Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Child Development, January/February 2000, Volume 71, Number 1, Pages 107Ð118

An Integrated Model of Emotion Processes and Cognition


in Social Information Processing
Elizabeth A. Lemerise and William F. Arsenio

Literature on the contributions of social cognitive and emotion processes to childrenÕs social competence is
reviewed and interpreted in the context of an integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social
information processing. Neurophysiological and functional evidence for the centrality of emotion processes in
personalÐsocial decision making is reviewed. Crick and DodgeÕs model is presented as a cognitive model of so-
cial decision making, and a revised model is proposed into which emotion processes are integrated. Hypothe-
ses derived from the proposed model are described.

INTRODUCTION the same coin,Ó Cowan, 1981, p. xiv), where affect


plays a role Ò. . . like gasoline, which activates the
The last two decades have witnessed rapid progress
motor of an automobile but does not modify its struc-
in research and theory regarding the contributions of
tureÓ (Piaget, 1981, p. 5). Although this book sparked
social information processing (e.g., Crick & Dodge,
considerable interest (Bearison & Zimiles, 1986; Brown,
1994; Dodge, 1986) and emotionality and regulation
1996; Cowan, 1981), the analogy of affect as gasoline
(e.g., Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992; Eisenberg et al., 1997;
and intelligence as the motor of human functioning
Hubbard & Coie, 1994; Saarni, 1999) to childrenÕs so-
reßects a key limitation found in various attempts to
cial competence. Although both research traditions
include emotion within cognitive models of develop-
share a focus on social competence, integration across
ment. For example, both Piaget (1981) and Crick and
the domains of social information processing and
Dodge (1994) acknowledge the central importance of
emotionality and regulation has been minimal. We
emotion, but provide a detailed and elaborated de-
argue here that (1) it is vitally important for develop-
scription of cognition without providing a compara-
mental psychologists to take a broader view of chil-
bly differentiated account of emotions.
drenÕs social and cognitive development, and (2) an
In this paper, we deÞne the domain of emotion
essential aspect of this broader view involves consid-
broadly to include processes that vary in duration
ering, both theoretically and empirically, how emo-
from brießy experienced feelings resulting from con-
tional and cognitive processes can be integrated in
scious or unconscious appraisal to more enduring
models of social competence.
affective styles (see, e.g., Ekman & Davidson, 1994;
Oatley & Jenkins, 1996). Therefore, we deliberately
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND AND OVERVIEW use the term emotion processes. Delineating the differ-
ence between emotion and cognition is a difÞcult task
There is a long-standing theoretical interest in under-
for which there is little consensus. To some extent,
standing the multiple ways affect and cognition inter-
whether one sees emotion and cognition as separate
act. Cognitive psychologists have been interested in
or as one process depends on how broadly cognition
the critical role of emotions in prioritizing and weight-
is deÞned (Dodge, 1991; Ekman & Davidson, 1994).
ing multiple goals and objectives in real-time human
Moreover, both emotion and cognitive processes de-
information processing (e.g., Simon, 1967). Attach-
velop, and the nature of their relations changes (e.g.,
ment and emotion researchers have been interested in
Denham, 1998; Saarni, 1999).
working models (e.g., Bowlby, 1988; Bretherton, Ridge-
At a very general level, both emotion and cogni-
way, & Cassidy, 1990) and emotion Ð event represen-
tion are types of information processing, but the func-
tations (e.g., Arsenio & Lover, 1995) of affectively
tions they serve are distinct. Izard (1994, p. 204) stated
charged relationships and events. Despite this inter-
ÒEmotion is about motivation, cognition about knowl-
est, and a journal devoted to the topic (Cognition and
edge.Ó This view is shared by functionalist theorists,
Emotion), progress has been slower than expected.
neurophysiologists, and some cognitive theorists who
An early reßection of the interest and difÞculties in
see the function of emotion as alerting individuals to
integrating emotion and cognition was PiagetÕs (1981)
Intelligence and Affectivity. Piaget described affect and © 2000 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
cognition as being indissociably linked (Òtwo sides of All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2000/7101-0013
108 Child Development

important features of the environment and providing hypothesized to occur relatively rapidly and in par-
direction for cognitive processes and behavior in ways allel, with numerous feedback loops (see Figure 1),
that are mostly adaptive (e.g., Campos, Mumme, Ker- but for clarity, we describe them sequentially (see
moian, & Campos, 1994; Damasio, 1994; Oatley & Jen- Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1986, 1991, for more
kins, 1996). Recent neurophysiological evidence (e.g., details).
LeDoux, 1995) suggests that emotion processes and Brießy, social information processing begins when
cognitive processes inßuence one another, making it the child attends to, encodes, and interprets social
difÞcult to isolate cases of pure emotion or pure cog- cues (steps one and two in the model). Imagine a
nition, except where brain damage has severed this child who gets hit by a ball while walking across the
connection (e.g., Damasio, 1994, and see below). We playground. The child must Þgure out what hap-
argue that emotion processes serve motivational, com- pened (attention, encoding) and why it happened (in-
municative, and regulatory functions within and be- terpretation: an accident or on purpose?). In the third
tween individuals that are distinct from the contribu- step of the model, the childÕs goals for the situation
tions of cognitive processes (attention, learning, are clariÞed. Possible goals might be to maintain
memory, logic) to social competence. Regardless of friendly relations with classmates, or to show others
how broadly or narrowly cognition is deÞned, in our you wonÕt tolerate this intrusion of your personal
view the inclusion of emotion processes in models of space. In steps four and Þve of the model, possible re-
personal Ð social decision making will expand their sponses to the situation are generated and evaluated
explanatory power. in terms of anticipated outcomes, relations to goal(s),
Our primary goal is to offer a model of social infor- and self-efÞcacy for performing the response. The
mation processing that (1) brings together affective child may consider retaliating in kind, but rejects that
and cognitive contributions without distorting or min- alternative because of a fear the conßict will escalate
imizing the literatures from which these contributions or a recognition that he/she canÕt throw the ball
are drawn, and (2) is theoretically coherent enough to hard enough. Finally, the most positively evaluated
spur empirical examination of many of these proposed response with respect to goals, anticipated out-
connections. To accomplish this goal, three basic top- comes, and self-efÞcacy is selected and behaviorally
ics will be covered. First, a brief overview is presented enacted (Crick & Dodge, 1994).
of Crick and DodgeÕs (1994) information processing Although Crick and Dodge explicitly assert that
model of social competence. The model was chosen emotion is an important component of social infor-
both because it is well articulated and has received mation processing, they also acknowledge that emo-
considerable empirical support, and because Crick tionÕs role is not well articulated in their model. In
and Dodge themselves acknowledge the need to con- what follows, we describe the kinds of emotion pro-
sider emotion-related processes more fully. Next, cesses that need to be integrated into cognitive mod-
there is a brief summary of the types of emotion pro- els of behavior. Finally, we illustrate how emotion
cesses that are relevant to social competence and are processes can be integrated into Crick and DodgeÕs
likely to be inßuential in social information process- (1994) model.
ing. The Þnal section illustrates the speciÞc ways
emotion processes can be integrated into a social in-
EMOTION PROCESSES
formation processing model.
Some aspects of emotional functioning apply more
generally, whereas others vary across individuals. Ac-
SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING
cordingly, we begin by describing normative emotion
A basic premise of social information processing (and processes and then consider individual variations.
of other social cognitive models, e.g., Piaget 1932/1965;
Turiel, 1998) is that childrenÕs understanding and in-
Normative Aspects of Emotions
terpretation of situations inßuences their related be-
haviors. Social information processing theory offers a Functionalist theories. Functionalist theories of emo-
detailed model of how children process and interpret tion emphasize the biologically adaptive role of
cues in a social situation and arrive at a decision that emotions in person/environment transactions (Laz-
is more or less competent (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, arus, 1991; Malatesta, 1990). Although there are impor-
1986). Crick and Dodge assume children enter a social tant differences in various versions of these theories
situation with past experiences and biologically de- (Saarni, Mumme, & Campos, 1998), many emphasize
termined capabilities which they may access during both the innate communicative and motivational as-
the encounter. The processing steps they describe are pects of emotion. Some theories (Ekman 1984, 1993;
Lemerise and Arsenio 109

Figure 1 Crick and DodgeÕs social information processing model of childrenÕs social adjustment. Note: From ÒA review and re-
formulation of social-information-processing mechanisms of childrenÕs social adjustment,Ó by N. R. Crick & K. A. Dodge (1994),
Psychological Bulletin, 115, p. 74 . Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission.

Izard, 1977, 1991; Plutchik, 1980) propose a limited emergence of emotion display rules (Saarni, 1999)
number of discrete emotions that are associated with makes these links less transparent.
more or less universal emotion expressions and re- Emotions also are seen as having an intrapsycho-
lated recognition abilities. These emotions are seen as logical function of organizing and motivating behav-
serving a vital interpsychological function by provid- iors and cognition to facilitate adaptive goal-directed
ing information about intended or likely behaviors behaviors. Given the numerous reviews available on
and underlying mental states of others, although the such theories (e.g., Magai & McFadden, 1995; Mala-
110 Child Development

testa, 1990; Saarni et al., 1998), little more will be said sion, Damasio (1994), Brown (1996), Oatley and Jen-
other than although there is a biologically based sub- kins (1996), and others have argued that emotions not
strate of adaptive emotional functioning, learning, only help to prioritize among different plans and op-
experience, and the active socialization of emotions tions; they also help to narrow the search space dur-
(Denham, 1998) all signiÞcantly inßuence this biolog- ing Òon-lineÓ information processing. In a summary
ical substrate (Malatesta, 1990). Similarly, emotions of PughÕs (1977) inßuential book, Brown (1996, p.
may be adaptive in general, but biological and social 154) noted that Òrigorously logical decisions are im-
stressors can distort emotional functioning, resulting possible in most of the situations adapting organ-
in psychopathology and the disruptive picture of emo- isms face. . . . affectivity provides a method for in-
tions typically emphasized in clinical psychology venting provisional or Ôgood enoughÕ knowledge
(Malatesta & Wilson, 1988; Plutchik, 1993). structures.Ó
Neurophysiology and functionalist theories. The func- A mechanism for emotional prioritizing is the for-
tionalist focus on the adaptive role of emotions is re- mation of somatic markers (Damasio, 1994). Brießy,
ceiving increasing attention and support from neuro- when a negative outcome becomes linked with a spe-
physiologists. Work by LeDoux (1993, 1995) and others ciÞc behavior or cognition, a negative somatic or gut
(e.g., Kandel & Kupferman, 1995) has documented feeling is experienced which Ò. . . protects you against
the complex interconnections between parts of the brain future losses, . . . and then allows you to choose from
that address emotional responsiveness and a variety fewer alternativesÓ (Damasio, 1994, p. 173). Once the
of cognitive functions. The Damasios, in particular, Þeld of alternatives has been narrowed, the individ-
have documented how disruptions in the connections ual can still use higher-level cognitive processes, but
between emotional and rational substrates of the brain with greater efÞciency. Damasio acknowledges that
can disrupt competent human functioning (Damasio, somatic markers are a form of bias that can contribute
1994; Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, Galaburda, & Da- to maladaptive behavior (see below), but for the most
masio, 1994). They acknowledge that emotions can part, somatic markers support adaptive behavior.
sometimes disrupt reason but also note that ÒReduc- Secondary emotions/affect-event links. Damasio
tion in emotion may constitute an equally important (1994) made an important distinction between primary
source of irrational behaviorÓ (Damasio, 1994, p. 53). and secondary emotions. Primary emotions are innate,
The Damasios have examined the consequences preorganized, and relatively inßexible responses to
for decision making of damage to ventromedial pre- certain combinations of stimuli. They are the sorts of
frontal cortex that leaves patients with motor and emotions described by functionalists where certain
cognitive functions intact but with impaired emo- classes of events (e.g., loss of a loved one, being at-
tional functioning. Unlike participants with no brain tacked by a wild animal) are likely to be linked with
damage and those with other kinds of brain damage, speciÞc emotions and their associated motivational
when patients with prefrontal lesions were shown tendencies. Although these primary emotions pro-
disturbing images, they showed no skin conductance vide a quick and efÞcient set of responses, Òthe next
response. These patients could report the content of step is the feeling of emotion in connection to the object
the slides and were aware of the emotional signiÞ- that excited itÓ (i.e., secondary emotion, Damasio, 1994,
cance of the images, but they did not feel the emotions p. 132; see also Piaget, 1981).
they knew they should (Damasio, 1994; Damasio, Awareness of the connections between object X and
Tranel, & Damasio, 1991). The practical effects of this certain emotions serves several adaptive purposes
deÞcit are seen most clearly for decision making where (Damasio, 1994). For example, knowing about the con-
outcomes are uncertain or for which there is no clear nection between seeing a large animal running nearby
correct answer (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Ander- and fear allows one to plan ahead (ÒWhere and when
son, 1994; Bechara, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1993). did I see it?Ó), and to make more Þne-tuned discrimi-
In one telling example, a patient who was asked to nations (ÒDoes it run toward me or away?Ó). Thus,
choose between two dates for his next appointment the more automatic eventÐemotion connections can
spent over thirty minutes calmly listing pros and cons be loosened somewhat, and conscious understanding
for each date until it was suggested that he come on of eventÐemotion links provides adaptive behavioral
the second date (Damasio, 1994). ßexibility. Damasio sees experience as important in
Emotional prioritizing and somatic markers. A criti- Þne-tuning secondary emotions, with adaptive func-
cal issue in the interaction of affect and cognition in- tioning the product of a normal brain and a normal
volves how the nearly unlimited considerations that set of experiences. Brain damage and/or experience
enter into any choice are reduced to a manageable that inßuences secondary emotions, however, can
number. Extrapolating from SimonÕs (1967) discus- produce maladaptive functioning.
Lemerise and Arsenio 111

Individual Variations in Emotional Functioning that may even alter the underlying physiology of
emotional reactivity (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997),
Although functionalists stress the normative, adap- it seems important to distinguish between the more
tive role emotions serve, there is a growing interest in innate aspects of emotionality and attempts to manage/
individual variations in affective systems (Thompson, regulate whatever those biological substrates provide.
1994). Just as psychologists have realized that basic For example, in their extensive, ongoing research on
similarities and constraints on human cognition do childrenÕs social and emotional competence, Eisen-
not preclude meaningful individual differences in berg and colleagues include separate assessments of
cognitive functioning, emotion theorists have begun emotionality and emotion regulation. Emotions may
to expand their understanding of emotions to include be regulated with attentional mechanisms, through
individual differences. Two somewhat separable lines approach or avoidance, or by cognitive or behavioral
of research focus on temperament/emotionality and coping (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992). Eisenberg and col-
the regulation of emotions. leagues found that a combination of regulatory abili-
Temperament/emotionality. Rothbart and Bates (1998, ties and low emotionality predicted social compe-
p. 109) deÞne temperament as Òconstitutionally based tence concurrently and longitudinally (e.g., Eisenberg,
individual differences in emotional, motor, and atten- Fabes, Nyman, Bernzweig, & Pinuelas, 1994; Eisen-
tional reactivity and self-regulation. . . . Reactive pa- berg et al., 1997). High emotionality combined with
rameters of temperament can be measured in terms poor regulation skills predicted poorer social func-
of the onset, duration, and intensity of expression tioning and problem behaviors, whereas children
of affective reaction. . . . variability in arousability, with high emotionality and good regulation skills
and distress to overstimulation, activity and atten- were not at risk for behavior problems (Eisenberg et
tion.Ó Although temperament includes more than al., 1996).
emotions, variations in emotionality, which are at least Mood state/background emotions. Mood states (Clark &
partially biologically based, are central to modern Isen, 1981) or background emotions (Damasio, 1994)
conceptualizations of temperament. also are likely to inßuence social information process-
Eisenberg and colleagues have conducted a sys- ing. There is an extensive social psychology literature
tematic program of research focusing on the contribu- on how experimentally induced moods inßuence a
tions of emotionality and emotion regulation to social wide variety of behaviors and cognitions (e.g., Berko-
competence. They deÞne emotionality as Òstable indi- witz, 1990; Isen, Shalker, Clark, & Karp, 1978; Moore,
vidual differences in the typical intensity with which Underwood, & Rosenhan, 1984). More recently, Lemer-
individuals experience their emotions . . . and in ise, Harper, Caverly, and Hobgood (1998) found that
threshold to relatively intense levels of emotional re- induced moods inßuence childrenÕs goals for hypo-
spondingÓ (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992, p. 122) and in- thetical provocation situations.
clude both positive and negative emotionality in this Little is known about how or whether ongoing
temperament-based deÞnition. Eisenberg and col- emotions or moods serve an adaptive role or whether
leagues developed methods to measure emotionality they are ÒunintendedÓ consequences of the larger
and found individual differences to be reasonably functional organization of human emotions. Another
stable from preschool to elementary school (Eisenberg unresolved issue is whether ongoing emotions act as
et al., 1997). EmotionalityÕs impact on social function- short-term inßuences or in more long-term, trait-like
ing, however, depends on skill at regulating emotion. ways (e.g., Arsenio, Cooperman, & Lover, in press; Ar-
Emotion regulation. Along with the focus on tem- senio & Lover, 1997; Denham, 1986; Denham, McKin-
peramental aspects of emotionality, there has been ley, Couchoud, & Holt, 1990). Regardless of their pre-
much interest in childrenÕs abilities to regulate emo- sumed functions or duration, it is clear that more
tions, that is, to control, modify, and manage aspects empirical attention should be directed to the inßuence
of their emotional reactivity and expressivity (e.g., of ongoing emotions on social information processing.
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992; Thompson, 1990, 1994). It
should be noted that children who vary in tempera-
INTEGRATING EMOTION PROCESSES
ment face quite different tasks in regulating their
AND SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING
emotions (Thompson, 1990, 1994). A child with a pos-
itive, even-tempered disposition has a very different An important way in which thinking about people is
set of regulatory tasks than one prone to intense, different from thinking about things is that social in-
long-lasting negative emotions. teraction is likely to be associated with a variety of
Although the more innate aspects of emotionality strong emotions (Hoffman, 1981). The peer entry and
clearly interact with socialization patterns in ways provocation situations examined by social informa-
112 Child Development

tion processing theory (e.g., being excluded from play Figure 2) which may not necessarily be related to that
or teased in front of others) are especially likely to be situation. For example, feelings of anxiety in a peer
emotionally arousing for children. Moreover, in these entry situation may be related to the terrible Þght your
situations, children are unlikely to know all relevant parents had this morning or to past experiences of re-
information which would assist problem solving, and buff by peers. Children also differ in their skill at reg-
the outcomes of these situations are uncertain. Despite ulating arousal or mood (Eisenberg et al., 1997). Be-
these limitations, the child still needs to do something. cause children who are poor regulators have a higher
Emotion theorists have argued that conditions of risk for maladjustment (Eisenberg et al., 1996), we
uncertainty and incomplete knowledge are the very hypothesize that poor regulators also will show social
contexts in which emotion processes can play an adap- information processing deÞcits.
tive (or maladaptive) role by reducing information
processing demands so the individual can arrive at
Encoding and Interpreting Cues
some course of action (e.g., Damasio, 1994; Oatley &
Jenkins, 1996). Crick and Dodge (1994) also acknowl- The Þrst two steps of the Crick and Dodge (1994)
edge that emotion processes play important roles in model involve encoding and interpreting social cues.
social information processing, but suggest that the Crick and Dodge point out that oneÕs own internal
cognitive nature of their model may prevent a full ex- emotion cues must be encoded and interpreted along
amination of the contribution of emotion. We argue with other situational cues. They also suggest that the
that it is possible to integrate emotion processes into process of encoding and interpreting cues can result
Crick and DodgeÕs model (see Figure 2), and the inte- in a change in the discrete emotion experienced or in
gration of emotion and social information processing the intensity of a pre-existing emotion (see Crick &
expands the modelÕs explanatory power. Ladd, 1993).
We agree that the child enters a given social situa- We would add that othersÕ affective cues are an im-
tion with a combination of Òbiologically limited capa- portant source of information and must be encoded
bilities and a database of memories of past experi- and interpreted (Saarni, 1999). For example, provoca-
encesÓ (Crick & Dodge, 1994, p. 76), and that selective teursÕ anger cues in the context of ambiguous provo-
attention, perception, memory, and processing speed cation facilitate hostile attributions (Lemerise, Gregory,
set limits on what children notice and process about a Leitner, & Hobgood, 1999; see also Dodge & Somberg,
situation. In addition, we suggest that emotion pro- 1987). Functionalist theories of emotion stress the mu-
cesses are part of these givens. An important compo- tual regulatory function of affective signals (Magai &
nent of the childÕs biological predispositions is his/ McFadden, 1995). OneÕs own and othersÕ affective
her emotion style or emotionality (Eisenberg & Fabes, signals provide ongoing information about how the
1992; Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). Children vary in encounter is proceeding, allowing for sensitive ad-
the intensity with which they experience and express justments to behavior. The mutual regulatory func-
emotions and in their skills for regulating emotions. tion of affective signals may work especially well
These individual differences in emotionality and reg- with familiar, well-liked interaction partners (at Þrst
ulatory abilities are related to social competence (e.g., parents, and later friends) whose signals may be easier
Eisenberg et al., 1997). We hypothesize that emotion- to read and interpret and with whom the child is
ality and regulatory ability will affect both processing more likely to feel empathy (Hoffman, 1981; Parker &
of social (and emotional) information and decision Gottman, 1989). The nature of emotional ties with an
making in challenging social situations (see Figure 2). interaction partner also may inßuence encoding and
In Crick and DodgeÕs (1994) model of social infor- interpretation. Being teased by a friend is quite differ-
mation processing, childrenÕs database includes a mem- ent from being teased by the class bully. Children
ory store of acquired rules, social knowledge, and social with conduct disorder have difÞculty reading their
schemas. Based on current models of the neurophys- own and othersÕ affective signals and have deÞcits
iology of emotion and cognition (Damasio, 1994; in their expressive behavior (Casey, 1996; Casey &
LeDoux, 1995), we argue that the childÕs representa- Schlosser, 1994) and in empathy (Cohen & Strayer,
tions of past experience include affective as well as cog- 1996). These deÞcits are thought to contribute to be-
nitive components, similar to what Arsenio and Lover havior problems.
(1995, p. 90) termed ÒaffectÐevent linksÓ (see Figure 2). Finally, encoding and interpretation can be inßu-
Thus, childrenÕs social knowledge can be cued by events enced by mood, level of arousal, or by discrete emo-
and/or by emotion cues, and events may cue emotions. tions (whether pre-existing or the result of appraisal).
Finally, children enter a social situation with a gen- Mood, emotions, and/or arousal can affect what is
eral level of physiological arousal and/or mood (see noticed about a social encounter and make the recol-
Lemerise and Arsenio 113

Figure 2 An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Items marked with Þlled circles
are from Crick and DodgeÕs model; those marked with Þlled diamonds represent emotion processes added to the model. Note: From
ÒA review and reformulation of social-information-processing mechanisms of childrenÕs social adjustment,Ó N. R. Crick & K. A.
Dodge, Psychological Bulletin, 115, p. 74. Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission.

lection of mood-congruent information more likely, Jenkins, 1996). The intensity with which children ex-
thus inßuencing interpretation of social cues. More- perience emotions and their skill at regulating emo-
over, the effects of a happy mood may be quite dis- tion also will inßuence what is noticed and the mean-
tinct from those of a sad or angry mood (Oatley & ing attributed to the situation.
114 Child Development

ClariÞcation of Goals partly because of the desire to maintain enjoyable


play activities (Arsenio & Lover, 1995; Parker &
Crick and Dodge (1994) consider the role of emotion
Gottman, 1989).
processes in a more explicit fashion for step 3 of their
model, clariÞcation of goals. They deÞne goals as
Òfocused arousal states that function toward produc- Response Generation, Evaluation, and Decision
ing (or wanting to produce) particular outcomesÓ
In steps 4 and 5 of the Crick and Dodge model,
(p. 87). Goals are conceptualized as either internal
children access possible responses to the situation
(maintaining or regulating emotion) or external (e.g.,
and evaluate these in terms of likely outcomes, goals,
instrumental goals, such as getting the swing, or so- and self-efÞcacy for performing the response; the most
cial relational goals, like getting another to play with positively evaluated response with respect to the childÕs
you). According to Crick and Dodge, emotions can goals is selected. They also suggest that childrenÕs ac-
act to energize particular goals. For example, being in cessing of responses could be inßuenced by an emo-
an angry mood makes it more likely that a child tion they are experiencing, and that accessing partic-
will focus on instrumental goals (Lemerise et al., ular responses may modify an emotion. Both of these
1998), whereas children in positive moods may possibilities are consistent with the idea that repre-
choose goals that maintain their pleasant moods. Fi- sentations of past experiences include an affective
nally, Crick and Dodge assert that goal selection and/ component (e.g., Damasio, 1994). Thus, feeling angry,
or attainment may modify mood or emotion. frightened, or happy may cue different response types.
We suggest that peersÕ affective cues also can inßu- Given that representations of these responses include
ence childrenÕs goals. Positive affective signals may an affective component, retrieval of particular strate-
promote afÞliative goals, whereas negative cues gies should cue certain emotions. For example, if ex-
may discourage afÞliation (Sroufe, Schork, Motti, periences of avoidance are associated with a reduction
Lawroski, & LaFreniere, 1984). Moreover, the intensity in anxiety, accessing avoidant responses may moderate
with which children experience emotions and their ef- feelings of fear.
Þcacy for regulating emotions will inßuence the types Other emotion processes that can inßuence access-
of goals pursued in social encounters (Eisenberg & ing and evaluation of responses are the intensity with
Fabes, 1992; Eisenberg et al., 1994; Saarni, 1999). Chil- which emotions are experienced and the capacity to
dren who are overwhelmed by their own and/or regulate emotion. Children who experience strong
othersÕ emotions may choose avoidant or hostile goals emotions may be too overwhelmed and self-focused
to reduce their own arousal. Children with deÞcits in to generate a variety of responses and evaluate them
affective cue detection and empathy may Þnd it easier from all partiesÕ perspectives. Such children may be
to pursue goals that are destructive to relationships likely to engage in Òpreemptive processingÓ (Crick &
because they literally do not Òfeel other childrenÕs Dodge, 1994) which can result in a response that is
painÓ (Cohen & Strayer, 1996). Poor regulatory abili- unlikely to further social interaction (e.g., running
ties may interfere with assessing the situation from away, angrily retaliating, or venting; see Eisenberg et
different cognitive and affective perspectives and al., 1994). Skill at regulating emotions in challenging
prevent a ßexible approach to goal selection which situations makes possible the more effortful process-
takes into account contextual factors (Saarni, 1999). In- ing involved in accessing and evaluating several re-
stead, childrenÕs goals may be rigid because they re- sponses. Good regulators may be more likely to con-
sult from Òpreemptive processingÓ (Costanzo & Dix, sider the situation from multiple cognitive and affective
1983; Crick & Dodge, 1994). perspectives which should facilitate selecting a more
Finally, the nature of the emotional ties between a competent response (Saarni, 1999).
child and others involved in an encounter may bias We agree with Crick and Dodge (1994) that chil-
goal selection. Social relational goals may be facili- drenÕs expectations about the emotional consequences
tated by friendship ties, whereas less positive rela- of various responses constitute an important compo-
tionships elicit quite different goals (e.g., avoidance, nent of the response evaluation process, and have
revenge). Social relational goals are both cognitively argued elsewhere that emotion expectations con-
and emotionally more complex because they require tribute importantly to socio-moral reasoning and
consideration and coordination of multiple cogni- behavior (Arsenio, 1988; Arsenio & Lover, 1995). Fi-
tive and affective perspectives. Friendship ties may nally, we add that childrenÕs emotional ties (or lack
motivate children to engage in this more effortful thereof) to others involved in the encounter and/or
processing, partly because of the negative emotional the reputations of the other children (Perry, Willard, &
consequences associated with hurting a friend and Perry, 1990) may motivate the child to engage in the
Lemerise and Arsenio 115

effortful processing involved in considering various deÞcits in social information processing (e.g., Murphy &
perspectives and response options. In other words, Eisenberg, 1997). Also, because emotions within the
a child may be more likely to consider anotherÕs re- person and in others function to mutually regulate
action if he/she cares about and wants that person encounters, we hypothesize that manipulating these
to like him/her. cues can affect each social information processing
step and that the effects of different discrete emotions
may be distinct. SpeciÞcally, manipulation of emotion
Response Enactment
or mood in the child via mood induction or situa-
In the Þnal step of Crick and DodgeÕs (1994) model, tional manipulations (see Lemerise & Dodge, 2000, for
the child enacts the chosen response. Several emotion- a review) will inßuence social information process-
related processes can inßuence response enactment. ing. Also, manipulation of emotion cues or the nature
The intensity with which emotions are experienced of emotional ties to targets (peers about whom judg-
and the childÕs regulatory capacities can inßuence re- ments are made) will inßuence social information
sponse enactment. For example, under calm condi- processing. Research on target effects on social infor-
tions, even maladjusted children are well aware of mation processing has been especially rare (Crick &
display rules for challenging situations (Underwood, Dodge, 1994). Given our hypothesis that emotionality
1997), but under highly arousing conditions, this and emotion regulation skills inßuence social infor-
knowledge may not be put into practice (Parker & mation processing, we hypothesize that the above de-
Hubbard, 1998). The ability to ßexibly display emo- scribed emotion manipulations will have a greater ef-
tions appropriate to the situation requires both con- fect on children who are high in emotionality and
trol over oneÕs expressivity and sensitivity to the sit- poor in regulatory abilities. In addition, we suggest
uation from multiple perspectives (Saarni, 1999). that there is a need to explore the emotion content of
Moreover, the childÕs and othersÕ emotion cues pro- childrenÕs database of knowledge about social situa-
vide an ongoing source of information concerning tions (e.g., Arsenio, 1988). Recent research has shown
how the encounter is proceeding, allowing the child that childrenÕs knowledge structures predict aggres-
to make adjustments to his/her response enactment. sive behavior (Burks, Laird, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates,
Children with deÞcits in reading and sending emo- 1999), but the emotion content of childrenÕs knowl-
tion cues may resort to relatively rigid approaches to edge structures has received little empirical attention.
situations (Casey, 1996; Casey & Schlosser, 1994; Saarni, Beyond these speciÞc hypotheses, we hope the
1999). Emotion cues also can inform the child about present model will encourage other systematic attempts
the ultimate success or failure of his/her response en- to integrate affect and cognition. These efforts should
actment. Finally, emotion cues are part of the repre- prove fruitful for a fuller understanding not only of chil-
sentation of the encounter stored in the childÕs data- drenÕs social competence, but also of other areas in
base of social knowledge. Consider a situation in which which motivation and reasoning combine in essential
a child wants to play with a peerÕs toy. An affectively ways, including socio-moral development, academic
positive offer to trade toys may be successful in that achievement, and developmental psychopathology.
the child not only gets the desired toy but both chil-
dren feel positive about the encounter and may even
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
play together. An angry demand that the peer hand
over the toy, however, may elicit anger and resistance, The contributions of the authors were equal; order
with the result that the child has neither the toy nor a of authorship was determined by a coin toss. Eliza-
playmate. In these examples, the affective cues that beth Lemerise was supported by a Western Ken-
were part of the childÕs enactment inßuenced the tucky University Faculty Summer Research Fellow-
peerÕs reaction and ultimately the outcome of the en- ship during the preparation of this article. We thank
counter, including associated emotions. K. Barnes, J. Bilotta, P. Garner, and J. Hubbard for
assistance.
FUTURE DIRECTIONS FOR RESEARCH
ADDRESSES AND AFFILIATIONS
A number of hypotheses can be derived from the pro-
posed model. At a general level, we hypothesize that Corresponding authors: Elizabeth A. Lemerise,
individual differences in emotionality and emotion Department of Psychology, Western Kentucky Univer-
regulation can inßuence each step of social informa- sity, 1 Big Red Way, Bowling Green, KY 42101; e-mail:
tion processing. SpeciÞcally, children who are high in elizabeth.lemerise@wku.edu; or to William F. Arsenio,
emotionality and poor at regulating emotion will show Ferkauf Graduate School of Psychology, Yeshiva Uni-
116 Child Development

versity, Rousso Building, 1300 Morris Park Avenue, Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1981). Toward understanding
Bronx, NY 10461; e-mail: warsenio@wesleyan.edu. the relationship between feeling states and social behav-
ior. In A. Hastorf & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psy-
chology (pp. 73Ð108). New York: Elsevier.
Cohen, D., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy in conduct-disordered
REFERENCES and comparison youth. Developmental Psychology, 32,
988Ð998.
Arsenio, W. (1988). ChildrenÕs conceptions of the affective Costanzo, P. R., & Dix, T. H. (1983). Beyond the information
consequences of socio-moral events. Child Development, processed: Socialization in the development of attribu-
59, 1611Ð1622. tional processes. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, & W. W.
Arsenio, W., Cooperman, S., & Lover, A. (in press). Affective Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A
predictors of preschoolersÕ aggression: and peer accep- sociocultural perspective (pp. 63Ð81). Cambridge, UK:
tance: Direct and indirect effects. Developmental Psychology. Cambridge University Press.
Arsenio, W., & Lover, A. (1995). ChildrenÕs conceptions of Cowan, P. (1981). Preface. In J. Piaget, Intelligence and affec-
sociomoral affect: Happy victimizers, mixed emotions, tivity: Their relationship during child development (pp. ixÐ
and other expectancies. In M. Killen & D. Hart (Eds.), xiv). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Morality in everyday life: Developmental perspectives (pp. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformula-
87Ð128). New York: Cambridge University Press. tion of social-information-processing mechanisms in chil-
Arsenio, W., & Lover, A. (1997). Emotions, conßicts and ag- drenÕs social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74Ð101.
gression during preschoolersÕ freeplay. British Journal of Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). ChildrenÕs perceptions of
Developmental Psychology, 15, 531Ð542. their peer experiences: Attributions, loneliness, social
Bearison, D., & Zimiles H. (Eds.). (1986). Thought and emo- anxiety, and social avoidance. Developmental Psychology,
tion: Developmental perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 29, 244Ð254.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. Damasio, A. R. (1994). DescartesÕ error: Emotion, reason, and
(1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following the human brain. New York: Avon Books.
damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7Ð12. Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. markers and the guidance of behavior: Theory and pre-
(1993). Failure to respond autonomically in anticipation liminary testing. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L.
of future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cor- Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp.
tex. Society for Neuroscience, 19, 791. 217Ð229). New York: Oxford University Press.
Berkowitz, J. (1990). On the formation and regulation of Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., &
anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: Clues
analysis. American Psychologist, 45, 494Ð503. about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Sci-
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and ence, 264, 1102Ð1105.
healthy human development. New York: Basic Books. Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior
Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation.
internal working models of the attachment relationship. Child Development, 57, 194Ð201.
In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & M. Greenberg (Eds.), Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young chil-
Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and in- dren. New York: Guilford.
tervention (pp. 273Ð310). Chicago: University of Chicago Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., & Holt, R.
Press. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of pre-
Brown, T. (1996). Values, knowledge, and Piaget. In E. Reed, school peer ratings. Child Development, 61, 1145Ð1152.
E. Turiel, & T. Brown (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model
137Ð170). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.),
Burks, V. S., Laird, R. D., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, Minnesota Symposium in Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77Ð
J. E. (1999). Knowledge structures, social information 125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
processing and childrenÕs aggressive behavior. Social De- Dodge, K. A. (1991). Emotion and social information pro-
velopment, 8(2), 220Ð236. cessing. In J. Garber & K. Dodge (Eds.), The development
Campos, J. J., Mumme, D. L., Kermoian, R., & Campos, of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 159Ð181). New
R. G. (1994). A functionalist perspective on the nature of York: Cambridge University Press.
emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. (1987). Hostile attributional
Development, 59(2Ð3, Serial No. 240), 284Ð303. biases are exacerbated under conditions of threat to the
Casey, R. J. (1996). Emotional competence in children with self. Child Development, 58, 213Ð224.
externalizing and internalizing disorders. In M. Lewis & Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion regulation
M. W. Sullivan (Eds.), Emotional development in atypical and the development of social competence. In M. S.
children (pp. 161Ð183). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior: Vol. 14. Review of
Casey, R. J., & Schlosser, S. (1994). Emotional responses to personality and social psychology (pp. 119Ð150). Newbury
peer praise in children with and without a diagnosed ex- Park, CA: Sage.
ternalizing disorder. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 60Ð81. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C.,
Lemerise and Arsenio 117

Maszk, P., Holmgren, R., & Suh, K. (1996). The relations (1999, April). The effect of affective cues on childrenÕs social
of regulation and emotionality to problem behavior in information processing. Poster session presented at the bi-
elementary school children. Development and Psychopa- ennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child De-
thology, 8, 141Ð162. velopment, Albuquerque, NM.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Nyman, M., Bernzweig, J., & Lemerise, E. A., Harper, B. D., Caverly, S., & Hobgood, C.
Pinuelas, A. (1994). The relations of emotionality and (1998, March). Mood, social goals, and childrenÕs outcome ex-
regulation to childrenÕs anger-related reactions. Child pectations. Poster session presented at the biennial Con-
Development, 65, 109Ð128. ference on Human Development, Mobile, AL.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphy, B. C., Magai, C., & McFadden, S. (1995). The role of emotions in so-
Guthrie, I. K., Jones, S., Friedman, J., Poulin, R., & cial and personality development. New York: Plenum Press.
Maszk, P. (1997). Contemporaneous and longitudinal Malatesta, C. (1990). The role of emotions in the develop-
prediction of childrenÕs social functioning from regula- ment and organization of personality. In R. Thompson
tion and emotionality. Child Development, 68, 642Ð664. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 36: Socio-
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotions. In emotional development (pp. 1Ð56). Lincoln: University of
K. Sherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. Nebraska Press.
329Ð343). New York: Prentice Hall. Malatesta, C., & Wilson, A. (1988). Emotion/cognition in-
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American teraction in personality development: A discrete emo-
Psychologist, 48, 384Ð392. tion, functionalist analysis. British Journal of Social Psy-
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emo- chology, 27, 91Ð112.
tion: Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford Univer- Moore, B., Underwood, B., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1984). Emo-
sity Press. tion, self, and others. In C. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. Zajonc
Gottman, J., Katz, L., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 464Ð483).
How families communicate emotionally. Mahwah, NJ: New York: Cambridge University Press.
Erlbaum. Murphy, B. C., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Young childrenÕs
Hoffman, M. L. (1981). Perspectives on the difference be- emotionality: Regulation and social functioning and
tween understanding people and understanding things: their responses when they are targets of a peerÕs anger.
The role of affect. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Eds.), Social Social Development, 6, 18Ð36.
cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures (pp. Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions.
67Ð81). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
Hubbard, J. A., & Coie, J. D. (1994). Emotional correlates Parker, E. H., & Hubbard, J. A. (1998, March). ChildrenÕs un-
of social competence in childrenÕs peer relationships. derstanding of emotion following hypothetical vignettes ver-
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 1Ð20. sus real-life peer interaction. Paper presented at the bien-
Isen, A., Shalker, T., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, ac- nial Conference on Human Development, Mobile, AL.
cessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cogni- Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional
tive loop. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, development in a relational context: Friendship inter-
1Ð12. action from early childhood to adolescence. In T. J.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child de-
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: velopment (pp. 95Ð131). New York: Wiley.
Plenum. Perry, D. G., Willard, J. C., & Perry, L. C. (1990). PeersÕ per-
Izard, C. E. (1994). Cognition is one of four types of emotion- ceptions of the consequences that victimized children
activating systems. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), provide aggressors. Child Development, 61, 1310Ð1325.
The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 203Ð Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgement of the child (M. Gabain,
207). New York: Oxford University Press. Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work published
Kandel, E. R., & Kupferman, I. (1995). Emotional states. In 1932)
E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Essen- Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship
tials of neural science and behavior (pp. 595Ð612). Norwalk, during child development (T. A. Brown & C. E. Kaegi,
CT: Appleton & Lange. Trans. & Eds.). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Ox- Plutchik, R (1980). Emotions: A psychoevolutionary synthesis.
ford University Press. New York: Harper & Row.
LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional networks in the brain. In M. Plutchik, R. (1993). Emotions and their vicissitudes: Emo-
Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. tions and psychopathology. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland
109Ð118). New York: Guilford. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 53 Ð 66). New York:
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Guilford.
Review of Psychology, 46, 209Ð235. Pugh, G. (1977). The biological origins of human values. New
Lemerise, E. A., & Dodge, K. A. (2000). The development of an- York: Basic Books.
ger and hostile interactions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland- Rothbart, M., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In N.
Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed, pp. 594Ð606). Eisenberg (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child
New York: Guilford. psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
Lemerise, E. A., Gregory, D., Leitner, K., & Hobgood, C. ment (5th ed., pp. 105Ð176). New York: Wiley.
118 Child Development

Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of tions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 289Ð319). New York:
individual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & Cambridge University Press.
A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology Thompson, R. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In R.
(Vol. 1, pp. 37Ð86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thompson (Ed.), R. Dienstbier (Series Ed.), Nebraska Sym-
Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. posium on Motivation: Vol. 36. Socioemotional development
New York: Guilford. (pp. 367Ð467). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Saarni, C., Mumme, D. L., & Campos, J. (1998). Emotional de- Thompson, R. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in
velopment: Action, communication, and understanding. search of deÞnition. Monographs of the Society for Research
In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of in Child Development, 59(2Ð3, Serial No. 240), 25Ð52.
child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality de- Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In N. Eisen-
velopment (5th ed., pp. 237Ð309). New York: Wiley. berg (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of chology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality develop-
cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29Ð39. ment (5th ed., pp. 863Ð932). New York: Wiley.
Sroufe, L. A., Schork, E., Motti, F., Lawroski, N., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). Peer social status and childrenÕs un-
LaFreniere, P. (1984). The role of affect in social compe- derstanding of the expression and control of positive and
tence. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emo- negative emotions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 610Ð634.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen