Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Journal http://jcn.sagepub.

com/
of Child Neurology

Use of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination With Children


Penny S. Besson and Elise E. Labbé
J Child Neurol 1997 12: 455
DOI: 10.1177/088307389701200708

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jcn.sagepub.com/content/12/7/455

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Child Neurology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jcn.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jcn.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jcn.sagepub.com/content/12/7/455.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Oct 1, 1997

What is This?

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014


Original Article

Use of the Modified Mini-Mental State


Examination With Children
Penny S. Besson, MS; Elise E. Labbé, PhD

ABSTRACT

The validity and reliability of the modified version of the Mini-Mental State Examination with children was examined. The
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination was administered to 99 children between 4 and 12 years of age (45 males and 54
females) to assess expected scores for nonclinical children and with a clinical sample. Concurrent validity was assessed
through correlations of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores with Wechsler Intelligence scores and Child
Behavior Checklist scores. The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination was administered to 26 children on two occa-
sions to determine test-retest reliability Means and standard deviations of scores are reported by age and grade level.
Test-retest reliability coefficients were positively significant. For the nonclinical sample, Modified Mini-Mental State
Examination scores were significantly and positively correlated with Verbal IQ and Child Behavior Checklist scores.
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores were significantly correlated with Verbal IQ scores in the total and clini-
cal samples. (J Child Neurol 1997;12:455-460).

Examination of cognitive function should be a routine part group and intelligence test scores were known for 32 of the
of neurologic examinations for children presenting with children.
learning disabilities or disturbances in development, think- Results indicated significant correlations between Mini-
ing process, or memory. 1>2 To date there has been only one Mental State Examination scores and age, Full Scale IQ
published study concerning the use of mental status exam- scores, and Mental Age for the sample as a whole. Corre-
inations with children.2 Ouvrier and colleagues (1993)2 con- lations with age were greater for the control group. The
ducted a preliminary study assessing the Mini-Mental State authors reported that Mini-Mental State Examination scores
Examination when used with children. The subjects in the tended to reach a ceiling at approximately 9 years of age.
study were 117 patients ranging in age from 4 to 15 years. This is contradictory to findings in studies with adults, in
All patients were referred to a pediatric neurology clinic, and which the 8th year of education tended to mark a ceiling
the Mini-Mental State Examination was administered at the point.3-5 Ouvrier and colleagues suggested that a cut-off
conclusion of the examination. Numerous changes in admin- score of 27 (on a 35-point scale) be used for children aged
istration and scoring of the Mini-Mental State Examination 10 years and older. However, the authors provided no cri-
were incorporated by the researchers to make the test eas- teria for evaluating the performance of younger children.
ier for younger children. Data from a subgroup of 29 of the Studies of adults have suggested using a cut-off score of 24,
children were separately analyzed to form a &dquo;quasi&dquo; control on a 30-point scale.s
In the present study, the Modified Mini-Mental State
Examination was administered. The inclusion of questions
concerning similarities between two objects or concepts
(verbal abstract reasoning) in the modified version may be
valuable in assessing higher cognitive functions that should
Received Dec 27, 1995. Received revised August 26, 1996. Accepted for pub- be developmentally present in children older than 6 years. 1
lication Sept 3, 1996.
Use of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination also
From the Department of Psychology, University of South Alabama, Mobile,
AL.
allows for a greater range of item difficulty and scores and
Address correspondence to Dr Elise E. Labbé, Department of Psychology, may eliminate the ceiling effect seen in the preliminary
University of South Alabama, Mobile, AL 36688. study. The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination also
provides standardized directions for administration and

455
Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014
456

scoring to ensure comparability between administrations. 2.38). Subjects were referred to the Psychological or Neurological
Standardization may be more important when using such Clinics at the University of South Alabama for psychological or neu-
a test with children because of the great variability of per- rologic testing, or both. Distribution of clinical subjects by med-
formance seen in children. ical or psychological diagnoses is presented in Table 1. Psychological
Although many tests are available to assess cognitive diagnoses were made using the DSM-IV criteria.
functioning in children, these tests usually are quite long and
require special training for administration and interpretation. Materials
Such tests may therefore be difficult to incorporate into rou-
tine pediatric examinations. Should the Modified Mini-Men- Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
tal State Examination prove to be a valid screening measure The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination consists of 15 ques-
of cognitive functioning in children, this test could easily be tionsdesigned to assess the individual’s cognitive functioning.
incorporated into the examination of children referred to The areas theorized to be measured by the test include registration,
a specialty clinic. This study was designed to gather pre-
recall, attention and calculation, temporal orientation, spatial ori-
liminary data on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examina- entation, and language. The test required approximately 15 minutes
tion when used with children. Our second goal was to to administer and was scored on a 100-point scale using standard
evaluate test-retest reliability of the Modified Mini-Mental scoring instructions.’7
State Examination. Finally, we wanted to evaluate the con-
current validity of the Modified Mini-Mental State Exami-
Wechsler Intelligence Scales
nation by comparing intellectual functioning measured
The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised
using Wechsler scales and behavioral functioning measured (WPPSI-R) was administered to 4- and 5-year-old children, and
using the Child Behavior Checklist to scores of the Modi- the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), Third Edition,
fied Mini-Mental State Examination.
was administered to older children. Verbal, Performance, and Full-
Scale IQ scores were obtained for comparison with Modified Mini-
Mental State Examination scores.

METHOD
Child Behavior Checklist
Subjects The Child Behavior CheckliSt8 is a questionnaire designed to

Subjects consisted of 99 children between the ages of 4 and 12 years assess behavior in school,
home, and social environments. Behav-
(mean, 7.48; SD, 2.53). The total sample included 45 boys and 54 ioral profiles obtained provide clinicians and researchers with a
girls. Subjects were divided into nonclinical and clinical samples comprehensive overview of a particular child’s behavior prob-
for separate data analysis. lems. The Behavior Problems scale consists of 118 items scored
from 0 to 2 points, depending on the frequency of the reported
Nonclinical Sample behavior. The parental report form, which required approximately
The subjects in the nonclinical sample consisted of 79 children 20 minutes to complete, was used in the present study. Separate
whose parents volunteered them for participation in the study. forms were provided for boys and girls of different ages. A global
The sample included 36 boys and 43 girls. The mean age of the nor- score based on the Total Behavior Problems index was calcu-

mal sample was 7.08 years (SD, 2.41). Volunteers were recruited lated for comparison with Modified Mini-Mental State Examina-
through local schools, community and media advertisements, and tion scores.

the Well Child program at the University of South Alabama Pedi-


atric Clinic. All subjects were free of neurologic impairment as Procedure
reported by parents and medical history. All tests were administered by a psychologist or trained psychol-
ogy graduate students. All reported testing was completed in one
Clinical Sample session, except in cases of reliability measurement, for which an
The clinical sample consisted of 20 subjects, including 9 boys and additional session was arranged for readministration of the Mod-
11 girls. The mean age for the clinical sample was 9.1 years (SD, ified Mini-Mental State Examination. Standard administration and
scoring procedures were followed for all tests.

Table 1. Diagnoses of Clinical Subjects Nonclinical Sample


Parents or guardians who responded to information about the
study published in local newspapers were contacted by the primary
investigator. The purpose of the study and expectations for par-
ticipation were fully explained, and an appointment for testing was
arranged. Upon arrival for their child’s appointment, parents were
given a consent form to read and sign and were then asked to com-
plete the medical history questionnaire, demographic information
*Six subjects were diagnosed in more than one category. questionnaire, and the Child Behavior Checklist. These children

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014


457

were given an intelligence test along with the Modified Mini-Men- Table 2. Distribution of Modified Mini-Mental
tal State Examination. Order of administration of tests was alter-
State Examination Scores by Age
nated to counter-balance any possible order effects: for half of the
subjects, the intelligence test was administered prior to the admin-
istration of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination, and for
the remaining subjects, the Modified Mini-Mental State Examina-
tion was administered prior to the administration of the intelligence
test. An additional appointment was then made for readministra-
tion of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination.
For children who participated through area schools and day-
care centers; consent forms, medical history and demographic
questionnaires, and a cover letter documenting scheduled testing
dates were sent home with selected students. Administration of the
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination was conducted at the

school after all forms had been returned.


For those children who participated through the Well Child
program at the University of South Alabama, parents were Reliability of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
approached by the primary investigator while they were waiting scores was assessed by calculating Pearson product-moment
to see their pediatrician. The child was then brought to an examina- correlation coefficients between scores obtained on the
tion room so the test could be administered without disturbance. first (Modified Mini-Mental State Examination,) and second
(Modified Mini-Mental State Examinatior~) administrations
Clinical Sample of the test. For all samples, test-retest scores were shown
Parents who brought their children to the Psychological or Neu- to be significantly correlated (nonclinical sample: r .96,=

rological Clinics at the University of South Alabama for testing were P = .000; clinical sample: r .97, P .000; total sample:
= =

asked to allow their child to participate in the study. If permission r = .96, P .000). Reliability of scores was also analyzed by
=

for participation was given, the parent was asked to read and sign retest group, and yielded significant correlations at 1-week,
the consent form and was given the medical history and demo- 2-week, and 4-week retest intervals (1-week retest: r .99, =

graphic questionnaires and the Child Behavior Checklist to com- P .00; 2-week retest: r .97, P .00; 4-week retest: r .85,
= = = =

plete. The subjects in this group were administered the Modified P .O1). As only one subject fell into the 3-week retest cat-
=

Mini-Mental State Examination at the same time as scheduled egory, this interval could not be analyzed.
intelligence testing. When initial testing was completed, some par- Measures of concurrent validity were assessed by ana-
ents were asked to schedule another testing appointment for re- lyzing the relationship between Modified Mini-Mental State
administration of the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination in Examination total scores and other measures of function-
order to obtain information on stability of scores. All testing and
ing. For the total sample, significant correlations were found
diagnosing of clinical subjects was completed by either a psy- between Modified Mini-Mental State Examination total
chologist (Neurological Clinic) or trained psychology graduate scores and Wechsler Verbal IQ scores (r .31, P .04). There
= =

students under the supervision of a licensed psychologist (Psy- were no significant correlations between Modified Mini-

chological Clinic). Subject diagnoses were based on evaluations Mental State Examination scores and Performance IQ, Full
that included complete psycho-educational test batteries, parental Scale IQ, or Child Behavior Checklist Total Behavior Prob-
interviews, and behavioral observations. lems scores for the total sample.
For the nonclinical sample, Modified Mini-Mental State
Examination scores were significantly correlated with both
RESULTS Verbal IQ scores and Child Behavior Checklist Total Behav-
ior Problems scores (Modified Mini-Mental State Exami-
For all samples, scores on the Modified Mini-Mental State nation vs Verbal IQ: r .46, P .01; Modified Mini-Mental
= =

and Mini-Mental State Examinations were significantly cor- State Examination vs Child Behavior Checklist: r = .44, P =

related (for all samples, r = .97, P .00). For the total sam-
=
.03). For the clinical sample, Modified Mini-Mental State
ple, scores on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination Examination scores correlated significantly with Verbal IQ
ranged from 16 to 96 (mean, 66.27; SD, 23.78). Pearson cor- scores (r .55, P .03). No other significant correlations
= =

relation coefficients were shown to be significant between between measures of functioning were found in this sample.
both Modified Mini-Mental State Examination score and To further analyze variability of scores by age and grade
age (r = .81, P = .000) and Modified Mini-Mental State Exam- and determine whether scores on the Modified Mini-Men-
ination score and grade (r = .81, P = .000). Means and stan- tal State Examination reach a ceiling at a certain age or grade
dard deviations of scores at each age level by sample are level, scores were plotted by age and grade using the regres-
presented in Table 2. Separate analysis of scores by gender sion approach and visually examined for a plateau. Figures
were not calculated because statistical analyses revealed a 1 and 2 present plots of Modified Mini-Mental State Exam-
nonsignificant relationship between these variables. ination scores by age and grade for the total sample. These

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014


458

Figure 1. The relationship between age and Modified Mini-Mental Figure 2. The relationship between grade and Modified Mini-Men-
State Examination total scores for the entire sample.The solid line rep- tal State Examination total scores for the entire sample. Grades pre-
resents the linear least square best fit. K through seventh are represented by the numbers 1 through 9,
respectively. The solid line represents the linear least square best fit.

plots suggest that Modified Mini-Mental State Examination


scores reach a plateau at approximately 9 years of age and DISCUSSION
at the sixth grade level. The resulting linear regression equa-
tion for predicting Modified Mini-Mental State Examina- Reliability of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores
tion score based on grade level was y 34.721 + 8.198x, the = was assessed through test-retest correlation coefficients.

equation for age was y 9.534 + 7.581x.


= Test scores for the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination
A 1-test for independent measures was performed to appear to be reliable for both clinical and nonclinical sam-
determine whether Modified Mini-Mental State Examination ples across 1-, 2-, and 4-week retest intervals. The stability
scores obtained by the clinical sample were significantly of these scores over time provides more meaningful inter-
different from scores obtained by the normative sample. pretation of results.
Results indicate no significant difference between these As in previous studies with adults, :1,4 the present study
groups. There was also no significant relationship between found that children’s Modified Mini-Mental State Examina-
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores and diag- tion scores are significantly related to both age and educa-
nostic category. tion. Reported means and standard deviations by age may
Post-hoc analyses indicating possible relationships thus be used as a criterion against which a particular child’s
between Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores and performance on the test can be measured.
demographic variables were also conducted. Significant As in the previous study by Ouvrier and colleagues2
correlations were found between these scores and race, edu- using the Mini-Mental State Examination, the present study
cational level of parents, and family income level for the total found that regression plots suggest that scores on the Mod-
and normal samples. In the clinical sample, Modified Mini- ified Mini-Mental State Examination appear to reach a
Mental State Examination scores were significantly corre- plateau at approximately 9 years of age. However, in both
lated with parent’s educational level, but not with race or the previous and present study the majority of the children
income. Table 3 presents a correlation matrix for these were 9 years of age or younger. This plateau may thus result

variables. from sampling distribution rather than a true ceiling effect,


especially for the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination.
Although Ouvrier and colleagues2 did not report the distri-
bution of scores by age, in the present study no perfect
scores were obtained on the Modified Mini-Mental State

Table 3. Correlations Between Modified Mini-Mental State Examination. This may indicate the possibility of increase
Examination Scores and Demographic Variables of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores with age
if more older children were included in the samples. Regres-
sion equations reported may also be used to measure a par-
ticular child’s performance on the Modified Mini-Mental
State Examination by comparing predicted score at the
child’s age level to obtained score. For example, predicted
score 9.534 + 7.581 (age). Thus, for a 9-year-old child, the
=

predicted score would be 77.76, and if that child scored sig-


MOED = mother’s educational level; FAED = father’s educational level;
nificantly lower than that, there may be cause for concern
Income = family annual income level. and indication for further testing. Determination of which

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014


459

scores differed significantly may be a basis for further status examinations are not designed to be diagnostic instru-
or be
research, may determined using the accepted measure ments, they should not be expected to distinguish between
of scores that are more than 1.5 standard deviations below diagnostic groups.
the mean for the child’s age. Based on the results of previous research with
Validity of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination adultsll-13 indicating that Mini-Mental State Examination
scores was assessed by comparing those scores with other scores were affected by socioeconomic status and eth-
established measures of intellectual and behavioral func- nicity, analysis of the relationship between Modified Mini-
tioning, as well as with clinical diagnoses. Although men- Mental State Examination scores and demographic
tal state examination scores were found to correlate variables was also conducted. Results indicated that these
significantly with both Verbal IQ and Full Scale IQ scores scores were significantly correlated with race, family
in both nonclinical and clinical adults,6>9 the present study income level, and educational level of parents for chil-
found that scores on the Modified Mini-Mental State Exam- dren in the nonclinical sample. These results may con-
ination correlated significantly with only Verbal IQ scores found the validity of mental status examinations as
in children. This may have been a result of the heavy reliance measures of cognitive functioning. However, because the
of the mental status examination on verbal abilities, as test is designed as a screening instrument for identifying
most questions require verbal response and all directions cognitive impairment, possible confounding factors to
are oral and thus rely heavily on verbal comprehension. predictive validity are most important in clinical samples.
Alternately, these results may suggest that mental status The present study found no significant relationship between
examinations for children are measuring something other Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores and either
than intellectual abilities, and therefore will not necessar- race or family income level for children in the clinical

ily correlate highly with Full Scale IQ scores. sample, indicating that scores may have more predictive
Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores were validity among clinical groups. Although Modified Mini-
significantly correlated with Child Behavior Checklist Total Mental State Examination scores were significantly cor-
Behavior Problems T-scores for children in the nonclinical related with parent’s educational level for children in the
sample. These correlations were not found for children in clinical sample, educational level of parents has been
the clinical sample. It is possible that the clinical group shown to affect the intellectual functioning of children, and
was a more homogeneous sample than the nonclinical group would therefore affect scores on any measure of a child’s
and this may have resulted in nonsignificant correlations with intellectual functioning. 14
behavioral tests. Furthermore, behavioral problems may Results of the present study suggest that the Modified
not have surfaced and interfered with the child’s perfor- Mini-Mental State Examination may prove to be a useful
mance, because the Modified Mini-Mental State Examina- screening measure for children, especially in cases in which
tion required the child to attend to the examiner for only a a child’s impairment may preclude or interfere with other

brief period of time. measures of functioning that require sustained attention


Previous studies using the Mini-Mental State Exami- and concentration. This is the first study to provide data on
nation with adult populations have found that the test is able central tendency and variability of scores at each age level
to distinguish between nonclinical and diagnostic groups. 6,10 that may be used as preliminary norms. An individual child’s
However, these findings were not replicated in the present performance on the test can then be compared to this data.
study. Results of an independent group’s t-test revealed no However, the small size and relatively homogeneous nature
significant differences of Modified Mini-Mental State Exam- of the present sample restricts interpretation of results.
ination scores between the nonclinical sample and the clin- Further research is needed before any definitive conclusions
ical sample. However, only 20% of subjects in the present can be drawn and results should be interpreted cautiously.

study comprise the clinical sample. Thus the nonsignifi- Future projects should employ larger and more varied
cant result may be due to the skewed distribution of the sam- samples. This study provides the basis for further research
ple. An examination of Table 3 indicates that mean scores on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination that would
for the clinical sample are lower than mean scores for the add significantly to the available information on the valid-
nonclinical sample at all age levels. Thus, increasing the size ity of this instrument as a screening measure for both adults
of the clinical sample to more closely approximate the size and children.
of the nonclinical sample may yield significant differences
of Modified Mini-Mental State Examination scores between
these two groups.
References
An analysis of scores obtained by the clinical sample 1. Franzen MD, Berg RA: Screening Children for Brain Impairment.
showed no significant relationship between subjects’ diag- New York, Springer Publishing Company, 1989.
noses and scores on the Modified Mini-Mental State Exam- 2. Ouvrier RA, Goldsmith RF, Ouvrier S, Williams IC: The value of
the Mini-Mental State Examination in childhood: A preliminary
ination. This again may be an effect of sampling distribution,
study. J Child Neurol 1993;8:145-148.
because 50% of the clinical sample was diagnosed as men-
3. Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF: Population based
tally retarded and 40% of the clinical sample was diagnosed norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educa-
with attention-deficit disorder. However, because mental tional level. JAMA 1993;269:2386-2391.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014


460

4. Holzer CE, Tischler GL, Leaf PJ, Myers JK: An epidemiological 9. Mitrushina M, Satz P: Reliability and validity of the Mini-Mental
assessment of cognitive impairment in a community population, State Exam in neurologically intact elderly. J Clin Psychol
in Greenley JR (ed): Research in Community and Mental Health. 1991;47:537-543.
Greenwich, CT, Jai Press Inc, 1984, pp 3-32. 10. Taylor MA, Abrams R: Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Am
5. Magaziner J, Bassett SS, Hebel JR: Predicting performance on J Psychiatry 1984;141:196-201.
the Mini-Mental State Examination. J Am Geriatr Soc 1987;35: 11. Cavanaugh SV, Wettstein RM: Emotional and cognitive dysfunc-
996-1000. tion associated with medical disorders. J Psychosom Res 1989;33:
505-514.
6. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: A prac-
12. Escobar JI, Burnham A, Karno M, et al: Mini-Mental State Exam-
tical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clin-
ination (MMSE) in a community population of mixed ethnicity:
ician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.
Cultural and linguistic artifacts. J Nerv Ment Dis 1986;174:607-614.
7. Teng EI, Chui HC: The modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) Exam- 13. O’Connor DW, Pollitt PA, Treasure FP, et al: The influence of edu-
ination. J Clin Psychiatry 1987;48:314-318.
cation, social class, and sex on mini-mental state scores. Psychol
8. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS: The Child Behavior Profile II: Med 1989;19:771-776.
Boys aged 12-16 and girls aged 6-11 and 12-16. J Consult Clin 14. Sattler JM: Assessment of Children: Revised and Updated Third
Psychol 1979;47:223-233. Edition. San Diego, Jerome M. Sattler, 1992.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on November 5, 2014

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen