial mind about empathic and selfish motives. Social Behavior and
motivation: A socialization study. Developmental Psychology, Personality, 31, 237244.
25, 509515.
Glover, E. (1968). The birth of the ego. London: Allen & Unwin.
Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2002, October). The
Goodman, R. (1994). A modified version of the Rutter Parent Ques-
role of sex segregation in young childrens prosocial behavior
tionnaire including extra items on childrens strengths: A research
and disposition. Paper presented at the Groningen Conference
note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 14831494.
on Prosocial Dispositions and Solidarity, Groningen, The
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and
Netherlands.
Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of
Fabes, R. A., Moss, A., Reesing, A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 13371345.
(2005, October). The ef fects of peer prosocial exposure on the
Gosselin, P., Warren, M., & Diotte, M. (2002). Motivation to hide
quality of young childrens social interactions. Data presented at
emotion and childrens understanding of the distinction between
the annual conference of the National Council on Family Rela-
real and apparent emotions. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163.
tions, Phoenix, AZ.
Graves, N. B., & Graves, T. D. (1983). The cultural context of proso-
Farver, J. A. M., & Branstetter, W. H. (1994). Preschoolers proso- cial development: An ecological model. In D. L. Bridgeman (Ed.),
cial responses to their peers distress. Developmental Psychology, The nature of prosocial development (pp. 243264). New York:
30, 334341. Academic Press.
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. H. (1997). Agreeableness: A di-
York: Norton. mension of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs
Feshbach, N. D. (1978). Studies of empathic behavior in children. In (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795824). San
B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(Vol. 8, pp. 147). New York: Academic Press. Greenberger, E., & Goldberg, W. A. (1989). Work, parenting, and the
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1982). Empathy training and the socialization of children. Developmental Psychology, 25, 2235.
regulation of aggression: Potentialities and limitations. Academic Gretarsson, S. J., & Gelfand, D. M. (1988). Mothers attributions re-
Psychology Bulletin, 4, 399413. garding their childrens social behavior and personality charac-
Fischer, W. F. (1963). Sharing in preschool children as function of teristics. Developmental Psychology, 24, 264269.
amount and type of reinforcement. Genetic Psychology Mono- Grusec, J. E. (1971). Power and the internalization of self denial.
graphs, 68, 215245. Child Development, 42, 93105.
References 709

Grusec, J. E. (1991). Socializing concern for others in home. Devel- Hay, D. F. (1994). Prosocial development. Journal of Child Psychol-
opmental Psychology, 27, 338342. ogy and Psychiatry, 35, 2971.
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental disci- Hay, D. F., Caplan, M. Z., Castle, M. Z., & Stimson, C. A. (1991).
pline methods on the childs internalization of values: A recon- Does sharing become rational in the second year of life? Devel-
ceptualization of current points of view. Developmental opmental Psychology, 27, 987993.
Psychology, 30, 419. Hay, D. F., Castle, J., Davies, L., Demetriou, H., & Stimson, C. A.
Grusec, J. E., Goodnow, J. J., & Cohen, L. (1996). Household work (1999). Prosocial action in very early childhood. Journal of Child
and the development of concern for others. Developmental Psy- Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 905916.
chology, 32, 9991007. Hay, D. F., & Murray, P. (1982). Giving and requesting: Social facili-
Grusec, J. E., Kuczynski, L., Rushton, J. P., & Simutis, Z. M. (1978). tation of infants offers to adults. Infant Behavior and Develop-
Modeling, direction instruction, and attributions: Effects on al- ment, 5, 301310.
truism. Developmental Psychology, 14, 5157. Hay, D. F., Nash, A., & Pedersen, J. (1981). Responses of 6-month-
Grusec, J. E., & Redler, E. (1980). Attribution, reinforcement, and olds to the distress of their peers. Child Development, 52,
altruism: A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychology, 10711075.
16, 525534. Hay, D. F., & Pawlby, S. (2003). Prosocial development in relation to
Grusec, J. E., Saas-Kortsaak, P., & Simutis, Z. M. (1978). The role of childrens and mothers psychological problems. Child Develop-
example and moral exhortation in the training of altruism. Child ment, 74, 13141327.
Development, 49, 920923. Hay, D. F., & Rheingold, H. L. (1983). The early appearance of some
Hall, K. R. L., & DeVore, I. (1965). Baboon social behavior. In I. De- valued behaviors. In D. L. Bridgeman (Ed.), The nature of proso-
Vore (Ed.), Primate behavior: Field studies of monkeys and apes cial development: Interdisciplinary theories and strategies
(pp. 53110). New York: Holt, Rhinehart, & Winston. (pp. 7394). New York: Academic Press.
Hampson, R. B. (1984). Adolescent prosocial behavior: Peer group Hayes, D. J., Felton, R. R., & Cohen, R. R. (1985). A natural occur-
and situational factors associated with helping. Journal of Person- rence of foster parenting by a female mountain bluebird. Auk,
ality and Social Psychology, 46, 153162. 102, 191193.
Hansen, B. K., & Bryant, B. K. (1980). Peer inf luence on sharing be- Helwig, C. C., & Turiel, E. (2003). Childrens social and moral reason-
havior of Mexican-American and Anglo-American boys. Journal ing. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of child-
of Social Psychology, 110, 135136. hood social development (pp. 476490). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Harmon-Jones, E., Vaughn-Scott, K., Mohr, S., Sigelman, J., & Har- Hertz-Lazarowitz, R. (1983). Prosocial behavior in the classroom.
mon-Jones, C. (2004). The effect of manipulated sympathy and Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5, 319338.
anger on left and right frontal cortical activity. Emotion, 4, Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., Fuchs, I., Sharabany, R., & Eisenberg, N.
95101. (1989). Students interactive and non-interactive behaviors in the
Hart, D., Atkins, R., & Ford, D. (1998). Urban America as a context classroom: A comparison between two types of classrooms in the
for the development of moral identity in adolescence. Journal of city and the kibbutz in Israel. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
Social Issues, 54, 513530. ogy, 14, 2232.
Hart, D., Burock, D., London, B., & Atkins, R. (2003). Prosocial ten- Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., & Sharan, S. (1984). Enhancing prosocial be-
dencies, antisocial behavior, and moral development. In A. Slater havior through cooperative learning in the classroom. In E. Staub,
& G. Bremmer (Eds.), An introduction to developmental psychol- D. Bar-Tal, J. Karylowski, & J. Reykowski (Eds.), Development
ogy (pp. 334356). Malden, MA: Blackwell. and maintenance of prosocial behavior: International perspectives
on positive morality (pp. 423443). New York: Plenum Press.
Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1995). Altruism and caring in adolescence:
Relations to self-understanding and social judgment. Child Devel- Hertz-Lazarowitz, R., Sharan, S., & Steinberg, R. (1980). Classroom
opment, 66, 13461359. learning style and cooperative behavior of elementary school chil-
dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 97104.
Hartmann, D. P., Gelfand, D. M., Smith, C. L., Paul, S. C., Cromer,
C. C., Page, B. C., et al. (1976). Factors affecting the acquisition Hieshima, J. A., & Schneider, B. (1994). Intergenerational effects on
and elimination of childrens donation behavior. Journal of Exper- the cultural and cognitive socialization of third- and fourth-gen-
imental Child Psychology, 21, 328338. eration Japanese Americans. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 15, 319327.
Hartup, W. W., & Coates, B. (1967). Imitation of a peer as a function
of reinforcement from the peer group and rewardingness of the Hobbes, T. (1962). Leviathan (M. Oakeshotte, Ed.). New York: Dut-
model. Child Development, 38, 10031016. ton. (Original work published 1651)

Haselager, G. J. T., Cillessen, A. H. N., Van Lieshout, C. F. M., Riksen- Hofer, M. A. (1981). The roots of human behavior. San Fran-
Walraven, J. M. A., & Hartup, W. W. (2002). Heterogeneity among cisco: Freeman.
peer-rejected boys across middle childhood: Developmental path- Hoffman, M. L. (1963). Parent discipline and the childs considera-
ways of social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 446456. tion for others. Child Development, 34, 573588.
Hastings, P. D., Zahn-Waxler, C., & McShane, K. (2005). We are, by Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
nature, moral creatures: Biological bases of concern for others. In Carmichaels manual of child development (Vol. 2, pp. 261359).
M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development New York: Wiley.
(pp. 483516). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Altruistic behavior and the parent-child rela-
Hastings, P. D., Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., Usher, B., & Bridges, tionship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 937943.
D. (2000). The development of concern for others in children with Hoffman, M. L. (1981). Is altruism part of human nature? Journal of
behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 36, 531546. Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 121137.
710 Prosocial Development

Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empa- Israel, A. C., & Raskin, P. A. (1979). Directiveness of instructions
thy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial and modeling: Effects on production and persistence on chil-
behavior (pp. 281313). New York: Academic Press. drens donations. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135, 269277.
Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral Jacobs, J. E., Vernon, M. K., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Relations be-
internalization. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup tween social self-perceptions, time use, and prosocial or prob-
(Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A sociocultural lem behavior during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent
perspective (pp. 236274). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Univer- Research, 19, 45 62.
sity Press. Jacobsen, C. (1983). What it means to be considerate: Differences in
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications normative expectations and their implications. Israel Social Sci-
for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press. ence Research, 1, 2433.
Hoffner, C., & Haefner, M. J. (1997). Childrens comforting of Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orienta-
frightened coviewers: Real and hypothetical television-viewing tion: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703726.
situations. Communication Research, 24, 136152. Janoski, T., Musick, M., & Wilson, J. (1998). Being volunteered? The
Holte, C. S., Jamruszka, V., Gustafson, J., Beaman, A. L., & Camp, impact of social participation and pro-social attitudes on volun-
G. C. (1984). Inf luence of childrens positive self-perceptions on teering. Sociological Forum, 13, 495519.
donating behavior in naturalistic settings. Journal of School Psy- Janoski, T., & Wilson, J. (1995). Pathways to volunteerism: Fam-
chology, 22, 145153. ily socialization and status transmission models. Social Forces,
Howe, N., & Ross, H. S. (1990). Socialization, perspective-taking, and 74, 271292.
the sibling relationship. Developmental Psychology, 26, 160165. Janssens, J. M. A. M., & Dekovic, M. (1997). Child rearing, prosocial
Howes, C., & Eldredge, R. (1985). Responses of abused, neglected, moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviour. International Journal of
and non-maltreated children to the behaviors of their peers. Jour- Behavioral Development, 20, 509527.
nal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 261270. Janssens, J. M. A. M., & Gerris, J. R. M. (1992). Child rearing, em-
Howes, C., & Farver, J. (1987). Toddlers responses to the dis- pathy and prosocial development. In J. M. A. M. Janssens &
tress of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol- J. R. M. Gerris (Eds.), Child rearing: Inf luence on prosocial and
ogy, 8, 441452. moral development (pp. 5775). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Howes, C., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). Sequences in the development Jennings, K. D., Fitch, D., & Suwalsky, J. T. (1987). Social cognition
of competent play with peers: Social and social pretend play. De- and social interaction in 3-year-olds: Is social cognition truly so-
velopmental Psychology, 28, 961974. cial? Child Study Journal, 17, 114.
Howes, C., Matheson, C. C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1994). Maternal, Johnson, D. B. (1982). Altruistic behavior and the development of the
teacher, and child care history correlates of childrens relation- self in infants. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 379388.
ships with peers. Child Development, 65, 264273. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1975). Learning together and
Howes, C., & Olenick, M. (1986). Family and child care inf luences alone: Cooperation, competition, and individualization. Engle-
on toddlers compliance. Child Development, 57, 202216. wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Huebner, A. J., & Mancini, J. A. (2003). Shaping structured out-of- Johnson, H. R., Thompson, M. J. J., Wilkinson, S., Walsh, L., Bald-
school time use among youth: The effects of self, family, and ing, J., & Wright, V. (2002). Vulnerability to bullying: Teacher-
friend systems. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 453463. reported conduct and emotional problems, hyperactivity, peer
Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (2000). Antisocial, relationship difficulties, and prosocial behaviour in primary
angry, and unsympathetic: Hard-to-manage preschoolers peer school children. Educational Psychology, 22, 553556.
problems and possible cognitive inf luences. Journal of Child Psy- Johnson, M. K., Beebe, T., Mortimer, J. T., & Snyder, M. (1998). Vol-
chology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 169179. unteerism in adolescence: A process perspective. Journal of Re-
Hume, D. (1975). Enquiry into the human understanding (P. Nidditch, search on Adolescence, 8, 309332.
Ed.). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. (Original work pub- Johnson, W., & Krueger, R. F. (2004). Genetic and environmental
lished 1748) structure of adjectives describing the domains of the big five
Huston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. (1998). Mass media and childrens de- model of personality: A nationwide U.S. twin study. Journal of
velopment. In W. Damon, I. S. Sigel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Research in Personality, 38, 448472.
Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, 5th ed., pp. 9991058). New Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A.,
York: Wiley. et al. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. In U. Bel-
Iannotti, R. J., Cummings, E. M., Pierrehumbert, B., Milano, M. J., & lugi & M. St. George (Eds.), Linking cognitive neuroscience and
Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Parental inf luences on prosocial behav- molecular genetics: New perspectives from Williams syndrome.
ior and empathy in early childhood. In J. M. A. M. Janssens & Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl. 1), 3046.
J. R. M. Gerris (Eds.), Child rearing: Inf luence on prosocial and Kagan, S., & Knight, G. P. (1981). Social motives among Anglo-
moral development (pp. 77100). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. American and Mexican-American children: Experimental and pro-
Ingls, C. J., Hidalgo, M. D., Mendz, F. X., & Inderbitzen, H. M. (2003). jective measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 93106.
The Teenage Inventory of Social Skills: Reliability and validity of Kagan, S., & Madsen, M. C. (1972). Experimental analyses of coop-
the Spanish translation. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 505510. eration and competition of Anglo-American and Mexican chil-
Israel, A. C., & Brown, M. S. (1979). Effects of directiveness of in- dren. Developmental Psychology, 6, 4959.
structions and surveillance on the production and persistence of Kalliopuska, M., & Tiitinen, U. (1991). Inf luence of two develop-
childrens donations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, mental programs on the empathy and prosociability of preschool
27, 250261. children. Perceptual and Motor skills, 72, 323328.
References 711

Kiang, L., Moreno, A. J., & Robinson, J. L. (2004). Maternal per- Knight, G. P., & Kagan, S. (1977b). Development of prosocial and
ceptions about parenting predict child temperament, maternal competitive behaviors in Anglo-American and Mexican-American
sensitivity, and childrens empathy. Developmental Psychology, children. Child Development, 48, 13851394.
40, 10811092. Knight, G. P., Kagan, S., & Buriel, R. (1981). Confounding effects of
Kaminsky, L., & Dewey, D. (2001). Sibling relationships of chil- individualism in childrens cooperation-competition social mo-
dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- tive measures. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 167178.
ders, 31, 339410. Knight, G. P., Nelson, W., Kagan, S., & Gumbiner, J. (1982). Cooper-
Kant, E. (1956). Groundwork of the metaphysics of moral law. Lon- ative-competitive social orientation and school achievement
don: Hutchinson. (Original work published 1785) among Anglo-American and Mexican-American children. Con-
Karafantis, D. M., & Levy, S. R. (2004). The role of childrens lay temporary Educational Psychology, 7, 97106.
theories about the malleability of human attributes in beliefs Knott, F., Lewis, C., & Williams, T. (1995). Sibling interaction of
about and volunteering for disadvantaged groups. Child Develop- children with learning disabilities: A comparison of autism and
ment, 75, 236250. Downs syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Karniol, R. (1995). Developmental and individual differences in pre- 6, 965976.
dicting others thoughts and feelings: Applying the transforma- Kochanska, G. (1985). Childrens temperament, mothers discipline,
tion rule model. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Review of personality and and security of attachment: Multiple pathways to emerging inter-
social psychology: Vol. 15. Social development (pp. 2748). Thou- nalization. Child Development, 66, 597615.
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kochanska, G., Forman, D. R., & Coy, K. C. (1999). Implications of
Karniol, R., Gabay, R., Ochion, Y., & Harari, Y. (1998). Is gender or the mother-child relationship in infancy for socialization in the
gender-role orientation a better predictor of empathy in adoles- second year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 249265.
cence? Sex Roles, 39, 4559. Koenig, A. L., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2004). Moral develop-
Karylowski, J. (1982). Doing good to feel good versus doing good to ment: The association between maltreatment and young childrens
make others feel good: Some child-rearing antecedents. School prosocial behaviors and moral transgressions. Social Develop-
Psychology International, 3, 149156. ment, 13, 87106.
Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Predicting kindergarten peer Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins
social status from toddler and preschool problem behavior. Jour- of empathic concern: A 26-year longitudinal study. Journal of
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 409423. Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709717.
Keith, J. G., Nelson, C. S., Schlabach, J. H., & Thompson, C. J. Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-develop-
(1990). The relationship between parental employment and three mental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Hand-
measures of early adolescent responsibility: Family-related, per- book of socialization theory and research (pp. 325480). New
sonal, and social. Journal of Early Adolescence, 10, 399415. York: Rand McNally.
Keller, B. B., & Bell, R. Q. (1979). Child effects on adults method of Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development: Vol. 2. The psy-
eliciting altruistic behavior. Child Development, 50, 10041009. chology of moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
Kestenbaum, R., Farber, E. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Individual Kojima, Y. (2000). Maternal regulation of sibling interactions in the
differences in empathy among preschoolers: Relation to attach- preschool years: Observational study in Japanese families. Child
ment history. New Directions in Child Development, 44, 51 64. Development, 71, 16401647.
Kienbaum, J., & Trommsdorff, G. (1999). Social development of Konner, M. (2002). Some obstacles to altruism. In S. G. Post & L. G.
young children in different cultural systems. International Journal Underwood (Eds.), Altruism and altruistic love: Science, philoso-
of Early Years Education, 7, 241248. phy, and religion in dialogue (pp. 192211). Oxford, England: Ox-
ford University Press.
Kienbaum, J., Volland, C., & Ulich, D. (2001). Sympathy in the con-
text of mother-child and teacher-child relationships. International Krebs, D., & Miller, D. (1985). Altruism and aggression. In G.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 302309. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology
(pp. 171). New York: Random House.
Killen, M., & Turiel, E. (1998). Adolescents and young adults eval-
uations of helping and sacrificing for others. Journal of Research Krebs, D. L., & Sturrup, B. (1982). Role-taking ability and altruistic
on Adolescence, 8, 355375. behavior in elementary school children. Journal of Moral Educa-
tion, 11, 94100.
Knight, G. P., Cota, M. K., & Bernal, M. E. (1993). The socializa-
tion of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic prefer- Krebs, D. L., & Van Hesteren, F. (1994). The development of altruism:
ences among Mexican American children: The mediating role Toward an integrative model. Developmental Review, 14, 103158.
of ethnic identity. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 15, Krevans, J., & Gibbs, J. C. (1996). Parents use of inductive disci-
291309. pline: Relations to childrens empathy and prosocial behavior.
Knight, G. P., Johnson, L. G., Carlo, G., & Eisenberg, N. (1994). A Child Development, 67, 32633277.
multiplicative model of the dispositional antecedents of a proso- Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). Altruism and anti-
cial behavior: Predicting more of the people more of the time. social behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality cor-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 178183. relates, distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 397402.
Knight, G. P., & Kagan, S. (1977a). Acculturation of prosocial and Kumru, A., Carlo, G., Mestre, V., & Samper, P. (2003, April). Proso-
competitive behaviors among second- and third-generation Mex- cial moral reasoning and prosocial behavior among Turkish and
ican-American children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Spanish adolescents. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of
8, 273284. the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.
712 Prosocial Development

Kumru, A., & Edwards, C. P. (2003, April). Gender and adolescent Lerner, R. M., Almerigi, J. B., Theokas, C., & Lerner, J. V. (2005).
prosocial behavior within the Turkish family. Poster presented at Positive youth development: A view of the issues. Journal of Early
the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Devel- Adolescence, 25, 1016.
opment, Tampa, FL. Lerner, R. M., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson, P. M. (2003). Positive
Laible, D. J., & Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of mater- youth development: Thriving as a basis of personhood and civil
nal and paternal support and control to adolescent social compe- society. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 172180.
tence, self-worth, and sympathy. Journal of Adolescent Research, Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Almerigi, J. B., Theokas, C., Phelps, E.,
19, 759782. Gestsdottir, S., et al. (2005). Positive youth development, partici-
Laible, D. J., Carlo, G., & Raffaelli, M. (2000). The differential rela- pation in community youth development programs, and commu-
tions of parent and peer attachment to adolescent adjustment. nity contributions of fifth-grade adolescents: Findings from the
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 4559. first wave of the 4-H study of positive youth development. Journal
Laible, D. J., Carlo, G., & Roesch, S. (2004). Pathways to self- of Early Adolescence, 25, 1771.
esteem: The role of parent and peer attachment, empathy, and so- Levin, I., & Bekerman-Greenberg, R. (1980). Moral judgment and
cial behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 703716. moral reasoning in sharing: A developmental analysis. Genetic
Ladd, G. W., Lange, G., & Stremmel, A. (1983). Personal and situa- Psychological Monographs, 101, 215230.
tional inf luences on childrens helping behavior: Factors that me- Lichter, D. T., Shanahan, M. J., & Gardner, E. L. (2002). Helping oth-
diate helping. Child Development, 54, 488501. ers? The effects of childhood poverty and family instability on
LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2002). Childrens percep- prosocial behavior. Youth & Society, 34, 89119.
tions of popular and unpopular peers: A multimethod assessment. Lindeman, M., Harakka, T., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (1997). Age
Developmental Psychology, 38, 635647. and gender differences in adolescents reactions to conf lict situa-
Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1995). False belief understand- tions: Aggression, prosociality, and withdrawal. Journal of Youth
ing goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of and Adolescence, 26, 339351.
coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Lindner-Gunnoe, M., Hetherington, E. M., & Reiss, D. (1999).
Emotion, 9, 167185. Parental religiosity, parenting style, and adolescent social respon-
Lamb, S., & Zakhireh, B. (1997). Toddlers attention to the distress sibility. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 199225.
of peers in a day care setting. Early Education and Development, Litvack-Miller, W., McDougall, D., & Romney, D. M. (1997). The
8, 105118. structure of empathy during middle childhood and its relationship
Larrieu, J., & Mussen, P. (1986). Some personality and motivational to prosocial behavior. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology
correlates of childrens prosocial behavior. Journal of Genetic Monographs, 123, 303324.
Psychology, 147, 529542. London, P. (1970). The rescuers: Motivational hypotheses about
Larrieu, J. A. (1984, March). Prosocial values, assertiveness, and sex: Christians who saved Jews from the Nazis. In J. Macaulay & L.
Predictors of childrens naturalistic helping. Paper presented at Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 241250).
the biennial meeting of the Southwestern Society for the Research New York: Academic Press.
in Human Development, Denver, CO.
Lopez, S. J., & Snyder, C. R. (2003). Positive psychological assess-
Lebra, T. S. (1994). Mother and child in Japanese socialization: A ment: A handbook of models and measures. Washington, DC:
Japan-United States comparisons. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. American Psychological Association.
Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child develop-
Lourenco, O. M. (1993). Toward a Piagetian explanation of the devel-
ment (pp. 259274). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
opment of prosocial behaviour in children: The force of negational
Lee, L. C., & Zhan, G. Q. (1991). Political socialization and parental thinking. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 91106.
values in the Peoples Republic of China. International Journal of
Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi-Schwartz, A., van IJzendoor n, J. H.,
Behavioral Development, 14, 337373.
Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., et al. (2003). Child care quality mat-
LeMare, L., & Krebs, D. (1983). Perspective-taking and styles of
ters: How conclusions can vary with context. Child Development,
(pro) social behavior in elementary school children. Academic
74, 10031021.
Psychology Bulletin, 5, 289298.
Ma, H. K. (1989). Moral orientation and moral judgment in adoles-
Lennon, R., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Gender and age differences in
cents in Hong Kong, mainland China, and England. Journal of
empathy and sympathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Em-
Cross Cultural Psychology, 20, 152177.
pathy and its development (pp. 195217). New York: Cambridge
University Press. Ma, H. K. (1992). The relation of altruistic orientation to human re-
lationships and moral judgment in Chinese people. International
Lennon, R., Eisenberg, N., & Carroll, J. (1983). The assessment of
Journal of Psychology, 27, 377400.
empathy in early childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 4, 295302. Ma, H. K., & Leung, M. C. (1991). Altruistic orientation in children:
Construction and validation of the child altruism inventory. Inter-
Leon, M. (1983). Chemical communication in mother-young inter-
national Journal of Psychology, 26, 745759.
action. In J. Vandenbergh (Ed.), Pheromones and reproduction in
mammals (pp. 3975). New York: Academic Press. Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming to-
Lepper, M. R. (1983). Social-control processes and the internal- gether. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
ization of social values: An attributional perspective. In E. T. MacLean, P. D. (1985). Brain evolution relating to family, play and
Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition the separation call. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 405417.
and social development: A sociocultural perspective Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Responses of abused and disadvan-
(pp. 294330). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University taged toddlers to distress in agemates: A study in the day care set-
Press. ting. Developmental Psychology, 21, 407412.
References 713

Markiewicz, D., Doyle, A. B., & Brendgen, M. (2001). The quality of Miller, P. A., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Shell, R. (1996). Rela-
adolescents friendships: Associations with mothers interper- tions of moral reasoning and vicarious emotion to young chil-
sonal relationships, attachments to parents and friends, and proso- drens prosocial behavior toward peers and adults. Developmental
cial behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 429445. Psychology, 32, 210219.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- Miller, P. A., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shell, R., & Gular, S.
tions for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Re- (1989). Socialization of empathic and sympathetic responding. In
view, 48, 224253. N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of empathy and related vi-
Marsh, D. T., Serafica, F. C., & Barenboim, C. (1981). Interrelation- carious responses: New Directions in child development
ships among perspective taking, interpersonal problem solving (pp. 6583). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
and interpersonal functioning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, Mills, R. S. L., & Grusec, J. (1989). Cognitive, affective, and behav-
138, 3748. ioral consequences of praising altruism. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., & Hollenstein, T. (in terly, 35, 299326.
press). Social dynamics in the preschool. Developmental Review. Moore, C., Barresi, J., & Thompson, C. (1998). The cognitive basis of fu-
Martin, G. B., & Clark, R. D. (1982). Distress crying in neonates: ture-oriented prosocial behavior. Social Development, 7, 198218.
Species and peer specificity. Developmental Psychology, 18, 39. Morris, W. N., Marshall, H. M., & Miller, R. S. (1973). The effect of
Matsuba, M. K., & Walker, L. J. (2005). Young adult moral exem- vicarious punishment on prosocial behavior in children. Journal of
plars: The making of self through stories. Journal of Research on Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 222236.
Adolescence, 15, 275287. Moore, C. W., & Allen, J. P. (1996). The effects of volunteering on
McGrath, M. P., & Power, T. G. (1990). The effects of reasoning and the young volunteer. Journal of Primary Prevention, 17, 231258.
choice on childrens prosocial behaviour. International Journal of Mulder, N. (1996). Inside Indonesian society: Cultural change in In-
Behavioural Development, 13, 345353. donesia. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Pepin Press.
McGrath, M. P., Wilson, S. R., & Frassetto, S. J. (1995). Why some
Munekata, H., & Ninomiya, K. (1985). Development of prosocial
forms of inductive reasoning are better than others: Effects of
moral judgments. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology,
cognitive focus, choice, and affect on childrens prosocial behav-
33, 157164.
ior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 347360.
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & van den Berg, F. (2003). The Strengths
McGuire, K. D., & Weisz, J. R. (1982). Social cognition and behav-
and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Further evidence for its
ior correlates of preadolescent chumship. Child Development,
reliability and validity in a community sample of Dutch chil-
53, 14781484.
dren and adolescents. European Child and Adolescent Psychia-
McLellan, J. A., & Youniss, J. (2003). Two systems of youth service: try, 12, 18.
Determinants of voluntary and required youth community ser-
Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., &
vice. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 4758.
Guthrie, I. K. (1999). Contemporaneous and longitudinal rela-
Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2000). Williams syndrome: tions of dispositional sympathy to emotionality, regulation, and
Cognition, personality, and adaptive behavior. Mental Retarda- social functioning. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 6697.
tion and Developmental Disabilities Research Review, 6, 148158.
Murphy, L. B. (1937). Social behavior and child personality: An ex-
Metz, E., McLellan, J., & Youniss, J. (2003). Types of voluntary ser-
ploratory study of some roots of sympathy. New York: Columbia
vice and adolescents civic development. Journal of Adolescent Re-
University Press.
search, 18, 188203.
Nadler, A., Romek, E., & Shapira-Friedman, A. (1979). Giving in the
Metz, E., & Youniss, J. (2003). A demonstration that school-based re-
kibbutz: Pro-social behavior of city and Kibbutz children as af-
quired service does not deter, but heightens, volunteerism. Politi-
fected by social responsibility and social pressure. Journal of
cal Science and Politics, 36, 281286.
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 10, 5772.
Midlarsky, E., & Bryan, J. H. (1967). Training charity in children.
Nagel, T. (1970). The possibility of altruism. Oxford, England:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 408415.
Clarendon Press.
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judg-
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Parental and early childhood
ment: How are conf licts between justice and interpersonal re-
predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kinder-
sponsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social
garten to high school. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 389394.
Psychology, 62, 541554.
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1994). Cultural inf luences on the National Center for Education. (1997). 1996 National Household Edu-
moral status of reciprocity and the discounting of endogenous mo- cation Survey: Student participation in community service activity.
tivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 592602. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. (NCES 97331)
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1998). The role of liking in percep- Nelson, D. A., & Crick, N. R. (1999). Rose-colored glasses: Examin-
tions of the moral responsibility to help: A cultural perspective. ing the social information-processing of prosocial young adoles-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 443469. cents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 1738.
Miller, J. G., Bersoff, D. M., & Harwood, R. L. (1990). Perceptions Nsamenang, A. B. (1992). Human development in cultural context: A
of social responsibilities in India and in the United States: Moral third-world perspective. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
imperatives or personal decisions? Journal of Personality and So- OConnor, M., Dollinger, S., Kennedy, S., & Pelletier-Smetko, P.
cial Psychology, 58, 3347. (1979). Prosocial behavior and psychopathology in emotionally
Miller, P., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggres- disturbed boys. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 301310.
sion and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, Oliner, S. P., & Oliner, P. M. (1988). The altruistic personality: Res-
103, 324344. cuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. New York: Free Press.
714 Prosocial Development

Olweus, D., & Endresen, I. M. (1998). The importance of sex-of- and religion in dialogue (pp. 5164). Oxford, England: Oxford
stimulus object: Age trends and sex differences in empathic re- University Press.
sponsiveness. Social Development, 7, 370388. Power, T. G., & Parke, R. D. (1986). Patterns of early socialization:
Owens, C. R., & Ascione, F. R. (1991). Effects of the models age, Mother- and father-infant interaction in the home. International
perceived similarity, and familiarity on childrens donating. Jour- Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 331341.
nal of Genetic Psychology, 152, 341357. Pratt, M. W., Hunsberger, B., Pancer, S. M., & Alisat, S. (2003). A
Pakaslahti, L., Karjalainen, A., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2002). longitudinal analysis of personal values socialization: Correlates
Relationships between adolescent prosocial problem-solving of a moral self-ideal in late adolescence. Social Development,
strategies, prosocial behaviour, and social acceptance. Interna- 12, 563585.
tional Journal of Behavioural Development, 26, 137144. Pratt, M. W., Skoe, E. E., & Arnold, M. L. (2004). Care reasoning de-
Pakaslahti, L., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2001). Peer-attributed velopment and family socialization patterns in later adolescence:
prosocial behavior among aggressive/preferred, aggressive/non- A longitudinal analysis. International Journal of Behavioral De-
preferred, non-aggressive/preferred, and non-aggressive/non- velopment, 28, 139147.
preferred adolescents. Personality and Individual Dif ferences, Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and
30, 903916. proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 172.
Panksepp, J. (1986). The psychobiology of prosocial behaviors: Sepa-
Pugh, M. J., & Hart, D. (1999). Identity development and peer group
ration distress, play, and altruism. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M.
participation. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Develop-
Cummings, & R. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biolog-
ment, 84, 5570.
ical and social origins (pp. 1957). Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press. Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1984). Roots, motives, and
patterns in childrens prosocial behavior. In E. Staub, D. Bar-Tal,
Pearl, R. (1985). Childrens understanding of others need for help:
J. Karylowski, & J. Reykowski (Eds.), Development and mainte-
Effects of problem explicitness and type. Child Development,
nance of prosocial behavior: International perspectives on positive
56, 735745.
behavior (pp. 8199). New York: Plenum Press.
Perry, D. G., Bussey, K., & Freiberg, K. (1981). Impact of adults ap-
Radke-Yarrow, M. R., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Chapman, M. (1983). Chil-
peals for sharing on the development of altruistic dispositions in
drens prosocial dispositions and behavior. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 127138.
Carmichaels manual of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 469546).
Perry, L. C., Perry, D. G., & Weiss, R. J. (1986). Age differences in New York: Wiley.
childrens beliefs about whether altruism makes the actor feel
good. Social Cognition, 4, 263269. Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Richardson, D. T., Susman,
A., & Martinez, P. (1994). Caring behavior in children of clini-
Persson, G. E. B. (2005). Young childrens prosocial and aggressive
cally depressed and well mothers. Child Development, 65,
behaviors and their experiences of being targeted for similar be-
14051414.
haviors by peers. Social Development, 14, 206228.
Ramsey, P. G. (1988). Social skills and peer status: A comparison of
Peterson, L. (1983). Inf luence of age, task competence, and responsi-
two socioeconomic groups. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 185202.
bility focus on childrens altruism. Developmental Psychology,
19, 141148. Rao, N., & Stewart, S. M. (1999). Cultural inf luences on sharer and
recipient behavior: Sharing in Chinese and Indian preschool chil-
Peterson, L., Ridley-Johnson, R., & Carter, C. (1984). The supersuit:
dren. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 219241.
An example of structured naturalistic observation of childrens
altruism. Journal of General Psychology, 110, 235241. Rehberg, H. R., & Richman, C. L. (1989). Prosocial behaviour in pre-
Phillips, D., McCartney, K., & Scarr, S. (1987). Child-care quality school children: A look at the interaction of race, gender, and
and childrens social development. Developmental Psychology, family composition. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
23, 537543. ment, 12, 385401.
Phinney, J., Feshbach, N., & Farver, J. (1986). Preschool childrens Rheingold, H. L. (1982). Little childrens participation in the
responses to peer crying. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, work of adults, a nascent prosocial behavior. Child Develop-
1, 207219. ment, 53, 114125.
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). Sharing in the
Press. (Original work published 1932) second year of life. Child Development, 47, 11481158.
Pilgrim, C., & Rueda-Riedle, A. (2002). The importance of social con- Rice, M. E., & Grusec, J. E. (1975). Saying and doing: Effects on ob-
text in cross-cultural comparisons: First graders in Columbia and server performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
the United States. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163, 283295. 32, 584593.
Plomin, R., Emde, R. N., Braungart, J. M., Campos, J., Corley, R., Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relation
Fulker, D. W., et al. (1993). Genetic change and continuity from among Australian children and implications for psychological
14 to 20 months: The MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study. Child well-being. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 3342.
Development, 64, 13541376. Roberts, W., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy, emotional expressive-
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Maiti, A. K. (1994). ness, and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67, 449470.
Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of emotion. Mono- Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Corley, R. P. (2001). Dispositional
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2/3, cheerfulness: Early genetic and environmental inf luences. In
Serial No. 240), 167186, 250283. R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early childhood: Ge-
Post, S. G. (2001). The tradition of agape. In S. G. Post & L. G. Un- netic and environmental inf luences on developmental change
derwood (Eds.), Altruism and altruistic love: Science, philosophy, (pp. 163177). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
References 715

Robinson, J. L., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Emde, R. N. (1994). Patterns of Saklofske, D. H., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1983). Impulsiveness and
development in early empathic behavior: Environmental and child venturesomeness in Canadian children. Psychological Reports,
constitutional inf luences. Social Development, 3, 125145. 52, 147152.
Robinson, J. L., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Emde, R. N. (2001). Relationship Savin-Williams, R. C., Small, S. A., & Zeldin, R. S. (1981). Domi-
context as a moderator of sources of individual difference in em- nance and altruism among adolescent males: A comparison of
pathic development. In R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy ethological and psychological methods. Ethology and Sociobiol-
to early childhood: Genetic and environmental inf luences on de- ogy, 2, 167176.
velopmental change (pp. 257268). Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- Sawyer, K. S., Denham, S., DeMulder, E., Blair, K., Auerbach-Major,
versity Press. S., & Levitas, J. (2002). The contribution of older siblings reac-
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Lon- tions to emotions to preschoolers emotional and social compe-
don: Oxford University Press. tence. Marriage and Family Review, 34, 183212.
Rohner, R. P. (1975). They love me, they love me not. New Haven, CT: Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi-
HRAF Press. ronments: A theory of genotype environment effects. Child De-
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (2004). Childrens strategies and goals in velopment, 54, 424435.
response to help-giving and help-seeking tasks within a friend- Schenk, V. M., & Grusec, J. E. (1987). A comparison of prosocial be-
ship. Child Development, 75, 749780. havior of children with and without day care experience. Merrill-
Rosenhan, D. L. (1970). The natural socialization of altruistic auton- Palmer Quarterly, 33, 231240.
omy. In J. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping Scourfield, J., John, B., Martin, N., & McGuffin, P. (2004). The devel-
behavior (pp. 251268). New York: Academic Press. opment of prosocial behaviour in children and adolescents: A twin
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 927935.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Tempera-
ment and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quar- Searcy, E., & Eisenberg, N. (1992). Defensiveness in response to
terly, 40, 2139. aid from a sibling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
62, 422433.
Rousseau, J. J. (1962). Emile. New York: Columbia University Press.
Sebanc, A. M. (2003). The friendship features of preschool children:
(Original work published 1773)
Links with prosocial behavior and aggression. Social Develop-
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking ment, 12, 249268.
during simulation of action: A PET investigation of agency. Na-
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychol-
ture and Neuroscience, 4, 546550.
ogy: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 514.
Rudolph, K. D., & Conley, C. S. (2005). The socioemotional costs and Semel, E., & Rosner, S. R. (2003). Understanding Williams syndrome:
benefits of social-evaluative concerns: Do girls care too much? Behavioral patterns and interventions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Journal of Personality, 73, 115137.
Serow, R. C., & Solomon, D. (1979). Classroom climates and stu-
Rushton, J. P. (1975). Generosity in children: Immediate and long- dents intergroup behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology,
term effects of modeling, preaching, and moral judgment. Journal 71, 669676.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 459466. Sharma, V. (1988). Effect of birthorder, age and sex on helping be-
Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. K. B., & haviour of children. Indian Journal of Psychometry and Educa-
Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability tion, 19, 9196.
of individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Shek, D. T. L., & Ma, H. K. (2001). Parent-adolescent conf lict and
chology, 50, 11921198. adolescent antisocial and prosocial behavior: A longitudinal study
Rushton, J. P., & Littlefield, C. (1979). The effects of age, amount of in a Chinese context. Adolescence, 36, 545555.
modeling, and a success experience on 7- to 11-year-old chil- Shigetomi, C. C., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1981). Sex dif-
drens generosity. Journal of Moral Education, 9, 5556. ferences in childrens altruistic behavior and reputations for help-
Rushton, J. P., Russell, R. J. H., & Wells, P. A. (1984). Genetic simi- fulness. Developmental Psychology, 17, 434437.
larity theory: Beyond kin selection altruism. Behavior Genetics, Silva, F. (1992). Assessing the child and adolescent personality: A decade
14, 179193. of research. Personality and Individual Dif ferences, 13, 11631181.
Rushton, J. P., & Teachman, G. (1978). The effects of positive rein- Skoe, E. E. A., Hansen, K. L., Morch, W.-T., Bakke, I., Hoffmann, T.,
forcement, attributions, and punishment on model induced altruism Larsen, B., et al. (1999). Care-based moral reasoning in Norwe-
in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 322325. gian and Canadian early adolescents: A cross-national compari-
Russell, A., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., & Olsen, S. F. (2003). Chil- son. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 280291.
drens sociable and aggressive behaviour with peers: A compari- Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., & Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of mind
son of the United States and Australia, and contributions of and peer acceptance in preschool children. British Journal of De-
temperament and parenting style. International Journal of Behav- velopmental Psychology, 20, 545564.
ioral Development, 27, 7486. Slote, M. (2001). Morals from motives. Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
Saarni, C., Mumme, D. L., & Campos, J. J. (1998). Emotional devel- versity Press.
opment: Action, communication, and understanding. In W. Slote, M. (2004). Autonomy and empathy. Social Philosophy and Pol-
Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of icy Foundation, 21, 293309.
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality devel- Smith, A. (1982). A theory of moral sentiments. Indianapolis, IN: Lib-
opment (5th ed., pp. 237309). New York: Wiley. erty Classics. (Original work published 1759)
Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the new- Smith, C. L. (1983). Exhortations, rehearsal, and childrens prosocial
born. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175176. behavior. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5, 261271.
716 Prosocial Development

Smith, C. L., Gelfand, D. M., Hartmann, D. P., & Partlow, M. E. Y. Stevenson, H. W. (1991). The development of prosocial behavior in
(1979). Childrens causal attributions regarding help giving. large-scale collective societies: China and Japan. In R. A.
Child Development, 50, 203210. Hinde & J. Groebel, (Eds.), Cooperation and prosocial behav-
Sneed, C. (2002). Correlates and implications for agreeableness in iour (pp. 89105). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
children. Journal of Psychology, 136, 5967. Press.
Sober, E., & Wilson, D. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psy- Stewart, R. B., & Marvin, R. S. (1984). Sibling relations: The role of
chology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- conceptual perspective-taking in the ontogeny of sibling caregiv-
sity Press. ing. Child Development, 55, 13221332.
Solomon, D., Battistich, V., & Watson, M. (1993, March). A longitu- Stewart, S. M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2000). Inf luences on chil-
dinal investigation of the ef fects of a school intervention program drens sharing in a multicultural setting. Journal of Cross-Cul-
on childrens social development. Paper presented at the biennial tural Psychology, 31, 333348.
meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Stewart, S. M., Rao, N., Bond, M. H., Fielding, R., McBride-Chang,
Orleans, LA. C.,& Kennard, B. D. (1998). Chinese dimensions of parenting:
Solomon, D., Battistich, V., Watson, M., Schaps, E., & Lewis, C. Broadening Western predictors and outcomes. International Jour-
(2000). A six-district study of educational changes: Direct and nal of Psychology, 33, 345358.
mediated effects of the Child Development Project. Social Psy- Stillwell, R., & Dunn, J. (1985). Continuities in sibling relationships:
chology of Education, 4, 351. Patterns of aggression and friendliness. Journal of Child Psychol-
Solomon, D., Watson, M. S., Delucchi, K. L., Schaps, E., & Battis- ogy and Psychiatry, 26, 627637.
tich, V. (1988). Enhancing childrens prosocial behavior in Stoneman, Z., Brody, G. H., & MacKinnon, C. E. (1986). Same-sex
the classroom. American Educational Research Journal, 25, and cross-sex siblings: Activity choices, roles, behavior, and gen-
527554. der stereotypes. Sex Roles, 15, 495511.
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal At- Strayer, F. F., Wareing, S., & Rushton, J. P. (1979). Social constraints
tributes Questionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and on naturally occurring preschool altruism. Ethology and Sociobi-
masculinity-femininity. JSAS: Catalog of Selected Documents in ology, 1, 311.
Psychology, 4(43, Ms. No. 617). Strayer, J. (1993). Childrens concordant emotions and cognitions
Spinrad, T. L. (1999, April). Toddlers vicarious emotional responses in response to observed emotions. Child Development, 64,
and prosocial behaviors: The role of infant temperament and ma- 188201.
ternal behavior. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1989). Childrens empathy and role tak-
Child Development, Albuquerque, NM. ing: Child and parental factors, and relations to prosocial behav-
Spinrad, T. L., Losoya, S., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, ior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 227239.
S. A., Cumberland, A., et al. (1999). The relation of parental af- Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1997a). Childrens personal distance and
fect and encouragement to childrens moral emotions and behav- their empathy: Indices of interpersonal closeness. International
iour. Journal of Moral Education, 28, 323337. Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 385403.
Stanhope, L., Bell, R. Q., & Parker-Cohen, N. Y. (1987). Tempera- Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1997b). Facial and verbal measures of
ment and helping behavior in preschool children. Developmental childrens emotions and empathy. International Journal of Behav-
Psychology, 23, 347353. ioral Development, 20, 627649.
Staub, E. (1971a). A child in distress: The inf luence of nurturance Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (2004a). Empathy and observed anger and
and modeling on childrens attempts to help. Developmental Psy- aggression in 5-year-olds. Social Development, 13, 113.
chology, 5, 124132. Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (2004b). Childrens anger, emotional ex-
Staub, E. (1971b). The use of role playing and induction in chil- pressiveness, and empathy: Relations with parents empathy,
drens learning of helping and sharing behavior. Child Develop- emotional expressiveness, and parenting practices. Social Devel-
ment, 42, 805817. opment, 13, 229254.
Staub, E. (1979). Positive social behavior and morality: Vol. 2. Social- Stukas, A. A., Snyder, M., & Clary, E. G. (1999). The effects of
ization and development. New York: Academic Press. mandatory volunteerism on intentions to volunteer. Psychologi-
Staub, E. (1992). The origins of caring, helping, and nonaggression: cal Science, 10, 5964.
Parental socialization, the family system, schools, and cultural Stukas, A. A., Jr., Switzer, G. E., Dew, M. A., Goycoolea, J. M., &
inf luence. In P. M. Oliner, L. Baron, L. A. Blum, D. L. Krebs, & Simmons, R. G. (1999). Parental helping models, gender, and
M. Z. Smolenska (Eds.), Embracing the other: Philosophical, psy- service-learning. Journal of Prevention and Intervention in the
chological, and historical perspectives on altruism (pp. 390412). Community, 18, 518.
New York: New York University Press. Suda, W., & Fouts, G. (1980). Effects of peer presence on helping
Staub, E. (2003). The psychology of good and evil: Why children, in introverted and extroverted children. Child Development,
adults, and groups help and harm others. New York: Cambridge 51, 12721275.
University Press. Sugiyama, K., Matsui, H., Satoh, C., Yoshimi, Y., & Takeuchi, M.
Staub, E., & Noerenberg, H. (1981). Property rights, deservingness, (1992). Effects of self-efficacy and outcome expectation on ob-
reciprocity, friendship: The transactional character of childrens servational learning of altruistic behavior. Japanese Journal of
sharing behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Psychology, 63, 295302.
40, 271289. Switzer, G. E., Simmons, R. G., Dew, M. A., Regalski, J. M., & Wang,
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view C. (1995). The effect of a school-based helper program on adoles-
from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: cent self-image, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Early Adoles-
Basic Books. cence, 15, 429455.
References 717

Szynal-Brown, C., & Morgan, R. R. (1983). The effects of reward on related responding from their effortful control and parents ex-
tutors behaviors in a cross-age tutoring context. Journal of Exper- pressivity. Developmental Psychology, 40, 911926.
imental Child Psychology, 36, 196208. Vandell, D. L., Henderson, V. K., & Wilson, K. S. (1988). A longitu-
Termine, N. T., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Infants responses to their dinal study of children with day-care experiences of varying qual-
mothers expressions of joy and sadness. Developmental Psychol- ity. Child Development, 59, 12861292.
ogy, 24, 223229. van der Mark, I. L., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg,
Tesson, G., Lewko, J. H., & Bigelow, B. J. (1987). The social rules M. J. (2002). Development of empathy in girls during the second
that children use in their interpersonal relations. Contributions of year of life: Associations with parenting, attachment, and tempera-
Human Development, 18, 3657. ment. Social Development, 11, 451468.
Thompson, C., Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1997). The development of Van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). A preliminary report on expressive
future-oriented prudence and altruism in preschoolers. Cognitive movements and communication in the Gombe Stream chim-
Development, 12, 199212. panzees. In P. Jay (Ed.), Primates (pp. 313374). New York: Holt,
Tietjen, A. (1986). Prosocial reasoning among children and adults in a Rhinehart, & Winston.
Papua, New Guinea society. Developmental Psychology, 22, 861868. Vitaglione, G. D., & Barnett, M. A. (2003). Assessing a new dimen-
Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., Piche, C., & Royer, N. sion of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and
(1992). A prosocial scale for the preschool behaviour question- punishing desires. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 301324.
naire: Concurrent and predictive correlates. International Journal Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1990). Predicting stable
of Behavioral Development, 15, 227245. peer rejection from kindergarten to grade 1. Journal of Clinical
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Child Psychology, 19, 257264.
Review of Biology, 46, 3557. Vitaro, F., Gagnon, C., & Tremblay, R. E. (1991). Teachers and
Trivers, R. L. (1983). The evolution of cooperation. In D. L. Bridge- mothers assessment of childrens behaviors from kindergarten
man (Ed.), The nature of prosocial development (pp. 95112). to grade 2: Stability and change within and across informants.
New York: Academic Press. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13,
Trommsdorff, G. (1991). Child-rearing and childrens empathy. Per- 325343.
ceptual Motor Skills, 72, 387390. Volling, B. L. (2001). Early attachment relationships as predictors of
Trommsdorff, G. (1995). Person-context relations as developmental preschool childrens emotion regulation with a distressed sibling.
conditions for empathy and prosocial action: A cross-cultural Early Education and Development, 12, 185207.
analysis. In T. A. Kindermann & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Development Wachs, T. D. (1994). Genetics, nurture and social development: An
of person-context relations (pp. 189208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. alternative viewpoint. Social Development, 3, 6670.
Trommsdorff, G., & Friedlmeier, W. (1999). Motivational conf lict Warden, D., Cheyne, B., Christie, D., Fitzpatrick, H., & Reid, K.
and prosocial behaviour of kindergarten children. International (2003). Assessing childrens perceptions of prosocial and anti-
Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 413429. social behavior. Educational Psychology, 23, 547567.
Trommsdorff, G., & Kornadt, H.-J. (2003). Parent-child relations in Warden, D., & Mackinnon, S. (2003). Prosocial bullies, bullies, and
cross-cultural perspective. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of victims: An investigation of their sociometric status, empathy,
dynamics in parent-child relations (pp. 271306). Thousand and social problem-solving strategies. British Journal of Develop-
Oaks, CA: Sage. mental Psychology, 21, 367385.
Tucker, C. J., Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. Waters, E., Hay, D., & Richters, J. (1986). Infant-parent attachment
(1999). Older siblings as socializers of younger siblings empathy. and the origins of prosocial and antisocial behavior. In D. Olweus,
Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 176198. J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial
Turnbull, C. M. (1972). The mountain people. New York: Simon and prosocial behavior: Research, theories, and issues
& Schuster. (pp. 97125). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Turner, P. H., & Harris, M. B. (1984). Parental attitudes and pre- Weissbrod, C. S. (1976). Noncontingent warmth induction, cogni-
school childrens social competence. Journal of Genetic Psychol- tive style, and childrens imitative donation and rescue effort
ogy, 144, 105113. behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
Uggen, C., & Janikula, J. (1999). Volunteerism and arrest in the tran- 274281.
sition to adulthood. Social Forces, 78, 331362. Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Widaman, K., & ONeil, R. (2001). Linkages
Underwood, B., & Moore, B. (1982). Perspective-taking and altru- between childrens social and academic competence: A longitudi-
ism. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 143173. nal analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 39, 463481.
Underwood, L. G. (2001). The human experience of compassionate Wentzel, K. R. (2003). Sociometric status and adjustment in middle
love: Conceptual mapping and data from selected studies. In S. G. school: A longitudinal study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 528.
Post & L. G. Underwood (Eds.), Altruism and altruistic love: Sci- Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. A. (2004). Friendship in
ence, philosophy, and religion in dialogue (pp. 7288). Oxford, middle school: Inf luences on motivation and school adjustment.
England: Oxford University Press. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 195203.
Ungerer, J. A., Dolby, R., Waters, B., Barnett, B., Kelk, N., & Lewin, Wentzel, K. R., & McNamara, C. C. (1999). Interpersonal relation-
V. (1990). The early development of empathy: Self-regulation and ships, emotional distress, and prosocial behavior in middle
individual differences in the first year. Motivation and Emotion, school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 114125.
14, 93106. White, G. M., & Burnam, M. A. (1975). Socially cued altruism: Ef-
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, fects of modeling, instructions, and age on public and private do-
A., & Losoya, S. H. (2004). Prediction of childrens empathy- nations. Child Development, 46, 559563.
718 Prosocial Development

Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1973). A cross-cultural analysis of Youniss, J., & Metz, E. (2004). Longitudinal gains in civic develop-
sex differences in the behavior of children aged 3 through 11. ment through school-based required service. Manuscript submit-
Journal of Social Psychology, 91, 171188. ted for publication.
Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of dif ferent worlds. Zaff, J. F., Moore, K. A., Papillo, A. R., & Williams, S. (2003). Im-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. plications of extracurricular activity participation during ado-
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of 6 cul- lescence on positive outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Research,
tures: A psychocultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- 18, 599630.
versity Press. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P. M., Welsh, J. D., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Psy-
Williams, W. (1991). Javanese lives: Men and women in modern In- chophysiological correlates of empathy and prosocial behaviors in
donesian society. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. preschool children with problem behaviors. Development and
Psychopathology, 7, 2748.
Wilson, B. J. (2003). The role of attentional processes in childrens
prosocial behavior with peers: Attention shifting and emotion. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., McKnew, D., & Radke-Yarrow,
Developmental and Psychopathology, 15, 313329. M. (1984). Altruism, aggression, and social interactions in
young children with a manic-depressive parent. Child Develop-
Wilson, C. C., Piazza, C. C., & Nagle, R. J. (1990). Investigation of ment, 55, 112122.
the effect of consistent and inconsistent behavioral example upon
Zahn-Waxler, C., Iannotti, R., & Chapman, M. (1982). Peers and
childrens donation behaviors. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
prosocial development. In K. H. Rubin & H. S. Ross (Eds.), Peer
151, 361376.
relationships and social skills in childhood (pp. 133162). New
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, York: Springer-Verlag.
MA: Harvard University Press.
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
University Press. The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 109137). New York:
Wise, P. S., & Cramer, S. H. (1988). Correlates of empathy and cogni- Academic Press.
tive style in early adolescence. Psychological Reports, 63, 179192. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child
Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect rearing and childrens prosocial initiations toward victims of dis-
induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social tress. Child Development, 50, 319330.
Psychology, 58, 899908. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1983). Early altru-
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal dispersion in relation to social ism and guilt. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5, 247259.
behaviour. New York: Hafner. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M.
Yarrow, M. R., & Scott, P. M. (1972). Imitation of nurturant and non- (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psy-
nurturant models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, chology, 28, 126136.
23, 259270.
Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. N. (1992). The development
Yarrow, M. R., Scott, P. M., & Waxler, C. Z. (1973). Learning con- of empathy in twins. Developmental Psychology, 28, 10381047.
cern for others. Developmental Psychology, 8, 240260.
Zahn-Waxler, C., Schiro, K., Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Schmitz,
Yarrow, M. R., Waxler, C. Z., Barrett, D., Darby, J., King, R., Pickett, S. (2001). Empathy and prosocial patterns in young MZ and DZ
M., et al. (1976). Dimensions and correlates of prosocial behavior twins: Development and genetic and environmental inf luences. In
in young children. Child Development, 47, 118125. R. N. Emde & J. K. Hewitt (Eds.), Infancy to early childhood
Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1996a). Community service and political- (pp. 141162). New York: Oxford University Press.
moral identity in adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1985). Why does
6, 271284. childrens generosity increase with age: Susceptibility to experi-
Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1996b). A development perspective on com- menter inf luence or altruism? Child Development, 56, 746756.
munity service in adolescence. Social Development, 5, 85111. Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D. P., Gelfand, D. M., & Vinciguerra, P.
Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1998). Community service and political (1985). Gender differences in altruistic reputation. Developmen-
identity development in adolescence. Journal of Social Issues, tal Psychology, 21, 97101.
43, 495512.
Zeldin, R. A., Savin-Williams, R. C., & Small, S. A. (1984). Dimen-
Young, S. K., Fox, N. A., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1999). The relations be- sions of prosocial behavior in adolescent males. Journal of Social
tween temperament and empathy in 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 123, 159168.
Psychology, 35, 11891197.
Zeldin, S., & Topitzes, D. (2002). Neighborhood experiences, com-
Youniss, J. (1980). Parents and peers in social development: A Sulli- munity connection, and positive beliefs about adolescents
van-Piaget perspective. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. among urban adults and youth. Journal of Community Psychol-
Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., & Mazer, B. (2001). Voluntary service, ogy, 30, 647669.
peer group orientation, and civic engagement. Journal of Adoles- Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S. H., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M.,
cent Research, 16, 456468. Guthrie, I. K., et al. (2002). The relations of parental warmth and
Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., Su, Y., & Yates, M. (1999). The role of positive expressiveness to childrens empathy-related responding
community service in identity development: Normative, uncon- and social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development,
ventional, and deviant orientations. Journal of Adolescent Re- 73, 893915.
search, 14, 248261.
Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., & Yates, M. (1999). Religion, community
service, and identity in American youth. Journal of Adolescence,
22, 243253.
CHAPTER 12

Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth


KENNETH A. DODGE, JOHN D. COIE, and DONALD LYNAM

DEFINING THE DOMAIN OF AGGRESSION AND COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL PROCESSES AS


ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 720 MEDIATORS 756
DIMENSIONS OF AGGRESSION AND OTHER Social-Situational Factors That Alter Cognitive-
ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 722 Emotional Processes 757
HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES ON THEORY AND Social Information Processing 759
EMPIRICAL INQUIRY 722 Mediation of Life Experiences through Acquired
CULTURAL DIFFERENCES AND AGGRESSION 723 Processing Patterns 763
AGGRESSIVE AND ANTISOCIAL DEVELOPMENT Latent Knowledge Structures 763
IN THE HUMAN SPECIES 723 TREATMENT AND PREVENTION OF ANTISOCIAL
The Emergence of Anger and Physical Aggression in BEHAVIOR 765
Infancy 724 Stimulant Medication 765
Aggression and Conf lict in the Second Year of Life 724 Parent-Based Approaches 765
Aggression during the Preschool Years 725 School-Based Approaches 767
Aggression during the Elementary School Years 725 Social Cognitive Skills Training 767
Aggression during the Adolescent Years 727 Combining Approaches 767
GENDER AND AGGRESSION 729 Comprehensive Approaches to Prevention 768
Physical Aggression 729 Early Comprehensive Approaches 768
Antisocial Behavior 730 CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS 769
Conduct Disorder 730 Conclusion 1: Predictors Differ across Aggressive Acts,
Female Aggression 730 Personalities, and Patterns 769
Gender Differences in Process 731 Conclusion 2: Risk Factors Operate in Biopsychosocial
Symbiotic Development 770
DETERMINANTS OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
IN ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 731 Conclusion 3: A General Consensus Model of the
Development of Aggressive Behavior Patterns Is
Genetics 731
within Reach 771
Dispositional Factors 734
Conclusion 4: The Most Important Discoveries in the
Neuropsychological Factors 737 Next Decade Will Come from Studies of Gene-
Biological Factors 740 Environment Interactions, Modeling of
Ecological Factors and Social Stressors 743 Developmental Trajectories, and Prevention
Processes in Early Family Socialization 746 Experiments 771
Childhood Peer Factors 750 REFERENCES 771
Adolescent Family Processes 751
Adolescent Peer Processes 752

Over the past 40 years, crime rates have risen steadily in homicide arrest rates and assault victimization reports
nearly all countries that keep reasonably accurate reveal little secular change since 1965, juvenile violent
records (Rutter, Giller, & Hagell, 1998). More dramatic crime rates have risen markedly: Since 1965, the homi-
has been the increase in violent crime by young juveniles cide rate by juveniles aged 18 or under has increased by
in the United States. Even though adult robbery and close to 400% (Blumstein, 2000).

The authors wish to thank Lynda Harrison for her careful Nancy Eisenberg for her valuable editing. The first author is
scrutiny of the text, Amber Runion for assembling over 1,000 grateful for the support of Senior Research Scientist Award 5
references, Terrie Moffitt for her insightful review, and K05 DA-015226.

719
720 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Secular swings in juvenile violence have been espe- 2. What stability and change occur in the life course of
cially dramatic. Between 1988 and 1997, the number of individual differences in antisocial behavior?
juvenile arrests increased by 35% and arrests of juveniles 3. Why do some individuals become more antisocial
under age 13 for violent crimes increased by 45% (Snyder, than others?
2003). Since then, however, juvenile arrests and violent 4. What promising avenues exist in the prevention and
crime receded back to the level of the 1980s (Blumstein & treatment of aggressive behavior in children?
Wallman, 2000). The decrease in adolescent violence,
however, is attributable primarily to decreases in crime
among older juveniles. While the aggravated assault arrest DEFINING THE DOMAIN OF AGGRESSION
rate among older juveniles in 2001 dropped 38% from its AND ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR
peak in 1994, the rate for juveniles under age 13 dropped
only 3% (Snyder, 2003). Thus, the violent crime rate Previous editions of the Handbook have restricted the
among younger children remains dramatically higher than discussion of antisocial behavior to aggression, perhaps
it was 40 years ago. Similarly, the prevalence of psychi- because that form of antisocial behavior has been stud-
atrically diagnosed conduct disorder is several times ied most often. Over the past 10 years, research has in-
greater than it was 70, or even 40, years ago, in both the creasingly emphasized individual differences in
United States (Robins, 1999), and the United Kingdom, aggressive behavior, and this shift has led to the recogni-
even controlling for reporter effects (Collishaw, Maughan, tion that aggressive behaviors often occur in a context of
Goodman, & Pickles, 2004). other antisocial behaviors, such as noncompliance with
The annual aggregate burden of crime in the United adults, lying, stealing, destruction of property in the
States now exceeds $1 trillion (D. Anderson, 1999). In childhood years, and more serious antisocial behaviors
the eyes of the U.S. public, crime and violence perenni- such as illegal drug selling, sexual assault, burglary, and
ally rank among the most important problems facing this other violent crimes. Even the psychiatric diagnosis of
country (Berke, 1994). Both the magnitude of the cur- conduct disorder has evolved over the past half-century
rent problem and the urgency of a solution are repre- to address the co-occurrence of physical aggression with
sented in the U.S. Surgeon Generals national objectives other forms of aggressive behavior (e.g., verbal aggres-
for 2010 to reduce the prevalence of physical fighting sion) and other antisocial behaviors (e.g., illicit sub-
among adolescents to less than 32% and to reduce the stance use; Robins, 1999). This comorbidity of
prevalence of weapon-carrying on high school property aggression with other antisocial behaviors suggests that
to less than 4.9% (U.S. Department of Health and an understanding of the etiology and developmental
Human Services, 2000). course of aggression might be enhanced by including it
The rapid swings in U.S. crime rates have been paral- in the broader class of antisocial behaviors (Menard &
leled by growth in research on the development of aggres- Elliott, 1994). Substance use and abuse, as well as par-
sive and other antisocial behavior. Research in the past 2 ticipation in drug sales activities, have particular signif-
decades has increasingly focused on the development of icance in any discussion of contemporary antisocial
chronically antisocial individuals, in contrast to research behavior because the increased rate of violence in the
on species-wide patterns in aggressive behavior. This shift United States, especially for teenage males, parallels the
in emphasis has grown from recognition that a small rise in certain kinds of illegal substance use in our soci-
group of chronically violent youth are responsible for over ety. Nonetheless, an explicit treatment of developmental
half of all crimes (Howell, Krisberg, & Jones, 1995), that issues relating to substance use and trafficking is not in-
criminal careers can be charted across the life span begin- cluded in this chapter because of space limitations.
ning in childhood (Blumstein & Cohen, 1987), that career Although definitions of aggression and antisocial be-
criminals cost society up to $2 million each (M. Cohen, havior usually have considerable overlap, there are im-
1998), and that citizens may be willing to pay a great deal portant differences between them. The definition of
in extra taxes for interventions that could confidently re- aggression embraced by Parke and Slaby (1983), be-
duce crime (Cohen, Rust, Steen, & Tidd, 2004). havior that is aimed at harming or injuring another per-
Four questions guide the organization of this chapter: son or persons (p. 50), is similar to the broader
definition that Loeber (1985) offered for antisocial be-
1. What is the human species-wide developmental haviors, namely those that inflict physical or mental
course of aggression and antisocial behavior? harm or property loss or damage on others, and which
Defining the Domain of Aggression and Antisocial Behavior 721

may or may not constitute the breaking of criminal quite obvious. For those who study individual differ-
laws (p. 6). This latter definition includes aggression ences, the need for precision in defining aggressive acts
but is not restricted to it. Sometimes aggression is de- is not as important as precision in identifying aggressive
fined broadly enough to include property loss or dam- persons. Because the identification of aggressive per-
age, as in the case of instrumental aggression; however, sons often depends on consensus in social judgments by
the feature of instrumental aggression that makes it ag- third parties, the need to be clear about what constitutes
gressive, in our view, is the use or threat of force to ob- aggression might not seem important. Nonetheless, the
tain possession. An important difference in the two need to be clear about the nature of this judgment invari-
definitions is the inclusion of intent in the Parke and ably brings us back to the definitional issue. The topo-
Slaby (1983) definition, a distinction that we endorse. graphical approach to defining aggression, taken from
Part of the ambiguity in definition arises from the ethology, focuses on the form of the act itself and has
fact that governmental and professional institutions have been successful with lower-order species but is less reli-
their own contextually based definitions for antisocial able with humans because of the greater range of diver-
behavioral problems. Loebers (1985) definition of anti- sity in human aggressive behavior (Hartup & deWit,
social behavior makes reference to violations of crimi- 1974). Antecedent approaches focus on the conditions
nal code. For children and adolescents, legal violations eliciting aggression and emphasize the importance of
constitute delinquency, although some delinquent acts determining the intent involved. This approach has its
such as truancy and running away from home do not con- origins in the concept of the goal response introduced by
form to usual definitions of antisocial behavior. Con- Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, and Sears (1939).
versely, fighting and threats of harm occur frequently Defining aggression by the outcome (Buss, 1961) avoids
among school-aged children, and yet these behaviors are the pitfalls of inferring intention but suffers from other
rarely considered as a possible cause for arrest even problems. First, injury can result unintentionally from
though the same acts by adults would be construed as as- the behavior of others. Second, this definition excludes
sault. In education, the terms Serious Emotional Distur- behaviors that have obvious aggressive intent but fail,
bance (SED) and Behavior Disorders (BD) have been somehow, to lead to injury. Third, such an approach em-
applied to children whose classroom behavior is too dis- phasizes the instrumental aspect of aggression while ig-
ruptive to allow for the education of other children. noring the emotional component. Finally, Walters and
These children are typically removed to special educa- Parke (1964) have suggested that aggression is deter-
tion classrooms with other similar children. Conduct mined by the social judgment of observers operating in a
disorder is a psychiatric term that refers to a disorder specific cultural context that must be understood as in-
that is diagnosed by the frequency of problem behaviors fluencing the definition of aggression. Such an approach
exhibited by a child or adolescent across a given period clearly fits legal approaches to defining guilt (Dodge,
(three or more across a 6-month period). The term is ap- 1991) and may be necessary to any culturally sensitive
plied to individuals rather than to acts and is considered research strategy for studying aggression, but it also
to reflect a diagnostic syndrome. Oppositional defiant suggests the challenge of defining aggression in the ab-
disorder, which includes disobedience or disrespect for stract. Viewed in the context of various cultures or so-
adults often accompanied by irritability, is a psychiatric cial contexts, the same act might be classified as
syndrome that is distinct from conduct disorder and is aggressive or not depending on how it contrasts with
most often applied to elementary-school children but is base rates for similar behavior in each of these contexts.
thought to predict conduct disorder. Antisocial personal- Also, identification might depend on the perspectives of
ity disorder is a chronic psychiatric condition that re- those whose resources are being controlled (Hawley,
quires a life-persistent pattern of antisocial behavior Little, & Pasupathi, 2002). One way to understand this
(Hinshaw & Anderson, 1996). point in the context of contemporary American culture
In earlier Handbook chapters on aggression, includ- is to note that some acts might be considered as acts of
ing our own, the merits and flaws in various approaches aggression when exhibited by a girl but not when exhib-
to defining aggression have been discussed at length. As ited by a boy.
noted earlier, the study of aggression prior to the 1980s These problems in defining aggression have led
focused more on the behavioral analysis of aggression Tremblay, Hartup, and Archer (2005) to suggest that a
than on individual differences in aggressive behavior weakness in the study of aggression is the ambiguity in
and the need for precision in defining aggression was its definition. We conclude that there is less ambiguity
722 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

to the definition of aggression than in observers ability forms and functions of aggression. Little, Brauner,
to determine when it has actually occurred. This ambi- Jones, Nock, and Hawley (2003a) have demonstrated the
guity plays itself out in everyday life, leading, perhaps, validity of a dimensional system from self-reports by
to much of the conflict and violence that surrounds us. 1,723 German adolescents from grades 5 to 10. The
We suggest that most people would readily agree with form of aggression varies as direct, involving verbal or
the very simple definition offered by Parke and Slaby physical attack, or relational, involving damage to the
(1983): Aggression is behavior aimed at harming or in- targets friendships or inclusion in the peer group (Crick
juring another person or persons. The problem, as & Grotpeter, 1995). The function of aggression is either
Tremblay and most other reviewers have pointed out, is instrumental, occurring in the anticipation of self-
that it is not always easy to decide whether harm was in- serving outcomes, or reactive, occurring as an angry de-
tended. This ambiguity, in our view, is not a defini- fensive response to goal blocking or provocation (Little,
tional issue but a measurement issue. To confound the Jones, Henrich, & Hawley, 2003b). This latter distinc-
conceptual definition with the criterion for measure- tion follows from Hartups (1974) distinction between
ment is to fall prey to the limits of operationisma instrumental and hostile aggression and Dodge,
philosophical approach to science that guided much of Lochman, Harnish, Bates, and Pettits (1997) empirical
early experimental psychology. Human aggression may distinction between reactive and proactive aggressive
best be viewed as a heterogeneous category of human behavior in chronically assaultive youth. Dodge and
behavior, defined simply as acts intended to harm oth- Coie (1987) demonstrated that reactive and proactive
ers, which requires a multifactor framework (Brain, aggressive boys differ in social information-processing
1994) to address the measurement problem. In other patterns. Little et al. (2003b) found that the reactive
words, no single statement can adequately bound the group showed consistent maladaptive behavior patterns
acts that we would want to describe as aggressive be- not found in the instrumental group. Finally, adaptive
haviors. Instead, a judgment is made that relies on cues functions of aggressive behavior, such as providing an
to intent, outcome potential, biological arousal, and so- opportunity for social-cognitive growth in very young
cial context. As with many other behavioral constructs, children attracting peers interest (Rodkin, Farmer,
aggression is a fuzzy set, and unreliability in assess- Pearl, & Van Acker, 2000), and maintenance of high-in-
ment is part of the price that one pays for investigating fluence status in adolescent peer groups (Prinstein &
an important aspect of human behavior. Cillessen, 2003) suggest that the complexity of this be-
havior pattern may be tied to its robustness to evolution-
DIMENSIONS OF AGGRESSION AND ary threats across the ages (Hawley, 2003).
OTHER ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR The perspective of this chapter is that human aggres-
sive behavior, because of its many adaptive features, has
Because antisocial or disruptive behavior is a heteroge- evolved to be part of a broader social communication
neous set, numerous attempts have been made to estab- system (Tedeschi & Felson, 1994). Aggression must be
lish dimensions of antisocial behavior by factor-analytic interpreted as a social event. It has meaningful subtypes
or multidimensional scaling techniques. Frick et al. and multiple topographies, antecedents, and functions.
(1993) conducted a meta-analysis of factor-analytic To understand processes in specific aggressive events
studies of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct may require subclassification and behavioral analysis.
disorder behaviors in over 23,000 youth and extracted To understand the broader adaptive (and maladaptive)
two dimensions of antisocial behavior. One dimension functions of aggression may require integration with
runs from overt to covert behaviors, a distinction that other antisocial patterns.
Loeber and Schmaling (1985) found to be quite robust,
and the second dimension ranges in level of destructive-
ness. The resulting quadrants constitute categories of HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES ON THEORY
aggression (overt, high destructive), oppositional be- AND EMPIRICAL INQUIRY
havior (overt, low destructive), property violations
(covert, high destructive), and status violations (covert, Through the 1980s, scholarly inquiry in aggressive be-
low destructive). havior was dominated by broad debates among theo-
The category of aggressive behaviors can be further rists. These debates addressed the very nature of the
subcategorized into dimensions that reflect both the human species. The frustration-aggression hypothesis
Cultural Dif ferences and Aggression 723

of Dollard et al. (1939) posited a drive theory that ag- opment of early starting aggressive behavior that has re-
gressive behavior is an inevitable, species-wide re- ceived growing consensus (Conduct Problems Preven-
sponse to perceived goal-blocking. They suggested that tion Research Group, 1992; Moffitt, 1990; Patterson,
frustration necessarily results in aggression and that all Reid, & Dishion, 1992). Much less studied and more
aggressive behavior is instigated by frustration. This hy- poorly understood are later-onset patterns of aggressive
pothesis led to the first major empirical studies of ag- behavior in adolescence and factors in desistance. The
gression, and the findings disputed that premise. early starter theory incorporates an understanding of
Berkowitz (1962) noted that frustration does not always how biological factors, sociocultural contexts, and early
lead to aggression but maintained the hypothesis that life experiences with parents, peers, and schooling con-
frustration creates a drivelike readiness to aggress, tribute to the development of enduring patterns of ag-
namely the arousal of anger. gressive behavior. Rather than understand these factors
The ethological approach of Lorenz (1966) similarly as orthogonal influences, the emergent theory highlights
suggested the inevitability of aggressive behavior but the power of the interaction effect between genes and
emphasized the instinctual system that relies on internal the environment (Caspi et al., 2002) and describes the
energy that is generated even in the absence of external social-cognitive processes by which children actively
stimuli and must be released periodically. Even though relate their experiences to future behavior. Second,
empirical studies fail to support this hypothesis, Lorenz methods have been developed to test these theories by
contributed the general perspective that the adaptive identifying trajectories of growth and change over time
species-preserving functions of intraspecific aggression (Nagin, 1999) and to parse influences that are nested
are to balance the distribution of the species across a across levels of the individual child who lives in a family
limited ecology, to allow for the natural selection of the nested with neighborhoods and schools (Bryk & Rau-
fittest of the species through combat, and to promote the denbush, 1992).
selection of the most able family defenders against ex-
traspecific threat. Thus, aggressive behavior has evolved
as a necessary, species-preserving, component of human CULTURAL DIFFERENCES
adaptation. AND AGGRESSION
Bandura (1973) hypothesized that aggressive behav-
ior develops through social learning processes, including How various cultures define aggression is beyond the
imitation of aggressive models, direct operant reinforce- scope of this chapter, but the impact of culture on the
ment for aggressive acts, and vicarious reinforcement development of aggression and whether developmental
through observational learning. Most empirical research models of aggression hold across cultures are impor-
on human aggression during the 1970s and 1980s was tant questions addressed here. Emerging literature sug-
inspired by this perspective. Bandura acknowledged the gests that many of the developmental processes first
biological constraints on human learning but argued that studied in the United States operate similarly across
these constraints are much less restrictive than in other cultures, including China (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, &
species. Biological and genetic theories, other than McBride-Chang, 2003; Chen, Wang, Chen, & Liu,
those relating to instinct, have been posed primarily to 2002) and Indonesia (Eisenberg, Pidada, & Liew,
account for age and individual differences in aggression 2001), but future work may clarify the circumstances
and have proliferated in the past decade. in which patterns diverge.
Two other developments have reshaped the nature of
theory and empirical enquiry over the past 2 decades.
First, the emerging field of developmental psychopathol- AGGRESSIVE AND ANTISOCIAL
ogy (D. Shaw, 2003) has come to dominate the study of DEVELOPMENT IN THE HUMAN SPECIES
aggressive development. Microbehavioral analyses of
aggressive events have been supplemented by epidemio- Charting species-wide patterns of growth and change in
logical studies of individual differences in the life aggressive behavior across the life span is complicated
course of aggressive behavior. Models of reciprocal in- by the qualitatively different measures of aggression
fluence, transactional development, and biological-psy- across contexts. Even though infancy and toddlerhood
chosocial interaction have been formulated (Dodge & represent the periods of highest frequency in aggression,
Pettit, 2003). What has emerged is a theory of the devel- the most dangerously aggressive periods are clearly late
724 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

adolescence and early adulthood. Different measures of all other negative expressions. In contrast, 5 of the 16
aggressive behavior provide discrepant pictures of ag- infants at 4 months and 6 of the 16 infants at 7 months
gressive development. Some of these discrepancies displayed the discrete anger template. Thus, even
imply that the construct of antisocial behavior itself though the capacity for negative emotional expression is
changes across development. Grabbing objects, tattling present by 1 month of age, only over the course of the
on others, and homicide are all valid measures, but de- first 4 months of life does a distinct anger response be-
velopmental norms and base rates alter their validity at come coordinated.
different ages. Tremblay et al. (2005) and Farrington At what age does anger expression come to have a so-
(1993) suggest that different measures at different ages cial communication function? Stenberg and Campos
(e.g., fighting at age 8, vandalism at age 12, and homi- (1990) found that, following restraint, 1-month-olds
cide at age 18) may be indicators of the same underlying turned their heads randomly, but 4-month-olds turned
antisocial construct. Cairns (1979) described the con- their heads toward the frustrator or the frustrators
cept of continuity across development as including in- hands. Immediately following the onset of the first dis-
traindividual continuity (absolute stability), play of anger, 7-month-olds, but not 4-month-olds,
interindividual continuity (relative stability in rank), or- turned their heads not toward the frustrator but toward
ganizational continuity (the fact that the organism is their mothers. Stenberg and Campos (1990) concluded,
fundamentally the same from birth to death), factor By at least 4 months anger facial displays may function
structure continuity (whether covariance matrices in as discrete social signals. These signals are at first di-
variables are identical across ages), process continuity rected proximally to the immediate source of frustra-
(whether rates of change or factors in change vary across tion, but by 7 months they become expressed directly to
age), and societal or generational continuity (whether social objects such as the mother (pp. 270271). The
the construct is conceptualized similarly across time by failure to observe a discrete anger expression in 1-
societies). Aggressive behavior is such a complex devel- month-olds does not rule out its existence at this age,
opmental construct partly because even though its in- because other stimuli might have elicited the anger re-
traindividual, factor-structure, and process continuities sponse or other observational codes might be necessary
are weak, its interindividual continuity is strong. to detect a different form of anger at an early age. Like-
wise, if intent-to-harm is a necessary component of ag-
gression as it is defined here, labeling these behaviors
The Emergence of Anger and Physical
as aggressive requires the scientist to make a high level
Aggression in Infancy
of inference.
The fundamental human emotion of anger is crucial to
Aggression and Conflict in the Second Year of Life
survival because of its self-regulatory and social com-
munication functions (Lemerise & Dodge, 2000). It pre- Trivers (1974) suggested that conflict, anger, and ag-
pares the body physiologically and psychologically to gression increase in frequency and intensity across the
initiate self-protective and instrumental activity (Fri- 2nd year of life in all mammalian species that undergo a
jda, 1986) and may be an important reason for the adap- prolonged period of symbiosis between mother and in-
tation and survival of the species (Lorenz, 1966). When fant. Following a period of total dependence by the in-
not controlled properly, anger is a source of much fant, the mother is motivated to help the infant achieve
human misery. independence for survival, but the infant is ambivalent
If anger is functional and innate, when does it and may be motivated to sustain the mothers attention.
emerge, and what are its earliest elicitors? Stenberg and The infants growing size and weight or the birth of a
Campos (1990) used a forearm restraint procedure to younger sibling may accelerate the mothers interest in
elicit responses in 1-, 4-, and 7-month-old infants. They pushing the infant /toddler toward independence and the
grasped the infants forearms, pulled them together, and toddlers interest in keeping the mothers attention
held them approximately 6 inches in front of the infants (Dunn, 1988). Mother-infant conflict is thus inevitable
torso for up to 3 minutes. Even though 1-month-olds did in the 2nd year of life as the individuation process inten-
display undifferentiated negative facial expressions, not sifies (Mahler, 1968).
one of the 16 1-month-olds displayed a discrete anger Although individual differences in intensity and ease
template that differentiated the anger expression from of recovery from attack can be measured from responses
Aggressive and Antisocial Development in the Human Species 725

to blood drawn as early as 2 days of life (Keenan, Gun- years. Caregivers demands for compliance increase
thorpe, & Young, 2002), the stability of individual dif- during these years, and the probability of noncompliant
ferences over the first several months of life is modest responses increases. Klimes-Dougan and Kopp (1999)
(Gunnar, Brodersen, & Krueger, 1996). Stable individ- found that, in response to a clean-up task, rates of non-
ual differences in anger expression emerge later during compliance were 68% among 18-month-olds but as high
the 1st year and into the 2nd year of life (Stifter, Spin- as 97% among 30-month-olds. Abramovitch, Corter, and
rad, & Braungart-Rieker, 1999). The earliest docu- Lando (1979) found that siblings become a predominant
mented observations of peer-directed aggression have source of agonistic behavior for preschoolers, with 45%
been found at the end of the 1st year of life. This period of all interactions between younger and older siblings in-
coincides with emerging interest in ones own posses- volving conflict.
sions, in control over ones own activities, and in peer Although relatively few sex differences have been
communication. Six-month-olds appear not to be both- found in infancy and toddlerhood in the rate and form of
ered by peers who grab their objects or invade their aggressive behaviors, by the time that children interact
space (Hay, Nash, & Pedersen, 1983). By 12 months of in naturally occurring preschool groups, the differences
age, however, infants respond to peer provocations with become striking (Underwood, 2003), especially in phys-
protest and aggressive retaliation (Caplan, Vespo, Peder- ical aggression.
sen, & Hay, 1991). Up to half of all peer exchanges
among children 12 to 18 months old involve conflict Aggression during the Elementary School Years
(Holmberg, 1977). Hay and Ross (1982) found that 87% It is not surprising that the start of elementary school is
of 21-month-old children participated in at least one often experienced with a sense of relief by parents, both
conflict during four 15-minute laboratory peer group because behavior management is now shared with a
observation sessions. teacher and because the overall level of aggressive be-
Aggression during the Preschool Years haviors decline. Analyses of mothers Child Behavior
Checklist Reports of the 1,195 children studied by the
Although physical aggression decreases, verbal aggres- National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
sion increases normatively between 2 and 4 years of age ment Early Child Care Research Network (2004) indi-
(Cairns, 1979), coinciding with growth in expressive vo- cate The most frequent form of early aggression, hits
cabulary. A twin study revealed that the negative corre- others, occurred in about 70% of the sample at ages 2 and
lation between physical aggression and expressive 3, but declined to 20% by ages 4 and 5 ( kindergarten),
vocabulary in toddlers cannot be dismissed as due to a and to 12% by third grade (p. 42). Keenan and Shaw
common genetic origin; rather, the most likely causal (2003) have suggested that growth in self-control over
path goes from the development of expressive vocabu- emotion, known as the development of emotion regula-
lary to decreases in physical aggression (Dionne, Boivin, tion, is responsible for the decline in aggression during
Tremblay, Laplante, & Perusse, 2003). But language these years. Lingering problems with aggression during
onset also provides children with a new means of ag- this period have been called adaptive disability (AD) and
gressing (e.g., through insults, threats, and name-call- deficient self-regulation by Barkley et al. (2002).
ing), and the general parental perspective is that Numerous factors may account for the joint growth in
problem behavior by toddlers increases during this era. emotion regulation and decline in aggression across the
Jenkins, Bax, and Hart (1980) found that parental con- period from 4 to 8 years of age, all of which are sub-
cerns about behavior problems and management peak at sumed in the development of executive functions. Rapid
age 3. Other epidemiological studies have revealed high neural development in the anterior cingulate gyrus dur-
rates (up to 13%) of tantrum, peer fighting, and frustra- ing these years has been hypothesized by Posner and
tion tolerance problems in 3-year-olds, with declines Rothbart (1998) to be responsible for the development of
thereafter (Crowther, Bond, & Rolf, 1981). effortful control and, indirectly, decreases in aggressive
Whereas the most frequent elicitors of aggression in behavior. Mischel (1974) suggested that the emerging
infancy are physical discomfort and the need for atten- ability to delay gratification is a crucial factor in de-
tion, elicitors become habit training in the 3rd year clines in aggression during this era. Through interper-
and peer conflicts and conflicts over material posses- sonal exchanges, children acquire cognitive strategies
sions (Fabes & Eisenberg, 1992) in the 4th and 5th for delaying gratification (e.g., distraction, mentally
726 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

representing delayed rewards) and effortful control hostile intent has an inflammatory effect (Hubbard,
(Eisenberg et al., 2004) that may help them avoid impul- Dodge, Cillessen, Coie, & Schwartz, 2001).
sive grabbing of others possessions and hitting. The An important distinction made during this age period
ability to delay gratification, in turn, may be aided by is between reactive and proactive aggression. Animal
the corresponding development of broader representa- behaviorists have long distinguished between hostile-af-
tional abilities (Gelman & Baillargeon, 1983), perspec- fective (reactive) aggression, characterized by intensive
tive taking (Selman, 1980), empathy (Zahn-Waxler, patterned autonomic arousal, anger, and defensive pos-
Radke-Yarrow, & King, 1979), emotion processing tures that lead to frenzied attacks in response to per-
(Schultz, Izard, & Bear, 2004), and memory strategies ceived threat, and instrumental (proactive) aggression,
(Brown, Bransford, Ferrara, & Campione, 1983). Repli- characterized by little autonomic activation but highly
cating the work of Eisenberg et al. (1999), Gilliom, patterned appetitive behavior oriented toward a reward
Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, and Lukon (2002) found that (Lorenz, 1966). These behaviors have been distin-
the ability to deploy attention-shifting strategies such as guished reliably in direct observations of children aged
seeking information about situational constraints and 5 to 9 (Coie et al., 1999), teacher ratings, and ratings of
ignoring frustrating stimuli is associated with anger violent behaviors in clinical records (Dodge et al.,
control at age 42 months and is predictive of teacher-re- 1997). Vitaro, Brendgen, and Tremblay (2002) found
ported externalizing problems at age 6. Eisenberg and distinct correlates of these two types of aggression in
Fabes (1999) have articulated a broader theory of emer- inattentiveness and anxiety (reactives higher) and in
gent emotional self-regulation during this period, with overt delinquency (proactives higher). Smithmyer, Hub-
children progressing from externally controlled regula- bard, and Simons (2000) found that proactive aggression
tion to internally mediated cognitive controls that lead is a function of outcome expectancies, whereas reactive
to reduced aggressive behavior. Quite a different expla- aggression grows out of maltreatment and emotion dys-
nation for declines in aggression during this period is regulation (Shields & Cicchetti, 1998).
that peers begin to provide feedback to aggressors that Although most children aggress less frequently dur-
extinguishes aggression. ing the elementary school years, a select few become
With the gradual decline in the rate of aggression highly troublesome to peers, parents, and teachers (Loe-
comes a shift in its form and function. An increasing ber et al., 1998). It is during the elementary school years
proportion of aggressive behaviors becomes directed to- that many children, boys especially, are referred to men-
ward specific dyadic relationships (Coie et al., 1999), tal health clinics for conduct problems. Lahey and Loe-
and its form becomes increasingly hostile, in contrast ber (1994) have outlined a developmental path for
with the relatively nonsocial, instrumental nature of ag- aggressive conduct problems, beginning with opposi-
gression in the preschool period. Aggressive behaviors tional defiant disorder (ODD), which is characterized by
also become more person-directed and relational (Crick temper tantrums and defiant, irritable, blameful, argu-
& Bigbee, 1998). Finally, covert forms of antisocial be- mentative, and annoying behavior. These behaviors are
havior such as lying, cheating, and stealing emerge with not uncommon at 4 to 5 years of age (Achenbach & Edel-
greater frequency (Loeber, Farrington, Stouthamer- brock, 1983) but become less common and clinically
Loeber, & van Kammen, 1998). problematic by age 8 (Loeber, Lahey, & Thomas, 1991).
Major elicitors of aggression come to include per- Clinically referred elementary school children typically
ceived threats and insults to ones ego (Schwartz, Mc- do not present with these problems as new symptoms;
Fadden-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1998). rather, these children have been unable to outgrow prob-
Although the emerging recognition that provocations lems that have carried over from earlier times (Loeber,
may be accidental contributes to declines in aggression, Tremblay, Gagnon, & Charlebois, 1989). Some ODD
the emergent recognition that another may be acting children begin to diversify their deviant repertoire in the
with intentional and hostile motives instigates increased elementary school years (usually about age 8 to 11) to
retaliatory, angry responding (Gifford-Smith & Ra- include setting fires, lying, fighting, weapon use, and
biner, 2004). During the early elementary school years, vandalism (Loeber et al., 1998). This pattern is called
children learn that some actions are unintended but oth- conduct disorder (CD), and its prevalence in the United
ers are under the volitional control of the actor; the re- States is about 9% of males and 2% of females (Ameri-
sult is that the attribution that a peer has acted with can Psychological Association, 1994). Canadian surveys
Aggressive and Antisocial Development in the Human Species 727

of 3,300 children indicate a prevalence of 7% of males Official arrest records reported by the Federal Bureau
and 3% of females (Offord, Boyle, & Racine, 1989). of Investigation (FBI; U.S. Department of Justice, 2003)
Between age 11 and 13, a subset of children who indicate 3 to 4 times lower prevalence rates of SVOs and
could be diagnosed as CD begin to diversify their de- a similar but lagged (delayed) curvilinear developmental
viant behaviors even further to include violent criminal function. At age 17, about 15% of all boys in the United
behavior such as mugging, breaking and entering, and States are arrested (Blumstein, 2000). Of those adoles-
forced sex. The group of children whose early aggres- cents who commit offenses, only 15% to 33% are in fact
sive behavior does not dissipate over time has been arrested (Farrington, 1989). Of those arrested, about
called early starters (Moffitt, 1993). Broidy et al. 64% are referred to juvenile or adult court, and of those
(2003) used Nagin and Tremblays (1999) semiparamet- referred to court, only about 2% to 10% are incarcer-
ric methods to identify trajectories of aggressive devel- ated. Thus, very few offenders are ever incarcerated. Be-
opment with six independent longitudinal samples from cause those arrested probably are a very biased group of
across the world. In all samples, a group of early start- all offenders, due to gender, socioeconomic, and racial
ing aggressive boys was found to persist in physically ag- discrimination, arrest records and self-reports provide
gressive behavior across the ages 7 to 13. different pictures of violence (Huizinga & Dunford,
1985). Based solely on arrest records, Weiner (1989)
concluded that most first-time SVOs occur between ages
Aggression during the Adolescent Years
18 and 24. However, as noted by D. Elliott (1994), If we
Loeber et al. (1998) concluded that most longitudinal assume the accuracy of both (self-report and official
studies show decrements in ratings of aggressive behav- record) measures, it appears that most first arrests for a
ior as children enter adolescence. However, adolescence violent offense in a serious violent career take place sev-
is a time when serious acts of violence increase, as age- eral years after the initiation into this type of behavior
crime curves regularly demonstrate (e.g., U.S. Depart- and extend into ages where the hazard rate for initiation
ment of Justice, 2003), when a second group of youth is close to 0 (p. 10).
joins the early starting group in antisocial behavior, and Ethnic differences in aggression in the social context
when aggressive behavior broadens to new contexts, in- of the United States are almost negligible in the elemen-
cluding romantic relationships. tary school years (Achenbach, 1991) but are more pro-
nounced in adolescence. Arrest record data indicate that
Growth of Serious Violence even though African American youth make up 15% of
Data from the National Youth Survey (NYS) of 1,725 the juvenile population, they account for 52% of those
youths first surveyed in 1976 (D. Elliott & Huizinga, arrested for juvenile violent crimes (Dryfoos, 1990).
1983) indicate that violent offending almost always be- The lifetime chances that an urban African American
gins in the adolescent years. Self-reports of serious vio- male will be arrested for an FBI index offense (mur-
lent offenses (SVOs, defined as aggravated assault, der, forcible rape, aggravated assault, robbery, burglary,
robbery, or rape, necessarily involving some injury or a larceny, and auto theft) is 50%, in contrast with 14% for
weapon) rise sharply from age 12 to 20. The onset haz- urban White males (Blumstein & Cohen, 1987). The
ard rate (first-time offending) for SVOs is almost zero problem is so great that more African American male
(< 0.5%) through age 11 but doubles between ages 13 adolescents are incarcerated or on probation for a crime
and 14 and rises sharply to 5.1% at age 16. The onset than are gainfully employed (Edelman, 1992). Self-
rate then halves between ages 16 and 18 and declines to reports indicate a much narrower race difference in ac-
less than 1% after age 20. Thus, over half of the persons tual behavior, however. The Black-to-White ratio in
who become involved in serious violent offending, prior prevalence of SVOs is about 5 to 4, a statistically signif-
to age 27, commit their first violent offense between the icant but substantially small effect (D. Elliott, 1994).
ages of 14 and 17, and almost all offenders commit their When socioeconomic class confounds are controlled,
first offense before age 21. Not only is the rise in first- this ratio is reduced to about 7 to 6. D. Elliott (1994) has
time offending dramatic, but the overall prevalence reported that the higher prevalence rate of self-reported
rates for offending in adolescence are also startlingly SVOs among African Americans can be almost entirely
high. At the peak age of 17, 19% of males and 12% of accounted for by the particularly high risk of onset be-
females reported committing at least one SVO. tween the ages of 13 and 16 for African American male
728 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

adolescents. This age period obviously is a risky one for other situations, quite a few display violence only in this
this group in terms of self-reported SVOs, but an even context (Holtzworth-Munroe & Stuart, 1994). Indeed,
more tragic time in terms of arrest rates and rates of existing theory and research concerning intimate part-
victimization. ner violence generally is based on the premise that inti-
These data suggest common developmental patterns mate partner violence is distinct from other forms of
in serious violence that begin with minor aggressive acts violence (Moffitt, Krueger, Caspi, & Fagan, 2000).
and delinquent behaviors in middle childhood and prog-
ress to frequent and serious offending by age 17. The Aggression during Adulthood
initiation of substance use and sexual activity adds in- Most self-report studies indicate that between ages 18
crementally to the risk for increased aggressive behavior and 25 the overall rate of aggressive behavior declines,
during this period (Jessor, Donovan, & Costa, 1991). D. and virtually no new cases of antisocial behavior begin
Elliott (1994) concluded that minor forms of delinquent in adulthood (Robins, 1966). Sampson and Laub (2003)
behavior and alcohol use typically precede more serious examined trajectories of adult development in the
forms of violence. Thus, the developmental path for a Glueck sample of delinquents and found that further de-
small portion of the population involves progressively clines in crime are found after age 35 in all groups of
serious and violent behaviors, although most offenders early offenders. In the Cambridge study, Farrington
usually desist from crime in early adulthood. (1993) found that self-reported prevalence of all crimi-
nal behaviors, including violence, decreased markedly
Adolescent Culture
in early adulthood, for example, self-reports of burglary
In American peer culture, physical aggression and delin- decreased from 11% at age 18 to 5% at age 21 and 2% at
quent deviance become more socially acceptable during both ages 25 and 32. In the NYS, D. Elliott, Huizinga,
adolescence (Coie, Terry, Zakriski, & Lochman, 1995). and Menard (1989) found that self-reported offending
Moffitt (1993) hypothesized that early starting children peaked at age 17 and declined linearly in subsequent
contribute to the growth of a deviant peer culture by act- years. Official arrest record data indicate a similar de-
ing as role models and by offering opportunities for de- cline in violent offending in adulthood, although the de-
viant behavior. Indeed, some deviant youth begin to hold cline as measured by arrests begins later and is less
positive status among peers (Miller-Johnson & sharp than that by self-reports (Blumstein, 2000).
Costanzo, 2004). The positive correlation between phys- An important caveat to these findings is that almost
ical aggression and being disliked by peers dissolves all of these studies fail to include child abuse and
during middle school (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004), and spousal battery as instances of violence. Thus, it might
the base rates of self-reported antisocial behavior rise. be misleading to conclude that adulthood brings about
In turn, as the contextual normativeness of antisocial be- less violence. Straus and Gelles (1990) reported that
havior increases, the effects of this context are to in- 16% of American couples report physically assaulting
crease the display of aggressive behavior by individual each other, and 11% report physically abusing their chil-
youth (Espelage, Holt, & Henkel, 2003). The new group dren, in the past 12 months; however, systematic epi-
of aggressors has been called adolescence-limited by demiological research that would adequately describe
Moffitt (1993), who asserted that this group engages in the life course and origins of child and spouse abuse
delinquent behavior only during adolescence. does not yet exist. Huesmann, Eron, Lefkowitz, and
Walder (1984) have reported some continuity between
Violence in Romantic Relationships
peer-directed aggression at age 8 and spousal and child
Studying a representative birth cohort in New Zealand, abuse at age 30, suggesting that this behavior may have
Magdol, Moffitt, Caspi, and Silva (1998) found that similar antecedents to other forms of violence.
32% of late-adolescent males and females reported per- There is a significant exception to the general pattern
petrating physical aggression in an intimate relationship of decline in serious violence in early adulthood, for
during the past year. This domain is one of the few in both early-starting and late-starting youth (Coie, 2004).
which violence by females equals that by males, al- Among African American males, there is no decline in
though male violence is more likely to result in partner violence from age 22 to 30. Nearly twice as many
injury (Archer, 2000). Although many individuals who African Americans continue their violent careers as do
are violent toward intimate partners are also violent in Whites; thus, the violent careers of African Americans
Gender and Aggression 729

last longer than they do for Whites (D. Elliott, 1994), in Pulkkinen (1990) described as a relapsed lifestyle, and
sharp contrast with the finding of few race differences less stable marriages that more frequently end in divorce
in the propensity for initial violence. It seems that the (Caspi, Elder, & Bem, 1987). Longitudinal studies also
underclass, especially poor African American males, are reflect substantial discontinuity. Not all early-starting
unable to escape the system of incarceration, labeling, antisocial youth become adult offenders or follow the
unemployment, and negative identity once the course of pathway of poor occupational or marital adjustment.
violence begins. As D. Elliott (1994), concluded: The idea that a stable marriage and regular employ-
ment provide support for desistance from crime is con-
Once involved in a lifestyle that includes serious forms of sistent with social control theories of crime. Sampson
violence, theft, and substance use, persons from disadvan-
and Laubs (2003) analyses of the Glueck and Glueck
taged families and neighborhoods find it very difficult to
(1968) longitudinal study indicated that strong ties to
escape. They have fewer opportunities for conventional
adult institutions, such as work and family, protect one
adult roles, and they are more deeply embedded in and de-
pendent upon the gangs and the illicit economy that flour- from subsequent criminal behavior among those with a
ish in their neighborhoods. . . . Poverty is related less to previous history of delinquency. Job stability had a con-
the onset of violence than to the continuity of violence, sistently inverse influence on crime and deviance in
once initiated. (p. 19) young adulthood (age 17 to 25) and in later adulthood
(age 25 to 32), whereas income did not have effects
It is important to note that there is no race disparity when other factors were controlled. Following up that
in continuing violence among persons who are employed part of the sample that had been married at some time,
at ages 18 to 20 or married or living with a partner. The Sampson and Laub found that marital cohesiveness had
evidence suggests that those persons who are able to es- a significant effect on crime and deviance, independent
tablish stable work and family life and careers, regard- of other factors. Sampson and Laub (1990) concluded
less of ethnicity, tend to give up their involvement in that job stability is central in explaining adult desis-
criminal violence (Rutter, 1989). As D. Elliott (1994) tance from crime; however, this effect is reduced among
pointed out, these findings have enormous implications those who were never married, for whom attachment to
for the focus of intervention, which might be directed wife assumes greater relative importance (p. 621).
toward job training and economic opportunity.

Continuity of Individual Differences in Adulthood GENDER AND AGGRESSION

The early starter model of antisocial individuals posits


Long-standing interest in the issue of gender differences
that early starters are more likely to continue breaking
in antisocial behavior has typically taken two forms.
the law in their adult years than are the late starters.
One is interest in the size of the difference between men
The distinction between early and late starters, how-
and women as a function of developmental phase and the
ever, cannot be sharply drawn because curves for the
type of antisocial behavior examined. The other focus is
onset of offending do not show clear demarcation and
on differences in the processes and outcomes of anti-
some so-called late starters do continue offending in the
social behavior for males and females. The evidence is
adult years. Continuity of offending into adult years has
stronger in the first case than in the second.
been shown by Farrington (1995). Nearly 75% of those
convicted of juvenile offenses (at age 10 to 16) were re-
Physical Aggression
convicted between age 17 and 24, and half of the juve-
nile offenders were reconvicted between age 25 and 32. Gender differences begin to emerge as early as 3 years
Thus, although the peak of offending in this study was of age, particularly for more serious and stable aggres-
age 17, a substantial number of juvenile offenders con- sion. Crick, Casas, and Mosher (1997) found that teach-
tinue in criminal careers well into adulthood. ers rated preschool boys aged 3 to 5 as more aggressive
The continuity of antisocial activity in adulthood than girls. Kingston and Prior (1995) used annual ma-
takes on other forms besides criminal offending. This ternal assessments taken between ages 3 and 8 in the
heterotypic continuity includes spouse abuse, drunk 1,721 participants in the Australian Temperament Proj-
driving, moving traffic violations, and severe punish- ect to find that 41 out of the 53 members of a stable ag-
ment of children (Huesmann, Eron, et al., 1984), what gressive group were boys.
730 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

These differences in physical aggression remain sta- rate for CD of 5.9% for boys compared to 2.8% for girls.
ble through childhood and adolescence. Broidy et al. This sex ratio is consistent with the ratios obtained from
(2003) examined developmental trajectories of physical other epidemiological studies. For example, Costello
aggression in six large, well-known, longitudinal sam- et al. (1996) reported a sex ratio of 4.81 in the Great
ples from three different countries: the Pittsburgh Youth Smoky Mountains study. The sex ratio in the New York
Study and Child Development Project from the United State study was 2.31 (P. Cohen et al., 1993) and in the
States, the Dunedin and Christchurch studies from New Ontario study it was 2.41 (Offord et al., 1989).
Zealand, and the Montreal and Quebec samples from The gender differences are especially pronounced
Canada. These authors found that girls exhibit lower for more severe conduct problems that begin early. Mof-
mean levels of physical aggression than do boys across fit and Caspi (2001), using data from the Dunedin
all four sites with comparable data for boys and girls. Study, identified two groups of antisocial individuals
Even among girls who exhibit chronic physical aggres- using data from age 5 through age 18: life-course per-
sion across assessment periods, their mean levels of sistent (LCP) offenders and adolescent limited (AL) of-
physical aggression are notably lower than those of fenders. Twenty-six percent of males were placed into
chronic physically aggressive boys in the same sample the AL group compared to 18% of females. In contrast,
(p. 232). Stanger, Achenbach, and Verhulst (1997) found 10% of males were placed in the LCP group compared
that boys were more aggressive than girls at every age to only 1% of females. Thus, the male-to-female ratio
from 4 to 18, using the aggression subscale of the Child for the LCP path was 101, whereas the sex ratio for the
Behavior Checklist (CBC) in a sample of over 2,000 AL path was 1.51.
Dutch children.
Female Aggression
Antisocial Behavior One type of antisocial behavior for which there is either
very little difference between boys and girls (Under-
Similar sex differences have been observed for anti-
wood, 2003) or for which girls actually score higher
social behavior more broadly defined. Stanger et al.
than boys (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003) has been stud-
(1997) reported that scores on the delinquency subscale
ied under several terms, each of which is meant to refer
of the CBC were higher for boys than girls at every age
to a type of aggression that is more subtle than physical
from 4 to 18. Gender differences on the externalizing
aggression and which may be used preferentially by
subscale, a combination of aggression and delinquency,
girls. Indirect aggression has been defined as a noxious
have been found across 12 different cultures (Crijnen,
behavior in which the target person is attacked not phys-
Achenbach, & Verhulst, 1997). In their intensive exami-
ically or directly through verbal intimidation but in a
nation of gender differences in antisocial behavior using
circuitous way, through social manipulation (Kauki-
data from the Dunedin Study, Moffitt, Caspi, Rutter,
ainen et al., 1999). Galen and Underwood (1997) de-
and Silva (2001) reported that males score higher on
fined social aggression as aggression directed toward
antisocial behavior from age 5 to age 21 on parent,
damaging and anothers self-esteem, social status, or
teacher, informant, and self-reports. The difference be-
both (p. 589). Relational aggression has been defined
tween boys and girls, averaged across method, ranged
as harming others through purposeful manipulation
from .15 standard deviations at age 15 to almost a half
and damage of their peer relationships (Crick & Grot-
standard deviation at age 21. Finally, Fergusson and
peter, 1995, p. 711). Although there may be subtle dif-
Horwood (2002) examined the offending trajectories
ferences between these conceptions (see Underwood,
from ages 8 until 20 for boys and girls enrolled in the
2003), they are highly overlapping constructs and we
Christchurch study and found that the female offending
treat them as assessing a single type of aggression that
rate was about half the male offending rate at all ages.
we label, following Underwood, social aggression.
Social aggression can be reliably identified using
Conduct Disorder multiple methods in children as young as 3 years old
(Crick et al., 1997). An explosion in interest on the topic
Not surprisingly, similar gender differences are found has resulted in dozens of studies that examine the con-
when CD is examined. In the Virginia Twin Study of struct from early childhood until late adolescence. In
Adolescent Development, Simonoff et al. (1997) found a terms of gender differences, there is agreement that this
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 731

construct references a form of aggression that is more ferences (Pakaslahti & Keltigangas-Jarvinen, 2000).
normative in girls than is physical aggression. There is Results are also mixed for studies of social aggression
disagreement, however, as to whether girls are more so- (Paquette & Underwood, 1999).
cially aggressive than boys.
Results are mixed in preschool studies. McNeilly- Gender Differences in Process
Choque, Hart, Robinson, Nelson, and Olsen (1996)
found that 4- and 5-year-old girls, relative to boys, dis- It is clear that for most forms of antisocial behavior boys
played more relationally aggressive behaviors on the are more antisocial than girls. What is not clear, how-
playground and were rated as more relationally aggres- ever, is whether the processes that lead to antisocial be-
sive by classmates and teachers. Crick et al. (1997) havior are the same or different for girls and boys.
found that girls were more relationally aggressive than Moffitt et al. (2001) examined correlations separately
boys according to teacher ratings but not according to for males and females between antisocial behavior and
peer nominations. A study by Hart, Nelson, Robinson, five classes of important risk factors: maternal factors,
Olsen, and McNeilly-Choque (1998) of Russian nursery family factors, cognitive and neurological factors, child
school children failed to find gender differences for re- behavior factors, and peer factors. Of the 35 correla-
lational aggression as did a study of Head Start and tions examined, 14 differed significantly for males and
community preschool participants (Kupersmidt, Bryant, females. These differences, however, were differences
& Willoughby, 2000). in magnitude rather than direction: All correlations
The issue of gender differences is no clearer in mid- were in the same direction and, in almost every case, the
dle childhood. Archer, Pearson, and Westeman (1998), correlation was significant for both males and females.
using observational data from a sample of 7- to 11-year-
olds in Great Britain, found that girls engaged in more
verbal aggression than did boys. However, a cross-cul- DETERMINANTS OF INDIVIDUAL
tural study using peer ratings found that boys were rated DIFFERENCES IN ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR
as more verbally aggressive than girls (Osterman et al.,
1994). Tiet, Wasserman, Loeber, McReynolds, and Causes of individual differences in antisocial behavior
Miller (2001) used parent reports of relational aggres- range from genetic to socialization, and contemporary
sion for boys and girls aged 4 to 18 and found no gender models integrate these factors through interactions,
differences. However, Crick (1996) used teacher reports transactions, moderation, and mediation.
to find that girls were more relationally aggressive than
boys. Studies using peer nominations are also discor-
Genetics
dant. Crick and colleagues often report that girls receive
more nominations for relational aggression than do boys It is homiletic to say that antisocial behavior is the result
(e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Other studies, using the of both nature and nurture. To move beyond homily, we
same methodology, have either failed to find gender dif- must disentangle these effects and examine their inter-
ferences in relational aggression (e.g., Rys & Bear, play. This is the realm of behavior genetics, which relies
1997) or have found that boys received more nomina- on genetically sensitive designs (e.g., twin and adoption
tions for relational aggression than girls (e.g., David & studies) to accomplish these goals. Genetically sensitive
Kistner, 2000). designs utilize the differing degrees of genetic similar-
In adolescence, the picture is the same. Several studies ity in relatives (e.g., monozygotic and dizygotic twins,
report that girls are more socially aggressive than boys, parents and children, stepsiblings) to determine how
whereas others find no differences or that boy are more much of the variance in a trait is due to variation in ge-
socially aggressive than girls. For example, for verbal ag- netic similarity and environmental similarity.
gression, Salmivalli, Kaukiainen, and Lagerspetz (2000) Contemporary behavior-genetics researchers provide
found that 15- and 16-year-old boys were rated by peers estimates of four types of influences. The first two, ad-
as higher in verbal aggression than girls, but Kashani and ditive and nonadditive genetic effects, constitute heri-
Shepperd (1990) failed to find gender differences in ver- tability. The third component, shared environment,
bal aggression. For indirect aggression, one study found indexes the degree to which environmental factors are
that girls were more aggressive than boys (Salmivalli responsible for the resemblance of family members. The
et al., 2000) and two other studies failed to find such dif- fourth component, nonshared environment, indexes the
732 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

degree to which environmental factors contribute to dif- that combined across raters and contexts as well as four
ferences between family members. specific factors corresponding to each informant, re-
Before examining the estimates themselves, two cau- vealed that 82% of the variance in the common latent
tions are important to note. First, it is understood that factor was influenced by genetic influences with the re-
genetic effects may be mediated environmentally maining 18% influenced by nonshared environment.
through gene-environment transactions in which genes Similarly, moderate proportions of the variance unique
influence surrounding environments, which, in turn, in- to each reporter were accounted for by genetic influ-
fluence phenotypic expression (cf., Scarr & McCartney, ences. Thus, measures that combine across social con-
1983). In behavior genetics studies, the effects of such texts yield estimates that minimize environmental
transactions are included in the heritability estimates estimates and maximize genetic estimates.
and not counted as environmental effects. Second, all es- OConnor, McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, and
timates are context specific. The influence of genes on Plomin (1998) reported results from the Nonshared En-
behavior varies across social contexts, and a change in vironment and Adolescent Development (NEAD) proj-
the social context may change the relative importance of ect. The goals of this project were to identify sources of
genes and environment (cf., Dunne et al., 1997). nonshared environment, estimate its impact on adoles-
Even with these cautions, the conclusions from this cent development, and integrate genetic and environ-
research regarding the relative influences of genes and mental models of adolescent development. The sample
environment on antisocial behavior are impossible to es- consisted of 720 same-sex adolescent siblings between
cape. Over 100 quantitative genetic studies on antisocial 10 and 18 years of age, including monozygotic and dizy-
behavior have been published from more than 60 differ- gotic twins, full siblings, half siblings, and unrelated
ent samples, with over one-third of these published in siblings. Antisocial behavior was assessed using multi-
the past 10 years. As Moffitt (2005) has written, there is ple instruments collected from multiple sources. Results
a new look about the current generation of behavior from model-fitting analyses yielded estimates of .56 for
genetics studies. The samples have grown larger, more the genetic component, .25 for shared environment, and
representative, and more global. Reports have appeared .19 for nonshared environment.
from large, representative samples in Australia, the Rhee and Waldman (2002) recently meta-analyzed
Netherlands, Scandanavia, the United Kingdom, and the the genetic influences on antisocial behavior from 42 in-
United States. Statistical techniques have grown more dependent twin samples and 10 adoption samples. These
sophisticated. Multiple sources (i.e., self-reports, re- studies varied in their operationalizations of antisocial
ports from others, and observational reports) have been behavior, sources of information, and ages of the partic-
utilized to measure antisocial behavior. Finally, re- ipants. The best-fitting model for the data included ad-
search has spanned the entire range of development from ditive genetic influences (.32), nonadditive genetic
early childhood to late adulthood. Two examples of the influences (.09), shared environmental influences (.16),
new behavior genetics approach appear below. and nonshared environmental influences (.43). Follow-
Arseneault et al. (2003) reported on the results of a ing this overall analysis, the authors examined potential
behavior-genetic analysis among 5-year-old children moderators of the estimates. They found no differences
from the Environmental Risk Longitudinal Twin Study as a function of gender. Differences, however, were
(E-risk), which drew an initial 1,210 families from a na- identified as functions of operationalization and age.
tional registry of twins born in England and Wales with For diagnosis (e.g., CD or antisocial personality disor-
an over-representation of high-risk families defined as der), aggression, and antisocial behavior (an omnibus
young age of the mother at first birth. Antisocial behav- operationalization) a model that included additive ge-
ior was assessed using mother, teacher, examiner, and netic, shared, and nonshared environmental influences
self-reports. In univariate analyses, considering one best fit the data. Heritability estimates were .44, .44,
antisocial behavior variable at a time, heritability esti- and .47 for diagnosis, aggression, and antisocial behav-
mates ranged from .42 for self-reports to .76 for teacher ior, respectively. Estimates for shared and nonshared
reports, nonshared environmental estimates ranged from environment were .11 and .45, .06 and .50, and .22 and
.24 for teacher reports to .58 for child reports, and .31 for diagnosis, aggression, and antisocial behavior, re-
shared environmental estimates were zero. Multivariate spectively. For antisocial behavior assessed via crimi-
analyses, which included a common latent ASB factor nality, however, the best-fitting model included an
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 733

additive genetic component (.33), a nonadditive genetic tal risk factors. Mednick and Christiansen (1977) pro-
component (.42), and a nonshared environment compo- vided the first evidence that genetic and environmental
nent (.25). Age also significantly moderated the magni- risk factors interact to produce offending. Among the
tude of genetic and environmental influences with the 6,000 families in the Danish Adoption Study, 14% of
magnitude of familial influences decreasing with age adoptees were convicted when neither their biological
and the magnitude of nonfamilial influences increasing. nor adoptive parents had been convicted of a crime; 15%
For children, additive genetic effects accounted for 46% were convicted if only their adoptive parent had been
of the variance, shared environment accounted for 20%, convicted; 20% were convicted if only their biological
and nonshared accounted for the remaining 34%. The parent had been; finally, 25% were convicted if both
corresponding figures for adolescents were .43, .16, and their adoptive and biological parents had been convicted.
.41; for adults, these figures were .41, .09, and .50. In The dynamic interaction effect was found to be even
general, these results suggest that there are moderate ge- stronger in several later studies. Cloninger, Sigvardsson,
netic (.41) and nonshared environmental effects (.43), Bohman, and van Knoring (1982), in a sample of 2000
and small shared environmental effects (.16) on anti- Swedish adoptees, found that 3% were convicted if both
social behavior. biological and rearing environments were normal; 7%
A recent review by Moffitt (2005) came to similar offended if only rearing were abnormal; 12% offended
conclusions as Rhee and Waldman. One moderator not in the face of offending in the biological parent; how-
examined by Rhee and Waldman (2002), but discussed ever, 40% offended if both the biological parent of-
in detail by Moffitt, is the type of offender in question. fended and the rearing environment was abnormal.
Moffitt argues that LCP antisocial behavior may be Cadoret, Cain, and Crowe (1983), in a sample of 500
more heritable than AL antisocial behavior. First, she adoptees, found that the most antisocial adoptees were
points to the results from studies of large representative those that had birth mothers with antisocial personality
samples of very young twins that yield higher heritabil- disorder or alcoholism and came from adoptive homes
ity coefficients than those reported by Rhee and Wald- marked by adverse circumstances. These findings were
man (2002), ranging from 50% for externalizing replicated in a separate sample by Cadoret, Yates,
behaviors among 2- to 3-year-old boys to 76% for Troughton, Woodworth, and Stewart (1995) who showed
teacher reports of antisocial behavior among 5-year- that adversity in adoptive homes (e.g., marital or prob-
olds. Second, Moffitt points to several studies that have lems, substance use, psychopathology) interacted with
shown higher heritability for the Aggression (around antisocial personality disorder in the biological parents
60%) than the Delinquency (around 30 to 40%) sub- to predict childhood and adolescent antisocial behavior.
scales of the CBCL (Achenbach, 1991). She argues that Similar findings have been reported in twin studies as
because the Aggression scale measures antisocial per- well. Jaffe et al. (in press), in the E-risk twin study,
sonality and physical violence and its scores are rela- showed that environmental risk interacted with genetic
tively stable across development, it is a better measure risk to predict CD; physical maltreatment was associ-
of LCP antisocial behavior than the delinquency scale, ated with a 24% increase in the probability of CD
which measures rule-breaking and shows a rise in its among twins at high genetic risk (i.e., having a co-twin
mean scores across adolescence. Third, Moffitt points to with conduct disorder), but only a 2% increase among
two studies that have contrasted preadolescent onset twins at low risk. Thus, there is a stronger environmental
antisocial behavior against antisocial behavior that be- impact among subgroups at higher genetic risk.
gins during adolescence, showing that preadolescent Caspi et al. (2002) provided evidence for the inter-
onset is substantially more heritable. Finally, she points action between a specific gene and environmental risk
to two studies demonstrating that the heritability of in predicting antisocial behavior. These authors exam-
antisocial behavior that persists from adolescence into ined the interaction between childhood maltreatment
adulthood is more heritable than antisocial behavior and a functional polymorphism in the MAO-A gene in
confined to adolescence. 442 males from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health
and Development Study. The MAO-A gene was chosen
Gene-by-Environment Interactions
because it encodes the MAO-A enzyme, which is re-
Studies of antisocial behavior were among the first to sponsible for metabolizing neurotransmitters, such as
document interactions between genetic and environmen- norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin, several of
734 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

which have been linked with antisocial behavior. Caspi contexts in which different raters have observed the in-
et al. reported a significant interaction between child- dividual, which represent real environmental effects.
hood maltreatment and MAO-A genotype, such that This conclusion is consistent with Arseneault et al.s
85% of the males with the low-activity allele and a his- (2003) finding that source-specific factors (separate
tory of childhood maltreatment developed some form parent, teacher, child, and examiner ratings) were less
of antisocial outcome. Importantly, this finding was heritable than the combined factor. Thus, these studies
recently replicated in the 514 male twins from the Vir- are most relevant to understanding antisocial personali-
ginia Twin Study for Adolescent Behavioral Develop- ties rather than antisocial behaviors.
ment (Foley et al., 2004). These authors found an There are also conclusions to be drawn about envi-
interaction between the MAO-A genotype and child- ronmental influence. The contribution of shared or com-
hood adversity (i.e., parental neglect, interparental vio- mon environment to antisocial behavior, although small,
lence, and inconsistent discipline) in predicting CD. As is larger than for other psychiatric disorders (Kendler,
in the Caspi et al. study, individuals with low MAO-A Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003). Consistent with re-
activity and an adverse childhood environment were the search in other areas, the contribution of nonshared en-
most likely to develop CD. Again, there is a stronger vironment is moderate. Taken together, these findings
environmental impact among subpopulations at higher indicate an important role for the environment. However,
genetic risk. they do suggest that it is the person-specific experi-
Thus, antisocial behavior is quite heritable, espe- ences of individuals in families that are the more impor-
cially for LCP antisocial behavior and individuals grow- tant environmental causes.
ing up in risky environments. Genes, and therefore
biological factors, play an important role in the causa-
tion of antisocial behavior. However, there are several Dispositional Factors
caveats to this conclusion. First, these results do not
imply that antisocial behavior is the result of a single Personality (i.e., characteristic ways of thinking, feeling,
gene; as noted by Carey and Goldman (1997) it defies and acting) and its earliest manifestations in tempera-
credulity to imagine that millions of years of primate ment have been studied in relation to antisocial behavior
and the hominid evolution produced a sequence of DNA for a number of years. The evidence for the influence of
whose raison detre is to forge checks or cheat on in- both personality and temperament is mounting. Large,
come taxes (p. 249). They do not imply anything about prospective studies have shown that early temperament
the cause of antisocial behavior in a given individual; is predictive of antisocial behavior in the preschool pe-
heritability is a population statistic. They do not imply riod (Keenan, Shaw, Delliquadri, Giovanelli, & Walsh,
immutability; phenylketonuria is a simple single-gene 1998), childhood (Raine, Reynolds, Venables, Mednick,
recessive disorder whose ill effects (i.e., mental retarda- & Farrington, 1998), and even into adolescence (Caspi
tion) can be eliminated by an environmental interven- et al., 1994). Personality is robustly related to antisocial
tion (i.e., avoiding phenylalanine in the diet). Next, behavior in childhood (e.g., Krueger, Caspi, Moffitt,
there are caveats about the estimates themselves. As White, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1996) and adulthood
noted earlier, the estimates of genetic effects include (Ball, Tennen, Poling, Kranzler, & Rounsaville, 1997).
gene-environment transactions in which genes influence Thus, the question is no longer is there a relation, but
surrounding environments, which, in turn, influence which dimensions of temperament and personality are
phenotypic expression. The ratings of aggressive and most strongly related to antisocial behavior?
antisocial behavior typically used (i.e., ratings by teach-
ers, parents, peers, or the self ) represent generalized Temperament
perceptions about someones aggressive behavior. They Buss and Plomin (1984, p. 84) define temperament as
necessarily ignore the context-specificity of antisocial inherited personality traits present in early child-
behavior. The latent modeling techniques used to com- hood; thus, we restrict our review to those studies that
posite data from multiple sources (e.g., multiple raters) have measured temperament in early life. Original work
typically lead to a loss of rater-specific information. Al- on temperament by Thomas, Chess, and Birch (1968)
though some information that is lost may be error, at identified nine separable dimensions: (1) activity
least some substantial portion may reflect the different level, (2) threshold, (3) mood, (4) rhythmicity, (5) ap-
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 735

proach/withdrawal, (6) intensity, (7) adaptability, (8) by parents and teachers was related to externalizing
distractibility, and (9) attention span /persistence. The problems at age 11 even after controlling for effortful
authors identified three constellations of these dimen- control and externalizing problems 4 years earlier.
sions that they believed were clinically significant: (1) These results typically hold for mother-reported tem-
difficult temperament, (2) easy temperament, and (3) perament and observationally assessed temperament
slow-to-warm-up temperament. The first of these, dif- (Eisenberg, Cumberland, et al., 2001).
ficult temperament, has been frequently examined in Even more impressive, however, are the results from
the context of antisocial behavior and conduct prob- longer-term longitudinal studies from several countries.
lems; it refers to children who are irregular in their be- Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, and Nagin (2003) identified
havior, tend to withdraw from novel situations, are slow developmental trajectories of conduct problems from
to adapt to environmental change, react intensely, and ages 2 to 8 in a sample of 284 low-income boys from an
experience predominantly irritable and negative mood. American city. They found that observed fearlessness at
Several studies have found that early measured difficult age 2 distinguished the chronically high trajectory
temperament is predictive of later antisocial behavior. group from all other trajectory groups. Raine et al.
Bates found that mothers ratings of infant tempera- (1998) found that measures of fearlessness and stimula-
ment as early as age 6 months significantly predict tion seeking at age 3 predicted aggression at age 11 in a
mothers ratings of child conduct problems at age 3 sample of 1130 male and female children from the is-
years (Bates, Maslin, & Frankel, 1985) and, to a lesser land of Mauritius. Tremblay, Pihl, Vitaro, and Dobkin
degree, mothers CBCL Externalizing Scores at age 7 to (1994), using data from the Montreal Longitudinal
8 years (Bates, Bayles, Bennett, Ridge, & Brown, Study on over 1,000 boys, showed that teacher-rated im-
1991). Similar results have been obtained from the Aus- pulsivity in kindergarten predicted self-reported delin-
tralian Temperament Project, a prospective, longitudi- quency at age 13. Finally, the relation between early
nal study of temperament and development in a large temperament and later antisocial behavior has also been
and representative sample of a whole state population. documented in several papers from the Dunedin Multi-
Kingston and Prior (1995) found that early difficult disciplinary Study of Health and Development. Caspi,
temperament significantly discriminated children Henry, Moffitt, and Silva (1995) used temperament rat-
whose aggression started early and maintained over ings derived from examiners behavioral ratings at ages
time from other aggressive and nonaggressive children. 3 and 5 to find that early lack of control was consistently
As noted above, the construct of difficult tempera- related to antisocial behavior at 9 and 11 and CD at 13
ment is a concretion of multiple elements of tempera- and 15. Henry, Caspi, Moffitt, and Silva (1996) found
ment and is therefore of limited use in understanding this same dimension to predict violent convictions at age
which specific elements of temperament are important 18. Moffitt and Caspi (2001) found that lack of control
predictors of outcomes. Studies that have utilized more distinguished LCP offenders from AL offenders.
specific measurements of temperament indicate that the These findings cohere with theoretical and empirical
strongest predictors of later antisocial behavior are, work on conscience development. Kochanska (1997)
borrowing terminology from Rothbart, Ahadi, and posited the existence of two regulatory processes neces-
Evans (2000), dimensions dealing with fearlessness, ir- sary for the adequate development of conscience: (1) af-
ritability/anger/frustration, and effortful control. Many fective discomfort and (2) behavioral control. She found
cross-sectional studies and short-term longitudinal that temperamental variations are important to these
studies have found these dimensions to be moderately processes, with fear being important for the first process
related to aggression and conduct problems in early, and inhibitory/effortful control important for the second.
middle, and late childhood (e.g., Lengua, West, & San- There is also evidence for the interaction between
dler, 1998). For example, in a study of 214 children temperament and the socialization context, particularly
aged 4.5 to 8 years, Eisenberg, Cumberland, et al. features of parenting. Coon, Carey, Corley, and Fulker
(2001) found that measures of fear, anger, and control (1992) found that among difficult-temperament young
distinguished children with externalizing problems children, only those with conjoint maladaptive parenting
from other children defined when using parent and were at risk for later conduct-disordered behavior.
teacher reports. In one of the strongest demonstrations, Kochanska (1997) has also reported an interaction be-
Valiente et al. (2003) found that effortful control rated tween temperament and parenting style in producing
736 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

compliance (see also Brennan, Hall, Bor, Najman, & dicted symptoms of antisocial personality disorder at
Williams, 2003). age 21 above and beyond the effect of antisocial person-
ality disorder at age 18.
Personality Although studied infrequently, there is evidence
Scientific inquiry in personality development has grown for personality by context interactions. Lynam et al.
rapidly, as documented by Caspi and Shiner (Chapter 6, (in press) found that impulsivity and neighborhood-
this Handbook, this volume). J. Miller and Lynam (2001) socioeconomic status interacted to produce violent and
meta-analyzed 59 studies that examined the relation be- nonviolent offending among 430 boys from the Middle
tween antisocial behavior, broadly defined, and one or Sample of the Pittsburgh Youth Study, such that impul-
more structural models of personality. Structural mod- sivity was more strongly related to self-reported of-
els are derived from basic research in personality and fending in poorer neighborhoods.
utilize multiple dimensions to organize the vast array of
Psychopathy
personality traits according to their interrelations. They
share the fundamental assumption that a finite number The construct of psychopathy has been studied inten-
of traits are the basic building blocks of personality, sively at the adult level. Interpersonally, the psychopath
providing comprehensive coverage of human personal- is grandiose, egocentric, manipulative, forceful, and
ity. Importantly, there is a great deal of overlap between cold-hearted; affectively, he or she displays shallow
various structural models (e.g., Five Factor Model; Tel- emotions, is unable to maintain close-relationships, and
legens three-factor model, Eysencks Psychoticism, Ex- lacks empathy, anxiety, and remorse. Behaviorally, the
traversion, Neuroticism [PEN] model), making it easier psychopath commits more types of crime, more crimes
to compare results from different studies. Approxi- of any type, and more violent crimes, in or out of prison,
mately one-third of the studies examined by J. Miller than nonpsychopathic counterparts (see Hare, 2003).
and Lynam were conducted in samples consisting of Lynam and colleagues (i.e., Lynam, 2002; Lynam et al.,
children and adolescents. Across the 18 dimensions in in press; Miller, Lynam, Widiger, & Leukefeld, 2001)
the 4 different structural models examined, 8 dimen- have argued that psychopathy can be understood as a
sions showed correlations greater than .25 with anti- specific constellation of personality traits; in particular,
social behavior: (1) Five Factor Model (FFM) they argue that psychopathy consists of extremely low
Agreeableness (negative) and Conscientiousness (nega- Agreeableness, low Conscientiousness, a mixture of
tive); (2) Eysencks Psychoticism (positive); (3) Telle- high and low Neuroticism (i.e., high in angry hos-
gens Negative Emotionality (positive) and Constraint tility and impulsiveness and low in anxiety, self-
(negative); and (4) Cloningers Novelty Seeking (posi- consciousness, and vulnerability), and a mixture of high
tive), Self-Directedness (negative), and Cooperativeness and low Extraversion (i.e., high in assertiveness and ex-
(negative). All of these scales can be understood as as- citement seeking and low in warmth). As a constellation
sessing either Agreeableness or Conscientiousness/Con- of traits, it warrants mention in the present discussion of
straint. The strength of these relations did not vary dispositional factors, particularly given that several in-
across age, type of sample (institutionalized versus not), vestigators have imported the concept of psychopathy to
or assessment source (e.g., self versus other, official juveniles (Frick, OBrien, Wooton, & McBurnett, 1994;
versus self ). Other dimensions, theorized to be impor- Lynam, 1997).
tant to antisocial behavior, such as Neuroticism and Ex- Over 20 studies have examined the relation between
traversion bore no significant overall relations to juvenile psychopathy and offending. Almost without ex-
antisocial behavior. ception, these studies have found similar relations be-
These effects are not due to predictor-criterion over- tween psychopathy and antisocial behavior in juveniles
lap. The personality inventories contain very little refer- as those found in adults. Juvenile psychopathy is moder-
ence to explicitly antisocial behavior; in cases where ately strongly related to age at onset (e.g., Corrado, Vin-
this is not true, results hold even when overlapping ele- cent, Hart, & Cohen, 2004), number and variety of
ments are removed (e.g., Krueger et al., 1996). These re- offenses (e.g., Kosson, Cyterski, Steuerwald, Neumann,
lations hold prospectively: in a particularly stringent & Walker-Matthews, 2002), stability of offending
test, Krueger (1999), in the Dunedin Study, found that across time (e.g., Lynam, 1997), and quantity and qual-
low levels of constraint and agreeableness at age 18 pre- ity of aggression (e.g., Frick et al., 1994; Murrie, Cor-
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 737

nell, Kaplan, McConville, & Levy-Elkon, 2004). For ex- Finally, and importantly, several studies have at-
ample, using his Childhood Psychopathy Scale, in a sam- tempted to demonstrate the incremental validity pro-
ple of 430 12- to 13-year-old boys from a high-risk vided by the construct of juvenile psychopathy in
study, Lynam (1997) reported that juvenile psychopathy predicting antisocial behavior (Lynam, 1997; Murrie
was moderately correlated (rs range from .19 to .39) et al., 2004; Stafford & Cornell, 2003). Lynam (1997)
with past and current delinquency and related to serious demonstrated that scores on the CPS were related to
delinquency that is stable across time. Kosson et al. concurrent serious delinquency above and beyond social
(2002) examined correlations between offending history class, IQ, impulsivity, and delinquency. In the strongest
and scores on the Personality Checklist Youth Version demonstrations, studies showed that psychopathy pre-
(PCL-YV) in a sample of 115 adolescent males on pro- dicted future antisocial behavior above and beyond cur-
bation. They report that psychopathy scores correlated rent antisocial behavior. Murrie et al. (2004), in a
with the number of nonviolent charges (r = .35), number sample of 113 incarcerated adolescents, reported that
of violent charges (r = .27), total number of charges (r = scores on the PCL-YV predicted institutional violence
.42), and the number of different charges (r = .45). above and beyond previous violence.
These results held across different measurement instru-
ments, reporting sources, samples, and ages.
Neuropsychological Factors
Moreover, several of these studies have examined the
predictive relations between juvenile psychopathy and The idea of a link between the physical health of an indi-
antisocial behavior, including the relation between juve- viduals brain and his or her level of antisocial behavior
nile psychopathy and institutional infractions (e.g., has been in the literature for centuries. Benjamin Rush
Edens, Poythress, & Lilienfeld, 1999; Murrie et al., (1812; cited in F. Elliott, 1978, p. 147) referred to the
2004; Stafford & Cornell, 2003). Spain, Douglas, total perversion of the moral faculties in people who
Poythress, and Epstein (2004) administered three psy- displayed innate preternatural moral depravity. Rush
chopathy instruments to 85 male adolescent offenders further suggested that there is probably an original de-
aged 11 to 18 in a residential treatment facility and fective organization in those parts of the body that are
found significant relations between the total number of occupied by the moral faculties of the mind. Since
infractions (physical, verbal, and administrative) and Rushs day, there have been numerous advances in our
each of the psychopathy indices, with rs of .27, .38, and understanding of the human brain and in our ability to
.43 for the PCL-YV, Antisocial Process Screening De- measure its functioning. There is strong evidence that
vice (APSD) and Child Psychopathy Scale (CPS), re- antisocial children, adolescents, and adults are im-
spectively. Additional studies have examined the paired, relative to nonantisocial controls, in verbal abil-
predictive relations between juvenile psychopathy mea- ity and executive functioning (see Lynam & Henry,
sures and recidivism (e.g., Gretton, Hare, & Catchpole, 2001, for a review). There is also some indication that
2004). Corrado et al. (2004) followed 182 male adoles- antisocial individuals may be impaired in spatial func-
cent offenders for, on average, 14.5 months. They found tions, particularly at young ages.
that high scorers on the PCL-YV reoffended earlier
than low scorers; this was true for both nonviolent (6.87 Verbal Ability
months versus 12.33 months) and violent offenses One of the most robust correlates of severe conduct
(13.55 months versus 18.17 months). Again, these re- problems is impaired verbal ability. Verbal deficits have
sults mirror those found among adults. been found in aggressive toddlers, conduct-disordered
Two studies examined the relation between juvenile children, serious adolescent delinquents, and adult crim-
psychopathy and treatment outcomes (ONeil, Lidz, & inals. There have been at least six comprehensive re-
Heilbrun, 2003; Spain et al., 2004). Using the PCL-YV views since the first by Prentice and Kelly in 1963; each
in a sample of 64 adjudicated youth in a substance treat- review includes additional confirming studies (see
ment program, ONeil et al. (2003) found that high scor- Lynam & Henry, 2001). The finding of impaired verbal
ers on the PCL-YV attended the program for fewer ability in antisocial persons continues to be replicated
days, participated more poorly when they did attend, (e.g., Dery, Toupin, Pauze, Mercier, & Fortin, 1999;
and showed less clinical improvement across the course Dionne et al., 2003; Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood,
of treatment. 1996; Lahey et al., 1995; Lynam, Moffitt, &
738 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Stouthamer-Loeber, 1993; Moffitt, Lynam, & Silva, considered in conjunction with a parental history of
1994). Deficient verbal functioning has been found in antisocial personality disorder (APD); only boys with
studies using Verbal IQ tests as well as in those using above average VIQ and without a parental history of
standard neuropsychological tests. APD improved across time. Similarly, Farrington and
The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Develop- Hawkins (1991) found that low VIQ at age 8 to 10 pre-
ment Study, a longitudinal study of a birth cohort of dicted persistence in crime after the 21st birthday (phi =
over 1,000 subjects who have been studied extensively .23), even after controlling for other predictors. It is im-
from birth to age 27 through comprehensive, biannual portant to note that the differences between antisocial
assessments, has provided some of the strongest evi- and nonantisocial youth remain even after controlling
dence. In addition to several biennial IQ assessments, for potential confounds, such as ethnicity (e.g., Lynam
the sample was administered a comprehensive neuropsy- et al., 1993), socioeconomic status (e.g., Dery et al.,
chological assessment battery when participants were 1999; Lynam et al., 1993, Moffitt, Gabrielli, Mednick,
13 years old. When Moffitt (1990) examined the devel- & Schulsinger, 1981), academic attainment (e.g., Denno,
opmental trajectories of boys with both conduct prob- 1989; Lynam et al., 1993), test motivation (e.g., Lynam
lems (CP) and hyperactive-impulsive-attention et al., 1993), and the differential detection of low-IQ
problems (HIA) and boys with only conduct problems or delinquents (Moffitt & Silva, 1988a).
only HIA from age 3 to 15, the comorbid cases were
found to have histories of extreme antisocial behavior Executive Functions
that remained stable across this period. Their neuropsy- Antisocial behavior has been associated with deficien-
chological problems were as long-standing as their anti- cies in the brains self-control or executive functions,
social behavior. At ages 3 and 5, these boys had scored which include operations such as sustaining attention
more than a standard deviation below the age-norm for and concentration, abstract reasoning and concept for-
boys on the Bayley and McCarthy tests of motor coordi- mation, formulating goals, anticipating and planning,
nation; at each age (5, 7, 9, 11, and 13), these boys programming and initiating purposive sequences of be-
scored a more than .75 of a standard deviation below the havior, and inhibiting unsuccessful, inappropriate, or
age-norm for boys on verbal IQ (VIQ). Moffitt et al. impulsive behaviors. Evidence of the relation between
(1994) demonstrated the ability of deficits in neuropsy- executive deficits and antisocial behavior has been
chological functioning at 13 to predict antisocial behav- found among incarcerated subjects, among nonconduct-
ior in later adolescence. Whether antisocial behavior disordered subjects in laboratory situations, and among
was measured with self-reports, police reports, or court general-population samples.
reports, the poorer a boys neuropsychological function- Several studies that have applied batteries of formal
ing at age 13, the more likely he was to have committed tests of executive functions to delinquent subjects have
crimes at age 18. The strongest relations were obtained shown that test scores discriminate between antisocial
on the verbal and verbal memory factors of the test bat- and nonantisocial children and adolescents (see Moffitt,
tery. Not only did scores on verbal and verbal memory 1990; Lynam & Henry, 2001, for reviews). Multivariate
factors relate to the early onset of offending, they also analyses of frontal lobe batteries, for example, the Wis-
related to the persistence of offending across time. In an consin Card Sorting Task, Verbal Fluency, Trails B,
even more recent report, these same measures of verbal Mazes, and the Rey Osterreith Complex Figure Test,
functioning distinguished LCP offenders from AL of- with a general population sample in New Zealand (Mof-
fenders, defined on the basis of antisocial behavior from fitt & Henry, 1989) have demonstrated that a linear
ages 5 to 18 (Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). combination of these scores significantly discriminated
The relation between poor verbal ability and the per- self-reported early delinquents from nondelinquents.
sistence of antisocial behavior has also been found This effect was most robust among the delinquents with
among clinic-referred children. Lahey et al. (1995) ex- co-occurring HIA; differences between this comorbid
amined factors related to the persistence of CD across 4 group and nondisordered controls ranged from two-
years in a relatively large, prospective study of clinic- thirds to over one standard deviation and remained even
referred boys. As expected, low VIQ was related to CD after controlling for IQ.
at Time 1. More important, low VIQ was related to the Other studies, although not focusing specifically
persistence of CD over time, particularly when VIQ was upon executive functions, have reported findings from
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 739

individual measures typically included in frontal lobe Longitudinal studies have demonstrated that execu-
batteries. Berman and Siegal (1976) found that delin- tive function deficits are associated with the stability
quents scored poorly on the Category Test and Trails B. and continuity of conduct problems. Seguin, Pihl,
Wolff, Waber, Bauermeister, Cohen, and Ferber (1982) Harden, Tremblay, and Boulerice (1995) found that boys
reported delinquency-related impairments on tests of who exhibited a stable pattern of aggression between the
selective attention and on the Stroop Color-Word Test. ages of 6 and 12 performed significantly more poorly on
A number of studies have shown delinquents to score measures of executive functions than did unstable ag-
poorly on various tests requiring sequencing of motor gressive or nonaggressive boys. Seguin, Nagin, Assaad,
behavior (e.g., Brickman, McManus, Grapentine, & and Tremblay (in press) have shown that deficits in
Alessi, 1984; Lueger & Gill, 1990). working memory, an aspect of executive functioning,
Recently, several investigators have employed more are most pronounced in adolescents who are consistently
sophisticated measures, such as the Self-Ordered Point- high in physical aggression and motoric hyperactivity.
ing (SOP; Petrides & Milner, 1982) task and the Condi- Taken together, these studies suggest that neuropsy-
tional Association Task (CAT; Petrides, 1985), to chological dysfunctions that manifest themselves as
investigate the relation between aggression and frontal poor scores on tests of self-control are linked with early
lobe functions, particularly working memory. Positron onset and persistence of antisocial behavior. Addition-
emission topography studies have found that the SOP is ally, the findings in regard to the group comorbid for
specifically associated with the mid-dorsolateral frontal HIA and CP are of considerable interest in light of evi-
region, whereas the CAT is specifically associated with dence that the co-occurrence of CP and HIA may repre-
the posterior dorsolateral frontal region (Petrides, sent a distinct subtype of CD that is particularly severe
Alivisatos, Evans, & Meyer, 1993). Lau, Pihl, and Peter- and persistent and places the child at risk for serious
son (1995) found that poor performance on these two antisocial behavior in adolescence and adulthood.
measures was associated with aggression in a laboratory
setting. Giancola and Zeichner (1994) reported that per- Spatial Deficits
formance on the CAT was associated with intensity of A few studies suggest that spatial deficits may also
shocks administered to a fictitious opponent in a labora- characterize severely antisocial children and adoles-
tory setting. cents, particularly at younger ages (Raine, Yaralian,
As was the case for verbal deficits, the evidence sug- Reynolds, & Venables, 2002). Dietz, Lavigne, Arend,
gests that poor executive functioning may be especially and Rosenbaum (1997) found that both performance
characteristic of the most antisocial group: boys with and VIQ were related to externalizing problems in a
symptoms of conduct problems and HIA. In the New sample of 2- to 5-year-old children. In perhaps the most
Zealand study, adolescent boys who exhibited symp- compelling report, Raine et al. (2002) examined the re-
toms of both CD and HIA scored more poorly on neu- lation between verbal and spatial abilities at 3 years of
ropsychological tests of executive functions than their age and antisocial behavior at ages 8 and 17. These au-
peers who had either CD or HIA alone (Moffitt & thors found that early spatial, not verbal, deficits pre-
Henry, 1989; Moffitt & Silva, 1988b). In a companion dicted later antisocial behavior, even after controlling
study of executive functions and conduct problems in for potential covariates including test behavior and so-
the Pittsburgh Youth Study (White et al., 1994), data cial adversity.
were gathered on self-control and impulsivity using
multiple tests and measures for 430 12-year-old boys. Neuroimaging
The impulsivity measures were strongly related to Researchers have employed methods that assess both the
delinquency at two ages even after controlling for IQ structural (e.g., computerized tomography, magnetic
and socioeconomic status; additionally, these measures resonance imaging) and functional (e.g., positron emis-
were related to the 3-year longevity of antisocial behav- sion tomography, single photon emission computed to-
ior, even after controlling for initial levels of delin- mography) characteristics of the brains of antisocial
quency. In a separate study, Aronowitz et al. (1994) individuals (for reviews, see Bassarath, 2001; Lynam &
reported that adolescents with both CD and HIA per- Henry, 2001). Almost all of these studies have been con-
formed more poorly on measures of executive function ducted with adults, using violent, aggressive, criminal,
than did CD-only adolescents. sexual, and/or psychopathic individuals. Results have
740 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

varied, but where significant findings do emerge, they anomaly. Natural experiments in which individuals have
generally involve dysfunction in the temporal and sustained severe head injury also suggest that changes in
frontal regions among offenders, a pattern supportive of neuropsychological status are associated with changes
results found in studies using performance tests. For ex- in antisocial status, with the case of Phineas Gage being
ample, Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse, and Colletti the best known. Finally, several studies have tested and
(2000) employed structural magnetic resonance imaging ruled out viable third variable explanations of the rela-
in 21 community volunteers with APD and 2 control tion (e.g., Lynam et al., 1993).
groups, one consisting of 27 men with substance de-
pendence but without APD and one of 34 men with nei- Biological Factors
ther substance dependence nor APD. They found that
the APD group had lower prefrontal volumes than both To the degree that temperament, personality, and neu-
control groups. The few studies that have been con- ropsychological health are based in biology, the evi-
ducted in children and adolescents are consistent with dence reviewed above demonstrates that biological
the findings among adults. For example, Lyoo, Lee, variables are consistently related to antisocial behavior.
Jung, Noam, and Renshaw (2002) administered mag- In the sections that follow, we examine the relations be-
netic resonance imaging (MRI ) assessments to over 400 tween more direct indicators of biological function and
children and adolescents with psychiatric disorders and antisocial behavior. Specifically, we review evidence
found that the group with attention deficit disorder and that links antisocial behavior to pre- and perinatal com-
CD had more severe levels of white matter signal hyper- plications, early exposure to nicotine, neurotransmitter
intensities in the frontal lobes than did controls. activity, sex hormones, and autonomic reactivity. As we
This literature, however, is far from complete, even at show, each of these factors has been linked to antisocial
the adult level. Reliance on small sample sizes, failure to behavior. Perhaps most interesting, across many studies,
use noncriminal control groups, and use of a wide vari- the effects of many of these biological variables are
ety of types of offenders precludes the drawing of any strongest under adverse environmental circumstances.
firm conclusions. The very tentative suggestion is that
the results of neuroimaging studies are consistent with Prenatal and Perinatal Complications
results from performance tests of neuropsychological Moffitts (1993) theory of LCP antisocial behavior
function. However, the two literatures are not inte- posits that prenatal and perinatal complications con-
grated, and much future research is needed to explore tribute to the neuropsychological problems that underlie
these issues more fully. this type of offending. These complications have been
Although it is difficult to demonstrate causality un- examined as predictors of antisocial behavior in multi-
equivocally in most areas of human behavior, studies of ple studies. At least 6 studies have found associations
the relation between neuropsychological health and anti- between minor physical anomalies (MPAs), presumed to
social behavior suggest that neuropsychological deficits be markers for fetal maldevelopment, and antisocial be-
can be one cause of serious antisocial behavior. The evi- havior in children (see Raine, 2002, for a review). Arse-
dence reviewed suggests that poorer neuropsychological neault, Tremblay, Boulerice, Seguin, and Saucier (2000)
health is associated with more severe antisocial behav- found that MPAs assessed in a sample of 170 males at
ior, with moderate effect sizes that survive frequent, age 14 predicted violent delinquency at age 17. In sev-
conservative controls for other variables. Additionally, eral studies, MPAs have been found to interact with so-
several observational studies suggest that poor neu- cial factors to predict antisocial behavior. For example,
ropsychological health is present before the onset of se- Brennan, Mednick, and Raine (1997) found that men
rious antisocial behavior (e.g., Denno, 1989), although with both MPAs and high family adversity had the high-
there is one published exception. Aguilar, Sroufe, Ege- est rates of adult offending in a sample of male offspring
land, and Carlson (2000) found differences between of psychiatrically ill parents. Pine, Coplan, et al. (1997)
childhood-onset and adolescent-onset offenders in late found that the presence of MPAs interacted with envi-
childhood but not early childhood; methodological prob- ronmental disadvantage to predict CD at age 17.
lems including the small, nonrepresentative sample and There have also been a number of studies from large,
psychometrically weak instruments may account for this longitudinal studies in multiple countries examining the
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 741

relation between birth complications and antisocial be- Moffitt (2004) found that after controlling for genetic
havior. Almost all have found that the relations between influences, parental antisocial behavior and depression,
birth complications and antisocial behavior are stronger and family disadvantage the effect of heavy maternal
when other psychosocial risk factors are present. In one smoking on CP at age 7, but not age 5, remained signifi-
of the most impressive demonstrations, Raine, Brennan, cant. The models estimated by Maughan et al. are con-
and Mednick (1994) found that birth complications and sistent with heavy maternal smoking playing a
maternal rejection at age 1 interacted to predict violent mediating role between the more distal risk factors and
offending at age 18 in a sample of over 4,200 men from the outcome.
Copenhagen. In the follow-up at age 34, the interaction As with other biological variables, there is evidence
between biological and social risk predicted early onset, that smoking during pregnancy interacts with social
serious violent behavior. These findings from Denmark risks to increase the likelihood of antisocial behavior.
have been replicated in 4 other countries. Piquero and Rasanen et al. (1999), using data from a large Finnish
Tibbett (1999), in the Philadelphia Collaborative Peri- birth cohort, found an almost 12-fold increase in recidi-
natal Project, found that the combination of vistic violent offending through age 26 in offspring
prenatal /perinatal complications and family disadvan- whose mothers smoked and who were born into single-
tage was linked with adult violent offending. Similar re- parent families.
sults have been obtained in large samples from Sweden
(Hodgins, Kratzer, & McNeil, 2001), Finland (Kemp- Autonomic Nervous System Activity
painen, Jokelainen, Jaervelin, Isohanni, & Raesaenen, There is extensive evidence for the relation between low
2001), and Canada (Arseneault, Tremblay, Boulerice, & resting heart rate and antisocial behavior. In fact, Raine
Saucier, 2002). (2002) calls low resting heart rate the best-replicated
biological correlate of antisocial behavior in child and
In Utero Exposure to Nicotine adolescent samples (p. 418). The relation is present in
Several large, longitudinal studies in various countries cross-sectional studies (e.g., Rogeness, Cepeda,
have shown that maternal smoking during pregnancy Macedo, Fischer, & Harris, 1990) and prospective stud-
places the offspring at increased risk for later antisocial ies (e.g., Farrington, 1997). Raine, Venables, and Med-
behavior. For example, in the Christchurch Health and nick (1997) examined the relation between low resting
Development Study, a large, longitudinal study based in heart rate at age 3 and antisocial behavior at age 11 in a
Christchurch, New Zealand, Fergusson, Woodward, and sample of 1,795 male and female children from Mauri-
Horwood (1998) found that smoking during pregnancy tius. These authors found that early low resting heart
almost doubled the risk for conduct problems in boys, rate predicted later aggressive behavior. Moreover, these
even after controlling for antenatal and postnatal risk results were the same across gender and ethnicity and
factors (see also, Wakschlag et al., 1997; Weissman, held despite controls for various biological, psychologi-
Warner, Wickramaratne, & Kandel, 1999). Relations be- cal, and psychiatric mediators and confounds. Similar
tween smoking during pregnancy and later adult offend- findings have been obtained in other large, prospective
ing have also been observed. Brennan, Grekin, and studies from England (Farrington, 1997), New Zealand
Mednick (1999) found a twofold increase in adult violent (Moffitt & Caspi, 2001), and Canada (Kindlon et al.,
offending in the offspring of mothers who smoked in a 1995). These findings hold after controlling for other
birth cohort of over 4,000 men. Several studies have variables including physique, exercise, socioeconomic
found that these relations hold even after controlling status, motor activity, substance use, and psychosical
many potentially confounding variables, including so- adversity. Farrington (1997), in the Cambridge Study in
cioeconomic status, maternal education, mothers age at Delinquent Development, found that low resting heart
first birth, family size, parenting behaviors, parental rate and poor concentration were the only two risk fac-
psychopathology, birth weight, and perinatal complica- tors, out of 48 examined, that were independently pre-
tions. One study found that the relation between heavy dictive of violence. Low resting heart rate does not
maternal smoking and CP at age 7 persisted even in the appear to interact with social adversity to increase of-
face of multiple, well-measured controls; using data fending. To the contrary, a single study reports that low
from the E-risk study, Maughan, Taylor, Caspi, and resting heart rate is related to antisocial behavior only in
742 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

children with nonadverse circumstances (Raine, Bren- increases testosterone levels. Observational and experi-
nan, & Farrington, 1997). mental research in humans supports this conclusion as
well. Schaal, Tremblay, Soussignan, and Susman (1996)
Sex Hormones examined the relations among testosterone, aggression,
Theoretically, testosterone is a likely candidate as an im- and social status in 13-year-old boys whose aggressive
portant biological factor in antisocial behavior. The dis- behavior and peer status had been assessed continuously
tribution of testosterone across the population and across for the previous 7 years. They found that testosterone
development parallels what is known about the distribu- was more closely linked to social dominance than to ag-
tion of antisocial behavior. Testosterone and antisocial gression. Those boys rated by newly acquainted peers as
behavior are more concentrated in men than women, and both tough and social leaders had the highest testos-
its level increases dramatically across adolescence. In terone levels, even though they were not rated by every-
nonhuman animals, the relation between testosterone day peers and teachers as being high on physical
and aggression has been unequivocally demonstrated aggression. Boys rated as tough, but not leaders, had
through correlational and experimental studies (Turner, testosterone levels no greater than the nontough boys,
1994). Despite the theoretical appeal, the evidence sug- even though the tough nonleaders were rated highest in
gests that testosterone has, at best, a relatively weak cor- physical aggression by peers and teachers. Boys who had
relation with antisocial behavior in humans. been highly aggressive from ages 6 through 12 had lower
Archer (1991) conducted three meta-analyses, in- testosterone levels at age 13 than boys who were consis-
cluding only 5 to 6 studies each, and found a weak, pos- tently low on fighting in elementary school. Salvador,
itive correlation between testosterone and aggression. A Simon, Suay, and Llorens (1987) studied male judo
more recent, larger, meta-analysis by Book, Starzyk, competitors and found that testosterone levels were pos-
and Quinsey (2001) confirms Archers initial results. itively related to success in competition. Gladue,
These authors analyzed 45 independent studies that Boechler, and McCaul (1989) found that winners in a re-
yielded 54 independent effect sizes. Their correlations action-time competition had elevated testosterone levels
ranged from .28 to .71 with a weighted mean correla- after the competition, but losers maintained their origi-
tion of .14. In contrast to several qualitative reviews, nal levels. Similar results were obtained even when the
these authors reported that the relation between testos- contest simply involved coin-tossing (McCaul, Gladue,
terone and aggression was actually somewhat larger in & Joppa, 1992).
younger, compared to older samples; this effect of age, One study has examined the interaction between
however, held for males but not females. Book et al. re- testosterone and environmental context. Rowe, Maughan,
ported weighted mean correlations of .21, .18, and .10 Worthman, Costello, and Angold (2004) examined the
for participants aged 13 to 20, 21 to 35, and over 35, re- relations among testosterone, peer deviance, antisocial
spectively. Interestingly, there was no difference in the behavior, and social dominance in a large sample of boys
effect size for testosterone across genders. from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. These authors
Not only is the relation between testosterone and ag- found that levels of testosterone were related to nonag-
gression relatively weak, the direction of effect is not gressive conduct problems, primarily among boys with
entirely clear. On one hand, a study by Finkelstein et al. deviant peers. Among boys with nondeviant peers,
(1997) in hypogonadal adolescents showed that changes levels of testosterone were related to social dominance
in testosterone may instigate changes in aggression. but not CP.
These authors administered depo-testosterone to 35
Neurotransmitters
adolescents in 3-month blocks alternating with placebo
at 3 dose levels approximating early, middle, and late The most widely studied of the neurotransmitters in re-
pubertal amounts. Results demonstrated significant hor- lation to aggression and antisocial behavior is serotonin.
monal effects on physically aggressive behaviors and There is an extensive research literature, including both
impulses. On the other hand, there is evidence that dom- human and animal studies, indicating that the central
inance may increase testosterone. Archer (1988) re- serotonergic system is involved in the regulation of im-
viewed animal studies relating to this question and pulsive aggressive behavior (Herbert & Martinez, 2001).
concluded that dominance or success in conflict often Decreased serotonergic functioning has been found
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 743

among adults with past histories of aggressive acts in- with a history of family conflict to predict violence at
cluding violent offenses and suicide (see Asberg, 1994). age 21 in the males of the Dunedin study. The nature of
Specifically, lower concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid the interaction was such that men with high levels of
(CSF) 5-HIAA, the major metabolite of serotonin, have whole blood serotonin, and therefore low levels of sero-
been found among individuals with past histories of sui- tonin in the brain, and history of psychosocial adversity
cide attempts, in violent offenders, in individuals with were the most violent by both official and self-report.
personality disorders characterized by aggression, and
in violent alcoholics (e.g., Tuinier, Verhoeven, & van
Praag, 1995; Virkkunen, Eggert, Rawlings, & Linnoila, Ecological Factors and Social Stressors
1996). Other researchers have indexed platelet levels of
As compelling as constitutional and biological factors
monoamine oxidase (MAO), which is responsible for
are in leading to aggressive behavior, ecological factors
metabolizing both serotonin and dopamine. Although
play just as strong a role, and an even stronger role for
MAO activity is an indirect measure, results from stud-
certain indicators of aggression. The 600% increase in
ies using it are consistent with studies that have exam-
juvenile murder arrests between 1965 and 1994 (Blum-
ined serotonin more directly. Low MAO activity in
stein, 2000) and wide variations across countries cannot
platelets has been shown to be associated with impulsiv-
be accounted for by enduring characteristics such as
ity, violent crime, and persistent criminality (e.g., Alm
genes and traits. Ecological contexts surely play a major
et al., 1994; Belfrage, Lidberg, & Oreland, 1992). In an-
role in predicting individual differences in aggressive
other noninvasive approach, Coccaro (1989) has shown
behavior as well.
that a blunted prolactin response to fenfluramine chal-
lenge, taken as an index of diminished serotonergic re-
Culture, Laws, and Policies
sponse, is associated with impulsive aggression. Some
experimental research is also consistent with the idea Cultural norms and public policies have enormous influ-
that levels of serotonin are related to levels of aggres- ence over community-wide rates of aggressive behaviors
sion. Cleare and Bond (1995) depleted levels of trypto- such as gun violence. Firearm homicide rates, including
phan, a serotonin precursor, in a sample of normal male rates for children, are 12 to 16 times higher in the United
participants. These authors found that among those par- States than in the average of 25 other industrialized
ticipants with preexisting aggressive traits, tryptophan countries, including Canada, simply because of differ-
depletion increased both subjective feelings and objec- ences in laws that allow gun ownership (U.S. Department
tive ratings of aggression. of Health and Human Services, 1997). In the United
In studies of children, however, results are more States, children in the 5 states with the highest levels of
mixed. Kruesi, Swedo, Leonard, and Rubinow (1990) gun ownership are 3 times more likely to die from
found, in a sample of children with disruptive behavior firearm homicide as are children from the 5 states with
disorders, that lower levels of CSF 5-HIAA were related the lowest levels of gun ownership (M. Miller, Azrael, &
to higher ratings of aggressive behavior. Moreover, Hemenway, 2002). Gun ownership is a product of laws
Kruesi et al. (1992) found that lower 5-HIAA levels and cultural norms, but surely these factors cannot be
were predictive of aggressive behaviors at a follow-up 2 dismissed as by-products of genetic or biological varia-
years later. Other studies have found opposite results. tions across states in this country (Wintemute, 2000).
Castellanos et al. (1994) found that CSF 5-HIAA was Nisbett and Cohen (1996) compiled population-level and
positively correlated with aggression in 29 boys with at- laboratory evidence to argue that a culture of honor is
tention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). responsible for consistently higher rates of violence in
Halperin et al. (1994) and Pine et al. (1997) both found the American South than in other geographic regions.
that increased prolactin response to fenfluramine chal-
lenge, indicating increased serotonergic activity, was re- Community Factors
lated to increased aggression. The crowded inner city has long been highlighted as a
One study examined the interaction between neuro- setting for high rates of violence (Hammond & Yung,
transmitters and environments. Moffitt, Caspi, and Faw- 1991). In a classic work, C. Shaw and McKay (1942) ar-
cett (1997) found that serotonergic activity interacted gued that the three community structural variables of
744 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

poverty, ethnic heterogeneity, and high residential mo- socioeconomic status. Controlling for other community
bility are associated with high violent crime rates that variables, poverty in the family is associated with
persist across time, even after the entire population in a higher rates of peer-directed aggressive behavior by
community changes. Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls children (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002), adolescents
(1997) used multilevel analyses of adolescents to find (Spencer, Dobbs, & Phillips, 1988), and adults (Samp-
large variations in violent behaviors that are associated son & Laub, 1994). The potential problem of selection
with structural characteristics across neighborhoods in bias into poverty limits confidence in the causal role
Chicago. Census tract data document that neighbor- that family poverty plays in child conduct problems. A
hood-level scores for the proportions of families charac- recent study by Costello, Compton, Keeler, and Angold
terized by poverty, unemployment, low education, (2003) capitalized on the natural experiment afforded
single-parent households, high residential mobility, and by a government policy enabling Native American fami-
low income represent significant risk factors for individ- lies in western North Carolina to reap the financial ben-
ual-level conduct problems (Beyers, Bates, Pettit, & efits of a new casino in their community. Of the
Dodge, 2003). previously poor families, those that were suddenly
A problem in interpretation of neighborhood and thrust out of poverty (21.3% of all poor families) had
community effects concerns the likelihood of self- children whose behavioral problem symptoms declined
migration into neighborhoods by families of varying by 40%, whereas never-poor children in the same com-
background characteristics. As Jencks and Mayer munity displayed no change in symptoms across the
(1990) noted, the most fundamental problem con- same period. This finding might suggest a causal role of
fronting anyone who wants to estimate neighborhoods family wealth in alleviating these symptoms, although
effects on children is distinguishing between neighbor- even this natural experiment has a caveat: not all fami-
hood effects and family effects (p. 119), known as the lies who received financial benefits were brought out of
omitted variable bias or social selection. The problem of poverty. Almost 79% of poor families remained below
self-selection has been partially solved by quasi-experi- the poverty level even after the casino brought them fi-
ments in housing afforded by government-assisted hous- nancial gains (either they squandered the resources or
ing programs that assign families somewhat randomly the resources were insufficient to have an enduring im-
into different neighborhoods. Votruba and Kling (2004) pact); their childrens symptoms increased by 21%
analyzed data from the Gautreux Program in Chicago across the same time period. The full effect (combining
and found that mortality of African American male ex-poor and always-poor families) of the introduction of
youth (mainly by violence) varied as a function of neigh- the casino on child behavior problem symptoms was ac-
borhood characteristics related to human capital and tually negligible.
work, such as unemployment rates and levels of educa- McLoyd (1990) has examined the family mechanisms
tion among the adults in the neighborhood. through which poverty might exert its impact. She found
Neighborhood effects go well beyond structural char- that poverty increases single African American parents
acteristics to social factors such as disorganization and psychological distress and impairs their social support
control. These latter factors have been called collective systems, which, in turn, diminishes their effectiveness
efficacy by Sampson et al. (1997) and are indexed by and increases their coerciveness toward their children.
levels of trust among neighbors, supportive social net- These effects, in turn, may lead to child aggression.
works, and the degree to which neighbors look out for Consistent with this formulation, Guerra, Huesmann,
one another. These factors partially mediate the effects Tolan, Van Acker, and Eron (1995) found that, among
of structural factors but also operate independently (In- urban Chicagoans, poverty is correlated with stressful
goldsby & Shaw, 2002). life events and a lack of parental involvement in reli-
gious support systems and that these two factors, in
Within-Family Ecological Factors
turn, account for the effect of poverty on peer-
Neighborhood factors influence a childs development at nominated aggression. Sampson and Laub (1994) reana-
least partly through their effects on the family unit lyzed the Glueck and Glueck (1950) longitudinal data
(McLoyd, 1990; Wilson, 1987), and the family social set involving 1,000 Depression-era White families and
context exerts its own independent effect on antisocial found that the structural variable of family poverty in-
development. The most important of these factors is low fluenced family processes of harsh discipline, low su-
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 745

pervision, and poor parent-child attachment, which, in Alternatively, analyses by Bolger, Patterson, Thomp-
turn, influenced juvenile delinquency. Family process son, and Kupersmidt (1995) indicate that the effects of
accounted for two-thirds of the effect of poverty on economic hardship are more dramatic among children in
delinquency. Likewise, Dodge, Pettit, and Bates (1994) the racial majority than among those who are already
found that harsh physical discipline by parents ac- stressed by the circumstances of racial minority status.
counted for about half of the effect of low socioeco- Being Black brings numerous social hardships in Ameri-
nomic status on childrens aggressive behavior. can society (Ogbu, 1990) and, with these hardships, rel-
A second major family context factor is marital con- ative risk for aggressive behavior. The effects of
flict. Cummings and Davies (2002) accumulated labora- economic hardship, above and beyond the hardships al-
tory and naturalistic evidence to determine that ambient ready imposed by racial stratification, are muted for
conflict increases child aggression. Cummings, Goeke- African American children (Bolger et al., 1995).
Morey, and Papp (in press) used daily home diary re-
ports to find that everyday marital conflicts increase Non-Family Child Care
childrens aggressive behavior. Fergusson and Horwood The experience of early out-of-home group child care
(1998) found robust correlations between observing do- has been posited as a cause of child aggressive behavior
mestic violence and later antisocial behavior. The stress (Belsky, 2001). However, families self-select into group
of child conduct problems can increase marital conflict, child care for a variety of reasons, including attitudes
and so the issue of temporal ordering is crucial in under- about day care, availability, and ability to pay for other
standing the role of this context factor in child behavior. kinds of care (including a parent staying at home). The
Recent advances in multilevel modeling of developmen- National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
tal trajectories afford the opportunity to evaluate ment (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network
within-individual changes in a childs behavior as a (2004) study of child care in the United States offers the
function of an environmental event, reducing the likeli- most comprehensive opportunity to study these effects.
hood that the environmental effects can be explained Analyses of the 1,081 children in this sample, controlled
away as being due to correlation with static traits or to for many potentially confounding variables, yielded a
reverse causal processes. Malone et al. (2004) followed positive effect size of about one-fourth standard devia-
356 boys and girls across 10 years, as some of their fam- tion of day care experience on aggressive behavior at
ilies experienced divorce, and found that for boys (espe- age 4.5 years and kindergarten as rated by mothers,
cially younger boys) the experience of parental divorce caregivers, and teachers. Borge, Rutter, Cote, and Trem-
increased their externalizing problems in the year of the blay (2004) analyzed data from the 15,579 families of
divorce. Furthermore, these problems continued for sev- the Canadian National Longitudinal Survey of Children
eral years following divorce. Finally, Jaffee, Moffitt, and Youth to find that once selection factors were taken
Caspi, Taylor, and Arseneault (2002) employed a twin into account, children reared in homes by their mothers
research design to find that adult domestic violence ac- were actually more physically aggressive than children
counted for 5% of the variance in child antisocial behav- reared in group day care. Further analyses of the
ior, even when genetic factors are controlled. NICHD study sample by Love et al. (2003) and Votruba-
Other early environmental conditions also increase Drzal, Coley, and Chase-Lansdale (2004) indicate that
childrens aggression, including being born to a teenage the effect of group day care depends on the quality of
(Morash & Rucker, 1989) or single (Blum, Boyle, & Of- that care. Furthermore, the effect of group care must be
ford, 1988) parent, being raised in a large family (Rut- interpreted in light of the alternative type of care that is
ter, Tizard, & Whitmore, 1970), and being parented by available to a childthe quality of available home-rear-
convicted felons (Farrington, 1992). These factors are ing may differ across families such that group day care
intercorrelated and likely share a common pathway might offer a better or worse experience for a child than
through effects on parenting quality, although they may available alternatives.
have more direct effects or may be linked to child ag-
gression through common genes. Furthermore, these A Case of Pervasive Environmental Influence:
risk factors are apparently not merely redundant in their The Effects of Media Violence
impact on the developing child; rather, their effects are Perhaps no greater cultural influence on childrens ag-
cumulative (Rutter & Garmezy, 1983). gressive development can be found than the effects of
746 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

viewing violence on television. Meta-analyses (Wood, Perhaps even more threatening than passive viewing
Wong, & Chachere, 1991) indicate that television vio- of television violence is the active experience of playing
lence-viewing accounts for about 10% of the variance in violent video games. C. Anderson (2004) has reviewed
child aggression, which approximately equals the mag- the growing laboratory and longitudinal evidence to
nitude of effect of cigarette smoking on lung cancer. conclude that chronic experience playing video games
Laboratory experiments demonstrate that viewing that require and reward the shooting of victims on a
televised aggressive models results in aggressive behav- computer screen increases childrens future aggressive
ior toward Bobo dolls (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 1963) behavior, through many of the same psychological mech-
and peers (Bjrkqvist, 1985). Field studies repeatedly anisms that hold for viewing television violence.
demonstrate significant correlations between television-
violence viewing and aggressiveness, even when self-
selection factors, such as parental supervision and Processes in Early Family Socialization
socioeconomic status, are controlled. Eron, Huesmann,
Lefkowitz, and Walder (1972) found that boys televi- There is ample evidence of differences in discipline and
sion-violence preferences at age 8 predicted aggressive- parenting practices between families of aggressive chil-
ness at age 18. Follow-ups to age 30 showed that age 8 dren and nonproblem children. Much of the early re-
television violence predicted self-reported aggression search on this point was cross-sectional, however, and
and seriousness of criminal arrests, even when social there has long been recognition of the difficulty in sepa-
class, intelligence, parenting, age 8 aggression, and age rating cause and effect in these relations (Bell &
30 TV violence viewing all were controlled statistically Harper, 1977). K. Anderson, Lytton, and Romney
(Huesmann, 1986). Huesmann and Eron (1986) repli- (1986) demonstrated that parents of normal children
cated these findings in urban Israel, Poland, and the will resort to more punitive discipline practices when
United States but not in Australia and an Israeli kibbutz. confronted with conduct-disordered boys than when
More recently, Huesmann, Moise-Titus, Podolski, and they manage their own sons. Despite these problems of
Eron (2003) followed 450 6- to 10-year-old Chicagoan interpretation, longitudinal, behavior-genetic, and inter-
boys and girls for 15 years and found that childhood ex- vention studies show convincingly the causal role of
posure to television violence predicted a composite early family socialization.
adult aggression score that included self-ratings, ratings
Mother-Infant Attachment Relationships
by others, and crime records, even when early parent-
ing, parent aggression, and socioeconomic status were The results of longitudinal studies on infant attachment
controlled. and conduct problems in childhood are mixed. Bates
A major moderator of this effect is age of the viewer: et al. (1991) failed to establish insecure attachment as a
the effects hold more strongly for children than for predictor of externalizing problems in a predominantly
adults (Huesmann & Miller, 1994), perhaps because the middle-class, two-parent sample followed from infancy
effects act more strongly on the individuals develop- into elementary school. The same held for a similar study
ment of a repertoire than on the accessing of responses by Fagot and Kavanagh (1990) and a longitudinal investi-
already in ones repertoire. Also, the effects of viewing gation by Lewis, Feiring, McGuffog, and Jaskir (1984).
television violence are greater if the child believes that Alternatively, Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangels-
the violence is real, perhaps because perceived reality dorf, and Sroufe (1989) have repeatedly found insecure
increases salience and the encoding of scripts, and if the attachments to predict childhood behavior problems in a
child identifies with the violent TV characterbelieves sample from low-income and predominantly single-par-
that the character is like the self (Huesmann, Lager- ent households. Lyons-Ruth found that disorganized at-
spetz, & Eron, 1984). The family context in which TV tachment status predicted hostile behavior to peers at age
violence is viewed is another qualifier, in that children 5 (Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & Repacholi, 1993) and teacher-
who watch violence without parental supervision and in rated externalizing problems at age 7 (Lyons-Ruth, East-
home contexts in which harsh discipline is utilized are erbrooks, & Davidson, 1995). Finally, D. Shaw et al.
subject to greater influence by TV violence (Singer & (1995) found that insecure attachment, particularly dis-
Singer, 1981). organized attachment, predicted CBCL aggression
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 747

scores at age 5. This latter sample was predominantly berg, Pidada, & Liew, 2001) found support for a second
lower income, having been recruited from a federally possible mechanism, that parental negative emotion ex-
funded nutritional support program for mothers, infants, pression directly interferes with the childs normal de-
and children. Greenberg, Spelz, and Deklyen (1993) velopment of self-regulation and regulation of emotion,
have argued that secure attachment is a protective factor which, in turn, mediate the childs development of ex-
for infants of low-income, highly stressed mothers but is ternalizing problems (Eisenberg et al., 2003). Cole,
less crucial to antisocial development in middle-class Teti, and Zahn-Waxler (2003) found that maternal nega-
families. Dishion and Bullock (2002) proposed a general tive expressed emotion directly observed in laboratory
nurturance hypothesis, whereby parents positive at- tasks operates through dyadic exchanges with their pre-
tention, emotional investment, and behavioral manage- school boys, leading to conduct problems. Zhou et al.
ment combine in ways that protect otherwise high-risk (2002) found support for yet another pathway, that
children from becoming aggressive. parental warmth leads to the childs development of em-
Support for the interaction between parent-infant pathy, which is known to protect a child from aggres-
warmth and a biological factor comes from Raine, Bren- sive behavior.
nan, and Mednick (1997), who found that Danish males Pettit, Bates, and Dodge (1997) have introduced the
with a history of birth complications and early rejection concept of proactive teaching by parents to indicate
by the mother (unwanted pregnancy, attempt to abort their positive attempts to teach their child appropriate
fetus, and public institutional care of the infant) were at behavior to prevent later discipline or conduct problems.
high risk for violent crime by age 19. Of children who They found that this construct is orthogonal to warmth
had both risk factors, 47% became violent, compared to and independently predicts child conduct problems.
20% of those who had just one factor. Thus, the strength
of the mother-child bond protected children from later Family Coercion and Inconsistent Discipline
violence, but only for high biological-risk children. In their classic longitudinal study of delinquency,
Glueck and Glueck (1950) reported that parents of boys
Parental Warmth and Proactive Teaching who became delinquent were less consistent in their dis-
Closely related to the attachment construct is the con- cipline practices than parents of matched control boys
cept of maternal warmth. Bates and Bayles (1988) found who did not become delinquent. In an experimental fol-
maternal affection to be negatively related to external- low-up of this hypothesis, Parke and Deur (1972)
izing problems in both boys and girls at ages 5 and 6, demonstrated that children are less inhibited from be-
and Booth, Rose-Krasnor, McKinnon, and Rubin (1994) having aggressively when an adult is inconsistent in ad-
found maternal warmth at age 4 negatively related to ex- ministering punishment. The same point has been made
ternalizing problems at age 8. As with many other par- about inconsistency between adults when one adult en-
enting factors, the problem of selection bias and forces a standard, but the other does not (Sawin &
alternate causal paths calls into question whether Parke, 1979).
parental lack of warmth causes child conduct problems Patterson (Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2000) has of-
or the reverse. Caspi et al. (2004) used a monozygotic fered a theory of coercive social learning that goes well
twin study that controls for genetic differences to find beyond inconsistency in parenting as a core feature of
that maternal expressed emotion (i.e., verbal statements antisocial development. According to this functionalist
of negative affect about a child) predicted childrens perspective, children who begin to display antisocial be-
antisocial behavior problems. Deater-Deckard (2000) haviors, such as aggression and disruptiveness, in the
used identical and fraternal twin pairs to reach the same early school years have been inadvertently trained in the
conclusion. effectiveness of these behaviors by parents. Snyder,
One social-learning explanation for the role of Reid, and Patterson (2003) describe coercion training as
parental warmth is that for a parent to be effective in a four-step process that begins with the aversive intru-
socializing a child to parental behavior standards, the sion of a family member into the childs activities (e.g.,
parent must be seen by the child as a potential source of a mother may scold her child for not going to bed). In the
reward, which occurs through the exchange of warmth. second step, the child counterattacks (e.g., by whining,
Eisenberg (Eisenberg, Cumberland, et al., 2001; Eisen- yelling, and complaining that he or she is being picked
748 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

as the only one being made to go to bed). The third step Study indicated that fathers use of physical punishment
is the crucial one, for it involves the negative reinforce- predicted their sons adult criminal records, even when
ment that increases the likelihood of future aversive re- paternal criminality was controlled.
sponding by the child: The adult stops her scolding and Although robust, this correlation is moderated by
her demands for compliance. The fourth step is rein- other factors such as the quality of the parent-child rela-
forcement of the mothers giving in to the child. When tionship and the degree of parent-child warmth (Camp-
the mother ceases her demands, the child stops the coun- bell, 1990). The adverse effects of physical punishment
terattack. According to this theory, it is the conditional seem to hold only when punishment is administered in
probabilities in this sequence that distinguish the early the absence of warmth and caring guidance. Deater-
parenting patterns of antisocial children from those of Deckard and Dodge (1997) reported that harsh physical
normal children. Observational data from clinical sam- discipline was positively correlated with later external-
ples (Patterson et al., 1992) indicate that a childs aver- izing problems only among the subset of children who
sive responses to the mothers intrusions in the second scored below the median in parent-child warmth. Thus,
step occur two to three times more often in distressed a warm parent-child relationship might buffer a child
families than in normal families. The success of these from deleterious effects of physical punishment.
child counterattacks is reflected in observations docu- The cultural context of parenting also moderates the
menting that mothers of aggressive boys more frequently impact of physical punishment. Deater-Deckard, Dodge,
reinforce negatively their sons aversive responses than Bates, and Pettit (1996) found that physical punishment
do mothers of nonaggressive boys (Snyder & Patterson, was positively correlated with later child aggressive be-
1995). Conversely, mothers of nonaggressive boys more havior among European American families, in which
often reinforce the prosocial responses (e.g., talking or this discipline style occurred relatively rarely. Among
positive nonverbal communication) of their sons than do African American families, corporal punishment was
the mothers of aggressive boys. The net result is that more common (and normative) and was not correlated
children in normal families can be successful with their with child aggressive outcomes. This finding has been
parents in two ways (aversive and nonaversive behavior), replicated by Lansford et al. (2004), who extended it to
whereas the aggressive boys succeed only with aversive other measures of parenting (e.g., including direct ob-
behavior. Furthermore, the coercion training process is servations, responses to hypothetical vignettes, rating
a reciprocal family dynamic. scales, and clinical judgments) and later periods in a
childs life. The authors hypothesized that it is not
Physical Punishment and Punitiveness physical punishment per se that is responsible for child
The role of physical punishment in promoting or reduc- outcomes. Rather, it is the message that the child re-
ing childrens aggressive and antisocial behavior has ceives during the discipline event. Among European
long been a matter of dispute among professionals and American families in which harsh discipline is nonnor-
laypersons (Straus, 2005). The practice of spanking mative, for a child who receives harsh discipline, the
children is almost ubiquitous in American culture: 94% message may be that the parent is rejecting the child.
of parents of 3- and 4-year-olds use spanking as a disci- Among African American families, for whom corporal
pline technique (Straus & Stewart, 1999). Gershoff s punishment is normative and good parenting, the
(2002) meta-analysis has revealed a consistent correla- message may be that the parent cares about the childs
tion between corporal punishment and child aggressive development. These different messages yield different
behavior, although the interpretation of that correlation outcomes, even though the parenting behavior superfi-
is still in doubt (Benjet & Kazdin, 2003). Longitudinal cially appears the same.
investigations have consistently supported the relation Disentangling the effects of parenting from the be-
between early punishment and later antisocial behavior. havior of the child that might lead to corporal punish-
Data from the 411 London males of the Cambridge Lon- ment is yet another problem. P. Cohen and Brook (1995)
gitudinal Study (Farrington & Hawkins, 1991) point to used cross-lagged analyses of longitudinal data to con-
harsh discipline practices at age 8 as an important pre- clude that physical punishment more strongly predicts
dictor of the early onset of delinquency. McCords growth in conduct problems than the reverse. Stronger
(1991) analyses of the Combridge-Somerville Youth evidence regarding the causal role of harsh parenting
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 749

comes from intervention experiments. Hinshaw et al. At ages 5 and 7, mothers and teachers scores on the
(2000) randomly assigned families of children with Child Behavior Checklist were .8 standard deviations
ADHD to behavioral or medical treatments. The behav- higher for abused than nonabused children, controlling
ioral treatment taught parents to engage in less nega- for genetic and other factors. Furthermore, Jaffee et al.
tive/ineffective discipline strategies. They found that (2004) found evidence for genetically mediated child ef-
this treatment led to fewer disruptive behavior problems fects on corporal punishment, but not on physical mal-
and measures of negative discipline at the end of treat- treatment, suggesting that effects of maltreatment
ment mediated the effect of assignment to treatment on cannot be attributed to genes or child effects.
those outcomes. As research on the effects of child abuse has ex-
panded, it has become important to distinguish among
Abusive Parenting several forms of maltreatment (Cicchetti, 1989). Physi-
The distinction between the use of physical punishment cal abuse has clear effects on subsequent child aggres-
and physically abusive parenting is not simply one of de- sive behavior, whereas sexual abuse and neglect have
gree. With abuse comes out-of-control, emotionally different negative consequences. Fagot, Hagan, Young-
volatile, and nonnormative actions by a caregiver, which blade, and Potter (1989) found important early child-
appear to have devastating effects on at least some chil- hood effects in the observed social behavior of
dren. Numerous studies have identified the experience preschool children (ages 3 to 5) who had been sexually
of physical abuse as one of the most important parenting abused or were victims of physical abuse and neglect.
factors in antisocial development. Luntz and Widom The physically abused children were more aggressive,
(1994) found long-term effects of child abuse on anti- less communicative, and more passive than either sexu-
social behavior in a 20-year follow-up of children who ally abused or control children, whereas the sexually
had been reported as abused or neglected prior to age abused children were simply more quiet and unrespon-
11. Compared with control children matched for age, sive to peers. The physically abused children were more
race, sex, and family socioeconomic status, the abused apt to respond to conflicts with aggressive behavior than
sample had twice the probability of being diagnosed as other children. Sternberg et al. (1993) contrasted 8- to
having an adult antisocial personality disorder. One of 12-year-old children who had been physically abused by
the problems with studies using children who have been parents in the previous 6 months with others who had
identified as abused by child protective services (CPS) witnessed spousal violence but not been abused them-
is that the experience of abuse is confounded with the selves, or had witnessed violence and been abused. Data
actions taken by CPS, including being removed from the on abuse and violence for this Israeli sample came from
home, publicly labeled as abused, and aggregated with social workers and the outcome data involved self-
deviant children in foster and group-home settings. reports or mothers ratings of internalizing or external-
Dodge, Bates, and Pettit (1990) assessed physical mal- izing problems. All 3 vulnerable groups showed more
treatment in a community sample of preschool children self-reported depression than a demographically
through extended clinical interviews and then followed matched control group, but only the 2 groups that had
this sample across childhood. They found short-term ef- been physically abused reported more externalizing
fects of maltreatment on aggressive behavior in kinder- problems. Mothers, however, reported more externaliz-
garten and long-term effects through late adolescence ing problems in the two groups who had witnessed do-
that included school suspensions and physical violence mestic violence.
(Lansford et al., 2002). The effects of physical abuse, neglect, and sexual
Given the possibilities that children with particular abuse also were contrasted in a summer camp study of
characteristics might elicit abuse and that common ge- 235 children 5- to 11-years-old that included a nonmal-
netic characteristics between parents and child might treated matched control group (Manley, Cicchetti, &
account for the correlation between abusive parenting Barnett, 1994). The subgroups differed on counselor
and child aggression, Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, and Taylor ratings of social competence and behavior problems, but
(2004b) studied 1,116 twin pairs in Great Britain and did not differ on peer ratings of disruptiveness or ag-
found that physical maltreatment plays a strong causal gressiveness. The sexually abused group was rated by
role in the development of childrens antisocial behavior. counselors as more socially competent and having fewer
750 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

behavior problems than the physically abused or the ne- sions were likely to be rejected by their new peers in so-
glected groups, and even somewhat more competent ciometric nominations by the fourth session of play. In a
than the nonmaltreated group. The physically abused third study, Dodge, Coie, Pettit, and Price (1990) ob-
group had more behavior problems than the nonmal- served groups of 5 or 6 unfamiliar boys at two grade lev-
treated group, with the other two maltreated groups els (first and third) for five consecutive play sessions.
falling between these extreme groups. Negative peer status determined at the end of the ses-
sions was associated with both angry reactive aggres-
sion and instrumental aggression. Bullying led to
Childhood Peer Factors
disliking only in the third grade groups, suggesting that
The peer social context exerts yet another influence on bullying becomes socially unacceptable by this age.
the childs behavioral development. Kellam and Rebok Three qualifying points need to be made about the re-
(1992) have found that kindergarten classrooms natu- lation between childhood aggression and rejection by
rally vary in the incidence of peer-nominated aggression peers. First, not all aggressive acts are viewed with dis-
(e.g., in one school, classrooms ranged from 33% to approval by peers. Aggression in response to direct
85% in the proportion of children rated as aggressive), provocation actually is evaluated positively by peers.
even when assignment to classrooms is random. Clearly, Children who are seen as standing up for themselves are
unidentified factors in the ecology of the classroom con- generally well-liked (Lancelotta & Vaughn, 1989). Sec-
text account for these differences in aggressive behavior ond, not all aggressive children are socially rejected by
problems. In turn, the ratio of peers who are aggressive peers. In fact, only about half of the children named by
influences a childs growing tendency to become ag- classmates as being highly aggressive are rejected by
gressive and to value aggression (Stormshak et al., them. Leadership and other social skills may enable
1999), and these influences last across several years of some aggressive children to avoid being rejected (Ku-
elementary school (Kellam, Ling, Merisca, Brown, & persmidt & Dodge, 2004). Bierman (1986) found differ-
Ialongo, 1998). ences in the social behavior of rejected versus
When young aggressive children enter school, they are nonrejected aggressive boys that account for the differ-
likely not only to fail academically but also socially, and ent evaluations by peers. For example, Bierman, Smoot,
these two kinds of failure can interact to accelerate the and Aumiller (1993) found that boys rated by peers as
growth of aggressive behavior. There is substantial evi- both aggressive and rejected were more argumentative,
dence that aggressive children are likely to be rejected more disruptive, and less socially attentive than boys
by their peers (Kupersmidt & Dodge, 2004). Although who were aggressive but not rejected.
most of the studies have been correlational, making it Finally, the normativeness and cultural context of ag-
difficult to determine whether aggression leads to rejec- gressive behavior moderate the relation between rejec-
tion or rejection to aggression, several laboratory play- tion and aggression. Lancelotta and Vaughn (1989)
group studies demonstrate that early aggression leads to found the correlation between aggression and peer re-
later rejection among new, unfamiliar peers. jection to be much stronger among girls (r = .73) than
Coie and Kupersmidt (1983) assembled groups of among boys (r = .37). The fact that aggression is gener-
previously unacquainted fourth grade boys of known so- ally more normative for boys than girls suggests that de-
ciometric status, one of whom was rejected, and ob- viation from norms is likely to result in rejection. Social
served them at play for 6 weekly sessions. By the 3rd contextual norms play a significant role in the factors
week, most of the rejected boys were rejected by their leading to peer rejection. Wright, Giammarino, and
new group mates. Verbal and physical aggression were Parad (1986) demonstrated the importance of social
most characteristic of the rejected boys, and in ratings context in a study of groups of cabin mates in a summer
after the last session, there was consensus among group program for behaviorally disturbed boys. Counselor-re-
members that the rejected boys most often started ported aggression was negatively correlated with peer
fights. In a second study, Dodge (1983) formed groups status in the groups characterized by low levels of ag-
of 8 previously unacquainted second grade boys who gression, but aggression was uncorrelated with status in
were not selected for social status and observed them for the high-aggression groups. Cillessen and Mayeux
eight sessions. Boys who were observed to make hostile (2004) followed 905 children from ages 10 to 14, a pe-
comments and hit other boys most often in initial ses- riod when aggressive behavior grows in normativeness.
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 751

They found that aggression was increasingly less dis- parental monitoring. Larzelere and Patterson (1990)
liked as children got older, consistent with the hypothe- used structural equation modeling to find that the link-
sis that when children behave in accordance with group age between family socioeconomic status, as measured
norms they are not disliked. when the boys were in fourth grade, and delinquency,
Findings consistent with a general person-group sim- measured in seventh grade from police records and self-
ilarity model positing that deviancy is rejected only if report, is mediated by parent management practices
different from group norms have been found in play- measured in sixth grade. Parental management included
groups by Boivin, Dodge, and Coie (1995) and in class- discipline practices such as consistency, control of
rooms by Stormshak et al. (1999). This model also parental anger during discipline, and negativity in inter-
partially explains the developmental differences in the actions reflecting aversive comments over trivial inci-
correlation between aggression and popularity: as ag- dents, and parental monitoring of the childs activities
gression becomes more normative in early adolescence, and associations.
its association with social rejection dissipates. The ef- Monitoring is a critical aspect of parenting during
fectiveness and status of bullies also varies with the cul- adolescence (Dishion & McMahon, 1998). Parental
tural context in which bullying occurs. Kim, Koh, and monitoring is particularly important in preventing ado-
Leventhal (2004) found high (40%) prevalence rates for lescent involvement with deviant peers. Snyder, Dishion,
bullying in a sample of 1,756 Korean middle school stu- and Patterson (1986) reported strong path relations be-
dents and a positive correlation between bullying and tween low levels of parental monitoring and increases in
high socioeconomic status. In that context, bullying is deviant peer associations in tenth grade. In their study
common among the successful upper class. of 169 adolescent boys living in small, midwestern
towns, Simons, Wu, Conger, and Lorenz (1994) found
Consequences of Peer Rejection
that problems in parental discipline practices (including
The reason for giving special attention to the peer status poor monitoring, harsh discipline, lack of consistency,
consequences of aggressive behavior is that peer rejec- and ill-defined standards) in seventh grade predicted in-
tion appears to contribute to subsequent problems of creased deviant peer associations and police arrests and
adaptation, including increased antisocial behavior sanctions in ninth grade.
(Haselager et al., 2002). Ladd (1990) found that rejec- Not surprisingly, parental monitoring is more impor-
tion by peers in the fall of the kindergarten year pre- tant in some circumstances and with some children than
dicted declining academic adjustment across the school others. Pettit, Bates, Dodge, and Meece (1999) found
year. Dodge et al. (2003) found that rejection increases that the positive effects of parental monitoring on keep-
aggressive behavior, especially among children who are ing antisocial behavior in check were stronger for fami-
aggressive initially. They found that peer rejection in lies living in dangerous neighborhoods than in safe
early school years (grades K, 1, and 2) predicted in- neighborhoods and for children with previous histories
creased aggressive behavior in third grade, even when of aggressive behavior than nonaggressive children.
previous aggression was controlled statistically. Similar Thus, youth who are at lower risk do not need, or benefit
findings have been reported by Patterson and Bank from, close monitoring as much as high-risk youth. This
(1989), Kupersmidt and Coie (1990), Bierman and difference helps explain why parents of low-risk youth
Wargo (1995), and Coie (2004). (and legislators who fund programs) are sometimes un-
able to grasp the importance of parent-training and su-
Adolescent Family Processes pervised after-school programs for high-risk youth.
Although it is easy to warn parents about the impor-
Just as early parenting practices contribute to peer rela- tance of monitoring their adolescents, several studies
tions problems that exacerbate antisocial behavior dur- have demonstrated that it is difficult to monitor certain
ing the elementary school years, so, too, parents of youth, especially high-risk antisocial youth who begin
adolescents are influenced by their aggressive children to engage in covert activities and learn to hide their de-
but also reciprocally contribute to growth in antisocial viance from their parents (Crouter & Head, 2002). Fur-
behavior during adolescence. Two aspects of parenting thermore, parent-child conflict is extremely stressful for
appear to be critical to controlling child antisocial activ- all parties involved, and monitoring can heighten con-
ity in early adolescence: discipline practices and flict when the parent confronts the youth with evidence
752 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

of misbehavior. Not surprisingly, early conduct prob- Following from Hirschis (1969) social control theory
lems lead to lower levels of parental monitoring in ado- of antisocial behavior in which a close bond between
lescence (Stattin & Kerr, 2000). Stattin and Kerr (2000) parent and adolescent is thought to function as a control
suggested that monitoring is more a consequence than a on antisocial activity, Hawkins and Lishner (1987) re-
cause of child antisocial behavior, but other studies have viewed studies that supported the hypothesis that posi-
shown that it increases adolescent delinquency beyond tive parent-child bonding in adolescence is negatively
the levels that led to poor monitoring (Laird, Pettit, related to adolescent problem behavior. Dishion, French,
Bates, & Dodge, 2003). Overall, levels of parental mon- and Patterson (1995) have challenged the bonding hy-
itoring decrease across adolescence, as parents recog- pothesis on the grounds that these studies fail to control
nize adolescents desires for greater autonomy and for previous problem-behavior levels and confound
growing maturity to act responsibly on their own (Laird bonding with measures of parental monitoring. They
et al., 2003). cited a multiagent and multimethod measurement study
Dishion, Nelson, and Bullock (2004) have provided that indicates that there is considerable overlap among
important observational evidence regarding the pattern these measures of parenting practices. Thus, they argued
of parental disengagement with aggressive boys. They that parent-child bonding is an outcome of previous con-
videorecorded families of aggressive and nonaggressive flicts and does not cause future problems; in contrast,
boys during interaction tasks in the laboratory and de- parental failure to monitor a child directly increases as-
tected patterns of disengagement by parents of aggres- sociations with deviant peers and antisocial behavior.
sive boys. These patterns predicted growth in An important validation of the causal role of parent-
associations with deviant peers and delinquent out- ing practices on adolescent antisocial activity comes
comes. Ironically and unfortunately, it is the very fami- from interventions designed to change these practices
lies for which monitoring and engagement should be and reduce antisocial activity. Patterson, Chamberlain,
increased that it decreases most rapidly, signaling the and Reid (1982) evaluated the effects of parent training
growing gap between parent and aggressive youth as that used the coercion model as a basis for altering par-
antisocial behavior becomes more serious. ent behavior and found significant reductions in ob-
The constructs of parental monitoring and engage- served deviant behavior compared with those youth
ment turn out to include several distinct parenting pat- randomly assigned to community practitioners. In a
terns, each of which has been related to growth in stronger test of the model, Dishion, Patterson and Ka-
antisocial behavior. Crouter and Head (2002) distin- vanagh (1991) randomly assigned parents of preadoles-
guished between parental monitoring, which involves cents at risk for substance abuse to training in
surveillance of child activities and whereabouts, and contingency management techniques and found signifi-
knowledge of ones child, which involves seeking infor- cant reductions in teacher ratings of antisocial behavior
mation from teachers and the child. Low levels of both compared with youth whose families were assigned to
constructs have been correlated with past and future placebo control conditions. Of greatest significance for
child antisocial behavior. These constructs, and disci- the validation of the causal role of parenting to antisocial
pline practices, are distinguished from a third construct, behavior was the fact that improvements in behavior cor-
psychological control, which involves parents coercive related significantly, controlling for baseline behavior,
attempts to use guilt and manipulation to control a with improvements in observed discipline practices.
youths whereabouts and activities (Barber, 1996). Pet-
tit, Laird, Bates, Dodge, and Criss (2001) found that
Adolescent Peer Processes
psychological control strategies grow out of a parents
relationship with a difficult child but, in turn, exacer- Whereas in earlier years when the major influence that
bate antisocial development and estrangement between peers had on antisocial development was to include or
the parent and child. Finally, Shumow and Lomax exclude a child from social acceptance, during adoles-
(2002), in a way that is consistent with Baumrinds cence the chief peer effect comes from the influence of
(1987) construct of authoritative parenting, have sug- particular kinds of peer groups. Bagwell, Coie, Terry,
gested that all of these parenting practices are mediated and Lochman (2000) have developed a method to iden-
and organized by a parents sense of efficacy in parent- tify peer cliques based on a consensus of group mem-
ing, and it is the communication of this sense of efficacy bers and have found that in preadolescence children
that leads to positive outcomes for the child. organize themselves into different peer cliques that have
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 753

distinctive features. Aggressive behavior is the primary dinal data on delinquency and substance use from the
factor associated with being a central member of deviant NYS point to the incremental predictive role of involve-
peer cliques. Deviant peer cliques offer both a home to ment with deviant peers. In support of this conclusion,
attract like-minded antisocial youth (called homophily) Patterson, Reid, et al. (1992) found that involvement
and an opportunity to expand the range and severity of with deviant peers in grade six predicted subsequent
antisocial behaviors. The question of social selection delinquency even controlling for prior antisocial behav-
versus social influence again looms as a methodological ior. The deviant peer association construct in this study
challenge, but current evidence supports both effects. was formed from parent, peer, and self descriptions of
The deviant peer group both assembles antisocial youth children who hang around with peers who get into
and contributes to their antisocial growth. Furthermore, trouble. Simons et al. (1994) also found that association
evidence has begun to accumulate that systematic ef- with deviant peers predicted subsequent arrests.
forts to intervene with antisocial adolescents, which Keenan, Loeber, Zhang, Stouthamer-Loeber, and van
often involve aggregating deviant youth for group inter- Kammen (1995) examined the effects of deviant peer as-
ventions, may have iatrogenic effects. This section is or- sociations on the onset of disruptive behaviors in two
ganized around the topics of deviant peer group effects, cohorts of the Pittsburgh Youth Study. The onset of
gangs, mechanisms of peer influence in adolescence, delinquency was assessed at 6-month intervals for five
and iatrogenic effects of interventions. successive waves in data collection beginning in fourth
grade for one cohort, or in seventh grade for the other.
Deviant Peer Influences Authority conflict was twice as likely to occur among
As noted above, parenting practices in early adolescence those disruptive boys who had truant or disobedient best
can contribute to the disengagement between parent and friends as among those who did not, and the odds ratios
youth that contributes to gravitation toward deviant for the correlation with covert and overt antisocial be-
friends and peer groups. Undoubtedly, normal develop- havior were 4.3 and 3.4, respectively. The odds ratios
mental processes of autonomy-seeking during this era for association with deviant peers on predicting disrup-
also contribute to the tendency to seek highly sensa- tive behavior in the following year were 2.2 and 1.9 for
tional stimuli and to gravitate toward deviant peers. covert and overt antisocial behaviors, respectively. Two
Highly visible antisocial peers come to be viewed posi- important features of this study were the fact that onset
tively by a large segment of the adolescent population. was the dependent variable, thus controlling for previous
Rodkin et al. (2000) identified a subgroup of popular disruptive behavior, and that peer influence was tested
adolescents who are aggressive, cool, and athletic. Cil- in a specific type of antisocial activity as a way of ex-
lessen and Mayeux (2004) have documented a general amining the extent to which peers truly were models of
developmental trend for children to move from censur- deviant behavior.
ing aggressive peers during elementary school to giving In a contrasting study, Tremblay, Msse, Vitaro, and
those peers high social status in early adolescence. They Dobkin (1995) failed to find peer influence on delin-
also note that this trend is moderated by gender, ethnic- quency in a longitudinal study of preadolescents. Mutual
ity, and the ethnic mix of the peer group, so that the fea- best friends were identified for 758 of the 1,034 French-
tures of popular adolescents depend heavily on the Canadian boys aged 10 to 12. Self-reported overt and
idiosyncratic culture of the peer setting. covert delinquency assessed 1 year later were predicted
In turn, deviant peers come to influence other adoles- by the participants earlier peer-rated aggressiveness,
cents in a deviant direction, especially when those peers but the peer-rated aggressiveness of the best friends did
are central to ones peer clique or have stable friend- not add to the prediction of either covert or overt delin-
ships with a child. Berndt, Hawkins, and Jiao (1999) quency. The delinquency of peers in this study was esti-
found that children who had a stable friendship with a mated by aggressive reputation and peer influence was
deviant peer were at increased risk for growing in their restricted to a single best friend rather than a group of
own deviant behavior as they made the transition from friends; however, it is possible that peer influence may
elementary to junior high school. D. Elliott, Huizinga, be greater at older ages than those studied here. In a fol-
and Ageton (1985) suggested that the modeling and rein- low-up analysis of the same sample, Vitaro et al. (1999)
forcement required to produce stronger and more diverse divided the boys according to teacher ratings of their
antisocial behavior does not usually come from the fam- disruptiveness at ages 11 to 12. When the impact of hav-
ily but resides in the deviant peer context. Their longitu- ing aggressive-disruptive friends was analyzed by the
754 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

boys prior history of conduct problems, Vitaro et al. interactions among aggressive youth suggests both posi-
(1999) found that it was the moderately disruptive boys tive friendship features like those of nondeviant friends
whose delinquent activity at age 13 showed the negative and aversive qualities that are lacking in the relationship
consequences of deviant friends influence, in contrast of nondeviant friends. In comparing delinquent and non-
to highly disruptive or socially conforming boys. Thus, delinquent 12- to 19-year-olds, Giordano, Cernkovich,
it may be the marginally deviant youth who are most and Pugh (1986) found more self-reported conflict be-
susceptible to the influence of deviant peers and are tween delinquent friends but also more willingness to
damaged most by placement in highly deviant peer set- confide and equivalent amounts of interaction. Gill-
tings (Caprara & Zimbardo, 1996). more, Hawkins, Day, and Catalano (1992) interviewed
One limitation in these studies is that direct measures preadolescent youth about their friends and nonfriends
of group affiliation were not used. A problem with rely- and found that those who described themselves as fre-
ing on adult or peer estimates of deviant peer influence quently getting into trouble were more attached to their
is that these indices are highly correlated (r = .89) with conventional friends than to their friends who also get
ratings of the target individuals own aggressive behav- into trouble. These sentiments about friendship mirror
ior (Bagwell et al., 2000), making it difficult to partial Dishion, Patterson, and Grieslers (1994) conclusion
out the influence of the targets aggression from esti- that many antisocial friendships are relationships of
mates of deviant peer influence. Cairns and his associ- convenience and not necessarily the preferred choices of
ates utilized a more complex approach that was less these youth. Many of the chronically antisocial youth in
susceptible to stereotyping on the basis of reputation. the Oregon Youth Study Sample (Dishion et al., 1994)
They used peer informants to identify the peer clique were rejected at age 10 and age 14, and their friendships
structure for a school-based peer cohort and then used were of relatively short duration. Thus, part of the dy-
behavioral characteristics of clique members as an index namics of peer associations for highly antisocial youth
of clique deviance. Using this approach, they demon- may be that they have limited conventional friendship
strated that aggressive youth not only tend to associate opportunities. Hawkins and Weis (1985) have argued
with other aggressive youth (Cairns, Cairns, Necker- that delinquency often results from a lack of social
man, Gest, & Gariepy, 1988) but that deviant peer asso- bonding to the conventional elements of society. The ab-
ciations influence dropping out of school (Cairns, sence of these bonds leads to a lessened influence of
Cairns, & Neckerman, 1989). conventional rules for behavior, and this contributes to
The relation between association with other deviant greater antisocial activity.
youth and delinquent activity is well-established in the The reciprocal influence of deviant peer associations
literature on gangs. Thornberry, Krohn, Lizotte, and and delinquent activity is illustrated in longitudinal find-
Chard-Wierschem (1993) found that gang members ac- ings from the Rochester Youth Developmental Study.
celerated their illegal activity during the time they were Thornberry, Krohn, Lizotte, and Chard-Wierschem
associated with their gang and decelerated this activity (1993) found that associating with deviant peers leads to
when they left the gang and were not enmeshed in the increased delinquency, partly through the formation and
gang environment. reinforcement of beliefs that it is not wrong to commit
Taken together, these findings suggest two important delinquent acts. Across time, adolescents who commit
conclusions about deviant peer associations and anti- delinquent acts are more likely to associate with peers
social behavior. Friendship activities between antisocial who are also delinquent. Their analyses suggested that
youth serve to promote greater deviant behavior, even bidirectional relationships are necessary to account for
though, or perhaps because, the interaction quality of longitudinal changes in delinquency, supporting the idea
the relationship is abrasive. Second, when youth are in a that a dynamic social developmental process is involved.
period of antisocial activity, they tend to associate with
Delinquent Gangs
other antisocial youth, a phenomenon that Kandel (1978)
referred to as homophily ( like seeking like), but when The phenomenon of delinquent gangs, particularly in the
they are no longer engaged in antisocial activity, they no United States, is an important contemporary manifesta-
longer associate with deviant peers. tion of deviant peer groups (Klein, in press). As Thorn-
Although the term homophily suggests a mutual at- berry, Krohn, Lizotte, Smith, and Tobin (2003) have
traction between antisocial adolescents, the quality of noted, gangs have changed across the past 50 years, as
Determinants of Individual Dif ferences in Antisocial Behavior 755

have explanations for their existence, one of the most boys tended to come from the same neighborhood and to
salient changes being the increased involvement with il- provide less satisfying friendships, suggesting that these
legal drug sales. Spergel, Ross, Curry, and Chance were friendships of convenience. The behaviors of
(1989, as cited in Goldstein, 1994) have found gangs in friends were highly correlated, with antisocial dyads
all 50 states, with most police and other informants in- having more negative qualities and more noxious behav-
dicating that gang participation and activity were on the ior. When highly antisocial dyads (i.e., both had prior
increase. Males outnumbered females in gang member- police contact and high antisocial ratings) were com-
ship by 20 to 1 and gang crime rates by gender mirrored pared to low antisocial dyads, the high antisocial dyads
this ratio. The age range of gang members appear to contingently reciprocated more negative behavior.
have extended in both directions across time. Goldstein In another report of the same set of friendship inter-
concluded that the reasons youths join gangs are: peer action data, Dishion et al. (1994) found that antisocial
friendship, pride, identity development, self esteem en- dyads engaged in more delinquent talk would tend to re-
hancement, excitement, the acquisition of resources, inforce delinquent behavior, than low antisocial dyads.
and in response to family and community tradition. In this latter paper, longitudinal data on antisocial ac-
These goals are often not available to young people tivity and deviant peer associations at ages 10 and 14
through legitimate means in the disorganized and low- suggested a synchrony between the two variables.
income environments from which most gang youth de- Dishion (in press) has proposed a general ecological
rive (p. 261). model of the mechanisms of deviant peer influence that
Even though adolescents join gangs for identity and locates the adolescent in a deviant peer group, which, in
friendship, one effect of gang involvement, like involve- turn, is located in the larger community. This model
ment with deviant peers in general, is increased anti- posits mechanisms at all levels, beginning with intraper-
social activity. Spergel et al. (1989) found that sonal effects of association with deviant peers on cogni-
individuals in a gang are 3 times more likely to engage in tive processes by the self and the observing world.
violent offenses than are those persons not affiliated Labeling oneself (and of one by others) that comes from
with a gang. Again, issues of selection bias threaten mere proximity to a group that is characterized by ag-
ones confidence in the conclusion of a causal role for gressive behavior may yield self-fulfilling prophecies.
gang membership. However, Thornberry et al. (1993, Bernberg (2002) found that official processing in juve-
2003) used longitudinal analyses to show that becoming nile courts systems has adverse effects on subsequent
involved in a gang increases an adolescents likelihood criminal offending once controlling for prior offending,
of violently offending and that leaving a gang leads to which he attributed to the labeling that comes from the
decreases in the likelihood of violently offending. Even official record. Once the stereotype of a deviant type
more convincingly, Gordon et al. (2004) used fixed- has been established, cognitive processes of stereotype
effects models that control for selection to conclude that threat (Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002) may operate
both selection and socialization effects of gang mem- to disrupt a deviant adolescents behavior during critical
bership occur among the 858 participants in the 10-year situations, thereby exacerbating deviant behavior. Ka-
longitudinal Pittsburgh Youth Study. plan and Liu (1994) suggested that self-derogation and
helplessness occur when an adolescent is labeled by the
Mechanisms of Deviant Peer Influence
mainstream world as a member of a deviant group and
Since Buehler, Patterson, and Furniss (1966) found that that participating in further deviant behavior (in their
adolescent inmates in a correctional facility tended to empirical analyses, drug use and dropping out) restores
reinforce delinquent behaviors and punish behaviors self-esteem and control.
conforming to mainstream social norms, Dishion has ex- At the interpersonal level, modeling and positive re-
amined the interactions of antisocial youth and their inforcement of deviant verbal statements during conver-
friends to understand processes of deviant peer influ- sations (called deviancy training) occur in deviant peer
ence. As part of the 13- to 14-year-old assessment of the groups, leading to increased deviant statements by a
Oregon Youth Study, friends of the study participants youth and serving to mediate the link between early
(named by both the boys and their parents) were invited antisocial behavior and young adult problem behavior
to participate in a peer interaction task (Dishion, An- and adjustment problems (Patterson, Dishion, and
drews, & Crosby, 1995). Friends of the more antisocial Yoerger, 2000). A dynamical systems process has been
756 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

observed by Granic and Dishion (2002), in which, once delinquent behavior. Feldman, Caplinger, and Wodarski
ignited, nonlinear acceleration of deviant talk occurs (1983) assigned youth randomly to one of three treat-
during peer conversations and sometimes leads to a ment groups: one composed exclusively of referred de-
frenzy of deviant statements. Dishion (in press) suggests viant youth, one involving nonreferred youth only, and
that, what is rewarded is not the behavior but the over- one involving a mix of referred-deviant and nonreferred
all set of values, attitudes, and behaviors that accom- youth. Deviant children assigned to all-deviant groups
pany a delinquent lifestyle. A contagion effect results, had more adverse outcomes than those assigned to
in which deviant behavior proliferates. mixed groups. Lavallee, Bierman, Nix, and Conduct
Evidence for more specific social learning from de- Problems Prevention Research Group (2005) found that,
viant peers comes from analyses by Bayer, Pintoff, and although random assignment to social skill training
Pozen (2004), who analyzed data from 15,000 juveniles groups led to improvements in aggressive behavior over-
serving sentences in 169 Florida correctional facilities. all, the magnitude of this improvement was moderated
They used facility fixed effects to find that access to by characteristics of the peers with whom a child was
peers in prison who have histories of specific crimes aggregated. Children assigned to groups with higher lev-
(e.g., burglary, felony drug, or weapon-related) leads to els of peer aggressive behavior improved less.
facilitation of later crimes of that very same type. These In education, both tracking children based on aca-
effects are strongest for adolescents who have had ini- demic achievement (Kerckhoff, 1988) and assignment to
tial experience with that type of crime (suggesting a fa- classrooms with deviant peers for special education
cilitation effect rather than initial exposure effect) and (Peetsma, Vergeer, Roeleveld, & Karsten, 2001) are as-
who are exposed to older adolescents than younger ado- sociated with growth in antisocial behavior, including
lescents. These findings suggest that deviancy training school crimes. Arum and Beattie (1999) found that high
extends beyond verbal talk to serious crimes, through school suspensions, which typically involve temporary
processes of social learning. placement with other suspended students, are associated
with a 200% increase in the likelihood of adult incarcer-
Iatrogenic Effects of Deviant Peer Aggregation ation, even after controlling for related risk factors such
Growing evidence indicates that peer effects on adoles- as family characteristics, socioeconomic status, prior
cent antisocial behavior occur not only in naturally delinquency, and years of education.
formed peer groups but also in groups that are formed In corrections, randomized experiments with juvenile
by government and interventionists. The frightening delinquents have demonstrated that assignment to prison
possibility that well-intentioned interventions can have visitation and exposure to more deviant inmates (e.g.,
harmful iatrogenic effects was proposed by Dishion, the Scared Straight Program) leads to harmful effects
McCord, and Poulin (1999), with support from the do- that include later crimes (Petrosino, Turpin-Petrosino, &
mains of mental health, education, and juvenile justice. Finckenauer, 2000). In the most comprehensive meta-
In all of these domains, deviant adolescents are rou- analysis to date, Lipsey (in press) examined 396 inter-
tinely aggregated with each other for intervention pur- vention studies, contrasting group-aggregation versus
poses (e.g., through tracking and special education individually based programs for juvenile delinquency.
placement in education, group therapies and group resi- Studies of prevention programs that intervene with
dential homes in mental health, and incarceration and youth in groups show a general pattern of main effects
group placements such as boot camps in corrections). on subsequent offense rates that average about one-third
These effects provide strong evidence of deviant peer in- smaller than those of comparable programs that inter-
fluences because self-selection biases are eliminated vene with juveniles individually. Analysis of interac-
through institutional placements. tions showed that this effect is concentrated in programs
In mental health, Dishion and Andrews (1995) ran- that work with younger juveniles (e.g., age 15 and under)
domly assigned 119 high-risk boys and girls to one of and those that work with lower risk juveniles.
four treatment conditions: (1) parent focus only, (2) peer
focus only, (3) combined parent and peer focus, and (4) COGNITIVE-EMOTIONAL PROCESSES
control. Analyses revealed adverse effects for peer-fo- AS MEDIATORS
cused interventions at both the 1-year and 3-year follow-
up. Specifically, subjects in the peer-focus condition A large body of evidence from laboratory, longitudinal,
showed increases in tobacco use and teacher reports of and intervention-experiment studies has accumulated to
Cognitive-Emotional Processes as Mediators 757

support the hypotheses that (a) cognitive-emotional threats of punishment, exerts special effort to cause the
processes contribute to antisocial behavioral responding harm, or seems happy when causing the harm (Rule &
in specific situations, ( b) individual differences in cog- Ferguson, 1986). Children also learn when to attribute
nitive-emotional processes account for a significant pro- hostile intent and make moral judgments of a provoca-
portion of chronic individual differences in aggressive teur through socialization (Rule & Ferguson, 1986).
behavior, and (c) cognitive-emotional processes at least Berkowitz (1993) suggested that environmental condi-
partially mediate the effects of socialization on aggres- tions leading to pervasive negative affect (such as high
sive behavior outcomes. These processes include a vari- temperatures, unpleasant living conditions, and foul
ety of constructs from online processing of current odors) will also increase the likelihood of anger re-
social stimuli to latent knowledge structures in memory. sponses to current threats, even when the unpleasant en-
This review begins with a discussion of social-situa- vironmental condition is unrelated to the current threat.
tional factors that proximally influence cognitive-emo- He argues that the conscious ascription of blame to the
tional processes and then moves to a review of social provocateur is not as essential as the experience of dis-
information processing, followed by a discussion of la- pleasure during the time that the stimulus is presented.
tent mental structures that guide processing. It is only a short inferential leap to suggest that attri-
butions of hostile intent and experiences of anger in re-
sponse to current provocative stimuli become more
Social-Situational Factors That Alter likely when a child is growing up under circumstances
Cognitive-Emotional Processes of pervasive violence, harm, and deprivation, such as
when others regularly assault the child, when assaults
Several kinds of situations instigate aggressive behav- regularly occur toward the childs family, peers, and
ioral responses through their impact on various aspects ethnic group, and when peer groups and family also in-
of social information processing. These situations are terpret provocateurs as being hostile. Longitudinal evi-
reviewed next. dence is consistent with this hypothesis. Dodge, Price,
Bachorowski, & Newman (1990) found that a history of
Factors That Enhance the Attribution of Threat physical maltreatment in the first 5 years of life leads to
One of the most consistent findings from laboratory hostile attributional biases during elementary school.
studies is that provocation leads to retaliatory aggres- Dodge et al. (2003) found that chronic peer social rejec-
sion, even in young children (Ferguson & Rule, 1988). tion during early elementary school years leads to hos-
Frustration and goal-blocking have long been known to tile attributional biases during later elementary school.
induce anger under both experimental and natural condi- Aber, Gershoff, Ware, and Kotler (2004) found that wit-
tions (Berkowitz, 1962). Studies over the past 3 decades nessing family violence around the time of the Septem-
have indicated that the perception of provocation is far ber 11th disaster predicted childrens tendencies to
more important than the provocation itself in instigating display hostile attributional biases a year later. Cassidy,
aggression (Dodge, Murphy, & Buchsbaum, 1984). If a Kirsh, Scolton, and Parke (1996) found that an early his-
child who is pushed from behind in line at school or who tory of insecure attachment to ones mother and the ex-
receives an excessively severe punishment from a perience of parental rejection were both antecedents of
teacher interprets such an environmental threat as the tendency to display hostile attributional biases dur-
malevolently intended and foreseeable, that child is ing elementary school. Finally, MacBrayer, Milich, and
likely to retaliate aggressively (Dodge et al., 2003). Hundley (2003) found that modeling of hostile attribu-
Even kindergarten children can refrain from aggressing tional biases by mothers is correlated with a childs own
when they make a nonmalevolent interpretation of a neg- tendency to attribute hostility to others.
ative event (Shantz & Voydanoff, 1973). Environmental
factors that facilitate a hostile attribution include infor- Factors That Increase the Accessibility of
mation about the provocateur as acting consistently neg- Aggressive Responses
atively over time, distinctively negatively toward the A second mechanism leading to aggressive responding is
perceiver, and others consensually interpreting the the accessing of aggressive responses from memory dur-
provocateurs actions similarly (Kelley, 1973). Infer- ing interpersonal exchanges. Banduras classic studies
ences of hostile intent are more likely when the provoca- (1973, 1983) indicate that children can acquire entire
teur continues to harm the perceiver despite feedback or repertoires of novel aggressive behavior from observing
758 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

aggressive models and can retain such response patterns neighborhood in which the heroes are violent, interact-
over extended periods (Bandura, 1983, p. 6). Imitation ing with antisocial peers who present repeated chances
is an evolved characteristic that is present even among to act aggressively, and watching TV violence all repre-
neonates (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977). Aggressive models sent opportunities to learn aggressive scripts.
can be observed in a childs family members, commu-
nity subculture, or mass media. Social learning theory Priming of Aggressive Constructs
(Bandura, 1983) articulates four processes by which The extraction of aggressive scripts and specific aggres-
modeling can activate aggressive behavior: (1) directive sive responses from memory can occur either through
functions, through which an aggressive response enters active problem solving (Rubin & Krasnor, 1986) or non-
a childs repertoire because of the positive conse- conscious priming. Graham and Hudley (1994) demon-
quences for aggressing that are vicariously taught; (2) strated that priming the perception that others
disinhibitory functions, in which modeled aggression re- negative actions are intentional can lead to hostile attri-
duces fear of negative consequences; (3) emotional butional biases, which, in turn, have been related to ag-
arousal, in which the aggressive model facilitates ag- gressive behavioral responses.
gression by arousing observers; and (4) stimulus-en- Berkowitz (1993) suggested that certain stimuli in-
hancing effects, in which the objects used in aggression duce an associative network of internal responses that
receive heightened attention and provide instruction for he calls the anger/aggression syndrome, which include
the aggressive use of these objects. physiological reactions, motor tendencies, feelings,
Aggressive models teach more than just specific be- thoughts, and memories. Wyer and Srull (1989) have
haviors. They teach general strategies for acting; when suggested that these responses are activated through an
observers synthesize modeled behaviors into patterns association in memory between a stimulus and one as-
that follow regularized sequences, those patterns form pect of the response, which then spreads the activation
scripts that are laid down in memory (Huesmann, to the entire syndrome through secondary associations.
1998). Information about the category of aggression, its Priming effects are short-lived but powerful and auto-
elicitors, and its consequences constitutes aggressive matic (Bargh, Chaiken, Raymond, & Hymes, 1995).
schemas (Shank & Abelson, 1977), which guide atten- Wann and Branscombe (1990), for example, found that
tion and interpretation to future stimuli and can there- experimentally giving students the names of aggressive
fore enrich scripts for action. Through observation and persons in sports increased the likelihood that the stu-
experience, children develop an understanding of ag- dent would subsequently attribute hostile intent to an
gression that includes expectations for what produces ambiguous target person.
aggression, how aggression is performed, elements of Many factors influence the probability that a particu-
an aggression category (including emotions and ac- lar stimulus will prime aggressive responses. Aggressive
tions), and the likely consequences of this behavior script responses that are laid down in memory with
(Rule & Ferguson, 1986). Once having acquired an ag- great frequency, drama, and recency are likely to be at
gression script, when a child encounters difficult situa- the top of the storage bin and primed (Wyer & Srull,
tions, that script governs expectations and prescribes 1989); scripts that have multiple complex associations
behavioral strategies. Strongly developed scripts, ac- in memory are likely to be easily and chronically acces-
quired through observational learning, can lead to ag- sible (Bargh & Thein, 1985) because many stimuli can
gressive responding. instigate associations to the script. Thus, growing up in
Huesmann (1988) suggested that aggression scripts an environment in which violence is normative will in-
are developed through active attention to aggressive crease the accessibility of aggressive constructs in fu-
models. Aggressive behaviors can be acquired through ture situations. Certain persons who are nonreflective
repeated exposure to multiple stimulating aggressive about their own behavior or are in impaired pathological
acts in diverse contexts displayed by models who are states (Bargh, 1989) are ripe for priming effects.
both heroes and identification figures for the observer, Stressful environments may render children vulnerable
followed by opportunities to practice aggressing with to aggressive priming. Finally, situational cues that are
impunity so that complex scripts for aggressing become self-relevant (Strauman & Higgins, 1987), not overly
part of a childs memorial repertoire for extraction in engrossing, but are aggression-related (such as easy ac-
future situations. Living with a violent family or in a cess to weapons; Goldstein, 1994) or semantically re-
Cognitive-Emotional Processes as Mediators 759

lated to ones ideographic aggressive schema (Carver, reinforced for aggressing. In this case, the person learns
Ganellan, Froming, & Chambers, 1983), are likely to through vicarious means that aggression leads to desired
have priming effects. consequences. Neuroimaging studies have revealed that
the positive consequences of aggressing could also be
Factors That Dysregulate Emotion Control entirely intrapersonalthrough activation of the dorsal
Lemerise and Arsenio (2000) suggested that a variety of striatum region of the brain (de Quervain et al., 2004).
emotion-related processes mediate aggressive behavior. In other words, no external reward is necessary to rein-
Although they integrate these processes with a more force reactive aggressive behaviors, which instead may
general social information-processing framework, they be reinforced merely by brain activation that is experi-
argue that the dysregulation of emotion plays a special enced as the intrinsic pleasure experienced in revenge.
role that differs from other cognitive processes. They
cite studies that relate child aggression to the childs Social Information Processing
ability to regulate emotion, to understand others emo-
tion, and to express emotion accurately. The work of Social information-processing models of aggressive be-
Eisenberg et al. (2003, 2004) buttresses the role of emo- havior were initially developed to describe at a proximal
tion-regulatory processes in mediating the development level how cognitive and emotional processes lead a child
of antisocial behavior. Other studies indicate that these to engage in aggressive behavior in a social event (e.g.,
processes develop through experience and account for Dodge, 1986; McFall, 1982). The conceptual grounding
the relation between life experiences and later behavior; for these models is work in cognitive science on how in-
for example, Cummings and Davies (2002) have argued dividuals store and retrieve information (Tulving &
that emotion dysregulation is the process that mediates Thomson, 1973), distribute processing in parallel and
the relation between witnessing marital conflict and hierarchical fashion (Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986),
child aggression. and ultimately solve problems (Newell & Simon, 1972).
According to current formulations (Dodge & Pettit,
Factors That Enhance the Attractiveness of Aggression
2003; Gifford-Smith & Rabiner, 2004), an individual
Accessing aggressive behavior from memory is only comes to a social situation with a set of neural pathways
partly a function of its salience and priming potential; it that have been honed over time through genetic and ex-
is also a function of the salience and accessibility of al- periential factors and a history of social experiences
ternatives to aggression, enhanced by modeling. Fur- that are represented in memory. The individual is pre-
thermore, social learning theory stipulates that sented with a new set of social cues (e.g., peers gently
environments induce aggression by promoting the belief tease a boy on the playground about his ugly shoes) and
that aggression is normative, morally appropriate, and responds behaviorally as a function of how he or she
will lead to desired positive consequences (through rein- processes those cues, which, in turn, is a function of the
forcement). According to Bandura (1983), (In) soci- interaction among biological, memorial, and current-cue
eties that provide extensive training in aggression, factors. Because processing occurs in real time, it can
attach prestige to it, and make its use functional, people have both conscious and unconscious (and controlled
spend a great deal of time threatening, fighting, maim- and automatic) components (Rabiner, Lenhart, &
ing, and killing each other (p. 11). Lochman, 1990). As Piaget (1965; Cowan, 1979) noted,
Bandura (1983) hypothesized that reinforcement can processing is a fully emotional, as well as cognitive,
take the form of tangible rewards, social and status re- phenomenon.
wards, reduction of aversive treatment, and expressions The first step of processing is encoding of the cues.
of injury by the victim. Patterson, Littman, and Bricker Because the cue array is so overwhelming, the individ-
(1967) found that children who were victimized by ual employs heuristics to encode only relevant portions.
peers and who occasionally succeeded in stopping those Both deficits (e.g., failure to encode mitigating cues)
attacks through counteraggression became more likely and biases (e.g., selective attention to hostile features of
to fight in the future; in contrast, those children who others behavior) in encoding could lead an individual to
avoided peers and thus avoided victimization did not be- respond aggressively; different biases (e.g., selective
come aggressive over time. Just as powerful as direct re- failure to encode actual hostile cues) could lead to
inforcement is the observation of other persons being nonaggressive responding. As cues are encoded, they
760 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

are interpreted, so the next step is mental representation response gets a reaction from another person, which be-
of the meaning of the cues, particularly with regard to comes the next cue for further encoding) and most likely
threat and others intentions. Both biases (e.g., a hostile occurs simultaneously with feedback loops in parallel
attributional bias, as dubbed by Nasby, Hayden, & De- processing (e.g., in the microsecond of evaluating a po-
Paulo, 1979) and errors (e.g., misinterpreting a benign tential response, the child encodes a change in a peers
teasing stimulus as malicious) in mental representation facial expression). As a description of social events, the
could enhance the likelihood of aggressive responding. model has heuristic strength (see Crick & Dodge, 1994,
The next step in processing is accessing of one or more for a review). In addition, the model proposes that indi-
possible behavioral responses from memory. Through viduals develop characteristic styles of processing cues
exposure, experience, and evolution, ones interpreta- at each step, in domains of situations, and that these pro-
tion of a stimulus becomes associated with emotional cessing styles will correlate with individual differences
and behavioral responses (e.g., threat-retaliate, disre- in aggressive behavior. This hypothesis has generated
spect-anger; see desired object, grab it); in some cases, over 200 empirical studies, with generally supportive
the individual actively generates responses, as in prob- findings (see reviews by Gifford-Smith & Rabiner,
lem solving. Often, single behavioral responses are not 2004; Dodge & Pettit, 2003).
accessed; rather, a program of behaviors and expected
responses to those behaviors is accessed, as in a script Selective Attention and Encoding
(Huesmann, 1988). Obviously, patterns of response ac- Using videotaped stimuli to assess the first step of pro-
cess will be closely linked to the probability of aggres- cessing, Dodge et al. (2003) found that aggressive chil-
sive responding. Accessing a response does not destine dren are less able than nonaggressive children to recall
one to that response, so the next step of processing is re- relevant social cues. Aggressive children have also been
sponse decision. The individual might evaluate a poten- found to attend selectively to aggressive social cues in a
tial response by its moral acceptability and its stimulus array more than nonaggressive peers do and
instrumental, interpersonal, and intrapersonal out- have difficulty diverting attention from aggressive cues
comes, weight the values of those outcomes, and decide (Gouze, 1987). When encoded cues are used to make in-
on a course of action. The response that is most posi- terpretations of others actions, aggressive children use
tively evaluated (e.g., a boy evaluates that hitting a peer fewer external cues than others do (Dodge & Newman,
will save face, which is more important than being pun- 1981) and instead rely more on their own stereotypes or
ished by authority, so he decides to aggress) will likely simply use the most recently presented cue (Dodge &
be enacted. An individual might consider responses si- Tomlin, 1987).
multaneously or sequentially, with varying thresholds Pollock has creatively used selective attention para-
of acceptability for enactment. In this context, impul- digms with emotional faces as stimuli to understand the
sivity is defined as the lowest possible acceptability influence of physical abuse on selective attention to
(i.e., a child responds with the first response that comes threat cues as a mechanism in the development of ag-
to mind, with no further evaluation). Thus, patterns of gressive behavior. Pollak and Tolley-Schell (2003) found
impulsivity or positive evaluation for aggressing are that physically abused children demonstrate selective
likely to eventuate in aggressive behavior. Also, re- attention to angry faces and reduced attention to happy
hearsal of behaviors in ones mind enhances the likeli- faces. Further, abused children display relative diffi-
hood of selection, without regard to its likely culty in disengaging from angry faces.
consequences (Huesmann, 1988). Finally, the selected
response gets transformed into behavior, which requires Attribution of Intent
motor and verbal skills. Skill deficiencies in enacting Over three dozen studies have shown that, given am-
aggressive responses could inhibit those behaviors, biguous provocation circumstances, aggressive children
whereas skill deficiencies in enacting competent, are more likely than nonaggressive children to make a
nonaggressive alternatives could enhance aggressive re- hostile interpretation of anothers intentions. In a typi-
sponding through default. cal study, participants are asked to imagine being the
Because the model describes social behavior as it un- object of an ambiguous provocation (e.g., a peer spills
folds in real time, the steps of processing are hypothe- water on you) and to make an interpretation of the
sized to recycle iteratively (e.g., the childs behavioral peers likely intent (Dodge, 1980). Positive correlations
Cognitive-Emotional Processes as Mediators 761

between hostile attributional biases and aggressive be- and the immediate circumstances of a particular social
havior have been found in many school-based samples, stimulus. A child may enter a situation with preexisting
including 8- to 12-year-old children (Guerra & Slaby, hopes of establishing a friendship with a peer partner in
1989), White children (Dell Fitzgerald & Asher, 1987), a game, but the stimulus of losing a game under ques-
African American middle school boys (Graham & Hud- tionable circumstances may lead the child to formulate a
ley, 1994), Latino children (Graham, Hudley, & goal of revenge that overwhelms other goals. All situa-
Williams, 1992), and British 8- to 10-year-old children tions require the balancing of multiple goals, but little is
(Aydin & Markova, 1979). Hostile attributional biases known about how children redirect their goals during so-
have also been found in aggressive clinical samples, in- cial interactions. Research on executive function
cluding children with diagnosed disruptive behavior dis- deficits in aggressive children suggests that this process
orders (MacBrayer et al., 2003), adolescent offenders may be difficult for aggressive children. Indeed, Taylor
(Dodge, Price, et al., 1990), incarcerated violent offend- and Gabriel (1989) found that aggressive children have
ers (Slaby & Guerra, 1988), and aggressive boys in resi- difficulty coordinating multiple goals.
dential treatment (Nasby et al., 1979). The findings
extend beyond hypothetical situations: Steinberg and Response Access
Dodge (1983) found similar evidence using a laboratory- The next step of processing is behavioral response ac-
based actual social interaction, in which children made cess. Both the automatic association between social cue
attributions about a peer partner. representation and aggressive responses and the con-
Although reciprocal effects are likely, prospective scious generation of aggressive solutions to social
analyses by Dodge, Pettit, Bates, and Valente (1995) in- dilemmas have been implicated in the genesis of aggres-
dicate that hostile attributional biases predict growth in sive behavior. Shure and Spivack (1980) have found that
aggressive behavior over time. Finally, experimental in- among preschool children, the number of responses that
tervention with aggressive African American boys in a child generates to hypothetical social problems is in-
which the focus was to reduce hostile attributional ten- versely related to that childs rate of aggressive behav-
dencies led to decreased aggressive behavior, relative to ior. Among children in elementary school, the quality,
a control group (Hudley & Graham, 1993). not quantity, of responses is linked to aggressive prob-
Several studies have found that aggressive children also lems. Aggressive children generate high proportions of
erroneously interpret hostile intent when the stimuli atypical responses (Ladd & Oden, 1979), bribery and
clearly depict benign intentions (Dodge, Murphy, & affect manipulation responses (Rubin, Moller, & Emp-
Buchsbaum, 1984; Dodge, Pettit, McClaskey, & Brown, tage, 1987), direct physical aggression responses
1986). Statistical controls indicate that this intention-cue (Dodge et al., 2003; Waas, 1988), and adult intervention
detection deficiency cannot be accounted for by general responses (Asher & Renshaw, 1981). They access fewer
information-processing deficits or impulsivity (Waldman, competent responses, including nonaggressive assertion
1988) or verbal intelligence (Dodge, Price, et al., 1990). (Deluty, 1981) and planning responses (Asarnow &
Callan, 1985).
Accessing Goals
A growing body of research has correlated childrens Response Decision
goals with individual differences in aggressive behavior There are crucial differences between accessing a re-
(Erdley & Asher, 1999). Aggressive behavior has been sponse and selecting it for enactment. Across develop-
linked to present-oriented (versus future-oriented) goals ment, children learn to lengthen the time between
(Caprara & Zimbardo, 1996), goals in friendships (Rose response accessing and behavior or to withhold impulses
& Asher, 1998), less-social goals (Murphy & Eisenberg, altogether, pending a mental evaluation of the likely ef-
2002), and performance-competitive (rather than rela- fects. Barkley et al. (2002) have argued that the inability
tional) goals (Asher & Renshaw, 1981). Crick and to inhibit accessed responses is the single major compo-
Dodge (1994) introduced the process of goals accessing nent of ADHD that is responsible for the aggressive be-
as the next step of processing, once the individual has havior problems of these children. Self-reports by
mentally represented the stimulus set. Thus, goals are aggressive children (Perry, Perry, & Rasmussen, 1986)
dynamically formulated online as a joint function of support the hypothesis that aggressive children have
preexisting long-term plans that may reside in memory difficulty inhibiting aggressive responses. Slaby and
762 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Guerra (1988) found that aggressive adolescent offend- child abuse, homicide, rape, or discrimination). Eu-
ers generate fewer possible outcomes for their own be- phemistic labeling (e.g., trains, get-back, just
haviors than others do, suggesting a failure to consider teaching him a lesson; McCall, 1994) further mini-
consequences by this group. mizes the perceived seriousness of the act.
Wilson and Herrnstein (1985) proposed that criminal A second set of dissociative processes involves dis-
behavior (and aggression more broadly) involves a ra- torting the relation between actions and their effects.
tional decision in which the participant considers the ex- When aggressing in a group, individuals can diffuse
pectation of benefits and their probabilities (e.g., peer their own responsibility for their actions or deflect re-
approval or instrumental gain) versus the expectation of sponsibility elsewhere (e.g., to authority, as in Mil-
costs and their probabilities (e.g., legal punishment or grams, 1974, obedience experiments, or to society more
parental disapproval). Crick and Dodge (1994) proposed broadly in war). A third set of disengagement processes
that children may consider instrumental, interpersonal, operates on the perceptions of the victims. Dehuman-
intrapersonal, and moral outcomes in their evaluations. ization through labels (e.g., cracker, nigger ) can
They may evaluate a possible behavior by these various lead to self-exoneration for inhumane acts. Modern
outcomes, assign weights (estimates of value) to these urban life tends to foster such dehumanization processes
categories, and perform mental arithmetic to decide on a as a result of high social mobility, anonymity, and the
behavioral response. Thus, environments that afford a castelike categorization of persons into in-group and
positive cost / benefit ratio will show high rates of ag- out-group members. Finally, aggressive actions are jus-
gression. Clarke and Cornish (1983) conducted a ra- tified by attributing blame for the action to the victim.
tional decision analysis of youthful burglary and found Because most aggressive acts occur in a sequence of es-
that important deciding factors included whether a calating interpersonal exchanges, aggressors often find
house was occupied, whether it had a burglar alarm or it easy to select from the chain of events an act by the
dog, and whether it reflected affluence. Not surpris- victim that merits aggressive retaliation. Studies by
ingly, Becker (1974) concludes that offenders typically Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, and Pastorelli (1996)
estimate the risk of being caught as very low (whether demonstrate that measures of moral disengagement by
valid or not); thus, a rational analysis can still lead to children based on these concepts are linked to their
risky (and ill-informed) behavior by some individuals. rates of aggressive behavior and emotional irascibility.
As young children develop the ability to represent Moral disengagement can occur quickly in a social
mentally the anticipated consequences of their behavior exchange, but more frequently it evolves gradually
(usually, between ages 5 and 10; Werner, 1961), they be- through cultural influence and reinforcement for partial
come more skilled at deciding when to aggress. The rate disengagement. Children learn across time whether dis-
of their aggression might not necessarily decrease, but engagement is allowed, and they learn to disengage more
their behavior will become more reasoned. quickly and with less apparent justification in environ-
One of the mental actions that can occur during deci- ments that provide little opposition. During this process,
sion making has been called moral disengagement by the individual misrepresents, minimizes, and disregards
Bandura (2002). Through socialization, most children the injurious effects of aggression while selectively fo-
learn self-restraints on aggressive behavior that involve cusing on the self-enhancing outcomes; diffuses respon-
anticipatory self-censuring and evaluations that aggres- sibility for the outcomes of aggression; generates
sion will be punished. Bandura (2002) argued that oth- palliative comparisons for ones own act of aggression;
erwise moral children perform aggressive acts through places euphemistic labels on ones own aggression; and
processes that disengage the usual self-reactions from vilifies and dehumanizes the victim of aggression. Ban-
such conduct. Several processes can contribute to moral dura et al. (1996) have demonstrated in a longitudinal
disengagement. Cognitive restructuring can involve jus- study of children outside of Rome, Italy, that these
tifying aggressive action on the grounds that it will lead processes predict interpersonal aggressive behavior.
to a higher moral end (e.g., self-respect). This restruc- Even otherwise moral human beings have been found to
turing involves the minimization of ones own moral vi- engage in these processes selectively during the course
olations by comparing them to more reprehensible acts. of morally defended aggressive acts (Bandura, 2002).
This minimizing is particularly easy when a child is When they are experimentally forced to make evalua-
growing up amidst truly flagrant inhumanities (e.g., tions and consider consequences, aggressive children,
Cognitive-Emotional Processes as Mediators 763

relative to nonaggressive peers, evaluate aggressive re- niques are used, the predictability of aggressive behav-
sponses as more legitimate (Erdley & Asher, 1998), less ior from aggregated processing measures is great
morally bad (Deluty, 1983), more friendly (Crick (Dodge et al., 1986). Findings from 9 samples (1 from
& Ladd, 1990), and globally more acceptable (Crick & Dodge & Price, 1994; 3 from Dodge et al., 1986; 2 from
Werner, 1998). They expect more positive instrumental Dodge et al., 2003; 1 from Slaby & Guerra, 1988; and 2
outcomes (Egan, Monson, & Perry, 1998), more positive from Weiss, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1992) indicate that
intrapersonal outcomes (Fontaine, Burks, & Dodge, processing variables from different steps provide unique
2002), fewer negative interpersonal outcomes (Quiggle, increments in predicting aggression, such that multiple
Panak, Garber, & Dodge, 1992), and fewer sanctional correlations range up to .94.
outcomes (Perry et al., 1986) for aggressing. In contrast, Several studies have found that the effects of adverse
aggressive children report lower levels of perceived self- life experiences on growth in aggressive behavior are
regulatory efficacy for prosocial behaviors and resisting mediated by the childs development of patterns of pro-
peer pressure (Caprara, Regalia, & Bandura, 2002). cessing social information. Dodge, Bates, et al. (1990)
Ethnographic analyses of urban communities also sup- found that the experience of physical maltreatment is as-
port the notion that codes of violence support aggres- sociated with an acquired tendency to become hypervig-
sive behavior as the only means of gaining status and ilant to hostile cues, to attribute hostile intent to others,
avoiding victimization (E. Anderson, 1990). to access aggressive responses readily, and to evaluate
aggressive responses as instrumentally successful. In
Enactment
turn, these social information-processing patterns were
The final step of processing is to transform a selected found to lead to later aggressive behavior and to account
response into motor and verbal behavior. Socially re- for the effect of maltreatment on aggression in middle
jected and aggressive children have been shown to be school (Dodge et al., 1995).
less competent when asked to enact and role-play nonag- Eisenberg et al. (2003) found that parents negative
gressive socially appropriate behaviors in laboratory expressed emotion influences the childs social adjust-
settings (Burleson, 1982). One of the difficulties with ment through its mediating effects on the development
these studies is that childrens enactments may be con- of self-regulatory processes, including attention focus-
founded by other mental processes, such as their expec- ing, attention shifting, and inhibitory control. In yet
tations about the likely outcomes of behavior, even in another study, Snyder, Stoolmiller, Wilson, and Ya-
laboratory role-play settings. mamoto (2003b) found that parents responses to child
misbehavior lead to growth in the childs antisocial be-
Mediation of Life Experiences through havior through the mediating process of childrens anger
Acquired Processing Patterns regulation.

Each of the processing-aggressive behavior correlations


Latent Knowledge Structures
previously described is rather modest in magnitude.
These correlations are enhanced by considering the situ- A growing body of evidence suggests that childrens life
ational context, the type of aggressive behavior, and the experiences lead to represented memories that predict
profile of processing patterns. Dodge et al. (1986) found future behavioral tendencies through an influence on the
that the correlation between processing and aggressive way that future social information is processed. This
behavior is stronger in situations than across situations work has been conducted under several different rubrics.
(i.e., processing about teasing events relates more
strongly to aggressive behavior in response to teasing Schemas and Scripts
than to aggressive behavior in peer group entry situa- Social-cognitive theories in psychology (Bargh et al.,
tions). Also, early-step processing variables (i.e., encod- 1995) suggest that processing of social cues is guided by
ing and hostile attributions) relate more strongly to latent knowledge structures, variously called schemas
reactive anger, whereas later-stage processing variables and scripts, that are stored in memory (Abelson, 1981).
(i.e., response evaluations) relate more strongly to These structures are hypothesized to be the evolving
proactive aggression (Crick & Dodge, 1996). Finally, representational products of experience, which guide
when profiles are assembled or multiple regression tech- processing of new cues. Baldwin (1992) has described
764 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

several ways that social knowledge structures exert an themselves for their negative outcomes (Cairns, 1991).
impact on social information processing, including se- The possibility that other-blaming may represent self-
lective attention to cues, premature judgments about defensive mechanisms in aggressive children (Keith,
stimuli, biased interpretations of ambiguous informa- 1984) indicates the inherent difficulty in measuring this
tion, and biased expectancies for the outcomes of events. latent knowledge structure.
Huesmann (1988, 1998) has articulated the hypothe-
sis that early development leads children to represent in Normative Beliefs
memory scripts for aggression that include acceptable Huesmann (1998) has proposed that childrens beliefs
antecedents, details of context and action, and likely about consensual social norms influence childrens ag-
consequences. Graham and Hudley (1994) employed gressive behavior. Children learn these norms through
priming techniques from cognitive-social psychology to perception, identification with reference groups, and
ascertain that aggressive children have highly accessible personal evaluation. Guerra et al. (1995) assessed chil-
aggressive constructs represented in memory. drens normative beliefs about the consequences of ag-
Stromquist and Strauman (1992) asked children to de- gressing and found that (a) the male culture more
scribe freely their social relationships and found that the strongly endorses the use of aggression than does the fe-
tendency to use aggressive constructs ( both in commis- male culture, ( b) the normative endorsement of aggres-
sion and omission, e.g., he hits others and he stays sion increases across age during the school years, and
away from fights) is correlated with aggressive behav- (c) these beliefs correlate with aggressive behavior.
ior. Burks, Laird, Dodge, Pettit, and Bates (1999) used Zelli, Dodge, Lochman, Laird, and the Conduct Prob-
the Stromquist-Strauman method to find that hostile lems Prevention Research Group (1999) found that third
representations of peer relationships predict later ag- graders who hold normative beliefs that aggression is
gressive behavior toward peers. MacKinnon-Lewis, Ra- acceptable are relatively likely to engage in deviant so-
biner, and Starnes (1999) found that cognitive cial information processing in fourth and fifth grade,
representations of familiar and unfamiliar peers are as- which, in turn, is related to aggressive behavior and me-
sociated with social maladjustment. Specifically, boys diates the effect of beliefs on aggression. The converse
who held negative feelings about known peers at the be- relation did not hold as strongly.
ginning of the school year were less likely to be ac-
cepted by peers 6 months later. Working Models
Bowlby (1980) proposed that early life experiences, es-
Self-Concept
pecially with regard to an infants attachment with a
Self-concept is a knowledge structure that has been hy- caregiver, lead to the formulation of working models of
pothesized to relate to aggression (Harter, 1982). How- how social relationships operate. These models reside in
ever, despite the speculation of psychodynamic theorists memory and guide future social behavior. Cassidy, Ziv,
(Keith, 1984) that aggressive children must have miser- Rodenberg, and Woodhouse (2003) found that measures
able self-concepts, empirical assessments have not borne of working models correlate with indices of childrens
out this hypothesis (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Zakriski & social adjustment. Cassidy et al. (1996) found that chil-
Coie, 1996). Even aggressive children who are also so- dren classified as insecurely attached or who reported
cially rejected by peers do not seem to recognize that experiencing parental rejection were more likely to
they are rejected by peers. Zakriski and Coie (1996) re- make hostile attributions in response to hypothetical,
ported that aggressive-rejected boys did not see them- ambiguous provocations than were other children.
selves as having been rejected by an experimental
confederate playmate even when this was observed to Moral Development and Perspective Taking
have happened. Hymel, Bowker, and Woody (1993) Arsenio and Lemerise (2004) have proposed that moral
found that unpopular aggressive children received self- development acts as a distal latent knowledge structure
concept scores in academic, athletic, appearance, and to guide more proximal online processing of social in-
social competence domains that were as high as those of formation. The structuralist perspective on cognitive
average peers, even though objective assessments of development evolving from Piagets (1965) theory and
their competence in these domains indicated otherwise. research has contributed two major ideas to the quest for
Aggressive children appear to blame others rather than understanding the development of antisocial behavior
Treatment and Prevention of Antisocial Behavior 765

(Nucci, 2001). First, social and cognitive egocentrism, lives but also to test developmental hypotheses more rig-
or the failure to recognize the perspective of social oth- orously. A comprehensive review is beyond the scope of
ers, has been related to the development of antisocial be- this chapter. Instead, the intervention experiments that
havior. Chandler (1973) found that aggressive childrens will be highlighted are those that include a theoretical
social perspective-taking level, scored in terms of Pi- basis in a developmental model, random assignment,
agetian developmental levels, was lower than their peers. evaluation of all children assigned to intervention
He then developed an intervention showing young ado- (whether they received it or not), independent replica-
lescent delinquents videotapes of their own behavior, tion, long-term follow-up, and blind assessment of out-
followed by discussion in which participants were en- come. These studies have in common the hypotheses that
couraged to take each others roles, to improve their conduct problem behavior develops (or is maintained) by
perspective-taking levels. Those adolescents who par- one or more of the developmental factors reviewed here
ticipated in the intervention displayed fewer delinquent and that intervention to alter those developmental factors
acts 18 months later than did a control group. Others will indirectly lead to the prevention or reduction of ag-
(e.g., Iannotti, 1978) have been unable to obtain shifts in gressive behavior. The interventions range from univer-
aggression through role-taking training. There have sal (population-based) to selected (groups identified
been some attempts to make children less aggressive by based on risk factors) to indicated (groups identified
training them to take the affective perspective of others based on aggressive behavior) approaches.
(e.g., Feshbach & Feshbach, 1982), but the results of
these efforts have been mixed, at best.
The second major contribution of the structuralist Stimulant Medication
perspective has been to relate moral development to
The success of psychostimulant medication in treating
antisocial behavior. The basic hypothesis has been that
attention deficits (Frick, 2001), coupled with the known
developmental lags in stages of moral development will
association between early biologically based attention
lead to antisocial behavior. Blasi (1980) reviewed stud-
deficits (ADHD) and CD has led to the hypothesis that
ies testing the relation between moral judgment and
psychostimulant medication could indirectly reduce
moral action and concluded that a majority of these
conduct problems. The Multimodal Treatment Study of
studies supported Kohlbergs (1986) thesis that higher
Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
moral reasoning would lead to personal honesty and al-
(MTA) is the largest randomized trial test of this hy-
truism. The fact that moral reasoning per se does not al-
pothesis, albeit with a subgroup of conduct-problem
ways relate to apparently altruistic behavior or
children who have comorbid ADHD. Although psychos-
resistance to temptation has led to an elaboration of the
timulant treatment (relative to no treatment) was found
theory in terms of personal identity: Moral reasoning is
to be effective in altering attention deficits in this group
related to moral behavior in individuals for whom moral
of 7- to 10-year-olds, by itself it had no substantial im-
values are central to their self-understanding (Blasi &
pact on oppositional and aggressive behavior (MTA Co-
Oresick, 1986). Recent studies have found correlations
operative Group, 1999). Other trials (reviewed by Frick,
between moral reasoning in domains of social encoun-
2001) indicate some success of psychostimulants in re-
ters (Horn, 2003) and racial exclusion (Killen, Lee-Kim,
ducing disruptive behavior; however, Frick points out
McGlothlin, & Stangor, 2002) and social adjustment.
that the medications primary effect may be to enhance
the childs responsiveness to other interventions
(p. 600) and that there is little evidence to date that
TREATMENT AND PREVENTION OF
stimulants reduce conduct problems in children without
ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR
a comorbid diagnosis of ADHD (p. 600).
The past decade has witnessed an explosion of random-
ized trials testing interventions to prevent aggressive be-
Parent-Based Approaches
havioral development and to treat conduct disorder.
Hinshaw (2002) has noted that intervention experiments A variety of family interventions designed to improve
offer the opportunity not only to apply the developmental parents discipline strategies, the quality of parent-child
knowledge reviewed in this chapter to improve childrens relationships, and parents monitoring and supervision
766 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

of children have proven efficacious in reducing aggres- of possible individual, family, peer, school, and commu-
sive behavior. The basis for most of these interventions nity risk factors in antisocial behavior. A meta-analysis
is coercion theory by Patterson et al. (1992) and Fore- of 11 outcome studies revealed a mean effect size of .55
hand and McMahons (1981) behavioral approach. The in reducing antisocial outcomes (Curtis, Ronan, & Bour-
primary goal of Parent Management Training (PMT) is duin, 2004).
to alter the pattern of exchanges between parent and For antisocial youth who are already in the child wel-
child during discipline events so that coercive behavior fare system, multidimensional treatment foster care
by each party is extinguished in favor of contingent, con- (MTFC) has been developed. The MTFC model was
sistent, and clear rules that lead to compliance. Parent- originally funded by the Oregon Youth Authority in
ing behaviors are taught through role-play, practice, 1983 to provide a community-based alternative to incar-
completion of homework, and discussion, sometimes ceration for boys with serious and chronic delinquency
with the child present. (Chamberlain, 2003). Two randomized trials have sup-
Rigorous evaluations have proven this approach to be ported the efficacy of MTFC. In the first trial, MTFC
efficacious. PMT is probably the most well-investi- boys had significantly lower rates of official and self-
gated therapy technique for children and adoles- reported delinquency in a 12-month follow-up and lower
cents . . . and has led to marked improvements in child rates of violent offending in a 24-month follow-up than
behavior (Kazdin, 2003a, pp. 261262). A meta-analy- did group-care youth (Chamberlain & Reid, 1998; Eddy,
sis by Serketich and Dumas (1996) yielded a large mean Whaley, & Chamberlain, 2004). A second trial with
effect size of .86 standard deviations for programs with adolescent girls found that MTFC girls spent fewer days
young children (up to age 10). A meta-analysis of pro- in locked settings, had fewer parent-reported delinquent
grams for delinquent and conduct-disordered children behaviors, and showed a trend toward fewer arrests at
aged 10 to 17 by Woolfendon, Williams, and Peat the 12-month follow-up (Leve & Chamberlain, 2005).
(2002) yielded a mean effect size of .56. The meta- Eddy and Chamberlain (2000) examined mediational
analysis of 40 studies by Farrington and Welsh (2003) factors in the boys sample, with several factors mediat-
yielded a mean effect size of .32 in preventing delin- ing the relationship between group condition and boys
quency outcomes. criminal referral and self-reported delinquency rates.
Kazdin (2003b) notes that one of the major problems Mediating variables included close and consistent su-
with PMT approaches is getting families to participate pervision, effective discipline, adult mentoring, and sep-
and to complete the intervention. Issues of cultural rel- aration from delinquent peers, all consistent with the
evance loom large, and practices to engage parents by developmental model described in this chapter.
focusing on family relationships (as in functional fam- Webster-Stratton (1998) has led the move to bring
ily therapy; Alexander & Parsons, 1982), and by attend- these principles to preventive intervention with high-
ing to their stressors (Kazdin & Whitley, 2003) have risk families of preschool-aged children (such as Head
proven important. Start), with marked short-term success as evaluated by
Several mediational analyses of PMT have supported randomized trials. The Triple P-Positive Parenting Pro-
the developmental hypothesis that parenting practices gram by Sanders, Markie-Dadds, Tully, and Bor (2000)
themselves play a role in aggressive behavioral growth. has been adapted for use in universal settings (media),
Hinshaw et al. (2000) found that child disruptive behav- on a selected basis for concerned parents, or in primary
ior gains in school as a function of randomly assigned care settings, also with success. A different parent-
treatment could be accounted for by improvements in based approach was taken by Olds et al. (1998), who
parenting practices. Dishion and Kavanagh (2000) re- made weekly home visits by a nurse-practitioner to high-
ported that changes in parenting accounted for the posi- risk mothers beginning in pregnancy for 3 years. The
tive effects of the parent intervention of the Adolescent visitor provided information about child-rearing, helped
Transitions Program (ATP) on child antisocial behavior. problem solve parenting and family issues, and helped
For families facing extremely high levels of anti- bring mothers to financial self-sufficiency. A random-
social behavior in adolescents who are on the verge of ized trial revealed no positive effects on childrens con-
incarceration, multisystemic therapy (MST) has proven duct problems during elementary school (Kitzman et al.,
efficacious (Henggeler et al., 1997, 1998). It is an inten- 1997), but, by age 15, 45% of the control group children
sive home-based approach based on an ecological model had been arrested in contrast with just 20% of the inter-
Treatment and Prevention of Antisocial Behavior 767

vention group children (Olds et al., 1998). Other home- Lochman has developed the Coping Power Program,
visiting programs have yielded less favorable long-term which is designed to enhance an array of social-cogni-
effects (Stone, Bendell, & Field, 1988). tive skills in aggressive fourth- and fifth-grade boys.
Lochman and Wells (2004) have found positive effects
School-Based Approaches of this program in reducing aggressive behavior as rated
by school teachers, which persisted into the following
The largest school-based approach to reducing aggres- school year, with an effect size of .42. Ross and Ross
sive behavior is that implemented by Olweus (1993), (1998) used a randomized trial to demonstrate positive
which involved the distribution of booklets and videos effects in preventing reoffending for their program
to teachers and parents in all schools in Norway, focus- aimed at helping youth to stop and think about social
ing on targeted parenting and discipline practices to re- problems, consider alternative strategies, and consider
duce bullying behaviors. Cross-time evaluations suggest consequences of their actions.
positive effects, but this program has not been evaluated Kazdin (2003a) has developed a variant of this ap-
by a randomized trial. proach that he calls Problem-Solving Skills Training
The most well-known classroom-based approach is (PSST). When implemented with aggressive children in
the Good Behavior Game (GBG), which is a behavior randomized trials, he has found success in reducing ag-
management program designed to reduce disruptive be- gressive behavior in both home and school settings that
havior and promote prosocial behavior by group-level lasts over 12 months, in at least five replicated studies.
contingencies. When implemented in first grade class-
rooms in randomized trials, it has proven efficacious in
Combining Approaches
reducing disruptive behavior at both proximal (Ialongo
et al., 1999) and distal (Ialongo, Poduska, Werthamer, & The developmental model described in this chapter
Kellam, 2001) time points. Van Lier, Muthn, van der posits risk factors and processes from multiple domains
Sar, and Crijnen (2004) found that these positive effects that provide unique, incremental power in predicting ag-
could be sustained across 24 months and were strongest gressive behavior outcomes. Given that single-domain
for the most disruptive children. approaches have proven modestly efficacious, several
Universally-administered classroom curricula have intervention scientists have hypothesized that compre-
been developed to teach social-cognitive and social- hensive approaches that cross multiple domains would
emotional skills for the purpose of preventing aggres- have even more powerful effects. In the MTA Study
sive behavior. Greenberg and Kusche (1993) have found (Hinshaw et al., 2000), a combined program that in-
success with their PATHS Program (Providing Alterna- cluded both stimulant medication and parent manage-
tive Thinking Strategies) in increasing prosocial behav- ment training yielded more positive effects on child
ior, and the Conduct Problems Prevention Research disruptive behavior than either approach alone.
Group (1999b) has demonstrated classroom-level suc- Lochman and Wells (2004) contrasted their child-fo-
cess in reducing aggressive behavior with its adaptation cused Coping Power program with an enhanced program
of this approach. that added 16 parent-group sessions based on behavioral
principles. The comprehensive program yielded more
Social Cognitive Skills Training positive effects on child self-reports of covert delin-
quency than either the child-focused program or a con-
A key component of developmental models of aggressive trol. Likewise, Kazdin (2003a) found that an
behavior is the childs social-cognitive skill deficits, in- intervention that combined parent management training
cluding attributional biases, problem solving, and deci- with child problem-solving skills training tended to be
sion making. Based on this model, numerous more effective than either intervention alone.
interventions have been developed to prevent aggressive Tremblay et al. (1995) implemented an important in-
behavior through enhancement of these skills and reduc- tervention that combined parent management training
tion of deficits and biases. Graham and Hudley (1993) based on Pattersons principles with social and problem-
developed an intervention designed to reduce hostile at- solving skills training with groups of 7-year-old boys
tributional biases in African American children, with over a 2-year period. In contrast with a randomly as-
demonstrated short-term success in a randomized trial. signed control group, by age 12 the intervention group
768 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

committed less burglary and were in fewer fights. By aggression when administered to older youth in an
age 17, analyses of developmental trajectories revealed inner-city context, a finding that is consistent with the
that fewer intervention-group boys (16.6%) than control deviant peer-contagion hypothesis that bringing deviant
boys (29.5%) followed a trajectory of high levels of peers together only enhances problem outcomes.
physical aggression.
Social Development Model

Comprehensive Approaches to Prevention Hawkins, von Cleve, and Catalano (1991) combined par-
ent training in behavior management, teacher training,
Several interventions have gone beyond the simple com- and child interpersonal cognitive problem-solving skills
bination of two approaches toward comprehensive ap- training with first-grade children in the Social Develop-
proaches that last multiple years. These approaches ment Model program. ODonnell et al. (1995) reported
tend to have greater and longer-lasting impact than that, by sixth grade, intervention boys from low-income
other programs. families reported less delinquency than low-income con-
trol boys. By age 18, Hawkins, Catalano, Kosterman,
Reaching Educators, Children, and Parents Program
Abbott, and Hill (1999) found that the full intervention
The RECAP Program (Reaching Educators, Children, group (receiving intervention for 6 years from grade one
and Parents; Weiss, Harris, Catron, & Han, 2003) pro- to grade six) reported less violence than a no-treatment
vided semistructured training with individual children, control group and an intervention group that received
small peer groups, classroom groups, classroom teach- intervention only in grades five and six.
ers, and parents. Child components focused on social
skills, reattribution training, communication skills, self- Fast Track
control, affect recognition, and relaxation. Parent and The Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group
teacher components focused on using praise and punish- (CPPRG) designed a 10-year-long intervention that
ment appropriately, improving adult-child communica- combined family, peer, academic, classroom, and child
tion, and strengthening the adult-child relationship. This social-cognitive skill-training components of the devel-
program was administered to children with comorbid opmental model into a cohesive and comprehensive in-
externalizing and internalizing problems in a random- tervention called Fast Track. It was delivered to 445
ized trial that revealed positive effects on teacher, self, first-grade children at high risk for adolescent violence
and peer ratings of externalizing problem behaviors at 1- and contrasted with a similar number of randomly as-
year follow-up. signed control-group children. After the 1st year of in-
tervention, compared with the control group, the
Metropolitan Area Child Study
intervention group displayed higher levels of targeted
The Metropolitan Area Child Study (MACS; Metropoli- skills in parenting, social cognition, and reading, and
tan Area Study Research Group, 2002) nested interven- less aggressive behavior (CPPRG, 1999a). These effects
tions in a research design that contrasted no treatment, a on aggressive behavior persisted through third grade
classroom program, a classroom-plus-small-group peer- (CPPRG, 2002a) and appeared to hold equally well
skills training program, and a classroom-plus-small- across gender, ethnic, and severity-level groups. Effects
group plus family-intervention program delivered in have persisted through fifth grade, with about a 25% re-
grades two and three and/or grades five and six. The duction in cases that could be classified as clinically de-
peer-group component focused on altering normative viant (CPPRG, 2004). Mediation analyses revealed that
beliefs about aggression and improving peer social intervention effects on outcomes could be partially ac-
skills, and the family intervention focused on parenting counted for gains in targeted areas of intervention, con-
skills and parent-child communication. Outcome analy- sistent with the premises of the developmental model
ses revealed that only the fully combined intervention, (CPPRG, 2002b).
when delivered in a community-rich context in the early
grades, had a positive effect on reducing peer- and Early Comprehensive Approaches
teacher-rated aggression, relative to a randomly as-
signed control group. The peer-group component ap- Finally, given the importance of early lack of stimula-
peared to have an iatrogenic effect on increasing tion of cognitive and social skills in developmental mod-
Conclusions and Future Directions 769

els of aggressive behavior, several comprehensive pro- account for well over half of the variance, too? Four gen-
grams that provide enriched preschool environments eral conclusions may resolve this paradox, but it is impor-
coupled with home-visiting for parent support have been tant to recognize that our review of determinant factors
evaluated for effects on aggressive behavior. includes studies that vary in the degree to which they
might evidence causality rather than statistical correlation.
Perry Preschool Project A number of developmental theorists have argued for
The Perry Preschool Project is the most well-known of research designs that strengthen the implication of
these efforts. Three-year-old African American children causality for individual risk factors, but this is not always
living in poverty were randomly assigned to intervention or a straightforward issue because, as Rutter et al. (1998)
control groups. The intervention group was provided daily have noted, causal processes are neither simple nor uni-
preschool and weekly home visits for 2 years. The inter- directional. They involve indirect chain effects rather
vention group was rated as less aggressive by teachers at than one basic cause, and they can also reflect a two-way
ages 6 to 9, rated as less delinquent by self-report at age 15, interplay between underlying biological features and be-
had fewer arrests at age 19, and had only half as many ar- havior (p. 378). The kinds of ABAB designs that were
rests by age 27 (Schweinhart, Barnes, & Weikart, 1993). used in behavioral analysis designs to demonstrate
causality of situational factors, such as reward contin-
Child-Parent Center
gency, are not applicable to the study of individual dif-
The Child-Parent Center approach is similar to the Perry ferences. Intervention designs have been proposed as one
Preschool approach in providing preschool along with fam- way to approximate this kind of rigor, but interventions
ily support. Long-term evaluations reveal that by age 18 that attempt individual change across multiple contexts
program participants, compared with randomly assigned and across time, as contrasted with lab analogue studies,
controls, had fewer nonviolent (17% versus 26%) and vio- typically address multiple aspects of the individual or
lent (9% versus 15%) arrests. A program in Syracuse, New parent functioning. Nonetheless, most of the factors re-
York, revealed similar positive effects (Lally, Mangione, viewed rely on longitudinal studies that attempt to con-
& Honig, 1988), but the well-administered Infant Health trol for other related risk factors to demonstrate some
and Development Program (IHDP) found no positive long- independence of influence. Some, such as the impact of
term effects on conduct problems (McCarton et al., 1997). peer factors, have tracked changes in individual aggres-
sive or antisocial behavior across time in relation to the
hypothesized risk factors. Some factors, such as TV
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS viewing, are inextricably confounded with other factors,
such as the propensity to choose to watch violent pro-
Recent advances reported in this chapter indicate a wide grams or the correlate changes in acceptability of vio-
range of risk factors for aggressive and antisocial behavior lence in the surrounding culture, and this makes it
and numerous processes in its development across the life- difficult to determine causality even though the potential
span. These factors cover domains as broad as genes, ac- for causality seems so inherently compelling.
quired neuropsychological deficits, personality
characteristics, ecological and cultural contexts, family
demographics, parenting strategies, peer relations, situa- Conclusion 1: Predictors Differ across
tional characteristics, and cognitive-emotional skills and Aggressive Acts, Personalities, and Patterns
biases. The processes range from neurological to social, at
intrapersonal, interpersonal, and institutional levels. A First, it is necessary to distinguish among predictors of
simple summing of the bivariate effect sizes of these risk aggressive acts, aggressive personalities, and aggressive
factors in predicting aggressive behavior would reveal a patterns. Aggressive acts are largely situationally and
striking paradox. On the one hand, the separate effect contextually determined. Handgun homicides are well
sizes would sum to well beyond 100% of the variance in modeled by variations in laws, policies, and access to
aggression; on the other hand, the sum would not come handguns across the world. The probability of an anti-
close to accounting for all of the variance in aggressive be- social act of looting is much greater during an electrical
haviors. How can genes account for a full half of the vari- blackout than during a well-policed spring afternoon.
ance while life experiences and situational parameters The occurrence of an act of reactive aggression almost
770 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

always follows some kind of provocation stimulus and parental social support on antisocial behavior are highly
almost never occurs in the absence of such a stimulus. In redundant and can be accounted for by their correlation
a hypothetical experiment in which all individuals are with inadequate parenting. Redundancy across all risk
exposed equally to all possible situational stimuli, most factors is not nearly complete, as indicated by the cumu-
of the variance in single acts of aggression would occur lative risk factor modeling pioneered by Rutter (1989).
within (not between) individuals. Individual acts of ag- Second, a large portion of the variance in aggressive
gression are poorly predicted by heredity. behavior is accounted for by interaction effects among
Second, stable patterns of aggression are well pre- risk factors. Merely cumulating nonredundant risk fac-
dicted by individual difference factors like heredity and tors will not exhaustively account for the variation in ag-
dispositions. These stable patterns emerge when acts of gressive behavior; rather, one of the most important
aggression are aggregated across time and situations. findings of the past decade is that risk factors often exert
When we look at these personalities, derived by averag- their influence contingentlyonly in the context of an-
ing across time and situations, we see the importance of other risk factor. Caspi et al.s (2002) findings that child
between individual factors. Just as some situations are maltreatment leads to child CD only in the presence of
more likely to lead to aggression, so some people are the MAO-A genotype is a prime example. Another exam-
more likely to act aggressively. ple is Dodge et al.s (2003) finding that peer social re-
Yet a third kind of aggressive behavior occurs as a jection enhances risk for growth in aggressive behavior
contingent pattern. Developmental studies suggest that only among a subgroup of children evaluated by their
life experiences have the effect of altering a persons be- mothers as showing difficult temperament. A third ex-
havior in particular contexts for as long as the contextual ample is Lynam et al.s (2000) demonstration that im-
parameters remain the same. As an example, the experi- pulsivity is a risk factor for delinquency primarily in
ence of physical abuse teaches a child that the social neighborhoods characterized by poor informal controls.
world is threatening and that aggressive behavior toward Third, risk factors transactthey reciprocally influ-
peers protects oneself from harm. This pattern of pre- ence each other across time. Harsh discipline of a child
emptive reactive aggressive behavior will endure as long may lead that child to fail to develop social skills of re-
as the balance of a childs life experiences continues to be lating effectively with other persons. In turn, a child
threatening. As a second example, contingent reinforce- who lacks social skills may get into conflicts with peers
ment of aggressive behavior as described in Pattersons and adults, leading adults to apply more harsh disci-
coercion theory (Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992) will pline. Further, these transactions may lead to a transfor-
lead to a pattern of high rates of aggressive behavior, as mation in the risk factors. The parent who applies harsh
long as the contingencies remain in place. Further compli- discipline in the above example may find that, over time,
cating the picture is empirical evidence suggesting that repeated conflicts with a child are very stressful and
these children may inadvertently act in such a way as to disruptive to family life. As this child moves into ado-
maintain the occurrence of threats from others or contin- lescence, empirical evidence suggests that this parent
gent reinforcement for their aggressive behavior. may withdraw from interaction, monitoring, and super-
vision of this child, perhaps to minimize overt conflicts.
Conclusion 2: Risk Factors Operate in The child may also play a role in becoming adept at mak-
Biopsychosocial Symbiotic Development ing it difficult for a parent to monitor his or her behav-
ior. The parents lack of monitoring, coupled with the
The second resolution of the paradox in risk factors for childs own social rejection by mainstream peers, may
aggression comes from an understanding of how risk fac- lead the child to gravitate toward deviant peers, thus
tors co-occur, interact, and transact. First, the co-occur- transforming the childs peer status from social rejec-
rence, or correlation, among risk factors suggests that tion to member of a deviant peer group.
they might share a common origin and might not account The co-occurrences, interaction effects, and transac-
for unique portions of the variance in aggressive behav- tional mediation effects among risk factors described
ior. Empirical evidence indicates that, for example, the here are more the rule than the exception in antisocial
effects of low socioeconomic status, large family size, development. They explain why a simple summing of bi-
single-parent status, teenage-parent status, and lack of variate effects would yield well beyond 100% of vari-
References 771

ance explained and yet would not exhaustively de- sets to reveal patterns in antisocial development, factors
scribe how chronic antisocial behavior develops. that predict trajectories, and life experiences that de-
flect individuals away from antisocial lives.
Conclusion 3: A General Consensus Model of Finally, prevention science is maturing at a rapid
the Development of Aggressive Behavior rate. Randomized clinical trials are proliferating, pro-
Patterns Is within Reach viding opportunities to test developmental theories.
These trials include both large-scale implementations of
Instead of a haphazard array of risk factors that may cu- broad models of multiple risk factors and single-compo-
mulate, interact, and transact in unknown ways, a rap- nent trials designed to identify specific clinical tech-
idly growing body of theory is developing in the field niques for achieving behavior change. Evidence is strong
such that a consensus model of the development of ag- that changing parenting behavior and improving social-
gressive behavior patterns may well cohere in the next cognitive skills can alter trajectories of antisocial be-
decade. The components of this model are described in havior, supporting the causal role of these factors.
this chapter, and they include genetic factors, sociocul- Future trials will refine developmental models through
tural contexts, early life experiences ( both biological the rigor of experiments and will bring the fruits of de-
exposures and psychosocial experiences in family and velopmental psychopathology to bear on the crucial
peer domains), middle childhood experiences (in fam- problem of violence in childrens lives. We anticipate
ily, peer, and school domains), adolescent experiences that the next decades edition of this chapter will benefit
(in family, peer, school, and community domains) and greatly from these advances.
transient situational stimuli. The mediating processes of
these influences are likely to be intrapersonal, at both
neuropsychological and cognitive-emotional levels.
REFERENCES

Conclusion 4: The Most Important Discoveries Abelson, R. P. (1981). The psychological status of the script concept.
in the Next Decade Will Come from Studies of American Psychologist, 36, 715-729.
Gene-Environment Interactions, Modeling of Aber, J. L., Gershoff, E. T., Ware, A., & Kotler, J. A. (2004). Esti-
Developmental Trajectories, and Prevention mating the effects of September 11th and other forms of vio-
lence on the mental health and social development of New York
Experiments
Citys youth: A matter of context. Applied Developmental Sci-
ence, 8, 111129.
Finally, this chapter points to the areas where the most
Abramovitch, R., Corter, C., & Lando, B. (1979). Sibling interaction
exciting discoveries are likely to occur in the next in the home. Child Development, 4, 9971003.
decade. Three areas seem especially ripe. First, as re- Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist
cent technological advances make it easier and less ex- and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department
pensive to identify specific genotypes, studies are likely of Psychiatry.
to test the correlation between various genotypes and in- Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child
Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burling-
dices of aggressive behavior and risk factors associated ton: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
with aggression. The studies by Caspi et al. (2002), re- Aguilar, B., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (2000). Distin-
ported in this chapter, provide models for this work. guishing the early-onset /persistent and adolescence-onset anti-
These studies are likely to yield important findings if social behavior types: From birth to 16 years. Development and
Psychopathology, 12, 109132.
they are completed with samples for which the measure-
Alexander, J. F., & Parsons, B. V. (1982). Functional family therapy.
ment of the environment is equally precise. It is antici- Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
pated that such studies will reveal interaction effects Alm, P. O., Alm, M., Humble, K., Leppert, J., Srensen, S., Lidberg,
between genotypes and environmental factors. Replicat- L., et al. (1994). Criminality and platelet monoamine oxidase ac-
ing such findings and integrating them into coherent the- tivity in former juvenile delinquents as adults. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 89(1), 4145.
ories will challenge this field.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
Second, recent methodological advances in the mod- manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
eling of developmental trajectories and changes in latent Anderson, C. A. (2004). An update on the effects of violent video
classes across time will be applied to longitudinal data games. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 113122.
772 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Anderson, D. A. (1999). The aggregate burden of crime. Journal of and the DSM-IV personality disorders in substance abusers. Jour-
Law and Economics, 42, 611642. nal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 545553.
Anderson, E. (1990). Streetwise: Race, class, and change in an urban Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Engle-
community. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Anderson, K. E., Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1986). Mothers in- Bandura, A. (1983). Psychological mechanisms of aggression. In
teractions with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects R. G. Geen & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and
whom? Developmental Psychology, 22, 604609. empirical reviews (pp. 124). New York: Academic Press.
Archer, J. (1988). Ethopharmacological approaches to aggressive be- Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of
havior. Nordisk Psykiatrisk tidsskrift, 42(2), 471477. moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101119.
Archer, J. (1991). The inf luence of testosterone on human aggression. Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996).
British Journal of Psychology, 82, 128. Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral
Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosex- agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 364374.
ual partners: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-medi-
126, 651680. ated aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
ogy, 66, 311.
Archer, J., Pearson, N. A., & Westeman, K. E. (1998). Aggressive be-
havior of children aged 6 to 11: Gender differences and their mag- Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a ne-
nitude. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 371384. glected construct. Child Development, 67, 32963319.
Aronowitz, B., Liebowitz, M. R., Hollander, E., Fazzini, E., Durlach- Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic
Misteli, C., Frenkel, M., et al. (1994). Neuropsychiatric and neu- inf luence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman &
ropsychological findings in conduct disorder and attention-deficit J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 351). New York:
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Guilford Press.
Neurosciences, 6(3), 245249. Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., & Hymes, C. (1995). The au-
Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Capsi, A., Taylor, A., Rijsdijk, F. V., tomatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude acti-
Jaffee, S. R., et al. (2003). Strong genetic effects on cross-situa- vation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social
tional antisocial behavior among 5-year-old children according to Psychology, 31, 221232.
mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins self reports. Bargh, J. A., & Thein, R. D. (1985). Individual construct accessibil-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 832848. ity, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of in-
Arseneault, L., Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, B., & Saucier, J. F. formation overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
(2002). Obstetrical complications and violent behaviors: Testing 49, 11291146.
two developmental pathways. Child Development, 73(2), 496508. Barkley, R. A., Shelton, T. L., Crosswait, C., Moorehouse, M.,
Fletcher, K., Barrett, S., et al. (2002). Preschool children with
Arseneault, L., Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, J. R., Seguin, J. R., &
disruptive behavior: Three-year outcome as a function of adaptive
Saucier, J. F. (2000). Minor physical anomalies and family adver-
disability. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 4567.
sity as risk factors for violent delinquency in adolescence. Ameri-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 917923. Bassarath, L. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of antisocial behavior.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 46, 728732.
Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral de-
velopment: Integrating social information processing and moral Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1988). Attachment and the development of
domain models. Child Development, 75, 9851002. behavior problems. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical
implications of attachment (pp. 253299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Arum, R., & Beattie, I. (1999). High school experience and the risk
of adult incarceration. Criminology, 37, 515537. Bates, J. E., Bayles, K., Bennett, D. S., Ridge, B., & Brown, M. M.
(1991). Origins of externalizing behavior problems at 8 years of age.
Asarnow, J. R., & Callan, J. W. (1985). Boys with peer adjustment
In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment
problems: Social cognitive processes. Journal of Consulting and
of childhood aggression (pp. 3120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clinical Psychology, 53, 8087.
Bates, J. E., Maslin, C. A., & Frankel, K. A. (1985). Attachment se-
Asberg, M. (1994). Monoamine neurotransmitters in human aggres-
curity, mother-child interaction, and temperament as predictors
siveness and violence: A selected review. Criminal Behaviour and
of behavior problem ratings at age 3 years. Growing points of at-
Mental Health, 4(4), 303327.
tachment theory and research: Monographs of the Society for Re-
Asher, S. R., & Renshaw, P. D. (1981). Children without friends: So- search in Child Development, 50(1/2, Serial No. 209).
cial knowledge and social skill training. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Baumrind, D. (1987). A developmental perspective on adolescent risk
Gottman (Eds.), The development of childrens friendships taking in contemporary America. In C. E. Irwin Jr. (Ed.), Adoles-
(pp. 273296). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. cent social behavior and health: Vol. 37. New directions for child
Aydin, O., & Markova, I. (1979). Attribution of popular and unpopu- development (pp. 3125). San Francisco: Josey-Bass.
lar children. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, Bayer, P., Pintoff, R., & Pozen, D. (2004). Building criminal capital
18, 291298. behind bars: Social learning in juvenile corrections. Unpublished
Bagwell, C. L., Coie, J. D., Terry, R. A., & Lochman, J. E. (2000). Peer manuscript, Yale University.
clique participation in middle childhood: Associations with socio- Becker, G. (1974). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. In
metric status and gender. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280305. G. Becker & W. Landes (Eds.), Essays in the economics of crime
Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of so- and punishment (pp. 154). New York: Macmillan.
cial information. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461484. Belfrage, H., Lidberg, L., & Oreland, L. (1992). Platelet monoamine
Ball, S. A., Tennen, H., Poling, J. C., Kranzler, H. R., & Rounsaville, oxidase activity in mentally disordered violent offenders. Acta
B. J. (1997). Personality, temperament, and character dimensions Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 85(3), 218221.
References 773

Bell, R. Q., & Harper, L. V. (1977). Child ef fects on adults. Hillsdale, Blumstein, A., & Wallman, J. (Eds.). (2000). The crime drop in Amer-
NJ: Erlbaum. ica. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Belsky, J. (2001). Emanuel Miller Lecture: Developmental risks Boivin, M., Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1995). Individual-group be-
(still) associated with early child care. Journal of Child Psychol- havioral similarity and peer status in experimental play groups of
ogy and Psychiatry, 42, 845859. boys: The social misfit revisited. Journal of Personality and Social
Benjet, C., & Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Spanking children: The contro- Psychology, 69, 269279.
versies, findings, and new directions. Clinical Psychology Review, Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., Thompson, W. W., & Kupersmidt, J. B.
23, 197224. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children experiencing
Berke, R. L. (1994, January 23). Crime joins economic issues as lead- persistent and intermittent family economic hardship. Child De-
ing worry, poll says. New York Times.1. velopment, 66, 11071129.
Berkowitz, L. (1962). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. Book, A. S., Starzyk, K. B., & Quinsey, V. L. (2001). The relation-
New York: McGraw-Hill. ship between testosterone and aggression: A meta-analysis. Ag-
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Towards a general theory of anger and emo- gression and Violent Behavior, 6(6), 579599.
tional aggression: Implications of the cognitive-neoassociationis- Booth, C. A., & Rose-Krasnor, L., McKinnon, J., & Rubin, K. H.
tic perspective for the analysis of anger and other emotions. In (1994). Predicting social adjustment in middle childhood: The
R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition: role of preschool attachment security and maternal styleFrom
Vol. 6. Perspectives on anger and emotion (pp. 145). Hillsdale, family to peer group: Relations between relationship systems
NJ: Erlbaum. [Special issue]. Social Development, 3, 189204.
Berman, A., & Siegal, A. W. (1976). Adaptive and learning skills in Borge, A. I. H., Rutter, M., Cote, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2004). Early
juvenile delinquents: A neuropsychological analysis. Journal of childcare and physical aggression: Differentiating social selection
Learning Disabilities, 9, 5158. and social causation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
45, 367376.
Bernberg, J. G. (2002). State reaction life-course outcomes and struc-
tural disadvantage: A panel study of the impact of formal criminal Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York:
labeling on the transition to adulthood. Albany, NY: State Univer- Basic Books.
sity of New York. Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and
Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Jiao, Z. (1999). Inf luences of friends child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371399.
and friendships on adjustment to junior high school. Merrill- Brain, P. F. (1994). Hormonal aspects of aggression and violence. In
Palmer Quarterly, 45, 1333. A. J. Reis Jr. & J. A. Roth (Eds.), Understanding and control of
Beyers, J. M., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2003). biobehavioral inf luences on violence (Vol. 2, pp. 177244). Wash-
Neighborhood structure, parenting processes, and the develop- ington, DC: National Academy Press.
ment of youths externalizing behaviors: A multilevel analysis. Brennan, P., Grekin, E. R., & Mednick, S. A. (1999). Maternal smok-
American Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 3553. ing during pregnancy and adult male criminal outcomes. Archives
Bierman, K. L. (1986). The relation between social aggression and of General Psychiatry, 56, 215219.
peer rejection in middle childhood. In R. J. Prinz (Ed.), Advances Brennan, P. A., Hall, J., Bor, W., Najman, J. M., & Williams, G.
in behavioral assessment of children and families (Vol. 2, (2003). Integrating biological and social processes in relationship
pp. 151178). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. to early-onset persistent aggression in boys and girls. Develop-
mental Psychology, 39, 309323.
Bierman, K. L., Smoot, D. L., & Aumiller, K. (1993). Characteristics
of aggressive-rejected, aggressive (nonrejected), and rejected Brennan, P., Mednick, S. A., & Raine, A. (1997). Biosocial interac-
(nonaggressive) boys. Child Development, 64, 139151. tions and violence: A focus on perinatal factors. In A. Raine, P.
Brennan, D. P. Farrington, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biosocial
Bierman, K. L., & Wargo, J. B. (1995). Predicting the longitudinal
bases of violence (pp. 163174). New York: Plenum Press.
course associated with aggressive-rejected, aggressive (non-re-
jected), and rejected (non-aggressive) status. Developmental Brickman, A. S., McManus, M., Grapentine, W. L., & Alessi, N.
Psychopathology, 7, 669682. (1984). Neuropsychological assessment of seriously delinquent
adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychia-
Bjrkqvist, K. (1985). Violent films, anxiety, and aggression. Helsin-
try, 23, 453457.
ski, Finland: Finnish Society of Sciences and Letters.
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A.,
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A criti- Fergusson, D., et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of child-
cal review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 593637. hood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site,
Blasi, A., & Oresick, R. J. (1986). Emotions and cognitions in self- cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 222245.
consistency. In D. J. Bearison & H. Zimiles (Eds.), Thought and Brown, A. L., Bransford, J. D., Ferrara, R. A., & Campione, J. C.
emotion (pp. 147165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In P. Mussen
Blum, H. M., Boyle, M. H., & Offord, D. R. (1988). Single-parent (Series Ed.) & J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), Hand-
families: Child psychiatric disorder and school performance. book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (4th ed.,
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- pp. 77166). New York: Wiley.
atry, 27, 214219. Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Advanced quantitative tech-
Blumstein, A. (2000). Disaggregating the violence trends. In A. niques in the social sciences: Vol. 1. Hierarchical linear models
Blumstein & J. Wallman (Eds.), The crime drop in America Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
(pp. 1344). New York: Cambridge University Press. Buehler, R. E., Patterson, G. R., & Furniss, J. M. (1966). The rein-
Blumstein, A., & Cohen, J. (1987). Characterizing criminal careers. forcement of behavior in institutional settings. Behavior Research
Science, 237, 985991. and Therapy, 4, 157167.
774 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Burks, V., Laird, R., Dodge, K., Pettit, G., & Bates, J. (1999). Knowl- Cassidy, J., Kirsh, S., Scolton, K., & Parke, R. (1996). Attachment
edge structures, social information processing, and childrens ag- and representations of peer relationships. Developmental Psychol-
gressive behavior. Social Development, 8, 220236. ogy, 32, 892904.
Burleson, B. R. (1982). The development of communication skills in Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Rodenberg, M., & Woodhouse, S. (2003, April).
childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 53, 15781588. Adolescent perceptions of parents: Associations with adolescent at-
Buss, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. New York: Wiley. tachment (AAI) and interaction with parents. Paper presented at
the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing
opment, Tampa, FL.
personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Castellanos, F. X., Elia, J., Kruesi, M. J., Gulotta, C. S., Mefford,
Cadoret, R. J., Cain, C. A., & Crowe, R. R. (1983). Evidence for gene-
I. N., Potter, W. Z., et al. (1994). Cerebrospinal f luid monoamine
environment interaction in the development of adolescent anti-
metabolites in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
social behavior. Behavior Genetics, 13(3), 301310.
Psychiatry Research, 52(3), 305316.
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stew- Chamberlain, P. (2003). Treating chronic juvenile of fenders: Ad-
art, M. A. (1995). Genetic-environmental interaction in the gene- vances made through the Oregon Multidimensional Treatment Fos-
sis of aggressivity and conduct disorders. Archives of General ter Care Model. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Psychiatry, 52, 916924. Association.
Cairns, R. B. (1979). Social development: The origins and plasticity Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1998). Comparison of two community
of interchanges. San Francisco: Freeman. alternatives to incarceration for chronic juvenile offenders. Jour-
Cairns, R. B. (1991). Multiple metaphors for a singular idea. Develop- nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 624633.
mental Psychology, 27, 2326. Chandler, M. J. (1973). Egocentrism and antisocial behavior: The as-
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., & Neckerman, H. J. (1989). Early school sessment and training of social perspective-taking skills. Devel-
drop-out: Configurations and determinants. Child Development, opmental Psychology, 9, 326332.
60, 14371452. Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. A.
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. D., & Gariepy, (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation
J. L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer sup- and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598606.
port or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24, 815823. Chen, X., Wang, L., Chen, H., & Liu, M. (2002). Noncompliance and
Campbell, S. B. (1990). Behavior problems in preschool children: child-rearing attitudes as predictors of aggressive behavior: A
Clinical and developmental issues. New York: Guilford Press. longitudinal study in Chinese children. International Journal of
Caplan, M., Vespo, J. E., Pedersen, J., & Hay, D. F. (1991). Conf lict Behavioral Development, 26, 225233.
over resources in small groups of 1- and 2-year-olds. Child Devel- Cicchetti, D. (1989). How research on child maltreatment has in-
opment, 62, 15131524. formed the study of child maltreatment: Perspectives from devel-
opmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.),
Caprara, G. V., Regalia, C., & Bandura, A. (2002). Longitudinal im-
Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and conse-
pact of perceived self-regulatory efficacy on violent conduct. Eu-
quences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 377431). New York:
ropean Psychologist, 7(1), 6369.
Cambridge University Press.
Caprara, G. V., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1996). Aggregation and amplifica-
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforce-
tion of marginal deviations in the social construction of personality
ment: Developmental changes in the association between aggres-
and maladjustment. European Journal of Personality, 10, 79110.
sion and social status. Child Development, 75, 147163.
Carey, G., & Goldman, D. (1997). The genetics of antisocial behav-
Clarke, R. V., & Cornish, D. B. (1983). Crime control in Britain: A re-
ior. In J. Breiling & D. M. Stoff (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial be-
view of policy research. Albany: State University of New York Press.
havior (pp. 243254). New York: Wiley.
Cleare, A. J., & Bond, A. J. (1995). The effect of tryptophan deple-
Carver, C. S., Ganellan, R. J., Froming, W. J., & Chambers, W.
tion and enhancement on subjective and behavioral aggression in
(1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility.
normal male subjects. Psychopharmacology, 118(1), 7281.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 403421.
Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., Bohman, M., & van Knoring, A. L.
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1987). Moving against the (1982). Predisposition to petty criminality in Swedish adoptees:
world: Life-course patterns of explosive children. Development Pt. 2. Cross-fostering analyses of gene-environmental interac-
Psychology, 23, 308313. tions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 12421247.
Caspi, A., Henry, B., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Tempera- Coccaro, E. F. (1989). Central serotonin and impulsive aggression.
mental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From British Journal of Psychiatry, 155(Suppl. 8), 5262.
age 3 to age 15. Child Development, 66, 5568.
Cohen, M. A. (1998). The monetary value of saving a high-risk youth.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 14, 533.
et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal- Cohen, M. A., Rust, R., Steen, S., & Tidd, S. (2004, February).
treated children. Science, 297, 851854. Willingness-to-pay for Crime Control Programs. Criminology,
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., 42(1), 86106.
Krueger, F. F., & Schmutte, P. S. (1994). Are some people crime- Cohen, P., & Brook, J. S. (1995). The reciprocal inf luence of punish-
prone? Replications of the personality-crime relationship across ment and child behavior disorder. In J. McCord (Ed.), Coercion
countries, genders, races, and methods. Criminology, 32, 163195. and punishment in long-term perspective (pp. 154164). Cam-
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Morgan, J., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., Arse- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
neault, L., et al. (2004). Maternal expressed emotion predicts Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Kasen, S., Velez, C. N., Hartmark, C., Johnson,
childrens antisocial behavior: Using MZ-twin differences to J., et al. (1993). An epidemiological study of disorders in late
identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Devel- childhood and adolescence: Pt. 1. Age and gender-specific preva-
opmental Psychology, 40, 149161. lence. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 34, 851867.
References 775

Coie, J. D. (2004). The impact of negative social experiences on the Crick, N., & Werner, N. (1998). Response decision processes in rela-
development of antisocial behavior. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. A. tional and overt aggression. Child Development, 69, 16301639.
Dodge (Eds.), Childrens peer relations: From development to in- Crick, N., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). The development of psycho-
tervention (pp. 243267). Washington, DC: American Psychologi- pathology in females and males: Current progress and future chal-
cal Association. lenges. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 719742.
Coie, J. D., Cillessen, A., Dodge, K. A., Hubbard, J., Schwartz, D., Crick, N. R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggres-
Lemerise, E., et al. (1999). It takes two to fight: A test of rela- sion, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of childrens future
tional factors and a method for assessing aggressive dyads. Devel- social adjustment. Child Development, 67, 23172327.
opmental Psychology, 35, 11791185.
Crick, N. R., & Bigbee, M. A. (1998). Relational and overt forms of
Coie, J. D., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1983). A behavioral analysis of emerg- peer victimization: A multi-informant approach. Journal of Con-
ing social status in boys groups. Child Development, 54, 14001416. sulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 610617.
Coie, J. D., Terry, R., Zakriski, A., & Lochman, J. E. (1995). Early
Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt
adolescent social inf luences on delinquent behavior. In J. McCord
aggression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33, 589600.
(Ed.), Coercion and punishment in long-term perspectives
(pp. 229244). New York: Cambridge University Press. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of
social information-processing mechanisms in childrens social
Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual emotion
adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101.
regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool
and early school age. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 118. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social-information-processing
mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Develop-
Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., Goodman, R., & Pickles, A. (2004).
ment, 67, 9931002.
Time trends in adolescent mental health. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry, 45, 13501362. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gen-
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1992). A develop- der, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development,
mental and clinical model for the prevention of conduct disorder: 66, 710722.
The Fas t Tr a ck program. Development and Psychopathology, 4, Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1990). Childrens perceptions of the
509527. outcomes of aggressive strategies: Do the ends justify being
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999a). Initial im- mean? Developmental Psychology, 26, 612620.
pact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems: Pt. 1. Crijnen, A. A., Achenbach, T. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (1997). Com-
The high-risk sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- parisons of problems reported by parents of children in 12 coun-
ogy, 67, 631647. tries: Total problems, externalizing, and internalizing. Journal
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999b). Initial im- of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
pact of the Fast Track prevention trial for conduct problems: Pt. 2. 36, 12691277.
Classroom effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitoring and
67, 648657. knowledge of children. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook on par-
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002a). Evaluation enting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed.,
of the first 3 years of the Fast Track prevention trial with children pp. 461484). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
at high risk for adolescent conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Crowther, J. K., Bond, L. A., & Rolf, J. E. (1981). The incidence,
Child Psychology, 30, 1935. prevalence, and severity of behavior disorders among preschool-
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002b). Using the aged children in day care. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
Fast Track randomized prevention trial to test the early-starter ogy, 9, 2342.
model of the development of serious conduct problems. Develop- Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. T. (2002). Effects of marital conf lict
ment and Psychopathology, 14, 927945. on children: Recent advances and emerging themes in process-ori-
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2004). The effects of ented research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
the Fast Track program on serious problem outcomes at the end of Allied Disciplines, 43, 3163.
elementary school. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psy- Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Papp, L. M. (in press).
chology, 33, 650661. Everyday marital conf lict and child aggression. Journal of Abnor-
Coon, H., Carey, G., Corley, R., & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Identifying mal Child Psychology.
children in the Colorado Adoption Project at risk for conduct dis- Curtis, N. M., Ronan, K. R., & Bourduin, C. M. (2004). Multisys-
order. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent temic treatment: A meta-analysis of outcome studies. Journal of
Psychiatry, 31, 503511. Family Psychology, 18, 411419.
Corrado, R. R., Vincent, G. M., Hart, S. D., & Cohen, I. M. (2004). David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (2000). Do positive self-perceptions have
Predictive validity of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth version a dark side? Examination of the link between perceptual bias and
for general and violent recidivism. Behavioral Sciences and the aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 327337.
Law, 22(1), 522. Deater-Deckard, K. (2000). Parenting and child behavioral adjust-
Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Burns, B., Stangl, D., Tweed, D., & Erk- ment in early childhood: A quantitative genetic approach to
lani, A. (1996). The Great Smoky Mountain study of youth: Pt. 1. studying family processes. Child Development, 71, 468484.
Prevalence and correlates of DSM-III-R disorders. Archives of Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior
General Psychiatry, 53, 11371143. problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation
Costello, E. J., Compton, S. N., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Rela- by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161175.
tionships between poverty and psychopathology: A natural experi- Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.
ment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 290, 20232029. (1996). Physical discipline among African-American and Euro-
Cowan, P. (1979). Piaget with feelings: Cognitive, social, and emo- pean-American mothers: Links to childrens externalizing behav-
tional dimensions. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. iors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 10651072.
776 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Dell Fitzgerald, P., & Asher, S. R. (1987, August /September). Ag- cess in the amplification of problem behavior. Journal of Adoles-
gressive-rejected childrens attributional biases about liked and cence, 27(5), 515530.
disliked peers. Paper presented at the 95th annual convention of Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., & Griesler, P. C. (1994). Peer adapta-
the American Psychological Association, New York, NY. tions in the development of antisocial behavior: A conf luence
Deluty, R. H. (1981). Alternative-thinking ability of aggressive, as- model. In L. R. Huesmann (Ed.), Aggressive behavior: Current
sertive, and submissive children. Cognitive Therapy and Research, perspectives (pp. 6195). New York: Plenum Press.
5, 309312. Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., & Kavanagh, K. (1991). An experi-
Deluty, R. H. (1983). Childrens evaluations of aggressive, assertive, mental test of the coercion model: Linking theory, measurement,
and submissive responses. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, and intervention. In J. McCord & R. Tremblay (Eds.), Preventing
12, 124129. antisocial behavior: Interventions from birth through adolescence
Denno, D. J. (1989). Biology, crime and violence: New evidence. Cam- (pp. 253282). New York: Guilford Press.
bridge: Cambridge University Press. Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and childrens aggressive be-
de Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., havior. Child Development, 51, 162170.
Schnyder, U., Buck, A., et al. (2004). The neural basis of altruis- Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status.
tic punishment. Science, 305, 12541258. Child Development, 54, 13861399.
Dery, M., Toupin, J., Pauze, R., Mercier, H., & Fortin, L. (1999). Neu- Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of so-
ropsychological characteristics of adolescents with conduct disor- cial competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota
ders: Association with attention-deficit-hyperactivity and Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77125). Hillsdale,
aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(3), 225236. NJ: Erlbaum.
Dietz, K. R., Lavigne, J. V., Arend, R., & Rosenbaum, D. (1997). Re- Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and
lation between intelligence and psychopathology among proactive aggression. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The
preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 99107. development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 201218).
Dionne, G., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R., Laplante, D., & Perusse, D. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2003). Physical aggression and expressive vocabulary in 19- Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms in the
month-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 39, 261273. cycle of violence. Science, 250, 16781683.
Dishion, T. J. (in press). Deviant peer contagion within interventions Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social information processing
and programs: An ecological framework for understanding inf lu- factors in reactive and proactive aggression in childrens peer
ence mechanisms. In K. A. Dodge & T. J. Dishion (Eds.), De- groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
viant peer contagion in therapeutic interventions: From 11461158.
documentation to policy. New York: Guilford Press. Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Pettit, G. S., & Price, J. M. (1990). Peer
Dishion, T. J., & Andrews, D. W. (1995). Preventing escalation in status and aggression in boys groups: Developmental and contex-
problem behaviors with high-risk adolescents: Immediate and 1- tual analyses. Child Development, 61, 12891309.
year outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S.,
63, 538548. Fontaine, R., et al. (2003). Peer rejection and social information-
Dishion, T. J., Andrews, D. W., & Crosby, L. (1995). Antisocial processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior
boys and their friends in early adolescence: Relationship charac- problems in children. Child Development, 74, 374393.
teristics, quality, and interactional process. Child Development, Dodge, K. A., Lockman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit,
66, 139151. G. S. (1997). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children
Dishion, T. J., & Bullock, B. M. (2002). Parenting and adolescent and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal
problem behavior: An ecological analysis of the nurturance hy- of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 3751.
pothesis. In J. G. Borkowski & S. L. Ramey (Eds.), Parenting and Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). The assess-
the childs world: Inf luences on academic, intellectual, and so- ment of intention-cue detection skills in children: Implications
cial-emotional development [Monograph]. (pp. 231249). Mah- for developmental psychopathology [Special issue]. Child Devel-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. opment, 55, 163173.
Dishion, T. J., French, D. C., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). The develop- Dodge, K. A., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision making
ment and ecology of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. processes in aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 421471). 90, 375379.
New York: Wiley. Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biophychosocial model of the
Dishion, T. J., & Kavanagh, K. (2000). A multilevel approach to fam- development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Devel-
ily-centered prevention in schools: Process and outcome. Addic- opmental Psychology, 39(2), 349371.
tive Behaviors, 25, 899911. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization medi-
Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions ators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child con-
harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist, duct problems. Child Development, 65, 649665.
54(9), 110. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social
Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitoring and the information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of
prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: A concep- early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnor-
tual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psy- mal Psychology, 104, 632643.
chology Review, 1, 6175. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, M. (1986).
Dishion, T. J., Nelson, S. N., & Bullock, B. M. (2004). Premature Social competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Re-
adolescent autonomy: Parent disengagement and deviant peer pro- search in Child Development, 51(2, Serial No. 213).
References 777

Dodge, K. A., & Price, J. M. (1994). On the relations between parental emotional expressivity, childrens regulation, and quality
social information processing and socially competent behavior of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology, 39,
in early school-aged children. Child Development, 65, 319.
13851398. Elliott, D. S. (1994). Serious violent offenders: Onset, developmental
Dodge, K. A., Price, J. M., Bachorowski, J. A., & Newman, J. P. course, and terminationThe American Society of Criminology
(1990). Hostile attributional biases in severely aggressive adoles- 1993 Presidential Address. Criminology, 32, 121.
cents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 385392. Elliott, D. S., & Huizinga, D. (1983). Social class and delinquent be-
Dodge, K. A., & Tomlin, A. (1987). Cue utilization as a mecha- havior. Criminology, 21, 149177.
nism of attributional bias in aggressive children. Social Cogni- Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Ageton, S. S. (1985). Explaining delin-
tion, 5, 280300. quency and drug use. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, Elliott, D. S., Huizinga, D., & Menard, S. (1989). Multiple problem
R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale youth: Delinquency, substance use, and mental health problems.
University Press. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and prevention. Elliott, F. A. (1978). Neurological aspects of antisocial behavior. In
New York: Oxford University Press. W. H. Reid (Ed.), The psychopath (pp. 146189). New York:
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, Brunner/ Mazel.
MA: Harvard University Press.
Erdley, C., & Asher, S. (1998). Linkages between childrens beliefs
Dunne, M., Martin, N., Statham, D., Slutske, W., Dinwiddie, S., Bu- about the legitimacy of aggression and their behavior. Social De-
cholz, K., et al. (1997). Genetic and environmental contributions velopment, 7, 321339.
to variance in age at first sexual intercourse. Psychological Sci-
Erdley, C., & Asher, S. (1999). A social goals perspective on chil-
ence, 8, 211216.
drens social competence. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Eddy, J. M., & Chamberlain, P. (2000). Family management and de- Disorders, 7, 156167.
viant peer association as mediators of the impact of treatment
Eron, L. D., Huesmann, L. R., Lefkowitz, M. M., & Walder, L. O.
condition on youth antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and
(1972). Does television violence cause aggression? American Psy-
Clinical Psychology, 68, 857863.
chologist, 27, 253263.
Eddy, J. M., Whaley, R. B., & Chamberlain, P. (2004). The prevention
Espelage, D. L., Holt, M. K., & Henkel, R. R. (2003). Examination of
of violent behavior by chronic and serious male juvenile offend-
peer-group contextual effects on aggression during early adoles-
ers: A 2-year follow-up of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of
cence. Child Development, 74, 205220.
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 12, 28.
Edelman, M. W. (1992). The measure of our success. Boston: Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1992). Young childrens emotional
Beacon Press. arousal and anger/aggressive behaviors. In A. Fraezek & H.
Zumkley (Eds.), Socialization and aggression (pp. 85102).
Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1999). Identify- Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag.
ing inmates at risk for disciplinary infractions: A comparison of
Fagot, B. I., Hagan, R., Youngblade, L. M., & Potter, L. (1989). A
two measures of psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law,
comparison of the play behaviors of sexually abused, physically
17, 435443.
abused, and nonabused preschool children. Topics in Early Child-
Egan, S., Monson, T., & Perry, D. (1998). Social-cognitive inf lu- hood Special Education, 9, 88100.
ences on change in aggression over time. Developmental Psychol-
Fagot, B. I., & Kavanagh, K. (1990). The prediction of antisocial be-
ogy, 34, 9961006.
havior from avoidant attachment classifications. Child Develop-
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, ment, 61, 864873.
S. A., Reiser, M., et al. (2001). The relations of regulation and
Farrington, D. P. (1989). Self-reported and official offending from
emotionality to childrens externalizing and internalizing prob-
adolescence to adulthood. In M. W. Klein (Ed.), Cross-national
lem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 72, 11121134.
research in self-reported crime and delinquency (pp. 399423).
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1999). Emotion, emotion-related regu- Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press.
lation, and quality of socioemotional function. In L. Balter & C. S. Farrington, D. P. (1992). Juvenile delinquency. In J. C. Coleman
Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contem- (Ed.), The school years (2nd ed., pp. 123163). London: Rout-
porary issues. Philadelphia: Psychology Press/ Taylor & Francis. ledge & Kegan Paul.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S., Guthrie, Farrington, D. P. (1993). The challenge of teenage antisocial behav-
I. K., Mazsk, P., et al. (1999). Prediction of elementary school ior. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Psychosocial disturbances in young people.
childrens socially appropriate and problem behavior from anger Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
reactions at age 4 to 6. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychol-
Farrington, D. P. (1995). The development of offending and antisocial
ogy, 20, 119142.
behavior from childhood: Key findings from the Cambridge study
Eisenberg, N., Pidada, S., & Liew, J. (2001). The relations of regula- in delinquent development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
tion and negative emotionality to Indonesian childrens social chiatry, 36, 136.
functioning. Child Development, 72, 17471763. Farrington, D. P. (1997). The relationship between low resting heart
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Cumberland, rate and violence. In A. Raine, P. A. Brennan, D. P. Farrington, &
A., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2004). The relations of effortful control S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biosocial bases of violence (pp. 89106).
and impulsivity to childrens resiliency and adjustment. Child De- New York: Plenum Press.
velopment, 75, 122. Farrington, D. P., & Hawkins, J. D. (1991). Predicting participation,
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, early onset and later persistence in officially recorded offending.
A., Reiser, M., et al. (2003). Longitudinal relations among Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 1, 133.
778 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Farrington, D. P., & Welsh, B. C. (2003). Family-based prevention of Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Social information
offending: A meta-analysis. Australian and New Zealand Journal processing and childrens social adjustment. In J. Kupersmidt &
of Criminology, 36(2), 127151. K. A. Dodge (Eds.), Childrens peer relations: From development
Feldman, R. A., Caplinger, T. E., & Wodarski, J. S. (1983). The St. to intervention (pp. 6984). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
Louis conundrum: The ef fective treatment of antisocial youths. logical Association.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Gilliom, M., Shaw, D. S., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon,
Ferguson, T. J., & Rule, B. G. (1988). Childrens evaluations of retal- J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys:
iatory aggression. Child Development, 59, 961968. Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. De-
Fergusson, D., Lynskey, M. T., & Horwood, L. J. (1996). Factors as- velopmental Psychology, 38, 222235.
sociated with continuity and changes in disruptive behavior pat- Gillmore, M. R., Hawkins, J. D., Day, L. E., & Catalano, R. F. (1992).
terns between childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Friendship and deviance: New evidence on an old controversy.
Child Psychology, 24, 533553. Journal of Early Adolescence, 12, 8095.
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1998). Exposure to interparental Giordano, P. G., Cernkovich, S. A., & Pugh, M. D. (1986).
violence in childhood and psychosocial adjustment in young adult- Friendships and delinquency. American Journal of Sociology,
hood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 339357. 91, 11701202.
Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2002). Male and female offend- Gladue, B. A., Boechler, M., & McCaul, K. D. (1989). Hormonal
ing trajectories. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 159177. response to competition in human males. Aggressive Behavior,
Fergusson, D. M., Woodward, L. J., & Horwood, J. (1998). Maternal 15, 409422.
smoking during pregnancy and psychiatric adjustment in late ado- Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1950). Unraveling juvenile delinquency.
lescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 721727. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1982). Empathy training and the Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1968). Delinquents and nondelinquents in
regulation of aggression: Potentialities and limitations. Academic perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Psychology Bulletin, 4, 399413. Goldstein, A. P. (1994). Delinquent gangs. In L. R. Huesmann (Ed.),
Finkelstein, J. W., Susman, E. J., Chinchilli, V. M., Kunselman, S. J., Aggressive behavior: Current perspectivesPlenum series in so-
dArcangelo, M. R., Schwab, J., et al. (1997). Estrogen or testos- cial/clinical psychology (pp. 255273). New York: Plenum Press.
terone increases self-reported aggressive behaviors in hypogo- Gordon, R. A., Lahey, B. B., Kawai, E., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-
nadal adolescents. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Loeber, M., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Antisocial behavior and gang
Metabolism, 82, 24332438. membership: Selection and socialization. Criminology, 42, 5587.
Foley, D. L., Eaves, L. J., Wormley, B., Silberg, J., Maes, H., Kuhn, Gouze, K. R. (1987). Attention and social problem solving as corre-
J., et al. (2004). Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase A lates of aggression in preschool males. Journal of Abnormal Child
genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Archives of General Psy- Psychology, 15, 181197.
chiatry, 61, 738744. Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1993). An attributional intervention to re-
Fontaine, R. G., Burks, V. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2002). Response deci- duce peer-directed aggression among African-American boys.
sion processes and externalizing behavior problems in adoles- Child Development, 64, 124138.
cents. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 107122. Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1994). Attributions of aggressive and
Forehand, R., & McMahon, R. (1981). The noncompliant child. New nonaggressive African-American male early adolescents: A study
York: Guilford Press. of construct accessibility. Developmental Psychology, 30, 365373.
Frick, P. J. (2001). Effective interventions for children and adoles- Graham, S., Hudley, C., & Williams, E. (1992). Attributional and emo-
cents with conduct disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, tional determinants of aggression among African-American and
46, 597608. Latino young adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 28, 731740.
Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L., Van Horn, Y., Granic, I., & T. J. Dishion. (2002, May). Measuring deviant talk be-
& Christ, M. A. G. (1993). Oppositional defiant disorder and con- tween adolescents as an attractor: Predictions to antisocial out-
duct disorder: Pt. 1. Meta-analytic review of factor analyses. Clin- comes (Invited talk). First Annual Workshop, Linking Dynamic
ical Psychology Review, 13, 319340. Systems and Reinforcement Mechanisms: Complementarities,
Frick, P. J., OBrien, B. S., Wooton, J. M., & McBurnett, K. (1994). Disparities and Data, Port Townsend, WA.
Psychopathy and conduct problems in children. Journal of Abnor- Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A. (1993). Promoting social and emo-
mal Psychology, 103(4), 700707. tional development in deaf children: The PATHS Project. Seattle,
Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press. WA: University of Washington Press.
Galen, B. R., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). A developmental investi- Greenberg, M. T., Spelz, M. L., & Deklyen, M. (1993). The role of at-
gation of social aggression among children. Developmental Psy- tachment in the early development of disruptive behavior prob-
chology, 33, 589600. lems. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 191213.
Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some Piagetian Gretton, H. M., Hare, R. D., & Catchpole, R. (2004). Psychopathy and
concepts. In P. Mussen (Series Ed.) & J. H. Flavell & E. M. Mark- offending from adolescence to adulthood: A 10-year follow up.
man (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive devel- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 636645.
opment (4th ed., pp. 167230). New York: Wiley. Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L. R., Tolan, P. H., Van Acker, R., & Eron,
Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Parental corporal punishment and associated L. D. (1995). Stressful events and individual beliefs as correlates
child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical of economic disadvantage and aggression among urban children.
review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539579. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 518528.
Giancola, P., & Zeichner, A. (1994). Neuropsychological perfor- Guerra, N. G., & Slaby, R. G. (1989). Evaluative factors in social
mance on tests of frontal-lobe functioning and aggressive behav- problem solving by aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Child
ior in men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 832835. Psychology, 17, 277289.
References 779

Gunnar, M. R., Brodersen, L., & Krueger, K. (1996). Dampening of Henggeler, S. W., Schoenwald, S. K., Bourduin, C. M., Rowland,
adrenocortical responses during infancy: Normative changes and M. D., & Cunningham, P. B. (1998). Multisystemic treatment of
individual differences. Child Development, 67, 877889. antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. New York: Guil-
Halperin, J. M., Sharma, V., Siever, L. J., Schwartz, S. T., Matier, K., ford Press.
Wornell, G., et al. (1994). Serotonergic function in aggression and Henry, B., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. W. (1996). Tem-
non-aggressive boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. peramental and familial predictors of violent and nonviolent
American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 243248. criminal convictions: From age 3 to 18. Developmental Psychol-
Hammond, W. R., & Yung, B. R. (1991). Preventing violence in at- ogy, 32, 614623.
risk African-American youth. Journal of Health Care for the Poor Herbert, J., & Martinez, M. (2001). Neural mechanisms underlying
and Underserved, 2, 116. aggressive behavior. In J. Hill & B. Maughan (Eds.), Conduct dis-
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare PCL-R (2nd ed.). Toronto, Ontario: orders in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
Multi-Health Systems. versity Press.
Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., & McNeilly- Hinshaw, S. P. (2002). Intervention research, theoretical mecha-
Choque, M. K. (1998). Overt and relational aggression in Russian nisms, and causal processes related to externalizing behavior pat-
nursery-school-age children: Parenting style and marital linkages. terns. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 789818.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 687697. Hinshaw, S. P., & Anderson, C. A. (1996). Conduct and oppositional
Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. defiant disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child Psy-
Child Development, 53, 8997. chology (pp. 113149). New York: Guilford Press.
Hartup, W. W. (1974). Aggression in childhood: Developmental per- Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Wells, K. C., Kraemer, H. C., Abikoff,
spectives. American Psychologist, 29, 336341. H. B., Arnold, L. E., et al. (2000). Family processes and treatment
outcome in the MTA: Negative/ineffective parenting practices in
Hartup, W. W., & deWit, J. (1974). The development of aggression:
relation to multimodal treatment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
Problems and perspectives. In J. deWit & W. W. Hartup (Eds.),
chology, 28, 555568.
Determinants and origins of aggressive behavior (pp. 595615).
The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Haselager, G. J. T., Cillessen, A. H. N., Hartup, W. W., van
Lieshout, C. F. M., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. A. (2002). Hetero- Hodgins, S., Kratzer, L., & McNeil, T. F. (2001). Obstetric complica-
geneity among peer rejected boys across middle childhood: De- tions, parenting, and risk of criminal behavior. Archives of Gen-
velopmental pathways of social behavior. Developmental eral Psychiatry, 58, 746752.
Psychology, 38, 446456. Holmberg, M. S. (1977). The development of social interchange pat-
terns from 12 months to 42 months: Cross-sectional and short-
Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., Kosterman, R., Abbott, R., & Hill,
term longitudinal analyses. Doctoral dissertation. Chapel Hill,
K. G. (1999). Preventing adolescent health-risk behaviors by
NC: University of North Carolina.
strengthening protection during childhood. Archives of Paedi-
atrics and Adolescent Medicine, 153, 226234. Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994). Typologies of male
batterers: Three subtypes and the differences among them. Psy-
Hawkins, J. D., & Lishner, D. M. (1987). Schooling and delinquency.
chological Bulletin, 116, 476497.
In E. H. Johnson (Ed.), Handbook on crime and delinquency pre-
vention (pp. 179221). New York: Greenwood Press. Horn, S. S. (2003). Adolescents reasoning about exclusion from so-
cial groups. Developmental Psychology, 39, 7184.
Hawkins, J. D., von Cleve, E., & Catalano, R. F. (1991). Reducing
Howell, J. C., Krisberg, B., & Jones, M. (1995). Trends in juvenile
early childhood aggression: Results of a primary prevention pro-
crime and youth violence. In J. C. Howell, B. Krisberg, J. D.
gramme. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Hawkins, & J. J. Wilson (Eds.), Serious, violent, and chronic juve-
cent Psychiatry, 30, 208217.
nile of fenders (pp. 135). Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA.
Hawkins, J. D., & Weis, J. G. (1985). The social development model:
Hubbard, J. A., Dodge, K. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Coie, J. D., &
An integrated approach to delinquency prevention. Journal of Pri-
Schwartz D. (2001). The dyadic nature of social information-pro-
mary Prevention, 6, 7395.
cessing in boys reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Per-
Hawley, P. H. (2003). Strategies of control, aggression, and morality sonality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 268280.
in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Experi- Hudley, C. A., & Graham, S. (1993). An attributional intervention to
mental Child Psychology, 85, 213235. reduce peer-directed aggression among African-American boys.
Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., & Pasupathi, M. (2002). Winning Child Development, 64, 124138.
friends and inf luencing peers: Strategies of peer inf luence in Huesmann, L. R. (1986). Psychological processes promoting the rela-
late childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Develop- tion between exposure to media violence and aggressive behavior
ment, 26, 466474. by the viewer. Journal of Social Issues, 42, 125139.
Hay, D. F., Nash, A., & Pedersen, J. (1983). Interactions between 6- Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information-processing model for the
month-olds. Child Development, 54, 557562. development of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 1324.
Hay, D. F., & Ross, H. S. (1982). The social nature of early conf lict. Huesmann, L. R. (1998). The role of social information processing
Child Development, 53, 105113. and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habit-
Henggeler, S. W., Melton, G. B., Brondino, M. J., Scherer, D. G., & ual aggressive behavior. In R. Green & E. Donnerstein (Eds.),
Hanley, J. H. (1997). Multisystemic therapy with violent and Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for social
chronic juvenile offenders and their families: The role of treat- policy (pp. 73109). New York: Academic Press.
ment fidelity in successful dissemination. Journal of Consulting Huesmann, L. R., & Eron, L. D. (1986). Television and the aggressive
and Clinical Psychology, 65, 821833. child: A cross-national perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
780 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Lefkowitz, M. M., & Walder, L. O. Jessor, R., Donovan, J. E., & Costa, F. M. (1991). Beyond adoles-
(1984). Stability of aggression over time and generations. Devel- cence: Problem behavior and young adult development. New York:
opmental Psychology, 20, 11201134. Academic Press.
Huesmann, L. R., Lagerspetz, & Eron, L. D. (1984). Intervening vari- Jimack, P. (1911). Introduction to Rousseaus Emile. London: Dent.
ables in the TV violence aggression relation: Evidence from two Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in ado-
countries. Developmental Psychology, 20, 746775. lescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 427436.
Huesmann, L. R., & Miller, L. S. (1994). Long-term effects of re- Kaplan, H. B., & Liu, X. (1994). A longitudinal analysis of mediating
peated exposure to media violence in childhood. In L. R. Hues- variables in the drug use-dropping out relationship. Criminology,
mann (Ed.), Aggressive behavior: Current perspectives 32, 415439.
(pp. 153186). New York: Plenum Press.
Kashani, J. H., & Shepperd, J. A. (1990). Aggression in adolescents:
Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C., & Eron, L. D. (2003). The role of social support and personality. Canadian Journal of
Longitudinal relations between childrens exposure to TV vio- Psychiatry, 35, 311315.
lence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adult-
hood: 19771992. Developmental Psychology, 39, 201221. Kaukiainen, A., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., Osterman, K., Salmi-
valli, C., Rothberg, S., et al. (1999). The relationship between so-
Huizinga, D., & Dunford, F. W. (1985, November). The delinquent
cial intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression.
behavior of arrested individuals. Paper presented at the 1985
Aggressive Behavior, 25, 8189.
annual meeting of the Academy of Criminal Justice Sciences,
Las Vegas, NV. Kazdin, A. E. (2003a). Problem-solving skills training and parent
management training for conduct disorder. In A. E. Kazdin & J. R.
Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, & E. (1993). Aggressive versus
Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and
withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in peer and self-
adolescents (pp. 241262). New York: Guilford Press.
perceptions in multiple domains. Child Development, 64, 879896.
Kazdin, A. E. (2003b). Psychotherapy for children and adolescents.
Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., Werthamer, L., & Kellam, S. (2001). The
Annual Review in Psychology, 54, 253276.
distal impact of two first-grade preventive interventions on con-
duct problems and disorder in early adolescence. Journal of Emo- Kazdin, A. E., & Whitley, M. K. (2003). Treatment of parental stress
tional and Behavioral Disorders, 9, 146160. to enhance therapeutic change among children referred for ag-
gressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
Ialongo, N. S., Werthamer, L., Kellam, S. G., Brown, C. H., Wang, S.,
cal Psychology, 71, 504515.
& Lin, Y. (1999). Proximal impact of two first-grade preventive
interventions on the early risk behaviors for later substance Keenan, K., Gunthorpe, D., & Young, D. (2002). Patterns of cortical
abuse, depression, and antisocial behavior. American Journal of reactivity in African-American neonates from low-income envi-
Community Psychology, 27, 599641. ronments. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, 113.
Iannotti, R. J. (1978). Effects of role-taking experiences on role-tak- Keenan, K., Loeber, R., Zhang, Q., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & van
ing, empathy, altruism, and aggression. Developmental Psychol- Kammen, W. B. (1995). The inf luence of deviant peers on the de-
ogy, 14, 119124. velopment of boys disruptive and delinquent behavior: A tempo-
ral analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 715726.
Ingoldsby, E. M., & Shaw, D. S. (2002). Neighborhood contextual fac-
tors and early-starting antisocial pathways. Clinical Child and Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. S. (2003). Starting at the beginning: Explor-
Family Psychology Review, 5, 2155. ing the etiology of antisocial behavior in the first years of life. In
B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct
Jaffee, S. R., Capsi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Dodge, K. A., Rutter, M., &
disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 153181). New York: Guil-
Taylor, A. (in press). Nature nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities in-
ford Press.
teract with physical maltreatment to promote conduct problems.
Development and Psychopathology. Keenan, K., Shaw, D. S., Delliquadri, E., Giovanelli, J., & Walsh, B.
(1998). Evidence for the continuity of early problem behaviors:
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Polo-Tomas, M., Price, T. S.,
Application of a developmental model. Journal of Abnormal Child
& Taylor, A. (2004a). The limits of child effects: Evidence for
Psychology, 26, 441452.
genetically mediated child effects on corporal punishment, but
not on physical maltreatment. Developmental Psychology, 40, Keith, C. R. (Ed.). (1984). The aggressive adolescent: Clinical per-
10471058. spectives. New York: Free Press.
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004b). Physical Kellam, S., Ling, X., Merisca, R., Brown, C. H., & Ialongo, N. (1998).
maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environ- The effect of the level of aggression in the first grade classroom
mentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, on the course and malleability of aggressive behavior into middle
113(1), 4455. school. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 165185.
Jaffee, S. R., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & Arseneault, L. Kellam, S. G., & Rebok, G. W. (1992). Building developmental and
(2002). Inf luence of adult domestic violence on childrens inter- ethiological theory through epidemiologically based preventive
nalizing and externalizing problems: An environmentally inform- intervention trials. In J. McCord & R. E. Tremblay (Eds.), Pre-
ative twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and venting antisocial behavior: Interventions from birth through ado-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 10951103. lescence (pp. 162195). New York: Guilford Press.
Jencks, C., & Mayer, S. E. (1990). The social consequences of grow- Kelley, H. H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American
ing up in a poor neighborhood. In L. E. Lynn & M. McGeary Psychologist, 28, 107128.
(Eds.), Inner-city poverty in the United States (pp. 117135). Kemppainen, L., Jokelainen, J., Jaervelin, M. R., Isohanni, M., &
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Raesaenen, P. (2001). The one-child family and violent criminal-
Jenkins, S., Bax, M., & Hart, H. (1980). Behaviour problems in preschool ity: A 31-year follow-up study of the Northern Finland 1966 birth
children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 518. cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 960962.
References 781

Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The Kupersmidt, J. B., Bryant, D., & Willoughby, M. (2000). Prevalence
structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common of aggressive behaviors in Head Start and community child care
psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. programs. Behavior Disorders, 26, 4252.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(9), 929937. Kupersmidt, J. B., & Coie, J. D. (1990). Preadolescent peer status,
Kerckhoff, A. C. (1988). Effects of ability and grouping in British aggression, and school adjustment as predictors of externalizing
secondary schools. American Sociological Review, 51, 842858. problems in adolescence. Child Development, 61, 13501362.
Killen, M., Lee-Kim, J., McGlothlin, H., & Stangor, C. (2002). How Kupersmidt, J., & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.). (2004). Childrens peer rela-
children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion. tions: From development to intervention to policyA festschrift to
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, honor John D. Coie. Washington, DC: American Psychological
67(4, Serial No. 271). Association.
Kim, Y. S., Koh, Y. J., & Leventhal, B. L. (2004). Prevalence of bul- Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, making friends,
lying in Korean middle school students. Archives of Pediatric and and being liked by peers in the classroom: Predictors of childrens
Adolescent Medicine, 158, 737741. early school adjustment. Child Development, 61, 312331.
Kindlon, D. J., Tremblay, R. E., Mezzacappa, E., Earls, F., Laurent, Ladd, G. W., & Oden, S. (1979). The relationship between peer ac-
D., & Schaal, B. (1995). Longitudinal patterns of heart rate and ceptance and childrens ideas about helpfulness. Child Develop-
fighting behavior in 9- through 12-year-old boys. Journal of the ment, 50, 402408.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(3), Lahey, B., & Loeber, R. (1994). Framework for a developmental
371377. model of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. In D.
Kingston, L., & Prior, M. (1995). The development of patterns of sta- Routh (Ed.), Disruptive behavior disorders in childhood
ble, transient, and school-age onset aggressive behavior in young (pp. 139180). New York: Plenum Press.
children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles- Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Hart, E. L., Frick, P. J., Applegate, B.,
cent Psychiatry, 34, 348452. Zhang, Q., et al. (1995). Four-year longitudinal study of conduct
disorder in boys: Patterns and predictors of persistence. Journal
Kitzman, H., Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Hanks, C., Cole, R.,
of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 8393.
Tatelbaum, R., et al. (1997). Effect of prenatal and infancy
home visitation by nurses on pregnancy outcomes, childhood Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2003). Par-
injuries, and repeated childbearing: A randomized controlled ents monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents delinquent
trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, behavior: Evidence of correlated developmental changes and re-
644 652. ciprocal inf luences. Child Development, 74, 752768.
Klein, M. (in press). Peer effects in naturally occurring groups: The Lally, J. R., Mangione, P. L., & Honig, A. S. (1988). Syracuse Univer-
case for street gangs. In K. A. Dodge & T. J. Dishion (Eds.), De- sity Family Development Research Program: Long-range impact
viant peer contagion in therapeutic interventions: From documen- of an early intervention with low-income children and their fami-
tation to policy. New York: Guilford Press. lies. In D. R. Powell (Ed.), Parent education as early childhood in-
tervention: Emerging directions in theory, research and practice
Klimes-Dougan, B., & Kopp, C. B. (1999). Childrens conf lict tactics (pp. 79104). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
with mothers: A longitudinal investigation of the toddler and pre-
Lancelotta, G. X., & Vaughn, S. (1989). Relation between types of
school years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 226241.
aggression and sociometric status: Peer and teacher perceptions.
Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 8690.
with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age 5. Devel-
Lansford, J. E., Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., &
opmental Psychology, 33, 228240.
Pettit, G. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in the link between physi-
Kohlberg, L. (1986). The Philosophy of Moral Development. San Fran- cal discipline and later adolescent externalizing behaviors. Jour-
cisco: Harper & Row. nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 801812.
Kosson, D. S., Cyterski, T. D., Steuerwald, B. L., Neumann, C. S., & Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., Crozier, J., &
Walker-Matthews, S. (2002). The reliability and validity of the Kaplow, J. (2002). A 12-year prospective study of the long-term
Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL:YV) in nonincarcer- effects of early child physical maltreatment on psychological, be-
ated adolescent males. Psychological Assessment, 14(1), 97109. havioral, and academic problems in adolescence. Archives of Pedi-
Krueger, R. F. (1999). Personality traits in late adolescence predict atrics and Adolescent Medicine, 156, 824830.
mental disorders in early adulthood: A prospective-epidemiologi- Larzelere, R. E., & Patterson, G. R. (1990). Parental management:
cal study. Journal of Personality, 67, 3965. Mediators of the effect of socioeconomic status on early delin-
Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., White, J., & Stouthamer- quency. Criminology, 28, 301323.
Loeber, M. (1996). Delay of gratification, psychopathology, and Lau, M. A., Pihl, R. O., & Peterson, J. B. (1995). Provocation, acute
personality: Is low self-control specific to externalizing prob- alcohol intoxication, cognitive performance and aggression. Jour-
lems? Journal of Personality, 64, 107129. nal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 150155.
Kruesi, M. J., Hibbs, E. D., Zahn, T. P., Keysor, C. S., Hamburger, Lavallee, K. L., Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., & Conduct Problems Pre-
S. D., Bartko, J. J., et al. (1992). A 2-year prospective follow-up vention Research Group. (2005). The impact of first grade friend-
study of children and adolescents with disruptive behavior disor- ship group experiences on child social outcomes in the Fast Track
ders: Prediction by cerebrospinal f luid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic Program. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 307324.
acid, homovanillic acid, and autonomic measures? Archives of Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. (2000). An integrated model of emo-
General Psychiatry, 49(6), 429435. tion processes and cognition in social information processing.
Kruesi, M. J., Swedo, S., Leonard, H., & Rubinow, D. R. (1990). CSF Child Development, 71, 107118.
somatostatin in childhood psychiatric disorders: A preliminary Lemerise, E. A., & Dodge, K. A. (2000). The development of anger
investigation. Psychiatry Research, 33(3), 277284. and hostile interactions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones
782 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

(Ed.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 594606). New York: neighborhood context on offending: The effects of impulsivity are
Guilford Press. stronger in poorer neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Lengua, L. L., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Temperament as a ogy, 109, 563-574.
predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing contamina- Lynam, D. R., & Henry, B. (2001). The role of neuropsychological
tion of measures. Child Development, 69, 164181. deficits in conduct disorders. In J. Hill & B. Maughan (Eds.),
Leve, L. D., & Chamberlain, P. (2005). Intervention outcomes for Conduct disorders in childhood and adolescence (pp. 235263).
girls referred from Juvenile Justice: Effects on delinquency. Jour- New York: Cambridge University Press.
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 339-347. Lynam, D. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. A. (1993).
Lewis, M., Feiring, C., McGuffog, C., & Jaskir, J. (1984). Predicting Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race,
psychopathology in 6-year-olds from early social relations. Child test motivation, school achievement, or self-control? Journal of
Development, 55, 123136. Abnormal Psychology, 102, 187196.
Lipsey, M. W. (in press). The effects of community-based group Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disorganized
treatment for delinquency: A meta-analytic search for cross-study infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial prob-
generalizations. In K. A. Dodge & T. J. Dishion (Eds.), Deviant lems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the pre-
peer contagion in therapeutic interventions: From documentation school classroom. Child Development, 64, 572585.
to policy. New York: Guilford Press. Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Davidson, C. (1995, April).
Little, T. D., Brauner, J., Jones, S. M., Nock, M. K., & Hawley, P. H. Disorganized attachment strategies and mental lag in infancy:
(2003). Rethinking aggression: A typological examination of the Prediction of externalizing problems at seven. Paper presented at
functions of aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 343369. biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ment, Indianapolis, IN.
Little, T. D., Jones, S. M., Henrich, C. C., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Dis-
entangling the whys from the whats of aggressive behavior. Lyoo, I. K., Lee, H. K., Jung, J. H., Noam, G. G., & Renshaw, P. F.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 122133. (2002). White matter hyperintensities on magnetic resonance im-
aging of the brain in children with psychiatric disorders. Compre-
Lochman, J. E., & Wells, K. C. (2004). The coping power program for
hensive Psychiatry, 43, 361368.
preadolescent aggressive boys and their parents: Outcome effects
at the 1-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- MacBrayer, E. K., Milich, R., & Hundley, M. (2003). Attributional
chology, 72, 571578. biases in aggressive children and their mothers. Journal of Abnor-
mal Psychology, 112, 698708.
Loeber, R. (1985). Patterns and development of antisocial child be-
havior. Annals of Child Development, 2, 77116. MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Rabiner, D., & Starnes, R. (1999). Predicting
boys social acceptance and aggression: The role of mother-child
Loeber, R., Farrington, D. P., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & van Kam- interactions and boys beliefs about peers. Developmental Psy-
men, W. B. (1998). Antisocial behavior and mental health prob- chology, 35, 632639.
lems: Explanatory factors in childhood and adolescence. Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum. Magdol, L., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. (1998). Hitting without
a license: Testing explanations for differences in partner abuse
Loeber, R., Lahey, B. B., & Thomas, C. (1991). Diagnostic conun- between young adult daters and cohabitators. Journal of Marriage
drum of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder. Jour- and the Family, 60, 4155.
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 379390.
Mahler, M. (1968). On human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of indi-
Loeber, R., & Schmaling, K. B. (1985). Empirical evidence for overt vidualization. New York: International Universities Press.
and covert patterns of antisocial conduct problems: A meta-analy-
Malone, P. S., Lansford, J. E., Castellino, D. R., Berlin, L. J.,
sis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 337352.
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., et al. (2004). Divorce and child
Loeber, R., Tremblay, R. E., Gagnon, C., & Charlebois, P. (1989). behavior problems: Applying latent change score models
Continuity and desistance in disruptive boys early fighting in to life event data. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(3),
school. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 3950. 401 423.
Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. New York: Harcourt. Manley, J. T., Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1994). The impact of sub-
Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi-Schwartz, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., type, frequency, chronicity, and severity of child maltreatment on
Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., et al. (2003). Child care quality mat- social competence and behavior problems. Development and
ters: How conclusions may vary with context. Child Develop- Psychopathology, 6, 121143.
ment, 74, 10211033. Maughan, B., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2004). Prenatal
Lueger, R., & Gill, K. (1990). Frontal-lobe cognitive dysfunction in smoking and early childhood conduct problems. Archives of Gen-
conduct disorder adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, eral Psychiatry, 61, 836843.
46, 696706. McCall, N. (1994). Makes me wanna holler: A young black man in
Luntz, B. K., & Widom, C. S. (1994). Antisocial personality disor- America. New York: Vintage Books.
ders in abused and neglected children grown up. American Jour- McCarton, C. M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Wallace, I. F., Bauer, C. R., Ben-
nal of Psychiatry, 151, 670674. nett, F. C., Bernbaum, J. C., et al. (1997). Results at age 8 years
Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the f ledg- of early intervention for low-birth-weight premature infants: The
ling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Psy- infant health and development program. Journal of the American
chology, 106(3), 425438. Medical Association, 277, 126132.
Lynam, D. R. (2002). Fledgling psychopathy: A view from personal- McCaul, K. D., Gladue, B. A., & Joppa, M. (1992). Winning, losing,
ity theory. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 255259. mood, and testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 26(2), 486504.
Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Wikstrm, P. O., Loeber, R., McCord, J. (1991). Questioning the value of punishment. Social Prob-
& Novak, S. P. (2000). The interaction between impulsivity and lems, 38, 167179.
References 783

McFall, R. M. (1982). A review and reformulation of the concept of and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. Cambridge, En-
social skills. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 133. gland: Cambridge University Press.
McLoyd, V. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black fami- Moffitt, T. E., Gabrielli, W. F., Mednick, S. A., & Schulsinger, F.
lies and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioe- (1981). Socioeconomic status IQ and delinquency. Journal of Ab-
motional development. Child Development, 61, 311346. normal Psychology, 90, 152156.
McNeilly-Choque, M. K., Hart, C. H., Robinson, C. C., Nelson, L. J., Moffitt, T. E., & Henry, B. (1989). Neuropsychological assessment of
& Olsen, S. F. (1996). Overt and relational aggression on the executive functions in self-reported delinquents. Development
playground: Correspondence among different informants. Journal and Psychopathology, 1, 105118.
of Research in Childhood Education, 11, 4767. Moffitt, T. E., Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., & Fagan, J. (2000). Partner
Mednick, S. A., & Christiansen, K. O. (1977). Biosocial bases of abuse and general crime: How are they the same? How are they
criminal behavior. Oxford, England: Gardner Press. different? Criminology, 38, 199232.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and man- Moffitt, T. E., Lynam, D. R., & Silva, P. A. (1994). Neuropsycholog-
ual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578. ical tests predicting persistent male delinquency. Criminology,
Menard, S., & Elliott, D. S. (1994). Delinquent bonding, moral be- 32, 277300.
liefs, and illegal behavior: A three-wave panel model. Justice Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988a). IQ and delinquency: A direct
Quarterly, 11, 173188. test of the differential detection hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal
Metropolitan Area Child Study Research Group. (2002). A cognitive- Psychology, 97, 330333.
ecological approach to preventing aggression in urban settings: Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1988b). Self-reported delinquency,
Initial outcomes for high-risk children. Journal of Consulting and neuropsychological deficit, and history of attention deficit disor-
Clinical Psychology, 70, 179194. der. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 553569.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. Morash, M., & Rucker, L. (1989). An exploratory study of the con-
New York: Harper & Row. nection of mothers age at child-bearing to her childrens delin-
Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). Structural models of personal- quency in four data sets. Crime and Delinquency, 35, 4593.
ity and their relation to antisocial behavior: A meta-analytic re-
Multimodal Treatment for Attention-Deficit /Hyperactivity Disorder
view. Criminology, 39, 765792.
Cooperative Group (MTA). (1999). Fourteen-month randomized
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit hyperac-
Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality tivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 10731086.
dimensions: Can the five-factor model adequately represent psy-
Murphy, B. C., & Eisenberg, N. (2002). An integrative examination
chopathy? Journal of Personality, 69(2), 253276.
of peer conf lict: Childrens reported goals, emotions and behav-
Miller, M., Azrael, D., & Hemenway, D. (2002). Firearm availability iors. Social Development, 11, 534557.
and unintentional firearm deaths, suicide, and homicide among 5-
Murrie, D. C., Cornell, D. G., Kaplan, S., McConville, D., & Levy-
to 14-year-olds. Journal of TraumaInjury, Infection, and Criti-
Elkon, A. (2004). Psychopathy scores and violence among juve-
cal Care, 52, 267275.
nile offenders: A multi-measure study. Behavioral Sciences and
Miller-Johnson, S., & Costanzo, P. (2004). If you cant beat
the Law, 22(1), 4967.
em . . . Induce them to join you: Peer-based interventions during
adolescence. In J. Kupersmidt & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), Childrens Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A
peer relations: From development to intervention (pp. 209222). semi-parametric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods,
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 4, 139177.
Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. In L. Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. (1999). Trajectories of boys physical
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physi-
7, pp. 249292). New York: Academic Press. cally violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Develop-
ment, 70, 11811196.
Moffitt, T. E. (1990). Juvenile delinquency and attention deficit dis-
orders: Boys developmental trajectories from age 3 to age 15. Nasby, W., Hayden, B., & DePaulo, B. M. (1979). Attributional bias
Child Development, 61, 893910. among aggressive boys to interpret unambiguous social stimuli as
displays of hostility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 459468.
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
antisocial behavior: A development taxonomy. Psychological Re- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early
view, 100, 674701. Child Care Research Network. (2004). Trajectories of physical ag-
Moffitt, T. E. (2005). The new look of behavioral-genetics in devel- gression from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Monographs of the
opmental psychopathology: Gene-environment interplay in anti- Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4, Serial No. 278).
social behaviors. Unpublished manuscript, Institue of Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Engle-
Psychiatry, London. wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor. Boulder, CO:
life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial path- Westview Press.
ways among males and females. Development and Psychopathol- Novak, S. P. (2000). The interaction between impulsivity and neigh-
ogy, 13, 135151. borhood context on offending: The effects of impulsivity are
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Fawcett, P. (1997). Whole blood sero- stronger in poorer neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
tonin and family background relate to male violence. In A. Raine, ogy, 109, 563574.
P. A. Brennan, D. P. Farrington, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Bioso- Nucci, L. (2001). Education in the moral domain. New York: Cam-
cial bases of violence (pp. 321340). New York: Plenum Press. bridge University Press.
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Sex dif- OConnor, T. G., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E., & Plomin,
ferences in antisocial behavior: Conduct disorder, delinquency, R. (1998). Co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and antisocial
784 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

behavior in adolescence: A common genetic liability. Journal of Perry, D. G., Perry, L. C., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social
Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 2737. learning mediators of aggression. Child Development, 57,
ODonnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., Abbott, R. D., & Day, 700711.
L. E. (1995). Preventing school failure, drug use and delinquency Petrides, M. (1985). Deficits on conditional associative-learning
among low income children: Long-term intervention in elemen- tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsy-
tary schools. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 87100. chologia, 23, 601614.
Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., & Racine, Y. A. (1989). Ontario Child Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Evans, A., & Meyer, E. (1993). Dissoci-
Health Study: Correlates of conduct disorder. Journal of the ation of human mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory pro-
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, cessing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
856860. 90, 873877.
Ogbu, J. U. (1990). Overcoming racial barriers to equal access. In I. Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks
Goodlad & P. Keating (Eds.), Access to knowledge: An agenda for after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsycholo-
our nations schools (pp. 5989). New York: College Entrance Ex- gia, 20, 249262.
amination Board.
Petrosino, A., Turpin-Petrosino, C., & Finckenauer, J. O. (2000).
Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Cole, R., Eckenrode, J., Kizman, H., Well-meaning programs can have harmful effects!: Lessons from
Luckey, D., et al. (1998). Long-term effects of nurse home visita- experiments in Scared Straight and other like programs. Crime
tion on childrens criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year fol- and Delinquency, 46, 354379.
low-up of a randomized trial. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 280, 12381244. Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parent-
ing, ecological context, and childrens adjustment. Child Develop-
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we
ment, 68, 908923.
can do. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Meece, D. W. (1999). The
ONeil, M. L., Lidz, V., & Heilbrun, K. (2003). Adolescents with psy-
impact of after-school peer contact on early adolescent external-
chopatic characteristics in a substance abusing cohort: Treatment
izing problems is moderated by parental monitoring, neighbor-
process and outcomes. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 299313.
hood safety, and prior adjustment. Child Development, 70,
Osterman, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., Kaukiainen, A.,
768778.
Huesmann, L. R., & Fraczek, A. (1994). Peer and self-estimated
aggression and victimization in 8-year-old children from five eth- Pettit, G. S., Laird, R. D., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Criss, M. M.
nic groups. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 411428. (2001). Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental
monitoring and psychological control in early adolescence. Child
Pakaslahti, L., & Keltigangas-Jarvinen, L. (2000). Comparison of
Development, 72(2), 583598.
peer, teacher, and self-assessments on adolescent direct and indi-
rect aggression. Educational Psychology, 20, 177190. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge
& Kegan Paul.
Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Young adolescents
experiences of peer victimization: Gender differences in ac- Pine, D. S., Coplan, J. D., Wasserman, G. A., Miller, L. S., Fried, J. E.,
counts of social and physical aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quar- Davies, M., et al. (1997). Neuroendocrine response to fenf luramine
terly, 45, 233258. challenge in boys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 839846.
Parke, R. D., & Deur, J. L. (1972). Schedule of punishment and inhibition Pine, D. S., Shaffer, D., Schonfeld, I. S., & Davies, M. (1997). Minor
of aggression in children. Developmental Psychology, 7, 266269. physical anomalies: Modifiers of environmental risks for psychi-
Parke, R. D., & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. atric impairment? Journal of the American Academy of Child and
In P. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 395403.
of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social Piquero, A., & Tibbett, S. (1999). The impact of per/perinatal distur-
development (4th ed., pp. 547641). New York: Wiley. bances and disadvantaged familial environment in predicting
Patterson, G. R., & Bank, C. L. (1989). Some amplifying mechanisms criminal offending. Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention, 8,
for pathologic processes in families. In M. Gunnar & E. Thelen 5270.
(Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on child psychology: Systems and de- Pollak, S. D., & Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to fa-
velopment (pp. 167210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. cial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal
Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. R. (1982). A compara- Psychology, 112, 323338.
tive evaluation of parent training procedures. Behavior Therapy, Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention, self-regulation, and
3, 638650. consciousness. Transactions of the Philosophical Society of Lon-
Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Yoerger, K. (2000). Adolescent don, B, 19151927.
growth in new forms of problem behavior: Macro- and micro-peer Prentice, N. M., & Kelly, F. J. (1963). Intelligence and delinquency:
dynamics. Prevention Science, 1, 313. A reconsideration. Journal of Social Psychology, 60, 327337.
Patterson, G. R., Littman, R. A., & Bricker, W. (1967). Assertive be- Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Forms and functions of
havior in children: A step toward a theory of aggression. Mono- adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer sta-
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 32(5, tus. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 310342.
Serial No. 113).
Pulkkinen, L. (1990). Adult life-styles and their precursors in the so-
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). A social learning cial behavior of children and adolescents. European Journal of
approach: Vol. 4. Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR: Castalia Press. Personality, 4, 237251.
Peetsma, T., Vergeer, M., Roeleveld, J., & Karsten, S. (2001). Inclu- Quiggle, N., Panak, W. F., Garber, J., & Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social
sion in education: Comparing pupils development in special and information processing in aggressive and depressed children.
regular education. Educational Review, 53, 125135. Child Development, 63, 13051320.
References 785

Rabiner, D. L., Lenhart, L., & Lochman, J. E. (1990). Automatic ver- Rogeness, G. A., Cepeda, C., Macedo, C. A., Fischer, C., & Harris,
sus ref lective social problem solving in relation to childrens so- W. R. (1990). Differences in heart rate and blood pressure in chil-
ciometric status. Developmental Psychology, 26, 10101016. dren with conduct disorder, major depression, and separation anx-
Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior iety. Psychiatry Research, 33, 199206.
in children and adults: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- Rose, A., & Asher, S. (1998). Childrens goals and strategies in re-
chology, 30(4), 311326. sponse to conf licts within a friendship. Developmental Psychol-
Raine, A., Brennan, P., & Farrington, D. P. (1997). Biosocial bases of ogy, 35, 6979.
violence: Conceptual and theoretical issues. In A. Raine, P. A. Ross, R. R., & Ross, B. D. (1998). Delinquency prevention through
Brennan, D. P. Farrington, & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biosocial cognitive training. New Education, 10, 7075.
bases of violence (pp. 120). New York: Plenum Press.
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Temperament
Raine, A., Brennan, P., & Mednick, S. A. (1994). Birth complications and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and
combined with early maternal rejection at age 1 year predispose Social Psychology, 78, 122135.
to violent crime at age 18 years. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Worthman, C. M., Costello, E. J., & Angold,
53, 984988.
A. (2004). Testosterone, antisocial behavior, and social domi-
Raine, A., Brennan, P., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Interaction between nance in boys: Pubertal development and biosocial interaction.
birth complications and early maternal rejection in predisposing Biological Psychiatry, 55(5), 546552.
to adult violence: Specificity to serious, early onset violence.
Rubin, K. H., & Krasnor, L. R. (1986). Social cognitive and social be-
American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 12651271.
havioral perspectives on problem solving. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2000).
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 168).
Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 57, 119127. Rubin, K. H., Moller, L., & Emptage, A. (1987). The preschool be-
havior questionnaire: A useful index of behavior problems in ele-
Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., Mednick, S. A., & Farring-
mentary school-age children. Canadian Journal of Behavioral
ton, D. P. (1998). Fearlessness, stimulation-seeking, and large
Science, 19, 86100.
body size at age 3 years as early predispositions to childhood ag-
gression at age 11 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, Rule, B. G., & Ferguson, T. J. (1986). The effects of media violence
745751. on attitudes, emotions, and cognitions. Journal of Social Issues,
42, 29-50.
Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Low resting
heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to aggression at age 11 Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Parallel distributed
years: Findings from the Mauritius Joint Child Health Project. processingExploration in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/ Bradford Books.
atry, 36, 14571464. Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. Journal of
Raine, A., Yaralian, P. S., Reynolds, C., & Venables, P. H. (2002). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 2531.
Spatial but not verbal cognitive deficits at age 3 years in life- Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental psychopathol-
course persistent antisocials: A prospective, longitudinal study. ogy. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington (Vol.
Development and Psychopathology, 14(1), 2544. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, per-
Rasanen, P., Hakko, H., Isohanni, M., Hodgins, S., Jarvelin, M. R., sonality and social development (4th ed., pp. 775911). New
& Tiihonen, J. (1999). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and York: Wiley.
risk of criminal behavior among adult male offspring in the Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial behavior by
northern Finland 1996 birth cohort. American Journal of Psychi- young people. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
atry, 156, 857862.
Rutter, M., Tizard, J., & Whitmore, K. (Eds.). (1970). Education,
Reid, J. B., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (Eds.). (2000). Antisocial health, and behavior. London: Longmans.
behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis
Rys, G. S., & Bear, G. G. (1997). Relational aggression and peer rela-
and model for intervention. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
tions: Gender and developmental issues. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
logical Association.
terly, 43, 87106.
Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., & Sroufe,
L. A. (1989). Early childhood antecedents of aggression and pas- Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. (2000). Aggression
sive-withdrawal in early elementary school. Journal of Personal- and socio-metric status among peers: Do gender and type of ag-
ity, 57, 257281. gression matter? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, 1724.
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental in- Salvador, A., Simon, V., Suay, F., & Llorens, L. (1987). Testosterone
f luences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and and cortisol responses to competitive fighting in human males: A
adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490529. pilot study. Aggressive Behavior, 13, 913.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore: Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1990). Crime and deviance over the
Williams & Wilkins. life course: The salience of adult social bonds. American Socio-
Robins, L. N. (1999). A 70-year history of conduct disorder: Varia- logical Review, 55, 609627.
tions in definition, prevalence, and correlates. In P. Cohen, C. Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1994). Urban poverty and the family
Slomkowski, & L. N. Robins (Eds.), Historical and geographical context of delinquency: A new look at structure and process in a
inf luences of psychopathology (pp. 3756). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. classic study. Child Development, 65, 523540.
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. (2000). Het- Sampson, R., & Laub, J. (2003). Life-course desisters? Trajectories
erogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial configura- of crime among delinquent boys followed to age 70. Criminology,
tions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 1424. 41(3), 555592.
786 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighbor- Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among mal-
hoods and violent crime: A multi-level study of collective effi- treated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dys-
cacy. Science, 277, 918924. regulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 381395.
Sanders, M. R., Markie-Dadds, C., Tully, L. A., & Bor, W. (2000). Shumow, L., & Lomax, R. (2002). Parental efficacy: Predictor of par-
The Triple P-positive Parenting Intervention for parents of chil- enting behavior and adolescent outcomes. Parenting: Science and
dren with early onset conduct problems. Journal of Consulting Practice, 2, 127150.
and Clinical Psychology, 68, 624640. Shure, M. B., & Spivack, G. (1980). Interpersonal problem-solving as
Sawin, D. B., & Parke, R. D. (1979). The effects of interagent incon- a mediator of behavioral adjustment in preschool and kindergarten
sistent discipline on childrens aggressive behavior. Journal of Ex- children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 1, 2944.
perimental Child Psychology, 28, 525538. Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H., Loe-
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- ber, R., et al. (1997). The Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Be-
ronments: A theory of genotype greater than environment effects. havioral Development: Inf luences of age, gender, and impairment
Child Development, 54, 424435. on rates of disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 801808.
Schaal, B., Tremblay, R., Soussignan, R., & Susman, E. (1996). Male Simons, R. L., Wu, C. I., Conger, R. D., & Lorenz, F. O. (1994). Two
testosterone linked to high social dominance but low physical ag- routes to delinquency: Differences between early and late
gression in early adolescence. Journal of the American Academy starters in the impact of parenting and deviant peers. Criminol-
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 13221330. ogy, 32, 247276.
Schultz, D., Izard, C. E., & Bear, G. (2004). Childrens emotion pro- Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1981). Television, imagination, and ag-
cessing: Relations to emotionality and aggression. Development gression: A study of preschoolers play. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
and Psychopathology, 16, 371387.
Slaby, R. G., & Guerra, N. G. (1988). Cognitive mediators of aggres-
Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Hubbard, J., Cillessen, A.,
sion in adolescent offenders: Pt. 1. Assessment. Developmental
Lemerise, E., et al. (1998). Social cognitive and behavioral corre-
Psychology, 24, 580588.
lates of aggression and victimization in boys play groups. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 431440. Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. (2000). Proactive
and reactive aggression in delinquent adolescents: Relations to ag-
Schwartz, D., McFadyen-Ketchum, S. A., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S.,
gression outcome expectancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychol-
& Bates, J. E. (1998). Peer group victimization as a predictor of
ogy, 29, 8693.
childrens behavior problems at home and in school. Development
and Psychopathology, 10, 8799. Snyder, H. N. (2003). Juvenile arrests 2001. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Ju-
Schweinhart, L. J., Barnes, H. V., & Weikart, D. P. (1993). Signifi-
venile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
cant benefits: The High /Scope Perry Preschool Study thorough age
27. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. Snyder, J., Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1986). Determinants and
Seguin, J. R., Nagin, D., Assaad, J. M., & Tremblay, R. E. (in press). consequences of associating with deviant peers during preadoles-
Cognitive-neuropsychological function in chronic physical ag- cence and adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 6, 2943.
gression and hyperactivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Snyder, J., Reid, J., & Patterson, G. (2003). A social learning model
Seguin, J. R., Pihl, R. O., Harden, P. W., Tremblay, R. E., & of child and adolescent antisocial behavior. In B. B. Lahey, T. E.
Boulerice, B. (1995). Cognitive and neuropsychological charac- Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juve-
teristics of physically aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Psy- nile delinquency (pp. 2748). New York: Guilford Press.
chology, 104, 614624. Snyder, J., Stoolmiller, M., Wilson, M., & Yamamoto, M. (2003).
Selman, R. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. New Child anger regulation, parental responses to childrens anger
York: Academic Press. displays, and early child antisocial behavior. Social Development,
12, 335360.
Serketich, W. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1996). The effectiveness of behav-
ioral parent training to modify antisocial behavior in children: A Snyder, J. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1995). Individual differences in so-
meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 27, 171186. cial aggression: A test of a reinforcement model of socialization
Shank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and under- in the natural environment. Behavior Therapy, 26, 371391.
standing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spain, S. E., Douglas, K. S., Poythress, N. G., & Epstein, M.
Shantz, D. W., & Voydanoff, D. A. (1973). Situational effects on (2004). The relationship between psychopathic features, violence
retaliatory aggression at three age levels. Child Development, and treatment outcome: The comparison of three youth measures
44, 149153. of psychopathic features. Behavioral Sciences and the Law,
22(1), 85102.
Shaw, C., & McKay, H. (1942). Juvenile delinquency and urban areas.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Spencer, M. B., Dobbs, B., & Phillips, D. (1988). African-American
Shaw, D. S. (2003). Innovative approaches and methods to the study adolescents: Adaptational processes and socioeconomic diversity
of childrens conduct problems. Social Development, 12, 322326. in behavioral outcomes. Journal of Adolescence, 11, 117137.
Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Spergel, I. A., Ross, R. E., Curry, G. D., & Chance, R. (1989). Youth
Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmen- gangs: Problem and response. Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile
tal Psychology, 39, 189200. Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., Owens, E. B., Winslow, E. B., Hood, N., & Stafford, E., & Cornell, D. G. (2003). Psychopathy scores predict
Garcia, M. (1995, April). Developmental precursors of externaliz- adolescent inpatient aggression. Assessment, 10, 102112.
ing behavior among two samples of low-income families: Ages 1 to Stanger, C., Achenbach, T. M., & Verhulst, F. C. (1997). Accelerated
5. Paper presented at biennial meeting of the Society for Research longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syn-
in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. dromes. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 4358.
References 787

Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpreta- Tiet, Q. Q., Wasserman, G. A., Loeber, R., McReynolds, L. S., & Miller,
tion. Child Development, 71, 10721085. L. S. (2001). Developmental and sex differences in types of conduct
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 10, 181197.
group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity Tremblay, R. E., Hartup, W. W., & Archer, J. (Eds.). (2005). Develop-
threat. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- mental origins of aggression. New York: Guilford Press.
chology (Vol. 34, pp. 379440). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Tremblay, R. E., Msse, L. C., Vitaro, F., & Dobkin, P. L. (1995). The
Steinberg, M. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Attributional bias in ag- impact of friends deviant behavior on early onset of delinquency:
gressive adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Social and Clinical Longitudinal data from 6 to 13 years of age. Development and
Psychology, 1, 312321. Psychopathology, 7, 649667.
Stenberg, C., & Campos, J. (1990). The development of anger expres- Tremblay, R. E., Pihl, R. O., Vitaro, R., & Dobkin, P. L. (1994). Pre-
sions in infancy. In N. Stein, T. Trabasso, & B. Leventhal (Eds.), dicting early onset of male antisocial behavior from preschool be-
Concepts in emotion (pp. 7599). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. havior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 732739.
Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Greenbaum, C., Cicchetti, D., Dawus, Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parental-offspring conf lict. American Zoolo-
S., Cortes, R. M., et al. (1993). Effects of domestic violence on gist, 46, 3557.
childrens behavior problems and depression. Developmental Psy- Tuinier, S., Verhoeven, W. M. A., & van Praag, H. M. (1995). Cere-
chology, 29, 4452. brospinal f luid 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid and aggression: A criti-
Stifter, C. A., Spinrad, T. L., & Baungart-Rieker, J. M. (1999). To- cal reappraisal of the clinical data. International Clinical
ward a developmental model of child compliance: The role of Psychopharmacology, 10(3), 147156.
emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 70, 2132. Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and re-
Stone, W. L., Bendell, R. D., & Field, T. M. (1988). The impact of so- trieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80,
cioeconomic status on teenage mothers and children who received 352-373.
early intervention. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, Turner, A. K. (1994). Genetic and hormonal inf luences on male vio-
9, 391408. lence. In J. Archer (Ed.), Male violence (pp. 233252). New
Stormshak, E. A., Bierman, K. L., Bruschi, C., Dodge, K. A., Coie, York: Routledge.
J. D., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999). Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. New York:
The relation between behavior problems and peer preference in Guilford Press.
different classroom contexts. Child Development, 70, 169182.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1997). Morbidity
Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1987). Automatic activation of and Mortality Weekly Report, 46(5). Atlanta, GA: Centers for Dis-
self-discrepancies and emotional syndromes: When cognitive ease Control and Prevention.
structures inf luence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2000). Healthy peo-
chology, 53, 10041014.
ple 2010: Understanding and improving health and objectives for
Straus, M. A. (2005). The primordial violence: Corporal punishment improving health (2nd ed., Vols. 12). Washington, DC: U.S. De-
by parents, cognitive development, and crime. Walnut Creek, CA: partment of Health and Human Services.
Alta Mira Press.
U.S. Department of Justice, Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2003).
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). How violent are American fam- Crime in the United States: Uniform Crime Reports, 2002. Wash-
ilies. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
American families (pp. 95112). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Reiser, M., Fabes, R. A.,
Straus, M. A., & Stewart, J. H. (1999). Corporal punishment by Losoya, S., et al. (2003). The relations of effortful control and re-
American parents: National data on prevalence, chronicity, sever- active control to childrens externalizing problems: A longitudi-
ity, and duration, in relation to child and family characteristics. nal assessment. Journal of Personality, 71, 11711196.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 5570.
Van Lier, P. A. C., Muthn, B. O., van der Sar, R. M., & Crijnen,
Stromquist, V. J., & Strauman, T. J. (1992). Childrens social con- A. A. M. (2004). Preventing disruptive behavior in elementary
structs: Pt. 2. Nature, assessment, and association with adaptive schoolchildren: Impact of a universal classroom-based inter-
and maladaptive behavior. Social Cognition, 9, 330358. vention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
Taylor, A. R., & Gabriel, S. W. (1989, April). Cooperative versus 467478.
competitive game-playing strategies of peer accepted and peer re- Virkkunen, M., Eggert, M., Rawlings, R., & Linnoila, M. (1996). A
jected children in a goal conf lict situation. Paper presented at the prospective follow-up study of alcoholic violent offenders and
biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- fire setters. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 523529.
ment, Kansas City, MO.
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Pagani, L., Trembley, R. E., & McDuff, P.
Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and (1999). Disruptive behavior, peer association, and conduct disor-
coercive actions. Washington, DC: American Psychological der: Testing the developmental links through early intervention.
Association. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 287-304.
Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1968). Temperament and behav- Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and
ior disorders in children. New York: New York University Press. proactively aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent
Thornberry, T. P., Krohn, M. D., Lizotte, A. J., & Chard-Wierschem, characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43,
D. (1993). The role of juvenile gangs in facilitating delinquent be- 495505.
havior. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30, 5587. Votruba, M. E., & Kling, J. R. (2004). Ef fects of neighborhood char-
Thornberry, T. P., Krohn, M. D., Lizotte, A. J., Smith, C. A., & acteristics on the mortality of Black male youth: Evidence from
Tobin, K. (2003). Gangs and delinquency in developmental per- Gautreaux (Working Paper No. 491). Industrial Relations Sec-
spective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. tion, Princeton University.
788 Aggression and Antisocial Behavior in Youth

Votruba-Drzal, E., Coley, R. L., & Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (2004). ining its relation to delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Child care and low-income childrens development: Direct and 103, 192205.
moderated effects. Child Development, 75, 296312. Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. (1985). Crime and Human Nature.
Waas, G. A. (1988). Social attributional biases of peer-rejected and New York: Simon and Schuster.
aggressive children. Child Development, 59, 969-992. Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged. Chicago: University of
Wakschlag, L. S., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Gordon, Chicago Press.
R. A., & Leventhal, B. L. (1997). Maternal smoking during preg- Wintemute, G. (2000). Guns and gun violence. In A. Blumstein & J.
nancy and the risk of conduct disorder in boys. Archives of Gen- Wallman (Eds.), The crime drop in America (pp. 4596). New
eral Psychiatry, 54, 670676. York: Cambridge University Press.
Waldman, I. D. (1988). Relationships between noon-social informa- Wolff, P. H., Waber, D., Bauermeister, M., Cohen, C., & Ferber, R.
tion processing, social perception, and social status in 7- to 12- (1982). The neuropsychological status of adolescent delinquent
year-old boys. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 267279.
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
Wood, W., Wong, F. Y., & Chachere, G. (1991). Effects of media vio-
Walters, R. H., & Parke, R. D. (1964). Social motivation, depen-
lence on viewers aggression in unconstrained social interaction.
dency, and susceptibility to social inf luence. In L. Berkowitz
Psychological Bulletin, 109, 371383.
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1,
pp. 231276). New York: Academic Press. Woolfenden, S. R., Williams, K., & Peat, J. (2002). Family and par-
enting interventions in children and adolescents with conduct dis-
Wann, D. L., & Branscombe, N. R. (1990). Person perception when
order and delinquency aged 10 to 17 (Cochrane Review). The
aggressive or nonaggressive sport are primed. Aggressive Behav-
Cochrane Library, 4. Oxford, England: Update Software.
ior, 16, 27-32.
Wright, J. C., Giammarino, M., & Parad, H. W. (1986). Social status
Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in Head
in small groups: Individual-group similarity and the social mis-
Start children: Strengthening parenting competencies. Journal of
fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 523536.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 715730.
Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its so-
Weiner, N. A. (1989). Violent criminal careers and violent career
cial context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
criminals: An overview of the research literature. In N. A.
Weiner & M. E. Wolfgang (Eds.), Violent crime, violent criminals Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child-
(pp. 35138). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. rearing and childrens prosocial initiations toward victims of dis-
tress. Child Development, 50, 319330.
Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., Bates, S. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1992). Some
consequences of early harsh discipline: Child aggression and a Zakriski, A. L., & Coie, J. D. (1996). A comparison of aggressive-
maladaptive social information processing style. Child Develop- rejected and nonaggressive-rejected boys interpretations of
ment, 63, 13211335. self-directed and other-directed rejection. Child Development,
67, 10481070.
Weiss, B., Harris, V., Catron, T., & Han, S. H. (2003). Efficacy of the
RECAP intervention program for children with concurrent inter- Zelli, A., Dodge, K., Lochman, J., Laird, R., & the Conduct Problems
nalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of Consulting and Prevention Research Group. (1999). The distinction between be-
Clinical Psychology, 71, 364374. liefs legitimizing aggression and deviant processing of social
cues: Testing measurement validity and the hypothesis that biased
Weissman, M. M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P. J., & Kandel, D. B.
processing mediates the effects of beliefs on aggression. Journal
(1999). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psychopathology
of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 150166.
in offspring followed to adulthood. Journal of the American Acad-
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 892899. Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M.,
Guthrie, I. K., et al. (2002). The relations of parental warmth and
Werner, H. (1961). Comparative psychology of mental development.
positive expressiveness to childrens empathy-related responding
New York: Science Editions. (Original work published 1948)
and social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development,
White, J., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles, D., & 73, 893915.
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impulsivity and exam-
CHAPTER 13

The Development of Morality


ELLIOT TURIEL

SETTING THE STAGE 790 Justice and Interpersonal Responsibilities 817


HISTORICAL OVERVIEW: THE FIRST AND EMPHASIZING JUDGMENT AND RECIPROCAL
SECOND PARTS OF THE TWENTIETH SOCIAL INTERACTIONS 819
CENTURY 792 The Construction of Moral Judgments through Social
ISSUES, EMPHASES, AND THEORIES 794 Interactions 822
BUILDING CHARACTER AND STRENGTHENING Moral Judgments in Early Childhood and Beyond 823
COMMUNITIES 796 Concepts of Distributive Justice and Fairness of Social
Character Traits and Moral Sensibilities 796 Practices 824
Habits and the Communitarian Spirit 797 Prosocial Moral Judgments 825
Moral Appraisal and Moral Recommendations 798 DOMAIN SPECIFICITY: EMPHASIZING
EMPHASIZING EMOTIONS 799 DISTINCTIONS IN JUDGMENTS 826
A Primacy for Empathy 800 Domains of Social Judgment 827
Evolution and Internalization 802 Social Judgments and Social Experiences 829
Conscience and Internalization 802 Social Judgments and Family Interactions 830
Beyond Family and beyond Incorporation of Societal Emotional Attributions and Social Judgments 832
Standards 803 Ambiguities, Uncertainties, and Deliberations 833
GENDER, EMOTIONS, AND MORAL Information, Assumptions about Reality, and Moral
JUDGMENTS 805 Decisions 834
Care and Justice as Moral Orientations 807 The Personal and the Social 836
Moral Judgments, Orientations, and Social Contexts 808 CULTURE AND CONTEXT REVISITED 838
Politics, Economics, Social Structure, and Womens Culture as Context or Context as Context? 839
Perspectives 810 Tradition, Social Hierarchy, Heterogeneity, and Social
EMPHASIZING CULTURE 811 Opposition 842
Social Communication and Cultural Coherence 813 CONCLUSIONS 846
Social Practices and Cultural Coherence 814 REFERENCES 847
Emotional Forms, Intuitions, and Rapid Processing 816

Philosophers have been concerned with the topic of the problems raised by them and by moral philosophers
morality for a long time. Socrates is referred to as the over the ages remain part of contemporary discussion.
patron saint of moral philosophy (Frankena, 1963, Psychological research in recent years has produced
p. 1). The moral philosophies of Plato and Aristotle in- new findings and changes in assumptions about chil-
cluded concerns with how individuals acquire or develop drens social propensities, social experiences, and ways
morality and how to create the best educational condi- of framing features of moral development. Neverthe-
tions for its acquisition. Moral development has been of less, many of the issues around definitions of morality,
central concern in the major psychological theoretical influences of emotions and thought, and the roles of so-
perspectives since the beginning of the twentieth cen- ciety or cultures are still debated. In these regards, lit-
tury. Psychoanalytic, behavioristic, Gestalt, and struc- tle has changed in theory and research on moral
tural-developmental theorists made the study of development since the publication of the previous vol-
morality central. The major figures in these approaches, umes of the Handbook of Child Psychology in 1998.
including Sigmund Freud, B. F. Skinner, and Jean Pi- This chapter, therefore, covers much of the same
aget, provided accounts of moral development. Many of ground as the previous version, with an updating of the

789
790 The Development of Morality

literature since that time. Because most of the theoriz- The important question of gender differences is dis-
ing and research discussed in the previous version of cussed later with regard to contemporary analyses and
this chapter remains relevant, changes in this version debates. Gender is important in analyses of morality be-
primarily reflect the inclusion of new research con- cause females are in subordinate positions in the social
ducted during the past few years. hierarchies of most societies and thereby are not treated
equally with males. Inequalities that stem from such so-
cial hierarchies or cultural practices and affect women
SETTING THE STAGE and others (e.g., minorities and people of lower social
castes or classes) are discussed in this chapter. In par-
Sigmund Freud wrote extensively about morality, incor- ticular, research is considered that has examined the
porating it into his general formulations of individual de- perspectives and moral reactions (through social oppo-
velopment in society. Central to his view were the sition, resistance, and subversion) of those in subordi-
concepts of conscience (tied to the idea of a duality) and nate positions.
concomitant tension between an individual and society. B. F. Skinner (1971, Chapter 6) presented his position
The root of this tension is the incompatibility of psycho- on morality in the latter part of his career and in a largely
logical and biological needs of individuals and strivings nontechnical book for a popular audience. In keeping
for long-term survival of individuals and the species. with his behavioristic formulations, Skinner proposed
The collectivity largely has the function of ensuring sur- that morality reflects behaviors that have been rein-
vival and protecting people from each others aggressive forced (positively or negatively) by value judgments as-
tendencies. Through the influences of the collectivity, sociated with cultural norms. Actions are not
particularly as reproduced in a family, the individuals intrinsically good or bad but are acquired and performed
needs for instinctual gratification become transformed as a consequence of contingencies of reinforcement. Cer-
and displaced in the developmental process to make room tain contingencies, consistent with the mores of the
for internalized standards (via parents as representatives group, are social in that they pertain to relationships
of society) and internalized emotional mechanisms for with others and are governed by verbal reinforcers such
regulating behaviors. This transformation, which is as good, bad, right, and wrong. Moreover, social control
grounded in emotions of fear and anxiety and facilitated over behavior is particularly powerful when it is exer-
by positive emotions of love and attachment, largely cised by institutional forces (e.g., religious, governmen-
stems from emotional conflicts producing psychological tal, economic, or educational). This is because the
transformations through the acquisition of a mental reinforcers of good and bad also take the form of
agency, a superego, and incorporating moral ideals and legal, illegal, pious, or sinful acts with their associated
guilt as the means for the regulation of conduct. rewards and punishments. Learned behaviors stemming
In the Freudian view, the acquisition of morality re- from the customary practices of a group are invariant be-
sults in a duality in the individual, including the forces cause reinforcement contingencies are maintained. For
of the superego and needs for instinctual gratification. Skinner, however, learned behaviors do not constitute
The moral side of the duality entails duties to uphold so- duties or obligations nor reflect a persons character;
cietal norms. Although fulfilling duties entails deep rather, they are due to the arrangement of effective so-
conflicts (most often of an unconscious nature), the du- cial contingencies.
ties are felt as inexorable and impersonal. An appropri- Knowledge and judgments about social relationships
ately internalized morality is invariable and applied were considered central to morality by Jean Piaget, who
inflexibly. In this regard, Freud proposed that women do wrote about the topic mainly in the early part of his ca-
not adequately internalize a superego. In what has be- reer (Piaget, 1932; see also Piaget, 1951/1195a,
come an infamous statement about gender differences in 1960/1995b). In keeping with his general views of devel-
morality, Freud (1925/1959) said: opment as stemming from reciprocal interactions of in-
I cannot escape the notion (though I hesitate to give it ex- dividuals and multiple features of social experiences
pression) that for women the level of what is ethically nor- (entailing constructions of understandings of experi-
mal is different from what it is in men. Their superego is ences), Piaget analyzed morality from the perspective of
never so inexorable, so impersonal, so independent of its how experiences result in the formation of judgments
emotional origins as we require it to be in men. (p. 196) about social relationships, rules, laws, authority, and so-
Setting the Stage 791

cial institutions. Piagets formulations on moral devel- the ideas of mutual respect and cooperation, key to Pi-
opment included the idea that social transmission does agets formulation, imply interdependence rather than
not solely result in the reproduction of that which is independence. By autonomy, Piaget (1960/1995b) meant
transmitted but also entails reconstructions. He also that the subject participates in the elaboration of
proposed that moral development is influenced by a va- norms instead of receiving them ready-made as happens
riety of experiences, including emotional reactions (e.g., in the case of the norms of unilateral respect that lie be-
sympathy, empathy, and respect), relationships with hind heteronomous morality (p. 315). Therefore, Pi-
adults, and relationships with other children. In Piagets aget used autonomy in reference to a process in which
view, moral judgments are fundamentally about rela- norms furthering interdependence are elaborated with
tionships, with development progressing (a) toward feel- the participation of the child.
ings of mutual respect among persons (with a The concept of autonomy, along with the propositions
developmentally prior set of feelings entailing a sense of that obligatory moral judgments are applied with flexi-
unilateral respect from child to adult or authority), ( b) bility of thought in social contexts, makes for a funda-
toward concerns with attaining and maintaining social mental contrast between Piagetian and Freudian or
relationships of cooperation (with rules and laws serv- behaviorist approaches. In both the Freudian and behav-
ing ends of cooperation rather than seen as fixed and iorist conceptions, the individuals morality is under
categorical), (c) toward the formation of concepts of some kind of psychological compulsion: In the Freudian
justice, and (d) toward an ability to consider the per- view, an internalized conscience or superego compels
spectives of others as possibly different from ones own behavior, and in the behavioristic conception, actions
(thus accounting for subjectivity and intentionality are compelled by habits of behavior. Contemporary
rather than viewing all perspectives as reflecting objec- analyses discussed in this chapter can also be contrasted
tive reality). As based on mutual respect, cooperation, on these dimensions as well. Contemporary researchers
and concepts of rules, laws, and duties as serving ends have examined moral judgments and how they are ap-
of fairness and justice, the developmentally advanced plied in situational and cultural contexts. There are also
form of morality, in Piagets view, is both inexorable various psychological and/or biologically based explana-
and flexible. Moral concepts and goals have an obliga- tions of moral functioning that are based on how psycho-
tory quality to individuals but are applied flexibly in ac- logical mechanisms compel actions. These include
cord with requirements of situations, appraisal of genetic traits and genetically based intuitions and emo-
intentions, and varying perspectives. The less develop- tions. Some explanations are based on propositions of
mentally advanced heteronomous morality of the young acquired, learned, or internalized features, such as char-
child entails conceptions of fixed rules, duties, and obe- acter traits and conscience. Some are based on internal-
dience to authority. In this regard, too, Piaget proposed ized values, norms, or rules, such as from parents,
gender differences of a less straightforward kind than society, and culture. Some of these explanations imply
those proposed by Freud. In some respects, Piaget moral absolutism, such as genes are fixed and conscience
(1932) viewed the morality of school-age girls as less or traits of character are regarded as unvarying. Other
advanced than boys (specifically, the legal sense is far explanations imply a degree of moral relativism, such as
less developed in little girls than in boys; p. 69), different parents, societies, or cultural ways result in dif-
whereas in other respects he viewed girls as more ad- ferent groups being compelled in different ways.
vanced than boys (specifically, girls more readily subor- As already noted, many issues and questions ad-
dinate rules to cooperation and mutual agreement and dressed by Freud, Skinner, and Piaget persist in con-
are more tolerant and more easily reconciled to innova- temporary analyses of moral development. Their
tions; p. 75). theoretical approaches influenced subsequent re-
Another aspect of Piagets formulation especially rel- searchers working from the 1950s to the 1970s, which
evant to contemporary analyses of culture and morality in turn have influenced contemporary researchers. The
is the concept of autonomy. Piaget proposed that as following section presents a brief historical overview
morality develops, there is a shift from a heteronomous connecting the ideas of Skinner and Freud to subsequent
to an autonomous orientation. Autonomy in this context research of behavioristically oriented thinkers, who
does not mean that individuals conceptions of morality also attempted to account for psychoanalytic concepts.
are based on the independence of individuals. Indeed, The overview includes a consideration of connections
792 The Development of Morality

between the ideas of Piaget and subsequent cognitively The dominant conceptions of morality were, there-
oriented thinkers. fore, either based on psychoanalytic explanations of
conscience and guilt, as transformed by learning theo-
ries, or straightforward behavioristic explanations of
moral learning. In either type of formulation, moral de-
HISTORICAL OVERVIEW: THE FIRST velopment was assumed to be a function of societal con-
AND SECOND PARTS OF THE trol over the individuals interests, needs, or impulses.
TWENTIETH CENTURY Since then a major shift, brought about in no small mea-
sure by the work of Kohlberg, has occurred in psycholo-
Freuds and Piagets formulations of morality were pro- gists approach to morality. Kohlberg critiqued the
duced in the context of a fair amount of interest in the dominant behavioristic and psychoanalytic conceptions
topic among social scientists writing in the early part of of morality (Kohlberg, 1963, 1964), argued for the need
the twentieth century (Baldwin, 1896; McDougall, to ground empirical study of moral development on
1908). Another influential direction was established by sound philosophical definitions of the domain
the French sociologist Emile Durkheim (1925/1961, (Kohlberg, 1970, 1971), and presented his own formula-
1912/1965), whose ideas contrasted with those of Piaget tions of the process of moral development (Kohlberg,
(see Piaget, 1932, chap. 4, for his critique of 1963, 1969), entailing modifications and elaborations of
Durkheims position). Durkheim conceptualized moral- Piagets (1932) earlier formulations.
ity as largely based on sentiments of attachment to the Kohlberg provided a comprehensive review of re-
group and respect for its symbols, rules, and authority. search pertaining to what was then the common wisdom
According to Durkheim, childrens immersion in the that parental practices of discipline determined the
group and participation in social life produce a natural strength and accuracy of the acquisition of conscience
attachment to the group and a willing adherence to its and moral behaviors. Kohlbergs review documented
moral norms. that there were no consistent relations between those
Many of the issues put forth in the first part of the parental conditions of child rearing postulated to lead to
twentieth century by Freud, Piaget, and others had a learning and the various measures of conscience or in-
major influence on later research on moral development. ternalized values used at the time (see Kohlberg, 1963,
It took some time, however, for those influences to have for details). Kohlberg also argued that the measures of
their impact. Whereas there was little research from moral development generally used in that body of re-
about the early 1930s until the late 1950s, a great deal of search were inadequate because they entailed projective
research on moral development has been conducted tests of guilt or anxiety, reactions to story stimuli of lit-
since the late 1950s to the time of this writing. During tle moral importance, ambiguous self-reports by parents
the late 1950s and early 1960s, there was steadily in- of their past child-rearing techniques, and contrived ex-
creasing interest in the child-rearing antecedents of con- perimental situations of little meaning to children.
science, guilt, and internalized moral values and The message Kohlberg wanted to convey regarding
behaviors (e.g., Hoffman, 1963; Sears, Maccoby, & his methodological critique was not only methodologi-
Levin, 1957). Although the influence of psychoanalytic cal. The inadequacies in methods were, in Kohlbergs
theory waned over the years, many of Freuds ideas were view, tied to theories that were not grounded in any sub-
incorporated into the work on child rearing, alongside stantive epistemology of the domain. Whereas morality
the increasingly influential behavioristic theories of that was treated as a substantive epistemological category by
time. Emphasis was placed on identification as a mecha- many philosophersfrom Plato and Aristotle to Hume,
nism for the acquisition of moral values and on guilt and Mill, and Kant and contemporary philosophers (e.g.,
anxiety as the basic motives for the childs inhibition of John Rawls)psychologists attempted to explain its ac-
needs or impulses and adherence to moral values. At quisition without considering the definition or meaning
about the same time, several researchers turned their at- of that which is acquired. Kohlberg argued that we could
tention to direct applications of behavioristic learning not consider mechanisms of moral acquisition without
principles for explanations of the acquisition of moral concern with definitions, meanings, and the substance
behaviors and the role of anxiety and guilt in moral ac- of morality. This idea was also based on psychological
tions (e.g., Aronfreed, 1961; Bandura & Walters, 1963). considerations. Kohlberg presumed that social scientists
Historical Overview: The First and Second Parts of the Twentieth Century 793

and philosophers were not the only ones who engage in moral value from power, sanctions, and instrumental
systematic thinking about psychological, social, or needs. In turn, Kohlberg proposed that mutual respect
moral matters: Laypersons do, too. He rejected the im- and concepts of justice and rights as part of an au-
plied duality between the psychologist and the layperson tonomous system of thought, whose emergence Piaget
evident in most psychological explanations. had placed in late childhood or early adolescence, do not
Kohlberg (1968) coined the phrase the child as a come about until, at the earliest, late adolescence and
moral philosopher. However, the metaphor was not usually not until adulthood (Kohlbergs postconven-
meant to convey the idea that children engage in reflec- tional level).
tive intellectual deliberations or formulate conceptual Kohlberg also proposed that the stages represent uni-
systems of the type seen in the writings of professional versal forms of moral judgment among individuals par-
moral philosophers. Rather, it was meant to convey the ticipating in social interactions and perspective taking.
idea that children form ways of thinking through their He proposed that the stages defined structural features
social experiences, which include substantive under- of moral thought, which represented commonalities
standings of moral concepts like justice, rights, equal- among cultures in the context of possible differences in
ity, and welfare. Implicit but important assumptions in the content of morality. By undertaking a series of stud-
this formulation are that morality is not solely, or even ies in several cultures, some Western and some non-
mainly, imposed on children and that morality is not Western (Kohlberg, 1969), Kohlberg gave greater
solely based on avoiding negative emotions like anxiety emphasis than existed before to empirical data to test
and guilt. As part of their orientation to social relation- propositions regarding cultural differences or common-
ships, and especially through taking the perspectives of alities in moral judgments. Kohlbergs research, as well
others, children generate judgments, built on emotions as many studies conducted by others, suggested both
like sympathy, empathy, respect, love, and attachment to that there may be similarities across cultures in develop-
which they have a commitment and which are not in con- ment through the first three or four stages and that there
flict with their natural or biological dispositions (re- is much ambiguity of thought corresponding to the
call Piagets definition of moral autonomy). higher stages (see Snarey, 1985). As discussed in subse-
Kohlberg studied moral development by focusing on quent sections, the question of cultural differences and
how children and adolescents make judgments about commonalities has provoked much controversy. Never-
conflicts, in hypothetical situations, around issues of theless, Kohlbergs work in this regard has been influen-
life, interpersonal obligations, trust, law, authority, and tial in framing discussions of morality and culture
retribution. He proposed a sequence of six stages, de- around empirical findings.
picting a progression of judgments. Stages 1 and 2, Kohlbergs influence on subsequent research and the-
grouped into a preconventional level, were primarily ories is, in important respects, separate from the influ-
based on obedience, punishment avoidance, and instru- ence of his particular formulation of stages of moral
mental need and exchange. Stages 3 and 4, grouped into development or even from the general theoretical view-
a conventional level, were based on role obligations, point he espoused. Many advance alternative theoretical
stereotypical conceptions of good persons, and respect paradigms, including paradigms based on the idea of the
for the rules and authority legitimated in the social sys- internalization of conscience and values or the idea of
tem. Stages 5 and 6, grouped into a postconventional culture-based morality. Among those who advance de-
level, were based on contractual agreements, established velopmental positions influenced by Piagets theory,
procedural arrangements for adjudicating conflicts, mu- many propose formulations divergent from that of
tual respect, and differentiated concepts of justice and Kohlberg. Yet, Kohlberg has influenced discourse about
rights. This sequence was also a reformulation of Pi- the psychology of moral development in several ways.
agets progression from heteronomy to autonomy One is that there is greater recognition of the need to
(Kohlberg, 1963). Kohlberg maintained that respect for ground psychological explanations in philosophical con-
rules and authority, which Piaget had attributed to siderations about morality. Another influence is that in
young children at the heteronomous level, does not come many current formulations morality is not framed by im-
about at least until adolescence (Kohlbergs conven- positions on children due to conflicts between their
tional level), and that young childrens moral judgments needs or interests and the requirements of society or the
are characterized, instead, by a failure to distinguish group. Many now think that children are, in an active
794 The Development of Morality

and positive sense, integrated into their social relation- bates are futile because both biology and environment
ships with adults and peers and that morality is not contribute to psychological functioning and develop-
solely or even primarily an external or unwanted imposi- ment. One reason for this state of affairs is that the
tion on them. Kohlberg had stressed childrens con- question is usually mischaracterized as whether biology
structions of moral judgments from social interactions. or environment is taken into account rather than how
Following Piagets formulations, Kohlberg proposed each feature is explained. An equally important reason
that emotions of sympathy for others, spontaneous inter- for the continual reemergence of the debates is that
ests in helping others, and respect were centrally in- these matters are not settled, yet researchers pursuing
volved in childrens moral development, especially as different and even opposite explanations tend to declare
part of the process of taking the perspective of others matters settled.
(Kohlberg, 1969, 1971, 1976). For example, consider assertions about supposedly
All the changes in perspectives on moral develop- new disciplines: One labeled cultural psychology
ment cannot be attributed solely to Kohlbergs influ- (Shweder, 1990a; Shweder & Sullivan, 1993) and the
ence. The issues noted are ones he addressed directly other evolutionary psychology (Cosmides & Tooby,
and for which he provided persuasive arguments. Nor do 1989; Tooby, 1987; Wright, 1994). Proponents of cul-
contemporary analyses of moral development exclude tural psychology maintain that thoughts, meanings,
elements of the positions taken by behavioristic and psy- emotions, and behaviors vary by culture. Consequently,
choanalytic theorists. There are researchers concerned they propose that there are no general psychological
with the internalization of values, the ideas of con- processes to be discovered: [T]he mind . . . is content
science and self-control, and with emotions like anxiety, driven, domain specific, and constructively stimulus
shame, and guilt. Nevertheless, the scope of inquiry has bound (Shweder, 1990a, p. 87), with an emphasis on
been broadened to include and emphasize positive emo- that which is local, contingent, and context-dependent.
tions; the intricacies of moral, social, and personal judg- Cultural psychology is said to be a newly emerging disci-
ments as part of individuals relations with the social pline entailing the study of the way cultural traditions
world; and social interactions contributing to develop- and social practices regulate, express, and transform the
ment, including with parents, peers, schooling, and cul- human psyche, resulting less in psychic unity for hu-
ture. Debates now center on the roles of emotions and mankind than in ethnic divergences in mind, self, and
judgments, on the individual and the collectivity, on the emotion (Shweder, 1990a, p. 73).
contributions of constructions of moral understandings By contrast, proponents of evolutionary psychology
and culturally based meanings, and on how to distin- maintain that mind and behavior have a firm evolution-
guish between universally applicable and locally based ary basis, which makes for a unity within the species
moralities. connecting the peoples of the world. According to evolu-
tionary psychologists, social relationships, and espe-
cially relationships between the sexes, including and
ISSUES, EMPHASES, AND THEORIES going well beyond reproductive functions, are highly in-
fluenced by evolutionary processes. Evolutionary
Discussions of moral development seem to involve processes extend to morality: Altruism, compassion,
strongly held and conflicting positions. It is frequently empathy, love, conscience, the sense of justiceall
asserted that positions held by others exclude a particu- these things, the things that hold society together, the
lar feature of central importanceusually the feature things that allow our species to think so highly of itself,
emphasized by those characterizing the others ap- can now confidently be said to have a firm genetic basis
proach. Portraying others as excluding a feature deemed (Wright, 1994, p. 12). Confidence in a firm genetic
of central importance extends well beyond debates over basis brings with it confidence in uniformities among
moral development. Probably the most common example people of different cultures: Evolutionary psycholo-
is seen in the debates over the roles of biology and envi- gists are pursuing what is known in the trade as the psy-
ronment. Those debates seem to be everlasting and there chic unity of humankind (Wright, 1994, p. 26).
is a recycling of the issues even though periodically Along with the striking confidence expressed by cul-
there appears something of a consensus that such de- tural and evolutionary psychologists in discoveries
Issues, Emphases, and Theories 795

against and for the psychic unity of humankind, they regarded important by others, it is usually mistaken to
make other parallel claims. They claim that the disci- say that there is a failure to account for the component.
pline has discovered knowledge contrary to the estab- Often, the relevance of a component is explicitly ex-
lished perspective in psychology that will be resisted by cluded. An example is Skinners (1971) arguments for
those with vested interests in the old paradigm. Cultural the exclusion of moral judgments, along with cognition
psychologists argue that psychology and other social sci- in general, as epiphenomena.
ences have been dominated by those seeking psychic It is important, therefore, to consider how a theoreti-
unity and proposing general psychological mechanisms. cal perspective frames the relevant issues. In current
Evolutionary psychologists argue that psychology and theoretical perspectives and research programs, it is
other social sciences have been dominated by those particularly important to consider how issues like emo-
seeking environmental and cultural explanations. tion, culture, gender, judgment, social influences, and
If the content were omitted, it would appear that cul- individual constructions are explained. Indeed, em-
tural and evolutionary psychologists are in agreement phases placed on these issues serve to distinguish points
about the past and future of psychology. Each asserts of view on moral development. Whereas most explana-
that there is a previously dominant paradigm about tions of moral development attempt to account for each
which its proponents are defensive and resistant to of these issues, there are differences in the importance
change but which is being displaced by a new paradigm. and roles given to them that result in varying explana-
Yet, they hold contradictory views about which para- tions of morality and its development.
digm was previously dominant and which one is taking This chapter is organized around theoretical ap-
over. Not only are levels of the state of knowledge and proaches to moral development, with the central issues
documentation and verification for discoveries exagger- emphasized. There is a tradition in which morality is de-
ated so that matters are prematurely claimed to be set- fined as the possession of habits or virtues or traits of
tled, but also opposing positions are characterized as character, which are usually linked tightly with emo-
accounting for mainly one type of feature. This is an old tions. In recent years, psychologists have not empha-
story. Behaviorists made similar claims early in the sized habits or traits. Many have moved away from
twentieth century. Most notably, Watson (1924) pro- explanations of morality as the formation of internal
claimed that behaviorism was the wave of the future that traits or dispositions of personality. Nonetheless, there
would replace the prescientific thinking of the mass of are sizable groups, including those concerned with
people and of psychologists through its experimental ap- moral education and certain sociologists and social
proach and the recognition of the ways environment commentators, who rely on the concept of character as
shapes behavior (Turiel, 2004a). linked more to emotions than to reasoning. Some of this
Among theorists of moral development, there seems literaturethat on character and moral education (often
to be a greater recognition of the viability of competing referred to as character education)will not be consid-
points of view. However, it is not uncommon to find ered here, as it is the topic of the chapter by Narvaez and
characterizations of others explanations of moral de- Lapsley, Chapter 7, this Handbook, Volume 4. Narvaez
velopment as excluding a feature judged to be of central and Lapsley also consider propositions using the notions
importance. It is implied that the omission, in itself, in- of character and moral identity as bridging moral judg-
validates the theoretical point of view. The most fre- ments to moral actions (for a critique and alternative
quent examples of this revolve around whether theorists view, see Turiel, 2003a). However, I do discuss some
account for emotions or judgments, for social influences views outside of psychology that are presented by social
or the individuals logical operations, for parental influ- critics and sociologists who link emotions to the forma-
ences or peer influences, and for cultural or individual tion of character traits and/or habitual moral practices.
constructions. There is a tendency to mischaracterize Their positions have connections to psychological con-
positions as failing to account for this or that instead of cepts and can be evaluated by psychological evidence.
recognizing that differences in theoretical perspectives After discussing their positions, I consider, in greater
have more to do with how different features (e.g., emo- detail, the concepts and research of developmental psy-
tions and judgments) are explained and emphasized. chologists who emphasize emotions, influences of
Even when a theorist excludes a particular component parental practices, and conscience. This is followed by
796 The Development of Morality

discussion of approaches that, though including emo- community have affinities to Durkheims (1925/1961)
tions and judgments, emphasize the role of gender and proposition that morality involves a collective sense of
gender-related experiences in moral development. Then, solidarity, experienced by individuals as feelings of at-
I consider approaches in which culture is regarded as tachments to and respect for the moral authority embed-
central and in which fairly sharp distinctions are drawn ded in society.
among moral orientations in different cultures. Next, I The common themes in these positions are that habits
discuss approaches emphasizing moral judgments and and character traits are at the core of morality and that
reciprocal interactions in development. Finally, a per- American society is in moral crisis, decay, or serious de-
spective is presented based on reciprocal interactions; cline. Myriad causes have been offered as bringing
the domains of personal, social, and moral judgments; about the decline. These include the culture of the 1960s
and their interplay with cultural practices. I approach (Bloom, 1987); changes in the family (Bloom, 1987; Et-
each of the positions, and associated research, from the zioni, 1993; Wilson, 1993); a failure to attend to tradi-
perspectives of their conceptualizations of the moral tions (Bennett, 1993; Etzioni, 1993; K. Ryan, 1989;
realm, the theoretical constructs on development, and Wynne, 1986, 1989); a failure to provide moral educa-
the ways development is influenced by biological and en- tion (Bennett & Delattre, 1978); an onset of radical in-
vironmental features. dividualism (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, &
In the course of this chapter, I do not solely review Tipton, 1985; Etzioni, 1993); the influences of femi-
the different positions on moral development. While nism (Bloom, 1987); and the teachings of elites (intel-
presenting the different positions, I comment on and lectuals, scholars) who, in contradiction with the
evaluate the positions. Those evaluations are connected common sense or natural propensities of ordinary peo-
to my own views and positions. In the latter parts of the ple, create theories hostile to virtues and character
chapter, I discuss my positions, which are shared by a (Bennett, 1992; Bloom, 1987; Wilson, 1993).
number of colleagues and collaborators.
Character Traits and Moral Sensibilities
BUILDING CHARACTER AND
STRENGTHENING COMMUNITIES Some who lament the moral decline of American society
propose that the remedy lies in promoting character in
Some philosophers who trace their roots to Aristotle children through firm controls by adults in the family
have proposed explanations of morality as entailing and schools. They find fault in programs of moral educa-
virtues and character. Aristotelian and neo-Aristotelian tion (especially those based on Kohlbergs theories)
accounts (e.g., MacIntyre, 1981) have included concep- whose pedagogical aims are to stimulate the develop-
tions of morality as the good life, reflected in habitual ment of moral judgments, reflection, or the considera-
practices and in living up to the virtues through action. tion of alternative moral decisions (see Bennett &
Although Aristotle linked virtues to tradition, he also Delattre, 1978). Judgment is deemed largely tangential
believed that traditions should not necessarily remain to morality, and its emphasis is said to divert children
fixed or immune from criticism. According to Nuss- from learning to behave in habitual ways consistent with
baum (1989), Aristotles position was If we reason well traditions and virtues.
we can make progress in lawmaking, just as we do in It is argued that, instead, there should be an emphasis
other arts and sciences (p. 36). However, the sociolo- on the inculcation of traits in children, with a focus on
gists and social critics who stress the concept of charac- influencing how they act and not on their states of mind
ter have not carried over the philosophical substance of (Wynne, 1986). The traits, which are based on tradi-
the Aristotelian and neo-Aristotelian propositions, at tions of the culture, are transmitted not only through re-
least as interpreted by Nussbaum. Instead, they have wards and punishments but also, especially, through the
used the concepts of character as a means of comment- example provided by the consistent actions of adults
ing on the state of American society. It has been pro- practicing the values and in the telling and retelling of
posed that American childrens acquisition of moral stories about people behaving in accord with those
habits requires a renewed valuing of cultural traditions values. The fundamental traits of character include
and commitment to a sense of community, along with a honesty, compassion, courage, responsibility, self-
de-emphasis of individualism. Positions emphasizing discipline, and loyalty.
Building Character and Strengthening Communities 797

The premise that traits of character are ingrained in moral point of view, than the earlier emerging moral
cultural traditions and held in respect by the majority of sensibilities. Such a reversal of often-held conceptions
Americans may appear contradictory with the proposi- of development (where reasoning and reflection are ad-
tion put forth by the same writers that American soci- vances built on earlier reflexive processes) is based on
ety has lost its moral compass and is experiencing the idea that morality stems from natural emotions,
moral decay (the latter premise suggests that Americans whose emergence is best facilitated by early experiences
do not possess the necessary traits). This potential con- in appropriate types of families (i.e., defined by Wilson
tradiction is explained as due to a discrepancy between as intact, heterosexual families where parents provide
the beliefs and values of the majority of Americans, love, nurturance, and act authoritatively). Evolution,
who constitute the mainstream, and an apparently Wilson argues, has selected for attachment or affiliative
highly influential minority of elites who have waged behavior. In addition to natural selection for reproduc-
an all-out assault on common sense and the common tive success, with a disposition toward self-interest,
values of the American people (Bennett, 1992, p. 13). there is a biologically based disposition for bonding and
In ways unspecified, the presumed elites rejection of attachment that takes the form of sociability. Innate so-
the idea of character, their antipathy to the culture, and ciability is the overriding component in producing four
their embrace of an ideology contrary to the beliefs of central sentiments that make up the moral life: sympa-
most Americans are said to have placed the country in thy (allowing people to be affected by the feelings and
moral crisis. experiences of others), fairness ( based on equity, reci-
These propositions are at root paradoxical because if procity, and impartiality), self-control (a necessary sen-
people acquire character and habits through the example sibility because conflicts arise between self-interest and
of others and if the majority of mainstream Americans the moral sense), and duty or conscience (which dictates
maintain the morally proper traits and beliefs, then it actions in the absence of sanctions).
would be expected that there are many examples from In keeping with a de-emphasis of individuals reason-
which children would learn. Presumably, children are ing and reflection, Wilson believes that morality is, in
exposed to exemplary virtues in most families, schools, most instances, local and parochial. In simple agricul-
and elsewhere. A similar paradox is seen in the proposi- tural communities, and in Western cultures prior to the
tions of Wilson (1993), another social scientist who Enlightenment, the moral sense applies to those who are
shares some of the emphasis on character, while at- similar and familiar to oneself ( kin and the local com-
tempting to account for evidence from psychology, an- munity). The idea that moral considerations should be
thropology, economics, and biology in formulating a universalized is a Western concept stemming from the
theory of morality and its acquisition based on emotions Enlightenment and the advent of individualism.
and innate sociability. Wilson sees a loss of confidence The idea that morality is constituted by moral senti-
in the use of the language of morality due to a prevailing ments guiding behavior in instinctive and reflexive ways
moral skepticism perpetuated by intellectuals who is akin to the idea that morality comprises habitual be-
question the scientific bases for morality and who fur- haviors reflecting traits or dispositions. Wilson goes be-
ther an ideology of individual autonomy and choice. At yond solely describing traits by attempting to explain
the same time, Wilsons explanation of morality is the sources of moral sensibility in biologically based
founded on the proposition that a natural moral sense dispositions toward sociability and attachments. This
emerged in the process of evolution and that most peo- type of link between emotions and habitual behaviors
ples moral behaviors are determined largely by emo- also has affinities with those who regard emotions as
tions and habits: Much of the time our inclination linked to habits based on commitments to community.
toward fair play or our sympathy for the plight of others
are immediate and instinctive, a reflex of our emotions
Habits and the Communitarian Spirit
more than an act of our intellect. . . . The feelings on
which people act are often superior to the arguments As noted, those who believe that individuals need to form
that they employ (Wilson, 1993, pp. 78). tighter and better attachments to communities that tran-
In Wilsons formulation, although reasoning, reflec- scend individual goals echo the theme of a moral
tion, and deliberation may emerge later in life than the decline in American society. Not surprisingly, the empha-
reflexive and habitual, they are less adaptive, from the sis on community is evident in works of sociologists
798 The Development of Morality

among the most visible are Etzionis (1993) The Spirit of tachments to family, church, and community. American
Community (also Etzioni, 1996) and Bellah et al.s society is hovering on the brink of disaster and slid-
(1985) Habits of the Heart. With regard to the moral sta- ing toward the abyss because individualism is no
tus of American society, Etzioni (1993) has asserted that longer balanced (Bellah et al., 1985, p. 284). There is
because of a waning of traditional values, without an af- now a radical individualism, characterized by isolation,
firmation of new values, we live in a state of everlasting separation, independence from the past (from a com-
moral confusion and social anarchy (p. 12). Bellah et al. munity of memory), with personal choice and individ-
(1985) assert that American individualism may have ual fulfillment placed over attachment to family, social
grown cancerous (p. vii), and that we seem to be hov- institutions (e.g., the church), and community. Moral
ering on the very brink of disaster . . . from the internal goals have been transformed into ones of economic ef-
incoherence of society (p. 284). They ask: How can we fectiveness, self-fulfillment, and personal satisfaction.
reverse the slide toward the abyss? (p. 284). The proposed solutionthe way to avoid the abyssis
The call for a greater sense of community is neces- to achieve a balance by attenuating individualism and by
sary to reverse a supposed breakdown in society con- restoring traditions, a sense of community, and concerns
nected to a supposed overemphasis on personal goals for the common good.
and individual rights that is part of the cultural ethos.
The cultural ethos of rights and individualism is seen Moral Appraisal and Moral Recommendations
as, in large measure, a contemporary phenomenon at
odds with the traditions of social commitment and re- The writings on character and the need for restoration
sponsibility. To a good extent, a return to past practices of a sense of community have applied components be-
is required. Much of the reason there is a need for a re- cause much of the focus is on changing society and peo-
turn to past practices and recommitment to moral val- ple. However, the validity of the proposed social
ues, restoration of law and order, and rebuilding of the recommendations rest on assertions about the moral
foundation of society is that Americans have become state of society in the present and past (i.e., moral crisis,
overly concerned with rights (there has been an explo- decay now and a better moral state of affairs then);
sion of rights, and incessant issuance of new rights) about the nature of individuals morality in the past and
and a concomitant elevation of the unbridled pursuit present (i.e., firm character traits and commitment to
of self-interest and greed to the level of social virtue virtues and community then but not now); and about the
(Etzioni, 1993, p. 24). However, the call is not for an causes of moral problems in the present (i.e., selfish-
elimination of all personal rights, but for a renewed ness, individualism, failures of commitment and commu-
balance of rights and responsibilities. To accomplish nity, and changes in family life). Those assertions are
such a balance, Etzioni recommends a moratorium on subject to social scientific analysis and imply assump-
virtually all new rights for a decade, a reaffirmation of tions about the process of successful moral development.
responsibilities, and a restoration of communities. Es- It is assumed that in the past morality was acquired
sential to the renewal are changes in family structure through training in character or commitment to family
ranging from maintaining two-parent families (such as and community and that society was then more success-
by legislating a lengthy waiting period for remarriage ful morally. On that basis, it is assumed that adequate
after divorce) to reinstating the ritual of the family moral development should proceed (and be facilitated)
meal. as it did in the past. These assumptions about the psy-
Bellah et al.s (1985) analyses of late-twentieth-cen- chology of moral development, however, are not
tury American culture adopt the idea of national char- grounded on detailed psychological and developmental
acter the American character is firmly, and analyses or empirical evidence. Instead, the line of rea-
legitimately from the moral point of view, individualis- soning rests on certain key untested assumptions.
tic. In the past, American individualism was character- One is the repeatedly stated assumption of the moral
ized by personal autonomy, self-reliance, and individual downslide. The causes attributed to the moral decline of
initiative, with beliefs in the dignity of the individual, a the society are quite varied, and many of those causes
valuing of equality, and questioning of fixed social reflect disagreements about the events with others who
ranks and subjugation of persons. would regard them as having promoted moral goals. As
In its traditional form, however, individualism was examples, many would regard as furthering moral ends
balanced with commitment to the moral order and at- events in the 1960s (especially the anti-Vietnam War
Emphasizing Emotions 799

and civil rights movements), feminism, and the asser- seeing stereotypic impressions and speculations of the
tion of rights for groups faced with discrimination. Fur- morality of the present, along with nostalgic views of the
thermore, the only evidence provided for the sweeping past (Turiel, 2002). Without solid evidence of negative
claims of moral decline is some data, open to varying changes in the morality of the society, it cannot be con-
interpretations, on increases (since the 1950s) of rates cluded that the ways morality was transmitted in the past
of suicide, homicide, and out-of-wedlock births are the most efficacious (similar considerations apply to
(Wynne, 1986). claims that recent activities, such as of the 1960s or fem-
The vast societal changes over the past two centuries, inism, have caused moral decay). It may be that the pro-
however, make it very difficult to document whether posed explanations of the process of moral development
there has been decay, improvement, or simply patterns would hold for the past (and present) even in the absence
of positive and negative changes associated with differ- of any considerations of moral decline. However, the as-
ent realms of social life. To cite some salient examples sertions about how morality was transmitted or acquired
of morally relevant (and often viewed as positive) socie- in the past are themselves undocumented.
tal shifts, there have been changes in (a) race relations At best, the propositions regarding moral develop-
and treatment of minority groups; ( b) the roles, burdens, ment as the acquisition of character traits or as commit-
and privileges of women; (c) the treatment of children ment to community must be seen as standing alongside
and the conditions of work for children; (d) the work- several other competing explanations. However, the
force and labor relations more generally; (e) the care of basic concepts used still require research so as to know
the elderly; (f ) the levels of political representation of more about the parameters of the habitual, the criteria
many groups (including women); (g) the numbers of for an adequate commitment to community, and how
people receiving higher levels of education; and ( h) the these are acquired. Several of the psychological, social,
power and authority relationships among those of higher and cultural issues raised by those lamenting the moral
and lower social classes. state of society arise in research on moral development.
No analyses have been provided of the ways all these For instance, the propositions regarding the family as a
changes might constitute some betterment of the lives of central influence on moral development through
people or of how past practices may have produced parental example and training represents only one per-
harm. Even with regard to violence and homicidefor spective on the family. Others have attempted to ac-
which there are statistics documenting its prevalence in count for the effects of the structure (e.g., extent of a
contemporary societythere are good indications that hierarchical structure) of the family, its particular prac-
they are traditional in American society and were preva- tices in terms of their fairness and justice, and the con-
lent in the past (Butterfield, 1995). Moreover, to the ex- tent of communications and proclaimed ideology.
tent that there is documentation regarding levels of Furthermore, many researchers have given a fair amount
honesty, it reveals that even in the early part of the twen- of emphasis to the influences of other social experiences
tieth century children exhibited a fair amount of dishon- (e.g., with peers, in school, or in relation to culture), to
esty in school activities and in experimental tasks the ways children account for heterogeneity of social ex-
(Hartshorne & May, 1928, 1929, 1930). And strikingly periences, and to their ways of constructing judgments
similar concerns with moral decay existed in Western about those different dimensions of social experience.
countries. During the 1920s, much concern was ex-
pressed in the United States regarding the moral state of
youth, cultural disintegration, and social chaos (see
Fass, 1977). Similarly, in fin-de-sicle France (the late EMPHASIZING EMOTIONS
1800s), there were widespread concerns with moral de-
generation, national decline, the declining morality of Emotions have been considered the basis for morality by
youth (e.g., an explosion of juvenile crime rates), and some philosophers, and have been central in certain psy-
calls for renewal of the society (see Norris, 1996). In chologists formulations. As already noted, the Freudian
each instance, similar claims were made about moral and behavioristic conceptions relied heavily on emo-
crisis and decay in society and about the good old days tions as the basis for the acquisition of morality
(see Turiel, 2002, for discussion of these issues). though in different ways from each other. However, both
These examples demonstrate the complexity of social saw the acquisition of morally obligatory actions as a
and moral change through history and suggest that we are process by which aversive emotions are central to moral
800 The Development of Morality

learning and maintenance. Therefore, most emphasis similar to the others feelings in that it entails an other-
was given to emotions of fear, anxiety, shame, and guilt oriented response and concern for that persons well-
in their explanations of moral acquisition. Aversive emo- being. Empathy can also result, by contrast, in personal
tions were seen as forces that served to transform the in- distress, which entails a self-focused aversive reaction
dividuals natural inclinations into needs and desires in (e.g., anxiety, discomfort) to the distress of another; the
a psychological make-up that included a conscience or motivation is to alleviate ones own aversive state (see
behaviors consistent with societal norms or values. Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990, 1991; Eisenberg, Fabes, &
However, a major shift in thinking about emotions in the Spinrad in Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume, for
late 1970s and through the 1980s entailed a focus on at- further discussion). There is also evidence that chil-
tachment, bonding, love, sympathy, and empathy. drens feelings of empathy are related to their prosocial
The emphasis on these emotions included continued actions (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987; Eisenberg & Strayer,
concerns with the influences of the family (in keeping 1987). In particular, measures of facial and psychologi-
with Freudian and behavioristic accounts), the role of cal indexes of affect have shown that sympathy, and not
aversive emotions, and a renewed interest in the evolu- personal distress, is positively related to prosocial ac-
tionary sources of emotions. Research demonstrating tions motivated by concerns for the welfare of others
that very young children display positive emotions and (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1991).
affiliate and bond with others was particularly influen- The research findings on sympathy and prosocial ac-
tial in the shift (Dunn, 1987, 1988; Dunn, Brown, & tions are inconsistent with the idea that children, before
Maguire, 1995; Hoffman, 1991a, 1991b; Kochanska, they have internalized parental values, or societal stan-
1993, 1994). Another set of relevant findings show that dards, or have been taught to behave in socially sanc-
young children are sensitive to the interests and well- tioned ways, will act solely in selfish and self-directed
being of others, producing actions of a prosocial or al- ways when they are not coerced or fearful of detection. It
truistic nature. That body of research is not reviewed in does not necessarily follow that sympathy and sponta-
this chapter as it is the topic of the chapter by Eisenberg, neous prosocial behaviors at a very young age reflect in-
Fabes, and Spinrad in Chapter 11, this Handbook, this nate dispositions or that morality is primarily based on
volume, but the general pattern of findings is that young emotions (to be discussed). Moreover, questions still
children engage in acts of sharing and helping or altru- exist regarding the validity of age-related findings in em-
ism. As put by Zahn-Waxler, Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, pathic responses, the need to draw further distinctions
and Chapman (1992): The evidence for early moral in- between closely aligned emotions, and the development
ternalization, however, highlights the need to reformu- of more adequate methods of measurement (Eisenberg &
late theories emphasizing the egocentricism and Fabes, 1991). The findings on sympathy and prosocial
narcissism of young children (p. 133). Studies con- actions are not inconsistent with emotive positions on
ducted in the home show that even children under 2 morality. In several formulations, it has been proposed
years of age share possessions (e.g., toys) with others, that morality is directed more by emotions than reason-
help mothers with household tasks, cooperate in games, ing (Dunn, 1987, 1988; Haidt, 2001; Hoffman, 1984,
and respond to the emotional distress of others (Radke- 1991b; Kagan, 1984; Kochanska, 1993, 1994).
Yarrow, Zahn-Waxler, & Chapman, 1983). Toddlers and
young children, in addition, show comfort and engage in
A Primacy for Empathy
caregiving of others. It also appears that reactions of
empathy emerge by age 3 (Lennon & Eisenberg, 1987; Empathy has been considered primary in moral devel-
Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992). Moreover, distinctions drawn opment by some who do not rely heavily on associations
among reactions of empathy have a bearing on the rela- of unpleasant and pleasant affect with morality. Hoff-
tions of emotions to prosocial or altruistic behaviors man (1991a, 1991b, 2000) has put forth a formulation
(Carlo, 2006; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990, 1991; Eisen- combining emotion due to evolution with internaliza-
berg, Spinard, & Sadorsky, 2006). tion, in that the societys moral norms and values [are
Empathy, defined as an emotional response stem- made] part of the individuals personal motive system
ming from anothers emotional state, can result in sym- (1991a, p. 106). In addition to emotion and internaliza-
pathy or personal distress. Sympathy goes beyond tion, this approach includes motives, cognition, moral
solely experiencing an emotional reaction to another principles of care and justice, and perspective taking.
Emphasizing Emotions 801

Despite the attempt to incorporate all these features, it for the fourth stage are childrens awareness of self and
can be said that primacy is given to emotion because the others with separate identities. These conceptions allow
linchpin is empathy. for awareness that others feel pleasure and pain in their
Hoffman distinguishes his approach from those giv- general life experiences. At this stage, empathy is felt in
ing primacy to moral judgments in that he defines moral particular situations, as well as for more general life cir-
actions in motivational terms. A moral act is a disposi- cumstances of others or of groups of people (e.g., the
tion to do something on behalf of another person, or to poor or the oppressed).
behave in accord with a moral norm or standard bearing Whether this sequence of stages is an accurate repre-
on human welfare or justice (Hoffman, 1991b, p. 276). sentation of how children develop is undetermined be-
The distinction between defining a moral act in terms of cause the stages were not, for the most part, based on
moral judgment or motives is not unambiguous (Blasi, empirical evidence. There is some evidence that infants
1993; Turiel, 2003a). It could be said that the moral respond to the actual crying of other infants to a greater
judgments one makessay that one should come to the extent than to sounds resembling the crying of human in-
aid of another in distress because it is wrong to allow fants (Sagi & Hoffman, 1976). However, it is not en-
sufferingmotivates one to act. The key to the distinc- tirely clear that this type of response is a form of very
tion is in the term disposition in the definition of a moral early empathy. The other stages have not been tested
actdisposition referring to an emotional reaction that empirically and, instead, rely on illustrations with the
propels action. The main source of moral motives is the types of anecdotal examples previously mentioned. Re-
feeling of empathy, which is defined as an affective search by Zahn-Waxler and her colleagues (Hastings,
response that does not necessarily match anothers Zahn-Waxler, & McShane, 2006; Zahn-Waxler, Robin-
affective state. By putting the matter in affective- son, & Emde, 1992; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992) does pro-
motivational terms, Hoffman poses the question, Why vide some evidence that young children show empathic
act morally? and answers in terms of feelings that need reactions to the distress of others and attempt to under-
to be acted on. stand the nature of the distress (see Eisenberg, Fabes, &
Although empathy is regarded as a biological predispo- Spinard, Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume, for
sition and a product of natural selection, it is character- discussion of further distinctions in the general con-
ized as developing through four stagelike manifestations struct of empathy).
that are partly determined by changing cognitive capabili- In later writings, Hoffman (2000) attempted to iden-
ties. The first of these stages is characterized simply by tify situations in which empathic reactions and moral
the global distress felt by infants (during the 1st year) actions occur. He labeled these moral encounters and
entailing a confusion of the infants own feelings with proposed that five encompass most of the prosocial
those of another. At the second stage of egocentric em- domain (Hoffman, 2000, p. 3). These include situa-
pathy (age of 1 year), the onset of object permanence al- tions in which a person is an innocent bystander (wit-
lows for an awareness that other people are physically nessing someone in pain or distress), a transgressor
distinct from the self and a concern (sympathetic dis- ( harming or about to harm someone), or a virtual
tress) with another person who is in distress. However, transgressor (an imagined harmful act). The two oth-
children do not distinguish between their own or others ers are situations in which there are multiple moral
internal states. claimants (where a person has to make choices about
Hoffman further asserts that role taking emerges at who to help) and in which there is a clash between car-
about 2 or 3 years of age (this, however, is a controver- ing and justice ( between considering others and ab-
sial issue), allowing for a differentiation of the childs stract issues of rights, duty, and reciprocity). These
own feelings from those of others. At the third stage, categories are meant to capture the situations that
therefore, children are responsive to cues about the evoke guilt and empathic responses. Empathy is also
other persons feelings and empathize with a range of supposedly associated with a variety of moral reac-
emotions other than distress (e.g., disappointment, feel- tions, including sympathy, aggression or anger at an-
ings of betrayal). Whereas the third stage is labeled other who injures people, guilt, and feelings of
empathy for anothers feelings, the fourth stage, injustice (empathy due to perceived unfairness of a sit-
emerging in late childhood is labeled empathy for an- uation). Indeed, in Hoffmans perspective, moral prin-
others life conditions. The relevant social cognitions ciples of care and injustice are validated by emotions.
802 The Development of Morality

Hoffman invoked a distinction between cool and the temptation to engage in a prohibited act (e.g., often
hot cognitions with regard to moral principles. Moral measured in experimental situations). A consistent find-
principles, in that view, can be so-called cool cognitions ing from these studies is that parental practices of in-
because they are detached from emotions. The associa- duction are the most successful method of discipline
tion of empathy with principles renders them hot (Hoffman, 1970; Hoffman & Saltzstein, 1967; Maccoby
morally meaningful and linked to action. At least as & Martin, 1983). Measures reflecting moral develop-
common, if not more common, a perspective, especially ment are correlated more with induction than love with-
among philosophers ( but also see Baldwin, 1896; drawal or power assertion. For example, a moral
Kohlberg, 1971; Piaget, 1932), is that to the extent that orientation based on fear of sanctions is correlated with
moral principles are understood by people in ways that parental practices of physical punishment, whereas ex-
are part of their belief systems and mental functioning, it pressions of guilt and an internal orientation are corre-
is not necessary that they receive their force from other lated with parental practices that emphasize explaining
elements ( like empathy) so as to render them meaning- reasons for avoiding or engaging in moral actions.
ful. Instead, there is a synthesis between judgments and
emotions, making it difficult to disentangle the two.
Conscience and Internalization

Evolution and Internalization By including natural or biological features, these per-


spectives go beyond earlier socialization perspectives
The formulations of morality emphasizing emotions il- by which it was assumed that morality could be ade-
lustrate that, in many instances, asking whether theories quately defined through consensual norms (Berkowitz,
are based on nature or nurture, or biology or environ- 1964; Maccoby, 1968; Skinner, 1971). However, some
ment, is not useful. These positions show a firm orienta- contemporary researchers have addressed hypotheses
tion to evolutionary-based biological processes and to regarding moral internalizationdefining morality
influences of the family, historical contexts, and cul- through consensual normswith the assumption that
ture. Much of the research on the internalization side morality entails the acquisition of a conscience serving
has focused on the family, examining the types of to internally regulate conduct consistent with societal
parental child-rearing practices producing more and values, norms, or rules (Kochanska, 1993, 1994;
stronger incorporations of moral standards by children. Thompson, Mayer, & McGinley, 2006). The concept of
A large body of research (for reviews, see Eisenberg, conscience, central to Freuds theory, was also central
Fabes, & Spinard, Chapter 11, this Handbook, this vol- to behavioristic conceptions in which internalization
ume; Hoffman, 1970; Maccoby & Martin, 1983) has ex- was theorized to be acquired through the anxiety associ-
amined, mainly through self-reports, parental ated with punishments for transgressions (Aronfreed,
child-rearing practices, along with various measures of 1968). Whether it be from a psychoanalytic or behavior-
moral functioning. Three types of parental practices istic perspective, the concept of conscience has been
were identified. One is referred to as power assertion, used to refer to a mechanism internalized by children for
mainly involving physical punishment, deprivation of exerting control on needs that would otherwise be acted
goods or privileges, and threats of force. The second, on: Conscience is the term that has been used tradi-
love withdrawal, involves disapproval and other expres- tionally to refer to the cognitive and affective processes
sions of the removal of affection or emotional supports. which constitute an internalized moral governor over an
The third type, referred to as induction, entails the com- individuals conduct (Aronfreed, 1968, p. 2).
munication of reasons or explanations for the prescribed In a contemporary formulation that has affinities
behavior, including appeals for concerns with the wel- with earlier positions on conscience and that includes
fare of others. elements of other socioemotional perspectives, Kochan-
Parental reports of their use of these discipline tech- ska (1993, 1994) has examined conscience as regulation
niques have been correlated with measures of childrens due to internalization marking successful socialization
guilt (e.g., childrens tendencies to confession to mis- as the gradual developmental shift from external to in-
deeds; projective measures of story completions), an ex- ternal regulation that results in the childs ability to
ternal or internal orientation to moral stories (i.e., if conform to societal standards of conduct and to restrain
they judge by fear of external sanctions or by an evalua- antisocial or destructive impulses, even in the absence
tion of the acts wrongness), and whether children resist of surveillance (1993, pp. 325326). Moreover, the for-
Emphasizing Emotions 803

mation of conscience is functional from the societal per- and entailed secure attachments (Kochanska, Aksan,
spective: Without reliance on internalized con- Knaack, & Rhines, 2004). Greater mutuality in the form
sciences, societies would have to instill ever-present of cooperation and shared positive affect was associated
surveillance in all aspects of social life (Kochanska, with the measures of conscience. Similarly, it has been
1994, p. 20). This position includes a shift in balance found that more secure attachments at 14 months of age
away from natural moral propensities of concerns with were correlated with measures of conscience. In this ap-
the welfare of others back to more of an emphasis on the proach, the quality of the mother-child relationship is
need to control antisocial and destructive tendencies. proposed to influence internalization of and compliance
Ultimately, it is society that has to control the behavior with parental standards. As put by Kochanska et al.
of individuals, either by instilling control internally in (2004), security may make the child eager to embrace
children or through continual and all-encompassing (in parental demands. A secure child is cooperative and re-
all aspects of social life) external control. ceptive to parental demands, emulates the parent and
In keeping with the traditional conception of con- follows parental suggestions (p. 1234).
science, it was proposed that it is encompassed by af-
fective discomfort or the various aversive emotional
Beyond Family and beyond Incorporation of
reactions to acts of transgression and behavioral con-
Societal Standards
trol. Reactions of sympathy and empathy contribute to
the process of development, but they do so through the Findings on temperament are not consistent. A longitu-
anxiety and distress they can arouse in a child. One focus dinal study by Dunn et al. (1995) showed, in contrast
is on anxiety, fear arousal, and discomfort in the process with the other studies, that shy children (i.e., inhibited,
of internalizing moral prohibitions. A significant aspect nonimpulsive, and anxious) scored lower on the same
of this process is that parental socialization contributes measures of moral orientation than children who were
greatly through arousal of childrens anxiety. not shy. Dunn et al. also found that along with some pos-
Kochanska and her colleagues have continued this itive correlations between nonpower-assertive parental
line of research in a series of studies aimed at examining practices and moral orientation, other factors were asso-
what they refer to as bidirectional models of mother- ciated with moral orientation, including the quality of
child relationships. In these cases, the bidirectional con- the childs relationships with older siblings (children
ceptualizations of relationships remain within the who had friendlier, more positive relationships with sib-
context of a conception of conscience or morality as the lings showed higher moral orientation scores) and the
internalization of values, norms, and behaviors estab- childs earlier level of understanding of emotions (chil-
lished by parents. Some of these studies, for example, dren who had shown better emotional understandings at
were designed to examine the role of childrens tempera- earlier ages scored higher on moral orientation at first
ment in the formation of conscience (Kochanska, 1997; grade). Moreover, Dunn et al. found differences among
Kochanska, Gross, Linn, & Nichols, 2002; Kochanska, the stories used in the assessments. At kindergarten and
Murray, & Coy, 1997). Anxiety, fearfulness, and first grade, children gave more empathic responses to a
arousal (e.g., as found for shy children) underlie the af- physical harm story than to a story dealing with cheat-
fective component of conscience, and impulsivity and ing in a game. Correspondingly, more children gave
inhibition are related to behavioral control. Specifically, guilt responses (i.e., reparative endings in the story
impulsive children are more likely to transgress and completions) to the physical harm story than to the
find it more difficult to internalize conscience than non- cheating story.
impulsive children. Thus, parents methods of socializa- Findings in the Dunn et al. (1995) study, as well as
tion may work differently for children with different from a study by Dunn, Cutting, and Demetriou (2000),
temperaments. The practice of induction (which in- suggest that influences on moral development extend be-
volves explanations and reasoning) may be less effective yond the practices of parents in disciplining children
with impulsive than nonimpulsive children. and that a childs reactions to transgressions are not uni-
In other aspects of the research program, the type of form. Other research indicates that young childrens de-
mother-child relationships was correlated with mea- velopment may proceed in several directions with regard
sures of conscience. In particular, measures were ob- to relationships with parents and in their orientations to
tained of the extent to which mother-child relationships morality. Along with an increased awareness of stan-
were mutually responsive (Kochanska & Murray, 2000) dards, at the age of 2 or 3 years, young children display
804 The Development of Morality

increased teasing of their mothers, more physical ag- tions in explanations of social development. There is ev-
gression and destruction of objects, and greater interest idence (some of which is reviewed by Grusec and Good-
in what is socially prohibited (Dunn, 1987). Along with now) that the effectiveness of particular parental
greater sympathy and empathy for others, with increas- practices are not uniform and that parents do not consis-
ing age children may begin to understand how to manip- tently use one type of discipline. Mothers use different
ulate situations and upset others. This increasing reasons for different kinds of transgressions. Smetana
complexity of young childrens social relationships is (1989b) found that mothers of toddlers used explana-
also evident by their abilities, by 18 to 36 months, to en- tions of needs and rights for acts entailing harm to oth-
gage in arguments and counterarguments in disputes ers, whereas they used explanations pertaining to social
with mothers (Dunn & Munn, 1987). By 36 months, order and conformity for violations of social conven-
children also provide justifications for their positions in tions. It also appears that mothers vary their methods in
disputes with mothers and siblings (see also Kuczynski, accord with the types of standard violated. Working
Kochanska, Radke-Yarrow, & Girnius-Brown, 1987). with families of children from 6 to 10 years of age, Chil-
Disputes occurred over issues such as rights and needs amkurti and Milner (1993) found that mothers report
of persons, conventions (manners, etiquette), and de- using reasons or explanations mainly for moral trans-
struction or aggression. Childrens emotional reactions gressions and forceful verbal commands for con-
also varied by the different kinds of disputes; distress ventional transgressions. Furthermore, parents use a
and anger were associated with disputes affecting chil- combination of power assertion and reasoning in reac-
drens rights and interests. These differentiations and tion to acts like lying and stealing, whereas reasoning is
extensions of the influences of social relationships are used in reaction to a childs failure to show concern for
consistent with a reconceptualization of moral internal- others (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994).
ization presented by Grusec and Goodnow (1994), Other findings in accord with these propositions
Grusec, Goodnow, and Kuczynski (2000), Kuczynski stem from studies of childrens evaluations of parental
and Hildebrandt (1997), Grusec (2006), and Kuczynski discipline, as well as of correspondences between the
and Navara (2006). judgments of children and adults (studies on parent-
Grusec and Goodnow (1994) maintained that the tra- adolescent relationships are discussed later in the chap-
ditional view of internalization as the process by which ter). Catron and Masters (1993) showed that 10- to 12-
children take over the values of society has significant year-old children and mothers endorsed corporal
limitations and is not consistent with existing data. A punishment (spanking) for prudential (i.e., acts harmful
better understanding of the process requires accounting to the self, such as opening a bottle of poison) and moral
for additional factors, including the nature of the act (the transgressions to a greater extent than for transgressions
misdeed or transgression), characteristics of parents, the of social conventions. These findings indicate both that
childs perspective on the position of parents, and the mothers make discriminations in the ways discipline
childs perceptions of the misdeed. Furthermore, they should be used and that by at least 10 years of age chil-
argue that it is necessary to consider the childs ability dren make similar judgments about that type of disci-
to move beyond the parents specific position to one of pline. Research by Saltzstein, Weiner, and Munk (1995)
his or her own, a consideration that points to successful on judgments about moral intentionality and conse-
socialization as more than an unquestioning adoption of quences shows that children evaluate the fairness of
anothers position (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994, p. 4). As mothers (in hypothetical situations) approval or disap-
summarized by Grusec et al. (2000): proval of actions in accord with their own judgments re-
garding those actions. For example, children regard a
A significant shift is required to understand the process of mother who disapproves of a well-intentioned act result-
socialization. This shift will be facilitated by an explicit ing in a negative outcome more unfair than a mother who
interest in the agency of parents and children, that is, in approves it. Moreover, children whose own judgments
the meanings they construct of each others behavior, in were based on the actors intentions made greater dis-
their capacity for strategic action, and in their ability to tinctions in evaluations of mothers approval or disap-
behave as if the other is also an agent. (p. 205) proval than children whose judgments were based on the
consequences of the act. All these findings indicate that
In essence, they call for a reorientation in research children apply their judgments to parental acts in ways
that would take seriously the idea of reciprocal interac- that involve both acceptance and critical scrutiny.
Gender, Emotions, and Moral Judgments 805

It has also been found that childrens own judgments sition and resistance. From the perspective of moral de-
about intentions and consequences are not concordant velopment as internalization of parental or societal
with the judgments they attribute to adults (Saltzstein norms, the good is defined as some form of compliance
et al., 1987). For acts with positive intentions and nega- to the social environment. Social accommodation on the
tive outcomes, children (incorrectly) believe adults part of the child is thus regarded as the desirable end-
judgments of wrongness would be harsher than their state. In a later section, I consider research on opposi-
own; whereas children judge by intentions, they believe tion and resistance to social norms, societal
that adults judgments are mainly based on disobedience arrangements, and cultural practices that stem from a
or rule violations. moral standpoint.
These findings indicate that it is necessary to account
for the childs perspective and, thereby, view the pro-
cess of discipline as interactive. In particular, Grusec
and colleagues maintained that because childrens judg- GENDER, EMOTIONS, AND
ments differ for different types of misdeeds (e.g., moral MORAL JUDGMENTS
as opposed to conventional transgressions; Turiel,
1983a), they would evaluate and judge the appropriate- The major issues considered thus faremotion, social-
ness of the reasons given by parents, or others, when dis- ization, and interactionalso have received scrutiny in
ciplining the child. It has been found that children are theory and research on gender differences in moral de-
more responsive to adults directives when the adults velopment. The question of gender differences has been
use reasons that correspond to the ways children classify posed regarding many aspects of development (Maccoby
moral actions. For example, when teachers simply point & Jacklin, 1974), but it has been of particular contro-
to rule violations in discussing acts like stealing or hit- versy in the moral realm because in the early part of the
ting, children are less responsive than when teachers un- century it was asserted (most notably by Freud) that the
derscore the welfare of others or fairness (Killen, 1991; morality of females is less developed than that of males,
Nucci, 1984). It has also been found that children are and then, in the latter part of the century, that the moral-
more likely to share with others when given reasons ity of females is qualitatively different from that of
based on empathy and concern for others than when to males (Gilligan, 1977, 1982; Gilligan & Wiggins, 1987).
adhere to norms (Eisenberg-Berg & Geisheker, 1979). Gilligan and her colleagues maintained that two moral
Several other features of communications from par- injunctions define two sequences of moral develop-
ents to children bear on the effectiveness of discipline. mentthe injunction not to treat others unfairly ( jus-
These include verifiability of its truth value, the level of tice) and the injunction to not turn away from someone
generality of reprimands, whether they are tangential or in need (care). Gilligan (1982) argued that a morality of
directly relevant to the misdeed, and whether statements care, mainly linked to females, had been overlooked in
are direct or indirect. Distinctions need to be made in favor of analyses of justice because mainly males had
discipline activities to understand how they are inter- formulated explanations of moral development. These
preted and how they might lead to changes in childrens assertions, however, have generated controversy among
behaviors. Along those lines, it is proposed that charac- students of moral development, as well as in other social
teristics of the parents and children would also make a scientific disciplines (Abu-Lughod, 1991; Okin, 1989;
difference in the ways discipline is interpreted and felt. Stack, 1990), within feminist scholarship (Faludi, 1991)
Unlike the traditional views of conscience or inter- and in journalistic accounts (Pollitt, 1992).
nalization, the model presented by Grusec and Goodnow In a way, Gilligan accepts Freuds (1925/1959) con-
includes the idea that internalization is not necessarily tention that women show less sense of justice than
the sole desired goal of parents or the only positive out- men. She does not accept Freuds contention that
come from the societal or individual perspectives. Par- women show less moral sense than men because women
ents may strive for flexibility and initiative on the part show more of a sense of the alternative form of care. A
of the child rather than simply the adoption of parental morality of justice fails to account for womens moral
standards. They may also be motivated by the goal that orientation because it focuses on rules and rights. Ac-
children acquire negotiation and thinking skills. Grusec cording to Gilligan (1982), justice links development to
and Goodnow raised the issue of noncompliance for pos- the logic of equality and reciprocity, which contrasts
itive goals and thereby raised the specter of social oppo- with the logic underlying an ethic of care [which] is a
806 The Development of Morality

psychological logic of relationships (p. 73). The moral- childs awareness of being smaller, less powerful, and
ity of care is one of fulfillment of responsibility and less competent than older children and adults (Gilligan
avoidance of exploitation and hurt and is linked to con- & Wiggins, 1987). For girls, the experience of inequal-
cepts of self as attached to social networks, whereas the ity is not as overwhelming as for boys because girls
morality of justice is linked to concepts of self as au- identify with the object of their attachment (mother).
tonomous and detached from social networks. Because boys identify with their fathers without a
It would appear then that the formulation of a moral- strong attachment with him, they relate more to the fa-
ity of care has affinities with those who emphasize emo- thers authority and power. Inequality and authority are
tions. Care entails avoidance of harm and concerns for therefore salient for boys, resulting in strivings for
the welfare of others (sympathy and empathy) and is ap- equality (part of fairness) and regulation as moral ends.
plied mainly to those in close relationships. Although Gilligan regards the care and justice orientations as
empathy and sympathy are relevant, this formulation systems of moral judgments. In fact, she considers the
differs in several respects from other perspectives em- conception of justice and fairness as one of the two
phasizing emotions. First, the central emotions for major types of morality and, at least implicitly, accepts
morality are defined differently from empathy, sympa- the validity of the stages of moral judgment formulated
thy, shame, and guilt and are associated with a different by Kohlberg (1969). The morality of care, too, proceeds
set of experiences and mechanisms for the development through a sequence of transformations culminating in a
of morality. Second, more emphasis is given to judg- level of thinking based on universal principles encom-
ments in both moralities. And third, there is a sequence passing self and others, with an understanding that self
of development for the morality of care progressing to- and other are interdependent, violence is destructive,
ward increasing inclusiveness of moral judgments. and care benefits others and self. That level of moral
Very young childrens relationships constitute the judgment is preceded by two less advanced levels, and
groundwork for the types of morality formed by individ- associated transitions, reflecting a conflict between
uals. Two dimensions of relationships are proposed as self and other that constitutes the central moral prob-
mechanisms for development at early ages, establishing lem for women. At the first level, there is a focus on
long-term moral orientations. First, the experience of caring for the self as a means of survival. At the second
attachment, which produces awareness that one can af- level, concepts of responsibility focus on care for de-
fect others and be affected by them, results in discover- pendent persons.
ies of the ways people care for and hurt one another. The sequence of womens conceptions of the moral-
Relying on neo-psychoanalytic accounts of identity for- ity of care was derived from interview studies. The main
mation (Chodorow, 1978), there is a basic difference in study entailed interviews of 29 pregnant women (ages 15
the social experiences of boys and girls that results, by to 33 years) about their decision to have or not to have an
an early age (3 or 4 years), in differences in their per- abortion. Follow-up interviews were conducted with 21
sonality and identity. For girls, identity formation oc- of the women a year after they had made their choices. A
curs in the context of a relationship with another female, necessary feature of this study, according to Gilligan
the mother, which maintains continuity and in which (1982), is that the interviews were about situations faced
mothers and daughters see themselves as alike. Most in the womens own lives. This is because womens
important, in forming her identity as a female, the young moral judgments are tied more closely to contexts than
girl maintains an attachment with her mother, and mens. Interviews about hypothetical situations, in her
thereby development progresses toward creating and view, are likely to provide misleading information since
sustaining relationships. Thus, the emotions associated women attempt to reframe hypothetical situations into
with attachment and care are co-feelings. real, contextualized ones. Gilligan also asserted that in-
For young boys, identity formation occurs in the con- terviews about hypothetical situations are more likely to
text of a sense of difference (in both mother and son), elicit justice concerns than would interviews about real-
and in the process of forming a masculine identity there life situations. Nevertheless, hypothetical situations
is separation from the mother and individuation. The (along with judgments about situations generated by
consequence is an orientation to differentiations from participants) have been used to study levels of develop-
others and independence on the part of boys. The second ment of judgments about care (Skoe & Gooden, 1993;
related dimension is the inequality that stems from the Skoe et al., 1999). Those studies indicate that levels of
Gender, Emotions, and Moral Judgments 807

care reasoning are associated with age and that there are and social order) more than women (Kohlberg &
some variations by nation in the levels attained by fe- Kramer, 1969). Briefly speculating on these results,
males and males (i.e., Canadian and U. S. females score Kohlberg and Kramer (1969) suggested that Stage 3
higher than males, but no gender differences were ob- moral thinking might be functional for the roles of
tained among Norwegians). Skoe, Eisenberg, and Cum- housewives and mothers. The generalizability of this
berland (2002) found that self-reports of sympathy by finding, with regard to the stages formulated by
adults are associated with care reasoning. Kohlberg, was accepted by Gilligan, but she was critical
Gilligan (1982) has argued that the study of the judg- of the idea that Stage 3 was functional for the roles of
ments of females serves to correct biases in influential housewives or mothers and proposed that instead
theories of moral development put forth by males who womens reasoning proceeds through the sequence of
largely overlooked females or who, when they addressed the morality of care.
the issue, superficially relegated females morality of However, the conclusion that women score lower on
care to a lesser form. Freud, for example, included Kohlbergs stages (as drawn by Kohlberg and Kramer
women in his observations and case studies but misin- and reaffirmed by Gilligan) has not been supported em-
terpreted their care orientation simply as a concern with pirically. Walker (1984, 1991) presented extensive re-
approval. Gilligan also contends that others, such as Pi- views of 80 studies, which included assessments of
aget (1932) and Kohlberg (1963), constructed their the- males and females on Kohlbergs stages. Those analyses
ories through research with samples of males and then reveal little in the way of sex differences on this dimen-
studied females from the inappropriate perspective of sion. In most studies (86% of the samples), no differ-
male-based theories. ences were obtained. In some samples (9%), males
In considering Piagets ideas, Gilligan imposes cer- scored higher than females, but in other samples (6%), it
tainty where ambiguity exists. Piaget did maintain that was the reverse. Walker also found that when re-
girls are less interested than boys with legal elabora- searchers controlled for educational and occupational
tion and that the legal sense is far less developed in lit- levels, no sex differences were observed.
tle girls than in boys (Piaget, 1932, pp. 69, 75, and Furthermore, it is not generally accepted that
quoted in Gilligan, 1977, and Gilligan & Wiggins, Kohlbergs concept of morality at the most advanced
1987). As noted earlier, however, in Piagets view, the stages actually fails to account for judgments about in-
developmentally advanced level of autonomous morality terdependence and concerns with welfare (Gilligan con-
was organized by concerns with mutuality, reciprocity, strued Kohlbergs formulation as focusing on rights,
and cooperation. Piaget saw a strict legal sense for fixed rules, and separation). It has been argued that embedded
rules that left little room for innovation and tolerance as in Kohlbergs formulations of justice and fairness are
part of the less advanced form of heteronomous moral- considerations of respect for others and ways of main-
ity. Thus, it is not at all clear that Piaget regarded girls to taining social relationships that are nonexploitive, non-
be less advanced than boys because he thought that girls harmful, and that promote the welfare of persons (see
were oriented to tolerance, innovation with rules, and co- Boyd, 1983; Habermas, 1990b; Kohlberg, Levine, &
operation. Aside from his studies of childrens practices Hewer, 1983; Nunner-Winkler, 1984; Walker, 1991).
with game rules, Piagets research was not conducted
only with males. Piaget supported his interpretations
Care and Justice as Moral Orientations
with many interview excerpts ( he did not report statisti-
cal analyses) that included both boys and girls. Gilligans propositions have received a good deal of at-
In contrast, Kohlbergs (1963) original formulation tention, with some providing positive evaluations
of stages of the development of moral judgments was (Haste, 1993; Shweder & Haidt, 1993), and others
based on interviews with males only. The first studies pointing to inadequacies in sampling, procedures, re-
assessing Kohlbergs stages that included females search designs, and data analyses (see Colby & Damon,
showed college-age and adult women scoring at Stage 3 1983; Greeno & Maccoby, 1986; Luria, 1986; Mednick,
(entailing judgments of morality focused on interper- 1989). Gilligans formulation was not based on exten-
sonal considerations) more than men, and men scoring at sive research but initially on a combination of (a) the ar-
higher stages (mainly Stage 4, which entails judgments gument that a conception of morality as justice did not
of morality focused on maintenance of rules, authority, adequately characterize the moral judgments of females
808 The Development of Morality

because they were usually assessed in stages lower than females, Gilligan and her colleagues appear, in later
males (a conclusion that, as already discussed, does not writings, to be inconsistent or ambiguous about sex dif-
hold), and ( b) subjectively analyzed excerpts from a ferences, asserting that care and justice are concerns
limited number of boys and girls responding to moral that can be part of the thinking of males or females. The
dilemmas in Kohlbergs interview (Gilligan, 1982, conclusion drawn from a meta-analysis of research on
Chapter 2). care and justice orientations was that neither is used
The construct of a morality of care was also based on predominantly by women or men, though there is a ten-
the studies of women discussing abortion and of inter- dency for females to use more care related reasoning
views of college students. Those studies were limited in than males (Jaffee & Hyde, 2000).
that the samples were small and restricted to either
pregnant women discussing one particular contested
Moral Judgments, Orientations,
issue (abortion) or students in elite universities. Perhaps
and Social Contexts
most important, the analyses of interview responses
were neither based on systematic coding schemes nor The ambiguities in the interpretations of gender differ-
analyzed statistically in any extensive ways (Colby & ences may very well stem from contextual variations in
Damon, 1983; Greeno & Maccoby, 1986; Luria, 1986). individuals judgments. Issues of context are considered
Furthermore, the propositions regarding the origins of in propositions regarding justice and care, but in a lim-
moral concepts in early relationships entailing inequali- ited way. In the first place, a broad contextual distinc-
ties, detachments, and attachments have not been sub- tion was drawn through the proposition that the life
jected to empirical study. circumstances of girls and women usually differ from
In subsequent research, a more circumscribed ap- those of boys and men. Especially for females, judg-
proach was taken, with a focus on defining the proposed ments in the context of a hypothetical situation may dif-
orientations of care and justice and on coding (Lyons, fer from judgments in the context of real-life situations
1983) the extent to which males and females use one or (Gilligan, 1982). The inclination to be distant from hy-
the other or combine the two. Studies assessing the dis- pothetical situations may be related to another proposed
tribution of care and justice orientations included male feature of the psychology of those with a care orienta-
and female adolescents and adults responding to ques- tionthat they are more attuned to contextual features
tions about moral conflicts in their lives (Gilligan & At- than those with a justice orientation. Those with a jus-
tanucci, 1988). Varying results were obtained. Lyons tice orientation are more likely to abstract from a situa-
(1983), for example, found that the majority of females tion (i.e., decontextualize it) in ways that generate
(75%) judged by a care orientation, whereas the major- judgments of likeness with other situations.
ity of males (79%) judged by a rights orientation. Other Because those formulations essentially propose
studies, with more refined analyses, indicated that only group differences in the ways people approach morality,
a minority of people exclusively use either care or jus- a more fundamental issue regarding social contexts is
tice orientation and that most use both in one fashion or unaddressed: People may apply their moral judgments in
another (Gilligan & Attanucci, 1988). Those studies also sufficiently flexible ways to take features of situations
suggested that the justice orientation was used more fre- into account in coming to decisions. In that case, moral
quently than care but with a tendency for females to use judgments would not be of one type for females or
care more than males and males to use justice more than males. Females and males may hold both concepts of
females. Research with preschoolers revealed no gender justice and concepts regarding the network of social re-
differences in care or justice orientations (Cassidy, Chu, lationships. Individuals may be oriented both to inde-
& Dahlsgaard, 1997). pendence and interdependence. How individuals apply
These types of studies (see Gilligan, Ward, Taylor, & these different judgments might depend on the situation.
Bardige, 1988, for reports of additional research) have Because of the different roles and status in social net-
provided some evidence that care and justice tend to be works and hierarchies of women and men, it may also be
associated with gender. However, the patterns are not that they would apply justice and care considerations
clear-cut because studies also show shifts by context differently. In some situations, men may even apply con-
(Johnston, 1988). Perhaps because of the combinations siderations regarding social networks and interdepend-
of care and justice found in the reasoning of males and ence more than women (e.g., situations in which men
Gender, Emotions, and Moral Judgments 809

wish to maintain the existing network of unequal rela- judgments. Other studies (Walker, 1991; Walker, de
tionships and role obligations), whereas in some situa- Vries, & Trevethan, 1987) have shown that only a mi-
tions women may apply justice considerations more than nority makes consistent judgments across the hypotheti-
men (e.g., situations in which women are more sensitive cal and real-life situations, and that about 50% of them
to the injustices of the existing inequalities, networks of showed consistency among the real-life situations.
role obligations, and interdependence; see Abu-Lughod, Whereas no sex differences were obtained in childrens
1993; Turiel & Wainryb, 1998; Wainryb & Turiel, or adolescents use of the care orientations on the real-
1994). Gilligan treats different moral and personal ori- life situations, adult women showed more use of care
entations as general characteristics of individuals. than men (60% versus 37%). In addition, the real-life
These multifaceted concerns are not unrelated to the conflicts were divided as to whether they involved a spe-
types of childhood experiences proposed to be sources cific person or group with whom the subject had or did
of the different orientations. It was proposed that attach- not have a relationship ( labeled personal and imper-
ments and detachments are the central social experi- sonal, respectively). Both female and male adults used
ences for girls, while inequalities and power are central the care orientation more on personal than impersonal
for boys (Gilligan & Wiggins, 1987). This is surely a conflicts. Whereas this shows that type of conflict can
one-sided characterization. In certain respects, issues of predict moral orientation better than gender (Walker,
equalities, inequalities, and power relationships are at 1991), overall the adult women showed more care re-
least as salient for girls as for boys. Perhaps starting in sponses than men. This means that women generated
the family (Okin, 1989), and then in school (Ornstein, more personal conflicts than men. Therefore, type of
1994) and the wider society, girls confront unequal orientation is related to the content of the situation (re-
treatment in more poignant ways than boys. Women, too, flecting contextual variations), but women are more
experience inequalities and unjust treatment in ways that likely than men to use the care orientation if they are
permeate their family and work experiences more likely to perceive moral conflicts as personal
(Hochschild, 1989; Nussbaum, 1999, 2000; Okin, 1989; rather than impersonal. These findings are generally
Turiel, 2002). Conversely, issues of attachment and de- consistent with findings from other studies (Jaffee &
tachment may be salient in the experiences of boys. The Hyde, 2000; Pratt, Diessner, Hunsberger, Pancer, &
prominence of groups, cliques, team sports, and gangs Savoy, 1991; Smetana, Killen, & Turiel, 1991).
are evidence of the pull for cooperation, attachments, The findings of these studies indicate that concerns
and solidarity pervasive in their experiences. This is not with fairness and with the maintenance of interpersonal
to say that researchers should simply reverse the ways relationships do not represent individual differences in
the proposed moral orientations have been linked to moral orientations. Furthermore, numerous studies on
gender, but that concerns with justice, fairness, individ- the development of moral judgments, considered in sub-
uation, care, solidarity, and interdependence are all im- sequent sections, have included females and males in the
portant coexisting aspects of childrens social initial investigations (theory building). Little in the way
experiences and developing judgments. of sex difference has been obtained in all that research.
A number of studies on how care and justice orienta- Some studies are worth noting at this juncture because
tions are used in different hypothetical and real-life sit- of their focus on judgments about positive actions to-
uations show that situational contexts affect whether a ward others. Using stories that pose conflicts between
justice or care orientation is used. A study by Rothbart, close friendship considerations, personal interests, and
Hanley, and Albert (1986) found that more reasoning the interests of nonclose friendships, Keller and Edel-
about rights was used in one of Kohlbergs hypothetical stein (1990, 1993) longitudinally studied the interper-
dilemmas (a husband is faced with deciding whether to sonal concepts of children and adolescents (7 to 15
steal a drug to save his dying wife) than in the real-life years). Along with understandings of friendship rela-
situations, but real-life situations produced more rea- tionships, there are age-related shifts, but no gender dif-
soning about rights than a Kohlberg hypothetical situa- ferences, in moral commitments based on obligations,
tion pertaining to physical intimacy. intimacy, and mutuality in relationships. Kahn (1992)
Therefore, the substance of the situations (e.g., phys- put forth a similar proposition on the basis of his find-
ical intimacy or saving a life), and not only whether they ings on childrens judgments about positive moral ac-
are hypothetical or real-life, has a bearing on peoples tions (i.e., whether to give money for food to hungry
810 The Development of Morality

persons). Females were no more likely than males to middle- and upper-class groups. The hierarchical rela-
judge that people should give to, or care for, hungry per- tionships between White middle-class women or men in
sons. Moreover, the reasons for these evaluations were relation to working-class or minority racial groups pose
mainly consideration of the welfare of others and issues interesting questions that have not been much investi-
of justice. Whereas younger children emphasized wel- gated. One such question bears on the racial and eco-
fare, older girls and boys embedded welfare into con- nomic injustices experienced by children in those
cepts of justice. groups, and its effects on their sensitivity to the issues.
This question was addressed in a study by Stack
Politics, Economics, Social Structure, and (1990) of the moral thinking of African American ado-
Womens Perspectives lescents and adults who were return migrants from the
north to rural, southern home places in the United
The proposition that care and justice tend to be organ- States. She interviewed the participants in the study
ized differently in males and females as a consequence about dilemmas relevant to their lives and found no dif-
of differences in childhood relationships carries a host ferences between responses of the adolescent girls and
of problems, including scientific verification, stereotyp- boys or between adult females and males, all of whom
ing of moral orientations, and the role of politics, eco- gave more justice than care responses. Stack proposed
nomics, and social structure in possible inequalities and that African American boys and girls are aware, from an
power relationships between men and women. Issues early age, of social and economic injustices. Men and
pertaining to scientific verification are raised by the as- women experience a good deal of injustice in the work-
sertion that male psychologists have imposed male-ori- place and other settings and are committed to combating
ented formulations of moral development that overlook it. Simultaneously, males and females are embedded in
a major strand of development associated with females. extended families, concerning themselves with their
This, however, is a criticism that turns on itself as a vi- own aspirations and the needs of their kin. Stacks find-
cious cycle. It could be said, for example, that Gilligans ings and theoretical analyses suggest that broader life
perspective is a consequence of various biases. It could experiences than identifications, attachments, and sepa-
be said that her ideas are colored by her status as a fe- rations are central to the development of moral concepts.
male of a rather advantaged position writing from the Another potential methodological problem in Gilli-
perspective of her memberships in a male-dominated gans (1982) research is that much of the data used to
field and in a highly elitist, well-endowed, and powerful formulate the levels of care reasoning were derived from
male-dominated educational institution. The myriad interviews about abortion, which is an issue with some
ways that such contexts can determine one point of view unique features that may not generalize to other moral
or another should be evident if too much credence is issues. Other research has shown people are divided as
given to the ways an individuals characteristics and to whether abortion should be classified as a moral
place color scientific or scholarly analyses. The alterna- issue, and those divisions are associated with assump-
tive is to evaluate the arguments and the evidence on tions people make about the fetus as a person and as
their own merits. constituting a life (Smetana, 1982; Turiel, Hildebrandt,
A related point made by Gilligan, closer to issues of & Wainryb, 1991). Whereas those who assume that the
evidence, is that some researchers (e.g., Kohlberg, 1969) fetus is a life with attributes of personhood judge abor-
used data from males only to build theory. An analogous tion as morally wrong, those who do not hold to that as-
criticism applies to the data used by Gilligan to build her sumption judge abortion as mainly a decision of
theory because her initial findings came from samples personal choice. Moreover, many individuals assump-
of largely White middle-class and upper-middle-class tions about the status of the fetus as a life include ambi-
women, most of whom were undergraduates at Harvard guities and uncertainties resulting in conflicting and
and Radcliffe (Pollitt, 1992; Stack, 1990). Gilligans contradictory judgments about abortion not evident in
(1977) focus has been on womens status in society, the same individuals judgments about welfare, harm,
which is that they are usually in subordinate and vulner- and life in other contexts (Turiel et al., 1991).
able positions relative to men (see also Nussbaum, 1999, Finally, in contrast with the way the issue has some-
2000; Okin, 1989, 1996; Turiel, 1996, 2002; Wainryb & times been couched, women have been involved in the
Turiel, 1994). However, working-class and racial minor- construction of theoretical approaches at variance with
ity groups are also in vulnerable positions relative to the proposition that there are sex differences in moral
Emphasizing Culture 811

orientations. Many women, including those writing from of influence and economic independence (at least mid-
a feminist perspective, have taken issue with the propo- dle-class men). Pollitts argument, it should be stressed,
sition that womens morality is mainly one of care and is not that caring and interdependence are negative and
interdependence (e.g., Abu-Lughod, 1991; Colby & independence is positive. Rather, it is that women, too,
Damon, 1983; Mednick, 1989; Nussbaum, 1999; Okin, can appropriately function independently, claiming
1989; Pollitt, 1992; Stack, 1990). These critiques have rights. In particular, the workplace in capitalist society
highlighted the stereotypical nature of gender-linked entails autonomy, concerns with personal advancement
distinctions, the significance of justice and fairness in and rights, along with caring and justice. Women appear
womens judgments and life circumstances, how mens less autonomous in the workplace as a consequence of
concepts of nurturance and interdependence serve to discrimination serving ends of men in positions of
maintain those circumstances, and how economics and power and influence.
social structural arrangements bear on the moral judg- The justice of distribution of resources, privileges,
ments of females and males. Writing from her perspec- and burdens in the family, especially as it affects
tive as a journalist and feminist, Pollitt (1992) has women, has been analyzed in depth by Okin (1989,
critiqued characterizations of women as nurturing, car- 1996). She argues that moral philosophers and social
ing individuals whose concerns are with relationships scientists have either ignored the justice of gender rela-
but not justice, rationality, or logic. Not that Pollitt tionships or accepted the legitimacy of unequal distribu-
would exclude nurturing and caring from the purview of tions and unjust treatment by relegating women to
women by any means. Rather, it is that women neither traditional roles. In that context, she also maintains that
have a monopoly on caring nor are they solely caring (a) justice and rights are spheres relevant to womens
nurturers of others. Women are caring, cooperative, thinking, ( b) there is no evidence that women are more
competitive, assertive of independence, and committed inclined to contextualism than universalism, and (c) the
to rights and justice. idea that women are oriented to care and not universally
The characterization of women as caring and nurtur- applicable concepts of rights and justice reinforces tra-
ing, according to Pollitt, stereotypes them in traditional ditional stereotypes. In Okins (1989) view, the distinc-
and restrictive ways. It is restrictive because it limits tion between care and justice has been overdrawn:
real concern with justice, rights, and independence
The best theorizing about justice, I argue, has integral to it
just as it is restrictive to attribute characteristics of
the notions of care and empathy, of thinking of the inter-
males solely to justice, rights, and autonomy. This
ests and well-being of others who may be very different
stereotyping serves several ends for females and males. from ourselves. It is, therefore, misleading to draw a di-
The positive end is that it provides women with an equal chotomy as though they were two contrasting ethics. The
moral status to men and challenges the division of men best theorizing about justice is not some abstract view
as rational and women as irrational. Women are said to from nowhere, but results from the carefully attentive
develop a type of rationality by which their morality is consideration of everyones point of view [emphasis in
different and equal to that of men. Despite the greater original]. (p. 15)
concern with equality in moral orientations, Pollitt ar-
gues that the formulation constitutes a stereotype serv- An implication of Okins contention is that justice needs
ing also to reinforce a status quo in which women retain to be inclusive. Those emphasizing emotions argue, as
positions subordinate to men. Men encourage the idea noted, that an inclusive or universal conception of
that women are concerned with care because men are, in morality is a Western one, largely promulgated by intel-
addition to children, the main beneficiaries of womens lectuals. In other cultures, and perhaps for ordinary
nurturance. people in Western cultures, morality is applied in a local
Pollitt also argues that propositions regarding the and parochial fashion. A similar position has been taken
sources of womens judgments in early identifications by those who propose that integrated cultural patterns
(Chodorow, 1978) overlook the important contributions are central in the development of morality.
of their roles in the economic and social structure.
Along with its positive aspects, caring is a consequence EMPHASIZING CULTURE
of economic dependence and subordination in the fam-
ily. The role of caretaker and nurturer is, in part, im- The idea of cultures forming integrated cohesive pat-
posed by a power structure in which men are in positions terns diverging from each other goes back at least to the
812 The Development of Morality

formulations of cultural anthropologists of the early social practices, thus, were used to argue for the incom-
part of the twentieth century. One of the most influen- parability of the moralities of different cultures, and in
tial proponents of the idea that cultures form integrated that sense empirical observations were used for proposi-
patterns was Ruth Benedict (1934), who proclaimed that tions about the nature of morality (i.e., to define it as
the diversity of cultures can be endlessly documented local and entailing an acquisition of the standards of the
(p. 45). Cultural anthropologists of the time also wrote culture). The core of these propositions is that varia-
about morality, often taking positions of cultural rela- tions in social practices stem from differences in the
tivism, in reaction to predominant late-nineteenth-cen- ways cultures are integrated (Benedict, 1934, p. 46): A
tury anthropological assumptions that cultures could be culture, like an individual, is a more or less consistent
classified in a hierarchy of lower to higher. Usually, pattern of thought and action.
Western cultures were placed at the apex of the hierar- In contemporary views of human development, the
chy. Cultural anthropologists argued that the classifica- role of culture has once again been emphasized (Bruner,
tions of cultures in a hierarchy of progress or 1990; Shweder, 1990a; Shweder & Sullivan, 1993) and
development were due to bias in favor of Western cul- has become increasingly part of research on moral de-
tural values and to intolerance and lack of respect for velopment. As already seen, those emphasizing emo-
the equally valid values of other cultures. Along with tions include cultural influences as a part of moral
relativism, therefore, it was asserted that cultures acquisition, along with the idea that morality is highly
should be treated as different and equal, and each ac- influenced by biologically based propensities. Others
cepted as functioning on its own moral standards with assert that culture must be given center stage (Markus
moral ends endemic to its system. Some critics of cul- & Kitayama, 1991; Miller & Bersoff, 1995; Shweder,
tural relativism (e.g., Hatch, 1983) have pointed out that 1990a; Shweder, Mahapatra, & Miller, 1987; Shweder,
the position actually includes nonrelativistic moral pre- Much, Mahapatra, & Park, 1997; Triandis, 1990). In
scriptions. In particular, relativists espouse the values giving culture center stage, sharp distinctions are drawn
of tolerance (that the validity of other cultures values between Western and non-Western cultures in morality
and perspectives should be accepted), freedom (that a and concepts of self. Westerners are said to place an em-
culture should not be obstructed from following its phasis on abstractions, justice, and the autonomy of in-
moral standards), and equality (that a cultures moral dividuals, whereas non-Westerners are said to place
standards should be regarded as of equal validity as emphasis on concrete contexts, duties, and interdepend-
those of any other). ence. Yet, when discussing Western cultures in the con-
Benedict (1934) sharply characterized the proposed text of critiques of some explanations of moral
variations among cultures through an example that development, such as Kohlbergs, culturalists end up
many would consider to epitomize moral concerns, tran- portraying people in Western cultures as holding both
scending time and place, and pertaining to justice, types of orientations (Shweder, 1982; Simpson, 1973).
rights, empathy, sympathy, and care: Shweder (1982), for instance, asserted that
Kohlbergs highest stages (Stages 5 and 6), which in-
We might suppose that in the matter of taking life all peo- clude the ideas of society as a social contract and the
ples would agree in condemnation. On the contrary, in a individual as possessing natural and inalienable rights
matter of homicide, it may be held that one is blameless if
prior to or outside society (p. 424), are culture spe-
diplomatic relations have been severed between neighbor-
cific. However, he also stated that these ideas are the
ing countries, or that one kills by custom his first two chil-
dren, or that a husband has right of life and death over his
domain of a small segment of Western culture: If they
wife, or that it is the duty of the child to kill his parents are advocated at all, and they rarely are, it is among
before they are old. It may be that those are killed who Western educated middle-class adults (p. 425). The
steal a fowl, or who cut their upper teeth first, or who are majority of people (nonintellectuals in Western culture
born on a Wednesday. (p. 46) and those in non-Western cultures), therefore, hold
views that do not revolve around individual autonomy
In this way, Benedict encompassed several cultural and separateness from society: Moral exegesis seems
practices commonly used to illustrate variations in to stabilize around the not unreasonable ideas that social
moral codes: parricide, infanticide, and family relation- roles carry with them an obligation to behave in a cer-
ships of deep inequalities. Observations of variations in tain way, that society is not of our own making, and that
Emphasizing Culture 813

self and society are somehow intimately linked (Stages love, marriage, sleeping patterns, and work are all in-
3 and 4) (p. 425). cluded. Despite appearances, it would not be correct to
Most often, Shweders (1986) position is that a vari- say that these researchers endorse moral relativism (al-
ety of systems of rationality exist that are framed by though questions can be raised about this) nor that they
culture. Western cultures have an individualistic orien- regard the reactions to social practices (e.g., women
tation (in contrast with the collectivistic orientations of being allowed to work or women not being allowed to
non-Western cultures) focusing on rights and autonomy. work) as arbitrary or fortuitous. This is because particu-
Therefore, while asserting that individualism is the cen- lar social practices are proposed to be part of sets of
tral ethos of Western cultures, it is argued that concepts moral qualities entailing rights, autonomy, duty, inter-
of freedom, contract, and rights are ways of thinking es- dependence, and sanctity. In turn, moral qualities are
poused mainly by intellectual elites and not others in the connected to more general patterns that make up cul-
West. Furthermore, whereas these writers often empha- tural communities. Cultures do not simply provide a se-
size the role of the elites in Western culture and draw ries of isolated standards, values or codes. Some worlds
differences in the thinking of elites and laypersons, they of moral meaning emphasize rights and justice, others
seldom do so with non-Western cultures. Because there emphasize duties and obligations, each part of general
is mention of elites or leaders in non-Western cultures, it orientations to individualistic (read Western cultures)
is on the premise that there is consistency in their think- and collectivistic (read non-Western cultures) concep-
ing with that of ordinary people (Shweder, 1986). tions of self, others, and society.
The proposed contrast between individualistic and
collectivistic cultural orientations is related to moral
Social Communication and Cultural Coherence conceptions, practices, and appraisals. However, these
orientations encompass much more; they are the bases
Like Benedict, contemporary researchers point to many for cultural constructions of how persons are defined,
areas of moral diversity that are said to be well docu- how they interact with each other, how society is de-
mented by anthropologists and historians: fined, and how the goals of persons and the group are
established and met (e.g., Geertz, 1984; Markus & Ki-
On the basis of the historical and ethnographic record we tayama, 1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1982; Triandis,
know that different people in different times and places 1990). As put by Markus and Kitayama (1991): In
have found it quite natural to be spontaneously appalled, many Western cultures there is a faith in the coherent
outraged, indignant, proud, disgusted, guilty and ashamed separateness of distinct persons. . . . Achieving the cul-
by all sorts of things: masturbation, homosexuality, sexual tural goal of independence requires construing oneself
abstinence, polygamy, abortion, circumcision, corporal as an individual whose behavior is organized and made
punishment, capital punishment, Islam, Christianity, Ju-
meaningful primarily by reference to ones own internal
daism, capitalism, democracy, flag burning, miniskirts,
repertoire of thought, feelings, and action (p. 225).
long hair, no hair, alcohol consumption, meat eating, med-
ical inoculations, atheism, idol worship, divorce, widow
And in the contrasting construal of interdependence
remarriage, arranged marriage, romantic love marriage, many non-Western cultures insist . . . on the funda-
parents and children sleeping in the same bed, parents and mental connectedness of human beings to each other. A
children not sleeping in the same bed, women being al- normative imperative of these cultures is to maintain
lowed to work, women not being allowed to work. this interdependence among individuals (p. 227).
(Shweder, 1994, p. 26) In these formulations, the United States is often iden-
tified as the quintessential individualistic society (also
The sweep of this statement is breathtaking. Being by Bellah et al., 1985), but individualism is also preva-
appalled, outraged, indignant, proud, disgusted, guilty lent in other countries such as Australia, Canada, En-
and ashamed are all seen as moral reactions. Most posi- gland, and New Zealand (Triandis, 1990). Prototypical
tive and negative reactions are regarded to have a moral collectivistic cultures are found in Japan, India, China,
component. Moreover, as evident in the long list given of and the Middle East, as well as in Africa, Latin Amer-
all sorts of things, many different behaviors can be ica, and southern Europe (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
and have been part of the moral domain. Little is ex- The person conceived as an autonomous agent, with per-
empt, given that sexuality, hairstyle, clothing style, sonal goals, is central in the individualistic frame,
814 The Development of Morality

whereas the group as an interconnected and interde- 1993; and in Israel, see Nissan, 1987). In large measure,
pendent network of relationships is central in the collec- the research aimed at ascertaining whether a distinction
tivistic frame. A core feature of individualistic cultures could be drawn across the two cultures between moral-
is that the highest value is accorded to the person as de- ity, as based on concepts of justice, rights, and welfare,
tached from others and as independent of the social and conventionality, as based on context-specific unifor-
order. People are, therefore, oriented to self-sufficiency, mities serving goals of social coordinationa distinc-
self-reliance, independence, and resistance to social tion that had been addressed by others and is considered
pressure for conformity or obedience to authority. Col- further in subsequent sections of this chapter (e.g.,
lectivistic cultures, by contrast, are oriented to tradi- Nucci, 1981; Smetana, 1981, 1984; Tisak, 1986; Turiel,
tion, duty, obedience to authority, interdependence, and 1979, 1983a).
social harmony (for a general review of evidence, see Shweder et al. (1987) hypothesized that a distinction
Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002; for conceptual between morality and convention is particular to cul-
analyses, see Mascolo & Li, 2004). tures which structure social relationships through the
A significant component of cultural meanings is the concept of autonomous individuals free to choose by
kind of moral orientation communicated to children and consensus. Accordingly, they included topics of consen-
reproduced by them as they grow into adulthood. sual choice in Western cultures such as issues about
Shweder et al. (1987) proposed a distinction between food, dress, and terms of address. Whereas some items
rights-based and duty-based moralities in their were straightforward (e.g., a son addressing his father
comparisons between the United States and India. In by his first name) others included religious and meta-
Western cultures, moral authority resides in individuals physical considerations for Indians because of their con-
who voluntarily enter into contracts and promises, with nections to ideas about an afterlife (e.g., a widow
the idea of rights as fundamental ( hence a rights- wearing jewelry and bright-colored clothing 6 months
based morality). In a contrasting duty-based morality, after the death of her husband, a widow eating fish 2 or
the social order is the organizing feature of moral ra- 3 times a week). Also many of the items entailed acts on
tionality. Customary social practices are viewed as part the part of women that might contradict the power and
of the natural moral order, so that social practices are desires of men (e.g., a woman wanting to eat with her
seen neither as within individual discretion nor as a husband and elder brother, a son claiming an inheritance
function of social consensus (thus the concept of con- over his sister). Shweder et al. (1987) included items re-
ventionality as agreement in a group is largely absent). flecting concepts they consider candidates for moral
The social order dictates specified duties based on roles universals (e.g., a father breaking a promise to his son,
and status in the social structure, while the individual cutting in line, refusing to treat an injured person) that
per se and his various interior states, preferences, ap- dealt with justice, harm, reciprocity, theft, arbitrary as-
petites, intentions, or motives are of little interest or sault, and discrimination. Still other issues dealt with
concern (Shweder et al., 1987, pp. 2021). Moreover, family practices that might vary by culture, including
Shweder et al. asserted that in a duty-based culture, in- those bearing on personal liberty, privacy, and equality
dividuals are not free to deviate from rules and that (considered central themes for Americans), and sanc-
there is little conception of a natural right (such as free tity, chastity, and respect for status (considered central
speech) that might lead to advocating deviation from the themes for Indians).
socially defined good. The assessments were adapted, in modified form,
from previous research on morality and convention
Social Practices and Cultural Coherence (Turiel, 1983a). Shweder et al. (1987) found that Amer-
icans and Indians rank the seriousness of transgressions
Propositions regarding cultural divergence in moral ra- in very different ways, such that there are high correla-
tionality were examined in a study conducted with tions among Americans and among Indians but little
samples of secular middle- and upper-middle-class chil- correlation between Americans and Indians. There was
dren and adults from the United States (Hyde Park in agreement in judgments about some moral issues be-
Chicago), and samples of untouchables and Brahmans tween Indians and Americans and a good deal of dis-
living in the old temple town of Bhubaneswar, Orissa, in agreement on issues pertaining to conventions, liberty,
India (for research in Brazil, see Haidt, Koller, & Dias, equality, sanctity, chastity, and status. The findings of
Emphasizing Culture 815

variations in judgments, aside from the few issues deal- after she disobeys him by going to a movie alone without
ing with harm, promises, assault, and so on, led Shweder his permission, and a son who claims most of his de-
et al. (1987) to conclude that many things viewed as ceased fathers property, not allowing his sister to ob-
wrong on one side of the Atlantic are not viewed as tain much inheritance. As put by Shweder et al. (1987):
wrong on the other side (p. 51).
On the side of India, according to the findings, more Oriya Brahmans do not view beating an errant wife as an
things are regarded as wrong than on the side of the instance of arbitrary assault, and they do not believe it is
United States. In particular, Indians regarded many unfair to choose the son over the daughter in matters of
breaches pertaining to food, dress, terms of address, and life and inheritance. . . . [T]hey] believe, that beating a
sex roles as wrong, as unalterable, and in some cases as wife who goes to the movies without permission is roughly
equivalent to corporal punishment for a private in the
universal. Shweder et al. (1987) maintained that conven-
army who leaves the military base without permission.
tional thinking is almost a nonexistent form of thought
For Oriyas there are rationally appealing analogical map-
in our Indian data (p. 52). Although convention was ex- pings between the family unit and military units (differen-
istent in the American data, it was much less prevalent tiated roles and status obligations in the service of the
than found in many other studies conducted in the whole, hierarchical control, drafting and induction, etc.).
United States. It was also found that with increasing age One thing the family is not, for Oriyas, is a voluntary asso-
Americans judged the issues in more relativistic ways ciation among equal individuals. (p. 71)
(i.e., judging that the practices are acceptable for other
people or in other countries) and were more likely to The overarching principle applied in the analyses of
take situational features into account. By contrast, with responses to these items is cultural meaning in a moral
increasing age, Indians judged the prohibitions as appli- system. Not considered is that different and varying
cable universally and across varying contexts. On the agendas may be at work in addition to moral duties.
basis of these findings, Damon (1988) has suggested For example, Indians may judge caning a son as right be-
that moral maturity in some parts of the world implies cause of their psychological assumptions regarding the
an ever-expanding tendency to universalize ones moral effectiveness of physical punishment on learning (see
beliefs, whereas in other parts of the world moral matu- Wainryb, 1991). Also, exerting power and asserting per-
rity means applying ones beliefs flexibly to an array of sonal entitlements may account for the acceptability,
changing situations (p. 109). Because it is Indians, in among Indians, of husbands beating their wives and sons
contrast to Americans, who universalize moral judg- claiming an inheritance over their sisters. The analogy
ments, Damons suggestion is in direct opposition to between the family and military units ignores some pos-
presumptions that moral universality is a post-Enlight- sibly important differences. Is it permissible for a pri-
enment Western idea. vate in the army to be beaten black and blue? What
In addition to differences in judgments between the about an officer who leaves the base without permis-
two cultural groups, on issues related to food, dress, sion? Is he not accountable for his actions, as opposed to
terms of address, and sex roles, Shweder et al. (1987) a husband in the family situation? In that sense, there
found that a number of issues were judged as wrong by may be more accountability and reciprocity between
both Indians and Americans (these are the candidates people in different ranks in the military than husband
for moral universals). Agreement occurred on issues and wife in the family. When a husband beats his wife,
pertaining to harm (e.g., hospital workers ignoring an is it in the service of the whole or in the service of the
accident victim, destroying another childs picture, husbands personal interests?
kicking a harmless animal), injustice (e.g., cutting in Nevertheless, the examples and analogy point to some
line, discriminating against invalids), breaking prom- hierarchical social relationships, entailing dominance
ises, and incest. However, not all issues bearing on dis- and subordination. Additional items used by Shweder
crimination or harm were judged as wrong by Indians et al. (1987) illustrate hierarchy in the family. Indians
and Americans. Three issues, in particular, were judged judged that it is wrong for a woman to eat with her hus-
as right by Indians and wrong by Americans. One of bands elder brother, that it is wrong for a husband to
these depicted a father who canes his son for a misdeed. massage the legs of his wife, and that it is wrong for a
Two others pertained to gender relationships. One de- husband to cook dinner for his wife. Intimacy should not
picted a husband who beats his wife black and blue exist among certain family members, such as between a
816 The Development of Morality

woman and her husbands elder brother. A husband must of community, which refers to status, hierarchy, and so-
not give his wife a massage or cook for her because The cial order, and the ethics of divinity based on concepts of
wife is the servant of the husband. The servant should sin, sanctity, duty, and natural order. In the United
do her work (Shweder et al., 1997, p. 137). States, by contrast, there is a specialization in the
Another area where hierarchical relationships exist in ethics of autonomy, with community, and divinity in even
a traditional culture like India is among people of differ- smaller part, providing a background. Reminiscent of Et-
ent castes. Shweder et al. (1987) also propose that the zionis (1993) position, Shweder et al. (1997) are of the
morality of Indians includes the idea of purity, com- opinion that the expertise in the ethics of autonomy in
municating to children that they should avoid sources of the United States has led to a wide extension of the con-
impurity and uncleanliness, and one such source of pol- cept of rights (e.g., to children or animals), to the desire
lution is contact with people of a lower caste: Just as the to be protected from every imaginable harm (e.g., from
pure must be protected from the impure, the higher sta- secondary cigarette smoke or psychologically offensive
tus and the lower status must be kept at a work environments), and to an enlargement of the idea of
distance . . . the culture is providing the child with a harm (to include all-embracing notions as harassment,
practical moral commentary in which one of the many abuse, exploitation, p. 142). Shweder et al. (1997)
messages is ultimately that menstrual blood, feces, and view these extensions as distortions, just as other distor-
lower status go together (pp. 7475). Again, these prac- tions may occur through the extensions of concepts of
tices are attributed to cultural meanings around duties, community and divinity in Indian culture.
without consideration of the possibility that they reflect In India, the ethics of autonomy is linked to the idea
the creation of distance in social relationships that bene- of a soul, which obligates respect (souls include human
fit those in positions of power (in a culture that is sup- and nonhuman animals). More dominant, however, is the
posed to be collectivistic and to stress interdependence). ethic of community, in which a persons identity is asso-
ciated with status and relationships to others to a much
greater extent than individuality. Relationships are part
Emotional Forms, Intuitions,
of hierarchical orderings in which people in subordinate
and Rapid Processing
and dominant positions are obligated to protect and look
The emphasis on the dictates of roles, status, and hierar- after each others interests (e.g., wives should be obedi-
chy appears to leave little room for the types of moral ent to husbands and husbands should be responsive to
concerns with justice, harm, and even rights (e.g., that it the needs and desires of wives). Shweder et al. (1997)
is wrong to discriminate against invalids) apparent in regard this as analogous to feudal ethics, where the feu-
some of the findings of the Shweder et al. (1987) re- dal lord does for others as much as they do for him (an
search. Recognizing that such judgments are made in asymmetrical reciprocity because one person is in a po-
that non-Western, sociocentric culture (as in their own sition of dominance and control).
findings and as in interpretations by Turiel, Killen, & Along with the three types of morality, another set of
Helwig, 1987), Shweder and his colleagues (1990b; modifications and extensions of the theory is that cul-
Shweder et al., 1997) attenuated somewhat the proposi- tural content is communicated to individuals who are
tion regarding the separation of a rights-based morality prepared by evolution with deep emotions to receive and
and a duty-based morality and elaborated on it. One rapidly process the content, making decisions intuitively
elaboration is the proposition that three ethics are (Shweder, 1994; Shweder & Haidt, 1993). Moreover,
found the world over: the ethics of autonomy, commu- emotions are regarded as the gatekeeper of the moral
nity, and divinity. Although the inclusion of three ethics world, revealing features of social reality.
broadens the scope of the analyses beyond the dichotomy The proposition that emotions are linked to intuitions
of rights and duties, it is still presumed that the social has been extended by Haidt (2001) in ways that render
order determines the interplay of different types of rationality and reasoning largely irrelevant in moral
goods in a worldview. Thus, in India, community and evaluations and decisions (which contrasts with
divinity are dominant, whereas in the United States au- Shweders, 1986, proposition that people in different
tonomy prevails (Shweder et al., 1997). In Indian society, cultures maintain different types of rationality). In
the ethics of autonomy, based on justice, harm, and Haidts view, immediate, reflexive reactions, such as re-
rights, is subordinated to and in the service of the ethics vulsion, disgust, and sympathy, trigger moral reactions.
Emphasizing Culture 817

Judging acts as wrong involves immediate gut reac- ample provided is of a brother and sister who go on va-
tions of intuitive kinds that do not involve reasoning. For cation and, with all precautions, decide to make love.
Haidt, the defining feature of intuitions is quantita- The act is intuitively grasped as wrong because most
tive: They occur rapidly, without effort, automatically, people say something like I dont know, I cant explain
and without intentionality. Haidt proposes that intu- it. I just know its wrong.
itions are due to evolutionary adaptations shaped by cul- A key question is the generality of this type of exam-
ture. Culture provides a context for the expression ple (or examples like people judging it wrong to eat
(referred to as externalization) of built-in moral intu- dogs, etc.) and whether it applies to peoples moral lives
itions. The outcome in children and among adults is a more generally and meaningfully. The research dis-
morality that is unique to their culture or group and cussed in subsequent sections provides a good deal of
often includes asymmetrical reciprocity with accep- evidence that children, adolescents, and adults explain
tance of dominance and subordination. many of their moral evaluations in ways that are very
In Haidts view, humans are reasoning beings only in different from the way they approach an issue like in-
secondary ways. Reasoning contrasts with intuitions in cest. A number of features in social situations are taken
that it is slow, requires effort, and makes use of evi- into account, including what has been referred to as in-
dence. Moral reasoning is used mainly after the fact to formational assumptions or assumptions about reality
justify to self and others why an act is intuitively (see the discussion that follows; for a critique of the
grasped as wrong when faced with a social demand for emotivist-intuitionist position, see Turiel, 2006).
a verbal justification one becomes a lawyer building a
case rather than a judge searching for the truth (Haidt,
Justice and Interpersonal Responsibilities
2001, p. 814). Moral reasoning is also used to persuade
and to rationalize but does not involve choices, rational- The position taken by Shweder and his colleagues
ity, deliberation, or use of evidence. According to (Shweder, 1986, 2002; Shweder et al., 1987; Shweder
Haidt, most people are not concerned with reflection on et al., 1997) gives more emphasis to systems of rational-
moral matters. It is philosophers and those with a high ity in moral judgments than is evident in the proposition
need for cognition who engage in private or personal that morality is based on intuitions given expression by
reflection. cultural practices. Moral systems of rationality, as al-
To the extent that evidence is provided for the propo- ready noted, vary by cultural orientations to self, other,
sition that moral evaluations are intuitive, it is from re- roles, and duties. Social hierarchy is seen to be central
search in nonmoral realms. Haidt cites a number of in the morality of Hindu Indians, as exemplified by
studies from social psychology that appear to support Shweders view that their morality includes treating the
the idea that people are biased, emotive, intuitive, and family unit like a military unit and acceptance of the le-
unconcerned with evidence. Moreover, Haidt proclaims gitimacy of a husband beating his wife black and blue
that research on moral reasoning only reveals what peo- when she is disobedient. Another aspect of hierarchy, in
ple do in the way of justification to convince others or to this moral orientation, is the necessity of members of
rationalize, in a post hoc way, positions they hold for the higher caste to avoid contact with members of lower
other reasons. However, he does not provide evidence as castes (to avoid pollution). Although social hierarchy is
to how the moral reasoning investigated in so many stud- portrayed as entailing asymmetrical reciprocity, it also
ies fails to account for moral evaluations or how it is that makes for a good deal of distance and separation be-
such reasoning is mainly used for purposes of persua- tween males and females and among social classes.
sion and rationalization. A good part of Haidts argu- Shweder et al.s (1987) depiction of the family unit as
ment is based on a few examples. One that he seems to akin to a military unit implies formality and depend-
regard as prototypical is that of incestan example that ence of women ( like privates) on men ( like commis-
could be viewed as shared within cultures, yet applicable sioned officers). The distance between members of
across cultures, and an evolutionary adaptation. Incest different castes due to the need to avoid pollution makes
is an act, even when it is specified that it is consensual for very little interdependence or care and concern for
and there is no risk of pregnancy occurring, to which the welfare of those of lower castes. These conclusions
people react immediately with a gut reaction that it is are at odds with the portrayal of non-Western cultures
wrong and are unable to explain why. The specific ex- as oriented to interdependence and social harmony.
818 The Development of Morality

Another series of studies comparing Americans and ans to classify the interpersonal transgressions as moral
Indians on their judgments about helping others, inter- rather than as matters of personal choice and the reverse
personal obligations, and justice was based on the pro- for Americans.
posed cultural distinction between independence and In addition, a study by Miller et al. (1990) showed
interdependence (Bersoff & Miller, 1993; Miller & that a large majority of Indians judged as wrong actors
Bersoff, 1992, 1995; Miller, Bersoff, & Hardwood, who, for selfish reasons, failed to help persons in ex-
1990; Miller & Luthar, 1989). In contrast to Gilligan treme, moderate, or minor need. This was true for rela-
(1982), Miller and her colleagues proposed that varia- tionships between parent and child, best friends, and
tions in judgments about interpersonal obligations and strangers. The same judgment was made by the large
justice reflect cultural, and not gender, differences. majority of Americans regarding situations of extreme
Miller and Bersoff (1995) maintained that Gilligan need and situations of moderate need involving parents
takes a narrow approach to culture by failing to consider and children. With regard to the situations of minor
differences in cultural meanings that affect individuals need with parents, and moderate need in relationships
concepts of self and morality. They believe Gilligans of friends and strangers, Americans (especially among
ideas lead to implausible predictionsthat concepts of the oldest groups) were less likely to see helping as an
self and morality would be more similar among individ- obligation.
uals of the same gender from different cultures (e.g., a Miller and Bersoff (1995) also proposed that selfless-
secular American woman versus a traditional Hindu In- ness is consistent with a duty-oriented culture and that
dian woman) than individuals of different genders from providing psychological support is consistent with a cul-
similar cultures (e.g., a traditional Hindu Indian man tural orientation to voluntary, personal decisions in in-
versus a traditional Hindu Indian woman). Miller and terpersonal relationships. More generally, for
Bersoff (1995) further argue that in Gilligans proposi- Americans, because of their orientation to individual-
tions regarding the influences of early childhood experi- ism, interpersonal relationships are not strictly moral
ence there is a failure to consider how they are related to obligations but are seen as either matters of personal
cultural meanings. choice or as involving a combination of the moral and
Miller and Bersoff (1995) proposed that American personal. For Indians, interpersonal relationships are
women, too, are influenced by the individualistic views seen as moral obligations that can be given priority over
of self in their culture and that Indian men are influ- matters of justice or rights.
enced by the relational or interdependent views of the However, the proposed commitment of Indians to in-
self in their culture. As a consequence, Americans have terpersonal obligations is discrepant with the findings of
a minimalist view of interpersonal moral obligations Shweder et al. (1987) showing detachment between
that contrasts with the maximalist views of Indians. The castes and among family members. Other aspects of the
thinking of Indians is contextual because self is concep- research conducted by Miller and her colleagues are dis-
tualized as part of the social order so that duty is not in crepant with findings obtained by Shweder et al. (1987).
contradiction with individual desires. In the first place, they (Bersoff & Miller, 1993; Miller
In a study (Miller & Luthar, 1989) comparing Indi- & Bersoff, 1992) found that Indians do think in terms of
ans and Americans, adults were presented with a set of social conventions. Both Indians and Americans judged
scenarios depicting transgressions of role-related inter- a violation of a dress code (not related to religious obli-
personal obligations (e.g., a son refusing to care for his gations) in social conventional and not moral terms, and
elderly parents, a man leaving his wife and children for these judgments differed from judgments about theft
another woman), and justice (e.g., a college student (which was judged in moral terms). Other research
cheating on a final exam because family responsibilities (Madden, 1992) conducted in the temple town of
do not allow time for study, a man leaving the city with- Bhubaneswar also showed that there was conventional
out paying back a personal loan). It was found that both thinking about nonreligious issues, which was distinct
groups evaluated the justice issues as wrong, but Indians from moral thinking.
were more likely than Americans to evaluate the inter- Additionally, Miller contends that Indians take con-
personal transgressions as wrong. Correspondingly, textual features into account to a greater extent than
each group classified the justice transgressions mainly Americans. Evidence for this proposition comes from
in moral terms. There was a greater tendency for Indi- studies of person descriptions and social explanations
Emphasizing Judgment and Reciprocal Social Interactions 819

indicating that Americans tend to explain behaviors with hood does not necessarily establish that significant as-
trait attributions, whereas Indians tend to do so with pects of development do not occur beyond early child-
references to context (Miller, 1984, 1986). Evidence hood; that judgments, deliberations, and reflections are
also comes from studies indicating that Indians are less unimportant; or that many experiences, in addition to
likely than Americans to hold individuals accountable parental practices, do not contribute. As noted earlier,
for violations of moral codes, attributing the causes of the theories and research of Piaget and Kohlberg have
behavior to contextual features (Bersoff & Miller, 1993; had much to do with the shift away from conceptualizing
Miller & Luthar, 1989). These propositions about the morality as entailing self-control over impulses through
contextual dependence of duty appear to be in opposi- their demonstrations that children think about the social
tion to the Shweder et al. (1987) findings that with age world, attempt to understand social relationships, form
Americans become more relativistic and flexible in their judgments of right and wrong, and thereby engage in re-
moral judgments and that Indians become more univer- ciprocal interactions with others. However, Piaget and
salistic, applying moral injunctions across contexts. Kohlberg thought that extrinsic features, such as basing
Millers propositions regarding moral accountability right and wrong on obedience and sanctions, structure
also appear to be different from the view of Shweder young childrens moral judgments. As is discussed, it
et al. (1997) that karma institutionalizes human tenden- appears that Piaget and Kohlberg failed to uncover not
cies to attribute consequences to personal responsibil- only the positive nature of young childrens moral feel-
ity. It is a great irony of Western understanding that ings but also that young children form relatively complex
karma is often misinterpreted as a description of how judgments that are not based on extrinsic features.
Indians excuse themselves from responsibility by de- Moral reasoning is multifaceted and can entail ambi-
scribing themselves as passive objects of the force of guities and uncertainties, certainties and unreflective
their past actions (p. 152). According to Shweder et al., apprehension, as well as deliberation and reflection.
some Indians do use an interpretation of karma to ac- Whether moral evaluations and judgments are
count for failures of responsibility, but other Indians are processed very quickly or slowly, with certainty or
critical of such thinking. given pause, with an apparent lack of self-awareness or
with reflection and deliberation, depends on the indi-
EMPHASIZING JUDGMENT AND viduals development, the situation or problem con-
RECIPROCAL SOCIAL INTERACTIONS fronted, and the points of view of other people. First,
how well a concept is understood has a bearing on the
In several approaches considered thus far, it is, for the rapidity of a moral evaluation. A well-understood con-
most part, proposed that children acquire morality from cept that is perceived as readily applicable to a particu-
the family and/or the culture, and that this occurs very lar situation may well be used in rapid fashion and give
early in life. It is presumed that the necessary compo- a false appearance to the outside observer that it is in-
nents of morality emerge very early in lifeinfants and tuitive or a habitual practice. The same concept for
very young children show positive social behaviors, that individual at an earlier time may have been applied
react with positive emotions to others, and form attach- with more uncertainty and less of a sense of being evi-
ments with them. This presumption, in turn, is linked to dently true. That does not mean that a concept, once
the propositions that much of it is naturally derived formed and accepted, will be produced rapidly and
(through evolution), that much of it is acquired from without self-awareness in all situations. Ambiguities in
parents because most of the childs early social experi- a situation, as well as awareness that others take a dif-
ences are in the family, and that much of it is reflexive ferent point of view, can produce deliberation, aware-
and habitual. ness of ambiguities, and argumentation.
The findings that young children show positive moral It will be recalled that Haidt (2001) used the quanti-
emotions and actions toward others indicate that the tative dimension of rapidity and lack of effort as criteria
foundations of morality are established in early child- for designating responses as intuitive and habitual. He
hood and do not solely entail the control and inhibition referred to social psychological research in nonmoral
of childrens tendencies toward gratifying needs or realms to support the propositions that peoples think-
drives or acting on impulses. However, that the founda- ing is intuitive, unreflective, and that they do not con-
tions of positive morality are established in early child- cern themselves with evidence. However, there is a large
820 The Development of Morality

body of evidence from developmental and cognitive psy- be said that the moral exemplars studied by Colby and
chology in realms like number, mathematical reasoning, Damon are among those with more philosophical orien-
classification, understandings of space and physics more tations or a high need for cognition. In a later section,
generally, causality, intentionality, and theories of mind however, research is discussed on how reflection and so-
showing that people make judgments that are not neces- cial critique are part of most peoples social lives
sarily immediate, rapid, and categorical and that can be (Turiel, 2003b; Turiel & Perkins, 2004).
intentional, deliberative, and reflective. People do rea- Studies of moral development, including several of
son and are not intuitive in many realms of knowledge. the ones already considered, suggest alternatives to the
The research also shows that such reasoning can become propositions that emotions are primary in morality, that
immediate and rapid. Conceptualizations of, for exam- moral acquisition is mainly due to effects of parental
ple, number and arithmetic may be acquired laboriously practices on children, or that morality largely reflects
over time but, once acquired, are applied in rapid fash- the acquisition of societal standards. Dunn et al. (1995)
ion (for further discussion, see Turiel, 2006). found differences in the two types of situations they as-
Research on young childrens psychological under- sessed (physical harm and cheating) and documented
standings also demonstrates that the quantitative dimen- that relationships with siblings influence development.
sion of rapidity of response is inadequate as a means for By 2 or 3 years of age, children display a fair amount of
ascertaining whether reasoning is at work. Many studies teasing of mothers, physical aggression, destruction of
of childrens understandings of others minds (see objects, and an increasing ability to engage in arguments
Flavell & Miller, 1998) show that cognitive processes of and disputes with mothers (Dunn, 1987; Dunn & Munn,
a slow and rapid nature are at work between 3 and 5 1987). This increasing variety in young childrens social
years of age. As the research clearly shows, 5-year-old relationships is consistent with the findings reviewed by
children have an understanding of others mental states, Grusec and Goodnow (1994) showing that parental prac-
including beliefs, desires, and intentions. For instance, tices are related to type of misdeed (e.g., moral or con-
5-year-olds readily understand that another person may ventional), children judge the appropriateness of reasons
hold a false belief about, for example, the contents of given by parents when communicating with them, and
a crayon box that actually contains candy. For adults, parents may encourage ways of behaving that differ
this is rapidly understood and readily appliedand it from those they engage in themselves.
appears to be so also for 5-year-olds. Yet, processes of An interactional perspective on parent-child relation-
development and thought are involved in these under- ships casts a different light on the types of child-rearing
standings given that 3-year-olds generally do not answer practices studied in research on moral development
correctly on tasks assessing false beliefs (or other as- (Turiel, 1983a, 1983b). In addition to how particular
sessments of understandings of mental states). The ra- practices shape childrens behaviors (the focus of much
pidity of the cognitive processing of 5- or 6-year-old of the research), it is necessary to consider how these
children can mask the uncertainties and ambiguities in very practices constitute forms of social communica-
younger childrens judgments, as well as the processes tion. Among the types of child-rearing practices, the
of reasoning in the older children. Furthermore, the de- most effective (the so-called induction method) entails
velopment of psychological understandings does not stop explanation of reasons for the required behaviors (Hoff-
there. Even adults can face difficulties and ambiguities man, 1970; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). That explicit
in understanding the psychological states and behaviors communications of this sort are more effective than
of persons (Ross & Nisbett, 1991). practices like physical punishment and love-withdrawal
Research on the moral decisions of people identified suggests that parents and children engage in reciprocal
by Colby and Damon (1992) as moral exemplars indi- interactions. Another body of findings on parental prac-
cated that judgments could include convictions, cer- tices has shown that authoritative forms of parenting
tainty, and openness to new ways of thinking. The are more effective than either authoritarian or per-
search for truth and openness to change in thinking do missive forms (Baumrind, 1973, 1989; Maccoby &
not reflect automatic, reflexive, or intuitively evident Martin, 1983; see also Collins & Steinberg, Chapter 16,
truths immediately apparent to an individual. However, this Handbook, this volume; Parke & Buriel, Chapter 8,
the certainty in the thinking of these individuals also this Handbook, this volume). One of the features distin-
leads them to make decisions that much of the time does guishing authoritative parenting from the others is an
not require belabored weighing of alternatives. It might emphasis on discussion, communication, and explana-
Emphasizing Judgment and Reciprocal Social Interactions 821

tion. It is also likely that the types of parental practices Even norms consistent with those of adults are discov-
that do not emphasize communications entail implicit ered by children through their interactions with friends.
communications. As implicit communications, the mes- Damon (1988) identified as primary to childhood re-
sages are less clear and more open to childrens own in- lationships reciprocity of a symmetrical kind, which is
terpretations. Perhaps this accounts for the findings that more likely to occur among children than between
the use of physical punishment is connected with greater adults and children because children perceive each other
aggressiveness on the part of children (Hoffman, 1970; as equals in status and power. Moreover, the mutuality
Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Physical punishment may and intimacy that develop among children entail close
convey the implicit message that inflicting physical collaboration and communication and are more likely to
harm and using a form of aggression is acceptable. foster decisions based on consensual agreements. Chil-
It is also likely that family influences on childrens dren, thereby, come to understand that social rules can
moral development go beyond the effects of parental be based on cooperation among equals in creating and
discipline practices. The structure of family interactions applying them and not solely on the authority of others.
is another important influence, especially as it relates to Children, through interactions involving give-and-take,
fairness in arrangements among males and females collaborations, the sharing of ideas, openness to new in-
(Hochschild, 1989; Okin, 1989, 1996). In most families, sights, and compromise co-construct knowledge and
there exist gender-related inequalities in the distribu- ways of thinking. Co-construction involves children to-
tion of power, the ways goods and privileges are allo- gether discovering solutions to problems and encourag-
cated, and the work opportunities encouraged or ing creative thinking.
discouraged. Such structural arrangements and prac- In the context of research on the development of con-
tices may well have an effect on childrens development, cepts of distributive justice, experimental work has
but this is an area largely neglected in research on fam- shown that children change more as a consequence of
ily influences. discussions with peers than with adults (Damon, 1981).
An interactional perspective, therefore, needs to ac- Other studies on the influences of peer discussion
count for many aspects of family life and social life in among college students were conducted in the context of
addition to family experiences (Turiel, 2004b). One of Kohlbergs stages of moral judgments. In these studies,
these that has received some attention from researchers students were paired for discussions about moral dilem-
is interaction among peers. Some have followed Piagets mas so as to create disparities in their previously as-
(1932) lead in proposing that peer interactions are im- sessed levels of moral judgment. It was found that
portant to childrens moral development (Damon, 1981, discussions between those whose levels were only
1988; Youniss, 1980). Piaget maintained that relation- slightly different served to stimulate change
ships of young children with adults were ones of con- (Berkowitz, Gibbs, & Broughton, 1980) and that the
straint, whereas relationships with peers are more likely most effective types of discussions entailed efforts at
to be ones of cooperation. According to Piaget, because transforming each others meanings into comprehensi-
peers are perceived more or less as equals, children are ble forms (Berkowitz & Gibbs, 1983).
more likely to take their perspectives and see them- Another source of development related to peer inter-
selves as responsible partners in social interchanges. actions occurs through social conflicts (Berkowitz &
Without necessarily presuming that interactions with Gibbs, 1985; Killen & Nucci, 1995), which are common
peers are more conducive than interactions with adults among children, and can stimulate them to take differ-
to the development of moral judgments, several re- ent points of view to restore balance to social situations,
searchers have examined the influences of childrens re- to produce ideas as to how to coordinate the needs of
lationships with each other. Damon (1981, 1984, 1988) others and self, and to consider the rights of otherses-
and Youniss (1980) propose that the effects of peer inter- pecially claims of ownership or possession of objects
actions are a consequence of the coordinating of ones (see Killen & Nucci, 1995, for more extensive discus-
perspective and actions with those of another, rather sion). Research by Killen and her colleagues (Killen,
than through the transmission of information and ideas 1989; Killen & Naigles, 1995; Killen & Nucci, 1995;
(Damon, 1981, p. 165). Furthermore, Damon maintains Killen & Sueyoshi, 1995; Rende & Killen, 1992) has
that important aspects of morality are first learned also demonstrated that in the absence of adult interven-
through play with friends, including norms that may be tion, young children are quite capable of addressing so-
discrepant with societal standards espoused by adults. cial conflicts and producing resolutions that take the
822 The Development of Morality

needs and interests of others into account. In another morality is not primarily driven by emotions. Emotions
study (Eisenberg, Lundy, Shell, & Roth, 1985), it was involve evaluative appraisals so that the entire distinc-
found that preschool children justified meeting the re- tion between reason and emotions begins to be called
quests of peers with references to the needs of others into question, and one can no longer assume that a
and to their relationships with others (requests of adults, thinker who focuses on reason is excluding emotion
by contrast, were justified with references to authority (Nussbaum, 1999, p. 72). From the psychological per-
and punishment). spective, emotional experiences inform the development
of thought and, reciprocally, thinking informs the devel-
opment and maintenance of emotions.
The Construction of Moral Judgments through
In line with Nussbaums propositions, the approaches
Social Interactions
to moral judgments linked to Piaget (1932) and
Conflicts, disputes, argumentation, and discussion are Kohlberg (1963) have considered emotional appraisals
all part of social interactions. For many who emphasize important to both the formation of morality and its ap-
the role of judgments in morality, such social interac- plications. However, aversive emotions are not the basis
tions are involved in the individuals constructions of of moral judgments. Rather, emotions of sympathy, em-
moral judgments that are not solely local or derived pri- pathy, and especially respect are central. In Piagets
marily from parental teachings or from an integrated, view, combinations of in-born or very early emerging
consistent cultural pattern. In these positions, generaliz- emotions of fear, affection, and sympathy, as well as
able, nonlocal moral judgments are not innately based vindictiveness and compassion, help form the basis for
but constructions through social interactions. A signifi- the development of morality. He regarded instinctive
cant aspect of approaches emphasizing judgments is to tendencies as a necessary but not a sufficient condition
have an epistemological grounding with regard to the for the formation of morality (Piaget, 1932, p. 344),
nature of the realm of morality. For a number of re- and maintained that the childs behavior toward per-
searchers, such grounding is provided by philosophical sons shows signs from the first of those sympathetic ten-
traditions that, as put by Nussbaum (1999), presume that dencies and affective reactions in which one can easily
human beings are above all reasoning beings. Nuss- see the raw material of all subsequent moral behavior.
baum has summarized such traditions going back to Im- But an intelligent act can only be called logical and a
manuel Kant and John Stuart Mill and including good-hearted impulse moral from the moment that cer-
contemporary philosophers like Rawls (1971, 1993), tain norms impress a given structure and rules of equi-
Dworkin (1977), Gewirth (1978), and Habermas librium upon this material (p. 405). It is especially the
(1990a, 1990b, 1993) as follows: combination of fear, affection, and sympathy in relation
to adults that are intertwined with social interactions
At the heart of this tradition is a twofold intuition about and processes of reasoning that make for the emergence
human beings: namely, that all, just by being human, are of heteronomywhich includes unilateral respect for
of equal dignity and worth, no matter where they are situ- those in authority. The transformation of unilateral re-
ated in society, and that the primary source of this worth spect into feelings of mutual respect is essential for the
is a power of moral choice within them, a power that con- emergence of autonomous moral judgments about wel-
sists in the ability to plan a life in accordance with ones fare and justice.
own evaluations of ends . . . the moral equality of persons
From philosophical perspectives, as well as psycho-
gives them a fair claim to certain types of treatment at the
logical ones, propositions regarding universalizable
hands of society and politics. What this treatment is will
be a subject of debate within the tradition, but the shared
moral reasoning are consistent with the idea that moral-
starting point is that this treatment must do two closely re- ity stems from social experiences and social con-
lated things. It must respect and promote the liberty of structions. One of the most extensive philosophical for-
choice, and it must respect and promote the equal worth of mulations of the social sources of moral reasoning can
persons as choosers. (p. 54) be seen in the neo-Kantian propositions of Habermas
(1993), based on his theory of communicative action
Nussbaum adds that a basic moral premise in these ap- and discourse ethics, that morality entails concepts of
proaches is that each person be treated as an end and not justice, rights, and welfare (of others and the general
as a means to other goals. She maintains that emotions welfare). Habermas bridges important distinctions: one
are intertwined with moral reasoning. In this view, between justification and application of moral norms,
Emphasizing Judgment and Reciprocal Social Interactions 823

the other between individual autonomy and social soli- Kohlberg. This includes research conducted by Kohlberg
darity. Whereas moral principles are justified or and his colleagues tracing developmental changes longi-
grounded in criteria of universalizability and impartial- tudinally (Colby, Kohlberg, Gibbs, & Lieberman, 1983)
ity, situational features are taken into account in their and reformulating the specific descriptions of the stages
application to concrete instances. Consequently, the way (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987a; Kohlberg, 1984; Kohlberg,
moral principles are understood is a necessary compo- Levine, & Hewer, 1983). Other researchers have used
nent, as is an understanding of the features of the con- Kohlbergs formulations to examine relationships be-
text. As put by Habermas (1993): tween perspective taking and moral development (e.g.,
Keller & Edelstein, 1991; Selman, 1980; Walker, 1980),
The principle of universalization that regulates discourses hypotheses regarding the invariance of the stages
of justification does not exhaust the normative sense of (Walker, 1982; Walker, de Vries, & Bichard, 1984), and
the impartiality of a just judgment. A further principle processes by which changes occur (Berkowitz & Gibbs,
must be adduced to guarantee the correctness of singular 1983; Walker & Taylor, 1991).
judgments. An impartial judge must assess which of the
Some studies have examined family variables and
competing norms of actionwhose validity has been es-
moral judgment, including correspondence between par-
tablished in advanceis most appropriate to a given con-
crete case once all the relevant features of the given
ents and childrens levels (e.g., Hart, 1988b; Walker &
constellation of circumstances have been accorded due Taylor, 1991). Other studies (Hart, 1988a; Hart &
weight in the situational description. Thus, principles of Chmiel, 1992) have related personality measures and
appropriateness and the exhaustion of all relevant contex- defense mechanisms to stages of moral judgment. There
tual features come into play here. (p. 129) have also been theoretical formulations aimed at com-
bining aspects of Kohlbergs stage sequence with other
Habermas maintained that both autonomy and social variables, such as affect, coping, and stress, to explain
solidarity are essential features for those participating unusual moral commitments on the part of individuals
in a network of reciprocal expectations. Discourse, com- (Haste, 1990). Still other research has attempted to ex-
munication, and argumentation are the means by which tend the analyses to the adult life span, including among
individuals function in a moral world. This places indi- the elderly (Pratt, Golding, & Hunter, 1983; Pratt, Gold-
viduals in a collectivity, attempting to maintain social ing, Hunter, & Norris, 1988).
solidarity by submitting their moral principles for veri- Studies were conducted on the influences of attend-
fication by others in moral dialogue, reflective discus- ing college on the development of moral judgments (e.g.,
sion, and argumentation. In the process, individuals take Rest & Narvaez, 1991). These studies deviate from
positions based on moral concepts but through consen- Kohlbergs procedures in that assessments were made
sus attempt to achieve resolutions that account for the using a paper and pencil questionnaire requesting indi-
general welfare and maintenance of solidarity. viduals to rate and rank a series of solutions to moral
Habermas also incorporated developmental research dilemmas corresponding to the six stagesin contrast
into his philosophical formulations, relying on his re- with Kohlbergs semistructured clinical interview
vised form of the progression of moral judgments for- aimed at ascertaining individuals ways of thinking (see
mulated by Kohlberg (the general outline of the stage Rest, 1979, for details on the standardized assessment;
progression was provided earlier). Like Kohlberg see Damon, 1977, Piaget, 1929; Turiel, 1983a, for dis-
(1976), Habermas regarded stages of perspective taking cussions of the aims and value of the clinical interview
(Selman, 1980) as part of the process of the formation of method). The research conducted by Rest is also linked
moral judgments. However, Habermass theory of dis- to what he refers to as a Four Component Model of
course ethics is not dependent on Kohlbergs particular moral development, including judgment, sensitivity,
formulation. It would be consistent with psychological motivation, and ego strength (it is detailed in a previous
formulations that postulate that individuals make moral edition of this Handbook; Rest, 1983; see also Rest,
judgments of a generalizable kind. Narvaez, & Thoma, 1999).
Kohlbergs formulations are not further reviewed
here, as they have been discussed extensively in previous
Moral Judgments in Early Childhood and Beyond
editions of this Handbook (see Hoffman, 1970, and, es-
pecially, Rest, 1983). Several researchers, however, have Kohlbergs stage formulation, in which young childrens
pursued hypotheses based on the stages proposed by moral judgments are based on obedience and sanctions,
824 The Development of Morality

was derived from responses to complex situations in goods (Damon, 1977, 1980). For example, in one situa-
which competing and conflicting issues are depicted. As tion, children in a class that made paintings to sell at a
an example, the often-cited situation of a man who must school fair must decide how to distribute the proceeds.
decide whether to steal an overpriced drug that might Children were asked to respond to examples of ways of
save his wifes life includes considerations of the value distributing the money (e.g., on the basis of merit, need,
of life, property rights, violating the law, interpersonal equality, and sex of the children) and to give their ideas
obligations, and personal responsibilities to each of on how the money should be distributed. The children
these. In that sense, Kohlberg was attempting to study were presented with examples of ways of distribution so
judgments in contexts. He constructed hypothetical situ- as to elicit reactions to three categories considered basic
ations in which the use of readily conceived values (e.g., in the literature on moral philosophy: equality, merit,
it is wrong to steal; it is wrong to allow someone to die) and benevolence.
would be complicated by situational circumstances It was found that childrens thinking about distribu-
(e.g., if you do not steal, you sacrifice a life; if you try to tive justice progresses through four levels encompassing
save a life, you violate anothers property rights). These equality, merit, and benevolence (though not at the first
situations, however, presented multifaceted problems level). At the first level, concepts of distribution ini-
requiring children to weigh and coordinate competing tially are tied to the childs own desires and perspec-
moral considerations and nonmoral considerations tives. After these initial judgments, children begin to
(Turiel, 1978a, 1978b, 1983a, in press-a). The complex- bring in external criteria (such as size or ability). Al-
ity of the judgments required by those situations led to though these external features ultimately are used to
the appearance that young childrens moral judgments justify a persons desires and goals, this way of thinking
are contingent on sanctions, are not based on under- leads to other-oriented concepts based on equality,
standings of morality as generalizable, and it is not until merit, and benevolence. Elementary school-aged chil-
after progressing to the fourth stage (usually not until at dren, at the next level, base their judgments on equality;
least adolescence) that morality is distinguished from everyone should be given the same amount and receive
nonmoral issues (Turiel & Davidson, 1986). Research the same treatment, regardless of merit or need. Next
into several aspects of moral judgments indicates that comes a shift to considerations of merit and reciprocity;
starting at a young age children make moral judgments distribution is based on the need to acknowledge good
that are not based on extrinsic features like obedience deeds, hard work, or personal attributes like intelli-
and sanctions. These include judgments about distribu- gence. The next shift includes judgments that take
tive justice, social justice in institutional settings, and benevolence into account, with greater awareness of
prosocial actions. competing claims and an understanding of the need for
compromises to resolve claims in a fair manner. There-
Concepts of Distributive Justice and Fairness fore, by the ages of 10 or 11 years, children take into ac-
of Social Practices count merit ( hard work, talent), advantages and
disadvantages, and other factors (e.g., investment, inher-
In accord with long-standing presumptions among itance).
philosophers (e.g., Aristotle, 1947; Mill, 1863/1963; This developmental sequence was supported by lon-
Rawls, 1971), Damon (1988, p. 31) has placed issues of gitudinal findings (see Damon, 1977, 1980). Further-
distributive justice at the forefront of moral concerns. more, similar patterns of judgment were obtained in
His research on childrens concepts of sharing and dis- studies of behavioral situations in experimental contexts
tribution revealed a developmental progression of moral (Gerson & Damon, 1978). Judgments on the real-life
judgments (Damon, 1975, 1977, 1980, 1988), with indi- situations (elicited individually and in group discus-
cations that very young children are somewhat attuned sions) were highly correlated with judgments on the hy-
to sharing. In their 2nd year, children take turns in play- pothetical situations, and the same age trends were
ing with objects and show awareness that food or candy evidenced. It was also found, however, that personal
can be divided. Information regarding how children 4 to concerns were coordinated with judgments about distri-
5 years of age and older conceptualize sharing comes bution to a greater extent in the real-life than the hypo-
from research on childrens judgments about hypotheti- thetical situations. For example, children who showed
cal and real-life situations entailing the distribution of an understanding of merit as a basis for fairness were
Emphasizing Judgment and Reciprocal Social Interactions 825

most likely to apply that understanding when they were sacrifice of self-interests. These are not the only types
themselves in a meritorious position than when they of positive social actions experienced by children. The
were not. In ways consistent with these findings, Blasi term prosocial moral reasoning has been used (Eisen-
(1993) proposed that an integration of concepts of self berg-Berg, 1979) with reference to judgments about pos-
and identity contributes to how individuals act on their itive social actions (e.g., helping, giving) serving to
moral judgments. benefit others in contexts in which a persons actions
An age-related sequence generally corresponding to are not based on rules, laws, or the dictates of authori-
the levels of concepts of distributive justice has been ties. Children were presented with hypothetical situa-
observed by Thorkildsen (1989a, 1989b, 1991) in chil- tions posing conflicts between the needs and desires of
drens judgments about the fairness of classroom prac- different actors and questioned about whether it would
tices pertaining to educational goods (e.g., ways of be right to help, give, or share with others at the expense
fostering learning, contests, and testing situations). In of their own goals (Eisenberg-Berg, 1979). One situa-
one study (Thorkildsen, 1989a), children and adoles- tion depicted people faced with deciding whether to
cents judged the fairness of several different teaching help feed those of another town who had lost their food
practices as to how faster workers would proceed, rela- in a flood; doing so would present a hardship to them.
tive to slower workers, in a class assignment (e.g., after Other situations included donating blood, helping an-
finishing, faster workers tutor slower ones; faster work- other who is being mugged or bullied, and helping phys-
ers move on to other learning experiences). It was found ically disabled children.
that younger children focus on equality and older ones A sequence of five age-related levels in judgments
on equity. In the focus on equality, there are also shifts about prosocial actions were identifieda sequence
with age in the goods considered relevant. The youngest proposed to reflect developmental advances in capabil-
children judged as fair attainment of an equality of re- ities for complex perspective taking and for understand-
wards, whereas somewhat older children consider as fair ing abstract concepts (Eisenberg, Miller, Shell,
those practices that result in equality in schoolwork McNalley, & Shea, 1991, p. 849). It was also proposed
completed. This is followed by an emphasis on learning that the levels do not constitute hierarchical, integrated
as the relevant good where practices that foster equality structures, and that the sequence is not entirely invariant
in learning are judged as fair. Finally, equity in learning nor necessarily universal (i.e., Eisenberg indicates that
is judged as the basis for fairness; it is fair that those ca- some aspects of the levels likely are invariant due to de-
pable of learning more than others do so. velopmental changes in perspective-taking skills and
Thorkildsen, in other research (1989b), has linked abstract reasoning and that the early levels are more
judgments of fair practices to individuals situational likely to be universal than later ones; Eisenberg, 1986).
definitions. What is considered a fair classroom practice This implies that an individuals reasoning can be spread
in the context of a learning activity differed from what over the different types and that the sequence may vary
is considered fair in a testing situation (e.g., helping by situations and life circumstances. At the first level,
slower workers to learn was judged as more fair than judgments are based on a hedonistic, self-focused ori-
helping them on a test). These findings indicate that entation (personal gain is linked to reciprocity with oth-
children coordinate their understandings of the goals of ers, based in identification and relationship with
events (e.g., to learn or to demonstrate what one knows), another, or liking for the other), whereas at the next level
participants perspectives on those events, and what there is an orientation to the needs of others. This is fol-
would constitute a breach of just expectations. Thork- lowed by judgments based on stereotypes of good or bad
ildsen (1989b, 1991) interprets these results as showing persons, along with concerns with the approval or disap-
that concepts of justice vary in accord with spheres of proval of others. The fourth level is characterized by a
activities (e.g., justice is one thing in economics, an- self-reflective and empathetic orientation, including
other in the family). sympathetic concern and caring for others, and taking
the perspective of others. At the fifth level, there is an
Prosocial Moral Judgments internalization of affect linked to self-respect and an in-
ternalization of laws, norms, duties, and responsibili-
Childrens judgments about sharing or distribution per- ties, as well as abstract types of reasoning about society,
tain to actions beneficial to others and possibly entail rights, justice, and equality.
826 The Development of Morality

A series of longitudinal assessments were conducted central to morality. Aristotle, like many philosophers
following children from preschool to ages 19 to 20 years after him (e.g., Dworkin, 1977; Gewirth, 1978, 1982;
(Eisenberg, Carlo, Murphy, & Van Court, 1995; Eisen- Habermas, 1990b; Rawls, 1971), considered justice as
berg, Lennon, & Roth, 1983; Eisenberg et al., 1987, other-regarding, impartial, and as characterized by
1991; Eisenberg-Berg & Roth, 1980; Valiente et al., universality (see Helwig, Turiel, & Nucci, 1996, for fur-
2003). The longitudinal findings yielded a heteroge- ther discussion). As already indicated, Piagets research
neous pattern of changes in prosocial judgments. In was consistent with moral epistemologies of this type.
broad terms, there was advance on the levels with in- However, Piaget proposed that understandings of wel-
creasing age and decreased use of the lowest levels. fare, justice, and rights did not emerge until after a pe-
However, along with increased use of the higher levels riod in which right and wrong are judged by the word of
with age, there were renewed uses of aspects of lower authorities and the necessity of adhering to their rules.
levels. Hedonistic reasoning decreased in mid-adoles- Such unilateral respect, according to Piaget, reflected
cence, but in late adolescence, along with increases in young childrens heteronomous thinking in which undif-
self-reflection and empathy, there was some increase in ferentiated concepts of authority are based on adults
hedonistic reasoning (i.e., in situations where costs of size, power, and knowledge. In this way of thinking, jus-
helping were high). The patterns obtained in the longitu- tice is subordinated to obeying rules and authority: if
dinal studies indicate, again, an interaction of different distributive justice is brought into conflict with adult au-
ways of thinking with situational contexts, as do studies thority, the youngest subjects will believe authority right
in Brazil (Carlo, Koller, Eisenberg, DaSilva, & Frolich, and justice wrong (Piaget, 1932, p. 304). However, sev-
1996) and on relations of prosocial reasoning to emo- eral studies conducted in the United States (Braine,
tions (Miller, Eisenberg, Fabes, & Shell, 1996; for fur- Pomerantz, Lorber, & Krantz, 19

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen