Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Psychological Services Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association

2008, Vol. 5, No. 1, 26 35 1541-1559/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1541-1559.5.1.26

Negative Emotions, Pain, and Functioning

Gabriel Tan Mark P. Jensen


Michael E. DeBakey VAMC and Baylor College of University of Washington School of Medicine
Medicine
John Thornby Paul A. Sloan
Baylor College of Medicine Michael E. DeBakey VAMC

We used linear structural equations (path model analysis) to examine associations


among negative emotions, pain, and functioning in a large sample (N 511) of
veterans with chronic pain. We postulated and tested a model where pain and func-
tioning affect negative emotions and where negative emotions affect pain and func-
tioning. The findings confirm a strong relationship between negative emotions, pain,
and functioning in our sample, particularly as the variable Pain Interference affects
Depression. In a significant but weaker relationship, we also found that Anxiety has a
direct effect on patients perception of their Disability. Specifically, the data support a
model where increased Pain Interference, Pain Severity, Depression and Anxiety all
lead to increased Disability. Findings that Pain Interference and Depression appear to
play a major role in the relationships between pain and negative emotions support the
need for experimental studies to understand the causal impact of these variables on
patient functioning. In the meantime, the findings suggest that Pain Interference,
Depression, and Anxiety, in addition to Pain Severity, should all be targets of chronic
pain treatment.

Keywords: pain, negative emotions, pain interference, pain functioning

A number of investigators have studied the tional states, and that disability specifically re-
relationships between negative emotions, par- sulting from pain is affected by both pain per-
ticularly depression, and pain. It is well ac- ception and emotional state. In support of these
cepted among pain experts that negative emo- models, evidence indicates that states of depres-
tions such as depression are related to pain sion lead to increases in the report of pain
perception and experience (e.g., Banks & Kerns, (Salovey & Birnbaum, 1989), decreases in the
1996; Holzberg, Robinson, Geiser, & Gremil- tolerance for experimentally induced pain
lion, 1996). However, questions concerning the (Zelman, Howland, Nichols, & Cleeland,
causal associations between pain and negative 1991), and interpretation of various sensa-
emotions (e.g., whether depression and other tions as painful (Pennebaker, 1982). How-
negative emotions such as anxiety and anger ever, the data are not conclusive, given the
result from chronic pain and/or whether nega- finding that depressed patients can also have a
tive emotions exacerbate pain experience) have higher sensory and pain tolerance then non-
remained largely unresolved. depressed patients (for a review and meta-
Current models of pain perception argue that analysis, see Dickens, McGowan, & Dale,
the experience of pain is influenced by emo- 2003).
The notion that pain is the catalyst for nega-
tive emotional states comes from the belief that
Gabriel Tan, Michael E. DeBakey VAMC and Baylor pain causes enough of a disruption in an indi-
College of Medicine; Mark P. Jensen, University of Wash- viduals life that negative affective states
ington School of Medicine; John Thornby, Baylor College emerge (see Banks & Kerns, 1996). For exam-
of Medicine; Paul A. Sloan, Michael E. DeBakey VAMC. ple, pain can lead to a sense of hopelessness and
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Gabriel Tan, ABPP, Michael E. DeBakey VA
helplessness, can interfere with reinforcing or
Medical Center, 2002 Holcombe Blvd (145 Anesthesiol- pleasurable activities, and can lead to isolation,
ogy), Houston, TX 77030, E-mail: tan.gabriel@med.va.gov sleep disturbances, and frustration. All of these
26
NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, PAIN, AND FUNCTIONING 27

effects can increase the likelihood of experiencing challenge to ones control appraisal. It is a con-
symptoms consistent with depression and anxiety comitant of depression (Wade, Price, Hamer, &
and are/may be associated with anger or irrita- Schwartz, 1990) and it has been hypothesized
bility. Pain may also initiate a sense of loss, that anger turned inward leads to increased anx-
particularly if associated with disability. iety and depression. Although anger has been
Studies that examine the association between associated with anxiety and depression and has
chronic pain severity and depression have been hypothesized to be related to pain, the
shown mixed results with respect to the concor- relationship between anger and pain has not
dance rates and the strength of the association, been explored in research as much as that be-
but most have demonstrated some relationship tween depression or anxiety and pain (Robinson
(see G. K. Brown, 1990; Magni, 1987; Romano & Riley, 1999). Speculation about the nature of
& Turner, 1985). However, there remains some the relationship between anger and pain comes
controversy over the causal direction(s) of this from limited reports indicating that patients
relationship. In one early study, G. K. Brown with chronic pain tend to report higher levels of
found that the association between chronic pain hostility and anger (F. F. Brown, Robinson,
intensity and depression was moderate, and that Riley, & Gremillion, 1996; Gaskin, Greene,
a model identifying chronic pain as a precursor Robinson, & Geisser, 1992). Suggestions about
to depression fit better with his data than a the nature of this relationship include the con-
model identifying depression as a precursor for cept of elevated cortisol levels, sympathetic
pain. Rudy, Kerns, and Turk (1988) proposed a arousal (Fernandez & Turk, 1995) and the idea
model in which specific cognitive appraisals that anger triggers somatic reactivity such as
that relate to chronic pain, such as perceived life muscle tension that actually causes or increases
interference and loss of self-control, are medi- pain perception (Flor & Turk, 1989).
ating factors that determine if someone with Overall, although a number of correlation
chronic pain will become depressed, and found studies have examined the associations between
support for this model in their data. In a later negative mood states, including depression,
study, Turk, Okifuji, and Scharff (1995) repli- anxiety, anger, and pain, the nature of the rela-
cated this finding in an elderly population and tionships among these domains is not well un-
also found that age (being over or under age 70) derstood. Four possible relationships among the
moderated the relationship between depression variables have been hypothesized (Robinson &
and pain severity. Riley, 1999): (a) that negative emotional states
In addition to depression, anxiety is often sensitize a person to experience pain, (b) that
identified as a correlate of chronic pain. There is negative emotional states actually cause pain,
evidence that suggests that states of anxiety can (c) that negative emotional states are the result
lead to lower pain tolerance (Carter et al., 2002; of the experience of pain, and (d) pain and
James & Hardardottir, 2002; Keogh & Co- negative emotional states are parallel processes
chrane, 2002) and higher perceptions of pain that co-occur because of a root biological cause.
(Granot & Lavee, 2005; McWilliams, Goodwin, A fifth option is that negative mood states and
& Cox, 2004; Pud & Amit, 2005; Tripp, Stan- pain impact each other, and that the causal
ish, Coady, & Reardon, 2004). However, this relationship does not lie in only one direction.
relationship may be moderated by several fac- There has been some suggestion that the link
tors such as gender (Dougher, Goldstien, & between pain and these negative emotions may
Leight, 1987; Jones & Zachariae, 2004; Jones, be mediated, at least in part, by pains effect on
Zachariae, & Arendt-Nielsen, 2003) and atten- functioning. However, the concept of function-
tion (James & Hardardottir, 2002). Sullivan, ing in the context of chronic pain may consist of
Thorn, Rodgers, and Ward (2004) suggested two distinct domains. For example, Dworkin et
that trait anxiety leads to higher levels of state al. (2005) suggested that if one were to look at
anxiety, which leads to catastrophizing, which functioning one should consider both generic
then in turn leads to increased pain perceptions. measures, which measure general functioning
A third negative emotion state occasionally and could allow for comparison across debil-
referred to with respect to pain is anger (also itating conditions (i.e., general disability,
referred to as irritability). Anger, as suggested which can result from many different factors),
by Henry (1986), can result from a perceived and disease-specific measures, which assess the
28 TAN, JENSEN, THORNBY, AND SLOAN

impact of a specific disorder on functioning pation in this study) only if they have not met
(e.g., the component of disability that is specif- the inclusion criteria.
ically caused by pain; that is, pain interference). Patients were required to complete a packet
Pain interference has been defined as the extent of self-report questionnaires prior to their initial
to which pain interferes with day-to-day func- IPMP assessment. The questionnaire packet
tioning (Jensen, 2003). Assessing both do- was mailed to patients with a cover letter ex-
mains of functioning may help to identify the plaining that the questionnaires were to be used
role general versus pain-specific dysfunction as primarily for clinical purposes but that the data
they relate to emotion. might also be used for program evaluation and
The purpose of the present investigation research. Prior to analyzing the data for the
was to clarify the relationships between neg- current study, approval from the Institutional
ative emotional states, particularly depres- Review Board was obtained. Out of the 1,265
sion, anxiety, and anger with chronic pain and packets mailed from 1995 through 1998, 564
functioning by testing the hypothesis that packets were returned, with a return rate
negative emotions (depression, anxiety, and of 44.6%. Thus, slightly less than half of all
anger) are significantly associated with pain patients referred to the IPMP followed through
and functioning (pain intensity, pain interfer- on their referral and completed the necessary
ence, and disability) using a structural equations paperwork to receive services.
modeling (LISREL) approach. We made five a The demographic characteristics of the par-
priori hypotheses concerning the causal direc- ticipant sample were reported in a previous
tions of associations, given the fact that viable study comparing responses to two measures of
models exist for a number of different causal pain coping administered in the packet (Tan,
paths (e.g., that emotions can impact pain, that Jensen, Robinson-Whelen, Thornby, & Monga,
pain can impact emotions, that emotions and pain 2001). The mean age of patients completing the
can impact each other). However, we did plan to questionnaires was 50.8 years (SD 11.4,
examine and test for additional or different possi- range 22 to 82 years). Most (84.5%) had at least
ble causal paths. a high school education; 12% were college
graduates. The majority of the participants were
Method male (90.3%). Although most were White
(62.4%), 22.6% were Black, 4.6% were His-
Participants panic, 0.5% was other, and 9.9% did not
respond to this item. Approximately half
The sample was obtained from the population (50.2%) were married. Almost half of the par-
of patients with chronic noncancer pain referred ticipants (48.0%) were already receiving dis-
to the Integrated Pain Management Program ability compensation for a pain-related condi-
(IPMP) of the Houston VA Medical Center, a tion, and 58.0% indicated disability due to pain
tertiary teaching hospital. The IPMP is a mul- for more than 5 years. Information about spe-
tidisciplinary outpatient pain assessment, con- cific pain diagnosis was not collected, but
sulting, and treatment program that receive re- breakdowns by primary pain sites were as fol-
ferrals from surgical, medical, and psychiatric lows: back (39.0%), limbs (32.0%), neck/
departments within the medical center. Patients shoulders (19.0%), head (6.0%), and all over
referred to the IPMP typically have an extensive (4.0%). Furthermore, 72% of the patients re-
history of chronic pain and have received prior ported that they had pain at multiple sites.
treatments for pain. Inclusion criteria for partic- To evaluate possible response bias, we col-
ipation in the program (and therefore, for par- lected demographic and disability information
ticipating in this study) include: (a) referral on all patients referred between January 1996
from primary care physicians or other medical and December 1996. Information was obtained
specialists documenting a history of chronic on 94% of the patients and resulted in a sample
pain complaints not responsive to treatment of 126 responders and 168 nonresponders. The
provided by the referral sources; and (b) com- responders and nonresponders did not differ
pleting the intake-questionnaire packet for treat- with regard to age (M 54.5 and 53.9 years,
ment (detailed below). Patients are excluded respectively). There was also no significant dif-
from the program (and therefore from partici- ference in gender, 2 1.92, ns, marital status
NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, PAIN, AND FUNCTIONING 29

2 0.03, ns, or disability status (i.e., receiving developed by Spielberger (1979). Although the
no disability, service-connected disability, or STPI was never published, its scales were used
nonservice-connected disability) 2 0.68, ns. in the subsequent development of the State
Because the data come from a decade old sam- Trait Anxiety Inventory and the StateTrait An-
ple and may be outdated, a comparison was ger Expression Inventory (Spielberger, 1994).
made with a more recent data set independently On the STPI, participants were asked to indicate
collected for another study (Tan, Jensen, how they feel at a particular moment by rating
Thornby, & Shanti, 2004). Results indicated themselves on a 4-point rating scale ranging
that the current data set consisted of a somewhat from 1 (not at all), 2 (somewhat), 3 (moderately
younger group (M age 50.80 vs. 55.78; p .05; so), to 4 (very much so). Alpha coefficients were
N 511 and 434, respectively); were less de- .84 to .92 for the SAnxiety scale and .90 to .92
pressed (M score of 14.26 vs. 17.88; p .05); for the SAnger scale, showing good internal
and reported less pain interference (M interfer- consistency. Correlations of the STPI with the
ence score of 7.19 vs. 7.47; p .05). However, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ)
the two groups do not differ in education, eth- Neuroticism subscale were 0.49/0.46 (for
nicity, gender, or pain intensity scores. The dif- men/women) for the SAnxiety scale
ferences at p .05 level are likely to be an and 0.44/0.32 (men/women) for the SAnger
artifact of the rather large sample sizes rather scale. Conversely, inverse correlations were
than meaningful sample differences. found between the SAnxiety and SAnger
subscales with the EPQ Extraversion sub-
Measures scales: 0.21/ 0.16 (men/women) and
0.07/ 0.10 (men/women), respectively.
Measures of Negative Emotion
Measures of Pain and Functioning
Measures of negative emotions assessed de-
pression, anxiety, and anger symptoms. The Measures of pain and functioning available
Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression for analysis included: the RolandMorris Dis-
Scale (CESD; Radloff, 1977) was used to as- ability Questionnaire (RMDQ; Roland &
sess depression. The State Anxiety (SAnxiety) Morris, 1983) to assess disability, the Inter-
subscale and the State Anger (SAnger) sub- ference scale of the West HavenYale Multi-
scale of the StateTrait Personality Inventory dimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI;
(STPI; Spielberger, 1979) were used to measure Kerns & Turk, 1985) to assess the extent to
anxiety and anger, respectively. which pain interferes with the patients life, and
CESD. The CESD was used as a measure the Pain Severity scale of the WHYMPI to as-
of psychological functioning in this study. The sess pain severity.
CESD was developed to assess the presence RMDQ. The RMDQ is used to assess over-
and severity of depressive symptoms in a gen- all/generalized disability. The RMDQ, derived
eral population. It includes 20 items that are from the Sickness Impact Profile, was originally
based on a 4 point scale ranging from 0 developed to assess disability associated with
(rarely) to 3 (most of the time), resulting in back pain (Roland & Morris, 1983). Items focus
scores ranging from 0 to 60 (higher scores in- almost exclusively on the physical dimensions
dicating greater depressive symptoms). The of disability (Deyo & Centor, 1986). Partici-
CESD has been widely used in pain research pants indicate which, if any, of 24 statements
(G. K. Brown, 1990; Jensen & Karoly, 1991; describe them today and are related to their
Monga, Tan, Ostermann, Monga, & Grabois, pain. (e.g., I stay at home most of the time
1998) and has adequate reliability and conver- because of my pain.). Scores range from 0
gent validity (Radloff & Locke, 1986). Crite- to 24, with higher scores indicating greater
rion validity has also been established as the disability.
CESD scores of depressed and nondepressed Chronic pain treatment centers have found
participants have been found to be significantly that the RMDQ is simple to use and provides a
different (Radloff & Locke, 1986). great deal of useful clinical information about
STPI. SAnger and the SAnxiety sub- patients disability (Williams, 1988). Research
scales were two of the six subscales of the STPI has supported the validity and reliability of the
30 TAN, JENSEN, THORNBY, AND SLOAN

scale for assessing disability with reference to endogenous: any of the six variables could have
persons presenting with mixed chronic pain a path to any of the other variables or have a
problems (Jensen, Strom, Turner, & Romano, path leading to it from any of the other vari-
1992), as well as low back pain (Deyo & Cen- ables. We further hypothesized the existence of
tor, 1986; Lanier & Stockton, 1988). at least one path leading from pain and func-
WHYMP. The WHYMPI (Kerns, Turk, & tioning variables to negative emotions and at
Rudy, 1985) is a 56-item measure that assesses least one path leading in the opposite direction.
the impact of pain on the patients life, the This family of relationships neither mandates
patients view of how significant others respond nor precludes the possibility of recursive or
to their communication of pain, and the pa- nonrecursive models. This is the fifth model as
tients general activity level. The validity and stated in the Introduction. We will also address
reliability of the WHYMPI has been well estab- the other four models.
lished (Kerns et al., 1985). The validity of the We hypothesized the existence of six specific
WHYMPI has been further supported by the paths between variables: Pain Severity to Inter-
results of exploratory and confirmatory factor ference, Interference to Disability, Interference
analytic procedures (Turk & Rudy, 1990). Two to Depression, Depression to Anxiety, Anxiety
of the scales, Interference and Pain Severity to Anger, and Anger to Interference.
were used as measures of function and pain for LISREL does not prove causal association be-
this study. tween variables, although the results of LISREL
The Interference scale of the WHYMPI con- analysis can be viewed as being consistent or
sists of 11 items that assess the extent to which inconsistent with some variables having influ-
pain has interfered with day-to-day activities ence on other variables. In essence the two sets
and functioning, including the ability to work, of variables, emotion and pain, were hypothe-
to enjoy family, to participate in social and sized to act in a nonrecursive manner, in which
recreational activities, and to perform house- the influence of emotion on pain may exist in
hold chores (Kerns et al., 1985). Scale scores one cycle and the reverse may occur in a sub-
range from 0 to 66, with higher scores indicat- sequent cycle. Thus within the same model, the
ing greater perceived interference over ones two sets of variables are allowed to potentially
daily functioning. The Interference scale has appear to influence those in the other set as well
been reported to have an excellent internal con- as being influenced by them.
sistency of 0.90 and a testretest stability Input data in the current application of LISREL
of 0.86 (Kerns et al., 1985). analysis consisted of a covariance matrix among
The Pain Severity subscale of the WHYMPI the six variables, along with estimates of measure-
was used to assess pain severity and was entered ment error for each variable. The measurement
in the analyses to control for this potential con- errors were estimated using predetermined values
found. The Pain Severity subscale consists of for reliability along with the observed variances in
three items that assess both pain intensity and the present data. When the measurement errors
suffering. Subscale scores range from 0 to 18, were applied, the six input variables were replaced
with higher scores indicating greater intensity with six latent variables with the same names
and suffering. The pain severity subscale has an but free of measurement error.
internal consistency of 0.72 and a testretest The estimation procedure we chose to utilize
stability of 0.82 (Kerns et al., 1985). in this application followed an iterative process
similar to stepwise regression but with impor-
Procedure tant differences. At the initial step a presumed
basic structure was defined, consisting of the six
The Linear Structural Relations program hypothesized paths among the six latent vari-
(LISREL, Version 8.30; Scientific Software In- ables. At each succeeding step the user has the
ternational, Inc., 1996) was used to examine the option of adding and/or removing particular
relationships between the negative emotions paths, based on the significance of paths cur-
(Anger, Anxiety, and Depression) on the one rently in the model. A modification index
hand, and Pain Interference, Disability, and indicates the approximate significance of add-
Pain Severity on the other. Our basic hypothesis ing any path not currently in the model. This
was that all six variables are presumed to be sequence continues until a path model has been
NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, PAIN, AND FUNCTIONING 31

obtained containing the minimum number of observation that Pain Severity had lower cor-
significant paths, although no path not currently relations with each of the emotional variables
in the model would contribute significantly to than did Interference and Disability. Similarly
the model. After the final solution is obtained, Anger had lower correlations with Interfer-
the latent variables can then be standardized to ence, Disability, and Pain Severity than did
variables with unit variance as an option. Depression and Anxiety. On the other hand
The degrees of freedom for the t tests of the the emotional variables tended to be more
significance of each path in the model and chi- strongly associated with each other than did
square for error are related to the number of the pain and functioning variables. These ob-
variables in the covariance matrix and the num- servations are reflected in the derived paths.
ber of parameters estimated. In the present ap- Figure 1 represents the path model with the
plication, because the covariance matrix con- most support derived from this process. The
sists of six variables, the degrees of freedom in weighted least squares chi-square for er-
the final solution were equal to 6*7/2 minus 6 ror 5.78 with 8 degrees of freedom ( p
(to account for estimates of the variances of .67). Illustrated are the seven estimated paths,
the 6 latent variables) minus the number of the path coefficients for the standardized so-
paths estimated. lution and the statistical significance of each
path. There are a total of eight paths, where
Results no variable except Pain Severity has both a
direct or indirect path leading away from it-
Table 1 shows basic descriptive statistics self and a direct or indirect path leading back
for all study variables. The means and stan- to itself. A unique aspect of this model is that
dard deviations represent values for the 511 it contains both a path from pain to emotion
patients used in this study. Reliabilities had and a path from emotion to pain. The path
been calculated previously from other data, from Anxiety to Disability is the weakest of
and the measurement errors were estimated the paths ( p .01) while the path from
from the formula: Anxiety to Anger is the strongest ( p
0.001). All variables but Anger have a direct
Measurement error (variance or indirect path ultimately leading to Disabil-
ity. This final model contains five of the six
in the sample) * (1 reliability). paths in our hypothesis. The hypothesized
path from Anger to Interference has been
Table 2 shows the correlation matrix replaced with the path from Anxiety to Dis-
among all six variables. Although all correla- ability, and a path from Disability to Pain
tions were significant, the strength of the as- Severity has been added.
sociations was variable. For example the cor- We further investigated the possibility of
relations within the pain variables and within finding a model that consists of paths among the
the emotional variables were all larger than three pain-associated variables and among the
all of the correlations between pain and emo- three emotion variables without crossing to or
tional variables. Of particular interest is the from either group. The best such model con-

Table 1
Descriptive Data for Self-Report Measures of Pain and Emotion
Variable Range of possible scores M (SD) Reliability Measurement error (Variance)
Interference 0 to 6 5.03 (0.89) 0.853 0.1157
Disability 0 to 24 16.19 (4.97) 0.701 7.3952
Pain severity 0 to 6 5.08 (0.85) 0.718 0.2047
Depression 0 to 60 28.32 (12.15) 0.886 16.7583
Anxiety 10 to 40 23.51 (7.22) 0.889 5.7854
Anger 10 to 40 15.85 (7.30) 0.936 3.3893
Note. N 511.
32 TAN, JENSEN, THORNBY, AND SLOAN

Table 2
Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between Self-Report Measures of Depression, Anxiety, Pain
Interference, Disability, and Pain Severity
Interference Disability Pain severity Depression Anxiety
*
Disability 0.54
Pain Severity 0.49* 0.43*
Depression 0.39* 0.34* 0.26*
Anxiety 0.30* 0.29* 0.23* 0.62*
Anger 0.24* 0.23* 0.22* 0.52* 0.69*
*
p .001.

sisted of three paths connecting the pain vari- (Pain Severity, Pain Interference, and Disabil-
ables and three paths connecting the emotion ity). We hypothesized that measures of negative
variables, with chi-square for error 102.64 emotion and measures of pain and functioning
and 9 degrees of freedom, p .001. The strong would be significantly associated with one another,
direct relationship between Interference and and we further hypothesized certain limitations
Depression is evident in both the correlation on the directions of influence as exemplified by
matrix and in our final path model. paths in a LISREL model. The findings are
consistent with our hypotheses and confirm the
Discussion common finding that negative emotional states
are associated with pain and functioning. In
The purpose of the present investigation was particular the findings support a model suggest-
to examine the relationships between negative ing that pain severity directly affects interfer-
emotional states (specifically depression, anxiety, ence, and indirectly also affects disability, de-
and anger) and pain and functioning measures pression, anxiety, and anger. We also found that

0.430***
Pain Interference Depression

0.467***
0.519*** 0.699***

0.186**
Disability Anxiety

0.421*** 0.757***

Pain Severity Anger

** p < 0.01
*** p < 0.001

Note: X2 for error = 5.78; df = 8; p = 0.67

Figure 1. Path diagram for the standardized solution.


NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, PAIN, AND FUNCTIONING 33

depression indirectly, and anxiety directly af- lated and do not impact each other; these were
fects disability. The findings would indicate that simply not the case based on our findings and
pain sufferers become disabled when they report clinical practice.
increased Pain Interference and Anxiety, and
indirectly increased pain severity and Depression. Limitations
This finding is consistent with the idea that a
person suffering from pain may avoid work Several issues limit the generalizability of
due to a fear of exacerbation. The more a our findings. First, the participant sample con-
person reports anxiety, depression, pain se- sisted entirely of veterans. Moreover, 90% were
verity and pain interference due to their pain, men, and most of the sample had a long history
the more disabled the person is likely to be- of severe chronic pain (e.g., 58% have been
come; thus the notion of a cycle of avoidance disabled due to pain for more than 5 years).
and fear playing a central role in disability Therefore, any generalization of results to non-
(Crombez, Vlaeyen, Heuts, & Lysens, 1999; veterans, women, and those with less severe
van den Hout, Vlaeyen, Houben, Soeters, & pain conditions should be made with caution.
Peters, 2001; Vlaeyen, Kole-Snijders, Boeren, & Another possible limitation is the fact that the
van Eek, 1995) is supported. data set is a decade old, although comparison
Our findings that negative emotions are with a more recent data set reveals only minor
strongly correlated with each other and that differences in some demographic and pain-
depression appears to be the source from which related variables.
other negative emotions (anxiety and anger) Also, it should be reiterated that LISREL
evolve for our chronic pain population would cannot be used to prove causal associations
suggest that efforts to decrease any of these between variables. In LISREL, the researcher
negative emotions would not only lead to cor- specifies the model in which one set of variables
responding reduction in the other negative emo- is assumed to predict another set of variables.
tions, but that efforts to decrease depression LISREL then computes and estimates the
would likely produce the most effective results. strength and directionality of the relations be-
Initially targeting pain severity would likely tween the variables that would best explain the
produce the most effective immediate outcome specified model. Thus, although the current
in alleviating pain and its debilitating effects, if findings are consistent with the hypothesis that
effective interventions that directly influence Pain Interference with function is a key medi-
pain severity are available (a significant chal- ating variable in the pain-mood relationship;
lenge, given that the lack of availability of such experimental research is needed to confirm this
interventions that do not themselves produce hypothesis. The same holds true for the finding
adverse events, is one of the primary reasons that Depression (through Anxiety) also has an
that chronic pain remains a significant prob- indirect effect on Disability, Pain Severity and
lem). Another option is to target pain interfer- Pain Interference, and that it is also a key me-
ence, which would appear to then alleviate the diating variable in the pain-mood relationship.
emotional effects associated with pain. Despite the limitations of the current find-
In terms of the hypotheses stated in the In- ings, the results point to Pain Severity, Depres-
troduction, we could not test Hypothesis 1 be- sion, Anxiety, and Pain Interference as key fac-
cause the concept of sensitizing a person to tors that appear to contribute to Disability. Until
experience pain could not be easily operation- experimental studies test and confirm the causal
alized. Our findings do not indicate a one-way impact of these variables on each other, clini-
causal direction between negative emotions and cians would do well to target each factor to
pain; therefore, Hypotheses 2 and 3 were not maximize patient functioning. Although iden-
supported in favor of the fifth option of negative tifying effective treatments that impact pain
emotions and pain as mutually impacting each intensity (without other adverse events) is a
other. Hypothesis 4 (pain and negative emotions significant challenge, there are treatments for
are parallel processes that co-occur because of a depression (e.g., antidepressants and cogni-
root biological cause) was not supported by our tive behavior therapy), anxiety (e.g., antianx-
findings since that would imply that negative iety medications, in vivo desensitization; cf.
emotions and pain are not significantly corre- Vlaeyen et al., 1995), and anger (e.g., psy-
34 TAN, JENSEN, THORNBY, AND SLOAN

chotropic medications, anger management), Fordyce, W. E. (1976). Behavioral methods for


and pain interference (e.g., operant condition- chronic pain and illness. St. Louis, MO: Mosby
ing, Fordyce, 1976). Adequate treatment of Year Book.
chronic pain requires that each of these treat- Gaskin, M. E., Greene, A. F., Robinson, M. E., &
Geisser, M. E. (1992). Negative affect and the
ment options should continue to be made avail-
experience of chronic pain. Journal of Psychoso-
able to individuals with chronic pain and sig- matic Research, 36, 707713.
nificant disability. Granot, M., & Lavee, Y. (2005). Psychological fac-
tors associated with perception of experimental
References pain in vulvar vestibulitis syndrome. Journal of
Sex Marital Therapy, 31, 285302.
Banks, S. M., & Kerns, R. D. (1996). Explaining high Henry, J. P. (1986). Mechanisms by which stress can
rates of depression in chronic pain: A diathesis- lead to coronary heart disease. Postgraduate Med-
stress framework. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 95 ical Journal, 62, 687 693.
110. Holzberg, A. D., Robinson, M. E., Geiser, M. E., &
Brown, F. F., Robinson, M. E., Riley, J. F., & Gremillion, H. A. (1996). The effects of depres-
Gremillion, H. A. (1996). Pain severity, negative sion and chronic pain on psychosocial and physical
affect, and microstressors as predictors of life in- functioning. Clinical Journal of Pain, 12, 118
terference in TMD patients. CRANIO, 14, 6370. 125.
Brown, G. K. (1990). A causal analysis of chronic James, J. E., & Hardardottir, D. (2002). Influence of
pain and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psy- attention focus and trait anxiety on tolerance of
chology, 99, 127137. acute pain. British Journal of Health Psychol-
Carter, L. E., McNeil, D. W., Vowles, K. E., Sorrell, ogy, 7, 149 162.
J. T., Turk, C. L., Ries, B. J., et al. (2002). Effects Jensen, M. P. (2003). The validity and reliability of
of emotion on pain reports, tolerance and physiol- pain measures in adults with cancer. The Journal
ogy. Pain Research Management, 7, 2130. of Pain, 4, 221.
Crombez, G., Vlaeyen, J. W., Heuts, P. H., & Lysens, Jensen, M. P., & Karoly, P. (1991). Control beliefs,
R. (1999). Pain-related fear is more disabling than coping efforts, and adjustment to chronic pain.
pain itself: Evidence on the role of pain-related Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain, 80, 329 ogy, 59, 431 438.
339. Jensen, M. P., Strom, S. E., Turner, J. A., & Romano,
Deyo, R. A., & Centor, R. M. (1986). Assessing the J. M. (1992). Validity of the Sickness Impact Pro-
responsiveness of functional scales to clinical file Roland scale as a measure of dysfunction in
change: An analogy to diagnostic test perfor- chronic pain patients. Pain, 50, 157162.
mance. Journal of Chronic Disease, 39, 897906. Jones, A., & Zachariae, R. (2004). Investigation of
Dickens, C., McGowan, L., & Dale, S. (2003). Im- the interactive effects of gender and psychological
pact of depression on experimental pain percep- factors on pain response. British Journal of Health
tion: A systematic review of the literature with Psychology, 9, 405 418.
meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 369 Jones, A., Zachariae, R., & Arendt-Nielsen, L.
375. (2003). Dispositional anxiety and the experience
Dougher, M. J., Goldstein, D., & Leight, K. A. of pain: Gender-specific effects. European Journal
(1987). Induced anxiety and pain. Journal of Anx- of Pain, 7, 387395.
iety Disorders, 1, 259 264. Keogh, E., & Cochrane, M. (2002). Anxiety sensi-
Dworkin, R. H., Turk, D. C., Farrar, J. T., Haythorn- tivity, cognitive biases, and the experience of pain.
thwaite, J. A., Jensen, M. P., Katz, N. P., et al. Journal of Pain, 3, 320 329.
(2005). Core outcome measures for chronic pain Kerns, R. D., Turk, D. C., & Rudy, T. E. (1985). The
clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. Pain, West HavenYale Multidimensional Pain Inven-
113, 9 19. tory. Pain, 23, 345356.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Manual Lanier, D. C., & Stockton, P. (1988). Clinical pre-
of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Lon- dictors of outcomes of acute episodes of low back
don: Hodder & Stoughton. pain. Journal of Family Practice, 27, 483 489.
Fernandez, E., & Turk, D. C. (1995). The scope and Magni, G. (1987). On the relationship between
significance of anger in the experience of chronic chronic pain and depression when there is no or-
pain. Pain, 26, 181197. ganic lesion. Pain, 31, 121.
Flor, H., & Turk, D. C. (1989). Psychophysiology of McWilliams, L. A., Goodwin, R. D., & Cox, B. J.
chronic pain: Do chronic pain patients exhibit (2004). Depression and anxiety associated with
symptom-specific psychophysiological responses? three pain conditions: Results from a nationally
Psychological Bulletin, 105, 215259. representative sample. Pain, 111, 77 83.
NEGATIVE EMOTIONS, PAIN, AND FUNCTIONING 35

Monga, T. N., Tan, G., Ostermann, H. J., Monga, U., assessment (pp, 292321). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
& Grabois, M. (1998). Sexuality and sexual ad- rence Erlbaum Associates.
justment of patients with chronic pain. Disability Sullivan, M. J., Thorn, B., Rodgers, W., & Ward, L. C.
& Rehabilitation, 20, 317329. (2004). Path model of psychological antecedents to
Morley, S., Eccleston, C., & Williams, A. (1999). pain experience: Experimental and clinical findings.
Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized Clinical Journal of Pain, 20, 164 173.
controlled trials of cognitive behavior therapy and Tan, G., Jensen, M. P., Robinson-Whelen, S.,
behavior therapy for chronic pain in adults, exclud- Thornby, J. I., & Monga, T. N. (2001). Coping
ing headache. Pain, 80, 113. with chronic pain: A comparison of two measures.
Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The psychology of physical Pain, 90, 127133.
symptoms. New York: Springer. Tan, G., Jensen, M. P., Thornby, J. I., & Shanti, B. F.
Pud, D., & Amit, A. (2005). Anxiety as a predictor of (2004). Validation of the Brief Pain Inventory for
chronic nonmalignant pain, Journal of Pain, 5,
pain magnitude following termination of first-
133137.
trimester pregnancy. Pain Medicine, 6, 143148.
Tripp, D. A., Stanish, W., Coady, C., & Reardon, G.
Radloff, L. S., & Locke, B. Z. (1986). The Commu- (2004). The subjective pain experience of athletes
nity Mental Health Assessment Survey and following anterior cruciate ligament surgery. Psy-
CESD Scale. In M. M. Weisman, J. K. Myers, & chology of Sport and Exercise, 5, 339 354.
C. E. Rose (Eds.), Community surveys of psychi- Turk, D. C., Okifuji, A., & Scharff, L. (1995).
atric disorders (pp. 177187). New Brunswick, Chronic pain and depression: Role of perceived
NJ: Rutgers University Press. impact and perceived control in different age co-
Robinson, M. E., & Riley, J. L. (1999). The role of horts. Pain, 61, 93101.
emotion in pain. In R. J. Gatchel & D. C. Turk Turk, D. C., & Rudy, T. E. (1990). The robustness of
(Eds.), Psychosocial factors in pain: Clinical per- an empirically derived taxonomy of chronic pain.
spectives (p. 74 88). New York: Guilford. Pain, 68, 423 430.
Roland, M., & Morris, R. (1983). A study of the Van den Hout, J. H., Vlaeyen, J. W., Houben, R. M.,
natural history of back pain. Part 1. Development Soeters, A. P., & Peters, M. L. (2001). The effects
of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in of failure feedback and pain-related fear on pain
low back pain. Spine, 8, 141144. report, pain tolerance, and pain avoidance in
Romano, J. M., & Turner, J. A. (1985). Chronic pain chronic low back pain patients. Pain, 92, 247257.
and depression: Does the evidence support a rela- Vlaeyen, J. W., Kole-Snijders, A. M., Boeren, R. G.,
tionship? Psychological Bulletin, 97, 18 34. & van Eek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/
Rudy, T. E., Kerns, R. D., & Turk, D. C. (1988). (re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation
Chronic pain and depression: Toward a cognitive- to behavioral performance. Pain, 62, 363372.
behavioral mediation model. Pain, 35, 129 140. Wade, J. B., Price, D. D., Hamer, R. M., & Schwartz,
Salovey, P., & Birnbaum, D. (1989). Influence of S. M. (1990). An emotional component analysis of
mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of chronic pain. Pain, 40, 303310.
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 539 551. Williams, R. C. (1988). Toward a set of reliable and
valid measures for chronic pain assessment and
Spielberger, C. D. (1979). Preliminary manual for
outcome research. Pain, 35, 239 251.
the StateTrait Personality Inventory (STPI). Un-
Zelman, D. C., Howland, E. W., Nichols, S. N., &
published manuscript, University of South Florida,
Cleeland, C. S. (1991). The effects of induced
Tampa. mood on laboratory pain. Pain, 46, 105111.
Spielberger, C. D. (1994). StateTrait Anxiety Inven-
tory and the StateTrait Anger Expression Inven- Received March 2, 2006
tory. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), The use of psycho- Revision received August 1, 2007
logical tests for treatment planning and outcome Accepted August 21, 2007

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen