Sie sind auf Seite 1von 20

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176 ISSN: 1136-1034 eISSN: 2254-4372

www.ehu.es/revista-psicodidactica © UPV/EHU
DOI: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.12531

Cognitive Profile for Children


with ADHD by Using WISC-IV:
Subtype Differences?
Javier Fenollar-Cortés*, Ignasi Navarro-Soria**, Carla González-Gómez**,
and Julia García-Sevilla**
*Universidad de Murcia, **Universidad de Alicante

Abstract
This study explores whether a specific cognitive profile for children with ADHD can be obtained
through the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) and whether this
profile is capable of differentiating between ADHD clinical subtypes. A control group of 47 children
was selected, together with a clinical group of 86 children diagnosed with ADHD and divided into
2 subgroups, according to their clinical characteristics. The clinical group was characterized by a
GAI > CPI with respect to the control group. The clinical subgroups did not score significantly lower in
any index, but they did in the difference between the working memory index and the processing speed
index. For those diagnosed with inattentive ADHD, this distance was positive; for those diagnosed with
ADHD combined group, it was negative. These findings contribute empirical evidence to the hypothesis
that there is a characteristic ADHD cognitive profile, with a potential ability of differentiating between
ADHD clinical subtypes.
Keywords: ADHD, WISC-IV, specific cognitive pattern, working memory, processing speed.

Resumen
El presente estudio explora si se puede obtener un perfil cognitivo específico para niños/as con TDAH
a partir de la Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Niños Cuarta Edición (WISC-IV), y si ese perfil es
capaz de diferenciar entre los subtipos clínicos de TDAH. Se seleccionó un grupo de control de 47 ni-
ños/as y otro grupo clínico de 86 niños/as diagnosticados/as con TDAH, éste último dividido a su vez
en dos subgrupos de acuerdo a sus características clínicas. El grupo clínico se caracterizó por una pun-
tuación ICG>ICC respecto al grupo de control. Los subgrupos clínicos no obtuvieron puntuaciones sig-
nificativamente diferentes en ninguno de los índices, pero sí lo hicieron respecto a la distancia entre
el Índice de Memoria de Trabajo y el Índice de Velocidad de Procesamiento. Para el subgrupo TDAH
predominantemente inatento esta distancia fue positiva, mientras que para el subgrupo TDAH-combi-
nado fue negativa. Estos resultados aportan evidencia empírica a la hipótesis de la existencia de un per-
fil cognitivo específico del TDAH, con potencial para discriminar entre subtipos clínicos de TDAH.
Palabras clave: TDAH, WISC-IV, patrón cognitivo específico, memoria de trabajo y velocidad de
procesamiento.

Correspondence concerning this article should addressed to Ignasi Navarro Soria, Departamento Psico-
logía Evolutiva y Didáctica, Universidad de Alicante, Dirección. E-mail: ignasi.navarro@ua.es
JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
158 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

Introduction lie in the extreme end of a normal-


ity continuum and change with de-
ADHD has a prevalence rate velopmental level rather than being
of around 5% in children and 2.5% static” (p. 181). Although the object
in adults, and is one of the most of analysis in the study of ADHD
common disorders in infancy and for a long time has been focused
adolescence (Adams, Lucas, & on attention, this interest has wid-
Barnes, 2008). The Diagnostic ened to include certain other pos-
and Statistical Manual of Mental sible deficits in cognitive abilities
Disorders(5th ed.; DSM-5; Ameri- that have been gathered under the
can Psychiatric Association, 2013) concept of “executive functions”
indicates that Attention Deficit (Gropper & Tannock, 2009). From
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the pioneering study by Barkley
characterized by a persistent pat- (1997) until today, numerous stud-
tern of inattention and/or hyper- ies have corroborated the involve-
activity/impulsivity that signifi- ment of neuropsychological deficits
cantly interferes with functioning in the development ADHD, espe-
and development, with symptoms cially in executive functions (e.g.,
being present before the age of 12. Lambek et al., 2011; Rubia, 2011;
This disorder leads to serious dif- Shimoni, Engel, & Tirosh, 2012;
ficulties in both academic context Toll, Van der Ven, Kroesbergen, &
(Barnard-Brak, Sulak, & Fearon, Van Luit, 2010).
2011; Frazier, Youngstrom, Glunt- The executive function is in-
ting, & Watkins, 2007, Massetti, volved in those activities that are
et al. 2008) and family environ- directly related to the cognitive
ment (Anastopoulos, Sommer, & control of information, working
Schatz, 2009; Schroeder & Kel- memory, and conscious temporary
ley, 2009). For this reason, it is es- handling of the information neces-
sential to find the most appropri- sary to carry out complex cogni-
ate strategies and tools to reduce tive operations (Miranda, Colomer,
the possibility of errors in the di- Fernández, & Presentación, 2012;
agnostic process (Skounti, Philali- Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht, 2010).
this, & Galanakis, 2007). Working memory is one of the most
The conception and definition studied cognitive skills of executive
of ADHD has gradually changed functions, as it is considered to be
over time (Stefanatos & Baron, the cognitive skill central to execu-
2007). As stated by Tillman, Enin- tive functions, and is particularly
ger, Forssman and Bohlin (2011): affected in ADHD (Kasper, Alder-
“(ADHD) is best viewed within a son, & Hudec, 2012; Soroa, Iraola,
dimensional and developmental Balluerka, & Soroa, 2009). Two im-
framework in which the symptoms portant meta-analytical studies con-
and their neuropsychological bases firmed the involvement of working

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 159

memory in ADHD (Martinussen, hension Index (VCI) and the Per-


Hayden, Hogg-Johnson, & Tan- ceptive Reasoning Index (PRI) (De-
nock, 2005; Willcutt, Pennington, vena & Watkins, 2012; Mayes &
Chhabildas, Olson, & Hulslander, Calhoun, 2006; Miguel-Montes,
2005). However, other studies have Allen, Puente, & Neblina, 2010).
questioned this relation, but accord- These results have been replicated
ing to Kofler, Rapport and Ander- in adults (Nelson, Canivez, & Wat-
son (2011), this could be due to as- kins, 2013). However, this ten-
pects related to the methodology of dency is not sufficiently strong to
these studies. be considered a diagnostic tool in
The need to carry out a cogni- itself, and in any case, it is inferior
tive study as a complementary tool to the classic method of diagnosis
for the diagnosis of ADHD was al- by using behavior rating scales (De-
ready noted by Prifitera and Dersh vena & Watkins, 2012).
(1993). Wechsler Intelligence Scales The relationship between spe-
for Children (Wechsler, 1974, 1991, cific cognitive patterns and clinical
2003) have been widely used to ADHD subtypes is still unclear. For
identify cognitive patterns in differ- example, Thaler et al. (2012) found
ent neurological disorders, such as that the cognitive profile character-
ADHD, autism, and acquired brain ized by average scores in the VCI,
damage (e.g. Scheirs & Timmers, PRI, and WMI, with reduced PSI,
2009; Schwean & McCrimmon, was associated with a higher rat-
2008; Thaler et al., 2010). However, ing of inattention symptoms and
the WISC-IV scale has been shown a larger proportion of subjects di-
to have greater sensitivity to ADHD agnosed with ADHD-inattentive.
symptoms than the WISC-III scale Solanto et al. (2007) found that the
(Mayes & Calhoun, 2006; Styck & ADHD-combined group had poorer
Watkins, 2014). scores on visuospatial working
Different studies have found memory than the ADHD-inatten-
that children evaluated by the tive group, whereas the inattentive
WISC-IV test showed a character- subtype scored worse in processing
istic pattern in their cognitive de- speed measured by the WISC-III.
velopment (Sattler, 2008). Although However, Tillman, Eninger, Forss-
they achieve scores that come close man and Bohlin (2011) concluded
to normative ranges in their general that working memory, whether vis-
intellectual functioning (Devena & uospatial or verbal, is only related
Watkins, 2012), children with to inattention symptomatology.
ADHD tend to perform worse than Yang et al. (2013) found no differ-
the control or normative group on ences between two clinical subtypes
the Working Memory Index (WMI) of ADHD (inattentive and com-
and Processing Speed Index (PSI) bined) in terms of their scores in the
with respect to the Verbal Compre- WISC-IV indices.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
160 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

In general, studies carried out comparison to the control group. We


to date have provided empirical ev- also hypothesize that the ADHD-in-
idence that subjects with ADHD- attentive group will be characterized
inattentive show a poorer perform- by a lower score in the PSI with re-
ance on PSI than ADHD-combined spect to the WMI, whereas the op-
(Calhoun & Mayes, 2005; Mayes, posite will be true for the ADHD-
Calhoun, Chase, Mink, & Stagg, combined group.
2009; Thaler et al., 2012). Chha-
bildas, Pennington and Willcutt
(2001) concluded that a poor per- Method
formance in the PSI was related to
inattention symptomatology, inde- Participants
pendent of the occurrence of hyper-
active/impulsive symptomatology, The clinical group included
whereas this poor performance was 86 children with a prior diagno-
not found in the ADHD-hyperactive sis of ADHD ranged in age from 6
group. Other studies have not found to 14 years (M = 9.66, SD = 2.39)
a significant deficit in processing and approximately three-quarters
speed, but it needs to be noted that (70.9%) were male. Forty-four of
the samples either have been low or them were diagnosed with ADHD-
have not distinguished between sub- Combined (51.2%) and forty-two
types (Loh, Piek, & Barrett, 2011). were diagnosed with ADHD-Inat-
There is a consensus to consider tentive (48.8%). Fifty-nine (68.6%)
that establishing a cognitive pro- were taking medication for ADHD
file in children with ADHD through at the time of the study.The con-
the WISC-IV may help in the di- trol group was made up of 47 sub-
agnostic process, as well as in in- jects collected from schools from
dicating the specific strengths and the area, aged between 6 and 14
weaknesses of ADHD (Yang et al., years, (M = 9.81, SD = 1.29), within
2013), and might become a predic- which twenty-eight (59.6%) were
tor of the severity of the symptoms male (Table 1).
and the prognosis (Thaler et al.,
2012). Measures
The aim of this study is to ex-
plore the relationships between The clinical tools used were as
the clinical and cognitive profiles follows:
through the main indices in the
WISC-IV. We hypothesize that the Strengths and Difficulties Ques-
clinical sample (ADHD) is char- tionnaires for Parents and Teach-
acterized by a lower Cognitive ers (Goodman, 1997): SDQ is a 25-
Processing Index (WMI+PSI) than item screening measure with and
General Ability Index (VCI+PRI) in emotional symptoms, conduct prob-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 161

Table 1
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of ADHD and Control Samples

ADHD ADHD Clinical Control


combined inattentive sample sample

N 44 42 86 47
Age M (SD) 8.3 (1.8) 10.7 (2.3) 9.6 (2.4) 9.8 (1.3)
Male n (%) 34 (77.3) 30 (71.4) 61 (70.9) 28 (59.6)
Medicated n (%) 31 (70.5) 28 (66.7) 59 (68.6) —
SDQ* 8.1 (1.8) 5.78 (2.1) 7 (2.3) 3.2 (2.1)
Hyp.
M(SD) 8.1 (2.2) 5.1 (2.1) 6.6 (2.7) 1.4 (1.5)
20.5 (4.6) 19.3(4.6) 19.4 (4.3)
Ina.
ADHD-RS-IV* 20.8 (4.6) 17.8 (3.8) 19.2 (4.9)
M (SD) Hyp/ 18.77 (4.9) 7.35 (4.8) 13.1 (7.3)
Imp. 19.3 (4.9) 4.2 (4.08) 10.6 (8.2)
N/A**
78.5 (9.6) 79.4 (11.6) 78.2 (11.3)
Ina.
Conners 3 (S)* 71.8 (7.1) 72 (8.6) 71.9 (9.4)
M (SD) 79.0 (9.9) 56.3 (11.4) 72.4 (14.6)
Hyp.
76.4 (11.4) 50.4 (8.2) 65.8 (16.3)
Note. Hyp. = Hyperactivity; Ina. = Inattention; Hyp/Imp. = Hyperactivity/Impulsivity; SDQ = Strengths
and Difficulties Questionnaire; ADHD RS-IV = ADHD Rating Scale IV; Conners 3 (S) = Conners 3
Short Version.
** Family score is on upper side and teacher score is on lower side.
** Typical controls did not complete neither parent and teacher Conners’ questionnaire or ADHD-Rat-
ing Scale-IV.

lems, hyperactivity, peers problems, duct problems) to .75 (emotional


and prosocial scales. Each item was symptoms), and was .68 for hyper-
rated on a 3-point frequency of oc- activity. The Cronbach’s alphas for
currence scale for the past 6-months teachers varied from .67 (emotional
(0 = “not true”; 1 = “somewhat symptoms) to .79 (hyperactivity).
true”; 2 = “certainly true”). Given For the study, we used the “hyper-
the five SDQ subscales demonstrate activity” and “prosocial behavior”
adequate psychometric properties subscales within the clinical limits
in earlier studies (Goodman, 2001). stipulated by the scale, except for
Cronbach’s alphas for our sample those subjects for whom the inatten-
for parents varied from .68 (con- tion dimension was predominant, in

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
162 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

which case the limit was established currence scale for the past month
in a score of 4 and above (Ullebo, (0 = not true at all; 1= just a little
Posserud, Heiervang, & Gillberg, true; 2 = pretty much true; 3 = very
2011). Those subjects who did not much true). Previous studies pro-
obtain a clinically significant score vide support for the psychometric
either from their family or their ed- properties of the Conners-3 (Con-
ucation center were excluded from ners, 2008). Cronbach’s alphas for
the clinical group. family varied from .62 (learning
The ADHD Rating Scale-IV problems scale) to .91 (hyperactiv-
(DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & ity scale), and was .85 for inatten-
Reid, 1998): ADHD-RS-IV is an tion scale, and from .86 (defiance/
18-item scale based on DSM-IV di- agression scale) to .91 (peers rela-
agnostic criteria for ADHD. Family tionship problems scale) for teach-
and teachers rated the occurrence ers. Cronbach’s alphas for teacher
of nine inattentive and hyperactiv- were .87 and .89 for inattention and
ity/impulsivity symptoms for the hyperactivity/impulsivity, respec-
past 6-months on a 4-point scale tively.
(0 = never or rarely; 1 = sometimes; Semistructured Family Inter-
2 = often; 3 = very often). ADHD- view: A research team member car-
RS-IV have demonstrated adequate ried out a semistructured interview
psychometric properties in studies with the family to test if the sub-
with American and Spanish sam- ject fell within the criteria set down
ples (Servera & Cardo, 2007). Cron- by the Diagnostic and Statistical
bach’s alphas for inattention and Manual of Mental Disorders (4 th
hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms ed., text rev; DSM-IV-TR; Ameri-
for family were .85 and .90, respec- can Psychiatric Association [APA],
tively, with the values being .91 and 2000), as well as to review the clini-
.93 for teachers, respectively. cal history to confirm that the sub-
Short Version of the Span- ject met the criteria to be included
ish Version of the Conners’ Rating in the sample design. In those cases
Scale 3rd Edition for Parents and in which the clinical team deemed it
Teachers (Conners, 2008): Family appropriate, an interview was also
and teachers completed the Con- carried out with the relevant person-
ners-3 (S), which measures inat- nel from the education center.
tention (5 items), hyperactivity/ WISC-IV Wechsler Intelli-
impulsivity (6 items), defiance/ag- gence Scale for Children. IV Edi-
gression (5 items), peer relationship tion (Wechsler, 2011): The WISC-
problems (5 items), and learning IV (Wechsler, 2003) is a four-factor
problems/executive functions (10 cognitive test for children who were
items for family form and 6 items between 6 and 16 years of age. The
for teachers form). Each item was Spanish version of the WISC-IV
rated on a 4-point frequency of oc- (Corral, Arribas, Santamaría, Sue-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 163

iro, & Pereña, 2005) was used to by a specialist in child psychia-


assess general intellectual function- try, child neurologist, or child re-
ing (FSIQ) through 10 mandatory searcher; an IQ < 70 (as measured
subtest (M = 10, SD = 3) that form by the WISC-IV); the occurrence of
four indices (M = 100, SD = 15) symptoms of severe mental disor-
including Verbal Comprehension ders (psychosis, major depression,
Index, Perceptive reasoning in- etc.); suffering from severe medi-
dex (PRI), Working Memory In- cal conditions (epilepsy, brain dam-
dex (WMI), and Processing Speed age, etc.); or being diagnosed with a
Index (PSI). To the extent possi- pervasive developmental disorder.
ble, General Ability Index (GAI) The control group met the same ex-
(M = 100, SD = 15) and Cognitive clusion criteria as the clinical group
Processing Index (CPI) (M = 100, but whose scores on the Strengths
SD = 15) was calculated. The GAI and Difficulties Questionnaire were
is a composite score that is based lower, in both informants, than the
on three verbal and three nonver- lowest clinically significant limit
bal subtest, and does not include in any SDQ subscales. In addition,
the Working Memory or Process- families and teachers were informed
ing Speed indices). CPI summarizes that study participation was vol-
performance on the Working Mem- untary and confidential. All fami-
ory and Processing Speed indices in lies who met the inclusion criteria
a single score. Supplemental subtest agreed to participate and no teach-
were not included in this study. The ers declined participation.
WISC-IV raw scores of the subtest With the results of the clinical
were converted into age-corrected tests as basis two subclinical groups
standard scores. were formed. The subclinical groups
were configured as follows:
Procedure
1. ADHD combined: Subjects who
The clinical sample was in- had attained a score equal to or
itially composed of 138 subjects higher than 7 on the SDQ scale
aged between 6 and 14 years, of as scored by both informants
both sexes, who were taken from (families and teachers), and
Child and Youth Mental Health whose “inattention” and “hy-
Units, Psychology and Education peractivity/impulsivity” scores,
practices, and associations for fami- both in the ADHD-RS-IV and
lies with children with ADHD from Conners 3, as scored by both
the regions of Murcia, Alicante, informants, were clinically sig-
and Valencia, Spain. The follow- nificant. Finally, the researcher
ing exclusion criteria were estab- confirmed the ADHD-com-
lished: not having a previous diag- bined diagnosis in a clinical in-
nosis of ADHD available, provided terview.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
164 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

2. ADHD inattentive: Subjects who significance (at 95% and at 99%).


obtained a score on the SDQ Cohen’s d were calculated for ef-
scale equal to or higher than 4, fect size, interpreted as 0.2, 0.5, and
and who obtained clinically sig- 0.8 reflecting small, medium, and
nificant scores only in the inat- large effect sizes, respectively (Co-
tentive dimension both in the hen, 1969). The normality and ho-
ADHD-RS-IV and Connors 3 moscedasticity of the distribution
scales as scored by both inform- of the variables studied were also
ants. Finally, the researcher con- tested. Finally, discriminant analy-
firmed the ADHD-inattentive sis was used to identify the cogni-
diagnosis in a clinical interview. tive variables best able to classify
in which clinical group would each
Data analysis participant be assigned.

First, differences were explored


between the means of the main in- Results
dices (VCI, PRI, WMI, and PSI)
and the FSIQ by gender (in both No significant differences were
the control and clinical groups) and found by gender in the main indi-
by medication taken (in the clini- ces and the WISC-IV general indi-
cal group only). Second, any possi- ces. Similarly, no significant dif-
ble significant differences between ferences were found between those
the means of the main indices were who were taking medication and
explored, as well as in the means of, those who were not, suggesting that,
and the mean distance between, the at least in our sample, neither medi-
CPI and GAI indices and the means cation nor gender affected to cogni-
of the subclinical groups. Further- tive performance.
more, any potential significant dif- With respect to the differences
ferences between the means of the between the control and clinical
differences between the VCI and groups, significant differences were
the PRI, as well as the WMI and found in WMI [F(1, 131) = 9.51;
the PRI. By setting the mean of the p = .002; d = 0.67], the PSI [F(1,
difference between these four indi- 131) = 7.88; p = .006; d = 0.51],
ces, not only does the gross value and the CPI [F(1, 131) = 11.99;
of the scores lose weight on the in- p = .001; d = 0.68] (Table 1). No
dices but also if patterns of relation further significant differences were
can be identified between the dif- found at any indices. With respect
ferent indices. ANOVAs statisti- to the mean differences between
cal tools were used for this purpose the GAI and the CPI, significant
(a method for finding out the least differences were found between
significant differences [LSDs]), as them [F(1, 131) = 14.90; p = .000;
well as t-test scores to discover the d = 0.67] with the difference in the

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 165

Table 2
WISC-IV Scores Comparison between Control vs ADHD vs Subclinical Groups

ANOVA
ADHD ADHD
Clinical Control Clinical vs ADHD-C vs
combined Inattentive
N = 86 N = 47 Control ADHD-I
N = 44 N = 42
F(1, 131) d F(1, 84) d

VCI 106.2 (13.9) 106.1 (15.7) 106.4 (14.9) 105.3 (12) 0.13 0.00
PRI 105.7 (15.7) 104.1 (12.1) 104.4 (15.5) 105.7 (13.2) 0.1 0.28
WMI 91.8 (13.2) 102.4 (16) 96.9 (15.5) 105 (12.3) 9.51** 0.67 11.15*** 0.75
PSI 104.9 (17.2) 90.4 (12.4) 98.1 (16.3) 105.5 (11.6) 7.88** 0.51 19.95*** 0.95
FSIQ 103 (16.7) 101.1 (13.5) 101.9 (15.5) 106.3 (11.3) 2.86 0.36
GAI 107.3 (15.1) 106.5 (13.1) 106.6 (14.4) 106.3 (11.5) 0.74 0.06
CPI 98.3 (16.8) 95.5 (14.5) 97.0 (15.5) 106.4 (11.5) 11.99*** 0.68 0.70
GAI-CPI 8.95 (13) 11.1 (15.1) 9.98 (14) 0.29 (13.5) 14.90*** 0.67 0.48
VCI-PRI 0.52 (13.1) 2.10 (15.4) 1.29 (14.2) –0.38 (13.8) 0.43 0.26
WMI-PSI –13.1 (13.1) 11.9 (12.1) –0.86 (17.8) –0.51 (15.1) 0.01 84.67*** 1.99

Note. VCI = Verbal Comprehension Index; PRI = Perceptual Reasoning Index; WMI = Working
Memory Index; PSI = Processing Speed Index; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; GAI = General
Ability Index; CPI = Cognitive Proficiency Index. ADHD-C = ADHD combined; ADHD-I = ADHD
inattentive; d = Cohen’s d.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

clinical group being larger than that ear Discriminant Analysis (LDA)
in the control group (Figure 1). Linear was applied to the data but in all
Discriminant Analysis (LDA) was ap- cases was lower than 75%.
plied to the data, but the null hypoth- Significant differences were
esis about poblational covariance ma- found [F(1, 84) = 84.67; p < .000;
trix was rejected (Box’s M > .05). d = 1.99] in the mean of the dif-
In the cognitive performance ference between the WMI and PSI
in the main indices between AD- for ADHD-combined (M = –13.1,
HD-combined and ADHD-inat- SD = 13.88) and for ADHD-inat-
tentive groups, significant differ- tentive (M = 12, SD = 12.1) groups
ences were found both for WMI (Figure 4). A positive result in this
[F(1, 84) = 11.15; p < .001; variable indicates a higher score in
d = 0.75] (Figure 2) and PSI [F(1, the WMI than in the PSI, whereas
84) = 19.95; p < .000; d = 0.95] a negative result indicates the op-
(Figure 3). The rest of the compari- posite. No significant differences
sons between the means of the main were found in this variable between
indices and the general ones of the the clinical sample and the control
WISC-IV were not significant.Lin- sample.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
166 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

13
Differences b egtween GAI and CPI

–3
ADHD combined ADHD inattentive

Figure 1. Differences between GAI and CPI by sample. LSD intervals with a confident le-
vel of 95%.
Note. GAI = General Ability Index; CPI = Cognitive Proficiency Index.

106

103
Working NMemory Index

100

97

94

91

88
ADHD combined ADHD inattentive

Figure 2. Working Memory Index between ADHD subgroups. LSD intervals with a 95%
confident level.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 167

111

107
Processing Speed Index

103

99

95

91

87
ADHD combined ADHD inattentive

Figure 3. Processing Speed Index between ADHD subgroups. LSD intervals with a 95%
confident level.

24
Differences b egtween GAI and CPI

14

–6

–16
ADHD combined ADHD inattentive

Figure 4. Differences between WMI and PSI. LSD intervals with a 95% confident level.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
168 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

Discussion that the ADHD-inattentive profile


was characterized by a significantly
The aim of this study was to ex- lower score on the Processing Speed
plore the relationships between the Index with respect to the Working
clinical profile of ADHD and the Memory Index, which is consist-
cognitive profile of each of the sub- ent with Thaler, Bello and Etcoff
jects, as obtained using the WISC- (2012). These authors found that
IV. For this, scrupulous sample de- the cluster characterized by aver-
sign, which consisted of a graded age scores on the Verbal Compre-
3-scale protocol, was administered hension, Perceptual Reasoning, and
to two independent informants, con- Working Memory indices, with low
cerning subjects who had previously scores on Processing Speed Index,
been diagnosed with ADHD. was related to a high level of inat-
With respect to the first hypoth- tention symptoms and a greater pro-
esis, our results suggest that there portion of ADHD predominantly
are significant differences between inattentive. In effect, the variable
the clinical and control groups on representing the difference between
the Cognitive Proficiency Index. the Working Memory Index and the
The General Ability Indexis practi- Processing Speed Index obtained
cally identical in both groups, which a mean of 12 points, which indi-
would confirm that Verbal Compre- cates that the difference is positive,
hension and Perceptual Reasoning that is, that higher scores were ob-
indices, was not affected in ADHD tained on the Working Memory In-
group (Devena & Watkins, 2012; dex than on the Processing Speed
Mayes & Clahoun, 2006). How- Index. Mayes et al. (2009) sug-
ever, a significant difference was gested that the Processing Speed is
obtained on the Cognitive Profi- affected in children with ADHD-
ciency Index between control and inattentive but not in ADHD-com-
clinical groups. There were signifi- bined. Therefore, given that there is
cant differences in the results ob- considerable empirical support for
tained in the difference between the the hypothesis that working mem-
General Ability Index/Cognitive ory is affected in ADHD (González,
Proficiency Index, something which Rodríguez, Cueli, Cabeza, & Álva-
is consistent with the literature and rez, 2014; Passolunghi & Cornoldi,
answers our first hypothesis. These 2008; Willcutt et al., 2005), the re-
results initially show that at least lation should be negative, that is,
one of the indices that comprise the lower scores should be obtained on
Cognitive Proficiency Index must the Working Memory Index than on
score clearly lower than the rest of the Processing Speed Index, with-
the indices. out affecting the Verbal Compre-
With respect to the second hy- hension and Perceptual Reasoning
pothesis, our results clearly suggest indices. From our data, we found

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 169

that the ADHD-combined group ob- more robust tool in establishing re-
tained negative scores in the differ- lations between cognitive and clini-
ence between the Working Memory cal profiles and would have enabled
Index/Processing Speed Index, that us to establish the relation between
is, higher scores on the Process- certain cognitive patterns and the
ing Speed Index. In the study by symptoms of ADHD on a contin-
Thaler et al. (2012), a cluster was uum. When creating clinical groups,
not found in which the Verbal Com- despite the robustness of the proto-
prehension, Perceptual Reasoning, col for establishing inclusion into
and Processing Speed scores were the various groups, the possibility of
normal and the Working Memory exploring the degree of clinical im-
Index was affected; however, it pact in one dimension with respect
could be seen in their clinical and to the scores in the clinical profile
demographic data that the Work- would be reduced. However, we
ing Memory Index was lower than consider that establishing clear di-
the scores for the Verbal Compre- agnostic groups and exploring their
hension and Perceptual Reasoning cognitive characteristics as meas-
indices. We consider that the rea- ured by the WISC-IV was a natural
son for not finding this cluster could step, following on from important
be the involvement of the two Ver- studies such as that by Thaler et al.
bal Comprehension and Perceptual (2012) or that by Devena and Wat-
Reasoning indices. kins (2012).
If we compare the control and Equally, we are aware that in
ADHD groups in the variable de- creating a new variable, such as
termined by the difference be- the difference between the Work-
tween Working Memory Index and ing Memory the Processing Speed
Processing Speed Index, no signif- indices, caution must be applied in
icant differences were found be- its use, and for it to be considered
tween the groups, as the trends of a useful tool, a detailed theoretical
both clinical subtypes were offset. justification would need to be pro-
In general terms, it is clear that vided to ensure that the psychomet-
the Verbal Comprehension and Per- ric qualities of the scale are main-
ceptual Reasoning indices do not tained. Once it was seen that that
provide any information that may there were significant differences
be discriminating; instead, it is the between the ADHD-inattentive and
interaction between the Working ADHD-combined groups in rela-
Memory and Processing Speed in- tion to the Working Memory and
dices that characterizes clinical sub- Processing Speed indices, we do not
types. know what the specific magnitude
This study has numerous limi- of this difference should be to have
tations. First, we are aware that a a potential discriminating value. In
cluster analysis would have been a the same way, it would be interest-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
170 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

ing to assess if the Sluggish Cog- correlations of the scores between


nitive Tempo has an effect on the the Working Memory and Process-
cognitive profiles, particularly on ing Speed indices. Therefore, what
the Processing Speed Index. is relevant is not the low scores on
The results of this study provide the Working Memory Index, for
empirical evidence to the hypoth- example, but rather, a lower score
esis that a relation exists between with respect to the other indices re-
ADHD clinical profiles and the cog- lated to the subject, irrespective of
nitive profiles obtained by WISC- whether that low score is, despite
IV. This relation is sufficiently everything, above the normative av-
robust to characterize the ADHD- erage.
inattentive group with a cognitive With respect to the characteri-
pattern in which the Processing zation of the clinical group through
Speed Index are lower than Work- the “cognitive stage” created by
ing Memory Index, and to char- the difference between the General
acterize the ADHD-combined by Ability Index and Cognitive Profi-
lower scores on the Working Mem- ciency Index, despite the fact that
ory Index than on the Processing our study found significant differ-
Speed Index. The Verbal Compre- ences between the control group
hension and Perceptual Reason- and the clinical group, we consider
ing indiceswe’re not affected in the that these differences are subject to
ADHD group. However, from our the characteristics of specific cog-
study, the relevant data in the main nitive profiles of the clinical sub-
indices of the WISC-IV in their re- types and, therefore, to the magni-
lation with the clinical profile were tude of the main index affected in
not the gross scores but rather the each case.

References

Adams, P. F., Lucas, J. W., & Barnes, P. wave activity in the EEG provides
M. (2008). Summary health statistics new marker of ADHD. Clinical Neu-
for U.S. children: National Health rophysiology, 119(1), 163-179. doi:
Interview Survey 2006. Vital Health 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.09.119
Statistics, 10, 1-104. Anastopoulos, A. D., Sommer, J. L., &
Alexander, D. M., Hermens, D. F., Schatz, N. K. (2009). ADHD and
Keage, H. A., Richard-Clark, C., family functioning. Current Atten-
Williams, L. M., Kohn, M. R., ... tion Disorders Reports, 1(4), 167-
Gordon, E. (2008). Event-related 170.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 171

American Psychiatric Association Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-


(2000). Diagnostic and statistical dren-Four Edition (WISC-IV) (Span-
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). ish Version). Madrid, Spain: TEA
Washington, DC: Author. ediciones.
American Psychiatric Association Devena, S., & Watkins, M. (2012). Di-
(2013). Diagnostic and statistical agnostic utility of WISC-IV gen-
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). eral abilities index and cogni-
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric tive proficiency index difference
Publishing. scores among children with ADHD.
Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibi- Journal of Applied School Psy-
tion, sustained attention and execu- chology, 28(2), 133-154. doi:
tive functions: Constructing a unify- 10.1080/15377903.2012.669743
ing theory of ADHD. Psychological Devito, E. E., Blackwell, A. D., Clark,
Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94. L., Kent, L., Dezsery, A. M., Turner,
Barnard-Brak, L., Sulak, T. N., & Fearon, D. C., & Sahakian, B. J. (2009).
D. D. (2011). Coexisting disorders Methylphenidate improves response
and academic achievement among inhibition but not reflection-impul-
children with ADHD. Journal of At- sivity in children with attention defi-
tention Disorders, 15(6), 506-515. cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
doi: 10.1177/1087054710369667 Psychopharmacology, 202(1-3),
Brookes, K. X. (2006). The analysis of 531-539. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-
51 genes in DSM-IV combined type 1337-y
attention deficit hyperactivity disor- DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastopoulos,
der: Association signals in DRD4, A., & Reid, R. (1998). ADHD rat-
DAT1 and 16 other genes. Molecu- ing scale-IV: checklists, norms, and
lar Psychiatry, 11(10), 934-953. doi: clinical interpretation. New York:
10.1038/sj.mp.4001869 Guilford.
Bruchmüller, K., Margraf, J., & Sch- Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Nikolas, M.
neider, S. (2012). Is ADHD diag- A., & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Distinct
nosed in accord with diagnostic cri- neuropsychological subgroups in typ-
teria? Overdiagnosis and influence of ically developing youth inform heter-
client gender on diagnosis. Journal ogeneity in children with ADHD.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- Proceedings of the National Acad-
ogy, 80(1), 128-138. doi: 10.1037/ emy of Sciencies, 109(17), 6768-
a0026582 6774.
Chhabildas, N., Pennington, B., & Will- Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glut-
cutt, E. (2001). A comparison of the ting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007).
neuropsychological profiles of the ADHD and achievement: Meta-anal-
DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD. Jour- ysis of the child, adolescent, and
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, adult literatures and a concomitant
29(6), 529-540. study with college students. Jour-
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power anal- nal of Learning Disabilities, 40(1),
ysis for the behavioral sciences. New 49-65.
York, NY: Academic Press. González-Castro, P., Rodríguez, C.,
Corral, S., Arribas, D., Santamaría, P., Cueli, M., Cabeza, L., & Álvarez,
Sueiro, M. J., & Pereña, J. (2005). L. (2014). Competencias matemá-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
172 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

ticas y control ejecutivo en estu- Kofler, M., Rapport, M., Bolden, J.,
diantes con trastorno por déficit de Sarver, D., & Raiker, J. (2011).
atención con hiperactividad y difi- ADHD and working memory: The
cultades de aprendizaje de las mate- impact of central executive deficits
máticas. Revista de Psicodidáctica, and exceeding storage/rehearsal ca-
19(1), 125-143. doi: 10.1387/RevPsi- pacity on observed inattentive behav-
codidac.7510 ior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and chology, 38, 149-161. doi: 10.1007/
Difficulties Questionnaire: A re- s10802-009-9357-6
search note. Journal of Child Psy- Lambek, R., Tannock, R., Dalsgaard,
chology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586. S., Trillingsgaard, A., Damm,
Gropper, R. J., & Tannock, R. (2009). D., & Thomsen, P. (2011). Execu-
A pilot study of working memory tive dysfunction in school-age chil-
and academic achievement in college dren with ADHD. Journal of Atten-
students with ADHD. Journal of At- tion Disorders, 15(8), 646-655. doi:
tention Disorders, 12(6), 574-581. 10.1177/1087054710370935
doi: 10.1177/1087054708320390 Loh, P. R., Piek, J. P., & Barrett, N. C.
Hellwig-Brida, S., Daseking, M., Keller, (2011). Comorbid ADHD and DCD:
F., Petermann, F., & Goldbeck, L. Examining cognitive functions using
(2011). Effects of methylphenidate the WISC-IV. Research in Develop-
on intelligence and attention compo- mental Disabilities, 32, 1260-1269.
nents in boys with Attention-Defi- doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.008
cit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-John-
of Child and Adolescent Psychop- son, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A
harmacology, 21(3), 245-253. meta-analysis of working memory
doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0041 impairments in children with At-
Hyman, S. (2010). The diagnosis of men- tention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Dis-
tal disorders: The problem of reifica- order. Journal of the American
tion. Annual Review of Clinical Psy- Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy-
chology, 6, 155-179. doi: 10.1146/ chiatry, 44, 377-384. doi: 10.1097/01.
annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532 chi.0000153228.72591.73
Jarrett, M., & Ollendick, T. H. (2008). Massetti, G., Lahey, B., Pelham, W.,
A conceptual review of the comor- Loney, J., Ehrhardt, A., & Lee, S.
bidity of Attention-Deficit/Hyper- K. (2008). Academic achievement
activity Disorder and anxiety: Im- over 8 years among children who
plications for future research and met modified criteria for Attention-
practice. Clinical Psychology Re- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4-6
v i e w , 2 8( 7 ) , 1 2 6 6 - 1 2 8 0 . d o i : years of age. Journal of Abnormal
10.1016/j.cpr.2008.05.004 Child Psychology, 36(3), 399-410.
Kasper, L., Alderson, R., & Hudec, K. Mayes, S., & Calhoun, S. (2006).
(2012). Moderators of working mem- WISC-IV and WISC-III profiles in
ory deficits in children with Atten- children with ADHD. Journal of
tion-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Attention Disorders, 9, 486-493.
(ADHD): A meta-analytic review. doi: 10.1177/1087054705283616
Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 605- Mayes, S., Calhoun, S., Chase, G., Mink,
617. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.07.001 D., & Stagg, R. (2009). ADHD sub-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 173

types and co-occurring anxiety, de- patterns among normal, learning-


pression, and oppositional-defiant disabled, and ADHD sample. In B.
disorder: Differences in Gordon di- Bracken, & R. McCallum (Eds.),
agnostic system and Wechsler work- Journal of psychoeducational assess-
ing memory and processing speed ment monograhp series, advances in
index scores. Journal of Atten- psychoeducational assessment: We-
tion Disorders, 12(6), 540-50. doi: schler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
10.1177/1087054708320402 dren-Third Edition (pp. 43-55). Ger-
Miguel-Montes, L.E., Allen, D., Puente, mantown, TN: Psychoeducational
A., & Neblina, C. (2010). Validity of Corporation.
the WISC-IV Spanish for a clinically Quintana, H., Snyder, S. M., Purnell,
referred sample of Hispanic children. W., Aponte, C., & Sita, J. (2007).
Psychological Assessment, 22(2), Comparison of a standard psychi-
465-469. doi: 10.1037/a0018895 atric evaluation to rating scales and
Miller, G. (2010). Psychiatry. Beyon EEG in the differential diagnosis of
DSM: Seeking a brain-based clas- Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Dis-
sification of mental illness. Sci- order. Psychiatry Research, 152(2-
ence, 327, 1537. doi: 10.1126/ 3), 211-222.
science.327.5972.1437 Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht,
Miranda, A., Colomer, C., Fernández, S. A. (2010). Working memory and
I., & Presentación, M. J. (2012). mathematics: A review of develop-
Executive functioning and motiva- mental, individual differences and
tion of children with Attention Defi- cognitive approaches. Learning and
cit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Individual Differences, 20, 110-122.
in problem solving and calculation doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005.
tasks. Revista de Psicodidáctica, Rubia, K. (2011). “Cool” inferior fronto-
17(1), 51-72. striatal dysfunction in Attention-Def-
Nelson, J. M., Canivez, G. L., & Wat- icit/Hyperactivity Disorder versus
kins, M. W. (2013). Structural and “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-
incremental validity of the Wechsler limbic dysfunction in conduct disor-
Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edi- der: A review. Biological Psychiatry,
tion (WAIS-IV) with a clinical sam- 69(12), e69-e87. doi: 10.1016/j.bi-
ple. Psychological Assessment, 25, opsych.2010.09.023
618-630. doi: 10.1037/a0032086 Sattler, J. (2008). Assessment of chil-
Pazvantoglu, O., Alptekin-Aker, A., Ka- dren: Cognitive foundations (5th
rabekiroglu, K., Akbas, S., Sarisoy, ed.). Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler.
G., Baykal, S., & Rifat-Sahin, A. Scheirs, J., & Timmers, E. (2009). Dif-
(2012). Neurospychological weak- ferentiating among children with
ness in adult ADHD; cognitive func- PDD-NOS, ADHD, and those with
tions as core deficit and roles of them a combined diagnosis on the basis of
in persistence to adulthood. Jour- WISC-III profiles. Journal of Autism
nal of the International Neurospy- and Developmental Disorders, 39(4),
chological Society, 18, 819-826. doi: 549-556. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-
10.1017/S1355617712000574 0657-4
Prifitera, A., & Dersh, J. (1993). Base Schroeder, V. M., & Kelley, M. L.
rates of WISC-III diagnostic subtest (2009). Associations between fam-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
174 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

ily environment, parenting practices, tivity Disorder (ADHD): Perform-


and executive functioning of chil- ance-based test and parents report.
dren with and without ADHD. Jour- Research in Developmental Disa-
nal of Child and Family Studies, 18, bilities, 33, 858-865. doi: 10.1016/j.
227-235. doi: 10.1007/s10826-008- ridd.2011.12.014
9223-0 Skounti, M., Philatithis, A., & Galana-
Schwean, V. L., & McCrimmon, A. kis, E. (2007). Variations in prev-
(2008). Attention-Deficit/Hyperac- alence of Attention Deficit hy-
tivity Disorder: Using the WISC-IV peractivity disorder world wide.
to inform intervention planning. European Journal Pediatric, 166,
In A. Prifitera, D. Saklofske, & L. 117-123.
Weiss (Eds.), WISC-IV clinical as- Solanto, M., Gilbert, S., Raj, A., Zhu, J.,
sessment and intervention (pp. 193- Pope-Boyd, S., Stepak, B.,... New-
215). San Diego, CA: Academic corn, J. (2007). Neurocognitive func-
Press. tioning in AD/HD, predominantly
Sciutto, M. J., & Eisenberg, M. (2007). inattentive and combined subtypes.
Evaluating the evidence for and a Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
General Ability Indexnst the over- ogy, 35(5), 729-744.
diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of At- Soroa, M., Iraola, J. A., Balluerka, N., &
tention Disorders, 11(2), 106-113. Soroa, G. (2009). Evaluación de la
doi: 10.1177/1087054707300094 atención sostenida de niños con tras-
Seidman, L. J., Doyle, A., Fried, R., torno por déficit de atención con hi-
Valera, E., Crum, K., & Matthews, peractividad. Revista de Psicodidác-
L. (2004). Neuropsychological func- tica, 14(1), 13-27.
tion in adults with Attention-Deficit/ Spencer, S. V., Hawk, L. W., Richards,
Hyperactivity Disorder. Psychiatric J. B., Shiels, K., Pelham, W. E., &
Clinics of North America, 27, 261- Waxmonsky, J. G. (2009). Stimulant
282. treatment reduces lapses in attention
Servera, M., & Cardo, E. (2007). ADHD among children with ADHD: The ef-
Rating Scale-IV in a sample of Span- fects of methylphenidate on intra-in-
ich schoolchidren: normalitive data dividual response time distributions.
and internal consistency for teachers Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
and parents. Revista de Neurología, ogy, 37(6), 805-816. doi: 10.1007/
45(7), 393-399. s10802-009-9316-2
Shiels, K., Hawk, L. W., Reynolds, B., Stefanatos, G., & Baron, I. (2007). Atten-
Mazzullo, R. J., Rhodes, J. D., Pel- tion-Deficit/Hiperactivity Disorder:
ham, W. E.,... Gangloff, B. P. (2009). A neuropsychological perspective to-
Effects of methylphenidate on dis- wards DMS-V. Neuropsychological
counting of delayed rewards in at- Review, 17, 35-38.
tention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Styck, K. M., & Watkins, M. W. (2014).
Experimental and Clinical Psychop- Structural validity of the WISC-IV
harmacology, 17(5), 291-301. doi: students with ADHD. Jour-
10.1037/a0017259 nal of Attention Disorders, 1-8.
Shimoni, M., Engel, B., & Tirosh, E. doi: 10.1177/1087054714553052
(2012). Executive dysfuntions among Thaler, N. S. (2013). WISC-IV profiles
boys with Attention Deficit Hyperac- are associated with differences in

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


COGNITIVE PROFILE FOR CHILDREN WITH ADHD BY USING WISC-IV:
SUBTYPE DIFFERENCES? 175

symptomatology and outcome in Ullebø, A., Posserud, M., Heiervang,


children with Attention Deficit hy- E., Gillberg, C., & Obel, C. (2011).
peractivity disorder. Journal of At- Screening for the attention deficit
tention Disorders, 17(4), 291-301. hyperactivity disorder phenotype us-
doi: 10.1177/1087054711428806 ing the strength and difficulties ques-
Thaler, N. S., Barchard, K. A., Parke, tionnaire. European Child & Ado-
E., Jones, W. P., Etcoff, L. M., & lescent Psychiatry, 20, 451-458. doi:
Allen, D. N. (2012). Factor struc- 10.1007/s00787-011-0198-9
ture of the Wechsler Intelligence Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelli-
Scale for Children: Fourth Edition gence Scale for Children-Four Edi-
in children with ADHD. Journal tion. San Antonio, TS: Psychological
of Attention Disorders, XX(X), 1-9. Corporation.
doi: 10.1177/1087054712459952 Willcutt, E., Pennington, B., Chhabil-
Tillman, C., Eninger, L., Forssman, das, N., Olson, R., & Hulslander, J.
L., & Bohlin, G. (2011). The re- (2005). Neuropsychological analy-
lation between working mem- ses of comorbidity between RD and
ory components and ADHD ADHD: In search of the common
symptoms from a developmental deficit. Developmental Neuropsy-
perspective. Developmental Neu- chology, 27, 35-78.
ropsychology, 36, 181-198. doi: Yildiz, O., Sismanlar, S. G., Memik,
10.1080/87565641.2010.549981 N. C., Karakaya, I., & Agaoglu, B.
Toll, S. W. M., Van der Ven, S. H. J., (2011). Atomoxetine and methyl-
Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, phenidate treatment in children with
J. E. H. (2010). Executive func- ADHD: The efficacy, tolerability and
tions as predictors of math learn- effects on executive functions. Child
ing disabilities. Journal of Learn- Psychiatry and Human Development,
ing Disabilities, 44(6), 521-532 42(3), 257-69. doi: 10.1007/s10578-
doi: 10.1177/0022219410387302 010-0212-3.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176


JAVIER FENOLLAR-CORTÉS, IGNASI NAVARRO-SORIA,CARLA GONZÁLEZ-GÓMEZ,
176 AND JULIA GARCÍA-SEVILLA

Dr. Javier Fenollar Cortés, Doctor in Psychology from the University of Murcia.
Specialized in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) neurocognitive
performance, he has authored several publications regarding ADHD evaluation
and treatment. Currently he develops his work activity at the University of Mur-
cia for the Applied Psychology Service Attention and Memory Unit.

D. Ignasi Navarro Soria, Associate Professor at the University of Alicante in the Ev-
olutionary and Educational Psychology area from the 2008-2009 academic year.
School psychologist between 2002-2003 and 2009-2010 academic years. Psy-
chologist at several underage protection services in the Province of Alicante. He
specializes in early detection of learning difficulties at school environments.

Dr. Carla González Gómez, Tenured lecturer at the University of Alicante in the Ev-
olutionary and Educational Psychology area. PhD in psychology from the Auton-
omy University of Barcelona. She specializes in psychoeducational identification
and intervention with gifted students in their early educational stages, a field in
which she has authored many publications.

Dr. Julia Garcia Sevilla, Doctor in Psychology from the University of Murcia. Ten-
ured lecturer at Basic Psychology and Methodology Department, University of
Murcia Faculty of Psychology. She is author of many publications related to at-
tention processes and cognitive rehabilitation.

Received date: 25-07-2014 Review date: 18-10-2014 Accepted date: 20-11-2014

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2015, 20(1), 157-176

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen