Sie sind auf Seite 1von 3

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 19911 249 645-647 NUMBER4 (wiNTEEL 1991)

AVOIDING THE COUNTERCONTROL OF


APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS
L. KEmI MtnX
UNIVERSITY OF KANSAS

Steve Fawcett (1991) has performed an impor- ysis of behavior. We have adopted much of our
tant service for the field ofapplied behavior analysis. approach from basic researchers. Yet behavior an-
His recommendations provide us with practical alysts conducting basic research often create aversive
methods for avoiding countercontrol of our prac- situations for their subjects. They endose their non-
tices by normal adults. Following his recommen- human subjects in experimental chambers and
dations may accelerate the acceptance of our prac- thereby prevent deprivation, low reinforcer density,
tices by normal adults in community settings. and aversive stimulation from evoking avoidance
Following them may also solve some perplexing and escape from the situation and aggressive re-
problems in the maintenance and dissemination of sponses toward the experimenter. The longstanding
our practices with dependent populations in insti- tendency for applied behavior analysts to select
tutional settings. human subjects who have been legally placed in
Skinner has suggested that attempts by one per- restrictive institutions that minimize these normal
son to control another may evoke countercontrol accompaniments of aversive stimulation mirrors this
(Skinner, 1953). He predicted that aversive, ex- practice. It also suggests that applied behavior an-
ploitative, or even deliberate control would evoke alysts, too, may create aversive situations.
countercontrol. He argued that countercontrol is Selecting legally dependent subjects for inter-
often a beneficial if crude step in the design of an vention has been a successful strategy for developing
effective culture (Skinner, 1973). The balance be- an applied methodology. The strategy has mini-
tween control and countercontrol may achieve a mized the size of the step from basic to applied
temporary equilibrium, but the resulting culture research because of the similarity of nonhumans-
may not take the future into account sufficiently to in-a-chamber to humans-in-an-institution. Conse-
have survival value (Skinner, 1978, pp. 16-32). quently, we have been able to develop a meth-
Skinner has consistendy recommended that we odology appropriate to applied problems. We have
build our interventions around positive reinforce- learned how to gain access to settings. We have
ment partly to avoid countercontrol (e.g., Skinner, learned how to discover and observe behavior with
1978, pp. 3-16). That may be easier said than applied significance. We have developed appro-
done. Anyone who has worked with normal adults priate experimental designs, intervention strategies,
knows how easily evoked is their countercontrolling and methods of analysis.
repertoire. One probable reason for this sensitivity However, that said, we should examine the price
is that attempts to control others in order to gain paid for this approach. Some of the most perplexing
an advantage are so pervasive in our culture. How- problems encountered by applied behavior analysis
ever, another reason may lie in our own approach stem from our failure to learn how to work with
to designing interventions. normal adults. Specifically, we have not learned
Our approach to designing interventions owes how to develop interventions that rely on positive
something to the history of the experimental anal- reinforcement for all participants, induding the nor-
mal adults who implement them. In short, we have
Address correspondence to L. Keith Miller, Department
not learned how to avoid evoking countercontrol.
of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, One perplexing problem is the failure of the
Kansas 66045. normal adults who staff and administer settings of
645
646 L. KEITH MILLER

interest to maintain interventions in our absence. the nation's cultural practices! Surely we need an
The behavior analysis literature is studded with approach that is more likely to come to pass. We
cases of these adults abandoning our interventions need to add to or even change some of our practices
soon after we terminate their dose supervision (Bas- so that our interventions do not evoke countercon-
sett & Blanchard, 1977; Couch, Miller, Johnson, trol from relevant normal adults.
& Welsh, 1986; Wolf, 1982). It has long been Steve Fawcett may be the applied behavior an-
accepted lore that such abandonment is typical alyst who is most experienced at avoiding coun-
(Malott, 1974). Schwartz and Baer (1991) have tercontrol by normal adults. He has developed suc-
argued that when an intervention is not acceptable cessful behavioral interventions with people from
to its consumers they may engage in a variety of all walks of life. He has collaborated with low-
countercontrolling behaviors including "not imple- income white, black, and Hispanic clients and staff
menting some or all of [a] program's procedures (Fawcett & Miller, 1975). He has collaborated with
after the program consultant leaves" (p. 190). Thus, people having various disabilities but fully capable
the widespread lack of program maintenance may of resisting control (Suarez de Balcazar, Fawcett, &
be an example of countercontrol. Balcazar, 1988). He has collaborated with normal
Another problem is the failure of normal adults householders and sanitation workers (Stokes &
who control the setting of interest to adopt our Fawcett, 1977). And he has collaborated with leg-
interventions in the first place (Lamal, 1986; Stolz, islators (Fawcett, Seekins, & Jason, 1987). That
1981). Hung (1987) has argued that our "pro- he has published elegant experimental analyses with
grams often require an extensive change in life or such a wide range of normal adults testifies to his
work style, vigorous training, continuous monitor- skill at avoiding countercontrol. That countercon-
ing, and they can become aversive since they take trol is readily evoked by such work is attested to
away reinforcing activities afforded by modern life by his several brushes with attempts to stop his
... (p. 13). Thus, not adopting such programs interventions.
may be a response that avoids the aversive features Fawcett has provided us with a highly practical
of the program. It may be an example of coun- guide for avoiding countercontrol. The essence of
tercontrol. his advice is to establish collaborative relationships
Yet another perplexing problem is that we have with the people one seeks to help. He suggests that
done little to implement Skinner's vision of be- asking these people and their advocates social va-
havior analysis as a method for solving community lidity questions is a step in the right direction. But
problems, let alone society's broader ills (e.g., Skin- he advises us to start earlier in the process and get
ner, 1972). Behavior analysis has not been notably their input on how to frame the question and what
prolific in publishing research reporting successful goals they value. He advises us to become their
solutions to problems involving normal adults. Thus, students and learn from them.
even though our interventions are demonstrably Underlying his approach is an axiom from com-
superior to alternatives, normal adults are not clam- munity organizing. The axiom is that the organ-
oring for their use. izer's goal is to work himself or herself out of a
If our failure to develop and disseminate inter- job. In other words, the goal is to develop an
ventions that survive in the real world among nor- organization and a membership with the skills nec-
mal adults reflects countercontrol, how can we avoid essary to run the organization without further help.
countercontrol? One whimsical answer, that may This parallels Skinner's (1978) admonition that an
reflect our roots in the methodology appropriate to intervention "is not finished until [it] works more
nonhumans-in-a-chamber only too well, has been efficiently as a system without further interven-
supplied by Ruben Ardila in Walden Three (1990). tion.... No cultural practice designed through the
His premise is that a military dictator comes to application of an experimental analysis of behavior
power and appoints a behavior analyst to design involves a behavior modifier who remains in con-
AVOIDING COUNTERCONTROL 647

trol" (p. 15). To date we have few, if any, ex- Couch, R. W., Miller, L. K., Johnson, M., & Welsh, T. M.
(1986). Some considerations for behavior analysts de-
emplers of such an intervention. I think that such veloping social change interventions. Behavior Analysis
exemplars will arise when we take Fawcett's advice and Social Action, 5, 9-13.
and collaborate with the staff, administration, and Fawcett, S. B. (1991). Some values guiding community
consumers of our interventions to develop inter- research and action. Journal ofApplied Behavior Anal-
ysis, 24, 621-636.
ventions cooperatively that will not evoke any form Fawcett, S. B., & Miller, L. K. (1975). Training public-
of countercontrol and will instead evoke and re- speaking behavior: An experimental analysis and social
inforce behaviors that maintain effective interven- validation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analsysis, 8,
125-135.
tions. Fawcett, S. B., Seekins, T., & Jason, L. A. (1987). Policy
Failing to evoke countercontrol while evoking research and child passenger safety legislation: A case
such maintenance behaviors is evidence that an in- study and experimental evaluation. Journal of Social
tervention is not aversive and that it leads to re- Issues, 43, 133-148.
Hung, D. W. (1987). A matter of self-reinforcing? ABA
inforcement. Altus, Welsh, and Miller (1991) have Newsletter, 10, 13-14.
recommended that we probe for program main- Lamal, P. A. (1986). On facing reality. ABA Newsletter,
tenance by withdrawing supervision. If the con- 9, 6.
Malott, R. W. (1974). Focus #4. Journal of Applied
sumers maintain the intervention, then this is ev- Behavior Analysis, 7, inside back cover.
idence that countercontrol will not be a problem. Schwartz, I. W., & Baer, D. M. (1991). Social validity
If the intervention is not maintained, supervision assessments: Is current practice state of the art? Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 189-204.
should be reinstated. Further collaboration can then Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New
be pursued until another probe seems justified. York: Macmillan.
Fawcett's recommendations greatly improve the Skinner, B. F. (1972). Beyondfreedom and dignity. New
chance that such collaboration will be fruitful. York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Skinner, B. F. (1973). Answers for my critics. In H. Wheel-
Fawcett labels his recommendations "values," er (Ed.), Beyond the punitive society (pp. 256-266).
which implies that following them may produce San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
reinforcing consequences (Skinner, 1972). The re- Skinner, B. F. (1978). Reflections on behaviorism and
society. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
inforcing consequences may be that they will enable Stokes, T. F., & Fawcett, S. B. (1977). Evaluating mu-
us to work with normal adults to alter situations nicipal policy: An analysis of a refuse packaging program.
constructively so as to solve important social prob- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 391-398.
Stolz, S. B. (1981). Adoption of innovations from applied
lems. Their independent survival would assure us behavioral research: "Does anybody care?" Journal of
of their validity to the target population. We should Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 491-505.
not be surprised if this is a necessary if not sufficient Suarez de Balcazar, Y. W., Fawcett, S. B., & Balcazar, F. E.
condition for dissemination. (1988). Effects of environmental design and police en-
forcement on violations of a handicapped parking or-
dinance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21,
291-298.
REFERENCES Wolf, M. M. (1982). Program survival: A case study in
the development and maintenance of a behavioral inter-
Altus, D. E., Welsh, T. M., & Miller, L. K. (1991). A vention program. In B. Bolton & R. Roessler (Eds.),
technology for program maintenance: Programming key Proceedings of the Symposium on Applied Research
researcher behaviors in a student housing cooperative. Methodology (pp. 43-49). Fayetteville: University of Ar-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 667-675. kansas Rehabilitation Research and Training Center.
Ardila, R. (1990). Walden three: A scientific utopia. New
York: Carlton Press. Received September 4, 1991
Bassett, J. E., & Blanchard, E. B. (1977). The effect of Final acceptance September 7, 1991
the absence of dose supervision on the use of response
cost in a prison token economy. Journal of Applied Action Editor, R. Mark Mathews
Behavior Analysis, 10, 375-379.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen