Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Psychiatry, Psychology and Law

Vol. 19, No. 2, April 2012, 209–220

Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients: A Survey of Australian


Psychologists about the Considerations that Influence Their Decisions
Rony E. Duncana,b,c, Ben J. Williamsc and Ann Knowlesc
a
Centre for Adolescent Health, Children’s Bioethics Centre, Bruce Lefroy Centre for Genetic Health
Research, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Flemington Rd, Parkville, Victoria, 3052, Australia;
b
Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Victoria, 3052, Australia; cPsychological
Sciences and Statistics, Faculty of Life and Social Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology,
Hawthorn, Victoria, 3122, Australia

Confidentiality is vital for building effective therapeutic alliances with clients, yet
determining when to breach confidentiality to prevent harm can be challenging. This is
especially true when clients are minors, as the primary concern often entails preventing
harm to the young person, as opposed to others. The current study sought to explore the
considerations that Australian psychologists take into account when making decisions
about breaching confidentiality with adolescents. Two hundred sixty-four psychologists
responded to an online survey and rated the importance of 13 considerations. Participants
were also able to list additional considerations. Factor analysis indicated that four
underlying constructs influence psychologists’ decisions: (1) the negative nature of the
behaviour; (2) maintaining the therapeutic relationship; (3) the dangerousness of the risk-
behaviour; and (4) legal protection. Qualitative analysis of the additional considerations
uncovered a range of complex and often competing priorities that are also utilised when
making decisions about confidentiality with adolescent clients.
Key words: adolescent; confidentiality; ethics; minors; psychiatry; psychology.

Introduction 2006; McSherry, 2001). Ethical codes


The importance of confidentiality for have provided professional guidance re-
psychological practice is well established garding confidentiality for many years
(Gustafson & McNamara, 1987; Isaacs & (American Psychological Association,
Stone, 1999; Kobocow, McGuire, & Blau, 2002; Australian Psychological Society,
1983; McCurdy & Murray, 2003; Sealan- 2008; British Psychological Society, 2006).
der, Schwiebert, Oren, & Weekley, 1999; These codes emphasise the importance
Sharkin, 1995). The ethical bases for of confidentiality but also note that it is
confidentiality have also been well de- not absolute. Circumstances under which
scribed and include respect for autonomy, psychologists are permitted or required to
the principle of beneficence, consequential- disclose confidential information include
ist arguments and deontological frame- situations in which the client (or the client’s
works (Beauchamp & Childress, 2008; legal guardian) has provided consent,
Kampf & McSherry, 2006; McMahon, where there is a legal obligation to do so,

Correspondence: Rony Duncan, Centre for Adolescent Health, 2 Gatehouse St, Parkville, 3052,
Victoria, Australia. Email: rony.duncan@mcri.edu.au
ISSN 1321-8719 print/ISSN 1934-1687 online
Ó 2012 The Australian and New Zealand Association of Psychiatry, Psychology and Law
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13218719.2011.561759
http://www.tandfonline.com
210 R.E. Duncan et al.

where there is an immediate risk of harm The APS has recently published a
that can only be averted by disclosing detailed set of guidelines for working with
information, or when colleagues or super- young people (Australian Psychological
visors need to be consulted (Australian Society, 2009). In relation to confidential-
Psychological Society, 2008). ity, these guidelines refer to the Code of
Despite the availability of guidance Ethics, section A.5.1, in re-stating that
about and support for confidentiality, ethical there is an obligation to ‘safeguard the
dilemmas concerning confidentiality remain confidentiality of information obtained’
widespread (Bourke & Wessely, 2008; during psychological consultations
Younggren & Harris, 2008). Research in- (p. 185). The guidelines add, in section
dicates that psychologists are uncertain and 5.1.4 relating to limits to confidentiality, ‘in
confused about when to breach confidenti- those unusual circumstances where failure
ality (Kampf, McSherry, Thomas, & Abra- to disclose a young person’s information
hams, 2008). It has also been argued that may result in clear risk to the young person
laws concerning confidentiality are overly or to others, a psychologist may disclose
complex and that more detailed guidelines information necessary to avert risk’
and better training in ethics are required (p. 185). The guidelines are also clear about
(Kampf et al., 2008). A key focus of past placing the best interests of young people
literature about breaching confidentiality first, noting that when conflicts arise
has been the notion of the ‘dangerous client’ between parents and young people, psy-
who poses a risk to others. These discussions chologists should ‘consider the young
typically revolve around clients who are person’s best interests as paramount’
adults (Kampf & McSherry, 2006; Kampf (p. 182).
et al., 2008; McSherry, 2001, 2008). In these
cases a decision about breaching confidenti-
ality generally entails a determination of the Young People
degree of risk to others (immediacy and Young people differ from adults in their
severity) and the public interest in preventing cognitive, emotional and social capabilities
this. (Hazen, Schlozman, & Beresin, 2008), as
When clients are minors, the considera- well as their legal status (Isaacs & Stone,
tions relevant to decisions about confiden- 1999; Lawrence & Kurpius, 2000; McCur-
tiality are different from those concerning dy & Murray, 2003; Mitchell, Disque, &
adults. With young people, concerns about Robertson, 2002; Sealander et al., 1999;
maintaining confidentiality often focus on Sobocinski, 1990). Nevertheless, past re-
the risk that young people pose to them- search has suggested that young people are
selves and their ability, or competence, to generally able to make competent, adult-
understand the consequences of their ac- like decisions from the age of 14–15 years
tions. Thus, dilemmas about confidentiality (Belter & Grisso, 1984; Grisso & Vierling,
with minors often entail a decision about 1978; Piaget, 1953; Weithorn & Campbell,
whether or not to inform parents about 1982). More recently, studies using tech-
risk behaviour. For these reasons, when nologies such as magnetic resonance ima-
psychological clients are minors, the com- ging (MRI) have demonstrated that the
plexity regarding confidentiality is in- human brain continues to develop well into
creased (Davis & Mickelson, 1994; the third decade of life (Giedd, 2008). This
Gustafson & McNamara, 1987; Isaacs & has implications for understanding young
Stone, 1999; Kaczmarek, 2000; Ledyard, people’s cognitive capacities and behaviour
1998; Myers, 1982; Taylor & Adelman, (McAnarney, 2008; Spear, 2000; White,
1989). 2009), although the precise way in which
Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients 211

current understanding should be revised is which they would breach confidentiality


yet to be clearly articulated (Giedd, 2008). with clients who were minors. The majority
As well as physiological maturation, of counsellors reported that they would
young people undergo a range of social and breach confidentiality for the following
emotional changes during adolescence as issues: impending suicide, planned retalia-
they grapple with identity formation issues. tion for victimisation (shooting a fellow
Experiences that are generally specific to student), use of crack cocaine, sex with
adolescence include, but are not limited to: multiple partners when HIV positive,
changes in the child-parent relationship, armed robbery, indications of depression,
the increasing influence of peers, greater abortion and marijuana use. Davis and
desires for autonomy and independence, Mickelson (1994) also surveyed school
involvement in romantic relationships and counsellors. They found that there was
engagement in risk-taking behaviours less than 50% agreement on the preferred
(Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Metzger, ethical or correct legal choices in relation to
2006). Accidents and injuries (both self- dilemmas about student privacy, confiden-
inflicted and unintentional) and behaviour- tiality and parental rights. Thus, although
al problems, such as substance use and a small body of empirical research exists
unsafe sexual experimentation account regarding the importance of confidentiality
for most of the morbidity and mortality with adolescents and the types of circum-
in the adolescent population (Viner & stances in which psychologists may choose
Booy, 2005). to breach confidentiality, little is known
about the decision-making process that
psychologists employ when making these
Young People and Confidentiality: decisions.
Empirical Research The current study replicated a study
A small body of empirical research has of American paediatric psychologists
looked at confidentiality with adolescents (Sullivan, Ramirez, Rae, Razo, & George,
in psychological practice. Kobocow et al. 2002). Participants in the American study
(1983) measured the effects of different (N ¼ 74) were presented with 13 items and
types of assurances about confidentiality were asked to rate the importance of each
on self-disclosure in adolescents and found item for their decisions about breaching
that the number of disclosures for sensitive confidentiality to report adolescent risk-
questions were higher when confidentiality taking behaviour to parents. The items
was explicitly assured. Research in medical covered a range of considerations including
settings also demonstrates that young the frequency, intensity and duration of the
people’s desires to disclose sensitive infor- risk-behaviour, upholding the law, not
mation is hampered when confidentiality disrupting the process of therapy and the
is not assured (Society for Adolescent potential for the risk-taking behaviour to
Medicine, 2004). stop without a breach of confidentiality.
Collins and Knowles (1995) surveyed Factor analysis of these responses provided
adolescents between the ages of 13 and 18 support for a two-factor model to fit the
years about confidentiality in the school data. These two factors were termed
counselling setting. They found that 98% ‘Negative Nature of the Behaviour’ and
of these young people agreed that con- ‘Maintaining the Therapeutic Process’. The
fidentiality within a school counselling current study aimed to identify the con-
setting was either essential or important. siderations that Australian psychologists
Isaacs and Stone (1999) surveyed school utilise when making decisions about
counsellors about the circumstances in breaching confidentiality with adolescent
212 R.E. Duncan et al.

clients to report adolescent risk-taking However, the APS collects data on its
behaviour to parents. membership and the demographics of the
study sample are summarised in Table 1
using the same reporting categories as
Method
the APS. For comparison, APS (2009)
Participants membership statistics are also shown in
A total of 282 people responded to the Table 1.
questionnaire. The target sample was The age distribution in the current
Australian psychologists who had previous study sample was broadly comparable to
experience working with young people. the APS membership profile, although the
Exclusion criteria included: not currently sample’s age profile was younger. This
working in Australia; no previous experi- difference may be due to the method of
ence working with young people; and not data collection (an online questionnaire)
studying to become or currently qualified but equally may represent the character-
as a registered psychologist. Eighteen istics of those who work with adolescents.
participants who did not satisfy these Male psychologists were underrepresented
criteria were excluded, as were participants in the sample. The reason for this is
who missed more than 10% of the ques- unknown, but once again may reflect the
tions. This left a total of 264 participants. characteristics of psychologists who work
Eighty-seven per cent of the participants with adolescents. The geographic distribu-
were female and 13% were male, with a tion of the participants closely matched the
mean age of 39 years (SD ¼ 11). A majority APS membership profile.
of participants were from New South
Wales or Victoria. Participants had a
mean number of 9.5 years’ experience Table 1. Participant demographics and com-
working with young people (SD ¼ 7.8). A parison with Australian Psychological Society
(APS) membership data.
total of 42% had completed a 4-year-
degree plus 2 years of supervision, 49% Study APS
had completed either a Doctorate of sample members*
Psychology or a Masters Degree, 7% had (%) (%)
completed a PhD and the remaining 3% of Age category
participants were still completing their 530 23 12
qualifications. Participants worked in a 30–39 35 26
40–49 20 24
range of settings, with 26% working in 50–59 16 24
schools, 22% in private practice, 11% in 60þ 6 14
the public sector and small numbers work-
Sex
ing for universities, in family therapy Male 13 28
environments, in community settings, for Female 87 72
the government, for the justice system, or State
in a combination of these settings. Eighty- New South Wales 29 32
two per cent of the participants were Victoria 29 34
members of the Australian Psychological Queensland 15 14
South Australia 6 6
Society (APS). Tasmania 5 2
Statistics are not available on the Western Australia 12 8
demographic profile of Australian psychol- Australian Capital 3 3
ogists who work with young people and Territory
Northern Territory 1 1
so there is no definitive basis for determin-
ing the representativeness of this sample. *APS (2009).
Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients 213

Materials the survey and a link to the survey website.


The online questionnaire developed for the This email distribution used the snowbal-
current study was an electronic version of ling technique, in which the researchers
the questionnaire used by Sullivan et al. emailed colleagues who were then asked to
(2002). The questionnaire was in two parts; forward the email to their colleagues and
Part A used a series of vignettes to explore so on. It was not possible to calculate a
situations in which psychologists would response rate for the current study as it is
breach confidentiality with adolescent cli- not known how many people received a
ents and disclose information to parents. notification about the survey. An incentive
Part B focused on the considerations was provided to potential participants in
utilised by psychologists when making the form of a $100 book/CD voucher prize
these decisions. This paper reports results draw. Participants who wanted to go into
from Part B. In Part B, a list of 13 the prize draw were asked to provide a
considerations was presented that might name and telephone number at the end of
influence psychologists’ decisions regarding the questionnaire. These details were sepa-
confidentiality with adolescent clients. rated from other questionnaire responses
Sullivan et al. identified these 13 considera- to maintain anonymity.
tions from a literature review of factors Quantitative responses were imported
that may influence ethical decisions about directly into SPSS for analysis. Basic de-
confidentiality and their own clinical scriptive statistics were calculated for the 13
experience. The resulting list was then considerations that respondents rated on a
reviewed by their colleagues. Participants 5-point Likert scale. Exploratory factor
were asked to rate how important each analysis was then carried out using max-
consideration was for making decisions imum likelihood estimation and oblimin
about breaking an adolescent’s confidenti- rotation in order to explore a factor model
ality and reporting risk-taking behaviours for the 13 considerations that had been
to the parents. They rated each item on a rated. Qualitative responses were analysed
5-point Likert scale from 1 (the considera- using interpretive content analysis (Hansen,
tion was extremely unimportant) to 5 (the 2006). This entailed searching the responses
consideration was extremely important). for considerations that were different from
Participants were then asked if there were those already provided within the pre-
‘any other important considerations you determined list of 13 considerations in the
take into account when making a decision questionnaire until an exhaustive list was
to break confidentiality and report adoles- obtained. This list was then categorised, and
cent risk-taking behaviour to parents’. re-categorised until a final mutually exclu-
sive list was compiled. Qualitative analysis
was conducted independently by RD and
Procedure AK and then compared and discussed until
Following receipt of ethics approval, in- consensus was reached.
vitations to participate were distributed via
two means. First, potential participants
were contacted through the APS newsletter Results
(sent to all APS members fortnightly via Table 2 presents the means and standard
email) and also via a notice on the APS deviations of participants’ responses to the
website notifying them of the survey and 13 considerations they rated on a 5-point
providing a link to the study website. Likert scale. Table 2 also includes compar-
Second, participants were invited via per- ison data from the previous study by
sonal emails which included details about Sullivan et al. (2002).
214 R.E. Duncan et al.

Table 2. Australian and American psychologists’ ratings of the relative importance of 13


considerations for decisions about confidentiality with adolescent clients (on a 5-point Likert scale
where 1 indicated ‘extremely unimportant’ and 5 indicated ‘extremely important’).

Australian American
psychologists* psychologists**
Consideration M SD M SD
Intensity of the risk-taking behaviour 4.64 0.56 4.61 0.82
Apparent seriousness of risk-taking behaviour 4.62 0.63 4.61 0.82
Protecting the adolescent 4.53 0.79 4.66 0.76
Frequency of the risk-taking behaviour 4.39 0.81 4.42 0.81
Duration of the risk-taking behaviour 4.32 0.80 4.42 0.88
Confidence that the risk-taking behaviour has actually 3.86 1.00 4.37 0.81
occurred
Potential for the risk-taking behaviour to stop without 3.86 1.00 3.92 1.00
telling parents
Upholding the law 3.44 1.13 3.59 1.32
The negative effects of reporting on the family 3.31 1.13 3.39 1.23
Not disrupting the process of therapy 3.25 0.97 3.31 1.18
Likelihood that the family will continue treatment after 3.15 1.16 3.08 1.20
breaking confidentiality
Avoiding legal problems for the adolescent 2.91 1.04 3.05 1.23
Gender of the client 1.68 0.99 1.86 1.13

*N ¼ 264, **N ¼ 74.

The most important consideration for p 5 .001). The rank ordering of importance
Australian participants’ decisions about of the 13 considerations was identical
confidentiality with adolescents was the across the two samples with the sole
intensity of the risk-taking behaviour, fol- exception that the American participants
lowed closely by the apparent seriousness of rated protecting the adolescent as most
the behaviour and a desire to protect the important (this was rated as third most
adolescent. On average these items were important by the Australian participants),
rated as important to extremely important. and this by a negligible margin.
The least important consideration for To compare the structure of Australian
participants’ decisions about confidential- participants’ responses with those of Sulli-
ity was the gender of the client which, van et al. (2002), the factor model implied
on average, was rated unimportant to by Sullivan et al.’s exploratory factor
extremely unimportant. As can be seen in analysis was applied to our data using a
Table 2 the Australian participants’ ratings confirmatory factor analysis, omitting vari-
were practically identical to those of the ables which did not load on either factor in
sample from the United States reported by Sullivan et al.’s original analysis. This was
Sullivan et al. (2002). Independent samples’ done using AMOS version 18. This in-
t-tests showed that Australian and Amer- dicated that the model was not a good fit
ican ratings differed significantly on only with our data (GFI ¼ .93, AGFI ¼ .88,
one item; confidence that the risk-taking PGFI ¼ .54, RMSEA ¼ .09).
behaviour has actually occurred, which Since Sullivan et al.’s (2002) model did
was slightly less important to Australian not fit our data, an exploratory factor
participants, and significant even when analysis of the 13 items was conducted,
the Bonferroni adjustment for multiple beginning with a principal components
comparisons was made (t(336) ¼ 4.03, analysis (PCA) using Varimax rotation.
Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients 215

The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin’s measure of sam- after the deletion of the two items. This
pling adequacy indicated a reasonable solution is presented in Table 3.
amount of shared variance between the Together, these four factors explained
variables (KMO ¼ .76). Bartlett’s test of 47.38% of the variance. After considering
sphericity was significant (w2(78) ¼ 752.00, which items loaded onto each of the four
p 5 .01). The maximum correlation be- factors, Factor 1 was labelled ‘Negative
tween items was .66 indicating collinearity Nature of the Behaviour’, Factor 2 was
problems would be unlikely. Taken labelled ‘Maintaining the Therapeutic Pro-
together, these statistics indicated the data cess’, Factor 3 was labelled ‘Dangerousness
were suitable for factor analysis. of the Risk-Behaviour’ and Factor 4 was
Results confirmed that a four-factor tentatively labelled ‘Legal Protection’. This
model was appropriate for these data with factor has only two items significantly
four eigenvalues 4 1, a scree plot sugges- loading on it; however, it suggests that
tive of four factors, Horn’s (1965) parallel legal considerations may form a distinct
analysis indicating a four factor model and dimension of the therapist’s deliberations.
the goodness-of-fit test indicating an ex- Participants were also able to list
cellent fit (w2(17) ¼ 10.25, p ¼ .89). Oblimin additional important considerations that
rotation produced a slightly cleaner struc- were not included in the list of 13 provided
ture than varimax. Two of the items in the questionnaire. A total of 167
(confidence that the risk-taking behaviour participants each provided a written re-
has actually occurred and gender of the sponse and 17 distinct considerations were
client) did not load significantly on any of identified that were different from the list
the factors and so were removed, and the of 13 pre-determined considerations pro-
remaining items reanalysed. The overall vided within the questionnaire. Table 4
pattern of loadings remained unchanged presents these additional considerations.

Table 3. Factor solutions for understanding Australian psychologists’ decisions about confidenti-
ality with adolescents, compared with American psychologists’ decisions.

Australian American
psychologists* psychologists**
factor factor
Item 1 2 3 4 1 2
Negative effects of reporting on the family .00 .43 .07 .07 .02 .73
Avoiding legal problems for the adolescent .02 .36 7.02 .31 .28 .38
Not disrupting the process of therapy 7.01 .74 7.04 7.08 7.05 .93
Potential for the risk-taking behaviour to stop .18 .55 .02 7.33 .36 .46
without telling parents
Likelihood that family will continue treatment 7.02 .75 7.03 .05 .10 .61
after breaking confidentiality
Frequency of the risk-taking behaviour .83 .03 7.01 .03 .70 .22
Duration of the risk-taking behaviour .76 .05 7.04 .16 .78 .10
Intensity of the risk-taking behaviour .58 7.04 .42 7.12 .86 7.01
Apparent seriousness of the behaviour .05 7.04 .69 7.07 .79 .18
Protecting the adolescent 7.04 .16 .47 .39 .40 7.06
Upholding the law .16 7.04 .01 .43 .03 7.18
Eigenvalue 3.22 1.82 1.22 1.00 3.84 2.25
% variance explained 24.63 12.68 5.38 4.69 22.24 17.96

Note: Items in bold indicate loading of the item on the specified factor. *N ¼ 264, **N ¼ 74.
216 R.E. Duncan et al.

Table 4. Additional considerations used by Other considerations included taking into


Australian psychologists when contemplating a account whether or not the adolescent was
breach of confidentiality with adolescent clients.
likely to inform his/her parents without
Consideration N %* a breach and also the competence, age,
previous history, protective factors and
How would the family respond if 43 27.8
told about their child’s risk best interests of the young person.
behaviour?
Will the adolescent talk to his/her 25 15.0
parents if I do not breach Discussion
confidentiality?
Does the adolescent understand 17 10.2 The current study surveyed Australian
why a breach is necessary? psychologists about the considerations
How competent is the young 16 9.6 they utilise when making decisions about
person? breaching confidentiality with adolescents.
What protective factors does the 11 6.6
young person have in his/her The study replicated a previous study of
life? American psychologists who worked with
How old is the young person? 12 7.2 minors. When asked to rate the importance
What is in the best interests of the 9 5.4
young person?
of 13 considerations for determining
Did I provide an initial 9 5.4 whether to inform parents about risk-
explanation about limits to behaviours in their children, participants
confidentiality? indicated that their most important con-
What is the policy of my 8 4.8
workplace? sideration was the intensity of the risk-
What sort of rapport do I have 7 4.2 behaviour. This was closely followed by the
with the young person? seriousness of the risk-behaviour, a desire to
What potential harm could come 4 2.4 protect the adolescent, the frequency of the
to the young person if I do
breach confidentiality? risk-behaviour and the duration of the risk-
What is the level of previous 4 2.4 behaviour. The least important considera-
parental knowledge about the tion was the gender of the client. These
child’s behaviour? responses were extremely similar to those
What is my duty of care to each 3 1.8
individual involved of Sullivan et al.’s American study (2002).
Is family therapy an option? 2 1.2 The order in which the 13 considerations
Is there a family history of risk 2 1.2 were ranked was also almost identical
behaviour and/or suicide? between Australian and American psychol-
What is the previous history of the 2 1.2
young person? ogists, with the only difference being
Is this a well thought-out decision? 1 0.6 placement of protecting the adolescent,
which was rated as slightly more important
N ¼ 167. by American psychologists. Given the time
*Respondents often described multiple considerations difference in administration of the two
in their written responses. surveys, and the sample size differences,
these results suggest a strong and continu-
Sullivan et al. (2002) did not ask partici- ing correspondence between the two sam-
pants if there were any additional impor- ples of psychologists.
tant considerations and so comparison is Factor analysis of ratings for the 13
not possible for this section of the considerations produced four meaningful
questionnaire. factors. These represent the underlying
The most frequently cited additional constructs that Australian psychologists
consideration entailed thinking about how take into account when making decisions
the family would respond if informed about breaching confidentiality with
about their child’s risk-taking behaviour. adolescent clients. The four factors were:
Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients 217

(1) the negative nature of the behaviour; (2) behaviour to stop without a breach of
maintaining the therapeutic relationship; confidentiality. Reflecting on the possibility
(3) dangerousness of the risk-behaviour; of legal problems is also part of this
and (4) legal protection. consideration. This factor, once again,
The first factor, negative nature of the closely mirrors the second factor identified
behaviour, encompasses a consideration by Sullivan et al. (2002), also termed
about how severe the risk-taking behaviour maintaining the therapeutic relationship.
is (i.e., how frequent and how intense) and Sullivan et al. highlight the importance of
the potential for negative consequences. attempting to maintain the therapeutic
Thus, a key consideration for Australian relationship even when a breach of con-
psychologists when making decisions about fidentiality is necessary. They stated this
breaching confidentiality with adolescents requires open and honest communication
is how serious the potential for harm is if a from the beginning of therapy, in order to
breach of confidentiality does not occur. minimise the possibility of the breach
This first factor closely reflects the first having a lasting negative impact on the
factor identified by Sullivan et al. (2002) young person; particularly in relation to
which they also termed negative nature of interactions with other health professionals
the behaviour. Sullivan et al. noted that in in the future. The current study lends
order for psychologists to consider the support to this assertion, highlighting that
severity of the risk-taking behaviour, a decisions about breaching confidentiality
thorough psychosocial history is required. with adolescents, and the process by which
These Australian findings add strength to breaches occur, have important implica-
this assertion, highlighting the fact that tions for young people’s engagement in
when psychologists are faced with difficult therapy both now and in the future.
ethical dilemmas about breaching confiden- The third factor, dangerousness of the
tiality with adolescents, detailed information risk-behaviour, encompasses consideration
about the nature of the behaviour and how of the intensity and seriousness of the
this fits within the young person’s wider behaviour, combined with a desire to
psychosocial context is vital. Ethical and protect the adolescent. It provides an
professional practice guidelines would ne- additional layer to Sullivan et al.’s (2002)
cessitate recording of this information also. findings, which only included two mean-
The second factor, maintaining the ingful factors. This factor, although some-
therapeutic relationship, relates to the im- what similar to the first factor (negative
portance of continuing therapy with the nature of the behaviour), seems to reflect
young person, as well as the broader risks consideration about the welfare of the
of breaching confidentiality such as effects young person. That is, their broader best
on the family and potential legal problems interests and a professional obligation to
for the adolescent. Thus, when Australian protect them from harm. The first factor
psychologists contemplate breaching con- differs from this third factor in that it does
fidentiality with adolescent clients, another not incorporate a specific consideration
key consideration is the impact that a about protecting the adolescent. This
breach would have on the therapeutic factor accounted for relatively little var-
relationship. This involves thinking iance, yet the items comprising this factor
through the negative impacts that a breach had high importance ratings and low
of confidentiality may have for the family, standard deviations. It is therefore likely
the likelihood that the family will continue that restriction of range explains the small
therapy if a breach of confidentiality occurs size of this factor and not its psychological
and the potential for the problematic risk importance.
218 R.E. Duncan et al.

The fourth factor, legal protection, represents a consequentialist approach to


again adds to the two-factor solution decision-making, that is, the course of
presented by Sullivan et al. (2002). It action chosen is at least partly dependent
reflects a concern about upholding the on the likely consequences of that action.
law and combines a consideration about Some of the other additional considerations
young people’s safety with the broader also related to the family context, including
legal context. It seems that this factor may the level of previous parental knowledge
relate to legal consequences for the psy- about the young person’s risk behaviour,
chologist (as opposed to the young person), whether family therapy might be an option
as it does not include a desire to avoid legal and the previous family history of risk-
problems for the adolescent (one of the 13 behaviour and suicide.
considerations that does not load onto this Another group of additional considera-
factor). Thus, it appears that this addi- tions focused on specific characteristics of
tional fourth factor reflects a fear about the adolescent. For example, the young
upholding professional legal obligations to person’s age, competence, protective fac-
keep young people safe and perhaps the tors, best interests, previous history and
possibility of adverse legal consequences if likelihood of talking to his/her parents
professionals fail to do this. without a breach of confidentiality. Yet
Qualitative analysis of the list of addi- another group of additional considerations
tional considerations provided by partici- related to the process of therapy and
pants identified 17 new considerations that decision-making. These included consider-
Australian psychologists take into account ing whether an initial explanation about
when making decisions about breaching confidentiality had been provided, thinking
confidentiality with adolescent clients. about whether the adolescent understood
These were not contained within the pre- the need for a breach of confidentiality and
determined list of 13 considerations pro- reflecting on whether the decision was a
vided in both the Australian and American well-considered one.
questionnaire. The original American Together, the four factors identified
study by Sullivan et al. (2002) did not through quantitative analysis and the list
include a question that asked participants of additional considerations identified
to list additional considerations. This ex- through qualitative analysis provide a
tensive list of additional considerations detailed and complex picture of the range
provided by Australian participants there- of issues that professionals might reflect
fore represents a significant finding that upon when contemplating a breach of
extends the original American study. It confidentiality with adolescent clients.
highlights the wide range of considerations Not only are they many and varied, but
that are utilised by psychologists when they have the potential to compete with
attempting to make decisions about con- one another at times. Thus, weighing up
fidentiality with adolescents, over and the degree of harm associated with a risk
above those that professionals might as- behaviour may prompt different action
sume would be key considerations. It also from consideration of the impact of a
draws attention to the high degree of breach of confidentiality on therapy. Simi-
complexity associated with such decisions. larly, thinking about a young person’s age
The most common additional consid- and competence may imply a different path
eration identified by Australian participants from consideration of how the family
was a consideration about how the family might respond if informed of the risk
might respond if told about their child’s behaviour. This highlights the ethical com-
risk behaviour. This is important because it plexity associated with decisions about
Breaching Confidentiality with Adolescent Clients 219

confidentiality with adolescents; emphasis- Australian Psychological Society. (2008). Ethi-


ing the challenging nature of such decisions cal guidelines: Complementing the APS code
of ethics (8th ed.). Melbourne: The Austra-
and stressing the importance of support, lian Psychological Society Ltd.
training and guidance for professionals Beauchamp, T.L., & Childress, J.F. (Eds.).
working in this area. (2008). Principles of biomedical ethics.
6th ed. New York: Oxford University Press.
Belter, R.W., & Grisso, T. (1984). Children’s
Conclusions recognition of rights violations in counsel-
ing. Professional Psychology: Research and
Determining whether or not to breach Practice, 15, 899–910.
confidentiality with a client in order to Bourke, J., & Wessely, S. (2008). Confidenti-
prevent harm can be highly challenging. ality. British Medical Journal, 336, 888–891.
This is especially true when clients are British Psychological Society. (2006). Code of
ethics and conduct. Leicester, UK: The
minors as their capacity for autonomous British Psychological Society.
and competent decision-making differs Collins, N., & Knowles, A.D. (1995). Adoles-
from that of adults and the potential for cents’ attitudes toward confidentiality be-
self-harm (both intentional and uninten- tween the school counsellor and the
tional) is significant. The current survey of adolescent client. Australian Psychologist,
30, 179–182.
Australian psychologists indicates that Davis, J.L., & Mickelson, D.J. (1994). School
when making decisions about confidential- counselors: Are you aware of ethical and
ity with young people, a wide variety of legal aspects of counseling? The School
considerations are contemplated and re- Counselor, 42, 5–13.
flected upon, many of which might compete Giedd, J. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from
neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health,
with one another. There is a need to ensure 42, 335–343.
that current and future health professionals Grisso, T., & Vierling, L. (1978). Minor’s
are trained in the process of ethical decision- consent to treatment: A developmental
making and the range of considerations that perspective. Professional Psychology, 9,
might be involved in such processes, parti- 412–427.
Gustafson, K.E., & McNamara, J.R. (1987).
cularly when clients are minors. It will also Confidentiality with minor clients: Issues and
be important to provide training on how to guidelines for therapists. Professional Psychol-
enact breaches of confidentiality when ogy: Research and Practice, 18, 503–508.
necessary, while at the same time sustaining Hansen, E.C. (2006). Successful qualitative
the therapeutic alliance. health research. Sydney. Allen and Unwin.
Hazen, E., Schlozman, S., & Beresin, E. (2008).
Adolescent psychological development: A
Acknowledgements review. Pediatric Review, 29, 161–167.
The authors would like to thank Jeremy Sullivan, Horn, J.L. (1965). A rationale and test for
Eleazar Ramirez, William Rae, Nancy Pena Razo the number of factors in factor analysis.
and Carrie George for sharing the questionnaire Psychometrika, 32, 179–185.
used in their previous study of American Psychol- Isaacs, M.L., & Stone, C. (1999). School
ogists. The authors would also like to thank the counselors and confidentiality: Factors af-
Australian Psychological Association for assis- fecting professional choices. Professional
tance in advertising the study and recruiting School Counseling, 2, 258–266.
participants. Dr Duncan is funded through an Kaczmarek, R. (2000). Ethical and legal com-
NHMRC Postdoctoral Training Fellowship. plexities inherent in professional roles with
children and adolescent clients. Counseling
and Human Development, 33, 1–21.
References Kampf, A., & McSherry, B. (2006). Confidenti-
American Psychological Association. (2002). ality in therapeutic relationships: The need
Ethical principles of psychologists and to develop comprehensive guidelines for
code of conduct. American Psychologist, mental health professionals. Psychiatry,
57, 1060–1073. Psychology and Law 13, 124–131.
220 R.E. Duncan et al.

Kampf, A., McSherry, B., Thomas, S., & Sharkin, B.S. (1995). Strains on confidentiality
Abrahams, H. (2008). Psychologists’ per- in college-student psychotherapy: Entangled
ceptions of legal and ethical requirements therapeutic relationships, incidental encoun-
for breaching confidentiality. Australian ters, and third-party inquiries. Professional
Psychologist, 43, 194–204. Psychology Research and Practice, 26,
Kobocow, B., McGuire, J.M., & Blau, B.I. 184–189.
(1983). The influence of confidentiality Smetana, J.G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger,
conditions on self-disclosure of early ado- A. (2006). Adolescent development in inter-
lescents. Professional Psychology Research personal and societal contexts. Annual
and Practice, 14, 435–443. Review of Psychology, 57, 255–284.
Lawrence, G., & Kurpius, S.E. (2000). Legal Sobocinski, M.R. (1990). Ethical principles in
and ethical issues involved when counseling the counseling of gay and lesbian adoles-
minors in nonschool settings. Journal of cents: Issues of autonomy, competence,
Counseling Development, 78, 130–136. and confidentiality. Professional Psychology
Ledyard, P. (1998). Counselling minors: Ethical Research and Practice, 21, 240–247.
and legal issues. Counseling and Values, 42, Society for Adolescent Medicine. (2004). Con-
171–178. fidential health care for adolescents: Posi-
McAnarney, E.R. (2008). Adolescent brain tion paper of the society for adolescent
development: Forging new links? Journal medicine. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35,
of Adolescent Health, 42, 321–323. 160–167.
McCurdy, K.G., & Murray, K.C. (2003). Spear, L.P. (2000). The adolescent brain and
Confidentiality issues when minor children age-related behavioral manifestations. Neu-
disclose family secrets in family counseling. roscience Biobehavior Review, 24, 417–463.
The Family Journal, 11, 393–398. Sullivan, J.R., Ramirez, E., Rae, W.A., Razo,
McMahon, M. (2006). Confidentiality, privacy N.P., & George, C.A. (2002). Factors
and privilege: Protecting and disclosing contributing to breaking confidentiality
information about clients. In S. Morrissey with adolescent clients: A survey of pedia-
& P. Reddy (Eds.), Ethics and professional tric psychologists. Professional Psychology:
practice for psychologists (pp. 74–88). Research and Practice, 33, 396–401.
Melbourne: Thomson Social Science Press. Taylor, L., & Adelman, H.S. (1989). Reframing
McSherry, B. (2001). Confidentiality of psychia- the confidentiality dilemma to work in
tric and psychological communications: children’s best interests. Professional Psy-
The public interest exception. Psychiatry, chology: Research and Practice, 20, 79–83.
Psychology and Law, 8, 12–22. Viner, R., & Booy, R. (2005). ABC of adoles-
McSherry, B. (2008). Health professional- cence. Epidemiology of health and illness.
patient confidentiality: Does the law really British Medical Journal, 330, 411–414.
matter? Journal of Law and Medicine, 15, Weithorn, L.A., & Campbell, S.B. (1982). The
489–493. competency of children and adolescents to
Mitchell, C.W., Disque, J.G., & Robertson, P. make informed treatment decisions. Child
(2002). When parents want to know: Re- Development, 53, 1589–1599.
sponding to parental demands for confiden- White, A.M. (2009). Understanding adolescent
tial information. Professional School brain development and its implications for
Counseling, 6, 156–161. the clinician. Adolescent Medicine State of
Myers, J.E. (1982). Legal issues surrounding the Art Reviews, 20, 73–90.
psychotherapy with minor clients. Clinical Younggren, J.N., & Harris, E.A. (2008). Can
Social Work Journal, 10, 303–314. you keep a secret? Confidentiality in psy-
Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the chotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. 64, 589–600.
Sealander, K.A., Schwiebert, V.L., Oren, T.A.,
& Weekley, J.L. (1999). Confidentiality and
the law. Professional School Counseling, 3,
122–127.
Copyright of Psychiatry, Psychology & Law is the property of Routledge and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen