Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

How online consumer reviews are influenced by the language and


valence of prior reviews: A construal level perspective
Goele Aerts a, *, Tim Smits a, P.W.J. Verlegh b
a
Institute for Media Studies, KU Leuven, Parkstraat 45, 3000, Leuven, Belgium
b
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, De Boelelaan 1105, 1081 HV, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Online reviews are a commonly used source of product information to guide consumers in their purchase
Received 4 January 2017 decisions. This source of information helps them to make better-informed decisions. Communicators on
Received in revised form online review platforms have multiple goals, but only one of them is to provide accurate information.
19 May 2017
Before posting comments, they could read previous reviews which will in turn influence their written
Accepted 13 June 2017
Available online 21 June 2017
comments. The impact of previous reviews on readers has been demonstrated. However, its influence on
writers has hardly been studied. We examined how language abstraction in reviews is influenced by
language abstraction in prior reviews, and whether this biased language use may have a subsequent
Keywords:
Online consumer reviews
effect on the persuasiveness of reviews. Building on literature about linguistic style matching and con-
Construal level theory strual level theory, the present paper reports two experiments (N ¼ 101 and N ¼ 189) showing that
Prior review people use more concrete language when prior reviews also use concrete language (i.e. words that refer
Language abstraction to tangible, qualities or characteristics), and that this concreteness leads to more favorable attitudes
Valence towards the reviewer and the product. These findings suggest that language abstraction is contagious,
review platforms could capitalize on this by seeding concrete reviews.
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction consumers only have to interact with their computers to post their
opinions about products or services. Their opinions are widely and
The Internet has changed the way consumers search for infor- easily accessible to other consumers, but are only disseminated if
mation, and more importantly the way they buy products and order and when prospective consumers search for them (Sen & Lerman,
services. Consumers increasingly look for online reviews to provide 2007). These product or service descriptions appear on review
them with valuable information about products and services. On- platforms such as Amazon.com or Tripadvisor. A review example
line reviews are often regarded as an extension of traditional word- could be: “The dinner in this restaurant was good, the food was
of-mouth (WOM) into the online domain, and are often referred to delicious.” These online comments are often seen as helpful and
as electronic word-of-mouth (eWOM). credible, which both can be driven by several types of factors (Chua
eWOM currently is one of the most often considered ways to & Banerjee, 2016; Huang, Chen, Yen & Tran, 2015; Shan, 2016).
gain knowledge about products and services (Hennig-Thurau et al., Contrary to verbal face-to-face word-of-mouth, online consumer
2010; Hong & Park, 2012; Kim & Hollingshead, 2015). According to reviews contain some textual and graphical elements that influence
Nielsen, online consumer reviews are the third most trusted consumers (Cheung & Thadani, 2012; King, Racherla, & Bush, 2014).
format; two-thirds trust consumer opinions posted online (Nielsen, Because of the lack of facial expressions and vocal fluctuations in
2015). Online consumer reviews are generated by consumers and computer-mediated communications environments, reviewers
written for prospective consumers. In these eWOM reviews, have to learn from online interaction which is available (Hong &
Park, 2012).
This suggests that the contents of one's review may be easily
influenced by the context in which it is provided. In line with this
* Corresponding author. Institute for Media Studies, Faculty of Social Sciences, KU notion, earlier research has found that the contents of reviews are
Leuven, Parkstraat 45, PO box 3603, B-3000, Leuven, Belgium. influenced by those of prior reviews that have been posted on the
E-mail addresses: goele.aerts@soc.kuleuven.be (G. Aerts), tim.smits@soc. platform (e.g., Moe & Schweidel, 2012; Purnawirawan, Dens, & De
kuleuven.be (T. Smits), p.verlegh@vu.nl (P.W.J. Verlegh).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.023
0747-5632/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
856 G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

Pelsmacker, 2012). The strong influence of online reviews on pur- 2. Literature review
chase decisions is to a larger extent caused by the perception that
reviews provide independent information from a large number of 2.1. Construal level theory
people who purchased the product. Contrary to this perception,
posting online reviews might be seen as context-dependent To better understand how the language used in online reviews is
communication (Hamilton, Schlosser, & Chen, 2017; Liang, 2016), influenced by a prior review, we discuss the properties of online
that is partly influenced by what (and how) previous reviewers review content from the perspective of construal level theory (Trope
wrote. & Liberman, 2010). CLT predicts that when psychological distance
On a more generalized level of theorizing, these findings are in decreases (a subjective feeling that something is close to the self),
line with the idea that humans are prone to imitating each other in one will think in a more concrete way (Fujita et al., 2006; Trope &
social interaction (Chen, Chartrand, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 1998). This Liberman, 2003). Conversely, when psychological distance in-
imitative behavior streamlines social interaction and aids in creases, one will think and write more abstractly. Prior research
learning to replicate actions and improves language comprehen- already found evidence for the impact of psychological distance on
sion (Adank, Hagoort, & Bekkering, 2010). Imitative behavior has construal and consumer evaluations (Huang, Burtch, Hong, &
been found to occur in many ways, including language use. Recent Polman, 2016; Zhao & Xie, 2011). These studies made use of two
research has shown that synchronization in conversational style, dimensions of psychological distance and found that the effect of
more specifically linguistic style matching (LSM) increases shared spatial distance (i.e., authoring a review about a geographically
perceptions among interlocutors (Pennebaker, 2011). Due to this distant restaurant, rather than a proximate one) increased the ef-
linguistic style matching, the words that one person uses go along fect of temporal distance (i.e., authoring a review after a lengthy
with the words used by the other person uses. In the present delay, rather than immediately) on consumer evaluations, and the
research, we conceive online reviews as partly conversational ut- other way around.
terances, which are prone to linguistic style matching, so that re- The impact of psychological distance on language abstraction
viewers display imitative behavior in their review writing, both in has been widely demonstrated (Trope & Liberman, 2010), with
terms of valence and linguistic style. different embodiments of distance creating the same respective
A small number of studies have examined language use in on- levels of construal. For example, people were found to use more
line reviews. Schellekens, Verlegh, and Smidts (2010) were among concrete language when describing their own actions than another
the first to analyze written comments. They found that product one's actions (Semin & Fiedler, 1989), or when instructed to address
experiences that were congruent with consumers' brand attitudes someone politely (i.e., “distant”) rather than in colloquial language
were communicated in a more abstract wording. In order to obtain (Stephan, Liberman, & Trope, 2010). Concrete language refers to
a more systematic understanding of the impact of prior reviews on things that are available to the senses, and can be observed and
the use of language abstraction in subsequent reviews the construal measured while abstract language refers to ideas or concepts.
level theory (CLT; Trope & Liberman, 2010) could be used. Specif- Similarly, concrete consumer reviews are those containing more
ically, construal level theory suggests that we form abstract mental detailed information about a product than abstract consumer re-
construals of distal objects or experiences and concrete construals views do. Consider for example the following reviews: “The laptop I
of close objects or experiences. Thus, although we cannot experi- bought, combines powerful performance and a great keyboard with
ence what is not present, we can make predictions and speculate. new eye-tracking technology for a genuinely innovative experi-
Predictions and speculations are all mental constructions, distinct ence.” (concrete) as opposed to “The laptop has great quality, lots of
from direct experience and they will transcend the actual moment. cool features and is easy-to-use!” (abstract). The greater level of
They represent psychological distance which is a subjective expe- detail is associated with a low level of cognitive construal, at which
rience that something is close or far away from the self, here, and people think more concretely and is, as earlier declared, associated
now (Freitas, Salovey & Liberman, 2001; Trope & Liberman, 2010). with psychological proximity. When people are thinking at low
Lots of research on this theory has been done (Fujita, Trope, levels of construal, they are focusing on details that are less
Liberman, & Levin-Sagi, 2006; Trope & Liberman, 2003, 2010), essential to the overall essence of the object. In this case we talk
however, the theory has not been widely applied in the online about the peripheral, secondary features. Contrary, a high level
consumer review context. construal is when people are thinking abstractly (Trope &
To the best of our knowledge, the present research is the first to Liberman, 2003). To conclude, online consumer reviews that are
examine how the use of language abstraction of reviewers is written in a concrete language style provide detailed information
influenced by the use of language abstraction in prior reviews, and about a product as opposed to abstract written reviews. Previous
how the resulting language use subsequently impacts the attitudes research already found that level of detail in a prior review affected
and intentions of readers. Previous research on word-of-mouth has the credibility of search products (Jime nez & Mendoza, 2013).
mainly focused on the reader, but little attention has been paid to Research from fields such as social psychology and interpersonal
the question of how consumers describe products and whether and communication has shown that the level of mental construal af-
how this influences the extent to which prior reviews influence fects the attitudes and behavior of the reader towards the sender
subsequent reviewers. This article aims to fill those gaps. We con- (e.g., Schellekens et al., 2010; 2012). They showed that language
ducted two experimental studies which were approved by the first abstraction in word of mouth influences receivers' inferences about
author's designated Ethics Committee. Study 1 investigates the the product attitudes of the sender (Schellekens et al., 2010). Ac-
effect of the level of abstraction in a backpack review on attitudes cording to the CLT, it was found that when a person has to make
towards the reviewer, the product and the subsequent reviewers' decisions for the near future, reviews from proximal social others
writing behavior. Study 2 examines the suggested effects of a have a larger impact on one's product attitudes than reviews from
search product (i.e., a smartphone) with valence as an extra factor to distant social others (Zhao & Xie, 2011). Based on these prior
assure the effects of Study 1 are not only limited to positive reviews. findings it can be argued that online review elements could be
In addition, the design of both studies differed. In Study 1 partici- understood from construal level theory. The question, however,
pants had to evaluate a backpack of an unfamiliar brand whereas in arises whether language abstraction in online consumer reviews is
Study 2 they had to evaluate their own smartphone. influenced by prior posts.
G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864 857

2.2. Linguistic style matching Ghose, & Wiesenfeld, 2008). If the reader agrees with the
reviewer, the initial feelings of the reader will be supported.
Previous research reported a positive relationship between prior Reviewer agreement refers to the degree of perceived agreement
reviews and subsequent reviews (Ma, Khansa, Deng & Kim, 2013), it regarding the evaluation of a product but the evaluation of the
was found that the average rating of prior posts can serve as a signal reviewer him- or herself as well. Recent investigations in online
for subsequent consumers which will positively affect their post- settings show that reviewer agreement is related to consumers'
consumption evaluations. Purnawirawan et al. (2012) demon- attitudes. For instance, Benedicktus, Brady, Darke, and Voorhees
strated sequence effects in the impact of reviews on readers. It has (2010) found that consumers agree more with senders who has
been argued that review sequence matters not only in prospective been evaluated favorably when their reviews are trustworthy.
customers' buying behavior, perceived usefulness, and trustwor- Based on the prior discussion, we expect that a concrete review
thiness but also in how the subsequent reviewers will write com- will increase the agreement with and the attitude towards the
ments (Walther, DeAndrea, Kim, & Anthony, 2010). Chen et al. reviewer.
(1998) found that people mimic each other's behaviors in social
H2. A prior concrete (abstract) review has a positive (negative)
interaction, which aids in learning to replicate actions and betters
effect on agreement with the reviewer (H2a) and leads to a more
language comprehension (Adank et al., 2010). Thus, the use of
positive (negative) attitude towards the reviewer (H2b). Agreement
language of a reviewer can play an important role in the decision
with the reviewer mediates the effect of the level of abstractness in
process and in the writing behavior of a following reviewer. Lin-
a prior review on readers' attitude towards the reviewer (H2c).
guistic style matching (LSM) states that the words someone uses
covary with the words someone else uses on the reciprocity level Previous research already followed somewhat the same idea
but also on the broader conversational level (Cappella, 1996). and suggests that for search products concrete, detailed informa-
Words one reviewer uses prime the reader to respond in a specific tion is more credible and persuasive than abstract information
way. However, because language use is reciprocal and coordinated, (Jimenez & Mendoza, 2013). Moreover, it was found that detailed
it is usually not clear who is following and leading. Thus, a reviewer reviews lead to higher purchase intentions compared to general
could influence the following reviewer's language or could be reviews. Early studies show that word-of-mouth which provides
influenced by the prior reviewer's language at the word level in details is more convincing than broader communication since the
natural conversation. Earlier research on mimicry already found recommendation becomes more diagnostic (Herr, Kardes, & Kim,
that one's nonverbal behavior could be affected by another's 1991). In addition, consumers infer that the recommender knows
movement (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Other research also offered the product well when detailed information is given which they
substantial evidence that individuals in dyadic interactions exhibit believe and trust more (Bansal & Voyer, 2000). Therefore, online
LSM on both the conversation level as well as on a turn-by-turn consumer reviews which contain concrete written elements should
level. Furthermore, LSM is unrelated to ratings of the quality of be more credible and persuasive than the more abstract and gen-
the interaction (Niederhoffer & Pennebaker, 2002). The base of the eral reviews (e.g., “the best product”, “amazing”, “nice in use”).
linguistic style matching findings is the observation that words are
H3. A prior concrete (abstract) review leads to a higher (lower)
predictive elements of language that capture the style rather than
star rating of the product (H3a), a more positive (negative) attitude
content of an expression. Earlier research found that women and
towards the product (H3b), a higher (lower) willingness to pur-
men react and accommodate to gender-preferential language in
chase the product (H3c) and a higher (lower) willingness to
electronic communication (Thomson & Murachver, 2001;
recommend the product (H3d).
Thomson, Murachver, & Green, 2001). The results revealed that
linguistic style had the greatest impact on participants' language In sum, the literature provides evidence that previous posts on
use. When people harmonize in language style, they are also likely online review platforms may offer interesting information. The
to share a common understanding and conceptualization of their question then arises whether language in prior reviews influences
conversation topics (Pennebaker, 2011). Recent research demon- review writers and whether this biased language use may have a
strated that linguistic style accommodation is associated with subsequent effect on the persuasiveness of reviews. To test our
positive social outcomes (Muir, Joinson, Cotterill, & Dewdney, 2016) claim that language abstraction in prior reviews will lead to lan-
and that congruence with the target group's typical linguistic style guage abstraction in subsequent reviews, and in turn will impact
increased the impact of online reviews on consumer decisions readers' attitudes and intentions, two experimental studies will be
(Ludwig et al., 2013). conducted.
Based on LSM, we predict that reviewers seek to match the
language use of prior reviews they are exposed to. As mentioned
earlier, people will mimic words on the broader conversational 3. Study 1
level (Cappella, 1996). More particularly, and based on CLT (Fujita
et al., 2006; Trope & Liberman, 2003), we propose that reviewers 3.1. Methods
display a tendency to mimic the level of abstractness/concreteness
in the content of prior reviews when writing their own reviews. In Study 1, we examined the effects for a product that is
commonly used by our subject population (a backpack). Study 1
H1. Reviewers are influenced by the language used in prior re-
used a cover story for a backpack of a non-familiar brand that
views: if reviewers read a concrete (abstract) review, they will write
extended into a new category. Using an unfamiliar product can
a concrete (abstract) review themselves.
exert experimental control over random deviations in people's
Prior literature indicates that detailed reviews contain con- opinion of an object under discussion. One could object that an
crete, specific elements contrary to general reviews (Jimenez & unfamiliar product creates potential for a demand characteristic on
Mendoza, 2013). Previous findings also revealed that consumers subjects' responses (Zizzo, 2010), but we follow the methodology of
believe and trust evaluations containing detailed information previous studies within the field of online consumer reviews (e.g.,
because they infer that the recommender knows the product well Schellekens, Verlegh, & Smidts, 2012), and supplement Study 1
(Bansal & Voyer, 2000). Consumers read reviews in order to find with Study 2, in which participants are asked to write a review for a
identity descriptive information about the reviewer (Forman, product they actually used themselves.
858 G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

3.1.1. Design and stimuli (Mage ¼ 23.84, SDage ¼ 3.15, Minage ¼ 18 and Maxage ¼ 34). 51 of
To evaluate the hypotheses, a between-subjects experiment was them were male, 50 were female.
conducted. The study had two treatment conditions: one with a
concrete consumer review, and one with an abstract consumer 3.1.3. Procedure
review. The stimuli were based on an existing review of the e- Those who agreed to participate were provided a link to a
commerce platform bol.com, with addition of concrete versus ab- questionnaire on Qualtrics. Qualtrics is an online software appli-
stract elements. To minimize potential confounds the length of the cation that hosts electronic surveys. All respondents were invited
reviews was kept constant, all the reviews were rather positive and for an experiment on consumer behavior and were randomly
the price information was excluded. Samples of the text in the assigned to one of two conditions. In the study instructions, par-
stimuli appear in the Appendix. We opted for a backpack, a widely ticipants were told that they would be participating in a study on
used product, of a not so familiar brand (Oakley). The selection of a how people think when writing a review and how this could impact
backpack was based on two criteria. First, the product presented in readers. After reading the survey instructions respondents were
the review had to be somewhat unfamiliar in order to avoid strong directed to the questionnaire. They first needed to fill out de-
preliminary position or biases of the product. Second, earlier mographic variables (gender and age). Respondents were then
research showed that product type can influence peoples' way of asked to look at the product pictures and read the accompanying
processing information (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983). More review of the Oakley backpack. Immediately after reading partici-
in specific, people tend to evaluate information about high pants were asked to answer a series of questions about the
involvement products such as audio and video devices systemati- reviewer, the product and their willingness to buy or recommend
cally rather than heuristically. We aimed to find a product with a the product. Then they were asked to write a review their own.
medium degree of involvement, which would be modestly relevant After writing, they were asked how much they write or read re-
to the participants in order to avoid a moderating effect possibly views and how abstract they think.
associated with product involvement. We selected a backpack of
Oakley, since it is not a famous brand, respondents were less likely 3.1.4. Measures
to have specific preconceived notions about it. The backpack used 3.1.4.1. Dependent variables. After exposure to one of two condi-
for this study was the black Gearbox 22 Oakley backpack. Next to tions, participants were asked to indicate their agreement with the
the review (either concrete or abstract) four pictures of different reviewer on a one-item seven-point Likert scale (To what extent do
angles of the backpack were presented, to visualize the backpack. you agree with the reviewer? Indicate on a scale ranging from “not
(see Fig. 1). This should help respondents to give their own product at all” (1) to “totally” (7)), followed by their attitude towards the
description without specifications. The pictures were stock-images reviewer on a five-item 7-point differential scale (Madden, Allen, &
downloaded from the platform bol.com. Participants in both con- Twible, 1988). Participants rated the five items in a seven-point
ditions viewed those pictures but read an accompanying review scale with one anchored to “boring/unpleasant/bad/unappealing/
differing in language abstraction. Participants were not provided artless” and seven anchored to “interesting/pleasant/good/
with any additional product details, as we wanted them to rely only appealing/artful” (a ¼ 0.90). Next, their attitude towards the product
on the product descriptions in the prior review. Giving product was assessed with the same scale (a ¼ 0.85). Participants then
details next to the pictures would prime them too much, prior evaluated the product on a seven-point star scale, they had to assign
research even showed that details about product specifications are one to seven stars to the product. Star ratings are commonly used in
persuasive (Herr et al., 1991), which could lead to more concrete online product reviews and can be processed with minimal
review writing. cognitive effort (Chevalier & Mayzlin, 2006). Participants were also
asked how likely they would be to buy and to recommend the product
on the one-item 11-point Juster scale (Wright & MacRae, 2007).
3.1.2. Participants
Next, they were asked to write a review about the backpack. Lan-
Data were collected online from 101 respondents through a
guage abstraction in participants' reviews was rated by five inde-
convenience sample from Dutch-speaking Belgian men and
pendent judges, who were blind to the experimental conditions.
women. By using this non-probability sampling technique subjects
The independent coders were trained in seeing the difference be-
were selected because of their convenient accessibility and prox-
tween concrete language and abstract language and were provided
imity to the researcher. The respondents consisted of the general
with examples. The coded language abstraction (a ¼ 0.79) in the
public were personally invited through email (by the first author)
participants' reviews provided a good level of intercoder reliability.
to participate in the study. No incentives were used to stimulate the
The rating of language abstraction in the open-ended descriptions
participation of the respondents. They all read an online consumer
was done by a one-item seven-point scale from “concrete” (1) to
review of a backpack that was either written in a concrete (N ¼ 53)
“abstract” (7) (Schellekens et al., 2010).
versus abstract (N ¼ 56) language style. Subject anonymity and
confidentiality of the data was guaranteed by not asking partici-
3.1.4.2. Covariates. To control for possible effects of reviewing
pants' names. Upon presentation of an informed consent form,
habits, participants were first asked to indicate how much they write
respondents were given the option of opting out of the study by
and read online reviews. This was done on two 7-point items
simply clicking out of the web browser that contained the online
ranging from “very little” (1) to “very much” (7): “I write … online
questionnaire. The average age of the participants was 24 years
reviews” and “I read … online reviews”. Both measures were used
as covariates. Next, brand familiarity was included as a covariate as
well. Subjects indicated how familiar they were with the brand
Oakley via a one-item seven-point Likert scale from “I am not at all
familiar with this brand” (1) to “I'm very familiar with this brand”
(7). We also controlled for chronic mental construal tendencies
using the Behavior Identification Form (BIF; Vallacher & Wegner,
1989), a personality measure of how abstractly or concretely in-
dividuals represent action. It is composed of 25 items, each ques-
Fig. 1. Stock-images of the reviewed backpack in Study 1. tion requires participants to describe an activity (e.g., “taking a
G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864 859

test”) by choosing an option that represents the action abstractly agreement (F(1,99) ¼ 11.920, p ¼ 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.107). In line with
(“showing one's knowledge”) or concretely (“answering ques- hypothesis 2a, we found that respondents agreed significantly
tions”). After writing their own review, participants were asked to more with the previous reviewer when exposed to a concrete re-
evaluate this review. They had to indicate to what extent they were view (M ¼ 5.14, SD ¼ 0.91) than when exposed to an abstract review
convinced of their own review via a one-item seven-point Likert (M ¼ 4.44, SD ¼ 1.11). We also found a difference in attitude towards
scale from “I am not at all convinced” (1) to “I'm very convinced” (7) the reviewer, with respondents being significantly more positive
and to what extent they would post their own review online via a when exposed to a concrete review (F(1,99) ¼ 15.641, p < 0.001,
one-item seven-point Likert scale from “I would not at all post it hp2 ¼ 0.136, M ¼ 4.76, SD ¼ 1.08) compared to an abstract one
online” (1) to “I would totally post it online” (7). This was included (M ¼ 3.85, SD ¼ 1.21). These results support H2b. A mediation
because the previous review could be seen as an example of the analysis tested H2c. Specifically, this hypothesis stated that agree-
consensus among posters. Prior research found that when people ment with the reviewer mediates the effects of language abstrac-
belong to the majority of opinion holders (Woong Yun & Park, 2011) tion on the attitude towards the reviewer. So, in this analysis,
or when the consensus is positive (Wu, Mattila, Wang, & Hanks, language abstraction in the previous reviews was the independent
2016) they are more willing to post online. Next, they were variable, attitude towards the reviewer was the dependent variable,
instructed to evaluate their own review via a two one-item seven- and agreement with the reviewer was the mediator. The analysis
point Likert scales on language abstraction ranging from “concrete” used 5000 bootstrap samples (Hayes, 2009), in order to estimate a
(1) to “abstract” (7). The last three covariates were only used in the 95% confidence interval. If zero falls outside the confidence interval,
analyses regarding the participants' written product descriptions. the indirect effect is significant and mediation is present. The
Demographic variables such as participants' age and gender were analysis shows that the indirect effect of language abstraction on
not controlled for in the final analysis, preliminary analyses indi- the attitude towards the reviewer is significant (B ¼ 0.319,
cated that demographics did not impact the results. SE ¼ 0.123, 95% CI ¼ [0.607, 0.121]). These results confirm H2c,
they suggest that reviewer agreement mediates the relationship
3.1.5. Pretest and manipulation check between language abstraction and attitude towards the reviewer.
In a pretest (20 students) we examined familiarity with Oakley For hypothesis 3, we analyzed the effects of exposure to a con-
as a brand for backpacks. Familiarity was measured on a scale from crete versus abstract review (i.e. language abstraction in reviews)
“I am not at all familiar with this brand” (1) to “I am very familiar on product evaluations. We only found a significant difference of
with this brand” (7). As anticipated, participants were not that language abstraction on product rating (F(1,99) ¼ 4.924, p ¼ 0.029,
familiar with the brand (Mu0 ¼ 4, M ¼ 1.55, SD ¼ 1.23; hp2 ¼ 0.047), participants' rating in the concrete condition was
t(19) ¼ 8.876, p < 0.001). In this pretest we also examined par- higher (M ¼ 4.96, SD ¼ 1.17) than in the abstract one (M ¼ 4.40,
ticipants' perceptions of the language abstraction for both online SD ¼ 1.33). We observed no main effects of language abstraction on
reviews in a within-subjects design. Participants were asked to product attitude (F(1,99) ¼ 0.229, p ¼ 0.634, hp2 ¼ 0.002), will-
indicate the language abstraction for each review on a 7-point scale ingness to buy (F(1,99) ¼ 1.129, p ¼ 0.290, hp2 ¼ 0.011), and will-
from “concrete” (1) to “abstract” (7). We also assessed the valence ingness to recommend (F(1,99) ¼ 1.327, p ¼ 0.252, hp2 ¼ 0.013).
of the manipulated online reviews from “negative” (1) to “positive” Thus, the results showed a positive effect of abstraction on product
(7), because they should be similarly positive. An independent evaluations in the form of star ratings (H3a), but not on other
sample t-test confirmed that the manipulated level of language evaluation scales (H3b,c,d).
abstraction had a significant effect on the perceived language The same univariate two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA's)
abstraction (t(38) ¼ 6.883, p < 0.001), the concrete written review were carried out with the addition of following covariates: fre-
(M ¼ 2.45, SD ¼ 1.05) was seen as significantly more concrete than quency of writing online reviews, frequency of reading online re-
the abstract written review (M ¼ 5.30, SD ¼ 1.53). However both views, mental construal (BIF), and brand familiarity. None of those
reviews did not significantly differ in valence (t(38) ¼ 0.992, covariates were significant (F < 1) and the main effect of our ma-
p ¼ 0.328). The concrete (M ¼ 5.55, SD ¼ 0.76) and the abstract one nipulations did not lose significance after including those
(M ¼ 5.80, SD ¼ 0.83) were mildly positive. We checked if the covariates.
manipulations of the perceived language abstraction and valence
were correctly in the experimental study. Similar to the pretest this 4. Study 2
indicated a successful manipulation of review abstraction
(t(99) ¼ 9.753, p < 0.001), while not affecting perceived valence 4.1. Methods
(t(94.590) ¼ 1.197, p ¼ 0.234). The abstract written review
(M ¼ 5.36, SD ¼ 1.43) received a higher abstraction score than the In Study 2, we used a different product (a smartphone). The
concrete written review (M ¼ 3.12, SD ¼ 1.29). Again, both the purpose of Study 2 was threefold. We wanted 1) to confirm and
concrete review (M ¼ 5.96, SD ¼ 0.88) and the abstract review generalize the effects of concrete elements in reviews for other
(M ¼ 5.75, SD ¼ 1.16) were mildly positive. products, 2) to test the role of valence in this earlier found effect,
and 3) to expand the methodological design to another type of
3.2. Results measurement in order to preclude that the results were an artifact
of the design of Study 1. That is why in Study 2 participants were
To test hypothesis one, an ANOVA compared the mean language asked to imagine that the review was about the smartphone that
abstraction rating between reviews that were written in a concrete they owned themselves e this approach was adapted from
way versus an abstract way. An ANOVA on language abstraction branding literature, where researchers often ask subjects about
rating, with language abstraction (concrete vs. abstract) in the perceptions of a liked or disliked brand that they can choose
previous review as independent variable revealed that participants' themselves (e.g., Batra, Ahuvia, & Bagozzi, 2012; Park, MacInnis,
reviews were affected by the prior review. Participants wrote more Priester, Eisingerich, & Iacobucci, 2010). One potential limitation
concretely after reading a concrete review (M ¼ 3.89, SD ¼ 1.04) of the first study is low ecological validity. On review websites,
compared to an abstract one (M ¼ 4.65, SD ¼ 0.92; F(l,99) ¼ 18.191, participants are not only exposed to positive online consumer re-
p < 0.001; hp2 ¼ 0.145), confirming our first hypothesis. views. Often, they get an overview of both positive and negative
A separate ANOVA also demonstrated the expected difference in reviews. We added valence as a between-subjects factor to assure
860 G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

that the effects found in our first study are not only limited to we could obtain spontaneous impressions. Next, we did not give
positive reviews. As we cannot predict the way in which valence any particular instructions to coders to fill the questionnaire apart
influences the abstractness effects on the reader and the subse- from 1) reading the review and 2) answering the questionnaire. All
quent reviewer, we propose the following research question: “Does participating raters received at the start of the survey a quick
valence in a prior online review have an impact on how abstract- training on the level of language abstraction and valence.
ness/concreteness of prior reviews affects how people think about
and write online consumer reviews?”. 4.1.5. Pretest and manipulation check
A pre-test (N ¼ 23) checked the manipulation of language
4.1.1. Design and stimuli abstraction and valence by means of a within-subjects design. As
The design for this study was a 2 (language abstraction in re- anticipated, an independent sample t-test indicated that the
view: concrete vs abstract) x 2 (valence: negative vs. positive) manipulated level of language abstraction had a significant effect
between-subjects design. We again focused on the effect of lan- on the perceived language abstraction (t(84.546) ¼ 5.080,
guage abstraction, but also the valence of a previous review, on the p < 0.001), the manipulated concrete review (M ¼ 2.57, SD ¼ 1.28)
mental construal of a next reviewer. To this end we carefully con- was seen as significantly more concrete than the manipulated ab-
structed online reviews, as in Study 1, based on an existing review stract review (M ¼ 4.13, SD ¼ 1.66). The t-test revealed as well that
from the website bol.com. Each respondent was randomly assigned the manipulated level of valence had a significant effect on
to view one of the four product descriptions (see Appendix). perceived valence (t(90) ¼ 18.457, p < 0.001), the manipulated
negative review (M ¼ 1.61, SD ¼ 0.93) was seen as significantly
4.1.2. Participants more negative than the manipulated positive review(M ¼ 5.96,
Students (N ¼ 189, 47 male, Mage 20.75, SDage ¼ 3.19, Minage ¼ 18, SD ¼ 1.30). We also checked for these manipulations in the actual
Maxage ¼ 36) participated in this study in return for course credits. study which revealed significant effects on the perceived language
Anonymity and confidentiality were guaranteed as the students did abstraction (t(187) ¼ 2.158, p ¼ 0.023), and on valence
not have to fill out their name but their student digit code number, (t(187) ¼ 28.893, p < 0.001). The manipulated concrete review
but the surveys and numbers were processed separately to warrant (M ¼ 3.77, SD ¼ 1.60) received a significant lower abstraction score
anonymity. Social science students were retrieved out of a database than the manipulated abstract review (M ¼ 4.29, SD ¼ 1.70). And
of a large European university and received an email on their stu- again, the manipulated negative review was rated as significantly
dent mail account inviting them to participate in the study. As in more negative (M ¼ 1.72, SD ¼ 0.95) than the manipulated positive
Study 1, they were given the option of opting out. review (M ¼ 5.89, SD ¼ 1.04).

4.1.3. Procedure 4.2. Results


The procedure was similar to the procedure of Study 1. However,
instead of a backpack, the participants were asked to read a review We used a univariate two-way ANOVA with language abstraction
of a productda smartphonedand imagine this was a review of (concrete vs. abstract) and valence (negative vs. positive) as
their own smartphone, which is an approach adapted from between-subjects variables. This analysis revealed a main effect of
branding literature (e.g., Batra et al., 2012; Park et al., 2010). Elec- language abstraction. Again, on the open-ended response abstrac-
tronic experience products, such as smartphones, are frequently tion index with language abstraction (concrete vs. abstract) as
purchased through online shopping websites and consumers tend between-subject factor we found a main effect. Reading abstract
to rely on comments from previous users due to the fact that they reviews resulted in more abstract review writing (F(1,185) ¼ 12.497,
have various functionalities (Park & Lee, 2009). As in Study 1, par- p < 0.01, hp2 ¼ 0.063), with a prior concrete review resulting in a
ticipants received an email containing a link, which led them to an subsequent concrete review (M ¼ 3.5,3 SD ¼ 0.69) as compared to a
online questionnaire. Each participant was randomly assigned to prior abstract one resulting in an abstract one (M ¼ 3.87, SD ¼ 0.62).
one of the four experimental conditions. Thus we again found evidence for H1. A marginally significant main
effect of valence on abstraction in participants' reviews was found
4.1.4. Measures as well (F(1,185) ¼ 3.709, p ¼ 0.056, hp2 ¼ 0.020). When re-
The measures of Study 2 were the same as in Study 1, except spondents read prior negative reviews they wrote significantly
from brand familiarity which was not measured. And again, we did more abstractly in their own review (M ¼ 3.80, SD ¼ 0.68) than
not control for demographics, because preliminary analyses indi- when they read prior positive reviews (M ¼ 3.61, SD ¼ 0.66). But no
cated that demographics did not impact the results. In addition, in interaction of language abstraction and valence on rated abstrac-
the open-ended descriptions we coded language abstraction, but tion occurred (F(1,185) ¼ 0.437, p ¼ 0.509, hp2 ¼ 0.002). No main
valence as well. This was done by a one-item seven-point scale effects of language abstraction (F(1,185) ¼ 1.156, p ¼ 0.284,
from “concrete” (1) to “abstract” (7) for language abstraction and hp2 ¼ 0.006) and valence (F(1,185) ¼ 2.081, p ¼ 0.151, hp2 ¼ 0.011),
from “negative” (1) to “positive” (7) for valence (Schellekens et al., nor an interaction effect of language abstraction and valence was
2010). Reading positive evaluations can lead to more favorable at- found on rated valence of the participants' reviews
titudes (Tsang & Prendergast, 2009), we include valence to control (F(1,185) ¼ 0.122, p ¼ 0.728, hp2 ¼ 0.001).
for this possibility. We also have to remark that the coding was In addition, we found that participants did not agree signifi-
differently from Study 1. Various raters coded 10 open-ended de- cantly more with the reviewer in the concrete condition than in the
scriptions which were randomly chosen from the initial sample of abstract condition (F(1,185) ¼ 0.171, p ¼ 0.679, hp2 ¼ 0.001).
the 189 generated reviews. This means that raters did not receive However, a main effect of valence on agreement with the reviewer
all descriptions to code but just a randomized subsample of 10. This occurred (F(1,185) ¼ 170.042, p < 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.479). Participants
method has been used before via MTurk to get large samples of data agreed significantly more in the positive condition (M ¼ 2.26,
into a content analysis (Biel & Gatica-Perez, 2013). Using this SD ¼ 1.39) as opposed to the negative one (M ¼ 4.86, SD ¼ 1.36). A
method, we intended to explore the possibility of an affordable and language abstraction by valence interaction was found as well
fast completion method that could truly scale to the annotation of (F(1,185) ¼ 4.209, p ¼ 0.042, hp2 ¼ 0.022). In the negative condition,
large amounts of written comments. Moreover, we obtained ratings participants agreed significantly more when exposed to a previous
of on average 10 coders for every review. By keeping the task short concrete review (M ¼ 2.51, SD ¼ 1.49) as compared to a previous
G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864 861

abstract one (M ¼ 2.02, SD ¼ 1.25). The univariate two-way ANOVA online consumer reviews on the reader's attitudes about the
on attitude towards the reviewer also revealed a main effect of reviewer and the product.
language abstraction with participants in the concrete condition Overall, the findings of our two studies validate our proposition
having a more positive attitude towards the reviewer that exposure to concrete written reviews results in concrete
(F(1,185) ¼ 12.201, p ¼ 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.062, M ¼ 4.05, SD ¼ 1.13) written comments of a subsequent reviewer. Perceived language
compared to the abstract condition (M ¼ 3.54; SD ¼ 1.26). We also abstraction in online consumer reviews was systematically influ-
found a main effect of valence (F(1,185) ¼ 96.990, p < 0.001, enced by language abstraction in previous reviews. These findings
hp2 ¼ 0.344), with respondents having a significant more positive suggest that language abstraction is contagious, so that the pres-
attitude towards the reviewer when exposed to a positive review ence of more concrete reviews leads new reviewers to use more
(M ¼ 3.10, SD ¼ 0.99) than when exposed to a negative one concrete language too. Earlier studies demonstrated that people
(M ¼ 4.50, SD ¼ 1.01). We did not observe an interaction between mimic each other's behavior (Adank et al., 2010; Cappella, 1996;
language abstraction and valence on attitude towards the reviewer Chen et al., 1998). Our studies suggest as well that people match
(F(1,185) ¼ 1.813, p ¼ 0.180, hp2 ¼ 0.010). These results do not each other's behavior, and more in particular match each other's
support H2a, but do support H2b. The mediation hypotheses (H2c) language when writing written comments and adapt to the level of
was done in the same way as in Study 1. Specifically, this hypothesis mental construal (Cappella, 1996; Trope & Liberman, 2010).
stated that agreement with the reviewer mediates the effects of Answering our research question, we identified an effect of valence
language abstraction on the attitude towards the reviewer. The on language abstraction. When reading a positive review, people
bootstrap confidence intervals of indirect effects were estimated will write more concretely. Next, we also found evidence for the
using a level of confidence of 95% and 5000 samples (Hayes, 2009). positive relationship between concreteness in a review and atti-
The analysis shows that the direct effect of indirect effect of lan- tudes towards a reviewer. Our findings suggest that a reader agrees
guage abstraction on the attitude towards the reviewer is not sig- more with and has a more positive attitude towards the previous
nificant (B ¼ 0.041, SE ¼ 0.110, 95% CI ¼ [0.253, 0.176]). The reviewer when that review was written concretely compared to
mediation hypothesis (H2c) cannot be confirmed. abstractly. Moreover, agreement with the reviewer mediated the
Regarding product evaluation, we again only found a significant relationship between language abstraction and attitude towards
difference of language abstraction on product rating that reviewer. And as anticipated, when reviews were written
(F(1,185) ¼ 4.914, p ¼ 0.037, hp2 ¼ 0.023). Respondents' rating in the positive, readers rated the reviewer as more positive. This can be
concrete condition was higher (M ¼ 3.90, SD ¼ 1.68) than in the explained by the fact that reviews which contain more information
abstract one (M ¼ 3.52,SD ¼ 1.65). Similar to Study 1, the effects of are more appreciated and seen as useful by the reviewer (Herr et al.,
language abstraction on product attitude (F(1,185) ¼ 1.141, 1991). The next finding is perhaps even more relevant to the con-
p ¼ 0.287, hp2 ¼ 0.006), willingness to purchase (F(1,185) ¼ 2.622, sumer behavior context. In our studies we show that participant's
p ¼ 0.107, hp2 ¼ 0.014) and willingness to recommend star rating for a product will be higher after reading an online re-
(F(1,185) ¼ 2.847, p ¼ 0.093, hp2 ¼ 0.015) were not significant in view that is more concrete as opposed to abstract. It could be, as
Study 2. However, a main effect of valence occurred for product star earlier mentioned, that concrete reviews are seen as more useful,
rating (F(1,185) ¼ 163.095, p < 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.469), product attitude resulting in a higher rating (Bansal & Voyer, 2000; Herr et al., 1991;
(F(1,185) ¼ 47.377, p < 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.204), willingness to purchase Jimenez & Mendoza, 2013). It could be that we only found an effect
(F(1,185) ¼ 31.810, p < 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.147) and willingness to on star rating as a measurement of product evaluations because
recommend (F(1,185) ¼ 31.369, p < 0.001, hp2 ¼ 0.145) with re- ratings provide the reader with shortcut means to quickly evaluate
spondents being significantly more positive in the positive the product (Tsang & Prendergast, 2009) in contrast to the other
compared to the negative condition. No significant interactions measurement scales. And as earlier widely demonstrated, positive
were found, nor on star rating (F(1,185) ¼ 0.448, p ¼ 0.504, prior reviews lead to more positive attitudes towards the product
hp2 ¼ 0.002), product attitude (F(1,185) ¼ 0.000, p ¼ 0.987; (Chevalier & Mayzlin, 2006; King et al., 2014). This study clearly
hp2 ¼ 0.000), willingness to purchase (F(1,185) ¼ 1.955, p ¼ 0.164; illustrates the contagious effect of language abstraction in the on-
hp2 ¼ 0.010) and willingness to recommend (F(1,185) ¼ 0.115, line review context. Further research should also investigate the
p ¼ 0.735; hp2 ¼ 0.001). Again, these findings showed a positive implications of this effect. This is important because concrete (vs.
effect of abstraction on product evaluations in the form of star abstract) reviews induce more favorable evaluations of reviewers,
ratings (H3a), but not on other evaluation scales (H3b,c,d). reviewed products.
The same univariate two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA's)
were performed with the addition of the covariates as in Study 1, 5.1. Implications
except from brand familiarity. In Study 2 we did not use a non-
familiar brand but asked participants to imagine that the review Our findings, which suggest that language abstraction is con-
was about the smartphone that they owned themselves. None of tagious, have important implications, not only for researchers, but
those covariates were significant (F < 1) and the main effect of our also for marketers and consumers. On the one hand, our findings
manipulations did not lose significance after including those confirm that linguistic style matching can be acquired through
covariates. language abstraction in prior online comments. In other words,
reviewers that express their product experience in a certain way
5. General discussion can influence subsequent reviewers. Moreover, our findings could
be applied in the context of service reviews such as experiences
Yet, despite the increased prevalence of consumer reviews, an with hotels, events, music concerts etc. On the other hand, online
increased availability of measures to index a review's popularity, consumer review platforms may be effective platforms for seeking
but also an increased consumer skepticism, little is known about product information, as readers perceive through this online plat-
the effect of language abstraction in reviews. Our studies addressed form what they are searching for. And more in particular, we found
this and examined language use and its effects on both sides of the that concrete reviews have a positive effect on readers' opinions
word-of-mouth communication twain. First, we assessed the about products. Thus it may be valuable for future research to
impact of language abstraction on the reviewer's writing behavior. explore the role of other concrete review elements as well.
Second, we gained insight in the impact of language abstraction in Analyzing the level of abstractness of the language that consumers
862 G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

use in product reviews could help review websites to build their single product categories in both studies may limit generalizability
website in such a way that consumers make the best thoughtful of the results. Although we limited ourselves to the use of two
decisions. For example, a marketer attempting to generate traffic to search products, their possess attributes can only be evaluated
a review web site or an ad should add concrete elements in or next prior to their purchases. Meaning that those reviews can give
to online consumer reviews. It could even be argued that when consumers more wisdom about the product in contrast to experi-
consumers are invited by e-mail to write an online consumer re- ence products. Fourth, a potential limitation of the first study is low
view after purchase, a concrete example should be added to that e- ecological validity. Consumers are not only exposed to positive
mail. Review platforms could capitalize on this too by seeding online consumer reviews on review platforms. Usually, there is an
concrete reviews, by building in features that promote the use of overview of both positive and negative reviews. In the second
concrete language, or by explicitly instructing reviewers to use study, negative reviews were as well included, however, partici-
more concrete language. Indicators such as an instruction about the pants were still exposed to only one review. Future studies should
minimum length of the review or pictures about the product could include more reviews to balance valence effects. Fifth, another
be used. The attractiveness of the content of the online consumer limitation is that earlier research on word of mouth in the offline
reviews will significantly enhance as the number of visits is gath- context seems to found opposite effects (Schellekens et al., 2010).
ering. Hence, the foregoing product evaluations are rather relevant The difference might lie in the fact that we, as compared to the
for products that are difficult to assess before use. earlier mentioned researchers, did not use the linguistic category
Next to the online consumer review context, our findings could model (Semin & Fiedler, 1988) as a framework. This model was used
be used in other social media communication contexts. Previous for studying the language that people use to describe social events
research already demonstrated that posters adapt their writing and thus not for studying the used language to describe product
style to the style of previous posters within the same discussion on experiences. Instead of using descriptive action verbs in the con-
internet fora (Welbers & de Nooy, 2014). Thus, it could as well be crete condition, our concrete contained more information and thus
that the language style of, for instance, Twitter writing and blog more details which are translated into more concrete features. The
writing depends on the language style used in a prior Tweet or blog contrast between these findings highlight the need for further
one is exposed to. Our findings do not only enrich the theoretical research.
knowledge about the writing of posts, but will also help social
media experts and marketers to develop effective social
networking and advertising strategies via online platforms. 5.3. Conclusion

In conclusion, these findings extend prior research on the rela-


5.2. Limitations and future research tionship between the level of detailed information in online con-
sumer reviews and readers' attitudes towards the reviewer and the
This study is the first to our knowledge that investigates the product, by looking at the role of mental construal. The results
impact of language abstraction in online reviews on the next demonstrated that readers' agree more and have a better attitude
reviewer, uncovering a potential concreteness leads to concrete- towards the reviewer when exposed to a prior concrete review.
ness effect. Next, our findings are consistent with the proposition Reviewer agreement served as a moderator on attitudes towards
that customers read and rely on information in written online the product. In addition, when presented such a concrete review,
consumer reviews during their decision making processes the reader rated the product higher via star rating. In spite of a large
(Chevalier & Mayzlin, 2006). However, various limitations of our amount of research on word of mouth, there has been little
research provide worthwhile avenues for future research. First, attention on the language that consumers use to describe their
although we consider the whole written comment, our empirical experiences with products and services to others. Our studies
study featured only one linguistic style: the language abstraction. addressed this and showed that concreteness in prior online con-
Other dimensions of written comments could also have an effect. sumer reviews leads to concreteness in subsequent online con-
Therefore, future research should include them and examine how sumer reviews.
factors such as the content, the source, the review context and the
design of the platform could influence the persuasiveness of online
consumer reviews. In this regard, it is especially interesting to look Acknowledgement
at factors that may influence construal level. Next to psychological
distance, self-construal is an additional factor that may come into This work was supported by a grant from the Flemish Fund for
play here (Bernritter, Loermans, Verlegh, & Smit, 2017). Second, Scientific Research (FWO), which was gained by the two last au-
other moderators could impact both reviewers and readers of thors [grant number: G078815N].
which one could be the volume of reviews. In our studies we pre-
sented participants towards just one review. Earlier studies showed
that consumers base their opinion and thus their decision on the Appendix
signaled consensus (Benedicktus et al., 2010; Zhu & Zhang, 2010).
Future studies should therefore employ different but balanced Examples of text in the manipulations.
consumer reviews (Purnawirawan et al., 2012). Third, the use of

Study 1

Concrete review Abstract review

“I recommend this backpack. The shoulder and back straps of the backpack are “This backpack is recommended by me. In my opinion, the backpack sits great,
padded for extra comfort. In the various spacious compartments you can which is also spacious. The use of this backpack is pleasant. The backpack has
store a lot of stuff. There is a separate compartment to protect your laptop several times used by me and I will do this many times in the future too.”
from scratches and other damage.”
*
translations, original texts can be obtained from the authors.
G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864 863

Study 2

Concrete review Abstract review

Negative review “A while ago, I bought this smartphone, which operates below par. The “This worthless smartphone was recently bought by me. The appliance
appliance has a low stand-by time and the Li-ion battery recharges will not last that long and has poor reception. I can hardly take pictures
slowly. I almost never have the maximum range, even with 3G internet with this phone. The smartphone is inconvenient and is worse than my
connection. The camera on the back makes blurry photos and videos. ” previous phone. I can do little with it, the appliance disappoints me.”
Positive review “A while ago, I bought this smartphone, which operates excellent. The “This valuable smartphone was recently bought by me. The appliance
appliance has a high stand-by time and the Li-ion battery recharges will last very long and has good reception. I can easily take pictures with
quickly. I almost always have the maximum range, even with 3G this phone. The smartphone is convenient and is better than my
internet connection. The camera on the back makes clear photos and previous phone. I can do a lot with it, the appliance satisfies me.”
videos. ”
*
translations, original texts can be obtained from the authors.

References Huang, N., Burtch, G., Hong, Y., & Polman, E. (2016,). Effects of multiple psycho-
logical distances on construal and consumer evaluation: A field study of online
reviews. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(4), 474e482. http://dx.doi.org/
Adank, P., Hagoort, P., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Imitation improves language
10.1016/j.jcps.2016.03.001.
comprehension. Psychological Review, 21, 1903e1909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Huang, A. H., Chen, K., Yen, D. C., & Tran, T. P. (2015). A study of factors that
0956797610389192.
contribute to online review helpfulness. Computers in Human Behavior, 48,
Bansal, H. S., & Voyer, P. A. (2000). Word-of-Mouth processes within a services
17e27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.010.
purchase decision context. Journal of Service Research, 3(2), 166e177. http://
nez, F. R., & Mendoza, N. A. (2013). Too popular to ignore: The influence of
Jime
dx.doi.org/10.1177/109467050032005.
online reviews on purchase intentions of search and experience products.
Batra, R., Ahuvia, A. C., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2012). Brand love. Journal of Marketing, 76(2),
Journal of Interactive Marketing, 27, 226e235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
1e16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jm.09.0339.
j.intmar.2013.04.004.
Benedicktus, R. L., Brady, M. K., Darke, P. R., & Voorhees, C. M. (2010). Conveying
Kim, Y. J., & Hollingshead, A. B. (2015). Online social influence: Past, present, and
trustworthiness to online consumers: Reactions to consensus, physical store
future. Annals of the International Communication Association, 39(1), 163e192.
presence, brand familiarity, and generalized suspicion. Journal of Retailing,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2015.11679175.
86(4), 322e335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretai.2010.04.002.
King, R. A., Racherla, P., & Bush, V. D. (2014). What we know and don't know about
Bernritter, S. F., Loermans, A. C., Verlegh, P. W. J., & Smit, E. G. (2017). .‘We’are more
online word-of-mouth: A review and synthesis of the literature. Journal of
likely to endorse than ‘I’: The effects of self-construal and brand symbolism on
Interactive Marketing, 28, 167e183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
consumers' online brand endorsements. International Journal of Advertising,
j.intmar.2014.02.001.
36(1), 107e120.
Liang, Y. J. (2016). Reading to make a decision or to reduce cognitive dissonance?
Biel, J. I., & Gatica-Perez, D. (2013). The youtube lens: Crowdsourced personality
The effect of selecting and reading online reviews from a post-decision context.
impressions and audiovisual analysis of vlogs. IEEE Transactions on Multimedia,
Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 463e471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
15(1), 41e55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TMM.2012.2225032.
j.chb.2016.07.016.
Cappella, J. N. (1996). Dynamic coordination of vocal and kinesic behavior in dyadic
Ludwig, S., de Ruyter, K., Friedman, M., Brüggen, E. C., Wetzels, M., & Pfann, G.
interaction: Methods, problems, and interpersonal outcomes. In J. Watt, &
(2013). More than words: The influence of affective content and linguistic style
C. Van Lear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in communication processes (pp. 353e386).
matches in online reviews on conversion rates. Journal of Marketing, 77, 87e103.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jm.11.0560.
Chartrand, T., & Bargh, J. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link
Madden, J., Allen, C. T., & Twible, J. L. (1988). Attitude toward the ad: An assessment
and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893e910.
of diverse measurement indices under different processing ‘sets’. Journal of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893.
Marketing Research, 25(3), 242e252. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3172527.
Chen, M., Chartrand, T. L., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Priming primates:
Ma, X., Khansa, L., Deng, Y., & Kim, S. S. (2013). Impact of prior reviews on the
Human and otherwise. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 21, 685e686. http://
subsequent review process in reputation systems. Journal of Management In-
dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98231748.
formation Systems, 30(3), 279e310. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/MIS0742-
Cheung, C. M. K., & Thadani, D. R. (2012). The impact of electronic word-of-mouth
1222300310.
communication: A literature analysis and integrative model. Decision Support
Moe, W. W., & Schweidel, D. A. (2012). Online product opinions: Incidence, evalu-
Systems, 54(1), 461e470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dss.2012.06.008.
ation, and evolution. Marketing Science, 31(3), 372e386. http://dx.doi.org/
Chevalier, J. A., & Mayzlin, D. (2006). The effect of word of mouth on sales: Online
10.1287/mksc.1110.0662.
book reviews. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(3), 4345e4354. http://
Muir, K., Joinson, A., Cotterill, R., & Dewdney, N. (2016). Characterizing the linguistic
dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.43.3.345.
chameleon: Personal and social correlates of linguistic style accommodation.
Chua, A. Y. K., & Banerjee, S. (2016). Helpfulness of user-generated reviews as a
Human Communication Research, 42(3), 462e484. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
function of review sentiment, product type and information quality. Computers
hcre.12083.
in Human Behavior, 54, 547e554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.08.057.
Niederhoffer, K. G., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Linguistic style matching in social
Forman, C., Ghose, A., & Wiesenfeld, B. (2008). Examining the relationship between
interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(4), 337e360. http://
reviews and sales: The role of reviewer identity disclosure in electronic mar-
dx.doi.org/10.1177/026192702237953.
kets. Information Systems Research, 19(3), 291e313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/
Nielsen. (2015). Global trust in Advertising: Winning strategies for an evolving media
isre.1080.0193.
landscape. http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/reports/2015/global-trust-
Freitas, A. L., Salovey, P., & Liberman, N. (2001). Abstract and concrete self-
in-advertising-2015.html (Accessed 20 December 2016).
evaluative goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 410e423.
Park, C., & Lee, T. (2009). Information direction, website reputation and eWOM
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.3.410.
effect: A moderating role of product type. Journal of Business Research, 62(1),
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-
61e67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.11.017.
control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351e367. http://
Park, C., MacInnis, D., Priester, J., Eisingerich, A., & Iacobucci, D. (2010). Brand
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351.
attachment and brand attitude strength: Conceptual and empirical differenti-
Hamilton, R. W., Schlosser, A., & Chen, Y.-J. (2017). Who's driving this conversation?
ation of two critical brand equity drivers. Journal of Marketing, 74, 1e17. http://
Systematic biases in the content of online consumer decisions. Journal of
dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.74.6.1.
Marketing Research. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0012 (in press).
Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). The secret life of pronouns: How our words reflect who we
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the
are. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Press.
new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408e420. http://
Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to
dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360.
advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Con-
Hennig-Thurau, T., Malthouse, E. C., Friege, C., Gensler, S., Lobschat, L.,
sumer Research, 10, 135e146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208954.
Rangaswamy, A., et al. (2010). The impact of new media on customer re-
Purnawirawan, N., Dens, N., & De Pelsmacker, P. (2012). Balance and sequence in
lationships. Journal of Service Research, 13(3), 311e330. http://dx.doi.org/
online reviews: The wrap effect. International Journal of Electronic Commerce,
10.1177/1094670510375460.
17(2), 71e98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/JEC1086-4415170203.
Herr, P. M., Kardes, F. R., & Kim, J. (1991). Effects of word-of-mouth and product-
Schellekens, G., Verlegh, P., & Smidts, A. (2010). Language abstraction in word of
attribute information on persuasion: An accessibility-diagnosticity perspec-
mouth. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 207e223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/
tive. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(4), 454e462. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/
651240.
208570.
Schellekens, G. A. C., Verlegh, P. W. J., & Smidts, A. (2012). Linguistic biases and
Hong, S., & Park, H. S. (2012). Computer-mediated persuasion in online reviews:
persuasion in communication about objects. Journal of Language and Social
Statistical versus narrative evidence. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3),
Psychology, 32(3), 291e310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0261927X12466083.
906e919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.12.011.
864 G. Aerts et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 75 (2017) 855e864

Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in of Marketing, 43(11/12), 1269e1280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/
describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and 03090560910989876.
Social Psychology, 54, 558e568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.558. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual
Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1989). Relocating attributional phenomena with a lan- variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
guage cognition interface: The case of actors' and observers' perspectives. Eu- 57(4), 660e671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.
ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 491e508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ Walther, J. B., DeAndrea, D., Kim, J., & Anthony, J. C. (2010). The influence of online
ejsp.2420190602. comments on perceptions of antimarijuana public service announcements on
Sen, S., & Lerman, D. (2007). Why are you telling me this? An examination into YouTube. Human Communication Research, 36(4), 469e492. http://dx.doi.org/
negative consumer reviews on the web. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21(4), 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01384.x.
76e94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dir.20090. Welbers, K., & de Nooy, W. (2014). Stylistic accommodation on an internet forum as
Shan, Y. (2016). How credible are online product reviews? The effects of self- bonding: Do posters adapt to the style of their peers? American Behavioral
generated and system-generated cues on source credibility evaluation. Com- Scientist, 58(10), 1361e1375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002764214527086.
puters in Human Behavior, 55, 633e641. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Wright, M., & MacRae, M. (2007). Bias and variability in purchase intention scales.
j.chb.2015.10.013. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 35(4), 617e624. http://dx.doi.org/
Stephan, E., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2010). Politeness and social distance: A 10.1007/s11747-007-0049-x.
construal level perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, Woong Yun, G., & Park, S. Y. (2011). Selective posting: Willingness to post a message
268e280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016960. online. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 16(2), 201e227. http://
Thomson, R., & Murachver, T. (2001). Predicting gender from electronic discourse. dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2010.01533.x.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 193e208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ Wu, L., Mattila, A. S., Wang, C. Y., & Hanks, L. (2016). The impact of power on service
014466601164812. customers' willingness to post online reviews. Journal of Service Research, 19(2),
Thomson, R., Murachver, T., & Green, J. (2001). Where is the gender in gendered 224e238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094670516630623.
language? Psychological Science, 12(2), 171e175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467- Zhao, M., & Xie, J. (2011). Effects of social and temporal distance on consumers'
9280.00329. responses to peer recommendations. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3),
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110, 486e496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.3.486.
403e421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403. Zhu, F., & Zhang, X. M. (2010). Impact of online consumer reviews on sales: The
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. moderating role of product and consumer characteristics. Journal of Marketing,
Psychological Review, 117(2), 440e463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018963. 74(2), 133e148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.74.2.133.
Tsang, A. S. L., & Prendergast, G. (2009). Is a “star” worth a thousand words? The Zizzo, D. J. (2010). Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Exper-
interplay between product-review texts and rating valences. European Journal imental Economics., 13(1), 75e98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-009-9230-z.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen