Sie sind auf Seite 1von 4

European Journal of Pharmacology 807 (2017) 71–74

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

European Journal of Pharmacology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ejphar

Behavioural pharmacology

Effects of chronic bryostatin-1 on treatment-resistant depression in rats MARK


b a a,⁎
Daniel L. Alkon , Jarin Hongpaisan , Miao-Kun Sun
a
Blanchette Rockefeller Neurosciences Institute, 8 Medical Center Dr., Morgantown, WV, USA
b
NeuroDiagnostics LLC, Rockville, MD, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Chemical compounds studied in this article: Despite over a half-century's intensive research worldwide, the currently available antidepressants remain sub-
Imipramine (PubChem CID:8228) optimal. Therapeutic options for treatment-resistant depression, for instance, are rather limited. Here, we found
Bryostatin-1 (PubChem CID: 5280757) that rats exhibited a lasting treatment-resistant depressive immobility in response to open space swim test at a
Keywords: high intensity of induction. The induced depressive behavior is associated with a dramatic impairment in spatial
Bryostatin 1 learning and memory. Both the depressive immobility and impairment in spatial learning and memory are
Depression sensitive to a period of chronic treatment with bryopstatin-1, a relatively selective activator of protein kinase Cε.
Refractory depression Bryostatin-1-like analogues therefore might have therapeutic values for the treatment of treatment-resistant
Spatial learning and memory
depression.
Refractory depression

1. Introduction in time, in addition to other adverse effects (Wan et al., 2015). Long-
term impacts on memory operation and retrieval also need to be
Major depressive disorder is one of the most prevalent forms of carefully evaluated, especially when a continuation-phase administra-
mental illness in humans, with prevalence rate of 16.2% in the United tion of i.v. ketamine infusions is necessary (Vande Voort et al., 2016).
States (Kessler and Bromet, 2013). The disorder is associated with high On the other hand, there is substantial evidence supporting acute
morbidity and risk of premature mortality (Nestler et al., 2002; Ursano efficacy of electroconvulsive therapy in treatment-resistant depression.
et al., 2015) and is predicted to be the leading cause of disability in Unfortunately, memory loss and the need for repetitive treatments
Western countries by 2030 (Mathers and Loncar, 2006). Despite preclude its use as a long-term treatment option.
intensive research worldwide since antidepressant drugs were intro- We have observed that when facing an open space, no-escape water
duced into clinical practice over a half-century ago, there has been environment, depressive immobility would quickly dominate in ro-
almost no progress in developing new drugs through novel targets dents' behavior after an initial (failed) intensive search for an escape
(Mathew et al., 2008; Belzung, 2014). At least 30% of patients with (Sun and Alkon, 2013). The impacts of the open space swim test on
major depressive disorder do not show effective therapeutic benefits to behavior are age-, sex-, and intensity (duration)-dependent (Sun and
the available antidepressants (Ionescu et al., 2015). The non-respon- Alkon, 2008). In the present study, we directly evaluate whether
sive cases were termed as treatment-resistant depression, or refractory induction at high intensity (longer duration) would lead to depressive
depression (Fava, 2003), and remain a tough clinical challenge today. behavior mimicking treatment-resistant depression and whether
Recently, blocking glutamatergic system in the hippocampus (Lefebvre chronic bryostatin-1 treatment, if induced, might be effective against
et al., 2017), such as by using a low dose of ketamine, a potent non- the treatment-resistant depressive immobility in animals. It has been
competitive N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) antagonist, has well established that protein kinase C is involved in the regulation and
been shown to produce rapid-onset antidepressant efficacy (Wang control of many types of memories (Alkon et al., 2005). Bryostatin-1, a
et al., 2015; Bobo et al., 2016). The glutamatergic system in the brain relatively specific protein kinase C (PKC)ε activator, enhances the
is widely involved in cognition and regulation of behaviors. The expression and activity of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the
potential problems with these antagonists as antidepressants include hippocampus (Sun et al., 2015) and reverses the induced treatment-
the facts that their intended selectivity for targeting negative memory is resistant depressive immobility. Furthermore, our results show that the
only partial at most and that the efficacy of ketamine tends to wear off induced depressive behavior mimics the human disease, exhibiting a


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: masun@brni.org (M.-K. Sun).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2017.05.001
Received 3 January 2017; Received in revised form 11 April 2017; Accepted 1 May 2017
Available online 02 May 2017
0014-2999/ © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
D.L. Alkon et al. European Journal of Pharmacology 807 (2017) 71–74

dramatic impact on the ability of learning and memory. Based on the find the platform, distance swum, heading angle, and percentage of
results presented above, chronic bryostatin-1 might have therapeutic time spent in the quadrants.
value for the treatment of treatment-resistant depression. After the training trials, a probe trial of 60 s was given 24 h after the
last trial, with the platform removed, to assess memory for platform
2. Materials and methods location by the distance swum in the quadrants.

2.1. Animals and drug treatment 2.4. Visible platform test

Adult male Wistar rats (175–200 g) were housed two per cage in a After the probe trial of the water maze task, rats were tested on a
temperature-controlled (20–24 °C) room for at least a week prior to visible platform task (a non-spatial task; with the platform marked with
experimentation, allowed free access to food and water, and kept on a a pole that protruded 9 in. above the water surface but at a new
12-h light/dark cycle. location) to evaluate their sensorimotor ability and motivation for an
Chronic effects of imipramine and bryostatin-1 on induced im- escape. The latency to reach the visible platform was recorded and
mobility were evaluated, starting 24 h after the last trial of the 3 open compared between different groups.
space swim test trials. Imipramine (Sigma), a prototypical tricyclic
antidepressant, was chronically used as a standard antidepressant 2.5. Statistic analysis
(15 mg/kg, i.p., daily for 5.5 weeks). Bryostatin-1 (Sigma) was admi-
nistered at 20 μg/m2 (tail i.v., 2 doses/week for 5.5 weeks). The dose of Statistical analysis was performed using the analysis of variance (a
bryostatin-1 was based on our preliminary dose-response studies that two-way ANOVA), followed by Newman-Keuls multiple comparisons
smaller doses were ineffective against disorders-induced synaptic and test, wherever appropriate. P < 0.05 was considered significant.
cognitive impairments. Non-treated groups received the same volume
of vehicle at the same frequency. 3. Results
All procedures were conducted according to National Institutes of
Health Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines, and were approved 3.1. Open space swim test induces depressive immobility in rats
by the Ethical Committee of the Institute.
The open space swim test (24 min/one trial/day for 3 days) induced
2.2. Open space swim test: induction of depressive behavior a significant immobility, which lasted for at least 1 week without
further sessions and weeks with separate open space swim test
Rats were placed individually in a round pool, which has a diameter of monitoring episodes (1/week) after the initial 3 induction sessions
152 cm and height of 60 cm and was filled with 40 cm H2O (24 ± 1 °C). (Fig. 1). Typically, rats would initiate active swim in the first min and
No escape was provided in these trials during the test. Rats were free to gradually developed into intermittent periods of immobility, which
swim (or not to swim) for 24 min and then removed and returned to their lasted longer and longer as the session continued. Without an effective
home cages after drying. The observer(s) were obscured from sight of the treatment, a rat in the immobility phase did not make any movements
rats during the trials, but were able to observe the animals' behaviors on a other than those just sufficient to keep its head above the water surface
PC monitor during the trials. The same procedure (24 min session/day) (Fig. 1 inset), a characteristic behavior of depression similar to those
was followed 24 h later for 2 more days. The induction of depressive reported in the forced swimming test, as we described previously (Sun
immobility is age- and sex-dependent (Sun and Alkon, 2008). The session and Alkon, 2004). Imipramine, as the standard antidepressant to
of 24 min represents 2 fold of the duration (thus intensity; Sun and Alkon, determine the sensitivity to antidepressant treatment, and bryostatin-
2008) needed to induce a significant depressive immobility in the male 1 were administered chronically to evaluate their impacts on the
rats at the age when they were tested. The swimming/drifting path was induced immobility. The results after the initial 3 induction sessions
recorded with a video-tracking system (Poly-Track Video Tracking showed a significant group difference with the treatments (group data
System, San Diego Instruments, Inc.). The distance moved (mobility) points 4–8; F2,191=10.12, P < 0.001), whereas the initial OSST sessions
includes all the distance moved during the entire 24 min, as caused by induced similar effects on the distance traveled among the groups (in
active swimming/searching as well as slow drifts. We have previously the first three sessions: F2,71=0.668, P > 0.05). The chronic treatment
reported that the swim activity monitored accurately reflects duration of with imipramine did not reduce the immobility as compared with the
mobility and that the maximum effect is induced on the third trial (Sun vehicle group (F1,127=0.892, P > 0.05), indicating a depressive behavior
and Alkon, 2003). For evaluating a long-term impact, the induction was with the treatment-resistant profile. Bryostatin-1, on the other hand,
followed by ‘monitoring’ sessions (24 min) at a frequency of 1/week (Sun was effective in reducing the mobility (F1,127=12.033, P < 0.001;
and Alkon, 2004). compared with the vehicle group), although 2–3 weeks of treatment
were needed to produce the therapeutic effects, which lasted 2.5 weeks
2.3. Spatial water maze learning and memory task after the last dose (the longest duration measured; Fig. 1).

Effects of the induced depressive behavior on spatial learning and 3.2. Effects of induced depressive behavior on spatial learning
memory were evaluated with the Morris water maze task, in a 152 cm-
diameter pool (24 ± 1 °C). The maze was divided into four quadrants. One day after the last monitoring session (Fig. 1), we tested the
Control rats were allowed to swim for 2 min in the pool without a ability of three groups of rats: vehicle-induction, induction-Bry, and
hidden platform, the same day when the other groups were on their last control (age-matched but without the induction), in spatial learning
monitoring session. Twenty-four h later, all rats were trained for 2 and memory, using the hidden-platform water maze. As shown in
trials/day for 4 days to find a hidden platform (9 cm diameter). The Fig. 2A, the speed and the extent of the learning were significantly
platform was centered in one of the quadrants and submerged about different among the groups (F2,191=15.431, P < 0.001), indicating that
2 cm below the water surface. At the start of all trials, rats were placed spatial learning in the rats after depressive behavior induction was
in the water facing the maze wall, using different starting positions, and significantly impaired, severer than the impairment associated with
allowed to swim until they found the platform, where they remained for shorter session-induced depressive immobility (Sun and Alkon, 2004).
20 s. Any rat that failed to find the platform within 2 min was guided Chronic bryostatin-1, however, essentially rescued rats' ability in
there, with the maximum latency of 120 s scored. The swimming path spatial learning (F1,127=0.912, P > 0.05; compared with the control
was recorded by a video-tracking system, which computed latency to without the behavioral induction).

72
D.L. Alkon et al. European Journal of Pharmacology 807 (2017) 71–74

Fig. 1. Chronic bryostatin-1 treatment produces a lasting therapeutic effect on the induced treatment-resistant depressive immobility. The mobility was induced in a 24-min OSST
session (1/day) for 3 consecutive days, followed by ‘monitoring’ sessions (1/week). The agents were administered one day after the last induction session for 5.5 weeks. It took weeks of
treatments with bryostatin-1 for observing the full therapeutic effects, while the imipramine treatment for same period had no effects. The swimming distance in the first trial session
(arbitrary units in OSST: 23,279.21 ± 773.56 [approximately 179 ± 5.9 m]; OSST/Imi: 23,526.31 ± 725.04; and OSST/Bry: 23,671.24 ± 698.25) differed insignificantly among the
groups (F2,23=0.0731, P > 0.05) and was calculated as 100% in each rat. Data are represented as the mean ± S.E.M. Bry: bryostatin-1; Imi: imipramine; OSST: open space swim test.
Inset: a photo taken during induced non-searching immobility (with eyes closed).

induction with chronic bryostatin-1 treatment spent more time search-


ing in the target quadrant (F2,23=6.719, P < 0.001; Fig. 2B). The results
reveal that chronic brystatin-1 recovered the rats’ ability in spatial
memory recall after the behavioral induction (F1,15=0.918, P > 0.05;
compared with the control).

3.3. Effects of induced depressive behavior on a visible platform test

Measurements of mobility and escape activity also depend on basic


functions of the tested subjects: their sensorimotor activity and
motivation for an escape. Therefore, the sensorimotor activity and
escape motivation of rats were tested in a visible platform test, in which
no learning is involved. The escape latency to a visible platform,
however, did not differ among the groups tested (after the water maze
spatial learning: F2,23=0.362; P > 0.05, Fig. 2C), indicating that the
depressive behavior induction or treatment did not significantly affect
their sensorimotor ability or escape motivation. Thus, the behavioral
changes observed cannot be attributed to changes in non-specific
sensorimotor ability and in basic motivation for an escape. During
Fig. 2. Effects of induced depressive behavior on spatial learning and memory in rats. A:
Impairment of spatial learning by induced depressive behavior. The water maze learning the experimental periods, no rats showed any apparent sign of
started 1 day after the last monitoring session. The learning impairment was sensitive to discomfort, hypervigilance or easy startle.
bryostatin-1 treatment, administered during the 5.5 weeks after the initial induction
phase. No drug was administered during the days when the rats were performing the
learning and memory task. Each point represents the mean value ( ± S.E.M.) from 8 rats 4. Discussion
per group. B: Results of the memory probe test, 24 h after the last water maze learning
trial, showing the target quadrant ratio (target quadrant distance over the distance The primary objective of this study was to investigate whether the
average in the remaining quadrants). “*” (P < 0.05) and “NS” (P > 0.05) mark the level of open space swim test when delivered at high intensity would induce
significant differences when compared with the “Control” group. C: The induced
treatment-resistant depression and whether such depressive behavior,
depressive behavior and bryostatin-1 administration did not affect the ability of the rats
to sense and reach the visible platform, a non-spatial test. Data are represented as the
if observed, would be sensitive to chronic treatment with PKCε
mean+S.E.M. “NS” indicates an insignificant difference when compared with the “OSST” activators. Treatment-resistant depression is defined as a lack of
group. Bry: bryostatin-1; Imi: imipramine; OSST: open space swim test. symptomatic response to adequate first-line pharmacological therapy
(Trivedi et al., 2006). The essential criterion used in our study is a lack
of behavioral response to a clinically effective antidepressant treat-
Memory retention was evaluated after the training. Quadrant tests ment, especially when treated chronically (Willner and Belzung, 2015).
24 h after the last training trial revealed that the rats after the The resistance is thus considered as “absolute” since the imipramine is
depressive behavior induction did not show significant preference for used at an adequate dose and for an adequate duration (Berman et al.,
the target quadrant, where the platform was previously placed during 1997). When tested at lower intensity (a shorter period of induction),
the training. The control rats and the rats after depressive behavior the type of induced immobility was associated with anhedonia (Sun

73
D.L. Alkon et al. European Journal of Pharmacology 807 (2017) 71–74

and Alkon, 2013), a core symptom of depression, and was however, Performed data analysis: Sun.
sensitive to acute imipramine, iproniazid, or mianserin (Sun and Wrote or contributed to the writing of the manuscript: Alkon,
Alkon, 2003), or chronic imipramine administration (Sun and Alkon, Hongpaisan, and Sun.
2004), consistent with a behavioral pattern of the depressive immobi-
lity. It remains to be studied whether it would be sensitive to low dose References
of ketamine (Willner and Belzung, 2015). Nevertheless, the increase in
swimming distance in the treated group is unlikely the result of a Alkon, D.L., Epstein, H., Kuzirian, A., Bennett, M.C., Nelson, T.J., 2005. Protein
gradually increased physical resistance, since it was not observed in the synthesis required for long-term memory is induced by PKC activation on days
before associative learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 16432–16437.
absence of the treatment. Fatigue is unlikely to be involved in the Belzung, C., 2014. Innovative drugs to treat depression: did animal models fail to be
immobility in the test, since unloaded rats can swim for many hours predictive or did clinical trials fail to detect effects? Neuropsychopharmacology 39,
without showing physical fatigue (Dawson and Horvat, 1970). 1041–1051.
Berman, R.M., Narasimhan, M., Charney, D.S., 1997. Treatment-refractory depression:
Evidence has been provided that an effective anti-depressive treatment definitions and characteristics. Depression Anxiety 5, 154–164.
relieves the rats from the OSST-induced impairments in spatial Bobo, W.V., VandeVoort, J.L., Croarkin, P.E., Leung, J.G., Tye, S.J., Frye, M.A., 2016.
learning and memory (Sun and Alkon, 2004), suggesting a direct Ketamine for treatment-resistant unipolar and bipolar major depression: critical
review and implications for clinical practice. Depression Anxiety 33, 698–710.
involvement of depressive behavior in the impaired learning and Cryan, J.F., Markou, A., Lucki, I., 2002. Assessing antidepressant activity in rodents:
memory. We have also observed that the type of the induced recent developments and future needs. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 23, 238–245.
depressive-like immobility was not sensitive to buspirone, an anxiolytic Dawson, C.A., Horvath, S.M., 1970. Swimming in small laboratory animals. Med. Sci.
Sports 2, 51–78.
(Sun and Alkon, 2004), indicating the specific nature of the responses.
Ellwart, T., Rinck, M., Becker, E.S., 2003. Selective memory and memory deficits in
The co-occurrence and association of depression and memory depressed inpatients. Depression Anxiety 17, 197–206.
impairment in humans is a striking feature of the two disorders. Fava, M., 2003. Diagnosis and definition of treatment-resistant depression. Biol.
Memory, especially explicit memory, is consistently impaired in Psychiatry 53, 649–659.
Ionescu, D.F., Rosenbaum, J.F., Alpert, E.A., 2015. Pharmacological approaches to the
depressed patients (Ellwart et al., 2003). It is interesting that the open challenge of treatment-resistant depression. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 17, 111–126.
space swim test intensity used in this study impaired rats’ ability to Kessler, R.C., Bromet, E.J., 2013. The epidemiology of depression across cultures. Annu.
learn and memorize the water maze task. The impairment was more Rev. Public Health 34, 119–138.
Lefebvre, D.L., Langevin, L.M., Jaworska, N., Harris, A.D., Lebel, R.M., Jasaui, Y.,
severe than the one evoked with the test at lower intensity (Sun and Kirton, A., Wilkes, T.C., Sembo, M., Swansburg, R., MacMaster, F.P., 2017. A pilot
Alkon, 2004). We have previously found that the impairment is not just study of hippocampal N-acetyl-aspartate in youth with treatment resistant major
associated with the swimming or water environment, since similar depression. J. Affect. Dis. 207, 110–113.
Mathers, C.D., Loncar, D., 2006. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease
memory impairment was also induced in the multi-trial passive from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med. 3, e442.
avoidance task (Sun and Alkon, 2004). In the present study of impacts Mathew, S.J., Manji, H.K., Charney, D.S., 2008. Novel drugs and therapeutic tar-gets for
of induced depressive behavior on learning and memory, bryostatin-1 severe mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 2080–2092.
Nestler, E.J., Barrot, M., DiLeone, R.J., Eisch, A.J., Gold, S.J., Monteggia, L.M., 2002.
was administered weeks before the learning and memory tasks, so that Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34, 13–25.
the differences in learning and memory could not be due to an acute Porsolt, R.D., Bertin, A., Jalfre, M., 1997. Behavioural despair in mice: a primary
effect of the agent. Our results show that the induced depressive screening test for antidepressants. Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn. Ther. 229, 327–336.
Sun, M.-K., Alkon, D.L., 2003. Open space swimming test to index antidepressant
behavior induced with the swim sessions indeed has a dramatic
activity. J. Neurosci. Methods 126, 35–409.
influence on subsequent learning and memory. Sun, M.-K., Alkon, D.L., 2004. Induced depressive behavior impairs learning and
Many features of human major depression, such as hopeless memory in rats. Neuroscience 129, 129–139.
immobility, despair behavior, and self-injurious behavior, occur across Sun, M.-K., Alkon, D.L., 2008. Effects of age on susceptibility to the induction of
depressive behavior and imipramine in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 19, 334–338.
other species. Animal models are indispensable in clarifying patho- Sun, M.-K., Alkon, D.L., 2013. Cerebral ischemia-induced difference in sensitivity to
physiology that underlies depression, depression-cognition interaction, depression and potential therapeutics in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 24, 222–228.
and in searching for new antidepressants. The validity of our model for Sun, M.-K., Nelson, T.J., Alkon, D.L., 2015. Towards universal therapeutics for memory
disorders. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 36, 384–394.
depression is indicated by emergence of non-searching immobility, Trivedi, M.H., Rush, A.J., Wisniewski, S.R., Nierenberg, A.A., Warden, D., Ritz, L.,
which is considered one core symptom of depressive behavior (Porsolt Norquist, G., Howland, R.H., Lebowitz, B., McGrath, P.J., Shores-Wilson, K., Biggs,
et al., 1997; Cryan et al., 2002). At lower intensity (11 min for rats at an M., Balasubramani, M.M., Fava, G.K., STAR*D Study Team, 2006. Evaluation of out-
comes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D:
age of 2 months), the swim induction induces depressive immobility, implications for clinical practice. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 28–40.
which is sensitive to all three major classes of effective antidepressants, Ursano, R.J., Kessler, R.C., Stein, M.B., Naifeh, J.A., Aliaga, P.A., Fullerton, C.S.,
including imipramine, and a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor Sampson, N.A., Kao, T.C., Colpe, L.J., Schoenbaum, M., Cox, K.L., Heeringa, S.G.,
2015. Suicide attempts in the US Army during the Wars in Afghanistan and Iraq,
(Sun and Alkon, 2003). We have also determined that the swim
2004 to 2009. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 917–926.
distance reliably reflects the active swimming status of the animals Vande Voort, J.L., Morgan, R.J., Kung, S., Rasmussen, K.G., Rico, J., Palmer, B.A.,
during the trial period in the test (Sun and Alkon, 2003) and with state Schak, K.M., Tye, S.J., Ritter, M.J., Frye, M.A., Bobo, W.V., 2016. Continuation
phase intravenous ketamine in adults with treatment-resistant depression. J. Affect.
of anhedonia (Sun and Alkon, 2013).
Dis. 206, 300–304.
The strengths of our study include the first, to our knowledge, Wan, L., Levitch, C.F., Perez, A.M., Brallier, J.W., Iosifescu, D.V., Chang, L.C., Foulkes,
evaluation of the sensitivity of rats to depressogenic events at higher A., Mathew, S.J., Charney, D.S., Murrough, J.W., 2015. Ketamine safety and
intensity, which actually transformed a treatment-sensitive type of tolerability in clinical trials for treatment-resistant depression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 76,
247–252.
depressive behavior to a treatment-resistant one. However, it remains Wang, J., Jing, L., Toledo-Salas, J.C., Xu, L., 2015. Rapid-onset antidepressant efficacy of
to be studied whether OSST induces higher stress intensity than other glutamatergic system modulators: the neural plasticity hypothesis of depression.
models (e.g. learned helplessness, repeated immobilization, etc.) used Neurosci. Bull. 31, 75–86.
Willner, P., Belzung, C., 2015. Treatment-resistant depression: are animal models of
to induce the treatment-resistant depression. Furthermore, our results depression fit for purpose? Psychopharmacology 232, 3473–3495.
suggest that chronic treatment with bryostatin-1-like agents might
have therapeutic value for the treatment-resistant depression, provid-
ing a lasting therapeutic effect on the disorder.

Authorship contributions

Participated in research design: Alkon and Sun.


Conducted experiments: Hongpaisan and Sun.

74

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen