Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

J Head Trauma Rehabil

Copyright 
c 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

The Role of Cognitive Reserve in


Recovery From Traumatic Brain Injury
Kayla A. Steward, BS; Richard Kennedy, MD, PhD; Thomas A. Novack, PhD;
Michael Crowe, PhD; Daniel C. Marson, JD, PhD; Kristen L. Triebel, PsyD

Objective: To examine whether cognitive reserve (CR) attenuates the initial impact of traumatic brain injury (TBI)
on cognitive performance (neural reserve) and results in faster cognitive recovery rates in the first year postinjury
(neural compensation), and whether the advantage of CR differs on the basis of the severity of TBI. Setting:
Inpatient/outpatient clinics at an academic medical center. Participants: Adults with mild TBI (mTBI; n = 28),
complicated mild TBI (cmTBI; n = 24), and moderate to severe TBI (msevTBI; n = 57), and demographically
matched controls (n = 66). Design: Retrospective, longitudinal cohort assessed at 1, 6, and 12 months postinjury.
Main Measures: Outcomes were 3 cognitive domains: processing speed/executive function, verbal fluency, and
memory. Premorbid IQ, estimated with the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, served as CR proxy. Results: Higher
premorbid IQ was associated with better performance on cognitive domains at 1 month postinjury, and the effect
of IQ was similarly beneficial for all groups. Cognitive recovery rate was moderated only by TBI severity; those with
more severe TBI had faster recovery in the first year. Conclusion: Results support only the neural reserve theory
of CR within a TBI population and indicate that CR is neuroprotective, regardless of the degree of TBI. Higher
premorbid CR does not allow for more rapid adaptation and recovery from injury. Key words: cognitive reserve,
longitudinal studies, neuropsychology, rehabilitation, traumatic brain injury

T RAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY (TBI) affects


1.5 million to 2 million Americans each year1,2
and is often associated with deficits in processing speed,
variability in cognitive outcomes even between individ-
uals classified with the same TBI severity.5,6 Given the
high personal, medical, and societal costs of rehabili-
attention, memory, language, visuospatial processing, tation services,7 it is not feasible to prescribe extensive
and executive functioning.3,4 While some individuals treatment programs to everyone. Therefore, it is cru-
experience a quick cognitive recovery and return to pre- cial to find additional variables that can reliably predict
morbid functioning within 3 months after TBI, oth- which patients would benefit most from an aggressive
ers maintain deficits a decade or longer despite reha- treatment approach during the rehabilitative process. In
bilitation attempts.4 Researchers have noted that injury addition, being able to reliably predict cognitive out-
severity, as measured by posttraumatic amnesia, loss of come and rate of recovery would assist clinicians in ad-
consciousness, and/or Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), is a vising patients and families on issues such as returning
major predictor of immediate and long-term cognitive to work, driving, and living independently.
outcome following TBI3 ; however, there is significant Recently, studies have begun to apply the theory of
cognitive reserve (CR) to TBI populations to explain
Author Affiliations: Department of Psychology (Ms Steward and this variability in cognitive outcomes following TBI
Dr Crowe), Department of Medicine (Dr Kennedy), Department of (see Mathias and Wheaton8 for review) Broadly, CR
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation (Dr Novack), and Department of
Neurology (Drs Marson and Triebel), University of Alabama at refers to the cognitive networks used when complet-
Birmingham. ing mental tasks. It was initially hypothesized to be a
This work was supported by the National Institute on Child Health and Hu- protective factor in the face of age- or disease-related
man Development (grant 1R01HD053074) (Dr Marson, PI). The authors neurodegeneration.9–11 As CR is a theoretical concept,
thank the following contributors: Sandra Caldwell, MA (UAB Department it is often measured using proxies, such as estimated
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, data collection); UAB Neuropsy-
chology Laboratory Staff (data collection); Pat R. Pritchard, MD (UAB premorbid intelligence and/or length of exposure to
Department of Surgery, referring study participants); and Sarah Nafziger, cognitively stimulating life events (ie, educational at-
MD (UAB Department of Emergency Medicine, referring study participants). tainment, challenging occupations, mentally stimulat-
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. ing leisure activities). The theory of CR can be divided
Corresponding Author: Kayla A. Steward, BS, Department of Psychology, into “neural reserve” and “neural compensation” mod-
University of Alabama at Birmingham, Campbell Hall 415, 1530 3rd Ave els, which are not necessarily exclusive.9–11
South, Birmingham, AL 35294 (ksteward@uab.edu). The neural reserve model states that there are indi-
DOI: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000325 vidual differences in the efficiency and capacity of a

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
2 JOURNAL OF HEAD TRAUMA REHABILITATION

person’s cognitive processing systems, which protect ined whether the benefit of CR differs on the basis of
these networks from disruption following injury to the TBI severity. We hypothesized that relative to controls,
brain. Essentially, when neuropathological damage is the advantage of CR on initial cognitive outcome will
equivalent between individuals, those with higher CR weaken as TBI severity level increases. We also hypothe-
will demonstrate fewer cognitive and functional deficits sized that, relative to controls, CR will have the strongest
relative to those with lower CR.9–11 Numerous studies effect on rate of recovery for those with msevTBI in
have determined that higher premorbid educational and comparison to the other TBI groups.
occupational attainment is associated with better imme-
diate and long-term postinjury cognitive and functional
outcome,12–19 providing support for this theory. Inter- METHODS
estingly, 2 studies have found evidence that the effect of
Participants
CR may differ depending on the severity of TBI. Jeon
et al17 found that having higher CR was a protective Participants with TBI (n = 109) and demographically
factor for adults with TBI, but the effect weakened as matched controls (n = 66) were enrolled from the Uni-
TBI severity increased. Similarly, Fay et al20 determined versity of Alabama at Birmingham (UAB) between 2007
that having lower CR was a risk factor for worse out- and 2011 as part of a larger NIH-funded longitudinal
come in children with complicated mild TBI (cmTBI), study investigating medical decision making in TBI.25
but not for those with mild TBI (mTBI). The results of Consistent with the protocol established by the TBI
these studies indicate that individuals may reach a level Model System program, participants with TBI were re-
of neuronal and axonal injury at which preinjury CR is cruited from the UAB hospital system and initially as-
no longer beneficial to their recovery. sessed within a window of 2 to 6 weeks after injury, with
The neural compensation model of CR posits that a goal of assessing at 1 month after injury.26 Participants
certain individuals’ existing brain networks are more were also assessed at 6 and 12 months postinjury with a
capable of adaptation and network reorganization fol- 2-week scheduling window on either side.
lowing injury. This model posits that individuals with A board-certified rehabilitation neuropsychologist as-
higher CR are able to recover more rapidly from patho- signed a TBI severity level of mTBI (n = 28), cmTBI
logical disruption of preexisting networks by enlisting (n = 24), or msevTBI (n = 57) using diagnostic cri-
compensatory networks (ie, neuroplasticity).9–11 Longi- teria that has been well-described previously27–29 (see
tudinal studies assessing whether CR predicts rate of Table 1). Individuals with penetrating brain injuries (eg,
cognitive recovery are critical for providing evidence for gunshot wound) were excluded from the study. Patients
the compensation model in a TBI population. Unfor- were excluded if they had received substance abuse treat-
tunately, few studies have approached this topic, using ment within 1 year of enrollment (per patient/family
statistical methods that allow for prediction of rate of report) or had a preexisting diagnosed central nervous
change (eg, mixed modeling). These studies failed to system disorder, developmental disorder, or severe psy-
find a relationship between CR and recovery trajecto- chiatric disorder. Individuals with a prior mTBI were
ries in the first year postinjury on any of the cognitive or included if their previous injury occurred at least 1 year
functional domains assessed.21–24 As previously stated, before enrollment; however, all those with a history of
some evidence exists that the benefit of CR during re- msevTBI were excluded.
covery may be tempered in those with more severe head Healthy controls were recruited through local adver-
injury.17,20 Since all of these studies primarily involved tisements and selected to match participants with TBI
individuals with moderate to severe TBI (msevTBI), it on demographic variables of age, gender, ethnicity, and
is possible that the compensation model applies only education. Controls were excluded if they had been di-
to those with milder TBI; however, this has yet to be agnosed with a psychiatric disorder (except mild depres-
investigated. sion), substance abuse, cerebrovascular disease, or other
To our knowledge, this is the first study to assess neurological disease. None of the controls were taking
whether CR, as measured by estimated premorbid IQ, medications known to affect cognition.
predicts cognitive outcome at 1 month postinjury and Participants completed a battery of neuropsychologi-
rate of recovery over the first year postinjury using the cal measures at each visit. Written informed consent was
full spectrum of TBI severity (healthy controls, mTBI, obtained from each participant or a legally authorized
cmTBI, and msevTBI). First, we hypothesized that those representative. The UAB institutional review board ap-
with higher levels of CR would have better initial postin- proved the study procedures. All enrolled patients with
jury cognitive function (neural reserve). Second, we hy- TBI were capable of participating in the cognitive evalua-
pothesized that those with higher levels of CR would tion as judged by the study neuropsychologist, although
have faster cognitive recovery rates in the first year not all patients had achieved full orientation at the time
after injury (neural compensation). Third, we exam- of the baseline assessment.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
The Role of Cognitive Reserve in Recovery From Traumatic Brain Injury 3

TABLE 1 Criteria for TBI severity classificationa


Structural
brain
Severity GCS LOC PTA changesb
Mild TBI 13-15 ≤30 min ≤24 h No
Complicated mild TBI 13-15 ≤30 min ≤24 h Yes
Moderate/severe TBI ≤12 >30 min >24 h Yes

Abbreviations: GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; LOC, loss of consciousness; PTA, posttraumatic amnesia; TBI, traumatic brain injury.
a Initial GCS scores were not available for intubated patients with moderate to severe TBI.
b Cranial magnetic resonance imaging or computed tomography scans were used to determine evidence of structural brain changes,

such as contusions, subdural hematoma, or diffuse axonal injury.

Twenty-five patients with TBI and 17 controls did 1. Processing speed/executive function: Trail Making Test
not complete all 3 visits due to the following reasons: Part A and B,38 Digit Symbol Coding and Symbol
scheduling problems (7%), no longer interested in partic- Search subtests from the WAIS-III35
ipating (7%), moved/distance (5%), and unknown/other 2. Verbal fluency: animal fluency, fruit/vegetable flu-
(81%). Noncompleters did not differ from completers ency, and clothing fluency39 ; and
with regard to demographic variables, estimated IQ, 3. Memory: California Verbal Learning Test, second
TBI classification/GCS score, or baseline cognitive edition40 (Trials 1–5 total recall, short delay free
outcomes. recall, and long-delay free recall), Logical Mem-
ory I and II (immediate and delayed recall) sub-
Measures test of the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition
(WMS-III).41
Wechsler Test of Adult Reading If participants were missing more than half of their
Word pronunciation tests are commonly used “hold” individual raw test scores for a domain, they were ex-
tests, which are neuropsychological measures thought to cluded from analyses. Otherwise, we averaged the tests
be unaffected by neurological damage. They have been that they had available.
used as premorbid IQ estimates in a variety of cogni-
tively impaired populations.30–33 One such word pro- Data analysis
nunciation task is the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
(WTAR).34 The WTAR is composed of 50 words with ir- Demographic and clinical variables were compared
regular pronunciations that participants read aloud. The between the 4 groups using 1-way analysis of variance
raw score can be transformed to an age-adjusted stan- for continuous variables and Pearson chi-square tests for
dard score, which is used together with the participants’ dichotomous variables.
demographic information (age, gender, race, education) Mixed-effects models were used to examine the in-
to predict IQ (M = 100; SD = 15). The WTAR was teraction between injury severity and CR on the initial
conormed with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, status and rate of change over time (or slope). Sepa-
Third Edition (WAIS-III),35 and has been validated even rate models were used for each of the 3 cognitive do-
in populations exhibiting questionable effort.36 mains. Mixed models were chosen for analyses as they
incorporate all participants in the estimation, regardless
of how many waves of data they contribute.42,43 Data
Neuropsychological measures
were first modeled using an unconditional means model,
All participants were administered a standardized bat- which describes and partitions outcome variation in the
tery of neuropsychological tests shown to be helpful in absence of any within- or between-subject predictors.
distinguishing TBI-related cognitive deficits from nor- Next, data were modeled using level 1 (ie, “uncondi-
mal cognition.37 As a data reduction step to control the tional growth”) models, which incorporated the effect
number of experiment-wise analyses, composite cogni- of time (measured in months; centered on 1 month
tive indices were created by transforming raw neuropsy- postinjury, which was the initial time point of assess-
chological test scores into z scores using published nor- ment). Because individuals completed only a maximum
mative data, referenced later. Domains were created by of 3 visits, a linear effect for time was selected. Level
averaging the z scores for each test within the domain 1 models assessed whether there was sufficient varia-
(M = 0; SD = 1). The domains are as follows: tion in initial status and growth trajectories that other
www.headtraumarehab.com

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
4 JOURNAL OF HEAD TRAUMA REHABILITATION

predictor variables might explain. These parameters (ini- Cognitive outcome


tial status, rate of change) are then treated as outcomes
Table 4 presents the group means and standard de-
in level 2 (ie, “conditional growth”) models. These mod-
viations for each of the cognitive domains over time.
els assess whether between-subjects variables, including
Table 5 presents the estimated parameters from the 2
WTAR-predicted IQ (centered on population mean of
main fitted models (level 2 conditional growth models
100), TBI severity (controls as reference group), and the
with all hypothesized predictors and interactions and
interaction between IQ × TBI severity, predict initial
the final, reduced level 2 models with only significant
status, and/or moderate rate of change (ie, have a sig-
predictors) for each of the cognitive domains.
nificant interaction with “Time”). As both the outcome
Examination of the variance components from the
variables and WTAR-predicted IQ were already adjusted
unconditional means model (not displayed in Table
for demographic variables, these were not added as co-
5) revealed that there was sufficient between-subjects
variates to the model. Final level 2 models were con-
variation in processing speed/executive function, mem-
structed using only significant predictors from the prior
ory, and verbal fluency, accounting for 74.8%, 61.0%,
model. Restricted maximum likelihood was used for pa-
and 62.5% of total variation for each domain, respec-
rameter estimation. An unstructured error covariance
tively. Variance components from level 1 unconditional
structure was selected, as it is the least restrictive and
growth models (also not displayed in Table 5) sug-
recommended for analyses using few waves of data.42
gested that the linear effect of time explained an ade-
The Aikaike Information Criterion was used to deter-
quate portion of within-person variation. In addition,
mine whether the inclusion of additional predictors im-
there was significant between-person variation in initial
proved the model over the previous one. The assigned
status (but not in slope) remaining, suggesting that a
significance level for analyses was P < .05.
level 2 conditional growth model with between-subjects
predictors would be beneficial. Finally, this model
showed that higher initial cognitive status was nega-
RESULTS
tively correlated with rate of change across all domains
Demographic and sample characteristics (all P < .05).
The results from the initial level 2 models, which
Table 2 lists the causes of TBI. Table 3 presents
included all hypothesized predictors and interactions,
demographic/clinical variables for the groups. Individ-
revealed that TBI severity and WTAR-estimated IQ, but
uals with cmTBI were older than the controls and those
not their interaction, significantly moderated initial level
with msevTBI, there was a higher proportion of non-
of impairment on all 3 cognitive domains. Only TBI
white participants in the mTBI group than the other
severity significantly moderated rate of change for each
2 TBI groups, and participants with msevTBI had a
of the cognitive domains.
higher percentage of males than all other groups (all
The final level 2 models included only these signif-
P < .05). As expected, the msevTBI group had the lowest
icant main effects. With regard to the effect of CR on
Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test (GOAT)
outcome, a 10-point increase in estimated premorbid
scores at 1 month postinjury. Of note, the msevTBI
IQ was associated with a cognitive domain standard de-
group had significantly lower estimated IQ than con-
viation increase of 0.31 for processing speed/executive
trols (mean difference of 9.8 IQ points); however, they
function domain, 0.37 for memory, and 0.25 for verbal
were still well within the normative “average” range.
fluency (calculated by multiplying the effect of IQ by
10; all P < .001).
At 1 month postinjury, controls did not differ from
TABLE 2 Causes of TBI in patient sample the population mean on processing speed/executive
(n = 109) function or memory domains; however, they did
significantly improve over time in both of these
Cause N % domains, indicating practice effects on these measures.
In contrast, controls performed approximately a quarter
Car collision 62 57
Falls 21 19 of a standard deviation lower than the population mean
Other motor vehicle collision 12 11 on verbal fluency at the first evaluation and remained
(motorcycle, all-terrain vehicle, stable over time. Individuals with mTBI performed
etc) significantly worse than controls at 1 month postinjury
Assault with blunt instrument 5 5
on all domains, but their recovery trajectories did
Bicycle 3 3
Sports 2 2 not differ from changes noted in performance among
Other 4 4 controls. Those with cmTBI and msevTBI performed
worse than controls on all domains at 1 month

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
The Role of Cognitive Reserve in Recovery From Traumatic Brain Injury 5

TABLE 3 Demographic and clinical characteristics of study participantsa


Variables Range Controls (n = 66) mTBI (n = 28) cmTBI (n = 24) msevTBI (n = 57) F/χ 2
Age, y 19-79 35.4 ± 14.4 35.4 ± 12.4 45.3 ± 18.4 35.5 ± 14.8 3.368b
Gender (male) 40 (60.6) 15 (53.6) 13 (54.2) 45 (78.9) 8.220b
Race (white) 48 (72.7) 15 (53.6) 20 (83.3) 48 (84.2) 10.465b
Education, y 8-20 12.9 ± 1.5 13.1 ± 1.9 13.9 ± 2.8 12.6 ± 2.4 2.232
WTAR IQ 68-125 101.4 ± 11.1 97.1 ± 11.6 101.9 ± 13.1 91.6 ± 14.5 7.194c
GOAT 0-100
1 mo NA 96.0 ± 4.9 95.3 ± 6.8 70.3 ± 32.5 14.928c
6 mo NA 94.3 ± 5.8 96.6 ± 6.0 92.0 ± 12.6 1.615
12 mo NA 93.1 ± 6.4 97.5 ± 3.7 94.4 ± 15.2 0.811
GCS 3-15 NA 14.6 ± 0.8 14.7 ± 0.6 5.8 ± 3.0

Abbreviations: cmTBI, complicated mild TBI; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; GOAT, Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test; msevTBI,
moderate to severe TBI; mTBI, mild TBI; NA, not applicable; WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.
a Values are mean ± SD or n (%). Gender and race differences analyzed using the Pearson chi-square test. All other variables were

assessed using analysis of variance. Differences in GCS were not assessed as this was used to classify TBI severity.
b P < .05.
c P < .001.

postinjury and had significantly more rapid rates of DISCUSSION


change than controls, reflecting recovery of cognitive
This study found evidence that higher CR is associ-
abilities. Figure 1 displays average recovery on each
ated with better cognitive performance in all tested do-
cognitive domain by severity group and high/low
mains, providing evidence for the neural reserve model
predicted IQ.
of CR in this population. To illustrate the clinical signif-
Given that our TBI groups differed in premorbid IQ,
icance of these findings, compare 2 individuals recover-
models were reanalyzed with the effect of TBI severity
ing from TBI: one with a high average premorbid IQ of
excluded to determine whether TBI severity significantly
115 and the other with a low average IQ of 85. The indi-
accounted for the IQ effect. The effect of IQ remained
vidual with higher IQ performs 0.6 standard deviations
nonsignificant, which indicates that the confounding
better on the verbal fluency domain, 0.9 standard de-
of TBI severity and premorbid IQ did not markedly
viations better on the processing speed/executive func-
influence results.
tion domain, and 1.2 standard deviations better on the

TABLE 4 Means and standard deviations for the 3 cognitive domains at each time point
across the TBI severity groupsa
Processing
Months speed/executive
Group postinjury N function Memory Verbal fluency
Controls 1 66 0.14 ± 0.69 − 0.16 ± 0.95 −0.23 ± 0.75
6 58 0.33 ± 0.77 0.22 ± 1.07 −0.20 ± 0.75
12 50 0.42 ± 0.75 0.43 ± 0.94 −0.10 ± 0.77
Mild TBI 1 28 − 0.51 ± 0.94 − 0.58 ± 0.91 −0.81 ± 0.90
6 27 0.01 ± 0.83 − 0.07 ± 0.90 −0.55 ± 0.69
12 26 − 0.03 ± 0.81 − 0.07 ± 1.09 −0.57 ± 0.84
Complicated mild TBI 1 24 − 0.85 ± 1.10 − 0.87 ± 1.37 −0.88 ± 0.97
6 21 − 0.16 ± 0.80 − 0.07 ± 0.93 −0.63 ± 0.78
12 17 − 0.06 ± 0.85 − 0.14 ± 1.01 −0.47 ± 0.72
Moderate to severe TBI 1 57 − 2.13 ± 0.80 − 2.46 ± 1.05 −2.01 ± 0.58
6 46 − 0.96 ± 0.78 − 1.01 ± 1.14 −1.25 ± 0.72
12 40 − 0.65 ± 0.89 − 0.96 ± 1.16 −1.10 ± 0.60

are averaged z scores presented as mean ± SD. Of those who completed the visit, the following numbers of subjects were
a Values

excluded from specific domains because they were missing >50% of the variables for the domain: At 1-month postinjury, 1 was
excluded from processing speed/executive function, 7 were excluded from verbal fluency, and 2 were excluded from memory. At 6
months postinjury, 1 was excluded from memory.
www.headtraumarehab.com

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
6

TABLE 5 Initial status and rate of change over 12-months for each of the cognitive domains (n = 175)a
Cognitive domain

Processing speed/
executive function Memory Verbal fluency
Full Final Full Final Full Final
Initial status Intercept 0.11 (0.09) 0.11 (0.09) 0.06 (0.12) 0.05 (0.11) − 0.27 (0.09)b − 0.28 (0.09)b
mTBI − 0.44 (0.16)b − 0.42 (0.16)c − 0.48 (0.21)c − 0.42 (0.20)c − 0.42 (0.16)b − 0.41 (0.16)b
cmTBI − 0.97 (0.17)d − 0.94 (0.17)d − 0.91 (0.22)d − 0.91 (0.22)d − 0.71 (0.17)d − 0.68 (0.17)d
msevTBI − 1.87 (0.14)d − 1.82 (0.14)d − 1.98 (0.18)d − 2.00 (0.17)d − 1.43 (0.14)d − 1.39 (0.13)d
IQ (centered) 0.03 (0.01)d 0.03 (0.00)d 0.03 (0.01)b 0.04 (0.00)d 0.02 (0.01)c 0.02 (0.00)d
IQ × mTBI − 0.01 (0.01) − 0.01 (0.02) 0.01 (0.01)
IQ × cmTBI 0.02 (0.01) 0.00 (0.02) 0.02 (0.01)
IQ × msevTBI − 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01)
Rate of change Time, mo 0.03 (0.01)d 0.03 (0.00)d 0.022 (0.01) 0.03 (0.01)c 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01)
Time × mTBI 0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) 0.03 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01)
JOURNAL OF HEAD TRAUMA REHABILITATION

Time × cmTBI 0.05 (0.01)d 0.05 (0.01)b 0.06 (0.02)c 0.06 (0.02)c 0.04 (0.02)c 0.04 (0.02)c
Time × msevTBI 0.11 (0.01)d 0.10 (0.01)d 0.12 (0.02)d 0.12 (0.01)d 0.08 (0.01)d 0.07 (0.01)d
Time × IQ − 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Time × IQ × mTBI 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Time × IQ × cmTBI 0.00 (0.00) − 0.00 (0.00) − 0.00 (0.00)
Time × IQ × msevTBI 0.00 (0.00) − 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00)
Goodness of fite AIC 903.4 836.8 1282.1 1218.7 910.4 842.5

Abbreviations: AIC, Aikaike Information Criterion; cmTBI, complicated mild TBI; msevTBI, moderate to severe TBI; mTBI, mild TBI; TBI, traumatic brain injury.
a SAS Proc Mixed, Restricted ML. Full level 2 conditional growth model includes the effects of TBI severity (controls as a reference group), WTAR IQ (centered on population mean of

100), and their interaction on both initial status and rate of change; final model includes only significant predictors from full model. “Intercept” and “Time” refer to average performance
at 1 month postinjury and rate of change per month, respectively, for controls with an average WTAR predicted IQ of 100 (with significance indicating that these values significantly differ
from 0). Parameter estimates and significance testing for TBI groups are relative to controls (ie, significance indicates that they significantly differ from those without head injury).
b P < .01.
c P < .05.
d P < .001.
e Smaller value indicates better model fit.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
The Role of Cognitive Reserve in Recovery From Traumatic Brain Injury 7

Figure 1. Average cognitive recovery trajectories for each group split by estimated premorbid intelligence. Outcomes for each
domain (panels A, B, and C) are presented as z scores, which are standardized values with a population mean of 0 and
standard deviation of 1. WTAR-estimated IQ was dichotomized into high/low average categories (±1 standard deviation from
the population mean of 100) for the purposes of visualization only.

memory domain shortly after injury in comparison to Contrary to our second hypothesis, we did not find
the individual with low average IQ. Given that this ef- that CR impacts rate of cognitive recovery following
fect was identical for healthy, demographically matched TBI. These results suggest that the compensation model,
controls and did not differ on the basis of the degree which states that CR enables more rapid restoration of
of brain injury (ie, there was no interaction between CR function via reorganization of brain networks, cannot
and TBI severity), these results did not support our hy- be extended to a population of individuals recovering
pothesis that having higher CR provides an additional from TBI. One limitation of this work is that our first
buffer specifically for those with TBI. Importantly, we point of assessment was 1 month following injury, when
did not find evidence that CR reaches a level of dam- some individuals, especially those with milder head in-
age at which it becomes less effective (ie, evidence of jury, may have already experienced a full recovery. Fu-
a brain damage threshold). Rather, these results sug- ture studies should seek to assess patients closer to the
gest that the positive correlation between intelligence date of injury to capture the full recovery period and
and cognition that exists in healthy adults is preserved allow for more accurate prediction. In addition, cogni-
following TBI. tive trajectory was assessed at only 3 time points over

www.headtraumarehab.com

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
8 JOURNAL OF HEAD TRAUMA REHABILITATION

the first year postinjury. Because of the relatively few mance on individual tests within a domain, overall abil-
time points, only linear slope could be estimated when ity should be broadly intact in these domains for indi-
in reality the recovery slope is likely asymptotic.44 It viduals with estimated premorbid IQ of 100 or more.
is possible that the imperfect fit of slope impacted our Interestingly, our msevTBI group’s predicted IQ was
ability to find significance. Another possibility is that nearly 10 points lower than those of the other groups,
the effects on slope were too modest to detect with our despite similar demographics. There is evidence that
relatively small sample size. word-reading tests may underestimate IQ in persons
These results are consistent with the study of Green with moderate to severe TBI,45–47 which could indicate
et al21 , who also found an association between estimated that this measure does not accurately reflect the “cogni-
premorbid IQ and performance on simple processing tive reserve” concept in this subgroup. An ideal study
speed, untimed executive function, and memory mea- would have information regarding intellectual perfor-
sures at 2 months postinjury in a msevTBI sample. Their mance before participants sustained their head injury,
study, along with others, also failed to find a moderating as this would be a true measure of premorbid IQ. How-
effect of CR variables on recovery trajectory.22–24 ever, this was not available for the current sample.
As expected, the severity of TBI negatively impacted In conclusion, these results support the neural re-
initial performance and rate of recovery in all cognitive serve theory of CR within a TBI population and there
domains. Notably, we found differences in both initial is no specific brain damage “threshold” at which reserve
status and rate of recovery between those with mild ceases to be beneficial. Our results suggest that the ef-
and complicated mild TBI. Clinicians should be aware fect of CR is due to preservation of the relationship
that although an individual may fall in to the “mild between intelligence and cognition following TBI. In
TBI” category based on traditional diagnostic criteria contrast, having higher levels of premorbid CR does
(eg, posttraumatic amnesia, GCS, loss of consciousness), not allow for more rapid adaptation and recovery from
presence of intracranial damage (ie, “complicated” mild injury. Thus, early intervention and cognitive rehabil-
TBI) will negatively impact prognosis. Because of the itation efforts should be equally targeted regardless of
insufficient number of participants with moderate TBI, estimated IQ. There are currently only a handful of
we were unable to separate them from the severe TBI studies using mixed modeling to determine which vari-
group for analyses. ables predict initial status and rate of recovery following
Another strength of this study is that we included injury. In addition, this is the first of these studies to as-
a wide breadth of cognitive domains to assess a larger sess whether there is an interaction between CR (ie, IQ)
spectrum of outcome in comparison to previous liter- and TBI severity on outcome and rate of change, using
ature. Interestingly, across all groups, performance in the full severity spectrum of TBI and matched controls.
the verbal fluency domain was the overall most im- The results of this study and others in this area have
paired initially and had the slowest rate of improve- concluded that many preinjury variables do not appear
ment. Although statistical comparison of interdomain to have a significant effect on recovery rate following
differences is outside the scope of this study, these re- TBI. These findings have favorable implications from
sults indicate that verbal fluency, which incorporates a clinical perspective, since these are variables that are
aspects of expressive language, processing speed, and often difficult, if not impossible, to change. Therefore,
executive function, may be particularly sensitive to the future studies should turn to modifiable postinjury vari-
neuropathological effects of head injury. Targeting these ables to account for variation in recovery. More detailed
skills heavily during the rehabilitation process may prove knowledge of the mechanisms influencing recovery will
beneficial to outcome. Finally, it is important to note hopefully allow clinicians to further refine their ability
that individuals with an average level of intelligence fell in identifying survivors of TBI most at risk of cogni-
within the “normal range” of functioning (±0.25 SD) tive impairment, individuals who are not recovering at
on the memory and processing speed/executive func- expected rates, and develop treatment plans to address
tion domains by a year following TBI of any severity. areas of cognitive function most likely to remain im-
Although there may still be some deviation in perfor- paired following injury.

REFERENCES

1. NIH consensus development panel on rehabilitation of persons 3. Dikmen S, Corrigan JD, Levin HS, Machamer J, Stiers W,
with traumatic brain injury. JAMA. 1999;282:974–983. Weisskopf MG. Cognitive outcome following traumatic brain
2. Rutland-Brown W, Langlois JA, Thomas K, Xi Y. Incidence of injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2009;24:430–438.
traumatic brain injury in the United States. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 4. Rabinowitz AR, Levin HS. Cognitive sequelae of traumatic brain
2006;21:544–548. injury. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2014;37:1–11.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
The Role of Cognitive Reserve in Recovery From Traumatic Brain Injury 9

5. Lingsma HF, Roozenbeek B, Steyerberg EW, Murray GD, Maas 24. Chu BC, Mills S, Arango-Lasprilla JC, Hanks R, Novack T, Hart
AI. Early prognosis in traumatic brain injury: from prophecies to T. Measuring recovery in new learning and memory following
predictions. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:543–554. traumatic brain injury: a mixed-effects modeling approach. J Clin
6. Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, et al. Long-term neuropsy- Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29:617–625.
chological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma 25. Triebel KL, Martin RC, Novack TA, et al. Treatment consent
Rehabil. 2001;16:343–355. capacity in patients with traumatic brain injury across a range of
7. Faul M, Wald MM, Rutland-Brown W, Sullivent EE, Sattin RW. injury severity. Neurology. 2012;78:1472–1478.
Using a cost-benefit analysis to estimate outcomes of a clinical 26. Kalmar K, Novack TA, Nakase-Richardson R, et al. Feasibility
treatment guideline: testing the Brain Trauma Foundation guide- of a brief neuropsychologic test battery during acute inpatient
lines for the treatment of severe traumatic brain injury. J Trauma rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.
Inj Infect Crit Care. 2007;63:1271–1278. 2008;89:942–949.
8. Mathias JL, Wheaton P. Contribution of brain or biological reserve 27. Kashluba S, Hanks RA, Casey JE, Millis SR. Neuropsychologic
and cognitive or neural reserve to outcome after TBI: a meta- and functional outcome after complicated mild traumatic brain
analysis (prior to 2015). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:573–593. injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89;904–911.
9. Steffener J, Stern Y. Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve 28. Brasure M, Lamberty GJ, Sayer NA, et al. Multidisciplinary Posta-
in aging. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2012;1822:467–473. cute Rehabilitation for Moderate to Severe Traumatic Brain Injury in
10. Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, et al. Brain networks associated with Adults (Prepared by the Minnesota Evidence-based Practice Cen-
cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex. ter under Contract No. 290-2007-10064-I). AHRQ Comparative
2005;15:394–402. Effectiveness Reviews Publication No. 12-EHC101-EF. Rockville,
11. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2012.
Assoc Disord. 2006;20:69–74. 29. Williams DH, Levin HS, Eisenberg HM. Mild head injury classi-
12. Garcia-Molina A, Ensenat-Cantallops A, Sanchez-Carrion R, fication. Neurosurgery. 1990;27:422–428.
Rodriguez P, Tormos JM, Roig-Rovira T. Interindividual variabil- 30. Dwan TM, Ownsworth T, Chambers S, Walker DG, Shum DH.
ity in recovery after traumatic brain injury: effect of cognitive Neuropsychological assessment of individuals with brain tumor:
reserve. Med Clin (Barc). 2013;140:527–531. comparison of approaches used in the classification of impairment.
13. Schneider E, Sur S, Raymont V, et al. Functional recovery af- Front Oncol. 2015;5:1–8.
ter moderate/severe traumatic brain injury: a role for cognitive 31. McGurn B, Starr JM, Topfer JA, et al. Pronunciation of irregular
reserve? Neurology. 2014;82:1636–1642. words is preserved in dementia, validating pre-morbid IQ estima-
14. Sigurdardottir S, Andelic N, Roe C, Schanke A. Cognitive recovery tion. Neurology. 2004;62:1184–1186.
and predictors of functional outcome 1 year after traumatic brain 32. Hanks RA, Millis SR, Ricker JH, et al. The predictive validity
injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:740–750. of a brief inpatient neuropsychologic battery for persons with
15. Novack T, Bush B, Meythaler J, Canupp K. Outcome after trau- traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89:950–957.
matic brain injury: pathway analysis of contributions from premor- 33. Green RE, Melo B, Christensen B, Ngo LA, Monette G, Bradbury
bid, injury severity, and recovery variables. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. C. Measuring premorbid IQ in traumatic brain injury: an exam-
2001;82:300–305. ination of the validity of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
16. Ponsford J, Draper K, Schonberger M. Functional outcome 10 (WTAR). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30:163–172.
years after traumatic brain injury: its relationship with demo- 34. Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading: WTAR. San Antonio,
graphic, injury severity, cognitive and emotional status. J Int Neu- TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2001.
ropsychol Soc. 2008;14:233–242. 35. Wechsler D. WAIS-III Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition.
17. Jeon I, Kim O, Kim M, Kim S, Chang C, Bai D. The effect of San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
premorbid demographic factors on the recovery of neurocognitive 36. Whitney KA, Shepart PH, Mariner J, Mossbarger B, Herman S.
function in traumatic brain injury patients. J Korean Neurosurg Soc. Validity of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR): effort
2008;44:295–302. considered in a clinical sample of U.S. military veterans. Appl
18. Sumowski J, Chiaravalloti N, Krch D, Paxton J, DeLuca J. Educa- Neuropsychol Adult. 2010;17:196–204.
tion attenuates the negative impact of traumatic brain injury on 37. Lezak M, Howieson D, Loring D. Neuropsychological Assessment.
cognitive status. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013;94:2562–2564. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.
19. Kesler S, Adams H, Blasey C, Bigler E. Premorbid intellectual 38. Reitan R, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test
functioning, education, and brain size in traumatic brain injury: Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tuscon, AZ: Neuropsy-
an investigation of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Appl Neu- chology Press; 1993.
ropsychol. 2003;10:153–162. 39. Spreen O, Strauss E. Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Ad-
20. Fay TB, Yeates KO, Taylor HG, et al. Cognitive reserve as a mod- ministration, Norms, and Commentary. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Ox-
erator of postconcussive symptoms in children with complicated ford University Press; 1998.
and uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol 40. Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. CVLT-II California Verbal
Soc. 2010;16:94–105. Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation;
21. Green R, Colella B, Christensen B, et al. Examining moderators of 2000.
cognitive recovery trajectories after moderate to severe traumatic 41. Wechsler D. WMS-III Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition. San
brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89:S16–S24. Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.
22. Spitz G, Ponsford J, Rudzki D, Maller JJ. Association between cog- 42. Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling
nitive performance and functional outcome following traumatic Change and Event Occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University
brain injury: a longitudinal multilevel examination. J Neuropsychol. Press; 2003.
2012;26:604–612. 43. Brown H, Prescott R. Applied Mixed Models in Medicine. 2nd ed.
23. Spikman JM, Timmerman ME, Zomeren van AH, Deelman BG. Chichester, UK: Wiley; 2006.
Recovery versus retest effects in attention after closed head injury. 44. Christensen BK, Colella B, Inness E, et al. Recovery of cogni-
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1999;21:585–605. tive function after traumatic brain injury: a multilevel modeling

www.headtraumarehab.com

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
10 JOURNAL OF HEAD TRAUMA REHABILITATION

analysis of Canadian outcomes. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2008;89: reading subtest as a measure of premorbid intelligence among
S3–S15. persons with brain injury. Rehabil Psychol. 2004;49:250–253.
45. Mathias JL, Bowden SC, Bigler ED, Rosenfeld JV. Is performance 47. Steward KA, Novack TA, Kennedy R, Crowe M, Marson DC,
on the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading affected by traumatic brain Triebel KL. The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading as a measure of
injury? Br J Clin Psychol. 2007;46:457–466. premorbid intelligence following traumatic brain injury. Arch Clin
46. Orme DR, Johnstone B, Hanks R, Novack TA. The WRAT-3 Neuropsychol. 2017;32:98–103.

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen