Sie sind auf Seite 1von 22

PAUL COBB, TERRY WOOD, ERNA YACKEL,

AND MARCELA PERLWITZ

A FOLLOW-UP ASSESSMENT OF A SECOND-GRADE


PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS PROJECT

ABSTRACT. Five second-grade classes in two schools participated in a project that was generally
compatible with a constructivist theory of knowing. At the end of the school year, the students in
these classes and their peers in six non-project classes in the same schools were assigned to ten text-
book-based third-grade classes on the basis of reading scores. The two groups of students were
compared at the end of the third-grade year on a standardized achievement test and on instruments
designed to assess their conceptual development in arithmetic, their personal goals in mathematics,
and their beliefs about reasons for success in mathematics. The levels of computation performance
on familiar textbook tasks were comparable, but former project students had attained more
advanced levels of conceptual understanding. In addition, they held stronger beliefs about the
importance of working hard and being interested in mathematics, and about understanding and col-
laborating. Further, they attributed less importance to conforming to the solution methods of others.

In this paper, we report a follow-up assessment of a second-grade mathematics


project s whose development was guided by a constructivist theory of knowl-
edge. In particular, we compare the arithmetical learning, beliefs, and motiva-
tions of project and non-project students at the end of third grade after both
groups had received a year of traditional, textbook-based instruction in the same
classrooms. During the discussion of the results, we will refer to a previously
reported assessment conducted at the end of the second-grade school year
(Cobb, Wood, Yackel, Nicholls, Wheatley, Trigatti, and Perlwitz, 1991). One
central issue will be the extent to which project students' demonstrated superi-
ority in terms of conceptual understanding and problem-solving in arithmetic
which is upheld after a year of traditional instruction. In addition, an analysis of
students' responses to the beliefs and personal goals will clarify how project
students coped with the contingencies of traditional instruction.

THEORETICAL ORIENTATION

Two years prior to this assessment study, we conducted a year-long classroom


teaching experiment in collaboration with one teacher, in the course of which
we developed a set of instructional activities for all areas of second-grade math-
ematics. In doing so, we were guided by a constructivist view of learning
(Piaget, 1971, 1980; von Glasersfeld, 1990a) and by detailed cognitive models
of young children's mathematical learning (Cobb and Wheatley, 1988; Steffe
and Cobb, 1988; Steffe, von Glasersfeld, Richards, and Cobb, 1983). These

Educational Studies in Mathematics 23: 483-504, 1992.


9 1992 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
484 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

models specify both the conceptual advances that children make and the pro-
cesses by which they might make them. In addition, two types of instructional
settings - - pair collaboration and class discussion - - were developed to facili-
tate the occurrence of learning opportunities during communicative interactions
in the classroom. The rationale for this instructional approach was derived from
analyses of the relationship between social interaction and conceptual develop-
ment in general (Barnes, 1976; Blumer, 1969; Mead, 1934), and in mathematics
in particular (Bartolini Bussi, 1991; Confrey, 1990a; Perret-Clermont, 1980;
Smith and Confrey, 1991; Yackel, Cobb, and Wood, 1991).
In general, the instructional approach is compatible with the constructivist
view that mathematics learning is a process in which students reorganize their
mathematical activity to resolve situations that they find personally problematic
(Confrey, 1986; Thompson, 1985; von Glasersfeld, 1984a). As a consequence,
all instructional activities, including those involving arithmetical computation
and numeration, were designed to be potentially problematic for children at a
variety of different conceptual levels (Cobb, Wood, and Yackel, 1991a). In
addition, the activities reflect the view that what are typically called conceptual
and procedural developments should, ideally, go hand in hand (Cobb, Yackel,
and Wood, 1988; Silver, 1986). For example, a situation in which a student's
current computational procedures prove inadequate can give rise to a problem,
the resolution of which involves a conceptual advance that makes possible the
construction of more sophisticated algorithms.
In this approach, it is students' experiential realities rather than formal mathe-
matics that constitute the starting point for developing instructional activities
(Treffers, 1987). Where possible, research-based models of children's mathe-
matical development were used to guide the development of instructional activi-
ties. These included the models of children's arithmetical learning developed by
Steffe and his colleagues (Steffe and Cobb, 1988; Steffe, von Glasersfeld,
Richards, and Cobb, 1983). However, in some non-arithmetical areas of second-
grade mathematics, where research on children's learning was not as extensive
or detailed, we relied primarily on intuitions derived from our own prior experi-
ences with young children. For example, the only relevant discussion of the
conceptual construction of units of time we identified was a general theoretical
analysis conducted by von Glasersfeld (1984b). In general, the research-based
models were used to anticipate what might be problematic for students at a vari-
ety of conceptual levels and what mathematical constructions they might make
to resolve their problems (Cobb, Wood, and Yackel, 1991a). A description of
the instructional activities, developed to facilitate students' construction of
arithmetical thinking strategies and computational algorithms, can be found in
Cobb, Wood, and Yackel (199 la).
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 485

Social Interaction and Mathematical Learning


Thus far, we have spoken as though problems arise for students only when they
make theft independent, solo interpretations of instructional activities. In doing
so, we have underemphasized the argument that mathematical learning is an
interactive, as well as an individual, constructive activity (Bauersfeld, 1980,
1988; Confrey, 1990a; Sinclair, 1990; Steffe, 1987; Voigt, 1985; Yackel, Cobb,
and Wood, 1991). An emphasis on social interaction brings with it the notion
that mathematical learning is a process of enculturation in which students come
to be able to participate increasingly in the mathematical practices institution-
alized by the wider society (Bishop, 1988; Bruner, 1986; Cobb, Wood, and
Yackel, in press-a; Minick, 1989; Rogoff, 1990; Saxe, 1991). Learning opportu-
nities can then be seen to arise for students as they and the teacher interactively
constitute taken-as-shared 2 mathematical interpretations and understandings.
These taken-as-shared mathematical ways of knowing, themselves the products
of prior interactive negotiations, both make mathematical communication possi-
ble in the classroom and serve to constrain individual students' mathematical
activity (Mehan, 1979; Voigt, 1989). In this characterization, mathematics as an
individual, experientially based constructive activity and as a process of encul-
turation are but complementary sides of the same coin. Students' individual
cognitive constructions both contribute to and are constrained by the classroom
community's negotiation and institutionalization of mathematical meanings and
practices (Cobb, Wood, and Yackel, in press-b).
Three points follow from this discussion on the role of social interaction in
mathematics learning. The first relates to the well-documented finding that
mathematical activity in school is typically a matter of following arbitrary pro-
cedural instructions for many students. As a consequence, one of our goals
when we conducted the classroom teaching experiment was to develop an alter-
native mathematics tradition in the classroom that had the characteristics of
what Richards (1991) called "inquiry mathematics." As Richards noted, the dis-
tinction between inquiry mathematics and traditional school mathematics is
analogous to that between the so-called "logic of discovery" of research mathe-
matics and the "reconstructed logic" of journal mathematics. In contrast to the
co-construction of procedural instructions, an inquiry mathematics tradition is
characterized by the teacher's and students' interactive constitution of truths
about a taken-as-shared mathematical reality (Cobb, Yackel, Wood, and
McNeal, 1992). The development of a mathematics tradition of this type in the
classroom requires that students have frequent opportunities to discuss, critique,
explain, and, when necessary, justify their interpretations and solutions. To
facilitate the occurrence of such opportunities in project classrooms, we encour-
486 P. COBB, T. WOOD, E. YACKEL, AND M. PERLWITZ

aged students to work collaboratively in small groups and to participate in


whole-class discussions of their problems, interpretations, and solutions.
The second point concerns the nature of the teacher's and students' roles as
they do and talk about mathematics. In all instructional settings, there is an
essential power imbalance between the teacher and students (Bishop, 1985). It
is the manner in which the teacher expresses his or her power in action that is
important. It is one thing for the teacher to cue students until they can act as
though they have learned what the teacher had in mind all along (Bauersfeld,
1980; Brousseau, 1984; Voigt, 1985) and another for the teacher to express his
or her authority in action by initiating and guiding the explicit negotiation of
mathematical meanings. Ideally, classroom interactions about mathematics
should be characterized by a genuine commitment to communicate in which the
teacher assumes that students' mathematical actions are reasonable from their
perspectives even if that sense is not immediately apparent to the teacher (von
Glasersfeld, 1990b; Wood, Cobb, and Yackel, 1992). The teacher's role in initi-
ating and guiding the negotiation of mathematical meanings is therefore highly
complex and involves a variety of activities:
9 framing conflicts between alternative interpretations or solutions as prob-
lems to be resolved (Lampert, 1990)
9 helping students develop small-group relationships (Wood and Yackel,
1990)
9 facilitating mathematical dialogue between students (Wood, Cobb, and
Yackel, 1990)
9 implicitly legitimizing selected aspects of students' contributions to a dis-
cussion in light of their potential fruitfulness for further mathematical
learning (Wood, Cobb, and Yackel, 1991)
9 redescribing students' explanations in more sophisticated terms that are
inferred to be comprehensible to students (Newman, Griffin, and Cole,
1989)
9 guiding the establishment of particular ways of representing and symbol-
izing mathematical activity (Cobb, Yackel, and Wood, 1992; Confrey,
1990b; Kaput, 1991)
The third point concerns the development of classroom social norms that
make an inquiry mathematics tradition possible (Cobb, Yackel, and Wood,
1989). For example, the establishment of social norms that enable children to
engage in small-group work without constant monitoring by the teacher is
essential to the success of the collaborative learning approach. Social norms for
small-group work include persisting to solve personally challenging problems,
explaining personal solutions to partners, listening to and trying to make sense
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 487

of a partner's explanations, and attempting to achieve consensus about an


answer, and, ideally, a solution process in situations where a conflict between
interpretations or solutions has become apparent. Social norms for whole-class
discussions include explaining and justifying solutions, trying to make sense of
explanations given by others, indicating agreement, disagreement, and lack of
comprehension, and questioning alternatives in situations where a conflict
between interpretations or solutions has become apparent.
As Mehan (1979) and Voigt (1985) note, social norms are not static prescrip-
tions or rules to be followed but are, instead, continually reconstructed in the
course of the classroom interactions. Thus, teachers would not be effective if
they merely gave their students a list of rules or principles to follow to engage
in inquiry mathematics; indeed no such list could ever be complete. Instead, our
observations indicate that specific incidents are typically framed as paradigm
cases in which teachers and students discuss what their obligations are to each
other and what it means to do mathematics (Cobb, Yackel, and Wood, 1989). In
the process, students come to view mathematics as an activity in which they are
obliged to resolve problematic situations by constructing personally meaningful
solutions that they can explain and justify.

PRIOR RESULTS

We inducted 18 second-grade teachers into the project during the year following
the classroom teaching experiment. At the end of their first year of participation
in the project, an assessment of students' arithmetical learning, beliefs, and
motivations was conducted in the three schools in which there were both project
and non-project classes.

Arithmetical Learning
Both the previously reported second-grade assessment and the third-grade
assessment discussed in this paper focus on students' arithmetical computation
abilities and their related numeration and whole number conceptions. This focus
was chosen because arithmetical computation is of central importance to many
constituencies and because computational proficiency seemed, a priori, to be
one of the most difficult goals to achieve in an instructional approach compati-
ble with constructivist theory.
Two tests of arithmetical competence were administered in the prior second-
grade assessment study. The first was a state-mandated standardized achieve-
ment test (ISTEP) composed entirely of multiple choice items. The mathematics
portion of this test was composed of two subtests, Computation, and Concepts
488 P. COBB, T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

and Applications. The mean grade equivalent scores of the two groups of stu-
dents were almost identical on the Computation subtest (3.50 and 3.51 respec-
tively), but the performance of project students was significantly superior on the
Concepts and Applications subtest (4.54 and 3.73 respectively).
The second arithmetic test, the Project Arithmetic Test, was developed by the
project staff. It was composed of two scales, the Instrumental and Relational
scales. The label "Instrumental" indicates that it is possible to perform well by
using standard computational algorithms without conceptual understanding. In
contrast, items on the Relational scale were designed to assess both students'
conceptual understanding of place value and their computational abilities in
non-textbook formats. The specific items on both scales are described by Cobb,
Wood, Yackel, Nicholls, Wheatley, Trigatti, and Perlwitz (1991). The mean
scores of the project and non-project students were similar for the Instrumental
scale (1.28 and 1.31 respectively, scored out of two), whereas project students'
mean score on the Relational scale was significantly superior to that of non-
project students (1.00 and 0.62 respectively). Taken together, the results for
ISTEP and the Project Arithmetic Test indicate that project students had con-
structed more advanced arithmetical conceptions than had non-project students.

Personal Goals and Beliefs about Mathematics


In the process of attempting to initiate a tradition of doing inquiry mathematics
in their classrooms, the second-grade project teachers guided the renegotiation
of classroom social norms and thereby influenced their students' motivations
and their beliefs about their role, the teacher's role, and the general nature of
mathematical activity. The first aspect of students' views about learning mathe-
matics in school, assessed in the prior study, concerned their motivational orien-
tations or personal goals during mathematics insl~uction. Students responded to
the items on the personal goals questionnaire on a five-point scale under each
question: "Yes, yes, ?, no, No" (i.e., strongly agree, agree, undecided, disagree,
strongly disagree).with the two extreme options being in bolder type. The ques-
tionnaire was composed of the four motivational scales shown in Table I.

TABLE I
Personal Goals Scales
(The stem for all items is "I feel really pleased in math when ...")
Effort M
I solve a problem by working hard. The problems make me think hard.
What the teacher says makes me think.
I keep busy.
I work hard all the time.
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 489

TABLE I (cont'd)
Understand and Collaborate M
S o m e t h i n g I learn makes me want to find out more.
Everyone understands the work.
We help each other figure things out.
Other students understand m y ideas.
Ego M
I know more than others.
I finish before my friends.
I get more answers right than m y friends.
I am the only one who can a n s w e r a question.
Work Avoidance M
It is easy to get the answers right.
I d o n ' t have to work hard.
All the work is easy.
The teacher doesn't ask hard questions.

The personal goals assessed by these scales are:


9 working hard (Effort M)
9 making sense and collaborating (Understand and Collaborate M)
9 being superior to peers (Ego M)
9 not having to do any work (Work Avoidance M)
The results indicated that both project and non-project students were motivated
to work hard and to understand and collaborate. The only significant difference
on this group of scales showed that project students were less motivated to be
superior to their peers (Ego M).
The second set of scales focused on students' beliefs about what one has to
do to succeed during mathematics instruction. The five scales that composed the
Beliefs questionnaire are shown in Table II. Both the belief and motivation
scales were developed in collaboration with John NichoUs by adapting scales
originally designed to assess the personal theories of older students.
TABLE II
Beliefs About the Reasons for Success Scales
(The stem for all items is "Students will do well in math if ...")

Effort B
They work really hard. T h e y always do their best. They like to think about math.
They are interested in learning.
Understand and Colla borate B
They try to explain their ideas to other students.
They try to understand each o t h e r ' s ideas about math.
They try to understand instead of just getting answers to problems.
They try to figure things out.
They d o n ' t give up on really hard problems.
490 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , A N D M. P E R L W I T Z

T A B L E II ( c o n t ' d )
Conform B
They solve the problems the way the teacher shows them and d o n ' t think up their own
ways.
They all solve the problems the same way and d o n ' t think up different ways.
They try to find their own ways of doing problems, a
They like to find different ways to solve problems, a
Competitiveness B
They try to get more things right than the others.
They try to do more work than their friends.
They are smarter than the others.
Extrinsic B
They are just lucky.
Their papers are neat.
They are quiet in class.
a These items were reversed when scored.

As before, students responded to each item on the Beliefs questionnaire using


a five-point scale. These scales assessed students' views about the following re-
asons for success in mathematics:
9 working hard and being interested in mathematics (Effort B)
9 persisting and collaborating to understand (Understand and Collaborate
B)
9 conforming to the solution methods of the teacher or peers rather than
developing one's own solution methods (Conform B)
9 being superior to peers (Competitiveness B)
9 being lucky, neat, or quiet (Extrinsic B)
The results showed that both groups of students believed that success stems
from working hard. This is consistent with their similar motivational orienta-
tions (Effort M). However, project students saw significantly less value in being
superior to their peers (Ego M), a result that is consistent with the finding that
they were less motivated to be better than others (Competitiveness B). In addi-
tion, project students rejected the conjecture that conformity to the teacher's or
peers' solution methods leads to success (Conform B). In contrast, non-project
students tended to agree with this conjecture. Project students also believed
more strongly that the mathematics classroom is a place where success derives
from attempts to understand and to explain one's thinking to others (Understand
and Collaborate B).
Taken together, the responses to these last two scales suggest that what is
meant by doing and understanding mathematics differed in project and non-
project classrooms. This inference is consistent with project students' superior
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 491

performance on the Relational scale. Thus, although both groups of students


were motivated to work hard (Effort M) and to understand (Understand and
Collaborate M), they differed in the nature of the activity in which they engaged
as they strove to understand and to do their best. This conclusion is consistent
with an interactional analysis of mathematical activity in project and non-
project classrooms (Cobb, Yackel, Wood, and McNeal, 1992). These findings
also lead to the speculation that there might have been significant differences
between the expectations of project students and their third-grade teachers at the
beginning of the school year. The analysis of data collected at the end of third
grade will allow us to infer the extent to which project students modified their
beliefs and goals as they attempted to be effective while interacting with their
teachers and non-project classmates.

METHOD
Subjects
The principals of each of the three schools that participated in the prior second-
grade assessment study assigned students heterogeneously to third-grade classes
on the basis of reading scores. The ratios of project to non-project second-grade
classes in the three schools were 5:2, 3:2, and 2:4. The students in the latter two
schools participated in the current study. Consequently, between one third and
two thirds of the students in each of the ten third-grade classes in these two
schools had been in a project second-grade class. Complete data sets from the
third grade were obtained for 79 former project students and 111 former non-
project students.
The third-grade teachers used the Addison-Wesley (1987) third-grade textbook
as the basis for their approximately 45 minutes of mathematics instruction each
day. They were aware that some of their second-grade colleagues participated in
the project, and some had voluntarily attended a half-day workshop conducted by
project staff. No systematic observations were made of the third-grade class-
rooms. There was, however, no indication that any of the ten teachers departed
significantly from their teachers' guide when they taught arithmetical topics.

Instruments and Procedures


Arithmetic Tests. Both the state-mandated achievement test (ISTEP) and a
Project Arithmetic Test were administered. As at the second-grade level, the
mathematics portion of the third-grade ISTEP was composed of two subtests,
Computation and Concepts and Applications. All items on both subtests were
492 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

taken from the California Achievement Test. This test was given to students in
the first week of March by the classroom teachers, who followed a test
administration manual. The students' responses were computer-scored by the
testing agency.
The Instrumental scale of the third-grade Project Test consisted of three two-
digit addition and three two-digit subtraction tasks, all but one of which
involved regrouping. The items of the Relational scale were grouped into ten
subscales as listed below.
1) Sequence: Two tasks that involved extending a number sequence (e.g., "52,
45, 38, " ) .
2) Everyday Language: Three addition and three subtraction tasks posed in
everyday language (e.g., "How many do you have to add to 75 to make
1047").
3) Horizontal Sentences: Four missing addend, missing subtrahend, or missing
minuend tasks posed symbolically as horizontal sentences (e.g., " + 28
= 54, 34 - = 16").

4) Multiplication Picture: Two tasks in which a multiplication word problem


was accompanied by a picture (e.g., A picture of five boxes and three
apples accompanied the task statement, "Each box has 4 apples in it and
there are 3 more apples. How many apples are there altogether?").
5) Multiplication-Division: Four multiplication and four division word prob-
lems (e.g., "Six pizzas are cut into pieces. The same number of pieces are
cut from each pizza. There are 48 pieces in all. How many pieces are there
in each pizza?").
6) Money: One addition and one subtraction story problem that involve
money (e.g., "Maria spends 87 cents. How much change does she get back
from $1.00?").
7) Numeration: Three addition and two missing addend tasks adapted from
interview tasks developed by Steffe and Cobb (1988) that involve visible
and screened collections (e.g., "There are 25 cubes hidden in the box. How
many cubes are there altogether?" The accompanying picture shows a large
box, five stacks of ten cubes, and nine individual cubes).
8) Puzzle: One task in which students were asked to find the sum of the num-
bers on each side of a triangle (see Figure Ia). A second task in which they
were asked to find a missing number so that the sums of the numbers on
each side of a triangle would be the same (see Figure Ib).
9) Less challenging Word Problems: Five addition and subtraction word prob-
lems that exemplify the lowest level of Carpenter and Moser's (1984) clas-
sification scheme.
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 493

(a) (b)
Fig. 1.

10) More Challenging Word Problems: Seven addition and subtraction word
problems that exemplify the higher levels of Carpenter and Moser's (1984)
classification scheme.
The Project Arithmetic Test was given to all ten third-grade classes by a
member of the project staff in the first week in May. Each item was scored as
either correct or incorrect.

Goals and Beliefs Questionnaires. The same questionnaires used by Cobb,


Wood, Yackel, Nicholls, Wheatley, Trigatti, and Perlwitz (1991) were
administered in the first week of May by a member of the project staff. The
items and the scales they c o m p o s e are shown in Tables I and II. The
administration procedure is described by Nicholls, Cobb, Wood, Yackel, and
Patashnick (1990).

D A T A A N A L Y S I S AND R E S U L T S

Arithmetic Tests
The students' achievement on the ISTEP Computation and on the ISTEP Con-
cepts and Applications subtests were compared by running ANOVAs with
instruction as the main effect and grade equivalent scores as the dependent mea-
sure. Differences were not significant on either subtest. The means and standard
deviations are given in Table III.
ANOVAs of the same format were run to compare performance on each scale
of the Project Arithmetic Test with raw scores (scaled out of 10) as the depen-
dent measure. Differences in favor of project students proved to be significant at
the p < .05 level on six of the ten subscales of the Relational scale. In addition,
494 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

differences on the Horizontal Sentence scale were very close to achieving sig-
nificance. The means and standard deviations of the raw scores for both groups
of students are given in Table III.

T A B L E III
Means (and Standard Deviations) for ISTEP and the Project Arithmetic Test
Group F P
Project Non-Project (1,189)
ISTEP a
Computation 4.66(1.21) 4.51(1.27) 0.66 .42
Concepts and Applications 6.17(2.74) 5.67(2.55) 1.69 .19

P r o j e c t Arith metic Test b


Instrumental Scale 8.58(2.02) 8.28(2.08) 0.93 .33
Rational Scale
Sequence 6.70(3.57) 5.95(3.88) 1.81 .18
Everyday Language 5.93(3.22) 4.75(2.88) 7.06 .01 *
Horizontal Sentences 3.93(3.64) 2.98(3.11) 3.58* .06
Multiplication Picture 7.10(3.36) 6.80(3.79) 0.33 .57
Multiplication Division 5.13(3.00) 4.13(3.02) 3.65 .05*
Money 7.25(3.19) 6.10(3.90) 4.72 .03*
Numeration 6.64(2.80) 5.32(3.22) 8.55 .00'
Puzzle 6.70(3.42) 5.55(3.48) 5.11 .03*
Less Challenging Word
Problems 8.28(2.58) 8.28(3.12) 0.00 .99
More Challenging Word
Problems 6.59(2.98) 5.43(2.87) 4.81 .03*
Note: n = 79 for project students and 111 for non-project students.
a Grade-equivalent scores, b Raw scores converted to a scale from 0 to 10.
* p<.05.

Personal Goals and Beliefs about Mathematics


Students' responses to each of the five beliefs scales and four personal goals
scales were compared by using ANOVAs with instruction as the main effect.
The results for the treatment comparisons are shown in Table IV. Higher scores
on the five-point scale indicate agreement and lower scores, disagreement. The
analysis of the reasons-for-success scales indicates that project students valued
working hard and being interested in mathematics (Effort B) and attempting to
understand and collaborate (Understand and Collaborate B) more than non-
project students, but saw less value in conforming to others' solution methods
(Conform B). With regard to the motivational orientation scales, both groups of
students were motivated to work hard (Effort M) and to understand and collabo-
rate (Understand and Collaborate M).
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 495

T A B L E IV
Means (and Standard Deviations) on Beliefs and Personal Goals
about Reasons for Success Scales
Group F P
Project Non-Project (1,189)
Reasons for Success
Effort B 4.71(0.54) 4.42(0.75) 3.50 .06
Understand and Collaborate B 4.34(0.74) 4.05(0.72) 7.54 .01"
Conform B 3.66(1.11) 4.52(1.05) 29.84 .00"
Competitiveness B 3.12(1.04) 3.12(0.99) 0.00 .99
Extrinsic B 2.73(1.14) 2.64(1.12) 0.25 .62
Personal Goals
Effort M 4.13(0.84) 3.94(0.97) 1.94 .17
Understand and Collaborate M 4.48(0.63) 4.35(0.79) 1.99 .16
EgoM 3.34(1.04) 3.31(1.18) 0.44 .51
Work Avoidance M 3.01(1.22) 3.24(1.18) 1.25 .27
Note: n = 79 for project students and 111 for non-project students
* p<.05.

DISCUSSION

Arithmetic Tests
The previously conducted comparison of students' performance at the end of
second grade supported the conclusion that project students had generally
attained more sophisticated levels of arithmetical reasoning than had non-
project students (Cobb, Wood, Yackel, Nicholls, Wheatley, Trigatti, and Perlwitz,
1991). It is not possible to directly compare differences in the two groups' mean
scores at the end of third grade with those at the end of second grade because
different Project Arithmetic Tests were used, and because students who partici-
pated in the current study constitute only a subsample of those who participated
in the previous study. The most that can be concluded from the students' perfor-
mance on the Relational scale of the third-grade Project Arithmetic Test is that
project students maintained their superiority over non-project students to some
degree after a year of traditional instruction. Differences on all but one of the
ten subscales, Less Challenging Word Problems, favored project students and
seven of these differences either attained or approached statistical significance.
It is also noteworthy that the three subscales on which there was no significant
difference in performance were generally composed of less conceptually chal-
lenging items. For example, mean scores on the Less Challenging Word Prob-
lem subscale were identical whereas project students significantly outperformed
non-project students on the More Challenging Word Problems subscale. Simi-
larly, the difference in group means was significant for the Multiplication-
496 P. COBB, T. WOOD, E. YACKEL, AND M. PERLWITZ

Division subscale but not for the Multiplication Picture subscale where word
problems were accompanied by a picture.
The relatively small magnitude of the difference in group means on the
ISTEP Concepts and Applications subtest is consistent with the observation that
project students tended to significantly outperform non-project students on the
more conceptually challenging tasks. Many of the items on the ISTEP Concepts
and Applications subtest could be solved by following the idiosyncratic conven-
tions of traditional, textbook-based instruction. These conventions were, in all
probability, directly taught to all students during the third-grade year.
The performance of the two groups of students on both the ISTEP Computa-
tion subtest and the Instrumental scale of the Project Arithmetic Test indicates
that their ability to complete routine computational tasks presented in familiar
textbook formats were similar. The same conclusion was reached when compu-
tational proficiency was compared at the end of second grade. There, it was
argued that the similarity in computational performance on tasks presented in
the column format masked differences in the extent to which students' algo-
rithms were conceptually based. The same conclusion seems warranted a year
later. Project students significantly outperformed non-project students when
additive tasks were presented in four other formats (Everyday I.amguage, I-Iori-
zontal Sentences, Money, and More Challenging Word Problems). The non-
project students' greater format-dependency in computational situations can be
at least partially attributed to their less sophisticated understanding of place
value as indicated by their performance on the Numeration subsc~e

Beliefs and Motivations


The five belief scales shown in Table II were developed to assess students'
interpretations of their classroom realities (Nicholls, Cobb, Wood, Yackel, and
Patashnik, 1990). The finding that there were significant differences on three of
the scales as shown in Table IV might at first seem surprising given that project
and non-project students sat side by side in the same third-grade classrooms.
Such differences are, however, reasonable from a constructivist perspective
which emphasizes that interpretation is an active process. Recall that during the
second-grade school year, project teachers initiated and guided the renegotiation
of classroom social norms that constituted the social reality of their classrooms
during mathematics instruction. For their part, project students modified their
beliefs about their own role, others' roles, and the general nature of mathemati-
cal activity as they participated in this renegotiation process (Cobb, Wood, and
Yackel, 1991b). As a consequence of these modifications, which were docu-
mented in the prior assessment, there were general differences in the interpre-
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 497

tive schemes within which project and non-project students made sense of class-
room events when they entered third grade. The current assessment study docu-
ments the ways in which the two groups of students modified their initially
differing beliefs during the third-grade school year as they strove to give a
meaning and coherence to classroom events (cf. von Glasersfeld, 1984a,
1990b). The differences in the beliefs of the two groups of students at the end of
the third-grade year demonstrate that they generally dealt with the issue of how
to be effective in different ways, and indeed had differing understandings of
what it meant to be effective.
With regard to the specific findings, the greater value that project students
placed on doing their best and being interested in mathematics (Effort B),
attempting to understand and collaborate (Understand and Collaborate B), and
developing personally meaningful solution methods (Conform B) indicates that
they had maintained some of the beliefs that the second-grade project teachers
had attempted to foster. These results are consistent with the general patterns of
performance on the arithmetic tests. In particular, project students' superior
conceptual understanding and problem solving is reflected in the beliefs that
supported the development of those capabilities. The responses of the two
groups of students to the two remaining belief scales were similar, with both
groups being generally neutral about the importance of showing they were supe-
rior to others (Competitiveness B) and being lucky, neat, and quiet (Extrinsic
B).
The students' responses to the Conform B scale are disturbing despite the rel-
atively large difference in group means. The means on this scale at the end of
the second-grade year were 2.01 and 3.64 for project and non-project students
respectively. These results indicated that project students generally rejected the
conjecture that success in school mathematics stems from attempts to conform
to the teacher's or other students' solution methods. The third-grade means indi-
cate that project students now tend to agree with the conjecture and non-project
students very firmly agree with it. Again, it should be noted that the students
who participated in the current study are a subsample of those who participated
in the prior second-grade assessment. Nonetheless, the magnitude of the change
between second and third grade strongly indicates that both groups came to
value conformity more strongly as a consequence of their instructional experi-
ences during the third-grade year.
A case study of one third-grade classroom conducted by McNeal (1991) dur-
ing the first six weeks of the school year goes some way towards explaining the
students' decrease in intellectual autonomy. In the classroom that McNeal stud-
ied, the teacher legitimized thinking strategy or derived fact solutions at the
beginning of the school year when she and her students reviewed the basic addi-
498 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

tion and subtraction facts. However, the teacher rejected efficient, non-standard
algorithms when she reviewed two-digit addition and subtraction. From the
teacher's point of view, it was essential that her students demonstrate that they
were following the steps of the standard algorithms by recording the conven-
tional regrouping procedures. More generally, the teacher's actions served to
foster a view of mathematics as an activity that involves following procedural
instructions. The establishment of this interpretive stance was not restricted to
the algorithmic aspects of mathematics, but also occurred when supposedly
more conceptual topics such as place value numeration were dealt with and
when manipulatives were used. It should be stressed that the teacher was a sin-
cere professional who genuinely wanted her students to learn with understand-
ing and who would be judged as highly competent by all traditional means of
assessment including those derived from the effective schools research. She was
merely doing her job as she understood it, an understanding that is compatible
with that of most parents, administrators, test constructors, and, apparently, text-
book authors. In doing so, she attempted to induct her students into the interpre-
tive stance of traditional school mathematics, and, in the process, her students
came to see increasingly less value in their own non-standard solution methods.
The most surprising finding concerning the students' beliefs about reasons
for success is that both groups agreed that attempting to understand and collabo-
rate is important, albeit with differing degrees of firmness (Understand and Col-
laborate B). At first glance, this interpretation of the social reality of the
classroom would seem to be contradicted by their responses to the Conform B
scale. If, however, we assume that the students' responses are sensible from
their points of view, then the apparent conflict allows us to clarify students'
notions of what it might mean to understand in mathematics. In particular, the
results suggest that, for non-project students, to understand meant to enact and
perhaps describe the steps of procedural instructions. Project students' less than
whole-hearted belief in the importance of conforming to the solution methods of
others suggests that mathematical understanding might, in their view, involve a
more extensive conceptual aspect. In general, the students' understandings of
mathematical understanding reflect the accommodations they made during the
school year as' their teachers attempted to induct them into the tradition of
school mathematics.
In conlrast to the differences in their beliefs about the reasons for success, the
personal goals of the two groups of students were similar. Both groups general-
ly achieved a sense of satisfaction when they worked hard (Effort M) and when
they attempted to understand and collaborate (Understand and Collaborate M).
Further, both groups were generally ambivalent about whether doing better than
others (Ego M) and not having to do any work (Work Avoidance M) were
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 499

pleasing experiences. It should also be noted that the students' responses to the
Understand and Collaborate M scale call into question a speculation made by
Cobb, Wood, Yackel, NichoUs, Wheafley, Trigatti, and Perlwitz (1991). There,
it was found that the interpretations of the reality of the classroom made by non-
project students were less closely aligned with the personal goal of understand-
ing and collaboration than were those of project students. It was therefore
suggested that the persistence of such a discrepancy might lead some non-
project students to revise their commitment to understand and collaborate, thus
giving rise to negative attitudes towards, or alienation from, mathematics. This
does not appear to have occurred during the third-grade year despite the fact
that non-project students continue to attribute less importance both to under-
standing and collaborating (Understanding and Collaborating B) and to devel-
oping non-standard solution methods (Conform B). Instead, it would seem that
these students and, to a lesser extent, the project students coped with a tension
between their personal goals and their beliefs about what they have to do to be
effective in their classrooms by modifying their understanding of what it means
to understand in mathematics. In other words, the students continued to hold
understanding and collaboration as a personal goal, but they modified their con-
ceptions of mathematical understanding as they interacted with teachers who
unknowingly attempted to induct them into the school mathematics tradition (cf.
Cobb, Yackel, Wood, and McNeal, 1992; Lampert, 1990).

CONCLUSIONS

The data presented in this paper give further credibility to the claim that a
problem-centered instructional approach which emphasizes mathematical argu-
mentation is feasible in public school classrooms. In particular, the finding that
project students' conceptual understanding and problem solving in arithmetic
was superior to that of non-project students after a year of traditional instruction
adds credibility to the claim that the problem-centered approach facilitated stu-
dents' construction of increasingly conceptually sophisticated operations. This
conclusion is further substantiated by the finding that project students tended to
outperform their non-project classmates on more conceptual challenging tasks.
In this regard, Carpenter, Fennema, Peterson, Chiang, and Loef (1989) reported
that the performance of students who had participated in their first-grade
Cognitively Guided Instruction (CGI) project was superior to that of non-CGI
students on more challenging addition and subtraction word problems. In con-
trast, CGI and non-CGI students' performance was similar on less challenging
word problems that exemplify the lowest level of Carpenter and Moser's (1984)
classification scheme. The results presented in this paper complement those of
500 P. C O B B , T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , A N D M. P E R L W I T Z

Carpenter, Fennema, Peterson, Chiang, and Loef (1989) by indicating that dif-
ferences of this sort can be maintained to a significant degree when students are
subsequently assigned to textbook-based classrooms.
The differences in project and non-project students' responses to the belief
scales also corroborate conclusions drawn from the prior second-grade study. It
seems clear that students who participated in the prior study did not respond to
the beliefs questionnaire by reporting an official classroom doctrine that was
unrelated to their mathematical activity. Instead, the findings of the prior and
the current study overwhelmingly indicate that the second-grade project teach-
ers were reasonably successful in influencing some of their students' fundamen-
tal beliefs about mathematics and themselves as learners. More generally, the
results indicate that project instruction encourages the development of intellec-
tual autonomy whereas textbook-based instruction as it was realized in the
third-grade classrooms fosters intellectual heteronomy (cf. Kamii and de Clark,
1985).
At the outset, we noted that the development of the project's instructional
approach was informed by a constructivist theory of knowledge. This should
not be interpreted to mean that the study reported in this paper constitutes a test
of constructivism. As a self-referential theory of knowing, constructivism
implies that the issue of whether people really do construct their own ways of
knowing is beside the point - - it is not worth asking the question (cf. Rorty,
1983). The interesting question is instead whether the metaphor of active con-
struction is a useful and ethically appropriate way to view people for particular
purposes. Readers whose views about what is important in students' mathemat-
ics education are similar to our own might conclude that constructivism offers
some promise as a theoretical orientation within which to frame questions,
develop alternative forms of classroom practice in collaboration with teachers,
and analyze learning and interaction in the classroom.

NOTES

1 The research reported in this paper was supported by the National Science Foundation under grant
No. MDR 885-0560 and by the Indiana Department of Education. The opinions expressed do not
necessarily reflect the views of either NSF or IDE.
2 The term "taken-as-shared" refers to whatever notions or meanings an individual believes are
equivalent to or shared with others. This term is preferred to the more usual "shared" because we do
not have direct access to each others' experiences and, consequently, cannot directly compare mean-
ings. The belief that meanings are shared can be thought of as a potentially revisable assumption
that makes communication possible. The term "taken-to-be-shared" was used by Schiitz (1962) and
was subsequently changed to "taken-as-shared" by Streeck (1979).
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 501

REFERENCES
Addison-Wesley Mathematics Teacher's Resource Book, Book 3: 1987, Addison-Wesley, Menlo
Park, CA.
Barnes, D.: 1976, From Communication to Curriculum, Penguin, New York.
Bartolini Bussi, M. B.: 1991, "Social interaction and mathematical knowledge". In F. Furinghetti
(ed.), Proceedings of the Fifteenth Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education, Program Committee of the 15th PME Conference, Genoa, Italy, pp.
1-16.
Bauersfeld, H.: 1980, "Hidden dimensions in the so-called reality of a mathematics classroom",
Educational Studies in Mathematics II, 23--41.
Bauersfeld, H.: 1988, "Interaction, construction, and knowledge: Alternative perspectives for math-
ematics education". In T. Cooney and D. Grouws (eds.), Effective Mathematics Teaching,
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics/Lawrence Erlbaum, Reston, VA, pp. 27-46.
Bishop, A. L: 1985, "The social construction of meaning - - A significant development for mathe-
matics education?", For the Learning of Mathematics 5(1), 24-28.
Bishop, A. J.: 1988, Mathematical Enculturation: A Cultural Perspective on Mathematics Educa-
tion, Kluwer, Dordrecht.
Blurner, H.: 1969, Symbolic Interactionism: Perspectives and Method, Prentice Hall, Englewood
Cliffs, NJ.
Brousseau, G.: 1984, aThe crucial role of the didactical contract in the analysis and construction of
situations in teaching and learning mathematics". In H. G. Steiner (ed.), Theory of Mathematics
Education (Occasional paper 54), IDM, Bielefeld, pp. 110-119.
Bmner, J.: 1986, Actual Minds, Possible Worlds, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Carpenter, T. P. Fennema, E., Peterson, P. L., Chiang, C., and Loef, M.: 1989, "Using knowledge of
children's mathematics in press thinking in classroom teaching: An experimental study", Ameri-
can Educational Research Journal 26, 499-532.
Carpenter, T. P. and Moser, J. M.: 1984, "The acquisition of addition and subtraction concepts in
grades one through three", Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 15, 179-202.
Cobb, P. and Wheafley, G.: 1988, "Children's initial understandings of ten", Focus on Learning
Problems in Mathematics 10(3), 1-28.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., and Yackel, E.: 1991a, "A constructivist approach to second grade mathemat-
ics". In E. von Glasersfeld (ed.), Constructivism in Mathemat&s Education, Kluwer, Dordrecht,
pp. 157-176.
Cobb, P., Wood, T. and Yackel, E.: 1991b, "Analogies from the philosophy and sociology of sci-
ence for understanding classroom life", Science Education 75, 23-44.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., and Yackel, E.: in press-a, "Discourse, mathematical thinking, and classroom
practice". In E. Forman, N. Minick, and A. Stone (eds.), Contexts for Learning: Sociocultural
Dynamics in Children's Develol~ment , Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., and Yackel, E.: in press-b, "Learning and interaction in classroom situations",
Educational Studies in Mathematics.
Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., Nicholls, J., Wheafley, G., Trigatti, B., and Perlwitz, M.: 1991,
"Assessment of a problem-centered second grade mathematics project", Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education 22, 3-29.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., and Wood, T.: 1988, "Curriculum and teacher development: Psychological
and anthropological perspectives". In E. Fennema, T. P. Carpenter, and S. J. Lamon (eds.), Inte-
grating Research on Teaching and Learning Mathematics, Wisconsin Center for Educational
Research, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, pp. 92-131.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., and Wood, T.: 1989, "Young children's emotional acts while doing mathemat-
ical problem solving". In D. B. McLeod and V. M. Adams (eds.), Affect and Mathematical Prob-
lem Solving: A New Perspective, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 117-148.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., and Wood, T.: 1992, "A constructivist alternative to the representational view
502 P. COBB, T. W O O D , E. Y A C K E L , AND M. P E R L W I T Z

of mind in mathematics education", Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23(1),


2-33.
Cobb, P., Yackel, E., Wood, T., and McNeal, B.: 1992, "Mathematics as procedural instructions and
mathematics as meaningful activity: The reality of teaching for understanding". In ll.B. Davis
and C.A. Maher (eds.), Schools, Mathematics and the World of Reality, Allyn and Bacon, Need-
ham Heights, MA, pp. 119--133.
Confrey, J.: 1986, "A critique of teacher effectiveness research in mathematics education", Journal
for Research in Mathematics Education 17, 347-360.
Confrey, J.: 1990a, "What constructivism implies for teaching". In R. B. Davis, C. A. Maher, and N.
Noddings (eds.), Constructivist Views on the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics (Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education Monograph No. 4), National Council of Teachers of Mathe-
matics, Reston, VA, pp. 107-122.
Confrey, J.: 1990b, "Student conceptions, representations, and the design of software". In Design
for Learning Apple Computer, External Research, Cupertino, CA, pp. 55-62.
Kamii, C. and de Clark, G.: 1985, Young Children Reinvent Arithmet&: Implications of Piaget's
Theory, Teachers College Press, New York.
Kaput, J. J.: 1991, "Notations and representations as mediators of constructive processes". In E. yon
Glasersfeld (ed.), Construct&ism in Mathematics Education, Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp. 53-74).
Lampert, M.: 1990, "When the problem is not the question and the solution is not the answer: Math-
ematical knowing and teaching", American Educational Research Journal 27, 29-63.
McNeal, B.: 1991, The Social Context of Mathematical Development. Unpublished doctoral disser-
tation, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.
Mead, G. H.: 1934, Mind, Self, and Society, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Mehan, H.: 1979, Learning Lessons, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Minick, N.: 1989, "Mind and activity in Vygotsky's work: An expanded frame of reference", Cu/-
tural Dynamics 2, 162-187.
Newman, D., Griffin, P., and Cole, M.: 1989, The Construction Zone: Working for Cognitive
Change in School, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Nicholls, J., Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., and Patashnik, M.: 1990, "Dimensions of success in
mathematics: Individual and classroom differences", Journal for Research in Mathematics Edu-
cation 21, 109-122.
Perret-Clermont, A. N.: 1980, Social Interaction and Cognitive Development in Children, Academic
Press, New York.
Piaget, J.: 1971, Biology and Knowledge, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Piaget, J.: 1980, Adaptation and Intelligence: Organic Selection and Phenocopy, University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Richards, 1.: 1991, "Mathematical discussions". In E. von Glasersfeld (ed.), Constructivism in
Mathematics Education, Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp. 13-52.
Rogoff, B.: 1990, Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context, Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
Rorty, R.: 1983, "Method and morality". In N. Haan, R. N. Bellah, and P. Robinson (eds.), Social
Science as Moral Inquiry, Columbia University Press, New York, pp. 155-176.
Saxe, G. B.: 1991, Culture and Cognitive Development: Studies on Mathematical Understanding,
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Schiitz, A.: 1962, The Problem of Social Reality, Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague.
Silver, E. A.: 1986, "Using conceptual and procedural knowledge: A focus on relationships". In J.
Hiebert (ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of Mathematics, Lawrence Erl-
baum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 181-198.
Sinclair, It.: 1990, "Learning: The interactive recreation of knowledge". In L. P. Steffe and T.
Wood (eds.), Transforming Children's Mathematics Education: International Perspectives,
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 19-29.
Smith, E. and Confrey, J.: 1991, Understanding Collaborative Learning: Small-Group Work on
PROBLEM-CENTERED MATHEMATICS 503

Contextual Problems Using a Multi-Representational Software Tool. Paper presented at AERA


annual meeting, Chicago, April.
Steffe, L. P.: 1987, Principles of Mathematical Curriculum Design in Early Childhood Teacher
Education. Paper presented at AERA annual meeting, Washington, DC, April.
Steffe, L. P., Cobb, P. (with E. von Glasersfeld):. 1988, Construction of ArithmeticalMeanings and
Strategies, Springer-Verlag, New York.
Steffe, L. P., yon Glasersfeld, E., Richards, J., and Cobb, P.: 1983, Children's Counting Types: Phi-
losophy, Theory, and Applications, Praeger Scientific, New York.
Streeck, J.: 1979, "Sandwich. Good for you". In J. Dittman (ed.), Arbeiten zurKonversationsanal-
yse, Neimeyer, Tfibingen, pp. 235-257.
Thompson, P.: 1985, "Experience, problem solving, and leaming mathematics: Considerations in
developing mathematical curricula". In E. A. Silver (ed.), Teaching and Learning Mathematical
Problem Solving: Multiple Research Perspectives, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 189-236.
Treffers, A.: 1987, Three dimensions: A Model of Goal and Theory Description in Mathematics
Instruction - - The Wiskobas Project, Reidel, Dordrecht.
Voigt, J.: 1985, "Patterns and routines in classroom interaction", Recherches en Didactique des
Mathdmatiques 6, 69-118.
Voigt, J.: 1989, "The social constitution of the mathematics province - A rnicroethnographical
study in classroom interaction", The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative
Human Cognition 11(1/2), 27-34.
von Glasersfeld, E.: 1984a, "An introduction to radical constmctivism". In P. Watzlawick (ed.), The
Invented Reality, Norton, New York, pp. 17-40.
von Glasersfeld, E.: 1984b, "Thoughts about space, time, and the concept of identity". In Book-
Conference, First Installment, Princelet Editions Living Books, Zurich, pp. 21-36
von Glasersfeld, E.: 1990a, "Constructivism in mathematics education: Why some like it radical".
In R. B. Davis, C. A. Maher, and N. Noddings (eds.), Constructivist Views on the Teaching and
Learning of Mathematics (Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Monograph No. 4),
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Reston, VA, pp. 19-30.
von Glasersfeld, E.: 1990b, "Environment and communication". In L. P. Steffe and T. Wood (eds.),
Transforming Children's Mathematics Education: International Perspectives, Lawrence Efl-
baum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 30-38.
Wood, T., Cobb, P., and Yackel, E.: 1990, "The contextual nature of teaching: Mathematics and
reading instruction in one second-grade classroom", Elementary School Journal 90, 497-514.
Wood, T., Cobb, P., and Yackel, E.: 1991, "Change in teaching mathematics: A case study", Ameri-
can Educational Research Journal 28(3), 587-616.
Wood, T., Cobb, P., and Yackel, E.: 1992, "Change in leaming mathematics: Change in teaching
mathematics". In H. Marshall (ed.), Redefining Student Learning: Roots of Educational Change,
Ablex, Norwood, NJ.
Wood, T. and Yackel, E.: 1990, "The development of collaborative dialogue within small group
interactions". In L. P. Steffe and T. Wood (eds.), Transforming Children's Mathematics Educa-
tion: International Perspectives, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Yackel, E., Cobb, P., and Wood, T.: 1991, "Small group interactions as a source of learning oppor-
tunities in second grade mathematics", Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 22(5),
390--408.

Paul Cobb
Vanderbilt University
Nashville, Tennessee
U.S.A.
504 P. COBB, T. WOOD, E. YACKEL, AND M. PERLWITZ

Terry Wood and Marcela Perlwitz


Department of Curriculum and Instruction
Purdue University
West Lafayette, Indiana, 47907-1402
U.S_4.

Erna Yackel
Department of Mathematics, Computer Science, and Statistics
Purdue University-Calumet
Hammond, Indiana, 46323-2094
U.S.A.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen