Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Ideological Values and the Votes of U.S.

Supreme Court Justices


Author(s): Jeffrey A. Segal and Albert D. Cover
Source: The American Political Science Review, Vol. 83, No. 2 (Jun., 1989), pp. 557-565
Published by: American Political Science Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1962405 .
Accessed: 05/05/2014 19:31

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Political Science Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
The American Political Science Review.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
IDEOLOGICALVALUESAND THE
VOTES OF U.S. SUPREME
COURTJUSTICES
A. SEGAL
JEFFREY
ALBERTD. COVER
State Universityof New York
Stony Brook

It is commonly assumed that Supreme Court justices' votes


largelyreflecttheir attitudes, values, or personalpolicy preferences.Nevertheless,this
assumptionhas never been adequatelytested with independentmeasuresof the ideo-
logical values of justices, that is, measuresnot taken from their votes on the Court.
Using content analytic techniques,we derive independentand reliablemeasuresof the
values of all Supreme Court justices from Earl Warrento Anthony Kennedy. These
values correlatehighly with the votes of the justices, providingstrong supportfor the
attitudinalmodel.

The fundamental from the nonunanimous decisions ren-


assumption about the behavior of dered by the justicesfrom 1937 to 1947.
SupremeCourt justices-that their votes Pritchett does not provide a theory of
are strongly dependent upon their atti- SupremeCourt decision making;there is
tudes, values, or personal policy prefer- no doubt a limit to how many break-
ences-has never been adequatelytested throughsa single book can make. Never-
with independentmeasures of the atti- theless, the theoreticalassumptionsof the
tudes or values of justices.1 We use a study were made clear in the introduc-
systematiccontent analysisto derive reli- tion: 'This book, then, undertakes to
able measuresof the ideologicalvalues of study the politics and values of the Roose-
SupremeCourt justicesthat are indepen- velt Court through the nonunanimous
dent of the votes they later cast. Despite opinions handeddown by its justices"(p.
potential problemsof measurementerror xii), acknowledgingthat the justices are
in the data, our scores correlate quite "motivatedby their own preferences"(p.
highly with the votes cast by the justices xiii).
in civil libertiescases from 1953 through It is Schubert (1965), drawing most
1988. heavily on the work of Coombs (1964),
who first provides a well-delineated
theory of judicial decision making. Ac-
Attitudes, Values, and Judicial cording to Schubert, justices have ideal
Behavior points (i-points) in multidimensional,
ideological space. These points represent
The study of judicial behavior begins the justices'"own syndromeof attitudes"
with C. HermanPritchett'sTheRoosevelt (Schubert 1965, 27). Case stimuli (j-
Court(1948).The book systematicallyex- points), presentedto the justices for ap-
amines dissents, concurrences, voting proval or disapproval, can similarly be
blocs, and ideological configurations measured in the same multidimensional

AMERICAN POLITICAL SCIENCE REVIEW


VOLUME 83 NO. 2 JUNE 1989

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
American Political Science Review Vol. 83

space: 'The decision of the Court in any cases by these two justices. While this
case will depend upon whether the case work is innovative, the reliabilityof the
dominates,or is dominatedby, a majority data is unknown, and only two justices
of i-points"(p.38). are studied. Independentevidencefor the
Rohdeand Spaeth(1976)also developa attitudinal model must rest on a firmer
multidimensionalattitudinalmodel. Their foundation.
most basic assumptionis that "eachmem- Greaterefforts have been made to ex-
ber of the Courthas preferencesconcern- amine the effects of attitudeson the deci-
ing the policy questions faced by the sions of lower courtjudges,often through
Court, and when the justices make deci- the use of surveys and questionnaires.
sions they want the outcomesto approxi- Nagel (1963) found differencesbetween
mate as nearly as possible those policy liberal and conservative judges across
preferences"(p. 72). The Court'sstructure four separate issues. Unfortunately,the
grants the justicesgreat freedom"tobase presentationof the resultsin terms of the
their decisions solely upon personal pol- percentageof judgesabove or below court
icy preferencesfor a numberof reasons: averages makes it difficult to assess the
(1) the lack of electoralaccountability,(2) strength of the relationship. Gibson
the lack of ambitionfor higheroffice, and (1978)foundthat attitudesalone could ex-
(3) the fact that the SupremeCourt is the plain only 14% of the variation in the
court of last resort"(p. 72; emphasisadd- decisionsof Iowa trial court judges. Nar-
ed). dulli, FHemming,and Eisenstein (1984)
The attitudes and values describedin report a negative bivariate relationship
these works are taken from the votes cast between a judge's belief in punishment
by the justices. While there is much to and plea-bargained sentences. In their
learn from this approach-these books multivariate model the relationship re-
are rightlyplaced among the leadingones mains negative for crimes that are not
in judicial behavior-there are limits as serious, and is positive only for the most
well. One cannot demonstratethat atti- serious crimes.
tudes affect votes when the attitudes are These low or negative correlations
operationalizedfrom those same votes. should not be surprisinggiven what is
As Schubert himself recognized, 'The known from the social psychology litera-
book is full of talk about the attitudesof ture. The earliest researchin social psy-
SupremeCourt justices, but the attitudi- chology found little or no relationshipbe-
nal differencesdelineatedand denotedob- tween attitudes and behavior. Wicker
viously are hypotheticalratherthan em- (1969) concludedfrom his review of nu-
pirical constructs, because the data ana- merousstudiesthat thereis little relation-
lyzed are based on observationsof judi- ship between the two. More recently,
cial votes in the decisions of cases-and however, scholarshave found some rela-
not even on judicial responses to ques- tionshipsbetween attitudesand behavior
tionnaireitems"(1974, xii). and have shiftedtheirfocus from whether
There is only one previous effort to attitudesaffectbehaviorto the conditions
deriveindependentmeasuresof the values underwhich attitudesaffectbehavior(see
of Supreme Court justices: Danelski's McGuire1985;Petty and Cacioppo1981).
1966 examinationof JusticesBrandeisand For example, relationshipsare enhanced
Butler.Danelskicontent-analyzedspeech- when subjects' thoughts are internally
es given by the justicesprior to their ap- focused (Scheir and Carver 1980) and
pointment to the Court. Support for or when attitudesare formedas the resultof
opposition to laissez faire correlatedwith directexperience(Fazioand Zanna1981).
the directionof solo dissentsin economic Relationshipsare strongestfor those high

558

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Votes of SupremeCourt Justices

in self-esteem(Sjoberg1978)and maturity of the few independentsources of infor-


(Henschel1971). Nevertheless,even when mation that contain comparabledata on
relationshipsare found, attitudestypical- each justice. Only editorialsprior to con-
ly explainonly 10% to 40% of the varia- firmationby the Senate were examined,
tion in behavior (Petty and Cacioppo for editorials after confirmationwill un-
1981, 24-27). doubtedlybe influencedby votes a justice
Despitethese results,the freedomgiven casts and thus are not independent of
to Supreme Court justices to act upon those votes. We selected four of the na-
their ideological values (Rohde and tion's leading papers, two with a liberal
Spaeth1976)gives hope that strongcorre- stance (the New York Times and Wash-
lations can be found betweenindependent ington Post) and two with a more con-
measuresof their values and their votes servative outlook (the Chicago Tribune
on the Court. We turn now to our efforts and Los Angeles Times).
at measuringthe values of the justices, To conduct the content analysis, we
trainedthree students to code each para-
graph for political ideology. Paragraphs
The Justices'IdeologicalValues were coded as liberal,moderate,conserv-
Acquiringindependentmeasuresof the ative, or not applicable. Liberal state-
ideology of SupremeCourtjusticesis not ments include (but are not limited to)
a simple task. As Tate stated, those ascribingsupport for the rights of
defendantsin criminalcases, women and
It is difficult to imagine what independent racialminoritiesin equalitycases, and the
measuresof judicialvalues could be used. . .. individualagainst the governmentin pri-
Attitude surveys, often used to measure the
values (and role perceptions)of sitting lower vacy and First Amendment cases. Con-
courtjudges,have theirlimitationswhenmost of servativestatementsare those with an op-
the justicesbeinganalyzedare dead. An alterna- posite direction.Moderatestatementsin-
tive method,inferringvaluesfromwrittenworks cludethose that explicitlyascribemodera-
of judges(Danelski1966),mightbe applicablein
principle,but would requirea researchinvest- tion to the nomineesor those that ascribe
ment whichis well beyondthe resourcesof most both liberaland conservativevalues.2The
researchersif appliedto 25 justices,(Tate1981, justice'sideology (II)is then measuredby
365) the formula1I = (liberal- conservative)/
As part of a separatestudy we conduct- (liberal + moderate + conservative).
ed a content analysis on the ideological This formulaleads to a scalerangingfrom
values of all justicesfrom EarlWarrento +1.0 (unanimously liberal) through .0
Anthony Kennedyfrom newspaperedito- (moderate) to -1.0 (unanimously con-
rials in several of the nation's leading servative).
newspapers(Segal, Cover, and Cameron The measures,presentedin Table 1, are
1988). Although these measuresmust be reliable. We assessed reliabilityby using
used with caution, we believe that such all threecoderson a 25% randomsample
measures can be justified under certain of the data. Usingpi as our index of inter-
circumstances.First,though, we describe coder reliability(see Krippendorff1980),
how the measureswere obtained. a resultof .72 was achieved.This is signif-
We started by conducting a content icant at p < .001.
analysisof a source that containscompa- We believe that the scores accurately
rable information on each justice since measure the perceptions of the justices'
Earl Warren: statements in newspaper values at the time of their nominations.
editorials from the nomination by the While not everyone would agree that
presidentuntil the confirmationvote by every score precisely measures the per-
the Senate. Newspapereditorialsare one ceived ideology of each nominee, Fortas,

559

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
American Political Science Review Vol. 83

Table 1. Justices'Values and Votes questionnairecompleted at each justice's


appointmentare likely to happen soon.
Justice Valuesa Votesb We must turn insteadto indirectmethods
78.1
of measuringthe ideologicalvalues of jus-
Warren .50
Harlan .75 41.9 tices. Some form of content analysis is
Brennan 1.00 77.9 needed. The criteriathat led us to choose
Whittaker .00 43.4 newspaper editorials follow. First, the
Stewart .50 51.5 data must have ideological content. Sec-
White .00 43.4
.50 89.6
ond, the data must be comparable;the
Goldberg
Fortas 1.00 80.4 data must exist in similar form for each
Marshall 1.00 79.7 justice. Third, the data must be indepen-
Burger -.77 29.7 dent of the votes the justicescast; the data
Blackmun -.77 42.9 cannot be rationalizationsof votes al-
Powell -.67 37.9
Rehnquistc -.91 19.5 ready cast. Fourth, there can be no sys-
Stevens -.50 56.3 tematicerrorsin the data.
O'Connor -.17 30.9 We consideredbut rejectedseveralpos-
Rehnquistd -.91 23.0 sibilities.Codingcases decidedby the jus-
Scalia -1.00 34.7 tices while on lower courts would not
Kennedy -.27 40.0
work because many justicesdo not have
aDerivedby authors.The rangeis -1.00 (extremely lower-courtexperience.Opinions written
conservative)to 1.00 (extremelyliberal). by the justicesare clearlynot independent
bPercentageliberalin civil libertiescases, 1953-88. of votes previouslymade. Speechesmade
cValuesand votes as Nixon appointee. or articleswritten by justices,when they
dValuesand votes as Reaganappointee. have ideologicalcontent,may be rational-
izationsor defensesof decisionsprevious-
ly cast. These writings and speeches are
Marshall, and Brennanare, expectedly, independentonly of votes not yet cast.
the most liberal, while Scalia and Rehn- Preappointmentarticles and speechesby
quist are the most conservative. Harlan justices would be appropriatefor analy-
and Stewartcome out liberalbecausethe sis, but too many justiceshave no articles
debate about them centeredaround their or speeches published prior to selection
support for the overriding issue of the for the Court. Further,while some jus-
day, desegregation.Goldbergis not per- tices, such as Douglas (1970), write and
ceived to be as liberal as Fortasor Mar- speak on ideologically charged subjects,
shall because of an even-handednessat others-for example,Scalia(1976)-write
the Departmentof Labor that even the on topicssuchas oversightof agencydeci-
Chicago Tribunecould support. O'Con- sion making.
nor comes out a moderate,given her pre- Senate confirmation hearings often
vious support for women's rights and have ideological content and contain
abortion. Indeed,the only hint of opposi- comparable data for the overwhelming
tion to her came from the Right.3 majority of justices. However, there is
Measures of perceived values are ob- likely to be systematicbias in such data.
viously imperfect measures of the real When a nomination is noncontroversial,
values of Supreme Court justices. In a we are likely to get little or no ideological
world more attuned to the needs of re- information from the nominee's state-
searchers,SupremeCourt justiceswould ments. O'Connor,for example,thoughtit
annuallycompleteattitudequestionnaires in her best interestto refuseto answervir-
on each issue the Court would consider tually all policy questions.Whena nomi-
duringthe year. Neither this nor a single nee does agree to addresssuch questions,

560

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Votes of SupremeCourt Justices

it is usually done in an attemptto satisfy The DependentVariable


senatorsthat the nomineeis not as liberal
or conservative as commonly believed. Because the statements in newspaper
Thus, if we were to code Bork's state- editorials deal almost exclusively with
ments before the Senate JudiciaryCom- support by the justices for civil liberties
mittee, we would find Bork consistently and civil rights, we use as our dependent
statinghis supportfor women'srightsand variablethe votes of all justicesappointed
FirstAmendmentrights,preciselybecause since the beginningof the WarrenCourt
senators had reasons to believe he op- in all formallydecidedcivil libertiescases
posed them. Therefore, the data from from the beginning of the 1953 term
confirmation hearings may well be bi- throughthe end of the 1987 term, as de-
ased. rived from the SupremeCourt data base
Newspaper editorials on nominations compiled by Harold Spaeth at Michigan
to the SupremeCourt exist on all nomi- StateUniversity.4Civil libertiesissues are
nees since Earl Warren, typically have those involving criminalprocedure,civil
ideologicalcontent, and are obviously in- rights, the FirstAmendment,due process,
dependentof the votes justiceslater cast. and privacy. Liberaldecisionsare (1)pro-
While editorialspresumablyare quite ac- person accusedor convictedof crime, (2)
curatein conveying preconfirmationper- pro-civil libertiesor civil rightsclaimant,
ceptions of the ideology of nominees, (3) proindigent, (4) pro-Indian, and (5)
these perceptionsare not necessarilyac- antigovernmentin due process and pri-
curate.Forexample,our measuressuggest vacy. The data are presentedin Table 1.
that Harlanis a liberaland O'Connoris a
moderate.Few would arguethat this rep- Results
resents their true values. What effects
would such errorshave on our analysis? The results are straightforward:the
First, we argue that such errorsare ran- correlationbetweenthe ideologicalvalues
dom. Prior to the editorials'publication, of the justicesand their votes in civil lib-
there is no reason to believe that any erty cases is .80 (r2 = .64, adjustedr2 =
nominee will be systematicallymisrepre- .62). Regressingvotes on our measureof
sented in one directionor another. Com- values yields a constant of 51.25 and a
pare this to statements from judiciary slope of 23.44 (t = 5.34). The residuals
committeehearings,where the errorsare are graphedin Figure1. The largestresid-
systematic-liberals under intense scru- uals belong to Harlan, who is 27.3 per-
tiny try to portray themselvesas less lib- centagepoints more conservativethanex-
eral, conservativesare less conservative. pected, and Goldberg, who is 26.4 per-
Second, to the extent that we have ran- centagepoints moreliberalthanexpected.
dom measurement error, we will un- Brennan,Marshall, Burger,Powell, and
doubtedly find weaker correlationsthan Kennedyare within 5 points of predicted
would otherwise be the case (Berryand values; Whitaker, White, Fortas, and
Feldman1985). Therefore,the correlation Rehnquist(R1)are within 10. Also within
betweenideologicalvalues and votes that 10 points are Rehnquist(R2), Scalia, and
we presentis lower than the true correla- Kennedy,who fit well despitethe relative-
tion. Undersuch circumstances,if we find ly few votes they have cast. Regression
low correlations, we cannot state that diagnostics demonstratethat the results
values are unrelatedto votes. If high cor- are remarkablystable; they are not de-
relations are found, we will have found pendent on the scores of any particular
the minimallevel existingin the truerela- justice. Systematicallyexcludingone jus-
tionship. tice at a time results in correlations

561

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
American Political Science Review Vol. 83

Figure1. Civil LibertySupport acceptedby the Court majority;they are


by Ideology free to use whatever doctrines fit their
own preferences.Precedentsare typically
100~~~~~~~0 found on both sides of any case reaching
_80- |OR30 FO the SupremeCourt;and even if the prec-
D 70- edents weigh heavily on one side, justices
60N
50-
are free to distinguishor overrule them.
50- OW 01W While precedentmight have some value
-40LHR
~~40 OUSE/KEA~Ej^
*B0L -- 038 0 for some justices, the empiricalevidence
on the importanceof precedentconsistsof
::30 4Su 00C
0~~~~~8 little more than Schubert's(1963)exposi-
~"10- tion of the votes of JusticeClarkin courts-
i10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 1
martial of civilian personnel and depen-
Predicted Support dents. Gibson (1978)demonstratesthe im-
portanceof roles-particularly as to judi-
Note: WA Warren, HA - Harlan, BR - Bren- cial restraint-to Iowa judges, but
nan, WH Whittaker, SW - Stewart, BW -
White, GO Goldberg, FO - Fortas, MA - Mar-
SupremeCourt justicesneed not feel any
shall, BU Burger, BL - Blackmun, PO - Powell, more constrained by judicial restraint
R1 - Rehnquist (1971), SV - Stevens, OC than they do by precedent. Evidenceon
O'Connor, R2 - Rehnquist (1986), SC - Scalia, KE the Court establishes that judicial re-
- Kennedy, straintis little more than a "cloakfor the
justices'policy preferences"(Spaeth and
Teger 1982, 277; see also Spaeth 1964).
rangingfrom .77 (JusticeMarshallexclud- Casefacts obviously influenceSupreme
ed) to .86 (JusticeHarlanexcluded).The Court decisions (Segal 1984, 1986), but
correlation is unchanged if Justice Ken- facts can only explain variance between
nedy's 40 votes are excluded. Given the cases of a particularCourtor betweenthe
limits on the relationship between atti- decisionsof a particularjustice;they can-
tudes and values, and the fact that our not explain differences in decisions be-
correlationis attenuatedby the measure- tween justices.That is, fact models try to
ment errorthat no doubt exists in the in- explain why JusticeA votes liberally in
dependent variable, the results provide one case and conservatively in another;
exceptional support for the attitudinal attitudinalmodels try to explainwhy Jus-
model as applied to civil liberties cases. tice A is moderateover a series of cases
Forthe firsttime supportis frommeasures while JusticeB is liberal. Attitudes must
of values independentof the votes of the have a referent.In the attitudinalmodel
justices.5 of judicial behavior that referent is the
We observe a correlation of .80 be- case stimuli, or in Schubert's term,
tween values and votes. If the true corre- j-points. There is no contradiction be-
lation is significantly higher than that, tween the models. They necessarilydiffer
does this mean that virtuallynothing else only becausethe units of analysis differ.
directly affects the aggregatedecisionsof The attitudinalmodel also fails to con-
justices?6Traditionalmodes of analyzing sider internal and external influenceson
judicial decisions emphasize the impor- the Court. WalterMurphy'sseminal Ele-
tance of legal doctrine and precedent. ments of Judicial Strategy (1964) docu-
This is not the place for a complete ments innumerablemethods for success-
defenseof legal realism,but we do briefly fully "marshalingthe Court"from within.
note the following. Supreme Court jus- Externalinfluencesare also likely to exist.
tices are not bound by the legal doctrines The solicitor general is remarkablysuc-

562

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Votes of SupremeCourt Justices

cessful at the certioraristage (Tanenhaus tion is as a moderate by Southern standards on race,


et al. 1963)and when cases are decidedon as a man with deep and humane concern for his
fellowman" (Los Angeles Times, 24 October 1971).
the merits (Puro 1971, 1981; Scigliano Conservative: "Judge Carswell himself admits to
1971; Segal1988). This influenceexists, at some amazement now at what he said in that 1948
least in sex discriminationcases, even speech. He should, for his were the words of pure
after the facts of the cases have been con- and simple racism" (Washington Post, 10 February
1970) and "He [Nixon] has strongly supported Rehn-
trolled (Segal and Reedy 1988). So while quist's articulation within the Justice Department of
justicesmay have a freereignto vote their broad authority for surveillance and wiretapping in
personal policy preferences, there are the pursuit of criminals, and in the qualified form of
forces that limit this discretion.Nonethe- martial law used during the May-Day demonstra-
less, the ability of the attitudinalmodel to tions" (Los Angeles Times, 5 December 1971).
3. Rehnquist is included twice, once as associate
explain the justices'voting behaviorindi- justice and once as chief justice. While his ideologi-
catesthat theseinfluencesareminimal.As cal scores are the same both times, the scores are
better measuresof the values of the jus- separate measures, taken from 1972 and 1986,
tices are developed across multidimen- respectively. That they are identical down to two
decimal places is partially a coincidence, but mostly
sional space, the limitationsof alternative a result of the fact that he was viewed as a strong
explanationsof the justices'aggregatebe- conservative both times.
havior should become ever more ap- 4. The paucity of editorials prior to Warren pre-
parent. vents us from beginning with earlier justices. Since
the data base begins with the appointment of Earl
Warren, we have the complete voting record on all
included justices.
Notes 5. Or one could suggest the following alternative
scenario: If judges base decisions on legal values
There are many people to thank for this article. (e.g., precedent, intent of the framers, etc.), not per-
Jeffrey Segal would like to thank Lee Epstein, sonal values, editorials on nominees with lower
Michael Giles, and Thomas Walker, whose conver- court experience would be based on those legal
sations on public law helped provide the initial im- values. Our measures will correlate with their latter
petus to write the manuscript. The final draft was votes on the Court, because as justices they will
prepared while Jeffrey Segal was a fellow at the Law again be basing their votes on their legal values. But
and Social Science Program, Northwestern Univer- if this were the case, lower court experience would
sity. The authors would also like to thank Renee Ad- provide crucial information that does not exist for
war, Yen Giang, and Regina Stephanie Good, who those without such experience: the legal values of the
coded the data used in the analysis, and Harold nominees. The correlation for those with that infor-
Spaeth, who provided us with a preliminary version mation should be higher than the correlation for
of the Supreme Court data base. The project is sup- those without that information. Though the n's are
ported by NSF Grant SES-8812935. small, the correlation between values and votes for
1. We recognize the existence and importance of those with judicial experience is .76 (adjusted r2 =
the Danelski 1966 study, and discuss it in further .53), while the correlation for those without such ex-
detail later. Nevertheless, as his study examined perience is .92 (adjusted r2 = .81). While this is not
but two justices, we cannot consider it a test of the sufficient to claim that lower court experience pro-
attitudinal model. vides disinformation (one might reasonably argue
2. We provide here a few examples of the types of that lower court judges are bound by stare decisis,
statements that would lead to a paragraph being while Supreme Court justices are free to follow their
coded liberal, moderate, or conservative. policy preferences), it is clear that lower court serv-
Liberal: "Scarcely more defensible were the ice provides no special evidence about how justices
numerous questions about Judge Harlan's affiliation will vote, contrary to the implications of legalistic
with the Atlantic Union. The country would have a explanations.
sorry judiciary indeed, if appointees were to be 6. The findings presented here do not foreclose the
barred for belonging to progressive and respectable possibility of indirect effects on the justices' deci-
organizations" (Washington Post, 2 March 1955) sions, such as personal attributes (see Tate 1981),
and "During the McCarthy era he [Fortas] was not which may influence decisions by affecting the
afraid to raise a strong voice against those who were justices' values. Our data enable testing of the first
bent upon destroying civil liberties under the banner link of the model (attributes affect values) and our
of anticommunism" (Washington Post, 30 July results demonstrate the validity of the second link
1965). Moderate: "On the contrary, Powell's reputa- (values affect votes).

563

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
American Political Science Review Vol. 83

References In The Self in Social Psychology, ed. Daniel M.


Wegner and R. R. Vallacher. New York: Oxford.
Schubert, Glendon. 1963. "Civilian Control and
Berry, William D., and Stanley Feldman. 1985. Stare Decisis." In Judicial Decision-Making, ed.
Multiple Regression in Practice. Beverly Hills: Glendon Schubert. New York: Macmillan.
Sage. Schubert, Glendon. 1965. The Judicial Mind: The
Coombs, Clyde Hamilton. 1964. A Theory of Data, Attitudes and Ideologies of Supreme Court Jus-
New York: Wiley. tices, 1946-1963. Evanston: Northwestern Uni-
Danelski, David. 1966. "ValuesAs Variables in Judi- versity Press.
cial Decision-Making: Notes toward a Theory." Schubert, Glendon. 1974. The Judicial Mind Revis-
Vanderbilt Law Review 19:721-40. ited: A Psychometric Analysis of Supreme Court
Douglas, William 0. 1970. Points of Rebellion. New Ideology. New York: Free Press.
York: Vintage. Scigliano, Robert. 1971. The Supreme Court and the
Fazio, R. H., and M. P. Zanna. 1981. "Direct Experi- Presidency. New York: Free Press.
ence and Attitude-Behavior Consistence." In Ad- Segal, Jeffrey A. 1984. "Predicting Supreme Court
vances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. Decisions Probabilistically: The Search and
14, ed. Leonard Berkowitz. New York: Seizure Cases (1962-1981)." American Political
Academic. Science Review 78:891-900.
Gibson, James L. 1978. "Judges'Role Orientations, Segal, Jeffrey A. 1986. "Supreme Court Justices As
Attitudes, and Decisions: An Interactive Model." Human Decision Makers: An Individual Level
American Political Science Review 72:911-24. Analysis of the Search and Seizure Cases." Jour-
Henschel, A. M. 1971. 'The Relationship between nal of Politics 48:938-55.
Values and Behavior: A Developmental Hypoth- Segal, Jeffrey A. 1988. "Amicus Curiae Briefs by the
esis." Child Development 42:1997-2007. Solicitor General during the Warren and Burger
Krippendorff, Klaus. 1980. Content Analysis. Courts." Western Political Quarterly 41:135-44.
Beverly Hills: Sage. Segal, Jeffrey A., Albert D. Cover, and Charles
McGuire, William J. 1985. "Attitudes and Attitude Cameron. 1988. 'The Puzzle of Roll Call Voting
Change." In Handbook of Social Psychology, on Supreme Court Nominees." Presented at the
vol. 2, ed. Gardner Lindzey and Elliot Aronson, annual meeting of the Southern Political Science
New York: Random House. Association, Atlanta.
Murphy, Walter F. 1964. Elements of Judicial Strat- Segal, Jeffrey A., and Cheryl D. Reedy. 1988. 'he
egy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Supreme Court and Sex Discrimination: The
Nagel, Stuart. 1963. "Off-the-bench Judicial Atti- Role of the Solicitor General." Western Political
tudes." In Judicial Decision-Making, ed. Glendon Quarterly 41:553-68.
Schubert. New York: Free Press. Sjoberg, Lars. 1978. "Beliefsand Values As Attitude
Nardulli, Peter F., Roy B. Flemming, and James Components." Presented at the International
Eisenstein. 1984. "Unraveling the Complexities of Symposium on Social Psychophysics, Mann-
Decision Making in Face-to-Face Groups: A heiem.
Contextual Analysis of Plea-Bargained Sen- Spaeth, Harold. 1964. 'The Judicial Restraint of Mr.
tences." American Political Science Review Justice Frankfurter: Myth or Reality?" Midwest
78:912-28. Journal of Political Science 5:165-80.
Petty, Richard, and John T. Cacioppo. 1981. Atti- Spaeth, Harold, and Stuart Teger. 1982. "Activism
tudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary and Restraint: A Cloak for the Justices' Policy
Approaches. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Preferences." In Supreme Court Activism and
Pritchett, Charles Herman. 1948. The Roosevelt Restraint, ed. Stephen C. Halpern and Clark M,
Court. New York: Macmillan. Lamb. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Puro, Steven. 1971. 'The Role of the Amicus Curiae Tanenhaus, Joseph, Marvin Schick, Matthew
in the United States Supreme Court." Ph.D. diss. Muraskin, and Daniel Rosen. 1963. 'The
State University of New York, Buffalo. Supreme Court's Certiorari Jurisdiction: Cue
Puro, Steven. 1981. 'The United States as Amicus Theory." In Judicial Decision-Making, ed. Glen-
Curiae." In Courts, Law, and Judicial Processes, don Schubert. New York: Macmillan.
ed. S. Sidney Ulmer. New York: Free Press. Tate, C. Neal. 1981. "Personal Attribute Models of
Rohde, David, and Harold Spaeth. 1976. Supreme Voting Behavior of U.S. Supreme Court Justices:
Court Decision Making. San Francisco; W. H, Liberalism in Civil Liberties and Economics Deci-
Freeman. sions, 1946-1978." American Political Science
Scalia, Antonin. 1976. "Oversight and Review of Review 75:355-67.
Agency Decision Making." Administrative Law Wicker, A. W. 1969. "Attitudes versus Actions: The
Review 28:569-742. Relationship of Verbal and Overt Behavioral Re-
Scheir, M. F., and C. S. Carver. 1980. "Individual sponses to Attitude Objects," Journal of Social
Differences in Self-concept and Self-processes," Issues 25:41-78,

564

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Votes of SupremeCourt Justices

JeffreyA. Segal and Albert D. Cover are Associate Professorsof Political Science,
State Universityof New York, Stony Brook, NY 11794.

565

This content downloaded from 163.117.159.73 on Mon, 5 May 2014 19:31:19 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen