Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 1998 by the Am n Psychological Association, Inc.

1998, Vol. 83, No. 5, 731-744 0021-9010/98/$3.00

What Motivates Fairness? The Role of Subordinate Assertive Behavior


on Managers' Interactional Fairness

M. Audrey Korsgaard Loriann Roberson


University of South Carolina Arizona State University

R. Douglas Rymph
University of South Carolina

This study investigated the proposition that a subordinate's communication style can
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

affect a manager's fairness behavior during decision making and, consequently, can affect
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the subordinate's attitudes toward the decision, manager, and organization. Two studies
were conducted to test these propositions in the context of performance appraisal deci-
sions. First, a laboratory study demonstrated that appraisers engage in more interactionally
fair behavior when interacting with an assertive appraisee than with an unassertive ap-
praisee. Second, a quasiexperimental field design showed that training employees on
assertiveness, when coupled with self-appraisal, is associated with positive attitudes to-
ward the appraisal and trust in the manager. Implications for understanding the causes of
fair behavior and improving the fairness of decisions are discussed.

Fairness in decision making has a powerful impact on toward management and the organizations (Cropanzano,
a wide variety of human resource-related decisions such 1993; Greenberg, 1993; Lind & Tyler, 1988).
as those pertaining to layoffs, staffing, pay raises, drug Procedural justice research has focused largely on indi-
testing, and discipline (Cropanzano, 1993; Folger & Ko- viduals' reactions to formal policies and procedures. More
novsky, 1989; Gilliland, 1994; Konovsky & Cropanzano, recently, a subset of procedural justice has emerged called
1991; Miceli, Jung, Near, & Greenberger, 1991). Until interactional justice (Folger & Bies, 1989; Greenberg,
recently, most of the research in this area has focused on 1993; Tyler & Bies, 1990), which refers to the interper-
two main categories of fairness: distributive justice, the sonal side of decision making, specifically to the fairness
fairness of resource allocation decisions, and procedural of the decision maker's behavior in the process of decision
justice, the fairness of the process of making those deci- making. Decision makers behave in an interactionally fair
sions (Lind & Tyler, 1988). In general, research indicates manner when they treat those affected by the decision
that the fair allocation of resources and the fair structuring properly and enact the decision policy or procedure prop-
of procedures are each uniquely important predictors of erly (Folger & Bies, 1989; Tyler & Bies, 1990). Proper
perceived fairness, acceptance of decisions, and attitudes interpersonal treatment is defined as (a) being truthful in
communication and treating people with courtesy and (b)
showing respect. Proper enactment of procedures is de-
M. Audrey Korsgaard and R. Douglas Rymph, The Darla
fined by five behaviors: (a) adequate consideration of the
Moore School of Business, University of South Carolina; Lori-
ann Roberson, College of Business Administration, Arizona employee's input, (b) suppression of personal biases, (c)
State University. consistent application of decision-making criteria, (d)
We thank M. Susan Taylor, William Click, and Donna Blan- timely feedback, and (e) justification for the decision.
cero for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article. These interactional factors play an important role in af-
Portions of this article were presented at the 11 th Annual Con- fecting employees' perceptions of fairness, acceptance of
ference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychol- decisions, and organizational attitudes (Konovsky & Cro-
ogy, April 1996, San Diego, California; the Australian Industrial/
panzano, 1991; Korsgaard, Schweiger, & Sapienza, 1995;
Organizational Psychology Conference, June 1997, Melbourne,
Schaubroeck, May, & Brown, 1994).
Australia; and the New Zealand Industrial/Organizational Psy-
The findings concerning interactional justice pose a
chology Conference, July 1997, Auckland, New Zealand.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed unique problem, for, although some degree of distributive
to M. Audrey Korsgaard, The Darla Moore School of Business, and procedural justice can be ensured through the design
University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina 29208. of equitable organizational policies and procedures, it is
Electronic mail may be sent to korsgaar@darla.badm.sc.edu. more difficult to control the fair enactment of decision-

731
732 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

making procedures. Because organizations typically do Cropanzano, 1992; Folger & Lewis, 1993; Greenberg,
not monitor or hold managers accountable for their inter- 1986, 1987; Korsgaard & Roberson, 1995; Taylor et al.,
actions with all employees, managers may adhere to poli- 1995). In this context, distributive justice has been opera-
cies without engaging in interactionally fair behavior. That tionalized as the fairness or favorableness of the final
is, managers may obey the letter but not the spirit of the evaluation and of any associated monetary reward (Fol-
law (Folger & Bies, 1989). In fact, research suggests ger & Konovsky, 1989; Greenberg, 1986). Numerous pro-
that, despite the instrumentality of acting fair (Greenberg, cedural factors that contribute to procedural justice, such
1990), decision makers are often unwilling to behave in as voice, adequate forewarning, and opportunity to appeal,
a fair manner, particularly when the decision outcome have been found to have a positive impact on employee
is unfavorable to the employee (Folger, 1993; Folger & attitudes when applied to the performance appraisal pro-
Skarlicki, 1998). Although studies show that managers cess (Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Korsgaard & Roberson,
can be successfully trained to act in an interactionally fair 1995; Taylor et al., 1995).
manner (Korsgaard et al., 1995; Taylor, Tracy, Renard, Interactional factors, particularly consideration and jus-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Harrison, & Carroll, 1995), little is known about what tification, are also recognized as an important component
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

motivates interactionally fair behavior (Folger & Skar- of creating perceptions of fairness in the performance
licki, 1998). appraisal system. Folger et al. (1992) included these two
The purpose of this investigation was to examine one factors in their due process model of performance ap-
factor that might affect the extent to which managers en- praisal. Specifically, they argued that two-way communi-
gage in interactionally fair behavior: the communication cation between a manager and a subordinate and the man-
style of their subordinates. We focus on what Greenberg ager's willingness to discuss how ratings were made in-
(1993) described as the informational component of inter- fluence the perception of fairness. Taylor et al. (1995)
actional justice, which pertains to the exchange of infor- tested and found support for this model using a compre-
mation (e.g., providing justification). Specifically, we ex- hensive intervention that included training managers to
amine two aspects of interactional justiceconsideration increase levels of consideration and justification.
and justificationthat are repeatedly mentioned as criti- Although consideration and justification are identified
cal for perceptions of fairness. Consideration refers to the consistently as important interactional factors, they have
extent to which the decision maker acknowledges and not been studied in isolation. Yet the repeated indications
considers the views and concerns of others affected by of the importance of these two factors warrant their sepa-
the decision (Tyler, 1987). Justification refers to the extent rate examination (Folger et al., 1992). Moreover, these
to which an adequate explanation or account for the deci- two factors concern the exchange of information and, as
sion is provided (Shapiro, 1993). Because information such, are apt to be influenced by communication pro-
exchange is a two-way process, it is possible that the cesses. For these reasons, the present research focuses on
subordinate's communication style can influence the ex- consideration and justification. As interactional factors,
tent to which the manager engages in these behaviors. these are behaviors largely within the control and discre-
Therefore, we propose that an assertive communication tion of the manager. The issue then arises of what moti-
style on the part of the employee will evoke greater inter- vates managers to engage in these behaviors.
actionally fair behavior on the part of the manager. To test
this proposition, we conducted two studies in the context
Assertive Communication and Fair Behavior
of the performance appraisal process. The first, a labora-
tory' experiment, examined the impact of assertiveness Although, as noted, there is evidence that direct inter-
on appraisers' levels of interactionally fair behavior. The ventions on decision makers' behavior can successfully
second, a quasiexperimental field study, examined the im- promote interactional justice (Greenberg, 1994; Kors-
pact of training subordinates to communicate assertively gaard et al., 1995; Schaubroeck et al., 1994; Taylor et al.,
on perceptions of fair behavior and related organizational 1995), managers may be unwilling to engage in interac-
attitudes. In the following paragraphs, we briefly review tionally fair behavior. Researchers are just beginning to
the literature that forms the basis of our propositions. understand why individuals may be less than fair. Folger
(1993) suggested several possible mechanisms linked to
Organizational Justice in Performance the potential psychological costs of interactionally fair
behavior for the decision maker. Specifically, Folger ar-
Appraisal Systems
gued that decision makers might believe that acting in a
Although justice concepts have been applied to a wide fair manner will result in a loss of control and influence
variety of organizational decisions, one area that has re- or in a loss of status and credibility. As a result, decision
ceived considerable attention is performance appraisal makers attempt to distance themselves from those affected
systems (Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Folger, Konovsky, & by the decisions. Folger and Skarlicki (1998) supported
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 733

these assertions, finding that when decision makers felt 1979). In addition, the use of assertiveness appears to
culpable for the decision outcome, they preferred to mini- increase knowledge and understanding between the parti-
mize their interactions with the individuals affected by cipants. Studies of couples in therapeutic settings have
the decision. Thus, managers may show less consideration reported increases in trust, empathy, and intimacy in rela-
of subordinates' input and provide inadequate justifica- tionships from the use of assertiveness (Epstein & Jack-
tion. For example, to avoid conflict with subordinates, son, 1978; Gordon & Waldo, 1984).
managers may attempt to control their interactions with In sum, we propose that one factor that determines
employees by holding a brief, one-sided performance re- an appraiser's propensity to engage in interactionally fair
view meeting. behavior is the extent to which the appraisee communi-
Although Folger's (1993; see also Folger & Skarlicki, cates assertively. Specifically, when an individual commu-
1998) work helps to understand why managers are reluc- nicates assertively, by seeking to ensure that the appraiser
tant to engage in interactionally fair behavior, little is understands his or her point of view and asking for expla-
known of the contextual factors that might encourage such nations from the appraiser, the appraiser is more likely to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior. One potential contextual factor is assertive com- exhibit interactionally fair behavior in the form of consid-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

munication on the part of the subordinate. Several dimen- eration of the individual's input and justification of the
sions of assertive communication have been identified decision. We thus propose the following:
(Cooley & Hollandsworth, 1977; Rakos, 1986). First, an
Hypothesis I (Study 1): The level of assertive communi-
assertive style involves stating one's own position in a
cation on the part of the appraisee will have a positive
confident manner while avoiding phrases that devalue the impact on the level of interactionally fair behavior exhibited
message (Hayes, 1991; Rakos, 1986). In the appraisal by the appraiser.
interview, an assertive subordinate would give clear, suc-
cinct statements of his or her opinions and perceptions,
Study 1
even when such views run counter to that of the manager.
A second component of assertiveness involves using non- Method
verbal behavior that signals to others that they should pay
Design and participants. Because, to date, the impact of an
attention to the speaker's point of view (Hayes, 1991;
individual's communication style on the fair behavior of another
Rakos, 1986). Studies have found that, in general, effec-
individual has not been examined, we conducted an initial exper-
tive assertiveness includes the use of an intermediate vol-
iment to test this causal link. In this study, we systematically
ume and firmness of speech, eye contact, and an involved varied the communication style of a confederate and examined
body posture (Rakos, 1986). A third aspect of assert- its impact on the behavior of participants. A two-group, single-
iveness is attentive listening to the statements of others factor experimental design was used, with assertiveness (high
(Bolton, 1979; Zuker, 1984). In the appraisal interview, vs. low) as the independent variable. Participants were recruited
this would involve the use of active listening techniques from an undergraduate management course at a large southeast-
such as paraphrasing and asking questions of the manager ern university. Forty-two undergraduates (16 female, 26 male)
so that his or her point of view is clarified and explained. volunteered to participate in this experiment in exchange for
We propose that these actions, when taken by subordi- extra credit.
nates, are likely to evoke greater consideration and justi- Procedure. Participants were told that they would be in an
experiment with another participant recruited from another col-
fication from superiors because this style actively seeks
lege in the university. The other participant was actually a con-
such responses. An employee who communicates assert-
federate (female undergraduate). On arriving for the experi-
ively would be more likely to ensure that the manager ment, the purpose and procedure were explained to the partici-
hears and acknowledges that employee's opinions. Em- pant and confederate, and each signed a consent form. The
ployee use of attentive listening, another component of participant and confederate were told they were participating in
assertiveness, should result in greater explanation of the a study about giving and receiving feedback in which one of
basis of the evaluation. them would perform a task and the other would take the role
Although much of the literature on assertiveness has of the appraiser. The participant and confederate then drew lots,
focused on the consequences of assertiveness for the as- which were rigged such that the participant was always given
serter, such as self-esteem and job satisfaction (e.g., Al- the role of appraiser. The experimenter then escorted the confed-
erate to another room to perform the task.
berti & Emmons, 1970; Rabin & Zelner, 1992), evidence
While the confederate was ostensibly completing the task,
indicates it can change the other's responses. Studies have
the participant received oral and written instructions on evaluat-
found that assertiveness is an effective influence tactic and
ing the confederate's performance. The confederate's task was
enhances interpersonal effectiveness. In general, assertive a critical thinking skills test composed of four sections: making
responses evoke more rapid short-term compliance from inferences, recognizing assumptions, deducing, and evaluating
others than does the use of passive or aggressive styles arguments. The participant was given a key to the test and a
(Hollandsworth & Cooley, 1978; Hull & Schroeder, rating form in which the participant rated each of the four skills
734 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

on a scale of 1 (poor) to 15 (exceptional). For each skill section 1 did pretty good, but at least not really bad, anyway. But
of the test, there were five multiple-choice questions, worth a if that's what you t h o u g h t . . . I mean I guess if you think
total of 10 points, and one short answer, worth 5 points. The so. You know, it was sort of hard but, I tried really hard.
rating for each skill was determined by totaling the points for Well, I guess . . . um yeah . . . all right, whatever.
that skill section. Identical versions of the completed test were
Measures. The measure of interactionally fair behavior was
given to all participants to control the range of ratings. Perfor-
derived from audiotaped statements of the participant that were
mance on the multiple-choice items was poor: Only one or two
transcribed by a typist who was unaware of the hypotheses.
items in each section were correct. The short-answer responses
Participants' responses to the eight scripted comments were
were designed to be consistent in quality and length, and partici-
each coded by a trained rater who was unaware of condition
pants exercised judgment in their evaluations of the short answer.
and hypotheses (participants' responses were unidentified and
Therefore, we were able to control the range of ratings while
randomly sorted). Two dimensions of interactionally fair behav-
still leading participants to feel personally responsible for the
ior were assessed: consideration, the extent to which the partici-
evaluations.
pant acknowledged and showed consideration of the confeder-
The participant was left alone to review the key while the
ate's views, and justification, the extent to which the participant
confederate completed the task. The experimenter returned 10
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

provided justification for his or her decision. Using pilot data,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

min later and gave the participant the confederate's answers to


two 8-point (0-7) behaviorally anchored rating scales were
evaluate. When the participant completed the evaluation, the
constructed to rate consideration and justification, respectively.
experimenter took the participant to the confederate's room and
Using pilot data, the rater was trained along with a second rater,
instructed the participant to review the evaluation with the con-
who was used as a reliability check. Based on a sample of 80
federate. This discussion was audiorecorded (with the informed
pilot statements, reliability was estimated, using an interclass
consent of the participant). After the discussion, the confederate
correlation, at .76. Ratings were averaged to form an overall
was escorted to another room to seemingly complete a second
index of interactionally fair behavior.
version of the test. The participant then completed a brief ques-
As a supplement to the content coding of the participants'
tionnaire, was debriefed, and was dismissed.
statements, we also administered a self-report scale of partici-
Manipulation of asseniveness. Assertiveness was manipu-
pants' interactionally fair behavior using seven 5-point Likert-
lated by having the confederate follow one of two scripts varying
type items. Example statements are "I explained the reasons
in level of assertiveness. Both scripts followed the same struc-
for the evaluation I gave the student" (justification) and "I
ture. First, as instructed, the participant reviewed the four rat-
listened carefully to the student's opinions on her performance''
ings. The confederate followed by making eight comments (two
(consideration). These items were averaged to form an overall
comments for each of the four ratings), each time waiting for
index (a = .89). This self-rated measure of interactionally fair
a response from the participant. The length of the confederate
behavior was moderately correlated with the content-coded
statements was approximately equal in the number of words and
measure (r = .28, p < .05).
syllables, and the general tone of the statements was parallel.
The manipulation check for the assertiveness manipulation
As noted earlier, assertive communication is characterized by
was assessed by eight 5-point Likert-type items measuring the
several behaviors, including stating one's own position in a
participants' perceptions of aspects of the confederate's assert-
confident manner (Hayes, 1991; Rakos, 1986); using active lis-
ive verbal and nonverbal behaviors. Example statements are
tening techniques, such as paraphrasing and asking questions of
"The student was confident in expressing her opinions" and
clarification (Bolton, 1979; Zucker, 1984); and using nonverbal
"The student looked me in the eye while she was talking." The
behaviors such as eye contact and an involved body posture
coefficient alpha of these eight items was .85.
(Hayes, 1991; Rakos, 1986). A high assertive communication
script was developed based on this definition in which the con-
federate directly stated opinions of his or her performance, cited Results
specific examples to support those opinions, asked the partici-
pant questions of clarification, and engaged in high eye contact A t test revealed a significant difference between condi-
and forward body posture. A sample response in the high-assert- tions in the manipulation check, f(40) = 5.17, p < .05.
ive condition is as follows: On average, participants in the low-assertiveness condi-
tion rated the confederate's assertiveness lower than did
I'm not sure I agree. How did you come up with that? I participants in the high-assertiveness condition, (low as-
thought I did pretty good. The question on arctic survival
sertiveness: M = 2.86, SD = 0.53; high-assertiveness: M
was ambiguous. And the other onethere wasn't much
information on Mr. Smith, so it was hard to come up with = 3.98,50 = 0.84).
explanations, "feu know what I'm saying? A t test was used to test the effects of the assertiveness
manipulation on ratings of interactionally fair behavior,
Similarly, a low-assertive script was developed based on the revealing a significant difference between conditions,
obverse of this definition: The confederate made vague or non-
?(40) = 4.35, p < .05; r (condition, interactionally fair
committal statements, asked indirect questions of the participant,
behavior) = .57. On average, participants in the high-
and engaged in low eye contact and slumped body posture. A
assertiveness condition displayed more interactionally fair
sample response in the low-assertive condition is as follows:
behavior than did participants in the low-assertiveness
That part was kind of tough, but. . . um . . . I still thought condition (high assertiveness: M = 2.31, SD = 0.49; low
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 735

assertiveness:M = 1.51,SZ> = 0.65).Thus, the hypothesis intervention evaluated was an attempt to train employees
that assertive communication evokes higher levels of inter- to communicate assertively in the context of discussing
actionally fair behavior was supported. However; it is their self-appraisal and receiving feedback.
noteworthy that the means in both conditions were quite
low given that the scale ranged from 0 to 7.
Study 2
An examination of participants' self-ratings of interac-
tionally fair behavior provides further support for the im- In this study, we investigated assertiveness training in
pact of assertive communication on interactionally fair the context of voice through formal self-appraisal. Provid-
behavior, t(40) = 3.54, p < .05; r (condition, interac- ing the opportunity for voice, particularly in the form of
tionally fair behavior) = .49. On average, participants in formal self-appraisals, is a common method for at-
the high-assertiveness condition reported higher levels of tempting to enhance the perceived fairness of appraisals
interactionally fair behavior (M = 4.07, SD = 0.79) than (Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Folger et al., 1992; Folger &
did participants in the low-assertiveness condition (M = Lewis, 1993; Korsgaard & Roberson, 1995; Taylor et al.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

3.18, SD = 0.84). Thus, significant results were obtained 1995). As discussed later, however, research suggests that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for both the content-coded and self-rated measures of in- the effectiveness of voice is qualified by the extent to
teractional fairness, although the levels of interactional which the decision maker shows consideration of em-
fairness indicated in the self report measure were higher ployee voice or provides justification for the decision
than those of the content-coded measure. (Shapiro, 1993). Therefore, in Study 2, we examined as-
sertive communication training designed to increase con-
sideration and justification when employees had voice
Discussion
opportunities.
The purpose of Study 1 was to demonstrate that the Voice, or process control, is defined as "the degree to
communication style of one individual can affect the de- which the procedure gives those affected by a decision an
gree of interactionally fair behavior exhibited by another opportunity to express their views about how the decision
individual. As hypothesized, our findings indicated that an should be made" (Tyler, Rasinski, & Spodick, 1985, p.
assertive communication style on the part of an individual 72). Voice can be distinguished from outcome control,
receiving an evaluation had a positive impact on the de- which refers to control over the decision outcome, in that
gree to which participants in the role of appraiser engaged voice does not necessarily involve influence over the deci-
in interactionally fair behavior. As noted, ratings of parti- sion outcome (Leung & Li, 1990; Shapiro, 1993; Tyler el
cipants' comments suggest that participants in both condi- al., 1985). When individuals do not have outcome control,
tions did not exhibit a high degree of interactionally fair such as in third-party dispute resolution or when the deci-
behavior. Given that participants in Study 1 were essen- sion is made by their manager, individuals' satisfaction
tially passing negative judgments on the confederate, this with the decision is influenced by the opportunity to voice
finding is not surprising. Folger (1993; Folger & Skarlicki, their views. The opportunity to voice one's views has a
1998) argued that decision makers often fail to engage in positive effect on perceptions of fairness and reactions to
interactionally fair behavior when delivering bad news. the decision independent of the level of actual influence
Specifically, managers, in their attempts to distance them- over the outcome (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Further, voice has
selves from the harm they may be inflicting on their subor- a particularly strong and positive effect when the decision
dinates, may unintentionally treat their subordinates in outcome is unfavorable (Brockner & Weisenfeld, 1996).
an unfair manner. Our findings suggest that an assertive In the performance appraisal process, voice is often
communication style on the part of the subordinate may operationalized by the use of self-appraisal (Folger &
mitigate this problem; however, subordinate assertiveness Lewis, 1993; Taylor et al., 1995), which involves the
alone may not be sufficient to yield high degrees of mana- formal self-evaluation of an employee's performance that
gerial interactional fairness. is used to supplement performance appraisals made by
In sum, the findings of Study 1 suggest that individuals supervisors or others. Self-appraisals are often submitted
who communicate assertively during the interview will be to the supervisor before the supervisor's evaluation of the
more likely to evoke interactionally fair behavior in the employee (Mohrman, Resnick-West, & Lawler, 1990).
form of consideration and justification from the decision Thus, because self-appraisal affords the employee the op-
maker. However, the generalizability of these findings to portunity to provide input into his or her supervisor's
a field setting is an issue. Thus, a second study was con- evaluation, it is expected to affect positively the employ-
ducted to examine further the relationship between the ee's attitudes toward the appraisal. However, self-apprais-
subordinate's communication style and the manager's in- als may not have the intended benefits. Research on the
teractionally fair behavior in the field by manipulating the benefits of self-appraisal is mixed and in some cases sug-
degree of assertive communication through training. The gests that self-appraisals can result in less acceptance and
736 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

satisfaction than when employees have no formal opportu- ficial effects maintained for as long as 1 year (Thurman,
nity to voice opinions (Roberson & Korsgaard, 1995; 1985).
Roberson et al., 1993). Given that self-ratings of perfor- Rich and Schroeder (1976) identified five basic assert-
mance tend be more favorable than supervisor ratings iveness training operations used in research. These in-
(Harris & Schaubroeck, 1988), self-appraisals may make cluded the use of instructions and modeling for response
disagreements salient without ensuring that they are ad- acquisition, behavioral practice-rehearsal for response re-
dressed or resolved (Korsgaard, 1996). production, self-feedback for response shaping, cognitive
Research on voice provides insight on the conditions restructuring for modification of dysfunctional self-state-
under which self-appraisal may have a positive effect on ments, and covert rehearsal for response transfer. Not all
attitudes. Specifically, research suggests that voice effects of these operations appear to be necessary to modify as-
are more likely to be observed when individuals perceive sertive behavior successfully. Research has demonstrated
that their opinions were considered by their managers that behavioral interventions alone tend to be as effective
(Shapiro, 1993; Tyler, 1987). Shapiro (1993) identified as cognitive or mixed cognitive-behavioral treatments
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

two dimensions of consideration: perceived listener re- (Hammen, Jacobs, Mayol, & Cochran, 1980). Other stud-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sponsiveness and potential decision influence. She pre- ies indicate that modeling and instruction may be suffi-
sented a model arguing that as long as either of these two cient for behavior change (Goddard, 1981; Hersen,
factors was present, voice would enhance perceptions of Eisler, & Miller, 1973; Rathus & Ruppert, 1973). In addi-
procedural justice. That is, voice will lead to perceptions tion, treatments have varied the amount of instruction
of justice when individuals believe that they have the op- and whether delivery is to individuals or groups. Group
portunity to influence the decision (i.e., voice is instru- treatments appear to be as effective as individual treat-

mental to influencing outcomes) or that the decision ments (Linehan, Walker, Bronheim, Haynes, & Yevzeroff,
1979). Treatments of varying length have been shown to
maker is interpersonally responsive to their concerns (i.e.,
be effective. Although 10- to 16-hr treatments are com-
voice has noninstrumental or intrinsic value). Shapiro
mon, in Lewittes and Bern (1983), a 3-hr treatment pro-
(1993) also found that individuals are more apt to believe
duced changes in participation in discussion groups. A
decision makers were interpersonally responsive to their
short 1-hr modeling and instructions intervention pro-
input when they provided adequate justification for the
duced changes in level of actualization in Goddard
decision.
(1981).
In sum, research on self-appraisal may have yielded
The present study used a short (1-hr) group-delivered
mixed results because critical conditions for the voice
assertiveness training intervention using modeling and in-
effect have not always been met. Showing consideration
struction to influence appraisee assertive behavior in the
for subordinate input or providing adequate justification
appraisal interview. Based on the results of Study 1, we
may ensure or enhance the effects of voice (Bies & Sha-
hypothesize the following:
piro, 1988; Shapiro, 1993; Tyler, 1987). Thus, interac-
tional justice, in the form of consideration of the subordi- Hypothesis I (Study 2): Individuals who have been
nate's self-appraisal and justification of the manager's trained to communicate assertively will perceive greater
interactionally fair behavior on the part of their managers
evaluation, in combination with self-appraisal, is likely to
than will individuals who have not been trained to commu-
result in perceptions of fairness and more positive atti- nicate assertively.
tudes toward the appraisal decision.
On the basis of the results of Study 1, we expect that As noted earlier, procedural and interactional justice
subordinates who are trained to be more assertive in their are known to influence attitudes toward the decision. Spe-
communication stylewho attempt to ensure that their cifically, individuals are apt to be more accepting of and
managers understand their point of view and who ask for satisfied with decisions when the decision maker enacts
explanations from their managersare likely to report the procedure in a fair manner (Korsgaard et al., 1995;
greater interactionally fair behavior on the part of their Sapienza & Korsgaard, 1996; Taylor et al., 1995). Given
managers during the appraisal interview, enhancing the that interactionally fair behavior has an impact on atti-
benefits of self-appraisal. tudes toward the decision, factors that increase managers'
propensities to be interactionally fair should have a posi-
Numerous studies have demonstrated that assertiveness
tive impact on these attitudes and perceptions. Specifi-
training can increase levels of assertive behaviors in a
cally, we expect training individuals to communicate as-
variety of two-person interactions (Lewittes & Bern,
sertively will be positively associated with a favorable
1983), job interviews (Austin & Grant, 1981), public
attitude toward the appraisal.
speaking situations (Grossberg, 1965), and group discus-
sions (Lewittes & Bern, 1983). Studies show both long Hypothesis 2a: Individuals who have been trained to
and short-term changes as a result of training, with bene- communicate assertively will have a more favorable attitude
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 737

toward the appraisal than will individuals who have not self-appraisal and training. Consequently, we omitted the condi-
been trained to communicate assertively. tion of no self-appraisal-training.
The sample was drawn from a group of 166 employees who
Interactionally fair behavior is also likely to influence worked in the accounts payable function. Employees in the ac-
an employee's relationship with the decision maker counts payable function were organized into 14 teams. The
(Lind & Tyler, 1988). Research shows that individuals are average respondent was 30 years old and had worked for the
likely to trust the decision maker more to the extent that firm for 1.5 years. Roughly two thirds of the sample had at least
principles of interactional justice are followed (Folger & 1 year of postsecondary education, and 85% of the respondents
Konovsky, 1989; Korsgaard et al., 1995; Sapienza & Kor- were female.
Initially, 128 employees agreed to participate in the study (42
sgaard, 1996). Consequently, we expect that assertive
in the training condition, 41 in the self-appraisal condition, and
communication training, which should be associated with
45 in the control condition), resulting in an initial response
greater interactionally fair behavior on the part of the
rate of 77%. In this first phase of the study, which occurred
manager, should he positively related to the employee's
approximately 2 weeks before the appraisals, all 128 partici-
trust in the manager. pants attended a training session in which assertiveness training
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and self-appraisal were manipulated. The second phase, which


Hypothesis 2b: Individuals who have been trained to
occurred after employees had received their performance re-
communicate assertively will be more trusting of their man-
agers than will individuals who have not been trained to views, was a survey that was distributed to the 128 participants.
communicate assertively. Eighty-four of the surveys were returned, resulting in a response
rate of 66%. Identifying information, necessary for determining
Similarly, theory suggests that the extent to which an the respondent's condition, was not included in 12 surveys,
individual is treated in an interactionally fair manner by resulting in a working sample of 72 (85%), with 25 in the
assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal condition, 21 in the
the agents of an organization should strengthen the indi-
formal self-appraisal condition, and 26 in the informal self-
vidual's relationship with that organization (Tyler & Lind,
appraisal condition. Response rates within conditions did not
1992). Research supports this link in work organizations
differ significantly. Further, there were no significant differences
in that interactionally fair behavior is positively associated
between the initial 128 participants, the 84 respondents to the
with commitment to the organization (Folger & Konov- survey, and the 72 matched respondents on demographic vari-
sky, 1989; Konovsky & Cropanzano, 1991; Korsgaard et ables (age, gender, education, and tenure). There were also no
al., 1995; Schaubroeck et al., 1994). Therefore, assertive differences between the sample of 84 and the sample of 72 on
communication training, because it is expected to be posi- any of the dependent variables.
tively associated with interactionally fair behavior exhib- Treatments. The intervention took place in the context of
ited by the manager, should be positively related to em- the firm's performance appraisal system. The appraisal form
ployees' organizational commitment. had two parts. The first part consisted of an objective, produc-
tion-based evaluation. The second part consisted of eight dimen-
Hypothesis 2c: Individuals who have been trained to sion ratings, each with a 4-point scale. Ratings were done on a
communicate assertively will be more committed to the quarterly basis; pay increases and bonuses were based on ratings
organization than will individuals who have not been in the fourth quarter. The appraisal period we investigated was
trained to communicate assertively. a non-pay increase time, although the results would count to-
ward the fourth-quarter rating. Participants in all three condi-
Method tions attended performance review training sessions that were
conducted by the experimenter and a member of the organiza-
Design and sample. This study was conducted at a large tion's human resource (HR) staff. Participants attended the ses-
retailing firm in the context of employees' quarterly perfor- sions in groups of 10 to 20. The sessions lasted roughly 1 hr
mance appraisals. The study was quasiexperimental, using a and were conducted approximately 2 weeks before the apprais-
nonequivalent groups single-factor design in which we manipu- als were scheduled to take place.
lated assertiveness through training. Six groups of volunteer In the assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal condition,
employees were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: participants were instructed on how to conduct a self-appraisal.
assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal; formal self-ap- In addition, participants in this condition were trained to commu-
praisal; and informal self-appraisal. We did not use a design in nicate assertively. First, the HR staff member reviewed the perfor-
which self-appraisal and training were fully crossed, because mance appraisal form with participants. The experimenter then
this would have resulted in a condition in which people were instructed participants how to prepare for the performance review.
trained but did not formally self-appraise. Because the training Participants were first given a self-appraisal form for rating their
was designed to enhance employees' assertiveness in discussing own performance on the same dimensions included in the manag-
self-opinions about their performance, we believed that the train- ers' form. Participants were told to rate their performance and to
ing would not make sense to employees if they were not also provide detailed behavioral reasons for their ratings. Examples
instructed on how to self-appraise. Further, it is possible that were used for illustrations. Subsequent parts of the self-appraisal
the training would have caused individuals to discuss their self- form asked for a listing of strengths and weaknesses and for
appraisals, which would have confounded the two factors of performance objectives for the coming quarter. Participants were
738 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

instructed to complete the self-appraisal form and to give it to participant. Five items adapted from previous research were
their superiors at least 1 week before the review discussion was used (Lind, Lissak, & Conlon, 1983; Tyler, 1987). Examples of
scheduled to take place. Participants were informed that their items are ' 'To what extent did your leader take steps to really
superiors were expecting to receive a copy of the self-appraisal, understand your views on your own performance?" and "To
which would be placed in their personnel file. what extent did your leader explain the reasons for the evalua-
In addition, participants received training on how to partici- tion you received?''
pate in the review discussion. The content of the training was Attitude toward the appraisal was measured by five items
taken from sources on assertive communication (Bolton, 1979; adapted from previous research (Giles & Mossholder, 1990;
Zuker, 1984) and focused on three main learning points: (a) Roberson et al., 1993) assessing agreement and satisfaction with
Give information by stating your self-appraisal rating and the the appraisal. Sample items are ' 'I agree with my final rating,''
reasons for your rating; (b) obtain information by asking your and "I feel good about the way the review was conducted."
superior to provide a justification for his or her ratings and for Trust in manager was measured by a four-item scale adapted
examples, especially if there is disagreement; and (c) use body from Roberts and O'Reilly (1974) assessing the participant's
language by maintaining eye contact, sitting up straight, leaning trust in his or her supervisor. An example is "I trust my leader."
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

forward, and taking notes. After a brief lecture on the three Organizational commitment was measured by Porter and col-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

learning points, participants watched two scripted role-plays: leagues' (Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979; Porter & Smith,
one showing assertive behavior in the review discussion and 1970) nine-item measure of commitment. A sample item is "I
another showing nonassertive behavior. They then participated really care about the fate of my company.'' In addition, manag-
in an exercise to identify the assertive and nonassertive behavior. ers' performance appraisal ratings of subordinates were ob-
In the formal self-appraisal condition, participants were in- tained on a subset of the sample (n = 41). Managers made four
structed on how to conduct a self-appraisal. As in the first treat- ratings: team effectiveness, individual job effectiveness, team
ment, the HR staff member first reviewed the performance ap- skills, and overall effectiveness. These ratings, made on a 5-
praisal form. The experimenter then instructed participants how point scale (1 = outstanding; 5 = not effective), were averaged
to complete the self-appraisal form, using the same instructions to form an overall performance ratings measure.
and illustrations given to participants in the self-appraisal-train- Two manipulation check procedures were administered. The
ing condition. Participants were instructed to complete the self- first check, which assessed the efficacy of training, consisted of
appraisal form and to give it to their superiors 1 week before a prepost test of knowledge of the training learning objectives.
the review discussion was scheduled to take place. Participants The test consisted of four open-ended items; each question was
were informed that their superiors were expecting to receive a a problem situation pertaining to assertive communication in
copy of the self-appraisal, which would be placed in their per- the appraisal process. A sample question was "During the dis-
sonnel file. cussion, your leader says: T rated you as not effective on flexi-
Self-appraisal can do more than create the opportunity for bility. It's something you really need to work on.' What do you
voice; it can increase the salience of self-opinions. Research say?'' Each item was coded for correctness on a 10-point scale
suggests that, apart from any voice effects, self-opinions affect by a rater who was unaware of whether the form was pretest
reactions to feedback from others (Blakely, 1993; Cawley & or posttest. The rater was trained against another rater on a
Levy, 1992; Korsgaard, 1996; Roberson & Korsgaard, 1995). pilot sample, and the resulting interrater reliability, based on an
Therefore, in the informal self-appraisal condition, we attempted intraclass correlation (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979), was estimated
to hold constant the salience of self-opinions so that any ob- at .85. Given the test's potential reactivity (e.g., participants in
served relationships with self-appraisal would be attributable the self-appraisal and control conditions could learn from the
to voice only. This condition involved informal, private self- test), it was only administered in the training condition. The
appraisal to make self-opinions salient without creating expecta- second manipulation check, which assessed the efficacy of self-
tions of voice or influence. As in the prior two conditions, the appraisal, consisted of one item in which respondents indicated
HR staff member reviewed the performance appraisal form. The whether they had completed a formal self-appraisal (yes-no).
experimenter then introduced the self-appraisal form as an aid Means, standard deviations, correlations, and reliabilities for
to help participants prepare for the review discussion. It was all variables are listed in Table 1. Cell means and standard
suggested that each participant privately complete the form a deviations are listed in Table 2. As noted in Table 1, the depen-
few days before his or her review discussion as a way to think dent measuresinteractionally fair behavior, attitude toward
about the things the participant would like to discuss with his the appraisal, trust in the manager, and organizational commit-
or her superior. mentwere all strongly correlated.
Measures. All measured variables were obtained from the
survey administered after the performance reviews were con-
ducted. Excepting demographic items and the manipulation Results
check, all items included 5-point, Likert-type scales. The train-
A dependent t test of the knowledge test results revealed
ing manipulation focused on two components of interactional
justice: consideration of input and justification. Therefore, inter-
that participants in the assertiveness training-formal self-
actionally fair behavior was operationalized by the extent that appraisal condition were significantly more knowledge-
the participant believed that his or her supervisor showed consid- able about the principles of assertive communication after
eration of the participant's input into the evaluation and pro- the training than they were before the training, f(35) =
vided adequate justification of the supervisor's evaluation of the 6.61, p < .05; pretest M = 4.52, SD = 1.61; posttest M
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 739

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, Correlations, and Reliabilities for Variables in Study 1

Variable M SD

1. Interactionally fair
behavior 3.64 1.03 (.94)
2. Attitude toward
appraisal 3.85 0.97 .69* (.92)
3. Trust in manager 3.85 0.92 .74* .67* (.89)
4. Organizational
commitment 3.55 0.77 .45* .40* .47* (.89)
5. Performance rating9 1.98 0.48 -.15 -.24 -.17 -.05 (.73)
6. Gender* 0.13 0.34 -.11 -.12 -.13 -.17 .16

7. Age 37.58 13.15 .05 -.05 -.07 .08 -.08 -.19
8. Education (years) 13.50 1.92 .00 .02 .08 .09 -.36 -.11 .17
9. Tenure 1.59 0.59 -.16 -.23* -.32* -.13 -.27 .04 .31* .20
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. Reliabilities are reported in parentheses along the diagonal.


a
Performance ratings were only available for 41 participants; correlations with this measure were estimated
on 41 participants. b Gender was coded: 1 male; 0 - female.
*p < .05.

= 6.48, SD = 1.77. This finding supports the efficacy of tionship between assertive communication training and
the training manipulation. Analysis of the self-appraisal subsequent attitudes, were each tested by one-way
manipulation check revealed a significant association be- ANO'VAs and a priori contrasts comparing the asser-
tween condition and whether respondents completed a tiveness trainingformal self-appraisal condition to the
formal self-appraisal, * 2 (2, N = 82) = 40.79, p < .05; remaining conditions. As noted in Table 3, the contrast
a greater proportion of respondents in the two appraisal comparing training to the other two conditions was sig-
conditions indicated that they had formally self-appraised nificant for attitude toward the appraisal. This finding
(assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal = .96; for- indicates that individuals who were trained to communi-
mal self-appraisal = .86; informal self-appraisal = .09). cate assertively are more accepting of and more satisfied
All hypotheses were tested by a one-way analysis of with their manager's performance evaluation decision.
variance (ANOVA) and an a priori contrast comparing Hypothesis 2a was, therefore, supported. The training con-
the assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal condition trast for trust in the manager was also significant, indicat-
to the formal self-appraisal and informal self-appraisal ing that training for assertive communication is associated
conditions, which are reported in Table 3. Hypothesis 1 with greater trust. Thus, Hypothesis 2b was supported.
stated that individuals who are trained to communicate The training contrast for organizational commitment (Hy-
assertively will perceive greater interactionally fair behav- pothesis 2c) was not significant. In sum, two of the three
ior on the part of their managers than will individuals hypotheses concerning attitudes were supported.
who are not trained. The contrast comparing assertiveness
training-formal self-appraisal to the other two conditions Discussion
was not significant, F( I , 71) = 3.05, p = .085. The purpose of Study 2 was to extend the findings
Hypotheses 2a through 2c, which concerned the rela- of Study 1 by investigating the relationships of assertive

Table 2
Cell Means and Standard Deviations for Dependent Variables in Study 2

Assertiveness
training-formal Formal Informal
self-appraisal self-appraisal self-appraisal
(n = 25) (n = 21) (n = 26)

Variable M SD M SD M SD

Interactionally fair behavior 4.01 0.82 3.70 1.08 3.45 1.10


Attitude toward appraisal 4.26' 0.57 3.99 0.83 3.55 1.15
Trust in manager 4.26" 0.77 3.84 1.05 3.65 0.88
Organizational commitment 3.73 0.83 3.65 0.63 3.44 0.80

a
Assertiveness training-formal self-appraisal condition differed significantly from the other two conditions.
740 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

Table 3 tudes. For example, it is possible that assertive communi-


Significance Tests for All Dependent Variables in Study 2 cation training may have cued participants to attend to
consideration and justification behaviors exhibited by the
Dependent variable and effect <y manager. Similarly, training may have cued participants
Interactionally fair behavior to expect a more favorable appraisal. However, high ex-
Treatment 1.96 2 .05
Training vs. no training 3.05** 1 .04
pectations alone would not likely produce favorable atti-
Attitude toward appraisal tudes, for unless the appraisal review were in actuality
Treatment 4.16* 2 .11 more favorable, unmet expectations would likely result
Training vs. no training 4.90* 1 .06
Trust in manager
in more negative reactions (Wanous, Poland, Premack, &
Treatment 2.90** 2 .08 Davis, 1992). Another possibility is that the assertiveness
Training vs. no training 4.98* 1 .07 training enabled participants to influence the actual out-
Organizational commitment
Treatment 0.96 2 .03
comes of the appraisal. However, we obtained managers'
Training vs. no training 0.97 1 .01 performance ratings on a subset of the sample (n = 41),
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

which revealed no significant differences in the favorable-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

**p<.10.
ness of ratings between conditions.
It is also possible that the training findings resulted
solely from changes in the participant's behavior rather
communication training to fairness perceptions and atti- than increased consideration and justification from the
tudes in a field setting. We expected assertive communica- manager. First, the findings could have resulted merely
tion training to be positively associated with perceptions from an increased level of talking on the part of the subor-
of interactionally fair behavior during the appraisal review dinate. This explanation seems unlikely, however, in light
and, consequently, be positively associated with a favor- of prior research indicating that the amount of talking
able attitude toward the appraisal, trust in the manager, itself does not predict employees' reactions beyond the
and organizational commitment. The findings provided impact of the quality of the interaction (Burke, Weitzel, &
support for two of the four hypotheses. Specifically, sig- Weir, 1978; Greller, 1975, 1978; Wexely, Singh, & Yukl,
nificant findings were obtained for the relationship be- 1973). Second, it is possible that training did affect assert-
tween training and attitude toward the appraisal and be- ive behavior, and this led to increased voice and appeal,
tween training and trust in the manager. In both cases, enhancing perceptions of procedural justice regardless of
participants in the training-self-appraisal condition re- the supervisor's behavior.
acted significantly more favorably than did participants Moreover, because participants were not randomly as-
in the formal and informal self-appraisal conditions. signed to condition, it is possible that the findings are
The results did not support the effect of training on attributable to preexisting differences between conditions.
perceptions of the manager's behavior, a finding that may Analyses with available demographic data indicated that
have reflected the relatively brief intervention used in this age, tenure, and level of education did not account for
study. Although our assessment of training efficacy indi- variance attributed to the training manipulation. Gender,
cated that participants' knowledge of assertive communi- however, may have played a role in the results of Study
cation significantly increased, we cannot ascertain 2, for males were unequally distributed across conditions.
whether such knowledge fully transferred to behavior on Because the overall proportion of males in the sample
the job. Consequently, the training could have produced was extremely small, the extreme skewness in the distribu-
relatively weak changes in participants' communication tion of males prohibited us from performing a valid and
styles. Indeed, because the pre-post knowledge test was sufficiently powerful test of the effects of gender (Co-
administered only to those in the formal training condi- hen & Cohen, 1983; Lord, 1967, 1970). Consequently,
tion, it is possible that differences attributable to training it is not clear from the current findings how assertive
were due in part to learning derived through testing. Fur- communication would relate to interactional fairness in a
ther, given that the study was conducted in a short time more diverse sample. For example, research indicates that
frame, additional research is needed to determine whether men and women often differ in their self-perceptions of
assertive communication training would have lasting assertiveness (Bridges, Sanderman, Breukers, Ranchor, &
effects. Arrindell, 1991; Cowen, Wilcox, & Nykodym, 1990);
Although Study 2 provides encouraging implications thus, men and women may respond differently to training
for enhancing the effectiveness of performance appraisals to increase assertiveness. Research also suggests that the
through training, the findings should be interpreted with gender of the actor may influence reactions of the observer
caution. First, because we did not directly measure subor- to the actor's assertive behavior (Wilson & Gallois,
dinates' behavior, it is unclear how training subordinates 1985), particularly in mixed dyads (Mathison, 1986).
may affect managers' behavior and subordinates' atti- Thus, a manager's response to an assertive employee may
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 741

depend on the gender of both the manager and the em- present investigation suggests that subordinates them-
ployee. However, the findings from Study 1, which pro- selves can be trained to influence their interactions with
vided a reasonably powerful test of gender effects, suggest management. Interventions targeted at subordinates may
that the gender of the appraiser was not relevant: partici- thus affect justice perceptions without the danger of pro-
pants' gender did not moderate the impact of assertive moting disingenuous attempts at fair behavior on the part
communication. of management.
The findings also have practical value for implementing
self-appraisals in showing that training employees to com-
General Discussion
municate assertively can enhance the effectiveness of such
To date, the justice literature has largely taken a one- techniques. The results suggest that such training may
sided view of the dynamics of social exchange relation- be critical when self-appraisals are incorporated into the
ships, such as managersubordinate dyads, by focusing appraisal system: Training had a significant, value-added
on the manager's role and neglecting the impact of the relationship to attitudes toward the appraisal and the man-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

subordinate's behavior on fair exchanges. In contrast, the ager, beyond the impact of the opportunity for voice
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

present investigation sought to examine the impact of through self-appraisal.1 Despite mixed empirical support
the subordinate's actions on the manager's propensity to (e.g., Roberson et al., 1993), self-appraisal is a common
engage in interactionally fair behavior. Together, the two technique used to involve employees in the appraisal pro-
studies provide evidence of the importance of the subordi- cess. The results of this study suggest that additional train-
nate's role. In Study 1, we found that appraisees can affect ing may be needed to ensure that this technique has its
the appraiser's actions, for appraisee assertive behavior intended effects.
resulted in greater interactionally fair behavior on the part Little is known about why individuals opt to act in a
of the appraiser. Study 2 supported the generalizability of fair manner. Research has examined individual differences
this result to the field: Subordinates who were trained to in fairness (Bretz & Judge, 1994) and factors related to
communicate assertively in the appraisal review reacted the decision context (Folger, 1993). Our findings suggest
more favorably to their appraisals than did those who a third factor: the behavior of the individuals affected by
were not trained. the decision. More broadly, our findings suggest that to
Our findings suggest that the processes underlying jus- understand justice in organizations fully, we need to exam-
tice effects may involve reciprocal relationships between ine how each party affects the exchange process. Our
the manager's fairness in decision making and the percep- investigation is a first step in exploring the dynamics of
tions, attitudes, and behaviors of the subordinate. This fair exchanges between managers and their subordinates,
conclusion is consistent with Folger's (Folger, 1993; Fol- and we hope it will stimulate more research in this direc-
ger & Skarlicki, 1998) view of the determinants of fair tion. Although we know from Study 1 that an appraisee's
behavior. He argued that a decision maker's tendency to behavior can affect the actions of an appraiser, it is less
avoid communicating fairly with a subordinate may be clear from Study 2 whether and how assertiveness training
driven by the anticipated reaction of the subordinate. Sup- changed the appraisee's behavior. Thus, further examina-
port for the view that subordinates' behavior can affect tion of the effects of assertiveness training is warranted.
decision makers' fairness can also be derived from leader- Such studies should measure more directly the differences
ship theory. Specifically, research on leader-member ex- in subordinate actions during the appraisal either through
change shows that, as newcomers, employee's initial re- observation or appraiser reports. This would strengthen
sponses to role expectations shape the manager's impres- the link between training and subordinate behavior. In
sions of the employee's capability (Graen & Scandura, addition, the effects of gender warrant further investiga-
1987). On the basis of these impressions, the manager tion. Future research should include an examination of the
provides greater latitude (in the language of justice theory, effects of gender on the impact of assertiveness training as
decision control) to employees (Graen & Uhl-Bien,
1995).
1
Current justice interventions tend to be "top-down," To provide further evidence that the findings were due to
typically involving a direct manipulation of the manager's the value-added impact of training, we tested contrasts compar-
ing the two formal self-appraisal conditions (assertiveness train-
behavior (Schaubroeck et al., 1994). These efforts are
ing-formal self-appraisal and formal self-appraisal) to the in-
valuable, for they demonstrate that managers are capable
formal condition. This contrast was significant for attitude to-
of acting in a fair manner. However, this approach can
ward the appraisal but not for interactionally fair behavior or
cultivate a belief that managers merely need to display a trust in the manager. These findings suggest that, although self-
"veneer of fairness" (Greenberg, 1990, p. 139) to gain appraisal played a role in the effects on appraisal attitudes,
acceptance of their decisions. Such tactics may backfire the observed relationships among training, interactionally fair
if they are perceived as insincere (Greenberg, 1990). The behavior, and trust cannot be explained by self-appraisal.
742 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

well as the impact of gender differences in subordinate- justice in human resource management. Hillsdale, NJ:
superior dyads on responses to assertiveness. Future re- Erlbaum.
search should also investigate how the subordinate influ- Epstein, N., & Jackson, E. (1978). An outcome study of short-
term communication training with married couples. Journal
ences the perceptions and beliefs of the manager. Finally,
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 207-212.
research is also needed on managerial attributes, such as
Folger, R. (1993, August). The "Churchillparadox"in manag-
the manager's skill in handling conflict, that may qualify
ing hard times. Paper presented at the 1993 Academy of Man-
how managers react to subordinate communication. Such agement Meetings, Atlanta, GA.
efforts will help develop a comprehensive view of the Folger, R., & Bies, R. J. (1989). Managerial responsibilities
processes underlying fair behavior and a broader theory and procedural justice. Employee Responsibilities and Rights
of justice. Journal, 2, 79-90.
Folger, R., & Konovsky, M. K. (1989). Effects of procedural
and distributive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions.
References
Academy of Management Journal, 32, 115-130.
Alberti, R. E., & Emmons, M. L. (1970). Your perfect right: A Folger, R., Konovsky, M. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1992). A due
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

guide to assertive behavior. San Luis Obispo, CA: Impact. process metaphor for performance appraisal. In B. M. Staw &
Austin, M. E, & Grant, T. N. (1981). Interview training for L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior
college students disadvantages! in the labor market: Compari- (Vol. 14, pp. 129-177). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
son of five instructional techniques. Journal of Counseling Folger, R., & Lewis, D. (1993). Self appraisal and fairness in
Psychology, 28, 72-75. evaluations. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the work-
Bies, R. I, & Shapiro, D. L. (1988). Voice and justification: place: Approaching justice in human resource, management
Their influence on procedural fairness judgments. Academy (pp. 107-131). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
of Management Journal, 31, 676-685. Folger, R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (1998). Tough times make tough
Blakely, G. L. (1993). The effects of performance rating dis- bosses: Managerial distancing as a function of layoff blame.
crepancies on supervisors and subordinates. Organizational Academy of Management Journal, 41, 79-87.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 57-80. Giles, W. F., & Mossholder, K. W. (1990). Employee reactions
Bolton, R. (1979). People skills: Haw to assert yourself, listen to contextual and session components of performance ap-
to others, and resolve conflicts. New York: Simon & Schuster. praisal, Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 371-377.
Bretz, R. D., & Judge, T. A. (1994). The role of human resource Gilliland, S. W. (1994). Effects of procedural and distributive
systems in job applicant decision processes. Journal of Man- justice on reactions to a selection system. Journal of Applied
agement, 20, 531-551. Psychology, 79, 691-701.
Bridges, K. R., Sanderman, R., Breukers, P., Rancher, A., & Goddard, R. C. (1981). Increase in assertiveness and actualiza-
Arrindell, W. A. (1991). Sex differences in assertiveness on tion as a function of didactic training. Journal of Counseling
the U.S. version of the scale for interpersonal behavior (SIB). Psychology, 28, 279-287.
Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1239-1243. Gordon, S., & Waldo, M. (1984). The effects of assertive train-
Brockner, J., & Wiesenfeld, B. M. (1996). An integrative frame- ing on couples' relationships. American Journal of Family
work for explaining reactions to decisions: Interactive effects Therapy, 12, 73-77.
of outcomes and procedures. Psychological Bulletin, 120, Graen, G. B., & Scandura, T. B. (1987). Toward a psychology
189-208. of dyadic organizing. In B. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
Burke, R. J., Weitzel, W., & Weir, T. (1978). Characteristics Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 175-208).
of effective employee performance review and development Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
interviews: Replication and extension. Personnel Psychology, Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based ap-
31, 903-919. proach to leadership: Development of leader-member ex-
Cawley, B. D., & Levy, P. E. (1992). Appraisal discrepancies change (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying
and feedback reactions: Afield investigation. Paper presented a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly,
at the 7th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial 6, 219-247.
and Organizational Psychology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Greenberg, J. (1986). Determinants of perceived fairness of
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/ performance evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). 340-342.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Greenberg, J. (1987). Using diaries to promote procedural jus-
Cooley, M. L., & Hollandsworth, J. G. (1977). A strategy for tice in performance appraisals. Social Justice Research, I,
teaching verbal content of assertive responses. In R. E. Alberti 219-234.
(Ed.), Assertivene-ss: Innovations, applications, issues (pp. Greenberg, J. (1990). Looking fair vs. being fair: Managing
75-82). San Luis Obispo, CA: Impact. impressions of organizational justice. In B. M. Staw & L. L.
Cowen, K. M., Wilcox, J. R., & Nykodym, N. (1990). A com- Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol.
parative analysis of female-male communication style as a 12, pp. 111-157). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
function of organizational level. Communications, 15, 291- Greenberg, J. (1993). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal
309. and informational classes of organizational justice. In R. Cro-
Cropanzano, R. (1993). Justice in the workplace: Approaching panzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching justice
WHAT MOTIVATES FAIRNESS? 743

in human resource management (pp. 79-103). Hillsdale, NJ: Linehan, M. M., Walker, R. O., Bronheim, S., Haynes, K. R, &
Erlbaum. Yevzeroff, H. (1979). Group versus individual assertion train-
Greenberg, J. (1994). Using socially fair treatment to promote ing. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 47,
acceptance of a work site smoking ban. Journal of Applied 1000-1002.
Psychology, 79, 288-297. Lord, F. M. (1967). A paradox in the interpretation of group
Greller, M. (1975). Subordinate participation and reactions to comparisons. Psychological Bulletin, 68, 304-305.
the appraisal interview. Journal of Psychology, 60, 544-549. Lord, P.M. (1970). Statistical adjustments when comparing
Greller, M. (1978). The nature of subordinate participation in preexisting groups. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 336-337.
the appraisal interview. Academy of Management Journal, Mathison, D. L. (1986). Sex differences in the perception of
21, 646-658. assertiveness among female managers. Journal of Social Psy-
Grossberg, 3. (1965). Successful behavior therapy in a case of chology, 126, 599-606.
speech phobia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Miceli, M. P., Jung, L, Near, 1. P., & Greenberger, D. B. (1991).
30, 285-288. Predictors and outcomes of reactions to pay-for-performance
Hammen, C. L., Jacobs, M., Mayol, A., & Cochran, S. D. plans. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 508-521.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1980). Dysfunctional cognitions and the effectiveness of Mohrman, A. M., Jr., Resnick-West, S. M., & Lawler, E. E. HI
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

skills and cognitive behavioral assertion training. Journal of (1990). Designing performance appraisal systems: Aligning
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 685-695. appraisals and organizational realities. San Francisco, CA:
Harris, M. M., & Schaubroeck, J. (1988). A meta-analysis of Jossey-Bass.
self-supervisor, self-peer, and peer-supervisor ratings. Person- Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. (1979). The mea-
nel Psychology, 41, 43-62. surement of organizational commitment. Journal of Voca-
Hayes, J. (1991). Interpersonal skills: Goal-directed behavior tional Behavior, 14, 224-247.
at work. London: Harper Collins Academic. Porter, L. W., & Smith, J. F. (1970). The etiology of organiza-
Hersen, M., Eisler, R. M., & Miller, P. M. (1973). Development tional commitment. Unpublished manuscript, University of
of assertive responses: Clinical measurement and research California, Irvine.
considerations. Behavior Research and Therapy, 11, 505- Rabin, C., & Zelner, D. (1992). The role of assertiveness in
521. clarifying roles and strengthening job satisfaction of social
Hollandsworth, J. G., & Cooley, M. L. (1978). Provoking anger workers in multidisciplinary mental health settings. British
and gaining compliance with assertive vs. aggressive re- Journal of Social Work, 22, 17-32.
sponses. Behavior Therapy, 9, 640-646. Rakos, R. P. (1986). Asserting and confronting. In O. Hargie
Hull, D. B., & Schroeder, H. E. (1979). Some interpersonal (Ed.), A handbook of communication skills (pp. 407-440).
effects of assertion, nonassertion, and aggression. Behavior London: Croom Helm.
Therapy, 10. 20-28. Rathus, S. A., & Ruppert, C. A. (1973). Assertion training in
Konovsky, M. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1991). Perceived fairness the secondary school and the college. Adolescence, 8, 257-
of employee drug testing as a predictor of employee attitudes 264.
and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, Rich, A. R., & Schroeder, H. E. (1976). Research issues in
698-707. assertiveness training. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1084-
Korsgaard, M. A. (1996). The impact of self-appraisals on reac- 1096.
tions to feedback from others: The role of self-consistency Roberson, L., & Korsgaard, M. A. (1995, May). Does public
and self-enhancement concerns. Journal of Organizational self-appraisal affect reactions to feedback? Presented at the
Behavior, 17, 301-311. Conference for Society for Industrial/Organizational Psy-
Korsgaard, M. A., & Roberson, L. (1995). Procedural justice chology, Orlando, FL.
in performance evaluation. Journal of Management, 21, 657- Roberson, L., Torkel, S. J., Korsgaard, M. A., Klein, D., Did-
699. dams, M., & Cayer, M. (1993). Self-appraisal and perceptions
Korsgaard, M. A., Schweiger, D. M., & Sapienza, H. J. (1995). of the appraisal discussion: A field experiment. Journal of
The role of procedural justice in building commitment, attach- Organizational Behavior, 14, 129-142.
ment, and trust in strategic decision-making teams. Academy Roberts, K. H., & O'Reilly, C. A. (1974). Measuring organiza-
of Management Journal, 38, 60-84. tional communication. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59,
Leung, K., & Li, W. (1990). Psychological mechanisms of pro- 321-326.
cess-control effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 613- Sapienza, H. J., & Korsgaard, M. A. (1996). Managing investor
620. relations: The impact of procedural justice in establishing and
Lewittes, H. J., & Bern, S. L. (1983). Training women to be sustaining investor support. Academy of Management Jour-
more assertive in mixed-sex task-oriented discussions. Sex nal, 39, 544-574.
Roles, 9, 581-596. Schaubroeck, J., May, D. R., & Brown, F. W. (1994). Proce-
Lind, E. A., Lissak, R. I., & Conlon, D. E. (1983). Decision dural justice explanations and employee reactions to eco-
control and process control effects on procedural fairness nomic hardship: A field experiment. Journal of Applied Psy-
judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 338- chology, 79, 455-460.
350. Shapiro, D. (1993). Reconciling theoretical differences among
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. (1988). The social psychology of proce- procedural justice research by re-evaluating what it means to
dural justice. New "Vbrk: Plenum. have one's views "considered": Implications for third party
744 KORSGAARD, ROBERSON, AND RYMPH

managers. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115-191). New Mwk: Aca-
Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. demic Press.
51-78). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tyler, T. R., Rasinski, K. A., & Spodick, N. (1985). Influence
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: of voice on satisfaction with leaders: Exploring the meaning
Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, of process control. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
420-428. chology, 48, 72-81.
Taylor, M. S., Tracy, K. B., Renard, M. K., Harrison, J. K., & Wanous, J. P., Poland, T. D., Premack, S. L., Davis, K. S.
Carroll, S. J. (1995). Due process in performance appraisal: (1992). The effects of met expectations on newcomer atti-
A quasi-experiment in procedural justice. Administrative Sci- tudes and behaviors: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of
ence Quarterly, 40, 495-523. Applied Psychology, 77, 288-297.
Thurman, C. W. (1985). Effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral Wexely, K. N., Singh, J. P., & Yukl, G. A. (1973). Subordinate
treatments in reducing type A behavior among university fac- personality as a moderator of the effects of participation in
ulty. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 74-83. three types of appraisal interviews. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 58, 54-59.
Tyler, T. R. (1987). Conditions leading to value expressive ef-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Wilson, L. K., & Gallois, C. (1985). Perceptions of assertive


fects in judgments of procedural justice: A test of four models.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

behavior: Sex combination, role appropriateness, and mes-


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 333-344.
sage type. Sex Roles, 12, 125-141.
Tyler, T. R., & Bies, R. J. (1990). Beyond formal procedures:
Zuker, E. (1984). Mastering assertiveness skills. New York:
The interpersonal context of procedural justice. In J. Carroll
AMACOM.
(Ed.), Applied social psychology and organizational settings
(pp. 77-98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Received July 24, 1996
Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of author- Revision received April 10, 1998
ity in groups. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental Accepted April 10, 1998

New Editors Appointed, 2000-2005

The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Associa-


tion announces the appointment of three new editors for 6-year terms beginning in 2000.

As of January 1,1999, manuscripts should be directed as follows:

For Experimental and Clinical Psy chopharmacology, submit manuscripts to


Warren K. Bickel, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, 38
Fletcher Place, Burlington, VT 05401-1419.

For the Journal of Counseling Psychology, submit manuscripts to Jo-Ida C.


Hansen, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East
River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344.

For the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-


mance, submit manuscripts to David A. Rosenbaum, PhD, Department of Psy-
chology, Pennsylvania State University, 642 Moore Building, University Park,
PA 16802-3104.

Manuscript submission patterns make the precise date of completion of the 1999 volumes
uncertain. Current editors, Charles R. Schuster, PhD; Clara E. Hill, PhD; and Thomas H.
Carr, PhD, respectively, will receive and consider manuscripts through December 31,
1998. Should 1999 volumes be completed before that date, manuscripts will be redirected
to the new editors for consideration in 2000 volumes.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen