Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

AJSLP

Article

The Association Between Expressive


Grammar Intervention and Social and
Emergent Literacy Outcomes for
Preschoolers With SLI
Karla N. Washingtona

Purpose: To determine whether (a) expressive grammar Results: Only intervention participants experienced significant
intervention facilitated social and emergent literacy outcomes gains in social and emergent literacy outcomes and main-
better than no intervention and (b) expressive grammar gains tained these gains for 3 months postintervention. Expressive
and /or initial expressive grammar level predicted social and grammar gains was the only single significant predictor of these
emergent literacy outcomes. outcomes.
Method: This investigation was a follow-up to a recently Conclusions: Expressive grammar intervention was asso-
published study exploring the impact of grammatical language ciated with broad impacts on social and emergent literacy
intervention on expressive grammar outcomes for preschoolers outcomes that were maintained beyond the intervention period.
with specific language impairment (SLI). Twenty-two 3- to Gains in expressive grammar predicted these outcomes.
5-year-old preschoolers received ten 20-minute intervention Social and emergent literacy skills were positively affected for
sessions addressing primary deficits in grammatical morphology. preschoolers with SLI during a grammatical language intervention
Participants’ social and emergent literacy skills were not program.
targeted. Twelve children awaiting intervention, chosen from
the same selection pool as intervention participants, served
as controls. Blind assessments of social and emergent literacy Key Words: social skills, emergent literacy, specific
outcomes were completed at preintervention, immediately language impairment, expressive grammar intervention,
postintervention, and 3 months postintervention. preschool outcomes

A
pproximately 7% of young children are identified with development (Cabell, Justice, Zucker, & McGinty, 2009;
specific language impairment (SLI; Tomblin et al., Cabell et al., 2010; McCabe, 2005).
1997). These children experience an unexpected Because children with SLI typically receive grammatical
failure to acquire language skills at a typical rate (Leonard, language intervention to address hallmark deficits in gram-
1998). This delay in development occurs despite the presence matical skills, researchers and practitioners in speech-language
of typical nonverbal cognitive abilities; hearing, emotional, pathology need to consider whether the intervention can also
or behavioral regulation; and neurological function needed simultaneously address other areas of development, such as
to support language acquisition (Bishop, 1997; Plante, 1998; social and literacy skills, as part of a preventative program,
Stark & Tallal, 1981). It is well established that children with even for those preschoolers who do not evidence deficits in
SLI are limited in their grammatical skills development these two areas (Washington, 2011; Washington & Warr-Leeper,
(Leonard, Camarata, Pawlowska, Brown, & Camarata, 2006, 2006). In addition, we need to consider whether initial level
2008). However, other core areas of impairment for some, of language functioning for grammatical skills and /or gains
but not all, children with SLI include social and literacy skills in grammatical skills achieved during intervention might
predict social and literacy outcomes. Earlier researchers
a have shown that inter-relationships exist between language
University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada
and social skills (Bishop, 1997; McCabe, 2005) and between
Correspondence to Karla N. Washington, who is now with the language and literacy skills (Cabell et al., 2009, 2010; Catts
University of Cincinnati: washink2@ucmail.uc.edu et al., 2002). Thus, exploring the inter-rationships between
Editor: Carol Scheffner Hammer language skills and social and literacy skills is important to
Associate Editor: Teresa Ukrainetz identifying the nature of these relationships in preschoolers
Received March 7, 2011 with SLI.
Revision received August 23, 2011 Information on outcomes and predictors of outcomes is
Accepted September 8, 2012 critical to demonstrating the broad impact of grammatical
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0026) skills (e.g., initial expressive grammar level and expressive

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013 • A American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 113
grammar gains) on other co-occurring areas of development morphosyntax, including the requirement to attend and listen
in which children may demonstrate ongoing deficits in critically to grammar, could enhance control of attention and
the later school years. The existence of social and literacy responsiveness and thus lead to gains in untargeted domains.
deficits in the later school years for older children with SLI Evidence for the spreading effects of morphosyntax therapy
supports the investigation of these co-occurring areas of on phonology has been demonstrated (e.g., Tyler, Kerry,
development during the preschool years (Baker & Cantwell, Haskill, & Tolbert, 2002). However, a study by Frome Loeb,
1987; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 2002; Fujiki, Spackman, Stoke, and Fey (2001) showed little evidence to support the
Brinton, & Hall, 2004). claim that FFW-Language treatment, which included train-
ing for morphosyntax, had a positive impact on prereading
Social Skills in Preschoolers with SLI and reading skills in a small sample of 5- to 8-year olds.
Furthermore, the observed positive changes in pragmatics
A study based on parent and teacher reports of 3- to skills could not be confidently linked to the intervention.
5-year-old preschoolers’ social and behavioral competencies Substantiation for the spreading effects of grammatical
found that some preschoolers with SLI experienced greater language intervention on untargeted domains such as social
social and behavioral difficulties compared with preschoolers and literacy skills is not yet apparent in the published peer-
without language disorders (McCabe, 2005). Good social reviewed literature. For preschoolers with SLI, grammatical
skills are needed for the developing child because they language intervention could affect other key developmental
provide access to well-needed social interactions necessary areas that might benefit from the attention and consistency
for continued language development, later academic success, provided during therapy. As a consequence of ongoing needs
and the formation and maintenance of friendships, one of the in later social and literacy functioning, it becomes important
most basic human needs (Liiva & Cleave, 2005; Washington, to investigate the possible benefits of grammatical language
2007). Evidence for the ongoing deficits in social skills intervention on the development of these skills for preschoolers
functioning in older children with SLI also supports the with SLI. Particular features of the grammar program could
investigation of these skills in the preschool years (Baker & help this population of children socialize better with others
Cantwell, 1987; Durkin & Conti-Ramsden, 2007; Fujiki, and attend to print, thus demonstrating the broad and positive
Spackman, Brinton, & Hall, 2004). impact of this type of therapy.
In an earlier study, the effects of paper-based and computer-
Emergent Literacy Skills in Preschoolers with SLI assisted expressive grammar intervention (EGI) were com-
Researchers have examined the emergent literacy skills pared with no intervention (NI) in a sample of thirty-four
of preschoolers, which lead to future reading, writing, and 3- to 5-year old preschoolers with SLI who were enrolled in
spelling abilities (Catts, 1993; Catts, Bridges, Little, & Tomblin, educational settings (Washington et al., 2011). During in-
2008; Catts et al., 2002; Scarborough, 2001; Whitehurst & tervention, multiple repeated and guided opportunities to
Lonigan, 2001). It has been reported that almost half of produce grammatically correct sentences were provided and
kindergartners with language impairment will demonstrate embedded within socialization development opportunities,
reading disability by the second grade (Catts et al., 2002). as well as print awareness and book knowledge activities.
Similar to social skills development, not all preschoolers Results demonstrated that EGI participants experienced sig-
with SLI demonstrate deficits in literacy development, in- nificantly greater expressive grammar outcomes compared
cluding alphabet knowledge and print concepts (Cabell et al., with NI participants immediately postintervention and at
2009, 2010; Justice, Bowles, & Skibbe, 2006). 3 months postintervention.
The development of preschoolers’ print concepts (e.g., The repeated practice opportunities that required atten-
words and knowledge of book conventions) is important, tion and critical listening to grammar could have helped
as it is a predictor of future reading success. Print concepts preschoolers to have better control and focus for specific
represent the understanding of the forms, functions, and social skills and print concepts. The specific social skills that
conventions of print (Justice et al., 2006). However, this area are of interest include interactions with others, using play
is under-researched compared with the vast research on and leisure time to engage with others, and demonstrations of
phonological awareness (Justice et al., 2006). Additional sensitivity and responsibility with others. The print concepts
information is needed to determine how print concepts could include letter and word awareness and book knowledge.
benefit from grammatical language intervention provided for For the remainder of this article, these two variables will be
children with SLI during the preschool years. This informa- named social skills and print concepts.
tion may help to establish the nature of the relationship be- The preschoolers did not demonstrate inordinately low
tween oral language skills and print concepts development, as abilities at preintervention for the social skills and print
well as effective methods to support development in both areas. concepts of interest. Consequently, it was believed that these
preschoolers could benefit from the nonspecific and inci-
dental social and print aspects of the grammatical language
Grammatical Language Intervention intervention provided. As both the EGI and NI preschoolers
There is growing evidence supporting the worth of inter- were enrolled in educational settings during the program,
vention for grammatical development in preschoolers with opportunities to attend to social skills and print concepts
SLI (Leonard et al., 2006, 2008; Washington, Warr-Leeper, were being provided. If the EGI preschoolers demonstrated
& Thomas-Stonell, 2011). The repeated opportunities and greater improvements in these skills compared with the NI
systematic support provided during therapy addressing preschoolers, who were similarly enrolled, then the gains of

114 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


the EGI preschoolers could be attributed to the grammatical 3. Do preschool children’s initial expressive grammar level
language intervention provided. Evaluation of these types and /or gains in expressive grammar predict social skills
of outcomes is needed to establish the broad-based and pos- outcomes, including maintenance of performance?
itive effects of grammatical language intervention. With the 4. Do preschool children’s initial expressive grammar level
growing need to establish the outcomes of speech-language and/or gains in expressive grammar predict print concepts
pathology interventions, evidence supporting the wide-range outcomes, including maintenance of performance?
impact of our services is considered beneficial (Threats, 2003).
Corresponding hypotheses were made for each research
The Current Study question: (a) social skills will significantly improve and will
be maintained for intervention participants only; (b) print
There is no available peer-reviewed published evidence
concepts will significantly improve and will be maintained
documenting the incidental positive effects, including main-
for intervention participants only; (c) initial expressive gram-
tenance of skills, for grammatical language intervention on
mar level and /or gains in expressive grammar will signif-
social skills and print concepts development in preschoolers
icantly predict social skills outcomes, including maintenance
with SLI. The primary goal of this investigation was to
of performance; and (d) initial expressive grammar level
determine whether the EGI offered in the Washington et al.
and/or gains in expressive grammar will significantly pre-
(2011) study provided enrichment that resulted in improved
dict print concepts outcomes, including maintenance of
social skills and print concepts outcomes in the sample of
performance.
preschoolers. Outcomes were established at preintervention,
immediately postintervention, and at 3 months post-
intervention. The author sought to determine whether clini- Method
cally meaningful changes in performance (i.e., at least one
SD gain above preintervention scores at postintervention and Only preschoolers from the Washington et al. (2011)
at 3 months postintervention) occurred and were maintained study participated in this project. An abridged version of
for EGI or NI participants. Bain and Dollaghan (1991) all procedures and protocols related to that study, including
highlighted that amount of change (e.g., outside the pretest measures utilized, is outlined in this article. The current
range) is one factor that needs to be considered in order to study represents a post hoc investigation of the spreading
conclude that change is clinically significant. Another factor effects of grammatical language intervention on these pre-
is the functional impact of the change. schoolers’ social skills and print concepts.
The effectiveness of the two EGI conditions, one paper-
based and the other computer-assisted, on social skills and Participants
print concepts was not compared, as the author did not have a
rationale for why the two conditions might have different Participants were a convenience sample of 34 preschoolers
outcomes. In the earlier study, the two EGI conditions resulted on a waitlist for intervention. Participants were recruited
in similar expressive grammar outcomes (cf. Washington et al., from a government-funded preschool speech and language
2011). The author explored the results and confirmed the initiative located in southwestern Ontario, Canada. As parents
suspicion that there also were no differences in social skills accessed services, their speech-language pathologists (SLPs)
and print concepts outcomes for the two groups of EGI invited them to participate. The preschoolers’ ages ranged
preschoolers. Ultimately, combining the two interventions from 3 years to 5 years (M = 52 months; SD = 5 months) at
provided a larger sample and better power to determine dif- preintervention. The participants were from urban and rural
ferences between EGI and NI outcomes. regions, were monolingual English speakers, and were
The secondary goal of the current study was to establish identified by parents as Caucasian (n = 32), Asian (n = 1),
the impact of initial expressive grammar level and /or or other (n = 1). There were 27 boys and seven girls. All
expressive grammar gains on preschoolers’ social skills and preschoolers were enrolled in structured educational envir-
print concepts outcomes. Exploring the nature of the rela- onments at the outset of intervention.
tionships among initial expressive grammar level, gains in Preschoolers met the diagnostic criteria for SLI with
expressive grammar, and social skills and print concepts primary expressive grammar deficits. These criteria included:
outcomes might offer some insight into relationships be- (a) normal hearing sensitivity; (b) parent or SLP report of
tween oral language skills and other co-occurring areas of absence of oral motor, neurological deficits, or pervasive devel-
development. The following research questions and were opmental disorder; (c) normal receptive language skills (i.e.,
addressed: standard scores of 85 or greater) at the word level, as measured
by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—IIIB (PPVT–IIIB;
1. Does EGI that includes socialization development Dunn & Dunn, 1997), and sentence level as measured by the
opportunities, along with print awareness and book Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool
knowledge activities, facilitate improved social skills (CELF–P; Wiig, Secord, & Semel, 1992); (d) normal non-
outcomes that are maintained after the intervention, verbal IQ (i.e., standard score of 85 or greater) as measured
compared with NI? by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—2 (KBIT–2; Kaufman
2. Does EGI that includes socialization development & Kaufman, 2004) and (e) expressive language skills at or
opportunities and print awareness and book knowledge below the 10th percentile as measured by two measures—
activities facilitate improved print concepts that are one a language test, the Structured Photographic Expressive
maintained after the intervention, compared with NI? Language Test—Preschool (SPELT–P; Werner & Kresheck,

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 115


1983), and the other a language sample collected using devel- 45-min spontaneous language sample obtained during play
opmental sentence scoring (DSS; Lee, 1974) procedures. with a clinician. Both the SPELT–P and DSS procedures
Although not a criterion for enrolment in the study, all parents were selected so that language skills in two distinct contexts
reported that they were already engaged in emergent literacy could be evaluated. The SPELT–P represented a standard-
activities at home (i.e., adult–child shared book-reading ized evaluation of language skills, and the language sample
activities). represented an evaluation of language skills with no prompting
After consent to participate was obtained, 22 children for correct responses. Inter-rater reliability for the scoring
were randomly selected for placement in the EGI group. The of the SPELT–P and DSS was completed by graduate
remaining 12 children from the same intervention waitlist speech-language pathology students. For the SPELT–P and
served as the NI group. DSS, inter-rater reliability was greater than 90%. Reliability
Results of univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for DSS included point-to-point comparisons for word
revealed no statistically significant differences between EGI transcription, appropriate DSS sentences, DSS category, and
and NI participants for age, F(1, 32) = 3.75, p = .062, h2 = .11; DSS scoring. Nonverbal IQ was evaluated using the matrices
nonverbal cognitive skills, F(1, 32) = 0.90, p = .350, h2 = .03; subtest of the KBIT–2 (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004).
receptive language skills at the word level, F(1, 32) = 3.24, The DSS-scored spontaneous language samples were
p = .081, h2 = .09, and sentence level, F(1, 32) = 0.85, p = .365, used to establish initial expressive grammar level and ex-
h2 = .03; and expressive language skills from a standardized pressive grammar gains. Use of spontaneous language samples
language test, F(1, 32) = 3.58, p = .067, h2 = .10, and a is considered a “gold standard” for assessing language skills
spontaneous language sample, F(1, 32) = 3.05, p = .090, in children and has been suggested as being more repre-
h2 = .09. Participants were also not significantly different in sentative of children’s language skills compared with lan-
their preintervention social skills, F(1, 32) = 1.20, p = .281, guage evaluations completed in more formal contexts where
h2 = .04, and print concepts, F(1, 32) = 1.54, p = .223, h2 = .05. optimal support is provided (Miller, 1996; Sealey & Gilmore,
See Table 1 for participant characteristics. 2008). DSS z-scores for spontaneous language samples
collected at preintervention were used to represent initial
expressive grammar level. The DSS language samples total
Procedures scores were used to determine expressive grammar gains.
Assessment protocol. Preschoolers were individually tested These scores represented the notion of “correct” production
using language and cognitive measures pursuant to the goals for the entire sentence and not just attempts at creating
of the Washington et al. (2011) study. The language mea- grammatically correct sentences. Expressive grammar gains,
sures included (a) the PPVT–IIIB (Dunn & Dunn, 1997), along with social skills and print concepts outcomes, were
used to evaluate receptive language skills at the word level; considered for change scores for preintervention to post-
(b) the receptive portions of the CELF–P (Wiig, Secord, & intervention (immediate gains) and postintervention to 3 months
Semel, 1992), used to evaluate receptive language skills postintervention (continued growth).
at the sentence level; (c) the SPELT–P (Werner & Kresheck, Social skills and print concepts measures served as the
1983), used to evaluate expressive grammar skills on a primary outcome measures for this study. Registered SLPs or
standardized test; and (d) DSS (Lee, 1974), used to analyze a graduate students in speech-language pathology supervised

TABLE 1. Participants’ preintervention characteristics.

Total sample EGI NI


Characteristic (n = 34) (n = 22) (n = 12)

Age range 3;6 to 4;11 3;11 to 4;10 3;6 to 4;11


Gender
Female 7 6 1.
Male 27 16 1.
Receptive-wd 101.85 (4.88) 103.18 (4.75) 99.42 (4.30)
Receptive-sent 101.21 (6.95) 102.05 (8.33) 99.75 (2.90)
Expressive-standardized language test 10.26 (3.97)a 11.18 (3.03)a 8.58 (4.99)a
Expressive-spontaneous language 4.84 (.82)a 5.02 (.90)a 4.51 (.54)a
Nonverbal IQ 109.15 (10.10) 110.36 (10.97) 106.92 (8.24)
Social skills 103.26 (18.52) 105.55 (19.25) 99.08 (17.10)
73.04 (11.28)a 74.59 (11.16)a 70.17 (11.41)a
Print concepts 105.03 (14.16) 105.82 (15.73) 103.58 (11.23)
8.26 (4.13)a 8.91 (4.14)a 7.08 (4.01)a

Note. Means are reported, with SDs in parentheses. Standard scores are reported for all tests (M = 100, SD =15), with the exception of expressive
language measures. Expressive-standardized language test = Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test—Preschool (SPELT–P),
and Expressive-spontaneous language sample = DSS. Receptive-word (Receptive-wd) = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III (PPVT–IIIB),
Receptive-sentence (Receptive-Sent) = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool (CELF–P), Nonverbal IQ = Kaufman Brief Intelligence
Test—2 (KBIT–2), Social skills = Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS), Print concepts = Preschool Word and Print Awareness test (PWPA).
a
Raw score.

116 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


by registered SLPs administered these measures. Examiners for 21 (i.e., 20%) of the 102 assessment sessions. The sessions
were trained on the measures to be used and were blind to were digitally recorded to facilitate rescoring of the original
assessment time point and group assignment. To evaluate testing. Each session selected was from a different participant
social skills, the Socialization domain of the Vineland Adaptive and was equally selected across groups and assessment time
Behavior Scales (VABS; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) points. Two trained research assistants were recruited to
Interview Edition Survey Form was completed. The VABS perform this task. For the VABS, point-by-point agreement
is an interview-based standardized assessment of everyday in scoring averaged 98% (range, 94%–100%). For the
adaptations for four major domains, including Communica- PWPA, point-by-point agreement in scoring averaged 99%
tion, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, and Motor Skills, (range, 88%–100%).
for children from birth to 18 years. The VABS is also an Assessment sessions. All testing was completed during
established norm-referenced measure with acceptable psy- 90-min assessment sessions at preintervention, immediately
chometric properties and can be used for progress monitor- postintervention, and 3 months postintervention, in a stan-
ing of a client’s level of functioning (Sparrow et al., 1984). dard clinical setting. During the assessment sessions, breaks
The Socialization domain provides information on a func- were provided as appropriate, based on child cues. Each
tional skill that is observed in the client’s day-to-day life. assessment session followed the same protocol, with the
Individuals who know the client best and can provide in- exception of the nonverbal cognitive skills evaluation. Lan-
formation about level of functioning are most preferable for guage measures were administered first, followed by the
completing the VABS domains (Sparrow et al., 1984). cognitive measure, and finally the literacy measure. For
Preschoolers’ parents acted as respondents for their chil- younger participants who were below the lower age limit of
dren’s social skills in day-to-day life. Using structured face- 4 years for cognitive testing using the KBIT–2, evaluation
to-face informal interviews, parents (32 mothers, two fathers) was completed in a separate 10-min session once they were
described their children’s social skills in the following order of appropriate age. The social skills measure was completed
according to the three Socialization subdomains: (a) inter- with participants’ parents in a co-occurring assessment session.
personal relationships—how does their child interact with EGI. The 22 participants in the EGI group received gram-
others; (b) play and leisure—how does their child spend his matical language intervention targeting the goal of accurate
or her play and leisure time; and (c) coping skills—how does production of third-person singular present progressive
their child demonstrate sensitivity and responsibility to others. sentences. Each sentence element (i.e., noun phrase subject
Items were scored according to parental responses, with slot, verb, noun phrase object slot) was elicited individually
scoring response options of usually, sometimes, or partially; from each participant. EGI preschoolers received ten 20-min
no/never; no opportunity; or don’t know. Using the VABS intervention sessions, once a week. The interventions were
interview protocol, basal (seven consecutive items scored designed with specific features to directly address expressive
usually) and ceiling (seven consecutive items scored no/never) grammar deficits. Half of the EGI participants received
rules were followed. Preschoolers’ ages were used to establish computer-assisted intervention, and half did not.
the 20- to 30-min interview starting point. The following procedure was used during EGI: (a) An
The Preschool Word and Print Awareness test (PWPA; introduction to the expected routine was made, followed by
Justice & Ezell, 2001) was used to evaluate print concepts. a 2- to 7-min practice block. (b) Each sentence element
The PWPA is an individually administered measure of (subject noun phrase, verb, object noun phrase) was elicited
children’s print concepts that can be used to evaluate progress individually (sentence breakdown) using the following
in print concepts development in preschoolers (Justice & questions: subject: “Who do you want to play with?”; verb:
Ezell, 2001; Justice et al., 2006; Justice, Kaderavek, Fan, “What is s/he doing?”; object: “What does s/he want to play
Sofka, & Hunt, 2009). Children’s knowledge of 14 concepts with?” (c) Each session continued to follow the same pro-
about print (e.g., directionality) was completed during a cedure until 80% accuracy on average over two consecutive
clinician–child shared book-reading activity using the com- sessions was achieved. (d) At the beginning of the sub-
mercially available picture book Nine Ducks Nine (Hayes, sequent session, participants were asked only to engage in
1990). The procedure used in this study adhered to the sentence build-up (“Put it all together”). No further sen-
protocol previously outlined in Justice et al. (2006) and Clay tence breakdown was required once the 80% criterion was
(1979). This procedure is noncontrived and elicits informa- achieved. See Appendix for a sample intervention routine.
tion about print concepts in a manner similar to parent Eleven EGI participants received computer-assisted in-
shared-reading activities. First, preschoolers were told, tervention using a software program named My Sentence
“We are going to read this book together and I need you to Builder (Washington & Warr-Leeper, 2006). This program
help me read.” Second, the examiner embedded a series of uses a syntactic slot-filler approach in which visual rep-
print concepts tasks into the shared book-reading activity, resentation (i.e., picture support) for every sentence element
which included requests such as, “Show me the front of the (e.g., nouns, verbs, grammatical morphemes) that is needed
book,” “Show me the name of the book,” and “Where do I to create a present progressive sentence is provided. With
begin to read?” Presentation of print concepts tasks was SLP support and guidance, participants moved from screen
the same for all preschoolers, and responses were scored as to screen while selecting elements needed to create a gram-
correct, alternate correct, or incorrect. matically correct sentence. Thus, participants were able to
Reliability for the VABS and PWPA. Double scoring for observe the location of the “who,” the “is doing,” and the
parental responses on the VABS and preschoolers’ performance “what” (noun and verb pictures) along with grammatical
on the PWPA was completed randomly and independently elements (is, ing, the; depicted in triangles) in a sentence. Using

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 117


this approach, there was a slow, deliberate construction of each of the two groups of preschoolers. Significant differences in
sentence component to provide these participants with in- outcomes were established using preset alpha levels (p < .05).
creased time to process and produce grammatical information. Planned follow-up tests were completed for ANOVAs with
The remaining 11 EGI participants received non–computer- F values that met the established significance level. An
assisted intervention. Typical conventional language training adjusted preset alpha level ( p < .017), representing a
procedures were utilized in clinician–client dyads. In lieu Bonferroni correction, was used in these follow-up tests.
of computer time, participants in this intervention received Effect sizes were reported using eta-squared (h2) values.
visual supports provided through turning the pages of books Regression analyses were completed to examine the
along with objects in play and picture cards with actions relationships between (a) initial expressive grammar level and
to facilitate grammatical productions in a drill–play format. social skills and print concepts outcomes and (b) expressive
Emphatic stress was included to facilitate attention to sentence grammar gains and social skills and print concepts outcomes.
components (e.g., subject, verb, object). This technique of Preintervention to postintervention (immediate gains) and
stressing language targets has been recommended to increase postintervention to 3 months postintervention (continued
the salience of unstressed grammatical targets (Fey, Long, & growth) performance for expressive grammar gains and
Finestack, 2003). social skills and print concepts outcomes were considered in
Social skills and print concepts intervention features. the regression analyses. The significance of the relationships
While directly addressing grammatical skills, the SLP pro- was determined using a preset alpha level (p < .05). Stan-
vided guidance for listening, taking turns, manipulating toy dard conventions (i.e., 0.10, 0.30, and 0.50), established to
objects, and interacting with others. When preschoolers indicate small, medium, and large coefficients, were used
made mistakes, the SLP helped them cope to avoid frustration to determine the magnitude of the regression coefficients
with the task and with mistakes. Thus, preschoolers were (Green & Salkind, 2011).
learning how to behave appropriately and demonstrate social Raw scores or raw change scores, not standard scores,
skills that facilitated inclusion with others. were used to represent outcomes (cf. Hadley, Olsen, & Earle,
In addition, during intervention sessions, the SLP used 2005). Raw scores on the VABS and the PWPA were entered
emphatic stress or visual representations to highlight mor- into SPSS to complete the ANOVAs. To complete the re-
phemes, including grammatical inflections. The SLP pointed gression analyses, raw change scores were used.
to words and letters while turning the pages of books and
moving from screen to screen in the computer program.
Because children went from screen to screen while talking Results
about pictures and words when using the computer program, Social Skills Outcomes After EGI
the activity could be considered similar to turning the pages EGI participants’ mean performance at postintervention
of a book. The SLP also highlighted specific book con- revealed scores that were at least one SD above preinter-
ventions (e.g., directionality, orientation) while targeting vention performance, indicating clinically meaningful changes
grammatical productions. Thus, preschoolers were required in social skills. This pattern was maintained at 3 months
to attend to these specific print concepts when producing postintervention. A similar outcome was not evident for NI
each target sentence element (i.e., subject, verb, object). participants. See Table 2 for mean performance on the VABS
Intervention fidelity. The author of the current study pro- at each time point.
vided all intervention services. To establish adherence to A mixed-model ANOVA was used to test for differences
intervention protocols, 20% of sessions (44 sessions) were between social skills scores of the EGI and NI groups of
randomly recorded and scored for accuracy of the interven- preschoolers. Results from the ANOVA met the preset alpha
tion implementation by two graduate students in speech- level of .05. A significant interaction was found between
language pathology. Sessions were observed equally for both time and group, F(2, 64) = 32.50, p < .001, partial h2 = .50.
intervention programs. A checklist of the critical intervention Thus, the main effects were not interpreted. Instead, follow-up
elements that included the intervention procedure, identifi- tests using one-way univariate ANOVAs for VABS scores
cation of the interventionist, the session number, group type, at each time point were completed (Portney & Watkins,
length of the session, and the techniques used during the 2009). Findings at preintervention did not meet the preset
session was used for scoring. The intervention SLP adhered
to the protocol 100% of the time for all 44 intervention sessions.
NI. Twelve participants who were on the intervention TABLE 2. Participants’ mean performance on the VABS.
waitlist did not receive EGI during the study, thus serving as
control participants. Similar to intervention participants, Group Time n M (SD )
the NI participants were enrolled in structured educational
EGI Pre 22 74.59 (11.16)
settings during the intervention study. Following the com- Post 22 94.95 (7.08)
pletion of this study, these participants were offered a block 3 months post 22 95.68 (7.72)
of grammatical language intervention. NI Pre 12 70.12 (11.41)
Post 12 74.33 (12.35)
3 months post 12 75.30 (10.86)
Design and Data Analysis
Note. Scores represent participants’ raw score performance at each
A pre–post–follow-up design was used. ANOVAs were time point.
used to compare the social skills and print concepts outcomes

118 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


alpha level of .017, F(1, 32) = 1.20, p = .281, h2 = .04. How- TABLE 3. Participants’ print concepts mean performance on the
ever, findings at postintervention, F(1, 32) = 38.68, p < .001, PWPA.
h2 = .55, and at 3 months postintervention, F(1, 32) = 47.95,
Group Time n M (SD )
p < .001, h2 = .60, met the set significance level for these
follow-up tests. Pairwise comparisons of means at post- EGI Pre 22 8.91 (4.14)
intervention and 3 months postintervention revealed that Post 22 13.32 (3.24)
EGI participants had higher means compared with controls. 3 months post 22 14.91 (1.90)
See Figure 1 for preintervention, postintervention, and NI Pre 12 7.08 (4.01)
Post 12 8.83 (4.99)
3 months postintervention performance on the VABS. 3 months post 12 8.43 (5.14)

Print Concepts Outcomes After EGI Note. Scores represent participants’ raw score performance at each
time point.
EGI participants’ mean performance at postintervention
revealed scores that were at least one SD above preinter-
vention performance, indicating clinically meaningful changes
in print concepts. This pattern was maintained at 3 months expressive grammar gains, and social skills. All partic-
postintervention. A similar outcome was not evident for NI ipants were included in these analyses. Raw change scores
participants. See Table 3 for mean performance on the PWPA for immediate gains (pre–post) and continued growth (post–
at each time point. 3 months post) on the VABS served as the dependent variables.
A mixed-model ANOVA was tested for differences be- Given the sample size in this study, no more than three
tween print concepts scores of the EGI and NI groups of predictor variables were selected.
preschoolers. Results from the ANOVA met the preset alpha Two-predictor and three-predictor regression models
level of .05. A significant interaction was found between were used. In the two-predictor models, preschoolers’ initial
time and group, F(2, 64) = 14.84, p < .001, partial h2 = .32. expressive grammar level and immediate expressive gram-
Thus, the main effects were not interpreted. Instead, follow-up mar gains served as predictor variables. In the three-predictor
tests using one-way univariate ANOVAs for PWPA scores models, preschoolers’ initial expressive grammar level,
at each time point were completed. Findings at preintervention immediate expressive grammar gains, and continued growth
did not meet the preset alpha level of .017, F(1, 32) = 1.54, in expressive grammar gains served as the predictor variables.
p = .223, h2 = .05. However, findings at postintervention, The regression model with VABS preintervention to
F(1, 32) = 10.12, p < .001, h2 = .24, and at 3 months post- postintervention raw change scores demonstrating immedi-
intervention, F(1, 32) = 28.56, p < .001, h2 = .47, met the set ate gains, initial expressive grammar level, and immediate
significance level for these follow-up tests. Pairwise com- expressive grammar gains was significant, R = .48, R2 = .23,
parisons of means at postintervention and 3 months post- F(2, 31) = 4.71, p = .016. In this two-predictor model, initial
intervention revealed that EGI participants had higher PWPA expressive grammar level did not significantly correlate with
mean scores than controls. See Figure 2 for preintervention, VABS scores, r(32) = .17, p = .167. However, a signifi-
postintervention, and 3 months postintervention performance. cant positive correlation was observed between immediate
expressive grammar gains and VABS change scores, r(32) = .45,
Factors Predicting Social Skills Outcomes p = .004 (see Table 4). Expressive grammar gains were a
significant predictor of VABS change scores, b = .45, t(31) =
Multiple regression analyses were completed to establish 2.87, p = .007. The magnitude of the regression coefficient
the relationships among initial expressive grammar level, (b) was medium. Initial expressive grammar level was not

FIGURE 1. Mean performance of social skills between the two


groups illustrated at the three assessment time points. VABS =
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; EGI = expressive grammar
intervention; NI = no intervention. FIGURE 2. Mean performance of print concepts between the two
groups illustrated at the three assessment time points. PWPA =
Preschool Word and Print Awareness Test.

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 119


TABLE 4. Correlations among regression variables.

DSS DSS VABS VABS PWPA PWPA DSS


pre–post post–3 months pre–post post–3 months pre–post post–3 months z-score

DSS pre–post — .34* .45* .07 .19 .36** .01


DSS post–3 months .34* — .37* .02 .26 .03 .25
VABS pre–post .45* .37* — .14 .34* .48** .17
VABS post–3 months .07 .02 .14 — .03 .03 –.003
PWPA pre–post .19 .26 –.01 .34* — .28 .14
PWPA post–3 months .36 .03 .48** .03 .28 — .09
DSS z-score .01 .25 .17 –.003 .14 .01 —

*p < .05. **p < .01.

found to be a significant individual predictor of VABS per- predictor of PWPA change scores, b = .36, t(30) = 2.14,
formance, b = .17, t(31) = 1.07, p = .294. p = .031. The magnitude of the regression coefficient (b) was
The three-predictor regression model with VABS post- medium. Neither initial expressive grammar level, b = .13,
intervention to 3 months postintervention raw change scores t(30) = 0.72, p = .477, nor continued growth in expressive
(continued growth), initial expressive grammar level, imme- grammar gains, b = .15, t(30) = –0.79, p = .439, predicted
diate expressive grammar gains, and continued growth continued growth in PWPA change scores.
in expressive grammar gains was not significant, R = .07,
R2 < .001, F(3, 30) = 0.05, p = .985. The corresponding
correlations among predictor and dependent variables in this Discussion
regression model were also not significant (see Table 4). The
finding for this regression model suggests that the independent Two major findings were reported in the current study. As
variables, initial expressive grammar level, b = –.002, t(30) = hypothesized, compared with NI, EGI for preschool SLI was
–0.01, p = .992, immediate expressive grammar gains, b = .07, associated with greater improvement of social skills—as
t(30) = 0.37, p = .715, and continued expressive grammar measured by parent report—and of print concepts outcomes,
gains, b = –.006, t(30) = –0.03, p = .978, did not predict as measured by a literacy test. The postintervention and 3
continued growth in participants’ social skills. months postintervention scores achieved for social skills and
print concepts outcomes were at least one SD above pre-
intervention performance for intervention participants only.
Factors Predicting Print Concepts Outcomes The functional impact associated with changes in social
The approach to regression analyses for social skills skills was that parents reported observing their children
was also followed for the regression analyses completed for socialize better with others in everyday “naturalistic” contexts
print concepts immediate gains and continued growth on after intervention. Because print concepts were observed
the PWPA. The two-predictor regression model with PWPA and evaluated in a structured setting, the functional impact
preintervention to postintervention raw change scores associated with these changes was not as clear as compared
(immediate gains), initial expressive grammar level, and with that for social skills. However, scoring of specific items
immediate expressive grammar gains was not significant, on the PWPA required the examiner to observe the pre-
R = .24, R2 = .06, F(2, 31) = 0.95, p = .40. The corresponding schoolers’ unprompted demonstration of print concepts (e.g.,
correlations among predictor and dependent variables in this turning the book to the front to identify the title). This pro-
regression model were also not significant (see Table 4). vided the opportunity to observe that preschoolers had an
Neither initial expressive grammar level, b = .14, t(31) = 0.82, increased ability to spontaneously engage in print-concepts
p = .421, nor immediate expressive grammar gains, b = .19, activities.
t(31) = 1.11, p = .277, predicted changes in preintervention The improvements for intervention participants were of
to postintervention print concepts. statistical and clinical significance and, most important, were
The three-predictor regression model with PWPA post- maintained for 3 months postintervention. Bain and Dollaghan
intervention to 3 months postintervention raw change scores (1991) highlighted the importance of such large changes.
(continued growth), initial expressive grammar level, imme- Because the intervention and control preschoolers did not
diate expressive grammar gains, and continued expressive demonstrate inordinately low abilities for social skills and
grammar gains also was not significant, R = .37 R2 = .14, print concepts at preintervention, EGI offered greater enrich-
F(3, 30) = 1.81, p = .167. Initial expressive grammar level, ment, not remediation, of these skills compared with NI.
r(32) = .09, p = .304, and continued growth in expressive Because preschoolers in both groups were enrolled in edu-
grammar gains, r(32) = .03, p = .445, did not significantly cational settings, which provided them with opportunities to
correlate with the dependent variable. There was, however, a engage in social skills and attend to print concepts, the ad-
significant positive correlation between immediate expres- ditional benefits of receiving the grammatical language
sive grammar gains and PWPA change scores for continued intervention are even more important.
growth, r(32) = .36, p = .019 (see Table 4). Immediate ex- The second finding of the current study was that a signif-
pressive grammar gains was found to be the only significant icant predictive value of the two-predictor model containing

120 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


initial expressive grammar level and preintervention to post- of the left-to-right orientation and directionality in reading,
intervention expressive grammar gains was evident for social thereby helping the children to produce grammatically correct
skills immediate gains. No other models were found to be sentences. As the systematic support was repeated session to
predictive of either social skills or print concepts outcomes. session, intervention participants had multiple practice oppor-
It is important to note, however, that expressive grammar tunities for exposure to directionality, orientation, letters, words,
gains were the only significant single predictor of both social and other print concepts that enhanced their literacy growth,
skills and print concepts outcomes. There was a differential compared with that of control participants. Consequently, inter-
effect of this predictor variable, with immediate expressive vention participants’ motivation toward, interest in, and con-
grammar gains predicting immediate social skills and con- tinued understanding of the target print concepts may have also
tinued growth (i.e., maintenance of gains) for print concepts. increased more than it did for those who had no intervention.
The hypotheses regarding the relationships between depen- Information on development of print concepts is needed
dent and independent variables were partially supported (Justice et al., 2006). The findings from the current study
on the basis of these results. expand the available knowledge about print concepts out-
comes for preschoolers with SLI. Because there are asso-
ciated long-term negative effects of having a language
Social Skills Outcomes After EGI impairment in kindergarten on later reading success, practi-
EGI was associated with significantly greater social skills tioners may want to include materials and activities that
outcomes compared with NI. These outcomes were evi- enhance print concepts as well as language skills.
dent immediately after intervention and were maintained for
3 months postintervention. Specific intervention features
Factors Predicting Social Skills and
may have been beneficial in facilitating the observed out-
comes for intervention participants. Print Concepts Outcomes
During the intervention program, preschoolers had multiple A secondary focus of the current study was determining
guided opportunities from the SLP for learning how to take the impact of initial expressive grammar level and /or ex-
turns, listen and play appropriately, and interact with others. pressive grammar gains on social skills and print concepts
The SLP encouraged and directed each preschooler to wait for outcomes over time. The specific time points investigated
his or her own turn before being permitted to produce sentence were preintervention to postintervention (immediate gains)
components. Preschoolers also had to learn to listen to in- and postintervention to 3 months postintervention (continued
structions in their entirety before completing a task. The SLP growth). Of equal importance to the other findings in the
also helped to model appropriate play behaviors for times current study is that expressive grammar gains were the only
when a preschooler engaged with toys and then encouraged significant single predictor of social skills and print concepts
the preschooler to demonstrate similar actions. In addition to outcomes. There was a differential impact of the predictor
these aspects of the intervention, the repeated opportunities variable depending on the time period of the gains in ex-
for producing target components also provided a guided con- pressive grammar as well as when the social skills and print
text for preschoolers to learn how to cope with making a concepts outcomes were achieved. These findings are dis-
mistake. Preschoolers in the control group did not have these cussed separately in the upcoming sections.
opportunities and also did not experience equal or better out- Factors predicting social skills outcomes. The two-
comes than their intervention cohorts. predictor model containing initial expressive grammar level
Older children with SLI show associated long-term dif- and immediate expressive grammar gains in relationship to
ficulties in social skills development (Baker & Cantwell, preintervention to postintervention social skills outcomes
1987; Durkin & Conti-Ramsden, 2007; Fujiki et al., 2004). showed a significant predictive value. The results of the
Interventions that facilitate the development of language regression indicated that the two grammar predictors explained
skills by incorporating socialization activities during the pre- 23% of the variance in social skills changes. As an inde-
school years might offer some benefits to children’s ongoing pendent variable, immediate expressive grammar gains was
development of social skills in the later school years. found to be a better predictor of social skills performance
than was initial language level. The direction of the corre-
lational coefficient was positive, suggesting that those pre-
Print Concepts Outcomes After EGI schoolers with greater gains in expressive grammar skills
EGI was also associated with significantly greater print during the intervention period also experienced greater social
concepts outcomes compared with NI. SLP guidance for skills changes during the same time period. Furthermore,
critical intervention features—such as pointing to and talking those children with fewer gains in expressive grammar also
about print, and turning the pages of a book or moving from experienced fewer gains in social skills.
screen to screen on a computer while showing preschoolers Previous researchers have highlighted the idea that oral
how to construct and “read” a sentence—appeared to help language skills are a principal means to achieve successful
increase the children’s awareness of print and knowledge of social interactions (Bishop, 1997; Washington, 2007). EGI
book conventions. For example, the SLP pointed to printed participants’ language skills improved from preintervention
grammatical forms, such as is, ing, and the, to help highlight to postintervention more than those of NI participants.
elements needed in a grammatically correct sentence. Thus, The indirect effects of the added benefits of grammatical
the SLP directed children’s attention to the letters and words in language intervention for treatment participants are con-
these grammatical forms, as well as increased their awareness sidered important to predicting immediate social skills changes.

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 121


Consequently, as oral language functioning for expressive must be based in literacy experiences (Cabell et al., 2009,
grammar improved, corresponding improvements in interper- 2010; Justice et al., 2006).
sonal interactions, play, and coping also were evident. Based Continued opportunities for exposure to literacy experi-
on this study, however, it is not possible to determine the ences likely occurred for preschoolers from the end of the
relative impact of improved grammar versus incidental activ- postintervention to 3 months postintervention. Thus, pre-
ities that enhance social skills during intervention. schoolers may have required more time for the integration,
The three-predictor model that included grammar pre- application, and manifestation of the incidental knowledge
dictors and continued changes in social skills was not found gained for print concepts during the intervention period. The
to have a significant predictive value. The predictors included lack of a significant relationship between continued growth
in the model also did not have individual effects on the in expressive grammar gains and continued growth in print
dependent variable. The lack of correlation between initial concepts changes could also be explained using this rationale.
expressive grammar level and changes in social skills for Therefore, for the effects of continued growth in grammar
continued growth could be a result of the limited variability to be manifested, the evaluation of continued growth in print
in initial expressive grammar level among the preschoolers concepts would need to extend past the postintervention to
as well as their initial level of social skills. The lack of sig- 3 months postintervention time frame used in this study.
nificance found for the correlation between both types of
expressive grammar gains and continued growth in social
skills could also be due to a leveling off of the language Limitations and Future Directions
differences between EGI and NI participants. Specifically, The main limitation of this study was that it was a post
the magnitude and impact of those differences decreased. hoc question. The incidental effects of grammatical language
Thus, all participants could have experienced language gains intervention on social skills and print concepts were not
large enough to facilitate the observed changes in social planned as a primary research question for the original study.
skills at this time point. Consequently, there was no control group to compare the
Factors predicting print concepts outcomes. Unlike the effects of incidental teaching of social skills and print con-
findings observed for the models of social skills changes, cepts to a context in which these incidental features were not
neither the two-predictor model nor the three-predictor model included. In future investigations, to determine the differ-
was found to have a significant predictive value for print ential effects of intervention on print concepts, researchers
concepts changes. This means that the combined effect of the should explore using a control group that receives the gram-
various predictor variables did not have a significant rela- mar intervention without literacy experiences.
tionship with the dependent variable. The limited variability Another limitation was that direct examination of pre-
in initial expressive grammar level as well as the high initial schoolers’ social skills was not completed; instead, the cur-
level of print concepts skills could explain the nonsignificant rent study relied on parental report of these skills using a
correlations observed. validated measure. These reports may have been affected by a
The benefit of conducting a standard multiple regression treatment participation bias. That said, recent literature indi-
is that the model’s predictive value and the value of each cates that parents can make accurate and reliable judgments
independent variable, controlling for other independent about children’s social skills, specifically regarding partici-
variables, can be determined (Green & Salkind, 2011; Portney pation (Thomas-Stonell, Oddson, Robertson, & Rosenbaum,
& Watkins, 2009). It was found that expressive grammar 2009). Direct evaluation and observation of preschoolers’
gains as a single independent variable was the best and only social skills in their everyday settings at different time points
significant predictor of print concepts changes. Immediate would provide more empirical insights into how grammatical
expressive grammar gains predicted continued growth in print language intervention affects the spontaneous use of social
concepts. Preschoolers who experienced greater immediate skills in different settings.
gains in expressive grammar also experienced greater con- The lack of a true randomization procedure in the larger
tinued growth in print concepts. In addition, preschoolers Washington et al. (2011) study is acknowledged as another
who experienced lesser gains in expressive grammar also limitation. It is possible that because parents in the larger
experienced lesser continued gains in print concepts. study self-selected their preschool-age children for enroll-
Learning through observation and interaction in early ment in the study, parents and their preschoolers were more
literacy experiences promotes the early development of print likely to complete the study than were others who would
concepts and other literacy skills more so than overt teaching have been randomly selected.
(Cabell et al., 2010; Justice et al., 2006). There are added The sample of preschoolers included in the current study
indirect positive effects for print concepts provided by lit- may not be representative of all preschoolers with SLI. This
eracy experiences associated with grammatical language fact is acknowledged as a significant limitation. The current
intervention. As the significance of the effect was observed preschoolers had normal receptive language skills and were
only for continued growth in print concepts, more time may already actively engaging in print concepts tasks such as
be needed for this effect to be demonstrated. It has been shared book-reading activities with their parents. Furthermore,
suggested that in contrast to social skills development, the performance within the average range for both social skills
abstraction of knowledge for print concepts might not occur and print concepts was also observed. Thus, the deficits in
as easily, as literacy is an artificial construct that does not language skills for grammatical production did not appear to
naturally occur by virtue of exposure to others (Washington, affect social skills and print concepts development for these
2011). Instead, as noted by other researchers, these exposures preschoolers. It is possible that the participants’ age as well as

122 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


the measures used to evaluate the social skills and print opportunities for guided socializing can occur are important
concepts might account for this discrepancy. As a result of factors that can lead to broader based outcomes for pre-
these limitations, application of the study findings to other schoolers with SLI. Because the observed social skills and
clinical populations of preschoolers with language impair- print concepts outcomes are best predicted by gains in gram-
ment may be limited. Children with more severe language matical skills, programs that enhance language, social, and
impairments, or those with poor social skills and print con- literacy development are encouraged. A cascade of positive
cepts, might not be able to benefit from the incidental learning experiences in these co-occurring areas of development
opportunities provided by the grammatical language therapy after grammatical language intervention is possible for
in this investigation. preschoolers with SLI.
Further research using a larger and more representative
sample of preschoolers with SLI could strengthen the con-
clusions that can be drawn about the impact of implemented Acknowledgments
grammatical language intervention on social skills and print
concepts. There is considerable evidence that most children This research project was completed as part of the author’s
doctoral thesis at the University of Western Ontario in London,
with a history of developmental language impairments are at
Ontario, Canada. The research was financially supported by the
risk for failure to develop literacy skills and establish suc- Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network, the Ontario
cessful and meaningful social interactions (Baker & Cantwell, Graduate Scholarship, the Ontario Graduate Scholarship in Science
1987; Cabell et al., 2010; Catts et al., 2002; Fujiki et al., 2004). and Technology, the Research Alliance for Children with Special
Thus, growth in both the social and literacy domains would Needs, and a University of Western Ontario Thesis Award. The
be a natural consequence of language intervention for this group author acknowledges the unwavering support and contributions of
of children. Genese Warr-Leeper. The author also acknowledges the families,
Print concepts, which are successful in predicting future children, speech-language pathologists, graduate students, and
reading success, remain under-researched in the preschool research assistants whose participation made possible the timely
population and should be the focus of continued investiga- completion of this study. Finally, the author would like to thank
Julia Colangeli and Nancy Creaghead for their editorial contribu-
tions. Researchers also should examine other areas of social tions as well as her family for their unconditional support.
skills, overall emergent literacy skills, and other predictors of
performance to provide more information about the potential
impact of intervention on the development of these skills.
Use of other measures of social and print concepts functioning References
should be considered for determining growth in these areas. Bain, B., & Dollaghan, C. (1991). Treatment efficacy: The notion
of clinically significant change. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 22, 264–270.
Conclusions and Implications for Clinical Practice Baker, L., & Cantwell, D. (1987). A prospective psychiatric
Statistically significant and clinically meaningful out- follow-up of children with speech/language disorders. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
comes in social skills and print concepts outcomes are asso-
26, 546–553.
ciated with grammatical language intervention for preschoolers Bishop, D. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and
with SLI. Gains in expressive grammar are a significant disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove, East
predictor for both of these outcomes. Therefore, gains in Sussex, United Kingdom: Psychology Press.
social skills and print concepts should coincide with improved Cabell, S., Justice, L., Zucker, T., & McGinty, A. (2009).
expressive grammar skills. Emergent name-writing abilities of preschool-age children with
The grammatical language intervention was associated language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
with improvements in social skills and print concepts out- in Schools, 40, 53–66.
comes that were achieved immediately postintervention and Cabell, S., Lomax, R., Justice, L., Breit-Smith, A., Skibbe, L.,
were maintained for 3 months afterward. NI participants & McGinty, A. (2010). Emergent literacy profiles of preschool-
age children with specific language impairment. International
failed to experience these outcomes. This is an impressive
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 472–482.
finding that attests to the effectiveness of the intervention Catts, H. (1993). The relationship between speech-language
provided. Specific intervention features provided added impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech,
learning benefits that offered enrichment rather than reme- Language, and Hearing Research, 36, 948–959.
diation for the development of social skills and print concepts Catts, H., Bridges, M., Little, T., & Tomblin, J. (2008).
in the sample of preschoolers. Ultimately, the mechanisms of Reading achievement growth in children with language
EGI spurred changes that were not apparent for the NI group impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
with the same characteristics and enrolled in the same edu- Research, 51, 1569–1579.
cational settings. Therefore, positive, broad-based effects of Catts, H., Fey, M., Tomblin, J., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longi-
grammatical language intervention provided by SLPs have tudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with
language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and
been supported. Hearing Research, 45, 1142–1157.
Because the control group did not experience a similar Clay, M. (1979). The early detection of reading difficulties: A
performance pattern, the outcomes evident in specific social diagnostic survey with recovery procedures. Portsmouth, NH:
skills and print concepts were beyond those attributed to Heinemann.
maturation alone. Attention in therapy to syntactic knowledge Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III.
that contains literacy experiences as well as therapy in which Circle Pines, MN: AGS.

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 123


Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Language, social Plante, E. (1998). Criteria for SLI: The Stark and Tallal legacy and
behavior, and the quality of friendships in adolescents with and beyond. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
without a history of specific language impairment. Child 41, 951–957.
Development, 78, 1441–1457. Portney, L., & Watkins, M. (2009). Foundations of clinical
Fey, M., Long, S., & Finestack, L. (2003). Ten principles of research: Applications to practice (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle
grammar facilitation for children with specific language impair- River, NJ: Prentice Hall Health.
ment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early langauge and literacy
3–15. to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice.
Frome Loeb, D., Stoke, C., & Fey, M. (2001). Language In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickson (Eds.), Handbook of early
changes associated with FastForward: Evidence from case literacy research (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.
studies. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, Sealey, L., & Gilmore, S. (2008). Effects of sampling context on
216–230. the finite verb production of children with and without delayed
Fujiki, M., Spackman, M., Brinton, B., & Hall, A. (2004). language development. Journal of Communication Disorders,
The relationship of language and emotion regulation skills to 41, 223–258.
reticence in children with specific language impairment. Journal Sparrow, S., Balla, O., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland
of Speech and Hearing Research, 47, 637–646. Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS): Interview edition. Circle
Green, S., & Salkind, N. (2011). Using SPSS for Windows and Pines, MN: AGS.
Macintosh: Analyzing and understanding data (6th ed.). Upper Stark, R. E., & Tallal, P. (1981). Selection of children with specific
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. language deficits. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46,
Hadley, P., Olsen, J., & Earle, C. (2005, November). Clinical 114–122.
decision-making with late-talking children. Miniseminar pre- Thomas-Stonell, N., Oddson, B., Roberston, B., & Rosenbaum, P.
sentation to the American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa- (2009). Predicted and observed outcomes in preschool children
tion Annual Convention, San Diego, CA. following speech and language treatment: Parent and clini-
Hayes, S. (1990). Nine ducks nine. Cambridge, MA: Candlewick cian perspectives. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42,
Press. 29–42.
Justice, L., Bowles, R., & Skibbe, L. (2006). Measuring pre- Threats, T. (2003). The conceptual framework of ASHA’s new
school attainment of print-concept knowledge: A study of scope of practice for speech-language pathology. Journal of
typical and at-risk 3- to 5-year-old children using item response Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 10, xvii–xxiv.
theory. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, Tomblin, J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E.,
224–235. & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language im-
Justice, L., & Ezell, H. (2001). Word and print awareness in pairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language,
4-year-old children. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
17, 207–226. Tyler, A., Kerry, E., Haskill, A., & Tolbert, L. (2002). Efficacy
Justice, L., Kaderavek, J., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. and cross-domain effects of a morphosyntax and phonology
(2009). Accelerating preschoolers’ early literacy development intervention. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
throught classroom-based teacher-child storybook reading and Schools, 33, 52–66.
explicit print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Washington, K. (2007). Using the ICF within speech-language
Services in Schools, 40, 67–85. pathology: Application to developmental language impair-
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelli- ment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9,
gence Test—2 (KBIT–2). Circle Pines, MN: AGS. 242–255.
Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis: A grammatical Washington, K. (2011, November). Social and emergent literacy
assessment procedure for speech and language clinicians. outcomes following expressive grammar intervention in pre-
Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. school SLI. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, San Diego,
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. CA.
Leonard, L., Camarata, S., Pawlowska, M., Brown, B., & Washington, K., & Warr-Leeper, G. (2006). A collaborative
Camarata, M. (2006). Tense and agreement morphemes in the approach to computer-assisted treatment of preschool children
speech of children with specific language impairment during with specific language impairment. OSLA Connection Journal,
intervention: Phase 2. Journal of Speech, Language, and 2(2), 10–11, 16.
Hearing Research, 49, 749–770. Washington, K., Warr-Leeper, G., & Thomas-Stonell, N.
Leonard, L., Camarata, S., Pawlowska, M., Brown, B., & (2011). Exploring the outcomes of a novel computer-assisted
Camarata, M. (2008). The acquisition of tense and agreement treatment program targeting expressive-grammar deficits in
morphemes by children with specific language impairment preschoolers with SLI. Journal of Communication Disorders,
during intervention: Phase 3. Journal of Speech, Language, and 44, 315–330.
Hearing Research, 51, 120–125. Werner, O., & Kresheck, J. (1983). The Structured Photographic
Liiva, C., & Cleave, P. (2005). Roles of initiation and respon- Expressive Language test—Preschool (SPELT–P). Dekalb, IL:
siveness in access and partcipation for children with specific Janelle Publications.
language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (2001). Emergent literacy:
Hearing Research, 48, 868–883. Development from prereaders to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K.
McCabe, P. (2005). Social and behavioral correlates of preschoolers Dickson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11–29).
with specific language impairment. Psychology in Schools, 42, New York, NY: Guilford Press.
373–387. Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). Clinical Evaluation of
Miller, J. (1996). Progress in assessing, describing, and defining Language Fundamentals—Preschool. San Diego, CA: Harcourt
child langauge disorder. In K. Cole, P. Dale, & D. Thal (Eds.), Brace & Company.
Assessment of communication and language (pp. 309–324).
Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

124 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 22 • 113–125 • February 2013


Appendix
Sample SLP–Preschooler Intervention Dyad

SLP: We are going to talk about boys or girls doing different things. You will have lots of time to practice telling
me what different boys or girls are doing. I will be helping you a lot. Now let’s start.
SLP: Who do you want to play with?
Preschooler: Girl.
SLP (using emphatic stress or pointing to grammatical image): Girl?
Preschooler: The girl.
SLP: What is the girl doing? The girl. . .
Preschooler: Eating.
SLP (using emphatic stress or pointing to grammatical image): Eating?
Preschooler: . . .is eating.
SLP: What is the girl eating? The girl is eating . . .
Preschooler: A hot dog.
SLP: Now put it all together.
Preschooler: The girl is eating a hot dog.

Washington: Grammar Intervention and Social and Literacy Outcomes 125


The Association Between Expressive Grammar Intervention and Social and
Emergent Literacy Outcomes for Preschoolers With SLI

Karla N. Washington
Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2013;22;113-125; originally published online Oct 15,
2012;
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0026)

The references for this article include 17 HighWire-hosted articles which you can
access for free at: http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/content/full/22/1/113#BIBL

This information is current as of May 5, 2013

This article, along with updated information and services, is


located on the World Wide Web at:
http://ajslp.asha.org/cgi/content/full/22/1/113

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen