Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247506182

Effects of Age of Retirement, Reason for


Retirement, and Pre-retirement Training on
Psychological and...

Article in Australian Psychologist · July 1998


DOI: 10.1080/00050069808257392

CITATIONS READS

37 122

2 authors, including:

Christopher F. Sharpley
University of New England (Australia)
298 PUBLICATIONS 3,259 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Alpha Asymmetry in depression: Roles of cerebral sites, gender and severity View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Christopher F. Sharpley on 18 February 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Effects of Age of Retirement, Reason for Retirement,
and Pre-retirement Training on Psychological
and Physical Health during Retirement
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY
Bond University

RENATY LAYTON
Monash University

planning, thus setting up alternative occupations (both finan¬


cial and nonfinancial) and income satisfactory to their
personal and material needs. By contrast, men who were
of retirement on psychological wellbeing, this paper forced to retire felt that their everyday way of life had been
As a first stepthe
reports in ainfluence
series of of
studies examining
age when thewhether
retired; effects disrupted, and had done little effective planning or prepara¬
retirement is because of age, redundancy, or illness, or is
tion for retirement. This finding is mirrored by a report from
voluntary; and the presence of pre-retirement education or
the Australian Council of Social Services (1980) which
training on anxiety, depression, stress, and physical health
following retirement. Data were collected from 349 males and found that mandatory (i.e., involuntary) retirement had
385 females, ranging from 44 to 90 years and spread across adverse effects on the mental and physical health as well as
the first 5 years following retirement from full-time work. the life expectancy of the retired person. These effects were
Results indicated that men who retired earlier reported better exacerbated when there was no kind of pre-retirement train¬
physical health (although there were no significant effects for ing available. Similarly, the Department of Social Security
women), that both men and women who retired voluntarily (1988) reported that 24% of people who were forced to
were significantly less anxious, depressed, or stressed than retire because of age would have preferred to remain in full-
those who retired for reasons of ill-health or were made time employment. Further, 25% of full-time employees over
redundant, and that the experience of pre-retirement educa¬
the age of 45 years would have preferred gradual retirement
tion or training appeared to be associated with reduced
psychological distress following the retirement act. In (i.e., moving from full-time to part-time employment over
addition, exploratory data suggested that pre-retirement time before ceasing work altogether). Atchley (1982) and
education which includes social, health, personal, and Walker, Kimmel, and Price (1981) found that people who
relationship aspects of the retirement process may be associ¬ were forced to retire earlier often encountered more difficul¬
ated with lower psychological distress during retirement. ties in adjusting to retirement than people who retired volun¬
Implications for psychological services and the training of tarily. Thus, it appears that the use of a specific age as a
psychologists are discussed. reason for retirement does not take into account the range of
individual preferences that exist in any sample of retired
persons, and that the reason for retirement may be at least as
Retirement has been conceptualised by Peterson (1989) as great a factor in determining adjustment to retirement as age
one of the major life crises. Atchley (1976) saw retirement itself.
as a condition in which an individual is forced to stop work Another reason for forced retirement apart from age is
and is employed at less than full-time rates, with implica¬ poor health. Muller and Boaz (1988) and Parker (1980)
tions for income which are usually addressed by pension or noted that poor health that leads to early retirement also
private sources. As a result of social and workstyle factors, predicts poorer adjustment to retirement. Feldman (1994)
many Australians are retiring earlier than the traditional 60 commented that "poor health often makes continuity in the
or 65 years, and thus are confronting the crisis of retirement job physically less possible, early retirement a necessity
at ages when they are still well able to work in terms of their rather than an option, and quality of life during retirement
physical health. For these people, whether they retire volun¬ distinctly lower" (p. 296). Barfield and Morgan (1978)
tarily or by way of forced redundancy may be a key factor in reported that the retiree's health status was one of the
determining their attitude to retirement and their consequent strongest predictors of satisfaction during retirement, with
adjustment to life without full-time work and income. Thus, the healthiest people claiming to be "very satisfied" or
the reason why a person retires, as well as the age when they "satisfied" with retirement.
retire may be "background" predictor factors which influ¬ Because it represents a major life crisis, retirement
ence retirement adjustment. There is some research evidence requires adequate preparation. Some data suggest that
to suggest that these factors do, in fact, affect physical and planning for retirement plays a large part in determining the
psychological health during retirement. degree of satisfaction experienced during retirement.
For example, Parker (1982) noted that many men who Feldman (1994) suggested that employees who received
had retired voluntarily had done so after several years of comprehensive pre-retirement training were more likely to

Address for correspondence: Dr C.F. Sharpley, Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast OLD 4229, Australia.

JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST


VOLUME 33 NUMBER 2 pp. 119-124

119
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY AND RENATY LAYTON EFFECTS OF AGE OF RETIREMENT

retire earlier, and that advance preparation, plus the was performed via mailed and anonymous paper-and-pencil Self-rating of health. Participants were asked to rate their tions with scores from tire SAS (.544) and the SDS (.538),
emotional and social support of retirement counselling and questionnaire. own health "during the last three months" on a 5-point scale suggesting that stress, as defined here, was a powerful
training, assisted in adjusting to retirement. This is Finally, several commentators have noted that the ranging from very bad, to bad, neither bad nor good, good, negative experience which related to, but did not completely
supported by Hornstein and Wapner (1985) and Richardson "impact of withdrawal from gainful employment is likely to and very good. overlap, anxiety and depression. The SPSR has three princi¬
and Kilty (1991). Those authors found that workers who did be different" for men and women (Encel & Studencld, 1996, pal factors: missing work, which accounts for 39% of the
Frequency of visits to a medical practitioner, injuries,
not want to retire and had no tangible plans for their retire¬ p. 25). The competing roles of nurturer and employee have variance; personal health, which accounts for 10% of the
accidents, and infections were measured by a self-rating
ment lifestyle consequently struggled to hold onto their been shown to complicate women's decisions about and variance; and relationship issues, which accounts for 7.9%
scale which asked participants to report how many of these
working identity and lifestyle. These workers retired angry, ways of proceeding through retirement (Prentis, 1992). On had occurred during the last 3 months. of the variance.
frustrated, and role-less compared to individuals who had the Australian scene, Winocur and Rosenman (1991)
planned for their retirement. Fretz, Kluge, Ossana, Jones, suggested that women approach the retirement experience Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS: Zung, 1965). The SDS Procedure
and Merikangas (1989) found that the strongest predictors differently. Therefore, the data were tested for significant consists of 20 items identified in factor analytic studies of
Each participant package also included a stamped,
of anxiety and depression among pre-retirees were a low differences between men and women on the major depen¬ the syndrome of depression. There are an equal number of
addressed envelop, and participants were asked to send the
sense of self-efficacy about being able to handle retirement, dent variables, and results were examined for each gender positively and negatively worded items to reduce response
completed questionnaire back to the first author. All useable
plus a low level of planning for retirement. Bearing these separately simply because of these hypothesised differences bias, and respondents are asked to indicate whether each
questionnaires received within 3 months were included in
findings in mind, it is salutary to note that a report from the in men's and women's experiences of retirement. symptom is true for themselves "a little or none of the
the data analysis. Following that analysis, a detailed report
Australian House of Representatives (House of time", "some of the time", "a good part of the time", or
was sent to the Australian Retired Persons' Association,
Representatives Standing Committee on Community Method "most of the time" over the past week. Raw scores range
with a suggested insert for inclusion in the ARPA newslet¬
Affairs, 1990) found that one of the most common difficul¬ from 20 to 80 and may be reduced to a depression index.
Participants ter.
ties facing people about to retire was lack of information The SDS has been found to possess split-half reliability of
about facilities, services, and opportunities regarding educa¬ A questionnaire was sent to 1,500 members of the .81 (Zung, 1973), .79 (DeJonghe & Baneke, 1989), and .94
tion about retirement, also commenting that "only about 5% Australian Retired Persons' Association in Victoria with (Gabrys & Peters, 1985). Internal consistency (alpha) is .88 Results
of Australians Hearing retirement have had the benefit of their monthly newsletter late in 1995. From this sample, 349 for depressed patients and .93 for nondepressed patients Although there were no significant differences between
retirement education" (p. 41). In spite of this, most pre¬ males and 385 females returned the questionnaire in useable (Gabrys & Peters, 1985). The SDS has also been found to males and females on any of the health measures, data from
retirement planning is in the form of financial advice, with form within 3 months of receiving it. The average age of be superior to the MMPI Depression scale and the Beck each gender were analysed separately because these differ¬
little emphasis on the physical and mental health aspects or males was 65 years (range = 44 to 90 years), and for Depression Inventory for assessing depression in male ences are of clinical and intrinsic (if not statistical) signifi¬
on social, family, and recreation aspects of retirement females also 65 years (range = 48 to 89 years). The most psychiatric inpatients (Schaefer et ah, 1985). cance, and because the size of each gender sample allowed
(Feldman, 1994). common marital status was married (77.9% of males and this to be performed without major risk of Type I errors. In
37.9% of females), followed by widow(ed) (8.3% of males, Self-rating Anxiety Scale (SAS: Zung, 1971). The SAS
Thus, the influence of age when retired, reason for addition, as has been noted above, the existing literature on
was developed by Zung as "a standardised method of evalu¬
retirement, and presence of any pre-retirement education 29.6% of females), single (6.3% of males, 15.8% of retirement has been "strongly biased towards men" (Encel
females), separated (2.0% of males, 2.1% of females) and ating and recording the presence of anxiety as a clinical
appear to be salient factors to investigate within an & Studencki, 1996, p. 25), and several studies (e.g.,
divorced (5.2% of males, 14.5% of females). Most (88.5% disorder" (Zung, 1971, p. 371), with items drawn from a list
Australian population in the mid-1990s. With the population Winocur & Rosenman, 1991) suggest that the retirement
of males and 76.9% of females) had children and saw them of symptoms associated with anxiety as a psychiatric disor¬
boom of the post-World War II years now nearing retire¬ experience is different for men and women. Hence, Table 1
an average of 7.8 (males) and 9.2 (females) times per der and selected from major American texts in the area
ment age, exploration of those variables which might help presents the mean and standard deviation scores for males,
month. Most participants lived in their own homes (92.0% (Zung, 1971). Five affective symptoms and 15 somatic
individuals deal more effectively with retirement is relevant females, and the total sample for the five dependent
of males, 90.6% of females), with 3.7% of males and 4.7% complaints make up the 20 items of the SAS, and responses
to everyday psychological practice. However, in spite of the variables: self-rating of health (health); frequency of visits
of females renting, 1.1% of males and .8% of females living are made on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from none or
relevance of data regarding the retirement adjustment to a medical practitioner, injuries, illnesses, or infections
with relatives, and 3.2% of males and 3.6% of females little of the time, some of the time, good part of the time, to
process, "we know little about the lives of people who have (illness); SDS scores (depression); SAS scores (anxiety);
living in a retirement village. The most common maximum most or all of the time for how respondents have felt during
retired" (Encel & Studencld, 1996, p. 4). Further, as noted and scores on the SPSR scale (stress).
level of education received was secondary school (37.7% of the previous week. Total raw scores may be converted to an
by Encel and Studencld, "although there is an extensive
males, 48.4% of females), followed by university (males = anxiety index score ranging from 25 to 100. Zung (1986)
literature on ageing in Australia, not much has been written Age When Retired
34.2%, females = 35.9%), technical. (23.8% of males, but used a morbidity cut-off score of 50, with those scoring
about retirement. Indeed, the subject has not attracted a Regression procedures indicated that, although none of the
only 7.3% of females), and primary school (4.3% of males above this having anxiety to a degree which is clinically
great deal of attention except in the United States" (p. 4). dependent variables was significantly predicted by age
and 8.3% of females). All participants were retired from significant. Psychometric data for the SAS are good, with
For that reason, we have set up a series of studies into retire¬ when retired for females, the decision to retire earlier was a
full-time work. Previous occupations included both white split-half reliability at .71 (Zung, 1971), and internal consis¬
ment, of which this paper reports the first step, that is, a significant predictor of self-rating of men's health during
and blue collar activities, including managerial/administra¬ tencies of .85 (Zung, 1980) and .81 (Jegede, 1977) for clini¬
survey of retirees to determine the effects on psychological
cal samples, and .75 (Sharpley & Rogers, 1985) and .69 the previous three months (F = 3.924, p < .05). That is, the
and physical health during retirement of (a) age when they tive, professional, trades, sales, self-employed, and house¬
(Jegede, 1977) for normal samples. The SAS differentiates earlier that men retired, the greater their chances of report¬
retired, (b) the reason for retirement, and (c) presence of any hold duties, suggesting that most sections of the overall
between normal and psychiatric populations (Jegede, 1977; ing that they were in good health. Because this result may
pre-retirement education or planning. Although physical Australian workforce were tapped, with the possible excep¬
Zung, 1980), and between psychiatric patients with an have been an outcome of their present age (i.e., younger
health is not easily assessed without medical evaluation, it is tion of unskilled manual workers. Incomes before retire¬
ment ranged from less than $10,000 to over $100,000, again anxiety disorder and those with other disorders (Zung, men may also have been expected to be healthier), this
possible to evaluate participants' self-ratings of their physi¬
cal health as an indicator of their overall outlook on life arguing for the overall representativeness of the sample
1980). analysis was run again, with age at the time of completing
here. Additionally, these data approximate the overall distri¬ the survey questionnaire included in the analysis. There was
(Greenberg, 1981). This procedure was followed here by Self-perceived stress (SPSR) was assessed by a 14-item
bution of the entire membership of the Australian Retired no significant effect due to present age, indicating that the
asking participants to rate their overall physical health, and scale developed by the first author (Sharpley, 1997), which
to indicate the number of times during the last 3 months that Persons' Association, asked participants to rate the "degree of stress you experi¬
predictive validity of age when retired on their present
they had contracted an illness or infection, or had visited a enced during the last three months from each of these
evaluation of their physical health was not a function of any
medical practitioner for assistance. Psychological health Questionnaire factors", and then presented the possible causes of stress "youthful" comparative lack of disease or infirmity in the
was assessed via two paper-and-pencil tests (Zung's self- Apart from an explanatory statement and letter of invitation (fear of death, loss of purpose, your mental health, loneli¬ present sample of male retirees.
rating scales for anxiety and depression). Additionally, to participate in the survey, the questionnaire booklet ness, missing work, housing, recreation, exercise, boredom,
because it is of value to determine which specific factors in included demographic information (gender, age, age when family affairs, personal relationships, your finances, your
participants' lives are contributing towards their self- retired, marital status, education, residence type); a question spouse's health, and your physical health) on a 5 -point
reported anxiety and depression, a measure of self- which asked participants if they had received any form of scale ranging from little or none, to a bit, moderate, high, or Mean (and Standard Deviation) Scores on All Dependent
perceived stress arising for 14 common factors was also pre-retirement education or training and, if so, whether this Variables
extreme. These 14 factors were generated by a group of
used (the Self-perceived Stress in Retirement Scale). covered more than simply financial matters; two physical retirees who were requested to list those factors which Variable Males Females Total
Because the process of adjusting to retirement is not health questionnaires (self-rating of health; frequency of "significantly contributed to the day-to-day stress" that
Health 2.19 (1.55) 2.21 (1.56) 2.20 (1.55)
immediate, data were collected from a sample of people visits to a medical practitioner, injuries, accidents, and infec¬ participants experienced. Initial psychometric data on the Illness 5.40 (3.98) 5.70 (4.25) 5.56 (4.13)
who had been retired from between 6 months and 5 years. tions); Zung's Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS) and Self- SPSR indicated internal consistency (alpha) of .88, and Anxiety . 30.65 (6.64) 31.24 (7.29) 30.95 (6.99)
As a first stage towards developing intervention programs rating Anxiety Scale (SAS); and a measure of self-perceived item-total correlations ranging from .38 to .74. The SPSR Depression 34.20 (8.86) 35.07 (9.37) 34.66 (9.04)
for helping people plan effectively for retirement, this study stress from 14 sources. Each of these is described below. total scores showed significant (p < .001) positive correla¬ Stress 26.07 (8,45) 25.94 (8.12) 26.09 (8.27)

JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST

120 121
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY AND RENATY LAYTON EFFECTS OF AGE OF RETIREMENT

retire earlier, and that advance preparation, plus the was performed via mailed and anonymous paper-and-pencil Self-rating of health. Participants were asked to rate their tions with scores from tire SAS (.544) and the SDS (.538),
emotional and social support of retirement counselling and questionnaire. own health "during the last three months" on a 5-point scale suggesting that stress, as defined here, was a powerful
training, assisted in adjusting to retirement. This is Finally, several commentators have noted that the ranging from very bad, to bad, neither bad nor good, good, negative experience which related to, but did not completely
supported by Hornstein and Wapner (1985) and Richardson "impact of withdrawal from gainful employment is likely to and very good. overlap, anxiety and depression. The SPSR has three princi¬
and Kilty (1991). Those authors found that workers who did be different" for men and women (Encel & Studencld, 1996, pal factors: missing work, which accounts for 39% of the
Frequency of visits to a medical practitioner, injuries,
not want to retire and had no tangible plans for their retire¬ p. 25). The competing roles of nurturer and employee have variance; personal health, which accounts for 10% of the
accidents, and infections were measured by a self-rating
ment lifestyle consequently struggled to hold onto their been shown to complicate women's decisions about and variance; and relationship issues, which accounts for 7.9%
scale which asked participants to report how many of these
working identity and lifestyle. These workers retired angry, ways of proceeding through retirement (Prentis, 1992). On had occurred during the last 3 months. of the variance.
frustrated, and role-less compared to individuals who had the Australian scene, Winocur and Rosenman (1991)
planned for their retirement. Fretz, Kluge, Ossana, Jones, suggested that women approach the retirement experience Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS: Zung, 1965). The SDS Procedure
and Merikangas (1989) found that the strongest predictors differently. Therefore, the data were tested for significant consists of 20 items identified in factor analytic studies of
Each participant package also included a stamped,
of anxiety and depression among pre-retirees were a low differences between men and women on the major depen¬ the syndrome of depression. There are an equal number of
addressed envelop, and participants were asked to send the
sense of self-efficacy about being able to handle retirement, dent variables, and results were examined for each gender positively and negatively worded items to reduce response
completed questionnaire back to the first author. All useable
plus a low level of planning for retirement. Bearing these separately simply because of these hypothesised differences bias, and respondents are asked to indicate whether each
questionnaires received within 3 months were included in
findings in mind, it is salutary to note that a report from the in men's and women's experiences of retirement. symptom is true for themselves "a little or none of the
the data analysis. Following that analysis, a detailed report
Australian House of Representatives (House of time", "some of the time", "a good part of the time", or
was sent to the Australian Retired Persons' Association,
Representatives Standing Committee on Community Method "most of the time" over the past week. Raw scores range
with a suggested insert for inclusion in the ARPA newslet¬
Affairs, 1990) found that one of the most common difficul¬ from 20 to 80 and may be reduced to a depression index.
Participants ter.
ties facing people about to retire was lack of information The SDS has been found to possess split-half reliability of
about facilities, services, and opportunities regarding educa¬ A questionnaire was sent to 1,500 members of the .81 (Zung, 1973), .79 (DeJonghe & Baneke, 1989), and .94
tion about retirement, also commenting that "only about 5% Australian Retired Persons' Association in Victoria with (Gabrys & Peters, 1985). Internal consistency (alpha) is .88 Results
of Australians Hearing retirement have had the benefit of their monthly newsletter late in 1995. From this sample, 349 for depressed patients and .93 for nondepressed patients Although there were no significant differences between
retirement education" (p. 41). In spite of this, most pre¬ males and 385 females returned the questionnaire in useable (Gabrys & Peters, 1985). The SDS has also been found to males and females on any of the health measures, data from
retirement planning is in the form of financial advice, with form within 3 months of receiving it. The average age of be superior to the MMPI Depression scale and the Beck each gender were analysed separately because these differ¬
little emphasis on the physical and mental health aspects or males was 65 years (range = 44 to 90 years), and for Depression Inventory for assessing depression in male ences are of clinical and intrinsic (if not statistical) signifi¬
on social, family, and recreation aspects of retirement females also 65 years (range = 48 to 89 years). The most psychiatric inpatients (Schaefer et ah, 1985). cance, and because the size of each gender sample allowed
(Feldman, 1994). common marital status was married (77.9% of males and this to be performed without major risk of Type I errors. In
37.9% of females), followed by widow(ed) (8.3% of males, Self-rating Anxiety Scale (SAS: Zung, 1971). The SAS
Thus, the influence of age when retired, reason for addition, as has been noted above, the existing literature on
was developed by Zung as "a standardised method of evalu¬
retirement, and presence of any pre-retirement education 29.6% of females), single (6.3% of males, 15.8% of retirement has been "strongly biased towards men" (Encel
females), separated (2.0% of males, 2.1% of females) and ating and recording the presence of anxiety as a clinical
appear to be salient factors to investigate within an & Studencki, 1996, p. 25), and several studies (e.g.,
divorced (5.2% of males, 14.5% of females). Most (88.5% disorder" (Zung, 1971, p. 371), with items drawn from a list
Australian population in the mid-1990s. With the population Winocur & Rosenman, 1991) suggest that the retirement
of males and 76.9% of females) had children and saw them of symptoms associated with anxiety as a psychiatric disor¬
boom of the post-World War II years now nearing retire¬ experience is different for men and women. Hence, Table 1
an average of 7.8 (males) and 9.2 (females) times per der and selected from major American texts in the area
ment age, exploration of those variables which might help presents the mean and standard deviation scores for males,
month. Most participants lived in their own homes (92.0% (Zung, 1971). Five affective symptoms and 15 somatic
individuals deal more effectively with retirement is relevant females, and the total sample for the five dependent
of males, 90.6% of females), with 3.7% of males and 4.7% complaints make up the 20 items of the SAS, and responses
to everyday psychological practice. However, in spite of the variables: self-rating of health (health); frequency of visits
of females renting, 1.1% of males and .8% of females living are made on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from none or
relevance of data regarding the retirement adjustment to a medical practitioner, injuries, illnesses, or infections
with relatives, and 3.2% of males and 3.6% of females little of the time, some of the time, good part of the time, to
process, "we know little about the lives of people who have (illness); SDS scores (depression); SAS scores (anxiety);
living in a retirement village. The most common maximum most or all of the time for how respondents have felt during
retired" (Encel & Studencld, 1996, p. 4). Further, as noted and scores on the SPSR scale (stress).
level of education received was secondary school (37.7% of the previous week. Total raw scores may be converted to an
by Encel and Studencld, "although there is an extensive
males, 48.4% of females), followed by university (males = anxiety index score ranging from 25 to 100. Zung (1986)
literature on ageing in Australia, not much has been written Age When Retired
34.2%, females = 35.9%), technical. (23.8% of males, but used a morbidity cut-off score of 50, with those scoring
about retirement. Indeed, the subject has not attracted a Regression procedures indicated that, although none of the
only 7.3% of females), and primary school (4.3% of males above this having anxiety to a degree which is clinically
great deal of attention except in the United States" (p. 4). dependent variables was significantly predicted by age
and 8.3% of females). All participants were retired from significant. Psychometric data for the SAS are good, with
For that reason, we have set up a series of studies into retire¬ when retired for females, the decision to retire earlier was a
full-time work. Previous occupations included both white split-half reliability at .71 (Zung, 1971), and internal consis¬
ment, of which this paper reports the first step, that is, a significant predictor of self-rating of men's health during
and blue collar activities, including managerial/administra¬ tencies of .85 (Zung, 1980) and .81 (Jegede, 1977) for clini¬
survey of retirees to determine the effects on psychological
cal samples, and .75 (Sharpley & Rogers, 1985) and .69 the previous three months (F = 3.924, p < .05). That is, the
and physical health during retirement of (a) age when they tive, professional, trades, sales, self-employed, and house¬
(Jegede, 1977) for normal samples. The SAS differentiates earlier that men retired, the greater their chances of report¬
retired, (b) the reason for retirement, and (c) presence of any hold duties, suggesting that most sections of the overall
between normal and psychiatric populations (Jegede, 1977; ing that they were in good health. Because this result may
pre-retirement education or planning. Although physical Australian workforce were tapped, with the possible excep¬
Zung, 1980), and between psychiatric patients with an have been an outcome of their present age (i.e., younger
health is not easily assessed without medical evaluation, it is tion of unskilled manual workers. Incomes before retire¬
ment ranged from less than $10,000 to over $100,000, again anxiety disorder and those with other disorders (Zung, men may also have been expected to be healthier), this
possible to evaluate participants' self-ratings of their physi¬
cal health as an indicator of their overall outlook on life arguing for the overall representativeness of the sample
1980). analysis was run again, with age at the time of completing
here. Additionally, these data approximate the overall distri¬ the survey questionnaire included in the analysis. There was
(Greenberg, 1981). This procedure was followed here by Self-perceived stress (SPSR) was assessed by a 14-item
bution of the entire membership of the Australian Retired no significant effect due to present age, indicating that the
asking participants to rate their overall physical health, and scale developed by the first author (Sharpley, 1997), which
to indicate the number of times during the last 3 months that Persons' Association, asked participants to rate the "degree of stress you experi¬
predictive validity of age when retired on their present
they had contracted an illness or infection, or had visited a enced during the last three months from each of these
evaluation of their physical health was not a function of any
medical practitioner for assistance. Psychological health Questionnaire factors", and then presented the possible causes of stress "youthful" comparative lack of disease or infirmity in the
was assessed via two paper-and-pencil tests (Zung's self- Apart from an explanatory statement and letter of invitation (fear of death, loss of purpose, your mental health, loneli¬ present sample of male retirees.
rating scales for anxiety and depression). Additionally, to participate in the survey, the questionnaire booklet ness, missing work, housing, recreation, exercise, boredom,
because it is of value to determine which specific factors in included demographic information (gender, age, age when family affairs, personal relationships, your finances, your
participants' lives are contributing towards their self- retired, marital status, education, residence type); a question spouse's health, and your physical health) on a 5 -point
reported anxiety and depression, a measure of self- which asked participants if they had received any form of scale ranging from little or none, to a bit, moderate, high, or Mean (and Standard Deviation) Scores on All Dependent
perceived stress arising for 14 common factors was also pre-retirement education or training and, if so, whether this Variables
extreme. These 14 factors were generated by a group of
used (the Self-perceived Stress in Retirement Scale). covered more than simply financial matters; two physical retirees who were requested to list those factors which Variable Males Females Total
Because the process of adjusting to retirement is not health questionnaires (self-rating of health; frequency of "significantly contributed to the day-to-day stress" that
Health 2.19 (1.55) 2.21 (1.56) 2.20 (1.55)
immediate, data were collected from a sample of people visits to a medical practitioner, injuries, accidents, and infec¬ participants experienced. Initial psychometric data on the Illness 5.40 (3.98) 5.70 (4.25) 5.56 (4.13)
who had been retired from between 6 months and 5 years. tions); Zung's Self-rating Depression Scale (SDS) and Self- SPSR indicated internal consistency (alpha) of .88, and Anxiety . 30.65 (6.64) 31.24 (7.29) 30.95 (6.99)
As a first stage towards developing intervention programs rating Anxiety Scale (SAS); and a measure of self-perceived item-total correlations ranging from .38 to .74. The SPSR Depression 34.20 (8.86) 35.07 (9.37) 34.66 (9.04)
for helping people plan effectively for retirement, this study stress from 14 sources. Each of these is described below. total scores showed significant (p < .001) positive correla¬ Stress 26.07 (8,45) 25.94 (8.12) 26.09 (8.27)

JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST

120 121
EFFECTS OF AGE OF RETIREMENT
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY AND RENATY LAYTON
pre-retirement education or training at all, scores were 31.5 retirement education is of greatest value to retirees. It must
Reason for Retirement form of pre-retirement education or training. Of these 60, 51 (7.43), 35.4 (9.55), 25.9 (8.2) respectively. Women who be reiterated that these findings are very radimentary and
received financial advice only, with no other information received financial advice plus information about social, based on mean data values rather than formal statistical
Participants were asked to classify their reason for retire¬
about social, personal, relationship, or health issues. With personal, relationship, and health issues had mean (and SD) comparisons. However, even taken with due caution, this
ment into one of four "causes": age, illness, forced (i.e.,
only 9 men who had received "financial-plus" information, scores of 26.3 (6.15) for SAS, 32.3 (7.69) for SDS, and 22.7 finding raises questions about the common practice of
because of redundancy), and voluntarily. Because this
formal statistical comparisons are not usually advisable (6.97) for stress. Again, these findings did not reach formal restricting pre-retirement training to financial matters as a
dependent variable was categorical, MANOVA was used to
because of the large risk of making a Type II error. significance levels, either for main or univariate effects means of lowering anxiety, depression, and stress following
test for the presence of significant differences across the
However, for the purposes of exploring these data with a (Table 3), and therefore are exploratory at best. The power retirement.
group of four reasons for retirement. MANOVA is particu¬
view to setting up more formal and planned comparisons of values for all cross-group comparisons were also low (about It is clear from these data (as well as from those reported
larly relevant for analyses of this sort, being relatively
effects between types of training in later studies, means and .3), again urging caution in determining that there were no earlier by other researchers) that retirement is not the
robust and free from weaknesses arising from violations of
standard deviations for anxiety, depression, and stress were significant differences in dependent variable results across "honeymoon" it has sometimes been described to be.
assumptions which underlie other statistical procedures. As
calculated and are presented below. In addition, data on the
pointed out by Glass, Peckham, and Saunders (1972), the the three forms of pre-retirement training sampled here. Although Atchley (1976) referred to the initial stage of
three dependent variables were compared via MANOVA. retirement as such a honeymoon, he also suggested that this
view that the validity of the probability and power state¬ For men who received financial advice only, means and
ments from analysis of variance statistics was subject to the Discussion was followed by a longer period of depression. It may be
standard deviations for SAS, SDS, and stress scores were, that this honeymoon-depression process is influenced by
three assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance, respectively, 29.4 (6.0), 34.8 (8.94), and 27.1 (9.33). For The data obtained here represent the Australian population,
and independence has been shown to be incorrect. This is the kind of pre-retirement education or training received,
men who received financial advice, plus information about with the possible exception of unskilled manual workers.
further elaborated in Glass and Hopkins (1984), with and this issue is currently being investigated by the authors.
social, personal, relationship, and health issues, the means Therefore, the conclusions drawn below need to be consid¬
detailed arguments supporting the use of MANOVA proce¬ ered as based on trades and professional occupations as well
Of obvious relevance to psychologists is the tentative
and standard deviations were 27.6 (4.72), 30.1 (7.13), and
dures in cases such as the present data. Taking males first, suggestion that financial-plus pre-retirement education and
22.6 (4.73) for SAS, SDS, and stress. For men who received as managerial and administrative occupations, but not
there was a significant main effect, ^(15, 933) = 3.654, p < necessarily representative of unskilled manual labourers.
training (using the expertise of the psychologist to inform
no pre-retirement training or advice at all, scores were 30.9
.001 (Wilks), and significant univariate effects for anxiety The first finding of interest to psychologists from these people about to retire of the ways in which they can adjust
(6.77), 34.2 (8.65), and 26.0 (8.37) respectively. MANOVA
F(3, 342) = 8.229, p < .001, depression F = 7.976, p < .001, data is that both men and women who are forced to retire for
more effectively to this life crisis) may be superior to simple
main effects are presented in Table 3, and indicate that
stress F = 5.966, p < .005, and illness F = 4.217, p < .01, but financial-only advice schemes. Development of standard¬
extreme caution should be used when interpreting these reasons of redundancy or ill-health are most at risk of suffer¬
not for health. Table 2 shows the comparative values for ised retirement education programs has received only minor
results because of the low estimates of power in most of ing adverse psychological reactions to retirement. Although
each cell for each of the four dependent variables that this is not an original finding, having been noted previously input from psychologists to date, and this is a challenge
these comparisons. While there were reasonably acceptable
showed significant effects. Direct Scheffe contrasts for each which faces psychologists who seek to become more
main effects, suggesting a significant trend towards their with data from other nations, it does represent the most
of these dependent variables indicated that men who retired relevant to the everyday person in society as well as to those
being a difference in dependent variable data across the recent data from Australia, where there has been compara¬
because of illness had significantly {p < .05) higher scores tively little attention paid to the wellbeing of the retired experiencing more pressing psychological difficulties. If
three types of pre-retirement training, univariate compar¬
for anxiety and depression than men who retired for any of technological and sociological factors push a greater propor¬
isons for men did not have sufficient power to enable firm person. The 1990s have witnessed many people being forced
the other three reasons; men who were forced to retire had tion of the Australian population towards retirement,
conclusions to be drawn. Comparisons across the three to retire because of industrial shedding of staff, and it can be
significantly higher stress scores than men who left work assumed that at least a proportion of these people have been
adequate preparation of psychologists in issues relating to
types of training revealed similar constraints on drawing
voluntarily; and men who retired because of illness also had the psychological concomitants of retirement will become a
firm conclusions, with power estimates in the region of .3, unwilling to retire. Moreover, Department of Social Security
significantly higher scores on the illness questionnaire than priority of satisfactory mental health of the population at
suggesting that it was not possible to reliably detect the statistics argue that about one quarter of employees 45 years
men who retired voluntarily or because of age. Women also large. Psychologist training programs in universities need to
presence of significant" differences in dependent variable old or more who have been retrenched fall into this category
showed a significant main effect for reason for retirement,
respond to this Challenge by including retirement and its
measures across pre-retirement training groups for men. (Department of Social Security, 1988). In terms of their
sequelae in curricula, and to assist what are often quite
F(15, 983) = 2.761, p < .001, with significant univariate However, although these comparisons should be considered immediate psychological adjustment to this crisis, it is clear
effects for anxiety F(3, 360) = 6.1%%, p < .001, depression F young students to come to grips with issues which may
exploratory at best, mean values for the dependent variables that psychologists could help these people cope with this
= 4.170, p < .01, stress F = 6.273, p < .001, and illness F = appear to be largely irrelevant to their immediate lives.
suggest that those men who received any form of pre-retire¬ unforseen and unwanted lifestyle change.
6.410, p < .001 (see Table 2 for these data). Scheffe ment education or training had lower anxiety, depression, Again, as previously noted in other cultures, lack of
contrasts again indicated that women who retired because of and stress scores than men who did not receive any form of adequate pre-retirement education and training appeal's to
illness had significantly higher anxiety and illness scores pre-retirement training, but that this overall effect was due be associated with poorer adjustment to retirement. Atchley, R. (1976). The sociology of retirement. Cambridge, MA;
than women who retired for any other reason, and signifi¬ to the very low scores from those men who received the Although this overall conclusion is extremely cautious Schenkman.
cantly higher depression scores than women who retired financial-plus form of pre-retirement training, with little because of the difficulty in determining the real value of Atchley, R. (1982). Retirement: Learning the world of work.
because of age or voluntarily; and women who retired positive influence from financial-only training on anxiety, statistical comparisons due to a high likelihood of making Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
because of illness or who were forced to retire had signifi¬ and a clear negative effect for depression and stress. Type II errors for both males and females, simple mean Sciences, 464, 120-131.
cantly higher stress scores than women who retired volun¬ Similar findings were apparent for females, where only values suggest that financial-only pre-retirement education Australian Council of Social Services. (1980). Early retirement:
tarily or because of age. 6 participants reported receiving financial-plus pre-retire¬ and training is only marginally better than no education or Blessing or curse. Melbourne: ACOSS.
ment education or training, and 32 having received finan¬ training at all. Also extremely exploratory (and strongly Barfield, R., & Morgan, J. (1978). Trends in satisfaction with
Pre-retirement Training cial-only education for retirement. Data (means, SDs) for arguing for a planned comparison of pre-retirement educa¬ retirement. The Gerontologist, 18, 19-23.
Supporting the statement made above by the Australian the women who received financial-only pre-retirement tion strategies) is the suggestion that financial-plus pre- DeJonge, J., & Banelce, J. (1989). The Zung self-rating depression
Government report on preparation for retirement, only 60 education or training were SAS = 29.1 (5.79), SDS = 32.2 scale: A replication study on reliability, validity and prediction.
(17.2%) of the 349 men sampled had participated in any (7.2) , and stress = 26.6 (6.0); for women who received no Psychological reports, 64, 833-834.
TABLE 3 Department of Social Security. (1988). Towards a national retire¬
ment incomes policy: An overview (Issues Paper No. 6).
Results of MANOVAs for Pre-retirement Education of
TABLE 2 Canberra; Author.
Anxiety, Depression, and Stress
Cell Values (Means and Standard Deviations) for Reason for Retirement for Each Significant Dependent Variable Encel, S., & Studencki, H. (1996). Retirement: A survey. Sydney:
Males University of NSW, Social Policy Research Centre.
Males Main effect: F(6, 690) = 1,809, p = .095, = .681 Feldman, D. (1994). The decision to retire early: A review and
Reason Anxiety Depression Stress illness Univariate effects (df= 2, 346):
25.24 7.69 4.89 3.50 conceptualisation. Academy of Management Review, 19,
Age 30.30 5.72 34.22 7.72 Anxiety F= 2.107, p= .135, ,6= .415
28,10 S.23 7.17 4.33 285-311.
Illness 34.73 7.46 39.08 9.65 Depression F= 1.163, p= .314, ,8= .253
Forced 30.31 6.10 34.09 8.52 28.82 8.24 5.41 3.77 Stress F = 1.281, p = .278, /? = .276 Fretz, B., Kluge, N., Ossana, S., Jones, S., & Merikangas, M.
Voluntary 29.50 6.71 32.27 8.33 24.29 8.58 5.07 4.12
Females (1989), Intervention targets for reducing pre-retirement anxiety
Females Main effect: F(6, 760) = 1.806, p = .095, = .682 and depression. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,
Reason Anxiety Depression Stress Illness Univariate effects [df= 2, 381): 301-307.
Age 31.23 7.01 35.00 10.04 25.90 8.31 5.38 4.27 Anxiety F= 3.029, p = .050, p = .584 Gabrys, J., & Peters, K. (1985). Reliability, discriminant and
Illness 35.97 9.35 39.77 10.05 29.14 10.06 8.46 5.00 Depression F= 2.002, p = .136, = .412
5.35 3.62
predictive validity of the Zung self-rating depression scaie,
Forced 30.92 7.17 35.83 9.91 29.15 8.50 Stress F= .584, p = .558, p = ,149
24.34 6.87 5.46 3.92 Psychological Reports, 57, 1091-1096.
Voluntary 30.22 6.64 33.96 8.62

JULY 1998 T AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST


JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST
123
122
EFFECTS OF AGE OF RETIREMENT
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY AND RENATY LAYTON
pre-retirement education or training at all, scores were 31.5 retirement education is of greatest value to retirees. It must
Reason for Retirement form of pre-retirement education or training. Of these 60, 51 (7.43), 35.4 (9.55), 25.9 (8.2) respectively. Women who be reiterated that these findings are very radimentary and
received financial advice only, with no other information received financial advice plus information about social, based on mean data values rather than formal statistical
Participants were asked to classify their reason for retire¬
about social, personal, relationship, or health issues. With personal, relationship, and health issues had mean (and SD) comparisons. However, even taken with due caution, this
ment into one of four "causes": age, illness, forced (i.e.,
only 9 men who had received "financial-plus" information, scores of 26.3 (6.15) for SAS, 32.3 (7.69) for SDS, and 22.7 finding raises questions about the common practice of
because of redundancy), and voluntarily. Because this
formal statistical comparisons are not usually advisable (6.97) for stress. Again, these findings did not reach formal restricting pre-retirement training to financial matters as a
dependent variable was categorical, MANOVA was used to
because of the large risk of making a Type II error. significance levels, either for main or univariate effects means of lowering anxiety, depression, and stress following
test for the presence of significant differences across the
However, for the purposes of exploring these data with a (Table 3), and therefore are exploratory at best. The power retirement.
group of four reasons for retirement. MANOVA is particu¬
view to setting up more formal and planned comparisons of values for all cross-group comparisons were also low (about It is clear from these data (as well as from those reported
larly relevant for analyses of this sort, being relatively
effects between types of training in later studies, means and .3), again urging caution in determining that there were no earlier by other researchers) that retirement is not the
robust and free from weaknesses arising from violations of
standard deviations for anxiety, depression, and stress were significant differences in dependent variable results across "honeymoon" it has sometimes been described to be.
assumptions which underlie other statistical procedures. As
calculated and are presented below. In addition, data on the
pointed out by Glass, Peckham, and Saunders (1972), the the three forms of pre-retirement training sampled here. Although Atchley (1976) referred to the initial stage of
three dependent variables were compared via MANOVA. retirement as such a honeymoon, he also suggested that this
view that the validity of the probability and power state¬ For men who received financial advice only, means and
ments from analysis of variance statistics was subject to the Discussion was followed by a longer period of depression. It may be
standard deviations for SAS, SDS, and stress scores were, that this honeymoon-depression process is influenced by
three assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance, respectively, 29.4 (6.0), 34.8 (8.94), and 27.1 (9.33). For The data obtained here represent the Australian population,
and independence has been shown to be incorrect. This is the kind of pre-retirement education or training received,
men who received financial advice, plus information about with the possible exception of unskilled manual workers.
further elaborated in Glass and Hopkins (1984), with and this issue is currently being investigated by the authors.
social, personal, relationship, and health issues, the means Therefore, the conclusions drawn below need to be consid¬
detailed arguments supporting the use of MANOVA proce¬ ered as based on trades and professional occupations as well
Of obvious relevance to psychologists is the tentative
and standard deviations were 27.6 (4.72), 30.1 (7.13), and
dures in cases such as the present data. Taking males first, suggestion that financial-plus pre-retirement education and
22.6 (4.73) for SAS, SDS, and stress. For men who received as managerial and administrative occupations, but not
there was a significant main effect, ^(15, 933) = 3.654, p < necessarily representative of unskilled manual labourers.
training (using the expertise of the psychologist to inform
no pre-retirement training or advice at all, scores were 30.9
.001 (Wilks), and significant univariate effects for anxiety The first finding of interest to psychologists from these people about to retire of the ways in which they can adjust
(6.77), 34.2 (8.65), and 26.0 (8.37) respectively. MANOVA
F(3, 342) = 8.229, p < .001, depression F = 7.976, p < .001, data is that both men and women who are forced to retire for
more effectively to this life crisis) may be superior to simple
main effects are presented in Table 3, and indicate that
stress F = 5.966, p < .005, and illness F = 4.217, p < .01, but financial-only advice schemes. Development of standard¬
extreme caution should be used when interpreting these reasons of redundancy or ill-health are most at risk of suffer¬
not for health. Table 2 shows the comparative values for ised retirement education programs has received only minor
results because of the low estimates of power in most of ing adverse psychological reactions to retirement. Although
each cell for each of the four dependent variables that this is not an original finding, having been noted previously input from psychologists to date, and this is a challenge
these comparisons. While there were reasonably acceptable
showed significant effects. Direct Scheffe contrasts for each which faces psychologists who seek to become more
main effects, suggesting a significant trend towards their with data from other nations, it does represent the most
of these dependent variables indicated that men who retired relevant to the everyday person in society as well as to those
being a difference in dependent variable data across the recent data from Australia, where there has been compara¬
because of illness had significantly {p < .05) higher scores tively little attention paid to the wellbeing of the retired experiencing more pressing psychological difficulties. If
three types of pre-retirement training, univariate compar¬
for anxiety and depression than men who retired for any of technological and sociological factors push a greater propor¬
isons for men did not have sufficient power to enable firm person. The 1990s have witnessed many people being forced
the other three reasons; men who were forced to retire had tion of the Australian population towards retirement,
conclusions to be drawn. Comparisons across the three to retire because of industrial shedding of staff, and it can be
significantly higher stress scores than men who left work assumed that at least a proportion of these people have been
adequate preparation of psychologists in issues relating to
types of training revealed similar constraints on drawing
voluntarily; and men who retired because of illness also had the psychological concomitants of retirement will become a
firm conclusions, with power estimates in the region of .3, unwilling to retire. Moreover, Department of Social Security
significantly higher scores on the illness questionnaire than priority of satisfactory mental health of the population at
suggesting that it was not possible to reliably detect the statistics argue that about one quarter of employees 45 years
men who retired voluntarily or because of age. Women also large. Psychologist training programs in universities need to
presence of significant" differences in dependent variable old or more who have been retrenched fall into this category
showed a significant main effect for reason for retirement,
respond to this Challenge by including retirement and its
measures across pre-retirement training groups for men. (Department of Social Security, 1988). In terms of their
sequelae in curricula, and to assist what are often quite
F(15, 983) = 2.761, p < .001, with significant univariate However, although these comparisons should be considered immediate psychological adjustment to this crisis, it is clear
effects for anxiety F(3, 360) = 6.1%%, p < .001, depression F young students to come to grips with issues which may
exploratory at best, mean values for the dependent variables that psychologists could help these people cope with this
= 4.170, p < .01, stress F = 6.273, p < .001, and illness F = appear to be largely irrelevant to their immediate lives.
suggest that those men who received any form of pre-retire¬ unforseen and unwanted lifestyle change.
6.410, p < .001 (see Table 2 for these data). Scheffe ment education or training had lower anxiety, depression, Again, as previously noted in other cultures, lack of
contrasts again indicated that women who retired because of and stress scores than men who did not receive any form of adequate pre-retirement education and training appeal's to
illness had significantly higher anxiety and illness scores pre-retirement training, but that this overall effect was due be associated with poorer adjustment to retirement. Atchley, R. (1976). The sociology of retirement. Cambridge, MA;
than women who retired for any other reason, and signifi¬ to the very low scores from those men who received the Although this overall conclusion is extremely cautious Schenkman.
cantly higher depression scores than women who retired financial-plus form of pre-retirement training, with little because of the difficulty in determining the real value of Atchley, R. (1982). Retirement: Learning the world of work.
because of age or voluntarily; and women who retired positive influence from financial-only training on anxiety, statistical comparisons due to a high likelihood of making Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social
because of illness or who were forced to retire had signifi¬ and a clear negative effect for depression and stress. Type II errors for both males and females, simple mean Sciences, 464, 120-131.
cantly higher stress scores than women who retired volun¬ Similar findings were apparent for females, where only values suggest that financial-only pre-retirement education Australian Council of Social Services. (1980). Early retirement:
tarily or because of age. 6 participants reported receiving financial-plus pre-retire¬ and training is only marginally better than no education or Blessing or curse. Melbourne: ACOSS.
ment education or training, and 32 having received finan¬ training at all. Also extremely exploratory (and strongly Barfield, R., & Morgan, J. (1978). Trends in satisfaction with
Pre-retirement Training cial-only education for retirement. Data (means, SDs) for arguing for a planned comparison of pre-retirement educa¬ retirement. The Gerontologist, 18, 19-23.
Supporting the statement made above by the Australian the women who received financial-only pre-retirement tion strategies) is the suggestion that financial-plus pre- DeJonge, J., & Banelce, J. (1989). The Zung self-rating depression
Government report on preparation for retirement, only 60 education or training were SAS = 29.1 (5.79), SDS = 32.2 scale: A replication study on reliability, validity and prediction.
(17.2%) of the 349 men sampled had participated in any (7.2) , and stress = 26.6 (6.0); for women who received no Psychological reports, 64, 833-834.
TABLE 3 Department of Social Security. (1988). Towards a national retire¬
ment incomes policy: An overview (Issues Paper No. 6).
Results of MANOVAs for Pre-retirement Education of
TABLE 2 Canberra; Author.
Anxiety, Depression, and Stress
Cell Values (Means and Standard Deviations) for Reason for Retirement for Each Significant Dependent Variable Encel, S., & Studencki, H. (1996). Retirement: A survey. Sydney:
Males University of NSW, Social Policy Research Centre.
Males Main effect: F(6, 690) = 1,809, p = .095, = .681 Feldman, D. (1994). The decision to retire early: A review and
Reason Anxiety Depression Stress illness Univariate effects (df= 2, 346):
25.24 7.69 4.89 3.50 conceptualisation. Academy of Management Review, 19,
Age 30.30 5.72 34.22 7.72 Anxiety F= 2.107, p= .135, ,6= .415
28,10 S.23 7.17 4.33 285-311.
Illness 34.73 7.46 39.08 9.65 Depression F= 1.163, p= .314, ,8= .253
Forced 30.31 6.10 34.09 8.52 28.82 8.24 5.41 3.77 Stress F = 1.281, p = .278, /? = .276 Fretz, B., Kluge, N., Ossana, S., Jones, S., & Merikangas, M.
Voluntary 29.50 6.71 32.27 8.33 24.29 8.58 5.07 4.12
Females (1989), Intervention targets for reducing pre-retirement anxiety
Females Main effect: F(6, 760) = 1.806, p = .095, = .682 and depression. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,
Reason Anxiety Depression Stress Illness Univariate effects [df= 2, 381): 301-307.
Age 31.23 7.01 35.00 10.04 25.90 8.31 5.38 4.27 Anxiety F= 3.029, p = .050, p = .584 Gabrys, J., & Peters, K. (1985). Reliability, discriminant and
Illness 35.97 9.35 39.77 10.05 29.14 10.06 8.46 5.00 Depression F= 2.002, p = .136, = .412
5.35 3.62
predictive validity of the Zung self-rating depression scaie,
Forced 30.92 7.17 35.83 9.91 29.15 8.50 Stress F= .584, p = .558, p = ,149
24.34 6.87 5.46 3.92 Psychological Reports, 57, 1091-1096.
Voluntary 30.22 6.64 33.96 8.62

JULY 1998 T AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST


JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST
123
122
CHRISTOPHER F. SHARPLEY AND RENATY LAYTON
Journal of Applied MealPk BekavmtA
Glass, G.V., & Hopkins, K. (1984). Statistical methods in educa¬ Schaefer, A., Brown, J., Watson, C., Plenel, D., DeMotts, J.,
tion and psychology. (2nd ed.). New York: Prentice-Hall. Howard, M., Petrik, N., & Ballweg, B. (1985). Comparison of
Glass, G.V., Peckham. P., & Saunders, J. (1972). Consequences of
the validities of the Beck, Zung, and MMPI depression scales.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 415—418.
failure to meet assumptions underlying the fixed effects analy¬
sis of variance and covariance. Review of Educational Sharpley, C.F. (1997). Psychometric properties of the Self- "What makes me happy now that I'm older".
Research, 42. 237-288. perceived Stress in Retirement Scale. Psychological Reports,

Greenberg, J.S. (1981). A study of stressors in the college student


81, 319-322. A retrospective report of attitudes and strategies used to
population. Health Education, 12, 8-12. Sharpley, C.F., & Rogers, H.J. (1985). Naive versus sophisticated

Homstein, G., & Wapner, S. (1985). Modes of experiencing and


item-writers for the assessment of anxiety. Journal of Clinical adjust to retirement as reported by older persons.
Psychological. 41. 58-62.
adapting to retirement. International Journal of Aging and
Human Development, 21, 291-315. Walker, J., Kimmel, D., & Price, K. (1981). Retirement style and
retirement satisfaction: Retirees aren't all alike. International
House of Representatives Standing Committee on Community
Affairs. (1990). Is retirement working? Canberra: Australian
Journal of Aging and Human Development, 12, 267-281. Christopher R Sharpley
Government Publishing Service. Winocur, S., & Rosenman, L. (1991, July). Women and retirement.
Paper presented at the SPRC National Policy Research Bond University
Jegede, R. (1977). Psychometric attributes of the self-rating anxiety Conference, University of NSW.
scale. Psychological Reports, 40, 303-306. Prisciila G. Yardley
Zung, W.W.K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of
Muller, C., & Boaz, R. (1988). Health as a reason for rationaliza¬
General Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.
tion for being retired? Research on Aging, 10, 37-55. Monash University
Zung, W.W.K. (1971). A rating insti-ument for anxiety disorders.
Parker, S., (1980). Older workers and retirement. London: HMSO.
Psychosomatics, 12, 371-379.
Parker, S. (1982). Work and retirement. London: Allen & Unwin. Send requests for reprints to: Professor C. F. Sharpley,
Zung, W.W.K. (1973). From art to science: The diagnosis and
Peterson, C. (1989). Looking forward through the lifespan: treatment of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29,
Institute for Health Sdences, Bond University, Gold
Developmental psychology (3rd ed.). Sydney: Prentice-Hall. 328-337. Coast, Qld, 4229.
Prentis, R. (1992). Passages of retirement. New York: Greenwood. Zung, W.W.K. (1980, December). How normal is anxiety? New
Richardson, V., & Kilty, K. (1991). Adjustment to retirement: York: Scope.
109 people who had been retired for at least 10 years in retirement (Fretz, Kluge, Ossana, Jones &
Continuity vs discontinuity. International Journal of Aging and Zung, W.W.K, (1986). Prevalence of anxiety in a family practice
completed questionnaires assessing their Merikangas, 1989). In a previous paper (Sharpley &
Human Development, 33, 151-169. setting. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 1471-1472.
depression-happiness, everyday stress, and their Layton, 1998) we reported that pre-retirement
own descriptions of what strategies they had used education, particularly of the type that included social,
personal and health issues, was significantly
to cope with stress, how successful those strategies
associated with lowered anxiety, stress, and depression
had been, and what advice they would give to in the first five years after retirement.
potential retirees. Data revealed that the sample
However, the data reported in these (and most
was more depressed than younger age groups, and other) studies of retirement have been collected from
that relatively happier participants showed people who have retired relatively recently (i.e., during
significantly different ratings of sources of daily the last five or 10 years). As noted by Atchiey (1976)
stress than the relatively depressed participants. with American men, and more recently in our own
work with Australian men and women (Sharpley &
Additionally, depressed participants tended to
Jacobs, 1997), adjustment to retirement apparently
report stress-coping strategies which were more
goes through a number of stages. First, immediately
avoidance-oriented, whereas happier participants following retirement and lasting for about 12 months,
reported that they were more goal-oriented and there is the "Honeymoon" period, during which
accepted challenges and change with active retirees feel better, engage in "those things I never
had the time for", and generally relish the retirement
responses, a positive outlook, and physical exercise.
experience. Following this, and lasting for about six
Similarly, when asked what advice they would give
to 12 months, is a period which Atchiey referred to as
to potential retirees, happier participants' responses "Disillusionment", when retirees experience

emphasised independence and ability to confront loneliness, despair, depression, and a general lowered
successfully the challenge of change, while health status. For men this seems to last for about six
depressed participants focussed upon ensuring months, but for women it has been reported as lasting
for about 18 months (Sharpley & Jacobs, 1997).
that finances were adequate. Finally, happier Following this unhappy period, retirees have been
retirees suggested that retirement should not reported as increasing in well-being up until about
necessarily be accepted a s an unavoidable four or five years after retirement (Atchiey, 1976;
consequence of age. Sharpley & Jacobs, 1997), when a general decline in
well-being occurs.

Because both Atchley's (1976) and Sharpley


How people adjust to retirement and older and Jacobs's (1997) data were mostly restricted to the
age has been the focus of a number of research studies first five years after retirement, no reliable conclusions
(e.g., Atchiey, 1976; Feldman, 1994; Muller & Boaz, about the years after that point can be drawn.
1988; Parker, 1982; Peterson, 1989; Walker, Kimmel However, for many people, that stage in post-
& Price, 1981). Several apparently consistent retirement represents an actual chronological age of
predictive factors have been identified, such as only about 50 to 65 years, too young to be accompanied
whether retirement was voluntary or forced by age or by the rapid decline in health and well-being which
redundancy (Walker, Kimmel & Price, 1981), poor accompanies much older persons as they approach
health (Muller & Boaz, 1988), pre-retirement death. Therefore, the specific state of personal
JULY 1998 V AUSTRALIAN PSYCHOLOGIST education and planning (Feldman, 1994; Homstein happiness reported by older people may be associated
& Wapner, 1985), and low expectations of being happy with attitudinal factors rather than purely physical
124
.Volume 1 Number 2, 1999. 31

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen