Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

www.elsevier.com/locate/healthplace

Social capital: a guide to its measurement


Kimberly Lochner a, Ichiro Kawachi a,*, Bruce P. Kennedy b
a
Department of Health and Social Behavior, Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA 02115, USA
b
Division of Public Health practice, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA

Abstract

The primary aims of this paper are to review the concept of social capital and related constructs and to provide a
brief guide to their operationalization and measurement. We focus on four existing constructs: collective ecacy,
psychological sense of community, neighborhood cohesion and community competence. Each of these constructs
taps into slightly di€erent, yet overlapping, aspects of social capital. The existence of several instruments to measure
each of these constructs calls for further study into their use as measures of social capital. Despite di€erences in the
approach to measurement, there is general agreement that community characteristics, such as social capital, should
be distinguished from individual characteristics and measured at the community level. # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd.
All rights reserved.

Keywords: Social capital; Cohesion; Community; Neighborhood

1. Introduction (Haan et al., 1987; LeClere et al., 1997). The emphasis


on the characteristics of places in these studies is in
Researchers are increasingly interested in studying marked contrast to much of contemporary epidemiolo-
the e€ects of the social environment on health gical practice, which tends to focus almost exclusively
(Marmot, 1998). Recent examples of studies in which on the individuals who live in them and their risk
researchers have attempted to establish aspects of the behaviors (Macintyre et al., 1993). However, even with
social environment as determinants of health include: the growing interest in examining the relationship of
the e€ects of income inequality on mortality (Kaplan the social environment to health, there remains the
et al., 1996; Kennedy et al., 1996; Wilkinson, 1996); challenging task of identifying which ecologic charac-
the links between residential segregation and black teristics of communities and societies that matter for
infant mortality (LaVeist, 1993; Polednak, 1997); and population health status.
the impact of neighborhood deprivation on coronary One characteristic that has generated considerable
risk factors (Diez-Roux et al., 1997), low birthweight attention is the concept of `social capital'. The concept
(Roberts, 1997), homicide (Shihadeh and Flynn, 1996), of social capital originated in the ®elds of sociology
morbidity (Robert, 1998) as well as all-cause mortality and political science to explain how citizens within cer-
tain communities cooperate with each other to over-
come the dilemmas of collective action, but recently
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-617-432-0235; fax: +1- public health researchers have turned to the notion of
617-432-3755. social capital to explain heterogeneities in population
E-mail address: ichiro.kawachi@channing.harvard.edu (I. health status across geographic areas (Kawachi and
Kawachi) Kennedy, 1997; Kawachi et al., 1997a). Indeed the rel-

1353-8292/99/$ - see front matter # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 1 3 5 3 - 8 2 9 2 ( 9 9 ) 0 0 0 1 6 - 7
260 K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

evance of `social capital' to public health has deep his- of social networks and support, which are attributes of
torical roots. Just over a hundred years ago, Emile individuals.
Durkheim demonstrated that social integration was
inversely related to the suicide rate in societies
(Durkheim, 1897 (1951). Since Durkheim, there have 3. Social capital and public health
been a variety of attempts to conceptualize and
measure societal characteristics such as social inte- The relationship of social capital to population
gration, social cohesion and most recently, social capi- health was examined recently in an ecologic analysis
tal. based on state-level mortality rates within the United
The aims of the present paper are: to provide a brief States (Kawachi et al., 1997b). The indicators of social
overview of the concept of social capital, to review the capital (adapted from the work of Putnam) were
theory and approaches to measuring constructs appar- obtained from the General Social Surveys conducted
ently related to social capital; and to point out direc- by the National Opinion Research Center. The indi-
tions for further research. cators of social capital were measured by responses to
We focus our review on four existing constructs: col- several survey questions. The survey asked respondents
lective ecacy, psychological sense of community, about their membership in a wide variety of voluntary
neighborhood cohesion and community competence. associations, including church groups, sports groups,
Each of these existing constructs seem to capture some hobby groups, fraternal organizations, labor unions
fundamental aspect of social capital. We discuss each and so on. Individual responses to these questions
of these constructs in terms of their measurement and were aggregated up to the state level to create an indi-
their relationship to social capital. cator of per capita membership in voluntary groups.
Respondents in each state were also asked whether
they thought ``Most people can be trusted Ð or you
can't be too careful in dealing with people'' (an indi-
2. Social capital: overview and de®nition cator of interpersonal trust) as well as whether they
agreed with the question ``Most of the time people try
Social capital, as de®ned by its principal theorists to be helpful Ð or are they mostly looking out for
(Coleman, 1990; Putnam, 1993a,b), consists of those themselves?'' (an indicator of perceived norms of reci-
features of social organization Ð such as networks of procity). Thirty-nine states were covered by the
secondary associations, high levels of interpersonal sampling frame of the survey. After adjusting for po-
trust and norms of mutual aid and reciprocity Ð tential sampling bias in the surveys, wide variations
which act as resources for individuals and facilitate were observed between states in their stocks of social
collective action. For example, a community rich in capital as measured by the above questions. For
stocks of social capital is supposedly more likely to example, less than 10% of those surveyed in North
possess e€ective civic institutions and, hence, to pros- Dakota believed that ``most people can't be trusted'',
per (Putnam, 1993a) and more likely to be e€ective in compared to more than 40% in Louisiana.
maintaining law and order (Hagan et al., 1995; The indicators of social capital were strongly corre-
Sampson, 1996). According to Robert Putnam, a pol- lated with each other: civic mistrust with group mem-
itical scientist whose seminal book Making Democracy bership (r=ÿ0.65); mistrust with perceived lack of
Work (1993a) stimulated much of the recent enthu- reciprocity (r = 0.81); and group membership to lack
siasm for this concept, the level of social capital in so- of reciprocity (r=ÿ0.54) (all correlations, P < 0.05).
ciety can be measured by indicators such as the density Each indicator of social capital was also strikingly cor-
of membership in voluntary associations of all kinds, related with state mortality rates, e.g., the level of mis-
the extent of interpersonal trust between citizens, and trust in each state was strongly inversely correlated
their perceptions of the availability of mutual aid with age-adjusted all-cause mortality rates (r = 0.79,
(Putnam, 1993b, 1995). Although several other de®- p < 0.0001) (Fig. 1).
nitions of social capital exist (Loury, 1992; Bordieu In regression analyses adjusted for household pov-
and Wacquant, 1992), which di€er slightly from erty rates, a 1% increment in the level of mistrust was
Putnam's, there is enough consensus to draw some im- associated with an increase in total mortality rate of
portant generalizations about the nature of social capi- 4.5 deaths per 100,000 (P < 0.0001). Variations in the
tal. The most important of these is that social capital level of trust explained 58% of the variance in total
is a collective dimension of society external to the indi- mortality across states. Lower levels of trust were also
vidual. Social capital is a feature of the social struc- associated with higher rates of most major causes of
ture, not of the individual actors within the social death, including coronary heart disease, malignant
structure; it is an ecologic characteristic. In this way, neoplasms, stroke, unintentional injury and infant
social capital can be distinguished from the concepts mortality. If these associations are causal, then it was
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270 261

Fig. 1. The relationship between age-adjusted mortality rates and lack of social trust.

estimated that a increase in level of trust by one stan- this construct for public health. On the other hand, the
dard deviation, or 10%, would be associated with loose de®nition of social capital leaves ample room for
about a 9% lower level of overall mortality. The two further re®nement of both theory and di€erent
other indicators of social capital, group membership measurement approaches. In the remainder of this
and perceived lack of reciprocity, were similarly corre- review, we describe four alternative community-level
lated with age-adjusted mortality rates (r=ÿ0.49, and constructs that overlap to varying extents with the
0.71, respectively; p < 0.0001). notion of social capital. These constructs are collective
In addition to social capital's relationship to state ecacy, psychological sense of community, neighbor-
mortality rates, social capital is strongly inversely cor- hood cohesion and community competence.
related with rates of violent crime (Wilkinson et al.,
1998, in press). The lower the level of social capital,
the higher the rate of violent crime, including homi- 4. Collective ecacy
cide, aggravated assault and burglary. Criminologists
have theorized for some time that low social capital is Based on the work of Bandura (1986), social psy-
associated with higher delinquency and violent crime. chologists have de®ned collective ecacy in several
According to social disorganization theory (Sampson, ways with all de®nitions sharing the notion that group
1988; Sampson and Groves, 1989; Sampson, 1996), the members believe in the overall ability of the collective
ability of a neighborhood to control crime depends to act e€ectively. Collective ecacy has been de®ned
upon levels of informal social control, e.g., the moni- as ``a sense of collective competence shared among in-
toring of spontaneous play groups among children, or dividuals when allocating, coordinating and integrating
a willingness to intervene to prevent acts such as tru- their resources in a successful concerted response to
ancy and vandalism. In turn, the willingness of local speci®c situational demands'' (Zaccaro et al., 1995, p.
residents to intervene on behalf of the common good 309). Like individual self-ecacy, collective ecacy is
depends on conditions of mutual trust and solidarity situational, not universal.
(social cohesion) among neighbors. Sampson et al. (1997) proposed that the di€erential
These studies linking social capital and community ability of neighborhoods to realize the common values
well-being (low crime and mortality rates), although of residents and maintain public order is a determinant
few in number, indicate the potential importance of of neighborhood variation in violence. They developed
262 K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

a measure of `collective ecacy', which is de®ned as ecacy was associated with a 39.7% reduction in the
social cohesion among neighbors combined with their expected homicide rate (Sampson et al., 1997).
willingness to intervene on behalf of the common
good. Their scale of `collective ecacy' represents a
combination of two separate subscales: `social cohe- 5. Psychological sense of community
sion' and `informal social control'. `Social cohesion' is
comprised of ®ve items, asking respondents how Turning to the ®eld of community psychology, at
strongly they agreed (on a 5-point Likert scale) that: least 30 separate studies have been published since the
``people in this neighborhood can be trusted''; ``this is 1970s on the concept of ``psychological sense of com-
a close-knit neighborhood''; ``people around here are munity'' (Hill, 1996). Originally formulated by Sarason
willing to help their neighbors''; ``people in this neigh- (1974), the construct was later re®ned by MacMillan
borhood generally don't get along with each other''; and Chavis (1986) to include four separate dimensions:
and ``people in this neighborhood do not share the membership (the sense of feeling part of a group); in¯u-
same values'' (the last two statements were reverse ence (a bidirectional concept that refers to the sense
coded). In other words, the `social cohesion' subscale that the individual matters to the group, and that the
tapped several elements (trust, norms of reciprocity) group can in¯uence its members, thereby creating
that resonate with the notion of social capital. cohesiveness through community norms); integration
The `informal social control' subscale, by contrast, is (the sense that members' needs will be met by the
much more rooted in criminology. Respondents were resources received through their membership in the
asked about the likelihood (on a 5-point Likert scale) group); and shared emotional connection (the sense of
that their neighbors could be counted on to intervene shared history in the community). Selected examples of
in the following situations: children were skipping existing instruments, along with evidence on their re-
school and hanging out on a street corner; children liability and validity, are shown in Table 1.
spray-painting grati on a local building; children From Table 1, it is clear that the existing measures
showing disrespect to an adult; a ®ght breaking out in of ``psychological sense of community'' include several
front of their house; and the ®re station closest to their individual items that tap into the same indicators of a
home being threatened with budget cuts. community's stock of social capital as de®ned by
The scales were developed on a 1995 survey of 8783 Putnam (1993a,b). For example, questions regarding
residents residing in 343 neighborhoods of Chicago, involvement/participation of residents in churches or
IL. Importantly, individual responses to the survey local neighborhood associations; whether community
were aggregated to the neighborhood level (equivalent leaders can be trusted; whether people in the neighbor-
in area to 2±3 census tracts), and analyzed as an ecolo- hood are sociable; and whether people can depend on
gic, not an individual, characteristic. The `social cohe- each other for help. All these items could be con-
sion' and `informal social control' subscales turned out sidered to tap aspects of a community's stock of social
to be strongly correlated across neighborhoods capital.
(r = 0.8, P < 0.001), hence they were combined to Despite the considerable progress that community
form a single scale of `collective ecacy'. Evidence for psychologists have made toward measuring sense of
the internal consistency reliability of these subscales community, a recent review concluded that (Hill, 1996,
was provided from an earlier pilot study (Barnes et al., p. 431):
1997). The alpha coecients for the pilot versions of
the `social cohesion' and `informal social control' sub- The development of a standardized, operational
scales were 0.89 and 0.82, respectively. Consistent with de®nition of the construct has eluded researchers.
the theory of social capital, the measure of `collective At least ®ve measures of the construct have been
ecacy' was signi®cantly correlated with voluntary developed, and there is still a lack of agreement as
group participation (r = 0.45; p < 0.01), as well as to to what speci®c dimensions make up psychological
neighborhood services (r = 0.21). sense of community.
The predictive ability of the `collective ecacy' scale
was examined in a cross-sectional multilevel analysis of Notwithstanding di€erences in the approach to
crime in the 343 neighborhoods of Chicago enrolled in measurement across instruments, there is some consen-
the Project on Human Development in Chicago sus that ``psychological sense of community'' refers to
Neighborhoods. After controlling for individual-level a collective characteristic, not to individual relation-
sociodemographic characteristics, neighborhood collec- ships and behaviors; and that, being an aggregate vari-
tive ecacy showed a strong inverse association with able, it is most usefully measured and studied at the
measures of perceived neighborhood violence, violent community level (Hill, 1996). For example Pretty et al.
victimization and homicide events. For example, a 2 (1994, 1996) hypothesized that although `sense of com-
standard deviation elevation in neighborhood collective munity' is related to individual-level social support and
Table 1
Summary of concepts used in the measurement of Sense of Communitya (NR=not reported)

Doolittle and Glynn, Bachrach and Chavis et al., Davidson and Davidson Perkins et al., Julian et al.,
MacDonald, 1981 Zautra, 1985 1986 Cotter, 1986; and Cotter, 1990 1997
1978 1991 1993

Membership
Participation in churches or local organizations [ [ [ [
(e.g. PTA, youth groups, business/civic groups)
Have a sense of civic duty [
Desire for organizational participation [
Being a member of this community is like [ [
being a member of a group of friends
Feel that you belong/at home in community [ [ [ [ [
In¯uence
Involvement in political clubs/organizations or [
issue- or action- oriented groups
Political ecacy, e.g. I feel I can contribute to [ [
local/city politics if I want to
Neighbors ask your opinions; you ask your [
neighbors opinions
Feel that I am an important part of this [ [
community
Care about what my neighbors think of my [
actions
Interested in knowing what goes on in community [ [
Can trust community leaders [ [
Sharing of values/ful®llment of needs
Perception of block's ability to solve problems (e.g. [ [ [ [
if there is a problem the community can solve it)
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

Have in¯uence on improving block/what this [ [ [


community is like
Perception of block/community, e.g.: [
Good place for children to play; good place to [ [
raise teenagers
Feel safe here [ [
Police in community are friendly; e€ective [ [
Pretty [
Important that community does well and care [ [ [
what community is like
Satisfaction with block [ [ [ [ [
Similarity with others, e.g.:
(continued on next page)
263
Table 1 (continued )
264

Doolittle and Glynn, Bachrach and Chavis et al., Davidson and Davidson Perkins et al., Julian et al.,
MacDonald, 1981 Zautra, 1985 1986 Cotter, 1986; and Cotter, 1990 1997
1978 1991 1993

People in community share the same values; [ [ [ [


degree to which you agree with values and beliefs
of neighbors
Feel similar to most people in my community [ [
Feel good when someone does something good [ [
for my community
Believe that what is good for my community is [ [ [
good for me; my neighbors and I want the same
things from this community
Shared Emotional Connection
Own or rent home [
Planned length of residency [ [ [
Length of residency [ [ [
Neighboring patterns and relationships with
people in neighborhood, e.g.:
Number of neighbors one can identify by ®rst [ [ [
name or recognize
Visited by neighbors frequently; visit neighbors [ [
frequently
Number of people known well enough to visit; [ [
know most of the adult residents in the
neighborhood
Number of people who run a business or store [
in community one can identify by ®rst name
Friends in my community are part of my [ [
everyday activities
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

People in neighborhood know/like each other; [ [ [


people on this block generally don't get along
Do/do not like my neighbors [ [
Dicult to make good friends here [ [
Very few of my neighbors know me [ [
Attachment to the community, e.g.:
My neighborhood is a good place for me to live; [ [
it is important for me to live here
Like my neighborhood/house [ [
Proud to tell others where I live [ [
Would be sorry if I had to move [ [ [
Supportive relationships, e.g.:
People can depend on each other/willing to help [ [
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270 265

Julian et al.,
social networks, it should be viewed as a distinct, eco-
logical attribute that is not dependent on actual experi-

a=0.90
ence of social support. To illustrate this point, Pretty
1997

NR
15
et al. (1994) quoted an adolescent in their study who

[
stated he would feel much more comfortable asking
Perkins et al.,

for help from a stranger who lived on his block than


he would approaching a stranger on any other block.

a=0.80
This was true even though the adolescent had not met
1990

NR
12
most of his neighbors.
The concept of social capital, like `sense of commu-

scales=0.80±
nity', applies to communities in both the geographic

Correlation
and Cotter,
Davidson

and relational sense, and should be considered distinct

between
a=0.84
from individual characteristics. For example, a particu-
1993

0.87
lar individual may be mistrustful of others (scoring
5

high on a personality scale such as the Cook±Medley


Factor analysis;

Hostility inventory), but his/her experience of commu-


Davidson and
Cotter, 1986;

nity is also likely to depend on the extent of trust


among others around them. In other words, whether
emerged
1 factor
a=0.75

or not the community to which the individual belongs


1991

17

is socially cohesive depends to a large extent on the


[

behaviors of others. Identical individuals might there-


Chavis et al.,

fore experience quite divergent health outcomes


explained

depending on the characteristics of the social environ-


criterion
variance
a=0.97

96% of

ment in which they grow up and live. Proof of the


1986

existence of such `contextual e€ects' on health out-


23

comes is the ultimate challenge confronting those who


Bachrach and

investigate the social environment.


Face validity
Zautra, 1985

The four domains of ``sense of community'' are based on MacMillan and Chavis (1986).
a=0.76

6. Neighboring and neighborhood cohesion


7

Reported

A related concept to `sense of community' is that of


validity
tests of
Glynn,

KR-20

several
=0.97

`neighboring' and `neighborhood cohesion'. There is


1981

some debate about the importance of neighbors in a


60
[

time when the de®nition of community is less


Factor analysis;

explained 54%

depended on geographical boundaries and when many


Doolittle and
MacDonald,

of the social relationships people engage in occur out-


of variance
6 factors

side their residential neighborhood (Unger and


Wandersman, 1985). Be that as it may, the measure-
1978

NR
26
[

ment of neighboring is relevant for capturing locally


available resources, in the form of a€ective and instru-
I can always ®nd someone to talk to; there are

mental support. Neighboring involves social inter-


people in community other than my friends who

Most friends in community are here to stay

actions, by which residents establish social connections


that are either personal or at the neighborhood level.
Neighborhood social networks frequently come about
as a result of the formation of neighborhood organiz-
ations or block groups, which in turn serve several
Can be alone when need to

functions besides encouraging sociability. In fact, block


groups are an example of what Coleman (1988)
referred to as `appropriable social organizations',
really care about me
Table 1 (continued )

which are themselves a form of social capital. For


Number of items

example, Coleman described a resident's organization


in an urban housing project, which initially formed for
Reliability

the purpose of pressuring builders to ®x various pro-


Validity

blems (leaks, crumbling sidewalks, etc). After the pro-


a

blems were solved, the organization remained as


266 K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

Table 2
Summary of concepts used in the measurement of neighboringa and neighborhood cohesion. (NR=not reported)

Hunter, Riger and Unger and Buckner, Perkins et al.,


1975 Lavrakas, Wandersman, 1988 1990
1981 1983

Social Interaction
Casual interaction, e.g.:
Number of people you can recognize on your [ [
block; number of people who live on this
block that you know by name
Frequency that you chat with neighbors [ [
Frequency that you visit informally in [ [ [
neighbors home; number of people whom
have a neighborly relationship with
Social support, e.g.:
socio-emotional support [ [ [
instrumental support [ [ [ [
informational support [ [ [ [
Social networks, e.g.:
personal networks (both within and outside [ [
neighborhood)
neighborhood social networks (e.g., [
neighborhood groups)
A€ective Bonds
Sense of mutual aid (e.g. feeling that you can [
rely on your neighbors; believing that potential
help is available)
Sense of community (e.g. membership, [ [ [
in¯uence, sharing of values and emotional
connection)
Attachment to place (e.g. plan to remain a [ [
resident of this neighborhood for a number of
years; would be sad if I had to move from this
neighborhood)
Number of items 13 6 10 18 10

Reliability NR a=0.56±0.59 a=0.88 a=0.95 a=0.76


NR Factor analysis; NR Criterion validity, NR
Validity
2 factors emerged p < 0.001

a
The dimensions of neighboring are based on Unger and Wandersman (1985). The cognitive component of neighboring
described in Unger and Wandersman (1985) is not shown because no studies measured this component.

available social capital to improve the quality of life social structure, not just those who `invest' in main-
for residents. The general point is that an organization, taining social relationships.
once brought into existence for one set of purposes The important aspect about the measurement of
can also be appropriated for other uses, thus constitut- neighborhood social support is that it is an aggregate,
ing a form of social capital. community-level characteristic, not an individual-level
Selected instruments for measuring neighboring and measure. In other words, individual responses to an
neighborhood cohesion are shown on Table 2. In ad- instrument such as Buckner's Neighborhood Cohesion
dition to tapping neighborhood networks and casual Index (1988) are aggregated up to the level of the com-
interaction with neighbors, existing instruments also munity in order to derive an overall neighborhood
attempt to directly measure the availability of instru- cohesion score. As Buckner (1988) (p. 775) notes:
mental and emotional social support within the neigh-
borhood. This approach captures the public goods . . . it is important to distinguish between an individ-
aspect of social capital, i.e., the notion that resources ual variable which is used to measure an individual-
are potentially available to all members within the level attribute and an aggregate individual variable
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270 267

(a mean) which is used to infer a neighborhood- ging the soup kitchen Ð can develop organizational
level attribute . . . if these individual-level scores are and communications skills that are transferable to
aggregated and averaged the resultant mean score politics. In addition, these nonpolitical institutions
(an aggregate individual-level variable) is said to can act as the locus of attempts at political recruit-
form a measure of the cohesiveness of that collec- ment.
tive of neighborhood residents.
In other words, the measurement of `community
This method for measuring `cohesion' follows tra- competence' seems to combine the outcome (political
ditional methods employed by social psychologists, ecacy) with its predictor (social capital). As with the
which often use a mean score from aggregated individ- concept of `sense of community', a review of existing
ual-level data to quantify some collective attribute. measures of community competence concluded that:
Buckner's neighborhood-level analysis (n = 3) found `the lack of a clear operational de®nition of commu-
that using the mean value scores did distinguish nity competence has resulted in an inconsistent array
between neighborhood cohesion levels in the predicted of indices . . . The concept itself is still too abstract,
order (Buckner, 1988). requiring more grounded theory building through dis-
covery in the ®eld' (Eng and Parker, 1994, p. 204).

7. Community competence

Turning ®nally to the ®eld of health promotion, the 8. Discussion


concept of `community competence' has potential over-
lap with social capital. Community competence can be Having reviewed the concept of social capital and
thought of as the problem solving ability of a commu- potentially related measures, what concluding com-
nity that arises through collective e€ort. According to ments can be made? First, we have at present no single
its principal theorist, Cottrell, it is a distinctly group de®nition of social capital, just as there are no single
phenomenon. Cotrell (1976) proposed eight essential de®nitions of the other community-level concepts
preconditions of a competent community (Table 3). reviewed. Instead, the di€erent instruments tap slightly
Among the conditions that are particularly relevant to di€erent Ð but also substantially overlapping Ð
social capital are: commitment to the community (e.g., characteristics of social relations at the community
whether residents volunteer for community activities); level, including civic participation in voluntary associ-
participation (whether residents belong to civic clubs, ations, norms of mutual aid and reciprocity, as well as
fraternal organizations and church); and community levels of interpersonal trust. The availability of several
social support, a dimension added by Eng and Parker instruments calls for a study in which all of them are
(1994). The remaining dimensions tap predominantly simultaneously administered, and the extent of shared
into the stock of political skills (or political ecacy) variance examined.
that are available within the community. These dimen- Despite di€erences in the approach to measurement,
sions include: articulateness; management of relations a strong and consistent theme emerging from this
with larger society; and the machinery for facilitating review is the almost universal agreement that commu-
participant interaction and decision making. The latter nity characteristics ought to be distinguished from indi-
dimensions re¯ect the practical interest of health pro- vidual characteristics, and measured at the community
motion and health education experts in assisting com- level. On the other hand, much work remains to be
munities to mobilize resources to implement social carried out to determine the most valid ways to go
change. about measuring collective attributes. To date, investi-
Interestingly, theorists of social capital would view gators have used the social survey approach (with indi-
political ecacy as a consequence of social capital. For vidual responses aggregated up to the community
example, Verba et al. (1995) have put forward the level). However, alternative approaches Ð such as sys-
`civic voluntarism model' to explain patterns of politi- tematic social observation, or using intrinsic measures
cal participation. According to this model, political of community characteristics Ð may yield additional
skills within the community are developed as a by-pro- insights into the processes of social cohesion. By
duct of civic engagement in secondary associations. `intrinsic' measures, we mean the measurement of com-
Thus (Verba et al., 1995, p. 40): munity characteristics that do not rely on aggregating
the responses of individuals. Creative examples of such
Political activity is embedded in the nonpolitical in- intrinsic measures might include directly observable
stitutions of civil society . . . Undertaking activities features of a community, such as the extent to which
that themselves have nothing to do with politics Ð neighborhood sidewalks are cleared after a snow storm
for example, running the PTA fund drive or mana- (an indicator of reciprocity), or whether local gas
268 K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

Table 3
Summary of concepts used in the measurement of community competencea

Goeppinger Eng and Parker,


and Baglioni, 1994
1985

Commitment
Length of residence [
Care about look of community [ [
Would be sad if had to move [
Casual interaction with other [ [
Use services in town [
People feel they have an active role in making community function [
People volunteer for community activities [
People engage in favors, e.g. lending of goods [
People feel that what happens in community can a€ect them [
Participation
People belong to local service or civic clubs, fraternal organizations, church [ [
People stay in the community for fun [
When things are done in the community, it is often the same few people doing [
all the work
Number of registered voters in community [
Self±other awareness and clarity of situational de®nition
Residents have strong opinions about way things are done [
Local government tries to in¯uence what goes on in community [
Compared with other towns, this town provides services, e.g. parks, libraries, [
health services, to take care of most people's needs
Social supportb
Instrumental, emotional and informational support [
Articulateness/E€ective communication
People will express an unpopular or dissenting opinion [
People are willing to stand before an outside group and state community needs [
Con¯ict containment and accommodation
People speak out about di€erences and work together/®nd ways to solve [ [
di€erences
Management of relations with the larger society
People in this community try to in¯uence what goes on in the larger [ [
surrounding areas
Local elected ocials represent my views [
Number of people in community who own their home [
Number of people who know about services/resources o€ered in larger society [
Machinery for facilitating participant interaction and decision making
People in town try to in¯uence what happens in this community [ [
Town council is e€ective [
People in community have most (some, few, or none) of friends living in [
community
People are willing to help work during a political campaign [
People are willing to contact, e.g. phone, write a letter, to local ocials [
22 41
Number of items
NR Reported separately
Reliability
for each dimension
Validity Discriminant and Face validity
criterion validity
assessed

a
The dimensions for `community competency' are based on Cottrell (1976). (NR=not reported).
b
Not one of Cottrell's eight dimensions included in Eng and Parker (1994).
K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270 269

stations demand prepayment before motorists can ®ll ment: results from a pilot survey. In: Earls, F., Buka, S.L.
their tanks (an indicator of trust). (Eds.), Project on Human Development in Chicago
The issue of measurement raises an additional issue: Neighborhoods. A Technical Report to the National
what is the relevant unit of aggregation? As with any Institute of Justice, pp. 34±45.
Bordieu, P., Wacquant, L., 1992. Invitation to Re¯exive
ecological characteristic, there are several possible
Sociology. Chicago University Press, Chicago.
choices for aggregating information on social capital.
Buckner, J.C., 1988. The development of an instrument to
It makes sense to measure social capital within neigh- measure neighborhood cohesion. American Journal of
borhoods, or at the level of states, or even whole Community Psychology 16, 771±791.
countries. There is every reason to believe that social Chavis, D.M., Hogge, J.H., McMillan, D.W., 1986. Sense of
capital at each of these levels can exert an in¯uence on community through Brunswick's Lens: a ®rst look.
population health. But the processes determining the Journal of Community Psychology 14, 24±40.
causes and consequences of social capital may be quite Coleman, J.S., 1988. Social capital in the creation of human
di€erent at various levels of aggregation. For example, capital. American Journal of Sociology 94, S95±S120.
at the level of the local community, social capital Coleman, J.S., 1990. Foundations of Social Theory. Harvard
depends much more on the day-to-day interactions University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Cottrel, L.S., 1976. The competent community. In: Kaplan,
between neighbors than on distal social policies. In
B.H., Wilson, R.N., Leighton, A.H. (Eds.), Further
contrast, the level of social capital at the state or
Explorations in Social Psychiatry. Basic Books, New
country level is more likely to re¯ect the in¯uence of York, pp. 195±209.
culture, social and economic policies and other macro- Davidson, W., Cotter, P., 1986. Measurement of sense of
social forces. More theoretical and empirical work is community within the sphere of the city. Journal of
needed to clarify these di€erences. Applied Social Psychology 16, 608±619.
In summary, despite the promise suggested by the Davidson, W., Cotter, P., 1991. The relationship between
title of this paper, more questions have been raised sense of community and subjective well-being: a ®rst look.
than answered. For example, is there added value in Journal of Community Psychology 19, 246±253.
talking about `social capital', over and above the exist- Davidson, W., Cotter, P., 1993. Psychological sense of com-
ing notions of social relations and community cohe- munity and support for public school taxes. American
Journal of Community Psychology 21, 59±66.
sion? Does social capital merely represent `old wine in
Diez-Roux, A.V., Nieto, J., Muntaner, C., Tyroler, H.A.,
new bottles'? At the present time, insucient theoreti-
Comstock, G.W., Shahar, E., Cooper, L.S., Watson, R.L.,
cal and empirical work has been carried out to o€er a Szklo, M., 1997. Neighborhood environments and coron-
de®nitive guide to the measurement of social capital. ary heart disease: a multilevel analysis. American Journal
Still, a variety of evidence from di€erent disciplines of Epidemiology 146, 48±63.
has linked elements of social capital to collective out- Doolittle, R., MacDonald, D., 1978. Communication and a
comes ranging from crime prevention, civic insti- sense of community in a metropolitan neighborhood: a
tutional performance, to population longevity. The factor analytic explanation. Communication Quarterly 26,
potential sweep of this concept in explaining such 2±7.
diverse outcomes is sucient by itself to justify further Durkheim, E., 1897. In: Simpson, G. (Ed.), Suicide. The Free
explorations in this area. Press, New York, p. 1951 (Spaulding, J.A. and Simpson,
G. (transl)).
Eng, E., Parker, E., 1994. Measuring community competence
in the Mississippi Delta: the interface between program
Acknowledgements evaluation and empowerment. Health Education Quarterly
21, 199±220.
I.K. and B.P.K. are recipients of the Robert Wood Glynn, T., 1981. Psychological sense of community: measure-
Johnson Foundation Investigator Awards in Health ment and application. Human Relations 34, 789±818.
Policy Research. Goeppinger, J., Baglioni, A.J., 1985. Community competence:
a positive approach to needs assessment. American
Journal of Community Psychology 13, 507±523.
Haan, M., Kaplan, G.A., Camacho, T., 1987. Poverty and
References health: prospective evidence from the Alameda County
Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 125, 989±998.
Bachrach, K.M., Zautra, A.J., 1985. Coping with a commu- Hagan, J., Merkens, H., Boehnke, K., 1995. Delinquency and
nity stressor: the threat of a hazardous waste facility. disdain: social capital and the control of right-wing extre-
Journal of Health and Social Behavior 26, 127±141. mism among East and West Berlin youth. American
Bandura, A., 1986. Social Foundations of Thought and Journal of Sociology 100, 1028±1052.
Action: a Social Cognitive Theory. Prentice-Hall, Hill, J.L., 1996. Psychological sense of community: sugges-
Englewood Cli€s, NJ. tions for future research. Journal of Community
Barnes, J., Sampson, R.J., Kindlon, D. and Reiss, A.J. Jr Psychology 24, 431±438.
1997. A community survey approach to ecological assess- Hunter, A., 1975. The loss of community: an empirical test
270 K. Lochner et al. / Health & Place 5 (1999) 259±270

through replication. American Sociological Review 40, Putnam, R.D., 1993. Making Democracy Work. Civic
537±552. Traditions in Modern Italy. Princeton University Press,
Julian, D.A., Reisch, T.M., Carrick, R.V., Katrencich, C., Princeton, NJ.
1997. Citizen participation-lessons from a local United Putnam, R.D., 1993b. The prosperous community. Social
Way planning process. Journal of the American Planning capital and public life. The American Prospect 13, 35±42.
Association 63, 345±355. Putnam, R.D., 1995. Bowling alone: America's declining
Kaplan, G.A., Pamuk, E., Lynch, J.W., Cohen, R.D., social capital. Journal of Democracy 6, 65±78.
Balfour, J.L., 1996. Income inequality and mortality in the Riger, S., Lavrakas, P.J., 1981. Community ties: patterns of
United States. British Medical Journal 312, 999±1003. attachment and social interaction in urban neighborhoods.
Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B.P., 1997. Health and social cohesion: American Journal of Community Psychology 9, 55±66.
why care about income inequality? British Medical Journal Robert, S.A., 1998. Community-level socioeconomic status
314, 1037±1040. e€ects on adult health. Journal of Health and Social
Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B.P., Lochner, K. 1997. Long live Behavior 39, 18±37.
community: social capital as public health, The American Roberts, E.M., 1997. Neighborhood social environments and
Prospect, Nov./Dec., pp. 56±59. the distribution of low birthweight in Chicago. American
Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B.P., Lochner, K., Prothrow-Stith, D., Journal of Public Health 87, 597±603.
1997b. Social capital, income inequality and mortality. Sampson, R.J., 1988. Local friendship ties and community
American Journal of Public Health 87, 1491±1498. attachment in mass society: a multilevel systems model.
Kennedy, B.P., Kawachi, I., Prothrow-Stith, D., 1996. Income American Sociological Review 53, 766±779.
distribution and mortality: test of the Robin Hood Index Sampson, R.J., Groves, W.B., 1989. Community structure
in the United States. British Medical Journal 312, 1004± and crime: testing social disorganization theory. American
1007. Journal of Sociology 94, 774±802.
LaVeist, T.A., 1993. Segregation, poverty and empowerment: Sampson, R.J., 1996. The community. In: Wilson, J.Q.,
health consequences for African Americans. The Milbank Petersili, J. (Eds.), Crime. Institute for Contemporary
Quarterly 71, 41±64. Studies, San Francisco, pp. 193±216.
LeClere, F.B., Rogers, R.G., Peters, K.D., 1997. Ethnicity Sampson, R.J., Raudenbush, S.W., Earls, F., 1997.
and mortality in the United States: individual and commu- Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of
nity correlates. Social Forces 76, 169±198. collective ecacy. Science 227, 918±924.
Loury, G. 1992. The economics of discrimination: getting to Sarason, S., 1974. The Psychological Sense of Community.
the core of the problem. Harvard Journal of African Prospects for a Community Psychology. Josey-Bass, San
American Public Policy. Francisco, CA.
Macintyre, S., MacIver, S., Sooman, A., 1993. Area, class Shihadeh, E.S., Flynn, N., 1996. Segregation and crime: the
and health: should we be focusing on places or people? e€ect of black social isolation on the rates of black urban
Journal of Social Policy 22, 213±234. violence. Social Forces 74, 1325±1352.
MacMillan, D.W., Chavis, D.M., 1986. Sense of community: Unger, D., Wandersman, A., 1983. Neighboring and its role
a de®nition and theory. Journal of Community Psychology in block organization: an exploratory report. American
14, 6±23. Journal of Community Psychology 11, 291±300.
Marmot, M., 1998. Improvement of social environment to Unger, D., Wandersmann, A., 1985. The importance of neigh-
improve health. Lancet 351, 57±60. bors: the social cognitive, and a€ective components of
Perkins, D.D., Florin, P., Rich, R.C., Wandersman, A., neighboring. American Journal of Community Psychology
Chavis, D.M., 1990. Participation and the social and 13, 139±169.
physical environment of neighborhood blocks: crime and Verba, S., Lehman Schlozman, K., Brady, H.E., 1995. Voice
community context. American Journal of Community and Equality. Civic Voluntarism in American Politics.
Psychology 18, 83±115. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Polednak, A.P., 1997. Segregation, Poverty and Mortality in Wilkinson, R.G., 1996. Unhealthy Societies. The A‚ictions of
urban African Americans. Oxford University Press, New Inequality. Routledge, London.
York. Wilkinson, R.G., Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B.P. 1998. Mortality,
Pretty, G., Andrews, L., Colett, C., 1994. Exploring adoles- the social environment, crime and violence. Sociology of
cents sense of community and its relationship to lonliness. Health and Illness (in press).
Journal of Community Psychology 22, 346±358. Zaccaro, S.J., Blair, V., Peterson, C., Zazanis, M., 1995.
Pretty, G., Conroy, C., Dugay, J., Fowler, K., 1996. Sense of Collective ecacy. In: Maddux, J.E. (Ed.), Self-Ecacy,
community and its relevance to adolescents of all ages. Adaptation and Adjustment: Theory, Research and
Journal of Community Psychology 24, 365±379. Application. Plenum Press, New York, NY.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen