Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Original Research Article

Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 Accepted: June 27, 2009


Published online: August 18, 2009
DOI: 10.1159/000235575

Revisiting Brain Reserve Hypothesis


in Frontotemporal Dementia:
Evidence from a Brain Perfusion Study
B. Borroni a E. Premi a C. Agosti a A. Alberici a V. Garibotto d G. Bellelli b
B. Paghera c S. Lucchini c R. Giubbini c D. Perani d A. Padovani a
a
Centre for Aging Brain and Dementia, Department of Neurology, University of Brescia, b Alzheimer Disease
Rehabilitation Unit, Cremona Hospital and Geriatric Research Group, and c Nuclear Medicine Unit, Brescia Hospital,
Brescia, and d Division of Neuroscience, Scientific Institute San Raffaele, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University,
Milan, Italy

Key Words tion was found. The correlation between a reserve index, ac-
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration ⴢ Behavioral variant counting for both educational and occupational level, and
frontotemporal dementia ⴢ Education ⴢ Occupation ⴢ rCBF showed the same pattern of hypoperfusion. Conclu-
Leisure activities ⴢ Brain reserve hypothesis ⴢ SPECT sions: This study suggests that education and occupation
act as proxies for reserve capacity in bvFTD. These lifestyle
attainments may counteract the onset of this genetic-based
Abstract disease in at-risk individuals. Copyright © 2009 S. Karger AG, Basel
Background: Literature data on Alzheimer’s disease suggest
that years of schooling and occupational level are associated
with a reserve mechanism. No data on patients with behav-
ioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) are available Introduction
yet. Objective: To evaluate the impact of education, occupa-
tion, and midlife leisure activities on brain reserve in bvFTD. The brain reserve hypothesis posits that there are in-
Methods: Fifty-four bvFTD patients entered the study and dividual differences in the ability to cope with brain pa-
underwent neuropsychological and behavioral assessment, thology, and that brain damage extent and clinical symp-
including the FTD-modified Clinical Dementia Rating for toms are not tightly linked [1].
FTD (FTD-modified CDR), and SPECT imaging. We tested for Greater brain reserve arises through either difference
the linear correlation of educational and occupational level, in brain anatomy, such as a greater synapse density or neu-
and midlife leisure activities with regional cerebral blood ron counts, or by more efficient or flexible use of brain
flow (rCBF), controlling for demographic variables (age and networks, resulting in greater ability to sustain brain dam-
gender) and for cognitive performance (FTD-modified CDR) age without observable deficits in brain functions [2].
(statistical parametric mapping). Results: A significant rela- Identification of factors associated with the ability to
tionship between higher educational and occupational at- tolerate the accumulation of brain pathology has impor-
tainments and lower rCBF in medial frontal cortex and dor- tant implications for neurodegenerative disease preven-
solateral frontal cortex, bilaterally, was found (p ! 0.005). tion. It has been demonstrated that high premorbid intel-
When midlife leisure activities were considered, no correla- ligence, education, occupational attainment, and an ac-

© 2009 S. Karger AG, Basel Barbara Borroni, MD


1420–8008/09/0282–0130$26.00/0 Clinica Neurologica, Università degli Studi di Brescia
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 Pza Spedali Civili, 1
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: IT–25100 Brescia (Italy)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/dem Tel. +39 03 0399 5632, Fax +39 03 0399 5027, E-Mail bborroni@inwind.it
tive lifestyle provide reserve capacity against the effects of plete mental status evaluation by 2 independent and experienced
aging and disease on brain function, decreasing the risk reviewers (B.B., C.A.). Only patients with full consensus agree-
ment by the reviewers and followed-up for at least 1 year from the
of incident dementia [1, 3]. This is in line with studies diagnosis were enrolled.
showing that up to 25% of individuals who fulfill the neu- Demographic characteristics, years of education, occupation
ropathological criteria for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) at au- and leisure activities were carefully recorded. Years of education
topsy remain cognitively spared during life. Accordingly, were defined as the number of completed years of formal educa-
the finding that higher education and occupational at- tion, including university or apprenticeship (only when formal
education was associated). Occupational attainment was rated ac-
tainments can somehow counterbalance AD pathology cording to the general project protocol, as previously published
has been widely demonstrated by neuroimaging studies. [9], with a score ranging from 0 to 4, corresponding to the last
These works have shown an inverse relationship between employment of each subject: (0) no occupation; (1) unskilled la-
the level of education and occupation and in vivo proxies borer, housewife; (2) skilled laborer, tradesman, lower-level civil
of AD pathological process, i.e. regional metabolic rate of servant, employee, self-employed small business, office or sales
person; (3) mid-level civil servant or management, head of a small
glucose utilization or regional blood flow (rCBF), after business, academician or specialist in a subordinate position; (4)
adjusting for dementia severity [5–9]. Higher educational senior civil servant or management, senior academic position,
and occupational attainment was related to greater hypo- self-employed with high degree of responsibility.
metabolism in the temporo-parietal lobes and precuneus, Leisure activities during midlife were also assessed (by A.A.),
brain areas specifically affected by the disease. More re- as previously proposed by Scarmeas et al. [15]. A participation in
13 activities was carefully recorded: (1) knitting or music or other
cently, the brain reserve hypothesis was also supported in hobby; (2) walking for pleasure or excursion; (3) visiting friends
pre-clinical AD, higher education and occupation level or relatives; (4) being visited by relatives or friends; (5) physical
delaying the clinical expression of dementia [9]. conditioning; (6) going to movies or restaurants or sporting
It has been demonstrated that educational levels are events; (7) reading magazines or newspapers or books; (8) watch-
higher in frontotemporal dementia (FTD) as compared ing television or listening to the radio; (9) doing unpaid commu-
nity volunteer work; (10) playing cards or games; (11) going to a
to AD [10]; no other previous work but Perneczky et al. club or center; (12) going to classes; (13) going to church or syna-
[11] have explored the brain reserve correlates in a small gogue or temple. One point was given for participation to each of
sample of patients with FTD. the above activities and an aggregate score was assigned to each
FTD is the second commonest cause of young-onset patient for the subsequent analysis (range 0–13).
dementia and no treatments affecting disease progression All participants were made fully aware of the research goals,
and the signature of an informed consent was required from all
are available yet [12]. Furthermore, no protective factors subjects. The work was conducted in accordance with Brescia
which might delay disease onset are currently known. Hospital clinical research regulations and in conformity with the
With these caveats in mind, we tested the brain reserve Declaration of Helsinki.
hypothesis in FTD patients by evaluating how the 3 proxy
measures of brain reserve (education, occupation, and Exclusion Criteria
Stringent exclusion criteria were applied as follows: (a) cere-
midlife leisure activities) might influence the clinical ex- brovascular disorders, previous stroke, hydrocephalus, and intra-
pression (as measured by neuropsychological and behav- cranial mass documented by MRI; (b) a history of traumatic brain
ioral assessments) of brain pathology (as reflected by injury or another neurological disease; (c) another neurodegen-
rCBF changes). erative syndrome associated with dementia or belonging to the
frontotemporal lobar degeneration spectrum (e.g. semantic de-
mentia, progressive nonfluent aphasia, corticobasal degeneration
Methods syndrome, frontotemporal dementia with motor neuron disease)
according to current clinical criteria; (d) monogenic causes of
Study Sample FTD (e.g. microtubule-associated protein tau or progranulin mu-
A total of 54 patients, fulfilling the clinical picture of behav- tations); (e) significant medical problems (e.g. poorly controlled
ioral variant FTD (bvFTD) according to current clinical criteria diabetes or hypertension; cancer within the past 5 years; clini-
[13, 14] and undergoing brain functional imaging study, were cally significant hepatic, renal, cardiac, or pulmonary disorders);
consecutively enrolled at the Centre for Aging Brain and Neuro- (f) major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia,
degenerative Disorders, Department of Neurology, University of substance abuse disorder, or mental retardation according to cri-
Brescia, Italy, from January 2001 to December 2007. teria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
All subjects underwent a somatic and neurological evaluation, 4th edition [DSM-IV reference].
and routine laboratory examination, a brain structural imaging
study, and a brain functional single photon emission tomography Cognitive, Behavioral and Functional Assessment at
(SPECT) study. Enrolment
The diagnostic assessment involved a review of full medical At first evaluation, global cognitive function assessment was
history, a semi-structured neurological examination, and a com- made according to a standardized battery, including the Mini-

Education and Occupation in FTD Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 131
Mental State Examination [16]. The neuropsychological assess- Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the included
ment was made with tests tapping the different cognitive do- patients according to gender
mains. The Instrumental Activities of Daily Living [17] and Basic
Activities of Daily Living [18] were assessed as well. Behavioral Variable bvFTD bvFTD bvFTD
and psychiatric disturbances were evaluated by the Neuropsychi- overall female male
atry Inventory [19] and the Frontal Behavioral Inventory [20]. To
assess dementia severity, the newly introduced FTD-modified Patients (%) 54 (100) 32 (59.3) 22 (40.7)
Clinical Dementia Rating (FTD-modified CDR) was computed, Age, years 65.487.4 65.388.1 66.086.6
as it includes executive dysfunction and behavioral disturbance Age at onset, years 63.587.6 63.288.7 64.086.0
evaluation [21]. FTD-modified CDR score was calculated by sum- FTD-modified CDR 4.784.0 4.884.1 4.783.9
ming the individual box scores [21]. MMSE 22.285.1 22.484.3 21.886.2
BADL lost 0.4181.0 0.2880.8 0.6081.1
Image Acquisition, Pre-Processing and Analysis IADL lost 1.482.0 1.782.2 1.181.6
Patients with bvFTD were administered an intravenous injec- FBI 16.3811.9 15.6811.7 17.3812.5
tion of 1110 MBq 99mTc-ethylcysteinate dimer (ECD) (Neurolite, NPI 16.4812.7 16.6811.3 16.3813.8
Bristol-Myers Squibb Pharma, Boston, Mass., USA) in a rest con-
dition, and were imaged using a dual-head rotating gamma cam- MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; BADL = Basic Ac-
era (VG MILLENIUM GE) fitted with a low-energy, high-reso- tivities of Daily Living; IADL = Instrumental Activities of Daily
lution collimator, 30 min after intravenous injection of 99mTc- Living; FBI = Frontal Behavioral Inventory; NPI = Neuropsychi-
ECD, as previously described [22]. atric Inventory.
Statistical parametric mapping (SPM2, Welcome Department
of Cognitive Neurology, University College, London), and Matlab
6.1 (Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, Mass., USA) were used for image
pre-processing [23]. Images were spatially normalized to a refer-
ence stereotactic template (Montreal Neurological Institute, Can- The overall group showed a mild disease severity, as
ada), and smoothed by a Gaussian kernel of 8 ! 8 ! 8 mm full- measured by the FTD-modified CDR [4.7 8 3.9 (range
width at half maximum. SPECT data analysis was performed in
blind to clinical data. 0.5–19)].
We conducted a regression analysis to evaluate the role
Statistical Analysis of education, occupation and leisure activities on brain
We calculated 3 multiple linear regression analyses across all reserve. Education and occupation scores were signifi-
subjects group, considering years of education, level of occupation, cantly related; the higher the education levels, the higher
or leisure activities as independent variables, and rCBF as the de-
pendent variable. In the analysis design, we also included 3 covari- the occupational attainment (Spearman’s ␳ = 0.493, p =
ates of no interest: dementia severity as measured by FTD-modi- 0.001).
fied CDR and demographic variables, i.e. age and gender. This sta- As shown in table 2, the correlation analysis showed a
tistical design allowed us to test for a correlation between education, significant relationship between higher education and
occupation, or leisure activities and rCBF, after excluding the ef- lower rCBF in the medial frontal cortex and in the dor-
fect of all covariates of no interest on brain perfusion variance.
Findings meeting a threshold of p ! 0.005, uncorrected, were con- solateral frontal cortex, bilaterally (see fig. 1a).
sidered significant. The extension threshold was set at 50 voxels. A significant relationship was also present between
In order to evaluate the combined effect of more than 1 reserve higher occupation attainment and lower rCBF in the me-
proxies on rCBF, we created indicator variables [reserve indexes dial frontal cortex and in the left dorsolateral frontal cor-
(RI)], as the pairwise sum of the two-rank transformation of all tex (table 2, fig. 1b). As the evaluation of occupation on
significant variables, in order to give equal weight to each contri-
bution (SPSS 16.0 software package for Windows, SPSS Inc., Chi- brain reserve might be affected by a floor effect, we re-run
cago, Ill., USA). the analyses using the dichotomous variable for occupa-
tion (subgrouping FTD patients according to the median
value). This approach allowed us to bypass the problem of
Results the linearity and minimize the effect of the skewed distri-
bution of occupation scores. Using dichotomous occupa-
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the in- tion as an independent variable (and adjusting for age, gen-
cluded bvFTD patients who underwent SPECT scan (n = der, and FTD-modified CDR), the results obtained were
54) are shown in table 1. comparable to those with the 5-point occupation scale.
The mean educational level in bvFTD patients was 6.4 When midlife leisure activities were considered in the
8 2.8 (range 4–19), the mean occupational level was 1.46 linear regression analysis, no correlation was found.
8 0.77 (range 1–4), and the mean leisure activity score We tested also the effect of a higher RI, accounting for
was 5.4 8 2.3 (range 1–10). both education and occupational level. We did not con-

132 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 Borroni et al.


Table 2. Results of the linear regression analysis between regional brain perfusion and education, occupation,
or RI in bvFTD, indicating the areas where higher reserve proxies correlate with lower brain perfusion

Region x y z T p Cluster size

Education
Medial frontal cortex 4 48 54 4.57 <0.001 913
20 26 –10 3.08 0.002 347
R dorsolateral frontal cortex 32 44 22 3.35 0.001 1,050
L dorsolateral frontal cortex –30 72 0 3.88 <0.001 647
Occupation
Medial frontal cortex 4 48 56 4.22 <0.001 391
–6 6 –12 3.61 <0.001 1,448
Anterior cingulate cortex –11 37 8 3.04 0.002 397
L dorsolateral frontal cortex –30 –4 24 3.79 <0.001 390
RI
Medial frontal cortex 6 48 56 5.08 <0.001 1,224
2 76 –4 4.79 <0.001 1,666
R dorsolateral frontal cortex 18 –4 30 3.52 <0.001 2,110
L dorsolateral frontal cortex –28 66 26 4.15 <0.001 574
R medial temporal cortex 46 0 –30 3.34 <0.001 1,187

Talairach coordinates of significant voxels, at threshold p < 0.005, are shown.


R = Right hemisphere; L = left hemisphere.

sider leisure activities in computing RI, because they did When the midlife leisure activities were considered,
not significantly correlate with rCBF. they were not found to affect rCBF.
We found a significant association between higher RI These results suggest that in FTD, environmental fac-
and lower regional brain perfusion in the same brain re- tors may have an influence on mechanisms by which pa-
gions identified by education and occupation separately thology is translated into clinical symptoms, and that ed-
(table 2, fig. 1c). ucation and occupation may help to delay the onset of
To test whether reserve variables are affected by brain dementia symptoms, by allowing to use compensatory
pathology, we subgrouped patients according to the FTD- networks that enable to cope better with brain damage.
modified CDR scores (median values, low vs. high FTD- Overall present data are consistent with the brain re-
modified CDR). The association between education, oc- serve hypothesis, which implies that people with more
cupation, and RI and lower regional brain perfusion was years of schooling and higher occupational attainment
greater in patients with lower FTD-modified CDR scores have greater reserve capacity, and that greater pathologi-
compared to higher scores, presumably a floor effect: as cal changes are needed for dementia to be manifest [1, 2].
the neuropathological process progresses, the effect of The protective effect may reflect processing mechanisms
proxies decreases (data not shown). learned as a byproduct of educational and occupational
experiences or may serve as proxies of innate differences
to use brain networks [1].
Discussion The brain reserve hypothesis has been largely explored
in AD, providing a wide body of evidence and demon-
In the present study, we found an inverse association strating that cognitive reserve takes part in the same brain
between educational and occupational attainments and regions where AD pathology is most often found [3–8].
the rCBF in medial and dorsolateral frontal cortex in Recently, a brain [18F]-fluorodeoxyglucose positron emis-
FTD patients; a higher RI, accounting for both education sion tomography study performed on a large sample of
and occupation levels, was associated to a broader pattern 242 patients with AD, 72 with mild cognitive impairment,
of rCBF found in those brain regions, characterized by and 144 healthy controls, further confirmed that educa-
the disease pathology. tion and occupation may be proxies for brain functional

Education and Occupation in FTD Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 133
Color version available online
R

Fig. 1. Brain hypoperfusion and correlation with proxies for re- perimposed on a standard MRI template. c The statistical para-
serve in bvFTD. a The statistical parametric map of the voxels metric map of the voxels showing a correlation between brain
showing a correlation between rCBF and education, and the 3D perfusion and RI, and the 3D Amira visualization of significant
Amira visualization of significant clusters superimposed on a clusters superimposed on a standard MRI template. In bvFTD,
standard MRI template (Amira, Mercury Computer System; higher reserve proxies (a–c, education and/or occupation) corre-
Chelmsford, Mass., USA). b The statistical parametric map of the late with lower rCBF in medial frontal cortex and dorsolateral
voxels showing a correlation between brain perfusion and occu- frontal cortex, bilaterally (see text for details). Threshold set at
pation, and the 3D Amira visualization of significant clusters su- p ! 0.005. R indicates right side in the figure.

reserve, reducing the severity and delaying the clinical ex- is exerted by the 2 proxies of reserve as measured by RI,
pression of AD pathology, and that functional reserve is acting on the brain substrates specifically involved in the
already at play in the predementia phase of AD [9]. disease. In bvFTD patients, higher occupational attain-
Educational level was found to play a different role in ment primarily affects the mesial frontal cortex and the
FTD and AD, with the role of educational level being sig- anterior cingulate cortex, whilst the protective effect of
nificantly higher in FTD as compared to AD [10]. Addi- education accounts for a recruitment of the dorsolateral
tionally, in this functional neuroimaging study we dem- frontal cortex, bilaterally. This latter finding is in agree-
onstrated that in FTD patients, a similar protective factor ment with the observation by Perneczky et al. [11] that

134 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 Borroni et al.


found a negative association between years of schooling flected in amount of educational and occupational at-
and bilateral inferior frontal cortex in 29 FTD patients; tainment, can withstand a greater amount of brain dam-
further studies are needed to single out whether these dif- age without delaying the dementia onset. Thus, it might
ferences are specific. be argued that in FTD, which has been largely considered
Some limitations should be acknowledged. Our vari- a genetic-based disease, environmental modifiable fac-
ables fail to capture all dimensions of mental stimulation; tors might play a crucial role in affecting disease course,
however, a larger sample group and a more detailed eval- and this could represent a target for interventions in pub-
uation of physical and leisure activities should be further lic health politics.
analyzed to confirm our negative results. Indeed, we con-
sidered leisure activities during midlife, but without con-
sidering the time and the degree of exposure, whilst oc- Acknowledgements
cupation and education are more easily measured. Final-
The authors wish to thank patients and their families for par-
ly, the differences in brain reserve in FTD individuals
ticipating in the study. This work was supported by a grant of the
could also result from genetic background or from health- Centro Universitario Disturbi del Comportamento e Malattie
ier lifestyle associated with higher education. Neurodegenerative, Ente Universitario Lombardia Orientale.
No protective factors are known in FTD. We claimed
that bvFTD patients with a greater brain reserve, as re-

References
1 Stern Y: Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 2008; 63: 16 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini-
disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2006; 112–118. Mental State’: a practical method for grading
20:112–117. 9 Garibotto V, Borroni B, Kalbe E, Herholz K, the cognitive state of patients for the clini-
2 Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP, Morris JC: Edu- Salmon E Holtoff V, Sorbi S, Cappa SF, Pado- cian. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198.
cation and Alzheimer disease without de- vani A, Fazio F, Perani D: Education and oc- 17 Lawton MP, Broody EM: Assessment of old-
mentia: support for the cognitive reserve hy- cupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI con- er people: self-maintaining and instrumen-
pothesis. Neurology 2007;68:223–228. verters and AD: FDG-PET evidence. Neu- tal activities of daily living. Gerontologist
3 Fratiglioni L, Wang HX: Brain reserve hy- rology 2008; 71:1342–139. 1969;9:179–186.
pothesis in dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2007; 10 Borroni B, Alberici A, Agosti C, Premi E, Pa- 18 Sheikh K, Smith DS, Meade TW, Goldenberg
12:11–22. dovani A: Education plays a different role in E, Brennan PJ, Kinsella G: Repeatability and
4 Price JL, Morris JC: Tangles and plaques in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s validity of a modified activities of daily liv-
nondemented aging and pre-clinical Alz- disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 23: ing (ADL) index in studies of chronic dis-
heimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 1999; 45: 358– 796–800. ability. Int Rehabil Med 1979;1:51–58.
368. 11 Perneczky R, Diehl-Schmid J, Drzezga A, 19 Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-
5 Alexander GE, Furey ML, Grady CL, Pietrini Kurz A: Brain reserve capacity in frontotem- Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J: The
P, Brady DR, Mentis MJ, Schapiro MB: As- poral dementia: a voxel-based 18F-FDG PET Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive
sociation of premorbid intellectual function study. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2007;34: assessment of psychopathology in dementia.
with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer’s 1082–1087. Neurology 1994;44:2308–2314.
disease: implications for the cognitive re- 12 Vossel KA, Miller BL: New approaches to the 20 Kertesz A, Nadkarni N, Davidson W, Thom-
serve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154: treatment of frontotemporal lobar degenera- as AW: The Frontal Behavioral Inventory in
165–172. tion. Curr Opin Neurol 2008;21:708–716. the differential diagnosis of frontotemporal
6 Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Hilton 13 Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, Passant dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6:
J, Flynn J, Van Heertum RL, Sackeim HA, U, Stuss D, Black S, Freedman M, Kertesz A, 460–468.
Stern Y: Association of life activities with ce- Robert PH, Albert M, Boone K, Miller BL, 21 Knopman DS, Kramer JH, Boeve BF, Caselli
rebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease: im- Cummings J, Benson DF: Frontotemporal RJ, Graff-Radford NR, Mendez MF, Miller
plications for the cognitive reserve hypoth- lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical BL, Marcaldo N: Development of methodol-
esis. Arch Neurol 2003;60:359–365. diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998; 51: ogy for conducting clinical trials in fronto-
7 Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Diehl-Schmid J, 1546–1554. temporal lobar degeneration. Brain 2008;
Schmid G, Wohlschlager A, Kars S, Grimmer 14 McKhann GM, Albert MS, Grossman M, 131(Pt 11):2957–2968.
T, Wagenpfeil S, Monsch A, Kurtz A: School- Miller B, Dickson D, Trojanowski JQ, Work 22 Borroni B, Grassi M, Agosti C, Paghera B,
ing mediates brain reserve in Alzheimer’s Group on Frontotemporal Dementia and Alberici A, Di Luca M, Perani D, Padovani A:
disease: findings of fluoro-deoxy-glucose- Pick’s Disease: Clinical and pathological di- Latent profile analysis in frontotemporal lo-
positron emission tomography. J Neurol agnosis of frontotemporal dementia: report bar degeneration and related disorders: clin-
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:1060–1063. of the Work Group on Frontotemporal De- ical presentation and SPECT functional cor-
8 Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Karrasch M, mentia and Pick’s Disease. Arch Neurol relates. BMC Neurol 2007;7:9.
Nagren K, Savisto N, Oikonen V, Viitanen 2001;58:1803–1809. 23 Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline
M, Parkkola R, Rinnie JO: Cognitive reserve 15 Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, JB, Frith CD, Frackowiack RSJ: Analysis of
hypothesis: Pittsburgh Compound B and Stern Y: Influence of leisure activity on the fMRI time-series revisited. Neuroimage
fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission to- incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1995;2:45–53.
mography in relation to education in mild 2001;57:2236–2242.

Education and Occupation in FTD Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009;28:130–135 135

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen