Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

School Psychology Review,

2007, Volume 36, No. 4, pp. 665-673

RESEARCH BRIEF

An Intervention to Promote Social Emotional School


Readiness in Foster Children: Preliminary Outcomes
From a Pilot Study

Katherine C. Pears, Philip A. Fisher, and Kimherly D. Bronz


Oregon Social Leaming Center

Abstract. Foster children are at great risk for poor school outcomes. Given that
school readiness is a powerful predictor of later school success, the promotion of
school readiness skills in foster children is an opportunity for preventive inter-
vention. Results are presented from a preliminary evaluation of a program
designed to improve school readiness in foster children. Twenty-four foster
children were randomly assigned to the intervention or comparison conditions.
The intervention consisted of therapeutic playgroups (twice weekly for 7 weeks
during the summer) focusing on social competence and self-regulation skills.
Attendance rates for the playgroups are reported. In addition, group differences on
data collected before and after the intervention are reported. Intervention group
children exhibited increased social competence and self-regulation. Comparison
group children exhibited poorer performance in these domains over time. Results
are discussed in terms of how the study has informed a current randomized
efficacy trial of a school-readiness intervention.

Children who lack the social and aca- in special education, school dropout, and dis-
demic skills to succeed in the early elementary cipline problems, as well as exhibiting poorer
grades are at risk for trajectories of increasing academic skills than their non-foster care
academic failure and behavior problems peers (Zetlin & Weinberg, 2004). This study
across their school years (Entwisle & Alex- describes the process by which a small-scale
ander, 1999). Foster children often fare poorly pilot evaluation of an intervention designed to
in school, showing higher rates of placement improve school outcomes in foster children

Support for this research was provided by the following grants: ROI DA021424 from the National Institute
on Dnig Abuse, U.S. Public Health Service (PHS); ROI MH059780 from the National Institute of Mental
Health, U.S. PHS; and P30 MH046690 from the National Institute of Mental Health and the Office of
Research on Minority Health, U.S. PHS.
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Katherine C. Pears, Oregon Social Leaming
Center, 10 Shelton McMurphey Boulevard, Eugene, OR 97401-4928; E-mail: katherinep@oslc.org
Copyright 2007 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015
665
School Psychology Review, 2007, Volume 36, No. 4

was used to examine preliminary outcomes The intervention in the current study
and to inform further development for a large- was specifically developed to increase foster
scale efficacy trial. children's self-regulation and social compe-
Researchers and policy makers often tence in school by teaching regulatory and
stress the necessity of social emotional compe- social skills in a classroom-based format in
tence (including self-regulation and social com- which the children were given multiple ex-
petence skills) for success in the transition to plicit opportunities to practice skills with a
school and in the early school years because group of unfamiliar peers. Before wide-scale
deficits in these processes are linked to poorer use, interventions should be subjected to em-
school performance (Raver, 2002). Foster chil- pirical validation, the gold standard of which
dren who have been maltreated show deficits in is the large-scale, randomized efficacy trial.
neurobiological self-regulatory systems (Fisher, However, given the vulnerabilities of foster
Gunnar, Dozier, Bruce, & Pears, 2006; Lewis, children and the complexity of working with
Dozier, Ackerman, & Sepulveda, in press). Fur-
the multiple agencies and individuals involved
ther, they show poorer social competence than
in each child's care (e.g., child welfare ser-
their peers who have not been maltreated
vices, foster parents, and biological families),
(Rogosch, Cicchetti, & Aber, 1995), perhaps in
before embarking on a large-scale efficacy
part because of connections observed in the gen-
trial, it is desirable to first pilot test a new
eral population between poor self-regulation and
intervention on a small sample. This can pro-
tendencies to react more negatively toward peers
vide evidence of feasibility. When working
(Gunnar, Tout, de Haan, Pierce, & Stansbury,
within public sector agencies such as the child
1997). Deficits in self-regulation and social com-
welfare system, establishing feasibility re-
petence might put foster children at particular
quires gaining the participation of a number of
risk when entering kindergarten for several rea-
sons. First, they are likely to have difficulty organizational and individual entities. The
inhibiting impulses that are disruptive to class- child welfare agency must agree to participate
room functioning, such as moving from their and allow randomization of the children into
seats or talking out of tum. Second, deficits in groups, the individual caseworkers must allow
self-regulation and social skills are likely to lead the foster families and children to be con-
to rejection by peers. Finally, difficulty in regu- tacted, and the foster families must agree to
lating behavior might impede abilities to concen- participate and attend the intervention with
trate and leam new academic skills. sufficient frequency. Pilot studies also allow
for the examination of preliminary out-
Given that foster children show particu- comes. Results from such pilot studies can
lar vulnerabilities in social skills and self- be used as a formative step in developing
regulation, intervention strategies designed to interventions for large-scale, randomized ef-
enhance functioning in these areas might help ficacy trials.
these children to succeed in the early school
years and might defiect them from trajectories The current study featured groups of foster
toward school failure, dropout, and related children randomly assigned to participate in the
psychosocial problems. A number of empiri- intervention (a twice-weekly therapeutic play-
cally validated programs to enhance children's group described in the Methods section, under
social skills exist. However, these programs Intervention and Comparison Conditions) or a
are generally targeted at children who are in "services as usual" comparison condition. The
first grade and above, tend not to include a goals of the study were to test the feasibility of
specific focus on self-regulatory skills (Web- the intervention by determining if the agency,
ster-Stratton & Taylor, 2001), and have not caseworkers, and foster families would agree to
been empirically validated with foster care participate, and by examining attendance rates,
populations (Pears, Fisher, Heywood, & to obtain preliminary results of intervention ef-
Bronz, 2007). fects on social competence and self-regulation.
666
Promoting School Readiness in Foster Children

Methods demographic variables listed in this section.


Because of subject attrition and missing data,
Participants complete data were available for 10 IG chil-
dren and 10 CG children. The institutional
All foster children in Lane County, Or-
review board of our research center approved
egon, entering kindergarten through second
all procedures.
grade in the fall of 2002 were eligible to
participate. Through a series of meetings with
key agency personnel, the study was intro-
Intervention and Comparison
duced, and permission was gained to recruit
Conditions
foster children and to randomize the children IG children attended 2-hr therapeutic
to one of the two conditions. Caseworkers playgroups twice weekly for 7 weeks during
were asked to allow project staff to contact the the summer. As noted previously, two compo-
foster parents to explain the study and ask nents of social emotional readiness were tar-
them to participate. If this was permitted and geted by the intervention: social competence
the foster parents agreed to participate in the (including sharing, initiating and maintaining
study, the caseworker and foster parents interactions, cooperating and problem solving
signed consent forms. The 24 foster children with peers, and recognizing emotions) and
whose caregivers consented to participate emotional and behavioral self-regulation (in-
were randomly assigned to the intervention cluding problem solving, managing negative
group (IG; n = 11) or the foster care services emotions, and using work-related skills). The
as usual comparison group (CG; n = 13). structure and content of the playgroups were
Children were assigned to intervention or specifically designed to build self-regulation
comparison groups before contacting case- and social competence skills in the following
workers; thus, the uneven group numbers re- ways: (a) because the classroom environment
flect those who subsequently consented to par- itself represents one source of stimulation
ticipate in the study. Eighty percent of eligible when children enter school (Blair, 2002), the
families consented to participate in the study. intervention employed a peer group context in
There were 6 IG males and 5 CG males. The a classroom setting to desensitize children to
mean age was 6.49 years (SD = 0.86) for IG this potential source of stress and dysregula-
children and 6.61 years (SD = 1.16) for CG tion; (b) the children were taught techniques
children. Children came from predominantly for self-regulation and social skills to increase
European American backgrounds (91% in IG; the resources to access in the school environ-
85% in CG). The children had been in their ment; and (c) the children were given oppor-
current foster placement for an average tunities to practice these skills across activi-
of 10.88 months {SD = 7.59 months) in the IG ties. The children's self-regulation strategies
group and 12.65 months {SD = 8.05 months) were explicitly and frequently reinforced.
in the CG group. Five (46%) of the IG chil-
The curriculum manual for the playgroup
dren and 5 (39%) of the CG children were in
was developed by the authors (and others) and
nonrelative foster care (vs. kinship foster
outlined the activities for each of the playgroup
care). Two of the IG children (18%) and 3 of
sessions. The basic routine included a welcom-
the CG children (23%) had received special
ing activity, a craft project, a snack, two circle
education services. Three IG children (27%)
times, projects, and group games. Each session
but none of the CG children had repeated or
focused on a single social skill (e.g., sharing),
were going to repeat a grade; this was a sig-
and skills were taught using instructional tech-
nificant group difference [x^(l) = 4.05, p <
niques that included preteaching, modeling, op-
.05]. Although controlling for the group dif-
portunities to practice skills, and immediate pos-
ference in grade repeaters would have been
itive reinforcement. Skills were introduced and
ideal, the small sample size precluded this.
modeled during circle time, and opportunities to
There were no other group differences on the
practice skills were embedded within subsequent
667
School Psychology Review, 2007, Volume 36, No. 4

classroom activities. Specific social skills in- The ratings indicated high consistency, with
cluded in the curriculum were sharing, initiating every checklist indicating that all elements had
and maintaining interactions, cooperating, prob- been observed.
lem solving, and recognizing emotions. A small CG children received foster care ser-
student-to-staff ratio (3:1) made it possible for vices as usual from the child welfare agency,
teachers to shape the children's skills and to which sometimes included early childhood
reward the children when they were successful. special education services. They did not attend
In addition to social skills, the children playgroups.
leamed techniques for self-regulation, which
were embedded within the routines and activ- Measures
ities of the playgroups. As transition times in
classrooms can be particularly difficult for Foster parents completed questionnaires
children with poor self-regulatory skills, chil- within 2 weeks before (preintervention) and 2
dren were taught to apply skills during transi- weeks after (postintervention) the interven-
tions and were given many additional oppor- tion. At both preintervention and postinterven-
tunities to practice. At circle times, the skills tion, laboratory assessors (who were blind to
necessary for self-regulation within a class- group assignment) administered tasks to mea-
room (e.g., paying attention and listening) sure neuropsychological functioning (not dis-
were explicitly taught, modeled, and rein- cussed here) and completed ratings of the
forced. Further, children were coached in the child's behavior. All postintervention assess-
use of techniques for regulating their emotions ments were completed before the start of the
during potentially difficult times, such as los- school year. One month after the start of
ing a game or not being invited to play with
school, the children's elementary school
peers. Children received high rates of positive
teachers (who were blind to group assignment)
verbal reinforcement for successfully manag-
completed questionnaires about the children's
ing their emotions during frustrating events.
behaviors. CG children were assessed on the
Finally, behavior modification strategies, in-
same timeline.
cluding redirection, limit setting, and differen-
tial reinforcement of other children, were em-
ployed to encourage prosocial behavior. Child Behavior Checklist (Achen-
Playgroup teachers were hired from a bach, 1991a). Foster parents completed this
pool of behavioral specialists who had expe- widely used measure of child competencies to
rience working with foster children and using indicate the frequency with which children
the behavior management techniques em- displayed a variety of problem behaviors.
ployed in the playgroups. They were trained Scores were calculated for the Social Compe-
using a variety of modalities, including curric- tence, Intemalizing Behavior, and Extemaliz-
ulum review, modeling and practice, and re- ing Behavior subscales. Using a sample of 4-
view of videotaped playgroup sessions. Dur- to 11-year-olds, Achenbach reported aver-
ing trainings before the start of the playgroups, age Cronbach's alphas of .56, .90, and .93,
teachers reviewed the entire curriculum, and respectively.
each teacher led two practice playgroup ses-
sions. Teachers received ongoing training and Teacher Report Form (Achenbach,
weekly supervision from a clinical supervisor 1991b). Elementary school teachers com-
during the intervention period. pleted this measure, which parallels the Child
Fidelity of implementation of the play- Behavior Checklist. Scores were calculated
group curriculum was assessed via a checklist for the Social Problems, Intemalizing Behav-
of the essential elements of the playgroup cur- ior, and Extemalizing Behavior subscales. In a
riculum and the behavior management prac- sample of 5- to 11-year-olds, Achenbach re-
tices. On a weekly basis, a trained observer ported average Cronbach's alphas of .86, .91,
indicated whether each element was observed. and .96, respectively.
668
Promoting School Readiness in Foster Children

Emotion Regulation Checklist (Shields sible for bringing them to the playgroups. The
& Cicchetti, 1997). Foster parents, laboratory median percentage of playgroups attended
assessors, and elementary teachers completed was 85, with 73% of children attending at least
this 27-item questionnaire designed to be used 80% of the groups.
by parents, teachers, and observers to measure a
number of aspects central to emotion regulation. Preparation of Data for Group
Laboratory assessors observed the children for Comparisons
11/2-2 hr while they greeted the children and
Because of the small sample size, the
their foster parents, escorted the children to tbe
use of nonparametric tests was considered;
assessment rooms, and worked with the children
however, by using parametric tests, we were
to complete tasks to measure neuropsychologi-
able to retain the continuous nature of the data
cal functioning. Items were answered on a scale
and greater power to detect group differences.
of 1 {Rarely/never) to 4 {Almost always). Two
Examination of skewness and kurtosis re-
subscales were derived from the items: Emotion
vealed that the variable distributions were es-
Regulation and Lability/Negativity. For foster
sentially normal. Exceptions were the differ-
parent ratings, mean Cronbach's standardized
ence score for foster parent ratings of internal-
alphas across assessment periods were .83 for
izing and externalizing and the teacher ratings
lability and .80 for emotion regulation. Because
of social problems, internalizing, and external-
assessors did not observe the children interacting
izing. As recommended by Tabachnick and
with their peers, items pertaining to the child's
Fidell (2001), these variables were log trans-
behaviors with peers were removed. This re-
formed; the resulting variables were normally
sulted in the removal of almost half of the emo-
distributed. For all variables, the assumption
tion regulation items. The standardized alpha for
of equal variances in the two samples was
the remaining items was not acceptable. Thus,
valid. Box plots of the data were examined to
this scale was not computed for assessors. The
locate outliers. Deleting these cases from the
lability scale was retained and consisted of items
sample could have resulted in the loss of
pertaining to the child's ability to make transi-
power. Instead, each case was recoded to the
tions, modulate affect, and accept limits from
next highest value within 2 standard devia-
adults. Two assessors worked with each child at
tions of the mean for the child's group, as per
each assessment period. Assessors' scores were
Tabachnick and Fidell (2001). Preliminary
well correlated (r = .56 preintervention and .48
analyses revealed no group differences on any
postintervention) and were averaged at each time
of the variables before the intervention.
point to produce mean assessor ratings of labil-
ity. The modified assessor scale for lability had a
Foster Parent Report and Assessor
mean Cronbach's standardized alpha across as-
Report Group Comparisons
sessments of .90. For elementary teachers, mean
Cronbach's standardized alphas were .91 for la- Mean scores and standard deviations for
bility and .84 for emotion regulation. the IG and CG children are presented in Table
For the foster parent and assessor mea- 1. Multivariate, two-sample Hotelling's T^
sures, the differences between scores at the tests were used to control for Type I error.
preintervention and postintervention assess- Separate analyses were conducted for the pre-
ment were computed. Because data were col- intervention to postintervention difference
lected only once from elementary teachers, scores on measures rated by foster parents,
difference scores were not computed. and for elementary school teacher data. For
the difference scores, the overall Hotelling's
Results T^ was significant [T^ (6,16) = 39.71, p <
.05]. The univariate t tests revealed a signifi-
Attendance Rates
cant group difference on foster parent ratings
Playgroup attendance logs were kept for of social competence {t = 3.27, p < .05). The
IG children, whose foster parents were respon- IG children showed increased social compe-
669
School Psychology Review, 2007, Volume 36, No. 4

* ^ ^ ^ ^_^ ^_^ ^_^ *_^


fN P o" ^ S" ^
00 oo IT) tN rH iri
O tn (vi o o O
^f ^5 ^2 ^3 ™H f—^
o o c4 o d o
I I I I

a.
2
O

^ ^ y . ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ , - ^

O fo m t~- ^o 00
^ O "! "1 "^ ^
d- C- id 2- 2.2-
lO -H \O (N t n - ^
r-; c^ Tt -H M ro
f^ t~- ON (S en -H

O r- • * -H fS| -H
r-l 00 ^o (N fv) en
^ m wS^ O, O, O,

-H r4 —I o o o
I I I I I u
•a
a
s
O
c o
o V
O
0 , 0 \ ^ 0 0 ^ 0 , 0 _ 0 , - ^ " od p-^ O O 3
aa OH 0 0 O O O O t S O O O > O — H
vorn'^OvcN^q—Hvoirjoq-H
•^wSod^cn-H^NJo^.-^rn

o
as
c
vo u-i ^ ? " ON" o?
rn OS rn >n en - ^
ci- S2- ci S- 2- 2-
o\ r- >o o cs tn
>/^ (S en -H csj ^
t n v£) o> fN t n -H"
"a

&S
g 3o
B c *
" 6V

H H H
670
Promoting School Readiness in Foster Children

tence over time, and the CG children showed higher rate than reported by other (longer)
slightly decreased social competence over summer programs for at-risk children (e.g.,
time. The strength of the association between August, Lee, Bloomquist, Realmuto, & Hekt-
group membership and the difference in social ner, 2003).
competence score was large, d = 1.55. There Results of group comparisons suggest
was also a significant group difference on as- that the intervention group exhibited increased
sessor ratings of differences in lability social competence as rated by their foster par-
(t = 2.42, p < .05). The CG children showed ents. On average, the comparison group exhib-
increased lability and the IG children showed ited slightly decreased social competence. In
slightly decreased lability. The association be- addition, on measures of self-regulation, the
tween group membership and the difference in intervention group showed lower assessor-
assessor rated lability was large, d = 1.15. rated emotional lability over time, whereas
Although there were no significant group the comparison group showed increased labil-
differences on the other scales, the means for ity. Thus, the intervention appeared to help
foster parent-reported lability indicated that IG stabilize children's behavior, whereas the be-
children showed a decrease in lability that was havior of children in the comparison group
greater than three times that shown by CG deteriorated.
children; further, 82% of IG children (vs. 50% In contrast, there were no group differ-
of CG children) showed decreased lability. In ences in teacher-reported school behavior.
addition, IG children showed a greater (al- This may have resulted from the 3-year age
though nonsignificant) decrease over time in span ofthe children (kindergarten through sec-
extemalizing behavior reported by foster ond grade). Because the older children had
parents. been in school before, their pattems of school
behavior might have been more established,
Teacher Report Group Comparisons perhaps making them less responsive to the
intervention. In addition, there were more
No significant group differences on the grade repeaters in the intervention group, sug-
teacher report measures were found (see Table 1). gesting that these children may already have
had histories of greater difficulties in school.
Discussion
Finally, school reputations become established
This small-scale, randomized efficacy very early (Morison & Masten, 1991), and •
trial was designed to test the feasibility of such reputations may have affected teachers'
delivering an intervention to increase social ratings of the older children. This result has
emotional competence in foster children and led us to focus our current efforts solely on
to examine intervention effects on key out- children entering kindergarten.
comes, with the goal of using the infonnation In sum, the results from this study sug-
to inform a larger randomized efficacy trial. In gest some positive effects. Like many pilot
terms of feasibility, it was important to estab- studies, this trial involved a small sample,
lish that key participants would be willing to which may have constrained the power to find
participate. We were able to obtain agency significant group differences. In addition, that
participation, and caseworkers allowed project the first- and second-graders in the study had
staff to contact the foster families to offer already attended school may have obscured
them the chance to be in the study. Of the differences pertaining to kindergarteners, who
eligible families, 80% agreed to participate. It were more likely to benefit from the play-
was also important to establish that foster par- groups' classroom-like setting. It should also
ents would bring children to intervention be noted that the foster parents of children in
groups, given that they must often attend mul- the intervention condition were not blind to
tiple appointments for the children in their the children's participation in an intervention,
care. In the present study, 73% of the children which could have influenced their behavior
attended 80% or more of the playgroups, a ratings. However, the children showed changes
671
School Psychology Review, 2007, Volume 36, No. 4

in emotional lability on ratings by assessors, groups) who enter kindergarten in the fall of
who were blind to group assignment. Finally, their recruitment year.
assessor-reported lability did not include items Past evidence has suggested that, with-
about peer relations. Thus, although differ- out intervention, foster children might fare
ences between groups in assessor ratings of much worse than their peers on school perfor-
lability might have reflected changes in the mance. The current study tested an interven-
children's reactions to working with adults in tion designed to promote positive school out-
tasks like those they might complete at school, comes in foster children. Preliminary results
they were unlikely to reflect changes in mod- suggested positive effects on children's social
ulating affect with peers. competence and self-regulatory skills, and led
As mentioned earlier, one of the primary to several modifications to the intervention. A
purposes of this pilot study was to inform randomized trial of the resulting Kids in Tran-
revisions of the intervention for use in a large- sition to School Program will further examine
scale, randomized efficacy trial. Based on the intervention effects on school readiness and
results of this study, a number of modifica- later school functioning. If the Kids in Tran-
tions were made to the intervention. Because sition to School Program can better prepare
the age span of the children might have con- foster children for kindergarten entry, this
tributed to difficulties in detecting group dif- might have longer term effects on their aca-
ferences in school behavior and because chil- demic and social outcomes as well as their
dren's success in kindergarten might affect overall psychosocial functioning.
their long-range academic trajectories, the re-
vised curriculum focuses exclusively on chil-
dren entering kindergarten. In addition, to sup- References
port the children through the transition to kin- Achenbach, T. M. (1991a). Manual for the Child Behavior
dergarten, the playgroups now continue once Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: Univer-
weekly during the first 8 weeks of school. To sity of Vermont.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991b). Manual for the Teacher's
assist children who might have more trouble Report Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington: University
with the transition to school, the option of of Vermont.
home and school behavioral consultation to August, G. J., Lee, S. S., Bloomquist, M. L., Realmuto,
G. M., & Hektner, J. M. (2003). Dissemination of an
address behavioral difficulties has been added evidence-based prevention innovation for aggressive
to the program. Further, given the increased children living in culturally diverse, urban neighbor-
focus on school readiness, the curriculum has hoods: The Early Risers effectiveness study. Preven-
tion Science, 4, 271-286.
been revised to feature emergent literacy skills Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition
in addition to foci on self-regulation and social and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of
competence. children's functioning at school entry. American Psy-
chologist, 57, 111-127.
Caregiver involvement in school is an Christenson, S. L. (1999) Families and schools. In R. C.
important component of success in kinder- Pianta & M. J. Cox (Eds.), The transition to kinder-
garten and continued academic achievement garten (pp. 143-177). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Entwisle, D. R., & Alexander, K. L. (1999). Early school-
(Christenson, 1999); during the pilot study, ing and social stratification. In R. C. Pianta & M. J.
foster parents commented that they would Cox (Eds.), The transition to kindergarten (pp. 13-38).
have liked more information on supporting Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
their children in school. Thus, foster parent Fisher, P. A., Gunnar, M. R., Dozier, M., Bruce, J., &
Pears, K. C. (2006). Effects of a therapeutic interven-
information groups focused on involvement tion for foster children on behavior problems, caregiver
in emergent literacy and school have been attachment, and stress regulatory neural systems. An-
added to the intervention. The resulting in- nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094,
215-225.
tervention, the Kids in Transition to School Gunnar, M. R., Tout, K., de Haan, M., Pierce, S., &
Program, is currently being tested via a ran- Stansbury, K. (1997). Temperament, social compe-
domized efficacy trial. It involves the re- tence, and adrenocortical activity in preschoolers. De-
velopmental Psychology, 31, 65-85.
cruitment of 200 children over 4 years (ran- Lewis, E., Dozier, M., Ackerman, J., & Sepulveda, S. (in
domized into intervention and comparison press). The effect of caregiving instability on adopted
672
Promoting School Readiness in Foster Children

children's inhibitory control skills. Developmental Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation
Psychology. among school-age children: The development and val-
Morison, P., & Masten, A. S. (1991). Peer reputation in idation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental
middle childhood as a predictor of adaptation in ado- Psychology, 33, 906-916.
lescence: ^L seven-year follow-up. Child Develop- Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate
ment, 62, 991-1007. statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Pears, K. C , I'isher, P. A., Heywood, C. V., & Bronz, Webster-Stratton, C , & Taylor, T. (2001). Nipping early
K. D. (2007). Promoting school readiness in foster risk factors in the bud: Preventing substance abuse,
children. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Con- delinquency, and violence in adolescence through in-
temporary perspectives on social leaming in early terventions targeted at young children ( 0 - 8 years).
childhood education (pp. 173-198). Charlotte, NC: Prevention Science, 2, 165-192.
Infonnation Age. Zetlin, A. G., & Weinberg, L. A. (2004). Understanding
Raver, C. C. (2002). Emotions matter: Making the case for the plight of foster youth and improving their educa-
the role of young children's emotional development for tional opportunities. Child Abuse and Neglect, 28,
early school readiness. Social Policy Report, 160), 917-923.
1-20.
Rogosch, F. A., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1995). The
role of child maltreatment in early deviations in cog- Date Received: February 17, 2006
nitive and Effective processing abilities and later peer
Date Accepted: July 17, 2007
relationship problems. Development and Psychopa-
thology. 7, 591-609. Action Editor: John Hintze

Katherine C. Pears, PhD, is a research scientist at Oregon Social Leaming Center. Her
reseiirch interests include school readiness in high-risk populations and interventions to
improve school readiness outcomes, the development of social cognitive and social-
emotional skills in high-risk preschool children, and the effects of early adversity on child
sociiil and cognitive development.

Philip A. Fisher, PhD, is a research scientist at Oregon Social Leaming Center and a
senior scientist at Center for Research to Practice. His research interests include preven-
tion research in the early years of life, the effects of early stress on the developing brain,
and the plasticity of neural systems in response to environmental interventions.

Kimberly Bronz, PhD, is a school psychologist and clinician at Oregon Social Leaming
Center, Center for Research to Practice, and Oregon Social Leaming Center Community
Programs. She is the clinical director for the Kids in Transition to School Program as well
as tlie Multidimensional Treatment Foster Care for Preschoolers Program.

673

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen