Sie sind auf Seite 1von 20

The

Very Idea(s) of James


Hillman: Dreaming of a
Black Dog and Sticking to
the Image
The following article by Michael Vannoy Adams is a presentation
delivered at the Montreal Jung Society, Montreal, March 15, 2013.

This presentation is about both the idea of James Hillman and the ideas
of James Hillman. Hillman died on October 27, 2011, at the age of 85.
That was, for me, a very sad moment, as I am sure it must have been for
all those who saw, heard, and read him - and especially, of course, for
those who knew him personally.

What does Hillman say about death? I ask this question in the present
tense, for, to me, the very idea of James Hillman and the very ideas of
James Hillman are still very much alive. "We do not die alone," Hillman
says. "We join ancestors and all the little people, the multiple souls who
inhabit our night world of dreams, the complexes we speak with, the
invisible guests who pass through our lives, bringing us the gifts of urges
and terrors, tender sighs, sudden ideas" (1992: 140). I very much like the
idea that Hillman is now with those who, as he says, bring us the gifts of
"sudden ideas." Hillman does not say that we come up with ideas - he
says that ideas come to us. They are given to us, suddenly.
I knew James Hillman for 30 years. In contrast to many others who knew
him much longer than I did, he was never my analyst, he was never my
supervisor; he was never my teacher. He was my friend. Hillman likes the
word "soul." I never have. He and I both, however, like the word "image."
What interests Hillman, he says, is "a psychology of soul," but he
immediately also says that what he bases that project on is "a psychology
of image" (1975: xi). In this respect, Hillman was not my "soul mate" - he
was my "image mate."

The title of a new book that I have written is For Love of the Imagination,
which I have dedicated in loving memory to Hillman. In the book, I say:
"Long ago I fell in love with the imagination. It was love at first sight. I
have had a lifelong love affair with the imagination. I would love for
others, through this book, to fall in love, as I once did, with the
imagination" (Adams 2014: xiii). Hillman also loved the imagination, and
that is one of the reasons, one of many reasons, why I loved him.

I consider the name "archetypal psychology" an infelicity if not a


misnomer. I prefer the name "imaginal psychology." V. Walter Odajnyk
persuasively argues that "imaginal psychology" is a much more accurate
name than "archetypal psychology" for what Hillman advocates (1984:
43). Hillman acknowledges as much when says that the most important
challenge of contemporary psychology is "the differentiation of the
imaginal" (1975: 37).

The title of this presentation is "The Very Idea(s) of James Hillman" - not
"The Very Image(s) of James Hillman." If Hillman is an imaginal
psychologist, why "ideas" and not "images"? Although Hillman
emphasizes imagination, he also appreciates ideation. He says that
although each individual "has some talent," it is "rare" for any individual
"to have more than one." What one talent does Hillman say he has? It is
not imagination. "Mine," Hillman says, "is ideation" (1992: 145). At the
"Festival of Archetypal Psychology in Honor of James Hillman" at Notre
Dame University in 1992, I delivered a presentation with the title "My
Imaginal Hillman" (Adams 1992). The title of this presentation could be
"My Ideational Hillman."

The title of one of the most important books that Hillman wrote is Re-
Visioning Psychology. It could, equally well, have been Re-Thinking
Psychology. I do not mean that Hillman was a "thinking type." As I
experienced him, he was at least as much a "feeling type." In Lectures on
Jung's Typology, which includes essays by Marie-Louise von Franz and
Hillman, the title of the contribution by Hillman is not "The Thinking
Function" but "The Feeling Function" (1981). The title of the first book that
Hillman wrote was not Imagination or Ideation but Emotion (1960). Think,
however, if you will, of Hillman in a pose with his elbow on his knee and
his fist under his chin.

When I think of Hillman, I think of a lecture that he presented several


years ago. The title of that lecture, which I attended, was "Getting in
Touch with Your Thinkings." Hillman presented the lecture at the New
York Open Center. The title was a provocation, a deliberate contrast to
the "New Age" slogan "Getting in Touch with Your Feelings." Hillman was
a thoughtful Jungian. He was a thinking Jungian's thinker. He re-thought
psychology - or at least Jungian psychology.

When Jung discusses thinking, he cites William James, who, as Jung


says, differentiates between "two kinds of thinking." These are "directed
thinking" and "non-directed thinking." Directed thinking is what Jung calls
"logical thinking" (1911-12/1952, CW 5: par. 11). In contrast, he says that
in non-directed thinking "image piles upon image" (1911-12/1952, CW 5:
par. 19). Non-directed thinking is what I might call "imaginal thinking" -
and this is the kind of thinking that Hillman emphasizes. It is thinking as
imagining: thoughts as images.

"By emphasizing ideation," Hillman says in Re-Visioning Psychology, "we


shall be assuming the passionate importance of psychological ideas." He
says: "There seems to be nothing more astounding in the field of
psychology than its scarcity of interesting ideas" (1975: 115). In Inter
Views, Hillman says that "it seems that Jungians are not interested in
ideas." He continues: "Many Jungians have the feeling that they have all
the ideas they need; Jung gave them the ideas, all they need do is apply
them or work with them. They are satisfied" (1983: 35). He says of these
Jungians: "They simply live off Jung's ideas (or Freud's, for that matter)
without working the field one inch further themselves" (1983: 36).

Jung had many, many ideas. Think of only some of them: archetype,
complex, imago, ego, persona, shadow, anima, animus, self, personal
unconscious, collective unconscious, compensatory function, prospective
function, transcendent function, individuation, amplification, active
imagination, introversion, extraversion, objective level, subjective level,
psychic reality, psychic energy, symbol of transformation, and
synchronicity. Is it any wonder that so many Jungians are satisfied with
Jung's ideas and feel that all they need do is apply them or work with
them? Hillman does not believe that Jungians should accept the whole of
Jung. He believes that they should accept the parts of Jung that appeal to
them and reject the parts that do not. Hillman has a take-it-or-leave it
attitude toward Jung's ideas. He takes some and leaves others.

For Hillman, contemporary Jungian psychology is not ideation but


ideology - that is, a belief system. In contrast to the many Jungians who
are satisfied with Jung's ideas and feel that all they need do is apply them
or work with them, Hillman says: "That's not how I work with ideas. I don't
'believe' Jung or 'believe in' his ideas. His ideas are valuable because
they are so good to work with and" - Hillman adds - "against" (1983: 33).
What does it mean to work both with and against Jung's ideas? It means
not to be a conventional, conservative Jungian. It means, as I have said,
not to do what so many Jungians do, which is "to repeat Jung, over and
over again, in a rote, uncritical, uncreative way" (Adams 2014: 76). To do
that is just imitation, not individuation, which Hillman defines as the
imagination "to live one's oddity" (1983: 161). To work both with and
against Jung's ideas is not to identify with them in a servile, slavish, or
sycophantic way but to relate to them in a critical, creative, and, if I may
say so, odd way. Surely, this is what Jung meant when he said: "Thank
God I am Jung and not a Jungian." I might even say that, for Hillman,
individuation is imagination.

In "The Archetypal School," the chapter that I contributed to The


Cambridge Companion to Jung, I quote a personal communication from
Hillman. Before I submitted the chapter for publication, I mailed a draft to
Hillman for comments. In reply, he said that he had not founded a
"school." Schools of psychology were anathema to Hillman. "To set up a
school creates immediately a new orthodoxy," he says in Inter Views. "We
certainly don't need more orthodoxies - if anything, we need more
heterodoxies" (1983: 33). Rather than found a school, Hillman modestly
said that he had merely pursued a certain "direction" in Jung (Adams
2008: 109). What direction was that? That direction, I have emphasized,
was toward the imagination (Adams 2012: 74).

In this respect, there is not just one Jung. There are at least two Jungs. I
do not mean the "No. 1" and "No. 2" Jungs in Memories, Dreams,
Reflections (1963: 57). I mean two Jungs with radically different
implications for the theory and practice of Jungian psychology. One of
these is what I might call the "conceptual Jung." The other is what I might
call the "imaginal Jung." Hillman, as I do, prefers the imaginal Jung to the
conceptual Jung.
What do I mean by the "conceptual Jung"? I mean the Jung who replaces
images with concepts. In contrast to this procedure, Hillman espouses "a
psychology that's not conceptual" (1983: 2). What do I mean by the
"imaginal Jung"? I mean the Jung who says: "To understand the dream's
meaning I must stick as close as possible to the dream images"
(1934, CW 16: par. 320). This is the Jung who says: "We have to keep it
very simple and stick to the image" (2008: 332). In The Dream and the
Underworld, Hillman says that this is "a method that Lopez-Pedraza
felicitously calls 'sticking to the image" (1979: 194). For Hillman, to "stick
to the image" (rather than replace it with a concept) is the fundamental
methodological principle of archetypal psychology - or, as I prefer to say,
imaginal psychology.

In a strict sense, to "stick to the image" is Jung's idea, not Hillman's idea.
That is, it does not originate with either Rafael Lopez-Pedraza or Hillman
but derives from Jung. It is Hillman, however, who promulgates the idea
as a maxim. Had Hillman not emphasized the idea of sticking to the
image, I would argue, it is quite probable that it would never have attained
the prominence and the importance that it has in contemporary Jungian
psychology. Before Hillman, to stick to the image was merely one idea
among many ideas in Jungian psychology - and hardly a conspicuous
idea. After Hillman, it assumed the status of a dictum.

Hillman worked against Jung's idea of a conceptual psychology and


worked with Jung's idea of an imaginal psychology - and, in that process,
he worked the Jungian field further than any other Jungian after Jung.
"Without Jung," Hillman acknowledges, "I would not have been able to
think any of the things I thought" (1983: 27). He says that he always says:
"I'm just working further from Jung's thought" (1983: 27-8). Note the
prepositions that Hillman employs in relation to Jung's thought - he works
"with" it, "against" it, and "from" it in an effort to further it.

When Hillman discusses ideas, he mentions an "intimacy" between them


and "visual metaphor." He says: "Ideas allow us to envision" (1975: 121).
They also, I would add, allow him to "re-vision." The English word "idea"
derives from the Greek word eidōs, which, Hillman notes, means "that
which one sees" (not, he emphasizes, in an abstract sense but "in a
concrete sense"), as well as "that by which one sees." (The
verb eidō means "to see.") Ideas are both an end and a means to an end.
Hillman says of ideas: "We see them, and by means of them." He says
that "having ideas to see with and seeing ideas themselves" imply "that
the more ideas we have, the more we see" (1975: 121). In this visual
metaphor, to see is not to have "sights" but, as Hillman says,
"psychological ideas, or insights" (1975: 122).

In contemporary English, perhaps the most common synonym for "idea"


is "concept." For Hillman, however, ideas are not concepts. He likes ideas
but dislikes concepts, for, to him, ideas are concrete and concepts are
abstract. Among the many definitions of eidōs one definition is "image" -
especially in the sense of "psychic image." For Hillman, "idea" retains that
etymological sense, which he restores. In this sense, imagination is
intrinsic to ideation. Hillman likes ideas to the extent that, unlike concepts,
they are like images - concrete particularizations rather than abstract
generalizations.

"I don't mean throw out all conceptual language," Hillman says, "but,
generally speaking, conceptual language is where we're caught" (1983:
56-7). He notes that the language of dreams is not a language of
concepts but a language of images. "The word in the dream is not
restricted to conceptual interpretation because the word in the dream is
not a concept," he says. "It's an image arriving out of the imagination"
(1983: 57). Hillman says that "the dream speaks in images, or
even is images" (1979: 55). When he criticizes interpretation, he criticizes
a specific variety of interpretation, which I have called "conceptualization
of the imagination" (Adams 2004: 49) - that is, interpretation that replaces
concrete, particular images with abstract, general concepts. "Dreams call
from the imagination to the imagination," Hillman says, "and can be
answered only by the imagination" (1979: 55).

What do conventional, conservative Jungians do with an image in a


dream? Hillman says that they replace the image - for example, "the
dream's black dog" - with a concept - for example, "sexual impulses,
mother complex, devilish aggression, guardian, or what have you" (1979:
122). Such Jungians are mere taxidermists. "A living dog," Hillman
emphasizes, "is better than one stuffed with concepts" (1979: 123).

Psychologically, perhaps the most famous black dog of the twentieth


century belonged to Winston Churchill. The black dog that belonged to
Churchill was neither a real dog nor a dream dog. It was an imaginal dog
- an example of what Alan Bleakley calls the "animalizing imagination"
(2000). Lord Moran (Sir Charles Wilson), the personal physician of
Churchill in World War II, kept a diary of conversations with Churchill. For
two or three years as a young man, Lord Moran quotes Churchill, "black
depression settled on me" (1966: 167). For many years, Churchill
periodically experienced depressive episodes, which included suicidal
impulses. In 1940, for example, Churchill says:

I don't like standing near the edge of a platform when an express train is
passing through. I like to stand right back and if possible to get a pillar
between me and the train. I don't like to stand by the side of a ship and
look down into the water. A second's action would end everything. (1966:
167)

Lord Moran diagnosed the recurrent depressions as hereditary. He says


to Churchill: "Your trouble - I mean the Black Dog business - you got from
your forebears" (1966: 167). Lord Moran notes that "Black Dog" was
"Winston's name for the prolonged fits of depression from which he
suffered" (1966: 167n.). Anthony Storr, a psychiatrist with Jungian
sympathies, speculates that this personal experience of depression may
have prepared Churchill to endure the vicissitudes of World War II (1988).
In Australia, since 2002, there has been a Black Dog Institute that
specializes in depression and other mood disorders.

Although Hillman never mentions the black dog of Churchill, he


emphasizes "the doggedness of depression, depressive doggedness"
(2008: 151). In contrast to "depression," which is, of course, a concept,
"dog" is an image. Churchill does not replace an image with a concept, as
conventional, conservative Jungians do - he reverses that procedure and
replaces the concept "depression" with the image "dog," as Hillman would
presumably do. Evidently, for Churchill, "black dog" conveys a much more
specific, qualitatively distinctive, vitally accurate impression of the
experience than does "black depression."

"An Inquiry into Image," an important essay from 35 years ago, is a


dialogue between Hillman and a "Protestor," an interlocutor who
questions what Hillman says about images. In the essay, Hillman
contrasts images not with concepts but with symbols. "I come from
Zurich; for the past quarter-century I have lived in a world of symbols," he
says. "They no longer hold my attention" (1977: 62). Why so? Hillman
contrasts the "generality" of symbols with the "particularity" of images
(1977: 64). He says that "there are no symbols as such, only images."
Symbols, he says, "are abstractions from images" (1977: 65). He
criticizes symbols on the same basis that he criticizes concepts. Both
symbols and concepts are abstract generalizations, while images are
concrete particularizations.
It should be no surprise when I say that there is not just one Hillman -
there are at least two Hillmans. The first Hillman experiences the image
"in a specific context, mood, and scene" (1977: 65). For him, the
specificity of the context, mood, and scene is what constitutes the image
as an image rather than a symbol - what endows the image with
particularity in contrast to the generality of a symbol. This is the Hillman
who "sticks to the image in its precise presentation" (1977: 68). The first
Hillman is what I might call the "precision" Hillman. What interest him are
"the actual qualities of the image" (1977: 69). From this perspective, the
actual qualities of the image in a specific context, mood, and scene are
the necessary and sufficient condition for a precise experience of the
image. The second Hillman is what I might call the "proliferation" Hillman.
This is the Hillman for whom the image is a point of departure for more
and more images - not just one image but many images. In contrast to the
Hillman who experiences the image in a specific context, mood, and
scene, this Hillman asks what the image is like. "The operative term is
'like,'" he says. "This is like that" (1977: 86). The result of this procedure is
that images proliferate. There are many images, none of which Hillman
privileges over any of the others.

Consider the actual qualities of the images in the specific context, mood,
and scene of a dream that Hillman presents:

There is a black dog, with a long tail, that shows its teeth at me. I am
terribly afraid. (1977: 86)

In this dream, what I call the "non-ego image" is a dog that is black with a
tail that is long and with teeth that it shows at what I call the "ego-image,"
which is terribly afraid. The "precision" Hillman would presumably stick to
these images just as the dream presents them, but that is not what
Hillman does in this instance. Instead, the "proliferation" Hillman asks
what the images are like:

We simply ask the dreamer, "What is this dog, this scene, this fear, like?"
Then we get: It's like when there is sudden sound and I jump with fright;
like coming to analysis and expecting you to pounce on everything I say;
like anger - sometimes I get so angry (or hungry) that I could savage
anyone who gets near me; like my ulcer gets angry and hungry at the
same time; like my mother used to look - her teeth; like going home after
work in the dark and being afraid my wife will bark at me, jump at me; it's
like dying - I'm so afraid - it's so vicious and low and degrading; it's like a
film I saw when I was little with black dogs in it and I had to leave the
movie theater I was so terrified; like the Jackal God, Anubis; like
Mephistopheles in Faust; like when I get sexy - I want to tear into the
meat and just eat and screw like a dog in the street, anywhere; it's like the
dog was a snake with a long tail. And so on. (1977: 86-7).

A conventional, conservative Jungian might amplify the image of the


black dog, might liken it to an image in some other source - for example,
the black dog in Faust. In this respect, Jung quotes Goethe and says "the
'black dog scampers through corn and stubble' - the poodle who is the
devil himself" (1911-12/1952, CW 5: par. 118). The devil, "in the form of
the black dog," Jung notes, introduces himself to Faust as an intention
with an ironically paradoxical effect: "Part of the power which would / Ever
work evil, but engenders good" (1911-12/1952, CW 5: par. 181). In fact,
Hillman does amplify the image in just this way - he likens the black dog
in the dream to the black dog in Faust. He also, however, likens the
image in the dream not only to that one image but also to many other
images, none of which he privileges.

The Protestor in the dialogue with Hillman questions this proliferation of


images. "I hear you saying more equals good," the Protestor says. "The
more you can say about that dog, the better it is." The Protestor says that
Hillman's method of proliferating seems no different from "Freud's method
of associating" or "Jung's method of amplifying." All of those methods, he
says, seem to depart from the image of the black dog in the specific
context, mood, and scene of the dream "which is your very concern to
stick to" (1977: 87). Are not "some two or three" of the images to which
Hillman likens the black dog in the dream "more relevant" for the dreamer,
the Protestor asks, than any of the others - "his mother's teeth, say, or
Goethe's Mephistopheles" - or are all of the images "equally good?"
(1977: 88)

I prefer the first Hillman, the "precision" Hillman, to the second Hillman,
the "proliferation" Hillman. I do not consider images, the more the better,
all to be equally good. An infinite regress of images does not appeal to
me. I prefer the Hillman who values quality over quantity and who sticks
to the image in a specific context, mood, and scene. This is the Hillman
who says of animals in dreams: "To find out who they are and what they
are doing there in the dream, we must first of all watch the image and pay
less attention to our own reactions to it." In this instance, the dreamer
would watch the black dog to find out what it is doing there in the dream
(showing its teeth) and would pay less attention to his own reaction
(terrible fright). "Then," Hillman says of the image, "we might be able to
understand what it means with us in the dream" (1979: 148).

In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Darwin observes


that, evolutionarily, humans are emotionally expressive much in the same
way that other animals are. Among the emotions that he discusses in
detail is anger. One of the ways that humans express anger, he notes, is
to show their teeth. "This retraction of the lips and uncovering of the teeth
during paroxysms of rage, as if to bite the offender, is so remarkable,
considering how seldom the teeth are used by men in fighting," that
Darwin asks a doctor "whether the habit was common in the insane
whose passions are unbridled" (1955: 242). The doctor affirms that this is
so and, as an example, mentions "an insane lady." Darwin describes the
way the woman expresses anger:

At first she vituperated her husband, and whilst doing so foamed at the
mouth. Next she approached close to him with compressed lips, and a
virulent set frown. Then she drew back her lips, especially the corners of
the upper lip, and showed her teeth, at the same time aiming a vicious
blow at him. (1955: 242)

Other animals also show their teeth to express anger. In this respect,
Darwin specifically mentions the dog: "When a dog is on the point of
springing on his antagonist, he utters a savage growl; the ears are
pressed closely backwards, and the upper lip is retracted out of the way
of his teeth, especially of his canines" (1955: 116). Hillman, too, describes
"the curled lip and bared teeth of the sudden dog jumping at you" (2008a:
154). In humans and other animals, the conical teeth between the lateral
incisor and the first premolar are canines, but so are dogs and other
carnivorous animals (wolves, jackals, foxes, and coyotes) canines,
or canidae. Darwin provides an illustration in which a dog angrily shows
his teeth (1955: 117):
John Rowan also provides an illustration that he entitles, simply, "Animal"
(1993: 55):
In the dream that Hillman presents, a black dog with a long tail shows its
teeth at the dreamer, who is terribly frightened. The non-ego image
expresses an emotion - anger - and the ego-image reacts (as ego-images
almost always do) defensively. The two most famous defenses are "fight"
and "flight." In this instance, the defense that the ego-image employs is
another "f-word." The defensive reaction of the ego-image is (a terrible)
"fright."

In the vast majority of dreams, the ego-image is anxious and suspicious


(even paranoid or phobic) rather than curious and inquisitive in relation to
non-ego images. In this dream, it never occurs to the ego-image to
inquire why the non-ego image shows its teeth. That the black dog shows
its teeth to the dreamer is just a fact, but why? What non-ego images do
not do in dreams is as important as what they do. In this dream, the non-
ego image does not attack or bite the ego-image. Even if the black dog
did attack or bite the dreamer, however, the question would still be the
same: why?

Jung says of animals in dreams that how they appear "depends on the
attitude of the conscious mind." He says that "if it is negative towards the
unconscious, the animals will be frightening" (in this instance, terribly
frightening), while, "if positive, they appear as the 'helpful animals' of
fairytale and legend" (1911-12/1952, CW 5: par. 264). Apparently, in this
instance, the attitude of the conscious mind is so negative toward the
unconscious that the ego-image just assumes that the black dog is not a
helpful animal but a harmful animal - not "man's best friend" but perhaps
even his worst enemy - and, as a result, reacts with fright. Rather than
pause and reflect, ponder and wonder, and ask (for example, in active
imagination) the black dog why it shows its teeth, the dreamer just reacts
defensively.

Why does the black dog in this dream show its teeth? "When a dog
approaches a strange dog or man," Darwin observes, it reacts with anger
(1955: 50). He notes that "the canine teeth are uncovered." When,
however, "the dog suddenly discovers that the man he is approaching, is
not a stranger, but his master," Darwin says, the dog reacts with affection.
He notes that "his tail, instead of being held stiff and upright, is lowered
and wagged from side to side" (1955: 51). Darwin provides an illustration
in which a dog affectionately wags his tail (1955: 55):
Perhaps the most famous image of a dog wagging its tail is Dynamism of
a Dog on a Leash by the futurist painter Giacomo Balla, who depicts
emotion by motion.
Presumably, the dreamer would like for the black dog in the dream to wag
its tail rather than show its teeth, but, as Jung says, dreams depict "the
inner truth and reality of the patient as it really is: not as I conjecture it to
be, and not as he would like it to be, but as it is" (1934, CW 16: par. 304).
What this dream indicates is that the ego-image and the non-ego image -
as they are - are not on familiar terms. This is why the black dog does not
wag its tail but shows its teeth at the dreamer. To the black dog, the
dreamer is a stranger, and so, to the dreamer, is the black dog. In this
sense, the "unfamiliar" is a synonym for the "unconscious." The black
dog, which is unfamiliar with (or unconscious of) the dreamer, reacts with
anger, and the dreamer, who is unfamiliar with (or unconscious) of the
black dog, reacts with fright.

A dog is a Canis familiaris, and a familiaris (or "familiar") is a spirit often


embodied as an animal and held to attend, serve, or guard a person. As
an example, Jung mentions the black dog in Goethe: "A parallel to the
dog-spirit is the poodle in Faust, out of whom Mephistopheles emerges as
the familiar of Faust the alchemist" (1955-56, CW 14: par. 177). In this
dream, however, the black dog is not at all a familiar. The black dog is
just as much a stranger to the dreamer as the dreamer is to the black
dog.
Once it is obvious that the ego-image and the non-ego image are
"estranged," the dreamer has an opportunity to consider how they might
be "familiarized." The question is: how might the dreamer engage the
black dog more effectively? This does not mean for the dreamer to be a
"master" and the black dog to be a "pet." A non-ego image in a dream is
not an animal that is domesticated and kept for the pleasure rather than
the utility of the ego-image. The purpose of psychoanalysis is not for the
ego-image to train non-ego images to obey commands or perform tricks.
(That would truly be "the tail wagging the dog.") Rather, psychoanalysis is
an opportunity for "familiarization" between the ego-image and non-ego
images.

Hillman says that he began to collect animal dreams in 1958. If so, he


collected them for more than half a century. Hillman mentions two
dominant themes in these animal dreams: "the dreamer trying to
eradicate the animal" and "the dreamer seeing the animal as more
dangerous than it turns out to be" (2008a: 184). The animal dreams that
dreamers recount to me confirm what Hillman says. These dreamers
almost always react defensively with "fright" and then with either "fight" or
"flight." They just immediately assume that the animals in these dreams
are dangerous and, as a result, tend either to attack or to escape. These
dreamers do not stick to the image to ascertain precisely why the image
does what it does in the dream and how they might engage the image
more effectively - that is, more familiarly - than they do.

What I might call the "danger assumption" toward non-ego images is what
is so problematic about the ego-image. In this respect, Jung says that the
unconscious is "a natural entity" that is "completely neutral." The
unconscious is not intrinsically dangerous. "It only becomes dangerous
when our conscious attitude to it is hopelessly wrong," Jung says. "To the
degree that we repress it, its danger increases" (1934, CW 16: par. 329).
Or, as I prefer to say, non-ego images are only assumptively dangerous
and then only to the extent that the ego-image is defensive (and, as
Hillman notes, the non-ego image frequently turns out to be less
dangerous than the ego-image immediately assumes it to be, or, I would
add, not dangerous at all). The dangerousness of non-ego images is,
ironically, a function of the defensiveness of the ego-image. The one is
directly proportional to the other.

In the dream that Hillman presents, the ego-image reacts defensively to


what it assumes is a dangerous non-ego image. The black dog shows its
teeth at the dreamer, and only then - after it does this - is the dreamer
terribly frightened. Jung, however, reverses this sequence of events. He
argues that the conscious attitude of the dreamer toward the unconscious
is always already defensive - that defense occurs not after but before
danger. The ostensible dangerousness of the non-ego image is the result
of the anticipatory defensiveness of the ego-image. From this
perspective, were this dreamer not always already so terribly frightened of
the unconscious, the black dog would not show its teeth at him.

Several years ago, a prominent Jungian analyst confided to me that he


aspired to rival Hillman as "the top dog" of Jungian psychology. I was no
competition, he said, for I was "the dog that walks alone." The ambition of
the Jungian analyst impressed me and amused me. The description of
me as solitary was accurate. If Jungian analysts are dogs, black or not, in
a pack, I do prefer to walk alone - but if I were to walk with any other
dogs, it would be with Jung and Hillman, sometimes showing my teeth,
sometimes wagging my tail, and - to be precise - always sticking to the
image, the actual qualities of it, in a specific context, mood, and scene.

References

Adams, M.V. (1992) "My Imaginal Hillman; or, James, I'll See You in My
Dreams," Boulder, CO: Sounds True Recordings: audiotape.

Adams, M.V. (2004) The Fantasy Principle: Psychoanalysis of the


Imagination, Hove and New York: Brunner-Routledge.

Adams, M.V. (2008) "The Archetypal School," in P. Young-Eisendrath and


T. Dawson (eds.), The Cambridge Companion to Jung, 2nd rev. ed.,
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 107-24.

Adams, M.V. (2014) For Love of the Imagination: Interdisciplinary


Applications of Jungian Psychoanalysis, London and New York:
Routledge.

Bleakley, A. (2000) The Animalizing Imagination: Totemism, Textuality


and Ecocriticism, Houndmills and London: Macmillan Press and New
York: St. Martin's Press.

Darwin, C. (1955) The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals,


New York: Philosophical Library.
Hillman, J. (1960) Emotion: A Comprehensive Phenomenology of
Theories and Their Meanings for Therapy, London: Routledge & Ken
Paul.

Hillman, J. (1975) Re-Visioning Psychology, New York: Harper & Row.

Hillman, J. (1977) "An Inquiry into Image," Spring: An Annual of


Archetypal Psychology and Jungian Thought, 62-88.

Hillman, J. (1979) The Dream and the Underworld, New York: Harper &
Row.

Hillman, J. (1981) "The Feeling Function," in M.-L. von Franz and J.


Hillman, Lectures on Jung's Typology, Dallas: Spring Publications.

Hillman, J., with Pozzo, L. (1983) Inter Views: Conversations with Laura
Pozzo on Psychotherapy, Biography, Love, Soul, Dreams, Work,
Imagination, and the State of the Culture, New York: Harper & Row.

Hillman, J, and Ventura, M. (1992) We've Had a Hundred Years of


Psychotherapy - And the World's Getting Worse, San Francisco:
HarperSanFrancisco.

Hillman, J. (2008a) "Let the Creatures Be: A Conversation with Thomas


Moore," in Animal Presences, Uniform Edition of the Writings of James
Hillman, Putnam, CT: Spring Publications, 9: 181-7.

Hillman, J. (2008b) "You Dirty Dog!" in Animal Presences, Uniform Edition


of the Writings of James Hillman, Putnam, CT: Spring Publications, 9:
150-60.

Jung, C.G. (1911-12/1952) Symbols of Transformation: An Analysis of the


Prelude to a Case of Schizophrenia, CW 5.

Jung, C.G. (1916/1948) "General Aspects of Dream Psychology", CW 8:


237-80.

Jung, C.G. (1934) "The Practical Use of Dream-Analysis", CW 16: 139-


61.

Jung, C.G. (1955-56) Mysterium Coniunctionis: An Inquiry into the


Separation an Synthesis of Psychic Opposites in Alchemy, CW 14.
Jung, C.G. (1963) Memories, Dreams, Reflections, ed. A. Jaffe, trans. R.
and C. Winston, New York: Pantheon Books.

Jung, C.G. (2008) Children's Dreams: Notes from the Seminar Given in
1936-1940, ed. L. Jung and M. Meyer-Grass, trans. E. Falzeder with T.
Woolfson, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Lord Moran (1966) Winston Churchill: The Struggle for Survival, 1940-
1965, London: Constable & Company.

Odajnyk, V.W. (1984) "The Psychologist as Artist: The Imaginal World of


James Hillman," Quadrant, 17,1: 39-48.

Rowan, J. (1993) Discover Your Subpersonalities: Our Inner World and


the People in It, London and New York: Routledge.

Storr, A. (1988) Churchill's Black Dog, Kafka's Mice, and Other


Phenomena of the Human Mind, New York: Grove Press.
 Home
 Jungian Analysis

o What Is It?
o How To Enter Analysis
o How To Become an Analyst
 Michael Vannoy Adams

o Books:
 For Love of the Imagination
o Book Reviews
o The Fantasy Principle
o Book Reviews
o The Mythological Unconscious
o Book Review
o The Multicultural Imagination

o Articles:
 For Love of the Imagination
 Interpretation and Creativity, Individuation and Destiny: Spielrein and Jung on the Real and the Symbolic
 The Very Idea(s) of James Hillman: Dreaming of a Black Dog and Sticking to the Image
 Thoughts Out of School: Thinking about Jungian Schools of Thought
 The Man Who Was So Pissed Off He Couldn't Pee: Anxiety, Anger, and Identification with the Aggressor
 The Listening Cure: Metaphorical Resonances and the Third Ear
 The Archetype of the Saboteur: Self-Sabotage from a Jungian Perspective
 War, Emotional Possession, and the Underworld: Affects in the Mythological Unconscious
 The Sable Venus on the Middle Passage: Images of the Transatlantic Slave Trade
 Imaginology: The Jungian Study of the Imagination
 Does Myth (Still) Have a Function in Jungian Studies? Modernity, Metaphor, and Psycho-Mythology
 Hillman Alone in Pursuit of the Imagination: Golden Calf Psychology
 If the University Won't Have Jungians, Then How Might Jungians Have the University?
 The Islamic Cultural Unconscious in the Dreams of a Contemporary Muslim Man
 "It Was All a Mistake": Jung's Postcards to Ernest Jones and Kipling's Short Story "The Phantom 'Rickshaw"

o Events and Encounters:


 Moscow Conference on "The Mythological Unconscious"
 "O My Prophetic Soul!" Lecture at Jung Society of Utah
 Red Book Dialogue between Sarah Silverman and Adams
 "The Misinterpretation of Dreams" at IASD Conference
 Derrida and Adams
 Hillman and Adams
 Kembra Pfahler and Adams
 Freud Museum
 Vocatus atque non vocatus deus aderit
 Attitude Comp(ensation)
 Curb Your Ego

o Art:
 Paintings
 Drawings
 Collages
 Photographs

o Presentations, Workshops, and Courses:


 Schedule

o Biography
o Articles, Interviews, and Reviews
 Other Jungian Information

o Links
o Favorite Jungian Books and Articles by Other Authors

Michael Vannoy Adams, D.Phil., L.C.S.W.


E-mail: adamsmv@aol.com | Telephone: 646-515-9513

Copyright © Michael Vannoy Adams. All rights reserved.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen