Sie sind auf Seite 1von 1

The Effect of Bitter Taste on

The Bitter Bias:


Trombetta, Gatley, Ryan, & Anastasio Ambiguous Target Appraisals
Introduction
Method
Embodied cognition is centered around the idea that as a way to process
• The study included 109 participants recruited from a northeastern liberal arts university as part of the research subject pool.
and structure mental processes, many thoughts, feelings, and
• Participants were randomly assigned to drink approximately one ounce of clear liquid that was either bitter, salty, sweet, or neutral
conceptualizations manifest themselves in ways in which the body interacts
(water) in flavor and were then instructed to read a short story about a purposely ambiguous target, taking sips of their flavored
with the environment. Several findings have demonstrated that this has a
solution at interval times throughout the reading
distinct effect in how humans judge and perceive each other. For
• After a short distraction task, participants then rated the target of the short story on eight personality traits (likable, warm, forgiving,
exampleWilliams and Bargh (2008) showed that holding a warm or cold
courteous, honest, hostile, happy, and intelligent) on a six point Likert scale.
drink can influence one’s warmness or coldness perception of someone
• The first four personality traits (likable, warm, forgiving, courteous) were combined into an index of warmth (alpha = .812) and
else. It has even been shown that the position of a stranger’s photo on a
participant’s scores on this index were analyzed across all conditions.
computer screen can bias how likely people are to assume the religiousness
of that stranger (Meier, Hauser, Robinson, Friesen, & Schjeldahl, 2007).
The purpose of this study was to explore this connection so as to learn
more about complex human judgment and social perception. Based on the
common concept that a bitter person is often portrayed as someone who is
4 Results
upset about someone, something, or just acts in a jaded manner, it was
theorized that something that tastes bitter is also considered unpleasant 3.5 An ANOVA showed that there
and avoided. was a significant effect of
3 flavored solution type on how
warmly participants rated an
Hypothesis 2.5
ambiguous target F(3, 105) =

Warmness Rating
3.41; p = .020.
We hypothesized that those in the bitter taste sensation 2 Pairwise comparisons
condition would rate an ambiguous target more negatively c
c
identified that those in the
compared to the other flavor conditions. Additionally, we 1.5 bitter condition gave
predicted that a sweet taste would facilitate more positive significantly higher ratings of
ratings of the ambiguous target person. warmth (M = 3.26, SD = .98)
1 than those in the sweet (M =
2.55, SD = .78), p = .007, and
0.5
Materials the neutral/water condition
(M = 2.50, SD = .97), p = .006.
0
Flavored Solutions Bitter Sweet Water Salty
• Bitter solution was flat tonic water
• Salty and sweet solutions were created by adding salt/sugar to water Taste Condition
Ambiguous Target Prompt
• Adapted from Erdley and D’Agostino (1998) Discussion
“Over our University's Fall Break, my friend Emily asked me to go to the mall with her to shop for a dress to
wear to her uncle’s wedding. She drove us there in her grandmother’s car, which had a handicapped license
plate. When we got to the mall parking lot Emily passed by the many empty handicapped parking spaces
• Results show that our hypothesis that people in the bitter condition would rate the target less favorably was incorrect. Data
because she was afraid she would be fined for parking in one of those spaces. The rest of the parking lot was very actually support the opposite effect in that the target was rated as significantly warmer after a bitter taste sensation than a sweet
full. When Emily spotted an empty space two rows away, another car cut in front of her before she could pull in,
and Emily honked at the driver and gave them a dirty look. We entered the mall and went into the closest or neutral taste.
clothing store. Emily chose several dresses to try on. When the clerk asked if she could help her, Emily said no – • One potential explanation for the unexpected result is that the bitter taste could result in the participant think the opposite,
she didn’t need the advice of a stranger. I looked around the store while Emily tried on dresses. When Emily
came out she said that her dressing room had had someone’s purse in it, and she had heard its owner knocking similar to the “Macbeth effect” (Zhong & Liljenquist, 2006), where instead of biasing focus on the negative aspects, the bitter
on several nearby dressing room doors inquiring about her purse, so Emily had opened her room door and
handed the purse out to the woman. Just as Emily finished the story, the clerk approached us and asked Emily if
drink primed participants to view the ambiguous target in a more positive manner.
she had made a selection. Emily replied that she would have to go to a better store because this one just did not • Future research should focus on the influences bitter tastes can have on individuals. Particularly, if these findings are indicative
have the quality of dress that she wanted.
of bitter taste sensation activating opposite feelings, more research on the subject would prove valuable to the embodied
“As we walked down the mall talking to each other, Emily noticed a nice bracelet lying on one of the mall cognition literature.
benches. She went over and picked it up, talking excitedly about how pretty it was. Just then a security guard
approached and Emily asked the guard to take it to the lost and found. We walked farther down the mall, and
Emily went into a gift shop while I went to the music store. We met half an hour later, and Emily showed me the
vase she had bought as a wedding gift. She suddenly noticed a scratch on it, and the next thing I knew Emily
References
returned to the store, cut to the front of the line, and demanded her money back. We then walked to another
clothing store, and on the way we passed by a person who was collecting money for the heart fund. The person Erdley, C. A., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1998). Cognitive and affective components of automatic priming. Journal of
collecting money tried to make eye contact, but Emily looked away and kept on going. We continued walking Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 741-747.
around the mall, stopping at several stores to look for a dress. Emily suggested that we drive to the other end of Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., & Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What's 'up' with God? Vertical
the mall to go to a dress shop there because the posted 3-hour time limit for her parking space was just about space as a representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 699-710.
out anyway. When we got to that store Emily was lucky, as she found 3 dresses she liked. One cost $120, another
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal
$150, and the third $160. Emily had decided on the least expensive of the 3 because she had promised her warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606-607. doi:10.1126/science.1162548
mother that she would not pay any more than $140 for a dress. Calling the shopping trip a success, Emily drove
me home, and we agreed to go out together again soon.” Zhong, C., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). “Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing.
Science, 313(5782), 1451-1452. doi: 10.1126/science.1130726

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen