Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications


journal homepage: WWW.JDCJOURNAL.COM

Impaired cognitive processing speed in type 1 diabetic patients who had


severe/recurrent hypoglycaemia
Stefano Bortolotti a,1, Lisa Zarantonello a,1, Ambra Uliana b, Nicola Vitturi b, Sami Schiff a, Patrizia Bisiacchi c,
Angelo Avogaro b, Piero Amodio a,⁎,2, Alberto Maran b,2
a
Unit of Internal Medicine 5, Department of Medicine, University of Padova, Padova 35128, Italy
b
Unit of Metabolic Disease, Department of Medicine, University of Padova, Padova 35128, Italy
c
Department General Psychology and CIRMANMEC, University of Padova, Padova 35128, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Aims: To detect whether adults with type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM) have lower cognitive performance than
Received 10 April 2018 healthy individuals and to detect risk factors for low cognitive performance.
Received in revised form 16 July 2018 Methods: Twenty-six adults with T1DM and twenty-six healthy subjects matched for age, gender and educational
Accepted 6 August 2018 level were compared for cognitive performance by a chronometric computerized test measuring visuo-spatial
Available online 10 August 2018
working memory (N-Back) and by two validated neuropsychological tests (Mini Mental State Examination, An-
imal Naming Test). Clinical data about diabetes duration, average daily insulin dosage, glycated haemoglobin, ret-
Keywords:
Diabetes type 1
inopathy, urine albumin-creatinine ratio, previous hypoglycaemic coma and awareness of hypoglycaemia were
Awareness of hypoglycaemia obtained from medical records. Basal pre-test glycemia and blood pressure were measured for each patient.
Unawareness of hypoglycaemia Results: No differences were found between patients (n = 26) and healthy controls (n = 26) in neuropsycholog-
Cognition ical tests. Within diabetic patients, those with impaired awareness of hypoglycaemia (n = 7) or history of coma
Cognitive processing speed in the recent 1–3 years (n = 5) had psychomotor slowing at the N-Back test (592 ± 35 vs. 452 ± 21 ms and
619 ± 40 vs. 462 ± 19 ms, respectively; both p b 0.01). The variables related to diabetic severity did not show
a relationship with reaction times of the N-Back test.
Conclusion: Psychomotor speed slowing is detectable in patients with T1DM who have a history of previous
hypoglycaemic episodes or coma.
© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction sustained attention, executive functions, general intelligence and


visuoconstruction is uncertain, and the pathogenesis of DACD is still
T1DM is characterized by acute and chronic complications. The unknown.
main long-term complication is diabetic angiopathy, which is The main variables affecting the risk appear to be the age of onset of
classified as microvascular (retinopathy, nephropathy, neuropathy) diabetes, the development of microvascular complications and the level
and macrovascular (atherosclerosis). Neurological sequelae can be of glycaemic control (hypoglycaemia or hyperglycaemia). Early onset of
neuropathic (distal symmetric polyneuropathy, autonomic neuropathy, diabetes (4–7 years of age) is linked to greater neurocognitive dysfunc-
mononeuropathy) or cognitive (depression, cognitive decline). tions and structural alterations than late onset. This event is probably
Evidence about type 1 diabetes-associated cognitive decline (DACD) is due both to the duration of diabetes and to its effects on neuro-
conflicting, both on magnitude of impairment and on which cognitive development.3,4 In adults with T1DM, the occurrence of microvascular
domains are involved. The most common deficits consist in the slowing complications is related to increased and early cognitive decline. 5,6
of cognitive processing speed (CPS) and the worsening of mental Moreover, in some neuroimaging studies, type 1 diabetic patients with
flexibility. 1,2 The specific nature of the deterioration of memory, retinopathy have shown an augmented focal cortical atrophy and a re-
duced white matter volume. 7,8 Longitudinal studies have demonstrated
Declaration of interest: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest rel- that patients with elevated glycated haemoglobin (N8%) present worse
evant to this article. cognitive efficiency and velocity than subjects with good glycaemic
⁎ Corresponding author at: Dipartimento di Medicina – DIMED – and CIRMANMEC, Via control.9,10 These deficits seem to be reversed by improved metabolic
Giustiniani 2, 35128 Padova, Italy.
E-mail address: piero.amodio@unipd.it (P. Amodio).
control. The intensive use of insulin to prevent hyperglycaemia and its
1
Shared first authorship. complications has incremented the incidence of hypoglycaemic crises.
2
Shared senior authorship. Patients treated with an intensive therapeutic regimen, especially

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdiacomp.2018.08.005
1056-8727/© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
S. Bortolotti et al. / Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045 1041

those with glycated haemoglobin near the non-diabetic range, have a 3 Table 1
times greater risk of severe hypoglycaemia than subjects with a less in- Demographic characteristics, neuropsychological and behavioural performance of the
study participants.
tensive insulin therapy. Subjects affected by T1DM have on average 2
episodes of mild hypoglycaemia a week; about one third of them pres- Patients with Control p
ent a severe episode annually. 11 DM1 subjects

The severe detrimental effects of acute hypoglycaemia on cognitive N 26 26 –


performance, especially on memory and executive functions, are widely Age (years) 45.8 ± 12.6 45.9 ± 12.8 0.97
Gender (M/F) 17/9 17/9 –
known. However, the role of hypoglycaemia and its complications on
Education (years) 13.4 ± 3.8 13.9 ± 3.7 0.63
long-term cognitive decline are still uncertain. Some studies support Humeral arterial Pressure Diastolic 75 ± 5 – –
the hypothesis that the brain may adapt to hypoglycaemic insult.5,12,13 (mm Hg) Systolic 129 ± 9 – –
Other studies, in contrast, argue that hypoglycaemia can sensitize neu- Mean 93 ± 5 – –
rons, increasing the negative effects of glycopenia.14 Recurrent episodes Glycaemia (mg/dl) 130 ± 34 – –
DM1 duration (years) 18 ± 10 – –
of hypoglycaemia cause both a down-regulation of counter-regulatory
Insulin (U/die) 49 ± 21 – –
response (hypoglycaemia-associated autonomic failure–HAAS) and a HbA1c (%) 7.3 ± 0.6 – –
reduction of autonomic hypoglycaemic symptoms (resulting in un- HbA1c (mmol/mol) 56 ± 6
awareness of hypoglycaemia), which are associated with a sixfold in- Retinopathy Absent 46% – –
Simple 39% – –
crease in the risk of severe hypoglycaemia and a prolonged encephalic
Proliferative 15% – –
exposure to glycopenic insult.15,16 Perros et al.17 underlined that alter- Urine albumin/creatinine (mg/mMol) 6 ± 3.5 – –
ation of EEG, induced by hypoglycaemia can become permanent after se- Previous hypoglycaemic coma 19% – –
vere or recurrent crises. Increased brain susceptibility to hypoglycaemia Unawareness of hypoglycaemia 27% – –
seems to occur especially from birth to 20 years of age. 4,18 However, CriQ Education* 115 ± 21 101 ± 13 0.03
Work 109 ± 15 111 ± 16 0.66
at a later age also, the brain may become unable to cope with
Free-time 118 ± 23 110 ± 16 0.2
hypoglycaemia. activity
Thus, since i) the occurrence of cognitive alterations in T1DM and Total 119 ± 24 108 ± 17 0.13
the role that hypoglycaemia may have in their pathophysiology is de- MMSE 29.4 ± 0.9 29.5 ± 0.9 0.97
ANT 24 ± 6 23 ± 5 0.6
bated and ii) there are few data about the cognitive sequelae of individ-
N-Back condition (ms) 0-Back 425 ± 93 385 ± 58 0.07
uals with or without hypoglycaemia awareness, we designed a study to 1-Back 450 ± 109 409 ± 94 0.16
detect whether adults with T1DM have lower cognitive performance 2-Back 593 ± 131 557 ± 148 0.37
than healthy individuals, as well as to investigate the risk factors for Mean 490 ± 31 451 ± 31 0.08
low cognitive performance in type 1 diabetic subjects. Data are mean ± SD; *, statistical significance; Significance level was p b 0.05.

2. Subjects, materials and methods 2.2. Neuropsychological evaluation

2.1. Subjects and data • Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE): it assesses cognitive de-
cline. The MMSE explores: spatial and temporal orientation, learn-
This study involved 26 participants with T1DM and 26 healthy con- ing, calculation, comprehension (written and oral), writing and
trol subjects matched for age, gender and educational level. The demo- constructional apraxia.19
graphic characteristics of the two groups are shown in Table 1. Diabetic • Beck Depression Inventory – II (BDI-II): a self-administered ques-
patients were recruited from the Diabetes Clinic at the University Hospi- tionnaire that identifies depressive symptoms and investigates the
tal of Padua where they were seen regularly each month. All partici- depression level.20
pants attended two sessions: in the first, they provided information • Cognitive Reserve Index Questionnaire (Cri-q): a test that evaluates
about their medical history, and then they submitted to preliminary cognitive reserve by educational level (school years and extra-
neuropsychological tests (Mini Mental State Examination, Back Depres- scholastic courses), work and free-time activities. 21
sion Inventory-II, Cognitive Reserve Index Questionnaire, Animal Nam- • Animal Naming Test (ANT): a verbal fluency test in which partici-
ing Test) to verify the presence of exclusion criteria. Exclusion criteria pants have to name as many animals as possible in 1 min. The
were: serious systemic disease (heart failure, chronic kidney disease, total sum of animals correctly reported (excluding errors of intru-
cirrhosis, severe lungs and airways diseases, addiction), cerebrovascular sion or perseveration) represents the final score.22
disease, coronary artery disease, severe neuropathy, major psychiatric
disorders and use of tranquillizer, antidepressant or anti-psychotic 2.3. Cognitive test protocol
drugs. Based on tests administered during preliminary evaluation, par-
ticipants were also excluded if they had cognitive decline (Mini Mental • N-Back Test: a computerized chronometric test providing insight
State Examination b26 and Animal Naming Test ≤14) or depression into both CPS in a go/no-go condition and working memory
(Back Depression Inventory N13). Clinical data about diabetes duration (WM). The task was an adapted version of the visuospatial N-Back
(years from onset of the disease), average daily insulin dosage (U/die), test from Cui et al.23 and Haberecht et al.24 The stimuli were admin-
glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c – %), presence of retinopathy (absent, istered using a personal computer (17-in. display; resolution: 1024
proliferative, non-proliferative), urine albumin-creatinine ratio (mg/ × 768 pixels) by E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pitts-
mMol), previous hypoglycaemic coma and awareness of hypoglycaemia burgh, PA). The blank screen was divided into 9 equal parts by a
were recorded for each patient. In the second session, participants per- black grid during the test. The stimulus was the letter O (66
formed the computerized N-Back test. Pre-test glycemia (mg/dl) and × 156 pixels), which appeared in one of the grid's sections for
pre-test blood pressure (mm Hg) were measured for each diabetic pa- 1500 ms. After this period, there was a fixed inter-stimulus interval
tient by glucometer and sphygmomanometer, respectively. The proto- of 500 ms in which only the grid was displayed. The position of the
col was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of letter changed in each trial. The test was block design, and it was
University of Padova. The purpose and the nature of the study were ex- composed of three conditions with increasing working memory
plained to all subjects, and informed written consent was obtained be- load. During the control condition (0-Back), participants had to
fore their participation. press the response button if the letter O appeared in the central
1042 S. Bortolotti et al. / Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045

part of the grid. This is a simple RT; however, it requires the inhibi- variables was performed using the Newman-Keuls correction for multi-
tion of the response if the letter O is not in the centre of the screen, ple comparison. Statistical analysis was performed by TIBCO Software
and thus it reflects both a go/no-go task and a control condition for Inc. (2017), Statistica (data analysis software system), version 13.
WM, which is not required for this task. In the 1-Back condition http://statistica.io.
(lower working memory load), subjects were required to respond
when the stimulus appeared in the same position as in the previous 3. Results
trial. Finally, in the 2-Back condition (higher working memory load),
the instruction was to press the response button every time the let- The demographic and neuropsychological variables were compara-
ter appeared in the same position as two trials before. One block was ble in participants with and without diabetes, except for Cri-Education
composed of 6 sub-blocks for the 1-Back alternate, with 6 sub- (Table 1). The RTW of the N-Back task increased with memory load in
blocks for 0-Back. The other block was composed of 6 sub-blocks both groups (ANOVA: N-Back condition (0,1,2) - F2,94 = 6.1, p b 0.01).
for the 2-Back condition as well as 0-Back. Before each block, there The education level improved the efficacy of WM, since the higher the
was a practice block in which participants performed the task until education level, the lower the increment of RTW from the 0-Back condi-
they understood it completely. The order in which 1-Back and 2- tion to the 2-Back condition in both groups (ANOVA: interaction N-Back
Back blocks were administered was counterbalanced among partici- condition (0,1,2) × Education - F2,94 = 6.1, p b 0.01). Age influenced the
pants to prevent confounding effects (learning and mental fatigue). RTW homogeneously in all conditions (F1,47 = 11,02 p b 0.01; interac-
Each condition was formed by 23 stimuli, 7 of which were targets tion age × N-Back condition (0,1,2) - F2,94 = 2.31 p = 0.10).
and 16 were non-targets, for a total of 552 stimuli, 168 of which Thus, all the analyses were adjusted by age and education.
were targets and 384 were non-targets. The percentage of targets The performance of the N-Back task was comparable in diabetic pa-
(33% of total stimuli) was decided based on the study by Jaeggi et al.25 tients and controls (F1,47 = 3.14, p = 0.08); however, a trend for cogni-
tive processing slowing in patients with T1DM in the 0-Back condition
2.4. Statistical analysis was detectable (Table 1).
Patients with T1DM and unawareness of hypoglycaemia had
Results are expressed as mean ± SD, unless otherwise indicated. The prolonged RTW in each condition compared both to patients aware of
comparison between groups (patients vs. controls, awareness vs. un- hypoglycaemia and control subjects (F2,45 = 5.8, p b 0.01; Table 2, Fig.
awareness of hypoglycaemia, previous vs. no previous coma) was per- 1a). The effect of WM load on RTW was similar for the three subgroups
formed by general linear model, using age and education years as (control subjects, aware patients, unaware patients): interaction
covariates. Between factors were the reaction times for different condi- subgroups × N-Back conditions (0,1,2) - F4,92 = 0.11, p = 0.98.
tions of the N-Back test (0-Back, 1-Back, 2-Back). The reaction time of Likewise, hypoglycaemic coma was associated with cognitive pro-
the N-Back were adjusted for the accuracy and expressed as weighted cessing slowing, both compared to patients who had not had previous
RT (RTW) in accordance with the following formula: RTW = RT + [RT hypoglycaemic coma and controls (F2,46 = 5.9, p b 0.01), with an effect
(1 − Accuracy)]. This was done to overcome the problem posed by of WM load similar in the three groups (interaction with the N-Back
the speed-accuracy trade-off. Post hoc comparison of more than two condition (0,1,2) - F4,92 = 0.28, p = 0.9) (Table 2, Fig. 1b).

Table 2
Demographic and clinical characteristics, neurophysiological and behavioural evaluation of patients.

Patients with DM1

Hypoglycaemia History of hypoglycaemic coma

Unawareness Awareness Yes No

N 7 19 5 21
Age (years)*, † 59 ± 8 41 ± 11 58 ± 10 43 ± 12
Gender (M/F) 4/3 13/6 2/3 15/6
Education (years) 13 ± 5.2 13 ± 3.2 11 ± 4,5 14 ± 3,4
Humeral arterial pressure (mm Hg) Diastolic 74 ± 5 75 ± 5 74 ± 5 76 ± 5
Systolic 131 ± 8 129 ± 9 132 ± 10 129 ± 9
Mean 93 ± 5 93 ± 4 93 ± 6 93 ± 4
Glycaemia (mg/dl) 121 ± 32 133 ± 35 128 ± 32 130 ± 35
DM1 duration (years) 21 ± 16 18 ± 8 24 ± 18 17 ± 8
Insulin (U/die) 45 ± 20 52 ± 21 41 ± 23 50 ± 21
HbA1c (%) 7.2 ± 0.6 7.3 ± 0.6 7.2 ± 0.7 7.3 ± 0.6
HbA1c (mmol/mol) 55 ± 6 56 ± 6 55 ± 7 56 ± 6
Retinopathy Absent 14% 58% 20% 52%
Simple 86% 21% 80% 29%
Proliferative 0% 21% 0% 19%
Urine albumin/creatinine (mg/mMol) 6.8 ± 4.5 5.6 ± 3.4 8.7 ± 3.8 5.2 ± 3.3
Previous hypoglycaemic coma 72% 0% – –
Unawareness of hypoglycaemia – – 100% 10%
CriQ Education 110 ± 25 117 ± 20 107 ± 28 117 ± 20
Work 109 ± 17 108 ± 15 102 ± 8 111 ± 17
Free-Time 118 ± 39 118 ± 13 109 ± 38 121 ± 16
Total 117 ± 36 119 ± 16 106 ± 32 123 ± 20
MMSE 29 ± 0.9 29 ± 0.9 29 ± 1.1 29 ± 0.8
ANT 27 ± 5 23 ± 6 26 ± 5 24 ± 6
N-Back condition (ms) 0-Back*, † 512 ± 99 393 ± 69 496 ± 46 408 ± 94
1-Back*, † 546 ± 119 412 ± 80 556 ± 104 423 ± 95
2-Back*, † 702 ± 149 552 ± 100 767 ± 113 551 ± 98
Mean*, † 592 ± 35 452 ± 21 619 ± 40 462 ± 19

Data are mean ± SD; ⁎, p b 0.05: Awareness vs. Unawareness of hypoglycaemia; †, p b 0.05 history of hypoglycaemic coma vs. no history of hypoglycaemic coma. Free-Time = Free-Time
Activities. The interval between previous hypoglycaemic coma and psychometric testing was 1–4 years.
S. Bortolotti et al. / Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045 1043

Fig. 1. Left panel: patients with unawareness of hypoglycaemia displayed slower RTW than patients with awareness of hypoglycaemia and controls; right panel: patients who had
hypoglycaemic coma displayed slower RTW than patients who did not have hypoglycaemic coma and controls. The bars represent confidence intervals.

The mean arterial pressure (MAP) of diabetic patients showed an in- intensive insulin regimen in diabetic population, the incidence of
verse correlation with RTW during the 2-Back condition; thus, the lower long-term complications is lower than in the past. Even if it is difficult
the MAP, the longer the RTW (r = 0.435, p = 0.03). to quantify correctly the prevalence and incidence of T1DM complica-
The variables related to diabetic severity (pre-test glycemia, T1DM tions, the cumulative incidence of proliferative retinopathy, nephropa-
duration, average daily insulin dosage, HbA1c, retinopathy, urine thy and cardiovascular disease after 30 years from the diagnosis is
albumin-creatinine ratio) did not show a relationship with the RTW of supposed to be 21%, 9% and 9%, respectively.27 Fifteen percent of the pa-
the N-Back task (Table 3). tients enrolled have a proliferative retinopathy, whereas the study has
Of note, no relationship was found between the N-Back and the ANT ruled out patients with overt nephropathy and cardiovascular disease
(p = 0.9), or between the N-Back and the MMSE (p = 0.3). to prevent confounding effects of these complications on cognitive
performance.
4. Discussion Both in controls and in diabetic subjects the RTW of the N-Back task
had the expected shape: the RTW in the lower cognitive load condition
Our study has shown that patients with T1DM who suffer from un- (0-Back) were significantly shorter than those in the 1-Back condition
awareness of hypoglycaemia as well as patients who have had previous which, in turn, were shorter than those in the 2-Back condition. This is
episodes of hypoglycaemic coma, present cognitive processing slowing in accordance with previous studies.28,29
on accurate chronometric neuropsychological investigation, despite the In the patients group, as well as in control subjects, high educational
fact that the MMSE and a semantic memory task did not reveal cognitive level (N16 years) facilitates the mnestic task. It appears that education is
dysfunction. In contrast, no relationship was found between psycho- a protective factor for cognitive efficiency in diabetic patients and in
metric tests and indexes of poor glycaemic control. Thus, our data sup- healthy people. This hypothesis is credible, given the numerous studies
port the view that overly strict control of diabetes, exposing patients to demonstrating the role of education in cognitive performance. 30,31
hypoglycaemic episodes, selectively impairs cognition in patients with The finding that patients with hypoglycaemia unawareness showed
T1DM. a reduction in CPS, compared to those with hypoglycaemia awareness,
The group of patients in the study was well representative of the supports the theory of a detrimental effect of recurrent hypoglycaemia
adult type 1 diabetic population. The average age of the sample was on cognitive functions. 32 The fact that cognitive performances on
46 years (maximum: 69; minimum: 25 years of age). Some 46% of the MMSE and ANT were comparable between healthy subjects and dia-
patients had a high-school degree, whereas 31% of them had an aca- betic patients, as well as between patients with or without awareness
demic degree. There was a slight prevalence of male gender (65%) in of hypoglycaemia, highlights that only sensitive cognitive tools oriented
the sample. This is not surprising as T1DM, despite being an immune- to studying specific domains vulnerable to hypoglycaemia are able to
mediated pathology, is more common in men and boys.26 Some recent detect the phenomenon.
longitudinal studies have shown that, since the spreading of the This finding is not surprising, as paper and pencil tests are adequate
only for detecting gross cognitive deficits, while the highly accurate
chronometric N-Back test, used in this study, is oriented to measuring
a potentially vulnerable area with more sensitive results. Thus, our in-
Table 3 vestigation has pointed out the importance of using the most precisely
Summary results of repeated measures ANOVA for variables related to diabetic severity for chosen methods in the study of cognitive impairment in T1DM to pre-
the three conditions of the N-Back. vent misleading conclusions.33,34
F p Cognitive processing slowing affects actions that require mental alert-
ness, like driving or practicing sports, while intellectual activities, which
DM1 duration 0.3 0.57
Average daily insulin dosage 0.2 0.63
can be performed more slowly, might not be involved at all. However, it
A/C ratio 0.3 0.59 cannot be overlooked that the cognitive slowing showed by patients
Retinopathy 2.9 0.07 with unaware hypoglycaemia, even if moderate, impairs everyday life.
Basal glycaemia 0.07 0.86 As regards patients with previous hypoglycaemic coma, they have
HbA1c 0.47 0.50
shown longer RTW compared to subjects without a history of coma.
1044 S. Bortolotti et al. / Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045

This result is in agreement with a previous study conducted by our 3. Gaudieri PA, Chen R, Greer TF, Holmes CS. Cognitive function in children with type 1
diabetes. Diabetes Care 2008;31:1892-7, https://doi.org/10.2337/dc07-2132.
centre35 and other data in the literature 36 that showed permanent cere- 4. Biessels GJ, Deary IJ, Ryan CM. Cognition and diabetes: a lifespan perspective. Lancet
bral sequelae caused by severe hypoglycaemia. Neurol 2008;7:184-90, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70021-8.
When considering all of these issues, our data do not support the 5. Ferguson SC, Blane A, Perros P, et al. Cognitive ability and brain structure in type 1
diabetes: relation to microangiopathy and preceding severe hypoglycemia. Diabetes
hypothesis that recurrent severe hypoglycaemia episodes do not 2003;52:149-56, https://doi.org/10.2337/diabetes.52.1.149.
have long-term adverse effects on the cognitive capacity of patients 6. Ryan CM, Geckle MO, Orchard TJ. Cognitive efficiency declines over time in adults
with T1DM. 9 An explanation to this discrepancy can be given by with type 1 diabetes: effects of micro- and macrovascular complications.
Diabetologia 2003;46:940-8, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00125-003-1128-2.
our data showing that MMSE and ANT were not influenced by a his-
7. Wessels AM, Simsek S, Remijnse PL, et al. Voxel-based morphometry demonstrates
tory of hypoglycaemia because of their probable lack of sensitivity reduced grey matter density on brain MRI in patients with diabetic retinopathy.
and the selectivity of the cognitive impairment produced by Diabetologia 2006;49:2474-80, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00125-006-0283-7.
8. Musen G, Lyoo IK, Sparks CR, et al. Effects of type 1 diabetes on gray matter density as
hypoglycaemia.
measured by voxel-based morphometry. Diabetes 2006;55:326-33.
The main strengths of this study consist of: (1) the use of a highly 9. DCCT study research group D. Long-term effect of diabetes and its treatment on cognitive
sensitive chronometric test (N-Back), which investigates cognitive pro- function. N Engl J Med 2007;356(18):1842-52. [http://www.nejm.org/doi/pdf/10.1056/
cessing slowing and WM, in addition to basic cognitive tests to study NEJMoa066397].
10. Reichard P, Pihl M, Rosenqvist U, Sule J. Complications in IDDM are caused by ele-
DACD; (2) a rigorous definition of hypoglycaemia unawareness, deter- vated blood glucose level: the Stockholm diabetes intervention study (SDIS) at 10-
mined using validated methods (Clarke and Gold Scores37,38), and of se- year follow up. Diabetologia 1996;39:1483-8.
vere hypoglycaemia (which only includes subjects with previous 11. Frier BM. Hypoglycaemia in diabetes mellitus: epidemiology and clinical implica-
tions. Nat Rev Endocrinol 2014;10:711-22, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrendo.2014.170.
hypoglycaemic coma); and (3) the usage of strict exclusion criteria 12. Boyle PJ, Nagy RJ, O'Connor AM, Kempers SF, Yeo RA, Qualls C. Adaptation in brain glu-
that have ruled out patients with nephropathies, cardiovascular disease, cose uptake following recurrent hypoglycemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1994;91:9352-6.
depression and other comorbidities, to ensure that healthy volunteers 13. McCrimmon R, Öz G. Cerebral adaptation to recurrent hypoglycemia. 2012;3:89-114.
14. Brismar T, Hyllienmark L, Ekberg K, Johansson B-L. Loss of temporal lobe beta power
are only compared with patients with the best cognitive performance. in young adults with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Neuroreport 2002;13:2469-73, https://
The limitation in our study is to be found in the relatively small number doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000047688.08940.ab.
of cases observed, which partly restricts analysis of subgroups. How- 15. Seaquist ER, Anderson J, Childs B, et al. Hypoglycemia and diabetes: a report of a
workgroup of the American Diabetes Association and the Endocrine Society. Diabetes
ever, the effect of recurrent hypoglycaemia seems to be sufficiently rel-
Care 2013;36:1384-95, https://doi.org/10.2337/dc12-2480.
evant to allow for the identification of cognitive changes even with a 16. Graveling AJ, Frier BM. Altération de la perception des hypoglycémies. Revue générale. Di-
small sample size. It would be important, for the future, to extend this abetes Metab 2010;36:S64-74, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1262-3636(10)70470-5.
17. Perros P, Bjorgaas M. In: Frier BM, Heller SR, McCrimmon RJ, eds. Hypoglycaemia in
study to a less select patient population and to validate the findings
Clinical Diabetes. 3rd edn. Wiley Blackwell; 2014. p. 305-22.
with an independent group of diabetic patients. 18. Blasetti A, Chiuri RM, Tocco AM, et al. The effect of recurrent severe hypoglycemia on
cognitive performance in children with type 1 diabetes: a meta-analysis. J Child
Neurol 2011;26:1383-91, https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073811406730.
5. Conclusion 19. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading
the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-98.
In conclusion, our study showed that a subset of patients with T1DM 20. Beck AT. Depression: Clinical, Experimental and Theoretical Aspects. University of Penn-
sylvania Press. 1967.
were found to have neuropsychological defects mainly regarding CPS. 21. Nucci M, Mapelli D, Mondini S. Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq): a new
This alteration was related to a history of previous hypoglycaemic epi- instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin Exp Res 2012;24:218-26,
sodes and coma; in contrast, complications related to poor glycaemic https://doi.org/10.3275/7800.
22. Sager MA, Hermann BP, La Rue A, Woodard JL. Screening for dementia in community-
control did not have any relationship to cognitive dysfunction, at least
based memory clinics. Wis Med J 2006;105:25-9.
in the observed individuals. 23. Cui X, Bray S, Bryant DM, Glover GH, Reiss AL. A quantitative comparison of NIRS and
fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. Neuroimage 2011;54:2808-21, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.069.
Author contributions 24. Haberecht MF, Menon V, Warsofsky IS, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of visuo-
spatial working memory in turner syndrome. Hum Brain Mapp 2001;14:96-107.
S.B. and L.Z. collected/analysed data, wrote the manuscript and con- 25. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Perrig WJ, Meier B. The concurrent validity of the N-back
task as a working memory measure. Memory 2010;18:394-412, https://doi.
tributed to the discussion. A.U. and N.V. collected data. S.S. collected data org/10.1080/09658211003702171.
and contributed to the discussion. P.B. contributed to the discussion and 26. Atkinson MA, Eisenbarth GS, Michels AW. Type 1 diabetes. Lancet 2014;383:69-82,
edited the manuscript. P.A. and A.M. designed the study, analysed the https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60591-7.
27. Nathan DM, Zimman B, Cleary PA, Backlund JYC, Genuth S, Miller R, et al. Modern-day
data, contributed to the discussion and reviewed/edited the manuscript. clinical course of type 1 diabetes mellitus after 30 years' duration. Arch Intern Med
A.A. contributed to discussion. 2009;169:1307-16.
28. Luu P, Caggiano DM, Geyer A, Lewis J, Cohn J, Tucker DM. Time-course of cortical net-
works involved in working memory. Front Hum Neurosci 2014;8:1-15, https://doi.
Additional information org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00004.
29. Chen Y-N, Mitra S, Schlaghecken F. Sub-processes of working memory in the N-back
P.A. is the corresponding author and guarantor of this work and, as task: an investigation using ERPs. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:1546-59, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.003.
such, had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility 30. Kaufman AS, Reynolds CR, McLean JE. Age and WAIS-R intelligence in a national sample of
for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. adults in the 20- to 74-year age range: a cross-sectional analysis with educational level con-
trolled. Dermatol Int 1989;13:235-53, https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(89)90020-2.
31. Berryhill ME, Jones KT. tDCS selectively improves working memory in older adults
Funding with more education. Neurosci Lett 2012;521:148-51, https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
NEULET.2012.05.074.
32. Hansen TI, Olsen SE, Haferstrom ECD, et al. Cognitive deficits associated with im-
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agen- paired awareness of hypoglycaemia in type 1 diabetes. Diabetologia 2017;60:
cies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. It was funded 971-9, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00125-017-4233-3. [Internet].
by Padova University funding to P.A. 33. Koski L, Brouillette MJ, Lalonde R, et al. Computerized testing augments pencil-and-
paper tasks in measuring HIV-associated mild cognitive impairment. HIV Med
2011;12:472-80, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-1293.2010.00910.x.
References 34. Silbert BS, Maruff P, Evered LA, et al. Detection of cognitive decline after coronary sur-
gery: a comparison of computerized and conventional tests. Br J Anaesth 2004;92:
1. Mijnhout GS, Scheltens P, Diamant M, et al. Diabetic encephalopathy: a concept in 814-20, https://doi.org/10.1093/bja/aeh157.
need of a definition. [1]Diabetologia 2006;49:1447-8, https://doi.org/10.1007/ 35. Maran A, Crepaldi C, Del Piccolo F, et al. Cognitive, neurophysiologic and metabolic
s00125-006-0221-8. sequelae of previous hypoglycemic coma revealed by hyperinsulinemic-
2. Brands AMA, Biessels GJ, DeHaan EH, Kappelle LJ, Kessels RP. The effects of type 1 di- hypoglycemic clamp in type 1 diabetic patients. Metab Brain Dis 2017;32:1543-51,
abetes on cognitive performance. Diabetes Care 2005;28:726-35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11011-017-0041-1.
S. Bortolotti et al. / Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications 32 (2018) 1040–1045 1045

36. Tonoli C, Heyman E, Roelands B, et al. Type 1 diabetes-associated cognitive decline: a hypoglycemic frequency and associated symptoms. Diabetes Care 1995;18:517-22,
meta-analysis and update of the current literature. J Diabetes 2014;6:499-513, https: https://doi.org/10.2337/diacare.18.4.517.
//doi.org/10.1111/1753-0407.12193. 38. Gold AE, Macleod KM, Frier BM. Frequency of severe hypoglycemia in patients with
37. Clarke WL, Cox DJ, Gonder-Frederick LA, Julian D, Schlundt D, Polonsky W. Reduced type I diabetes with impaired awareness of hypoglycemia. Diabetes Care 1994;17:
awareness of hypoglycemia in adults with IDDM: a prospective study of 697-703, https://doi.org/10.2337/diacare.17.7.697.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen