Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Visual Perception of Colour

Colour may be viewed in two modes (Figure 1) namely:

1. Illuminant mode- when the light stimulus comes directly from a source of light, say, an
incandescent lamp or a candle.
2. Object mode- when stimulus comes from an object either from its surface (surface
mode) or through the object (volume mode). In this mode, the perception of an object is
mostly in reference to the surrounding objects. Three ingredients are required to visualise
the colour of an object, namely a light source, object itself and the human observer.

Fig-1: Two modes of viewing colours

A light source illuminates the object and is characterised by the energy at different wavelengths,
which is denoted by the term spectral power distribution (SPD). When a light beam falls on an
object, it is modified by absorption, scattering and other physical processes depending on the
physical and chemical construction of the object. The light (colour stimulus) that reaches the eye
of the observer in the form of reflected light interacts with the photosensitive pigments present in
the eye. This gives rise to nerve impulses which are transmitted to brain. The human eye-brain
mechanism makes rapid and continuous evaluation of object appearance and colour. The light,
which enters our eyes, contains the characteristic imprints of the light source and the object.
Light can also fall directly (without interaction with object) on our eyes creating nerve impulses.

Colour Perception
The Committee on Colorimetry of the Optical Society of America in 1922 defined colour as the
general name for all sensations arising from the activity of the retina of the eye and its attached
nervous mechanisms, this activity being, in nearly every case in the normal individual, a specific
response to radiant energy of certain wavelength and intensity (OSA, 1953).
Colour is sometimes used as a name for materials such as dyes and pigments, but the name
Colorant will be more appropriate in these cases. The spectral composition of radiant energy and
the spectral transmittance or reflectance of substances is also called colour, but it is again an
inappropriate name for these purely physical concepts. Psychophysical concept of colour as
characteristics of light dependant on human vision is most appropriate.

Perception of colour which is solely a prerogative of human beings is purely individualistic and
hence subjective. As a result, objective communication of a particular colour becomes difficult.
For expressing colour objectively one must measure and quantify it. The colour can be
objectively expressed either by creating calibrated coloured physical samples or by an
instrumental colour measuring devise and expressing it by numbers.
Colour Mixing
According to one reliable estimate, we can distinguish among ten million different colours (Judd
and Wyszecki, 1975). Kuehni further estimated that the humans with normal colour vision can
distinguish among some two million colours when viewed against a mid-grey background and
perhaps double when the background is widely varied (Kuehni, 2005).

Newton explicitly stated that colour is a perceptual property, not a physical attribute, which
meant that the light mixtures occurred in the eye, not in the light. Newton recognised three
aspects of colour perception: brightness, hue and intensity. Painters and dyers already from
before Newton’s time believed in three fundamental chromatic colours that they sometimes
equated with pigments or dyes: yellow, red and blue, from which all other hues can be created.
With long experience with paints and dyes they also affirmed that material colour mixtures and
light mixtures behaved very differently.

Colour perception for humans is three-dimensional, a fact almost certainly stems from the
existence in the retina of three different classes of light-receptive cells. LeBlon (1756) was first
to make clear distinction between mixing pigment colours and mixing colours of light. He stated
that all visible objects can be represented by three colours, yellow, red and blue and mixture of
these three colours makes black or all other colours. He named those as material colours or those
used by painters. He further added that for a mixture of spectral colours those proposed by Sir
Isaac Newton cannot produce black, but the very contrary, white.

Primary colours are sets of colours that can be combined to make a useful range of colours. For
human applications, three primary colours are usually used, since human colour vision is
trichromatic.

Primary colours are not a fundamental property of light but are often related to the physiological
response of the eye to light. Fundamentally, light is a continuous spectrum of the wavelengths
that can be detected by the human eye in an infinite-dimensional stimulus space. However, the
human eye normally contains only three types of colour receptors, called cone cells. Each colour
receptor responds to different ranges of the colour spectrum. Humans and other species with
three such types of colour receptors are known as trichromats. These species respond to the light
stimulus via a three-dimensional sensation, which generally can be modelled as a mixture of
three primary colours (Sobel, 1989).
Many birds and marsupials are tetrachromats (Backhaus and Werner, 1998), and it has been
suggested that some human females are tetrachromats as well (Mollon and Jordan, 1993), having
an extra variant version of the long-wave (L) cone type.

Most mammals other than primates have only two types of colour receptors and are, therefore,
dichromats; to them, there are only two primary colours.

Any choice of primary colours is essentially arbitrary; for example, an early colour photographic
process, autochrome, typically used orange, green, and violet primaries. However, unless
negative amounts of a colour are allowed the gamut of colour generated will be restricted by the
choice of primaries.

Fundamental laws of colour mixing can, therefore, be classified into two types namely

1. Additive colour mixing (Figure 2 a) occurs when two or more lights mix together.
2. Subtractive colour mixing (Figure 2 b) occurs when colorants are mixed together.

Virtually all our visible colours can be produced by utilizing some combination of the three
primary colours, either by additive or subtractive processes.

Fig-2: Additive and subtractive color mixing


For additive combination of colours, as in overlapping projected lights or in CRT displays, the
primary colours normally used are red (R), green (G), and blue (B). The result of additive mixing
of the primaries are listed below,
R + G = Y (Yellow),
G + B = C (Cyan or blue-green)
B + R = M (Magenta)
R + G + B = W (white)

For subtractive combination of colours, as in mixing of pigments or dyes, such as in dyeing or


printing, the primaries normally used are cyan (C), magenta (M), and yellow (Y), though the set
of red, yellow, blue is popular among artists.
The subtractive primaries can be obtained by removing red, green and blue from white light
using respective coloured filters namely

W - R = C (- R)
W - G = M (- G),
W – B = Y (- B),

The mixing of subtractive primaries yellow, magenta and cyan can be predicted mathematically
as follows:

Y + M = (W - B) + (W - G) = W - B - G = R,
Y + C = (W - B) + (W - R) = W - B – R = G
M+ C = (W - G) + (W - R) = W - G - R = B,
Y + M + C = W - R - G - B = K (Black)

When the three subtractive primaries are mixed in proper proportions as in case of mixing
colorants, the mixture is unable to reflect light anymore and appear black (K) in colour. Similarly
when white light is passed through filters of three subtractive colours, the whole of white light
will be removed. Mixture of two primaries is called secondary. The most commonly used
additive colour primaries are the secondary colours of the most commonly used subtractive
colour primaries, and vice versa.

The age-old belief that three primary colours are required to make white was also proved false
(Helmholtz, 1852). The idea of complimentary spectral colours came into existence and was
subsequently defined by Grassman (1853) as follows:
To every colour belongs another homogeneous colour, which when mixed with it, gives
colourless light.

Subsequently Helmholtz confirmed that every spectral colour does not have a spectral
complement as proposed by Grassman. A range of green colours ranging from about 484 nm to
about 570 nm, has complimentary colours in the non-spectral purple range.
Colour Communication
In colorant production and application industries, colours are to be communicated, compared,
recorded and formulated on regular basis. This necessitates systematic classification of colours.
The objects can be classified in various ways in terms of colour. The classification may be based
on visually or instrumentally-assessed colour parameters (Roy Choudhury, 2000) .
While communicating or talking about colour, a language which is understandable by both the
parties must be followed. A logical scheme for ordering and specifying colours on the basis of
some clearly defined attributes is known as colour notation system. The attributes are generally
three in number as our vision is trichromatic and they constitute the coordinates of the resultant
‘colour space’. Colour notation systems also encompass ‘colour order systems’ which are
typically comprised of material standards in the form of colour atlas. Due to constraints of
colorant gamut, the atlases may depict only a physically realisable subset of a colour order
system.
Colour notations can be classified into three categories (Rhodes, 2002):

1. Device dependant systems – the most common imaging devices used for reproducing
colour are the computer controlled CRT displays and the colour printers. The associated
colour order system and colour spaces are hardware-oriented and they lack perceptually
based attributes.
2. Mathematical systems – uniform colour spaces based on mathematical transformation of
CIE tristimulus values such as CIELUV and CIELAB belong to this category.
3. Systems based on database of aim points – colour order systems existing principally in
physical form, the colour samples of which can be measured to establish a database of
aim points. Using interpolation techniques among limited available samples, many more
colours can be defined.

The monitors and printers follow device-dependant specification systems. In cathode ray tube
(CRT) displays, colour television, and most computer video displays, colour stimuli are
generated with three different types of phosphors after activated by electron beams. The three
additive primary colours generated by such activation are orange-red, leaf green, and violet. A
large number of colours can be created by their mixture.

Two most common additive systems used in connection with computer display are RGB (based
on mixing of three additive primary colour red, green and blue produced by the phosphors of the
display unit in cubic space) and HSB (hue, saturation and brightness in cylindrical form).

For instrumental colour measurement of an object, additive primaries are used to predict the
quantity of subtractive colours required to reproduce that colour. This entails the use of standard
source of light, viewing geometry and a standard observer. The expression of colour as numerals
has been standardized by Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage (CIE) set up in 1931 for
standardization of measurement of colour. Thus the instrumental measurement of colour is
helpful in, communication of colour across the entire supply chain as well as comparing samples
and setting pass-fail criterion for sourcing and supply of products. In this form it is a useful tool
for quality control.

It is very convenient, easily understandable and memorable when the colours are called by
names similar to common objects orange, ultramarine, olive, malachite green, bottle-green,
peanut-green, sea-green etc. But such colour names are very approximate, unreliable and
temporary. Their meaning also changes with observer, time, place, style, technology, language,
culture etc.

When we deal with a reasonable number of specimen, say a few thousands, to cover the whole
range of possible colours (1millions or more), the specimen must be selected according to a
system or plan. It is well known that the colours are three-dimensional. However, the dimensions
of colour are expressed in various ways in different fields. For systematic arrangements, the
dimensions should be independent of each other.
Visual Description of Colour
Wright (1984) identified two sets of visual attributes namely:

 Group A attributes are lightness, hue and chroma


 Group B attributes are whiteness, blackness and chromaticness.

According to him, group B attributes are more useful because it is most easily understood and is
more fundamental for observers to represent colour appearance. However they are less studied in
psychometric (equal perception) terms.

A colour order system is a systematic and rational method of arranging all possible colours or
subsets by means of material samples. Once the colours are arranged systematically they are
named in some descriptive terms and/or are numbered (Graham, 1985).

The colour order systems are of three types (Wyszecki, 1986):

1. Colorant-mixture system based on subtractive mixture of colorants e.g. Pantone


2. Colour-mixture system based on additive mixture of colour stimuli e.g. Ostwald system.
3. Colour appearance system based on the principles of colour perception or colour
appearance e.g. Munsell.

Examples of colorant-mixture systems are the colour atlases developed by different dye
manufacturers. ICI colour atlas (1969) was a collection of 1379 original colours and 27,580
variations printed on papers.

Pantone Colour System (Figure 3) is basically a colourant mixture system. The Pantone system
(www.pantone.co.uk) began life in 1963 in the USA, for defining colours for printers, but
expanded into other fields later, e.g. textiles in 1984, plastics in 1993, and architecture and
interiors (1925 colours) in 2002, each of which has a 6-digit numerical notation (e.g. # 19-1764)
and an inspirational colour name. This is widely used in graphic art and also in textile industry
mainly because of its low cost, though the colours are not equally spaced. The shades are
prepared on paper using printing inks. It is not a colour order system since it does not include a
continuous scale. It is more appropriately considered a colour naming system.
Fig-3: Pantone color guide
Colour appearance systems are based on the perception of colours by an observer with normal
colour vision. The scales of these systems are chosen to represent attributes of perceived colours.
However, attributes represented in various systems are different.

The main emphasis of appearance-based systems is the uniform visual spacing. The systems thus
allow easy interpolation between the samples represented and extrapolation of colours not
illustrated in a given collection. The collections of samples are generally represented in pages of
constant hue.

Six popular colour order systems, country of origin and their respective colour attributes are as
follows (Roy Choudhury, 2010) :

1. Munsell (USA) – Hue, Value and Chroma


2. Natural Colour System (Sweden) – Hue, Blackness and Chromaticness
3. Ostwald system (Germany) – Hue, Lightness and Saturation
4. DIN system (Germany) – Hue, Saturation degree and Darkness degree
5. OSA-UCS (USA) - no separate scaling of three attributes
6. Coloroid System (Hungary) – Hue, Saturation and Lightness.

Most popular appearance-based colour order system is Munsell system. The system (Figure 4,
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Albert_Henry_Munsell) consists of the following three independent
dimensions which can be represented cylindrically in three dimensions as an irregular colour
solid.

1. Hue (H), measured along circumference of the horizontal circles


2. Chroma (C) or purity of colour, measured radially outward from the neutral (grey)
vertical axis
3. Value (V), measured vertically from 0 (black) to 10 (white).
The complete Munsell specification of a sample is expressed as H V/C (e.g. 5R 4/8).

Fig-4: Munsell color system

Munsell system divides each horizontal hue circle into five unique or principal hues: Red (5R),
Yellow (5Y), Green (5G), Blue (5B), and Purple (5P), along with 5 intermediate hues (5YR,
5GY, 5BG, 5PB, 5RP) halfway between adjacent principal hues.

The Munsell atlas is usually available on painted paper in glossy (1488 chips) and matt forms
(1277 chips). A method for specifying opaque object colours such as textiles, painted panel etc.
by Munsell colour system has been described by ASTM (1980).

SCOTDIC, a textile version of Munsell created by fusion of two quite different systems -
Standard Colour of Textile (Japan) and Dictionnaire Internationale de la Couleur (France), is
adopted by over 8,000 companies worldwide. Textile standard colours of the SCOTDIC colour
system are widely used as colour tools by fashion colour professionals. The system has three
versions - glossy (2468 colours on polyester crepe fabric), matt (2038 colours on cotton poplin
fabric) and yarn (1100 colours on wool yarns). It has incorporated many bright colours and the
number of constant hue chart has been increased to 54 (20 for Wool). SCOTDIC system uses a
six digit code for each standard colour – the first two digits for hue, the second two digits for
value and the third two digits for chroma. The prefix corresponds to the material of textile bases
– P for Polyester, C for Cotton, W for Wool. Therefore, the notation C-155010 means cotton
Standard sample having hue = 15, value = 50 and chroma = 10 (Roy Choudhury, 2008).
Most of the material based atlases are now available in digitised form e.g. NCS Digital Atlas
(www.ncscolour.com), Digital Colour Atlas 3.0 (www. dtpstudio.de) etc.
Figure 5 shows two pages from the Munsell Book of Colours. Figure 5 (a) shows hue circle
having samples of 100 hues in two depths (moderate and dark shades) (samples of some hues are
missing). Figure 5 (b) shows constant hue page of hue = 05. In these pages, rows represent
constant values samples, while columns represent constant chroma samples.

Fig-5: SCOTDIC book of color (a) Hue circle (b) Constant Hue page (Hue = 05)

Instrumental Measurement of Colour


Newton said that (light) rays are not coloured, but merely has the power to simulate certain
sensations in the mind of the observer. Human eye is a highly versatile detector of light and
colour. An observer can perceive chromatic attributes and various geometric factors (direction,
texture, shape and many others) simultaneously. An instrument till date is far behind in
versatility. It can measure only one attribute at time. In other words, we need several instruments
to measure various aspect of visual perception.

Basically there are three types of colorimetric instruments in use - colorimeter,


spectrophotometer and spectroradiometer. They are available in the market with varying degrees
of sophistication and specialisation. While the spectroradiometer measures in illuminant-mode,
the other two generally measure in object mode. The recent trend on instrumental process control
has resulted in use of on-line instruments. However majority of the colorimetric instruments till
date are off-line and mostly used in laboratories. Laboratory instruments should be highly
accurate and standardised, while on-line instruments should be rugged under various
environments and should have good precision and firmness.
The colorimeters measure colour in terms of the quantities of the three primaries required to
match the colour. On the other hand, spectrophotometer measures percent reflectance or
transmittance of the object plotted against wavelength at regular intervals of 1 nm, 5 nm, 10 nm,
20 nm throughout the visible range of light i.e. 380-750 nm or for practical purposes 400-700
nm.
Colour by Numbers!
The colour of an object depends on the relative quantity of the light reflected at different
wavelengths within the visible range (400-700 nm), but our colour sensation is not analytical in
nature. We cannot judge the existence of lights of different wavelengths individually. We get the
sensation from cumulative effect. As this cumulative quantity can be matched by mixing three
primary lights it is proved that our eyes have three types of colour-detecting cones, the stimuli
generated by them are mixed before reaching to brain. Various other phenomena of colour have
also lead to the conclusion that our eyes have three types of cones only. Each object colour is
sensed by each type of cone separately and each type sends a stimulus to the brain.

So, for each object colour, the brain receives three separate stimuli. Keeping similarity with
colour mixing experiment, we can consider the three types of cones as red-sensitive r, green-
sensitive g and blue-sensitive b equivalent to the three additive primaries. The spectral sensitivity
of the three colour-detecting cones has been measured and named as colour matching data r(λ),
g(λ), b(λ) (bar stands for statistical average data of a number of colour normal observers) and
subsequently transformed into more usable CIE standard observer functions, x(λ), y(λ), z(λ). The
area under the functions signifies the amounts of three stimuli to be transmitted to the brain for
the incidence of light having one unit of energy at each of visible wavelength. These three
stimuli are represented by three numbers called CIE tristimulus values (X, Y, Z) which may be
calculated as follows:

Where E(λ) is the relative spectral energy distribution of the illuminant, R(λ) is the spectral
reflectance factor of the object and x(λ), y(λ), z(λ) are the colour matching functions of the CIE
standard observer. K is a normalising constant.
A light source is an essential component of visualisation and measurement of colour. Various
light sources, such as daylight (D65), tungsten lamp (A), fluorescent lamp (F1 to F12),
departmental lamp (TL84) etc., emit different amounts of energy in the visible region of the
spectrum that can be expressed in the form of its relative spectral power distribution (SPD)
curve. An illuminant is an ideal form of a light source with defined SPD. The SPD of light
source may vary, but that of illuminant is constant or defined and hence, it is used for
quantification of colour as mentioned above.
In the visual observing situation, the observer is the human eye that receives the light reflected
from or transmitted through an object and the brain which perceives the vision. Since different
human perceive colour in different ways, subjectively, attempts have been made to standardise
the human observer as a numerical representation of what the average person sees. This standard
observer could then be used in lieu of a human observer when assessments are made
instrumentally. In 1931 CIE published the 2° CIE Standard Observer function based on colour
matching by viewing through a hole of 2° field of view. Later it came to know that cones present
in a larger area of the eye. Hence, in 1964, the 10° Standard Observer function was developed
which is now universally used.

When two objects have equal tristimulus values under a particular illuminant, they will look
alike in colour under the said illuminant. If their reflectance curves are same they will look alike
in colour under any illuminant (universal match). Otherwise they may or may not differ in colour
when the illuminant is changed. On the other hand, two alike colours with different lightness
may have different tristimulus values. To express colours in two-dimensional space independent
of lightness, chromaticity coordinates may be calculated as follows:

As x + y + z = 1, only two chromaticity coordinates x and y have been recommended by CIE to


specify chromaticity. Instead of tristimulus values (X, Y, Z), colours can also be specified by a
luminance parameter Y and two colour coordinates x and y (Yxy colour space).
However, none of the chromaticity coordinates is correlated with any meaningful visual attribute
of colour.

When the chromaticity coordinates of spectral colours are plotted in such diagram, a horse-shoe
shaped curve called chromaticity diagram is obtained. Chromaticity diagram is of great help in
finding colours generated by additive colour mixing. If two lights are represented by two points
on the chromaticity diagram, any additive mixture of the two will correspond to a point on the
straight line joining the two points. Since the locus of spectral colours is concave, all real colours
must fall within the area bounded by the spectrum locus and joining the ends.

Figure 6 shows CIE 1976 chromaticity diagram along with location of different spectral colours
(i.e. monochromatic lights of different wavelengths in nm). The figure also shows the regions of
locations of different surface colours viewed under daylight. Approximately in the centre of the
curve is the neutral point, which represents the chromaticities of white, grey or blacks. The
illuminant C having chromaticity co-ordinates x = 0.310 and y = 0.317 also lies at the centre of
the curve (point C). The locations of other illuminants depend on their colour temperatures. The
chromaticity diagram is closed by line indicating the locations of non-spectral purple colours.
Fig-6: Location of Spectral Colors in 1976 CIE Chromaticity Diagram

CIE system is very successful for colour specification and is universally used for colour
measurement. The system is unchanged since 1931 except some minor change in 1964. CIE
tristimulus values are related only to the colour. It ignores all other aspects like surface texture,
gloss etc. which influence colour appearance significantly. It does not take into account
geometrical arrangements for illumination and viewing and the instrumental measures will match
visual assessments only if the above geometries are similar.

The main limitation of CIE system is its visual non-uniformity. Equal changes in Yxy colour
space do not correspond to equal colour perception. In other words, the distribution of colours in
CIE colour space is non-uniform with regard to visual perception.

Uniform Colour Scales


CIE tristimulus values or chromaticity coordinates are not very convenient for identifying colour
of the objects, because these were designed for colour stimuli of different modes. None of the
values are directly correlated with any visual attributes of colour. Only Y value has high
correlation with luminance and object lightness. The spacing of colours in chromaticity diagram
is not visible uniform. A number of uniform colour scales are, therefore, developed which can
represent colours with equal visual spacing and are directly related to meaningful attributes of
colour appearance.
In 1976 CIE recommended CIELUV and CIELAB uniform colour spaces. Colorant industries
were in favour of a formula similar to Adam-Nickerson (AN40) formula, popular at that time.
The CIELAB formula was acceptable as colour-difference values were about 1.1 times those
produced by AN40 formula. On the other hand, television industries preferred a colour space
(CIELUV) associated with a chromaticity diagram because of its simple way of presentation of
additive mixture which also occur in television and other display devices. No simple relation
exists between the two colour scales.

Both CIELUV and CIELAB formulae are plotted on rectangular coordinates. Lightness L*
function is same for both colour spaces and is represented by the formula,

L* = 116(Y/Yn)1/3 - 16 if Y/Yn = 0.008856

For CIELAB Colour Space, Red-green attribute, a* = 500[f(X/Xn) - f(Y/Yn)]

Yellow-blue attribute, b* = 200[f(Y/Yn) - f(Z/Zn)]

Subscript n represent nominally white object colour stimulus given by a perfect reflecting
diffuser as reference surface illuminated by standard illuminant. For standard daylight illuminant
D65, the values are: Xn = 95.047, Yn = 100.000, Zn =108.883. White object has been taken into
account because we perceive colours in relation to surrounding colours.

In recent years efforts have been made to define CIE correlates for perceptual attributes like
lightness, chroma and hue. Hence, two new attributes corresponding to visual attributes have
been derived from a* and b* values namely:

Metric chroma, C*ab = [(a*)2 + (b*)2] 1/2

Hue angle, h = tan -1 (b*/a*).

CIELAB colour space is shown in the Figure 7. Lightness L* is represented in vertical axis with
white (L* = 100) at the top and black (L* = 0) at the bottom. Chromatic colours are represented
by two opponent a* and b* axes. Red and green are represented by a* axis – the positive values
are for red and negative for green. Similarly positive b* values are for yellow and negative b*
values are blue.
Fig-7: CIELAB color space

Colour Difference
Measurement of difference in colour between two objects is one of the most complicated aspects
of colour vision. The colour discrimination may be general/overall or of specific psychophysical
attribute like hue, chroma or lightness. For colorant users like textile, leather, paper or paint
industries, the difference in colour of two specimens namely a standard and a sample or of
different portions of a coloured specimen may be more important than the measurement of
absolute colour (Luo, 1986). The prime difficulty is that the perception of colour-difference by
an individual is not a precise phenomenon and may vary on successive assessment (Zeller and
Hemmendinger, 1978). Colour-difference perception and evaluation may also vary widely
among individuals.

The colour-difference evaluation is necessary for day to day colour control and for colour
matching in colouration industries like textile, paint etc. Colour-difference formulae have
accelerated the instrumental pass-fail device a success.

The colour differences are calculated by subtracting values of the standard from the respective
values of the sample. The total colour-difference (ΔE) is intended to be single number metric for
pass/fail decisions and in the CIELAB system ΔE is given by the following equation:

ΔE = [ (ΔL*)² + (Δa*)² + (Δb*)² ]1/2

In addition to the overall colour difference (ΔE), the difference in individual parameters of the
standard and a sample are also estimated e.g. ΔL = L (sample) - L (standard).
These may indicate some specific visual difference such as
if ΔL < 0 or > 0, the sample is darker or lighter respectively,
if Δa* < 0 or > 0, the sample is greener or redder respectively,
if Δb* < 0 or > 0, the sample is bluer or yellower respectively.
ΔE (CIELAB) values are not always reliable in predicting perceptible differences between object
colours, especially when the variations are in different visual attributes. This is due to the fact
that the visual spacing along L, a* and b* axes are unequal.

The formulae based on surface-mode colour discrimination data mainly aimed at single number
shade-passing. Much of the available visual data related to physical samples are supplied by the
textile and dye industries, where prime criteria is that whether the colours will be acceptable
against the respective standards.

Main reason for poor correlation with visual data of the earlier formulae was equal weighting of
the colour parameters. The weighted values of lightness, chroma and hue showed significant
improvement in performance of colour-difference equations. The weights can be determined by
empirical fitting to experimental data-sets. These formulae are optimized by visual acceptability/
perceptibility scaling. They represent most closely the average visual results of judgments of
colour difference of textile and other physical samples under normal evaluation conditions
(Kuehni, 1984)

A few colour-difference formulae based on surface-mode colour discrimination data are:

1. JPC79 colour-difference Formula


2. CMC (l:c) colour-difference Formula
3. BFD (l:c) colour-difference Formula
4. CIE 94 colour-difference Formula
5. CIE 2000 colour-difference Formula

However, none of the above formulae is completely satisfactory and acceptability of a particular
formula is decided mutually by producers and users/salers.

Colorimetry is the science of quantitative measurement of colour. Even though study on colour
science started as far back as the Newtonian age, research continues even today. Colour Science
is a vast field. Hunt (1977) identified three phases of development of colorimetry - colour
matching, colour difference evaluation and lastly, prediction of colour appearance. It is now
possible to predict colour appearance of an object under a test illuminant from the colour
appearance data under a reference illuminant with the help of complex mathematical
transformations.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen