Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Changes in Cooperation and Self-Other Differentiation during the Second Year

Author(s): Celia A. Brownell and Michael Sean Carriger


Reviewed work(s):
Source: Child Development, Vol. 61, No. 4 (Aug., 1990), pp. 1164-1174
Published by: Blackwell Publishing on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130884 .
Accessed: 12/12/2011 11:50

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Blackwell Publishing and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org
Changes in Cooperation and Self-Other
Differentiation during the Second Year

Celia A. Brownell and Michael Sean Carriger


University of Pittsburgh

BROWNELL,CELIAA., and CARRIGER, MICHAELSEAN. Changes in Cooperation and Self-Other


Differentiation during the Second Year.CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1990,61, 1164-1174. Cooperationin
peer interactionemerges duringthe second half of the second year. A considerationof the skills and
knowledge entailed in these early forms of cooperationsuggests that young children's emerging
ability to differentiate self from other as causal agents may relate to their ability to coordinate
behavior with age mates toward a common goal. Children at 12, 18, 24, and 30 months were
observed in same-age,same-sexdyads (8 dyads per age) while attemptingto solve a simple coopera-
tion problem. They were also individually administeredan elicited imitationtask used to index
decentration,or self-otherdifferentiation.No 12-month-olddyadcould cooperate,18-month-oldsdid
so infrequentlyand apparentlyaccidentally,whereas 24- and 30-month-oldswere able to coordinate
behavior with one anotherquickly and effectively. Childrenwho were betterable to accommodate
their behavior to one another during cooperationalso representedthe agency of others at a more
advanced, decentered level.

Children begin to engage in prosocial be- (Kagan, 1981), awareness of external stan-
havior in the second year of life, including dards set by others and attempts to achieve
cooperation, sharing, helping, and responding them (Bullock & Lutkenhaus, 1988; Kagan,
empathically to emotional distress in others. 1981; Kopp, 1982), and several aspects of
These developments become especially rapid early prosocial behavior (Radke-Yarrow et al.,
and differentiated between 18 and 24 months 1983; Zahn-Waxler et al., 1982). Building on
(Radke-Yarrow, Zahn-Waxler, & Chapman, these arguments, the present study examines
1983; Zahn-Waxler, lannotti, & Chapman, the relation between developments in self-
1982). Why should such behaviors emerge so other differentiation and cooperation with
systematically during the second year? One peers during the second year.
possibility is that the motivational base for re- In 1979, Hay noted that cooperation had
sponding to others begins to mature during not been systematically studied in children
this period. A second, and perhaps related,
under 3 years, and she set about remedying
possibility is that certain cognitive or social- the situation with an exploration of children's
cognitive abilities universally develop during
this age period that are especially germane to cooperation with their parents. She found that
decentered interactions with others. only one of eight 12-month-olds engaged in a
cooperative game with a parent, whereas
Several investigators have independently seven of eight 18- and 24-month-olds did so.
suggested that understanding and differen- Furthermore, cooperative games increased in
tiating self versus others is a salient devel- frequency between 18 and 24 months. Similar
opmental task during the second year, and findings were reported 2 years earlier by Ross
that this fundamental cognitive change facili- and Goldman (1977).
tates numerous other social developmental In the ensuing decade relatively little re-
changes. These include acquisition of a vari- search has been conducted on early forms of
ety of early peer interaction skills (Brownell, cooperation. A handful of investigators, how-
1986), sustained conversations with adults ever, have provided some additional insights

Portionsof these data were presented at the InternationalConferenceon InfantStudies, 1988,


and at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1989. Appreciationis
extended to Earnestine Brown who helped to design and test the stimuli and procedure. This
researchwas facilitatedby an Awardfor Young Scholarsin Social and Affective Development from
the Foundation for Child Development to the first author.Correspondencemay be addressed to
Celia Brownell, Psychology Department,Universityof Pittsburgh,Pittsburgh,PA 15260.
[Child Development,1990, 61, 1164-1174. ? 1990 by the Society for Researchin Child Development,Inc.
All rights reserved.0009-3920/90/6104-0026$01.00]
Brownell and Carriger 1165
into its development. Although 12-month- coordinate two independent sources of be-
olds interact socially with one another, not havior in cooperation, the child must be able
until 18 to 24 months do peer interactions re- to represent specific causal relations between
volve around "shared meanings" with obvi- his or her own actions and the independent
ous themes, roles, and turn-taking. The term actions of the partner (see Schaffer, 1979). It
"roles" in this article refers to interactional or can be hypothesized that as children come
behavioral roles, as used by previous investi- more fully to differentiate self from other,
gators such as Eckerman and Stein (1982), able to represent self's and other's actions as
Mueller and Lucas (1975), and Ross (1982). independently caused, they should also be-
Interactional roles are comprised of actions by come more skilled at adopting behavior that is
each child that relate to the actions of the part- independent of but complementary to a part-
ner. Younger children tend to act in ways ner's behavior (Brownell, 1986).
tangential to, rather than thematically related
to, the partner's behavior (Brenner & Mueller, Although prior work has pointed to de-
1982; Eckerman & Stein, 1982; Howes, 1985). velopmental advances between 18 and 24
Correspondingly, 18-24-month-olds engage months in cooperative abilities, there has
in more games with both peers and adults been no previous effort to examine relations
than do younger toddlers, and their games be- with cognitive or social-cognitive develop-
come longer and more reciprocal over the sec- ment. There is little reason to expect broad
ond half of the second year (Eckerman, Davis, correspondences between general interac-
& Didow, 1989; Eckerman & Stein, 1982; tional skills and cognitive or social-cognitive
Ross, 1982). Twenty-four-month-olds in same- developments, as abundant research has
age dyads also can coordinate their behavior made clear in the last few years. Many schol-
with a peer to achieve a goal collaboratively ars, however, now suggest that it may be fruit-
(Brownell, 1989), but 18-month-olds cannot. ful to pursue more limited relations across
Hence, over the second year toddlers increas- specific domains that are hypothesized to
ingly coordinate their behavior with each share specific skills (e.g., Brownell, 1986;
other to achieve effective social interaction, Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1986; Hartup & Brownell,
and they more frequently engage in coopera- 1988; McCune-Nicolich, 1981). As a first step
tive play. in extending our understanding of young chil-
dren's emerging cooperative abilities, then, it
Existing studies of the skills that contrib- would be appropriate to explore concor-
ute to these early forms of cooperation have
dances between developments in under-
emphasized children's regulation of roles.
Eckerman and Stein (1982) explored toddlers' standing of self-other relations and in chil-
dren's skills at adopting and managing
ability to establish a joint interactive topic behavior cooperatively. That was the purpose
around an object and to assume comple-
of this study.
mentary roles in a game. Brownell (1989)
similarly studied toddlers' emerging ability to Self-other differentiation (decentration)
adopt complementary behavior during same- has been conceptualized as the ability to rep-
age and mixed-age cooperative problem solv- resent self as one sort of object among, but
ing. Ross (1982) observed the content and se- distinct from, all other objects (Piaget, 1954,
quencing of roles in peer games among
toddlers, and Ross and Lollis (1987) studied 1962). The child comes to separate the world
toddlers' ability to recognize and respond to from his or her actions on it, and can repre-
role violations in games with parents. In each sent self and self's actions in relation to ob-
case, evidence was provided for develop- jects and to others. Age-related transitions in
mental advances during the second year in this domain occur at regular intervals over the
children's ability to manage interactional second year (see Fein, 1981). At first (12-15
roles. This emphasis is probably no accident. months) the child does not represent self and
The joint coordination of related behavior is other as different causal agents, instead repre-
at the heart of cooperation, whether in play or senting self as the universal agent. The child
in joint problem solving. Children must be is unaware of other's causal agency. During
the second phase (15-20 months), the child
able to represent, monitor, and regulate both
their own and the partner's behavior relative differentiates the other as a passive recipient
to their relation to a single, common goal. of self's actions, but still conceives of self as
the universal agent. Finally, the child con-
These abilities may rely on the develop- ceives of others as active, autonomous agents
ment of several more fundamental sorts of in relation to their own actions, independ-
understanding, such as causal relations and ent of the child's actions or wishes (20-24
self-other relations. That is, to understand and months).
1166 Child Development
The typical way to operationalize and tablished that there were no differences in
study the young child's conception and repre- children's performance as a function of the
sentation of self and others has been via pre- experimenter's sex. Each session was video-
tend play with animate replicas (dolls, stuffed taped with equipment located in a separate
animals). This is presumed to be an accurate room. Visual access was achieved through a
window onto the child's representation of the one-way mirror and audio recording from a
agency of actual animates (see Fenson & microphone in the playroom.
Ramsay, 1980, 1981; Nicolich, 1977; Watson
& Fischer, 1977, 1980; Wolf, 1982). In the After a warm-up free-play period with
the experimenter, the children were pre-
present study, decentration was assessed us-
sented with two tasks in succession. One task
ing an elicited pretense procedure. Actions
with animate replicas were modeled at pro- involved elicited imitation of a series of ex-
gressively higher levels of agency, and the perimenter-modeled pretense scenarios. This
child was asked to imitate these actions. As in task provided the measure of self-other differ-
research on language development, which entiation (decentration). It was administered
employs elicited imitation, the assumption is individually to each child. The second task
that the child will imitate at his or her current was a problem to be solved jointly by the two
level of competence, even when the level toddlers. This provided the measures of coop-
modeled is more advanced than the child's eration. The two tasks were counterbalanced
current one. Previous research using this pro- for order.
cedure to index self-other differentiation has
During the decentration task, children
shown that modeling elicits the child's high- were seated on the floor with a parent. Each
est level of competence, and that this corre- child was presented with five different pre-
lates with the highest step shown in spon- tend scenarios, one at a time. Each scenario
taneous free play (Bretherton, O'Connell, consisted of three distinct actions (e.g., stir-
Shore, & Bates, 1984; Fenson & Ramsay, ring, pouring, and eating). A scenario was
1981; Watson & Fischer, 1980). modeled twice in succession along with a ver-
Children's ability to manage roles in co- bal description of the actions modeled, after
which the experimenter said, "Now, it's your
operation was indexed by several aspects of
their performance during joint problem solv- turn." If the child did not attempt to imitate
within a few seconds, the experimenter said,
ing of a simple task. Coordinating com-
"Can you show me what I did? Can you do
plementary behavior was necessary for suc-
cess. Thus, as in playful social interaction, the just what I did?" If the child still did not at-
children had to manage their roles by timing tempt to imitate, the experimenter asked the
and sequencing their behavior in relation to parent to encourage the child to reproduce
the partner's behavior. the "story."
Five levels of agency were presented by
Method the experimenter, one per scenario. Level 1
Subjects.- Sixty-four children, 16 at each represented self as agent (e.g., pretend to feed
of four ages, participated: 12-15 months (M = self with a spoon). Level 2 represented an ani-
13.8 months), 18-21 months (M = 19.7 mate replica as a passive recipient of self's
months), 24-27 months (M = 24.8 months), acts (e.g., pretend to feed a doll with a spoon).
and 30-33 months (M = 30.8 months). There Level 3 represented an animate replica as an
were equal numbers of boys and girls at 12 active agent (e.g., put the spoon in the hand of
months; at each of the other ages there were the doll and pretend to make it feed itself).
10 boys and six girls. Subjects were recruited Level 4 represented two animate replicas in-
through birth lists made available by a local teracting (e.g., put the spoon in the hand of
obstetrics hospital that serves an urban popu- the doll and pretend to make it feed another
lation. Families were of middle to upper- doll). Level 5 represented an animate replica
middle socioeconomic status by parents' re- as an active agent along with the use of inter-
port. nal state words (e.g., put the spoon in the
hand of the doll and pretend to make it feed
Procedure.-Upon being recruited, each itself, along with making the doll say "I'm so
child was unsystematically paired with an un- hungry"). Although there is substantial em-
familiar, same-age, same-sex peer. Children pirical evidence for the first three levels,
came to the laboratory playroom in pairs, ac- there is no research concerning the ordering
companied by parents, who remained with of the last two. This ordering was suggested
the children at all times. A male experimenter by age trends reported in Bretherton et al.
conducted all sessions. Pilot testing had es- (1984), Watson and Fischer (1980), and Wolf
Brownell and Carriger 1167
(1982). As it turned out, few children imitated tion to the animals in the cup, and said, "Do
at Level 4 or Level 5 anyway; hence the ques- you see those animals in there? You can play
tion of whether the sequence indeed occurs with them if you get them out," as he demon-
in this order is moot for the purposes of this strated the operation of the handle. As long as
study. at least one child remained on task, their ac-
There were five scenario themes (eating, tivity was not interrupted. If both children
bedtime, going to the doctor, fixing a "boo- wandered off task for 15 sec, the experimenter
boo," and grooming). These were randomly redirected their attention to the animals and
assigned to level of agency for each subject, repeated the instructions. If the children re-
so that no particular theme was systematically trieved the animals, they were permitted to
associated with any level of agency. Order of play with them briefly, whereupon the cup
presentation was randomized across subjects. was reloaded, and the children allowed to
The scenarios and the elaboration of levels solve the problem again.
of agency were adapted from Watson and Measures.-For each pretend scenario
Fischer (1977, 1980), Fenson and Ramsay
presented, the number of different actions the
(1981), and Bretherton et al. (1984). child imitated was recorded (maximum of
For the cooperation task, each dyad en- three per scenario), as was the level of decen-
countered one of three cooperation problems tration represented in each of the child's ac-
for 7 min, counterbalanced across dyads. tions. The child did not have to preserve the
Each problem required the use of a simple sequence of the actions to be credited with an
tool, a handle that one child could push to imitation. One measure of decentration, then,
make available to the second child two toy was the highest level of agency represented
animals held in a small cup attached to the in the child's imitations. For purposes of sta-
end of the handle. The problems each op- tistical analysis, a weighted composite score
erated on the same general principle. The was also created by multiplying the number
spring-loaded handle, with the attached cup of actions imitated at each level by the value
containing the animals, was rotated horizon- of that level (1 to 5) and summing the prod-
tally beneath the surface of a transparent table ucts. For example, three actions imitated at
top in one problem ("rotator"), or pushed Level 1 and two actions at Level 2 would re-
horizontally through a transparent cylinder in ceive a composite score of 7, while two ac-
the second problem ("piston"), or pushed tions imitated at Level 1 and three at Level 2
down vertically against a fulcrum inside a would receive a score of 8. Children who im-
transparent box in the third problem itated more often at more advanced levels of
("lever"). The animals became accessible decentration thus received higher composite
through a hole in the end of the cylinder, or in scores.
the top of the table or the box. Because the
For each solution attempt on the coopera-
handle was spring-loaded, the child operating tion problem, several goal-directed behaviors
it had to hold it in its fully extended position
were coded for their frequency of occurrence.
to permit access to the animals. No strength A solution was counted when a child touched
was required to push or to hold the handle. If
the animals in the cup, and latency to solu-
the child released the handle the animals
tion was recorded. Other behaviors were cho-
once again became unavailable. The child sen to capture the children's attempts to coor-
who operated the handle was prevented from dinate their behavior, regardless of whether
reaching the animals by a clear Plexiglas bar- solution was actually achieved or not. Coordi-
rier. If the second child stood next to the first nation of roles spatially was indexed by the
child, access to the animals was blocked for
that child also. Thus the second child had to changes in position children made relative to
one another (move to same side as partner;
position herself opposite the first to retrieve move to side opposite partner).Moving to the
the animals as they were made available by side opposite the partner reflects coordination
the first child. The problems could not be of behavior with the partner, whereas moving
solved by an individual child working alone. to the same or adjacent side reflects absence
The children were therefore required to coor- of coordination. Amount of time opposite the
dinate complementary roles both spatially, by partner was also recorded. Temporal coor-
positioning themselves opposite one another, dination was indexed by whether a child
and temporally by sequencing their behavior
paused when pushing the handle and making
so that as one child operated the handle the the animals accessible to permit the other
other child retrieved the animals.
child time to retrieve them (manipulate-
The experimenter escorted the children pause). Lack of coordination was evidenced
to one side of the apparatus, drew their atten- when the child simply manipulated the han-
1168 Child Development
TABLE 1
BEHAVIORS DURING COOPERATIVEPROBLEM SOLVINGAS A FUNCTION OF AGE

AGE

12 18 24 30
MEASURE Months Months Months Months

Number of solutions ......................... 0 .5 4.8 4.5


(0) (.5) (3.7) (5.6)
Latency to Solution 1 ...................... 7.0 4.7 3.3 4.5
(0) (2.9) (2.0) (2.5)
Latency to Solution 2a . ...................... ..... 1.1 .9
(.82) (1.3)
Latency to Solution 3 ....................... ...... .4 .4
(.49) (.5)
Frequency move opposite partner ............ 1.0 3.4 3.7 4.1
(1.1) (2.6) (2.4) (2.5)
Frequency move adjacent partner ............ .8 2.8 3.1 4.2
(.8) (2.6) (1.6) (2.8)
Proportiontime opposite .................... .54 .50 .62 .57
(.33) (.22) (.17) (.28)
Proportion coordinated behavior .............. .03 .21 .38 .33
(.07) (.17) (.17) (.23)
NOTE.-Values represent mean frequency or proportion per dyad. Standard deviations are in parentheses.
a 12- and 18-month-olds never achieved more than one solution.

die without consideration of the partner (ma- Results


nipulate handle without pause). A child Analyses were directed to three ques-
could also coordinate behavior with the part-
tions: (1) age differences in children's ability
ner by anticipating the lure (animals) while to coordinate behavior with one another dur-
the partner pushed the handle. Finally, a
child could attempt to alter the other child's ing cooperative problem solving, (2) age dif-
ferences in children's ability to differentiate
goal-directed activity by directing requests/ self and other during pretense, (3) relations
directions/commands for task-related behav-
between these two abilities. Preliminary anal-
ior to the partner, and the partner could coor-
dinate behavior with the peer's by complying yses revealed no sex differences on any mea-
with the request, or could fail to coordinate sure, or differences as a function of prior play
behavior by ignoring, resisting, or refusing to experience with other children. Nor were
there differences as a function of task order or
comply. A similar failure to coordinate behav-
ior was evidenced when one child attempted cooperation problem. Subsequent analyses
were thus performed on data collapsed over
to physically displace the other child during
these conditions. All post hoc analyses were
goal-directed activity. Simple exploration of conducted using the Scheff6 test, and all re-
the problem (independent of other goal-
directed activity) was also recorded. A com- ported differences were significant at p < .05.
posite variable of proportion coordinated be- Cooperation.-No 12-month-old dyad
havior was then created by summing the was able to solve the problem cooperatively.
frequencies of manipulate-pause, anticipate Half of the 18-month-old dyads (4 out of 8) did
lure, direct other's behavior, and comply with so once, and most of the 24- and 30-month-old
partner's direction and dividing this sum by dyads solved it multiple times (6 out of 8, and
the sum of all on-task behavior. Movements to 5 out of 8, respectively). One-way analysis of
the same and opposite sides of the task rela- variance with four levels of age revealed that
tive to the partner were analyzed separately. these age differences were significant, F(3,28)
= 4.56, p < .01. Post hoc analyses indicated
Interobserver reliability.-Videotapes that 12-month-olds achieved fewer solutions
were independently coded by two observers
who had been trained to criterion interrater than did 18-month-olds, who achieved fewer
than 24- or 30-month-olds, who did not differ
reliability of .85 overall (range = .79 to .97 from one another (see Table 1).
percentage agreement). Reliability checks
were conducted on one-third of the observa- Age differences were also significant for
tions, randomly selected, and reliability re- latency to the first solution, F(3,28) = 3.92, p
mained at or above initial training levels. < .02. Twelve-month-olds spent more time
Brownell and Carriger 1169
than did any other age. Because they never TABLE 2
solved the problem, they spent the entire 7 DISTRIBUTION OF COORDINATED BEHAVIORSOVER
min on their first attempt. Time to the first A SOLUTION ATTEMPT AS A FUNCTION OF AGE
solution did not differ among 18-, 24-, and 30-
month-olds, however. That finding is all the DISTRIBUTION OF BEHAVIORS
more interesting because, of those 18-month-
olds who solved the problem, not one dyad 10 Sec Prior All Other
was able to solve it again. In a two-way AGE to Solution Times
ANOVA (solution number x age) for latency
to solution among 24- and 30-month-olds 18 months ........ 6 (.12) 45 (.88)
24 months ........ 86 (.49) 89 (.51)
only, the interaction effect was not significant. 30 months ........119 (.73) 45 (.27)
In other words, 30-month-olds did not get
progressively faster over solutions compared NOTE.-Values represent mean frequency per dyad.
to 24-month-olds. Thus, both 24- and 30- Proportions are in parentheses.
month-olds were able to solve the problem
quickly and repeatedly in the allotted time,
whereas 12-month-olds were unable to coop-
erate with one another at all, and 18-month- .002, for solution 2; r = -.38, p < .002, for
olds apparently did so only accidentally since
solution 3), and marginally to the number of
solutions for the dyad (r = .21, p < .09).
they could not re-achieve solution.
These findings are buttressed by the data Finally, coordinated behavior was dis-
on children's coordination of behavior during tributed differently across the solution at-
problem-solving attempts. There were no tempt for each age (see Table 2). A two-way,
repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted
significant age differences for the time chil- on the frequency of coordinated behaviors.
dren spent in complementary positions oppo-
site one another at the task. However, the Factors were age (18, 24, 30 months) and time
amount of time children spent in complemen- period during the solution attempt (10 sec
tary positions was related to the number of prior to solution vs. all other times). There
times they solved the problem (r = .79, p < were no main effects, but the interaction was
.001). There was an age-related effect for the marginally significant, F(2,15) = 3.17, p <
.07. This suggests that the oldest children's
frequency of movements around the task, coordinated behavior was most likely to occur
both to the same side as the partner and to the
side opposite the partner, F(3,28) = 6.48, p < just prior to solution, whereas the youngest
children's coordinated behavior was least
.001, and 6.96, p < .001, respectively. Post
hoc tests indicated that the 12-month-olds likely to occur then. Among 24-month-olds,
coordinatedbehavior was equally likely to oc-
produced both types of movement less often cur immediately prior to solution as at other
than did the other three-ages, which were not
different from one another. In other words, times during a solution attempt.
the older dyads appeared to move around the In a supplemental analysis, which can
task both to join (or displace) their partner and only be suggestive because of the small num-
to complement their partner's behavior, while ber of dyads, 18-month-olds who solved the
the youngest dyads' movements relative to problem (n = 4 dyads) were compared to 18-
one another were minimal. month-olds who did not (n = 4 dyads). There
In line with this finding, which suggests were no differences between them for propor-
active attempts to coordinate behavior among tion of coordinated behavior, suggesting again
the older toddlers, age differences also that solutions at this age were accidental
rather than being due to active coordination
emerged on the summary variable of coor- of behavior between partners. Older children,
dinated behavior, F(3,28) = 13.64, p < .001.
The 24- and 30-month-old dyads engaged in then, more frequently exhibited coordinated
behavior and distributed this behavior closer
proportionally more coordinated behavior in time to the solution, and this, in turn, re-
than did the 18-month-olds, who in turn en-
lated to successful problem solving.
gaged in more coordinated behavior than did
the 12-month-olds. Further, the amount of Decentration.-Age was significantly re-
coordinated behavior children exhibited was lated to the highest level of decentration at
related to how often they changed positions which children represented at least one im-
relative to their partners, both to the same itated action, Kendall's tau = .42, p < .001
side (r = .29, p < .02) and to the opposite side (see Table 3). Most 12-month-olds imitated at
(r = .30, p < .02). Coordinated behavior was Level 1 (self as agent) or Level 2 (other as
also related to the time to achieve solutions (r passive recipient). Most 18-month-olds im-
= -.29, p < .02, for solution 1; r = -.38, p < itated at Level 2, while most 24- and 30-
1170 Child Development
TABLE 3
RELATION BETWEEN AGE AND DECENTRATION

DECENTRATION

AGE Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4


12 months ............. 5 9 1 0
18 months ............. 0 10 4 1
24 months ............. 0 6 8 2
30 months ............. 0 7 8 1
NOTE.-Values are the number of children for whom a given level of decentration
was their highest level represented during imitation.

month-olds imitated at Level 3 or 4 (other as composite decentration score was also related
active agent; two active others interacting). to how often children changed position to
complement the partner's behavior on the op-
The average age at which Level 1 was posite side of the task (r = .37, p < .003), a
achieved as the highest level imitated was 12 separate indication of their ability to coordi-
months. For Level 2 it was 20.1 months; for nate behavior. Interestingly, the composite
Level 3, 24.6 months; and for Level 4, 24.0 decentration score was not related to the fre-
months, F(3,58) = 6.91, p < .01. Similarly, the quency with which children moved to the
composite decentration score (number of ac- same side of the problem as the partner,
tions represented at each level) increased which is arguably a more primitive strategy
with age, F(3,58) = 15.3, p < .001. Post hoc (Brownell, 1989). When age was partialed out
analyses revealed that 24- and 30-month-olds' of the correlation between decentration and
decentration scores (M = 3.8, and 4.4, respec- coordinated behavior, their relation was no
tively) were higher than 18-month-olds' (M = longer significant. This is not altogether sur-
2.9), which were higher than 12-month-olds' prising since age is closely associated with
(M = 1.5). Hence, over the second year chil- each (see Tables 1 and 3).
dren became progressively more skilled at
representing decentered relations between At a slightly finer grain, we asked at
self and other in pretense. which level of decentration children began to
show cooperation. Although our measures of
Relation between cooperation and de- cooperation are continuous rather than cate-
centration.-Within dyads, partners' decen- gorical, we approximated a categorical mea-
tration scores were unrelated to one another sure by using a cut-off for proportion of coor-
(intraclass r = .14), as were'their scores for dinated behavior above which children were
coordinated behavior (intraclass r = .05). considered to be cooperating. We chose the
Thus, the scores of both dyad members were arbitrary and somewhat stringent but intui-
entered into subsequent correlational anal- tively appealing cut-off of 50%. Thus, if half
yses. Across dyads, children's composite de- or more of children's task-related behavior in-
centration score was related to the amount of cluded some attempt at coordination with the
coordinated behavior they exhibited on the partner, the child was said to be cooperating.
cooperation problem (r = .40, p < .001). Thus When children were cross-classified accord-
children at higher levels of decentration were ing to their level of decentration and whether
also more able to cooperate with a peer. The they cooperated or not (see Table 4), it be-

TABLE 4
RELATION BETWEEN LEVEL OF DECENTRATION AND COOPERATION

DECENTRATION

COOPERATION Level 1 Level 2 Level 3/4

H igha............................. 0 2 7
Low .......................... 5 31 16
NOTE.-Values
a Children were
represent number of children.
classified as high in cooperation if 50% or more of their task-related
behavior was coordinated with the partner's behavior.
Brownell and Carriger 1171
came evident that the number of children nipulations and opportunistically exploited
who regularly cooperated with a peer in- their advantage.
creased at Levels 3 and 4, X2(2) = 7.85, p <
.05. In other words, systematic efforts to coor- That there were no age differences in la-
dinate behavior with the partner appeared to tency to the first solution suggests that this
emerge in conjunction with the ability to rep- characterization may also fit the 24- and 30-
resent others' agency. month-olds. However, at the latter two ages
children solved the problem two or more
Pairs of children can also be distin- times after the first solution, whereas those
guished by the match between partners' 18-month-olds who did solve it could not re-
levels of decentration, and we asked whether achieve a solution. Further, for the older chil-
the composition of a dyad with respect to de- dren subsequent solutions were achieved
centration affected solutions. For single solu- more quickly than the first. This pattern sug-
tions, dyad composition made no difference. gests purposeful, jointly regulated coordina-
However, only one of the six dyads in which tion of behavior among the 24- and 30-month-
both children exhibited Level 2 decentration olds, but not among the younger children.
was able to solve the problem more than
once. In contrast, half the dyads (6 out of 11) This suggestion gains additional cre-
in which one child exhibited Level 2 decen- dence from the finding that older children
tration and the other was at Level 3 solved the more often produced coordinated behavior
problem multiple times. And two-thirds of than did 12- or 18-month-olds. That is, they
dyads (4 out of 6) with both children at or were more likely to manipulate the handle
above Level 3 solved it multiple times. These with a pause for the other child to respond, to
differences were significant, X2(2) = 7.73, p < anticipate or to wait for the other child's ac-
.05. Thus, for dyads to re-achieve solution, tion on the task, to give gestural or vocal di-
suggesting that their solutions were purpose- rections to the partner about problem-related
ful and intentionally coordinated, at least one behavior, and to comply with the partner's di-
child in the pair had to be able to represent rections. Older children also moved to posi-
others as causing their own behavior. tions opposite one another at the task more
often, positions necessary for solution, and in-
Discussion dicative of the ability to adopt complementary
behavioral roles. Although 30-month-olds no
Cooperation between young peers de- doubt command better planning and self-
pends on the joint coordination of social be- regulation skills than do 24-month-olas, as
havior between social novices. Although it is well as more complex representationL of self
clear that children cooperate with adults very and other, the simplicity of the cooperation
early, even in the first year (e.g., Ross & Kay, task used in the present study did not permit
1980; Ross & Lollis, 1987), the first instances the demonstration of such differences. Hence,
of systematic, coordinated, thematic peer- one profitable direction for future endeavors
peer cooperation do not appear until the sec- may be to provide greater detail about how
ond half of the second year. Why is that?'It these basic abilities change after their emer-
has been suggested here that fundamental gence between 18 and 24 months.
cognitive or social-cognitive abilities may be
a necessary part of this transition into skilled How do the cooperative problem-solving
skills documented here compare with cooper-
peer interaction. That is, while a parent can
"scaffold" an infant into successful joint en- ation in spontaneous social interaction be-
tween young peers? Research has concen-
gagement, giving the appearance of infant
skill where it may be only very rudimentary, trated on cooperative or thematic play and
the burden falls on young peers to mutually games (Brenner & Mueller, 1982; Bronson,
manage their own interactions based on their 1981; Eckerman & Stein, 1982; Eckerman et
actual skill and understanding. al., 1989; Goldman & Ross, 1978; Howes,
1987, 1988; Ross, 1982), all of which contain
As the findings have indicated, at 12 some element of behavioral coordination be-
months children were unable to cooperate to tween peer partners, as well as shared under-
solve a simple problem. Eighteen-month-olds standing of interactional content. Previous
were just beginning to evidence some basic investigators have noted that to establish
collaborative abilities, but their performance cooperative play with another, children must
was uneven, and cooperation was infrequent communicate their intentions, understand the
and apparently serendipitous. Rather than peer's signals and intentions, establish a joint
planfully coordinating behavior to achieve so- topic and roles, and regulate the temporal
lution, it appeared that they simply happened structure of the interchange (Eckerman &
to notice the effects of the other child's ma- Stein, 1982; Ross, 1982). The games and
1172 Child Development
shared themes that begin to emerge at about plementary/reciprocal peer play at 24 months.
18 months are more advanced than earlier Similarly, Eckerman (Eckerman et al., 1989)
and simpler contingent exchanges because of reported that although young toddlers pre-
the children's recognition of their related dominantly imitate one another during
roles and their attempts to maintain interac- games, at 28 months they also begin to incor-
tions around a single, mutually recognized porate complex complementary behavior into
theme. Thus, cooperative problem solving in their games. These latter games seemed "to
the current study requires skills that are simi- reuire ways of directing another's behavior in
lar in many ways to those involved in the addition to . . . imitation" (p. 449). In other
games and thematic social play that appear work on young children's cooperative prob-
during the latter half of the second year. lem solving, it was found that 24-month-olds
adopted complementary behavioral roles, but
However, it is also clear that social play 18-month-olds could not unless they were di-
does not require the same amount of plan- rected to do so by their 24-month-old partners
ning, the same understanding of interpersonal (Brownell, 1989). It appears, then, that chil-
causal relations, and the precision of coordi- dren's ability to manage complex social be-
nation that is required by cooperative prob- havior that is different from but thematically
lem solving. To play a game of ball or chase, related to a partner's may emerge at 24 to 30
or a mutual imitation game, does indeed re- months in both peer freeplay and in the more
quire both children to subordinate their indi- constrained setting of cooperative problem
vidual behavior to a dyadic theme and to se-
solving.
quence their behavior temporally to maintain
the theme. But there is a great deal of latitude One potential limitation on the breadth of
in the particular behaviors the children can our conclusions is the possibility that our re-
individually produce within any given dyadic sults are unique to this class of cooperation
theme, and there are few necessary causal re- problems. Although we used three different
lations between the two children's behavior. cooperation tasks to avoid confounding the re-
It would be legitimate, for example, to turn sults with a particular task, the possibility re-
a game of ball into a game of chase and still mains that these physical world problems are
be viewed as achieving the common goal of not representative of the social-cognitive de-
maintaining joint focus and keeping the ex- mands made by cooperation more generally.
change going. And interactional roles can be, However, correspondences with other inves-
and often are, imitative rather than com- tigators' findings of similar age-related pat-
plementary (Eckerman et al., 1989). terns in unstructured social play (reviewed
above) provide indirect evidence for the va-
In contrast, the cooperative problem- lidity of our tasks. Additionally, all children
solving task in the present study required the in the present study manipulated the tasks ap-
children to adopt particular, fairly narrowly propriately to achieve an individual solution
defined roles both spatially and temporally, had the barriers and springs not prevented it.
which were necessarily complementary. And recent research has confirmed that even
There is little leeway in such a situation for 12-month-olds can solve compound means-
alternate forms of behavior that can equally ends problems (Willatts & Rosie, 1989), sug-
well achieve the joint goal. The goal itself, gesting that our tasks were not too cognitively
moreover, is specified for the children in complex. Nevertheless, it would be informa-
cooperative problem solving rather than spon- tive to compare the performance of young
taneously invented and re-invented as in children across a wider variety of cooperation
play. Finally, in a successful solution, one tasks, settings, and partners.
child's behavior is not only temporally related
to the other child's, it also is causally related Other important questions revolve
to the achievement of the solution. Hence, it around the factors that influence the emer-
is not altogether surprising that while simple gence and development of these early forms
games and shared themes emerge in peer of cooperation, particularly when cooperation
play at 18 months, the more demanding coop- involves the adoption of complementary
eration skills studied here do not appear until roles. In the present study it was found that
24 months. children who were better able to cooperate
also were better able to represent others'
Parallels exist, however, if we restrict agency, consistent with the hypothesis moti-
comparisons to children's complementary vating the study.
role relations in peer play. In both cross-
sectional and longitudinal samples, Howes The research design of this study does
(1988) reported marked increases in com- not, of course, permit us to address the ques-
Brownell and Carriger 1173
tion of whether self-other differentiation is a of social understanding (pp. 271-298). Or-
prerequisite for cooperation. It can only show lando, FL: Academic Press.
that the two processes are developmentally Bronson, W. (1981). Toddlers' behavior with age-
related to one another. It is possible that both mates: Issues of interaction,affect and cogni-
cooperation and decentration are determined tion. Monographs on infancy (Vol. 1). Nor-
by a different, still more fundamental age- wood, NJ: Ablex.
related change. Alternatively, they may be Brownell, C. (1986). Convergent developments:
separately caused by independent mecha- Cognitive-developmental correlates of growth
nisms that both happen to coincide closely in infant//toddler peer skills. Child Develop-
with age. Others have demonstrated, for ex- ment, 57, 275-286.
ample, that between 18 and 24 months chil- Brownell, C. (1989). Cooperationand social under-
dren begin to make causal inferences and to standing in toddlers. Manuscriptsubmittedfor
evidence foresight (Kagan, 1981; McCall, publication.
Eichorn, & Hogarty, 1977; Wachs & Hubert, Bullock, M., & Lutkenhaus,P. (1988).The develop-
1981), both apparent requirements of cooper- ment of volitional behavior in the toddler
ative problem solving. Perhaps the empirical years. Child Development, 59, 664-674.
relation between self-other understanding Eckerman,C., Davis, C., & Didow, S. (1989). Tod-
and cooperation during the second year is a dlers' emerging ways of achieving social coor-
simple by-product of the many other age- dinations with a peer. Child Development, 60,
related changes that occur during this period. 440-453.
It is also possible that cognitive changes Eckerman, C., & Stein, M. (1982). The toddler's
such as those entailed in self-other differ- emerging interactive skills. In K. Rubin & H.
entiation derive from experience in coopera- Ross (Eds.), Peer relationships and social skills
tive social encounters that force the young in childhood (pp. 41-72). New York:Springer-
child to consider others' perspectives. Hence, Verlag.
certain kinds of social experiences may serve Fein, G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An
as a context for certain cognitive develop- integrative review. Child Development, 52,
ments rather than vice versa as often as- 1095-1118.
sumed. Fenson, L., & Ramsay,D. (1980). Decentrationand
integration of the child's play in the second
Finally, as Hinde and Bateson (1984) year. Child Development, 51, 171-178.
have cogently argued, it is altogether possible Fenson, L., & Ramsay,D. (1981). Effects of model-
that apparent discontinuities in development, ing action sequences on the play of 12-, 15-,
such as the emergence of self-other differ- and 19-month-olds. Child Development, 52,
entiation or cooperation, may be produced by 1028-1036.
continuous rather than discontinuous changes Goldman, B., & Ross, H. (1978). Social skills in ac-
at underlying levels of organization. Although tion: Analysis of early peer games. In J. Glick
evidence for behavioral reorganization often & A. Clarke-Stewart(Eds.), The development
comes from patterns of correlations (as in the of social understanding (pp. 177-212). New
present study), such patterns can be ex- York:Gardner.
plained in numerous ways, not always having Gopnik, A., & Metzoff, A. (1986). Relations be-
to do with changes in psychological structure. tween semantic and cognitive development in
We may be misled, then, if we continue to the one-word stage: The specificityhypothesis.
assume that "a behavioral act is isomorphic Child Development, 57, 1040-1053.
with an underlying structure" (Hinde & Bate- Hartup,W., & Brownell, C. (1988). Early social de-
son, 1984). Longitudinal research on the indi- velopment: Transitionsand concordances.Eta
vidual components of cooperative problem Evolutiva, 29, 5-17
solving, as well as on conceptually related as- Hay, D. (1979). Cooperative interactionsand shar-
pects of self-other understanding, will be nec- ing between very young children and their par-
essary to clarify the actual nature of the corre- ents. Developmental Psychology, 15, 647-653.
spondences detected in this study. Hinde, R., & Bateson, Pt (1984). Discontinuities vs.
continuities in behavioural development and
References the neglect of process. InternationalJournal of
Brenner,J., & Mueller, E. (1982). Shared meaning Behavioral Development, 7, 129-143.
in boy toddlers' peer relations.Child Develop- Howes, C. (1985). Sharing fantasy: Social pretend
ment, 53, 380-391. play in toddlers.Child Development, 56, 1253-
Bretherton,I., O'Connell, B., Shore, C., & Bates, E. 1258.
(1984). The effect of contextual variation on Howes, C. (1987). Social competence with peers in
symbolic play from 20 to 28 months. In I. young children: Developmental sequences.
Bretherton(Ed.), Symbolic play: Development Developmental Review, 7, 252-272.
1174 Child Development
Howes, C. (1988). Peer interaction of young chil- social relations in infancy. In T. Alloway, P.
dren. Monographsof the Societyfor Research Pliner, & L. Krames (Eds.), Advances in com-
in Child Development, 53(1, Serial No. 217). munication and affect (pp. 177-212). New
Kagan,J. (1981). The second year: The emergence York: Plenum.
of self-awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Ross, H., & Kay, D. (1980). Origins of social games.
University Press. In K. Rubin (Ed.), Children's play: New direc-
Kopp, C. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: tions in child development (Vol. 5, pp. 17-32).
A developmental perspective. Developmental San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Psychology, 18, 199-214. Ross, H., & Lollis, S. (1987). Communication within
McCall, R., Eichorn, D., & Hogarty, P. (1977). Tran- infant social games. Developmental Psychol-
sitions in early mental development. Mono- ogy, 23, 241-248.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Schaffer, H. (Ed.). (1979). Acquiring the concept of
Development, 42(3, Serial No. 171). the dialogue. In M. Bornstein & W. Kessen
McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic (Eds.), Psychological development from in-
functioning: Structure of early pretend and po- fancy (pp. 279-306). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tential parallels with language. Child Develop- Wachs, T., & Hubert, T. (1981). Changes in the
ment, 52, 785-797. structure of intellectual performance during
Mueller, E., & Lucas, T. (1975). A developmental the second year. Infant Behaviorand Develop-
analysis of peer interaction among toddlers. In ment, 4, 151-161.
M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Friendship Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A developmental
and peer relations (pp. 223-257). New York: sequence of agent use in late infancy. Child
Wiley. Development, 48, 828-836.
Nicolich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intel- Watson, M. W., & Fischer, K. (1980). Development
ligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity of social roles in elicited and spontaneous be-
through analysis of pretend play. Merrill Pal- havior during the preschool years. Develop-
mer Quarterly,23, 89-99. mental Psychology, 16, 483-494.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the Willatts, P., & Rosie, K. (1989, April). Planning by
child. New York: Basic. 12-month-old infants. Paper presented at the
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in meetings of Society for Research in Child De-
childhood. New York: Norton. velopment.
Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Chapman, Wolf, D. (1982). Understanding others: A longi-
M. (1983). Children's prosocial dispositions tudinal case study of the concept of indepen-
and behavior. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), dent agency. In G. Forman (Ed.), Action and
P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child thought: From sensorimotor schemes to sym-
psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality bolic operations (pp. 297-328). New York: Ac-
and social development (pp. 469-545). New ademic Press.
York: Wiley. Zahn-Waxler, C., lannotti, R., & Chapman, M.
Ross, H. (1982). Establishment of social games (1982). Peers and prosocial behavior. In K. Ru-
among toddlers. Developmental Psychology, bin & H. Ross (Eds.), Peer relationships and
18, 509-518. social skills in childhood (pp. 133-162). New
Ross, H., & Goldman, B. (1977). Establishing new York: Springer-Verlag.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen