Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/12241577

The Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale detects differences among


newborn infants of optimal health

Article  in  Journal of Pediatric Psychology · January 2001


Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

30 1,070

2 authors:

Cristina Lundqvist Persson Karl-Göran Sabel


Region Västra Götaland Södra Älvsborgs Sjukhus, Borås, Sweden
15 PUBLICATIONS   494 CITATIONS    62 PUBLICATIONS   1,582 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Neonatal nutrition View project

A Thesis about Art Therapy; Subject Health and Lifestyle at the University of Halmstad. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Karl-Göran Sabel on 11 August 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Pediatric Psychology, Vol. 25, No. 8, 2000, pp. 577–582

Brief Report: The Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral


Assessment Scale Detects Differences Among
Newborn Infants of Optimal Health
Cristina Lundqvist,1 MS, and Karl-Göran Sabel,2 MD, PhD
1
Lund University and 2Borås Hospital

Objective: To determine whether the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) can detect
behavioral differences in newborn infants of optimal health and, if such differences appear, also detect
gender differences among those neonates.
Methods: Participants were a group of healthy Swedish neonates, 20 boys and 18 girls. The infants were as-
sessed by the NBAS under standardized conditions at 48–72 hours of age, at the midpoint between two
meals.
Results: All items except those in the dimensions Autonomic System and Motor System had a wide inter-
quartile range. The trend was that girls had higher median item profiles, which means a higher level of
functioning than boys. Four out of seven median values in the dimension Social Interactive Organization, as
well as the median value in the self-quieting item in the dimension State Regulation, were significantly
higher for girls. The interquartile range of the items seemed wider for boys than for girls.
Conclusions: The results indicate behavioral variability among healthy neonates. Gender differences were
also observed with girls showing higher levels of functioning than boys.

Key words: assessment; neonates; infant development; infant health.

Investigators have been attracted recently to the be- (1990) emphasized the importance of caring for the
havioral repertoire and functioning of the newborn parent and infant as a unit since the infant develops
infant. Despite many studies addressing the impact within this system and suggested a transdisciplinary
of prenatal events and the risks of prematurity, approach. Schore (1994) suggested that failures in
there are still many unanswered questions related the dyadic processes of affect regulation lead to de-
to early prevention of later developmental difficul- velopmental psychopathology. Many researchers
ties. For example, according to Kadesjö and Gillberg (e.g., Nordberg, 1995) have demonstrated that the
(1998), children apparently are born healthy and quality of social-interactive experiences has impor-
develop dysfunctional behaviors later on. tant implications for children’s mental develop-
Scientists have stressed different requirements ment. Some researchers have focused on the baby’s
for successful development. Brazelton and Cramer contribution to this interactive process (Murray,
1994) whereas others have focused on the impor-
All correspondence should be sent to Cristina Lundqvist, Department
of Paediatrics, Borås Hospital, 501 82 Borås, Sweden. E-mail: cristina.
tance of the parents’ behaviors, especially the moth-
lundqvist@psychology.lu.se. er’s. Thompson (1990) stated the fundamental

䉷 2000 Society of Pediatric Psychology


578 Lundqvist and Sabel

principle that others initially regulate emotions, but infants and whether such possible variations are
the baby, as development proceeds, becomes in- gender-related. Specifically, we hypothesized that
creasingly self-regulated because of neurophysiolog- behavioral differences among newborn infants are
ical maturation. gender-related.
With insights from clinical pediatrics, psychia-
try, and developmental and clinical psychology,
Brazelton and his colleagues (1973) constructed the Method
Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (NBAS) to
demonstrate both positive and negative characteris- Participants
tics of behavior. In contrast to the pathological
models evaluating deficits, Brazelton, Lester, and Thirty-eight neonates participated in this study (20
their colleagues (1976) considered the baby’s whole boys and 18 girls). The mean weight of the girls was
behavioral repertoire as an important factor in his 3455 g (SD ⫽ 396 g) and of the boys 3605 g (SD ⫽
or her development. The NBAS has been revised 434 g). The mean length of the girls was 50.5 cm
twice, in 1984 and 1995. The scale combines neuro- (SD ⫽ 1.5 cm) and of the boys 51.3 cm (SD ⫽ 2.0
logical items with an extended behavioral repertoire cm). The mean gestational age at birth for the girls
of the infant in an interactional process. It measures was 39.5 weeks (SD ⫽ 0.9) and for the boys 39.6
the coping capacities and adaptive strategies of the weeks (SD ⫽ 0.9). Gestational age was determined
infant. The optimal interactional process is de- from the date of the last menstrual period and by
scribed as a condition wherein the newborn baby ultrasound verification at 15–16 weeks of gestation.
uses most of his or her energy to interact with the
environment, has physiological control, and is con- Procedure
trolling his or her motor activity and state
(Brazelton, 1990). These integrative tasks proceed in Very healthy infants were recruited for the study to
a developmentally hierarchical manner with Auto- reduce the impact of perinatal factors. Midwives at
nomic Regulation preceding Motor Organization, the maternity ward were asked to register healthy
followed by the task of State Regulation and finally 3-day-old infants on the mornings of prespecified
Social Interaction (Als, 1982). days. To be classified as “very healthy,” the baby
Gender-based behavioral differences have been had to fulfill certain requirements: (1) an Apgar
established in studies on young children. Trevar- score (Apgar, 1954) of at least eight points at 5 min-
then (1996) found that asymmetries in emotional utes; (2) the baby must not have been instrumen-
and communicative gestures are seen earlier among tally delivered; (3) the baby must not have a twin;
girls. Lindahl and Heimann (1997) reported that (4) the baby must have had a minimum gestational
among 9-month-old children mother-daughter dy- age of 37 weeks; (5) the baby must have had a birth
ads displayed significantly higher degrees of social weight of at least 2500 g yet not more than 4500 g;
proximity than mother-son dyads, measured (6) the baby’s mother must not have been given any
through nine items focusing on both communica- analgesics during labor.
tion and social interaction. Nordberg, Rydelius, and These infants were then checked in accordance
Zetterström (1991) found that boys at 1 year and 4 to Kyllerman and Hagberg’s optimal health condi-
years of age were more vulnerable to psychosocial tions, a modification of Prechtl’s optimal concept
stress. Davis and Emory (1995) found that boys had (Kyllerman & Hagberg, 1983). The optimal health
a lower threshold for stress reactivity. Gender-based conditions included prepartal, partal, and postpar-
differences have been found among certain groups tal items. Examples of prepartal items are maternal
of children with dysfunctional behaviors and dis- age of 18–30 years, mother’s pregnancy number 4
eases; that is, a higher percentage of boys have prob- or less, and no maternal disorders in pregnancy (in-
lems related to attention, perception, and motor fection, diabetes, surgery, anemia-Hb 8.0g/ml, etc.).
control (Kadesjö & Gillberg, 1998). Also, more boys Examples of partal items are amniotic fluid clear
than girls die in Sudden Infant Death Syndrome and no pathological labor (duration less than 1/2 or
(SIDS) (McLaughlin et al., 1995). more than 24 hours). Examples of postpartal items
The aim of this study was to explore whether are no apnea, no hypoglycemia, and blood sugar
the NBAS would detect behavioral variations in levels ⬍30 mg/100 ml. The postpartal items were
an optimally healthy group of Swedish newborn recorded on the third day of life. Optimal health
Behavioral Differences in Newborns 579

in this study was defined as scoring positive to all cluster); (7) regulation of states (which includes
optimal conditions except the age of the mother cuddliness, consolability, self-quieting activity, and
(our upper limit is 35 years instead of 30 years). hand-to-mouth activity).
The infants were examined at 48–72 hours of The interactive character of the scale distin-
age to minimize the influences of the delivery. The guishes it from other assessments. It demands thor-
aim was to examine the baby at the midpoint be- ough knowledge of newborn babies and of an
tween two meals. This goal was not always achieved infant’s developmental course as well as examiner
since breastfed infants often are fed at irregular in- flexibility. Examiner training procedures contain
tervals. both a training phase and a reliability phase.
No more than three babies were examined per This study used two of the supplementary items,
day and if there were more babies fulfilling the re- specifically examiner facilitation and examiner’s
quirements that day, the ones examined were cho- emotional response to that infant. These were cho-
sen consecutively. All parents gave their consent to sen with regard to their theoretical connection to
the assessment. All of the babies were examined by attachment behavior and also because neonatal so-
one of the authors (CL), who has been trained and cial behavior with an examiner during the NBAS ex-
certified to be reliable in administering and scoring amination has been found to be related to neonatal
the NBAS by the Boston group at Harvard Medical social behavior with the mother and to maternal
School. The babies were examined in a small and functional stimulation (Arco, DeMeis, Self, & Gu-
quiet room, in daylight, and in the presence of at trecht, 1984).
least one parent. Complete assessment was not pos- The behavioral items, including the supplemen-
sible for five of the infants. Three babies, one boy tary items, are scored on 9-point scales, and reflexes
and two girls, woke up during the measurement of are scored on a 4-point scale. The infant’s reflex
the responses decrement to light, rattle, bell, and score is the total number of diverging scores. In
tactile stimulation. One boy and one girl fell asleep some of the behavioral items the end-point score 1
during the orientation items. or 9 represents the optimal status of function, in
others the central score of 5 represents it. The rea-
Materials son for this is that Brazelton did not want to give
the baby a sum score, which may be misused. In
The NBAS is a multidimensional, multi-item scale this study the item scores have been transformed to
(Brazelton & Nugent, 1995). The basic score sheet labels (italicized numbers) representing an in-
includes 28 behavioral items, 18 reflex items, and 6 creased level of functioning, where 9 means a high
supplementary items. The supplementary items level of functioning and 1 a very low level of func-
were constructed to measure the quality of the ba- tioning. The item consolability, which measures the
by’s responsiveness, the help the examiner has to number of maneuvers the examiner has to utilize in
invest to get the infant’s optimal performance, and order to bring the baby to a quite state, may serve
also the response of the examiner to the infants. In as an example. In that case, 1 point stands for the
order to compare the evaluations of the infants’ be- baby not being consolable at all and 9 points means
havior, the items that interact in similar ways have that simply seeing the examiner’s face consoles the
been grouped into seven clusters describing global baby. The measurement level of data is ordinal,
functions, following Lester, Als, and Brazelton meaning that there is a defined rank order of the
(1982). The clusters are as follows: (1) reflexes (e.g., category levels, but the levels do not represent any
plantar grasp, babinski, ankle tonus); (2) motor sys- mathematical values. Furthermore, there are no de-
tem (which includes tonus, motor maturity, pull- fined distances between adjacent levels. These prop-
to-sit, defensive movements and level of activity); erties mean that arithmetic that creates sum scores
(3) autonomic stability (which includes tremors, cannot be applied (Coste, Fermanian, & Venot,
startles and skin color); (4) habituation (which in- 1995).
cludes response decrement to light, bell, and tactile
stimulation); (5) social interactive organization Analysis
(which includes animate and inanimate visual, au-
ditory orientation items, and alertness); (6) range of Since normal distribution of data was not assumed,
states (which includes peak of excitement, rapidity median values and nonparametric statistical meth-
of build-up, irritability, lability of states and the ods were used. Gender differences in the median
580 Lundqvist and Sabel

values for each item of the clusters reflexes, auto- Table I. Clusters and Behavioral Items
nomic system, motor system, habituation, state Clusters Boys Girls
organization, state regulation, social interactive or-
Reflexes
ganization, and two of the supplementary items
Diverging score 4 (2–5) 3 (1–5)
have been analyzed by applying the Wilcoxon- Motor system
Mann Whitney test. In order to obtain an overall General tone 5 (5–6) 5 (5–5)
significance level of at least .05, the p values within Motor maturity 5 (5–6) 6 (5–6)
a cluster have been adjusted according to Holm’s Pull to sit 6 (5–8) 8 (6–8)
(1979) multiple test procedure. Defensive movements 5 (5–6) 6 (5–6)
Activity level 4 (3–5) 5 (4–5)
Autonomic stability

Results Tremulousness 8 (8–8) 8 (8–8)


Startles 8 (8–8) 8 (8–8)
Lability of skin color 5 (3–5) 5 (4–5)
Except the items startle, tremulousness, and most
Habituation
of the motor items, the other behavioral items had
Response decrement to light 6 (4–7) 6 (4–7)
a wide interquartile range (see Table I). The median Response decrement to rattle 4 (3–7) 5 (4–7)
profiles of the girls were in general higher than Response decrement to bell 6 (3–7) 5 (5–6)
those of the boys (see Table I). According to the Response decrement to foot stimulation 5 (4–6) 5 (4–6)
Wilcoxon-Mann Whitney test, four median values Social interactive organization
in the social interactive organization cluster were Animate visual 3 (2–7) 6 (5–7)
significantly higher for girls (the orientation items Animate visual and auditory 5 (3–7) 7 (6–8)*
animate auditory, p ⬍ .01; animate visual and audi- Inanimate visual 5 (3–7) 6 (4–7)

tory, p ⬍ .05; inanimate auditory, p ⬍ .05; inani- Inanimate visual and auditory 4 (3–7) 8 (7–8)**
Animate auditory 4 (3–6) 7 (6–8)**
mate visual and auditory, p ⬍ .01). Girls’ median
Inanimate auditory 4 (4–7) 7 (4–8)*
value in the item self-quieting (cluster state regula-
Alertness 5 (4–6) 6 (5–8)
tion) was also significantly higher than that for boys
Range of states
( p ⬍ .01). Further, most of the interquartile ranges Peak of excitement 3 (2–4) 4 (3–4)
of the items seemed wider for boys (see Table I). Rapidity of build-up 5 (3–7) 5 (5–7)
Irritability 4 (2–6) 6 (5–8)
Lability of states 6 (4–8) 7 (6–8)
Discussion Regulation of states
Cuddliness 4 (4–5) 5 (5–7)
The aim of this study was to explore whether the Consolability 5 (2–6) 6 (5–7)

NBAS could discover behavioral differences among Self-quieting 4 (2–6) 6 (6–7)**


Hand to mouth 4 (2–7) 7 (5–8)
medically very healthy infants and demonstrate
Supplementary items
that a baby may be healthy and seem strong and
Examiner facilitation 6 (4–7) 6 (5–8)
robust yet may also be vulnerable. For example,
Examiner’s emotional response 5 (3–6) 6 (5–8)
even healthy babies may have difficulties in self-
regulation and therefore be hard to interact with. The values represent median and interquartile ranges.
*p ⬍ .05.
The study indicated that the NBAS detects behav- **p ⬍ .01.
ioral differences among optimally healthy infants.
The fact that the supplementary items examiner
facilitation and examiner’s emotional response had and boys. The girls had higher functioning scores,
a wide interquartile range emphasized that babies especially in the social interactive organization clus-
of optimal health need different amounts of help to ter. This result is in accordance with Trevarthen
organize themselves and provoke different feelings. (1996) and points in the direction of gender-based
This in turn has an impact on the interactive pro- behavioral differences as innate. Since a higher per-
cesses, attachment behavior, and infant develop- centage of boys develop dysfunctional behaviors
ment (Murray, Fiori-Cowlley, Hooper, & Cooper, and are overrepresented among certain diseases,
1996). such as nervous disorders and lower respiratory
Further, the study confirmed the hypothesis tract infections (van den Bosch, Huygen, van den
that there are behavioral differences between girls Hoogen, & van Weel, 1992), we suggest it would be
Behavioral Differences in Newborns 581

valuable to further explore qualitative gender- based on the NBAS, with which to identify healthy
based differences. infants who are at risk for attachment disturbances
As clinicians, we see a demand for a diagnostic and developmental disorders, is desirable. The latter
instrument that discriminates between immaturity, builds the premises for the next study.
stress effects, and individual characteristics in neo-
natal wards, maternity wards, and health care cen-
ters. It seems that the NBAS is a satisfactory tool in Acknowledgments
this work. The results of this study are parsimonious
to previous research that has demonstrated the ef- This study was supported by grants from The First
fectiveness of the NBAS (Brazelton & Nugent, 1995). of May Flower Foundation, Sweden. Special thanks
Therefore, we suggest that the NBAS be used com- go to Dr. Elisabeth Svensson, Biostatistics Branch,
plementary to neurological and pediatric examina- Chalmers University of Technology, Göteborg, Swe-
tion when specific questions about the baby exist den, for her support and professional advice.
and when the baby already has been identified as
an infant at risk. Infants who show considerable de- Received March 10, 1999; revisions received August 3,
viations should be monitored with repeated assess- 1999, and October 22, 1999; accepted November 30,
ments. 1999
A simplified and shorter screening instrument

References

Als, H. (1982). Toward a synactive theory of development: Kadesjö, B., & Gillberg, C. (1998). Attention deficits and
Promise for the assessment and support of infant indi- clumsiness in Swedish 7-year-old children. Develop-
viduality. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 229–243. mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 12, 796–804.
Apgar, V. A. (1954). Proposal for a new method of evalua- Kyllerman, M., & Hagberg, G. (1983). Reduced optimality
tion of the newborn infant. Current Researches in Anes- in pre- and perinatal conditions in a Swedish newborn
thesia and Analgesia, 32, 260–267. population. Neuropediatrics, 14, 7–4.
Arco, C. M. B., DeMeis, P. A., Self, P. A., & Gutrecht, N. Lester, B., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1982). Regional ob-
(1984). Interrelationships among maternal and infant stetric anesthetic and newborn behavior: A reanalysis
characteristics during the neonatal period. Journal of towards synergistic effects. Child Development, 53,
Pediatric Psychology, 2, 131–147. 687–692.
Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Lindahl, L. B., & Heimann, M. (1997). Social proximity in
Scale. Clinics in Developmental Medicine, No 50. Lon- early mother-infant interactions: Implications for gen-
don: Spastics International Medical Publications. der. Early Development and Parenting, 2, 83–88.
Brazelton, T. B. (1990). Saving the bath water. Child Devel- McLaughlin, S. A., Valdes, M. G., Jacobsen, R. M., Wollan,
opment, 61, 1661–1671. P. C., Beard, C. M., Weissman, L., & Jacobson, S. J.
Brazelton, T. B., & Cramer, B. G. (1990). The earliest rela- (1995). Incidence of sudden infant death syndrome in
tionship. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Olmsted County, Minnesota: 1945 through 1992.
Brazelton, T. B., & Lester, B. M. (1976). New approaches to Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 9, 837–843.
developmental screening of infants. New York: Elsevier. Murray, L. (1994, June). The infant’s contribution to the
Brazelton, T. B., & Nugent, J. K. (1995). Neonatal Behav- mother-infant relationship in the context of postnatal de-
ioral Assessment Scale. 3rd ed. Clinics in Developmental pression. Paper presented at the 9th International Con-
Medicine, No. 137. London: MacKeith Press. ference on Infants Studies, Paris.
Coste, J., Fermanian, J., & Venot, A. (1995). Methodologi- Murray, L., Fiori-Cowlley, A., Hooper, R., & Cooper, P.
cal and statistical problems in the construction of (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associ-
composite measurement scales: A survey of six medi- ated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and
cal and epidemiology journals. Statistics in Medicine, later infant outcome. Child Development, 5, 2512–
14, 331–345. 2526.
Davis, M., & Emory, E. (1995). Sex differences in neonatal Nordberg, L. (1995). Psychmotor and mental development
stress reactivity. Child Development, 66, 4–27. at four years of age. Relation to psychosocial condi-
Holm, S. (1979) A simple sequentially Rejective Multiple tions and health. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 409
Test Procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, (Suppl), 1–46.
65–70. Nordberg, L., Rydelius, P-A., & Zetterström, R. (1991). Psy-
582 Lundqvist and Sabel

chomotor and mental development from birth to age Trevarthen, C. (1996). Lateral asymmetries in infancy: Im-
of four years; Sex differences and their relation to plications for the development of the hemispheres.
home environment. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, 378 Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4, 571–586.
(Suppl), 1–25. Van den Bosch, W. J., Huygen, F. J., van den Hoogen,
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the H. J., & van Weel, C. (1992). Morbidity in early child-
self. The neurobiology of emotional development. Hills- hood, sex differences, birth order and social class.
dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health Care, 2, 118–123.
Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. Ne-
braska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 38, 367–467).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen