Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Child Development, July/August 2009, Volume 80, Number 4, Pages 1301–1313

A Good Story: Children With Imaginary Companions Create


Richer Narratives
Gabriel Trionfi Elaine Reese
Clark University University of Otago

In line with theories that children’s pretend play reflects and extends their narrative skills, children with
imaginary companions were predicted to have better narrative skills than children without imaginary com-
panions. Forty-eight 5½-year-old children and their mothers participated in interviews about children’s imagi-
nary companions. Children also completed language and narrative assessments. Twenty-three of the children
(48%) were deemed to have engaged in imaginary companion play. Children with and without imaginary
companions were similar in their vocabulary skills, but children with imaginary companions told richer nar-
ratives about a storybook and a personal experience compared to children without imaginary companions.
This finding supports theories of a connection between pretend play and storytelling by the end of early
childhood.

Imaginary companion play is captivating for chil- fied objects (Singer & Singer, 1990; Taylor, 1999).
dren who engage in it, for parents who are sur- Critical to both forms of pretend play is that chil-
prised by it, and for developmental researchers dren view their imaginary companion as a sepa-
who want to understand it. Early research often rate other. Harris (2000) characterized both types
attributed imaginary companion play to psychopa- of imaginary companion play as sustained forms
thology, personality defects, or deficiencies in social of role play. In both, children’s interactions with
skills (Ames & Learned, 1946; Svendsen, 1934). their imaginary companions are a form of simu-
Taylor (1999) argued that these early studies, taken lated social exchange. Indeed, the similarity
largely from clinical populations, were flawed between imaginary companions and real friend-
because they did not include comparison samples ships is a defining feature of this play: ‘‘As with
from nonclinical populations. Indeed, contempo- real friends, children play with their imaginary
rary wisdom is that imaginary companion play companions, pretend with them, involve them in
may even offer some developmental benefits for their daily routines and occasionally argue with
children (e.g., Bouldin & Pratt, 2002; Gleason, Seb- them’’ (Gleason, 2004a, p. 205; cf. Gleason et al.,
anc, & Hartup, 2000; Taylor & Carlson, 1997). In 2000; Taylor, 1999).
this study, we explore the potential developmental One’s definition of imaginary companions influ-
benefits of imaginary companion play for children’s ences one’s estimate of the number of children who
narrative skills. have an imaginary companion. Some researchers
Traditionally, the definition of an imaginary have reported imaginary companion play to be
companion was restricted to repeated play with engaged in by half of all children (e.g., Singer &
an invisible other (Svendsen, 1934), but later the Singer, 1990), whereas others using stricter defini-
definition of an imaginary companion expanded tions have reported that only one fifth of all chil-
to include certain instances of play with personi- dren have imaginary companions (Newson &
Newson, 1968). A longitudinal study revealed new
Gabriel Trionfi is currently with IDEO.
information about the occurrence of imaginary
The Marsden Fund of the Royal Society of New Zealand companion play over time (Taylor, Carlson,
funded this research. We thank Kathleen McMenamin, Smita Sri- Maring, Gerow, & Charley, 2004). Three- and
vastava, and the members of the Language and Memory team
for data collection, transcription, and coding. Special thanks go
four-year-olds were interviewed about their
to Marjorie Taylor and Eric Amsel for their insights throughout engagement in imaginary companion play, and the
the project. We are also grateful to the families for sharing with same children were interviewed again 3 years later.
us their imaginary friends and their stories, both imagined and
real.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Elaine Reese, Psychology Department, University of Otago, P.O.  2009, Copyright the Author(s)
Box 56, Dunedin, New Zealand. Electronic mail may be sent to Journal Compilation  2009, Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
ereese@psy.otago.ac.nz. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2009/8004-0026
1302 Trionfi and Reese

Using Taylor’s (1999) definition, a surprising 31% imaginary companion play do show an inclination
of 6- and 7-year-olds reported engaging in imagi- toward other forms of fantasy (Bouldin, 2006;
nary companion play, and 49% of children were Bouldin & Pratt, 1999; Taylor & Carlson, 1997),
identified as engaging in imaginary companion and 4-year-old children with imaginary compan-
play at some point across the duration of the study. ions show an advanced understanding of minds,
Even under the most stringent double-interview even after controlling for verbal ability (Taylor &
process, Taylor and Carlson (1997) still found a Carlson, 1997).
28% rate of occurrence of imaginary companion Language skills are another established correlate
play in their sample of one-hundred and fifty-two of imaginary companion play. Preschool boys with
3- to 4-year-olds. Despite some variations in esti- imaginary companions speak more and use longer
mating how many children engage in imaginary utterances than boys without imaginary compan-
companion play, it is clear that researchers have ions (Singer & Singer, 1990). Preschool children
been able to identify consistently two groups of with imaginary companions also have more
children: those who engage in imaginary compan- advanced receptive vocabularies than children
ion play at some point in development and those without imaginary companions (Taylor & Carlson,
who do not engage in imaginary companion play. 1997). Finally, children with imaginary companions
The question then becomes: How are children with demonstrate more complex syntax in their speech,
imaginary companions different from those chil- with higher rates of adverbial and relative clauses,
dren without imaginary companions? compared to children without imaginary compan-
Imaginary companion play is engaged in most ions (Bouldin, Bavin, & Pratt, 2002). These linguistic
often by firstborn children (Bouldin & Pratt, 1999; correlates of imaginary companion play may reflect
Gleason et al., 2000; Manosevitz, Prentice, & a broader and well-founded developmental connec-
Wilson, 1973) and is somewhat more common in tion between children’s play and language. From
the play of young girls than young boys (Carlson an early age, children’s symbolic play is linked to
& Taylor, 2005; Gleason, 2004a; Pearson et al., their language use (e.g., Shore, O’Connell, & Bates,
2001; Taylor & Carlson, 1997; but see Taylor 1984), although this link can at times be quite spe-
et al., 2004). Boys, in contrast, are more likely cific. For instance, Tamis-LeMonda and Bornstein
than girls to impersonate a character, another (1994) found a correlation between toddlers’ early
form of role play (Carlson & Taylor, 2005; cf. symbolic play and their later language, but only
Harris, 2000). Imaginary companion play is also with a measure of semantic diversity, not with total
positively related to children’s sociability, such productive vocabulary.
that children with imaginary companions are The main aim of this study is to extend prior
reported to have just as many or more real research on the language skills of children with and
friendships (Bouldin & Pratt, 1999; Gleason et al., without imaginary companions by moving into the
2000; Taylor, Cartwright, & Carlson, 1993) and realm of narrative. Play researchers theorize that
experience the same level of peer acceptance as children need to use explicit language to negotiate
children without imaginary companions (Gleason, meaning in the pretend scene (Bruner, 1996; Fein,
2004a). Adults who provide retrospective reports 1989; Pellegrini, 1985). These linguistic negotiations
of imaginary companions have a stronger orienta- are found in the co-construction of roles, settings,
tion toward others than adults who do not report goals, and conflicts (Göncü, 1993; Göncü &
having had an imaginary companion in childhood Katsarou, 2000), as well as in discussion of other
(Gleason, Jarudi, & Cheek, 2003). Moreover, chil- elements, such as the meaning of props used
dren with imaginary companions are no more or (Garvey, 1977). Such linguistic negotiations high-
less likely to be shy than children without imagi- light the similarities that theorists have noted
nary companions (Bouldin & Pratt, 2002), and between play and narrative in form and composi-
adults who report having had an imaginary com- tion, particularly in the use of characters, a scene, a
panion do not rate themselves as shyer than goal, and a conflict (Bruner, 1990; Kavanaugh &
adults who report not having had an imaginary Engel, 1998; Pellegrini & Galda, 1993; Sutton-Smith,
companion (Gleason et al., 2003). In one study, 2001). Symbolic play and narrative have been char-
parents reported that children with imaginary acterized as ‘‘two distinct expressions of symbolic
companions were more anxious than children thought’’ (Kavanaugh & Engel, 1998, p. 81). Alter-
without imaginary companions, but the reported natively, pretend play and storytelling have been
levels of anxiety were not of clinical concern identified as elements in a continuum of narrative
(Bouldin & Pratt, 2002). Children who engage in activities (Nicolopoulou, 2002). Nicolopoulou (2006)
Play and Narrative 1303

proposed that pretend play and storytelling have with imaginary companions show stronger narra-
different origins in social interaction, and only late tive skills, it is possible that children with imagi-
in the preschool period do the two coalesce into nary companions will develop into stronger readers
complex sociodramatic play. Critically, pretend in the elementary school years.
play interventions can improve children’s narrative To explore our initial hypothesis that imaginary
skills (e.g., Baumer, Ferholt, & Leçusay, 2005). companion play is linked to children’s narrative
Our specific goal in this study was to explore the skill, we drew upon measures from a larger longi-
relations between imaginary companion play and tudinal study of children’s language and social-
children’s narrative skills. Imaginary companion cognitive development from ages 1½ to 5½ (see
play is theorized to be a sustained and richly Reese, 2002, for an overview of this study). At the
detailed form of role play (Harris, 2000), which 5½-year data point, children and their parents par-
over time can become more layered or abstract. We ticipated in interviews about the children’s imagi-
view imaginary companion play as one of the most nary companions, past or present. Children also
complex expressions of play in early childhood. completed two standardized language measures, a
Similarly, narrative, as an instance of decontextual- measure of story understanding, and two narrative
ized language, is one of the most complex linguistic production tasks at this same age. We hypothe-
expressions of early childhood (Dickinson & Snow, sized that children who engaged in imaginary
1987; Snow, 1983). Somers and Yawkey (1984) theo- companion play would produce narratives that
rized that imaginary companion play facilitates were qualitatively richer and more detailed than
cognitive development through a number of children who did not engage in imaginary com-
abstract thought processes. One such process is panion play. Peterson and McCabe (1983), draw-
decontextualization, which they identify as ‘‘the ing upon Labov and Waletzky’s (1967 ⁄ 1997) high-
use of real situations out of their contexts during point analysis, noted that a child narrator’s use of
play’’ (p. 86). Following this thinking, we further orientations and evaluations could be used to clas-
offer that imaginary companion play is highly de- sify the complexity and quality of a narrative. In
contextualized because it primarily involves ‘‘real’’ contrast to referential statements that simply tell
social interactions with an imagined other during what happened in the story, orientations (to char-
play. Play with an invisible companion may even acters, time, and place) contextualize the story for
promote social interactions with real others when the listener, whereas evaluations (adjectives, emo-
children attempt to share details about their invisi- tions, and dialogue) emphasize meaning or high
ble friend with interested adults (see Gleason, points within the narrative. Both orientations and
2004b). Conceptually, then, both imaginary com- evaluations in children’s narratives are positively
panion play and narrative rely heavily on mental correlated with their later reading skill (Griffin
and linguistic constructions to create context. Based et al., 2004). We predicted that these individual
on these similar requirements for decontextualiza- differences in narrative quality would be robust
tion in the two activities, we predicted that children and evident across the two contexts, whether or
with imaginary companions would demonstrate not they involved an element of pretense. Finally,
more complex narrative skills than children with- we predicted that these differences in narrative
out imaginary companions. skill would still be evident even when we con-
This extension to narrative is especially impor- trolled for potential differences in the vocabulary
tant given increasing evidence that narrative skill in levels of children with and without imaginary
early childhood is linked to reading success and to companions.
school achievement (e.g., Griffin, Hemphill, Camp,
& Wolf, 2004; O’Neill, Pearce, & Pick, 2004; Reese,
Suggate, Long, & Schaughency, in press), even after Method
children’s vocabulary and syntax skills are taken
Participants
into account. Mastery of the elements of a canonical
narrative is essential for story comprehension, both All participants were part of a larger longitudi-
when reading independently and when listening to nal study conducted in New Zealand (see Reese,
stories. Skill at telling stories in a culturally appro- 2002, for more details). Participants included 48
priate way is also appreciated by teachers mother–child dyads (24 males and 24 females)
(Michaels, 1981) and, we suspect, by peers, recruited initially by posting fliers on community
although research has not yet addressed the latter notice boards and contacting parents through
hypothesis. Thus, if we are correct that children public birth records. Data for this study were taken
1304 Trionfi and Reese

from the 65-month time point (M = 65.2 months, nation and imaginary companion play. The
SD = 13.7 days). Firstborn children represented researcher explained that children of this age were
37% of the sample (18 of 48). All participants identi- often secretive about their imaginary companions
fied themselves as living in households where Eng- and that it was common for parents to be unaware
lish was the primary language. Forty-four mothers of them. Mothers were then asked if they were
identified their children’s ethnicity as New Zealand aware of their child engaging in imaginary com-
European and four mothers identified their chil- panion play currently or at a previous time. If they
dren’s ethnicity as New Zealand M aori. Demo- responded affirmatively, the remainder of the imag-
graphic information collected at the beginning of inary companion interview was conducted. If moth-
the study when children were 19 months old ers or children identified multiple imaginary
revealed that the sample was on average of middle- companions, they were asked to describe only their
class status (Elley & Irving, 1976) and that mothers three favorites.
had an average of 13.3 years of education Children were considered to have an imaginary
(SD = 2.26 years). Mothers ranged in their educa- companion if either the children or their mothers
tion levels from not having completed high school currently acknowledged an imaginary companion
to having completed a postgraduate degree. Fewer or indicated the presence of an imaginary compan-
than half of the mothers (45.8%) had continued ion in the past. Mothers reviewed children’s imagi-
their education past high school. nary companion interviews and clarified if children
had mistakenly identified a real friend. These chil-
dren’s reports were not included as evidence of an
Materials
imaginary companion. We were unable to use a
A video and audio recorder was used during more stringent double-interview process (Taylor &
data collection. An unfamiliar story, A Perfect Carlson, 1997) due to time constraints associated
Father’s Day (Bunting, 1991, 32 pages), and a puppet with the larger longitudinal study.
were used during the story retelling and compre- Coding of imaginary companion interviews was
hension tasks. At the end of the second visit, a completed by two independent coders for 22% of
small gift was presented to the child. the interviews. Interrater reliability on the presence
or absence of an imaginary companion was 100%.
The remaining interviews were coded by one of the
Procedure
coders.
Two female researchers met with participants in
their homes over two sessions within 2 weeks of
Vocabulary Skills
children reaching 65 months of age. Each session
lasted an average of 1 hr. All children completed the Peabody Picture
Vocabulary Test–IIIB (PPVT–IIIB; Dunn & Dunn,
1997) during the first session and the Expressive
Imaginary Companion Interview
Vocabulary Test (EVT; Williams, 1997) during the
The imaginary companion interview was based second session. The PPVT measures children’s
on the procedure utilized by Taylor et al. (1993). receptive vocabulary and the EVT measures chil-
After interacting with the child for a few minutes, dren’s expressive vocabulary. Standard scores were
the researcher said ‘‘Now I’m going to ask you used in analyses.
some questions about friends. Some friends are
real, like the kids who go to your school, the ones
Story Comprehension
you play with. And some friends are pretend
friends, ones that are make-believe, or imaginary. During the second session, researchers read an
Do you have a pretend or imaginary friend, or have unfamiliar story (A Perfect Father’s Day; Bunting,
you ever had one’’? If the child answered ‘‘yes,’’ 1991) to children. Children were asked 12 compre-
then the researcher asked the child 15 questions hension questions as a researcher read the story to
about the pretend friend, including its gender, age, them. Comprehension questions focused on plot
physical appearance, likes, and dislikes. Mean- information (what happened), inferences (causal
while, another researcher conducted the imaginary connections between plot events and inferring
companion interview separately with the child’s meaning from pictures), and real world knowledge
mother with a slightly different preamble, in which that impacted story understanding (see Reese, 1995,
the researcher stated an interest in children’s imagi- for a full description of this task and scoring).
Play and Narrative 1305

Children could receive a total of 15 points for their ments that arose during the reliability process were
answers, with one plot question worth 2 points and then resolved, and these resolved scores were used
a formal definition question (‘‘What is a surprise?’’) in analyses. Each of the two coders proceeded to
worth 3 points. Coding of the story comprehension code half of the remaining transcripts indepen-
task was completed by two independent coders for dently. Following this coding, narrative quality
25% of the sample, achieving an inter-rater agree- points were divided by the number of memory
ment of Cohen’s j = .97. Each of the coders pro- units the child provided to create a story retelling
ceeded to code half of the remaining transcripts narrative score for each participant. This score rep-
independently. resents the average narrative quality score per
memory unit produced by the participant during
the retelling narrative. These average narrative
Story Retelling
quality scores reduced the variability associated
At the conclusion of the story comprehension with sheer talkativeness.
task, children were introduced to a puppet. Chil-
dren were told that the puppet did not hear the
Past Event Narratives
story and were asked to retell the story to the pup-
pet ‘‘from beginning to end’’ (see O’Neill et al., Mothers selected three past events that children
2004, for a similar procedure). Researchers assisted had experienced in the past year (typically within
the children in their retelling by first reminding the the past month) for researchers to discuss with chil-
children of the story’s title. Researchers provided a dren (see Cleveland & Reese, 2005, for a full
maximum of two supportive, yet nonspecific, description of the procedure). The events were
prompts per page. If children did not respond to selected to be unique and to have occurred over the
the second prompt, researchers turned the page. time span of 1 day. Thus, birthdays and holidays
Researchers’ prompts included generic questions were excluded because they are either routine or
(e.g., ‘‘Now what’s happening?’’), and generic sup- extended events. Researchers discouraged selection
port (e.g., ‘‘That’s neat’’), but avoided any elabora- of movies and plays because these events contain
tions of children’s retelling. Retellings were an inherent storyline. Past event narratives were
recorded and transcribed for coding. provided at the end of the same session as the story
Children’s retellings were then coded for story retelling for half the children, and at the end of the
memory and narrative quality (adapted from Peter- other session for the remaining children. Research-
son & McCabe, 1983; Reese, 1995). Initially, the text ers started off the discussion with a general prompt
of the storybook was divided into 63 propositions. ‘‘Your mum told me you went to Tunnel Beach but
Children’s retellings, which ranged from 1 to 40 she didn’t tell me what happened. Tell me every-
propositions, were first compared to the text propo- thing that happened when you went to Tunnel
sitions. One point was issued for each text proposi- Beach.’’ Researchers followed up this initiation with
tion children mentioned in their retelling, termed a prompts such as ‘‘Tell me some more about that’’
memory unit. Each memory unit was then coded for and ‘‘What else’’? and confirmed children’s
narrative quality in terms of evaluative and orient- responses (‘‘Wow’’) but did not ask specific ques-
ing devices that were additional to those provided tions about the event. Children’s responses were
in the text. Five separate categories of narrative recorded and transcribed for coding.
quality included: descriptors (adjectives and Children’s performance across the three narra-
adverbs), dialogue, character names, temporal– tives in this task was variable, probably because
locative–causal, and verbatim recall from text. we were relying on mother-nominated events, not
Children received a maximum of 1 point for each all of which were of similar interest to children.
category of narrative quality per memory unit. This Therefore, we selected the highest scoring narra-
practice prevented unfairly inflating the scores of tive from each child for analysis to capture chil-
children who repeatedly used the same narrative dren’s best performance on this task. A memory
device in one given memory unit. For example, ‘‘a unit consisted of a child’s utterance containing
big red balloon’’ found in one memory unit would unique information about the past event, as
only receive 1 point for the descriptor category. opposed to repeated or off-topic information (see
Coding of memory units and narrative quality was Cleveland & Reese, 2005, for coding details). Simi-
completed by two independent coders for 25% of lar to the story retelling coding, these memory
the story retelling transcripts, achieving an inter- units were then coded for narrative quality. The
rater reliability of Cohen’s j = .83. Any disagree- narrative quality coding scheme for the past event
1306 Trionfi and Reese

narratives parallels the coding scheme used in the ple). Sixty-five percent of children with imaginary
story retelling task but did not include verbatim companions identified having only one, 13% identi-
recall because there was no documentation of the fied two, and 22% acknowledged having three or
original events. Thus, the four categories of narra- more. Of the 36 imaginary companions described
tive quality for past event narratives were descrip- by children and their mothers, half were identified
tors (adverbs and adjectives), dialogue, character as people, with the remainder identified as animals
names, and temporal–locative–causal terms. As in (14%), fantasy beings such as a cucumber boy
the story retelling task, children only received 1 (25%) or unclear (11%). Only three imaginary com-
point for each subcategory of narrative quality panions were based on personified objects (toys).
within a given memory unit to prevent the skew- Almost all described imaginary companions were
ing of scores in favor of using only one type of current companions with only three identified as
narrative device. former companions.
Coding of memory units was completed by two Children with and without imaginary compan-
independent coders as part of a more extensive ions did not differ as a function of their mothers’
coding scheme for a previous study (Cleveland & level of education, t(46) = 0.36, ns (Ms = 13.44 and
Reese, 2005). On 25% of the past event narrative 13.20 years, respectively). Among children who
transcripts, the two coders achieved an interrater engaged in imaginary companion play, 10 were
reliability of Cohen’s j = .77. For narrative quality, male (42% of all males in the sample) and 13 were
a different team of two independent coders female (54% of all females in the sample). This gen-
achieved an interrater reliability of Cohen’s j = .80 der difference was not statistically significant, v2(1,
on 25% of the transcripts. Any disagreements that N = 48) = 0.75, ns. Children’s imaginary companion
arose during the reliability process were then play, however, did differ as a function of birth
resolved, and these resolved scores were used in order. Thirteen (56%) of the children with imagi-
analyses. Each of the two coders proceeded to code nary companions were firstborns, but only 5 (20%)
half of the remaining transcripts independently. As of the children without imaginary companions
with the story retelling narratives, the past event were firstborns. This birth order difference was
narrative quality points were divided by the num- statistically significant, v2(1, N = 48) = 6.81, p < .01.
ber of memory units to create average past event Participants’ descriptions of imaginary compan-
narrative scores for each participant. These scores ions were often detailed and colorful. In almost all
represent the average narrative quality scores per cases, the gender of the imaginary companion
memory unit produced by participants during their matched that of the child. Cross-gender friendships
past event narratives and adjusts for talkativeness. occurred only among female participants, particu-
Coding for imaginary companions and narrative larly when more than one imaginary companion
quality was completed independently. Specifically, was identified. Every male (n = 10) who described
imaginary companion interviews were coded from an imaginary companion identified the companion
video by participant name, whereas narratives were as male even when multiple imaginary companions
scored from transcripts using participant ID as the were described. One female participant classified
sole identifier. The first author was the primary her imaginary companion named ‘‘Batman’’ as both
coder on all tasks except for story comprehension a ‘‘girl and a boy.’’ Participants’ description of
and past event narrative memory units; five imaginary companions’ ages ranged from a ‘‘baby
research assistants served as reliability coders. puppy’’ (named Brown Puppy) to a 1-million-year-
old wrestler (named Giant Strongman, who had no
toenails), with one participant describing ‘‘Creka’’
(who looks like a shadow) as both ‘‘old and
Results
young.’’ Imaginary companions’ names ranged
Analysis of the imaginary companion interviews from nondescript (e.g., Her) and mundane (e.g.,
identified 23 of the 48 children in this study as hav- Emily) to the truly unique (e.g., Holibola). When
ing an imaginary companion (48% of the sample). asked who would win in a race, most participants
These children’s imaginary companions were iden- predicted imaginary companions to be the winners,
tified in the following manner: Eight were reported although predictions were often justified based on
by children only (35% of the imaginary companion factors that would determine the outcome (i.e., the
sample), nine by mothers only (39% of the imagi- size or age of the imaginary companion, or acces-
nary companion sample), and six by both mother sories such as a flying car or high-heeled shoes).
and child (26% of the imaginary companion sam- When asked who was more likely to be naughty,
Play and Narrative 1307

Table 1 lary and story comprehension skills of participants


Language and Narrative Scores for Participants Without Imaginary who engaged in imaginary companion play and
Companions (No IC) and With Imaginary Companions (With IC) those who did not.
No IC With IC
(n = 25) (n = 23) Measures of Narrative Skill
M SD M SD t(46) d Because the literature on narrative performance
during story retelling tasks is distinct from the liter-
PPVT–III 106.32 10.62 105.22 14.45 )0.30 0.09
ature on children’s personal narratives (see O’Neill
EVT 106.71 12.41 106.13 12.46 )0.16 0.05
Story comprehension 9.15 2.85 9.02 2.73 )0.16 0.05
et al., 2004; Peterson & McCabe, 1983), we first ana-
Story retelling lyzed these two narrative tasks separately as a func-
Memory units 10.60 4.30 15.57 12.54 1.87 0.50 tion of children’s imaginary companion status.
Narrative quality 2.76 2.49 9.43 14.61 2.25* 0.64 Supporting the distinct nature of the two narrative
Narrative score 0.22 0.18 0.51 0.42 3.10* 0.90 tasks, the children’s overall narrative scores in
Past event narratives the two tasks were not significantly correlated,
Memory units 10.36 7.36 11.22 9.57 0.35 0.10 r(46) = .10, ns.
Narrative quality 8.6 6.6 10.65 8.70 0.92 0.27 Story retelling. Some children in each group had
Narrative score 0.79 0.23 0.97 0.36 2.02* 0.60 narrative scores of zero in this task, but children in
Note. PPVT–III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III;
the two groups differed dramatically in the maxi-
EVT = Expressive Vocabulary Test. mum narrative scores obtained (0.70 for children
*p < .05. without imaginary companions and 2.0 for children
with imaginary companions). Children with imagi-
almost all participants (81%) identified imaginary nary companions were significantly higher in their
companions as the likely offender. overall story retelling narrative score than children
without imaginary companions, t(46) = 3.10, p < .01
(see Table 1), with an effect size (d = 0.9) that quali-
Main Analyses
fies as large (Cohen, 1988). Children with and with-
Table 1 contains descriptive statistics for the out imaginary companions did not differ
vocabulary, story comprehension, and narrative significantly in the number of memory units pro-
tasks for children with and without imaginary com- duced, t(46) = 1.87, p < .10, d = 0.5, but the trend
panions. One child did not complete the EVT and was for children with imaginary companions to
the story comprehension task. Group means were produce more memory units than children without
substituted for these two missing scores in analy- imaginary companions.
ses. Unpaired t tests were used to compare the two Based on the difference in story retelling narra-
groups of children on all measures. For several of tive scores between the two groups and the large
the narrative subcategories (dialogue in both narra- effect size associated with that difference, we
tive tasks and verbatim recall for story retelling),
significant heterogeneity of variance was found
using Levene’s test. We reanalyzed these variables Table 2
using t tests that did not assume equal variances, Story Retelling Narrative Subcategory Scores for Participants Without
but in all cases the pattern of significant findings Imaginary Companions (No IC) and With Imaginary Companions
was the same. Therefore, we present only the (With IC)
results with equal variances assumed.
No IC With IC
(n = 25) (n = 23)
Measures of Vocabulary Development and Story
M SD M SD t(46) d
Comprehension
Participants without imaginary companions and Descriptors 0.17 0.38 0.36 0.58 1.35 0.39
those with imaginary companions did not differ in Dialogue 0.02 0.05 0.09 0.14 2.23* 0.63
Characters 0.14 0.10 0.23 0.30 1.35 0.38
their performance on the PPVT–III, t(46) = )0.30,
Temporal–locative– 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.70 0.95 0.29
ns, or on the EVT, t(46) = )0.16, ns. Neither was
causal
story comprehension a function of engagement in Verbatim recall 0.02 0.07 0.11 0.27 1.57 0.44
imaginary companion play, t(46) = )0.16, ns. Thus,
there were no significant differences in the vocabu- *p < .05.
1308 Trionfi and Reese

conducted an analysis of the subcategories that analyses revealed a significant difference between
comprise this composite narrative score (see the groups in their use of temporal–locative–causal
Table 2). Although these analyses identified only terms and again showed scores that were higher
the subcategory of dialogue to be significantly dif- for children with imaginary companions than those
ferent between the two groups, mean scores for without imaginary companions in all narrative sub-
children with imaginary companions were higher categories. The probability for children with imagi-
than children without imaginary companions in all nary companions to exceed the narrative skills of
narrative quality subcategories. The probability for children without imaginary companions in all four
children with imaginary companions to exceed the subcategories was .06 (binomial test). Most effect
narrative skills of children without imaginary com- sizes were in the small to medium range.
panions in all five subcategories was only .03 (bino- Although children’s overall narrative scores in
mial test). Effect sizes for the story retelling the two narrative tasks were not correlated, an
subcategories were all in the small to medium identical pattern of a narrative advantage for chil-
range. dren with imaginary companions was demon-
Past event narratives.. Similar to the story retelling strated across all narrative subcategories for both
task, although the minimum best past event narra- narrative tasks. The probability for children with
tive scores were similar for both groups (0.42 for imaginary companions to exceed the narrative skills
children without imaginary companions and 0.43 of children without imaginary companions in all
for children with imaginary companions), the same nine subcategories across the two tasks was only
was not true of maximum best past event narrative .002 (binomial test). To capture this general pattern
scores (1.17 for children without imaginary com- in a single score, we created an overall narrative
panions and 2.0 for children with imaginary com- score composed of an average of the children’s nar-
panions). Indeed, there was a significant difference rative scores across the story retelling and past
between the best past event narrative scores of par- event narrative contexts. Children with imaginary
ticipants who engaged in imaginary companion companions had a significantly higher overall
play and those who did not, t(46) = 2.02, p < .05 narrative score (M = 0.74, SD = 0.27), than chil-
(see Table 1). This significant difference between dren without imaginary companions (M = 0.51,
the two groups’ past event narrative scores was SD = 0.16), t(46) = 3.69, p < .001. The effect size for
supported by a medium effect size (d = 0.60). There this comparison was large (d = 1.06). Children with
was no significant difference between the two imaginary companions did not provide signifi-
groups in the number of memory units they pro- cantly more memory units across the two narratives
vided for their best past event narrative, compared to children without imaginary compan-
t(46) = 0.35, ns. ions, t(46) = 1.5, ns. All subsequent analyses are
Based on the difference in past event narrative performed on children’s overall narrative score.
scores between the groups and the medium effect
size associated with that difference, an analysis of
Imaginary Companion Play as a Unique Predictor of
the subcategories that comprise this composite nar-
Children’s Narrative
rative score was completed (see Table 3). These
Because children who engaged in imaginary
Table 3 companion play also tended to be firstborns, how-
Past Event Narrative Subcategory Scores for Participants Without ever, we needed to explore the unique role of
Imaginary Companions (No IC) and With Imaginary Companions imaginary companion play in children’s narrative
(With IC) skills. Could these differences instead be
accounted for by children’s birth order? We con-
No IC With IC
(n = 25) (n = 23)
ducted a hierarchical regression analysis to predict
children’s overall narrative skill as a function of
M SD M SD t(46) d birth order and imaginary companion status, after
controlling for children’s vocabulary skill. Because
Descriptors 0.58 0.20 0.63 0.29 0.73 0.21 children’s PPVT and EVT scores were correlated,
Dialogue 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.08 1.07 0.31
r(46) = .47, p < .01, we computed an overall vocab-
Characters 0.11 0.15 0.12 0.20 0.32 0.09
ulary score composed of the average of the two
Temporal–locative– 0.09 0.12 0.18 0.17 2.02* 0.58
causal
vocabulary scores. We entered this vocabulary
score in the first step of a regression analysis pre-
*p < .05. dicting children’s narrative score. In a second step,
Play and Narrative 1309

Table 4 narrative score along a continuum of social interac-


Predicting Children’s Narrative Skill From Vocabulary, Birth Order, tion possibilities, from those children without imag-
and Imaginary Companion Status inary companions (n = 25), to those children with
B SE b nR2
imaginary companions of which only the child had
knowledge (child knowledge; n = 8), to those chil-
Step 1 dren whose mothers knew about their imaginary
Vocabulary .004 0.003 .18 .02 companions (mother knowledge; n = 15). Only chil-
Step 2 dren with imaginary companions could benefit
Birth order ).16 0.07 ).32 .21** from social interactions with those companions, but
Step 3 children whose mothers know about their imagi-
Imaginary companion status .18 0.06 .37 .12**
nary companions may have an added source of
Note. Final beta weights are reported. social interactions about the companion.
**p < .01. We used nonparametric analyses to test this pre-
diction due to the small number of children with
we added children’s birth order, and in a third child-knowledge-only imaginary companions. A
step, children’s imaginary companion status. Chil- Kruskal–Wallis test indicated a significant differ-
dren’s vocabulary was not correlated with either ence in children’s narrative scores as a function of
their imaginary companion status, r(46) = ).05 or social-interaction possibilities, v2(2, N = 48) = 12.85,
with their birth order, r(46) = .08, nor was vocabu- p < .002. On their overall narrative scores, children
lary a significant predictor of children’s narrative without imaginary companions had a mean rank of
skill (see Table 4). In contrast, both birth order 18.38, children with child-knowledge-only imagi-
and imaginary companion status uniquely pre- nary companions had a mean rank of 24.38, and
dicted children’s narrative skill (see Table 4). Chil- children with mother-knowledge imaginary com-
dren who were firstborns and children with an panions had a mean rank of 34.77. A follow-up
imaginary companion had higher narrative skills Mann–Whitney U test indicated a significant differ-
across the two contexts than children who were ence in the narrative scores of children with
laterborns or who did not have an imaginary com- mother-knowledge imaginary companions and chil-
panion. dren without imaginary companions (Z = )3.74,
We also conducted a second regression analysis p < .01). Children with child-knowledge-only com-
with an interaction term (birth order by imaginary panions did not differ significantly from either
companion status) entered in the final step to deter- group in their narrative scores.
mine if firstborns with imaginary companions were
primarily responsible for the main effect of imagi-
nary companion status on narrative. The results of
Discussion
this analysis indicated no significant interaction
between birth order and imaginary companion sta- The main finding of this study was that 5½-year-
tus (b = ).05, ns). Because only 5 firstborns did not old children who currently or previously engaged
have imaginary companions, however, we consider in imaginary companion play had more advanced
this analysis exploratory. narrative skills than children who did not engage
in this type of play. Although children in the two
groups did not differ significantly in their vocabu-
Social Interaction as a Potential Mechanism of the
lary skills or in their story understanding, the chil-
Narrative Advantage for Children With Imaginary
dren with imaginary companions told richer stories
Companions
in two different contexts compared to children
Nearly all (92%) of the imaginary companions in without imaginary companions. Firstborns also told
our sample comprised invisible friends, not person- richer narratives than laterborns, but imaginary
ified objects. If children with invisible imaginary companion play uniquely predicted children’s
companions want to share their imaginary compan- narrative skill, even after accounting for birth
ions with the real people in their lives, they must order and vocabulary skill. These findings add to
describe their imaginary companion to others the growing body of evidence that imaginary
(Gleason, 2004b). We hypothesized that these companion play is associated positively with chil-
decontextualized conversations may serve to dren’s linguistic and social-cognitive development
further develop children’s narrative skills. We con- (Bouldin et al., 2002; Singer & Singer, 1990; Taylor
ducted an exploratory analysis of children’s overall & Carlson, 1997). These findings also fit well with
1310 Trionfi and Reese

Nicolopoulou’s (2006) proposal that pretend play et al., 2004). A word of caution is in order, though.
and storytelling have become integrated by the end Imaginary companion play has not yet been impli-
of early childhood. Because the PPVT in particular cated directly in children’s reading achievement,
can be viewed as a measure of children’s general nor is there causal evidence for the benefits of
cognitive ability (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997), imaginary companion play in any developmental
these findings point to the unique role of imaginary domain. We are not advocating that parents or
companion play, over and above cognitive ability, teachers encourage children without imaginary
in children’s narrative skills. friends to create such friends. Rather, if a child has
Specifically, participants who engaged in imagi- already created an imaginary companion, parents
nary companion play used more dialogue in their and teachers could allow this play to flourish.
story retelling narratives and more temporal– Although the link between imaginary companion
locative–causal terms in their past event narratives play and narrative quality is clear, the reason for
when compared to participants who did not engage this link is not. We speculate that one reason that
in imaginary companion play. Although we did not children with imaginary companions have richer
have specific predictions about the aspects of narra- narrative skills is that they gain practice in decon-
tive on which children with imaginary companions textualized conversations both during interactions
would excel, both dialogue (a type of evaluation) with their imaginary companions, and when they
and temporal–locative–causal terms (types of orien- tell others about their imaginary companions.
tation) are advanced narrative elements. The story- Advanced narratives rely on decontextualized
book for the retelling task, A Perfect Father’s Day, language skill. All of the anecdotal and case study
included a generous amount of dialogue in the text. evidence indicates that at some point during the
In their use of created dialogue in their retellings, duration of children’s imaginary companion play,
children with imaginary companions appeared to most will tell their parents and family members a
be better at emulating the style of the text com- story about their imaginary companions. It is possi-
pared to children without imaginary companions. ble that this storytelling leads to conversations in
In the past event narrative context, temporal, loca- which adults promote children’s engagement in
tive, and causal terms are essential for making the and understanding of complex decontextualized
event narrative understandable to the listener, and language practices. Because children have sole
it is not until middle childhood that most children knowledge of their imaginary companions, adults
can use these elements coherently (Reese et al., may be required to ask a greater number of ques-
2008). Thus, we believe that the children with imag- tions about imaginary companions than they ask
inary companions in our study were especially when conversing about topics for which both adult
good at using the most advanced narrative ele- and child share knowledge. Overcoming this lack
ments in each context. Moreover, they excelled at of shared knowledge is practically impossible
different elements in each context—those elements unless children describe these absent entities, a task
that were most essential for conveying that particu- that involves decontextualized language. Children
lar story effectively to the listener. who participate in these storytelling exchanges may
Furthermore, mean scores of children who begin to produce richer narratives in general as a
engaged in imaginary companion play were higher result of these unique exchanges. The results of our
than those of children who did not engage in imag- exploratory analyses were in line with this interpre-
inary companion play for all of the narrative sub- tation, in that only children whose mothers knew
categories across both tasks, and effect sizes for about their imaginary companions showed a narra-
these differences were almost all in the small to tive advantage over children without imaginary
medium range. The effect size for the overall differ- companions. Only a small number of children had
ence in children’s narrative skill as a function of mothers who did not know about their compan-
imaginary companion status was large. All of these ions, however, so additional research needs to be
findings confirm our hypothesis and identify a clear conducted with children who do not share their
link between imaginary companion play and chil- imaginary companions with parents to garner firm
dren’s storytelling abilities. This finding has practi- support for this hypothesis.
cal importance given a growing body of evidence Synergistic support for these hypothetical con-
that children’s narrative skill upon school entry versations can be found in Gleason’s (2004b) work
strongly predicts their later reading ability, espe- on the correspondence in the reports about imagi-
cially their reading comprehension in mid-elemen- nary companions of parents and children who
tary school (e.g., Dickinson & Tabors, 2001; Griffin engage in imaginary companion play. Gleason
Play and Narrative 1311

found that parents’ descriptions of imaginary com- better language or narrative skills are driven to cre-
panions reached a higher level of agreement if a ate an imaginary companion as an outlet for their
child’s imaginary companion was a classically verbal expression, or that a third variable can
invisible rather than a personified object. Gleason account for this relation. Although we have ruled
theorized that ‘‘intangibility of invisible friends out children’s vocabulary skills in accounting for
may promote better efforts by parents to learn the relation we found between imaginary compan-
about or to describe their children’s invisible ions and narrative, other potential candidates are
friends. Parents can see personified objects and children’s personality characteristics or their per-
might even hear their children interacting with spective-taking skills. In earlier time points from
them, meaning that neither parent nor child may this longitudinal study, we found no unique rela-
think to discuss a personified object’s age or activi- tions between children’s early temperament at ages
ties because both witness its existence’’ (p. 211). 1½–3½ and their imaginary companion play at age
Recording actual conversations of this sort would 5½ once birth order had been taken into account
confirm their occurrence beyond the anecdotal (McMenamin, 2000). Nor did we find any signifi-
report of parents. It would also provide conversa- cant links between children’s earlier language or
tional evidence that could be used to support our understanding of mind at ages 3½ and 4 years and
theory regarding children’s increased use of their imaginary companion play at age 5½. These
decontextualized language when speaking about null findings mirror those of Taylor et al. (2004),
their imaginary companions. the only other study to explore older children’s
This stance echoes other theorists’ positions that imaginary companions in depth. In that study, chil-
the developmental benefits of play are not solely a dren’s imaginary companion play at ages 6–7 was
result of engagement in play activities, but play can not predicted by their earlier characteristics or the-
also promote secondary interactions with added ory of mind, nor was imaginary companion play
developmental benefits (Pellegrini & Galda, 1993). correlated with concurrent personality and emotion
Although it would be difficult to execute such a understanding. It is possible that the imaginary
study, collecting children’s spontaneous narratives companions of older children arise through differ-
about their imaginary companions would allow for ent processes and for different reasons from the
the elaboration of the proposed theory and poten- imaginary companions of early childhood. Taken
tially reveal how parents influence the development together with Taylor et al.’s results, we propose
of these children’s higher order language practices. that imaginary companion play arises from a more
In previous analyses with this sample (e.g., general predilection for fantasy. The reason that
Cleveland & Reese, 2005; Farrant & Reese, 2000), as different correlates of imaginary companion play
well as in other samples (e.g., Haden, Haine, & are found at different ages could result from the
Fivush, 1997), parents who adopt an elaborative, necessary skills to support fantasy at that age (e.g.,
open-ended questioning style during reminiscing perspective-taking at younger ages and complex
have children with stronger personal narrative storytelling at older ages). The developmental out-
skills. A next step in our research program will be comes of engaging in imaginary companion play
to explore a range of parent–child storytelling prac- at different ages could also be expected to differ.
tices in early childhood that support children’s Longitudinal research on imaginary companion
imaginary companion play and their narrative play and narrative in the early elementary school
skills. Given the enriched language environment years is needed to tease apart cause and effect in
for firstborns (e.g., Jones & Adamson, 1987), par- this relation.
ent–child storytelling practices may also help us We acknowledge several other limitations to this
understand the higher incidence of imaginary com- study. Our sample was relatively small, and we
panions and better narrative skills for firstborns in were unable to conduct the more stringent double-
our study. Once again, however, we found that the interview process for identifying children with
link between imaginary companion status and nar- imaginary companions (Taylor & Carlson, 1997;
rative was independent of children’s birth order. A although see Taylor et al., 2004). Despite these limi-
richer language environment for firstborns cannot tations, we were able to replicate several findings
completely account for our findings. from previous research on imaginary companion
Because our main finding is limited to a concur- play. For instance, our identified rate of 48% of 5½-
rent correlation between children’s imaginary com- year-old children with past or present imaginary
panion play and their narrative skills, however, companions is in line with Taylor et al.’s
there is always the possibility that children with (2004) identification of 49% in a sample of 6- to
1312 Trionfi and Reese

7-year-olds, and with Pearson et al.’s (2001) identifi- Bunting, E. (1991). A perfect father’s day. Boston: Clarion.
cation of 46.2% in a sample of 5- to 12-year-olds. Carlson, S. M., & Taylor, M. (2005). Imaginary compan-
Also similar to Taylor et al.’s research with older ions and impersonated characters: Sex differences in
children, we found no significant gender difference children’s fantasy play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51, 93–
118.
in the incidence of imaginary companions in our
Cleveland, E. S., & Reese, E. (2005). Maternal structure
school-age sample (but see Pearson et al., 2001). As
and autonomy support in conversations about the past:
in other studies, there was no difference in the Contributions to children’s autobiographical memory.
mothers’ education levels as a function of their chil- Developmental Psychology, 41, 376–388.
dren’s imaginary companion play (e.g., Bouldin & Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
Pratt, 1999). Finally, similar to other studies, the sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
majority of the children in our study with imagi- Cunningham, A., & Stanovich, K. (1997). Early reading
nary companions were firstborns (e.g., Bouldin & acquisition and its relation to reading experience and
Pratt, 1999; Gleason et al., 2000). ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33, 934–
In conclusion, this work reveals a connection 945.
between children’s engagement in imaginary com- Dickinson, D. K., & Snow, C. E. (1987). Interrelationships
among prereading and oral language skills in kinder-
panion play and the qualitatively richer stories that
gartners from two social classes. Early Childhood
children tell about fictional and personally experi-
Research Quarterly, 1, 1–25.
enced events. The value of this finding is threefold. Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (2001). Beginning literacy
First, this finding extends our understanding of with language. Baltimore: Brookes.
imaginary companion play and its developmental Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocab-
correlates. Second, this finding deepens the litera- ulary Test–III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
ture on the theoretical relation between play and Service.
language. Third, this finding highlights children’s Elley, W. B., & Irving, J. C. (1976). Revised socio-eco-
engagement in play as an important factor to con- nomic index for New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of
sider in understanding their developing narrative Educational Studies, 11, 25–36.
skill, which in turn is critical for children’s reading Farrant, K., & Reese, E. (2000). Maternal style and chil-
dren’s participation in reminiscing: Stepping stones
skill.
in autobiographical memory development. Journal of
Cognition and Development, 1, 193–225.
Fein, G. G. (1989). Mind, meaning and affect: Proposals
References for a theory of pretense. Developmental Review, 9, 345–
363.
Ames, L. B., & Learned, J. (1946). Imaginary companions Garvey, C. (1977). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
and related phenomena. Journal of Genetic Psychology, University Press.
69, 147–167. Gleason, T. R. (2004a). Imaginary companions and peer
Baumer, S., Ferholt, B., & Leçusay, R. (2005). Promoting acceptance. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
narrative competence through adult-child joint pre- ment, 28, 204–209.
tense: Lessons from the Scandinavian educational prac- Gleason, T. R. (2004b). Imaginary companions: An evalu-
tice of playworld. Cognitive Development, 20, 576–590. ation of parents as reporters. Infant and Child Develop-
Bouldin, P. (2006). An investigation of the fantasy ment, 13, 199–215.
predisposition and fantasy style of children with Gleason, T. R., Jarudi, R. N., & Cheek, J. M. (2003). Imagi-
imaginary companions. Journal of Genetic Psychology, nation, personality, and imaginary companions. Social
167, 17–29. Behavior and Personality, 31, 721–738.
Bouldin, P., Bavin, E. L., & Pratt, C. (2002). An investiga- Gleason, T., Sebanc, A., & Hartup, W. (2000). Imaginary
tion of the verbal abilities of children with imaginary companions of preschool children. Developmental Psy-
companions. First Language, 22, 249–264. chology, 36, 419–428.
Bouldin, P., & Pratt, C. (1999). Characteristics of pre- Göncü, A. (1993). Development of intersubjectivity in the
school and school-age children with imaginary com- dyadic play of preschoolers. Early Childhood Research
panions. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 160, 397–410. Quarterly, 8, 99–116.
Bouldin, P., & Pratt, C. (2002). A systematic assessment of Göncü, A., & Katsarou, E. (2000). Constructing sociocul-
the specific fears, anxiety level, and temperament of tural approaches to literacy education. In K. A. Roskos
children with imaginary companions. Australian Journal & J. F. Christie (Eds.), Play and literacy in early childhood
of Psychology, 54, 79–85. (pp. 221–230). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Har- Griffin, T., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004).
vard University Press. Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy
Bruner, J. S. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, skills. First Language, 24, 123–147.
MA: Harvard University Press.
Play and Narrative 1313

Haden, C., Haine, R., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1993). Ten years after: A
narrative structure in parent-child conversations about reexamination of symbolic play and literacy research.
the past. Developmental Psychology, 33, 295–307. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 162–175.
Harris, P. L. (2000). The work of the imagination. Malden, Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycho-
MA: Blackwell. linguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative.
Jones, C. P., & Adamson, L. B. (1987). Language use in New York: Plenum.
mother-child and mother-child-sibling interactions. Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children’s literacy from
Child Development, 58, 356–366. mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10,
Kavanaugh, R., & Engel, S. (1998). The development of 381–405.
pretense and narrative in early childhood. In O. Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of
Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Multiple perspectives on play autobiographical memory: The state of the art. Social
in early childhood education (pp. 80–99). Albany: State Development, 11, 124–142.
University of New York Press. Reese, E., Haden, C. A., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P. J.,
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1997). Narrative analysis: Oral Fivush, R., & Ornstein, P. (2008). Coherence of personal
versions of personal experience. Journal of Narrative & narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model.
Life History, 7, 3–38. (Original work published 1967) Manuscript under review.
Manosevitz, M., Prentice, N. M., & Wilson, F. (1973). Indi- Reese, E., Suggate, S., Long, J., & Schaughency, E.
vidual and family correlates of imaginary companions (in press). Children’s oral narrative and reading skills in the
in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 8, 72– first three years of reading instruction. Reading and Writing.
79. Shore, C., O’Connell, B., & Bates, E. (1984). First sentences
McMenamin, K. (2000). Predicting imaginary companions in language and symbolic play. Developmental Psychol-
in five-year-old children: The multivariate use of lan- ogy, 20, 872–880.
guage, temperament, attachment, and birth order. Unpub- Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (1990). The house of make-
lished dissertation, University of Otago, Dunedin, believe: Children’s play and developing imagination. Cam-
New Zealand. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Michaels, S. (1981). ‘‘Sharing time’’: Children’s narrative Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships
styles and differential access to literacy. Language and during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review,
Society, 10, 423–442. 53, 165–189.
Newson, J., & Newson, E. (1968). Four years old in an Somers, J. U., & Yawkey, T. D. (1984). Imaginary play
urban community. London: Allen & Unwin. companions: Contributions of creative and intellectual
Nicolopoulou, A. (2002). Peer-group culture and narra- abilities of young children. Journal of Creative Behavior,
tive development. In S. Blum-Kulka & C. E. Snow 18, 77–89.
(Eds.), Talking to adults: The contribution of multiparty Sutton-Smith, B. (2001). The ambiguity of play. Cambridge,
discourse to language acquisition (pp. 117–152). Mahwah, MA: Harvard University Press.
NJ: Erlbaum. Svendsen, M. (1934). Children’s imaginary companions.
Nicolopoulou, A. (2006). The interplay of play and narra- Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 32, 985–999.
tive in children’s development: Theoretical reflections Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1994). Speci-
and concrete examples. In A. Göncü & S. Gaskins ficity in mother-toddler language-play relations across
(Eds.), Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, the second year. Developmental Psychology, 30, 283–292.
and functional perspectives (pp. 247–274). Mahwah, NJ: Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children
Erlbaum. who create them. New York: Oxford University Press.
O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (1997). The relation between
children’s narratives and performance of the Peabody individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind.
Individualized Achievement Test–Revised: Evidence of Child Development, 68, 435–455.
a relation between early narrative and later mathemati- Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., Maring, B., Gerow, L., &
cal ability. First Language, 24, 149–183. Charley, C. M. (2004). The characteristics and correlates
Pearson, D., Rouse, H., Doswell, S., Ainsworth, C., of fantasy in school-age children: Imaginary compan-
Dawson, O., Simms, K., et al. (2001). Prevalence of ions, impersonation, and social understanding. Develop-
imaginary companions in a normal child population. mental Psychology, 40, 1173–1187.
Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 13–22. Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S., & Carlson, S. M. (1993). A
Pellegrini, A. D. (1985). The relations between symbolic developmental investigation of children’s imaginary
play and literate behavior: A review and critique of the companions. Developmental Psychology, 29, 276–285.
empirical literature. Review of Educational Research, 55, Williams, K. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle
107–121. Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen