Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

Childhood Education

ISSN: 0009-4056 (Print) 2162-0725 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uced20

Searching for Deeper Meaning in Children's


Drawings

Cathleen S. Soundy

To cite this article: Cathleen S. Soundy (2012) Searching for Deeper Meaning in Children's
Drawings, Childhood Education, 88:1, 45-51, DOI: 10.1080/00094056.2012.643718

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2012.643718

Published online: 13 Jan 2012.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 3561

View related articles

Citing articles: 5 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uced20
Searching for
© Vinicius Tupinamba

Deeper Meaning
in Children’s
Drawings
by Cathleen S. Soundy

C
Cathleen S. Soundy is Associate Professor, Curriculum Instruction and
Technology in Education,Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.

hildren delight in “giving voice” to their drawings. “Their pictures can be worth a thousand words, if
you let them tell you their story.” This reconstructed adage, offered by a preservice teacher, points to the
inextricable way that language is interwoven into the artistic lives of young children. If provided the
opportunity, children can express powerful and imaginative ideas and make meaning through visual and verbal modes. When
adults spend time talking with children about their artwork, they see glimpses of imagination at work, as well as effective uses
of language. One poignant example emerged as a small group of kindergartners participated in a read-aloud activity in which
they were asked to draw a picture in response to The Snowy Day (Keats, 1962). The following excerpt from a preservice teach-
er’s journal describes how her interaction with one child led to an improved understanding of the child’s graphic choices.

When 5-year-old Emily began her drawing, she said, “I’m drawing a night picture.” Moments later, when finished, she
requested help in writing her caption to “Snowmen at Night.” Looking at her drawing, I was puzzled. Emily was a
bright child—very creative, verbal, and artistically talented—so why, I wondered, had she resorted to scribbling when she
could have easily produced the snow-covered landscapes and scenes of children at play that were being created by the other
kindergartners in her small group? (See Figure 1.) I asked Emily to tell me more about her picture. “Well,” she said, “It’s
about the night before the boy makes the snowman in the snow.” Intrigued by her comment, but still not understanding,
I inquired, “What are these brown lines?” Emily responded, “Sticks; the little boy needs to make a snowman.” As I sat in
silence, gazing at this abstract piece of art, Emily continued, “The little boy needs sticks to make tracks . . . the little boy
needs sticks to smack the snow-covered tree. And the little boy needs a place to build the snowman.” She pointed to the
lighter shade of blue space in the lower part of the picture frame and announced, “This is the spot where Peter will build
the snowman. This is the brightest spot under the moon.”
Oh, I thought, this all makes sense, and wow, this thinking
is a little “out on a limb.” I was truly moved by this small
child’s vision, and felt strongly reassured that Emily was in-
deed the creative, highly verbal girl that I had seen in action
during earlier visits to the classroom. Now I can add “vi-
sionary” to the list of words I will use to characterize Emily.

Ms. Morrison, the preservice teacher in the opening vignette,


experienced a child’s dynamic, meaning-making process.
Unfortunately, many images produced by children are not
amenable to adult interpretation. Emily’s artwork lacked
recognizable attributes and features, and she adopted a rather
unconventional way of depicting her ideas. But when Ms.
Morrison entered into a dialogue about the context of the
drawing, Emily explained how the various marks were distin-
Figure 1 guished and she ascribed meaning. When invited to be inter-

January/February 2012 / 45
preters of their drawings, children are able to com- ward process. Although practitioners often provide
municate about novel and highly personal imagery time for children to create art, most feel that they
(Hurwitz & Carroll, 2008). Such conversations lack the requisite artistic and aesthetic training, as
place teachers in a better position to understand well as the higher-level interpretive skills, necessary
children’s thought processes, extend their meaning to talk with children and guide their learning about
through interaction, and contribute to their future art (Eglinton, 2003). In order to help teachers
development as visual meaning-makers. understand children’s responses to literature and
This article will examine the drawings that chil- the ideas on which their expressions are based, it is
dren produce in response to read-alouds. The focus important to consider the ways that children arrive
of interest is on the variety of ways that children at the images they draw.
construct meaning and use imaginative and meta- Art activities have the potential to help individu-
phoric thinking in their drawings. The goal is to als think. Literal and metaphoric responses can
move early educators beyond reading images liter- be tools for intellectual discovery (Egan, 2005).
ally, or responding to the surface spectacle, and into Children experience strong emotions, and “how
engaging them in joint attention episodes critical to they feel about a particular object, person, or event
the process of discovering the underlying organiza- is reflected in the way they treat it in a drawing”
tion and deeper meaning of children’s drawings. (Eisner, 2002, p. 113). In addition, children act on
impulses as they draw, accompanied by dramatic
What Does the Literature Say? gestures and speech. Many researchers have com-
Sensitive educators know intuitively that children’s mented on the striking similarity between children’s
talk informs the nature and content of their work drawings and early “role-play” or make-believe play
and helps clarify their intentions and processes of (Cooke, Griffin, & Cox, 1998; Hopperstad, 2008).
thinking. But adults may not realize that the ways Interconnected moments of thinking, feeling, and
children treat the material and the features of the acting often spark innovative ideas and creative
images they create are not solely determined by responses that can be given form through various
stages of artistic development. Early childhood modes of symbolic representation (Wright, 2010).
educators need to be prepared to include the art- As young artists respond to and produce their
ist’s purpose in discussions as well as to find ways to own visual texts, they intuitively improvise and add
stimulate children’s imaginations and metaphoric decorative elements to their artwork. Some indi-
fluency (Egan, 1999). Thus, teachers must learn to viduals turn to designing or playing “visual games”
see children as competent individuals whose views, and engaging in intricate pattern-making activities
actions, and choices, as well as visual representa- (Kolbe, 2007). These children enjoy exploring
tions, are of value. “repetitions, symmetries, and divisions of spaces
Drawing affords young children with a means to into even smaller spaces” (Kolbe, 2005, p. 30) and
express real and imaginary ideas, a form of commu- orienting the picture toward pattern rather than
nication that is worthy of teacher recognition and toward action. On other occasions, youngsters elect
support. Unfortunately, as Wright (2010) notes, to use drawing as a mechanism for telling stories
“Many traditional systems of schooling suppress (Gallas, 2003). Sometimes, children narrate these
children’s free composing through drawing in favour experiences through internal dialogues; other times,
of teaching children the more rule-bound, struc- they speak openly with peers and adults. Either
tured symbol systems of numeracy and literacy” (p. way, the talk that accompanies drawing has the
7). Teachers tend to focus their time and energies power to enhance meaning (Hopperstad, 2008).
on three critical categories of early literacy—oral During the early years, “Children are commu-
language, phonological awareness, and print knowl- nicators who have a natural desire to discover and
edge—areas that have been identified in recent communicate what they know, understand, wonder
reviews of research as essential to early literacy in- about, question, feel, and imagine” (Pitri, 2007, p.
struction (Roskos, Christie, & Richgels, 2003). 34). Unfortunately, few frameworks exist for exam-
In general, spoken and written languages have ining children’s artwork or the processes that reveal
been considered the dominant system of meaning communicative intentions and differing purposes
used in schools, with the visual mode often rel- for drawing. In this exploratory study, the author
egated to the role of supporting written text (Kress, attempts to make sense of this rich diversity of
1997). Little attention has been paid to analyz- meaning-making by creating a classification system
ing and understanding visual images (Kendrick & that presents a way for preservice teachers to un-
McKay, 2009), perhaps, in part, because ascribing derstand students’ visual responses to picturebooks
meaning to children’s artwork is not a straightfor- with a more critical and informed eye.

46 \ Childhood Education
Categories of Childhood Experience from preservice teachers’ journal entries related to
Using grounded theory techniques, the author informal conversations with the children; reflective
developed categories of childhood experience comments; and the children’s writings, drawings,
that were evident in the drawings. This typology and “tellings.” The preservice teachers related their
evolved from the author’s examination of over 450 findings to the categories by identifying the major
pieces of artwork from a larger study produced by purpose of their respective children’s drawings. The
kindergarten and 1st-grade children in response to examples featured in the next section appear in the
The Snowy Day and Snowmen at Night (Buehner, respective category that best represents the child’s
2002), two picturebooks about snowy landscapes. purpose for drawing.
Five categories of childhood experience were identi-
fied and used to illuminate the nature of children’s Scenes From the Classroom
drawings: 1) Thinking—Using Verbal Memory, 2) The artwork in this article was selected from a set of
Feeling—Expressing Emotions, 3) Acting—Moving 120 drawings produced by children ranging from
Into the Drawing, 4) Creating—Making Designs 5 to 7 years. The five drawings presented for com-
and Patterns, and 5) Narrating—Telling a Story. mentary were chosen based on the perception that
The goal was to expose preservice teachers to a tool the works were highly representative of the respec-
for discussing children’s finished drawings as well tive category, and were the most interesting and
as for understanding the creative and conceptual typical of the age group under study. They served
thinking that guides its development. as models for explaining how young children create
This emerging set of categories was used by and express meaning through art and design work.
undergraduate students who were enrolled in the A foundation for thinking about these categorical
author’s early childhood courses. Although some themes is provided and attention is given to the met-
overlapping of purposes existed, initial student aphorical, analogical nature of children’s artwork.
feedback indicated that the categories worked rea-
sonably well in identifying and describing all of the The Categorical Framework
examples that were encountered. But the number
of drawings was small and some refinement to the inking—Using Visual Memory. Picture-
Th
category system will likely occur over time. During books provide an array of beautiful illustrations and
the course of this study, however, students were lim- may offer a way for children to visualize a full and
ited to the five categories. rich picture in their mind’s eye. Anning’s (1997)
research on visual systems of meaning shows that
Context of the Study “from the earliest stages of drawing, children can
This article explores children’s artistic responses, demonstrate remarkable powers of accurate repre-
along with written and verbal expressions, to gain sentation of what they have seen and learned” (p.
insight into how kindergarten and 1st-grade chil- 230). When a child thinks about a favorite scene
dren use imaginative and metaphorical thinking. from the book, the image he sees in his mind’s eye
One task, the focus of this article, was a research exists in his thinking (Efland, 2002). The following
assignment that asked preservice teachers to con- excerpt from a preservice teacher’s journal illustrates
duct a read-aloud of The Snowy Day and to observe how one child held an image of her favorite scene in
two children drawing, writing, and speaking about her mind’s eye as she created her picture.
their choice of a favorite scene from the book. The
theme, “Celebrate Winter,” was chosen because the When Victoria drew the mountain scene from The Snowy
study took place in February and snow was a topic Day, she saw no need to converse or talk her way through
that participants could easily recall through previ- the process. She approached the task with an idea already
ous experiences. formed about what she wanted to express. Victoria drew en-
The interpretations of the classroom reality were tirely from memory, “out of her head.” Her finished picture
accessed directly through field notes written by the bore a visual likeness to the mountain scene illustrated on the
preservice teachers during the instructional phase of last double-page layout. (See Figure 2.) Victoria completed
the assignment. Small-group, follow-up discussions her project by writing, “It is very cold outside. It (snow) is
of the children’s work samples occurred during very deep.” Victoria responded “No” when I asked her if she
regular class time near the end of the semester. In wanted to tell me anything else about her picture.
analyzing the data, the author and her students used As I reflected on Victoria’s finished drawing, I continued
a qualitative, interpretative research approach with to think about how much her image looked like the one in
a specific focus on image-based research (Kress & the picturebook. Victoria’s ability to re-create the scene in
van Leeuwen, 1996). The evidence included notes the book was quite good, especially considering that the il-

January/February 2012 / 47
Figure 3
Figure 2

lustrations were not available to her during the drawing their art (Jolley, 2010). Picard, Brechet, and Baldy
phase. Victoria accessed her short-term memory to re-create (2007) studied children’s use of expressive strategies
the overall configuration of the mountain scene. Her finished and found that the children typically used facial ex-
drawing revealed that she had absorbed the organizational pressions and body positions to provide literal cues.
structure and spatial relationships created by the illustrator. Support for identifying non-literal aspects of expres-
Most remarkably, she seemed attuned to the emotional power sion also have appeared in the literature. Eisner
of the scene. By including the short platform at the bottom (2002) states, “What is important is often exagger-
of the page, Victoria was able to position Peter and his friend ated, what is important is often made more visible,
in an open doorway and to re-create the illusion of wonder what is important secures a prominence on the paper
felt by the boys as they looked out on a winter landscape that that confers on it the significance they want to ex-
was fresh, new, and bright. Although Victoria was probably press” (p. 113).
not fully conscious of the particular emotion embodied in the The drawing chosen to exemplify the second
artist’s powerful imagery, she no doubt chose to reproduce the category features the work of a child who is quite
image that held meaning for her—on both an intellectual capable of transmitting feelings through an artistic
and an emotional level. channel of communication. The image of this par-
ticular snowman displays the qualities of vitality and
An analysis of Victoria’s drawing showed that exuberance indicative of children’s highly expressive
some children are adept at picturing visual images abilities. The following reflection from a student
and “reading out” this imagery in the form of draw- teacher’s journal provides insight on a child who
ing. Victoria’s drawing was highly imitative of a seemed more intent on expressing emotion rather
scene from the picturebook. In her role as meaning- than replicating visual likeness to the graphic model.
maker, Victoria used her visual memory for color,
form, and content, as well as her developed graphic Donald’s drawing is a compelling piece of art, an expression of
skills, to respond to The Snowy Day and to produce a pure joy. (See Figure 3.) Donald used the snowman’s face to
drawing with a high level of visual realism. portray emotion and he drew an upwardly curving mouth as
a symbol of happiness. He also created a metaphorical form
Feeling—Expressing Emotion. Teachers have of expression. The snowman’s stance and the angle of his arms
long recognized the power of picturebooks to awak- is one clear example. The elongated arms that reach into the
en feelings and emotions. But practitioners may sky may be representing Donald’s experience of reaching for
not have realized that children approach artwork joy. The “thumbs up” gesture, signaling “way to go,” adds to
already responsive to an inner world of feelings and the sense of liveliness. Even the snowman’s location in space
psychological moods (Golomb, 2004). Although provides relevant information. The snowman is suspended in
certain human qualities can be difficult to com- air, not touching the baseline. Donald completed his project
municate, children disclose emotions and attitudes by saying, “The snowman is jumping for joy, but I don’t know
in many ways, and even very young children seem how to write it.” He left the writing space blank. When I
particularly adept at producing expressive content in asked Donald to tell me more about his drawing, he respond-

48 \ Childhood Education
ed, “I drew a happy snowman. The snowman is having fun. picture. I drew the boy red to look like Peter and the other
Wheee. . . . He is jumping for joy. I liked the part when he to look like me. I don’t have a snow outfit like he did. My
bounced in the air.” mom lets me play in the snow, but I don’t have a snow outfit.
Peter and I are looking at the snow on the mountains. It is
Clearly, children have the capacity to reveal feel- beautiful. The snow is falling on our heads (gestures by cov-
ings within the very form and content of their ering her head with her hands). I am having fun walking
work. Their drawings often contain a symbolic and with Peter. I like to do that, too.”
communicative purpose. Donald’s snowman func-
tioned similarly to a metaphor in that it was repre- By virtually moving into the action zone, children
sentative of an intangible emotional state. He used become agents with storybook characters and
both literal and nonliteral qualities in his drawing directly identify with events, images, or anything
to convey expressions of joy and excitement. else that attracts their interest. Tatianna’s picture
Acting—Moving Into the Drawing. Picture- demonstrates how she took on a role in her own
books have the potential to become a source of in- drawing and acted out meaning. Her motivation
spiration for young readers’ active learning and cre- may have stemmed from a desire to join Peter in his
ative interactions. Kolczyski (1989) states, “When exploration of the snowy landscape.
children become engaged in a story, character, or Creating—Making Designs and Patterns. Il-
situation, they often form an identification with lustrations in high-quality picturebooks are valued
aspects of the story that hold personal meaning for for their aesthetic pleasure. Children admire these
them” (p. 79). Children move “inside the experi- pictures as pleasantly decorative compositions
ence” by using first-person narrative to talk to them- (Nodelman, 1988); at a very early age, they also
selves or others, and they attempt to weave events begin to exhibit an interest in applying decora-
around the marks being made, almost as a parallel to tion and making designs and patterns in their own
pretend play (Wright, 2010). As this study reveals, drawings (Hope, 2008). Cooke, Griffin, and Cox
it’s not unusual for children to become active par- (1998) found that children make shapes “for their
ticipants and imaginatively enter the picture frame. own sake, with the artist taking delight in pattern,
The following excerpt from a student teacher’s color and texture” (p. 10). Kolbe (2005) discusses
journal highlights how one child “slipped into the children’s desire for visual order and states that
picture” and became a character on the page. they take “pleasure in arranging things—whether
lining up pebbles or tiny toys in a row, or arrang-
Tatianna placed herself next to the main character in the ing lines, shapes and colors on a sheet of paper” (p.
mountain scene. When it came time to write, she simply
labeled the characters as “Me and Peter.” (See Figure 4.)
In essence, her purpose was to step into the picture frame so
she could interact with Peter and experience the beauty of
the falling snow. Tatianna’s casual story-like unfolding of
events indicates that she is living in the literary experience
of the moment. When I asked her to tell me more about her
picture, she readily supplied additional details: “I am in the

Figure 4 Figure 5

January/February 2012 / 49
29). The following excerpt from a student teacher’s win, 2009). Research on narrative development
journal explains how one child took delight in demonstrates that “telling stories helps children
decorating her composition. frame their thoughts, emotions, and social cultural
identity” (Curenton, 2006, p. 78). Children fre-
Five-year-old Rayna showed strong imagination and interest quently tell stories and draw pictures about their
in including patterns in her unfinished composition. Rayna experiences—both lived and imagined (Wright,
playfully distributed the elements of color and form across the 2010). Golomb (2004) states that “the act of draw-
page. Skills of visual improvisation were used when Rayna ing becomes a vehicle for the expression of personal
drew the oval-shaped hills. She applied multi-layers of colors fantasies” (p. 161), and visual narratives often point
between the hills to communicate her excitement in being to children’s themes of interest, such as contests be-
able to freely explore the outdoors. (See Figure 5.) Peter tween opposing forces of good and evil—of victory
is drawn as a simple tadpole person, with his arms and legs and defeat. The following excerpt from a student
radiating outward from his head. Rayna talked as she drew teacher’s journal shows how one child begins to tell
and her words provided insight to her underlying thoughts: a story based on events from The Snowy Day, but
“I like using all the colors. My favorite part is when Peter then proceeds to weave a new, personal narrative
went on the hills. He could go anywhere he wanted.” Rayna out of the fantasy and play it inspires.
carried through with an equally high level of interest in color
as she wrote about her picture. Each word of her written text Michael drew a snow-covered mountain and then added
was printed in a different color: “My favorite part was when two teams of boys engaged in a snowball fight. (See Figure
he went on the hill because I liked when . . . he went with 6.) He thought about writing the caption “The snowball
his friend.” As the activity neared closure, I asked Rayna if fight” under his picture, but then declined. He was much
she would like to tell me anything else about her picture. She more interested in talking about the event. Michael told
responded, “Peter is getting ready for the steep hills. He is his version of the story, explaining that the people dressed
doing jumping jacks to warm-up.” Rayna didn’t have time in blue represented Peter and the good guys. Michael even
to finish her picture because she was so intent on coloring the pointed to Peter and to each person, identified his respective
spaces between the mountains. team, and provided detail on who started the fight. Here
are some of his words: “The other guys, in purple, are bad
Children often let their minds roam freely as guys and they started a snowball fight with Peter and his
they work to understand their world. Rayna’s style friends. Peter and his friends win, but only after a boy from
of drawing was influenced by her playful explora- the purple team falls off the mountain. He gets hurt and
tion with color and repetitive forms within her leaves the fight. The other guys start throwing bigger snow-
picture as part of the total image. The aesthetic balls and they win.”
pleasure derived from arranging horizontal layers of
colors between the hills may have enhanced Rayna’s Children often combine spoken, written, and
visual creativity. visual representation to communicate that some-
thing has happened. While some children’s visual
Narrating—Telling a Story. Picturebooks play a and verbal narratives display a progression of events
central role in enabling young readers to understand from a clear beginning and on to a satisfactory end-
narrative structure, character, and setting (Good- ing, Michael chose to represent partial aspects of
experience in order to better understand the charac-
ters and the main event. He used knowledge from
his real life and from the picturebook to weave his
story elements together and create a new ending.

Conclusion
As with any form of communication, drawing can
be influenced by many aspects of the child’s home
background or cultural life, as well as by experi-
ences in the classroom setting. Children’s graphic
strategies, communicative intentions, and differing
purposes for drawing provide teachers with mul-
tiple vantage points from which to view children’s
creativity and efforts to make meaning. Close
observation of the drawing activity itself, as well as
Figure 6 of the talk and writing that accompanies it, reveals

50 \ Childhood Education
how children become interested in a topic and mo- Teachers College Press.
tivated to produce their inner ideas and thoughts. Golomb, C. (2004). The child’s creation of a pictorial
The importance of feelings or affect and desires world (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
to act out or dramatize an event through drawing Goodwin, P. (2009). Developing understanding of nar-
must not be underestimated, because these differ- rative, empathy and inference through picturebooks.
ent forms of inspiration often become part of the In J. Evans (Ed.), Talking beyond the page: Reading and
complex mix of social and emotional forces at work. responding to picturebooks (pp. 152-167). London, UK:
Children tap into their own unique thoughts, feel- Routledge.
ings, and impulses, as well as an interest in aesthet- Hope, G. (2008). Thinking and learning through drawing
ics and telling stories, as they discover new strategies in primary classrooms. London, UK: Sage.
for communicating. Hopperstad, M. H. (2008). How children make mean-
The topology presented in this article can be ben- ing through drawing and play. Visual Communication,
eficial to educators as they talk with children about 7(1), 77-96.
their drawings and design work. By adopting a cat- Hurwitz, A., & Carroll, K. L. (2008). Studying the
egorical system with five components of childhood drawings. In A. Hurwitz & K. L. Carroll (Eds.),
experience—thinking, feeling, acting, creating, and Memory & experience: Thematic drawings by Qatari,
narrating—the author provided a framework for Taiwanese, and American children (pp. 193-199).
understanding the meaning-making capacities and Reston, VA: National Art Education Association.
the purposes of young children’s drawings. This Jolley, R. P. (2010). Children and pictures: Drawing and
classification, however artificial, can function as understanding. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
a pedagogical tool to initiate thinking about chil- Keats, E. J. (1962). The snowy day. New York, NY:
dren’s intentions and to examine both literal and Viking.
metaphoric levels of meaning. Until the field de- Kendrick, M. E., & McKay, R. A. (2009). Researching
velops sound techniques to analyze children’s visual literacy with young children’s drawings. In M. Narey
images and meaning-making capacities, exploratory (Ed.), Making meaning: Constructing multimodal per-
attempts must continue to be made to understand spectives of language, literacy, and learning through arts-
the dynamic and complex relationships between based early childhood education (pp. 53-70). New York,
verbal and visual modes of expression. NY: Springer.
Kolbe, U. (2005). It’s not a bird yet: The drama of draw-
References ing. Byron Bay, Australia: Peppinot.
Anning, A. (1997). Drawing out ideas: Graphicacy and Kolbe, U. (2007). Rapunsel’s supermarket: All about young
young children. International Journal of Technology and children and their art. Byron Bay, Australia: Peppinot.
Design Education, 7, 219-239. Kolczyski, R. G. (1989). Reading leads to writing. In
Buehner, C. (2002). Snowmen at night. New York, NY: J. W. Stewig & S. L. Sebesta (Eds.), Using literature
Dial. in the elementary classroom (pp. 75-90). Urbana, IL:
Cooke, G., Griffin, D., & Cox, M. (1998). Teaching National Council of Teachers of English.
young children to draw: Imaginative approaches to repre- Kress, G. (1997). Before writing: Rethinking the paths to
sentational drawing. London, UK: Falmer. literacy. London, UK: Routledge.
Curenton, S. M. (2006). Oral storytelling: A cultural Kress, G., & van Leeuwen, T. (1996). Reading im-
art that promotes school readiness. Young Children, ages: The grammar of visual design. London, UK:
61(5), 78-87. Routledge.
Efland, A. D. (2002). Art and cognition: Integrating the Nodelman, P. (1988). Words about pictures: The nar-
visual arts in the curriculum. New York, NY: Teachers rative art of children’s picture books. Athens, GA: The
College Press. University of Georgia Press.
Egan, K. (1999). Children’s minds, talking rabbits, and Picard, D., Brechet, C., & Baldy, R. (2007). Expressive
clockwork oranges: Essays on education. New York, NY: strategies in drawing are related to age and topic.
Teachers College Press. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 243-257.
Egan, K. (2005). An imaginative approach to teaching. Pitri, E. (2007). Art books for early childhood classroom
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. communication. Art Education, 60(2), 33-39.
Eglinton, K. A. (2003). Art in the early years. London, Roskos, K. A., Christie, J. F., & Richgels, D. J. (2003).
UK: RoutledgeFalmer. The essentials of early literacy instruction. Young
Eisner, E. W. (2002). The arts and the creation of mind. Children, 58(2), 52-60.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Wright, S. (2010). Understanding creativity in early
Gallas, K. (2003). Imagination and literacy: A teacher’s childhood: Meaning-making and children’s drawings.
search for the heart of learning. New York, NY: Los Angeles, CA: Sage.

January/February 2012 / 51

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen