Sie sind auf Seite 1von 2

Effects of Collectivistic and Individualistic Cultures on Imagination Inflation in Eastern and Western jobs are interdependent, collectivism is preponderant,

reponderant, whereas in complex, stratified societies, where


Cultures affluence, independence, and differences are emphasized, individualism is preponderant.

By Iulia O. Basu-Zharku 2011, VOL. 3 NO. 02 | PG. ½ In particular, individualism is mostly seen in the cultures of Western Europe and North America, whereas
collectivism is mostly seen in the cultures of Asia, Africa, and parts of Europe and Latin America (Triandis,
ABSTRACT Part I 1993; Nelson & Fuvish, 2004). The emphasis on one or another starts in the family, even with the very
structure of the family: a large, multigenerational one emphasizes collectivism, whereas a smaller, nuclear
Previous research suggests that culture influences our autobiographical memories. This study sought to family emphasizes individualism (Triandis, 1993). Studies examining differences in collectivistic and
determine if the collectivism/individualism dimension of culture influences the process of imagination individualistic cultures often use either Asian Americans or people from Asian cultures, such as
inflation. Forty college students were given an Life Events Inventory (LEI) with individualistic and Vietnamese or Filipino and compare them to Caucasians or Americans (Skillman, 2000; Desai, 2007).
collectivistic events, and had to rate their confidence that each event happened or not in their childhood. These studies on families and intergenerational conflict show how individualistic societies value self-
Afterwards, they were asked to imagine a set of predetermined individualistic and collectivistic events and reliance, independence, autonomy, personal achievement (Skillman, 2000), and a definition of self apart
a week later they rated their confidence on a new LEI. Participants showed imagination inflation for the from the group and personal goals (Desai, 2007).
probed events. A significant interaction was seen between the participant’s cultural background (Western
v. Eastern) and the type of event (individualistic v. collectivistic). For individualistic events, people coming Collectivistic societies value family cohesion, cooperation, solidarity, and conformity (Skillman, 2000), and
from an Eastern cultural background showed greater imagination inflation. thus people is these societies tend to make more references to others, emphasize group goals, and follow
the expectations and regulations of the group (Desai, 2007). Such cultural differences mean that people in
What do we remember from the vast quantity of events happening to us, involving us, and relevant to our different cultures have fundamentally different construals of the self and others. For more collectivistic
life-story? For example, the fact that you went on a trip with your classmates in the 5th grade and the societies, interdependent construals are the norm: The self is a part of a community, defined relative to
details of the trip might be relevant to your life-story if many of your current friends were still some of the others, concerned with belongingness, dependency, empathy, reciprocity and focused on small, selective
children that back then were your classmates. People’s memories for their experiences are not a veridical in-groups at the expense of out-groups. The interdependent self exercises control to the interior, so that
recording of such experiences, however (e.g., Schacter, 2001), and can be influenced by many factors. An cognition and representation involve attentiveness to others, and personal attributes and actions are
important and influential theory focusing on the emergence and content of autobiographical memory is the situationally bound. Autonomy becomes secondary, whereas relationships with others are emphasized,
social cultural developmental theory (Nelson & Fivush, 2004), which emphasizes the role of society and being ends in themselves.
culture in shaping people’s memories of their autobiographical past.

In essence, autobiographical memory is about defining the self in time and in relation to others, so that Thus, it is crucial to be aware of other people’s desires, needs, and goals and to work towards them to help
individuals gain a sense of who they are by relating to others within a culture and creating a shared past. the other, even read their minds (Mark & Kitayama, 1991). For more individualistic societies, independent
This theory views autobiographical memory as a function of various socio-cultural factors that interact with construals are the norm: The distinctiveness of people, the uniqueness of a person, autonomy, and
basic memory systems, such as the acquisition of language, talk with parents, the style of parental talk, independence are emphasized. This requires construing oneself as an individual and speaking one’s mind.
and psychological understanding. Autobiographical memory emerges gradually and is influenced by Social responsiveness is determined by the need to assert and express the self, and thus the independent
cognitive developments and social interactions, thus becoming a social-cultural-cognitive system. self exercises control to the exterior. The consequence is that larger, more inclusive but superficial in-
groups are the norm, as opposed to the small, selective in-groups of the interdependent self construals
One area that has received attention from researchers is the influence of cultural differences in (Mark & Kitayama, 1991).
collectivism/individualism on autobiographical memory. This introduction presents the definitions for
collectivism and individualism and some of the studies that illustrate the particularities of collectivistic
societies and individuals, as well as those of individualistic societies and individuals. The influence of the Socio-Cultural Influences on Memory
socio-cultural background on people’s autobiographical memory is then depicted in studies that show the According to the socio-cultural developmental theory, socio-cultural influences can be seen both in the
influence of the linguistic and cultural environment on children’s autobiographical memories in Western formation and content of autobiographical memories (Nelson & Fivush, 2004). Research has examined this
(e.g., American) and Eastern (e.g., Chinese) cultural backgrounds. in several ways. One line of research has looked at cultural differences in autobiographical memory by
comparing Caucasian Americans with various Asian ethnicities (e.g., Korean, Chinese, and Japanese). An
Some studies look at the conversational style between mother-children dyads of different ethnicities, and analysis of conversations about reminiscing about one’s experiencess in Caucasian mother-child dyads
others are more specifically focused on autobiographical remembering and look at the content and and Korean mother-child dyads (the children’s age ranged between 3-4 years of age) revealed that
characteristics of the children’s and adult’s autobiographical memories. Studies focusing on bicultural Caucasian dyads talked on average as much as three times more than the Koreans dyads (Mullen &
individuals will be examined to show that these individuals integrate the norms of both cultures and apply Soonhyung, 1995).
them accordingly. Finally, the phenomenon of imagination inflation is discussed and evidence towards it is
brought through studies that show its effect on past events and future expectations, for both children and
adults. Moreover, it is noted that imagination can also create false memories, particularly if the events In addition, Caucasian mothers talked more during their turns and were more likely to portray the child as
imagined are highly plausible or if the participants are children the protagonist in the talk, and to emphasize the child’s and others’ feelings and thoughts, whereas Korean
mothers focused on norms, social roles, and emphasized behavioral expectations. This suggests that
Differences Between Individualistic and Collectivistic Cultures children’s linguistic experiences are related to the development of autobiographical memories, and that the
latter are culturally modeled (Mullen & Soonhyung, 1995).
One important dimension of culture is the extent of individualism or collectivism exhibited (Desai, 2007).
Collectivism puts an emphasis on distinguishing between in-groups and out-groups, engaging in
Another study that suggests the influence of the socio-linguistic environment on autobiographical memory
cooperative tasks, and focusing on what people have in common. Conversely, individualism is
at the early stages of childhood looked at conversations about children’s past experiences between
characterized by engagement in competitive tasks, by public situations, and by an emphasis on what
American mother-child dyads and Chinese mother-child dyads (the children were 3 years old). The
makes the individual distinct. In general, in societies in which agreeing on social norms is important and
analysis of these conversations revealed that American mother-child dyads had an elaborative,
independently oriented conversational style in which the focus was on the child’s predilections and When considering the influence of culture on autobiographical memory, it is important to realize that people
opinions, whereas Chinese mother-child dyads had a low-elaborative, interdependently oriented can internalize more than one culture, in equal measure, so as to form a bicultural identity (Devos, 2006).
conversational style in which the mother repeated factual questions and emphasized moral rules and For example, young adults in the United States in a Chinese family might be competitive and expose their
behavioral expectations (Wang et al., 2000). achievements in the society at large, but inside their community and/or family, they will be respectful to
their elders and try to blend in. Studies have primed bicultural individuals with one cultural identity or
another, in order to see how that influences their behavior and cognition (Hong, Ip, Chiu, Morris & Menon,
Part II 2001; Wang, 2008). In one such study, Chinese Americans were primed with their Chinese cultural identity,
by being given collective, Chinese-related statements. Results showed that the participants became more
These results show that parent-child talk focuses on what types of events are considered memorable, on aware of their duties (e.g. “I ought to understand Chinese history,” “We have to pay taxes”).
what aspects of those events are more important, on how to organize events in a temporal fashion, and on
how to make inferences about people and causality. All these differ according to the values of a specific
culture (Mullen & Soonhyung, 1995; Wang et al., 2000). Child talk appears to be more valued in Western However, Chinese Americans primed with their American cultural identity, by being given individualistic,
societies, where children are encouraged to talk more about their experiences and talk more about American-related statements, they tended to become slightly more aware of their rights (e.g. “I can vote
themselves (Mullen & Soonhyung, 1995). when I’m 18”) (Hong et al., 2001). Moreover, when Asian Americans were primed with their American self
before recalling important autobiographical events, they were more likely to recall personal experiences in
which they were the protagonists and they tended to emphasize their own perspective (e.g., “I got the
Other research also showing how the socio-linguistic environment in which children grow shapes their acceptance letter for Cornell. I did not like my high school at the time and most of the people in, so this was
autobiographical memories has focused on memory specificity and the amount of detail found in young very good news for me. […] I was getting out of town”) (Wang, 2008). However, when Asian Americans
children’s memory reports. When American children (4 and 6 year old) were interviewed about a story were primed with their Asian self before recalling important autobiographical events, their recollections
presented to them a day before, they gave more voluminous and elaborate accounts for both their own were more likely to focus on social interactions, and persons from in-groups (e.g., “The day I got my letter
experiences and for the story than did Korean children (Han, Leichtman & Wang, 1998). In addition, of acceptance to Cornell gave me a sense of relief. […] So it’s not the fact of accomplishing that makes my
American children were more specific and descriptive about specific past events than both Korean and parents happy […]. It’s the ability to plan”). These results point to an influence of the cultural dimension of
Chinese children, and the American children referred to emotions more and categorized negative collectivism/individualism on the mechanism of retrieval (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Wang, 2008).
emotions, whereas Asian children tried to emphasize the positive aspect of an event and talked more
about other people than American children did.
Thus, previous research provides strong evidence that the cultural dimension of collectivism/individualism
influences autobiographical memories, starting with their formation in early childhood and continuing its
This suggests that the content of memory differs with the cultural background of the individual (Han et al., influence into adulthood, when it influences the retrieval of autobiographical memories.
1998). Research involving preschoolers describing autobiographical events shows that American children’s
memories are generally focused on specific events, individual experiences, and feelings (Wang, 2004). In
addition, American children’s memories tend to be expressive, detailed and lengthy, and they focus on the Imagination Inflation
child as being the protagonist in the narrative and present the child in a positive light. In contrast, Chinese In autobiographical memories, connections between self and the past are made. The self, as a personality,
children’s memories were found to be general, skeletal, less emotional, more neutral in their expression, is considered as having three levels: traits, characteristic adaptations (cognitive, motivational,
and focused on routine events, on collective activities, on social interactions, on others or relations with developmental components), and a life story (McLean & Fournier, 2008). But how do we know that these
others. memories that compose our life story are actually events that we experienced? Was it really me that had a
costume party for my 10th birthday? Did I really go the museum on a class trip as I think I went to? How do
people differentiate between events that really happened and events that they only thought about, inferred,
These patterns are seen because Western cultures promote autonomy and put an emphasis on the or imagined?
individual’s qualities, and children in these cultures are encouraged to stand out and talk about themselves,
whereas Eastern cultures promote cohesiveness and put an emphasis on the group, and children in these
cultures are discouraged to talk about themselves and the past and focus more on those around them Research shows that memories of experienced events generally have more sensory and perceptual details
(Han et al., 1998). than memories for events that did not really occur but were products of the imagination (Sporer &
Sharman, 2006). Such qualitative details allow people to differentiate between memories of events that
actually happened versus those that they only imagined happening (Johnson, Hashtroudi & Lindsay,
Cultural influences on memory persist into adulthood. In one study, American and Chinese college 1993), and apparently people use the same criterion to judge the life-story narratives of other people
students were asked to recollect early childhood experiences, and they showed the same biases as (Sporer & Sharman, 2006).
American and Chinese preschoolers (Wang, 2001). In the study, the American and Chinese college
students were asked to recollect their earliest childhood memories and provided self-descriptions. Results
indicated that the American participant’s earliest childhood memories were from around the age of 3.5 Nonetheless, imagining events that never happened can have consequences, as the phenomenon of
years, whereas the Chinese participant’s earliest childhood memories were dating from approximately 4.1 imagination inflation shows. Imagination inflation refers to an increase in confidence that a fictional event
years of age. that was imagined actually happened (Garry, Manning, Loftus & Sherman, 1996; Garry, Sharman, Wade,
Hunt & Smith, 2001). The classic way of testing imagination inflation is with the Life Events Inventory (LEI),
and the methodology consists of three steps. First, participants complete an LEI consisting of a long list of
In addition, American college students’ memories were discrete, focused on specific events, and the
individual’s feelings, whereas Chinese college students’ memories were more general, about routine possible childhood events that they rate their confidence that each event has or has not happened
activities, and focusing on family and in-groups. Americans also stressed personal preferences and
autonomy in lengthier narratives than the ones reported by the Chinese.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen