Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

UNIVERSITY STUDENTS’ PERCEPTION OF PEOPLE LIVING

WITH HIV/AIDS: DISCOMFORT, FEAR, KNOWLEDGE AND A


WILLINGNESS TO CARE

J arm o H outsonen , PhD


Police College o f Finland

J ari K ylm A, P h D , RN, RUT University o f Tampere


University o f Tampere School o f Health Sciences

T eua K orhonen , P hD, RN


Helsinki Metropolia University o f Applied Sciences

M aritta VA lim Ak i , P hD, RN


University o f Turku Department o f Nursing Science, Faculty o f Medicine

T arja S uom inen , P h D


University o f Tampere School o f Health Sciences

People living with HIV/AIDS (PLWHA) are often subject to blame,


fear and avoidance, particularly if they are perceived as personally re­
sponsible for their infection due to their risky behaviour or life style
choices. Some people however, react to PLWHA with sympathy and
a willingness to care. This paper explores how university students (n
= 282) perceive different types of PLWHA, in terms of how comfort­
able it is to be in contact with them. Students were most sympathetic
towards children with HIV/AIDS, and felt most discomfort with in­
travenous drug users and prostitutes. Bi- and homosexuals with HTV/
AIDS were mostly positively perceived, while many respondents had
difficulties in expressing an opinion on haemorrhagic disease patients.
Discomfort was associated with a fear o f infection, unwillingness to
care for PLWHA and an ignorance of HIV/AIDS. Women and older
students tended to fear infection less than men and younger students.
We conclude that although over time the perception of PLWHA has
improved, there is still a need for enlightenment, particularly with re­
spect to the most vulnerable groups such as intravenous chug users,
haemorrhagic disease patients and prostitutes.
Keywords: Student, Types o f PLWHA, Care, Discomfort, Fear,
Knowledge

534
U n iv e rs ity S tu d e n ts ’ P e rc e p tio n o f P e o p le Living w ith H IV /A ID S / 5 3 5

Introduction negatively. This may result in discrimination


The UNAIDS Report on the Global AIDS and the exclusion of the stigmatized person.
Epidemic (2012) estimated that at the end Stigma is a context-specific social construct
o f 2011 there were about 34 million people and Scambler (2006, 293) remarks that in the
living with HIV. Approximately 2.5 million post welfare state, stigma is often justified by
people acquired HIV infection and about 1.7 the idea of personal responsibility. Particular­
million people died from AIDS-related causes ly, health related stigma indicates a personal
in 2011 (UNAIDS 2012). People living with responsibility because many illnesses such as
HIV/AIDS (PLWHA) are often subject to HIV/AIDS are regarded as being outcomes of
blame, fear and avoidance, though there are personal choice and a risky life style.
also those who perceive PLWHA with sympa­ People acquire HTV in various ways, and
thy and willingness to care (e.g. Paxton et al., this has implications for the assessment of
2005; Weiss, Ramakrishna, & Somma 2006.) the personal responsibility o f the unwell.
Fear and avoidance due to illness may lead This is not a totally new development. In­
to stigma. A stigmatized person may deviate deed, Small (1993, 90) observes that the
from the model of standard human being, and link between an epidemic caused by a virus
therefore be negatively perceived (Goffman, and individual moral failure (particularly in
1990; Link & Phelan, 2001; Major & O’Bri­ sexual behaviour) was established as early
an, 2005; Paxton et al,. 2005; Weiss, Ram­ as the 19th century. For instance, the vulnera­
akrishna & Somma, 2006; Wong & Wong, bility to typhoid fever, cholera, tuberculosis,
2006; Heijnders & Van Der Meij, 2006). In a cancer and in particular venereal diseases
post-welfare state, stigma such as that relating was often associated with individual moral
to HIV/AIDS is largely founded on the idea deficiency and social marginality.
of personal responsibility (Scambler, 2006, It is revealing that the terms HIV or AIDS
293). The responsibility for health lies with are often used as adjectives, together with
the individual who is presumed to be able some other category attached to a person
to manage their health risks by making the (Lupton, 1999, 38). A category is a typifica-
right choices and life style changes (Musso & tion, which simplifies the object of perception
Wakefield, 2009). Perceptions of responsibil­ and knowledge. However, we use categories
ity and dangerousness tend to lead to either not only as cognitive tools for understanding
fear and avoidance, or pity and a willingness reality, but also to signify a person’s life-style
to care (see Small, 1993; Lupton, 1999; Cobb or choices that are liable to moral judgment.
& De Chabert, 2002; Hegarty & Golden, Consequently, depending on the social cate­
2008; Persson & Newman, 2008). gory of a person living with HIV/AIDS (e.g.
‘HIV man’, ‘HIV child’, ‘AIDS victim’, or
Background ‘AIDS sufferer’), people are invited to adopt
Major and O ’Brian (2005, 395; see also a particular moral stance towards the PLWHA
(see also Persson, 2005).
Link & Phelan, 2001; Parker & Aggleton,
Indeed, the research shows that the early
2003) define stigma as an attribute that con­
veys a devalued social identity in some par­ discourse on HIV/AIDS in the 1980s made
ticular social relationship. Stigma does not a distinction between the innocent victims
and the guilty risk-takers. The responsibility
reside in the person as such, but in the way
members of the community oversimplify of PLWHA is seen to depend on how much
certain social identities and evaluate them control the person can have over the infection.
For instance haemorrhagic disease patients,
536 / College Student Journal

children, and those who became infected from various social categories of PLWHA is chang­
blood products were depicted as innocent vic­ ing (Persson 2005; Persson & Newman, 2008).
tims, whereas drug addicts, homosexuals and At the early stages of the HIV/AIDS epidemic
sex workers were considered as guilty and in the 1980s, gay men with HIV/AIDS were
therefore responsible for their state of affairs. regarded as guilty and responsible for then-
The latter types of PLWHA were deemed to disease, because the infection was regarded as
have taken a chance by being reckless in then- a consequence of a conscious life-style choice
behaviour (Lupton, 1999; Scambler, 2006; (Lupton, 1999; Small, 1993). In later times, ho­
Small, 1993). The difference between these mosexuality has become more widely socially
two modes of infection is that in the latter accepted, and according to Lupton (1999)
cases the person is perceived as being respon­ there was little evidence in the Australian press
sible for the outcome, because the activity is during the mid-1990s that indicated gay men
self-initiated and would have been controlla­ were depicted as being guiltier than other peo­
ble. In the former cases, the infection came ple with HIV/AIDS.
from an external and uncontrollable source, A person living with HTV/AIDS can be
which removes the person’s responsibility seen as a source of infection—an AIDS carri­
for the outcome. Thus, the social category er. An AIDS carrier is often described as lack­
of PLWHA suggests information about then- ing self-control, of being negligent, or being
life-styles, relative risks for infection, and simply revengeful (Lupton, 1999, 46-48). In
their levels o f personal responsibility (Steins the early 1990s, infected heterosexual males
& Weiner, 1999,488). were often considered to be innocent victims,
Steins and Weiner (1999) systematically whereas infected women who ‘spread the
studied how perceived responsibility and virus’ were not only regarded as guilty, but
personality characteristics influence on the also as a potential threat (Small, 1993, 103).
emotional and behavioural reactions towards Today the guilty parties are often represented
PLWHA. They analysed the association be­ as black African men with a ‘criminal intent
tween the transmission mode, personality to spread the virus’ through unprotected het­
characteristics of the PLWHA, responsibility erosexual relationships (Persson & Newman,
and the perceivers’ emotional (anger or pity) 2008). Furthermore, intravenous drug users
and behavioural (avoidance or willingness and prostitutes can be seen as potentially dan­
to care) responses. People tend to be angry gerous sources of infection because of then-
to a person if he or she is held personally presumed irresponsible behaviour and risky
responsible o f a negative event. By contrast, life-styles—for example, the drug addicts
people feel pity if a person is regarded free who might pass the virus to innocent children
from responsibility. It is also suggested that by leaving their contaminated syringes in
these emotional reactions trigger different playgrounds. Stigma, deviance, and negative
behaviours towards the person. Pity increas­ feelings of blame are dynamically changing
es willingness to care, whereas anger tends social processes (Parker & Aggleton, 2003,
to channel behaviour into to avoidance. 14), and so consequently, the norms of devi­
(Steins & Weiner, 1999; see also Cobb & ant behaviour, moral judgments and personal
De Chabert, 2002; Corrigan et al., 2003; He- responsibility may change over time.
garty & Golden, 2008) The perception of dangerousness may
The distinction between the innocent and arouse a sense of fear and lead to social avoid­
the guilty has remained in HIV/AIDS discourse ance. The more you fear the illness, the more
since the very beginning, but the evaluation of you try to avoid it and the less you want to
U n iv e rs ity S tu d e n ts ’ P e rc ep tio n o f P e o p le Living w ith H IV /A ID S / 5 3 7

care the sick. By contrast, the more you are The first question pertains to how stu­
familiar with the illness and its mechanism, dents perceive different types of PLWHA.
the less you fear infection and you are more The respondents were asked to give their
likely to empathise the victims and be willing opinion about the statement T would feel
to care them (Corrigan et al., 2003; Paxton et uncomfortable in contact with a [type of
al., 2005). If knowledge serves to decrease PLWHA], who has HIV/AIDS.’ The types
fear and avoidance, and increase feelings of we used were ‘child’, ‘bisexual’, ‘homo­
pity and the willingness to care for a sick sexual’, ‘haemorrhagic disease patient’,
person, then educating the general public ‘prostitute’, and an ‘intravenous drug user.’
with respect to HIV/AIDS would be likely to The labels imply the manner o f infection,
improve the situation of PLWHA. responsibility and dangerousness as dis­
This paper looks to answer the following cussed in the theoretical section of this pa­
questions: per. However, we did not specifically assess
1. How do students perceive different the grounds by which students formulate
types of PLWHA? their opinion of comfort or discomfort, and
thus we do not know exactly if they regard
2. How are their feelings of discomfort these types of PLWHA as responsible, inno­
related to their fear of infection? cent victims, or threats.
3. How is the fear of infection (and dis­ The original 5-point Likert scale (agree
comfort) related to their knowledge of strongly, agree, cannot say, disagree, disa­
HIV/AIDS, familiarity with PLWHA, gree strongly) was transformed into three
willingness to care for PLWHA, gen­ options (agree, cannot say, disagree) in order
der, and age? to avoid categories with few observations
(less than 5 %). To find the ranking order of
Methods and procedures the types of PLWHA, we combined all single
Design and setting tables into a 6 x 3 cross-table with six types
o f PLWHA in rows and three response alter­
This study used a descriptive research de­
natives in columns (Table 1).
sign. The data was obtained from basic degree
The second question focused more close­
students (N = 9715) at a Finnish university.
ly on how the perception of different types of
Research permission was obtained from the
PLWHA is associated with the fear o f infec­
department of student affairs at the universi­
tion. We gauged response by use o f the state­
ty. With approximately 15,000 students, this
ment: ‘I would be/was afraid of getting HIV
multidisciplinary university offers teaching in
if I were/was in contact with an HIV positive
more than 100 major subjects. The sample (n
person.’ Previous research suggests that
= 950) was randomly picked from the elec­
discomfort and fear are closely associated,
tronic student directory of registered basic
and our data showed that fear had a strong
degree students with no exclusion criteria.
association with the discomfort relating to
Instrument different types of PLWHA. The gamma val­
ues of six 3-way tables were: 0.649 (child),
The survey was an adapted version of 0.644 (bisexual), 0.569 (homosexual), 0.509
two North American questionnaires (Chng & (prostitute), 0.518 (haemorrhagic disease
Moore, 1994; Held, 1993) first used in Fin­ patient) and 0.475 (IV drug user).
land by Muinonen et al. (2002) among early
adolescents.
538 / College Student Journal

Data collection the variance of the table, but were used only
to interpret the results.
A postal survey was conducted. Ques­
tionnaires and introduction letters were ‘Knowledge of HIV/AIDS’ was con­
sent to students’ home addresses, obtained structed on the basis of modified scales from
Held’s (1993) instrument. The knowledge
from the student register. In the introduction
test about HIV and AIDS consisted of 25
letter was a short description of the study,
and information regarding confidentiality, items (Cronbach’s alpha 0.908) and the max­
imum from correct answers was 25 points.
anonymity and voluntary participation. The
For the purposes of analysis, we grouped the
opportunity to contact the researcher by
respondents into four categories from ‘K 1’
phone or email was also given. The ques­
(least knowledge) to ‘K 4 ’ (most knowledge).
tionnaires were returned to researchers at the
university in sealed and pre-paid envelopes. ‘Willingness to care’ was measured by the
question ‘are you willing to care for a person
The students had the possibility to return an
empty questionnaire. Return of the question­ with HIV/AIDS?’ (yes/no). ‘Known a PL­
WHA’ was measured by the question ‘have
naire was considered as giving consent to
you ever known a family member, friend or
participate in the study. 950 questionnaires
other close person with HIV/AIDS?’ (yes/
were sent out, with 333 returns (35 %). After
no). Age was categorized into four uneven
removing insufficiently completed question­
classes that roughly capture the range o f typ­
naires to avoid missing data, the final sample
ical Finnish university students: ‘the young’
size amounted to 282 students.
(<21), ‘the normal age of graduation’ (22-
Analysis 25), ‘mature students’ (26-34), and ‘second
career students’ (>35).
We cross-tabulated the variable of ‘fear of
We used Simple Correspondence Anal­
infection’ with the six variables concerning
ysis (SCA) as our statistical tool. SCA
discomfort towards PLWHA types. For cor­
searches the latent structure of the data table
respondence analysis, we stacked these six
and transforms it into a few (usually two or
cross-tabulations into one grand cross-tabu­
three) interpretable dimensions (axes), and
lation with 18 rows of discomfort and three
presents categories of variables as configura­
columns of fear (Table 2). The stacked table
tions of points in geometric plains. SCA fo­
shows associations between each type of PL­
cuses more on the patterns of the data and is
WHA and fear, without containing informa­
less concerned about traditional significance
tion about the interactions between the types
tests (Greenacre, 2007; Le Roux & Rouanet,
of PLWHA (Greenacre, 2007).
2010, 23-65).
We wanted to determine how the fear of in­
fection (and discomfort) is associated with the Results
willingness to care for PLWHA, familiarity
Students ’ perceptions o f different types o f
with HIV positive people, and knowledge of
PLWHA
HIV/AIDS, gender and age. Thus we includ­
ed ‘willingness to care’, ‘known a PLWHA’ Table 1 presents the relative frequencies of
and ‘knowledge of HIV/AIDS’ in addition to responses to the question concerning whether
‘gender’ and ‘age’ as supplementary variables they would feel comfortable in contact with
in our 1 8 x 3 cross-tabulation. Altogether we different types HIV/AIDS patients. On aver­
formed a 32 x 3 cross-tabulation, where the age, about 25 % of respondents find it uncom­
supplementary variables did not contribute to fortable to be in contact with a person who
University Students’ Perception of People Living with HIV/AIDS / 539

has H IV /A ID S. The unpleasantness o f types To reveal the ranking order o f the six types
o f PLW H A varies from 47.5 % (T V drug o f H IV /A ID S patients, w e perform ed SCA
u sers’) to 6.4 % ( ‘ch ild ’). M ore than 30 % on Table 1. The results o f the analysis are
o f respondents w ould feel uncom fortable in presented in Figure 1. The total eigenvalue
contact w ith a ‘p rostitute’ or a ‘haem orrhagic (variance) is 0.1417, o f w hich the first axis
disease p atient’ w ho has H IV /A ID S and stu­ accounts for 90.12 %. T he first dim ension
dents have difficulties to form an opinion in o f the graph clearly opposes ‘disagree’ (con­
these cases (17.7 % and 21.3 % respectively). tributes 34.5 % ) on the left side, and ‘ag ree’

Table 1: Discomfort of Types of PLWHA (row profiles)

I would feel uncomfortable if in contact with [...] who Respondents’ opinion


has HIV/AIDS. Agree Cannot say Disagree
Child 6.4* 6.4 87.2 282
Bisexual 14.2 11.0 74.8 282
Homosexual 14.5 13.5 72.0 282
Prostitute 30.9 17.7 51.4 282
Haemorrhagic disease patient 31.9 21.3 46.8 282
Intravenous drug user 47.5 11.0 41.5 282
Average row profile 24.2 13.5 62.3 1692
* Figures are row percentages, that is, row profiles. The average row profile is calculated by first summing
up the column frequencies. Then each summed column frequency is divided by the sum of the average
row total frequency. Due to rounding, percentages do not necessarily sum up to 100.

Figure 1: Simple Correspondence Analysis (SCA) of Table 1. Discomfort of Types of


PLWHA
Argument: ‘I would feel uncomfortable if in contact with [...] who has HIV/AIDS.’
Categories: ‘Agree’, ‘Cannot say’, ‘Disagree’, Child (‘CHILD’), Bisexual (‘BI’), Homosexual (‘HOMO’), Prostitute
(‘PROST’), Haemorrhagic disease patient (‘HEMO’), Intravenous drug user (‘IV DURG’)
540 / College Student Journal

(contributes 60.7 %) on the right side. These user, which together contribute 67.9 % of the
two categories are also well represented in the variance in the first dimension.
first axis since their squared cosines are 0.97 The second dimension captures the re­
and 0.99 respectively. The types of HIV in­ maining 9.88 % of the total variance and
fected persons (child, bisexual, homosexual, depicts the opposition between those who
prostitute, haemorrhagic disease patient and cannot formulate their opinion and those
IV drug user) are clearly ordered from left who ‘agree’ with the statement. However, the
(most comfortable) to right (least comfort­ category of ‘cannot say’ contributes only 3.2
able). The categories of child, bisexual and % of the variance in the second axis. People
homosexual are on the ‘disagree’ side (left) tended to be particularly certain that they do
of the map, whereas prostitute, haemorrhagic not feel comfortable with IV drug users (con­
disease patient and IV drug user are on the tributes 46.3 %). On the other hand, 21.3 % of
‘agree’ side (right). The strongest opposition respondents were uncertain about their opin­
is between the categories of child and IV drug ion regarding a haemorrhagic disease patient

Table 2: Fear of Infection and Discomfort of Types of PLWHA (row profiles)


I would be/was afraid of getting HTV if I were/was in contact
with HIV positive
I would feel uncomfortable if in contact with n
Agree Cannot say Disagree
[...] who has HIV/AIDS.
Child Agree 66.7 5.6 27.8 18
Cannot say 72.2 16.7 11.1 18
Disagree 30.5 7.7 61.8 246
Bisexual Agree 72.5 5.0 22.5 40
Cannot say 54.8 16.1 29.0 31
Disagree 25.6 7.6 66.8 211
Homosexual Agree 70.7 4.9 24.4 41
Cannot say 44.7 18.4 36.8 38
Disagree 26.6 6.9 66.5 203
Prostitute Agree 60.9 5.7 33.0 87
Cannot say 30.0 14.0 56.0 50
Disagree 22.1 7.6 70.3 145
Haemorrhagic disease patient Agree 58.9 4.4 36.7 90
Cannot say 33.3 20.0 46.7 60
Disagree 20.5 5.3 74.2 132
Intravenous drug user Agree 49.3 7.5 43.3 134
Cannot say 38.7 16.1 45.2 31
Disagree 18.8 6.8 74.4 117
Total 35.5 8.4 56.4 1692

* Figures are row percentages, that is, row profiles. The average row profile is calculated by first summing
up the column frequencies. Then each summed column frequency is divided by the sum of the average
row total frequency. Due to rounding, percentages do not necessarily round to 100.
U n ive rs ity S tu d e n ts ’ P e rc ep tio n o f P e o p le Living w ith H IV /A ID S / 541

Figure 2: Simple correspondence analysis of Table 3. Fear of Infection and Discomfort of


Types ofPLWHA
Argument: ‘I would be/was afraid o f getting HTV if I were/was in contact with HTV positive.’
Categories: ‘Fear agree’, ‘Fear cannot say’, ‘Fear disagree’
Argument: ‘I would feel uncomfortable if in contact with [...] who has HIV/AIDS.’
Categories: Child agree (‘Child agr’), Child cannot say (‘Child cnnt’) Child disagree (‘Child dis’), Bisexual agree
( Bi agr ), Bisexual cannot say (‘Bi cnnt’), Bisexual disagree (‘Bi dis’), and so on for each type o f People living with
HIV/AIDS.

(contributes 33.1 %). There were also quite for 86.2 % o f the total eigenvalue (0.1357),
many ‘cannot say’ answers in the case of a and opposes those who fear the infection
prostitute (17.7 %). (contributes 60.1 %) and those who do not
We should also notice that the category of (contributes 39.7 %). The second dimension
‘cannot say’ is quite well represented in the (eigenvalue 0.0187) and 13.8 % o f the ex­
first axis, having a squared cosine of 0.35. plained variance is dominated by those who
Both categories ‘cannot say’ and ‘agree’ are are uncertain about their opinion.
on the right side o f the map, and their distance On average, those who fear infection
in the first axis is 0.36 units. The distance tend to feel discomfort towards all types of
between ‘cannot say’ and ‘disagree’ is 0.48 PLWHA, whereas those who do not fear
units. Therefore, we can conclude that un­ infection tend to sympathize with PLWHA.
certainty is closer to discomfort rather than In addition, Figure 2 shows that the analysis
comfort with PLWHA. reproduces almost exactly the same rankings
of the types of PLWHA as discovered in the
Fear o f Infection and the Discomfort first correspondence analysis. It is also noted
Table 2 and Figure 2 present the results that those students who are uncertain in their
of the second SCA concerning the fear of assessment of fear, also tend to be uncertain in
getting HIV if in contact with an HIV pos­ their assessment of the discomfort pleasant­
itive person. The first dimension accounts ness of all types ofPLWHA.
542 / College Student Journal

All of the ‘agree’ and ‘disagree’ categories user and homosexual correlated strongly with
concerning the feelings of discomfort towards the second dimension.
the types of PLWHA, except for ‘child’ con­
tributed strongly to the first dimension. In Fear o f infection and knowledge o f HIV/
addition, all ‘agree’ and ‘disagree’ categories AIDS, familiarity with PLWHA, willingness
of the discomfort variable are very strongly to care, gender and age
associated with the first dimension. All ‘can­ Figure 3 shows the location of supple­
not say’ categories of types o f PLWHA, ex­ mentary variables of ‘willingness to care’,
cept ‘prostitute’ are located on the ‘fear’ side ‘known a PLWHA’ and ‘knowledge of HIV/
(right) of the map. Furthermore, ‘child cannot AIDS’, together with an active variable of
say’ (0.91), ‘bisexual cannot say’ (0.77) and ‘Fear of infection.’ Unwillingness to care for
‘homosexual cannot say’ (0.41) correlated PLWHA and the fear of infection are clearly
strongly with the first axis. This means that associated. The categories of ‘will - no’ and
those who were uncertain about their feelings ‘fear - agree’ are located on the right side of
of discomfort towards child, bisexual and ho­ the map, whereas ‘will - yes’ and ‘fear - dis­
mosexual categories tended to fear infection agree’ are located on the opposite side. The
more than average. two categories of the supplementary variable
The second dimension opposes those of ‘willingness to care’ correlate almost com­
respondents who were uncertain and those pletely with the first dimension, as they both
who certainly fear or don’t fear infection. Of have squared cosines of 0.95 (Table 3).
the PLWHA types, ‘haemorrhagic disease ‘Known a PLWHA’ and fear of infection
patient’ and ‘homosexual’ have the strongest have an association. If the respondent has
contribution to the second dimension (35.1 % known a HIV positive person, they tended
and 15.8 % respectively). Furthermore, hae­ to be less than averagely concerned about
morrhagic disease patient, prostitute, IV drug infection. However, only 8.9 % of the sample

Figure 3: Supplementary Variables Willingness to Care (‘Will no’, ‘Will yes’), Known
HIV Positive (‘Known yes’, ‘Known no’), and ‘Knowledge of HIV/AIDS’
(‘K T = least knowledge, ‘K2’, ‘K 3 \ ‘K4’ = most knowledge), and an active Variable o f Fear o f Infection (‘Fear
agree’, ‘Fear cannot say’, ‘Fear disagree’)

Axis 2 - 13 78 %
University Students’ Perception of People Living with HIV/AIDS / 543

Table 3: Relative Frequencies of Supplementary Variables and Categories (/? = 282).


Variable Category Relative frequency
Gender Man 17.4
Woman 82.6
Age -21 18.4
22-25 36.5
26-34 26.2
35- 18.8
Are you willing to care for a person with HTV/AIDS? Yes 66.7
No 33.3
Have you ever known a family member, friend or other close person Yes 8.9
with HIV/AIDS?
No 91.1
Knowledge about HTV/AIDS IK (lowest) 11.0
2K 35.8
3K 33.3
4K (highest) 19.9

knew someone with HIV. It is interesting to getting the virus if in contact with HIV posi­
note that this type of familiarity with HIV also tive people. These interpretations of Figure 3
correlates slightly with the second dimension, are supported by the squared cosines present­
which indicates that knowing a PLWHA re­ ed in Table 4.
duces the level of uncertainty in addition to In our final analysis, we show that women
the level of fear (Table 3). and older students tended to fear infection less
The association between knowledge, fear frequently than men and younger students
and uncertainty is illustrated by the spread of (see Figure 4 and Table 4). Because fear and
points of the variable - ‘knowledge of HIV/ discomfort are closely related, it also means
AIDS.’ Figure 3 clearly shows that the points that women and older students tend to feel
indicating the levels of knowledge have an more sympathy towards PLWHA. In addition,
orderly distribution in the first dimension the order of the categories in the second di­
from right to left. The more knowledgeable mension shows that there is more uncertainty
a person is, the less afraid they are of getting among the youngest students (<21), whereas
an infection from being in contact with HIV the oldest students most often have a certain
positive people. The most knowledgeable opinion concerning the fear of infection.
categories (K 4 and K 3) are on the left side We cannot analyse the interactions be­
o f the map close to the category of ‘Fear - tween supplementary variables in the SCA
disagree.’ The knowledge categories are also of stacked tables. However we performed
clearly ordered in the second dimension. The some separate cross-tabulations and found a
least knowledgeable categories of K 1 and K statistically significant relationship between
2 are located in the upper right part of the map age and gender, in which the men tended to
in the direction of ‘Fear - cannot say.’ Thus, be younger than the women. There were no
ignorance of HIV/AIDS tends to go hand in significant relations between age and the
hand with the uncertainty about the fear of level of knowledge. Instead, there was a veiy
544 / College Student Journal

significant association between age and the significant association between age and a
respondent’s familiarity with an HIV positive willingness to care for an HIV positive person
person (x2 =41,209, df= 3, p = 0.000). No- (X2 =8,129, df= 3, p = 0.043). On average, the
one in the youngest age group was closely oldest age group was more willing to care for
acquainted with a person with AIDS, but a person with H1V/AIDS than the other age
about third of the oldest age group had such groups (83% vs. 63.2%).
an experience. There was also a statistically

Table 4: Squared Cosines and Relative Weights of Supplementary Variables


Variable Category Squared Cosine Squared Cosine Relative Weight
1" dimension 2°d dimension

Gender Man 0.83 0.00 17.38


Woman 0.83 0.00 82.62
Age -21 0.12 0.24 18.44
22-25 0.69 0.02 36.52
26-34 0.00 0.00 22.58
35- 0.74 0.10 18.79
Will Yes 0.95 0.00 66.67
No 0.95 0.00 33.33
Known Yes 0.85 0.08 8.87
No 0.34 0.04 88.18
Knowledge 1 (least) 0.45 0.20 10.99
2 0.14 0.40 35.82
3 0.07 0.13 32.33
4 (most) 0.72 0.11 19.89

Figure 4: Supplementary Variables of Gender (‘Man’, ‘Woman’) and Age (‘-21’, ‘22-25’,
‘26-34’, ‘35-’), and an active Variable of Fear of Infection (‘Fear agree’, ‘Fear cannot
say’, ‘Fear disagree’)
U n ive rs ity S tu d e n ts ’ P e rc ep tio n o f P e o p le Living w ith H IV /A ID S / 5 4 5

Discussion respondents’ uncertainty about fear (and in­


We studied how students at a Finnish directly discomfort). Finally, in regard to age
university perceived different categories of and gender, men and younger students tended
PLWHA. On average, about a quarter (24.2 to fear infection more than women and older
%) of respondents would feel uncomfortable students and uncertainty was seen to decrease
if they came into a contact with PLWHA. with age.
Almost half of the students perceived an in­ Our research results demonstrate for the
travenous drug user negatively, while very first time in a statistically systematic way, how
few respondents found it uncomfortable to people’s perceptions tend to place different
be in contact with a HIV positive child (6.4 types of PLWHA into a ranking system. The
%). A prostitute and a haemorrhagic disease ranking implies a moral judgement, but we
patient were also perceived more negatively did not directly study whether the perceptions
than the overall average. A clear majority of are based on the assessment of responsibility
respondents perceived bisexual and homo­ and a fear of PLWHA, as has been suggested
sexual subjects with HIV/AIDS in positive by previous research. Our results show that
terms. Over one-fifth of students could not the situation of bisexuals and homosexuals
assess their feelings of discomfort in the case has become much improved since the 1980s,
of a haemorrhagic disease patient. although many still look down on intravenous
Discomfort was linked to the fear of ac­ drug users and prostitutes.
quiring infection. Those who feared infection HIV and AIDS arouse negative feelings in
tended to feel discomfort with PLWHAs as a general, but people tend to perceive various
whole, whereas the sympathisers did not fear types of people with HIV/AIDS differently
infection. Uncertainty about fear was closely in terms of their moral worth. We know that
associated with the uncertainty of having an HIV itself does not have any morals and that
opinion about the feelings of discomfort to­ people should be treated equally and in the
wards certain types of PLWHA. Particularly, best possible manner, regardless their mode
uncertainty concerning the unpleasantness of of infection. It is worrying however, that dis­
haemorrhagic disease patients, prostitutes, criminatory attitudes and behaviour towards
IV drug users and homosexuals was corre­ certain categories of HIV positives may also
lated with fear. Furthermore, those who had be observed in health care settings (Heijnders
difficulties to formulate their opinion about & Van Der Meij, 2006; Paxton et al., 2005;
a child or a bisexual tended to fear infection Weiss, Ramakrishna & Somma, 2006; Wong
more than average. & Wong, 2006).
Next we analysed if fear (and indirectly Although there has been progress in terms
discomfort) was associated with a willingness of improved attitudes, we should be more
to care for PLWHA, familiarity with an HIV concerned about the position of the most
positive person, and the level of knowledge vulnerable groups of PLWHA, such as in­
of HIV/AIDS. Our results show that fear (and travenous drug users, haemorrhagic disease
indirectly discomfort) reduces the willingness patients and prostitutes. Our results from stu­
to care for PLWHA. In addition, familiarity dents suggest that a better knowledge of HIV/
with PLWHA and a good knowledge o f HIV/ AIDS and familiarity with PLWHA might
AIDS reduces fear, and thus is likely to in­ reduce the fear of infection and so increase
crease the sympathy felt towards PLWHA. sympathy towards PLWHA. Direct contact
The lack of knowledge and personal expe­ with PLWHA together with some educational
rience of an HIV positive person increases programs, could decrease negative attitudes
546 / College Student Journal

and discriminative behaviors, but there is no


clear evidence that this approach produces re­
sults (Foreman & Breinbauer, 2003). Yet, it is
always important to correct false beliefs about
the disease and any unnecessary fears about
infection (Parker & Aggleton, 2003; Weiss,
Ramakrishna & Somma, 2006). Therefore we
need to arouse public sympathy towards the
sick and emphasize that the disease is just one
aspect of the person’s identity.
The final data base cannot be regarded
as representative of the population of basic
degree students, and the results should be
evaluated as preliminary. The questionnaires
were mailed to each subject’s home address,
obtained from the student register and it was
noticed that it was not possible to reach some
students at the address given. Also, the ques­
tionnaire was rather long and basic degree
students might not be used to participating in
surveys. There were also those who may not
have been interested in or did not value the
research topic and chose not to respond.

Conclusion
Although the perception of PLWHA has
improved over the years, there is still a need
for enlightenment, particularly with to the
most vulnerable groups such as intravenous
drug users, haemorrhagic disease patients and
prostitutes.
U n iversity S tu d e n ts ’ P e rc e p tio n o f P e o p le Living w ith H IV /A ID S / 5 4 7

References Musso, E., and S.E.L. Wakefield. 2009. Tales o f Mind


over Cancer: Cancer Risk and Prevention in Canadi­
Chng, C. L., and A. Moore. 1994. AIDS: Its Effects on
an Print Media, Health, Risk & Society 11(1): 17-38.
Sexual Practices among Homosexual and Heterosex­
Parker, R., and P. Aggleton. 2003. HIV and AIDS-related
ual College Students. Journal o f Health Education
Stigma and Discrimination: A Conceptual Frame­
25: 154-160.
work and Implications for Action. Social Science &
Cobb, M., and J.T. De Chabert. 2002. HIV/AIDS and
Medicine 57: 13-24.
Care Providers Attributions: Who’s to Blame. Aids
Paxton, S., and G. Gonzales, K. Uppakaew, K.K. Abra­
Care 14 (4): 545-548.
ham, S. Okta, C. Green, K.S. Nair, T. Parwati Me-
Corrigan, P., and F.E. Markowitz, A. Watson, D. Rowan,
rati, B. Thephthicn, M. Marin, A. Quesada. 2005.
M.A. Kubiak. 2003. An Attribution Model o f Public
AIDS-Related Discrimination in A sia AIDS Care 17
Discrimination Towards Persons with Mental Ill­
(4): 413-424.
ness. Journal o f Health and Social Behavior 44 (1):
Pcrsson, A. 2005. Facing HIV: Body Shape Change and
162-179.
the (in)Visibility of Illness. Medical Anthropology
Foreman, M., and L.P. Breinbauer. 2003. Understanding
24: 237-264.
and Responding to HIV/AIDS Related Stigma and
Persson, A., and C. Newman. 2008. Making Monsters:
Discrimination in the Health Sector. Washington
Heterosexuality, Crime and Race in Recent Western
DC: Pan American Health Organization.
Media Coverage o f HIV. Sociology o f Health &
Goffinan, E. 1990. Stigma: Notes on the Management o f
Illness 30 (4): 632-646.
Spoiled Identity. London: Penguin Books.
Scambler, G. 2006. Sociology, Social Structure and
UNAIDS. 2012. Global Report: UNAIDS Report on the
Health-Related Stigma Psychology, Health & Medi­
Global AIDS Epidemic 2012. Joint United Nations
cine 11 (3): 288-295.
Programme on HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS). Accessed
Small, N. 1993. “Dying in a Public Place.” In The Sociol­
August 8 2013. http://www.unaids.org/en/media/un-
ogy o f Death, edited by D. Clark, (87-111). Oxford:
aids/contentassets/documents/epidemiology/2012/
Blackwell.
gr2012/20121120 UNAIDS GIobal Report 2012
Steins, G., and B. Weiner. 1999. The Influence o f Per­
en.pdf.
ceived Responsibility and Personality Characteristics
Greenacre, M. 2007. Correspondence Analysis in Prac­
on the Emotional and Behavioral Reactions to People
tice. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall.
with AIDS. The Journal o f Social Psychology 139
Hegarty, P., and A.M. Golden. 2008. Attributional Beliefs
(4): 487-195.
about Controllability o f Stigmatized Traits: Anteced­
Weiner, B. 1995. Judgment o f Responsibility. New York:
ents or Justifications o f Prejudice? Journal o f Applied
Guilford Press.
Social Psychology 38 (4): 1023-1044.
Weiss, M. G. and J. Ramakrishna, D. Somma. 2006.
Hcinjders, M., and S. Van Der Meij. 2006. The Fight
Health-related Stigma: Re-thinking Concepts and
against Stigma: An Overview of Stigma-Reduction
Interventions. Psychology, Health & Medicine 11
Strategies and Interventions. Psychology, Health &
(3): 277-287.
Medicine 11 (3): 353-363.
Wong, V., and L. Wong. 2006. Management o f Stigma
Held, S. L. 1993. The Effects o f anAIDS Education Pro­
and Disclosure of HIV/AIDS Status in Health Care.
gram on the Knowledge and Attitudes o f a Physical
Journal o f Health Organization and Management 20
Therapy Class. Physical Therapy 73: 156-164.
(2): 95-114.
Le Roux, B., and H. Rouanet. 2010. Multiple Correspon­
dence Analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Link, B.G., and J.C. Phelan. 2001. Conceptualizing Stig­
m a Annual Review o f Sociology 27: 363-385.
Lupton, D. 1999. Archetypes o f Infection: People with
HIV/AIDS in the Australian Press in the mid 1990s.
Sociology o f Health and Illness 21 (2): 37-53.
Major, B., and L.T. O’Brian. 2005. The Social Psychol­
ogy o f Stigma. Annual Review o f Psychology 56:
393-421.
Muinonen, U., and T. Suominen, M. Valimaki, C. Lohr-
mann, I. Peate. 2002. Early Adolescents’ HIV-related
Knowledge, Attitudes, and Behavior in Finland.
International Journal o f Nursing Practice 2: 81-88.
Copyright of College Student Journal is the property of Project Innovation, Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen