Sie sind auf Seite 1von 190

Reproductive strategies in Scandinavian brown bears

Bjørn Dahle

Dr. scient. thesis

Department of Biology
Norwegian University of Science and Technology

Trondheim 2003
© Bjørn Dahle 2003
© International Association for Bear Research and Management 2001
© American Society for Mammalogist 2003
© British Ecological Society 2003
© Zoological Society of London 2003
© Springer Verlag 2003

Authors address:

Bjørn Dahle
Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
Agricultural University of Norway
Post Box 5014
N-1432 Ås
Norway
E-mail: bjorn.dahle@ibn.nlh.no

“Descended from the apes! My dear, let us hope that it is not true, but if it is, let us pray that
it will not become generally known”; the wife of the Bishop of Worcester after she heard
about Charles Darwins “the Origin of Species”

“How extremely stupid not to have thought about that!”; Thomas Huxley after reading “the
Origin of Species”

ISBN

1
Preface
It could be argued that the brown bear is not the ideal model species for studying reproductive
strategies, because it occurs at low densities, matures late, gives birth to young only every
second or third year, and bears are notoriously expensive to handle and monitor. However,
brown bears exhibit behaviors restricted to certain groups of animals, such as sexually
selected infanticide, which make them suitable for studying reproductive strategies and
counterstrategies against sexually selected infanticide. Further, individual brown bears vary
greatly in important life history traits, leading to questions such as; Why do some females stay
with their young for one year and others for two or even three years? Why does the body mass
of weaned yearlings vary by a factor of five? And, what consequences might this have? For a
Darwinist it is important to answer such questions, as these phenomena have fitness
consequences and should be under strong selective pressures. My hope is that this thesis
contributes to our understanding of some aspects of maternal care and how space use may be
influenced by reproductive strategies and ecological factors. Although brown are a difficult
and expensive species to study, my research has given me nearly 100 observations of wild
brown bears, some from quite far away and some so close that I could almost touch them.
They are really fascinating creatures...

Rather than dedicate this thesis to my wife and children, I apologize for my ego trip, being
absent both physically and mentally when I wrote this thesis

2
Contents

Preface ............................................................................................................................... 2
List of papers .................................................................................................................... 4
Abstract ............................................................................................................................. 5
Sammendrag ..................................................................................................................... 8
Synopsis ............................................................................................................................. 11
Introduction …………………………………………………………………….... 11
Parental care ..........................................................……………………..... 11
Space use in relation to ecological factors and reproductive strategies ..... 14
Aims ....................................................................................................................... 16
Methods .................................................................................................................. 16
Study species .............................................................................................. 16
Study areas ................................................................................................. 17
General methods ......................................................................................... 17
Main results ............................................................................................................ 18
Offspring size ............................................................................................. 18
Length of maternal care; 1.5 or 2.5 years? ................................................. 18
Family breakup: are young forced to leave? .............................................. 19
Abandonment and reduced maternal defense as reproductive strategies ... 19
Intraspecific predation ................................................................................ 20
Space use in relation to ecological factors ................................................. 20
Space use in relation to reproductive strategies ......................................... 21
Discussion ............................................................................................................... 21
Maternal care and offspring size, sex and number ..................................... 21
Space use in relation ecological factors and reproductive strategies ......... 25
Perspectives ........................................................................................................... 27
Acknowledgements .......................................................................................................... 30
References ........................................................................................................................ 32
Individual papers ............................................................................................................. 41

3
List of papers
The thesis includes the following papers, which are later referred to by their Roman numerals
in the text.

Paper I
Dahle B, Swenson JE, Body size and mass relationships in yearling brown bears: effects of
maternal size, litter size, sex, cohort and population density. Manuscript

Paper II
Dahle B, Swenson JE, Factors influencing length of maternal care and its consequences for
offspring in brown bears Ursus arctos. Behav Ecol Sociobiol (in press)

Paper III
Dahle B, Swenson JE, (2003) Family breakup in brown bears: are young forced to leave?
J Mammal 84:(in press)

Paper IV
Zedrosser A, Dahle B, Vik JO and Swenson JE. Offspring abandonment and reduced maternal
defense as reproductive strategies in European brown bears. Manuscript

Paper V
Swenson JE, Dahle B, Sandegren F, (2001) Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown
bears older than cubs of the year. Ursus 12: 81-92

Paper VI
Dahle B, Swenson JE, (2003) Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears Ursus arctos:
effect of mass, sex, reproductive status, population density and habitat type. J Zool
260:(in press)

Paper VII
Dahle B, Swenson JE (2003), Seasonal range size in relation to reproductive strategies in
brown bears Ursus arctos. J Anim Ecol (in press)

4
Abstract
This thesis deals with two main aspects of brown bear Ursus arctos reproductive strategies.
First, it seeks to understand strategies of maternal care and how these affect the size and fate
of offspring. Second it focuses on how home range size relates to ecological factors and
reproductive strategies. All these aspects were studied using empirical data from a long-term
study of brown bears in southcentral and northern Sweden.
Size of offspring is an important life history variable, because it might affect
their survival and extend into adulthood and affect their reproductive success. Yearling body
size and mass were positively related to maternal body size and negatively related to litter
size. Males were larger and heavier than females, suggesting that mothers invest more in their
male offspring, in accordance with the sex allocation theory for polygynous species. Further,
yearling body mass showed a pronounced variance among cohorts, probably a result of
variable food abundance among years. Yearling size and mass did not differ between the
southern and northern study area. No significant relationship was found between population
density and yearling body mass, but population density was probably below carrying capacity.
Subadult bears (1-3 years-old) that died (excluding human caused mortalities) tended to be
smaller as yearlings than subadults that did not die.
Length of maternal care, i.e. the interval between successfully raised litters, is the most
important factor explaining the variation in reproductive rates among brown bear populations,
making it an interesting life history trait. In southcentral Sweden 95% of the litters separated
from their mother as yearlings and the rest as two-year-olds. In northern Sweden litters
separated as yearlings (53%), two-year-olds (44%) and three-year-olds (3%). In northern
Sweden the probability of yearling litters staying with their mother for a second year
decreased with increasing yearling body mass, and was higher for litters with 2 offspring than
for litters with 1 or 3-4 offspring. Staying with their mothers for a second year had a positive
effect on mass gain in yearlings and this effect was most pronounced in litters with 2
offspring. Body mass of 2-year-olds was not related to age of weaning and our results
suggested that keeping offspring for an additional year mainly compensated for low yearling
body mass. In the southern study area we examined the behavior of family groups during
family breakup. With few exceptions, all yearlings within a litter separated at the same time.
Yearlings separated from their mothers during the mating season in May-early July, and most
family breakups occurred with an adult male present. Our results suggest that termination of
maternal investment was not initiated by the offspring. Because yearling body mass did not

5
differ between the study areas, body mass itself can not explain why many females cared for
their yearlings for a second year in the northern study area. Although cohort effects on
yearling body mass was evident in both study areas, prolonged care for small offspring in the
northern study area probably reduced the cohort effect by the age of 2 year.
Abandonment of dependent offspring has been related to poor nutritional conditions,
but also to unusual small broods/litters. We modeled the number of offspring recruited to the
population when female brown bears varied their behavior regarding abandoning single cubs
to evaluate whether there might be a selective advantage of abandoning single cubs. In the
southern subpopulation, with a 2-year reproductive cycle, females would not gain by
abandoning single-cub litters, whereas in the northern subpopulation, females with a 3-year
reproductive cycle would gain marginally (about 2%) by doing so. Females with single-cub
litters did not seem to abandon single cubs after leaving the den, but we were not able to judge
whether females abandoned single cubs shortly after birth. In the southern subpopulation,
where sexually selected infanticide (SSI) was suggested to be a major agent of cub mortality,
the probability of loosing one or more cubs from a litter decreased with litter size. In the
northern subpopulation, where cub mortality was low and SSI less common, the probability of
loosing one or more cubs from a litter increased with litter size. Our results support the
maternal investment theory, that mothers invest in offspring defense in positive relation to the
number of offspring. Protecting cubs from males is costly for mothers, as they tended to be
killed more often by conspecifics than females without cubs. Excluding human caused
mortality, intraspecific predation is the most important cause of mortality in brown bears.
Staying with their mother did not significantly reduce intraspecific predation among
yearlings, suggesting that the benefit for yearlings by staying for second year is mainly
increased growth rate. The number of adult males that died 3 years previously and whether an
adult male died 2 years previously explained part of the variation of the number of female
yearlings killed. This was similar to the results previous reported for cub mortality, further
pointing out that killing of adult males might has a strong demographic effect.
In polygynous mammals the size of male home ranges has been considered to be
determined by the distribution of females and the size of female ranges has been considered to
be determined by the spatial distribution of food. On a large geographical scale home range
size reported for brown bear populations was negatively related to food availability. However,
for the Scandinavian populations we rejected the hypothesis that home range size is
proportional to individuals’ metabolic needs, because this hypothesis was not able to explain
the larger annual or seasonal ranges in males than estrous females nor the differences among

6
females of different reproductive categories. Males and estrous females increased their ranges
in the mating season, suggesting a roam-to-mate strategy in both sexes. Roaming behavior of
estrous females may be a strategy to consort several males for mate selection and to hide
paternity as a counterstrategy against sexually selected infanticide, apparently the most
important cause of cub mortality. Females with cubs, on the other hand, decreased their
ranges during the mating season, probably to avoid contact with infanticidal males, because
the presence of small cubs was not able to explain the small ranges used in the mating season.
The size of annual ranges was negatively related to population density along a population
density gradient not related to food availability, probably because movements are restricted by
conspecifics at higher population density.

7
Sammendrag
Denne avhandlingen tar for seg noen sentrale aspekt ved reproduktive strategier hos
brunbjørn. Første del tar for seg strategier for foreldreomsorg og hvordan disse påvirker
størrelsen og overlevelsen på avkommet. Deretter undersøkes hvordan størrelsen på
hjemmeområdet påvirkes av økologiske faktorer og reproduktive strategier. Disse aspektene
ble undersøkt ved å bruke data fra et langtidsstudie av brunbjørn i et sørlig (Dalarnas og
Gävleborgs län og Hedmark fylke) og et nordlig (Norrbottens län) studieområde.
Størrelse på avkom er en viktig livshistorievariabel fordi det kan ha en innvirkning på
avkommets overlevelse, størrelse som voksen og reproduktiv suksess. Kroppsstørrelse og vekt
på ettåringer (ca 16 måneder gamle) økte med størrelsen på mora og avtok med antall unger i
kullet. Hanner var større og tyngre enn hunner, som forventet ut i fra teorien om at foreldre
skal investere mer i hannavkom hos arter med kjønnsdimorfi i størrelse. Dette fordi
betydningen av å være stor antas å ha en større effekt på reproduktiv suksess hos hanner enn
hos hunner. Vekta på ettåringene varierte mellom år, trolig på grunn av variasjon i mattilgang
mellom ulike år. Størrelse og vekt på ettåringene var ikke forskjellig i de to studieområdene,
og var upåvirket av bestandstetthet.
Lengden på foreldreomsorg, dvs. tidsintervallet mellom ungekull som overlever, er
den faktoren som forklarer mest av variasjonen i reproduksjonsraten mellom ulike
brunbjørnbestander. I det sørlige studieområdet skilte 95% av kullene lag med mora som
ettåringer og resten som toåringer. I det nordlige studieområdet skilte 53% av kullene lag med
mora som ettåringer, 45% som toåringer og 3% som treåringer. I det nordlige studieområdet
avtok sannsynligheten for at et ungekull fulgte mora et ekstra år (til de er 2,5 år gamle) med
økende gjennomsnittsvekt på ungene (målt ved 16 måneders alder) i kullet. Vi forventet at
sannsynligheten for å gå med mora et ekstra år skulle øke med kullstørrelsen, ettersom
foreldreinvesteringen ofte øker med kullstørrelse. Det viste seg at kull med to unger hadde
høyere sannsynlighet for å gå sammen med mora et ekstra år enn kull med en eller tre-fire
unger. Unger som gikk sammen med mora et ekstra år hadde raskere vekst det året enn unger
som ble avvent som ettåringer, og den positive effekten av å gå sammen med mora var høyere
i kull med to unger sammenlignet med kull med tre-fire unger. Kroppsvekt på toåringer var
ikke avhengig av om de hadde gått med mora i ett eller to år, noe som antyder at forlenget
foreldreinvestering hovedsakelig var en kompensasjon for lav vekt på ettåringer. I det sørlige
studieområdet, hvor 95% av kullene skilte lag med mora som ettåringer, undersøkte vi
omstendighetene rundt familieoppløsningen. Med få unntak separerte hele kullet samtidig.

8
Oppløsningen av familiegruppene skjedde i parringstida i mai til begynnelsen av juli, og i de
fleste tilfeller var en voksen hannbjørn i nærheten. Vi konkluderte med at familieoppløsning
ikke ble initiert av ungene. Siden det ikke var noen forskjell i kroppsvekt på ettåringer i de to
studieområdene kan ikke kroppsvekt i seg selv forklare hvorfor mange mødre gikk med
ungene et ekstra år i det nordlige studieområdet.
Desertering (frastøtning) av avhengige unger er generelt satt i sammenheng med at
mora er i svært dårlig kondisjon, men også med at kullet er uvanlig lite. Vi modellerte antall
unger rekruttert til bestanden når brunbjørnbinner varierte atferden sin med hensyn på å
desertere eller ikke desertere kull med en unge for å evaluere om det kan være en fordel å
desertere slike kull. Fordelen med å desertere et slikt kull ligger i at de da kan produsere et
nytt og forhåpentligvis større kull tidligere enn hvis de oppfostret kullet med en unge. I det
sørlige studieområdet med en toårs reproduktiv syklus (ungene avvennes som ettåringer) var
det ingen fordel å desertere slike kull, mens binner i det nordlige studieområdet med en treårs
reproduktiv syklus økte antallet avvente avkom i løpet av livsløpet med 2% ved å desertere
slike kull. Uansett så det ikke ut til at binnene deserterte kull med en unge etter at de forlot
hiet om våren i det nordlige studieområdet. Vi kunne ikke avgjøre om dette hadde skjedd
tidligere, før de forlot hiet.
Teorien om foreldreinvestering sier at investering i avkom bør justeres etter hvilke
fordeler denne investeringen har for avkommet og hvilke ulemper dette har for foreldrenes
overlevelse og framtidige reproduksjon. I det sørlige studieområdet var seksuelt selektert
infanticid (hannbjørner dreper unger som følger mora for å få tilgang til å parre seg med
mora) en viktig årsak til dødelighet hos årsunger. I dette området avtok sannsynligheten for at
en eller flere av ungene døde i løpet av sommeren med økende kullstørrelse. I det nordlige
studieområdet, hvor seksuelt selektert infanticid var mindre vanlig, økte sannsynligheten for å
at en eller flere unger døde i løpet av sommeren med økende kullstørrelse. Disse resultatene
kan best forklares ved at binnene øker forsvaret mot hannbjørner med økende kullstørrelse. Å
forsvare unger mot hannbjørner er kostbart for binnene ettersom de hadde en større
sannsynlighet for bli drept av andre bjørner enn voksne binner uten årsunger. Når det gjelder
predasjon på bjørner eldre enn årsunger av andre bjørner var ettåringer mest utsatt. Det var
ingen forskjell på predasjon på ettåringer som var avvent og på ettåringer som gikk sammen
med mora, så fordelen med å følge mora et ekstra år er i første rekke økt vekst. Ingen bjørner
eldre enn tre år (med unntak av binner med årsunger) ble drept av andre bjørner.
Predasjonsraten på ettårige binner økte med antallet voksne hannbjørner som døde tre år
tidligere og om en hannbjørn døde to år tidligere. Dette resultatet er likt med tidligere

9
undersøkelser som fant økt dødelighet av årsunger to år etter at voksne hannbjørner døde og
understreker at avskytning av voksne hannbjørner kan ha en sterk demografisk effekt.
Hos polygyne pattedyr er størrelsen på hjemmeområdet hos voksne hanner antatt å
være bestemt av fordelingen av voksne hunner, og størrelsen på hjemmeområdet hos voksne
binner er antatt å være bestemt av den romlige fordelingen av mat. Vi estimerte størrelsen på
hjemmeområdene i løpet av året og i parringstida (1 mai-10 juli) og etter parringstida (10
juli- til de går i hi om høsten). På en stor geografisk skala avtok størrelsen på
hjemmeområdene med økt mattilgang. Vi forkastet hypotesen om at størrelsen på
hjemmeområdet øker proporsjonalt med metabolsk behov fordi forskjeller i metabolsk behov
ikke kunne forklare størrelsesforskjellen i hjemmeområde (både års og sesong) mellom
hanner og brunstige binner. Den kunne heller ikke forklare variasjonen mellom binner
tilhørende ulike reproduktive kategorier (brunstige binner, binner med årsunger og binner
med fjorårsunger). Hanner og brunstige binner økte størrelsen på hjemmeområdet i
parringstida, noe som antyder at begge kjønn utvider hjemmeområdet for å oppsøke
potensielle partnere. Hos hanner er fordelen med denne atferden åpenbar ettersom antallet
unger som han produserer er avhengig av hvor mange binner han parrer seg med. Hos binner
kan dette være en strategi for makevalg, og å spre farskapsusikkerhet ved å parre seg med
flere hanner som ett mottrekk mot seksuelt selektert infanticid. Binner med årsunger derimot
reduserte størrelsen på hjemmeområdet i parringstida, trolig for å unngå kontakt med hanner
som kan drepe ungene for etterpå å kunne parre seg. For årsområdene avtok størrelsen med
økende bestandstetthet for både hanner og binner, og dette skyldes antakeligvis at
interaksjoner mellom individene øker med økende bestandstetthet.

10
Synopsis
Introduction
Wilbur (1977) defined reproductive strategies as a set of “tactics” that has been selected as the
adaptations that have contributed, on the average, the greatest number of offspring to recent
generations. Thus, reproductive strategies include the ways individuals acquire sexual
partners and how they invest in their offspring. The first part of this thesis (Papers I-V)
focuses on aspects of maternal care in brown bears (Ursus arctos), whereas the second part
(Papers VI-VII) evaluate spatial strategies in relation to reproduction and ecological factor as
revealed by analyses of brown bear home range size.

Parental care
Parental care is defined as any action that is likely to increase offspring survival and
reproduction (Clutton-Brock 1991). Parental care, such as provisioning of food and protection
against predators, usually imposes a cost to the reproductive potential of the parent (Trivers
1972) as parents might sacrifice growth, (Green and Rothstein 1991) survival (Clutton-Brock,
et al. 1989; Reguera and Gomendio 1999; 2001) and future reproduction (Clutton-Brock et al.
1989). However the costs of parental care are often related to environmental conditions, for
example in red deer (Cervus elaphus), differences in subsequent fecundity between milk-
producing and dry hinds are only found when population density is high and vary with the
mothers’ dominance rank (Clutton-Brock et al. 1989). Parental care that reduces any
component of the parents’ fitness is termed parental investment (Trivers 1972; Clutton-Brock
1991). Above some threshold level of investment, the benefits of care are likely to show a
diminishing return function as the amount of care received by the offspring increases (Smith
and Fretwell 1974; Festa-Bianchet et al. 1994). The amount of resources a parent invests may
vary according to offspring attributes, such as litter size (Jonsson et al. 2002; Verboven and
Tinbergen 2002) and offspring sex (Lee and Moss 1986; Georges and Guinet 2001). Parents
are reported to invest more in larger litters (review in Clutton-Brock 1991; Jonsson et al.
2002) and invest more in the sex with the largest variation in reproductive success (Lee and
Moss 1986; Georges and Guinet 2001). Furthermore, parental investment is often positively
related to parental size or condition (e.g. Andersen et al. 2000), but might also be influenced
by parity and environmental factors (see Clutton-Brock 1991 for a review).
Body mass is one of the most important factors affecting an individual’s fitness (e.g.
Festa Bianchet et al. 2000; McElligot et al. 2001; Perrins and McCleery 2001; but see Gaillard
et al. 2000 for a discussion). Body mass at birth or weaning/fledging is usually positively

11
associated with early survival (e.g. Baker and Fowler 1992; Festa-Bianchet et al. 1997), and
even survival to adulthood (e.g. Albon et al. 1987). Additionally, body mass at birth or
weaning is often reported to be positively correlated with the body mass later in life (Shultz
and Johnson 1995; Birgerson and Ekvall 1997), the size they reached as adults (Myers and
Master 1983; Albon et al. 1987; Festa-Bianchet et al. 2000), and their reproductive success
(Festa-Bianchet et al. 2000). Thus, conditions experienced during early development (i e.
maternal investment) can influence growth, survival and later reproduction in birds and
mammals (Lindström 1999). How body size early in life affects survival, growth, age at
maturity, adult size and reproductive performance is poorly understood in large carnivores,
including brown bears. A first step is taken in paper I, where we examine how yearling size
and mass is related to maternal size, litter size, sex, environmental conditions (cohort effect)
and population density and how yearling body mass affects survival during the subadult
period.
In mammals, offspring rely on milk after birth and in most species only females
provide care for offspring (Clutton-Brock 1991). The relative costs of lactation is usually
much higher than that of gestation (Oftedahl and Gittleman 1989), and the energy transfer
during lactation varies among species, resulting in varying growth rates. For example in a
typical capital breeder (which relies on stored resources for reproduction), such as the hooded
seal (Cystophora cristata), offspring might be weaned after 3.6 days after increasing its birth
mass by 60% (Mellish et al. 1999). A general trend in mammals seems to be that offspring are
weaned when they reach a threshold size, hence the length of maternal investment often varies
in relation to nutritional and maternal condition (Trillmich 1986; Cheney et al. 1988;
Fairbanks and McGurie 1995). However this might be influenced by how maternal care
affects different kinds of mortality factors. For example, high care-independent mortality, e.g.
predation, might select for earlier weaning (Lycett et al. 1998). Further, in some seasonal
breeders, the length of maternal investment may be fixed (e.g. 1 or 2 years) and thus variation
in nutritional conditions may result in increased variance in size of weaned offspring among
populations or cohorts (Hauser and Fairbanks 1988). The effect of litter size and offspring
mass and sex on the length of maternal care in brown bears is examined in paper II.
Conflicts may arise between parents and offspring over parental investment (Trivers
1974). The theoretical foundation for conflict is sound but conflicts are seldom observed in
nature (Bateson 1994; Mock and Parker 1997). Most studies of parent-offspring conflict have
focused on sex-ratio adjustment or parent-offspring communication in small organisms (Mock
and Parker 1997); relatively little information is available for large long-lived species, in

12
which parent-offspring conflict might be expected due to high fitness differential (Clutton-
Brock et al. 1982; Clutton-Brock 1991). In mammals the most commonly studied parent-
offspring conflict cases involve “weaning conflict” over when parental largess (milk) should
be discontinued (Mock and Parker 1997). The termination of maternal care and who initiates
it is poorly documented in the brown bear, although the limited information available suggests
that it usually takes place during the mating season and that adult males may be involved in
the process (Egbert and Luque 1975; Herrero and Hamer 1977; Ternent and Garshelis 1998).
Behavior of mothers and offspring during the breakup of families is examined in paper III in
order to determine how this potential conflict is solved.
In some instances mothers may terminate maternal care at an early stage, before
offspring are capable of surviving on their own. This behavior is observed in both birds (e.g.
Yorio and Boersma 1994) and in mammals (Fairbanks and McGurie 1995) and is often
associated with very poor nutritional conditions, i.e. when it is unlikely that the parents are
capable of successfully raising the offspring or because it may be too expensive for the
parents. A similar, but in some ways different situation appears when mothers for some
reason give birth to unusually small clutches or litters or the clutch/litter size is reduced e.g.
due to predation. Whereas Mendl (1994) suggested that mothers should increase investment in
such small litters to produce large and competitive young, Verboven and Tinbergen (2002)
found that great tit (Parus major) parents abandoned reduced litters early in the incubation
period. For brown bears, which have a long lactation period and young that reach
independence at 2.5 years of age, Tait (1980) suggested that mothers may benefit by
abandoning unusually small litters, re-mating and producing a new, larger litter the next year.
Tait’s (1980) paper is often cited, but nobody has ever tested his model. In paper IV we use
reproductive parameters from two populations with different litter intervals to model the
optimal behavior of brown bear females giving birth to single-cub litters.
Besides provisioning offspring milk, mothers protect offspring from predators (Cote et
al. 1997; Lingle and Pellis 2002), which enhances offspring survival (Cote et al. 1997). In
defense of their offspring parents are at risk of being injured or killed (Packer and Pusey
1983; McLellan 1994; Lingle and Pellis 2002), but in general it has been difficult to find a
direct link between parental care and survival cost in the parents (Reguera and Gomendio
1999). Female brown bears can be severely wounded or killed when defending offspring from
attacking adult males (reviewed by McLellan 1994). As the reproductive value of a litter
increases with litter size (assuming relatively similar values of all offspring regardless of litter
size) females should invest more in the defense of large than small litters. Koskela et al.

13
(2000) and Jonsson et al. (2002) reported that female bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus)
adjusted their defense against infanticidal males in relation to litter size, and similar results
have been reported for brood defense in fish (Carlisle 1985) and clutch defense in birds
(Windt and Curio 1986). In paper IV we tested the hypothesis of maternal defense using cub
survival data from two brown bear populations, one where infanticide by males was
considered to be a major cause of cub mortality, and one where infanticide was uncommon.
Further we examined whether maternal defense had a survival cost.
Swenson et al (1997; 2001) concluded that infanticide was a major agent of cub
mortality in Scandinavian brown bears, but older bears might also be killed by conspecifics
(Garshelis 1994; McLellan 1994; Derocher and Wiig 1999; McLellan et al. 1999). However,
causes for such killings are not known, although they have been associated with extremely
poor food conditions (Smirnov and Shurygin 1991). In paper V we report intraspecific
predation rates on bears older than cubs and seek to document the victims and perpetrators,
whether yearlings that stay with their mothers survive better than those that separate as
yearlings, and the reason for intaspecific predation.

Space use in relation to ecological factors and reproductive strategies


Home ranges are the areas in which animals acquire necessary resources and carry out the
biological requirements for life (Burt 1943). Home range size is therefore an important
biological parameter reflecting aspects such as body mass, diet, food abundance and
dispersion, as well as sex and reproductive strategy (e.g. Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1978;
Harestad and Bunnell 1979; Sandell 1989; Palomares 1994; Fisher and Owens, 2000).
McNab (1963) suggested that food controlled home range size through an animal’s size-
dependent metabolic rate and the productivity of its habitat. High food productivity and
quality allows an animal to meet its energy requirements in a small home range. In
polygynous and promiscuous species without male parental care, female home range is
expected to be a function of the availability and spatial distribution of food (Clutton-Brock
and Harvey 1978; Litvaitis et al. 1986; Sandell 1989; Tufto et al. 1996; Powell et al. 1997).
Male home range size in this mating system, on the other hand, is proposed to be influenced
by two limited resources: receptive females and food (Sandell 1989). In polygynous species
males are usually larger than females (e.g. Clutton-Brock et al. 1977) and larger home ranges
could thus be due to both an increased movement of males to consort with several females
and sexual size dimorphism, because of increased metabolic demands in the larger males
compared to nonreproducing females (McNab 1963). Another factor proposed to influence

14
home range size is population density, and several studies have found a negative relationship
between home range size and population density (e.g. Mares et al. 1982; Nagy and Haroldson
1990). In paper VI we test hypotheses proposed to explain variation in annual home range
size in adult brown bears.
Male and female strategies for maximizing their reproductive success differ in most
mating systems, but few studies have focused on how mating may affect range size of females
in species with a promiscuous mating system. Female marsupials (e.g. Fisher and Lara 1999)
and ungulates (San Jose and Lovari 1998; Lamberti et al. 2001) are known to roam, increasing
their ranges during the mating season, and females tend to visit males with higher mating
success (Liberg et al. 1998). Large ranges in estrous females during the mating season should
increase the probability of meeting several prospective mates, or just of being mated. Females
may benefit from this through mate selection, mediated through male-male competition or
female choice (Andersson 1994), hiding paternity as a counterstrategy against infanticide
(Ebensperger 1998), fertilization insurance (Gray 1997), sperm competition (Stockley and
Purvis 1993), and selection of the most genetically compatible sperm (Wilson et al. 1997).
Infanticide, the killing of conspecific young, may influence the mating system in many
mammalian species (Agrell et al. 1998; Ebensperger 1998), but also in birds (Hansson et al.
1997). One proposed explanation for infanticide is that a male may gain mating opportunities
by killing the dependent offspring of females, because this would shorten the interval to the
female’s next conception (Hrdy 1979). This sexually selected infanticide hypothesis has
gained support in several studies of birds (e.g. Smith et al. 1996), social primates (e.g. Borries
et al. 1999; Soltis et al. 2000), rodents (e.g. Soroker and Terkel 1988) and carnivores (e.g.
Pusey and Packer 1994), including the brown bear in our study populations (Swenson et al.
1997; 2001). Swenson et al. (1997; 2001) suggested that sexually selected infanticide was a
major agent of cub mortality in the southern study population, whereas it was less common,
but still occurring in the northern study population. As a counterstrategy, females with
dependent offspring should therefore avoid males during the mating season to increase the
survival of their offspring (Ebensperger 1998). In paper VII we analyze home ranges during
the mating and postmating season in relation to male and female reproductive strategies.

15
Aims
The main aims of this thesis were first to understand patterns of maternal care and how this
influences offspring size, and second examine how space use is related to ecological factors
and reproductive strategies in brown bears from an evolutionary perspective. It seeks to
answer the questions: Which factors influence offspring size? (Paper I) and what
consequences might this have? (Papers I-II) Why does the length of maternal care vary so
much (Papers II-IV), and what consequences might this have for the offspring? (Papers II and
IV-V) Who initiates the termination of maternal care? (Paper III) How does space use vary
seasonally in relation to reproductive strategies? (Paper VII) and, more generally, which
factors influence space use in brown bears? (Paper VI).

Methods
Study species
The brown bear is a solitary carnivore with a circumpolar distribution and inhabiting a variety
of habitats from arctic tundra through temperate deciduous forest to deserts (Servheen et al.
1999). In Europe, large viable populations are found in the east and north, whereas the
populations in southcentral and southwestern Europe are small, isolated, and probably not
viable (Zedrosser et al. 2001). Brown bears are promiscuous, and during the mating season in
mid May to early July (Curry Lindahl 1972), a male may mate with several females, and a
female may mate with several males (Hensel et al. 1969; Craighead et al. 1995). Implantation
is delayed until November (Renfree and Calaby 1981; Tsubota et al. 1989) and in January,
when they are hibernating in dens, females give birth to 1-4 squirrel-sized offspring after 6-8
weeks of effective gestation (Pasitschniak-Arts 1993). Only females care for the offspring,
which follow their mother for 1.4-3.5 years (McLellan 1994). Throughout the text, cubs refer
to offspring in their first year, and yearlings refer to offspring in their second year. Females do
not mate until they have separated from their offspring, resulting in long and variable
interbirth intervals. Cub mortality rates vary among different populations (McLellan 1994;
Swenson et al. 1997; 2001) and mortality is much higher in the southern study population
than in the northern study population (see later description). In the studied populations young
reach sexual maturity at the age of 3-5 years (Swenson et al. 1995), whereas they usually are
older in North American populations (McLellan 1994; Hilderbrand et al. 1999).

16
Study areas
The study was performed in two areas. The southern area (hereafter named south) is situated
in Dalarna and Gävleborg counties, in southcentral Sweden, and Hedmark County in
southeastern Norway (61o N, 18o E) and covers the southern part of the southernmost brown
bear subpopulation in Scandinavia. The elevation ranges from about 200 m in the
southeastern part to about 1,000 m in the western part at the Norwegian border, but only a
minor part of the area is above timberline, which is at about 750 m. Lakes and bogs are
common, but most of the hilly landscape is covered with intensively managed coniferous
forest, dominated by Scots pine (Pinus silvestris) and Norway spruce (Picea abies).
The northern study area (hereafter named north) is situated in the southwestern part of
Norrbotten County in Sweden (67° N, 18° E). The area is rolling with elevations below 300 m
in the east, but is dominated by mountains that rise to over 2,000 m in the west. Northern
boreal coniferous forest dominates, but there are extensive subalpine birch (Betula pubescens)
forests.
On average, bears are active from April to November, reflecting the length of the
snowfree period, which is about a month shorter in the northern study area (Sandegren and
Swenson 1997). Both areas are sparsely populated by humans. For a detailed description of
the study areas see Grundsten (1997) and Lundqvist (2002).

General methods
In mid-April – mid-May brown bears were darted from a helicopter with immobilizing drugs
(a mixture of tiletamin, zolazepan and medetomidin (Kreeger et al. 2002)). All captured bears
were weighed and measured and blood, hair, and tissue samples were taken for later analysis.
Most bears were fitted with radio-transmitters mounted on neck collars or implanted into the
peritoneal cavity. Radio marked bears were located from an airplane or helicopter using
receivers and antennas mounted on the aircraft or from the ground using receivers and hand-
held 6-element antennas. All bears were located weekly, but many were located more
frequently in order to achieve better measures of movements. For more complete descriptions
of methods see the respective papers.

17
Main Results
Offspring size (Paper I)
Body size and mass is reported to have a strong effect on individuals’ fitness, and conditions
experienced early in life may affect survival to adulthood, age and size at maturation and
reproductive success. For this reason body size and mass of yearling brown bears were
analyzed in relation to maternal size, litter size, sex, cohort and population density in two
study areas in Sweden. The body mass of yearlings varied tremendously, from 8.5 to 48 kg.
As predicted, yearling body size and mass were positively related to maternal size and
negatively related to litter size. Males were larger and heavier than females, suggesting that
mothers invest more in their male offspring in accordance with the sex allocation theory for
polygynous species, although we could not rule out that males just ate more solid food than
females. Further, yearling body mass showed a pronounced variance among cohorts, by a
factor of 1.7. The difference among cohorts was probably a result of changing food
abundance among years, influencing maternal condition and investment as well as the
offsprings’ intake of solid food, but data on food availability was not available to evaluate
this. No significant relationship was found between population density and yearling body size
or mass, but population density was probably below carrying capacity. Much of the variance
in body mass of yearlings was due to variation within litters, especially in litters with 3
offspring, where the heaviest yearling was 30 % heavier on average than the lightest one. This
suggests that competition among offspring increases with litter size, having a pronounced
negative effect on the smallest offspring.

Length of maternal care; 1.5 or 2.5 years? (Paper II)


A fundamental life history traits in animals with parental care is the interval between
successive reproductions, that is, how long parents should invest in their offspring.
In a population of brown bears in northern Sweden mothers cared for their offspring for 1.5-
3.5 years. Length of maternal care varied among individuals, but also varied within many
individuals. Offspring became independent with body masses ranging from 17.5 to 69 kg. The
probability of yearling litters staying with their mother for a second year decreased with
increasing yearling body mass and surprisingly was higher for litters with 2 offspring than for
litters with 1 or 3-4 offspring. Mass gain was higher in yearlings that stayed with their mother
than in yearlings that separated. The positive effect on yearling mass gain by staying with
their mothers was most pronounced in litters with 2 offspring. Mass gain of yearlings was

18
positively related with maternal size. At the age of two years, offspring from litters with 2
offspring were heavier than offspring from litters with 3-4 young and males were heavier than
females. However body mass was weakly, if at all, related to age at weaning, suggesting that
prolonged maternal care was mainly a compensation for low yearling body mass.

Family breakup: are young forced to leave? (Paper III)


Because the timing of family breakup might have different optima for parents and offspring,
we recorded breakups of brown bear families in southcentral Sweden. Sixty of 63 litters
separated from their mother as yearlings and the remainder as 2-year-olds. Breakups occurred
from 3 May to 15 July, which corresponds with the mating season. With few exceptions all
yearlings within multiple cub litters separated from their mother at the same time. The
presence of adult males was associated with most breakups and suggests that yearlings were
forced to depart when the family approached or were approached by an adult male.
Observations of yearlings in the top of Scots pine trees at breakups both with and without
adult males present suggest that both the mother and adult males might act aggressively
towards yearlings. We concluded that family breakup was not initiated by the offspring. After
separation from their mother, yearlings in 13 of 18 litters with > 1 young stayed together for
1-22 d before they separated from one another. We observed that yearlings tried to reunite
with the mother after family breakup in 2 families.

Abandonment and reduced maternal defense as reproductive strategies (Paper IV)


Here we tested whether two proposed reproductive strategies, abandonment of single cubs and
reduced maternal defense of small litters, is able to explain observed patterns of cub mortality.
We modeled the number of offspring recruited to the population when female brown bears
varied their behavior regarding abandoning single cubs to evaluate if there might be a
selective advantage for abandoning single cubs. In the southern subpopulation, with a 2-year
reproductive cycle, females would not gain by abandoning single-cub litters, whereas in the
northern subpopulation area, females with a 3-year reproductive cycle would gain marginally
(about 2%) by doing so. Females with single-cub litters did not seem to abandon single cubs
after leaving the den, but we were not able to judge whether females abandoned single cubs
shortly after birth. In the southern subpopulation, where sexually selected infanticide (SSI)
was identified as a major agent of cub mortality, the probability of loosing one or more cubs
from a litter decreased with litter size. In the northern subpopulation, where cub mortality was
low and SSI less common, the probability of loosing one or more cubs from a litter increased

19
with litter size. Our results support the maternal invest theory, that mothers invest in offspring
in relation to their reproductive value. Protecting cubs from males is costly for mothers, as
they tended to be killed more often by conspecifics than adult females without cubs.

Intraspecific predation (Paper V)


In this paper we documented 13 cases of intraspecific predation in brown bears in
southcentral and northern Sweden during 668 bear-years of radiotracking 238 brown bears.
We found area differences in the rates of intraspecific predation only for yearling females.
Annual yearling female mortality due to intraspecific predation was higher (0.162, 6 of 38) in
the south than in the north (no mortality recorded, 28 yearlings followed). No older subadult
females were killed by other bears. Annual mortality rates due to intraspecific predation for
males, areas combined, were: 0.032 for yearlings, 0.040 for 2-year-olds, and 0.061 for 3-year-
olds, for a combined rate from age 1 through 3 years of 0.127. Neither population density, at
the levels we observed, nor reduced food abundance influenced rates of intraspecific
predation on yearlings in our areas. Intraspecific predation on yearling females was correlated
positively with the number of adult males that had died 3 years previously and whether any
adult male had died 2 years previously. Staying with their mother did not significantly reduce
intraspecific predation among yearlings.

Space use in relation to ecological factors (Paper VI)


We estimated annual home ranges using 95% minimum convex polygon (MCP) based on
weekly positions. The estimated male home ranges were larger than those for lone females,
also after controlling for sexual size dimorphism. The difference between male and female
ranges was less prominent in the northern study area than in the southern study area. Home
range size estimates for males and for lone females and females with cubs decreased with
increasing relative population density. Based on the cost of lactation, home ranges was
predicted to be larger for females with young than for lone females, but the opposite was true
regarding females with cubs. Further, range size of females with yearlings was not different
from that of lone females, but was larger than in females with cubs. Home ranges estimated
based on a more frequently positioning suggested that the real size of the home ranges were
more than 1.5 times larger than our estimates based on weekly positions. On a large
geographical scale home range size estimates decreased markedly with increased food
abundance.

20
Space use in relation to reproductive strategies (Paper VII)
Here we used seasonal range size of 93 radio-collared adult brown bears to test hypotheses
explaining variation in range size in relation to male and female reproductive strategies. Both
males and estrous females used large ranges in the mating season, but decreased their ranges
after the mating season. These results suggested that both sexes in this species roam to mate,
because the results could not be explained by a seasonal change in food availability nor by
increased foraging movements of estrous females to replenish body reserves after previous
cub raising. Females with cubs restricted their range size in the mating season and increased
their ranges in the postmating season. This finding suggests that females with cubs restricted
their ranges to avoid contact with infanticidal males, an important cause of cub mortality,
because the proposed alternative explanation; limited mobility of small cubs, was unable to
explain the small size of mating season ranges. Our results suggest that home range size in
females is influenced by sexually selected infanticide, selecting for large mating season
ranges and multiple mating in estrous females to hide paternity and for restricted mating
season ranges in females with cubs to avoid infanticidal males.

Discussion
Maternal care and offspring size, sex and number (papers I, II, III, IV and V)
Many studies have found a positive relationship between maternal body mass and
reproductive parameters. For example in monotocus species (one offspring per reproductive
event) heavier mothers raise heavier offspring than lighter mothers (Clutton-Brock et al.1988;
Arnbom et al. 1997; Festa Bianchet et al. 1998) and also commonly adjust sex ratio towards
males in sexually dimorphic polygynous species (Clutton-Brock et al. 1988). In polytocous
species (one or several offspring per reproductive event) litter size is reported to increase with
maternal mass in some species (e.g. roe deer (Capreolus capreolus), Andersen et al. 2000,
alpine marmot (Marmota marmota), Allaine et al. 1998), whereas such a relationship is less
evident or absent in American black bears (U. americanus) (Schwartz and Franzmann 1991;
Miller 1994) and polar bears (U. maritimus) (Derocher and Stirling 1995).
Positive relationships between maternal size or mass and offspring size or mass have
been reported in most taxa (reviewed by Clutton Brock 1991), and this seems to be the case
for bears as well (Derocher and Stirling 1998a, Paper I). Smith and Fretwell (1974) developed
the first quantitative model of the trade off between size and number off offspring. Because

21
litter size might be positively related to maternal mass (e.g. Allaine et al. 1998; Andersen et
al. 2000) and maternal expenditure often increases with litter size (e.g. König et al. 1988),
such a trade off might be masked if maternal traits are not considered. We found that size and
mass of yearling brown bears were negatively related to litter size (Paper I), in accordance
with reports from other species (e.g. Rogers 1976; König et al. 1988; Wauters et al. 1993;
Derocher and Stirling 1998a)
The sex allocation theory predicts that mothers should invest more in male than female
offspring in sexually dimorphic polygynous species (Maynard Smith 1980; Charnov 1982).
Male yearlings are generally larger and heavier than female yearlings in brown bears
(Blanchard 1987; Paper I), but length of maternal care was not related to the sex ratio in the
litter (Paper II). The present study only provides limited support for the sex allocation theory,
because different feeding patterns of males and females could not be ruled as an explanation
for the sex difference in yearling size and mass, and we do not know whether males are more
costly to produce than females. The number of studies that have found differential investment
in polygynous species is roughly equal to that which have not (Byers and Moodie 1990 and
references therein), and the sex allocation theory is questionable in polytocous species (Frank
1990).
Unfortunately in the present study pregnant females were not weighed in fall, when
they have stored resources for gestation and lactation. In a typical capital breeder as the brown
bear, such data are necessary to fully evaluate the effect of maternal characteristics on litter
size and offspring size. For this reason we used body size as a maternal trait. We could not
use litter mass as a measure of maternal care, because young bears start to feed on solid food
shortly after leaving their den, although the lactation peak is during summer.
The large variation in size and mass of yearlings (Paper I) suggests that offspring size
at this age is not strongly related to survival and adult reproductive performance, or that
females are not able to adjust the size of their litter in accordance with the prevailing
nutritional conditions. If cub size had a strong impact on survival and later reproductive
success, there should be a general selection for large offspring, and for mechanisms to adjust
litter size in relation to the prevailing conditions. In bears, fertilization and implantation takes
place before and after females add lean body mass and fat in the autumn, respectively. In roe
deer, which also have delayed implantation, the number of corpora lutea (reflecting the
number of ova released) was positively related with body mass, whereas the implantation
success was age related, being higher in prime age does than young and old does (Hewison
and Gaillard 2001). However, the roe deer is a typical income breeder, where energetic cost of

22
reproduction is not meet by stored resources (Andersen et al. 2000). In brown bears, a litter
size adjustment at implantation in late fall, at the time they know their condition during
gestation and lactation, could optimize litter size to secure size of offspring under poor
conditions, but whether such mechanism exist is unknown. Reproductive failure after total
failure of major food resources has been reported in American black bears (Rogers 1976,
Miller 1994), but in general it seems that litter size in bears is weakly or not related to
nutritional conditions (Shcwartz and Franzmann 1991, Miller 1994, Derocher and Stirling
1995), although differences in litter size among brown bear populations have been associated
with maternal body mass and food abundance (Hilderband et al. 1999). It might be that bears
within a single population sacrifice size rather than the number of offspring during poor
conditions.
In general offspring of mammals are weaned when they reach a threshold body mass
(Lee et al. 1991). Brown bears are reported to wean their offspring when they are 1.5-3.5
years old (McLellan 1994), but why this life history trait exhibits such huge variation is
poorly understood. Craighead et al. (1995) suggested that females in good condition could
wean their offspring as yearlings, whereas others weaned their offspring one year later.
Derocher and Stirling (1995) reported that the proportion of polar bear yearlings that followed
their mother in western Hudson Bay increased during a period when body mass of most age
classes decreased. Derocher and Stirling (1998b) argued that yearlings were commonly
weaned in that population but not in others because yearlings in the western Hudson Bay
populations were larger than in the other populations. In paper II we found that age at
weaning was related to body mass of yearlings in spring. Litters with light yearlings had a
higher probability of being nursed for a second year than litters with heavy yearlings.
Yearling mass is positively related to maternal size (Paper I) so it seems like larger mothers
might wean their offspring as yearlings. However as yearling body mass fluctuates among
years (Paper I) length of maternal care varies between litters in many females (Paper II).
Little is known about lactation in brown bear females with yearlings, but Hensel et al.
(1969) reported that 7 of 10 females with yearlings inspected in July or August had substantial
quantities of milk. Of the 3 without substantial milk, two had little milk in the anterior glands,
whereas lactation was believed to be arrested in the third. Thus it seems as though most
yearlings following their mother for an additional year also suckle, which can explain why
dependent yearlings have a higher growth rate than independent yearlings (Paper II).
According to the theory of paternal investment parents are expected to adjust investment
according to the reproductive value of offspring (Clutton-Brock 1991). In brown bears,

23
yearlings in litters with 2 rather than 3-4 offspring had the greatest probability of being cared
for a second year (Paper II). This was unexpected because we assumed that the reproductive
value of a litter increased with litter size. However, growth of yearlings in litters with two
offspring was more enhanced when following their mother for a second year than was the
growth of yearlings in larger litters (Paper II), probably because the maternal resources
provided to offspring were not proportional to litter size. Also young in larger litters may
compete more with themselves in autumn when they feed on berries.
The time when maternal care is to be terminated might have different optima for
mothers and offspring (Mock and Parker 1997). Breakup of brown bear families in the
southern study population was not initiated by the offspring (Paper III), suggesting that the
offspring would benefit from continued care, as they in fact do (Paper II), whereas mothers
increase the number of weaned offspring if they wean their yearlings and produce a new litter
the next year. In the southern study area yearlings were weaned whatever their body mass
(Paper III), whereas light yearlings in the north had an increased probability to be cared for
another year (Paper II). Thus environmental conditions associated with light yearlings might
result in increased litter interval in the northern study area only. This suggests that body mass
is more strongly related to survival from the yearling age, and adult reproductive performance
in the northern study area. The actual cause for a stronger relationship in the northern study
area is unknown.
Abandonment of offspring before nutritional independence has commonly been
associated with poor maternal condition (e.g. Keech at al. 2000), but also when mothers give
birth to unusually small litters (Tait 1980; Lee and Cockburn 1985). Tait (1980) showed that
female brown bears might increase the number of offspring recruited to the population by
abandoning single-cub litters, and an extension of this model gave the same result for
Scandinavian brown bears following a three-year reproductive cycle (Paper IV). However,
females following a two year cycle, which is common in most European brown bear
populations, did not gain by doing so (Paper IV). We where not able to judge whether females
abandoned single cubs in the den, but they probably did not abandon them after leaving the
den (Paper IV). Under all circumstances the increase in offspring recruited to the population
by abandoning single-cub litters would be small, because if the proportion of single-cub litters
born is low, the average female will rarely experience this, and if the proportion of single-cub
litters increases, so do the chances of producing a new single cub after a abandonment.
Parents might be expected to adjust their expenditure on parental care in relation to
variations in its benefits to their offspring and its cost to themselves so as to maximize their

24
fitness (Winkler 1987). Maternal defense against infanticidal males is an aspect of maternal
care that has immediate cost and benefits, as both the offspring and mother might be severely
injured or killed in attacks (Packer and Pusey 1983; 1984; McLellan 1994). In brown bears,
the probability of loosing one or more cubs during the summer decreased with litter size in an
area where sexually selected infanticide (SSI) was suspected to be a major cause of cub
mortality (Paper IV). This pattern was the opposite from what we found in a population where
SSI was less important. Thus there appeared to be a positive relationship between maternal
defense and litter size in the population where SSI was common (Paper IV). Similarly,
parental defense is found to be positively related to brood size in fish (Carlisle 1985), birds
(Windt and Curio ) and rodents (Koskela et al. 2000, Jonnson et al. 2002). Maternal defense
in brown bears was dangerous for females and caused the only recorded natural mortalities in
adult females (Paper IV).
Mattson et al. (1992) and Mordosov (1993) suggested that intraspecific predation was
mostly confined to yearlings and other subadults. Results from Scandinavia confirms this
(Paper V), but adult females caring for cubs were also killed during the mating season (Paper
IV). Survival is usually higher in offspring following their mother because she defend them
against predators (Mastripieri 1992). However there was no difference in predation rate
between weaned yearlings and yearlings still following their mother (Paper V), which suggest
that the main benefit obtained by yearlings by following their mother for a second year is
increased growth (Paper II). Swenson et al. (1997; 2001) found that cub mortality was
elevated 1 and 2 years after adult males died in an area. The number of adult males killed 3
years previously and whether a adult male died 2 years explained part of the temporal
variation in intraspecific predation on female yearlings (Paper V). This similarity of the
results on cub mortality and predation on yearlings underlines that the killing of adult males
might have a strong demographic effect.

Home ranges in relation ecological factors and reproductive strategies (Papers VI and
VII)
Sandell (1989) analyzed reported home range sizes for solitary carnivores and found that on
the average, males had larger annual home ranges than expected from metabolic needs.
Sandell (1989) argued that the larger home ranges in males than females reflects the
polygynous/promiscuous mating system, in which males may increase fitness by roaming
over large areas to consort females. Later work supports this suggestion (e.g. Clutton-Brock
1989; Fisher and Lara 1999; Papers VI and VII). More interestingly, the metabolic hypothesis

25
did not explain variation in home range size among female brown bears of different
reproductive categories either (Paper VI). Estrous females used larger areas in the mating
season than in the postmating season, most likely to enhance opportunities to meet
prospective mates, thus allowing increased mate selection opportunities (Paper VII). This
result was consistent with findings in other polygynous species (Alt et al. 1976, Rogers 1987;
Rootes and Chabreck 1993; Labisky & Fritzen 1998; Liberg et al. 1998; San Jose and Lovari
1998; Fisher and Lara 1999). Mating with several males will also increase paternal
uncertainty, thereby possibly reducing the probability of loosing dependent offspring to
infanticidal males (Hrdy 1979; Ebensperger 1998; Soltis et al. 2000). Thus, sexually selected
infanticide may represent another selective pressure favoring roaming in estrous females.
Avoidance of infanticidal males has been proposed as a counterstrategy against
infanticide (Wielgus and Bunnel 1995; Ebensperger 1998). In accordance with this brown
bear females with cubs increased their range size from the mating season to the postmating
season and used smaller ranges than estrous females and females with yearlings during the
mating season (Paper VII). Alternative explanations for such changes in seasonal range size
were rejected, suggesting that females decreased their ranges in the mating season to avoid
contact with males (Paper VII). Several studies have reported restricted movements in females
due to the presence of cubs (Lindzey and Meslow 1977, Hirsch et al. 1999). However, no
authors have discussed whether these restricted movements were actually due to low mobility
of the cubs, which is an unlikely explanation considering their high activity levels (Powell et
al. 1997), or whether this was an adaptive behavior by the female to avoid contact with
conspecifics, as we suggest. Although the evidence for female avoidance of infanticidal males
as a counter strategy to infanticide is rather limited (Ebensperger 1998), it has been suggested
to be operating in brown bears (Murie 1981; Wielgus and Bunnel 1995).
It is generally believed that females utilize the minimum area that meets their energy
requirements (e.g. Tufto et al. 1996 for roe deer). In brown bears, home range size varies
inversely with population density (Nagy and Haroldson 1990, Paper VI), but in contrast to
most other studies, food availability was ruled out as an explanation for the observed
population density effect in Scandinavia (Paper VI). Female black bears are reported to
extend their home range into areas left vacant when neighboring females were killed (Rogers,
(1977), so the density effect was most likely due to interactions and competition among
individuals (Paper VI), as has also been reported in voles (Microtus californicus, Ostfeld
1986; M canicaudus, Wolff and Schauber 1996). Assumptions about home range size are
sometimes used when estimates of population size are carried out (Linnell et al. 1998). The

26
inflated population estimates of brown bears in Norway during the 1970-80’s (e.g. Kolstad et
al. 1986) might have been influenced both by dispersing male bears from Sweden, and the
large home ranges used by bears at very low population densities. (McLoughlin et al. 1999)
reported that home range size in North American bear populations was negatively related to
net primary production. We found that brown bear home range size decreased with food
abundance when including data from the rest of Europe (Paper VI). Thus, food abundance
sets the scale within which other factors might work.

Perspectives
In this thesis I have explored some reproductive strategies/life history aspects in the brown
bear, a challenging species to study, because stable funding is needed for many years
(decades) to obtain sample sizes and time spans necessary for such analyses. Paper I explored
size and mass relationships in yearling brown bears, and is so far the most comprehensive
study of this topic in this species. Especially interesting was the strong cohort effect, which to
my knowledge has not been reported previously in any bear species, except for a long-term
trends of decreasing body mass of Yellowstone brown bears after dump closures (Blanchard
1987) and of body mass of polar bears in Western Hudson Bay (Derocher and Stirling 1995),
associated with earlier break up of the sea ice due to long-term warming (Stirling et al. 1999).
The cohort effect in we found yearling body mass probably had an effect on the age at
weaning in the northern study area (Paper II). Nursing yearlings are energetic costly
(Hilderbrand et al. 2000), because yearlings are provided milk (Hensel et al 1969), and our
findings imply that reproductive cost is likely to be more pronounced in the northern study
area in years with low food availability, because many females then nurse their yearlings for a
second year, thereby postponing their next litter by one year. Why light yearlings are cared for
a second year in the northern population, but not in southern population is still an unanswered
question. Future work will hopefully give an answer.
We found that females probably adjust their defense of offspring in relation to their
litter size (Paper IV). Such a relationship has been reported in some species of fish (Carlisle
1985) birds (Windt and Curio 1986) and rodents (Maestripieri and Alleva 1991; Koskela et al.
2000; Jonnson 2002), but not in larger mammals. Furthermore, most of these studies have
used a predator dummy and recorded vigilance and aggressive behavior, whereas we recorded
mortality of offspring and mothers. Sexually selected infanticide might alter the expected

27
pattern, that the probability that 1 or more offspring are lost from a litter increases with
increasing litter size, to a decreasing probability of loosing one or more offspring from a litter
with increasing litter size. I encourage others to investigate this topic in other populations and
other species
Unfortunately the majority of studies of home range size have been purely descriptive,
and at best only analyzed home range size in relation to ecological factors. The common
perception that home range size of females is only related to food might be to simplistic, as
other factors might also be important. We identified sexually selected infanticide and
counterstrategies against it to be important in brown bears (Paper VII), and this might also
influence home range size in other species where sexually selected infanticide occurs (see van
Schaik 2000). In estrous females large ranges in the mating season can be a strategy for mate
selection (Andersson 1994) and/or paternity confusion (Ebensperger 1998). Genetic analyzes
confirm mixed paternity in ? % of the litters with more than 2 cubs (Bellemain et al. unpubl.).
Although sexually selected infanticide was considered to be far less important in the northern
study area, this is probably a temporal phenomenon. In most of the study period few adult
males (for several years only one large adult male) were present in the study area. Due to high
mortality in the surrounding population in the east, south and north and unsuitable mountains
in the west, immigration to the study area was severely limited (Swenson et al. 2001). Several
three-year-old males were able to mate successfully (Bellemain et al. unpubl.), but were
probably less likely to kill cubs of similar sized or larger females. Because sexually selected
infanticide probably has been important in a evolutionary time span, reproductive strategies of
females in the north to avoid infanticide by multimale mating by estrous females and reduced
home range size in the mating season when caring for cubs are likely to be present also during
periods when infanticide is less likely. The low cub mortality in the northern study area might
also suggest that the counterstrategies are effective. Higher cub mortality rates during the two
last years indicates that cub mortality rates likely will increase in the future after some adult
males are shot from a population with a more normal sex structure.
During the coming years the data collected by the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research
Project will allow us to delve deeper into several aspects unexplored in this thesis. Among the
important topics to investigate is the relationship between offspring size and later
reproductive success. That is, does the size difference early in life last into adulthood or does
it vanish due to compensatory growth; and how does size, mass and condition influence
reproductive success? Immobilizing females that we expect are pregnant in the fall would
enable us to explore the relationship between maternal size, mass and condition on litter size

28
and offspring growth and survival. A study of the reproductive physiology of brown bears in
the study populations will start this fall. Also the relationship between yearling body mass,
litter size and length of maternal care should be further analyzed, and preferably such
analyzes should include North American populations. Among North American brown bear
populations, reproductive rate is positively related to body size (Hilderbrand et al. 1999).
European brown bears have higher reproductive rates than North American brown bears
(Sæther et al. 1998), although the European bear is somewhat smaller than most interior bear
populations in North America. This continental difference might be a result of selection for
higher reproduction in Europe due to human persecution during the last millenium in Europe,
whereas in North America the brown bear was wiped out in less than a century, a time span
too short for evolutionary changes in a species with a long generation time.

29
Acknowledgements
I started studying brown bears in 1994 for my Master’s degree with Jon Swenson and Arne
Moksnes as my supervisors. Since Norwegian carnivore “management” has proven to be very
efficient, I had to go to Sweden to carry out the fieldwork. There I spent several months
together with bears, moose and my old no-longer-alive Lada. After finishing my M. Sc. in
1996 I wanted to continue in the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project. However, as
most readers of this thesis know, it is hard to find money to go on with a PhD. Finally, in
spring 1998, I obtained a 4-year position as a research fellow at the Norwegian University of
Science and Technology with Jon and Arne as supervisors. Jon has been a brilliant supervisor,
although he was often extremely busy and stressed, he always gave feedback on manuscripts
quickly. Jon gave me room to develop on my own, but at the same time guided me around
some of the numerous pitfalls that are easy to stumble into. This freedom and the large
amount of data available in the project changed the direction of the thesis to an extent that the
working title back in 1998 would not suit the present thesis very well! I really appreciate your
constructive feedback.
My part of the fieldwork was carried out in Dalarna, from our field station in
Kvarnberg. Together with numerous bears (I will not mention by their name), Sven Brunberg,
Kristin and Bosse Florén and the students, Helena Busk, Andrea Friebe, Wiebke Neumann,
Jonna Katajisto. Kristin Høivik Fossum, Paul Antoni Nilsen, Oddmund Rønning, Solveig
Kristoffersen, Christer Zakrisson, Kalle Wikstrand, Lars Plahn and Torleiv Myhre contributed
to an enjoyable stay. I would also like to thank Robert Franzén for always providing me with
data from the northern study area instantly, and for the helicopter sightseeing in Sarek
National Park when I visited the northern study area. Arne Söderberg and Sven Brunberg
provided data from the southern study area. Much of the data were collected by volunteers,
and I am grateful to you for your impressive work.
In September 2001 we moved to Lillestrøm because my wife Kari obtained a position
at HELTEF. This was positive for my work as well, as I then went to NLH in Ås where Jon
had been since 1999. Here I shared an office with Andreas Zedrosser, who was then about to
start his PhD in the project, and who I knew from previous co-operation on an European
action plan for the conservation of brown bears. Later, in April 2002, Ole-Gunnar Støen
joined the bear group as a PhD student. I appreciate the bear group I became a part of, both
receiving more feedback on my own work and at the same time giving feedback on the work

30
of Jon, Andreas and Ole-Gunnar. We have had some really interesting discussions and I
enjoyed it. I hope that we can continue the good cooperation.
I would also like to thank the other bear students and friends Thomas Johansen and Ole
Opseth and colleagues at the former Department of Zoology at NTNU and Andreas Zedrosser
and Ole-Gunnar Støen and colleagues at the Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
at NLH. You have all made these years enjoyable.
I also appreciate the efforts made by Arne Moksnes to improve the quality of this
thesis, as well as taking care of various administrative tasks during these years. Thor Harald
Ringsby solved some of my statistical problems and introduced me to the AIC. Gunilla
Rosenqvist, Bård Gunnar Stokke, Ole-Gunnar Støen and Andreas Zedrosser read part of this
thesis and gave valuable comments on it.
I want to thank my parents for initialising my interest for nature and biology early in
my childhood when they took me and my siblings out to the mountains, forest and the
seashore, where I spent hours turning over stones in search for all kinds of marine creatures.
Finally, but not least, I would like to thank my wife Kari for encouragement and support, and
together with our children Steinar and Ingrid, keeping me away from the office! However, I
finally finished!

31
References
Agrell J, Wolff JO, Ylönen H (1998) Counter-strategies to infanticide in mammals: cost
and consequences. Oikos 83:507-517
Albon SD, Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE (1987) Early development and population
dynamics in red deer. II. Density-independent effects and cohort variation. J Anim
Ecol 56:69-81
Allaine D, Graziani L, Coulon J (1998) Postweaning mass gain in juvenile alpine marmots
Marmota marmota. Oecologia 113:370-376
Alt GL, Alt FW, Linzey JS (1976) Home range and activity patterns of black bears in
northeastern Pennsylvania. Trans Northeast Sect Wildl Soc 33:45-46
Andersen R, Gaillard JM, Linnell JDC, Duncan P (2000) Factors affecting maternal care in an
income breeder, the European roe deer. J Anim Ecol 69:672-682
Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Baker JD, Fowler CW (1992) Pup weight and survival of northern fur seals Callorhinus
ursinus. J Zool 227:231-238
Bateson P (1994) The dynamics of parent-offspring relationships in mammals. Trends Ecol
Evol 9:399-403
Birgersson B, Ekvall K (1997) Early growth in male and female fallow deer fawns. Behav
Ecol 8:493-499
Blanchard BM (1987) Size and growth patterns of the Yellowstone grizzly bear. Int Conf
Bear Res and Manage 7:99-107
Borries C, Launhardt K, Epplen C, Epplen JT, Winkler P (1999) DNA analyses
support the hypothesis that infanticide is adaptive in langur monkeys. Proc Roy
Soc Lond B 266:901-904
Burt, W. H. (1943). Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to mammals. J
Mammal 24:346-352
Byers JA, Moddide JD (1990) Sex-specific maternal investment in pronghorn, and the
question of a limit on differential provisioning in ungulates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol
26:157-164
Carlisle TR (1985) Parental response to brood size in a cichlid fish. Anim Behav 33:234-238
Charnov EL (1982) The theory of sex allocation. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Andelman SJ, Lee PC (1988) Reproductive success in vervet
monkeys. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success: studies of individual

32
variation in contrasting breeding systems. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp
384-402
Clutton-Brock TH (1989) Mammalian mating systems. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 236:339-372
Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton University Press,
Princeton
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE (1989) Fitness costs of gestation and lactation in
wild mammals. Nature 337:260-262
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE (1988) Reproductive success in male and female
red deer. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success. University of Chicago
Press, Chicago, pp 325-343
Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD (1982) Red deer. Behavior and ecology of the
two sexes. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD (1983) The cost of reproduction to red deer
hinds. J Anim Ecol 52:367-383
Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1978) Mammals, resources and reproductive strategies.
Nature 273:191-195
Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH, Rudder B (1977) Sexual dimorphism, socionomic sex ratio
and body weight in primates. Nature 269:797-800
Cote SD, Peracino A, Simard G (1997) Wolf, Canis lupus, predation and maternal defensive
behavior in Mountain Goats, Oreamnos americanus. Can Field-Nat 111:389-392
Craighead JJ, Sumner JS, Mitchell JA (1995) The grizzly bears of Yellowstone: their ecology
in the Yellowstone ecosystem 1959-1992. Island Press, Washington DC
Curry Lindal K (1972) The brown bear (Ursus arctos) in Europe: decline, present distribution,
biology, and ecology. Int Conf Bear Res and Manage 2:74-80
Derocher AE, Stirling I (1995) Temporal variation in reproduction and body mass of polar
bears in western Hudson Bay. Can J Zool 73:1657-1665
Derocher AE, Stirling I (1998a) Maternal investment and factors affecting offspring size
in polar bears (Ursus maritimus). J Zool 245:253-260
Derocher AE, Stirling I (1998b) Geographic variation in growth of polar bears (Ursus
maritimus). J Zool 245:65-72
Derocher AE, Wiig Ø (1999) Infanticide and cannibalism of juvenile polar bears (Ursus
maritimus) in Svalbard. Arctic 52:307-310
Ebensperger LA (1998) Strategies and counterstrategies to infanticide in mammals.
Biol Rev 73:321-346

33
Egbert AL, Luque MH (1975) Among Alaska’s brown bears. Nat Geogr Mag 148:428-442
Fairbanks LA, McGurie MT (1995) Maternal condition and the quality of maternal care in
vervet monkeys. Behavour 132: 733-754
Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Berube CH, Porter C, Wishhart WD (1997) Body mass and
survival of bighorn sheep. Can J Zool 75:1372-1379
Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Reale D (2000) Early development, adult mass, and
reproductive success in bighorn sheep. Behav Ecol 11:633-639
Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Wishart WD (1994) Early weaning in bighorn sheep, Ovis
canadensis, affects growth of males but not females. Behav Ecol 5:21-27
Fisher DO, Lara MC (1999) Effects of body size and home range on access to mates and
paternity in male bridled wallabies. Anim Behav 58:121-130
Fisher DO, Owens IPF (2000) Female home range size and the evolution of social
organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol 69:1083-1098
Frank SA (1990) Sex allocation theory for birds and mammals. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 21:13-55
Gaillard JM, Festa-Bianchet M, Delorme D, Jorgenson J (2000) Body mass and individual
fitness in female ungulates: bigger is not always better. Proc Roy Soc Lond B
267:471-477
Garshelis DL (1994) Density dependent population regulation of black bears. In: Taylor M
(ed) Density dependent population regulation in black, brown and polar bears. Int
Conf Bear Res Manage Mongr 3:3-14
Georges JY, Guinet C (2001) Prenatal investment in the subantartic fur seal, Arctocephalus
tropicalis. Can J Zool 79:601-609
Gray EM (1997) Do female blackbirds benefit genetically from seeking extra-pair
copulations? Anim Behav 53:605-623
Green WCH, Rothstein A (1991) Trade offs between growth and reproduction in female
bison. Oecologia 86:521-527
Grundsten C 1997 The Laponian area. A Swedish world heritage site. Naturvårdsverket,
Stockholm
Hansson B, Bensch S, Hasselquist D (1997) Infanticide in great reed warblers: secondary
females destroy eggs of primary females. Anim Behav 54:297-304
Hauser MD, Fairbanks LA (1988) Mother-offspring conflict in vervet monkeys: variation in
response to ecological conditions. Anim Behav 36:802-813
Hensel RJ, Troyer WH, Erickson AW (1969) Reproduction in the female brown bear. J
Wildl Manage 33:357-365

34
Herrero S, Hamer D (1977) Courtship and copulation of a pair of grizzly bears, with
comments on reproductive plasticity and strategy. J Mammal 58:441-444
Hewison AJM, Gaillard JM (2001) Phenotypic quality and senescence affect different
components of reproductive output in roe deer. J Anim Ecol 70:600-608
Hilderbrand GV, Schwartz CC, Robbins CT, Hanley TA (2000) Effects of hibernation and
reproductive status on body mass and condition of coastal brown bears. J Wildl
Manage 64:178-183
Hilderbrand GV, Schwartz CC, Robbins CT, Jacoby ME, Hanley TA, Arthur SM, Servheen C
(1999) The importance of meat, particularly salmon, to body size, population
productivity, and conservation of North American brown bears. Can J Zool 77:132-
138
Hirsch JG, Bender LC, Haufler JB (1999) Black bear Ursus americanus movements and home
ranges on Drummond Island, Michigan. Can Field-Nat 113:221-225
Hrdy SB (1979) Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination of the
implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol Sociobiol 1:1-13
Jonsson P, Agrell J, Koskela E, Mappes T (2002) Effects of litter size on pup defence and
weaning success of neighboring bank vole females. Can J Zool 80:1-5
Keech MA, Bowyer RT, VerHoef JM, Bortje RD, Dale BW, Stephenson TR (2000) Life-
history consequences of maternal condition in Alaskan moose. J Wildl Manage
64:450-462
Kolstad M, Kvam T, Mysterud I, Sørensen OJ, Wikan S (1986) Status of the brown bear in
Norway: distribution and population 1978-1982. Biol Conserv 38:79-99
Koskela E, Juutistenaho P, Mappes T, Oksanen TA (2000) Offspring defence in relation to
litter size and age: experiment in the bank vole Clethrionomys glareolus. Evol Ecol
14:99-109
König B, Riesler J, Markl (1988) Maternal care in house mice (Mus musculus) II. The energy
cost of lactation as a function of litter size. J Zool 216:195-210
Kreeger TJ, Arnemo JM, Jacobus PR (2002) Handbook of wildlife and chemical
immobilization, international edition. Wildlife Pharmaceuticals Inc, Fort Collins
Labisky RF, Fritzen DE (1998) Spatial mobility of breeding female white-tailed deer in a low-
density population. J Wildl Manage 62:1329-1334
Lamberti P, Rossi I, Mauri L, Appollonio M (2001) Alternative use of space strategies of
female roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) in a mountainous habitat. Ital J Zool 68:69-73
Lee AK, Cockburn A (1985) Evolutionary ecology of marsupials. Cambridge University

35
Press, Cambridge
Lee PC, Majluf P, Gordon IJ (1991) Growth, weaning and maternal investment from a
comparative perspective. J Zool 225:99-114
Lee PC, Moss CJ (1986) Early maternal investment in male and female African elephant
calves. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:353-361
Liberg O, Johansson A, Andersen R, Linnell JDC (1998) Mating system, mating tactics and
the function of male territoriality in roe deer. In Andersen R, Duncan P, Linnell JDC
(eds) The European roe deer: the biology of success. Scandinavian University Press,
Oslo, pp 221-256
Lindström J (1999) Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends Ecol
Evol 14:343-348
Lindzey FG, Meslow EC (1977) Home range and habitat use by black bears in southwestern
Washington. J Wildl Manage 41:413-425
Lingle S, Pellis SM (2002) Fight or flight? Antipredator behavior and the escalation of coyote
encounters with deer. Oecologia 131:154-164
Litvaitis JA, Sherburn JA, Bissonette JA (1986) Bobcat habitat use and home range size in
relation to prey density. J Wildl Manage 50:110-117
Lundqvist, R. 2002. Fulufjället. Nationalpark i Dalafjällen. Naturvårdsverket, Stockholm
Lycett JE, Henzi SP, Barrett L (1998) Maternal investment in mountain baboons and the
hypothesis of reduced care. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:49-56
Maestripieri D (1992) Functional aspects of maternal aggression in mammals. Can J Zool
70:1069-1077
Maestripieri D, Alleva E (1991) Litter defense and parental investment allocation in house
mice. Behav Proces 23:223-230
Mares MA, Lacher TE Jr, Willig MR, Bitar NA (1982) An experimental analysis of social
spacing in Tamias striatus. Ecology 63:267-273
Mattson DJ, Knight RR, Blanchard BM (1992) Cannibalism and predation on black bears by
Grizzly bears in the Yellowstone Ecosystem, 1975–1990. J Mammal 73:422–425
Maynard-Smith J (1980) A new theory of sexual investment. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:247-
251
McElligott AG, Gammell MP, Harty HC, Paini DR, Murphy DT, Walsh JT, Hayden TJ
(2001). Sexual size dimorphism in fallow deer (Dama dama): do larger, heavier males
gain greater mating success? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:266-272
McLellan B (1994) Density dependent population regulation of brown bears. In Taylor M ed.

36
Density dependent population regulation in black, brown and polar bears. Int Conf
Bear Res Manage Mongr 3:15-24
McLellan B, Hovey FW, Mace RD, Woods JG, Carney DW, Gibeau ML, Wakkinen WL,
Kasworm WF (1999) Rates and causes of grizzly bear mortality in the interior
mountains of British Columbia, Alberta, Montana, Washington and Idaho. J Wildl
Manage 63:911-920
McLoughlin PD, Case RL, Gau RJ, Ferguson SH, Messier F (1999) Annual and seasonal
movement patterns of barren-ground grizzly bears in the central Northwest
Territories. Ursus 11:79-86
McNab BK (1963) Bioenergetics and the determination of home range size. Am Nat 162:
133-140
Mendl M (1994) How should parents respond to a reduction in litter size abandonment or
extra care? Behav Process 31:285-291
Mellish JE, Iverson SJ, Bowen WD, Hammill MO (1999) Fat transfer and energetics during
lactation in the hooded seals: the roles of tissue lipoprotein in milk fat secretion and
pup blubber deposition. J Comp Physiol B 169:377-390
Miller SD (1994) Black bear reproduction and cub survivorship in south-central Alaska. Int
Conf Bear Res and Manage 9:263-273
Mock DW, Parker GA (1997) The evolution of sibling rivalry. Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Mordosov II (1993) Yakutia. In: Vaisfeld MA, Chestin IE (eds) Bears- brown bear, polar
bear, Asian black bear. Nauka, Moscow, pp 301–318 (In Russian with English
summary)
Murie A (1981) The grizzlies of Mount McKinley. U. S. Department of the Interior,National
Park Service, Washington D C
Myers P, Master LL (1983) Reproduction by Peromyscus maniculatus: size and compromize.
J Mammal 64:1-18
Nagy JA, Haroldson MA (1990) Comparisons of some home range and population parameters
among four grizzly bear populations in Canada. Int Conf Bear Res and Manage 8:227-
235
Oftedal OT, Gittleman JL (1989) Patterns of energy output during reproduction in carnivores.
In: Gittleman JL (ed) Carnivore behaviour, ecology, and evolution 1. Cornell
University Press, New York, pp 355-378
Ostfeld RS (1986) Territoriality and mating system of California voles. J Anim Ecol 55:691-

37
706
Packer C, Pusey AE (1983) Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming males.
Am Nat 121:716-728
Packer C, Pusey AE (1984) Infanticide in carnivores. In: Hausfater G, Hrdy SB (eds)
Infanticide: comparative and evolutionary perspectives. Aldine Publishing Co, New
York, pp 31-42
Palomares F (1994) Site fidelity and effects of body mass on home-range size of Egyptian
mongooses. Can J Zool 72:465-469
Pasitschniak-Arts M (1993) Ursus arctos. Mammalian Species 439:1-10
Perrins CM, McCleery RH (2001) The effect of fledging mass on the lives of great tits Parus
major. Ardea 89:135-142
Powell RA, Zimmerman JW, Seaman DE (1997). Ecology and behaviour of North American
black bears. Chapman and Hall, New York
Pusey AE, Packer C (1994) Infanticide in lions: consequences and counterstrategies. In:.
Parmigiani S, von Saal F (eds) Infanticide and parental care, Hardwood, London, pp
279-299
Reguera P, Gomendio M (1999) Predation costs associated with parental care in the golden
egg bug Phyllomorpha laciniata (Heteroptera: Coreidae). Behav Ecol 10:541-544
Reguera P, Gomendio M (2001) Egg carrying in the golden egg bug (Phyllomorpha
laciniata): parental care, parasitism, or both? Reply to Kaitala et al. Behav Ecol
12:369-373
Renfree MB, Calaby JH (1981) Background to delayed implantation and embryonic diapause.
J Reprod Fert Suppl 29:1-9.
Rogers LL (1987) Effects of food supply and kinship on social behaviour, movements, and
population dynamics of black bears in northeastern Minnesota. Wildl Monogr 97:1-72
Rogers LL (1976) Effects of mast and berry crop failures on survival, growth and productive
success of black bears. Trans North Am Wildl Nat Res 41:431-438
Rogers LL (1977) Movements and social organization of black bears in northeastern
Minnesota. Ph.D. Thesis. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis
Rootes WL, Chabreck RH (1993) Reproductive status and movements of adult female
alligators. J Herpet 27: 21-126
Sandegren F, Swenson JE (1997) Björnen - viltet, ekologin och människan. Svenska
Jägareförbundet, Uppsala (In Swedish)
Sandell M (1989) The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. In: Gittleman

38
JL (ed) Carnivore behaviour, ecology, and evolution 1. Cornell University Press, New
York, pp 164-182
San Jose C, Lovari S (1998) Ranging movements of female roe deer: do home-loving does
roam to mate? Ethology 104:721-728
Schultz SR, Johnson MK (1995) Effects of birth date and body mass at birth on adult body
mass of male white-tailed deer. J Mammal 76:575-579
Schwartz CC, Franzmann AW (1991) Interrelationship of black bears to moose and forest
succession in the northern coniferous forest. Wildl Monogr 113: 1-58
Servheen C, Herrero S, Peyton B (compilers) (1999) Bears. Status and conservation action
plan. IUCN/SSC Switzerland and Cambridge
Smith CC, Fretwell SD (1974) The optimal balance between size and number of offspring.
Am Nat 108:499-506
Smith HG, Wennerberg L, von Schantz T (1996) Adoption or infanticide: options of
replacement males in the European starling. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:191-197
Smirnov MN, Shurygin VV (1991) Buryi medved’ v Tuve. In: Zavaskii BP, Shvetsov YG
(eds) Medvedi v SSSR. Nauka, Novosibirsk, pp 162-170 (In Russian)
Soltis J, Thomsen R, Matsubayashi K, Takenaka O (2000) Infanticide by resident males and
female counterstrategies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behav Ecol
Sociobiol 48:195-202
Soroker V, Terkel J (1988) Changes in incidence of infanticidal and parental responses during
the reproductive cycle in male and female wild mice (Mus musculus). Anim Behav
36:1275-1281
Stirling I, Lunn NJ, Iacozza J (1999) Long term trends in the population ecology of polar
bears in western Hudson Bay in relation to climatic change. Artic 52:294-306
Stockley P, Purvis A (1993) Sperm competition in mammals – a comparative-study of male
roles and relative investment in sperm production. Funct Ecol 7:560-570
Swenson JE, Sandegren F, Bjärvall A, Wabakken P, Söderberg A, Franzen R (1997)
Infanticide caused by hunting of male bears. Nature 386:450-451
Swenson JE, Sandegren F, Segerström P, Brunberg S (2001) Factors associated with loss of
brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12:69-80
Swenson JE, Wabakken P, Sandegren F, Bjärvall A, Franzen R, Söderberg A (1995) The near
extinction and recovery of brown bears in Scandinavia in relation to the bear
management policies in Norway and Sweden. Wildl Biol 1:11-25
Tait DE (1980) Abandonment as a reproductive tactic – the example with grizzly bears.

39
Am Nat 115:800-808
Ternent MA, Garshelis DL (1998) Male–instigated break-up of a family of black bears. Ursus
10:575-578
Trillmich F (1986) Maternal investment and sex allocation in the Galapagos fur seal,
Arctocepahalus galapagoensis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:157-164
Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B (ed) Sexual
selection and the descent of man. Aldine, Chicago, pp 136-179
Trivers RL (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool 14:249-264
Tsubota T, Kanagawa H, Mano T, Aoi T (1989) Corpora albicantia and placental scars in the
Hokkaido brown bear. Int Conf Bear Res and Manage 8:125-128
Tufto J, Andersen R, Linnell JCD (1996) Habitat use and ecological correlates of home range
size in a small cervid: the roe deer. J Anim Ecol 65:715-725
van Schaik CP (2000) Vulnerability to infanticide by males: patterns among mammals. In:
van Schaik CP, Janson CH (eds) Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge pp 61-71
Verboven N, Tinbergen JM (2002) Nest desertion: a trade-off between current and future
reproduction. Anim Behav 63:951-958
Wielgus RB, Bunnel FL (1995) Test of hypotheses for sexual segregation in grizzly bears.
J Wildl Manage 59:552-560
Wilbur HM (1977) Propagule size, number, and dispersal patterns in Ambystoma and
Asclepias. Am Nat 111:43-68
Wilson N, Tubman SC, Eady PE, Robertson GW (1997) Female genotype affects male
success in sperm competition. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 264:1491-1495
Windt W, Curio E (1986) Clutch defence in great tit (Parus major) pairs and the Concorde
fallacy. Ethology 72:236-242
Winkler DW (1987) A general model of parental care. Am Nat 130:526-543
Wauters L, Bijnens L, Dhondt AA (1993) Body mass at weaning and local recruitment in the
red squirrel. J Anim Ecol 62:280-286
Wolff JO, Schauber EM (1996) Space use and juvenile recruitment in gray-tailed voles in
response to intruder pressure and food abundance. Acta Ther 41:35-43
Yorio P, Boersma PD (1994) Causes of nest desertation during incubation in the Magellanic
penguin (Spheniscus magellanicus). Condor 96:1076-1083
Zedrosser A, Dahle B, Swenson JE, Gerstl N (2001) Status and management of the brown
bear in Europe. Ursus 12:9-20

40
Paper I

Manuscript

Body size and mass relationships in yearling brown bears: effects of


maternal size litter size, sex, cohort and population density.

By

B Dahle and JE Swenson

I seek to convince you that any biological study must first consider size as the most
important characteristics of an animal
-Calder
Body size and mass relationships in yearling brown bears: effects of
maternal size, litter size, sex, cohort and population density.

Bjørn Dahle, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural


University of Norway, Postbox 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway

Jon E. Swenson, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural


University of Norway, Postbox 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway and Norwegian Institute for
Nature Research, Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway

Short title: Brown bear body mass and size relationship

Correspondence author:
Bjørn Dahle
Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
Agricultural University of Norway
Post Box 5014
N-1432 Ås
e-mail: bjorn.dahle@ibn.nlh.no

1
Abstract
Body size and mass have a strong effect on an individuals’ fitness, and conditions experienced
early in life may affect survival to adulthood, age and size at maturation and reproductive
success. For this reason body size and mass of yearling brown bears (Ursus arctos) were
analysed in relation to maternal size, litter size, sex, cohort and population density in two
study areas in Sweden. Body mass of yearlings varied from 8.5 to 48 kg ( X = 24.5 ± 0.5 (SE)
kg). As predicted, yearling body size and mass were positively related to maternal size and
negatively related to litter size. Males were on average larger and heavier than females,
suggesting that mothers invest more in their male offspring in accordance with the sex
allocation theory for polygynous species, although we could not rule out the possibility that
males just ate more solid food than females. Yearling body mass showed a pronounced
variance among cohorts, by a factor of 1.7. The difference among cohorts was probably a
result of changing food abundance among years thereby influencing maternal condition and
investment, as well as the offsprings’ intake of solid food, but data on food availability was
not available to evaluate this. No significant relationship was found between population
density and yearling body size or mass, but population density was probably below carrying
capacity. Much of the variance in body mass of yearlings was due to variation within litters,
especially in litters with 3 offspring, where the heaviest yearling was on average 29.5 ± 2.8
(SE) % heavier than the lightest one. This suggests that competition among offspring
increases with litter size, thereby having a pronounced negative effect on the smallest
offspring. Survival of radiomarked subadult brown bears tended to increase with increasing
body size.

Key words: body mass, maternal effects, population density, litter size, Ursus arctos

2
Introduction
Body mass is one of the most important factors affecting an individual’s fitness. Body mass at
birth or weaning/fledging is usually positively associated with early survival (e.g. Baker &
Fowler, 1992; Festa-Bianchet et al., 1997), and even with survival to adulthod (e.g. Albon,
Clutton-Brock & Guinness, 1987). Additionally, body mass at birth or weaning is reported to
be positively correlated with body mass later in life (Schultz & Johnson, 1995; Birgersson &
Ekvall, 1997), size as adults (Myers & Master, 1983; Albon et al., 1987; Festa-Bianchet,
Jorgenson & Reale, 2000), and lifetime reproductive success (Festa-Bianchet et al., 2000;
Steinheim et al., 2002). Thus, conditions experienced during early development influence
growth, survival and later reproduction in birds and mammals (Lindström, 1999). Body mass
measurements of offspring just prior to the separation from their mothers varies considerably
in many species e.g. alpine marmots (Marmota marmota), (Allaine, Graziani & Coulon, 1998)
and polar bears (Ursus maritimus) (Derocher & Stirling, 1998). Because body mass may have
a pronounced effect on survival and reproductive success it is important to explain this
variation.
Brown bears (Ursus arctos) exhibit large seasonal fluctuations in body mass because
they store adipose tissue during summer and fall for winter hibernation (Hildebrand et al.,
2000). Thus, body mass of brown bears is a function of both skeletal size and body condition
(both muscles and stored fat). For this reason we analysed skeletal size (hereafter body size)
and body mass separately, although most previous studies only have focused on body mass.
We considered 5 factors that are likely to affect body size and mass. Maternal size.
Larger females may produce larger and heavier offspring (Myers & Master 1983, Clutton-
Brock, Albon & Guinness, 1988; Wauters, Bijnens & Dhondt, 1993; Arnbom, Fedak & Boyd,
1997) and offspring of larger mothers may show increased pre weaning growth (Myers &
Master, 1983, Arnbom et al., 1997; Barbraud et al., 1999). Most studies have used maternal
mass as the maternal trait (citations). We measured mothers and offspring near the end of
maternal investment. No measurement of maternal body mass was available at implantation or
birth. Due to ethical reasons females were not captured in spring when they leave the den,
because capture them might have increased cub mortality. For this reason we used maternal
size as the maternal trait in the analyses. We predicted (1) a positive relationship between
maternal size and offspring size and mass.
Litter size. Life history theory predicts a compromise between the number and size of
offspring (Smith & Fretwell, 1974), and such a negative relationship has been reported in a

3
wide variety of taxa (see Lloyd, 1987 and Roff, 1992 for reviews). Thus we predicted (3) a
negative relationship between litter size and body size and mass.
Sex. It is generally accepted that in species where the variance in reproductive success
is greater in one sex and where parental investment influences reproductive success, parents
should invest more in the sex with the highest variance, which in polygynous species usually
is the male (Trivers, 1972; Maynard-Smith, 1980). In brown bears, little is known about the
relationship between body size and mass early in life and in adulthood. The assumption that
parental investment influences reproductive success therefore remains untested. However,
since adult males are on average twice as heavy as females, body size and mass are suspected
to positively affect male mating success (LeFrank et al., 1987), as is reported for other
polygynous species (e.g. McElligott et al., 2001). On this background we predicted (2) that
yearling males should be larger and heavier than females, both within and among litters.
Cohort. Changing food availability among years might have a pronounced effect on
maternal condition and the transfer of resources from mother to offspring, thus affecting
offspring growth rate and body mass (e.g. Ono, Boness & Oftedal, 1987; Allaine, Graziani &
Coulon, 1998). Additionally, cubs feed on solid food especially after the lactation peak
around midsummer (Farley & Robbins 1995). Preliminary analyses suggest that yearling body
mass differs among cohorts (Swenson et al., 2001a). Although no measurements exist from
the study areas food availability most likely varies among years because food habits seem to
show such yearly variation (Opset, 1998; Nilsen, 2002). For this reason we predicted (5) that
body size and mass of yearlings should differ among cohorts.
Population density. In general, an increase in population density will increase
competition for food. This might result in a decrease in body mass as have been reported in
ungulates (e.g. Hjeljord & Histøl, 1999) and birds (e.g. Cooch et al., 1991), but this have to
our knowledge not been demonstrated in bears. Studying a continuos population ranging from
low to high density, we predicted (4) that body size and mass should decrease along a gradient
of increasing population density.
Most studies concerning offspring size have explained offspring size or mass in
relation to environmental conditions, maternal traits, litter size and sex (e.g. Allaine et al.
1998, but see Gaillard et al., 1998) and rarely focused on the variation in offspring size within
litters. Comparisons of offspring within litters should also be a powerful way to test for sex
differences because it removes the “noise” introduced by other factors. Gaillard et al. (1998)
reported that variation in growth within litters increased with litter size in roe deer (Caperolus
capreolus), and in polar bears Derocher & Stirling (1998) found that the difference in body

4
mass between siblings in triplet litters was approximately 3-6 times that found in twins. They
argued that the disparity in mass of individuals in triplets might represent competition among
siblings for milk, because mothers may be constrained in their ability to control the variation
in offspring size. Although brown bears have 6 functional mammae, compared to 4 in polar
bears, litter size in brown bears is on average larger than in polar bears (Derocher & Taylor
1994, McLellan 1994). Based on the findings of Derocher & Stirling (1998) and Gaillard et
al., (1998) we predicted (6) that variation in offspring size and mass within litters increases
with increasing litter size.
Early survival (Baker & Fowler, 1992; Derocher & Stirling, 1996) and survival to
adulthood (Albon et al., 1987; Festa Bianchet et al., 1997) is generally positively associated
with offspring size, so we predicted (7) that subadult survival (1 to 3 years old) should be
positively related to yearling body size and mass.

Methods
Study areas
The study was performed in Dalarna and Gävleborg counties, south central Sweden (61o N,
18o E) and southwestern Norrbotten County in northern Sweden (67° N, 18° E). The
landscape is covered with coniferous forest, dominated by Scots pine (Pinus sylvestris) or
Norway spruce (Picea abies), but in the northern study area mountains rise to over 2000 m,
and subalpine forest dominated by birch (Betula. pubescens) and willows (Salix spp.) are
common. Both areas are further described in Dahle & Swenson (2003a). In the southern study
area, 95% of the litters are weaned as yearlings (Dahle & Swenson 2003b) whereas only 53%
of the litters are weaned as yearlings in the North (Dahle & Swenson 2003c).

Capture and handling


Yearling brown bears and their mothers were immobilised from a helicopter in mid-April in
the southern study area and early May in the northern study area, shortly after den emergence.
We used 2.5 mg Tiletamin, 2.5 mg Zolazepam and 0.02 mg Medetomidin per kg to
immobilize the bears. Atipamezol was used as an antidote for Medetomidin (5 mg per 1 mg
Medetomidin) (Kreeger, Arnemo & Raath 2002). Until 1998 most yearlings were equipped
with radiotransmitters mounted on neck collars or implanted into the peritoneal cavity.
Immobilised bears were weighed and skull circumference (measured in front of the ears) and
body length (from the tip of the nose to the tail root) were measured with a tape measure. We

5
combined body length and scull circumference through a principal component analysis to
produce an index of skeletal body size of yearlings (hereafter body size) and an index of
skeletal size of mothers (hereafter maternal size). This procedure is likely to be a more
reliable indicator of skeletal size than a single measure, such as body length (Green, 2001),
and should reflect skeletal dimensions, rather independent of body condition. Because the
bears were captured within a 2-week period in each study area, we did not adjust body size or
mass for capture date.
Cub mortality was higher in the southern than the northern study area, but in both
areas most of the cub mortality took place in May-June (Swenson et al., 1997; 2001b), before
the lactation peak during summer (Farley & Robbins, 1995). For this reason statistical
analyses were carried out twice, using both litter size in spring (counted after leave the den)
and in fall (before den entrance). When unmarked females with yearlings were captured, the
number of yearlings present was assumed to reflect the number of cubs present the previous
fall.

Relative population density estimates


In Sweden, female bears are concentrated in four geographically isolated areas termed
reproductive areas (Swenson et al., 1994). In general, the relative density is halved about
every 24 km from the centre of a reproductive area towards the edge (Swenson, Sandegren &
Söderberg, 1998). This density gradient is a result of a sustained increase in population size,
both in density and in distribution (Swenson et al., 1995; Swenson et al., 1998). The
population growth rate was about 16% annually during the period 1985-1995 (Sæther et al.,
1998), but decreased to about 3% afterwards, due to an increase in bear hunting offtake
(Swenson et al., unpubl. data). During the study period (1985-2002) the population has
expanded its distribution (Swenson et al., unpubl. data) so we estimated the increase in
population density to be 10% annually in the period 1989-1995 and 2% thereafter. We
combined the spatial (see Dahle & Swenson, 2003a) and temporal density aspects to produce
a relative density index for each bear at a given distance from the centre of the reproductive
area in a given year.

Statistical analyses
Because we wanted to examine the relationship between body mass and several categorical
and covariate factors, we used General Liner Models (GLM) in our analyses. To select the
most appropriate model we used the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AICc, Akaike, 1973,

6
Burnham & Anderson, 1998), corrected for small sample sizes, which is based on the
2 K ( K + 1)
( )
principle of parsimony. AICc is defined as AICc = n log σˆ 2 + 2 K +
n − K −1
, where K is the

( )
number of parameters estimated in the model, and σ̂ 2 =
RSS
n
, where RSS is the residual

sum of squares. The model with the lowest AICc is assumed to be the one explaining most of
the variation by using fewest parameters, but models with ∆ AICc < 2 should receive
consideration in making inferences (Burnham & Anderson, 1998). Two-tailed tests were used,
and an α level of 0.05 was selected for statistical significance. SPSS/Win v. 10.0 (SPSS Inc.,
Illinois, USA) was used in all statistical analyses. Several females produced more than one
litter during the study, indicating pseudoreplication. However, litter size varied among
reproductive events, as well did the size and mass of yearlings. Because the cohort effect was
also of particular interest in this study we regarded pseudoreplication to be a minor problem.

Results
We obtained body measurements from 227 yearlings (133 in the south and 94 in the north)
and their respective mothers during 1989-2002 (Table 1). Yearling mass ranged from 8 to 48
kg ( X = 24.5 ± 0.5 (SE)), and body length and head circumference varied from 75 to 133 cm
( X = 104.1 ± 0.7) and 36 to 48 cm ( X = 40.3 ± 0.2), respectively. There was a strong linear
relationship between body mass and size (F1,223 = 501.12, R2 = 0.693, P < 0.001, Fig. 1) and
the slope of the regression lines did not differ between the sexes (F = 0.226, df = 1, P = 0.64).
A set of candidate models (GLM) explaining body size and body mass was selected based on
the predicted relationships and ranked by their AICc values. Yearlings captured in 1989-1991
were excluded from these analyses due to the small annual sample sizes. Yearling body size
and mass were positively related to maternal size and negatively related to litter size (Tables
2-3). The model including litter size in spring and the model including litter size in fall were
quite similar, but the latter gave a somewhat better explanation of the variance. Males were
larger and heavier than females.
Body mass of yearlings varied among cohorts and did not co-vary in the 2 study areas
(Table 3, model 3, ∆AICc = 0.884), whereas including cohort, study area and the cohort
*study area interaction term in the model for body size, had an ∆AICc of 22.394, suggesting
that this model was an overfitting of the data (Table 2, model 3). The cohort effect and the
cohort*study area interaction explained a large proportion of the variation in yearling body

7
mass (Table 2-3, Fig. 2). The estimated marginal mean body mass in a study area differed
among some years by a magnitude of 1.7 (Fig. 2).
Population density estimates were only available in the southern area, so we reran the
GLM for the southern area including population density as a covariate. Population density did
not explain a significant amount of the variation in body size or mass (P > 0.1).
We obtained body measurements from all littermates in 40 twin and 37 triplet litters
for analysis of within-litter variation. For simplicity we focused on body mass only. In twins,
the heaviest yearling ( X = 27.9 ± 1.2 kg) was on average 2.8 ± 0.4 kg heavier than the
lightest yearling ( X = 25.2 ± 1.2 kg, paired t = 6.61, df = 39, P < 0.001). The average
difference between the heaviest and lightest littermate in twins was not related to the sex ratio
in the litter (male litter mates, X = 3.1 ± 0.8 kg, n = 12, female littermates, X = 1.9 ± 1 kg, n
= 10, or mixed sex, X = 3.0 ± 0.5 kg, n = 18, F2,37 = 0.69, P = 0.51). In twins of mixed sex,
males were heavier than females by an average of 2.3 ± 0.7 (SE) kg (paired t = 3.42, df = 17,
P = 0.003). In triplet litters there was also variation in body mass within a litter (GLM
repeated measurement, F2,35, = 101.1, P < 0.001). The mean mass of the heaviest yearling was
25.9 ± 0.8 kg, the middle yearling 23.5 ± 0.8 kg, and the lightest 20.3 ± 0.8 kg, all of which
were different (P < 0.001). In 17 of 24 triplets of mixed sex, the heaviest yearling was a male,
different from the expected 50:50 (χ2 = 4.167, P = 0.041). The average difference in body
mass between the lightest and the heaviest littermate was larger in triplets than in twins (5.6 ±
0.5 kg or 29.5 ± 2.8 % and 2.8 ± 0.4 kg or 12.2 ± 2.2 %, respectively, t = 4.48, df = 75, P <
0.001). Body mass of all littermates in quadruplets was only obtained in 2 litters, but the
difference between the heaviest and lightest yearling was 50 and 67% in these litters.
During the period 1989-1998, 121 yearlings were radiomarked. Thirteen died at ages
of 1-3 years from intraspecific predation or unknown causes (all causes except human induced
mortality). Survival tended to increase with body size as a yearling (Wald = 3.35, df = 1, R =
0.125, P = 0.067), although body mass seemed to be less important (Wald = 2.60, df = 1, R =
0.086, P = 0.107).

Discussion
As predicted (1) body size and mass of yearlings was positively related to maternal size,
which is consistent with the findings in many mammals (e.g. polar bears Derocher & Stirling,
1998, red deer Cervus elaphus Clutton-Brock et al., 1988; southern elephant seals Mirounga

8
leonina Arnbom et al., 1997, roe deer Andersen et al. 2000). Although large mothers probably
invest more resources in their offspring in absolute terms, their relative investment might be
less than that of smaller mothers (Arnbom et al., 1997). A positive relationship between
offspring and maternal size might be a result of maternal effects, genetic influences or both
(Barbaurd et al., 1999), but these factors could not be distinguished here.
Body size and mass decreased with increasing litter size as predicted (2), and this is
consistent with the trade-off between number and size of offspring (Smith & Fretwell 1974;
Lloyd, 1987) which has been reported in several species (e.g. Foltz, Hoogland & Koscielny,
1988; Allaine et al., 1998; Derocher & Stirling, 1998). Within-litter variance in offspring
body mass was larger in litters with 3-4 than 2 offspring as predicted (6). Derocher & Stirling
(1998) reported the same pattern in polar bear cubs, and suggested that this was a result of
increasing competition for limited maternal resources (milk), and because mothers might be
constrained in their ability to control the variation in offspring size (McGinley, Temme &
Geber, 1987). Thus, from an offspring’s point of view, the size and mass disadvantage of
being born in a large litter is dependent upon its competitive ability.
As predicted (3) we found that males were on average larger and heavier than females
among litters and analyses of within-litter variation revealed that in a litter of mixed sex the
heaviest yearling usually was a male, supporting the idea that mothers should invest more in
males than females in sexually dimorphic polygynous species (Maynard-Smith 1980).
Derocher & Stirling (1998) reported that body mass of polar bear cubs in fall when 8-11
months old, was not related to sex except in single-cub litters where males were heavier than
females. We measured yearlings that were 16 months old, but they had been hibernating
during the last 5.5-6.5 months, and thus the period under maternal investment should be
similar. Derocher & Stirling (1998) argued that biased maternal investment in male polar bear
cubs might be impossible because juvenile mortality seemed to be dependent on juvenile size
(Derocher, Andriashek & Arnould, 1993, Derocher & Stirling, 1996), and investing
disproportionally in males could increase mortality of female cubs. Although survival of
subadults (1-3 years old) tended to be positively related to body size as predicted (7),
mortality in brown bear cubs, at least in the southern study area, seem to be more related to
social factors (sexually selected infanticide) than environmental conditions (Swenson et al.,
1997; 2001b; Zedrosser et al., unpubl. data). Thus, mothers could probably invest more in
male than female offspring without sacrificing survival of female offspring. Analysis of
maternal investment patterns in brown bears when the offspring are 1.4 years old may be
confounded by sibling competition, sex-biased feeding patterns of solid food (as indicated for

9
bison (Bison bison Wolff 1988) and white tailed deer (Verme 1989), or differences in
allocation of resources to structural growth and adipose tissue (Arnould, Boyd & Socha,
1996). The latter explanation is nevertheless unlikely, as the slopes of the regression lines
between body size and body mass did not differ between males and females. Green and
Rothstein (1991) argued that sexual size dimorphism in bison probably is mostly a result of
different postweaning growth rates and a longer period of growth in males (10 years) than in
females (6 years). Since the sexual size and mass differences we recorded in yearling brown
bears were small, questions arise as to their significance in terms of total maternal investment.
The cohort effect in yearling body mass (prediction 4) was probably related to
different food availability. Brown bears, which are typical capital breeders, largely relies on
stored resources for reproduction. Thus, early maternal expenditure is dependent on the food
availability the summer and especially the fall when pregnant females add adipose tissue and
muscles for meeting her own metabolic needs as well as the energetic costs associated with
gestation and lactation during hibernation. The food availability the year cubs are born
influence lactation during summer and fall as well as the offsprings’ own feeding. For this
reason it is likely that abundant food in two following years will result in a cohort with heavy
yearlings. Food availability is likely influenced by climatic conditions, but how they
influences the various food resources important for bears (moose Alces alces, semi domestic
reindeer Rangifer tarandus, ants Formica spp and Camponotus spp, grass and forbs, and
berries Vaccinium myrtillus, V. Vitis-idaea and Empetrum spp Dahle et al., 1998, Persson,
2000) might be complex and cannot be evaluated here.
Our results revealed that environmental variation among years and between the study
areas had a stronger impact on body mass than body size of yearlings. Body mass is a
function of body size and body condition, the latter thus being more sensitive to food
availability. Similar results have been reported for juvenile adders (Vipera berus) ( Forsman
& Erik, 1996) and juvenile greater snow geese (Anser caerulescens) (Reed & Plante, 1997).
Swenson et al. (2001a) reported that brown bear yearlings in the north were heavier than in
the south, whereas we did not find any difference in yearling body mass between the areas.
However, our sample size was considerably larger and collected over a longer time period,
which would dampen the cohort effects on the average body mass. Also there may have been
temporal dynamics in body mass with a greater difference between the study areas early in the
study than later.
If food abundance was similar throughout the entire study area we would expect that
individuals in areas with higher population densities should have been smaller and lighter than

10
those living at lower densities, because the per capita food abundance should decrease with
increasing population density. Contrary to prediction (5) body size and mass in yearlings was
not influenced by population density, at least at the densities observed in this study.
Population density was probably below carrying capacity because the population growth rate
was high (Sæther et al., 1998). Similarly, Andersen & Linnel (1997) reported that a fourfold
increase in population density did not affect postnatal growth rates in roe deer. When
population density increases, there may also be a selection for heavier offspring (Clutton-
Brock et al., 1988), and as we measure offspring body mass in yearlings, offspring born small
may not have survived to yearling age. However this is not likely because no significant
relationship has been found between population density and cub mortality (Swenson et al.
2001b).
We have demonstrated that size and mass of yearlings are influenced by a number of
factors, but how offspring size and mass influence their later survival and reproduction is not
well understood. Heavier offspring are often reported to survive better than small ones in
many species e.g. red deer (Albon et al., 1987); bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis) (Festa
Bianchet et al., 1997), polar bear (Derocher & Stirling, 1996). Although we measured
offspring size and mass as yearlings we assume that their size reflects their size as cubs. In the
southern study population the major cause of cub mortality seemed to be infanticide by males
(Swenson et al., 1997; 2001b). There, cub survival was positively related to litter size,
probably because females defended large litters more than small litters, whereas in the
northern study area where infanticide was less common (Swenson et al., 1997; 2001b), cub
mortality seemed to be rather independent of litter size (Zedrosser et al., unpubl. data).
Because offspring size decreases with litter size the relationship between cub size and survival
might be complex and differ among populations. Survival from the age of 1 to 3 years was
weakly related to yearling size. Intraspecific predation caused most of the natural death
among subadults, and some of the bears that died of unknown causes might also have been
killed by other bears (Swenson et al. 2001a). Perhaps small subadults are easier to capture and
kill than larger ones.
The difference in body size and mass as yearlings might disappear with age if small
yearlings show compensatory growth. Sikes (1998) reported that size differences at weaning
in northern grasshopper mice (Onychomys leucogaster) did not persist into adulthood due to
compensatory growth in small offspring. However, weaned alpine marmots did not show
compensatory growth (Allaine et al., 1998). In species with multi year growth, such as
bighorn sheep (Festa Bianchet et al., 2000) and alpine ibex (Capra ibex ibex) (Toïgo, Gaillard

11
& Michallet, 1999), compensatory growth might occur in females (associated with delayed
maturity), but not in males, because they probably are selected to always gain as much mass
as possible (Festa Bianchet et al., 2000). On the other hand, Atkinson, Stirling & Ramsay
(1996) found that body length of 2.6-2.9 year old polar bears was correlated with adult size in
females, but not in males, suggesting that compensatory growth occurred in males but not in
females.
In bears, age of first reproduction appears to be the reproductive parameter that is most
sensitive to environmental conditions (Noyce & Garshelis, 1994), and unless compensatory
growth occurs, small yearlings will mature later, which has a negative impact on their
reproductive success. This might be of special importance in the studied populations where
expected lifetime is less than the average age at first reproduction, due to a high hunting
pressure (Swenson et al., unpubl data).
Dahle & Swenson (2003c) analysed length of maternal care in relation to offspring
size and litter size in the northern study population and found that the probability that mothers
cared for offspring for an additional year beyond the yearling age increased with decreasing
body mass of yearlings. They speculated that yearlings were almost always weaned in more
southern populations because yearlings are larger in more southern populations (Swenson, et
al. unpubl.). However, as we did not find any difference in size or mass between the two study
areas, size or mass of yearlings per se is not able to explain the difference in length of
maternal care in the two populations
We concluded that maternal size positively affects size and mass of yearlings and that
yearling size and mass were negatively related to litter size. Yearling males were larger and
heavier than females similar to findings in most sexually dimorph polygynous species. Body
mass of yearlings varied significantly among cohorts, whereas population density in the range
we recorded did not seem to affect yearling body size or mass.

Acknowledgements
This study was funded by the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, the Swedish
Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management, the
Swedish Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, WWF Sweden and the Research
Council of Norway. All animal experimentation reported in this paper complies with the
current laws regulating the treatment of animals in Sweden and Norway and were approved
by the appropriate ethical committees in both countries.

12
References
Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory as an extension of the maximum likelihood principle.
In Second international symposium on information theory: 267-281. Petrov, B.N. &
Csaki, F. (Eds.) Budapest: Akademiai Kiado.
Albon, S. D., Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Guinness, F. E. (1987). Early development and
population dynamics in red deer. II. Density-independent effects and cohort variation.
J. Anim Ecol. 56: 69-81
Allaine, D., Graziani, L. & Coulon, J. (1998). Postweaning mass gain in juvenile alpine
marmots Marmota marmota. Oecologia 113: 370-376.
Andersen, R. & Linnell, J. D. C. (1997). Variation in maternal investment in a small cervid;
the effects of cohort, sex, litter size and time of birth in roe deer (Capreolus
capreolus) fawns. Oecologia 109: 74-79.
Andersen, R., Gaillard, J. M., Linnel, J. D. C. & Duncan, P. (2000). Factors affecting maternal
care in an income breeder, the European roe deer. J. Anim. Ecol. 69: 672-682.
Arnbom, T., Fedak, M. A. & Boyd, I. L. (1997). Factors affecting maternal expenditure in
southern elephant seals during lactation. Ecology 78: 471-483.
Kreeger, T., J., Arnemo, J. M. & Raath, J. P. (2002). Handbook of wildlife chemical
immobilization, international edition. Ft. Collins, Colorado: Wildlife Pharmaceuticals
Inc.
Arnould, J. P. Y., Boyd, I. L. & Socha, D. G. (1996). Milk consumption and growth
efficiency in Antarctic fur seal (Arctocephalus gazella). Can. J. Zool. 74: 254-266.
Atkinson, S. N., Stirling, I. & Ramsay, M. A. (1996). Growth in early life and relative body
size among polar bears (Ursus maritimus). J. Zool Lond. 239: 225-234.
Baker, J. D. & Fowler, C. W. (1992). Pup weight and survival of northern fur seals
Callorhinus ursinus. J. Zool. Lond. 227: 231-238.
Barbraud, C., Weimerskirch, H., Robertson, G. G. & Jouventin, P. (1999). Size–related life
history traits: insight from a study of snow petrels (Pagodroma nivea). J. Anim. Ecol.
68: 1179-1192.
Birgersson, B. & Ekvall, K. (1997). Early growth in male and female fallow deer fawns.
Behav. Ecol. 8: 493-499.
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. (1998). Model selection and inference: a practical
information-theoretic approach. New York: Springer Verlag.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Albon, S. D. & Guinness, F. E. (1988). Reproductive success in male

13
and female red deer. In Reproductive success: 325-343. Clutton-Brock, T. H. (Ed.).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cooch, E. G., Lank, D. B., Rockwell, R. F. & Cooke, F. (1991). Long term decline in body
size in a snow goose population – evidence of a environmental degradation. J. Anim.
Ecol. 60: 483-496.
Dahle, B. & Swenson, J. E. (2003c). Factors influencing length of maternal care and its
consequences for offspring in brown bears Ursus arctos. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. (in
press?) (send forespørsel til F. Trillmitch).
Dahle, B. & Swenson, J. E. (2003b). Family breakup in brown bears: are young forced to
leave? J. Mammal. 84: (in press).
Dahle, B. & Swenson, J. E. (2003a). Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears Ursus
arctos: effect of mass, sex, reproductive status, population density and habitat type. J
Zool Lond. (in press).
Dahle, B., Sørensen, O. J., Wedul, H., Swenson, J. E. & Sandegren, F. (1998). The diet of
brown bears in central Scandinavia: effect of access to free-ranging domestic sheep.
Wildlife Biology 3: 147-158.
Derocher, A. E., Andriashek, D. & Arnould, J. P. Y. (1993). Aspects of milk composition and
lactation in polar bears. Can. J. Zool. 71: 561-567.
Derocher, A. E. & Stirling, I. (1996). Aspects of survival in juvenile polar bears. Can J. Zool.
74: 1246-1252.
Derocher, A. E. & Stirling, I. (1998). Maternal investment and factors affecting offspring size
in polar bears (Ursus maritimus). J. Zool. Lond. 245: 253-260.
Derocher, A. E. & Taylor, M. (1994). Density dependent population regulation of polar bears.
In Density dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar bears: 25-30.
Taylor, M. (Ed.). International Association for Bear Research and Management.
Farley, S. D. & Robbins, C. T. (1995). Lactation, hibernation, and mass dynamics of
American black bears and grizzly bears. Can. J Zool. 73: 2216-2222.
Festa-Bianchet, M., Jorgenson, J. T., Berube, C. H., Porter, C. & Wishhart, W. D. (1997).
Body mass and survival of bighorn sheep. Can J. Zool. 75: 1372-1379.
Festa-Bianchet, M., Jorgenson, J. T. & Reale, D. (2000). Early development, adult mass, and
reproductive success in bighorn sheep. Behav. Ecol. 11: 633-639.
Foltz, D. W., Hoogland, J. L. & Koscielny, G. M. (1988). Effects of sex litter size, and
heterozogsity on juvenile weight in black tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus).
J. Mammal. 69: 611-614.

14
Forsman, A. & Erik, L. (1996). Resource dependent growth and body condition dynamics in
juvenile snakes: an experiment. Oecologia 108: 669-675.
Gaillard, J. M., Andersen, R., Delorme, D. & Linnel, J. D. C. (1998) Family effects on growth
and survival of juvenile roe deer. Ecology 79: 2878-2889.
Green, W. C. H. & Rothstein, A. (1991). Sex bias or equal opportunity? Patterns of maternal
investment in bison. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29: 373-384.
Green, A. J. (2001). Mass/length residuals: measures of body condition or generators of
spurious results. Ecology 82: 1473-1483.
Hjeljord, O. & Histøl, T. (1999). Range-body mass interactions of a northern ungulate – a test
of hypothesis. Oecologia 119: 326-339.
Kreeger, T. J., Arnemo, J. M. & Jacobus, P. R. (2002). Handbook of wildlife and chemical
immobilization, international edition. Fort Collins, Wildlife Pharmaceuticals Inc.
LeFrank, M. N. Jr., Moss, M. B., Patnode, K. A. & Sugg, W. C. (1987). Grizzly bear
compendium. Washington D.C.: Interagency Grizzly Bear Committee.
Lindström, J. (1999). Early development and fitness in birds and mammals. Trends Ecol.
Evol. 14: 343-348.
Lloyd, D. G. (1987). Selection of offspring size at independence and other size versus number
strategies. Am. Nat. 129: 800-817.
Maynard-Smith, J. (1980). A new theory of sexual investment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7:
247-251.
McElligott, A. G., Gammell, M. P., Harty, H. C., Paini, D. R., Murphy, D. T., Walsh, J. T. &
Hayden, T. J. (2001). Sexual size dimorphism in fallow deer (Dama dama): do larger,
heavier males gain greater mating success? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49: 266-272.
McLellan, B. (1994) Density-dependent population regulation of brown bears. Density
dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar bears: 15-24. Taylor, M,
(Ed.). International Association for Bear Research and Management.
Myers, P. & Master, L. L. (1983). Reproduction by Peromyscus maniculatus: size and
compromize. J. Mammal. 64: 1-18.
Nilsen, P. A. (2002). Scandinavian brown bear (Ursus arctos L.) foraging on temporary and
spatially variable berry resources in the boreal forest. Cand scient. thesis, Agricultural
University of Norway, Ås.
Noyce, K. V. & Garshelis, D. L. (1994). Body size and blood characteristics as indicators of
condition and reproductive performance on black bears. In International Conference
on Bear Research and Management 9: 481-496. Claar, J. J. & Schullery, P. (Eds.)

15
International Association for Bear Research and Mangement.
Ono, K. A., Boness, D. J. & Oftedal, O. T. (1987). The effect of natural environmental
disturbance on maternal investment and pup behavior in the Galapagos sea lion.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21: 109-118.
Opseth, O. (1998). Brown bear (Ursus arctos) diet and predation on moose calves in the
southern taiga zone in Sweden. Cand. scient. thesis, Norwegian University of Science
and Technology, Trondheim.
Reed, A. & Plante, N. (1997). Decline in body mass, size and condition of greater snow geese,
1975-94. J. Wildl. Manage. 61: 413-419.
Schultz, S. R. & Johnson, M. K. (1995). Effects of birth date and body mass at birth on adult
body mass of male white-tailed deer. J. Mammal. 76: 575-579.
Sæther, B. E., Engen, S., Swenson, J. E., Bakke, Ø. & Sandegren, F. (1998). Assessing the
viability of Scandinavian brown bear Ursus arctos populations: the effect of uncertain
parameter estimates. Oikos 83: 403-416.
Smith, C. C. & Fretwell, S. D. (1974). The optimal balance between size and number of
offspring. Am. Nat. 108: 499-506.
Steinheim, G., Mysterud, A., Holand, Ø., Bakken, M. & Ådnøy, T. (2002). The effect of
initial weight of the ewe on later reproductive effort in domestic sheep (Ovis aries). J.
Zool. Lond. 258: 515-520.
Swenson, J. E., Dahle, B. & Sandegren, F. (2001a). Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian
brown bears older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus 12: 81-92.
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F., Segerström, P. & Brunberg, S. (2001b). Factors associated
with loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12: 69-80.
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Söderberg, A., Wabakken, P. & Franzén, R.
(1994). Size, trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear (Ursus arctos)
population in Sweden. Biol. Conserv. 70: 9-17
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Wabakken, P., Söderberg, A. & Franzen, R.
(1997). Infanticide caused by hunting of male bears. Nature 386: 450-451.
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F. & Söderberg, A. (1998). Geographic expansion of an increasing
brown bear population: evidence for presaturation dispersal. J. Anim. Ecol. 67: 819-
826.
Swenson, J. E., Wabakken, P., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Franzen, R. & Söderberg, A.
(1995). The near extinction and recovery of brown bears in Scandinavia in relation to
the bear management policies in Norway and Sweden. Wildl. Biol. 1: 11-25.

16
Toïgo, C., Gaillard, J. M. & Michallet, J. (1999). Cohort affects growth of males but not
females in alpine ibex (Capra ibex ibex). J. Mammal. 80: 1021-1027.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual selection and the
descent of man: 136-179. Campbell, B. (Ed.) Chicago: Aldine.
Verme, L. J. (1989). Maternal investment in white-tailed deer. J. Mammal. 70: 438-442.
Wauters, L., Bijnens, L. & Dhondt, A. A. (1993). Body mass at weaning and local
recruitment in the red squirrel. J. Anim. Ecol. 62: 280-286.
Wolff, J. O. (1988). Maternal investment and calf sex ration adjustment in American bison
calves. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23: 127-133.

17
Table 1. Body mass and body size indices (see text for explanation) of brown bear yearlings.
Litter size is the number of cubs the previous fall.
Litter size Sex Body mass kg ± SE Body size index ± SE
South N North N South North
One Males 33.8±3.3 5 48 1 0.65±0.49 2.94
Females 34,6±4.0 4 22.2±2.8 3 1.17±0.28 -0.02±0.67

Two Males 27.7±1.5 25 29.1±1.8 21 0.27±0.20 0.7±0.25


Females 23.0±1.3 23 24.1±1.4 17 -0.05±0.15 -0.05±0.23

Three Males 24.5±0.8 35 22.4±1.3 23 0.13±0.11 -0.35±0.23


Females 22.8±0.8 35 21.0±1.2 25 -0.11±0.13 -0.60±0.20

Four Males 13.3±1.2 3 27.8±2.8 2 -1.18±0.29 0.36±0.03


Females 10 1 20.8±2.8 2 -1.73 -1.20±0.47

18
Table 2. General linear models with body size of brown bear yearlings as the dependent
variable, and maternal size, litter size (number of cubs in the previous fall), sex, cohort, and
study area (area) as explanatory variables. The models are ranked by their AICc values (the 2
best models out of a set with candidate models and the global model based on all the
predictions (after successive exclusion of non significant terms)). Models in italics and
numbered for reference to the text. ***, ** and * denotes P < 0.001, P < 0.01 and P < 0.05,
respectively.
Explanatory variables β SE df F Effect size ∆AICc
1) Maternal size, litter size 2,208 21,55*** 0.172 0
Maternal size 0.252 0.065 1 14.96*** 0.067
Litter size -0,498 0.094 1 28.36*** 0.120
2) Maternal size, litter size, 2 15.95*** 0.188 0.174
sex
Maternal size 0.250 0.065 1 15.03*** 0.068
Litter size -0.496 0.093 1 28.50*** 0.121
Sex Male 0.248 0.123 1 4.09 0.019
3)a Maternal size, litter size, 0.394 22.394
sex, area, cohort, area*cohort
a
parameter estimates is not given due to the large number of estimates.

19
Table 3. General linear models with body mass (squared) of brown bear yearlings as the
dependent variable, and maternal size, litter size, sex, cohort, and study area (area) as
explanatory variables. The models are ranked by their AICc values (the 3 best out of a set
candidate models, the best model given with parameter estimates). Models in italics and
numbered for reference to the text. ***, ** and * denotes P < 0.001, P < 0.01 and P < 0.05,
respectively.
Explanatory variables β SE df F Effect size ∆AICc
1) Maternal size, litter size, sex 3,205 26,48*** 0.279 0
Maternal size 0.249 0.044 1 32.07*** 0.135
Litter size -0,397 0.063 1 39.08*** 0.160
Sex Male 0.24 0.084 1 8.14** 0.038
2) Maternal size, litter size, sex, 13.195 11.42*** 0.432 0.537
cohort
3) Maternal size, litter size, sex, 24,184 10.49*** 0.578 0.884
cohort, area, cohort*area

20
Figure legends

Figure 1. The relationship between body size index and body mass of yearling brown bears
with the regression lines for males and females.

Figure 2. Estimated marginal means of square root of body mass during the period 1992-2002
in the two study areas. Estimated values are controlled for maternal size and litter size
(average value of maternal size and litter size).

21
[Fig. 1. Dahle & Swenson]

50

Females
40 Males
Body mass kg

30

20

10

0
-3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3

Body size index

22
[Fig. 2. Dahle & Swenson]

6,0
Est. marg. means of square root body mass kg

South
North

5,0

4,0
1992 1994 1996 1998 2000 2002

23
Paper II

Behavoral Ecology and Sociobiology XX:XXX-XXX

Factors influencing length of maternal care in brown bears Ursus arctos


and its effects on offspring

By

B Dahle and JE Swenson

“The first half of our life is ruined by our parents and the second half by our children”
-Clarence Darrow
Factors influencing length of maternal care in brown bears Ursus
arctos and its effect on offspring

Bjørn Dahle Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural University


of Norway, Post Box 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway

Jon E. Swenson, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural


University of Norway, Post Box 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway and Norwegian Institute for
Nature Research, Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway

Running head: brown bear maternal care

Correspondence author:
Bjørn Dahle
Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
Agricultural University of Norway
Post Box 5014
N-1432 Ås
e-mail: bjorn.dahle@ibn.nlh.no

1
Factors influencing length of maternal care in brown bears Ursus
arctos and its effect on offspring

Bjørn Dahle and Jon E. Swenson

Abstract
Length of maternal care, i.e. the interval between successfully raised litters, is the most
important factor explaining the variation in reproductive rate among brown bear Ursus arctos
populations. In this paper we examine the variation in length of maternal care in radio-marked
brown bears and its effect on their offspring in northern Sweden. Young stayed with their
mothers for 1.4-1.5 or 2.4-2.5 (in one case 3.5) years and were weaned with body masses
varying from 17 to 69 kg. The probability of yearling litters staying with their mother for a
second year increased with decreasing yearling body mass, and was higher for litters with 2
offspring than for litters with 1 or 3-4 offspring. Staying with their mothers for a second year
had a positive effect on mass gain in yearlings and this effect was more pronounced in litters
with 2 than 3-4 offspring. Body mass of 2-year-olds was weakly, if at all, related to age of
weaning, suggesting that keeping offspring for an additional year mainly compensated for low
yearling body mass. If large offspring body mass positively affects later offspring survival
and reproduction, mothers may be able to optimize the length of maternal care according to
the litter size and the size of their yearlings.

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (00:00-00)

Key words: litter interval, maternal care, litter size, body mass, Ursus arctos

2
Introduction
Life history theory predicts that animals should show optimal levels of parental investment,
with energy expenditure for current offspring balanced against the effects on the parents’
probability of survival and future reproduction (Roff 1992; Stearns 1992). In most
polygynous species males provide no parental care and the optimal time span for maternal
care then depends only on the effect that maternal care has on offspring survival and the
mother’s future probability of reproduction. Thus, in such species maternal investment should
be terminated when the fitness pay-off for further investment in an offspring is lower than the
pay-off for investing in future reproduction.
Craighead et al. (1995) presented a conditional model based on theories of behavioral
polymorphism (Maynard Smith 1982) to explain the age of weaning in North American
brown bears. They argued that females in good condition could wean yearling offspring (at
least small litters), whereas females in poor condition weaned their offspring as 2.5-year-olds.
However they failed to determine the factors influencing the cessation of maternal care.
In this paper we examine variation in the length of maternal care in Swedish brown
bears. Brown bear offspring separate from their mothers when they are 1.4-3.5 years old
(McLellan 1994). A female brown bear does not mate until she has separated from her
offspring, which in nearly all cases coincides with the mating season in late May-early July
(Dahle and Swenson in press) (Table 1). Thus, accompanying their yearling offspring through
the mating season increases the litter interval by 1 year, and may impose fitness costs if the
positive effect of prolonged maternal care on the offspring does not compensate for the loss of
potential reproduction due to the increased litter interval. Length of maternal care, i.e. the
interval between successfully raised litters, is more important than litter size and age of
maturity in influencing the long-term reproductive rate in brown bear populations (Swenson
and Sandegren 1999). Determining the factors influencing this life history trait is therefore
very important. The study population is suitable to study this life history trait because
preliminary data suggested that about 40% of the offspring separated from their mothers as
yearlings and the rest as 2-year-olds (Swenson et al. 1994).
According to the resource allocation theory (Williams 1966), maternal investment may
vary according to litter size, offspring sex, and maternal attributes, such as body mass or
condition and parity, and environmental factors (see Clutton-Brock 1991 for a review). A
general trend in the age of weaning seems to be that offspring are weaned when they reach a
threshold size (Lee et al. 1991), hence the length of maternal investment often varies in

3
relation to nutritional and maternal condition (Trillmich 1986; Cheney et al. 1988; Fairbanks
and McGurie 1995).
A trade off exists between the number and size of offspring in a wide variety of taxa
(Lloyd 1987; Roff 1992). Because the reproductive value of a litter increases with number of
offspring, we should expect prolonged nursing and care in large litters compared to small
litters. In species such as the brown bear, which has a long interbirth interval, it may even be
beneficial for females to abandon single cubs of the year, because they could then produce a
new and perhaps larger litter the next year (Tait 1980).
From the general trend that weaning occurs after a given threshold size is reached, and
because the reproductive value of a litter increases with the number of offspring, we predicted
that the probability that a litter will stay with its mother for 2 years should be (1) negatively
related to average yearling body mass and (2) positively related to litter size.
Any relationship between the probability of staying with the mother for 2 years and
litter size could be confounded by female size and condition. Litter size has been reported to
be positively correlated with female condition in many species, including both the brown bear
(Craighead et al. 1995) and the American black bear (U. americanus) (Samson and Huot
1995), but no such relationship was reported in the polar bear (Ramsay and Stirling 1988). If
litter size is positively related to female condition, then Craighead et al.’s (1995) model
predicts that large litters should be weaned earlier than small ones. However, in contrast with
Craighead et al. (1995), we include body mass of yearlings, which is positively related to the
size and condition of their mothers (Dahle and Swenson et al. unpublished data) as a factor to
explain length of maternal care. Further, in the present study, we consider litter size as
yearlings, not the number of cubs born, which may be both difficult to assess and different
from yearling litter size due to both care-independent and care-dependent mortality, although
the first-year mortality is very low in the studied population (Swenson et al. 2001a). We feel
that litter size as yearlings is relevant in this context as the options for a mother are to separate
or not, based on the given number of yearlings, and this decision should be irrespective of the
number of cubs she gave birth to the previous year.
If the amount of parental investment partly determines a young’s subsequent adult
body size, sex allocation theory predicts that parents should then invest more in individual
offspring of the sex with the greater variance in reproductive success (Maynard Smith 1980;
Charnov 1982). In polygynous mammals, including the brown bear, adult size of the sexes
often differs greatly (e.g. Jarman 1983, Swenson et al. unpubl. data), and large adult size is
likely to increase fitness (survival and/or reproductive success) more in males than in females

4
(e.g. Maynard Smith and Parker 1976; Clutton-Brock et al. 1986, but see Hewison and
Gaillard 1999). To date, the empirical support of differential parental investment in wild
mammals is equivocal, but Lee and Moss (1986) and Ono and Boness (1996) found that male
offspring were weaned at an older age than females. For this reason we predicted (3) that the
probability of a litter staying with the mother for 2 years should be higher in litters with a
male-biased sex ratio.
The theory of parent-offspring conflict (Trivers 1974) assumes that an increase in
maternal investment will increase offspring fitness. Thus, this theory assumes a positive
correlation between offspring size or condition at the time parental effort ceases and lifetime
reproductive success. Lifetime reproductive success is very hard to measure in long-lived
free-ranging mammals, so with few exceptions (e.g. Festa-Bianchet et al. 2000) some
surrogate measure of reproductive success must be used. Survival and growth are assumed to
increase during maternal care (Clutton-Brock 1991), and survival has been reported to be
positively related to the size at weaning (e.g. McMahon et al. 2000). Growth rate may
influence the age for attainment of sexual maturity (e.g. Congdon and Sels 1993) and also
later reproductive success (e.g. Tummaruk et al. 2001). In some ungulates lactating females
that do not conceive in the following breeding season may continue to nurse their offspring
through the winter, resulting in increased calf growth rate (Clutton-Brock et al. 1982, Green
and Rothstein 1991a) and reproductive success of offspring (Green and Rothstein 1991b). In
Scandinavian brown bears, yearlings that stayed with their mother did not experience
significantly lower intraspecific predation, perhaps the most important mortality factor except
human-caused mortality (Swenson et al. 2001b) and the same was found among yearling
polar bears (all sources of mortality included) (Ramsay and Stirling 1988). For this reason we
predicted (4) that staying with their mother should positively affect growth rate in yearling
brown bears. In general, growth rate in young is negatively related to litter size (e.g. Mendl
1988; Koskela 1998), and thus we predicted (5) that growth rate among yearlings that stayed
with their mother should be negatively related to litter size, whereas no such effect was
expected in yearlings that separated from their mother. In accordance with the sex allocation
theory (e.g. Maynard Smith 1980), several studies have reported faster growth in males than
females in sexually dimorphic species where males compete for access to females (e.g.
Clutton-Brock et al. 1982; Lee and Moss 1986) and we predicted (6) faster growth in male
than female yearlings.

5
Staying with their offspring for 2 years could be a strategy for producing larger and
more competitive young. If so, this predicts (7) that, at the age of 2 years, offspring weaned at
this age should be larger than those weaned as yearlings.

Methods
The study was performed in southwestern Norrbotten County, Sweden, (67° N, 18° E) during
1989-2000. The rolling landscape is covered with coniferous forest, dominated by Scots pine
(Pinus sylvestris) or Norway spruce (Picea abies), but mountains rise to about 2000 m in
large parts of the area. There, subalpine forest dominated by birch (Betula pubescens) and
willows (Salix spp.), as well as alpine areas, are common. Bears hibernate for about 7 months,
but the length of hibernation may vary according to age, sex and reproductive status.
Subadult bears (1-2 years old) and their mothers, which had been almost always radio-
marked previously, were darted with immobilizing drugs from a helicopter during early May,
prior to any separation between mothers and their offspring. We used 2.5 mg Tiletamin, 2.5
mg Zolazepam and 0.02 mg Medetomidin per kg to immobilize the bears. Atipamezol was
used as an antidote for Medetomidin (5 mg per 1 mg Medetomidin). Altogether, 110 bears (54
females and 56 males) were immobilized in 1989-2000, but the present paper is based on the
capture/recapture of 17 adult females and their offspring. The immobilized bears were
weighed and measured, and fitted with VHF radio transmitters (adults: Telonics model 500
with 3 years life-time, yearlings: Telonics model 400 with 1 year life-time) mounted on neck
collars (until 1996). In 1997-2000 transmitters (Telonics model 400Lwith 4 years life-time)
were implanted into the peritoneal cavity of yearlings following the surgical procedures
described in Arnemo et al. (1999). Until 1997, growing bears were recaptured each spring to
change neck collars and to obtain weights and body measurements. Because bears were
captured within a three-week period in May, and bears generally do not gain weight during
this time of the year, we did not adjust mass for capture date. Bears were localized once a
week from fixed-winged aircraft or helicopter, weather permitting, and the status of yearlings
and 2-year-olds (separated or following their mother) was determined. Throughout the paper
weaning is synonymous with the permanent separation of offspring from their mother.
We combined body length (from the tip of the nose to the base of the tail) and skull
circumference (measured in front of the ears) through a factor reduction (principal component
analysis) to produce an index of structural body size of adult females (hereafter maternal

6
size). These measures should reflect structural dimensions and be rather uninfluenced by body
condition. These body size indices were regressed against body mass to produce indices (the
residuals) of body condition (Cattet et al. 2002).
Staying with the mother for (at least) 2 years can be considered to be a binomial
process. Therefore, to explore which factors influenced the probability that litters stayed with
their mother for two years, we applied logistic regression analysis that fits a generalized
model to the data by maximum likelihood techniques (Proc Genmod, logit link function, SAS
1996) (McCullagh and Nelder 1989). The other statistical tests were executed in SPSS/Win v.
10.0 (SPSS Inc., Illinois, USA).

Results
Length of maternal care varied among individuals, but also varied within many individuals
(Table 2). For this reason we treated each litter as independent data in our analyses. Yearling
litters were made up of 1 (N = 7), 2 (N = 13), 3 (N = 8) and 4 (N = 1) offspring, with an
average litter size of 2.10 ± 0.15 (SE). For statistical analyses we pooled the litters with 3 and
4 offspring. Offspring became independent with body masses ranging from 17.5 to 69 kg.
Using a logistic regression model we tested if yearling body mass in the litter (average
littermate size), litter size, and sex ratio in the litter, including all possible interactions among
these factors, affected the probability for staying with the mother for (at least) 2 years. The
probability of staying with the mother for 2 years increased as predicted (1) with decreasing
body mass as a yearling (Table 3). Litter size also affected the length of maternal investment,
but not in the predicted way. The probability of yearlings of a given body mass to stay with
their mother for an additional year was similar for litters with 1 and 3-4 offspring, but higher
for litters with 2 young (Fig. 1, Table 3). Contrary to prediction (3) the probability of staying
for an additional year was not related to the sex ratio in the litter (the average proportion of
males in litters was 0.49 ± 0.42 (SD))
Maternal size and condition were not related to yearling litter size (F2,29 = 1.05, P =
0.36, and F2,28 = 0.957, P = 0.40, respectively). Thus, yearling litter size was not merely a
function of maternal size or condition, strengthening our findings that the mothers’
willingness to invest another year in a litter was higher for litters with 2 offspring than for
litters with 1 or 3-4 offspring when the yearlings average body mass was the same.

7
Thirty-one yearlings were recaptured as 2-year-olds (Table 4). Due to the small
sample of litters with one offspring (N = 3) they were excluded from statistical analyzes of
yearling growth and body mass at the age of 2 year. General linear models explained 44 and
60% and of the variation in absolute and percent mass gain among young bears during their
second year, respectively (Table 5). As predicted (4) mass gain (absolute and percent mass
gain) was higher for yearlings that stayed with their mother than for yearlings that separated
(Table 5). Mass gain was not related to litter size, but there was a litter size*age-at-weaning
interaction. A profile plot indicated, as predicted (5), that the positive effect on yearling mass
gain by staying with their mothers was more pronounced in litters with 2 than 3-4 offspring,
but that mass gain in weaned yearlings was higher in litters with 3-4 than 2 offspring (Fig. 2).
Percent mass gain of yearlings was positively related with maternal size, whereas the
relationship between absolute mass gain and maternal size was less clear (F = 3.03, df = 1, P
= 0.96). Contrary to prediction (6) mass gain was not higher in males than females (P = 0.18
and 0.47 for absolute and percent mass gain, respectively).
The effect of staying with the mother for 2 years was tremendous in some cases. After
the capture of a mother (BD 1) with 2 yearlings, one of the yearlings (female BD 25) was
separated from the mother and the female littermate (BD 24) and did not reunite. It is most
likely that this separation was inflicted by the capture and marking (1 of 2 marking induced
separations in 85 family groups handled by the research project). However all bears were
healthy and BD 24 and BD 25 weighed 23.5 and 23 kg, respectively. The next spring, BD 24,
which stayed with her mother for 2 years, weighed 63 kg, whereas BD 25 weighed only 33
kg.
At the age of 2 years offspring body mass was higher in litters with 2 than 3-4
offspring (F = 6.69, df = 1, P = 0.016). Offspring weaned as 2-year-olds had only a weak
tendency, if at all, to be heavier than offspring weaned as yearlings, at the age of 2 years (F =
2.95, df = 1, P = 0.099). Males only tended to be heavier than females at the age of 2 years (F
= 3.39, df = 1, P = 0.78), but males also tended to be heavier than females as yearlings (t =
1.84, df = 26, P = 0.077). Offspring body mass at the age of 2 years was not related to
maternal size (P = 0.84).

8
Discussion
Our most important findings were that the probability of offspring to stay with their mother
for 2 years increased with decreasing yearling body mass, and that this relationship was
related to litter size, with litters with 2 offspring having a higher probability of staying for 2
years than litters with 1 or 3-4 offspring (Fig. 3). Staying with their mother also had a more
pronounced positive effect on growth rate in litters with 2 than 3-4 offspring (Fig. 3). The
body mass of 2-year-olds was weakly, if at all, related to age at weaning, suggesting that
prolonged nursing mainly compensated for small size of yearlings, bringing them to about the
same size as offspring weaned as yearlings by the age of 2 years.
Derocher and Stirling (1995) reported annual variation in the percentage of yearling
polar bears that were still following their mothers. Despite annual variation in the proportion
of yearlings that followed their mothers, the proportion was significantly less in years when
both yearling and maternal body masses were higher and population density lower. They
suggested that mothers might wean offspring as yearlings under favorable conditions, whereas
mothers may keep their offspring for an additional year when conditions are poor. The same
suggestion was made by Craighead et al. (1995) for brown bears. However, neither Derocher
and Stirling (1995) nor Craighead et al. (1995) showed this statistically. The negative
relationship between yearling body mass and probability of staying with their mothers for 2
years, which we predicted (1) and found, supported these suggestions, as offspring mass and
condition are positively related to maternal mass and condition (Dahle and Swenson
unpublished data).
Craighead et al. (1995) did not find any significant relationship between litter size and
age at weaning, and Derocher and Stirling (1995) only reported that yearling litter size varied
annually, but did not analyze this variation in relation to the probability of yearlings following
their mother for an additional year. Contrary to prediction (2) we found that litters with 2
offspring had the highest probability of staying with their mother for 2 years. Prolonged
nursing had a more pronounced positive effect on the mass gain litters with 2 than 3-4
offspring. Thus the benefit obtained by offspring of staying another year may be reduced in
large litters. The relative importance of milk transfer to yearlings from their mother compared
to their own feeding is not known, but milk production by polar bears with yearlings is about
only 25% of the production by females with cubs (Arnold and Ramsay 1994). However, milk
production is probably not related to litter size in bears (Arnold and Ramsay 1994) and
competition for both milk and food would increase with litter size, having a negative impact

9
on mass gain. Hilderbrand et al. (2000) studied body mass and fat content of adult female
brown bears and suggested that keeping yearlings from spring to fall is as energetically costly
as nursing cubs during the same period, Thus, nursing yearlings is probably not a cheap
alternative to mating and raising a new litter the next year.
Many females varied their length of maternal investment among litters, so it is likely
that this life history trait is flexible for each litter size, reflecting variation in food availability
and maternal condition acting on offspring size. As litter size of yearlings was unrelated to
maternal body size or condition, it is unlikely that the observed effect of litter size on the
probability of offspring to stay with the mother for 2 years, was related to size or condition of
mothers.
Age of weaning was not related to the sex ratio in the litter. This contradicts prediction
(3), but confirms the results of Craighead et al. (1995). It seems that female brown bears do
not adjust the length of maternal care in relation to the sex of offspring.
In sum, it seems that litters with 1 and 3-4 offspring are only kept for 2 years in our
study area if the yearlings are very small. Rather than being weaned at a threshold body mass,
as seems to be the general trend for mammals, the litters were weaned as yearlings only if
their body mass was above a certain threshold. However this threshold was dependent on
litter size, a result not reported previously, to our knowledge. Our results suggest that females
assess both the size and number of their yearlings before deciding to end or continue further
care.
Young brown bears in northern Sweden were on average weaned earlier than in North
American populations living under comparable climatic and nutritional conditions (see
McLellan 1994 for a review). In these populations young are rarely weaned as yearlings and
most young stay with their mother for 2.4-2.5 or 3.4-3.5 years. In southern Sweden and in
populations elsewhere in Europe, most brown bears are weaned as yearlings (Danilow et al.
1993; Swenson et al. 1994; Frković et al. 2001). Brown bear yearlings are heavier in southern
European populations than in northern European populations (Swenson et al. unpubl. data),
which may explain why all offspring are reported to be weaned as yearlings in the southern
European populations.
As predicted (4) yearlings staying with their mothers gained more mass than yearlings
staying on their own, and as discussed above, this effect was more pronounced in litters with
2 than 3-4 offspring. In most species prolonged nursing has a positive effect on the mass gain
in young ( e.g. Green and Rothstein 1991; Festa-Bianchet et al. 1994), but it is also reasonable
to assume that mass loss during hibernation may be reduced in bears hibernating together in

10
the same den, as they may save energy by decreasing the surface/volume relationship if
huddling with conspecifics (Arnold 1990).
As predicted (5) mass gain was higher in litters with 2 offspring than in litters with 3-4
offspring. We have previously found a negative relationship between litter size and yearling
body mass (Dahle and Swenson unpublished data), and as discussed above it suggests that
resources provided by females are not proportional to litter size and/or that yearlings in large
litters suffer from competition for other food. We found no significant difference in growth of
male and female from 1 to 2 years of age, but males tended to be heavier than females at the
age of two years, which may be explained by a tendency for males being heavier than females
as yearlings. Mass gain in yearlings was affected by factors other than those we considered,
because much of the variation remained unexplained in our models. Due to the small sample
size we were not able to include year as a variable in our models of mass gain. Yearling body
mass varies among years (Swenson et al. 2001b) and thus growth probably varies among
years as well.
Because body mass of 2-year-olds was only weakly, if at all, related to age at weaning,
it seems that prolonged nursing was mainly a compensation for low yearling body mass. As
prolonged nursing is energetically costly for the mother and increases the interbirth interval
by 1 year, reproductive success in 2-year-old bears should be positively affected by body
mass. Little is known about this relationship in brown bears, but in mammals in general, large
young survive and reproduce better than small ones (Green and Rothstein 1991b; Festa
Bianchet et al. 2000; Hall et al. 2001).
We have used the term maternal care and not maternal investment, because we did not
measure reproductive costs. However, considering the increased litter interval and the large
energetic cost associated with nursing yearlings measured by Hilderbrand et al. (2000), this
maternal care probably reflects maternal investment.

Acknowledgements
This study was funded by the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management, the Swedish
Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, the Swedish
Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, WWF Sweden and the Research Council
of Norway. We thank the personnel in the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project
especially, Robert Franzén, Peter Segerström and Sven Brunberg for their assistance in the

11
field, and we thank Thor H. Ringsby for assistance with the logistic regression model.
Andreas Zedrosser, Ole-Gunnar Støen, Phyllis C. Lee and an anonymous reviewer gave
valuable comments on a draft of the paper. All animal capture and handling reported in this
paper complies with the current laws regulating the treatment of animals in Sweden and was
approved by the appropriate ethical committee.

12
References
Arnemo JM, Linnell JDC, Wedul SJ, Ranheim B, Odden J, Andersen R (1999) Use of
intraperitoneal radio-transmitters in lynx Lynx lynx kittens: anaesthesia, surgery and
behaviour. Wildl Biol 5:245-250
Arnold JPY, Ramsay MA (1994) Milk production and milk consumption in polar bears
during the ice-free period in western Hudson Bay. Can J Zool 72:1365-1370
Arnold W (1990) The evolution of marmot sociality: II Costs and benefits of joint
hibernation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 27:239-246
Cattet MRL, Caulkett NA, Obbard ME, Stenhouse GB (2002) A body-condition index
for ursids. Can J Zool 80:1156-1161
Charnov EL (1982) The theory of sex allocation. Princeton University Press, Princeton
Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Andelman SJ, Lee PC (1988) Reproductive success in vervet
monkeys. In: Clutton-Brock TH (ed) Reproductive success: studies of individual
variation in contrasting breeding systems. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp
384-402
Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton University Press,
Princeton
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE (1986) Great expectations: dominance, breeding
success and offspring sex ratios in red deer. Anim Behav 34:460-471
Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD (1982) Red deer: behavior and ecology of
two sexes. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Congdon JD, Sels RVC (1993) Relationships of reproductive trait and body-size with
attainment of sexual maturity and age in blandings turtles (Emyoidea blandingi)
J Evol Biol 6:547-557
Craighead JJ, Sumner JS, Mitchell JA (1995) The grizzly bears of Yellowstone: their ecology
in the Yellowstone ecosystem 1959-1992. Island Press, Washington
Dahle B, Swenson JE, (in press) Family break-up in brown bears: are young forced to
leave? J Mammal 84
Derocher AE, Stirling I (1995) Temporal variation in reproduction and body mass of polar
bears in western Hudson Bay. Can J Zool 73:1657-1665
Fairbanks LA, McGurie MT (1995) Maternal condition and the quality of maternal care in
vervet monkeys. Behaviour 132:733-754
Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Wishart WD (1994) Early weaning in bighorn sheep, Ovis

13
canadensis, affects growth of males but not females. Behav Ecol 5: 21-27
Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT, Reale D (2000) Early development, adult mass and
reproductive success in bighorn sheep. Behav Ecol 11:633-639
Frković A, Huber D, Kusak J (2001) Brown bear litter sizes in Croatia. Ursus 12: 103-106
Green WCH, Rothstein A (1991a) Sex bias or equal opportunity? patterns of maternal
investment in bison. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:373-384
Green WCH, Rothstein A (1991b) Trade offs between growth and reproduction in female
bison. Oecologia 86:521-527
Hall AJ, McConnell BJ, Barker RJ (2001) Factors affecting first-year survival in grey seals
and their implications for life history strategy. J Anim Ecol 70:138-149
Hewison AJM, Gaillard JM (1999) Successful sons or advantaged daughters? The Trivers-
Willard model and sex-biased maternal investment in ungulates. Trends Ecol Evol
14:229-234
Hilderbrand GV, Schwartz CC, Robbins CT, Hanley TA (2000) Effects of hibernation and
reproductive status on body mass and condition of coastal brown bears. J Wildl
Manage 64:178-183
Jarman P (1983) Mating system and sexual size dimorphism in large terrestrial mammalian
herbivores. Biol Rev 58:485-520
Koskela E (1998) Offspring growth, survival and reproductive success in the bank vole: a
litter size manipulation experiment. Oecologia 115:379-384.
Lee PC, Moss CJ (1986) Early maternal investment in male and female African elephant
calves. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:353-361
Lee PC, Majluf P, Gordon IJ (1991) Growth, weaning and maternal investment from a
comparative perspective. J Zool 225:99-114
Lloyd DG (1987) Selection of offspring size at independence and other size versus number
strategies. Am Nat 129:800-817
McCullagh P, Nelder JA (1989) Generalized linear models. Chapman and Hall, London.
McLellan B (1994) Density dependent population regulation in brown bears. In: Taylor M
(ed) Density dependent population regulation in black, brown and polar bears.
Monograph Series No. 3. International Association for Bear Research and
Management, Port City Press, Washingthon D.C. pp 15-25
McMahon CR, Burton HR, Bester MN (2000) Weaning mass and the future survival of
juvenile southern elephant seals, Mirounga leeonina at Macquarie Island. Antarct
Sci 12:149-153

14
Maynard Smith J (1980) A new theory of sexual investment. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:247-251
Maynard Smith J (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge
Maynard Smith J, Parker G (1976) The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim Behav 24:159-
175
Mendl M (1988) The effects of litter size on mother offspring relationships and behavioural
and physical development in several mammalian species (principally rodents). J Zool
215:15-34
Ono KA, Boness DJ (1996) Sexual dimorphism in seas lion pups: differential maternal
investment, or sex specific differences in energy allocation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol
38:31-41
Ramsay MA, Stirling I (1988) Reproductive biology and ecology of female polar bears
(Ursus maritimus). J Zool 214:601-634
Roff DA (1992) The evolution of life histories; theories and analysis. Chapman Hall, New
York
Samson C, Huot J (1995) Reproductive biology of female black bears in relation to body mass
in early winter. J Mammal 76:68-77
Stearns SC (1992) The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press, New York
Swenson JE, Dahle B, Sandegren F (2001b) Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown
bears older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus 12:81-92
Swenson JE, Sandegren F (1999) Conservation of European brown bear populations:
-experinces from Scandinavia. In: Layna JF, Heredia B, Palomero G, Doadr I (eds) La
conservación del oso pardo en Europa: un reto de cara al siglo XXI. (io), Madrid
Swenson JE, Sandegren F, Bjärvall A, Söderberg A, Wabakken P, Franzén R (1994) Size,
trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear Ursus arctos population in
Sweden. Biol Conserv 70:9-17
Swenson JE, Sandegren F, Segerström P, Brunberg S (2001a) Factors associated with
loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12:69-80
Tait DE (1980) Abandonment as a reproductive tactic – the example with grizzly bears.
Am Nat 115:800-808
Trillmich F (1986) Maternal investment and sex allocation in the Galapagos fur seal,
Arctocepahalus galapagoensis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:157-164
Trivers RL (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool 14:249-264
Tummaruk P, Lundeheim N, Einarsson S, Dalin AM (2001) Effect of birth litter size, birth

15
parity number, growth rate, backfat thickness and age at first mating of gilts on their
reproductive performance as sows. Anim Reprod Sci 66:225-237
Williams GC (1966) Natural selection, the cost of reproduction and a refinement of Lack’s
principle. Am Nat 100:687-690

16
Table 1. Reproductive cycles in female brown bears illustrating the effect of varying length of
maternal care.

Time 2-year 3-year


Year Month reproductive cycle reproductive cycle
0 January Cubs born Cubs born
May
June
1 May Separates from yearlings
June Mates
2 January Cubs born
May Separates from 2-year-olds
June Mates
3 January Separates from yearlings Cubs born
May Mates
June

17
Table 2. Documented length of maternal care for offspring by individual female brown bears
in northern Sweden. Figures in parentheses refer to the number of litters where sufficient data
was obtained to include the litter in the logistic regression model.
Bear Litters nursed for 1.5 Litters nursed for 2.5 Litter nursed for 3.5
year years years
BD 1 1 (1) 3 (3)
BD 7 6 (3)
BD 10 1 (0)
BD 16 1 (0)
BD 18 3 (3)
BD 23 3 (2) 2 (2)
BD 25 1 (1) 1 (1)
BD 27 1 (0)
BD 33 1 (1) 1 (1)
BD 46 1 (1)
BD47 1 (1)
BD 49 1 (1) 1 (1)
BD51 2 (2) 1 (1)
BD 62 1 (0) 2 (2)
BD 64 1 (1)
BD 71 1 (0)
BD 95 1 (1)
Total 20 (13) 17 (15) 1 (1)

18
Table 3. To test factors that influence the probability that a female keeps her young for 2
years (versus 1 year) we applied logistic regression statistics (PROC GENMOD, SAS Inc.
1996). The global model included; whether a litter was kept for two years (0,1) as a response
variable, and average body mass of yearling(s) in each litter, litter size, and sex ratio as
explanatory factors. After a successive exclusion of the least significant terms, based on
likelihood ratio-tests (Type 3-tests), the final model included the variables presented in the
table. β denotes the regression coefficient, SE denotes the standard error, df denotes degrees
of freedom, χ2 is the chi-square value of the type 3 test. ** denotes P<0.01. The sample size
of the final model was n=29.

Explanatory β SE df χ2
variables
Intercept 5.0143 2.9893 1
Yearling -0.2521 0.1288 1 7.13**
weight
Litter size 1 0.1536 1.1563 1 11.15**
Litter size 2 4.0860 1.7485 1
Litter size 3 0 0

19
Table 4. Body growth as absolute mass gain ± SE (standard error) and as percent mass gain ±
SE of brown bears from age 1.4 to 2.4 years and body mass ± SE at the age of 2.4 years.
Litter size Weaned as Mass gain (kg) % mass gain Body mass of N
X ± SE X ± SE two-year-olds
X ± SE
1 1.5-year-old 19.8 ± 9.3 50 ± 10.4 57 ± 20 2
1 2.5-year-old 15.5 67.4 38.5 1
2 1.5-year-old 9.3 ± 8.7 28.2 ± 21.1 46 ± 11.1 3
2 2.5-year-old 30 ± 2.5 122.8 ± 15.2 56 ± 2.5 9
3 1.5-year-old 15.1 ± 4.2 57.7 ± 13.8 40.1 ± 5.3 7
3-4 2.5-year-old 20.2 ± 1.4 94.2 ± 9.1 42.4 ± 1.9 9

20
Table 5. To test factors that influence the mass gain (absolute and percent) of yearlings during
their second year we applied General Linear Models (SPSS. 10.0). The global models
included; absolute (Abs) or percent (%) mass gain of yearlings during their second year as
response variables, and age at weaning, maternal size, litter size (2 or 3-4), and sex as
explanatory factors (including their interactions). After a successive exclusion of the least
significant terms, the final model included the variables presented. β denotes the regression
coefficient for variable values in parentheses, *, **, and *** denotes P<0.05, P<0.01 and P<
0.001, respective; Effect size denotes the proportion of total variability attributable to a factor.
a
is df in the model for absolute mass gain, b is df in the model for percent mass gain.
Explanatory variables df β SE F Effect size
Abs % Abs % Abs % Abs %
Age-at-weaning (1.5) 1 -5.2 -26.4 4.4 16.9 12.79** 19.69*** 0.348 0.461
Maternal size 1 14.5 5.2 7.95* 0.257
Litter size (2) 1 9.8 32.4 4.1 15.5 0.31 0.031 0.013 0.001
Age-at-weaning*litter 1 -15.5 -69.6 7.2 27.7 4.62* 6.31* 0.161 0.215
size (1.5 y. litt. size 2)
Intercept 1 20.22 85.0 2.88 11.4 106.86*** 99.28*** 0.817 0.812
Final model 3,24a 6.20** 8.60*** 0.437 0.599
b
4,23

21
Figure legends

Figure 1. Probability that young brown bears stay with the mother for 2 years rather than 1
year as a function of average yearling body mass and litter size, based on the multiple logistic
regression model presented in Table 3. a includes 1 litter with 4 offspring. Real data are shown
with symbols.

Figure 2. Profile plot (interaction plot) of estimated marginal means of percent mass gain of
yearling brown bears (adjusted for maternal size) in relation to age at weaning and litter size.
Vertical bars indicate the SE.

Figure 3. Conceptual model illustrating the factors influencing the length of maternal care and
the effect on offspring in brown bears in northern Sweden Litters with one offspring are
excluded for growth of yearlings and for body mass at the age of 2 year due to the small
sample size.

22
[Figure 1, Dahle and Swenson]

1,0 Litter size 3


Litter size 2
Probability of staying for 2 years

Litter size 1
0,8

0,6

0,4

0,2

0,0

10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50

Body mass kg

23
[Figure 2, Dahle and Swenson]

140
Est. marg. means of percent mass gain %

1-2 offspring
120 3-4 offspring

100

80

60

40

20

0
yearlings 2-year-olds
Weaned as

24
[Figure 3, Dahle and Swenson]

Yearling Yearling Yearling Stays with


litter size body mass litter size mother

Growrth of yearlings
for 2 years
Probability of staying
Yes
Low High
2 2 Yes
No
Low High
1 or 3-4 3-4
No

25
Paper III

Journal of Mammalogy 84: XXX-XXX

Family breakup in brown bears: are young forced to leave?

By

B Dahle and JE Swenson

“The calf cannot fling its mother to the ground and suckle at will”
-Trivers 1974
MAMMALOGY
Wednesday Feb 05 2003 09:34 PM mamm 84_202 Mp_9
Allen Press • DTPro System GALLEY 9 File # 02TQ

Journal of Mammalogy, 84(2):000–000, 2003

FAMILY BREAKUP IN BROWN BEARS: ARE YOUNG


FORCED TO LEAVE?

BJøRN DAHLE* AND JON E. SWENSON

Department of Zoology, Faculty of Chemistry and Biology, Norwegian University of Science and
Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway (BD)
Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural University of Norway,
Post Box 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway, and Norwegian Institute for Nature Research,
Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway (JES)
Present address of BD: Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural University
of Norway, N-1432 Ås, Norway

In brown bears (Ursus arctos), nursing of young .1 year of age has a positive effect on
their growth rate but is energetically costly for mothers and reduces the number of litters
?1 a female can produce during her lifetime. We followed radiomarked families to study their
behavior during the period of family breakup. Yearlings separated from their mothers during
the mating season and most family breakups were associated with the presence of an adult
male, suggesting that termination of maternal care was not initiated by offspring.

Key words: brown bear, family breakup, parent–offspring conflict, Ursus arctos, weaning

Conflicts may arise between parents and factor determining reproductive rate in the
offspring over parental investment (Trivers species (Swenson and Sandegren 1999). As
1974). The theoretical foundation for con- in other large, slowly reproducing species
flict is sound, but conflicts are seldom ob- (e.g., bison, Bison bison—Green and Roth-
served in nature (Bateson 1994; Mock and stein 1991), prolonged nursing in the brown
Parker 1997). Most studies on parent–off- bear contributes to increased rate of growth
spring conflict have focused on sex-ratio of yearlings (B. Dahle and J. E. Swenson,
adjustment or parent–offspring communi- in litt.).
cation in small organisms (Mock and Park- Hilderbrand et al. (2000) reported that
er 1997); relatively little information is body mass and body fat content in autumn
available for large long-lived species, in were similar for brown bear mothers with
which conflicts between parent and off- cubs or yearlings, and both were lower than
spring might be expected due to high fitness those for lone females (without offspring).
differential between them (Clutton-Brock This suggests that energetic costs associated
1991; Clutton-Brock et al. 1982). with tending cubs and yearlings are similar.
In brown bear (Ursus arctos), offspring In addition, tending yearlings reduces the
separate from their mothers at 1–3 (occa- number of litters a female can produce dur-
sionally 4) years of age (McLellan 1994). ing her reproductive life. For these reasons,
Females do not mate until after they sepa- a conflict between mothers and offspring
rate from their offspring, so maternal care over the timing of family breakup might be
beyond the mating season could impose a expected. Very little is known about the be-
fitness cost by increasing the interval be- havior of females and their offspring at this
tween litters, which is the most important time.
Clevenger and Pelton (1990) and Rogers
* Correspondent: bjorn.dahle@ibn.nlh.no (1987) reported on the breakups of several

Ms #BEM-027e
MAMMALOGY
Wednesday Feb 05 2003 09:34 PM mamm 84_202 Mp_10
Allen Press • DTPro System GALLEY 10 File # 02TQ

JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 84, No. 2

families of the American black bear (U. located weekly, and many were located 1 to sev-
americanus), but none was observed close- eral times per day during the mating season.
ly, so behavior associated with the breakups Movements of and distances between mothers
was unknown. Rogers (1987) argued that and offspring were estimated with the Ranges V
computer package (Kenward and Hodder, in
the mother was probably responsible for
litt.). ?2
breakups because all yearlings separated
from the mother at about the same time. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Clevenger and Pelton (1990) noted that
breakup of families took place before and Timing of family breakups.—We record-
during the mating season. Herrero and ed family breakups of 33 families, 19 with-
Hamer (1977) reported that a pair of twin in 612 h and others up to 63.5 days.
yearling brown bears accompanying their Breakups occurred from 3 May to 15 July
mother moved away when an adult male (median 22–28 May, when grouped in 7-
approached the family. day periods; Table 1; Fig. 1). This period
Based on Rogers’ (1987) suggestion that corresponds with the mating season, which
mothers prompt breakups of families, we we defined as the period when radiomarked
predicted that sibling yearling brown bears adult males and females were observed to-
should separate from the mother almost si- gether (Fig. 1). There was no difference be-
multaneously. Furthermore, if mothers ini- tween median date of family breakup and
tiate breakup as they enter estrus, they observation of adult pairs (Mann–Whitney
should be accompanied by adult males dur- U-test, Z 5 0.35, n 5 33 and 50, respec-
ing or soon after family breakup. The same tively; P 5 0.73). In contrast, family break-
predictions can be made if males chase up in the American black bear might be in-
away yearlings to mate with their mother or stigated by the mother before the mating
if yearlings benefit by staying with their period (Clevenger and Pelton 1990).
mother as long as possible but separate In our study, 24 of 33 litters had .1 year-
from her when she is ready to mate, allow- ling, and all yearlings within a litter sepa-
ing her to produce a new litter. Predictions rated from their mother within the same
of these hypotheses overlap, but the last one week, with 2 exceptions: both were males
predicts that little aggression is involved in in mixed-sex litters that separated 1–2
breakup. weeks earlier than their siblings (Table 1).
Of the 13 most intensively studied families
MATERIALS AND METHODS with .1 yearling, all siblings separated
The study was performed in Dalarna and Gäv- within 24 h. Sixty of 63 litters separated
?4 leborgs counties in southcentral Sweden from from their mothers as yearlings and the re-
1987 to 2000. The study area (618N, 188E) is mainder as 2-year-olds, as in most Euro-
dominated by coniferous forests of scotch pine pean brown bear populations (B. Dahle and
(Pinus sylvestris) and Norway spruce (Picea J. E. Swenson, in litt.). In contrast, most
abies), with deciduous species including birch young separate from their mothers as 2- or
(Betula spp.). In mid-April, female brown bears 3-year-olds in North American populations
and their yearling offspring were darted with im- of the species (McLellan 1994). Conflict
mobilizing drugs (a mixture of tiletamine, zola- between mother and offspring over the tim-
zepan [Virbac Laboratories, Carros, France], and
ing of family breakup might be expected to
medetomidine [Orion Corporation Farmos, Es-
be less in the latter situation because the
poo, Finland]) from a helicopter. Bears were fit-
ted with radiotransmitters on neck collars, or gain in fitness of offspring resulting from
these were implanted into the body cavity. Fam- staying with the mother probably decreases
ily groups were located from the ground using with age, and offspring are more likely to
receivers and handheld 6-element antennas benefit through inclusive fitness if the
(Macdonald and Amlaner 1980). All bears were mother breeds again. Murie’s (1981) obser-
MAMMALOGY
Wednesday Feb 05 2003 09:34 PM mamm 84_202 Mp_11
Allen Press • DTPro System GALLEY 11 File # 02TQ

May 2003 DAHLE AND SWENSON—BROWN BEAR FAMILY BREAKUP

TABLE 1.—Breakup of brown bear family groups in central Sweden, 1987–2000.

All yearlings Time siblings


Mother’s Litter Date of family Adult male separated at remained together after
ID no. Year size breakup present? the same time? family breakup (days)
1 1987 1 19 May Yes
2 1992 3 30 June–1 July Unknowna Yes 1–3
2 1994 2 28 May–29 May Yes Yes 3
2 1996 3 18–23 May (1), Unknowna No 0
31 May–2 June (2)
2 1999 2 24–25 May No Yes 7
3 1989 2 1 June Yes Yes 7
3 1992 2 21–23 June Unknown Yes 1
4 1997 1 1 June Unknowna
5 1991 1 9–10 May Unknowna
5 1993 3 17–18 May Yes Yes 5
5 1995 3 20–25 May Unknown Yes 7–13
5 1997 2 6–11 May Unknown Yes
5 1999 3 26–27 May No Yes 4
6 1989 2 29 April–7 May Unknown Yes
7 1992 2 4–12 June Unknown Yes 1
8 1997 2 14 May Yes Yes 0
8 1999 2 28 June–2 July Unknown
9 2000 3 28–31 May Unknown
9 1994 2 26–27 May Unknowna Yes 0
9 1996 3 26–27 May Unknowna Yes 2
9 1998 2 17–18 May Yes Yes 5
10 1998 1 25–27 May Unknown
11 1996 2 12–18 July Unknown Yes
12 1997 2 8–15 June Unknown Yes 10–22
13 1998 2 25 April–3 May (1), Unknown No
14 2000 1 10–13 May (1)
15 1993 2 8 June Unknowna
15 1995 3 18–19 June Yes Yes 0
15 1999 1 22–29 June Unknown Yes
16 1998 1 2–10 June Unknown
17 2000 2 8–16 June Unknown
18 1999 2 31 May Yes Yes 6
19 1999 2 9 June Yes Yes 0
25 June Unknowna Yes
a Breakups ascertained with an accuracy of 24 hs and where no radiomarked males were present (about 56% of adult males

were radiomarked).

vations of a mutual inclination of brown proached or were approached by an adult


?5 bear families to break up with 2- and 3- male. Two females were observed alone 10
year-old offspring support this view. h after they were last located with their
Factors associated with family break- yearlings, suggesting that no adult males
ups.—For the 19 breakups estimated to were present when their families broke up.
612 h, mothers were located with a radio- In 8 cases no radiomarked adult male was
marked adult male in 7 cases or observed present during family breakup. However,
with an unmarked adult bear (suspected to because the females were not observed and
be a male) twice within 24 h after the fam- only about 56% of adult males were radio-
ily was last located together (Table 1). marked during the study (Swenson et al.
These observations suggest that yearlings 2001), it is possible that unmarked adult
were forced to depart when the family ap- males were present when many of the fam-
MAMMALOGY
Wednesday Feb 05 2003 09:34 PM mamm 84_202 Mp_12
Allen Press • DTPro System GALLEY 12 File # 02TQ

JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 84, No. 2

in our study population, and the risk of ju-


veniles being killed is highest during the
mating season (Swenson et al. 2001).
Clutton-Brock (1991) and Clutton-Brock
et al. (1982) showed that fitness of yearling
red deer (Cervus elaphus) calves (in terms
of growth and overwintering survival) is
enhanced if they continue suckling, but fit-
ness of the mother is maximized (in terms
of lifetime production of surviving off-
spring) if she is impregnated each year. This
is held to be the weaning conflict with the
FIG. 1.—Timing of brown bear family break- most convincing field data (Mock and Park-
up (n 5 33) and observations of radiomarked er 1997). The situation we report seems
adult males and females together (n 5 50) in similar, but we do not know the effect of
southcentral Sweden. Only the 1st observation prolonged nursing on yearlings except that
of a pair was included for each year. their growth is increased.
Relationship of siblings and reassocia-
tions of families.—After separation from
ilies broke up. Family breakups of brown their mother, yearlings in 13 of 18 litters
bears in North America also have been as- with .1 young stayed together for 1–22
sociated with the presence of adult males days before they separated from one anoth-
(Egbert and Luque 1975; Herrero and Ham- er (Table 1). In other litters, yearlings sep-
er 1977; Murie 1981), and Rogers (1987) arated from their mother and siblings si-
reported that 3 female American black multaneously. Similarly, Murie (1981) re-
bears were joined by males 1–4 days after ported that brown bear littermates often as-
family breakup. sociated (for up to 2 years) after separation
Whether yearlings are chased away by from the mother. In contrast, it is uncom-
the male or by the mother is not known, but mon for yearlings of the American black
in 2 cases, yearlings were observed near the bear to stay together for even a few days
top of scotch pine trees after family break- (Clevenger and Pelton 1990; Rogers 1987).
up. In 1 case, the mother and an adult male We observed that yearlings tried to re-
were resting 300 m away; in the other, no unite with the mother after family breakup
radiotagged adult male was nearby and the in 2 families. Three siblings were with their
mother was observed alone 10 h later, so mother at 1200 h on 30 June, but at 1730
the mother may have chased her yearling h on 1 July they were 7 km from her. One
into the pine. Ternent and Garshelis (1998) of the male yearlings was with the mother
observed that a female American black bear at 1630 h on 2 July. However, at 1645 h on
turned her aggression toward her yearlings 4 July the mother was with a radiomarked
and separated from them after initially de- adult male, and the male yearling was 18
fending them against an approaching male. km away. In the 2nd case, a single male
In the family breakup reported by Herrero yearling was with his mother at 0750 h on
and Hamer (1977), yearlings made several 8 June, but at 1640 h on 8 June the yearling
attempts to reunite with the mother by ap- was near the top of a scotch pine, 1.2 km
proaching her and the adult male, but each from the mother; she moved 7 km the fol-
time they ran away, on 1 occasion in re- lowing night, but the yearling followed her
sponse to a threatening approach by the and rejoined her the next morning. The ?3
male. Yearlings have reason to fear males yearling followed his mother for 4 days,
because intraspecific predation is common falling behind when she moved (in total,
MAMMALOGY
Wednesday Feb 05 2003 09:34 PM mamm 84_202 Mp_13
Allen Press • DTPro System GALLEY 13 File # 02TQ

May 2003 DAHLE AND SWENSON—BROWN BEAR FAMILY BREAKUP

more than 50 km during these 4 days), but ka’s brown bears. National Geographic Magazine
148:428–442.
catching up with her when she rested. Clev- GREEN, W. C. H., AND A. ROTHSTEIN. 1991. Trade-offs
enger and Pelton (1990) documented reas- between growth and reproduction in female bison.
sociations of 2 American black bear fami- Oecologia 86:521–527.
HERRERO, S., AND D. HAMER. 1977. Courtship and cop-
lies, but Rogers (1987) reported that only 1 ulation of a pair of grizzly bears, with comments on
of 51 American black bear families reunited reproductive plasticity and strategy. Journal of
after family breakup, so reunion is probably Mammalogy 58:441–444.
HILDERBRAND, G. V., C. C. SCHWARTZ, C. T. ROBBINS,
not common in bears. AND T. A. HANLEY. 2000. Effects of hibernation and
reproductive status on body mass and condition of
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS coastal brown bears. Journal of Wildlife Manage-
ment 64:178–183.
This study was funded by the Norwegian Di- MACDONALD, D. W., AND C. J. AMLANER. 1980. A prac-
rectorate for Nature Management, the Swedish tical guide to radio tracking. Pp. 405–424 in A hand-
book on biotelemetry and radio tracking (C. J. Am-
Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwe- laner and D. W. Macdonald, eds.). Pergamon Press,
gian Institute for Nature Research, the Swedish Oxford, United Kingdom.
Association for Hunting and Wildlife Manage- MCLELLAN, B. 1994. Density dependent population
ment, the Norwegian University of Science and regulation in brown bears. Ninth International Con-
ference on Bear Research and Management, Mono-
Technology, World Wildlife Fund Sweden, and
graph Series 3:15–25.
the Research Council of Norway. We thank per- MOCK, D. W., AND G. A. PARKER. 1997. The evolution
sonnel in the Scandinavian Brown Bear Re- of sibling rivalry. Oxford University Press, Oxford,
search Project for their assistance in the field and United Kingdom.
2 anonymous referees for comments on a draft MURIE, A. 1981. The grizzlies of Mount McKinley.
United States Department of the Interior, National
of the article. All capture and handling reported Park Service, Washington, D.C.
in this article comply with current laws regulat- ROGERS, L. L. 1987. Effects of food supply and kinship
ing the treatment of animals in Sweden and were on social behavior, movements, and population
approved by the appropriate ethical committee. growth of black bears in northeastern Minnesota.
Wildlife Monographs 97:1–72.
SWENSON, J. E., B. DAHLE, AND F. SANDEGREN. 2001.
LITERATURE CITED Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown bears
older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus 12:81–92.
BATESON, P. 1994. The dynamics of parent-offspring
SWENSON, J. E., AND F. SANDEGREN. 1999. Conservation
relationships in mammals. Trends in Ecology and
of European brown bear populations: experiences
Evolution 9:399–403.
from Scandinavia. Pp. 111–116 in La conservacion
CLEVENGER, A. P., AND M. R. PELTON. 1990. Pre and
del oso pardo en Europa: un reto de cara al siglo
post breakup movements and space use of black bear
XXI (J. F. Layna, B. Heredia, G. Palomero, and I.
family groups in Cherokee national forest, Tennes- Doadrio, eds.). Grafistaff, S.L., Madrid, Spain.
see. International Conference on Bear Research and TERNENT, M. A., AND D. L. GARSHELIS. 1998. Male-
Management 8:289–295. instigated break-up of a family of black bears. Ursus
CLUTTON-BROCK, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental 10:575–578.
care. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New TRIVERS, R. L. 1974. Parent-offspring conflict. Amer-
Jersey. ican Zoologist 14:249–264.
CLUTTON-BROCK, T. H., F. E. GUINNESS, AND S. D. AL-
BON. 1982. Red deer. Behavior and ecology of the Submitted 26 February 2002. Accepted 28 September
two sexes. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 2002.
Illinois.
EGBERT, A. L., AND M. H. LUQUE. 1975. Among Alas- Associate Editor was Edward H. Miller.
Paper IV

Manuscript

Abandonment and reduced maternal defense as reproductive strategies in


brown bears

By

A Zedrosser, B Dahle, JO Vik and JE Swenson

Murphy’s law of covariates says that the things we measure will turn out to be
unimportant, while the things we do not (cannot) measure will turn out to be vitally
significant.
-Crawley, 1993
Offspring Abandonment and Maternal Defense as Reproductive
Strategies in European Brown Bears.

Andreas Zedrosser¹², Bjørn Dahle², Jon Olav Vik³, and Jon E. Swenson²

¹ Institute for Wildlife Biology and Game Management, Agricultural University of


Vienna, Peter-Jordan Strasse 76, 1190 Vienna, Austria
² Institute for Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural University of Norway,
Pb.5014, 1432 Ås, Norway
³ Department of Biology, University of Oslo, Pb. 1066 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway

Corresponding author:
Andreas Zedrosser, Institute for Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural
University of Norway, Pb.5014, 1432 Ås, Norway; tel: +47-6494-8516; fax: +47-
6494-8502; email: andreas.zedrosser@ibn.nlh.no

Keywords: brown bear, abandonment, maternal defense, reproductive strategy, Europe


Running head: Abandonment and maternal care in brown bears

1
ABSTRACT:
We tested whether two proposed reproductive strategies, abandonment of single cubs and
reduced maternal defense of small litters, explained observed patterns of cub mortality in
brown bears. We modeled the number of offspring recruited to the population, allowing
female brown bears to vary their behavior regarding abandoning single cubs to evaluate if
there might be a selective advantage to abandon single cubs. In the southern Scandinavian
study population, with a 2-year reproductive cycle, females did not gain by abandoning single
cub litters, whereas in the northern Scandinavian study area, females with a 3-year
reproductive cycle would gain marginally (about 2%) by doing so. Females with single cub
litters did not seem to abandon single cubs after leaving the den, but we were not able judge
whether females abandoned single cubs shortly after birth. In the southern study population,
where sexually selected infanticide (SSI) was identified as a major agent of cub mortality, the
probability of loosing one or more cubs from den emergence in spring to den entrance in fall
decreased with litter size. In our northern study population, where cub mortality was low and
SSI less common, the probability of loosing one or more cubs from a litter increased with
litter size. Our results support the maternal investment theory that mothers invest and defend
their offspring in relation to their reproductive value (i.e. the number of offspring). Protecting
cubs from males is costly for mothers, because they tended to be killed more often by
conspecifics than adult females without cubs. In conclusion, we did not find that females
abandoned single cubs, although there was a small gain in future reproduction to do so, but
females did appear to provide less defense of single cubs and small litters where SSI was
common.

2
Abandonment of dependent offspring has been documented for a variety of reasons in several
species of birds and mammals (e.g. Fair 1978; Bustnes and Erikstad 1991; Fairbanks and
McGuire 1995; Szekely and Cuthill 2000). Unprovoked abandonment (i.e. not by human
disturbance) in mammals has often been associated with reduced maternal condition, for
example in vervet monkeys (Cercophithecus aethiops sabaeus), infants are rejected by
mothers at the extremes of the reproductive age-distribution or below a critical body mass
(Fairbanks and McGuire 1995). Abandonment is most often viewed as detrimental to the
females’ fitness, because it reduces the number of offspring produced in a lifetime.
Nevertheless, in species with a variable number of offspring, abandonment of dependent
offspring by the mother may be a reproductive strategy. A female could increase her expected
number of recruits to the population by rejecting a single offspring, if she has the possibility
to produce a second, larger litter within a short time (Tait 1980).
Reduction of maternal care can have an effect similar to abandonment, if it results in the
loss of offspring. A situation where reduced maternal care can be of importance exists during
the mating season, when males may seek mating opportunities by killing dependent offspring,
i.e. sexually selected infanticide (SSI) (Hrdy 1979). According to the parental investment
theory, defense of offspring should be related to the reproductive value of the offspring
(Maynard-Smith 1984). Studies of maternal defense in relation to number of offspring have
been carried out in fish (e.g. Carlisle 1985), birds (e.g. Windt and Curio 1986) and in
mammals (e.g. Koskela et al. 2000). Experimental tests of this hypothesis in rodents have
shown that maternal defense activity increased with the number of offspring (Maestripieri and
Alleva 1991; Koskela et al. 2000).
In brown bears (Ursus arctos) the costs of maternal defense are probably independent of
litter size and can be considerable, because females can be severely wounded or killed by
males while defending their offspring (Rogers 1987; Garshelis 1994; McLellan 1994). A
female might be less prone to defend a small litter against infanticidal males in comparison to
a large litter, simply because the reproductive value is less than that of a large litter. In
addition, when losing a small litter, the female might be able to compensate this loss by
subsequently producing a larger litter.
Empirical tests of theories of reproductive behavior and life history strategies in large
mammals (> 2kg) are rare, because they require long-term individually based data, such as
growth patterns, age and size at maturity, or number, size and sex ratio of offspring. Large
mammals usually occur at low densities, are long-lived and are difficult and costly to follow
over a long time. Nevertheless, a long-term study of brown bears (Bjärvall et al. 1989;

3
Taberlet et al. 1995; Swenson et al. 1997a; Sæther et al. 1998; Dahle and Swenson 2003a) has
enabled us to study reproductive strategies and maternal care in this species. The brown bear
is a suitable species to study these topics, because it has variable litter sizes ranging from 1 to
4 cubs per litter (Pasitschniak-Arts 1993; McLellan 1994; Schwartz et al. unpublished),
exhibits 2-, 3-, and 4-year reproductive cycles (Pasitschniak-Arts 1993), and SSI is suggested
to be the major mortality agent of brown bear cubs-of-the-year during the breeding season in
Scandinavia (Swenson et al.1997a; 2001).
Tait (1980) used the brown/grizzly bear as an example to demonstrate abandonment as a
reproductive strategy. This strategy can only be of selective advantage if a female has the
ability and the possibility for rapid breeding. Female bears come into estrous only if they are
not accompanied by dependent offspring. Nevertheless, if a female bear looses her dependent
offspring prior or during the breeding season, she becomes estrous within a few days (Rogers
1987; Swenson et al. 1997a) and thus has the possibility to mate and produce another litter the
next year. In Scandinavia 8 out of 10 females that lost all young gave birth the following year,
compared with none of 40 that successfully raised cubs (Swenson et al. 1997a). We predict
that a female brown bear increases her expected number of recruits to the population by
abandoning a singleton cub prior to or during the mating season.
Maternal defense and cub survival should depend upon several factors, including social
and nutritional factors. A major social factor affecting bear cub survival is SSI (Swenson et al.
1997a; 2001), which probably occurs in varying degrees in bear populations. Because
offspring defense can be very costly for female brown bears, they should invest more in the
defense of a large litter compared to a small litter. Thus we predict that in areas where SSI is
suspected to be a major cause of cub mortality, the probability of loosing one or more
offspring is negatively related to litter size. Offspring survival has been reported to positively
correlate with offspring condition and maternal condition (Baker and Fowler 1992; Derocher
and Stirling 1996; Festa-Bianchet et al. 1997), therefore we controlled for maternal condition
in our analyses. We should only expect females to adjust their defense in relation to litter size
if offspring defense would be costly for them. Therefore we predict that females caring for
cubs should have a higher probability of being killed by conspecifics than other adult females
(females with yearlings and females without cubs).

4
Methods
Species Description
The brown bear has a circumpolar distribution and inhabits habitat types ranging from
tundra and boreal forest to temperate deciduous forests (Servheen et al. 1999). Age at first
reproduction, litter size, and interbirth intervals vary among populations and are linked to
body size and condition, which in turn are linked to nutrition (Stringham 1990, Hilderbrand
et al. 1999). In brown bears reproductive longevity was thought to approximate physical
longevity (Pasitschniak-Arts 1993), but new data presented by Schwartz et al. (unpublished)
suggest that reproductive senescence occurs approximately at the age of 27.
Mating occurs in late spring (Dahle and Swenson 2003b); brown bears are known to
exhibit obligate delayed implantation or embryonic diapause (Renfree and Calaby 1981) and
implantation is assumed to occur in late November to early December (Tsubota et al. 1989).
Cubs are born during hibernation in January. Brown bears exhibit 2-, 3-, and 4-year
reproductive cycles (Pasitschniak-Arts 1993), i.e. after giving birth the female is accompanied
by her cubs for 1.4-3.4 years. Females accompanied by offspring do not come into estrous or
mate, but usually do so the same breeding season that the offspring are weaned. Nevertheless,
after loosing dependent offspring before or during the breeding season, a female bear can
come into estrous within a few days and mate successfully (Rogers 1987; Swenson et al.
1997a).

Study Area
Data for this study were collected in two areas in Scandinavia. The southern study area
is situated in Dalarna and Gävleborg Counties in south-central Sweden, and Hedmark County
in southeastern Norway (61º N, 18º E). The northern study area is situated in Norrbotten
County in northern Sweden (67º N, 18º E). For a detailed description of the study areas see
Swenson et al. (2001), Grundsten (1997) and Lundqvist (2002).
The study populations differed in some demographic parameters (Sæther et al. 1998).
Females of the southern study population have a 2-year reproductive cycle, whereas in the
northern study area some females exhibit a 2-year reproductive cycle, and others a 3-year
cycle. Some individuals even switch between 2- year and 3-year reproductive cycles (Dahle
and Swenson 2003a). The study populations differed significantly in cub survival rates, with
cub survival being significantly lower in the south (table 2). Swenson et al. (1997a; 2001)
suggested that SSI in the southern study area was the main reason for the difference in cub
survival.

5
Field Methods
During 1984-2001, 326 different bears were captured (203 in the south, 123 in the north),
of which 284 were radio-collared (172 in the south, 112 in the north). The methods used to
capture, mark, age and radio locate bears have been described in Dahle and Swenson (2003a).
We estimated the reproductive success of females by counting the cubs of radio-collared
females from the air or the ground three times during the year: just after leaving the natal den,
at the end of the breeding season (late June-early July), and just prior to entering the den in
fall.

Model of Abandonment
The following dynamic optimization model considers whether to keep or abandon
offspring, so as to maximize fitness, defined as the expected future production of offspring
reared to independence. It extends the model by Tait (1980) by 1) allowing the female's age
and life/fertility expectancy influence her decision whether to abandon (i.e. true dynamic
optimization (Mangel and Clark 1988) rather than comparing fixed strategies), 2) including an
age-dependent risk of reproductive senescence (equivalent to death for fitness considerations),
rather than having all bears die at the exact age of 25 as in Tait (1980), and 3) allowing all
kinds of litter-size reduction, rather than considering only abandonment of singleton cubs-of-
the-year. We do not include energy reserves in the state variable of female and offspring,
because Swenson et al. (1997, 2001) found that cub survival was negatively correlated with a
condition index based on body mass of adult females and yearlings in Scandinavia. Thus, the
dynamic optimization model amounts to testing whether the time-savings can justify
abandoning offspring.
Females representing the southern study area were modeled using a two-year cycle,
females representing the northern study area with a three-year cycle. The yearly event cycle of
the model is shown in fig. 1. In the model we assume that the mother makes her choice
whether to keep or abandon her offspring at the time of leaving the den. Afterwards she may
enter reproductive senescence in the current year. She may mate if she has no offspring,
which can happen for several reasons, i.e. the previous offspring has reached independence,
all offspring have died before independence, or the female might have abandoned the
offspring. If the female then mates, she will have 0-4 cubs when leaving the den after
hibernation (the probability distributions of litter sizes is given in table 1). Offspring have an
age-dependent survival probability from year to year (table 2).

6
The state of the mother and her offspring can be characterized by 1) the number and age
of offspring, 2) the mother's reproductive status (immature, mature, senescent), and 3) the
mother's age. Tait (1980) modelled female bears as a seven-state system, where state 1
represented a potentially reproductive female with no cubs; states 2, 3, and 4 represented an
anoestrous female with, respectively, one, two, or three cubs; states 5, 6, and 7 represented an
anoestrous female with, respectively, one, two, or three yearlings. In the model presented
here, the state of a female bear is characterized by her fertility status (juvenile, fertile, or
senescent), and the number (1-4) and age (0+ to 1+) of her offspring in the current litter, if
any. The females’ age appears explicitly in the equations (see below). Not all combinations of
these state variables are possible: Juveniles have no offspring, and states with no offspring
have no associated age. Meaningful state combinations were abbreviated as listed in table 3.
These states were numbered consecutively as i = 1, 2, … in the computer implementation (see
table 4 for a list of all symbols for the equations used).
State transitions in the model can happen for four distinct reasons. Abandonment is a
behavioral decision and leads to a reduction or elimination of the current litter. Fertility status
may change by sexual maturation (for juveniles) or reproductive senescence (for fertile
females), as specified by the model of Schwartz et al. (unpublished). Litter size may change
by conception and litter bearing (specified by empirical litter-size distributions), thereafter by
natural mortality of offspring (assumed to be age-dependent, but equal and independent
among litter mates), and by rearing of offspring to independence. Finally, the age of offspring
increases by necessity when entering the next year. Figure 2-4 shows the possible state
transitions by behavioral choice (fig. 2), fertility changes (fig. 3), and litter bearing and
mortality (fig. 4), for the scenario in table 3.
Above assumptions yield the following transition probabilities: Pr (Ready before
upcoming mating season, given Juvenile in current spring): see appendix 1. Pr (Senescent
before upcoming mating season, given Ready in current spring) see appendix 1. Pr (k cubs
alive in next spring, given Ready before upcoming mating season) is given by observed litter-
size distributions. Pr (k offspring alive in next spring, given n>0 offspring in current spring) =
nCk pak (1–pa)n–k, where pa is the yearly survival of offspring of age a+ and nCk is the binomial
coefficient n!/[k!(n–k)!].
The following describes the backward optimization procedure: f ( j, a ) = max g (i, a )
i∈c j

identifies 1) the optimal decision and 2) the fitness of an a-year-old female in pre-choice state

7
j. It equals the maximum post-choice fitness a female is allowed to choose under the
behavioral strategy being examined in fig. 2.
g ( j, a ) = ∑ sij h (i, a ) represents the post-choice fitness as a weighted average of the female's
i

pre-mating fitness if her fertility status changes, or if it does not change. The weighting
corresponds to the chances for maturation or not (for juveniles), or the risk of senescence vs.
the chance of avoiding it (for fertile females). Senescent states have sjj = 0, other states have
two nonzero sij for each j (fig. 3)
h ( j, a ) = pa ∑ {tij [vij + f (i, a + 1)]} represents the pre-mating fitness as the weighted
i

expectation, over all possible fates i (fig. 4), of current fitness rewards vij (which are positive
only for j involving offspring with less than one year left to independence) plus expected
future offspring production f(i,a+1) given each fate i. It is assumed that that the mother's
survival (with probability pa) is a prerequisite for offspring survival to independence.
Above equations can be solved by going backwards one step at a time, provided an end
condition is specified. We used the end condition f ( j,50 ) = 0 for all j, equivalent to assuming
that all bears die at age fifty, if not earlier. The exact choice of end condition has negligible
impact on the fitness estimates for wild bears living to the end of the known natural lifespan
of about 30 years (Schwartz et al., unpublished) because it is unlikely that they would live to
experience the model end condition.

Statistical analyses of cub loss


We used body mass of yearlings as the basis to construct an index of the general
condition of the study populations for each year. Rather than using the actual values and just
controlling for sex (Garshelis 1994; Swenson et al. 2001), we regressed yearling body mass as
a function of maternal size, litter size and sex. In this way we controlled for maternal size,
litter size and sex of yearlings, which influence yearling mass independently of environmental
conditions (Dahle 2003). The standardized residuals from this regression were sorted by study
area and year and the average value for each year and area was then used as condition index
in later analyses.
Loss of cubs as a process can be considered to be binomial, where a female either
succeeds in raising an entire litter, or she fails, that is loosing one or more cubs. Such data are
suitable for logistic regression analysis, which fits a generalized linear model of the data by
maximum likelihood techniques (Proc Genmod, binomial distribution, logit link function, Sas

8
Inc. Institute,1996) (McCullagh and Nelder 1989). To account for overdispersion in the data
we estimated a scale parameter for a global model that accounted for female number, year and
area by using the aggregate function in Proc Genmod, for which the deviance was calculated.
However, the value of the scale parameter was estimated to be only 1.055, which suggests
that the problem of overdispersion was insignificant, thus the scale parameter was set to 1 for
further analyses.
Furthermore, we composed global models that accounted for the effects of multiple
explanative variables on the probability of successful survival of the litter. Then, after a
stepwise exclusion of the least significant terms, the final model was revealed. The
significance of all variables was tested statistically with likelihood-ratio tests (type III-tests,
McCullagh and Nelder 1989). All tests of significance were two-tailed.
To select the most appropriate model we used the Akaike’s Information criterion (AICc,
Akaike 1973; Burnham and Anderson 1998), corrected for small sample sizes, which is based
on the principle of parsimony. AICc is defined as

( ( ))
AICc = − 2 log L θˆ + 2 K +
2 K ( K + 1)
n − K −1
,

where K is the number of parameters estimated in the model, and L(θˆ) is the log likelihood of
the model. The model with the lowest AIC is assumed to be the one explaining most of the
variation by using fewest parameters. To ensure a successful estimation of the likelihood-
function we excluded the year 1989, since only two observations were available. Because all
models had similar structure and sample size, the respective AIC values from the different
models were comparable (e.g. Burnham and Anderson 1998).

Results
Model of Abandonment
A five-year-old female following a three-year reproductive cycle and representing the
population average (of litter size distribution and survival in different age classes) of our
northern study population would gain a 2.2% improvement in the expected number of
offspring throughout her lifetime if she adopts a “flexible” abandonment strategy (= a female
can decide if she abandons a single cub or not), and 2.18% improvement if she adopts an
“inflexible” abandonment strategy (= a females always abandons a single cub) (table 5)
compared to never abandoning a single cub. A five-year-old female with a two-year
reproductive cycle and representing the population average of the southern study population
would gain only a 0.11% improvement by using a “flexible” strategy, and 0.03% when using

9
an “inflexible” strategy. In comparison, Tait’s (1980) original result was a 2.1% improvement
in the expected number of offspring, but Tait (1980) had chosen the example of an eight-year-
old female, due to the later age of primiparity in North American brown bear populations in
comparison to Europe.

Statistical analysis of cub loss


The initial global model of the probability of loosing one or more cubs included the
following explanatory variables: year, area, condition index, litter size, in addition to the
interaction terms (area* condition index) and (area*litter size). After a successive exclusion of
the least significant terms, the final model included the effects of year, area, condition index,
litter size and the interaction term (area* litter size) (table 6). This model was also best ranked
according to the AICc (table 7) and suggests that the probability of loosing one or more cubs
during the summer varies among years. During the 12 years of the study there was a
significant annual variation in the average probability of cub loss from spring to fall (table 6).
In particular, this was due to the years 1991 and 2000, where the average loss of litters was
high. The average proportion of litters that lost one or more cubs varied significantly between
study areas, and the probability of loss was higher in the south compared to the north (table
6). There was no significant effect of the condition index or litter size in spring on the average
probability of successful litter survival from spring to fall (P>0.1 for both tests) when both
study areas were combined. As predicted, the interaction term between litter size and area
suggests that in the north the probability of cub loss was low, but increased with increasing
litter size. In contrast, in the south the probability of cub loss was high, but decreased with
increasing litter size (table 6, fig. 5).
Unexpectedly, the condition index also had a positive effect on the probability of
loosing one or more offspring and was the opposite of what we expected. However, the model
including area*condition interaction ranked third by AICc (table 7) showed that the
relationship between condition and the probability of loosing one or more offspring was
positive in the southern study area, but negative in the northern study area.
A possible explanation for this unexpected positive relationship in the southern study
area is that males might have increased their mating season ranges when they were in good
condition. Increasing their ranges in the mating season may result in increased infanticide
because it would increase the probability of males meeting females with cubs witch they did
not mate the previous mating season. Range size during the mating season of radiomarked
males, as revealed by 100% minimum convex polygon based on positions obtained at 7 days

10
interval, in the southern study area tended to be larger in 1997 (condition index =1) than the
average pooled size in the two previous and two following years (condition index = -0.26, -
0.05, -0.09 and 0.17 in 1995, 1996, 1998 and 1999, respectively) (paired t = 2.014, df = 6, P =
0.091).
We also evaluated the costs of maternal defense. Three adult females (one in the
northern and two in the southern study area) were killed by males during the study period
from 1986-2001, and all of them were caring for cubs. These were the only natural mortalities
in adult females followed for 329 bear years (a bear year reflects an individual bear followed
for 12 months). The probability of adult females being killed by conspecifics tended to be
higher for females caring for cubs, including females that lost all cubs after leaving the den
(three females killed during 138 bear years), than for other adult females (no females killed
during 191 bear years, Fishers exact test, P=0.076). Two of the killed females had litters with
three cubs and 1 female had a litter with two cubs.

Discussion
Abandonment of cubs by female bears for various reasons has been mentioned
frequently in the literature for American black bears (Ursus americanus) (Fair 1978; LeCount
1983; Godfrey et al. 2000) and brown bears (Erickson and Miller 1963; Craighead et al. 1969;
Swenson et al. 1997b; 1998). Most authors relate abandonment to human disturbance at den
sites of nursing females, in which case the female often abandons her den and thus the cubs
inside (Swenson et al. 1997b; 2001; Godfrey et al. 2000). Human disturbance after the family
has left the den can also cause cub abandonment (Swenson et al. 1997b). Fair (1978)
suggested that that female American black bears do not jeopardize their own survival but
rather abandon their offspring as a response to severe environmental conditions, and
Fairbanks and McGuire (1995) suggested the same in their study of the vervet monkey.
Support for opportunistic abandonment in bears, i.e. abandonment of single offspring
in order to maximize lifetime reproduction, is to our knowledge very rare in the literature.
Dean et al. (1992) reported anecdotal observations of grizzly bears that could be interpreted as
support of Tait’s (1980) hypothesis, however no support for opportunistic abandonment was
found in polar bears (Ursus maritimus) (Ramsay and Stirling 1986; Taylor et al. 1987;
Amstrup and Durner 1995).
The results of our model calculations, whether or not using a flexible or inflexible
strategy, for the northern study area and recalculation of Taits original data are similar to
Tait’s (1980) original results, approximately in the 2%-range (table 5). Females in the

11
northern study area and North American female brown bears both have a three-year cycle. In
comparison, the fitness improvement for a female of the southern study area, with a two-year
reproductive cycle, is only marginal. Accordingly, females with longer reproductive cycles (3
or more years) would gain most by abandoning single offspring. This can be explained by the
time a female would gain if she shortens the period to her next litter; the longer this interval
is, the more a female should gain.
Tait (1980) did not consider reproductive senescence in his model and a given female
could only use an “always abandon” vs. a “never abandon” strategy. We accounted for
reproductive senescence in our model. Compared to Tait's fixed-strategy model, our
optimization model, which bases decisions based on offspring number and age of offspring
and mother, shows that "always abandon" is sub-optimal for old bears, which have a high risk
of entering senescence before the next mating season. Although strategies may differ strongly
in fitness for old individuals, selection pressure is weak at older ages, because there is little
chance that a bear lives long enough to have the opportunity to benefit from its optimal
behavior (Stearns 1992). Thus, although being able to refrain from abandonment might be
useful for old bears, it may not be evolutionarily important. Tait (1980) assumed that the
population growth rate was zero and that the fitness of all offspring was equal and
independent of litter size. In addition Tait (1980) assigned his model to work with stable
populations, whereas we are working with data from increasing populations (Sæther et al.
1998), where offspring produced early in life have a higher relative fitness than offspring
produced later (Stearns 1992). We did not control for this in our model, but the benefit by
abandoning a singleton litter should be smaller in an increasing population.
The model showed that abandonment resulted in a small theoretical improvement of
the number of offspring during a lifetime, but it cannot determine whether female bears
abandon or not. Our methods did not allow us to evaluate the occurrence of early
abandonment of offspring. Brown bear cubs are born in the den, and in contrast to American
black bears, it is not advisable to visit a female brown bear in the den for human safety
reasons. Thus we did not know the actual litter size, which may be larger than the litter size
we observed after the female left the den. It makes sense to assume that a female might decide
whether she should abandon or not soon after birth, thus avoiding investing energy into
offspring she would leave behind. We could not differentiate between failure of conception
and offspring abandonment at an early stage. If the timing of abandonment occurred when
leaving the den or shortly thereafter, as Tait (1980) suggested, our methods were adequate to
test the model’s results. All natal dens in the southern study area were examined for cub

12
remains, and the litter size was visually determined in both study areas. A methodological
drawback is the fact that dens in the northern study area are not examined for cub remains
because of accessibility reasons. Nevertheless, if the timing of abandonment would be when
leaving the den or shortly thereafter, it should have resulted in a u-shaped function in the
northern study are (see fig. 5). Because this was not found, we suggest that a female brown
bear does not opportunistically abandon single offspring after she has raised them to an age
where they can follow her out of the den.
Mortality of cubs can be high during the first weeks after the den emergence (Ramsay
and Stirling 1988; Elowe and Dodge 1989; Swenson et al. 1997a). Nevertheless, if a single
cub manages to survive these first critical weeks after leaving the den, it might be better off
than cubs in larger litters (Mendl 1994), because it will probably be heavier as a yearling than
cubs from larger litters (Derocher and Stirling 1996; Dahle 2003).
In conclusion, a female brown bear would theoretically gain from opportunistic
abandonment, especially if she has a three-year or longer reproductive cycle, although the
gain would be small. Opportunistic abandonment after leaving the den was not observed, but
we were not able to document whether abandonment occurred earlier.
Several factors have been proposed as important for bear cub survival. Swenson et al
(2001) grouped these factors into nutritional (e.g. food availability, maternal condition), social
(intraspecific predation, SSI), and disturbance factors (e.g. human disturbance causes a female
to leave her den and thus abandon the cubs). The decrease of the probability of loosing one or
more cubs with increasing litter size in the southern study area, an area where SSI is thought
to be a major mortality factor in cubs (Swenson et al. 1997a; 2001), is best explained by
varying levels of maternal offspring defense according to litter size. This is consistent with the
predictions of the parental investment theory and studies carried out in other mammals,
especially rodents (e.g., Maestripieri and Alleva 1991; Koskela et al. 2000), and other taxa
(e.g. Carlisle 1985; Windt and Curio 1986).
Offspring survival has been reported to correlate positively with offspring mass and
maternal mass and condition in several mammalian species (e.g., moose (Alces alces): Keech
et al. 2000; northern fur seal (Callorhinus ursinus): Baker and Fowler 1992; bighorn sheep
(Ovis canadensis): Festa-Bianchet et al. 1997). The same relationship has been suggested for
bears (American black bears: Rogers 1987; polar bears: Ramsey and Stirling 1988; Derocher
and Stirling 1996). Our results show that condition had a positive effect on the probability of
loosing one or more cubs. This result was counter-intuitive, based on literature and logic.

13
Nevertheless, Swenson et al. (1997a; 2001) also found negative relationships between cub
survival and yearling body mass and cub survival and adult female spring body mass.
In polar bears, Derocher and Stirling (1995) did not find a decrease in litter size during
a time period of decrease in body mass and litter production. Due to our methods we were not
able to measure cub mass, but the yearling mass used in our analyses should reflect cub mass.
In brown bears offspring mass decreases with increasing litter size (Dahle 2003), which has
also been reported in other species (e.g., Allainé et al. 1998; Derocher and Stirling 1998;
Andersen et al. 2000). Additionally polar bear cub survival increases with mass and size
(Derocher and Stirling 1996). Thus, if cub survival were a function of cub mass and
nutritional factors, we would expect a higher probability of cub loss in large litters. Another
reason for increased mortality in large litters is, that variance in yearling size increases with
litter size (Derocher and Stirling 1996; Dahle 2003), often resulting in one very small
yearling. Derocher and Stirling (1996) showed that in triplet polar bear litters individual
survival varied according to cub size. A third reason for an increased cub mortality in a large
litters is that the probability of no cub loss in a litter is the independent cub survival rate
raised by the power of the number of cubs in the litter. Thus, our results from the southern
study area where probability of loosing one or more cubs decreased with litter size strongly
indicates that mothers invest in litters in relation to their reproductive value. In areas where
SSI is not suspected to play a major role, such as the northern study area, cub-mortality
patterns should reflect random events. Accordingly, the probability of losing one or more
offspring should increase with increasing litter size, as we found (fig. 5).
Maestripieri (1992) reviewed the functional aspects of maternal aggression in mammals,
and suggested that female aggressiveness during lactation is aimed at protecting offspring.
Offspring defense against an infanticidal male is potentially very dangerous for females, and
probably independent of litter size, because the chances for a female to be wounded or killed
are the same regardless of the number of offspring. In lions (Panthera leo) the invading males
usually commit infanticide after a male coalition replacement, and the lionesses of the pride
often engage in direct defense of the cubs (Packer and Pusey 1984). Packer and Pusey (1983)
reported that in two attempted male takeovers, three of five females were wounded and two
other disappeared and were assumed to have died as a result of cub defense. In bears several
authors report females being severely injured or killed by conspecifics while defending their
offspring (Rogers 1987; Garshelis 1994; McLellan 1994). However, these authors did not test
whether females caring for cubs had a higher probability of being killed by conspecifics than
other adult females. Our results, although not significant at the 0.05 level, suggest that

14
females defending cubs have a greater risk than other adult females of being killed by
conspecifics.
However, offspring defense should be related to the reproductive value of a litter (i.e.,
offspring number). Maestripieri and Alleva (1991) found that female house mice (Mus
musculus) with large litters defend their offspring more aggressively than females with small
litters, as Koskela et al. (2000) and Jonsson et al. (2002) found in several studies of the bank
vole (Clethrionomys glareolus). In the southern study area, where SSI is a major mortality
factor of cubs (Swenson et al. 1997a; 2001), cub survival increased with increasing litter size.
A three-cub litter represents a higher reproductive value than a singleton litter; accordingly
the mother has more to “win”, and thus apparently defended this litter more than a smaller
litter. This is in accordance with the parental investment theory and suggests that females
adjusted their defense intensity according to the reproductive value of their litter. Anecdotal
field observations support the hypothesis of reduced maternal defense activity for singleton
litters (Troyer and Hensel 1962).
The results from the southern study area were in contrast with the results from the
northern study area, where SSI was not common, and where the cub mortality pattern seemed
to reflected random mortality. In areas where SSI is suspected to be important, we do not
consider nutritional factors to be important in explaining cub loss either on a spatial or
temporal scale. Swenson et al. (1997a; 2001) reached the same conclusion. A possible
explanation for the negative relationship between condition and cub survival might be that in
years when males are in better than average condition, they may increase their breeding home
ranges. This would increase the chances for males to meet a female with dependent offspring
that they had not mated with during the last breeding season, and increase the potential to
commit SSI. Derocher and Stirling (1996) mention that if adult male polar bears are a major
factor affecting juvenile survival, it should occur independently of maternal mass and
maternal condition. However, our test of adult male brown bears increasing their mating
season ranges in years of good conditions can only be regarded as suggestive (P=0.091).
In conclusion, we did not find that females abandoned single cubs, although there was a
small gain in future reproduction to do so. However, females did appear to provide less
defense of single cubs and small litters where SSI was common.

Acknowledgements
We thank the Austrian Science Fund, Swedish Environmental Protection Agency, Swedish
Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, Norwegian Institute for Nature Research,

15
Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management, WWF (World Wide Fund for Nature)
Sweden, and the Research Council for financial support to the project. We thank especially T.
H. Ringsby for statistical support.

16
Literature Cited

Allainé, D., L. Graziani, and J. Coulon. 1998. Postweaning mass gain in juvenile alpine
marmots Marmota marmota. Oecologia 113:370-376.
Andersen, R, G. M. Gaillard, J. D. C. Linnell, and P. Duncan. 2000. Factors affecting
maternal care in an income breeder, the European roe deer. Journal of Animal Ecology
69:672-682.
Akaike, H. 1973. Information theory as an extension of a maximum likelihood principle.
Proceedings of the Second International Symposium on Information theory:267-281.
Armstrup, S. C., and G. M. Durner. 1995. Survival rates of radio-collared female polar bears
and their dependent young. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73:1312-1322.
Baker, J.D., and C. W. Fowler. 1992. Pup weight and survival of the northern fur seal
Callorhinus ursinus. Journal of Zoology 227:231-238.
Bjärvall, A., F. Sandegren, and P. Wabakken.1989. Large home ranges and possible early
maturity in Scandinavian bears. Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on
Bear Research and Management:237-241.
Burnham, K. P., and D. R. Anderson. 1998. Model selection and inference, a practical
information-theoretic approach. Springer Verlag, New York.
Bustnes, J. O., and K. E. Erikstad. 1991. Parental care in the common eider (Somateria
molissima) – factors affecting abandonment and adoption of young. Canadian Journal of
Zoology 69:1538-1545.
Carlisle, T. R. 1985. Parental response to brood size in a cichlid fish. Animal Behaviour
33:234-238.
Craighead, J. J., F. C. Craighead, Jr., and J. Sumner. 1969. Reproductive biology of young
female grizzly bears. Journal of Reproductive Fertility, Supplement 6:447-475.
Dahle, B. 2003. Reproductive strategies in Scandinavian brown bears. Dr. scient.-thesis,
Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway.
Dahle, B., and J. E. Swenson. 2003b. Family break-up in brown bears; are young forced to
leave? Journal of Mammalogy (In press).
Dahle, B., and J. E. Swenson. 2003a. Factors influencing the length of maternal care and its
consequences for offspring in brown bears Ursus arctos. Behavioural Ecology and
Sociobiology (In press)
Dean, F. C., R. McIntyre, and R. A. Sellers. 1992. Additional mixed-age brown bear, Ursus
arctos, associations in Alaska. Canadian Field Naturalist 106:257-259.

17
Derocher, A. E., and I. Stirling. 1995. Temporal variation in reproduction and body mass of
polar bears in western Hudson Bay. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73:1657-1665.
Derocher, A. E., and I. Stirling. 1996. Aspects of survival in juvenile polar bears. Canadian
Journal of Zoology 74:1246-1252.
Derocher, A. E., and I. Stirling. 1998. Maternal investment and factors affecting offspring size
in polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Journal of Zoology (London) 243:253-260.
Derocher, A. E., and O. Wiig. 1999. Observation of adoption in polar bears (Ursus
maritimus). Arctic 52:413-415.
Elowe, K. D., and W. E. Dodge. 1989. Factors affecting black bear reproductive success and
cub survival. Journal of Wildlife Management 53:962-968.
Erickson, A. W., and L. H. Miller. 1963. Cub adoption in the brown bear. Journal of
Mammalogy 44:584-585.
Fair, J. S. 1978. Unusual dispersal of black bear cubs in Utah. Journal of Wildlife
Management 42:642-644.
Fairbanks, L. A., and M. T. McGuire. 1995. Maternal condition and the quality of maternal
care in vervet monkeys. Behaviour 132:733-754.
Festa-Bianchet, M., J. T. Jorgenson, C. H. Berube, C. Porter, and W. D. Wishhart. 1997. Body
mass and survival of bighorn sheep. Canadian Journal of Zoology 75:1372-1379.
Garshelis, D. L. 1994. Density-dependent population regulation of black bears. International
Conference on Bear Research and Management, Monograph Series 3:3 – 14.
Godfrey, C. L., K. Needham, M. R. Vaughan, J. H. Vashon, D. D. Martin, and G. T. Blank.
2000. A technique for and risks associated with entering tree dens used by black bears.
Wildlife Society Bulletin 28:131-140.
Grundsten, C. 1997. The Laponian area. A Swedish world heritage site. Naturvårdsverket,
Stockholm, Sweden.
Hilderbrand, G. V., C. C. Schwartz, C. T. Robbins, M. E. Jacoby, T. A. Hanley, S. M. Arthur,
and C. Servheen. 1999. The importance of meat, particularly salmon, to body size,
population productivity, and conservation of North American brown bears. Canadian
Journal of Zoology 77:132-138.
Hrdy, S. B. 1979. Infanticide among mammals: a review, classification, and examination of
the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology
1:13-40.

18
Jonsson, P., J. Agrell, E. Koskela, and T. Mappes. 2002. Effects of litter size on pup defence
and weaning success of neighbouring bank vole females. Canadian Journal of Zoology
80:1-5.
Keech, M. A., R. T. Bowyer, J. M. VerHoef, R. D. Bortje, B. W. Dale, and T. R. Stephenson.
2000. Life-history consequences of maternal condition in Alaskan moose. Journal of
Wildlife Management 64:450-462.
Koskela, E., P. Juutistenaho, T. Mappes, and T. A. Oksanen. 2000. Offspring defence in
relation to litter size and age: experiment in the bank vole Clethrionomys glareolus.
Evolutionary Ecology 14:99-109.
LeCount, A. L. 1983. Evidence of wild black bears breeding while raising cubs. Journal of
Wildlife Management 47:264-268.
Lundqvist, R. 2002. Fulufjället. Nationalpark i Dalafjällen. Naturvårdsverket, Stockholm,
Sweden.
Maestripieri, D. 1992. Functional aspects of maternal aggression in mammals. Canadian
Journal of Zoology 70:1069-1077.
Maestripieri, D., and E. Alleva. 1991. Litter defense and parental investment allocation in
house mice. Behavioural Processes 23:223-230.
Mangel, M., and C. W. Clark. 1988. Dynamic modeling in behavioral ecology. Princeton
University Press, Princeton, N. J.
Maynard-Smith, J. 1984. The ecology of sex. In Behavioural Ecology. Edited by Krebs, J.R.,
and Davies, N.B. Blackwell Scientific Publications, USA:201-221.
McCullagh, P., and J. A. Nelder. 1989. Generalized linear models. Second edition Chapman
and Hall, New York, N. Y.
McLellan, B. 1994. Density-dependent population regulation of brown bears. International
Conference on Bear Research and Management, Monograph Series 3:15 – 24.
Mendl, M. 1994. How should parents respond to a reduction in litter size: Abandonment or
extra care? Behavioural Processes 31:285-292.
Packer, C., and A. E. Pusey. 1983. Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming
males. American Naturalist 121:716-728.
Packer, C., and A. E. Pusey. 1984. Infanticide in carnivores. In Infanticide: comparative and
evolutionary perspectives. Edited by G. Hausfater and S. B. Hrdy. Aldine Publishing Co.,
New York. pp. 31-42
Pasitschniak-Arts, M. 1993. Ursus arctos. Mammalian Species No. 439:1-10.

19
Ramsay, M. A., and I. Stirling. 1986. On the mating system of polar bears. Canadian Journal
of Zoology 64:2142-2151.
Ramsay, M. A., and I. Stirling. 1988. Reproductive biology of female polar bears (Ursus
maritimus). Journal of Zoology (London) 214:601-634.
Renfree, M. B., and J. H. Calaby. 1981. Background to delayed implantation and embryonic
diapause. Journal of Reproductive Fertility, Supplement 29:1-9.
Rogers, L. L. 1987. Effects of food supply and kinship on social behavior, movements, and
population growth of black bears in northeastern Minnesota. Wildlife Monographs 97:1-
72.
Servheen, C., S. Herrero, and B. Peyton. 1999. Bears. Status survey and conservation action
plan. IUCN/SSC Bear and Polar Bear Specialist Groups. Gland, Switzerland and
Cambridge, UK.
Stearns, S. C. 1992. The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press, New York, USA.
Stringham, S. F. 1990. Grizzly bear reproductive rate relative to body size. Proceedings of the
International Conference on Bear Research and Management 8:433-443.
Swenson, J. E., F. Sandegren, A. Bjärvall, A. Söderberg, P. Wabakken, and R. Franzén. 1994.
Size, trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear Ursus arctos population in
Sweden. Biological Conservation 70:9-17.
Swenson, J. E., F. Sandegren, A. Söderberg, A. Bjärvall, R. Franzén, and P. Wabakken.
1997a. Infanticide caused by hunting of male bears. Nature 386:450-451.
Swenson, J. E., F. Sandegren, S. Brunberg, and P. Wabakken. 1997b. Winter den
abandonment by brown bears Ursus arctos: causes and consequences. Wildlife Biology
3:35-38.
Swenson, J. E., R. Franzén, P. Segerström, and F. Sandegren. 1998. On the age of self-
sufficiency in Scandinavian brown bears. Acta Theriologica 43:213-218.
Swenson, J. E., F. Sandegren, S. Brunberg, and P. Segerström. 2001 Factors associated with
the loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12:69-80.
Szekely, T., and I. C. Cuthill. 2000. Trade-off between mating opportunities and parental
care: brood desertion by female Kentish plovers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London, Series B-Biological Sciences 26:2087-2092.
Sæther, B.-E., S. Engen, J. E. Swenson, Ø. Bakke, and F. Sandegren. 1998. Assessing the
viability of Scandinavian brown bear, Ursus arctos, populations: the effect of uncertain
parameter estimates. Oikos 83:403-416.

20
Taberlet, P., J. E. Swenson, F. Sandegren, and A. Bjärvall. 1995. Localization of a contact
zone between two highly divergent mitochondrial DNA lineages of the brown bear Ursus
arctos in Scandinavia. Conservation Biology 9:1255-1261.
Tait, D. E. N. 1980. Abandonment as a reproductive tactic – the example of grizzly bears.
American Naturalist 115:800-808.
Taylor, M. K., D. P. DeMaster, F. L. Bunnell, and R. E. Schweinsburg. 1987. Modeling the
sustainable harvest of female polar bears. Journal of Wildlife Management 51:811-820.
Troyer, W. A., and R. J. Hensel. 1962. Cannibalism in brown bears. Animal Behaviour
10:321.
Tsubota, T., H. Kanagawa, T. Mano, and T. Aoi. 1989. Corpora albicantia and placental scars
in the Hokkaido brown bear. Proceedings of the International Conference on Bear
Research and Management 8:125-128.
Windt, W., and E. Curio. 1986. Clutch defense in great tit (Parus major) pairs and the
concorde fallacy. Ethology 72:236-242.

21
Table 1: Parameters of brown bear litter size distribution used for model calculations.
0 cubs 1 cub 2 cubs 3 cubs 4 cubs
Southern study area¹ 0.200 0.112 0.360 0.256 0.072
Northern study area¹ 0.200 0.104 0.336 0.336 0.024
Tait’s original data² 0.150 0.204 0.3655 0.2805³ -
¹ Sæther et al. (1998)
² Tait (1980)
³ Tait (1980) combined data for litters of 3 and 4 cubs

22
Table 2: Survival parameters (survival of young brown bears from spring to the following
spring for a given age) used for model calculations.
age 0 age 1 age 2 age 3 age 4+
Southern study area¹ 0.7720 0.8697 0.9270 0.9507 0.9685
Northern study area¹ 0.9601 0.9517 0.9416 0.9296 0.9153
Tait’s original data² 0.9000 0.9000 0.8500 0.8500 0.8500
¹ Swenson et al. (1997a), Sæther et al. (1998)
² Tait (1980)

23
Table 3: Coding of state variables for a population with independence at age 2 (so 1+ is the
last age represented in the female's state) and a maximum litter size of 4. For other ages of
independence or maximum litter size, the numbering is changed accordingly.
Acronym State number Description
J 1 Juvenile
R 2 Ready to mate, having no current offspring
C1…C4 3…6 1-4 cubs (age 0+), mother still fertile
Y1…Y4 7…10 1-4 yearlings (age 1+), mother still fertile
S 11 Reproductively senescent, having no current offspring
SC1…SC4 12…15 As C1-C4, but can have no further litters
SY1…SY4 16…19 As Y1-Y4, but can have no further litters

24
Table 4: List of symbols for the equations in the dynamic optimization model. Fitness is
defined as the expected future production of offspring reared to independence. The terms
"pre-choice", "post-choice" and "pre-mating" fitness refer to the timeline shown in fig. 1.
Symbol Description
i State number; when referring to state transitions, i denotes the state after the
transition (called the "to-state" below)
j When referring to state transitions, j denotes the state before the transition
(called the "from-state" below)
f(i,a) Pre-choice fitness of female in state i at age a+
g(i,a) Post-choice fitness of female in state i at age a+
h(i,a) Pre-mating fitness of female in state i at age a+
pa Probability of spring-to-spring survival from age a to a+1
cj The set of possible destination to-states (i) that can be chosen from a given
from-state (j)
sija Probability of transition to state i from state j by change in fertility status, for a
female of age a+
tij Probability of transition to state i from state j by litter-bearing, aging and
survival of offspring, or rearing to independence
vij Value of the transition corresponding to tij (equal to number of offspring with
less than one year to independence, times their probability of surviving through
that time interval, given that the mother survives).

25
Table 5: Expected number of recruits for a female brown bear.
Expected number of recruits
5-year-old 8-year-old
Tait original
Study area North South
results
Without abandonment 5.501 11.919 2.341
With abandonment/inflexible strategy 5.621 11.923 2.391
Change (%) 2.18 0.03 2.1
Without abandonment 5.501 11.919 -
With abandonment/flexible strategy 5.622 11.932 -
Change (%) 2.2 0.11 -

26
Table 6: The global logistic regression model explaining the probability of successful litter
survival in brown bear included the variables year, area, condition index, litter size in spring,
(area* litter size in spring), (area* condition index). After a successive exclusion of the least
significant terms, the final model is shown in the table. Df is degrees of freedom, β is the
logistic regression coefficient, SE is the standard error, χ2 denotes the chi-square value, and P
denotes the significance level, obtained by likelihood ratio tests (type III). N=124. See text for
further details.
Explanatory variables df β SE χ2 P
Year 11 27.85 0.0034
Area 1 10.86 0.0010
Condition index 1 -4.2016 2.2272 10.73 0.0011
Litter size in spring 1 2.1751 1.4115 1.48 0.2237
(Area* litter size in spring) 1 5.80 0.0161
Southern study area 1 -2.7690 1.4433
Northern study area 0 0.0000 0.0000

27
Table 7: Logistic regression models explaining the probability of successful litter survival
ranked by their AICc.
Model rank Explanatory model AICC AICC, DEV
1 Year, area, condition index, litter size in 156.844
spring, area*litter size in spring
2 Area 158.479 1.635
3 Year, area, condition index, litter size in 158.974 2.129
spring, area*litter size in spring,
area*condition index
4 Year, area, condition index 158.993 2.149
5 Year, area, condition index, litter size in 160.004 3.160
spring
6 Year, area, condition index, 161.552 4.708
area*condition index
7 Year, area 166.029 9.185
8 Litter size in spring 171.019 14.175
9 Year 174.875 18.031

28
Figure legends

Figure 1: Timeline for the abandonment model, depicting the two life cycles used.

Figure 2: Possible state transitions by choice, i.e. reducing or eliminating the current litter, if
any. This can be modified to represent various fixed or more-or-less flexible strategies. For
instance, the strategy "never abandon" only allows staying in the current state, which would
be represented by a diagonal matrix. The strategy "always abandon a single cub" would not
allow staying in state C1, leaving state "Ready" as the only possible choice. The matrix shown
here allows any kind of litter-size reduction, although here the only plausible alternatives are
for a fertile female to desert her entire litter. This is because 1) reducing litter size improves
neither the quality nor the survival of the remaining offspring, under our present assumptions,
and 2) senescent females have nothing to gain by abandoning offspring.

Figure 3: Possible state transitions by change in fertility status. Juveniles may become fertile,
fertile females may enter senescence, and senescent females will stay senescent.

Figure 4: Possible state transitions by reproduction, aging and mortality of offspring, and
rearing offspring to independence. Transitions to Ready from states with Yearlings may or
may not mean that the yearlings survived to independence (the expected "fitness reward"
equals the number of yearlings times their probability of over-winter survival). All transitions
are assumed to be conditional on the mother's survival.

Figure 5: Probability of loosing one or more brown bear cubs in litters from spring to fall in
relation to litter size in the study areas in Scandinavia based on the logistic regression model
in table 6.

29
[Figure 1, Zedrosser et al.]

Timeline for abandonment model: Life cycle


Cycle of females in southern Cycle of females in northern study
study area area and North America

Model State of offspring Female action Time Female action State of offspring Behavior

Year 0:
Event sequence: J
Collect reward if any F
cubs M
Mating A
Assessment of state Mating M Mating
Behavioral decision J Mating
J
A
S
O Enter den
Enter den N
D
Year 1:
Cubs with mother Birth J Birth Cubs with mother
F
M
Leave den A Period when abandonment should take
Assess state (1 cub?) (No mating) M Leave den place:
Decide (abandon?) J (No mating)
J after leaving den
A and before (during) mating
S season
O Enter den
Enter den
N
D
Year 2:
J
F
M
Collect reward Leave den A Cubs with mother (Could abandon
Cubs alone (yearlings) Mating M Leave den (yearlings) yearlings)
Mating J (No mating)
J
A
S
Enter den O Enter den
N
D
Year 3:
Next birth J
F
M
A Leave den Cubs alone (2-year-olds) Collect reward
M Mating
J Mating
J
A
S
Enter den O Enter den
N
D
Year 4:
J Next birth
F
M
A
M
J
J
A
S
O

30
[Figure 2, Zedrosser et al.]

From state

SC1
SC2
SC3
4
1
2
3
4
To state

SC

SY
SY
SY
SY
1
2
3
4
Y1
Y2
Y3
Y4
R
C
C
C
C

S
J

J z
R z z z z z z z z z
C1 z z z z
C2 z z z
C3 z z
C4 z
Y1 z z z z
Y2 z z z
Y3 z z
Y4 z
S z z z z z z z z z
SC1 z z z z
SC2 z z z
SC3 z z
SC4 z
SY1 z z z z
SY2 z z z
SY3 z z
SY4 z

31
[Figure 3, Zedrosser et al.]

From state

SC1
SC2
SC3
4
1
2
3
4
To state

SC

SY
SY
SY
SY
1
2
3
4
Y1
Y2
Y3
Y4
R
C
C
C
C

S
J

J z
R z z
C1 z
C2 z
C3 z
C4 z
Y1 z
Y2 z
Y3 z
Y4 z
S z z
SC1 z z
SC2 z z
SC3 z z
SC4 z z
SY1 z z
SY2 z z
SY3 z z
SY4 z z

32
[Figure 4, Zedrosser et al.]

From state

SC1
SC2
SC3
4
1
2
3
4
To state

SC

SY
SY
SY
SY
1
2
3
4
Y1
Y2
Y3
Y4
R
C
C
C
C

S
J

J z
R z z z z z z z z z
C1
C2
C3
C4
Y1 z z z z
Y2 z z z
Y3 z z
Y4 z
S z z z z z z z z z
SC1
SC2
SC3
SC4
SY1 z z z z
SY2 z z z
SY3 z z
SY4 z

33
[Figure 5, Zedrosser et al.]

0.8
Probability of loosing one or more cubs

0.7
South
0.6

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2 North

0.1

0.0
1 2 3 4

Litter size

34
Appendix: deriving maturation probability and senescence risk
Maturation
C. Schwartz et al. (in prep) modeled the probability density of age at maturity as
ξM
−1  ξM

1  a θM   a θM 
f M (a ) =   exp −    , where a is the age, ξM = 4.480 (the modal age at
θ M  ξ M   ξM  
 
maturity) and θM = 0.730 (related to the variance in age at maturity).
a
Letting FM(a) = ∫ f M (t )dt = Pr(mature at age ≤ a) denote the cumulative distribution
0

function of the age at maturity, we can derive the state-transition probability required for the
dynamic optimization model. This is:
Pr(become mature [state "Ready"] before the upcoming mating season, given immature [state
FM (a ) − FM (a − 1)
"Juvenile"] in the current spring) = ,
1 − FM (a − 1)
In words, the above equation gives the probability of maturing between age a–1 and a, as a
proportion of those not yet mature at age a–1.
Senescence
An analogous calculation gives the risk that nonsenescent females enter senescence in the
current year. C. Schwartz et al. (in prep) modeled the probability density of age at senescence
as
ξS
−1  ξS

1  a θS   a θS 
f S (a ) =   exp −    , where ξS = 27.324 (the modal age at senescence) and θS
θS ξS   ξS  
 
= 3.274 (related to the variance in age at senescence).
Letting FS(a) = Pr(enter senescence at age ≤ a) denote the cumulative distribution of the age
at senescence, we get
Pr(become Senescent before the upcoming mating season, given nonsenescent in the current
FS (a ) − FS (a − 1)
spring) =
1 − FS (a − 1)
This is the probability of senescing between ages a–1 and a, as a proportion of those not yet
senescent at age a–1.

35
1.00

0.75
Pr(mat=age)
probability

Pr(sen=age)
Pr(mat <= age)
0.50
Pr(mat=age | mat>=age)
Pr(sen <= age)
Pr(sen=age | sen >=age)
0.25

0.00
0 10 20 30 40
age (years)

Figure 1. Distributions of age at maturity (solid lines) and senescence (dashed lines), used in
our dynamic optimization model on the profitability of abandoning offspring. Thick lines
show state-transition probabilities, i.e. the probability of maturation (resp. senescence) in the
current year, given that it has not yet occurred. These were derived from the probability
density (area charts) and cumulative distribution (thin lines) functions given by Schwartz et
al. (in prep.).

36
Paper V

Ursus 12: 81-92

Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown bears older than cubs of the


year

By

JE Swenson, B Dahle and F Sandegren

Birth is painful, old age is painful, sickness is painful, death is painful


-The teachings of the compassionate Buddha
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN SCANDINAVIAN BROWN BEARS OLDER THAN
CUBS-OF-THE-YEAR
JON E. SWENSON, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural University of Norway, Box 5014, N-1432 Ås,
Norway and Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway, email: jon.swenson@ibn.nlh.no
BJØRN DAHLE, Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway
FINN SANDEGREN, Research Unit, Swedish Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, Bäcklösavägen 8, SE-756 51 U
Uppsala, Sweden

Abstract: Intraspecific predation of bears >1 year old is observed occasionally, but the mechanisms behind it are poorly understood and often
debated. We documented 13 cases of intraspecific predation in brown bears (Ursus arctos) in 2 Scandinavian study areas during 668 bear-years of
radiotracking 238 brown bears. We found area differences in the rates of intraspecific predation only for yearling females. Annual yearling female
mortality due to intraspecific predation was higher (0.162, 6 of 38) in the south than in the north (no mortality recorded, 28 yearlings followed). No
older subadult females were killed by other bears. Annual mortality rates due to intraspecific predation for males, areas combined, were: 0.032 for
yearlings, 0.040 for 2-year-olds, and 0.061 for 3-year-olds, for a combined rate from age 1 through 3 years of 0.127. One adult female was killed.
Staying with their mother did not significantly reduce intraspecific predation among yearlings. Neither population density, at the levels we observed,
nor reduced food abundance influenced rates of intraspecific predation on yearlings in our areas. In our study areas, intraspecific predation on
yearling females was correlated positively with the number of adult males that had died 3 years previously and whether any adult male had died 2
years previously. In an earlier study, we found that cub mortality was elevated during the breeding season 2 years after the death of adult males. As
we found a similar pattern for intraspecific predation on yearling females, we speculate that infanticidal males may be prone to kill subadult bears.
Although based on a relatively small sample of mortalities, our results strengthen our earlier conclusion that the killing of adult males may have a
population consequence that managers should consider.

Ursus 12:81–92

Key words: brown bear, intraspecific predation, management, population dynamics, Sweden, Ursus arctos

Bears kill other bears, both cubs-of-the-year (called and P. pumila), fail simultaneously. Smirnov and Shurygin
“cubs” in this paper) and older bears, but most authors (1991) wrote that intraspecific predation and cannibalism
consider the killing of bears older than cubs by conspecif- is a typical feature of brown bears in lean years in Tuva,
ics to be rare (Rogers 1983, Mattson et al. 1992, Garshelis central Siberia. Intraspecific predation during starvation
1994, McLellan 1994). However, McLellan et al. (1999) years was reported to reduce adult survival and popula-
found that 5 of 12 natural mortalities among subadult and tion size in central Siberia (Zavatskii 1993) and the Baikal
adult female brown bears in the Rocky Mountains were region (Ustinov 1993), although these conclusions were
apparently due to conspecifics. Garshelis (1994) reported not based on studies of radiomarked bears. In addition,
that about 2 North American black bears (U. americanus) Bunnell and Tait (1981) hypothesized that the killing or
per year were reported killed by conspecifics during the evicting of subadult males by adult males could be im-
last 20 years, which is a low number considering the mul- portant in the regulation of bear populations.
titude of telemetry studies that have been conducted on Mattson et al. (1992) encouraged bear researchers not
this species. Intraspecific predation has also been reported only to report instances of intraspecific predation, but also
occasionally in polar bears (U. maritimus; Lunn and to report sample sizes and effort, such that mortality rates
Stenhouse 1985, Derocher and Wiig 1999). Two attempts could be analyzed with respect to demographic and envi-
have been made to quantify intraspecific predation in ronmental parameters. Here we report mortality rates due
brown bears using scat analysis. Mattson et al. (1992) to intraspecific predation in 2 brown bear populations in
found that 0.17% of 6,976 brown bear scats in Yellowstone Scandinavia based on 13 instances of intraspecific preda-
National Park, USA, contained remains of conspecifics tion during 668 bear-years (1 bear-year is the equivalent
(meat or >50% indigestible remains). Zhiryakov (1993) of 1 bear followed for 12 months, n = 238 bears) of radio-
found that a similar proportion, 0.1%, of brown bear fe- telemetry tracking during 1984–99. We use the term “in-
ces contained bear hair in and near the Alma-Ata State traspecific predation” to mean intraspecific killing.
Reserve, Tian-Shan Mountains, Kazakhstan. McLellan Because we usually recovered the carcass quickly, we are
et al. (1999) found that 5 of 34 total known mortalities of unsure how much of it would finally have been consumed
radiomarked subadult and adult female brown bears were by the perpetrator. We only considered bears >1 year old
due to conspecifics, but did not report whether the 2 natu- here; we considered mortality of cubs earlier (Swenson et
ral mortalities of 25 total mortalities of males were due to al. 1997, 2001). Beyond reporting rates, we addressed
intraspecific predation. In some areas of Russia, intraspe- the following questions:
cific predation is much more common among brown bears Are bears in certain age or sex categories particularly
in years when the major food crops, usually berries and vulnerable to intraspecific predation?— Mordosov (1993)
the very important seeds of stone pines (Pinus sibirica stated that adults killed yearlings in spring, and Mattson
82 Ursus 12:2001

et al. (1992) suggested that young males might be espe- cific predation and that there may be different reasons in
cially vulnerable, but this was based only on 1 dead young different circumstances.
male.
Do yearlings that stay with their mothers survive better
than those that separate as yearlings, as predicted by STUDY AREAS
Stringham (1983)? The study areas were located in northern Sweden and
Who are the perpetrators?—The general pattern from central Sweden–southeastern Norway, as described in
literature reviews in North America seems to be that adult Bjärvall and Sandegren (1987). These study areas are
males kill other bears of all age and sex classes, but adult about 600 km apart and are near the northern and south-
females kill few adult males (Garshelis 1994, McLellan ern edges of the species’ range in Sweden. The northern
1994), although there is not sufficient evidence to make a study area was about 8,000 km2 in central Norbotten
definite conclusion. County in northern Sweden, around Kvikkjokk (66°–68°
When does the mortality occur?— Mattson et al. (1992) N, 16°–20° E). It included alpine vegetation in the moun-
found a tendency for brown bear feces to contain more tains, which were up to 2000 m high, in the western part
bear remains (both brown and black bear) in March–May of the area. Birch (Betula pubescens) formed the timber-
than later in the year in Yellowstone National Park. line at about 800 m. Boreal forest dominated and con-
Zyryanov (1991) and Mordosov (1993) reported that in- sisted primarily of Scots pine (Pinus sylvestris) and
traspecific predation among brown bears in Siberia takes Norway spruce (Picea abies) in the U-shaped valleys and
place in spring and autumn. There are several reports of the lower (down to 300 m) and more rolling terrain in the
bears being killed by conspecifics while in or at the den eastern part of the study area. The southern study area
for American black bears (Rogers 1983, Tietje et al. 1986, was about 13,000 km2 in northern Dalarna County and
Davis and Harestad 1996) and brown bears (Zetterberg western Gävleborg County in Sweden and eastern
1951, Pearson 1975, Smirnov and Shurygin 1991, Hedmark County in Norway (60°–61° N, 11°–16°E). The
Zyryanov 1991, Mordosov 1993). area was dominated by gently rolling terrain between 200
What is the reason for intraspecific predation?— No and 1000 m in elevation. Only a small part of the study
universal reason has yet been found, but several explana- area was above timberline. Intensively managed boreal
tions have been proposed. (1) The Russian authors cited forest dominated the study area, and the important tree
above have proposed that bears kill conspecifics for food species were Scots pine, Norway spruce, and the exotic
when other food is scarce. This has also been suggested lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta). The study populations
by some North American authors (Tietje et al. 1986, Olsen belong to separate mitochondrial DNA lineages (Taberlet
1993), although data from Yellowstone National Park did et al. 1995). However, nuclear DNA analyses have re-
not support this explanation (Mattson et al. 1992). Tietje vealed that, although there are differences between the
et al. (1986) hypothesized that a male could improve his populations, there has been extensive gene flow through-
breeding success by killing and eating other bears to im- out Scandinavia (Waits et al. 2000). An analysis of re-
prove his condition during the breeding season in years productive and mortality data indicated rapid growth in
of poor food availability. (2) Both adult males and fe- both populations during 1984–95: λ = 1.14 in the north
males could benefit from killing unrelated conspecifics and λ = 1.16 in the south (Sæther et al. 1998).
of the same sex, usually older bears killing younger ones. Bear hunting was generally allowed during the autumn
This would reduce the number of offspring of competi- in both areas, but the northern area included 3 national
tors, and thus remove future competitors to themselves parks, where bear hunting was forbidden throughout the
and their own offspring for space, food, and mates (Rogers study period. Hunting pressure has increased in the south-
1983, Tietje et al. 1986, LeCount 1987, Garshelis 1994, ern study area in recent years, from an average of 2.8 bears
Hessing and Aumiller 1994). (3) Several authors have killed legally per year in the 2 provinces encompassing
suggested that higher density or concentration of bears in the study area (Dalarna and Gävleborg) during 1985–95,
of itself leads to increased intraspecific predation to 23 in 1998. There was evidence of considerable illegal
(Stringham 1983, Tietje et al. 1986, Davis and Harestad killing of bears, especially in the northern area (Swenson
1996). The pattern of intraspecific predation in and Sandegren 1999). Illegal killing appeared to be less
Yellowstone did not contradict this hypothesis (Mattson on the study areas than in the surroundings, and it ap-
et al. 1992). peared to have a major limiting effect on the population
These explanations were usually based on a few ob- in the north. We have documented this in the north through
served cases of intraspecific predation. There are cer- the loss of radiomarked bears, especially males leaving
tainly other valid explanations and it is reasonable to the study area, and changes in the distribution of females,
believe that there are actually several reasons for intraspe- as determined from the distribution of legally killed fe-
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN BEARS • Swenson et al. 83

male bears (Sandegren and Swenson 1997, Swenson and predation was based on bite marks, particularly in the skull.
Sandegren 1999). Swenson et al. (2001) concluded that a Most carcasses were found so quickly that there was no
higher immigration rate of newly recruited males occurred question as to the cause of death. However, some dead
in the southern study area than in the northern area after bears in the north were found later, due to the wilderness
adult males died. This was based on an apparent stable character of the study area and poor weather that often
population trend for adult males in the south, in spite of hindered telemetry work from aircraft and access to the
the loss of adult males, and the decline in number of adult carcass with a helicopter. In cases where decomposed
males in the north after adult males died. carcasses were found, judgment regarding cause of death
was based on bite marks on the bones, particularly the
skull. Thus, our estimate of mortality due to intraspecific
METHODS predation may have been somewhat low in the north, al-
During 1984–99, 101 bears were marked in the north- though we do not believe it caused a major bias. During
ern study area and during 1985–99, 137 were marked in this study we documented 12 cases of intraspecific mor-
the south. All were immobilized early in the spring from tality of radiomarked subadult bears, 1 of a yearling that
a helicopter and marked with radiotransmitters (Telonics, had been ear-tagged but had no radiotransmitter, and 1 of
Mesa, Arizona, USA). Most of the subadult bears cap- an adult (>3 years old).
tured after 1994 were equipped with a transmitter with a Almost half of the bears in the north (43%, n = 35 to
mortality sensor, which changed the pulse rate of the trans- 1998) and almost all in the south (89%, n = 57) separated
mitter after it had not been moved for 4.7 hours. Grow- from their mothers as yearlings (Swenson et al. 1994). It
ing bears were recaptured each spring to provide them can be difficult to decide whether a yearling killed by an-
with larger collars, although in the latter years of the study, other bear in the spring had separated from its mother
yearlings received a transmitter that was implanted into before being killed. We attempted to locate bears at least
the body cavity. Except for yearlings of radiomarked fe- once a week, and we considered a yearling to have been
males, we collected a premolar tooth, which we sent to with its mother when killed if it had been with her during
Matson, Inc., Milltown, Montana, USA, for age estima- the last time it was located alive.
tion based on the cementum annuli (Craighead et al. 1970). Following Garshelis (1994) and Swenson et al. (2001),
Bears were weighed every time they were captured. Dur- we used yearling body mass as a surrogate measure of
ing 1988–91, 86% of the females observed in the com- food abundance the previous year. Yearlings were weighed
pany of radiomarked males during the breeding season in to the nearest 0.5 kg shortly after den emergence (n = 62
the north had functioning radiotransmitters. The corre- in the north and 80 in the south). A body mass index for
sponding value in the south, based on this type of obser- a given year was calculated as the mean of the deviations
vations made in 1988, 1989, and 1993, was 47% (Swenson in body masses for yearlings from the appropriate area,
et al. 1994, 1995). Judging from observations of males expressed in standard deviation units, from the overall
with radiomarked females in the breeding season, Swenson mean for all yearlings for all years for that sex and area.
et al. (2001) concluded that virtually all adult males in the Thus, body masses from both sexes in an area were stan-
north and about 56% of them in the south were marked. dardized and could be combined. The yearling body mass
As marking occurred only during one short period each index was termed “fall condition index”, because most of
year, we calculated annual survival rates using the Kaplan– the body mass was gained during the previous fall sea-
Meier procedure (Kaplan and Meier 1958). In this method, son. The body mass index was also considered to pro-
survival is calculated each month based on the number of vide a “spring condition index” for the year in which
radiomarked bears alive each month, and the monthly sur- yearlings were weighed. Thus for example, a body mass
vival rates are multiplied to obtain an annual survival rate. index obtained in early spring 1997 was used as a “spring
Because we were only interested in intraspecific preda- condition index” for 1997 and as a “fall condition index”
tion, we censored other causes of mortality, as recom- for 1996.
mended by Pollock et al. (1989). The rates of intraspecific Loss of cubs has been correlated with the death of adult
predation reported here are 1 minus the censored annual males in our study areas (Swenson et al. 1997, 2001). We
survival rates. All bears were located at least once a week therefore examined for this effect among subadult bears.
from the ground or from an aircraft, weather permitting. The locations where adult males (>5 years old) were
When a mortality pulse was received or death was sus- known to have died were compared with the locations of
pected, the site was visited as soon as possible, usually radiomarked females with cubs for a given year, called a
within a day in the south and a few days in the north. All “cub area”. A high proportion of the adult males were
carcasses were examined by veterinarians at the National radiomarked: almost all (>90%) in the north and about
Veterinary Institute of Sweden. Determining intraspecific 56% in the south (Swenson et al. 2001). In addition, all
84 Ursus 12:2001

bears killed by hunters must be shown to the authorities, et al. (1989). Pollock et al. (1989) recommended a sample
the exact location of the kill must be reported (and can be size of 40–50 animals to obtain a mortality estimate with
verified), and samples must be turned in, including a tooth. good precision. This exceeds our sample sizes for every
We tallied the number of adult males known to have died age–sex–area category, so we accepted an ∝ level of 0.10
within each cub area and within 1 male home-range di- for statistical significance.
ameter from it at time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years. This We used stepwise multiple logistic regression to inves-
method is described in more detail and data are presented tigate correlates of annual rates of intraspecific predation
elsewhere (Swenson et al. 2001). mortality among radiomarked yearlings. Yearling fate
Indices of the trend in density of all adult females were (killed by another bear or not) was treated as the depen-
derived by estimating the densities of breeding adult fe- dent variable. The independent variables were: area, sex,
males and correcting these estimates for the proportion of spring body mass of the yearling, annual spring condition
radiomarked adult females that were breeding that year. index, annual fall condition index, adult female bear den-
Density of breeding adult females was estimated from the sity, number of adult males known to have died in or near
ratio of marked and unmarked females seen in the com- the cub areas (with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years), and
pany of marked adult males during the breeding season whether or not adult males were known to have died in or
using a simple Petersen estimate (Swenson et al. 1994). near the cub areas (with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years).
This was expanded to all adult females using the observed Independent variables were entered into the model, and
proportion of adult females that were breeding that year. removed by backward elimination using the Wald χ2 sta-
We calculated a mean density from the years we have es- tistic for removal (Norušis 1997). Statistical tests were
timates (6 years in the north, 3 years in the south) and carried out in SPSS.
assigned this density to the year that was the mid-point of
the years for which we had obtained estimates. With this
starting point, we calculated annual density estimates be- RESULTS
fore and after that year using the rates of population growth
found from a demographic analysis of our data, λ = 1.14 Are Bears in Certain Age or Sex
in the north (1984–95) and λ = 1.16 in the south (1985– Categories More Vulnerable to
95; Sæther et al. 1998). Intraspecific Predation?
We also estimated the relative bear density in the home Yearlings—The mortality rate due to intraspecific pre-
ranges of the subadults in a spatial manner, not tempo- dation was not significantly different for yearling males
rally as above. In Sweden, female bears are concentrated in the north and south (z = 0.17, 1 df 1, P = 0.91, Table 1).
in four geographically isolated areas termed female core The combined mortality rate for yearling males in all ar-
areas (Swenson et al. 1994). These core reproductive ar- eas was 0.032 (SE = 0.022, n = 64). In yearling females,
eas are defined as the 90% harmonic mean areas (Dixon
and Chapman 1980) of hunter-killed female bears during Table 1. Mortality rates of radiomarked brown bears in
Scandinavia due to intraspecific predation by age, sex, and
1981–93 (Swenson et al. 1998a). In general, the relative
study area (north and south), 1984–98.
density of bears is halved about every 24 km from the
center of a core reproductive area toward the edge Age
(years) Sex Area Mortality (SE) Na
(Swenson et al. 1998a). This density gradient is a result
of a sustained increase in population size, both in density 1 M N 0.037 (0.036) 30 (1)
and in distribution (Swenson et al. 1995, 1998a). In the S 0.029 (0.029) 34 (1)
F N 0 28 (0)
south, our field station is located approximately in the
S 0.162 (0.061) 38 (6)
center of the southernmost core reproductive area in Swe- 2 M N 0.082 (0.056) 31 (2)
den. For practical reasons, to reduce the effect of a mis- S 0 28 (0)
match between the location of the center of the core F N 0 23 (0)
S 0 26 (0)
reproductive area and the location of our field station, bear 3 M N 0 18 (0)
density was considered to be constant within 15 km from S 0.096 (0.064) 25 (2)
our field station. Outside this area, the relative density of F N 0 21 (0)
S 0 22 (0)
bears was estimated as 0.5((x-10)/24), where x = the distance >3 M N+S 0 179 b (0)
(in km) of a home range center from our field station F N+S 0.0037 (0.004) 268 c (1)
(Swenson 1998a). We were not able to obtain this rela- a Number of radiomarked bears for subadults and number of years
tive spatial density for the northern study area. and partial years the bears were followed for adults. The number in
Estimated survival functions from radiomarked bears parentheses is the number documented as killed by another bear.
b The actual time was 136.9 bear-years.
were compared using the z test recommended by Pollock c The actual time was 251.0 bear-years.
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN BEARS • Swenson et al. 85

however, the bear-caused mortality was higher in the south cubs were observed in mud on a road about 400 m from
than in the north (z = 2.66, 1 df, P = 0.008, Table 1). When the kill site. These indications suggest that an adult male
comparing sexes of yearlings within an area, we found no may have killed W9405, although the female with cubs
statistical difference in mortality rate between males and or other bears can not be ruled out. We concluded that
females in the north (z = 1.00, 1 df, P = 0.32). In the this bear had been with its mother when killed, but it is
south, however, females had a mortality rate due to in- possible that it had separated from her less than a day
traspecific predation that was over 5 times higher than before it was killed.
males (z = 1.97, 1 df, P = 0.049, Table 1). Female year- Case 2.—Some evidence suggests that a female with
lings were 25% smaller than males in the north and 18% cubs may have killed yearling female W9805. This year-
smaller in the south (Swenson et al. 2001). ling was observed alone on 11 June 1998. The next day,
Subadult bears.—Intraspecific predation was also ob- a radiotelemetry position was obtained at 21:30 and was,
served in 2- and 3-year-old bears, but only in males (Table according to the activity sensor on the transmitter, inac-
1). In both areas combined, the annual rate of intraspe- tive at the site where it was later found dead; it may have
cific predation was 0.040 (SE = 0.028, N = 59) among 2- already been dead at this time. Another bear, W9404, a
year-old males and 0.061 (SE = 0.042, N = 43) among 5-year-old female with 3 cubs, was recorded, using radio-
3-year-old males. Thus, the cumulative rate of intraspe- telemetry, to have passed this site about 20:30 on the same
cific predation for male bears from age 1 through 3 years day. No other radiomarked bears were located in the area,
(i.e., the second through fourth years of life) in both areas but an unmarked bear can not be excluded as the killer.
combined was 0.127. No radiomarked female 2-year-olds The kill site was visited on 13 June. It was evident that
(N = 49) or 3-year-olds (N = 43) were killed by other W9805 had been chased for about 50 m, based on torn up
bears. When combining areas and ages (2- and 3-year- moss and tufts of hair. The bite marks suggested that she
olds), we found a significantly higher intraspecific preda- had been killed by an adult bear and the carcass was par-
tion rate among males (0.048, SE = 0.024, N = 102) than tially consumed. W9404 and W9805 were half-sisters
females (0 mortalities, 92 females followed, z = 2.03, 1 born in separate litters to the same mother.
df, P = 0.042). Case 3.—An adult male most likely killed adult female
Adult bears—We followed bears older than 3 years for W8905. The 12-year-old female was observed with 3
a total telemetry time of 388 bear-years and recorded only cubs 300 m from their den on 21 May 1999. At 05:25 on
a single death due to another bear. This was a 12-year- 25 May, she was located at the same site with a passive
old female with 3 cubs. The annual mortality rate for signal (her transmitter did not have a mortality function),
females older than 3 years due to intraspecific predation together with an 11-year-old male (W9807), who had an
was 0.0037 (SE = 0.0037, based on 251 bear-years of ra- active signal. The site was checked by airplane the same
diotelemetry (Table 1). day. The activity signals were the same; one adult bear
was seen (probably W9807) and 2 cubs were observed in
Who Are the Perpetrators? a tree. A visit to the site in the evening revealed that the
As with most studies of bear mortality, identifying the adult female was dead, covered with moss, and the poste-
perpetrator was difficult. We identified 3 potential perpe- rior portions had been eaten. The site showed evidence
trators, but do not have conclusive evidence for any of of a fight. Two of the young were found alive in a tree.
them: (1) an adult male likely killed a male yearling; (2) They were kept alive by providing dog food at the den
a 5-year-old female with 3 cubs may have killed a female site, but were killed by an unknown bear on 11 June. They
yearling, who was her half-sister; and (3) an 11-year-old were in good condition when killed.
resident male probably killed a 12-year-old female, who
had 3 cubs. Details of these incidents are given below. Do Yearlings that Stay with their Mothers
Case 1.—An adult male was the most likely killer of Survive Better?
yearling male (W9405). The yearling was located with In the south, where 89% of the yearlings separated from
its mother on 27 May 1994, but was located alone and their mothers during the spring, we were unable to reject
inactive on 28 and 29 May at the site where the partially the hypothesis of no difference in the rate of intraspecific
eaten bear carcass later was found. A large Scots pine predation among yearlings with their mothers (0.200, SE
tree at the kill site had many fresh claw marks from what = 0.179, N = 5) and those separated from their mothers,
appeared to be a large bear and a smaller bear, suggesting sexes combined, (0.090, SE = 0.035, N = 68, z = 0.60, 1
that a large bear had dragged a smaller bear out of the df, P = 0.55). Likewise, in the north, where 43% of the
tree. We assume that the smaller bear was W9405, but yearlings separated from their mother, there was no sta-
his littermate (W9404, female) was also in the vicinity at tistical difference in intraspecific predation between those
the time. Tracks from a large male bear and a female with with their mother (no mortalities, N = 26) and those sepa-
86 Ursus 12:2001

rated from their mother (0.037, SE = 0.036, N = 31, z = fore he had entered the den.
0.96, 1 df, P = 0.30). Similarly, for both areas combined, Factors Correlated with Intraspecific
yearlings with their mother had an intraspecific predation Predation
rate (0.032, SE = 0.032, N = 31) that was not signifi- In the stepwise multiple logistic regression, using year-
cantly different from those not with their mother (0.074, lings of both sexes and areas and including sex and area
SE = 0.027, N = 99, z = 1.00, 1 df , P = 0.32). as independent variables, 3 independent variable were sta-
tistically significant: the number of adult males known
When Does the Mortality Occur? dying in the area 3 years earlier, sex, and fall condition
Of the 14 bears identified as killed by conspecifics, 13 index. A positive relationship was found between intraspe-
of which were radiomarked and 1 earmarked, 86% were cific predation and the 2 continuous variables (Table 3).
killed during May–July (Table 2). This pattern seemed to Because sex was a significant variable, similar regressions
hold for all age and sex classes of victims. One bear, a 3- were performed on the sexes separately. No significant
year-old male, was killed at his den site. He was located independent variables were found for yearling males.
there alive in October; we thought that the subsequent However for yearling females, a positive relationship was
mortality signal was due to hibernation, but he was found found between intraspecific predation and both the num-
dead outside the den the following spring. He had obvi- ber of adult males known dying 3 years previously and
ously been killed there before the snow arrived, i.e. be- the fall condition index (Table 4). In the south, the rela-
tive density of bears within the yearlings’ home ranges
Table 2. Months in which brown bears were killed by did not influence the probability of being killed by an-
conspecifics in Scandinavia, according to age and sex,
1984–99. Data from both study areas are combined. other bear for males (Wald χ2 = 0.004, 1 df, P = 0.95) or
Females Males
females (Wald χ2 = 0.165, 1 df, P = 0.68). The data, in-
Month Yearling Adult Yearling 2-yr old 3-yr old Total cluding the independent variable and the selected depen-
May 1 1 1 1 4 dent variables, are presented in Table 5.
Jun 4a 4 Because we earlier found a relationship between cub
Jul 2 1 1 4 mortality and whether any adult males had died (Swenson
Aug 1 1
Sept 0 et al. 2001), we also compared intraspecific predation rates
Oct 1 1 for yearlings where any adult male had died (using time
a One of these bears was ear-tagged but not radiomarked and was lags of 1, 2, and 3 years) to those where no adult male was
found without the help of radiotelemetry. known to have died. No significant results were found

Table 3. Results of a stepwise multiple logistic regression with whether a radiomarked yearling brown bear was killed by
another bear in two study areas in Sweden as the dependent variable. Independent variables were study area, sex of the
yearling, spring body mass of the yearling, annual spring condition index, annual fall condition index, bear density (based on
adult females), number of adult males known to have died in or near the cub areas, with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years, and
whether or not adult males were known to have died in or near the cub areas, with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years.

Variable Slope SE Wald χ2 df P

Constant -8.149 2.668 9.33 1 0.002


Number of adult males dying 3 years earlier 0.671 0.322 4.35 1 0.037
Sex 2.584 1.352 3.66 1 0.056
Fall condition index 1.320 0.699 3.56 1 0.059
Entire model 10.29 3 0.016

Table 4. Results of a stepwise multiple logistic regression with whether a radiomarked yearling female brown bear was killed
by another bear in two study areas in Sweden as the dependent variable. Independent variables were study area, spring body
mass of the yearling, annual spring condition index, annual fall condition index, bear density (based on adult females), number
of adult males known to have died in or near the cub areas, with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years, and whether or not adult males
were known to have died in or near the cub areas, with time lags of 1, 2, and 3 years.

Variable Slope SE Wald χ2 df P

Constant -3.476 0.955 13.24 1 0.000


Number of adult males dying 3 years 0.891 0.378 5.57 1 0.018
earlier
Fall condition index 2.209 1.053 4.40 1 0.036
Entire model 9.83 2 0.007
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN BEARS • Swenson et al. 87

for males (Table 6). For yearling females, however, the the smallest subadult bears, it is possible that they were
rate of intraspecific predation was higher when adult males the easiest to kill.
had died than if none were known to have died 2 years In spite of our relatively large sample sizes, we were
previously and a tendency in that direction was found with unable to demonstrate a survival advantage to yearlings
a 3-year time lag (Table 7). of staying with their mothers, contrary to that predicted
In a similar test, we examined whether a 2- or 3- year- by Stringham (1983). Furthermore, we found that 3 or-
old male being killed by another bear was related to any phaned cubs from 2 litters experienced normal growth in
of the same dependent variables used for the test for year- the north (Swenson et al. 1998b). We do not know why
lings, except that age was included and sex excluded. some females keep yearlings for an additional year, when
Body mass was also excluded, as it was not available for it means also postponing reproduction for an additional
several individuals. None of these variables entered into year. Factors that may influence this situation are that our
the final model. In the south, the relative density of bears populations are growing rapidly (Sæther et al. 1998) and
within the subadult males’ home ranges did not influence they are expanding in distribution, with no evidence that
the probability of being killed by another bear (Wald χ2 = carrying capacity has been reached in any areas (Swenson
0.062, 1 df, P = 0.80). There may have been too few et al. 1998a).
instances of intraspecific predation in these subadult males The one adult female that was killed by a conspecific
in our data set to adequately test the effects of the inde- most probably was killed by an adult male and likely was
pendent variables. killed while defending her young, as has been recorded
elsewhere for American black bears and brown bears
(Garshelis 1994, McLellan 1994). The annual rate of in-
DISCUSSION traspecific predation for females >3 years old in our rela-
In our 2 study areas, intraspecific predation was almost tively large sample (251 bear-years) was only 0.0037. No
entirely confined to yearlings and subadults, as also re- adult males were killed by conspecifics, as also found by
ported by Mordosov (1993) and suggested by Mattson et McLellan et al. (1999).
al. (1992). The probability of being killed by another bear Intraspecific predation was greatest during the spring
from our sample was 0.127 for males during ages 1 through breeding season, as also reported by Mattson et al. (1992).
3 years (both areas), 0.162 for yearling females in the Although intraspecific predation may be associated with
south, and 0 for yearling females in the north. Thus, in- breeding behavior, it may also be associated with a greater
traspecific predation can be an important demographic probability for bears to encounter each other during the
factor in some populations. Yearling females were the breeding season. At this time, adult males and adult fe-
most vulnerable to intraspecific predation. As they were males without cubs travel the greatest distances (Zakrisson
2001).

Table 5. Loss of radiomarked yearling female brown bears to intraspecific predation in the northern and southern study areas
in Sweden, 1987–98, including the yearling masses (i.e., spring condition index) and number of adult (>5 years) males dying
within the cub areas 1, 2, and 3 years previously. (One female yearling was followed in the north in 1984. She survived the year.)

North South
a
Yearling Spring No. males dying Yearling Spring No. males dying
Year female lossb indexc -1 yr -2 yr -3 yr female lossb Index -1 yr -2 yr -3 yr

1987 0/0 — 0/2 — 0 0 0


1988 0/0 — 0 0 0 0/0 — 0 1 0
1989 0/2 -0.17 2 0 0 0/1 -0.39 0 0 3
1990 0/0 +0.17 0 1 0 0/1 +0.60 0 0 0
1991 0/0 +2.07 0 0 1 0/2 -1.68 1 2 0
1992 0/1 +0.69 0 0 0 1/4 -0.46 4 1 2
1993 0/3 +0.19 0 0 0 0/3 -0.48 2 4 1
1994 0/4 -0.28 0 0 0 2/5 -0.34 0 2 4
1995 0/8 +0.27 0 0 0 0/4 -0.21 1 0 2
1996 0/2 -0.98 0 0 0 1/1 -0.19 0 1 0
1997 0/1 -0.47 1 0 0 0/1 +2.01 3 1 1
1998 0/2 +0.35 0 1 0 1/6 +0.41 0 3 0
1999 0/4 -0.38 0 0 1 1/8 +0.52 4 3 3
a Adult males. Numbers can change when tracked diagonally because the cub areas were different each year.
b Number killed by intraspecific predation/total number followed with radiotelemetry.
c Spring condition index in 1989 corresponds to fall condition index in 1988.
88 Ursus 12:2001

Table 6. Annual rate of intraspecific predation in yearling male brown bears in Scandinavia, in relation to whether adult (>5
years) males were known to have died within the cub areas at various time lags, 1984–99. Data are combined from both study
areas.

Following known adult male death No known adult male death


Time before the
mortality of yearlings Mortality SE N Mortality SE N z P
1 year 0.029 0.029 36 0.037 0.036 27 0.17 0.86
2 years 0.027 0.027 37 0.044 0.042 26 0.34 0.73
3 years 0.039 0.038 26 0.029 0.029 37 0.21 0.83
a Two-tailed z test as described in Pollock et al. (1989).

Table 7. Annual rate of intraspecific predation in yearling female brown bears in Scandinavia, in relation to whether adult (>5
years) males were known to have died within the cub areas at various time lags. Data are combined from both study areas.

Following known adult male death No known adult male death


Time before the
mortality of yearlings Mortality SE N Mortality SE N z P
1 year 0.114 0.054 36 0.069 0.047 30 0.63 0.53
2 years 0.182 0.067 33 0 0 33 6.14 0.007
3 years 0.132 0.055 38 0.037 0.036 28 1.45 0.15
a Two-tailed z test as described in Pollock et al. (1989).
We did not find an unequivocal reason for intraspecific spite an annual mortality rate of about 10%. The conclu-
predation. However, it is probably not possible to find sion was that infanticide caused by these immigrating
one explanation for all intraspecific predation in bears. males was probably the most important cause of cub loss.
In our study areas, we only found significant relationships, Intraspecific predation on yearling females was correlated
among the factors we tested, for intraspecific predation with the number of adult males dying 3 years earlier in
on yearling females. In a multivariate analysis, positive the multivariate analysis, in contrast to the 2-year time
relationships were found between this and the number of lag found for cubs. However, the same result, higher mor-
adult males known to have died in the area 3 years previ- tality after 2 years, was found in the univariate analyses.
ously and the fall condition index. In a univariate analy- Based on these results, we speculate that infanticidal males
sis, a positive relationship was found between intraspecific might be more prone to kill young bears, including sub-
predation and whether any adult male was known to have adults. Also, the 2-year time lag for cub mortality and 3-
died 2 years earlier, with a tendency in that direction after year time lag for yearling mortality means that the cohort
3 years. As both variables had been included in the mul- that experiences an elevated mortality rate as cubs fol-
tivariate analysis, the number of males dying 3 years pre- lowing the loss of adult males on the area, also experi-
viously was the factor that best explained the observed ences a higher mortality the following year, at least the
variation. However, both results suggest that disruption females in the cohort. Subadult bears were also killed by
of the adult male social organization, perhaps resulting in conspecifics most often in the breeding season, which is
the immigration of new males (Swenson et al. 1997, 2001), the same pattern we observed regarding cub loss (Swenson
may have been involved in elevating the rates of intraspe- et al. 2001).
cific predation. A higher loss of subadults with a time lag following
The pattern of intraspecific predation of yearling fe- high adult mortality also appears to have occurred in griz-
males in relation to mortality of adult males was similar zly bears in the Yellowstone Ecosystem. Stringham (1983)
to the pattern we observed for loss of cubs in our 2 study analyzed data from the Yellowstone brown bear study
areas (Swenson et al. 2001). Mortality of cubs was also during 1959–79 and found that the mean number of adult
considerably higher in the south, where mortality of adult males censused before and after a cohort was born was
males was higher, than in the north, and cub mortality in negatively correlated with the cohort sizes at ages 0.5 years
the south was correlated with number of adult males dy- (r2 = 0.31), 1.5 years (r2 = 0.50), and 2.5 years (r2 = 0.82).
ing 2 years earlier in a multivariate analysis. Cub mortal- The highest numbers of censused adult males were in
ity was also higher when any adult males had died 2 years 1968–70 (Stringham 1983). The known mortality of adult
earlier in a univariate analysis. Swenson et al. (2001) males in the Yellowstone Ecosystem was also highest dur-
concluded that there was a higher immigration rate of new ing these 3 years, mean of 9/year, compared to 1959–67,
males in the south than in the north. This was based on a with a mean of 2.7/year (Craighead et al. 1995:278). Thus,
high emigration rate of radiomarked subadult males and low cohort size may also have been correlated with high
no significant change in the number of adult males, de- losses of adult males with the reported time lags. The
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN BEARS • Swenson et al. 89

elevated losses of cubs and subadults apparently contin- intraspecific predation was 0.162 in the south and 0 in the
ued for 2 or 3 years after many adult males were killed. north. Assuming that many of the cubs were killed by
Again, the high losses may have been related to a disrup- other bears, we conclude that intraspecific mortality can
tion of the adult male social organization, and perhaps be an important demographic factor in some bear popula-
occurred after new males entered the studied subpopula- tions. As the level of intraspecific predation can be re-
tion of bears using the garbage dumps. lated to the killing of adult males, we repeat the caution
A second factor that was correlated with intraspecific we have given earlier (Swenson et al. 2001): “Until this
predation in yearling females was fall condition. How- question is adequately resolved, managers should act con-
ever, it was difficult to interpret this result, because the servatively and assume a population consequence of har-
relationship was positive. It is not intuitively obvious why vesting adult male bears.”
better condition in the fall should lead to higher intraspe-
cific mortality, especially when most of the mortality oc-
curs in the spring. A similar relationship was found for ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
cubs (Swenson et al. 2001). Thus, there may be a factor This study was supported by the Norwegian Institute
involved here that we have not identified. Loss of cubs for Nature Research, the Swedish Environmental Protec-
and intraspecific predation on yearling females may be tion Agency, the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Man-
correlated with a factor or factors other than intraspecific agement, the Swedish Association for Hunting and
predation that is also correlated with the death of adult Wildlife Management, WWF–Sweden, the Research
males 2 or 3 years earlier. We have not identified such a Council of Norway, Olle och Signhild Engkvists Stiftelser,
factor, but our results suggest that it is not poor food avail- and Orsa Communal Forest. We thank the personnel in
ability. the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project for their
Relative population density was not found to influence help in the field, especially S. Brunberg, P. Segerström,
the probability of intraspecific predation in either year- R. Franzén, E. Maartmann, and P. M. Lokholt. A.
lings of both sexes or subadult males. We tested for den- Söderberg also provided invaluable assistance with data
sity effects both temporally and spatially. We conclude analysis. We also thank D. Garshelis, A. Derocher, and
that population density, within the range of densities ob- S. Schmid-Holmes for helpful comments on the manu-
served in our study, was not an important factor influenc- script.
ing intraspecific predation. We earlier failed to find
evidence that brown bears had reached carrying capacity
in any areas in Sweden (Swenson et al. 1998a). LITERATURE CITED
BJÄRVALL, A., AND F. SANDEGREN. 1987. Early experiences with
the first radio–marked brown bears in Sweden. International
Conference on Bear Research and Management 7:9–12.
MANAGEMENT IMPLICATIONS BUNNELL, F.L., AND D.E.N. TAIT. 1981. Population dynamics of
There has been considerable debate in the scientific lit- bears—implications. Pages 75–98 in C.W. Fowler and T.D.
erature about potential negative or positive effects on popu- Smith, editors. Dynamics of large mammal populations.
lation growth of killing adult males. After reviewing the John Wiley and Sons, New York, New York, USA.
evidence and presenting his own data, Miller (1990) rec- CRAIGHEAD, J.J., F.C. CRAIGHEAD, JR., AND H.E. MCCUTCHEN.
ommended that managers should not assume that the kill- 1970. Age determination of grizzly bears from fourth
ing of adult males would have a positive effect on cub premolar tooth sections. Journal of Wildlife Management
survivorship. Wielgus and Bunnell (1994, 2000) went 34:353–363.
even further, suggesting that killing adult males might even ———, J.S. SUMNER, AND J.A. MITCHELL. 1995. The grizzly
bears of Yellowstone, their ecology in the Yellowstone
decrease cub survival. Our studies of cub survival
Ecosystem, 1959–1992. Island Press, Washington, D.C.,
(Swenson et al. 1997, 2001) have led us to agree with USA.
Wielgus and Bunnell (1994, 2000). However, this debate DAVIS, H., AND A.S. HARESTAD. 1996. Cannibalism by black
has only been concerned with cub survival. bears in the Nimpkish Valley, British Columbia. Northwest
The similarity of the results for cubs and yearling fe- Science 70:88–92.
males leads us to speculate that the killing of adult males DEROCHER, A.E., AND Ø. WIIG. 1999. Infanticide and cannibalism
and disruption of the resident adult male social organiza- of juvenile polar bears (Ursus maritimus) in Svalbard. Arctic
tion, with presumed immigration of new males, has even 52:307–310.
a greater demographic effect on a bear population than DIXON K.R., AND J.A. CHAPMAN 1980. Harmonic mean measure
of animal activity areas. Ecology 61:1040–1044
we proposed previously (Swenson et al. 1997, 2001). Loss
GARSHELIS, D.L. 1994. Density-dependent population regulation
of cubs was 0.35 in the south and 0.04 in the north of black bears. Pages 3–14 in M. Taylor, editor. Density-
(Swenson et al. 2001). Loss of female yearlings due to dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar
90 Ursus 12:2001

bears. International Conference on Bear Research and (In Swedish.)


Management, Monograph Series 3. SMIRNOV, M.N., AND V.V. SHURYGIN. 1991. Buryi medved’ v
HESSING, P., AND L. AUMILLER. 1994. Observations of conspecific Tuve. Pages 162–170 in B.P. Zavatskii and Y.G. Shvetsov,
predation by brown bears, Ursus arctos, in Alaska. Canadian editors. Medvedi v SSSR. Nauka, Novosibirsk, Russia. (In
Field-Naturalist 108:332–336. Russian.)
KAPLAN, E.L., AND P. MEIER. 1958. Nonparametric estimation STRINGHAM, S.F. 1983. Roles of adult males in grizzly bear
from incomplete observations. Journal of the American population biology. International Conference on Bear
Statistics Association 53:457–481. Research and Management 5:140–151.
LECOUNT, A.L. 1987. Causes of black bear cub mortality. SWENSON, J., AND F. SANDEGREN. 1999. Mistänkt illegal björnjakt
International Conference on Bear Research and Management i Sverige. Pages 201–206 in Bilagor till Sammanhållen
7:75–82. rovdjurspolitik; Slutbetänkande av Rovdjursutredningen.
LUNN, N.J., AND G.B. STENHOUSE. 1985. An observation of Statens offentliga utredningar 1999:146. Stockholm,
possible cannibalism by polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Sweden. (In Swedish.)
Canadian Journal of Zoology 63:1516–1517. ———, R. FRANZÉN, P. SEGERSTRÖM, AND F. SANDEGREN. 1998b.
MATTSON, D.J., R.R. KNIGHT, AND B.M. BLANCHARD. 1992. The age of self-sufficiency in Scandinavian brown bears.
Cannibalism and predation on black bears by grizzly bears Acta Theriologica 43:213–218.
in the Yellowstone Ecosystem, 1975–1990. Journal of ———, F. S ANDEGREN , A. B JÄRVALL , A. S ÖDERBERG , P.
Mammalogy 73:422–425. WABAKKEN, AND R. FRANZÉN. 1994. Size, trend, distribution
MCLELLAN, B. 1994. Density-dependent population regulation and conservation of the brown bear Ursus arctos population
of brown bears. Pages 15–24 in M. Taylor, editor. Density- in Sweden. Biological Conservation 70:9–17.
dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar ———, ———, S. BRUNBERG AND P. SEGERSTRÖM,. 2001.
bears. International Conference on Bear Research and Factors associated with loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden.
Management, Monograph Series 3. Ursus 12:000–000.
MCLELLAN, B.N., F.W. HOVEY, R.D. MACE, J.G. WOODS, D.W. ———, ———, AND A. SÖDERBERG. 1998a. Geographic
CARNEY, M.L. GIBEAU, W.L. WAKKINEN, AND W.F. KASWORM. expansion of an increasing brown bear population: evidence
1999. Rates and causes of grizzly bear mortality in the for presaturation dispersal. Journal of Animal Ecology
interior mountains of British Columbia, Alberta, Montana, 67:819–826.
Washington, and Idaho. Journal of Wildlife Management ———, ———, ———, A. BJÄRVALL, R. FRANZÉN, AND P.
63:911–920. WABAKKEN. 1997. Infanticide caused by hunting of male
MILLER, S.D. 1990. Impact of increased bear hunting on bears. Nature 386:450–451.
survivorship of young bears. Wildlife Society Bulletin ———, P. WABAKKEN, F. SANDEGREN, A. BJÄRVALL, R. FRANZÉN,
18:462–467. AND A. SÖDERBERG. 1995. The near extinction and recovery
MORDOSOV, I.I. 1993. Yakutia. Pages 301–318 in M.A. Vaisfeld of brown bears in Scandinavia in relation to the bear
and I.E. Chestin, editors. Bears—brown bear, polar bear, management policies of Norway and Sweden. Wildlife
Asian black bear. Nauka, Moscow, Russia. (In Russian with Biology 1:11–25.
English summary.) TABERLET, P., J.E. SWENSON, F. SANDEGREN, AND A. BJÄRVALL.
NORUŠIS, M.J. 1997. SPSS professional statistics 7.5. SPSS 1995. Localization of a contact zone between two highly
Incorporated, Chicago, Illinois, USA. divergent mitochondrial DNA lineages of the brown bear
OLSEN, T.L. 1993. Infanticide in brown bears, Ursus arctos, at Ursus arctos in Scandinavia. Conservation Biology 9:1255–
Brooks River, Alaska. Canadian Field-Naturalist 107:92– 1261.
94. TIETJE, W.D., B.O. PELCHAT, AND R.L. RUFF. 1986. Cannibalism
PEARSON, A.M. 1975. The northern interior grizzly bear Ursus of denned black bears. Journal of Mammalogy 67:762–766.
arctos L. Canadian Wildlife Service, Report Series No. 34. USTINOV, S.K. 1993. The Baikal region. Pages 275–301 in
POLLOCK, K.H., S.R. WINTERSTEIN, C.M. BUNCK, AND P.D. CURTIS. M.A. Vaisfeld and I.E. Chestin, editors. Bears—brown bear,
1989. Survival analysis in telemetry studies: the staggered polar bear, Asian black bear. Nauka, Moscow, Russia. (In
entry design. Journal of Wildlife Management 53:7–15. Russian with English summary.)
R OGERS , L.L. 1983. Effects of food supply, predation, WAITS, L, P. TABERLET, J.E. SWENSON, F. SANDEGREN, AND R.
cannibalism, parasites, and other health problems on black FRANZÉN. 2000. Nuclear DNA microsatellite analysis of
bear populations. Pages 194–211 in F.L. Bunnell, D.S. genetic diversity and gene flow in the Scandinavian brown
Eastman, and J.M. Peek, editors. Symposium on natural bear (Ursus arctos). Molecular Ecology 9:421–431.
regulation of wildlife populations. Proceedings No. 14, WIELGUS, R.B., AND F.L. BUNNELL. 1994. Dynamics of a small,
Forest, Wildlife and Range Experiment Station, University hunted brown bear Ursus arctos population in southwestern
of Idaho, Moscow, Idaho, USA. Alberta, Canada. Biological Conservation 67:161–166.
SÆTHER, B.-E., S. ENGEN, J.E. SWENSON, Ø. BAKKE, AND F. ———, AND ———. 2000. Possible negative effects of adult
SANDEGREN. 1998. Viability of Scandinavian brown bear male mortality on female grizzly bear reproduction.
Ursus arctos populations: the effects of uncertain parameter Biological Conservation 93:145–15.
estimates. Oikos 83:403–416. ZAKRISSON, C. 2001. Do brown bear (Ursus arctos) females
SANDEGREN, F., AND J. SWENSON. 1997. Björnen—viltet, ekologin with cubs alter their movement pattern in order to avoid
och människan. Svenska Jägareförbundet, Spånga, Sweden. infanticidal males? Undergraduate Thesis, Swedish
INTRASPECIFIC PREDATION IN BEARS • Swenson et al. 91

University of Agricultural Sciences, Umeå, Sweden. with other predatory animals in Tian-Shan. Pages 86–92 in
ZAVATSKII, B.P. 1993. The middle Siberia. Pages 249–275 in I.E. Chestin and S.M. Uspensky, editors. Bears of Russia
M.A. Vaisfeld and I.E. Chestin, editors. Bears—brown bear, and adjacent countries—state of populations. Volume 1.
polar bear, Asian black bear. Nauka, Moscow, Russia. (In Argus, Moscow, Russia. (In Russian with English summary.)
Russian with English summary.) ZYRYANOV , A.N. 1991. Biotopicheskoe razmeshchenie i
ZETTERBERG, H. 1951. Björnen i sägen och verkelighet. J.A. povedenie burogo medvedya v Sayanakh. Pages 171–181
Lindblads Förlag, Uppsala, Sweden. (In Swedish.) in B.P. Zavatskii and Y.G. Shvetsov, editors. Medvedi v
ZHIRYAKOV, V.A. 1993. Brown bear cannibalism and relations SSSR. Nauka, Novosibirsk, Russia. (In Russian.)
Paper VI

Journal of Zoology 260:XXX-XXX

Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears Ursus arctos: effect of


mass, sex, reproductive status, population density and habitat type

By

B Dahle and JE Swenson


Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect
of mass, sex, reproductive status, population density and habitat type

Bjørn Dahle, Department of Biology, Norwegian University of Science and


Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway

Jon E. Swenson, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural


University of Norway, Post Box 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway and Norwegian Institute for
Nature Research, Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway

Short title: brown bear home range

Present address for Bjørn Dahle:


Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
Agricultural University of Norway
Post Box 5014
N-1432 Ås
Norway

1
ABSTRACT
Annual home range size indices for 36 male and 52 female adult brown bears Ursus arctos in
two study areas in central and northern Scandinavia were estimated to evaluate factors
proposed to influence home range size. Male home ranges were larger than home ranges of
lone females after controlling for the sexual size dimorphism acting on metabolic needs.
Further, home ranges of females with cubs were smaller than home ranges of lone females
and females with yearlings. Thus, differences in metabolic need were not able to explain the
variation in range size among females of different reproductive categories or between males
and females suggesting roaming behaviour of males in this promiscuous species. Home range
size in both males and females was inversely related to population density along a density
gradient that was not linked to food availability. This contradicts the hypothesis that females
utilise the minimum areas that sustain their energy requirements. However, on a large
geographical scale a negative relationship between range size and food availability was
evident. The annual home ranges in inland boreal environments in Scandinavia are the largest
reported for brown bears in Eurasia, and similar to those in inland boreal and montane
environments in North America.

Journal of Zoology 260: 000-000

Key words: brown bear, home range size, population density, Ursus arctos,

2
INTRODUCTION
Home ranges are the areas in which animals acquire necessary resources and carry out the
biological requirements for life (Burt, 1943). Home range size is therefore an important
biological parameter reflecting aspects such as body mass, diet, food abundance and
dispersion, as well as sex and reproductive strategy (e.g. Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1978;
Harestad & Bunnell, 1979; Sandell, 1989; Palomares, 1994; Fisher & Owens, 2000).
McNab (1963) suggested that food controlled home range size through an animal’s
size-dependent metabolic rate and the productivity of its habitat. High food productivity and
quality allows an animal to meet its energy requirement in a small home range. In polygynous
and promiscuous species without male parental care, female home range is expected to be a
function of the availability and spatial distribution of food (Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1978;
Sandell, 1989). Data supporting this has been reported for several mammalian species such as
bobcat lynx rufus, (Litvaitis, Sherburn & Bissonette, 1986) roe deer Capreolus capreolus
(Tufto, Andersen & Linnell, 1996) and American black bear Ursus americanus (Powell,
Zimmerman & Seaman, 1997). Male home range size in this mating system, on the other
hand, is proposed to be influenced by two limited resources: receptive females and food
(Sandell, 1989). In polygynous species males are usually larger than females (e.g. Clutton-
Brock, Harvey & Rudder, 1977) and larger home ranges could thus be due to both increased
movement of males to consort with several females and sexual size dimorphism increasing
the metabolic demands in males compared to females, which we term the “metabolic”
hypothesis. Sandell (1989) reviewed the literature on home range size in solitary carnivores
and argued that male ranges were larger than expected from metabolic needs. However,
Sandell (1989) used a metabolic exponent 0.75 to control for body size, whereas Harestad &
Bunnell (1979) found that home ranges for species categorised as having a carnivore diet
increased with body mass to the 1.32 power. Furthermore, Sandell (1989) compared male and
female ranges regardless of the reproductive status of females.
The brown bear Ursus arctos is a solitary species with a polygynous/ promiscuous
mating system (Craighead, Sumner & Mitchell, 1995) and the size of male ranges exceeds the
size of female ranges (McLoughlin et al., 1999). This size difference in home ranges has
rarely been discussed, but some have ascribed it to males roaming to consort females (e.g.
McLoughlin et al., 1999), although the effect of sexual size dimorphism on metabolic needs
and home range size has hardly been considered (but see McLoughlin et al., 1999). The
brown bear is an omnivore, and Harestad & Bunnell (1979) found that home ranges of

3
omnivores scaled to body mass with an exponent of 0.92, based on an interspecific analysis.
Thus, the “metabolic” hypothesis predict that the size of male ranges would be lone female
range size * male mass0.92 / lone female mass0.92. Metabolic needs of females with young
should exceed those in lone females as cubs are provided milk, and the total body mass of a
family group could be twice that of a lone female. Thus, the “metabolic” hypothesis predict
ranges to be larger in females with young than in lone females.
Several studies on a variety of species have reported an inverse relationship between
home range size and population density (e.g. Vincent et al., 1995 in roe deer; Nagy &
Haroldson, 1990 and McLoughlin, Fergusson & Messier, 2000 in brown bears), whereas
others have not found such a relationship (e.g. Sheperd & Swihart, 1995 in fox squirrel
Scirius niger;). Except from a few experimental studies (e.g. , Mares et al., 1982 on the rodent
Tamias striatus) it is hard to separate the effect of food availability from the pure density
effect because population density often relates positively to food availability (e.g in wild boar
Sus scrofa, Massei et al., 1997). Using brown bears, we tested the “population density”
hypothesis that home range size relates inversely to population density, and predicted that,
within an area with similar food availability, home range size would decrease with increasing
population density. Our study contrasts most other studies because we evaluated the effect of
population density from home ranges of individuals within a single population, and not from
different populations. Thus confounding inter-population factors such as differences in habitat
quality and food availability were eliminated.
In general, high food productivity and quality allows an animal to meet its energy
requirement in a small home range. This “food abundance” hypothesis predicts a negative
relationship between food abundance and home range size, and most available data are
consistent with this (e.g. Ostfeld, 1986 on California voles Microtus californicus), although
the relationship may be more complex depending on food dispersion and vulnerability
(Macdonald 1983). Brown bears have a circumpolar distribution on the Northern Hemisphere
extending from productive temperate and subtropical forests to arctic tundra (Servheen,
Herrero & Peyton, 1999). We predicted that home range size in brown bears should decrease
along a habitat gradient, represented by arctic tundra, boreal and montane forests, mixed
temporal forest with hard masts (oaks Quercus and beech, Fagus), and coastal areas with
anadromous salmonids Oncorhyncus, as food abundance increases along this gradient.

4
METHODS
Study areas
The study was performed in two areas. The southern area (hereafter named south) is situated
in Dalarna and Gävleborg counties, in south-central Sweden, and Hedmark County in south-
eastern Norway (61o N, 18o E) and covers the southernmost part of the brown bear population
in Scandinavia. The elevation ranges from about 200 m in the south-eastern part to about
1,000 m in the western part at the Norwegian border, but only a very small part (< 10%) of
the area is above timberline, which is at about 750 m. Lakes and bogs are common, but most
of the hilly area is covered with pine Pinus sylvestris and spruce Picea abies dominated
coniferous forest. Bear density ranged from 20-25/ 1000 km2 in the centre of the reproductive
core area (see later definition) to as low as 0.5 bears /1000 km2 at the edge of the peripheral
area (Sandegren & Swenson, 1997). In contrast, moose density averaged 700-800/1000 km2
in the reproductive core area (Cederlund & Wallin, 1998) and 400-1340/1000 km2 in the
peripheral areas (Persson, 1998). Thus, bear density was not related to density of moose,
which is the major prey and second most important food item (Swenson, Dahle & Sandgren,
2001). The northern study area (hereafter named north) is situated in the south-western part of
Norrbotten County in Sweden (67° N, 18° E). The area is rolling with elevations below 300 m
in the east, but is dominated by mountains that rise to over 2,000 m in the west. Northern
boreal coniferous forest dominates, but there are extensive subalpine deciduous forests. Both
study areas are sparsely populated by humans. On average, bears are active from April to
November, reflecting the length of the snowfree period, which is somewhat shorter in the
northern study area (Sandegren & Swenson, 1997).

Capture and radio-monitoring


During 1984-2000 brown bears were captured using immobilising drugs darted with a gas
driven rifle from a helicopter. Bears were located either by tracking them on snow in the
spring or locating adult radiomarked females to capture their young. Captured bears were
weighed on a spring scale, and several body measurements, as well as blood, tissue and hair
samples, were taken. The first premolar was extracted from bears other than the yearlings of
radio-marked females and sent to Matson’s, Inc., Milltown, Montana, for age estimation by
counting cementum annual layers (Craighead, Craighead & McCutchen, 1970).
Radio transmitters, mounted on collars, were placed on the bears. Bears were located
about once a week during their active period using standard triangulation methods from the

5
ground or from a plane or helicopter. Mean error in position obtained from the ground was
452 m ± 349 m (SD), for test transmitters with minimum bearing lengths between 400 and
2,200 m (Dahle, unpublished data). The relatively large error was related to the distance at
which positions are obtained from and the hilly terrain.
Both males and females may reach sexual maturity at the age of 3 (4 years for females
in the northern study area) and have mated successfully at this age in both study areas
(Swenson, unpublished data). However, especially males are active dispersers when they are
2-3 years old, but all but one had settled before the age of 5 years. Female dispersal is limited
and the range size of dispersing 2 year old females is similar to the size of the home range of a
adult female. For this reason males were considered adult at the age of 5 and females were
considered adult the year before they gave birth to their first litter, at latest aged 6 and 7 years
in the south and north, respectively. Adult females belonged to three reproductive categories
1) lone females, 2) females with cubs and 3) females with yearlings. The number of bears
followed by radiotelemetry varied among years, but sufficient data to estimate annual range
size indices were collected for 88 adult bears in the period 1986-2000 in the south and 1987-
1998 in the north.

Population density
In Sweden, female bears are concentrated in four geographically isolated areas termed
reproductive core areas (Swenson et al., 1994). These core reproductive areas are defined as
the 90% harmonic mean areas (Dixon & Chapman, 1980) of hunter-killed female bears during
1981-1993 (Swenson, Sandegren & Söderberg, 1998). In general, the relative density of
females is halved about every 19 km from the centre of a core reproductive area towards the
edge, for males about every 31 km (Swenson et al., 1998). In contrast with most other studies
comparing home range size at different population densities, our study deals with a
continuous population. Further the population density gradient is not related to a
corresponding gradient in food abundance, but is rather a result of historical events. After the
abandonment of the former extermination policy the population started to recover, and since
1930 there has been a sustained increase in population size, both in density and in distribution
(Swenson et al., 1995; 1998).
In the south, our field station is located around the centre of the southernmost core
reproductive area in Sweden, although the exact centre of this area is difficult to locate. For
practical reasons, to reduce the effect of a mismatch between the location of the centre of the
core reproductive area and the location of our field station, female and male density was

6
considered to be constant within 10 and 15 km from our field station, respectively. Outside
this area (with assumed constant density) the relative density of females, y, x km from our
field station could be estimated as y = 0.5((x-10)/19) and the relative density of males z = 0.5((x-
15)/31)
.

Home range estimators and statistics


Annual home ranges were estimated by 95% minimum convex polygon (MCP, excluding 5%
of the locations furthest away from the kernel coordinate which is the equivivalent of the
Gaussian kernel estimator (Worton 1989)) using the Ranges V computer package (Kenward
& Hodder, 1996). To follow Burt’s (1943) definition of home range, in which occasional
sallies outside the area should be excluded, we used the 95% MCP because the most outlying
position (which might be exploratory movements) are excluded (1 position for 16<n<31, 2
positions for 30<n<50). Home ranges were additionally estimated by 100% MCP to
accomplish comparisons with other studies. To obtain similar sample sizes for all individuals
and simultaneously eliminate autocorrelated data, only locations separated by least 100 h
were used. This corresponds to the minimum time between the weekly localisations. Because
the time between successive locations was long, and the bears spend 5-7 months in their dens
annually, a relatively low number of fixes was obtained for each individual. Small samples of
locations underestimate home range size when using the MCP method (Macdonald, Ball &
Hough, 1980) so throughout the paper our presented figures of home range size is more of an
index, rather than a measure. Some bears in the south were followed more intensively and
home ranges estimated from these bears were used to evaluate the validity of the home range
estimates based on weekly positions. On average, bears leave their dens later in the spring and
enter them earlier in the autumn in the north than in the south. Further, the collection of
telemetry positions was hampered by weather conditions more often in the north, where
almost all positions were obtained from the air, than in the south, where most positions were
obtained from the ground. For this reason, the minimum number of locations to estimate
annual MCP home range was set to 16 and 14 during their active period in the southern and
northern study areas, respectively.
Home range size estimates were log10-transformed prior to analyses to meet
assumptions of normality and equal variance among groups of data (Sokal & Rohlf, 1995).
Individuals were often followed more than one year, and the mean of all home range
estimates for each individual when it belonged to the same reproductive category was used in
the analyses. Because females were followed as they changed reproductive category we were

7
not able to use a single global general linear model (GLM) to test our specific predictions on
how range size changes in relation to reproductive status. For this reason we used several
GLM (both with independent data and with a repeated measurements procedure) and when
necessary, paired t-tests. Two-tailed tests were used, and an α level of 0.05 was selected for
statistical significance. SPSS/Win v. 10.0 (SPSS Inc., Illinois, USA) was used in all statistical
analyses. The literature was reviewed for studies providing home range estimates for brown
bears living in different habitat types.

RESULTS
During the period bears were active, more independent positions (> 100 h between positions)
of each bear were obtained in the south (28.5 ± 4.9 SD for males and 27.6 ± 3.8 SD for
females) than in the north (18.2 ± 3.8 SD for males and 19.45 ± 3.2 SD for females, F1,286 =
238.96, P < 0.001) but it was not related to sex (F1,286 = 0.235, P = 0.63. Because the
distributions of home range estimates were skewed before log-transforming the data, the
median is presented instead of the mean.
The estimated male home ranges (95% MCP), 1055 and 833 km2 in the south and
north, respectively, were larger than the corresponding figures for lone females (217 and 280
km2 respectively, F1.85 = 14.23, P < 0.001) (Table 1), while controlling for the sexual size
dimorphism. Study area was not a significant factor in this model (F1.85 = 0.15, P = 0.70, but
there was a significant study area*sex interaction (F1.85 = 4.15, P = 0.045) revealing that the
difference between male and female ranges was less prominent in the north.
Data on relative population density was only available for the southern study area, and
thus the inclusion of this factor as a covariate removed the northern study area from the
model. Home range size estimates in both sexes decreased with increasing relative population
density (F1,58 = 22.97, P < 0.001), (Fig. 1) and sex was still significant (F1,58 = 49.09, P <
0.001). Similarly home range size estimates of females with cubs decreased with increasing
density (Fig. 1). The slopes of these regression lines were not different from each other (F2,77
= 0.13, P = 0.88).
The metabolic hypothesis predicted ranges to be larger for females with young than
for lone females, but a GLM with repeated measurements revealed that the opposite was true
regarding females with cubs (F1.35 = 34.64, P<0.001) and that this pattern was independent of
study area (F1.35 = 1.0, P = 0.33). Further, range size of females with yearlings was not

8
different from that of lone females (paired t = 1.24, df = 10, P = 0.25), but was larger than in
females with cubs (paired t = 2.43, df = 9, P = 0.038) (the study areas were pooled due to the
small sample size of females with yearlings, especially in the south).
Home ranges estimated by the 100% MCP method from the weekly positions were on
average 1.22 ± 0.27 SD times larger than the 95% MCP. Further, home ranges estimated on a
more frequently positioning (more than 75 positions with a minimum of 12 h interval) were
on average 1.46 ± 0.52 SD and 2.0 ± 0.80 SD times larger for 95% and 100% MCP,
respectively, than our presented figures for 95% MCP based on weekly positions. The relative
increase in home range estimate by using a more frequent positioning was not related to the
category of individuals (male, lone female and female with cubs, F2,36 = 0.35, P = 0.71, F2,36 =
0.72, P = 0.49, respectively). The increase in home range size estimate from 95% MCP based
on the weekly positions to 95% MCP based on the more frequent positioning was not related
to the increase in the number of positions used in the home range size estimates (F1,36 = 0.82,
P = 0.32), whereas the increase from 95% MCP bases on the weekly positions to 100% MCP
based on the more frequent positioning was related to the increased number of positions used
in the home range estimates (F1,36 = 101.28, P < 0.001).
The literature review resulted in 13 references providing home range estimates of
brown bears living in the four major habitat/environmental types. Data from arctic tundra
(AT) and coastal areas with salmon spawning rivers (C) were only found in North America
whereas data from mixed forests (MF) were only found in Europe (Fig. 2). Home range size
estimates decreased markedly with increased food abundance in these habitat types both in
males (r = -0.885, P < 0.001, N = 15) and females (r = -0.927, P < 0.001, N = 13, Spearman
rank correlation).

DISCUSSION
The accuracy of home range estimates based on such small numbers of fixes is questionable
and they are surely underestimates (McDonald et al., 1980). The home range estimates based
on a more frequently positioning suggested that the true size of the home ranges were at least
1.5-2 times larger than our figures obtained from the weekly positions depending on the
estimator (95 or 100% MCP). The increase in home range size estimates from 95% MCP
based on weekly position to MCP estimates based on a more frequent obtained positions was
only related to the increase in number of positions for the 100% MCP, and this suggest that

9
the 95% MCP are less dependent on the sample size, because as new positions are added,
more outlying positions are excluded. The underestimation of home range size was probably
more pronounced in the north, because ranges were estimated based on relatively fewer
telemetry positions than in the south, even when considering the somewhat shorter period of
activity in the north. However, they should be comparable indices of the home range size for
different individual categories. Further, most other studies also report home ranges estimated
from a small number of fixes. Therefore, we considered them adequate to address the
questions posed in this study.
On the average, males had home range estimates that were 3.0-4.9 times larger than
those of lone females, and significantly larger than predicted from metabolic needs in males
relative to females. This excludes sexual size-dimorphism and the “metabolic” hypothesis as
the major explanation for the difference in home range size, and supports the common view
(e.g. Sandell 1989) that the larger home ranges in male than female reflects the
polygynous/promiscuous mating system, in which males may increase fitness by roaming
over large areas to consort females (e.g. Clutton-Brock, 1989; Fisher & Lara, 1999).
To our knowledge, we are the first to report a significant relationship between annual
home range size estimates and reproductive status in female brown bears, although several
studies have indicated trends for ranges of females with cubs to be both smaller or larger than
those of other reproductive status (IGBC, 1987). From the estimated home range sizes, female
with young did not use larger ranges than lone females, in fact, females with cubs used the
smallest ranges. This is contrary to what was predicted from the “metabolic” hypothesis. In
spring and early summer cubs are small, and may perhaps limit the movements of their
mother (e.g. Lindzey & Meslow, 1977; Hirsch, Bender & Haufler, 1999). This might explain
why females with cubs used smaller home ranges than other females. However, in fall, and
when they reach yearling age, their size per se should not limit their mother’s movement.
Our results showed that sizes of home range size estimates varied inversely with
population density, providing support for the “population density” hypothesis. In contrast to
other studies, food availability can be ruled out as explanation for the observed population
density effect in our study because habitat quality and moose density (the major prey,
Swenson et al. 2001) were similar along the brown bear population density gradient. This
gradient was a result of the expansion of a remnant population into suitable habitat. In fact,
the per capita food availability should decrease with increasing population density. Our
findings contradict the hypothesis that females utilise the minimum area that meets their
energy requirements (Tufto et al., 1996). Nagy & Haroldson (1990) compared home range

10
size estimates among four different populations of brown bears in Canada, and observed a
negative relationship between population density and home range size. They argued that this
relationship was due to population density and not an effect of food availability, because the
high-density populations with the smaller home ranges consisted of individuals with smaller
body size and later sexual maturity, i.e. factors associated with poor nutritional conditions
(Stringham, 1990). Thus, the relationship between food availability and home range size was
positive (Nagy & Haroldson, 1990). As female black bears are reported to extend their home
range into areas left vacant when neighbouring females were killed (Rogers, (1977), the
density effect we observed in females was most likely due to interactions and competition
among individuals, which has also been reported to decrease home ranges in species such as
gray-tailed voles Microtus canicaudus (Wolff & Schauber, 1996). Ostfeld (1986) suggested
the same mechanism to be operating in male California voles. Our results are also consistent
with this explanation. Assumptions about home range size are sometimes used when
estimates of population size are carried out (Linnell et al., 1998). As home range size indices
are inversely related to population density, this may lead to an overestimation of population
size where bears are found at low densities, if the home range estimates have been obtained at
higher densities.
The literature study revealed that the largest brown bear home ranges were found in
the arctic Northwest Territories and may be explained by the unproductive arctic tundra with
migrating food resources they inhabit (McLoughlin et al., 1999). The estimated home range
sizes we found for brown bears in Scandinavia were comparable to those reported from North
American boreal and montane populations with substantial use of large mammals in spring,
and no use of spawning salmon (Blanchard & Knight, 1991). Bears inhabiting productive
European temperate mixed forests, in which hard masts (oak and beech) provide a high-
quality food resource, have somewhat smaller ranges. Anadromous salmonids provide coastal
brown bear populations in North America with abundant food, rich in protein and lipids, and
home ranges are the smallest reported for the species. Even though population densities are
highest in these coastal areas, body sizes are also the largest (Stringham, 1990), suggesting
that the small home ranges are mainly due to the high food abundance and not the high
population density. Thus, the decrease in home range size as the food abundance increases
over the range of the species, supports the “food abundance hypothesis” and are consistent
with results obtained within North America (McLoughlin et al., 2000).
We concluded that home range sizes were negatively related to population density and
that differences in metabolic needs are not able to explain the variation in home range size

11
between males and females and among females of different reproductive categories. However,
on a large geographical scale, home range size was negatively related to food availability.

Acknowledgements
This study was funded by the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, the Swedish
Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management, the
Swedish Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, WWF Sweden and the Research
Council of Norway. We thank the personnel in the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research
Project for their assistance in the field and Orsa Communal Forest for field support. All
animal experimentation reported in this paper complies with the current laws regulating the
treatment of animals in Sweden and Norway and were approved by the appropriate ethical
committees in both countries.

12
REFERENCES
Ballard, W. B., Miller, S. D. & Spraker, T. H. (1982). Home range, daily movements, and
reproductive biology of brown bears in southcentral Alaska. Can. Field-Nat.
96: 1-5.
Barnes, V. J. Jr. (1990). The influence of salmon availability on movements and range of
brown bears on southwest Kodiak Island. In International Conference on Bear
Research and Management 8: 305-313. Darling, L. M. & Archibald, W. R. (Eds.).
Vancouver, British Columbia: International Association for Bear Research and
Management.
Blanchard, B. M. & Knight, R. R. (1991). Movement of Yellowstone grizzly bears. Biol.
Conserv. 58: 41-67.
Burt, W. H. (1943). Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to mammals. J.
Mammal. 24: 346-352.
Cederlund, G. & Wallin, K. (1998). Orsaprosjektet, en slutrapport från populations-
undersøkningarna på älg. Unpublished report, Grimsö and Göteborg (In Swedish).
Clarkson, P. L. & Liepins, I. S. (1989). Inuvialuit wildlife studies: grizzly bear research
progress report 1987-1988. Tec. Rep. No 3. Wild: Manage. Advis. Council,
Inuvik, Northwest Territories, Canada.
Clevenger, A. P., Purroy, F. J. & Pelton, M. R. (1990). Movement and activity patterns of a
European brown bear in the Cantabrian mountains, Spain. In International Conference
on Bear Research and Management 8: 205-211. Darling, L. M. & Archibald, W. R.
(Eds.). Vancouver: Hemlock Printers Ltd. for International Association for Bear
Research and Management.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1989). Mammalian mating systems. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 236: 339-
372.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Harvey, P. H. (1978). Mammals, resources and reproductive
strategies. Nature 273: 191-195.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Harvey, P. H. & Rudder, B. (1977). Sexual dimorphism, socionomic
sex ratio and body weight in primates. Nature 269: 797-800.
Craighead, J. J., Craighead, F. C. & McCutchen, H. E. (1970). Age determination of grizzly
bears from fourth premolar tooth sections. J. Wildl. Manage. 34: 353-363.
Craighead, J. J., Sumner, J. S. & Mitchell, J. A. (1995). The grizzly bears of Yellowstone.
Washington D.C.: Island Press.

13
Dixon, K. R. & Chapman, J. A. (1980). Harmonic mean measure of animal activity areas.
Ecology 61: 1040-1044.
Fisher, D. O. & Lara, M. C. (1999). Effects of body size and home range on access to mates
and paternity in male bridled wallabies. Anim. Behav. 58: 121-130.
Fisher, D. O. & Owens, I. P. F. (2000). Female home range size and the evolution of social
organization in macropod marsupials. J. Anim. Ecol. 69: 1083-1098.
Harestad, A. S. & Bunnell, F. L. (1979). Home range and body weight- a reevaluation.
Ecology 60: 389-402.
Hirsch, J. G., Bender, L. C. & Haufler, J. B. (1999). Black bear Ursus americanus movements
and home ranges on Drummond Island, Michigan. Can. Field-Nat. 113: 221-225.
Huber, D. & Roth, H. U. (1993). Movements of European brown bears in Croatia. Acta
Theriol. 38: 151-159.
Interagency Grizzly Bear Committee (IGBC) (1987). Grizzly bear Compendium. Washington,
DC: The National Wildlife Federation.
Kenward, R. E. & Hodder, K. H. (1996). Ranges V. An analysis system for biological
location data. Institute of Terrestrial Ecology, Furzebrook Research Station,
Wareham, Dorset, BH20 5AS.
Lindzey, F. G. & Meslow, E. C. (1977). Home range and habitat use by black bears in
southwestern Washington. J. Wildl. Manage. 41: 413-425.
Linnell, J. C. D., Swenson, J. E., Landa, A. & Kvam, T. (1998). Methods for monitoring
European large carnivores - A worldwide review of relevant experience.
Oppdragsmelding 549. Trondheim: Norwegian Institute for Nature Research.
Litvaitis, J. A., Sherburn, J. A. & Bissonette, J. A. (1986). Bobcat habitat use and home range
size in relation to prey density. J. Wildl. Manage. 50: 110-117.
Macdonald, D. W., Ball, F. G. & Hough, N. G. (1980). The evaluation of home range size and
configuration using radio tracking data. In A handbook on biotelemetry and radio
tracking: 405-422. Amlaner, C. J. & Macdonald, D. W. (Eds.). Oxford: Pergamon
Press.
Macdonald, D. W (1983). The ecology of carnivore social behavior. Nature 301: 379-384.
Machutchon, A. G., Himmer, S. & Bryden, C. A. (1993). Khutzeymateen valley grizzly bear
study. Final Report. Wildlife Report No. R-25, Wildlife Branch, Ministry of
Environment, Land and Parks, British Columbia, Canada
Mares, M. A., Lacher, T. E. Jr., Willig, M. R. & Bitar, N. A. (1982). An experimental analysis
of social spacing in Tamias striatus. Ecology 63: 267-273.

14
Massei, G., Genov, P. V., Staines, B. W. & Gorman, M. L. (1997). Factors affecting home
range and activity of wild boar (Sus scrofa) in a Mediterranean coastal area. J. Zool.
242: 411-423.
McLoughlin, P. D., Case, R. L., Gau, R. J., Ferguson, S. H. & Messier, F. (1999). Annual and
seasonal movement patterns of barren-ground grizzly bears in the central Northwest
Territories. Ursus 11: 79-86.
McLoughlin, P. D., Fergusson, S. H. & Messier, F. (2000). Intraspecific variation in home
range overlap with habitat quality: a comparison among brown bear populations. Evol.
Ecol. 14: 39-60.
McNab, B. K. (1963). Bioenergetics and the determination of home range size. Am. Nat.
162: 133-140.
Nagy, J. A. & Haroldson, M.A. (1990). Comparisons of some home range and population
parameters among four grizzly bear populations in Canada. In International
Conference on Bear Research and Management 8: 227-235. Darling, L. M. &
Archibald, W. R. (Eds.). Vancouver: Hemlock Printers Ltd. for International
Association for Bear Research and Management.
Nagy, J. A., Hawley, A. W. L. & Barret, M. V. (1988). Characteristics of grizzly bear home
ranges in west-central Alberta. Draft report, Alberta Environmental Centre,
Vegreville, Alberta, Canada.
Ostfeld, R. S. (1986). Territoriality and mating system of California voles. J. Anim. Ecol.
55: 691-706.
Palomares, F. (1994). Site fidelity and effects of body mass on home-range size of Egyptian
mongooses. Can. J. Zool. 72: 465-469.
Persson, I. L. (1998). Brown bear Ursus arctos predation upon adult moose Alces alces in
Scandinavia: a study at two levels of scale. MSc thesis. Oslo: University of
Oslo.
Powell, R. A., Zimmerman, J. W. & Seaman, D. E. (1997). Ecology and behavior of North
American black bears. New York: Chapman and Hall.
Rogers, L. L. (1977). Movements and social organization of black bears in northeastern
Minnesota. Ph.D. Thesis. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.
Russell, R. H., Nolan, J. W., Woody, N. G. & Anderson, G. (1979). A study of the grizzly
bear (Ursus arctos L.) in Jasper National Park, 1975-78. Final report. Can. Wild.
Serv., Edmonton, Alberta, Canada.
Sandegren, F. & Swenson, J. E. (1997). Björnen - viltet, ekologin och människan (The bear –

15
the game, the ecology and man). Uppsala: Svenska Jägareförbundet (In Swedish).
Sandell, M. (1989). The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. In
Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution 1: 164-182. Gittleman, J. L. (Ed.). New
York: Cornell University Press.
Schoen, J. W., Lentfer, J. W. & Beier, L. (1986). Differential distribution of brown bears on
Admirality Island, southeast Alaska: a preliminary assessment. In International
Conference on Bear Research and Management 6: 1-5. Zager, P. (Ed.). Washington
D.C.: Port City Press for International Association for Bear Research and
Management.
Servheen, C., Herrero, S. & Peyton, B. (compilers) (1999). Bears. Status survey and
conservation action plan. IUCN/SSC bear and polar bear specialist groups. Gland and
Cambridge: IUCN.
Sheperd, B. F. & Swihart, R. K. (1995). Spatial dynamics of fox squirrels (Sciurus niger) in
fragmented landscapes. Can. J. Zool. 73: 2098-2105.
Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry. New York: Freeman and Company.
Stringham, S. F. (1990). Grizzly bear reproductive rate relative to body size. In
International Conference on Bear Research and Management 8: 433-443. Darling, L.
M. & Archibald, W. R. (Eds.). Vancouver: Hemlock Printers Ltd. for International
Association for Bear Research and Management.
Swenson, J. E., Dahle, B, & Sandegren, F. (2001). Bjørnens predasjon på elg. (Brown bear
predation on moose) NINA fagrapport 048, Trondheim: Norwegian Institute for
Nature Research (In Norwegian with English summary).
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Söderberg, A., Wabakken, P. & Franzén, R.
(1994). Size, trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear (Ursus arctos)
population in Sweden. Biol. Conserv. 70: 9-17.
Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F. & Söderberg A. (1998). Geographic expansion of an increasing
brown bear population: evidence for presaturation dispersal. J. Anim. Ecol.
67: 819-826.
Swenson, J. E., Wabakken, P., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Franzen, R. & Söderberg, A.
(1995). The near extinction and recovery of brown bears in Scandinavia in relation to
the bear management policies in Norway and Sweden. Wildl. Biol. 1: 11-25.
Tufto, J., Andersen, R. & Linnel, J. C. D. (1996). Habitat use and ecological correlates of
home range size in a small cervid: the roe deer. J. Anim. Ecol. 65: 715-725.
Vincent, J. P., Bideau, E., Hewison, A. J. M. & Angibault, J. M. (1995). The influence of

16
increasing density on body weight, kid production, home range and winter grouping in
roe deer (Capreolus capreolus). J. Zool. 236:371-382.
Wolff, J. O. & Schauber, E. M. (1996). Space use and juvenile recruitment in gray-tailed
voles in response to intruder pressure and food abundance. Acta Ther. 41: 35-43.
Worton, B. J. (1989). Kernel methods for estimating the utilisation distribution in home range
studies. Ecology 70: 164-168.

17
Table 1. Annual 95% minimum convex polygon (MCP) home ranges of adult brown bears in
the southern and northern study areas in Scandinavia (sample size in parentheses).

Category 95% MCP in km2


Median Range 25-75
percentiles
South
m (27) 1055 314-8264 745-1673
f lone (34) 217 81-999 144-348
f with cubs (22) 124 46-478 93-182
f with yearlings (3) 161 119-264
North
m (9) 833 245-2029 511-1084
f lone (18) 280 106-816 217-466
f with cubs (15) 137 21-723 96-318
f with yearlings (8) 370 188-469 279-437

18
Figure legends

Figure 1. Relationship between brown bear home range size and relative population density
in the south central Sweden; males (F1,26 = 7.38, R2 = 0.23, P = 0.012), lone females (F1,33 =
17.04, R2 = 0.35, P < 0.001), females with cubs (F1,21 = 9.27, R2 = 0.32, P = 0.006).

Figure 2. Annual 100% minimum convex polygon (MCP) home range estimates of brown
bears in different habitat types. AT – arctic tundra, (Clarkson & Liepins (1989), McLoughlin
et al. (1999). BF – boreal forest or montane forests with substantial use of large mammals,
(this study, Blanchard & Knight (1991), Russel et al. (1979), Ballard, Miller & Spraker
(1982), Nagy, Hawley & Barret (1988)). MF – mixed forest with hard mast (oak and beech)
(Huber & Roth (1993), Kazsencky P (pers. com), Mertzanis Y (pers. com.), Clevenger,
Purroy & Pelton (1990)) .C – coastal areas with salmon spawning rivers (Barnes (1990),
Schoen, Lentfer & Beier (1986), MacHutchon, Himmer & Bryden (1993)).

19
[Figure 1, Dahle & Swenson]

Lone females

Females with cubs

Males
4
Log 10 95% MCP

0 1

Relative population density

20
[Figure 2, Dahle & Swenson]

8000
3500

3000 Females
2
Home range size km

Males
2500

2000

1500

1000

500

AT BF MF C
Habitat

21
Paper VII

Journal of Animal Ecology XX:XXX-XXX

Seasonal range size in relation to reproductive strategies in brown bears.

By

B Dahle and JE Swenson

It isn’t that bad to be a bear after all; after a long sleep they wake up, get laid, eat,
and go to sleep again
- A. Zedrosser, Philosophizing at lunch time
Seasonal range size in relation to reproductive strategies in brown
bears Ursus arctos

Bjørn Dahle, Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and


Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway,

Jon E. Swenson, Department of Biology and Nature Conservation, Agricultural


University of Norway, Post Box 5014, N-1432 Ås, Norway and Norwegian Institute for
Nature Research, Tungasletta 2, N-7485 Trondheim, Norway

Present address for Bjørn Dahle:


Department of Biology and Nature Conservation
Agricultural University of Norway
Post Box 5014
N-1432 Ås
Norway
e-mail: bjorn.dahle@ibn.nlh.no

Running head brown bear seasonal range size

1
Summary
1. Data on seasonal ranges of 93 radio-collared adult brown bears (Ursus arctos) were used
to test hypotheses explaining variation in range size in relation to male and female
reproductive strategies.
2. Both males and oestrous females used large ranges in the mating season, but decreased
their ranges after the mating season. These results suggested that both sexes in this species
roam to mate, because the results could not be explained by a seasonal change in food
availability nor by increased foraging movements of oestrous females to replenish body
reserves after previous cub raising.
3. Females with cubs-of-the-year (cubs) restricted their range size in the mating season and
increased their ranges in the postmating season. This finding suggests that females with
cubs restricted their ranges to avoid contact with infanticidal males, an important cause of
cub mortality, because the proposed alternative explanation; limited mobility of small
cubs, was unable to explain the small size of mating season ranges.
4. Our results suggest that range size in females is influenced by sexually selected
infanticide, selecting for large mating season ranges and multiple mating in oestrous
females to hide paternity and for restricted mating season ranges in females with cubs to
avoid infanticidal males.
5. To our knowledge, we are the first to report a significant relationship between seasonal
range size and reproductive status in female brown bears and the first to report an effect of
oestrus on seasonal range size in female carnivores.

Journal of Animal Ecology 00, 000-000

Key words: brown bear, seasonal home range size, reproductive status, infanticide
avoidance, Ursus arctos,

2
Introduction
The spatial and temporal distribution of resources, the defendability of these resources, and
thereby the potential for polygamy, are important factors determining animal mating systems
(Clutton-Brock & Harvey 1978; Clutton-Brock 1989). Male and female strategies for
maximising their reproductive success differ in most mating systems, because the important
resources differ for males and females (e.g. Clutton-Brock 1989). The availability and spatial
distribution of food is probably the single-most important factor determining the spacing and
size of female home ranges in species without male parental care (Clutton-Brock & Harvey
1978). Results supporting this have been reported for several mammalian species where
polygynous and promiscuous mating systems predominate (e.g. Ims 1987a; Tufto, Andersen
& Linnel 1996; Powell, Zimmerman & Seaman 1997). In the absence of male parental care, a
male’s reproductive success is proportional with the number of females with which he mates
and successfully fertilises. Where females range widely and are solitary or live in small
groups that are unpredictably distributed at low population density, males range widely in
search for oestrous females (see Clutton-Brock 1989 for a review). Ims (1987b) argued that
the temporal distribution of receptive females would be critical for male reproductive strategy,
resulting in large home ranges when female receptivity is asynchronous.
Brown bears (Ursus arctos L.) are solitary, occur at low densities, at least in northern
European and North American inland populations (McLellan 1994; Swenson et al. 1994), and
have a mating season that extends for about two months. From the male’s perspective, this
makes the formation of harems impossible and makes it difficult and uneconomical to defend
territories (Clutton-Brock 1989). The mating system in brown bears is therefore a scramble
competition polygyny or promiscuity; one male may mate with several females, and a female
may mate with several males (Craighead, Sumner & Mitchell 1995). Males may therefore
benefit by having large home ranges (hereafter ranges) during the mating season, as has been
reported for bridled wallabies (Onychogalea fraenata Gould) (Fisher & Lara 1999) and
thirteen-lined ground squirrels (Spermophilus tridecemlineatus Mitchill) (Schwagmeyer
1988), especially in low-density populations. Since male ranges overlap in bears (e.g. Huber
& Roth 1993; Powell et al. 1997), ranges are predicted to be larger in the mating season than
in the postmating season (Sandell 1989). However, few studies have analysed seasonal
variation in range size in relation to mating behaviour.
How mating may affect range size of females in species with a promiscuous mating
system has received less attention. Large ranges in oestrous females during the mating season

3
should increase the probability of meeting several prospective mates, or just of being mated.
Females may benefit from this through mate selection, mediated through male-male
competition or female choice (Andersson 1994), hiding paternity as a counterstrategy against
infanticide (Ebensperger 1998), fertilisation insurance (Gray 1997), sperm competition
(Stockley & Purvis 1993), and selection of the most genetically compatible sperm (Wilson et
al. 1997). Female marsupials (e.g. Fisher & Lara 1999) and ungulates (San Jose & Lovari
1998) are known to roam, increasing their ranges during the mating season, and females tend
to visit males with higher mating success (Liberg et al. 1998).
We hypothesised that both male and female brown bears roam to mate (the “roam-to-
mate hypothesis” (1)), and predicted (1.1) that males and oestrous females would have larger
ranges in the mating season than in the postmating season. Further we predicted (1.2) that
ranges in the mating season would be larger in oestrous females than in females with
dependent offspring (non breeding females). As successful males would benefit by siring
several litters each year, the selective forces favouring males mating with several partners
should far exceed those in females (Trivers 1972). We therefore predicted (1.3) larger mating-
season ranges in males than oestrous females. However, we did not expect ranges to be larger
in males than females during the postmating season, after controlling for the effect of the
sexual size dimorphism on metabolic needs, because females should not be a limiting
resource for males at this time of the year. The adult sex ratio in brown bears was lower
(fewer males per female) in our northern than in our southern study area during our study,
probably due to a male bias in illegal hunting in the north (Swenson et al. 2001a). Oestrous
females may therefore need to roam over larger areas to meet males in the north than in the
south. From this we predicted (1.4) that ranges of oestrous females in the mating season
would be larger in the north than in the south, but predicted no such difference in the
postmating season.
Larger range size of oestrous females during the mating season may also be explained
by an alternative hypothesis (2), that oestrous females are no longer encumbered by young,
and to replenish lost energy reserves they have increased foraging movements. This
hypothesis predicts (2.1) that only females that raised cubs the previous year would have
larger mating season than postmating season ranges. We tested this “increased foraging
hypothesis” (2) by using females that were in oestrous for the first time, and thus not affected
by previous cub raising.
Infanticide, the killing of conspecific young, may influence the mating system in many
mammalian species (Agrell, Wolff & Ylönen 1998; Ebensperger 1998). One proposed

4
explanation for infanticide is that a male may gain mating opportunities by killing the
dependent offspring of females because this would shorten the interval to the female’s next
conception (Hrdy 1979). This sexually selected infanticide hypothesis has gained support in
several studies of social primates (e.g. Soltis et al. 2000), rodents (e.g. Soroker & Terkel
1988) and carnivores (e.g. Pusey & Packer 1994; Swenson et al. 1997; 2001a).
Infanticide in brown bears and American black bears (Ursus americanus Pallas) has
been reported throughout the species’ range (see Taylor 1994 for a review). Although adult
females may kill cubs of neighbouring females, it is more common that cubs are killed by
adult males (LeCount 1987; McLellan 1994). Swenson et al. (1997; 2001a) concluded that
sexually selected infanticide was a major agent of cub mortality in our studied populations,
although cub survival was higher in the northern area. As a counterstrategy, females with
dependent offspring should therefore avoid males during the mating season to increase the
survival of their offspring (Ebensperger 1998). This could be achieved by females with
dependent offspring selecting unfavourable habitats and avoiding habitats selected by males
(Wielgus & Bunnel 1995), avoiding areas of overlap with males (Powell et al. 1997), and/or
by restricting the size of their range during the mating season. Swenson, Dahle & Sandegren
(2001b) analysed data on intraspecific predation on bears older than cubs from our study
areas. Predation rates were generally low and did not differ between yearlings that separated
from their mothers and yearlings that followed their mothers for one more year. Thus, females
with yearlings would have little to gain by reducing their range during the mating season. For
this reason we hypothesised (3, the “infanticide avoidance hypothesis”) that females with
cubs have smaller ranges than oestrous females and females with yearlings due to restricted
movements in the mating season to avoid contact with males. We predicted (3.1) that mating-
season ranges of females with cubs would be smaller than for oestrous females and females
with yearlings, and (3.2) that mating season ranges of females with cubs would be smaller
than postmating season ranges.
Alternatively, females with cubs may have reduced ranges in the mating season
because cubs are small, with limited mobility, at this time of the year (e.g. Lindzey & Meslow
1977, but see Powell et al. 1997) a hypothesis we term “immobility of cubs hypothesis” (4).
Dahle & Swenson (2003) reported an inverse relationship between range size and population
density for brown bears in Scandinavia. The mobility of cubs should be independent of
population density. Thus, in the mating season (when the cubs are small) the “immobility of
cubs hypothesis” predicts (4.1.) that mating season ranges of females with cubs should be
uninfluenced by population density. Because all but one of the analyses are based on paired

5
statistics, (comparing range size of the same individuals in different seasons and when they
belong to different reproductive categories) population density in general should not influence
our results. The four hypotheses and seven predictions are summarised in Table 1 of the
Results section.

Methods
Study areas and species
The study was performed in two areas. The southern area (hereafter named south) is situated
in Dalarna and Gävleborg counties, in south-central Sweden, and Hedmark County in south-
eastern Norway (61o N, 18o E) and covers the southern part of the southernmost brown bear
population in Scandinavia. The elevation ranges from about 200 m in the south-eastern part to
about 1,000 m in the western part at the Norwegian border, but only a minor part of the area is
above timberline, which is at about 750 m. Lakes and bogs cover large areas, but most of the
hilly area is covered with coniferous forest. In the southern study area, our field station is
located in the centre of a female reproductive area (Swenson et al. 1995, 1998) from which
the relative population density of females is halved for each 19 km (Swenson et al. 1998). The
relative population density for each female with cubs was estimated from the distance
between the field station and the location of the individual range (Dahle & Swenson 2003).
The northern study area (hereafter named north) is situated in the southwestern part of
Norrbotten County in Sweden (67° N, 18° E). The area is rolling with elevations below 300 m
in the east, but is dominated by mountains that rise to over 2,000 m in the west. Northern
boreal coniferous forest dominates, but there are extensive subalpine deciduous forests and
alpine areas. Both study areas are sparsely populated by humans. On average, bears are active
from April to November, reflecting the length of the snowfree period, which is somewhat
shorter in the northern study area (Sandegren & Swenson 1997). The brown bear is a solitary
species with no paternal care, young follow their mothers for one or two years, separating
from them prior to, or early in, the mating season, which takes place during May-early July.
Due to delayed implantation, cubs are not born until January the following year.

Capture and radio-monitoring


During 1984-2000 brown bears were captured using immobilising drugs (a mixture of
tiletamin, zolazepan and medetomidin) administered by a gas driven rifle from a helicopter.

6
Bears were captured either after tracking them on snow in the spring or using adult
radiomarked females to locate their yearling young. Captured bears were weighed on a spring
scale, and several body measurements, as well as blood, tissue and hair samples, were taken.
The first premolar was extracted from bears other than the yearlings of radio-marked females
and sent to Matson’s, Inc., Milltown, Montana, for age estimation by counting cementum
annual layers (Craighead, Craighead & McCutchen 1970).
Bears were equipped with neck-mounted radio transmitters. Bears were located about
once a week during their active period using standard triangulation methods from the ground
or from a plane. Mean error in positions obtained from the ground was 452 m ± 349 m (SD),
for test transmitters with minimum bearing lengths between 400 and 2,200 m (Dahle
unpublished data).
Males were considered adult at the age of 5, as males usually reach puberty at 4.5
years and younger males may still be in a dispersal phase. Females were considered adult the
year before they gave birth to their first litter (when they are in oestrous for the first time),
which for all females in the south had taken place before they were 6 years old and before
they were 7 year in the north. Adult females were categorised as 1) oestrous females (females
without dependent offspring and known to have given birth to a litter the subsequent year;
they are called oestrous females even in the postmating season), 2) females with cubs
throughout the year, and 3) females with yearlings throughout the year. The number of bears
followed by radiotelemetry varied among years, but sufficient data to estimate seasonal
ranges were collected for 93 adult bears in the period 1986-2000 in the south and 1987-1998
in the north.

Seasonal variation in food availability


Seasonal changes in food availability and energetic demands may confound the effect of
female reproductive status on range size. To evaluate how seasonal changes in food
availability and energetic demand influence seasonal range size, we studied the seasonal
ranges of 2-year-old nondispersing males and females. These individuals separated from their
mothers the previous year, were not dispersing and were not reproductively active. Thus,
seasonal range size in these individuals was expected to more closely reflect seasonal changes
in food availability and energetic demands.

7
Range size estimators and statistics
Seasonal ranges (mating season: 1 May-15 July and postmating season: 15 July-20 October)
were estimated by the 100% minimum convex polygon (MCP) method using the Ranges V
computer package (Kenward & Hodder 1996). To obtain similar sample sizes for all
individuals and simultaneously eliminate autocorrelated data, only locations separated by
least 100 h were used. This corresponds to the minimum time between the weekly
localisations. Because the time between successive locations was long, and the bears spend 5-
7 months in their dens annually, a relatively low number of fixes was obtained for each
individual. We used 8 locations as the minimum to estimate seasonal ranges. Boxplots were
used to screen data on range size estimates for extremes, which are cases with values more
than 3 times larger or smaller than the interquartile range (SPSS/Win v. 8.0 (SPSS Inc.,
Illinois, USA). Extremes were excluded prior to statistical analyses, because they can greatly
influence the results of statistical tests (Norusis 1998).
Range size estimates were log10-transformed prior to analyses to meet assumptions of
normality and equal variance among groups of data (Sokal & Rohlf 1995). Individuals were
often followed more than one year, and the mean of all ranges for each individual, when it
belonged to the same reproductive category and in the same season, was used in the analyses.
An α level of 0.05 was selected for statistical significance, but the α level was adjusted by the
sequential Bonferroni method in the 7 t-tests used (Sæther 1997). SPSS/Win v. 8.0 (SPSS
Inc., Illinois, USA) was used in all statistical analyses. Two-tailed tests were used unless the
direction of the difference was predicted. Because the distributions of range estimates were
skewed before log-transforming the data, the median is presented instead of the mean.

Results
Due to the large number of hypotheses and predictions these are summarised in Table 1. The
effects of season and area on the seasonal ranges of males and oestrous females were analysed
using a GLM with repeated measurements. As predicted (1.1) by the “roam to mate
hypothesis” both males and oestrous females used larger ranges in the mating season (m: 894
and 736 km2; f: 161 and 278 km2 in the south and north, respectively) than in the postmating
season (m: 501 and 424 km2; f: 107 and 123 km2 in the south and north, respectively) (F1,74
= 39.72, P < 0.001, Fig 1). Further, as predicted (1.3) males used larger seasonal ranges than
females (F1,74 = 22.84, P < 0.001), but unexpectedly this pattern was not related to season

8
(F1,74 = 0.05, P = 0.94). The size of seasonal ranges was not related to study area alone (F1,74 =
0.30, P = 0.58), but there was a significant sex*study area interaction (F1,74 = 6.28, P = 0.014),
and profile plots indicated that the sex difference in range size was less prominent in the north
than in the south during both seasons (Fig. 2). Although range size was not related to study
area alone, we tested specifically the prediction (1.4.), that oestrous females in the north
would use larger ranges than oestrous females in the south in the mating season, but that they
should not be different in the postmating season. As predicted, mating season ranges of
oestrous females tended to be larger in the north than in the south (t47 = 2.14, P = 0.019,
(Bonferroni adjusted α = 0.013), but not postmating season ranges (t47 = 0.84, P = 0.40).
Contrary to prediction (2.1) by the “increased foraging hypothesis”, females that were
oestrous for the first time also had larger ranges in the mating season (191 km2) than in the
postmating season (121 km2, t28 = 3.74, P = 0.001).
As predicted (1.2) both by the “roam to mate hypothesis” and (3.1) by the “infanticide
avoidance hypothesis”, oestrous females used larger ranges during the mating season (161
and 278 km2 in the south and north, respectively) than females with cubs, (76 and 61 km2 in
the south and north, respectively) (F1,32 = 96.97, P < 0.001, Fig 1). This result was not
influenced by study area (F1,32 = 1.85, P = 0.18), and there was no significant study
area*reproductive status interaction (F1,32 = 2.69, P = 0.11). As predicted (3.2) females with
cubs used smaller ranges in the mating season (76 and 61 km2 in the south and north,
respectively) than in the postmating season (132 and 169 km2 in the south and north,
respectively) (F1,33 = 62.061, P < 0.001, Fig. 1). Range size was not related to study area in
itself, but there was a significant study area*season interaction (F1,33 = 5.546, P = 0.025). A
profile plot indicated that the seasonal change in range size of females with cubs was more
prominent in the northern area, due to larger range size in the postmating season, as also
indicated in Fig. 1. Seasonal ranges of females with cubs decreased significantly with
increasing relative population density (F1,23 = 6.47, R2 = 0.22, P = 0.018), contrary to
prediction (4.1) by the “immobility of cubs hypothesis”.
Only three females stayed together with their yearlings in the south so data from the
two study areas were pooled. Contrary to prediction 1.3 in the “roam-to-mate hypothesis”,
range size in females with yearlings in the mating season (226 km2) was not smaller than
when in oestrus (t8= 1.36, P = 0.10), but tended to be larger than when with cubs, as predicted
(2.1) by the “infanticide avoidance hypothesis” (t7 = 2.71, P = 0.015, Bonferroni adjusted α =
0.01). No significant seasonal variation in range size was apparent in females with yearlings
(261 km2 in the postmating season) (t8 = 1.16, P = 0.28, Fig. 1).

9
Seasonal ranges in non dispersing 2-year-old males and females showed a slight, but
statistically significant increase from 69 km2 in the mating season to 76 km2 in the post
mating season (t22 = 3.37, P = 0.003).

Discussion
To our knowledge, we are the first to report a significant relationship between seasonal range
size and reproductive status in female brown bears and the first to report an effect of oestrus
on range size in a carnivore. Oestrous females used larger areas in the mating season than in
the postmating season, most likely to enhance opportunities to meet prospective mates, thus
allowing increased mate selection opportunities, supporting the “roam-to-mate
hypothesis”(1). In polygynous roe deer (Capreolus capreolus L.), females often expand their
ranges during the rut, probably for the same reason ( Liberg et al. 1998), especially those with
small ranges (San Jose & Lovari 1998). A similar increase in range size during the mating
season has been reported in female alligators (Alligator mississippiensis Daudin) (Rootes &
Chabreck 1993) and bridled wallabies (Fisher & Lara 1999). In a low-density hunted
population of white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus Zimmermann), females adopted an
active search-for-mate strategy during the rut (Labisky & Fritzen 1998). The male/female
ratio in adult brown bears was lower in the north than in the south during our study, probably
due to a male bias in illegal hunting (Swenson et al. 2001a). Oestrous females may therefore
need to roam over larger areas to meet adult males in the north than in the south. The median
of range size of oestrous females in the mating season was nearly twice as large in the north
than in the south. Because the median range in the postmating season was similar in these
areas, the difference in range size in the mating season was probably not due to differences in
food availability, but that females roamed more to search for mates in the north than in the
south. In species where sexually selected infanticide occurs, mating with several males will
increase paternal uncertainty, thereby possibly reducing the probability of loosing dependent
offspring to infanticidal males (Hrdy 1979; Ebensberger 1998; Soltis et al. 2000). Thus,
sexually selected infanticide may represent another selective pressure favouring roaming in
oestrous females.
Mating ranges were larger than postmating ranges in males, providing support for the
“roam-to-mate hypothesis”. The seasonal changes in range size in males was therefore
probably due to a change in limiting resources, from receptive females in the mating season to

10
food availability and dispersion in the postmating season. Extreme seasonal changes in range
size in male stouts (Mustela erminea L.) was also explained in this way (Erlinge & Sandell
1986). However, because male range sizes were larger than those of oestrous females also in
the postmating season, range size in males during this season seemed to be influenced by
some other unknown factors as well. We speculate that males may be updating information on
competitors, immigrants and potential mates for the next breeding season.
Range size in females with yearlings contradicted the “roam-to-mate hypothesis”, as
they had mating season ranges that were not different from those of oestrous females. The
large ranges in the mating season perhaps may be explained by an increased energy demand
in these family groups (in which the total body mass may be twice as large as that of an
oestrous female), when compared to those of oestrous females and females with cubs.
The increase in range size from the mating season to the postmating season observed
in females with cubs and the fact that females with cubs used smaller ranges than oestrous
females and females with yearlings during the mating season, provided support for the
“infanticide avoidance hypothesis” (3), but could also be expected from the “immobility of
cubs hypothesis” (4). However, the inverse relationship that we observed between population
density and range size of females with cubs during the mating season contradicted the
“immobility of cubs hypothesis” (4). Thus, the increase in range size observed in females with
cubs from the mating season to the postmating season was not merely a result of increasing
mobility of cubs as they grew older. Several authors have reported that the presence of cubs
restricted movements of female bears for several months (e.g. Lindzey & Meslow 1977),
whereas others have reported restricted movements only for a short period immediately after
emergence from the dens (Reynolds & Beecham 1980) and that females with cubs were more
active during spring than any other age and sex category, including adult males (Powell et al.
1997). However, no authors have discussed whether these restricted movements were actually
due to low mobility of the cubs, which is an unlikely explanation considering their high
activity levels (Powell et al. 1997), or whether this was an adaptive behaviour by the female
to avoid contact with conspecifics, as we suggest. Although the evidence for female
avoidance of infanticidal males as a counter strategy to infanticide is rather limited
(Ebensperger 1998), it has been suggested to be operating in brown bears (Murie 1981;
Wielgus & Bunnel 1995).
Most previous studies of seasonal movements and range size in bears have related
these to seasonal shifts in food habits, not mating behaviour, and thus, seasons have been
defined differently than in our study. Alt, Alt & Linzey (1976) and Rogers (1987) observed

11
that adult female American black bears increased their daily movements when in oestrous,
though they did not calculate ranges during the mating season.
One alternative explanation for the seasonal shift in range size that we observed could
be that movements were linked to seasonal changes in food availability and dispersion
patterns, as food habits differ between these seasons (Dahle et al. 1998). However, range size
of nondispersing two-year-olds only increased slightly (10%) from the mating to the
postmating season and thus in the opposite direction of the change in range size observed in
oestrous individuals. Further, the increase in range size of two-year-olds (10%) was not large
enough to explain the increase (74-177%) in range size from the mating to the postmating
season in females with cubs. Although range size of individual bears is probably influenced
by food availability, the seasonal change in range size of two-year-olds suggests that seasonal
changes in food availability and dispersion only have minor impact on seasonal range size in
this study.
Even though the accuracy of range estimates based on such small numbers of fixes is
questionable, they should be comparable indices of the range size for different individual
categories. Therefore, we considered them adequate to address the questions posed in this
study.
We conclude that mating activities strongly influenced range size in Scandinavian
brown bears. Both males and oestrous females probably roam widely to mate and decrease
their range after the mating season. Females with cubs, on the other hand, minimise their
range size during the mating season, probably to reduce the risk of infanticide and also
perhaps partly due to reduced mobility of the small cubs in the mating season. Thus sexually
selected infanticide seem to influence range size in females, selecting for large mating season
ranges and multiple mating in oestrous females to hide paternity and for restricted mating
season ranges in females with cubs to avoid infanticidal males.

Acknowledgements
This study was funded by the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, the Swedish
Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwegian Directorate for Nature Management, the
Swedish Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management, WWF Sweden and the Research
Council of Norway. We thank the personnel in the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research
Project for their assistance in the field and Orsa Communal Forest for field support. A.

12
Moksnes, G. Rosenqvist and B. Stokke gave valuable comments on an earlier draft of the
manuscript. All animal experimentation reported in this paper complies with the current laws
regulating the treatment of animals in Sweden and Norway and were approved by the
appropriate ethical committees in both countries.

13
References
Agrell J., Wolff, J.O. & Ylönen, H. (1998) Counter-strategies to infanticide in mammals: cost
and consequences. Oikos 83, 507-517.
Alt, G.L., Alt, F.W. & Linzey, J.S. (1976) Home range and activity patterns of black bears in
northeastern Pennsylvania. Transactions of the Northeastern Section Wildlife Society
33, 45-46.
Andersson, M. (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1989) Mammalian mating systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London B 236, 339-372.
Clutton-Brock, T.H. & Harvey, P.H. (1978) Mammals, resources and reproductive strategies.
Nature 273, 191-195.
Craighead, J.J., Craighead, F.C. & McCutchen, H.E. (1970) Age determination of grizzly
bears from fourth premolar tooth sections. Journal of Wildlife Management 34, 353-
363.
Craighead, J.J., Sumner J.S. & Mitchell, J.A. (1995) The grizzly bears of Yellowstone. Island
Press,Washington D.C.
Dahle, B. & Swenson, J.E. (2003) Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus
arctos): effect of mass, sex, reproductive status, population density and habitat type.
Journal of Zoology 260, (In press).
Dahle, B., Sørensen, O.J., Wedul, H., Swenson, J.E. & Sandegren, F. (1998) The diet of
brown bears in central Scandinavia: effect of access to free-ranging domestic sheep.
Wildlife Biology 3, 147-158.
Ebensperger, L.A. (1998) Strategies and counterstrategies to infanticide in mammals.
Biological Review 73, 321-346.
Erlinge, S. & Sandell, M. (1986) Seasonal changes in the social organisation of male stoats,
Mustela erminea: An effect of shifts between two decisive resources. Oikos
47, 57-62.
Fisher, D.O. & Lara, M.C. (1999) Effects of body size and home range on access to mates
and paternity in male bridled wallabies. Animal Behaviour 58, 121-130.
Gray, E.M. (1997) Do female blackbirds benefit genetically from seeking extra-pair
copulations? Animal Behaviour 53, 605-623.
Hrdy, S.B. (1979) Infanticide among animals: a review, classification, and examination
of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol Sociobiol

14
1, 1-13.
Huber, D. & Roth, H.U. (1993) Movements of European brown bears in Croatia. Acta
Theriologica 38, 151-159.
Ims, R.A. (1987a) Responses in spatial organization and behaviour to manipulation of the
food resource in the vole Cltethrionomys rufocanus. Journal of Animal Ecology 56,
585-596.
Ims, R.A. (1987b) Male spacing systems in microtine rodents. Amercan Naturalist 130, 475-
484.
Kenward, R.E. & Hodder, K.H. (1996) Ranges V. An analysis system for biological location
data. Institute of Terrestrial Ecology, Furzebrook Research Station, Wareham,
Dorset.
Labisky, R.F. & Fritzen, D.E. (1998) Spatial mobility of breeding female white-tailed deer in
a low-density population. Journal of Wildlife Management 62, 1329-1334.
LeCount, A.L. (1987) Causes of black bear cub mortality. International Conference on Bear
Research and Management 7. (ed P. Zager), pp75-82. International Association for
Bear Research and Management.
Liberg, O., Johansson, A., Andersen, R. & Linnel, J.D.C. (1998) Mating system, mating
tactics and the function of male territoriality in roe deer. The European roe deer: the
biology of success. (eds R. Andersen, P. Duncan & J.D.C Linnell), pp 221-256.
Scandinavian University Press, Oslo.
Lindzey, F.G. & Meslow, E.C. (1977) Home range and habitat use by black bears in
southwestern Washington. Journal of Wildlife Management 41, 413-425.
McLellan, B. (1994) Density-dependent population regulation of brown bears. Density
dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar bears. (ed M. Taylor), pp
15-24. International Association for Bear Research and Management.
Murie, A. (1981) The grizzlies of Mount McKinley. U. S. Department of the Interior,
National Park Service, Washington, D. C.
Norusis, M.J. (1998) SPSS 8.0 Guide to data analysis. New Jersey, Prentice Hall.
Powell, R.A., Zimmerman, J.W. & Seaman, D.E. (1997) Ecology and behaviour of North
American black bears. Chapman and Hall, New York.
Pusey, A.E. & Packer, C. (1994) Infanticide in lions: consequences and counterstrategies.
Infanticide and parental care. (eds S. Parmigiani & F. von Saal), pp 279-299.
Hardwood, London,
Reynolds, D.G. & Beecham, J.J. (1980) Home range activities and reproduction of black

15
bears in west central Idaho. International Conference on Bear Research and
Management 4. (eds C.J. Martinka & K.L. McArthur), pp 181-190. Bear Biology
Association.
Rogers, L.L. (1987) Effects of food supply and kinship on social behaviour, movements,
and population dynamics of black bears in northeastern Minnesota. Wildlife
Monographs 97.
Rootes, W.L. & Chabreck, R.H. (1993) Reproductive status and movements of adult female
alligators. Journal of Herpetology 27, 121-126.
Sandegren, F. & Swenson, J.E. (1997) Björnen - viltet, ekologin och människan.
Svenska Jägareförbundet, Uppsala (In Swedish).
Sandell, M. (1989). The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. Carnivore
behaviour, ecology, and evolution 1. (ed J.L. Gittleman), pp 164-182. Cornell
University Press, New York.
San Jose, C. & Lovari, S. (1998) Ranging movements of female roe deer: do home-loving
does roam to mate? Ethology 104, 721-728.
Sæther, S.A. (1997) Stastistiske metoder. Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of
Science and Technology, Trondheim (In Norwegian).
Schwagmeyer, P.L. (1988) Scramble-competition polygyny in an asocial mammal: male
mobility and mating success. American Naturalist 131, 885-892.
Sokal, R.R. & Rohlf, F.J. (1995) Biometry. Freeman and Company, New York.
Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., Matsubayashi, K. & Takenaka, O. (2000) Infanticide by resident
males and female counterstrategies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata).
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48, 195-202.
Soroker, V. & Terkel, J. (1988) Changes in incidence of infanticidal and parental responses
during the reproductive cycle in male and female wild mice (mus musculus). Animal
Behaviour 36, 1275-1281.
Stockley, P. & Purvis, A. (1993) Sperm competition in mammals – a comparative-study of
male roles and relative investment in sperm production. Functional Ecology 7, 560-
570.
Swenson, J.E., Dahle, B. & Sandegren, F. (2001b) Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian
brown bears older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus 12, 81-92.
Swenson, J.E., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Söderberg, A., Wabakken, P. & Franzén, R.
(1994) Size, trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear (Ursus arctos)
population in Sweden. Biological Conservation 70, 9-17.

16
Swenson, J.E., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Wabakken, P., Söderberg, A. & Franzén, R.
(1997) Infanticide caused by hunting of male bears. Nature 386, 450-451.
Swenson, J.E., Sandegren, F., Segerström, P. & Brunberg, S. (2001a) Factors associated with
loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12, 69-80.
Swenson, J.E., Sandegren, F. & Söderberg, A. (1998) Geographic expansion of an increasing
brown bear population: evidence for presaturation dispersal. Journal of Animal
Ecology 67, 819-826.
Swenson, J.E., Wabakken, P., Sandegren, F., Bjärvall, A., Franzén, R. & Söderberg, A.
(1995) The near extinction and recovery of brown bears in Scandinavia in relation to
the bear management policies in Norway and Sweden. Wildlife Biology 1, 11-25.
Taylor, M. (1994) Density dependent population regulation in black, brown, and polar
bears. International Association for Bear Research and Management.
Trivers, R. (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. Sexual selection and the descent
of man. (ed B. Campbell), pp 136-179. Aldine, Chicago.
Tufto, J., Andersen, R. & Linnel, J. (1996) Habitat use and ecological correlates of home
range size in a small cervid: the roe deer. Journal of Animal Ecology 65, 715-725.
Wielgus, R.B. & Bunnel, F.L. (1995) Test of hypotheses for sexual segregation in grizzly
bears. Journal Wildlife Management 59, 552-560.
Wilson, N., Tubman, S.C., Eady, P.E. & Robertson, G.W. (1997) Female genotype affects
male success in sperm competition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B
264, 1491-1495.

17
Table 1. Summary of hypotheses and predictions
Hypothesis Prediction Supported
1 Roam to mate hypothesis Yes
1.1 Mating-season ranges would be larger than postmating-season ranges in
males and oestrous females yes
1.2 Mating-season ranges of oestrous females would be larger than for females
with dependent offspring yes/no
1.3 Mating-season ranges would be larger in males than oestrous females yes
1.4 Mating-season ranges of oestrous females would be larger in the north than
in the south yes
2 Increased foraging hypothesis No
2.1 Only females that raised cubs the previous year would have larger ranges in
the mating season than in the postmating season no
3 Infanticide avoidance hypothesis Yes
3.1 Mating-season ranges of females with cubs would be smaller than for
females in oestrous and females with yearlings yes
3.2 Mating-season ranges would be smaller than postmating-season ranges in
females with cubs yes
4. Immobility of cubs hypothesis No
4.1 Mating-season ranges of females with cubs would be uninfluenced by
population density no

18
Figure legends

Figure 1. Seasonal 100% minimum convex polygon (MCP) in Scandinavian brown bear
males, females in the south and females in the north. Boxes represent the interquartile range
containing 50% of the values. The error bars are the 5th and 95th percentiles, and are
th th
outliers beyond the 5 and 95 percentiles. Extremes (Ñ) are cases with values more than 3
a
times smaller or larger than the interquartile range and were excluded in statistical analyses.
Two extremes are not displayed in the mating season in the south (8407 and 15305 km2). b
Includes three females with yearlings in the south.

Figure 2. Estimated marginal means of lg10 seasonal 100% minimum convex polygon
(MCP) of males and oestrous females in the southern and northern study area in the mating
season and postmating season.

19
[Figure 1, Dahle & Swenson]

Mating season
Postmating season
5000
Males
4000
2
100% MCP km

3000

2000

1000

0
N = 31 25 10 9
Southa North

900
Females south
800
700
100% MCP km2

600
500
400
300
200
100
0
N= 33 33 25 22
Oestrous With cubs

800 Females north


700
2

600
100% MCP km

500
400
300
200
100
0
N = 17 17 13 13 10 11
Oestrous With cubs With yearlingsb

20
[Figure 2, Dahle & Swenson]

South
North
3,1
Mating season
Est. marg. means of log10 100% MCP

3,0

2,9

2,8

2,7

2,6

2,5
Males Females

2,8
Postmating season
Est. marg. means of log10 100% MCP

2,7

2,6

2,5

2,4

2,3

2,2
Males Females

21

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen