Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Special section: Emotion regulation across the life span

International Journal of
Behavioral Development
Emotional and adrenocortical regulation 113
The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
in early adolescence: Prediction by sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0165025414520808
attachment security and disorganization ijbd.sagepub.com

in infancy

Gottfried Spangler1 and Peter Zimmermann2

Abstract
The aim of the present study was to examine differences in emotion expression and emotion regulation in emotion-eliciting situations in
early adolescence from a bio-psycho-social perspective, specifically investigating the influence of early mother-infant attachment and
attachment disorganization on behavioural and adrenocortical responses. The sample consisted of 96 children of the Regensburg
Longitudinal Study IV. At age 12 months, attachment security and disorganization were assessed in the Strange Situation. At age 12 years,
the adolescents were observed together with their mother during a computer game (eliciting anger) and the Talk Show Task (eliciting
fear). Analyses included self-ratings and mother-ratings of the adolescents emotions (anger and fear), observations of the adolescents
emotional expression and emotional regulation (social regulation, effective regulation) as well as concurrent maternal emotional support.
In addition, adrenocortical activity was assessed from saliva samples before and after observation. The findings revealed different patterns
of social-emotional responses depending on early attachment security. Adolescents with secure infant attachment reported more anger,
when anger was induced, were rated as less anxious by their mothers, and their emotion self-ratings were more similar to their mothers
ratings compared to adolescents with an early insecure attachment. An increased adrenocortical response was only found in the group of
adolescents with attachment disorganization in infancy, especially with increased fear.

Keywords
adolescence, attachment, cortisol, emotion regulation, psychobiological regulation

In his conception of attachment theory, Bowlby (1969) emphasized 2012; Zimmermann & Grossmann, 1997). As the attachment system
the childs protection as the biological function of the attachment is a security regulation system (Bowlby, 1980) that is elicited in
relationship, while the psychological function of attachment refers situations inducing intense negative emotions, as it is the case in the
to felt security (M. S. Ainsworth, 1989; Sroufe & Waters, 1977), strange situation procedure, the effects of infant attachment on later
and emotion regulation (see Zimmermann, 1999). In the first adaptation mainly may be observable in comparable situations in
months of life, infants are very restricted in their emotional self- which the individuals emotion regulation capacities are intensly
regulation and are reliant on the caregivers external regulation and challenged or overwhelmed. This was obvious in the Regensburg
organization (Spangler, Schieche, Ilg, Maier, & Ackermann, 1994). longitudinal study I where infant attachment only predicted cooper-
However, with development, the infants themselves increasingly ation (i.e., proximity) or disruptive withdrawal (i.e., avoidance) at
contribute to their emotion regulation by acquiring and refining age 16 when the adolescents experienced negative emotions (e.g.
individual and social emotion regulation strategies. Autonomous helpless, or insecure) induced by the task. If no intense negative emo-
emotion regulation and self regulation becomes more prominent tions were elicited, no attachment-related differences in behavior
with increasing age (Sameroff, 2010). However, individual differ- appeared (Zimmermann, Maier, Winter, & Grossmann, 2001).
ences in emotion regulation may remain throughout the life-span
and, in part, are influenced by socialization and attachment.
Based on a transactional developmental model, attachment theory Attachment as emotion regulation
postulates that attachment in infancy and childhood influences adap-
tion when children are confronted with later stage-salient issues From that perspective, infant attachment as assessed in the strange
(Bowlby, 1980; Cicchetti, Toth, Bush, & Gillespie, 1988; Sroufe & situation (M. D. S. Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) can
Waters, 1977). This has been shown empirically, especially for dis-
organized attachment for preschool, middle childhood, and adoles- 1
Friedrich-Alexander-University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany
cence with moderate but replicated associations, for example, 2
Wuppertal University, Germany
regarding externalizing behavior or internalization of rules (Fearon,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, Corresponding author:
2010; Kochanska et al., 2010; Zimmermann & Grossmann, 1997). Gottfried Spangler, Institute of Psychology, University of Erlangen-
However, the effects of early attachment are not found in every Nuremberg, Naegelsbachstrasse 49a, D-91052 Erlangen, Germany.
domain and in all situations in later development (Kochanska & Kim, Email: Gottfried.Spangler@fau.de

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


2 International Journal of Behavioral Development

be interpreted as a first form of a specific organized emotion regu- 2005), and preschool children (Erickson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 1985;
lation pattern in reaction to the caregiver with specific social or Lutkenhaus, Grossmann, & Grossmann, 1985; Suess, Grossmann,
individual emotion regulation strategies in contact with the care- & Sroufe, 1992; see Thompson, 2008). Also at later age, in middle
giver when the attachment system is activated (Zimmermann childhood or adolescence, there is evidence for concurrent associa-
et al., 2001). Differences in the quality of attachment can be tions between attachment security or attachment disorganization
described with respect to attachment security and attachment disor- and characteristics of emotion expression, and regulation (Allen,
ganization, which in infancy are commonly assessed by the strange Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, & Marsh, 2007; Kobak, Cole,
situation. Depending on the infants behavioral strategies in the Ferenz-Gillies, Fleming, & Gamble, 1993; Zimmermann, 1999;
context of separation from and reunion with the attachment figure, Zimmermann, Mohr & Spangler, 2009).
infants are classified as securely attached, insecure-avoidantly The existing evidence of attachment influences on emotion
attached, or insecure-resistantly attached. Securely attached infants expression and emotion regulation in adolescence is mainly based
use the caregiver as a secure base for exploration, show emotional on concurrent and not on longitudinal studies. In addition, many stud-
concern and attachment behaviour after separation, and are able to ies use using representational assessment procedures for attachment
gain their emotional stability again by establishing proximity or (e.g. story stem procedure, and child or adult attachment interviews)
contact with the attachment figure. Thus, they use an effective and no behavioural attachment assessments (e.g. separation-reunion
social emotion regulation strategy in interaction with the caregiver. paradigms). While there are many indicators for the validity of repre-
In contrast, infants with an insecure-avoidant pattern hardly show sentational attachment procedures, there is only modest evidence for
emotional concern or attachment behaviour during separation and longitudinal stability between the two types of assessment from early
avoid proximity upon reunion, masking their emotional expression infancy to adolescence (Becker-Stoll, Fremmer-Bombik, Wartner,
despite high arousal and physiological stress (Spangler & Gross- Zimmermann, & Grossmann, 2008; Groh et al., 2013; Lewis, Feir-
mann, 1993). This can be interpreted as ineffective individual emo- ing, & Rosenthal, 2000; Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell &
tion regulation, as the suppression of their emotional expression Albersheim 2000; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000; Zimmer-
does not regulate their negative emotions. Insecure-resistantly mann, Fremmer-Bombik, Spangler, & Grossmann, 1997).
attached infants are intensely distressed by the separation, and show One of the reasons for this missing concordance may be that dif-
ambivalent behaviour towards the caregiver after reunion. They are ferent levels of the organization of attachment are assessed during
not able to re-establish emotional security and decrease their emo- the different developmental periods. While in the strange situation
tional arousal despite close proximity to the caregiver. Their emo- attachment is assessed in terms of specific behavioural strategies
tion regulation pattern is social but ineffective. In children when the attachment system is activated by eliciting fear and sad-
classified as disorganized regarding attachment (Main & Solomon, ness, the assessment of attachment using the Adult Attachment
1990) the core characteristic is the absence or a breakdown of a Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996) provides patterns based
coherent attachment strategy in coping with the emotional chal- on the coherence of the report and the evaluation of ones attach-
lenge caused by the separation from the mother. In many disorga- ment history (Zimmermann, 1999). Thus, beside the long time-
nized infants, an underlying but not complete secure or insecure span from infancy to adolescence there is a confounding of age and
attachment pattern can be observed. Thus, attachment security and level of assessment, testing heterotypic continuity. Attachment pat-
disorganization are regarded as two orthogonal and conceptually terns assessed at the behavioural level in negative emotion eliciting
different behavioral dimensions of attachment by Main and Solo- situations like in the separation-reunion paradigms constitute social
mon (1990; see also Spangler & Grossmann, 1999). The ineffective interactive emotion regulation patterns with the caregiver. Predict-
emotion regulation of infants with insecure or disorganized attach- ing later social interactive emotion regulation in situations that eli-
ment can also be shown at the biological level, especially in the cit intense negative emotions from infant attachment patterns might
reactivity of the adrenocortical system (Bernard & Dozier, 2010; also reflect an aspect of continuity of attachment over time (Zim-
Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson, & Nachmias, 1995; Nachmias, Gun- mermann et al., 2000) in the sense of coherence over time (Sroufe,
nar, Mangelsdorf, & Parritz, 1996; Spangler & Grossmann, 1993). 1989). Such an approach comes closer to homotypic continuity as
The influence of attachment experiences on emotional regula- it compares similar behaviours or phenotypes across age periods
tion even beyond the parentchild dyad and at later periods of (Caspi & Roberts, 2001). Thus, age-appropriate interaction situa-
development is explained by internal working models of attachment tions, which challenge the adolescents emotion regulation capaci-
(Bowlby, 1969; Bretherton & Munholland, 2008; Zimmermann, ties, offer the possibility to observe social emotion regulation with
1999) that are based on caregiving experiences with the attachment the caregiver where the attachment system comes into play.
figures. They include emotional and motivational characteristics as
well as cognitive knowledge and scripts about when and how to
show attachment behaviour, the availability of attachment figures, Attachment effects on different assessment levels of
their expected reaction when displaying distress, and about the self
emotion regulation
as being worthy or unworthy of love. Internal working models of
attachment are substantially involved in behavioural organization A comprehensive assessment of emotional processes necessarily
and emotional regulation (Bowlby, 1980; Cassidy, 1994; Zimmer- includes different levels of assessment of emotions and emotion
mann et al., 2001) and tend to stabilize over time. There is plenty of regulation, including psychological characteristics (e.g. subjective
longitudinal evidence that these early attachment patterns are based experience as feelings, expressive/communicative behaviours) as
on experiences of maternal sensitive responding to infants expres- well as biological markers of the effectiveness of emotion regula-
sion of needs and emotions (e.g. M. D. S. Ainsworth et al., 1978; tion processes (that is, cortisol).
Grossmann, Grossmann, Spangler, Suess, & Unzner, 1985), and are Bowlby (1969) suggested in concordance with many emotion
predictive of processes of emotional expression and regulation in theorists that emotions are phases enabling a person to immediately
toddlers (e.g. Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978; Schieche & Spangler, evaluate a given context and to activate behavioural strategies to

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 3

cope with the situation. In addition to this internal function, the Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the adolescent measures.
expression of emotions serves the social function of informing con-
specifics about ones own emotional state and to gain external sup- Standard
Mean deviation N
port for emotion regulation (relational function, Campos, Campos,
& Barrett, 1989; Coan, 2008). While the childs or adolescents Computer game (anger elicitation)
feelings (subjective experience) in a given context represent the Rating of adolescents anger
affective component of emotion, the emotional expression can be Self-rated 1.63 1.28 93
observed in mimic responses or verbal communication indicative Mother-rated 2.53 1.70 93
of the underlying emotions. The social function of emotions Observation Absolute frequency of 2.36 2.49 88
brought about by emotional expression depends on the perception anger expression
of emotional expression by the caregiver. Relative frequency of overall social .66 .19 87
emotion regulation
There is empirical evidence that attachment patterns predict the
Effective emotion regulation .60 .27 87
concordance between the adolescents self-reports of own prob-
Talk show (fear elicitation)
lems, symptoms, or negative emotions and the perception of other Rating of adolescents fear 1.57 .83 93
persons like parents, peers or independent observers. In self-reports, Self-rated
adolescents with insecure attachment representation often present Mother-rated observation 2.47 .97 93
themselves more positive, less distressed, and as suffering from Absolute frequency of fear expression 11.32 7.01 91
fewer symptoms than they are described by parents or close friends Relative frequency of overall social emotion- .65 .32 91
(Berger, Jodl, Allen, McElhaney, & Kuperminc, 2005; Kobak & regulation
Sceery, 1988; Zimmermann, Gliwitzky, & Becker-Stoll, 1996). In Effective emotion regulation .46 .32 91
addition, these adolescents show low concordance between their Maternal emotional support 5.00 1.29 91
Cortisol (ln)
self-report on their current emotional states and their concurrent
Before assessment 1.24 .71 83
emotion expression either observed (Zimmermann et al., 2001) or
Before emotion induction .82 .77 87
assessed by electromyogram (EMG) (Spangler & Zimmermann, After emotion induction .81 .98 90
1999). Thus, their communication of emotions in interactions is
either restricted or they have difficulties in accessing their emotions
or answer with social desirability.
In this study, we applied the conception of infant attachment pat- expected that all adolescents will experience increased negative emo-
terns as emotion regulation patterns differentiating between social tions induced in the experimental situations, we assumed that
or individual and effective or ineffective regulation to adolescence. depending on differences in the effectiveness of emotional regula-
Likewise, for adolescents emotion regulation in interaction situations tion, lower maternal ratings of adolescents negative emotions should
can be social (e.g. by signalling ones emotions to others) or individual be expected in the group with secure infant attachment history.
(e.g. by avoiding the situation, allocating attention away from the eli- According to Gunnar and Adam (2012), adrenocortical reactiv-
citor). In addition, the effectiveness of emotion regulation can be ity can be used as a physiological parameter of emotional regulation
observed by assessing the duration of the emotion after the regulation processes specifically in adolescence. From the psychobiological
strategy or can be derived from physiological responses (e.g. cortisol). coping model, we would expect an increase of adrenocortical activ-
ity in insecurely attached adolescents during the emotion eliciting
situations (see Spangler & Grossmann, 1993). However, as inse-
curely attached adolescents in contrast to adolescents with attach-
Aims of the study ment disorganization in infancy may apply more individual
The main objective of our study was to investigate influences of emotion regulation strategies, the adrenocortical reactivity in the
early attachment on emotional processing and regulation in early adolescents with attachment disorganization may be more intense.
adolescence. The assessment of emotion and emotion regulation
was accomplished in two different emotion-inducing situations
(induction of anger and fear) and included the adolescents emo- Method
tional expression and feelings, the perception of their emotions
by their mothers, and the adolescents efforts of social emotion-
Participants
regulation and the effectiveness of these emotion regulation strate- The current study was conducted as part of the 12-year longitudinal
gies. In addition, the adolescents adrenocortical activity offers follow-up assessment of the Regensburg Longitudinal Study IV, a
additional insights in the effectiveness of emotion regulation. sample of, originally, 106 healthy German, Caucasian, low-risk
Regarding emotional expression and feelings, we expected that infants (53 girls/53 boys), first assessed at 12 months of age. At the
adolescents with early secure attachment would report their nega- 12-year assessment, a total of 96 participants (49 girls/47 boys) and
tive feelings more openly and in higher concordance with their their mothers could be seen again. According to maternal education
mothers than those with early insecure attachment. We also assessed at follow-up, the families represent a fairly wide range of
expected that adolescents with a secure infant attachment history socioeconomic status, including 28% high school education
would communicate their negative feelings through directed (including university entrance certificate), 33% medium-
expression of negative emotions as a form of social emotion- secondary school certificate, and 39% lower-secondary education
regulation. In addition, we expected an effect of infant attachment (most of them with additional vocational training).
on the effectiveness of emotion regulation. The complete data set for this report was not available for all
Maternal emotion ratings are expected to provide valid informa- subjects due to missing values (see Table 1). Missing values
tion about the adolescents current emotional state. Although we occurred because of inadequately filled in rating questionnaires,

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


4 International Journal of Behavioral Development

technical problems of video tapes in observational analyses and designed to make adolescents attachment behaviour and emotion
insufficient amounts of saliva for cortisol analyses. Due to the miss- regulation in the parentadolescent dyad observable. In contrast,
ing values sample size is somewhat reduced in the specific statisti- other social-evaluative procedures (e.g. TSST, Kirschbaum, Pirke,
cal analyses. & Helhammer, 1993) have the goal to globally induce stress inde-
pendent of the participants specific emotion, emotion regulation,
or attachment behaviour. All analyses were carried out by indepen-
Procedures dent and reliable coders using a standardized observational system.
At the age of 12 months, the infants were observed during the
strange situation, a 20-minute situation involving a sequence of epi-
sodes which progressively activates the attachment system by an
Measures
unfamiliar environment, the arrival of a stranger and two brief 12 months attachment security and disorganization. The quality
separations from the mother (M. D. S. Ainsworth et al., 1978). At of infant-mother attachment was analysed by trained observers
the age of 12 years, adolescents and their mothers were invited to from the strange situation video tapes using the procedures described
the university. After obtaining informed consent from the parents, by M. D. S. Ainsworth et al. (1978) for the traditional ABC classifi-
the adolescents were interviewed to assess their attachment pat- cation, and by Main and Solomon (1990) for the D-classification.
terns. Afterwards, the adolescents and their mothers participated Reliability was 90% (kappa .87) for attachment security and
in two standardized interaction tasks designed to induce negative 81% (kappa .63) for disorganization. In case of disagreement, con-
emotions (anger and fear), first a dyadic computer game (anger eli- ference scores were used (for further details see Spangler &
citation) and afterwards the dyadic talk show (fear elicitation). Schieche, 1998). Among the 96 subjects included in the 12-year
assessment, infant attachment was secure in 61 cases and insecure
Computer game (anger elicitation). The dyadic computer game is a in 32 cases (18 avoidant, 14 ambivalent). Moreover, infant attach-
modification of a task used in adolescent aggression research to suc- ment was disorganized in 23, and non-disorganized in 73 cases.
cessfully induce anger (De Castro, Slot, Bosch, Koops, & Veerman,
2003). Both mother and adolescent together play a jump-and-run 12-year measures of emotion expression and emotion regulation.
game with the aim to free a princess (Esmeralda) by controlling The analysis of the 12-year emotion assessments included emotion
the play figure (Quasimodo) through several levels. The ado- expression, observed emotional regulation, and self-reported emo-
lescent has two keys to play the game, the mother one key. Both tions during both tasks.
were instructed about the game and asked not to press the
ESC-Key (which was situated near both partners keys), Emotion expression. In each of the two emotion induction situa-
because this led to a game crash. After a short exercise phase, both tions, adolescents negative emotions were coded by means of an
mother and adolescent played a manipulated version, in which event-based coding system from their facial and verbal expressions
shortly before successfully reaching the aim, the game stops with of fear (including lower intensity level expressions, that is, uneasi-
a sound announcing a game crash because the ESC-Key has been ness and discomfort), anger, and sadness. Inter-rater reliability was
pressed. The adolescents behaviour was coded regarding emotion good (kappa .75). In the talk show (fear induction), the frequency
expression, and emotion regulation before and after the manipulated of fear expressions during the preparation phase was included for
game crash. All analyses were carried out by independent and reli- this analysis. For the computer game (anger elicitation) the fre-
able coders using a standardized observational system. quency of anger expressions was coded during the complete task
including the number of anger expression before and after the
Talk show (fear elicitation). The dyadic talk show situation manipulated game crash.
(Zimmermann et al., 2009) was designed to induce primarily fear
by creating an age-appropriate social evaluative context, and to Emotion regulation. Emotion regulation was assessed by coding
offer a context where parent-adolescent interaction and cooperation behaviours that modulate or change the emotional arousal of the
can be observed. Social evaluation is a major elicitor of fear in ado- adolescent. This is in line with widely-used definitions of emotion
lescence (Adrian, Zeman, & Veits, 2011; Westenberg, Gullone, regulation (Thompson, 1994). The coding separates the observed
Bokhorst, Heyne, & King, 2007). The talk show is divided into a frequency or intensity of the emotion from the emotion regulation
preparation phase and a presentation phase. First, participants were process (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004). Two different emotion-
informed that the mother had to interview the adolescent on per- regulation dimensions were assessed. The first dimension differ-
sonal topics in front of a public audience, and that they will be entiates the person who regulates (social vs. individual emotion),
video-taped during the presentation. In the preparation phase, the second dimension the effectiveness of the regulation (effective
mother and adolescent had 10 minutes to develop and write down vs. ineffective reduction of emotion intensity). The coding is simi-
six personal topics which would characterize the adolescent best lar to the approach used by Buss and Goldsmith (1998), who sepa-
during the public presentation. Both mothers and adolescents could rately assessed emotion regulation strategy and emotion expression
make suggestions and discuss them for the final list of questions. In after strategy use. Thus, each negative emotional event was coded
the performance phase of the talk show, the adolescent was inter- regarding social or individual emotion regulation and regarding
viewed by the mother in front of a small audience and a video cam- effective or ineffective emotion regulation. Social emotion regula-
era. The notion of the adolescent being under social evaluation was tion was coded each time the negative emotion was signalled or
emphasized in the instructions to ensure the emotional impact of the communicated directly towards the mother, addressing her verb-
situation. The 10 minutes of the preparation phase were videotaped ally, visually, or physically by proximity-seeking. Individual emo-
and coded for adolescent attachment and autonomy behaviours, tion regulation was coded each time a negative emotion appeared
specifically indicating emotional expression, emotional communi- and then the adolescent looked away from the caregiver, did not
cation, and emotion regulation. The Talk-Show Task is primarily address her verbally or physically, or looked in mothers direction

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 5

only when mother looked away and no eye-contact was established. accept vs. devaluate the adolescent in reacting to his/her sugges-
Each emotion regulation behaviour was coded as either effective tions, comments, and non-verbal behaviour during preparation of
when the adolescents emotion was reduced in intensity or no lon- the talk show (fear elicitation). Ratings for these scales were done
ger was expressed after a time interval of maximum 7 seconds, by two independent observers who were not involved in adolescent
starting with the beginning of the emotional expression, followed behaviour analyses. The scales were applied separately for each of
by the emotion regulation strategy (e.g. turning to mother), and then three consecutive 3-minute intervals of the preparation phase.
coding the intensity of negative emotion afterwards. Emotion reg- Reliability for the maternal scales was calculated by using Cohens
ulation was coded as ineffective if the duration of the negative emo- kappa. Reliability for the scales (based on 31 to 40 intervals of dif-
tion lasted longer. The time interval was chosen based on ferent subjects) was .86, .87, .78, .83 for sensitivity, patience and
preliminary analyses of the video-tapes. The coding separates pure emotional warmth, emotional motivation and acceptance vs. deva-
emotion expression as a symptom of emotion from intentional emo- luation, respectively. For each scale, a mean score of the scores of
tion signalling as appeal (Scherer, Mortilaro, & Mehu, 2013).Thus, the three intervals was calculated. The final score of emotional sup-
each negative emotional event of the adolescent received two port is the mean of the four subscales.
codes, one regarding the direction of emotion regulation and one
regarding the effectiveness. 12 year assessment of adrenocortical activity. The cortisol
For the current analysis, we used the relative frequency of social response was assessed from saliva. To collect saliva, the adoles-
and effective emotion regulation on all emotion regulation events cents were asked to place a cotton roll into their mouth and to keep
(that is, dividing the frequency of the of social or effective emotion it until it was saturated. Saliva samples were collected immediately
regulation events by the total number of all emotion regulation at the beginning of the laboratory assessment and before and after
events). The proportion of social emotion regulation indicates the the emotion induction situations. All saliva samples were frozen
percentage of all negative emotions regulated by referring to the until being assayed to 20 oC after the end of the assessment pro-
mother. The proportion of effective emotion regulation indicates cedure. Cortisol analyses were conducted at the Department of Psy-
the percentage of negative emotions that are reduced in intensity chology at the Technical University of Dresden (Prof. Clemens
by either social or individual emotion regulation. Kirschbaum). After thawing, salivettes were centrifuged at 3,000
rpm for 5 min, which resulted in a clear supernatant of low viscos-
Self-reported emotions. Three-times during the assessment, the ity. Salivary concentrations were measured using commercially
adolescent and their mothers were asked to rate the adolescents available chemiluminescence-immunoassay with high sensitivity
feelings by using a list of emotional adjectives and to rate the inten- (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany).The intra and inter-assay
sity of experienced feeling (child) or perceived feeling (mother) on coefficients for cortisol were below 8%. The present analysis
a 7-point scale (from 1 indicating no specific emotion, to 7 indicat- includes the assessments before and after the emotion inducing
ing high intensity of the emotion). The emotion self-rating scales situations. To control for extreme values, the cortisol scores were
have already been used in earlier studies with adolescents and log-transformed. For correlational analyses, delta cortisol was cal-
adults (e.g. Zimmermann et al., 2001) and show good internal con- culated by subtracting the cortisol score before from the cortisol
sistency. The emotion ratings took place immediately after the score after the situation. As there was a standardized time schedule,
computer game (anger elicitation) as well as after the preparation the time difference between the two measures was comparable
phase of the talk show (that is, before the performance phase) and (about 75 min) and therefore had not to be controlled for.
after performance. The total anger ratings are calculated as mean Due to the circadian rhythm of cortisol daytime of assessment
scores of the single ratings of the emotional states anger and fury. has to be considered. The majority of assessments (76%) were con-
The total fear scores were calculated as the mean of the single ratings ducted in the afternoon (starting between 13.00 and 17.00), 23%
of fear, uneasiness, and tension. The following measures were were conducted in the morning (starting between 9.00 and 12.00),
derived from the emotion ratings: adolescents self-rated and one assessment was conducted in the evening (19.00). To control
mother-rated anger after anger elicitation (computer game) and ado- for circadian influences, all analyses resulting in significant asso-
lescents self-rated and mother-rated fear after fear elicitation (mean ciations with cortisol were repeated with controlling for daytime
scores of ratings after the preparation and the presentation phase). of assessment as a covariate. This did not result in substantial
changes of the results in any analysis.
12-year assessment of maternal emotional support. Maternal
emotional support during the talk show preparation phase was ana-
Overview of measures and statistical analyses
lysed by means of four nine-point scales: sensitivity, patience and
emotional warmth, emotional motivation, and acceptance vs. deva- In sum, the independent measures or predictors in this study were
luation. The scale sensitivity, derived from Ainsworths sensitiv- two measures of infant attachment, attachment security (secure
ity scale (M. D. S. Ainsworth et al., 1978), describes the mothers vs. insecure) and attachment disorganization (D vs. non-D) and
tendency to perceive and appropriately interpret the adolescents observed maternal emotional support at age 12 years. The depen-
expression of emotion, and to promptly and adequately respond dent measures included the adolescents anger and fear indepen-
to them. The scale patience and emotional warmth describes the dently rated by the adolescents and their mothers and observed in
general tendency of the mother to be patient with the adolescents expressive behaviour, the adolescents emotional regulation (social
verbal contribution, to respond warmly and friendly, specifically to regulation, effective regulation) as well as their adrenocortical
the adolescents contradictory and autonomous remarks to mothers responses during the task.
suggestions. The scale emotional motivation describes the moth- For analyses of the predictability of the dependent measures by
ers tendency to adequately motivate the adolescent, to try to pro- the independent measures, correlation and regression analyses were
vide or facilitate fun and pleasure during the task. The scale used. In addition, repeated measure analyses of variance were con-
acceptance vs. devaluation describes the mothers tendency to ducted with the attachment groups as independent measures.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


6 International Journal of Behavioral Development

Results 3.5
insecure secure
Preliminary analyses 3.0

emotion intensity
Descriptive statistics of the adolescents measures are depicted in 2.5
Table 1. First, t tests were conducted to test for possible gender dif- 2.0
ferences. The only significant effect was found for social regulation
of emotion in the fear situation indicating that girls more often 1.5
applied mother directed social emotion regulation, t(89) 2.08, 1.0
p < .05. self-rated mother-rated self-rated mother-rated
In a next step, bivariate correlation analyses were conducted to
assess the relations between raters (mother and adolescent) regard- anger during computer game fear during talk show
ing the adolescents anger and fear, as well as the associations
Figure 1. Self-rated and mother-rated adolescent anger in the computer
between the anger and fear ratings and the adolescents respective
game (anger elicitation) and adolescent fear in the talk show task (fear eli-
emotional expressions observed during the two tasks. Only one out
citation) depending on early attachment security; n 31 and 59 for securely
of three correlations between adolescent self-rating and mother rat- and insecurely attached children, respectively; an asterisk indicates signifi-
ings was significant (that is, the anger rating after the computer cant differences between the 2 groups.
game: r .23, p < .05). Similarly, the adolescents self-ratings were
not significantly associated with their emotional expression. One
correlation approached significance (anger rating after the com- maternal emotional support. However, there was a significant pos-
puter game: r .19, p < .10). In contrast, maternal ratings of adoles- itive correlation between maternal emotional support and effective
cents emotions were significantly associated with the adolescents regulation during the talk show (fear elicitation), r(90) .34, p < .001.
emotional expression (r .24, p < .05 for anger; r .36, p < .001, In addition, there was a negative correlation between maternal
for fear). emotional support and the delta cortisol nearly approaching statis-
tical significance, r(83) .21, p .054), which indicates that an
adrenocortical increase during the elicitation of negative emotions
Contribution of adolescents emotional regulation to may be associated with low maternal emotional support.
emotion rating, emotional expression, and
adrenocortical response
Correlational analyses between adolescents emotional regulation
Prediction of emotional rating, emotional expression,
and their emotional states and delta cortisol revealed no significant and emotional regulation in early adolescence by early
correlations between emotional regulation and the emotion ratings attachment security and disorganization
of both mother and adolescents. However, emotional regulation
was significantly associated with measures of emotional expression In order to analyse the influence of early attachment experiences on
and cortisol. During the computer game (anger elicitation), the fre- the emotion ratings, two separate 2-way MANOVAs with rater (self,
quency of anger expressions was negatively correlated with social mother) as repeated measure factors and attachment security (secure,
emotion regulation (r .23, p < .05) and effective emotion reg- insecure) and attachment disorganization (disorganized, non-disor-
ulation (r .47, p < .001). During the talk show (fear elicitation), ganized), respectively, as an independent factor were conducted for
the frequency of fear expression fear was negatively associated the computer game (anger elicitation) as well as the talk show (fear
with effective emotion regulation (r .39, p < .001). Thus, ado- elicitation). While MANOVAS did not reveal any main effects or
lescents who showed more social emotion regulation strategies interaction effects on the emotion measures including attachment
exhibited less anger expression in the anger situation. In addition, disorganization in both situations, there were significant effects of
effective emotion regulation resulted in a reduced frequency of infant attachment security for both the anger and the fear situation.
emotion expression of anger and fear in the respective emotion Regarding the adolescents anger intensity after the computer
inducing situations. game the MANOVA showed a main effect for rater, F(1, 88)
To analyse associations between emotion regulation and the cor- 23.27, p < .001, eta2 .21, modified by an interaction between
tisol response, we aggregated the relative frequency of social as attachment security and rater approaching significance, F(1, 88)
well as effective regulation across the two situations. Effective 3.03, p < .10, eta2 .033, on adolescents anger intensity.
emotion regulation was significantly negatively correlated with Post-hoc t tests revealed that adolescents who had been securely
delta cortisol, r(84) .22, p < .05). Thus, less effective emotion attached to mother in infancy reported feeling more anger after the
regulation during the two emotion eliciting situations was associ- anger situation than those who had been insecurely attached in
ated with an adrenocortical increase. infancy (p < .05, see Figure 1). However, this difference was not
found for the mother-ratings. For both infant attachment groups,
mother-rated anger after the anger situation was significantly higher
Influence of maternal emotional support on emotional than the adolescents self-rated anger (p < .05), but the difference
was twice as much in the insecurely attached groups compared to
and adrenocortical regulation the securely attached group. Thus, while according to the mothers
Correlation analyses were conducted to test relations between perceptions all adolescents seem to be angry after the anger-
maternal emotional support and measures of adolescent emotion eliciting computer game, adolescents with a history of insecure
expression, emotion regulation, and adrenocortical activity. There attachment in infancy reported far less anger in the self-report than
were no significant correlations between emotional expression and their mothers perceived. There seems to be more agreement

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 7

between adolescent and mother regarding the adolescents anger in 2.0 pre post
the securely attached group.
Regarding the fear-eliciting talk show situation, the MANOVA 1.5
revealed significant main effects for rater, F(1, 88) 62.44, p < .001,

cortisol (ln)
eta2 .42, and for attachment security, F(1, 88) 5.03, p < .05,
1.0
eta2 .054, modified by a significant interaction effect between
these factors on adolescents fear, F(1, 88) 5.99, p < .05, eta2 .063.
Post-hoc t tests revealed that there was a difference between 0.5
securely and insecurely attached subjects regarding mother-rated
adolescent fear (p < .05) with higher ratings for adolescents with 0.0
insecure infant attachment (see Figure 1). In contrast, there was non-D D
no difference between the two groups regarding adolescent-rated
fear. For both groups, mother-rated scores were higher, but adoles- pre post
cent and mother-scores again were more similar in securely 2.0
attached subjects. Thus, while the adolescents reported comparable
levels of fear during the talk show (which were lower than the 1.5

cortisol (ln)
maternal ratings), mothers of adolescents with early insecure
attachment reported higher fear levels for their children. Again, 1.0
in the secure infant attachment group, there was a higher agreement
between adolescent and mother ratings regarding adolescents fear 0.5
compared to the insecurely attached group.
To test the predictability of the expression of anger and fear and 0.0
non-D D non-D D
the regulation measures in the emotion situations, t tests were con-
ducted separately for attachment disorganization and security. While high fear low fear
there were no effects for attachment disorganization, there was a sig-
nificant finding for attachment security regarding social regulation. Figure 2. Adrenocortical activity before and after emotion elicitation
depending on attachment disorganization (above) and depending on
The proportion of emotion episodes socially regulated in the anger
attachment disorganization and fear (below); n 66 and 18 for non-D and
situations was higher in the group of adolescents with early secure D, respectively; n 30 and 36 for low and high fear children, respectively,
attachment (M .70) than in the group with early insecure attach- within non-D; n 10 and 8 for low and high feat children, respectively,
ment (M .59; t(82) 2.35, p < .05). The same difference was within D; an asterisk indicates significant differences between the 2 values.
found for the talk show (M .67 and .60 for the secure and the inse-
cure group, respectively), but did not reach statistical significance.
p < .05, eta2 .07. As can be seen from Figure 2, the effect of infant
attachment disorganization on the adolescents adrenocortical
response was most intense when the adolescents were feeling highly
Prediction of the adrenocortical stress response by afraid during the negative emotion eliciting situation. Post hoc t tests
early attachment security and disorganization revealed a significant increase only for this group.
In a next step, we tested the influence of infant attachment patterns As significant associations of adrenocortical activation were
on the adolescents adrenocortical responses to the emotionally found with different concurrent and early measures, and these dif-
challenging situations. Two-way MANOVAs with a within subject ferent predictors were inter-correlated, regression analysis was con-
factor for time (pre, post) and an independent factor for attachment ducted predicting cortisol at task end in order to identify the specific
security (secure, insecure) and disorganization (D, non-D), on the contribution of each of these associated measures with simulta-
cortisol response revealed no significant main effect for attachment neous control of the other measures. To control for baseline differ-
security or attachment disorganization on the cortisol response. ences, the cortisol level before task was entered in a first step. In a
However, we found a significant interaction of time and attachment second step, the adolescents mean effective regulation and mater-
disorganization, F(1, 82) 4.21, p < .05, eta2 .049) on the corti- nal emotion support were entered (stepwise), and in the third step
sol response. Post-hoc t tests revealed that cortisol-values in adoles- early disorganization and the interaction term early disorganization
cents who had been disorganized in infancy did not, compared to  fear group were entered stepwise. As can be seen from Table 2,
non-disorganized ones, differ before the emotion-eliciting situa- the results show that after control for baseline cortisol, effective
tions but were significantly higher afterwards (p < .05, one-tailed, emotion regulation made a significant contribution with a signifi-
see Figure 2). cant increase in explained variance and, then, the interaction term
The talk show intends to elicit fear in the adolescent. Thus, we between attachment disorganization and fear made an additional
further explored the role of fear in the cortisol stress response. The significant contribution in explained variance. In the final model
adolescents were grouped into a low- and high-fear group by med- the beta was .18 (t 2.17, p < .05) for effective emotion regula-
ian split based on their self-rated fear. A time (pre, post), attach- tion and .24 (t 2.95, p < .01) for the interaction between attach-
ment disorganization (D, non-D), and fear (low, high) MANOVA ment disorganization and fear.
resulted in two significant two-way interactions between time and
attachment disorganization, F(1, 80) 5.76, p < .05, eta2 .07,
Discussion
and between fear and time on the cortisol response, F(1, 80) 6.58,
p < .05, eta2 .08, which were moderated by a three-way interaction The findings of this study clearly demonstrate a longitudinal influ-
between time, fear, and attachment disorganization, F(1, 80) 6.43, ence of infant attachment on emotional processes and emotional

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


8 International Journal of Behavioral Development

Table 2. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses: prediction of adolescents adrenocortical response.

Predictors beta R2 R2 F P

Step 1
Cortisol before (baseline) .609*** .371 .371 47.83 .000
Step 2
Maternal emotional support .116
Effective emotional regulation .186* .406 .034 4.64 .034
Step 3
Disorganization .179*
Fear .118 .448 .042 15.80 .000
Step 4
Disorganization  Fear .826* .488 .040 14.67 .000

Note. N 82. Final model: F(5, 77) 14.67, p < .001; the beta values are reported at the step of inclusion.

regulation in early adolescence. While early organized attachment to be reflected in their emotional expression as well as in the
security seems to be related to adolescents perception of own maternal perception of their feelings. In addition, mothers rated
emotions (feelings) and social emotional regulations strategies, the intensity of the adolescents emotions higher compared to the
attachment disorganization in infancy predicts psychobiological adolescents themselves.
processes of emotion regulation. These low or even missing associations between the adoles-
cents self-reported feelings and their emotional expression may
have different sources. From a theoretical point of view, low asso-
Emotion elicitation in the computer situation and in the ciations may reflect the different sub-systems of emotions with
talk show task their different functions (Campos et al., 1989). In addition, the ado-
lescents display rules in the video-taped situations or their difficul-
In line with recommended research strategies in emotion regulation
ties to openly admit negative emotions in the self-report
(Cole et al., 2004), we intended systematically to elicit negative
questionnaire might explain these low associations. Possible attach-
emotions, contrast different conditions, and assess the temporal
ment influences will be discussed below. Finally, from a methodo-
relations between emotion and emotion regulation strategy. We
logical perspective the rated time sequence might be different for
used two different situational contexts designed to elicit negative
adolescents, their mothers, and the observers. Self-ratings were
emotions in adolescents. A blocked goal was the intended elicitor
assessed after the emotion elicitation, whereas emotional expres-
of anger in the computer game task and an expected threat to the
sion was assessed during the tasks. Possibly, the adolescents
self should elicit fear in the talk show task. Mothers ratings of their
self-rating was more focused on the current feeling afterwards,
adolescents negative emotions, and to a smaller extent also the
while the maternal rating referred more to the preceding time period
adolescents self-ratings, support the validity of these two situa-
while interacting with their adolescent child leading to higher emo-
tional contexts as emotionally challenging situations. Thus, it can
tional intensity ratings by the mothers. Similar discrepancies
be assumed that the situations were suitable to observe emotion reg-
between emotion measures during an emotion regulation task have
ulation processes in adolescents (Adrian et al., 2011). Not all emo-
also been reported by Borelli, David, Crowley, and Mayes (2010) in
tional stressors also may activate the attachment system (Allen &
late childhood. Thus, in agreement with Mauss and Robinson
Miga, 2010), especially when they do not challenge the individuals
(2010), there seems to be little convergence between different emo-
emotion regulation capacity enough and when no attachment figure
tion measures for a single emotion, emphasizing the necessity of
is available. Therefore, we combined both the emotional challenge
multi-dimensional assessments of emotions.
and the presence of an attachment figure (mother) to make attach-
Regarding emotional regulation, the results clearly showed that
ment behaviour observable (Zimmermann et al., 2009).
the adolescents mainly used social emotion regulation strategies
while interacting with their mothers (about two thirds in each of the
Self-reported feelings, emotional expression, and two situations). In addition, social emotion regulation strategies
seem to be more effective. While in the talk show (fear elicitation),
emotion regulation in adolescence social emotion regulation was correlated with effective regulation,
The correlational pattern between the adolescents emotional during the anger situation social emotion regulation was more
expression, their self-reported feelings, and the maternal perception effective than individual emotion regulation in reducing the inci-
of their emotions indicated that the mothers seem to use the adoles- dence of anger episodes. Finally, in both situations, effective
cents emotional expression as basis for their interpretation of the emotion regulation in specific episodes was associated with a lower
adolescents emotional states. This was found for the adolescents incidence of negative emotion expressions. Thus, the findings sug-
anger as well as for fear. However, a significant association gest that social emotion regulation processes are quite important for
between the adolescents emotional expression and their self- effective regulation even in early adolescence.
reported feelings only appeared for anger. Similarly, only in the Adolescents emotion regulation in interaction with the mother
case of anger the adolescents reported feeling showed some over- cannot be seen independently of the emotional support concurrently
lap with the maternal perception of their feelings. Thus, there seems provided by the mothers. This was clearly observable in the talk
to be some, albeit modest, common variance of the emotion mea- show task. In dyads with emotionally highly-supporting mothers,
sures for anger, while feelings of fear in the adolescents seem not the adolescents were more effective in regulating their negative

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 9

emotions. As maternal emotional support was not directly related to difference between adolescent and mother ratings of adolescent
the emotion measures, the effect of maternal support seems to be feeling was smaller in securely attached adolescentmother pairs
mediated by the effectiveness of the adolescents regulation efforts than in insecurely attached pairs. Regarding the mothers as reli-
which significantly contributed to reduction of negative emotions. able observers of their childrens emotional state, these findings
Thus, adolescents still gain in effectiveness of emotion regulation indicate that adolescents with an early insecure attachment history
from the caregivers emotional support. feel more intense fear during this situation compared to securely
From a developmental perspective, we would expect increasing attached adolescents, but do not communicate their fear and uncer-
autonomous emotion regulation and self regulation competencies in tainty, neither by emotional expression nor in a questionnaire.
childhood and adolescence, compared to infancy or early childhood Regarding the prediction of emotional regulation by early
where emotion regulation may more dominantly lie in the caregiver attachment, our hypotheses were confirmed, at least in part. While
(Bell & Ainsworth, 1972; Matas et al., 1978; Schieche & Spangler, there was no effect of infant attachment security on the effective-
2005; Spangler et al., 1994). The findings of this study show that ness of emotion regulation, we found an effect on the proportion
adolescents, at least in situations in which the caregiver is available, of social emotion regulation. Adolescents with a history of early
still use the caregiver for emotional regulation. Thus, adolescents secure attachment more often used social emotional-regulation
may try to be autonomous and not to rely on attachment figures, but towards their caregiver compared to their insecurely attached coun-
they actually do rely if possible (Allen & Miga, 2010), when the terparts. This finding was indicated in both emotion-eliciting situa-
caregiver is emotional available. Moreover, the findings show that tions, however reached significance only in one of them.
social regulation of negative emotions with the concurrent support Taken together, the findings indicate restrictions in feelings of
of the caregiver is still adaptive even in adolescence, as it still anger and fear, and a discrepancy between the feelings and the
makes emotion regulation more effective. maternal perceptions and interpretation in emotionally challenging
situations in adolescents who had been insecurely attached in
infancy. Similar to findings in infancy (M. D. S. Ainsworth et al.,
Prediction of emotional expression and emotional 1978; Spangler & Schieche, 1998) or preschool (Waters et al.,
2010) individuals with a history of a secure attachment show an
regulation by early attachment
effective communication of their negative feelings still even in ado-
Bowlby (1980) emphasized the strong connection between attach- lescence, resulting in a higher concordance between emotional self-
ment and intense emotional processes. The results of this study perception and mother-perception. These longitudinal results
show that early attachment has this strong connection even over a expand empirical evidence from concurrent studies showing a
long developmental period of 11 years. The longitudinal analyses higher concordance between self-rating of negative emotions or
revealed significant effects of infant attachment security on the psychological symptoms in securely attached adolescents com-
adolescents feelings reported by themselves or reported by their pared to their observed emotional expression (Zimmermann
mothers, and also on social emotion regulation strategies. et al., 2001), their mimic reactions in the EMG (Spangler & Zim-
However, the findings differed depending on the situational con- mermann, 1999) or from parents ratings (Berger et al., 2005).
text and the related emotion. During the anger-eliciting situation, Similar to results in preschool (Lutkenhaus et al., 1985), where
mothers perceived increased anger in their adolescents independent infant attachment security did not predict the amount of the
of their attachment history in infancy. However, adolescents classi- expressed negative emotions after failure but whether this was
fied as securely attached in infancy reported more intense anger directed towards an adult (secure attachment) or away from an adult
than the insecurely attached ones. In addition, adolescents and (insecure attachment), we did not find associations between infant
mothers ratings were similar in early securely-attached pairs com- attachment and frequency of negative emotional expression. This is
pared to the early insecurely attached pairs validating the securely in line with a concurrent attachment measure at the age of 12 years
attached adolescents ratings. Thus, adolescents with a secure infant (Zimmermann et al., 2009). However, adolescents who had been
attachment history seem to be more aware of their angry feelings, securely attached in infancy showed more social emotion-
the appropriate emotion in a situation designed to provoke anger, regulation towards their mother than adolescents with an insecure
and they reported this openly in the self-report. In contrast, adoles- infant attachment. Attachment behaviour may change in form but
cents with insecure infant attachment may either have difficulties in not in function during development. Whereas infants mainly use
their access to their negative emotions (Cassidy, 1994) or present their repertoire of emotional expression as a communicative tool
themselves in a not affected way in the self-report. A closer com- towards their caregiver, adolescents in addition have elaborated
parison revealed a higher agreement between mother and adoles- verbal abilities to communicate their emotional needs. Attachment
cent concerning anger in the secure group which may facilitate behaviour develops from communicating emotion needs by emo-
sensitive regulation processes during interaction. In contrast, the tional expression and bodily proximity seeking as age-appropriate
high discrepancy in insecure adolescents, between themselves in infancy to the verbal communication and psychological
reporting only little anger and being perceived as angry by their proximity-seeking as age-appropriate in adolescence (Zimmer-
mothers, may make interactions between them and their mothers mann, 1999). Independent of the specific communicative tool,
more difficult and may hinder regulation processes. infant attachment influences the emotional communication towards
In the talk show (fear elicitation) the adolescents report of feel- the caregiver even later in adolescence.
ings as well as their emotional expression did not vary according to According to attachment theory, securely attached children are
early attachment security. This was in accordance with the associa- able to freely perceive and experience as well as to openly express
tions to concurrent attachment at age 12 (Zimmermann et al., 2009). and communicate both positive and negative feelings, as they have
However, the mothers of the adolescents with a history of insecure experienced their parents as sensitive and accepting and promptly
attachment perceived more fear and uneasiness in their children responding and providing emotional support (Cassidy, 1994; Laible
than the mothers of earlier securely attached adolescents. Again, the & Thompson, 1998). As securely attached childrens conversations

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


10 International Journal of Behavioral Development

with the caregivers are characterized by a focus on emotions or regulation, low maternal emotional support, and the adolescents
internal states (Meins et al., 2002; Raikes & Thompson, 2006), they early attachment disorganization.
may come to an enhanced understanding of emotions, their causes, Thus, interestingly and comparable to findings in early child-
and their functions in a given context. In contrast, restrictions in hood or adulthood (Lam, Dickerson, Zoccola, & Zaldivar, 2009),
emotional expression and communication of negative feelings can similar individual and social processes are involved in adrenocorti-
be expected in insecurely attached children, who, due to having cal regulation in early adolescence. Even at that age, we find indi-
experienced rejection and anger when expressing their feelings and cations that efficient emotional regulation on the behavioural level
basic emotional needs, are expected to show a restricted ability for prevents an adrenocortical stress response (see Nachmias et al.,
perception and communication of negative feelings. According to 1996; Spangler & Grossmann, 1993). In addition, comparable to
Cassidy (1994), insecure avoidantly attached children may suppress findings from early infancy (Gunnar, Larson, Hertsgaard, Harris,
their emotions and minimize emotional expression to avoid being & Brodersen, 1992; Spangler et al., 1994) sensitive emotional sup-
rejected, and they do not rely on the caregivers support for emo- port by the caregiver seems to contribute to adrenocortical regula-
tional regulation, while insecure ambivalently-attached children, tion as an external organizer also during adolescence.
cannot rely on the caregivers availability (Cassidy & Berlin, Adolescents with infant attachment disorganization showed an
1994) and try to get and maintain the caregivers attention by max- increase of adrenocortical activity compared to the non-
imization of emotional expression. A comparison of the two disorganized group. This indicates intense emotional stress during
insecure attachment groups (avoidant vs. ambivalent) did not seem these situations in this group indicating lowered competencies in
appropriate in this study because of the small sub-group sample adolescents who had been disorganized in infancy to effectively
sizes. On a descriptive level, the data of the two groups did not indi- regulate negative emotions. This finding is in agreement infant
cate huge differences between them. studies on the associations between adrenocortical activation and
While we were able to predict measures of feelings and emo- attachment disorganization when observed in emotionally
tional regulation from early attachment-security in this study, this challenging situations (Hertsgaard et al., 1995; Spangler & Gross-
was not the case for attachment disorganization. Disorganized chil- mann, 1993). According to a coping model, disorganized children
dren, due to having experienced frightened or frightening behaviour and adolescents do not have coherent strategies for emotional reg-
in attachment-related situations (Main & Solomon, 1990), or due to ulation in contact with the parent. Their restrictions in behavioural
a generally restricted ability for behavioural organization (Span- strategies in stressful situations may lead to heightened cortisol.
gler, Fremmer-Bombik, & Grossmann, 1996), may be overly The influence of early attachment disorganization on adrenocor-
alarmed by indications of negative emotions or may be hyper- tical activity is even more prominent when the adolescents are feel-
vigilant to negative emotions (Steele, Steele, & Croft, 2008). ing anxious. Comparable findings were reported by Schieche and
During preschool age, however, they tend to acquire a strategy of Spangler (2005). In a challenging task situation, disorganized tod-
controlling behaviour towards the caregiver to cope with their dis- dlers exhibited an increase in cortisol when they were behaviourally
organization (Main & Cassidy, 1988; Wartner, Grossmann, inhibited. Thus, with respect to Gunnar and Adam (2012), the acti-
Fremmer-Bombik, & Suess, 1994). The findings of this study did vation of the adrenocortical system is manifested, when there is
not indicate the expected restrictions in emotional regulation in subjective threat (high anxiety) combined with restricted emotion
adolescents with disorganized attachment history. The coding may regulation ability based on attachment disorganization.
not reflect the behavioural aspects of attachment disorganization as In this longitudinal study, attachment security could not predict
it did not include controlling behaviour. However, the adreno- the adrenocortical response, neither as a singular predictor nor in
cortical responses of adolescents with a history of disorganized interaction with the adolescents fear. This is in contrast to expec-
attachment in infancy may indicate that these adolescents have dif- tations from a coping model, and different from findings reporting a
ficulties in effective emotion regulation appearing at a biological heightened adrenocortical response in insecurely attached children
level, that are not obvious or not coded at the behavioural level in infancy (Nachmias et al., 1996; Spangler & Grossmann, 1993;
in the sense of a fast reduction of negative emotional intensity. Spangler & Schieche, 1998). However, Borelli and colleagues
(2010) did also not find associations with concurrent attachment
security and an adrenocortical response in older children. It may
Prediction of adrenocortical activity by early well be that the organized forms of attachment in infancy offer a devel-
opmental basis for the development of emotion regulation capacities
attachment that offer a behavioural strategy that remains functional to some extent
Gunnar and Adam (2012) argued in their review (with reference to for the chosen emotion-eliciting situations in this study. It may well be
Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004) that not all situations that provoke that the insecure attachment patterns, due to their organized structures
emotional responses necessarily may be suitable to provoke adreno- (Main & Solomon, 1990), enable these adolescents maintaining a
cortical activity. Rather, uncontrollable and unpredictable situa- behavioural organization at least to a certain degree, although, accord-
tions, with threats to the social self, would activate the HPA axis. ing to Main (1981), this may be second best strategies. However,
We assumed that the two challenging situations used for this study for adolescents with insecure infant attachment history, the effect of
would have the potential for a certain amount of social threat. While the emotion-elicitation situations is observable at the level of emotions
the adolescents in one of the situations were threatened by an unex- and communication of emotions. The breakdown of organized attach-
pected failure during the computer game, the talk show provided a ment strategies found in infant attachment disorganization seems to be
potential threatening of the self by expecting to have to present one- replicated in adolescence.
self in front of a public audience. The findings showed that in a sig- The final regression model showed that the adolescents effec-
nificant proportion of the adolescents there actually was an tive emotion regulation as observed in the tasks, as well as infant
adrenocortical response to the emotion eliciting situations, and that attachment disorganization, especially when concurrently being
this response was predicted by the adolescents ineffective emotion highly afraid, were the significant predictors of the adolescents

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 11

adrenocortical response. In contrast, contemporary maternal emo- References


tional support did not explain additional variance. Thus, both con- Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implications
current competencies in individual psychological emotion of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation
regulation as well as early acquired deficits in emotional regulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
within the attachment relationship influence the adrenocortical 110, 171197.
stress response. Although concurrent maternal emotional support Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Pat-
is associated with the adolescents emotion regulation, the adreno- terns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation.
cortical regulation depends more on the adolescents internalized Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
ability to regulate negative emotions. Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psy-
chologist, 44, 709716.
Allen, J. P., & Miga, E. M. (2010). Attachment in adolescence: A move
Conclusions and limitations to the level of emotion regulation. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships, 27, 181190.
In conclusion, these findings suggest that abilities and strategies of
Allen, J. P., Porter, M., McFarland, C., McElhaney, K. B., & Marsh, P.
emotional regulation developed on the basis of infant attachment
(2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents paternal
organization may have power to predict emotional regulation pro-
and peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior.
cesses even until early adolescence. While infant attachment secu-
Child Development, 78, 12221239.
rity seems to be associated with social emotion regulation processes
Becker-Stoll, F., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Wartner, U., Zimmermann, P.,
at the behavioural or psychological level, infant attachment disor-
& Grossmann, K. E. (2008). Is attachment at ages 1, 6 and 16 related
ganization seems to influence the psychobiological level of emo-
to autonomy and relatedness behavior of adolescents in interaction
tion regulation.
towards their mothers? International Journal of Behavioral Devel-
Clearly, limitations of this study must be taken into consider-
opment, 32, 372380.
ation, due to methodological issues. Although the sample size is not
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal
small compared to many longitudinal samples in attachment
responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 11711190.
research, it was too small to allow for multivariate analyses com-
Berger, L. E., Jodl, K. M., Allen, J. P., McElhaney, K. B., & Kuper-
bining attachment security and disorganization to detect interac-
minc, G. P. (2005). When adolescents disagree with others about
tions between these two dimensions. In addition, the effect sizes
their symptoms: Differences in attachment organization as an expla-
in the analyses of this study were quite small, indicating additional
nation of discrepancies between adolescent, parent, and peer reports
influences on variations in emotion regulation beside infant attach-
of behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 17,
ment. Furthermore, the two emotion-elicitation contexts were not
509528.
counterbalanced so that we cannot disentangle whether one of them
Bernard, K., & Dozier, M. (2010). Examining infants cortisol
contributes stronger to the cortisol effects, or whether the situations
responses to laboratory tasks among children varying in attachment
have an additive effect. Finally, the mothers emotionality should
disorganization: Stress reactivity or return to baseline? Develop-
be assessed in future studies using this paradigm and other social
mental Psychology, 46, 17711778. doi:10.1037/a0020660
interaction situations eliciting negative emotions and other interac-
Borelli, J. L., David, D. H., Crowley, M. J., & Mayes, L. C. (2010).
tion partners (e.g. fathers, peers) should be considered as well.
Links between disorganized attachment classification and clinical
Thus, extended replications are required.
symptoms in school-aged children. Journal of Child and Family
Despite these limitations, the longitudinal effects of infant attach-
Studies, 19, 243256.
ment on adolescents emotion regulation corroborated the concept on
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol 1: Attachment. New York,
coherence in development (Sroufe, 1989), where the effect of early
NY: Basic Books.
secure attachment is still observable in later effective social emotion
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, Vol 3: Loss, sadness and
regulation.
depression. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in
Funding attachment relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R.
This research was supported by the Koehler-Stiftung (Munich, Ger- Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clin-
many) and the German Research Foundation (SP 312/16-1 and ZI ical applications. (pp. 102127). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
511/13-1). Buss, K., & Goldsmith, H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy:
Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child
Development, 69, 359374.
Acknowledgements
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes
We want to thank Michael Schieche for organizing infancy data in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation.
assessment and Fabienne Becker-Stoll and Klaudia Kramer for Developmental Psychology, 25, 394402.
infant attachment analyses. Many thanks also to Clemens Kirsch- Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (2001). Personality development across the
baum and his laboratory at the University of Dresden for conduct- life course: The argument for change and continuity. Psychological
ing cortisol analyses. We are very indebted to Iris Reiner for Inquiry, 12(2), 4966.
organizing early adolescent data assessment, Stephanie Ast- Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment rela-
Scheitenberger, Melanie Pillhofer, and Julia Popp for conducting tionships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
analyses of maternal behavior, and Fatma Celik for coding emo- opment, 59, 228283.
tional expression and emotion regulation. Finally, we want to Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. J. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of
underline our gratitude to the families for their extraordinary coop- attachment: Theory and research. Child Development, 65, 971991.
eration over the years.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


12 International Journal of Behavioral Development

Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., Bush, M. A., & Gillespie, J. F. (1988). Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2012). Toward a new understanding of
Stage-salient issues: A transactional model of intervention. New legacy of early attachments for future antisocial trajectories: Evi-
Directions for Child Development, 39, 123145. dence from two longitudinal studies. Development and Psycho-
Coan, J. A. (2008). Toward a neuroscience of attachment. In J. Cassidy pathology, 24, 783806.
& P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, Kochanska, G., Woodard, J., Kim, S., Koenig, J. L., Yoon, J. E., &
and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 241265). New York, NY: Barry, R. A. (2010). Positive socialization mechanisms in secure
Guilford Press. and insecure parent-child dyads: Two longitudinal studies. Journal
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 9981009.
as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (1998). Attachment and emotional
for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317333. understanding in preschool children. Developmental Psychology,
De Castro, B. O., Slot, N. W., Bosch, J. D., Koops, W., & Veerman, J. 34, 10381045.
W. (2003). Negative feelings exacerbate hostile attributions of Lam, S., Dickerson, S. S., Zoccola, P. M., & Zaldivar, F. (2009). Emo-
intent in highly aggressive boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Ado- tion regulation and cortisol reactivity to a social-evaluative speech
lescent Psychology, 32, 5665. task. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 13551362.
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time.
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory Child Development, 71, 707720.
research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355391. Lutkenhaus, P., Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1985). Infant-
Erickson, M., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The relationship mother attachment at twelve months and style of interaction with
between quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool a stranger at the age of three years. Child Development, 56,
in a high-risk sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in 15381542.
Child Development, 50(1/2), 147166. Main, M. (1981). Avoidance in the service of attachment: A working
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., paper. In K. Immelmann, G. Barlow, L. Petrinovich & M. Main
Lapsley, A.-M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of (Eds.), Behavioral development: The Bielefeld interdisciplinary
insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of project (pp. 651693). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
childrens externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Categories of response to reunion with
Development, 81, 435456. the parent at age 6: Predictable from infant attachment classifica-
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attachment Inter- tions and stable over a 1-month period. Developmental Psychology,
view. Unpublished manuscript. 24, 415426.
Groh, A., Roisman, G., Booth-LaForce, C., Fraley, C. R., Owen, M., Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as
Cox, M., & Burchinal, M. (2013, April). Stability of attachment disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In
security from infancy to late adolescence. Paper presented at the M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attach-
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- ment during the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention
ment, Seattle. (pp. 121160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., & Unzner, Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. (1978). Continuity of adaption in the
L. (1985). Maternal sensitivity and newborns orientation responses second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and
as related to quality of attachment in Northern Germany. Mono- later competence. Child Development, 49, 547556.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Measures of emotion:
233256. A review. In J. de Houwer & D. Hermans (Eds.), Cognition and
Gunnar, M. R., & Adam, E. K. (2012). The hypothalamic-pituitary- emotion: Reviews of current research and theories (pp. 99127).
adrenocortical system and emotion: Current wisdom and future New York, NY: Psychology Press.
directions. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E.,
Development, 77, 109119. & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment
Gunnar, M. R., Larson, M. C., Hertsgaard, L., Harris, M. L., & Brodersen, security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Devel-
L. (1992). The stressfulness of separation among nine-month-old opment, 73, 17151726.
infants: Effects of social context variables and infant temperament. Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., & Parritz, R. H. (1996).
Child Development, 63, 290303. Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of
Hertsgaard, L., Gunnar, M., Erickson, M. F., & Nachmias, M. (1995). attachment security. Child Development, 67, 508522.
Adrenocortical responses to the Strange Situation in infants with Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2006). Family emotional climate,
disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships. Child Develop- attachment security and young childrens emotion knowledge in a
ment, 66, 11001106. high risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The Trier 24, 89104.
Social Stress TestA tool for investigating psychobiological Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic inte-
stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, gration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81, 622.
7681. Scherer, K. S., Mortillaro, M., & Mehu, M. (2013) Understanding the
Kobak, R. R., Cole, H. E., Ferenz-Gillies, R., Fleming, W. S., & mechanisms underlying the production of facial expression of emo-
Gamble, W. (1993). Attachment and emotion regulation during tion: A componential perspective. Emotion Review, 5, 4753.
mother-teen problem solving: A control theory analysis. Child Schieche, M., & Spangler, G. (2005). Individual differences in biobeha-
Development, 64, 231245. vioral organization during problem-solving in toddlers: The influence
Kobak, R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Work- of maternal behavior, infant-mother attachment, and behavioral
ing models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. inhibition on the attachment-exploration balance. Developmental
Child Development, 59, 135146. Psychobiology, 46, 293306.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015


Spangler and Zimmermann 13

Spangler, G., Fremmer-Bombik, E., & Grossmann, K. (1996). Social Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L.
and individual determinants of infant attachment security and disor- (2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A
ganization. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 127139. twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 71, 684689.
Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. (1999). Individual and physiological Waters, S. F., Virmani, E. A., Thompson, R. A., Meyer, S., Raikes, H.
correlates of attachment disorganization in infancy. In J. Solomon A., & Jochem, R. (2010). Emotion regulation and attachment:
& C. George (Eds.), Attachment disorganization (pp. 95124). Unpacking two constructs and their association. Journal of Psycho-
New York, NY: Guilford Press. pathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 3747.
Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from
in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, infancy to early adulthood in a high-Risk sample: Continuity, dis-
64, 14391450. continuity, and their correlates. Child Development, 71, 695702.
Spangler, G., & Schieche, M. (1998). Emotional and adrenocortical Westenberg, P. M., Gullone, E., Bokhorst, C. L., Heyne, D. A., & King,
responses of infants to the strange situation: The differential func- N. J. (2007). Social evaluation fear in childhood and adolescence:
tion of emotional expression. International Journal of Behavioral Normative developmental course and continuity of individual dif-
Development, 22, 681706. ferences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25,
Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Ilg, U., Maier, U., & Ackermann, C. (1994). 471483.
Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral Zimmermann, P. (1999). Structure and functions of internal working
regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 425437. models of attachment and their role for emotion regulation. Attach-
Spangler, G., & Zimmermann, P. (1999). Attachment representation ment & Human Development, 1, 291306.
and emotion regulation in adolescents: A psychobiological per- Zimmermann, P., Becker-Stoll, F., Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E.,
spective on internal working models. Attachment & Human Devel- Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Wartner, U. (2000). Langsschnittliche
opment, 1, 270290. Bindungsentwicklung von der fruhen Kindheit bis zum Jugendalter
Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and maladaptation: [Longitudinal attachment development from infancy through ado-
Psychopathology as developmental deviation. In D. Cicchetti (Ed.), lescence]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 47, 99117.
The emergence of a discipline: Rochester symposium on develop- Zimmermann, P., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Spangler, G., & Grossmann,
mental psychopathology (pp. 1340). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence K. E. (1997). Attachment in Adolescence: A longitudinal perspec-
Erlbaum Associates. tive. In W. Koops, J. B. Hoeksma & D. C. van den Boom (Eds.),
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational Development of interaction and attachment: Traditional and
construct. Child Development, 48, 11841199. non-traditional approaches (pp. 271280). Amsterdam, The Neth-
Steele, H., Steele, M., & Croft, C. (2008). Early attachment predicts erlands: North-Holland.
emotion recognition at 6 and 11 years old. Attachment and Human Zimmermann, P., Gliwitzky, J., & Becker-Stoll, F. (1996). Bindung und
Development, 10, 379393. Freundschafts-beziehungen im Jugendalter [Attachment and
Suess, G. J., Grossmann, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Effects of infant friendship-relations in adolescence]. Psychologie in Erziehung und
attachment to mother and father on quality of adaptation in pre- Unterricht, 43, 141154.
school: From dyadic to individual organization of self. International Zimmermann, P., & Grossmann, K. E. (1997). Attachment and adapta-
Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 4365. tion in adolescence. In W. Koops, J. B. Hoeksma & D. C. van den
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of def- Boom (Eds.), Development of interaction and attachment: Tradi-
inition. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Develop- tional and non-traditional approaches (pp. 271280). Amsterdam,
ment, 59(23), 2552. The Netherlands: North-Holland.
Thompson, R. A. (2008). Early attachment and later development: Zimmermann, P., Maier, M. A., Winter, M., & Grossmann, K. E.
Familiar questions, new answers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (2001). Attachment and adolescents emotion regulation during a
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 348365). New York, NY: joint problem-solving task with a friend. International Journal of
Guilford Press. Behavioral Development, 25, 331343.
Wartner, U. G., Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., & Suess, G. Zimmermann, P., Mohr, C., & Spangler, G. (2009). Genetic and attach-
(1994). Attachment patterns at age six in south Germany: Predict- ment influences on adolescents regulation of autonomy and aggres-
ability from infancy and implications for preschool behavior. Child siveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50,
Development, 65, 10141027. 13391347.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at TEXAS SOUTHERN UNIVERSITY on January 3, 2015

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen