Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Physiology and Behavior, Vol. 6, pp. 577-588. Pergamon Press, 1971.

Printed in Great Britain

THEORETICAL REVIEW
The Nature and Determinants of Adjunctive Behavior'
J O H N L. F A L K
Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, N.J. 08903, U.S.A.

(Received 25 N o v e m b e r 1970)

FALK,J. L. The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. PHYSIOL.BEHAV. 6 (5) 577--588, 1971--A review of
the various behaviors induced as adjuncts to the behaviors under direct schedule control is given. The similarities
among these adjunctive behaviors are described in terms of their intensities, temporal loci, and the variables which
produce them. The relation of adjunctive behavior to displacement activities, adjustive, and toxic behavior is discussed.
The general nature of adjunctive behavior is described in terms of a formal definition.
Adjunctive behavior Ingestive behavior Displacement activities Polydipsia Pica Aggression
Activity Schedule control

SCHEDULE-INDUCED POLYDIPSIA: AN ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR unusual finding--it was an outright absurd one. It was
PROTOTYPE absurd because food deprivation in rats yields a decrease in
water intake, not an increase. It was absurd because heating
WHeN a rat is reduced to about 80 per cent of its free-feeding a large quantity of room-temperature water to body heat and
body weight by limiting daily food intake and allowed to expelling it as copious urine is wasteful for an animal already
earn most of its food by lever-pressing on a variable-interval pressed for energy stores by food deprivation. It is absurd
one-rain schedule for 45-rag Noyes food pellets, a curious for an animal to drink itself into a dilutional hyponatremia
phenomenon occurs when water is concurrently available. bordering on water intoxication. But perhaps most absurd
Although the animal is never deprived of water during the was not the lack of a metabolic or patho-regulatory reason
daily lever-pressing session (3.17 hr) for intermittent food, for the polydipsia, but the lack of an acceptable behavioral
the water intake amounts to almost one-half the body weight account. That is, the behavior is absurd in the sense of
[9]. The behavior pattern is evident from the session shown philosophical existentialism. Now most animals are probably
in Fig. 1. The upper (cumulative) channel shows the lever- not philosophical existentialists, but perhaps we have run
pressing performance; the point at which each food pellet was across a class of behaviors in animals which was imputed to
earned is indicated by a short vertical hatch mark. The lower man as his exclusive, ff obscure, property.
(event) channel indicates each twelfth lick at a water tube as a A previous publication has outlined the available evidence
pen deflection. When a food pellet was earned it was quickly against interpreting schedule-induced polydipsia as an
consumed and immediately followed by about a 0.5 ml instance of various conventional behavioral effects [16]. To
draught of water. Fourteen females weighing about 200 g summarize briefly, the polydipsia is not the result of food
drank an average of 92.5 ml of water per session. Little or no delivery directly or adventitiously reinforcing water intake.
water was consumed during the almost 21 hr between sessions, N o r does it serve a problem-solving mediational, or timing
although it was freely available in the animal's home cage. function. Furthermore, drinking is not an unconditioned
This pattern of excessive drinking has been repeatedly response to eating. It is important to note that the relation
confirmed. It is not specific to one strain of rats and has been between scheduled eating and the resultant polydipsia is
observed in the rhesus monkey [41], chimpanzee (Kelleher, not one of elicitation. The stimulus-response relation of a
personal communication), and pigeon [45] as well. salivary or pupillary reflex is relatively invariant with respect
About a decade ago a rather fortuitous experimental to the complex of environmental conditions and consequences.
arrangement enabled the discovery of the phenomenon of But schedules do not elicit polydipsia, or other adjunctive
schedule-induced polydipsia. From the context of research behaviors, in such a predetermined fashion. Given the
on the control of fluid intake in the rat the production of proper environmental circumstances, certain schedules can
polydipsia as a food-schedule by-product was not only an increase the probability of particular behaviors enormously.

tSupported by Grant MH 18409 from the National Institute of Mental Health, National Center for Prevention and Control of Alcoholism
and Grant AM 14180 from National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Arthritis and Metabolic Diseases. This paper is a slightly
revised version of the one presented at the Conference on Schedule-Induced and Schedule-Dependent Behavior, University of Toronto,
27-29 May, 1970.
577
578 FALK

w
R)I/II c

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . m m.. ~ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

FIG. 1. Polydipsia occurring concurrent with a variable-interval one-min schedule for 45-mg
food pellets. Upper (cumulative) channel shows lever-pressing on food schedule, and food
deliveries as short, vertical hatch marks. Lower channel shows every 12th lick on water tube
as deflection.

But these behaviors are not elicited or generated as new One offers up a new class of behavior with some trepida-
responses by the schedule conditions. Polydipsia takes at tion. The multiplication of new behavior classifications has,
least a few sessions to develop completely, unlike elicited for the most part, contributed little to the advancement of
responses, and this acquisition is not simply due to adaption basic problems in behavior analysis. A new classification
to the feeding schedule [37]. should be more than just suggestive of new ways of viewing
Thus, a decade of research has yielded no traditional known facts. It must prove itself useful not only in reorganiz-
physiological or behavioral explanation for schedule-induced ing current data, but in making certain fresh lines of investiga-
polydipsia. Attempts to account for the behavior as an tion compellingly obvious. Such studies should either bear
emotional side-effect of schedules, or as the animal's way out the generality of the suggested scheme or render it
of producing emotional pacification are not necessarily unconfirmed.
wrong, but they are largely untestable notions. To this end, some additional behavioral phenomena will be
described which seem to be examples of adjunctive behavior
generated by intermittent food schedules. The following
SOME BEHAVIORS GENERATED AS ADJUNCTS TO
section will then attempt to indicate certain correspondences
SCHEDULE CONTROL
among these behaviors.
The conditions which produce schedule-induced polydipsia
are not complex. The experimental design is simple, yet the
behavioral effect is strong and durable. It has been indicated
Schedule-induced Aggression and Escape
that: "Whenever a simple operation is found to exert a While behaviorists often consider attack and escape
powerful behavioral effect, we may suspect that the phenome- behaviors as quite different and even antithetic responses,
non can be widely generalized" [46]. It was previously ethologists classify them together with threat and appease-
pointed out that another behavioral phenomenon, S-delta- ment postures and movements, as agonistic behaviors. This
induced aggression reported by Azrin and his associates, classification stems from the recognition that all of these
stands in the same relation as polydipsia to its generating behavioral outcomes can result from essentially the same
schedule [12, 13]. It was suggested that when certain schedules stimulus situation and behavior may vaseillate rapidly from
induce extra, concurrent phenomena which are strong enough one to another. Behaviorists have made little headway with
to sustain scheduled behavior in their own right, that these threat and appeasement responses, but the environmental
phenomena be called adjunctive behaviors [12, 13]. There are commonalities producing both attack and escape have
certain correspondences in the initiating conditions of schedule- received recent attention in a series of experiments.
induced polydipsia and displacement activities which suggest Electric shock and other forms of aversive stimulation
that these are instances of a more general behavioral pheno- have been used for a number of years to study escape and
menon [16]. It is the purpose of this paper to expand further avoidance behaviors. Lately, several of these same aversive
the behavioral phenomena and their correspondences in stimuli have been shown to produce attack in paired animals
order to provide an empirical and analytic context for the [54]. Certain ranges and parameters of unconditioned
recognition of the new class of behaviors termed adjunctive. aversive stimuli can produce avoidance, escape or attack
DETERMINANTS OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR 579

as a function of the environmental context. Meanwhile, of licking for a length of time equal to a normal scheduled-
more complex conditioned aversive stimuli were being food session (Mendelson and Chillag, personal communica-
investigated. It was shown that time out from schedules of tion).
positive reinforcement could function as an aversive stimulus
[17]. Also, it was found that when periods during which CORRESPONDENCES AMONG ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIORS
each key peek delivered grain to a pigeon were alternated with It is one thing to assert that a group of behavioral pheno-
periods of extinction, the onset of the extinction periods mena appearing in conjunction with food schedules are
produced vigorous attack on a nearby restrained pigeon [4]. analogous side-effects of schedules, but it is quite another
This result indicated that the onset of the extinction (time matter to maintain that these side effects form a class (adjunc-
out) period was an aversive condition producing aggression tive behavior) the members of which yield similar dynamic
just as various unconditioned aversive stimuli did. N o t only properties and are functions of the same variables. In this
extinction but other aspects of intermittent food schedules can section accordingly, the current evidence linking various
produce aggression in primates and pigeons as an adjunct to adjunctive behaviors into a functional group is reviewed by
the food schedule. Biting attack in squirrel monkeys occurs noting commonalties both in the controlling variables and
not only after transition to an extinction component, but the behavioral outcomes.
also as an adjunct to various fixed-ratio schedules during the
pauses between completed ratios [26]. Similar results were
obtained in pigeons with a fixed-ratio 50 schedule [22] and
Effective Range in Consummatory Rate
with various fixed-time schedules in which food was presented The adjunctive drinking phenomenon is determined by
non-contingently [18]. several variables, but the one of prime importance involves
Schedules of food reinforcement can induce not only the length of time between eating episodes. This is specified
adjunctive aggression, but also escape. Both pigeons and by the interreinforcement time in schedules of reinforcement,
rats will terminate certain schedules of reinforcement by and by the interfeeding time in noncontingent, fixed-time
producing time out periods [2, 49]. This termination (escape) schedules in which the episodic delivery of food is not con-
behavior appears to be produced by the aversive properties tingent upon a reinforced operant response. Reinforcement
of certain aspects of schedules of positive reinforcement. schedules, other than rather small fixed or variable ratio
This phenomenon will be referred to as schedule-induced schedules, take time to complete and produce various
escape. intermittence values in feeding. These schedule-enforced
Thus, as a function of certain food-schedule parameters intermittences necessarily decrease the rate of consummatory
both attack and escape behaviors can be induced as adjuncts behavior and give it an episodic character when compared to
to the behavior under schedule control. It appears that the similarly deprived animals allowed to consume the same
common origin of these adjunctive behaviors lies in the aver- daily food ration in a continuous feeding bout.
sive properties of the food schedule parameters. The degree of polydipsia as a function of the fixed-interval
length between food pellets was systematically explored
([13]; Fig. 2). As the fixed-interval value was increased from
Schedule-induced Pica 2 see through values up to 300 see, the amount of water drunk
When Rhesus monkeys at approximately 80 per cent of increased linearly up to some maximum point, but fell off
their normal free-feeding body weights were placed on a at fixed interval 300 see to a scarcely polydipsic value. This
fixed-time 15-min schedule for food pellets, pellet ingestion general relation was observed again in later work [16]. In a
was quickly followed by a bout of ingesting wood shavings recent study, the value of the fixed-interval schedule was
which lay on the floor of the living space [56]. These animals varied over a range from 1 to 480 see and a quite similar
did not ingest this material even though food-deprived bitonic relation was obtained [20]. It was also noted in
until placed on the intermittent feeding schedule. While fixed-time schedules that the percentage of pellets followed
some of the shavings were stored in their cheek pouches, a by drinking decreased at comparably long interpellet times
good deal of it was ingested. Animals were observed selecting [43]. The bitonic relation found in the previously-described
the largest and freshest pieces available for consumption. response-contingent temporal schedules was confirmed in a
Under extinction conditions such ingestion rapidly fell to zero. variable-time schedule [23].
Short fixed-interval lengths were carefully explored in
order to determine the intermittence threshold of polydipsia
Schedule-induced Wheel Running [20]. This proved to be about fixed-interval 4 see for two
Rats held to 80 per cent of their normal body weights by rats and 5 see in the case of two other rats. At these points, the
food restriction did much more wheel-running concurrent with ratio of fixed-interval session water intake to baseline water
a variable-interval one-min food schedule than with fixed- intake reached a value of about 2--unequivocal polydipsia.
ratio 1 [30]. The running rapidly declined under extinction It appears, then, that for rats at 80 per cent of their normal
conditions. free-feeding body weight, a fixed-interval food schedule of
45-rag Noyes pellets yields a polydipsia at F I 4 or 5 see and
this polydipsia increases up to approximately F I 2 or 3 rain.
Schedule-induced Air Licking A t larger F I values the polydipsia progressively falls off to
Rats held to 85 per cent of their normal body weights by food lower values. There is a range of fixed-interval values which
restriction were placed on a fixed-time one-min schedule for produces a strong polydipsic effect with the effect decreasing
food pellets with a continuous airstream available from a as one approaches low or high F I values on either side of this
drinking tube in place of water [33]. Protracted post-pellet effective range. It seems likely that the critical defining
licking of the airstream developed. The level of licking was feature of the effective range is not the time between the
far in excess of that produced by giving an animal a com- eating episodes as such, but rather the rate of consummatory
parable amount of food all at once and recording the amount behavior determined by this temporal parameter in concert
H
580 FALK

IO0 m ~1

RAT 1-10 ~ /~'"" \


80 - - MEAN RUNNING WT. = 14 9.7g ./,/ o t
/ ¢. ~.

--- ( " §2"/// }~


O---~-BOX
o ..,,,"" o 'P - - " - - ' H O M E CAGE
z ~-" t,

,~ i PRE-EXPERIMENTAL /"~
~¢ ! 24-hr WATER INTAKE ~,#
ON FREE FEEDING IV IPB

,1. t[I,-'111 •
l]lji."IP • 4
lO • •
• ,,, ,

O0 ZO 40 60 80 I00 120 140 160 i80 300


FIXED INTERVAL (SECONDS)
FIG. 2. Polydipsia as a function of length of food fixed-interval schedule. Food limited to 180 pellets (45 mg each)
for all sessions. Copyright 1966 by the Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Inc.

with the food portion magnitude at each reinforcement. on schedule-induced escape have not been specifically
By varying the portion magnitude as well as interreinforce- designed to search for an effective range.
ment time, results obtained supported the notion that rate of
consummatory behavior was the factor determining the
Deprivation Level
effective range [15, 16], but not all results entirely support
this interpretation [20]. Further work is required to clarify Since the occurrence and the degree of adjunctive behavior
exactly what schedule parameters can account for the bitonic is closely related to the degree of intermittence in consum-
function, but all research agrees that this function exists. The matory feeding responses, it seems reasonable to suppose
region of the function where the adjunctive drinking approach- that interfering with strong feeding responses would induce
es the maximal point from either side is called the effective stronger adjunctive behavior than interfering with weak
range. If the notion of a general class of adjunctive behaviors feeding responses. In polydipsia studies, the animals typically
is supportable, then other such adjunctive behaviors should are held at 80 per cent of their normal free-feeding body
yield similar functions with their own effective ranges. weights by limiting their daily food intake. This procedure
In studies dealing with schedule-induced aggression, only ensures that feeding response-strength remains high through-
one [18] has specifically explored aggression as a function of out each daily session. Consequently, feeding intermittences
interfeeding time in fixed-time schedules. As the fixed-time imposed upon this baseline would be expected to induce
values were increased from 15 see through several values to relatively strong adjunctive drinking. The strength of the
960 sec, attacks increased up to I or 2 min and then fell to feeding responses can, of course, be progressively weakened
low levels at the longer times. The confirmation of an effective by allowing the body weight to drift upward. If this is
range for schedule-induced aggression in the pigeon lends accomplished in a slow, controlled fashion, animals will
some generality to this notion. Other studies have explored still meet the contingencies of schedules of reinforcement and
parts of what is probably the rising limb of the function. consume the delivered food with alacrity. In one experiment,
Increasing the size of food-reinforced fixed-ratios greatly bar-pressing in rats was reinforced on a fixed-interval 90 sec
increased biting attacks in squirrel monkeys, while decreasing schedule with Noyes pellets and the usual strong polydipsic
the fixed-ratio to its former value decreased the attacks [26]. response was induced. When the post-session food supple-
Similarly, pigeons attacked during the pause before a fixed- ment allowed was increased so that their body weights
ratio 100, but not before a fixed ratio 25, in a multiple F R 100, slowly increased over a three-week period, adjunctive drink-
F R 25 food schedule [19]. ing remained at the same high level until the weights reached
In schedule-induced escape, the time spent in self-imposed about 95 per cent of the free-feeding weight. As the weights
time out was an increasing function of the size of the food- increased through the 95-105 per cent range, polydipsia
reinforced fixed ratio both for pigeons [2] and rats [49]. decreased progressively and linearly to about 20 per cent of
Pigeons will escape from schedules other than fixed-ratio. its original value (Fig. 3). It is important to note that while
Escape occurred from the extinction component of a multiple the adjunctive drinking was decreasing as a function of body
variable-interval 30 see, extinction schedule, and from the weight increases, the rate of bar pressing did not substantially
variable-interval 5 rain component of a multiple VI 30 sec, decrease until about the 104 or 105 per cent body weight level
VI 5 rain schedule [38]. Thus far data on schedule-induced was attained. Thus, animals can show well-controlled
escape do not suggest the existence of a bitonic function. schedule behavior and be consuming the food pellets in spite
Only a rising limb has been demonstrated in connection with of slowly-instituted body weight increases, yet the adjunctive
increasing fixed-ratio size. On the other hand, experiments drinking reveals marked, concomitant attenuation [16].
DETERMINANTS OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR 581

generation of adjunctive behavior. Even though the con-


o 8 summatory activity unfailingly occurs and the appetitive
'2°t °
IlO
o
0. sequence (operant pattern) remains at its customary rate,
O the operations must define a rather intense deprivation
IOOI-~ 8 ° state in order for adjunctive behavior to be induced in its
o
• 8Qo° fully-developed, exaggerated form.
• 8
Excessive Aspect of Adjunctive Behavior
70 The amount of water drunk during variable-interval one-
o rain sessions lasting a little longer than 3 hr is almost 10
o
6o o times the amount drunk when the same number of 45-mg
0000 Noyes pellets is given all at once and water intake over the
o Rot 61 O
ensuing 3.5 hr is noted [15]. The state of deprivation (80
oRot 62 per cent of the free-feeding body weight) and the type and
40
amount of food are the same in both cases, yet the concomitant
FI 90Sec water intake yields about a tenfold difference. The absolute
180 Pellets amount and rate o f fluid intake is, of course, excessive. The
bottom of the experimental chamber below the stainless-
I0 steel bars the animal stands on is awash with dilute urine by
Jill l,IT1 IIIII lllll 11111 1 the end of each daily session, unless this output is minimized
80 85 90 95 I00 105 by a presession dose of vasopressin [11]. It would be interest-
PERCENT FREE-FEEDING BOOY WEIGHT ing, but not especially noteworthy, if the animals simply took
FIG. 3. Adjunctive drinking as a function of body weight. Body a little water after consuming each pellet but did not consume
weight allowed to increase from 80 per cent of free-feeding weight an overall excessive amount of fluid. Such a phenomenon
to 104 or 105 per cent. Ann. N.Y. Acad. ScL 157, Art. 2, Fig. 7, could be simply explained away as prandial drinking. But
p 579, J. L. Falk. © The New York Academy of Sciences; 1969. it is primarily the persistent and excessive aspect of the drink-
Reprinted by permission. ing which commands our attention.
The aggressive responses induced as adjuncts to various
There is an important implication for adjunctive behavior in food schedules also display persistent and excessive aspects.
this observation. The maintenance of operant response rates The attack topographies are strong and typically do not
and eating are not sufficient conditions for inducing strong diminish as a function of repeated evocation. In the pigeon
adjunctive behavior sequences. they are described as "strong pecks at the throat and head
As body weight increases beyond the 95 per cent point, one of the target bird, especially around the eyes. The feathers
of the required conditions for the production of adjunctive of the target bird were often pulled out and the skin bruised"
drinking becomes increasingly attenuated. This required [4]. Such attacks are strong enough to produce repeated
condition is perhaps the strength of feeding behavior, but the injury to restrained target birds. Fortunately, pigeons will
experiment does not directly measure this factor. Nonetheless, often attack taxidermically prepared pigeon models used in
given sudden unlimited access to food, one would expect the place of live target birds. The attacks are so forceful that one
probability of eating to decline more quickly in the animal investigator found it necessary to cover the head and throat
starting from the higher "per cent free-feeding weight" areas of the model with closely cropped white rabbit fur to
value. When the post-session food supplements were omitted prevent its destruction [18].
in the present experiment, driving the body weights progressive- In initial control conditions, pigeons were placed in the
ly back down to 80 per cent, adjunctive drinking progressively experimental chamber for several daily sessions prior to any
increased and returned to its former value. history of reinforcement by food in order to ascertain the
If comparable manipulations of body weight in connection baseline level of attack duration [4]. It is important to note
with other schedule-induced behaviors result in similar that on the first day most pigeons had high attack durations.
changes in the rates of these behaviors, it would imply that On succeeding days, these attack durations decreased to near-
deprivation degree is one direct determinant of a class of zero levels for all birds. Thus, the initial response to the situa-
possible adjunctive behaviors. That is, it would indicate tion was a fairly high operant attack rate. This attack rate
that the relation between body weight alterations and poly- became excessively high, in comparison with even the first-
dipsia is not peculiar to this particular adjunctive behavior. session values, when the food schedule was imposed. Similar
Recently, a similar relation between body weight changes and relations also held in another study [22].
schedule-induced air licking has been observed (Chillag and With respect to the phenomenon of schedule-induced
Mendelson, in press). When weight changes were imposed escape, one cannot obtain baseline consummatory rates of
upon rats exposed to a one-min fixed time schedule for 45-rng behavior as in the case of drinking or attack. But this is not
Noyes pellets, the degree of adjunctive air licking was an a serious drawback in estimating the excessive aspect of the
inverse function of per cent free-feeding body weight. The escape behavior. Any escape from a food or water schedule
effects of varying body weight remain to be explored for by an appropriately deprived animal is, in a real sense,
schedule-induced attack. However, in food-satiated pigeons excessive. Furthermore, the excessive time spent in time out
attack durations decreased to low levels [4]. This operation from positive reinforcement was shown to increase systematic-
is comparable to allowing the body to rise 100 per cent or ally as a function of F R size [2, 49].
greater which markedly attenuates both polydipsia and air In the case of schedule-induced wheel running [30],
licking. excessive running was shown to occur in conjunction with a
Simply imposing an effective range intermittence value on variable interval one-minute schedule compared to the running
consummatory rate is not a sufficient condition for the which occurred with an F R 1 schedule, or in extinction.
582 FALK

Schedule-induced pica, the ingestion of wood shavings, the general observation that adjunctive responses occur in the
was consistently observed in monkeys on a fixed-time 15 min immediate post-reinforcement period. A trivial case of this is
schedule [56]. It was not seen at fixed-time 5 rain or in where a second pellet can be obtained shortly after a first
extinction. Thus, the pica is excessive relative to the near- pellet is earned, such as an F R 5 always following a D R L 30
zero rates at the shorter interval and in the extinction con- see component [16]. Here, adjunctive drinking occurs after
dition. the second pellet and not the first, since a favourable food
In the schedule-induced air licking situation, the amount contingency successfully competes with the drinking response.
of air licking was much increased on a fixed-time one-rain Of greater importance are cases in which the adjunctive
schedule compared to rats given the same number of food behavior migrates away from the immediate post-reinforce-
pellets all at once at the beginning of the session (Mendelson ment period. With large fixed-ratio components (FR 60 and
and Chillag, personal communication). 120), attacking occurred in episodes such that the ratio
Thus, in all cases of schedule-induced phenomena, the performance was interrupted, perhaps as a function of ratio
induced behavior not only occurs at predictable times in strain [29]. With fixed-interval schedules on the order of
relation to the feeding events, but such behaviors occur in 3 rain or so, drinking is sometimes distributed throughout
quantitative excess as a function of schedule intermittence the interval in a series of shorter drinking bursts, rather than
relative to their base rates of occurrence when comparable in the typical, sustained single burst. Also drinking is not
consummatory behaviors are not paced by an intermittent initiated in the immediate post-reinforcement period on
schedule. these longer fixed-interval schedules (Falk, unpublished
observations). Quite similar effects have been noted on
fixed-time (noncontingent) schedules [43]. A recent paper
Temporal Locus of Adjunctive Behavior
described the case of one rat in which the usual polydipsic
Adjunctive behavior typically occurs in the immediate pattern on F I 3 min gradually changed its time distribution
post-reinforcement period. In the polydipsic case, a burst of and when the schedule was changed to F I 4 min the drinking
drinking ensues immediately after each pellet is consumed pattern took on F I scallop characteristics [42]. In spite of the
[9]. This occurs on a variety of interval and ratio schedules complex experimental history of this animal, it is entirely
[10, 13]. While it is tempting to view this drinking burst as a possible that given the proper sequence of conditions schedule-
fluid-intake response to meal termination, adjunctive be- induced polydipsia can be transformed into operant drinking.
haviors other than polydipsia occur in the immediate post- In fact, it would be surprising if adjunctive behaviors, which
reinforcement period when water is not present. monopolize such a large segment of session time did not
In pigeons, attack rates were high in the immediate sometimes make contact with the contingencies programmed.
post-reinforcement period where the schedule changed from Perhaps one of the most striking ways in which this takes
F R 1 to extinction [4]. Attack rates were a decreasing function place is in the modulation of elicited behavior [35].
of time from the start of the post-reinforcement period
[4, 29]. Similarly, squirrel monkeys on fixed-ratio food sched-
ules showed biting attack during the post-reinforcement Response Contingency Not Crucial to the Generation of
pause or early in the ratio run [26]. The attack sequences in Adjunctive Behavior
pigeons on fixed-ratio schedules also occur mainly in the Since it is conceivable that the excessive degree of schedule-
post-reinforcement period [22]. induced polydipsia could be a displacement from an operant
In schedule-induced escape situations, the time outs occur system which is momentarily at low probability (lever
not directly after food, but just before responding on fixed- pressing) to another activity, it is worth ascertaining whether
ratio is initiated again [2]. In a mult VI 30 see, ext schedule, the omission of a response contingency makes a major
more time outs occur in the first half of the extinction com- difference. To this end, a noncontingent variable-interval
ponent (4 rain presentation) than in the second half [38]. one-min schedule (viz. a variable-time one min schedule)
Likewise, most of the time outs were taken in the first half of was used to deliver pellets to animals irrespective of their
the VI 5 rain component of a mult VI 30 sec, VI 5 min behavior [10]. This made no appreciable difference in the
schedule. level of polydipsia (see also [16]).
Where food pellets are delivered noncontingently each rain, Likewise, the typical attack responses occurred when a
schedule-induced air licking is initiated shortly after pellet period of free feeding was interrupted by a no-food period in
consumption, but often continues throughout the entire pigeons which had never been reinforced for key pecking [4].
interval [33]. These results showed "that the pecking attacks were not
In schedule-induced pica in monkeys, the consumption of simply a 'displacement' of the conditioned key-pecking
wood shavings is a post-pellet event [56]. responses" [4]. Fixed-time schedules of food presentation
Schedule-induced wheel running is initiated shortly after have been used in additional research [18].
pellet consumption [30]. Both the schedule-induced pica and the schedule-induced
Thus, in most cases, the adjunctive behavior occurs as an air licking experiments were done with fixed-time schedules
event following rather closely upon reinforcement. The main [33, 56].
exceptions to this seem to involve attacking or escaping close Thus, given an appropriate deprivation level, various
to the initiation of large fixed-ratios, or in the early segments schedule-induced phenomena seem to be determined primarily
of such ratios. The initiating factors for the adjunctive by the rate of food consumption, rather than by some
behaviors in such cases may involve certain aversive pro- feature of response contingency defining a schedule of re-
perties of large fixed-ratios [50]. Rather long inter-food inforcement. In accordance with this, the major determinant
times do not in themselves seem to relocate the attack period of schedule choice is the rate of food consumption, indepen-
away from its usual immediate post-feeding position [18]. dent of whether or not responses are necessary to acquire the
There are, however, certain cases which do not fit the above food [27]. The rate of consummatory behavior defined by a
description. These cases constitute interesting exceptions to schedule determines both its relative preference and the
DETERMINANTS OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR 583

degree of adjunctive behavior it generates. But schedule period [56]. When both water and shavings are present, some
preference, as well as response rate [25], are both direct animals engage in polydipsia preferentially, while others time-
functions of reinforcement rate, while the rate of adjunctive share between these two activities (Villarreal, personal com-
behavior is a bitonic function. munication).
An indication that responses other than attack can occur as
Adjunctive Behavior Can Sustain a Schedule of Reinforcement adjuncts to food schedules in the pigeon is the recent finding
that the fixed and variable-interval two-minute food schedules
If water is not freely available from a drinking tube con-
produced polydipsia [45]. Also, fixed ratio food schedules
currently with a food schedule, but is available in small
result in attack [19, 22, 29] or escape [2] in pigeons, probably
portions contingent upon the completion of a fixed-ratio
as a function of the behavior possibilities afforded the animal
schedule, polydipsia is acquired and will sustain large
in the situation. There is obviously a need to compare the
fixed-ratios [12]. This establishes the polydipsia as a rein-
substitutability of attack and polydipsia, as well as attack and
forcing activity capable of sustaining scheduled behavior.
escape, on the same animals using the same food inter-
It also implies that the animal is not simply engaging in some
mittence as a generator schedule for these adjunctive behaviors.
arbitrary, time-filling response, or that drinking is just
The strategy of testing for substitutability of behaviors
reflexly elicited by eating.
with respect to intracranial-stimulation-induced behaviors
Extinction-induced aggression will also support a fixed-
(so-called stimulus-bound behaviors) and peripberal-shock-
ratio schedule [3]. Pigeons will respond on a second key
induced behaviors has revealed some unsuspected non-
with the onset of extinction, the reinforcing relation being the
specificities. When eating, drinking or gnawing is produced
provision of a target bird which is then attacked.
by electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus, one of these
Schedule-induced escape, in the form of 30 see of time
behaviors can substitute for the one originally and consistently
out from a fixed-ratio schedule for water, was sustained when
produced by changing the options available to the animal
a F R 3 was required to produce the time outs [49]. Again, this
showed that the escape behavior functioned as a reinforcer. while holding the hypothalamic stimulation parameters
constant [55]. Peripherally-administered electric shock
results in a highly-predictable and immediate attack response
Environmental Control of the Adjunctive Behavior Manifested in rats and other animals [54]. Yet, such stimulation when
Relatively little research has tested the extent to which at3plied to virgin male rats paired with receptive females
one adjunctive behavior may be substituted for another. results in a much higher incidence of copulatory behavior than
This would be an obvious direction for research on adjunctive in non-shocked controls [5]. Male-male pairings resulted in
behavior to take since the link between the food schedule and fighting.
the particular adjunctive behavior manifested is probably not Not only can changing the environmental options available
one of high specificity. That is to say, the linkage is probably exercise critical control over the behavior induced by electrical
amenable to stimulus facilitation by the environmental stimulation, but also the historical sequencing of these
situation. options and contingencies apparently can alter the sign of a
w h e n access to a running wheel in place of water was reinforcer. Thus, shock-elicited responses can be modulated
provided for rats showing schedule-induced polydipsia on a so as to produce the shocks on a fixed-interval schedule [35]
variable-interval one rain food schedule, wheel-running and a shock-avoidance performance can be altered into a
activity was approximately double the level associated with maintenance of responding under an FI electric shock
an F R 1 food schedule [30]. w h e n both water and the wheel presentation schedule [32, 48].
were provided, drinking occurred post-pellet and running The environmental controls exercised by stimulus facilita-
occurred throughout the inter-reinforcement interval. This tion and the sequencing of options play a major role in
intermittent food schedule produced excessive drinking or determining the behaviors induced not only by feeding
running when either one or the other of these commodities schedules but also by centrally and peripherally administered
was provided. When both opportunities were provided, both electrical stimulation [57].
were engaged in excessively relative to their baseline rates
when not associated with the intermittent food schedule.
Clearly, the excessive, adjunctive behavior engaged in is to a THE PROXIMAL EVENTS CONTROLLING ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR
large extent a function of the environmental opportunities
provided. Some of the major determinants of adjunctive behavior have
It became evident early in polydipsia research that if paper been discussed, such as feeding schedule and deprivation level.
towels or other absorbent material were placed beneath the In the present section, the possible environmental events con-
bars forming the floor of the experimental space, some rats trolling the temporal locus of the behavior will be considered.
pulled such material up through the bars, shredding and First, interpretations of the bouts of adjunctive behavior
manipulating it. This behavior attenuated the level of based upon notions about the animal being confused,
drinking, especially in the latter portions of a session. A disappointed, harassed, teased, or frustrated by an inter-
simple solution to this if one is primarily interested in studying mittent food schedule are untenable. There are methodological
the polydipsia is to build rather deep catch pans and place problems concerning the confirmability of such notions.
only a thin layer of Sanlcel on the bottom. Similar observa- But more cogently, it is difficult to maintain that a rat sub-
tions have been made by others who emphasized the sub- jected to a fixed-interval 90 sec schedule for several months is
stitutability of these chewing-manipulatory behaviors for still reacting with an innocent disappointment and incredulity,
polydipsia [21]. which might describe its initial transition from a fixed-ratio 1
~ A somewhat similar relation holds with respect to schedule- in some circles, by consoling itself with post-pellet drinking
induced polydipsia in the rhesus monkey. These monkeys bouts. Rather than being a response to an uncertain feeding
show a h i , l e v e l of polydipsia [41]. They also show schedule- situation, the behavior seems rather to be a precise product of
induced pica, consuming wood-shavings in the post-pellet the imposed controlling conditions. Adjunctive behavior, then,
584 FALK

is a result of schedule control, not a transition state into, or a of this characteristic pause period is precisely where rein-
confusional state concerning such control. forcement probability is lowest. Where it is not low, as in the
Adjunctive behavior is not elicited by the schedule con- post D R L component of an alternating D R L 30 see, F R 5
ditions. In the first section of the paper, certain reasons were schedule, post-reinforcement drinking does not occur [16].
outlined for not viewing the relation as an unconditioned There are really two notions involved here. One is that the
reflex. The striking regularity and exaggerated level of adjunc- drinking burst is an event controlled by the recency of pellet
tive behavior gives the appearance of an elicited response. consumption unless another pellet is imminent. The other
Nonetheless, the schedule conditions probably do not generate notion is that the low probability of reinforcement is the
such behavior as new responses, but as large increases in the crucial event in the immediate post-pellet period, rather than
base rate of a behavior already present as a response to the any recency effect of the pellet consumed.
current situation. A n ethologist might say that the schedule- The role of recent pellet consumption has been explored by
environment context releases an adjunctive behavior. This using schedules involving conditioned reinforcement and
would be a fair characterization providing one realizes that a second-order schedules. The possibility of attaining drinking
releaser is not an elicitor, but has functions more analogous to burst after a conditioned reinforcer was explored (Falk and
a discriminative stimulus than to an unconditioned or Bryant, unpublished research). Pellet delivery was paired
conditioned stimulus. "Like the discriminative stimulus, it with a 3-sec buzzer for rats on an F I 60 sec schedule. This
(a releaser) increases the probability of occurrence of a unit was later changed to a sequence in which the F I 60 sec
of behavior but does not force it" [47]. schedule resulted in the pellet and buzzer, as before, or with
The notion that adjunctive behavior is an increase in 50 per cent probability yielded the buzzer alone. The F I 60
behavior already present in the situation at a lower base rate sec schedule resulted in polydipsia, and the second schedule
does have some empirical support. When rats eat following a resulted in a much higher level, since the inter-reinforcement
deprivation period, a few draughts of water usually occur in time was lengthened to 120 sec on the average. But relatively
conjunction with the feeding [12, 15]. In accordance with this, little post buzzer drinking occurred. The major portion of
when the base rate of drinking is suppressed by presession the drinking still occurred as post-pellet events. A more
stomach loads of water each day, animals can be prevented successful procedure for attaining drinking following non-
from acquiring polydipsia [7]. Comparable loads given to food stimuli utilized a second-order F R 3 (FI l-rain) schedule
rats with established polydipsia leave the drinking mainly intact with rats in which every completion of an F I 1-min yielded a
[16]. In aggression research, the initial response of animals 2-sec light flash and every third completion also yielded a
to the restrained, target animal is a fair base rate of attack pellet [39]. Considerable control of drinking in the immediate
[4, 22]. The wheel-running base rate of food-deprived rats is post-light-flash period was demonstrated. Wolfgang Wiittke
quite evident under ordinary laboratory conditions and can (personal communication) obtained similar results with the
be seen under the fixed-ratio 1 and extinction conditions same schedule.
imposed [30]. Under conditions where water-drinking is It is of interest to ascertain whether transition to an S-delta
probable, air-licking was shown to be probable [24]. The (no reinforcement) component, which also required a zero
base rate for wood-shavings pica in monkeys is quite low, response rate to terminate (DRO), would produce drinking.
but not zero [56]. It is also a case of what the ethologist would Such a component comprises a low probability of reinforce-
call a re-direction activity since the motor pattern is quite ment and also an enforced period of no operant lever
similar to that of the activity interfered with, namely eating. responses. The basic schedule used was a chain with a short
The motor pattern, then does have an appreciable base rate in F R as the initial member. This led either to a discriminative
the situation. stimulus for the D R O component or one for an F I of the
The stimuli producing the base rates of adjunctive behavior same length. The D R O was followed by the initial F R
are largely unknown. Attempts to account for post-pellet member, while the F I was reinforced with a food pellet and
drinking bursts in terms of a response to a dry mouth have followed by the F R member. Although various parameters
been dealt with previously and found wanting [15, 16]. F o r of the schedule were changed, at no time did appreciable
example, polydipsia occurs when the food used is a liquid, drinking occur except following pellet delivery. Transition
one-third water by weight. Wetting the rat's mouth post- to, or completion of, the D R O period never produced appreci-
pellet by means of an intra-oral tube does not stop the poly- able drinking, nor did completion of the initial (FR) member
dipsia unless the amount administered approximates the leading into the F I component. The drinking was at a high
amount that would have been ingested [7]. By contrast, a polydipsic level, but it was entirely post-pellet in locus (Falk,
ten-times smaller post-pellet, intra-oral injection of water Flory and Bryant, unpublished research). At this time, there
normalizes the p o s t p r a n d i a l drinking pattern of free- is not yet enough research to make a definitive statement
feeding, desalivate rats [28]. Attempts to vary or give accounts concerning the proximal controlling events for adjunctive
of the initial base rates of other adjunctive behaviors have behavior.
been minimal.
The pattern of drinking typical of effective-range polydipsia DISPLACEMENT ACTIVITIES AS ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIORS
is a bout of drinking which follows food pellet consumption
with short latency. This temporal locus is characteristic F o r several decades, ethologists have produced a consider-
of other adjunctive behaviors and was discussed previously. able empirical and theoretical literature on behaviors which
A few attempts have been made to ascertain the significance are characterized as irrelevant, incongruous, or out-of-
of this temporal relation using schedule-induced polydipsia. context. These displacement activities aroused continuing
The post-reinforcement occurrence of drinking on a fixed- interest because they were puzzling and difficult to predict.
interval schedule militates against a simple violation of expect- F o r example, two skylarks in combat might suddenly cease
ation account of the behavior. At this time the lever-pressing fighting and peck at the ground with feeding movements.
rate is low; it is the usual F I pause period, where any Or in a similar situation, two starlings might stop, preen their
expectation of a pellet would also be low. The initial portion feathers, and then resume fighting. In the midst of courting
DETERMINANTS OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR 585

behavior, ducks are often observed to engage in short bouts situations, the interruption of a consummatory behavior in
of preening. The black-headed gull can be observed to an intensely motivated animal induces the occurrence of
commence nest-building behavior when its brooding is another behavior immediately following the interruption
interrupted [36]. In all these examples, the displacement which is facilitated by environmental stimuli.
activity seems to be out of context with the stimulus situation
and the behavior immediately preceding or following [52]. ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR : ADJUSTIVE, TOXIC OR CREATIVE
The most widely-accepted definition of displacement RESPONSE 9.
activity is Tinbergens' [52]: " A displacement activity is an
activity belonging to the executive motor pattern of an instinct Certain values of variables which compose contingencies
other than the instinct(s) activated." If instead of instinct of reinforcement yield behavioral effects in unexpected
activated, the major deprivation operations or stimulating directions and magnitudes. These behaviors are not in-
conditions are substituted in the definition, it can be pro- eluctably elicited by the contingencies. However, under
visionally rephrased. The "displaced" behavior can be certain contingency-environment combinations, rather pro-
referred to as a response sequence which is ordinarily a tracted sequences of adjunctive behavior occur with high
function of variables other than those which presumably probability. The contingency relation alone is not sufficient
dominate the current situation. to induce the adjunctive behavior in the absence of certain
Displacement activities are described as arising from supporting environmental conditions. The particular en-
various kinds of situations, but one such situation is the viroumental circumstances which exist will determine what
thwarting of consummatory behavior. For most ethologists, adjunctive behaviors are generated by a given contingency.
a typical thwarting situation is not one in which a physical But there is no necessity to assume that if a particular con-
barrier is placed in the way of the consummatory behavior, tingency gives rise to sequences of one adjunctive behavior
but one in which the necessary releasing stimuli are not under one environmental condition that it will necessarily
present. Thus, the thwarting of a drive is said to occur when induce some other form of adjunctive behavior under a
one or more eggs are removed from the nest, effectively different environmental condition. Since the contingency-
removing a portion of the releasing stimuli for incubation environment combination is regarded as the main stimulus
behavior in the brooding bird [36]. This results in displace- condition generating an adjunctive behavior, there is no
ment nest-building or preening. Or fanning (egg-ventilating reason to assume that if the environment is altered so that a
movements) which occurs in the male three-spined stickleback certain adjunctive behavior cannot occur a substitute behavior
during courtship has been interpreted as displacement fanning will necessarily emerge. Such a notion would be reminiscent
due to the inadequate releasing stimuli presented by the of symptom substitution which attributes the major stimulus
female, e.g. failure to follow the male toward the nest [53]. control of symptoms to variables other than the immediate
These investigators also observed that displacement activities environmental situation. The extent to which one form of
occur when "an external stimulus, after having activated a adjunctive behavior may be replaced by another one under
drive, suddenly stops." The emphasis here is not upon the changed environmental circumstances is an empirical question.
simple absence of an appropriate releasing stimulus in the But there is no theoretical necessity for some other intrusive
presence of a strong drive, but upon the dynamic change in behavior to supervene if, for instance, the water source is
the situation. Aggressive behavior in the cormorant changes removed from a polydipsia situation.
suddenly to a sexual displacement activity when the opponent Since adjunctive behavior is such a conspicuous aspect of
flees [52]. the behavioral output in those situations which produce it,
Thus, displacement activities are described as occurring several possible theoretical and interpretive notions quite
in situations where an animal under high drive conditions is naturally arise. We have already set aside those explanations
engaged in a phase of the consummatory behavior and for framed in terms of simple, physiological bases, mediating
some reason is prevented from continuing this behavior. functions, or adventitiously reinforced behaviors. But there
These are also the conditions producing adjunctive behaviors: remain certain categories which, while not giving an account
a lean animal engaged in eating is prevented from continuing of the genesis of the behavior, nonetheless favor a particular
this behavior by the intermittence imposed by the feeding evaluative context for interpreting it.
schedule.
We have already described the important role played by Adjunctive Behavior as Adjustive
environmental controls in determining the type of adjunctive It is highly probable that when a psychologist observes
behavior manifested. Similar considerations apply to the intense and prolonged consummatory behavior that he assumes
displacement activity shown. In the older literature, the it serves some function for the organism. Eating is said to
displacement activity was assumed to be determined by a serve certain hunger or tissue needs. If the organism is
kind of sparking-over from one internal energy state sub- demonstrably obese and without other specific nutrient needs,
serving one instinct to another one. In recent research, the the eating is presumed to be either a regulatory or metabolic
importance of current facilitating stimuli in determining maladjustment. It is a pathological condition of the adjust-
what particular displacement activity will occur has been ment mechanism. By this view, behavior is either adjustive or
recognized. The Zebra finch can show different displacement the outcome of disordered adjustive mechanisms. In cases where
activities depending upon the current stimulus situation. an input-output exchange appears to work to the detriment
When threatened, it will feed if food is nearby, sexually mount of that relation, an alternate adjustive reason is sought or
a female if one is present, or if neither food nor a female is assumed. For example, if body water is evaporated in the
available, it will preen or assume a sleeping posture [34]. heat, it is not in the interest of fluid adjustment, but for the
Detailed analyses of the importance of stimulus facilitation in regulation of body temperature. Assuming this general
determining the displacement activity manifest were provided species of adjustive model, eating which does not serve
by further research [40, 44]. tissue needs, or is not a demonstrably regulatory or metabolic
In both adjunctive behavior a n d displacement activity disorder of adjustive mechanisms, is said to be a functional
586 FALK

disorder. Even this type of malfunction is also assumed to situation. Adjunctive behavior may be toxic and intrude
serve certain adjustive ends. It is said to discharge impulses upon possible adaptive responses in one environment or
or relieve tensions which cannot for other reasons obtain have adjustive or creative results in an environment with
motor expression. Thus, what might appear to be maladaptive more prosthetic possibilities [31 ].
behavior in terms of energy exchange is claimed to have an
alternate adjustive function: the relief of tensions which
otherwise would be debilitating. ON THE NATURE OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR
Given the above general orientation, it is not surprising that
attempts have been made to account for adjunctive behavior Having come this far in indicating certain functional
within a framework of adjustment. With regard to adjunctive correspondences among adjunctive behaviors, even to the
drinking, it has been claimed that it maintains the mouth in a territorial borders of ethology, the attempt should be made
condition to swallow dry food, that it maintains the food/ to provide a working definition of adjunctive behavior, if
water ratio in the stomach, that it decreases the body tempera- only to serve as a summary.
ture which is increased by eating, etc. As these explanations Adjunctive behavior is a stable increase in behavior prob-
in adjustive terms have been found wanting, there has been ability not attributable to variables which directly affect the
no lack of alternate suggestions along the lines of functional unconditioned, conditioned, or operant probability of that
disorders. For example, polydipsia has been said to be a behavior, but which is a function of variables primarily
frustration reaction to the food schedule. determining some other class of behavior with respect to:
If all behavior is assumed to be either adjustive or a patho- (1) its unconditioned probability; (2) its consummatory
logical distortion of adjustment, then by definition adjunctive rate; or, (3) the operant probability sustained by it.
behavior must either be directly adjustive (which it seems not Less explicitly, but more succinctly, adjunctive behavior is
to be), serve an ancillary adjustive role (which has not been behavior maintained at high probability by stimuli whose
demonstrated and seems highly improbable), or be a dis- reinforcing properties in the situation are derived primarily as
ordered adjustive reaction. This last would have to reduce a function of schedule parameters governing the availability
to a functional disorder in that no pathological alterations of another class of reinforcers.
have been instituted. While the neurotic may derive certain The behavior is referred to as a stable increase to distin-
secondary gains from his neurotic behavior, the case for guish it from behavioral contrast phenomena and other more
interpreting adjunctive behavior in this regard is rather opaque. transitory effects. It clearly should not be attributable to
Nor does adjunctive behavior seem to have the functional unconditioned or conditioned determinants, otherwise it
properties of neurotic behavior. It develops rapidly rather could not be considered as adjunctive. The operant prob-
than slowly, and it is characteristically predictable of all ability could, of course, be strengthened in many other ways,
members of the species rather than idiosyncratic as to occur- such as making water intake a shock avoidance operant.
rence and form. This too is scarcely adjunctive. The unconditioned prob-
ability of some other class of behavior refers to such variables
as per cent free-feeding body weight as a determinant of
Adjunctive Behavior: Toxic or Creative Response? feeding probability. The consummatory rate refers to
When one loses heart in searching for the adjunctive ends variables which control the rate of eating. These are primarily
served by adjunctive behavior, it is perhaps understandable schedule considerations, but other variables can certainly
that the quest could turn toward darker interpretations affect this: desalivation, local muscle relaxants, punishment
framed in terms of behavioral toxicity. Certain combinations contingencies, etc. "The operant probability sustained by it"
of drug and schedule parameters have been described as takes into account those schedule factors which favor pauses
behaviorally toxic [51] in that performance outcomes are in the operant performance. Such pauses perhaps favor
disrupted relative to undrugged behavioral baselines. The change-overs to the adjunctive behavior. For example, with
same drug dosage levels may leave behavior controlled by ratio strain, the pigeon engages in attacks during portions of
other schedule parameters intact. Just as certain drug and the pause [29].
schedule parameter combinations exhibit behavioral toxicity With respect to the second, short definition, those with
certain environment and schedule parameter combinations interests in the area of motivational theory should note the
also might be viewed as yielding another mode of toxic implication that schedule of reinforcement can be considered as
response: adjunctive behavior. According to this notion, a drive operation. Drive operation has been defined as "any
adjunctive behavior is the manifestation of a maladaptive operation that changes the effectiveness of a stimulus as a
response to an unfavorable environmental situation similar reinforcer... "([6], p. 334). In accordance with this definition,
to the stereotyped responses shown by institutionalized re- certain food schedules are drive operations for adjunctive
tardates and caged animals. behaviors.
The question as to whether adjunctive behavior is a toxic When the environmental conditions producing adjunctive
manifestation or a creative deviation cannot be answered by a behavior are maintained, the behavior tends to persist and
description x)f the topography of the response. If the adjunc- remain in its exaggerated form. It is difficult to satiate. In
tive behavior results in the organism failing to adjust to preliminary work, it was found that polydipsia was relatively
environmental contingencies, or if the behavior itself leads to difficult to discourage by punishment contingencies compared
damaging consequences, then it can be considered as mal- to deprivation-induced drinking (Falk and Bryant, unpublished
adaptive or as a toxic response. If the behavior results in a research). Likewise, relatively large quantities of highly-
new behavioral emphasis working to the benefit of the hypertonic NaCI are ingested adjunctively [11, 14].
organism, then it can be viewed as a powerful mechanism The reinforcement intermittence and thwarting conditions
for adaptive change. Whether the result is toxic or creative which yield adjunctive and displacement behaviors increase
will be a function not only of the generator schedule but also the organism's probability of responding in strength to other
of the environmental circumstances--the ecology of the possibilities in the environmental context by increasing the
DETERMINANTS OF ADJUNCTIVE BEHAVIOR 587

gain on operant units receiving relatively low, but appreciable, advantage over other species. Tberfore, ceteris paribus, a
facilitation from current environmental stimuli. I n this displacement-prone species will be more adaptable, and
regard, Armstrong [1] comments that "a species which is consequently more successful, than a species not so equipped."
able to modify its behavior to suit changed circumstances by But he also ironically observes: "No doubt many dysgenic
means of displacements, rather than by the evolution of displacements have been eliminated with their performers
ad hoc modifications starting from scratch, will have an in the course of evolution."

REFERENCES
I. Armstrong, E. A. The nature and function of displacement 25. Hernstein, R. J. On the law of effect. J. exp. Analysis Behav.
activities. Syrup. Soc. exp. Biol. 4: 361-384, 1950. 13: 243-266, 1970.
2. Azrin, N. H. Time-out from positive reinforcement. Science 26. Hutchinson, R. R., N. H. Azrin and G. M. Hunt. Attack
133: 382-383, 1961. produced by intermittent reinforcement of a concurrent
3. Azrin, N. H. Aggression. Paper read at American Psycho- operant response. J. exp. Analy,~is Behav. 11: 485-495, 1968.
logical Association, Los Angeles, September, 1964. 27. Killeen, P. Response rate as a factor in choice. Psychonom.
4. Azrin, N. H., R. R. Hutchinson and D. F. Hake. Extinction- Sci. 12: 34, 1968.
induced aggression. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 9: 191-204, 1966. 28. Kissileff, H. R. Oropharyngeal control of prandial drinking.
5. Caggiula, A. R. and R. Eibergen. Copulation of virgin male J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 67: 309-319, 1969.
rats evoked by painful peripheral stimulation, d. comp. physiol. 29. Knutson, J. F. Aggression during the fixed-ratio and extinc-
Psychol. 69: 414-419, 1969. tion components of a multiple schedule of reinforcement.
6. Catania, A. C. fEd) Contemporary Research in Operant Be- J. exp. Analysis Behav. 13: 221-231, 1970.
havior. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman, 1968. 30. Levitsky, D. and G. Collier. Schedule-induced wheel running.
7. Chapman, H. W. Oropharyngeal determinants of non- Physiol. Behav. 3: 571-573, 1968.
regulatory drinking in the rat. UnpublishedPh.D. Dissertation, 31. Lindsley, O. R. Direct measurement and prosthesis of re-
University of Pennsylvania, 1969. tarded behavior. J. Educat. 147: 62-81, 1964.
8. Chillag, D. and J. Mendelson. Schedule-induced airlicking as 32. McKearney, J. W. Maintenance of responding under a fixed-
a function of body-weight deficit in rats. Physiol. Behav. 6: interval schedule of electric shock-presentation. Science 160:
603-605, 1971. 1249-1251, 1968.
9. Falk, J. L. Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an 33. Mendelson, J. and D. Chillag. Schedule-induced air licking in
intermittent food schedule. Science 133: 195-196, 1961. rats. Physiol. Behav. 5: 535-537, 1970.
10. Falk, J. L. The behavioral regulation of water-electrolyte 34. Morris, D. The reproductive behavior of the Zebra finch
balance. In: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, edited by (Poephila guttata), with special reference to pseudofemale
M. R. Jones. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1961, behaviorand displacement activities. Behaviour 6: 271-322,1954.
pp. 1-33. 35. Morse, W. H., R. N. Mead and R. T. Kelleher. Modulation
11. Falk, J. L. Studies on schedule-induced polydipsia. In: of elicited behavior by a fixed-interval schedule of electric
Thirst: First International Symposium on Thirst in the Regulation shock presentation. Science 157: 215-217, 1967.
of Body Water, edited by M. J. Wayner, New York: Pergamon 36. Moynihan, M. Some displacement activities of the Black-
Press, 1964, pp. 95-116. headed Gull. Behaviour 5: 58-80, 1953.
12. Falk, J. L. The motivational properties of schedule-induced 37. Reynierse, J. H. and D. Spanier. Excessive drinking in rats'
polydipsia. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 9: 19-25, 1966. adaptation to the schedule of feeding. Psychonom. Sci. 10:
13. Falk, J. L. Schedule-induced polydipsia as a function of fixed 95-96, 1968.
interval length. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 9: 37-39, 1966. 38. Rilling, M., H. R. Askew, J. E. Ahlskog and T. J. Kramer.
14. Falk, J. L. Analysis of water and NaCI solution acceptance by Aversive properties of the negative stimulus in a successive
schedule-induced polydipsia. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 9: discrimination. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 12: 917-932, 1969.
111-118, 1966. 39. Rosenblith, J. Z. Polydipsia induced in the rat by a second-
15. Falk, J. L. Control of schedule-induced polydipsia: type, order schedule. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 14: 139-144, 1970.
size, and spacing of meals. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 10: 199-206, 40. Rowell, C. H. F. Displacement grooming in the chaffinch.
1967. Anim. Behav. 9: 38-63, 1961.
16. Falk, J. L. Conditions producing psychogenic polydipsia in 41. Schuster, C. R. and J. H. Woods. Schedule-induced polydipsia
animals. Ann. N. Y. Aead. Sci. 157: 569-593, 1969. in the monkey. Psychol. Rep. 19: 823-828, 1966.
17. Ferster, C. B. Control of behavior in chimpanzees and pigeons 42. Segal, E. F. Transformation of polydipsic drinking into
by time out from positive reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. 72: operant drinking: a paradigm? Psychonom. Sci. 16: 133-135,
1-38 (no. 461), 1958. 1969.
18. Flory, R. K. Attack behavior as a function of minimum 43. Segal, E. F., D. L. Oden and S. A. Deadwyler. Determinants
inter-food interval. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 12: 825-828, 1969. of polydipsia: IV. Free-reinforcement schedules. Psychonom.
19. Flory, R. K. Attack behavior in a multiple fixed-ratio schedule Sci. 3: 11-12, 1965.
of reinforcement. Psychonom. Sci. 16: 156-157, 1969.
20. Flory, R. K. The control of schedule-induced polydipsia: 44. Sevenster, P. A causal analysis of a displacement activity
frequency and magnitude of reinforcement. Unpublished (Fanning in Gasteroteus aculeatug L.). Behaviour Suppl. No. IX:
Ph.D. Dissertation, Arizona State University, 1969. 1-170, 1961.
21. Freed, E. X. and N. Hymowitz. A fortuitous observation 45. Shanab, M. E. and J. L. Peterson. Polydipsia in the pigeon.
regarding "psychogenic" polydispia. Psychol. Rep. 24: 224-226, Psychonom. Sci. 15: 51-52, 1969.
1969. 46. Sidman, M. Normal sources of pathological behavior.
22. Gentry, W. D. Fixed-ratio schedule-induced aggression. Science 132: 61-68, 1960.
J. exp. Analysis Behav. 11: 813-817, 1968. 47. Skinner, B. F. The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior.
23. Hawkins, T. D. Pellet frequency: effects on schedule-induced Science 153: 1205-1213, 1966.
polydipsia and variable-interval barpress performance. Paper 48. Stretch, R., E. R. Orloff and S. D. Dalrymple. Maintenance
read at Psychonom. Sci. Meeting, Chicago, 1967. of responding by fixed-interval schedule of electric shock
24. Hendry, D. P. and R. H. Rasche. Analysis of a new non- presentation in squirrel monkeys. Science 162: 583-586, 1968.
nutritive positive reinforcer based on thirst. J. comp. physiol. 49. Thompson, D. M. Escape from SD associated with fixed-ratio
Psychol. 54: 477-483, 1961. reinforcement. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 7: 1-8, 1964.
588 FALl(

50. Thompson, D. M. Punishment by SD associated with fixed- 55. Valenstein, E. S., V. C. Cox and J. W. Kakolewski. Re-
ratio reinforcement. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 8: 189-194, 1965. examination of the role of the hypothalamus in motivation.
51. Thompson, T. and C. R. Schuster. Behavioral Pharmacology. Psychol. Rev. 77: 16-31, 1970.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J. : Prentice-Hall, 1968. 56. Villarreal, J. Schedule-induced pica. Paper read at Eastern
52. Tinbergen, N. "Derived" activities; their causation, biological Psychological Association, Boston, April, 1967.
significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. 57. Wayner, IM'.J. Motor control functions of the lateral hypothala-
Q. Rev. Biol. 27: 1-32, 1952. mus and adjunctive behavior. Physiol. Behav. 5: 1319-1325,
53. Tinbergen, N. and J. J. A. Vanlersel. "Displacement reactions" 1970.
in the three-spined stickleback. Behaviour 1 : 56-63, ! 947.
54. Ulrich, R. E. and N. H. Azrin. Reflexive fighting in response
to aversive stimulation. J. exp. Analysis Behav. 5: 511-520,
1962.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen