Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Japanese Psychological Research doi: 10.1111/jpr.

12130
2016, Volume 58, No. 4, 355–366

Emotion Regulation and Coping Strategies in


Pedagogical Students with Different Attachment Styles


SIMONA PROSEN* and HELENA SMRTNIK VITULIC University of Ljubljana

Abstract: Emotion regulation (ER) and coping strategies were compared in 242 peda-
gogical students from the Faculty of Education, University of Ljubljana, with different
attachment styles: secure (51.3%), fearful (31.9%), preoccupied (14.2%), and dismis-
sive (2.7%). The students’ ages ranged from 18 to 24 years (M = 19.02; SD = .92),
and the majority were female (95.0%). They completed three questionnaires asses-
sing their attachment styles and their use of ER and coping strategies. It was deter-
mined that the students used the majority of ER and coping strategies sometimes.
Somewhat different patterns in the use of ER and coping strategies were, however,
present in each attachment-based group. Significant differences between these
groups appeared in their use of the ER strategies of social support, suppression, and
comfort eating, and in the coping strategies of seeking social support and escaping
or avoiding stressful situations.
Key words: attachment style, coping, emotion regulation, pedagogical students.

Emotion regulation (ER) and coping strategies examine the students’ attachment styles and
were explored in pedagogical students with dif- their relationships to their ER and coping pro-
ferent attachment styles. Both ER and coping cesses. ER, coping and attachment styles will
processes are closely connected to internal therefore be introduced in greater detail.
representations of self and others in relation-
ships – attachment styles (Bartholomew & Hor- ER
owitz, 1991) – as these representations influence ER has been defined as the process by which
the individual’s regulatory capacities (Fonagy, individuals influence which emotions they
Steele, & Steele, 1991; Skinner & Zimmer- have, when they have them and how they
Gembeck, 2007). The pedagogical students in experience and express them (Gross, 1998).
our study will become human-relations profes- ER includes the behaviors, strategies, and
sionals, working with various groups of people; skills by which one monitors, evaluates, modu-
being sensitive, trustworthy, and cooperative in lates, inhibits, and enhances emotional experi-
interpersonal relationships are therefore impor- ence and expression in a goal-oriented way
tant competencies they should strive to have (Gross & Thompson, 2009; Thompson &
(Razdevšek Pucko & Rugelj, 2006). Given that Meyer, 2009). Gross and Thompson’s (2009)
these characteristics are closely related to the ER process model offers different ways of
individual’s attachment style, our aim was to influencing the emergence of emotions or
dealing with emotions when they have already

*Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to: Simona Prosen, Department of Education
Studies, Faculty of Education, University of Ljubljana, Kardeljeva ploš
cad 16, Ljubljana 1000, Slovenia. (E-mail:
simona.prosen@pef.uni-lj.si)

© 2016 Japanese Psychological Association. Published by John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.
356 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

been experienced. The situation may be at a reduction of environmental stressors) or


avoided (Situation Selection) or changed emotion-focused (i.e., focusing on the regula-
(Situation Modification). Within the situation, tion of stress-related negative emotional reac-
one may redirect one’s attention to non- tions; Folkman et al., 1986). The individual
emotional content (Attentional Deployment). may apply more active coping strategies aimed
ER may also be accomplished by changing the at facing the stress situation (e.g., planful
situation’s meaning or importance (Cognitive problem-solving, seeking social support) or
Reappraisal). Finally, response tendencies can more passive strategies leading to situation
be altered, including the experiential, physio- avoidance (e.g., escape-avoidance). Active
logical, and behavioral components of coping strategies are generally considered to
response (Response Modulation). The behav- be more efficient when dealing with stress,
ioral component of response modulation may while passive coping strategies are thought to
be achieved via physical activation, suppres- be less efficient (Glasscock, Andersen, Lab-
sion, social support seeking, comfort eating, or riola, Rasmussen, & Hansen, 2013).
psycho-substance use. Among all of these ER Learning how to cope with stressful events
strategies, some – such as reappraisal – are represents an important task of the indivi-
considered more efficient, as they have posi- dual’s development. An overview of various
tive connections with well-being (John & research shows that the biggest qualitative
Gross, 2009), self-esteem, and social support change in development of coping occurs from
(Gross & John, 2003), and negative associa- middle childhood to adolescence, although the
tions with psychopathology (Aldao, Nolen- capacity to use particular coping strategies
Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). On the other (such as planning) may not fully emerge until
hand, some ER strategies – such as suppres- late adolescence or early adulthood
sion – are considered less efficient, as they are (Skinner & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007). Coping
associated negatively with well-being (John & has many psychological correlates with physi-
Gross, 2009; Karademas, Tsalikou, & Tell- cal health (Folkman, Lazarus, Pimley, &
arou, 2010) and social functioning (Gross & Novacek, 1987) and psychological functioning,
John, 2003), and positively to psychopathology such as the quality of social relationships
(Aldao et al., 2010; Gross, 1998). (Zambianchi & Ricci Bitti, 2014). It is there-
By the end of adolescence, individuals become fore important for everyone, but especially for
more responsible for managing their emotions human-relations professionals.
and use more flexible ER strategies tailored to Coping and ER are both conceptualized as
specific sociocultural contexts and personal goals processes of regulation, yet their interrelation
(Thompson & Meyer, 2009). However, ER has not been fully explored (Compas et al.,
development may continue throughout one’s life 2014). In a study with adults by Smrtnik Vitulic
(Diamond & Aspinwall, 2003). As ER plays an and Prosen (2016), these two processes were
important role in many psychological processes, found to be mostly non-associated, suggesting
such as social relationships (Lopes, Salovey, their distinct nature. Consequently, in the
Cote, & Beers, 2005), it is particularly important present study, ER and coping were analyzed
for human-relations professionals. separately regarding their possible specificities
in students with different attachment styles.
Coping
Coping has been defined as “constantly chan- Attachment Style
ging cognitive and behavioral efforts to man- Experiences in child–caregiver interactions are
age specific external and/or internal demands internalized early in life and mold the child’s
that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the expectations and evaluations of the degree of
resources of the person” (Folkman, Lazarus, security, trust, intimacy, autonomy, and so
Gruen, & DeLongis, 1986, p. 572). Coping forth, to be expected in close relationships
strategies may be problem-focused (i.e., aimed (Bowlby, 1997; Fonagy et al., 1991). These

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Regulation regarding students’ attachment 357

internal representations – attachment style – programs in Slovenia (Žvelc & Žvelc, 2006),
continue to influence the quality of relation- 10%, 13%, and 29% reported having dismis-
ships throughout one’s life (Bowlby, 1997). sive, preoccupied and fearful attachment
Several classifications of attachment styles styles, respectively.
in adolescence/adulthood exist, including the Attachment style has several psychological
four-category classification by Bartholomew correlates in adolescence/adulthood. Positive
and Horowitz (1991). Classification is made correlations have been found between secure
according to the relative valences attachment and self-esteem and cognitive
(i.e., positive or negative) of individuals’ cor- functioning (Gross & Thompson, 2009; Jacob-
responding internal representation of self and sen, Edelstein, & Hofmann, 1994; Siegel,
other within relationships. A person with a 1999). On the other hand, some authors
secure attachment style has internalized a pos- (Broberg, Hjalmers, & Nevonen, 2001) report
itive model of self and others, and therefore insecure attachment as more frequent in per-
expresses comfort with both closeness and sons with mental health problems. In late ado-
separateness in close relationships. This lescence – the period to which the participants
attachment style is the most prevalent in the present study belong – Mattanah, Han-
(Bowlby & Ainsworth, 1965). Using their cock, and Brand (2004) found securely
Relationship Questionnaire, Bartholomew and attached students to be better adjusted to col-
Horowitz (1991) found secure attachment in lege than their colleagues with insecure
47% of psychology students from North attachment.
America, while Žvelc and Žvelc (2006) – also Even though attachment styles are consid-
using the mentioned questionnaire – found it ered resistant to change, they can be modified
in 48% of students attending different study when significant changes occur in an indivi-
programs in Slovenia. By contrast, a person dual’s close relationships (Thompson, 1999).
with a dismissive attachment style has positive Persons outside the family (e.g., teachers,
self-evaluations but negative evaluations of trainers, counselors) who might become
others. Such a person acknowledges discom- attachment figures may provide secure
fort with closeness and prefers to remain sep- experiences for those with insecure attach-
arate from others. If a person evaluates ment (Cugmas, 2003).
him/herself negatively and others positively, Attachment style may be thought of as pre-
this is referred to as a preoccupied attach- disposing the individual to a particular percep-
ment style. Such internal representations dis- tion of relationships and consequently his/her
pose a person to expressing a strong need for response, including ER and coping processes.
intimacy and a strong fear of abandonment, Some authors (Carr, Colthurst, Coyle, &
welcoming support from others but fre- Elliott, 2013; Mikulincer, Shaver, & Pereg,
quently worrying about relationships. Finally, 2003) emphasize that early attachment experi-
a person with a fearful attachment style nega- ence represents an important source of a per-
tively evaluates him/herself and others. son’s future ER competence, as ER capacities
He/she therefore fears both rejection and grow and develop most effectively in the con-
emotional closeness and typically avoids text of secure attachment relationships. Some
close relationships (Lopez & Gormley, 2002). associations may also be found between a per-
The last three described attachment styles son’s attachment style and his/her ability to
are referred to as insecure. In psychology stu- cope (Lopez & Gormley, 2002). However,
dents from North America, Bartholomew studies of attachment and its associations to
and Horowitz (1991) found that 18%, 14%, ER or coping in human-relations professionals
and 21% of them reported dismissive, preoc- are scarce. Therefore, in the present study we
cupied, and fearful attachment styles, respec- were interested in how pedagogical students –
tively. Of students attending different study future human-relations professionals – with

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


358 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

different attachment styles used their coping feeling the unpleasant emotions), and behav-
and ER strategies. ioral. Within the behavioral response modula-
tion strategy, a distinction was made between:
(a) physical activation (i.e., go for a walk
Methodology when feeling the unpleasant emotion);
(b) suppression, defined as the reduced
Sample expression or non-expression of emotions
The sample comprised 242 pedagogical stu- (i.e., try to hide unpleasant emotions in front
dents (study programs: primary education, of others); (c) seeking social support (i.e., try
preschool education, social pedagogy) from to find someone to help me when feeling the
the Faculty of Education of the University of unpleasant emotion); (d) comfort eating
Ljubljana, Slovenia. The age of the students (i.e., eat something good when feeling the
ranged from 18 to 24 years (M = 19.02; SD = unpleasant emotions); and (e) psycho-active
.92), and the majority were female (95.0%). substance use (i.e., take a pill or drink alcohol
when feeling the unpleasant emotions). These
Instruments ER strategies are defined in the introduction
The Relationships Questionnaire (Bartholom- of this article. The Spearman rho correlations
ew & Horowitz, 1991) was used to assess between ERSQ items revealed 35% of signifi-
four attachment styles: secure, fearful, preoc- cant ones, yet they were all low, suggesting
cupied, and dismissive. Descriptions of these separate use of all included items. Some
attachment styles are given in the question- authors (Gross, 1998; Schutte, Manes, & Mal-
naire, with one description for each attach- ouff, 2009) have described Situation Selection,
ment style (as described above). The Situation Modification, Attentional Deploy-
students self-rated the degree to which the ment, and Cognitive Reappraisal ER strate-
four descriptions of attachment styles applied gies as prior to emotion occurrence and
to them on a 7-point scale: 1 (almost never), Response Modulation as following such an
2 (very rarely), 3 (sometimes), 4 (moderately), occurrence. However, the ERSQ question-
5 (quite often), 6 (very often), and 7 (almost naire employs a different perspective of these
always). strategies regarding whether they are placed
The Emotion Regulation Strategies Ques- before or after the experience of emotions, as
tionnaire (ERSQ), designed by the authors of Gross and Thompson’s (2009) model of ER
the present article and based on Gross and does not unequivocally explain where within
Thompson’s (2009) model of ER, was used. It the model the emotion itself emerges (see
includes different ER strategies, each repre- more in Prosen & Smrtnik Vitulic, 2014). As
sented by one item: (a) Situation Selection can be observed from the items of the ques-
(i.e., try to avoid the potentially unpleasant sit- tionnaire, Situation Selection and Situation
uation); (b) Situation Modification (i.e., try to Modification represent strategies aimed at the
change the potentially unpleasant situation); emergence of emotions, while all of the other
(c) Attentional Deployment (i.e., try to think ER strategies influence emotions after their
of something pleasant when feeling the occurrence. For each ER strategy, the partici-
unpleasant emotions); (d) Cognitive Reap- pants of the study assessed how often they
praisal of a situation’s meaning (i.e., try to used it on a 5-point Likert-type scale
look at the situation from another perspective (1 = almost never use, 2 = rarely use, 3 = som-
when feeling the unpleasant emotions) and etimes use, 4 = frequently use, 5 = almost
importance (i.e., try to lessen the situation’s always use).
importance when feeling the unpleasant emo- The Slovene translation of the revised Ways
tion); and (e) Response Modulation: experien- of Coping Questionnaire (as cited in Lamovec,
tial (i.e., try to switch off unpleasant emotions), 1994) was used to assess coping strategies. The
physiological (i.e., try to relax the body when questionnaire includes cognitive and behavioral

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Regulation regarding students’ attachment 359

strategies that people use to manage internal confirmed (Shapiro–Wilk tests: all ps < .00), a
and/or external demands in stressful situations. set of non-parametric statistical procedures
All 66 items were self-rated by the students on was applied. Even though a certain level of
a 4-point scale (0 = never use, 1 = sometimes different attachment styles may be found in
use, 2 = frequently use, 3 = almost always use). each individual, one of them is usually prevail-
Eight scales are included in the original ques- ing (Broberg et al., 2001). Such predominant
tionnaire; however, three of these scales attachment style was identified for each partic-
(Confrontive Coping, Self-Controlling, and ipant in our study on the basis of the highest
Accepting Responsibility) were excluded from self-rated attachment description. The students
the further analysis due to their low internal were assigned to one of the four attachment
reliabilities (Cronbach’s alphas below .60), as groups: secure (n = 116), fearful (n = 72), pre-
recommended by Ferligoj, Leskošek, and occupied (n = 32), or dismissive (n = 6). There
Kogovšek (1995). The five scales included in were 16 students whose results could not be
the further analysis were: (a) Distracting assigned to any of the groups because their
(e.g., went on as if nothing had happened); assessment matched two or more attachment
(b) Seeking Social Support (e.g., talked to descriptions equally.
someone who could do something concrete Spearman correlations were insignificant
about the problem); (c) Escape-Avoidance between dismissive and secure (rho = −.06),
(e.g., tried to make myself feel better by eating, dismissive and fearful (rho = −.01), and dis-
drinking, smoking, using drugs or medications, missive and preoccupied attachment (rho =
etc.); (d) Planful Problem-Solving (e.g., made a −.10). According to Cohen’s (1988) sugges-
plan of action and followed it); and (e) Positive tions (values of correlations ≥.10 and <.30
Reappraisal (e.g., found new faith). The inter- indicate relationships of a small magnitude,
nal reliabilities of Distracting, Seeking Social and values of .30–.49 and >.50 indicate moder-
Support, Escape-Avoidance, Planful Problem- ate and high magnitudes, respectively). Corre-
Solving, and Positive Reappraisal were .65, .74, lations in our study were low but significant
.64, .68, and .62, respectively. The Planful between preoccupied and secure (rho = −.28)
Problem-Solving scale is described as primarily and preoccupied and fearful attachment
being problem-focused coping, while Distract- (rho = .26). Only correlation between fearful
ing and Positive Reappraisal are described as and secure attachment was moderate and sig-
emotion-focused coping. The Seeking Social nificant (rho = −.53).
Support scale serves both functions (Lazarus & In the further analysis, the attachment-
Folkman, 1987). based groups of students were compared.
Medians were calculated for the use of each
Procedures ER and coping strategy for each attachment-
The pedagogical students (of primary educa- based group of students. Kruskal–Wallis tests
tion, preschool education, and social peda- were used to compare the use of each ER and
gogy) completed the questionnaires during a coping strategy by these groups. If the differ-
lecture on developmental psychology. Their ences between the groups were significant,
participation was based on their informed con- Mann–Whitney U-tests were applied to deter-
sent, and was voluntary and anonymous. One mine the differences between each pair of
of the authors of this article was present at all groups.
times during the completion of the
questionnaire.
Results
Data Analysis When classified regarding their prevalent
Since the normality of distribution for each attachment style, the majority of the pedagogi-
item in the Emotion Regulation Strategies and cal students in our study were assigned to the
Ways of Coping questionnaires was not secure group (51.3%). There were 31.9% of

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


360 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

Table 1 Medians for ER and coping strategies and their frequency categories in different
attachment styles in students
Secure Fearful Preoccupied Dismissive
(n = 116) (n = 72) (n = 32) (n = 6)

Me Use Me Use Me Use Me Use

ER strategies
Situation selection 3.59 F 3.49 S 3.63 F 3.67 F
Situation modification 3.62 F 3.90 F 3.84 F 3.50 S
Attentional deployment 3.55 F 3.40 S 3.34 S 3.67 F
Cognitive reappraisal
Meaning 3.43 S 3.35 S 3.16 S 3.83 F
Importance 2.72 S 3.69 F 3.38 S 3.17 S
Response modulation
Experiential 1.91 R 1.78 R 1.66 R 2.33 R
Physiological 2.87 S 2.54 S 2.44 R 2.83 S
Behavioral
Physical activation 3.37 S 3.15 S 3.09 S 3.50 S
Suppression 3.27 S 3.68 F 3.19 S 3.00 S
Social support 3.91 F 3.49 S 4.22 F 2.67 S
Comfort eating 2.60 S 2.56 S 3.03 S 1.67 R
Substance use 1.44 R 1.35 R 1.59 R 1.67 R
Coping strategies
Distracting 1.22 S 1.21 S 1.21 S 1.25 S
Seek social support 1.61 F 1.37 S 1.69 F 1.11 S
Escape-avoidance 1.09 S 1.17 S 1.34 S 0.71 S
Planful problem-solving 1.53 F 1.43 S 1.48 S 1.56 F
Positive reappraisal 1.61 F 1.55 F 1.58 F 1.38 S

Note. Me = median of frequency of ER strategy use; F = frequently used; S = sometimes used; R =


rarely used.

the students who demonstrated a prevalent used by most of the pedagogical students with
fearful attachment style, 14.2% who had a different attachment styles, with the exception
prevalent preoccupied attachment style, and of their moderate use in the fearfully attached
2.7% who indicated a prevalent dismissive (Situation Selection) and dismissively attached
attachment style. The use of ER and coping (Situation Modification) groups of students.
strategies by students with different prevalent All of the other ER strategies included in
attachment styles were outlined (Table 1) and our study were aimed at influencing emotion
compared (Table 2). Average scores (Mes) for after its emergence. The first, Attentional
all ER strategies ranged between 1.35 and Deployment, was frequently used by students
4.22. For ER strategies, the criteria of Me > 3.5 with prevalent secure and dismissive attach-
as frequently, 2.5 ≤ Me ≤ 3.5 as sometimes, ments, while the other two groups of students
and Me < 2.5 as rarely used were applied. used it sometimes. Cognitive Reappraisal of
Average scores for all coping strategies ranged the situation’s meaning and importance is an
between .71 and 1.69. For coping strategies, ER strategy that was sometimes used by the
the criteria of Me > 1.5 as frequently, students, with the exceptions of its frequent use
.5 ≤ Me ≤ 1.5 as sometimes, and Me < .5 as by students who had a dismissive attachment
rarely used were applied. style (Reappraisal of Meaning) and by fearfully
Situation Selection and Situation Modifica- attached students (Reappraisal of Importance).
tion, the two ER strategies aimed at the proc- All of the groups of students used Experiential
ess of emotion emergence, were frequently Response Modulation and Substance Use

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Regulation regarding students’ attachment 361

Table 2 Significant differences in mean ranks of ER and coping strategies in different


attachment styles in students
Secure 1 Fearful 2 Preoccupied 3 Dismissive 4 Kruskal–Wallis Mann–Whitney
R R R R

ER strategies
Social 119.37 96.52 141.36 55.17 χ2 (3) = 18.05 1 > 2,4
support p = .00 3 > 2,4
Suppression 106.31 132.33 100.94 93.50 χ2 (3) = 10.04 2 > 1,3
p = .02
Comfort 112.89 109.53 134.98 58.33 χ2 (3) = 8.51 4 < 1,2,3
eating p = .04
Coping strategies
Seek social 121.90 94.71 128.02 64.58 χ2 (3) = 13.05 1 > 2,4
support p = .01 3>2
Escape- 105.10 116.63 136.17 57.50 χ2 (3) = 10.51 2>4
avoidance p = .02 3 > 1,4

Note. R = mean rank; >the first group perceived ER and coping strategies as more frequently used than the
second; <the first group perceived ER and coping strategies as less frequently used than the second;
1 = secure attachment; 2 = fearful attachment; 3 = preoccupied attachment; 4 = dismissive attachment.

rarely. Physiological Response Modulation was than each of the other attachment-based
rarely used by the preoccupied students, while groups.
the other three attachment-based groups used With regard to coping strategies, pedagogi-
it sometimes. The remaining ER strategies, cal students with different attachment styles
which are all defined as Behavior Response sometimes used Distracting and Escape-
Modulation (physical activation, suppression, Avoidance. On the other hand, students with
social support, comfort eating), were sometimes secure and preoccupied attachment styles used
used by most of the students in the various Seek Social Support frequently, while the
attachment-based groups. However, social other two groups of students used this strategy
support was frequently used by securely sometimes. Planful Problem-Solving was used
attached and preoccupied students, while sup- frequently by the securely and dismissively
pression was frequently used by fearfully attached students, while the other groups used
attached students. On the other hand, the use it sometimes. Lastly, Positive Reappraisal was
of food to comfort the emotions was a rarely sometimes used only by dismissively attached
used ER strategy by the dismissively attached students, while the other groups used it
students. frequently.
Significant differences between the Significant differences between the
attachment-based groups of students (Kruskal– attachment-based groups of students (Kruskal–
Wallis test; p > .05) are confirmed for the use Wallis test; p > .05) were confirmed for the use
of three ER strategies: social support, suppres- of two coping strategies: seeking social support
sion, and comfort eating. According to Mann– and escape-avoidance. Mann–Whitney U-tests
Whitney U-tests, securely attached and preoc- further indicated that securely attached and pre-
cupied students used social support more fre- occupied students more frequently sought social
quently than their colleagues with fearful or support than fearfully attached students.
dismissive attachment. Fearfully attached stu- Securely attached students also used this strat-
dents suppressed their emotions more fre- egy more than the dismissively attached group
quently than securely attached or preoccupied of students. Fearfully attached and preoccupied
ones. Lastly, dismissively attached students students escaped and avoided stressful situations
used the ER strategy of comfort eating less more than their colleagues with a dismissive

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


362 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

attachment style. Furthermore, preoccupied stu- attachment-based groups of students most fre-
dents used the escape-avoidance coping strategy quently used strategies aimed at influencing
more than securely attached ones. emotions when they had not yet emerged: Situ-
ation Selection and Situation Modification.
This implies an active relationship towards
Discussion
potentially emotion-eliciting situations. How-
The pedagogical students in our study are ever, the efficiency of such ER strategies
future human-relations professionals. As such, should be discussed within each context. In the
their internal representations of self and others continuation, there was a discussion of ER
within relationships – attachment styles strategies aimed at emotions when they had
(Bowlby & Ainsworth, 1965) – are relevant to already emerged. Firstly, attentional deploy-
them not only because of their private relation- ment was used sometimes or frequently in all
ships but also because of their future work com- of the attachment-based groups of students.
petencies (found in individuals with secure This may be due to the students’ experience
attachment) of sensitivity to others, coopera- with applying this strategy when interacting
tion, and trust in interpersonal relationships with children during their practical study expe-
(Razdevšek Pucko & Rugelj, 2006). As the rience, which they undertake as pedagogical
selection of the majority of study programs – students. Since they may apply this strategy
including pedagogical programs – in Slovenia is when interacting with children quite often – as
based on students’ academic achievement in this strategy is frequently used by teachers
secondary school, there is no information on (Sutton, Mudrey-Camino, & Knight, 2009) –
whether the selected students had all of the they may also use it more themselves. Further-
aforementioned characteristics. The predomi- more, cognitive reappraisal of the situation’s
nance of the secure attachment style is gener- meaning and importance was predominantly
ally expected (Bowlby & Ainsworth, 1965). used sometimes by the attachment-based
Approximately half of the students included in groups of students. This strategy could be
our study had a prevalent secure attachment, encouraged in students, as it is generally con-
which is in line with the results of some other sidered to be an effective ER strategy con-
authors (Žvelc & Žvelc, 2006), yet it might be nected to well-being, self-esteem, and having
somewhat concerning regarding their future social support (Gross & John, 2003). Experien-
work orientation. In a further comparison of tial response modulation, including “turning
students in our study with Slovenian psychology off” one’s emotional experience, was only
students (Žvelc & Žvelc, 2006), the percentages rarely used by any of the students, as it is prob-
of students with fearful and preoccupied attach- ably difficult to achieve. Psycho-active sub-
ment styles were similar while the percentages stances were also rarely used by any of the
of students with prevalent dismissive attach- students, perhaps indicating their awareness of
ment were lower in our study. However, it the negative consequences. Other response-
should be noted that, despite the fact that each oriented ER strategies were sometimes used
individual usually has one prevailing attach- by most of the participants. Summarizing the
ment style, a certain level of other attachment use of ER strategies aimed before or after the
styles may also be found (Broberg et al., 2001). experience of emotions, a clear distinction
In the present study, we were also interested between them could not be clearly recognized.
in how pedagogical students with different Significant differences between the
attachment styles used their ER and coping attachment-based groups of students appeared
strategies, as these may be closely intertwined in the use of three ER strategies: social support,
with attachment characteristics (Mikulincer suppression, and comfort eating. Students
et al., 2003). Detailed analysis of ER strategies whose prevalent attachment style was secure or
selected in accordance with Gross and Thomp- preoccupied used social support frequently, sig-
son’s model (2009) reveals that all of the nificantly more than their colleagues with

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Regulation regarding students’ attachment 363

fearful or dismissive attachment. This may be of two of the coping strategies: seeking social
connected to the fact that both attachment support and escape-avoidance. Seeking social
styles – secure and preoccupied – include a pos- support is a frequently used coping strategy in
itive evaluation of others (Bartholomew & Hor- the group of students with a prevalent secure
owitz, 1991), resulting in students with this kind attachment style, being used significantly more
of attachment having a greater belief that than in the fearfully and dismissively attached
others may offer efficient help. Another ER groups. Preoccupied students also used the
strategy in which differences between the seeking social support coping strategy fre-
attachment-based groups appeared was sup- quently, significantly more than their fearful
pression, defined as the reduced or non- colleagues. Securely attached and preoccupied
expression of emotions (Gross, 1998). The fact students shared a positive model of others
that fearfully attached students suppress their (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), and their
emotions frequently – significantly more than more frequent search for help in stressful
securely attached or preoccupied students – situations than students in the other two
may be due to their negative evaluation of attachment styles was therefore to be
others, whom they may perceive as being criti- expected. However, why significant differences
cal towards them (Bartholomew & Horowitz, were not confirmed between preoccupied and
1991). The question as to why this difference dismissive students in their use of social sup-
does not apply to the group of dismissively port seeking remains unclear. Seeking social
attached students, who also negatively evalu- support is described as a more active coping
ated others, may be answered by the fact that strategy, and as such is generally considered to
the latter group simply cared less about the be more efficient when dealing with stress
opinions of others, as they were not interested (Glasscock et al., 2013). Its more frequent use
in establishing close relationships, a characteris- in securely attached and preoccupied students
tic known as compulsive self-sufficiency is therefore encouraging.
(Bowlby, 1997). This use of suppression may Escaping and avoiding stressful situations is
be somewhat concerning, as the frequent use more present in fearfully attached and preoc-
of this strategy is considered less efficient and cupied students than in their colleagues with a
negatively connected to well-being (John & dismissive attachment style. Given that both
Gross, 2009; Karademas et al., 2010) and social fearfully attached and preoccupied students
functioning (Gross & John, 2003), and posi- share a negative internal representation of
tively connected to psychopathology (Aldao themselves (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991),
et al., 2010). Lastly, the use of food to comfort this is probably why they do not want to face
one’s emotions was rarely used and was signifi- others in stressful situations, unlike their col-
cantly less characteristic of the dismissively leagues with a dismissive attachment style. This
attached group in comparison with all of the is not in line with some previous research
other attachment-based groups of students. (Mikulincer, 1995), which claimed that dismis-
This may be due to their somewhat self- sive attachment is connected to avoidance of
restricting way of functioning: these individuals problem-situations. Interestingly, preoccupied
may not allow themselves to be comforted by students also use the escape-avoidance coping
“external” means (Brennan & Shaver, 1995). strategy more than securely attached students,
The coping regulatory process was also ana- implying that the negative self-representation
lyzed in pedagogical students with different in preoccupied students leads to more frequent
attachment styles. All of the attachment-based escaping or avoiding stressful situations that
groups sometimes used the majority of the could evoke their negative evaluation of them-
coping strategies included, with a number of selves. As escaping or avoiding stressful situa-
the strategies being used frequently. Signifi- tions is considered a more passive and
cant differences between the attachment-based therefore less efficient coping approach
groups of students were confirmed for the use (Glasscock et al., 2013), its significantly greater

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


364 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

use in fearful and preoccupied students may be to these students because of their future work.
of concern, and demands further attention. The majority of ER strategies included in the
To summarize, the less frequent use of study were sometimes used by students, while
social support (as an ER and coping strategy) some were used frequently and a few used
in fearfully and dismissively attached students rarely. Similarly, the majority of the coping stra-
should be addressed in particular, as these tegies included in the study were used some-
students will also be modeling their ER and times, while the others were used frequently.
coping approaches to people they work with The four attachment-based groups of students
in their future profession. For the same rea- (secure, fearful, preoccupied, and dismissive)
son, the more frequent use of suppression in have somewhat different patterns of frequency
students with fearful attachment could be of ER and coping strategy use. The significant
challenged. The coping strategy of escaping differences between the attachment-based
and avoiding, which is more frequently used groups of students were, however, confirmed in
by students with fearful and preoccupied their use of the ER strategies of social support,
attachment styles, should also be discussed suppression, and comfort eating, and in the cop-
with the students, and possibly replaced with ing strategies of seeking social support and
more effective strategies. This can be accom- escaping or avoiding stressful situations. These
plished via the systematic inclusion of these differences might be explained by the specifici-
themes in university study programs. Theoret- ties of attachment styles that include positive or
ical explanations for attachment, emotion reg- negative self and other internal representations,
ulation, and coping strategies are already a with the latter perhaps having an even more
part of some subjects within study programs important role. It may be concluded that,
(e.g., developmental and educational psychol- despite the distinction between ER and coping
ogy), yet these themes could be further ana- processes (Smrtnik Vitulic & Prosen, 2016),
lyzed and processed within smaller groups of both sets of processes were connected to rela-
students allowing for their more individua- tional schemas. Efficient ER and coping strate-
lized work. Even though the changes of gies can be encouraged throughout study
(especially) attachment style, ER, and coping programs, as these strategies are more suscepti-
strategies are not an easy task to accomplish, ble to change in the period of late adolescence
previously mentioned work in smaller groups (Diamond & Aspinwall, 2003; Skinner &
could encourage students’ reflection and pos- Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007).
sible changes of self and other representa- The relatively high percentage of insecure
tions, of dealing with emotions, and stressful attachment styles among the participating stu-
situations. Regarding specific ER and coping dents was somewhat concerning, especially
strategies, positive group experience may considering their future work as human-
show the students the effects of the social relations professionals. Although attachment
support strategy (thus enhancing its use), and styles remain relatively stable during the indi-
represent a safe environment for expressing vidual’s development, they can be altered at
emotions (thus diminishing the use of important life-events or in long-term relation-
suppression). ships. Even though changing students’ inse-
cure attachment styles within a university
context is a difficult task, it is nonetheless
possible to provide information regarding
Conclusion
attachment and its importance for the estab-
The students participating in our study are lishment and maintenance of relationships.
future human-relations professionals, a group Such information may encourage students
whose attachment, ER, or coping mechanisms with insecure attachment styles to start trans-
have been only rarely studied by other authors, forming their relational representations and
despite these areas being of special importance behaviors.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Regulation regarding students’ attachment 365

In conclusion, some methodological improve- Williams, E. K. (2014). Coping and emotion


ments to our study are suggested. Even though regulation from childhood to early adulthood:
the percentages of students in each of the four Points of convergence and divergence.
Australian Journal of Psychology, 66, 71–81.
attachment-based groups in our study were sim-
Cugmas, Z. (2003). Narisal sem sonce zate: Izbrana
ilar to the previously obtained data, the num- poglavja o razvoju otrokove navezanosti in
bers of students within some of these groups samozaznave [I drew a sun for you: Selected
(especially the dismissive group) were low – chapters on child’s attachment development].
representing a shortcoming of our study. Also, Ljubljana: Center za psihodiagnosticna sredstva,
attachment styles, ER, and coping strategies d.o.o. (In Slovenian, translated by the authors
of this article.)
could be assessed by other methods, such as
Diamond, L. M., & Aspinwall, L. G. (2003). Emo-
observations, providing external information on tion regulation across the life span: An integra-
the individual’s behavior. Furthermore, reas- tive perspective emphasizing self-regulation,
sessment of these psychological measures could positive affect, and dyadic process. Motivation
be undertaken at the end of the study program, and Emotion, 27, 125–156.
in order to determine whether and how the Ferligoj, A., Leskošek, K., & Kogovšek, T. (1995).
study program has influenced them. ER and Zanesljivost in veljavnost merjenja [Reliability
and validity of measurement]. Ljubljana: FDV
coping strategies could also be studied in spe-
(In Slovenian, translated by the authors of this
cific relationships, such as interactions with par- article.)
ents, siblings, peers, or romantic partners. Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Gruen, R. J., &
DeLongis, A. (1986). Appraisal, coping, health
status, and psychological symptoms. Journal of
References Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 571–579.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Pimley, S., &
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. Novacek, J. (1987). Age differences in stress
(2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across and coping processes. Psychology and Aging, 2,
psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clini- 171–184.
cal Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attach- representations of attachment during pregnancy
ment styles among young adults: A test of a predict the organization of infant-mother attach-
four-category model. Journal of Personality and ment at one year of age. Child Development, 62,
Social Psychology, 61, 226–244. 891–905.
Bowlby, J. (1997). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Glasscock, D. J., Andersen, J. H., Labriola, M.,
Attachment (2nd ed.). London: Pimlico. Rasmussen, K., & Hansen, C. D. (2013). Can neg-
Bowlby, J., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1965). Child care ative life events and coping style help explain soci-
and the growth of love. London: Penguin. oeconomic differences in perceived stress among
Brennan, K.A., & Shaver, P.R. (1995). Dimensions adolescents? A cross-sectional study based on the
of adult attachment, affect regulation, and West Jutland cohort study. BMC Public Health,
romantic relationship functioning. Personality 13, 532.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 267–283. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion
Broberg, A. G., Hjalmers, I., & Nevonen, L. (2001). regulation: An integrative review. Review of
Eating disorders, attachment and interpersonal General Psychology, 2, 271–299.
difficulties: A comparison between 18- to 24- Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differ-
year-old patients and normal controls. ences in two emotion regulation processes:
European Eating Disorders Review, 9, 381–396. Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
Carr, S., Colthurst, K., Coyle, M., & Elliott, D. being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
(2013). Attachment dimensions as predictors of ogy, 55, 348–362.
mental health and psychological well-being in Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2009). Emotion regu-
the transition to university. European Journal lation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross &
of Psychology of Education, 28, 157–172. R. A. Thompson (Eds.), Handbook of emotion
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the regulation (pp. 3–24). New York, London: Guil-
behavioural sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ford Press.
Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Dunbar, J. P., Jacobsen, T., Edelstein, W., & Hofmann, V. (1994).
Watson, K. H., Bettis, A. H., Gruhn, M. A., & A longitudinal study of the relation between

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


366 S. Prosen and H. Smrtnik Vitulic

representations of attachment in childhood and Razdevšek Pucko, C., & Rugelj, J. (2006). Kompe-
cognitive functioning in childhood and adoles- tence v izobraževanju uciteljev [Competencies
cence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 112–124. in primary teacher education]. In S. Tancig &
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Individual differ- T. Devjak (Eds.), Prispevki k posodobitvi peda-
ences in emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross & goških študijskih programov (pp. 30–44). Lju-
R. A. Thompson (Eds.), Handbook of emotion bljana: Pedagoška fakulteta (In Slovenian,
regulation (pp. 351–372). New York, London: translated by the authors of this article.)
Guilford Press. Schutte, N. S., Manes, R. R., & Malouff, J. M. (2009).
Karademas, E. C., Tsalikou, C., & Tellarou, M.-C. Antecedent-focused emotion regulation, response
(2010). The impact of emotion regulation and modulation and well-being. Current Psychology,
illness-focused coping strategies on the relation 28, 21–31.
of illness-related negative emotions to subjec- Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind. New York,
tive health. Journal of Health Psychology, 16, London: Guilford Press.
510–519. Skinner, E. A., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2007).
Lamovec, T. (1994). Psihodiagnostika osebnosti 1 The development of coping. Annual Review of
[Psychodiagnostics of personality I]. Ljubljana: Psychology, 58, 119–144.
Oddelek za psihologijo, Filozofska fakulteta Smrtnik Vitulic, H., & Prosen, S. (2016). Coping and
(In Slovenian, translated by the authors of this emotion regulation strategies in adulthood: Spe-
article.) cificities regarding age, gender and level of edu-
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional cation. Društvena istraživanja, 25, 43–62.
theory and research on emotions and coping. Sutton, R. E., Mudrey-Camino, R., & Knight, C. C.
European Journal of Personality, 1, 141–169. (2009). Teachers’ emotion regulation and class-
Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Cote, S., & Beers, M. room management. Theory Into Practice, 48,
(2005). Emotion regulation and the quality of 130–137.
social interaction. Emotion, 5, 113–118. Thompson, R. A. (1999). Early sociopersonality
Lopez, F. G., & Gormley, B. (2002). Stability and development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook
change in adult attachment style over the first- of child psychology: social, emotional, and per-
year college transition: Relations to self-confi- sonality development (5th ed., Vol. 3,
dence, coping, and distress patterns. Journal of pp. 25–104). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Counseling Psychology, 49, 355–364. Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2009). Socialization
Mattanah, J. F., Hancock, G. R., & Brand, B. L. of emotion regulation in the family. In
(2004). Parental attachment, separation – individ- J. J. Gross & R. A. Thompson (Eds.), Hand-
uation, and college student adjustment: A struc- book of emotion regulation (pp. 249–268).
tural equation analysis of mediational effects. New York, London: Guilford Press.
Journal of Counselling Psychology, 51, 213–225. Zambianchi, M., & Ricci Bitti, P. E. (2014). The role
Mikulincer, M. (1995). Attachment style and the of proactive coping strategies, time perspective,
mental representation of the self. Journal of perceived efficacy on affect regulation, diver-
Personality and Social Psychology, 69, gent thinking and family communication in pro-
1203–1215. moting social well-being in emerging adulthood.
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Social Indicators Research, 116, 493–507.
Attachment theory and affect regulation: The Žvelc, M., & Žvelc, G. (2006). Stili navezanosti v
dynamics, development, and cognitive conse- odraslosti [Adult attachment styles]. Horizons
quences of attachment-related strategies. Moti- of Psychology, 15, 51–64 (In Slovenian, trans-
vation and Emotion, 27, 77–102. lated by the authors of this article.)
Prosen, S., & Smrtnik Vitulic, H. (2014). Different
perspectives on emotion regulation and its effi-
ciency. Psihologijske Teme, 23, 389–405. (Received December 23, 2015; accepted June 15, 2016)

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Copyright of Japanese Psychological Research is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen