Sie sind auf Seite 1von 19

Samantha N.N. Cross & Mary C.

Gilly

Cultural Competence and Cultural


Compensatory Mechanisms in
Binational Households
Although it is well-known that the U.S. population is increasingly culturally diverse, cultural diversity within U.S.
households is less recognized. This study investigates the effects of cultural dynamics on decision roles and
influence within the binational household. In particular, the authors study households in which one spouse is from
the United States and the other is an immigrant. The analysis uses survey data and in-depth interviews. Cultural
competence (i.e., knowledge of country of residence) as a source of expert power and as a form of cultural capital
in family decision making emerge as overarching themes. The authors also find that one family member may
engage in cultural compensatory mechanisms in consumption in response to the immigrant family member’s
sacrifices (i.e., moving to the United States). The authors close with a discussion of the implications for family
decision-making theory, marketers, and society.

Keywords: family decision making, expertise, cultural competence, cross-cultural, cultural compensatory
mechanisms

“...for better or for worse, for richer, for poorer, in sick- When I Love Lucy was introduced in 1951, the foreign-born
ness and in health, to love and to cherish, [in my country population was less than 7%, dropping to its nadir of 4.7%
or in yours?]” (9.6 million) in the 1970 census (Malone et al. 2003). How-
In the early 1950s, one of the most popular television ever, since the Hart-Celler Act removed race-based restric-
sitcoms in U.S. history aired, starring Lucille Ball and her tions on immigration, the number of foreign-born residents
real-life husband Desi Arnaz as Lucy and Ricky Ricardo. I in the United States has steadily increased to more than
Love Lucy is considered an American television classic 12% (36.7 million) (U.S. Census Bureau 2010). The rate of
today; yet at the time, the networks were hesitant to feature intermarriage between immigrants and natives in the United
a sitcom starring an American of Scottish ancestry married States still remains below the marriage rate between immi-
to a Cuban bandleader, as network producers felt that view- grants who were born in the same country. Yet the increas-
ers would not believe that Arnaz was truly Ball’s husband ing number of immigrants has clearly had an impact. Bean
(Andrews 1985; Brooks and Marsh 2003). The academic and Stevens’s (2003) analysis of 1995 U.S. Census data
literature has mirrored this perspective. Although researchers reveals that 29% of foreign-born men had U.S.-born wives
increasingly recognized the household as a critical and and 33% of foreign-born women had U.S.-born husbands.
central decision-making and consumption unit (Commuri In light of this demographic reality, it is evident that bina-
and Gentry 2000; Davis 1976), they also made a crucial tional households (with partners from different countries)
underlying assumption about family composition: cultural are a new, unique, and relevant context for an investigation
homogeneity. of consumer decision making and influence.
The foreign-born population of the United States has Epp and Price (2008) suggest that family consumption
burgeoned since the passage of the Hart-Celler Act in 1965. research has also ignored the notion that households act as
collective enterprises. In their study of family identity, they
argue that the focus on the relative influence of individual
spouses has led researchers to ignore family “collective iden-
Samantha N.N. Cross is Assistant Professor of Marketing, College of

tity tensions” (Epp and Price 2011, p. 38). The challenges of


Business, Iowa State University (e-mail: snncross@iastate.edu). Mary C.

“being a family” are viewed as central to the consumption


Gilly is Professor of Marketing, The Paul Merage School of Business, Uni-

experiences of contemporary families; such challenges, we


versity of California, Irvine (e-mail: mcgilly@uci.edu). The authors grate-

argue, are exacerbated by the cultural heterogeneity within


fully acknowledge financial support to the first author through the Ray

binational families.
Watson Doctoral Fellowship at University of California, Irvine, and through

This research extends knowledge on household decision


the Academy of Marketing Science Jane K. Fenyo Best Paper Award for
Student Research. The first author thanks her family for their unwavering

making, focusing on binational households. Here, one part-


support of her research. The authors also thank their colleagues Terry

ner in the household is an immigrant to the United States—


Childers, John Graham, Hope Schau, Maura Scott, and Bill Qualls for

born and raised in another country and immigrated to the


their critical feedback and encouragement, as well as three anonymous

United States after high school. The other partner is a U.S.


JM reviewers for their guidance throughout the review process. Robert
Kozinets served as area editor for this article.

© 2014, American Marketing Association Journal of Marketing


ISSN: 0022-2429 (print), 1547-7185 (electronic) 121 Vol. 78 (May 2014), 121–139
citizen—born and raised in the United States, the family’s field, Linton and Herskovits 1936). At the group level,
country of residence. acculturation has received a great deal of attention in
We ask the following key research questions: (1) What anthropology (Rudmin 2003), although individual accultur-
is the individual and collective impact of cultural diversity ation (sometimes called transculturation) has received less
in binational homes? (2) What contributes to relative influ- notice. In consumer research, there are a few studies on the
ence and drives the allocations of decision-making roles? impact of culture on the consumption decisions of mono-
and (3) How do the decisions that prevail in the formative cultural immigrant families (Peñaloza and Gilly 1986,
stages of the household affect decisions made in later stages
1999; Wallendorf and Reilly 1983b; Webster 2000) and a
of the household? We intend to examine not only who
few articles on the role of culture within binational families
makes decisions but also the context in which decisions are
made, during both the formative and maintenance stages of and partnerships (Lauth Bacas 2002; Nelson and Desh-
the household. This research thus challenges another pande 2004; Nelson and Otnes 2005).
implicit assumption in the family decision-making literature Nelson and Deshpande (2004) examine the decision
stream, which to date has presumed that household decision making of binational couples in wedding planning, extend-
making takes place only after the household exists. We take ing cultural ambivalence (Merton and Barber 1976) to
a discovery-oriented approach, using a mixed-method cross-cultural ambivalence, defined as a mixed emotional
embedded design for data collection (Creswell and Plano state arising from conflict between two cultures. They find
Clark 2011). This multiperspective approach enhances con- that binational couples modify wedding rituals and try to
textual understanding, corroborates the completeness of the accommodate different ritual audiences (parents and friends
findings, and assists in illustrating the quantitative results from each culture). Integrating multiple cultural elements
with the qualitative findings (Bryman 2006). into one ceremony results in greater creativity, as Breger
We empirically demonstrate that relative cultural compe- and Hill (1998) also note in other contexts. Nelson and
tence (i.e., knowledge of the traditions, social norms, mar- Otnes’s (2005) netnography of postings on wedding mes-
kets, language, and expections of the culture of residence)
sage boards indicates that binational couples use compro-
in culturally heterogeneous households is both a source of
mise to cope with conflicts concerning various wedding
expertise and a form of cultural capital. Our findings com-
plicate the conceptualization of gender roles and conjugal elements. While studies of brides and grooms with subcul-
power found in prior research. We also show that household tural differences (Otnes, Lowrey, and Shrum 1997) have
decision roles and relative influence at various stages of the found similar coping mechanisms, Nelson and Otnes (2005)
household are interrelated and often counterbalanced. Alter- attribute cross-cultural brides’ wider range of coping
native strategies to emphasize the immigrant spouse’s con- mechanisms to the complexity of their task. Note that these
sumption preferences and enhance the relevance of the studies examine binational couples solely at the formative
immigrant spouse’s culture within the home—“cultural stage of the family, and only in the context of one event.
compensatory mechanisms”—are revealed. These cultural In her research on cross-border marriages between
compensatory mechanisms are used to even out perceptions Greek–German couples, Lauth Bacas (2002) notes that
of “sacrifice,” highlighting the need for a more holistic binational partners must develop coping strategies to deal
approach to the study of family decision making. Our find- with disparate family networks as well as specific compe-
ings have implications for expanding family decision-making tencies such as tolerance, patience, and the ability to find
theory and furthering understanding of the adaptation patterns
and live with compromise. These couples face both a need
of immigrant populations in the United States. Our results
and an opportunity to create a common family culture that
will also help managerial and political decision makers
appreciate the dynamics involved in cross-cultural interac- bridges their cultural differences. Meng and Gregory (2005,
tions in the most fundamental group: the family. p. 1) define intermarriage as a union of an immigrant with a
In this article, we present our theoretical framework, non-English-speaking background and a native. They sug-
followed by our methodology. We provide a detailed analy- gest that in Australia, through intermarriage, immigrants
sis and discussion of the findings and highlight the main acquire “host country customs, language skills, knowledge
themes and implications stemming from the study. of the local labour market, and obtain contacts and connec-
tions,” speeding economic assimilation. They find signifi-
Theoretical Framework cantly higher incomes for immigrants who intermarry ver-
sus immigrants who do not. We argue that, in addition to a
Binational and Bicultural Partnerships labor market advantage, intermarriage offers a consumption
advantage because immigrants who intermarry learn the
Although there is an increasing body of research on bicul-
tural consumers, the emphasis has been on immigrant consumer market as well. In her study of children of Asian
groups and their ability to traverse and balance cultural immigrants, Park (2005) documents an important role for
norms and identity-related expectations in their home and consumption in creating an American household identity.
host countries (Lau-Gesk 2003; Luna, Ringberg, and Perac- Cross and Gilly (2013a) note that binational families act as
chio 2008; Oswald 1999; Park 2005). Acculturation occurs links between cultures, both within and outside the house-
as a result of two cultures coming into contact with subse- hold, and provide a setting for decision making and creative
quent changes in the cultural patterns of both groups (Red- consumption.

122 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


Family Decision Making family decision-making literature stream has focused on
For 50 years, research in household decision making focused economic sources of power, ignoring the power and status
on the influence of gender on decision-making roles (Davis that cultural knowledge bestows. Robbins (2005) examines
1970; Green and Cunningham 1975; Shuptrine and Samuel- the development of Bourdieu’s concept of cultural capital,
son 1976) and the decision-making process (Davis and concluding that Bourdieu’s interest was in understanding
Rigaux 1974; Putnam and Davidson 1987). Wilk and Net- cultural tastes and the use of objective culture. We argue
ting (1984, p. 2) argue that “as a culturally defined emic that the understanding of one’s culture of residence in gen-
unit, the household is certainly of analytic value.” Studies eral, not merely understanding cultural tastes, is also an asset
on household decision making have been conducted in dif- that confers power in decision making within the family.
ferent countries (Davis and Rigaux 1974; Webster 2000), as Bourdieu’s (1986) concept of cultural capital has been
researchers have discovered that marital roles differ across investigated extensively in educational sociology. DiMaggio
cultures (Green et al. 1983). However, these non–North and Mohr’s (1985) influential longitudinal study examines
American studies remained culturally homogeneous within the effects of status-culture participation on educational
the family units studied. attainment and marital selection. The authors operationalize
Research has shown that the base of conjugal power in a cultural capital as participation in high culture (e.g., attend-
particular household often drives gender differences in mari- ing symphony concerts, arts events, having a “cultivated
tal roles (Blood and Wolfe 1960; Raven, Centers, and self-image”). Other authors have used similar operational-
Rodrigues 1975; Wolfe 1959). These gender differences izations of cultural capital to investigate outcomes such as
have evolved over time as marital roles have become more educational attainment (De Graaf 1986) and English lan-
egalitarian (Belch and Willis 2002; Lackman and Lanasa guage achievement (Khodadady, Alaee, and Natanzi 2011).
1993; Qualls 1987). Commuri and Gentry (2000, p. 10) criti- Kingston (2001) questions the way scholars have
cize this literature stream for suggesting that many family applied cultural capital theory in education research, calling
decisions are “either/or” without recognizing that these it “the unfulfilled promise.” Lareau and Weininger (2003, p.
decisions “not only constitute conscious choice, but also 587) are critical of what they call the “dominant interpreta-
incorporate a shared consensus, mutual trust, and a desire to tion” of cultural capital as competence in “high-brow” aes-
maintain harmony.” Epp and Price (2008) support this cri- thetic culture, deeming it a narrower definition than Bour-
tique, contending that the family research literature has dieu intended: “the critical aspect of cultural capital is that
ignored the notion that households act as collective enter- it allows culture to be used as a resource that provides
prises and experience the dynamics and tensions manifest access to scarce rewards, is subject to monopolization, and,
in any collective identity. Economic capital as a source of under certain conditions, may be transmitted from one gen-
conjugal power has also been overemphasized in the family eration to the next.” We agree that previous literature has
decision-making literature stream, to the detriment of con- conceptualized cultural capital too narrowly, ignoring the
sidering other forms of capital. resources that accrue to people who have a deep under-
standing of the culture in which they reside.
Expertise and Capital in the Home We suggest that understanding the dominant culture in
A major focus of the family decision-making research has terms of day-to-day norms and expectations—what we term
been the power that stems from the economic capital the “cultural competence”—is a basic form of cultural capital
spouses contribute, with a proliferation of theories such as that is readily available to people raised in that culture and
resource theory (Blood and Wolfe 1960), social power at least partially hidden from nonnatives. Although such
theory, and gender- or category-based theories (Davis 1970; knowledge may lead to understanding high culture (e.g.,
Qualls 1987). It is argued that shifts in power occur as a becoming familiar with Chinese opera when living in
result of shifts in economic resources. Thus, greater egali- China), we argue that understanding how Norwegians shop
tarianism in decision making in the home coincided with for bread in Norway or the basics of cricket in India is also
women joining the workforce (Lackman and Lanasa 1993; a form of cultural capital and a source of expertise. Lan-
Putnam and Davidson 1987). guage can also be a form of cultural capital (Blackledge
Bases of power have been categorized as coercive, 2001). Immigrants find their cultural knowledge lacking
reward-based, legitimate, referent, and expert (Belch and when engaging in consumption activities such as shopping,
Willis 2002; Raven, Centers, and Rodrigues 1975; Webster eating, and socializing. Those who possess cultural compe-
2000). Expert power is relevant here and is defined as the tence are advantaged in daily life both outside and within
power that comes from reliance on a family member’s supe- the home.
rior knowledge and experience. Expertise as a source of Wilk (1989, p. 30) criticizes consumer research for
power is also based on trust and the belief that the particular elaborating particular concepts and tools to understand
family member indeed possesses substantial knowledge or household decision making “in a narrowly defined cultural
experience (French and Raven 1959; Raven 1992; Raven, context,” and yet little has improved in the ensuing years.
Schwarzwald, and Koslowsky 1998). Commuri and Gentry (2005) also advocate attention to the
Bourdieu’s (1986) concept of cultural capital—defined diversity of household forms (e.g., households in which
as assets in the form of one’s talents and intellect (embodied wives are breadwinners, second marriages), which we
state), cultural goods (objectified state), and educational extend to include culturally diverse households. By examin-
qualifications (institutionalized state)—is relevant here. The ing binational households, we expose the role of cultural

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 123


competence as a dimension of expert power and a form of decisions with respect to marital roles. The locations of the
cultural capital. In addition, we record efforts to creatively decisions on the triangle are based on an average across the
address cultural differences. four aspects of the decision process as well as across par-
Our focus is on family decision making in binational ticipants. Decisions for which the mean relative influence is
families, in which we find that cultural competence influ- 1.5 or less are husband-dominant, 2.5 or more are wife-
ences the allocation of decision roles. Cultural competence dominant, and between 1.5 and 2.5 are joint: either syn-
can be considered a type of cultural capital in that resources cratic (defined as more than 50% of families surveyed make
are more available to those who possess it. a decision jointly) or autonomic (less than 50% of families
make a decision jointly). The decision phases (syncratic or
Methodology autonomic) can have implications for the type of hierarchi-
cal models and communication strategies to be used for
Data for the study were collected through surveys and 60- these families (Davis and Rigaux 1974). The feasibility tri-
to 90-minute in-depth interviews with spouses in binational angle represents the theoretical distribution of husband–
households. Data were collected from 16 couples and 2 indi- wife authority relationships. The greater the shared author-
vidual spouses whose partners were unable to participate. ity, the more equal the relative authority of the husband and
The 18 families consist of 10 immigrant women married to wife.
U.S. men and 8 immigrant men married to U.S. women. Analysis of the interview data followed the grounded
The study uses a purposive sample, and spouses were inter- theory approach, using differing levels of coding and an
viewed separately and consecutively. The sample included iterative back-and-forth process between the emerging cate-
participants from key immigrant groups in the United States gories and the literature to make sense of the data and
(Filipino and Mexican) as well as participants from a wider
develop themes (Glaser and Strauss 1967; Locke 1996;
range of countries: Taiwan, Chile, Australia, Iran, Belarus,
Strauss and Corbin 1998). Our emphasis is on acquiring
Vietnam, and South Africa. The split by gender and nation-
deep insights into binational families, their decision mak-
ality was driven by availability and the demographic char-
ing, and the processes by which these households accom-
acteristics of out-marriage (i.e., marriage outside one’s eth-
modate cultural preferences. Our ultimate aim is to generate
nic group) in the United States. Breger and Hill (1998, p. 2)
theory about culture’s influence on family decision making
argue that “there are features common to almost all cross-
and consumption.
cultural marriages,” even when comparing very different
As part of a follow-up study, we interviewed six addi-
cultural contexts. The 34 participants all have at least some
college education and reside in middle-class neighborhoods tional adult participants, two men and four women, who
with incomes ranging from $45,000 to $113,000. Table 1 grew up in binational homes in the United States. These
provides a profile of the interview participants. participants had parents from Ghana and the United States,
At the beginning of the interview sessions, participants Germany and the United States, Japan and the United
completed a survey that asked about the decision maker in States, and Nigeria and the United States. These partici-
the household for different aspects of the decision-making pants described their childhood, current experiences, and
process for 15 product categories. Categories are based on the influence of the dual cultures on their consumption,
questionnaires used by Davis and Rigaux (1974) and Put- societal interactions, and perceptions of identity. We used
nam and Davidson (1987), who viewed these categories as these additional data to explicate some of the findings in
representative of the larger group of economically conse- this study. We audiotaped all interviews and later tran-
quential decisions within families. The survey was used to scribed and extensively reviewed the interview data.
assist informants in thinking about the subject matter, facil-
itate further discussion, and gather crucial decision-making Decision-Making Roles and
data about decision roles within these families. This mixed-
method embedded design (Creswell and Plano Clark 2011)
Influence
enabled us to collect both quantitative and qualitative data Survey responses showed spousal agreement at an average
within a single participant interaction. of 70% and 73% for the decision categories of “when to
We mapped the survey data using the perceptual mapping purchase” and “what to purchase,” respectively, and 67%
technique (feasibility triangle) originally suggested by Wolfe and 65% for the decision categories of “where to purchase”
(1959) and later employed by Davis and Rigaux (1974). and “how much to pay.” This result is consistent with prior
The vertical axis of the triangle represents the relative influ- literature using similar types of decision categories (Davis
ence of the husband and wife in the decision, ranging from and Rigaux 1974; Putnam and Davidson 1987). Decisions
husband-dominant = 1, joint = 2, to wife-dominant = 3, as involving other groceries, vehicles, children’s toys/clothing,
indicated by the participant. The horizontal axis shows the and children’s schools/colleges had the highest levels of
extent of role specialization as measured by the percentage agreement (same), whereas decisions on computer-related
of families reporting that a decision is jointly made. The equipment had the lowest agreement levels (different) (see
two axes converge at a point 2, 100%; the triangular feasi- Table 2).
bility region reflects that the axes are nonindependent. Figure 1 plots the mean relative influence of all 15 deci-
Mapping the data enables us to easily gauge both the aver- sion categories across all participants. Although they are not
age relative influence and extent of role specialization for generalizable due to the small size and purposive nature of
each decision as well as similarity or dissimilarity among the sample, the decision plots suggest a high degree of joint

124 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


TABLE 1
Participant Profiles
Cultural Identity Immigrant Spouse

Participant Country of Perception of Age Years in Years of


Couple Name Origin Self Report Children’s Identity Gender (Years) Citizen United States Marriage Children Residence
1 Alonso Mexico Mexican Mexican American Male 41 Yes 20 10 Yes Orange County, CA
Edith United States American American Female 47 Yes N.A.
2 Gillian Philippines Filipino Filipino American Female 41 Yes 20 13 Yes Orange County, CA
3 Hannah Philippines Non-American Citizens of the world Female 41 No 20 11 Yes Orange County, CA
John United States American No label, just people Male 50 Yes N.A.
4 Lanna United States American Mexican American Female 40 Yes N.A. 11 Yes Orange County, CA
Jorge Mexico Mexican Mexican American Male 40 Yes 15
5 Candice United States American American Female 42 Yes N.A. 20 Yes Orange County, CA
6 Cassie Philippines Filipino American American Filipino Female 39 Yes 22 9 Yes Orange County, CA
Ed United States Filipino American American Filipino Male 38 Yes N.A.
7 Jim United States Filipino Filipino American, fused Male 41 Yes N.A. 1 Expecting Honolulu, HI
w/local Hawaiian values
Sarah Philippines Filipino —c Female 31 No 2
8 Julio Mexico Mexican American with Male 53 No 24 23 Yes Orange County, CA
Mexican roots
Sally United States American —c Female 59 Yes N.A.
9 Terry United States American Filipino Black American Male 49 Yes N.A. 15a Yes Los Angeles
County, CA
Ria Philippines Filipino Filipino American Female 44 Yesb 28 6
Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 125

10 Rosa United States Mexican American Mexican American Female 26 Yes N.A. 6 Yes Orange County, CA
David Mexico Mexican American Mexican American Male 32 Yes 14
11 Leila Taiwan Asian American American Female 48 Yes 17 17 Yes Orange County, CA
Kenny United States Irish Chinese American Male 51 Yes N.A.
12 Simone Chile Chilean and American American Female 28 No 4 4 Yes Orange County, CA
Charlie United States Caucasian Caucasian Male 28 Yes N.A.
13 Sharon United States White/Caucasian White/Caucasian Female 38 Yes N.A. 7 Yes Orange County, CA
Colin Australia Caucasian or Australian Australian American Male 34 No 9
14 Karen United States Caucasian Caucasian Female 36 Yes N.A. 5 Yes Orange County, CA
Alan Iran White/Caucasian American Male 45 Yes 23
15 Leona Former Soviet Caucasian Caucasian Female 31 Yes 10 2 Yes Orange County, CA
Union, now Belarus
Simon United States Caucasian Not sure Male 35 Yes N.A.
16 Liang Vietnam Chinese Half Chinese, Female 48 Yes 19 10 Yes Orange County, CA
half American
Daniel United States Caucasian Asian and Caucasian Male 47 Yes N.A.
17 Linda United States White not Hispanic White/American Female 45 Yes N.A. 8 Yes Orange County, CA
Gerald South Africa Caucasian Caucasian Male 42 Yes 8
18 Kent United States Caucasian Japanese or Asian or Male 30 Yes N.A. 5 Yes Orange County, CA
more than one ethnicity
Tokimi Japan Japanese Japanese American Female 36 No 8
aRespondent seems to be including number of years of cohabitation before marriage.
bNaturalized citizen at birth through father.
cRespondent did not provide this response.
Notes: N.A. = not applicable. Participants’ names have been disguised.
TABLE 2
Response Agreement Between Spouses
When to Purchase? Where to Purchase? What to Purchase? How Much to Pay?

Same Different Same Different Same Different Same Different


(%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%)
Food 63 38 69 31 56 44 56 44
Other grocery items 81 19 88 13 75 25 81 19
Small appliances 75 25 63 38 56 44 56 44
Major appliances 81 19 67 33 67 33 67 33
Computer equipment 53 47 47 53 53 47 53 47
Furniture 69 26 69 31 63 38 56 44
Vehicles 94 6 69 31 88 13 75 25
Home decorations/decor 60 40 50 50 69 31 44 56
Music 60 40 57 43 71 29 57 43
Children’s toys and clothing 88 13 69 31 75 25 75 25
Children’s schools/colleges 80 20 73 27 87 13 73 27
Investments and savings plans 69 31 75 25 81 19 81 19
Insurance (life, auto, home) 69 31 69 31 69 31 69 31
Vacations/family trips 69 31 73 27 75 25 69 31
Capital improvements/remodeling 80 20 67 33 71 29 57 43
Average agreement 73 27 67 33 70 30 65 35
Notes: Responses are from 16 families. This table does not include responses from surveys with only one spouse or responses for couples
with missing data for the particular question in a category.

FIGURE 1
Decisions for 18 Binational Families Surveyed

3.0

Wife-
Dominant Home decorations/decor

Children’s toys and


clothing

2.5

Relative Influence of Husbands and Wives


Other groceries
Food

Small appliances
Children’s schools/college
Furniture
Syncratic Vacation Autonomic
Major appliances
Investments
Vehicles Capital exp./remodeling
Insurance
Music

Computer equipment

1.5

Husband-Dominant
1.0
100% 50% 0%
Extent of Role Specialization

126 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


decision making—both autonomic and syncratic—within FIGURE 2
these families. Influence of the husbands and wives is shared Decisions for Families
in all but 2 of the 15 decisions. Greater egalitarianism is
A: Families with Immigrant Wife and American
consistent with the findings of Putnam and Davidson (1987),
Husband
who compare the Davis and Rigaux (1974) study with their
study for 12 data points. They hypothesized that over the
ten or more years following the Davis and Rigaux study, 3.0
D

movement away from sex-role-dominated decision making

Relative Influence of Husbands and Wives


toward more joint decisions reflected women’s increased
Wife-Dominant

resources and corresponding shifts toward egalitarianism.


Our data show a complete shift away from husband-
Other grocery

2.5
dominant decisions, and yet the wife still retains a dominant
Children’s toys and clothing Food

Home decorations/Decor

influence over children’s toys and clothing and home deco-


rations and decor. The data also portray a heavier influence
Small appliances

of the wife over decisions regarding food and other gro-


ceries, a reflection of the prevailing roles of the wives in
C s/college Vacation Autonomic
Syncratic Furniture Major appliances

this study as primary caretakers of children and home, as


Vehicles
Investments Insurance

1.5
the interviews confirm. The main decision over which hus-
Capital exp./Remodeling
Music

bands still have a higher relative influence is computer


Computer equipment

equipment. 1.0
Husband-Dominant

Data were then divided between families with American 100% 50% 0%
husbands and immigrant wives and families with immigrant Extent of Role Specialization
husbands and American wives. Our analysis again shows
that husbands and wives tend to share influence over deci- B: Families with Immigrant Husband and American
sions, as evidenced by the large proportion of joint decisions Wife
depicted in both triangles in Figure 2, Panels A and B. In
addition, decisions regarding children’s toys and clothing and 3.0
D

home decor still tend to be wife dominant, whereas deci-

Relative Influence of Husbands and Wives


sions regarding computer equipment still have a relatively
Wife-Dominant

greater husband influence. These findings are consistent


Home decorations/Decor

with the overall mapping across the families in Figure 1. 2.5


Children’s toys & clothing

In the realm of shared influence, the pattern of decisions


differs. Within the immigrant husband/American wife fami-
lies, we observe less dispersion and a stronger balance of
Children䇻s schools/college Investments

wife–husband influence across role specialization for joint


Small appliances
Other grocery
Major appliances Insurance

decisions versus the American husband/immigrant wife


Vacation
Syncratic Food
Furniture Capital Exp/Remodeling
Autonomic
Music

households. Decisions regarding food purchases and other


Vehicles

Computer equipment

groceries are wife dominant in the American husband/


1.5
immigrant wife households but remain a joint decision in
the immigrant husband/American wife households. Deci- 1.0
sions regarding investments, insurance, capital expendi-
Husband-Dominant

100% 50% 0%
tures, and remodeling show a greater husband influence in Extent of Role Specialization
the American husband/immigrant wife households but have
a stronger wife influence in the immigrant husband/Ameri-
can wife families. These findings differ from Figure 1 and decisions, consistent with prior literature (Childers and Fer-
from those of prior literature. The interview data provide rell 1981; Davis and Rigaux 1974). The wife handles
additional insights into product categories for which differ- investment and insurance decisions in only one American
ences are most distinct between the two types of binational husband/immigrant wife household, in which the wife
families: investment decisions and food purchasing. works in the accounting/finance field.
Jim (American husband) has been married to Sarah
Investment Decisions (immigrant wife) for one year. He notes that for investments
and savings plans, he tends to make the decisions because
In the American husband/immigrant wife households,
he is the one working:
investment and insurance decisions are either handled
jointly or by the husband. One reason for this allocation is Yes, and that is more a function of my work and because
the woman’s role in these households. Seven of the ten they offer these incentives and programs … built into the
immigrant wives interviewed in these households no longer employment package. So it is more a matter of practicality
rather than anything else.
work outside the home. Role allocation in these households
tends to be gender based, with money earned by the male However, in four of the eight immigrant husband/
spouse, who also tends to handle investment and insurance American wife households, the wives handle the investment

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 127


decisions. In two of the remaining four households, invest- American spouse and family. However, in six of the eight
ment decisions are handled jointly. Both spouses work out- immigrant husband/American wife households, both spouses
side the home in the eight immigrant husband/American play a role in meeting the differing culinary needs of the
wife households. Couples choose this household style for family. Karen (American wife) hates grocery shopping, but
several reasons, including prior experience, the origin of the as she discusses food purchase and preparation roles, it is
investments (earned vs. inherited), knowledge of the pre- clear that it is a shared process with her Iranian spouse:
vailing tax laws, and investment norms and prior estab-
I’m largely responsible for what we need. And for getting
lished relationships. Edith (American wife) notes, it out of the fridge and onto the table. And he’s in charge
[My husband] has some of his own investments, but my of getting it into the house.... I get it from the fridge to the
income is substantially greater than his, and he just lets table; he gets it to the fridge.
me invest my stuff myself. We talk about it, but he would
never say, “Do this” or “Don’t do this.”... It’s really up to Interviews indicate that food purchase is primarily
me. linked to food preparation. Yet meals in binational families
often reflect different cultural backgrounds. The immigrant
Alonso (her immigrant husband), corroborates her statement: wife knows how to prepare the dishes from her home coun-
Yeah, it’s because when I met her, she always had some- try and learns to prepare those of her husband. However, for
body doing that and I just didn’t want to. She was fine, several American wives, the dishes of their immigrant hus-
and the person [handling her investments] knew her very bands, while tolerated, may be regarded with disfavor. For
well.
example, Karen describes her immigrant husband’s weekly
yogurt drinks as “vile.” Linda also dislikes the South
Food Purchase Decisions African delicacy Bovril (a beef or yeast extract spread),
The data depicted in Figures 2 and 3 reveal that food pur- even though it is one of her immigrant husband’s “must-
chases are wife-dominant decisions in American husband/ have” purchase items in their home. Lanna (American wife)
immigrant wife households but remain a joint decision in explains,
immigrant husband/American wife households. Our inter-
Like, he will make seafood soup every great once in a
views revealed that either the immigrant spouse shops or while and put [in] octopus and all these weird little things
both spouses shop together. In seven of the ten American that I won’t eat, but [my daughter] will just pick out the
husband/immigrant wife households, the wife does the octopus and eat it. And [my son] will have nothing to do
shopping, both to meet her culinary needs and those of her with it, like me.

FIGURE 3
The Decision Chain

Formative Stage of Household

Decisions Regarding:

•! Whom to marry

•! Where to wed

•! Where to reside
Level of
Cultural Competence

Basis for Role Allocation


Source of Expertise
Form of Cultural Capital

Native spouse Relative Influence Immigrant spouse

Later Stage of Household


Cultural Compensatory
Decisions Regarding:
Mechanisms

Acquiring Food Items •! Where to vacation


Language Exposure
Facilitating Trips •! Which schools to choose

•! What to eat

128 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


Thus, immigrant husbands purchase and prepare food to more on her decision making: “She would need to be my
meet their own culinary needs. seeing-eye dog in certain ways.” Simon (American hus-
The results of the decision-making survey, together with band) is the dominant decision maker in his home. Yet even
the interview data related to those decisions, confirm that he acknowledges that his role in the home is tied to his
decision-making roles and role allocations in binational knowledge of the United States: “In a lot of respects, some-
families differ from the monocultural families studied by times, I am the guy for us here because I know how things
Davis and Rigaux (1974) and Putnam and Davidson (1987). work.”
In addition, American husband/immigrant wife households Several of the countries from which our sample’s spouses
differ in important ways from American wife/immigrant (male and female) immigrated—including Mexico, the
husband households. Using the in-depth interviews, we next Philippines, Taiwan, Chile, Iran, and Vietnam—are consid-
identify the important themes revealed about binational ered highly patriarchal societies. However, we find the gen-
families. der allocations reported in the early family decision-making
literature to occur in our American husband/immigrant wife
Cultural Competence families. In contrast, we observe the reverse when the male
spouses are the immigrants. The finding that differences
Cultural Competence, Expert Power, and Cultural exist when husbands are immigrants versus wives may be
Capital expected given the hierarchical nature of most of the cul-
tures represented. Immigrant wives defer to higher-status
Interviews reveal the bases for the allocation of decision- husbands. However, American wives expect egalitarian
making roles identified in the survey. In their discussions of treatment, and immigrant husbands accept this given the
decision-making roles, participants suggest that a mixture family’s residence in the United States. In addition, most
of factors are important: gender, knowledge, convenience, participants acknowledge that the current roles are context
experience, interest, consensus, and expertise. This finding based and would probably change if the families were to
overlaps with the bases of conjugal power established in the reside in the immigrant spouse’s home country. Thus, con-
literature. Expert power, in particular, is described as “the text matters. The data suggest that the interaction between
belief that one family member has superior knowledge or the families’ culture of origin and culture of residence influ-
ability which will result in the best possible outcome” (Belch ences role allocation—an impact that may override other
and Willis 2002, p. 114). Attribution of expert power has previously examined bases for spousal role allocations
also been shown to increase with education, social class, and (which may be cultural constructions themselves). However,
more joint decision making. Yet the cultural dynamic per- the question remains: What drives the allocations of these
vading these binational households suggests another source roles in American husband/immigrant wife households ver-
of expert power: cultural competence. Participants identified sus immigrant husband/American wife households?
situational factors, such as location or country of residence,
Wallendorf and Reilly (1983a) note that a conceptual
as a basis for allocating decision-making roles. Some par-
definition of culture, as a set of socially acquired behavior
ticipants believed that expertise, experience, or personality
patterns, includes language, traditions, customs, dress, food,
would override situational factors, leaving decision-making
religious practices, shared meanings, and institutions. This
roles unaffected by country of residence. However, most
combination of behavior patterns and value systems stems
couples interviewed believe that the country of residence is
from the interplay of geography, history, technology, politi-
a crucial factor. They suggest that their current roles are
cal economy, and social institutions of the nation or society.
based at least in part on their choice to reside in the United
Sahlins (1999, p. 415) posits that culture signs mark bound-
States and the native spouse’s greater familiarity with the aries, but their “referent is an unseen, good and potent cul-
system and culture of the United States. tural presence.” These signs have an impact on people’s
Sharon (American wife), who has an accounting back- consumption decisions and behaviors within the given
ground, maintains that both her background and the culture social context.
of residence of her family play a role in allocating decision-
Several immigrant participants indicated that their spouse
making roles:
has been a guide, offering a perspective that has enabled
A lot of times if it’s me it’s because of my background, them to understand U.S. societal norms. For example, Cassie
what I do for a living, or because I grew up here. I have (Filipino immigrant wife) discusses her attraction to her
some special knowledge that he doesn’t have. American husband, Ed, who is of Filipino ancestry. With an
Charlie (American husband), married to Simone (Chilean accounting and finance background, Cassie believes that
wife), states, location is not a factor in the allocation of decision-making
roles.
I think a lot of the decision making came down to the fact
that this is my home country and when we need to get cer- I think it’s because the [roles are] based on my expertise
tain things like small appliances or things like that, I have and experience—you cannot really change that. So it
a better understanding of the products here. doesn’t matter what location we’re in, it will still remain
the same.
Simone succinctly concurs: “He knows more than me, and
better.” John (American husband) compares himself to a Yet even Cassie acknowledges Ed’s “Americanness” as part
blind man and notes that if he and his family ever moved to of his attraction for her and talks about the role Ed has
the Philippines, where his wife was born, he would rely played in her adjustment to the United States:

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 129


Yeah, we’re very compatible, my husband and I. Some- cultural competence is inseparable from insensible acquisi-
times he jokes about my accent and like I get attracted to tion of a ‘sense’ for sound cultural investment.”
him in the fact that he’s, like I said, he’s more American In the binational family, the native spouse possesses this
than Filipino.... I think I needed a different set of values
competence, manifested in his or her innate familiarity with
that will help me adjust.... I mean, I know English lan-
guage, but there are things, like those idioms, that I still the traditions, social norms, and institutions of the country
don’t get, that, you know, at least he’s my husband, I can in which the family resides. It is clear that for the spouses
ask him any time I want. So I’ve learned also ... American whose fortune it is to reside in the country in which they
history. I’ve learned from him. were born and raised, they possess an advantage: cultural
competence. However, this form of cultural competence is
Cassie believes that if they had both been immigrants, they not rooted in high culture activities in the narrow Bour-
would have been “a little bit more clueless.” She views her dieuian interpretations (Lareau and Weininger 2003) but
marriage as an aid to understanding U.S. traditions such as instead based on the intersection of culture of origin and
Thanksgiving. culture of residence. The culture of residence is the domi-
Cultural competence has perhaps been overlooked as a nant culture, and the native spouse functions as a cultural
source of expert power because it may only manifest itself intermediary—again, not necessarily of social class (unless
clearly in situations in which a person is outside his or her one considers that cultural competence is inflected by bina-
customary cultural context. Gilly (1995, p. 509) alludes to tional versions of social class) but of cultural milieu, a way
this base of knowledge and competence in her study of for the family to more easily navigate the practices of the
American expatriates in Spain, where the familiarity with particular societal context. In this role, the native spouse
“how things work” was the aspect of American life that was possesses a disproportionate influence, with a resultant
most missed. American expatriates in Spain relied on dependence on the part of the immigrant spouse.
longer-term expatriates who had “conquered the unknown” Thus, we propose that cultural competence is both a
for product and shopping advice. source of expert power and a particular form of cultural
The interview data help explain the feasibility triangles capital based on a pragmatic understanding of how things
in Figures 2 and 3. The native spouse’s cultural competence work and society functions in a particular culture. To extend
is drawn on when immigrant husbands and American wives Holt (1997), we refer to this as “household-specific cultural
engage in greater joint decision making for issues that are capital”—a relative competence manifested in the bina-
husband dominant in immigrant wife/American husband tional household. Cultural competence as a form of cultural
households (i.e., investments, insurance, capital expendi- capital allows for greater influence in the home as a source
tures, and remodeling). Our data show that decision-making of expert power and a basis for role allocation and is funda-
power in binational households can stem from several mental to a person’s ability to navigate a given culture. It
sources. Prior studies have outlined various bases of conju- augments Bourdieu’s (1984) social capital created through
gal power, including power based on expertise. Yet there lineage, wealth, education, income, greater understanding
has been little exploration into the subdimensions of these of social norms and cultural practices, and the resultant for-
power bases, particularly in the context of nontraditional mation of influential social networks.
family structures. We propose that a closer examination of
Acquiring Cultural Competence
expertise as a source of conjugal power indicates that there
are four sources of expert power: expertise based on (1) Over time, immigrants learn about the norms of the culture
vocational knowledge or training (e.g., formal schooling), of residence and acquire some cultural competence through
(2) experience (e.g., skill, proficiency), (3) related roles and their own observation. Colin (immigrant husband) reflects,
responsibilities (e.g., task allocations influenced by gender, Well, when I first came over here, I didn’t realize how
country of origin of the spouses, country of residence), and much they [Americans] consume food. When I first came
(4) cultural competence (i.e., knowledge of the traditions, over, we’d go out for breakfast, and lunch, and dinner. I
social norms, markets, language, and expectations within a got to a point where I would have breakfast and that would
given society, nation, or culture). We also contend that just do me in until dinner. I grew up with the attitude that you
eat everything that’s on your plate. But I got to a point
as the literature has overlooked the subdimensions of exper- where I couldn’t eat three meals a day. Sitting down and
tise as a base of power, it has also failed to account for all people saying, “How do you want your steak?” and, “Do
aspects of the capital resources partners bring to a relation- you want a salad?” When I grew up, it was like everything
ship. We therefore argue that cultural competence is also a just came out.... It wasn’t a main meal that comes with all
form of cultural capital. these sides. Very different. A lot of decision making when
it comes to food. That’s totally different.
Holt (1997, p. 97) extends Bourdieu’s construct of cul-
tural capital and refers to “field-specific cultural capital,” a However, immigrants married to native spouses have an
currency that is socially consequential within particular advantage over other immigrants; they have a culturally
fields of consumption. Holt argues that cultural capital is competent guide to explain what cannot be easily observed.
not just an abstract notion but also a concept that manifests Colin notes,
itself in various forms in social life. Bourdieu (1984, p. 85)
She [American wife] manages the money and she’s more
also speaks of the family as a market, a site where use of in tune as to how the system works. She even got me my
particular competences substantiate the importance of the first credit card and told me how to use it. She’s tried once
competences themselves. He states that “the acquisition of or twice to get me to try and take responsibility for it,

130 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


she’s always taking control of it and I’m pretty comfort- And then she taught me because I’ve never had a Thanks-
able with it. giving feast.”
Immigrant spouses must learn more than the mysteries of All of the participant families interviewed had minor
the marketplace, however. Julio (immigrant husband from children, with one couple expecting their first child. Partici-
Mexico) shares his experience in navigating gender role pants discussed the very positive effect they believe, and
expectations: hope, the dual cultures in the home will have on their chil-
dren’s development. Leila (immigrant wife) tries to expose
I know, from my personal experience at least, some [rela- her children to both Taiwanese and Irish American culture
tionships] like Sally and myself [are] not easy, because the through food and customs by celebrating both Chinese New
American ladies have more independence in the way that Year and St. Patrick’s Day: “I try to get the kids to under-
they think and they are also [financially] independent.
They don’t need somebody there to pay the bills; they
stand that they have different cultures and where they come
don’t. So to start, the first two years [are] difficult, it’s not from—Irish and Chinese.” Kenny (Leila’s American hus-
easy, because you need to learn and you need to give, band) hopes that his children
because if only one side gives and the other not, it are familiar with just hearing different languages, having
[doesn’t] work.... Sometimes for one, because you don’t different cultures, seeing people that look a little different,
know the system, you can’t say a word or make some act a little different. I mean they are a little different. So I
action.... So, I learn. think it’s positive. I mean, it’s got to be.
Our research indicates that it is not only the immigrant John (American husband) argues the importance of children
spouse who benefits from intermarriage, as Meng and Gre- seeing differences work:
gory (2005) suggest in their study of economic assimilation.
Rather, native spouses gain, both from bringing their own I think that it has been positive because I think they see. I
do not believe children learn very much by what they are
cultural characteristics from the background to the fore- told. I think they learn most of what they learn by observ-
ground and from learning intercultural competencies. Hall ing other people. So what they are seeing is two very dif-
(1959) suggests that exposure to another culture is impor- ferent people, and they will choose—they can choose.
tant not only because it helps us learn about another way of
doing things but because of what it reveals about ourselves. Hannah (John’s Filipina wife) views this ability to choose
as “having the best of both worlds” for her children, both in
Simon (American husband) thinks about his own cul-
their upbringing and in their exposure to both cultures. Yet
ture and the things that are an “education every day”
as John relates, having both worlds is simultaneously chal-
because his immigrant wife from Belarus asks questions
lenging and rewarding for the partners as well.
about it:
We even had an argument one time where she was upset
There are things that you, growing up in the United States, at me—she said we just don’t click and we are not the
could eventually just come to take for granted. When you same. I said, “I am glad that we are not the same, I am
are with someone whose eyes are opened by all the things glad that we have different perspectives”—that gives us
that are here, you come to appreciate those a little more so much more power as a couple if we can work together
than you might elsewise. on something; we can see things from totally different
perspectives. I told her my first wife and I could have
The native spouse, in learning about his or her spouse’s cul-
been brother and sister, both blond hair, both green eyes....
ture, also gains an appreciation for cultural differences in I mean, we were practically twins in almost every way—
general, is able to view his or her own culture analytically, philosophies, everything. It was great in a lot of ways and
and acquires an increase in intercultural competence, which we always got along but ... a lot about it was boring,
can have personal and professional implications. Kipnis, whereas with [Hannah] it is never boring. Because there is
Broderick, and Demangeot’s (2013, p. 13) “consumer multi- a tension as a result of different contexts not jiving, differ-
ent expectations not jiving, but if we can deal with that—
culturation” refers to changes in the cultural identification which is not always easy to deal with—but if we can deal
and consumption behavior of mainstream consumers who with that, it is a whole lot more interesting and [creates] a
come into continuous contact with multiple cultures. The whole lot richer experiences in the relationship.
authors argue that continuous contact with other cultures in
Aspirations of the binational parents for their children
the marketplace or, as we argue, in the home, generates
are corroborated in interviews conducted with adult chil-
multicultural awareness and facilitates the development of
dren of other binational families. These interviewees reveal
affiliative cultural identities.
that while they had both positive and negative experiences
In addition, we find that the influence of the dual cul-
growing up in binational homes, the dual cultures in the
tures occurs both at the individual and the wider group lev-
home had long-lasting benefits that all binational children
els, as Peñaloza (1989) asserts in her discussion of consumer share. As one participant with Ghanaian and American par-
learning within a multicultural context. Thus, the notion of ents notes,
cultural competence extends beyond the dyadic relationship
of the spouses to other family members. Immigrant spouses In terms of everyone else, I’d say that my food experience
learn not only from their partners but also from their in- is far more diverse than theirs, because I’ve been growing
up with Ghanaian food. As a reality, it made me so much
laws and even their children. Hannah (immigrant wife) dis- more open to other things, because my parents would be
cusses preparing her first Thanksgiving meal: “So I would like, “Oh, be open, and try whatever,” but I think this
call my mother-in-law. I’d say, Mom, help me out here.... openness hasn’t only affected me in terms of foods, but it

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 131


affected me in terms of life, like, hey, I can’t say I don’t Cultural Compensatory
like it until I’ve tried it.
Mechanisms
Another participant, whose parents are from Germany and The family decision-making literature stream has focused
the United States, believes that binational children do grasp on decisions and decision roles in established households.
the pros and the cons of being binational: Yet it is clear that decision making in these households does
[There is a] tolerance of difference and of people being not simply begin after the household is formed. Our data
different. The ability to step into someone else’s shoes show that decisions made in the formative stages of the
like if it was language issues or just trying to get along.... household are just as important and indeed influence future
It’s like we’re all just really different from one another choices and roles. For example, in these families, decisions
and that’s okay.... I think it’s also the negative side of about where to get married and where to reside can be key
when cultures hit each other, I think we understand that points of discussion and even contention, as couples take
and so someone who grows [up] in a one-culture family into account dual country affiliations, familial relationships,
never sees the fireworks that set off when you mix reli- and mobility. In discussing the reasons behind her family’s
gion and race and culture and language.... And the fact move to California, Lanna (American wife) concedes that it
that culture doesn’t always mix.... So I think it’s this just was a decision based on her desire to be near her family—
acknowledgment, this culture and it’s so tough. It’s so an option not available to her Mexican-born husband, who
hard inside people that sometimes it doesn’t work, these has no family in the United States:
combinations that we make. But I do think it’s an appreci-
ation for it and when it does work ... it’s like wow, isn’t That is my fault again.... So the only family we have in
that cool? the United States was in California and his whole family
lives in Mexico where he is from, so I wanted to be close
A participant of Japanese and Jewish American heritage to one or the other.... He said yes, but he really wasn’t
corroborates this ability to tolerate, appreciate, and yet crazy about moving.
question the status quo, stating, Residential decisions not only are based on factors such as
Everyone grew up believing that certain types of rules and neighborhood demographics or proximity to work but can
regulations were inherently natural and correct, and I did also include extensive deliberations on the country in which
not.... Everyone must eat certain foods; ... it is like I to start a home. Sharon (American wife) discusses her deci-
totally did not have that. sion to live in the United States with her Australian husband:
It is clear that dual cultures in the home have an impact We just talked about it. Went through the scenarios, and
not only on the partner spouses but also on their offspring. my parents are here, my sister is deceased, so it was
Binational children growing up with both cultures will have important for me to stay close to my parents. I had lived in
Australia, obviously, when I met him and the part that I
negative experiences and sometimes feel like “fish out of lived in I had friends, but it just didn’t feel like home. He
water”; however, they will also assimilate many of the qual- had to be comfortable with living here, but the agreement
ities—such as openness, tolerance for difference, and the was that if he was willing to move to the States, then he
ability to critically question the status quo—that can aid got to pick where we lived in the States.
success in an increasingly diverse and global marketplace. These spouses all made the United States their home for
In discussing the acquisition of cultural competence, reasons including greater opportunities, higher standards of
Bourdieu (1984, p. 28) notes that “such competence is not living, and even safety considerations. Yet the decision to
necessarily acquired by means of the ‘scholastic’ labours in live in the United States means a distancing from the immi-
which some ... indulge.... Most often it results from the grant spouse’s family members, childhood associations, and
unintentional learning made possible by a disposition cultural history. Consequences of these choices are fre-
acquired through domestic or scholastic inculcation of quently reflected in subsequent, seemingly unrelated every-
legitimate culture.” He describes the family as one of the day decisions. These decisions are often manifested through
social spaces or “sites where competence is produced.” The cultural compensatory mechanisms—defined as acts that
other is school (Bourdieu 1984, p. 85). Again, although benefit the immigrant spouse to recompense for moving
Bourdieu is interpreted as referring to competence rooted in away from his or her culture—in these binational homes.
social class, the parallels are clear. This is the unintentional Previous consumer literature has discussed compen-
satory mechanisms as alternative strategies or processes for
learning that our participant with German and American
reliance when an internal cue (Solomon 1983) or an experi-
parentage refers to when asserting that family meal times in
ential opportunity (Peck and Childers 2003) is lacking or
multicultural homes are “a chance for cultural learning, not unavailable. For binational families, only one of the societal
book learning, where we share beliefs and opinions and anchors of the household is available and accessible on a
learn how to negotiate these different views of the world, daily basis: the culture of the native spouse. Outside the
around that table, around the food.” Binational spouses and home, one culture dominates, which has implications for
their offspring acquire cultural and intercultural compe- the roles of both the immigrant spouse and that of his or her
tence through the unintentional and intentional learning, native partner within the home.
both positive and negative, that takes place—and, it may be For the immigrant spouse, the decision to reside in
argued, can only take place—in a culturally diverse home. another country entails sacrifice, a term both immigrant and

132 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


native participants use in this study. Julio (immigrant hus- For these families, the compromise takes place in the deci-
band) found it difficult to give up his successful career as a sions about school and language, as they seek ways to ensure
military officer in Mexico to move to the United States, a that they and the children are conversant in both languages.
decision he thought would be better for his U.S.-born wife, That her family lives in a country that is not home to her
Sally. Australian husband has an underlying impact on the prod-
Well, in my specific case, when I explained to my boss in
ucts Sharon (American wife) considers buying and where.
Mexico that I am going to move, he was very unhappy, Previously, she had noted that their compromise was his
and he [offered] me a house and a car and everything if I choosing where to live within the United States. However,
don’t move. But, I make some evaluation because at that she continues to compensate for their choice of residence
time Sally [didn’t] speak the language, Spanish, and she by considering his food preferences as she shops: “I have to
didn’t know the area.... So it was not fair to put my wife in be sensitive to the fact that this isn’t his home and he didn’t
Mexico. grow up here, so I’ll try to incorporate things here that he
There is often a lingering struggle to come to terms with the likes.” For binational participants, food compromises also
initial decision that was made. This effort to reconcile the encompass combining and alternating dishes in the same
two worlds is continuous and, in some ways, can become meal or different meals from week to week as well as
more difficult as the years pass. Alonso (immigrant hus- adjusting or combining ingredients for a particular dish
band), describes this struggle: (Cross and Gilly 2013b). Returning to the survey data in
Figures 2 and 3, immigrant husbands’ greater involvement
What happens is I was 21 when I came. It seems to me in food purchases reflects the need to accommodate their
like half of my life has been here. So even though I have preferences in the pantries of immigrant husband/American
the feeling of always going back, it’s not exactly the same
wife households.
because I have like half and half ... half of the culture that
I learned here and what I learned there, which is much The choice of vacation destination is a major area of
stronger because that was childhood. But it’s still ... very compromise for several families, with regular trips planned
hard.... I try to fit. I try to behave like everybody does to the immigrant spouse’s home country. Indeed, trips are
here, but I always know that I’m not the same.... I feel like not necessarily always vacations in the sense of a relaxing
I am different. I mean different like ... from another coun- getaway but rather a “going home” for the immigrant
try. I don’t feel from here yet. I don’t feel like I am from spouse. For example, Lanna (American wife) and Jorge
here. I always feel like I am a foreigner. (immigrant husband) travel to Puebla, Mexico, to visit
Even as immigrants gain their own cultural competence, Jorge’s family:
they never experience the comfort level of their native We go to Puebla every other year, which is down south
spouse. Moreover, over time, immigrants feel less cultural and that’s where his family is from.... I have to say, Puebla
competence in their own country as it changes in their is not my favorite place in Mexico because it is a big city
absence. This struggle is fully acknowledged and salient in ... lots of pollution, all that stuff. So, it’s not my favorite
the everyday lives and decisions of the family. Partners are place, but that’s OK, that’s where they live.... When I go
there, I have to work overtime because I don’t get to
very aware of the compromises made in decisions regarding speak English with anybody. It’s kind of exhausting for
neighborhood, food, vacations, schools, and languages that me.
shift the spousal influence from one decision context to
another and make the relative advantage of the spouses less With flexible jobs, Daniel and Liang travel annually to visit
clear-cut. There is often a conscious effort to expose the family and friends. Daniel says,
family to the “other” culture—the missing societal anchor Once a year, during the summer time, we return to either
stemming from the early choices made in these binational Taiwan or mainland China and we live there for the sum-
homes. The native spouse is cognizant of the immigrant mer time.... My daughter went to a local school there,
spouse’s needs, preferences, and yearnings and often con- which she really loves, and then my wife could go shop-
sciously aims to fill this gap through cultural compensatory ping, we could visit friends and things like that, and then
we go traveling around and have fun.
mechanisms. He or she finds and tolerates food items that
the immigrant spouse favors, ensures that the family is Thus, we ask: In the binational family, is the role of cul-
exposed to both cultures and languages, and facilitates fam- tural expert advantageous, or does the immigrant spouse
ily trips or vacations to the immigrant spouse’s home coun- also possess a more subtle source of influence within the
try, even if it is not his or her preference. decision-making process? We find that this other source of
Lanna (American wife) and Jorge (immigrant husband) influence is based not on cultural competence but on the very
speak only Spanish in the home, and their children attend a lack thereof—an advantage in certain types of decisions
Spanish immersion school. Liang (immigrant wife) and her that stems from those early struggles and choices. However,
American husband, Daniel, only speak Mandarin Chinese we also acknowledge the boundaries of this relative influ-
in their home with their children ence and the limits of the compensatory mechanisms
engaged in by the native spouse. The native spouse com-
because they’ll get that elsewhere, and the rule is that at
home, you speak Chinese. In fact, we even homeschool
pensates, but only to a certain degree.
our children ... because we can give them a bilingual edu- As we have observed from previous examples, cherished
cation and there is not the social pressure, like at schools items from the immigrant spouses’ cuisine are seen as a
to kind of conform. “must have” in the home—encouraged, tolerated, and even

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 133


purchased by the native spouse. Yet in several participant with a single utility curve is limited in the context of bina-
homes, it is the immigrant spouse who must prepare and tional or bicultural households, just as it likely is in other
even consume his or her own dishes. The native spouse will household types (e.g., same-sex households, multigenera-
“have nothing to do with it” (Mexican seafood dish), “does tional households). Our theoretical development illustrates
not cook Filipino,” or “will not touch it” (Iranian yogurt the cultural complexity underlying power, decision making
drinks). In these homes, the immigrant spouse can have the in general, and family decision making specifically. This
desired food item if he or she procures it, prepares it, and line of inquiry suggests that our current portraits of fami-
does not necessarily expect other family members to join in lies, power, and decisions are due for an overhaul.
consuming it. Thus, one of this study’s main contributions is that it
We find that even with vacations, trips to the immigrant adds to the knowledge base in the family decision-making
spouse’s family may be confined to once every other year, literature stream through an examination of the types of
with the extended family coming to visit instead or both capital and consumption decisions in binational households.
parties meeting midway. As we noted previously, Lanna and Expertise as an established base of conjugal power (Raven,
Jorge visit Jorge’s family in Puebla every other year, a trip Centers, and Rodrigues 1975) remains valid in this context.
that takes its toll on Lanna. They alternate those visits with However, we find that expertise also has different dimen-
meeting Jorge’s parents at a resort town, Rosarito, which is sions: expertise due to vocation, expertise arising from
closer to home, just over the border in Mexico. experience, expertise based on related roles and responsibil-
Power or influence in the family is relative, complex, ities, and expertise resulting from cultural competence. For
and peculiar to the specific decision type, process, or stage binational families, a particularly important dimension is
in the decision process. Relative spousal influence shifts expertise based on cultural competence and savvy. The
depending on the stage of the household and the earlier partner with greater expertise or knowledge of the residen-
influence of each spouse. As Figure 3 illustrates, in the for- tial consumptionscape plays the role of cultural viaduct,
mative stage of the binational household, the partners col- arbitrator, and translator.
lectively make decisions such as where to marry and where This research also posits that cultural competence can
to reside. Later, the native spouse may have more influence be viewed as a particular form of cultural capital in bina-
as a result of greater cultural competence compared with tional and bicultural relationships, which differs from and
the immigrant partner. At this stage, cultural competence is augments Bourdieu’s (1984) analysis of the influence and
manifested as a basis for role allocation, a source of expert impact of cultural capital. By introducing the notion that
power and a form of cultural capital. However, over time, cultural competence (based on knowledge of the traditions,
cultural compensatory mechanisms emerge. These cultural norms, markets, and institutions of the country of resi-
compensatory mechanisms emphasize the consumption dence) is another dimension of cultural capital, we offer a
preferences of the immigrant spouse regarding items and new way to use Bourdieu’s theory in the context of cultural
decisions that enhance the relevance of the immigrant interaction. Recognition that expertise can come from a
spouse’s culture. These items and decisions include cuisine, variety of sources (with cultural competence the focus here)
language, children’s schools, and vacation destinations. We enriches the family decision-making literature stream. Its
refer to Figure 3 as a decision chain because each stage in narrow focus on gender roles and identifying who domi-
the binational family’s life is linked to the next; decisions nates a particular purchase decision has limited our under-
made in a prior stage influence decisions made in subse- standing of how decisions are made within households.
quent stages because of the cultural compensatory mecha- Extending Epp and Price (2011), we demonstrate that the
nisms adopted. efforts to accommodate cultural differences within the fam-
Thus, relative advantage in one area means relinquishing
ily reflect the goal of “being a family.” Unlike the children
control in another. These compromises in decision making
of immigrants (Park 2005), who consciously use consump-
should be expected because as with any partnership, negoti-
tion to demonstrate American identity, binational families
ation, or decision process, concession in one area usually
embrace the fact that they are not a “typical American fam-
means gaining an advantage in another. Reciprocity expec-
ily.” The cultural competence everyone in the family gains
tations also differ across cultures (Shen, Wan, and Wyer
distinguishes these households.
2010), which adds another element to decisions made
By taking into account the impact of decisions in the
within bicultural families. For culturally heterogeneous
formative stages of the home, we show that decisions are
households, the magnitude of decisions in the formative
stages of the home plays a significant part in the develop- interconnected and have direct effects on the partners’
ment of cultural compensatory mechanisms for both spheres of influence for any particular decision. Our find-
spouses and the determination of relative spheres of influ- ings indicate that the native spouse, who possesses a rela-
ence in the ongoing family decision-making process. tive advantage from cultural know-how, compensates by
relinquishing control in other decisions. By virtue of being
the immigrant, the less culturally competent spouse may
Implications and Conclusion therefore gain greater influence over decisions related to
vacations, children’s education, and food. We find that there
Implications for Family Decision Making are limits to cultural compensatory mechanisms, because
Our research shows that Becker’s (1981) rational choice spouses’ abilities and resources do not always allow for tit-
approach to analyzing households as decision-making units for-tat compromises. Our research addresses Wilk’s (1989,

134 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


p. 30) concern that decision-making models portray house- are “especially hard to see,” becoming visible when new-
hold decision-making processes as linear, with a beginning comers, such as immigrants, “retrieve them from their
and an end, “rather than as parts of continuing social rela- familiarity.”
tionships.” Compromise as a compensatory mechanism is Figure 4 maps the relationships of the key concepts our
manifested throughout the interconnected phases and research reveals. The native spouse possesses cultural com-
spheres of influence. This study thus extends the work of petence that confers both expert power and cultural capital.
Epp and Price (2011), who demonstrate that family decision The imbalance of cultural competence between the native
making involves finding solutions that will meet individual and immigrant spouse leads to cultural compensatory
goals as well as the needs of the family. We find that con- mechanisms that benefit the immigrant spouse in maintain-
cession and compromise occur over time as the family ing ties with his or her culture. Over time, the immigrant
forms consensus around decisions that require trade-offs in spouse gains cultural competence through the cultural
future decisions. Vacations may be a joint decision, but the understanding of the native spouse.
choice of where to live has implications for those choices in
the future. Although this phenomenon is brought to the Implications for Marketers and Organizations
foreground in binational homes, it is expected that this com- With growing immigrant populations and increases in cul-
pensatory mechanism operates in all partnerships. turally diverse families around the world, marketers should
If we extend our view beyond conjugal power, we consider this market segment as offering opportunities. Our
observe that the dynamics of cultural heterogeneity in bina- data show that binational families want to accommodate
tional marriages can have wider societal and economic both cultures. The importance of keeping both cultures
impacts. In their study of intermarriage in Australia, Meng salient makes it imperative for binational families to seek
and Gregory (2005) argue that it is important to examine out items from the immigrant partner’s culture. An example
the effect of family structure on assimilation. Their findings of a retailer that meets the needs of immigrants and their
reveal that there is an “intermarriage premium” for immi- native spouses is Wholesome Choice, located in Southern
grant men and women who marry native Australians—a California. According to its website, it intends to be “an
premium not shown to exist for the native partner in the International specialty market which offers a spectacular
marriage or for immigrants who marry other immigrants. array of International specialty and ethnic foods from
Our research supplements and extends Meng and Gregory’s around the world, as well as traditional and conventional
findings to show the advantage of intermarriage for native groceries.” Another example is the recently opened C-Fresh
and immigrant spouses as well as their offspring. Market in central Iowa, described in the news media as a
The immigrant spouse clearly benefits from the union market with “global appeal, [catering] to different national-
because he or she more quickly acquires cultural compe- ities,” with a “wide and unusual selection” of international
tency and know-how because of access to the native part- foods. “Everything from hot dogs and Rice Krispies to
ner. However, as our data show, the native spouse benefits spiny eel fish and black fungus.” According to a customer,
as well. He or she has a relative advantage in certain aspects “It’s like something new for everybody; different cultures
of decision making, possessing an innate source of cultural mixing into one” (KCCI News 2013). Whereas California is
expertise that can lead to a disproportionate influence; one of the two most culturally diverse states, note that Iowa
learns more about his or her culture as cultural differences
are brought to the foreground; and gains increased cultural FIGURE 4
competence through exposure to another culture, possibly a A Model of Cultural Competence and Cultural
different language, unusual foods, other countries, and Compensatory Mechanisms in Binational Homes
diverse perspectives. Brannen and Thomas (2010, p. 3)
define biculturals as “people who have internalized more
than one cultural profile.” They empirically find that bicul-
turals are more skilled than monoculturals (those exposed
Native

only to their own culture) in intercultural effectiveness. Fur-


Spouse

thermore, they speculate that biculturals may be particularly


Possesses SOURCE OF

well suited to be boundary spanners in multicultural organi-


EXPERT
POWER

zational teams. Cultural competency may lead to what man-


CULTURAL

agement scholars call “cultural intelligence”—that is, the


COMPETENCE
Gains

ability to function and manage effectively in culturally


FORM OF

diverse settings (Ang and Van Dyne 2008). Although


CULTURAL
CAPITAL

spouses in binational marriages may not internalize the oth-


Through Leads to

ers’ culture in the same way children do, they gain


increased multicultural awareness and can develop an affil-
iative cultural identity with the immigrant spouse’s culture
(Kipnis, Broderick, and Demangeot 2013). McCracken
(2009, p. 47) states that culture has both fast and slow com-
CULTURAL
COMPENSATORY

ponents. He notes that attaining “slow” culture depends on


Immigrant
MECHANISMS
Spouse
cultural knowledge and that the components of slow culture
Benefit

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 135


is the sixth least-diverse state (Emerson 2011), suggesting sphere, playing the intermediary role of bridge, broker, and
that demand for culturally diverse products is widespread. boundary spanner (Thomas, Brannen, and Garcia 2010). A
Hermans and Kempen (1998, pp. 1117–18) insist that reviewer noted that one of his or her Chinese American stu-
researchers need to place more emphasis on the “contact dents had a summer internship at Paramount Studios
zones between cultures; the interactional meeting places” involving market entry negotiations in China. As the only
where exchanges of cultural information occur. We propose team member who could speak the language, this student
that in multicultural communities, retailers and marketers immediately played a more substantial role than a less cul-
need to capitalize on opportunities to meet changing market- turally competent intern would. In his argument for a chief
place expectations, envisioning interactional contact zones culture officer, McCracken (2009, p. 128) reiterates the
with broad creative product offerings and service experi- need for people who can traverse different worlds, with
ences that meet the needs of natives and nonnatives alike. some knowledge of “worlds alien to their own ... worlds
If we view the binational family as a microsetting for that proceed according to other assumptions.” He argues
bridging cultural divides (Lauth Bacas 2002; Cross and that organizations should seek out people who are able to
Gilly 2013a), our results also offer insights into targeting accept disruption, extend beyond their own preferences and
binational families as well as families in general. For exam- comfort zone, and see issues from different perspectives.
ple, salespeople select a strategy for selling to a particular Because of their skills related to intercultural effectiveness,
client (Dixon, Spiro, and Jamil 2001). If the salesperson which are necessary in today’s global enterprise, members
makes assumptions about gender roles without considering of binational households should be attractive potential
the background of each spouse, the sales strategy may be employees (Thomas, Brannen, and Garcia 2010).
ineffective. The salesperson should consider all aspects of Diversity is said to beget creativity in organizational
expert power—experience, vocational training, related settings (Roberge and Van Dick 2010). Our research reveals
roles, and the spouses’ cultural competency—in deciding on manifestations of cultural compensatory mechanisms in
a sales strategy. binational families’ attempts to incorporate practices from
Media depictions of families also need to reflect more both cultures. Culturally diverse households act as sociali-
diversity. The gay couple depicted on the television show zation units in which intentional and unintentional learning
Modern Family is perceived to represent progress in depict- takes place. The family’s cultural diversity brings differing
ing nontraditional households. However, less attention has cultural characteristics to the fore and inevitably changes
been paid to the show’s binational couple, Jay (American) the culture of the household. Similarly, members of cultur-
and Gloria (Colombian), perhaps because storylines empha- ally diverse organizations engage in intentional and unin-
size their May–December marriage over their binational tentional learning, resulting in greater creativity. Organiza-
status. Advertisers who rely on stereotypes of gender roles tions can encourage a global mindset through both formal
will likely receive negative feedback. Ragú recently initiatives and informal learning. Cayla and Arnould (2013,
incurred a social media storm after stereotyping fathers as p. 13) contend that ethnographic stories “disrupt organiza-
poor cooks who make “breakfast for dinner” and need Ragú tional paradigms, [helping] organizations to develop new
brand chicken recipes to help them. In reality, many fathers strategic foundations.” We argue that culturally diverse
enjoy cooking. Sadly, some recent attempts to show diverse units and teams are also disruptive, and it is this disruptive
families in ads were also controversial. The Cheerios ad influence that provides inspiration and insights into creative
featuring a biracial child received so many racist slurs that combinations of products, services, and other cultural and
the comments section on YouTube was shut down. How- marketing touchstones.
ever, in posts on Facebook, many consumers expressed
their appreciation for the brand’s decision to depict a bira- Implications for Society
cial family. The microsetting of culturally diverse families provides the
Kumar and Steenkamp (2013, p. 130) suggest that cor- ideal initial venue for cross-fertilization of ideas, where flu-
porations from emerging markets can establish global idity of perspective, openness, and tolerance for difference
brands by targeting emigrants from their homelands. If the is encouraged. This leads to enhanced awareness of the
diaspora are sufficiently large in number, geographically members’ own and others’ cultures and fosters the develop-
spread but with concentrations in key regions, and have a ment of a certain sense of intercultural competence among
socioeconomic profile similar to the host population, they group members and, ideally, increased collaboration and
can “serve as stepping-stones for brands to go national.” innovation, with positive implications for the wider com-
Kumar and Steenkamp point to biculturals (i.e., immigrants munity. As binational families become more common, more
who incorporate both home and host cultures into their citizens will not simply coexist with other cultures in sepa-
identities) as particularly relevant targets. We argue that rate ethnic enclaves but rather encounter other cultures in
binational households are also likely to have the requisite their neighborhoods, offices, and even in their extended
ties to home and host cultures to act as conduits for foreign families. Cross-cultural and cross-border discussions and
brands to the host culture. negotiations between organizations in corporate, political,
As in binational family contexts, cultural competence is and economic realms will be more productive and collabo-
an asset in organizational settings. The member with the rative when the parties involved have had prolonged close
dominant expertise and knowledge in the chosen setting interactions with more than one culture, in either familial or
may serve as the facilitator for other team members in that business settings.

136 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


Peñaloza and Gilly (1999) point out that immigration focusing on the link between the particular nuances of the
leads to marketplace changes, both explicit and subtle. decision-making process and the way the spouses have
However, despite increased immigration to the United been socialized in their respective countries would be bene-
States, societal institutions are designed for, and cater to, ficial. Developing an understanding of the situations that
the dominant population rather than those who lack cultural sculpt intercultural competence and how this competence is
capital. This marginalization also applies to others, in addi- cultivated in children of binational families would also
tion to immigrants, who do not fit the mold of the typical extend this research.
consumer (e.g., the disabled, the illiterate, the “minority” Our participants were all comfortably middle class and
consumer). Is this right? Is this even smart? Societal institu- rather sophisticated regarding global travel and different
tions must inculcate and incorporate more than a surface cultures. Although Ustuner and Holt (2007) consider immi-
appreciation for diversity for the benefit of the marketplace, grant rather than binational households, their work on how
the society, and those who interact within them. acculturation operates for poor immigrants suggests that
Conclusion and Next Steps underclass binational households could have very different
experiences. Our work should be extended to include a
Given the small size of the survey sample, the generaliz- broader range of socioeconomic groups, across different
ability of the survey findings is necessarily limited. How- national settings, to provide a better understanding of the
ever, the results follow the trend observed in prior studies relative influence of country of origin, country of encounter
and are consistent within the binational family context.
(i.e., where the partners met), and country of residence on
Conducting similar types of studies of other multicultural or
consumption decisions, dyadic interactions, and power
other nonhomogeneous households with a larger sample
dynamics within diverse partnerships. Such research will
and mapping the data using Wolfe’s (1959) feasiblity trian-
properly complicate the lives of marketers and researchers
gle would augment the findings of this study. We find that
alike, but given the increasingly heterogeneous composition
families make trade-offs over time, with decisions favoring
of households (e.g., multicultural, same-sex, multigenera-
one spouse being compensated in other ways to even out
sacrifice. The compensatory mechanisms used by binational tional), digging deep to understand these dynamics is
families likely apply in parallel ways to other families. For unavoidable.
marketers to understand a family’s purchase at t = 1, they This study is one more step in addressing the “intramicro-
must appreciate the purchase at t = –1 and the anticipated cultural research” gap in consumer acculturation studies
purchase at t = 1+. suggested by Ogden, Ogden, and Schau (2004). In their
Although we find that immigrant spouses become more review of the ethnicity and acculturation literature streams,
culturally competent over time, we have insufficient data to they propose that there are intracultural differences that
extend Figure 3 for binational couples who have been mar- directly affect purchase decisions. Our research argues that the
ried for decades or are empty nesters, and for families in impact of these cultural differences on purchases and other
which the children’s exposure to the other culture is less family decision making are amplified within multicultural
salient. Further research should study binational empty family structures in diverse societies such as the United
nesters who have co-resided in one country many years to States. Studying decision making in culturally diverse
determine whether cultural competence remains a source of binational families reveals a different perspective on how
expertise in binational families. In addition, a deeper probe decisions are formulated and conflicts are resolved in cross-
into the nature of decision making in the binational home, cultural familial, organizational, and societal settings.

REFERENCES
Andrews, Bart (1985), The “I Love Lucy” Book. New York: Bourdieu, Pierre (1984), Distinction: A Social Critique of the
Doubleday. Judgment of Taste, Richard Nice, trans. Cambridge, MA: Har-
Ang, Soon and Linn Van Dyne (2008), “Conceptualization of Cul- vard University Press.
tural Intelligence: Definition, Distinctiveness, and Nomologi- ——— (1986), “The Forms of Capital,” in Handbook of Theory of
cal Network,” in Handbook of Cultural Intelligence, Soon Ang Research for the Sociology of Education, John E. Richardson,
and Linn Van Dyne, eds. Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe, 3–15. ed. New York: Greenwood Press, 241–58.
Bean, Frank D. and Gillian Stevens (2003), America’s Newcomers Brannen, Mary Yoko and David C. Thomas (2010), “Understand-
and the Dynamics of Diversity. New York: Russell Sage Foun- ing Bicultural Individuals in Organizations: Implications and
Opportunity,” International Journal of Cross-Cultural Man-
dation.
agement, 10 (1), 5–16.
Becker, Gary S. (1981), A Treatise on the Family. Cambridge,
Breger, Rosemary and Rosanna Hill (1998), “Introducing Mixed
MA: Harvard University Press. Marriages,” in Cross-Cultural Marriage, Identity and Choice,
Belch, Michael A. and Laura A. Willis (2002), “Family Decision Rosemary Breger and Rosanna Hill, eds. Oxford, UK: Berg, 1–
at the Turn of the Century: Has the Changing Structure of 32.
Households Impacted the Family Decision-Making Process?” Brooks, Tim and Earle F. Marsh (2003), The Complete Directory
Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 2 (2), 111–24. to Prime Time Network and Cable TV Shows: 1946–Present,
Blackledge, Adrian (2001), “The Wrong Sort of Capital,” Interna- 8th ed. New York: Ballantyne Books.
tional Journal of Bilingualism, 5 (3), 345–69. Bryman, Alan (2006), “Integrating Quantitative and Qualitative
Blood, Robert O. and Donald Wolfe (1960), Husbands and Wives. Research: How Is It Done?” Qualitative Research, 6 (1), 97–
New York: The Free Press. 113.

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 137


Cayla, Julien and Eric Arnould (2013), “Ethnographic Stories for Hall, Edward T. (1959), The Silent Language. New York: Random
Market Learning,” Journal of Marketing, 77 (July), 1–16. House.
Childers, Terry L. and O.C. Ferrell (1981), “Husband-Wife Deci- Hermans, Hubert J.M. and Harry J.G. Kempen (1998), “Moving
sion Making in Purchasing and Renewing Auto Insurance,” Cultures: The Perilous Problems of Cultural Dichotomies in a
Journal of Risk and Insurance, 48 (3), 482–93. Globalizing Society,” American Psychologist, 53 (10), 1111–
Commuri, Suraj and James W. Gentry (2000), “Opportunities for 20.
Family Research in Marketing,” Academy of Marketing Sci- Holt, Douglas B. (1997), “Distinction: Recovering Bourdieu’s
ence Review, 8, (accessed February 19, 2014), [available at Theory of Tastes from Its Critics,” Poetics, 25 (2/3), 93–120.
http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1009 KCCI News (2013), “New Grocery Store Has Global Appeal:
&context=marketingfacpub]. Market Caters to Different Nationalities,” (January 16),
——— and ——— (2005), “Resource Allocation in Households (accessed February 19, 2014), [available at http://www.kcci.
with Women as Chief Wage Earners,” Journal of Consumer com/news/central-iowa/New-grocery-store-has-global-appeal/
Research, 32 (2), 185–95. 18146190#ixzz2YfZ4AXS9].
Creswell, John W. and Vicki L. Plano Clark (2011), Designing and Khodadady, Ebrahim, Farnaz Farokh Alaee, and Mottahareh
Conducting Mixed Methods Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Natanzi (2011), “Factors Underlying the Social and Cultural
Sage Publications. Capitals of High School Students and Their Relationship with
Cross, Samantha N.N. and Mary C. Gilly (2013a), “Bridging Cul- English Achievement,” Theory and Practice in Language Stud-
tural Divides: The Role and Impact of Binational Families,” ies, 1 (11), 1618–27.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 32 (Special Issue), 106– Kingston, Paul W. (2001), “The Unfulfilled Promise of Cultural
111. Capital Theory,” Sociology of Education, 74 (Extra Issue), 88–
——— and ——— (2013b), “Consumption Compromises: Negotia- 99.
tion and Unification Within Contemporary Families,” Journal Kipnis, Eva, Amanda J. Broderick, and Catherine Demangeot
of Business Research, 67 (4), 449–56. (2013), “Consumer Multiculturation: Consequences of Multi-
Davis, Harry L. (1970), “Dimensions of Marital Roles in Con- Cultural Identification for Brand Knowledge,” Consumption
sumer Decision Making,” Journal of Marketing Research, 7 Markets & Culture, 17 (3), 1–23.
(May), 168–77. Kumar, Nirmalya and Jan-Benedict E.M. Steenkamp (2013), “The
——— (1976), “Decision Making Within the Household,” Journal Globe: Diaspora Marketing,” Harvard Business Review, 91
of Consumer Research, 2 (4), 241–60. (10), 127–31.
——— and Benny P. Rigaux (1974), “Perception of Marital Roles Lackman, Conway and John M. Lanasa (1993), “Family Decision-
in Decision Processes,” Journal of Consumer Research, 1 (1), Making Theory: An Overview and Assessment,” Psychology &
51–62. Marketing, 10 (2), 81–93.
De Graaf, Paul M. (1986), “The Impact of Financial and Cultural Lareau, Annette and Elliot B. Weininger (2003), “Cultural Capital
Resources on Educational Attainment in the Netherlands,” in Educational Research: A Critical Assessment,” Theory and
Sociology of Education, 59 (4), 237–46. Society, 32 (5/6), 567–606.
DiMaggio, Paul and John Mohr (1985), “Cultural Capital, Educa- Lau-Gesk, Loraine G. (2003), “Activating Culture Through Per-
tional Attainment, and Marital Selection,” American Journal of suasion Appeals: An Examination of the Bicultural Consumer,”
Sociology, 90 (6), 1231–61. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 301–315.
Dixon, Andrea L., Rosann L. Spiro, and Maqbul Jamil (2001), Lauth Bacas, Jutta (2002), “Cross-Border Marriages and the Forma-
“Successful and Unsuccessful Sales Calls: Measuring Sales- tion of Transnational Families: A Case Study of Greek-German
person Attributions and Behavioral Intentions,” Journal of Couples in Athens,” working paper, (accessed February 19,
Marketing, 65 (July), 64–78. 2014), [available at www.transcomm.ox.ac.uk/working%
Emerson, Greg (2011), “The Least Diverse States in America,” 20papers/WPTC-02-10%20Bacas.pdf].
MainStreet.com, (February 28), (accessed February 26, 2014), Locke, Karen (1996), “Rewriting the Discovery of Grounded
[available at http://www.mainstreet.com/slideshow/lifestyle/ Theory After 25 Years?” Journal of Management Inquiry, 5
least-diverse-states-america]. (3), 239–45.
Epp, Amber M. and Linda L. Price (2008), “Family Identity: A Luna, David, Torsten Ringberg, and Laura A. Peracchio (2008),
Framework of Identity Interplay in Consumption Practices,” “One Individual, Two Identities: Frame Switching Among
Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (1), 50–69. Biculturals,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (2), 279–93.
——— and ——— (2011), “Designing Solutions Around Customer Malone, Nolan, Kaari F. Baluja, Joseph M. Costanzo, and Cynthia
Network Identity Goals,” Journal of Marketing, 75 (March), J. Davis (2003), “The Foreign-Born Population: 2000,” U.S.
36–54. Census Bureau, (accessed February 19, 2014), [available at
French, John and Bertram H. Raven (1959), “The Bases of Social http://www.census.gov/prod/2003pubs/c2kbr-34.pdf].
Power,” in Studies in Social Power, D. Cartwright, ed. Ann McCracken, Grant (2009), Chief Culture Officer: How to Create a
Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, 150–67. Living, Breathing Corporation. New York: Basic Books.
Gilly, Mary C. (1995), “The Consumer Acculturation of Expatriate Meng, Xin and Robert G. Gregory (2005), “Intermarriage and the
Americans,” in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 22, Economic Assimilation of Immigrants,” Journal of Labor Eco-
Frank R. Kardes and Mita Sujan, eds. Provo, UT: Association nomics, 23 (1), 135–75.
for Consumer Research, 506–510. Merton, Robert K. and Elinor Barber (1976), “Sociological
Glaser, Barnet G. and Anselm L. Strauss (1967), The Discovery of Ambivalence,” in Sociological Ambivalence, Robert K. Mer-
Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. ton, ed. New York: The Free Press, 3–31.
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. Nelson, Michelle R. and Sameer Deshpande (2004), “Love With-
Green, Robert T. and Isabella C.M. Cunningham (1975), “Femi- out Borders: An Examination of Cross-Cultural Wedding Ritu-
nine Role Perception and Family Purchasing Decisions,” Jour- als,” in Contemporary Consumption Rituals: A Research
nal of Marketing Research, 12 (August), 325–32. Anthology, Cele C. Otnes and Tina M. Lowrey, eds. Mahwah,
———, Jean-Paul Leonardi, Jean-Louis Chandon, Isabella C.M. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 125–48.
Cunningham, Bronis Verhage, and Alain Strazzieri (1983), ——— and Cele C. Otnes (2005), “Exploring Cross-Cultural
“Societal Development and Family Purchasing Roles: A Cross- Ambivalence: A Netnography of Intercultural Wedding Mes-
National Study,” Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (4), 436–42. sage Boards,” Journal of Business Research, 58 (1), 89–95.

138 / Journal of Marketing, May 2014


Ogden, Denise T., James R. Ogden, and Hope Jensen Schau Sahlins, Marshall (1999), “Two or Three Things That I Know
(2004), “Exploring the Impact of Culture and Acculturation on About Culture,” Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute,
Consumer Purchase Decisions: Toward a Microcultural Per- 5 (3), 399–421.
spective,” Academy of Marketing Science Review, 3, (accessed Shen, Hao, Fang Wan, and Robert S. Wyer Jr. (2010), “Cross-Cul-
February 19, 2014), [available at http://www.amsreview.org/ tural Differences in the Refusal to Accept a Small Gift: The
articles/ogden03-2004.pdf]. Differential Influence of Reciprocity Norms on Asians and
Oswald, Laura (1999), “Culture Swapping: Consumption and the North Americans,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Ethnogenesis of Middle-Class Haitian Immigrants,” Journal of ogy, 100 (2), 271–81.
Consumer Research, 25 (4), 303–318. Shuptrine, F. Kelly and G. Samuelson (1976), “Dimensions of
Otnes, Cele, Tina M. Lowrey, and L.J. Shrum (1997), “Toward an Marital Roles in Consumer Decision Making: Revisited,”
Understanding of Consumer Ambivalence,” Journal of Con- Journal of Marketing Research, 13 (February), 87–91.
sumer Research, 24 (1), 80–93. Solomon, Michael R. (1983), “The Role of Products as Social
Park, Lisa Sun-Hee (2005), Consuming Citizenship: Children of Stimuli: A Symbolic Interactionism Perspective,” Journal of
Asian Immigrant Entrepreneurs. Stanford, CA: Stanford Uni- Consumer Research, 10 (3), 319–29.
versity Press. Strauss, Anselm and Juliet Corbin (1998), Basics of Qualitative
Peck, Joann and Terry L. Childers (2003), “To Have and to Hold: Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing
The Influence of Haptic Information on Product Judgments,” Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Journal of Marketing, 67 (April), 35–48. Thomas, David C., Mary Yoko Brannen, and Dominie Garcia
Peñaloza, Lisa N. (1989), “Immigrant Consumer Acculturation,” (2010), “Bicultural Individuals and Intercultural Effective-
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 16, Thomas K. Srull, ness,” European Journal of Cross-Cultural Competence and
ed. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 110–18. Management, 1 (4), 315–33.
——— and Mary C. Gilly (1986), “The Hispanic Family— U.S. Census Bureau (2010), “Nation’s Foreign-Born Population
Consumer Research Issues,” Psychology & Marketing, 3 (4), Nears 37 Million,” (October 19), (accessed February 19, 2014),
291–304. [available at https://www.census.gov/newsroom/releases/
——— and ——— (1999), “Marketer Acculturation: The Changer archives/foreignborn_population/cb10-159.html].
and the Changed,” Journal of Marketing, 63 (July), 84–104. Ustuner, Tuba and Douglas B. Holt (2007), “Construction of Poor
Putnam, Mandy and William R. Davidson (1987), “Family Purchas- Migrant Women’s Consumer Identity Projects in a Turkish
ing Behavior: Family Roles by Product Category,” in The RIS Squatter,” Journal of Consumer Research, 34 (1), 41–56.
Consumer Focus Series. Dublin, OH: Management Horizons. Wallendorf, Melanie and Michael D. Reilly (1983a), “Distinguish-
Qualls, William J. (1987), “Household Decision Behavior: The ing Culture of Origin from Culture of Residence,” in Advances
Impact of Husbands’ and Wives’ Sex Role Orientation,” Jour- in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice
nal of Consumer Research, 14 (2), 264–79. M. Tybout, eds. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer
Raven, Bertram H. (1992), “A Power/Interaction Model of Inter- Research, 699–701.
personal Influence: French and Raven Thirty Years Later,” ——— and ——— (1983b), “Ethnic Migration, Assimilation, and
Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 7 (2), 217–44. Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (3), 292–
———, Richard Centers, and Aroldo Rodrigues (1975), “The 302.
Bases of Conjugal Power,” in Power in Families, Ronald E. Webster, Cynthia (2000), “Is Spousal Decision Making a Cultur-
Cromwell and David. H. Olson, eds. New York: Sage Publica- ally Situated Phenomenon?” Psychology & Marketing, 17 (12),
tions, 217–32. 1035–58.
———, Joseph Schwarzwald, and Meni Koslowsky (1998), “Con- Wilk, Richard R. (1989), “Decision Making and Resource Flows
ceptualizing and Measuring a Power/Interaction Model of Within the Household: Beyond the Black Box,” in The House-
Interpersonal Influence,” Journal of Applied Social Psychol- hold Economy: Reconsidering the Domestic Mode of Produc-
ogy, 28 (4), 307–332. tion, Richard R. Wilk, ed. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 23–
Redfield, Robert, Ralph Linton, and Melville J. Herskovits (1936), 52.
“Memorandum on the Study of Acculturation,” American ——— and Robert McC. Netting (1984), “Households: Changing
Anthropologist, 38 (1), 54–60. Forms and Functions,” in Households: Comparative and His-
Robbins, Derek (2005), “The Origins, Early Development and torical Studies of the Domestic Group, Robert McC. Netting,
Status of Bourdieu’s Concept of ‘Cultural Capital,’” The Richard R. Wilk, and Eric J. Arnould, eds. Berkeley: Univer-
British Journal of Sociology, 56 (1), 13–30. sity of California Press, 1–28.
Roberge, Marie-Elene and Rolf van Dick (2010), “Recognizing Wolfe, Donald M. (1959), “Power and Authority in the Family,” in
the Benefits of Diversity: When and How Does Diversity Studies in Social Power, Dorwin Cartwright, ed. Ann Arbor,
Increase Group Performance?” Human Resource Management MI: University of Michigan, 99–117.
Review, 20 (4), 295–308.
Rudmin, Floyd W. (2003), “Critical History of the Acculturation
Psychology of Assimilation, Separation, Integration, and Mar-
ginalization,” Review of General Psychology, 7 (1), 3–37.

Cultural Competence and Cultural Compensatory Mechanisms / 139

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen