Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

NEUROSCIENCE AND

BIOBEHAVIORAL
REVIEWS

PERGAMON Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309


www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev
Review

Micro-nutrient supplementation and the intelligence of children


D. Benton*
Department of Psychology, University of Wales Swansea, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK
Revised 16 March 2001; accepted 19 March 2001

Abstract
A growing number of double-blind placebo-controlled studies have considered the in¯uence of micro-nutrient supplementation on the
intelligence of children. Earlier studies prevented the drawing of conclusions as they did not systematically approach the topic. However,
over the last 10 years, a series of studies have compared the impact of supplementation on either verbal or non-verbal measures of
intelligence. In 10 out of 13 studies a positive response has been reported, always with non-verbal measures, in at least a sub-section of
the experimental sample. A selective response to non-verbal tests was predicted as they re¯ect basic biologically functioning that could be
expected to be in¯uenced by diet. The evidence is that not all children respond to supplementation, rather there is a minority who bene®t,
whose diet offers low amounts of micro-nutrients. Such observations are consistent with dietary surveys that typically report a sub-set of
children with a low intake. The topic is at a very early stage and needs the clari®cation gained from a series of large-scale studies that consider
children of a wide range of ages, dietary styles and social backgrounds. q 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Intelligence; Iron; Micro-nutrients; Minerals; Non-verbal intelligence; Vitamins

Contents
1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 297
1.1. Earlier studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 297
1.2. Iron status . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 299
1.3. Non-verbal intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 300
2. Evaluation of vitamin supplementation and intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 301
3. Does nutritional status in¯uence the response to supplementation? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 302
4. The composition of the micro-nutrient supplement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 303
5. The diet of British school-children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 304
6. Micro-nutrient intake and neural functioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 305
7. Changes in intelligence over time . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 306
8. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 307
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 308

1. Introduction suggestions that vitamin supplementation may improve


problems such as hyperactivity [19], autism [60] and
When the suggestion that the intelligence of children may Downs Syndrome [38] have not been reviewed, rather
be susceptible to vitamin/mineral supplementation is samples of the general population are considered.
considered, we can distinguish two stages of interest. Initi-
ally isolated reports considered the in¯uence of vitamin 1.1. Earlier studies
supplements on intelligence. Over the last 10 years, more
speci®cally, the possibility that supplementation selectively Table 1 summarizes some earlier studies and outlines
in¯uences non-verbal intelligence has been studied. The both the methodology and outcomes. As a group, these
early studies are dif®cult to evaluate. The supplements
* Tel.: 144-1792-295607; fax: 144-1792-295679. used were never the same twice, so the nature of the active
E-mail address: d.benton@swansea.ac.uk (D. Benton). ingredients has not been established. The psychological
0149-7634/01/$ - see front matter q 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0149-763 4(01)00015-X
298

Table 1
Early studies of the impact of micro-nutrient supplementation on intelligence

Study Double blind Active supplement Duration Number active/placebo Subjects Outcome

Harrell [37] Yes 2 mg Thiamine 1 year 55/55 Children in orphanage After thiamine taller, better
9±19 years memory, eyesight and
intelligence
Harrell et al. [39] Yes 1. Vitamin C 1 year Norfolk Offspring of mothers given IQ higher at age 3 and 4
2. Thiamine ribo¯avin niacin Age 3: 385/133 supplements during pregnancy
3. Thiamine Age 4: 448/163 and lactation
Kentucky
Age 3: 607/204 IQ non-signi®cant
Kubala and Katz [42] ? Orange juice each day 6 months 46 matched pairs low/high 9±14 years If initial vitamin C status poor IQ
vitamin C status increased signi®cantly

Boggs et al. [14] Yes Multi-vitamin/minerals 1 month 6 cross-over design 4.5 years IQ , 80 Better behaviour
Trend for increased IQ

Mora et al. [51] No Food plus iron 1 year 116/76 no supplement Malnourished Colombians SupplementÐIQ 1 6.5
4±30 months No supplement IQ 1 2.1

Maeseck [50] No Multi-vitamins or candies Half school year 11/11 10 years with reading problems Vitamins increased IQ 6.7 points
Waber et al. [74] No Vitamins/minerals to mother 3 years 174 supplemented for Malnourished Colombians 3 Improved speech and hand-eye
Iron to children various periods/54 not months prior to birth to 3 years co-ordination
Food to both supplemented
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

Aukett et al. [1] Yes Iron alone or with vitamin C 2 months 97 in total English inner-city children Increased developmental rate
17±19 months
Colgan and Colgan [19] Yes Individually designed 5 months 16/16 5±15 years reading and IQ increased 9.5 points more in
supplement vs standard behaviour problems experimental group
supplement
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 299

tests varied from study to study making comparison dif®- iron, they have not been independently examined in these
cult. The methodological quality of these early studies subject groups.
varied and in some instances failed to achieve the standards
demanded today. As the studies do not re¯ect a coherent 1.2. Iron status
body of research, that has considered the same question
systematically, it is impossible to draw conclusions. Usually A study of malnourished Colombians found that an
estimates of the subjects' diet were missing and it is unclear improved diet and iron supplement increased intelligence
whether their nutritional status was signi®cantly worse than scores [51]. It is possible that iron played a role in these
found typically in industrialized societies today. The most ®ndings as iron de®ciency anaemia is the world's most
that can be said of such data is that they are positive, common single nutrient de®ciency. A representative study
suggesting that the topic should be further considered. [1] of the impact of iron supplementation is included in
However, the generally signi®cant ®ndings probably create Table 1. However, as the in¯uence of iron status has been
too positive an impression. It is impossible to know, over the reviewed frequently [13,45,47,56,57,59] only general
years, how many negative ®ndings have simply not been observations will be made. In developing countries an asso-
published. ciation has been reported repeatedly between poor iron
It is worth drawing attention to the better of these earlier status, poor performance on tests of intellectual ability and
studies. In a double-blind study, 2 mg of thiamine were school performance. ªIron de®ciency affects selective learn-
administered each day for a year, to children who lived ing aptitudes required in the classroom, such as concentra-
together in an orphanage eating the same diet [37]. Analysis tion and the appropriate selection of informationº [56].
of the diet, based on the recommended daily amount (RDA), Iron de®ciency is also a common nutritional problem in
suggested that thiamine supplementation was not needed. industrialized countries. In the USA, among poor African-
However, supplementation resulted in taller children who American and Hispanic babies, the rate of anaemia is as
had better eye-sight, memory and intelligence. Another high as 20±24% [58]. A North American sample of adoles-
study matched children for socio-economic background cents, from poor backgrounds, had a poorer cognitive
who differed in terms of the vitamin C levels in the blood performance if they were anaemic [75]. As it is easy to
[42]. A glass of orange juice a day for 6 months increased suggest an association between poverty, iron status and a
the IQ of those whose vitamin C status had been initially range of other developmental risk factors, the causal
low, whereas those with an adequate status did not respond. mechanism in such correlational studies is dif®cult to estab-
The importance of an index of nutritional status is illu- lish. However, the possibility that low iron status has devel-
strated by a consideration of pregnant women who attended opmental signi®cance was illustrated by a double-blind
a clinic in a slum area of Norfolk, and were given either intervention in Birmingham, UK, where the iron supple-
vitamin supplements, or a placebo, during pregnancy and mentation of anaemic infants resulted in a greater weight
lactation [39]. At 3- and 4-years-of-age the children of the gain and rate of development [1]. However, in older British
mothers who received the vitamins scored signi®cantly children there was no evidence of a general association
better on an intelligence test. The response was greater in between iron-status and performance on intelligence tests
those who received a mixture of thiamine, ribo¯avin, niaci- [53,69]. Such ®ndings are not surprising as you would only
namide and iron, rather than thiamine or ascorbic acid alone. expect a problem in a sub-group who are iron de®cient. In
However, a group of Kentucky mountain-people did not fact, in a British study, the intelligence scores of only those
respond similarly. The authors wondered if the difference with low iron reserves increased following iron supplemen-
in response re¯ected dietary factors. The Kentucky sample tation [49].
raised their own meat and produced their own fruit and The long-term implications of iron-de®ciency have been
vegetables, whereas those in Norfolk relied on shops for little considered, although there is a suggestion that iron-
their food. Such inconsistencies illustrate the dif®culty in de®ciency anaemia has a long-lasting adverse effect. When
drawing conclusions, and making generalizations, without a group of Costa Rican children with a good haematological
assessing diet. status were considered at age 5, those who in infancy had
As the in¯uence of nutritional supplementation on the been anaemic, performed more poorly on tests of mental and
malnourished in developing countries has been reviewed motor functioning [46]. Although the children who were
previously [2,56], only representative studies [51,74] that anaemic in infancy tended to come from poorer homes,
have included micro-nutrients are included in Table 1. A when such factors were controlled, the association between
review of the topic concluded that the major studies had ªall early anaemia and depressed intellectual performance at a
demonstrated signi®cant effects of proteinÐenergy supple- later stage remained.
mentation on behavioural developmentº [2]. In those suffer- The impact of iron de®ciency may differ with age.
ing from protein-calorie malnutrition, it is probable that Although there is evidence of adverse in¯uences of iron
macro-nutrients play a substantial role in enhancing de®ciency in infancy [1], other stages, such as the growth
behavioural development. However, a role for micro-nutri- spurt of puberty, should be examined. Those with particular
ents cannot be excluded, although with the exception of dietary styles, for example dieting or consuming a
300 D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

vegetarian diet, should be speci®cally considered as they are non-verbal, but not verbal measures of intelligence [62].
known to be associated with poorer iron status [32,61]. Thus two studies, without knowledge of each others ®nding,
Females beginning to menstruate may require more iron had produced the same result.
in the diet. Although the required replication was available immedi-
Interpretation of the ®ndings in this area is limited by ately, two out of the next three studies reported negative
methodological considerations, ideally we need two groups ®ndings (Table 2). In a study in London no effect on intelli-
that are similar in all respects other than iron status, an gence was found [53], although the methodology has been
outcome that is dif®cult to achieve as one risk factor is extensively criticised as it differed from the original in many
often correlated with another. The generalization of data ways [17,30,36,55]. One reviewer commented that this
from the developing world [44,56±58] to industrialized study only added to the confusion [55]. Carroll [17] ques-
countries should be made with caution. Poor iron status in tioned whether the intelligence tests used had the necessary
developing countries is typically associated with poorer validity, whether they measured intelligence, and whether
nutrition in general; it can also be associated with a less they were sensitive to small changes.
stimulating environment that lacks the potential to amelio- In contrast, a study in Dundee [22,70] went to some trou-
rate the impact of dietary problems. ble to replicate the original design [12] and produced a
Given the good evidence that iron de®ciency is common small, but non-signi®cant, advantage on the non-verbal
in industrialized societies [32,56,58] it is surprising that the test following supplementation. Although broadly similar
in¯uence of iron status on intellectual functioning has been to the original study, problems with the Dundee study
little considered. If there is a single nutrient whose de®- included a failure to independently consider boys and
ciency in¯uences cognition, then iron is a powerful candi- girls. Eysenck and Schoenthaler [29] pointed out that the
date. It is likely that the effect of iron de®ciency will prove Dundee study assigned subjects to treatment groups on the
to be more subtle in industrialized countries, given that it is basis of verbal rather than non-verbal scores and commen-
more likely to occur in individuals who are generally better ted on various statistical short-comings.
nourished and will often have the bene®ts of a more stimu- At the same time a Belgian study gave school-children
lating environment. either a supplement or placebo for 5 months [7]. Although
not all children responded, boys who ate diets supplying
1.3. Non-verbal intelligence fewer vitamins and minerals scored signi®cantly better on
a non-verbal intelligence test following supplementation.
More recently, a series of studies have considered supple- The implication that only children who consume a poor
mentation in a more systematic manner. Renewed interest in diet respond to supplementation is further considered below.
the topic was stimulated by the report that the non-verbal By far the largest study on this topic administered 23
intelligence of 12-year-old Welsh children was increased by micro-nutrients at 50, 100 or 200% of the RDA [63].
multi-vitamin/mineral supplementation [12]. Several Again, it was the non-verbal rather than verbal intelligence
attempts to replicate the ®ndings have been reported. scores of those taking the active supplement that signi®-
Although some of the resulting data are negative, it can be cantly improved, but only when the 100% RDA tablets
argued that a consistent picture is emerging. were taken. The ®nding that only those receiving 100%
The Benton and Roberts paper [12] stimulated an acrimo- RDA bene®ted from supplementation detracted greatly
nious response [42]. The vast majority of the responses from the credibility of the ®ndings. If children did not
concerned not the clinical trial, but a 3-day dietary diary respond to 50% RDA then one might assume that the dose
that was used to estimate the dietary status of the subjects. was insuf®cient. However, if 100% was suf®cient then
There are many problems in measuring dietary intake [43] 200% RDA must also offer a suf®cient supply. Why then
that were accepted by the authors [4]. However, Southon didn't the children respond to 200% RDA? The 200% dose
[67] contrasted the estimates of micro-nutrient intake in the was not suf®cient to expect a toxic reaction so the data
original study with a 7-day weighted intake of the diet of appeared inconsistent. In the event, as discussed below, it
children of a similar age [26]. She commented that the transpired that those taking 100% RDA were less well nour-
patterns of data in the two studies were not too dissimilar. ished. Todman [70] commented that in this study multiple
Essentially there was a small-scale trial, that used the tests may have produced Type 1 errors, and that data
accepted double-blind procedure [12], that produced an concerning some intelligence tests had been ignored,
unexpected ®nding. As the ®nding questioned the received while relying on results with the Wechsler Intelligence
wisdom of many nutritionists, that children receive an Scale for Children (WISC). In answer the authors claimed
adequate intake of micro-nutrients, it was greeted with skep- that the WISC had been, a priori, the test of choice, as it was
ticism. Benton and Roberts [12] were cautious and the most reliable available measure.
concluded, ªClearly the study must now be replicated ¼º Of the 13 studies that have examined the in¯uence of
Although not published until a later date, at the same time supplementation on non-verbal of intelligence, 10 reported
a similar study was being carried out in California. The a positive in¯uence in at least a sub-set of children. To save
supplementation of delinquent juveniles again increased space the more recent studies are not discussed in detail but
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 301

Table 2
The effect of vitamin/mineral supplementation of children on non-verbal and verbal measures of intelligence. Only study [41] is not a double-blind placebo
controlled trial. The composition of the supplements is given in Table 3. The studies used multi-vitamins/minerals with the exception of Lynn and Harland [49]
who provided only iron and ascorbic acid. Various individually or group presented standard intelligence tests were used. The exception was the use of visual
and auditory reaction times in study [41]. Reaction times are known to correlate with non-verbal measures of intelligence

Study Age Sample placebo/active Superiority of active treatment over placebo

Non-verbal scores Verbal scores

Benton and Roberts [12] 12±13 30/30 17.2 P , 0.001 11.5 n.s.
Nelson et al. [53] 7±12 105/105 13.3 n.s 21.2 n.s.
Crombie et al. [21] 11±13 44/42 12.4 n.s. 10.5 n.s
21.5 n.s
Benton and Buts [7] 13 87/80 Better diet 11.6 n.s. 20.2 n.s.
Poorer diet 13.0 P , 0.02
Schoenthaler et al. [62] 13±16 11/15 17.4 P , 0.05 Data not reported but n.s.
Benton and Cook [8] 6 22/22 19.6 P , 0.001 17.9 n.s.
Schoenthaler et al. [63] 12±16 /105 RDA
100/105 11.0 (50%) n.s 21.1 n.s
/105 13.6 (100%) P , 0.01 20.2 n.s.
11.4 (200%) n.s 22.6 n.s.
Nidich et al. [54] 8±9 16/18 14.9 P , 0.04 N.A.
Lynn and Harland [49] 12±16 28/42 Ferritin .12 ng/ml 10.70, N.A.
n.s.
177/166 Ferritin .12 ng/ml n.s

Southon et al. [69] 13±14 29/22 10.3 n.s. 11.2 n.s


Snowden [66] 12 7/11 19.2 P , 0.02 14.5 n.s.
Kerimova and Aleskerova [41] 6 20/20 Signi®cantly faster reaction N.A.
times after supplementation
Schoenthaler et al. [64] 6±12 120/125 12.5 P , 0.04 N.A.

are summarized in Table 2. Others [29] have commented in verbal measures. A possible answer could be that measures
detail about the design of some of these additional studies. of the speed of information processing, rather than acquired
Although most are similar in nature to the earlier studies of knowledge, is susceptible to increased motivation/effort.
non-verbal intelligence, one used a herbal source of vita- Although only a minority of the studies on this topic
mins [54] and did not report the doses administered. One reported such data, some asked subjects whether they
study was carried out in Azerbaijan using children whose believed they were taking the placebo or active supplement.
diet offered 25±60% of the recommended levels of micro- The study in Dundee [22] found that, of those actually
nutrients [41]. As such, the diet was poorer than that taking the active supplement, 13 believed that it was active
consumed by the vast majority of children in Western socie- and 23 thought that it was the placebo. Of those actually
ties and the data should be generalized with caution. taking the placebo 12 believed that they were taking the
active tablet and 24 the placebo. Clearly this study was
blind. Although the original study [12] failed to collect
2. Evaluation of vitamin supplementation and similar data, as they gave exactly the same supplements as
intelligence in the Scottish study [22], it is reasonable to suggest that the
children were similarly unable to distinguish the tablets. In
A worry was expressed that the nature of the supplements studies of related topics older and better informed subjects
may have allowed some subjects to guess whether they were have failed to distinguish multi-vitamins from a placebo
taking the active or placebo tablets. This is a potential [10].
problem as thiamine has a yeast-like ¯avour and ribo¯avin Twelve out of the 13 studies in Table 2 were double-
will turn urine more yellow than normal. Thus, it is possible blind, placebo-controlled, and allocated subjects randomly
that the active treatment had become obvious and a placebo to treatment conditions. In 10 of the 13 studies listed in
response generated. The question arises as to why a placebo Table 2, in at least a sub-set of children, supplementation
response would selectively in¯uence non-verbal but not resulted in improved non-verbal scores. In all studies the
302 D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

active supplement produced a greater response than the responded to supplementation in the Belgian study [7]. The
placebo, although it did not always reach statistical signi®- authors of the Scottish study commented that the poor nutri-
cance. If there was no bene®t from supplementation, by tional status of the children who responded in the Belgian
chance you would expect the placebo to have produced a study means the ®ndings have little important for general
greater response on 50% of the occasions. The pattern of public health [20]. It is a matter of judgement whether 6% of
response does not appear to be random. the population is unimportant; it should be considered whether
In contrast, on no occasion were verbal measures signi®- dietary problems that occur rarely may be concentrated in
cantly increased. Whereas on ®ve occasions the supplement speci®c sections of the population.
produced a greater response, on six occasions the placebo Although some popular books have implied that if too
resulted in higher scores. There is a very strong impression many of your calories come in the form of re®ned sugar
that verbal and non-verbal responses differed. This selective then your diet might be de®cient in micro-nutrients, the
impact on non-verbal scores is of theoretical importance levels of minerals and vitamins in the body are more closely
[12,18,63] and makes these data dif®cult to dismiss. related to total energy intake than sugar intake. However, a
Cattell [18] argued that there are two types of intelligence problem can occur if you consume a low-calorie diet with a
that he labelled `¯uid' and `crystallized'. Fluid intelligence high proportion of calories in the form of sugar, a conclu-
re¯ects basic problem solving and reasoning power. It sion based on the diet of adults [28].
allows deduction and an understanding about the relation- A child with a poor appetite, who favoured sweet tasting
ships between ideas. `Fluid' intelligence was viewed as foods, might have a particular problem, although the topic
being closely associated with innate, biological potential: has been largely ignored. In a Welsh study a crude measure
it can be estimated using non-verbal tests. Crystallized intel- of sugar intake was obtained by asking the parents of 6-year-
ligence, in contrast, relies on speci®c information and voca- olds to report the frequency that particular foods were eaten
bulary. It is a re¯ection of experience and environment and [8]. The more a child's diet contained sugar, the greater was
can be measured using verbally based intelligence tests. the response to vitamin/mineral supplements. It was the
Within this framework you would predict that any supple- children with high amounts of sugar in their diet who
ment induced improvement in brain biochemistry would responded to supplementation with improved non-verbal
stimulate non-verbal rather than verbal measures. The intelligence scores. Did those children with a high sugar
predicted pattern has been observed (Table 2). intake have a low vitamin intake? It is not known but it is
a suggestion that should be considered in future studies.
The recording of a dietary diary is a procedure known to
3. Does nutritional status in¯uence the response to be associated with many methodological problems and the
supplementation? measurement of blood samples is known to offer more valid
indices of vitamin status. Children were distinguished
Although Table 2 presents a body of positive data, there whose levels of blood micro-nutrients had, or had not,
are inconsistencies that need to be considered. It is clear that increased after micro-nutrient supplementation [62]. It was
no generalization about the impact of supplementation assumed that if the levels in the blood had not increased this
would apply to all children. An obvious suggestion is that indicated good vitamin status. If levels increased following
where inconsistencies exist they re¯ect a sub-group of chil- supplementation then the initial vitamin status must have
dren, whose diet offers insuf®cient vitamins and minerals, been poor. The non-verbal intelligence of those whose
that bene®t from supplementation. Did those who responded blood micro-nutrients improved increased markedly [62],
have a poor diet? Did those who did not respond have a although the lack of detail of the biochemical assessments
better diet? makes the contribution of this paper dif®cult to establish.
A Belgian study recorded what children ate for 15 days. Much of the controversy associated with the largest study
About a third of the sample was selected, who consumed [63] re¯ected the ®nding that an improvement in non-verbal
less than 50% of the RDA of ®ve vitamins or minerals. The intelligence was associated with the taking of the 100%
non-verbal intelligence of this group of poorly nourished RDA tablets but not those offering either 50 or 200%. The
boys increased after taking a supplement [7]. The majority answer became apparent after several years. When frozen
of those who responded to supplementation came from blood samples were analyzed [29], the children who had
schools for the less academically able, in less economically responded to supplementation with changes in non-verbal
privileged areas. The majority who responded to supple- intelligence were found to have had signi®cantly greater
mentation came from two out of the seven schools that increases in micro-nutrient status. The levels of blood
were studied. Clearly the choice of subjects was critical. micro-nutrients can only rise if the bodies stores are not
The boys who responded in the Belgian Study had a signif- already full, that is you are poorly nourished. Importantly
icantly lower intake of calcium, iron, zinc, copper, and vita- the proportion of children who initially had low levels of
mins A, B1, B2, B6 and C than the boys who failed to respond in vitamins in their body differed between the groups who had
the London study [53]. Similarly the diets of only 6% of the received 50, 100 and 200% of the RDA [63]. The reason that
children in the Scottish study [21] were as poor as those who the children responded to 100% of the RDA was that, by
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 303

Table 3
The composition of supplements used in studies of intelligence

Studies

a,c,i a b d e fb g h j

Retinol equivalents (mg) 375 1000 5000 IU 5000 IU 5000 IU 300 0 750
Thiamine (mg) 3.9 2.2 1.5 4.5 1.5 0.7 1 0.75
Ribo¯avin (mg) 5 2.8 1.7 5.1 1.7 0.9 1.4 0.85
Nicotinic acid (mg) 50 24 20 60 20 10 16 10
Pyrodoxine (mg) 12 2 2 6 2 1.3 1.8 1
Vitamin B12 (mg) 10 5 6 18 6 2.5 3 3
Folate (mg) 100 450 400 400 400 200 400 200
Biotin (mg) 100 100 0 300 300 10 100 0
Pantothenic acid (mg) 50 50 0 30 10 25 4 5
Ascorbic acid (mg) 500 50 60 120 60 70 25 40
Vitamin D (mg) 3 15 400 IU 200 IU 400 IU 10 10 5
Vitamin E (mg) 70 IU 10 15 IU 30 IU 30 IU 6 0 20
Vitamin K (mg) 100 100 0 0 50 100 0 0

Calcium (mg) 100 100 1.6 122 200 30 0 0


Chromium (mg) 200 200 0 100 100 100 50 50
Copper (mg) 0 2 2 2 2 0.03 2 1
Iron (mg) 1.3 15 18 18 18 2.4 12 9
Iodine (ug) 50 100 0 0 150 50 150 0
Magnesium (mg) 7.6 25 25 59 80 8 0 0
Manganese (mg) 1.5 1.5 1 5 2.5 1 2.5 1.25
Molybdenum (mg) 100 0 0 50 250 100 0 0.12
Phosphorus (mg) 0 0 0 122 0 0 0 0
Selenium (mg) 0 150 0 100 100 0 50 50
Zinc (mg) 10 15 10 15 15 4 15 7.5

Bio¯avanoids (mg) 50 0 0 0 0 20 0 0
Choline (mg) 70 0 0 40 0 35 0 0
Inositol (mg) 30 0 0 40 0 15 0 0
Para-aminobenzoic acid (mg) 10 0 0 50 0 5 0 0
a
Study (a) Benton and Roberts [12]; (b) Nelson et al. [53]; (c) Crombie et al. [21]; (d) Benton and Buts [7]; (e) Schoenthaler et al. [62]; (f) Schoenthaler et al.
[63]; (g) Benton and Cook [8]; (h) Southam [68]; (i) Snowdon [66]; (j) Schoenthaler et al. [64]).
b
Study used 50, 100 and 200% of RDA. Data for 100% RDA presented.

chance, they were more poorly nourished. The vitamin both in the substances they offer and in the doses. Some
status of those children responding to the 100% RDA supplements included substances other than vitamins or
were used to distinguish children in the 50 and 200% minerals. The fact that only three of the studies have used
RDA groups who were poorly nourished. When this was the same preparation has attracted adverse comment [55].
done, it was found that those with a poor vitamin status in There are, however, also advantages to using varied supple-
the 50 and 200% RDA groups responded to supplementa- ments, as a comparison of the compositions offer some
tion with increased non-verbal scores. Those who were preliminary indications as to which nutrients are essential
well-nourished did not respond. (Table 3).
The data relating nutrient status to the response to supple- A positive response has been reported with supplements
mentation are limited, however, they suggest an approach with that did not contain calcium, chromium, copper, iodine,
the potential to account for the inconsistencies in this area. magnesium, molybdenum, phosphorus or selenium. In
contrast iron, manganese and zinc are common to the
studies that report a positive response. The presence of the
4. The composition of the micro-nutrient supplement bio¯avinoids, choline, inositol and para-aminobenzoic acid
in a minority of the supplements was not critical. In contrast
From a scienti®c perspective there was much to be gained a wide range of vitamins were present in all the supplements
by keeping the composition of the original supplement that stimulated non-verbal intelligence, including the B vita-
constant, as this prevents any negative data being explained mins and vitamins A, D and E. In contrast Biotin, Pantothe-
in terms of differences in the nature and dose of the micro- nic Acid and Vitamin K were not essential.
nutrients offered. Table 3 lists the supplements used in the Benton and Roberts [12] argued that the level of iron in
studies of non-verbal intelligence. The supplements varied their supplement was so low (1.3 mg) that it was unlikely to
304 D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

be the active ingredient. However, Lynn and Harland [49] quoting dietary surveys, Naismith [52] stated ªI would be
later found that a much higher dose of iron (17 mg) stimu- surprised if mental ability could be affected by diet ¼ in my
lated non-verbal intelligence, but only in those with poor opinion the majority of school children now-a-days are well
iron status as judged by ferritin status (17% of the sample). nourishedº.
Although this study adds to the growing evidence that a poor It is apparent from these quotes that there was a wide-
iron status disrupts cognition, the low level of iron in many spread belief amongst professional nutritionists that chil-
of the supplements (Table 3) makes it is unlikely that iron dren consume adequate levels of micro-nutrients. Can it
accounts for the stimulation of non-verbal intelligence. really be stated with total con®dence that all children are
Previously it has been reported that iron status correlates well fed?
with performance on the Block Design Test of the Wechsler The demonstration, in a double-blind trial, that the giving of
Adult Intelligence Scale and spatial tests, but not Ravens vitamin/mineral supplements improved functioning, caused
Progressive Matrices [31]. These ®ndings suggested that problems for many nutritionists as they believed that it
iron status in¯uenced aspects of spatial ability rather than con¯icted with received wisdom. However, the problem was
`¯uid' intelligence. more apparent than real. There was no reason to believe that
These comments about the role of particular nutrients are judgements based on the recording of food-intake and the
extremely preliminary. It may well be that given the wide- calculation of the RDA (or Dietary Reference Values) would
spread physiological functions that rely on an adequate tell us anything about intellectual performance.
supply of micro-nutrients, many vitamins and minerals In Britain, following the publication of the Benton and
play a role, perhaps to differing degrees in different indivi- Roberts paper [12], many nutritionists quoted a survey of
duals. To date there is evidence that thiamine [37], iron [49] the diets of schoolchildren [26] as evidence that there was
and vitamin C [42] stimulate scores on intelligence tests, little or no reason to worry about their nutrition. In this type
although the possibility that other nutrients may prove of survey the RDA is used as the bench-mark. The rule of
important should not be excluded. Following supplementa- thumb is that if the average intake of a population is more
tion the stimulation of intelligence scores by over nine than the RDA then the diet can be said to be adequate. A
points has been reported in a group of adults with low RDA is a population statistic that aims to satisfy the require-
serum folic acid levels [16]. ments of two standard deviations of the population. What
this approach ignored was that a satisfactory average intake
may hide a sub-set of the population with very low intakes
5. The diet of British school-children or very high needs.
If you consume 100% of the RDA, then statistically it
The Benton and Roberts [12] report attracted a great deal is likely that you are well-nourished, as the intake
of criticism from British nutritionists, who tended to believe declines the probability that it is de®cient increases.
that the diet of children typically supplied adequate levels of There is a second rule of thumb, sometimes used by
micro-nutrients. They responded as if it was not reasonable nutritionists. If the intake is less than 70% of the
to even suggest that the diet of some children was so poor RDA then this gives grounds for concern; if it is even
that micro-nutrient status could in¯uence cognitive func- less, then the concern is greater. On average the intake
tioning. Yudkin [72] claimed that ªThis is the most scanda- of various vitamins by British schoolchildren reached
lous paper I've seen printed in The Lancet. The study is the RDA, and the conclusion was drawn that they
ludicrous meaningless nonsenseº. Several years later he were well-nourished [26]. However, when these data
added, following a study with which he had been associated, were reanalyzed groups of children were found who
ªOur studies show, we believe conclusively, that adding ate less than 70%, or even 50%, of the RDA [5]. The
vitamins and minerals to the diets of children who have satisfactory average hid groups of children with a low
no obvious physical signs of nutrient de®ciency can never- micro-nutrient intake. It should be remembered that
theless produce an increase in their IQ scoresº [73]. White- although such data are suggestive of a problem, the
head, one of the most in¯uential nutritionists in the United RDA is a statistic that says nothing about an individual.
Kingdom, commented that ªNo physiological explanation The acid test, needed to show nutritional de®ciency, is
exists of how vitamin and mineral supplementation could the demonstration in a double-blind trial that supple-
affect brain function in a well nourished subjectº [76]. The mentation improved functioning. As such, if the data
quote re¯ects the received wisdom of many nutritionists. summarized in Table 2 can be shown to apply reliably
Even if you ignore the tautology, it is a strange observation, to sub-groups of children there will be hard evidence of
given the well-described metabolic roles of vitamins [3,20]. an inadequate diet.
For example they play important roles in the functioning of In fact it is questionable whether the RDA should be used
co-enzymes and hundreds of mechanisms will be disrupted when commenting about intelligence. The RDA is set at a
if vitamins are present in sub-optimal levels. What seems to level that satis®es the `needs' of practically all healthy indi-
be implied is that because it is so certain that children are viduals. The relevant question is what is meant by `needs'.
well fed such responses are inconceivable. In a similar vein, The United Kingdom committee charged with establishing
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 305

Dietary Reference Values (similar to RDA) commented girls were de®cient. Although no child younger than 14
that: years was de®cient, 1% of boys and 8% of girls aged 15±
18 years had serum values of vitamin B12 below the accep-
table level. The erythrocyte transketolase activation coef®-
Classically, the requirement of an individual for a
cient indicated that less than 0.5% of boys and 2% of girls
nutrient has been the amount required to prevent clin-
were thiamine de®cient. Although 76% of girls and 72% of
ical signs of de®ciency. Claims have also been made
boys had eaten no citrus fruit during a 7-day dietary diary,
that at very high levels of intake some nutrient have
no more than 5% of any age groups had plasma levels of
especially bene®cial or therapeutic effects but the
vitamin C below the acceptable range [35]. The level of
panel decided that these effects did not fall within
serum ferritin, the storage protein for iron, was lower than
their de®nition of requirement [27].
the acceptable range for 13% of boys overall, and in girls the
`Need' can mean many things; the avoidance of a de®- numbers who were de®cient ranged from 9 to 27% depend-
ciency disease; the establishing of adequate reserves; the ing on age [35].
achieving of optimal functioning. The stated objective of These data were similar to the report that 20% of young
a RDA is to avoid de®ciency rather than achieve optimal adult males and 29% of females were ribo¯avin de®cient
functioning. There is no logical reason to look to a RDA to and a further 17% of males and 28% of females were of
assess the likelihood that a child will, or will not, respond to marginal status [11]. Such observations are potentially
micro-nutrient supplementation with improved non-verbal important as there is only a 35% difference in ribo¯avin
intelligence scores. The type of evidence used to establish a concentration between de®ciency and saturation [3].
RDA, and the objectives of RDAs, tell us nothing about the However, it is important to note that a similar study [34]
optimal functioning of the brain or intelligence. Dietary reported that only 1% of young adult males and 3% of
surveys and RDAs were never intended to consider such females had a ribo¯avin status in the de®cient range. Clearly
questions. As such there is no reason for nutritionists to be different sections of the population eat markedly different
concerned that some children respond to supplementation diets. This is not surprising as vitamin intake has been
by performing better on an intelligence test. If they do reported to vary with age, the area of the country where
happen to be concerned then the RDA offers little guidance. you live, social class and family structure [34]. Such obser-
Even if for some reason it was believed that the RDA vations imply that data concerning nutritional status should
approach to dietary surveys tells us something about the be generalized with great caution.
relationship between nutritional status and intelligence, In summary, although when judged by estimations of
the problem that concerned many nutritionists arguably dietary intake or biochemical markers the diet of the major-
did not exist. There was no necessary inconsistency between ity of British children is adequate, a minority fail to meet the
the improvement in non-verbal intelligence in some chil- relevant criteria.
dren (Table 2) and surveys of diet [26,35]. Both suggested
that some, but not all children, have a poor diet. There was
no suggestion that all children responded to supplementa- 6. Micro-nutrient intake and neural functioning
tion with improved non-verbal scores, there was no sugges-
tion that all children were poorly nourished. In fact the Benton [6] marshalled evidence that very often the ®rst
limited evidence available suggested the opposite, only a symptoms associated with micro-nutrient de®ciency are
minority bene®ted from supplementation, those with diets psychological. In addition to the studies presently reviewed
that supplied low levels of vitamins and minerals. there is a series of well designed studies that report, in at
There are many problems associated with the use of diet- least some of the population, that micro-nutrient supplemen-
ary diaries, for example, often they only poorly predict tation is associated with better mood [9,10,40,65,66], better
biochemical indices, better indicators of nutritional status. memory [25,44], attention [8] and eye-hand co-ordination
There are few biochemically based data from children, [15]. Thus there is increasing evidence that particular vita-
however, a British survey found that the micro-nutrient mins may modulate psychological functioning. The brain is
intake of the majority of children reached accepted levels the most complex and metabolically active organ in the
[35]. With many vitamins there was, however, a few chil- body and cognition is the summated outcome of countless
dren who had very low blood levels. An erythrocyte millions of metabolic processes, thus even minor metabolic
glutathione reductase activation coef®cient of greater than inef®ciencies could create a cumulative adverse effect.
1.30 is generally considered to indicate ribo¯avin de®- As one example, the status of several B vitamins are
ciency. By this index, 59% of boys aged 4±6, and 78% assessed by measuring the activity of various red-blood
aged 7±10 years, were de®cient. In girls aged 4±6 years, cell enzymes. A small decline in the activity of a single
75% were ribo¯avin de®cient, something true for 95% of enzyme may be ignored as physiologically unimportant,
those aged 15±18 years [35]. The use of the erythrocyte measurement error, or part of the normal range. However,
aspartate aminotransferase coef®cient as a measure of vita- when you are examining the output of billions of neurones it
min B6 status indicated that about 10% of both boys and is possible that the accumulated impact of billions of small
306 D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

decreases in enzymatic activity may produce a cumulative 7. Changes in intelligence over time
physiologically signi®cant disruption of functioning. If this
argument has validity then the functioning of the brain, Our knowledge of the changes in intelligence in the popula-
given its complexity, would be expected to be the ®rst tion, over time, owes a great deal to the work of Flynn [30].
organ to demonstrate disrupted functioning. The parallels with the changes in non-verbal intelligence, that
Although you can speculate that the brain may be suscep- result from micro-nutrient supplementation, are striking. He
tible to a marginal micro-nutrient intake, there is no evidence reported very consistent and substantial changes in intelli-
to suggest a speci®c mechanism that underlies the improved gence over many years. For example in the Netherlands, for
performance of tests of non-verbal intelligence. In the absence generations, every man when he started his national service
of any evidence a few comments will be made to demonstrate completed the same intelligence test. Thus, with few excep-
the plausibility of the suggestion that micro-nutrient tions, we have test results from the entire male population.
de®ciency may in¯uence neural functioning. The widespread Over a period of 30 years, intelligence increased 21 points,
role of vitamins as part of co-enzymes ensures that micro- an enormous change. Flynn was able to ®nd similar evidence
nutrient de®ciencies have the potential to in¯uence any meta- from no less than 14 countries. Clearly such massive improve-
bolic pathway. Given its critical role in neuronal functioning ments in intelligence test performance must re¯ect powerful
the in¯uence on neurotransmission will be considered for factors, but what are they? Obvious candidates are better
illustrative purposes. schooling; more verbal stimulation from watching television;
One effect of thiamine de®ciency is a reduction of the greater access to books made possible by increased af¯uence.
brain content of acetyl CoA with a consequent decreased If any of these mechanisms were responsible for the increases
turnover of acetylcholine. Thiamine de®ciency is also asso- then you would expect that performance on verbally based
ciated with reduced levels of brain GABA, glutamate and tests would be stimulated. The results are very clear; it was
aspartate [3]. Ascorbic acid in¯uences the activity of several non-verbal rather than verbal intelligence that had increased
hydoxylases involved in the metabolism of neurotransmit- over the decades.
ters, for example the action of dopamine-B-monoxygenase Lynn [48] argued that it was likely that the increases in
that forms noradrenaline from dopamine [20]. Pyridoxal non-verbal intelligence re¯ected improved diet. One clue
phosphate is involved in practically all reactions involved that nutrition may be important is that as intelligence
in amino acid metabolism. Pyridoxal-dependent enzymes increased, there have been parallel increases in height. In
are involved in the biosynthesis of serotonin (tryptophan fact the increases in height have been large, about 1.2 cm
decarboxylase), dopamine, noradrenaline (DOPA decarbox- every decade. The most widely accepted explanations for
ylase) and GABA (glutamate decarboxylase) [3,20]. the increase in height is that it re¯ected improved nutrition,
In making these comments we should be careful in draw- although at earlier stages a reduction in infectious diseases
ing an analogy between the gross de®ciency of a micro- may have played a role. Lynn [48] considered the hypoth-
nutrient and the marginal de®ciency presently reviewed. It esis that the increase in intelligence re¯ected increased
needs to be demonstrated that a marginal dietary intake environmental stimulation. When adopted children were
adversely in¯uences enzymic activity, perhaps in a particu- examined after moving from biological parents, to more
lar sub-cellular compartment. There is some evidence that stimulating adoptive parents, it was the performance of
within the normal range vitamin status in¯uences the level verbal tests that tended to improve. Thus, the evidence is
of neurotransmitters. It has been reported that in synapto- that environmental stimulation improves verbal rather than
somes dopamine release was in¯uenced by concentrations non-verbal scores. Given the evidence that it is non-verbal
of ascorbic acid within the endogenous range [33]. Ascorbic performance that has dramatically improved over recent
acid may have a neuromodulatory role that involves changes decades, this suggests that the underlying mechanism is
at the level of carrier-membrane translocation and orienta- biological, rather than social in origin.
tion [24]. The treatment of rats with pyridoxine has been The massive increases in intelligence are not experimen-
found to increase the serotonin content of various areas of tal observations and it is impossible to establish their
the brain. It was suggested that the level of pyridoxal phos- origins. At the best all that can be argued is that nutrition
phate regulates the decarboxylation of 5-hydroxytrypto- is likely to play a role and the possible alternative explana-
phan, the precursor of serotonin. A continuum existed tions do not stand examination [48]. There are interesting
from pyridoxine de®ciency, to a moderate excess, that parallels between the improvement in performance on non-
was re¯ected in the synthesis and release of serotonin [23]. verbal tests and the response of some children to the provi-
Although these data are consistent with the suggestion sion of vitamin and mineral supplements. There is no way to
that micro-nutrient status has the potential to in¯uence disassociate the possible contribution of micro-nutrients
neural functioning, we have no idea of the mechanism by from that resulting from other aspects of diet. However,
which supplementation improves the performance of non- the size of the increase in measures of ¯uid intelligence
verbal tests. Given the ubiquitous metabolic roles of vita- have led others to suggest that they are in part, at least,
mins, potentially, they can be expected to in¯uence any artefacts. It is unclear to what extent the changes re¯ect
aspect of the brain's structure and functioning. test sophistication and experience of educational toys that
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 307

encourage the types of thinking tapped by non-verbal females of different ages should be contrasted. The associa-
reasoning tests. tion between socio-economic background, dietary style and
micro-nutrient status should be considered. Given the many
factors that in¯uence what we eat, and the wide spread
8. Discussion individual need for nutrients, it is perhaps less surprising
that the data are inconsistent than that there is a considerable
An independent observer of this area might comment that, amount of consistency.
on occasions, the debate has been between individuals with Finally, it is important that if future studies support these
closed minds. Accepting that with any study there are matters small-scale studies, the phenomenon is kept in context.
of methodological concern, is there reason to further consider Many parents will readily believe that diet can both cause
the possibility that for some individuals the intake of micro- and solve complex and worrying problems. In most cases
nutrients limits intellectual functioning? The number of this is an unreasonable expectation. There is no reason to
studies is such that it is unreasonable to consider each in isola- expect a supplement to produce dramatic change in a child's
tion rather we should examine the overall pattern. functioning, even if they have a low intake of micro-nutri-
Attention may be drawn to various global observations. ents. We function intellectually using `crystallized' intelli-
gence, yet it was the `¯uid' scores (non-verbal) that were
1. Of the 13 studies in Table 2, 10 reported a positive stimulated by supplementation. Assuming that your child
response in at least a subsection of the subjects. would in fact bene®t from supplementation all that you
2. Where a positive response has been reported it has can expect to improve is intellectual potential. Unless that
always been with non-verbal measures, it has never potential interacts with a stimulating environment, it
been reported for verbal measures of intelligence. remains as nothing more than potential, something that
3. For theoretical reasons a response to non-verbal, and not has not been exploited. Improved nutritional status of itself
verbal tests, was predicted a priori. Non-verbal tests tap will bring no intellectual gain unless it takes place in a
the biologically based functioning that could be expected stimulating, emotionally secure environment. Even then
to re¯ect the adequacy of diet. Verbal tests re¯ect knowl- the changes will take place gradually over a long period.
edge and vocabulary that would not be expected to Eysenck and Schoenthaler [29] concluded, ªwith a certain
improve, at least in the short-term, following micro-nutri- degree of con®denceº, that ªinadequate levels of vitamins
ent supplementation. and minerals in the blood reduce a child's IQº and ªsupple-
4. The evidence is that not all children respond to supple- mentation by vitamin and mineral pills can raise the child's
mentation, rather it occurs in a minority whose diet offers non-verbal IQ signi®cantlyº, although it ªhas no effect on
low amounts of micro-nutrients [7,29]. Such an observa- children with an adequate level of vitamins and mineralº.
tion is consistent with dietary surveys that typically The only non-controversial aspect of this summary is the
report a sub-set of children with a low intake of micro- ®nal part. Nobody has suggested that those with an adequate
nutrients [26,35]. vitamin status will respond to supplementation, it is not a
way of achieving a level of supra-normal functioning.
Those wishing to dismiss the data in Table 2 must explain You may accept the rest of this conclusion so long as the
the above pattern of results. As the results have been reported operative phrase is ªa certain degree of con®denceº. Is this
by different investigators, using different methodologies, it is certain degree large or small? The tone of the Eysenck and
dif®cult to point to common aspects of methodology that Schoenthaler chapter [29] is that they have a large degree of
would produce the distinctive pattern. The time has passed con®dence. Many skeptical scientists, after a sober evalua-
when a single study, that does not correspond to your precon- tion, will have a smaller degree of con®dence. Arguably we
ceptions, can be subject to destructive criticism. Those are not, however, at a stage where that degree of con®dence
wishing to totally dismiss the positive ®ndings need, when is so limited that the idea that micro-nutrient status can limit
making a methodological evaluation, to point to common intellectual potential can be safely dismissed as ªludicrous
faults not shared by those reporting negative data. Any such and meaningless nonsenseº [71]. There are too many posi-
fault must be shown to produce a highly distinctive pattern of tive results to dismiss the ®ndings out of handÐthere is a
results. The nature of such idiosyncrasies are not obvious. need for further study.
If the conclusion is accepted that there is a phenomenon A tentative conclusion re¯ects the many questions that need
worthy of further study, such a conclusion should be drawn to be systematically addressed. The topic is at a very early
with caution. We have a series of small-scale studies that stage and needs the clari®cation gained from a series of
have inconsistencies that await satisfactory explanation. large-scale studies. Children of a wide range of ages, dietary
There is an urgent need for large-scale studies that use reli- styles and social backgrounds need to be considered. The
able biochemical measures of dietary status. Ideally the nutrients and dosage required should be established. What is
sample should be large enough to consider the response of it that is missing from the diet, a little bit of everything or are
groups who differ in the nature of their nutritional de®cien- particular nutrients important? Are other aspects of psycholo-
cies and social background. The reaction of males and gical functioning disrupted as well as non-verbal intelligence?
308 D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309

Is the phenomenon more likely to occur at some stages of [25] Deijen JB, van der Beek EJ, Orlebeke IF, van den Berg H. Vitamin B6
development rather than others? What proportion of children supplementation in elderly men: effects on mood memory perfor-
mance and mental effort. Psychopharmacol 1992;109:489±96.
have an inadequate diet? These are questions that we have not [26] Diets of British Schoolchildren. Department of Health Report 36.
begun to consider, never mind answer. London: HMSO, 1989.
[27] Dietary Reference Values for food energy and nutrients for the United
Kingdom. Department of Health Report on Health and Social
References Subjects, vol. 41. London: HMSO, 1991.
[28] Dietary sugars and human disease. Department of Health Report on
[1] Aukett MA, Parks YA, Scott PH, Wharton BA. Treatment with iron Health and Social Subjects, vol. 37. London: HMSO, 1989.
increases weight gain and psychomotor development. Arch Dis Child [29] Eysenck HJ, Schoenthaler SJ. Raising IQ level by vitamin and
1986;61:849±57. mineral supplementation. In: Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL, editors.
[2] Barrett DE, Frank DA. The effects of undernutrition on childrents Intelligence heredity and environment, Cambridge: Cambridge
behavior. New York: Gordon and Breach, 1987. University Press, 1997. p. 363±92.
[3] Bender DA. Nutritional biochemistry of the vitamins. Cambridge: [30] Flynn JR, Massive IQ. gains in 14 nations: what IQ tests really
Cambridge University Press, 1992. measure. Psychol Bull 1984;101:171±91.
[4] Benton D. Letter. Lancet 1988;i:408. [31] Foley D, Hay DA, Mitchell RJ. Speci®c cognitive effects of mild iron
[5] Benton D. Food for thought. London: Penguin, 1996. de®ciency and associations with blood polymorphisms in young
[6] Benton D. Vitamin-mineral supplements and intelligence. Proc Nutr adults. Ann Hum Biol 1986;13:417±25.
Soc 1992;51:295±302. [32] Fordy J, Benton D. Does low iron status in¯uence psychological
[7] Benton D, Buts J-P. Vitamin/mineral supplementation and intelli- functioning? J Hum Nutr Dietet 1994;7:127±33.
gence. Lancet 1990;335:1158±60. [33] Girbe F, Ramassamy C, Piton C, Costentin J. Ascorbic acid increases
[8] Benton D, Cook R. Vitamin and mineral supplements improve the synaptosomal potassium-induced dopamine release. Neuroreport
intelligence scores and concentration of six year children. Personal 1994;9:1027±9.
Indi Diff 1991;12:1151±8. [34] Gregory J, Foster K, Tyler H, Wiseman M. The dietary and nutritional
[9] Benton D, Grif®ths R, Haller J. Thiamine supplementation mood and survey of British adults. London: Of®ce of Population Censuses and
cognitive functioning. Psychopharmacol 1997;129:66±71. Surveys, HMSO, 1990.
[10] Benton D, Haller J, Fordy J. Vitamin supplementation for one year [35] Gregory J, Lowe S. National diet and nutrition survey: young people
improves mood. Neuropsychobiol 1995;32:98±105. aged 4 to 18 years. London: The Stationery Of®ce, 2000.
[11] Benton D, Haller J, Fordy J. The vitamin status of young British [36] Haller J. The action of vitamins and other nutrients on psychological
adults. Int J Vit Nutr Res 1997;67:34±40. parameters. In: Hindmarch I, Stonier PD, editors. Human psychophar-
[12] Benton D, Roberts G. Effect of vitamin and mineral supplementation macology, vol. 5. Winchester: John Wiley, 1995. p. 229±61.
on the intelligence of a sample of schoolchildren. Lancet 1988;i:140± [37] Harrell RF. Mental responses to added thiamine. J Nutr 1946;31:283±
3. 98.
[13] Booth IW, Aukett MA. Iron de®ciency in infancy and early child- [38] Harrell RF, Capp RH, Davis DR, Peerless J, Ravitz LR. Can nutri-
hood. Arch Dis Child 1997;76:549±53. tional supplements help mentally retarded children? Proc Nat Acad
[14] Boggs UR, Scheaf A, Santoro D, Ritzman R. The effects of nutrient Sci USA 1981;78:574±8.
supplements on the biological and psychological characteristics of [39] Harrell RF, Woodyard ER, Gates AI. The in¯uence of vitamin supple-
low IQ preschool children. J Orthomol Psychiat 1965;14:97±127. mentation of the diets of pregnant and lactating women on the intelli-
[15] Bonke D, Nickel B. Improvement of ®ne motoric movement control gence of their offspring. Metabolism 1956;V:555±62.
by elevated dosages of Vitamin B1 B6, and B12 in target shooting. In: [40] Heseker H, Kubler W, Westenhofer J, Pudel V. Psychische Verander-
Walter P, Stahelin H, Brubacher G, editors. Elevated dosages of vita- ungen als Fruhzeichen emer suboptimalen Vitaminversorgung.
mins, Toronto: Hans Huber, 1989. p. 198±204. Ernahrungs-Umschau 1990;37:87±94.
[16] Botez MI, Fontaine F, Botez T, Bachevalier J. Folate-responsive [41] Kerimova MG, Aleskerova IG. In¯uence of ration correction and
neurological and mental disorders: report of 16 cases. Eur Neurol varying doses of glutamevitum on the vitamin status and working
1977;16:230±46. capacity of children attending school form the age of six. Voprosy
[17] Carroll HCM, et al. A psychometric critique of Nelson's 1990 paper: Pitaniia 1990:Nov±Dec;30±34 [In Russian].
nutrient intakes vitamin-mineral supplementation and intelligence in [42] Kubula AL, Katz MM. Nutritional factors in psychological test beha-
British schoolchildren. Personal Indi Diff 1995;18:669±75. vior. J Genet Psychol 1960;96:343±52.
[18] Cattell RB. The heritability of ¯uid gf and crystallized gc intelligence: [43] Letters to Editor. Lancet 1988;I:407±8.
a critical experiment. J Educ Psychol 1963;54:1±22. [44] Loriaux SM, Deijen JB, Orlebeke JF, de Swart JR. The effects of
[19] Colgan M, Colgan L. Do nutrient supplements and dietary changes nicotinic acid and xanthinol nicotinate on human memory in different
affect learning and emotional reactions of children with learning dif®- categories of age. Psychopharmacol 1985;87:390±5.
culties? A controlled series of 16 cases. Nutr Health 1984;3:69±77. [45] Lozoff B. Behavioral alterations in iron de®ciency. Adv Pediatr
[20] Combs GF. The vitamins. 2nd ed. San Diego: Academic Press, 1998. 1988;35:331±59.
[21] Crombie IK, Todman J, McNeill O, Florey C du V. Vitamin-mineral [46] Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Woll AW. Long-term developmental outcomes
supplementation and intelligence. Lancet 1990;336:175. of infants with iron de®ciency. New Eng J Med 1991;325:687±94.
[22] Crombie IK, Todman J, McNeill G, Florey C du V, Menzies I, [47] Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon
Kennedy RA. Effect of vitamin and mineral supplementation on ME. Behavior of infants with iron-de®ciency anemia. Child Dev
verbal and non-verbal reasoning of schoolchildren. Lancet 1998;69:24±36.
1990;335:744±7. [48] Lynn R. The role of nutrition in secular increases in intelligence.
[23] Dakshinamurti K, Sharma SK, Bonke D. In¯uence of B vitamin on Personal Indi Diff 1990;11:272±85.
binding properties of serotonin receptors in the CNS of rats. Klin [49] Lynn R, Harland EP. A positive effect of iron supplementation on the
Wochenschr 1990;68:142±5. IQs of iron de®cient children. Personal Indi Diff 1998;24:883±5.
[24] Debler EA, Sershen H, Hashim A, Lajtha A, Reith ME. Carrier- [50] Maseck D. Vitamins: the get-smart pills? J Orthomol Psychiat
mediated ef¯ux of [3H] dopamine and [3H] 1-methyl-4-phenylpyri- 1980;9:58±65.
dine: effect of ascorbic acid. Synapse 1991;7:99±105. [51] Mora JO, Amezquita A, Castro L. Nutrition health and social factors
D. Benton / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25 (2001) 297±309 309

related to intellectual performance. World Rev Nutr Diet [65] Smidt LJ, Cremin FM, Grivetti LE, Clifford AJ. In¯uence of thiamin
1974;19:205±36. supplementation on the health and general well-being of an elderly
[52] Naismith DJ. Daily Telegraph, London, 22 February, 1988. Irish population with marginal thiamin de®ciency. J Gerontol
[53] Nelson M, Naismith DJ, Burley V, Gatenby S, Geddes N. Nutrient 1991;46:M16±22.
intake vitamin/mineral supplementation and intelligence in British [66] Snowden W. Evidence from an analysis of 2000 errors and omission
Schoolchildren. Brit J Nutr 1990;64:13±22. made in IQ tests by a sample of schoolchildren undergoing vitamin
[54] Nidich SI, Morehead P, Nidich RJ, Sands D, Sharma H. The effect of and mineral supplementation that speed of processing is an important
the Mahaarishi Student Rasayana food supplement on non-verbal factor in IQ performance. Personal Indi Diff 1997;22:131±4.
intelligence. Personal Indi Diff 1993;15:599±602. [67] Southam S. Micronutrients and IQ. J Micronut Anal 1990;7:179±91.
[55] Po LW. Vitamins IQ and the law. Pharmaceut J 1992:506±8. [68] Southon S, Wright A, Finglas P, Bailey A, Beisten J. Micro nutrient
[56] Pollitt B. Malnutrition and infection in the classroom. Paris: intake and psychological performance of school-children: considera-
UNESCO, 1990. tion of the value of calculated intake for the measurement of micro-
[57] Pollitt B. Iron de®ciency and cognitive function. Ann Rev Nutr nutrient status in children. Proc Nutr Soc 1992;51:315±24.
1993;13:521±37. [69] Southon S, Wright A, Finglas P, Bailey AL, Louchridge JM, Walker
[58] Pollitt B. Poverty and child development: relevance of research in AD. Dietary intake and micronutrient status of adolescents: effect of
developing countries to the United States. Child Dev 1994;65:283± vitamin and trace element supplementation on indices of status and
95. performance in tests of verbal and non-verbal intelligence. Brit J Nutr
[59] Pollitt B. Iron de®ciency and educational de®ciency. Nutr Rev 1994;71:897±918.
1997;55:133±41. [70] Todman J. Vitamins and IQ. The Psychologist, August 1993. p. 357±9.
[60] Rimland B, Callaway B, Drey¯is P. The effect of high doses of [71] Todman J, Crombie I, Elder L. An individual difference test of the
vitamin B6 on autistic children: a double-blind crossover study. effect of vitamin supplementation on non-verbal IQ. Personal Indi
Amer J Psychiat 1978;135:472±5. Diff 1991;12:1333±7.
[61] Sanders TA. Vegetarian diets and children. Ped Clin North Amer [72] Yudkin J. Daily Telegraph, 22 February, 1988.
1995;42:955±65. [73] Yudkin J. Intelligence of children and vitamin-mineral supplements:
[62] Schoenthaler SJ, Amos SP, Doraz WE, Kelly MA, Wake®eld J. the DRF study. Discussion conclusions and consequences. Personal
Controlled trial of vitamin-mineral supplementation on intelligence Indi Diff 1991;12:363±5.
and brain function. Personal Indi Diff 1991;12:343±50. [74] Waber DP, Vuori-Christiansen LV, Ortiz N, Clement JR, Christiansen
[63] Schoenthaler SJ, Amos SP, Eysenck HJ, Peritz B, Yudkin J. NE, Mora JO, Reed RB, Herrera MG. Nutritional supplementation
Controlled trial of vitamin-mineral supplementation: effects on intel- maternal education and cognitive development of infants at risk of
ligence and performance. Personal Indi Diff 1991;12:351±62. malnutrition. Amer J Clin Nutr 1981;34:807±13.
[64] Schoenthaler SJ, Bier I, Young K, Nichols D, Jansenns S. The effect [75] Webb TE, Oski FA. Iron de®ciency anemia and scholastic achieve-
of vitamin-mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American ment in young adolescents. J Pediatr 1973;82:827±30.
schoolchildren: a randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J [76] Whitehead RG. Vitamins minerals schoolchildren and IQ. Brit Med J
Alternat Compl Med 2000;6:19±29. 1991;302:548.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen