Sie sind auf Seite 1von 19

Psychological Review Copyright 1984 by the

1984, Vol. 91, No. 3, 328-346 American Psychological Association, Inc.

Achievement Motivation: Conceptions of Ability, Subjective


Experience, Task Choice, and Performance
John G. Nicholls
Purdue University

Achievement behavior is denned as behavior directed at developing or demonstrating


high rather than low ability. It is shown that ability can be conceived in two ways.
First, ability can be judged high or low with reference to the individual's own past
performance or knowledge. In this context, gains in mastery indicate competence.
Second, ability can be judged as capacity relative to that of others. In this context,
a gain in mastery alone does not indicate high ability. To demonstrate high capacity,
one must achieve more with equal effort or use less effort than do others for an
equal performance. The conditions under which these different conceptions of
ability function as individuals' goals and the nature of subjective experience in
each case are specified. Different predictions of task choice and performance are
derived and tested for each case.

In this article, predictions of task choice, and thought, one must first specify individuals'
performance, and subjective experience in ex- goals and predict when a given goal will govern
perimental achievement settings are derived, behavior. Here, achievement behavior is de-
and relevant evidence is examined. An inten- nned as that behavior in which the goal is to
tional view of behavior (Dennett, 1978) is develop or demonstrate—to self or to others—
adopted. In this, action is construed as a ra- high ability, or to avoid demonstrating low
tional attempt to attain goals or incentives. In ability. This implies that in achievement sit-
commonsense terms, individuals' actions serve uations individuals desire success to the extent
to achieve purposes efficiently or economically. that it indicates high ability and seek to avoid
In the terms of games theory, action maximizes failure to the extent that it indicates low ability
gains and minimizes losses. An action can fail (Kukla, 1978; McFarland& Ross, 1982). This
to achieve a purpose but cannot be useless in is more precise than McClelland, Atkinson,
the sense that it would be if it were not designed Clark, and Lowell's (1953) definition of
to serve a purpose. In other words, the term achievement motivation in terms of affect as-
rational refers to the way goals are pursued. sociated with performance that is evaluated
Only in a limited sense (e.g., p. 332 of this in terms of standards of excellence (p. 79).
article), does it imply anything about the Standards of excellence apply to moral as well
adaptive value of any goal.1 as to achievement behavior. V. J. Crandall,
To use this approach to predict behavior Katkovsky, and Preston (1960), Heckhausen
(1967), Kukla (1972, 1978), and Maehr and
This article is based on a paper presented in a sym-
Nicholls (1980) hold that the distinguishing
posium, Attributional Approaches to Human Motivation feature of achievement behavior is that its goal
(W-U. Meyer and B. Weiner, chairmen), that was held at is competence or perception of competence.
the Center for Interdisciplinary Research, University of The present theory adds the assumption that
Bielefeld, West Germany, in July 1980. Preparation of the adolescents and adults can construe compe-
article was facilitated by National Science Foundation
Grant BNS 7914252, University of Illinois subcontract. tence or ability in at least two different ways.
The author is grateful for discussions with Carol Dweck,
Martin Maehr, and Donald Ferris, and comments on earlier
1
versions by Charles Carver, Virginia Crandall, Heinz Consideration of actions in terms of individuals' goals
Heckhausen, Carolyn Jagacinski, Martin Maehr, Ernest is the stance adopted by naive observers when they em-
McDaniel, Arden Miller, Klaus Schneider, Deborah Stipek, pathize with others (Hoffman, Mischel, & Mazze, 1981).
and anonymous reviewers. This gives derivations within the intentional framework a
Requests for reprints should be sent to John G. Nicholls, commonsense or naive flavor and might account for the
Educational Psychology, SCC-G, Purdue University, West fact that, despite its long history (Bolles, 1974), the ap-
Lafayette, Indiana 47907. proach may be considered heretical (Garcia, 1981, p. 151).

328
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 329

Two forms of achievement goal are thereby fail. Capacity is inferred by interpersonal
distinguished. According to the intentional comparison of performance and effort. In this
framework, our subjective experience and overt case, one could learn through effort or could
behavior should differ in predictable ways master a task that was personally very chal-
when we have different goals. The two con- lenging but still fail to demonstrate high ability.
ceptions of ability are, therefore, keys to this Indeed, the more effort or time one needs to
theory. learn something (compared to the effort or
time it takes others) the less capacity is implied.
Conceptions of Ability These conceptions of ability were first noted
The two conceptions of ability have in com- in research on the development of the concept
mon the notions that task mastery is improved of ability (Nicholls, 1978, 1980; Nicholls &
by effort or learning and that mastery is not Miller, 1983). Young children conceive of abil-
normally lost. In the first and less differentiated ity in a self-referenced manner as learning
conception, levels of ability and task difficulty through effort. For them, to have low ability
are judged in relation to one's own perceived means mere failure to master a task or to im-
mastery, understanding, or knowledge. The prove as much as one had hoped. After a num-
more individuals feel they have learned, the ber of intermediate levels of differentiation,
more competent they feel. The more differ- adolescents conceive of ability as capacity (not
entiated conception is embodied in standard merely performance) relative to that of others.
intelligence testing practices (Nicholls, 1978). In this case, perception of low ability more
In this conception, learning is an insufficient clearly involves perception of inadequacy in
basis for perception of competence. Rather, the self—lack of capacity that cannot readily
task difficulty and ability are judged high or be altered. The motivational significance of
low with reference to the ability of members these conceptions of ability is indicated
of a normative reference group. High ability by concomitant developmental changes in
means above average and low ability means achievement affect, task choice, and perfor-
below average. In addition, valid inferences of mance (Nicholls & Miller, in press). The con-
ability are presumed to require evidence that cern here is with causes and consequences of
effort is equal and optimum across individuals. activation of the different conceptions in ad-
This implies a conception of ability as capacity olescents and adults.
that is not always revealed in performance. To judge our capacity we must compare the
Only optimum effort reveals the present limit effort and attainment of self and others. In
of one's capacity and this capacity limits the other words, we must adopt a relatively ex-
effect of effort on performance.2 ternal or social self-evaluative perspective. For
The two conceptions of ability embody dif- this reason, the term ego involvement is applied
ferent criteria for judging one's ability or to states where individuals seek to demonstrate
chances of demonstrating ability. In the less ability in the differentiated sense. Use of the
differentiated conception, difficulty and ability less differentiated conception involves a less
judgments are self-referenced. Tasks are judged explicitly self-evaluative stance. The concern
difficult if we expect to fail on them, and the here is with improving one's mastery of tasks
more difficult they appear, the more does suc- rather than with one's ability relative to that
cess indicate high ability. A greater gain in of others. Accordingly, the term task involve-
mastery or mastery of a task that one was
uncertain of being able to master indicates 2
greater competence. Furthermore, because Among researchers and, no doubt, among naive adults,
there are diverse views on the relative importance of hered-
more effort is seen to lead to more learning ity and environment in determining the levels of individ-
(that indicates more ability), the higher the uals' available or present capacities. These diverse views
effort needed for mastery, the higher the per- are not of concern here. There is, however, agreement that
ceived ability. In the more differentiated con- a test score does not reveal one's present capacity if one
ception of ability as capacity, task difficulty does not apply optimum effort and that it takes much
more than optimum effort during a test to improve one's
(normative difficulty) is judged from the per- capacity. It is this understanding, which is implicit in in-
formance of others, and demonstration of high telligence test administration practices, that is embodied
ability demands success on tasks where others in the differentiated conception.
330 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

ment is applied to states where individuals seek tually adequate evaluation of the extent to
to demonstrate ability in the less differentiatedwhich mastery reflects ability as opposed to
sense. I do not thereby imply that feelings of effort or task difficulty.
competence are absent in task involvement. It follows that, when one faces skill tasks,
Young children who lack the differentiated the basis of performance evaluation shifts from
conception gain strong feelings of competence the less to the more differentiated conception
from their accomplishments (Heckhausen, in as the strength of factors that induce concerns
press); so do adults when they are task in- about evaluation of our competence increases.
volved. However, task involvement requires a (I do not consider situations involving explicit
less social or external perspective on the self. behavioral standards such as a specific level
Studies confirming that both conceptions can of achievement or situations where altruistic
define achievement goals for adults are noted or other task-extrinsic goals are salient.) An-
after the following two sections. nouncements that skill tasks are being used
The foundations of the present theory are to test subjects should induce concerns about
now laid. The most general postulate is that personal competence, especially if important
overt action and cognitive activity is rational or valued skills such as intelligence are at issue.
in the sense described. The second postulate Interpersonal competition on skill tasks also
is that individuals can have development or poses the question, who is best? Competition—
demonstration of ability, in either sense, as a especially on valued tasks—is, therefore, pre-
goal. We can, therefore, begin derivation of dicted to increase use of the differentiated
predictions—first, with the factors that engage conception to evaluate oneself. Finally, the
the more rather than the less differentiated more differentiated conception involves a
conception of ability. more public perspective on the self. Thus, in
skill situations, manipulations such as use of
Conditions of Usage of Each Conception an audience (Carver & Scheier, 1981) that in-
crease public self-awareness are likely to in-
It has frequently been observed that humans crease self-evaluation in terms of the differ-
(and monkeys) attempt to improve their level entiated conception. Tests of these predictions
of mastery (a) if they are presented skill tasks are considered after the following section.
offering a moderate challenge and are not
placed under physiological or psychological Subjective Experience
stress and (b) if task-extrinsic incentives are Use of the differentiated conception involves
not salient (Csikszentmihalyi, 1977; Deci, a more actively self-evaluative or social view
1975; Elkind, 1971; Harlow, 1950; White, of the self. It is also predicted that learning or
1959). That is, these conditions produce task mastery through effort is experienced more as
involvement. Under such relatively neutral an end in itself as task involvement increases
conditions, the less differentiated conception and more as a means to an end as ego in-
of ability is a goal of action, and perception volvement increases. This follows from con-
of improved mastery through effort indicates sideration of the processes necessary to infer
competence. The intentional framework in- ability in each sense.
dicates that individuals do not make complex To assess their chances of demonstrating
judgments if simple ones serve their purposes. ability, ego-involved individuals must assess
In task involvement, the differentiated con- what they can master and whether this implies
ception is superfluous. We do not need to higher capacity than that of others. They must
compare our own and others' attainment and judge whether mastery will serve their end.
effort to establish whether we have gained in They might, for example, believe they can im-
mastery. The differentiated conception should prove through effort but expect such attempts
not, therefore, be employed. But, as when we to imply low capacity more clearly than in-
seek to assess a person's intelligence, when our activity would. Mastery, therefore, tends to be
goal is to establish whether mastery reflects experienced as a means to an end rather than
ability rather than task ease or effort, we must an end in itself: Action is more exogenously
employ the differentiated conception. Only the attributed (Kruglanski, 1975; see also Deci &
differentiated conception permits a concep- Ryan, 1980).
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 331

In task involvement, however, improvement effort improves mastery conflicts with the
is the goal. An increase in mastery is, therefore, "fact" that higher effort implies lower capacity.
an end in itself. In task involvement, percep- No such conflict was apparent when a learning
tion of an opportunity to develop or exercise for learning's sake situation was simulated.
an increased level of skill, therefore, reliably These findings (replicated four times) support
occasions attempts to do so. Furthermore, as the prediction that competitive rather than
learning is an end in itself, when individuals learning-oriented conditions engage the dif-
feel they are mastering a task, they feel they ferentiated conception. Furthermore, pride
are doing what they want to do. They feel and a sense of accomplishment were higher
more intrinsically motivated than when they when effort was higher in task involvement
are ego involved.3 and lower when effort was higher in ego in-
volvement.
Conceptions of Ability and Subjective Presentation of tasks as tests of intelligence
States: Evidence should induce ego involvement. Test-anxiety
researchers have often compared this manip-
First, it is claimed that either conception of ulation with neutral conditions and found it
ability can be employed by adults to evaluate to increase concerns to meet the performance
their performance in skill situations. Second, norms (integral to the differentiated concep-
it is predicted that the differentiated concep- tion) implied in the manipulation (I. G. Sar-
tion is activated as a goal when, instead of ason, 1975; Wine, 1971). (This research is
being presented in a neutral fashion, (a) tasks considered in the section on performance.)
are presented as tests of valued skills, (b) in- Patten and White (1977) found that presenting
terpersonal competition is fostered, and (c) self- a task as an intelligence test (ego involving)
awareness is induced. Ego-involving conditions and asking students to make causal attributions
are also predicted to increase exogenous at- for their performance had the same effects on
tribution and diminish interest in mastery. performance. (Performance differed in a neu-
Diener and Srull (1979) used television and tral condition.) When attributing one's out-
voice recordings—a method that appears to comes to ability, effort, or difficulty, the dif-
induce public self-awareness (Carver & Scheier, ferentiated conception of ability must be ac-
1981, chap. 16). This manipulation produced tivated if one is to respond meaningfully. Thus,
self-reinforcement when peer norms were sur- Patten and White's finding is also consistent
passed. When self-awareness was not induced, with the hypothesis that presentation of tasks
however, subjects self-reinforced on the basis as tests of a valued ability is more likely than
of their own previous performance (indicating is a neutral presentation to activate the dif-
self-evaluation in terms of the less differen- ferentiated conception.
tiated conception). When self-awareness was Comparisons of attributions and affect in
manipulated and when individual variation in competitive (ego-involving) and noncompet-
public self-awareness was assessed, Scheier and itive (task-involving) conditions also indicate
Carver (1983) also found self-awareness as- more self-evaluation in terms of the less dif-
sociated with more attention to social com- ferentiated conception (where what is accom-
parison norms. These results support the hy- plished through effort is the basis of perceived
potheses that increases in public self-awareness competence) in noncompetitive conditions.
increase use of the differentiated conception More effort attribution (C. Ames & R. Ames,
to evaluate one's competence.
Jagacinski and Nicholls (in press) asked stu-
dents to anticipate feelings of competence on 3
The exogenous quality of ego-involved action can be
passing a language test after high versus low distinguished from the examples of exogenously attributed
effort. All of the students expected higher effort skill behavior most commonly found in the literature but
to lead to greater gains in competence. How- not discussed here. For example, surveillance or offers of
ever, in a competitive (ego-involving) situation, prizes can transform endogenous to exogenous involvement
(Lepper & Greene, 1975). In ego involvement, the end is
students anticipated feeling less able when ef- to demonstrate superior capacity, whereas in the above
fort was high. This indicates use of the dif- examples, demonstration of skill is a means to nona-
ferentiated conception where the "fact" that chievement incentives.
332 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

1981) and a stronger positive association be- Schwartz, Sheinman, & Ryan, 1981). Fur-
tween perceived effort and satisfaction (C. thermore, children whose teachers used coer-
Ames, R. Ames, & Felker, 1977) emerged in cion and social comparison (not separated in
noncompetitive conditions. Competitive con- analyses) reported less intrinsic interest in
ditions produced a positive association be- school.
tween own perceived ability (not effort) and There is, then, support for the claim that
satisfaction with performance (C. Ames et al., both conceptions of ability can be achievement
1977). In the above studies, experimenters goals and for the predictions of the circum-
elicited performance attributions. When sub- stances under which each goal is activated and
jects could report either attributions or self- of subjective experience in each case. These
instructions (which indicate a focus on mas- phenomena are of interest in their own right
tery), a complementary pattern emerged (C. and are necessary for the derivation and testing
Ames & McKelvie, 1983). Competition pro- of predictions of task choice and performance.
duced more ability and difficulty attributions
than did an individual goal condition, which Task Choice
produced more effort attributions and self-in-
structions. Thus there appears to be more use It is assumed that, in achievement situa-
of the less differentiated conception in non- tions, each individual's purpose is to dem-
competitive conditions and more evaluation onstrate high ability and to avoid demonstrat-
of capacity in competitive conditions. ing low ability. According to the intentional
Csikszentmihalyi (1977) has described the framework, individuals should select those
subjective states of individuals when they are tasks they expect to enable them to maximize
highly involved in tasks as marked by feelings their chances of demonstrating high ability and
of competence and reduced public self-aware- avoiding demonstrating low ability.
ness. As the concept of task involvement im- Individuals can be committed to goals they
plies, these states required tasks offering a do not expect to attain (Klinger, 1975;
chance to improve or perfect one's mastery. McFarlin & Blascovich, 1981). But, ultimately,
Activities that induced these states were seen the rational response to an unattainable goal
as satisfying for endogenous rather than ex- is rejection of that goal and selection of the
ogenous reasons (p. 17). This was less marked next most attractive goal (Klinger, 1975). Thus,
for activities that produced less task involve- as individuals become more certain they can-
ment. It supports the proposed link between not demonstrate high ability they should tend
task-involvement and endogenous attribution to adopt the less attractive goal of avoiding
for action. demonstrating low ability. This goal should,
Similarly, Deci, Betley, Kahle, Abrams, and in turn, be relinquished as hope of attaining
Porac (1981) found more interest in puzzles it dies.
after individual performance (task involve- To ascertain the most economical action for
ment) than after competitive success. Ryan demonstrating ability, individuals must form
(1982) found more interest in tasks after per- subjective probabilities of demonstrating high
formance in a neutral condition than after an versus low ability on the available tasks. The
intelligence test condition. Diener and Srull's intentional framework indicates that they em-
(1979) induction of self-awareness also di- ploy only the relevant data and process these
minished students' feelings of freedom about no more than necessary for this purpose. Be-
dispensing self-reinforcement. Ego involve- cause a less differentiated conception of ability
ment appears to increase feelings of constraint is employed in task involvement, the process
about self-evaluation as well as for achievement of forming expectations of demonstrating
behavior itself. Evidence of a different type ability is less complex in this state.
also suggests the link between use of social Task Involvement
norms and exogenous attribution. Teacher re-
ports of use of social comparison to control When individuals are task involved, they
children were more highly correlated with re- see more effort as leading to more mastery
ports of use of coercive (exogenous) methods and higher ability. Tasks seen as demanding
than with use of noncoercive methods (Deci, no effort for success and tasks expected to not
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 333

yield to maximum effort offer no chance of tasks are predicted to depend on how certain
demonstrating high ability. Tasks that appear they are that they lack ability and on their
likely to yield to high effort offer the optimum associated level of commitment to demon-
balance between high chances of demonstrat- strating high ability or to avoiding demon-
ing little ability and low chances of demon- stration of low ability. Consider first those
strating high ability (which would involve whose doubts about their competence are not
waste of much effort). That is, tasks should firm enough to have extinguished commitment
be most attractive at an intermediate level of to demonstrating high ability. For these in-
expectancy of success where one's highest dividuals, choice of easy tasks would be ir-
likely level of competence might be demon- rational in that it could not conceivably lead
strated. Individuals should differ in the level to demonstration of high ability. Only nor-
of objective difficulty at which they have mod- matively difficult tasks offer any chance of at-
erate expectancies of success.4 They should taining the more attractive goal of demon-
differ correspondingly in the level of objective strating high ability as well as the certainty of
difficulty they prefer; however, all should prefer avoiding the demonstration of low ability.
tasks close to their own perceived level of com- (McFarlin & Blascovich, 1981, show that
petence.5 commitment to achievement goals can exist
despite very low expectations of attaining
them.) Thus, those who suspect their ability
Ego Involvement
is low but are committed to demonstrating
When individuals are ego involved, their high ability should prefer normatively difficult
chances of demonstrating ability depend on tasks. Here, their expectation of success is very
the ability of others. Thus, one might dem- low but failure cannot imply low ability and
onstrate incompetence but not competence on the possibility that they have high ability can-
normatively easy tasks (on which many can not be ruled out.
succeed). One might demonstrate ability but Repeated failure or other experiences may,
not incompetence on normatively difficult however, have produced virtual certainty that
tasks. Moderate normative difficulty levels al- one lacks high ability. The intentional frame-
low the possibility of above- or below-average work leads to the prediction that, for individ-
performance that would indicate high or low uals in this second category, commitment to
capacity. demonstrating high ability is low and the goal
Individuals with high perceived ability have of avoiding the demonstration of low ability
moderate expectancies of success on norma-
tively moderate to difficult tasks where success
4
indicates high ability. Therefore, they should Objective difficulty refers to the objective properties
prefer tasks at or above moderate difficulty of tasks, such as number of pieces in a jigsaw puzzle or
levels, depending on how able they believe they distance from a target. This is distinguished from normative
difficulty that is inferred from the performance of others
are. They do not expect to decline in the level (Nicholls & Miller, 1983).
they can attain. Therefore, when repeated suc- 5
The less differentiated conception of ability, being self-
cess at a given level indicates possession of the referenced, does not allow the possibility of demonstrating
corresponding level of ability, they can gain low ability in the decisive fashion that is possible when
ability is judged relative to that of others. Nor does low
but not lose in perceived ability by attempting ability in this sense clearly indicate the personal inadequacy
more difficult tasks. Thus they should prefer associated with a lack of capacity. Perception of low ability
normatively moderate to difficult tasks where in the less differentiated sense can occur in environments
their expectations of success are moderate. that offer no opportunity to improve one's level of mastery.
The picture is more complex for those with Consider, for example, the effect of an extremely unre-
sponsive environment on infants, who do not have the
low perceived ability. With tasks of moderate differentiated conception of ability. They appear to almost
normative difficulty they expect to fail and, never perceive an opportunity to develop or demonstrate
thereby, to demonstrate low ability. They competence and rarely attempt to do so (Hethcrington &
should, therefore, avoid such tasks. Choice of Parke, 1975). In the framework of the less differentiated
conception, this represents something close to absolute
either very easy or very difficult tasks enables zero of perceived ability. This could be termed a state of
them to avoid demonstrating low ability. These learned helplessness—perceived noncontingency of action
individuals' preferences for easy versus difficult and outcome.
334 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

is more salient. Withdrawal from the situation relative to that of others and predict expec-
would achieve this. If the situation mandates tations of success or perceived ability on ex-
a choice, the rational choice is a normatively perimental tasks. Self-esteem or self-concept
easy task that demands little effort and where scales serve this purpose effectively. Such scales
success indicates that one does not lack the refer primarily to the adequacy of one's com-
low level of ability that failure would indicate. petence (R. Crandall, 1973), and authors (e.g.,
Thus, those who are more certain that their Brockner, 1979; McFarlin & Blascovich, 1981;
ability is not high are predicted to prefer nor- Shrauger, 1975) commonly employ and de-
matively easy tasks where their expectations scribe them as perceived-competence mea-
of success are high. Those in the third and sures. The fact that they predict perceived
most extreme category—those who are certain ability on experimental tasks (McFarlin &
their ability is low and who are not committed Blascovich, 1981) supports their validity for
to avoiding demonstration of low ability— the present purpose. Jagacinski and Nicholls
should see very easy tasks as offering the most (in press) also found that college students in-
economical way of leaving the situation. terpreted the statement that someone is able
In summary, three types of individuals with as meaning that they have high capacity rel-
low perceived ability are distinguished. Some ative to others. Conversely, a statement that
are committed to demonstrating high ability. someone succeeds through high effort was not
Others, more certain that their ability is not interpreted as implying that they have high
high, lack commitment to demonstrating high ability. This indicates that typical self-esteem
ability. Others are certain their ability is low items would elicit evaluations of ability in the
and have accepted this. The first type should differentiated sense. Many questions about
tend to select tasks where they have very low personal competence would heighten a self-
expectancies of success. The second and third evaluative stance and amplify this trend. An
types should tend to prefer tasks where they association between a self-concept scale and
have high expectancies of success.6 ratings of own skill relative to that of others
Atkinson's (1957, 1965) theory predicts (Roberts, Kleiber, & Duda, 1981) also supports
greater preference for moderate probabilities this thesis.
of success in those with high resultant achieve-
6
ment motivation and preference for high or Predictions are stated in terms of normative difficulty
low probabilities in those with low resultant and expectancies of success for ego involvement and in
motivation. These parallel present predictions terms of expectancy of success for task involvement. Even
if normative cues are absent, ego-involved individuals ap-
for high and low perceived ability in ego in- pear able to estimate their chances of demonstrating ability.
volvement. However, Atkinson's theory does Individuals with either high perceived attainment, general
not distinguish preference for high versus low expectancies, or perceived ability are more inclined than
probabilities of success. Nor does it predict a are others to attribute success but not failure to ability
on tasks of unspecified normative difficulty (C. Ames, 1978;
general preference for challenging tasks in task- Feather & Simon, 1971; Nicholls, 1976, 1979a; Simon &
involving situations. Others hold that Atkinson Feather, 1973; Valle & Frieze, 1976). It seems that when
is wrong: Kukla (1978), and Meyer, Folkes, they have moderate expectancies of success, individuals
and Weiner (1976) predict greater preference with high perceived ability see tasks as moderately nor-
for intermediate probabilities in all subjects. matively difficult and infer high ability from success. When
individuals with low perceived ability have moderate ex-
The present position is that Atkinson is correct pectancies of success they appear to see tasks as easy and
though incomplete in ego involvement and do not infer high ability from success. Predictions can,
that Kukla and Meyer et al. are correct in task therefore, be tested with reference to expectancies of suc-
involvement. cess. This is important because studies of task choice often
do not employ normative cues.
Measurement of Perceived Ability 'According to Atkinson's (1957) theory, subjective
probabilities of success and task difficulty are equivalent.
Hypothesis testing does not demand esti- Except in young children (Nicholls & Miller, 1983) and
mates of perceived ability in the undifferen- in task involvement, where the less differentiated concep-
tiated sense beyond expectations of success. tion of ability is employed, they are not. This means that
Atkinson's predictions (framed in terms of probabilities
An index of perceived ability relevant to ego of success) are ambiguous. In this instance I assume that
involvement is, however, needed. This should his predictions refer to subjective probability of success,
indicate individuals' evaluations of their ability not to task difficulty.
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 335

Test-anxiety scales correlate quite highly tasks (Atkinson, 1957, 1969) supports this
with self-concept or self-esteem scales (R. view.8 Klinger and McNelly's (1969) claim that
Crandall, 1973) and thus lack discriminant this motive measure indicates perceived status
validity. Although one would not guess it from is also consistent with this thesis.9
test-anxiety-scale titles, the content of these Although they lack discriminant validity,
scales refers directly to perceived ability and the above measures are obviously not identical
expected adequacy of performance. These facts and the unique components of each merit
alone provide a case for considering them more study (Nicholls, 1976). Yet there is jus-
measures of (low) perceived ability. But there tification for considering them measures of
is more. In the case of the Test Anxiety Scale perceived ability. Confirmation of present pre-
for Children, it has been shown that perceived- dictions with these different measures supports
competence content accounts for the associ- their construct validity as indexes of perceived
ations of the scale with performance and other ability and indicates that a theoretical inte-
variables that have been presumed to be a gration has been achieved.
consequence of anxiety (Nicholls, 1976). Lie-
bert and Morris (1967) separated test-anxiety- Task Choice: Evidence
scale content into worry and emotionality
components. A number of studies show that In task involvement, preference for inter-
worry rather than emotionality is related to mediate subjective probabilities is predicted
performance and that worry but not emo- for all individuals. In ego involvement, those
tionality scores are influenced by performance with high perceived ability are predicted to
feedback in exactly the ways one would expect prefer moderate subjective probabilities and
perceived ability to be affected (Morris, Davis, those with low perceived ability are predicted
& Hutchings, 1981; Wine, 1971). Morris et to prefer more extreme probabilities. I consider
al. characterized the tendency to worry as a evidence on the general contrast between task
tendency toward negative self-evaluation and and ego involvement and then the extra pre-
negative expectation. Indeed, worry items are dictions for low-perceived-ability individuals
accurately described as low perceived com- when they are ego involved.10
petence rather than worry items (e.g., "I do
not feel confident about my performance on
8
this test", "I do not feel self-confident"; Morris Although consistent with the present position, this ev-
et al., 1981). Thus, the active ingredient of idence contradicts a fundamental assumption of Atkinson's
mathematical model, namely, that task difficulty and sub-
test-anxiety scales is perceived competence. jective probability of success can be equated. It also threat-
The validity of these scales for the present pur- ens the implication of his model that the differences be-
pose is further indicated by evidence that test- tween low- and high-resultant-motive individuals reflect
anxious students perceive themselves as less individual differences in the values associated with success
and failure rather than individual differences in expec-
able on experimental tasks (Arkin, Detchon, tations of success and failure. Alternatively, this evidence
& Maruyama, 1982) and as encountering more could be seen as indicating that, in terms of Atkinson's
problems demonstrating ability on exams (Ar- theory, his measure of resultant motivation lacks construct
kin, Kolditz, & Kolditz, 1983). validity; it produces results contrary to the assumptions
Resultant achievement-motivation (Atkin- of the theory.
9
Mehrabian's (1969) resultant achievement-motivation
son & Feather, 1966) scores incorporate test- scale has been used extensively. Many of its items elicit
anxiety scores. For this reason and in accord preferences for challenging skill tasks versus easy or nonskill
with the evidence and arguments of Kukla tasks. Evidence that higher scorers on this scale choose
(1972, 1978), individuals high in need for more challenging tasks might, therefore, merely show that
achievement and low in test anxiety (high re- people do what they say they do. Such evidence is of ques-
tionable relevance to achievement theory (Nicholls, Licht,
sultant achievement motivation) can be con- & Pearl, 1982). Therefore, studies of task choice using this
sidered high in perceived ability, and those low scale are not considered.
10
in need for achievement and high in test anx- In the method now commonly employed to estimate
iety (low resultant motivation) can be consid- subjective probabilities of success, individuals who prefer
tasks on which they have low or high success rates are
ered low in perceived ability. Evidence that assumed to prefer low or high probabilities. By anchoring
higher resultant motivation is associated with estimates of subjective probabilities of success to each in-
higher expectancies of success on experimental dividual's rate of success, this method takes account of
336 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

In an ego-involving condition, Raynor and making of a visible public record of perfor-


Smith (1966) presented puzzles as an intelli- mance outcomes in these studies should have
gence measure and emphasized the test's im- induced ego involvement. Use of academic
portance. In another condition, the experi- material (DeCharms & Carpenter 1968) and
menter acted in a nonevaluative manner and anagrams (Moulton, 1965) presented as im-
minimized the importance of the task. It ap- portant tests of ability would induce ego in-
peared that subjects experienced the conditions volvement. Brody (1963) presented under-
as less different than was intended (Raynor & graduates with what was said to be a test of
Smith, p. 187). Nevertheless, as predicted here, their ability, on which they recorded their
there was a strong tendency for low- more than names. Both actions would induce ego in-
high-resultant-achievement-motive students to volvement. Finally, Mahone (1960) studied
select extreme probabilities in the ego-involv- occupational choices that are likely to occur
ing condition and a greater general preference in ego involvement. As predicted here, in these
for moderate probabilities in the neutral con- seven studies where there was evidence of fac-
dition. tors that would induce ego involvement, low-
A similar test arose out of work by Schneider resultant-motive individuals preferred more
(1973, chap. 4), who, in a series of studies extreme probability levels than did high-mo-
under neutral conditions, found no difference tive subjects.
between individuals high and low in resultant Trope (1979) employed a nonevaluative sit-
motivation. However, in one study (chap. 5), uation in which the task was said to be un-
low-motive subjects preferred more extreme related to intelligence and anonymity was as-
probabilities. In this study the experimenter sured. Thus it is probable that task involve-
was a teacher in the subjects' school rather ment was maintained. Regardless of perceived
than an unfamiliar nonevaluative adult. This ability, all of the subjects in this study preferred
led to a comparison of a nonevaluative pre- tasks that discriminated between levels of
sentation with'presentation of the task as a ability closest to their own perceived level.
valid test of ability. Consistent with present With similar conditions, Buckert, Meyer, and
predictions, more extreme probability pref- Schmalt (1979) obtained the same results.
erences on the part of low- than high-resultant- Thus, the available results are consistent with
motive students occurred only the more ego- predictions.
involving condition (Jopt, 1974, p. 196). The remaining question is whether high
Studies without direct comparisons of two versus low subjective probability of success
conditions provide less adequate tests of the choices occur as predicted. When ego involve-
theory. Nevertheless, such studies could dis- ment is induced, low-perceived-ability indi-
confirm it. Seven studies of choice under (ap- viduals who retain commitment to demon-
parently) ego-involving conditions were found. strating high ability should choose low prob-
DeCharms and Dave (1965), Roberts (1974), abilities. Those who are more certain their
and Hamilton (1974) used physical skill tasks ability is low should choose high probabilities.
with males. The very high value males place Two studies with relevant data were found.
on physical skills (Roberts, in press) and the Sears (1940, 1941) gave 9- to 12-year-olds
academic tasks in a testlike ego-involving
manner. Children who chose higher and lower
individual differences in subjective probabilities. Deviations probabilities had failed consistently in school.
of individual preferences from group (rather than indi- Thus, they would have lower perceived ability
vidual) performance means have also been employed (e.g., (Bloom, 1976; Nicholls, 1979a). Within this
McClelland, 1958). This method appears to reflect the
assumption (Atkinson, 1957) that "degree of difficulty can
group, those selecting very low probabilities
be inferred from the subjective probability of success" (p. of success showed a stronger wish for high
362). Because this method fails to deal with individual achievement in diverse activities and made
differences in expectation of success (Heckhausen, 1968), more negative evaluations of their competence
studies employing it are avoided in this review. Note that in these activities than did others. They acted
studies that use individual rather than group data to test
Atkinson's theory (e.g., Hamilton, 1974; Moulton, 1965) "as if they never felt they were doing well
embody a rejection of Atkinson's assumption that difficulty enough" (Sears, 1940, p. 523). This indicates
and subjective probability are equivalent. the predicted commitment to demonstration
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 337

of high ability despite perception of low ability. entiated sense. For those with high perceived
Those selecting high probabilities showed ability, this is on normatively moderate to dif-
greater responsiveness to nonachievement in- ficult tasks. For these individuals, effort and
centives and greater readiness to lower their thus performance is predicted to be low on
goals after manipulated success (Sears, 1940, tasks that appear to demand little effort and
1941). Both phenomena indicate the predicted where failure appears certain despite maxi-
rejection of the goal of demonstrating high mum effort—tasks perceived as normatively
ability. easy and extremely difficult, respectively.
Further support comes from Moulton's In ego involvement, individuals with low
(1965) study of high school students' choices perceived ability expect to demonstrate low
on tasks presented as valid tests of their in- ability in the moderate normative difficulty
tellectual ability. Students low and interme- range. Performance on such tasks should be
diate in resultant motivation chose more ex- impaired, but the mechanism involved de-
treme subjective probability levels than did pends on how certain they are that they lack
high-motive students. Furthermore, the lowest ability (see Carver & Scheier, 1981, for an
group chose high probabilities more than the analysis in terms of increasing expectancies of
intermediate group, who favored low proba- failure.) First, consider those whose perceived
bilities. Given that the lowest group was most ability is low, but not low enough to have ex-
certain that they lacked ability, these results tinguished commitment to demonstrating high
also support the predictions. ability. This commitment will maintain effort.
In summary, the present theory resolves the However, the aversive expectation of demon-
contradiction between the predictions of At- strating lack of personal capacity should pro-
kinson and those of Kukla and Meyer et al. duce the self-derogation, negative affect, and
It successfully predicts choices in task versus impaired performance associated with test
ego involvement. Unlike the above positions, anxiety (Arkin et al., 1982; I. G. Sarason, 1975;
it also predicts high- versus low-probability Wine, 1971).
choices in ego involvement. (See also Heck- As predicted in the task-choice section, in-
hausen, 1977.) dividuals whose expectations of demonstrating
high ability are extremely low should have less
Performance or Attainment commitment to this goal and should be more
Some theories (e.g., Kukla, 1972) hold that committed to avoiding demonstration of low
level of performance increases with intensity ability. The fact that, in ego involvement, fail-
of effort. The present position adds the as- ure implies low ability less decisively when
sumption that performance can be impaired effort is low is then relevant. Thus, at moderate
by self-derogatory ability evaluations even normative-difficulty levels, the probability of
when effort is high (Arkin et al., 1982; I. G. a self-protective reduction of effort (Frankel
Sarason, 1975; Wine, 1971). Consistent with & Snyder, 1978) should be higher for these
the intentional framework, it is assumed that individuals than for others. A more extreme
effort and self-evaluations are a function of result is predicted for individuals who are so
expectations that effort leads to demonstration certain that their ability is low that they have
of high rather than low ability. The derivation relinquished commitment to avoiding dem-
of performance predictions therefore depends onstration of low ability. Such individuals
on the previous statements of expectations of should avoid becoming ego involved with the
demonstrating ability as a function of task dif- tasks in question. In this case, effort is em-
ficulty. Ego involvement is considered first and ployed only to the degree that other incentives
then is contrasted with task involvement. appear contingent on it and even perception
of the possibility of demonstrating high ability
Performance in Ego Involvement should not produce high effort. The term
Predictions. According to the intentional learned helplessness might be used here, al-
framework, effort should be high (and produce though in practice it has been applied to any
effective performance or attainment) when it performance impairment consequent on per-
is perceived that high effort is necessary for ceived noncontingency of action and outcome
demonstration of high ability in the differ- (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978).
338 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

Evidence of the first (Arkin et al., 1982; conception of ability as capacity (Nicholls,
Brockner, 1979) and last effects (Marecek & 1978). On the other hand, sixth graders who
Mettee, 1972) exists. Many studies do not dis- had not yet mastered this conception per-
tinguish the different levels of low perceived formed similarly in moderate, high-difficulty,
ability or the effects predicted to be associated and control conditions. The two groups had
with them. Nevertheless, because performance similar levels of perceived ability, but expec-
impairment is predicted for all levels of low tations of demonstrating low ability only im-
perceived ability (on normatively moderately paired the performance of those who had at-
difficult tasks), such studies can be used to test tained the differentiated conception. These
the more general predictions of performance. findings support the proposed role of expec-
Furthermore, it is likely that few individuals tations of demonstrating low ability—as op-
with the lowest level of perceived ability would posed to mere expectancies of failure (Carver
enter or survive college long enough to find & Scheier, 1981) or perceived noncontingency
their way into subject pools. Thus, it is as- (Abramson et al., 1978)—and, more specifi-
sumed that they play a negligible role in effects cally, of the conception of ability as capacity.
obtained with students simply identified as low Task difficulty, perceived ability, and per-
in perceived ability. formance: Evidence. Returning to individual
When low-perceived-ability individuals face differences under ego involvement, it is pre-
normatively easy or difficult tasks, they expect dicted that high-perceived-ability individuals
to avoid demonstrating low ability, and self- perform their worst on tasks perceived as nor-
derogatory evaluations do not occur. Effort is matively easy, whereas low-perceived-ability
high on easy tasks where failure—which would individuals perform their worst at moderate-
indicate low ability—appears avoidable by difficulty levels. Furthermore, low-perceived-
high effort. This effort and, thus, performance ability individuals perform better than those
is higher for low- than for high-perceived-abil- with high perceived ability on tasks perceived
ity individuals who expect an easier success as normatively easy, whereas this is reversed
on easy tasks. If the task is seen as normatively at intermediate-difficulty levels. These predic-
difficult, high effort will (except at the lowest tions resemble those of Revelle & Michaels's
level of perceived ability) be forthcoming be- (1976) revision of Atkinson's theory."
cause its absence would be inconsistent with The following studies are relevant to the
commitment to demonstration of high ability. above hypotheses in that normative difficulty
Accordingly, performance of low-perceived- cues were manipulated and ego-involving ma-
ability individuals is predicted to be higher nipulations, such as presentation of tasks as
when tasks are perceived as normatively easy tests of intelligence, were employed. Kara-
or difficult rather than moderate. benick and Youssef (1968) found that low-
Expectations of demonstrating ability and resultant-achievement-motive students per-
performance: Evidence. According to the formed most poorly and below high-motive
preceding predictions, expectations of failure students at an intermediate normative diffi-
impair performance to the extent that failure culty level. High-motive students performed
indicates low ability in the differentiated sense. worst at an easy level, but this performance
Part of this thesis is supported by Frankel and was not, as predicted, below that of low-motive
Snyder (1978). They contrived a series of fail- students. Comparing conditions of easy and
ures calculated to make subjects doubt their intermediate normative difficulty, Kukla
ability and then presented tasks perceived as (1974) found all relevant predicted effects.12
normatively difficult or moderate. Perfor-
mance was not impaired in the difficult con-
'' Like the original theory, this revision holds that task
dition where expectancies of failure and per- difficulty and subjective probability of success can be
ceived noncontingency would be higher. As equated. Because they are not equivalent, the predictions
predicted, performance was only impaired in of the theory are ambiguous. In this case, I assume that
the moderately difficult condition where failure Revelle and Michaels's predictions refer to normative task
difficulty—the factor manipulated in relevant studies (c.f.
would indicate low ability. Miller (1982) rep- Footnotes 7 and 10).
licated these findings (in an evaluative situa- 12
Kukla employed the Mehrabian (1969) Resultant
tion) with sixth graders who had mastered the Achievement Motivation Scale. High scores on this scale
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 339

I. G. Sarason (1961) found test-anxious lowered. When feedback indicates above-av-


(low-perceived-ability) students' anagram per- erage performance, these individuals should
formance lower at moderate to moderately low (if not certain their ability is low) believe that
difficulty than at high-difficulty levels. Low- they are demonstrating high ability and apply
anxious students performed better with mod- high effort to maintain this perception. (If
erate- than with high-difficulty instructions. subjects are certain they have low ability, suc-
These results accord with predictions for the cess feedback should not produce high effort.)
anxious, but not the low-anxious students for Feedback indicating below-average perfor-
whom similar performance is expected in each mance would violate high-perceived-ability
case. However, the task was presented as an individuals' expectancies and produce high ef-
intelligence measure only in the moderate-dif- fort and performance. High normative feed-
ficulty condition. This could account for low- back should confirm their high perceived abil-
anxious students' higher performance in the ity, imply that less effort is necessary for dem-
moderate- than the high-difficulty condition. onstration of high ability, and lead to lower
S. B. Sarason, Mandler, and Craighill (1952) performance.
found no difference between high- and low- Results consistent with all the above pre-
anxious students in a normatively difficult dictions (except that in parentheses, which was
condition, whereas low-anxious students out not tested) were obtained by Weiner (1966)
performed those high in anxiety in an easy who told high- and low-resultant-motive sub-
condition. If the easy task was seen as mod- jects that they were doing much better or worse
erately easy, these results accord with predic- than most others during performance on a
tions. Description of a task as highly difficult task described as a test of general ability. These
produced higher performance in test-anxious findings were replicated by Weiner and
students than did a neutral condition where Schneider (1971). Perez (1973) and Schalon
difficulty was presumably seen as moderate (1968) examined performance of high- and
(I. G. Sarason, 1958). As predicted, low-anx- low-self-esteem students after they had been
ious students' performance was best in the lat- told that their initial intelligence test perfor-
ter condition. Though not perfectly consistent, mance was below average. High-self-esteem
most findings support the present predictions. students' performance improved. Performance
Feedback and performance: Evidence. Nor- of those with low self-esteem declined slightly.
mative difficulty cues are important in that Thus, the studies of effects of performance
they affect expectancies of demonstrating abil- feedback are consistent with predictions.
ity in the differentiated sense. Normative feed- The further prediction that individuals who
back during performance indicates the level are certain their ability is low would avoid
of ability the individual is likely to demonstrate demonstrating ability was confirmed by Ma-
with much less ambiguity. It is also likely to recek and Mettee (1972). When told they had
increase or maintain ego involvement. There- displayed above-average ability, subjects with
fore, studies using normative feedback provide high and low self-concepts, but not those with
better controlled tests of predictions of per- low self-concepts who were certain of this
formance in ego involvement. evaluation, showed improvement on a retest.
When, in ego involvement, feedback indi- The additional finding that the latter individ-
cates that performance is below that of others, uals improved when performance was pre-
low-perceived-ability individuals should expect sented as dependent on luck rather than on
to appear incompetent and experience anxiety ability supports the view that, in the skill con-
or reduce effort. Performance would thus be dition, they avoided demonstrating high ability.
There was, in this study, no evidence of the
diminished performance predicted for high-
are gained by asserting a preference for challenging skill perceived-ability subjects after success feed-
tasks. Thus, the study can be seen as indicating that those back. However, ego involvement was probably
who prefer such tasks perform better on them, whereas not aroused on the pretest because the initially
those who do not prefer such tasks perform better on easy
tasks. In this light, the concept resultant achievement mo- stated purpose of the session was to determine
tivation is marginally relevant to Kukla's results. With this group baseline performance levels. Thus, a de-
caveat, the results are consistent with present predictions. cline in performance would be unlikely.
340 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

In this section, predictions for individuals (in ego involvement) learning is a means to
with low perceived ability were confirmed with an end is unlikely to impair performance. In-
considerable consistency. The expected di- deed, a concern with scoring as high as possible
minished performance of high-perceived-abil- could lead to item-selection strategies that
ity individuals after success feedback was not would increase scores at the expense of learn-
found in one of three cases. Variation in degree ing (Harter, 1978). However, task involvement,
of ego involvement was probably implicated where learning is more an end in itself, should
in this exception. In summary, the better con- be superior to ego involvement in sustaining
trolled tests of this section of the theory sup- significant real-world achievements such as the
port it. development of logical thinking (Piaget, 1960)
and original scientific thought (T. S. Kuhn,
Performance: Task Involvement 1968). Here, individuals must themselves de-
Versus Ego Involvement tect problems or inconsistencies in their own
Predictions. When individuals are task in- logic or in scientific thought. Endogenous at-
volved, effort reflects the extent to which it tribution appears likely to foster sensitivity to
appears likely to produce improved mastery such problems (Condry & Chambers, 1978)
or mastery of tasks that individuals are un- and to maintain the necessary long-term in-
certain about their ability to master. If indi- volvement (Campbell, 1960; D. Kuhn, 1974).
viduals believe that high effort is necessary to Effort would be less consistent over long pe-
produce improvement they apply high effort. riods and sensitivity to contradictions would
Effort, and thus performance, is predicted to be reduced by concern with one's score (Con-
be lower if they believe that little effort is dry & Chambers, 1978) when, as in ego in-
needed or that high effort has no effect. When volvement, learning is a means to an end.
individuals differ in the level of difficulty at Task involvement versus ego involvement:
which they expect gains in mastery, they will Evidence. Here I review studies where brief
correspondingly differ in the level of difficulty experimental tasks were presented as mod-
at which they perform most effectively. How- erately normatively difficult or allowed a wide
ever, provided difficulty levels (normative or range of scores and would, therefore, allow the
objective) are not extreme, all or most indi- possibility of demonstrating high or low ca-
viduals will expect to be able to gain in mas- pacity. (See Footnote 6.) In such cases, ego
tery. As this is an end in itself, they should involvement is predicted to impair perfor-
apply high effort to maximize their mastery mance for those with low perceived ability so
and perceived ability. Consequently, they that they perform more poorly than when task
should perform effectively. These predictions involved and more poorly than high-perceived-
resemble those of Kukla (1972) more than ability individuals in task or ego involvement.
those of Atkinson. There are strong reasons for not predicting
Many studies that compare ego- and task- diminished performance in ego involvement
involving conditions enable testing of these for high-perceived-ability individuals and
predictions and of the differences between task weaker reasons for expecting them to perform
involvement and ego involvement. High-per- better in ego than task involvement. In the
ceived-ability ego-involved individuals' con- following studies, a variety of manipulations
cern to perform well compared to others, and that should induce ego involvement were
their expectancy of being able to do so, was compared with more neutral conditions.
predicted to maintain intense effort and ef- Entin and Raynor (1973) compared high-
fective performance on tasks of moderate nor- and low-resultant-motive students in testlike
mative difficulty. For low-perceived-ability in- and in neutral conditions.13 High-motive sub-
dividuals facing moderate normative difficulty
levels, impaired performance is predicted in 13
Entin and Raynor (1973) characterize the ego-in-
ego involvement but not in task involvement. volving condition as one where opportunity to work on
The preceding predictions apply to exper- later problems is contingent on success on the test. However,
this, but not the neutral condition, used social comparison
imental settings where tasks are of relatively norms and was testlike. As identical results occur when
short duration and task requirements are there is no reference to a contingent future task, this aspect
clearly specified. In these cases, the fact that of the methodology is of uncertain relevance.
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 341

jects performed better under ego than task in- anxious subjects (Ganzer, 1968), but not in
volvement, whereas the reverse occurred for high-perceived-ability and low-anxious sub-
low-resultant-motive students. Similar findings jects.
were obtained by Raynor and Rubin (1971). There is, then, good evidence of the pre-
Gjeseme (1974) obtained identical results with dicted effects of ego versus task involvement
anagrams presented as a school test versus a when difficulty is perceived as moderate,
type of problem students would be tested on problems are clearly defined, and time periods
one year later. are relatively short. Compared to task involve-
Presentations of tasks as tests (usually of ment, ego involvement produces lower per-
intelligence) have also been compared with formance in low-perceived-ability individuals
neutral conditions with high- and low-test- and equal or higher performance in high-per-
anxious students. Studies that have reported ceived-ability individuals.
(a) the presently predicted poorer performance There appears to be no relevant experi-
for anxious students in test than in neutral mental evidence with tasks involving problem
conditions and (b) no condition effect for low- finding or the development of logical thought.
anxious students are Paul and Eriksen (1964), However, Amabile (1979) found that an eval-
I. G. Sarason (1959), and S. B. Sarason, Man- uative condition lowered artistic creativity.
dler, and Craighill (1952). I. G. Sarason and Furthermore, evidence that individuals who
Minard (1962) obtained this result for com- make outstanding creative achievements or
prehension but not for three other tests. With who are more successful in school and in sci-
a digit-symbol task, I. G. Sarason and Palola ence are distinguished from others by higher
(1960) also found lower performance for anx- levels of task involvement rather than ego in-
ious students and higher performance for low- volvement (Spence & Helmreich, 1983; Nich-
anxious students in an intelligence test con- olls, 1979b) provides support for the present
dition. (With very easily discriminable sym- position.
bols, anxious students, as expected, scored
higher than did others in the test condition.)
Other Theories of Performance
Russell and I. G. Sarason (1965), however,
found no effect of testlike instructions on ana- A major difference between the present the-
gram performance. Given the number of fac- ory and those of Atkinson (1965; Atkinson &
tors that could lead to failure to demonstrate Raynor, 1974) and Kukla (1972) is that the
the expected effects (e.g., tasks could appear present one distinguishes and makes separate
easy), the above studies favor the hypothesis predictions for task and for ego involvement.
of impaired performance at moderate diffi- The importance of this distinction is sustained
culty levels in low- but not in high-perceived- by the evidence reviewed above. (This is not
ability individuals in ego-involving conditions. to say that further distinctions are unnecessary;
Also consistent with present predictions is e.g., Maehr, 1983, and Roberts, in press.)
Perez's (1973) finding that an intelligence test The present theory also distinguishes nor-
condition led to lower performance for low- mative difficulty and expectancies of success.
self-esteem subjects than did a neutral pre- They are imperfectly distinguished by Kukla,
sentation, whereas high-self-esteem subjects whose predictions are framed in terms of ob-
performed similarly in each condition. Sim- jective difficulty that cannot be specified as
ilarly, induction of self-awareness produced precisely as can normative difficulty. This leads
poorer performance in low-self-esteem subjects to inconsistent interpretations of data. At one
than did a task focus (Brockner, 1979; Brock- point, Kukla (1972) presents as consistent with
ner &Hulton, 1978). High-self-esteem subjects his theory, evidence that low-resultant-motive
were not affected by conditions, and task focus individuals perform better when half of their
produced similar performance in high- and in peers are predicted to fail than when the odds
low-self-esteem subjects. Also as predicted, the of success are clearly higher or lower (Atkinson,
task condition reduced anxiety in low-self-es- 1958). At another point, evidence (Kukla,
teem subjects (Brockner, 1979). The presence 1974) that low-resultant-motive individuals
of observers also lowered performance in low- perform better on a normatively easy than on
perceived-ability (Shrauger, 1972) and in test- a moderately difficult task is held to support
342 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

his theory. Such inconsistency is unlikely when and evidence reviewed here show, performance
predictions are framed in terms of normative- impairment is reduced in task-involved states.
difficulty cues. The differences between the Carver and associates have demonstrated sim-
present position and Kukla's stem largely from ilar effects (Carver, Blaney, & Scheier, 1979;
his use of the less differentiated conception of Carver, Peterson, Follansbee, & Scheier, 1983;
ability. Consequently, the predictions of Ku- Carver & Scheier, 1981), as has Kuhl (1981).
kla's theory resemble present predictions for This very brief comparison of these different
task involvement. There is no place in his for- positions indicates that the present one deals
mulation for the various performance im- relatively effectively with the evidence on per-
pairment and enhancement effects that occur formance on tasks of relatively short duration,
in ego involvement. where the problem to be solved is specified
The predictions of Atkinson's theory as and major cognitive restructuring is not re-
modified by Revelle and Michaels (1976) are quired.
more compatible with the present predictions
for ego involvement. It seems that in Revelle Conclusion
and Michaels's revision, as in the present the-
ory, low-resultant-motive (or low-perceived- I have focused on task choice and perfor-
ability) individuals would be predicted to mance in experimental settings because these
perform their worst at moderate normative- topics have long been of interest to researchers
difficulty levels and high-perceived-ability in- and because data enabling comparisons with
dividuals would be expected to perform their previous theories were available. The follow-
worst at easy and at extremely difficult tasks. ing examples illustrate the applicability of
However, as Revelle and Michaels note (p. the approach to other achievement-related
400), their position does not predict higher phenomena.
performance in low- than in nigh-resultant- First, R. Ames (1983) has analyzed students'
motive subjects after feedback indicating requests for academic assistance in terms of
above-average performance (Weiner, 1966; (a) ego involvement versus task involvement
Weiner & Schneider, 1971), whereas this the- and (b) individual differences in perceived
ory does. More critical, however, is the failure ability. He proposed that in ego-involving
to deal with task involvement where perfor- conditions, students with low perceived ability
mance is often, as predicted, as good as or would be more likely than those with high
better than in ego involvement.14 perceived ability to see a request for assistance
The concept of learned helplessness has been as a demonstration of lack of capacity. Thus,
used to account for impaired performance. they would be less likely to seek assistance. In
Abramson et al. (1978) have distinguished task-involving conditions, on the other hand,
personal and universal helplessness. This par- Ames predicted that most students would view
allels the present distinction between percep- seeking assistance as a way of learning or dem-
tion of low capacity and perception of high onstrating ability in the less differentiated
normative difficulty. However, the claim that sense. When task involved, students with low
impaired performance is a consequence of perceived ability should, therefore, not avoid
perceived noncontingency of action and out- seeking assistance. Ames's (1983) review of
come embodies the less differentiated concep- the available evidence supports his predictions.
tion of ability. Bandura's (1977) distinction Second, there has been considerable interest
between lack of a sense of efficacy and belief in the nature of the associations of attributions
in an unresponsive environment also suggests of effort, ability, and other factors with different
the differentiated conception of ability. How-
ever, when Bandura claims that increased per- 14
It might be argued that task-involving situations are
ception of personal efficacy follows improve- not achievement situations. The problem then would be
ments in mastery (p. 195), the less differen- to explain why individuals perform effectively and choose
tiated conception is implied. But, neither challenging tasks in these situations. This position would
also have the anomalous implication that the motivation
position explicitly distinguishes the concep- that makes the most distinctive contribution to outstanding
tions of ability or makes different predictions creative achievement is not a form of achievement mo-
for task and for ego involvement. As the theory tivation.
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 343

affects (e.g., Weiner, Russell, &Lerman, 1978). Ames, C., Ames, R., & Felker, D. (1977). Effects of com-
In this context, researchers have assumed that petitive reward structure and valence of outcome on
children's achievement attributions. Journal of Edu-
the meanings of effort and ability are fixed. cational Psychology, 69, 1-8.
However, because the meaning of effort and Ames, C., & McKelvie, S. (1983, April). Achievement at-
ability can change, the links between affect tributions and self-instructions under competitive and
and perceived effort and ability may also individualistic goal structures. Paper presented at the
meeting of the American Educational Research Asso-
change. For example, in ego-involving con- ciation, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
texts, Jagacinski and Nicholls (in press) found Ames, R. (1983). Help-seeking and achievement orien-
perception of lower effort associated with guilt tation: Perspectives from attribution theory. B. M.
and perception of higher effort (implying lower DePaulo, A. Nadler, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), New directions
capacity) associated with embarrassment. In in helping (pp. 165-188). New \brk: Academic Press.
Arkin, R. M., Detchon, C. S., & Maruyama, G. M. (1982).
task-involving contexts—where effort and Roles of attribution, affect, and cognitive interference
ability are imperfectly differentiated—percep- in test anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
tion of lower effort was associated with in- chology, 43, 1111-1124.
creases in both guilt and embarrassment. This Arkin, R. M., Kolditz, T. A., & Kolditz, K. K. (1983).
Attributions of the test anxious student: Self-assessments
was expected because lower effort generally in the classroom. Personality and Social Psychology
implies lower ability in task-involving contexts. Bulletin, 9, 271-280.
Thus, the present approach provides new in- Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-
sights on achievement-related affect. taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359-372.
Finally, the conceptions of ability that play Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of
human motives in terms of motives, expectancies, and
a central role in this theory also play an im- incentives. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy,
portant role in the development of achieve- action, and society (pp. 288-305). New York: Van Nos-
ment motivation. As the conception of ability trand.
as capacity develops, children's achievement Atkinson, J. W. (1965). The mainsprings of achievement-
oriented activity. In J. D. Krumboltz (Ed.), Learning
affect and overt behavior increasingly approx- and the educational process (pp. 25-66). Chicago: Rand
imates that of adults when both are in ego- McNally.
involving situations (Nicholls & Miller, in Atkinson, J. W. (1969). Comments on papers by Crandall
press). Whereas young children and adults dif- and Veroff. In C. P. Smith (Ed.), Achievement-related
motives in children (pp. 200-206). New York: Russell
fer in ego-involving situations, they react sim- Sage.
ilarly in task-involving situations. There is also Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N. T. (Eds.). (1966). A theory
evidence that, as children progress through of achievement motivation. New York: Wiley.
the grades, school environments generally be- Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. Q (Eds.). (1974). Personality,
come more ego involving (Eccles, Midgley, & motivation, and achievement. New \brk: Hemisphere.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory
Adler, in press). Thus, there is a close inter- of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-
dependence between the present approach to 215.
adult achievement motivation and research on Bloom, B. S. (1976). Human characteristics and school
developmental change in achievement moti- learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.
vation. Bolles, R. C. (1974). Cognition and motivation: Some his-
torical trends. In B. Weiner (Ed.), Cognitive views of
human motivation (pp. 1-20). New York: Academic
References Press.
Brockner, J. (1979). Self-esteem, self-consciousness, and
task performance: Replications, extensions, and possible
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D.
explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
(1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and
chology, 37, 447-461.
reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87,49-74.
Arnabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluation on Brockner, J., & Hulton, A. J. B. (1978). How to reverse
artistic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- the vicious cycle of low self-esteem: The importance of
chology, 37, 221-233. attentional focus. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Ames, C. (1978). Children's achievement attributions and chology, 14, 564-578.
self-reinforcement: Effects of self-concept and compet- Brody, N. (1963). «-Achievement, test anxiety and sub-
itive reward structure. Journal of Educational Psy- jective probability of success in risk taking behavior.
chology, 70, 345-355. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 413-
Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1981). Competitive versus indi- 418.
vidualistic goal structures: The salience of past perfor- Buckert, U., Meyer, W-U., & Schmalt, H-D. (1979). Effects
mance information for causal attributions and affect. of difficulty and diagnosticity on choice among tasks in
Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 411-418. relation to achievement motivation and perceived ability.
344 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

Journal of'Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1172- opment of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI
1178. Press.
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective re- Elkind, D. (1971). Cognitive growth cycles in mental de-
tention in creative thought as in other knowledge pro- velopment. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium
cesses. Psychological Review, 67, 380-400. on Motivation (pp. 1-31). Lincoln: University of Ne-
Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (1979). Reas- braska Press.
sertion and giving up: The interactive role of self-directed Entin, E. E., & Raynor, J. Q (1973). Effects of contingent
attention and outcome expectancy. Journal of Personality future orientation and achievement motivation on per-
and Social Psychology, 37, 1859-1870. formance in two kinds of task. Journal of Experimental
Carver, C. S., Peterson, L. M., Follansbee, D. J., & Scheier, Research in Personality, 6, 314-320.
J. F. (1983). Effects of self-directed attention on per- Feather, N. T, & Simon, J. G. (1971). Attribution of re-
formance and persistence among persons high and low sponsibility and valence of outcome in relation to initial
in test anxiety. Cognitive Research and Therapy, 7, 333- confidence and success and failure of self and other.
354. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 173-
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- 188.
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. Frankel, A., & Snyder, M. L. (1978). Poor performance
New York: Springer-Verlag. following unsolvable problems: Learned helplessness or
Condry, J. D., & Chambers, J. (1978). Intrinsic motivation egotism? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
and the process of learning. In M. R. Lepper & D. 36, 628-634.
Greene (Eds.), The hidden costs of reward: New per- Ganzer, V. J. (1968). Effects of audience presence and test
spectives on the psychology of human motivation (pp. anxiety on learning and retention in a serial learning
61-84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Crandall, R. (1973). The measurement of self-esteem and 8, 194-199.
related constructs. In J. P. Robinson & P. R. Shaver Garcia, J. (1981). Tilting at the paper mills of academe.
(Eds.), Measures of social psychological attitudes (pp. American Psychologist, 36, 149-158.
45-168). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Gjesme, T. (1974). Goal distance in time and its effects
Crandall, V. J., Katkovsky, W., & Preston, A. (1960). A on the relations between achievement motives and per-
conceptual formulation for some research on children's formance. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 161-
achievement development. Child Development, 31, 787- 171.
797. Hamilton, J. O. (1974). Motivation and risk-taking be-
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1977). Beyond boredom and anxiety. havior: A test of Atkinson's theory. Journal of Personality
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. and Social Psychology, 29, 856-864.
DeCharms, R., & Carpenter, V. (1968). Measuring mo- Harlow, H. F. (1950). Learning and satiation of response
tivation in culturally disadvantaged school children. In in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance
H. J. Klausmeier and G. T. O'Hearn (Eds.), Research by monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
and development toward the improvement of education Psychology, 43, 289-294.
(pp. 31 -41). Madison, WI: Dembar Education Services. Harter, S. (1978). Pleasure derived from challenge and the
DeCharms, R., & Dave, P. N. (1965). Hope of success, effects of receiving grades on children's difficulty level
fear of failure, subjective probability, and risk-taking choices. Child Development, 49, 788-799.
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Heckhausen, H. (1967). The anatomy of achievement mo-
1, 558-568. tivation. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum. Heckhausen, H. (1968). Achievement motive research:
Deci, E. L., Betley, G., Kahle, J., Abrams, L., & Porac, Current problems and some contributions toward a
J. (1981). When trying to win: Competition and intrinsic general theory of motivation. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.),
motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 103-174).
7, 79-83. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical explo- Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its
ration of intrinsic motivational processes. In L. Ber- constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion,
kowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology 1, 283-329.
(Vol. 13, pp. 39-80). New York: Academic Press. Heckhausen, H. (in press). Emergent achievement behavior:
Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, Some early developments. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The
R. M. (1981). An instrument to assess adults' orien- development of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT:
tations toward control versus autonomy with children: JAI Press.
Reflections on intrinsic motivation and perceived com- Hetherington, E. M., & Parke, R. D. (1975). Child psy-
petence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 642- chology: A contemporary viewpoint. New Vbrk: McGraw-
650. Hill.
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays Hoffman, C., Mischel, W., & Mazze, K. (1981). The role
on mind and psychology. Montgomery, VT: Bradford. of purpose in the organization of information about
Diener, E., & Srull, T. K. (1979). Self-awareness, psycho- behavior: Trait-based versus goal-based categories in
logical perspective, and self-reinforcement in relation person cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
to personal and social standards. Journal of Personality chology, 40, 211-225.
and Social Psychology, 37, 413-423. Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (in press). Conceptions
Eccles, J., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. (in press). Age-related of ability and related affects in task involvement and
changes in the school environment: Effects on achieve- ego involvement. Journal of Educational Psychology.
ment motivation. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The devel- Jopt, J. U. (1974). Extrinsiche motivation and leistungs-
ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 345

verhalten. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ruhr McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1982). Impact of causal at-
University, Bochum, West Germany. tributions on affective reactions to success and failure.
Karabenick, S. A., & Youssef, Z. I, (1968). Performance Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 937-
as a function of achievement motive level and perceived 946.
difficulty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, McFarlin, D. B., & Blascovich, J. (1981). Effects of self-
10, 414-419. esteem and performance feedback on future affective
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and preferences and cognitive expectations. Journal of Per-
disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, sonality and Social Psychology, 40, 521-531.
82, 1-25. Mehrabian, A. (1969). Measures of achieving tendency.
Klinger, E., & McNelly, F. W. (1969'). Fantasy need Educational and Psychological Measurement, 29, 445-
achievement and performance: A role analysis. Psy- 451.
chological Review, 76, 574-591. Meyer, W-U., Folkes, V, & Weiner, B. (1976). The perceived
Kruglanski, A. W. (1975). The endogenous-exogenous informational value and affective consequences of choice
partition in attribution theory. Psychological Review, behavior and intermediate difficulty task selection.
82, 387-406. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 410-423.
Kuril, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: Miller, A. T. (1982). Self-recognitory schemes and achieve-
The moderating effect of state versus action orientation. ment behavior: A developmental study. Unpublished
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155- doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.
170. Morris, L. W., Davis, M. A., & Hutchings, C. H. (1981).
Kuhn, D. (1974). Inducing development experimentally: Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: Lit-
Comments on a research paradigm. Developmental erature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale.
Psychology, 10, 590-600. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 541-555.
Kuhn, T. S. (1968). A function for thought experiments. Moulton, R. W. (1965). Effects of success and failure on
Ontario Journal of Educational Research, 10, 211-231. level of aspiration as related to achievement motives.
Kukla, A. (1972). Foundations of an attributional theory Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 399-
of performance. Psychological Review, 79, 454-470. 406.
Kukla, A. (1974). Performance as a function of resultant Nicholls, J. G. (1976). When a scale measures more than
achievement motivation (perceived ability) and perceived its name denotes: The case of the Test Anxiety Scale
difficulty. Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 374- for Children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
383. chology, 44, 976-985.
Kukla, A. (1978). An attributional theory of choice. In Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts
L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social of effort and ability, perception of own attainment, and
psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 113-144). New \brk: Academic the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability.
Press. Child Development, 49, 800-814.
Lepper, M. R., & Greene, D. (1975). Turning play into Nicholls, J. G. (1979a). Development of perception of own
work: Effects of adult surveillance and extrinsic rewards attainment and causal attributions for success and failure
on children's intrinsic motivation, Journal of Personality in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71,94-99.
and Social Psychology, 31, 479-486. Nicholls, J. G. (1979b). Quality and equality in intellectual
Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and development: The role of motivation in education.
emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction American Psychologist, 34, 1071-1084.
and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20, 975- Nicholls, J. G. (1980). The development of the concept
978. of difficulty. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 271-281.
Maehr, M. L. (1983). On doing well in science: Why Johnny Nicholls, J. G., Licht, B., & Pearl, R. (1982). Some dangers
no longer excels; Why Sarah never did. In S. G. Paris, of using personality questionnaires to study personality.
G. M. Olson, & H. W. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and Psychological Bulletin, 92, 572-580.
motivation in the classroom (pp. 179-210). Hillsdale, Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1983). The differentiation
NJ: Erlbaum. of the concepts of difficulty and ability. Child Devel-
Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and opment, 54, 951-959.
achievement motivation: A second look. In N. Warren Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (in press). Development
(Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. and its discontents: The differentiation of the concept
221-267. New York: Academic Press. of ability. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of
Mahone, C. H. (1960). Fear of failure and unrealistic vo- achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
cational aspiration. Journal of Abnormal and Social Patten, R. L., & White, L. A. (1977). Independent effects
Psychology, 60, 253-261. of achievement motivation and overt attribution on
Marecek, J., & Mettee, D. R. (1972). Avoidance of con- achievement behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 1,39-59.
tinued success as a function of self-esteem, level of self- Paul, G. L., & Eriksen, C. W. (1964). Effects of test anxiety
esteem certainty, and responsibility for success. Journal on "real-life" examinations. Journal of Personality, 32,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 98-107. 480-494.
McClelland, D. C. (1958). Risk taking in children with Perez, R. C. (1973). The effect of experimentally induced
high and low need for achievement. In J. W. Atkinson failure, self-esteem, and sex on cognitive differentiation.
(Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society (pp. 306- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, 74-79.
321). New York: Van Nostrand. Piaget, J. (1960). The general problems of the psycho-
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W, Clark, R. W., & Lowell, biological development of the child. In J. M. Tanner &
E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Ap- B. Inhelder (Eds.), Discussions on child development
pleton-Century Crofts. (Vol. 4, pp. 3-27). London: Tavistock.
346 JOHN G. NICHOLLS

Raynor, J. Q, & Rubin, I. S. (1971). Effects of achievement Schalon, C. L. (1968). Effect of self-esteem upon perfor-
motivation and future orientation on level of perfor- mance following failure stress. Journal of Consulting
mance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and Clinical Psychology, 32, 497.
17, 36-41. Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1983). Self-directed attention
Raynor, J. Q, & Smith, C. P. (1966). Achievement related and the comparison of self with standards. Journal of
motives and risk-taking in games of skill and chance. Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 205-222.
Journal of Personality, 34, 176-198. Schneider, K. (1973). Motivation under Erfolgsriskia.
Revelle, W., & Michaels, E. J. (1976). The theory of Gottingen, West Germany: Hogrefe.
achievement motivation revisited: The implication of Sears, P. S. (1940). Levels of aspiration in academically
inertial tendencies. Psychological Review, 83, 394-404. successful and unsuccessful children. Journal of Ab-
Roberts, G. C. (1974). Effect of achievement and social normal and Social Psychology, 35, 498-536.
environment on risk-taking. Research Quarterly, 45,42-55. Sears, P. S. (1941). Levels of aspiration in relation to some
Roberts, G. C. (in press). Children's achievement moti- variables of personality: Clinical studies. Journal of So-
vation in sport. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development cial Psychology, 14, 311-336.
of achievement motivation. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Shrauger, J. S. (1972). Self-esteem and reactions to being
Roberts, G. C., Kleiber, D. A., & Duda, J. L. (1981). An observed by others. Journal of Personality and Social
analysis of motivation in children's sport: The role of Psychology, 23, 192-200.
perceived competence in participation. Journal of Sport Shrauger, J. S. (1975). Responses to evaluation as a function
Psychology, 3, 206-216. of initial self-perceptions. Psychological Bulletin, 82,
Russell, D. G., & Sarason, I. G. (1965). Test anxiety, sex, 581-596.
and experimental conditions in relation to anagram so- Simon, J. G., & Feather, N. T. (1973). Causal attributions
lution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, for success and failure at university examinations. Jour-
493-496. nal of Educational Psychology, 64, 46-56.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the in- Spence, J. X, & Helmreich, R. L. (1983). Achievement-
trapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation related motives and behavior. In J. X Spence (Ed.),
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Achievement and achievement motives: Psychological and
43, 450-461. sociological approaches (pp. 10-74). San Francisco:
Sarason, I. G. (1958). The effects of anxiety, reassurance, Freeman.
and meaningfulness of material to be learned in verbal Trope, Y. (1979). Uncertainty-reducing properties of
learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 472- achievement tasks. Journal of Personality and Social
477. Psychology, 37, 1505-1518.
Sarason, I. G. (1959). Relationships of measures of anxiety Valle, V. A., & Frieze, I. H. (1976). Stability of causal
and experimental instructions to word association test attributions as a mediator in changing expectations of
performance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
chology, 59, 37-42. 33, 579-587.
Sarason, I. G. (1961). The effects of anxiety and threat Weiner, B. (1966). The role of success and failure in the
on solution of a difficult task. Journal of Abnormal and learning of easy and complex tasks. Journal of Person-
Social Psychology, 62, 165-168. ality and Social Psychology, 3, 339-343.
Sarason, I. G. (1975). Anxiety and self-preoccupation. In Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1978). Affective
I. G. Sarason & C. D. Spielberger, (Eds.), Stress and consequences of causal ascriptions. In J. H. Harvey,
anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 27-44). Washington, DC: Wiley- W. J. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in
Hemisphere. attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 59-90). Hillsdale, NJ:
Sarason, I. G., & Minard, J. (1962). Test anxiety, exper- Erlbaum.
imental instructions and the Wechslcr Adult Intelligence Weiner, B., & Schneider, K. (1971). Drive versus cognitive
Scale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 299-302. theory: A reply to Boor and Harman. Journal of Per-
Sarason, I. G., & Palola, E. G. (I960). The relationship sonality and Social Psychology, 18, 285-263.
of test and general anxiety, difficulty of task, and ex- White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept
perimental instructions to performance. Journal of Ex- of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297-333.
perimental Psychology, 59, 185-191. Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention.
Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92-104.
Sarason, S. B., Mandler, G., & Craighill, P. G. (1952). The
effect of differential instructions on anxiety and learning.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 561- Received September 19, 1983
565. Revision received December 7, 1983 •

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen