Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

FOCUS: LEARNING AS A

GEOGRAPHICAL PROCESS
Introduction: Learning as a Geographical Process

Dragos Simandan
Brock University

In this introduction to the Focus Section Learning as a Geographical Process, I provide a context for the
four articles that follow, by means of (1) making explicit the threefold rationale for this initiative; (2) relating
this initiative with previous geographical scholarship on the problematic of learning; (3) highlighting the
significance of the self-referential character of our work; (4) providing a brief outline of the articles that
follow; and (5) pointing out the important fact that both learning and geographical process constitute
semantically rich categories and that relating the two involves a many-to-many type of logical mapping.
Key Words: geographical process, learning, many-to-many mapping, self-reference.

En esta introduccion al tema de El aprendizaje como un proceso geografico de la Seccion Focus contribuyo
un contexto para los cuatro artculos que siguen, mediante las siguientes acciones: (1) haciendo explcita la triple
racionalidad de esta iniciativa; (2) relacionando la iniciativa con la erudicion geografica precedente relacionada
con la problematica del aprendizaje; (3) destacando la significacion del caracter auto-referencial de nuestro
trabajo; (4) suministrando un breve esquema de los artculos de la Seccion; y (5) senalando el hecho notable de
que tanto aprendizaje como proceso geografico constituyen unas categoras sistematicamente ricas y que
para relacionarlas entre s se involucra una logica cartografica del tipo muchos-a-muchos [many-to-many].
Palabras clave: proceso geografico, aprendizaje, mapeo de muchos-a-muchos, auto-referencia.

T he rationale for this Focus Section is


threefold. First, by embracing an instru-
mentalist epistemological viewpoint, we aim to
strumentalist and pluralistic flavor and espouse
the implicit belief that there is no one cor-
rect way to think about learning. Instead, in
explore the potential usefulness of learning as the spirit of pluralistic epistemology (Preston
a geographical process as a way of thinking. 2005), we operate from the belief that the best
Can we learn something new and interesting way to understand a given topic is to tackle
about learning if we think of it as a geograph- it from a diversity of perspectives, each con-
ical process? What problems is the learning as ceived as a constellation of epistemic gains and
a geographical process perspective particularly losses. By not asking the question that a real-
suited to address? Enlightened by looking at ist epistemologist would askis learning as a
learning in geographical terms, what new phe- geographical process the correct way to think
nomena might we get the chance to observe? about learning?we cast our work as a use-
Note that these questions share a distinctly in- ful application of geographical reasoning to the

The Professional Geographer, 65(3) 2013, pages 363368 Copyright  C 2013 Crown copyright.
Initial submission, June 2011; revised submissions, October and December 2011; final acceptance, December 2011.
Published by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
364 Volume 65, Number 3, August 2013
problematic of learning and not as an imperi- organized this Focus Section. Indeed, it seems
alistic disciplinary attempt to turn the study of to me that much of the previous geographi-
learning into exclusive geographical territory. cal work on learning shares two less desirable
Second, by choosing to dwell on this par- properties: it engages the theme of learning
ticular topic, we hope to attack from a new in implicit ways, more as a background for
angle the problem of the excessive internal some other interest (for a recent exception see
divisiveness and lack of communication among McFarlane 2011), and it usually discusses the
the diverse subdisciplines that constitute con- learning of humans and their institutions with-
temporary human geography. The work culmi- out pointing out that the very process of
nating in this Focus Section started in Septem- learning can be thought of as quintessentially
ber 2010, when I began to put together a panel geographical.
for the annual meeting of the Association of An illustration of the first property is the
American Geographers (AAG) in Seattle. From recent debate in human geography over the
the very beginning, I aimed to select a num- centrality of representations (Anderson and
ber of distinguished geographers from a variety Harrison 2010). Whereas the conventional
of subdisciplinary backgrounds: cultural geog- view emphasizes that we learn by acquiring,
raphy, economic geography, geographic infor- creating, and updating mental representations
mation systems (GIS), urban geography, po- of the world out there, nonrepresentational
litical geography, and so on. It seems to me theorists (Thrift 2008) have challenged this en-
that learning is a topic that can play the role trenched perspective, suggesting instead that
of connective tissue for the discipline of hu- the world is its own representation, that we get
man geography, precisely because it is a central by through being immersed in the world and
attribute of what makes us human. By high- dwelling in the middle of things. Put differ-
lighting the spatiality of a process that all hu- ently, if a representationalist would argue, like
mans engage inlearningwe foreground a Simon (1996), that memory is the environment
unifying concern for all human geographers, of thought, a nonrepresentationalist would flip
regardless of the particular subdiscipline with things around to claim that the environment
which they identify themselves. is the memory of our thought. Even though
These last observations bring me to the third I worded this debate to show that it really is
rationale for this Focus Section, namely, the de- about how we learn, the fact of the matter is
sire to make the problematic of learning truly that the organizing label for it has been repre-
central to the concerns of human geographers. sentation(s) or lack thereof and that this explicit
The history behind this rationale consists, in focus has obfuscated the underlying theme of
my detection, of a discrepancy between the fun- learning.
damental role ascribed to learning in the hu- Two illustrations of the second property are
man species by philosophers, psychologists, and (1) the earlier scholarship undertaken within
comparative biologists and the relatively pe- behavioral geography (including cognitive ge-
ripheral place it has occupied on the agenda of ography; for historical overviews of these fields
human geographers. One of the chief concerns see Johnston and Sidaway [2004, 13963]; Gold
of the former group of scholars has been with [2009]; and Montello [2009]), and (2) the
trying to single out those attributes that most surge of interest about the learning of eco-
distinguish our species from the rest (Adler and nomic agents in economic geography (Hudson
Rips 2008). Although the debate is far from be- 1999; Bathelt, Malmberg, and Maskell 2004).
ing concluded, one of the key ideas springing Behavioral geography is a label used to de-
from it is that humans have an ability to learn note, first, a broader intellectual movement
that is unprecedented in any other species and highly influential in geography between 1965
that the success of the human race in achieving and 1980 and concerned with the role of
ecological dominance can be traced back to that cognitive processes in spatial decision mak-
ability. If learning carries such a significant ex- ing and, second, a distinct subdiscipline of
planatory power for the human saga, it stands to human geography, incorporating cognitive ge-
reason that human geographers should system- ography as one of its foci. Although behav-
atically consider it in their research and schol- ioral geography has generated useful research
arship. Had this been the case, I would not have on topics such as the learning of geographical
Introduction: Learning as a Geographical Process 365

knowledge (Golledge and Stimpson 1997), the The Problem of Self-Reference


role of cognitive processes in spatial decision
making (Wolpert 1964; Kitchin and Blades That it is hard work to explicitly think of learn-
2002), cognitive maps (Gould and White ing as a geographical process became apparent
1974; Portugali 1996), human perceptions of to me in an ironic kind of way at the AAG
natural hazards (Burton, Kates, and White meeting in Seattle. Even though I had been re-
1978), and the relation among geography, flecting on the topic for months, it was only the
experience, and imagination (Lowenthal 1961), immediate pressure of finding something intel-
it has come under the attack of both humanis- ligent to say in the introduction to the panel
tic and radical geographical approaches, which that made me realize in the evening before the
has resulted in its increasing peripherality and panel debate that our discussion bears the in-
remnant status (Gold 2009, 283) and its being teresting characteristic of being self-referential.
perceived as a limited extension of spatial sci- Self-referentiality has long been identified as
ence (Gold 2009, 282). From the standpoint of the most perplexing attribute with which lo-
this Focus Section, my main criticism of behav- gicians have had to wrestle and refers to the
ioral geography is that it left the processes of curious worlds of thought that open up when
learning themselves undertheorized and failed a set of statements refers to itself (Hofstadter
to think of them as geographical through and 1980; Adler and Rips 2008). Metaphorically,
through. It is one thing to study the learning the image of a dog vainly chasing its own tail is
of geographical knowledge (e.g., mental maps, apt to elicit the same kind of intellectual dizzi-
navigation, way-finding); it is quite another to ness that accompanies any and every attempt
analyze how the process of learning itself is ge- at self-referential reasoning. It occurred to me
ographical. Not surprisingly, even some of the that there are two loops of self-referentiality
key proponents of the field were led to admit in our discussion of learning as a geographical
in retrospect the conceptual weakness that be- process.
devilled behavioural geography (Gold 2009, The first pertains to learning and the second
287). to its geographical character. With regard to
My second illustration of the second prop- learning, most of the panelists confessed at one
erty is much more recent and pertains to eco- point or another that they were intrigued by
nomic geography. Economic geographers have the theme of the panel and that they accepted
been increasingly under the sway of the narra- my invitation from a spirit of adventure and
tive of the knowledge economy and, therefore, curiosity, fully aware that it meant that they
have come to emphasize not only the explana- had to go out on a limb and force themselves
tory function of learning in understanding the to think outside their comfort zone. That is,
nature of competitive advantage but also the in the very process of meeting to discuss about
learning benefits ensuing from the geograph- learning, we ourselves learned new things.
ical clustering of economic agents. In so do- But this more obvious learning by talk-
ing, they have come very close to theorizing ing about learning self-referential loop was
learning as a geographical process (Hudson itself embedded into a second loop of self-
1999; Bathelt, Malmberg, and Maskell 2004). referentiality that pertains to our specific fo-
It is one thing to notice that the aim of learning cus on learning as a geographical process. The
has geographical consequences (e.g., the clus- very process of having a set of panelists from
tering of economic agents and the creation of around the world meet in Seattle in a par-
agglomeration economies), however, and quite ticular conference room at a table in front
another to become aware of how the learning of of a geographically diverse audience to talk
economic agents itself is a geographical process. about learning as a geographical process was
These twin properties of much of the earlier ge- itself an instance of learning as a geographi-
ographical scholarship on learning explain why cal process. The loop of self-referentiality can
learning (as such) does not currently have a be extended forward in time to our return-
distinct entry in either the Dictionary of Hu- ing home, writing down the final drafts of
man Geography (Gregory et al. 2009) or the our papers with the memories of the debate
International Encyclopaedia of Human Geography fresh in our minds (the intellectual haunt-
(Kitchin and Thrift 2009). ing of one place by an earlier one, preserved
366 Volume 65, Number 3, August 2013
as memory; i.e., learning), and submitting politically, the discipline will not thrive intel-
them to the assessment of peer reviewers of un- lectually. Le Heron thus shows how our future
known geographical locations. Hopefully, the as a community of scholars depends on forms
loop will close with the dissemination of this of collective or institutional learning that must
issue of The Professional Geographer containing be thought of as geographical. Finally, the ar-
our Focus Section to a broad audience of ge- ticles by Godlewska and Simandan investigate,
ographers, who themselves will be able to learn albeit at radically different scales, how spatial
about how a set of scholars think about learn- dislocations are a geographical process that en-
ing as a geographical process precisely because genders deeper forms of learning. Godlewska
their learning is itself a geographical process discusses an experiment in teaching her grad-
(spatial diffusion of knowledge in a geographi- uate students about place by dis-placing and
cal network of scholars through the mediation re-placing them from one seminar to the next
of an immutable mobilea printed journal in different meaningful places in Kingston, On-
issue or a PDF file). tario (ranging from art galleries to cemeter-
ies). The same kind of place-rich pedagogy,
but stripped of its university context and of
The Articles a teacherstudent framework, is of interest in
my own article, where I aim to enrich existing
I encouraged the contributors to the Focus Sec- theorizations of how people learn to be wiser
tion to be heterodox, daring, and creative, to ei- by proposing that the large-scale geographical
ther open up new ways of thinking about learn- dislocations inherent in the process of interna-
ing or to reflect on how mainstream meanings tional migration constitute one process that is
of learning (e.g., in the context of teaching) conducive to the acquisition of wisdom.
can be enriched by a geographical sensitivity.
Indeed, the four articles in this Focus Sec-
tion amply illustrate the observation that there An Exercise in Many-to-Many
are many ways to think geographically about Mapping
learning. Schuurmans article engages with the
relevant literatures in technology studies and I would like to end this introduction by sug-
cognitive neuroscience to investigate how the gesting that these ensuing four articles, how-
spread of the Internet and related technologi- ever diverse and daring, can only begin to hint
cal advances is creating a new geographical re- at the rich opportunities encapsulated in the
ality that compels us to learn in new ways. It theme of learning as a geographical process.
thus sensitizes us to how seemingly intraindi- The explanation for this wealth of potential-
vidual processes of learning actually depend on ities is ultimately traceable to semantics and
broader social and technological shifts that are formal logic. The theme of learning as a ge-
profoundly reconfiguring the environments in ographical process connects two semantically
which (and about which) we learn. Le Herons rich categories: learning and geographical pro-
article has the great merit of connecting our cess. The category of learning encompasses,
Focus Section with a set of long-standing in- among other things, subcategories as diverse
stitutional projects1 in a number of places in as immunological learning (how our immune
geography that have been pushing the agenda system learns to deal with previously encoun-
of learning and geographic pedagogy. Through tered pathogens; Simandan [2011b]), Pavlovian
a situated interrogation of New Zealand ge- learning (automatic conditioned responses to
ographys crisis and reawakening, Le Heron stimuli in our environment such as stopping
takes the idea of collective learning further than when seeing a red light in traffic), declara-
most, by emphasizing what institutions do for tive learning (the conscious acquisition of facts
disciplines and how they might be used dif- and theories; know that), procedural learn-
ferently. He provides a compelling interpreta- ing (the mastery of skills such as drawing a
tion of the political dimensions of geographical map using GIS; know how), perceptual learn-
learning, reminding us that unless geographers ing (the ability to notice ever finer and sub-
know how to operate effectively in institutions, tler distinctions in the world as the result of
create institutions, and play their institutions the sedimentation of prior knowledge; e.g.,
Introduction: Learning as a Geographical Process 367

a meteorologist who first learns the basic types Burton, I., R. W. Kates, and G. F. White. 1978. The
of clouds and later learns to distinguish sub- environment as hazard. New York: Oxford Univer-
types within each type; know with), and social sity Press.
or institutional learning (how cultures and col- Doel, M. 1999. Poststructuralist geographies: The diabol-
ical art of spatial science. Edinburgh, UK: Edinburgh
lective actors learn; emphasis on the distributed
University Press.
and shared attributes of knowledge; e.g., Le Gold, J. R. 2009. Behavioral geography. In Interna-
Heron, this issue). The category geographical tional encyclopedia of human geography, ed. R. Kitchin
process is just as semantically rich as learn- and N. Thrift, 28293. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
ing is (Doel 1999; Gould 1999; Johnston and Golledge, R., and R. J. Stimpson. 1997. Spatial
Sidaway 2004). When speaking of geograph- behaviour: A geographic perspective. New York:
ical processes, we might have in mind things Guilford.
as diverse as the dynamic properties of the en- Gould, P. 1999. Becoming a geographer. Syracuse, NY:
vironments in which we dwell (see Simandan Syracuse University Press.
[2011a] for an elaboration of Hogarths dis- Gould, P., and R. R. White. 1974. Mental maps. Har-
mondsworth, UK: Penguin.
tinctions between kind environments which
Gregory, D., R. Johnston, G. Pratt, M. Watts, and
facilitate learning from experience, and wicked S. Whatmore, eds. 2009. The dictionary of human
environments which hinder it), place-based geography. 5th ed. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
processes, issues pertaining to scalar shifts, the Hofstadter, D. R. 1980. Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal
hidden logic of landscapes, processes of spatial golden braid. New York: Vintage Books.
diffusion or spatial aggregation, and the nature Hudson, R. 1999. The learning economy, the learn-
of geographical dislocations. In formal logical ing firm and the learning region: A sympathetic
terms, because both learning and geographical critique of the limits to learning. European Urban
processes are semantically rich concepts, the at- and Regional Studies 6:5972.
Johnston, R. J., and J. Sidaway. 2004. Geography and
tempt to marry them generates a combinatorial
geographers: Anglo-American human geography since
explosion of potential new meanings character- 1945. 6th ed. London: Arnold.
istic of many-to-many mappings. We hope that Kitchin, R., and M. Blades. 2002. The cognition of
this Focus Section will be a stone thrown in a geographic space. London: I. B. Tauris.
pond and that the many ripples that will follow Kitchin, R., and N. Thrift, eds. 2009. Interna-
will unsettle and refresh human geography in tional encyclopedia of human geography. Oxford, UK:
ways that will allow us all to learn.  Elsevier.
Lowenthal, D. 1961. Geography, experience, and
imagination: Towards a geographical epistemol-
Note ogy. Annals of the Association of American Geographers
1 51:24160.
Three examples that readily come to mind are the
McFarlane, C. 2011. The city as a machine for learn-
work carried by the Education Commission of the
ing. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers
International Geographical Union, by the Jour-
36 (3): 36076.
nal of Geography in Higher Education, and by the
Montello, D. R. 2009. Cognitive geography. In
International Network for the Teaching and Learn-
International encyclopedia of human geography, ed.
ing of Geography in Higher Education.
R. Kitchin and N. Thrift, 16066. Oxford, UK:
Elsevier.
Literature Cited Portugali, J., ed. 1996. The construction of cogni-
tive maps. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer
Adler, J. E., and L. J. Rips, eds. 2008. Reasoning: Stud- Academic.
ies of human inference and its foundations. Cambridge, Preston, C. 2005. Pluralism and naturalism: Why the
UK: Cambridge University Press. proliferation of theories is good for the mind. Philo-
Anderson, B., and P. Harrison, eds. 2010. Taking- sophical Psychology 18 (6): 71535.
place: Non-representational theories and human geog- Simandan, D. 2011a. Kinds of environments: A
raphy London: Ashgate. framework for reflecting on the possible contours
Bathelt, H., A. Malmberg, and P. Maskell. of a better world. The Canadian Geographer 55 (3):
2004. Clusters and knowledge: Local buzz, 38386.
global pipelines and the process of knowl- . 2011b. Making sense of place through mul-
edge creation Progress in Human Geography 28: tiple memory systems. New Zealand Geographer 67
3156. (1): 2124.
368 Volume 65, Number 3, August 2013
Simon, H. 1996. The sciences of the artificial. 3rd ed. DRAGOS SIMANDAN is a Professor in the Depart-
Boston: MIT Press. ment of Geography at Brock University, 500 Glen-
Thrift, N. 2008. Non-representational theory: Space, pol- ridge Avenue, St. Catharines, Ontario, Canada, L2S
itics, affect. London and New York: Routledge. 3A1. E-mail: simandan@brocku.ca. His research in-
Wolpert, J. 1964. The decision process in spatial per- terests include geographical reasoning, social theory,
spective. Annals of the Association of American Geog- and the economic geography of knowledge and hu-
raphers 54:53758. man capital formation.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen