Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

Face-ism: Five Studies of Sex Differences in Facial Prominence

Dane Archer, Bonita Iritani, Debra D. Kimes, and Michael Barrios


University of California, Santa Cruz

Five studies are presented on relative facial prominence in depictions of men and
women. This hypothesized sex difference may be theoretically important because
visual representations presumably contribute to generic conceptions of what is
unique about each of the two sexes. Three studies describe the prevalence of "face-
ism"—greater facial prominence in depictions of men—in three different contexts:
in American periodicals, in publications from 11 cultures, and in artwork over six
centuries. A fourth study reports experimental evidence that this difference also
occurs in amateur drawings of men and women. A final study varies facial prom-
inence experimentally in photographs and finds consequent changes in rated in-
telligence and other characteristics. The implications for sex differences, and par-
ticularly for the perceived intellectual qualities of women, are discussed.

Research on sex differences is burgeoning. differences and stereotyping. One of the first
This literature has developed in two general of these, conducted by Courtney and Lockeretz
areas: (a) actual or purported intellectual and (1971), analyzed 312 ads from eight general
physiological differences between men and magazines and found that although 45% of
women (e.g., Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974), and the men were depicted in paid-work roles, only
(b) social and psychological differences in the 9% of the women were portrayed with this
ways in which men and women are treated status. Similarly, Sexton and Haberman (1974)
and perceived (e.g., Henley, 1977; Thome & examined 1,827 magazine advertisements and
Henley, 1975; Weitzman, 1979). This article reported that only 16% of them presented
is in the second of these two traditions and women in any sort of nontraditional situation.
presents five related studies concerning a con- Finally, Venkatesan and Losco (1975) studied
sistent and previously uninvestigated differ- 14,378 magazine ads published between 1959
ence between the visual images and represen- and 1971. They found that the three roles most
tations of men and women. frequently portrayed were (a) woman as sexual
Previous research has produced the less than object, (b) woman as physically beautiful, and
startling conclusion that the communications (c) woman as dependent on a man.
media tend to depict women in "traditional" Although most media research appears to
roles. Most of these studies have focused on have focused on advertising, Miller (1975) an-
specific aspects of the media, such as television alyzed 3,661 news photographs published in
scripts (e.g., Miles, 1975; McArthur & Resko, The Washington Post and The Los Angeles
1975). Studies of media advertising have con- Times during 1973 and 1974. This study also
sistently reported widespread, pervasive sex found pronounced sex differences: (a) 81% of
the photographs were exclusively of men; (b)
although 75% of the men were presented in
This research was supported by Faculty Research Funds political, professional, or sports-oriented roles,
granted by the University of California and by a Guggen- women usually appeared when they were so-
heim Fellowship to the first author. The authors would
like to thank Ellen Hipschman, Barbara Curbow, Jeanne cialites, celebrities, or the wives of famous men;
Tschann, Robin Akert, and Guntram Wolski for valuable and (c) fully one fourth of all the women pho-
assistance. The authors would also like to thank Myron tographed in The Washington Post were brides.
Rothbart and the two anonymous reviewers for their valu- These studies demonstrate that mass-media
able suggestions and skilled editorial assistance. depictions of the sexes differ strikingly in con-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Dane Archer,
Stevenson College, University of California, Santa Cruz, tent. Women and men are shown pursuing
California 95064. different activities and saying different things.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1983, Vol. 45, No, 4, 725-735
Copyright 1983 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

725
726 ARCHER, IRITANI, K1MES, AND BARRIOS

This content-oriented research has been highly Based on casual observations of media pho-
fruitful and has yielded differences that are tographs of men and women, it is our hy-
both dramatic and important. A somewhat pothesis that the essence of a man is thought
different approach to the study of sex differ- to reside in his face and head, whereas that of
ences involves qualitative aspects of the man- a woman is thought to reside more generally
ner or nonverbal style in which women and in her body.
men are presented. The empirical test of this general hypothesis
One of the researchers concerned with these is the degree to which the face dominates the
more qualitative differences has been Erving images of men and women. This article reports
Goffman (1976). Using 508 photographs from five studies on facial prominence. Three of
newspapers and magazines, Goffman identi- these are nonexperimental and descriptive; two
fied a variety of ways in which published pho- are experiments designed to examine the pos-
tographs foster ail image of women as sub- sible consequences of facial prominence.
ordinate and dependent. According to Goff-
man, there are consistent sex differences in General Method: A Face-ism Index
these photographs in terms of posture, gesture, A simple method was designed to measure relative facial
touching, and other nonverbal behaviors. For prominence. This method was designed for use with pho-
example, women tend to be shown in second- tographs, drawings, or other visual images. The method
ary or deferential poses, for example, lying consists of creating an index that is the ratio of two linear
down or standing with a bent or "canted" pos- measurements. The numerator of this index is the distance
(in millimeters or any other unit) in a depiction from the
ture. Men, on the other hand, are often po- top of the head to the lowest point of the chin. The de-
sitioned to be physically elevated above nominator of the index is the distance from the top of the
women. There are also more specific sex dif- head to the lowest visible part of the subject's body.
ferences, for example, men are shown behaving This index of facial prominence is simply the first mea-
aggressively toward women in what Goffman ratio, it candivided
surement by the second. Because the index is a
be applied to photographs, artwork, or images
calls "mock assault." Goffman's interesting of any size. In theory, this index ranges from a low of .00
descriptive work remains relatively unique in (the face is not shown at all) to a high of 1.00 (the picture
the research literature. In general, we know shows only the face and no other part of the body is visible).
more about the content of sex differences in The higher the index, the more the face is used to represent
the media than we do about the stylistic or the person. A low index means the face is minimized; a
high index means the face dominates the image.
qualitative dimensions of these differences. For want of an existing or better term, we have called
This article reports five studies on one pre- this measure the face-ism index. The definition of this
viously unexamined aspect of the stylistic rep- index is straightforward: Face-ism is the relative promi-
resentations of women and men. These studies nence of the face in a photograph, drawing, or other de-
piction of a person. This index appears to be highly reliable,
address the degree to which visual depictions and this is not surprising because physical measurements
of men and women differ in terms of facial are involved. In independent measurements of a pilot
prominence. It is our working hypothesis that sample of 30 photographs, the face-ism index scores of
facial prominence is consistently higher in the two judges were correlated at better than .99. The index
depictions of men than in those of women. If is, therefore, both simple to compute and stable across
coders.
this difference is found, it may have some In any given instance, the application of the face-ism
theoretical significance. Visual representations index may require specific coding rules. In some photo-
like those in the communications media im- graphs, for example, the top or bottom of a person's head
plicitly provide a rendering of the identity, in- may be masked by hats, beards, hairstyles, hands on the
chin, and so forth. In these cases, the location of the head's
dividuality, character, personality, or essence boundaries are estimated by the coder. In addition, use
of the person depicted. of the face-ism index in working with communications
If media depictions of men and women differ media may call for additional coding rules regarding the
in terms of facial prominence, these differences eligibility of certain types of photographs, repeated ap-
may (wittingly or not) communicate something pearances of the same pictures, and so forth. An example
of coding rules for this purpose is described in the first
important about the relative importance for study.
each sex of mind and body. Because the face
and head are the centers of mental life—in- Study 1: American Photographs
tellect, personality, identity, and character—
the relative prominence of this part of the As a descriptive, nonexperimental test of
anatomy may be symbolically consequential. our working hypothesis, the face-ism index was
SEX DIFFERENCES AND FACIAL PROMINENCE 727

used to code 1,750 published photographs. The pictures illustrated text rather than were ad-
photographs were selected from at least 12 vertisements. Sex and photograph type were
issues of each of five American periodicals: related—only 20.1% of the men in photo-
Time, Newsweek, Ms., The San Francisco graphs were found in ads, but 33.4% of the
Chronicle, and a small city newspaper, The women in photographs were in ads. This dif-
Santa Cruz Sentinel. One fifth (350) of our ference was significant (x2 = 38.09, p < .001).
1,750 photographs were obtained from each The major finding of Study 1 concerns sex
publication. differences in the face-ism index. In the 1,750
The issues were from the same period of photographs examined, there is a pronounced
time for all five publications, and the photo- tendency to represent men with their faces
graphs coded were the first pictures encoun- and women with their bodies. The mean face-
tered using a simple set of eligibility rules. The ism index for the pictures of men (n = 1,058)
purpose of these rules was to limit comparisons was .65, whereas the mean face-ism index for
to photographs of individuals and to eliminate women (« = 692) was only .45. This difference
photographs biased toward specific regions of was easily significant (t = 18.23, p < .001). In
the body. Coding rules were as follows: (a) addition to being statistically significant, this
photographs may contain only one human difference is also very large in absolute terms.
subject, (b) photographs that aim to capture The mean face-ism scores of men and women
some particular body region, movement, differ by almost an entire standard deviation
problem, or gesture cannot be counted (e.g., (d= .91; Cohen, 1977).
performing athletes, people modeling clothes These mean values of the face-ism index
or cosmetics, using tools, in weight loss ads, are readily interpretable in verbal terms be-
etc.), (c) photographs with a "co-subject" can- cause the index reflects the proportion of a
not be used (a person next to an elephant, a picture devoted to the person's face. For
driver with a car, etc.), and (d) photographs women, the mean face-ism index of .45 in-
printed several times are counted only once dicates that, in general, less than half of each
(photographs of regular columnists, entertain- photograph was devoted to a woman's face.
ment ads, etc,), and (e) photographs of dis- By contrast, the mean face-ism score of .65
embodied heads are not counted. for men indicates that approximately two
The purpose of these eligibility rules was to thirds of each picture was devoted to the man's
limit the comparison to photographs in which face.
the visible fraction of the depicted person's In addition to the main effect for sex, the
body was entirely arbitrary. In these cases, in design makes it possible to test for the effect
theory, any portion of the person's body could of publication type and of whether the picture
have been shown. In the five publications ex- appeared in a story or advertisement. This
amined, the photographs were of two types: design lends itself to a 2 X 5 X 2 (Sex of
those in advertisements and those that accom- Picture X Publication X Story or Ad) analysis
panied articles. The face-ism index was com- of variance (ANOVA). The mean face-ism scores
puted separately for both types of pictures. of the 1,750 photographs in the 20 cells in
thijs design are shown in Table 1.
Results This analysis indicates significant main ef-
fects for all three variables at better than the
There were more pictures of men than .001 level: sex of picture, F(\, 1730) = 215.49;
women. Of the 1,750 eligible photographs, publication, F(4, 1730) = 14.68; story or ad,
1,058 (60.5%) were of men. The visual "land- F(l, 1730) = 39.96. These differences indicate
scape" of these publications is therefore pre- that newspapers tend to be high on face-ism
dominantly male. For four of the five peri- (i.e., show a lot of face) and that pictures ac-
odicals, more than 70% of the photographs companying stories emphasize faces more than
were of men. The significantly different pe- those accompanying advertising. There were
riodical, as might be expected, is Ms. Only also two significant interactions: Sex X Pub-
14.3% of the pictures in this publication were lication, F(4, 1730) = 3.05, p < .02, and Pub-
of men. In general, eligible photographs were lication X Story or Ad, F(4, 1730) = 4.96, p <
niore commonly found in articles and stories .001. These interactions indicate that the face-
than in advertisements: 1,306 (74.6%) of the ism effect is somewhat larger for newspapers
728 ARCHER, IRITANI, KIMES, AND BARRIOS

Table 1
Face-ism Index Scores for Women and Men in Stories and Advertisements in Five Publications
The San Francisco The Santa Cruz
Photograph Time Newsweek Ms Chronicle Sentinel Total n

Stories
Women .39 .52 .42 .52 .51 .46 461
Men .65 .63 .56 .69 .70 .66 845
Ads
Women .39 .32 .42 .43 .47 .41 231
Men .51 .48 .49 .58 .69 .55 213
Total .56 .57 .44 .63 .65 .57 1750

Note, n = 350 per publication.

than magazines and that this effect varies in Results


some publications according to whether the
picture appears with a story or advertisement. This analysis yielded several findings. In
The most striking effect in Study 1 is the general, women were outnumbered by about
strong sex difference. Of the 10 comparisons four to one in these photographs. The most
in Table 1, all 10 indicate a tendency to rep- important result in Table 2 concerns face-ism.
In every culture examined, the photographs
resent men by their faces and women by their
bodies. This is true for ads and stories and for showed a significant face-ism effect. Even when
all five publications, including Ms. magazine. the relative prominence of the face is entirely
arbitrary, face-ism affects the images of men
Because the face^ism "effect" occurs even in
a publiction identified with women's rights, it and women in each of these cultures. These
suggests that this sexual difference in repre- face-ism effects were large in absolute terms
sentational styles is widespread, deep-seated, as well as significant (median d = .75; Cohen,
1977). This study indicates that face-ism is
and perhaps unconscious. The face-ism dif-
ferences shown in Table 1 are particularly in- not purely an American invention. When one
examines the images of men and women across
teresting because the pictures examined did
not show women modeling swimsuits and cultures, there is strong evidence that face-ism
men modeling hats. The coding rules used in plays a consistent role in the depictions of the
this study limited comparisons to photographs sexes.
in which the relative prominence of the face
was entirely arbitrary. In theory, each pho- Study 3: Cross-Era Images
tograph could have shown any portion of the Just as Study 2 presented a cross-national test
person's body. Even when photographs of this of the face-ism hypothesis, Study 3 presents
controlled nature are examined, however, there a "cross era" test for the presence of face-ism
are dramatically different representational over time. This approach has been recom-
styles of men and women. mended as a methodological analogue to cross-
Study 2: Cross-National Photographs national research (McGuire, 1976) to provide
additional comparative evidence. Like cross-
Because the sex differences identified in cultural and cross-national methods, cross-era
Study 1 might be limited to American media, research can provide valuable evidence about
a second study was conducted to examine print the generality or limits of a given finding.
media from other cultures. Using the same In this study, artwork produced across 6
eligibility rules used in Study 1, more than centuries was examined to provide a cross-era
3,500 pictures were coded for face-ism in 13 test of the face-ism hypothesis. Paintings were
publications from 11 other nations. The results examined across this long time span to see
of this procedure are indicated in Table 2. For whether face-ism is a purely modern phenom-
purposes of comparison, the American data enon. The most appropriate paintings for this
reported in Study 1 are included in Table 2. analysis are portraits and self-portraits. These
SEX DIFFERENCES AND FACIAL PROMINENCE 729

works provide the artist (and subject) with an- strictions were used: (a) only paintings and
atomic freedom because the relative promi- drawings were coded, and photographs and
nence of the face in these works is entirely sculpture were not used; (b) works whose titles
arbitrary. The subject of a portrait or self-por- specified that specific rooms or spaces are being
trait can be shown in full length, as head and depicted were not coded (e.g., "Napoleon in
torso only, or as only the face. Unlike paintings His Study"); (c) pictures in which the bound-
of landscapes, portraits and self-portraits are aries of the head or body cannot be discerned
intended to capture the identity, character, and were not counted (this occurs in some 20th-
essence of the person portrayed. For this rea- century art); and (d) works in which the per-
son, these works constitute an important data son's sex is unclear and not clarified in ac-
set for the face-ism hypothesis. companying text were not used.
A total of 920 portraits and self-portraits
were scored for face-ism. These works of art Results
were drawn from more than 30 volumes of
reproductions (e.g., Gasser, 1963; Gibson, Because the number of reproduced portraits
1978; Kerslake, 1977). The artworks in this from the earliest centuries studied was rela-
study were coded for face-ism using the same tively small, the artworks were grouped in the
general eligibility rules used in the first two following four eras: (a) 15th and 16th centuries,
studies. In addition, the following coding re- for example, works of Leonardo da Vinci and

Table 2
Face-ism Index Scores for Women and Men in Photographs From Periodicals in 12 Societies
Face-ism score

Nation Women n Men n /

Chile
Ercilla .34 100 .52 100 6.35***
England
Illustrated London News .49 100 .55 100 2.06*
France
Paris Match .33 100 .52 100 5.48***
Le Nouvel Observateur .49 59 .62 291 3.92***
Federal Republic of Germany
Der Spiegel .49 60 .60 290 3.38***
Hong Kong
Far Eastern Economic Review .56 13 .67 337 2.11*
India
Link .45 45 .64 305 6.67***
Italy
L'europeo .33 32 .49 100 3.84***
L'Expresso .49 35 .61 315 2.84**
Kenya
The Weekly Review .47' 25 .72 325 8.93***
Mexico
La Revista Inter-American Vision .49 34 .60 316 3.13**
Middle East •
Middle East Magazine .55 32 .72 318 4.21***
Spain
Gambia 16 .39 98 .58 100 5.36***
U.S.
(see Table 1) .45 692 .65 1058 18.23***
Mdn .49 .60
Note. N = 5,480.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
730 ARCHER, IRITANI, KIMES, AND BARRIOS

Raphael; (b) 17th and 18th centuries, for ex- Table 3


ample, Rembrandt and Velazquez; (c) 19th Face-ism Index Scores for Paintings and
century, for example, Renoir and Cezanne; Drawings From Four Eras
and (d) 20th century^ for example, Modigliani Mean face-ism
and Matisse, In each of these four intervals, score"
230 paintings (115 portraits of women and
115 portraits of men) were scored for face- Era Women Men
ism. These 920 paintings comprised the first 15th& 16th centuries .39 .41 <(
works encountered using the eligibility rules 17th & 18th centuries .30 .38 3.00*
already indicated. The results of this cross-era 19th century .28 .40 5.35**
analysis of artwork are shown in Table 3. 20th century .34 .50 5.93**
These data indicate that face-ism is centuries
old and not merely a modern invention. With Note. N = 920.
" The means for each era are based on 230 works of art
the exception of the very earliest work ex- (115 portraits of women, 115 portraits of men).
amined, each time period in this analysis is * p < .01. **p <.001.
characterized by significant sex differences in
facial prominence. There is also some indi-
cation that this difference has increased rather A small experiment was conducted using
than diminished over the past 6 centuries. Sex 80 university undergraduates (40 women and
differences in face-ism appear to have in- 40 men). These subjects were randomly as-
creased with the passage of each century, con- signed to one of two conditions. In the first
cluding with the .50-.34 difference shown for condition, subjects were given a nearly blank
20th-century artwork. Just as the first two piece of paper, with only this paragraph of
studies found pervasive sex differences in pub- instructions at the top of it:
lished photographs across a large number of We are doing a study on drawing styles. Could you help
societies, the study of artwork over time finds us by spending a few minutes to draw a woman? The
parallel differences across several centuries. drawing can be as simple as you like—we are just interested
Considered as a whole, these three studies in- in the ways people draw. If you can, please try to capture
dicate that sex differences in facial prominence the character of a real person in your drawing.
appear to characterize a very wide range of The second condition was identical, except
public images of women and men. that subjects were asked to draw a man. An
equal number of men and women were as-
Study 4: Experimental Research signed to each condition, and this divided the
on Drawings 80 subjects evenly across the four cells of the
resulting 2 X 2 design (Sex of Drawing X Sex
Published photographs and art forms are of Subject). The 80 drawings were scored for
important in that they present images to a face-ism using the same method used to code
large audience. These images constitute a pub- photographs in the first three studies.
lic visual definition of the unique qualities and
identity of the person represented—for ex-
Results
ample, "This is the person this story is about"
or "Here is a portrait of what this person is This experiment provides further evidence
like." These images are widely distributed, and that face-ism may be pervasive. A 2 X 2 ANOVA
if they contain a consistent representational on the face-ism index scores of the 80 pho-
difference between men and women, this dif- tographs produced only one significant effect:
ference may have important effects. a highly significant main effect for the sex of
The three face-ism studies already reported the person in the drawing, F(l, 76) = 7.94,
present nonexperimental evidence concerning p = .006. The mean face-ism scores of the four
face-ism. These studies indicate that public conditions in this experiment on amateur
depictions of men and women contain con- drawings are shown in Table 4.
sistent, significant differences in terms of facial As indicated in Table 4, the sex of the artist
prominence. Unlike these three studies, Study was unimportant. Both men and women made
4 presents experimental evidence drawn from drawings in which the man's face was relatively
unpublished drawings of men and women. prominent and the woman's was not. In ad-
SEX DIFFERENCES AND FACIAL PROMINENCE 731

Table 4 perimentally to see if changes in facial prom-


Face-ism Index Scores for 80 Drawings inence would affect attributions about the de-
of Men and Women picted person. In the absence of prior research,
Subjects it is of course impossible to know what qual-
ities (if any) would be most affected by changes
Instruction Women Men Total in relative facial prominence: wit, wisdom,
warmth, and so forth. Because the face and
Draw a woman .22 .19 .21
Draw a man .39 .36 .37
head are the centers of mental life, however,
it seemed important to include in this study
Note, n = 40 for women; n = 40 for men. attributions about perceived intellectual qual-
ities of the person.
In this experiment, 60 university students
were asked to rate stimulus persons in one of
dition to this quantitative face-ism effect, there two test booklets. These two test booklets both
also seemed to be a qualitative face-ism effect contained photographs of the same 12 people
in this study. The 80 amateur artists in this (six women and six men). Each of these 12
experiment showed a tendency to detail the people appeared once in each booklet. In one
men's facial features (eyes, nose, mouth, etc,). booklet, the person was shown in a "low" face-
In the drawings made of women, however, the ism condition (with a mean face-ism score of
face was left relatively featureless. Using a .15 in the photographs used). In the other
crude "facial detail" scale (one point for each booklet, the same person was shown in a
of the following: eye, eyebrow, nose, mouth, "high" face-ism condition (with a mean face-
lips), the faces of male figures were significantly ism score of .60). The high- and low-face-ism
more detailed, F(l, 73) = 7.82, p < .01. pictures of a person were printed from the
This study suggests that face-ism may occur same negative so that the face of the depicted
in the private representational systems used person in both conditions was identical in every
by individuals as well as in the public depic- way except relative prominence.
tions of the mass media. This experimental Each of the two booklets included some low-
evidence indicates that even when the relative face-ism pictures and some high-face-ism pic-
prominence of the face can take on any value tures. All 12 photographs were taken against
in a drawing, there is a tendency to emphasize the same background. The 60 raters were as-
the importance of the face for men and di- signed to one of two test booklets, with equal
minish it for women. numbers of men and women rating each
booklet. Using a 10-point scale, these 60 judges
Study 5: An Experiment on the rated the people in the photographs on their
Effects of Face-ism apparent intelligence, ambition, physical ap-
pearance, and warmth.1 These four qualities
The four studies already reported indicate were not chosen systematically, nor as rep-
that face-ism appears to be pronounced and resentatives of general factors in person per-
widespread. In this sense, the first four studies ception, but merely as illustrations of the kinds
address the prevalence of this previously un- of attributions that could be investigated using
investigated sex difference. This final study this experimental procedure.
addresses the possible effects of face-ism. Be-
cause the first four studies indicate that face- Results
ism exists, it is interesting to ask whether this
sex difference is likely to produce important By means of repeated measures ANOVAS,
differences in attributions, perceptions, and the judges' ratings were compared across three
conceptions of men and women.
An experiment was conducted to provide
some evidence on this question. The basic 1
method used in this experiment involves a The judges indicated their ratings on a 10-point scale,
ranging from "low" to "high." In retrospect, physical at-
manipulation of face-ism in a person-percep- tractiveness would have been a better term than the more
tion task. In this study, the degree of face-ism ambiguous physical appearance (which would mean at-
in the depiction of a person was varied ex- tractiveness but also neatness, etc.).
732 ARCHER, IRITANI, KIMES, AND BARRIOS

Table 5
Experimental Data on Ratings of Photographs Varying in Facial Prominence: F Values
from Four Repeated Measures ANOVAS
Variable

Source of variation Intelligence Ambition Physical appearance Warmth


Subject's sex (SUBSEX) 2.32 <, <, 1.82
Facial prominence (PROM) 7.41* 13.21** 16.38** 1.58
Stimulus's sex (STIMSEX) 1.01 <1 11.21** 16.00«*
PROM X STIMSEX 2.41 <1 3.25 <1
PROM X SUBSEX <1 1.49 <1 <1
STIMSEX X SUBSEX <1 <1 1.37 1.23
PROM X STIMSEX X SUBSEX <1 1.33 1.54 <1
Note. N = 60. For each analysis of variance (ANOVA) df = \, 58.
*p< .01. **p < .001.

factors: subject (judge) sex, the relative facial prominence received more favorable ratings
prominence (high or low) of each photograph, on intelligence, ambition, and physical ap-
and sex of stimulus (the sex of the person in pearance. This finding suggests that perceived
each photograph). The results of this analysis intellectual (and other) qualities may be sig-
are shown in Table 5. The most important nificantly and favorably affected by something
finding to emerge from this analysis concerns as simple as the relative prominence of the
facial prominence. For three of the four rating person's face. This finding is perhaps partic-
variables (intelligence, ambition, and physical ularly striking because each person photo-
appearance), relative facial prominence sig- graphed in this study served as his or her own
nificantly affected the ratings given to each control in an unusually precise manner. The
photograph. In addition, the sex of the person same person appears in both the high- and
photographed affected two of the rating vari- low-facial-prominence conditions, and the very
ables (physical appearance and warmth). As same negative was used to print both pho-
indicated in Table 5, the other main effects tographs. The same negative was used to elim-
and interactions in the analysis were not sta- inate the danger that a person might look quite
tistically significant. different in two consecutive photographs (e.g.,
In addition to being significant, the effect a flattering vs. unflattering picture).
of facial prominence was also positive. As Finally, it should be mentioned that other
shown in Table 6, photographs higher in facial designs could have been used in Study 5, and

Table 6
Mean Ratings on Four Characteristics by Sex and Facial Prominence of Person Photographed
Physical
Intelligence Ambition appearance Warmth

Photograph" M SD M SD M SD M SD
Women
High facial prominence 21.00 3.65 20.45 4.11 17.03 3.69 21.00 3.68
Low facial prominence 19.59 4.61 19.21 3.70 15.00 3.97 21.14 4.58
Men
High facial prominence 20.12 4.16 20.25 3.50 17.64 3.60 19.22 3.42
Low facial prominence 19.71 3.70 18.93 3.50 16.84 4.29 20.11 3.69

Note. N = 60.
* For each rating variable, the mean shown is the sum of three ratings (e.g., each judge rated three women in the high-
facial-prominence condition), and the maximum mean rating is 30 (three ratings of 10 each).
SEX DIFFERENCES AND FACIAL PROMINENCE 733

these alternative approaches merit further at- sequential. When the degree of facial promi-
tention. For example, a much wider range of nence is experimentally manipulated, it seems
rating variables should be employed. Although to affect attributions about a person's qualities.
this study indicates that experimental manip- The same person is perceived more favorably
ulation of relative facial prominence does affect when the face is prominent than when it
person perception, more multivariate rating is not.
studies are required for a comprehensive un- Considered together, these five studies in-
derstanding of what specific qualities are most dicate that facial prominence may be a variable
affected. In addition, further experimental of some theoretical interest. The prevalence
work could test more refined hypotheses. For of face-ism means that the representational
example, it is well established that people tend images of the two sexes are asymmetric. These
to attribute a variety of desirable qualities to studies suggest that essential aspects of personal
persons seen as physically attractive (e.g., Dion, identity are thought to be centered in different
Berscheid, & Walster, 1972). Experimental anatomic locations in men and women. Men
work on facial prominence could reflect this are represented by their heads and faces;
fact by including ratings of physical attrac- women are represented using more of their
tiveness in a covariance analysis to partial out bodies.
the effect of this apparently powerful variable. Although this difference seems to be both
This could not be done in the current study clear and pervasive, it should be noted that it
because our ratings included the somewhat could result either from a tendency to em-
different quality of physical appearance rather phasize men's faces or from a tendency to em-
than a direct rating of attractiveness. phasize women's bodies. The term face-ism is
The first four studies address the existence used in this article but one could argue that
and pervasiveness of face-ism, and this final the phenomenon in fact reflects "body-ism,"
experiment indicates that face-ism may have a tendency to include the body in represen-
notable consequences. The first four studies tations of women. A longer and somewhat
show that facial prominence is highly asym- more neutral term, relativefacial prominence,
metric: Faces tend to be much more prominent also could have been used in place of face-
in depictions of men than in those of women. ism. What seems clear, however, is that ana-
This final study indicates that this sex-related tomic differences do occur in the represen-
asymmetry may affect important qualities of tations of men and women and that the net
the ways in which a person is perceived. In result involves greater facial prominence for
particular, images high in relative facial prom- men than for women.
inence may produce more favorable attribu- Anatomic differences in sex images may
tions concerning intelligence, ambition, phys- have important effects. If the unique qualities
ical appearance, and other fundamentally im- of men are associated with the face, these
portant qualities. qualities are likely to be conceptualized in
terms of intellect, personality, character, wit,
General Discussion and other dimensions of mental life. If the
unique qualities of women are associated less
The first three studies in this article address with the head and more with the body, these
the nature and prevalence of face-ism—the qualities are likely to be conceptualized in
relative prominence of the face in depictions nonintellectual terms like weight, physique,
of men and women. These three studies in- attractiveness, or emotion. Facial prominence
dicate that there is a consistent asymmetry in may both reflect and contribute to thematic
the images of the sexes in published photo- conceptions about the relative importance for
graphs across several cultures and in artwork women and men of hearts and minds. The
across several centuries. The fourth study in- existence of these differential visual images in
dicates that this representational difference the communications media may prove to be
may be pervasive in that it also seems to occur particularly influential because there is some
in casual drawings of men and women. Finally, evidence for "nonverbal primacy"—that is,
the fifth study presents some experimental ev- that nonverbal channels of information play
idence that these sex differences may be con- a central role in person perception and
734 ARCHER, IRITANI, KIMES, AND BARRIOS

impression formation (Archer & Akert, 1977; metaphorical heart, and body shape and size.
Rosenthal, Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, & Archer, If the uniqueness of men and women is con-
1979). ceived of in these different ways, men would
Relative facial prominence appears to be a seem to have an advantage in that their identity
pervasive and theoretically interesting instance rests in mental qualities that are more durable
of sex differences. Based on the five studies than ephemeral somatic features like physique
reported in this article, the phenomenon war- and physical attractiveness.
rants further attention, and a number of av- Apart from these unresolved questions
enues for new work seem indicated. For ex- about its origins, face-ism may be of continuing
ample, one could focus on the process by importance if it contributes to and perpetuates
which individuals "frame" depictions of men stereotyped conceptions of what is important
and women. One might investigate this process about men and women. In particular, because
by asking subjects in field settings to approach this difference in facial prominence appears
a man (or a woman) from a distance and take to slight the intellectual qualities of women,
a photograph that captures the "essence" of its persistence and effects may be both note-
the person. The subject in such a study would worthy and problematic. These differences also
determine the distance between the camera may be of some practical concern in the com-
and the person photographed, and the re- munications media, in which the represen-
searcher could then examine the relative facial tations of men and women seem certain to
prominence in the resulting pictures. contribute to generic images about sex differ-
Other areas of new work might include de- ences. Although previously unrecognized and
velopmental studies of children's drawings to unexamined, differential facial prominence
see when differences in the depiction of men seems to constitute a major difference in the
and women first appear. One might search for way these images of men and women are con-
sex differences in facial prominence in natu- structed.
ralistic instances of "visual presentations of
the self"—for example, photographs of au-
thors on book jackets. One could also vary References
relative facial prominence in a series of stim-
ulus photographs and ask subjects to write Archer, D., & Akert, R. M. Words and everything else:
imaginative stories about the depicted indi- Verbal and nonverbal cues in social interpretation.
viduals. These stories could be coded for the- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977,35,
443-449.
matic content like achievement, corporal and Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
cerebral traits, and the presence of favorable ences. New York: Academic Press, 1977.
and unfavorable story outcomes. These studies Courtney, A,, & Lockeretz, S. W. A women's place: An
and other research could increase our under- analysis of the roles portrayed by women in magazine
advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 1971,
standing of the extent and effects of this rep- 8, 92-95.
resentational difference between men and Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. What is beautiful
women. is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Although the studies reported here indicate 1972, 24 285-290.
that face-ism exists across societies and over Gasser, M. Self-portraits: From thefifteenth century to the
present day. New York: Appleton, 1963.
time, the roots of this phenomenon remain Gibson, R. 20th century portraits: Exhibition at 15 Carlton
unexplained. This asymmetry in facial prom- House Terrace, SWI. London: National Portrait Gallery,
inence may lie deep in historic conceptions 1978.
about essential differences between the sexes. Goffman, E. Gender advertisements. New %rk: Harper &
Row, 1976.
The defining, identifying, and pivotal qualities Henley, N. Body politics: Power, sex, and nonverbal com-
associated with men may center on key di- munication. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1977.
mensions of mental life. The qualities uniquely Kerslake, 3. National Portrait Gallery: Early Georgian por-
associated with women, however, may be more traits (Vol. 1). London: Oxford University Press, 1977.
corporal than cerebral. The unique, defining Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. The psychology of sex
differences. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press,
features of women may be thought to lie in 1974.
physical attractiveness, emotions located in the McArthur, L. Z., & Resko, B. G. The portrayal of men
SEX DIFFERENCES AND FACIAL PROMINENCE 735

and women in American television commercials. Journal Sexton, D. E., & Haberman, P. Women in magazine ad-
of Social Psychology, 1975, 97, 209-220. vertisements. Journal of Advertising Research,. 1914,14,
McGuire, W. J. Historical comparisons: Testing psycho- 41-46.
logical hypotheses with cross-era data. International Thorne, B., & Henley, N. (Eds,). Language and sex: Dif-
Journal of Psychology, 1976,11, 161-183. ference and dominance. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House,
Miles, B. Channeling children: Sex stereotyping in prime 1975.
time TV: An analysis. Princeton, N.J.: Women on Words Venkatesan, M., & Losco, J. Women in magazine ads:
and Images, 1975. ' 1959-1971. Journal of Advertising Research, 1975, 15,
Miller, S. The content of news photos: Women's and men's 49-54.
roles. Journalism Quarterly, 1975, 52, 70-75. Weitzman, L. J. Sex role socialization: A focus on women.
Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P., Palo Alto Calif.: Mayfield, 1979.
& Archer, D. Sensitivity to nonverbal communication:
A profile approach to the measurement of differential
abilities. .Baltimore, Md.: Johns Hopkins University Received August 5, 1982
Press, 1979. Revision received February 1, 1983 •

Instructions to Authors
APA policy prohibits an author from submitting the same manuscript for concurrent con-
sideration by two or more journals. Authors should prepare manuscripts according to the
Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (3rd edition). All manuscripts
are subject to editing for sexist language. Manuscripts must include an abstract of 100-ISO
words. Instructions on tables, figures, references, metrics, and typing (all copy must be double-
spaced) appear in the Manual. Articles not prepared according to the Manual will not be
reviewed. For further information on content, authors should refer to the editorial in the March
1979 issue of this journal (Vol. 37, No, 3, pp. 468-469).
The reference citation for any article in any JPSP section follows APA's standard reference
style for journal articles; that is, authors, year of publication, article title, journal title, volume
number, and page numbers. The citation does not include the section title.
Manuscripts should be submitted in quadruplicate (the original and three photocopies) to
the appropriate section editor. All copies should be clear, readable, and on paper of good quality.
Editors' addresses appear on the inside front cover of the journal. Authors should keep a copy
of the manuscript to guard against loss.
Blind review is optional and must be specifically requested when a manuscript is first submitted.
In those cases, authors' names and affiliations must appear only on a separate title page, included
with each copy of the manuscript. Footnotes containing the identity of authors or their affiliations
must be on a separate sheet.
Section editors reserve the right to redirect papers among themselves as appropriate unless
an author specifically requests otherwise. Rejection by one section editor is considered rejection
by all, therefore a manuscript rejected by one section editor should not be submitted to another.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen