Sie sind auf Seite 1von 18

Journal of Educational Psychology

Practicing Versus Inventing With Contrasting Cases: The


Effects of Telling First on Learning and Transfer
Daniel L. Schwartz, Catherine C. Chase, Marily A. Oppezzo, and Doris B. Chin
Online First Publication, August 22, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0025140

CITATION
Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., Oppezzo, M. A., & Chin, D. B. (2011, August 22). Practicing
Versus Inventing With Contrasting Cases: The Effects of Telling First on Learning and
Transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:
10.1037/a0025140
Journal of Educational Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association
2011, Vol. ●●, No. ●, 000 – 000 0022-0663/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025140

Practicing Versus Inventing With Contrasting Cases:


The Effects of Telling First on Learning and Transfer

Daniel L. Schwartz, Catherine C. Chase, Marily A. Oppezzo, and Doris B. Chin


Stanford University

Being told procedures and concepts before problem solving can inadvertently undermine the learning of
deep structures in physics. If students do not learn the underlying structure of physical phenomena, they
will exhibit poor transfer. Two studies on teaching physics to adolescents compared the effects of
“telling” students before and after problem solving. In Experiment 1 (N ⫽ 128), students in a tell-and-
practice condition were told the relevant concepts and formulas (e.g., density) before practicing on a set
of contrasting cases for each lesson. Students in an invent-with-contrasting-cases (ICC) condition had to
invent formulas using the same cases and were told only afterward. Both groups exhibited equal
proficiency at using the formulas on word problems. However, ICC students better learned the ratio
structure of the physical phenomena and transferred more frequently to semantically unrelated topics that
also had a ratio structure (e.g., spring constant). Experiment 2 (N ⫽ 120) clarified the sources of the
effects while showing that ICC benefited both low- and high-achieving students.

Keywords: transfer, science education, contrasting cases, inventing, proportional reasoning

In a review of the Third International Mathematics and Science However, there are unresolved debates on the timing of explicit
Study, Hiebert and Stigler (2004) noted that instruction in the instruction (see Kirschner, Sweller, & Clark, 2006; Tobias &
United States largely takes a form that we will label tell and Duffy, 2009). Sweller (1988), for instance, proposed that with-
practice (T&P). Teachers or texts first explain concepts and their holding explicit instruction incurs a needless cognitive load that
formulaic expression, and then students practice on a set of well- detracts from learning. In contrast, Bransford, Franks, Sherwood,
designed problems. It is a convenient and efficient way to deliver and Vye (1989) proposed that students first need to experience the
accumulated knowledge. Nevertheless, many scholars are working problems that render told knowledge useful. The persistence of
on instructional alternatives. Catrambone (1998) summarized a these debates has been attributed to the lack of experimental
prevailing concern with T&P: “Students tend to memorize the evidence (Mayer, 2009). One reason for the lack of evidence is that
details of how the equations are filled out rather than learning the T&P and guided discovery can differ on so many dimensions that
deeper, conceptual knowledge” (p. 356). it is difficult to maintain fidelity to their respective instructional
Many T&P alternatives use some form of guided discovery, models while isolating the causal differences between them.
including problem-based learning (Barrows & Tamblyn, 1976), For example, Schwartz and Bransford (1998) asked students to
project- and design-based activities (Barron et al., 1998), inquiry invent graphs to characterize simplified data sets from classic
(Edelson, Gordin, & Pea, 1999), and modeling (Lesh & Doeer, psychology experiments. The students learned and transferred
2003). The mechanics of these alternatives withhold didactic more from a subsequent lecture than other students who first
teaching at first, lest it undermine the processes of discovery. summarized an explicit chapter on the experiments and then heard
the same lecture. Schwartz and Bransford argued that there is a
“time for telling.” For novices, explicit instruction is effective if
they have been prepared with appropriate experiences. But if they
Daniel L. Schwartz, Catherine C. Chase, and Marily A. Oppezzo, have not been prepared, then explicit instruction provides much
School of Education, Stanford University; Doris B. Chin, Human-Sciences less value. Supporting this claim, Schwartz and Martin (2004)
and Technologies Advanced Research Institute, Stanford University. found that students who invented a variance formula for a set of
This material is based upon work supported by the National Science
specially designed cases exhibited superior transfer from a subse-
Foundation under grants SMA-0835854 & DRL-1020362. Any opinions,
findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material
quent worked example, as compared with students who completed
are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the T&P instruction before the worked example. In response to these
National Science Foundation. The authors thank Kimi Schmidt for help in findings, Sweller (as cited in Schwartz, Lindgren, & Lewis, 2009,
executing the empirical study; Dylan Arena for his help in the survey of the p. 53) correctly questioned
transfer literature; Melissa Gresalfi for her help in designing and imple-
menting the video protocol; and, Carl Wieman for encouraging us to the appropriateness of the control groups used in these studies. If
demonstrate the point. multiple factors are varied simultaneously, as they were in these
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Doris B. experiments, does this procedure not break the “vary one thing at a
Chin, Human-Sciences and Technologies Advanced Research Institute, time” rule essential to all randomized, controlled experiments, result-
Stanford University, 450 Serra Mall, Building 160, Stanford, CA 94305- ing in it being impossible to determine exactly what caused the
2055. E-mail: dbchin@stanford.edu effects?

1
2 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

In other words, it is impossible to know whether the results were The cases in Figure 1 contain at least three levels of structure.
due to the specially designed cases, the timing of explicit instruc- The first level is the surface features, or irrelevant structure. Two
tion, or both. examples of surface features are the type of clown and the lines
The following studies highlight a guided discovery pedagogy delineating the bus exterior. These incidental details are irrelevant
called inventing with contrasting cases (ICC). The contrasting to the concept of density. If this were the only information students
cases are designed to help students find “deep structure” (we say gained from the cases, they would not have learned much about
more below). The contrasting cases can also serve as practice density.
problems for T&P. Therefore, they provide an opportunity to A second level of structure is the density used within a given
maintain both instructional fidelity and experimental control. In company. For example, the company in the first row uses two
the following two studies, all conditions used the same sets of buses with different numbers of clowns and bus sizes, but both
contrasting cases. The principal difference between conditions was buses use a density of one clown per compartment. This level of
that T&P students were told the concepts and solution methods information better instantiates the idea of deep structure, because
beforehand and then practiced with the materials. Students in the the density is a fixed relation between mass and volume, even
ICC condition invented their own solutions for the same materials though the amounts of mass and volume vary.
and were not told about the conventional solutions and concepts The third level is the structure of ratio as it occurs across the
until the end. Given the close comparison, the current experiments companies. Although the specific densities differ for each com-
begin to identify the specific effects of telling first on subsequent pany, all three use proportionate ratios. This last level of structure
learning and transfer compared with one form of guided discovery. is termed the invariant under transformation (J. J. Gibson, 1979).
The invariant of a ratio structure persists in all the cases, despite
Inventing With Contrasting Cases the varying ratios for each company.
An invariant under transformation is a specific class of deep
ICC is similar to other instructional approaches that ask students structure that differs from the qualitative relations (Forbus, 1984)
to generate symbolic representations or models of data (e.g., Bow- often investigated in research on analogical transfer (e.g., an atom
ers, Cobb, & McClain, 1999; Lehrer & Schauble, 2004; Lesh & is like the solar system; Gentner & Markman, 1997). For this class
Doerr, 2003). The unique aspects of ICC are that the inventing of deep structure, there is a lawful parametric relation (ratio) that
tasks take a few minutes rather than days or weeks, and they use must be preserved across changes to the quantities, and a given
predefined contrasting cases. We start by explaining the contrast- ratio is defined by its position along a continuum of possible ratios.
ing cases. Afterward, we describe the inventing activity. The contrasting cases in Figure 1 are designed on the assumption
that picking up the invariant of a ratio structure across situations is
Contrasting Cases the key to effective transfer and that without this recognition, it
seems unlikely students would transfer to other ratio phenomena
Contrasting cases, which originally came from work in percep- such as speed or springiness.
tual learning (J. J. Gibson & Gibson, 1955), are instructional
materials designed to help students notice information they might Inventing
otherwise overlook. As with tasting wines side by side, contrasts
can improve discernment (e.g., Biederman & Shiffrar, 1987; Students need a productive task orientation to benefit from the
Bransford et al., 1989; Eisner, 1972). Rittle-Johnson and Star contrasting cases. The literature provides several good examples.
(2007, 2009), for example, found that students developed more Williams and Lombrozo (2010) proposed orienting students with
procedural flexibility in algebraic manipulations if they compared an “explanation” directive. In their research, participants received
alternative solutions side by side than if they saw them sequen- artificial stimuli that comprised contrasting cases of category in-
tially. By contrasting different solutions, students noticed what stances. Participants who were asked to explain category member-
made each procedure unique and useful. ship exhibited better learning and transfer compared with thinking
In addition to supporting procedural flexibility with symbolic aloud or simply describing the instances. Similarly, Schmidt, De
representations, contrasting cases can be configured to highlight Volder, De Grave, Moust, and Patel (1989) asked students to work
empirical regularities. Figure 1 shows a set of contrasting cases in small groups to explain the contrasting case of a blood cell that
designed to help students learn about density. The narrative of the expands in water but shrinks in saltwater. These explanation ac-
worksheet is that each row represents a company that ships clowns tivities prepared students to learn from a relevant text compared
to events. A given company always packs its clowns into buses by with students who had not done the group explanation activities.
the same amount, but each company packs its clowns to different Taking a different approach, Gentner, Loewenstein, and Thomp-
degrees. son (2003) found that students who received directives to draw
The systematic variability across the cases is intended to help analogies across negotiation strategies transferred more than stu-
students notice the ratio structure of density. Gick and Paterson dents who did not.
(1992), for example, demonstrated that the inclusion of near In ICC, students are asked to invent a quantitative index. For the
misses— contrasts that differ on a single dimension—improves cases in Figure 1, students invent an index for density, disguised as
schema induction and transfer. In Figure 1, the buses in the upper- a “crowdedness index” for each company. They have to come up
and lower-right corners have the same number of clowns but vary with a single, common way to compute a consumer index that
in size (number of bus compartments). This should help students works for each company, so that consumers can compare the
notice that bus size is important and not just the total number of crowdedness used by each company (because crowded clowns are
clowns. grumpy clowns). The directive to make an indexing scheme differs
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 3

Figure 1. Crowded clowns worksheet. Students in the tell-and-practice condition were told the formula for
density and then practiced applying it to determine the crowdedness of the clowns for each company. Students
in the invent-with-contrasting-cases condition were not told about density but instead had to invent their own
crowdedness index for each company.

from asking students to explain, draw an analogy, or simply and Sweller (1999) found that worked examples improved data-
compare and contrast, because it specifically pushes students to- base programming compared with free exploration, especially for
ward a single, quantitative explanation of the deep structure. novices. Although unsupported exploration has low fidelity to
Ideally, the inventing task also recruits a confluence of productive most models of guided discovery, these results indicate that ex-
psychological processes including explanation, analogy, and com- plicit instruction can improve procedural knowledge. However,
pare and contrast. One question addressed by the current research success at procedural learning does not address a major challenge
is whether inventing is a useful task orientation or whether it for many topics of instruction. In science, students also need to
would be simpler and more direct just to tell students about density learn the structure of scientific phenomena.
and have them practice with the cases.
Our hypothesis is that explicit instruction beforehand may not
lead students to learn the deep structure very effectively. When
Learning Predictions learning science, students often focus on the surface features of
In debates over the timing of explicit instruction, studies that single instances rather than the deep structures that define a phe-
have found advantages for “telling first” have focused on the nomenon across instances (e.g., Goldstone & Son, 2005). For
learning of multistep procedures. For example, Klahr and Nigam example, students may hear about density and learn its formula,
(2004) found an advantage for explicit instruction over unsup- and then facilely map the variables of the formula to discrete
ported discovery for the control-of-variable procedure. Tuovinen features of the subsequent problems. In short, they may simply
4 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

divide the relevant values to compute density. Though successful, of the ratio structure should open up a deeper understanding of
they may not understand the significance of ratio in the structure of many physics concepts.
density. Thus, we are making the possibly unintuitive prediction
that telling students the structure of a phenomenon, in this case
Method
with a verbal explanation and an encapsulating mathematical for-
mula, may limit their learning of the structure from subsequent Participants. Participants (N ⫽ 128) were eighth graders
practice problems. from four classes at a diverse middle school (35% Asian, 25%
Initial support for this hypothesis comes from the previously Latino, 22% Filipino, 11% White, 4% African American, and 3%
mentioned time-for-telling studies (Schwartz & Bransford, 1998; other; 37% qualified for free-lunch programs). Within each class,
Schwartz & Martin, 2004). Kapur (2008, 2010) also compared students were assigned to the T&P or ICC condition through
“delay of structure” treatments with T&P instruction in a series of stratified random assignment based on their cumulative class
studies on Newtonian physics and rate–time– distance problems. In scores in science. Treatments occurred in separate rooms with
the delay of structure treatments, students did not initially receive rotating instructors to ensure there were no teacher effects (each
explicit instruction that ensured they could solve the problems. The instructor taught both T&P and ICC for each lesson). In both
students often failed, but it was a “productive failure,” because the conditions, students worked in self-elected and sometimes chang-
students exhibited better overall learning. One interpretation is that ing pairs, consistent with regular class practice. All tests were
the T&P students, who were told how to solve the problems taken individually.
beforehand, had no need to search for the deep structure. In Design and instruction. This section describes the complete
contrast, the delay-of-structure condition may have led students to treatments for both conditions. The experiment had two phases that
search for the deep structure so they could solve the problems. served to test the two predictions. The first phase tested the
To gather more direct evidence on the effects of T&P for the prediction that T&P students would not learn the structure of
learning of structure, the following studies took a simple measure- density as well as ICC students. The second phase of the study
ment approach. Students practiced or invented with a set of con- tested the prediction that T&P does not support transfer as well as
trasting cases, and a day later they redrew the cases. Our first ICC instruction. Figure 2 shows the full timeline for instruction
prediction was that T&P students would not reproduce the ratio throughout the study. Measures of student learning were inter-
structure of the problems as well as ICC students who received the leaved at four points (shown centered and bolded).
same problems without being told first. Phase 1. On Day 1, T&P students received an instructional
A second prediction was that T&P students would transfer page titled “Finding Density” (see Figure 3A). It contained some
poorly. Gick and Holyoak (1983) demonstrated that if people do everyday examples, the formula, and a worked example that was
not learn the deep structure, they rarely exhibit spontaneous trans- different from the contrasting cases they would receive. Students
fer to problem isomorphs with different surface features. There- took turns reading sections of the page aloud, while the rest of the
fore, we predicted that at posttest, T&P students would transfer class followed along. Student questions were answered by refer-
relatively poorly to scientific phenomena that involve ratio but not ring them to the instruction page. Afterward, students were told,
density (e.g., spring constant), despite being able to solve proce- “On the next page, compute the density for each company that
dural problems on density. People can have knowledge of a math- buses clowns.” Students then practiced on the contrasting cases
ematical formula, or any formal theory, without recognizing the worksheet in Figure 1. The instructional page was kept by students
structure it describes. For example, Michael, Klee, Bransford, and for reference.
Warren (1993) showed that clinical psychology students could In the ICC condition, students did not receive any density
recite the relevant theories but could not apply those theories to instruction beforehand, but instead they received an instruction
patients. They had not learned to recognize the structure of symp- page titled “Inventing an Index” (see Figure 3B). It described an
toms in patients. index in everyday language and gave several examples, such as
class grade and batting average. It developed the narrative of compa-
Experiment 1 nies shipping clowns and the need for a crowdedness index. The
instructional page did not provide any examples for how to compute
In Experiment 1, we examined eighth-grade students’ learning an index, but it provided a set of constraints at the end of the page (i.e.,
of structure and their spontaneous transfer from lessons on density a company only gets one index value, the same procedure should be
and speed to problems on surface pressure and the spring constant. used to find the index for each company, and a larger index means
In terms of physical phenomena, these four are quite different. more crowded). The instructional page was read aloud, and questions
Nevertheless, they share the deep structure of a ratio between were answered in the same fashion as in the T&P condition. Then,
distinct physical properties. Density is described as mass over students were told to “invent a procedure for computing a crowded
volume, speed is distance over time, surface pressure is force over clown index for each company.” They used the same contrasting
area, and the spring constant is force over displacement. cases worksheet that the T&P group received. The instructional page
Ratio is a critical deep structure for many physical phenomena. was kept by students for reference.
By adolescence most children have the cognitive wherewithal to The critical measurement for Phase 1 came on Day 2. Twenty-
reason about ratio (Siegler, 1981), but this does not mean they four hours after completing the clowns worksheet, and prior to any
spontaneously recognize or learn the ratio structure in novel situ- further potentially contaminating instruction or experiences, stu-
ations. If students do not pick up the ratio structure during instruc- dents were asked to reconstruct the worksheet. This memory test
tion, they should do poorly on transfer problems that have a ratio provided a measure of the structure they had learned from the
structure. In contrast, helping students understand the prevalence previous day’s lesson (Chase & Simon, 1973; Jee, Gentner, For-
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 5

effect of T&P is specific to deep structure. A primary coder


evaluated this and all other measures, and a secondary coder
evaluated 25% of the data for each measure. Measures were
blind-coded, and agreement was greater than 98% for each mea-
sure for both studies.
Phase 2. Students completed three more lessons using T&P
or ICC on Days 2 and 5. The lessons were designed to cover
discrete and continuous versions of both density and speed. (The
full set of materials is available at http://aaalab.stanford.edu/
transfer.html.) As in Day 1, the T&P condition always received
and read aloud an instructional page that included everyday ex-
amples of the target concept, an introduction to the relevant for-
mula, and a worked example that used a different cover story from
the contrasting cases. The ICC instructions also kept to their
format from Day 1. Students were asked to invent an index for the
contrasting cases. However, it was no longer necessary to explain
the concept of inventing an index or introduce the constraints on
the index (e.g., each company only gets a single index score). After
their respective instructional pages, students received the worksheets,
which had the same data and contrasting cases regardless of condition.
The insets of Figure 2 show one case from each of these worksheets.
The popcorn task had students find the popping rate of different
machines (discrete speed). The gold cube task had students find the
purity of gold used by different companies (continuous density). The
race car task had students find the speeds of different cars and, further,
determine which cars have the same speed (continuous speed) and
therefore come from the same company. (Note that this additional
instruction for the cars task requests the T&P and ICC students to
explicitly compare across cases for the first time.)
In addition to the instructional pages, a second treatment differ-
Figure 2. Design of Experiment 1. The tell-and-practice (T&P) students ence involved the timing of an explicit lecture on ratio (shown in
received identical practice cases and tests as the invent-with-contrasting-
bold italics in Figure 2). T&P students received a lecture on the
cases (ICC) students. T&P students were told key formulas and concepts,
and then practiced on the cases. ICC students had to invent an index for
importance of ratio in physics at the beginning of Phase 2, on Day
each set of cases without being told the appropriate formulas. The images 2. This lecture was separate from the instructional pages the T&P
provide snippets of the materials students received. Picture recall, word students received before each of the remaining three lessons. The
problem, transfer, and delayed transfer tests served as measures of learning lecture directly explained the prevalence of ratio in physics includ-
and transfer. ing density, speed, and force, and it explicitly indicated the anal-
ogous ratio structures in the clowns worksheet, an approach dem-
onstrated to support transfer (Brown & Kane, 1988; Gentner et al.,
bus, Sageman, & Uttal, 2009). Students received a sheet that
2003; Gick & Holyoak, 1983). The lecture helped to evaluate the
stated: “Yesterday, you received a sheet that had an activity with
common intuition that if one just told the students that ratio was
clowns and buses. Use the space to redraw how the sheet looked
important and explicitly pointed it out, they would begin to learn
the best you can.” Students were given 10 min to complete their
the structural information from the subsequent contrasting cases
drawings. We coded two aspects of their recreations. The first was
and use it later at transfer.1 ICC students received this same
whether students reconstructed the ratio structures. We call this
deep structure recall. Students received 1 deep structure point for
each pair of buses that shared one ratio between them, unique from 1
A reasonable question is why the overarching lecture on ratio was
the other pairs of buses (3 points ⫽ 100%). The ratios did not have given after the T&P students had worked on the clowns worksheet and not
to be the same as in the original worksheet, but they had to be before. We did not want to use the lecture before students completed Phase
proportional across pairs of buses. The second coding captured 1 because the lecture would cloud the strict comparison of T&P instruction
students’ memory for surface features. Students received 1 point versus ICC instruction on the redrawing measure. If the T&P students had
for each of the following surface features that we had a priori received a lecture and then an introductory lesson specific to density before
seeded into the cases (6 points ⫽ 100%): elaborated clown fea- the memory measure, one might argue that a broad orienting lecture is not
typical of T&P instruction. Alternatively, one might argue that the T&P
tures, clowns positioned on lines between bus compartments,
students were overloaded by the extra instruction, which might hinder their
different line styles for the buses, wheels that did not correspond learning from the subsequent density lesson with the clowns. Therefore, we
to the compartments, a company name, and incidental text features avoided using the lecture in Phase 1. To determine whether the broader
such as “Name ___.” Our prediction was that surface feature recall ratio lecture overcame problems with T&P, we can look at the transfer
would be the same in both conditions but that structural recall measures that also test whether students had learned the deep structure of
would be worse in the T&P condition. This would indicate that the ratio, though less directly than the redrawing measure.
6 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

Figure 3. Instructional pages for crowded clowns worksheet. Instructional information was read aloud by
students. (A) Tell-and-practice instructions contained an introduction to the concept with everyday examples, the
relevant formula, and a worked example. Students were then told, “On the next page, compute the density for
each company that buses clowns.” (B) Invent-with-contrasting-cases instructions explained the concept of an
index with everyday examples, provided a narrative for finding a crowdedness index, and introduced rules for
the index. Students were then told, “Invent a procedure for computing a crowded clown index for each
company.”

lecture, but on Day 8, after they had completed all the inventing The second measure was a word problem test given on Day 11.
tasks. The lecture also served as the ICC students’ explicit instruc- The test comprised three problems each for density and speed. An
tion on the concepts of density and speed. example problem is “Brenda packs 120 marshmallows into 4 soda
The last piece of instruction for both groups was a worksheet of cans. Sandra packs 300 marshmallows into 11 soda cans. Whose
word problems, given on Day 8. This gave students practice soda cans are more densely packed?” This test served two pur-
applying the density and speed formulas. Across the 4 days of poses. First, as is characteristic in transfer research, it is important
instruction, the ICC instruction required approximately 10 more to ensure that a lack of transfer is not due to a simple failure to
minutes of instructional time. The T&P students used this time learn the concept or procedure. Therefore, we wanted to be sure
completing extra word problems on this last day. the T&P students had learned how to compute ratio quantities and
To test the effects of T&P and ICC on transfer, three learning could do so in the context of solving standard word problems. The
measures were taken during Phase 2. The first, an immediate second purpose was to find out whether the ICC condition put
transfer task, was given at the beginning of Day 8, after ICC these students at risk for not learning how to apply the formulas,
students had completed the four ICC cycles but had not yet given that they did not practice on the contrasting cases and
received the ratio lecture. This permitted us to evaluate transfer for received much less overall direct instruction on the density and
the ICC students before they had received any direct instruction, speed concepts and their formulas.
whereas the T&P students had completed all their instruction The third Phase 2 measure was a delayed transfer task that
(except practice on word problems). Students received a sheet that occurred 21 days after all students had completed their instruc-
showed diagrams of four aerosol cans with internal springs press- tional treatments. Ideally, this delay would determine whether the
ing on plates. Students were asked to describe the plate pressure effects of the treatments were somewhat lasting, and it would
used in the aerosol cans. The word describe had not been used determine whether the final lecture helped or hurt the ICC stu-
previously in either treatment. Students received 1 point for each dents. All students attempted the transfer problem embedded in a
instance that was described with a ratio (e.g., springs over plate biology test. Figure 4 shows that the transfer problem was on the
area; 4 points ⫽ 100%). spring constant. Students were asked to “determine the stiffness of
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 7

the analogy across density, speed, and other physics concepts. For
each lesson, they had received a unique worked example and then
practiced on a new set of cases. The means in Table 1 indicate that
the T&P students used ratio to describe surface pressure signifi-
cantly less often than the ICC students, who had not yet been told
about the formulas or the importance of ratio, F(1, 94) ⫽ 6.8, p ⫽
.011, d ⫽ 0.53. (Due to an implementation error, data from both of
the conditions in one class could not be included in the preceding
analysis of immediate transfer. The delayed transfer analysis,
which is next, included all the students.)
Figure 6 provides prototypical examples of how students an-
swered the delayed transfer problem on the spring constant (tram-
poline). (The immediate transfer problem on surface pressure had
the same classes of response.) To receive credit for transfer,
students had to produce answers similar to the one in Figure 6D,
which indicates a ratio between people and rungs. The delayed
transfer problem occurred 3 weeks after both groups had com-
pleted the final word problems and the ICC students had received
their lecture on ratio. Table 1 indicates that the ICC students
performed significantly better than the T&P students on the de-
layed transfer test, F(1, 124) ⫽ 13.6, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 0.66.
Further analysis indicated that learning the ratio structure during
the clown task was important for transfer, regardless of condition
Figure 4. Transfer task on spring constant. Students were asked to
determine the stiffness of the trampoline fabrics, which is the equivalent of
(see Klahr & Nigam, 2004, on common causes of transfer despite
finding their spring constants. Though a far semantic transfer from speed instructional differences). A three-step regression analysis used
and density, it shares the deep structure of ratio. performance on the delayed transfer problem (trampoline) as the
dependent measure. In the first step, structural recall of the clown
task was regressed against transfer performance, F(1, 119) ⫽ 5.79,
the mat fabric for each trampoline.” Students received 1 point for p ⫽ .018, r ⫽ .22. Students who recalled the deep structure of the
each trampoline described by ratio (e.g., number of people over clowns were more likely to transfer. In the second step of the
displacement; 4 points ⫽ 100%). analysis, experimental condition was added to the regression equa-
tion. It improved the prediction of the model, Fchange(1, 118) ⫽
8.84, p ⫽ .004, rmodel ⫽ .34. Thus, students in the ICC condition
Results
were more likely to transfer, even when taking into account their
Redrawing test: Encoding of surface features and deep structural recall of the clowns worksheet. One possible explanation
structure. Figure 5 shows excerpts of student recreations of the is that ICC students who did not induce the ratio structure during
clowns worksheet. The examples show what surface and deep the clown activity learned it over the three subsequent inventing
feature recall looks like for this task. Table 1 shows the average activities. Finally, in the third step, the interaction term of condi-
percentages of recall for deep structures and surface features in tion by clown recall was added to the equation. It did not improve
Experiment 1. The T&P students did not recreate the deep struc- the fit, Fchange(1, 117) ⫽ 0.91, p ⫽ .342, rmodel ⫽ .35. The lack of
ture as well as the ICC students. There was a strong interaction an interaction indicates that finding the clown structure improved
between treatment and the type of feature recalled, F(1, 120) ⫽ transfer for both conditions. The primary difference was that
11.9, p ⬍ .001. There was a treatment effect for deep structure, students in the ICC condition found the structure more often.
F(1, 120) ⫽ 11.0, p ⫽ .001, d ⫽ 0.60, but not for surface features,
F(1, 120) ⫽ 1.5, p ⫽ .223, d ⫽ 0.22. There were no significant
correlations between surface and deep recall (rICC ⫽ ⫺.21, p ⫽
.10; rT&P ⫽ .09, p ⫽ .52). Interestingly, 19% of the T&P students
wrote down the formula, which was not present on the original
worksheet, compared with only 3% of the ICC students. Recall of
the ratio structure did not differ appreciably for those T&P stu-
dents who wrote down the formula (M ⫽ 57%, SD ⫽ 33.6%)
versus those who did not (M ⫽ 51%, SD ⫽ 43.9%), F(1, 56) ⫽
0.2, p ⫽ .65. This result highlights that remembering the structure
of the formula does not entail knowing the structure of the phe-
nomena to which the formula refers.
Transfer effects. By the first transfer task on surface pres-
sure, T&P students had received lessons on discrete and continu-
ous versions of density and speed, and they had received a separate Figure 5. Samples of clown recall. Samples of students’ worksheet
lecture on the importance of ratio in physics that explicitly mapped recreations, partitioned by the inclusion of surface and deep features.
8 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

Table 1
Means (in Percent) of Outcome Measures by Experiment and Treatment, Including a Further Breakout of Experiment 2 Based on a
Median Split of Achievement Using Students’ Prior Class Performance

Experiment 1 Experiment 2

Broken out by prior achievement

ICC T&P ICC T&P ICC T&Pa

Measure M SD M SD M SD M SD High M Low M High M Low M

Deep structures 75.5ⴱⴱ 35.3 52.3 41.9 58.6ⴱⴱ 40.6 38.4 36.0 73.1 46.9 48.1 23.2
Surface features 27.1 18.9 31.3 19.0 33.1 14.8 28.5 15.2 35.3 31.2 29.2 27.5
Ratios at transfer surface pressure 35.2ⴱ 46.2 13.8 32.9
Ratios at transfer spring constant 53.5ⴱⴱ 50.0 23.0 42.4 50.9ⴱⴱ 49.9 23.3 41.8 64.0 40.9 32.9 8.7
Word problems 70.1 23.1 75.2 20.3 64.8 19.6 66.4 19.5 75.0 57.0 72.9 56.5

Note. ICC ⫽ invent with contrasting cases; T&P ⫽ tell and practice.
a
Two T&P students did not have achievement scores.

Comparison of treatment means, p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ Comparison of treatment means, p ⬍ .005.

Overall, these results should not be interpreted as demonstrating of the fabrics 96% of the time. Students who did not use ratios
that finding the structure of the clowns worksheet was sufficient succeeded 1% of the time, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 126) ⫽ 113.7, p ⬍ .001. (The
for transfer. A more cautious interpretation is that those who found results are similar for the surface pressure problem, ␹2(1, N ⫽
the structure on the first lesson were more likely to find structure 96) ⫽ 66.1, p ⬍ .001.) Students who did not use ratios tended to
on the second, third, and fourth lessons, which would have a rank the stiffness of the fabric based on the value of a single
cumulative effect for improving transfer. feature such as the number of people on the trampoline (see Figure
One potential concern with the focus on ratio is that students 6A) or the number of rungs the fabric stretched (see Figure 6B).
may have been learning math, but they may not have been learning Thus, without ratios, students had very little chance of forming a
physics. A post hoc coding of the transfer problems sheds light on qualitative understanding of the physics, because they focused on
this concern. We coded whether students ranked the trampoline only one feature in the physical situation. As an analogy, students
cases correctly in terms of their physical property of stiffness. The will often conflate density and mass, because they do not appre-
instructions did not have an explicit directive to rank the cases, but ciate that density is a structural relation between two physical
the students implicitly ranked them, for example, by writing “stiff- properties.
est,” or a specific ratio, next to a trampoline. Students who used Word problem test: Computation. The transfer differences
ratios for over half the cases correctly ranked the relative stiffness were not due to differences in students’ knowledge of the formu-

Figure 6. Sample solutions for the trampoline problem. Students’ solutions for delayed transfer captured the
total number of people (A), the total number of rungs (B), a global impression of stretchiness (C), or the ratio
structure of rungs by people (D). Only the latter was considered an instance of transfer.
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 9

las. Table 1 indicates that the T&P and ICC conditions exhibited sequentially, making fewer transitions between cases. As Gentner
similar percentages correct on the word problem test, F(1, 124) ⫽ et al. (2003) concluded from their studies on analogical encoding,
1.7, p ⫽ .20. “Learners cannot be counted on to spontaneously draw appropriate
comparisons, even when the two cases are presented in close
Discussion juxtaposition” (p. 403).
The second issue is the question of what transferred. One
The T&P and ICC students had learned to apply formulas to hypothesis is that students transferred the ability to recognize
word problems equally well. Nevertheless, T&P instruction was ratios. An alternative hypothesis is that students transferred the
suboptimal for helping students learn the structures that are im- strategy of looking across multiple cases. To test these alternatives
portant for transfer. T&P students exhibited a relatively low rate of at transfer, we gave half the students from each condition the
recreating the deep structure of the contrasting cases at recall, and original four-case trampoline problem, and the other half received
they also transferred less often than the ICC students, both before the same scenario but with only one trampoline. If the ICC
and after the ICC students had received a lecture on ratio in students transfer on the basis of having a problem with multiple
physics. Our favored explanation is that many T&P students fo- cases, like the problems at instruction, then the ICC advantage
cused on applying what they had been told, and missed the deep over T&P should disappear for the one-case transfer problem. If
structure in the problem situations. the ICC students transfer on the basis of ratio, then they should
The T&P students’ poor recall of the ratio structure of the exhibit greater transfer than T&P students for both the one- and
crowded clowns cannot easily be attributed to a lack of interest or four-case transfer problems.
general memory effects. If the T&P results were due to general A final practical question addressed by Experiment 2 is whether
effects, then the T&P students’ memory for both surface features ICC and T&P instruction interacts with students’ prior achieve-
and deep structures should have been lower than that of the ICC ment. There are concerns that lower achieving students may be
students, but the T&P and ICC students demonstrated equal sur- better served by the greater guidance of T&P instruction. For
face feature recall. example, Hiebert and Wearne (1996) found that early elementary
Regardless of condition, students who found and recalled the students with low initial understanding did not fare very well with
ratio structure of the crowded clowns worksheet transferred better. invention tasks when learning place value. Similarly, Tuovinen
However, more of the ICC students found the ratio structure, and and Sweller (1999) compared adult learning through free explora-
therefore more ICC students transferred. Under this explanation, tion with learning from worked examples. Free exploration was
the ICC students’ deeper understanding of ratio led to the transfer. effective only for adults with high prior knowledge. Therefore, we
An alternative explanation is that the ICC students had “learned to collected permission to analyze students’ cumulative class grades
learn” (e.g., Brown & Kane, 1988). They developed the strategy of in science to see whether it would indicate that the T&P instruction
looking across multiple problems to find a deep structure. By this was relatively more effective for the low-achieving students.
alternative, the ICC students did not transfer the ability to recog-
nize the ratio structure, but instead they transferred the idea of
looking for patterns given multiple cases. The transfer problems Method
included multiple cases that may have cued the same strategy they
Experiment 2 followed a nearly identical protocol with 120
had used during instruction. If true, it means that ICC students
eighth-grade students at the same school. There were four primary
should not transfer the use of ratios to situations that only involve
changes: (a) The experiment used instructors blind to the hypoth-
a single instance. Experiment 2 tests this prediction.
eses; (b) 24 pairs of students were selected at random to be
videotaped in class while they worked on the clowns worksheet (3
Experiment 2 pairs ⫻ 4 classes ⫻ 2 treatments); (c) the timeline was condensed
by removing the first transfer task on surface pressure and com-
What led to superior levels of transfer for the ICC students?
bining the word problem and delayed transfer test into a single
Experiment 2 was designed to help answer this question by exam-
posttest, given 1 week after instruction; and (d) students in each
ining (a) which behaviors helped students learn the structural
treatment received either the one- or four-case trampoline transfer
information and (b) what learning led to the transfer. We assume
problem (spring constant) at random.
that the contrasting cases themselves were an important element
that aided students in learning the deep structure. But they could
not have been sufficient because the T&P students also used them. Results
Our hypothesis is that the inventing directive orients students to
search the cases for a common deep structure on which to base Table 1 shows the results for Experiment 2. The data replicated
their index. In contrast, T&P instruction focuses students on ap- Experiment 1 closely. There were no differences between treat-
plying solutions, one problem at a time. This reduces their chances ments for surface feature recall and word problem performance,
of finding the deep structure across the cases. To investigate but the ICC condition showed a substantial advantage for deep
further, we videotaped a subset of students. We coded the video- structure recall and transfer performance. Although science
tapes specifically for how many times the students transitioned achievement had an overall effect on performance, it did not
among the cases. The prediction was that students in the ICC interact with the instructional conditions. The right-most columns
condition would shuttle among the cases as they searched for the show the Experiment 2 data partitioned by a median split on the
structure on which to base the index. In contrast, the T&P students students’ cumulative grades in their science class. The treatment
would be more inclined to complete each case separately and effect was sufficiently strong that lower achieving ICC students
10 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

descriptively outperformed higher achieving T&P students at sheets they received during instruction (these data were not avail-
transfer, 40.9% versus 32.9%. able for Experiment 1). The T&P condition did modestly better.
To statistically dissect the possible interactions of treatment by An analysis of variance compared the average percentage correct
achievement, we performed separate analyses of covariance on the per worksheet and demonstrated a significant effect of condition
four measures in Table 1. The analyses used the achievement (MICC ⫽ 84.7%, SD ⫽ 22.7%; MT&P ⫽ 91.8%, SD ⫽ 12.2%), F(1,
covariate, the treatment factor, and the Treatment ⫻ Achievement 113) ⫽ 4.5, p ⫽ .037. The following disaggregates worksheet
interaction. Two students in the T&P condition did not have performance across the four lessons: clowns (discrete density;
cumulative grades and were omitted from these analyses. MICC ⫽ 85.1%, SD ⫽ 31.7%; MT&P ⫽ 91.5%, SD ⫽ 23.6%), F(1,
For deep structure recall, ICC significantly outperformed T&P, 113) ⫽ 1.52, p ⫽ .22; popcorn (discrete speed; MICC ⫽ 82.7%,
F(1, 111) ⫽ 8.7, MSE ⫽ 0.12, p ⫽ .004, d ⫽ 0.53; achievement SD ⫽ 31.8%; MT&P ⫽ 90.4%, SD ⫽ 23.2%), F(1, 113) ⫽ 2.19,
was a significant predictor of deep structure recall, F(1, 111) ⫽ p ⫽ .14; gold (continuous density; MICC ⫽ 85.7%, SD ⫽ 33.5%;
23.7, p ⬍ .001, but there was no Treatment ⫻ Achievement MT&P ⫽ 96.6%, SD ⫽ 13.4%), F(1, 113) ⫽ 5.33, p ⫽ .023; race
interaction, F(1, 111) ⫽ 0.15, p ⫽ .70. For surface feature mem- car (continuous speed; MICC ⫽ 85.1%, SD ⫽ 29.1%; MT&P ⫽
ory, there was no difference by treatment, F(1, 111) ⫽ 2.8, MSE ⫽ 88.7%, SD ⫽ 28.8%), F(1, 113) ⫽ 0.44, p ⫽ .51. In isolation, only
0.02, p ⫽ .10, d ⫽ 0.31; no effect of achievement, F(1, 111) ⫽ 1.6, the gold worksheet exhibited a significant treatment difference.
p ⫽ .22; and no interaction, F(1, 111) ⫽ 0.09, p ⫽ .77. As in This is driven by an uptick in T&P performance, which one may
Experiment 1, there were no significant correlations between sur- speculate is the result of the worksheet’s overt numerical presen-
face feature and deep structure recall (rICC ⫽ .09, p ⫽ .49; rT&P ⫽ tation of weight and volume, which in turn made it easier for the
⫺.18, p ⫽ .17). T&P students to map in the density formula. In sum, despite doing
With respect to posttest performance, the ICC students exhibited somewhat worse on the basic classroom assignments, the ICC
greater transfer, F(1, 111) ⫽ 10.6, MSE ⫽ 0.20, p ⫽ .002, d ⫽ students did better at transfer and equally well on the word prob-
0.60; there was an effect of achievement on transfer, F(1, 111) ⫽ lem test. In absolute terms, the ICC students exhibited a high
9.2, p ⫽ .003, but there was no Treatment ⫻ Achievement inter- degree of success at the inventing task.
action, F(1, 111) ⫽ 0.18, p ⫽ .67. For the word problems, there What learning transferred? Figure 7 indicates a consistent ad-
were no treatment differences, F(1, 111) ⫽ 0.21, MSE ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ vantage for the ICC condition for both one- and four-case prob-
.65, d ⫽ 0.08; there was a very strong effect of achievement, F(1, lems, F(1, 112) ⫽ 10.8, p ⫽ .001. Although the four-case problem
111) ⫽ 34.0, p ⬍ .001, but again there was no interaction of was solved more frequently than the one-case problem, the differ-
treatment by achievement, F(1, 111) ⫽ 0.09, p ⫽ .76. ence was not significant, F(1, 112) ⫽ 2.7, p ⫽ .105. Importantly,
In this study, we also coded the percentage of correct answers there was no Treatment ⫻ Number of Cases interaction, F(1,
students produced for each of the four contrasting cases work- 112) ⫽ 0.5, p ⫽ .481, which indicates that the ICC students

Figure 7. Transfer performance by treatment and number of cases included in the problem. The advantage for
invent with contrasting cases (ICC) over tell and practice (T&P) on both one- and four-case problem types
indicates that students were transferring on the basis of picking up the ratio structure and not on a strategy of
searching for structure across multiple cases.
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 11

transferred their understanding of ratio. If ICC students had trans- The videotapes were collected to test a specific behavioral
ferred only a strategy of inventing across multiple cases, they prediction (number of transitions) rather than generate a corpus for
would not have shown a similar advantage over the T&P condition protocol analysis. However, to enrich the picture of the process in
for the one- and four-case versions. Notably, the T&P students also action, we provide protocol excerpts from a pair of students from
benefited from the four-case version, which makes sense, given each treatment as they found their first answer for one of the clown
that the T&P students were confronting a set of contrasting cases companies. It should be noted that these excerpts were specifically
without being told a formula beforehand, similar to the ICC chosen to highlight treatment differences. The preceding statistical
students’ original instruction. analysis shows that they reflect general differences, although there
As in Experiment 1, regardless of treatment, students who was natural variation across the pairs. With each transcript, we
recalled the deep structure of the clowns worksheet were more provide a brief commentary pointing out the key features from our
likely to transfer than those who did not. A three-step regression perspective. For clarity, companies are referred to as Company A,
analysis used the performance on the transfer problem (trampo- B, or C, from top to bottom on the Figure 1 worksheet.
line) as the dependent measure. In the first step, structural recall of T&P pair. The following transcript reflects the T&P stu-
the clown task was regressed against transfer performance, F(1, dents’ tendency to focus on the formula such that the task becomes
111) ⫽ 14.37, p ⬍ .001, r ⫽ .34. Students who recalled the deep a problem of mapping the variables of the formula to the discrete
structure of the clowns were more likely to transfer. In the second features of a specific case. Notice that these students begin by
step of the regression analysis, experimental condition was added explicitly stating the formula “D equals M over V” and then work
to the regression equation. It improved the fit of the model, from there. These students, like others, spend a good deal of time
Fchange(1, 110) ⫽ 5.13, p ⫽ .025, rmodel ⫽ .39. Overall, students figuring out that each compartment (rather than a complete bus)
in the ICC condition were more likely to transfer, presumably represents one unit of volume. Moreover, these students make only
because they noticed the ratio structure on the subsequent invent- one transition—from Company C to Company B—throughout the
ing tasks. Finally, in the third step, the interaction term of condi- entire segment.
tion by structural recall was added to the equation. It did not
improve the model’s fit, Fchange(1, 109) ⫽ 0.76, p ⫽ .385, rmodel ⫽ Student 1: Okay, this one kinda makes more sense [points to Company
.40. The benefit of finding the structure of the clowns for transfer C]. Okay, this one, I think, is 2 objects. There’s 6 things in here, so . . .
did not differ by condition. It was just that the ICC students learned Student 2: Or are there 3?
that structure more often. Student 1: Oh, okay, I see, I see . . . I’m gonna start with this one first
One possible source of the ICC advantage is that students [Company C].
searched the contrasting cases to find a deep structure on which to
Student 2: Let me write it. D equals M over V. [writes on sheet]
base their index. To find out whether the ICC students searched
across the cases more, we coded the frequency with which each Student 1: Okay. Well . . . I’m just going off how . . . what she
videotaped pair transitioned their attention from one clown com- [the instructor] did up there. So there’s 2 objects there [points to
Company C] . . . and add.
pany (row) to another. Pointing to, writing on, or discussing a
particular company was coded as a “reference” to that case. A Student 2: Do we put this in there? [points to Company C answer line]
transition occurred when at least one student of the pair shifted Student 1: No, I’m just doing it [the compartments] together, rather
from referencing one company to another. The minimum number than separate. So, hold on here.
of transitions is two, one between each of the three companies. The Student 2: The mass is how many things are inside of it.
ICC pairs transitioned an average of 20.1 times (SD ⫽ 10.3),
Student 1: So together there’s 6, individually there’s 4. So I think . . .
compared with 6.0 (SD ⫽ 4.7) for the T&P pairs, F(1, 22) ⫽ 18.7,
Okay, it’s for each bus [compartment].
p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 1.76. As with the full sample, the videotaped ICC
pairs recalled the ratio structure of the clowns more than the Student 2: So this [compartment] is separate?
videotaped T&P pairs (average of pair recall: MICC ⫽ 81.3%, Student 1: Think so? So each one . . . are separate ones, yeah.
SD ⫽ 33.0%; MT&P ⫽ 40.3%, SD ⫽ 29.7%), F(1, 22) ⫽ 4.71, p ⫽ Student 2: But why is it dashed line?
.041.
Student 1: I have no clue. The same thing right here. Hmm, well. So
T&P students already had the formula, and they tended to apply
I don’t think . . .
it to each case separately, which reduced their chances of noticing
the invariant of ratio across the cases. Within the T&P condition, Student 2: [points to Company B] This one 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, and
nine of 10 pairs exhibited 10 or fewer transitions, and there was no 3 buses [compartments] plus 1 bus [compartment] is 4, so 12, so 12
divided by . . . 4 is 3.
correlation between number of transitions and a pair’s average
structural recall, r(12) ⫽ .05, p ⫽ .87. For the ICC condition, eight Student 1: There you go! So 3 here.
of the 10 pairs showed more than 10 transitions. This suggests that Student 2: Three objects per cube.
these eight ICC pairs were searching the cases to find the invariant
property on which to base their index. For ICC, there was a ICC pair. The following transcript, of about the same length
nonsignificant negative correlation between number of transitions from an ICC pair, illustrates how they made far more transitions as
and structural recall, r(12) ⫽ ⫺.25, p ⫽ .38. Past a certain they looked across cases to notice critical features and tried to
threshold, more transitions did not yield better learning. For ex- develop an indexing scheme.
ample, the ICC pair with the most transitions (42) kept searching
the cases because they never discovered the invariant structure. At Student 3: So, we’re writing down a number?
recall, they only recreated one ratio. Student 4: Yeah, we’re writing down a number, a value.
12 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

Student 3: Do we count the clowns, or do we count the buses? ’Cause How did students come to understand the underlying structure in
this one’s separate from the other ones, I think . . . the first place? The video analysis demonstrated that students in
Student 4: I think . . . Lemme read the . . . [flips to instructions then the ICC condition made far more transitions between the cases as
back to worksheet]. Okay, so it’s not about the number of clowns, it’s they searched for common structure. The T&P students, on the
about how crowded it is. Which one do you think is the least crowded? other hand, went through the cases in a linear fashion, mapping the
Student 3: [points to Company A] It’s a bigger bus. formula to each company in turn.
The relative effects of the ICC instruction compared with T&P
Student 4: The most crowded?
did not differ systematically for individuals of different levels of
Student 3: [points to Company C, then Company B] One of these two? achievement. Low-achieving students benefited from ICC com-
Student 4: I think it’s this one. [points to Company C] pared with T&P as much as high-achieving students. It is useful to
Student 3: Because, if these two get added in, they’re really crowded note that students were working in pairs, so it is still possible that
[points to Company C], and this one too. [points to Company B] working individually would lead to a prior Treatment ⫻ Achieve-
ment interaction.
Student 4: Let’s see. 1, 2, 3, 4 — 4 in 2 sections [points to Company
C]. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9. [points to Company B]
Student 3: There’s 3. [points to Company B]
General Discussion
Student 4: There’s 3 in each section [points to Company B], this is 2 Standard T&P instruction is important because it delivers the
in each section [points to Company C], so this seems more crowded explanations and solutions invented by experts, and students need
[points to Company B]. So what do you think we should do? opportunities to hear and practice these ideas. For students to gain
Student 3: Should we add these guys in here? [points to Company C] this benefit without undermining transfer, the current studies sug-
gest that expositions should happen after students have explored
Student 4: Um, no, I think that’s good. I think this is less [points to
novel deep structures. The inventing activity can serve as “prep-
Company A], this is most [points to Company B], this is middle.
[points to Company C] aration for future learning” by readying students to appreciate
more fully the expert solutions and deep structures when they
The group has a total of 12 transitions within this segment of are explained (Bransford & Schwartz, 1999). Giving students the
conversation, reflecting how they moved back and forth from one end-product of expertise too soon short-cuts the need to find the
case to another. It is useful to note that the transitions were in the deep structure that the expertise describes. Students in the T&P
service of finding a single account. They were not simply com- condition focused on what they had been told, and they applied the
paring and contrasting the cases, but rather they were driving formulas sequentially to the problems, which reduced their
toward a single mathematical explanation of crowdedness. In chances of finding the deep structure. Without an appreciation of
comparing companies to figure out which was the most crowded, deep structure, students are less likely to see the structure in new
the students realized that they could not discriminate the crowd- situations that differ on the surface, and they will fail to transfer.
edness level between Companies B and C without counting. It was The current studies tried to address the challenge of maintaining
only after counting the clowns and bus compartments that they instructional fidelity to different pedagogies while isolating a
decided Company B was the most crowded. Finally, they checked causal variable. The difficulty of doing both simultaneously may
their indexing procedure by their intuitive sense of crowdedness. be one reason why much of the transfer research is predicated on
Their quantitative activity and their intuitive sense of crowdedness comparing T&P instruction to itself. This makes it possible to vary
worked together to sharpen what they learned from the cases. only one thing, while maintaining fidelity to the dominant model
of instruction. We sampled transfer articles of math and/or science
learning published between 2003 and 2008.2 Among the 70 articles
Discussion
(136 unique authors) that resulted from our search, 75% of the
As in Experiment 1, the T&P and ICC groups performed sim- studies used T&P instruction for both treatment and control con-
ilarly on the word problem test and remembered similar numbers ditions while investigators manipulated other variables. T&P in-
of surface features on the clown recall task. The ICC group struction was not a variable in these studies and therefore not
recalled the density structure better and exhibited higher rates of suspected as a potential contributor to transfer success or failure.
transfer. Performance on the clown recall task was a good predic- As the current studies show, the model of instruction is a large
tor of success on the delayed transfer task, regardless of condition. contributor to transfer, and therefore many of the psychological
The ICC group was just more likely to understand the structure by claims based on transfer studies may not generalize beyond T&P
the first day of instruction. Interestingly, the ICC group was instruction. For example, consider the relation between abstract
slightly less successful than the T&P group in solving the con- and concrete elements of instruction. Some scholars favor abstract
trasting cases, indicating that initial rates of success at the learning presentations to avoid obscuring problem structure (e.g., Bassok &
activity do not necessarily predict later transfer rates (cf. Kapur,
2008). 2
An ISI Web of Knowledge search used transfer conjoined with think-
What transferred? The ICC advantage over T&P did not in-
ing, procedure, learning, or psychology. The resulting 350 articles were
crease for the four-case problem. Instead, the ICC students main- winnowed to those in English that reported original data on the transfer of
tained the same relative advantage for the single-case problem. math and/or science learning. For each study, two coders separately eval-
This makes it likely that they transferred an understanding of the uated whether a solution procedure or concept was provided prior to
ratio structure, rather than being dependent on a problem with students’ independent problem solving. The 3% disagreements were re-
multiple cases. solved by discussion.
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 13

Holyoak, 1989; Harp & Mayer, 1998; Kaminski, Sloutsky, & With regard to the practice portion of T&P, the task of comput-
Heckler, 2008), whereas others favor concrete instances to connect ing answers to problems is fairly typical, and students had further
to prior knowledge (Goldstone & Sakamoto, 2003; McNeil, practice on subsequent word problems. The formulas for density
Uttal, Jarvin, & Sternberg, 2009). Many of the relevant studies, and speed are relatively straightforward equations. The T&P stu-
however, have used T&P instruction for both conditions. By dents were able to apply them effectively to the T&P cases and
hypothesis, T&P instruction does not help students pick up the posttest. This leads to the possibility that the practice was so easy
deep structure, so it makes sense that surface features would for the T&P students that they did not have to do very deep
appear to be an issue in many studies. In the current studies, if processing, and therefore they learned less. Perhaps T&P becomes
surface features interfered with picking up deep structure, then more effective when tasks become more difficult. Before embrac-
there should have been a negative correlation between surface ing this hypothesis fully, it is important to note that the T&P
feature and deep structure recall (e.g., Rothkopf & Billington, students engaged in a good deal of germane cognitive effort as
1979), but there was not. Moreover, Schwartz, Chase, and Brans- they tried to map the quantities to the variables of the equation (see
ford (in press) had students work with either the crowded clowns transcript in Experiment 2).
worksheet or an analogous abstract worksheet that used dots in A second issue is that telling first may be more effective for
cubes. For T&P instruction, the abstract worksheet led to better learning complex procedures, though less effective for complex
structural recall, consistent with other studies that have used T&P structures. The current studies do not bear on this hypothesis
instruction (e.g., Bassok & Holyoak, 1989). However, for the ICC except in demonstrating that early explicit instruction in science,
instruction, the students performed the same for either version of which is designed to help students handle procedural complexities,
the worksheet, and the ICC students doubled the rate of structural runs the risk of focusing student attention on the procedures
recall found for the T&P students who received the abstract themselves at the expense of the structures that the procedures
worksheet. These examples demonstrate the risk of generalizing were designed to encapsulate. There are alternatives to T&P for
psychological claims (e.g., about the value of abstract examples) teaching complex procedures. Schwartz and Martin (2004), for
without taking into consideration the broader instructional context. example, demonstrated that an ICC sequence better prepared stu-
dents to understand the complex formulas for statistical variance
compared with procedural instruction at the outset.
Alternative Hypotheses
On the relatively high performance of ICC. These studies
On the relatively low performance of T&P. Our execution were not meant to isolate the causal effect of each ingredient in
of the telling portion of T&P was consistent with prevalent class- ICC instruction. Here we offer some speculation about these
room practice (Hiebert & Stigler, 2004), although we fortified the ingredients, plus predictions about possible studies to test those
approach with multiple worked examples, practice across far anal- ingredients. We assume that the contrasting cases are one crucial
ogies, and a lecture that explicitly described the importance of ratio component because they can help students find structure in phe-
and further pointed out the ratio analogy across separate physical nomena. Without these specially designed cases, which highlight
domains. Our goal was to maximize the reach of the findings critical similarities and differences, it would be difficult to find the
across current educational practices by demonstrating a natural underlying structure. A simple study might compare the outcomes
psychological response to being told the form of an answer before of inventing for the current crowded clowns worksheet (see Figure
working on problems. Nevertheless, it is possible that our results 1) versus a worksheet where all the companies use the same ratio
will not generalize across all variants of direct instruction (see (with no built-in contrasts). Our prediction is that the latter con-
Atkinson, Derry, Renkl, & Wortham, 2000, for a review). For dition would not fare as well.
instance, direct instruction may be relatively effective for teaching Another key ingredient is the behavior triggered by the directive
discrete facts or procedural steps, whereas density involves a to come up with a single account, or index, for all the cases. The
principled relation between dimensions (volume and mass). More protocols of student problem solving showed that ICC students
pertinent to the current studies, it seems reasonable that if the revisited each company about seven times on average. In the
teachers had shown the general ratio structure in the clowns process, the students were actively comparing and contrasting the
worksheet before students had worked on it, the students would cases as they transitioned back and forth between them.
have exhibited better recall of the deep structure. The benefits of comparison for transfer have been well docu-
Showing students the ratio structure may improve structural mented (e.g., Gentner et al., 2003), and it is presumably necessary
encoding for the specific problems, but it may not transfer as well for the effect of ICC. But it may not be sufficient. A useful study
as having students induce the structure on their own. In the current would compare how students perform in a compare-and-contrast
studies, T&P students were explicitly told and shown the ratio condition versus an ICC condition. Our prediction is that if stu-
structures in the lecture after the clowns worksheet. However, they dents were simply instructed to compare and contrast the cases
did not transfer very well, despite having three more lessons that without driving toward a single parametric explanation, the stu-
depended on ratios. Perhaps T&P students were less able to rec- dents would compare and contrast familiar features rather than find
ognize the ratio structure on the transfer problems because they the underlying structure that makes apparent differences the same.
never had the experience of actively recognizing the ratio structure A final ingredient for the current instance of ICC is the quan-
on their own (Roll, 2009). Another possibility is that students in titative information that helped students to learn the ratio structure
typical classrooms, compared with laboratory settings, may not with greater precision. Without countable quantities, the students
allocate critical lessons on structure the special attention they would probably make comparisons with terms like more and less,
deserve. They may be more interested in just learning how to solve which lack the precision required to find the ratio structure.
the problem. Schwartz, Martin, and Pfaffman (2005), for example, found that
14 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

children who were asked to give verbal explanations to balance The contrasting cases are designed to support incremental, even
scale problems did not learn to relate the dimensions of weight and partial, induction rather than one-shot insight. Thus, when used in
distance. In contrast, children who were asked to invent mathe- classrooms, it is important to help students tolerate the short-term
matics to explain their answers were more likely to discover that ambiguity of not being told the right answer. The effort to find and
proportionate ratios determine whether the scale balances. Without characterize the structure can improve learning and test perfor-
the precision and demand of quantitative reasoning, students may mance in the long run.
gloss over important specifics and possible relations. For instance, Toward a theoretical account. Cognitive psychology theo-
we would predict that students would not learn as well if they were ries provide one way to describe the mechanisms behind the
asked to explain the companies instead of inventing a quantitative effects of ICC. For example, one might propose that the inventing
index. activities helped students use analogical processes to abstract a
The brief inductive activities of the ICC physics lessons did not ratio schema and this abstract schema enabled the transfer (e.g.,
cover many important aspects of density and speed. One concern Gick & Holyoak, 1983). An alternative class of theories starts with
is that the mathematical focus of inventing may have been at the perception (e.g., Greeno, Smith, & Moore, 1993; Hofstadter,
expense of qualitative understanding. Our assumption, however, is 1995). In particular, J. J. Gibson’s (1979) ecological theories of
that helping students learn the deep structure prepares them to perception and perceptual learning provided useful guidance for
transfer to learn more about these concepts later. For instance, developing the predictions, measures, and contrasting cases. Al-
understanding density as a ratio of mass to volume should help though the ICC tasks engage cognitive mechanisms, and therefore
students understand buoyancy, where density, mass, and volume should not be reduced to purely perceptual mechanisms, many of
are frequently conflated. Thus, a useful study would determine the insights from perceptual learning apply.
whether this is true. With respect to predictions and measures, a perceptual learning
The ICC instruction did not improve strategic knowledge for account proposes that students need to learn to pick up or notice
finding patterns across multiple instances, as demonstrated by the information in the environment (E. J. Gibson, 1969). For example,
lack of a special benefit for the four-case transfer problem over the expert radiologists can see diagnostic details in X-rays overlooked
one-case problem. However, the current implementation did not by residents (Myles-Worsley, Johnston, & Simons, 1988), and
have the time to emphasize the development of strategic knowl-
sommeliers can differentiate wines that simply taste “red” to the
edge and scientific dispositions (Gresalfi & Cobb, 2006). Taylor,
uninitiated. If these experts had not learned to perceive the relevant
Smith, van Stolk, and Spiegelman (2010) found that students who
information in the stimulus array, their problem solving, judgment,
received a full course of inventing activities in college-level cell
and abilities to learn from future experiences would be seriously
biology were more able to generate explanations for novel cell
hampered.
phenomena compared with T&P students.
We included the redrawing test on the prediction that T&P
would not help students notice the ratio structure in the clowns
Conclusions worksheet, and therefore they would have no structures to remem-
ber when redrawing the worksheet. In addition, because they had
Practical applications of ICC. There are different types of
never learned to perceive ratio structures, they would not see them
learning that range from skill acquisition to identity formation, and
it seems unlikely that a single pedagogy or psychological mecha- embedded in the transfer tasks, and therefore they would fail to
nism will prove optimal for all types of learning. ICC is one among transfer. Our prediction was based on the assumption that telling
many possible ways to support students in learning deep structure. first would lead T&P students to pay attention to what they had
Other alternatives to T&P include problem-, project-, and inquiry- just learned (the formulas) rather than search for the new infor-
based instruction, as well as approaches that attempt to “problema- mation in the worksheet (the physical ratio structure). In this
tize” tasks so that students will not take the eventual solutions for regard, T&P can sometimes exacerbate a more general phenome-
granted (Bransford et al., 1989; Fensham & Kass, 1988; Hiebert et non, where prior knowledge filters out perceptual information, in
al., 1996; Limón, 2001; Needham & Begg, 1991). All these ap- part, because people presume they have seen all that is necessary
proaches will support productive transfer to the extent they help to complete the task at hand (Neisser & Becklen, 1975; Nickerson
students find the deep structure that generalizes across situations. & Adams, 1979; Simons & Levin, 1998; Stevens & Hall, 1998).
The ICC activities differ from many of these pedagogies, however, With respect to the materials, the explicit use of contrasting
because ICC strategically precedes standard T&P pedagogy rather cases for learning found initial expression in J. J. Gibson and
than replaces it. Moreover, the ICC activities do not sacrifice Gibson’s (1955) demonstration that people learn to pick up infor-
overall classroom efficiency for the sake of transfer. The ICC mation in the environment by progressively noticing structure
treatment required about 10 extra minutes of instruction across 4 across systematic variation. According to J. J. Gibson (1979), a
days, which was mostly spent on explaining the novel task of major task for psychology involves identifying the physical infor-
inventing an index. mation that makes perception possible. This differs from the cog-
The inventing task differs from discovery tasks because students nitive enterprise of identifying the mental structures that enable
do not have to rediscover the answer discovered by experts. people to enrich or go beyond the available information (see also
According to Experiment 2, students actually did reinvent versions J. J. Gibson & Gibson, 1955). Identifying the information that
of the true formulas over 80% of the time. But other work has makes perception possible is highly relevant to the design of
demonstrated that if students notice a subset of the relevant struc- guided discovery lessons, where the goal is to have students induce
ture, this is sufficient to help them make sense of the rest, given a the structure of phenomena. If the cases used for instruction do not
formal exposition (Kapur, 2008; Schwartz & Bransford, 1998). include sufficient information, students will not be able to perceive
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 15

the underlying structure. Perceptual theory provides guidance for problem such as missing the mental schema of ratio. It would also
deciding what information to include in the examples. be an interesting exercise to determine whether cognitive and
Perception and induction depend on finding structure within perceptual theories would make the same prescriptions for creating
variability (e.g., Paas & Van Merriënboer, 1994). For the topic of the cases from which students learn. In the meantime, a major take
physics learning, the theory of a perceptual gradient helps specify away from the current research is that it is not sufficient for
what variability to include in the examples. According to J. J. students only to hear and practice symbolic explanations, lest the
Gibson (1979), gradients make perception possible. A simple structure of the phenomenon gets lost in plain sight.
example of a gradient is how objects in the distance appear smaller
than nearby objects. Paintings capitalize on the receding-into-the-
distance gradient so that people can see relative distances and References
object size on flat canvas. The gradient makes it possible to see
Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning
depth. Painting a single object would not suffice. Creating instruc-
from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples re-
tional examples is similar to creating a painting. Examples need to search. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214. doi:10.3102/
include sufficient information for students to perceive the desired 00346543070002181
property. If we take the example of density, a single instance does Barron, B. J. S., Schwartz, D. L., Vye, N. J., Moore, A., Petrosino, A.,
not provide sufficient information for students to perceive density; Zech, L., . . . The Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt.
there needs to be a gradient of densities. (1998). Doing with understanding: Lessons from research on problem-
The theory of gradients led to the specific contrasting cases used and project-based learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 7, 271–
in this study. For the crowded clowns worksheet (see Figure 1), we 311. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0703&4_2
created a density gradient across three companies. We chose three Barrows, H. S., & Tamblyn, R. M. (1976). An evaluation of problem-based
companies, instead of two, to specify the linear aspect of the learning in small groups utilizing a simulated patient. Journal of Medical
Education, 51, 52–54.
gradient. We also used the same context (clowns within buses) so
Bassok, M., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Interdomain transfer between iso-
that students could more readily discern the gradient than if we had morphic topics in algebra and physics. Journal of Experimental Psy-
used three different contexts (e.g., an example of clowns in buses, chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 153–166. doi:10.1037/
an example of atoms in a sphere). For the students in the current 0278-7393.15.1.153
studies, it was also necessary to include information specifying a Biederman, I., & Shiffrar, M. M. (1987). Sexing day-old chicks: A case
second gradient. Students of this age also needed to notice the study and expert systems analysis of a difficult perceptual-learning task.
proportionality of mass-to-volume for a given density. Therefore, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
within each company, we created a gradient by varying the number tion, 13, 640 – 645. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.4.640
of buses and clowns for its given density. Bowers, J., Cobb, P., & McClain, K. (1999). The evolution of mathemat-
Our methodology for designing the contrasting cases involved ical practices: A case study. Cognition and Instruction, 17, 25– 64.
doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1701_2
identifying and creating the relevant gradients. It is a method for
Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J., Vye, N. J., & Sherwood, R. D. (1989). New
deciding what similarities and differences to include in the cases.
approaches to instruction: Because wisdom can’t be told. In S. Vosnia-
It differs from asking students to explore or freely experiment in dou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp.
the hopes they will generate the relevant gradient information. It 470 – 497). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
also differs from picking instructional examples based on other CBO9780511529863.022
considerations, such as everyday familiarity. Bransford, J. D., & Schwartz, D. L. (1999). Rethinking transfer: A simple
Although the materials were created to include the sufficient and proposal with multiple implications. In A. Iran-Nejad & P. D. Pearson
relevant gradients, the inventing directive was included so that the (Eds.), Review of research in education (Vol. 24, pp. 61–101). Wash-
students would search for the property we intended them to per- ington, DC: American Educational Research Association.
ceive. The T&P condition demonstrated that the presence of in- Brown, A. L., & Kane, M. J. (1988). Preschool children can learn to
formation does not guarantee its uptake. Critically, the inventing transfer: Learning to learn and learning from example. Cognitive Psy-
chology, 20, 493–523. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(88)90014-X
task was also designed so that students would develop a rule that
Catrambone, R. (1998). The subgoal learning model: Creating better ex-
accounts for the gradients. In paintings, observers can see depth amples so that students can solve novel problems. Journal of Experi-
because artists have included the relevant gradient information. mental Psychology: General, 127, 355–376. doi:10.1037/0096-
The artists, however, differ from casual observers, because they 3445.127.4.355
also have a set of rules that enable them to produce the relevant Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive
gradients. They understand—rather than only experience—the re- Psychology, 4, 55– 81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2
lations among objects that produce the perception of depth (e.g., Edelson, D. C., Gordin, D. N., & Pea, R. D. (1999). Addressing the
how much to change relative sizes, shapes, and positions to indi- challenges of inquiry-based learning through technology and curriculum
cate the desired depth). With the inventing task, we simultaneously design. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8, 391– 450. doi:10.1207/
wanted students to pick up the gradient of density and to develop s15327809jls0803&4_3
Eisner, E. W. (1972). Educating artistic vision. New York, NY: Mac-
an understanding of the invariant that produces the gradient (ratios
millan.
of mass to volume).
Fensham, P. J., & Kass, H. (1988). Inconsistent or discrepant events in
It would take more refined research to parcel the relative value science instruction. Studies in Science Education, 15, 1–16. doi:10.1080/
of perceptual and cognitive theories for describing the learning in 03057268808559946
the current studies. For example, it would be useful to determine Forbus, K. (1984). Qualitative process theory. Artificial Intelligence, 24,
whether poor recall on the clown redrawing task is due to a failure 85–168. doi:10.1016/0004-3702(84)90038-9
to perceive the ratios in the first place or due to a cognitive Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and
16 SCHWARTZ, CHASE, OPPEZZO, AND CHIN

transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning.
Psychology, 95, 393– 408. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.393 Psychological Science, 15, 661– 667. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004
Gentner, D., & Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and .00737.x
similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45–56. doi:10.1037/0003- Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2004). Modeling natural variation through
066X.52.1.45 distribution. American Educational Research Journal, 41, 635– 679.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. doi:10.3102/00028312041003635
New York, NY: Meredith. Lesh, R. A., & Doerr, H. M. (Eds.). (2003). Beyond constructivism: Models
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, and modeling perspectives on mathematics problem solving, learning,
MA: Houghton Mifflin. and teaching. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning: Differentiation Limón, M. (2001). On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for
or enrichment. Psychological Review, 62, 32– 41. doi:10.1037/h0048826 conceptual change: A critical appraisal. Learning and Instruction, 11,
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical 357–380. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(00)00037-2
transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38. doi:10.1016/0010- Mayer, R. E. (2009). Constructivism as a theory of learning versus con-
0285(83)90002-6 structivism as a prescription for instruction. In S. Tobias & T. M. Duffy
Gick, M. L., & Paterson, K. (1992). Do contrasting examples facilitate (Eds.), Constructivist instruction: Success or failure (pp. 184 –200).
schema acquisition and analogical transfer? Canadian Journal of Psy- New York, NY: Routledge.
chology, 46, 539 –550. doi:10.1037/h0084333 McNeil, N. M., Uttal, D. H., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2009). Should
Goldstone, R. L., & Sakamoto, Y. (2003). The transfer of abstract princi- you show me the money? Concrete objects both hurt and help perfor-
ples governing complex adaptive systems. Cognitive Psychology, 46, mance on mathematics problems. Learning and Instruction, 19, 171–
414 – 466. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00519-4 184. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.03.005
Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles Michael, A. L., Klee, T., Bransford, J. D., & Warren, S. (1993).
using concrete and idealized simulations. Journal of the Learning Sci- The transition from theory to therapy: Test of two instructional
ences, 14, 69 –110. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1401_4 methods. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 139 –153. doi:10.1002/
Greeno, J. G., Smith, D. R., & Moore, J. L. (1993). Transfer of situated acp.2350070206
learning. In D. K. Detterman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Transfer on trial: Myles-Worsley, M., Johnston, W. A., & Simons, M. A. (1988). The
Intelligence, cognition, and instruction (pp. 99 –167). Norwood, NJ: influence of expertise on X-ray image processing. Journal of Experi-
Ablex. mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 553–557.
Gresalfi, M. S., & Cobb, P. (2006). Cultivating students’ discipline-specific doi:10.1037/0278-7393.14.3.553
dispositions as a critical goal for pedagogy and equity. Pedagogies, 1, Needham, D. R., & Begg, I. M. (1991). Problem-oriented training pro-
49 –57. doi:10.1207/s15544818ped0101_8 motes spontaneous analogical transfer: Memory-oriented training pro-
Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1998). How seductive details do their damage: motes memory for training. Memory & Cognition, 19, 543–557. doi:
A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational 10.3758/BF03197150
Psychology, 90, 414 – 434. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.3.414 Neisser, U., & Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: Attending to visually
Hiebert, J., & Stigler, J. W. (2004). A world of difference: Classrooms specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 480 – 494. doi:10.1016/0010-
abroad provide lessons in teaching math and science. Journal of Staff 0285(75)90019-5
Development, 25, 10 –15. Nickerson, R. S., & Adams, M. J. (1979). Long-term memory for a
Hiebert, J., Thomas, C. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, K., Human, P., Murray, H., common object. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 287–307. doi:10.1016/0010-
. . . Wearne, D. (1996). Problem solving as a basis for reform in curric- 0285(79)90013-6
ulum and instruction: The case of mathematics. Educational Researcher, Paas, F. G. W. C., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of
25, 12–21. doi:10.3102/0013189X025004012 worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A
Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding, and skill in cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–
multidigit addition and subtraction. Cognition and Instruction, 14, 251– 133. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122
283. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1403_1 Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2007). Does comparing solution methods
Hofstadter, D. H. (1995). Fluid concepts and creative analogies: Computer facilitate conceptual and procedural knowledge: An experimental study
models of the fundamental mechanisms of thought. New York, NY: on learning to solve equations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99,
Basic Books. 561–574. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.561
Jee, B. D., Gentner, D., Forbus, K., Sageman, B., & Uttal, D. H. (2009). Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2009). Compared with what? The effects
Drawing on experience: Use of sketching to evaluate knowledge of of different comparisons on conceptual knowledge and procedural flex-
spatial scientific concepts. In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), ibility for equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101,
Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science 529 –544. doi:10.1037/a0014224
Society (pp. 2499 –2504). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. Roll, I. (2009). Structured invention activities to prepare students for future
Kaminski, J. A., Sloutsky, V. M., & Heckler, A. F. (2008). The advantage learning: Means, mechanisms, and cognitive processes (Doctoral dis-
of abstract examples in learning math. Science, 320, 454 – 455. doi: sertation). Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA.
10.1126/science.1154659 Rothkopf, E. Z., & Billington, M. J. (1979). Goal guided learning for text:
Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 379 – Inferring a descriptive processing model from inspection times and eye
424. doi:10.1080/07370000802212669 movements. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 310 –327. doi:
Kapur, M. (2010). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. 10.1037/0022-0663.71.3.310
Instructional Science, 38, 523–550. doi:10.1007/s11251-009-9093-x Schmidt, H. G., De Volder, M. L., De Grave, W. S., Moust, J. H. C., &
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal Patel, V. L. (1989). Explanatory models in the processing of science
guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure text: The role of prior knowledge activation through small-group dis-
of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and cussion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 610 – 619. doi:10.1037/
inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75– 86. doi: 0022-0663.81.4.610
10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1 Schwartz, D. L., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition
Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early and Instruction, 16, 475–522. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1604_4
COMPARING USES OF CONTRASTING CASES 17

Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., & Bransford, J. D. (in press). Adaptive technoscience. In M. Lampert & M. L. Blunk (Eds.), Talking mathe-
transfer: The tension of routines and novelty. Educational Psychologist. matics in school: Studies of teaching and learning (pp. 107–148).
Schwartz, D. L., Lindgren, R., & Lewis, S. (2009). Constructivism in an Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
age of non-constructivist assessments. In S. Tobias & T. M. Duffy Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.
(Eds.), Constructivist instruction: Success or failure (pp. 34 – 61). New Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1202_4
York, NY: Routledge. Taylor, J. L., Smith, K. M., van Stolk, A. P., & Spiegelman, G. B. (2010).
Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future Using invention to change how students tackle problems. CBE—Life
learning: The hidden efficiency of encouraging original student produc- Sciences Education, 9, 504 –512. doi:10.1187/cbe.10-02-0012
tion in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 129 –184. Tobias, S., & Duffy, T. M. (Eds.). (2009). Constructivist instruction:
doi:10.1207/s1532690xci2202_1 Success or failure. New York, NY: Routledge.
Schwartz, D. L., Martin, T., & Pfaffman, J. (2005). How mathematics Tuovinen, J. E., & Sweller, J. (1999). A comparison of cognitive load
propels the development of physical knowledge. Journal of Cognition associated with discovery learning and worked examples. Journal of
and Development, 6, 65– 88. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0601_5 Educational Psychology, 91, 334 –341. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.334
Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between con- Williams, J. J., & Lombrozo, T. (2010). The role of explanation in
cepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, discovery and generalization: Evidence from category learning. Cogni-
46(1, Serial No. 189). tive Science, 34, 776 – 806. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01113.x
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people
in a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 644 – Received January 15, 2010
649. doi:10.3758/BF03208840 Revision received July 20, 2011
Stevens, R., & Hall, R. (1998). Disciplined perception: Learning to see in Accepted July 20, 2011 䡲

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen