Sie sind auf Seite 1von 18

European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology

ISSN: 1359-432X (Print) 1464-0643 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pewo20

Creative and innovative performance: a meta-


analysis of relationships with task, citizenship, and
counterproductive job performance dimensions

Michael B. Harari, Angela C. Reaves & Chockalingam Viswesvaran

To cite this article: Michael B. Harari, Angela C. Reaves & Chockalingam Viswesvaran
(2016): Creative and innovative performance: a meta-analysis of relationships with task,
citizenship, and counterproductive job performance dimensions, European Journal of Work
and Organizational Psychology

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2015.1134491

Published online: 20 Jan 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 13

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pewo20

Download by: [Gazi University] Date: 26 January 2016, At: 12:16


European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2015.1134491

Creative and innovative performance: a meta-analysis of relationships with task, citizenship,


and counterproductive job performance dimensions
Michael B. Hararia*, Angela C. Reavesb and Chockalingam Viswesvaranb
a
Department of Management, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL, USA; bDepartment of Psychology, Florida International
University, Miami, FL, USA
(Received 25 May 2015; accepted 16 December 2015)

Many studies have examined creative and innovative performance (CIP)–task performance, CIP–organizational citizenship
behaviour (OCB), and CIP–counterproductive work behaviour (CWB) relationships in order to differentiate CIP from these
established job performance dimensions. However, the overlap between CIP and these performance dimensions is still not
clear due to mixed findings evident in the literature. To address this issue, we conducted a comprehensive meta-analytic
review of empirical research into CIP–task performance, CIP–OCB, and CIP–CWB relationships derived from 39 studies
and 40 independent samples. Overall, CIP was positively related to task performance (ρ = .55) and OCB (ρ = .56) and
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

negatively related to CWB (ρ = −.23). We did not observe evidence suggesting that CIP measurement, rating source, OCB
target, or CWB type moderated these relationships. Implications of our findings and directions for future research are
discussed.
Keywords: Innovation; creativity; creative and innovative performance; job performance

Individual job performance—behaviours engaged in or modern organizations (Eisenhardt & Tabrizi, 1995;
outcomes brought about by employees that contribute to Geroski, Machin, & Van Reenen, 1993; Gong, Zhou,
the functioning of organizations—is one of the most & Chang, 2013), research has explicated creativity and
central constructs in Industrial, Work, and innovation as critical dimensions of individual job per-
Organizational (IWO) Psychology and Human formance (e.g., Oldham & Cummings, 1996; Tierney &
Resource Management (Campbell & Wiernik, 2015). Farmer, 2002).
Considerable work has been conducted in order to Individual creative and innovative performance (CIP)
explicate the dimensionality of job performance and has been taking a more prominent role in models of job
this remains an important issue (see Viswesvaran & performance and the construct has been the focus of a
Ones, 2000 for a review). While ubiquitous job perfor- great deal of research. Many studies have investigated
mance dimensions, such as task performance, organiza- interventions (e.g., selection, job design) that can impact
tional citizenship behaviours (OCBs), and individual CIP (e.g., Pace & Brannick, 2010; Zhou, 1998).
counterproductive work behaviours (CWBs), have been When novel dimensions of job performance are intro-
identified in the literature, as the nature of work con- duced in the literature, understanding their relationships
tinues to change, new job performance dimensions are with existing dimensions of job performance is a critical
introduced. For example, due to the dynamic nature of issue. Estimates of these relationships are necessary for
jobs in the modern workplace, research has explicated empirically differentiating the performance dimensions
adaptive performance as a critical dimension of indivi- and for advancing our understanding of the structure of
dual job performance (Pulakos, Arad, Donovan, & individual job performance (Hoffman, Blair, Meriac, &
Plamondon, 2000). Similarly, due to the emergence of Woehr, 2007; Viswesvaran, Schmidt, & Ones, 2005).
the knowledge economy and the importance of knowl- Understanding CIP’s relationship with task performance,
edge sharing for organizational effectiveness, knowl- OCB, and CWB is particularly important, given their
edge transfer has been described as an important ubiquity in the literature, and is the focus of the present
dimension of job performance (Harari, Jain, & Joseph, study.
2014). In much the same way, due to the importance of Given the importance of this issue, many studies have
individual creativity and innovation for the success of examined CIP–task performance, CIP–OCB, and CIP–

*Corresponding author. Email: mharari@fau.edu

© 2016 Taylor & Francis


2 M.B. Harari et al.

CWB relationships. However, there is great disparity in following sections, we discuss CIP in greater depth.
findings across these studies and it is therefore not possi- Following this, we review the dimensions of task perfor-
ble to draw firm conclusions from the literature. In the mance, OCB, and CWB and discuss potential mechanisms
present study, we use meta-analysis to synthesize findings linking CIP to these dimensions.
across studies, providing a summary of the state of the
literature and point estimates of population correlations.
Such an approach has been useful in many other studies Creative and innovative performance
for understanding this important issue of performance Changes to the competitive landscape of many organiza-
dimension intercorrelations. For example, meta-analyses tions have led to creativity and innovation being consid-
have been conducted to synthesize relationships between ered key variables in the organizational sciences with
task performance and OCB (Hoffman et al., 2007), task implications for firm performance (Anderson, Potocnik,
performance and CWB (Carpenter & Berry, in press), and & Zhou, 2014). Creativity refers specifically to idea gen-
OCB and CWB (Dalal, 2005). eration, while innovation refers to idea generation and
Obtaining robust point estimates of the population implementation (Anderson et al., 2014; Hulsheger,
correlations (e.g., CIP–task performance) allows us to Anderson, & Salgado, 2009). While the role of creativity
test whether the confidence intervals do not include 1.0, and innovation as determinants of organizational perfor-
suggesting that ratings of CIP and the other performance mance is most immediately relevant in organizations that
dimensions are empirically distinct. Before investigating introduce innovative products into the marketplace, the
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

whether the different dimensions of performance (e.g., importance of creativity and innovation spans across jobs
CIP, task performance, OCB) have differential validity and organizations (Zhou, 2008). Indeed, across work
(or unique distinct patterns of correlations with external environments, developing or altering products, processes,
variables), we need to establish that the corrected correla- or procedures has the potential to improve efficiencies,
tions are significantly different from 1.0. Thus, establish- reduce waste, enhance operational outcomes, and impact
ing robust point estimates of the correlations between the bottom line. Therefore, creativity and innovation can
performance dimensions is a necessary first step before contribute to the effectiveness of most (if not all) organi-
embarking on a campaign to discover different patterns of zations and jobs (Eisenhardt & Tabrizi, 1995; Geroski
correlations with external variables. et al., 1993).
As an example, for many decades we believed that As a job performance dimension, CIP encompasses
peers and supervisors were rating different constructs creative and innovative behaviours and outcomes (e.g.,
when they assessed different performance dimensions introducing new ideas into the work environment in a
(e.g., compliance). Viswesvaran, Schmidt, and Ones systematic way; generating original solutions for pro-
(2002) showed that when correlations between supervi- blems; Janssen, 2000) versus creative and innovative
sory and peer ratings of a dimension (e.g., compliance) processes that result in those behaviours and outcomes
were corrected for intersupervisor and interpeer reliability, (Campbell & Wiernik, 2015; Zhang & Bartol, 2010b).
the corrected correlations were largely not statistically Oldham and Cummings (1996) summarized that defini-
different from 1.0—suggesting that peers and supervisors tions of CIP focus on “the product or outcome of a
were typically rating the same dimension, although they product development process” (p. 608). Zhang and
may be sampling different behaviours from that same Bartol (2010b) noted that CIP “refers to creative out-
construct domain. Theoretically, it is one thing to say comes” (p. 862). Janssen (2000) defined CIP as “the
that peers and supervisors are rating two different con- intentional creation, introduction, and application of
structs and another thing to say that they are sampling new ideas within a work role, group or organization”
different samples of behaviour from the same construct (p. 288). Thus, CIP refers to the proficiency with which
domain. The same issue is relevant here. While CIP is employees generate and implement novel ideas in the
considered distinct from existing performance dimensions, workplace (see also Scott & Bruce, 1994), while crea-
research is needed to test this notion by first establishing tive and innovative processes—methods that result in
that the CIP–performance dimension relationships differ creative ideas, such as problem identification and infor-
from 1.0. Assuming these correlations do differ from 1.0, mation searching—are conceptualized as predictors
subsequent research can then advance this literature by (Zhang & Bartol, 2010a).
examining differential validity between the dimensions. In the literature under study, CIP has been operationa-
Considering the growing interest in CIP, a meta-ana- lized as either creative performance (i.e., idea generation)
lysis into CIP–task performance, CIP–OCB, and CIP– or innovative performance (i.e., idea generation and
CWB relationships is needed. In conducting such a implementation). In order to provide a fine-grained analy-
meta-analysis, we respond to several calls in the literature sis of CIP–performance dimension correlations, where
for meta-analyses into the correlates of CIP (Anderson, De possible, we conduct all analyses using: (a) all measures
Dreu, & Nijstad, 2004; Anderson & King, 1993). In the (i.e., creative performance or innovative performance), (b)
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 3

innovative performance only, and (c) creative performance CIP and the other performance dimensions (Viswesvaran
only. et al., 2005). In addition to a general factor of job perfor-
mance, halo error can account for inflated covariation
between ratings of different performance dimensions if
CIP and job performance dimensions they are obtained from the same rater.
While CIP has been operationalized as a novel and unique One theoretical framework for understanding halo
dimension of job performance in the literature, empirical error is the cognitive information processing perspective
evidence is needed to differentiate CIP from other dimen- of performance evaluation (Cooper, 1981). Such models
sions of performance. Two factors contribute to the over- focus on the cognitive processes involved in rating job
lap between ratings of different performance dimensions: a performance and how they could result in rating errors
general factor of job performance and halo error (Cooper, such as halo. Several such models exist (e.g., Borman,
1981; Viswesvaran et al., 2005). Both of these are impor- 1978; DeNisi, Cafferty, & Meglino, 1984; Feldman, 1981)
tant considerations here. A general factor of job perfor- and, while there are differences between them, they share
mance represents substantive overlap between the the following basic cognitive processes involved in rating
dimensions of job performance. This can occur as a result performance: observation, encoding, storage, retrieval, and
of shared determinants between the performance dimen- integration.
sions. Halo error is a psychological process that occurs Cooper (1981) reviewed how errors involving several
when raters hold a particular impression of a ratee that of these processes could result in halo. For example, when
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

influences their performance ratings similarly across observing performance, an undersampling of ratee beha-
dimensions (Thorndike, 1920). With this in mind, in this viour can result in halo error because a lack of actual
section, we review the interactionist model of CIP as an performance observations would require raters to rely on
overarching theoretical framework guiding our study. In overall impressions when rating performance. When
addition, we review cognitive information processing encoding performance observations, raters are likely to
models of performance evaluation. Note that this discus- use idiosyncratic factors and available organization
sion is meant to provide a broad framework for our study. themes, which can result in halo error. Finally, when
In subsequent sections, we discuss more specific mechan- these encoded observations are stored in memory, ineffi-
isms linking CIP to each of the individual performance ciencies and loss of detail over time can result in mem-
dimensions. ories that are saturated by a general impression. In short,
The interactionist model of CIP (Woodman, Sawyer, all these processes result in a global evaluation (idiosyn-
& Griffin, 1993) proposes that individual-level CIP is a cratic to that rater) of the ratee that influences ratings of
function of complex person-by-situation interactions. that ratee in all specific dimensions. The effects of halo
Specifically, cognitive (e.g., cognitive ability, cognitive can be summarized as follows. Between-rater reliabilities
styles, knowledge) and non-cognitive (e.g., personality, and between-rater inter-dimension correlations are reduced
motivation) individual differences account for CIP and by halo error, whereas within-rater reliabilities and within-
these effects are moderated by social influences (e.g., rater inter-dimension correlations are inflated by halo
social facilitation, social rewards) and contextual influ- error. The psychological process underlying halo error—
ences (e.g., task and time constraints). Many individual idiosyncratic rater impressions of each ratee—is symme-
characteristics highlighted as CIP predictors in the inter- trical: It inflates all within-rater inter-dimension correla-
actionist model are valid predictors of other dimensions of tions and deflates all between-rater inter-dimension
job performance. For example, cognitive predictors of CIP correlations in comparison to values that would be
such as cognitive ability and job knowledge are predictors observed in the absence of halo error (Viswesvaran
of task performance as well (Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, et al., 2005).
2004; Salgado, Anderson, Moscoso, Bertua, & de Fruyt, Beyond the role of a general factor of job performance
2003; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Cognitive ability is also and halo error, causal relationships between job perfor-
linked to CWBs (Dilchert, Ones, Davis, & Rostow, 2007). mance dimensions are possible (MacKenzie, Podsakoff, &
Research also supports that an internal locus of control, Ahearne, 1998; Viswesvaran, 2002). Competing theoreti-
which is specified in the interactionist model as a predictor cal perspectives exist that predict different relationships
of CIP, predicts OCB (Motowidlo & Van Scotter, 1994). between CIP and the different performance dimensions
CIP and OCB are also both predicted by prosocial motiva- and, due to discrepant findings in the literature, these
tion (Grant & Berry, 2011; Grant & Mayer, 2009). relationships are not clear. As noted earlier, a great deal
Because CIP, in part, is a function of individual differ- of research has been conducted in order to explicate the
ences that are also determinants of task performance, dimensionality of job performance, with many multidi-
OCB, and CWB, we would expect overlap between rat- mensional models proposed (Viswesvaran & Ones,
ings of these dimensions. That is, a general factor of job 2000). This vast literature has resulted in three broad,
performance could account for overlap between ratings of ubiquitous dimensions of job performance that are
4 M.B. Harari et al.

applicable across jobs: task performance, OCBs, and .80 on a sample of N = 170 (Janssen & VanYperen, 2004)
CWBs. We discuss each in the following sections. indicates that the estimated true score correlation does not
include 1.0 in its 95% confidence interval—suggesting the
two dimensions are distinct and have the potential for
Task performance differential validity. On the other hand, an observed
Task performance speaks to the proficiency with which CIP–task performance correlation of .75 with the two
employees carry out the core requirements of their jobs, dimensions measured with a coefficient alpha of .80 on a
such as those tasks that are specified in a job description sample of N = 171 (Oldham & Cummings, 1996) indicates
(Motowidlo, Borman, & Schmit, 1997; Murphy, 1989). that the estimated true score correlation does include 1.0 in
Task performance constitutes behaviours that contribute its 95% confidence interval—suggesting the two dimen-
directly or indirectly to the technical core of the organiza- sions are not distinct and unlikely to have the potential for
tion. Creative and innovative behaviours by individuals differential validity. If we were to use an inter-rater relia-
should enhance task performance as the behaviours are bility estimate of .52 (Viswesvaran, Ones, & Schmidt,
introduced to solve a workplace issue (Walberg & Stariha, 1996; Viswesvaran, Ones, Schmidt, Le, & Oh, 2014)
1992). Drawing on expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964), instead of a coefficient alpha of .80, the potential for
Yuan and Woodman (2010) proposed that perceived per- overlap between ratings of CIP and task performance
formance improvements were a determinant of CIP—that appears even greater. Furthermore, to test theoretical mod-
is, employees would be more likely to engage in creative els that include CIP and task performance with meta-
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

and innovative behaviours if they believed that doing so analysed correlation matrices (cf. Viswesvaran & Ones,
would positively impact their task performance. Results 1995), it is essential to estimate the exact magnitude of
supported this contention. Other research also suggests this relationship. A point estimate of the true score corre-
that employees engage in CIP in order to facilitate perfor- lation is also required for utility analysis (selection or
mance in core work-related tasks, for example, by devel- training that focuses on CIP and task performance). It
oping and implementing new methods for carrying out does not suffice to say the relationship is positive. Thus,
tasks or by modifying existing procedures (Gong, the purpose of our study is to advance cumulative knowl-
Huang, & Farh, 2009). Thus, there is likely to be a edge with respect to the CIP–task performance relation-
positive relationship between CIP and task performance. ship, going beyond the conclusion that it is positive,
However, a competing perspective exists. As refer- towards an understanding of the actual magnitude of the
enced earlier, CIP occurs as a result of creative processes, relationship.
such as idea generation (Zang & Bartol, 2010a, 2010b).
Engaging in creative processes needed for superior CIP
can detract from individual task performance. Attention Organizational citizenship behaviours
capacity theory (Kahneman, 1973) suggests that indivi- While early research into the content domain of job per-
duals possess finite attentional capacity and cognitive formance focused squarely on core requirements, research
resources. As a result, employees are not able to apply began to take the perspective that job performance entailed
equivalent resources towards competing demands faced at more than task performance alone. This thinking gave rise
work. An emphasis placed on superior creative perfor- to research into OCBs—discretionary behaviours that,
mance could detract from task performance, while an while not formally recognized as constituting performance
emphasis on superior task performance could detract in a given job, nonetheless contribute to the functioning of
from creative performance (Scott, 1995). Thus, even organizations (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993; Organ, 1997).
though CIP might be engaged in with the ultimate aim OCB is multidimensional. While several dimensional
of improving task performance, employees might have models exist, the most broadly applicable distinction con-
less attention and fewer cognitive resources available to cerns the target of the OCB: individuals versus the orga-
perform core work tasks after engaging in the processes nization. OCB-I refers to extra-role behaviours directed
needed for CIP. This could lead to a smaller or null towards individuals in the workplace, while OCB-O refers
relationship between CIP and task performance (though to extra-role behaviours directed towards the organization
not likely a negative relationship). itself.
The extant literature has estimated the CIP–task per- OCB is often conceptualized as a predictor of CIP
formance relationship as varying between .17 and .75. (e.g., Xerri & Brunetto, 2013). OCB is an important
While we can conclude from the existing literature that means by which employees build social capital (i.e.,
the relationship is positive, it is still not clear exactly what strong interpersonal relationships shared among employ-
the relationship is and if it differs significantly from 1.0. ees; Bolino, Turnley, & Bloodgood, 2002). This is impor-
As we discuss here, this is an important issue. An tant for CIP for a few reasons. First, social capital
observed CIP–task performance correlation of .17 with facilitates the development of shared perspectives among
the two dimensions measured with a coefficient alpha of employees. Drawing on motivated information processing
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 5

theory (e.g., Kunda, 1990), Grant and Berry (2011) argued have a negative impact on employees’ relationships with
that perspective taking facilitates CIP because those who their superiors. For example, research suggests that orga-
can take the perspective of others are more likely to nizational records of absenteeism are negatively associated
identify issues that can be addressed by creative and with supervisory ratings of employee effort and interper-
innovative solutions. Social capital is also necessary for sonal behaviour (Viswesvaran, 2002). An inability to gain
idea implementation. As noted in Janssen (2000), “the the support of one’s supervisors due to CWB would be
innovation process consists of idea promotion to potential detrimental to CIP. CIP and CWB should therefore be
allies. That is, once a worker has generated an idea, he or negatively related.
she has to engage in social activities to find friends, back- Alternatively, as noted in Anderson et al. (2014),
ers, and sponsors surrounding an idea, or to build a coali- CIP can disrupt organizational norms and the effect of
tion of supporters who provide the necessary power CIP on organizational members might not be uniformly
behind it” (p. 288). In fact, Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) positive. For example, CIP can have the effect of influ-
found empirical support for the effect of social capital on encing established organizational hierarchies and can be
innovation in organizations. Also along these lines, viewed by some organizational members as an interfer-
Grodal, Nelson, and Siino (2015) found that innovative ence with their established work roles (Adolfsson,
work by individuals is predicated on help seeking and help Smide, Gregeby, Fernstrom, & Wikblad, 2004). Thus,
giving. In modern complex organizations, the information others can perceive employee CIP as disruptive and
for successful performance is distributed across indivi- problematic and as having the effect of harming others
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

duals and co-operation is required for initiating new pro- within the organization. This could result in a positive
cedures. We therefore expect a positive relationship CIP–CWB relationship. CIP can also influence CWB
between ratings of OCB and CIP. when creative and innovative behaviours have the effect
of bypassing procedures that ensure safety by introdu-
cing what are termed in the literature as workaround
Counterproductive work behaviours behaviours (Burke, Sarpy, Tesluk, & Smith-Crowe,
Research has also recognized that employees can engage 2002; Reason, Parker, & Lawton, 1998). Thus, the
in behaviours on the job that detract from effective func- direction of the CIP–CWB relationship is unclear.
tioning of the organization, referred to as CWBs. CWB
encompasses a wide range of behaviours that have been
examined in the literature for some time, including theft, Moderators
aggression, and withdrawal (Viswesvaran & Ones, 2000). The primary focus of the present study is to differentiate
Note that CWB is not merely the opposite end of the same ratings of CIP from ratings of task performance, OCB, and
continuum as OCB (Dalal, 2005). Indeed, the low end of CWB by estimating population correlations between these
OCB reflects the absence of discretionary behaviours that variables using psychometric meta-analysis. As reviewed
contribute to the effectiveness of the organization, rather earlier, we will assess measurement characteristics of the
than the presence of behaviours that run counter to the performance dimensions as moderators. Specifically, we
organization’s goals. will compute population correlations separately by mea-
CWB is multidimensional (cf. Robinson & Bennett, surement of CIP (i.e., all measures, innovative perfor-
1995); however, our ability to examine CIP–CWB rela- mance, creative performance), OCB (i.e., overall, OCB-I,
tionships at the dimension level is limited by the avail- OCB-O), and CWB (i.e., overall, deviance, withdrawal).
ability of different CWB dimensions examined in the Outside of these measurement characteristics, as our focus
relevant literature. Nonetheless, we are able to draw the is on performance ratings, rater level variables are relevant
following distinction between types of CWB: deviance— as well. In particular, our focus is on rating source. Based
behaviours that violate organizational norms in such a on the availability of ratings provided by different sources
manner as to impair the well-being of the organization or in the relevant literature, we examine effects separately for
its members (e.g., counterproductive interpersonal beha- the following groups of raters: overall (i.e., across all
viours, theft, loafing)—and withdrawal (e.g., absenteeism, raters), self, other (i.e., supervisors, subordinates, peers),
tardiness, turnover intentions; Carpenter & Berry, in and supervisors. Insufficient studies were available to
press). examine the effects for subordinate and peer ratings in
Innovation requires generating support from others for isolation.
creative ideas in the workplace (Janssen, 2000). While we do not form any specific hypotheses for the
Particularly important are relationships with one’s super- effect of rating source on the relationships examined here,
visors. As noted in Klein and Knight (2005) “the decision it is an important consideration for research into perfor-
to adopt and implement an innovation is typically made by mance measurement. For example, Viswesvaran et al.
those higher in the hierarchy than the innovation’s targeted (2005) found that the effect of halo error on performance
user” (p. 244). Withdrawal and deviant behaviours should dimension intercorrelations was stronger for peers as
6 M.B. Harari et al.

compared to supervisors (this study did not include self- Inclusion criteria and coding scheme
ratings). Sackett (2002) argued that supervisors, peers, and To be included in the present analysis, the study had to
subordinates might base ratings of CWB to a greater report a zero-order correlation between ratings of CIP and
degree on general impressions of ratees versus observa- task performance, OCB, or CWB. Studies had to be con-
tions of behaviours because, in many cases, employees ducted using field samples of employees. CIP had to be
engage in CWBs in a discrete manner. Thus, other ratings measured as a behaviour rather than as an individual
of CWB should be influenced by halo error to a greater difference (e.g., creative personality), consistent with the
extent than self-ratings and thus, the CIP–CWB correla- definition offered earlier in the paper. Since our aim was to
tion could be larger for other raters than for self-raters. distinguish between ratings of CIP and the three perfor-
The same has been argued for OCBs (Schnake, 1991). mance dimensions, only studies where the performance
Indeed, Dalal (2005) found that the OCB–CWB relation- dimensions were rated by the same rater were included.
ship was significantly stronger when assessed via super- Studies that utilized multisource data (e.g., Janssen &
visory versus self-ratings. Therefore, we explore if the Giebels, 2013) and objective measures of performance
CIP–task performance, CIP–OCB, and CIP–CWB rela- (e.g., Martinaityte & Sacramento, 2013) were excluded.
tionships are moderated by rating source. A total of 39 studies (and 40 independent samples) met
these criteria and were included in the meta-analysis.
Studies were coded for sample size, correlations, relia-
Summary
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

bility (i.e., coefficient alphas), and rating source by the


Investigating how CIP relates to task performance, OCB, first two authors. We also coded whether CIP was mea-
and CWB advances our understanding of CIP as a job sured as creative performance or innovative performance,
performance dimension and illuminates theories of indivi- whether OCB was directed towards individuals (OCB-I)
dual job performance and organizational effectiveness. or the organization (OCB-O), and the type of CWB (i.e.,
The current empirical literature is scattered and seemingly deviance, withdrawal). Finally, we coded the industry in
contradictory and there are competing predictions for which the primary study was conducted for descriptive
these relationships. By cumulating results across studies purposes. Where multiple measures of the same construct
using psychometric meta-analytic methods (Schmidt & were reported, we formed composites using the methods
Hunter, 2014), we can estimate the sample-size-weighted outlined in Schmidt and Hunter (2014). We estimated
correlations between the performance dimensions across composite reliability using the formulas provided in
studies. We examine the role of rating source, measure- Mosier (1943). Agreement between the two authors was
ment of CIP, OCB target (i.e., OCB-I vs. OCB-O), and 95%. Discrepancies were resolved between the authors
type of CWB (i.e., deviance vs. withdrawal) as and therefore, agreement was ultimately 100%.
moderators. Descriptive information from studies included in the
meta-analysis is reported in Table 1.

Method
Literature search Analyses
To identify studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis, we We conducted our analyses using the psychometric meta-
searched PsychInfo, ABI/Inform, and Google Scholar. We analysis procedures outlined in Schmidt and Hunter
searched for articles that included, anywhere in the text, (2014). Consistent with the recommendations in Schmidt
either creativ* or innovat* and any of the following: job and Hunter (2014), we used random effects models. For
performance, task performance, in-role performance, each meta-analytic cumulation (i.e., CIP–task perfor-
organizational citizenship behavior, organizational citi- mance, CIP–OCB, CIP–CWB), we computed the sample
zenship behaviour, contextual performance, contextual size-weighted observed correlation. Corrections for mea-
behavior, contextual behaviour, extrarole behavior, surement error in CIP and other job performance dimen-
extra-role behavior, extra role behavior, extrarole beha- sion scores were made using artefact distributions
viour, extra-role behaviour, extra role behaviour, counter- comprised of coefficient alphas derived from the studies
productive work behavior, counterproductive work included in the meta-analysis (see Table 2). Using the
behaviour, organizational deviance, interpersonal artefact distributions, we calculated sample size-weighted
deviance, work* deviance, sabotage, theft, absenteeism, corrected correlations as well as their associated sample
lateness, tardiness, bullying, turnover, or counterproduc- size-weighted standard deviations. We estimated 80%
tivity. This search returned a total of 877 unique studies. credibility intervals as well as 95% confidence intervals
Based on a review of the abstracts of these 877 studies, we (using the methods outlined in Viswesvaran et al., 2002)
identified 74 as being potentially relevant for the present around each estimate of ρ. In order to conclude that ratings
study. of CIP and the other performance dimensions are
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

Table 1. Descriptive information of studies included in meta-analysis.

Study CIP N Task OCB OCB-I OCB-O CWB Withdrawal Deviance Rating source Industry

Alge, Ballinger, Tangirala, and Oakley (2006) IP 303 0.46 0.45 0.40 Other Variety
Anthony (2012) IP 98 0.63 Supervisor Variety
Aryee, Walumbwa, Zhou, and Hartnell (2012) IP 193 0.50 Supervisor Telecommunications
Binnewies, Sonnentag, and Mojza (2009) IP 358 0.42 0.32 Self Non-profit
Cheung (2011) IP 252 0.41 Supervisor Manufacturing
Chughtai and Buckley (2011) IP 168 0.30 Self Research centre
Eder (2007) CP 269 0.58 0.81 Supervisor Variety
Eschleman, Madsen, Alarcon, and Barelka (2014) IP 341 0.58 0.50 0.55 Self Variety
Eschleman et al. (2014) IP 92 −0.07 −0.21 0.09 Other Military
Fluegge (2008) CP 205 0.57 0.73 Supervisor Variety
Gilson (2000) IP 686 −0.21 Self Variety
Gong et al. (2009) IP 200 0.73 Supervisor Insurance
Hirst, Van Knippenberg, Zhou, Zhu, and Tsai (in press) CP 317 0.56 Supervisor Variety
Hu, Kaplan, Wei, and Vega (2014) IP 140 0.51 0.59 Supervisor Technology
Janssen and Huang (2008) IP 157 0.63 0.59 Supervisor Bank
Janssen and Van Yperen (2004) IP 170 0.17 Supervisor Energy supplier
Kahya (2009) CP 143 0.44 0.49 0.45 0.52 Supervisor Manufacturing
Karatepe, Kilic, and Isiksel (2008) IP 170 0.00 Self Hotel
Lassk and Shepherd (2013) IP 460 0.58 Self Health and beauty
Lee, Tan, and Javalgi (2010) IP 497 0.59 Supervisor Hospital
Liden, Wayne, Liao, and Meuser (2014) IP 952 0.43 Supervisor Restaurant
Lu, Zhou, and Leung (2011) IP 166 0.42 Supervisor Variety
Luksyte (2011) IP 215 −0.04 −0.01 −0.04 Self Community college
Miao, Newman, and Lamb (2012) IP 322 0.49 Supervisor Manufacturing
Moss and Ritossa (2007) IP 263 0.67 Supervisor Government
Ng and Feldman (2009) CP 162 0.44 0.58 −0.25 Peer Variety
Oldham and Cummings (1996) IP 171 0.75 Supervisor Manufacturing
Pace and Brannick (2010) CP 83 0.48 Supervisor Variety
Raja and Johns (2010) IP 383 0.42 0.51 0.42 0.42 Peer Variety
Rank, Nelson, Allen, and Xu (2009) IP 152 0.50 Supervisor Variety
Reaves (2015) IP 299 0.29 0.48 Self Variety
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology

Tse and Chui (2014) IP 250 0.06 0.03 0.08 Supervisor Banking
Wang, Begley, Hui, and Lee (2012) IP 176 0.54 0.47 0.51 Supervisor Electronics
Wang, Chiang, Tsai, Lin, and Cheng (2013) IP 261 0.51 0.57 0.52 0.46 Supervisor Technology
Wang and Ma (2013) IP 210 −0.49 Self
Xerri and Brunetto (2013) IP 210 0.20 0.19 0.12 Self Hospital
Yu & Frankel (2013) IP 206 0.42 Supervisor Bank
Zhang & Bartol (2010b) IP 367 0.33 Supervisor Technology
Zhang, Lepine, Buckman, and Wei (2014) IP 339 0.57 0.70 0.57 0.73 −0.23 Supervisor Variety
Zhen and Aryee (2007) IP 171 0.38 Supervisor Manufacturing
Note: CIP–performance correlations appear below performance dimensions.
CP, creative performance. IP, innovative performance.
7
8 M.B. Harari et al.

Table 2. Reliability distributions for the different performance dimensions.

CIP IP CP Task OCB OCB-I OCB-O CWB Deviance Withdrawal

Overall .90 (.06) .90 (.07) .94 (.03) .86 (.09) .87 (.09) .84 (.06) .83 (.08) .89 (.03) .89 (.06) .88 (.02)
Self .88 (.07) .88 (.07) – .84 (.08) .87 (.09) .86 (.10) .83 (.11) .87 (.02) – –
Other .91 (.06) .90 (.06) .94 (.03) .86 (.09) .87 (.10) .83 (.06) .83 (.08) – – –
Supervisor .92 (.05) .92 (.05) .95 (.04) .86 (.09) .89 (.03) .84 (.06) .85 (.06) – – –

Note: Mean frequency-weighted alphas are reported in cells with standard deviations in parentheses.
CIP, all measures; IP, innovative performance; CP, creative performance.

empirically distinct, it would be necessary that the 95% correlations between CIP and task performance where all
confidence intervals around each estimate of ρ do not CIP measures (i.e., innovative performance and creative
reach 1.00. We also calculated the percentage of variance performance) were included, the middle section reports
in observed correlations accounted for by statistical arte- results of analyses involving innovative performance
facts (%Var). This value is useful for identifying the pre- only, and the bottom section reports results of analyses
sence of moderator variables. Specifically, consistent with involving creative performance only. When reviewing
the 75% rule of thumb, where statistical artefacts these three sections of the table, it is apparent that relation-
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

accounted for less than 75% of the variance in observed ships do not vary substantively between these three sets of
correlations, we concluded that moderators of the relation- analyses. We therefore limit our in-text review of results to
ship are likely present (Schmidt & Hunter, 2014). the top part of the table where both measures of innovative
We repeated our analyses for each level of each mod- performance and creative performance were included.
erator examined. Differences between relationships at each Overall, the CIP–task performance relationship was
level of each moderator were examined by assessing the large in magnitude, positive, and non-zero (ρ = .55).
overlap in their 95% confidence intervals. If the confi- While we did detect substantial variability in population
dence intervals did not overlap, we concluded that the effect sizes underlying this effect (as evident by the width
relationships were significantly different. In determining of the 80% credibility intervals), the value was always
the number of studies needed to be included in each meta- greater than 0, suggesting that CIP and task performance
analytic calculation, we implemented a frequently used are positively related across samples. The 95% confidence
rule of thumb; we required at least three studies be avail- intervals around this estimate of rho ranged from .50 to
able (Viswesvaran et al., 2002). .60, supporting a distinction between ratings of CIP and
task performance (as the confidence intervals do not
extend to 1.00). We did not observe evidence suggesting
Results that rating source moderated the CIP–task performance
Results of the CIP–task performance meta-analysis are relationship, as the estimates of ρ were similar in magni-
reported in Table 3. The table is divided into three sec- tude and not significantly different from one another
tions. The top section reports a meta-analysis of across ratings sources. The CIP–task performance

Table 3. Meta-analysis of CIP–task performance relationships.

K N r ρ σρ %Var CVL CVU CIL CIU

All measures
Overall 28 7660 0.49 0.55 0.12 22.28% 0.40 0.71 0.50 0.60
Self 4 1285 0.43 0.50 0.13 17.74% 0.34 0.67 0.36 0.64
Other 24 6375 0.50 0.56 0.11 24.32% 0.42 0.71 0.51 0.61
Supervisor 22 5830 0.50 0.57 0.12 22.21% 0.42 0.72 0.51 0.63
Innovative performance
Overall 22 6481 0.48 0.55 0.13 19.26% 0.38 0.71 0.49 0.61
Self 4 1285 0.43 0.51 0.13 17.83% 0.34 0.67 0.37 0.65
Other 18 5196 0.49 0.56 0.12 20.31% 0.40 0.71 0.50 0.62
Supervisor 17 4813 0.50 0.56 0.13 18.66% 0.40 0.72 0.49 0.63
Creative performance
Other 6 1179 0.53 0.59 0.00 100.00% 0.59 0.59 0.54 0.64
Supervisor 5 1017 0.54 0.61 0.00 100.00% 0.61 0.61 0.56 0.66

Note: K, number of independent samples included in analysis; N, pooled sample size; r, observed sample size-weighted correlation; ρ, sample size-
weighted corrected correlation; σρ, sample size-weighted standard deviation of corrected correlations; %Var, percentage of variance accounted for in
correlations by statistical artefacts (i.e., sampling error, measurement error); CV, 80% credibility intervals; CI, 95% confidence intervals.
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 9

relationship was ρ = .50 for self-ratings, ρ = .56 for other performance appear in the middle, and those for measures
ratings, and ρ = .57 for supervisor ratings. While there was of creative performance appear on the bottom. The results
a great degree of variability left in these estimates once across sets of analyses are consistent and we therefore
accounting for statistical artefacts (i.e., sampling error, limit our in-text review of results to the overall analyses
measurement error), suggesting the likely presence of reported in the top portion of the table.
moderator variables, results indicated that these values Overall, we estimated a population correlation
are always positive across populations. From these results, between CIP and OCB of ρ = .56. Results indicated that
we concluded that CIP is positively related to task multiple population correlations likely underlie this value
performance. (i.e., that the relationship is influenced by moderators), as
At this point, we do note one unique finding with only 9.26% of the variability in correlations could be
respect to the creative performance–task performance rela- accounted for by statistical artefacts. However, our results
tionship reported in the bottom third of Table 3. When indicated that, across populations, the CIP–OCB correla-
focusing only on creative performance, we found mean tion was positive, as our 80% credibility intervals ranged
correlations of similar magnitude as just discussed, but from .29 to .83. The value of ρ estimated in our analyses
once accounting for the effect of statistical artefacts, our was also non-zero, as the 95% confidence intervals (.46 to
results indicated no variability in effect sizes across stu- .66) excluded zero. Note also that the 95% confidence
dies. That is, our analysis suggested that no moderators intervals excluded 1.00 and our analyses therefore also
influence the creative performance–task performance rela- indicated a distinction between ratings of CIP and OCB.
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

tionship and that creative performance appears to share a Further, the relationship estimated across rating sources
much more consistent relationship with task performance did not vary by the target of the OCB—the population
than does innovative performance. correlation estimated for OCB-I was ρ = .46 and was
Table 4 reports the results of the meta-analysis of CIP– ρ = .49 for OCB-O. The 95% confidence intervals around
OCB relationships. Similar to the analyses involving task these estimates overlapped. The CIP–OCB relationship
performance just discussed, Table 4 is broken into three did not vary by rating source and accounting for rating
sections: analyses of correlations involving all CIP mea- source as a moderator did not appreciably reduce the
sures appear at the top, those for measures of innovative variability in our estimated correlations. For self-ratings,

Table 4. Meta-analysis of CIP–OCB relationships.

K N r ρ σρ %Var CVL CVU CIL CIU

All measures
Overall 19 4352 0.50 0.56 0.21 9.26% 0.29 0.83 0.46 0.66
OCB-I 12 2821 0.40 0.46 0.21 9.42% 0.20 0.72 0.34 0.58
OCB-O 10 2498 0.42 0.49 0.22 8.27% 0.21 0.77 0.35 0.63
Self 4 1208 0.41 0.47 0.15 14.89% 0.29 0.66 0.31 0.63
Other 15 3144 0.53 0.59 0.22 8.92% 0.31 0.87 0.47 0.71
OCB-I 10 2270 0.41 0.47 0.22 8.79% 0.19 0.74 0.33 0.61
OCB-O 8 1947 0.43 0.50 0.21 8.65% 0.23 0.78 0.34 0.66
Supervisor 11 2204 0.56 0.62 0.22 6.00% 0.34 0.90 0.48 0.76
OCB-I 7 1492 0.43 0.49 0.20 9.61% 0.23 0.75 0.33 0.65
OCB-O 5 1169 0.47 0.54 0.25 5.68% 0.22 0.86 0.31 0.77
Innovative performance
Overall 15 3573 0.46 0.52 0.20 10.31% 0.26 0.77 0.41 0.63
OCB-I 11 2678 0.40 0.46 0.21 8.75% 0.19 0.73 0.32 0.60
OCB-O 9 2355 0.42 0.48 0.23 7.68% 0.20 0.77 0.33 0.63
Self 4 1208 0.41 0.48 0.15 15.45% 0.29 0.67 0.32 0.64
Other 11 2365 0.48 0.54 0.22 9.39% 0.26 0.82 0.41 0.67
OCB-I 9 2127 0.40 0.47 0.22 8.05% 0.18 0.75 0.31 0.63
OCB-O 9 2355 0.42 0.48 0.22 7.67% 0.19 0.77 0.33 0.63
Supervisor 8 1587 0.51 0.56 0.22 7.06% 0.28 0.84 0.40 0.72
OCB-I 6 1349 0.43 0.49 0.21 8.32% 0.22 0.77 0.31 0.67
OCB-O 4 1026 0.47 0.53 0.27 4.69% 0.19 0.87 0.26 0.80
Creative performance
Other 4 779 0.68 0.75 0.12 21.08% 0.60 0.91 0.62 0.88
Supervisor 3 617 0.71 0.79 0.13 12.15% 0.63 0.96 0.63 0.95
Note: K, number of independent samples included in analysis; N, pooled sample size; r, observed sample size-weighted correlation; ρ, sample size-
weighted corrected correlation; σρ, sample size-weighted standard deviation of corrected correlations; %Var, percentage of variance accounted for in
correlations by statistical artefacts (i.e., sampling error, measurement error); CV, 80% credibility intervals; CI, 95% confidence intervals.
10 M.B. Harari et al.

we estimated a population correlation of ρ = .47. It was measures only. Our findings were parallel across analyses,
not possible to assess OCB target as a moderator. For and therefore we review findings from our analyses that
other ratings, we estimated a population correlation of included measures of both innovative performance and
ρ = .59. The value was similar for OCB-I (ρ = .47) and creative performance. Overall, we estimated a population
OCB-O (ρ = .50). For supervisory ratings, the estimated correlation of ρ = −.23. The 80% credibility intervals
population correlation was ρ = .62 and this value also did (−.40 to −.07) excluded zero and thus, we concluded that
not differ by OCB target (ρ = .49 and .54 for OCB-I vs. CIP and CWB are negatively related across populations.
OCB-O, respectively). All of the 95% confidence intervals Further, the 95% confidence intervals around the estimate
around all of the estimates reviewed here overlapped. of ρ ranged from −.34 to −.12. Thus, we can conclude that
Therefore, we did not observe evidence suggesting that the estimated CIP–CWB relationship is non-zero (as the
the CIP–OCB relationship varied by rating source or by confidence intervals did not include zero) and that raters
the target of the OCB. In each of these analyses, there was do distinguish between CIP and CWB (as the confidence
a great degree of heterogeneity in population correlations intervals did not include −1.00). This relationship did not
suggesting the presence of additional moderators. vary significantly between types of CWB, as the relation-
Nonetheless, inspection of the 80% credibility intervals ship for withdrawal and deviance was ρ = −.22 and
indicated that all of these estimates were greater than ρ = −.20, respectively. Two studies included in these
zero across populations. Thus, while the magnitude of analyses used raters classified as “other,” while the
the effect does likely differ as a result of unaccounted remaining four used self-ratings. When analysing results
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

for moderators, we concluded that CIP and OCB are for self-ratings only, findings were consistent with the
positively related. overall analyses (i.e., ρ = −.23).
In terms of our analyses involving only measures of
creative performance (reported in the bottom section of
Table 4), we did estimate population creative perfor-
mance–OCB correlations that were larger in magnitude Discussion
than those observed in our earlier analyses. These analyses As the nature of work continues to change, so will con-
could only be carried out for other raters and among those ceptualizations of job performance. With research increas-
other rater studies, four included supervisors. The popula- ingly recognizing the importance of creativity and
tion correlation estimated for other raters was ρ = .75 and innovation for the success of modern organizations, so
when including supervisor raters only, the correlation was too has creative and innovative performance been recog-
ρ = .79. Based on an inspection of the 95% confidence nized as a critical dimension of individual job performance
intervals, these values were not significantly different from (e.g., Janssen & Van Yperen, 2004; Oldham & Cummings,
the lower point estimates derived from studies including 1996). Despite the research accumulated thus far into the
innovative performance measures. relationship between CIP and established dimensions of
Finally, results of the meta-analysis of CIP–CWB cor- individual job performance, firm conclusions concerning
relations are reported in Table 5. We calculated this rela- the magnitude and direction of these relationships could
tionship using (a) all measures (i.e., innovative not be reached due to mixed findings in the literature.
performance and creative performance; reported in the Responding to calls in the literature for the use of meta-
top half of Table 5) and (b) innovative performance only analysis to clarify the state of the creativity and innovation
(reported in the bottom half of Table 4). It was not possi- literature (e.g., Anderson et al., 2004; Anderson & King,
ble to conduct analyses involving creative performance 1993), we used meta-analysis to synthesize CIP–task

Table 5. Meta-analysis of CIP–CWB relationships.

K N r ρ σρ %Var CVL CVU CIL CIU

All measures
Overall 6 1782 −0.21 −0.23 0.13 18.42% −0.40 −0.07 −0.34 −0.12
Withdrawal 4 1281 −0.19 −0.22 0.17 11.73% −0.43 −0.01 −0.40 −0.04
Deviance 3 716 −0.18 −0.20 0.07 49.08% −0.29 −0.11 −0.31 −0.09
Self 4 1281 −0.20 −0.23 0.16 12.57% −0.44 −0.02 −0.40 −0.06
Innovative performance
Overall 5 1620 −0.21 −0.23 0.14 15.58% −0.41 −0.05 −0.36 −0.10
Self 4 1281 −0.20 −0.23 0.16 12.58% −0.44 −0.02 −0.40 −0.06
Note: K, number of independent samples included in analysis; N, pooled sample size; r, observed sample size-weighted correlation; ρ, sample size-
weighted corrected correlation; σρ, sample size-weighted standard deviation of corrected correlations; %Var, percentage of variance accounted for in
correlations by statistical artefacts (i.e., sampling error, measurement error); CV, 80% credibility intervals; CI, 95% confidence intervals.
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 11

performance, CIP–OCB, and CIP–CWB relationships that these dimensions are redundant. Indeed, the correla-
across 39 studies that spanned nearly two decades. tions involving CIP are all smaller than the task perfor-
Results indicated that CIP was related to the other mance–OCB correlation. The pattern of correlations
performance dimensions. However, while these perfor- reported here is consistent with our initial conclusions
mance dimensions were meaningfully related to one that an empirical distinction between CIP and these other
another, the overlap was not so great as to suggest that performance dimensions is evident.
CIP was redundant with any of these constructs. Indeed, in Our findings have implications for our understanding
no instance did the 95% confidence intervals around each of creative and innovative behaviours as dimensions of job
estimate of ρ overlap with 1.00. Our analyses supported performance. First, with the emerging research highlight-
the conceptual distinction made between CIP and all three ing the importance of creativity and innovation as deter-
performance dimensions (i.e., task performance, OCB, minants of organizational performance (Gong et al.,
CWB) by all raters (self, others) included in our analyses. 2013), understanding the determinants of CIP is a critical
In order to ground our findings more clearly within the pursuit in the literature. The moderate overlap between
existing literature into the intercorrelations among perfor- CIP and both task performance and OCB suggests that
mance dimensions, we constructed a true score correlation there are likely many similar determinants of performance
matrix between the four performance dimensions exam- on each of these dimensions, as suggested by interactionist
ined in the present study, reported in Table 6. The correla- model (Woodman et al., 1993). For example, research
tions involving CIP were derived from the present study. suggests that cognitive ability, an established predictor of
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

The correlation between OCB and task performance was task performance, is also correlated with creativity in
reported in Hoffman et al. (2007). The task performance– organizations (Kuncel et al., 2004). However, there also
CWB and OCB–CWB correlations were reported in exists substantial variability in CIP that is not shared with
Carpenter and Berry (in press). The Carpenter and Berry task performance, OCB, and CWB. While CIP likely
meta-analysis is useful for our purposes because, much shares predictors with other dimensions, it is also likely
like our meta-analysis, it included the CWB sub-dimen- that unique predictors exist that predict CIP but not task
sions of deviance and withdrawal. The true score task performance, OCB, or CWB. As an example, Pace and
performance–deviance and task performance–withdrawal Brannick (2010) found that a contextualized openness to
correlations were ρ = .09 and .02, respectively (averaged experience scale predicted creative performance but not
to ρ = .06) and the true score OCB–deviance and OCB– task performance. More research is needed into the deter-
withdrawal correlations were ρ = −.27 and −.17, respec- minants of CIP, including dispositional, attitudinal, demo-
tively (averaged to ρ = −.22). graphic, and organizational variables.
Among the correlations included in Table 6, the largest The results of our study have implications for future
relationship is between task performance and OCB meta-analyses involving predictors and outcomes of CIP.
(ρ = .74). The relationships observed in the present study In order to test comprehensive measurement and path
between CIP and both task performance and OCB were models involving individual job performance, the inter-
much smaller; ρ = .55 and .56, respectively. The relation- correlations between different performance dimensions are
ship between CIP and CWB is virtually identical to the needed (Viswesvaran & Ones, 1995). The same approach
relationship between OCB and CWB (ρ = −.23 vs. −.22), is used to test outcomes of performance dimensions. For
while the relationship between task performance and example, Podsakoff, Whiting, Podsakoff, and Blume
CWB is much smaller in magnitude (ρ = .06). Overall, (2009) used meta-analysis to test the incremental validity
as would be expected, the performance dimensions are of OCBs for predicting individual- and organizational-
intercorrelated (Viswesvaran et al., 2005). However, the level outcomes beyond task performance—an analysis
correlations involving CIP are not so large as to suggest that required a true score point estimate of the task per-
formance–OCB relationship. As research into the out-
comes of CIP continues to accumulate, our estimates
would facilitate a similar test of incremental prediction
Table 6. True score correlation matrix between CIP, task per-
formance, OCB, and CWB. of individual- and organizational-level outcomes by CIP
beyond what can be accounted for by task performance,
1 2 3 OCB, and CWB.
While creativity and innovation in organizations are
1 CIP
2 Task performance .55a associated with favourable outcomes, the predominant
3 OCB .56a .74b focus in the literature on productive outcomes has been
4 CWB −.23a .06c −.22c referred to as a pro-innovation bias (Kimberly, 1981),
based on the notion that research has emphasized how
Note:aValue derived from current study.
b
Value derived from Hoffman et al. (2007). innovation contributes towards, while ignoring how it
c
Value derived from Carpenter and Berry (in press). might detract from, organizational and individual
12 M.B. Harari et al.

effectiveness. The lack of research into counterproductive companies (k = 3), where one might hypothesize that the
aspects of creativity and innovation has led to an innova- relationship would be stronger in the latter as compared to
tion maximization fallacy (Anderson et al., 2014); the the former. However, this was not the case, as the cor-
belief that creativity and innovation are always a good rected sample size-weighted mean correlations estimated
thing and that more is always better. This is potentially in each subsample did not differ significantly from one
problematic because, as noted in Anderson et al. (2014), another. In manufacturing industries, the relationship was
“creativity and innovation are often experienced as dis- ρ = .55 (95% CI = .43 to .67) and in technology industries,
ruptive events, do not always benefit all parties affected, the relationship was ρ = .49 (95% CI = .37 to .61). Thus,
may be initiated in response to distress-related stimuli, and we present some preliminary evidence that the CIP–task
excessive innovation may be counter-productive to other performance relationship might not vary by industry.
aspects of individual, team, or organizational perfor- However, we note that this is a comparison only between
mance” (p. 1320). Our study has implications for our two industries. We recommend that future research exam-
understanding of the pro-innovation bias. ine this issue across different industries and jobs for the
First, in the job performance literature, it is apparent different performance dimensions.
that research has paid greater attention to productive (e.g., Our interest in the present study was to understand the
task performance, OCB) versus counterproductive (e.g., differentiation between ratings of CIP and task perfor-
CWB) correlates of CIP. Our analysis of the CIP–task mance, OCB, and CWB, requiring a reliance on single
performance relationship included 28 correlations and source data. For example, correlations between CIP and
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

our analysis of the CIP–OCB relationship included 19 these other dimensions where different raters rated each
correlations, while our analysis of the CIP–CWB relation- performance dimensions would not reflect the effect of
ship included only 6 correlations. Thus, consistent with halo error, which was an important factor in our study.
the contention of the pro-innovation bias, the literature Our study demonstrated that raters do differentiate
into the relationships between CIP and job performance between CIP and these other performance dimensions;
dimensions has in fact focused on productive correlates to that there is an empirical distinction. However, it is also
a much greater degree than counterproductive correlates. useful to understand relationships between ratings of per-
However, our findings were contrary to the notion that formance dimensions with halo error factored out
CIP is detrimental in terms of individual effectiveness. (Scullen, Mount, & Goff, 2000). By using multisource
Indeed, we found strong evidence to suggest that CIP is rating data, Viswesvaran et al. (2005) were able to factor
consistently positively related to productive performance the effect of halo error out of performance dimension
dimensions and consistently negatively related to counter- intercorrelations, finding evidence of a general factor of
productive performance dimensions. This is not to say, job performance. At present, it is not clear how much of
however, that CIP (or creativity and innovation in organi- the overlap between CIP and the three performance
zations more broadly defined) is associated with only dimensions examined here was due to halo error versus
productive correlates and outcomes. Indeed, many studies a general factor of job performance. While the focus of
suggest that, under some circumstances, creativity and our study was same source performance ratings, future
innovation are associated with negative outcomes (e.g., research can separate these two competing sources of
Gong et al., 2013). variance by using multisource data.
Another important point is that causal relationships
between CIP and the other performance dimensions are
Limitations and future directions possible, as noted earlier in the paper. While we did
While we discussed literature earlier suggesting that CIP is discuss some potential mechanisms linking CIP to the
a relevant performance dimension across industries and other performance dimensions, testing these causal rela-
jobs, the extent to which CIP–performance dimension tionships was not the focus of our study and more robust
relationships vary across industries and jobs is not clear. research designs are needed to address this issue. Most
For example, it is possible that in creative industries and useful would be studies using both multisource and multi-
jobs, such as technology or research and development, the wave data collections. Also, as our study emphasized the
CIP–task performance relationship would be higher extent to which raters differentiate between different per-
because CIP could be perceived by raters as a core formance dimensions, consistent with other research into
requirement of the job. As can be seen in Table 1, our this issue (e.g., Dalal, 2005), relevant moderators included
database included studies carried out in a wide variety of rating source and sub-dimensions of the various criterion
industries and since very few studies were conducted in measures (e.g., OCB-I vs. OCB-O). When examining
the same or similar industries, it was generally not possi- causal relationships, other moderators might be more rele-
ble to carry out comparative analyses. However, it was vant. Jobs differ in the extent to which creativity or inno-
possible to compare the CIP–task performance relation- vation is conceptualized as a core requirement of the job.
ship between manufacturing (k = 5) and technology Creativity requirements might moderate the relationship
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 13

between CIP and task performance, such that the relation- they cluster closely together or vary widely from one
ship is stronger when creativity requirements are high. another only by chance; a large sample of correlations is
One’s position in the organization’s hierarchy might be needed for a more accurate determination of variability.
relevant as well, as it is those in higher levels of the However, this is not a problem when it comes to inter-
hierarchy who have more latitude for implementing crea- preting the observed and population correlations. Even
tive ideas (Klein & Knight, 2005). The relationship when based on only a small number of studies, sample
between OCB and CIP could be weaker for employees size-weighted correlations provide a more stable estimate
at higher hierarchical levels because, with formal decision- of the correlation than can be obtained from any of the
making authority, they are less likely to need social capital primary studies included in the analysis (Valentine, Pigott,
to gain support for creative ideas before they can be & Rothstein, 2010). For example, the fewest number of
implemented. In short, more robust research designs are correlations (i.e., 3) were used to calculate the supervisor-
needed to test causal relationships between CIP and other rated creative performance–OCB relationship and overall
performance dimensions and moderators of these CIP–deviance relationship. In these cases, while the num-
relationships. ber of independent correlations included was 3, the esti-
In order to differentiate ratings of CIP from those of mated correlations were based on total sample sizes of 617
task performance, OCB, and CWB, we focused on asses- and 716, respectively. Thus, these point estimates are
sing the correlations between ratings of these different based on much larger sample sizes than any primary
performance dimensions. However, other lines of evidence study, are less affected by sampling error than any esti-
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

can be used to address this issue as well. Specifically, mates available in a primary study, and provide the best
ratings of these different performance dimensions could estimate of these relationships available.
also be differentiated by examining their relationships
with external variables. Similar patterns of relationships
would suggest similarities in the performance dimensions Conclusion
while different patterns of relationships would further As the generation and implementation of novel ideas has
support an empirical distinction between the dimensions become central for the success of modern organizations,
(Nunnally, 1978). Once a sufficient body of literature CIP has emerged as a crucial dimension of individual job
emerges examining external correlates of CIP, research performance. However, the literature assessing the rela-
can use meta-analysis to synthesize these findings and tionships between CIP and other performance dimensions
compare relationships involving CIP to those involving was scattered and contradictory. In the present study, we
these other performance dimensions. This research would synthesized this literature using psychometric meta-analy-
be very useful in building on our findings and further sis. Our results suggested that, across samples, CIP was
highlighting the similarities and differences between CIP positively related to task performance and OCB and nega-
and existing performance dimensions. tively related to CWB. Importantly, the relationships
We examined the intercorrelations among the perfor- between CIP and these other job performance dimensions
mance dimensions when they were all assessed at the were not so large as to suggest that there is no empirical
same point in time. Longitudinal research should address distinction between them. Indeed, CIP appears to add to
whether these intercorrelations vary over time. There are our understanding of the structure individual job
two aspects to investigating this dynamicity of relations. performance.
First, we need to examine whether CIP at time 1 is related
to OCB and task performance at time 2 (and how they
vary as the time interval between time 1 and time 2 Disclosure statement
changes). This form of dynamicity can be affected by No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
situational characteristics such as job and organizational
features. Second, an alternate form of dynamicity is
whether the intercorrelations among CIP, task perfor- References
mance, OCB, and CWB all measured at the same time References marked with an asterisk (*) were included in the
vary as a function of the employee’s tenure. Both forms of meta-analysis
Adolfsson, E. T., Smide, B., Gregeby, E., Fernstrom, L., &
dynamicity have theoretical and practical implications for Wikblad, K. (2004). Implementing empowerment group edu-
our field. cation in diabetes. Patient Education and Counseling, 53,
Some of our analyses were based on as few as three 319–324. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2003.07.009
primary studies. This is potentially problematic when it *Alge, B. J., Ballinger, G. A., Tangirala, S., & Oakley, J. L.
comes to interpreting indices of variability in correlations (2006). Information privacy in organizations: Empowering
creative and extrarole performance. Journal of Applied
across samples due to the effect of second-order sampling Psychology, 91, 221–232. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.221
error (Schmidt & Hunter, 2014). When a small number of Anderson, N., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2004). The
correlations are included in analyses, it is possible that routinization of innovation research: A constructively critical
14 M.B. Harari et al.

review of the state-of-the-science. Journal of Organizational counterproductive work behaviors. Journal of Applied
Behavior, 25, 147–173. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-1379 Psychology, 92, 616–627. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.616
Anderson, N., & King, N. (1993). Innovation in organizations. In *Eder, P. J. (2007). Integrating the componential and interac-
C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review tionist models of employee creativity (Doctoral dissertation).
of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 1– Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database
34). Chichester: Wiley. (UMI No. 3277817).
Anderson, N., Potocnik, K., & Zhou, J. (2014). Innovation and Eisenhardt, K. M., & Tabrizi, B. N. (1995). Accelerating adap-
creativity in organizations: A state-of-the-science review, tive processes: Product innovation in the global computer
prospective commentary, and guiding framework. Journal industry. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 84–110.
of Management, 40, 1297–1333. doi:10.1177/ doi:10.2307/2393701
0149206314527128 *Eschleman, K. J., Madsen, J., Alarcon, G., & Barelka, A.
*Anthony, E. L. (2012). What about the leader? An examination (2014). Benefiting from creative activity: The positive rela-
of the antecedents and consequences of leader empowerment tionships between creative activity, recovery experiences,
(Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest and performance-related outcomes. Journal of
Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No. 3543347). Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 87, 579–
*Aryee, S., Walumbwa, F. O., Zhou, Q., & Hartnell, C. A. 598. doi:10.1111/joop.2014.87.issue-3
(2012). Transformational leadership, innovative behavior, Feldman, J. M. (1981). Beyond attribution theory: Cognitive
and task performance: test of mediation and moderation processes in performance appraisal. Journal of Applied
processes. Human Performance, 25, 1–25. doi:10.1080/ Psychology, 66, 127–148. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.66.2.127
08959285.2011.631648 *Fluegge, E. R. (2008). Who put the fun in functional? Fun at
*Binnewies, C., Sonnentag, B., & Mojza, E. J. (2009). Feeling work and its effects on job performance (Doctoral disserta-
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

recovered and thinking about the good side of one’s work. tion). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 243–256. database (UMI No. 3322919).
Bolino, M. C., Turnley, W. H., & Bloodgood, J. M. (2002). Geroski, P., Machin, S., & Van Reenen, J. (1993). The profit-
Citizenship behavior and the creation of social capital in orga- ability of innovating firms. The RAND Journal of
nizations. Academy of Management Review, 27, 505–522. Economics, 24, 198–212. doi:10.2307/2555757
Borman, W. C. (1978). Exploring upper limits of reliability and *Gilson, L. L. (2000). The role of procedural justice in the
validity in job performance ratings. Journal of Applied relationship between demographic diversity, dissimilarity,
Psychology, 63, 135–144. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.63.2.135 work-related affective outcomes, and creative performance
Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1993). Expanding the (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest
criterion domain to include elements of contextual perfor- Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No. 9978382).
mance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel *Gong, Y., Huang, J., & Farh, J. (2009). Employee learning
selection in organizations (pp. 71–98). San Francisco, CA: orientation, transformational leadership, and employee crea-
Jossey Bass. tivity: The mediating role of employee creative self-efficacy.
Burke, M. J., Sarpy, S. A., Tesluk, P. E., & Smith-Crowe, K. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 765–778.
(2002). General safety performance: A test of a grounded doi:10.5465/AMJ.2009.43670890
theoretical model. Personnel Psychology, 55, 429–457. Gong, Y., Zhou, J., & Chang, S. (2013). Core knowledge
doi:10.1111/peps.2002.55.issue-2 employee creativity and firm performance: The moderating
Campbell, J. P., & Wiernik, B. M. (2015). The modeling and assess- role of riskiness orientation, firm size, and realized absorp-
ment of work performance. Annual Review of Organizational tive capacity. Personnel Psychology, 66, 443–482.
Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2(1), 47–74. doi:10.1111/peps.2013.66.issue-2
Carpenter, N. C., & Berry, C. M. (in press). Are counterproduc- Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. W. (2011). The necessity of others is
tive work behavior and withdrawal empirically distinct? A the mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations,
meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Management. perspective taking, and creativity. Academy of Management
*Cheung, S. Y. (2011). Refinement or breakthrough? The link Journal, 54, 73–96. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2011.59215085
between goal orientation, employee learning, creativity, and Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good
job performance Available from ProQuest Dissertations and actors: Prosocial and impression management motives as
Theses database (UMI No. 3468586). interactive predictors of affiliative citizenship behaviors.
*Chughtai, A. A., & Buckley, F. (2011). Work engagement Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 900–912. doi:10.1037/
antecedents, the mediating role of learning goal orientation a0013770
and job performance. Career Development International, 16, Grodal, S., Nelson, A. J., & Siino, R. M. (2015). Help-seeking
684–705. doi:10.1108/13620431111187290 and help-giving as an organizational routine: Continual
Cooper, W. H. (1981). Ubiquitous halo. Psychological Bulletin, engagement in innovative work. Academy of Management
90, 218–244. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.218 Journal, 58, 136–168. doi:10.5465/amj.2012.0552
Dalal, R. S. (2005). A meta-analysis of the relationship between Hoffman, B. J., Blair, C. A., Meriac, J. P., & Woehr, D. J. (2007).
organizational citizenship behavior and counterproductive Expanding the criterion domain? A quantitative review of
work behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1241– the OCB literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(2),
1255. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1241 555–566.
DeNisi, A. S., Cafferty, T., & Meglino, B. (1984). A cognitive view Harari, M. B., Jain, N. K., & Joseph, T. (2014). The five-factor
of the performance appraisal process: A model and research model of personality and knowledge transfer in the United
propositions. Organizational Behavior and Human Arab Emirates. International Journal of Selection and
Performance, 33, 360–396. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(84)90029-1 Assessment, 22, 399–410. doi:10.1111/ijsa.2014.22.issue-4
Dilchert, S., Ones, D. S., Robert, D. D., & Rostow, C. D. (2007). *Hirst, G., van Knippenberg, D., Zhou, Q., Zhu, C. J., & Tsai, P.
Cognitive ability predicts objectively measured C.-F. (in press). Exploitation and exploration climates’
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 15

influence on performance and creativity: Diminishing returns *Lu, L., Zhou, F., & Leung, K. (2011). Effects of task and
as function of self-efficacy. Journal of Management. relationship conflict on individual work behaviors.
*Hu, X., Kaplan, S., Wei, F., & Vega, R. P. (2014). Employees’ International Journal of Conflict Management, 22, 131–
metaperceptions of supervisor ratings on job performance. 150. doi:10.1108/10444061111126675
The Psychologist-Manager Journal, 17, 30–48. doi:10.1037/ *Luksyte, A. (2011). How can organizations maximize their
mgr0000010 overqualified employees potential? Examining organization-
Hulsheger, U. R., Anderson, N., & Salgado, J. F. (2009). Team- ally-based strategies (Doctoral dissertation). Available from
level predictors of innovation at work: A comprehensive ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No.
meta-analysis spanning three decades of research. Journal of 3467997).
Applied Psychology, 94, 1128–1145. doi:10.1037/a0015978 MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Ahearne, M. (1998).
Janssen, O. (2000). Job demands, perceptions of effort-reward Some possible antecedents and consequences of in-role and
fairness and innovative work behaviour. Journal of extra-role salesperson performance. Journal of Marketing,
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 287– 62, 87–98. doi:10.2307/1251745
302. doi:10.1348/096317900167038 Martinaityte, I., & Scramento, C. A. (2013). When creativity
Janssen, O., & Giebels, E. (2013). When and why creativity- enhances sales effectiveness: The moderating role of lea-
related conflict with coworkers can hamper creative employ- der-member exchange. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
ees’ individual job performance. European Journal of Work 34, 974–994.
and Organizational Psychology, 22, 574–587. doi:10.1080/ *Miao, Q., Newman, A., & Lamb, P. (2012). Transformational
1359432X.2012.669524 leadership and the work outcomes of Chinese migrant work-
*Janssen, O., & Huang, X. (2008). Us and me: Team identification ers: The mediating effects of identification with leader.
and individual differentiation as complementary drivers of Leadership, 8, 377–395. doi:10.1177/1742715012444055
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

team members’ citizenship and creative behaviors. Journal Mosier, C. I. (1943). On the reliability of a weighted composite.
of Management, 34, 69–88. doi:10.1177/0149206307309263 Psychometrika, 8, 161–168. doi:10.1007/BF02288700
*Janssen, O., & Van Yperen, N. W. (2004). Employees’ goal *Moss, S. A., & Ritossa, D. A. (2007). The impact of goal
orientations, the quality of leader-member exchange, and the orientation on the association between leadership style and
outcomes of job performance and job satisfaction. Academy of follower performance, creativity, and work attitudes.
Management Journal, 47, 368–384. doi:10.2307/20159587 Leadership, 3, 433–456. doi:10.1177/1742715007082966
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, Motowidlo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. (1997). A
NJ: Prentice-Hall. theory of individual differences in task and contextual per-
*Kahya, E. (2009). The effects of job performance on effective- formance. Human Performance, 10, 71–83. doi:10.1207/
ness. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 39, s15327043hup1002_1
96–104. doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2008.06.006 Motowidlo, S. J., & Van Scotter, J. R. (1994). Evidence that task
*Karatepe, O. M., Kilic, H., & Isiksel, B. (2008). An examina- performance should be distinguished from contextual perfor-
tion of the selected antecedents and outcomes of work- mance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 475–480.
family conflict and family-work conflict in frontline service doi:10.1037/0021-9010.79.4.475
jobs. Services Marketing Quarterly, 29, 1–24. doi:10.1080/ Murphy, K. R. (1989). Dimensions of job performance. In R.
15332960802218620 Dillon & J. Pelligrino (Eds.), Testing: Applied and theoreti-
Kimberly, J. R. (1981). Managerial innovation. In P. C. Nystrom cal perspectives (pp. 218–247). New York, NY: Praeger.
& W. H. Starbuck (Eds.), Handbook of organizational design *Ng, T. W. H., & Feldman, D. C. (2009). Occupational embedd-
(pp. 84–104). Oxford, Engald: Oxford University Press. edness and job performance. Journal of Organizational
Klein, K. J., & Knight, A. P. (2005). Innovation implementation. Behavior, 30, 863–891. doi:10.1002/job.v30:7
Overcoming the challenge. Current Directions in Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York, NY:
Psychological Science, 14, 243–246. doi:10.1111/ McGraw-Hill.
cdir.2005.14.issue-5 *Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (1996). Employee creativity:
Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic Personal and contextual factors at work. Academy of
performance, career potential, creativity, and job perfor- Management Journal, 39, 607–634. doi:10.2307/256657
mance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It’s
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 148–161. construct clean-up time. Human Performance, 10, 85–97.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148 doi:10.1207/s15327043hup1002_2
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. *Pace, V. L., & Brannick, M. T. (2010). Improving prediction of
Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498. doi:10.1037/0033- work performance through frame-of-reference consistency:
2909.108.3.480 Empirical evidence using openness to experience.
*Lassk, F. G., & Shepherd, C. D. (2013). Exploring the relation- International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 18,
ship between emotional intelligence and salesperson creativ- 230–235. doi:10.1111/(ISSN)1468-2389
ity. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 33, Podsakoff, N. P., Whiting, S. W., Podsakoff, P. M., & Blume, B.
25–38. doi:10.2753/PSS0885-3134330103 D. (2009). Individual- and organizational-level consequences
*Lee, O. F., Tan, J. A., & Javalgi, R. (2010). Goal orientation of organizational citizenship behaviors: A meta-analysis.
and organizational commitment: Individual difference pre- Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 122–141. doi:10.1037/
dictors of job performance. International Journal of a0013079
Organizational Analysis, 18, 129–150. doi:10.1108/ Pulakos, E. D., Arad, S., Donovan, M. A., & Plamondon, K. E.
19348831011033249 (2000). Adaptability in the workplace: Development of a
*Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., Liao, C., & Meuser, J. D. (2014). taxonomy of adaptive performance. Journal of Applied
Servant leadership and serving culture: Influence on indivi- Psychology, 85, 612–624. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.4.612
dual and unit performance. Academy of Management *Raja, U., & Johns, G. (2010). The joint effects of personality
Journal, 57, 1434–1452. doi:10.5465/amj.2013.0034 and job scope on in-role performance, citizenship behaviors,
16 M.B. Harari et al.

and creativity. Human Relations, 63, 981–1005. doi:10.1177/ meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral
0018726709349863 Statistics, 35, 215–247. doi:10.3102/1076998609346961
*Rank, J., Nelson, N. E., Allen, T. D., & Xu, X. (2009). Viswesvaran, C. (2002). Absenteeism and measures of job perfor-
Leadership predictors of innovation and task performance: mance: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Selection and
Subordinates’ self-esteem and self-presentation as modera- Assessment, 10, 12–17. doi:10.1111/ijsa.2002.10.issue-1&2
tors. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (1995). Theory testing:
Psychology, 82, 465–489. doi:10.1348/096317908X371547 Combining psychometric meta-analysis and structural equa-
Reason, J., Parker, D., & Lawton, R. (1998). Organizational tions modeling. Personnel Psychology, 48, 865–885.
controls and safety: The varieties of rule-related behaviour. doi:10.1111/peps.1995.48.issue-4
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 71, Viswesvaran, C., & Ones, D. S. (2000). Perspectives on models
289–304. doi:10.1111/joop.1998.71.issue-4 of job performance. International Journal of Selection and
*Reaves, A. C. (2015). Work creativity as a dimension of job Assessment, 8, 216–226. doi:10.1111/ijsa.2000.8.issue-4
performance (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Florida Viswesvaran, C., Ones, D. S., & Schmidt, F. L. (1996).
International University, Miami, FL. Comparative analysis of the reliability of job performance
Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1995). A typology of deviant ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 557–574.
workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.81.5.557
Academy of Management Journal, 38, 555–572. Viswesvaran, C., Ones, D. S., Schmidt, F. L., Le, H., & Oh, I. (2014).
doi:10.2307/256693 Measurement error obfuscates scientific knowledge: Path to
Sackett, P. R. (2002). The structure of counterproductive work cumulative knowledge requires corrections for unreliability and
behaviors: Dimensionality and relationships with facets of psychometric meta-analyses. Industrial and Organizational
job performance. International Journal of Selection and Psychology, 7, 507–518. doi:10.1111/iops.2014.7.issue-4
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

Assessment, 10, 5–11. doi:10.1111/ijsa.2002.10.issue-1&2 Viswesvaran, C., Schmidt, F. L., & Ones, D. S. (2002). The
Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., Moscoso, S., Bertua, C., & Fruyt, F. moderating influence of job performance dimensions on
(2003). International validity generalization of GMA and convergence of supervisory and peer ratings of job perfor-
cognitive abilities: A European community meta-analysis. mance: Unconfounding construct-level convergence and rat-
Personnel Psychology, 56, 573–605. doi:10.1111/ ing difficulty. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 345–354.
peps.2003.56.issue-3 doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.2.345
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of Viswesvaran, C., Schmidt, F. L., & Ones, D. S. (2005). Is there a
selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and general factor in ratings of job performance? A meta-analytic
theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. framework for disentangling substantive and error influ-
Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274. doi:10.1037/0033- ences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 108–131.
2909.124.2.262 doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.108
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (2014). Methods of meta-analy- Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY: Wiley.
sis: Correcting error and bias in research findings (3rd ed.). Walberg, H. J., & Stariha, W. E. (1992). Productive human capital:
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Learning, creativity and eminence. Creativity Research Journal,
Schnake, M. (1991). Organizational citizenship: A review, pro- 5, 323–340. doi:10.1080/10400419209534445
posed model, and research agenda. Human Relations, 44, *Wang, A.-C., Chiang, J. T.-J., Tsai, C.-Y., Lin, -T.-T., & Cheng,
735–759. doi:10.1177/001872679104400706 B.-S. (2013). Gender makes the difference: The moderating
Scott, R. K. (1995). Creative employees: A challenge to man- role of leader gender on the relationship between leadership
agers. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 29, 64–71. styles and subordinate performance. Organizational
doi:10.1002/jocb.1995.29.issue-1 Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 122, 101–113.
Scott, S. G., & Bruce, R. A. (1994). Determinants of innovative doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.06.001
behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the *Wang, G., & Ma, X. (2013). The effect of psychological climate
workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 580–607. for innovation on salespeople’s creativity and turnover inten-
doi:10.2307/256701 tion. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 33,
Scullen, S. E., Mount, M. K., & Goff, M. (2000). Understanding 373–388. doi:10.2753/PSS0885-3134330402
the latent structure of job performance ratings. Journal of *Wang, H., Begley, T., Hui, C., & Lee, C. (2012). Are the effects
Applied Psychology, 85, 956–970. doi:10.1037/0021- of conscientiousness on contextual and innovative perfor-
9010.85.6.956 mance context specific? Organizational culture as a modera-
Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its tor. The International Journal of Human Resource
potential antecedents and relationship to creative perfor- Management, 23, 174–189. doi:10.1080/
mance. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1137–1148. 09585192.2011.561246
doi:10.2307/3069429 Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993). Toward
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). A constant error in psychological rat- a theory of organizational creativity. Academy of
ings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 496–502. Management Review, 18, 293–321.
Tsai, W., & Ghoshal, S. (1998). Social capital and value creation: *Xerri, M. J., & Brunetto, Y. (2013). Fostering innovative beha-
The role of intrafirm networks. Academy of Management viour: The importance of employee commitment and orga-
Journal, 41, 464–476. doi:10.2307/257085 nisational citizenship behaviour. The International Journal
*Tse, H. H. M., & Chui, W. C. K. (2014). Transformational of Human Resource Management, 24, 3163–3177.
leadership and job performance: A social identity perspec- doi:10.1080/09585192.2013.775033
tive. Journal of Business Research, 67, 2827–2835. *Yu, C., & Frenkel, S. J. (2013). Explaining task performance and
doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2012.07.018 creativity from perceived organizational support theory: Which
Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). How mechanisms are more important? Journal of Organizational
many studies do you need?: A primer on statistical power for Behavior, 34, 1165–1181. doi:10.1002/job.1844
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 17

Yuan, F., & Woodman, R. W. (2010). Innovative behavior in the explaining work stressor-job performance relationships.
workplace: The role of performance and image outcome Academy of Management Journal, 57, 675–697.
expectations. Academy of Management Journal, 53, 323– doi:10.5465/amj.2011.1110
342. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2010.49388995 *Zhen, X. C., & Aryee, S. (2007). Delegation of work outcomes:
Zang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010a). Linking empowering leader- An examination of the cultural context of mediating pro-
ship and employee creativity: The influence of psychological cesses in China. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 226–
empowerment, intrinsic motivation, and creative process 238. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.24162389
engagement. Academy of Management Journal, 53, 107– Zhou, J. (1998). Feedback valence, feedback style, task auton-
128. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2010.48037118 omy, and achievement orientation: Interactive effects on
*Zang, X., & Bartol, K. M. (2010b). The influence of creative creative performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83,
process engagement on employee creative performance and 261–276. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.261
overall job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, Zhou, J. (2008). Promoting creativity through feedback. In J.
826–873. Zhou & C. E. Shalley (Eds.), Handbook of organizational
*Zhang, Y., Lepine, J. A., Buckman, B. R., & Wei, F. (2014). It’s creativity (pp. 125–145). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum
Not Fair . . . Or Is It? The role of justice and leadership in Associates.
Downloaded by [Gazi University] at 12:16 26 January 2016

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen