Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1996, 293, 67-78 NUMBER I (SPRING 1996)

DEFINING, VALIDATING, AND INCREASING INDICES OF


HAPPINESS AMONG PEOPLE W1ITH PROFOUND MULTIPLE DISABILITIES
CAROLYN W. GREEN AND DENNIS H. REID
ROSEWOOD SCHOOL, CAROLINA BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS AND SUPPORT CENTER, LTD.
MORGANTON, NORTH CAROLINA AND
LOUISIANA STATE UNIVERSITY MEDICAL CENTER

In this study we attempted to operationalize, measure, and increase happiness among


people with profound disabilities. Happiness indices were defined and observed among
5 individuals. Validation measures indicated that (a) increases in happiness indices were
observed when individuals were presented with most preferred stimuli relative to least
preferred stimuli, (b) increases in unhappiness indices were observed when they were
presented with least preferred relative to most preferred stimuli, and (c) practitioner
ratings of participant happiness coincided with observed indices. Subsequently, classroom
staff increased happiness indices through presentation and contingent withdrawal of ac-
tivities. Results suggested that a behavioral approach can enhance happiness as one aspect
of quality of life among people with profound disabilities. Research directions are offered
that focus on using a behavioral approach to investigate other private events that are
important among people with disabilities.
DESCRIPTORS: happiness, profound multiple disabilities, social validation, assess-
ment

Promoting a desirable quality of life for quality of life for people with pro-
evant to
persons with severe developmental disabili- found multiple disabilities (Reid et al.,
ties is an important but unmet challenge in 1991).
our field. The existing challenge is in part One area that has not yet received atten-
due to difficulties in operationally defining tion from behavioral researchers is the degree
variables that are relevant to quality of life of enjoyment or happiness experienced by
for people who, for example, have profound persons with profound multiple disabilities
mental and physical impairment (Evans & who have little or no verbal or mobility
Scotti, 1989). Currently, there is no readily skills. Individuals with functional verbal rep-
available behavioral technology designed to ertoires often gain access to desired stimuli
assist individuals with the most profound
disabilities in achieving a desirable lifestyle (items, activities, etc.) that may promote
(Bailey, 1981; Reid, Phillips, & Green, happiness and improve their quality of life
1991). However, recently there has been a simply by stating (i.e., manding) what they
small but apparent increase in research di- want. These individuals also often avoid or
rected to providing appropriate supports rel- terminate unfavorable stimuli that may be
associated with decreased happiness through
Appreciation is expressed to LeVon McKee and Su- verbal mands. In contrast, persons with pro-
sie Tompkins for their competence in conducting the found disabilities may have less access to
classroom procedures, to Martin Ivancic for his com- stimuli associated with happiness because
ments on an earlier manuscript draft, and to Mary
Keller for her skill and patience in preparing the they do not effectively communicate their
manuscript. preferences. Similarly, individuals with am-
Requests for reprints should be addressed to either bulation skills can contact preferred stimuli
author at the Carolina Behavior Analysis and Support
Center, Ltd., P.O. Box 425, Morganton, North Car- associated with increased happiness and
olina 28680. avoid nonpreferred stimuli much easier than
67
68 CAROLYN W GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
persons with physical impairment that pre- erationally define behaviors that represent
vents mobility. what people agree to indicate "happiness."
One likely reason for the lack of research Subsequently, those behaviors could be ob-
on happiness among people with profound served and quantified and an intervention
multiple disabilities is difficulty in defining developed to increase the occurrence of the
and documenting what constitutes "being happiness indices (Iwata, 1991). Taking such
happy." Typically, measures of affect such as an approach to happiness and related qual-
happiness among the nonhandicapped pop- ity-of-life variables has been suggested as one
ulace as well as among people with mild or of the greatest challenges facing persons
moderate disabilities involve self-reports working with people with the most severe
(Chadsey-Rusch, DeStefano, O'Reilly, Gon- disabilities (Sailor, Gee, Goetz, & Graham,
zalez, & Collet-Klingenberg, 1992). How- 1988).
ever, communication difficulties due to ex- This investigation represented an initial
treme mental and physical impairment gen- attempt to demonstrate a means of opera-
erally disallow verbal self-reports as indicators tionalizing, measuring, and altering happi-
of happiness among people with profound ness among people with profound multiple
multiple disabilities. disabilities. Specifically, in two experiments,
Behavioral studies that have included the purpose was to define and reliably ob-
measures of affect potentially related to hap- serve happiness indices, to socially validate
piness among people with disabilities have the defined indices, and to determine if the
primarily relied on subjective rating scales happiness indices could be increased by ed-
(Dunlap, 1984; Dunlap & Koegel, 1980; ucation personnel.
Kennedy, 1994; Koegel & Egel, 1979). Sin-
gular behaviors such as smiling or laughing GENERAL METHOD
have also been used as indicators of positive
affect Jordan, Singh, & Repp, 1989). All of Setting and Participants
these investigations have focused on popu- The setting was an adult education class-
lations with less serious disabilities than pro- room serving 6 individuals with profound
found mental and physical impairment. mental and physical impairment. The class-
Also, behavioral studies have generally in- room was staffed by a certified teacher, who
cluded affective measures to assess collateral was intermittently present for oversight re-
effects of interventions designed to affect sponsibilities, and two full-time teacher as-
other behaviors, such as self-injury (Lin- sistants. The assistants had high school de-
scheid, Pejeau, Cohen, & Footo-Lenz, grees and at least 12 years of experience.
1994). Few, if any, studies have focused on Students' ages ranged from 18 years to 41
happiness as a dependent variable per se or years (M = 30 years). Each student was non-
on the development of interventions de- ambulatory and was unable to propel his or
signed to directly increase happiness. her wheelchair or recliner. The students
From one perspective, happiness can be lacked any conventional communication
viewed as a private event or hypothetical skills and were dependent on support staff
construct that is considered to be important for fulfillment of all basic needs (e.g., eating,
by professionals as well as by the general dressing). Medical diagnoses included diple-
populace (cf. Iwata, 1991). When viewed in gia, hemiplegia, quadriplegia, and scoliosis.
this light, a potential behavioral strategy to Five students had seizure disorders, 3 had
assist people with profound disabilities in in- visual impairments, and 2 had hearing im-
creasing their happiness would be first to op- pairments.
INDICES OF HAPPINESS 69
Behavior Definitions and Observation System each participant to determine stimuli that
To develop happiness indices, we selected the participants consistently approached or
observable responses generally associated avoided.
with subjective feelings of happiness that (a) EXPERIMENT 1
were similar to behaviors previously de-
scribed (e.g., Derrickson, Neef, & Cataldo, The purpose of Experiment 1 was to at-
1993; Dunlap & Koegel, 1980; Hunt, Far- tempt to reliably observe and validate the
ron-Davis, Beckstead, Curtis, & Goetz, defined indices of happiness and unhappi-
1994), (b) were simple and applicable across ness. Two validation evaluations were con-
different situations (Alevizos, DeRisi, Liber- ducted in separate phases.
man, Eckman, & Callahan, 1978), and (c) Method
would have reasonably clear face validity
(Anderson, Ball, & Murphy, 1975). Happi- Participants. Initially, 5 students were ob-
ness was defined as any facial expression or served while a teacher assistant interacted in-
vocalization typically considered to be an in- dividually with each student during typical
dicator of happiness among people without classroom activities. One student was not
disabilities including smiling, laughing, and observed because of absences. Each student
yelling while smiling. Unhappiness was de- was observed on three occasions using the
fined as any facial expression or vocalization procedures described earlier. Indices of hap-
typically considered to be an indicator of un- piness and unhappiness were observed for
happiness among people without disabilities each student. Subsequently, 4 students were
such as frowning, grimacing, crying, and selected. These participants were selected be-
yelling without smiling. cause they displayed approach or avoidance
The observation system consisted of a 10- responses during the preference assessment
s partial-interval recording process for hap- conducted prior to the study at a level in-
piness and unhappiness. Each 10-s observa- dicating a strong preference for or against at
tion interval was separated by a 5-s record least one stimulus, using previously estab-
interval. Each observation session lasted 10 lished criteria (Green, Reid, Canipe, &
min. During observer training, observers Gardner, 1991; Green et al., 1988; Pace,
were instructed to record indices of happi- Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985).
ness and unhappiness only if they were cer-
tain such indices were observed. For exam- Phase 1
ple, if an observer was not sure whether a The purpose of Phase 1 was to determine
student's mouth movement was sufficient to whether (a) happiness indices would occur
constitute a smile, the observer would not more frequently in the presence of the most
score a happiness index occurrence based on preferred relative to the least preferred stim-
that movement. Interobserver agreement was ulus and (b) unhappiness indices would oc-
assessed on an interval-by-interval basis for cur more frequently in the presence of the
overall, occurrence, and nonoccurrence least preferred relative to the most preferred
agreement using the formula of agreements stimulus.
divided by agreements plus disagreements Procedures. Two stimuli were selected for
and multiplied by 100%. each participant based on the preference as-
sessment. With this assessment, at least 12
Preference Assessments stimuli were presented to each participant
Prior to the study, a preference assessment one at a time in a series of trials. Each stim-
(Green et al., 1988) was conducted with ulus was placed in front of the participant
70 CAROLYNWW GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
at least 30 times across six sessions. Ap- Lrsea TamffI
proach responses (i.e., apparent voluntary
movement toward the stimulus, continued
contact with the stimulus, positive facial ex- c
pression or vocalization) were used as the 1!
measure of preference (Green et al., 1991).
Avoidance responses (i.e., pushing or turning Iii:
away from the stimulus, negative vocaliza-
tion) were used as the measure of nonpref-
erence. The stimulus approached most often
by a given participant was used as the most gnny I Don Ba Tammy I
75 -
preferred stimulus, and the stimulus avoided
most often was used as the least preferred
stimulus during the subsequent assessment.
One client (Don) did not display avoidance
responses to any stimuli. Therefore, the
stimulus associated with the fewest approach
responses was used as the least preferred
stimulus. The following stimuli (with per-
centage of approach or avoidance responses
1 j25-
0- ~-1m-- m I
Types of Stimull Presented
- Most Preferred W Least Preferred
during the preference assessment) were se-
lected as the most and least preferred stim- Figure 1. Average percentage of observation inter-
uli, respectively: Bea, juice (approached, vals with happiness (top panel) and unhappiness (bot-
tom panel) indices for each participant when present-
87%) and vibrator (avoided, 77%); Tammy, ed with stimuli previously assessed to be the most and
mechanical toy (approached, 70%) and pud- least preferred.
ding (avoided, 90%); Sonny, verbal interac-
tion (approached, 90%) and juice (avoided,
30%); Don, vibrator (approached, 100%) each participant, by a student intern or
and colored light display (avoided, 0% and teacher who was unaware of which stimuli
approached, 0%). represented most and least preferred stimuli.
Observations were conducted while a Overall agreement for happiness averaged
teacher assistant provided the two target 98% (range, 93% to 100%), occurrence
stimuli to each participant. The assistant ini- agreement was 79% (77% to 80%), and
tially presented one stimulus continuously nonoccurrence agreement was 97% (90% to
for 1 to 3 min. The other stimulus was then 100%). Respective averages for unhappiness
presented for 1 to 3 min such that during were 99% (95% to 100%), 88% (50% to
each session, the most and least preferred 100%), and 99% (95% to 100%).
stimuli were presented for equal time periods Results. Results for individual students in-
at least twice each per session. All stimuli dicated two general patterns (Figure 1).
were presented in the same manner as dur- First, Bea and Tammy displayed essentially
ing the previous preference assessment. no indices of happiness (respective means of
Three sessions were conducted with each 1% and 0%) but displayed relatively fre-
participant. quent indices of unhappiness. For both stu-
Observations were conducted by an ex- dents, unhappiness was more frequent when
perimenter. Agreement observations were presented with the least preferred stimulus
conducted on 42% of the sessions, involving (68% for Bea and 47% for Tammy) than
INDICES OF HAPPINESS 71

with the most preferred stimulus (40% and groups of practitioners. One group consisted
0%, respectively). Second, Sonny and Don of 18 practitioners (teachers, group home
showed minimal unhappiness (respective managers, etc.) attending a graduate class on
means of 2% and 0%) but showed relatively severe disabilities. None of these practition-
frequent happiness. Both students showed ers had met the 4 individuals observed in
more happiness when presented with the the tapes. The second group consisted of 8
most preferred stimulus (50% for Sonny and teacher assistants (2 assistants per target stu-
25% for Don) than with the least preferred dent) who were very familiar with the stu-
stimulus (7% and 3%, respectively). dents. Each assistant had previously worked
with a student for at least 9 months.
Phase 2 The practitioners were instructed to ob-
Phase 2 attempted to obtain a second type serve the degree to which each participant
of validation for the happiness and unhap- appeared to be happy or unhappy in each
piness indices, involving opinions of practi- tape segment. The tape presentation process
tioners who were experienced with people involved first observing both tape segments
with multiple disabilities. A 7-point Likert to familiarize the practitioners with the on-
scale was used to solicit opinions regarding going activity, then observing one segment
how happy or unhappy a respective partici- and completing the rating form, and then
pant appeared to each practitioner. The neu- viewing the second tape segment for that
tral item on the scale was neither happy nor participant and completing the rating scale.
unhappy (scale value of 4) and the extremes After viewing both tape segments, the prac-
of the scale were extremely unhappy (1) and titioners recorded in which of the two seg-
extremely happy (7). In addition, practition- ments the participant appeared to be hap-
ers were asked to indicate in which of two pier. Because one segment for each partici-
situations each participant appeared happier. pant contained one of the target indices of
Procedures. During Phase 1, videotape happiness or unhappiness and one segment
samples of the 4 participants were obtained. contained no indices of happiness or unhap-
For Bea and Tammy, who displayed unhap- piness, the order of presentation of the two
piness but essentially no happiness, one tape types of segments was counterbalanced
segment was obtained for behavior that had across participants. The practitioners who
been scored by observers as demonstrating were unfamiliar with the participants viewed
indices of unhappiness, and one segment tape segments for all participants, whereas
was obtained that had been scored as dem- practitioners who were familiar with a par-
onstrating neither happiness nor unhappi- ticipant viewed tape segments only for the
ness. For Sonny and Don, who displayed participant with whom he or she was famil-
happiness but essentially no unhappiness, iar.
one tape segment was obtained for behavior
that had been scored by observers as dis- Results
playing happiness, and one segment was ob- Ratings of both groups of practitioners re-
tained for behavior that had been scored as garding happiness and unhappiness coincid-
neither happiness nor unhappiness. Each ed closely with the systematically observed,
tape segment was brief, in accordance with behaviorally defined indices of happiness
the rather short duration of each observed and unhappiness for 3 participants and cor-
instance of happiness or unhappiness, en- responded generally for 1 participant (Figure
compassing approximately 2 min. 2). For Bea and Tammy, whose tape seg-
The tape segments were viewed by two ments presented behavior that had been pre-
72 CAROLYN W GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
Ranking of Unfamiliar Persons
W= Ranking of Familiar Persons

sonny I Don I

i 5- i 5-
0 4- 0 4-
0 3- 3-
12 -

1- m: 1 -
Happines N Happineg Neither Happines
nor Unhappines

Pias I
7 -1_rntr~
,Ia r MY]>
>'
x a
5-
i

4 L a) 4

0 3 3
2

>It!'
Unhappinen Neiter Hamp Unhappine Newer Hai
nor Unhappy nor Unhappln
Behavioral Indices Reflected on Behavioral Indices Reflected on T
Figure 2. Average happiness rankings across practitioners who were unfamiliar with each participant, and
across practitioners who were familiar with a participant, when the participant exhibited behaviors previously
recorded as happiness indices (for Sonny and Don), unhappiness indices (Bea and Tammy) and neither hap-
piness nor unhappiness indices (all 4 participants).

viously scored as unhappy by observers in Bea (just above neither happy nor unhappy on
Phase 1, the practitioners' ratings (averaged the scale) and 5.2 for Tammy (just above
across practitioners familiar and unfamiliar happy).
with the participants) fell in the unhappy For the 2 participants whose tape seg-
range of the Likert scale, averaging 1.5 for ments included previously observed indices
Bea and 2.8 for Tammy. In contrast, for the of happiness, practitioner ratings also coin-
tape segments presenting behavior that had cided with the observed behavioral indices,
been previously scored as showing no indices although the correspondence for Don was
of unhappiness (or happiness), the practi- less apparent. The average rating for both
tioners' ratings were higher, averaging 4.1 for Sonny's and Don's tape segments that in-
INDICES OF HAPPINESS 73
cluded previously observed indices of hap- There were also similarities between the def-
piness was 5.9 (just below very happy). In inition of an avoidance response and the def-
contrast, ratings of the segments that had inition of unhappiness. However, the stimuli
been previously observed to show no hap- used in Phase 1 generally involved partici-
piness or unhappiness averaged 3.8 (just be- pant interactional responses related to the
low neither happy nor unhappy) for Sonny stimuli as approach and avoidance behaviors
and 5.0 (happy) for Don. during the preference assessment, not just fa-
The practitioners' responses to the ques- cial expressions and vocalizations that con-
tion regarding the tape segment in which stituted the happiness and unhappiness in-
each participant appeared to be happier dices. Hence, it seems unlikely that the in-
strongly coincided with the systematically creased happiness and unhappiness indices
observed, behaviorally defined indices. For that occurred in the presence of most and
both Bea and Tammy, 95% of the practi- least preferred stimuli, respectively, were due
tioners reported that the participants ap- solely to overlap in the definitions. Never-
peared to be happier in the tape showing no theless, additional validation support was
observed indices of happiness or unhappi- sought in Phase 2.
ness relative to the tape showing unhappi- Results of Phase 2 indicated that there
ness. Similarly, for Sonny, 100% of the prac- was a relatively strong consensus among 22
titioners and for Don, 95% of the practi- practitioners that participants appeared to be
tioners reported that the students appeared happier when (a) they exhibited behaviors
to be happier in the tape showing indices of previously defined as indicating happiness
happiness relative to the tape showing no relative to exhibiting no behaviors indicating
observed indices of happiness or unhappi- happiness and (b) they exhibited no behav-
ness. iors indicating unhappiness relative to exhib-
iting behaviors previously defined as indicat-
Discussion ing unhappiness. The latter results occurred
Results of Experiment 1 provide several whether the practitioners were familiar or
types of support for the validity of the be- unfamiliar with the participants. As indicat-
haviorally defined indices of happiness and ed earlier, results for Don were less apparent
unhappiness. Systematic observations in than with the other participants, although
Phase 1 indicated that participants appeared they still tended to conform to the basic hy-
to be happier when presented with preferred pothesis. When considered in conjunction
stimuli relative to less preferred stimuli. It with results of Phase 1, the results appear to
was hypothesized that participants would be represent convergent validation (Anderson et
happier in the presence of stimuli they typ- al., 1975) of the defined indices of happiness
ically approached than in the presence of and unhappiness.
stimuli they avoided, and the results sup-
ported this hypothesis. However, the asso- EXPERIMENT 2
ciation between the presence of high-pref- The purpose of Experiment 2 was to
erence stimuli and indices of happiness may demonstrate that indices of happiness could
have been due in part to similarities in the be systematically increased by classroom
definition of an approach response that was staff.
used to identify high-preference stimuli and
the definition of happiness used in Phase 1. Method
For example, one part of the definition for Participants. The 3 students with the
approach was a positive facial expression. highest and most consistent classroom atten-
74 CAROLYN W GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
dance participated. All experimental proce- acted individually in ways that they believed
dures were conducted by the two teacher as- resulted in the most enjoyable experiences for
sistants. the participants. These interactions included
Observation system. Observation sessions verbal interactions, air from hair dryer blown
occurred as described for Phase 1. Agree- on arm and light tickling on arms for Bea,
ment checks occurred during 31 % of all ob- light tickling on arms and rubbing of arms
servations, including each experimental con- with a mitt for Sonny, and verbal interaction
dition and participant. A teacher who was and bouncing in a reclining wheelchair for
unaware of the ongoing experimental con- Don. Thus, throughout a 10-min observa-
ditions conducted 13% of the checks. Over- tion session, the assistants intermittently in-
all agreement averaged 98% (range, 90% to teracted with the client in the ways that they
100%) for happiness and 99% (range, 98% believed pleased the client or presented stim-
to 100%) for unhappiness. Occurrence uli identified as preferred by a formal pref-
agreement averaged 83% (range, 60% to erence assessment. Each interaction or stim-
100%) for happiness and 97% (range, 75% ulus presentation lasted from 1 to 3 min.
to 100%) for unhappiness. Nonoccurrence The third component of the intervention
agreement averaged 92% (range, 89% to consisted of planned initiation and termi-
100%) for happiness and 99% (range, 97% nation of the presentation of stimuli based
to 100%) for unhappiness. on observed happiness-unhappiness indices.
Experimental conditions-baseline. During Assistants were informed of the behavioral
baseline, the teacher assistants conducted definitions of happiness and unhappiness
their usual classroom routine. Each assistant prior to the first intervention session. Sub-
rotated among students to implement one- sequently, they were instructed (a) to im-
to-one teaching programs. Between teaching mediately discontinue an ongoing item pre-
trials and individual programs (which typi- sentation or activity upon any indication of
cally occurred for approximately 10 min at the participant's unhappiness and (b) to
a time), the assistants interacted briefly with change items or activities after 1 min during
all other students. Participants who were not which no indication of happiness was ob-
receiving formal one-to-one teaching pro- after each experimenter provided
served. An feedback
session, indicating whether
grams were provided with stimulation de- ipants displayed more happiness indices partic-
vices, such as a switch-activated vibrator, and ative to baseline. rel-
received social interaction from an assistant Following the investigation, the assistants
or an interaction to prompt or praise acti- continued to implement the fun time pro-
vation of stimulation devices approximately gram intermittently during the classroom
once every 3 min on average. Follow-up observations were conducted
Experimental conditions-fimn time pro- day. at periods ranging from 22 to 24 weeks
gram. The intervention to increase indices of across participants.
happiness, the "fun time program," consist- Experimental design A multiple probe de-
ed of three components. First, the assistants sign across participants was used to evaluate
presented participants with their previously the fun time program. In addition, an exper-
assessed, most preferred items and activities imental reversal design was conducted for
intermittently for 1 to 3 min during each Bea.
session. These stimuli were as follows: Bea,
hand-held leisure items; Sonny, hug and ver- Results and Discussion
bal interaction; and Don, vibration from a As indicated in Figure 3, low frequencies
hand-held vibrator. Second, assistants inter- of happiness indices occurred during the first
INDICES OF HAPPINESS 75
Fun Time Baseline Fun time Follow-up
50 - Baseline U

40 -
* 30-
_

X
c
20- [Bea
O 10-
) 0_
a& 70- L-- -
- -1 24
(U 60-
I
.c 50 U

340-
._

() 30
ISonny |
> 20-
0
ow 10-
c
*11 0
0 Lo --. 24
0 50 -
X 40-
0 30-
0
~20- U

a.
10 -
0
1 5 10 15 20 25 22
Sessions Weeks
Figure 3. Percentage of observation intervals with happiness indices during each session of all experimental
conditions for each of 3 participants.

baseline for Bea (M = 3%; range, 0% to curred for each participant during the fun
5%), Sonny (M = 9%; range, 0% to 30%) time program. The increase for Bea was
and Don (0%). Subsequent increases oc- small, with a mean of 10% (range, 0% to
76 CAROLYN W GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
15%), although it was consistent and appar- with which they implemented the contin-
ent after the first intervention session. Be- gent aspects of the program in terms of
cause of the relatively small change in Bea's quickly terminating specific activities that ei-
happiness indices, a return to baseline was ther were not accompanied by indices of
conducted. During the second baseline, Bea's happiness or were initially accompanied by
indices of happiness decreased to a mean of happiness indices that subsequently ceased.
2% (range, 0% to 10%) and then increased
to 16% (range, 5% to 30%) during the sec-
ond intervention. Increases in happiness in- GENERAL DISCUSSION
dices during the intervention were more ap- Overall, the results of the two experiments
parent for Sonny (M = 45%; range, 18% to indicate that indices of happiness among
63%) and Don (M = 17%; range, 3% to people with profound multiple disabilities
40%). During each intervention condition, can be defined, reliably observed, and sys-
increasing trends in frequency of happiness tematically increased. The happiness defini-
indices occurred. Follow-up observations in- tion was successfully used to denote differ-
dicated that the frequency of observed hap- ences in happiness across and within partic-
piness indices was maintained well above ipants. The observation system was straight-
baseline levels. forward to use and was reliably implemented
No consistent changes in unhappiness in- by the experimenters, the teachers, and an
dices occurred across experimental condi- intern. These features suggest that the ob-
tions. Bea's unhappiness averaged 9% during servation system may be amenable to routine
baseline and 11% during the intervention. application in classroom settings.
Sonny and Don displayed minimal unhap- Perhaps the most critical aspect of at-
piness, averaging 2% or less for both condi- tempting to objectively observe indices of af-
tions. The latter results are noteworthy be- fect such as happiness among people who
cause withdrawal of less preferred stimuli cannot use conventional means to self-report
contingent on behaviors indicating unhappi- their affect is to ensure that what is being
ness could conceivably negatively reinforce observed is what is intended to be observed.
those behaviors. Although such an outcome Results of Experiment 1 provide initial sup-
did not occur in this investigation, future ap- port for the validity of the happiness indices
plication of similar interventions should in- in several ways. First, it seems reasonable to
clude measurement of unhappiness indices to assume that individuals are likely to be hap-
ensure that such an outcome does not result. pier when presented with preferred stimuli
Results of Experiment 2 indicated that the relative to less or nonpreferred stimuli. Re-
classroom assistants effectively increased in- sults of Phase 1 in Experiment 1 provide
dices of happiness among the participants. empirical support for this proposition. Sec-
Increases in observed happiness indices oc- ond, ratings of professional and paraprofes-
curred during each implementation of the sional personnel experienced with people
fun time program with each participant. with profound disabilities generally coincid-
Happiness indices for each participant also ed with the behaviorally defined, systemati-
appeared to increase with continued expo- cally observed happiness indices.
sure to the program. One explanation for Despite the relatively consistent support
the latter results is that the assistants became across different measures of the validity of
more proficient over time in conducting the definitions, caution is warranted in in-
components of the program. In particular, terpreting the results. In essence, happiness
they appeared to improve the proficiency is a private event and, as such, is not readily
INDICES OF HAPPINESS 77

amenable to direct study in the manner typ- could be increased or decreased in frequency
ically used in behavior analysis (Kennedy & as appropriate through systematic interven-
Souza, 1995). Rather, as in this investiga- tion. This type of research model may en-
tion, private events are studied indirectly, by hance further study of important experiences
focusing on public behaviors that are pre- for people with profound disabilities that
sumed to correlate with the private event of heretofore were considered to be beyond the
concern. Due to the correlational compo- realm of the behavior-analytic field.
nent, definitive conclusions regarding the The classroom intervention used to in-
degree to which the private event (i.e., hap- crease happiness indices was relatively simple
piness) was truly observed and altered are to implement. The assistants required only
difficult to derive. a few minutes of initial instruction along
One concern that illustrates the caution with a few minutes of daily feedback to im-
with which the results should be interpreted plement the fun time program. In addition,
pertains to whether individuals might inten- the assistants continued to implement the
tionally engage in behaviors that are typically program after completion of the study, with
correlated with a private event when such increases in happiness indices that were
behaviors are not indicative of the private maintained during follow-up observations
event at the time. Specifically, an individual conducted 5 months later. However, one
might smile for social or operant reasons component of the program involved proce-
when he or she is not happy, such as to avoid dures that can be rather complex and time
displeasing someone who is attempting to consuming. Specifically, the assistants relied,
humor the individual. However, this con- in part, on presentation of stimuli that had
cern seems to be less of an issue with people been previously identified through system-
who have profound mental and physical im- atic assessments to be strongly preferred by
pairment and minimal communicative be- the participants. Systematic preference as-
haviors and who also exhibit few behaviors sessments with people who have profound
that are under apparent social control. In- mental and physical impairments can be
dividuals with less serious disabilities than procedurally complex (Green et al., 1988).
profound multiple handicaps who engage in Future research to analyze the effective com-
frequent interpersonal interactions may be ponents of the program, and especially the
more likely to display indices of happiness degree to which systematically assessed pre-
that are not representative of their private ferred stimuli affect the program's efficacy, is
experience for social reasons. Hence, the warranted. It should also be noted that hap-
methodology used in this investigation may piness indices were increased during brief,
be less appropriate for these populations. circumscribed sessions. Research is needed to
When applied with caution, the behavior- determine which components of the pro-
analytic approach used in this investigation gram could affect these indices during longer
may offer assistance to individuals with se- portions of an individual's day.
vere disabilities to experience a satisfying If future research supports the approach
quality of life. Other important private to observing and increasing happiness indi-
events (e.g., loneliness) could be operation- ces as presented in this investigation, then
alized into behavioral indices, and a system- several practical applications of the proce-
atic observation process could be developed dures are possible. For example, the system
to monitor the indices reliably. Validation for monitoring happiness could be used in
could then be sought for the indices as quality improvement applications as one
shown in this investigation, and the indices measure for evaluating (and comparing) the
78 CAROLYN W GREEN and DENNIS H. REID
quality of various supports for people with profound multiple handicaps. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 24, 537-552.
profound disabilities. The degree to which Green, C. W., Reid, D. H., White, L. K., Halford, R.
different programs are accompanied by high C., Brittain, D. P., & Gardner, S. M. (1988).
rates of happiness indices may represent one Identifying reinforcers for persons with profound
handicaps: Staff opinion versus systematic assess-
measure of the most promising practices for ment of preferences. Journal ofApplied Behavior
evaluating the programs (Meyer, Eichinger, Analysis, 21, 31-43.
& Park-Lee, 1987). Another application Hunt, P., Farron-Davis, F, Beckstead, S., Curtis, D.,
& Goetz, L. (1994). Evaluating the effects of
may be to identify individuals who exhibit placement of students with severe disabilities in
frequent indices of unhappiness and then general education versus special education classes.
determine means of reducing sources of un- Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
happiness. If these and related areas of re- Iwata,Handicaps, 19, 200-214.
B. A. (1991). Applied behavior analysis as
search are pursued, the contributions of be- technological science. Journal of Applied Behavior
havior analysis for enhancing the quality of Analysis, 24, 421-424.
life among people with profound multiple Jordan, J., Singh, N. N., & Repp, A. C. (1989). An
evaluation of gentle teaching and visual screening
disabilities may be increased significantly. in the reduction of stereotypy. Journal ofApplied
Behavior Analysis, 22, 9-22.
Kennedy, C. H. (1994). Manipulating antecedent
REFERENCES conditions to alter the stimulus control of prob-
lem behavior. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis,
Alevizos, P., DeRisi, W, Liberman, R., Eckman, T., 27, 161-170.
& Callahan, E. (1978). The Behavior Observa- Kennedy, C. H., & Souza, G. (1995). Functional
tion Instrument: A method of direct observation analysis and treatment of eye poking. Journal of
for program evaluation. Journal ofApplied Behav- Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 27-37.
ior Analysis, 11, 243-257. Koegel, R. L., & Egel, A. L. (1979). Motivating au-
Anderson, S. B., Ball, S., & Murphy, R. T. (1975). tistic children. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 88,
418-426.
Encyclopedia of educational evaluation. San Fran- Linscheid, T. R., Pejeau, C., Cohen, S., & Footo-
cisco: Jossey-Bass. Lenz, M. (1994). Positive side effects in the treat-
Bailey, J. S. (1981). Wanted: A rational search for the ment of SIB using the Self-Injurious Behavior In-
limiting conditions of habilitation in the retarded. hibiting System (SIBIS): Implications for operant
Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Dis- and biochemical explanations of SIB. Research in
abilities, 1, 45-52. Developmental Disabilities, 15, 81-90.
Chadsey-Rusch, J., DeStefano, L., O'Reilly, M., Gon- Meyer, L. H., Eichinger, J., & Park-Lee, S. (1987). A
zalez, P., & Collet-Klingenberg, L. (1992). As- validation of program quality indicators in edu-
sessing the loneliness of workers with mental re- cational services for students with severe disabili-
rardation. Mental Retardation, 2, 85-92. ties. Journal of the Association for Persons with Se-
Derrickson, J. G., Neef, N. A., & Cataldo, M. F. vere Handicaps, 12, 251-263.
(1993). Effects of signaling invasive procedures Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata,
on a hospitalized infant's affective behaviors. Jour- B. A., & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stim-
nal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 26, 133-134. ulus preference and reinforcer value with pro-
Dunlap, G. (1984). The influence of task variation foundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied
and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect Behavior Analysis, 18, 249-255.
of autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child Reid, D. H., Phillips, J. F, & Green, C. W. (1991).
Psychology, 37, 41-64. Teaching persons with profound multiple handi-
Dunlap, G., & Koegel, R. L. (1980). Motivating au- caps: A review of the effects of behavioral research.
tistic children through stimulus variation. Journal Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 24, 319-336.
ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 13, 619-628. Sailor, W, Gee, K., Goetz, L., & Graham, N. (1988).
Evans, I. M., & Scotti, J. R. (1989). Defining mean- Progress in educating students with the most severe
ingful outcomes for persons with profound dis- disabilities: Is there any? Journal of the Association
abilities. In F. Brown & D. Lehr (Eds.), Persons for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 13, 87-99.
with profound disabilities: Issues and practices (pp. Received May 2, 1995
83-107). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Initial editorial decision July 13, 1995
Green, C. W., Reid, D. H., Canipe, V. S., & Gardner, Revisions received August 28, 1995; October 16, 1995
S. M. (1991). A comprehensive evaluation of re- Final acceptance November 8, 1995
inforcer identification processes for persons with Action Editor, Wayne Fisher

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen