Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Sot. Sci. Med. Vol. 18. No. IO, pp. 889-898, 1984 0277-9536/84 S3.W + 0.

00
Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved CopyrIght Q 1984 Pergamon Press Ltd

STRESS AND COPING IN THE EXPLANATION OF


PSYCHOLOGICAL ADJUSTMENT AMONG
CHRONICALLY ILL ADULTS*

BARBARA J. FELTON’, TRACEY A. REVENS~N’ and GREGORY A. HINRICHSEN~


‘Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003,
2University of California-Irvine, Irvine, CA 92717 and ‘Long Island Jewish-Hillside Medical Center, NY,
U.S.A.

Abstract-This study evaluates the utility of a stress and coping paradigm for explaining individual
differences in psychological adjustment to chronic illness. Using data from the first wave of a longitudinal
study of 170 middle-aged and elderly adults faced with one of four chronic illnesses (hypertension, diabetes
mellitus, cancer and rheumatoid arthritis), this paper examines the relationship between the stresses of
chronic illness and coping, and the ability of coping to explain psychological adjustment. Results show
coping strategy use tends to be minimally explained by medical diagnosis. Cognitive strategies, including
information seeking, are related to positive affect while emotional strategies, particularly those involving
avoidance, blame and emotional ventilation, are related to negative affect, lowered self-esteem and poorer
adjustment to illness. While the findings suggest that a stress and coping model may be valuable in
understanding adjustment among the chronically ill, the general modesty of coping effects and the failure
of the stress buffering hypothesis to explain adjustment indicates a need for new research approaches and
some modification of current theories of coping.

Physical health is strongly linked to the emotional vestigating the coping responses made by a normative
well-being of older adults, as documented by numer- sample of adults faced with a variety of self-named
ous studies of mental health in late life [ 1,2]. Declines stressors including health problems, Folkman and
in health, frequently prompted by the onset and Lazarus [5] found the context of the stressful situ-
persistence of chronic illnesses, are a frequently ex- ation and the individual’s appraisal of its amenability
perienced form of stress in middle age and later to personal control to be important predictors of
adulthood, and the impact of such declines on psy- emotion-focused vs problem-focused modes of cop-
chological adjustment may be profound [3]. Individ- ing. Health-related stressors, particularly those ap-
ual differences in reaction exist, however, with some praised as requiring acceptance, were significantly
adults far more distressed by illness than others. The more likely to prompt emotion-focused coping, i.e.
study described here evaluates the utility of a ‘stress strategies directed at reducing the emotional distress
and coping paradigm’ [4] for explaining individual prompted by the problematic situation, rather than
variations in older adults’ psychosocial adjustment to those directed at altering the source of the stress itself.
illness. Similarly, Pearlin and Schooler [6] found situation
Current theories of stress and coping describe modification strategies, those most closely akin to
coping behavior as goal directed and responsive to problem-focused coping, to appear more frequently
stress; such cognitions and behaviors are seen as when the stresses (or, in their terminology, ‘enduring
critical in determining with what success people role strains’) occurred in the areas of marriage or
handle life stresses. Coping is presumably elicited parenting, both close interpersonal situations, than
when the individual cognitively appraises a situation when these strains lay in occupational or household
as posing threat, harm, loss or challenge; appropri- economic situations. More recently, McCrae [7], in
ately selected coping strategies buffer the individual two cross-sectional studies, found that differences in
under stress from emotional distress and mal- the types of stresses faced by adults were responsible
adjustment. for the apparent age differences which appeared in
Lazarus [4] proposes that the particular kinds of adults’ use of different coping strategies.
coping behaviors which people use are determined by A critical assumption of current conceptions of
both personal characteristics and those of their social coping is that coping strategies may have different
environment, and, most importantly, by the nature of consequences when used in response to different types
the stress with which they are contending. In- of stress. Thus, the effectiveness of any given strategy
depends upon its appropriateness for the particular
*This research was supported by the National Institute of stress faced. In Pearlin and Schooler’s study, coping
Mental Health Grant MH 29822. The authors gratefully strategies through which the individual remained
acknowledge the helpful comments made by Drs Stanley ‘committed to and engaged with the relevant others’
Lehmann and Marybeth Shinn on earlier drafts of this were not only more prevalent but were most success-
paper. ful in reducing emotional distress when prompted by

889
890 BARBARA J. FELTON er al.

the stresses of close interpersonal situations; coping particularly the illness’ amenability to medical and
strategies which distanced the individual from the personal control, which prompt the use of particular
problem, on the other hand, were most successful in coping strategies. It describes which coping strategies
dealing with the stresses of more impersonal situ- are most effective in warding off the negative psycho-
ations (p. 18). In addition to demonstrating that the logical consequences of illness.
effectiveness of different coping strategies depends on This study’s examination of coping strategy
the nature of the stress faced, these results demon- effectiveness includes an examination of the stress-
strate that strategies which involve avoidance, dis- buffering hypothesis, or the idea that the relationship
tance and cognitive distortions of the situation may, between stress and psychological adjustment is non-
under some circumstances, be adaptive. Thus these linear when coping responses are taken into account
results suggest that an accurate perception of reality [14]. In its most specific form, the stress-buffering
and direct confrontation with the stressor are not hypothesis maintains that particular levels of stress
always necessary for successful coping [4,8] and may, must be experienced together with an absence of
in fact, be an adaptive approach to uncontrollable coping in order for the negative effects of stress to
stresses. emerge [15] or, expressed differently, that coping will
Studies of adults’ experiences with serious illnesses, be positively and most strongly related to adjustment
mostly anecdotal or case studies of specific illnesses, when stress is severe [16, 171. While empirical studies
have identified coping behaviors presumably re- have documented the presence of direct elects of
sponsive to the particular stresses of illness. De- coping on adjustment, empirical evidence for a stress-
pending on the definition of coping used and on the buffering conception of coping is minimal so far:
nature of the illness studied, these studies have found Pearlin and Schooler’s [18] efforts to locate statistical
typical coping strategies to include: denial, selective ‘stress-buffering’ interaction effects between stress
ignoring, information seeking, taking refuge in activ- and coping behavior in relation to distress were
ity, avoidance, reminiscence about former good largely unsuccessful. Nonetheless, the theoretical
times, learning specific illness-related procedures, proposition deserves further testing, and is particu-
blaming others and seeking comfort from others larly relevant in a study of illnesses which vary in the
[9-l 11.These studies have provided valuable descrip- amount and type of stress which they present.
tions of the types of coping strategies provoked by
illness, and have pointedly raised the question of just
how effective individual coping efforts are at im- METHOD
proving emotional well-being and maintaining phys- Sample
ical health. These studies, however, have most often The study was designed to allow comparisons
confounded measures of coping and adjustment, for among individuals faced with one of four chronic
example, by defining groups of good vs poor capers illnesses: hypertension, diabetes mellitus, rheumatoid
[12, 131; consequently, the question of the role of arthritis and systemic blood cancers. In selecting the
coping in adjustment to illness has not yet been illnesses, those which ran too rapid a course for study
directly addressed. or which were not moderately prevalent among
Research showing that emotion-focused or pal- middle-aged and older Americans were excluded
lative strategies are most effective in situations which from consideration; the illnesses chosen were fairly
are not amenable to individual control [6] suggests equally ‘systemic’, i.e. none was primarily localized in
that the stresses of illness, at least those which are a specific body site. Most importantly, the four
unmanageable by individual or medical efforts, may illnesses were selected to represent a three-point
be most successfully dealt with through strategies continuum of ‘controllability’ and, in the case of
that focus on the emotional distress aroused by the ‘least controllability, two qualitatively different types
illness. Most studies of illness, however, have been of stress.
restricted to a single illness, a research strategy which Hypertension is largely controllable by diet and/or
inevitably ignores variations in the controllability of medication; both forms of treatment are self-
illnesses as well as other variations in other character- administered and adherence to prescribed treatment
istics of the multiple stresses or ‘adaptive tasks’ of is largely the responsibility of the patient [19]. Di-
illness. Research on the role of health in psychosocial abetes mellitus lies in the middle of the continuum:
adjustment in late life has relied for the most part on though the symptoms of diabetes mellitus are usually
a single continuum of self-rated health status as its quite responsive to diet and/or medication, the long-
measure [l] and thus assumes a uniformity in the range effects of disease progression, particularly on
nature of illness’ impact on adjustment which has not the circulatory and renal systems, are largely uncon-
been empirically demonstrated. trollable [20,21]. The diseases characterized as ‘least
The study described here evaluates the utility of a controllable’- cancer and rheumatoid arthritis-are
stress and coping paradigm for explaining psycho- both largely unpredictable in their progression and
logical adjustment to chronic illness by comparing variable in response, if not unresponsive, to treat-
adults with one of four different illnesses. Using ment. These illnesses, however, present very different
closed-ended self-report data about the problem- and types of stress. The forms of cancer included in this
emotion-focused strategies used to cope with illness, study* are often fatal and may run a course almost
this study considers the characteristics of illness, completely beyond personal and medical control;
current cancer treatment necessitates a strong de-
*Three cancer diagnoses were studied: chronic lymphocytic pendency on physicians and often requires adjuvant
leukemia, lymphoma and multiple myeloma. All three therapy, such as chemotherapy or radiation therapy
were treated as a single group in data analyses. [22-251. Though it also has a fairly uncontrollable
Coping and adjustment in chronically ill adults 891

course, rheumatoid arthritis, in contrast, is character- tensives and diabetics; contrary to expectation, how-
ized by extreme pain and decreased joint mobility, ever, they did not report being more limited than
symptoms which are quite debilitating in their impact cancer patients (F = 16.93, P < 0.001). F-tests of
on daily functioning [26] but which pose no immedi- illness group scores on a ten-item version of the
ate life threat. The differences between these two Health Locus of Control Scale [31] revealed a three-
illnesses allowed comparisons between the qualitative point continuum of controllability, with cancer and
impact of life threat and of non-life threatening rheumatoid arthritis patients perceiving themselves as
disability as aspects of chronic disease. having comparably low levels of control over their
The sample was drawn from non-hospitalized illnesses. Diabetics and hypertensives reported more
patients aged 40 and older who had a primary control than patients with cancer and rheumatoid
diagnosis of one of these four illnesses. Referrals to arthritis, as expected, although it was diabetics who
the research project were made by private physicians, perceived themselves as having the greatest amount
specialty clinics at metropolitan hospitals, and a of control, possibly because of the special meaning of
hypertensive screening agency. Respondents were the term ‘control’ in diabetes treatment.
contacted initially through their physician and inter- These data provide good support for the assump-
viewed by research staff with a structured protocol tions that these forms of cancer and rheumatoid
including both open- and closed-ended questions on arthritis are equally uncontrollable but different from
illness characteristics, details of the treatment regi- each other in the degree to which they are perceived
men, coping strategy use, social support, emotional as serious, or life threatening. Diabetics and hyper-
well-being, and demographic data. Of those adults tensives did not order themselves on controllability in
referred to the study who were deemed eligible for relation to each other as medical data would have
participation by virtue of the above criteria, 170, or predicted, but their responses confirm that they per-
68x, consented to participate in the study. ceive their illnesses as more controllable than cancer
The illness groups were roughly comparable in size: and rheumatoid arthritis. While subjective reports of
38 respondents had a primary diagnosis of hyper- health status are not necessarily more accurate than
tension, 44 had diabetes, 45 had rheumatoid arthritis ‘objective’ reports [32], subjective data may reflect the
and 42 had cancer. The average length of time over medical conditions of these particular respondents. In
which subjects had had their illnesses was 65 months this study, medical diagnosis was treated as a nominal
(SD = 91.2). Demographic differences among the ill- variable so that inferences about the effects of con-
ness groups were present in a few instances: the trollability and of qualitative differences in stress
rheumatoid arthritis and hypertensive groups con- could be drawn on the basis of both objective and
tained more women than the other two groups; social subjective characterizations of the illnesses.
class, measured according to Hollingshead and Red- Coping. The coping measures consisted of six scales
lich’s [27] education and occupation indices, was describing qualitatively distinct coping strategies de-
lowest among rheumatoid arthritics, next lowest rived through factor analysis of 55 self-report items.
among hypertensives and highest among respondents Most of the items (45) were drawn from the ‘Ways of
with cancer or diabetes. No differences were found Coping’ scale [5]; ten additional items were adapted
among diagnoses for age or in the proportion of from the work of Pearlin and Schooler [6] or devel-
respondents married or living alone. Overall, the oped through pilot testing. Respondents were asked
sample consisted of primarily white, married, middle- to indicate on a five-point scale how often they used
to-upper middle class adults (67 men, 103 women), each particular coping behavior, if ever, in response
aged 41-89, with a mean age of 61. to their illness; instructions read to respondents said,
“I’m going to read you a list of things that people do
Measures in reaction to being ill. I would like you to tell me if
Medical diagnosis. The four illness groups, defined you have ever done any of these things in reaction to
by respondents’ primary medical diagnosis, were your illness and if you have, do you do it seldom,
assumed to represent a three-point continuum of sometimes, often, or most of the time?’
medical controllability and, in the case of cancer and The factor solution from which the six strategies
rheumatoid arthritis, a contrast between life threat were derived, an orthogonal (varimax) rotation of a
and ongoing disability as qualitative differences in six-factor principal axis solution, was that which
stress. Self-report data were used to verify that re- produced maximally independent factors with sets of
spondents’ perceptions of their illnesses conformed to conceptually meaningful items of sufficient length
these medically-derived characterizations of the ill- and internal consistency to permit their use as
nesses. F-tests of mean differences in these measures scales[33]. Table 1 presents the individual items and
used Duncan multiple range tests (P < 0.05) to eval- their factor loadings. Scales were computed for each
uate specific disease-to-disease differences on these factor by unit weighting and summing items with
variables. factor loadings greater than or equal to 0.30. While
Consistent with expectations, cancer patients per- the relatively small cases-to-variables ratio (about
ceived the ‘seriousness of their illness’, a concept 3-to-l) poses the threat of unreliability, all of the
closely akin to perceived life threat [28] and measured factors describe strategies similar or identical to
here with a single question, significantly greater than strategies described in research on other life stresses
did other respondents (F = 7.56, P c 0.001). Patients [34,35].
with rheumatoid arthritis, in response to a list of eight Cognitive Restructuring (13 items, alpha = 0.80)
activities of daily living derived from scales by Rosow describes individual efforts at finding positive aspects
and Breslau [29] and by Caplan et al. [30], reported of the illness experience and at regarding the illness
more limitations in daily functioning than hyper- as an opportunity for inner growth. Through com-
892 BARBARA J. FELTON et al.

parisons with past stressful times and redefinition of Minimzation (11 items, alpha = 0.65) invokes a re-
the illness, this strategy reflects a cognitive reap- fusal to dwell on thoughts about the illness and a
praisal for the illness which makes it appear less conscious decision to put distressing thoughts aside.
threatening and distressing. Emotional Expression (8 The illness is implicitly acknowledged, but main-
items, alpha = 0.72) describes a coping response con- tained at a comfortable psychological distance by a
sisting of expressions of emotional strain, such refusal to discuss it with others. e.g. ‘Went on as if
as getting angry at other people or joking about nothing happened’, ‘Didn’t let it get to you’.
the illness. Wish -Fulfilling Fantasy (7 items, Comparison of mean levels of endorsement of each
alpha = 0.79) describes an indulgence in pining and coping strategy across the sample shows that Threat
longing for the illness to go away or be over with. Minimization was used most often and Self-Blame
This strategy presumably allows people to alleviate most infrequently used (see Table 2). Informa-
the emotional distress of being ill by providing an tion Seeking, the one instrumental or problem-
escape in fantasy. Self-Blame (7 items, alpha = 0.75) focused strategy, was used with only moderate fre-
is a strategy in which blaming oneself for the illness quency. Intercorrelations among coping scales were
coincides with other efforts to cloud the issue and modest, averaging 0.26.
refocus attention in order to avoid accepting the Psychological adjustment. Four outcome measures
illness as a chronic problem (e.g. ‘Got mad at people were used. Acceptance of Illness (8 items.
or things that caused the problem’). Information alpha = 0.83) assessed respondents’ success in feeling
Seeking (5 items, alpha = 0.67) describes the individ- acceptant of, and valuable despite, the disability,
ual’s search for information and advice about the dependency, and feelings of uselessness which illness
illness and its treatment. More so than the other occasions. Items were drawn from Linkowski’s [36]
factors, Information Seeking is characterized by an Sickness Impact scales and rated on a five-point
active, instrumental orientation to illness. Threat ‘agree-disagree’ continuum. Exemplary items include

Table I. Factor analysis of coping items


Factor
I II III IV V VI

I. Cognitive Resrructuring (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.80)


Concentrated on something good that could come out
of the whole thing 0.78 -0.02 0.09 0.09 0.01 0.06
Rediscovered what is important in life 0.59 0.15 0.10 0.03 0.10 0.05
Felt like you changed or grew as a person in a good
way 0.58 0.35 0.01 0.17 0.02 -0.01
Found new faith or some truth about life 0.53 0.3 I 0.11 0.10 0.09 0.03
Remembered times when your life was more difficult 0.42 -0.02 0.05 0.06 0.02 0.01
Turned to work or other things to take your mind off
the problem 0.39 0.06 0.18 0.05 0.24 0.10
Religion became more important 0.36 0.38 0.08 0.12 0.06 -0.00
Thought about people who were worse off than you 0.35 0.17 0.19 -0.06 0.16 0.16
Reminded yourself that things could be worse 0.34 0.15 0.26 -0.15 0.18 -0.16
Looked for the silver lining, so to speak; tried to
look on the bright side of things 0.34 -0.06 - 0.17 -0.21 0.01 0.1 I
Did something totally new that you never would have
done if this hadn’t happened 0.32 0.17 0.07 0.00 021 0.05
Changed the way you did things so that the illness
was less of a problem 0.3 I 0.00 0.19 0.23 0.10 -0.10
Got away from it for a while; tried to rest or take
a vacation 0.30 0.12 0.14 -0.07 0.23 0.18

II. Emotional Expression (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.72)


Took it out on other people 0.12 0.62 0.01 0.23 0.06 0.01
Got help with day-to-day chores or travel 0.14 0.48 0.13 -0.08 0.09 -0.05
Joked about it 0.21 0.44 0.03 0.05 0.15 0.02
Let your feelitigs out somehow 0.13 0.40 0.15 0.23 0.28 -0.21
Avoided being with people in general 0.10 0.40 0.07 0.08 0.06 0.14
Recalled past successes 0.17 0.38 0.3 I 0.13 0.09 0.09
Daydreamed or imaged a better time or place than
the one you were in 0.08 0.37 0.24 0.29 0.12 0.07
Slept more than usual 0.06 0.34 0.14 0.03 0.1 I -0.05

III. Wish-Furfihg Fantasy (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79)


Wished that you could change what had happened 0.07 -0.01 0.67 0.13 0.05 0.02
Wished that you could change the way you felt 0.08 0.25 0.67 0.03 -0.02 -0.05
Felt bad that you couldn’t avold the problem 0.11 0.17 0.61 0.14 -0.09 -0.01
Wished that the situation would go away or somehow
be over with 0.25 0.02 0.58 -0.06 0.21 -0.00
Hoped a miracle would happen 0.12 0.14 0.49 0.07 0.14 -0.20
Wished you were a stronger person 0.16 0.3 I 0.40 0.08 -0.14 -0.01
Had fantasies or wishes about how things might turn
O”t 0.06 0.33 0.35 0.29 0.21 -0.02

IV. Self-f?/ome (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75)


Blamed yourself 0.20 0.15 0.17 0.56 0.07 0.07
Coping and adjustment in chronically ill adults 893

Table I (conhued)
Factor
I II III IV V VI
__.__
Thought about fantastic or unreal things that made
you feel better 0.20 0.25 0.2 I 0.47 0.22 0.15
Saw the doctor and did what he recommended 0.06 0.16 0.00 -0.47 0.09 0.05
Got mad at the people or things that caused the
problem 0.02 0.32 0.03 0.44 0.10 0.09
Criticized OT took it out on yourself 0.19 0.40 0.22 0.42 -0.01 0.01
Realized you brought the problem on yourself 0.30 0.10 -0.01 0.40 0.12 -0.06
Refused to b&eve it had happened 0.14 0.25 0.20 0.39 -0.02 0.21
V. h~formarion-seeking (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.67)
Looked up medical information 0.08 0.11 0.09 0.05 0.80 -0.04
Read books or magazine articles (or watched TV)
about (your illness) 0.1 I 0.24 0.21 0.10 0.61 0.04
Came up with a couple of different solutions to the
problem 0.24 0.29 -0.05 0.13 0.42 0.10
Asked someone other than a doctor you respected
for advice and followed it 0.26 0.20 -0.01 0.14 0.38 -0.23
Made a plan of action and followed it 0.33 -0.05 -0.25 0.07 0.32 0.03
VI. Threar Minimizarion (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.65)
Kept your feelings to yourself 0.06 -0.04 0.21 -0.00 -0.10 0.58
Went on as if nothing had happened -0.06 -0.04 -0.20 0.08 -0.00 0.50
Talked to someone about how you were feeling 0.27 0.39 0.04 0.00 0.06 -0.46
Didn’t let it get to you: refused to think too
much about it 0.06 0.10 -0.09 -0.07 0.17 0.44
Kept others from knowing how bad things were 0.24 0.04 0.18 0.04 -0.08 0.37
Tried to forget the whole thing 0.19 0.01 -0.02 0.07 0.01 0.36
Talked to someone other than a doctor who could do
something about the problem for you 0.23 0.28 -0.04 0.23 0.25 -0.32
Tried to work it out by yourself 0.12 -0.06 -0.16 0.03 0.02 0.31
Accepted sympathy and understanding from someone 0.13 0.39 0.14 -0.05 -0.14 -0.31
Made light of the situation; refused to get too
serious about it 0.15 0.23 -0.10 0.12 0.02 0.31
Went along with fate; sometimes you just have bad
luck -0.18 0.25 0.15 -0.03 -0.03 0.30
items Loading Eeh 0.30, Nor Included in Scales
Concentrated on following the doctor’s orders 0.10 -0.20 0.10 -0.27 -0.07 0.08
Tried to make yourself feel better by eatmg.
drinking. smokmg, etc 0.19 0.25 0.04 0.18 0.09 0.03
Faced the situation head on 0.01 -0.05 -0.11 -0.25 -0.03 -0.02
Accepted it, since nothing could be done -0.08 0.05 0.05 -0.07 -O.lY 0.21
These factor analysis results were computed with the FACTOR program from the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS: Nie,
Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner and Bent, 1975) using orthogonal (varimax) rotation. Two items were dropped prior to factor analysis
because of low vanance. and the remaining 55 items were submitted to factor analysis.
Coping scales were computed by summing the respondent’s scores on all of the items listed under each factor name. Scores on items with
negative loadings were reversed before scale construction.

Table 2. Mean levels of coping strategy use for diagnostic groups


Total
sample Hypertension Diahetes Cancer arthritis
(N = 170) fN = 39) (N =44) (N = 42) (N = 45)

Cognitive Mean 2.85 2.78 2.84 2.66 3.13


Restructuring SD 0.73 0.64 0.83 0.69 0.68
F(3, 166) = 3.30, P < 0.05

Emotional Mean 2.17 2.03 1.97 2.20 2.47


Expression SD 0.71 0.74 0.74 0.74 0.53
F(3, 166) = 4.79, P < 0.01

Wish-Fulfilling Mean 2.87 2.49 2.65 2.81 3.48


Fantasy SD 0.99 0.99 0.94 0.80 0.93
F(3, 166) = 9.92, P < 0.001

Self-Blame Mean I.61 I .66 1.58 1.54 1.67


SD 0.66 0.77 0.57 0.71 0.61
(F(3, 166) c 1.00
Infonnatmn Mean 2.52 2.53 2.63 2.25 2.67
Seeking SD 0.91 0.74 1.03 0.86 0.93
F‘(3, 166) = 1.88, NS
Threat Mean 3.38 3.56 3.32 3.38 3.30
Minimization SD 0.66 0.58 0.77 0.64 0.63
P(3, 166) = 1.33, NS
Summated scores were dwided by the number of items in each scale in order to permit comparisons across scales;
thus coping scale scores presented above range from I (low use) to 5 (high use).
894 BARBARA J. FELTON et al.

Table 3. The relationship between diagnosis and strategy use [9] analyses (whose results are not
coping controlling for the effects of demographic shown) indicated that this variable was unrelated to
factors
coping in this sample.
Diagnosis F* P
Cognitive Restructuring 0.50 NS The relationship between coping and ps~&ologicui
Emotional Expression 1.85 NS adjustment
Wish-Fulfilling Fantasy 4.90 <O.Ol
Self-Blame 0.47 NS Multiple regression analysis was used to provide
Information Seeking 0.29 NS information on the direction and strength of re-
Threat Minimization 0.76 NS lationships between coping and adjustment with the
effects of demographic factors and diagnosis re-
The set of demographic covariates includes sex, age, moved. In order to test the unique contribution of
socioeconomic status, marital status and em-
ployment status. The proportions of variance in
each type of coping strategy, a set of six separate
coping strategies explained by the set of de- hierarchical regression equations was computed for
mographic variables ranged from 3 to 9’?/,. each of the coping strategies with each of the adjust-
*F-ratios were computed using a hierarchical analy- ment measures. The hierarchical entry of the vari-
sis of covariance with the set of demographic
ables in each equation reflected the most likely causal
variables entered on the first step.
ordering among the sets of predictors: the set of
demographic factors was controlled for first, followed
by a set of three dummy variables representing the
‘I have a hard time adjusting to the limitations of my four diagnostic groups in the sample; then the partic-
illness’, and ‘Because of my health, I miss the things ular coping strategy under consideration was entered
I like to do most’. to assess main effects, and finally, the term carrying
Rosenberg’s [37] ten-item Self-Esteem scale was the statistical interaction between diagnosis and the
used to evaluate the individual’s overall sense of particular coping strategy. The interaction term in
being capable, worthwhile and competent. Items were each case consisted of the arithmetic product of the
answered using a five-point agree-disagree format. effects-coded dummy variable and a coping scale. The
This scale, used extensively and found to have high significance of this term demonstrates ‘non-linearity‘
construct validity across diverse samples [38], pro- in the relationship between stress and adjustment
duced a coefficient alpha of 0.81 in this sample. when coping is taken into account [14].
Both positive and negative affective components of Results of these analyses are presented in Table 4.
well-being were measured, since the factors which Overall, the full regression equations explained small
contribute to positive affect have been shown to be to moderate proportions of the variance in adjust-
different from those which create feelings of dissatis- ment: 9-21% in positive affect, 11-219, in negative
faction [39]. The five-item Positive and Negative affect, 17-25% in self-esteem and 26-347; in accept-
Affect subscales of the Bradburn Affect Balance scale ance of illness. Information about the power of
[40] were used to assess positive and negative mood coping to explain individual differences in adjustment
states over the past few months; the scales obtained is contained in column 3, which shows the increase in
alpha coefficients of 0.72 and 0.64 respectively. the proportion of variance in adjustment due to
Demographic data. Variables considered particu- coping after the effects of demographic factors and
larly important in this study and thus measured diagnosis have been removed.
through self-reports and used in analyses, were those Each of the adjustment measures was significantly
previously shown to be related to stress, coping explained by two or three coping strategies which,
and/or psycho-social well-being: sex, current marital depending on the particular equation under consid-
status, age, current employment status (employed vs eration, uniquely accounted for up to 11”; of the
non-employed), and social class [27,41,6]. variance in adjustment. Positive affect, unrelated
to medical diagnosis, was significantly related to
RESULTS two coping strategies: cognitive restructuring
(AR2 = 0.11, P < 0.01) and information seeking
Illness-to -illness dtferences in coping (AR2 = 0.10, P < 0.01). These two coping strategies
One-way analyses of variance assessing the effect of were, with only one exception, unrelated to the other
diagnosis on the frequency with which each coping outcome variables.
strategy was used were performed to determine Negative affect, related to demographic factors but
whether illness controllability or other characteristics independent of diagnosis, was significantly related to
of illness were related to coping strategy use. Results the use of three coping strategies: emotional expres-
(Table 2) show the strategies of cognitive restruc- sion (AR2 = 0.88, P < O.Ol), wish-fulfilling fantasy
turing, emotional expression and wish-fulfilling fan- (AR2 = 0.11, P < 0.01) and, to a somewhat lesser
tasy to be more frequently used by rheumatoid extent, self blame (AR’ = 0.03, P < 0.05). The direc-
arthritis patients than other patients (Duncan range tion of this relationship shows greater use of these
tests, P < 0.05). Two of these effects, however, disap- strategies linked to reports of greater negative affect.
peared when statistical controls for demographic Results using self-esteem as the dependent variable
factors were introduced into the analyses. After de- were quite similar: even with the significant effects
mographic differences between the illness groups of diagnosis removed, emotional expression
were partialled (Table 3), wish-fulfilling fantasy alone (AR2 = 0.07, P < 0.01), wish-fulfilling fantasy
persisted as a significant correlate of diagnosis. (AR2 = 0.03, P < 0.05), and self-blame (AR2 = 0.06,
Though the length of the respondents’ illness has P < 0.01) were significantly linked to lowered self-
been proposed by some to be a determinant of coping esteem. In addition, cognitive restructuring explained
Table 4. Hierarchical multiple regression of demographic factors, diagnosis, coping and diagnosis-by-coping interactions 011 psychological adjustment
Demographic Diagnosis x Coping Total Percent of
factors Diagnosis Coping Interactions Variance Explained
Adjustment
Measure AR’ Ft AR2 Ff AR= 4 Beta B AR= _!I1 ~~~ AR’ Fl
Positive affect Cognitive Restructuring 0.05 I .92 0.03 2.20 0.1 I 21.85” 0.34 0.14 0.01 0.20 3.17”
Emotional Expression I .76 2.01 0.00 0.06 0.05 0.04 2.44 0.13 I.81
Wish-Fulfilling Fantasy I .68 1.92 0.00 -0.07 -0.02 0.00 0.09 1.20
Self-Blame 1.70 I .93 0.00 0.04 0.03 0.01 0.09 1.30 0
Information Seeking 1.95 2.23 0.10 20.33” 0.33 0.1 I 0.04 2.7l’tt 0.21 3.89” +.
Threat Minimization I .70 1.94 0.00 -0.04 -0.01 0.01 0. IO 1.34
tz
Negative alTect Cognitive Restructuring 0.09 3.24’* 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.11 1.9lf :
Emotional Expression 3.43” 0.08 15.79” 0.31
-0.00 0.12 0.01 0.19 3.75” n
Wish-Fulfilling Fantasy 3.64.’ 0.11 22.31** 0.37 0.11 0.00 0.21 3.34’. h
_.
Self-Blame 3.43” 0.03 5.72* 0.17 0.06 0.03 I.91 0.16 2.40**
2.20 E
Information Seeking 3.371. 0.02 3.76 0.16 0.06 0.03 I .88 0.15
Threat Minimization 3.28” 0.02 3.66 -0.14 -0.07 0.01 0.13 I .79 $
SelCesteem Cognitive Restructuring 0.12 4.83” 0.04 2.45 0.02 3.96* 0.13 0.08 0.02 1.32 0.19 2.97
Emotional Expression 5.18’ 2.83’ 0.07 14.85” -0.29 -0.23 0.01 - 0.25 4.08,. 5’
Wish-Fulfilling Fantasy 4.91 l * 2.88* 0.03 6.03’ -0.18 -0.08 0.02 1.34 0.20 3.21” B
Self-Blame 5.03” 2.95’ 0.06 12.35” -0.24 -0.18 0.01 0.23 3.71**
3.17” 2
I.
Information Seeking 4.89’. 2.87. 0.00 - -0.04 -0.05 0.04 8.02tt 0.20
Threat Minimization 4.69’. 2.75’ 0.00 0.03 0.01 0.01 I .92 0.17 2.51** P
=
v:
Acceptance of Cognitive Restructuring 0.12 6.16** 0.1 I 7.96’. 0.00 0.06 0.03 0.03 2.17 0.26 4.44**
5.55” 6
illness Emotional Expression 5.52** 8.49** 0.07 16.21” -0.29 -0.29 0.01 0.3 I
0
Wish-Fulfilling Fantasy 5.77” 8.87” 0.1 I 26.62” -0.36 -0.27 0.00 0.34 6.37..
4.83** 2
Self-Blame 4.77” 8.12” 0.05 10.35” -0.22 -0.22 0.00 0.28
E
Information Seeking 5.20” 7.99” 0.00 -0.07 -0.09 0.03 2.18 0.27 4.50**
Threat Minimization 5.19’1 1.99” 0.03 6.53’ 0.17 0.16 0.01 0.27 4.87**
lf cnns. **P <nOI df.=t5. 16n: t1. 160: 61.l60: 113.160:712.160.
tjThbiit;re of this interaction-.&/ i~&h;hat’if x 2 inform&n Seeking, the prediction of Positive alText(Y) is as follows: Y = 0.20X + 0.98 for respondents with rheumatoid arthritis: Y = O.Ol.\’ + I .47
for those with cancer: Y =0.10X + I.31 for those with diabetes; and Y = 0.1 IX + 1.37 for those with hypertension.
$$The nature of this interaction eflect is such that if X = Information Seekmg, the prediction of Self-Esteem is as follows: Y =0.01X + 3.15 for respondents with rheumatoid arthritis: Y = -0.14X + 3.70
for those with ‘cancer; Y = 0.1 IX + 3.09 for those with diabetes; and Y = -0.20X + 3.94 for those with hypertension.
896 BARBARA J. FELTON er al.

a small but significant proportion of unique variance arthritis, on the one hand. and diabetes melhtus and
in self-esteem (AR* = 0.02, P < 0.05), with this form hypertension, on the other, made the difference be-
of coping related to greater self-esteem. With accept- tween being low in self-esteem and nonacceptant of
ance of illness as the dependent variable, emotional illness rather than high in self-esteem and acceptant
expression, wish-fulfilling fantasy and self-blame of the illness. Thus, illness controlability. as rated by
again proved significantly related to poorer adjust- subjects or by medical opinion. did not completely
ment (AR* = 0.07, 0.11 and 0.05, respectively; explain coping or adjustment; level of disability mat
P < O.Ol), with the effects of demographic factors also be important in explaining these aspects ot
and diagnosis removed. The coping strategy of threat adjustment. Cancer and rheumatoid arthritis patients
minimization also explained a unique significant pro- reported larger numbers of daily tasks which they
portion of the variance in acceptance of illness could not perform better than the other patients.
(AR* = 0.03, P < 0.05), with coping being related to Further, in response to an open-ended question
better adjustment. Examination of the dummy vari- about the stresses of illness, patients with the cancer
able sets show that relationships between diagnosis and rheumatoid arthritis named more illness-related
and the dependent variables of illness acceptance and problems overall, the vast majority of which were
self-esteem were due to the higher levels of adjust- descriptions of disabilities, as compared to re-
ment among hypertensives and diabetics as compared spondents with hypertension and diabetes. These
to cancer patients and rheumatoid arthritics (Duncan findings and those showing disability to be a central
range, P < 0.05). element in people’s perceptions of their overall health
Evidence that the relationship between coping and [32] help clarify the persistent relationship between
adjustment conforms to that predicted by a stress- physical health and emotional well-being in middle
buffering perspective was minimal. As shown in age and late life [44].
column 4 of Table 4, in only two of the 24 analyses Neither level of disability nor any other distinction
(8%) did the diagnosis x coping interaction term among illnesses, however, played a role in deter-
attain a 0.05 level of statistical significance. Although mining which coping strategies were effective in pro-
both cases showed information seeking to be posi- moting adjustment and alleviating distress, as the
tively related to adjustment for diabetics, the overall small number of interaction effects which proved
paucity and inconsistency of interaction effects leaves significant could well have occurred by chance. The
the stress-buffering hypothesis largely unsupported. consequences of coping seem to be relatively consis-
tent for the different types of illness examined, not
DISCUSSION
varied as they would be if the ‘stress-buffering hy-
pothesis’ described the primary modus operandi for
The issue of central importance in this study was the health-protective effects of coping. The failure of
that of the utility of a stress and coping paradigm [4] interaction terms to explain psychological adjustment
for explaining individual differences in psychological may mean that theories of adaptation have to modify
adjustment to chronic illness. Our results suggest that their conception of the way in which coping buffers
a modest part of the variation in adjustment can be adults from the harmful effects of stress.
explained by considering the nature of individual The nature of the significant relationships found
coping responses. All of the qualitatively different between outcome measures of adjustment and the
coping strategies examined in this study made a coping strategies of wish-fulfilling fantasy, emotional
significant contribution to the explanation of vari- expression and self-blame suggests that the con-
ance in at least one of the adjustment measures even sequences of individual coping effects are not alto-
after the effects of demographic factors and type of gether positive. Using such strategies did not prevent
illness had been removed. While the proportions of loss of self-esteem, promote acceptance of illness, nor
the variance in adjustment explained by coping are protect the individual from feeling sad or depressed;
not always large, the results suggest that further on the contrary, emotion-based coping was related to
efforts to clarify the role of coping in the explanation poorer adjustment. The cross-sectional nature of
of psychological adjustment is warranted. these data makes it unclear whether use of these
Despite wide variation in medical treatment de- strategies prompted negative feeling states or whether
mands, levels of disability and immediacy of life these strategies were in fact behavioral reactions to
threat posed by the illnesses in this study, the types stress. Despite the fact that causal direction cannot.be
of coping strategies used by respondents with clearly established, however, the findings are note-
different illnesses were largely the same. Differences worthy in that they are clearly discrepant with a
among illnesses may have been partially obscured in major current conception of coping: that strategies
this study by the use of a coping measure which asked involving avoidance and perceptual distortions of
about reactions to the illness in general rather than reality may well be adaptive, particularly when the
to specific kinds of stress. Nonetheless, in contrast stressful situation is an unalterable one [4. 6,451.
with some psychosomatic research which, in studies The emergence of two distinct patterns of re-
of single illnesses, has identified ‘syndromes’ of be- lationships between coping behavior and adjustment
havior or personality traits characteristic of specific shows that their interrelationship is not a constant
types of illness [43], the findings of this study suggest one. The patterns showed that greater positive
that particular diseases are not exclusively linked with affective states, while unrelated to stress, were associ-
particular styles of coping. ated with primarily cognitive strategies, i.e. incorpo-
The illnesses studied here did vary in their impact rating relevant information or changing the nature of
on two aspects of psychological adjustment. The one’s cognitions in order to construe the illness as an
distinction between having cancer and rheumatoid occasion for personal growth. A pattern of height-
Coping and adjustment in chronically ill adults 897

ened negative affect coupled with lowered self-esteem 2. Palmore E. and Luikart C. Health and social factors
and poorer adjustment, on the other hand, was also related to life satisfaction. J. Hlth sot. Behac. 13, 68,
linked to stress and, independently, to the use of 1972.
emotionally expressive, avoidant and blaming coping 3. Zarit S. H. Aging and Mental Disorders. Free Press,
New York, 1980.
strategies. That there is divergence between the types
4. Lazarus R. S. The stress and coping paradigm. In
of psychological adjustment states coinciding with Models for Clinical Psvchopathologv (Edited by Eis-
particular coping strategies is consistent with other dorfer 6. er ol.), pp. 177-214. Q&rum, New York,
research demonstrating that positive and negative 1981.
affect are linked to different personality styles [39]. 5. Folkman S. and Lazarus R. S. An analysis of coping in
The specialized consequences of different coping stra- a middle-aged community sample. J. Hlrh sot. Eehac.
tegies may provide an alternative basis for 21, 219. 1980.
classifications of coping: in addition to distinguishing 6. Pearlin L. I. and Schooler C. The structure of coping.
between coping strategies according to the locus of J. HIth sot. Behav. 19, 2. 1978.
7. McCrae R. R. Ane differences in the use of copina . _
their efforts (e.g. problem-focused vs emotion- mechanisms. J. Geronf. 37, 454, 1982.
focused), it may be useful in future investigations to 8. Mechanic D. Social structure and personal adaptation:
distinguish among coping strategies according to the some neglected dimensions. In Coping and Adaptation
locus of their effects. (Edited by Coelho G. V. et al.), p. 32. Basic Books, New
It is important to note the possibility that a stress York, 1974.
and coping paradigm is a more viable conception of 9. Cohen F. and Lazarus R. S. Coping with the stresses of
coping than suggested by these data: evidence of its illness. In Health Psychology (Edited by Stone G. C. ef
operation might appear only when variations in stress al.), pp. 217-254. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, 1979.
are more extreme than those studied here. The nature 10. Moos R. H. and Tsu V. D. The crisis of nhvsical illness:
An overview. In Coping wifh Physical Ili& (Edited by
of chronic illness, and the kind of underlying uncon-
Moos R. H.), pp. 3-21. Plenum Press, New York, 1977.
trollability characteristic of a health condition which, 11 Weisman A. D. and Worden J. W. The existential plight
by definition, will not go away, may be sufficiently in cancer: significance of the first 100 days. Inr. J.
great to overwhelm differences in coping effectiveness Psychiar. Med. 7, I, 1976.
across the range of stresses represented by the illness 12. Hackett T. P. and Cassem N. H. Psychological manage-
studied. Future tests of the stress-buffering theory of ment of the mycardial infaction patient. J. Hum. Stress
coping ought to consider differences in the effects of 1, 25, 1975.
coping across diverse arrays of health conditions and 13. Sanders J. B. and Kardinal C. G. Adaptive coping
mechanisms in adult acute leukemia patients in re-
treatments, as the nature and critical range of stress
mission. J. Am. med. Ass. 238, 952, 1977.
in illness conditions may not yet have been fully
14. Marshall J. R. Stress, strain and coping. (Comment on
explored. Pearlin and Schooler, JHSE, March 1978.) J. Hlth sot.
Taking the current findings at face value, however, Behan. 20, ‘200, 1979.
prompts the conclusion that the effects of individual 15. Wheaton B. The sociogenesis of psychological disorder:
coping efforts on emotional distress are rather an attributional theory. J. Hlth sot. Behar. 21, 100,
modest. This conclusion is consistent with research 1980.
showing that the emotional consequences of uncon- 16. Cobb S. Social support as a moderator life stress.
trollable stresses other than illness are largely Psychosomat. Med. 38, 300, 1976.
unaffected by individual coping efforts [6]. The con- 17. Felton B. J.. Brown P.. Lehmann S. and Liberatos P.
The coping function of sex role attitudes during marital
sistency of the relationship between physical and
disruption. J. Hlth sot. Behac. 21, 240, 1980.-
emotional well-being [ 1,2] reinforces the view that 18. Pearlin L. I. and Schooler C. Some extensions of “The
individuals, acting with their own solitary capacities, Structure of Coping” (Reply to comments by Marshall
can do relatively little to alter the emotional distress and Gore). J. filtksoc. Behav. 20, 202, 197%.
which illness so often occasions. 19. Robbins S. L. Pathologic Basis of Disease. W. B.
As evidence accumulates to demonstrate that long Saunders, Philadelphia, 1974.
term physical health can be a consequence of emo- 20. Cahill G. F., Etzwiler D. D. and Frienkel N. “Control”
tional state [46,47], the ineffectiveness of individual and diabetes. New En~l. J. Med. 294. 1004. 1976.
coping efforts becomes even more significant: further 21. Rifkin H. Why controidiabetes? Med: Clin. ‘N. Am. 62,
747, 1978.
deterioration of physical health may be the outcome
22. National Cancer Institute. What You Need to Knon
of a vicious cycle of illness-based stress, ineffective About Hodgkins Disease. U.S.D.H.E.W., U.S. Govem-
coping, and poor emotional adjustment [48]. While ment Printing Office of Cancer Communications, NIH
we lack definitive information about the value of Publication No. 80-1555, 1979.
alternative coping agents, research and theory on 23. National Cancer Institute. Whor You Need fo Know
social supports [16,49] points to relationships with About Adult Leukemia. U.S.D.H.E.W., U.S. Govem-
significant others-including the physician [30l_as ment Printing Office, Office of Cancer Communications,
important areas of influence. Subsequent research NIH Publication No. 80-1572, 1980.
24. National Cancer Institute. What You Need ro Know
should examine the variations in illness stresses and
About Multiple Myeloma. U.S.D.H.E.W., U.S. Govem-
social supports and their interaction with individual ment Printing Office, Office of Cancer Communications,
coping efforts to determine how we can best prevent NIH Publication No. 80-1575. 1980.
physical illness from inevitably compromising mental 25. National Cancer Institute. What You Need ro Knon
health. About Non-Hodgkin’s Lvmphoma. U.S.D.H.E.W., U.S.
REFERENCES Government Printing Office, Office of Cancer Commu-
nications, NIH Publication No. 80-1567, 1981.
1. Larson R. Thirty years of research on the subjective 26. Bienenstock H. and Fernando K. R. Arthritis in the
well-being of older Americans. J. Geronf. 33, 109, 1978. elderly. Med. Clin. N. Am. 60, 1173, 1976.
898 BARBARA J. FELTON -et al.

21. Hollingshead A. B. and Redlich F. C. Social Class and 40. Bradbum N.. The Structure qf Psychological Well-
Mental Illness. Wiley, New York, 1958. Being. Aldine. Chicago. 1969.
28. Wyler A. R., Masuda M. and Holmes T. H. Seriousness 41. Myers J. K., Lindenthal J. L. and Pepper M. P. Social
of illness rating scale. J. Psychosomat. Res. 11, 363. Class, life events, and psychiatric symptoms: a longi-
1968. tudinal study. In Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and
29. Rosow I. and Breslau N. A Guttman health scale for the Effects (Edited by Dohrenwend B. S. and Dohrenwend
aged. J. Geront. 21, 556, 1966. B. P.). DD. 191-205. Wilev. New York, 1974.
30. Caplan R. D., Robinson E. A. R., French J. R. P. Jr. 42. Srole L. and Fischer A. K. (Eds) Mental Health in the
Caldwell J. R. and Shinn M. Adhering to Medical Metropolis: The Midtown Manhattan Study. Harper &
Treatment Regimens. Institute for Social Research. Ann Row, New York, 1975.
Arbor, MI, 1976. 43. Dunbar H. F., Wolfe T. P. and Rioch J. McK.
31. Wallston B. S.. Wallston K. A.. Kanlan G. D. and Psychiatric aspects of medical problems: the psychic
Maides S. A. Development and validation of the health comoonent in the disease nrocess (including con-
locus of control (HCL) scale. J. Consult. clin. Psychol. valescence) in cardiac, diabetic and fracture patients.
44, 580, 1976. Am. J. Psychiat. 93, 479. 1936.
32. Ferraro K. F. Self-ratings of health among the old and 44. Herzog A. R. and Rodgers W. L. The structure of
the old-old. J. Hlth Sot. Behav. 21, 377, 1980. subjective well-being in different age groups. J. Geront.
33. Felton B. J., Hinrichsen G. A., Revenson T. A. and 36, 472. 1981.
Efron R. C. Coping with chronic illness: a factor 45. White R. W. Strategies of adaptation: an attempt at
analytic exploration. Paper presented at the annual systematic description. In Coping and Adaptation (Edi-
meetings of the American Psychological Association, ted by Coelho G. V. et al.), pp. 47-68. Basic Books.
Montreal, 1980. New York, 1974.
34. Chiriboga D. A. Stress and coping during the divorce 46. Bradley C. Psychophysiological effects of stressful ex-
process. J. Divorce In press. periences and the management of diabetes mellitus. In
35. Aldwin C., Folkman S., Schaefer C., Coyne J. and Research in Psychology and Medicine (Edited by
Lazarus R. S. Ways of coping: a process measure. Paper Obome D. J. et al.), pp. 133-139. Academic Press, New
presented at the annual meeting of the American Psy- York, 1979.
chological Association, Montreal, 1980. 47. Derogatis L. R., Abeloff M. D. and Melisaratos N.
36. Linkowski D. C. A scale to measure acceptance of Psychological coping mechanisms and survival time in
disabilitv. Rehab. Counsel. Bull. 14. 236, 1971. metastatic breast cancer. J. Am. Med. Ass. 242, 1504,
37. Rosenberg M. I. Society and the Adolescent Serf-Image. 1979.
Princeton-University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1965. 48. Felton B. J. and Revenson T. A. Coping with chronic
38. Robinson J. P. and Shaver P. R. Measures of Social illness: a study of illness controllability and the influence
Psychological Attitudes, rev. edition. Institute for Social of coping strategies on psychological adjustment.
Research, Ann Arbor, MI, 1973. J. Consult. clin Psychol. In press.
39. Costa P. T. Jr and McCrae R. R. Influence of extra- 49. Revenson T. A., Wollman C. A. and Felton B. J. Social
version and neuroticism on subjective well-being: happy Supports as stress buffers for adult cancer patients.
and unhappy people. J. Personal. sot. Psychol. 38, 668, Psychosomat. Med. 45, 321, 1983.
1980.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen