Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

Contributors to young drivers’ driving styles – A comparison between Israel MARK


and Queensland

Vera Skvirskya, Orit Taubman Ben-Aria, , Timothy J. Greenburyb, Carlo G. Pratob
a
The Louis and Gabi Weisfeld School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
b
School of Civil Engineering, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, 4072, Brisbane, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Among the numerous factors that contribute to young novice drivers’ driving styles, personality characteristics,
Driving styles sociodemographic variables, family atmosphere, and friends’ norms are known to have an important impact.
Young drivers However, cross-cultural comparisons are relatively rare in the safety literature concerning young drivers. This
Separation-individuation study aimed at comparing young drivers from Israel and Queensland (Australia) and examining the contribution
Family climate for road safety
of personality, sociodemographic, family and friends’ aspects to their driving styles (reckless and careless; hostile
Safe driving climate among friends
and angry; anxious; patient and careful). More specifically, this study examined the associations between young
drivers’ driving style and their perceptions of separation-individuation, the family climate for road safety, and
the safe driving climate among friends. We also examined sociodemographic and driving history variables such
as gender, the marital status of parents, and personal exposure to traffic crashes. The study consisted of two
samples of male and female young drivers (age 17–22) from Israel (n = 160) and Queensland (n = 160), who
completed a set of valid and reliable self-report questionnaires. Findings indicate that in general, maladaptive
driving styles are associated with lower family tendency to engage in promoting road safety, higher pressure and
costs of driving with peers, and unhealthier separation-individuation aspects. The opposite is observed for the
patient and careful driving style that relates to higher engagement of the family in road safety, lower pressure
from friends, and healthier separation-individuation. Some differences were found regarding specific styles
between the two samples. In addition, women scored lower than men in the reckless and careless style, and
higher (in the Israeli sample) in the anxious as well as the patient and careful styles. Overall, similarities in the
associations between the study variables in the samples exceed the differences, and the importance of examining
variables on multi-levels when referring to young drivers’ driving styles, is confirmed. The findings attest to the
universal utility of the MDSI, together with the understanding that only a wider examination of personal and
environmental contributors enables true insights into the complex behavior of driving among young drivers.

1. Introduction (Bener et al., 2013; Özkan et al., 2006; Warner et al., 2011). In addition,
studies have indicated demographic as well as cultural factors related to
A recent review of the utility and uses of the Multidimensional driver behavior or to involvement in traffic crashes (e.g., Nordfjarn
Driving Style Inventory (MDSI; Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004) revealed et al., 2014; Özkan et al., 2006). The current study aimed at comparing
that it has been the subject of studies conducted around the world, young drivers from Israel and Queensland (Australia). Moreover, the
especially in Israel, where the instrument was originally designed study looked into the role played by various contributors to the young
(Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky, 2016). The MDSI has been translated drivers’ driving styles: socio-economic and driving history variables,
into numerous languages (e.g., English, Italian, Russian, Arabic), and their level of separation-individuation, their perceptions of the family
used in various countries (e.g., England, Italy, Romania, the United climate for road safety (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami, 2012,
States, Argentina); however, direct cross-cultural comparisons had not 2013), and their understanding of the safe driving climate among
been undertaken thus far. friends (Guggenheim and Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2016).
Previous cross-cultural studies tended to use the Driver Behaviour The MDSI assesses four broad driving styles: (a) reckless and care-
Questionnaire (DBQ; Reason et al., 1990), showing differences in re- less; (b) anxious; (c) angry and hostile; and (d) patient and careful. The
ported driving behaviours of drivers in culturally different countries reckless and careless driving style refers to deliberate violations of safe


Corresponding author at: The Louis and Gabi Weisfeld School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 52900, Israel.
E-mail address: taubman@biu.ac.il (O.T. Ben-Ari).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2017.08.031
Received 12 April 2017; Received in revised form 25 August 2017; Accepted 29 August 2017
Available online 11 October 2017
0001-4575/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

driving norms and the seeking of sensations and thrills while driving. It with feedback and encouraging comments for safe and considerate
is characterized by driving at high speeds, passing in no-passing zones, driving; Communication indicating direct and open communication
and driving while intoxicated. The anxious driving style reflects feelings between parents and adolescents on driving behavior in general, and
of alertness and tension, as well as ineffective engagement in relaxing especially on risky driving; Monitoring denoting the extent parents
activities during driving. The angry and hostile driving style refers to supervise their offspring, and how much they insist on being informed
expressions of irritation, rage, and hostility while driving, along with a of where youngsters are taking the car, who is going with them, and
tendency to act aggressively on the road, including cursing other dri- when they intend to be home; Commitment to safety relating to the
vers, honking the horn, or flashing headlights. The patient and careful comprehensive commitment parents hold towards road safety; Mes-
driving style reflects well-adjusted driving behaviors, such as planning sages referring to clarity of verbal safety messages from parents to
ahead, paying full attention to the road, displaying patience, courtesy, young drivers; and Limits indicating the magnitude of parental sys-
and calm behind the wheel, and obeying the traffic rules. These various tematic and clear-cut limits setting on adolescents' driving behavior and
driving styles were found to be systematically related to socio- the degree to which they discipline them for traffic violations
demographic variables, driving history elements, personality char- (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami, 2013).
acteristics, as well as features of the environment, such as family and Studies indicate that positive dimensions of the family climate are
peers’ safety-related attitudes and behavior (Taubman-Ben-Ari and related to safe driving among young drivers. More specifically, when
Skvirsky, 2016). parents are better role models, provide encouraging and empowering
Separation-individuation is a concept which represents the extent feedback for safe driving, enable more open communication, convey
to which a teenager relinquishes the power of internalized parents in clearer messages regarding safe driving, monitor their driving to higher
order to attain greater intrapsychic autonomy (Blos, 1979), and is extent, and tend to set clear limits on violating traffic laws, young
considered an important sign of adolescent development. Importantly, drivers tend to endorse a more patient and careful driving style and less
the disengagement from the internalized parents, should not come at reckless and careless and angry and hostile styles. On the other hand,
the expense of severing external family ties; both individuality and young drivers who perceive their parents to be uncommitted to safety
connectedness in parent–adolescent relationships are important for report more endorsement of reckless and careless and angry and hostile
optimal adolescent development (Grotevant and Cooper, 1998). The styles. Regarding the anxious driving style the results of previous stu-
separation-individuation process enables one to be psychologically in- dies are less unequivocal, showing that some positive aspects of FCRS
dependent yet at the same time more capable of establishing deep in- may relate to greater utilization of this driving style (Taubman-Ben-Ari,
timate relationships with others outside of the family unit, such as 2016; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami, 2012, 2013).
friends (Årseth et al., 2009). Notwithstanding the importance of the family, a third significant
More specifically, there appears to be two major components of the aspect which should be considered when discussing young drivers’
individuation process: A risk component, reflected by separation or driving styles is the central influence of their peers (e.g., Curry et al.,
emotional detachment from the family, and a protective component, 2012; Horvath et al. 2012 ; Winston and Jacobsohn, 2010). An attempt
reflected by the development of autonomy while maintaining intimacy to comprehensively understand this aspect was undertaken recently by
and connection to the family (Baer and Bray, 1999). As such, the se- Guggenheim and Taubman-Ben-Ari (2016) with the construction of a
paration-individuation concept entails alongside with healthy separa- new scale – the Safe Driving Climate among Friends (SDCaF), tapping
tion, also more problematical aspects, such as strong dependency needs, four dimensions of driving with friends: friends' pressure; social costs of
separation anxiety, need denial, high degree of narcissism and self- driving with friends; communication between friends about driving;
centeredness, and more (Levine, Green, and Millon 1986). Individua- and shared commitment to safe driving. These dimensions have been
tion is considered a developmental process that occurs with all ado- validated by significant associations, such as with self-disclosure (Miller
lescents regardless of ethnic background (Baer and Bray, 1999; Bray et al., 1983), resistance to peer influence (Steinberg and Monahan,
et al., 2000). 2007), the global tendency for peer pressure (Santor et al., 2000), and
It has been suggested that risky behaviors such as substance abuse, personal commitment to safe driving (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-
are partially a result of problems with the individuation process, and Ami, 2012).
that emotional isolation and a lack of differentiation from parents in- When attempting to compare two countries with different licensing
terfere with the adolescent's separation-individuation process. systems, it is important to review similarities and differences in the laws
Evaluations of this model with cross-sectional samples of a broad age of both countries: 1) In Israel, teenagers can begin driving lessons at the
range of adolescents provide support for the role of individuation in age of 16.5. In Queensland, teenagers can start their learning period at
alcohol use (Baer and Bray, 1999; Bray et al., 2000; Bray et al., 2001), 16 years old, and in both countries this is depending on passing the
so that higher healthy separation was related to lower alcohol con- learner theory test; 2) In Israel, lessons are given by professional in-
sumption. Although its importance has been proven in risky alcohol structors on specially equipped vehicles; learners are permitted to drive
use, to the best of our knowledge, this personal feature has never before only during their lessons. In Queensland, there is no obligation of taking
been tested in the context of driving styles, though its established as- lessons from professional instructors, but there is an obligation to log at
sociation with familial and social aspects may hint to its importance in least 100 hours on a logbook (including 10 hours at night); 3) A driving
this context as well. license is issued in Israel upon successfully passing an on-road driving
However, the personal level is not the only one that reflects on test. The road test cannot be taken until the learner has turned 16 and 9
young drivers’ driving style. One of the most important contributors, months and has completed a minimum of 28 on-road driving lessons. In
investigated in recent years is the model set by the family (e.g., Hartos Queensland, after a minimum of 1 year of accompanied driving, lear-
et al., 2000; Shope and Bingham, 2008; Gil et al., 2016). To assess the ners take a practical driving test that enables them to have a provisional
contribution of the family in the current study, we employed the Fa- license where restrictions are maintained on mobile phone use and
mily Climate for Road Safety (FCRS; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben- high-powered vehicles. After another minimum 1 year, holder of pro-
Ami, 2012, 2013), a multi-dimensional construct referring to the va- visional license take a hazard recognition test that allows them to earn
lues, perceptions, priorities, and practices of the family, and in parti- an open license; 4) Israeli new young drivers (under the age of 24), have
cular – the parents, in regard to safe driving. The seven dimensions to be accompanied by an experienced driver, someone over the age of
comprising the FCRS refer to different angles of this concept, with 24 who has held a valid driving license for a minimum of five years, for
Modeling reflecting the model given by parents’ driving, parents’ safety the first three months during day and night trips, and during the next
attitudes, as well as their tendency to obey traffic regulations; Feedback three months, only in night hours. New drivers under the age of 21 are
referring to the degree to which parents tend to provide their offspring restricted from carrying more than two passengers for a period of two

48
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

years after licensure, unless accompanied by an experienced driver. In implies violating deliberately road safety norms; (ii) anxious driving
Queensland, the accompanying person needs to have an open license that reflects a feeling of tension and distress behind the wheel; (iii)
for the same vehicle for at least 1 year, and every hour supervised by a angry and hostile driving that denotes irritability and aggression on the
professional instructor counts for 3 logged hours (with a maximum of road; (iv) patient and careful driving that indicates a well-adjusted
30 logged hours). Restrictions are posed to the use of mobile phones, driving style. The Cronbach’s Alpha reliability coefficients were rea-
including hands-free, on both learners and passengers; 5) In Israel, sonable for all four dimensions across both the Israeli and the
there are aggravations on traffic violations for novice young drivers. A Queensland group, respectively for reckless and careless driving (0.806
young novice driver who will be caught on violations such as driving and 0.865), anxious driving (0.829 and 0.861), angry and hostile
after alcohol consumption or without an accompanied driver, will lose driving (0.790 and 0.762), and patient and careful driving (0.775 and
his or her driving license and will have to pass an on-road driving test 0.727). Accordingly, scores for the four dimensions of the MDSI were
again. Notably, Queensland law imposes zero tolerance on alcohol for computed for each young driver as the average over the items asso-
all drivers until 25 years old (CARRS-Q, 2015; Israel Government ciated with each dimension.
Portal- gov., 2017; Ministry of Transport State of Israel, 2016). The Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA, Levine et al.,
In view of the literature review, we hypothesized that higher reports 1986) was used to measure different dimensions of the second se-
on the maladaptive driving styles (reckless and careless; angry and paration-individuation process. This self-report instrument consists
hostile; anxious), and lower endorsement of the careful and patient originally on 103 items, which are assessed on a five-point scale, ran-
driving style, will be associated with: a) lower healthy separation in- ging from “strongly agree” or “is always true of me” to “strongly dis-
dividuation, and higher levels of problematic separation-individuation, agree” or “is never true of me.” For the current study, we used 43 items
b) higher noncommitment of parents to safety and lower scores on the from four dimensions with respective Cronbach’s Alpha measures of
positive aspects of the family climate for road safety, and c) lower re- reliability reasonable across the dimensions for both the Israeli and the
ports on the communication and shared commitment dimensions of Queensland group: (i) practicing mirroring that relates to narcissistic
driving with friends, as well as higher reports of friends’ pressure and strivings (0.904 and 0.871); (ii) separation anxiety that links to sig-
social costs. Based on previous studies (Bener et al., 2013; Özkan et al., nificant others experienced as abandoning (0.824 and 0.837); (iii)
2006; Warner et al., 2011), we also examined exploratively the cross- nurturance seeking that links to strong caretaker attachment (0.716 and
cultural differences in the MDSI factors, as well as in the associations 0.717); (iv) healthy separation that reflects a balance of dependence
between the independent variables and the driving styles. and independence strivings (0.824 and 0.791). Accordingly, the four
SITA dimensions were computed for each young driver as the average
2. Methods scores over the items linked to each dimension.
The Family Climate for Road Safety Scale (FCRSS, Taubman-Ben-Ari
2.1. Participants and procedure and Katz-Ben-Ami, 2012, 2013) scale assesses the degree to which the
young drivers consider 54 items to apply to their family on a 5-point
This study proposed a multidimensional analysis of the relations scale ranging from “not at all” to “very much”. The items allow eliciting
between the four MDSI dimensions (Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004) and the perceptions of young drivers about their family attitudes and be-
the effects of the relationships between young drivers and parents as haviour with respect to road safety. Seven dimensions of family climate
well as peers for two samples of young drivers between 17 and 22 years were defined and the respective Cronbach’s Alpha measures of relia-
old, who participated voluntarily in surveys in Israel and Queensland. bility were reasonable for the Israeli and the Queensland groups re-
The two groups were administered the survey in their respective mother spectively: modelling (0.844 and 0.842), feedback (0.941 and 0.873),
tongue through an electronic data collection program, via social media, communication (0.895 and 0.869), monitoring (0.858 and 0.784), non-
and convenience sampling. They were asked questions concerning their commitment (0.637 and 0.789), messages (0.787 and 0.877), and limits
socio-demographic characteristics as well as about their driving styles, (0.803 and 0.795). Accordingly, scores for each FCRS dimension were
separation-individuation dimensions, and family and peer climate of computed for each young driver as the average over the items be-
road safety. longing to each dimension.
The two groups were similar in terms of size (Israeli sample The Safe Driving Climate among Friends (SDCaF) scale (Guggenheim
size = 160, Queensland sample size = 164) and gender distribution and Taubman-Ben-Ari, 2016) evaluates the effect of peers on the safety
(both the Israeli and the Queensland sample have 50.0% for male and of young drivers on four dimensions consisting on 19 items with a 5-
female young drivers). The two groups were different in terms of age point scale ranging from “completely disagree” to “completely agree”.
distribution (Israeli mean = 21.6 and st.dev. = 1.9 years old, The respective Cronbach’s Alpha measures of reliability were reason-
Queensland mean = 19.5 and st.dev. = 1.7), household size (Israeli able across the dimensions for both the Israeli and the Queensland
mean = 4.8 and st.dev. = 1.7, Queensland mean = 3.3 and group: (i) pressure from friends while driving (0.736 and 0.814), (ii)
st.dev. = 1.4), parents’ marital status (15.6% of Israeli parents are se- social costs of driving with friends (0.868 and 0.818), (iii) commu-
parated or divorced versus 34.8% of Queensland parents), and vehicle nication about driving between friends (0.749 and 0.786), and (iv)
ownership (40.6% for Israeli young drivers versus 62.2% for shared commitment to safe driving with friends (0.815 and 0.781).
Queensland). When looking at having experienced road crashes with at Accordingly, scores for the four dimensions of the SDCaF scale were
least one light injury, similarities were reported in regard to whether calculated for each young driver as the average over the items asso-
the young drivers had acquaintances who suffered injuries (56.3% for ciated with each dimension.
Israelis and 56.7% for Queensland), although differences were men-
tioned in regard to whether the young drivers themselves have suffered 2.3. Model
injuries (16.9% for Israelis and 22.6% for Queensland).
This study measured the association of the four MDSI dimensions
2.2. Instruments with the perceptions of the young driver about him/herself and the
safety climate of parents and peers. Accordingly, it should be noted that
The MDSI (Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004) assesses the driving styles the model selection should satisfy the following known criteria for
of the young drivers through their answers to 44 statements about their model formulation: (i) the model should allow for a multidimensional
feelings and behaviors while driving. Responses are marked on a 6- analysis of the concurrent factors affecting the MDSI dimensions; (ii)
point scale ranging from “not at all” to “very much”. The inventory the model should allow for linear regression of the factors on the cal-
consists of four driving styles: (i) reckless and careless driving that culated values of the MDSI dimensions; (iii) the model should capture

49
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

the well-known correlations across the MDSI dimensions; (iv) the model where yj are (n × 1) vectors of scores for the MDSI dimension j, Xj are
should be robust to the relatively small sample size for both the Israeli (n × vj) matrices for vj observed variables, βj are (vj × 1) vectors of
and the Queensland groups. parameters to be estimated, and uj are (n × 1) vectors of error terms. It
The first three criteria indicated the need for a model where linear should be noted that the four equations may have different independent
regressions could be correlated, while the last criteria suggested the variables and error term variances, and that the four diagonal elements
need for Bayesian modelling that relaxes the very restrictive assumption as well as the six off-diagonal elements of the covariance matrix Ω are
that frequentist modelling poses when sample sizes are small. The parameters to be estimated (Zellner and Ando, 2010). It should also be
model that satisfies all these four criteria is the seemingly unrelated noted that two different SUR models were estimated separately for the
regression (SUR) model (Zellner, 1962). The SUR model enabled us to Israeli and the Queensland groups.
express the linear variation of four dependent continuous variables The specification of the model considered as observed variables the
while accounting for their correlation, in light of previous findings seven dimensions of the FCRS, the four dimensions of the SDCaF, the
about the four MDSI dimensions (Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004; four dimensions of the SITA, and the socio-demographic characteristics
Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky, 2016). Moreover, the estimation of the of the young drivers. The model estimation consisted of the DMC ap-
SUR model was performed with a Bayesian approach with a direct proach that allowed obtaining posterior marginal densities from prior
Monte Carlo (DMC) approach (Zellner and Ando, 2010), given that the marginal densities for the parameters to be estimated, after re-
procedure has demonstrated its ability and efficiency to retrieve sig- formulating the SUR models as follows (Zellner and Ando, 2010):
nificant relations even with small sample sizes of 50 observations.
Lastly, the Bayesian approach removed any assumption on the dis- ⎧ y1 = X1 β1 + e1 with u1 = e1
tribution of the error terms, a desired feature when having small sample ⎪ y2 = X2 β2 + ρ21 u1 + e2
sizes and non-normal dependent variables. ⎨ y3 = X3 β3 + ρ31 u1 + ρ32 u2 + e3
Given the average scores yj of each MDSI dimensions for each young ⎪
⎩ y4 = X 4 β4 + ρ41 u1 + ρ42 u2 + ρ43 u3 + e4
male driver n, the linear SUR model of the four scores formulates four
0 (i ≠ j )
regression equations with correlated error terms having different var- with E [e1e'j] = ⎧ 2 and Σ = diag [σ12, σ22, σ32, σ42]
iances (Zellner and Ando, 2010): ⎨
⎩ iI
σ (i = j ) (3)
y = Xβ + u with u ∼ N (0, Ω ⊗ I ) (1) The DMC is applicable and the sampling in sequence is quite simple
to perform because the parameters of the second equation are now
⎧ y1 = X1 β1 + u1 related to the ones of the first one, the parameters of the third equation
⎪ y2 = X2 β2 + u2
are now related to the ones of the first two, and the parameters of the
⎨ y3 = X3 β3 + u3 fourth equation are now related to the ones of the previous three (for

⎩ y4 = X 4 β4 + u 4 details about the sampling procedure, see Zellner and Ando, 2010). The
ωij I (i ≠ j ) DMC estimation procedure did not require prior knowledge on the
with E [u1u'j] = ⎧ 2 parameter distributions and hence Jeffrey’s invariant priors (Jeffreys,
⎨ ωi I (i = j ) (2)
⎩ 1946) were suitable highly non-informative priors for the parameters to

Table 1
Sample characteristics.

Israel Queensland

Variable mean st.dev. min max mean st.dev. min max


MDSI − reckless and careless 2.446 0.756 1.000 5.090 3.203 0.979 1.000 5.091
MDSI − anxious 2.109 0.599 1.110 3.950 3.139 0.787 1.526 4.789
MDSI − angry and hostile 2.474 1.096 1.000 6.000 3.102 1.058 1.000 5.600
MDSI − patient and careful 4.567 0.752 2.110 5.890 4.039 0.699 2.111 5.889
FCRS − modelling 3.755 0.703 1.730 5.000 3.294 0.592 2.000 5.000
FCRS − feedback 3.200 1.255 1.000 5.000 3.224 0.779 1.400 5.000
FCRS − communication 3.389 0.971 1.000 5.000 3.348 0.656 1.889 4.889
FCRS − monitoring 2.690 1.034 1.000 4.860 3.146 0.779 1.143 5.000
FCRS − non commitment 1.934 0.581 1.000 3.880 2.666 0.624 1.000 3.875
FCRS − messages 4.361 0.637 2.000 5.000 3.541 0.665 2.250 5.000
FCRS − limits 3.038 0.976 1.000 5.000 3.297 0.716 1.333 5.000
SDCaF − friends' pressure 1.429 0.556 1.000 3.200 2.599 0.936 1.000 4.600
SDCaF − social cost of driving 1.721 0.836 1.000 4.600 2.617 0.842 1.000 4.600
SDCaF − communication 3.903 0.789 1.000 5.000 3.427 0.668 1.400 4.800
SDCaF − shared commitment 3.114 0.949 1.000 5.000 3.090 0.817 1.000 5.000
SITA − mirroring 2.993 0.664 1.000 4.670 2.969 0.631 1.000 4.800
SITA − anxiety 2.608 0.659 1.000 4.290 2.819 0.623 1.000 4.143
SITA − nurturance seeking 2.971 0.795 1.000 5.000 2.760 0.688 1.000 4.286
SITA − healthy separation 4.120 0.681 1.000 5.000 3.306 0.706 1.000 4.857
Female 0.500 0.502 0.000 1.000 0.500 0.502 0.000 1.000
Age 21.631 1.889 17.000 24.000 19.549 1.731 17.000 22.000
Household size 4.756 1.711 1.000 11.000 3.341 1.433 1.000 7.000
Household siblings 2.625 1.605 0.000 10.000 1.512 1.313 0.000 6.000
Parents married 0.794 0.406 0.000 1.000 0.530 0.501 0.000 1.000
Parents separated 0.006 0.079 0.000 1.000 0.177 0.383 0.000 1.000
Parents divorced 0.150 0.358 0.000 1.000 0.171 0.377 0.000 1.000
Single parent household 0.050 0.219 0.000 1.000 0.122 0.328 0.000 1.000
Own vehicle 0.406 0.493 0.000 1.000 0.622 0.486 0.000 1.000
Injured in a crash 0.169 0.376 0.000 1.000 0.226 0.419 0.000 1.000
Acquaintance injured in a crash 0.563 0.498 0.000 1.000 0.567 0.497 0.000 1.000

Note: MDSI scale 1–6; FCRS, SITA and SDCaF scales 1–5.

50
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

be estimated. The DMC estimation procedure generated 10,000 pos- Table 2


terior samples for the computation of the means, standard deviations Bayesian estimates of the SUR models.
and confidence intervals at the 90% and 95% level for each element of
Variables Israel Queensland
the vectors βj as well as each element of the covariance matrix Ω. Lastly,
posterior simulations from the posterior marginal densities allowed mean st.dev. sig. mean st.dev. sig.
computing empirically vectors of predicted and residual values. Given
MDSI reckless and careless
the average var(res) of the residuals and the average var(pred) of the
FCRSS − feedback – – −0.133 0.086 *
predictions over the sample for each of the four equations in the two FCRSS − communication −0.147 0.074 **
−0.223 0.114 *

models, an empirical proportion of variance explained for each equa- FCRSS − non commitment 0.291 0.113 **
0.323 0.136 **

tion in each model was then calculated as 1–var(res)/var(pred) (Gelman FCRSS − limits −0.142 0.073 *
– –
and Pardoe, 2006). SDCaF − friends' pressure 0.249 0.104 **
0.498 0.078 **

SDCaF − social cost of 0.133 0.065 **


– –
driving
3. Results SDCaF − communication – – −0.199 0.096 **

SDCaF − shared −0.144 0.060 **


−0.200 0.077 **

3.1. Sample characteristics commitment


SITA − mirroring 0.165 0.083 **
0.331 0.090 **

SITA − nurturance seeking −0.208 0.113 *


−0.166 0.092 *
The two groups consisted of 160 Israeli and 160 Queensland young SITA − healthy separation −0.154 0.092 *
– –
drivers. When looking at their average scores on the self-reported Female −0.062 0.034 *
−0.062 0.037 *

measures and recalling that higher average scores on each dimension Parents separated or – – 0.195 0.126 *

reflect the young drivers’ perception and behaviour on that specific divorced
Own vehicle 0.245 0.101 **
– –
dimension, differences emerge between the two groups that are worth *
Constant 1.084 0.599 0.503 0.397
observing (Table 1).
MDSI anxious
The dependent variables, namely the four MDSI dimensions, are
FCRSS − communication −0.133 0.062 **
−0.222 0.097 **

significantly skewed for both groups with a fatter tail on the right hand FCRSS − non commitment 0.166 0.085 *
– –
side of the reckless and careless, anxious and angry and hostile style FCRSS − limits −0.148 0.055 **
−0.179 0.086 **

distributions, and a fatter tail on the left hand side of the patient and SDCaF − friends' pressure 0.234 0.078 **
0.303 0.060 **

careful style distribution. This demonstrates that the SUR model is SDCaF − social cost of 0.120 0.049 **
0.108 0.064 *

driving
appropriate for the task at hand of estimating the variation of mildly- SDCaF − communication – – −0.214 0.074 **

tailed distributed variables, given that this model does not assume the SITA − mirroring 0.133 0.062 **
0.162 0.069 **

error terms to be normally distributed and its performance is good also SITA − anxiety 0.326 0.063 **
0.145 0.088 *

with heavy-tailed error distributions (Ng, 2002). When comparing the SITA − nurturance seeking −0.088 0.051 *
−0.181 0.079 **

SITA − healthy separation −0.131 0.069 *


−0.105 0.067 *
scores on the MDSI dimensions, Queensland young drivers appear to
Female 0.204 0.075 **
– –
score higher in the reckless and careless, anxious and angry and hostile Parents separated or 0.192 0.102 *
– –
dimensions, while Israeli young drivers seem to score higher in the divorced
patient and careful dimension. Own vehicle – – 0.392 0.157 **

* **
The scores on the FCRS dimensions show comparable perceptions of Constant 1.072 0.560 0.793 0.304

young drivers belonging to the two groups, with difference emerging in MDSI angry and hostile
that Queensland young drivers feel that their parents are more active in FCRSS − feedback −0.146 0.088 *
−0.237 0.115 **

FCRSS − communication −0.152 0.091 *


−0.210 0.101 **
monitoring and setting limits to their driving, while Israeli young dri-
FCRSS − non commitment 0.257 0.125 **
0.195 0.109 *

vers feel that their parents are better role models and are more com- SDCaF − friends' pressure – – 0.307 0.093 **

mitted to discussing road safety within the family. Similarly, the scores SDCaF − social cost of 0.198 0.107 **
– –
on the SDCaF dimensions illustrate that Israeli young drivers experience driving
SDCaF − shared −0.228 0.100 **
−0.180 0.093 **
less pressure, associate less cost and communicate more about driving
commitment
with their friends, while the level of commitment is comparable to the SITA − mirroring 0.234 0.138 *
0.359 0.108 **

one experienced by the Queensland young drivers. Lastly, the scores on SITA − nurturance seeking −0.208 0.113 *
– –
the SITA dimensions suggest that Israeli young drivers live a relation SITA − healthy separation −0.332 0.152 **
−0.177 0.104 *

with their parents and peers characterised by lower anxiety, higher Parents separated or – – 0.245 0.134 *

divorced
level of nurturing, and healthier style of separation to reach in- **
Constant 1.273 0.992 2.056 0.476
dependence.
MDSI patient and careful
FCRSS − modelling 0.166 0.096 *
0.210 0.103 **
3.2. Model estimates FCRSS − feedback 0.090 0.051 *
– –
FCRSS − communication 0.230 0.079 **
0.209 0.098 **

The DMC estimation of the linear SUR model aimed at finding the FCRSS − non commitment −0.252 0.109 **
– –
best specification of the four equations representing the relation be- FCRSS − limits – – 0.212 0.087 **

SDCaF − friends' pressure −0.221 0.100 **


−0.118 0.060 **
tween the four MDSI dimensions and the scores of the young drivers on
SDCaF − social cost of −0.135 0.062 **
– –
the aforementioned instruments as well as the socio-demographic driving
characteristics of the young drivers. The best specification of the SDCaF − communication 0.136 0.074 *
– –
equations was found after testing for significance of the independent SDCaF − shared – – 0.121 0.060 **

commitment
variables in the four equations and comparing the deviance information
SITA − mirroring −0.143 0.080 *
−0.155 0.069 **
criterion (DIC) of the models (Spiegelhalter et al., 2002) for both the SITA − nurturance seeking 0.140 0.065 **
– –
Israeli and the Queensland samples. Namely, the independent variables SITA − healthy separation – – 0.122 0.067 *

** **
that are not presented in each equation were not significantly corre- Female 0.114 0.032 0.067 0.028
lated with the variation of the scores on the MDSI dimensions. Own vehicle −0.217 0.097 **
−0.328 0.090 **

** **
Constant 3.529 0.719 1.077 0.307
The best model specifications are presented in Table 2 for both
samples. The posterior distributions of the estimated parameters are covariance matrix of error terms
(continued on next page)
presented in terms of posterior means, standard deviations and

51
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

Table 2 (continued) When looking at the specific styles, similarities emerge across the
two samples: (i) the reckless and careless style is associated with lack of
Variables Israel Queensland
communication and non-commitment to road safety in the family,
mean st.dev. sig. mean st.dev. sig. higher peer pressure and lower peer commitment to safe driving, as
well as higher narcissistic needs accompanied by higher nurturance
ω11 1.413 0.299 **
1.421 0.294 **
requirements; (ii) the anxious style is linked to lower tendency of the
ω22 1.203 0.286 **
1.243 0.271 **
family to communicate and provide boundaries to the young drivers,
ω33 1.243 0.285 **
1.230 0.274 **

ω44 1.531 0.299 **


1.491 0.300 ** higher peer pressure and perceived social costs of driving with friends,
ω12 0.205 0.210 0.673 0.208 ** higher anxiety in the relation with the parents, and lower search for
ω13 0.601 0.210 **
0.807 0.185 **
nurturing and healthy separation from the parents and friends; (iii) the
ω14 −0.668 0.214 **
−0.673 0.209 **
angry and hostile style is related to a lower level of communication and
ω23 0.356 0.219 *
0.555 0.186 **

ω24 −0.626 0.214 **


−0.655 0.185 ** feedback provision by the family, a lack of commitment to safe driving
ω34 −0.448 0.239 **
−0.451 0.182 ** by both family and peers, higher narcissistic needs and lower healthy
independence from parents and friends; (iv) the patient and careful
* statistically significant difference from zero (90% credible set shows the same sign). style is connected to positive role modelling in the family, good com-
** statistically significant difference from zero (95% credible set shows the same sign). munication with the parents, lower peer pressure, and lower narcissistic
needs. Interestingly, for both samples the age of the survey participants
was not found related to the scores on the MDSI dimensions.
significance levels for the constant α, the parameters β, and the terms In addition, when looking at the specific styles, some differences
ωij of the covariance matrix. Moreover, the post-estimation analysis emerge in the instrument dimensions. The reckless and careless style is
estimated the proportion of variance explained for the four equations associated negatively with the imposition of limits by the family in the
across the two samples: for the Israeli sample, the proportion of var- Israeli sample and the provision of feedback by the family in the
iance explained was estimated as equal to 58.8% for the reckless and Queensland sample. The same style is also related to higher perceptions
careless style, 66.0% for the anxious style, 54.1% for the angry and of social costs of driving and lower levels of healthy separation in the
hostile style, and 32.9% for the patient and careful style; for the Israeli sample, and lower level of communication with friends and
Queensland sample, the proportion of variance explains was estimated higher nurturance seeking with parents and friends in the Queensland
as equal to 72.8% for the reckless and careless style, 75.9% for the sample. The lack of communication with friends is also related to the
anxious style, 67.6% for the angry and hostile style, and 71.0% for the anxious style and the peer pressure is associated with the angry and
patient and careful style. hostile style in the Queensland sample, while perceptions of the social
It should be noted that the estimation of the covariance matrix al- cost of driving appear linked to the angry and hostile style for the Israeli
lows the computation of the correlation matrix across MDSI dimensions sample. The patient and careful driving style is related to higher pro-
for both samples. Table 3 presents the correlation across the dimen- vision of feedback, lower perception of social costs, higher commu-
sions. It shows how unobserved correlation of the error terms of the nication with friends about safe driving, and nurturance seeking be-
four equations is more pronounced for the Queensland sample. More haviour in the Israeli sample. The same driving style is related to the
importantly, it shows that the correlation patterns are logical definition of limits by the family and the commitment to safe driving
(Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky, 2016) being shared with peers in the Queensland sample.
and significant, and hence support the formulation of the SUR model When looking at the socio-economic characteristics, males score
with the four equations estimated simultaneously. It should be noted higher than females in the reckless and careless style while the opposite
that having taken care of the unobserved correlation between dimen- applies to the anxious (only for the Israeli sample though) and patient
sions makes the parameter estimates unbiased. and careful styles. Owning a vehicle makes young drivers tend towards
When looking at the estimates of the two SUR models, it seems that more reckless and careless and less patient and careful driving in the
similarities may be observed in the relations across the two samples, but Israeli sample, and more anxious and less patient and careful driving in
also differences may be highlighted in terms of what dimensions from the Queensland sample. Having separated or divorced parents is posi-
the instruments and which socio-demographic characteristics are sig- tively related to higher scores in the reckless and careless and anxious
nificantly related to the MDSI dimensions. styles for the Israeli sample, as well as higher scores in the anxious and
A general pattern is that the driving styles that relate to risky be- angry and hostile styles for the Queensland sample. Age was not sig-
haviour such as the reckless and careless, the anxious, and the angry nificant for.
and hostile (Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2016), are associated with lower
family tendency to engage in promoting road safety, higher pressure 4. Discussion and conclusions
and cost of driving with peers, and unhealthier separation aspects. The
opposite is observed for the patient and careful driving style that relates This cross-cultural comparison between Israeli and Queensland
to higher engagement of the family in road safety, lower pressure from young drivers yielded an interesting result showing that Queensland
friends, and healthier separation. young drivers tend to report higher reckless, angry and anxious driving

Table 3
Correlation matrix of the error terms of the four equations in the SUR models.

MDSI Israel Queensland

Reckless and careless Anxious Angry and hostile Patient and careful Reckless and careless Anxious Angry and hostile Patient and careful

Reckless 1.000 1.000


Anxious 0.157 1.000 0.507 1.000
Angry 0.453 0.291 1.000 0.611 0.449 1.000
Careful −0.454 −0.461 −0.325 1.000 −0.463 −0.481 −0.333 1.000

Note: the correlation terms are statistically significantly different from zero at the 95% level.

52
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

styles, whereas Israeli young drivers seem to report higher patient and Some limitations of the current study should be mentioned with the
careful driving. In addition, an examination of the FCRS dimensions most pronounced being the fact that the samples were not nationally
shows that Queensland young drivers experience their parents as more representative, and that all measures were based on self-reports.
active in monitoring and setting limits to their driving, whereas Israeli However, studies have already attested to self-report measures as valid
young drivers perceive their parents as better role models and as more instruments investigating risk taking behavior. More specifically, pre-
committed to discussing road safety within the family. Similarly, the vious findings connected the MDSI scores with performance measures
scores on the SDCaF dimensions illustrate that Israeli young drivers collected in a driving simulator experiment (driving speed, number of
experience less pressure, associate less cost and communicate more driving maneuvers, and passing gaps; Farah et al., 2007; Farah et al.,
about driving with their friends, while the level of commitment is 2009) and using an In-Vehicle Data Recorder (IVDR; Taubman-Ben-Ari
comparable to the one experienced by the Queensland young drivers. et al., 2016). Nevertheless, the most important asset of the current
Lastly, the scores on the SITA dimensions suggest that Israeli young study is the ability to validate results using two culturally-different
drivers reached a higher level of separation-individuation. The picture samples. Future studies would do well to adopt this line of investiga-
being drawn here is a coherent one, with those drivers who are more tion, with more attention drawn to cross-cultural comparisons, re-
mature and with tighter positive connections with parents and peers, presenting diverse populations, and additional countries.
also the ones to report safer driving. It is also interesting to note that the To conclude, the findings of the current study point to the im-
safe driving is not related in this group to stricter discipline such as portance of cross-cultural comparisons as indicators of the universality
limit setting, but to better communication and mutual respect. In con- of various driving styles, as well as the factors that might contribute to
trast, the more maladaptive driving styles may reflect the fact that more them. They also attest to the significance of looking at multi-dimen-
Australians in this sample reported having divorced parents and owning sional driving styles and not only on the uni-dimension aspect of risky
their own car, thus they may be less dependent on their parents in this driving. The findings demonstrate the universal utility of the MDSI,
regard. The differences indicated here between the groups may well together with the understanding that only a wider examination of
represent cultural variations, as Israel is a more familistic and col- personal and environmental contributors enables true insights into the
lectivistic culture compared to other western countries, with more complex behavior of driving among young drivers. It highlights the fact
close-knit ties, greater intergenerational interdependence and intimacy that both personal aspects, family and friends play a role in youngsters’
among Israeli families than, for example among German ones risk taking, pointing to the need to integratively consider these external
(Kagitcibasi et al., 2010) forces in any intervention aimed at moderating risks commonly taken
More importantly, despite differences in magnitude of scores, the by young drivers. They also highpoint the positive aspects of shared
associations between the study variables and the driving styles follow family and friends’ commitment to safety, which may be key elements
previous findings (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky, 2016), showing that in such interventions. Such findings indicate that even if some cultural
generally, maladaptive driving styles (the reckless and careless, an- differences in driving styles do exist in different countries, still, friends
xious, and angry and hostile) are associated for both samples with lower and family are crucial elements in young drivers’ safety. This compre-
parental commitment to road safety and lower communication with hension should set the grounds for campaigns rising awareness to safe
them in issues concerning safe vs. risky driving, resembling earlier driving targeting both parents and young drivers.
findings with the MDSI Israel (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami,
2012, 2013) and with a risky driving score in Belgium (Carpentier et al., References
2014). These styles are also connected to higher pressure from friends
and a higher perception of peers as being collectively uncommitted to Årseth, A.K., Kroger, J., Martinussen, M., Bakken, G., 2009. Intimacy status, attachment,
safe driving, echoing previous findings (e.g., Curry et al., 2012; Horvath separation-individuation patterns, and identity status in female university students. J.
Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 26, 697–712.
et al., 2012; Winston and Jacobsohn, 2010). Finally, the elements of Baer, P.E., Bray, J.H., 1999. Adolescent individuation and alcohol usage. J. Stud. Alcohol
separation-individuation indicate that the maladaptive driving styles 13, 52–62.
are related to less mature connections between young drivers and their Bener, A., Verjee, M., Dafeeah, E.E., Yousafzai, M.T., Mari, S., Hassib, A., Lajunen, T.,
2013. A cross ‘ethnical’ comparison of the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (DBQ) in
family and friends, especially highlighting their association with the an economically fast developing country. Glob. J. Health Sci. 5, 165–175.
narcissistic aspect of this concept, following previous findings regarding Blos, P., 1979. The Adolescent Passage. International Universities Press, New York.
the relation between narcissistic tendencies and risky behaviour Bray, J.H., Baer, P.E., Getz, J.G., 2000. Adolescent individuation and alcohol use in
multiethnic youth. J. Stud. Alcohol 61, 588–597.
(Buelow and Brunell, 2014; Schreer, 2002). Such findings are also in Bray, J.H., Adams, G.J., Getz, G., Baer, P.E., 2001. Developmental, family, and ethnic
line with the claim that risky behaviors are partially a result of pro- influences on adolescent alcohol usage: a growth curve approach. J. Fam. Psychol.
blems with the individuation process, and that emotional isolation and 15, 301–314.
Buelow, M.T., Brunell, A.B., 2014. Facets of grandiose narcissism predict involvement in
a lack of differentiation from parents interfere with the adolescent's
health-risk behaviors. Pers. Individ. Differ. 69, 193–198.
separation-individuation process (e.g., Baer and Bray, 1999; Bray et al., CARRS-Q, 2015. Graduated Driver Licensing. State of the Road. www.carrsq.qut.edu.au.
2000; Bray et al., 2001). Carpentier, A., Brijs, K., Declercq, K., Brijs, T., Daniels, S., Wets, G., 2014. The effect of
The opposite was found for the patient and careful driving style. family climate on risky driving of young novices: the moderating role of attitude and
locus of control. Accid. Anal. Prev. 73, 53–64.
This style was related to higher engagement of the family in promoting Curry, A.E., Mirman, J.H., Kallan, M.J., Winston, F.K., Durbin, D.R., 2012. Peer passen-
road safety, lower pressure from friends, and healthier separation from gers: how do they affect teen crashes? J. Adolesc. Health 50, 588–594.
parents. These findings validate prior studies which found a link be- Farah, H., Polus, A., Bekhor, S., Toledo, T., 2007. Study of passing gap acceptance be-
havior using a driving simulator. Adv. Transp. Stud. Int. J. 23–31 (Special Issue).
tween these variables and less risk taking (Baer and Bray, 1999; Bray Farah, H., Bekhor, S., Polus, A., Toledo, T., 2009. A passing gap acceptance model for
et al., 2001; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Katz-Ben-Ami, 2012, 2013). two- lane rural highways. Transportmetrica 5, 159–172.
When looking at background variables, the most evidenced is the Gelman, A., Pardoe, I., 2006. Bayesian measures of explained variance and pooling in
multilevel (hierarchical) models. Technometrics 48, 241–251.
association between gender and driving styles, with women endorsing Gil, S., Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Toledo, T., 2016. A multidimensional intergenerational
more patient and careful and less reckless and careless driving styles model of young males’ driving styles. Accid. Anal. Prev. 97, 141–145.
than men. This is in accordance with other studies showing that men Grotevant, H.D., Cooper, C.R., 1998. Individuality and connectedness in adolescent de-
velopment: review and prospects for research on identity, relationships, and context.
tend to take more risks behind the wheel (Holland et al., 2010; Poó
In: Skoe, E.E.A., von der Lippe, A.L. (Eds.), Personality Development in Adolescence:
et al., 2013; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Yehiel, 2012; Taubman-Ben-Ari and A Cross National and Lifespan Perspective. Taylor & Francis/Routledge, Florence, KY,
Skvirsky, 2016). Overall, it seems that there are more similarities than pp. 3–32.
Guggenheim, N., Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., 2016. Safe Driving Climate Among Friends
differences in the associations between the independent variables and
(SDCaF): A New Scale. (Under Review).
the four driving styles for both cultures, and that the “bigger picture” Hartos, J.L., Eitel, P., Haynie, D.L., Simons-Morton, B.G., 2000. Can I take the car?: re-
seems to be quite the same. lations among parenting practices and adolescent problem-driving practices. J.

53
V. Skvirsky et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 109 (2017) 47–54

Adolesc. Res. 15, 352–367. aggressive driving? North Am. J. Psychol. 4, 333–342.
Holland, C., Geraghty, J., Shah, K., 2010. Differential moderating effect of locus of control Shope, J.T., Bingham, C.R., 2008. Teen driving: motor-vehicle crashes and factors that
on effect of driving experience in young male and female drivers. Pers. Individ. Differ. contribute. Am. J. Prev. Med. 35, 261–271.
48, 821–826. Spiegelhalter, D., Best, N., Carling, B.P., Linde, A.V.D., 2002. Bayesian measures of model
Horvath, C., Lewis, I., Watson, B., 2012. Peer passenger identity and passenger pressure complexity and fit. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Stat. Methodol.) 64, 583–639.
on young drivers’ speeding intentions. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 15, Steinberg, L., Monahan, K.C., 2007. Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Devel.
52–64. Psy. 43, 1531–1543.
Israel Government Portal-gov. http://oldgov.gov.il/firstgov/english. Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Katz-Ben-Ami, L., 2012. The contribution of family climate for road
Jeffreys, H., 1946. An invariant form for the prior probability in estimation problems. safety and aspects of the social environment to the reported driving behavior of
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. A: Math. Phys. Sci. 186, 453–461. young drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 47, 1–10.
Kagitcibasi, C., Ataca, B., Diri, A., 2010. Intergenerational relationships in the family: Taubman- Ben-Ari, O., Katz- Ben-Ami, L., 2013. Family climate for road safety: a new
ethnic socioeconomic, and country variations in Germany, Israel, Palestine, and concept and measure. Accid. Anal. Prev. 54, 1–14.
Turkey. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 41, 652–670. Taubman- Ben-Ari, O., Skvirsky, V., 2016. The multidimensional driving style inventory a
Levine, J., Green, C., Millon, T., 1986. The separation-individuation test of adolescence. J. decade later: review of the literature and re-evaluation of the scale. Accid. Anal. Prev.
Pers. Assess. 50, 123–137. 93, 179–188.
Miller, L.C., Berg, J.H., Archer, R.L., 1983. Openers: Individuals who elicit intimate self- Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Yehiel, D., 2012. Driving styles and their associations with per-
disclosure. J. Personal. Social Psychol. 44, 1234–1244. sonality and motivation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 45, 416–422.
Ministry of Transport, State of Israel. http://rishuy.mot.gov.il/en/. Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., 2004. The multidimensional driving
Ng, V.M., 2002. Robust Bayesian inference for seemingly unrelated regressions with el- style inventory -scale construct and validation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 36, 323–332.
liptical errors. J. Multivar. Anal. 83, 409–414. Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Eherenfreund-Hager, A., Prato, C.G., 2016. The value of self-report
Nordfjarn, T., Simsekoglu, O., Rundmo, T., 2014. Culture related to road traffic safety: a measures as indicators of driving behaviors among young drivers. Transp. Res. Part F:
comparison of eight countries using two conceptualisations of culture. Accid. Anal. Traff. Psychol. Behav. 39, 33–42.
Prev. 62, 319–328. Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., 2016. Parents’ perceptions of the family climate for road safety:
Özkan, T., Lajunen, T., Chliaoutakis, J., Parker, D., Summala, H., 2006. Cross-cultural associations with parents’ self-efficacy and attitudes toward accompanied driving,
differences on driving behaviours: a comparison of six countries. Transp. Res. Part F and teens’ driving styles. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 40, 14–22.
9, 227–242. Warner, H.W., Özkan, T., Lajunen, T., Tzamalouka, G., 2011. Cross-cultural comparison
Poó, F.M., Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Ledesma, R.D., Díaz-Lázaro, C.M., 2013. Reliability and of drivers’ tendency to commit different aberrant driving behaviours. Transp. Res.
validity of a Spanish-language version of the multidimensional driving style in- Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 14, 390–399.
ventory. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 17, 75–87. Winston, F.K., Jacobsohn, L., 2010. A practical approach for applying best practices in
Reason, J.T., Manstead, A.S.R., Stradling, S., Baxter, J., Campbell, K., 1990. Errors and behavioural interventions to injury prevention. Inj. Prev. 16, 107–112.
violations on the roads: a real distinction? Ergonomics 33, 1315–1332. Zellner, A., 1962. An efficient method of estimating seemingly unrelated regression
Santor, D.A., Messervey, D., Kusumakar, V., 2000. Measuring peer pressure, popularity, equations and tests for aggregation bias. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 57, 348–368.
and conformity in adolescent boys and girls: Predicting school performance, sexual Zellner, A., Ando, T., 2010. A direct Monte Carlo approach for Bayesian analysis of the
attitudes, and substance abuse. J. Youth Adolesc. 29, 163–182. seemingly unrelated regression model. J. Econometr. 159, 33–45.
Schreer, G.E., 2002. Narcissism and aggression: is inflated self-esteem related to

54

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen