Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

Acta Pædiatrica ISSN 0803–5253

REGULAR ARTICLE

Sex differences in newborn interaction with mother or father during skin-


to-skin contact after Caesarean section
Marianne Velandia (Marianne.Velandia@ki.se)1, Kerstin Uvnäs-Moberg2, Eva Nissen1
1.Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, Division Reproductive Health, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
2.Department of Animal Environment and Health, Swedish University of Agriculture, Skara, Sweden

Keywords ABSTRACT
Breastfeeding, Caesarean section, Newborn
behaviour, Sex, Skin-to-skin contact
Aim: To investigate differences between the breast-seeking and crying behaviour of
girls and boys in skin-to-skin contact (SSC) with their mother or their father after Caesarean
Correspondence
Marianne Velandia, Department of Women’s and
section as well as the point-in-time for the first breastfeeding and to compare mothers’ and
Children’s Health, Division Reproductive Health, fathers’ interactive behaviour with their newborn girl or boy.
Karolinska Institutet, Retzius väg 13A, S-171 77 Methods: Twenty girls and 17 boys were randomized to 25 min of SSC with one or
Stockholm, Sweden.
Tel: +46 8-524 824 15 |
the other parent immediately after birth. The interaction was videotaped.
Fax: +46 8-524 824 00 | Results: Girls started rooting movements earlier than boys in SSC with either parent
Email: Marianne.Velandia@ki.se (p = 0.027). Infants started to breastfeed significantly earlier if having been in SSC with
Received mothers compared with SSC with fathers during the first 5–30 min (p = 0.018). Girls cried
15 July 2011; accepted 8 November 2011 more than boys in SSC with either parent (p = 0.02). Mothers used more touching behav-
DOI:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02523.x iour towards their newborn infant than fathers (p = 0.001). Mothers touched girls less than
boys (p = 0.038). Fathers directed less speech towards girls compared with boys
(p = 0.042).
Conclusion: Early mother–infant SSC immediately after Caesarean section should
be promoted until the occurrence of the first breastfeed. If the mother is unable to provide
SSC immediately after birth, the father–infant SSC is a valuable alternative because it
enhances paternal interaction.

INTRODUCTION In a previous study, we explored parent–infant vocal


Early mother–infant SSC induces a biological programme communication when the baby is in SSC with either the
that facilitates breastfeeding and interaction. Oxytocin mother or the father after a planned Caesarean section. It
released during mother–infant interaction may facilitate was found that infants, placed in SSC, performed soliciting
several aspects of the interaction between mother and child calls to initiate communication with the parents after hear-
(1–3). A healthy newborn put in SSC on their mother’s ing the parents’ communication, as quickly as 15 min after
chest perform an inborn breast-seeking behaviour to find birth (6). Furthermore, SSC increased parental vocal com-
the breast and start to breastfeed (3). The newborns’ inborn munication with the infant, especially in the fathers. The
behaviour is triggered and modulated by sensory cues dur- hypothesis that there might be sex difference in behaviour
ing SSC, such as maternal breast odour (4), warm tempera- already at birth is based on the idea that the brain is influ-
ture (5) and voices (6). enced by sex hormones during foetal development leading
Fathers who have SSC with their newborn after birth to sex differences in the structure of the brain and thus
reduce the infants’ cry, and they feel satisfied when they feel
they can offer similar care as mothers (7).
Parental tactile contact with their newborn may assist in Key notes
eliciting feelings of love for the newborn infant and • The result highlights the importance of immediate
enhance parental sensitivity and responsiveness to their infant–parent skin-to-skin contact (SSC), even after
infant’s cues. Tactile touch has been utilized as a marker of Caesarean section. The parents interacted differently
attachment (8). with their infant. Uninterrupted SSC with the mother
facilitated the initiation of breastfeeding in the mother–
infant dyad exposed to a planned Caesarean section. If
the mother is unable to provide SSC immediately after
Abbreviation birth, the father is a valuable alternative, because SSC
SSC, skin-to-skin contact. promotes his interaction with the newborn infant.

360 ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367
Velandia et al. Sex differences in infant–parent interaction

behaviour (9–12). Different sex-related reaction patterns scrub (including the breasts), using chlorhexidine, the day
may also be seen in adults (13,14). before and the day of surgery. For anaesthesia, all women
Our model involving SSC between parents and infants received a spinal block with 12.5 mg of high-density bupiva-
might represent an ideal model to study inborn reaction pat- caine and 10 lg fentanyl. Additional drugs were used to reg-
tern because no learning has occurred. Considering the ulate blood pressure, when needed. Before the surgical
increased use of Caesarean section, studies providing a dee- procedure, the project leader placed a video recorder 1.5 m
per understanding of infant–parent interaction in regard to from the mother. The lens focused on the upper parts of the
the sex of the parent and infant are extremely important as infant’s body and on the chest and face of the parent, allow-
they may contribute to develop guidelines for care in con- ing for the observation of the facial expressions and hand
nection with Caesarean section. movements of the infant and his ⁄ her parent.
Therefore, this study aims to (i) investigate differences Immediately after birth, at approximately 1 min after
between the breast-seeking and crying behaviours of birth, the naked newborn was placed in SSC in a trans-
newborn girls and newborn boys, in SSC with their mother verse position on the mother’s naked chest. The infant’s
or their father after a planned Caesarean section; (ii) head was placed to the right, and his ⁄ her feet were placed
compare differences in mothers’ and fathers’ interactive to the left, allowing the infant to touch the mother’s nipple.
behaviours with their newborn girl or boy; and (iii) Placing the infant in this position also enabled the surgery
evaluate the time point for the first breastfeeding event, for to continue.
girls and boys, after having been in SSC with their mother In each case, the infant’s body was covered with warm
or their father. towels to maintain the newborn in a stable temperature. All
babies stayed with their mothers in this position for 5 min.
Thereafter, the baby either stayed in SSC with his ⁄ her
PATIENTS AND METHODS mother or was placed in SSC on his ⁄ her father’s chest for
This study is a part of a larger, randomized controlled study the next 25 min, according to the outcome of randomiza-
to investigate parent–infant interaction immediately after tion. The baby was put on the father’s chest, with its eyes
birth with a planned Caesarean section (6). The study took levelling the nipples of the father. The baby could turn his
place between 1997 and 2001, in a large obstetric clinic in upper body. The fathers sat comfortably in an armchair,
Stockholm, Sweden. beside the head-end of the operating table, so the parents
could engage in eye contact and freely communicate and
Inclusion criteria touch each other and the baby. The parents were encour-
To be included in the study, the primiparas needed to have aged to interact with each other and the baby and to act as
had a healthy, uncomplicated pregnancy and a planned natural as possible. The approach of the staff in the operat-
Caesarean section at term (38–42 gestational weeks). The ing theatre was gentle, kind and supportive towards the par-
woman should not have smoked during the third trimester. ents.
The baby should be healthy at birth, have had an Apgar
score of at least 7, at the 1-min testing mark, and should be Data collection and management
immediately placed in SSC. All women fulfilling the inclu- Obstetric background data were collected from the birth
sion criteria were consecutively recruited at the maternity records. The time lapse between birth and first breastfeed-
ward the day before surgery. The only inclusion criterion for ing was noted. The video recording began immediately at
fathers was that they were willing to participate in the study. birth and continued for up to 90 min. After 30 min, all
Primiparas and their partners were informed of the study the babies were kept in SSC with the mother for the fol-
design, and both women and men were asked separately to lowing 90-min postpartum. Thereafter, the infant was
sign if they agreed to participate. All consented to partici- removed from the mother’s chest and underwent routine
pate. examination, performed by a midwife. Then, the infant
was dressed and returned to his ⁄ her parents. Four hours
Randomization postpartum, the family was transferred to the postpartum
The newborns were randomized to SSC with either their ward where they stayed overnight, together, in a family
mother or their father, for the 5- to 30-min period after room.
birth, by a person who was not directly involved in collect-
ing research data for this study. Information about the treat- Assessment of infant–parent interactive behaviours
ment was placed into an opaque envelope, which was The following behaviours that occurred for the first time
marked with an identity number. On the day of the surgery, were identified for the newborn and were recorded: strong
the envelope was opened, and the parents were informed as rooting (breast-seeking behaviours, distinct head turning
to whether the father or the mother would have the new- and movements, sometimes followed by smacking sounds);
born in SSC. breast-massaging movements (the infant performs repeated
breast-massaging movements); and breastfeeding (appear-
Description of the intervention ance of the first latch on and initiation of sucking the
For women having a Caesarean section at this clinic, rou- breast). Crying (dissatisfied groaning sounds) and the fol-
tine procedures prescribed a shower and a thorough body lowing parental behaviours were recorded as (1) if it

ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367 361
Sex differences in infant–parent interaction Velandia et al.

occurred during the 30-sec interval observed or as (0) if it Statistical analysis


did not occur during the studied 30-sec interval. The inter- For statistical analysis, the SPSS ⁄ PASW Statistics 18 (SPSS,
active parental behaviours coded were finger-tipping touch Chicago, IL, USA) software package was used. As a central
(light touching with the fingertips on any part of the measure to describe background data, the mean is pre-
infant’s body); touching (tactile touching and tapping hand sented, and as a measure of dispersion, 95% confidence
movements on any part of the infant’s body); smiling intervals or standard deviation (SD) is presented. Median
(mouth movements with retraction of lips comeasure- (Md) was used as a central measure, and interquartile dis-
ments); kissing (touching the baby with the lips) and tances (Q25–Q75) were used as a measure of dispersion for
parental-directed speech (any verbal speech directed the time from birth to first occurrence of a specific behaviour
towards the infant). like rooting, breast massage movements and breastfeeding.
If an observed behaviour was difficult to identify, a so- To test differences in crying, touching, smiling, kissing
called conference score of observed behaviours was devel- and speech, over time and between groups, ANOVA for
oped between the four observers (3). A ‘conference score’ repeated measurements was used. The following nonpara-
was achieved among the observers by simultaneously study- metric tests were used: the Mann–Whitney U-test, the Krus-
ing the videos in real time and then summarizing the behav- kal–Wallis test for independent samples and the
iours for each 30-sec periods. The tape was stopped, while Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test. For calculating correlations in
the notes were made but if the observers disagreed on the behaviours with parents and infants, Spearman rank coeffi-
coding of a behaviour within the period, the tape was cient (Rs) was used. p-values <0.05 were considered as sta-
rewound, and the sequence was restudied until the observ- tistical significant.
ers reached an agreement.
To test the reliability of the observations of the video- Ethical considerations
tapes, over time, a second observer scrutinized five ran- The ethical issue is whether it is acceptable to separate
domly selected videotapes. The interobserver agreement mother and infant immediately after birth with Caesarean
ranged from 65% to 100% (mean 80%) and was considered section and hand the baby over to the father. The common
satisfactory. The outcome for the different variables is dis- ward routine at the time when the study was performed was
played in Table 1. to separate the infant from its parents for at least 2 h. The
design of the study brought about an improvement for the
Data transcription and analysis parents and infant, which was more in accordance with the
All behaviours observed during each 30-sec periods were guidelines for normal birth and Baby Friendly Hospital Ini-
monitored by an experienced observer who was not tiative. The study was approved by the ethical committee at
informed about the aims of this study. Data sheets for tran- Karolinska Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden.
scription was developed in Excel and coded manually. The
behaviours were listed in the vertical plane and the time in
the horizontal plane for each observation. This type of cod- RESULTS
ing has previously been described (15). Forty-two couples were asked to participate, and the new-
During a few observation periods, technical reasons pre- borns were randomly assigned either to SSC with their
vented certain behaviours from being visible. As a result, mother or to SSC with their father. Thus, all infants were in
these were monitored as missing. These missing periods SSC with either the mother or the father.
were equally distributed between participating infant boys
and girls. This type of video transcription has been Drop-outs and exclusions
described previously. Two newborns dropped out of the study after the randomi-
zation, because their fathers had changed their minds and
Table 1 Inter-rater reliability test
no longer wanted to participate. Furthermore, one newborn
infant was excluded, because of a low Apgar score, and two
Mean ratio
newborns were excluded as the randomization protocol
Infant behaviours was not followed because their mothers felt sick; in these
Skin-to-skin contact 100 instances, the fathers kept their babies on their chests, but
Crying 77 no SSC was established. On the day of the surgery, two
Strong rooting 92
mothers arrived at the hospital without the father of their
Breast-massaging movements 65
babies’, but accompanied by another person; however, these
Parental behaviours
Finger-tipping touch 67
mothers were able to participate in the study because the
Touching 77 outcome of the randomization protocol indicated infant-to-
Smiling 80 parent SSC with the mother. Ultimately, study participants
Kissing 99 included 37 healthy, full-term infants [mean gestational age
Mother’s speech directed to the infant 90 38.5 weeks (95% CI 38.4–39.0)] and their first-time mothers
Father’s speech directed to the infant 74 and fathers. Of the 37 newborns, a total of 20 girls and 17
Interobserver agreement ranged from 65 to 100% (mean 80%). boys participated in the study and randomized to SSC with
their mother (n = 17) or father (n = 20).

362 ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367
Velandia et al. Sex differences in infant–parent interaction

There were no significant differences in background data. this difference was not significant (p = 0.886) and is shown
For mothers, the mean use of high-density bupivacaine (mg) in Table 2.
was 12 (95% CI 11–12.4), of fentanyl (lg) was 13 (95% CI
7.6–19), and of ephedrine (mg) was 26 (95% CI 20–32.7). Girls’ and boys’ breast-seeking and breastfeeding behav-
The total mean bleeding for mothers was 874 mL (95% CI iour in skin-to-skin contact with mother or father
721–1027). All infants were healthy at birth with an Apgar To further analyse differences in girls’ and boys’ first breast-
score of at last 7, at 1 min of age. There was no significant feeding in SSC with the mother or in SSC with the father, a
difference in mean birthweight between girls and boys subgroup analysis was performed.
(df = 35, F = 1.139, p = 0.6). The mean birthweight for girls Girls (n = 11) who were in SSC with the mother during
was 3555.8 g (95% CI 3388.3–3723.2) and for boys the first 30 min after birth started their first breastfeeding
3474.3 g (95% CI 3201.3–3748.3). significantly earlier than girls (n = 9) who had been in SSC
with the father (p = 0.037) during the first 30 min. No sig-
Infants’ breast-seeking and breastfeeding behaviour in nificant difference was found among boys (p = 0.525).
skin-to-skin contact with father or mother
There were no significant differences in the appearance of Girls’ and boys’ crying behaviour in skin-to-skin contact
the first strong rooting or breast-massaging movements with mother or father
between infants (regardless of their sex), in SSC with their During the observation period, girls cried significantly more
mothers or their fathers, Table 2. Infants’ first breastfeeding than boys (df = 1, F = 5.972, p = 0.02) (Fig. 1). Addition-
in SSC with mothers appeared significantly earlier than in ally, girls cried significantly more when in SSC with the
SSC with the father (p = 0.018) (Table 2). For infants in mother than in SSC with the father (df = 1, F = 10.977,
SSC with the mother (n = 17), the first breastfeeding p = 0.004). Girls cried for a mean of 13 min (95% CI 9.46–
occurred at a median of 117.5 min (69.5–208), and for 15.99) in SSC with the mother and for a mean of 6 min
infants in SSC with the father (n = 20) at a median of (95% CI 2.635–9.145) in SSC with the father. In contrast,
235 min (120–430). Infants’ first breastfeed appeared signif- no significant differences were found in crying behaviours
icantly earlier if they had been continuously in SSC with the among boys when they were in SSC with the mother or in
mother during the first 30 min after birth. SSC with the father (df = 1, F = 1.433, p = 0.251). Boys
cried for a mean of 7 min (95% CI 1.175–15.505) in SSC
Girls’ and boys’ breast-seeking and breastfeeding with the mother and for a mean of 3 min (95% CI )3.35 to
behaviour in skin-to-skin contact with parents 6.935) when in SSC with the father.
The first strong rooting behaviour in girls (n = 20)
appeared significantly earlier than in boys (p = 0.035) in Parental behaviour in skin-to-skin contact with their baby
SSC with either parent. For girls, it appeared at a median Touching
of 7 min (3.63–15.5), and for boys (n = 17) at a median of Mothers in SSC touched their infants significantly more
17 min (11.0–21.0). There were no significant differences than fathers in SSC [df = 1, F = 14.138, p = 0.001 (Fig. 2)].
in the median duration of rooting behaviours, between Furthermore, mothers touched girls significantly less than
girls and boys (p = 0.56), either in SSC with the mother boys (df = 1, F = 5.152, p = 0.038). Mothers touched girls
(p = 0.301) or in SSC with the father (p = 0.661). For girls, for a mean of 7.59 min (95% CI 2.955–12.225) and boys for
the median duration for this behaviour was 5 min (1–15) a mean of 14.5 min (95% CI 6.81–22.19).
and for boys was 6 min (0.7–13.5). There was also a statis- There was no significant difference in the time fathers
tical difference in the appearance of the first breast-mas- spent touching girls or the time they spent touching boys
saging movements between girls and boys in SSC with (df = 1, F = 1.538, p = 0.232). Touching occurred for a
mother or father (p = 0.027), shown in Table 2. The first mean of 1.5 min (95% CI 0.01–2.99) in girls and for a mean
breastfeeding for girls appeared at a median of 157 min of 4.5 min (95% CI )0.18 to 9.18) in boys. Mothers per-
(101–304) and for boys at a median of 170 min (74–345); formed significantly more finger-tipping touch than fathers

Table 2 First appearance of a specific breast-seeking behaviour during the first hours of life after Caesarean section. Comparison of the development of feeding behaviour between
infants in skin-to-skin contact (SSC) with (a) mother or father and (b) between girls and boys irrespective of placement with mother or father. Data presented as median minutes and
interquartile distances (Q25–Q75) values
(a) Newborn breast-seeking behaviour in SSC with mother (b) Girls and boys breast-seeking behaviour in SSC
or father with parent

Mothers (n = 17) Fathers (n = 20) Girls (n = 20) Boys (n = 17)

Variable Md (Q25–Q75) Md (Q25–Q75) p Md (Q25–Q75) Md (Q25–Q75) p

First time of strong rooting 12.5 (5.5–19.0) 14.75 (4.6–20.6.3) 0.603 7.0 (3.63–15.5) 17.0 (11.0–21.0) 0.035
First time of breast massage movements 11.0 (7.13–16.6) 16.0 (4.4–27.0) 0.576 7.75 (3.5–15.5) 18.5 (9.9–26) 0.027
First breastfeeding 117.5 (70–208) 235.1 (120–430) 0.018 157 (101–304) 170 (74–345) 0.886

ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367 363
Sex differences in infant–parent interaction Velandia et al.

3.5 3.5
Girls Mothers

Minutes of finger-tipping touch (mean)


Boys Fathers
3.0 3.0
Minutes of crying (mean)

2.5 2.5

2.0 2.0

1.5 1.5

1.0 1.0

0.5 0.5

0.0
0.0
10 15 20 25 30
10 15 20 25 30
Time (minutes after birth)
Time (minutes after birth)

Figure 1 Newborn girls and boys crying after birth (Mean, SEM), in skin-to-
Figure 3 Mothers and fathers finger-tipping touch (mean, SEM) of the newborn
skin-contact with either parent.
infant after birth.

3.5
Mothers
(95% CI 5.05–15.175) in SSC with girls and for a mean of
Fathers
3.0 7 min (95% CI 2.865–11.635) in SSC with boys. However,
Minutes of touching (mean)

there was a significant negative correlation between the


2.5 amount of smiling in fathers and the crying in newborn girls
(Rs = )0.601, p = 0.005).
2.0

Kissing
1.5
No significant differences were found between mothers
kissing their infants and fathers kissing their infants
1.0
(p = 0.922). In SSC, five (5 ⁄ 17) of the mothers and six
0.5 (6 ⁄ 20) of the fathers kissed the newborn during the first
30 min after birth. For mothers, this behaviour occurred for
0.0 a mean of 1.5 min (95% CI 0.26–2.74); and for fathers, it
10 15 20 25 30 occurred for a mean of 1.75 min (95% CI 0.77–2.73).
Time (minutes after birth)
Speech directed towards the newborn
Figure 2 Minutes of parental touching of the newborn infant after birth (mean,
The mothers length of directed speech towards the newborn
SEM), when mother or father having the infant in skin-to-skin contact.
girls and boys did not differ (df = 1, F = 0.349, p = 0.558).
When directed at newborn girls, this behaviour occurred for
a mean of 2.68 min (95% CI 0.24–5.125); when directed at
(df = 1, F = 26.701, p £ 0.01) (Fig. 3). The sex of the new- boys, it occurred for a mean of 2.76 min (95% CI )1 to 6.5).
borns did not influence this behaviour in mothers (df = 1, In fact, fathers directed significantly less speech towards
F = 2.048, p = 0.173) or in fathers (df = 1, F = 0.959, girls than towards boys, (df = 1, F = 4.481, p = 0.042). It
p = 0.341). occurred for a mean of 2.5 min (95% CI 1.055–3.835)
towards girls and for a mean of 5.6 min (95% CI 1.67–9.53)
Smiling towards boys.
Mothers in SSC smiled significantly less during the observa-
tion period than fathers in SSC (df = 1, F = 14.785,
p = 0.001). When this behaviour was analysed separately to DISCUSSION
determine differences between mothers smiling at girls and Newborns’ first breastfeeding appeared significantly earlier
mothers smiling at boys, no significant difference was found when in SSC with the mother compared with when in SSC
(df = 1, F = 0.403, p = 0.53). Mothers’ smiling occurred for a with the father. Girls performed rooting and breast-massag-
mean of 2 min (95% CI 0.09–3.91) in SSC with girls and for a ing movements earlier than boys, and they also cried more
mean of 2.25 min (95% CI )2.52 to 7.02) in SSC with boys. than boys.
No significant differences were found between the length Mothers provided more tactile stimulation to their new-
of time fathers smiled at girls or at boys (df = 1, F = 0.028, borns, while fathers engaged more in vocal communication
p = 0.868). Fathers’ smiling occurred for a mean of 10 min in particular with their newborn boys.

364 ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367
Velandia et al. Sex differences in infant–parent interaction

Differences in the first breast-feeding behaviours may play a role in the formation of bonding and
Previous studies performed with nonmedicated normally attachment between parents and their infants and vice
delivered mothers in SSC with their infants demonstrate versa.
that the infants express an innate breast-seeking behaviour Fathers as well as newly delivered mothers start to touch
and start to breastfeed within an hour after birth (3,16). the newborn by examining the infant with their hands and
However, in the present study, which involved infant–par- fingertips (14). In this study, a difference between the ways
ent interactions after Caesarean section, the first breastfeed- fathers and mothers touched their newborns was observed.
ing was delayed for more than 1 h, compared with infants Mothers approached and explored the newborn with finger
delivered vaginally (3,16). In other studies, the occurrence tipping, whereas the father more commonly touched the
of the first breastfeeding has been shown to be delayed in infant with the palm of his hand. A specific finding was that
infants subjected to Caesarean section (17,18). In these mothers in SSC touched their newborn boys significantly
studies, the time for the first breastfeeding most often more than they touched their girls.
includes a postponed SSC between mother and infant, In the present study, fathers in SSC with their newborns
whereas in the present study, SSC was initiated immediately directed significantly more speech towards their newborn
after birth. boys than they did towards their newborn girls. The
The delay of the first breastfeeding might be associated increased parental interaction towards boys, i.e. the moth-
with a reduced alertness and wish to suck because of lower ers increased touch and the fathers increased speech, might
levels of catecholamines and possibly a reduced secretion of also have made them cry less. It has been suggested that
oxytocin in the infants delivered by Caesarean section, vocalization may be as important as touch in the activation
when compared to infants delivered vaginally (19). Factors of neuroendocrine mechanisms involved in the regulation
related to the mother such as reduced oxytocin secretion or of social bonding in humans (26).
other types of stress related to the Caesarean section and the The effects observed in parents and their infants during
spinal anaesthesia can of course not be excluded (20,21). SSC may in part be caused by activation of sensory nerves
Infants’ first breastfeeding appeared significantly earlier and a consequent release of oxytocin (27). Touch, warmth,
when the newborns had been in SSC with the mother than stroking and light pressure have been shown to stimulate
with the father. This result demonstrates that SSC with the oxytocin release and to induce oxytocin-related effects (28).
mother during the first 30 min after birth facilitates breast- Skin-to-skin contact may therefore via the activation of sen-
feeding. sory nerves stimulate oxytocin release in brain areas
involved in the control of different aspects of social interac-
Differences in newborn girls’ and boys’ behaviour tive behaviour. This interpretation is supported by the find-
Some differences between newborn girls’ and boys’ behav- ings that oxytocin administered as nasal spray in humans
iour when in SSC during the first 30 min after birth were has been demonstrated to stimulate pro-social behaviour,
found. Newborn girls rooted and performed breast-massag- increase eye gaze and increase the sensitivity to sensory
ing movements earlier than boys irrespective of being in cues (29). Also, the calming effects of SSC may be related to
SSC with mothers or fathers. This finding is in line with pre- oxytocin release, as administration of oxytocin also has
vious observations showing that newborn girls and female been shown to reduce anxiety and to decrease stress levels
foetuses perform more mouth movements (often associated (28). Also, other sensory cues such as visual, auditory and
with rooting movements) than boys (22,23). olfactory cues contribute to the activation of oxytocin
Such inborn behavioural differences have been attributed release and oxytocin-mediated effects (26,28).
to differential actions by the sex steroids oestrogen and ⁄ or Studies are in progress to elucidate oxytocin release pat-
testosterone on the foetal brain. It has e.g. been shown that terns in mothers and fathers with or without SSC in the
the greater fine motor skills (22,23) and the enhanced sen- immediate postpartum period.
sory sensitivity (11,24) and females’ preference of faces and Our result suggests the existence of some sex-related
face recognition (12) are linked to prenatally induced effects effects in the behaviour of newborn boys and girls and also
of oestrogen (25). in the mothers’ and fathers’ interaction with the newborn.
In this study, girls were shown to cry more than boys, The differences observed between the behaviour of new-
especially when in SSC with their mothers. The reason for born girls and boys in the immediate postnatal period are
this is not known. One possible reason could be that the most likely to a great extent inborn and caused by prenatal
newborn girls react more than boys to the unnatural and effects of sex steroids on the brain in utero. The differences
unpleasant smell of chlorhexidine used to disinfect the observed in the type of interaction with the newborn
mother’s breast, as girls are more sensitive to some olfactory expressed by mothers and fathers and also the modification
cues (24). Another possible reason could be that the par- of the behaviour by the sex of the newborn may represent
ents’ differential behaviour towards boys and girls has influ- inborn sex-specific behaviour (30).
enced the newborns’ behaviour. Interestingly, the finding of different behaviours towards
boys and girls is consistent with studies on maternal behav-
Differences in parental interaction with their newborn iour in rats, showing that maternal touching behaviours
Both fathers and mothers showed spontaneous affectionate (grooming) were sexually dimorphic, with males receiving
feelings; they smiled and kissed the newborn. These types of more touch than females during the neonatal period (31).

ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367 365
Sex differences in infant–parent interaction Velandia et al.

Methodological considerations and limitations of the References


study
1. Nissen E, Lilja G, Widstrom AM, Uvnas-Moberg K. Elevation
The findings reported in this study are unique but should be of oxytocin levels early post partum in women. Acta Obstet
interpreted with caution because of the small sample size. Gynecol Scand 1995; 74: 530–3.
To be able to draw conclusions, we used strict inclusion cri- 2. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Neuroendocrinology of the mother-child
teria limiting variance in background, obstetrical data, interaction. Trends Endocrinol Metab 1996; 7: 126–31.
infant status and birthweight. Reducing the variation in 3. Widstrom AM, Lilja G, Aaltomaa-Michalias P, Dahllöf A, Lin-
tula M, Nissen E. Newborn behaviour to locate the breast when
background variables to control the influence of potentially
skin-to-skin: a possible method for enabling early self-regula-
confounding variables in a randomized trial prior to ran- tion. Acta Paediatr 2010; 100: 79–85.
domization strengthens the result when the sample is small. 4. Varendi H, Porter RH. Breast odour as the only maternal stimu-
Additionally, all comparisons were planned, and no post lus elicits crawling towards the odour source. Acta Paediatr
hoc comparisons were made. Some results did not reach 2001; 90: 372–5.
statistical significant differences, probably due to a too small 5. Christensson K, Siles C, Moreno L, Belaustequi A, De La
sample size (Type I error) even though the results pointed Fuente P, Lagercrantz H, et al. Temperature, metabolic adapta-
tion and crying in healthy full-term newborns cared for skin-to-
in the same direction as in previous studies (22,23). In the
skin or in a cot. Acta Paediatr 1992; 81: 488–93.
present study, as a result of the small sample size and a non- 6. Velandia M, Matthisen AS, Uvnas-Moberg K, Nissen E. Onset
parametric distribution of certain variables, nonparametric of vocal interaction between parents and newborns in skin-to-
statistical analysis has been performed. We suggest that this skin contact immediately after elective Caesarean section. Birth
is a hypothesis-generating study, as a part of the research 2010; 37: 192–201.
process and larger studies are needed. 7. Erlandsson K, Christensson K, Fagerberg I. Fathers’ lived expe-
The data collection was extended in time; however, no rience of getting to know their baby while acting as primary
caregivers immediately following birth. J Perinat Educ 2008;
changes took place either in the surgical techniques or in
17: 28–36.
the use of pharmaceuticals or in the operating theatre 8. Grossmann K, Thane K, Grossmann KE. Maternal tactual con-
setting. tact of the newborn after various postpartum conditions of
mother–infant contact. Dev Psychol 1981; 17: 158–69.
9. Hines M. Sex-related variation in human behavior and the
CONCLUSIONS AND CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14: 448–56. Epub 2010 Aug 18.
The result highlights the importance of immediate infant– 10. Berenbaum SA, Beltz AM. Sexual differentiation of human
behavior: effects of prenatal and pubertal organizational hor-
parent SSC, even after a Caesarean section, and provides an mones. Front Neuroendocrinol 2011; 32:183–200. Epub 2011
increased understanding for sex differences in infants’ and Mar 17.
parents’ behaviour. Mothers showed more tactile stimula- 11. Bell RQ, Costello NS. Three tests for sex differences in tactile
tion, while fathers engaged more in speech, in particular sensitivity in the newborn. Biol Neonat 1964; 7: 335–47.
directed to the newborn boy. The fathers showed more signs 12. Connellan J, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Batki A,
of affection and communicated more when in SSC with Ahluwalia J. Sex differences in human neonatal social
perception. Infant Behav Dev 2000; 23: 113–8.
their newborn compared with mothers in SSC with their
13. Cohen-Bendahan C, van de Beek C, Berenbaum SA. Prenatal
newborn. Girls cried less when in SSC with the father than sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior:
with the mother. methods and findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005; 29:
Skin-to-skin contact with the mother accelerated the time 353–84.
point for the infants’ first breastfeed as compared to SSC 14. Eidelman AI, Hovars R, Kaitz M. Comparative tactile behavior
with the father. Our data support that uninterrupted SSC of mothers and fathers with their newborn infants. Isr J Med Sci
with the mother facilitates the initiation of breastfeeding in 1994; 30: 79–82.
15. Erickson F. Audiovisual records as a primary data source.
the mother–infant dyad exposed to a planned Caesarean
Sociological Methods & Research 1982; 11, No. 2: 213–32.
section. Michigan State University.
In case the mother is unable to provide SSC immediately 16. Righard L, Alade MO. Effect of delivery room routines on suc-
after birth, the father is a valuable alternative, because SSC cess of first breast-feed. Lancet 1990; 336: 1105–7.
promotes his interaction with the newborn infant. 17. Chalmers B, Kaczorowski J, Darling E, Heaman M, Fell DB,
O’Brien B, et al. Cesarean and vaginal birth in Canadian
women: a comparison of experiences. Birth 2010; 37: 44–9.
18. Rowe-Murray H, Fisher J. Baby friendly hospital practices: cae-
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
sarean section is a persistent barrier to early initiation of breast-
This study was supported by grants from Vardalsstiftelsen, feeding. Birth 2001; 29: 124–30.
the Swedish Research Council, (K 1999-27P-13085-01A 19. Faxelius G, Irestedt L, Lagercrantz H, Lundell B, Persson B.
and K2001-27P-13085-036); the Swedish Order of Freema- Catecholamine surge and metabolic adaptation in the newborn
son; the Mjölkdroppen Foundation in Stockholm; the after vaginal delivery and caesarean section. Acta Paediatr
Swedish Foundation for Health Care Sciences the Solstic- Scand 1984; 73: 602–9.
20. Ransjo-Arvidson AB, Matthiesen AS, Lilja G, Nissen E, Wid-
kan Foundation, Strategic Research Program for in Care
ström AM, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Maternal analgesia during labor
Sciences, Karolinska Institutet. We would like to greatly disturbs newborn behaviours: effects on breastfeeding, temper-
thank Ann-Sofi Matthiesen for statistical advice and Gud- ature, and crying. Birth 2001; 28: 5–12.
run Sandgren for helpful transcription of videotapes.

366 ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367
Velandia et al. Sex differences in infant–parent interaction

21. Nissen E, Uvnas-Moberg K, Svensson K, Stock S, Widström 27. Loken LS, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F, Olausson H.
AM, Winberg J. Different patterns of oxytocin, prolactin but not Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans.
cortisol release during breastfeeding in women delivered by Nat Neurosci 2009; 12: 547–8.
caesarean section or by the vaginal route. Early Hum Dev 1996; 28. Uvnäs-Moberg K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of posi-
45: 103–18. tive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinolo-
22. Korner AF. Sex differences in newborns with special reference gy 1998; 23: 819–35.
to differences in the organization of oral behaviours. J Child 29. Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G. Oxytocin, vasopressin,
Psychol Psychiatry 1973; 14: 19–29. and human social behaviours. Front Neuroendocrinol 2009; 30:
23. Hepper PG, Shannon EA, Dornan JC. Sex differences in fetal 548–57. Epub 2009 Jun 6.
mouth movements. Lancet 1997; 350: 1820. 30. Moffatt CA. Steroid hormone modulation of olfactory process-
24. Varendi H, Porter RH, Winberg J. Natural odour preferences of ing in the context of socio-sexual behaviours in rodent and
newborn infants change over time. Acta Paediatr 1997; 86: humans. Brain Res Rev 2003; 43: 192–206.
985–90. 31. Kurian JR, Olesen KM, Auger AP. Sex differences in epigenetic
25. McEwen BS. Invited review: estrogens effects on the brain: regulation of the estrogen receptor-alpha promoter within the
multiple sites and molecular mechanisms. J Appl Physiol 2001; developing preoptic area. Endocrinology 2010; 151: 2297–305.
91: 2785–801. Epub 2010 Mar 17.
26. Seltzer LJ, Ziegler TE, Pollak SD. Social vocalizations can
release oxytocin in humans. Proc Biol Sci 2010; 277: 2661–6.
Epub 2010 May 12.

ª2011 The Author(s)/Acta Pædiatrica ª2011 Foundation Acta Pædiatrica 2012 101, pp. 360–367 367
Copyright of Acta Paediatrica is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed
to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However,
users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen