Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

DOI 10.1007/s11199-012-0176-9

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Children’s Gender-Typed Activity Choices Across Preschool


Social Contexts
Priscilla Goble & Carol Lynn Martin & Laura D. Hanish &
Richard A. Fabes

Published online: 30 May 2012


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Abstract Variability in children’s gender-typed activity Introduction


preferences was examined across several preschool social
contexts–solitary play, interactions with female peers, male Research on children in the U.S. has consistently indicated
peers, and both, and interactions with teachers. Participants that gender-typed activity preferences are a salient aspect of
were preschool children (N 0264; 49 % girls, M age 0 children’s play – girls tend to prefer feminine activities and
52 months, range 37–60) attending Head Start classes in boys prefer masculine activities (Ruble et al. 2006). U.S.
the Southwest United States. Seventy-three percent were researchers have shown that children’s preferences for
Mexican/Mexican-American, and 82 % of families earned gender-typed activities emerge during the early years of life
less than $30,000 per year. Children’s preferences for and continue into middle and late childhood (Campbell et al.
gender-typed activities varied as a function of their own 2000; McHale et al. 2004), but may be particularly evident
gender and the identity of their interactional partners. Girls during preschool. Interestingly, in a study of 1-, 3-, and 5-
and boys preferred gender-typed activities (e.g., girls pre- year old Swedish children, gender-typed activity preferen-
ferred feminine activities) when in solitary play but activity ces peeked during preschool, a finding that is likely similar
preferences changed across social contexts. Specifically, for U.S. children (Servin et al. 1999).
girls played significantly more with masculine activities Despite these well-documented preferences, significant
when with male peers and boys played significantly more variability exists in preschool girls’ and boys’ involvement
with feminine activities during interactions with teachers. with gender-typed activities. Unfortunately, the sources of
Findings suggest that through social interactions with peers this variability are not well understood (Green et al. 2004).
and teachers, children are exposed to a greater range of One source of variability is the social situation in which the
activities than what they experience when they play by child engages in the activity, that is, whether a child is
themselves. playing with girls, boys, or is interacting with the teacher.
Understanding situational variability in children’s activity
Keywords Gender-typed activities . Gender differences . preferences is important because certain social situations
Preschool occur more frequently than others (e.g., play with same-
gender peers), thereby potentially influencing the rate at
which girls and boys engage with particular activities (e.g.,
P. Goble (*) : C. L. Martin : L. D. Hanish : R. A. Fabes
Martin and Fabes 2001).
Arizona State University,
Tempe, AZ, USA In the present study, we explored gender-typed activity
e-mail: Priscilla.Goble@asu.edu choices for a sample of U.S. preschoolers; importantly, we
C. L. Martin did so with an eye toward examining the situational vari-
e-mail: Carol.Martin@asu.edu ability in girls’ and boys’ involvement in stereotypically
L. D. Hanish feminine, stereotypically masculine, and gender-neutral play
e-mail: Laura.Hanish@asu.edu activities. Cognitive theories of gender development allow
R. A. Fabes for context sensitivity (e.g., Martin 2000), thereby suggest-
e-mail: RFabes@asu.edu ing that variability in children’s preferences for gender-
436 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

typed activities is to be expected depending on the social session, girls played with neutral and feminine activities at
context (Durkin 2005). Consistent with cognitive approaches, equally high rates, whereas boys primarily played with
we assumed that children vary their behaviors in relation to masculine activities, suggesting that boys are more rigid in
their immediate social context. Preschool classrooms provide their gender-typed activity preferences (Cherney et al.
numerous opportunities for children to move from solitary 2003). A few studies have also focused on the stability
play into playing in one of a variety of social contexts: with and consistency of children’s gender-typed activity prefer-
male peers, with female peers, with mixed-groups of both ences over time. These studies found that preferences re-
male and female peers, and with teachers. These contexts main stable or get stronger over early childhood (Golombok
afford children with opportunities to engage in different ac- et al. 2008; Maccoby and Jacklin 1987; Martin and Fabes
tivities (Fabes et al. 2003; Fagot 1978). 2001; Martin and Ruble 2009). For instance, in research
Exploring children’s gender-typed activity preferences conducted in southwest England, Golombok et al. (2008)
when playing alone, with same-, other-, and mixed-gender found that children who were most gender-typed in their
peers, and with teachers is important for understanding how activity preferences in preschool were the same children
social contexts may shape children’s exposure to various who were most gender-typed at age 8 years.
experiences that are likely to promote skill development Fewer studies have examined variability in children’s
during preschool. The current study builds on existing re- gender-typed activity preferences. Green et al. (2004) con-
search documenting the development of children’s gender- sidered children’s gender-typed activity preferences across
typed activity preferences and extends the work of research- both time (stability across time or within individual variabil-
ers in the U.S. and other countries by shedding light on ity) and after reading counter-stereotypic stories (across
sources of variability in children’s gender-typed activity situation variability). Results indicated that girls but not
preferences during preschool. We explored variability by boys showed variability in gender-typed activity preferences
comparing children’s gender-typed activity preferences over time and across situations. Although this study pro-
across various social contexts. All of the cited studies in vides insight into how children’s activity preferences vary
the following review of literature are based on U.S. samples across a specific gendered situation (reading counter-
unless otherwise noted. stereotypic stories), we know less about how children’s
gender-typed activity preferences vary across situations that
Children’s Gender-Typed Activities are more common in their everyday lives. For preschool-
aged children, gender-typed activity preferences may vary
Gender-typed activity preferences begin to emerge between considerably across the various social situations they en-
12 and 24 months, but early gender differences are found on counter in the preschool setting, but, to date, this has not
only a few activities (Caldera et al. 1989; O’Brien and been fully examined.
Huston 1985; Ruble and Martin 1998). As toddlers, boys
typically choose to play with blocks and balls, whereas girls The Social Context of Preschool and its Impact on Girls
tend to play with dolls and doll clothes (Ruble et al. 2006). and Boys
By the time they are in preschool, however, gender differ-
ences cover a wider array of activity choices; boys choose to As noted previously, the preschool classroom provides a
play with balls, bikes, blocks, and transportation toys, range of possible contexts in which children can play (i.e.,
whereas girls prefer art activities, dolls, kitchen items, and solitary play, play with peers, and interactions with teach-
dress-up clothes (see Ruble, et al. 2006 and Berenbaum et ers); each context may afford children different opportuni-
al. 2008 for a full review). ties to engage in particular activities. That is, preschool
Given that children’s gender-typed activity preferences children may vary their activity involvement and have very
have been well established for several decades, current different experiences in play with specific activities depend-
research on children’s gender-typed activity preferences fo- ing on whether or not the context involves others (social
cuses on the strength and rigidity, stability, and variability of contexts). For example, Holmes-Lonergan (2003) found that
activity preferences across gender, time, and context. Stud- preschool girls and boys varied their interaction styles to
ies examining these constructs, however, are relatively rare. accommodate their peer partner’s gender and the type of
Examining strength and rigidity of preferences, Powlishta et activity in which they were engaged. Specifically, in same-
al. (1993) showed that although both genders evidence gender dyads, girls were more likely to agree with each
gender-typed preferences, the strength of these preferences other than boys were in same-gender dyads; however, in
in the preschool years differs for boys and girls, with boys mixed-gender dyads the boys were more agreeable than the
showing stronger gender-typed activity preferences than girls. Furthermore, children altered their verbal and nonver-
girls. Similarly, in a study examining young children’s bal behaviors depending on the demands of the activity in
gender-typed activity preferences during a laboratory play which they were engaged so that there was more controlling
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 437

and mitigating behaviors in activities that required more with opportunities to explore activities typically preferred by
problem solving and collaboration. the other gender or activities that are gender-neutral. In partial
To accurately assess the role of preschool social contexts, support of this, Fabes et al. (2003) found that, for girls,
it is important to consider children’s gender-typed preferen- preferences were less gender-typed when playing with
ces when playing alone. Solitary play makes up about 25– other-gender groups than when playing with mixed-gender
45 % of preschool children’s activities during free play and peer groups. Conversely, for boys, preferences were more
is the preschool play context in which children’s activities gender-typed when playing with other-gender groups than
are least likely to be influenced by others, as compared to when playing with mixed-gender peer groups. In other words,
other preschool contexts such as during play with peers or girls engaged in fewer feminine activities when playing with
interactions with teachers (Rubin et al. 1978). When alone, boys than when playing with a group of boys and girls,
boys prefer masculine activities and girls prefer feminine whereas boys engaged in more masculine activities when
activities (Ruble et al. 2006), but as the Cherney et al. playing with girls than they did when playing with a group
(2003) study showed, the genders differ in how often they of both girls and boys.
play in neutral activities. Girls play in neutral activities at Although many of the findings from these studies are
rates that are almost equal to their rates of engagement in consistent with what one would expect, some of the findings
feminine activities whereas boys play in neutral activities were surprising. As we would expect, boys were found to
less than they play in masculine activities. Accordingly, in engage in masculine activities more with groups of male
the solitary play context we might expect that girls and boys than female peers. Contrary to what we would expect,
would have strong gender-typed activity preferences, but engagement in masculine activities was higher with groups
that girls also will engage in high levels of neutral activity. of female peers than mixed-gender peer groups where boys
To our knowledge, only two studies have considered how were playing with both male and female peers. However, in
the preschool peer social context (including play with male-, the Fabes et al. (2003) study, comparisons of children’s
female-, mixed-gender peer groups) may enhance or mini- levels of gender-typed activity involvement were only made
mize children’s tendencies to play with feminine, masculine, between social contexts (e.g., play with same-gender peers
and neutral activities (Martin and Fabes 2001; Fabes et al. versus play with other-gender peer groups), and there was
2003). These studies found that girls and boys demonstrated no measurement of engagement in masculine or feminine
a strong preference for engaging in social interactions with activities when children were playing alone from which to
same-gender peers and that children in same-gender play compare each social context. To more fully understand the
groups tended to engage with gender-typed toys (e.g., extent to which changes occur in different social contexts as
groups of boys engage in masculine activities; Fabes et al. compared to solitary contexts, it is necessary to compare
2003). Martin and Fabes (2001) showed that, for both girls children’s behavior with and without the influence of peers.
and boys, there was a positive relation between playing with Given this, we examined variability across social contexts
same-gender peers in the fall and increased gender-typed relative to children’s baseline levels of activity involvement
activity preferences in the spring even after controlling for during solitary play.
gender-typed preferences in the fall, suggesting that both In addition to the peer social context, research suggests
girls’ and boys’ activity choices were influenced by their that interactions with teachers have the potential to influence
interactions with same-gender peers. Moreover, when com- children’s gender-typed activity preferences. In general, pre-
paring play in gender-typed activities in same-gender versus school teachers are female and they tend to spend more time
mixed- and other-gender peer groups, both boys and girls participating in feminine-typed activities over masculine-
engaged in significantly more gender-typed activities in typed activities, likely because many feminine activities
same-gender peer groups (Fabes et al. 2003). Based on these are academically based (e.g., reading, writing, art, etc.;
findings, we expected that when children play with same- Fabes et al. 2003; Fagot 1978). This finding is particularly
gender play partners they would have a stronger preference for important because children are more likely to engage in
gender-typed activities than they would during solitary play. activities in which teachers are involved (i.e., feminine
Fabes et al. (2003) explored the nature of children’s activities). Specifically, research conducted in Germany
interactions when they were in mixed-gender (i.e., playing and the U.S. shows that children’s activity preferences have
with both girls and boys) and other-gender (i.e., playing been correlated with teacher proximity suggesting that
only with members of the other gender) peer groups. teacher presence is influential (Oettingen 1985; Tomes
Although neither of these social contexts occurred as 1995). In particular, when teachers were present in academic
often did same-gender peer groups, children interacted areas, children were more likely to play in those areas,
in mixed-gender peer groups more often than they inter- suggesting that teacher location has an effect on children’s
acted with other-gender peers. Peer interactions with activity preferences. Moreover, it has been demonstrated
other- and mixed-gender peers likely provide children through the manipulation of teacher location in a classroom
438 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

that teacher presence in activity areas has an impact on children would have a tendency to behave in gender-
children’s gender-typed activity choices. Indeed, teacher consistent ways but that these tendencies would be moderated
presence increased rates of play significantly across both by expectations of one’s play partner activity preferences.
masculine- and feminine-typed activities, although the find- Given the findings from the research by Fabes and col-
ing was stronger for girls than for boys (Serbin et al. 1981). leagues, we might also expect children’s tendency engage in
different activities in various social contexts to be moderat-
Theoretical Background for Exploring Social Contextual ed by children’s own gender. In support of this idea, Durkin
Variations in Gender-typed Activity Preferences (2005) suggested that the interaction between gender
schemas and intergroup processes are differentiated by gen-
Cognitive theorists have argued that children develop gen- der. Durkin suggested that boys have more rigid gender
der schemas (organized networks of mental representations schemas and a stronger desire to solicit positive social
about gender) that then influence preferences for gender- evaluations than girls. For example, in a UK study where
typed activities (e.g., Martin 2000; Martin et al. 2011; 64 children (ages 4–9 years) were asked to show their
Martin and Halverson, 1981). Specifically, gender schema activity preferences once alone and once in front of a group
theorists posit that because gender is extremely salient in our of same-gender peers, researchers showed that young boys
culture, children use gender as a framework to process new were particularly gender-typed in their activity preferences
information in a manner that often conforms to societal in front of the peer audience (Banerjee and Lintern 2000).
expectations. Based on Gender Schema Theory (GST), once Based on the ideas presented in this section and on
children know their own gender they become motivated to previous research, we expected some similarities and some
learn more about gender. Children develop their gender differences between girls and boys. Grounded in previous
schemas through exposure to information about gender from research and GST, we expected that in a situation where
their environment (e.g., through parents, peers, media), and children are playing alone, such as during solitary play, both
through their own experiences. They then use gender girls and boys would engage in gender-typed activities (e.g.,
schemas to organize the information and to guide their feminine for girls) at higher rates than they engage in
future behavior. The use of gender schemas to guide behav- gender-atypical activities. Similarly, we would expect that
ior has been confirmed in studies of novel objects (e.g., relative to solitary play, when playing with same-gender
kitchen tools) in which researchers varied gender labels for peer groups, girls and boys would increase engagement in
the objects (e.g., other girls really like this toy). Children gender-typed activities. However, we expected more varia-
paid more attention to and played more with objects labeled tion between girls and boys during play with other-gender
for their gender than objects labeled for the other gender peer groups. Specifically, research by Fabes et al. (2003)
(e.g., Bradbard et al. 1986; Martin et al. 1995). and Durkin’s (2005) perspective on the interaction between
More recently, cognitive approaches have been expanded child gender and GST suggest that when playing with other-
to provide a framework for understanding variability in gender peers, girls but not boys would be expected to
children’s gendered behavior (e.g., activity preferences) engage in gender-atypical activities (e.g., masculine) at
across social contexts (Martin 2000; Durkin 2005). As an higher rates than during solitary play. Additionally, boys
extension of gender schema theory, Martin (2000) suggested but not girls would be expected to engage in even more
that children may develop broader theories of gender (i.e., a gender-typed activities (i.e., masculine) during play with
“naïve social psychology”) that guide children’s interpreta- other-gender peers than during solitary play. As suggested
tion of social environments. The main tenets of this idea are by previous research we also expected that in mixed-gender
that children develop “within-group similarity” theories peer groups both girls and boys would increase levels of
(girls are the same) and “between-group differences” theo- engagement in neutral activities. Finally, previous research
ries (girls and boys are different) and that these gender suggests that teachers spend more time in feminine activities
essentialist theories inform children’s decisions about how than other activities and girls and boys are more likely to
to act in various social situations. Ruble et al. (2004) also play with activities when teachers are present in the play area.
suggested that this type of intergroup process or a collective Accordingly, we expected that, during interactions with teach-
identity likely guides children to behave in gender consis- ers, girls and boys would engage in more play with feminine
tent ways. According to these ideas, children may act in activities than they would during solitary play.
gender-consistent ways when with a group of same-gender We suggest that variability in activity choices across
peers and in gender-inconsistent ways when with other- situations may be due to children’s developing theories of
gender peers because of their assumptions about the social gender and their perceptions of social environments. Unfor-
context. Based on these ideas, we would expect that children tunately, most research has treated gender-typed differences
would modify their behavior depending on the gender of in play preferences as a stable attribute and only a few
their social partners. Specifically, we would expect that studies have considered variability due to social context.
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 439

The present research contributes to the literature by provid- activities across social contexts. We expected that children’s
ing additional evidence concerning variability in activities social contexts would either increase or decrease their base-
across social contexts. line play tendencies, depending on the identity of the social
partner and children’s own gender. Given that most research
Present Study and theory regarding variability in children's gender-typed
activity preferences across social situations focus on femi-
In the current study we explored how the social context in nine and masculine activities, the majority of the following
preschool impacts girls’ and boys’ masculine, feminine, and hypotheses also focus on feminine and masculine activities.
neutral activity choices. To expand upon the research con- Research by Fabes et al. (2003), however, provides some
ducted by Fabes et al. (2003), we compared children’s support for expected changes in neutral activity engagement
behavior in various social contexts with their behavior in for children when playing with mixed-gender peer groups.
solitary play, allowing us to assess the degree to which Separate hypotheses were made regarding many possible
social situations contribute to variability in children’s play. effects for children within each social context. When no a
We also expanded the types of social contexts studied by priori hypothesis was made, this was also noted.
considering how children behave with teachers. Understand-
ing how children’s activity preferences vary when with H3 Consistent with theory and research, girls were expected to
teachers is important because teachers’ ability to influence increase levels of engagement in feminine activities and
children’s activity preferences may have implications for decrease levels of engagement in masculine activities dur-
practice (Oettingen 1985; Tomes 1995). In summary, the ing play with female peers relative to rates of feminine and
present study builds on the Fabes et al. (2003) study by masculine activity engagement during solitary play. No a
examining children’s gender-typed activity preferences in a priori hypotheses were made for girls’ engagement in
variety of social contexts including peers and teachers. neutral activities when playing with female peers relative
Furthermore, the study expands upon the earlier research to their neutral activity engagement during solitary play.
by comparing children’s preferences in social contexts to H4 Due to the rigidity of boys’ gender schemas and to
their preferences in solitary play. findings from previous research, boys were expected to
The first goal of this study was to explore the degree to increase levels of engagement in masculine activities
which boys and girls exhibit gender-typed activity preferences during play with female peers relative to levels of mas-
when engaged in solitary play. For these analyses the depen- culine activity engagement during solitary play. No a
dent variable was within-subject differences between levels of priori hypotheses were made for boys’ engagement in
play in feminine, masculine, and neutral activities during the feminine or neutral activities when playing with female
solitary play context. Based on theory and previous research, peers relative to their feminine and neutral activity en-
two hypotheses were developed to address this goal. gagement during solitary play.
H5 During play with male peers, however, girls and boys were
H1 Activity involvement would be largely gender-typed dur- both expected to increase levels of engagement in mascu-
ing solitary play, such that boys would play with mascu- line activities and decrease levels of feminine activity
line activities more than with feminine activities, and girls engagement relative to levels of engagement in masculine
would play with feminine activities more than masculine and feminine activities during solitary play. No a priori
activities. hypotheses were made for girls’ or boys’ engagement in
H2 Based on previous research suggesting that girls play neutral activities when playing with male peers relative to
with feminine and neutral activities at equally high their neutral activity engagement during solitary play.
rates, girls were also expected to play with neutral H6 Although theory is lacking to guide this hypothesis,
activities more than masculine activities. based on previous research, play with mixed-gender
peer groups was expected to increase engagement in
The second goal of this study focused on understanding neutral activities compared to neutral activity engage-
the degree to which preschoolers’ involvement in mascu- ment in solitary play for girls and boys. No a priori
line, feminine, and neutral activities changed as a function hypotheses were made for girls’ or boys’ engagement
of their preschool social context. Analyses comparing girls’ in feminine or masculine activities when playing with
and boys’ activity preferences when playing with male-, mixed-gender peer groups relative to their feminine and
female-, and mixed-gender peer groups, and teachers rela- masculine activity engagement during solitary play.
tive to preferences in solitary play allowed us to examine the H7 Guided by research and theory, we expected that inter-
influence of these differing classroom contexts. In these actions with teachers would increase girls’ and boys’
analyses, the dependent variable was within-subject change play with feminine activities and decrease play with
in the levels of play in feminine, masculine and neutral masculine activities relative to their levels of engagement
440 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

with feminine and masculine activities in solitary play. Table 1 Demographic characteristics
No a priori hypotheses were made for girls’ or boys’ Girls Boys
engagement in neutral activities during interactions with
teachers relative to their neutral activity engagement # % # %
during solitary play.
Ethnicity
African-American 7 5.30 % 11 8.30 %
Anglo-American 11 8.40 % 10 7.50 %
Method
Native American 2 1.50 % 2 1.50 %
Participants Asian 3 2.30 % 2 1.50 %
Hispanic 93 71.00 % 99 74.40 %
Participants were preschool children enrolled in 18 Head Other 3 2.30 % 2 1.50 %
Start classrooms in an urban southwestern city in the US. Unknown 12 9.20 % 7 5.30 %
Classrooms were selected in collaboration with Phoenix and Marital status
Maricopa County Head Start Programs, and families were Married, never divorced 55 51.80 % 45 38.80 %
recruited 2–3 weeks into the start of the academic school Divorced 6 5.40 % 10 9.90 %
year at pre-arranged parent meetings. Consent rates were Separated 5 5.40 % 5 5.00 %
99 % at recruitment (N0308 out of a possible 311). Children Widowed 0 0 1 0.80 %
who were chronically absent or who left in the fall semester Single, never married 13 11.60 % 21 18.20 %
were dropped from the analyses (n028). Children who Together, never married 25 25.00 % 30 24.80 %
repeated preschool during years 2 and 3 of data collection Other 1 0.80 % 2 2.50 %
(n016) had data collected twice; only data from the second Socioeconomic status
year was used in analyses to prevent dependencies. The Under 10,000 17 17.00 % 25 25.00 %
final sample consisted of N0264 preschoolers (M age0 10–20,000 44 42.90 % 32 29.50 %
52 months; range: 37–60 months). Approximately half of 20–30,000 13 21.00 % 31 29.50 %
the participants were girls (49.6 %). The ratio of girls to 30–40,000 10 11.40 % 11 9.80 %
boys per classroom ranged from 31 % to 62 % girls, and 40,000 plus 8 7.70 % 7 6.30 %
85 % of the teachers were female. The majority of partic-
ipants (73 %) were Mexican or Mexican-American, most of Girls and boys did not significantly differ on any demographic char-
acteristics, ts(215–262)0−.61 to 1.37, p>.05
whom were Spanish-speaking; 62 % of the participants
primarily spoke Spanish. The remaining participants were
Anglo-American (8 %), African-American (7 %), and other and semi-structured class activities (e.g., a limited choice
(12 %). Participants were predominately of low socioeconom- between activities) were included in the analyses. Observa-
ic status (82 % earned below $30,000 per year). Almost half of tions which occurred during teacher structured large or small
the children (45 %) came from two-parent married families, group activities were not included in analyses as these times
with the remaining children from single parent homes (see do not reflect children’s freedom of choice for peers or activi-
Table 1 for a full breakdown of participant demographic data ties. Trained classroom observers (8–10 per year; 92 % female)
by child gender). Independent-samples t-tests were conducted focused on individual children in a random order for 10 s and
to examine gender differences on the demographic variables. recorded children’s primary play activity using a checklist of 29
Independent-samples t-tests examining gender differences available activities, including masculine (e.g., balls and bikes),
revealed no significant differences between girls and boys feminine (e.g., kitchen and dress-up), and gender-neutral activ-
on any of the demographic variables. ities (e.g., board games and sensory activities; see Appendix for
a complete list and description of activities). Observers also
Procedures and Measures recorded the nature of children’s social contexts. For the pur-
pose of the present study, relevant social context codes includ-
As part of a larger investigation, data were collected using an ed: playing alone (solitary play), playing with teachers, playing
observation protocol in which children were observed indoors with male peers, playing with female peers, or playing with
and outdoors several times per week, in 10-second segments, both male and female peers. To be coded as an interaction with
over the fall and spring semesters of preschool. This method a peer or teacher, there had to be a verbal or physical exchange
has been used successfully to document preschool children’s during the 10-sec observation. Unoccupied behaviors (e.g.,
activities and play partners (see Martin and Fabes 2001). Only child is alone with lack of focus or intent) were also coded.
those observations that occurred during free-play (e.g., chil- A total of 42,615 observations were collected for this
dren freely decide what to do, with whom, and where to do it) sample (M0134 observations per child, SD065; range0
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 441

30–325). The large range in observations recorded for each differences in each of the 29 individual activities using
child was due to differences in attendance and availability of independent-samples t-tests (with modified degrees of free-
the children. Only 7 % of the sample had fewer than 100 dom, to correct for heterogeneity in variances; see Table 2).
observations. To determine reliability, two observers inde- One advantage of this method of classifying activities is
pendently coded the same child’s behavior. Based on 6,481 that play categories are derived directly from the sample
simultaneous observations (15 % of all observations), kappas
were .81 for solitary play, .80 for play with males, .80 for play
with females, .82 for play with mixed-gender peer groups, and
Table 2 Descriptive statistics for the proportion of time spent in each
.91 for interactions with teacher codes and .92 for feminine, activity across all social contexts
.87 for masculine, and .90 for neutral activity codes.
Child Gender
Categorization of Social Context
Girls Boys

The social contexts of interest were solitary play, play with Activity M SD M SD
male, female-, and mixed-gender peer groups or dyads, and
interactions with teachers. Children were coded as engaged Feminine
in solitary play if the target child was playing alone and Art 0.12 0.06 0.06 0.04
engaging in either “constructive” play (e.g., building, color- Music 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.01
ing, reading a book) or “nonconstructive” play (e.g., jump- Puzzle 0.04 0.04 0.03 0.03
ing, bouncing a ball, playing with a toy in a repetitive Books 0.03 0.02 0.02 0.02
manner). Times when children were transitioning between Writing 0.02 0.02 0.01 0.02
activities, onlooking (i.e., watching other children play), or Phone 0.00 0.01 0.00 0.00
unoccupied behavior (i.e., lack of focus or intent) were not Dress-up 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.01
included in the coding of solitary play. Categorization of Kitchen 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.02
social context was based on with whom a target child was Pretend Feminine 0.04 0.03 0.00 0.01
playing, not based on the composition of the group as a Figure Play Feminine 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.01
whole. This approach has been consistently used in the Masculine
literature and allows for the distinction between mixed- Balls 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.03
gender and other-gender play (Colwell and Lindsey 2005; Bikes 0.05 0.05 0.07 0.06
Fabes et al. 2003). For example, if a target girl was playing Blocks 0.04 0.03 0.07 0.06
with one or more girls, this would be classified as play with Computer 0.05 0.04 0.07 0.05
females. If a girl was playing with one or more boys, this Toy Animals 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.03
would be classified as play with males. Alternatively, if a target Toy Vehicles 0.01 0.01 0.04 0.04
girl was playing with at least one boy and at least one girl, this Pretend Masculine 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.03
would be classified as mixed-gender peer group play. Using Figure Play Masculine 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.01
codes for the five social contexts (solitary play, play with male, Figure Play Neutral 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02
female and mixed-gender peer groups, and interactions with Neutral
teachers) separate data files were created for each of the social Clay 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.03
contexts at the non-aggregated level. Data were then aggregat- Board Games 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02
ed within each file. This allowed for the computation of Digging 0.04 0.03 0.03 0.03
gender-typed activity preferences based on observations in Large Motor 0.05 0.00 0.05 0.00
each specific context. After all variables were created, the five Math/Science 0.02 0.00 0.02 0.00
separate context specific data files were remerged for analyses. Other 0.10 0.05 0.11 0.06
Pretend Neutral 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03
Calculation of Feminine, Masculine, and Neutral Activity Sensory 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.03
Scores Snack 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.02
TV 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
Proportion scores for frequency of involvement in an activ-
ity (e.g., balls) divided by total observations across all T-tests were used rather than an overall MANOVA due to data dependen-
activities were created for each of the 29 individual activi- cy in the activity variables, such that activities sum to 1 resulting in a lack
of variance from which to estimate group differences (Tabachnick and
ties, this method is consistent with methods used in previous Fidell 2006). Activities categorized as Feminine or Masculine significantly
research (Martin and Fabes 2001). To classify the activities differ between girls and boys at p<.05. Mean values are based on propor-
as feminine, masculine, and neutral, we tested for gender tion scores ranging from 0 to 1. df for t-tests ranged from 130 to 260.71.
442 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

(Connor and Serbin 1977). Ten activities were categorized To assess children’s tendencies to engage in each type of
as feminine (art, music, puzzles, books, writing, phone, activity in solitary contexts, similar scores were calculated
dress-up, kitchen, pretend feminine, figure play feminine), using the proportions of times children engaged in activities
ts(151.25–260.69) ranged from −12.34 to −1.97, ps<.05, while in solitary play as the denominator (for this, we also
and nine activities were categorized as masculine (balls, included children’s interactions during unoccupied play in
bikes, blocks, computer, toy animals, toy vehicles, pretend the denominator to ensure that the three gender-typed activ-
masculine, figure play masculine, figure play neutral), ts ity scores—feminine, masculine, neutral—did not sum to 1).
(140.36–260.71) ranged from 3.06 to 9.57, ps<.01. The Thus, separate composite scores were computed for child-
remaining 10 activities were categorized as neutral (clay, ren’s engagement in feminine, masculine, and neutral activ-
board games, digging, large motor, math/science activities, ities while playing alone. The descriptive statistics for all
other, pretend neutral, sensory, snack, tv), ts(132–260.21) composite scores can be seen in Table 3.
ranged from −1.85 to 1.72, all ns (see Table 2 for activity To compare children’s activities during solitary play and
means by child gender). Composite scores for each activity each of the social contexts, difference scores were computed
category (feminine, masculine, neutral) were created for by subtracting the activity composite score in solitary play
each child by totaling the number of observed times children from the same activity composite score in each of the social
engaged in each play activity (e.g., art+books+music, etc.) play contexts. For example, the score representing engagement
in each category (e.g., feminine) and dividing by the total in feminine activities in solitary play was subtracted from the
number of times they engaged in all 29 activities within the score representing engagement in feminine activities with male
relevant context. For example, the number of times a child peers. Similarly, the score representing engagement in femi-
engaged with each of the 11 feminine play activities in the nine activities in solitary play was separately subtracted from
context of playing with girls was summed and divided by the the score representing engagement in feminine activities with
sum of the number of times that child engaged in all 29 female peers, and so on. (For the creation of difference scores,
activities while playing with girls. In this example, the com- we removed unoccupied play from the denominator for the
posite reflects the proportion of feminine activities the child solitary play activity composite scores to ensure comparability
engaged in while playing with girls out of all activities en- in denominators prior to calculating difference scores.) These
gaged. To assess the number of times children engaged in difference scores represented the degree to which children’s
feminine, masculine, and neutral activities within each social engagement in gender-typed activities varied as a function of
context, the denominators alternately represented the total context. Thus, for example, if a child received a positive
number of times children engaged in all activities in play with difference score for engaging in dress up when playing with
male peers, female peers, mixed- gender peers, and with female peers, this indicated that the child played more dress up
teachers, resulting in a total of 12 variables. when with female peers than when in solitary play. Difference

Table 3 Mean scores and standard deviations for each activity in each context

Context

Activity Solitary Male peers Female peers Mixed-gender peer groups Teacher

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Girls
Feminine 0.35 0.12 0.19*** 0.16 0.32* 0.13 0.25*** 0.24 0.27*** 0.13
* *** ***
Masculine 0.24 0.13 0.28 0.21 0.17 0.11 0.18 0.20 0.07*** 0.07
Neutral 0.40 0.13 0.35* 0.20 0.33*** 0.11 0.47** 0.26 0.32*** 0.14
Boys
Feminine 0.18 0.09 0.08*** 0.06 0.15* 0.13 0.16 0.20 0.23*** 0.15
Masculine 0.43 0.15 0.45 0.15 0.27*** 0.19 0.29*** 0.25 0.13*** 0.11
Neutral 0.38 0.13 0.33*** 0.14 0.39 0.21 0.47*** 0.26 0.30*** 0.15

Mean scores in each social context that were significantly different from mean scores in solitary play indicated using asterisks (see text for statistics,
ts (130–132)01.98–22.73). Mean values are based on proportion scores ranging from 0 to 1
*p<.05
**p<.01
***p<.001
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 443

scores were used in the overall analyses examining gender- typed (e.g., feminine for girls) and (2) girls would play
typed activity preferences across social contexts. The use of with neutral activities at higher rates than masculine
difference scores, although controversial, is necessary in these activities. To do this, we conducted a repeated measures
analyses as they provide meaningful information about the analysis of variance (ANOVA) with one between-subjects
direction of change across contexts (Maguire 1999). factor (gender of child: boy, girl) and one within-subject
factor (activity category within solitary context: feminine
activities, masculine activities, neutral activities). The
Results sphericity assumption was not met, resulting in a loss of
power, so the Huynh-Feldt correction was applied (Huynh
The goal of the study was to assess how children’s tendencies and Feldt, 1976). Results revealed a significant gender of
to engage in gender-typed activities (feminine, masculine, and child by activity category interaction, F(1.85, 484.35)0
neutral) during preschool differed according to the social 89.82, p<.001. Simple effects tests were done for each
context of play with peers and teachers. Before making these gender separately. For both genders, the main effect for
comparisons, children’s patterns of play when alone were activities was significant, for girls, F(2, 260) 091.01,
examined. Subsequently, we used children’s gender-typed p<.001; for boys, F(1.6, 211.27)035.54, p<.001. Again,
activity preferences in solitary play as a baseline (i.e., child- the sphericity assumption was not met, so the Huynh-
ren’s baseline tendencies to engage in gender-typed activities) Feldt correction was applied.
and examined the degree to which engagement in gender- To compare children’s play patterns for different
typed activities varied as a function of their social context types of activities, Tukey’s HSD test (within-subjects)
(play with male peers, with female peers, with mixed-gender was used to make pairwise comparisons of activities.
peer groups, interactions with teachers). The Huynh-Feldt corrected mean square error and
degrees of freedom were used in calculating the HSD
Preliminary Analyses critical values. Effect sizes for these comparisons were
calculated using the d statistic. The interpretation of this
Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine the de- index suggests that a d of .20 is a small effect, d of .50
scriptive statistics, skew and kurtosis of all activity variables is a medium effect, and a d of .80 or greater is a large
in all contexts (feminine, masculine, neutral; play with male effect (Cohen 1988; Rosnow and Rosenthal 1989).
peers, with female peers, with mixed-gender peer groups, These analyses provided support for the hypothesis that chil-
interactions with teachers). For all created activity variables, dren play with gender-consistent activities during solitary
skew and kurtosis were low, indicating all variables were play. During solitary play, girls engaged most in feminine
normally distributed (Tabachnick and Fidel, 2006). activities (M0.30, SD0.11) and neutral activities (M0.34,
Additionally, independent-samples t-tests were conducted SD0.12); the difference between these was not significant.
to examine ethnicity and SES differences on the study varia- As expected, girls engaged in feminine and neutral activities
bles (i.e., gender typed activities and social contexts) and by significantly more than masculine activities (M 0.20,
child gender. Independent-samples t-tests examining ethnicity SD0.11), ps<.01, and the effect sizes for these were large,
differences revealed that Hispanic and non-Hispanic children ds01.22 and .91 respectively. Boys engaged in masculine
did not significantly differ on any of the activity variables in activities during solitary play (M0.37, SD0.14) significantly
any social context, ts(262)0range .05 to 1.7, ps>.05. Addi- more than in feminine activities (M0.15, SD0.09) and neutral
tionally, there were no differences in ethnicity between girls activities (M0.32, SD0.12), ps<.05, effect sizes were large
and boys (see Table 1 for analyses by child gender). Results and small; ds01.87 and .38, respectively.
also revealed that high and low poverty children did not
significantly differ on any study variables including activities Analyses of Gender-Typed Activities Across Social
within social contexts, ts(202)0range .02 to 1.8, ps>.05. Ad- Contexts
ditionally, there were no differences in SES by child gender
(see Table 1 for analyses by child gender). Given that ethnicity To address the second goal of this study, the social contexts
and SES differences were not found for any of the study of children’s play were explored and the five remaining
variables, neither ethnicity nor SES were used as control hypotheses were tested (H3–H7). A repeated measures anal-
variables in analyses testing the main study hypotheses. ysis of variance was conducted on the difference scores for
each type of activity using one between-subjects factor
Analyses of Gender-Typed Activities in Solitary Play (gender) and two within-subject factors, play context (play
with male peers, with female peers, with mixed-gender peer
We first addressed the hypotheses that (1) for girls and groups and interactions with teachers) and activity category
boys activity play while alone would be largely gender- (feminine, masculine, neutral; see Table 4). The sphericity
444 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

Table 4 Repeated measures analysis of variance in activity difference between each social context (when playing with male peers,
scores across context
female peers, mixed-gender peer groups, and interacting
Source df F with teacher) and solitary play. For these analyses, to im-
prove interpretability, the gender-typed composite scores for
I. Solitary Analyses feminine, masculine, and neutral activities were used rather
Activities 1.85, 484.35 37.89* than difference scores. For example, the mean proportion of
Activities X Gender 1.85, 484.35 89.82* observations with feminine activities in each social context
II. Context Analyses (when playing with male peers, female peers, mixed-gender
Activities 1.92, 502.53 17.31* peer groups and interacting with teacher) was compared to the
Activities X Gender 1.92, 502.53 14.29* mean proportion of observations with feminine activities
Activities X Context 4.71, 1233.29 55.74* when in solitary play. Because the focus was on exploring
Activities X Gender X Context 4.71, 1233.29 5.42* how involvement in feminine, masculine, and neutral activi-
ties varied by social context and child gender, the results are
Degrees of freedom adjusted using Huyn-Feldt correction
organized by social context. Effect sizes for these comparisons
*p<.001
were calculated using the d statistic. The descriptive statistics
for the proportion scores reported here can be seen in Table 3.

assumption was not met, so the Huynh-Feldt correction was Play with Female Peers
again applied. There was a significant main effect for activ-
ity category, F(1.98, 517.36)0122.92, p<.001, and signifi- Recall that for girls, play with female peers was
cant two-way interactions for gender of child by activity expected to increase levels of engagement in feminine
category, F (1.98, 517.36)012.17, p <.001, and activity activities and decrease levels of engagement in mascu-
category by context, F(4.91, 1258.63)0105.37, p<.001. line activities relative to rates of feminine and masculine
However, these were subsumed by a significant three-way activity engagement during solitary play (H3). Contrary
gender of child by activity category by context interaction, F to H3, the paired t-test analyses revealed that there was
(4.91, 1258.63)04.94, p<.001. Simple effect analyses of the no significant difference between girls’ engagement with
3-way interaction were done by testing for the two-way feminine activities during solitary play versus feminine
interaction between activities and context for each gender activity engagement when playing with female peers.
separately. For both girls and boys, there were significant However, in support of H3, girls’ engagement with
two-way interactions of activities and context, F(4.38, masculine activities occurred significantly less when
569.96)040.38, p<.001 and F(4.9, 646.8)069.25, p<.001, playing with female peers (d0.44) than when engaged
respectively. in solitary play, t(130) 05.0, p < .001. Although no a
Further simple effect analyses were conducted within priori hypotheses were made regarding girls’ engage-
gender to examine whether the activity category effect ment in neutral activities when playing with female
(e.g., feminine activity play) was significant within each peers relative to neutral activity engagement in solitary
context (e.g., girls with male peers, girls with female peers, play, engagement with neutral activities occurred significant-
etc.). For both girls and boys, there was a significant main ly less when playing with female peers (d0.48) than when
effect of activities when playing with male peers, F(2, playing in solitary play, t(130)05.41, p<.001.
260)03.88, p<.05 and F(1.86, 244.85)07.02, p<.001, re- Because boys have more rigid gender schemas and
spectively. The activity effect was also significant for both because of findings from previous research, it was
girls and boys when playing with female peers F(2, 260)0 expected that during play with female peers, boys
53.85, p<.001 and F(1.98, 260.89)050.47, p<.001, respec- would increase levels of engagement in masculine ac-
tively; when playing with mixed-gender peer groups, F tivities relative to levels of masculine activity engage-
(1.89, 245.69)05.56, p<.005 and F(1.94, 256.65)023.68, ment during solitary play (H4). Contrary to our
p<.001, respectively; and when interacting with teachers F hypothesis, boys’ engagement with masculine activities
(1.93, 251.03) 0132.75, p < .001 and F(1.94, 256.15) 0 occurred significantly less when playing with female
366.23, p<.001, respectively. To probe these interactions, peers (d 0.79) than when playing in solitary play,
paired t-tests were conducted to compare patterns of child- t(132)09.07, p<.001. For boys, no a priori hypotheses
ren’s activity preferences (e.g., feminine) during each social were made regarding engagement in feminine or neutral
context (e.g., play with female peers) to the same activity activities when playing with female peers relative to
during solitary play. feminine and neutral activity engagement in solitary
Specifically, for each gender-typed activity category play. Nonetheless, it was found that boys’ engagement
(feminine, masculine, neutral), a comparison was made in feminine activities occurred significantly less often
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 445

when playing with female peers (d0.17) than during than when playing alone, ts(130)04.09 and 3.25, respec-
solitary play, t(132)01.98, p<.05. There was no signif- tively, ps<.001. Similar to the finding for girls, and unex-
icant difference in levels of engagement in neutral ac- pectedly, the proportion of boys’ engagement in masculine
tivities relative to solitary play for boys when playing activities was less during mixed-gender peer group interac-
with female peers. tions relative to when they played alone (d0.59), t(132)0
6.55, p<.001. There was no significant difference in levels
Play with Male Peers of engagement in feminine activities relative to solitary play
for boys when playing with a mixed-gender peer groups.
For girls and boys, play with male peers was expected
to increase levels of engagement in masculine activities Interactions with Teachers
and decrease levels of feminine activity engagement
relative to levels of engagement in masculine and fem- Guided by previous research, we expected that interac-
inine activities during solitary play (H5). Hypothesis 5 tions with teachers would increase girls’ and boys’ play
was fully supported for girls and partially supported for with feminine activities and decrease play with masculine
boys, Specifically, girls’ engagement with masculine activities relative to their levels of engagement with fem-
activities occurred significantly more when playing with inine and masculine activities in solitary play (H7). Con-
male peers (d0.19) than when engaged in solitary play, trary to our expectation, however, girls’ engagement in
t(130)02.1, p < .05. Contrary to our hypothesis, there feminine activities was significantly less likely to occur
was no significant difference in the proportion of boys’ during interactions with teachers (d01.38) than it was to
masculine activity engagement relative to solitary play occur in solitary play, t(130)015.77, p<.001. Girls’ en-
when playing with male peers. However, engagement in gagement with masculine activities also occurred signifi-
feminine activities occurred significantly less during cantly less when interacting with teachers (d01.13) than
play with male peers than during solitary play, for girls when playing in solitary play, t(130)012.36, p<.001, as
(d0.89) and boys (d0.94), ts(130 and 132)010.02 and expected. Consistent with H7, boys’ engagement in fem-
10.49, respectively, ps<.001. Again, no a priori hypoth- inine activities occurred significantly more when interact-
eses were made for girls’ or boys’ engagement in neu- ing with teachers (d02.20) than when engaged in solitary
tral activities when playing with male peers relative to play, t(132)03.846, p<.001. Furthermore, boys’ engage-
their neutral activity engagement during solitary play. ment with masculine activities occurred significantly less
Surprisingly, however, the proportion of engagement when interacting with teachers (d02.21) than when play-
with neutral activities occurred significantly less often ing in solitary play, t(132)022.73, p<.001. Surprisingly,
when playing with male peers than during solitary play given that no hypotheses were made regarding neutral
for girls (d0.23) and boys (d0.31), ts(130 and 132)0 activities, for girls (d0.42) and boys (d0.46), engage-
2.59 and 3.58, ps<.05 and .001, respectively. ment with neutral activities occurred significantly less
when interacting with teachers than when playing in
Play with Mix-Gender Peer Groups solitary play, ts(130 and 132)04.81 and 5.28, respectively,
ps<.001.
Play with mixed-gender peer groups was expected to in-
crease engagement in neutral activities compared to neutral
activity engagement in solitary play for girls and boys (H6). Discussion
In line with this hypothesis, girls’ engagement in neutral
activities occurred significantly more in mixed-gender peer The goal of this study was to investigate preschool
groups (d0.25) than when observed in solitary play, t(130)0 children’s gender-typed activity preferences and how
2.7, p<.01. Similarly, boys’ engagement with neutral activ- these varied across different preschool contexts–solitary
ities occurred significantly more when playing with mixed- play, play with female peers, male peers, play with
gender peer groups (d0.35) than when engaged in solitary mixed-gender peer groups, and interacting with teachers.
play, t(132)0−3.71, p<.001. There were also a number of Both girls and boys in this study preferred gender-
significant findings for girls’ and boys’ engagement in fem- typical activities to gender-atypical activities (e.g., girls
inine and masculine activities during play with mix-gender preferred feminine activities) when in solitary play.
peer groups relative to their engagement in feminine and However, children’s preferences for gender-typed activi-
masculine activity engagement during solitary play that ties were found to vary in strength depending upon their
were not expected. For girls, engagement in feminine activ- social contexts.
ities (d0.53) and masculine activities (d0.29) occurred sig- Overall, the findings of the present study generally were
nificantly less during play with mixed-gender peer groups consistent with gender-schema theories, previous research,
446 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

and the study hypotheses. Girls and boys tended to prefer at home or in the laboratory, with a limited set of play
gender-typed activities when playing alone. Relative to sol- materials available and for a limited period of time
itary play, play with male peers increased children’s play (Campbell et al. 2002; Roopnarine 1986; Servin et al.
with masculine activities, play with mixed-gender peer 1999). In the present study, although children’s solitary
groups increased play with neutral activities, and interac- play was embedded within the preschool classroom,
tions with teachers increased play with feminine activities. during solitary play children were not directly interact-
Contrary to our hypotheses, boys did not engage in more ing with peers or teachers. The use of solitary play
masculine activities during play with female peers relative allowed for a baseline level of children’s activity pref-
to solitary play as was expected based on previous erences in a natural environment during a time when chil-
research and the rigidity of their gender schemas. We dren are not being encouraged to play with a toy by peers or
discuss the implications of these findings for children’s adults. Nonetheless, it is possible that children in the preschool
skill development. setting may be more likely to conform to gender roles in their
activity choices due to the presence of peers and teachers who
Activity Engagement in Solitary Play are nearby in their classroom. Further research is needed to
explore whether solitary play in preschool differs from solitary
Because preschool children spend 25–40 % of their free play in a home or lab setting.
time playing by themselves (Rubin et al. 1978), it is There were interesting and important variations in
important to identify what children are doing when they girls’ and boys’ tendencies to engage in gender-neutral
are alone. The findings of the study confirmed that both activities during solitary play. As expected, girls en-
girls and boys engaged in gender-typed play when play- gaged in neutral activities almost as often as they en-
ing alone. Girls engaged in feminine activities signifi- gaged in feminine activities whereas boys spent most of
cantly more than masculine activities during solitary their time during solitary play in masculine activities
play, and boys engaged in masculine activities signifi- only. These findings are consistent with those reported
cantly more than feminine activities during solitary play. by Cherney et al. (2003) in which girls played with
Moreover, for boys but not girls, engagement in gender- neutral and feminine activities at equally high rates,
typed activity play was more common than engagement whereas boys primarily played with masculine activities.
in neutral play activities. In other words, boys played Additionally, our findings conform to other findings that
with masculine toys and activities more than they played suggest that boys play more with gender-typed activities
with any other type of toy or activity. Girls, however, and they are less flexible in their activity preferences
engaged in feminine and gender-neutral activities at rela- than girls (Fabes et al. 2003; Green et al. 2004; Powlishta et al.
tively equal rates and at rates that surpassed their play in 1993). As a result, we might expect that girls experience
masculine activities. greater diversity in opportunities for social and cognitive skill
These findings are consistent with cognitive perspec- development.
tives of gender development and a number of studies
showing that, from a young age, boys prefer masculine Activity Engagement Across Social Contexts
activities and girls prefer feminine activities (see Ruble et
al. 2006). Although the findings are consistent with other Even if children show preferences for gender-typed ac-
studies, the present study is unique in that it documented tivities when alone, they may have opportunities for
young children’s gender-typed play preferences over sev- exposure to a wider range of activities, especially
eral months in a preschool setting in which a wide counter-stereotypic activities, when interacting with
variety of gender-typed and non-gender typed toys were others, including other-gender peers and teachers. From
available and separately assessed children’s engagement the perspective of promoting skill development, experi-
with activities during solitary play. ences with peers and teachers that increase children’s
Most prior studies have assessed young children’s toy engagement in gender-atypical and neutral activities may
preferences while children were engaged in play with be important because different types of activities (i.e.,
parents, siblings, or other individuals (e.g., researchers, feminine, masculine, and neutral) have been shown to
peers), without considering whether and how the social promote different skills. For example, consistent engage-
partner may impact those preferences (Caldera et al. ment in masculine activities may expose children (often
1989; Connor and Serbin 1977; Eisenberg et al. 1984; boys) to a narrow range of academic experiences that
Green, et al. 2004; Levy 1994; Powlishta et al. 1993; are associated with masculine activities (e.g., spatial
Serbin and Connor 1979). Moreover, in the few prior skill development), potentially limiting academic skill
studies in which children were studied while playing development thought to be associated with feminine or
alone, children have been limited to brief play episodes gender-neutral activities (e.g., literacy skill development;
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 447

Serbin and Connor 1979). Thus, engagement with a Implications for Children’s Skill Development
variety of interactional partners may increase engage-
ment in a variety of activities. It is for this reason that Despite the correlational nature of our data, the findings
it is important to examine how children’s gender-typed from the current study suggest that when interacting
activity preferences change as a function of the social with peers and teachers, children were exposed to a
context in which they are engaged. greater range of activities than what they are when they
Overall, the findings suggested that the social context played alone. Girls played significantly more with mas-
encourages children to engage in different activities than culine activities when they were engaged with male
they would select during solitary play, and the patterns peers. This suggests that, for girls, playing with boys
are generally consistent with cognitive theories of gen- facilitates their engagement in a different set of activi-
der development. For both girls and boys, play with ties, and many of these activities are thought to be
neutral activities occurred more when interacting with associated with the development of spatial skills, math-
mixed-gender peer groups than when playing alone. ematics, and motor skills. For example, Serbin and
This finding confirms the idea that children develop Connor (1979) found that for preschool-aged children,
theories about each gender so that when girls and boys high levels of play in masculine activities and low
interact with a group of children that includes both levels of play in feminine activities were related to the
genders, children may choose activities that are enjoy- development of spatial abilities. Additionally, masculine-
able for both genders. The present findings also illus- typed activities (such as playing with balls and bikes)
trate the social contexts (i.e., male peers and teachers) have been linked with supporting gross motor develop-
most likely to promote gender-atypical activity engage- ment (Pellegrini and Smith 1998).
ment. Because these data are correlational, one might For boys, play with feminine activities was increased
argue that gender-atypical activity preferences motivate during interactions with teachers. Teachers appear to
children to play with other-gender peers (or vice versa), encourage boys to engage in activities that are thought
but the comparison of solitary play to social contexts to be associated with skill development in other areas
suggests otherwise. For example, for girls, engagement different from those related to masculine activities. Spe-
in masculine activities increased relative to solitary play cifically, feminine-typed activities such as pretend play
only during interactions with male peers (other-gender have been related to coordinated social play with peers,
peers). For boys, engagement in feminine activities in- and engagement in feminine activities such as writing
creased relative to solitary play when interacting with and reading books has been shown to support language,
teachers but not during play with female peers. Based literacy, and fine motor development (Bredekamp and
on GST, the results for boys playing with girls were Copple 1997).
surprising. The boys did not increase their feminine The findings also suggest that during mixed-gender
play as we might predict if they changed their behavior interactions, girls and boys played with more neutral
based on expectations about what girls like to do. activities. Unfortunately, little research exists examining
There were other surprising findings. Based on re- the relations between children’s engagement in gender-
search conducted by Fabes et al. (2003), we hypothe- neutral activities and their skill development; however,
sized that boys’ engagement with masculine activities hypotheses regarding general flexibility and skill devel-
would increase during play with female peers relative to opment could be made. For example, research has
solitary play. The present findings, however, did not shown that flexibility in adolescents’ extracurricular ac-
support this hypothesis. Rather, boys in the current tivity involvement predicts positive social and academic
study did not engage in significantly more masculine outcomes (Fredricks and Eccles 2006). Thus, we might
activities when playing with female peers, relative to expect that greater flexibility or variability across pre-
their levels of masculine activity engagement during school gender-typed activities would predict greater so-
solitary play. It is possible that other factors come into cial and academic abilities.
play when boys play with girls. For instance, boys may The idea that having a range of behaviors, or being
recognize that they are more successful in exerting psychologically androgynous is associated with healthy
power over girls than girls are able to with them, and outcomes was promoted by Bem (1981). Bem argued
so boys are able to continue to engage in masculine that psychological androgyny in personality was consid-
activities (and so do girls) and not to engage in femi- ered ideal because it allowed individuals to have both
nine activities. This interpretation is bolstered by the the ability to act in masculine and feminine ways. For
studies of influence in children in preschool in which only children, having a wide range of activity exposure may
teachers (not female peers) were successful at exerting influ- be the ideal, because it would allow children to acquire
ence over boys’ activity preferences (Serbin et al. 1982). a broader repertoire of skills through their activities
448 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

(Serbin and Connor 1979) but little research has been Although the current research examines the gender-
conducted on this topic. However, in one recent study, typed activities children prefer in different social con-
both gender-typicality and flexibility of play partners texts, the direction of effects for these associations are
and activities positively predicted preschool children’s not well understood. The findings in the present study
social and cognitive adjustment (DiDonato et al. only indicate the degree to which children’s engagement
2012). The findings of the present study suggest multi- with gender-typed activities varied relative to engage-
ple routes to developing a broad skill set. One route is ment in solitary play. However, the relation between
to encourage children to try many different activities, activity participation and social context could conceiv-
such as encouraging children to try out different sports ably operate in two directions. It could be the case that
and games. Interestingly though, for girls and boys, children choose peer partners with similar gender-typed
interactions with teachers decreased activity play over- activity preferences or children choose peer partners
all. Thus, another route to activity exposure is to en- regardless of activity preferences and consistent engage-
courage children to broaden their social contexts, so that ment with peers over time make preferences for gender-
they spend more time in settings with same- and other- typed activities more similar. In fact, recent work
gender peers rather than with teachers and adults. Fur- showed that both selection and influence effects occur
thermore, the present findings suggest that gender between children’s peer and activity preferences (Martin
researchers should pay attention to variability and how et al. in press). Further research on the ways that
settings impact variability in gender typing. children are drawn to each other and influence each
other in activity engagement would be useful for fur-
thering understanding of social context effects.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research Aside from causality, another issue that should be
considered is the types of specific activities within each
A primary consideration for the interpretation of the context and how specific activities may drive varied
present results is that the sample is not representative behavior across social contexts. For example, children
of the larger population. Previous research on children’s may be willing to engage in a certain degree of gender-
gender-typed activity preferences primarily focused on atypical activity play depending on the activity. Within
non-Hispanic white middle class students. The majority both feminine and masculine activity categories there
of children in this study were Mexican or Mexican- are activities that could conceivable be considered
American, used Spanish as their primary language, and “more” gender-typed than others. For example, a boy
came from lower socioeconomic status families. These might be willing to play with art but less willing to play
sample characteristics could influence the results. Spe- with dolls, both feminine activities in the present study. In a
cifically, in Hispanic families, researchers have shown similar vein, children may utilize gender-atypical activities in
that parents are more traditional in gender attitudes and gender-typical ways. For example, a girl may play with
the degree to which they socialize their children in blocks, a masculine activity, but use them to build a house
gender-typical ways (Adams et al. 2007). This tradition- for her doll. Future research should consider the possibility of
alism may translate into encouraging children to play more nuanced variations in children’s activity engagement
with gender-typed toys and activities (e.g., masculine across social situations.
for boys). In this way, the children may have entered In conclusion, the goals of the present study were to
preschool with more gender-typical activity preferences describe preschool boys’ and girls’ gender-typed activity
than what would be expected in other populations. Fu- preferences during solitary play, and to describe how
ture research should replicate the current study by ex- activity preferences differ from solitary play when chil-
amining children’s preschool gender-typed activity dren engage with various play partners. Consistent with
preferences in different samples. previous research, both girls and boys in this study
A second limitation to consider when interpreting the preferred gender-typed activities (e.g., girls preferred
results of this study is the gender of the researchers and feminine activities) when they were engaged in solitary
teachers. The majority of observational coders and play. Findings also showed that children engaged in
teachers were female. It is possible that the gender different activities than they would select during solitary
similarity between the researchers and teachers biased play depending on the identity of their interactional
the behavior of both parties (e.g., Pellegrini et al. 2011). partners (i.e., peers and teachers). Results from this
A more rigorous design would have equal numbers of study suggest that engagement in a variety of preschool
male and female researchers and teachers and each social contexts may provide children with different
possible combination of researcher-teacher gender would learning opportunities through engagement in a greater
be included. variety of gender-typed activities.
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 449

Appendix

Table 5 Description of gender-


typed activities Category Activity Name Description

Feminine
Art materials crayons, paints, glue, other art materials
Music musical instruments, singing, dancing
Puzzles any type of puzzle
Books any type of book
Writing writing letters or words; different from arts and crafts
Phone telephone; no theme involved
Dress -up pretend clothes, accessories, make-up; no theme involved
Kitchen kitchen props; no theme involved
Pretend feminine nurse, mommy, teacher, waitress, etc.
Figure play feminine feminine figurines such as dolls
Masculine
Balls any type of ball
Bikes any type of bike, wheeled riding toys
Blocks blocks, Lincon Logs, Legos
Computer computer
Toy animals toy animal figurines, stuffed animas
Toy vehicles trucks, planes, cars, busses, helicopters, etc.
Pretend masculine doctor, superhero, fire fighter, policeman, etc.
Figure play masculine masculine figurines such as action figures
Figure play neutral neutral figurines such as Fisher Price figures
Neutral
Clay clay, play dough, goop
Board Games any type of board game or games with rules
(i.e. cards games such as UNO or Monster Mash);
this does not include educational games or
physical games like Duck-duck goose
Digging digging in a sandbox
Large Motor running, climbing, swinging, duck duck goose, ring
around the rosie, etc.
Math/Science flashcards, counting manipulatives, science experiments,
test tubes
Other any activity that could not be categorized as tone of the
listed activities
Pretend neutral animals, monsters, etc.
Sensory sand, water, beans, rice, shaving cream, etc.
Snack any type of meal (e.g., breakfast, snack, lunch)
TV television programs, short videos, or movies

References Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex


typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354-354-364. doi:10.1037/
0033-295X.88.4.354
Adams, M., Coltrane, S., & Parke, R. D. (2007). Cross-ethnic applica- Berenbaum, S. A., Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender
bility of the gender-based attitudes toward marriage and child development. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Advanced child
rearing scales. Sex Roles, 56, 325–339. doi:10.1007/s11199-006- and adolescent development (pp. 647–696). New York: Wiley.
9174-0. Bradbard, M. R., Martin, C. L., Endsley, R. C., & Halverson, C. F. (1986).
Banerjee, R., & Lintern, V. (2000). Boys will be boys: The effect of Influence of sex stereotypes on children’s exploration and memory:
social evaluation concerns on gender-typing. Social Development, A competence versus performance distinction. Developmental Psy-
9, 397–408. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00133. chology, 22, 481–486. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.481.
450 Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451

Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate Levy, G. D. (1994). Aspects of preschoolers’ comprehension of indoor
practice in early childhood programs (Rev. ed.). Washington, and outdoor gender-typed toys. Sex Roles, 30, 391–405.
DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. doi:10.1007/BF01420600.
Caldera, Y. M., Huston, A. C., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social inter- Maccoby, E. E., Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. In
actions and play patterns of parents and toddlers with feminine, H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior
masculine, and neutral toys. Child Development, 60, 70–76. (Vol. 20, pp. 239–287). San Diego: Academic.
doi:10.2307/1131072. Maguire, M. (1999). Treating the dyad as the unit of analysis: A primer
Campbell, A., Shirley, L., Heywood, C., & Crook, C. (2000). Infants’ on three analytic approaches. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
visual preference for sex-congruent babies, children, toys and ily, 61, 213–223. doi:10.2307/353895.
activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Martin, C., & Ruble, D. N. (2009). Patterns of gender development.
Psychology, 18, 479–498. doi:10.1348/026151000165814. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353–381. doi:10.1146/
Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Caygill, L. (2002). Sex-typed preferences annurev.psych.093008.100511.
in three domains: Do two-year-olds need cognitive variables? Martin, C. L. (2000). Cognitive theories of gender development. In T.
British Journal of Psychology, 93, 203–217. doi:10.1348/ Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psy-
000712602162544. chology of gender (pp. 91–121). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Cherney, I. D., Kelly-Vance, L., Glover, K. G., Ruane, A., & Ryalls, B. Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences
O. (2003). The effects of stereotyped toys and gender on play of young children’s same-sex peer interactions. Developmental
assessment in children aged 18-47 months. Educational Psychol- Psychology, 37, 431–446. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.431.
ogy, 23, 95–105. doi:10.1080/01443410303222. Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453–1471.
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. doi:10.2307/1131657.
Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine- Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing
and feminine-activity-preference scales for preschoolers: Corre- model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Develop-
lates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child ment, 5, 1119–1134. doi:10.2307/1129498.
Development, 48, 1411–1416. doi:10.2307/1128500. Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L., Leonard, S., & Dinella, L. M.
Colwell, M. J., & Lindsey, E. W. (2005). Preschool children’s pretend and (2011). Experienced and expected similarity to same-gender
physical play and sex of play partner: Connections to peer compe- peers: Moving toward a comprehensive model of gender segre-
tence. Sex Roles, 52, 497–509. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-3716-8. gation. Sex Roles, 65, 421–434. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-0029-y.
DiDonato, M. D., Martin, C. L., Hessler, E. E., Amazeen, P. G., Martin, C.L., Schaefer, D., Kornienko, O., Hanish, L.D., Fabes, R.A.,
Hanish, L. D., & Fabes, R. A. (2012). Gender consistency and & Goble, P. (in press). The Role of Sex of Peers and Gender-
flexibility: Using dynamics to understand the relation between Typed Activities in Young Children’s Peer Affiliative Networks:
gender and adjustment. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and A Longitudinal Analysis of Selection and Influence. Child
Life Sciences, 16, 159–184. Development.
Durkin, K. (2005). Children’s understanding of gender roles in society. McHale, S. M., Kim, J., Whiteman, S., & Crouter, A. C. (2004). Links
In M. Barrett & E. Buchanan-Barrow (Eds.), Children’s under- between sex-typed time use in middle childhood and gender
standing of society (pp. 133–167). London: Psychology Press. development in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology,
Eisenberg, N., Tryon, K., & Cameron, E. (1984). The relation of 40, 868–881. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.868.
preschoolers’ peer interaction to their sex-typed toy choices. O’Brien, M., & Huston, A. C. (1985). Activity level and sex-
Child Development, 55, 1044–1050. doi:10.2307/1130156. stereotyped toy choice in toddler boys and girls. Journal of
Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L., & Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children’s Genetic Psychology, 146, 527–533.
play qualities in same-, other-, and mixed-sex peer groups. Child Oettingen, G. (1985). The influence of the kindergarten teacher on sex
Development, 74, 921–932. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00576. differences in behavior. International Journal of Behavioral De-
Fagot, B. I. (1978). Reinforcing contingencies for sex-role behaviors: velopment, 8, 3–13. doi:10.1177/016502548500800102.
Effect of experience with children. Child Development, 49, 30– Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The
36. doi:10.2307/1128589. nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Develop-
Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Extracurricular involvement ment, 69, 577–598. doi:10.2307/1132187.
and adolescent adjustment: Impact of duration, number of activ- Pellegrini, A. D., Bohn-Gettler, C. M., Dupruis, D., Mickey, M.,
ities, and breadth of participation. Applied Developmental Sci- Roseth, C., & Soberg, D. (2011). An empirical examination of
ence, 10, 132–146. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads1003_3. sex differences in scoring preschool children’s aggression. Jour-
Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T., Golding, J., & nal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 232–238.
Hines, M. (2008). Developmental trajectories of sex-typed behav- doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.003.
ior in boys and girls: A longitudinal general population study of Powlishta, K. K., Serbin, L. A., & Moller, L. C. (1993). The stability of
children aged 2.5–8 years. Child Development, 79, 1583–1593. individual differences in gender typing: Implications for under-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01207.x. standing gender segregation. Sex Roles, 29, 723–737.
Green, V. A., Bigler, R., & Catherwood, D. (2004). The variability and doi:10.1007/BF00289214.
flexibility of gender-typed toy play: A close look at children’s Roopnarine, J. L. (1986). Mothers’ and fathers’ behaviors toward the
behavioral responses to counterstereotypic models. Sex Roles, 51, toy play of their infant sons and daughters. Sex Roles, 14, 59–68.
1583–1593. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000049227.05170.aa. doi:10.1007/BF00287848.
Holmes-Lonergan, H. (2003). Understanding of affective false beliefs, Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1989). Statistical procedures and the
perceptions of parental discipline, and classroom behavior in justification of knowledge in psychological science. American Psy-
children from head start. Early Education and Development, 14, chologist, 44, 1276–1284. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.10.1276.
29-29-46. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1401_3 Rubin, K. H., Watson, K. S., & Jambor, T. W. (1978). Free-play
Huynh, H., & Feldt, L.S. (1976) Estimation of the Box correction for behaviors in preschool and kindergarten children. Child Develop-
degrees of freedom from sample data in randomized block and ment, 49, 534–536. doi:10.2307/1128725.
split-plot designs. Journal of Educational Statistics, 1, 69–82. Ruble, D. N., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W.
doi:10.2307/1164736 Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th
Sex Roles (2012) 67:435–451 451

ed.: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. Serbin, L. A., Connor, J. M., & Citron, C. C. (1981). Sex-differentiated free
933–1016). Hoboken: Wiley. play behavior: Effects of teacher modeling, location, and gender. Devel-
Ruble, D. N., Alvarez, J. M., Bachman, M., Cameron, J. A., Fuligni, A. opmental Psychology, 17, 640–646. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.17.5.640.
J., Garcia Coll, C., & Rhee, E. (2004). The development of a Serbin, L. A., Sprafkin, C., Elman, M., & Doyle, A. (1982). The early
sense of “we”: The emergence and implications of children’s development of sex-differentiated patterns of social influence. Ca-
collective identity. In M. Bennett & F. Sani (Eds.), The develop- nadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des
ment of the social self (pp. 29–76). East Sussex: Psychology Sciences Du Comportement, 14, 350–363. doi:10.1037/h0081269.
Press. Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and
Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender 5-year-olds’ toy-choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian
development. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. (Eds.), Journal of Psychology, 40, 43–48. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00096.
Handbook of child psychology, 6th ed.: Vol 3. Social, emotional, Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2006). Using multivariate statistics
and personality development (pp. 858-932). Hoboken: Wiley. (5th ed.). New York: HarperCollins
Serbin, L. A., & Connor, J. M. (1979). Sex-typing of children’s play Tomes, R. E. (1995). Teacher presence and child gender influences on
preferences and patterns of cognitive performance. Journal of children’s activity preferences in preschool settings. Child Study
Genetic Psychology, 134, 315–316. Journal, 25, 123–140.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen