Sie sind auf Seite 1von 18

LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Infants & Young Children


Vol. 18, No. 2, pp. 86–103
c 2005 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Inc.

The Emerging Down


Syndrome Behavioral
Phenotype in Early Childhood
Implications for Practice
Deborah J. Fidler, PhD
Previous studies have reported a specific behavioral phenotype, or a distinct profile of behav-
ioral outcomes, associated with Down syndrome. Until recently, however, there has been little
attention given to how this behavioral profile emerges and develops over time. It is argued here
that some aspects of the Down syndrome behavioral phenotype are already emerging in infants
and toddlers, including emerging relative strengths in some aspects of visual processing, recep-
tive language and nonverbal social functioning, and relative weaknesses in gross motor skills and
expressive language skills. Research on the early developmental trajectory associated with Down
syndrome (and other genetic disorders) is important because it can help researchers and practi-
tioners formulate interventions that are time-sensitive, and that prevent or offset potential future
negative outcomes. This article reviews evidence for the emerging Down syndrome behavioral
phenotype in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. This is followed by a discussion of intervention
approaches that specifically target this developing profile, with a focus on language, preliteracy
skills, and personality motivation. Key words: behavioral phenotypes, Down syndrome, early
intervention

D OWN syndrome is the most common ge-


netic (chromosomal) mental retardation
syndrome, occurring in from 1 in 700 to 1
endocrine system abnormalities (Pueschel &
Pueschel, 1992).
Over the past few decades, research has
in 1000 live births (Hassold & Jacobs, 1984; begun to converge on a specific behavioral
Stoll, Alembik, Dott, & Roth, 1990). In 95% phenotype, or a distinct profile of behav-
of cases, Down syndrome is caused by an ex- ioral outcomes, associated with Down syn-
tra chromosome 21 (trisomy 21). Common drome as well. According to recent studies,
physical features associated with Down syn- the Down syndrome behavioral phenotype in-
drome are a distinctive craniofacial structure cludes relative strengths in some aspects of
and health-related issues like congenital heart visuospatial processing (Jarrold & Baddeley,
disease, middle ear disease, and immune and 1997; Jarrold, Baddeley, & Hewes, 1999; Klein
& Mervis, 1999; Wang & Bellugi, 1994), and
social functioning (Gibbs & Thorpe, 1983;
Rodgers, 1987; Wishart & Johnston, 1990),
From the Human Development & Family Studies, as well as relative deficits in verbal pro-
Colorado State University, Fort Collins. cessing (Byrne, Buckley, MacDonald, & Bird,
The author thanks Susan Hepburn, PhD, and Amy 1995; Hesketh & Chapman, 1998; Jarrold
Philofsky, MA, CCC-SLP, for their ideas and comments et al., 1999; Laws, 1998) and some aspects
on early drafts of this article. The author also thanks of motor functioning (Chen & Woolley, 1978;
Sally Rogers, PhD, for her guidance on this project.
Dunst, 1988; Fidler, Hepburn, Mankin, &
Corresponding author: Deborah J. Fidler, PhD, 102 Gif- Rogers, in press; Jobling, 1998; Mon-Williams
ford Building, 502 West Lake St, Colorado State Uni-
versity, Fort Collins, CO 80523 (e-mail: fidler@cahs. et al., 2001). Language has been described as
colostate.edu). a “major area of deficit” in Down syndrome
86
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 87

(Sigman & Ruskin, 1999), with particu- behavioral phenotypes have shown that ar-
lar difficulties manifested in expressive lan- eas of purported relative strength at one stage
guage (Miller & Leddy, 1999). In addition, of development (middle childhood or adoles-
individuals with Down syndrome have been cence) may not have been relatively stronger
described as showing a distinct personal- at other stages of development (early child-
ity motivation profile (Pitcairn & Wishart, hood; Paterson, Brown, Gsoedl, Johnson, &
1994). Karmiloff-Smith, 1999). There may be crucial
Researchers often acknowledge 2 impor- windows of opportunity in early development
tant issues when studying behavioral pheno- to target areas that pose potential problems to
types (Dykens & Hodapp, 2001). First, they children with Down syndrome before they be-
acknowledge that behavioral phenotypes are come pronounced areas of weakness. Thus,
probabilistic. As such, groups with a certain understanding how the Down syndrome be-
syndrome are more likely to show one or havioral phenotype emerges over the first few
more “characteristic” behaviors than other in- years of early development may help shape ef-
dividuals with mental retardation, but not fective, time-sensitive intervention for young
every child with a specific syndrome nec- children with Down syndrome and their
essarily shows all etiology-specific behaviors families.
(Dykens, 1995; Hodapp, 1997). Addition-
ally, researchers note that some syndromes
share certain behavioral outcomes with other EARLY EMERGENCE OF THE DOWN
genetic disorders, so outcomes are often SYNDROME BEHAVIORAL PHENOTYPE
not specific to a particular syndrome. In
several genetic disorders of mental retarda- Compared to other genetic disorders, early
tion (eg, fragile X syndrome, Williams syn- development in Down syndrome has received
drome), for example, many children show hy- a good deal of research attention. Develop-
peractivity or attention problems (Hodapp, ment in infancy and toddlerhood has rarely
1997). been studied in other genetic disorders such
as Prader-Willi syndrome, Williams syndrome,
or Smith-Magenis syndrome. Even in those
DEVELOPMENTAL CONSIDERATIONS few existing studies on early development
in other syndromes, the focus is primarily
Amidst the recent advances in behav- on issues such as early feeding in infancy
ioral phenotype research (Dykens & Hodapp, (Morris, Demsey, Leonad, Dilts, & Blackburn,
2001), researchers have also begun to ar- 1988), rather than on the development of var-
gue for the importance of understanding ious aspects of cognitive-linguistic or social-
how phenotypes develop and change over emotional functioning.
time (Karmiloff-Smith, 1997). Rather than The wealth of research on early develop-
considering outcomes as preserved or dam- ment in Down syndrome may be attributed
aged modules that are wholly intact or im- to the higher incidence of Down syndrome
paired uniformly throughout development, than that of other genetic syndromes, as well
Karmiloff-Smith (1998) argues that “tiny vari- as technological advances enabling early iden-
ations in the initial state” can become magni- tification of Down syndrome. Since the late
fied throughout development into domains of 1960s, it has been possible to screen preg-
relative strength and weakness. Early develop- nant women for Down syndrome via amnio-
ment may be a crucial window of opportu- centesis and karyotyping of fetal cells. In addi-
nity for intervention, as these “tiny variations” tion, in most neonatal units, diagnostic testing
have not yet snowballed into impairments in is standard procedure for any newborns
whole domains of processing. Studies to date showing the cardiovascular, craniofacial, or
that have taken a developmental approach to other physical features associated with Down
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

88 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

syndrome. This stands in contrast with the de- than others in older children and young adults
lay of diagnosis often seen in other genetic with Down syndrome (Fidler, 2005). In par-
disorders, for example, Williams syndrome ticular, visual memory visual-motor integra-
(Huang, Sadler, O’Riordan, & Robin, 2002; tion, and especially visual imitation seem to
Morris et al., 1988). be areas of relative strength within visuospa-
Thus, early identification has facilitated the tial processing, whereas spatial memory and
description of early social-emotional function- visuoconstructive tasks seem to be areas of
ing, cognitive-linguistic development, person- relative weakness (Fidler, 2005).
ality motivation, and motoric functioning in
young children with Down syndrome over the Early developmental precursors
first few years of life. This research can be Can evidence of this cognitive profile al-
seen as a description of the early initial states ready be found in early development? Later
of an emerging behavioral phenotype and can deficits in auditory processing could be linked
help shed light on how the specific outcomes to atypical auditory brain-stem responses in
in genetic disorders change and become more infants with Down syndrome in the first year
pronounced throughout development. of life (Folsom, Widen, & Wilson, 1983). In
The following section discusses research on addition, the high incidence of congenital
different domains of development, including anomalies of the ear in this population—
cognitive, linguistic, motor, social-emotional, otitis media, for example—has been linked to
and motivational functioning. For each do- deficits in auditory processing in early devel-
main of development, functioning in older opment as well (Downs & Balkany, 1988). But
children, adolescents, and young adults with a distinction must be made between auditory
Down syndrome is discussed. Then, research perception and short-term/working mem-
on related findings in early development—or ory for auditorally presented information, as
what could be considered the “developmen- Jarrold et al. (2002) have identified a short-
tal precursors” to these later outcomes—is term memory deficit for auditorally presented
discussed. verbal information in older individuals that
cannot be attributed to sensory deficits. It
Cognitive functioning may be that poor vocal imitation in infants
with Down syndrome is linked to later deficits
Children, adolescents, and young adults in verbal working memory in this popula-
Research on the cognitive phenotype in tion, but this connection should be explored
adolescents and adults with Down syndrome further (Mahoney, Glover, & Finger, 1981;
has most recently focused on deficits in ver- Rondal, 1980). It may also be important to
bal working memory, and on how they re- consider that precursors to deficits in verbal
late to poor expressive language and learn- working memory and related cognitive skills
ing outcomes (Byrne et al., 1995; Hesketh & may not be present in early childhood and
Chapman, 1998; Jarrold, Baddeley, & Phillips, may emerge later in development.
2002; Laws, 1998). In addition, studies have Evidence of strengths in visual processing
found relative strengths in visuospatial pro- in early development in Down syndrome can
cessing in this population, and many individ- be found in studies of infant visual recognition
uals with Down syndrome have a profile of memory, where infants with Down syndrome
stronger visuospatial than verbal processing show similar event-related brain potential
skills (Jarrold et al., 1999; Klein & Mervis, morphology, visual attention, and visual fix-
1999; Wang & Bellugi, 1994). Amidst these ation to typically developing infants (Karrer,
relative strengths in visuospatial processing, Karrer, Bloom, Chaney, & Davis, 1998; Karrer,
there is preliminary evidence that some as- Wojtascek, & Davis, 1995). Infants with Down
pects of visuospatial processing are stronger syndrome have even shown evidence of faster
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 89

information processing than have typical in- & Kay-Raining Bird, 1991). Individuals with
fants on some components of visual mem- Down syndrome also show particular deficits
ory (Karrer et al., 1995). In line with findings in the development of grammar, and many
of strong visual imitation skills in older chil- adults with Down syndrome do not progress
dren, there are also reports of early visual (not beyond the early stages of morphological
vocal) imitative competence in infants with and syntactic development (Fowler, 1990). In
Down syndrome, similar to the performance terms of speech, Miller and Leddy (1999) re-
of typically developing infants (Heiman & port that articulation and speech intelligibil-
Ulstadius, 1999). ity is a major challenge for many individuals
However, not all areas of visuospatial func- with Down syndrome as well. However, de-
tioning are relatively strong in young children spite deficits in language and speech, older in-
with Down syndrome, a finding that could dividuals with Down syndrome show relative
be associated with ocular abnormalities com- strengths in nonverbal communication (Miller
monly found in this population (Niva, 1988; & Leddy, 1999).
Woodhouse et al., 1996). Gunn, Berry, and
Andrews (1982) report that 6-month-old in-
fants with Down syndrome show delays in vi- Early developmental precursors
sual exploration in play situations with their A similar profile of deficits in language
mothers (Gunn et al., 1982). Other reports de- and speech development, but strengths in
scribe impaired visual attention performance communicative competence, can already be
on a habituation task in infants with Down found in early development of children with
syndrome (Miranda & Fantz, 1973), and de- Down syndrome. In terms of speech and ex-
lays in various aspects of eye contact in infants pressive language, atypical vocalizing is al-
with Down syndrome, including the func- ready evident in infants with Down syndrome
tional use of eye contact to explore the en- from 2 to 12 months, who produce atypical
vironment in a parent-child interactive set- prelinguistic phrases compared to those pro-
ting (Berger & Cunningham, 1983). These duced by typically developing infants (Lynch,
early development findings also suggest that Oller, Steffens, & Buder, 1995). In the first 6
in both infancy and later development, mix- months of life, infants with Down syndrome
tures of strengths and weaknesses can be also produce more non–speech-like sounds
found within this area of functioning. than speech-like sounds, which may nega-
tively impact the later development of nor-
Language, speech, and communication mal vocal behavior (Legerstee, Bowman, &
Fels, 1992). Additionally, delays in age of on-
Children, adolescents, and young adults set of canonical babbling have been found in
Many children with Down syndrome have infants with Down syndrome (Lynch, Oller,
severe language delays (Sigman & Ruskin, Steffens, Levine, et al., 1995). In contrast
1999). Part of the Down syndrome lan- with the relatively strong visual imitative com-
guage phenotype includes pronounced im- petence in young children with Down syn-
pairments in expressive language relative to drome, as mentioned earlier, vocal imitation
receptive language, including large deficits seems to be greatly impaired (Mahoney et al.,
in vocabulary size relative to mental age 1981; Rondal, 1980). Decreased vocal imi-
(Chapman, 1999; Fabretti, Pizzuto, Vicari, & tation in Down syndrome has been shown
Voterra, 1997). In terms of receptive lan- to be associated with lower expressive and
guage, whereas receptive vocabulary is MA receptive language skills (Mahoney et al.,
appropriate in later childhood and adoles- 1981).
cence, comprehension of syntax lags behind Nevertheless, other aspects of prelinguis-
(Abbeduto et al., 2003; Chapman, Schwartz, tic vocal development seem to be on par
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

90 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

with typically developing infants, including Longobardi, & Caselli, 2003). Yet, even in the
the amount of vocalization produced, de- context of these communicative strengths,
velopmental timetable of vocalizations, and other aspects of early communicative com-
characteristics of consonants and vowels pro- petence seem to be impaired. In particular,
duced during babbling (Oller & Seibert, 1988; young children with Down syndrome show
Smith & Oller, 1981; Smith & Stoel-Gammon, deficits in nonverbal requesting behaviors
1996; Steffens, Oller, Lynch, & Urbano, (Mundy et al., 1988, 1995; Fidler et al., in
1992). press; Wetherby et al., 1989).
One of the most important studies of early
speech and language functioning in Down Social-emotional functioning
syndrome has demonstrated that the major-
ity (64%) of children with Down syndrome Children, adolescents, and young adults
aged 0 to 5 years fit a profile of receptive lan- Although deficits in speech, language, and
guage that is mental age appropriate while ex- communication are common, many older in-
pressive language lags behind (Miller, 1999). dividuals with Down syndrome nonetheless
In addition, this study found that over time, show relative strengths in social function-
the number of children who fit this profile ing. For example, individuals with Down syn-
increased to 72%, suggesting that some chil- drome may show relative competence in
dren may be “growing into”this profile as they forming relationships with others. Freeman
develop. Miller (1999) reported that there and Kasari (2002) found that the majority of
seemed to be 2 distinct groups of young chil- children with Down syndrome in their sam-
dren with Down syndrome—one group that ple showed relationships with peers that met
showed impairment from the onset of first criteria for true friendships—reciprocal nom-
words and a second group that acquired vo- ination in the friendship dyad, convergence
cabulary but showed expressive language lags between parental and child nomination, and
when language learning advanced to more dif- at least 6-month stability of friendship in that
ficult skills, such as the combining of words dyad. Children with Down syndrome have
into phrases. also been shown to be more empathic than
In terms of early communicative compe- other children with developmental disabili-
tence, some areas seem to be intact whereas ties, showing more prosocial responses in a
others are impaired. Young children with simulated distress situation (Kasari, Freeman,
Down syndrome show MA-appropriate levels & Bass, 2003). Children with Down syn-
of nonverbal joint attention (Fidler, Philofsky, drome may also “overuse” their social skills
Hepburn, & Rogers, in press; Mundy, Kasari, to compensate for other weaker domains of
Sigman, & Ruskin, 1995; Mundy, Sigman, functioning (Freeman & Kasari, 2002). In an
Kasari, & Yirmiya, 1988; Wetherby, Yonclas, impossible task study, for example, young chil-
& Bryan, 1989). In addition, despite deficits in dren with Down syndrome showed looks to
expressive language development, the early the experimenter and more “party pieces,”
use of gestures in children with Down syn- or charming off-task behaviors that engaged
drome seems to be intact. One study found the experimenter socially (Pitcairn & Wishart,
a “gesture advantage” in young children with 1994).
Down syndrome compared with controls Children with Down syndrome may also
matched for word comprehension (Caselli send more positive emotional signals than
et al., 1998). Another study found that despite may other children with mental retardation.
a smaller repertoire of gestures, no differ- In one study, 5- to 12-year-olds with Down syn-
ences could be found for overall usage of drome smiled more frequently than children
gestures between young children with with other mental retardation syndromes, al-
Down syndrome and a comparison group though this finding of increased smile fre-
of language-age–matched children (Iverson, quency changed as individuals with Down
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 91

syndrome approached adulthood (Fidler & play relative strengths in certain types of
Barrett, in press). nonverbal social interaction including more
play acts, turn taking, invitations, and object
Early developmental precursors shows compared to typically developing chil-
Some aspects of this socioemotional profile dren (Mundy et al., 1988; Sigman & Ruskin,
are already present in infancy. Visual imitative 1999).
competence in infancy has been described One aspect of social-emotional function-
as evidence of “an innate social competence” ing in Down syndrome that may be of par-
(Heiman & Ulstadius, 1999). In terms of early ticular interest is the ability to communicate
looking behavior, Crown, Feldstein, Jasnow, positive affect through frequent emotion dis-
and Beebe (1992) found that infants with plays such as smiles. Initial studies of emo-
Down syndrome look longer at their moth- tion communication in infants with Down
ers than typically developing infants even at 4 syndrome reported muted emotion displays
months of age, a behavior that may promote and less emotional lability than typically devel-
connections with others. These findings are oping infants (Berger & Cunningham, 1986;
echoed in a study by Gunn et al. (1982), who Buckhalt, Rutherford, & Goldberg, 1978;
found that 6- and 9-month-olds with Down Cicchetti & Sroufe, 1978; Emde & Brown,
syndrome spent nearly half of their interac- 1978; Rothbart & Hanson, 1983). Later stud-
tion time looking toward their mother, and by ies, however, that were conducted with more
Kasari, Freeman, Mundy, and Sigman (1995), objective coding systems (ie, MAX and FACS),
who found increased looking behavior at par- suggested that although there may be more
ents during an ambiguous situation. However, frequent low-intensity smiling in young chil-
in the context of increased looking behav- dren with Down syndrome, this may be in ad-
ior, Kasari et al. (1995) and Walden, Kneips, dition to frequent high-intensity smiling, such
and Baxter (1991) found decreased social that there may actually be more smiling and
referencing. increased emotional lability in young children
Other evidence of social competence in with Down syndrome than in typically de-
infancy can be found in increased melodic veloping children (Kasari, Mundy, Yirmiya, &
sounds, vocalic sounds, and emotional sounds Sigman, 1990; Kneips, Walder, & Baxter,
in 4-month-old infants with Down syndrome 1994). These findings are in line with the
when interacting with people rather than finding of increased smiling behavior in older
with objects (Legerstee, Bowman, & Fels, children and preadolescents with Down syn-
1992). Evidence of continued social com- drome (Fidler & Barrett, in press).
petence seems to continue throughout tod-
dlerhood and pre-school–aged children with Motor functioning
Down syndrome. At 17.5 months, infants
with Down syndrome show responses to ma- Overview
ternal requests that are similar to those re- Another aspect of the Down syndrome be-
sponses made by typically developing infants havioral phenotype described in older individ-
(Bressanutti, Sachs, & Mahoney, 1992). In a uals involves difficulties with motor skills and
modified strange situation, 24-month-olds motor planning (Jobling, 1998; Mon-Williams
with Down syndrome show distress when et al., 2001). Jobling (1998) reported that 10-
their mothers are absent, with increased cry- to 16-year-old children with Down syndrome
ing and noncrying distress and increased have specific motor impairments, including
looks at the door—behavior described as sim- difficulty with precise movements of limbs
ilar to that observed in typically develop- (eg, stepping over a stick while on a bal-
ing children (Berry, Gunn, & Andrews, 1980; ance beam) and fingers (eg, pivoting thumb
see also Vaughn et al., 1994). Toddlers and and index finger) as well as gross motor tasks
preschoolers with Down syndrome also dis- such as sit-ups and push-ups. Similar relative
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

92 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

weaknesses have been demonstrated in mo- In terms of motor planning, infants with
tor planning or praxis (Mon-Williams et al., Down syndrome show more deviation from
2001). However, in other domains such as straight lines and changes in plane of motion
running speed and agility and visual-motor during reaching behavior than typically de-
control, Jobling (1998) reports that child per- veloping infants, evidence of a deficit in the
formance in Down syndrome can be at CA organization of reaching movement (Cadoret
levels. & Beuter, 1994). Fidler et al. (in press) ex-
plored whether motor delays in Down syn-
Developmental precursors drome include deficits in motor planning, and
Most infants and toddlers with Down syn- whether motor planning is related to adaptive
drome show extreme motor delays relative functioning in this population. Toddlers with
to CA-matched typically developing infants, Down syndrome in this study performed sig-
moving through stages of early motor de- nificantly worse on a battery of motor plan-
velopment more slowly and exhibiting more ning tasks, including reaching into a jar to
within-group variability than typically devel- grasp a nerf ball, and stringing beads, a find-
oping infants (Chen & Woolley, 1978; Dunst, ing specific to Down syndrome and not at-
1988). Abnormal movement patterns, hypoto- tributable to disability status in general. Fur-
nia, and hyperflexibility are common in this thermore, partial correlations demonstrated
population (Harris & Shea, 1991). In addition, a strong association between overall adap-
delays in the emergence and termination of tive motor functioning and motor planning
reflexes are prevalent in early motor develop- performance in both disability groups even
ment in this population (Block, 1991; Harris when age was partialled out. Similar asso-
& Shea, 1991). These atypical outcomes seem ciations were found between motor plan-
to become more evident toward the end of ning and daily living skills, suggesting that
the first year of life (Dunst, 1988; Henderson, motor planning deficits in Down syndrome
1985). may also be associated with day-to-day adap-
Dmitriev (2001) describes 4 different types tation, and not only motor-related adaptive
of infants with Down syndrome on the basis skills.
of muscle tone and motor functioning. Type 1
(15%–25%) babies have good muscle tone and Personality motivation
show milestones like head control, bearing
weight on feet with support and lifting the Overview
torso on extended arms by 4 months. Types Individuals with Down syndrome have fre-
2 and 3 (50%–60%) babies show a discrep- quently been described as having charm-
ancy between upper and lower body mo- ing personalities, often in accordance with a
tor functioning. Type 2 infants have strong positive Down syndrome personality stereo-
upper back, neck, shoulders, and arms, but type (Gibbs & Thorpe, 1983; Rodgers, 1987;
are unable to bear weight on their legs as Wishart & Johnston, 1990). Older children
other infants are able to do, whereas Type 3 and young adults with Down syndrome are de-
infants have strong legs and lower torso, scribed as of primarily positive mood and pre-
but weaker upper torso, neck, head, shoul- dictable in their behavior, but less active and
ders, and arms. Finally Type 4 babies (15%– persistent and more distractible than other
25%) are weak all over, with flaccid arms and children as well (Gunn & Cuskelly, 1991).
legs, and often have accompanying cardio- In one study, over 50% of 11-year-old chil-
vascular challenges. These groupings suggest dren with Down syndrome were described as
that although there is variability within Down “affectionate,” “lovable,” “nice,” and “getting
syndrome motor functioning in infancy, the on well with other people,”and many children
majority of infants do face serious motor chal- were also described as “cheerful,”“generous,”
lenges that warrant intervention. and “fun” (Carr, 1995).
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 93

Alongside these positive perceptions, many Morss (1983) has similarly reported that
individuals with Down syndrome are also infants with Down syndrome repeat their
described as showing inconsistency in mo- successes on tasks less often than mental,
tivational orientation. Many children with age-matched, typically developing children.
Down syndrome also show lower levels of Hasan and Messer (1997) found that children
task persistence and higher levels of off-task with Down syndrome in their sample showed
behavior tasks, interfering with task com- more stability in performance on executive
pletion (Landry & Chapieski, 1990; Pitcairn function/object permanence and other cog-
& Wishart, 1994; Ruskin, Kasari, Mundy, & nitive tasks although 20% of their sample
Sigman, 1994; Vlachou & Ferrell, 2000). These did show some regressions. Researchers sug-
individuals are sometimes described as stub- gest that these regressions often result from a
born or strong willed, traits that may con- child’s unwillingness to engage in a task, sug-
tribute to inconsistent performance on tasks gesting that motivation may be an important
due to task refusal (Carr, 1995; Gibson, 1978). factor for assessing development in Down syn-
drome (Pitcairn & Wishart, 1994; Wishart &
Developmental precursors Duffy, 1990).
Several studies report no significant tem- According to Wishart (1993), “[F]rom a
perament differences between infants with very early age, it would appear that the Down
Down syndrome and typical infants in early syndrome children are avoiding opportunities
infancy, at 2 months (Ohr & Fagen, 1994) and for learning new skills, making poor use of
later at 12 to 36 months (Vaughn, Contreras, & skills that are acquired, and failing to consoli-
Seifer, 1994). Other studies, however, report date skills into their repertoires.” Along these
that young children with Down syndrome lines, increased level of help elicitation has
(M = 30 months) are rated as of more positive also been found in Down syndrome and may
mood, more rhythmic, and less intense than relate to persistence issues as well. In the mo-
CA-matched children (Gunn & Berry, 1985). tor planning study described above (Fidler,
These findings echo the findings of increased Hepburn, Mankin, & Rogers, in press), it was
predictability, increased positive mood, and also found that toddlers with Down syndrome
decreased persistence in older children with elicited significantly more help on the ob-
Down syndrome. However, nearly one third ject retrieval task than did children in both
of children with Down syndrome in Gunn and comparison groups, a finding also reported in
Berry’s (1985) study showed signs of difficult other studies (Freeman & Kasari, 2002).
temperament as well, a possible precursor to
stubbornness and other behavior problems. Overall profile of early development
The developmental precursors of task per- in Down syndrome
sistence findings may also be identifiable In addition to exploring the early devel-
in early development. Young children with opment of various aspects of functioning
Down syndrome often show inconsistent per- in Down syndrome, it may also be impor-
formance on assessment measures from time- tant to explore “cross-domain relations,” or
point to timepoint (Morss, 1983; Wishart & how different domains of functioning develop
Duffy, 1990). Wishart and Duffy (1990) found together (Hodapp, 1996). Are pronounced
that children with Down syndrome aged 6 dissociations between areas of strength and
months to 4 years show highly inconsistent weakness already observable in early child-
performances on the same testing battery hood? In a recent study, Fidler et al. (in press)
across sessions 2 weeks apart. The authors described the performances of young chil-
suggested that this inconsistency is the result dren with Down syndrome on measures of
of motivational issues, often the result of re- visual processing, expressive language, recep-
fusal to engage fully in tasks at either time- tive language, fine and gross motor function-
point (Wishart & Duffy, 1990). ing, and social functioning on the Mullen
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

94 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

Scale of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995). Their associated with their genetic disorder. Rather
performance was compared to the perfor- than waiting for a dissociated pattern to take
mance of a group of children with other devel- its full form, interventions can focus on pre-
opmental disabilities and a group of typically venting these dissociations from taking place.
developing children, with all groups equated
on mental age. USING BEHAVIORAL PHENOTYPE
Toddlers with Down syndrome in this study RESEARCH TO INFORM EARLY
did show relative strengths in the areas of vi- INTERVENTION IN DOWN SYNDROME
sual processing and receptive language, and
relative weaknesses in gross motor skills and One of the most interesting questions that
expressive language, although it is important arise from behavioral phenotype research
to note that these dissociations were small in concerns the influences of child transactional
magnitude. In terms of parent-reported skills history on the developing behavioral phe-
in adaptive behavior in real-life situations, the notype. Is it possible to help children with
children with Down syndrome in this study Down syndrome follow more optimal devel-
showed relative strengths in socialization and opment pathways? This section will explore
relative weaknesses in communication and approaches that show promise—intervention
motor skills. This is evidence that phenotypic approaches that are informed by behavioral
pattern of strengths and weaknesses associ- phenotype research.
ated with Down syndrome is emerging by Amidst the many proposed theoretical
the age of 2, with between-group differences approaches to intervention in Down syn-
in sociability, and within-group patterns of drome, researchers have introduced yet an-
relative strengths and weaknesses that fore- other approach—focusing on behavioral phe-
shadow the phenotype described in studies of notype research (Hodapp & DesJardin, 2003;
older persons. Hodapp & Fidler, 1999). This approach argues
It is notable that the dissociations observed that education and intervention may be more
within the individuals with Down syndrome effective when it specifically targets the de-
were significant, but also relatively small at velopmental trajectory associated with a par-
these early developmental ages. Even in the ticular syndrome. The behavioral phenotype
significant difference between expressive and approach is housed within the larger move-
receptive language, differences averaged only ment of developmental interventions, where
2.5 months in age-equivalent scores. In other programming decisions are informed by de-
studies with older children with Down syn- velopmental theory (see Spiker, 1990, for a
drome, dissociations between domains of review).
functioning can be much larger. This does not The importance of time sensitivity and early
minimize the rapid changes that take place implementation in intervention has also been
over several months in early development. But demonstrated in this population. In one study,
the relatively small dissociation is also notable a 2-month delay of treatment for young chil-
for intervention purposes—because areas of dren with Down syndrome was associated
strength and weakness are less pronounced with lower gross motor, fine motor, language,
early on, it may be possible to reduce these and social outcomes at 18 months (Sanz &
dissociations and set areas of potential weak- Menendez, 1995). In another study, infants
ness on more optimal pathways. who received language intervention begin-
With this understanding of the early emer- ning as newborns showed more optimal out-
gence of the Down syndrome behavioral phe- comes than did infants who started the inter-
notype in infants and toddlers, it may be possi- vention at 90 and 180 days of age (Sanz &
ble to shape intervention that is sensitive not Balana, 2002).
only to the current functioning level of the Yet, the efficacy of intervention in Down
child but also to the developmental trajectory syndrome and other groups remains in
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 95

question (for reviews, see Gibson & Fields, tial, rather than verbal information, might
1984; Gunn & Berry, 1989; Guralnick, 1996; this information be used to improve develop-
Nilholm, 1996). The implementation of just mental outcomes in this population? Several
any intervention is not sufficient for im- suggestions have been made to this effect
proving developmental outcomes (Crombie & (Byrne et al., 1995; Chapman, 1995; Gibson,
Gunn, 1998; Gibson & Fields, 1984). In ad- 1991). Pueschel, Gallagher, Zartler, and
dition, some interventions that have become Pezzullo (1987) noted that “[t]eaching strate-
popular at different points have been ineffec- gies should capitalize on Down syndrome
tive. For example, popular high-dosage mul- children’s strengths and should focus on
tivitamin and mineral supplements that have visual-vocal and visual-motor processing
been administered to infants and children modalities in remediation” (p. 35). They also
with Down syndrome aged 7.5–63 months note that “increasing emphasis on auditory
have been shown to be associated with de- teaching strategies may lead to frustration in
creased, rather than increased, developmen- the child and may impede academic process”
tal progress, according to one study (Bidder, (p. 35). Indeed, a recent study demonstrates
Gray, Newcombe, & Evans, 1989). These high- that children with Down syndrome respond
dosage multivitamins and supplements may better to scaffolding that involves both
also be associated with unpleasant side effects speech and gestures (visual) than to scaffolds
as well. Nevertheless, parents report improve- that involve only speech (Wang, Bernas, &
ments in child appearance and skin tone with Eberhard, 2001).
these products, and some parents report that Yet amidst the many recommendations for
they would recommend the vitamin therapy an increased attention on visual processing in
to other parents of children with Down syn- Down syndrome, there have been relatively
drome (Bidder et al., 1989). few efforts to utilize this processing mode
With the increased prevalence of alterna- to improve outcomes. According to Nadel
tive and unconventional therapies aimed at (1996), “there has been scant application of
parents of children with disabilities and their knowledge about the specific learning abili-
children, there is a strong need for inter- ties and disabilities of Down syndrome indi-
ventions that are rooted in good science. viduals to the development of these programs”
At this point in time, interventions that are (p. 22).
informed by behavioral phenotype research One exception is the movement to em-
have not been tested in the literature. Test- phasize early reading in young children with
ing the tenability of such an approach will Down syndrome (Buckley, Bird, & Byrne,
need to involve scientific rigor and the high 1996; Oelwein, 1995; Oelwein, Fewell, &
standards found in other types of treatment Pruess, 1985). Buckley and colleagues sug-
trials (Kasari, 2002). Yet, there is promise in gest that it is possible to improve language
this approach that it is rooted in good sci- and memory functioning by establishing early
ence, and it is in line with recommendations sight vocabularies in children with Down syn-
that educational programs target “the specific drome (Buckley et al., 1996). They argue that
learning abilities and disabilities of Down syn-
drome individuals” (Nadel, 1996). A prelimi- [T]he benefits of learning to read go beyond sim-
nary sampling of intervention ideas that are ply acquiring a functionally useful level of read-
ing and writing skill . . . reading can develop speech
informed by behavioral phenotype research is
and language skills, auditory perceptional skills and
described in the following section. working memory function; all areas where children
with Down syndrome usually display difficulties.
Cognitive-linguistic functioning
(p. 269)
and intervention
If individuals with Down syndrome do Early sight vocabularies for children with
show an advantage for processing visuospa- Down syndrome capitalize on their strengths
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

96 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

in visual memory to recognize and identify and as such, a diagnosis of Down syndrome
words, making logographic reading possible should automatically make a child eligible for
at young ages. Buckley et al. (1996) cite case speech and language intervention services.
studies of children with Down syndrome who Miller (1999) also argues that language inter-
are 2 and 3 years old and who have greatly vention should focus on targeting and pre-
benefited from the establishment of sight vo- venting expressive language impairments in
cabularies, findings that have been echoed Down syndrome before they become pro-
by parents as well (eg, Carter, 1985; Duffen, nounced. Continuous reinforcement for vo-
1976). calizations in infants between 2 and 8 months
Another group advocating the use of early has been shown to increase vocalization rates
sight vocabularies is team at the University (Poulson, 1988), a promising technique to en-
of Washington Model Preschool Program for courage precursors to expressive language.
Children with Down Syndrome and Other In addition, some have suggested that
Developmental Delays (Oelwein, 1988). Ac- language intervention should promote oral
knowledging that most preschoolers do not motor functioning in Down syndrome, while
receive formal reading instruction, Oelwein facilitating communication regardless of
described the decision to teach reading to modality (verbal or nonverbal; Miller, 1999).
preschoolers with Down syndrome as one As such, it may be beneficial to target oral
that provided a solution for children who dyspraxia early in speech therapy. Parents
“had very well-developed visual discrimina- can be taught to use techniques such as
tion skills, but virtually no spoken language.” back-chaining, prompt-fading, and social
This approach advocates scheduling 5 to 7 praise as a reward for effort. Other rec-
minutes of reading instruction during short, ommendations have been made to target
individualized sessions 2 to 4 days per week. the difficult transition in Down syndrome
Data collected on children in this program from babbling to meaningful speech. For
suggest that children with Down syndrome example, Stoel-Gammon (2001) recommends
can develop sight vocabularies at all levels of that adults offer phonetically contingent re-
IQ, and that reading level is highly related to sponses to prelinguistic vocalizations during
receptive language scores—not IQ. These sug- infancy. These responses can help an infant
gestions are well justified and target areas of understand and start to produce meaningful
distinct strength in the developing Down syn- utterances. Stoel-Gammon (2001) also sug-
drome behavioral phenotype. gests the use of sound games to facilitate the
However, more evidence is needed to sub- awareness of sound-meaning relationships in
stantiate the claim that reading can impact infants and toddlers with Down syndrome.
other areas of development (Kemp, 1996; It may also be helpful to provide nonverbal
Lorenz, Sloper, & Cunningham, 1985). means of communicating in the first years of
life (eg, gestures, picture exchange, sign lan-
Language outcomes guage), in addition to building language skills,
More direct routes to improving language as a way of minimizing frustration. These and
outcomes can also be informed by behav- other recommendations specifically target
ioral phenotype research in Down syndrome the developing linguistic phenotype in Down
as well. Miller (1999) argues that it is un- syndrome and may be more effective because
necessary for—and may be detrimental to— they keep an eye toward Down syndrome
children with Down syndrome to wait for language outcomes in general.
almost inevitable deficits in expressive lan-
guage to become apparent and then docu- Motivation orientation, social
mented. He argues that linguistic phenotype functioning, and intervention
research in Down syndrome demonstrates the In planning early interventions, caregivers,
inevitability of expressive language deficits, educators, and therapists must be aware of
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 97

the propensity of children with Down syn- tle, the feeling of accomplishment and a mother’s
drome to avoid challenging tasks via social praise when a toddler successfully pulls on a pair of
initiation. The development of cognitive, lan- socks . . . quickly teach the child which behaviors
guage, and motor skills relies upon frequent guarantee success. (p. 68)
practice with supports. Avoidance of these An operant conditioning approach may be
tasks will lead to a broader gap in skills over particularly helpful for children with Down
time and thus significantly inhibit the emer- syndrome, who can be prone to inconsis-
gence of adaptive skills. tent performance due to motivational issues
Help elicitation observed in some studies of (Wishart & Duffy, 1990). A steady flow of pos-
children with Down syndrome (Fidler et al., in itive motivational feedback may serve to con-
press) can be interpreted in several ways. On tinue to motivate children as they proceed
one hand, children may be using their ability through a challenging task, especially given
to relate to others in ways that help them com- the social orientation of many children with
plete tasks more successfully. This may bode Down syndrome.
well for individuals who might otherwise not In addition, children with Down syndrome
be able to perform certain daily living skills should be encouraged to use their social skills
tasks. On the other hand, most new tasks are in adaptive and appropriate ways. Lloveras
difficult for children at various points of de- and Fornells (1998), for example, recommend
velopment, and part of the growth process symbolic play group approaches that facili-
involves challenging oneself to develop skills tate “the construction of relational competen-
in order to overcome obstacles. If individuals cies which are needed for . . . social integra-
with Down syndrome are eliciting help this tion and . . . global satisfactory development”
early in development, they may be missing out (p. 89).
on important challenging early experiences
that may promote their growth.
To address the motivational issues in Down Motor skills and intervention
syndrome early development, errorless learn- Recent findings suggest that various
ing techniques may be important ways to pre- approaches to early motor intervention—
vent task abandonment in Down syndrome approaches that focus on developmental
(Fidler, in press; Oelwein, 1995). To maximize functioning and approaches that focus on
task persistence during interventions, practi- functional skills—may have little effect
tioners and parents may opt for alternate ac- on improving developmental outcomes in
tivities by skill domain (ie, social, expressive Down syndrome and other disability groups
language, receptive language, motor), begin- (Mahoney, Robinson, & Fewell, 2001). How-
ning and ending with domains of strength (eg, ever, these findings may not be generalizable
social, receptive language). Intervention ap- to all intervention studies, particularly be-
proaches can also more readily target areas cause parents were not included in the
of deficit by imbedding them in tasks that in- intervention approaches studied (see discus-
volve areas of strength. For example, to in- sion below).
crease practice of motor foundation skills, it Building a stronger motor foundation in-
may be useful to imbed motor tasks in play volves participation in purposeful, relevant
and other social contexts. activities that incorporate specific compo-
Dmitriev (2001) recommends an operant nents of motor foundation. For example, to
conditioning-grounded approach in Down improve and maintain appropriate posture
syndrome that involves rewarding desired be- and position, a child may work on these
haviors. He suggests that skills while sitting at a table engaging in a
task. The skill development component of the
actions that result in success or the attainment of Denver Model intervention involves targeting
a desired goal—the fun of playing with a new rat- initiation actions, imitation of others, hand
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

98 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

development, coordination, and dexterity. In addition to focusing on the specific child


This may be especially helpful for young chil- profile, a focus on family ecology and the
dren with Down syndrome who may have parent-child may be crucial for successful in-
difficulty with initiation due to hypotonia tervention in Down syndrome and other dis-
and a less persistent temperament, but who ability groups (Spiker, 1990). Bronfenbrenner
show strengths in visual imitation due to so- (1974) was among the first to argue that early
cial and other factors. In this model, skills intervention is most effective if the family is an
are taught using shaping and other prompt- active agent in implementation. Early motor
ing techniques, with each skill broken down intervention that includes parental involve-
and chaining procedures are used in multistep ment has been shown to have a positive ef-
sequences (Osaki, Roger, & Hall, 2000). This fect on early development in Down syndrome
recommendation is echoed by others for chil- (Torres & Buceta, 1998), while intervention
dren with Down syndrome (Dmitriev, 2001). studies that do not involve parents have been
Finally, the compensatory strategies compo- shown to be less effective (Mahoney et al.,
nent of the model includes adaptations to the 2001; see Spiker, 1990, for a review). Chil-
task that allow for independence rather than dren may also show better outcomes when
a dependence on prompting and help elicita- parents are trained directly by practitioners,
tion throughout the task. as studies show that parents who are trained
directly by clinicians fare better than those
General recommendations given written instructions (Sanz, 1988, 1996).
Other more general recommendations may Thus, the larger movement toward targeting
also be effective in Down syndrome early in- intervention to both the child and the context
tervention. For example, interventions that in which the child develops may play an im-
are informed by an understanding of the portant role in improving developmental out-
emerging phenotype in Down syndrome may comes in Down syndrome as well.
enable practitioners to focus on areas of
strength as a “way in” for interventions that
target potential areas of weakness. While in- SUMMARY
tervention is typically drawn to the relative
weaknesses in an individual’s developmental As a part of a larger movement toward
phenotype (Hodapp & Zigler, 1990), it may studying the behavioral phenotypes associ-
also be interesting to consider the emerging ated with different genetic disorders, this
phenotype as a reflection of strengths, a com- article explored the early developmental pre-
pensatory pattern constructed from areas of cursors to the Down syndrome behavioral
greatest competency that promotes adapta- phenotype. There is evidence that cognitive,
tion and access to preferred people and activ- linguistic, social, emotional, motivational, and
ities. Working from this framework, interven- motoric aspects of the Down syndrome be-
tion may choose to target strengths as strongly havioral phenotype are already emerging in
as weaknesses, in helping people build a life the earliest years of life. In addition, cross-
that highlights their talents and interests. For domain relations observed in older individuals
example, children with Down syndrome may with Down syndrome also seem to be emerg-
be encouraged early on to pursue tasks that in- ing already in toddlers, although findings are
volve potential future strengths like visual pro- less pronounced at this early stage than they
cessing and visual-motor coordination, as well are in older children and adults.
as their relative strengths in social function- In light of this new understanding of devel-
ing and forming social relationships. Further- opment in genetic syndromes, it may now be
more, promotion of strengths in targeted ways possible to target domains of development,
may facilitate the bootstrapping of weaker such as expressive language, before they be-
skills. come areas of pronounced weakness. These
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 99

areas can be targeted via time-sensitive in- memory outcomes. The behavioral pheno-
terventions that are informed by phenotype type approach remains unsupported by evi-
research on older individuals with the syn- dence at this point in time and warrants rig-
drome. It may also be possible to use areas orous scientific testing to verify its utility. But
of relative strength as a “way in” to those this approach is grounded in good science and
areas of weakness, to prevent or offset fu- may prove to be the next shift in how services
ture delays, as suggested by early reading ap- are delivered to young children with Down
proaches that seek to impact language and syndrome and other genetic disorders.

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M. M., Cawthon, S. W., Richmond, reaching in Down syndrome infants. Early Human
E. K., Weissman, M. D., Karadottir, S., et al. (2003). Development, 36, 157–173.
Receptive language skills of adolescents and young Carr, J. (1995). Down’s syndrome: Children growing up.
adults with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 149– Carter, S. (1985, May). Darwin’s world of books. Excep-
160. tional Parent, 23–25.
Berger, J., & Cunningham, C. C. (1983). Development of Caselli, M. C., Vicari, S., Longobardi, E., Lami, L., PIzzoli,
early vocal behaviors and interactions in Down’s syn- C., & Stella, G. (1998). Gestures and words in early
drome and nonhandicapped infant–mother pairs. De- development of children with Down syndrome. Jour-
velopmental Psychology, 19, 322–331. nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41,
Berger, J., & Cunningham, C. C. (1986). Aspects of 1125–1135.
early social smiling by infants with Down’s syndrome. Chapman, R. S. (1999). Language development in chil-
Child: Care, Health and Development, 12, 13–24. dren and adolescents with Down syndrome. In J.
Berry, P., Gunn, P., & Andrews, R. (1980). Behavior of Miller, M. Leddy, & L. A. Leavitt (Eds.), Improving
Down syndrome infants in a strange situation. Ameri- the communication of people with Down syndrome
can Journal of Mental Deficiency, 85, 213–218. (pp. 81–92). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks Press.
Bidder, R. T., Gray, P., Newcombe, R. G., & Evans, B. Chapman, R. S. (1995). Language development in chil-
K. (1989). The effects of multivitamins and miner- dren and adolescents with Down syndrome. In P.
als on children with Down syndrome. Developmental Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of child
Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 532–537. language (pp. 641–663). Oxford: Blackwell.
Block, M. E. (1991). Motor development in children with Chapman, R. S., Schwartz, S. E., & Kay-Raining Bird, E.
Down syndrome: A review of the literature. Adapted (1991). Language skills of children and adolescents
Physical Activity Quarterly, 8, 179–209. with Down syndrome. I: Comprehension. Journal of
Bressanutti, L., Sachs, J., & Mahoney, G. (1992). Predictors Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 1106–1120.
of young children’s compliance to maternal behavior Chen, H., & Woolley, P. V. (1978). A developmental assess-
requests. International Journal of Cognitive Educa- ment chart for non-institutionalized Down syndrome
tion and Mediated Learning, 2, 198–209. children. Growth, 42, 157–165.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Developmental research pub- Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). An organizational
lic policy and the ecology of childhood. Child Devel- view of affect: Illustration from the study of Down’s
opment, 45, 1–5. syndrome infants. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenbaum
Buckhalt, J. A., Rutherford, R. B., & Goldberg, K. E. (Eds.), The development of affect. New York: Plenum
(1978). Verbal and nonverbal interaction of mothers Press.
with their Down’s syndrome and nonretarded infants. Crombie, M., & Gunn, P. (1998). Early intervention,
American Journal on Mental Deficiency, 82, 337– families and adolescents with Down syndrome.
343. International Journal of Disability, Development
Buckley, S., Bird, G., & Byrne, A. (1996). Reading acqui- and Education, 45, 253–281.
sition by young children. In B. Stratford & P. Gunn Crown, C. L., Feldstein, S., Jasnow, M. D., & Beebe, B.
(Eds.), New approaches to Down syndrome (pp. 268– (1992). Down’s syndrome and infant gaze: Gaze be-
279). London: Cassell. havior of Down’s syndrome and nondelayed infants in
Byrne, A., Buckley, S., MacDonald, J., & Bird, G. (1995). In- interactions with their mothers. European Journal of
vestigating the literacy, language, and memory skills of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Acta Paedopsychi-
children with Down’s syndrome. Down’s Syndrome: atrica, 55, 51–55.
Research and Practice, 3, 53–58. Dmitriev, V. (2001). Early intervention for children with
Cadoret, G., & Beuter, A. (1994). Early development of Down syndrome: Time to begin. Austin: Pro-Ed.
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

100 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

Downs, M. P., & Balkany, T. J. (1988). Otologic prob- drome: Emerging or true friendships? American Jour-
lems and hearing impairment in Down syndrome. In nal on Mental Retardation, 107, 16–31.
V. Dimitirev & P. L. Oelwein (Eds.), Advances in Down Gibbs, M. V., & Thorpe, J. G. (1983). Personality stereo-
syndrome (pp. 19–34). Seattle: Special Child Publica- type of noninstitutionalized Down syndrome children.
tions. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 87, 601–
Duffen, I. W. (1976). Teaching reading to children with 605.
little or no language. Remedial Education, 11, 139– Gibson, D. (1978). Down’s syndrome: The psychology of
142. mongolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dunst, C. (1988). Stage transitioning in the sensorimotor Gibson, D. (1991). Down syndrome and cognitive en-
development in Down’s syndrome infants. Journal of hancement: Not like the others. In K. Marfo (Ed.), Cur-
Mental Deficiency Research, 32, 405–410. rent perspectives on programs for handicapped chil-
Dykens, E. M. (1995). Measuring behavioral phenotypes: dren (pp. 61–90). New York: Praeger Publishers.
Provocations from the “new genetics.” American Gibson, D., & Fields, D. L. (1984). Early infant stimulation
Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 522–532. programs for children with Down syndrome: A review
Dykens, E. M., & Hodapp, R. M. (2001). Strengthening of effectiveness. Advances in Developmental and Be-
behavioral research on genetic mental retardation syn- havioral Pediatrics, 5, 331–371.
dromes. American Journal on Mental Retardation, Gunn, P., & Berry, P. (1985). The temperament of Down’s
106, 4–15. syndrome toddlers and their siblings. Journal of Child
Emde, R. N., & Brown, C. (1978). Adaptation after birth Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
of a Down’s syndrome infant. Journal of American 26, 973–979.
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 299–323. Gunn, P., & Berry, P. (1989). Education of infants with
Fabretti, D., Pizzuto, E., Vicari, S., & Voterra, V. (1997). Down syndrome. European Journal of Psychology of
A story description task in children with Down’s syn- Education, 2, 235–246.
drome: Lexical and morphosyntatic abilities. Journal Gunn, P., Berry, P., & Andrews, R. J. (1982). Looking be-
of Intellectual Disability Research, 41, 165–179. havior of Down syndrome infants. American Journal
Fidler, D. J. (2005). Refining visuo-spatial aspects of of Mental Deficiency, 87, 344–347.
the cognitive phenotype in children and adolescents Gunn, P., & Cuskelly, M. (1991). Down syndrome temper-
with Down syndrome. Manuscript in preparation. ament: The stereotype at middle childhood and ado-
Fidler, D. J. (in press). Motivation and etiology-specific lescence. International Journal of Disability, Devel-
cognitive-linguistic profiles. International Review of opment, and Education, 38, 59–70.
Research in Mental Retardation and Developmental Guralnick, M. (1996). Future directions in early inter-
Disabilities. vention for children with Down’s syndrome. In J. A.
Fidler, D. J., & Barrett, K. C. (in press). Age-related differ- Rondal, J. Perera, L. Nadel, & A. Comblain (Eds.),
ences in smiling and personality in Down syndrome. Down’s syndrome: Psychological, psychobiological,
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities. and socio-emotional perspectives (pp. 147–164). San
Fidler, D. J., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, S. (in press). Early Diego, CA: Whurr Publishers.
learning and adaptive behavior in toddler with Down Harris, S. R., & Shea, A. M. (1991). Down syndrome. In
syndrome: Evidence for an emerging behavioral phe- S. K. Campbell (Ed.), Pediatric neurologic physical
notype? Down Syndrome: Research and Practice. therapy (2nd ed., pp. 131–168). Melbourne, Australia:
Fidler, D. J., Hepburn, S., Mankin, G., & Rogers, S. (in Churchill Livingstone.
press). Motor planning and adaptive functioning in Hasan, P. J., & Messer, D. J. (1997). Stability ore instabil-
toddlers with Down syndrome. American Journal of ity in early cognitive abilities in children with Down’s
Occupational Therapy. syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Disabil-
Fidler, D. J., Philofsky, A., Hepburn, S. L., & Rogers, S. J. ities, 43, 93–107.
(in press). Nonverbal requesting and problem solving Hassold, T. J., & Jacobs, P. A. (1984). Trisomy in man. An-
in young children with Down syndrome. American nual Review of Genetics, 18, 69–97.
Journal on Mental Retardation. Heiman, M., & Ullstadius, E. (1999). Neonatal imita-
Folsom, R. C., Widen, J. E., & Wilson, W. R. (1983). Audi- tion and imitation among children with autism and
tory brain-stem responses in infants with Down’s syn- Down’s syndrome. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.),
drome. Archives of Otolaryngology, 109, 607–610. Imitation in infancy (pp. 235–253). Cambridge:
Fowler, A. E. (1990). Language abilities in children with Cambridge University Press.
Down syndrome: Evidence for a specific syntactic de- Henderson, S. E. (1985). Motor skill development. In
lay. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children D. Lane & B. Stratford (Eds.), Current approaches
with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective to Down syndrome (pp. 187–218). London: Holt,
(pp. 302–328). New York: Cambridge University Rinehart & Winston.
Press. Hesketh, L. J., & Chapman, R. S. (1998). Verb use by indi-
Freeman, S. F. N., & Kasari, C. (2002). Characteristics and viduals with Down syndrome. American Journal on
qualities of the play dates of children with Down syn- Mental Retardation, 103, 288–304.
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 101

Hodapp, R. M. (1996). Cross-domain relations in Down’s Karrer, R., Wojtascek, Z., & Davis, M. G. (1995). Event-
syndrome. In J. A. Rondal, J. Perera, L. Nadel, & related potentials and information processing in in-
A. Comblain (Eds.), Down’s syndrome: Psychologi- fants with Down syndrome. American Journal on
cal, psychobiological, and socio-educational perspec- Mental Retardation, 100, 146–159.
tives (pp. 65–80). London: Whurr Publishers. Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early intervention
Hodapp, R. M. (1997). Direct and indirect behavioral ef- programs for children with autism. Journal of Autism
fects of different genetic disorders of mental retarda- and Developmental Disorders, 32, 447–461.
tion. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, Kasari, C., Freeman, S. F. N., & Bass, W. (2003). Empathy
67–79. and response to distress in children with Down syn-
Hodapp, R. M., & DesJardin, J. (2003). Genetic syndromes drome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
of mental retardation: Should they matter for the and Allied Disciplines, 44, 424–431.
early interventionist? Infants and Young Children, Kasari, C., Freeman, S., Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. D.
16, 152–160. (1995). Attention regulation by children with Down
Hodapp, R. M., & Fidler, D. J. (1999). Special education syndrome: Coordinated joint attention and social ref-
and genetics: Connections for the 21st century. Jour- erencing looks. American Journal on Mental Retar-
nal of Special Education, 33, 130–137. dation, 100, 128–136.
Hodapp, R. M., & Zigler, E. F. (1990). Applying the devel- Kasari, C., Mundy, P., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1990).
opmental perspective to individuals with Down syn- Affect and attention in children with Down syndrome.
drome. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children American Journal on Mental Retardation, 95, 55–
with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective 67.
(pp. 1–28). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kemp, C. (1996). Does teaching young children with dis-
Huang, L., Sadler, L., O’Riordan, M., & Robin, N. H. (2002). abilities to read facilitate their language development?
Delay in diagnosis of Williams syndrome. Clinical Pe- A critical review of current theory and empirical evi-
diatrics, 41, 257–261. dence. International Journal of Disability, Develop-
Iverson, J. M., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (2003). Rela- ment and Education, 43, 175–187.
tionship between gestures and words in children with Klein, B. P., & Mervis, C. B. (1999). Contrasting patterns of
Down’s syndrome and typically developing children in cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams
the early stages of communicative development. Inter- syndrome or Down syndrome. Developmental Neu-
national Journal of Language and Communication ropsychology, 16, 177–196.
Disorders, 38, 179–197. Kneips, L. J., Walden, T. A., & Baxter, A. (1994). Affec-
Jarrold, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Short-term memory tive expressions of toddlers with and without Down
for verbal and visuospatial information in Down’s syn- syndrome in a social referencing context. American
drome. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 101–122. Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 301–312.
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (1999). Ge- Landry, S. H., & Chapieski, M. L. (1990). Joint atten-
netically dissociated components of working memory: tion and infant toy exploration: Effects of Down syn-
Evidence from Down’s and Williams syndrome. Neu- drome and prematurity. Child Development, 60, 103–
ropsychologia, 37, 637–651. 118.
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Phillips, C. E. (2002). Verbal Laws, G. (1998). The use of nonword repetition as a test
short-term memory in Down syndrome: A problem of of phonological memory in children with Down syn-
memory, audition, or speech? Journal of Speech, Lan- drome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
guage, and Hearing Research, 45, 531–544. and Allied Disciplines, 39, 1119–1130.
Jobling, A. (1998). Motor development in school-aged Legerstee, M., Bowman, T. G., & Fels, S. (1992). People
children with Down syndrome: A longitudinal per- and objects affect the quality of vocalizations in in-
spective. International Journal of Disability, Devel- fants with Down syndrome. Early Development and
opment and Education, 45, 283–293. Parenting, 1, 149–156.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Crucial differences between Lloveras, R. B., & Fornells, M. G. (1998). The group: An
developmental cognitive neuroscience and adult neu- instrument of intervention for the global development
ropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, of the child with Down syndrome in the process of
513–524. social inclusion. Down Syndrome: Research and
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key Practice, 5, 88–92.
to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Lorenz, S., Sloper, T., & Cunningham, C. (1985). Reading
Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389–398. and Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Special Ed-
Karrer, J. H., Karrer, R., Bloom, D., Chaney, L., & Davis, ucation, 12, 65–67.
R. (1998). Event-related brain potentials during an ex- Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., & Buder, E. H.
tended visual recognition memory task depict delayed (1995). Phrasing in prelinguistic vocalizations. Devel-
development of cerebral inhibitory processes among opmental Psychobiology, 28, 3–25.
6-month-old infants with Down syndrome. Interna- Lynch, M. P., Oller, D. K., Steffens, M. L., Levine, S. L.,
tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 29, 167–200. Basinger, D. L., & Umbel, V. M. (1995). Development
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

102 INFANTS & YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2005

of speech-like vocalizations in infants with Down syn- Down syndrome (pp. 45–58). Seattle: Special Child
drome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, Publications.
100, 68–86. Oelwein, P. (1988). Preschool and kindergarten pro-
Mahoney, G., Glover, A., & Finger, I. (1981). The rela- grams: Strategies for meeting objectives. In V. Dmitriev
tionship between language and sensorimotor devel- & P. L. Oelwein (Eds.), Advances in Down syndrome
opment among Down syndrome and developmentally (pp. 131–158). Seattle, WA: Special Child Publications.
normal children. American Journal of Mental Defi- Oelwein, P. L. (1995). Teaching reading to children with
ciency, 86, 21–27. Down syndrome. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House.
Mahoney, G., Robinson, C., & Fewell, R. R. (2001). The Oelwein, P. L., Fewell, R. R., & Pruess, J. B. (1985). The ef-
effects of early motor intervention on children with ficacy of intervention at outreach sites of the program
Down syndrome or cerebral palsy: A field-based study. for children with Down syndrome and other develop-
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 153– mental delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Ed-
162. ucation, 5, 78–87.
Miller, J. F. (1999). Profiles of language development in Ohr, P. S., & Fagen, J. W. (1994). Contingency learning in
children with Down syndrome. In J. Miller, M. Leddy, 9-month-old infants with Down syndrome. American
& L. A. Leavitt (Eds.), Improving the communication Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 74–84.
of people with Down syndrome (pp. 11–40). Balti- Oller, D. K., & Seibert, J. M. (1988). Babbling of prelin-
more: Brookes Publishing. guistic mentally retarded children. American Journal
Miller, J. F., & Leddy, M. (1999). Verbal fluency, speech on Mental Retardation, 92, 369–375.
intelligibility, and communicative effectiveness. In J. F. Osaki, D., Roger, S. J., & Hall, T. (2000). The Denver Model
Miller, M. Leddy, & L. A. Leavitt (Eds.), Improving the curriculum. Denver: JFK Partners.
communication of people with Down syndrome (pp. Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsoedl, M. K., Johnson, M.
81–91). H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Cognitive modularity
Miranda, S. B., & Fantz, R. L. (1973). Visual preferences of and genetic disorders. Science, 286, 2355–2358.
Down’s syndrome and normal infants. Child Develop- Pennington, B. F., Moon, J., Edgin, J., Stedron, J., & Nadel,
ment, 44, 555–561. L. (2003). The neuropsychology of Down syndrome:
Mon-Williams, M., Tresilian, J. R., Bell, V. E., Coppard, V. Evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Devel-
L., Jobling, A., & Carson, R. G. (2001). The preparation opment, 74, 75–93.
of reach to grasp movements in adults with Down syn- Pitcairn, T. K., & Wishart, J. G. (1994). Reactions of young
drome. Human Movement Science, 20, 587–602. children with Down’s syndrome to an impossible task.
Morris, C. A., Demsey, S. A., Leonard, C. O., Dilts, C., & British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12,
Blackburn, B. L. (1988). Natural history of Williams 485–489.
syndrome: Physical characteristics. Journal of Pedi- Poulson, C. L. (1988). Operant conditioning of vocaliza-
atrics, 113, 318–326. tion rate of infants with Down syndrome. American
Morss, J. R. (1983). Cognitive development in the Down’s Journal on Mental Retardation, 91, 57–63.
syndrome infant: Slow or different? British Journal of Pueschel, S. R., Gallagher, P. L., Zartler, A. S., & Pezzullo, J.
Educational Psychology, 53, 40–47. C. (1987). Cognitive and learning profiles in children
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental
(AGS Edition ed.). Circle Pines: American Guidance Disabilities, 8, 21–37.
Service. Pueschel, S. R., & Pueschel, J. K. (1992). Biomedical con-
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Yirmiya, N. (1988). cerns in persons with Down syndrome. Baltimore:
Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome Paul H. Brookes.
children. Child Development, 59, 235–249. Rodgers, C. (1987). Maternal support for the Down’s
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1995). syndrome stereotype: The effect of direct experience
Nonverbal communication and early language acqui- of the condition. Journal of Mental Deficiency Re-
sition in children with Down syndrome and normally search, 31, 217–278.
developing children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Rondal, J. (1980). Verbal imitation of Down syndrome and
Research, 38, 157–167. nonretarded children. American Journal of Mental
Nadel, L. (1996). Learning, memory and neural function Deficiency, 85, 318–321.
in Down’s syndrome. In J. A. Rondal, J. Perera, L. Rothbart, M. K., & Hanson, M. J. (1983). A caregiver re-
Nadel, & A. Comblain (Eds.), Down’s syndrome: Psy- port comparison of temperamental characteristics of
chological, psychobiological, and socio-educational Down syndrome and normal infants. Developmental
perspectives (pp. 21–42). London: Whurr Publishers. Psychology, 19, 766–769.
Nilholm, C. (1996). Early intervention with children with Ruskin, E. M., Kasari, C., Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1994).
Down syndrome: Past and future issues. Down Syn- Attention to people and toys during social and object
drome: Research and Practice, 4, 51–58. mastery in children with Down syndrome. American
Niva, R. A. (1988). Eye abnormalities and their treatment. Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 103–111.
In V. Dimitirev & P. L. Oelwein (Eds.), Advances in Sanz, M. T. (1988). Effect of observational training of
LWW/IYC lwwj057-03 March 9, 2005 14:26 Char Count= 0

Early Development in Down Syndrome 103

parents in the early stimulation of trisomy-21 babies. Vaughn, B. E., Contreras, J., & Seifer, R. (1994). Short-term
Early Child Development and Care, 41, 89–101. longitudinal study of maternal ratings of temperament
Sanz, M. T. (1996). A comparison of vicarious and writ- in samples of children with Down syndrome and chil-
ten training techniques applied to early stimulation by dren who are developing normally. American Journal
parents of their Down syndrome babies. Early Child on Mental Retardation, 98, 607–618.
Development and Care, 126, 111–119. Vaughn, B. E., Goldberg, S., Atkinson, L., Marcovitch, S.,
Sanz, M. T., & Balana, J. M. (2002). Early language stim- MacGregor, D., & Seifer, R. (1994). Quality of toddler–
ulation of Down’s syndrome babies: A study on the mother attachment in children with Down syndrome:
optimum age to begin. Early Child Development and Limits to interpretation of strange situation behavior.
Care, 172, 651–656. Child Development, 65, 95–108.
Sanz, M. T., & Menendez, J. (1995). A study of the effect Vlachou, M., & Farrell, P. (2000). Object mastery motiva-
of age of onset of treatment on the observed develop- tion in pre-school children with and without disabili-
ment of Down’s syndrome babies. Early Child Devel- ties. Educational Psychology, 20, 167–176.
opment and Care, 93–101. Walden, T., Knieps, L., & Baxter, A. (1991). Contingent
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in provision of social referential info by parents of chil-
the social competence of children with autism, Down dren with and without developmental delays. Ameri-
syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of can Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, 177–187.
the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, Wang, P. P., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two
v-114. genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal
Smith, B. L., & Oller, D. K. (1981). A comparative study and visuo-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clin-
of pre-meaningful vocalizations produced by normally ical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317–
developing and Down’s syndrome infants. Journal of 322.
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46, 46–51. Wang, X., Bernas, R., & Eberhard, P. (2001). Effects of
Smith, B. L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1996). A quantitative teachers’ verbal and nonverbal scaffolding on every-
analysis of the reduplicated and variegated babbling day classroom performances of students with Down
in vocalizations by Down syndrome infants. Clinical syndrome. International Journal of Early Years Ed-
Linguistics and Phonetics, 10, 119–130. ucation, 9, 71–80.
Spiker, D. (1990). Early intervention from a developmen- Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G., & Bryan, A. A. (1989).
tal perspective. In D. Ciccehtti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Communicative profiles of preschool children with
Children with Down syndrome: A developmental handicaps: Implications for early identification. Jour-
perspective (pp. 424–448). New York: Cambridge Uni- nal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 148–
versity Press. 158.
Steffens, M. L., Oller, D. K., Lynch, M., & Urbano, R. Wishart, J. G. (1993). The development of learning diffi-
C. (1992). Vocal development in infants with Down culties in children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of
syndrome and infants who are developing normally. Intellectual Disability Research, 37, 389–403.
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97, 235– Wishart, J. G., & Duffy, L. (1990). Instability of perfor-
246. mance on cognitive tests in infants and young chil-
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Down syndrome phonology: dren with Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Ed-
Developmental patterns and intervention strategies. ucational Psychology, 60, 10–22.
Down Syndrome: Research and Practice, 7, 93–100. Wishart, J. G., & Johnston, F. H. (1990). The effects of ex-
Stoll, C., Alembik, Y., Dott, B., & Roth, M. P. (1990). Epi- perience on attribution of a stereotyped personality
demiology of Down syndrome in 118,625 consecu- to children with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Men-
tive births. American Journal of Medical Genetics— tal Deficiency Research, 34, 409–420.
Supplement, 7, 79–83. Woodhouse, J. M., Pakeman, V. H., Saunders, K. J., Parker,
Torres, C., & Buceta, J. (1998). Effect of parental inter- M., Fraser, W. I., Lobo, S., et al. (1996). Visual acu-
vention on motor development of Down syndrome in- ity and accommodation in infants and young children
fants between birth and age two years. British Journal with Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Dis-
of Developmental Disabilities, 44, 94–101. ability Research, 40, 49–55.

Current articles- http://depts.washington.edu/isei/iyc/iyc_comments.html

Back to the list of previous articles:


http://depts.washington.edu/isei/iyc/iyc_previous.html

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen