Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

LSHSS

Research Article

Interactive Storybook Reading Instruction


for Preschoolers Who Are Deaf and
Hard of Hearing: A Multiple Probe
Across Behaviors Analysis
Jessica W. Trussell,a Janna Hasko,b Joy Kane,c Brittany Amari,a and Alison Brusehabera

Purpose: Interactive storybook reading (ISR) improves the Results: A functional relation was established between ISR
picture labeling vocabulary of children who are deaf and and the increase in the preschoolers’ word labeling and
hard of hearing (DHH). Vocabulary knowledge consistently meaning knowledge. The preschoolers’ word knowledge
predicts the later reading achievement of children who are was generalized and was maintained over time.
DHH. In this study, ISR was modified to include teaching Conclusions: ISR may be an effective vocabulary labeling
word meanings along with the vocabulary picture label. and word meaning instructional strategy for young children
Method: A multiple probe across behaviors single-case who are DHH and use spoken English. Teachers and related
experimental design was implemented to determine the service providers who work with this population may want
effects of ISR with word meaning instruction on picture to implement ISR with word meaning in 1-to-1 or small
labeling and word meaning knowledge of 6 preschoolers groups to individualize the target vocabulary and maximize
who are DHH and use spoken English. The student and the benefit. Future researchers should replicate this to
teacher participants engaged in ISR for 15–20 min a day, expand its generalizability to other subpopulations of
4 days a week for 3 weeks. children who are DHH.

E
arly vocabulary knowledge is a predictor of later 2016; grapheme–phoneme correspondence: Tucci, Trussell,
reading ability for students who are deaf and hard & Easterbrooks, 2014). These reviews noted that vocabu-
of hearing (DHH; Kyle & Harris, 2010). Some lary is the most heavily examined reading component.
young children who are DHH have more difficulty acquir- This finding seemed promising until the definition of vocab-
ing new vocabulary knowledge and decreased opportu- ulary and the outcomes of these studies are looked at more
nities to learn new words (Trezek, Wang, & Paul, 2011). carefully.
Undoubtedly, teachers and parents need to pay attention Vocabulary is a repository of words and their mean-
to vocabulary learning for this population. Several litera- ings that students use to plan expressive language, under-
ture reviews examined research conducted with students stand receptive language, and comprehend written text
who are DHH regarding the component areas of reading (Moats, 2005). Luckner and Cooke (2010) discovered that,
(see reviews of vocabulary: Luckner & Cooke, 2010; reading of 41 vocabulary studies with students who are DHH, only
comprehension: Luckner & Handley, 2008; fluency: Luckner 10 were intervention studies. Even fewer addressed learn-
& Urbach, 2011; morphology: Trussell & Easterbrooks, ing new words as well as their meanings (2/10; Paatsch,
Blamey, Sarant, & Bow, 2006; Robbins & Hatcher, 1981).
a
Since Luckner and Cooke, there have been three additional
National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of intervention studies (Bowers & Schwarz, 2013; Dimling,
Technology, NY
b 2010; Messier & Wood, 2015) that measured vocabulary
Disability and Psychoeducational Studies, University of Arizona,
Tucson comprehension.
c
Idaho Educational Services for the Deaf and Blind, Meridian Of these five studies, four studies (Dimling, 2010;
Correspondence to Jessica W. Trussell: jwtnmp@rit.edu Messier & Wood, 2015; Paatsch et al., 2006; Robbins &
Editor-in-Chief: Julie Barkmeier-Kraemer
Hatcher, 1981) included DHH students as old as 8–12 years
Editor: Patricia Brooks old. Although intervention for older DHH students is impor-
Received August 24, 2017 tant, vocabulary knowledge predicts DHH children’s later
Revision received December 18, 2017
Accepted March 20, 2018 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0085 of publication.

922 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018 • Copyright © 2018 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
reading achievement (Harris, Terlektsi, & Kyle, 2017; Kyle the child. Then, each time the adult asks the child a ques-
& Harris, 2006; Kyle & Harris, 2010). With this in mind, tion related to the pictures or story, the PEER cycle is
vocabulary interventions should occur earlier rather than followed (P = Prompt, E = Evaluate, E = Expand, R =
later (Meinzen-Derr, Wiley, Grether, & Choo, 2011). Reprompt; Whitehurst & Arnold, 1994). Prompt is the first
Furthermore, Dimling (2010), Paatsch et al. (2006), and step in the cycle. The adult prompts the child using one
Robbins and Hatcher (1981) used very limited words for of five question types also known as CROWD questions,
instruction (Dolch words: Dimling, 2010; monosyllabic because the first letter of each question type is noted in the
consonant–vowel–consonant words: Paatsch et al., 2006; acronym. “C” indicates a completion question (The ____ is
taught three unknown words: Robbins & Hatcher, 1981). flying.), “R” indicates a recall question (Where did Peter
Some final limitations to these studies were limiting instruc- Pan come from?), “O” indicates an open-ended question
tion to three words a week (Dimling, 2010), providing (Why do you think she is surprised?), “W” indicates a wh-
instruction that was not interactive (Messier & Wood, 2015), question (What is the captain standing on?), and “D” in-
not requiring an expressive linguistic response to demon- dicates a distancing question (What kinds of boats have
strating vocabulary understanding (Bowers & Schwarz, 2013), you been on?). Next, the adult evaluates and expands the
and implementing the intervention using a researcher (Bowers child’s response (Yes! That is a ship. That is a wooden
& Schwarz, 2013; Robbins & Hatcher, 1981) instead of a ship.). Last, the adult reprompts the child (What is the
teacher of the DHH (TODHH). Having teachers imple- captain standing on?). The end goal of dialogic reading is
ment the study rather than researchers maximizes ecological for the child to be able to retell the story independently
validity or the likelihood that the research can be imple- (Zevenbergen & Whitehurst, 2003).
mented in real classroom settings with fidelity (Kratochwill, ISR deviates from dialogic reading in that ISR com-
1978). bines the two levels of dialogic reading. ISR intends to
The purpose of this study was to address some of address the gaps in vocabulary knowledge of some children
the limitations of previous studies regarding instruction who are DHH while at the same time providing a chal-
on vocabulary meaning with young DHH students. We lenge. During ISR, the adult asks CROWD questions
implemented a modified interactive storybook reading using the PEER cycle, and the adult focuses on what is
(ISR) intervention. We provided instruction including five happening in the pictures rather than the story. The pur-
words (three nouns, one adjective, and one verb) a week, pose of the interaction is to develop targeted vocabulary
provided interactive instruction by the classroom TODHH, for the story. For example, a recall question in ISR would
and required the preschool-age student participants who be related to the events depicted rather than what is hap-
were DHH to express a definition to measure their under- pening in the text of the storyline.
standing. When looking at the vocabulary intervention Dialogic reading and ISR have been implemented
literature with young students who are DHH, ISR and dia- with students who were DHH in preschool through second
logic reading seem to be common intervention methods grade with a positive influence for picture labeling (Fung,
implemented to increase picture labeling abilities among Chow, & McBride-Chang, 2005; Trussell & Easterbrooks,
this group. 2014; Trussell, Hasko, Kane, & Cascioli, 2017). Fung and
colleagues (2005) compared dialogic reading, shared reading,
and a “business-as-usual” condition using a quasi-experimental
ISR and Students Who Are DHH design with kindergarten and first- and second-grade stu-
ISR instruction is grounded in the theory that chil- dents who were DHH (N = 28) matched on age and hear-
dren learn language through practicing language and ing ability. The student participants and their parents used
gaining feedback about language from adults, who are spoken language. The parents implemented dialogic read-
scaffolding the interactions carefully (Bandura, 1977; ing using scripted CROWD questions and picture prompts
Vygotsky, 1978). ISR was derived from dialogic reading during the storybook reading sessions. Upon conclusion
(Whitehurst et al., 1988). During dialogic reading, the of the study, the students who were in the dialogic reading
adult asks the child questions about the pictures or story condition gained more picture vocabulary than the student
using the book as a shared referent. There are two levels participants in the other two conditions. Furthermore,
of dialogic reading. The first level of dialogic reading was Trussell and Easterbrooks (2014) implemented ISR with
developed to increase the vocabulary skills of 2- to 3-year- five students who were DHH in preschool, kindergarten,
old children; the second level was designed to increase and first grades using a multiple probe across behaviors
the vocabulary and language skills of 4- to 5-year-old chil- single-case experimental design (SCED). The student par-
dren. The first level of dialogic reading, developed for ticipants used speech-supported sign language (speaking and
younger children, focuses on asking wh-questions to label signing at the same time using American Sign Language
pictures in the books. Once the children have learned the signs following English grammar). The researcher imple-
vocabulary related to the book, the adult can ask open- mented ISR using scripted CROWD question prompts and
ended questions about the book. The second level of dia- picture prompts during the ISR instructional sessions. A
logic reading, developed for preschool- or school-age functional relation was established between ISR and the
children, focuses on developing language and vocabulary picture labeling growth of the student participants who were
connected to the book. First, the adult reads the book to DHH. In 2017, Trussell and colleagues replicated the

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 923


previous study with two preschool classrooms. One class- to the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (2004),
room served students who were DHH and used spoken lan- had a vocabulary goal on their Individualized Education
guage, and one served students who were DHH and used Program indicating a need for specially designed vocabu-
speech-supported sign language. In the replication study, lary instruction, and attended a half-day preschool pro-
TODHHs implemented ISR using scripted CROWD ques- gram. All of the student participants were in their second
tion prompts and followed the PEER cycle. The TODHHs year of preschool (see Table 1 for further description of
did not use picture vocabulary prompts during the ISR the student participants). The teacher participant was
instructional sessions. A functional relation was established a certified teacher with 2 years of teaching experience
between ISR using the PEER cycle and picture vocabulary and 10 years of experience as a paraeducator in the
labeling. Implementing the PEER cycle may negate the same program.
need for picture vocabulary prompts during the instructional The preschool classroom was located in a public
sessions because the PEER cycle doubles the number of school in the northwest portion of the United States. Dur-
student responses elicited during ISR. ing the assessment probe sessions, the teacher worked with
Although the previously discussed studies indicate each student participant individually. They sat at a small
that ISR may be effective to increase the picture labeling table with the laptop between them. The student partici-
of students who are DHH, the studies do have limitations. pants could clearly see the TODHH’s face. ISR took place
Trussell and Easterbrooks (2014) and Fung et al. (2005) in the preschool classroom in a small group setting of six
do not mention using the PEER sequence during instruc- student participants. The student participants sat in chairs
tion. Furthermore, both studies used picture prompts during in a semicircle setup with the teacher participant across
instruction. Trussell et al. (2017) addressed the limitations them. The teacher participant used a sound-field FM system
of Trussell and Easterbrooks (2014) and Fung et al. by in- during ISR instruction. The FM system was not used dur-
cluding the PEER sequence and not using picture prompts ing assessment probe sessions because the student participants
during instruction. However, none of the studies reviewed did not have personal FM systems. The student participants
included a generalization measure or went beyond picture could clearly see the TODHH’s face and the book.
labeling to address the second part of the definition of
vocabulary—word meaning. Because ISR is developmen-
tally appropriate for young students who are DHH (Aram
Materials
& Biron, 2004) and appears to improve the picture labeling Books
of this group (Fung et al., 2005; Trussell & Easterbrooks, The books met the following criteria: aligned with
2014; Trussell et al., 2017), we offer that ISR may be an the teacher participant’s unit-based curriculum (Trussell &
effective strategy to teach new words and their meanings in Easterbrooks, 2014), contained many illustrations with
the context of a storybook. few words on the page and a simple plot (Zevenbergen &
In the current study, we modified ISR to include teach- Whitehurst, 2003), and were readily available children’s
ing the meaning of the target vocabulary as well as the picture books. The three books were Hansel and Gretel retold by
label. The vocabulary meaning portion of the intervention Ronne Randall during Week 1 (Book 1), Puss in Boots by
was added by asking questions such as “What does family Lisa Findlay during Week 2 (Book 2), and Peter Pan re-
mean?”, then following the PEER cycle to provide a kid- told by Carol Ottolenghi during Week 3 (Book 3). Before
friendly definition, and, finally, reprompting the child to instruction, the books were given a letter and taught in
elicit the definition given by the TODHH. random order determined by random.org. Books 1, 2, and
The purpose of this study was to extend the findings 3, as detailed above, were implemented with all student
and address the limitations of previous research (Fung participants.
et al., 2005; Trussell & Easterbrooks, 2014; Trussell et al.,
2017) to determine what effect ISR with word meaning Technology
instruction has on preschoolers’ ability to learn new words The primary author designed PowerPoint presenta-
and their meanings. The research questions were as fol- tions containing pictures of the target vocabulary from the
lows: What effect does ISR with word meaning instruction books. In addition, the primary author designed generali-
have on the vocabulary knowledge of preschoolers who zation PowerPoint presentations containing black and
are DHH? Will the knowledge generalize to other pictures? white line drawings from the clipart. The primary author
Will the knowledge maintain over time? shared the material via cloud storage. The teacher partic-
ipant recorded all sessions using the digital camera inte-
grated into her laptop.
Method
Participants and Setting Research Design
Six preschool students who are DHH and one TODHH A multiprobe across behaviors SCED (Gast &
were included in the study. The student participants used Ledford, 2014) was utilized to determine the effect of
spoken English, were found eligible for special education ser- ISR with word meaning instruction on student participants’
vices under the category of deaf or hard of hearing according picture labeling and word meaning knowledge. Multiprobe

924 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
Table 1. Participant demographics.

Participant Agea Gender Amp 4-Hz unaided PTA (L/R) (dB) Comm mode Language at home

Cynthia 5;5 F 2 HA 23/4 Speech English


Gadina 5;7 F 2 HA 46/48 Speech Spanish
Kaliyah 5;8 F 2 HA 60/55 Speech English
Lashya 4;10 F CI 74/CI Speech English
Parlan 4;5 M None 5/65 Speech English
Zachariah 5;10 M CI CI/15 Speech English

Note. Amp = amplification; PTA = pure-tone average; L = left; R = right; Comm = communication; F = female;
HA = hearing aid; CI = cochlear implant; M = male.
a
Age expressed in years;months.

across behaviors design is useful to answer demonstrative- Teacher (evaluation and expansion): “Almost right,
type research questions where the skill or behavior (vocabu- captain means the boss of a ship.”
lary knowledge) may not be unlearned in a short period Teacher (reprompt): “What does captain mean?”
(3–4 weeks). Multiple probe across behaviors SCED stud- During the first or second session for each book,
ies demonstrate experimental control when the following the student participants did not know the answers to the
requirements are met: (a) a minimum of three tiers (i.e., sets book. In these cases, the teacher participant answers the
of target vocabulary with three nouns, one adjective, and questions and provides a reprompt:
one verb; Trussell & Easterbrooks, 2014; Trussell et al., 2017)
that are similar yet functionally independent are identified, Teacher (prompt): “Who is standing on the ship?”
(b) condition change criteria (i.e., stability of behavior in Student: (No response)
baseline phase and increasing tread in intervention phases) Teacher (evaluation and expansion): “The captain.
are selected, (c) the dependent variable is measured repeat- The captain is standing on the ship.”
edly and simultaneously, (d) changes in the dependent Teacher (reprompt): “Who is standing on the ship?”
variable coincide with the introduction of the independent The teacher participant used paperback books and
variable, and (e) consistent behavior change occurs every did not show the student participants the PowerPoint slides
time the independent variable is introduced for a minimum during ISR.
of three different and temporally related time points (Gast &
Ledford, 2014). Furthermore, this design type mirrors the in-
Dependent Variable
dividualized nature of special education (Horner et al., 2005).
The dependent variable was correctly labeling the
In addition, all students receive instruction, and instruction is
picture (retrieved from the book) of the target vocabulary
not concluded until some level of mastery is reached (Gast
as well as explaining what the word meant. The vocabulary
& Ledford, 2014). This study had four phases across the
pictures were displayed on a laptop using PowerPoint soft-
three behaviors or sets of vocabulary words and definitions.
ware. The PowerPoint slides were white with a single-color
picture from the book (Sarchet et al., 2014; Trussell et al.,
Intervention 2017). The dependent variable was scored in two parts, label-
Independent Variable ing and definition. For labeling, the student participant
The teacher participant implemented ISR for 15– could receive a “1” (knew) or “0” (did not know). For the
20 min a day, 4 days a week for approximately 3 weeks. definition, the student participant could receive a “0” (did
The teacher participant used the book as a shared referent not know),”1” (knew the definition partially; e.g., family
with the students and did not read the book. The text in means connected), or “2” (knew the definition; e.g., family
the book could be seen by the teacher and student partici- means people who are connected; Dimling, 2010). The stu-
pants during instruction. The teacher participant followed dent participants were given one opportunity to provide
the PEER cycle by asking the CROWD questions about the label and the definition. They could earn 3 points per
the book’s pictures. Here are two examples from this study: word, equaling 15 points overall.
(a) Teacher (prompt): “Who is standing on the ship?”
Student: “Captain.” Procedure
Teacher (evaluation and expansion): “Yes, the
Recruitment
captain is standing on the ship.”
The primary author had professional connections
Teacher (reprompt): “Who is standing on the ship?”
with the state’s deaf education outreach program; therefore,
Student: “Captain is on the ship.”
the teacher and student participants were a convenience
(b) Teacher (prompt): “What does captain mean?” sample based on these connections. The primary author
Student: “Boss.” obtained institutional review board approval as well as

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 925


recruited and obtained consent from the teacher participant. about adding the word meaning to the ISR instruction.
The teacher participant gave letters to the parents of those The primary author searched the literature base to deter-
students who qualified for the study. One hundred percent mine if ISR including word meaning instruction had been
of the parents granted permission for their children to done. No studies were located. The primary author and
participate in the study. Finally, the teacher participant teacher participant decided to modify ISR to include
gained assent from each student participant at the begin- word meaning instruction.
ning of each assessment probe and ISR session by asking
a version of the following questions: “Do you want to Professional Development for ISR With
look at some pictures? Do you want to talk about the pic- Word Meaning Instruction
tures in this book?” The primary author and teacher participant met four
times for 2 hr via videoconferencing technology. During
Professional Development for Traditional ISR the first session, the two chose books and potential target
The initial professional development regarding ISR vocabulary. During the second session, the primary author,
without word meaning instruction was conducted during the teacher participant, and a third veteran TODHH met
the summer before the study. The primary author conducted to determine how word meaning instruction would be
two 4-hr sessions for TODHHs on how to implement tradi- added to ISR without modifying it greatly. The three indi-
tional ISR. During the first session, the primary author viduals came to an agreement to add the question “What
explained how to choose books and target vocabulary, write does (insert target vocabulary here) mean?” and follow
CROWD questions, and conduct the PEER cycle. During the PEER cycle to reinforce the meaning for each target
the second session, the teacher participants role-played ISR vocabulary. During the third session, the teacher partici-
with each other and received feedback from the primary pant role-played ISR with word meaning with the third
author. The role-play sessions were recorded, and the pri- teacher, and the session was recorded. The primary author
mary author applied a fidelity checklist (see Figure 1 for an conducted procedural fidelity (see Figure 1) and provided
example). The primary author reviewed the results from feedback. During the last session, the teacher participant
the checklist and provided actionable feedback to the teacher role-played a final session and achieved 92% (Kennedy,
participant. The teacher participant began implementing 2005; Trussell et al., 2017) procedural fidelity. The teacher
traditional ISR in the fall. Near the end of the fall semester, participant was prepared to implement ISR with word
the teacher participant approached the primary author meaning component in her classroom.

Figure 1. A portion of the procedural fidelity data collection tool.

926 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
Vocabulary Targets conclusion of this research study: strongly agree–5–4–3–2–
Before implementing the ISR instruction, the teacher 1–strongly disagree) and two open-ended questions (e.g.,
participant and primary author chose books that aligned DHH children differ greatly from one another. Do you see
with the curriculum. After the books were chosen, the this strategy as appropriate for all children or for only
teacher participant and primary author chose 10 poten- some and why?). The teacher participant’s response to the
tial vocabulary words from each book. Kid-friendly defini- survey are detailed in the Results section.
tions for the potential vocabulary (Beck, McKeown, &
Kucan, 2006) were cross-referenced from two online, read- Procedural Fidelity and Interrater
ily available dictionaries from Merriam-Webster (www. Reliability Data Collection
learnersdictionary.com [developed for students who are The probe assessment and intervention instructional
English language learners] and www.wordcentral.com sessions were digitally recorded to collect procedural fidel-
[developed for students]). The potential vocabulary and ity and interrater reliability (see Figure 1 for an example
definitions were also reviewed and approved by the teacher of the fidelity data sheet). Before the study, the primary
participant and the preschool program’s lead TODHH author randomly chose 50% of the assessment and instruc-
with 10 years of teaching experience. The student partici- tional sessions to collect procedural fidelity. The primary
pants were pretested on the 30 potential vocabulary words. author numbered the sessions and entered them into www.
The words and meanings that most of the students did random.org by phase. The chosen sessions were equally
not know were chosen as target vocabulary for the study. spaced throughout baseline, intervention, and maintenance
The final list of targets and their definitions are presented phases.
in Table 2. The teacher participant and other related service
providers (e.g., speech-language therapists, physical thera- Fidelity and Reliability Training
pists, and paraprofessionals) agreed not to instruct the The primary author trained the fourth and fifth au-
target vocabulary labels or meanings during any time out- thors (GRAs) how to rate the intervention sessions using
side the modified ISR instruction. practice videos developed from ISR trainings for teachers.
The GRAs coded one assessment probe and intervention
ISR With Word Meaning Scripts session from the first week to determine if they were rating
A graduate research assistant (GRA) drafted poten- similarly. The primary author compared their ratings and
tial CROWD question prompts to elicit the target vocabu- clarified the evaluation part of PEER. The GRAs coded
lary and work meanings. The primary author ensured a second assessment probe and intervention session, and
each word and meaning was targeted four to five times in their ratings were compared. The reliability between the
each book and that a variety of CROWD question types GRAs was 96% for assessment sessions and 98% for in-
were drafted. The primary author and teacher participant tervention sessions. This met the preestablished criteria
revised and approved the scripted CROWD questions for of 95% to continue collecting procedural fidelity and
Books 1–3 (see Figure 1 for an example of the script). reliability.

Social Validity Data Collection Preassessment


At the conclusion of the study, the primary author The student participants were pretested on 30 po-
emailed the teacher participant a researcher-created social tential vocabulary words, 10 from each book. Words
validity survey. The survey included five Likert-style state- and their meanings that were unfamiliar to most of the
ments (e.g., I will implement this intervention after the students were chosen for instruction. ISR was implemented

Table 2. Target vocabulary words and definitions.

Book Word Word meaning

Hansel and Gretel Family A group of people who are related to each other
Worried Feeling or showing fear because you think that something bad happened or might happen
Trunk The thick part of the tree growing out of the ground
Wake up To stop sleeping
Evil Very bad
Puss in Boots Sack A bag made of paper, plastic, or cloth
Carriage Has four wheels and is pulled by horses
Field An open area of land without trees or houses
Scared Feeling afraid or nervous
Hold To keep something in your hands or arms
Peter Pan Captain The boss of a ship
Leave To go away quickly
Plank A thick piece of heavy wood
Fairy A made-up small person with magic powers and wings
Surprised Feeling when something unexpected happens

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 927


with three books; each book had five target vocabulary Generalization
words (three nouns, one adjective, and one verb; Trussell A generalization probe session occurred after mas-
& Easterbrooks, 2014; Trussell et al., 2017) and their tery criteria were met for each intervention phase. The gen-
definitions. eralization probe consisted of a black-and-white line clipart
drawing of the target vocabulary from the recently con-
cluded intervention phase. The black-and-white drawing
Baseline
was not from the books. The teacher participant fol-
During the baseline phase, each student participant
lowed the same assessment procedures as described during
worked with the teacher participant individually during
baseline.
probe assessment sessions. First, the teacher participant
asked for the picture label. The teacher participant showed
the student participant the slide of the picture vocabulary Maintenance
on a laptop and asked “What is that?” The student partici- Maintenance data were collected four and eight ses-
pant was allowed 5 s to respond. The teacher participant sions after the generalization data were collected. During
wrote a “1” (knew) or “0” (did not know) to indicate if those weeks, none of the target vocabulary was instructed.
the student labeled the picture correctly. The student par- During the maintenance probe, the teacher participant
ticipants’ articulation errors were not considered if the followed the same procedures as described during baseline.
teacher participant could understand the student partici- The maintenance probe consisted of the same pictures that
pant. Then, the teacher participant checked for the were used during baseline and intervention (i.e., the pic-
meaning by asking “What does that mean?” The student ture of the target vocabulary from the book).
participant was allowed 8 s to respond. After the student
participant did or did not respond, the teacher partici- Fidelity and Reliability
pant recorded a “0” (did not know),”1” (knew the defi- Assessment Sessions
nition partially; e.g., family means connected), or “2” The fourth author collected procedural fidelity on
(knew the definition; e.g., family means people who are 50% of the probe assessment sessions. For procedural fidel-
connected; Dimling, 2010). If the student participant ity, the following teacher participant behaviors were noted:
responded incorrectly or did not respond, the teacher asking for assent, saying the scripted prompt for each
participant moved on to the next picture. The student picture, providing no feedback to the student participant
participants were given one opportunity to provide the la- about each item, and delivering general praise for effort
bel and the definition. The teacher participant did not to conclude the session. Furthermore, the fourth author
provide feedback about the accuracy of the student par- watched 50% of the assessment sessions and marked the
ticipants’ responses but provided only general feedback student participants’ responses. The teacher followed the
on effort, such as “You really tried hard today! Thank assessment protocol 97% of the time on average (range =
you!” The number of vocabulary targets assessed in each 75%–100%). The teacher and fourth author agreed on the
session varied from 5 to 15 depending on which books score (0 or 1 for labeling and 0, 1, or 2 for the definition)
were being probed at the time. for the students’ responses 94% of the time on average
Baseline for each set of target vocabulary was estab- (range = 70%–100%). The fifth author watched 30% of
lished through a stable data path (i.e., minimum variabil- the assessment videos included in the 50% of videos watched
ity in the data path with no trend or slope; Kazdin, 2011). by the fourth author. The same behaviors were noted.
Once baseline was established for Book 1, intervention Point-by-point agreement was calculated (i.e., Agreements/
was initiated. Books 2 and 3’s baseline data were collected Agreements + Disagreements × 100; Kazdin, 2011). For
through probes. Baseline data for Books 2 and 3 were estab- the assessment sessions, interrater reliability average was
lished through a minimum of five probes, with three of 91.7% (range = 80%–100%). Most disagreement among
those probes occurring consecutively before intervention the teacher participant and fourth and fifth authors was
with stable data path (Kazdin, 2011). scoring the word meanings (0 = did not know, 1 = partially
knew, and 2 = knew). With that being said, agreement was
Intervention still in the acceptable range.
During the intervention phase, individual assessment
sessions occurred before instruction each day, and the pro- Intervention Sessions
cedure just described was followed. The procedure for ISR The fourth author watched 50% of the intervention
instruction during the intervention phase is the same as sessions and recorded if the teacher participant asked the
described under the previous section labeled Independent scripted questions and followed the PEER cycle. During
Variable. Mastery criteria for phase change were a mini- procedural fidelity and reliability for the intervention
mum of five data points demonstrating an increasing trend sessions, we measured the following teacher behaviors:
in the phase. If the student participant persisted for eight (a) asking for assent, (b) saying question prompt as scripted,
sessions without reaching mastery criteria, the student par- (c) evaluating the students’ responses (defined as saying
ticipant would move on to the next book (Gast & Ledford, similar words or phrases—“Yes!”, “Right!”, “Good”, “Let’s
2014). The next phase was generalization. try/look again”, and/or “No”—or a head shake or head

928 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
nod paired with an appropriate facial expression; Trussell all the books. The only exception was Kaliyah. Kaliyah’s
et al., 2017), (d) expanding the student response (defined data path during intervention instruction for Book 3 did
as rephrasing and adding one piece of information to the not demonstrate a trend but did indicate a change in level
student participant’s response), and (e) reprompting the stu- from baseline to intervention phase of Book 3. In regard
dent participants using the scripted question (see Figure 1 to immediacy of effect, Cynthia’s data paths demonstrated
for the procedural fidelity checklist). At times, the student immediacy of effect of the intervention instruction for all
participants did not respond to the scripted questions. We three books. Kaliyah, Lashya, and Zachariah’s data paths
noted a nonresponse as “NR” (no response) and did not in- indicated immediacy of effect of the intervention instruc-
clude evaluation as part of that PEER cycle opportunity. tion for Books 1 and 2. Parlan’s data paths showed an im-
For the intervention sessions, the teacher participant asked mediacy of effect of the intervention instruction for Book 3
the scripted questions 91% of the time on average (range = only. Gadina’s data paths did not indicate an immediacy
82%–98%) and completed the PEER sequence 81% of of effect for the intervention instruction. Generally, all
the time on average (range = 65%–88%; prompt = 91%, of the student participants’ data paths were consistent be-
evaluation = 91%, expansion = 79%, reprompt = 62%). tween phases with the exception of Parlan and Zack’s base-
The teacher participant most often did not ask the reprompt line data paths in Books 1 and 2, respectively. Five student
at the end of the PEER sequence when the student partici- participants’ data paths were consistent across phases for
pants were able to answer the first prompt accurately. To Books 1–3 with the exception of Kaliyah’s data path from
determine interrater reliability, the fifth author watched baseline to intervention phase for Book 2. All of the stu-
30% of 50% of the intervention videos. Point-by-point dent participants were able to generalize the word meaning
agreement was calculated. For the intervention sessions, knowledge they gained during intervention instruction.
interrater reliability average was 97% (range = 96%–98%). All of the student participants remembered what they
learned during intervention. There are no generalization or
maintenance data for Gadina. She was absent on those ses-
Results sions. Overall gains for word definitions before and after
intervention instruction can be found in Figure 5. All
Student participants’ data are presented in Figures 2
of the student participants had less than 20% average
and 3. All student participants’ names are pseudonyms.
word meaning before instruction, and all of them gained
Data for Session 6 are missing for all student participants
a minimum of three times their previous knowledge after
because of a school holiday. All other missing data points
intervention instruction began, from 0% to more than 90%
indicate student participant absences. Because of timing,
word meaning knowledge on average.
maintenance data for Book 1 were taken at 2 and 4 weeks
out; however, maintenance data for Books 2 and 3 were
taken early because the school year ended. Data were ana- Word Labeling
lyzed at the individual level for the following features: sta-
bility (50% on either side of the mean; Kazdin, 2011), level Word labeling results are indicated by a triangle in
(mean of the data in a phase), trend (direction the data Figures 2–4. All of the student participants’ baseline data
path is displaying), immediacy of effect (the last data point paths exhibit stability before intervention instruction began
in the baseline phase compared with the first data point in with the exception of Kaliyah for Book 1. Her baseline
the intervention phase), consistency (data paths look simi- data path increases slightly before baseline. Most of the
lar within phases [comparing baseline to baseline] and student participants’ data paths demonstrated a change in
across phases [baseline to intervention of one tier compared level from the baseline to intervention phase for all books.
with baseline to intervention of another tier]; Gast & Parlan’s data path did not demonstrate a change in level
Ledford, 2014), and percentage of nonoverlapping data from baseline to intervention for Book 1. Similarly, Lashya’s
(percentage of data points that do not overlap when com- data path did not demonstrate a change in level for Book 2.
paring baseline with intervention phases; Kratochwill Five of the six student participants’ data paths during the
et al., 2013). The student participants’ performance on intervention phase for all the books demonstrated an increas-
word meaning is indicated by a circle, and word labeling ing trend. On the other hand, Gadina’s data path during
is indicated by a triangle. the intervention phase for Book 3 was variable. For Books 1
and 3, all of the student participants’ data paths indicated
immediacy of effect of the intervention instruction on word
Word Meaning labeling with the exception of Gadina for Book 1. For
As seen in Figures 2–4, the student participants had Book 2, Lashya was the only student participant whose data
stable baseline data paths before interventions for Books path indicated an immediacy of effect of the intervention in-
1–3 with the exception of Parlan for Book 1. However, struction. In regard to consistency between phases, Cynthia
Parlan’s baseline data path for Book 1 was stable before and Zachariah’s data paths during the baseline phase for
intervention. All of the student participants’ data paths Book 2 had a higher mean than their other baselines. Parlan
demonstrated a change in level from baseline to interven- had more variability in his baseline and intervention data
tion for each book. Similarly, all of the students’ data paths for Book 1 when compared with Books 2 and 3. In
paths trended upward during the intervention phases for regard to consistency across phases, the student participants’

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 929


Figure 2. Cynthia and Gadina’s graphs. BL = baseline; Int. = intervention; G = generalization; M = maintenance.

data paths demonstrated consistency across phases with the she mentioned that she thought ISR word meaning instruc-
exception of Parlan discussed previously. All of the stu- tion “was only helpful if the students already understand
dent participants remembered and were able to generalize the standard classroom routines.” She agreed strongly that
word labels they learned during intervention. Overall gains the intervention itself was easy to teach and sustainable.
for word labeling before and after intervention instruction When asked what she would change, the teacher partici-
can be found in Figure 5. Similar to the word meaning pre- pant would like to find a way to make the script easier to
average and postaverage, five of the students (Cynthia, use. She had to flip a book page and a script page. Then,
Gadina, Kaliyah, Lashya, and Parlan) learned three times she had to look down at her script before speaking. She
the number of word labels they knew before intervention reported this process as being “cumbersome.”
instruction. Zachariah learned about double the number of
word labels he knew before intervention instruction.
Discussion
Social Validity The purpose of the current study was to determine
The teacher participant strongly agreed that ISR what effect ISR with word meaning instruction had on the
with word meaning instruction benefited her students, but vocabulary knowledge of preschoolers who are DHH and

930 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
Figure 3. Kaliyah and Lashya’s graphs. BL = baseline; Int. = intervention; G = generalization; M = maintenance.

use spoken English. We implemented a multiple probe These findings support previous research (Fung et al., 2005;
across behaviors SCED study to answer the first research Mueller & Hurtig, 2009; Trussell & Easterbrooks, 2014;
question: What effect does ISR with word meaning instruc- Trussell et al., 2017) demonstrating that ISR promotes
tion have on the vocabulary knowledge of preschoolers picture vocabulary learning in young children who are
who are DHH? When looking at the graphs for the student DHH. In addition, this finding extends previous research
participants, a functional relation was established between to indicate that ISR with word meaning instruction can
ISR with word meaning instruction and the student par- promote word definition knowledge in preschool children
ticipants’ ability to express a definition. For Cynthia, the who are DHH and use spoken English.
mean change across conditions was higher than the mean The secondary research questions (Will the knowl-
change within conditions for all three books. This was repli- edge generalize to other pictures? Will the knowledge main-
cated by Gadina, Kaliyah, Lashya, Parlan, and Zachariah. tain over time?) were answered through probe sessions after
Furthermore, all the students’ data paths trended upward the conclusion of the intervention. The student partici-
during instruction (Gast & Ledford, 2014). The incon- pants were able to generalize the vocabulary knowledge
sistency among the students regarding the immediacy of they gained from color pictures that were taken from
effect of the intervention weakens the functional relation the books to black-and-white line drawings. In addition, the
and requires further discussion (Kratochwill et al., 2013). student participants maintained what they learned over

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 931


Figure 4. Parlan and Zachariah’s graphs. BL = baseline; Int. = intervention; G = generalization; M = maintenance.

time. These findings indicate that ISR with word meaning providing a kid-friendly definition, there are other devel-
instruction may have a lasting effect on the vocabulary opmentally appropriate ways to measure word meaning
knowledge of children who are DHH and use spoken with young children. One way would be to notice the
English. Furthermore, the preschoolers may be able to child using the new word correctly in a different context.
take what they learn in ISR and apply it to novel mate- In addition, the teacher or parent could ask the child
rials without explicit instruction on how to generalize a question or a series of questions about the word that
information. would indicate comprehension such as “Can you tell me
The initial point for discussion is the measure used in about a time you have been scared?” Although the stu-
the current study. Preschool state standards (Idaho State dent participants in this study could recite the definition
Department of Education, 2012; New Jersey State Depart- taught during the intervention, this finding must be inter-
ment of Education, 2014; New York State Department preted with caution. These student participants may merely
of Education, 2011; Ohio State Department of Education, have been memorizing the definition and would not have
n.d.) suggest that expressing the meaning of unknown been able to apply the meaning to other contexts outside
words in the context of a storybook is a benchmark for this ISR interaction.
emergent readers. As is common with standards, there is The lack of immediacy of effect and the low percent-
little guidance on what this benchmark looks like in the age of nonoverlapping data for Book 2 require further
classroom. Although we interpreted this standard to include investigation. When looking at the data paths, the student

932 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
Figure 5. The average of the last three data points in baseline phases compared with the average of the last three
data points in intervention phases for all the books, separated by student.

participants knew more of the target vocabulary words instruction in one-to-one sessions may lend better to the
during the baseline phase of Book 2 when compared with therapeutic or itinerant TODHH model. Speech-language
the baseline phases for Books 1 and 3. Several of the stu- pathologists and itinerant TODHHs tend to work with
dent participants knew the target words scared, field, and children who are DHH in one-to-one settings or very
carriage before instruction. Not only did they label scared, small groups (no more than two students). ISR with word
many of them could partially define scared. In addition, meaning instruction may be more effective if delivered in
the teacher participant reported that the picture of field smaller settings that are not always feasible for a preschool
was confusing. For example, Parlan could label and define classroom with one teacher and many students.
“field” during the intervention sessions when the picture When looking at the data paths for Book 3, the stu-
was in context. Yet, when only the portion of the picture dent participants learned the words and definitions more
from the book was presented, he struggled to label “field.” quickly than those for Books 1 and 2. This finding con-
The pictures used for assessment were not vetted by the firms previous findings (Trussell & Easterbrooks, 2014;
TODHH. Perhaps, we should have replaced the picture Trussell et al., 2017) that it takes a few weeks for students
when the TODHH noticed Parlan’s confusion. However, to become familiar with the ISR instruction itself and the
it is difficult to determine what is confusing for the stu- expectations during ISR. These student participants have
dent participants and what is generalizing information been participating in ISR with their teacher throughout
from instruction to assessment. Although the books were the year to learn picture labels; however, this was the first
implemented in random order, the difference in perfor- time the student participants were expected to learn more
mance for Book 2 could be due to the fact that it was im- than only the picture label. After Books 1 and 2, the stu-
plemented in the middle. In the current study, we were dent participants are familiar with the instruction itself and
attempting to implement larger group instruction (six stu- may be better able to predict what is going to occur during
dents and one teacher) to fit the established routines of instructional sessions.
the preschool classroom. In a similar classroom situation, When taking a closer look at the data for Books 1–3,
teachers may consider taking the opportunity to teach a the student participants were generally more successful
Tier 2 (Beck & McKeown, 2007) vocabulary word (e.g., learning the noun labels and definitions when compared
teaching terrified instead of scared) to increase the breadth with the verbs and adjectives. The only exception was the
and depth of students’ vocabulary knowledge. Another adjective mentioned above—scared. Learning nouns more
alternative would be one-to-one or small groups of two or easily is consistent with language acquisition norms for
three students engaging in ISR with the teacher. Smaller hearing children (MacNamara, 1972) and supports the qual-
groups or individual sessions might be more effective. If itative similarity hypothesis (Paul, Wang, Trezek, & Luckner,
the groups were smaller or one-to-one, the teacher could 2009), which proposes that children who are DHH learn
tailor the target vocabulary to each student’s needs and literacy skills in a similar developmental pattern as hearing
learning speed. Conducting ISR with word meaning children.

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 933


Gadina and Parlan appeared to benefit less from the research in ecologically valid settings (e.g., classrooms,
ISR instruction when compared with the other students. school therapy settings; Gast & Ledford, 2014) is important
In both cases, these student participants had different to determine if the instruction can be carried out in the
demographics and backgrounds than the other students. classroom by a teacher as it was designed by the researcher.
Parlan is younger than the other children, and Gadina The implications of this study begin in the classroom.
had frequent absences as well as a second language in the The importance of vocabulary knowledge to reading is
home. Furthermore, Gadina was given a secondary diag- well documented (Harris, Terlektsi, & Kyle, 2017; Kyle &
nosis of intellectual disability after the conclusion of the Harris, 2006, 2010, 2011). TODHHs are required to imple-
intervention instruction. When looking at the preaverage ment evidence-based vocabulary strategy practices (Every
and postaverage in Figure 5, it appears that ISR with word Student Succeeds Act, 2015). This study provides emergent
meaning instruction does not overcome the Matthew effect. evidence for using ISR with word meaning instruction to
Cynthia and Zachariah who started the intervention in- teach vocabulary words and definitions to preschool chil-
struction with more knowledge also ended the intervention dren students who are DHH and use spoken English. In
instruction with more knowledge. Perhaps, ISR with word addition, we designed this study to address a gap in the
meaning instruction is not the most effective instructional vocabulary strategies research addressing word meaning
strategy for those students who are DHH and use two or knowledge of students who are DHH. When the goal is to
more languages or have other diagnoses. A second explana- teach picture labeling with a kid-friendly definition, ISR
tion is the implementation fidelity average for the PEER with word meaning instruction has an emergent evidence
sequence. Although this study had a similar implementation base to support its use in the DHH preschool classroom
fidelity for the PEER sequence as past studies (Trussell that focuses on the development of spoken English.
et al., 2017), the TODHH most often forgot to reprompt The findings of this study are significant to the field
the student participants with the question again. This de- of special education because they further build the knowl-
creased the intensity of the ISR instruction and may have edge base surrounding effective vocabulary instruction strate-
had an effect on Gadina and Parlan’s performance. When gies. The most consistent predictor of later reading success
deciding the type of vocabulary instruction to use in the for children who are DHH is vocabulary knowledge (Harris,
classroom, TODHHs may consider modifying the ISR with Terlektsi, & Kyle, 2017). With this in mind, researchers
word meaning instruction for pacing, group instruction size, and teachers need to work together to determine the most
intensity, or duration. efficient and effective ways to increase the size of the vo-
The procedural fidelity averages were high in this study. cabularies of young children who are DHH. In addition,
The teacher participant was able to adhere to the plan of the student participants’ data paths indicate that ISR with
instruction and most often forgot to reprompt the student word meaning may be more effective in one-to-one settings or
participants. It must be noted that the teacher participant smaller groups. Potentially, speech-language pathologists
was already implementing ISR using the PEER cycle and or itinerant TODHHs could implement ISR with word
CROWD question prompts in her classroom before the meaning instruction. Similar to TODHHs, speech-language
study. We added the word meaning component because the pathologists who work in schools are required to deliver
teacher participant wondered if she could use ISR to teach evidence-based practices. ISR may be one to consider.
word definitions. Her previous experience with ISR may
have been critical to her ability to implement the instruction
with such high fidelity. In addition, the fourth author was Limitations and Future Research
collecting data on her procedural fidelity and reporting the The first limitation of this study was the small sam-
information to the primary author. If the teacher partici- ple size. The sample size was appropriate for an SCED.
pant fell below 80% fidelity for two consecutive sessions, However, the findings may not be generalizable to students
the primary author would provide feedback and coaching. or settings that are different from those represented in the
Initial professional development, but more importantly, current study. In addition, group design studies with a large
ongoing professional development, appears to be important enough sample population to ensure power is the gold stan-
when the goal is to implement ISR with fidelity (Guskey & dard for determining the effectiveness of an instructional
Yoon, 2009). When beginning ISR with word meaning strategy (Institute of Educational Sciences, 2014). Future
instruction, teachers should seek training as well as have researchers should consider implementing ISR with word
a fellow teacher or an administrator conduct fidelity meaning instruction with a larger number of preschoolers
checks. who are DHH and use spoken English using a randomized
Because of the fact that the researchers were not in controlled trial design. When considering scaling up to a
the classroom every day at all times, there is a small chance larger sample size, there are several findings from this study
that the preschoolers may have learned the vocabulary that future researchers should take into account. We in-
words and their meanings from other instruction. However, cluded extensive teacher training (8 hr). That amount of
the students’ data paths indicated that the students were teacher training may not be feasible in a large-scale study.
learning the words from the ISR with word meaning instruc- In addition, our assessment fidelity was at 94% on average
tion. This type of threat to validity is always a concern with a small number of students. When introducing more
when conducting studies in schools. However, conducting students, teachers, and variability, it may be difficult for

934 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
teachers to use the measurement system (0 = did not know, Without reinforcement, they responded inconsistently.
1 = partially knew, and 2 = knew) we used herein reliably. Implementing planned praise as part of the assessment
Furthermore, future researchers should replicate this study procedure (Gast & Ledford, 2014) may lead the students
with additional subpopulations of students who are DHH. to more consistent responding.
These subpopulations may include students who are DHH
and use American Sign Language or some combination of
spoken English and sign language. Conclusion
The second limitation of this study was the manner Previous ISR intervention studies (Trussell &
in which word meaning was defined and measured. Herein, Easterbrooks, 2014; Trussell et al., 2017) have mentioned
we used kid-friendly definitions to teach the meaning as that measuring only picture vocabulary labeling and not
well as measure if the students knew the meaning, although vocabulary comprehension was a limitation to their stu-
teaching kid-friendly definitions is a recommended practice dies. The current study aimed to address that limitation.
for instructing unfamiliar vocabulary (Beck et al., 2006). A functional relation was established between ISR with
Reciting definitions may not be the most authentic way word meaning instruction and the students’ ability to label
to measure if a student understands what a word means. a picture and give a kid-friendly definition. In addition,
Future researchers may want to consider teaching all as- the student participants were able to maintain what they
pects of a word (e.g., meaning [multiple if needed], exam- learned and generalize the knowledge to different pic-
ples, nonexamples, synonyms, antonyms; Easterbrooks tures. ISR appears to be a dynamic instructional strategy
& Beal-Alvarez, 2013) and measuring comprehension by that can help TODHHs meet various goals in their class-
eliciting a sentence or phrase using the vocabulary word rooms (e.g., picture labeling or word definitions). Future re-
to measure if the student knows what the word means as searchers may want to implement the strategy with larger
well as the picture label (Dimling, 2010) or prompting the groups of children who are DHH and determine a more
students’ use of the word in authentic contexts. authentic manner to measure vocabulary comprehension
The third limitation of this study was that mainte- in this population.
nance data were not collected properly for Book 3 because
school came to an end. However, predicting the students’
maintenance outcomes using the data from Books 1 and 2, References
one may project that the students would remember the
Aram, D., & Biron, S. (2004). Joint storybook reading and joint
words and meanings from Book 3 for at least 4 weeks post- writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differen-
instruction. At the same time, further data collection on tial contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research
maintenance would be helpful to investigate if ISR with Quarterly, 19(4), 588–610. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.
word meaning lends to sustainable vocabulary knowledge. 10.003
Future researchers may want to collect maintenance data Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
at 6 and 8 weeks postintervention to see how the students’ Prentice Hall.
new vocabulary knowledge changes over time. Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2007). Increasing young low-
income children’s vocabulary repertoires through rich and
Fourth, each book was used in multiple sessions
focused instruction. The Elementary School Journal, 107(3),
limiting our ability to determine if ISR with word mean- 251–271. https://doi.org/10.1086/511706
ing instruction, repeated experiences with the book and Beck, I. L., McKeown, M.G., & Kucan, L. (2006). Bringing words to
words, or the combination of both caused the change in life: Robust vocabulary instruction. New York, NY: Guildford
the students’ behavior. Future researchers may implement Press.
ISR with word meaning instruction using a different book Bowers, L. M., & Schwarz, I. (2013). Assessing response to basic
for each session. Designing the study with many different concept instruction: Preliminary evidence with children who
books may show if ISR with word meaning instruction are deaf. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 34(4), 221–231.
has the same or different effect on students’ vocabulary https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740112469662
Dimling, L. M. (2010). Conceptually based vocabulary interven-
knowledge. In addition, the social validity findings are
tion: Second graders’ development of vocabulary words.
most likely biased. The teacher participant and primary American Annals of the Deaf, 155(4), 425–448. Retrieved from
author had a working relationship, and it was impossible http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21305978
for the teacher participant to answer the survey anony- Easterbrooks, S., & Beal-Alvarez, J. (2013). Literacy instruction
mously. In the future, researchers could collect social valid- for students who are deaf and hard of hearing. New York, NY:
ity from the student participants or attempt to find a way Oxford University Press.
not to introduce bias. Every Student Succeeds Act. (2015). 20 U.S.C. 6301 et seq. Re-
Similar to previous studies (Trussell & Easterbrooks, trieved from https://www.whitehouse.gov/the-press-office/2015/
2014; Trussell et al., 2017), some student participants’ 12/10/white-house-report-every-student-succeeds-act
Fung, P. C., Chow, B. W. Y., & McBride-Chang, C. (2005).
baseline data paths demonstrated variability. The teacher The impact of a dialogic reading program on deaf and hard-
participant was instructed not to give praise or reinforce- of-hearing kindergarten and early primary school-aged
ment on the basis of performance during assessment sessions, students in Hong Kong. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
and this may explain the variability. The student partici- Education, 10(1), 82–95. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/
pants may not have been completely sure of their answers. eni005

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 935


Gast, D. L., & Ledford, J. R. (2014). Single case research method- Meinzen-Derr, J., Wiley, S., Grether, S., & Choo, D. I. (2011).
ology: Applications in special education and behavioral sciences. Children with cochlear implants and developmental dis-
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. abilities: A language skills study with developmentally
Guskey, T. R., & Yoon, K. S. (2009). What works in professional matched hearing peers. Research in Developmental Dis-
development? Phi Delta Kappan, 19(7), 495–500. https://doi. abilities, 32(2), 757–767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.
org/10.2307/20446159 11.004
Harris, M., Terlektsi, E., & Kyle, F. E. (2017). Concurrent and Messier, J., & Wood, C. (2015). Facilitating vocabulary acquisi-
longitudinal predictors of reading for deaf and hearing chil- tion of children with cochlear implants using electronic story-
dren in primary school. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Edu- books. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 20(4),
cation, 22(2), 233–242. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enw101 356–373. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/env03
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., Mcgee, G., Odom, S., & Moats, L. C. (2005). Language essentials for teachers of reading
Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject research to and spelling: Module 4. Boston, MA: Sopris West Educational
identify evidence-based practice in special education. Excep- Science.
tional Children, 71(2), 165–179. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Mueller, V., & Hurtig, R. (2009). Technology-enhanced shared
001440290507100203 reading with deaf and hard-of-hearing children: The role
Idaho State Department of Education. (2012). Idaho early child- of a fluent signing narrator. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
hood resource: Early learning guidelines. Retrieved from www. Education, 15(1), 72–101. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/
idahotc.com/earlychildhood enp023
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 New Jersey State Department of Education. (2014). Preschool
(2004) teaching and learning standards. Retrieved from http://www.
Institute of Educational Sciences. (2014). WWC procedures and state.nj.us/education/ece/guide/standards.pdf
standards handbooks version 3.0. Retrieved from http://ies.ed. New York State Department of Education. (2011). Prekinder-
gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_procedures_v3_0_ garten foundation for the common core. Retrieved from http://
standards_handbook.pdf www.p12.nysed.gov/ciai/common_core_standards/pdfdocs/
Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Single-case research designs: Methods for nyslsprek.pdf
clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford Ohio State Department of Education. (n.d.). Ohio’s early learning
University Press. and development standards: Birth to kindergarten entry. Lan-
Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educational research guage and literacy domain. Retrieved from https://education.ohio.
(2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. gov/getattachment/Topics/Early-Learning/Early-Learning-
Kratochwill, T. R. (1978). Single subject research: Strategies for Content-Standards/Birth-Through-Pre_K-Learning-and-
evaluating change. Cambridge, UK: MA Academic Press. Development-Stand/ELDS-Language-and-Literacy.pdf.aspx
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J. H., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R., Paatsch, L. E., Blamey, P. J., Sarant, J. Z., & Bow, C. P. (2006).
Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, W. R. (2013). The effects of speech production and vocabulary training on
Single-case intervention research design standards. Remedial different components of spoken language performance. Journal
and Special Education, 34(1), 26–38. https://doi.org//10.1177/ of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11(1), 39–55. https://doi.
0741932512452794 org/10.1093/deafed/enj008
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and pre- Paul, P., Wang, Y., Trezek, B., & Luckner, J. (2009). Phonology
dictors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf and is necessary, but not sufficient: A rejoinder. American Annals
hearing school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf of the Deaf, 154(4), 346–356. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.
Education, 11(3), 273–288. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20066917
enj037 Robbins, N., & Hatcher, C. (1981). The effects of syntax on the
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2010). Predictors of reading develop- reading comprehension of hearing-impaired children. The
ment in deaf children: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Volta Review, 83(2), 105–115.
Experimental Child Psychology, 107(3), 229–243. https://doi. Sarchet, T., Marschark, M., Borgna, G., Convertino, C. M.,
org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.011 Sapere, P., & Dirmyer, R. (2014). Vocabulary knowledge
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2011). Longitudinal patterns of emerg- of deaf and hearing postsecondary students. Journal of Post-
ing literacy in beginning deaf and hearing readers. Journal of secondary Education and Disability, 27(2), 161–178. https://
Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(3), 289–304. https://doi. doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1237430
org/10.1093/deafed/enq069 Trezek, B. J., Wang, Y., & Paul, P. V. (2011). Processes and com-
Luckner, J., & Cooke, C. (2010). A summary of the vocabulary ponents of reading. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.),
research with students who are deaf or hard of hearing. Ameri- The Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education
can Annals of the Deaf, 155(1), 38–67. Retrieved from http:// (2nd ed. Vol. 1, pp. 99–114). New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20503907 versity Press.
Luckner, J., & Handley, C. M. (2008). A summary of the reading Trussell, J. W., & Easterbrooks, S. R. (2014). The effect of enhanced
comprehension research undertaken with student who are storybook interaction on signing deaf children’s vocabulary.
deaf or hard of hearing. American Annals of the Deaf, 153(1), Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(3), 319–332.
6–36. https://doi.org/10.1353/aad.0.0006 https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent055
Luckner, J., & Urbach, J. (2011). Reading fluency and students Trussell, J. W., & Easterbrooks, S. R. (2016). Morphological knowl-
who are deaf or hard of hearing: Synthesis of the research. edge and students who are deaf or hard-of-hearing: A review of
Communications Disorders Quarterly, 33(4), 1–12. https://doi. the literature. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 38(2), 67–77.
org/10.1177/1525740111412582 https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740116644889
Macnamara, J. (1972). Cognitive basis of language learning in Trussell, J. W., Hasko, J., Kane, J., & Cascioli, T., (2017). The
infants. Psychological Review, 79(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/ effects of interactive storybook reading with preschoolers
h0031901 who are deaf or hard of hearing. Topics in Early Childhood

936 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 49 • 922–937 • October 2018
Special Education, 37(3), 146–163. https://doi.org/10.1177/ families. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679–689. https://doi.
0271121417720015 org/10.1037//0012-1649.30.5.679
Tucci, S. L., Trussell, J. W., & Easterbrooks, S. R. (2014). A re- Whitehurst, G., Falco, F., Lonigan, C., Fischel, J., Debaryshe, B.,
view of the evidence on strategies for teaching children who & Caulfield, M. (1988). Accelerating language develop-
are DHH grapheme–phoneme correspondence. Communication ment through picture book reading. Developmental Psychol-
Disorders Quarterly, 35(4), 191–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/ ogy, 24(4), 552–559. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.24.
1525740114523776 4.552
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of Zevenbergen, A., & Whitehurst, G. (2003). Dialogic reading: A
higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In
University Press. A. van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading
Whitehurst, G., & Arnold, D. (1994). A picture book reading inter- books to children: Parents and teachers (pp. 177–200).
vention in day care and home for children from low-income Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Trussell et al.: Interactive Storybook Reading 937


Copyright of Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools is the property of American
Speech-Language-Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to
multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen