Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Moral Development, Cultural Differences in

Klaus Helkama and Florencia M Sortheix, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland


Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Abstract

Drawing on current conceptualizations of values (Schwartz’s basic human values theory) and the moral domain (Haidt’s
moral foundations theory; Shweder’s big three of morality), as well as the cognitive developmental approach to morality, we
examine research on cross-cultural differences in the emphases on different parts of the moral domain and in the pathways of
moral development in the light of cultural variations in wealth, relatedness, and hierarchy. We also review studies of age-
related changes in values and justice-oriented reasoning, and examine influences on cultural and individual inclusiveness of
the moral ingroup.

The Moral Domain among more than 34 000 respondents from all over the world
(Graham et al., 2011). Conformity and tradition-related values
Morality has two functions: to promote ideals and justice, and (self-discipline, politeness, honoring of parents and elders;
to maintain social stability. Among the classics in psychology being devout, respect for tradition) are related to the two
and sociology, Piaget and Weber stressed the former, Freud foundations of authority, and purity (Boer and Fischer, 2013),
and Durkheim the latter function. The current dominant possibly also to ingroup (Sverdlik et al., 2012), see Table 1.
definition of the moral domain (Haidt, 2012) includes six Except for benevolence, the moral domain varies across
moral foundations: harm/care (not hurting anybody, caring cultures. Conformity and tradition are more often regarded
for the weak), liberty (freedom from oppression), fairness/ as moral values in less wealthy than in affluent countries,
reciprocity (equal treatment of all), ingroup/loyalty (not whereas universalism values (e.g., broadmindedness, equal-
betraying one’s group, being proud of the history of one’s ity, world peace, social justice, protecting environment) and
country), authority/respect (showing respect for authority), even some self-direction values (freedom) are regarded as
and purity/sanctity (not violating standards of decency). The moral values more often in wealthy individualistic than in
first three foundations are, roughly, related to the promotion poor collectivistic countries (Sverdlik et al., 2012; Vauclair,
function of morality, the three latter to its prevention func- 2010).
tion. Another taxonomy of moral ideologies is Shweder et al.’s Morality is neither sociologically nor psychologically
(1997) ‘big three’: Ethic of autonomy (corresponds to Haidt’s a coherent system. Max Weber pointed out in 1918 that our
care, liberty, and fairness), community (ingroup and conceptions of justice involve a fundamental antinomy or
authority), and divinity (purity). Obviously, the foundations incompatibility, between egalitarianism and merit. Neuro-
could be in conflict, for instance, in a choice between ingroup psychological research shows that we regard it as right to kill
loyalty and honesty. In line with the two opposite functions one person by turning a switch in order to save five people,
of morality, politically conservative people in the United but regard it as wrong to kill one person by pushing him
States emphasize the three foundations related to stability from a bridge to save five persons. The thought of pushing
(loyalty, authority, and purity), whereas liberals stress the another activates the ‘old’ regions in our brain, whereas the
change-oriented harm and fairness foundations. thought of turning the switch activates the ‘new’ parts of the
Values, abstract, general, desirable goals, represent what brain.
is most important in life. They are the foundations based However, there are universally shared moral intuitions, for
on which people live and relate to others (Schwartz, 1992). instance, that dishonesty is wrong. Vauclair and Fischer (2011)
Studies relying on Schwartz’s (1992) value model suggest reviewed responses of more than 200 000 people from 84
that there is also universal agreement that some values are countries to the ‘morally debatable behaviors’ scale, used in the
moral. His model postulates 10 universal values. They can be World Value Survey since 1981. By means of multilevel anal-
described in terms of a circular structure with two dimensions, yses they found that there was very little variation across
self-enhancement – self-transcendence and conservation – cultures or across individuals in the opinion that such
openness to change. Values relate in consistent ways to moral ‘dishonest and illegal’ acts as accepting a bribe were wrong. By
foundations (Boer and Fischer, 2013). Benevolence is regarded contrast, there was a great deal cross-cultural as well as within-
as a moral value in widely different cultures (Vauclair, 2010), culture variation with regard to ‘personal and sexual’ acts like
and is positively associated with all five moral foundations abortion, homosexuality, or divorce.

Table 1 The moral domain in different psychological approaches

Schwartz’s values Self-direction, universalism, benevolence Conformity Tradition


Haidt’s Moral foundations Liberty, fairness, care Ingroup/loyalty, authority/respect Purity
Schweder’s ‘big three’ Autonomy Community Divinity

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edition, Volume 15 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23212-1 781
782 Moral Development, Cultural Differences in

Moral development is here defined as any change in Poortinga (2012) examined 195 regions in Europe
a morally relevant psychological variable that occurs in human comparing the influence of historical factors and current
beings as they age. Culture is here seen as the primary mode of economic situation. They found that contemporary affluence,
human adaptation, which reflects the demands of the ecolog- measured by the gross national product (GNP), was far more
ical, demographic, and institutional environment, as well as important than history in explaining preference for self-
historical experiences. Culture and development are inter- transcendence values (benevolence and universalism). Using
twined in many senses. Culture sets the developmental tasks the third wave of the European Social Survey, we calculated
for individuals and largely defines developmental pathways of correlations of universalism with age. Despite the confounding
development. of cohort effects, correlations were positive in all countries,
A hunting and gathering culture is egalitarian and low in ranging from 0.12 (Denmark) to 0.31 (Russia). Democracies,
conformity, often with benevolent attitudes to outgroups. then, seem to favor the moral developmental trajectory toward
A culture adapted to an agrarian socioeconomic environment universalism values.
is organized by the dimension of relatedness, where the indi- A second consequence of wealth is increasing value
vidual is part of the social system, defined by cooperation, differentiation (Boer and Fischer, 2013). In wealthier and more
acceptance of hierarchy, striving for harmonious relationships. individualistic societies, there is less value consensus. Schwartz
A culture, adapted to an urban socioeconomic postindustrial and Sagie (2000) suggest that with increasing de-
environment, is organized by the dimension of autonomy; the mocratization, value consensus further decreases. This means
individual is self-contained in a social system defined by that a new developmental task emerges: how to solve value
a mixture of competition and cooperation, and plurality of conflicts? That is the focus of Kohlberg’s (1984) cognitive–
values. We examine sociocultural contexts that range from developmental theory of moral judgment.
countries and geographic areas to smaller units such as
professions or families. In characterizing those contexts, the
focus will be on the dimensions of relatedness – expressed Kohlberg’s Theory of Moral Development
by autonomy vs. embeddedness (Schwartz, 2008) – and
hierarchy – egalitarianism (expressed by Schwartz’s and Hof- The cross-cultural studies on Kohlberg’s developmental
stede’s well-known dimensions) as well as economic affluence theory of moral judgment are far more numerous than on any
of the social unit. other approach (see, e.g., Eckensberger and Zimba, 1997).
His basic focus was moral judgment, which is the ability to
reason about value conflicts. He attempted to formulate
Pathways of Moral Development a system of universal moral categories. Kohlberg’s category
system covered, broadly, Haidt’s (2012) six foundations. In
As the moral domain varies cross-culturally, so do pathways of Kohlberg’s Moral Judgment Interview (MJI), two commonly
moral development. This is seen already in infancy. In cultures accepted moral principles clashed in a dilemma, for instance,
where relatedness values (conformity, tradition) are empha- saving a life vs. obeying the law, or authority vs. honesty
sized, socialization goals and parenting strategies of mothers (promise keeping).
for their infants differ from the goals and strategies of mothers Moral development was defined by the justifications
in postindustrial cultural contexts (Keller, 2007). There is (structure) people gave for their choices, not by the choices
no explicit developmental cross-cultural research on the (content). The justifications were rank ordered into stages
authority, ingroup, and purity foundations. Jensen (2011) according to their cognitive sophistication and level of social
advanced a cultural–developmental theory, in which she out- perspective taking. Thus, the criteria of development were more
lined a series of developmental trajectories for the autonomy, stringent than mere age-related change. After a radical revision
community, and divinity in different cultural contexts. She of the scoring system, the final sequence consisted of five
noted that community-related arguments increase with age, as empirical stages (preconventional: (1) heteronomous morality,
shown by the findings that in late adolescence and adulthood, punishment, and obedience to authority; (2) individualism,
there is an increase in moral concepts associated with societal instrumental purpose, and exchange; conventional: (3) mutual
organization, and with what is best for the society. These results interpersonal expectations, relationships, and interpersonal
are found not only in western countries, but also in India and conformity; (4) social system and conscience; and post-
Zambia. Adulthood age trends from representative national conventional: (5) social contract or utility and individual
samples are consistent with this view, as they demonstrate an rights).
increase in the importance of conformity and security values. Although Kohlberg initially thought that development
Wealth has two important consequences for values and would lead to agreement on universal ethical principles at Stage
moral development pathways. First, it increases the likelihood 6, this stage was not found in empirical studies. At the post-
of individualism and democracy (causality is reciprocal). conventional level, there is no value consensus but post-
Inglehart and Welzel (2005) found that the ratio of societal conventional arguments could be employed to defend a variety
changes toward democracy vs. changes toward autocracy for of values. While research has supported many Kohlbergian
the period 1950–90 increased with wealth. In countries with criteria of development, e.g., the hierarchical nature of the
per capita incomes greater than 7000 dollars, changes toward stages, there is more situational variation in the arguments
democracy occurred 28 times as often as the reverse changes. than Kohlberg’s idea of stages as integrated wholes implied.
Even within democracies, wealth increases preference for fair- Therefore, current usage relies on the concept of a schema:
ness and care (self-transcendence values). Van Herk and personal interest schema for Stage 2/3, maintaining norms
Moral Development, Cultural Differences in 783

Table 2 Developmental approaches to morality reasoning. The structural nature of the stages implies that
development is mainly about increasing sophistication, i.e.,
Fairness differentiation and hierarchical integration of moral concepts,
Kohlberg (1984): Stages of moral reasoning
as well as increasing universality, but does not necessarily lead
Focus: Solving value conflicts presented in hypothetical dilemmas
Methods of assessment: to adoption of any definite values.
- Moral Judgment Interview (MJI; Colby and Kohlberg: The measure- The variety of approaches and measures of different aspects
ment of moral judgment, Cambridge University Press, 1987; produc- of moral development are summarized in Table 2. Findings on
tion measure) comprehension and preference formed the basis of Rest’s
- Defining Issues Test (DIT; Rest: Development in judging moral issues, Defining Issues Test (DIT), which measures the preference for
1979: comprehension and preference) DIT-2 (Rest et al., 1999) postconventional schema and has been frequently used in
- Sociomoral Reflection Measure (SRM; Gibbs, Widaman, and Colby, cross-cultural research. Gibbs’s measures, on the other hand,
Child Development, 1982) deal with development up to Stage 4.
- Sociomoral Reflection Measure-Short Form (SRM-SF; Gibbs,
Is there evidence for the Rousseau-derived hypothesis of a
Basinger, and Fuller, Moral maturity: measuring the development of
‘natural’ universal trend toward postconventional Stage 5 judg-
sociomoral reflection, Erlbaum, 1992). (The SRM-SF, unlike the other
instruments, is not based on dilemmas.) ment? Studies carried out with the MJI, where respondents
- Moral Judgment Test (MJT; assessment of moral arguments); Lind produce moral arguments, show that Stage 5 is extremely rare
(www.uni-konstanz.de/ag-moral/lind.htm) (Eckensberger and Zimba, 1997; Snarey, 1985). By contrast,
Care studies that have used the DIT, a comprehension and preference
Gilligan: In a different voice (1982)–Skoe (1998): Ethic of care test, suggest that the preference for postconventional arguments
Focus: Solving dilemmas on care of others vs. care of self (partly own real- increases with age and education in a number of countries (Gielen
life dilemmas) and Markoulis, 2001). The postconventional schema reflects an
Countries: Norway, Canada, Finland (Juujärvi, Myyry, and Pesso, J. Moral outside-society or prior-to-society perspective, as illustrated by
Education, 2010)
such items of the DIT as “would (X) in this case bring about more
Method of assessment: Ethic of Care Interview (Skoe et al. Early
total good for the whole society or not” or “what values are going
Adolescence, 1999)
Eisenberg et al. (Developmental Psychology, 1987): Prosocial reasoning to be the basis of governing how people act toward each other.”
Focus: Reasons for prosocial behavior Social perspective taking has been shown to be the factor
Countries: USA, Brazil (Eisenberg, Zhou, and Koller, Child Development, underlying the progress along the stages of justice reasoning
2001) judgment, irrespective of method of assessment (Gibbs et al.,
Method of assessment: Prosocial dilemma interview 2007). The same holds for the ethic of care. Therefore, when
Community, autonomy, and divinity (the ‘big three’) looking at culture-based differences in Kohlbergian moral
Jensen: Coding manual for the ethics of autonomy, community, and development, the natural question is: What are the dimensions
divinity (2004).www.LeneArnettJensen.com of culture that are related to social perspective-taking oppor-
Focus: Coding moral reasons from moral discourse
tunities and actual perspective taking?
Guerra and Giner-Sorolla: Community, Autonomy, Divinity Scale (CADS),
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 2010
Moral exemplarity
Studying morally outstanding persons who have shown extraordinary Culture-Based Differences in Justice Reasoning
commitments promoting benevolence, justice, or basic human welfare
(Life review interview; List of personal strivings; Frimer, Walker, and The prototypes of Gesellschaft (society) and Gemeinschaft
et al., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2011) (community) have been used as explanatory constructs for
cultural differences in the attainment of higher stages (Snarey,
1985). Gesellschaft, an urban, complex, heterogeneous envi-
schema (Stage 3/4), and postconventional schema (Rest et al., ronment, with a variety of contacts with strangers, supposedly
1999). provides more varied social perspective-taking opportunities
Most importantly, there is empirical support for the than does Gemeinschaft, rural, face-to-face, homogeneous
assumption that higher stage solutions to moral problems are environment, where contacts are mostly with familiar people.
felt to solve those problems in a more balanced and just way. Movement of any ecological variable (e.g., diversity of occu-
When presented with Stage 5 postconventional arguments and pations or formal schooling) in a Gesellschaft direction shifts
Stage 4 conventional arguments, both right-wing and left-wing cultural values toward more individualism and developmental
students in Britain prefer Stage 5 to Stage 4. High scorers are less pathway toward more independent behavior (Greenfield,
self-serving in judging their own and others’ bad deeds and 2009).
more likely to rely on the procedural justice principle of bias However, in addition to a movement toward a larger society
suppression in reporting personal real-life dilemmas (Myyry (Gesellschaft), the cultural value of autonomy vs. embedded-
and Helkama, 2002). However, high scorers are hardly more ness could be related to perspective taking. One prerequisite of
likely to resist temptation or show altruism or moral respon- social perspective taking is the existence of value plurality,
sibility than others. which means increasing likelihood of encountering situations
Kohlberg’s theory conceptualizes environmental influence where different values are represented by different people, i.e.,
in terms of social perspective-taking opportunities (participa- in a sociocognitive conflict. Experimental studies show that
tion in peer groups, decision making in the family, and sense of sociocognitive conflict leads to cognitive development. In
participation in societal institutions of work and politics in autonomous societies individuals are socialized for indepen-
general). These experiences stimulate the growth of justice dent thinking and personal freedom is highly valued. In more
784 Moral Development, Cultural Differences in

embedded societies religion, group norms and social conven- individuals high on justice-oriented reasoning to act demo-
tions govern everyday life giving less freedom for individual cratically, the findings on university students’ moral judgment
choices in moral justice-oriented reasoning. The weaker value development suggest that the causal link goes from a hierar-
consensus in more democratic societies implies that individ- chical culture to individuals.
uals growing up in such societies are more exposed to socio- The US business world is hierarchical, and the business
cognitive value conflicts, so we could expect that they score students score lower than expected on measures of moral
higher on moral judgment tasks. reasoning (Maeda et al., 2009). Also the medical profession is
The role of egalitarian vs. hierarchical cultural values also known to be fairly hierarchical. In contrast to students in other
seems to influence moral development. Hofstede’s power fields of study, European and Canadian medical students do
distance (hierarchy) refers to the extent to which those lower in not appear to progress on moral judgment. This is suggested by
organizational hierarchy regard inequality as natural. Table 3 findings of regression in at least four recent longitudinal
shows that moral judgment development scores tend to be studies, which used different Kohlbergian methods to assess
higher in countries which score lower on power distance. The justice-oriented reasoning. Presumably, for the medical
upper half of Table 3 shows the scores for the highest scoring profession, like other life-and-death occupational groups (fire
(higher middle class) adults in Snarey’s (1985) review of studies brigades, armies), a hierarchical organization is often a natural
using the Moral Judgment Interview, and the bottom half shows one for effective functioning. Solving value conflicts is not
the P% (percentage of postconventional arguments regarded as easily compatible with its functional requirements. However,
most important) scores of freshman samples from different the hierarchical organization of medical profession and its
countries in Gielen and Markoulis’ (2001) review, with consequences on moral reasoning is no natural law, as
Spearman rho ¼ 0.80 (p < .01). The notable exception from the shown by findings of clear progress among medical students
pattern in Table 3 is Ireland, low in power distance but also low in the United States (Maeda et al., 2009). Likewise, business
postconventional morality. This is probably due to the influence school students in Finland obtained high scores on post-
of Catholic Church. conventional moral judgment in the 1980s.
Religious orthodoxy is associated with lower postconven- Over the past decades, inequality has increased almost
tional scores (Rest et al., 1999). Where a high proportion of everywhere in the Western world. The findings that in the
moral rules are seen as absolute, there is less room for value United States, university students’ P% scores have declined,
conflicts. narcissism increased and empathy decreased are consistent
The role of egalitarianism is also seen in smaller cultural with each other in suggesting a weakened disposition of taking
units; e.g., families. Adolescents who participate in decision- into account the feelings and consequences of own actions for
making score higher on the DIT than adolescents in hierar- other people.
chical role–oriented families. Husbands and wives who make Democracies provide a sense of participation in the political
decisions together score higher on the MJI than those who do process, an essential input for the attainment of the system
not. Although these findings could be due to the tendency of perspective and the prior-to-society perspective, on which

Table 3 Moral judgment development indices and power distance

MMS Power distance

A. Kohlberg’s Moral Judgment Interview (MMS); Snarey (1985)


Israel (Kibbutz) 458 13
USA 391 40
Taiwan 370 58
India 363 77

P% Power distance

B. Defining Issues Test (P %; Gielen and Markoulis, 2001, p. 87)


Australia 43.8 36
USA 42.3 40
South Korea 41.5 60
Taiwan 41.4 58
Greece 40.3 60
Poland 39.9 68
Hong Kong 37.9 68
Ireland 34.1 28
Nigeria (West Africa) 30.0 77
Sudan 27.8 80
Kuwait 24.6 80
Egypt 22.6 80

MMS; moral maturity score.


P % ¼ weighted percentage of postconventional arguments ranked among the most important ones.
Moral Development, Cultural Differences in 785

Kohlberg’s Stages 4 and 5 are based. On the other hand, development of moral reasoning could be attributed to the
a substantial proportion of citizens with the Stage 4 and 5 close association of universalism with social perspective taking
systems and prior-to-society perspective seems to be a prereq- and empathy.
uisite of a well-functioning democracy. These perspectives
enable them to see the common good in the midst of
competing values and ingroup as well as religious loyalties. To Moral Inclusiveness, Country-Level,
summarize, Gesellschaft (as opposed to Gemeinschaft), and Individual-Level
autonomy (as opposed to embeddedness), brought about by
sufficient wealth, as well as democracy and egalitarianism in The two ingroup values, benevolence and conformity, while
the sense of low power distance (as opposed to autocracy, motivationally close, relate to perspective taking and empathy
hierarchy, and high power distance) seem to be those dimen- differently. Across eight studies in four countries, benevolence
sions of culture that are conducive to individual development showed consistent associations with both cognitive perspective
of justice-oriented reasoning. taking and emotional empathy, while conformity did not
show any associations (Helkama, 2011). The fact that the
patterns in the associations of benevolence and universalism
Other Developmental Dimensions are very similar – almost identical – could be used to argue that
universalism could be understood as sympathy extended
After Kohlberg, several moral–developmental dimensions have beyond the boundaries of the ingroup, to all people. This idea
emerged (Table 2). Far less cross-cultural research exists on is supported by the notion of the inclusiveness of the moral
these dimensions than on Kohlberg’s justice. An attempt to universe (Schwartz, 2007), which is defined as the extent
build a comprehensive moral–developmental psychology, toward which universalism values are applied to outgroup
relying on neo-Kohlbergian findings and brain research is members. At the country level, inclusiveness increases with
Narvaez’s (2008) Triune ethics. It unites findings from brain egalitarianism and democratization and decreases with
and clinical research with studies of moral emotions, and embeddedness.
singles out three basic ethical mindsets, which represent global Schwartz (2007) operationalized the notion of the inclu-
brain states. Ethic of safety, shared with all animals, is based siveness of moral universe by looking at the separation of
primarily on instincts for survival, and located in the brainstem benevolence and universalism values from a psychometric
and lower limbic system. Ethic of engagement is based on point of view. He took as his starting-point four universalism
mammalian emotional systems that guide people toward values, broadmindedness, equality, social justice, and a world
intimacy, as e.g., in parental care, or play in a peer group. Ethic at peace. If all four items were intermixed with benevolence or
of imagination relies on recently evolved parts of the brain (neo other moral value items, the inclusiveness score was 0, and if all
cortex and prefrontal cortex) and makes possible will power as of them formed a distinct region, then it was 4. Among 66
well as moral problem solving by means of reflection. How countries, democratization was the strongest predictor
much one favors one mindset over another is shaped by (r ¼ 0.73) of inclusiveness. In a sample of 21 European
experience in early life (i.e., caregiving practices) and other countries, inclusiveness of moral universe predicted acceptance
sensitive periods. of immigrants. Moreover, the country inclusiveness moderated
the associations of universalism with acceptance of outgroup
immigrants and participation in prosocial activity, so that the
Moral Values and Development of Justice-Oriented association was stronger in those countries where moral
Reasoning universe was more inclusive, i.e., where universalism values
were understood as applying to members of outgroups, too.
If the structural development of moral judgment does not lead The inclusiveness of an individual’s moral ingroup should
to agreement about the hierarchy of moral principles or moral increase with his or her moral judgment level, if Kohlberg’s
values, then it is natural to ask how value priorities and moral stages follow the Kantian notion of universality, as he assumed.
judgment development are related. Few associations of value Because history provides many examples of members of highly
priorities with Kohlbergian moral development have been developed cultures whose belief in their moral superiority has
published, which is consistent with the idea of the indepen- led them to barbarous acts toward outgroup members, it is
dence of structure (moral stage) and content (value). A review appropriate to ask whether and when high scores on Kohl-
(Helkama, 2011) found that 70% of the 24 significant positive bergian measures of moral judgment predict inclusiveness of
associations of Kohlbergian measures with values were for the moral ingroup. Two studies seem relevant.
universalism values, and the rest were evenly divided between One study (Helkama, 2009) examined opinions about
benevolence and self-direction. development cooperation in relation to Kohlberg’s moral
Conformity values have shown negative correlations with stages at two points of time among ordinary adults in a Finnish
Kohlbergian measures of moral development in educationally rural community. When economy was thriving (1982), moral
homogenous samples. Universalism values are by definition judgment level predicted inclusiveness of the moral universe in
universalistic in the sense of being related to concern and the sense that those who scored higher on moral judgment
protection for the welfare of all people and nature. Benevolence were more likely than low scorers to see contributing to
and conformity are ingroup values, concerned with the welfare development cooperation as a moral duty. However, during
of close ones and restraint from violation of ingroup norms, the massive economic recession in 1993, no association was
respectively. The association of universalism with the found between moral reasoning and inclusiveness of the moral
786 Moral Development, Cultural Differences in

ingroup, defined in terms of the same questions on develop- democracy, and equality. While the weight of conservation
ment cooperation. The inclusiveness of the moral universe in values in morality is reduced, no value consensus on univer-
that specific rural community in 1993 was much narrower than salism (fairness) along the lines of ‘natural’ Rousseau-like
the Finnish average over 1990s. Thus, the relation of moral development is observed in wealthy societies, but increased
judgment development to the inclusiveness of the moral value diversity, also among moral values. ‘Human nature’
ingroup varies as a function of the economic situation. seems equally inclined to believe in ingroup superiority and
The other study (Passini, submitted for publication) ingroup loyalty as in the Golden Rule and universal human
addressed ordinary Italians, who were given Rest’s DIT and rights. The moral–developmental trajectory for citizens in
measures of social distance tapping prejudice toward im- postindustrial democracies is characterized by growth of
migrant groups. There were two kinds of dilemmas on the DIT. In reflective moral problem-solving skills, based on social
some dilemmas, the protagonist was Italian, an ingroup perspective taking; i.e., the developmental trajectory described
member, in others, Romanian, an outgroup member. Overall, by Kohlberg’s stage sequence.
the postconventional score was related to lower prejudice. Moral change follows the route predicted by Bardi’s and
However, respondents with a narrow moral ingroup (i.e., prej- Goodwin’s (2011) theory of value change: With increase in
udiced ones) scored lower on postconventional reasoning when wealth and societal equality, the domain of moral values
the protagonists were Romanian (outgroup members) than in moves along Schwartz’s circle from conformity toward
case they were Italian. By contrast, those respondents with an universalism and even self-direction. In affluent contexts,
inclusive moral ingroup obtained identical P% scores on both ingroup norms, which in less affluent environments are all
types of dilemmas. Inclusiveness of the moral ingroup at the regarded as moral, get differentiated into conventional and
individual level is, accordingly, not reducible to the develop- moral ones. As the increase in wealth makes democracy and
mental level of moral judgment. value plurality more likely, also self-direction values (liberty)
In sum, these studies suggest, first, that the value choices become to be thought of as moral values.
that higher stage moral arguments justify are susceptible to While the above review suggests that moral codes tend to be
prevailing value climate and that the inclusiveness of the rational given the life circumstances (self-discipline is stressed
ingroup may regulate the social perspective-taking process. in conditions of scarcity, universalism in affluent societies,
Categorizing some people as members of the outgroup leads to which can afford it), continuity of traditions is a force that
switching off empathy toward them (Demoulin et al., 2009). is obviously also at work. Wealth does not automatically
Second, they suggest that the inclusiveness of the moral bring about individualistic values and democracy. As Boer
ingroup may vary quickly as a function of the economic situ- and Fischer (2013) show in their review, the increased oppor-
ation. Disease stress as well as economic threat narrow the tunities that arise from wealth only give rise to more choice
boundaries of the ingroup and increase the importance of through a cultural emphasis on autonomy (as op-
conservation values. In the Finnish rural community just posed to embedded and traditional societies) and, we would
mentioned, both local and national identifications increased add, egalitarianism. The possibility of choice (value plurality) is
from 1993 to 2007, simultaneously with the increase in important, but so is the responsibility that egalitarian societies
importance of conservation values (Sortheix et al., 2013). foster in their citizens (see Helkama, 2011).
These Finnish observations, then, suggest that societal With decrease in wealth, increase of threats in the envi-
moral code adapts to economic and other threats by shifting ronment, or increase in inequality, moral change goes to the
toward relatedness values and narrower ingroups. As value opposite direction, as illustrated above: Universalism values
consensus increases, moral judgment in the sense of ability to become less important and conservation values more impor-
solve value conflicts is less needed. tant. Since moral judgment (in the Kantian and Kohlbergian
sense of ability to solve value conflicts) is less required, level of
moral judgment goes down. Possibly, in these contexts the
Conclusions and Future Directions responsibility of making such choices is left to the collective or
the authority.
The concept of moral development, introduced by Piaget and The cross-cultural study of moral development has been
Kohlberg, is an Enlightenment notion, based on common fairly one-sided in many ways. Most of it has been carried out
human nature and equality of all people. The centrality of starting from Kohlberg’s theory. Developmental studies have
benevolence values in morality and increase in reasoning about been restricted to care and fairness. In the future, there is
societal organization with age seem to be universal features. a need for more research using other methods than question-
Piaget and Kohlberg assumed that there is a ‘natural’ trajectory naires or interviews about hypothetical situations. Mixed-
of development, which is only diverted by poor institutions. method approaches that combine qualitative and quantitative
Rousseau’s idea of a free and egalitarian society is consistent approaches will become more common.
with observations on hunter-gatherers, who live in an ecolog- Brain research is one of the most rapidly expanding areas
ical environment where autonomy is needed and equality is in psychology. Integrating results from these studies (like
functional. By contrast, in conditions with high disease stress Narvaez’s (2008) Triune Ethics) with cross-cultural data will
and economic scarcity, hierarchy is functional, and the increase our knowledge of the interaction of affective and
authority, ingroup loyalty, and religious purity (social-stability cognitive factors in moral development as well as of long-term
maintaining) components of morality concur in promoting influence of childhood experiences.
self-discipline. Increase in economic affluence decreases the The accumulation of representative databases from earlier
need for conformity and creates pressure toward individualism, time periods and development of more sophisticated
Moral Development, Cultural Differences in 787

statistical techniques like multilevel modeling make it Helkama, Klaus, 2009. Ingroup and outgroup in morality and moral development. In:
possible to better understand the short-term historical and Jasinskaja-Lahti, Inga, Mähönen, Tuuli-Anna (Eds.), Identities, Intergroup Relations
and Acculturation. Gaudeamus, Helsinki, pp. 137–149.
temporal dynamics of societal and psychological change in
Helkama, Klaus, 2011. Social psychology of morality and moral development:
moral development and, e.g., the interplay of normative and a functional system approach. In: Valentim, Joaquim Pires (Ed.), Societal
value regulation. Approaches in Social Psychology. Peter Lang, Bern, pp. 131–149.
The increased heterogeneity of societies implies increase in Inglehart, Ronald, Welzel, Cristian, 2005. Modernization, Cultural Change, and
the number of people who simultaneously belong to groups Democracy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Jensen, Lene Arnett, 2011. The cultural–developmental psychology of moral devel-
that differ in their values and moral outlooks. As Sverdlik et al. opment. In: Jensen, Lene Arnett (Ed.), Bridging Cultural and Developmental
(2012) point out, one important future direction is to find out Approaches to Psychology. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 3–25.
how membership in multiple ingroups could be translated into Keller, Heidi, 2007. Cultures of Infancy. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
a permissive moral outlook. Kohlberg, Lawrence, 1984. The Psychology of Moral Development. Harper & Row,
New York.
Maeda, Yukiko, Thoma, Steve J., Bebeau, Muriel J., 2009. Understanding the rela-
See also: Behavioral Problems, Effects of Parenting and Family tionship between moral judgment development and individual characteristics: the
Structure On; Defiant Behavior During Adolescence across role of educational contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology 101, 233–247.
Cultures; Defiant Behavior During Infancy and Early Childhood; Myyry, Liisa, Helkama, Klaus, 2002. Moral reasoning and the use of procedural justice
rules in hypothetical and real-life dilemmas. Social Justice Research 15, 373–391.
Empathy During Early Childhood Across Cultures,
Narvaez, Darcia, 2008. Triune ethics: the neurobiological roots of our multiple
Development of; Moral Development, Theories of; Parenting moralities. New Ideas in Psychology 26, 95–119.
Attitudes and Beliefs across Cultures; Prosocial Behavior, Passini, Stefano. The Effect of the Group Membership of the Person to be Judged on
Effects of Parenting and Family Structure On; Self-Regulation Moral Reasoning. Manuscript, University of Bologna, submitted for publication.
During Early Childhood Across Cultures, Development of; Rest, James, Narvaez, Darcia, Bebeau, Muriel, Thoma, Steve, 1999. Post-conventional
Moral Thinking. A Neo-Kohlbergian Approach. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Values Across Cultures, Development of. Schwartz, Shalom H., 1992. Universals in the content and structure of values: theo-
retical, advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In: Zanna, M.P. (Ed.),
Advances in Experimental Social, Psychology, vol. 25. Academic Press, San Diego,
CA, pp. 1–65.
Schwartz, Shalom H., 2007. Universalism values and the inclusiveness of our moral
Bibliography universe. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 38, 711–728.
Schwartz, Shalom H., 2008. Cultural Value Orientations: Nature and Implications of
Bardi, Anat, Goodwin, Robin, 2011. The dual route to value change: individual processes National Differences. Publishing House of SU HSE, Moscow.
and cultural moderators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42, 271–287. Schwartz, Shalom H., Sagie, Galit, 2000. Value consensus and importance: a cross-
Boer, Diana, Fischer, Ronald, 2013. How and when do personal values guide our national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 31, 465–497.
attitudes and sociality? Explaining cross-cultural variability in attitude-value link- Shweder, Richard, Much, Nancy, Mahapatra, Manmohan, Park, Lawrence, 1997. The
ages. Psychological Bulletin. “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “big three”
Demoulin, Stephane, Leyens, Jacques-Philippe, Dovidio, John (Eds.), 2009. Intergroup explanations of suffering. In: Brandt, A., Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and Health.
Misunderstandings: Impacts of Divergent Social Realities. Psychology Press, Routledge, New York, pp. 119–169.
London. Snarey, John, 1985. Cross-cultural universality of social–moral development: a critical
Eckensberger, Lutz, Zimba, Roderick, 1997. The development of moral judgment. In: review of Kohlbergian research. Psychological Bulletin 97, 202–232.
Berry, John, Dasen, Pierre, Saraswathi, T.S. (Eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Sortheix, Florencia, Olakivi, Antero, Helkama, Klaus, 2013. Values, life events and
Psychology, vol. 2. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, pp. 299–338. health: a study in a rural Finnish community. Journal of Community and Applied
Gibbs, J.C., Basinger, K.S., Grime, R.L., Snarey, J.R., 2007. Moral judgment devel- Social Psychology 23 (4), 331–346.
opment across cultures: revisiting Kohlberg’s universality claims. Developmental Sverdlik, Noga, Roccas, Sonia, Sagiv, Lilach, 2012. Morality across cultures: a values
Review 27, 443–500. perspective. In: Miculincer, Mario, Shaver, Phillip R. (Eds.), The Social Psychology
Gielen, Uwe P., Markoulis, Diomedes C., 2001. Preference for principled moral of Morality. APA, Washington, DC, pp. 219–235.
reasoning: a developmental and cross-cultural perspective. In: Adler, Leonore Van Herk, Hester, Poortinga, Ype H., 2012. Current and historical antecedents of
Loeb, Gielen, Uwe P. (Eds.), Cross-Cultural Topics in Psychology. Praeger, individual value differences across 195 regions in Europe. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Westport, CT, pp. 81–101. Psychology 43 (8), 1229–1248.
Graham, Jesse, Nosek, Brian A., Haidt, Jonathan, Iyer, Ravi, Koleva, Spassena, Vauclair, Christine-Melanie, 2010. What Is Right? What Is Wrong? And Does the
Ditto, Peter H., 2011. Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Answer Tell Us Something about Culture? An Investigation of the Culture and
Psychology 101, 366–385. Morality Using the Values, Concept. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Victoria
Greenfield, Patricia, 2009. Linking social change and developmental change: shifting University of Wellington, NZ.
pathways of human development. Developmental Psychology 45, 401–418. Vauclair, Christine-Melanie, Fischer, Ronald, 2011. Do cultural values predict indi-
Haidt, Jonathan, 2012. The Righteous Mind. Why Good People Are Divided by Politics viduals’ moral attitudes? A cross-cultural multi-level approach. European Journal of
and Religion. Allen Lane, London. Social Psychology 41, 645–657.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen