Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Sight-reading

Sight-reading, also called a prima vista (Italian meaning "at first sight"), is the
reading and performing of a piece of music or song in music notation that the
performer has not seen before. Sight-singing is used to describe a singer who is
sight-reading. Both activities require the musician to play or sing the notated
rhythms and pitches. Many believe that sight-singing is the more challenging of the
two, because singers do not have any keys, frets or valves (on keyboard instruments,
guitars, and valved brass instruments, respectively) to help them obtain the correct
pitches. Singers must also read and sing the correct lyrics, which adds another layer
beyond pitch and dynamics. However, difficulty is related both to the instrument and
the difficulty of the piece itself. For instance, sight-reading on a polyphonic
instrument, such as the classical guitar, can be more challenging due to the fact that
the guitarist has to read polyrhythms and polyphonic passages that can often be
played in different positions using different frets.

Caravaggio's Rest on the Flight into


Egypt (1594–96)
Contents
Terminology
Sight-reading
Sight transposition
Sight-playing
Sight-singing
Psychology
Professional use
Pedagogy
Assessment and standards
See also
References
Further reading
External links

Terminology

Sight-reading
People in music literature commonly use the term "sight-reading" generically for "the ability to read and produce both instrumental
and vocal music at first sight […] the conversion of musical information from sight to sound".[1] Udtaisuk and some other authors
prefer the use of the more specific terms "sight-playing" and "sight-singing" where applicable. This differentiation leaves a third,
more restricted use of the term "sight-reading" for the silent reading of music without creating sound by instrument or voice.

Highly skilled musicians can sight-read silently; that is, they can look at the printed music and hear it in their heads without playing
or singing (see audiation).[2] Less able sight-readers generally must at least hum or whistle in order to sight-read effectively. This
distinction is analogous to ordinary prose reading in late antiquity, when the ability to read silently was notable enough for St.
Augustine to comment on it. [3]
The term a prima vista is also used, as Italian words and phrases are commonly used in music and music notation. To play a musical
piece a prima vista means to play it 'at first sight'. According to Payne, "the ability to hear the notes on the page is clearly akin to
music reading and should be considered a prerequisite for effective performance.... Egregious errors can occur when a student,
[4]
analyzing a piece of music, makes no effort to play or hear the composition but mechanically processes the notes on the page".

The ability to sight-read is important for all musicians, even amateur performers, but with professional orchestra musicians and
session musicians, it is an essential professional skill. Some professional orchestras require sight-reading as part of an audition.

Sight transposition
Some musicians can transpose music during performance to suit particular instruments or vocal ranges, to make the playing of the
instrument(s) or singing easier, or a number of other uses.

Sight-playing
According to Udtaisuk, "many [authors] use the term sight-playing for instrumental sight-reading performance." However, Udtaisuk
and some other authors use the more descriptive term "sightplaying" (or "sight-playing") for instrumental sight-reading, because
sight-playing combines two unique skill sets: music reading and music making.

Sight-singing
Some authors, according to Udtaisuk, use the term "sight-singing" for vocal sight-reading. As with sight-playing, Udtaisuk advocates
and uses the more descriptive term "sightsinging" for vocal sight-reading because sight-singing combines sight-reading and singing
skills.

Psychology
The ability to sight-read partly depends on a strong short-term musical memory. An experiment on sight reading using an eye tracker
indicates that highly skilled musicians tend to look ahead further in the music, storing and processing the notes until they are played;
this is referred to as theeye–hand span.

Storage of notational information in working memory can be expressed in terms of the amount of information (load) and the time for
which it must be held before being played (latency). The relationship between load and latency changes according to tempo, such that
t = x/y, where t is the change in tempo, x is the change in load, and y is the change in latency. Some teachers and researchers have
proposed that the eye–hand span can be trained to be larger than it would otherwise be under normal conditions, leading to more
robust sight-reading ability.

Human memory can be divided into three broad categories: long-term memory, sensory memory, and short-term (working) memory.
According to the formal definition, working memory is "a system for temporarily storing and managing the information required to
carry out complex cognitive tasks such as learning, reasoning, and comprehension." The paramount feature that distinguishes the
working memory from both the long-term and sensory memory is this system's ability to simultaneously process and store
information. The knowledge has what is called a "limited capacity", so there is only a certain amount of information that can be
stored and it is easily accessible for only a small window of time after it has been processed, with a recall time block of roughly
fifteen seconds to one minute.

Experiments dealing with memory span have been conducted by George Miller in 1956 that indicated, "Most common number of
items that can be stored in the working memory is five plus or minus two.” However, if this information is not retained and stored
(“consolidated”) in one's long-term memory
, it will fade quickly.

Research indicates that the main area of the brain associated with the working memory is the prefrontal cortex. The prefrontal cortex
is located in the frontal lobe of the brain. This area deals with cognition and contains two major neural loops or pathways that are
central to processing tasks via the working memory: the visual loop, which is necessary for the visual component of the task, and the
phonological loop, which deals with the linguistic aspects of the task (i.e. repeating the word or phrase). Although the hippocampus,
in the temporal lobe, is the brain structure most frequently paired with memories, studies have indicated that its role is more vital for
consolidation of the short-term memories into long-term ones than the ability to process, carry out, and briefly recall certain tasks.

This type of memory has specifically come into focus when discussing sight reading, since the process of looking at musical notes for
the first time and deciphering them while playing an instrument can be considered a complex task of comprehension. The main
conclusion in terms of this idea is that working memory and short-term memory capacity and mental speed are three important
predictors for sight reading achievement. Although none of the studies discredits the correlation between the amount of time one
spends practicing and musical ability, specifically sight-reading proficiency, more studies are pointing to the level at which one’s
working memory functions as the key factor in sight-reading abilities. As stated in one such study, "Working memory capacity made
a statistically significant contribution as well (about 7 percent, a medium-size effect). In other words, if you took two pianists with
the same amount of practice, but different levels of working memory capacity, it's likely that the one higher in working memory
capacity would have performed considerably better on the sight-reading task."

Based on the research and opinions of multiple musicians and scientists, the take home message about one's sight-reading ability and
working memory capacity seems to be that “The best sight-readers combined strong working memories with tens of thousands of
hours of practice.”

Sight-reading also depends on familiarity with the musical idiom being performed; this permits the reader to recognize and process
frequently occurring patterns of notes as a single unit, rather than individual notes, thus achieving greater efficiency. This
phenomenon, which also applies to the reading of language, is referred to as chunking. Errors in sight-reading tend to occur in places
where the music contains unexpected or unusual sequences; these defeat the strategy of "reading by expectation" that sight-readers
typically employ.

Professional use
Studio musicians (e.g., musicians employed to record pieces for commercials, etc.) often record pieces on the first take without
having seen them before. Often, the music played on television is played by musicians who are sight-reading. This practice has
developed through intense commercial competition in these industries.

McNerney, jazz musician, professor, and private instructor, describes auditions for University of North Texas Jazz Lab Bands as
being almost completely based on sight-reading: "you walk into a room and see three or four music stands in front of you, each with a
[5]
piece of music on it (in different styles ...). You are then asked to read each piece in succession."

This emphasis on sight-reading, according to McNerney, prepares musicians for studio work "playing backing tracks for pop
performers or recording [commercials]." The expense of the studio, musicians, and techs makes sight-reading skills essential.
Typically, a studio performance is "rehearsed" only once to check for copying errors before recording the final track. Many
professional big bands also sight-read every live performance. They are known as "rehearsal bands", even though their performance
is the rehearsal.

According to Frazier, score reading is an important skill for those interested in the conducting profession and "Conductors such as the
late Robert Shaw and Yoel Levi have incredibly strong piano skills and can read at sight full orchestral scores at the piano" (a process
which requires the pianist to make an instantpiano reduction of the key parts of the score).[6]

Pedagogy
Although 86% of piano teachers polled rated sight-reading as the most important or a highly important skill, only 7% of them said
they address it systematically. Reasons cited were a lack of knowledge of how to teach it, inadequacy of the training materials they
use, and deficiency in their own sight-reading skills. Teachers also often emphasize rehearsed reading and repertoire building for
[7]
successful recitals and auditions to the detriment of sight-reading and other functional skills.
Hardy reviewed research on piano sight-reading pedagogy and identified a number of specific skills essential to sight-reading
proficiency:

Technical fundamentals in reading andfingering


Visualization of keyboard topography
Tactile facility (psychomotor skills) and memory
Ability to read, recognize, and remember groups of notes (directions, patterns, phrases,
chords, rhythmic groupings,
themes, inversions, intervals, etc.)
Ability to read and remember ahead of playing with more and wider progressive fixations
Aural imagery (ear-playing and sight-singing improves sight-reading)
Ability to keep the basic pulse, read, and remember rhythm
Awareness and knowledge of the music's structure and theory
Beauchamp (1999) identifies five building blocks in the development of piano sight-reading skills:

1. Grand-staff knowledge
2. Security within the five finger positions
3. Security with keyboard topography
4. Security with basic accompaniment patterns
5. Understanding of basic fingering principles
Grand-staff knowledge consists of fluency in both clefs such that reading a note evokes an automatic and immediate physical
response to the appropriate position on the keyboard. Beauchamp asserts it is better to sense and know where the note is than what
the note is. The performer does not have time to think of the note name and translate it to a position, and the non-scientific note name
does not indicate the octave to be played. Beauchamp reports success using a Key/Note Visualizer, note-reading flashcards, and
computer programs in group and individual practice to develop grand-staf
f fluency.

Udtaisuk also reports that a sense of keyboard geography and an ability to quickly and efficiently match notes to keyboard keys is
important for sight-reading. He found that "computer programs and flash cards are effective ways to teach students to identify notes
[and] enhance a sense of keyboard geography by highlighting the relationships between the keyboard and the printed notation."

Most students do not sight-read well because it requires specific instruction, which is seldom given. A major challenge in sight-
reading instruction, according to Hardy, is obtaining enough practice material. Since practicing rehearsed reading does not help
improve sight-reading, a student can only use a practice piece once. Moreover, the material must be at just the right level of difficulty
for each student, and a variety of styles is preferred. Hardy suggests music teachers cooperate to build a ge
lar lending library of music
and purchase inexpensive music from garage sales and store sales.

Assessment and standards


In some circumstances, such as examinations, the ability of a student to sight-read is assessed by presenting the student with a short
piece of music, with an allotted time to peruse the music, then testing the student on the accuracy of the performance. A more
challenging test requires the student to perform without any preparation at all.

The Washington Assessment of Student Learning has piloted a classroom based assessment which requires 5th and higher grade
students to sight-sing or perform on instruments from sheet music they have written. It is suggested that students use solfege or
numbering systems or fingering without instruments as aids. 8th graders are expected to sing by sight: "Students are asked to perform
a sight-singing exercise of four measures of music. Students will be assessed on their understanding of rhythm and steady beat and
[8]
their ability to perform in the designated key with accurate interval changes, acappella."

Many students and adults cannot sight-sing, and even some professional singers cannot sing by sight. However, in combination with
an assessment which requires composing music on a staff as early as 5th grade, it is hoped that such a requirement will raise arts
achievement. Pilot data show that many students can meet or exceed such standards.
The Standard Assessment of Sight Reading (SASR) is a non-subjective sight reading evaluation method. It was created with a
scientific/electronic platform to insure a non-subjective approach to grading and administering that test. It consists of several
thousand pieces of music over 80 graded levels of difficulty that have been reviewed by 135 teachers and students to ascertain the
correct difficulty levels. Their scores were averaged electronically in order to insure a scientific approach to graduating the difficulty
levels of the music.

See also
Count singing
Ear training
Eye movement in music reading
Shape note
Subvocalization

References
Notes

1. (Udtaisuk 2005)
2. True sight-reading or sight-singing—not code-deciphering—is actuallynotational audiation.
3. (Manguel 1996)
4. Payne (2005)
5. McNerney (2008)
6. Frazier (1999)
7. (Hardy 1998)
8. "Arts Revised Classroom-Based Performance Assessment (CBP
A)" (http://www.k12.wa.us/assessment/WASL/Arts/
CBPANewRevised2009.aspx)

Sources

Beauchamp, Laura (May 1, 1999),"The 'Building Blocks' of Reading: Suggestions for Developing Sight Reading
Skills in Beginning Level College Piano Classes", Piano Pedagogy Forum, Columbia, SC: University of South
Carolina School of Music,2 (2), retrieved 2014-10-29.
Frazier, Ivan (May 1, 1999), "The Well-Furnished Keyboardist", Piano Pedagogy Forum, Columbia, SC: University of
South Carolina School of Music,2 (2).
Hardy, Dianne (May 1, 1998),"Teaching Sight-Reading at the Piano: Methodology and Significance", Piano
Pedagogy Forum, Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina School of Music,1 (2).
Manguel, Alberto (1996),"The Silent Readers", A History of Reading, New York: Viking.
McNerney, Kevin (May 2, 2008),"My UNT Degrees Came In Handy T onight", The Musings of Kev: Random
Thoughts and Rants from an Everyday Saxophonist (Blog) .
MENC (2001), Careers in Music, Reston, VA: MENC: The National Association of Music Education, archived from
the original on October 15, 2007
Payne, Dorothy (2005),"Essential Skills, Part 1 of 4: Essential Skills for Promoting a Lifelong Love of Music and
Music Making", American Music Teacher, Cincinnati, OH: Music Teachers National Association (published February–
March 2005)
Udtaisuk, Dneya (May 2005),A Theoretical Model of Piano Sightplaying components(PDF), Diss. U. of Missouri-
Columbia, pp. 54–55, archived fromthe original (PDF) on 2006-09-15.

Further reading
Bower, Bruce (15 December 2011). "For Sight-Reading Music, Practice Doesn't Make Perfect : Discovery News."
Discovery News: Earth, Space, Tech, Animals, History, Adventure, Human, Autos. Science News.
Hambrick, David Z. and Meinz, Elizabeth J. (19 November 2001)."Sorry, Strivers: Talent Matters".
External links
See Music Interactive Sight-reading Software for all instruments and voice

Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Sight-reading&oldid=850069133


"

This page was last edited on 13 July 2018, at 11:31(UTC).

Text is available under theCreative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License ; additional terms may apply. By using this
site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of theWikimedia
Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen