Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Texting/iPod dependence, executive function and sleep quality


in college students
F.R. Ferraro a,⇑, B. Holfeld b, S. Frankl a, N. Frye a, N. Halvorson a
a
University of North Dakota, United States
b
University of Victoria, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Participants were classified as ‘‘good’’ sleepers (n = 70) or ‘‘poor’’ sleepers (n = 66) based on the Pittsburgh
Available online 9 March 2015 Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysee, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989). Differences between good
and poor sleepers were examined on executive function, texting and iPod dependence, depression, and
Keywords: anxiety. Poor sleepers reported higher levels of depression, higher levels of state and trait anxiety, greater
Texting texting dependence and iPod dependence, and poorer executive function performance, as compared to
iPod good sleepers. Poor sleep quality among college students may contribute to a range of negative psycho-
Psychological health
logical, neuropsychological, and social consequences.
Executive function
Sleep quality
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Texting/iPod dependence, executive function and sleep 1.1. Texting and iPod dependence
quality in college students
A shortened version of the self-perception of text messaging
Poor sleep quality is a common problem among college stu- dependency scale (SPTMDS; Igarashi, Motoyoshi, Takai, &
dents (Byrd et al., 2014; Gellis, Park, Stotsky & Taylot, 2015; Yoshida, 2008) has been used frequently to assess individual’s tex-
Shochat, Cohen-Zion, & Tzischinsky, 2014; White, Buboltz, & ting dependency. The instrument includes four subscales that
Igou, 2011) and has been linked to a host of negative outcomes assess sensitive responses to text messages, self-perception about
including poor psychological health (e.g., anxiety, depression) compulsive usage of text messaging, fear of disrupting relation-
and decreased academic performance (Do, Shin, Bautista, & Foo, ships without text messaging, and psychological/behavioral symp-
2013; Lund, Reider, Whiting, & Prichard, 2010; Telzer, Fuligni, toms concerning the heavy usage of text messaging. Using this
Lieberman, & Galvan, 2013). However, the impact of sleep quality scale with a sample of Japanese high school students, Igarashi
on technology use among college students is not completely et al. (2008) found that texting self-perception and texting depen-
understood and is necessary as technology continues to develop dence was associated with compulsive use of texting, inability to
and become a larger part of our society. For example, text messag- control texting behavior, and neglect and isolation from friends
ing is one of the most popular methods for college students to stay who do not immediately respond to their text messages. In turn,
connected with their friends at any time of the day or night this may lead to increases in anxiety about being ostracized.
(Derbyshire et al., 2013). As the average number of text messages More recently, Ferraro et al. (2012) found that higher scores on
that are sent and received increases, along with increases in cell each of the four subscales of the SPTMDS were associated with
phone usage, concern from medical and psychological profession- higher levels of anxiety (e.g., state and trait) and depression among
als is also growing (Sansone & Sansone, 2013). Accumulating 204 college students. These researchers also found a negative asso-
research suggests that greater dependence on texting and other ciation between SPTMDS scores and a global measure of executive
forms of technology (e.g., iPod usage) can negatively impact college function, the Executive Function Index (EFI; Spinella, 2005).
students’ behavior and psychological functioning (Ferraro, Ferraro and colleagues concluded that the increased dependence
Wunderlich, Wyrobek, & Weivoda, 2014; Ferraro et al., 2012; on texting appeared to disrupt basic emotional, behavioral, and
Sansone & Sansone, 2013). psychological processes. However, the impact of this dependency
on sleep quality is unclear and may shed further light on the nega-
⇑ Corresponding author at: Dept. Psychology, Univ. North Dakota, Grand Forks, tive consequences associated with texting dependency. It is also
ND 58202, United States. Tel.: +1 701 777 2414 (O); fax: +1 701 777 3454. possible that increases in texting and the need to stay connected
E-mail address: f.richard.ferraro@email.und.edu (F.R. Ferraro). with friends may also contribute to sleep deficiencies.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.043
0747-5632/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
F.R. Ferraro et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49 45

The usage of iPods have also grown dramatically over the past background information questionnaire that asked general demo-
decade such that college students now listen to their iPod more graphic information (e.g., education level, self-rated health,
than traditional radio (Ferguson, Greer, & Reardon, 2007). Like tex- medications taken, etc.) followed by a packet of questionnaires
ting, it is believed that iPod usage can result in a similar form of that included the measures described below. The entire experi-
dependence and may relate to problems in hearing (Danhauer ment lasted 60 min and all participants were thoroughly debriefed
et al., 2009). However, little attention has been given to examine as to the purpose of the experiment upon completion. The study
the potential negative effects associated with iPod dependence. was approved by the University’s Institutional Review Board (IRB).
One exception is a study by Ferraro et al. (2014) who used a mod-
ified version of the SPTMDS (i.e., iPod usage substituted for text
2.2. Measures
messaging) and found that iPod dependence was not only common
in college students but was positively associated with texting
2.2.1. Sleep quality
dependence. Moreover, the researchers found that higher levels
Participants’ self-reported quality and patterns of sleep was
of texting and iPod dependence were related to higher levels of
assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysee,
anxiety and depression. Since texting and iPod dependence are
Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989). Consistent with past
related, it may also be the case that some aspects of executive
research (Carney, Edinger, Meyer, Lindman, & Istre, 2006; Clegg-
function may be negatively impacted by increases in iPod
Kraynok et al., 2011) participants were classified as either ‘‘good’’
dependence.
or ‘‘poor’’ sleepers based on their responses to seven domains of
sleep behaviors including sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep dura-
1.2. Technology and sleep quality tion, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep
medication, and daytime sleep over the past month. Scores in each
These are important issues because technological advances are domain ranged from 0 (not during the past month) to 3 (three or
occurring more rapidly in our society and are having more negative more times per week) with higher scores reflecting greater distur-
impacts in the social lives of individuals, especially as it relates to bances in sleep quality (a = .83). Responses to each item were
individual’s everyday functioning including their sleep quality. For summed with scores of 0–5 representing ‘‘good’’ sleepers (n = 70)
instance, shorter overall sleep duration and excessive Internet and scores of 6 or more representing ‘‘poor’’ sleepers (n = 66).
usage has been shown to dramatically (and negatively) impact
measures of health (e.g., depression, suicidal ideation, obesity),
suggesting a link between poor sleep quality and psychological 2.2.2. Texting dependence
functioning (Do et al., 2013). Furthermore, shorter sleep duration Participants’ self-reported perceptions of their text messaging
has been referred to as a form of social jet lag, resulting in poor behavior were assessed using the self-report Self-Perceptions of
sleep quality, fatigue, poor academic achievement and many Text Messaging Dependency Scale (SPTMDS; Igarashi et al.,
behavioral and social problems (Touitou, 2013). It is also important 2008). The SPTMDS includes four subscales with five-items each
to realize that while technology (in the form of social media, that assessed particular aspects of text messaging behavior.
texting, etc.) can hinder behavior, it can also be used to reduce Emotional Reaction (e.g., ‘‘I feel disappointed if I don’t get a reply
sleepiness and improve performance (Teixeira et al., 2013). to my text message immediately’’), Excessive Use (e.g., ‘‘I spend
many hours on text messages’’), and Disruption of Relationships
(e.g., ‘‘I can’t form any new relationships without using text mes-
1.3. The current study
sages’’) that were rated on a Likert-type scale that ranged from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The final subscale,
The current study extends past research with a more compre-
Psychological/Behavior Symptoms was related to heavy texting
hensive examination of the impact of poor sleep quality on college
usage (e.g., ‘‘I sometimes worry that life would be boring and emp-
students’ behavior and psychological functioning. We know that
ty without text messages’’) and was rated on a Likert-type scale
texting and iPod usage is high among college students and is
that ranged from 1 (not true at all) to 5 (extremely true). Higher
negatively related to psychological (e.g., anxiety and depression)
scores on each subscale represented higher levels of texting depen-
and executive functioning. How texting and iPod dependence are
dency (a range .70 to .85).
impacted by sleep quality has not been empirically examined,
although specific predictions can be made based on past research.
Specifically, if texting and/or iPod dependence is high, and that 2.2.3. iPod dependence
continues to negatively tax executive function, one would expect A modified self-report iPod version of the SPTMDS (Igarashi
a difference between ‘‘good’’ and ‘‘poor’’ sleepers (as defined by et al. (2008) was used to assess participants’ level of iPod usage.
scores on the PSQI), such that good sleepers would outperform The same four subscales of the SPTMDS with five-items each and
poor sleepers across a number of psychological domains. That is, equivalent response options were used. The only difference was
good sleepers were expected to reported lower levels of depression that the ‘‘iPod’’ was substituted with ‘‘text messages’’ for each item.
and state/trait anxiety, higher levels of executive function, and Higher scores on each subscale reflected greater iPod dependency
report lower dependence on texting and iPod usage compared with (a = .79)
poor sleepers.
2.2.4. Depression
2. Method Similar to past research among college students, participants’
overall level of depression was examined using the self-report
2.1. Participants and procedure Geriatric Depression Scale-Short Form (Holfeld, Cicha, & Ferraro,
2014). The GDS-SF contains 15 dichotomous items that asked par-
Participants included 136 college students (Mage = 19.0 years of ticipants about their feelings over the past week (e.g., ‘‘Are you in
age, SD = 1.08, range = 18–21; n = 55 males) from a Midwestern good spirits most of the time?’’). ‘‘Yes’’ responses were summed to
University who participated in the study in exchange for extra create an overall level of mood for each participant in which higher
credit in their psychology classes. All participants first read and scores represented higher levels of depressive symptoms. Score of
signed an informed consent form. Next, participants were given a 5 of higher were indicative of probable depression.
46 F.R. Ferraro et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49

2.2.5. State/trait anxiety PSQI correlated positively with both depression and anxiety, as
Participants’ self-reported level of state/trait anxiety was well as with many of the iPod, texting and executive function
assessed using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; variables.
Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1983). Considerable evidence Participants were classified as ‘‘good’’ and ‘‘poor’’ sleepers based
attests to the construct and concurrent validity of the scale on their PSQI score. A total of 70 participants scored between 0 and
(Spielberger et al., 1983). The State scale was comprised of 20- 5 on the PSQI and were categorized as ‘‘good’’ sleepers (M = 3.73,
items that asked respondents about their current emotional state SD = 1.12). Alternatively, 66 participants scored between 6 and
(e.g., ‘‘How are you feeling right now?’’). Participants’ rated each 16 on the PSQI and were categorized as ‘‘poor’’ sleepers
item on a scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much (M = 7.98, SD = 2.33). The difference between ‘‘good’’ and ‘‘poor’’
so). The Trait scale contained 20 items that asked respondents sleepers regarding their PSQI scores was significant,
about their typical emotional state (e.g., ‘‘How do you generally F(1, 134) = 187.92, p < .001.
feel?’’). Participants’ rated each item on a scale that ranged from
1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). Higher scores indicate higher
3.2. Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA)
levels of state and/or trait anxiety (a = .86 for state, a = .95 for
Trait).
MANOVAs were used to examine differences between good and
poor sleepers on their behavior and psychological functioning (see
2.2.6. Executive function Table 2). Our 23 measures listed in Table 1 were grouped into 5
Participants’ self-reported level of executive function was categories which included demographic (Self-Rated Health, #
assessed using the 27-item Executive Function Index (EFI; medications, vocabulary, processing speed), psychological (depres-
Spinella, 2005).The EFI has demonstrated good internal consisten- sion, state and trait anxiety), iPod (iPod 1–4, iPod Total), texting
cy (a = .82) and good convergent validity with other self-report (Text 1–4; Text Total) and executive function (EFI 1–5, EFI Total)
measures of executive function such as the Frontal Systems to reduce the total number of comparisons to be made. Similar
Behavior Scale (FrSBe), the Barrat Impulsiveness Scale (BIS), and constructs were grouped together and were validated by the corre-
the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). In addition to measuring lation estimates between items within each grouping. A Bonferroni
participant’s overall level of executive function, the EFI contained corrections table was consulted for calculations of the critical F val-
5 subscales that were used to assess specific aspects of par- ue using 130 degrees of freedom and 3, 4, 5, and 6 simultaneous
ticipants’ executive function: motivational drive (four-items), comparisons.
impulse control (five-items), empathy (six-items), organization Good and poor sleepers did not differ on any of the four demo-
(five-items), and strategic planning (seven-items). Participants graphic measures (critical F value = 6.42 for 4 comparisons). For
were asked to rate each item in the EFI on a scale from 1 (not at the three psychological measures (critical F value = 5.88 for 3 com-
all) to 5 (very much so). After reverse scoring the negatively worded parisons), poor sleepers reported higher levels of depression, and
items, an overall level of executive function was created by sum- higher levels of state and trait anxiety than good sleepers. For
ming up the responses for each item so that higher scores repre- the iPod dependence scale (critical F value = 6.83 for 5 compar-
sented higher overall levels of executive function. Similarly, isons), poor sleepers reported higher iPod dependence scores on
higher scores on each of the EFI subscales represented higher levels all four iPod subscales (iPod 1–4) and a higher iPod dependence
of executive function on each respective scale (a range = .55 to .74). total score as compared to the good sleepers. Good and poor sleep-
ers differed significantly on iPod Total score. Poor sleepers reported
2.2.7. Vocabulary higher texting dependence scores on all four texting dependence
Participants’ knowledge and comprehension was assessed using subscales and reported a higher texting dependence total score
35 vocabulary items taken from the vocabulary subtest of the (critical F value = 6.83 for 5 comparisons). Good and poor sleepers
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, differed significantly on texting total score. Finally, poor sleepers
1981). Each item was scored on a range from 0 (i.e., incorrect), 1 reported lower scores on all 5 EFI subscales and also reported a
(i.e., demonstrated partial knowledge of the item), and 2 (i.e., correct) lower EFI total score (critical F value = 7.18 for 6 comparisons), as
based on the accuracy of the response (Wechsler, 1981). Since the compared to good sleepers. Poor sleepers reported poorer EFI per-
vocabulary subtest is strongly associated with overall intelligence, formance on the EFI-4 subscale and the EFI Total score, as com-
higher scores on the subscale reflected higher levels of intelligence pared to good sleepers.
(Thompson, 1987).

4. Discussion
2.2.8. Speed of processing
The Crossing Off test (Botwinick & Storandt, 1973) was used to The present study investigated the role of sleep quality in col-
assess participants’ processing speed. Participants were given a lege students’ behavior and psychological functioning.
sheet of paper (8.5  11 in.) with 96 horizontal lines (12 rows of Participants were categorized as good or poor sleepers using the
8). They were told to make a vertical or diagonal mark across each PSQI and compared across a range of demographic, psychological,
horizontal line as fast as possible and work from left to right one and technology related factors. Overall, poor sleepers reported
row after another. This test was scored by counting up the number decreased levels of functioning including higher depression and
of lines crossed per second. A maximum of 3 min were given to anxiety (state and trait) scores, greater overall texting and iPod
complete the task. The more lines crossed out, the faster the dependence, and poorer overall executive functioning compared
response time. to good sleepers.
The findings highlight the negative impact of poor sleep quality
3. Results among college students and are consistent with recent studies that
demonstrated negative behavioral and psychological consequences
3.1. Descriptive Statistics of poor sleep quality among adolescents and college students
(Lund et al., 2010; Shocat et al., 2014; Telzer et al., 2013). It also
Table 1 provides the correlations between all study variables. extends the work of Ferraro et al. (2012) and Ferraro et al. (2014)
Age and gender did not correlate with many of the study variables. by showing that poor sleep quality is associated with greater
F.R. Ferraro et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49 47

Table 1
Correlations.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1 Age – .01 .01 .05 .02 .27 .07 .14 .12 .01 .01 .12 .06 .04
2 Gender – – .22 .01 .17 .06 .04 .04 .09 .04 .02 .03 .08 .03
3 SRH – – – .11 .06 .06 .18 .13 .16 .01 .02 .03 .01 .01
4 #Meds – – – – .05 .01 .08 .15 .05 .03 .10 .05 .08 .08
5 Intelligence – – – – – .04 .11 .11 .02 .06 .01 .03 .03 .00
6 Processing Speed – – – – – – .03 .17 .14 .12 .02 .01 .03 .04
7 Depression – – – – – – – .49 .72 .18 .09 .04 .10 .12
8 State – – – – – – – – .67 .17 .13 .03 .18 .17
9 Trait – – – – – – – – – .29 .16 .11 .26 .27
10 iPod 1 – – – – – – – – – – .58 .37 .61 .85
11 iPod 2 – – – – – – – – – – – .34 .56 .83
12 iPod 3 – – – – – – – – – – – – .41 .61
13 iPod 4 – – – – – – – – – – – – – .83
14 iPod Total – – – – – – – – – – – – – –
15 Text 1 .05 .18 .02 .08 .08 .07 .23 .31 .35 .26 .16 .09 .36 .29
16 Text 2 .04 .29 .05 .11 .16 .07 .01 .15 .12 .01 .16 .19 .07 .04
17 Text 3 .18 .07 .00 .14 .10 .04 .12 .27 .24 .21 .24 .27 .31 .32
18 Text 4 .12 .13 .01 .06 .13 .04 .12 .22 .24 .11 .06 .19 .21 .17
19 Text Total .16 .15 .00 .12 .18 .05 .18 .35 .35 .14 .23 .12 .34 .27
20 EFI 1 .00 .11 .21 .17 .13 .01 .55 .34 .49 .11 .01 .11 .11 .09
21 EFI 2 .08 .13 .03 .19 .01 .06 .16 .17 .16 .13 .18 .12 .18 .19
22 EFI 3 .05 .16 .18 .12 .02 .01 .10 .14 .11 .04 .06 .18 .13 .11
23 EFI 4 .11 .13 .16 .07 .09 .07 .32 .18 .29 .27 .14 .15 .24 .26
24 EFI 5 .06 .06 .20 .12 .05 .01 .28 .24 .27 .24 .10 .11 .24 .22
25 EFIT .08 .14 .19 .18 .04 .01 .40 .32 .39 .25 .15 .21 .25 .27
26 PSQI .03 .10 .21 .10 .05 .08 .37 .32 .42 .24 .17 .07 .07 .18
15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26
15 Text 1 – .36 .19 .29 .68 .13 .15 .12 .10 .20 .20 .05
16 Text 2 – – .17 .25 .68 .07 .21 .02 .01 .18 .13 .12
17 Text 3 – – – .39 .64 .16 .08 .09 .07 .15 .16 .03
18 Text 4 – – – – .69 .11 .15 .07 .05 .04 .15 .08
19 TextT – – – – – .16 .25 .12 .10 .23 .27 .11
20 EFI 1 – – – – – – .09 .36 .27 .30 .57 .13
21 EFI 2 – – – – – – – .21 .33 .31 .61 .24
22 EFI 3 – – – – – – – – .18 .42 .66 .05
23 EFI 4 – – – – – – – – – .44 .67 .28
24 EFI 5 – – – – – – – – – – .74 .19
25 EFIT – – – – – – – – – – – .28
26 PSQI – – – – – – – – – – – –

Note: iPod Total indicates iPod Dependence Scale total score; Text Total indicates Texting Dependence Scale total score; EFI indicates Executive Function Index; PSQI indicates
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; p < .05; p < .01.

texting and iPod dependence and poorer overall executive function start time (as little as 25 min) has shown promise with regard to
performance among college students (see also Byrd et al., 2014; Do alleviating some of the sleep problems associated with adolescents
et al., 2013; Gellis et al., 2015; Touitou, 2013; White et al., 2011). It and college students (Boergers, Gable, & Owens, 2014; Lund et al.,
also extends work by Anderson (2001) who showed that Internet 2010), a more integrated approach which requires college students
use negatively affected sleep patterns in college students (Do to set limits and boundaries on their social media usage may be in
et al., 2013; Touitou, 2013). Likewise, our results are consistent order (Adams & Kisler, 2013). For instance, Teixeira et al. (2013)
with recent work by Adams and Kisler (2013) who showed that has shown that newer technologies (bright light therapy) can
sleep quality was a mediator between technology usage (texting, reduce sleepiness in college students, thereby increasing their
phone calls) after the onset of sleep and the rise in depression sleep quality. In turn, this increase in sleep quality would poten-
and anxiety. Derbyshire et al. (2013) have also shown that moder- tially result in a reversal of the many negative behaviors and con-
ate to severe Internet usage was associated with lower GPA, less ditions that often accompany individuals who exhibit poor sleep
frequent exercise, more depressive symptoms and higher stress. quality. Our data and those of others clearly show the downside
Clearly, staying connected to various social media (texting, iPod, regarding the lack of adequate sleep quality across a wide range
Internet) adversely impacts a broad range of social, psychological, of activities and behaviors.
and behavioral domains in college students, resulting in a range
of associated problems in the short- and long-term. These issues 4.1. Limitations and conclusions
are especially relevant to college students, as the vast majority of
them are connected to the Internet and various other social media, While our results are both unique and troubling, there are some
on what often seems like a 24/7 time frame (Derbyshire et al., shortcomings in the present study that need to be addressed. First,
2013), even interrupting sleep and other necessary activities (e.g., our design was cross-sectional and a cause and effect relationship
eating, hygiene, performing school work, non-academic work) to with sleep quality and our selected variables could not be deter-
continually participate (Adams & Kisler, 2013). While a later school mined. Longitudinal studies conducted over multiple semesters
48 F.R. Ferraro et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49

Table 2 of the last week, while some questionnaires had no temporal


Descriptive Statistics of good and poor sleepers. window).
Measure Good Poor df F p Power Despite these shortcomings, our study expanded on past
Self-Rated Health 2.30 (.77) 2.50 (.73) 1134 2.42 .12 .268 research to demonstrate the combined impact of sleep quality on
(M(SD)) social media dependence and executive function. Clearly, college
# Meds. .81 (.98) 1.09 1132 2.67 .105 .312 students in our sample who are experimentally defined as poor
(1.02) sleepers are suffering from a range of psychological (depression,
Intelligence 43.57 42.09 1134 .71 .40 .125
(10.60) (9.82)
anxiety), neuropsychological (executive function) and social media
Speed of 46.09 45.33 1134 .09 .77 .073 dependence issues (texting and iPod dependence). Collectively,
Processing (15.32) (15.06) these experiences may be particularly detrimental to the academic
Depression 1.06 2.39 1134 14.44 .00 .948 performance of college students. As Shochat et al. (2014) highlight,
(1.45) (2.54)
interventions need to be developed and implemented that aid col-
State Anxiety 28.77 34.71 1134 18.75 .00 .989
(6.72) (9.16) lege students suffering from poor sleep to promote more healthy
Trait Anxiety 32.73 40.08 1134 21.38 .00 .993 sleep patterns and practices.
(8.35) (10.14)
iPod 1 9.74 11.48 1134 5.93 .016 .657
(3.88) (4.46)
iPod 2 10.46 12.30 1134 6.60 .01 .729
References
(3.97) (4.41)
iPod 3 5.29 6.08 1134 3.32 .07 .396
Adams, S. K., & Kisler, T. S. (2013). Sleep quality as a mediator between technology-
(1.64) (3.21)
related sleep quality, depression and anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, &
iPod 4 8.01 9.29 1134 4.15 .044 .480
Social Networking, 16, 25–30.
(3.52) (3.81)
Anderson, K. (2001). Internet use among college students: An exploratory study.
iPod Total 33.49 39.15 1134 8.26 .005 .793 Journal of American College Health, 50, 21–26.
(10.92) (12.04) Boergers, J., Gable, C., & Owens, J. A. (2014). Later school start time is associated
Text 1 14.21 15.35 1134 2.96 .088 .482 with improved sleep and daytime functioning in adolescents. Journal of
(3.75) (3.94) Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 11–17.
Text 2 16.93 17.80 1134 1.45 .23 .288 Botwinick, J., & Storandt, M. (1973). Speed functions, vocabulary ability and age.
(4.51) (3.91) Perceptual & Motor Skills, 36, 1123–1128.
Text 3 9.54 10.70 1134 3.10 .081 .473 Buysee, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The
(3.60) (4.06) Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and
Text 4 8.91 10.41 1134 5.99 .016 .736 research. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 193–213.
(3.11) (3.98) Byrd, K. L., Gelaye, B., Tadesse, M. G., Williams, M. A., Lemma, S., & Berhane, Y.
Text Total 49.46 54.26 1134 7.50 .007 .832 (2014). Sleep disturbances and common mental disorders in college students.
Health Behavior and Policy Review, 1, 10–16.
(10.70) (9.67)
Carney, C. E., Edinger, J. D., Meyer, B., Lindman, L., & Istre, T. (2006). Daily activities
EFI 1 15.44 14.71 1132 2.88 .092 .382
and sleep quality in college students. Chronobiology International, 23, 623–637.
(2.39) (2.58) Clegg-Kraynok, M. M., McBean, A., & Montgomery-Downs, H. E. (2011). Sleep
EFI 2 17.07 15.68 1133 7.15 .008 .690 quality and characteristics of college students who use prescription
(3.13) (2.89) psychostimulants nonmedically. Sleep Medicine, 12, 598–602.
EFI 3 25.44 24.23 1132 4.31 .04 .629 Danhauer, J. L., Johnson, C. E., Byrd, A., DeGood, L., Meuel, A., Pecile, A., et al. (2009).
(3.21) (3.56) Survey of college students on iPod use and hearing health. Journal of the
EFI 4 22.00 20.32 1132 9.80 .002 .853 American Academy of Audiology, 20, 5–27.
(2.88) (3.33) Derbyshire, K. L., Lust, K. A., Schreiber, L. R. N., Odlaug, B. L., Christenson, G. A.,
EFI 5 21.04 19.89 1132 5.66 .019 .628 Golden, D. J., et al. (2013). Problematic internet use and associated risks in
(2.74) (2.86) college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54, 415–422.
EFI Total 101.89 94.83 1132 14.66 .00 .962 Do, Y. K., Shin, E., Bautista, M. A., & Foo, K. (2013). The associations between self-
(9.14) (9.77) reported Sleep duration and adolescent health outcomes: What is the role of
time spent on Internet use? Sleep Medicine, 14, 195–200.
Note: iPod Total indicates iPod Dependence Scale total score; Text Total indicates Ferguson, D. A., Greer, C. F., & Reardon, M. E. (2007). Uses and gratifications of MP3
Texting Dependence Scale total score; EFI indicates Executive Function Index; PSQI players by college students: iPods more popular than radio? Journal of Radio
indicates Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Studies, 14, 102–121.
Ferraro, F. R., Winger, A., Kreiger, M., Langseth, M., Weivioda, L., Palmiscno, J., et al.
(2012). Text-message dependence, impulsivity, and executive function. In M. A.
Cyders (Ed.), Psychology of impulsivity. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Ferraro, F. R., Wunderlich, L., Wyrobek, K., & Weivoda, L. (2014). Does iPod
are needed to gain a more fine-grained analysis of the various fac- dependence relate to texting dependence? Psychological Record, 64, 501–504.
Gellis, L. A., Park, A., Stotsky, M. T., & Taylor, D. J. (2015). Associations between sleep
tors associated with sleep quality and possible interventions. hygiene and insomnia severity in college students: Cross-sectional and
Second, not all of the group differences between good and poor prospective analyses. Behavior Therapy (in press).
sleepers emerged as predicted. For instance, we expected that poor Holfeld, B., Cicha, R., & Ferraro, F. R. (2014). Executive functioning and action
gaming among college students. Current Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
sleepers to be slower in processing speed than good sleepers. s12144-014-9263-0.
Perhaps a computer-based test of speed of processing may have Igarashi, T., Motoyoshi, T., Takai, J., & Yoshida, T. (2008). No mobile, no life: Self-
produced the desired group difference. This is critical as speed of perceptions and text message dependency among Japanese high school
students. Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 2311–2324.
processing is required for the successful completion of a number Lund, H. G., Reider, B. A., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and
of information processing tasks. Third, we expected more of the predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of
texting and iPod dependence subscales to show group differences, Adolescent Health, 46, 124–132.
Sansone, R. A., & Sansone, L. A. (2013). Cell phones: The psychological risks.
although in each case the total iPod and texting dependence levels Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 10, 33–37.
did reveal group differences, with poor sleepers showing more Shochat, T., Cohen-Zion, M., & Tzischinsky, O. (2014). Functional consequences of
overall dependence. Fourth, we did not include some additional inadequate sleep in adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews,
18, 75–87.
variables that may have been relevant, such as GPA, or physical
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1983). Manual for state/trait
activity. Examining these (and other) variables over the course of anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
multiple semesters are needed to determine the short-and long- Spinella, M. (2005). Self-rated executive function: Development of the executive
term impacts of sleep quality. Fifth, the temporal window covered function index. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115, 649–667.
Teixeira, L., Lowden, A., Luz, A. A., Turte, S. L., Moreno, C. R., Valente, D., et al. (2013).
by the various questionnaires varied tremendously (e.g., PSQI cov- Exposure to bright light during evening class hours increases alertness among
ered a period of the last month while the GDS-SF covered a period working college students. Sleep Medicine, 14, 91–97.
F.R. Ferraro et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 49 (2015) 44–49 49

Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Galvan, A. (2013). The effects of poor Touitou, Y. (2013). Adolescent sleep misalignment: A chronic jet lag and a matter of
sleep quality on brain function and risk taking in adolescence. Neuroimage, 71, public health. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 107, 323–326.
275–283. Wechsler, D. H. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. NY: Academic Press.
Thompson, A. P. (1987). Methodological issues in the clinical evaluation of two- and White, A. G., Buboltz, W., & Igou, F. (2011). Mobile phone use and sleep quality and
four-subtest short forms of the WAIS-R. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, length in college students. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science,
142–144. 1, 12–34.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen